You are on page 1of 9

ARTICLE IN PRESS

Behaviour Research and Therapy xxx (2009) 1–9

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Behaviour Research and Therapy


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/brat

Treatment of obsessions: A randomized controlled trial


Maureen L. Whittal a, b, *, Sheila R. Woody b, Peter D. McLean b, S.J. Rachman b, Melisa Robichaud a, b
a
University of British Columbia Hospital, Vancouver, BC, Canada V6T 2A1
b
Department of Psychiatry, University of British Columbia and the Vancouver CBT Centre, Canada

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: This study tested Rachman’s cognitive behavioral method for treating obsessions not accompanied by
Received 8 July 2009 prominent overt compulsions. The cognitive behavioral treatment was compared to waitlist control and
Received in revised form an active and credible comparison of stress management training (SMT). Of the 73 adults who were
16 November 2009
randomized, 67 completed treatment, and 58 were available for one-year follow-up. The active treat-
Accepted 17 November 2009
ments, compared to waitlist, resulted in substantially lower YBOCS scores, OCD-related cognitions and
depression as well as improved social functioning. Overall, CBT and SMT showed large and similar
Keywords:
reductions in symptoms. Pre–post effect sizes on YBOCS Obsessions for CBT and SMT completers was
Obsessions
Cognitive therapy d ¼ 2.34 and 1.90, respectively. Although CBT showed small advantages over SMT on some symptom
Stress management measures immediately after treatment, these differences were no longer apparent in the follow-up
period. CBT resulted in larger changes on most OCD-related cognitions compared to SMT. The cognitive
changes were stable at 12 months follow-up, but the differences in the cognitive measures faded. The
robust and enduring effects of both treatments contradict the long-standing belief that obsessions are
resistant to treatment.
Ó 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

To meet diagnostic criteria for obsessive–compulsive disorder Because these appraisals raise anxiety about the self and the
(OCD), an individual must experience recurrent, egodystonic, future, they also motivate efforts to neutralize the thought or
anxiety-producing obsessions or excessive, ritualistic compulsive decrease the perceived probability of the feared event (e.g., acting
behaviors (APA, 1994). Early OCD studies focused on people with on the content of the thought) by avoidance or compulsive
observable compulsive behaviors (e.g., checkers and washers), as behavior. For example, egodystonic intrusions of intentionally
the overt nature of the compulsions was more easily adapted to the harming children may be interpreted as signaling that the person is
behavioral treatment methods of the time. Covert compulsions and evil and potentially dangerous and hence should avoid being alone
obsessions were more difficult to address. Behavioral strategies of with children. Consistent with other cognitive theories, Rachman
imaginal or in vivo exposure to obsessions typically produce high (1997, 1998, 2003) emphasized that the appraisal of personal
levels of anxiety and can be difficult to tolerate. significance, not the content of the thought, is pivotal in generating
Occasional intrusive thoughts are common. Rachman and de the distress, frequency and behavioral disturbance associated with
Silva (1978) and Salkovskis and Harrison (1984) indicated that at obsessions. Cognitive–behavioral treatment derived from this
least 90% of community and analogue samples report experiencing theory is designed to reduce or eliminate the person’s maladaptive
unwanted intrusive thoughts, the content of which can be similar to appraisals of the personal significance of the intrusive thoughts.
those reported by people with OCD. However, the meaning given to Just two randomized trials have examined the treatment of
these intrusions is believed to differentiate clinical from nonclinical obsessions without prominent overt compulsions. In one of these
samples. Whereas the latter group tends to appraise intrusions as trials, Freeston et al. (1997) used a combined treatment with
relatively benign, individuals with OCD appraise the occurrence of imaginal exposure (i.e., loop tape exposure to the obsession) as the
such thoughts as personally significant (i.e., having the thought core of the treatment package but also included other cognitive
means I am ‘‘mad, bad, or dangerous’’). strategies. At post-test, 67% of the participants showed significant
change, declining to 53% at follow-up. These gains were produced
with considerable treatment effort (approximately 40 h/partici-
pant). Freeston, Leger, and Ladouceur (2001) subsequently pub-
lished a promising case series using cognitive therapy without
* Corresponding author at: University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
exposure. O’Connor et al. (2005) conducted a small randomized
V6T 2A1. Tel.: þ1 604 822 1789; fax: þ1 604 822 7452.
E-mail address: whittal@interchange.ubc.ca (M.L. Whittal). controlled study with obsessionals who were treated with

0005-7967/$ – see front matter Ó 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.brat.2009.11.010

Please cite this article in press as: Whittal, M.L., et al., Treatment of obsessions: A randomized controlled trial, Behaviour Research and Therapy
(2009), doi:10.1016/j.brat.2009.11.010
ARTICLE IN PRESS

2 M.L. Whittal et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy xxx (2009) 1–9

individual versus group therapy using a combination of contem- (a) primary diagnosis of OCD, (b) few or no overt compulsions, (c)
porary cognitive methods and ERP (loop tape or in vivo exposure to functional impairment from OCD for a minimum of one year, (d)
obsessions). Both formats resulted in a significant decline in YBOCS 18–65 years of age, (e) fluency in written and spoken English, (f)
total scores from pre to post-treatment. Not surprisingly, individual willingness to be randomized to type of treatment and start time,
treatment produced a greater decline than group treatment. (g) either free of psychoactive medications or on a stable medica-
To date there are no randomized trials examining the efficacy of tion regimen for three months prior to intake assessment and
contemporary cognitive treatments in the absence of prolonged willingness to refrain from making any medication changes
and explicit imaginal or in vivo exposure for obsessions with few to throughout treatment. Exclusion criteria included: (a) severe
no overt compulsions. The current study sought to compare depression with accompanying suicidal intent precluding ethical
Rachman’s (2003) cognitive behavior therapy for obsessionals with randomization to delayed treatment, (b) organic mental disorder,
few or no overt compulsions to a credible and active comparison thought disorder, current alcohol or drug dependence, (c) concur-
treatment of stress management training (SMT) as well as to rent psychological treatment for any Axis I or II disorder, aside from
a waitlist control. This CBT does not use repetitive imaginal or marital therapy or supportive therapy for depression, (d) prior
explicit in vivo exposure for the purpose of habituation. In a small adequate trial of CBT for OCD.
study (n ¼ 18), Lindsay, Crino, and Andrews (1997) compared the One hundred sixty-two individuals were judged appropriate for
efficacy of an SMT protocol to exposure treatment for broad-based clinical interview based on an initial telephone screening. Of these,
OCD. YBOCS scores were unchanged over the course of treatment 80 were excluded because they did not meet inclusion criteria; nine
with SMT (mean YBOCS at pre ¼ 24.4 and post ¼ 25.9). As expected, individuals declined to participate in the study. Seventy-three
the exposure group showed significant decreases in YBOCS scores participants completed the initial evaluation, consented to partic-
at post-treatment. ipate, and were randomized.
Turning to the present study, in comparison to both the waitlist
control and SMT, CBT treatment was expected to produce Treatments
substantial reductions in OCD symptom severity; the YBOCS
Obsessions subscale was the primary outcome measure for the CBT for obsessions was completed as described in the manual
study. Additional secondary outcomes (accompanying anxiety, published by Rachman (2003). Although treatments were individ-
depression, and social functioning) were also expected to change ually tailored to the presenting obsession and the associated
significantly with CBT. Importantly, CBT was expected to produce appraisals and beliefs, treatment began by normalizing the pres-
a significant reduction in maladaptive appraisals of the personal ence of unwanted thoughts as indicated by previous analogue and
meanings of the thoughts as well as other OCD-related cognitions. community studies (Rachman & de Silva, 1978; Salkovskis & Har-
These changes were expected to be stable through a one-year rison, 1984) and illustrating the importance of the appraisal process
follow-up. (e.g., ‘‘This is an odd thought but meaningless’’ versus ‘‘This thought
means I can’t be trusted around children’’). Participants received
Method a list of intrusions that have been reported by individuals who do
not have OCD (see Rachman, 2003) and were encouraged to talk
Design with others regarding unwanted thoughts. Participants were also
encouraged to use the list to identify additional unwanted thoughts
Participants were randomized to type of treatment (CBT or SMT) that are not upsetting and to examine the non-threatening
and to a start time (immediate or 3-month delay). Treatment appraisals of those thoughts.
consisted of 12 h-long individual sessions plus an information The CBT model for maintenance of obsessions was illustrated
gathering session following the diagnostic interview. Assessments within the context of normalizing intrusions. Specifically, the
were completed at intake, following 12 weeks of waitlist for those presence of an unwanted intrusive thought is not problematic per
randomized to receive delayed treatment, and post-treatment, as se, but the associated interpretation can give rise to emotional
well as six and 12 months follow-up. distress, avoidance, and the urge to neutralize the intrusion. The
Based on previous research (Freeston et al., 1997; Lindsay et al., target of treatment is the maladaptive interpretation of unwanted
1997), the study was powered to test for large effect sizes in the ideation. Unwanted thoughts are part of the human experience
comparison of cognitive therapy with either stress management but they do not persist or provoke upset if they are interpreted as
training or waitlist control. These previous results led us to antic- having no personal significance. Specific treatment tactics
ipate large effect sizes for CBT and near-zero effect sizes for both depended upon the details of the intrusion and the appraisal.
SMT and the waitlist control. Using the smallest effect size observed Some of these strategies include a demonstration of the paradox
in Freeston et al., 30 participants per condition would provide of thought control (e.g., try to not think of a pink elephant),
statistical power greater than 0.80. thought action fusion experiments, ‘on duty’ versus ‘off duty’
exercises, decreasing concealment of obsessions from significant
Participants others, focus of attention experiments and the relationship
between values and obsessions (i.e., the intrusion is upsetting
Participants were recruited between November 2002 and July and recurrent because it clashes with important values). For
2006. Several methods were used to draw attention to the study additional details see Rachman (2003) and Whittal, Robichaud,
including television and print media, letters to local psychiatrists and Woody (in press).
and other mental health providers, a study website and physician The treatment manual for the SMT was prepared internally
referral to our hospital-based anxiety disorders specialty treatment (Woody, O’Neill, Fairbrother, & McLean, 2003) drawing from sour-
clinic, where all treatment was subsequently conducted. Diagnoses ces that included Öst (1987), Paterson (2000), and Clark et al.
were based on the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID; (1998). (The full manual is available upon request from the
First, Spitzer, Givvon, & Williams, 1996), and OCD severity was authors.) The rationale for SMT was discussed at length in the first
assessed with the Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale (YBOCS; one or two sessions and was tailored to the participant’s particular
Goodman et al., 1989). Participants were provided a written concerns. The positive correlation between stress and obsessions
description of the study if they met the following inclusion criteria: was emphasized as the foundation of the SMT rationale. The

Please cite this article in press as: Whittal, M.L., et al., Treatment of obsessions: A randomized controlled trial, Behaviour Research and Therapy
(2009), doi:10.1016/j.brat.2009.11.010
ARTICLE IN PRESS

M.L. Whittal et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy xxx (2009) 1–9 3

bidirectional nature of this relationship, which quickly results in As is indicated by the high inter-rater reliability, the way in
a vicious circle was also emphasized: life stress, be it from positive which the YBOCS was used was very consistent within this study.
or negative events, can increase obsessions, which in turn further However, given that there may be differences in the use of the
increase stress. Accordingly, gaining skills to more effectively cope YBOCS and what is considered a covert compulsion, the following
with and reduce the impact of stressful life events is expected to discussion may be helpful in accounting for variance between
positively influence obsessions. The goal of SMT is to help partici- research groups. In comparison to other researchers (e.g., Freeston
pants recognize and manage both the external sources of stress in et al., 1997) who used a broad definition, we considered compul-
their life and their reactions to it. The skills of stress management sions to be rule-governed overt or covert actions that were
require practice and effort to maintain, and patients were informed completed in a rigid and stereotypic way. Examples include
that if they applied the skills on a regular basis, they would likely repeating a prayer following a blasphemous intrusion, altering an
observe a reduction in stress levels that would naturally reduce the aggressive image from stabbing to hugging a beloved pet, or
frequency of obsessions. repeating an action such as crossing a threshold free of an intrusion.
After identifying stressful areas of the patient’s life, skills Other forms of neutralization including distraction, thought
training followed a modular approach, individualized to the suppression and internally debating the veracity of the intrusion
participant. Typically treatment began with applied relaxation (Öst, were not considered compulsions as they were not completed in
1987). By reducing the frequency and intensity of the physiological stereotypic ways or according to rigid rules.
signs of stress, applied relaxation can allow the patient to feel more
confident about staying in control when obsessional thoughts Cognitions
become more intense. A second module was designed to boost The revised 44-item version of the Obsessive Belief Question-
skills relevant for coping with interpersonal stress, such as dealing naire (OBQ; Obsessive–Compulsive Cognitions Working Group,
with criticism, providing negative feedback when necessary, 2005) was used to measure the strength of beliefs related to
resolving conflicts and assertiveness. The focus of the interpersonal obsessions. Items are rated on a 1–7 scale with higher scores
effectiveness module is to promote interacting with others based reflecting a stronger belief. The revised OBQ has excellent internal
on genuineness rather than fear of their opinions or attitudes. The consistency (a ranging from 0.89 to 0.95 for all 3 subscales), as well
third module of the SMT package is organizational skills. Partici- as demonstrated convergent and discriminant validity (OCCWG,
pants learn to identify areas of disorganization and procrastination 2005). The OBQ-44 has three empirically-derived subscales:
that increase personal stress and learn more effective time Responsibility/Harm, Perfectionism/Certainty, and Importance/
management and problem-solving strategies. As with the CBT, the Control of Thoughts. The 31 items on the revised Interpretation of
therapist determined the order and emphasis of various prescribed Intrusions Inventory (III; OCCWG, 2005) are designed to measure
elements of SMT. appraisals associated with idiographic intrusions. Items are rated
on a 0–100 scale and averaged for a total score. Although originally
developed with subscales, the III appears to be unifactorial
Measures (OCCWG, 2005). The III has acceptable psychometrics.
As the aim of the CBT was to reduce or eliminate the person’s
Diagnosis maladaptive appraisals of their intrusive thoughts, it was essential
The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID; First et al., to measure these appraisals before and after treatment. Hence, the
1996) was used to diagnosis Axis I disorders at intake. The inter- Personal Significance Scale (PSS; Rachman, 2003), a 17-item ques-
viewers, all having prior training and experience with the SCID, tionnaire that assesses such appraisals, was used. Participants are
were doctoral level psychologists or postdoctoral fellows super- instructed to respond to the items with reference to the primary
vised by the investigators. In cases of diagnostic uncertainty, the obsession for which they were seeking treatment. Items assess the
final decision was made by investigators after review and degree to which respondents believe the occurrence of the obses-
consensus discussion of all case material. Post-treatment and sion indicates they are untrustworthy, mentally unstable,
follow-up SCIDs were completed by an interviewer who was dangerous, weird or a bad person. Other items assess the degree to
uninformed about group assignment or outcome. which the individual believes it is important to conceal or control
the thoughts. in the present study, the PSS displayed excellent
Obsessions and compulsions internal consistency (a ¼ 0.90) and was strongly correlated with
The Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale (YBOCS; Goodman the III (r ¼ 0.71). The correlation between the PSS and the OBQ total
et al., 1989) is a clinician-administered and rated scale that was score was r ¼ 0.62, which is due to a rather strong association with
used to assess symptom severity and to identify the type of the Importance and Control of Thoughts subscale of the OBQ
obsessions present. The YBOCS has five items each on the obses- (r ¼ 0.78). Correlations with the Responsibility/Threat Estimation
sions and compulsions subscales; the total score (possible range (r ¼ 0.40) and Perfectionism/Certainty (r ¼ 0.34) subscales were
0–40) is the sum of the subscales scores. The YBOCS displays lower. As might be expected, there is a moderate relationship
excellent inter-rater and test–retest reliability over one week between PSS scores and depression as measured by the Beck
(Goodman et al., 1989; Kim, Dysken, & Kuskowski, 1990), although Depression Inventory – II (r ¼ 0.35). The PSS, however, is not
internal consistency findings for the measure are somewhat mixed, significantly related to general anxiety as measured by the Beck
with acceptable alpha for the total scale (a ¼ 0.69–0.91; Goodman Anxiety Inventory (r ¼ 0.12), nor does it simply reflect obsession
et al., 1989; Woody, Steketee, & Chambless, 1995) and variable severity, showing a low correlation with the YBOCS Obsession scale
alphas for the briefer subscales (a ¼ 0.77 and 0.51 for the Obses- (r ¼ 0.13).
sions and Compulsions scale, respectively; Woody et al., 1995).
Discriminant validity is weak for this instrument (Taylor, 1998); Depressed mood and general anxiety
nevertheless, the YBOCS was used to facilitate comparison with The Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II; Beck, Steer, & Brown,
other studies. Inter-rater reliability in this study for the YBOCS was 1996) is a widely used 21-item measure of depression with strong
excellent (r ¼ 0.87 for Obsessions subscale and 0.97 for Compul- psychometric properties. The Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI; Beck &
sions subscale), as assessed by independent ratings of a randomly Steer, 1993) is a 21-item measure of state anxiety with excellent
selected 33% of audiotapes of the intake YBOCS interviews. psychometric properties (Beck, Epstein, Brown, & Steer, 1988).

Please cite this article in press as: Whittal, M.L., et al., Treatment of obsessions: A randomized controlled trial, Behaviour Research and Therapy
(2009), doi:10.1016/j.brat.2009.11.010
ARTICLE IN PRESS

4 M.L. Whittal et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy xxx (2009) 1–9

Functional impairment Therapists also rated homework compliance on a weekly basis.


The Self-Report Social Adjustment Scale (SAS-SR; Weissman & There were no significant differences between CBT and SMT on any
Bothwell, 1976) was used to measure overall social functioning. session (ts < 1.0). Mean homework compliance for the CBT group
Each of its 42 items is rated on a 5-point scale with higher numbers was 3.50 (SD ¼ 0.95) and 3.58 (SD ¼ 1.09) for the SMT group,
reflecting greater impairment. The SAS-SR has good internal indicating that both groups did most of their assigned tasks each
consistency and test–retest reliability (Edwards, Yarvis, Mueller, week.
Zingale, & Wagman, 1978). The self-report scale shows good
agreement with interviewer assessment of functioning, good Results
criterion-related validity, and sensitivity to treatment change
(Weissman & Bothwell, 1976). Data analytic strategy

Treatment expectancy Analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was first used to compare


The Credibility/Expectancy Questionnaire (Borkovec & Nau, outcome variables following the waitlist with participants who
1972) was used to assess credibility of each treatment as well as received immediate treatment with CBT or SMT, controlling for
patient expectations of successful outcome. This scale is commonly initial status. To compare CBT and SMT with more power, we next
used in treatment outcome studies and has acceptable psycho- repeated ANCOVA analyses comparing all patients who received
metrics (Devilly & Borkovec, 2000). CBT or SMT, regardless of whether treatment was immediate or
delayed. Several multivariate outliers were detected in initial
Homework compliance (Primakoff, Epstein, & Covi, 1986) examination of the data. In most cases, results did not differ when
Therapists rated homework compliance using a scale from 0 (did these observations were excluded, so the full sample results are
not attempt the assigned homework) to 5 (attempted more than reported. In one case, noted below, the results changed slightly
was requested). This scale has not been subjected to psychometric with exclusion of an observation with a very large residual and high
evaluation. leverage statistics.

Treatment integrity Patient characteristics

Postdoctoral fellows with experience in the treatment of anxiety The average age of participants at intake was 31.5 years
and OCD treated 87% of the participants. MW, SW, SR, and PM (SD ¼ 9.7). Of the 73 participants, 34 were female (46.6%). Most
treated the remaining 9 people. PM and SR respectively supervised participants reported a European ethnic background (84.9%); the
the SMT and CBT. Training consisted of reading the treatment remaining reported Asian (10.9%) or other backgrounds. Partici-
manuals, listening to tapes of the therapies being conducted, pants reported an average of 14.7 (SD ¼ 2.5) years of education.
participating in case conferences and treating practice cases that About half of the participants were single, 42.5% were living in
were not part of the study. Both supervisors met regularly with the a marital relationship, and 6.9% were divorced. Although only 52.0%
postdoctoral fellows for case review and to plan future sessions. of participants were employed full-time, 31.5% were unemployed
The investigators rated therapist competence and protocol by choice (e.g., student, parent of young children). There were no
adherence without knowledge of the outcome of treatment (i.e., significant differences between treatment groups on any of these
supervisors, therapists, and independent evaluators involved in demographic variables.
a specific case were excluded from rating tapes of that case). All Regarding psychiatric status, 75.3% of patients had at least one
sessions were audiotaped, and 20% of these tapes were randomly additional Axis I diagnosis; 39.7% had at least two additional
selected for ratings of competence and integrity, independent of diagnoses, the most common being major depression (34%), dys-
the supervision process. Tapes were rated blindly, without knowl- thymia (16%), or generalized anxiety disorder (16%). Global
edge of treatment condition or symptom status. Tapes from eight Assessment of Functioning (Axis V) scores ranged from 45 to 80,
patients (four in each condition) were not available for rating. with a mean of 59.5 (SD ¼ 6.3). Half of participants (52.0%) were
The raters correctly classified 100% of the tapes in terms of the taking stable doses of medication prior to intake.
type of therapy being delivered. Of 141 sessions rated, only one Participants recalled the onset of their OCD as occurring, on
instance of protocol failure was noted. This involved a tape of a CBT average, at age 17.5 (SD ¼ 8.6). Content of participants’ predomi-
session; the other rated session for that patient showed adequate nant obsessions were primarily aggressive (53.2%) or sexual (41.6%),
adherence to the CBT protocol. Although overall ratings indicated although some religious obsessions were also evident (5.2%). About
no gross violations of protocol, there were examples of less serious a third of participants (32.5%) reported obsessions that fell into
cross-contamination of intervention strategies, particularly for the more than one content domain. There were no significant differ-
SMT condition. Of those tapes involving SMT, 23% were rated as ences between treatment groups on any of these indicators of
using some procedures consistent with the CBT protocol; of the psychiatric status or on any measure of OCD symptoms.
tapes involving CBT, just 5% were rated as using some procedures Patients in the SMT condition showed greater variability in their
consistent with the SMT protocol. Of the 15 SMT patients for whom ratings of treatment expectancy than did patients in the CBT
some CBT procedures were noted during integrity ratings, all condition, F (31, 37) ¼ 3.21, p < 0.001, but a t-test comparing means
patients but one had other tapes rated as fully adherent (i.e., no with unequal variances did not show differences in level of
protocol contamination) to the SMT protocol. All of the three CBT expectancy ratings between the treatments, t (46.7) ¼ 1.05,
patients with some SMT procedures had other tapes rated as fully p > 0.30.
adherent to the CBT protocol.
Raters also judged the overall quality of the therapy, taking into Treatment completers versus dropouts
account the apparent difficulty level of the patient as well as
nonspecific therapy skills and specific protocol skills. Respectively, Patients were considered to have completed treatment if they
95% and 97% of the SMT and CBT sessions were rated as satisfactory received at least eight of the 12 sessions and provided post-treat-
or better in terms of overall quality, which did not differ by treat- ment data. One participant assigned to SMT was withdrawn from
ment condition, t (138) ¼ 1.50, p ¼ 0.14. the study due to worsening depression and a consequent

Please cite this article in press as: Whittal, M.L., et al., Treatment of obsessions: A randomized controlled trial, Behaviour Research and Therapy
(2009), doi:10.1016/j.brat.2009.11.010
ARTICLE IN PRESS

M.L. Whittal et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy xxx (2009) 1–9 5

Table 1 standard deviations, and effect sizes for the waitlist group are
Means (and standard deviations) for participants on waitlist (n ¼ 28). presented in Table 1.
Time 1 Time 2 d 90% CI for d
YBOCS obsessions 10.93 (2.16) 10.54 (2.74) 0.13 0.18 to 0.44 Secondary outcomes
YBOCS total 17.54 (5.94) 16.50 (6.33) 0.18 0.14 to 0.49 The same results were observed for YBOCS Total scores, F (3, 69) ¼
Beck depression inventory 19.32 (13.24) 18.32 (12.05) 0.14 0.17 to 0.45 14.43, p < 0.001, h2p ¼ 0.29, with lower scores following treatment
Beck anxiety inventory 18.96 (9.90) 17.79 (10.43) 0.18 0.14 to 0.49
than following the waitlist. The treatments likewise produced
Overall social adjustment 2.21 (0.44) 2.19 (0.46) 0.04 0.27 to 0.35
OBQ-responsibility/harm 4.40 (1.30) 4.34 (1.36) 0.08 0.23 to 0.39 significantly lower BDI scores than did the waitlist, F (3, 67) ¼ 3.97,
OBQ-perfectionism/certainty 4.19 (1.17) 4.00 (1.30) 0.14 0.17 to 0.45 p ¼ 0.02, h2p ¼ 0.11, and lower BAI scores, F (3, 67) ¼ 6.16, p ¼ 0.003,
OBQ-importance/control 4.39 (1.28) 4.19 (1.28) 0.34 0.02 to 0.66 h2p ¼ 0.16, as well as better social adjustment, F (3, 67) ¼ 7.80,
Interpretation of intrusions 60.70 (20.74) 58.35 (21.20) 0.17 0.14 to 0.48 p < 0.001, h2p ¼ 0.19.
Personal significance scale 80.58 (27.37) 80.92 (28.73) 0.02 0.34 to 0.29

Note: YBOCS ¼ Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale, OBQ ¼ Obsessional Beliefs Cognitions
Questionnaire (44-item version).
Participants in treatment had lower scores on the PSS than did
participants on the waitlist, F (3, 66) ¼ 24.42, p < 0.001, h2p ¼ 0.43.
medication change. Two SMT participants left the study before Participants also had lower scores on measures of OC cognitions
completing eight sessions, as did three participants in the CBT following treatment than was evident following waitlist, 6.64  F
condition. Patients who completed treatment and those who (3, 66)  17.68, p < 0.002, 0.17  h2p  0.35 for the III and three
terminated prematurely were not significantly different with subscales of the OBQ-44.
regard to any demographic variable, treatment expectancy, or any
indicator of severity of OCD or psychiatric status. Comparing SMT and CBT

To maximize power for the comparison between CBT and SMT,


Comparing treatments to waitlist patients who received immediate and delayed treatment were
included in the same dataset. Table 2 shows the means, standard
Primary outcome deviations, and within-group effect sizes for these groups. For
A one-way ANCOVA was used to compare post-treatment scores patients who participated in the waitlist conditions, pretreatment
on YBOCS Obsessions after controlling for scores on this variable at score in these analyses corresponds to the assessment immediately
intake. Intent-to treat analyses using multiple imputation for prior to treatment (i.e., following the waiting period).
missing observations yielded the same results as those for treat-
ment completers, so only the completer analyses are reported here. Primary outcome
The independent variable had three levels: waitlist, CBT, and SMT. A one-way ANCOVA compared post-treatment scores on YBOCS
For this analysis, however, post-hoc tests were examined only for Obsessions controlling for pretreatment scores using a two-level
the comparisons involving waitlist, as the two treatments were independent variable: CBT versus SMT. Analysis of patients who
compared in analyses presented below. This analysis showed completed treatment showed CBT produced significantly lower
a strong treatment effect, F (3, 69) ¼ 19.44, p < 0.001, h2p ¼ 0.36. YBOCS Obsessions scores than did SMT, F (2, 64) ¼ 4.38, p ¼ 0.04,
Planned post-hoc comparison showed YBOCS Obsessions scores h2p ¼ 0.06. The difference between treatments remained true in the
were lower following treatment than following the waitlist. Means, analysis of intent-to-treat data with multiple imputation for

Table 2
Means (and standard deviations) for all treatment completers – immediate and delayed.

Pre-treatment Post-treatment Pre–post Pre–post 6 Months follow-up 12 Months follow-up Pre-12 mo. Pre-12 mo.

Mean (SD) Mean (SD) d 90% CI Mean (SD) Mean (SD) d 90% CI
Cognitive therapy (n ¼ 37) (n ¼ 37) (n ¼ 34) (n ¼ 34)
YBOCS obsessions 11.27 (2.78) 4.62 (2.88) 2.34 1.80–2.86 4.85 (3.32) 5.21 (3.90) 1.52 1.09–1.93
YBOCS total 18.03 (6.29) 6.43 (4.77) 1.76 1.32–2.19 8.68 (6.83) 8.00 (6.54) 1.23 0.85–1.60
BDI 18.62 (9.63) 10.89 (8.99) 0.81 0.49–1.11 9.87 (7.33) 11.06 (7.25) 0.93 0.59–1.27
BAI 18.03 (9.72) 9.22 (7.04) 0.97 0.63–1.29 11.06 (9.35) 11.50 (8.39) 0.67 0.35–0.98
Social adjustment 2.15 (0.41) 1.88 (0.31) 1.07 0.72–1.40 1.98 (0.43) 2.00 (0.46) 0.53 0.22–0.83
OBQ-response/harm 4.47 (1.39) 2.75 (1.23) 1.29 0.91–1.65 2.66 (1.48) 2.79 (1.51) 1.39 0.97–1.79
OBQ-perfect/certain 4.16 (1.50) 2.89 (1.36) 0.99 0.65–1.33 2.81 (1.43) 2.99 (1.42) 1.05 0.69–1.41
OBQ-import/control 4.34 (1.30) 1.84 (1.02) 1.84 1.38–2.28 1.84 (1.06) 2.03 (1.11) 1.50 1.06–1.92
III 60.28 (20.56) 19.63 (23.11) 1.50 1.09–1.89 16.47 (20.44) 18.43 (20.91) 1.58 1.14–2.00
PSS 82.28 (27.20) 22.10 (25.16) 1.86 1.39–2.30 19.38 (24.14) 27.88 (24.31) 1.78 1.31–2.23

Stress management (n ¼ 30) (n ¼ 30) (n ¼ 27) (n ¼ 24)


YBOCS obsessions 11.03 (3.31) 5.77 (3.07) 1.90 1.38–2.39 5.59 (3.48) 5.59 (3.37) 1.41 0.96–1.86
YBOCS total 17.73 (7.73) 9.10 (6.48) 1.49 1.05–1.93 8.93 (7.78) 9.33 (7.76) 1.09 0.68–1.48
BDI 17.90 (11.10) 10.17 (9.43) 0.82 0.46–1.16 10.33 (8.32) 8.29 (8.15) 0.75 0.36–1.12
BAI 14.48 (8.90) 9.13 (7.32) 0.59 0.25–0.92 9.15 (6.46) 7.88 (7.58) 0.98 0.56–1.38
Social adjustment 2.30 (0.46) 1.94 (0.44) 1.07 0.67–1.44 1.99 (0.52) 1.97 (0.55) 0.90 0.49–1.29
OBQ-response/harm 4.17 (1.31) 3.17 (1.40) 0.97 0.57–1.34 3.13 (1.33) 2.92 (1.30) 1.16 0.69–1.61
OBQ-perfect/certain 4.29 (1.06) 3.14 (1.31) 1.25 0.81–1.66 3.20 (1.44) 3.06 (1.41) 1.22 0.74–1.68
OBQ-import/control 4.39 (1.22) 2.90 (1.39) 1.20 0.78–1.61 2.73 (1.33) 2.56 (1.34) 1.32 0.82–1.79
III 60.15 (18.85) 31.43 (20.47) 1.30 0.88–1.71 30.84 (21.17) 26.49 (21.82) 1.36 0.88–1.81
PSS 86.02 (23.97) 46.41 (27.71) 1.35 0.92–1.77 46.07 (28.39) 40.46 (26.35) 1.35 0.87–1.80

Note: YBOCS ¼ Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale, BDI ¼ Beck Depression Inventory, BAI ¼ Beck Anxiety Inventory, OBQ ¼ Obsessional Beliefs Questionnaire 44-item
version, III ¼ Interpretations of Intrusions Inventory, PSS ¼ Personal Significance Scale.

Please cite this article in press as: Whittal, M.L., et al., Treatment of obsessions: A randomized controlled trial, Behaviour Research and Therapy
(2009), doi:10.1016/j.brat.2009.11.010
ARTICLE IN PRESS

6 M.L. Whittal et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy xxx (2009) 1–9

incomplete cases, t (63.41) ¼ 2.12, p ¼ 0.04. After removing one


multivariate outlier, the effect was slightly smaller and the p value
120
slightly larger, F (2, 63) ¼ 3.57, p ¼ 0.06, h2p ¼ 0.05. The intent-to-
treat analysis showed the same result without the multivariate CBT
outlier case, t (65.12) ¼ 1.96, p ¼ 0.06. Fig. 1 demonstrates the 100 SMT
YBOCS Obsessions raw scores over time between groups. Waitlist
80
Secondary outcomes
Among patients who completed treatment, CBT resulted in
60
significantly lower YBOCS Total scores than did SMT, F (2,
64) ¼ 5.60, p ¼ 0.02, h2p ¼ 0.08, an effect that was also observed in
the intent-to-treat analysis, t (65.21) ¼ 2.37, p ¼ 0.02. Analysis of 40
neither treatment completers nor intent-to-treat datasets showed
differences between the treatments on BDI scores, F (2, 69) ¼ 0.02, 20
p ¼ 0.88, h2p < 0.01, BAI scores, F (3, 69) ¼ 0.62, p ¼ 0.43, h2p < 0.01, or
overall social adjustment, F (2, 69) ¼ 0.04, p ¼ 0.85, h2p < 0.01
0
(completer statistics presented). Intake Post-treatment 6 months 12 months
Assessment Time
Cognitions
As expected, patients who completed CBT also endorsed fewer Fig. 2. PSS scores by group across assessment occasions. Notes: Membership in the
appraisals of personal significance of their obsessive thoughts on the waitlist group is not independent of the two treatment groups. These participants were
randomized to a treatment that followed the waitlist. Thus, the ‘‘post-treatment’’
PSS than did SMT patients, F (2, 62) ¼ 13.97, p ¼ 0.0004, h2p ¼ 0.18 (see
assessment time for the waitlist participants is more accurately considered as ‘‘post-
Fig. 2). Patients who completed CBT showed less endorsement of waitlist’’. The standard deviation bars are not given for all groups in order to improve
OC-related cognitions on the III than did those who completed readability of the figure, but they are similar in size to those represented in the graph.
SMT, F (2, 63) ¼ 4.54, p ¼ 0.04, h2p ¼ 0.07. This difference was also
observed for two subscales of the OBQ-44: Responsibility/Harm,
F (2, 62) ¼ 5.09, p ¼ 0.03, h2p ¼ 0.08, and Importance/Control of elements: reliable change score of at least 1.96 and a post-treat-
Thoughts, F (2, 62) ¼ 16.91, p ¼ 0.0001, h2p ¼ 0.21. The post-treatment ment YBOCS score that meets criterion a (i.e., post-test YBOCS
score on the Perfectionism/Certainty subscale of the OBQ-44 did not Obsessions score below 5.79). Of those receiving CBT, 27 of 37
differ between treatments, F (2, 62) ¼ 0.92, p ¼ 0.34, h2p ¼ 0.01. Intent- patients met criterion a, and 30 showed reliable change, so that 59%
to-treat results show a similar pattern: OBQ-Resp/Harm t achieved clinically significant change on the YBOCS Obsessions
(56.60) ¼ 2.18, p ¼ 0.03; OBQ-Perf/Cert t (65.00) ¼ 0.60, p ¼ 0.55; subscale. Among patients receiving SMT, 17 of 30 met criterion a,
OBQ-Import/Control t (52.44) ¼ 3.90, p < 0.001; III t (62.73) ¼ 2.22, and 18 showed reliable change, with 43% achieving clinically
p ¼ 0.03; PSS t (58.11) ¼ 3.84, p < 0.001. significant change. These proportions were not significantly
different with this sample size, c2(1, N ¼ 67) ¼ 1.73, p ¼ 0.19. When
Clinical significance examining the YBOCS Total scores, 20 of 37 (54%) receiving CBT
Based on the recommendations outlined by Jacobson and Truax and 7 of 30 (23%) receiving SMT achieved clinically significant
(1991), we defined clinically significant change as comprised of two change, resulting in proportions that were significantly different,
c2(1, N ¼ 67) ¼ 6.50, p ¼ 0.01.

20 Moderators
We examined numerous variables, including dichotomous
indicators of medication use, comorbid depression, referral source,
CBT and psychosocial stressors as well as years of education, therapist,
15 and homework compliance. Proposed moderator variables were
SMT
Waitlist
entered into the model as a main effect and in interaction with
treatment type. There was no evidence that the effect of treatment
varied significantly according to patient status on any of these
10 variables, 0.18  t  1.07, 0.29  p  0.86.

Treatment integrity
As described above, some SMT sessions were coded as having
5 elements of CBT in them (although blinded coders accurately
classified all sessions). To test whether this cross-contamination
was responsible for the strong effect of SMT on outcome, we
examined residual gains scores of the primary and secondary
0
outcome variables. One-tailed (directional) t-tests were conducted
Intake Post-treatment 6 months 12 months
to ascertain whether outcomes of SMT patients with cross-
Assessment Time
contamination were better than those who received SMT with no
Fig. 1. YBOCS Obsessions Subscale by group across assessment occasions. Notes: evidence of CBT procedures. Results consistently showed SMT
Membership in the waitlist group is not independent of the two treatment groups. patients with evidence of cross-contamination had similar or
These participants were randomized to a treatment that followed the waitlist. Thus, worse outcomes than did those with no evidence of cross-
the ‘‘post-treatment’’ assessment time for the waitlist participants is more accurately
contamination. This pattern is contrary to the expected result if
considered as ‘‘post-waitlist’’. The standard deviation bars are not given for all groups
in order to improve readability of the figure, but they are similar in size to those
elements of CBT observed in the SMT tapes were responsible for
represented in the graph. the strong effects of SMT.

Please cite this article in press as: Whittal, M.L., et al., Treatment of obsessions: A randomized controlled trial, Behaviour Research and Therapy
(2009), doi:10.1016/j.brat.2009.11.010
ARTICLE IN PRESS

M.L. Whittal et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy xxx (2009) 1–9 7

Maintenance of treatment gains general OCD but also has unique features. Using this specialized
CBT protocol, YBOCS totals at post-treatment were similar to scores
Six-month follow-up reported by a college student sample (Frost & Steketee, 2002).
An ANCOVA, using pretest status as a covariate, showed no Appraisals of personal significance declined appreciably with
differences between treatment conditions on any measure of treatment. Two of the three OBQ subscales and the III score also
symptoms (ps > 0.28). In contrast, all measures of cognition differentiated the SMT and CBT groups at post-treatment, although
showed lower scores for patients who had participated in CBT than Perfectionism/Certainty was not significantly different between
for those who had participated in SMT, Fs (2, 56)  6.11, ps < 0.02, CBT and SMT. As discussed earlier, Rachman’s treatment is focused
h2p > 0.10, with the exception of the Perfectionism/Certainty on the meaning of the intrusions – that they are not important or
subscale of the OBQ-44, which continued to be similar between revealing in any way, and as such, they are not threatening. Given
treatments, F (2, 56) ¼ 1.94, p ¼ 0.17, h2p ¼ 0.03. See Table 1. the focus of the treatment used in the current study, the observed
pattern of OBQ results and other cognitive measures is consistent
12-Month follow-up with Rachman’s theory.
As was observed at the six months point, there were no differ- Contrary to expectations, SMT produced results that were nearly
ences between treatment conditions on any measure of symptoms as strong and well maintained as CBT. Although not unwelcome,
(p > 0.26). Differences on the cognitive measures remained low and the finding that SMT produced robust and positive changes that
by 12 months were not significantly different. Two measures, the were maintained over a year was a surprising one. The SMT manual
PSS and the Importance/Control of Thoughts subscales of the OBQ- was developed specifically for this study, but a similar protocol
44, showed statistical trends for the CBT patients to have lower used in a small study by Lindsay et al. (1997) resulted in no change
scores, F (2, 53) ¼ 3.74, p ¼ 0.06, h2p ¼ 0.06 and F (2, 53) ¼ 3.14, in YBOCS scores. One relevant difference may have been that the
p ¼ 0.08, h2p ¼ 0.06, respectively. Lindsay et al. protocol did not include skills for interpersonal
effectiveness (e.g., dealing with criticism, being assertive). The
Discussion Lindsay et al. protocol consisted of breathing retraining, progressive
muscle relaxation and structured problem solving of non-OCD
According to the cognitive theory, obsessions are caused by concerns. Perhaps a more important difference was the duration of
serious misinterpretations of the personal significance of intru- treatment. Participants in the Lindsay et al. study received 15 h of
sions. It follows that if these maladaptive interpretations are therapy massed over three weeks compared to the current study
reduced or eliminated by formal therapy, or by any other means, that provided 12 h in once-weekly sessions over three months. The
then the frequency and the distress of the obsessions will steeply relatively compressed treatment schedule in the Lindsay study may
decline. CBT and SMT for primary obsessions resulted in a strong have provided insufficient time to alter response to life stressors
and broad treatment effect when compared to waitlist. Large effect and therefore failed to produce a change in OCD severity. It is also
sizes emerged for both treatments on the YBOCS obsessions unclear whether participants in the Lindsay et al. study saw SMT as
subscale at post-treatment as well as the YBOCS total score (ds a credible treatment, as treatment expectancy was not reported.
ranging from 1.49 to 2.34). Large treatment effects were also found Simpson et al. (2008) also used the Lindsay version of SMT as an
for the personal significance measure and the OCD cognition active comparator. In this study, pharmacotherapy was augmented
measures (ds ranging 1.27–1.86). More modest treatment effects with either SMT or ERP fpr 108 patients with OCD who had failed to
were observed on measures of general anxiety, depression and respond satisfactorily to the medication. The patients received 17
social functioning (ds from 0.59 to 1.07). Two thirds to three sessions over eight weeks of ERP or SMT while continuing on the
quarters of participants in both treatments achieved clinically medication. The ERP augmentation produced large and significant
significant change that was still evident at a 12-month follow-up. reductions in YBOCS total scores (25.4 at pre-treatment to 14.2 at
Mediators of treatment outcome will be examined in a subsequent post-treatment), but the SMT augmentation produced minimal
paper. Consistent with cognitive theory, both groups of partici- change (YBOCS total 26.2–22.6). However, it should be noted that
pants’ PSS scores were near the top of the scale prior to treatment the homework compliance for the SMT group was only fair (mean
and after the waitlist period; that is, the intrusions were being of 2.6/5) whereas the compliance for the ERP group was 3.1/5.
seriously misinterpreted. Given the previous literature and cognitive theories of obses-
The present findings compare favorably to effect sizes obtained sions, our SMT results appear to be exceptional, leaving the
in general OCD controlled trials as well as OCD with primarily pressing question of why the our SMt protocol worked so well.
obsessional content. Two recent meta-analyses reported pre–post Differential treatment credibility is not likely to be responsible, as
effect sizes of d ¼ 1.48 and d ¼ 1.50 (Abramowitz, Franklin, & Foa, both CBT and SMT were rated as credible. Treatment expectancy did
2002; Eddy, Dutra, Bradley, & Westen, 2004). In comparison to the vary more within the SMT group, but there were no group differ-
two published randomized controlled trials on treatment of ences in mean expectancy ratings. There were instances of cross-
primary obsessions (Freeston et al., 1997 and O’Connor et al., 2005), contamination within the SMT group but this is also unlikely to be
the mean post-treatment YBOCS total score for CBT completers responsible for the robust SMT findings. As described earlier, cross-
(6.4) in the current study is slightly lower. Freeston reported a post- contamination appears to have been minimal and thus seems an
treatment mean YBOCS total score of 7.2 (SD ¼ 5.2) for completers outside possibility to account for the strength of the SMT. Moreover,
and 9.8 (SD ¼ 8.2) for the total sample including the 17% who SMT patients who had some cross-contamination of CBT proce-
dropped out of treatment. O’Connor et al. (2005) reported a post- dures did not show better outcomes than did SMT patients with
treatment YBOCS total score of 8.0 (2.8) for the 17 participants who uncontaminated treatments. As such, the CBT procedures received
completed individual treatment. As neither treatment study by a small portion of the SMT patients appear not to be responsible
reported separate scores for obsessions and compulsions, for the robust SMT treatment result.
a comparison of subscales is not possible. Cognitive theories of obsessions would suggest that CBT, which
The current study is the first randomized trial of an intervention directly focuses on normalizing and reappraising intrusive thoughts,
based on Rachman’s (1997, 1998) cognitive theory of obsessions should be a substantially more powerful intervention than SMT,
using patients with few or no overt compulsions. CBT for obses- given that the latter is devoid of intervention focused on the obses-
sions overlaps somewhat with contemporary CBT treatments for sions. Nevertheless, the observed findings may be interpretable from

Please cite this article in press as: Whittal, M.L., et al., Treatment of obsessions: A randomized controlled trial, Behaviour Research and Therapy
(2009), doi:10.1016/j.brat.2009.11.010
ARTICLE IN PRESS

8 M.L. Whittal et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy xxx (2009) 1–9

the perspective of cognitive theory. Patients in both treatments them accordingly. There was no repetitive, prolonged or intentional
showed large changes in OCD-related cognitions and threat exposure to obsessions via loop tapes or any other planned expo-
appraisals, indicating that cognitive change did occur during SMT sures. Incidental, occasional unplanned exposures of unknown
even though it was not specifically addressed in treatment. duration may have occurred, but exposure and response prevention
How might this cognitive change have occurred? Therapists as properly defined and employed was not included in either
providing SMT did not react negatively upon hearing the content of treatment protocol.
the obsessions and in fact encouraged increased social contact and the
eventual application of relaxation skills to anxiety provoking situa- Study limitations
tions such as interpersonal conflict or stressful life circumstances. A
focus on enhanced social contact likely provided the implicit message The primary dependent measure, the YBOCS, has limitations for
that the therapist regards the patient’s thoughts as non-significant this OCD subtype. It was developed to test the effect of medication
and does not fear for the safety of others when the patient is nearby. on OCD and contains no items that address metacognitions. There
Another possibility is the relaxation component of SMT, implemented is also confusion in the scoring of items related to resistance and
under cue control and applied in daily situations was sufficiently control of obsessions and compulsions. We did, however, train
distracting and may have averted efforts at thought control and assessors in the use of these items to maintain reliable and
permitted a natural decay in the frequency of the unwanted thoughts. consistent assessments throughout the study. Another limitation is
It is also possible that a measurement effect influenced the that avoidance behavior is measured with only one item on the
results. Prior to each treatment session, all participants completed YBOCS that is not included in the total score. The difficulties with
the Personal Significance Scale in addition to ratings of the severity the use of the YBOCS as a primary outcome measure are aggravated
of their obsessions during the previous week. By repeatedly elic- with select OCD subtypes. For example, Frost, Steketee, and Gri-
iting patients’ appraisals of their intrusions on the PSS, we may sham (2004) developed a distinct measure of hoarding, as the
have implicitly conveyed our view of the importance of these existing YBOCS was inadequate to properly assess the nature and
appraisals. Even the participants in the SMT condition had to pay severity of this subtype.
attention to their personal interpretations by monitoring and The initial YBOCS Total scores for our sample were also mark-
reporting them before every session, although the SMT therapists edly lower than those observed in the Freeston et al. (1997) study,
did not discuss the PSS ratings during sessions. As patients do not despite the fact that the samples were otherwise apparently
typically notice their appraisals of obsessions without direct similar. These differences may be due to the samples, to variation in
questioning, those in the SMT condition likely became aware of how the YBOCS is used in different research groups, or to some
their appraisals in a way that would not have occurred without the other factor. Notably, the O’Connor et al. (2005) study reported
measurement undertaken for the study. initial YBOCS Total scores similar to the Freeston study for patients
Prior studies have observed cognitive changes in the context of randomized to individual therapy and scores similar to the present
behavioral activation (Jacobson et al., 1996) or behavioral stress study for patients randomized to group therapy. As this subtype of
management (Clark et al., 1998). In a similar vein, the SMT in the OCD has not been well studied, the sources of these differences are
current study may have shifted patients’ energies from attempting not yet clear. Abramowitz et al. (in press) recently developed and
to understand and control their thoughts to becoming more tested an alternate dimensional version of the YBOCS that focuses
engaged and effective in their lives, which, although unmeasured, on four dimensional empirically-supported OCD presentations
may have reduced severity and distress of obsessions. In this way, (contamination, responsibility for harm, unacceptable thoughts
SMT may have served as a healthy and constructive diversion from and symmetry/completeness/‘just right’). The initial promise of this
obsessions, as therapists helped patients to focus attention on measure awaits use in research and clinical settings to determine
identifying and achieving important short-term life goals. This its ultimate utility, but it may solve some of the problems inherent
implicit behavioral activation may have reduced obsessions via with the YBOCS.
improvement in depression. Although speculative, it is also In retrospect, the failure to include process measures relevant to
possible that disconfirmation of feared consequences may have understanding the effects of SMT was a clear omission. Given the
occurred as patients reduced general avoidance; homework unexpected finding of an exceptionally strong SMT treatment
assignments may have inadvertently involved people or situations effect, a better understanding of the components associated with
the patient had avoided in part due to obsessions (e.g., visiting this finding is necessary. Also, because the rating of homework
friends who happen to have children). compliance is completed by the therapist, it is subject to bias and
The robust treatment outcome with both protocols contradicts should be considered a limitation.
the long-standing belief that obsessions are particularly resistant to In summary it appears that an OCD subtype once thought to be
treatment (e.g., Jenike, 1993; Rachman, 1983). These results are difficult to treat is amenable to relatively brief contemporary
particularly encouraging in light of the relatively low dropout rates cognitive behavioral treatment and may also prove to be treatable
(6.8%) and amount of therapist time (12 treatment hours and 3–4 with stress management strategies. In view of the inconsistency
assessment hours). Although the treatment time in the current between our results with SMT and those reported with other
study is consistent with recent treatment trials of general OCD (e.g., studies (Lindsay et al., 1997; Simpson et al., 2008), this result is
van Oppen et al., 1995; Whittal, Thordarson, & McLeanWhittal, plainly in need of independent replication. The strong and broad
2005), previous trials with obsessionals utilized protocols of longer effects of CBT are encouraging but also need independent replica-
duration. Freeston et al. (1997) reported the average duration of tion before the approach can be recommended. However, if our
treatment involved 40 therapist hours, whereas O’Connor et al. SMT results are replicated by other centers, the clinical implications
(2005) reported 20 h. are promising, as SMT is a relatively straightforward treatment to
Dropout rates were also higher in the Freeston et al. (17.2% disseminate – albeit theoretically threadbare.
dropout) and O’Connor et al. (10.5%) studies than in the current one.
One methodological difference that might account for the differ- Author note
ential dropout rates is the use of loop tapes. CBT participants in our
study were encouraged to decrease efforts to block or suppress the The authors would like to thank Melanie O’Neill and David
intrusions and instead regard them as mental noise and dismiss Jacobi, who served as postdoctoral fellows on the study.

Please cite this article in press as: Whittal, M.L., et al., Treatment of obsessions: A randomized controlled trial, Behaviour Research and Therapy
(2009), doi:10.1016/j.brat.2009.11.010
ARTICLE IN PRESS

M.L. Whittal et al. / Behaviour Research and Therapy xxx (2009) 1–9 9

Additionally, we appreciate the contributions of Sarah Newth and Jacobson, N. S., Dobson, K. S., Truax, P. A., Addis, M. E., Koerner, K., Gollan, J. K., et al.
(1996). A component analysis of cognitive–behavioral treatment for depression.
Nichole Fairbrother. David A. Clark and Arnoud Arntz kindly read an
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 295–304.
earlier version of the manuscript and provided helpful feedback. Jacobson, N. S., & Truax, P. A. (1991). Clinical significance: a statistical approach to
This research was funded by the Canadian Institutes of Health defining meaningful change in psychotherapy research. Journal of Consulting
Research. and Clinical Psychology, 59, 12–19.
Jenike, M. A. (1993). Obsessive-compulsive disorder: efficacy of specific treatments
as assessed by controlled trials. Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 29, 487–499.
Kim, S., Dysken, M., & Kuskowski, M. (1990). The yale-brown obsessive compulsive
References scale: a reliability and validity study. Psychiatry Research, 41, 37–44.
Lindsay, M., Crino, R., & Andrews, G. (1997). Controlled trial of exposure and
Abramowitz, J. S., Deacon, B. J., Loatunji, B. O., Wheaton, M. G., Berman, N., Losardo, D., response prevention in obsessive–compulsive disorder. British Journal of
Timpano, K., McGrath, P., Riemann, B., Adams, T., Bjorgvinsson, T., Storch, E. A., & Psychiatry, 171, 135–139.
Hale, L. Assessment of obsessive–compulsive symptoms dimensions: develop- O’Connor, K., Freeston, M. H., Gareau, D., Careau, Y., Dufour, M. J., Aardema, R., et al.
ment and evaluation of the dimensional obsessive–compulsive scale. Psycho- (2005). Group versus individual treatment in obsessions without compulsions.
logical Assessment, in press. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 12, 87–96.
Abramowitz, J. S., Franklin, M. E., & Foa, E. B. (2002). Empirical status of cognitive– Obsessive–Compulsive Cognitions Working Group. (2005). Psychometric validation
behavioral therapy for obsessive–compulsive disorder: a meta-analytic review. of the obsessive belief questionnaire and interpretation of intrusion inventory –
Romanian Journal of Cognitive and Behavioral Psychotherapies, 2, 89–104. part 2: factor analyses and testing of a brief version. Behaviour Research and
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental Therapy, 43, 1527–1542.
disorders (DSM-IV) (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. van Oppen, P., de Haan, E., van Balkom, A., Spinhoven, P., Hoogduin, K., & van
Beck, A. T., Epstein, N., Brown, G., & Steer, R. A. (1988). An inventory for measuring Dyck, R. (1995). Cognitive therapy and exposure in vivo in the treatment of
clinical anxiety: psychometric properties. Journal of Consulting and Clinical obsessive compulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33, 379–390.
Psychology, 56, 893–897. Öst, L. G. (1987). Applied relaxation: description of a coping technique and review
Beck, A. T., & Steer, R. A. (1993). Manual for the beck anxiety inventory. San Antonio, of controlled studies. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 25, 397–409.
TX: Psychological Corporation. Paterson, R. J. (2000). The assertiveness workbook. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger Press.
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Beck depression inventory manual (2nd Primakoff, L., Epstein, N., & Covi, L. (1986). Homework compliance: an uncontrolled
ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. variable in cognitive therapy outcome research. Behavior Therapy, 17, 433–446.
Borkovec, T. D., & Nau, S. D. (1972). Credibility of analogue therapy rationales. Rachman, S. (1983). Obstacles to the successful treatment of obsessions. In E. B. Foa,
Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 3, 257–260. & P. Emmelkamp (Eds.) (pp. 38–57). New York: Wiley.
Clark, D. M., Salkovskis, P. M., Hackmann, A., Wells, A., Fennell, M., Ludgate, J., et al. Rachman, S. J. (1997). A cognitive theory of obsessions. Behaviour Research and
(1998). Two psychological treatments for hypochondriasis: a randomized Therapy, 35, 793–802.
controlled trial. British Journal of Psychiatry, 173, 218–225. Rachman, S. (1998). A cognitive theory of obsessions: elaborations. Behaviour
Devilly, G. J., & Borkovec, T. D. (2000). Psychometric properties of the credibility/ Research and Therapy, 36, 385–402.
expectancy questionnaire. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Rachman, S. (2003). The treatment of obsessions. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Psychiatry, 31, 73–86. Rachman, S. J., & de Silva, P. (1978). Normal and abnormal obsessions. Behaviour
Eddy, K. T., Dutra, L., Bradley, R., & Westen, D. (2004). A multidimensional meta- Research and Therapy, 16, 232–248.
analysis of psychotherapy and pharmacotherapy for obsessive-compulsive Salkovskis, P. M., & Harrison, J. (1984). Abnormal and normal obsessions: a repli-
disorder. Clinical Psychology Review, 24, 1011–1030. cation. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 22, 549–552, 549–552.
Edwards, D., Yarvis, R., Mueller, D., Zingale, H., & Wagman, W. (1978). Test-taking Simpson, H. B., Foa, E. B., Liebowitz, M. R., Ledley, D. R., Huppert, J. D., Cahill, S., et al.
and the stability of adjustment scales: can we assess patient deterioration? (2008). A randomized, controlled trial of cognitive-behavioral therapy for
Evaluation Quarterly, 2, 275–291. augmenting pharmacotherapy in obsessive–compulsive disorder. American
First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Givvon, M., & Williams, J. B. (1996). Structured clinical Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 621–630.
interview for DSM-IV axis I disorders – patient edition (SCIS-I/P, version 2.0 4/97 Taylor, S. (1998). Assessment of OCD. In R. P. Swinson, M. M. Antony, S. Rachman, &
revision). Unpublished manual: Biometrics Research Department, New York M. A. Richter (Eds.), Obsessive–compulsive disorder: Theory, research and treat-
State Psychiatric Institute, New York, NY. ment. New York: Guildford Press.
Freeston, M. H., Ladouceur, R., Gagnon, F., Thibodeau, N., Rheaume, J., Letarte, H., et Weissman, M. M., & Bothwell, S. (1976). Assessment of social adjustment by patient
al. (1997). Cognitive–behavioral treatment of obsessive thoughts: a controlled self-report. Archives of General Psychiatry, 33, 1111–1115.
study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65, 405–413. Whittal, M.L., Robichaud, M. & Woody, S.R. Treatment of obsessions:enhancing
Freeston, M. H., Leger, E., & Ladouceur, R. (2001). Cognitive therapy of obsessive dissemination with videos. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, in press, doi:10.
thoughts. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 8, 61–78. 1016/j.cbpra.2009.07.001.
Frost, R. O., & Steketee, G. (2002). Cognitive approaches to obsessions and compul- Whittal, M. L., Thordarson, D. S., & McLean, P. D. (2005). Treatment of obsessive-
sions: Theory, assessment and treatment. Amsterdam: Pergamon. compulsive disorder: cognitive behavior therapy vs exposure and response
Frost, R. O., Steketee, G., & Grisham, J. (2004). Measurement of compulsive prevention. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43, 1559–1576.
hoarding: saving inventory – revised. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42, Woody, S. R., O’Neill, M., Fairbrother, N., & McLean, P. D. (2003). Stress management
1163–1182. training for obsessions: treatment manual. Unpublished.
Goodman, W. K., Price, L. H., Rasmussen, S. A., Mazure, C., Fleischmann, R. L., Woody, S. R., Steketee, G., & Chambless, D. L. (1995). Reliability and validity of the
Hill, C. L., et al. (1989). The yale-brown obsessive–compulsive scale, I: devel- yale-brown obsessive–compulsive scale. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33,
opment, use and reliability. Archives of General Psychiatry, 40, 1006–1011. 597–605.

Please cite this article in press as: Whittal, M.L., et al., Treatment of obsessions: A randomized controlled trial, Behaviour Research and Therapy
(2009), doi:10.1016/j.brat.2009.11.010

You might also like