You are on page 1of 6

Personality and Individual Differences 104 (2017) 98–103

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Personality and Individual Differences

journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid

Revisiting the structural and nomological validity of the Zimbardo time


perspective inventory
Mark A. Davis ⁎, Daniel A. Cernas Ortiz
a
Department of Management University of North Texas, 1155 Union Circle #305429, Denton, TX 76203-5017, United States
b
Contaduría y Administración Universidad Autónoma del Estado de México, Mexico

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: In this study, we examined empirical relationships between temporal perspective “biases” measured by the
Received 30 April 2016 Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory (ZTPI) and psychological strengths from the domain of positive psycholo-
Received in revised form 18 July 2016 gy. The ZTPI factorial structure proposed by Zimbardo and Boyd (1999) ostensibly captures five distinct temporal
Accepted 27 July 2016
dimensions. Yet, a perusal of research using the ZTPI suggests that conclusions about its structural validity are
Available online xxxx
premature. We revisited the structural validity of this instrument via confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) of ZTPI
Keywords:
scores obtained from a sample of undergraduate and graduate business students (N = 720). CFA results pro-
Time perspective duced fit indices short of acceptable thresholds indicating that support for the five-factor structure remains ten-
Structural validity tative. Although psychometric analyses revealed meaningful links between ZTPI dimensions and measures of
Psychological strengths vitality, resilience, and hope, findings also suggest that the Past-Positive subscale has questionable reliability. Im-
plications and recommendations for future research on temporal perspective in general and the ZTPI in particular
are discussed.
© 2016 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction link between future orientation and academic achievement (Mello &
Worrell, 2006; Shell & Husman, 2001) suggest a favorable influence.
Time perspective theory posits that individuals engage in a habitual, However, a bias towards a hedonistic or fatalistic present appears to
largely unconscious partitioning of personal experience into past, pres- be associated with tobacco use and alcohol consumption (Daugherty
ent, and future time frames. An important consequence of this process is & Brase, 2010). Given the burgeoning literature that encompasses
that judgment, decision-making, and action are affected by the time perspective, effective construct measurement is clearly a priority.
individual's preferences for particular frames (Zimbardo & Boyd, Perhaps the most popular measure, the Zimbardo Time Perspective In-
1999). For example, any emphasis on recollecting one's past, whether ventory (ZTPI, Zimbardo & Boyd, 1999) captures time perspective as a
aversive or positive, is likely to govern ongoing interpretation of and re- multidimensional construct, yielding separate scores for each of five
sponse to decision situations. By comparison, one would expect a decid- temporal factors: Past-Negative, Past-Positive, Present-Hedonistic, Pres-
edly different interpretation and decision process if the individual is ent-Fatalistic, and Future. This multidimensional attribute is an obvious
preoccupied with a future replete with means-ends thinking and alter- strength of the measure as it allows one to explore the entire range of
native goal states. time perspective.
Evidence confirming the latent structure of ZTPI scores using U.S.
adults is largely confined to Zimbardo and Boyd's (1999) seminal
1.1. Structural validity of the Zimbardo time perspective inventory
study. Unfortunately, the authors' omission of currently recommended
measures of model fit precludes definitive support for structural validi-
In support of this theory, a growing body of research demonstrates
ty. Subsequent studies addressing the structural validity of the ZTPI
that temporal preference relates to a variety of attitudes, attributes,
have reported poor fit as well as problems with particular scales (e.g.,
and behaviors. For example, Zhang and Howell (2011) observed signif-
Shipp, Edwards, & Lambert, 2009; Worrell & Mello, 2007). For instance,
icant relationships between life satisfaction and both present and future
Worrell and Mellow reported poor fit indices (comparative fit
time perspectives, while Drake, Duncan, Sutherland, Abernathy, and
index [CFI] = 0.636) and weak reliability for the Past-Positive scale
Henry (2008) found that an emphasis on the past, whether positive or
items (α = 0.61) in a sample of 815 American adolescents. Moreover,
negative, is linked to self-reported happiness. Studies demonstrating a
their post hoc exploratory factor analysis (EFA) yielded both five and
⁎ Corresponding author.
six-factor solutions. Similarly, Shipp et al. obtained a CFI of 0.63 and
E-mail addresses: Mark.Davis@unt.edu (M.A. Davis), danielarturoc@yahoo.com.mx significant cross-loadings for 80% of the items in a sample of U.S. college
(D.A. Cernas Ortiz). students.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.07.037
0191-8869/© 2016 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
M.A. Davis, D.A. Cernas Ortiz / Personality and Individual Differences 104 (2017) 98–103 99

Subsequent research with the ZTPI has either ignored its structur- 1.3. The present study
al shortcomings or avoided the issue by limiting investigations to
specific subscales (e.g., Bernstein & Benfield, 2013; Ryack, 2012). Previous empirical work using the ZTPI reveals inconsistent findings
Various assessments of structural validity and measurement invari- with respect to the latent structure of the measure. Still, ongoing inves-
ance involving translated versions of the ZTPI have also generated tigations linking time perspective to a sizeable number of attitudes and
poorly-fitting solutions (e.g., Cretu, 2012; Liniauskaite & Kairys, attributes including SWB attest to the fecundity of the construct. Ac-
2009; Milfont, Andrade, Belo, & Pessoa, 2008; Mitina & Blinnikova, cordingly, the aims of this study are to: (a) reexamine the latent struc-
2008). In particular, Sircova et al. (2014) describe a multi-sample ture and psychometric properties of ZTPI scores and (b) extend the
study involving 24 countries that produced invariance indices con- nomological validity of time perspective by examining interrelation-
spicuously lower than acceptable thresholds (CFI = 0.86). Further- ships with psychological strengths commonly linked to SWB.
more, negatively worded items from the Past-Positive scale were
either eliminated or rekeyed by the authors. Of course, establishing 1.4. Analytic strategy
measurement invariance across different cultures and groups may
be premature in the absence of strong initial evidence for the scale's To achieve these aims, we analyze responses to the ZTPI using CFA to
structural validity. In sum, if the ZTPI is to be the instrument of choice assess whether the factor structure from our sample is consistent with
in time perspective research, additional evidence is required to sup- the five-factor structure reported by Zimbardo and Boyd (1999). Our
port its use. examination includes an assessment of ZTPI items and subscale scores
(i.e., descriptive statistics and internal consistency). Finally, we investi-
gate the contribution of the five temporal factors to differences in vital-
1.2. Time perspective and psychological strengths ity, resilience, and hope.

Since the onset of research on subjective well-being (SWB), scholars 2. Method


have attempted to identify personal attributes or psychological
strengths that relate to happiness and life satisfaction. For example, re- 2.1. Participants and procedure
silience, the ability to cope with adversity, is a personal resource that
has been shown to promote life satisfaction (Cohn, Fredrickson, The sample consisted of 748 graduate and undergraduate business
Brown, & Mikels, 2009), while character strengths such as hope and vi- students attending two large state universities in the southwestern
tality are acknowledged as the foundation for a happy and healthy life United States. Twenty-eight subjects (3.7%) were eliminated due to
(Park, Peterson, & Seligman, 2004). As noted previously, time perspec- missing data. This resulted in a final sample of 720 subjects (360
tive scholars have demonstrated that the manner in which individuals women, 360 men) who ranged in age from 18 to 70 years (M = 35,
recall, experience, or anticipate circumstances in life also accounts for SD = 5). The ethnic composition was Caucasian (n = 414; 57.5%),
significant variance in life satisfaction and happiness. Thus, while em- Asian (n = 93; 12.9%), Black or African American (n = 92; 12.8%), His-
pirical research shows that time perspective and psychological panic or Latino (n = 86; 11.9%) and Other (n = 35; 4.9%). Participants
strengths relate to SWB, the interrelations among these antecedents voluntarily completed the ZTPI, a large set of established scales, and de-
are less well understood. Within the context of time perspective, the mographic items presented as an online survey.
psychological strengths of resilience, vitality, and hope merit special at-
tention. The import of resilience stems from its role in inoculating indi-
2.2. Measures
viduals from negative experiences in the past as well as the present
(Cohn et al., 2009). With respect to character strengths, we note that
2.2.1. ZTPI
Park et al. (2004) demonstrated that vitality (zest) and hope had the
The ZTPI (Zimbardo & Boyd, 1999) is a 56-item self-report measure
most substantial relationships with life satisfaction among the 24
consisting of five subscales: Past-Negative, Past-Positive, Present-Hedo-
strengths they identified.
nistic, Present-Fatalistic, and Future. Participants respond to questions
Enhanced as well as diminished levels of resilience, vitality, and
using a 5-point Likert scale with anchors ranging from ‘1’ (very unchar-
hope are likely to accompany any disproportionate emphasis on partic-
acteristic) and ‘5’ (very characteristic). Zimbardo and Boyd reported in-
ular time frames. First, prior research suggests that positive emotions in-
ternal consistency estimates for the five subscales that ranged from
crease life satisfaction by building resilience (Cohn et al., 2009).
0.74 to 0.82.
Accordingly, personal bias towards a joyful or contented past should
predict the emergence of resilience. Second, vitality is defined as a pos-
itive feeling of energy and aliveness (Ryan & Frederick, 1997). Given 2.2.2. Vitality
that a Present-Hedonistic bias is characterized by a desire for immediate Ryan and Frederick's (1997) measure of vitality uses seven Likert
gratification and excitement, this time perspective is especially likely to scale items (e.g., “I nearly always feel alert and awake”) anchored by
give rise to heightened vitality. Third, Snyder et al. (1996) define hope not at all true (1) and very true (7). The internal consistency estimate de-
as goal-directed thinking such that individuals believe that they can rived from the present sample was 0.84.
produce routes to desired goals (pathways thinking) and they will
have the motivation to use those routes (agency thinking). Goal-direct- 2.2.3. Resilience
ed behavior connects individuals to future outcomes, and any sense of Smith et al. (2008) developed the Brief Resilience Scale (BRS) to
effective agency emerges from prior successes or failures. In as much measure individual differences in the ability to bounce back or recover
as a future orientation stresses planning and personal achievement from stress. The six-item scale consists of Likert scale items (e.g., “I
and one's view of the past can emphasize negative or positive fea- tend to bounce back quickly after hard times”) anchored by strongly dis-
tures that relate to a sense of agency, we anticipate that hope will agree (1) and strongly agree (7). Cronbach's alpha estimate from the cur-
have one or more connections with Future, Past-Positive, and Past- rent sample was 0.79.
Negative perspective. In sum, the links among time perspective and
psychological strengths are untested relationships. Hence, the inclu- 2.2.4. State hope scale (SHS)
sion of psychological strengths into time perspective research has Snyder et al. (1996) developed the six-item SHS to assess the
the potential to enhance understanding of the nomological network individual's perceived capacity to sustain action and reach goals. Partic-
of the construct. ipants respond to the items using an 8-point Likert scale (1 = definitely
100 M.A. Davis, D.A. Cernas Ortiz / Personality and Individual Differences 104 (2017) 98–103

false; 8 = definitely true). In the current study, the reliability estimate for Table 2a
the measure was 0.81. Correlations of ZPTI subscales and psychological strength measures.

Vitality Resilience Hope


3. Results Past-Positive 0.243⁎⁎ 0.091⁎ 0.207⁎⁎
Past-Negative −0.334⁎⁎ −0.466⁎⁎ −0.326⁎⁎
3.1. Psychometric properties of ZTPI subscales Present-Hedonistic 0.244⁎⁎ 0.027 0.122⁎⁎
Present-Fatalistic −0.150⁎⁎ −0.337⁎⁎ −0.298⁎⁎
Future 0.232⁎⁎ 0.165⁎⁎ 0.373⁎⁎
Table 1 presents the means, standard deviations, measures of skew-
ness and kurtosis, item-total correlations, and internal consistency sta- ⁎ p b 0.05.
⁎⁎ p b 0.01.
tistics for the major variables in the study. Although responses to the
Past-Positive subscale exhibited moderate levels of skewness and kurto-
sis, the distribution of this variable did not depart significantly from nor- current practice employs so-called approximate fit indices to judge
mality. Corrected item-total correlations for the Past-Positive subscale model adequacy. The use of approximate fit tests has evolved, and re-
ranged from −0.41 to 0.51. Ideally, the values should be positive in ad- searchers have transitioned to a small number of indices. Bentler
dition to being of moderate to high magnitude. The Past-Positive sub- (2007) specifically recommends the standardized root mean square re-
scale also generated the lowest reliability estimate (α = 0.57) among sidual (SMSR), which is scaled as “badness-of-fit” index. A value of zero
the ZTPI subscales. This value is substantially lower than the estimate signifies perfect model fit, though values below 0.08 are regarded as ev-
reported by Zimbardo and Boyd (1999). Internal consistency estimates idence of acceptable fit. Bentler also recommends “at most two other in-
for the remaining ZTPI subscales were comparable to estimates from dices of fit such as the CFI and RMSEA (p. 826).” Values for the CFI range
prior research (mdn α = 79.5). from 0–1.0 with acceptable fit operationalized as ≥0.95. For the RMSEA
(root mean square error of approximation), acceptable fit is operation-
3.2. Bivariate correlations alized as values less than or equal to 0.05. For CFAs, the SMSR, RMSEA,
and CFI are among the most widely reported approximate fit tests
Table 2a contains the intercorrelations among ZTPI subscales and the (Kline, 2011). It is noteworthy that Zimbardo and Boyd (1999) based
psychological strength measures. The Past-Negative subscale and Pres- their conclusions on a single fit index, the normed chi-square (NC). Im-
ent-Fatalistic subscale scores were significantly and negatively correlat- portantly, Kline argues that this statistic should have no role in the as-
ed with scores on each of the psychological strength measures. Both sessment of model fit because “there is little statistical or logical
subscales were most strongly correlated with resilience (− 0.47 and foundation for NC (p. 204).”
−0.34 for Past-Negative and Present-Fatalistic, respectively). This pat- Results of the CFAs appear in Table 3. Although χ2 difference tests
tern of inverse relationships likely reflects the negative affective tone demonstrated that the five-factor model was superior to both the one-
of the Past-Negative and Present-Fatalistic items. By comparison, the factor and three-factor models, the fit metrics reveal none of the models
Past-Positive and Future subscale scores were positively correlated provided an acceptable fit to the data. An additional model considers the
with the psychological strength measures. In this instance, the Past-Pos- potential existence of method effects. In this model eight error covari-
itive scores related most strongly to vitality (r = 0.24), whereas Future ances representing two separate method factors (i.e., items with high
time perspective was most strongly correlated with hope (r = 0.37). content overlap and negatively worded items) were freely estimated.
Lastly, Present-Hedonistic subscale scores were positively correlated Although this respecification resulted in significant improvement
with measures of vitality (r = 0.24) and hope (r = 0.12). (χ2diff (12) = 534.93, p b 0.01), the overall fit of the method effects
model was still unsatisfactory.
3.3. Structural validity of ZTPI scores Standardized item loadings for the CFA appear in Table 4. Cross-
loadings and their significance levels for items with the highest modifi-
3.3.1. CFA cation indices (top 20%) are also presented. The greatest number of
We performed CFAs to investigate the underlying factor structure of large cross-loadings are found on the Past-Positive and Present-Hedo-
ZTPI scores. One-factor, three-factor, and five-factor models were exam- nistic subscales. In the case of the Past-Positive subscale, the four salient
ined. The five-factor model was identical to the model reported by cross-loadings impacts over 40% of the scale items.
Zimbardo and Boyd (1999), whereas the three-factor model consisted
of Past (Past-Positive and Past-Negative), Present (Present-Hedonistic 3.4. Predicting psychological strengths with demographics and time
and Present-Fatalistic) and Future items. The CFAs were performed on perspective
the variance-covariance matrices using LISREL 8.80 (Joreskog &
Sorbom, 2007). Maximum likelihood estimation was used and good- A prerequisite to evaluating structural relations among latent vari-
ness-of-fit was evaluated using several indices of fit. The model χ2 as- ables is a viable measurement model (Brown, 2006). In view of the un-
sesses the exact-fit hypothesis (i.e., no differences between population satisfactory results of the CFA, this requirement was not met. Therefore,
covariances and those predicted by the model). However, large N solu- we analyzed the unique contribution of ZTPI subscale scores to the
tions are generally rejected on the basis of this fit index. Consequently, prediction of psychological strengths using standard hierarchical

Table 1
Psychometric Properties of Study Variables.

Scale Items M SD Skew Kurtosis Item-total correlations α

Past-Positive 9 3.53 0.45 −0.598 1.352 −0.41 to 0.51 0.57(0.52–0.61)


Past-Negative 10 2.95 0.69 0.020 −0.237 0.23 to 0.58 0.83(0.81–0.85)
Present-Hedonistic 15 3.36 0.50 −0.163 0.653 0.09 to 0.54 0.80(0.78–0.82)
Present-Fatalistic 9 2.44 0.63 0.461 0.136 0.13 to 0.39 0.78(0.76–0.81)
Future 13 3.74 0.52 −0.356 0.226 0.26 to 0.35 0.79(0.76–0.81)
Vitality 7 4.89 1.07 −0.561 0.372 0.52 to 0.74 0.85(0.84–0.87)
Resilience 6 4.78 1.05 −0.290 −0.191 0.10 to 0.68 0.79(0.77–0.82)
Hope 6 5.28 1.00 −0.712 0.566 0.47 to 0.66 0.81(0.79–0.83)

Note: N = 720.
M.A. Davis, D.A. Cernas Ortiz / Personality and Individual Differences 104 (2017) 98–103 101

regression analysis. CFA results notwithstanding, no items were elimi- Table 3


nated or reassigned to a different latent factor. That is, the subscales Fit Indices for ZTPI scores.

used in the analysis were identical to those originally formulated by Model χ2 df CFI SRMR RMSEA (90% CI)
Zimbardo and Boyd (1999). Given the poor reliability of the Past-Posi- One-factor 8658.30⁎ 1484 0.774 0.104 0.116, 0.119
tive subscale, results based on this subscale should be interpreted Three-factor (past, present, 6524.55⁎ 1481 0.840 0.095 0.088, 0.091
with caution. The results show that time perspective subscale scores ac- future)
count for significant variance in the psychological strength measures Five-factor 5264.09⁎ 1474 0.879 0.089 0.065, 0.069
Five-factor – method effects 4729.16⁎ 1466 0.896 0.087 0.059, 0.063
after controlling for gender and age (see Table 2b). It is noteworthy
that the Past-Negative subscale was a significant predictor in each re- Note: CFI = comparative fit index; SRMR = standardized root mean square residual;
gression model. By comparison, the Past-Positive subscale was a weak, RMSEA = root mean square error of approximation.
⁎ p b 0.001.
but significant predictor for one strength, vitality. Across all regressions,
scores from the Past-Negative subscale generated several large struc-
ture coefficients (rs) with a mean rs of 0.70. In order of magnitude, the
mean rs for the remaining time perspective subscales were 0.49 (Pres-
ent-Fatalistic), 0.47 (Future), 0.33 (Past-Positive), and 0.24 (Present- 4.2. Nomological network
Hedonistic).
Prior research indicates that judgments of SWB vary reliably with in-
dividual differences in time perspective (Drake et al., 2008; Zhang &
4. Discussion Howell, 2011). We posited that the nomological net in which time per-
spective is embedded would also include the psychological strengths
Neither the original validation research reported by Zimbardo and that contribute to SWB. Our regression models clearly demonstrate
Boyd (1999) nor the more recent work on structural equivalence of that personal variations in time perspective have implications for vital-
the ZTPI follow well-established recommendations on model fit indices. ity, resilience, and hope.
Consequently, evaluating the ZTPI factor structure's replicability in an A Past-Negative orientation produced the most robust relationships
independent sample was an important aim of the study. with the three psychological strengths. This result is consistent with the
well-established social psychological phenomenon that the experience
of negative events, negative emotions, and negative information has
4.1. Structural validity of the ZTPI greater power than positive encounters (Baumeister, Bratslavsky,
Finkenauer, & Vohs, 2001). The relationships involving Present-Fatalis-
On the basis of goodness-of-fit criteria, the CFA failed to replicate the tic orientation suggest a similar influence. That is, the sense of resigna-
five-factor structure reported by Zimbardo and Boyd (1999). Yet, the tion and powerlessness that characterize this orientation conveys a
five-factor model proved to be superior to both the one-factor and decidedly negative tone. As such, an emphasis on negative events of
three-factor models we tested. While too few or too many factors repre- the past or correspondingly cynical views of the present is likely to be
sent a potential basis of poor fit, we consider another possible source, in- detrimental to the emergence of psychological strengths. In contrast
dicator-factor misspecifications, in our discussion of subscale reliability. to these detriments, a dominant focus on the future bodes well for the
With one exception, internal consistency estimates for the ZTPI sub- development of psychological capacities as Future orientation was pos-
scales can be characterized as good. Specifically, confidence intervals in- itively linked to vitality, resilience, and hope.
dicated that reliability estimates for most subscales exceeded 0.75.
However, scores on the Past-Positive subscale demonstrated an insuffi-
cient level of reliability. The range of negative and positive item-total 4.3. Limitations and future directions
correlations for the Past-Positive subscale suggests some items may be
confusing or ambiguous for respondents. Indeed, the CFA revealed Our use of a business student sample limits any generalization to
four items from the Past-Positive subscale that had salient cross-load- non-student adult populations and, perhaps non-business student pop-
ings with other subscales. It is important to note that CFA procedures ulations. Nonetheless, given that CFA is the technique of choice for
work best with simple structure, wherein each item loads highly on assessing factor replicability, our results raise some concerns about the
one and only one factor. In effect, the flaws evident in this subscale structural validity of the ZTPI. Recent efforts to resolve these psycho-
may account for some, though not necessarily all sources of CFA metric problems have met with limited success (Worrell et al., 2016).
model misspecification in our analysis. On the one hand, some personality scholars question the suitability of

Table 2b
Predicting Psychological Strengths with Time Perspectives.

Vitality Resilience Hope

Predictor ΔR2 β ΔR2 β ΔR2 β

Step 1 0.002 0.017⁎⁎ 0.001


Age −0.008 0.051 0.003
Gender −0.052 −0.097⁎⁎ −0.058
Step 2 0.290⁎⁎ 0.257⁎⁎ 0.306⁎⁎
Past-Positive 0.090⁎⁎ 0.033 0.051
Past-Negative −0.367⁎⁎ −0.400⁎⁎ −0.263⁎⁎
Present-Hedonistic 0.369⁎⁎ 0.197⁎⁎ 0.312⁎⁎
Present-Fatalistic −0.027 −0.178⁎⁎ −0.160⁎⁎
Future 0.245⁎⁎ 0.085⁎ 0.349⁎⁎
Total R2 0.292⁎⁎ 0.274⁎⁎ 0.307⁎⁎

Note: N = 720.
⁎ p b 0.05.
⁎⁎ p b 0.01.
102 M.A. Davis, D.A. Cernas Ortiz / Personality and Individual Differences 104 (2017) 98–103

Table 4
ZTPI factor loadings.

Item Past-Positive (PP) Past-Negative (PN) Present-Hedonistic (PH) Present-Fatalistic (PF) Future (F)

PP2 0.50** 0.27**


PP7 0.49**
PP11 0.61**
PP15 0.32** 0.34**
PP20 0.49**
PP25 –0.59** 0.59**
PP29 0.32** 0.38**
PP41 –0.36** 0.55**
PP49 0.44**
PN4 0.66**
PN5 0.26**
PN16 0.93**
PN22 0.54**
PN27 0.53**
PN33 0.48**
PN34 0.74**
PN36 0.50**
PN50 0.93**
PN54 0.72**
PH1 0.44**
PH8 0.62**
PH12 0.28**
PH17 0.55** 0.26**
PH19 0.34**
PH23 0.62** –0.62**
PH26 0.29** 0.50**
PH28 0.42** 0.68**
PH31 0.68**
PH32 0.40**
PH42 0.70**
PH44 0.47**
PH46 0.48**
PH48 0.55**
PH55 0.20**
PF3 0.37**
PF14 0.54**
PF35 0.58**
PF37 0.67**
PF38 0.65**
PF39 0.68**
PF47 0.43**
PF52 0.50**
PF53 0.60**
FU6 0.59**
FU9 −0.39**
FU10 0.44**
FU13 0.53**
FU18 0.40**
FU21 0.42**
FU24 0.61** −0.59**
FU30 0.38**
FU40 0.56**
FU43 0.53**
FU45 0.44**
FU51 0.27**
FU56 0.72** –0.45**

Note: N = 720. Bold factor loadings were obtained from our CFA, whereas the cross-loadings and their significance levels were obtained from the modification indices of expected change
if allowed to cross-load.
⁎⁎ pb 0.01.

CFA for assessing personality structure because the assumption of sim- rekeying of items from the subscale (e.g., Sircova et al., 2014). In our
ple structure is regarded as implausible for factor analysis of personality view, revisions of this subscale could remedy the poor fitting CFA solu-
structure (see McCrae, Zonderman, Costa, Bond, & Paunonen, 1996). tions that have characterized previous research.
However, alternatives to CFA such as EFA with Procrustes methods of Despite the strong associations among time perspective dimensions
factor rotation have been subject to criticism as well (e.g., Paunonen, and psychological strengths, the exact causal directions among the core
1997). On the other hand, consistent with previous findings, our analy- variables remain indeterminate. Our focus on the presumed conse-
sis revealed problems with the Past-Positive subscale. Hence, evidence quences of time perspective excluded any exploration of possible ante-
for the reliability of this subscale remains tenuous. A perusal of the cedents. Of course, time perspective is largely viewed as “trait-like.”
core items comprising this subscale suggest they tap a nostalgic or ap- Accordingly, any discussion of antecedents raises the question of genet-
preciative sense for the past. Yet, several cross-loaded items from the ic versus environmental influences. From a hereditary perspective, fu-
subscale connote unpleasantness or negative aspects of the past. Some ture research incorporating measures of adult temperament (Evans &
researchers have attempted to address the problem by eliminating or Rothbart, 2007) could provide insights regarding the innate forces
M.A. Davis, D.A. Cernas Ortiz / Personality and Individual Differences 104 (2017) 98–103 103

that shape time perspective preferences. Alternatively, if one adopts the Joreskog, K., & Sorbom, D. (2007). LISREL 8.80 [computer software]. Chicago: Scientific Soft-
ware International, Inc.
view that time perspective is influenced by personal circumstances and Kline, R. B. (2011). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. New York:
experiences, then the incidence and intensity of personal traumas or tri- Guilford Press.
umphs qualify as potential antecedents. In addition, one can speculate Liniauskaite, A., & Kairys, A. (2009). The Lithuanian version of the Zimbardo time perspec-
tive inventory (ZTPI). Psicotecnia, 40, 66–86.
whether modifications of time perspective can be achieved through in- McCrae, R. R., Zonderman, A. B., Costa, P. T., Bond, M. H., & Paunonen, S. V. (1996). Evalu-
struction, coaching, or therapeutic intervention. Consequently, methods ating replicability of factors in the revised NEO personality inventory: Confirmatory
for altering maladaptive time perspective biases and promoting more factor analysis vs Procrustes rotation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70,
552–566.
constructive time perspective should be explored. Mello, Z. R., & Worrell, F. C. (2006). The relationship of time perspective to age, gender,
and academic achievement among academically talented adolescents. Journal for
5. Conclusion the Education of the Gifted, 29(3), 271–289.
Milfont, T. L., Andrade, P. R., Belo, R. P., & Pessoa, V. S. (2008). Testing Zimbardo time per-
spective inventory in a Brazilian sample. Revista Internacional de Psicologica, 42,
Research on time perspective has clearly demonstrated that individ- 49–58.
ual bias towards the future, present, or past has implications for a wide Mitina, O., & Blinnikova, S. (2008). Adjusting the structure of the Zimbardo time perspec-
tive inventory scales using Russian data. International Journal of Psychology, 43(3–4),
range of perceptions and behavior. In particular, prior research has re- 42.
vealed strong connections between time perspective and SWB. The Park, N., Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. P. (2004). Strengths of character and well-being.
present study shows that differences in psychological strengths can Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 23(5), 603–619.
Paunonen, S. V. (1997). On chance factor congruence following orthogonal Procrustes ro-
also be understood in terms of time perspective biases. While the ZTPI tation. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 57, 33–59.
has become one of the most widely used measures of time perspective, Ryack, K. (2012). Evidence that time perspective factors depend on group: Factor analyses
evidence of factor model deficiency remains and should not be of the CFC and ZTPI scales with professional financial advisors. Personality and
Individual Differences, 52, 723–727.
dismissed. Refinements directed towards improving the internal consis-
Ryan, R. M., & Frederick, C. (1997). On energy, personality, and health: Subjective vitality
tency of the Past-Positive subscale is a reasonable first step towards ad- as a dynamic reflection of well-being. Journal of Personality, 65(3), 529–565.
dressing that deficiency. Until the obligatory reassessment of the ZTPI Shell, D. F., & Husman, J. (2001). The multivariate dimensionality of personal control and
scores is conducted, evidence for the instrument's structural validity future time perspective beliefs in achievement and self-regulation. Contemporary
Educational Psychology, 26, 481–506.
will remain tenuous. Shipp, A. J., Edwards, J. R., & Lambert, L. S. (2009). Conceptualization and measurement of
temporal focus: The subjective experience of past, present, and future. Organizational
Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 110, 1–22.
References Sircova, A., van de Vijver, F. J. R., Osin, E., Milfont, T. L., Fieulaine, N., Kislali-Erginbilgic, A., ...
Boyd, J. N. (2014). A global look at time: A 24-country study of the equivalence of the
Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C., & Vohs, K. D. (2001). Bad is stronger than
Zimbardo time perspective inventory. Sage Open, 1–12. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
good. Review of General Psychology, 5, 323–370.
2158244013515686 January–March.
Bentler, P. M. (2007). On tests and indices for evaluating structural models. Personality
Smith, B. W., Dalen, J., Wiggins, K., Tooley, E., Christopher, P., & Bernard, J. (2008). The
and Individual Differences, 42(5), 825–829.
brief resilience scale: Assessing the ability to bounce back. International Journal of
Bernstein, M. J., & Benfield, J. A. (2013). Past perspective is related to present relation-
Behavioral Medicine, 15(3), 194–200.
ships: Past positive and negative time perspectives differentially predict rejection
Snyder, C. R., Sympson, S. C., Ybasco, F. C., Borders, T. F., Babyak, M. A., & Higgins, R. L.
sensitivity. The Psychological Record, 63, 615–628.
(1996). Development and validation of the state hope scale. Journal of Personality
Brown, T. A. (2006). Confirmatory factor analysis for applied research. New York: Guilford
and Social Psychology, 70(2), 321–335.
Press.
Worrell, F. C., & Mello, Z. R. (2007). The reliability and validity of Zimbardo time perspec-
Cohn, M. A., Fredrickson, B. L., Brown, S. L., & Mikels, J. A. (2009). Happiness unpacked:
tive scores in academically talented adolescents. Educational and Psychological
Positive emotions increase life satisfaction by building resilience. Emotion, 9(3),
Measurement, 67(3), 487–504.
361–368.
Worrell, F. C., Temple, E. C., McKay, M. T., Zivovic, U., Perry, J. L., Mello, Z. R., ... Cole, J. C.
Cretu, R. Z. (2012). A confirmatory approach of the structure of Zimbardo's time perspec-
(2016). A theoretical approach to resolving psychometric problems associated with
tive concept. Cognition, Brain, and Behavior: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 16(4),
the Zimbardo time perspective inventory. European Journal of Psychological
481–494.
Assessment. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000313.
Daugherty, J. R., & Brase, G. L. (2010). Taking time to be healthy: Predicting health behav-
Zhang, J. W., & Howell, R. T. (2011). Do time perspectives predict unique variance in life
iors with delay discounting and time perspective. Personality and Individual
satisfaction beyond personality traits? Personality and Individual Differences, 50,
Differences, 48(2), 202–207.
1261–1266.
Drake, L., Duncan, E., Sutherland, F., Abernathy, C., & Henry, C. (2008). Time perspective
Zimbardo, P. G., & Boyd, J. N. (1999). Putting time in perspective: A valid, reliable individ-
and correlates of well-being. Time & Society, 17, 47–61.
ual-difference metric. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 1271–1288.
Evans, D. E., & Rothbart, M. K. (2007). Developing a model for adult temperament. Journal
of Research in Personality, 41, 868–888.

You might also like