You are on page 1of 3

Clinical Neurology and Neurosurgery 192 (2020) 105747

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Clinical Neurology and Neurosurgery


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/clineuro

Case Report

‘I see colors when I touch them’. Color agnosia with visuo-tactile facilitation T
in a patient with posterior cortical atrophy
Andrea Zangrandia,*, Alessandro Miolia, Emma Falatoa,b, Federico Ranieria,b, Giovanni Di Pinoa,
Vincenzo Di Lazzarob
a
NeXT: Neurophysiology and Neuroengineering of Human-Technology Interaction Research Unit, Università Campus Bio-Medico, Roma, Italy
b
Research Unit of Neurology, Neurophysiology and Neurobiology, Università Campus Bio-Medico, Roma, Italy

1. Introduction pronounced visuospatial and visuoperceptual deficits with relative


sparing of other cognitive functions. Consistently, FDG-PET at the age
Posterior cortical atrophy (PCA) is a neurodegenerative syndrome of 66 showed bilateral occipital and temporal hypometabolism (Fig. 1).
characterized by a progressive impairment of visuospatial and praxis When he came to our attention, at age 69, the patient was unable to
skills, in the absence of impaired visual acuity or ocular causes. Patients ambulate without aid in the outpatient clinic room and even to sit on a
manifest a series of symptoms including simultanagnosia, alexia/agra- chair autonomously. Neurological examination showed impaired visual
phia, dyscalculia, ideomotor apraxia, achromatopsia, environmental fixation and difficulties in simple orders execution. In the attempt to
disorientation, visual and color agnosia. Neuroimaging usually shows reach objects, he missed the target most of the times, and he behaved as
evidence of atrophy in the occipitoparietal or occipitotemporal areas, if he relied only on touch to catch objects. MMSE score had worsened
and/or decreased metabolism on positron emission tomography (PET) from 24/30 at age 64 to 9/30. The patient was not oriented in time and
[1]. PCA is associated with different pathologies of which Alzheimer’s space (he could not recall the name of the hospital and the city) and he
disease is the most common. As part of the diagnostic algorithm, a was not autonomous in activities of daily living. Brain amyloid PET
formal neuropsychological assessment is mandatory to evaluate the scan was negative. Brain MRI showed diffuse cortical atrophy.
involvement of each cognitive domains and to confirm that the disorder A detailed neuropsychological evaluation showed a severe multi-
is relatively restricted to posterior regions [1]. modal cognitive impairment. Speech was fluent but poor in content,
verbal comprehension was good, and he sporadically used paraphasias.
2. Case report Short-term verbal memory (digit span) and some language domains
(grammatical comprehension and spontaneous production) were pre-
We describe the case of a 69-year-old left-handed male, former served with respect to the remaining cognitive functions. His mood
butcher, with five years of formal education. He had a family history of appeared good throughout the assessment.
dementia (mother), and his prior medical history was noteworthy for Notably, a verbal memory test for object color was impaired (18/
low vision of the right eye due to unspecified trauma in childhood. He 30): the pipe was defined "light yellow", the banana "gray", the crocodile
had undergone a first neuropsychological assessment at the age of 63, "of many colors … gray … yellow", the traffic light "of three colors …
due to subjective memory complaints and spatial disorientation started white, gray and red”. The patient had no previous history of color
5 years before. Scores were pathological on tests for verbal memory blindness.
(short story delayed recall, word list with interference) and executive Furthermore, object naming from description was impaired (32/
functions (TMT-A, TMT-B, phonemic fluency). Most visuospatial tests 48). Patient’s performance was worse when he was provided with se-
were impaired: he was unable to draw figures, to copy figures, and he mantic cues based on the color (8 errors out of 12 items), e.g. “it is a
failed the clock drawing test. Both writing and reading skills were af- stone fruit, it grows in couples, it is red” (correct answer: cherry; patient
fected (agraphia and alexia). Instead, tests for auditory-verbal com- answer: pineapple) rather than when the description was based on
prehension (Token test), cognitive estimation (CET) and short-term functional and/or other characteristics (8 errors out of 36 items), e.g.
memory (digit span) were within the normal range. At that stage, there “it is an animal, it lives in Australia, it has paws and pouch” (correct
were no signs of ideative or ideomotor apraxia. Despite his cognitive answer: kangaroo; patient answer: camel). Patient’s performance in
profile, his behaviour was normal, without personality changes. Head objects naming improved when he was holding and manipulating them.
CT scan was normal. In addition, an ad-hoc brief color naming test was administered. In
During the following years, a PCA was suspected due to the this test, the patient had to say the color of real (e.g. telephone, pen,


Corresponding author at: Via Álvaro del Portillo, 200, 00128, Roma, Italy.
E-mail address: a.zangrandi@unicampus.it (A. Zangrandi).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clineuro.2020.105747
Received 23 July 2019; Received in revised form 10 February 2020; Accepted 23 February 2020
Available online 24 February 2020
0303-8467/ © 2020 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
A. Zangrandi, et al. Clinical Neurology and Neurosurgery 192 (2020) 105747

Fig. 1. 18F-FDG PET at age 69 showing diffuse bilateral occipital and temporal hypometabolism.

etc.) or abstract objects (paper squares of different colors), which were a certain shape in object manipulation (touch-color synesthesia).
visually presented. In case of errors, he could touch and manipulate the However, such phenomenon does not account for our patient's im-
object, before trying to answer again. With real objects, the perfor- proved performance during visuo-tactile manipulation of abstract ob-
mance improved when he was allowed to manipulate the objects: e.g., jects, which had identical shape; nonetheless our observation might
the telephone was defined yellow (wrong answer) when it was presented subtend a common neurophysiological background with synesthesia
visually, and became "grey" (correct answer) when he was allowed to characterized by a cross-talking between the cortical pathways pro-
manipulate it. Surprisingly, the same phenomenon occurred with ab- cessing tactile and visual stimuli.
stract objects (i.e. with black, green and brown squares) Indeed, the most fascinating possibility is that haptic objects ex-
(Supplementary videoclip). ploration facilitated color perception trough a cross-modal information
processing. While the importance of the ventral pathway is largely es-
tablished, it has been suggested that the dorsal stream from V1 to
3. Discussion
parietal areas participates in color perception [3]. It may be speculated
that information coming from the dorsal stream could be sufficient to
We reported the interesting case of a patient with PCA, who had a
partially compensate the compromised ventral pathway. This may en-
severe cognitive impairment with prominent visuospatial and visuo-
hance the possibility of object recognition through multisensory facil-
perceptual deficits, and a degradation of semantic knowledge of colors.
itation mechanisms that are likely mediated by parietal neurons [4].
Patients with PCA have difficulties in objects perception, in space
While PCA patients often exhibit a wide range of manifestations of vi-
analysis, and in distinguishing the objects from the background [2]
sual deficits, presumably there are areas of the occipital cortex that are
Patient’s better performance in objects naming when holding and ma-
relatively preserved. In this way, under somatosensory guidance, the
nipulating them, could be readily explained by the contribution of
information about the visual features of the object could have been
tactile information. This may account also for improvement of color
more easily accessed, and therefore processed by the functionally
naming for real known objects, in which manipulation facilitates object
spared cortical regions.
recognition and, in turn, known objects have likely known colors.
Surprisingly, the performance in colors discrimination improved even
with manipulation of real objects without a prototypical color (e.g. 4. Conclusion
telephone) and abstract objects (colored squares), despite object re-
cognition did not increase the chance for that object to be of a parti- For PCA patients, several non-pharmacological interventions have
cular color. been proposed [5]. This case report should be taken as an interesting
Different mechanisms could account for this unexpected finding. clinical suggestion to be explored more deeply with further patients or
One possible explanation is that by moving the object, the color con- clinical population. If results will be replicated, new rehabilitative ap-
trast between the object and background increased, thus helping object proaches based on visuo-tactile integration could be attempted.
color identification. Another possible explanation may be related to an
impaired gaze orientation showed by the patient, who let his gaze
wander as in the attempt to focus the objects visually presented. He Acknowledgements
could have taken advantage of his residual tactile and motor abilities to
relocate the object in a more favourable position that matched his gaze The authors received no financial support for the publication of this
direction. Also, some people can experience color perception by feeling article.

2
A. Zangrandi, et al. Clinical Neurology and Neurosurgery 192 (2020) 105747

Appendix A. Supplementary data Posterior cortical atrophy, Lancet Neurol. 11 (2) (2012) 170–178.
[2] M.N. Maia da Silva, R.S. Millington, H. Bridge, M. James-Galton, G.T. Plant, Visual
dysfunction in posterior cortical atrophy, Front. Neurol. 8 (2017) 389.
Supplementary material related to this article can be found, in the [3] B.R. Conway, Color signals through dorsal and ventral visual pathways, Vis.
online version, at doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clineuro.2020.105747. Neurosci. 31 (2) (2014) 197–209.
[4] D. Brang, Z.J. Taich, S.A. Hillyard, M. Grabowecky, V.S. Ramachandran, Parietal
connectivity mediates multisensory facilitation, Neuroimage 78 (2013) 396–401.
References [5] M. Roca, E. Gleichgerrcht, T. Torralva, F. Manes, Cognitive rehabilitation in posterior
cortical atrophy, Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 20 (4) (2010) 528–540.
[1] S.J. Crutch, M. Lehmann, J.M. Schott, G.D. Rabinovici, M.N. Rossor, N.C. Fox,

You might also like