You are on page 1of 9

Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 98 (2012) 188–196

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Neurobiology of Learning and Memory


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ynlme

Slow wave sleep during a daytime nap is necessary for protection


from subsequent interference and long-term retention
Sara E. Alger a,⇑, Hiuyan Lau a,b, William Fishbein a
a
Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Sleep, The City College of the City University of New York, 138th Street and Convent Ave., New York, NY 10031, USA
b
Center of Excellence on Brain Aging, Langone Medical Center, New York University, 145 East 32nd Street, 2nd & 5th Floors, New York, NY 10016, USA

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: While it is now generally accepted that sleep facilitates the processing of newly acquired declarative
Received 24 January 2012 information, questions still remain as to the type and length of sleep necessary to best benefit declarative
Revised 12 June 2012 memories. A better understanding could lend support in one direction or another as to the much-debated
Accepted 18 June 2012
role of sleep, be it passive, permissive, or active, in memory processing. The present study employed a
Available online 23 June 2012
napping paradigm and compared performance on a bimodal paired-associates task of those who obtained
a 10-min nap, containing only Stages 1 and 2 sleep, to those whose nap contained slow-wave sleep (SWS)
Keywords:
and rapid eye movement (REM) sleep (60-min nap), as well as to subjects who remained awake.
Memory
Sleep
Measurements were obtained for baseline performance at training, after a sleep/no sleep interval for
Nap short-term retention, after a subsequent stimulus-related interference task, and again after a weeklong
Paired-associates retention period. While all groups learned the information similarly, both nap groups performed better
Declarative than the Wake group when examining short-term retention, approximately 1.5 h after training
SWS (10-min p = .052, 60-min p = .002). However, performance benefits seen in the 10-min nap group proved
to be temporary. Performance after a stimulus-related interference task revealed significantly better
memory retention in the 60-min nap group, with interference disrupting the memory trace far less than
both the Wake and 10-min nap groups (p < .001, p = .006, respectively). After a weeklong retention per-
iod, sleep’s benefit to memory persisted in the 60-min nap group, with performance significantly greater
than both the Wake and 10-min nap groups (p < .001, p = .004, respectively). It is our conclusion that
SWS, obtained only by those in the 60-min nap group, served to actively facilitate the consolidation of
learned bimodal paired-associates, supported by theories such as the Standard Theory of Consolidation
as well as the Synaptic Homeostasis Hypothesis.
Ó 2012 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction & Coulombe, 1989; Fishbein, Kastaniotis, & Chattman, 1974; Smith,
Nixon, & Nader, 2004; Smith, Young, & Young, 1980), increased
The idea that sleep contributes to the processing of new mem- sleep spindle density (Clemens, Fabo, & Halasz, 2005; Fogel &
ories is widely accepted. Behavioral research examining both ani- Smith, 2006; Gais, Mölle, Helms, & Born, 2002; Schabus et al.,
mals and humans using declarative and procedural tasks have 2004), increases in localized slow wave activity (Huber, 2007),
repeatedly demonstrated better retention of the learned material and reactivation of neuronal networks during sleep thought to
after a period of sleep, compared to an equal period spent awake represent the memory trace (Hoffman & McNaughton, 2002; Ji &
(Born, Rasch, & Gais, 2006; Fishbein, McGaugh, & Swarz, 1971; Wilson, 2007; Kudrimoti, Barnes, & McNaughton, 1999; Louie &
Plihal & Born, 1997; Smith, 2001; Walker & Stickgold, 2004). Addi- Wilson, 2001; Maquet et al., 2000; Nadasdy, Hirase, Czurko, Csic-
tionally, many physiological studies using imaging techniques, vari, & Buzsaki, 1999; Pavlides & Winson, 1989; Skaggs &
tracking cerebral blood flow, and recording cellular activity further McNaughton, 1996; Wilson & McNaughton, 1994). A period of
support this idea, demonstrating changes in physiological sleep has been seen to lead to preservation from decay or even
phenomena during post-learning sleep, such as augmented rapid enhancement of explicit, declarative memories, both episodic
eye movement (REM) sleep (De Koninck, Lorrain, Christ, Proulx, memories for events and experiences as well as semantic memo-
ries for facts (Alger, Lau, & Fishbein, 2010; Gais et al., 2008; Kali
& Dayan, 2004; Mednick, Nakayama, & Stickgold, 2003; Nishida,
⇑ Corresponding author. Address: Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Pearsall, Buckner, & Walker, 2009; Pare, Collins, & Pelletier, 2002;
Sleep, Department of Psychology, The City College of New York – NAC 7120, 138th Tucker et al., 2006; Wagner, Gais, & Born, 2001), as well as a gain
Street and Convent Ave., New York, NY 10031, USA
of insight (Wagner, Gais, Haider, Verleger, & Born, 2004) and the
E-mail address: sealger78@gmail.com (S.E. Alger).

1074-7427/$ - see front matter Ó 2012 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2012.06.003
S.E. Alger et al. / Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 98 (2012) 188–196 189

formation of indirect relational memory (Ellenbogen, Hu, Payne, by Ellenbogen, Hulbert, Jiang, and Stickgold (2009) to address the
Titone, & Walker, 2007; Lau, Tucker, & Fishbein, 2010; Preston, Shr- passive/permissive/active conundrum and investigate whether a
ager, Dudukovic, & Gabrieli, 2004). brief nap actively protects a memory trace from a subsequent stim-
Declarative memory has been shown to benefit from non-REM ulus-related interference task, as well as examined long-term con-
(NREM) sleep, specifically slow wave sleep (SWS), compared to solidation persisting for a 1-week retention period. Using a
REM sleep, due to the inherently ideal properties of the stage, bimodal paired-associates task, which pairs visually presented
namely a high sensory threshold for external stimulation, low lev- words with recognizable sounds to create a complex memory, it
els of acetylcholine, and global synchronous brain activity (Gais & was hypothesized that SWS must be obtained in order to actively
Born, 2004; Hasselmo, 1999; Hasselmo & Bower, 1992). Studies protect and consolidate declarative memory, rather than simply
involving the manipulation of slow oscillations (Kirov, Weiss, Sieb- the act of falling asleep and obtaining only Stages 1 and 2 sleep
ner, Born, & Marshall, 2009; Marshall, Helgadottir, Molle, & Born, alone.
2006; Marshall, Molle, Hallschmid, & Born, 2004) or introduction
of contextual learning cues during SWS (Rasch, Büchel, Gais, &
2. Methods
Born, 2007) have shown the direct connection between this sleep
stage and declarative memory consolidation. However, the ques-
2.1. Participants
tion still remains as to how much sleep, and of what type of sleep,
is necessary to benefit declarative memory.
Forty-one participants with an average age of 19.54 years
Daytime naps of 60–90 min have been found to benefit memory
(range 18–28) were recruited from the undergraduate population
similar to overnight sleep studies, without inherent circadian con-
at the City College of the City University of New York via an online
founds or implementing sleep deprivation, providing an economic
voluntary subject pool through which students make appoint-
method to both study sleep and obtain memory facilitation (Alger
ments to participate in projects of their choosing. All subjects were
et al., 2010; Backhaus & Junghanns, 2006; Lau et al., 2010; Mednick
reportedly in good health, free of sleep disorders or drugs that
et al., 2003; Schabus, Hodlmoser, Pecherstorfer, & Klosch, 2005;
might impair or facilitate sleep, as determined by a screening inter-
Tucker et al., 2006). A nap of this length typically contains Stages
view. Participants were required to maintain a regular sleep sche-
1 and 2, SWS, and potentially a period of REM sleep. What is un-
dule for the week prior to each experimental day, with similar bed
known is whether a nap of a shorter duration, not containing
and wake times from day to day, as verified by a subjective sleep
SWS, generally thought to be the predominant stages facilitating
log. In order to ensure similarity between subjects, they were re-
declarative memory, will also result in better memory compared
quired to go to bed no later than midnight and awaken no later
to remaining awake.
than 8am. Participants were also asked to refrain from alcohol or
A 2008 paper by Lahl et al. described the results of two experi-
unnecessary drugs the day prior to as well as the day of the study,
ments using free recall testing of previously viewed word lists after
and caffeine the day of the study. Those who failed to meet these
a retention period containing wakefulness, a short nap, or a long
requirements were excluded prior to beginning the experiment.
nap. They found that an ultra short, 6-min nap benefited memory,
Of the original 41 participants, five subjects were excluded from
although not providing as great a benefit as a longer nap. However,
data analysis due to: inability to fall asleep (one nap participant),
within the nap groups, they found no correlation between perfor-
resulting in extended sleep latency and/or excessively fragmented
mance and total sleep time (TST) or any other sleep parameter.
sleep; inability to remain awake (one wake participant); statisti-
They, therefore, concluded that the onset of sleep potentially initi-
cally outlying poor performance during testing (one participant);
ates a cascade of consolidation continuing even after awakening a
and training performance reflecting a ceiling effect (two partici-
short time later. While the results of this study are intriguing, there
pants). The remaining 36 participants consisted of eight males
are a few questions related to the consequences of consolidation
and 28 females, reflecting the uneven distribution of sexes enrolled
that are not addressed. The process of consolidation is thought to
in psychology courses at the university. All participants signed in-
strengthen a new memory trace as well as stabilize and protect
formed consent. This study was approved by the City College of
it from subsequent interference. While the Lahl et al. study exam-
New York Institutional Review Board.
ined short-term retention after sleep, they did not explore further
than this, details that the present design addresses.
There is an ongoing debate as to whether sleep plays an active 2.2. Tasks
or a passive role in memory processing, or perhaps something in-
between, a permissive role. As elegantly outlined by Ellenbogen, 2.2.1. Bimodal paired-associates task
Payne, and Stickgold (2006), both the passive and permissive the- The bimodal paired-associates task was created as a modified
ories assume that there is nothing inherent in the state of sleep version of the traditional verbal paired-associates task used in
that aids memory processing. Rather, it is the relative reduction many declarative learning studies. The task consisted of 36 two-
of interference during sleep that allows information to consolidate syllable nouns ranked high (>6 on 7 pt. scale) for both concreteness
more efficiently than while awake. Conversely, the active hypoth- and imagery, chosen from the Toronto Word List (Friendly, Frank-
esis ascribes unique properties to the inherent physiological phe- lin, Hoffman, & Rubin, 1982) and randomly paired with 36 2-s
nomena and dynamics of sleep stages, specifically SWS when sound clips of highly concrete, easily recognizable sounds. Sounds
discussing declarative memory, a role in facilitating the stabiliza- were chosen by compiling duplicate categorical suggestions from
tion and strengthening of a memory trace that cannot be achieved 50 polled individuals, arriving at six categories (animal, vehicle,
in other states. The emphasis of the experimental design imple- human sounds, machines/appliances, sounds in the home, nature)
mented in this study addresses the length of nap issue, but, more- with six easily recognizable sound clips per category. All sound
over, attempts to tackle the passive/permissive/active controversy. clips were controlled and matched for volume level and duration.
In the present study, subjects who obtained a brief 10-min nap, Participants were seated in front of a desk mounted 2000 computer
containing only Stages 1 and 2 sleep, were compared to those who monitor approximately 2 feet from the screen. During the learning
either remained awake or achieved a 60-min nap, containing SWS, phase, participants heard/viewed 36 audio/visual (A/V) stimuli via
in order to attempt to replicate the short-term benefit of sleep on a Microsoft PowerPoint presentation. Participants were instructed
declarative memory found in the Lahl et al. study. Additionally, the to pay close attention to each pair and try to associate the sound in
present study implemented a protocol adapted from one proposed the audio clip and the word displayed on the screen together using
190 S.E. Alger et al. / Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 98 (2012) 188–196

whatever strategy worked for them. For each pair, the audio clip (at approximately 1:00 pm), participants were randomly assigned
was presented first, for 2 s, immediately followed by the paired to either lie down for a nap or remain awake engaged in a passive
word, presented in the middle of the screen for 2 s. The A/V pair activity (i.e. watching a nature video). Subjects were divided into
was then presented simultaneously for 2 s, followed by an inter- one of three groups: a wake group, a 10-min nap group, or a 60-
stimulus delay of 3 s with a crosshair fixation point mid-screen. min nap group (Fig. 1). The 10-min nap group was awakened after
The entire 36 pairs were presented once through. Immediately fol- approximately 10 min of physiologically recorded sleep, with tim-
lowing the presentation, all 36 pairs were tested for encoding using ing beginning with the first two consecutive epochs of Stage 1
randomized cued recall, in which the audio clips were presented sleep. The 60-min nap group was given a 60-min sleep opportu-
alone and subjects asked to write the paired visual words into nity, from the time of lights-out until the subject either naturally
the provided space on an answer sheet. Responses were immedi- awakened or was woken from Stages 1 or 2, as determined using
ately scored for accuracy and followed by another run-through of the international criteria of Rechtschaffen and Kales (1968). Sub-
the 36 pairs until participants performed better than a 75% crite- jects were not woken from SWS or REM sleep to reduce sleep iner-
rion level. A criterion level of 75% was set to ensure that subjects tia and the associated disorientation and confusion. After the nap
learned the material well, but still had room for possible sleep period, subjects remained awake and engaged in a passive activity.
facilitated improvement. Hippocampal activation during sleep At 2:30 pm, participants completed the second SSS and were then
has been found to be contingent on acquisition level during train- tested on the first 10 of the learned audio/visual (A1/V1) pairs, Test
ing (Peigneux et al., 2003; Tucker & Fishbein, 2008). No feedback 1, again using cued-recall in which the sound was played and par-
was given during the encoding tests. ticipants recorded the word on an answer sheet. This test mea-
Participants were tested three times on three counterbalanced, sured short-term memory of the learned material.
mutually exclusive subgroups of 30 of the learned A/V pairs (10 Immediately following this testing period, subjects were trained
pairs per test session), with the first and last three pairs removed on the interference task (A2/VInt). After a 10-min delay, during
from the pool to control for primacy and recency effects. Within which subjects sat quietly with no visual or auditory stimulation,
each test, the same 10-pairs were included, but whether the test participants were tested using cued recall, Test 2. The sound clip
was Test 1, 2, or 3 was counterbalanced across subjects. (A2) was presented as the cue and the participants were asked to
recall both the original learned paired word (V2) and the new inter-
2.2.2. Interference task fering paired word (Vint) to avoid competition among responses,
This task was inspired by a task designed by Ellenbogen et al. although the recalled item of interest was the original paired word,
(2009), modified to reflect the bimodal stimuli used in this study. V2. This test was used to measure the stabilization and protection
Participants were trained on a stimulus-related interference task of the memory trace. After completion of this test session, partici-
immediately following the first testing session, to >90% criterion pants had their electrodes removed and were allowed to leave the
level to ensure task encoding. A higher criterion level was used laboratory.
for the interference task because it was not necessary to allow One week after initial participation, participants returned to the
for improvement. This task involved learning new audio/visual laboratory at 11:30 am and completed the final SSS. They were
pairs corresponding with 10 of the untested (A2/V2) pairs to create then tested on the remaining 10 originally learned audio/visual
10 interfering pairs (A2/VInt) with new words paired with previ- pairs (A3, V3), Test 3. This test provided a measure as to how
ously learned sounds. Learning occurred in the same manner as long-lasting the potential benefit of sleep was. Subjects were then
the original pairs. debriefed, credited, and thanked for participation.

2.3. Procedures
3. Results
At least 1 week prior to the experimental day, subjects were
Performance on the bimodal paired-associates task was as-
contacted via email to confirm their intent to participate, informed
sessed after a nap/no-nap interval (Test 1), after a stimulus-related
of prerequisites, and given the first sleep log. On the day of the
interference task (Test 2), and again after a week of retention (Test
study, participants arrived at the Laboratory for Cognitive Neuro-
3). These measures were taken to assess short-term stabilization of
science and Sleep at 11:00 am, signed informed consent, and were
learned material, protection of a memory from interference, and
introduced to the sound and light attenuated bedroom sleep cham-
long-term consolidation after sleep versus no sleep, respectively.
bers in order to facilitate adaptation to the surroundings. A brief
Subjects were trained on the paired-associates task to a minimum
description of the nature of the experiment was given, questions
75% criterion level to ensure subjects (n = 12 per group) encoded
were answered, the week’s sleep logs were collected, and the fol-
the material similarly, before being divided into one of three con-
lowing week’s sleep logs were distributed.
ditions (Wake, 60-min Nap, 10-min Nap). Performance was calcu-
At 11:30am, nine electrodes were applied to all subjects in
lated by dividing the correctly recalled words, which had been
preparation for online standard polysomnograph recordings of
paired to the cued sound clips during learning, by the total number
electroencephalography (EEG; C3-A2, C4-A1), electro-oculography
of possible correct answers to arrive at a percentage correct score
(EOG), and electromyography (EMG) using a five-channel poly-
for each test session (see Table 1 for a summary). Answers that
somnographic montage in digital EEG acquisition software (Gam-
were incorrect, yet semantically related or visually similar to the
ma System-Grass/Telefactor™). In order to reduce experimental
target word (e.g. castle versus palace, figure versus finger), were gi-
confounds, all participants were fitted with electrodes regardless
ven half credit, although this only occurred during early stages of
of nap/no-nap grouping, and subjects were not informed of group
the learning phase and never during testing.
assignment until after the learning phase. All subjects’ brain activ-
ity was monitored online to continuously assess state of sleep/
wake. 3.1. Sleepiness measures
At 12:00 pm each participant was directed to a bedroom for the
remainder of the experimental day. They completed the first of The Stanford Sleepiness Scale uses a numerical scale 1–7 (one
three Stanford Sleepiness Scales (SSS), then sat before a computer being least sleepy, seven most) to rate levels of alertness/sleepi-
monitor and spent approximately 45 min learning the 36 audio/vi- ness. Participants completed three SSS scales, upon arriving at
sual (A/V) pairs to criterion. Immediately after the training session the laboratory immediately before beginning the learning session
S.E. Alger et al. / Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 98 (2012) 188–196 191

Fig. 1. Summary of experimental protocol: Two experimental groups and the wake control group were all trained at 12 pm on the declarative bimodal paired-associates task
to a criterion level of 75%. After the training phase, the nap groups immediately slept, either for a short 10-min nap or a longer 60-min nap, while the control group remained
awake. At 2:30, all groups were tested by cued recall on 10 of the learned pairs (A1V1) and then immediately trained on the interference task. 10 min after training, subjects
were tested by cued recall on both original and interfering pairs (A2V2, Vint). One week after initial learning, subjects were again tested on remaining learned pairs (A3V3).

Table 1 hoc analysis using Least Significant Difference (LSD) showed that
Percentage of correctly recalled bimodal paired-associates during testing session the Wake group performed significantly poorer than the 60-min
across groups (mean ± SEM). Nap group (p = .002), and trended toward significantly poorer per-
Condition Wake 10-min Nap 60-min Nap formance compared to the 10-min Nap group (p = .052).
Learning 77.58 ± 1.58 80.75 ± 1.57 81.33 ± 2.29 In order to measure whether sleep protects a new memory trace
Trials to criterion 2.50 ± .261 2.67 ± .225 2.08 ± .193 from competing interference, we examined the change in perfor-
Test 1a 75.83 ± 3.58 84.17 ± 2.88 90.00 ± 2.13 mance from Tests 1 to 2. The mean percentage scores ± SEM on Test
Interference learning 93.33 ± 2.84 95.42 ± 1.68 96.67 ± 1.42 2 A2/V2 recall were calculated, which measured recall of words orig-
Trials to int. criterionb 2.67 ± .188 2.08 ± .083 1.75 ± 179
inally paired with cued sounds after interference (Wake = 57.08 ±
Test 2 (A2V2)c 57.08 ± 3.51 60.42 ± .234 75.83 ± 2.03
Test 3d 37.50 ± 6.85 37.50 ± 3.05 60.00 ± 2.75 3.51, 10-min Nap = 60.42 ± .234, 60-min Nap = 75.83 ± 2.03). Using
a
repeated-measures ANOVA, with Condition (Wake, 60-min Nap, 10-
One-way ANOVA F2,33 = 5.93, p = .006.
b min Nap) as the between-subject factor and Interference (No Inter-
One-way ANOVA F2,33 = 8.67, p = .001.
c
Repeated-measures ANOVA (Tests 1–2) F2,33 = 3.78, p = .033. ference = Test 1, Interference = Test 2 A2/V2) as the within-subject
d
Repeated-measures ANOVA (Tests 1–3) F2,33 = 3.78, p = .033. factor, a highly significant main effect of Interference was found
(F1,33 = 176.31, p < .001) indicating that, in all groups, the related
interference task compromised the paired-associates memory
at approximately 12:00 pm, before the first test session occurring trace. A significant interaction between Interference and Condition
after the nap/no-nap interval at approximately 2:30 pm, and again was found (F2,33 = 3.78, p = .033). Post-hoc analyses using LSD re-
1 week later before the final test session at approximately vealed that the 60-min Nap group performed significantly better,
11:30 am. Group means ± SEM for each measure were, respec- with interference disrupting the memory trace far less than both
tively: Wake = 2.75 ± .131, 3.25 ± .179, 1.75 ± .179; 10-min the Wake and 10-min Nap groups (p < .001, p = .006, respectively)
Nap = 3.33 ± .284, 3.33 ± .432, 2.08 ± .229; 60-min Nap = 2.83 ± (Fig. 2). Using independent t-tests comparing the groups on the
.271, 2.67 ± .333, 1.92 ± .313. There were no group differences change in performance from Tests 1 to 2, we found that the 60-
using these subjective ratings using One-way ANOVA at any ses- min and 10-min nap groups significantly differ (t = 2.85, p = .009),
sion (SSS1 F2,33 = 1.75, p = .190; SSS2 F2,33 = 1.20, p = .314; SSS3 while all other comparisons were not significant. This again demon-
F2,33 = .457, p = .637). strates the decline in performance in the 10-min nap group after the
interference manipulation. All subjects learned the interfering pairs
similarly (Wake = 93.33% ± 2.84, 10-min Nap = 95.42% ± 1.68, 60-
3.2. Bimodal paired-associates memory min Nap = 96.67% ± 1.42; One-way ANOVA F2,33 = .658, p = .524).
However, the Wake group required significantly more trials to learn
All subjects were trained to a minimum criterion level of 75% the material to criterion (Wake = 2.67 ± .188, 10-min Nap = 2.08 ±
while those who scored higher than 95% during learning were ex- .083, 60-min Nap = 1.75 ± 179; One-Way ANOVA F2,33 = 8.67,
cluded to avoid a ceiling effect. Mean percent correct during the p = .001).
learning session for each group ± SEM were: Wake = 77.58 ± 1.58, In order to measure the effects of sleep on long-term consolida-
10-min Nap = 80.75 ± 1.57, 60-min Nap = 81.33 ± 2.29. One-way tion of the paired-associates, the change in performance from Tests
ANOVA indicated the groups encoded the information similarly 1 to 3 was examined. Test 3 performance, mean recall percent-
(F2,33 = 1.06, p = .358). The number of trials necessary to reach cri- age ± SEM, for each group was: Wake = 37.50 ± 6.85, 10-min
terion were also calculated for each group. Means ± SEM for each Nap = 37.50 ± 3.05, 60-min Nap = 60.00 ± 2.75. Using repeated
group were: Wake = 2.50 ± .261, 10-min Nap = 2.67 ± .225, 60- measures ANOVA with Condition (Wake, 10-min Nap, 60-min
min Nap = 2.08 ± .193. One-Way ANOVA showed that the groups Nap) as the between-subject factor and Time (Test 1, Test 3) as
were not significantly different (F2,33 = 1.74, p = .192). the within-subject factor, a highly significant main effect of Time
Test 1, the impact of sleep on the short-term retention of the was revealed (F1,33 = 239.68, p < .001). This finding indicates that
new memory trace, was examined. Mean recall percentage ± SEM all groups’ performance deteriorated over the week-long retention
for each group was: Wake = 75.83 ± 3.58, 10-min Nap = 84.17 ± period regardless of the subjects having slept or remained awake
2.88, 60-min Nap = 90.00 ± 2.13. One-Way ANOVA revealed a sig- after learning. A significant interaction between Condition and
nificant difference between groups (F2,33 = 5.93, p = .006). Post- Time was found (F2,33 = 3.78, p = .033), and post-hoc analysis using
192 S.E. Alger et al. / Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 98 (2012) 188–196

Table 2
Summary of sleep parameters across nap groups (mean ± SEM, in minutes).

Condition 10-min Nap 60-min Nap


TST 10.75 ± .305 52.50 ± 2.66
Latency 6.21 ± .667 7.67 ± 1.33
Stage 2 8.21 ± .570 28.08 ± 2.37
Stage 3 0 10.38 ± 1.70
Stage 4 0 3.25 ± 1.48
SWS 0 13.63 ± 2.47
REM 0 4.00 ± 1.54

difference was found between the 60-min and 10-min nap groups
(t = 4.43, p < .001), again demonstrating the degraded performance
in the 10-min nap group over the week long retention period. No
other significant differences were found.

3.3. Sleep data


Fig. 2. Performance on the Bimodal Paired-Associates Task without (Test 1) and
with (Test 2) Interference: The y-axis represents the percentage of correctly recalled
PSG recordings were scored for the 10-min and 60-min nap
words paired with cued sounds during testing. The x-axis represents performance
for the two nap groups and Wake control groups during Test 1, after the nap/no nap groups according to Rechtschaffen and Kales standard scoring
retention interval and before training on the interference task, and Test 2, which criteria. Total sleep time (TST), sleep latency, Stages 2, 3, and 4,
measured post-interference recall of originally paired words. Performance at Test 1 slow wave sleep (stages 3 + 4), and REM sleep were calculated
was significantly different between groups (p = .006), with the Wake group for each condition. Mean ± SEM in minutes for the 10-min Nap
performing significantly worse. There was a significant interaction between
group for each parameter was: TST = 10.75 ± .305, latency =
Condition and Interference, with change in performance as a result of stimulus
related interference significantly different between groups (p = .033). Post-hoc 6.21 ± .667, S2 = 8.21 ± .570, and 0 min for remaining parameters.
analysis revealed that the 60-min Nap group performed better than both the 10- Mean ± SEM in minutes for the 60-min Nap group for each param-
min Nap (p = .006) and Wake (p < .001) groups. eter was: TST = 52.50 ± 2.66, latency = 7.67 ± 1.33, S2 = 28.08 ±
2.37, S3 = 10.38 ± 1.70, S4 = 3.25 ± 1.48, SWS = 13.63 ± 2.47,
REM = 4.00 ± 1.54 (see Table 2 for a summary). While the two sleep
LSD revealed that the 60-min Nap group retained the learned
groups were not comparable on all measures, an independent t-
material from Tests 1 to 3 better than both the Wake and 10-min
test comparing the sleep latencies of the two groups found them
Nap groups (p < .001, p = .004, respectively) (Fig. 3). Independent
to be similar (t = .979, p = .338).
t-tests were performed to compare each group to the others, using
Correlation and regression analyses were conducted to deter-
change in performance from Tests 1 to 3, and a highly significant
mine if any particular stage of sleep could predict or explain per-
formance in the two sleep groups. However, no significant
statistics were found for any parameter after analyses. Addition-
ally, Independent t-tests were used to compare the subgroups of
the 60-min nap group, those who obtained REM sleep (n = 5) and
those who did not (n = 7), on all performance measures (Learning
and trials to criterion, Test 1, Interference training and trials to cri-
terion, Test 2, Test 3, change in performance from Tests 1 to 2, and
change in performance from Tests 1 to 3) as well as comparable
sleep parameters. We found no significant differences between
these subgroups on any measure, although a trend toward signifi-
cance in the total sleep time (p = .076).

4. Discussion

The present study investigated the stabilizing and protecting ef-


fects of a period of sleep containing SWS, compared to a shorter
nap reaching only Stage 2 or a period spent awake, on newly
encoded bimodal pairs of sounds and written words. Similar to
results found by Lahl, Wispel, Willigens, and Pietrowsky (2008),
who used an ultra-short 6-min nap, a short-term benefit to mem-
Fig. 3. Performance on the Bimodal Paired-Associates Task from Tests 1 to 3 ory in both nap groups was revealed, although not as great in the
Reflecting Long-Term Consolidation: The y-axis represents the percentage of 10-min nap group as the 60-min nap group. In their study, Lahl
correctly recalled words paired with cued sounds during testing. The x-axis
et al. attributed the retention of memory within the short nap
represents performance for the two nap groups and Wake control groups during
Test 1, after the nap/no nap retention interval, and Test 3, 1 week after initial group to a possible cascade of consolidating processes begun
learning. Performance at Test 1 was significantly different between groups merely with the onset of sleep. While this possible explanation
(p = .006), with the Wake group performing significantly worse. There was a gives an active role to sleep, the results could very well fit with
significant interaction between Condition and Time, with change in performance the passive theory of the role of sleep in memory processing, in
over a 1-week retention interval significantly different between groups (p = .033).
Performance at Test 3 was significantly different between groups, with the 60-min
that it was simply the time spent ‘‘off-line’’, without the ability
Nap group performing better than both the 10-min Nap (p = .002) and Wake for outside interference to disrupt the memory trace, which
(p = .002) groups. allowed better retention. It also fits within the permissive theory
S.E. Alger et al. / Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 98 (2012) 188–196 193

framework, that the off-line period prevented interference and also group either did not spend enough time off-line, in general, or in
benefited retention as it occurred within a critical window of time. Stage 2 sleep, specifically, to see protective and long-term benefits.
In order to clarify these results and delve further into the role of However, our results are consistent with a large body of literature
sleep (passive/permissive/active) in memory processing, two addi- supporting active processes acting on declarative memory during
tional components were added to the present study. The first of sleep. The subjects in the 60-min nap group all obtained a period
which was to incorporate the stimulus-related interference task. of SWS, the prime stage thought to be implicated in hippocam-
As suggested by Ellenbogen et al. (2009), introducing this particu- pal-dependent memory processing, as well as approximately
lar interference task, which required subjects to learn to pair com- 20 min, on average, more Stage 2 sleep. There are two theoretical
peting words with previously paired sounds and then test them on frameworks from which the majority of sleep and declarative
their recall of both the old and newly paired words, tapped into the memory research is derived that give a starring role to SWS and
stabilizing and protective aspect of active consolidation processes. could potentially explain our results: the standard theory of con-
Subjects were tested first on their pure recall of the learned mate- solidation (Buzsaki, 1989; Buzsaki, Leung, & Vanderwolf, 1983;
rial after the sleep/no sleep retention interval (Test 1) and then McClelland, McNaughton, & O’Reilly, 1995) and the Synaptic
immediately trained and tested on the interference task (Test 2). Homeostasis Hypothesis (Tononi & Cirelli, 2003; Tononi & Cirelli,
If sleep simply serves in a passive or permissive capacity, allowing 2006).
consolidation of the learned material to occur more efficiently than The two-stage standard consolidation theory essentially states
it would while awake due to relative lack of interference, both re- that declarative information is first rapidly encoded into the hippo-
lated and environmental, then it may be reasonable to expect that campus through the sensory cortices during wake. During the sub-
all the subjects who slept should either perform just as well on the sequent sleep period, particularly during SWS, sharp wave ripples,
related-interference test as on the first recall test, all protected or fast hippocampal neural oscillations (150–250 Hz), grouped by,
from subsequent interference, or at least be similarly impaired, and occurring in the transition between, the slow oscillations of
proportional to the amount of time they spent ‘‘off-line’’. However, SWS delta waves (.5–2 Hz) (Battaglia, Sutherland, & McNaughton,
it was found that, while all groups were impaired by the interfer- 2004; Sirota, Csicsvari, Buhl, & Buzsáki, 2003), participate in the
ence task, the 60-min nap group’s memory was relatively pre- reactivation of neuronal networks that were most recently fired
served compared to the 10-min nap group, whose performance during waking representing declarative memory (Hoffman &
declined to nearly as poor as the wake group. In this framework McNaughton, 2002; Ji & Wilson, 2007; Kudrimoti et al., 1999; Louie
of a proportional benefit model, this finding suggests that the & Wilson, 2001; Maquet et al., 2000; Nadasdy et al., 1999; Pavlides
short-term benefit found in the 10-min nap group after the & Winson, 1989; Skaggs & McNaughton, 1996; Wilson & McNaugh-
sleep/wake retention was possibly just that, short-term and not ton, 1994). The theorized result of this reactivation is a perpetua-
reflective of true consolidation, which requires SWS found in the tion of long-term potentiation along the network through the
longer nap. Alternatively, it is also possible that a threshold of time activation of the NMDA receptors and an influx of calcium which
spent off-line may be required to passively benefit memory, but activates calcium-dependent kinases during SWS, followed by
that we did not achieve this threshold in the current study. How- expression of immediate early genes (IEGs), such as zif-268 and
ever, in support of an active role for sleep, a 2002 study by Mednick c-fos, during subsequent REM sleep (Aton, Seibt, & Frank, 2009;
et al. similarly found that napping for 60 min in the middle of a ser- Cheng, Williams, & Meck, 2009; Ribeiro, Goval, Mello, & Pavlides,
ies of testing sessions on a visual texture discrimination task, com- 1999; Stickgold, 2005; Stickgold & Walker, 2005; Tononi & Cirelli,
pared to an equal amount of time spent quietly resting blindfolded 2001). Gene expression and protein synthesis is necessary for
to prevent stimuli-related interference (i.e. ‘‘off-line), was required strengthening the synaptic connections through structural modifi-
to reverse performance deterioration caused by the saturation of cations of pre- and post-synaptic cells, such as dendritic growth
specific neural networks over time. In fact, they discovered that and increases in post-synaptic receptors and proteins (McGaugh,
subjects experienced augmentation of SWS and REM sleep, with 2000). On a small scale and with a faster time course of minutes
increases after training compared to baseline levels, at the expense to hours, synaptic consolidation occurs, which leads to the stabil-
of Stage 2 sleep, concluding that active processes during SWS are ization of the memory trace within the hippocampus through Heb-
primarily implicated in their results. In their study, wake subjects bian connections, wherein the repeated co-activation of neurons
continued to deteriorate, while those who obtained a 30-min nap within a specific neural circuit causes a strengthening between
were able to maintain performance levels in subsequent testing synapses through expression and upregulation of immediate early
session, rather than show further decline, demonstrating a small genes (Aton et al., 2009; Saha & Datta, 2005; Ulloor & Datta, 2005),
benefit of a few minutes of SWS (Mednick et al., 2002). increasing the odds of the same neurons firing together in the fu-
The second component added to this study not present in the ture (Hebb, 1949). In theory, reactivation during SWS leads to
Lahl et al. study was an exploration of the persistence of the mem- LTP, triggering IEG expression and protein synthesis during REM
ory trace over time. Subjects returned to the lab after a week-long sleep (Cheng et al., 2009). On a larger scale and over a longer period
retention period and those who had obtained a 60-min nap re- of time, days to weeks, systems consolidation occurs, which in-
tained significantly more learned bimodal pairs than the 10-min volves large-scale neuronal synchrony, best achieved during the
nap and wake groups, who performed virtually the same as one an- slow oscillations of slow wave sleep, effectively allowing reactiva-
other. The benefits seen in the 10-min nap group after the initial tion of the memory trace and communication between the hippo-
sleep/no sleep interval were nonexistent after a week, indicating campus and the neocortex leading to a gradual shift in the burden
that simply spending a very short period of time ‘‘off-line’’ is not of future activation from the hippocampus to the neocortex and an
sufficient for long-term consolidation of the learned material. integration of the new information into existing stores (Behrens,
Again, if sleep served in a passive or permissive role, rather than van den Boom, de Hoz, Friedman, & Heinemann, 2005; Diba & Buz-
actively contributing to the processing of the new information, saki, 2007; Sirota et al., 2003). This process is also NMDA receptor-
we might expect to see benefits proportional to the amount of time dependent and involves expression of IEGs, such as zif-268 along
spent sleeping persist over time in both nap groups. the hippocampo-cortical circuit, upregulated during REM sleep
The results of the present study strongly lend support to an ac- (Aton et al., 2009; Ribeiro & Nicolelis, 2004).
tive role for sleep in the consolidation of declarative memory, The second, although not mutually exclusive, theory is the Syn-
although we cannot exclude the possibility that perhaps a thresh- aptic Homeostasis Hypothesis, which gives an indirect role to SWS
old model might be applied and that subjects in the 10-min nap in memory processing. Through everyday activities, and more so
194 S.E. Alger et al. / Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 98 (2012) 188–196

with focused learning, synapses are continuously potentiated to mance on memory tests (Schabus et al., 2004). Spindles have been
the eventual point of saturation, at which point the synaptic re- temporally correlated with sharp-wave ripples in animals (Siapas
sources are depleted and synaptic firing thresholds are raised to & Wilson, 1998; Sirota et al., 2003) as well as slow oscillations
such a point that new information cannot be easily acquired. Tied (Steriade & Timofeev, 2003), and are thought to represent memory
to this is an increasing homeostatic need for regulation of SWS, so processing. While we did not collect baseline sleep data in the
that as the need increases, more SWS is experienced at sleep onset. present study, future iterations will include this to examine possi-
SWS is thought to downscale, or decrease, synaptic weights, by ble augmentations to sleep parameters, such as increases in spin-
way of the synchronous hyper- and depolarization of the cells dur- dling, or, perhaps, reduction of Stage 2 sleep in compensation for
ing the slow oscillations eventually returning the membrane po- more SWS, as seen in Mednick et al. (2002).
tential to baseline levels and making the cells once again plastic. While the present study was able to replicate, in the 10-min nap
Memory benefits from this process as downscaling essentially in- group, similar benefits found with an ultra short 6-min nap (Lahl
creases the signal-to-noise ratio, with the synapses representing et al., 2008), our results indicate that neither Stage 2 sleep process-
the memory beginning more heavily weighted and remaining ing alone nor remaining briefly off-line, or even simply falling
above the firing threshold, while the background ‘‘noise’’ of every- asleep to begin consolidation processes, appears to be sufficient
day experiences falls below threshold. for protection from interference and long-term retention. It re-
Both the standard consolidation theory and the homeostasis mains unclear, then, as to what caused this temporary preservation
theory can be used to potentially explain the present results using of the memory for the paired-associates in the 10-min nap group.
an ‘‘active role’’ framework. In those who obtained SWS, the mem- While the self-reported sleepiness scales reflect that all groups
ory trace had actively begun the consolidation process and/or the were similarly alert, it may be that these measures did not pick
potentiated synapses had been downscaled, increasing the signal up on other differences in mood that may have the potential to
to noise ratio and allowing the memory to stand out, an advantage confound the results, although this is unlikely. Perhaps with a short
that persisted for at least a week compared to those who did not nap, subjects felt more relaxed and were better able to focus on the
reach SWS. While our study was a behavioral one, we can specu- task occurring shortly after awakening. While it may have been
late as to the physiological occurrences leading to our results. On beneficial to perhaps use a vigilance task or other subjective
the first day of the study, those who obtained SWS benefited from reporting, it does not diminish the findings regarding the long-
sleep-dependent reactivation of the memory and experienced a de- term and protective aspects of attaining more NREM sleep, but
gree of synaptic consolidation which stabilized and protected the importantly SWS. Had the 10-min nap subjects performed better
memory trace. Because only five subjects obtained REM sleep, due to a more relaxed, focused state, this should have carried over
our data does not fully support the literature demonstrating the to the interference task, which followed immediately after this ini-
importance of IEG expression during REM sleep in synaptic consol- tial test. It is more likely that there is some truth to passive benefits
idation. When comparing the two subgroups in the 60-min nap of sleep, in that the lack of interference during the initial retention
group, those who obtained REM (n = 5) to those who only obtained period allowed subjects to better hold the memory in short-term
NREM (n = 7), we found no performance advantage for those who stores for recall compared to the Wake subjects, who experienced
obtained REM sleep (on average 9.60 min of REM). Therefore, we more environmental interference, but did little to store and stabi-
cannot speak to any possible role for REM sleep or a benefit from lize the memory beyond this, either due to insufficient time spent
a SWS-REM sleep succession in processing the new memories, as off-line or in Stage 2 sleep, or because active processing was re-
discussed earlier. However, we speculate that possibly the second quired in later sleep stages.
messenger cascade begun during reactivation in SWS carries Interestingly, the participants who remained awake required
through regardless of whether REM sleep was achieved. While significantly more trials to learn the interference task to criterion,
the interference task impaired memory for all subjects, there was compared to both sleep groups. Because subjects reported similar
a marked difference in the impairment of those who had actively levels of sleepiness on the SSS, this difference cannot be attributed
processed the information versus those who briefly slept or re- to fatigue or lack of interest. It may be the consequence of higher
mained awake. In the interim between the first day of testing synaptic thresholds in the Wake group compared to both sleep
and the final, 1 week later, systems consolidation would have oc- groups, but significantly different from the 60-min nap group,
curred in all groups, with all subjects having similar sleep sched- who experienced synaptic downscaling during SWS, allowing for
ules during the retention week. However, the stronger synaptic greater encoding efficiency.
connections of the subjects in the 60-min nap group, having been A correlation anticipated but not found was one between per-
synaptically consolidated on the first study day, put them at an formance and amount of SWS or TST within the 60-min nap group.
advantage. Although the memory degraded in all groups over time, One could hypothesize that as more SWS was achieved, subjects
it was significantly more preserved after having been actively pro- would perform better during the testing sessions. However, previ-
cessed by SWS during the nap. When considering the homeostasis ous studies, including Lahl et al. (Tucker et al., 2006), also failed to
theory, SWS served to replenish and reset the synaptic properties, find a significant correlation between performance and TST or
allowing new information to be encoded, and perhaps protecting amount of SWS within sleep conditions. In a recent study con-
the original memory by ‘‘freeing up’’ synaptic real estate and ducted in our laboratory (Alger et al., 2010), it was found that
avoiding competition of synaptic resources. the longer the subjects remained awake between the learning ses-
While SWS appears to be essential for our results, we consid- sion and sleep onset (explored up to 4 h post-learning), the better
ered the possibility that Stage 2 sleep may have also contributed. they performed from test to retest on a recognition memory test.
Studies have demonstrated functional changes in Stage 2 sleep Additionally, the longer the subjects were awake between learning
parameters that implicate this stage in declarative memory pro- and napping, the more SWS was seen in the nap. The conclusion,
cessing (Clemens et al., 2005; Gais et al., 2002; Schabus et al., based on the mechanistic theories involving a role for SWS in
2004). Subjects’ baseline sleep spindle density, measured as the declarative information processing, was that the significantly great
number of 12–16 Hz bursts of activity within Stage 2 sleep per amount of SWS present in the later nap group was possibly respon-
30s epoch, increases after a period of intensive learning compared sible for better performance. The amount of Stage 2 sleep was com-
to non-learning activity. A correlation has been found, in previous parable between nap groups and could not account for
literature, demonstrating that only those showing increased spin- performance differences. However, in that study, due to the high
dle density during post-learning sleep show an increase in perfor- variability between subjects for both amount of SWS and change
S.E. Alger et al. / Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 98 (2012) 188–196 195

in performance, no significant correlation was found between Hoffman, K. L., & McNaughton, B. L. (2002). Coordinated reactivation of distributed
memory traces in the primate neocortex. Science, 297(5589), 2070–2073. http://
these variables. It is possible that this also explains the inability
dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1073538.
to find a correlation between SWS and performance in the present Huber, R. (2007). Memory formation: Sleep enough before learning. Current Biology,
study. 17(10), 367–368.
It is inconclusive whether the amount of Stage 2 and SWS Ji, D., & Wilson, M. A. (2007). Coordinated memory replay in the visual cortex and
hippocampus during sleep. Nature Neuroscience, 10(1), 100–107.
achieved in a nap is a factor deciding how well a person performs Kali, S., & Dayan, P. (2004). Off-line replay maintains declarative memories in a
on a declarative task, or whether the act of entering the state of model of hippocampal-neocortical interactions. Nature Neuroscience, 7(3),
SWS begins consolidation or downscaling of the synapses that car- 286–294.
Kirov, R., Weiss, C., Siebner, H. R., Born, J., & Marshall, L. (2009). Slow oscillation
ries through beyond the end of the stage. Either way, it is a likely electrical brain stimulation during waking promotes EEG theta activity and
explanation, supported both from the results of this study as well memory encoding. PNAS, 106(36), 15460–15465.
as a large body of literature, that NREM sleep including SWS is nec- Kudrimoti, H. S., Barnes, C. A., & McNaughton, B. L. (1999). Reactivation of
hippocampal cell assemblies: Effects of behavioral state, experience, and EEG
essary for the brain to actively facilitate declarative memory. dynamics. Journal of Neuroscience, 19, 4090–4101.
Lahl, O., Wispel, C., Willigens, B., & Pietrowsky, R. (2008). An ultra short episode of
sleep is sufficient to promote declarative memory performance. Journal of Sleep
References Research, 17, 3–10.
Lau, H., Tucker, M. A., & Fishbein, W. (2010). Daytime napping: Effects on human
direct associative and relational memory. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory,
Alger, S. E., Lau, H., & Fishbein, W. (2010). Delayed onset of a daytime nap facilitates
93(4), 554–560.
retention of declarative memory. PLoS ONE, 5(8), e12131. http://dx.doi.org/
Louie, K., & Wilson, M. A. (2001). Temporally structured replay of awake
10.1371/journal.pone.0012131.
hippocampal ensemble activity during rapid eye movement sleep. Neuron,
Aton, S.J., Seibt, J., & Frank, M.G. (2009). Sleep and Memory. In: Encyclopedia of Life
29(1), 145–156.
Sciences (ELS). John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.: Chichester. doi: 10.1002/
Maquet, P., Laureys, S., Peigneux, P., Fuchs, S., Petiau, C., Phillips, C., et al. (2000).
9780470015902.a0021395.
Experience-dependent changes in cerebral activation during human REM sleep.
Backhaus, J., & Junghanns, K. (2006). Daytime naps improve procedural memory.
Nature Neuroscience, 3, 831–836.
Sleep Medicine, 7(6), 508–512.
Marshall, L., Helgadottir, H., Molle, M., & Born, J. (2006). Boosting slow oscillations
Battaglia, F. P., Sutherland, G. R., & McNaughton, B. L. (2004). Hippocampal sharp
during sleep potentiates memory. Nature, 444, 610–613.
wave bursts coincide with neocortical ‘‘up-state’’ transitions. Learning &
Marshall, L., Molle, M., Hallschmid, M., & Born, J. (2004). Transcranial direct current
Memory, 11(6), 697–704.
stimulation during sleep improves declarative memory. The Journal of
Behrens, C. J., van den Boom, L. P., de Hoz, L., Friedman, A., & Heinemann, U. (2005).
Neuroscience, 24(44), 9985–9992.
Induction of sharp-wave ripple complexes in vitro and reorganization of
McClelland, J. L., McNaughton, B. L., & O’Reilly, R. C. (1995). Why there are
hippocampal networks. Nature Neuroscience, 8(11), 1560–1567.
complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: Insights
Born, J., Rasch, B., & Gais, S. (2006). Sleep to remember. Neuroscientist, 12(5),
from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and
410–424.
memory. Psychological Review, 102(3), 419–457.
Buzsaki, G. (1989). Two-stage model of memory trace formation: A role for ‘‘noisy‘‘
McGaugh, J. L. (2000). Memory – A century of consolidation. Science, 287(5451),
brain states. Neuroscience, 31, 551–570.
248–251. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.287.5451.248.
Buzsaki, G., Leung, L. W., & Vanderwolf, C. H. (1983). Cellular bases of hippocampal
Mednick, S. C., Nakayama, K., Cantero, J. L., Atienza, M., Levin, A. A., Pathak, N., et al.
EEG in the behaving rat. Brain Research, 287(2), 139–171.
(2002). The restorative effect of naps on perceptual deterioration. Nature
Cheng, R.-K., Williams, C. L., & Meck, W. H. (2009). Neurophysiological mechanisms
Neuroscience, 5(7), 677–681.
of sleep-dependent memory consolidation and its facilitation by prenatal
Mednick, S., Nakayama, K., & Stickgold, R. (2003). Sleep-dependent learning: a nap
choline supplementation. Chinese Journal of Physiology, 52(4), 223–235. http://
is as good as a night. Nature Neuroscience, 6(7), 697–698.
dx.doi.org/10.4077/CJP.2009.AMH085.
Nadasdy, Z., Hirase, H., Czurko, A., Csicvari, J., & Buzsaki, G. (1999). Replay and time
Clemens, Z., Fabo, D., & Halasz, P. (2005). Overnight verbal memory retention
compression of recurring spike sequences in the hippocampus. Journal of
correlates with the number of sleep spindles. Neuroscience, 132, 529–535.
Neuroscience, 19(21), 9497–9507.
De Koninck, J., Lorrain, D., Christ, G., Proulx, G., & Coulombe, D. (1989). Intensive
Nishida, M., Pearsall, J., Buckner, R. L., & Walker, M. P. (2009). REM sleep, prefrontal
language learning and increases in rapid eye movement sleep: evidence of a
theta, and the consolidation of human emotional memory. Cerebral Cortex, 19,
performance factor. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 8, 43–47.
1158–1166.
Diba, K., & Buzsaki, G. (2007). Forward and reverse hippocampal place-cell
Pare, D., Collins, D. R., & Pelletier, J. G. (2002). Amygdala oscillations and the
sequences during ripples. Nature Neuroscience, 10(10), 1241–1242.
consolidation of emotional memories. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6,
Ellenbogen, J. M., Hu, P. T., Payne, J. D., Titone, D., & Walker, M. P. (2007). Human
306–314.
relational memory requires time and sleep. Proceedings of the National Academic
Pavlides, C., & Winson, J. (1989). Influences of hippocampal place cell firing in the
Sciences USA, 104(18), 7723–7728.
awake state of the activity of these cells during subsequent sleep episodes.
Ellenbogen, J. M., Hulbert, J. C., Jiang, Y., & Stickgold, R. (2009). The sleeping brain’s
Journal of Neuroscience, 9, 2907–2918.
influence on verbal memory: Boosting resistance to interference. PLoS ONE, 4(1),
Peigneux, P., Laureys, S., Fuchs, S., Destrebecqz, A., Collette, F., Delbeuck, X., et al.
e4117. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0004117.
(2003). Learned material content and acquisition level modulate cerebral
Ellenbogen, J. M., Payne, J. D., & Stickgold, R. (2006). The role of sleep in declarative
reactivation during posttraining rapid-eye-movements sleep. Neuroimage,
memory consolidation: passive, permissive, active or none? Current Opinion in
20(1), 125–134.
Neurobiology, 16, 716–722.
Plihal, W., & Born, J. (1997). Effects of early and late nocturnal sleep on declarative
Fishbein, W., Kastaniotis, C., & Chattman, D. (1974). Paradoxical sleep: Prolonged
and procedural memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 534–547.
augmentation following learning. Brain Research, 79, 61–75.
Preston, A. R., Shrager, Y., Dudukovic, N. M., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2004). Hippocampal
Fishbein, W., McGaugh, J. L., & Swarz, J. R. (1971). Retrograde amnesia:
contribution to the novel use of relational information in declarative memory.
Electroconvulsive shock effects after termination of rapid eye movement
Hippocampus, 14(2), 148–152.
sleep deprivation. Science, 172, 80–82.
Rasch, B., Büchel, C., Gais, S., & Born, J. (2007). Odor cues during slow-wave sleep
Fogel, S. M., & Smith, C. T. (2006). Learning-dependent changes in sleep spindles and
prompt declarative memory consolidation. Science, 315(5817), 1426–1429.
Stage 2 sleep. Journal of Sleep Research, 15(3), 250–255.
Rechtschaffen, A., & Kales, A. (Eds.). (1968). A manual of standardized terminology,
Friendly, M., Franklin, P. E., Hoffman, D., & Rubin, D. C. (1982). The Toronto word
techniques and scoring system for sleep stages in human subjects. Los Angeles:
pool: Norms for imagery, concreteness, orthographic variables, and
Brain Information Service/Brain Research Institute, University of California.
grammatical usage for 1,080 words. Behavior Research Methods &
Ribeiro, S., Goval, V., Mello, C. V., & Pavlides, C. (1999). Brain gene expression during
Instrumentation, 14(4), 375–399.
REM sleep depends on prior waking experience. Learning & Memory, 6(5),
Gais, S., & Born, J. (2004). Low acetylcholine during slow-wave sleep is critical for
500–508.
declarative memory consolidation. Proceedings of the National Academic Sciences
Ribeiro, S., & Nicolelis, M. A. L. (2004). Reverberation, storage, and postsynaptic
USA, 101(7), 2140–2144.
propagation of memories during sleep. Learning & Memory, 11, 686–696. http://
Gais, S., Köster, S., Sprenger, A., Bethke, J., Heide, W., & Kimmig, H. (2008). Sleep is
dx.doi.org/10.1101/lm.75604.
required for improving reaction times after training on a procedural visuo-
Saha, S., & Datta, S. (2005). Two-way active avoidance training specific increases in
motor task. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 90(4), 610–615.
phosphorylated cAMP response element-binding protein in the dorsal
Gais, S., Mölle, M., Helms, K., & Born, J. (2002). Learning-dependent increases in
hippocampus, amygdala, and hypothalamus. European Journal of Neuroscience,
sleep spindle density. Journal of Neuroscience, 22(15), 6830–6834.
21, 3403–3414.
Hasselmo, M. E. (1999). Neuromodulation: Acetylcholine and memory
Schabus, M., Gruber, G., Parapatics, S., Sauter, C., Klösch, G., Anderer, P., et al. (2004).
consolidation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3(9), 351–359.
Sleep spindles and their significance for declarative memory consolidation.
Hasselmo, M. E., & Bower, J. M. (1992). Cholinergic suppression specific to intrinsic
Sleep, 27(8), 1479–1485.
not afferent fiber synapses in rat piriform (olfactory) cortex. Journal of
Schabus, M., Hodlmoser, K., Pecherstorfer, T., & Klosch, G. (2005). Influence of
Neurophysiology, 67, 1222–1229.
midday naps on declarative memory performance and motivation. Somnologie,
Hebb, D. O. (1949). The organization of behavior: A neuropsychological theory. New
9(3), 148–153.
York: John Wiley & Sons.
196 S.E. Alger et al. / Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 98 (2012) 188–196

Siapas, A. G., & Wilson, M. A. (1998). Coordinated interactions between Tononi, G., & Cirelli, C. (2003). Sleep and synaptic homeostasis: a hypothesis. Brain
hippocampal ripples and cortical spindles during slow-wave sleep. Neuron, Research Bulletin, 62, 143–150.
21, 1123–1128. Tononi, G., & Cirelli, C. (2006). Sleep function and synaptic homeostasis. Sleep
Sirota, A., Csicsvari, J., Buhl, D., & Buzsáki, G. (2003). Communication between Medicine Review, 10, 49–62.
neocortex and hippocampus during sleep in rodents. Proceedings of the National Tucker, M. A., & Fishbein, W. (2008). Enhancement of declarative memory
Academic Sciences USA, 100(4), 2065–2069. performance following a daytime nap is contingent on strength of initial task
Skaggs, W. E., & McNaughton, B. L. (1996). Replay of neuronal firing sequences in rat acquisition. Sleep, 31(2), 197–203.
hippocampus during sleep following spatial experience. Science, 271, Tucker, M. A., Hirota, Y., Wamsley, E. J., Lau, H., Chaklader, A., & Fishbein, W. (2006).
1870–1873. A daytime nap containing solely non-REM sleep enhances declarative but not
Smith, C. T. (2001). Sleep states and memory processes in humans: procedural procedural memory. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 86(2), 241–247.
versus declarative memory systems. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 5(6), 491–506. Ulloor, J., & Datta, S. (2005). Spatio-temporal activation of cyclic AMP response
Smith, C.T., Nixon, M.R., & Nader, R.S. (2004). Posttraining increases in REM sleep element -binding protein, activity-regulated cytoskeletal-associated protein
intensity implicate REM sleep in memory processing and provide a biological and brain derived nerve growth factor: A mechanism for pontine-wave
marker of learning potential. Learn. Mem.; 2004 11, 714–719. generator activation-dependent two-way active-avoidance memory
Smith, C., Young, J., & Young, W. (1980). Prolonged increases in paradoxical sleep processing in the rat. Journal of Neurochemistry, 95, 418–428.
during and after avoidance-task acquisition. Sleep, 3, 67–81. Wagner, U., Gais, S., & Born, J. (2001). Emotional memory formation is enhanced
Steriade, M., & Timofeev, I. (2003). Neuronal Plasticity in Thalamocortical Networks across sleep intervals with high amounts of rapid eye movement sleep. Learning
during Sleep and Waking Oscillations. Neuron, 37(4), 563–576. and Memory, 8, 112–119.
Stickgold, R. (2005). Sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Nature, 437(27), Wagner, U., Gais, S., Haider, H., Verleger, R., & Born, J. (2004). Sleep inspires insight.
1272–1278. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nature04286. Nature, 427(6972), 352–355.
Stickgold, R., & Walker, M. P. (2005). Memory consolidation and reconsolidation: Walker, M., & Stickgold, R. (2004). Sleep-dependent learning and memory
What is the role of sleep? Trends in Neuroscience, 28(8), 408–415. consolidation. Neuron, 44(1), 121–133.
Tononi, G., & Cirelli, C. (2001). Modulation of brain gene expression during sleep and Wilson, M. A., & McNaughton, B. L. (1994). Reactivation of hippocampal ensemble
wakefulness: A review of recent findings. Neuropsychopharmacology, 25(5), memories during sleep. Science, 265(5172), 676–679.
28–35.

You might also like