You are on page 1of 14

Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2019, 11–24

doi:10.1093/deafed/eny028
Advance Access publication September 12, 2018
Empirical Manuscript

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jdsde/article/24/1/11/5095366 by Instituto Federal da Paraíba (IFPB) user on 04 July 2021
EMPIRICAL MANUSCRIPT

Culture and Deaf Women’s Body Image


Aileen Aldalur* and Deborah Schooler
Gallaudet University
*Correspondence should be sent to Aileen Aldalur, Department of Psychology, Gallaudet University, 5356 Stevely Ave., Lakewood, CA 90713, USA
(e-mail: aileen.aldalur@gallaudet.edu)

Abstract
The sociocultural model of eating disorders highlights the role of internalization of the thin ideal in the development of
negative body image and disordered eating. Considering the limited diversity of models in mainstream media, individuals
belonging to minoritized cultural groups may resist identifying with and internalizing mainstream beauty messages.
The extent of internalization may also depend on an individual’s acculturation status and experiences of acculturative
stress. This study applied the sociocultural model to culturally Deaf women. Data were collected from 96 deaf, female,
undergraduate students. Results support the sociocultural model; higher rates of internalization were associated with
negative body image and disordered eating behaviors. However, neither Deaf nor Hearing acculturation predicted
internalization. Despite this finding, stronger acculturation with either Deaf or Hearing culture predicted more positive body
image, and higher rates of acculturative stress predicted negative body image and disordered eating behaviors. Implications
for clinical practice are discussed.

Body image concerns are found among girls and women across Thus, factors including an individual’s acculturation status and
the lifespan (Dohnt & Tiggeman, 2005; Runfola et al., 2013) and their levels of acculturative stress are important to consider
are cause for concern due to the association between body dis- when examining body image among culturally minoritized
satisfaction (BD) and psychological disturbances (Mond et al., women (Gordon et al., 2010; Schooler, 2008). Researchers are
2013; Rosenstrom et al., 2013). Collectively, research on body taking a keen interest in furthering investigations of body image
image primarily focuses on White women and girls; however, among various cultural groups to identify the impact of these
varying rates of negative body image and disordered eating be- factors, individually and collectively, on body image. This may
haviors are reported across ethnic, racial, and other diverse cul- aid in understanding, preventing, and treating body image con-
tural groups (Greenwood & Dal Cin, 2012; Mussap, 2009; Nouri, cerns and related disorders among various cultural populations.
Hill, & Orrell-Valente, 2011; Schooler, 2008). Research has identi- Deaf individuals represent one such cultural group. Many
fied several unique variables associated with body image among deaf individuals consider themselves as part of a separate cul-
culturally diverse individuals that may serve as either protec- ture with its own language, belief systems, values, standards,
tive or risk factors depending on the unique circumstances. For and community (Andrews, Leigh, & Weiner, 2004). The Deaf
instance, people of color may reject dominant sociocultural in- community contains schools, clubs, organizations, traditions
fluences and internalization of the Western thin ideal, provid- and customs, all of which depict a Deaf way of life separate
ing some buffering against negative body image (Austin & from the Hearing world or the English language (Edwards, 2005;
Smith, 2008; Mussap, 2009; Rogers Wood & Petrie, 2010; Holcomb, Holcomb, & Holcomb, 2011). Not all deaf individuals
Schooler, Ward, Merriweather, & Caruthers, 2004). On the other share this experience of deafness, however, and differences
hand, cultural identity confusion and the stress associated with exist in the extent to which deaf individuals adopt the cultural
acculturation may have negative impacts on body image and norms and identity inherent in Deaf culture and the extent to
eating behaviors among culturally minoritized women (Gordon, which they participate in the Deaf community (Maxwell-McCaw &
Castro, Sitnikov, & Holm-Denoma, 2010; Menon & Harter, 2012). Zea, 2011).

Received December 22, 2017; revisions received July 25, 2018; editorial decision July 26, 2018; accepted August 22, 2018
© The Author(s) 2018. Published by Oxford University Press. All rights reserved. For Permissions, please email: journals.permissions@oup.com.

11
12 | Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2019, Vol. 24, No. 1

Few studies have examined deaf women and body image, status plays a role in determining which set of cultural values
even though an initial investigation concluded that negative are most salient to a given individual. Standards of beauty and
body image and disordered eating behaviors were a serious con- body shape ideals differ among various cultural groups (Gordon
cern for deaf women (Hills, Rappold, & Rendon, 1991). The cur- et al., 2010; Smart & Tsong, 2014) and cultural groups differ in
rent study seeks to examine the relationships between deaf the amount of emphasis given to the body or individual accom-
women’s acculturation status, levels of acculturative stress, plishments (Austin & Smith, 2008; Hesse-Biber, Livingstone,
internalization of the Western thin ideal, body image, and disor- Ramirez, Barko, & Johnson, 2010). The sociocultural model of BD
dered eating behaviors to further research on how cultural dif- and eating disorders presumes that internalization of cultural
ferences may interact with dominant sociocultural influences messages regarding body shape plays a causal role in the devel-

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jdsde/article/24/1/11/5095366 by Instituto Federal da Paraíba (IFPB) user on 04 July 2021
and impact the body image of culturally minoritized women. opment of negative body image and disordered eating behaviors
(Thompson & Stice, 2001). Individuals that prescribe to a differ-
ent set of cultural norms may differ in the extent to which they
Deaf Acculturation
internalize, or personally subscribe to, the Western thin ideal
Acculturation is a process involving a sequence of psychological (Austin & Smith, 2008). For instance, findings from research sug-
and behavioral adaptations which occur as individuals negotiate gest that Black women largely reject comparison with White
the adoption of various strategies that will allow a successful characters in the media, and their body image is generally unre-
adaptation to a multicultural environment (Berry, Phinney, Sam, lated to mainstream media usage which primarily portrays
& Vedder, 2006; Schwartz & Zamboanga, 2008). Many scholars in White models (Greenwood & Dal Cin, 2012; Schooler et al., 2004).
cultural psychology use a bidirectional model to represent the Results from the two studies conducted on body image and
process of acculturation as individuals navigate between the disordered eating among deaf women are decidedly mixed
dominant culture of a society and various minoritized cultures. regarding the benefits of Deaf acculturation. Dewalt (1998)
This bidirectional model includes adoption of the dominant explored eating-disordered attitudes and behaviors among a
culture on one end and adoption of a minoritized culture on sample of eighty–nine 12–17-year-old, deaf, adolescent females
the other end (Berry et al., 2006; Schwartz & Zamboanga, 2008). attending a Deaf residential school. All the girls included in the
Parallels may be drawn between the cultural experiences of sample were involved in Deaf culture to some extent. The girls
deaf individuals and those of ethnic, racial, and other culturally were administered the Eating Disorders Inventory (EDI) and in-
minoritized groups in the United States. For example, culturally terviewed about their feelings regarding their bodies, personal
minoritized individuals in the United States experience dual strengths and weaknesses, membership in the Deaf commu-
and conflicting pressures from the dominant society to assimi- nity, and Deaf cultural knowledge. Results from this study indi-
late into American culture and fully adopt the language, values, cated that overall the deaf girls reported less eating disorder
customs, and behaviors of the U.S. people and from their minor- attitudes than a normed sample of similarly-aged hearing girls.
itized cultural communities to avoid fully assimilating and Further analyses confirmed that of the deaf girls included in the
retain their unique cultural background (Castillo, Zahn, & Cano, sample, those that were closely affiliated with Deaf culture re-
2012; Rodriguez, Myers, Mira, Flores, & Garcia-Hernandez, 2002; ported less eating-disordered attitudes than those who were
Thompson, Lightfoot, Castillo, & Hurst, 2010). These pressures less affiliated. The author suggested that Deaf culture instills
parallel those experienced by deaf individuals regarding self-esteem in deaf girls which in turn helps prevent the devel-
whether they should correct their hearing, claim English as opment of eating-disordered attitudes and behaviors. This is in
their primary language, and adopt the values, behaviors, and line with findings from research among Muslim women that
mannerisms of mainstream Hearing culture or whether they demonstrated that self-esteem mediated the positive relation-
should use American Sign Language (ASL) and be active within ship between acculturation to Muslim culture and positive body
the Deaf community (Edwards, 2005; Leigh & Lewis, 2010; image (Mussap, 2009), as well as findings from research among
Mitchiner, 2015). Considering the similarities in the processes of Black women that indicated that those who scored higher in
cultural adaptation, Maxwell-McCaw and Zea (2011) developed ethnic identity and self-esteem had significantly lower at-risk
a scale that could reliably assess acculturation patterns among eating-disordered scores (Rhea & Thatcher, 2013).
deaf individuals. The researchers note that like any other min- Despite the significant findings for the protective benefit of
oritized cultural group within the larger society, all deaf indivi- affiliation with Deaf culture, it is important to note that Dewalt
duals must, to varying extents, interact with the hearing (1998) used a unidirectional measure for affiliation by assessing
majority around them (Maxwell-McCaw & Zea, 2011). Through knowledge of Deaf culture and participation in the Deaf com-
their interactions within both the Hearing and Deaf worlds, deaf munity. The study did not account for the degree to which the
individuals develop a unique acculturation strategy in which deaf adolescents were affiliated with Hearing culture. Building
they have the option of acculturating into Hearing society, on this study, Moradi and Rottenstein (2007) examined the rela-
acculturating into Deaf culture, or adopting aspects of both cul- tionships between internalization of the thin ideal, body sur-
tures. Based on the various acculturative experiences of deaf in- veillance, body shame, and eating disorder symptoms among a
dividuals in the United States, Maxwell-McCaw and Zea (2011) sample of deaf women aged 17–73. This study measured an
identified four Deaf acculturation strategies. Similar to Berry’s important aspect of acculturation, Deaf identity, using the
(1997) model of acculturation, deaf individuals may be classified revised Deaf Identity Development Scale (DIDS; Fischer &
as Deaf Acculturated, primarily identifying with and adhering McWhirter, 2001), which measures both identification with Deaf
to Deaf cultural values; Hearing Acculturated, primarily identi- culture and identification with Hearing culture. Results from
fying with and adhering to Hearing cultural values; Bicultural, this study supported the generalizability of objectification the-
identifying with and adhering to both Deaf and Hearing cultural ory (Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997) to deaf women as positive re-
values; or Marginal, feeling disconnected from both Deaf and lations between internalization of the thin ideal, body
Hearing cultures. surveillance, body shame, and eating disorder symptoms were
Acculturation is an important factor to consider when con- found. In terms of Deaf identity, Deaf, Hearing, and Bicultural
ducting research on body image as an individual’s acculturation identities were not uniquely associated with the body image or
A. Aldalur and D. Schooler | 13

eating disorder variables, but feeling disconnected from both to alleviate the psychological distress associated with accultura-
Deaf and Hearing cultures was indirectly or directly associated tive stress and fit in with the dominant society by attainment of
with greater internalization, body shame, body surveillance, the Western thin ideal (Gordon et al., 2010).
and eating disorder symptoms. This is in line with research on Lastly, examining the associations between acculturative
the various Deaf acculturation statuses that has found that feel- stress, negative body image, and disordered eating through
ing disconnected from both Deaf and Hearing cultures is associ- uses and gratification theory allows the integration of an addi-
ated with greater negative mental health outcomes than tional factor, namely that of mass media. According to uses and
maintaining a Deaf, Hearing, or Bicultural acculturation status gratification theory, individuals use mass media in a goal-
(Andrews et al., 2004). Deaf individuals that do not strongly directed manner such that they seek out media that will provide

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jdsde/article/24/1/11/5095366 by Instituto Federal da Paraíba (IFPB) user on 04 July 2021
identify with either culture or actively participate in either com- gratification by satisfying their psychological and social needs
munity are likely to experience more pressures from people (Johnson, 1996; Yang, Wu, Zhu, Brian, & Southwell., 2004). The
within both communities to acquire the “appropriate” language, process of acculturation often involves feelings of unfamiliarity
values, behavioral roles, and customs. Moradi and Rottenstein and uncertainty as individuals are exposed to various cultural
(2007) suggest that rather than the specific acculturation strat- perspectives and negotiate their identities within differing cul-
egy chosen by deaf women, it may be the extent to which they tural contexts. Furthermore, as mentioned above, individuals
experience conflict due to their acculturation status, combined often experience acculturative stress as they are pressured to
with the internalization of the thin ideal, that may be the corre- learn specific languages or adopt specific behavioral norms to
late of body image and disordered eating behaviors. Previous fit-in. As Yang et al. (2004) explain, one possible avenue for
research with ethnic, racial, and other minoritized cultural reducing these feelings of uncertainty is to turn to mass media
groups obtained similar results such that among Black and to acquire knowledge and familiarity of a specific culture. In
Latina women, greater feelings of acculturative stress, rather fact, research demonstrates that individuals with strong desires
than a particular acculturation status, was predictive of more to acculturate into the dominant culture or learn English, have a
negative body image and disordered eating behaviors (Gordon greater tendency to select English-based or mainstream media,
et al., 2010). and they perceive this media as a good source of information
about the dominant culture, and an avenue to adjust to the
dominant society and improve their English (Tudsri & Hebbani,
Acculturative Stress
2015; Yang et al., 2004). Furthermore, evidence from research
Acculturative stress represents the effects of the struggles suggests that people often use television in times of stress and
involved in acculturation, including pressures from community that using mass media may assist people in altering prevailing
members to retain aspects of the minoritized culture as well as mood states (Johnson, 1996; Shao, 2009). Lack of social support
pressures from the larger society to acquire aspects of the domi- is associated with acculturative stress among ethnically and
nant culture (Rodriguez et al., 2002; Schwartz & Zamboanga, racially minoritized individuals (Hovey, 2000); thus, an individ-
2008). Similar to other diverse cultural groups living in North ual who is experiencing acculturative stress and struggling to
America, many deaf individuals experience competing pres- fit-in with others in their environment may turn to mass media
sures from the Hearing society and the Deaf community regard- to satisfy their internal need for relationships and a sense of
ing which language, behavioral roles, values, and customs they connection. The recurring nature of television series allows in-
should adopt (Leigh & Lewis, 2010; Mitchiner, 2015). dividuals to develop a sense of intimacy with specific charac-
As mentioned above, some evidence suggests that rather ters, similar to real-life acquaintances (Te’eni-Harari & Eyal,
than an individual’s specific acculturation status, it may be the 2015). This connection creates a perceived sense of similarity,
amount of stress they feel due to the pressures of acculturation affinity, and identification with the characters. These characters
that is associated with negative body image and disordered eat- then become common targets for comparisons on physical at-
ing behaviors (Gordon et al., 2010; Moradi & Rottenstein, 2007). tributes and have a greater potential to impact viewer’s lives
Several theories provide explanations about the relationship (Te’eni-Harari & Eyal, 2015). Results from research suggest that
between acculturative stress and negative body image. First, re- identification with characters in the media and making compar-
searchers suggest that the strain associated with perceived isons to those characters is associated with negative body
pressures to acculturate may place an individual in a height- image (Bell & Dittmar, 2011; Te’eni-Harari & Eyal, 2015).
ened state of sensitivity and vulnerability rendering them more Therefore, acculturative stress may be a key factor in the links
likely to accept sociocultural pressures to conform and ulti- between media exposure, internalization, and negative body
mately internalize the thin ideal (Menon & Harter, 2012). A image.
cross-sectional study conducted among a sample of 399 male
and female Hispanic University students in West Texas found
Media Use and Internalization
that acculturative stress predicted greater internalization of the
thin ideal (Menon & Harter, 2012). Furthermore, acculturative Media in Western societies uniformly idealizes and promotes
stress was associated with negative body image and this rela- an image of female beauty that is extremely thin, well below
tionship was mediated by internalization of the thin ideal the average weight of women in society (Bell & Dittmar, 2011;
(Menon & Harter, 2012). Fouts & Burgaff, 2000; Tucci & Peters, 2008). As mentioned
Second, acculturative stress may lead to maladaptive coping above, the sociocultural model of BD and eating disorders high-
behaviors (Gowen, Hayward, Killen, Robinson, & Taylor, 1999). lights the role of internalization of the thin ideal in the develop-
Evidence from research suggests that coping with the pressures ment of negative body image and disordered eating behaviors
of acculturation and experiencing higher levels of associated (Thompson & Stice, 2001), and mainstream media is a major
stress is linked to bulimic behaviors and a drive for thinness vehicle through which the cultural message of the thin ideal is
(DFT) among Black and Latina women, respectively (Gordon distributed. Research examining the relationship between
et al., 2010). The researchers suggest that these behaviors may media influences and negative body image among women and
function as escape behaviors or maladaptive coping strategies girls indicates that higher rates of exposure to mass media are
14 | Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2019, Vol. 24, No. 1

associated with greater BD, DFT, and disordered eating beha- buffer against internalization of the thin ideal, we predicted
viors (Ashikali, Dittmar, & Ayers, 2014; Bell & Dittmar, 2011; that Deaf acculturation would negatively predict internaliza-
Dohnt & Tiggeman, 2006; Grabe, Ward, & Hyde, 2008; Groesz, tion. As for Hearing acculturation, previous research among
Levine, & Murnen, 2002). Internalization of the thin ideal is diverse ethnic, racial, and other cultural groups has primarily
believed to be one of the processes through which exposure to utilized unidirectional measures of acculturation (Austin &
mainstream media asserts its influence on body image and dis- Smith, 2008; Schooler et al., 2004); thus, the hypothesis that
ordered eating (Fauquet, Sánchez-carracedo, Levine, & López- stronger acculturation with the dominant culture is associated
Guimerà, 2010; Jones, Vigfusdottir, & Lee, 2004). In addition, to greater internalization of the Western thin ideal has not been
social comparison theory states that individuals are likely to frequently tested. Under the tenants of social comparison the-

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jdsde/article/24/1/11/5095366 by Instituto Federal da Paraíba (IFPB) user on 04 July 2021
make upward comparisons to similar others that are faring bet- ory, however, it would be expected that Hearing acculturation
ter on the given dimension of comparison (Festinger, 1954). would predict greater internalization of the thin ideal as the
Models in mainstream media provide sources for upward com- deaf women would be more likely to make upward comparisons
parisons in terms of appearance and body shape as they repre- to the hearing women in mainstream media. Thus, we hypothe-
sent the cultural ideal, and appearance-based comparisons sized that Hearing acculturation would positively predict inter-
with characters in mainstream media is a primary correlate of nalization. Also, based on Menon and Harter’s (2012) findings
negative body image (Myers & Crowther, 2009; Pinkasavage, among Hispanic individuals that acculturative stress positively
Arigo, & Schumacher, 2015). predicted internalization of the thin ideal, we predicted that
Representations of women in media are not only unrealisti- acculturative stress would positively predict internalization
cally thin, but they also primarily represent the dominant among the sample of deaf women.
American cultural ideals. According to social comparison the-
ory, the tendency to compare oneself with another person de-
Hypothesis 2
creases as the difference between the two persons increases
(Festinger, 1954). Thus, culturally minoritized women may be Next, we examined the associations between internalization of
less likely to compare themselves to images of women in the the thin ideal and body image and disordered eating. Based on
media who do not represent their cultural identities and values the wealth of previous research documenting the associations
(Greenwood & Dal Cin, 2012; Schooler et al., 2004). Whether an between internalization of the thin ideal and negative body
individual considers the characters in mainstream media as image and disordered eating behaviors among a variety of po-
accurate sources for comparisons depends on their accultura- pulations (Cafri, Yamamiya, Brannick, & Thompson, 2005; Jones
tion status. For instance, research suggests that Latina adoles- et al., 2004; Nouri et al., 2011; Rogers et al., 2010; Vartanian,
cents who are more acculturated into American culture are 2009), we predicted that internalization would positively predict
more likely to turn to mainstream media as a source of cultural negative body image and disordered eating behaviors among
values, norms, and ideals, while those more acculturated with the deaf women.
their Latin culture are less likely to accept these sources as a
guide to their beliefs (Schooler, 2008). In addition, acculturative
Hypothesis 3
stress may render individual’s more likely to accept characters
in the media as accurate sources of comparison due to constant Lastly, we examined whether deaf women’s acculturation sta-
pressures from the dominant society and a lack of fulfilling tus in both Deaf and Hearing cultures, as well as their levels of
social relationships. Thus, it is important to consider accultura- acculturative stress would be associated with their body image
tion status, acculturative stress, and internalization of sociocul- and eating behaviors. Based on previous findings with diverse
tural values such as the thin ideal, when conducting research cultural populations (Hesse-biber et al., 2010; Mussap, 2009), as
among culturally diverse groups. Research has yet to consider well as findings among deaf female adolescents (Dewalt, 1998)
the relationships between these factors to body image and dis- that acculturation with a different ethnic, racial, or cultural
ordered eating behaviors among deaf women. group is associated with a more positive body image and less
disordered eating behaviors, we predicted that Deaf accultura-
tion would predict a more positive body image and less disor-
The Current Study
dered eating behaviors. As for Hearing acculturation, the one
The primary goal of the current study was to further research previous study that examined the relationship did not find a
examining the body image of culturally diverse women. significant association between maintaining a Hearing identity
Specifically, this study examined the extent to which accultura- and body image or disordered eating behaviors (Moradi &
tion status, acculturative stress, and internalization of the Rottenstein, 2007). As mentioned previously, research among
Western thin ideal predict deaf women’s body image and eating diverse ethnic and racial groups has primarily utilized unidirec-
behaviors. tional measures of acculturation (Lau, Lum, Chronister, &
Forrest, 2006; Rogers et al., 2010; Schooler, 2008); thus, the
hypothesis that stronger acculturation to the dominant culture
Hypothesis 1
is associated with a more negative body image has not been fre-
First, we examined whether deaf women’s acculturation status quently tested; however, one study that assessed acculturation
in both Deaf and Hearing cultures, as well as their levels of among Muslim women bidirectionally found that stronger
acculturative stress, are associated with internalization of the acculturation with the dominant Australian culture was associ-
thin ideal. Under the premises of social comparison theory ated with BD and disordered eating (Mussap, 2009). Considering
(Festinger, 1954) and considering previous research findings these findings, we did not have a specific hypothesis regarding
among Black women (Rogers et al., 2010; Schooler et al., 2004) the association between Hearing acculturation and body image
and Latina women (Austin & Smith, 2008; Schooler, 2008) sug- or disordered eating. In terms of acculturative stress, however,
gesting that acculturation with one’s ethnic or racial culture via considering previous findings of the association between accul-
identification and adoption of important values provides a turative stress and negative body image (Menon & Harter, 2012)
A. Aldalur and D. Schooler | 15

and disordered eating behaviors (Gordon et al., 2010), we pre- approximated a sixth-grade reading level. Although, the re-
dicted that acculturative stress would predict negative body ported median reading level achieved by deaf high school grad-
image and disordered eating behaviors among the sample of uates is approximately the fourth-grade level (Cawthon, 2004;
deaf women. Traxler, 2000), the current sample consisted of college educated
deaf students that are more likely to fall within the top 50th per-
centile of deaf students reading above the fourth-grade level.
Methods
Deaf acculturation status
Procedure
The 58-item DAS (Maxwell-McCaw & Zea, 2011) was used to

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jdsde/article/24/1/11/5095366 by Instituto Federal da Paraíba (IFPB) user on 04 July 2021
This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board at measure levels of acculturation to both Deaf and Hearing cul-
Gallaudet University in Washington, DC. tures. The DAS measures the strength of acculturation to Deaf
Data were collected through an anonymous online survey and Hearing cultures on two separate acculturation scales; the
throughout the fall semester of 2012. Students were recruited DASd (Deaf) and the DASh (Hearing). Items on both accultura-
from undergraduate psychology courses and select courses tion scales (DASd and DASh) measure acculturation across five
from other departments offering extra credit for participation, domains including cultural identification, cultural involvement,
as well as via flyers posted in campus buildings. Participants cultural preferences, cultural knowledge, and language compe-
were told that the purpose of the study was to examine how tence. Responses are rated on a Likert-type scale ranging from 1
deaf individuals think about themselves and mass media and (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Participants were given
were given the URL to access the survey on their own time. The a separate acculturation score (scored as a continuous variable)
data collected from the male participants was retained for for each culture. The acculturation scores were obtained by
future investigations. Upon accessing the survey, participants totaling the average scores of each of the individual subscales
viewed the informed consent form and indicated their agree- within the DASd and DASh and then dividing them by the num-
ment to participate by clicking “Yes” before continuing to the ber of subscales (5). The acculturation scores indicate partici-
survey. Participants then answered questions regarding their pants’ level of acculturation to both Deaf and Hearing cultures
background, Deaf acculturation status, acculturative stress, in which higher numbers indicate higher cultural involvement.
body satisfaction, and eating behaviors. Strong internal reliability for the DAS has been established;
Maxwell-McCaw and Zea (2011) reported Cronbach alphas rang-
Participants ing from 0.88 to 0.92 for the subscales of the DASd with an alpha
of 0.95 for the overall scale, and Cronbach alphas ranging from
A total of 175 survey responses were collected from male and 0.71 to 0.85 for the subscales of the DASh with an alpha of 0.91
female students during the data collection period. The initial for the overall scale. Validity has been established for the DAS
data was screened to eliminate responses from hearing stu- through factor analysis and the demonstration of its ability to
dents (n = 16). After the initial screening, a total of 159 partici- discriminate between groups based on school background,
pants remained. Due to the focus of the current study, however, parental hearing status, and the use of self-labels. For the cur-
only the responses from female participants (n = 96) were rent sample, Cronbach alphas for the subscales of the DASd
included in the data analyses. ranged from 0.75 to 0.93 with an alpha of 0.92 for the overall
Participants consisted of 96 deaf, female, undergraduate scale and the Cronbach alphas for the subscales of the DASh
students, aged 18–54 (M = 21.99) at Gallaudet University. 71% ranged from 0.63 to 0.86 with an alpha of 0.88 for the overall
were White, 10% were Hispanic/Latino, 7% were Black/African scale.
American, and 12% were classified as multiracial. In terms of
hearing loss, ~64% reported a profound hearing loss of Acculturative stress
90–120 dB, 14% a severe loss of 70–89 dB, 14% a moderate loss Levels of acculturative stress were measured by a modified ver-
of 40–69 dB, 1% a mild loss of 10–39 dB, and 8% reported that sion of the 24-item Societal, Attitudinal, Familial, and
they did not know their degree of hearing loss or did not Environmental Acculturative Stress Scale (SAFE; Fuertes, &
respond to the question. The body mass index (BMI) scores Westbrook, 1996). In order to measure acculturative stress spe-
⎡ (Weight in pounds) ⎤ cifically with regard to Hearing and Deaf cultures, several modi-
⎢ × 703⎥ for this sample ranged from 16.3
⎣ (Height in inches)2 ⎦ fications were made to the scale including, word and phrase
(underweight) to 44.9 (obesity; M = 24.62, SD = 4.93). substitutions, removal of two irrelevant items, and the addition
of one item regarding ASL. For example, the word “ethnicity”
was substituted with “hearing status” and references to “cul-
Measures
tural background” were substituted with references to impor-
Several steps were taken to maximize the applicability of the tant values in Deaf culture (educational background) to avoid
English survey measures for use with deaf participants. First, a confusion with ethnic/racial cultures, the item “It bothers me to
professor at Gallaudet University with expertize in Deaf culture, think that so many people use drugs” was deemed irrelevant to
identity, communication, and research, served as a consultant Deaf culture and thus omitted from the scale, and an item
and reviewed the survey. The survey was evaluated in terms of related to the use of ASL “People think I am unsociable when in
its readability and modifications were recommended to fact I have trouble communicating in ASL” was added to parallel
increase the clarity of the survey items. Recommended modifi- the item related to English “People think I am unsociable when
cations included slight changes to diction, format, and syntax of in fact I have trouble communicating in English.” The resultant
the survey items and directions to reduce possible misinterpre- 23 items measured participants’ acculturative stress in social,
tations. Resultant changes are described in the individual de- attitudinal, familial, and environmental contexts, as well as per-
scriptions of measures. In addition, the researchers used ceived discrimination towards one’s identity as a deaf individ-
Microsoft Word to calculate the Flesch–Kincaid grade level score ual. Using a five-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (not
of the survey and the result indicated that the survey stressful) to 5 (extremely stressful), participants rated how
16 | Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2019, Vol. 24, No. 1

stressful certain situations were for them, such as “I often feel 1983) establishing their internal reliability. Validity for the EDI
ignored by people who are supposed to assist me.” The addi- has been constructed through a cross-validation procedure of
tional response of (not applicable) was also an available choice. anorexia patients and a female comparison group, comparison
Those items that were not applicable to subjects were assigned of female self-report and clinician’s ratings, and the establish-
a score of 0. Scores were summed across the items for each par- ment of convergent and discriminative validity (Garner et al.,
ticipant with higher scores indicating higher levels of stress. 1983). The Cronbach alphas for the EDI subscales in this sample
In terms of reliability, the modified SAFE scale demonstrated were DFT 0.91 and BD 0.92.
excellent internal reliability among the sample of deaf women The BSQ measured cognitive aspects of body image such as
with a Cronbach alpha of 0.93, and this is comparable to the reli- concerns about body shape. The scale contains 34 items such

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jdsde/article/24/1/11/5095366 by Instituto Federal da Paraíba (IFPB) user on 04 July 2021
ability of the original scale among a large sample of Asian as, “Has feeling bored made you brood about your shape?”
American (α = 0.91), African American (α = 0.87), and Latina (α = Items are rated on a six-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (never)
0.91) undergraduate college students (Van Diest, Tartakovsky, to 6 (always), and respondents indicated how often each state-
Stachon, Pettit, & Perez, 2014). The validity of the modified SAFE ment had applied to them over the previous four weeks. Scores
scale has been established via factor analysis and demonstra- were summed across the items for each participant, with higher
tion of the linkage between the observed factors and accultura- scores indicating a more negative body image. Previous studies
tion scores on the DAS among the sample of male and female have reported high Cronbach alphas such as .98 for the BSQ
participants from which the current data was obtained (Aldalur, (Schooler et al., 2004), and validity for the BSQ has been demon-
2017). strated through its correlation with the BD subscale of the EDI
and by its ability to discriminate between known groups,
Internalization of the western thin ideal including women who fulfilled the self-report diagnostic criteria
Participants’ internalization of mainstream societal influences for bulimia nervosa classified as “probable cases” and a control
was measured by the Internalization: General subscale of the group of women classified as “definite noncases” (Cooper et al.,
Sociocultural Attitudes Towards Appearance Scale-3 (SATAQ-3; 1987). The Cronbach alpha for the BSQ in the present sample
Thompson, van den Berg, Roehrig, Guarda, & Heinberg, 2004). was 0.98.
The Internalization: General subscale contains nine items, such
as “I compare my body to the bodies of people who are on TV”; Disordered eating behaviors
and respondents indicated how strongly they agreed with each The Bulimia subscale of the EDI (Garner et al., 1983) was used to
item from 1 (definitely disagree) to 5 (definitely agree). For assess the participant’s disordered eating behaviors. The sub-
emphasis, the consultant recommended capitalizing the nega- scale measures the tendency towards episodes of bingeing and
tive words in items in which double negatives were created by towards the impulse to engage in compensatory behaviors. The
disagreeing with the item such as, “It does NOT matter if my subscale contains seven items such as “I eat when I’m upset,”
body looks like the body of people who are on TV.” Appropriate and responses are rated on a Likert scale ranging from 1 (never)
items were reverse scored, and item ratings were summed to 6 (always). Scores were summed across the items for each
across the subscale, with higher scores indicating greater levels participant, with higher scores indicating more disordered eat-
of internalization. ing behaviors. The Bulimia subscale has a reported high
The Internalization: general subscale has established reli- Cronbach alpha of 0.83, establishing its internal reliability
ability, and a reported high Cronbach alpha of 0.92. In regards of (Garner et al., 1983). Evidence for the validity of the Bulimia sub-
validity, construct validity has been established for the sub- scale has been demonstrated through its ability to discriminate
scales of the SATAQ-3 by demonstrating their ability to discrim- between known groups, including between a group of female
inate between known groups of eating-disordered inpatients, controls and a group of women who met the diagnostic criteria
eating disturbed nonpatients and a control group of individuals for bulimia, as well as between groups of patients with bulimic
showing no signs of disturbed eating or body image disturbance type anorexia and those with restrictive type anorexia (Garner
(Thompson et al., 2004). The Cronbach alpha for the subscale in et al., 1983). The Cronbach alpha for the Bulimia subscale in the
the present sample was 0.91. present sample was 0.86.

Body image
Data Analyses
Two subscales of the EDI (Garner, Olmstead, & Polivy, 1983), DFT
and BD, along with the Body Shape Questionnaire (BSQ; Cooper, Missing data were handled via pairwise deletion for each analy-
Taylor, Cooper, & Fairbum, 1987) were used to assess psycholog- sis. First, the researchers examined the partial correlations
ical and cognitive aspects of the participants’ body image. among the main study variables, namely Deaf and Hearing
The DFT subscale measures preoccupation with weight, acculturation, acculturative stress, internalization of the thin
excessive dieting, and entrenchment in the pursuit of thinness. ideal, body image, and disordered eating behaviors. The re-
The subscale contains seven items such as “I feel extremely searchers controlled for age, BMI, and ethnicity as previous
guilty after overeating,” and responses are rated on a Likert studies demonstrated the associations between these variables
scale ranging from 1 (never) to 6 (always). The BD subscale mea- and body image and disordered eating behaviors (e.g., Moradi &
sures the belief that specific parts of the body are disproportion- Rottenstein, 2007; Nouri et al., 2011; Schooler et al., 2004). Next,
ate. The subscale contains nine items such as “I think my a hierarchical regression was run to investigate the first hypoth-
thighs are too large.” Responses for this subscale were rated on esis that Hearing acculturation and acculturative stress would
a scale of 1 (never) to 5 (always) due to a technical error which positively predict internalization of the thin ideal, and that
limited the response options to 1–5 rather than 1–6. For both Deaf acculturation would negatively predict internalization.
subscales, appropriate items were reverse coded and item rat- Internalization was designated the dependent variable, age,
ings were summed across the subscale with higher scores indic- BMI, and ethnicity were included as covariates in the first step,
ating more negative body image. Both subscales have reported and Deaf acculturation, Hearing acculturation, and accultura-
uniformly high Cronbach alphas (DFT: 0.85; BD: 91; Garner et al., tive stress were included as predictors in the second step.
A. Aldalur and D. Schooler | 17

Following this analysis, the researchers ran four hierarchical modified SAFE scale and 0–120 for the original 24-item SAFE
regressions to investigate the second and third sets of hypothe- scale). However, considering the means obtained in previous
ses, which combined, stated that internalization of the thin studies, the levels of acculturative stress in the current sample
ideal, Hearing acculturation, and acculturative stress would pre- are similar to those reported among samples of late immigrant
dict a more negative body image and disordered eating beha- college students (Mena, Padilla, & Maldonado, 1987) or English
viors, while Deaf acculturation would predict a more positive as a Second Language students (Hovey, 2000). The sample’s
body image and less disordered eating behaviors. The three mean score on the Internalization: general subscale is close to
body image variables and the disordered eating variable served the mean score reported by Thompson et al. (2004) for the con-
as the dependent variables in the separate regression analyses. trol group of undergraduate University students and the mean

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jdsde/article/24/1/11/5095366 by Instituto Federal da Paraíba (IFPB) user on 04 July 2021
Age, BMI, and ethnicity were included as covariates in the first reported by the deaf participants in Moradi and Rottenstein’s
step, Deaf acculturation, Hearing acculturation, and accultura- (2007) study. In terms of body image and disordered eating, the
tive stress were included as predictors in the second step, and sample’s mean scores on the DFT and the Bulimia subscales of
internalization was included as a predictor in the third step. the EDI, as well as on the BSQ were close to the mean scores of
The decision to include internalization in the final step of the the female control groups included in the development and val-
regression analyses was made based on previous research find- idation of the respective scales (Cooper et al., 1987; Garner et al.,
ings that have suggested that internalization may mediate the 1983). Scores on the BD subscale, however, cannot be compared
relationship between acculturation and negative body image to scores from previous studies due to the technical error men-
and disordered eating behaviors (Austin & Smith, 2008; Menon tioned above that resulted in a different scoring range as com-
& Harter, 2012; Rogers et al., 2010). Including internalization in pared to previous studies.
the final step of the regression analyses allows for an examina-
tion of the possible mediating role of this variable between the Assumptions
cultural variables (Deaf acculturation, Hearing acculturation,
and acculturative stress) and body image and disordered eating Prior to interpreting each multiple regression analysis, correlation
behaviors. If the data indicated that (1) internalization was matrixes, plots of standardized residuals, and various test statis-
associated with negative body image and disordered eating be- tics were examined to determine whether the assumptions—
haviors among the deaf women, (2) the cultural variables were linearity, homoscedasticity, independent errors, multicollinearity,
associated with both internalization and negative body image and normally distributed errors—were met. Examining the plot of
and disordered eating behaviors, and (3) the associations standardized residuals against standardized predicted values for
between the cultural variables and body image and disordered each model, it was determined that the assumptions of linearity
eating behaviors became nonsignificant after controlling for and homoscedasticity were met as the points were randomly and
internalization, this would suggest that internalization may evenly dispersed throughout the plots and there were no curvilin-
serve as a mediator. If this was found to be the case for any of ear relationships between the variables. The Durbin–Watson test
the associations between the cultural variables, internalization, was used to examine the assumption of independent errors. The
and body image and disordered eating, the researchers planned values for the models ranged from 1.557 to 2.324, remaining close
to conduct further analyses to test directly for the mediating to the desired value of 2, indicating that the assumption of inde-
role of internalization. pendent errors was met. The assumption of multicollinearity was
assessed by visually inspecting the correlation matrixes for highly
correlated variables, as well as interpreting the variance inflation
Results
factor (VIF). None of the predictor variables exhibited correlations
above 0.80 and the average VIF statistic for each model was close
Descriptive Statistics
to the desired value of 1 indicating there was no problem with
The mean scores and partial correlations between the variables multicollinearity among the predictor variables entered in the
of interest are shown in Table 1. The sample’s mean scores on models. Lastly, the assumption of normally distributed errors was
the separate acculturation subscales of the DAS (DASd and assessed by examining the distribution of the standardized resi-
DASh) were comparable to those obtained by Maxwell-McCaw duals. An examination of the histogram and P–P Plots for each
and Zea (2011). The sample’s mean level of acculturative stress model revealed some deviations from normality, but the distribu-
on the modified SAFE scale is not directly comparable to the tions of errors for the models predicting the Internalization:
means reported in previous studies with samples consisting of General, DFT, and BSQ scales did not deviate substantially from
hearing participants due to the modifications made to the scale the normal distribution. The model predicting the BD and Bulimia
and the different possible ranges of scores (0–115 for the subscales, however, deviated from the normal distribution more

Table 1 Summary statistics and partial intercorrelations between variables of Interest with age, BMI, and ethnicity controlled

Measure 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 M SD

1. DASd — 3.90 0.59


2. DASh −0.379*** — 2.92 0.55
3. Modified SAFE −0.214 0.067 — 42.90 20.01
4. Internalization: general 0.051 −0.111 0.117 — 23.43 8.91
5. DFT −0.087 −0.069 0.172 0.626*** — 21.56 8.93
6. BD −0.128 −0.160 0.279* 0.764*** 0.699*** — 24.71 9.39
7. BSQ −0.224 −0.087 0.251* 0.758*** 0.823*** 0.781*** — 86.69 38.60
8. Bulimia 0.000 −0.079 0.249* 0.375*** 0.512*** 0.526*** 0.633*** — 14.25 6.27

Note: *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.


18 | Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2019, Vol. 24, No. 1

substantially with a platykurtic distribution for the BD scale and a Hypothesis 3


leptokurtic and positively skewed distribution for the Bulimia
scale. Attempts to achieve normality through transformation Partial correlations indicated significant, positive associations
were unsuccessful, and accordingly the researchers decided to between acculturative stress and scores on the BD, BSQ, and
proceed with the planned analyses. Although regression analyses Bulimia measures. As can be seen in Tables 4 and 5, when con-
are often robust enough to withstand small violations of assump- sidered together, the cultural variables accounted for significant
tions, the results of these analyses should be interpreted with variance, beyond age, BMI, and ethnicity, in the BD and BSQ
more caution. measures. Deaf acculturation emerged as a significant predictor
of scores on the BD and BSQ only after controlling for internali-
zation, and consistent with the stated hypothesis, stronger Deaf

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jdsde/article/24/1/11/5095366 by Instituto Federal da Paraíba (IFPB) user on 04 July 2021
acculturation was associated with a more positive body image.
Hypothesis 1
Similarly, Hearing acculturation emerged as a significant predic-
As shown in Table 1, partial correlations controlling for age, tor of BD only after controlling for internalization, such that
BMI, and ethnicity, did not indicate any significant relationships stronger Hearing acculturation was associated with a more pos-
between the cultural variables (Deaf/Hearing acculturation and itive body image. Acculturative stress was a significant predictor
acculturative stress) and internalization. As can be seen from of scores on the BD and the Bulimia subscale, such that higher
the results of the regression analysis provided in Table 2, when levels of acculturative stress were predictive of a more negative
considered together, the cultural variables did not account for
significant variance in internalization. Neither Deaf/Hearing Table 3 Hierarchical multiple regression analysis predicting scores
acculturation nor acculturative stress were significant in pre- on the drive for thinness subscale
dicting levels of internalization among the deaf women.
Model 3
Identifying as Black/African American, however, was predictive
of lower levels of internalization among the deaf women
Variable Model 1 B Model 2 B B 95% CI
included in this sample.
Constant 13.84* 23.63 3.15 (−18.59, 24.88)
Age −0.25 −0.26 −0.06 (−0.36, 0.23)
Hypothesis 2 BMI 0.57** 0.52* 0.56** (0.22, 0.90)
Hispanic/Latina −3.82 −4.50 −3.53 (−8.77, 1.70)
Partial correlations (Table 1) indicated significant and positive Black/African −3.42 −3.99 1.43 (−4.96, 7.81)
relations between internalization and scores on the body image American
measures (DFT, BD, and BSQ) and disordered eating measure Multiracial −1.57 −2.49 0.39 (−5.00, 5.78)
(Bulimia). The results of the regression analyses predicting the DASd −1.49 −1.86 (−4.99, 1.28)
three body image measures are presented in Tables 3–5 and the DASh −1.83 −0.80 (−3.96, 2.36)
results of the regression predicting the disordered eating mea- Modified SAFE 0.07 0.03 (−0.05, 0.12)
sure are presented in Table 6. The final model in each analysis Internalization 0.62*** (0.43, 0.81)
was significant, and combined, the predictor variables ac- R2 0.11 0.15 0.48
counted for 41–68% of the body image variables and 20% of the F 1.78 1.51 6.94***
R2 Change 0.04 0.33
disordered eating variable. Consistent with the stated hypothe-
F Change 1.06 43.02***
sis, internalization accounted for significant variance, beyond
all the variables, in scores for all the body image and disordered
Note: *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.
eating behaviors measures, such that greater internalization
was predictive of a more negative body image and more disor-
dered eating behaviors. Table 4 Hierarchical multiple regression analysis predicting scores
on the body dissatisfaction subscale
Table 2 Hierarchical multiple regression analyses predicting deaf
women’s internalization Model 3

Model 2 Variable Model 1 B Model 2 B B 95% CI

Variable Model 1 B B 95% CI Constant 7.57 26.96* 1.95 (−15.20, 19.09)


Age 0.05 0.03 0.27* (0.04, 0.50)
Constant 31.30*** 33.13* (6.92, 59.33) BMI 0.68** 0.59** 0.63*** (0.36, 0.90)
Age −0.29 −0.32 (−0.68, 0.05) Hispanic/Latina −4.69 −5.75 −4.57* (−8.70, −0.44)
BMI −0.01 −0.06 (−0.49, 0.37) Black/African −0.89 −2.05 4.56 (−0.48, 9.60)
Hispanic/Latina −1.32 −1.56 (−0.8.14, 5.02) American
Black/African American −8.39* −8.75* (−16.52, −0.99) Multiracial −0.49 −2.12 1.40 (−2.85, 5.65)
Multiracial −4.932 −4.658 (−11.35, 2.03) DASd −2.57 −3.02* (−5.49, −0.55)
DASd 0.602 (−3.34, 4.54) DASh −3.86 −2.61* (−5.10, −0.11)
DASh −1.65 (−5.61, 2.31) Modified SAFE 0.12* 0.07* (0.01, 0.14)
Modified SAFE 0.058 (−0.01, 0.16) Internalization 0.76*** (0.61, 0.90)
R2 0.11 0.13 R2 0.16 0.27 0.71
F 1.77 1.36 F 2.60* 3.08** 18.32***
R2 Change 0.03 R2 Change 0.11 0.45
F Change 0.71 F Change 3.44* 103.24***

Note: *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001. Note: *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.
A. Aldalur and D. Schooler | 19

Table 5 Hierarchical multiple regression analysis predicting scores internalization of the thin ideal and negative body image and
on the body shape questionnaire scale disordered eating behaviors among various populations
(Fauquet et al., 2010; Jones et al., 2004; Rogers et al., 2010), and
Model 3
results of the current study extend the application of the model
Variable Model 1 B Model 2 B B 95% CI to deaf women. Internalization was consistently associated
with scores on the body image and disordered eating behavior
Constant 24.01 116.68* 13.85 (−60.13, 87.83) measures. This finding is in line with the results from Moradi
Age −0.44 −0.41 0.57 (−0.43, 1.57) and Rottenstein’s (2007) study that found positive relations
BMI 3.10** 2.81** 2.99*** (1.82, 4.15) between internalization of the thin ideal and body shame, body
−24.75 −29.51* −24.66** (−42.47, −6.84)

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jdsde/article/24/1/11/5095366 by Instituto Federal da Paraíba (IFPB) user on 04 July 2021
Hispanic/Latina surveillance, and eating disorder symptomology among a sam-
Black/African −14.67 −19.41 7.77 (−13.98, 29.51) ple of deaf women.
American
In contrast to previous findings among other cultural popu-
Multiracial −2.20 −12.64 1.82 (−16.52, 20.15)
lations, however, acculturation with Deaf culture was not asso-
DASd −16.12 −17.99*** (−28.65, −7.32)
ciated with less internalization of the thin ideal. Previous
DASh −12.87 −7.74 (−18.50, 3.02)
research has suggested that among minoritized ethnic and
Modified SAFE 0.38 0.20 (−0.08, 0.49)
racial groups, stronger acculturation with one’s ethnic or racial
Internalization 3.10*** (2.46, 3.74)
culture may provide a buffer against internalization of the thin
R2 0.177 0.28 0.73
F 2.66* 2.88** 17.08*** ideal presented in mainstream media, as women are likely to
R2 Change 0.10 0.45 reject identification and comparisons with characters in main-
F Change 2.85* 94.25*** stream media that do not reflect their cultural norms (Austin &
Smith, 2008; Greenwood & Dal Cin, 2012; Rogers et al., 2010;
Note: *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001. Schooler et al., 2004; Schooler, 2008). This does not seem to be
the case for Deaf cultural standards, however, as neither Deaf
acculturation nor Hearing acculturation was associated with
Table 6 Hierarchical multiple regression analysis predicting scores
on the Bulimia subscale internalization. Thus, regardless of their acculturation status,
deaf women may be likely to internalize the thin ideal pre-
Model 3 sented in mainstream media and make upward comparisons
with the hearing characters presented in the media.
Variable Model 1 B Model 2 B B 95% CI Although, Deaf acculturation was not associated with less
internalization of the thin ideal, consistent with previous
Constant 7.34 7.62 −0.24 (−18.07, 17.59)
Age −0.19 −0.21 −0.13 (−0.37, 0.11)
research findings (Rogers et al., 2010), identifying as Black/
BMI 0.47** 0.42** 0.43** (0.15, 0.71) African American was significantly associated with less inter-
Hispanic/Latina −1.98 −2.55 −2.18 (−6.47, 2.12) nalization among the deaf women. Deaf individuals come from
Black/African −3.05 −3.79 −1.71 (−6.95, 3.53) a variety of diverse backgrounds and this creates a unique inter-
American sectionality of cultural identities. Deaf individuals belonging to
Multiracial −0.73 −0.64 0.46 (−3.96, 4.88) minoritized ethnic or racial groups simultaneously experience
DASd 0.23 0.09 (−2.48, 2.66) multiple acculturation processes, and their status in each can
DASh −0.95 −0.56 (−3.15, 2.03) impact how they relate to mainstream media. Among deaf
Modified SAFE 0.08* 0.06 (−0.00, 0.13) women, it appears that in terms of internalizing beauty mes-
Internalization 0.24** (0.08, 0.39) sages from mainstream media, ethnic and racial identity may
R2 0.14 0.20 0.30 supersede Deaf acculturation. Previous research has not investi-
F 2.27 2.12* 3.17** gated beauty standards or body shape ideals within Deaf culture
R2 Change 0.06 0.10 and thus, it is unknown whether a cohesive standard of beauty
F Change 1.75 9.44** exists within the Deaf community. It may be the case that the
appearances, beauty standards, and body shape ideals of deaf
Note: *p< .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.
individuals vary, reflecting the diverse standards of various eth-
nic and racial cultures rather than a single cohesive Deaf stan-
body image and more disordered eating behaviors. After con- dard of beauty. Or, it may be the case that despite the existence
trolling for internalization, however, acculturative stress re- of beauty standards within Deaf culture, the intersectionality of
mained a significant predictor for scores on the BD subscale, but identities creates unique experiences for deaf women of color.
became nonsignificant for the Bulimia subscale [B = 0.06, p = Research indicates that deaf people of color face unique chal-
0.064]. lenges within a predominately White Deaf community (Foster &
Kinuthia, 2003; Parasnis, Samar, & Fischer, 2005; Stapleton,
2015) and these experiences in combination with their cultural
Discussion
experiences in minoritized ethnic or racial groups likely impacts
This study examined the relationships between Deaf accultura- their acceptance of Deaf cultural values and standards.
tion, acculturative stress, internalization of the Western thin In line with the hypothesis that ethnic or racial identity may
ideal and body image and disordered eating behaviors among a supersede Deaf acculturation in terms of internalizing the thin
sample of deaf women. In addition, the study examined ideal, reported levels of Deaf acculturative stress were not associ-
whether Deaf acculturation and acculturative stress are associ- ated with internalization. Results from previous research suggest
ated with internalization of the Western thin ideal. The results the strain associated with perceived pressures to acculturate
of this study provide further support for the sociocultural model may place an individual in a heightened state of sensitivity and
of body image and eating disorders. A wealth of research has vulnerability rendering them more likely to accept sociocultural
documented a strong and consistent relationship between pressures to conform and ultimately internalize the thin ideal
20 | Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2019, Vol. 24, No. 1

(Menon & Harter, 2012). This finding does not appear to extend to have noted that aligning with the larger Hearing society or
acculturative stress arising from the Deaf acculturation process. aligning with Deaf culture does not have differential effects on
Again, this finding may be due to the fact that Deaf culture may psychosocial outcomes such as self-esteem (Leigh, Maxwell-
not necessarily have a unified set of beauty standards to serve as McCaw, Bat-Chava, & Christiansen, 2009). Within the larger
a key cultural difference or that beauty standards from ethnic or Hearing society, deafness is seen as a disability and adopting
racial cultures may predominate in terms of beauty ideals. this perspective may have negative effects on a deaf person’s
Therefore, when seeking to reduce stress or alleviate pressures self-esteem. Within Deaf culture, however, the medical per-
from the larger society to conform to Hearing cultural standards, spective of deafness as a disability is lost and many Deaf indivi-
rather than focusing on the physical attributes of media char- duals do not consider themselves as disabled. Instead, they

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jdsde/article/24/1/11/5095366 by Instituto Federal da Paraíba (IFPB) user on 04 July 2021
acters to alleviate their feelings of acculturative stress, Deaf simply consider themselves as belonging to a separate culture.
women may focus on characteristics that are sources of key Thus, acculturation with Deaf culture may facilitate a positive
cultural differences between Hearing and Deaf cultures such view of oneself as a deaf individual and thereby increase a deaf
as the characters’ language use, social, educational, and individual’s self-esteem. Despite the fact that deafness is con-
employment experiences, interests, and hobbies. sidered a disability within a Hearing society, a deaf individual
Although the cultural variables were not significantly related may successfully navigate their disability by mastering various
to internalization of the thin ideal, Deaf acculturation, Hearing strategies to overcome barriers and successfully integrate them-
acculturation, and acculturative stress all demonstrated associa- selves within the Hearing world, and these individuals are likely
tions with either the body image or disordered eating behaviors to be those who maintain strong acculturation to Hearing cul-
variables. Specifically, both Deaf and Hearing acculturation were ture. Thus, it may be said that successfully navigating the pro-
predictive of a more positive body image among the deaf women cess of acculturation and finding a comfortable place within the
such that they were associated with greater body satisfaction and Deaf and Hearing worlds may benefit the self-esteem of deaf in-
holding fewer negative thoughts about one’s body. Notably, the dividuals. Previous research has established that high self-
associations between Deaf and Hearing acculturation and the esteem is associated with a more positive body image (Grogan,
body image variables were only significant after controlling for 2010). Therefore, a strong Deaf acculturation status may assist
internalization. This result suggests that Deaf and Hearing accul- deaf women in maintaining a positive view of themselves as
turation may have a small and direct influence on the body image deaf individuals and a higher self-esteem which may in turn
of deaf women and this influence is only visible after accounting protect them against the development of negative body image
for the large influence of internalization. Furthermore, this sug- and disordered eating behaviors.
gests that stronger acculturation does not promote a more posi- In line with this explanation, higher levels of acculturative
tive body image by reducing internalization of the thin ideal, but stress were predictive of more negative body image and disor-
instead maintaining a strong cultural identity may provide direct dered eating behaviors among the deaf women. Previous
protective benefits to deaf women despite internalization of the research has noted that rather than a specific acculturation sta-
thin ideal. The finding that both Deaf and Hearing acculturation tus, it is the amount of stress that an individual feels in relation
were associated with a more positive body image is in line with to the acculturation process that is predictive of negative body
the results reported by Moradi and Rottenstein (2007) that cultural image and disordered eating (Gordon et al., 2010). This fits with
identification, regardless of whether it was with Deaf or Hearing the current pattern of results such that those with weaker
culture was beneficial, and feeling disconnected was associated to acculturation to either Deaf or Hearing culture and higher levels
several eating disorder constructs. Thus, this pattern of results of acculturative stress were more likely to report a negative
suggests that for deaf women, rather than a specific acculturation body image and more disordered eating behaviors. The path
status, maintaining strong acculturation to either Deaf or Hearing through which acculturative stress may influence body image
culture can be beneficial in terms of body image and eating beha- and disordered eating, whether it be through greater exposure
viors. This contrasts with previous results among Muslim women to mainstream media and comparison with popular media
which found that only acculturation to Muslim culture was associ- characters for the purposes of cultural learning, greater vulnera-
ated with more positive body image, while acculturation to the bility to sociocultural pressures and higher rates of internaliza-
dominant Australian culture was associated with more negative tion, or the development of maladaptive coping behaviors, is
body image and disordered eating behaviors (Mussap, 2009). unclear. The significance of the association between accultura-
Again, the unique intersectionality among deaf women may pro- tive stress and disordered eating behaviors disappeared after
vide an explanation for this contradiction. For the Muslim women controlling for internalization which suggests that internaliza-
in Mussap’s (2009) study, acculturation with the dominant tion may serve as a mediator of the relationship between accul-
Australian culture likely implied changes in beauty standards and turative stress and disordered eating. However, the results also
values among the Muslim women to fit the dominant ideal; how- suggest that acculturative stress may have some direct influence
ever, for deaf women, acculturation with Hearing culture, although on body image as the association between BD and acculturative
a dominant culture in terms of Deaf acculturation statuses, does stress remained significant after controlling for internalization.
not mean that one is acculturated with the dominant ethnic or Overall, this pattern of findings suggests that internalization
racial group. Thus, a deaf woman who is Hearing acculturated of the Western thin ideal, Deaf acculturation status, and levels
may also be predominately acculturated into a Latin culture rather of acculturative stress are important factors when considering
than American culture, and the woman’s ethnic or racial accultur- body image and disordered eating among deaf women. The
ation may be more influential in determining to which set of findings from this study may inform clinical practice with deaf
beauty ideals she adheres. women. For instance, because internalization is central to the
Rather than operating through body shape ideals, the influ- body image of deaf women, strategies that focus on the evalua-
ence of Deaf acculturation status may operate through self- tion and critique of the female body standards presented in
esteem. While a few studies have found Deaf and Bicultural mainstream media and help women adopt healthier and more
identities to be associated with higher self-esteem among deaf realistic perspectives on beauty and self-evaluation are likely to
individuals (Bat-Chava, 2000; Jambor & Elliott, 2005), others be as beneficial with deaf women as they are with other groups.
A. Aldalur and D. Schooler | 21

However, because Deaf acculturation status and acculturative may still have been the preferred language for many partici-
stress are also important correlates of body image and disor- pants. Indeed, some individuals in the population of interest
dered eating behaviors, it is critical that these factors are may have chosen not to take the survey because it was only
included in clinical practice with deaf women. Therefore, facili- provided in English, thus creating a response bias. Providing
tating open discussions of Deaf identity, culture, and feelings of future surveys in both ASL and English would be more accessi-
marginalization, as well as adopting healthy strategies for ble and culturally affirming to a diverse population of deaf
reducing levels of acculturative stress may be critical in the individuals.
treatment of body image issues and disordered eating among A possible avenue for future research could be to examine
deaf women. the interaction of Deaf acculturation status and ethnic/racial

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jdsde/article/24/1/11/5095366 by Instituto Federal da Paraíba (IFPB) user on 04 July 2021
acculturation as they relate to the body image and disordered
eating behaviors among a culturally diverse population of
Limitations and Directions for Future Research
women. In addition, although no standards of female beauty or
The current study addresses an important gap in the body of lit- body shape ideals are easily identifiable within Deaf culture, it
erature examining the body image and disordered eating beha- is possible that they do exist among Deaf women. Further
viors of culturally diverse women; however, the findings must be research could examine the beauty standards that Deaf women
interpreted in light of several limitations. First, generalization of use to define their body shape ideals and how they relate to
the findings from this study to the American deaf population as body image. Lastly, to the authors’ knowledge, no study has
a whole is limited by the small sample size and the homogeneity examined how deaf women interact with mainstream media
of the participants in terms of age, education, ethnic/racial back- and how exposure to such media influences their body image or
grounds, and Deaf acculturation statuses. The current sample eating behaviors. Deaf women are beginning to appear more
was drawn from a culturally Deaf university and thus, represents frequently in various forms of media and knowing the potential
a unique sub-group of deaf individuals in the United States. The effects of such exposure is beneficial to our understanding of
experiences of this mostly young and higher educated sample body image development and the treatment and prevention of
cannot be assumed to be a representative sample of deaf indivi- related disorders.
duals in the United States. Similarly, the majority of the partici- Despite these limitations, the current study expands the lit-
pants identified as White. The cultural experiences of ethnically erature on deaf women’s body image in important ways. Very
and racially minoritized deaf individuals differ from those of few studies of body image and disordered eating have been con-
White deaf individuals (Foster & Kinuthia, 2003; Parasnis et al., ducted with deaf women and this is the first study to examine
2005; Stapleton, 2015) and this must be taken into consideration how Deaf acculturation status and acculturative stress relate to
when interpreting the results of research. Although a significant the body image and eating behaviors of deaf women. This study
difference was identified for the Black deaf participants in terms opens the door for future investigations of culture, body image,
of internalization, some other differences may not have been and disordered eating among various samples of deaf indivi-
identified due to the low numbers of ethnically and racially min- duals. The importance of considering Deaf acculturation status
oritized participants. Lastly, as the sample was drawn from a and acculturative stress when examining the body image of
Deaf cultural environment, the vast majority of the participants deaf women was highlighted and this could have important im-
were classified as Deaf acculturated or Bicultural. There was lim- plications for clinical interventions with deaf individuals.
ited representativeness of deaf individuals with Hearing accul-
turation statuses or of individuals not identifying with either
Hearing or Deaf culture. Due to the limited diversity of accultura- Conflict of Interest
tion experiences, specific investigations of the influence of the
No conflicts of interest were reported.
separate acculturation statuses was not possible. The Deaf and
Hearing acculturation processes occur simultaneously within in-
dividuals and thus, investigations that account for these simulta-
neous processes are important to make distinctions between
References
those deaf individuals with unidirectional strong acculturation Aldalur, A. (2017). Deaf Acculturative Stress: Preliminary
and Bicultural identities and those with unidirectional weak Psychometric Properties of an Acculturative Stress Measure for
acculturation or disconnection from both cultures. Replication of Deaf Individuals (Unpublished pre-dissertation). Gallaudet
the current findings with larger and more diverse samples that University, Washington, D.C.
include deaf individuals from both Deaf cultural and Hearing cul- Andrews, J. F., Leigh, I. W., & Weiner, M. I. (2004). The deaf com-
tural environments and those with diverse ethnic and racial munity: a diverse entity. Deaf people: evolving perspectives from
backgrounds is critical for evaluating the generalizability of the psychology, education, and sociology (pp. 15–35). Boston, MA:
results. Pearson Education.
Second, the acculturative stress measure was adapted spe- Ashikali, E., Dittmar, H., & Ayers, S. (2014). The effect of cosmetic
cifically for this study and although preliminary analyses indi- surgery reality TV shows on adolescent girls’ body image.
cate that the modified SAFE demonstrated good reliability and Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 3, 141–153. doi:10.1037/
validity for this sample, a structured and formal measure of ppm0000022
acculturative stress specific to the Deaf acculturation process is Austin, J. L., & Smith, J. E. (2008). Thin ideal internalization in
needed. It is possible that in this study, the reports of accultura- Mexican girls: a test of the sociocultural model of eating dis-
tive stress collected from the ethnically and racially minoritized orders. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 41, 448–457.
participants overlapped with their experiences of acculturative doi:10.1002/eat.20529
stress stemming from their ethnic and racial cultures. Bat-Chava, Y. (2000). Diversity of deaf identities. American
The measures in the current study were also provided only Annals of the Deaf, 145, 420–428. doi:10.1353/aad.2012.0176
in English. Although all participants were undergraduate stu- Bell, B. T., & Dittmar, H. (2011). Does media type matter? The
dents who likely use academic English on a regular basis, ASL role of identification in adolescent girls’ media consumption
22 | Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2019, Vol. 24, No. 1

and the impact of different thin-ideal media on body image. stress scale. Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and
Sex Roles, 65, 478–490. doi:10.1007/s11199-011-9964-x Development, 29, 67–76. Retrieved from http://journals.
Berry, J. W. (1997). Immigration acculturation and adaptation. sagepub.com/home/mecb (last accessed: 22/08/2018).
Applied Psychology: An International Review, 46, 5–68. doi:10. Garner, D. M., Olmstead, M. P., & Polivy, J. (1983). Development
1093/deafed/ent001 and validation of a multidimensional eating disorder inven-
Berry, J. W., Phinney, J. S., Sam, D. L., & Vedder, P. (2006). tory for anorexia nervosa and bulimia. International Journal of
Immigrant youth: acculturation, identity, and adaptation. Eating Disorders, 2, 15–34. doi:10.1002/1098-108X(198321)
Applied Psychology, 55, 303–332. doi:10.1111/j.1464-0597.2006. 2:2<15::AID-EAT2260020203>3.0.CO;2-6
00256.x Gordon, K. H., Castro, Y., Sitnikov, L., & Holm-Denoma, J. M.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jdsde/article/24/1/11/5095366 by Instituto Federal da Paraíba (IFPB) user on 04 July 2021
Cafri, G., Yamamiya, Y., Brannick, M., & Thompson, J. K. (2005). (2010). Cultural body shape ideals and eating disorder symp-
The influence of sociocultural factors on body image: a meta- toms among White, Latina, and Black college women.
analysis. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 12, 421. doi:10. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 16, 135–143.
1093/clipsy/bpi053 doi:10.1037/a0018671
Castillo, L. G., Zahn, M. P., & Cano, M. A. (2012). Predictors of Gowen, K. L., Hayward, C., Killen, J. D., Robinson, N., & Taylor, B. C.
familial acculturative stress in Asian American college stu- (1999). Acculturation and eating disorder symptoms in adoles-
dents. Journal of College Counseling, 15, 52–64. doi:10.1002/j. cent girls. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 9, 67–83. doi:10.1207/
2161-1882.2012.00005.x s15327795jra0901_4
Cawthon, S. (2004). Schools for the deaf and the no child left Grabe, S., Ward, M. L., & Hyde, J. S. (2008). The role of the media
behind act. American Annals of the Deaf, 149, 314–323. doi:10. in body image concerns among women: a meta-analysis of
1353/aad.2005.0002 experimental and correlational studies. Psychological Bulletin,
Cooper, P. J., Taylor, M. J., Cooper, Z., & Fairbum, C. G. (1987). The 134, 460–476. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.134.3.460
development and validation of the Body Shape Questionnaire. Greenwood, D. N., & DalCin, S. (2012). Ethnicity and body con-
International Journal of eating disorders, 6, 485–494. doi:10.1002/ sciousness: Black and white American women’s negotiation
1098-108X(198707)6:4<485::AID-EAT2260060405>3.0.CO;2-O of media ideals and other’s approval. Psychology of Popular
Dewalt, J. L. R. (1998). I’m proud that I’m deaf: Deaf culture as a pre- Media Culture, 1, 220–235. doi:10.1037/a0029411
ventive factor against eating-disordered attitudes and behaviors for Groesz, L. M., Levine, M. P., & Murnen, S. K. (2002). The effect of
adolescent females. (Doctoral Dissertation), University of experimental presentation of thin media images on body
California, Berkeley, CA. satisfaction: a meta‐analytic review. International Journal of
Dohnt, H. K., & Tiggeman, M. (2005). Peer influences on body dis- Eating Disorders, 31, 1–16. doi:10.1002/eat.1000
satisfaction and dieting awareness in young girls. British Grogan, S. (2010). Promoting positive body image in males and
Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23, 103–116. doi:10.1348/ females: contemporary issues and future directions. Sex
026151004X20658 Roles, 63, 757–765. doi:10.1007/s11199-010-9894-z
Dohnt, H., & Tiggeman, M. (2006). The contribution of peer and Hesse-Biber, S., Livingstone, S., Ramirez, D., Barko, E. B., &
media influences to the development of body satisfaction Johnson, A. L. (2010). Racial identity and body image among
and self-esteem in young girls: a prospective study. Black female college students attending predominately
Developmental Psychology, 134, 929–936. doi:10.1037/0012-1649. White colleges. Sex Roles, 63, 697–711. doi:10.1007/s11199-010-
42.5.929 9862-7
Edwards, R. A. R. (2005). Sound and fury: or, much ado about Hills, C. G., Rappold, E. S., & Rendon, M. E. (1991). Binge eating
nothing? Cochlear implants in historical perspective. The and body image in a sample of the deaf college population.
Journal of American History, 92, 892–920. doi:10.2307/3659972 Journal of the American Deafness & Rehabilitation Association, 25,
Fauquet, J., Sánchez-carracedo, D., Levine, M., & López-Guimerà, 20–28. Retrieved from https://repository.wcsu.edu/jadara/
G. (2010). Influence of mass media on body image and eating (last accessed: 22/08/2018).
disordered attitudes and behaviors in females: a review of ef- Holcomb, R. K., Holcomb, S. K., & Holcomb, T. K. (2011). Deaf cul-
fects and processes. Media Psychology, 13, 387–416. doi:10. ture: our way (4th ed). San Diego, CA: Dawnsign press.
1080/15213269.2010.525737 Hovey, J. D. (2000). Psychosocial predictors of acculturative
Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. stress in Mexican immigrants. The Journal of Psychology, 134,
Human Relations, 7, 117–140. doi:10.1177/001872675400700202 490–502. doi:10.1080/00223980009598231
Fischer, L. C., & McWhirter, J. J. (2001). The Deaf Identity Jambor, E., & Elliott, M. (2005). Self-esteem and coping strategies
Development Scale: a revision and validation. Journal of among deaf students. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf
Counseling Psychology, 48, 355. doi:10.1037//0022-0167.48.3.355 Education, 10, 63–81. doi:10.1093/deafed/eni004
Foster, S., & Kinuthia, W. (2003). Deaf persons of Asian Johnson, M. A. (1996). Latinas and television in the United
American, Hispanic American, and African American back- States: relationships among genere identification, accultura-
grounds: A study of intraindividual diversity and identity. tion, and acculturative stress. The Howard Journal of
Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 8, 271–290. doi:10. Communications, 7, 289–313. doi:1064-6175/96
1093/deafed/eng015 Jones, D. C., Vigfusdottir, T. H., & Lee, Y. (2004). Body image and
Fouts, G., & Burggraf, K. (2000). Television situation comedies: the appearance culture among adolescent girls and boys: an
female weight, male negative comments and audience reac- examination of friend conversations, peer criticism, appear-
tions. Sex Roles, 42, 925–932. doi:10.1023/A:1007054618340 ance magazines, and the internalization of appearance
Fredrickson, B. L., & Roberts, T. A. (1997). Objectification theory: ideals. Journal of Adolescent Research, 19, 323–339. doi:10.1177/
toward understanding women’s lived experiences and men- 0743558403258847
tal health risks. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 21, 173–206. Lau, A. S. M., Lum, S. K., Chronister, K. M., & Forrest, L. (2006).
doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.1997.tb00108.x Asian American college women’s body image: a pilot study.
Fuertes, J. N., & Westbrook, F. D. (1996). Using the social, attitu- Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 12, 259–274.
dinal, familial and environmental (S. A. F. E.) acculturation doi:10.1037/1099-9809.12.2.259
A. Aldalur and D. Schooler | 23

Leigh, I. W., & Lewis, J. W. (2010). Deaf therapists and the deaf Rosenstrom, T., Jokela, M., Hintsanen, M., Josefsson, K., Juonala,
community: issues to consider. In I. W. Leigh (Ed.), M., Kivimaki, M., …Keltikangas-Jarvinen, L. (2013). Body-
Psychotherapy with deaf clients from diverse groups (pp. 39–64). image dissatisfaction is strongly associated with chronic
Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press. dysphoria. Journal of Affective Disorders, 150, 253–260. doi:10.
Leigh, I. W., Maxwell-McCaw, D., Bat-Chava, Y., & Christiansen, 1016/j.jad.2013.04.003
J. B. (2009). Correlates of psychosocial adjustment in deaf Runfola, C. D., Von Holle, A., Trace, S. E., Brownley, K. A.,
adolescents with and without cochlear implants: a prelimi- Hofmeier, S. M., Gagne, D. A., & Bulik, C. M. (2013). Body dis-
nary investigation. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, satisfaction in women across the lifespan: results of the
14, 244–259. doi:10.1093/deafed/enn038 UNC-SELF and gender and body image (GABI) studies.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jdsde/article/24/1/11/5095366 by Instituto Federal da Paraíba (IFPB) user on 04 July 2021
Maxwell-McCaw, D., & Zea, M. C. (2011). The deaf acculturation European Eating Disorders Review, 21, 52–59. doi:10.1002/erv.
scale (DAS): development and validation of a 58-item mea- 2201
sure. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 16, 325–342. Schooler, D. (2008). Real women have curves: a longitudinal
doi:10.1093/deafed/enq061 investigation of TV and the body image development of
Mena, F. J., Padilla, A. M., & Maldonado, M. (1987). Acculturative latina adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Research, 23, 132–153.
stress and specific coping strategies among immigrant and doi:10.1177/0743558407310712
later generation college students. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Schooler, D., Ward, M. L., Merriweather, A., & Caruthers, A.
Sciences, 9, 207–225. doi:10.1037/1099-9809.13.4.347 (2004). Who’s that girl: television’s role in the body image
Menon, C. V., & Harter, S. L. (2012). Examining the impact of development of young white and black women. Psychology of
acculturative stress on body image disturbance among his- Women Quarterly, 28, 38–47. doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.2004.
panic college students. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority 00121.x
Psychology, 18, 239–246. doi:10.1037/a0028638 Schwartz, S. J., & Zamboanga, B. L. (2008). Testing berry’s model
Mitchiner, J. C. (2015). Deaf parents of cochlear-implanted chil- of acculturation: a confirmatory latent class approach.
dren: beliefs on bimodal bilingualism. Deaf Studies and Deaf Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 14, 275–285.
Education, 20, 51–66. doi:10.1093/deafed/enu028 doi:10.1037/a0012818
Mond, J., Mitchison, D., Latner, J., Hay, P., Owen, C., & Rodgers, B. Shao, G. (2009). Understanding the appeal of user-generated
(2013). Quality of life impairment associated with body dis- media: a uses and gratification perspective. Internet Research,
satisfaction in a general population sample of women. BMC 19, 7–25. doi:10.1108/10662240910927795
Public Health, 13, 920–931. doi:10.1037/a0028638 Smart, R., & Tsong, Y. (2014). Weight, body dissatisfaction, and
Moradi, B., & Rottenstein, A. (2007). Objectification theory and disordered eating: Asian American women’s perspectives.
deaf cultural identity attitudes: roles in deaf women’s eating Asian American Journal of Psychology, 5, 344–352. doi:10.1037/
disorder symptamology. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 54, a0035599
178–188. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.54.2.178 Stapleton, L. (2015). When being deaf is centered: d/deaf women
Mussap, A. J. (2009). Acculturation, body-image, and eating be- of color’s experiences with racial/ethnic and d/deaf identities
haviours in Muslim-Australian women. Health & Place, 15, in college. Journal of College Student Development, 56, 570–586.
532–539. doi:10.1016/j.healthplace.2008.08.008 doi:10.1353/csd.2015.0061
Myers, T. A., & Crowther, J. H. (2009). Social comparison as a pre- Te’eni-Harari, T., & Eyal, K. (2015). Liking them thin: adoles-
dictor of body dissatisfaction: A meta-analytic review. doi: cents’ favorite television characters and body image. Journal
10.1037/a0016763 of Health Communication, 20, 607–615. doi:10.1080/10810730.
Nouri, M., Hill, L. G., & Orrell-Valente, J. K. (2011). Media expo- 2015.1012241
sure, internalization of the thin ideal, and body dissatisfac- Thompson, J. K., & Stice, E. (2001). Thin-ideal internalization:
tion: comparing Asian American and European American mounting evidence for a new risk factor for body-image dis-
college females. Body Image, 8, 366–372. doi:10.1016/j.bodyim. turbance and eating pathology. Current directions in psychologi-
2011.05.008 cal science, 10, 181–183. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.00144
Parasnis, I., Samar, V. J., & Fischer, S. D. (2005). Deaf college stu- Thompson, J. K., van den Berg, P., Roehrig, M., Guarda, A. S., &
dents’ attitudes toward racial/ethnic diversity, campus cli- Heinberg, L. G. (2004). The sociocultural attitudes towards
mate, and role models. American Annals of the Deaf, 150, 47–58. appearance scale-3 (SATAQ-3): development and validation.
doi:10.1353/aad.2005.0022 Wiley Periodicals, 293–304. doi:10.1002/eat.10257
Pinkasavage, E., Arigo, D., & Schumacher, L. M. (2015). Social Thompson, K. V., Lightfoot, N. L., Castillo, L. G., & Hurst, M. L.
comparison, negative body image, and disordered eating (2010). Influence of family perceptions of acting white on
behavior: the moderating role of coping style. Eating acculturative stress in African American college students.
Behaviors, 16, 72–77. doi:10.1016/j.eatbeh.2014.10.014 International Journal for the Advancement of Counselling, 32,
Rhea, D. J., & Thatcher, W. G. (2013). Ethnicity, ethnic identity, 144–152. doi:10.1007/s10447-010-9095-z
self-esteem, and at-risk eating disordered behavior differ- Traxler, C. B. (2000). The Stanford Achievement Test, 9th edi-
ences of urban adolescent females. Eating Disorders, 21, tion: national norming and performance standards for deaf
223–237. doi:10.1080/10640266.2013.779177 and hard-of-hearing students. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf
Rodriguez, N., Myers, H. F., Mira, C. B., Flores, T., & Garcia- Education, 5, 337–348. doi:10.1093/deafed/5.4.337
Hernandez, L. (2002). Development of the multidimensional Tucci, S., & Peters, J. (2008). Media influences on body satisfac-
acculturative stress inventory for adults of Mexican origin. tion in female students. Psicothema, 20, 521–524. Retrieved
Psychological Assessment, 14, 451–461. doi:10.1037//1040-3590. from http://www.psicothema.com/pdf/3517.pdf (last ac-
14.4.451 cessed: 22/08/2018).
Rogers Wood, N. A., & Petrie, T. A. (2010). Body dissatisfaction, Tudsri, P., & Hebbani, A. (2015). ‘Now I’m Part of Australia
ethnic identity, and disordered eating among African and I Need to Know What Is Happening Here’: case of
American women. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 57, 141. Hazara male former refugees in Brisbane strategically se-
doi:10.1037/a0018922 lecting media to aid acculturation. Journal of International
24 | Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2019, Vol. 24, No. 1

Migration and Integration, 16, 1273–1289. doi:10.1007/ Vartanian, L. R. (2009). When the body defines the self: self-
s12134-014-0373-1 concept clarity, internalization, and body image. Journal of Social
Van Diest, A. M. K., Tartakovsky, M., Stachon, C., Pettit, J. W., & and Clinical Psychology, 28, 94–126. doi10.1521/jscp.2009.28.1.94
Perez, M. (2014). The relationship between acculturative Yang, C., Wu, H., Zhu, M., Brian, G., & Southwell, G. (2004).
stress and eating disorder symptoms: is it unique from gen- Tuning in to fit in? Acculturation and media use among
eral life stress? Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 37, 445–457. Chinese students in the United States. Asian Journal of
doi:10.1007/s10865-013-9498-5 Communication, 14, 81–94. doi:10.1080/01292980420001951512

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jdsde/article/24/1/11/5095366 by Instituto Federal da Paraíba (IFPB) user on 04 July 2021

You might also like