You are on page 1of 98

Editorial Staff

Editor Book Reviews Marketing and the Law


P. RAJAN VARADARAJAN PEGGY H. CUNNINGHAM ANITA CAVA
JAMS Queen’s University ANN MORALES OLAZÁBAL
Department of Marketing School of Business RENÉ SACASAS
Texas A&M University 229 Dunning Hall Business Law Department
4112 TAMU 99 University Avenue School of Business
College Station, TX 77843-4112 Kingston, Ontario K7L 3N6 Administration
Phone: (979) 862-1019 Canada University of Miami
Fax: (979) 862-1020 Phone: (613) 533-2327 P.O. Box 248022
E-mail: jams@cgsb.tamu.edu Fax: (613) 583-2321 Coral Gables, FL 33124
E-mail: pcunningham@ Phone: (305) 284-4633
business.queensu.ca Fax: (305) 284-3762

Editorial Review Board


MANOJ K. AGARWAL DAVID W. CRAVENS DEBBIE MACINNIS SANJIT SENGUPTA
Binghamton University Texas Christian University University of Southern California San Francisco State University
CHRISTIE H. AMATO PEGGY H. CUNNINGHAM SCOTT B. MACKENZIE VENKATESH SHANKAR
University of North Carolina–Charlotte Queen’s University Indiana University University of Maryland
JONLEE ANDREWS MICHAEL R. CZINKOTA GREG MARSHALL JAGDIP SINGH
Indiana University Georgetown University Oklahoma State University Case Western Reserve University
KWAKU ATUAHENE-GIMA PRATIBHA DABHOLKAR CHARLOTTE MASON JAMES M. SINKULA
City University of Hong Kong University of Tennessee University of North Carolina University of Vermont
RICK BAGOZZI PETER A. DACIN AJAY MENON K. SIVAKUMAR
Rice University Queen’s University Colorado State University Lehigh University
SHARON BEATTY PETER DICKSON ANIL MENON AMY K. SMITH
University of Alabama Florida International University IBM Corporation George Washington University
DAN BELLO PAM SCHOLDER ELLEN BANWARI MITTAL DANIEL C. SMITH
Georgia State University Georgia State University Northern Kentucky University Indiana University
*HAROLD W. BERKMAN ELLEN GARBARINO DAVID B. MONTGOMERY N. CRAIG SMITH
University of Miami Case Western Reserve University Stanford University London Business School
SUNDAR BHARADWAJ JIM GENTRY MITZI MONTOYA-WEISS RICHARD SPRENG
Emory University University of Nebraska North Carolina State University Michigan State University
DAVID M. BOUSH RONALD C. GOODSTEIN ROBERT M. MORGAN DEVANATHAN SUDHARSHAN
University of Oregon Georgetown University University of Alabama University of Illinois at Urbana-
JAMES R. BROWN EVERT GUMMESSON KENT NAKAMOTO Champaign
Virginia Tech Stockholm University Virginia Tech DAVID M. SZYMANSKI
STEVEN P. BROWN GREG GUNDLACH CHERYL NAKATA Texas A&M University
Southern Methodist University University of Notre Dame University of Illinois at Chicago STEPHEN S. TAX
TOM J. BROWN CHRISTIAN HOMBURG DAS NARAYANDAS University of Victoria
Oklahoma State University University of Mannheim Harvard Business School SHIRLEY TAYLOR
MICHELE BUNN G. TOMAS M. HULT RICHARD C. NETEMEYER Queen’s University
University of Alabama Michigan State University University of Virginia GLENN VOSS
ALAN BUSH MICHAEL HYMAN DAVID J. ORTINAU North Carolina State University
University of Memphis New Mexico State University University of South Florida BRIAN WANSINK
ROGER CALANTONE CHARLES A. INGENE AMY L. OSTROM University of Illinois at Urbana-
Michigan State University University of Mississippi Arizona State University Champaign
JOSEPH P. CANNON JEAN L. JOHNSON THOMAS J. PAGE ROBERT B. WOODRUFF
Colorado State University Washington State University Michigan State University University of Tennessee
GOUTAM CHAKRABORTY SUSAN KEAVENEY A. PARASURAMAN JOHN WORKMAN
Oklahoma State University University of Colorado at Denver University of Miami Creighton University
GOUTAM CHALLAGALLA AMNA KIRMANI ROBERT A. PETERSON MANJIT S. YADAV
Georgia Institute of Technology Southern Methodist University University of Texas at Austin Texas A&M University
RAJESH CHANDY MASAAKI KOTABE S. RATNESHWAR GEORGE M. ZINKHAN
University of Minnesota Temple University University of Connecticut University of Georgia
BRUCE CLARK VICKI LANE WILLIAM T. ROBINSON SHAOMING ZOU
Northeastern University University of Colorado at Denver Purdue University University of Missouri at Columbia
JOSEPH COTE MICHAEL R. LEVY SAEED SAMIEE
Washington State University Babson College University of Tulsa

*Founding Fellow

The JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF The Academy of Marketing


MARKETING SCIENCE is the official journal
Science is a member of the
of the Academy of Marketing Science. It is an
international, refereed journal intended to International Association for
further the science of marketing throughout the Management Education
world by promoting the conduct of (AACSB). It gratefully
research and the dissemination of research acknowledges the financial
results through the study and improvement of
support of the Mary Kay
marketing as an economic, ethical, and social
force. For manuscript submission information, Cosmetics Excellence in
refer to the inside back cover. Marketing Fund.

For Sage Publications: David Neyhart, Gillian Dickens, Ken Berthel, and Kelli Palma
Journal of the
Academy of
Marketing
Science
Fall 2001 Volume 29 Number 4

Salesperson Cooperation:
The Influence of Relational, Task, Organizational, and Personal Factors
Cengiz Yilmaz and Shelby D. Hunt 335

The Influence of Complementarity, Compatibility,


and Relationship Capital on Alliance Performance
MB Sarkar, Raj Echambadi, S. Tamer Cavusgil, and Preet S. Aulakh 358

Customer Switching Behavior in Online Services:


An Exploratory Study of the Role of Selected
Attitudinal, Behavioral, and Demographic Factors
Susan M. Keaveney and Madhavan Parthasarathy 374

Managing Culturally Diverse Buyer-Seller Relationships:


The Role of Intercultural Disposition and Adaptive Selling
in Developing Intercultural Communication Competence
Victoria D. Bush, Gregory M. Rose, Faye Gilbert, and Thomas N. Ingram 391

COMMISSIONED ARTICLE
Guidelines for Conducting Research and Publishing in Marketing:
From Conceptualization Through the Review Process
John O. Summers 405

REVIEWS OF BOOKS 416


MARKETING AND THE LAW 424
INDEX 427

The ACADEMY of MARKETING SCIENCE


Central Office: School of Business Administration Published by Sage Publications
University of Miami Thousand Oaks • London • New Delhi
JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY
Yilmaz, Hunt
OF /MARKETING
SALESPERSON
SCIENCE
COOPERATION FALL 2001

Salesperson Cooperation:
The Influence of Relational, Task,
Organizational, and Personal Factors
Cengiz Yilmaz
Gebze Institute of Technology, Turkey

Shelby D. Hunt
Texas Tech University

Salesperson cooperation has become a crucial issue for coordinated efforts of salespeople and other participants,
the overall performance of most sales organizations. The both within and across product lines, functional depart-
authors examine the antecedents of task-specific, coopera- ments, and geographic districts. Cooperation, defined as
tive behaviors of salespersons toward other salespeople the willful contribution of individuals, groups, and so on,
working in the same organization. The main theses of the to the successful completion of common tasks and/or to
study are that (1) the four major antecedent categories of the achievement of mutual objectives (J. Anderson and
factors—relational, task, organizational, and personal— Narus 1990; Deutsch 1949; Wagner 1995) has become a
constitute, collectively, the primary determinants of sales- critical issue in sales management. Many companies seek
person cooperation and (2) each antecedent category ex- sales forces composed of cooperative salespersons who
erts, independently, significant influence on the co- can work effectively in groups. In such sales forces, sales-
operative behaviors of salespersons. The results support people share their skills, knowledge, time, and effort with
the main theses and provide useful insights for sales man- coworkers to achieve common objectives. This emerging
agers attempting to foster cooperation among salespeo- “era of the cooperative salesperson” is manifested in the
ple. The relative impact of each antecedent category, as growing use of team selling (Moon and Armstrong 1994),
well as the effects of specific variables within each, is relationship selling (Weitz and Bradford 1999), selling
discussed. centers (Hutt, Johnston, and Ronchento 1985), and key
account programs (Cohen 1996).
As a result of the growing importance of cooperative
selling, research in sales force management has begun to
Recent decades have witnessed a dramatic change in focus on understanding the dynamics of a salesperson’s
the nature of the selling job for many companies. The tra- interpersonal relationships with coworkers. Issues investi-
ditional view of a salesperson—a single, individualistic, gated include feedback provided by coworkers (Kohli and
persistent person who works independently on a commis- Jaworski 1994), sales force socialization (Dubinsky,
sion basis and who competes fiercely against even fellow Howell, Ingram, and Bellenger 1986), peer mentoring
salespersons—has given way to a strikingly different con- (Pullins, Fine, and Warren 1996), and altruistic behaviors
ceptualization (Cespedes, Doyle, and Freedman 1989; toward coworkers as a form of organizational citizenship
Weitz and Bradford 1999). Selling in many businesses behaviors (e.g., Netemeyer, Boles, McKee, and
today has become an integrated process that requires the McMurrian 1997). Nonetheless, salesperson cooperation,
a critical determinant of the effectiveness of selling efforts
for many businesses, has received little attention.
Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science.
Volume 29, No. 4, pages 335-357. Consider the problem faced by a sales manager who
Copyright © 2001 by Academy of Marketing Science. believes that salesperson cooperation is important for
336 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

sales performance and wants to take action or develop pol- cooperation is in their long-term self-interests based on
icies to increase such cooperation. The literatures of the their rational calculations. According to K. Smith et al.
different research traditions that have examined coopera- (1995), most well-known theoretical explanations of
tion give different, sometimes conflicting, advice. As sug- cooperation belong to this first category (e.g., transaction
gested by the relationship marketing literature (e.g., cost theory and game theory). A second research tradition
Dwyer, Schurr, and Oh 1987; Morgan and Hunt 1994; addresses the noneconomic aspects of cooperative rela-
J. Smith and Barclay 1997), should the sales manager tionships (e.g., Thibaut and Kelley 1959). Rooted in the
focus on taking steps to increase the trust and commitment social exchange literature, research in this tradition
of salespeople? Or, should the manager focus on increas- focuses on the effects of interpersonal attraction, psycho-
ing the task interdependence of the salespeople, as sug- logical attachment, and norms of reciprocity.
gested by Deutsch (1973); Van De Ven, Delbecq, and A third approach relies heavily on power and conflict
Koenig (1976); and Wageman and Baker (1997)? Or, theories (e.g., Emerson 1962). Conflict, the opposite of
should the manager simply focus on hiring salespeople cooperation according to some authors and a key concept
who have a general proclivity toward cooperativeness, as in these theories, stems from diversity in individuals’
suggested by the works of Argyle (1991) and Chatman and resources, perceptions of injustice, values, and goals. A
Barsade (1995)? Answering these questions requires fourth approach relies on social-structure theories and
research that crosses disciplinary lines. emphasizes dimensions outside the focal relationship to
Using an interdisciplinary approach, we address the explain cooperation (e.g., P. Blau 1974). Social, cultural,
question: Why do some salespeople, more than others, and structural aspects of the environment in which the
cooperate with coworkers? We develop and test a model of relationship occurs are seen as drivers of cooperation.
antecedent factors that affect salesperson cooperation, Finally, the fifth approach involves modeling theories and
which is viewed as task-specific, cooperative behaviors emphasizes the impact of social learning and imitation on
among salespeople. On the basis of a review of the cooperative tendencies (e.g., Bandura 1971). Given the
multidisciplinary literature on interpersonal cooperation differing underlying assumptions and units of analysis
in organizations and workgroups, we propose that each of adopted by each research tradition, the current state of
the antecedent factors suggested by prior research can be inquiry on cooperation is replete with explanatory vari-
categorized into one of four categories: relational, task, ables (K. Smith et al. 1995).
organizational, and personal. The main theses of our study Differences notwithstanding, at least three similarities
are that (1) the four major antecedent categories constitute, exist across the research traditions that explore coopera-
collectively, major determinants of salesperson coopera- tion. First, definitions of cooperation in the traditions con-
tion; (2) each antecedent category exerts, independently, verge on a common conceptual domain, and all include a
significant influence on cooperative tendencies among willful-contribution element and a common task or objec-
salespeople; and therefore, (3) sales managers should tive element.1 Second, the resulting outcome for most task
endeavor to address factors in all four categories and not situations is increased productivity, especially in complex
just focus on one or two. Thus, our study aims to provide task situations (Tjosvold 1984; Tjosvold and Tsao 1989),
sales managers with guidance on how to promote coopera- because of cooperating individuals tending to (1) provide
tion among their salespeople. each other with necessary information, (2) more willingly
The article is organized as follows. First, we briefly assist and help each other, (3) understand each other’s
review the literature on interpersonal cooperation in orga- points of view, (4) be influenced by each other’s interests
nizations. Next, we describe the four main antecedent cat- and ideas, and (5) rely on division of labor (Laughlin
egories and develop a structural model that incorporates 1978).2 Third, some conceptual overlap exists among the
predictor variables from each. Third, we test the proposed explanatory variables suggested by each approach, even
model using a large sample of salespersons (N = 531) from though research in each tradition—true to the “silo” view
112 different automobile dealerships. The final sections of academia—seldom crosses lines (K. Smith et al. 1995).
include implications and suggestions for future research. Perhaps this lack of an interdisciplinary approach
accounts for the low variance explained in most studies of
cooperation.
INTERPERSONAL COOPERATION Indeed, research in each of the traditions has (necessar-
ily) been limited in scope (i.e., in terms of including all
K. Smith, Carroll, and Ashford (1995) suggest that major antecedents of cooperation). For example, studies
approaches to the study of cooperation can be grouped into using game theory generally emphasize structural and
five broad traditions. First, an influential research tradition psychological determinants such as task characteristics
explains the emergence of cooperation based on the and personalities of the participants (e.g., Murnighan
calculative orientations of individuals (e.g., Williamson 1994), whereas studies based on social-exchange theory
1975). In this view, individuals will cooperate if and only if focus on the aspects of the relationship between
Yilmaz, Hunt / SALESPERSON COOPERATION 337

cooperating parties. Similarly, while social-structure theo- interpersonal attraction, psychological attachment, and
ries focus solely on the broader context in which a cooper- norms of reciprocity—stimulated by loyalties, friendship,
ative relationship occurs, such as the structural and cul- and faithful expectations—affect individuals’ behavioral
tural environment, modeling theories highlight the choices in relationships. Although such relational vari-
influence of third parties outside the focal relationship ables as communication quality (J. Anderson and Narus
(e.g., managers). However, as Pinto, Pinto, and Prescott 1990), shared values (Chatman 1991; Morgan and Hunt
(1993) note, factors that act as facilitators of cooperation in 1994), cultural differences (McAllister 1995), person-
organizations may belong to a broad set of antecedent cate- organization fit (Chatman 1991; Netemeyer et al. 1997),
gories, ranging “from individual factors such as personali- and expectations regarding the future behaviors of role
ties of group members, interpersonal relations and training partners (Wiener and Doescher 1994) have been theorized
and skills . . . to organizational factors such as strategy, to affect cooperative tendencies, the most prominent rela-
structure, reward systems, and cultural norms” (p. 1282). tional factors are trust and commitment (Achrol 1991;
Therefore, using inferences from each of the traditions, we Morgan and Hunt 1994).
argue that the cooperative behaviors of salespeople Indeed, commitment and trust are considered key for
emerge from the combined effects of variables in four dis- distinguishing social from purely economic exchange
tinct categories: (1) the quality of interpersonal relation- (K. Cook and Emerson 1978; G. McDonald 1981). Coop-
ships between organizational members, that is, relational eration entails vulnerability, and both commitment and
factors; (2) specific properties and requirements of the task trust are considered necessary for individuals to value a
at hand, that is, task factors; (3) the structural, cultural, pro- relationship and to be willing to be vulnerable (Mayer,
cedural, and managerial dimensions of the organization, Davis, and Schoorman 1995; Weitz and Bradford 1999).
that is, organizational factors; and (4) individual charac- Morgan and Hunt (1994) theorize that an individual’s
teristics of organizational members, that is, personal fac- commitment to a relationship and trust in the exchange
tors. Table 1 provides a review of the explanatory variables partner are key determinants of several behavioral tenden-
in the cooperation research. Each antecedent variable used cies in the relationship, including a disposition to cooper-
in the various research approaches can be grouped into one ate. Similarly, we argue that a salesperson’s trust in
of the four categories. coworkers and his or her commitment to the organization
are central to understanding how relational factors facili-
tate cooperation. Specifically, with respect to salesperson
A MODEL OF SALESPERSON
COOPERATION cooperation, we model (1) organizational commitment as
mediating the effects of intrinsic and extrinsic job satisfac-
tion, (2) trust in coworkers as mediating the effects of past
Our model of salesperson cooperation is shown in Fig-
opportunistic behaviors of coworkers and communication
ure 1. Although the model incorporates antecedent factors
quality, and (3) both trust and commitment as mediating
from each main category, it is obvious that not all potential
the effect of shared values.
factors can be included. Thus, the factors from each cate-
gory included in our model are those we propose are most Organizational commitment and cooperation. Organi-
relevant to salesperson cooperation in the context of the zational commitment was originally defined as “the
present study. For example, factors such as organizational strength of an individual’s identification with and involve-
commitment and job satisfaction are included in the model ment in a particular organization” (Porter, Steers,
because these factors are frequently used attitudinal vari- Mowday, and Boulian 1974:604). Stated this way, high
ables in the sales management literature in explaining levels of organizational commitment are characterized by
salesperson behaviors. Similarly, factors such as trust in positive affective responses toward various subgroups, in-
coworkers and task interdependence are included since cluding coworkers, that form the organization (Becker
such factors are key explanatory factors suggested in at 1992). Thus, a salesperson’s commitment to the organiza-
least one of the research traditions exploring cooperation. tion should facilitate his or her cooperative tendencies to-
We discuss each variable in the four antecedent categories ward coworkers. Salespeople who are committed to the
and the theoretical and empirical grounds for 15 specific organization should attach more importance to their rela-
hypotheses. tionships with coworkers, anticipate future interactions
with coworkers for a longer time horizon, and highly value
Relational Factors their associations with coworkers (O’Reilly and Chatman
1986). Each of these variables, in turn, positively affects
Relational factors are those that cause salespersons to cooperative tendencies (Axelrod 1984; Heide and Miner
value their relationships with coworkers and develop 1992). Supporting this view, organizational commitment
mutually beneficial, long-term orientations in working has been shown to promote several forms of constructive
relationships. The social-exchange literature implies that organizational behaviors (O’Reilly and Chatman 1986),
338 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

TABLE 1
Factors Affecting Cooperative Behaviors in Organizations
Factor Effect Exemplar Studies
Relational factors
a. Trust (+) Jones and George (1998); McAllister (1995); Ring and Van De Ven (1994)
b. Commitment (+) Dwyer, Schurr, and Oh (1987); Morgan and Hunt (1994)
c. Value congruence (+) Chatman (1991); McAllister (1995); Morgan and Hunt (1994)
d. Group homogeneity (+) Chatman and Barsade (1995); Kidwell and Bennett (1993)
e. Communication quality (+) J. Anderson and Narus (1990); J. Smith and Barclay (1997)
f. Communication frequency, modality, direction, and content McAllister (1995); Mohr and Nevin (1990)
g. Past opportunistic behaviors of coworkers (–) McAllister (1995); Morgan and Hunt (1994)
h. Anticipated future interactions with coworkers (+) Heide and Miner (1992); Kelley and Thibaut (1978)
i. Expectations regarding future behaviors of coworkers Seabright (1993); Wiener and Doescher (1994)
Task factors
a. Task interdependence (+) Van De Ven, Delbecq, and Koenig (1976); Wageman and Baker (1997)
b. Goal interdependence (+) Tjosvold (1984); Tjosvold and Tsao (1989)
c. Outcome interdependence (+) Deutsch (1973); Guzzo and Shea (1992)
d. Task complexity (+) Van De Ven et al. (1976); Wageman (1995)
e. Costs/benefits of cooperation (–) Deutsch (1973); Wiener and Doescher (1991)
f. Task identifiability/visibility (+) George (1992); Wagner (1995)
g. Personal accountability (+) Kidwell and Bennett (1993); Wagner (1995)
Organizational factors
a. Organizational design and structure Chatman and Barsade (1995); Pinto, Pinto, and Prescott (1993)
b. Organizational culture Chatman and Barsade (1995); J. Smith and Barclay (1993)
c. Reward system Axelrod (1984); Drago and Turnbull (1991); Petersen (1992)
d. Sales force control system E. Anderson and Oliver (1987)
e. Leadership style Podsakoff, MacKenzie, and Bommer (1996)
f. Organizational rules and procedures Galbraith and Nathanson (1978); Moenart and Souder (1990)
g. Turnover rate (–) Kidwell and Bennett (1993); Spicer (1985)
h. Accessibility of coworkers (+) Keller and Holland (1983); Pinto et al. (1993)
i. Number of coworkers (–) Steiner (1972); Wagner (1995)

Personal factors
a. Collectivist orientation (+) Jones and George (1998); Wagner (1995)
b. Personal cooperativeness (+) Argyle (1991); Chatman and Barsade (1995)
c. Agreeableness (+) Chatman and Barsade (1995)
d. Extraversion (+) Thorne (1987)
e. External locus of control (+) Eby and Dobbins (1997); Vancouver and Ilgen (1989)
f. Need for social approval (+) Eby and Dobbins (1997); Hui and Villareal (1989)
g. Social competence (+) Argyle (1991); Dodge (1985)
h. Empathy (+) Eisenberg and Miller (1987)
i. Positive past experience in teams (+) Eby and Dobbins (1997); Loher, Vancouver, and Chajka (1994)
j. Self-efficacy for teamwork (+) Eby and Dobbins (1997); Paulhus (1983)
k. Age (+) Argyle (1991)
l. Gender Colman (1982)
m. Education (+) Burke, McKeen, and McKenna (1990)
n. Organizational tenure (+) Pullins, Fine, and Warren (1996)

NOTE: Those factors for which the direction of effect was not shown in the table are either higher order, general factors that may influence cooperation in
different ways through their various dimensions (e.g., leadership style) or categorical variables (e.g., gender).

including organizational citizenship (Tompson and Hypothesis 1: Organizational commitment and salesper-
Werner 1997) and level of effort exerted for group mainte- son cooperation are positively related.
nance (G. Blau and Boal 1987). Specifically, Dubinsky, Trust in coworkers and cooperation. A salesperson’s
Kotabe, Lim, and Wagner (1997) demonstrate that sales- trust in coworkers stems from his or her perceptions of
people who value pro-social behaviors are also more com- such trust-generating qualities of coworkers as integrity,
mitted to the organization, and MacKenzie, Podsakoff, reliability, and competence (Larzelere and Huston 1980;
and Ahearne (1998) show that organizational commitment Morgan and Hunt 1994; J. Smith and Barclay 1997). Trust
is associated strongly in sales force contexts with various exists when the salesperson believes that coworkers pos-
supportive, extrarole activities, including those directed to sess these major qualities of trustworthiness and is confi-
peers.
Yilmaz, Hunt / SALESPERSON COOPERATION 339

FIGURE 1
Structural Model of Salesperson Cooperation

Task Collectivist Number


Financial Nonfinancial
Interdependence Organizational of
Rewards Rewards
Norms Co-workers

Intrinsic
Job H10(+) H11(+)
H12(+)
Satisfaction H13(+)
H4(+)
H14(-)
Extrinsic
Job H5(+)
Satisfaction Organizational
H1(+)
Commitment
H6(+)
Salesperson
Shared H3(+) Cooperation
Values H7(+)

Trust H2(+)
in
Past H8(-) Co-workers
Opportunistic
Behaviors
H9(+) H15(+)

Communication
Quality Personal Tenure
Age Education in
Cooperativeness
Organization

NOTE: Relational factors: Trust in Coworkers, Organizational Commitment, Communication Quality, Past Opportunistic Behaviors of Coworkers,
Shared Values With Coworkers, Intrinsic Job Satisfaction, Extrinsic Job Satisfaction. Task factor: Task Interdependence. Organizational factors: Financial
Rewards, Nonfinancial Rewards, Collectivist Organizational Norms, Number of Coworkers. Personal factors: Personal Cooperativeness, Age, Education,
Tenure in Organization.

dent that they will be reflected in future behaviors of to develop positive affective responses, and therefore fa-
coworkers. Confidence is crucial because this is what cilitate organizational commitment (Nyhan 1999). Thus, a
causes the most important outcome of trusting relation- positive relationship between trust in coworkers and orga-
ships: the willingness to rely on “the words, actions, and nizational commitment is expected. In support of this
decisions of the other party” (McAllister 1995:25). Trust view, Hrebiniak and Alutto (1972) find trust among new
reduces perceived uncertainty, facilitates risk-taking be- employees as positively related to the subsequent develop-
havior, and fosters a cooperative and/or constructive orien- ment of organizational commitment, J. Cook and Wall
tation (Mayer et al. 1995; Moorman, Deshpande, and (1980) report strong correlations between various dimen-
Zaltman 1993; Morgan and Hunt 1994). Consistent with sions of trust in peers and organizational commitment, and
its properties, several authors have posited trust as an im- Morgan and Hunt (1994) find trust to influence relation-
mediate antecedent of cooperation (e.g., Jones and George ship commitment.
1998; Ring and Van De Ven 1994) and as a key mediating
construct between various relational factors and coopera- Hypothesis 3: Trust in coworkers and salesperson orga-
tion (Morgan and Hunt 1994). nizational commitment are positively related.

Hypothesis 2: Trust in coworkers and salesperson coop- Intrinsic and extrinsic job satisfaction. Empirical stud-
eration are positively related. ies in sales force contexts show that job satisfaction and
several forms of cooperative and/or constructive behav-
Trust facilitates organizational commitment. Relation- iors, such as peer mentoring (Pullins et al. 1996) and orga-
ships with peers, especially the degree and quality of so- nizational citizenship (Netemeyer et al. 1997), are
cialization with coworkers, are among the primary drivers positively related. Similarly, Argyle (1991) notes that job
of commitment to the organization (Hunt, Chonko, and satisfaction is higher in cooperative groups. While expla-
Wood 1985; Mottaz 1988). High levels of interpersonal nations for the relationship between job satisfaction and
trust allow mutual respect to prevail, reduce the complex- various forms of cooperative and/or constructive behav-
ity of organizational life, enable organizational members iors are based on the premise that those who are satisfied
340 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

with their jobs will respond in reciprocation to those who Past opportunistic behaviors of coworkers. Empirical
have contributed to their positive job experience, whether evidence on trust in working relationships suggests that
this relationship is direct or mediated by organizational people, when assessing competence and trustworthiness,
commitment, or both, is still an issue that warrants further consider whether partners have carried out role-related re-
research (cf. Tompson and Werner 1997). Much research sponsibilities reliably (J. Cook and Wall 1980). Coworkers
has found a positive and strong relationship between job who carry out role responsibilities reliably and in a manner
satisfaction and organizational commitment (e.g., consistent with norms of fairness and reciprocity will en-
Johnston, Parasuraman, Futrell, and Black 1990). Further- hance partners’ assessments of their trustworthiness
more, the preponderance of empirical and conceptual evi- (McAllister 1995). In contrast, when coworkers engage in
dence (see Brown and Peterson 1993) suggests that opportunistic behaviors, which Williamson (1975) defines
satisfaction precedes organizational commitment causally as “self interest seeking with guile” (p. 6) and which John
in sales force settings “because it is more specific, less sta- (1984) characterizes as deceitful violations of appropriate
ble, and more rapidly formed” (MacKenzie et al. role behavior, the subsequent level of trust placed in co-
1998:90). Therefore, we suggest that the satisfaction- workers will decrease.
cooperation relationship is mediated by organizational
commitment. Hypothesis 8: Past opportunistic behaviors of coworkers
We further distinguish between the intrinsic and extrin- and salesperson trust in coworkers are negatively re-
sic aspects of job satisfaction. The former refers to an em- lated.
ployee’s satisfaction with the specific nature of the job Communication quality. Prior research has focused on
itself, while the latter concerns those aspects of the job that two general aspects of the communication process:
are outside the specific scope but still within the general (1) mechanistic aspects such as frequency, modality, di-
context of the job (Lucas, Parasuraman, Davis, and Enis rection, and content (e.g., Churchill, Ford, and Walker
1987). Major components of (1) intrinsic job satisfaction 1976; Mohr and Nevin 1990) and (2) qualitative aspects
include the joy of actually performing the job, feelings of (e.g., E. Anderson and Weitz 1989; J. Anderson and Narus
accomplishment received from the job, and the degree of 1990). Consistent with much research on trusting relation-
freedom in the job and of (2) extrinsic job satisfaction in- ships (e.g., Morgan and Hunt 1994; J. Smith and Barclay
clude fair pay, financial earnings, work conditions, and 1997), we limit our discussion to the qualitative aspects of
benefit plans (Lucas et al. 1987). the communication process among salespeople.
Communication quality is defined as timely and accu-
Hypothesis 4: Intrinsic job satisfaction and salesperson rate sharing of information through both formal and infor-
organizational commitment are positively related.
mal means (E. Anderson and Weitz 1989; J. Anderson and
Hypothesis 5: Extrinsic job satisfaction and salesperson
organizational commitment are positively related. Narus 1990; Morgan and Hunt 1994; J. Smith and Barclay
1997). The timely and accurate sharing of information al-
Shared values with coworkers. Shared values are de- lows salespeople to be more confident in their attributions
fined as “the extent to which [organizational members] regarding the trustworthiness of coworkers and enables
have beliefs in common about what behaviors, goals, and them to better assess the motives and intentions behind the
policies are important or unimportant, appropriate or inap- actions of coworkers (Boorom, Goolsby, and Ramsey
propriate, and right or wrong” (Morgan and Hunt 1998). Thus, communication quality results in increased
1994:25). The relationship between shared values and de- trust (Mayer et al. 1995).
velopment of commitment and trust is well documented in
the marketing (Dwyer et al. 1987; Morgan and Hunt 1994) Hypothesis 9: Communication quality with coworkers
and organizational behavior literatures (Chatman 1991). and salesperson trust in coworkers are positively
Shared values positively influence organizational commit- related.
ment because salespeople sharing values with coworkers
can be expected to develop stronger affinities with their Task Factors
overall organization. Similarly, shared values positively
influence trust in coworkers because, as Brewer (1979) ob- Ever since Morton Deutsch published his theory of
serves, individuals tend to perceive socially dissimilar in- cooperation in 1949, task factors have been the most com-
dividuals as dishonest, untrustworthy, and uncooperative. monly used explanatory variables in cooperation research.
Deutsch’s theory viewed cooperation as a form of social
Hypothesis 6: Shared values with coworkers and sales- interaction that can be characterized by perceptions of
person organizational commitment are positively positive interdependence. That is, Deutsch (1949, 1973,
related. 1980) argued that individuals will be more likely to coop-
Hypothesis 7: Shared values with coworkers and sales- erate if they view (1) one another’s goals as (positively)
person trust in coworkers are positively related. related and (2) task characteristics as requiring coop-
Yilmaz, Hunt / SALESPERSON COOPERATION 341

eration to achieve those goals (Tjosvold 1984, 1986). This develop and maintain cooperative organizational systems
notion of interdependence, further developed by Deutsch (Pinto et al. 1993).
and Krauss (1960) and Thompson (1967), has resulted in Three specific organizational factors are hypothesized
the extensive interest in structural factors, especially in in the present study to influence salesperson cooperation:
task factors, among researchers investigating cooperative collectivist organizational norms, reward system, and
relationships. Variables such as task complexity, task number of coworkers. These three variables are thought to
interdependence, and outcome and goal interdependence represent major structural, cultural, and procedural
have been posited as key explanatory factors in studies of dimensions of the organization affecting cooperative ten-
cooperation (Kumar, Scheer, and Steenkamp 1995a, dencies in our sampling context. Research about pro-
1995b; Tjosvold 1984, 1986; Wageman 1995; Wageman social organizational behaviors indicates that several man-
and Baker 1997). Another research stream has investi- agerial variables, particularly leadership style and leader
gated task characteristics in the context of free riding and behaviors, may also influence cooperative tendencies in
social loafing. Findings reveal that identifiability of indi- organizations (Podsakoff et al. 1996). The rationale for the
vidual contributions to the task at hand and personal potent effects of leadership variables is based on the “mod-
accountability influence the degree of within-group coop- eling theories” in K. Smith et al.’s (1995) review of the
eration (Kidwell and Bennett 1993; Wagner 1995), espe- cooperation literature. Based on this view, a sales manager
cially in reciprocal task-flow situations (i.e., when each can promote cooperation among salespeople by (1) acting
person acts on the output of the other). as a “role model” and/or (2) communicating the appropri-
Consistent with Deutsch’s theory, we posit that task in- ate behavioral patterns in the form of “guiding principles”
terdependence, defined as the extent to which salespersons (Larson and LaFasto 1989), which further contribute to the
depend on one another for information and aid to ac- development of organizational norms. The former process
complish their tasks and improve their performance involves imitation of the leader’s behaviors and therefore
(Thompson 1967), will have a direct and positive effect on is unlikely to bear a substantive effect in our sampling con-
salesperson cooperation. However, Deutsch viewed inter- text (i.e., a commission-based, retail selling context where
dependence as central, or even equivalent, to cooperation— salespeople work in a relatively independent manner). The
other factors affecting cooperation can do so only indirectly potential effects of the latter process is captured largely by
through their impact on perceptions of interdependence the collectivist organizational norms variable that we dis-
(Tjosvold 1986). Hence, for example, trust and commit- cuss next.
ment can have no direct effect on cooperation in Deutsch’s
theory but can only exert indirect influence by magnifying Collectivist organizational (cultural) norms. An orga-
perceived interdependence. In contrast, the perspective nization’s internal culture is an important determinant of
taken in the present study is that variables from each of the how organizational members interact with each other
major antecedent categories exert direct influence on (Deshpande, Farley, and Webster 1993). Socially shared
cooperation. rules and acceptable forms of behaviors within an organi-
zation, commonly labeled as organizational (cultural)
Hypothesis 10: Task interdependence and salesperson norms, tend to limit the variation across behaviors of orga-
cooperation are positively related. nizational members by suppressing or supporting certain
types of behaviors (Moch and Seashore 1981). As such,
Organizational Factors the norms embedded in the internal culture of an organiza-
tion prescribe behavioral patterns (Kahn, Wolfe, Quinn,
The structural, cultural, managerial, and procedural Snoek, and Rosenthal 1964). One important dimension of
dimensions of the organization have long been thought to organizational culture closely relevant to cooperative
affect cooperative tendencies among organizational mem- work environments is the extent to which collectivist ver-
bers (Mintzberg 1979; Shapiro 1977). Within this context, sus individualistic norms are embedded within the organi-
variables such as physical proximity of participants and zation’s culture (Chatman and Barsade 1995).
their opportunity to interact (Wagner 1995), organiza- Individualism-collectivism, as a determinant of coop-
tional cultural norms (Moch and Seashore 1981), leader- eration, has been studied at societal (e.g., Hofstede 1980),
ship style (Podsakoff, MacKenzie, and Bommer 1996), individual (e.g., Eby and Dobbins 1997), and organiza-
and the degree to which organizational control systems tional (e.g., Chatman and Barsade 1995; Earley 1993) lev-
reward cooperative efforts versus individual achievement els. As to organizational cultures, individualism-
(E. Anderson and Oliver 1987; Petersen 1992) have been collectivism captures the relative importance organiza-
shown to influence cooperative and/or constructive orga- tional members give to the interests of a larger workgroup
nizational behaviors. Incorporating organizational factors (i.e., coworkers) as opposed to personal interests (Wagner
into models explaining cooperation is important because and Moch 1986). Specifically, collectivist organizational
they provide managers with actionable guidance on how to cultures encourage the subordination of personal interests
342 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

to the goals of a larger work group and, therefore, put more promote cooperative behaviors by making them more
emphasis on sharing, cooperation, and harmony (Wagner feasible.
1995).
Hypothesis 14: The number of coworkers is negatively
Hypothesis 11: Collectivist organizational norms and related to salesperson cooperation.
salesperson cooperation are positively related.
Personal Factors
Reward system. The motivation literature maintains
that financial rewards (e.g., compensation plans, bonuses, Some people are simply more cooperative than others
profit sharing plans) and nonfinancial rewards (e.g., hon- (Argyle 1991). An individual’s disposition to behave
ors, opportunities for personal growth, job security, pro- cooperatively may stem from such personal factors as per-
motion) influence the behaviors of organizational sonality traits (Baron 1983) and demographic characteris-
members (Pritchard, Jones, Roth, Stuebing, and Ekeberg tics (Argyle 1991). For example, Baron (1983) distin-
1988). We define reward system in this study as the degree guishes between “cooperators,” “competitors,” and
to which rewards in the organization, both financial and “individualists” as personality types. Cooperators prefer
nonfinancial, encourage cooperation among salespeople. to work in close collaboration with other people and are
Petersen (1992) notes that managers should carefully primarily interested in the achievement of group objec-
design reward systems if certain types of behavioral pat- tives. Competitors put more emphasis on their personal
terns, such as cooperation, are to be developed. Axelrod goals. Individualists will either cooperate or compete,
(1984) suggests that cooperation can be reinforced by depending on which best fits their personal needs.
making cooperative behaviors more attractive through the Researchers have used several personality measures as
usage of rewards. Research on team effectiveness shows proxies for personal cooperativeness. Examples include
that when rewards are linked to group performance, a re- collectivist orientation (Wagner 1995), agreeableness
ward system that Campion, Medsker, and Higgs (1993) re- (Chatman and Barsade 1995), extraversion (Thorne 1987),
fer to as “interdependent rewards” and Guzzo and Shea locus of control and need for social approval (Eby and
(1992) refer to as “outcome interdependence,” group per- Dobbins 1997), social competence (Dodge 1985), and
formance is facilitated through increased motivation to- empathy (Eisenberg and Miller 1987). In addition,
ward group-oriented behaviors. Finally, J. Anderson and although empirical evidence is scant, such demographic
Narus (1990) and Wiener and Doescher (1991) note that variables as age, gender, education, and tenure in the orga-
individuals will be more likely to cooperate if they believe nization have been proposed as predictors of cooperative
that the outcome of cooperation is going to be positive. In- dispositions (Argyle 1991; Lu and Argyle 1991; Wagner
deed, the supposed relationship between financial rewards 1995). We focus on personal cooperativeness and several
and all individual behaviors is so strong in the motivation demographic variables.
literature that including financial rewards as an antecedent Personal cooperativeness. Personal cooperativeness,
to cooperation may be considered a control variable. That as examined here, is a personality trait that determines the
is, once one controls for financial rewards, do other factors predisposition of an individual toward working in close
explain variance in individual cooperation? collaboration with others in all life activities. A salesper-
son high in this trait
Hypothesis 12: The degree to which financial rewards
encourage cooperative behaviors is positively re- places priority on associating with others for mutual
lated to salesperson cooperation. benefits, gaining social approval, and working to-
Hypothesis 13: The degree to which nonfinancial re- gether with others toward a common end or purpose,
wards encourage cooperative behaviors is positively while a person with low disposition to cooperate
related to salesperson cooperation. places priority on maximizing his or her own wel-
fare regardless of others’ welfare. (Chatman and
Number of coworkers. Research on work groups has Barsade 1995:424)
posited group size as an important predictor of within-
group cooperation (Hechter 1987; Wagner 1995). Because Hypothesis 15: The personality trait of cooperativeness
and salesperson’s cooperative behaviors are posi-
individuals’ workplace behaviors and incremental task
tively related.
contributions are easier to assess, more visible, and/or
“identifiable” in small groups, people in such groups tend Demographic differences. While it has been argued that
to (1) avoid free riding and social loafing and (2) display demographic differences are indicators of several drivers
cooperative and/or constructive behaviors (George 1992). of cooperative behaviors, such as empathy and perspective
Furthermore, Pinto et al. (1993) argue that physical prox- taking (e.g., Davis 1983), several decades of research
imity and accessibility of organizational members may have, in fact, failed to yield conclusive evidence regarding
Yilmaz, Hunt / SALESPERSON COOPERATION 343

the effects of demographic variables on cooperative and/or car salespeople have relatively similar task requirements,
constructive tendencies (Podsakoff, MacKenzie, Paine, which eliminates such concerns as “cooperate in what
and Bachrach 2000). Concerning the impact of age, for in- manner?” and enables a consistent operational definition
stance, Wagner (1995) reports a positive and significant for the cooperation construct. Fourth, the dealerships in
correlation between age and cooperative behaviors, while our sample are relatively small organizations (a majority
Lu and Argyle (1991) report a negative correlation. Simi- of them employ less than 10 salespeople), which mini-
larly, some studies report significant effects of experience, mizes the possibility of confusion on the part of the
education, and organizational tenure (e.g., Kidwell and respondents as to the question of “cooperate with whom?”
Bennett 1993; Pullins et al. 1996; Spicer 1985), and Finally, the fact that our sample is drawn from what is gen-
yet others fail to support the view that these variables erally considered to be a relatively competitive selling
are substantively important predictors of cooperation— context facilitates a strong test of our thesis that each of the
especially when personality differences are accounted for four main antecedent categories exerts a significant and
(Argyle 1991). Given that the literature does not allow us distinct influence on salesperson cooperation.
to specify directional hypotheses, we examine the effects
of age, education level, and organizational tenure from an Data Collection
exploratory perspective.
Preliminary investigation. The study began with un-
structured field interviews with managers and salespeople
METHOD
from four local dealerships. The purpose of the interviews
with managers was to explore whether sales managers in
The research setting involved mail surveys of salespeo- this sales context regarded salesperson cooperation as im-
ple and sales managers from new-car automobile dealer- portant. All four dealership sales managers maintained
ships. Salespeople from the participating dealerships were that they wanted their salespeople to cooperate with each
asked to respond to self-administered questionnaires in other because they believed such cooperation increased
which they were instructed to state their opinions regard- overall sales force performance. These interviews also
ing their coworkers, defined as other salespersons working provided useful insights for developing the specific tasks
in the same dealership. While several “more cooperative” for measuring the cooperation construct. The interviews
selling contexts (such as those that apply team selling) with salespeople provided an on-site pretest of the ques-
exist, new-car salespeople represent a pertinent sample for tionnaire. Ten salespeople from the same four dealerships
our research for several reasons. First, contrary to the ste- commented on items and suggested changes. The final
reotype image of the automobile salesperson, cooperative draft of the questionnaire was developed after making the
selling is a rapidly growing practice in this industry. In required modifications.
response to the competition from the Internet and the
demands of the manufacturer firms, many dealerships Sampling procedure. A sample frame of 1,181 new-car
have initiated relationship marketing and customer reten- dealerships in the state of Texas was developed from a
tion programs. Mixed compensation plans (as opposed to mailing list provided by an independent research firm.
full-commission plans), formal or informal commission Dealership sales managers were contacted by mail to so-
sharing, and year-end bonuses and several forms of manu- licit their cooperation in return for the summary of results.
facturer incentives based on overall dealership perfor- One hundred and sixty-five dealerships agreed to partici-
mance are common practices. Thus, it is not only the case pate in the study, providing access to 1,975 salespeople.
that some reasonable level of cooperation exists among These dealership managers also responded to a short ques-
new-car salespeople but also many dealership managers tionnaire designed to measure several organizational-level
consider such cooperation desirable for the performance variables. These variables include number of vehicles sold
of the overall firm. Our preliminary interviews with deal- per year, number of employees, number of salespeople,
ership managers and salespeople and the data we collected perceived overall degree of cooperation within the sales
for the present research support this view, as we demon- force, and importance of cooperation. Ninety percent of
strate in the following sections. responses to the question “How important is it for the suc-
Second, note that our purpose at this initial stage of the- cess of your dealership that salespersons cooperate with
ory testing is to explain variance and explore relationships. each other?” were above the midpoint of the scale, ranging
Since sales teams are usually composed of people from from 1 (very unimportant) to 7 (very important).3
different functional areas and with diverse backgrounds Four weeks after the initial mailing, the salesperson
(Weitz and Bradford 1999), using such a diverse sample questionnaires were mailed to the managers of the 165
would have decreased our ability to explore the true nature participating dealerships for distribution to their salespeo-
of the relationships due to substantial amount of extrane- ple. Each questionnaire packet also included a cover letter
ous variation that cannot be modeled directly. Third, new- explaining the purpose of the study and return envelopes to
344 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

assure respondent anonymity. Five hundred and eighty- “core task” of our respondents, that is, automobile selling.
five individual salesperson responses from 112 different Thus, our conceptualization of salesperson cooperation,
dealerships were received. After the elimination of care- based on the work of Laughlin (1978) and Morgan and
less respondents and a listwise deletion of missing cases, Hunt (1994), requires a measure capturing various forms
531 questionnaires were retained, resulting in an effective of task-specific cooperative behaviors that respondents are
response rate of 27 percent. The mean within-dealership likely to display toward their coworkers. Both in-role and
response rate was 50 percent. extrarole task-specific behaviors (i.e., those that include
and transcend beyond what is formally prescribed by a
Nonresponse bias. Tests for nonresponse bias rely on salesperson’s organizational role) belong to the domain of
Armstrong and Overton’s (1977) argument that late re- cooperation.
spondents are similar to nonrespondents (in comparison to
Measurement items are developed through an interac-
early respondents). Two different tests were conducted:
tive process with dealership managers and salespeople
one for the first sampling stage (dealership managers) and
who participated in our preliminary interviews. These
one for the second sampling stage (salespeople). For deal-
informants provided us with valuable insights concerning
ership managers, we compared late and early respondents
(1) the nature of cooperation in automobile selling, (2) spe-
on the means of two critical variables, namely, perceived
cific types of cooperative behaviors in various stages of the
overall degree of cooperation within the salesforce and im-
selling process, and (3) clarity and completeness of the
portance of cooperation. For individual salespersons, we
items in the measure. Relatively higher emphasis is given
compared the two groups on the covariance matrix of con-
in the scale to cooperative behaviors involving relation-
struct items (Morrison 1976). No significant differences
ships with customers (e.g., sharing information about
were found in either of the tests, suggesting that
potential and current customers, helping one another’s
nonresponse bias may not be a problem.
customers, etc.), based on the unanimous agreement
Sample characteristics. Our sampling process resulted among our informants that customer-related cooperation
in a sample that varied greatly on both dealership and is of critical importance for the success of selling efforts
salesperson characteristics. The dealerships vary in size as and most representative of a cooperative sales force. Other
measured by number of employees (M ≅ 40, SD = 49.16), facets of salesperson cooperation frequently mentioned by
salespeople (M ≅ 12, SD = 9.5), and vehicles sold per year the informants include assisting coworkers during sales
(M ≅ 943, SD = 937.5). Individual respondents vary widely presentations, sharing information about vehicle specifics,
in age (M = 39.26 years, SD = 11.49), sales experience and providing support in terms of activities that facilitate
(M = 10.65 years, SD = 9.78), organizational tenure (M = the selling process (e.g., handling of paperwork). Respon-
2.57 years, SD = 3.34), and education (≤ high school di- dents rated the extent to which they engage in each type of
ploma, 18.15%; some college, 52.45%; college graduate, cooperative behavior on a 7-point scoring format, ranging
20.33%; graduate work, 9.07%). Most of the respondents from very little to very much.
are male (90.91%) and full-commission salespeople Trust in coworkers and organizational commitment.
(69.78%). The scale in Morgan and Hunt (1994) is used for measur-
ing trust in coworkers. Based on the Dyadic Trust Scale of
Measures Larzelere and Huston (1980), this measure captures re-
spondents’ confidence in the integrity, reliability, compe-
Constructs are measured using multiple-item mea- tence, and general trustworthiness of relationship partners.
sures, whenever applicable. All scales use a 7-point scal- An additional item, “I consider my coworkers as people
ing format with anchors strongly disagree to strongly whom I would be willing to let make important job-related
agree, unless otherwise noted. Measurement items are decisions without my involvement,” was included to put
provided in the appendix. The reliabilities of the multiple- more emphasis on the competence dimension. Organiza-
item, reflective measures are presented in Table 2. The tional Commitment is measured using the nine-item ver-
coefficient alphas, Lisrel-based internal consistency esti- sion of Mowday, Steers, and Porter’s (1979) Organiza-
mates (i.e., composite reliability), and the amount of vari- tional Commitment Scale, which has been used exten-
ance captured by each construct in relation to measure- sively in prior research (Mathieu and Zajac 1990).
ment error (i.e., average variance extracted) are well
beyond the acceptable threshold levels suggested by Measures of exogenous constructs. Shared Values With
Nunnally (1978) and Fornell and Larcker (1981). Coworkers and Past Opportunistic Behaviors of Cowork-
ers use the scales in Morgan and Hunt (1994). The assess-
Cooperation. For the sake of operational and ment of shared values involves a two-stage procedure (cf.
nomological clarity, we limit the domain of the coopera- Enz 1988): respondents are asked to state the degree to
tion construct to cooperative behaviors that represent the which (1) they agree and (2) their coworkers would agree
TABLE 2
Descriptive Statistics for the Scales, Reliability Estimates,a and Latent Factor Correlations
Composite Variance
Scale M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 Reliability Extracted
1. Cooperation 5.38 1.18 .87 .87 .63
2. Organizational Commitment 5.72 1.13 .31 .91 .89 .78
3. Trust 4.73 1.37 .39 .41 .95 .95 .84
4. Intrinsic Job Satisfaction 5.70 1.08 .39 .78 .45 .85 .86 .70
5. Extrinsic Job Satisfaction 5.20 1.35 .23 .61 .36 .61 .89 .89 .84
6. Shared Values 6.16 1.06 .20 .23 .47 .31 .28 .86 .87 .70
7. Opportunistic Behaviors 3.61 1.62 –.19 –.22 –.54 –.31 –.32 –.40 .87 .89 .81
8. Communication Quality 5.10 1.18 .45 .31 .52 .40 .31 .28 –.38 .89 .89 .84
9. Task Interdependence 4.97 1.35 .49 .34 .25 .36 .18 .11 –.04 .36 NA NA NA
10. Collectivist Organizational Norms 5.34 1.15 .41 .44 .44 .42 .30 .27 –.20 .34 .49 .84 .85 .66
11. Financial Rewards 5.22 1.60 .44 .40 .39 .42 .41 .24 –.26 .42 .28 .43 NA NA NA
12. Nonfinancial Rewards 4.99 1.62 .38 .37 .37 .33 .30 .16 –.21 .42 .22 .39 .61 NA NA NA
13. Numbers of Coworkers 16.7 11.4 –.08 –.05 –.05 –.09 –.03 –.01 .05 –.03 –.08 .02 –.02 –.08 NA NA NA
14. Personal Cooperativeness 5.17 0.96 .52 .25 .28 .30 .13 .01 –.11 .31 .35 .37 .37 .33 .00 .74 .77 .65
15. Age 39.3 11.5 .04 –.03 –.06 –.17 –.16 –.15 .17 –.04 .04 .03 –.05 .04 .03 .07 NA NA NA
16. Tenure in Organization 2.57 3.34 .04 .05 –.02 .02 .07 –.04 –.01 .00 .07 .01 –.05 .02 –.07 –.04 .32 NA NA NA
17. Education –.05 –.09 .04 –.13 –.11 –.06 .08 –.05 –.04 .01 –.16 –.04 –.07 .07 .07 .01 NA NA NA

NOTE: Discriminant validity is obtained if the variance extracted for a construct is greater than the squared latent factor correlation between a pair of constructs. NA = not applicable because the construct was mea-
sured with a formative scale or had fewer than three items.
a. Coefficient alphas are reported on the diagonal.
345
346 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

with five statements concerning ethical values. The differ- versus competitive behavioral dispositions in specific
ences between the two responses (subtracted from 7) are environments. Wordings of the items borrowed from each
then used to reflect shared values. For opportunistic be- scale are altered slightly to develop a measure of General
haviors, we added the following item to the original three- Cooperativeness that would apply in all environments—
item scale: my coworkers avoid fulfilling their responsibil- work, school, family, and so on. Thus, as a significant dif-
ities unless they are watched closely. ference from the Cooperation Scale, which is limited to
Selected items from the marketing practitioner’s Job task-specific cooperative behaviors directed toward
Satisfaction Scale of Hunt and Chonko (1984) and the coworkers, items in the Personal Cooperativeness Scale
salesperson Intrinsic Job Satisfaction Scale of Lucas et al. measure a salesperson’s predisposition toward working in
(1987) are used to measure intrinsic aspects of the close collaboration with others in general.
repondents’ job satisfaction. Extrinsic Job Satisfaction
items are drawn from Lucas et al.’s (1987) study. Items in Measure Purification and Validation
both scales come from the Job Dimensions Scale (Groves
1981; Schletzer 1965). Similarly, for Communication Following the two-step procedure recommended by
Quality, we use selected items from the Communication J. Anderson and Gerbing (1988), we estimate and
Quality Scales in Morgan and Hunt (1994) and J. Smith respecify the measurement model prior to incorporating
and Barclay (1997). Both scales measure the degree of the structural restrictions. Maximum-likelihood LISREL 8
timely and accurate sharing of information, and both are (Jöreskog and Sörbom 1993) is used in the analyses, and
based on the Communication/Participation/Feedback the sample covariance matrix is used as input.4 In addition,
Scale of E. Anderson, Lodish, and Weitz (1987). because some of the scales in this research are either com-
Reward System, the degree to which the rewards in the pletely new (e.g., Cooperation) or composed of selected
organization encourage (discourage) cooperation between items from previously used scales (e.g., Intrinsic Job Satis-
salespeople, is operationalized for both financial rewards faction), it is reasonable to anticipate that several items
and nonfinancial rewards. Single items for both dimen- will have to be dropped during respecification of the mea-
sions are developed to assess the degree to which such surement model. Cross validation is recommended for
rewards in the dealership favor cooperative behaviors. A such measure purification processes to minimize error
7-point scoring format ranging from strongly discourage probability and capitalization on chance. Accordingly,
cooperation to strongly encourage cooperation is used. responses were randomly split into two halves so as to
For Collectivist Norms embedded within the culture of the cross validate the measurement model.
organization, we use the Norms subscale of Individualism- The initial model, which consisted of all 78 measure-
Collectivism, developed in Wagner and Moch (1986) and ment items and 17 factors, was estimated using the first
further validated in Wagner (1995). Items of the original split sample. However, several items had high standard-
scale were modified slightly to assess organizational-level ized residuals and modification indices, making the model
cultural norms. fit not acceptable: χ2(2,796) = 5,362, Comparative Fit Index
For Task Interdependence, we use the three-item Task (CFI) = .82, Goodness-of-Fit Index (GFI) = .66, root mean
Interdependence Scale in Campion et al. (1993), which square error of approximation (RMSEA) = .058, standard-
measures the degree to which respondents depend on each ized root mean square residual (SRMR) = .067. We
other to accomplish their tasks and improve their perfor- respecified the model by eliminating three items from the
mance. While the third item in the scale is a direct measure Intrinsic Job Satisfaction Scale, four items from Extrinsic
of interdependence, the first two items tap the degree of Job Satisfaction, three from Organizational Commitment,
interdependence from a dyadic perspective in that the first four from Cooperation, two from Trust, three from Com-
item is a measure of the respondent’s dependence on munication Quality, one from Opportunistic Behaviors,
coworkers and the second item is a measure of the respon- and four from Personal Cooperativeness. Considering the
dent’s perception of coworkers’ dependence on him or her. large number of constructs and items, the respecified
For this reason, responses to the first two items are first model fits the data well, χ2(1,248) = 2002.7, CFI = .91, GFI =
averaged and then combined with the third item to gener- .88, RMSEA = .046, SRMR = .049.5
ate a task interdependence score for each respondent. Next, we tested the respecified model on the second
Finally, Personal Cooperativeness is measured using split sample. The resulting fit indices indicate that the
items from the Work-Cooperativeness Scale of Lu and measurement model has a good fit to the data. While the
Argyle (1991), the School-Cooperativeness Scale of Rob- GFI is an acceptable .88, the RMSEA value of .044 and the
erts (1991), and the Acceptance of Cooperation/ SRMR value of .046 indicate a very good model fit. Simi-
Teamwork Scale of Oliver and Anderson (1994). These larly, in terms of incremental fit, the CFI for the model is
scales have been used to determine manifest personality .93, which exceeds the recommended .90 acceptance crite-
differences across individuals in terms of cooperative rion (R. McDonald and Marsh 1990). The fit of the model
Yilmaz, Hunt / SALESPERSON COOPERATION 347

is even better when it is estimated using the full sample, therefore necessary for sustained membership in the
χ2(1,248) = 2,420, CFI = .93, GFI = .88, RMSEA = .041, organization.
SRMR = .044. In addition, all items load significantly on Table 3 reports goodness-of-fit indices and standard-
their respective constructs (with the lowest t-value being ized parameter estimates for the structural model. The
11.1), providing support for the convergent validity of overall chi-square statistic is significant, χ2(1,275) = 2530.6,
measurement items. p < .01, as is expected given the large sample size (Bagozzi
and Yi 1988). All other goodness-of-fit indices are within
Unidimensionality and discriminant validity. Proce-
the acceptable ranges (CFI = .93, GFI = .88, RMSEA =
dures for examining the measurement scales for
.042, SRMR = .051). Taken collectively, these results
unidimensionality are based on exploratory and confirma-
show that the hypothesized structural relationships fit the
tory factor analyses of scale items, taken one scale at a
data well. Overall, the hypothesized structural relation-
time, to see if the items in each scale share a single underly-
ships explain 45 percent of the observed variance in coop-
ing factor. Exploratory factor analyses reveal that only one
eration. In addition, 11 of the 15 hypothesized paths are
factor accounts for a major portion of the total variance in
supported, and at least one factor from each of the four
each scale (i.e., only one factor is extracted using an
antecedent categories exerts significant influence on sales-
eigenvalue of 1.0 as the cutoff point). Similarly, the
person cooperation.
goodness-of-fit indices obtained from one-factor confir-
matory factor analyses of the scales are all acceptable (i.e., Also included in Table 3 are the parameter estimates
GFI > .90, CFI > .90). and associated test statistics of the hypothesized relation-
Tests for discriminant validity are based on compari- ships adjusted for common method variance. Given that
sons of the chi-square statistics obtained from confirma- the same informants provided the data for most of the
tory factor analyses of pairwise combinations of the study exogenous and endogenous constructs in our model, the
constructs when the correlation between the constructs are possibility exists that common method variance may have
(1) constrained to unity and (2) freed for estimation. A sig- inflated or deflated the magnitudes of the parameter esti-
nificantly lower chi-square value for the unconstrained mates for the hypothesized paths. Thus, it is necessary to
model indicates that the two constructs are distinct. assess the degree of this form of bias in our results. The
Discriminant validity is obtained for all the study con- adjusted estimates in Table 3 are obtained after partialing
structs using this test (∆χ2[1] > 3.84 for all pairwise compar- out the portion of variance that is common across all our
isons), as well as the more stringent procedure suggested observed variables obtained from the same source (i.e.,
by Fornell and Larcker (1981) (see Table 2). salespeople), using the procedure in MacKenzie,
Podsakoff, and Paine (1999).
As shown in Table 3, the overall pattern of significant
RESULTS relationships in the sample is not affected much by com-
mon method variance. Of the 11 paths that are significant
Descriptive statistics for the scales are provided in in the unadjusted analysis, 10 are significant in the
Table 2. The standard deviations indicate a substantial adjusted analysis, with the path from collectivist organiza-
amount of variance in the responses.6 More important, the tional norms to cooperation dropping just slightly to the
large standard deviations for the three endogenous point of being nonsignificant at the traditional .05 level.
constructs—Cooperation (1.18), Trust in Coworkers More important, given that the adjusted estimates have
(1.37), and Organizational Commitment (1.13)—suggest much greater standard errors because of the inclusion of an
that each of these constructs has considerable amount of additional “common method” factor in the model and
variance to be explained. In addition, most means are fewer degrees of freedom, the absolute sizes of the coeffi-
within one-half point of the scale centers. While the mean cients should be the primary basis of comparison, not the
for the Cooperation Scale is 5.38, the dispersion of this significance levels. Note that the magnitudes of the
variable is also reasonably high, indicating that the sample adjusted path coefficients in our results are very close to
includes both cooperative and noncooperative respon- the magnitudes of the unadjusted estimates,7 and the corre-
dents (13% of the responses are below the center of the lation between the two sets of estimates is .93 (p value <
scale). Furthermore, the fact that most of the responses are .000). Furthermore, a chi-square difference test indicates
at the higher end of the Cooperation Scale is not unex- that the model representing the adjusted estimates is not
pected. Studies on organizational members commonly statistically different from the (more parsimonious) model
report similar results (e.g., Chatman and Barsade 1995; representing the unadjusted estimates (∆χ2[62] = 71.2).
Eby and Dobbins 1997). One explanation for this pattern Accordingly, our discussion in the following paragraphs
of results lies in the very notion of “the organization.” concerning the effects of specific antecedents is based on
Organizations exist because individuals come together to the unadjusted estimates. We discuss the potential impact
work for a common purpose. Some level of cooperation is of same-source bias in cases where significant deviations
348 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

TABLE 3
Summary of Results
a
Path Estimate t-Value Adjusted Estimate t-Value
Relational factors
Organizational Commitment → Cooperation –.04 –0.78 –.07 –1.62
Trust → Cooperation .14 3.44** .11 2.16*
Trust → Organizational Commitment .09 2.49** .08 2.01*
Intrinsic Job Satisfaction → Organizational Commitment .65 10.52** .63 6.96**
Extrinsic Job Satisfaction → Organizational Commitment .21 4.65** .42 6.18**
Shared Values → Organizational Commitment –.04 –1.33 .01 0.20
Shared Values → Trust .26 6.01** .33 5.03**
Opportunistic Behaviors → Trust –.30 –6.90** –.44 –6.96**
Communication Quality → Trust .34 8.43** .17 2.62**
Task factors
Task Interdependence → Cooperation .30 5.08** .31 5.69**
Organizational factors
Collectivist Organizational Norms → Cooperation .10 2.08* .08 1.47
Financial Rewards → Cooperation .14 2.35** .15 2.45**
Nonfinancial Rewards → Cooperation .06 1.06 .04 0.88
Number of Coworkers → Cooperation –.05 –1.24 –.07 –1.24
Personal factors
Personal Cooperativeness → Cooperation .29 5.42** .38 5.89**
Age → Cooperation .01 0.23 .12 2.31*
Education → Cooperation –.06 –1.36 –.02 –0.38
Organizational Tenure → Cooperation .04 0.90 .10 1.97*
2
χ (1,275) = 2,530.64, CFI = .93, GFI = .88, RMSEA = .042, SRMR = .051
SMCCOOPERATION = .45, SMCTRUST = .47, SMCORGANIZATIONAL COMMITMENT = .63

NOTE: CFI = Comparative Fit Index; GFI = Goodness-of-Fit Index; RMSEA = root mean square error of approximation; SRMR = standardized root mean
square residual; SMC = squared multiple correlation.
a. Adjusted estimates are obtained after adding to the model a first-order “common method” factor that has each measure obtained from the same source as
an indicator.
*p < .05 (one-tailed test). **p < .01.

exist between the adjusted and unadjusted estimates for munication Quality, Past Opportunistic Behaviors of
specific paths. Coworkers, and Shared Values With Coworkers, are also
important for cooperation. All three of these constructs
Effects of Specific Antecedents have significant indirect effects on cooperative behaviors
of salespeople through their influence on trust in
Of the eight constructs hypothesized to exert direct coworkers.
influence on salesperson cooperation, task interdepen- The paths hypothesizing direct effects of organiza-
dence (standardized path coefficient, γi = .30, p < .01) and tional commitment (Hypothesis 1), nonfinancial rewards
personal cooperativeness (γi = .29, p < .01) have the high- (Hypothesis 13), and number of coworkers (Hypothesis 14)
est levels of explanatory power according to both adjusted are not supported. The results of the unadjusted analysis
and unadjusted analyses, providing strong support for also suggest that none of the three demographic indicators—
Hypotheses 10 and 15. Other significant antecedents of age, organizational tenure, and education level—are sig-
cooperation include financial rewards (Hypothesis 12, |γi = nificant predictors of salesperson cooperation. However,
.14, p < .01), trust in coworkers (Hypothesis 2, |βi = .14, p < there is a sharp contradiction between the adjusted and
.01), and collectivist organizational norms (Hypothesis unadjusted estimates concerning the potential effects of
11, |γi = .10, p < .05). However, while the adjusted and age and organizational tenure. While the unadjusted esti-
unadjusted estimates of the path coefficient linking collec- mates for these variables do not bear any form of statistical
tivist organizational norms to cooperation are close in and/or substantive significance, the magnitudes of the
magnitude, the adjusted estimate is slightly below the tra- adjusted estimates are much greater and reach the point of
ditionally accepted .05 significance level because of the being statistically significant. These results suggest that
inflated standard error value. Finally, the results suggest same-source effects may be an explanation for the mixed
that three exogenous relational factors, namely, Com- empirical findings in prior studies that explored the effects
Yilmaz, Hunt / SALESPERSON COOPERATION 349

of such demographic variables in combination with other while an interdependent task design will produce more
potential antecedents of cooperative behaviors. cooperation, focusing merely on task interdependence
Concerning the antecedents of trust in coworkers and does not seem to guarantee a highly cooperative sales
organizational commitment, our findings support strongly force. Developing and maintaining a cooperative sales
Hypotheses 7, 8, and 9, as shared values (γi = .26, p < .01), force requires attention to personal, organizational, and
past opportunistic behaviors (γi = –.30, p < .01), and com- relational factors as well, since these factors are also found
munication quality (γi = .34, p < .01) are significantly to exert influence on salesperson cooperation.
related to levels of trust placed in coworkers. Similarly, Concerning the effects of personal factors, there is
Hypotheses 3, 4, and 5 are supported because trust in strong empirical support that personal cooperativeness is a
coworkers (βi = .20, p < .01), intrinsic job satisfaction (γi = major predictor of salesperson cooperation. At the same
.65, p < .01), and extrinsic job satisfaction (γi = .21, p < .01) time, while our results suggest that education level is not a
are significant predictors of organizational commitment. significant predictor, the potential effects of age and orga-
Hypothesis 6 is not supported, however, as shared values nizational tenure are somewhat unclear. Both of these vari-
with coworkers are unrelated to organizational commitment. ables have relatively weak zero-order correlations with
salesperson cooperation (see Table 2), and our unadjusted
estimates for the effects of these variables are far from hav-
DISCUSSION ing statistical and substantive significance. However, after
controlling for common method variance, age and organi-
This study explores the antecedent conditions that pro- zational tenure become significant predictors of salesper-
mote or inhibit salesperson cooperation. To this end, sev- son cooperation. This finding is interesting, given that
eral antecedent factors were identified, each factor was research about constructive employee behaviors in general
categorized into one of the four broader sets of antecedent has failed to reveal conclusive evidence regarding the
conditions, and each factor was tested within a nomologi- effects of such demographic factors. Additional research
cal network for its effect on cooperative behaviors of sales- is needed before this issue can be resolved conclusively.
persons directed toward coworkers, that is, other salespeo- Thus, overall, our results regarding the influence of per-
ple. The main thesis of the hypothesized structural model is sonal factors highlight both the importance and difficulty
that each antecedent category of factors—relational, task, of recruitment procedures if a cooperative sales force is
organizational, and personal—exerts significant influence desired. Recruiting salespeople who are cooperators by
on cooperation, independently from the effects of others. the very nature of their personality is crucial, but identify-
On the basis of this thesis, the study explores the relative ing cooperative candidates could be a difficult task. The
effects of each main category on salesperson cooperation. correlation coefficients relating personal cooperativeness
Our findings provide strong support for the main thesis and to demographic variables are all small and nonsignificant
valuable insights regarding specific predictors of salesper- (see Table 2), suggesting that personal cooperativeness is a
son cooperation. First and foremost, the hypothesized personality trait that is not manifested in demographic
structural relationships explain 45 percent of the observed characteristics. As a result, sales managers who wish to
variance in cooperation, which exceeds that found in most develop cooperative sales forces, rather than relying solely
studies within each of the five research traditions explor- on demographic indicators, should attempt to recruit
ing cooperation. Second, at least one variable from each of salespersons who (1) have a history of cooperative behav-
the four antecedent categories is shown to exert significant iors and/or (2) score high on personality tests of
influence on cooperation. The proportion of variance in “cooperativeness.”
cooperation accounted for by the significant predictors Regarding organizational factors, organizational
captures almost all of the total variance explained, since rewards are traditionally seen as one of the most effective
the proportion of variance explained by nonsignificant managerial tools to influence the behaviors of organiza-
predictors is negligible (less than 1%). Using statistically tional members—and rightly so. Our findings suggest a
significant effects only, task factors and personal factors strong effect of financial rewards on salesperson coopera-
each explain approximately 15 percent of the observed tion. As expected, the degree to which financial rewards
variance in salesperson cooperation, while organizational are designed and awarded in a manner that encourages
factors and relational factors explain 10 percent and 5 per- cooperation between salespeople influences cooperative
cent, respectively.8 behaviors. On the other hand, nonfinancial rewards, for
Consistent with several decades of research, the results example, honors, opportunities for personal growth, job
support the view that task interdependence is an important security, and promotion, do not seem to affect cooperative
predictor of cooperation. When salespersons believe that tendencies. This latter finding should be interpreted with
their personal success is dependent on the support of caution, however. The fact that the path coefficient con-
coworkers, they have a greater tendency to cooperate with necting nonfinancial rewards to cooperation is not signifi-
coworkers. However, consistent with our main thesis, cant does not necessarily mean that these two concepts
350 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

have no relationship at all. The correlation between these finding is in line with the growing interest in marketing on
constructs is, in fact, large and significant (r = .38, p value < relational variables, particularly on trust. Indeed, a sales-
.000). Nonetheless, the relationship is attenuated in the person’s trust in coworkers is not only a significant predic-
structural model (i.e., when other predictor variables are tor of cooperation even when task, organizational, and per-
controlled for), suggesting that nonfinancial rewards are sonal factors are accounted for but is also a key factor
not among the primary motivational drivers for our mediating the impact of communication quality, past
respondents. Given the specific nature of our sampling opportunistic behaviors of coworkers, and shared values
context, further research is required to determine the with coworkers. Several of these exogenous relational fac-
extent to which this finding generalizes to other selling tors in the hypothesized model also influence salesperson
contexts. cooperation indirectly through their effects on trust.9
Our research indicates a moderately strong effect of In contrast, however, the results do not support the
collectivist organizational norms on salesperson coopera- paths from shared values with coworkers to organizational
tion. This finding highlights the importance of norm devel- commitment and from organizational commitment to
opment and enforcement processes for sales managers cooperation. All three of these constructs are, in fact, posi-
attempting to establish a cooperative sales force. Based on tively and significantly correlated (see Table 2), but the
their standing in the organizational hierarchy, managers in relationships become statistically nonsignificant when
most organizations have the ability to exert a substantial other antecedent factors are controlled for in the structural
influence on the evolution of organizational norms. This is model. Concerning the relationship between organiza-
particularly true for sales managers who have developed tional commitment and cooperation, for example, a com-
effective means to communicate the expected behavior mon antecedent, trust in coworkers, seems to be the driv-
patterns and who set examples through their own actions ing factor. An explanation for these results may lie in the
(Feldman and Arnold 1983). As Larson and LaFasto notion of multiple commitments (Becker 1992; Reichers
(1989) report, members of workgroups are more likely to 1985, 1986).
practice the “guiding principles” suggested by their lead- The multiple-commitments view suggests that organi-
ers when the leaders themselves live up to the prescribed zational commitment is “a collection of multiple commit-
behavioral patterns. ments to various groups that compromise the organiza-
Next, in contrast to that hypothesized, we found no sig- tion” (Reichers 1985:469). Note that the conceptual
nificant relationship between number of coworkers and domains of shared values and cooperation constructs in
the degree of cooperative behaviors directed toward our model concern, specifically, the salesperson’s rela-
coworkers. This result contrasts with research about tionships with coworkers. The nomological role that orga-
workgroups that suggest a strong effect of group size on nizational commitment plays in our model depends on the
cooperative tendencies. One explanation for this unex- degree to which salespersons associate coworkers directly
pected finding relies on the differences in the types of with their overall notion of “the organization.” The more
interdependence observed in other workgroups and our influence coworkers have on one’s affective state regard-
sample. According to a typology suggested by Thompson ing the organization, the more important should be the role
(1967), workgroup members are in reciprocal interdepen- of organizational commitment. The respondents in our
dence when each acts on the output of the other. In recipro- sample may not have viewed their relationships with
cal interdependence, workgroup size is an important deter- coworkers as a strong determinant of what they feel toward
minant of free riding, social loafing, and cooperation their respective dealerships and vice versa, thereby yield-
(Wagner 1995). On the other hand, the type of interdepen- ing the result that organizational commitment is unrelated
dence in the present sample is what Thompson (1967) to both shared values with coworkers and cooperative
refers to as pooled interdependence, in which each respon- behaviors toward coworkers.
dent is individually responsible for performing his or her
job from the beginning to end and dependent on coworkers Post Hoc Model Respecification
for only certain types of aid and support that enhance per-
formance. The number of coworkers may be less impor- Although the hypothesized model fits the data, one
tant in pooled interdependence, as cooperation is more of a would not expect a simple model such as Figure 1 to be the
voluntary act and not required by the flow of interdepen- best fit for the data set. Accordingly, in an exploratory
dent tasks. manner, we reviewed LlSREL modification indices and
Finally, the results show that relational factors, those conducted additional analyses to determine whether there
that cause an individual to value his or her association with exist additional, nonhypothesized structural paths that are
coworkers and develop a mutually beneficial, long-term likely to (1) have statistical significance and (2) improve
orientation in his or her relationships with coworkers, have the model fit. Two observations that emerged as a result of
considerable effect on salesperson cooperation. This this post hoc specification search deserve further
Yilmaz, Hunt / SALESPERSON COOPERATION 351

discussion. First, all relatively high modification indices Another area for future research concerns the potential
suggest additional paths from some of the exogenous fac- effects of leadership style and leader behaviors in sales
tors, specifically from Collectivist Organizational Norms, forces where salespeople view their manager as a key per-
Nonfinancial Rewards, Intrinsic Job Satisfaction, and Per- son in their work environment. Podsakoff et al. (1996)
sonal Cooperativeness, to trust in coworkers. Second, show that the effects of leadership variables on pro-social
when these paths are incorporated into the model, (1) three organizational behaviors are not only significant but also
of these additional parameters, those linking trust with col- independent from those of several substitutes for leader-
lectivist norms, nonfinancial rewards, and intrinsic satis- ship. Thus, given the similarities between the literatures on
faction, are significant; (2) model fit is only marginally pro-social behaviors and cooperative behaviors, leader-
improved, χ2(1,271) = 2473.8, CFI = .93, GFI = .88, RMSEA = ship variables may bear some distinct influence on sales-
.041, SRMR = .047; and (3) all previously significant person cooperation, particularly in team-selling situations.
paths remain significant with only slight changes in Finally, inconsistent with expectations, the study
parameter estimates. These findings suggest that trust in reveals that organizational commitment is unrelated to
coworkers might be even more crucial for salesperson both shared values with coworkers and cooperative behav-
cooperation, fully or at least partially mediating the impact iors. Our expectation at the inception of the study, that is,
of several organizational, personal, and relational factors. that coworkers constitute a primary group among those
However, because exploratory search processes such as that form a salesperson’s “overall view of the organiza-
the preceding require cross validation, we urge the readers tion,” is brought into question. As is often the case, this
to be cautious when interpreting these findings. unexpected finding suggests fruitful avenues for further
research. Researchers might examine several forms of
Limitations and Future Research Directions constituency-specific commitments (e.g., commitment to
coworkers, supervisors, top management, union, etc.) to
Generalizability is a concern for all studies. Even better understand the interrelationships between these
though the sample used in the study, due to homogeneity concepts; how they form the global notion of organiza-
across respondents, allowed us to control for the back- tional commitment; and how they affect attitudinal and
ground factors and conduct a strong test of the hypothe- behavioral dispositions of salespersons toward coworkers,
sized relationships, caution should be taken when general- supervisors, and other targets.
izing the results to other selling contexts. In particular, the
sample is composed of automobile salespeople, all of
whom engage in face-to-face, retail selling activities. Most CONCLUSION
of the respondents are male (90.91%), full-commission
salespeople (69.78%), and work in relatively independent In conclusion, in this “era of the cooperative salesper-
working environments. Researchers might study the theo- son,” although many sales managers see overall sales per-
retical model in different selling contexts, particularly in formance as being closely linked to the coordinated efforts
team-selling and industrial-selling contexts. of their salespeople, getting salespeople to cooperate is
A closely related issue involves investigating potential often perceived to be a difficult task. While many “highly
moderators. Future research could examine the moderat- cooperative” sales forces exist, it is often difficult to iden-
ing effects of several factors, many of which we control for tify the specific factors that contribute to the development
in this study. A nonexhaustive list of such moderators of cooperation. Our study suggests that each one of the
includes (1) type of sales force and the nature of the selling four major antecedent categories of factors—relational,
job (team selling versus individual selling, retail selling task, organizational, and personal—is important for a
versus industrial selling, face-to-face selling versus dis- cooperative sales force. Specifically, we find that sales
tance selling, etc.), (2) type of interdependence in the sell- managers seeking to encourage cooperation should (1) take
ing task (i.e., whether the task flow generates pooled, steps to increase task interdependence, (2) attempt to hire
sequential, or reciprocal interdependence, etc.), (3) nature salespeople who have a history of cooperative behaviors,
of the compensation system (i.e., whether the compensa- (3) develop reward systems that reward cooperative behav-
tion system is based on individual versus group perfor- iors, (4) foster trust among their employees, (5) work
mance; whether it is full-commission, a combination sys- toward shared values, (6) discourage opportunistic behav-
tem, or full salary, etc.), and (4) performance-reward iors, (7) promote high-quality communication among
contingencies (i.e., the degree to which rewards are salespeople, and (8) foster collectivist organizational
awarded in proportion to performance). norms. Our study, however, is but one step toward under-
standing salesperson cooperation.
352 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

APPENDIX
Measurement Items

Standardized
Scale Item Factor Loadings t-Value
Cooperation To what extent do you cooperate with your coworkers by
a
1. Working with them to develop sales presentation techniques. — —
2. Providing support during sales presentations. .74 16.4
3. Taking care of their customers during their absence. .52 11.5
4. Assisting them by handling paperwork for them. .73 16.4
5. Providing feedback for improving their performance. .78 17.7
a
6. Handling their customers’ complaints in their absence. — —
7. Assisting them in collecting and storing customer-related data. .75 16.9
8. Sharing information about vehicle attributes. .71 16.1
a
9. Sharing information about competitors. — —
10. Sharing information about potential customers. .70 15.8
a
11. Sharing information about existing customers. — —

Trust in Coworkers I consider my co-workers as people who(m)


b
1. Cannot be trusted at times (R). — —
b
2. Are perfectly honest and truthful. — —
3. Can be trusted completely. .85 23.4
4. Can be counted on to do what is right. .91 29.4
5. Can be counted on to get the job done right. .87 26.5
6. Are always faithful. .89 27.8
7. I have great confidence in. .91 29.8
8. Have high integrity. .92 29.8
9. I would be willing to let make important job-related decisions without my involvement. .68 18.2

Organizational These questions concern your feelings toward your dealership.


Commitment 1. I am willing to put in a great deal of effort beyond than normally expected to help
this company be successful. .73 16.3
2. I talk up this company to my friends as a great company to work for. .85 19.9
b
3. I would accept almost any type of job assignment to keep working for this organization. — —
4. I find that my values and the company’s values are similar. .77 17.8
5. I am proud to tell others that I am part of this organization. .83 21.4
c
6. This organization really inspires the very best in me in the way of job performance. — —
7. I am extremely glad that I chose this company to work for over others I was considering at
the time I joined. .83 19.4
8. I really care about the fate of this company. .78 18.1
c
9. For me, this is the best of all possible organizations for which to work. — —

Shared Values Please indicate the degree to which you believe that (1) your coworkers would agree with the
With Coworkers following statements, and (2) you agree with the following statements.
1. To succeed in this business, it is often necessary to compromise one’s ethics (R). .66 11.1
2. Top management in a business must let it be known in no uncertain terms that unethical
behaviors will not be tolerated. .74 14.5
3. If an employee is discovered to have engaged in unethical behavior that results primarily
in personal gain (rather than corporate gain), he or she should be promptly reprimanded. .83 15.8
4. If an employee is discovered to have engaged in unethical behavior that results primarily in
corporate gain (rather than personal gain), he or she should be promptly reprimanded. .82 15.7
5. Employers should assure that their employees are behaving in a business-like manner. .73 14.5

Past Opportunistic My experience with my coworkers tells me that, to accomplish their personal objectives,
Behaviors of they would sometimes, in their interactions with me,
Coworkers 1. Alter the facts slightly. .80 18.4
2. Promise to do things without actually doing them later. .90 21.9
3. Fail to provide me with the support that they are obliged to. .81 20.0
b
4. Avoid fulfilling their responsibilities unless they are watched closely. — —

Intrinsic Job I am satisfied with


Satisfaction 1. How interesting my job is. .65 12.1
2. How this job makes good use of my abilities. .77 14.9
(continued)
Yilmaz, Hunt / SALESPERSON COOPERATION 353

APPENDIX
Continued

Standardized
Scale Item Factor Loadings t-Value
c
3. The degree of freedom I have in my job. — —
c
4. The opportunities my job gives me to complete tasks from beginning to end. — —
5. The feelings of accomplishment I get from my job. .79 15.3
6. The opportunities my job provides me to interact with others. .81 15.6
c
7. How my company encourages professional growth. — —
8. The opportunities for independent thought and action in my job. .74 14.6

Extrinsic Job I am satisfied with


Satisfaction 1. The compensation plan under which I work. .82 20.7
2. My earnings as a sales consultant. .87 23.3
3. Fairness of my earnings in relation to efforts I expend. .89 24.0
b
4. My probable future earnings in this business. — —
a
5. The attitude of the public toward my company. — —
b
6. My benefit plan in general. — —
a
7. The attitude of the public toward the industry I am working in. — —

Communication In working relationships with my coworkers,


Quality 1. We keep each other informed of new developments. .92 30.1
2. We provide each other with timely information. .94 32.5
3. We frequently discuss accounts and opportunities. .72 20.9
4. We sometimes hold back on telling each other what we know about accounts and
b
opportunities (R). — —
a
5. We communicate well about our expectations for each other’s performance. — —
a
6. We provide each other with frequent positive feedback on our performance. — —

Reward System The financial rewards (compensation plans, bonuses, profit-sharing plans, etc.) and
nonfinancial rewards (honors, opportunities for professional growth, job security,
promotion, etc.) in auto dealerships influence the behaviors of sales consultants.
Some reward systems encourage sales consultants to cooperate with each other, others
encourage competition, and some others may encourage both cooperation and competition.
How would you describe the financial and nonfinancial rewards in your dealership?
The financial rewards in this dealership
Strongly discourage cooperation 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Strongly encourage cooperation
The nonfinancial rewards in this dealership
Strongly discourage cooperation 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Strongly encourage cooperation

Collectivist In general, people in this dealership believe that


Organizational 1. If one is going to be part of this company, then he or she is sometimes going to have to do
Norms things that are not in his or her immediate self-interests. .66 12.9
2. Everyone should sometimes make sacrifices for the sake of the larger group of coworkers. .78 14.9
3. Everyone should realize that one is not always going to get what he or she personally wants. .73 14.2
4. Everyone should be willing to make sacrifices for the sake of the well-being of people they
work with. .83 15.5
5. All employees should do their best to cooperate with others instead of trying to work things
out individually. .65 12.3

Task 1. I cannot do my job well without information and assistance from my coworkers.
d
Interdependence 2. My coworkers depend on me for information and assistance to perform their jobs.
3. Doing a good job in selling cars requires that sales consultants support each other.

Personal Throughout my life, I have always


Cooperativeness 1. Enjoyed activities that involve a high level of cooperation with other people. .81 16.9
b
2. Been known as a team player. — —
3. Preferred to work independently rather than in a group (R). .75 14.7
4. Found more satisfaction working toward a common group goal than working toward my
b
individual goals. — —
(continued)
354 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

APPENDIX
Continued

Standardized
Scale Item Factor Loadings t-Value
b
5. Found it more difficult to do things with others than by myself (R). — —
b
6. Found joint projects with other people very satisfying. — —
7. Believed that teamwork is the best way of getting results. .60 12.5

NOTE: (R) denotes a reverse-coded item.


a. These items were eliminated because LISREL modification indices indicated that they have high error correlations with other items in their respective
scales.
b. These items were eliminated because their squared multiple correlations were less than .2.
c. These items were eliminated because LISREL modification indices indicated that they have high shared variance with items in other scales.
d. Composite scale.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS terdependence. For each of these measures, our study uses perceptions of
(multiple) salespersons from each dealership, hence incorporating some
level of within-dealership variability to the analyses. Ideally, however,
The authors thank Roy Howell, James B. Wilcox, Dale
the only reason for the variability in these measures should be differences
F. Duhan, Kimberly B. Boal, and Mike Whitman (all of between the dealerships (i.e., across-dealership variability). To assess the
Texas Tech University) for their helpful comments and degree to which differences in the perceptions of respondents within each
assistance in this research. The insightful comments of the dealership contribute to the overall variability in these measures, we con-
editor and three anonymous reviewers on drafts of this arti- ducted a series of one-way analyses of variance using dealerships as a
treatment factor. The results of these analyses reveal that although some
cle are also acknowledged.
within-dealership variability exists in the measures of organizational and
task characteristics (on average, less than 40%), most of their total vari-
ability is due to differences between the dealerships.
NOTES 7. Also note that for several paths, the magnitudes of the adjusted esti-
mates are actually larger than the unadjusted estimates and therefore con-
1. These two elements distinguish cooperation from other forms of stitute a stronger case for our overall model.
pro-social workplace behaviors such as peer mentoring and helping be- 8. These figures are calculated by multiplying the standardized effect
havior. Unlike such related constructs, the purpose of cooperation is the size of each predictor with the zero-order correlation between the predic-
improvement of the welfare of all participants (including the cooperating tor and cooperation; therefore, they do not represent the proportion of
individual), not just the other party. variance in cooperation uniquely attributable to each type of predictor
2. See Weitz and Bradford (1999) for an excellent discussion of how (i.e., incremental variance explained in cooperation when a predictor
these common threads of cooperation literature apply in this new era of variable is “added” to the model). The unique contribution of each pre-
“partnering-oriented” selling. dictor variable to the variance explained in cooperation is as follows: task
3. This question was included in the managers’ questionnaires to interdependence, 5 percent; personal cooperativeness, 5 percent; finan-
build a priori confidence that cooperation matters in this context. We also cial rewards, 2 percent; trust, 1 percent; collectivist organizational norms,
conducted a post hoc test for the relationship between salesperson coop- 1 percent.
eration and a self-reported, long-term performance measure. The partial 9. LISREL modification indices do not suggest direct paths from any
correlation coefficient between salesperson cooperation and perfor- of the exogenous relational factors to cooperation.
mance (controlling for the influence of all other study variables) is signif-
icant (r = .11, p = .019), which implies that highly cooperative
salespeople tend to evaluate themselves as also being high in perfor-
mance. REFERENCES
4. Measurement error terms for the composite task interdependence
measure and other single-item measures are set at 0.1 times the variance Achrol, Ravi. 1991. “Evolution of the Marketing Organization: New
of each measure. Forms for Turbulent Environments.” Journal of Marketing 55 (4): 77-
93.
5. It is worthwhile to note that several items across the Intrinsic Job
Anderson, Erin and Richard L. Oliver. 1987. “Perspectives on Behavior-
Satisfaction, Extrinsic Job Satisfaction, and Organizational Commitment
Based versus Outcome-Based Salesforce Control Systems.” Journal
Scales tend to cross-load on the other constructs even after the of Marketing 51 (October): 76-88.
respecification. However, the modification indices for these items are
 and Barton Weitz. 1989. “Determinants of Continuity in Conven-
much smaller in magnitude in comparison with (1) those for the items
tional Industrial Channel Dyads.” Marketing Science 8 (Fall): 310-
that were eliminated and (2) total chi-square of the model. Thus, taking 323.
into account the substantive meaning of each item, we decided that the
, Leonard M. Lodish, and Barton Weitz. 1987. “Resource Alloca-
measurement model has a reasonable level of goodness of fit and stopped tion Behavior in Conventional Channels.” Journal of Marketing Re-
the respecification process. search 24 (February): 85-97.
6. An interesting issue concerns the sources of variability in the scales Anderson, James C. and David W. Gerbing. 1988. “Structural Modeling
measuring organizational and task characteristics, that is, collectivist or- in Practice: A Review and Recommended Two-Step Approach.” Psy-
ganizational norms, financial rewards, nonfinancial rewards, and task in- chological Bulletin 103 (3): 411-423.
Yilmaz, Hunt / SALESPERSON COOPERATION 355

 and James A. Narus. 1990. “A Model of Distributor Firm and  1973. The Resolution of Conflict. New Haven, CT: Yale Univer-
Manufacturer Firm Working Partnerships.” Journal of Marketing 54 sity Press.
(January): 42-58. . 1980. “Fifty Years of Conflict.” In Retrospections on Social Psy-
Argyle, Michael. 1991. Cooperation: The Basis of Sociability. London: chology. Ed. L. Festinger. New York: Oxford University Press, 46-77.
Routledge.  and R. M. Krauss. 1960. “The Effect of Threat upon Interper-
Armstrong, J. Scott and Terry S. Overton. 1977. “Estimating sonal Bargaining.” Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology
Nonresponse Bias in Mail Surveys.” Journal of Marketing Research 61:181-189.
14 (August): 396-402. Dodge, K. A. 1985. “Facets of Social Interaction and the Assessment of
Axelrod, Robert M. 1984. The Evolution of Cooperation. New York: Ba- Social Competence in Children.” In Children’s Peer Relations: Is-
sic Books. sues in Assessment and Intervention. Eds. B. H. Schneider, K. H. Ru-
Bagozzi, Richard P. and Youjae Yi. 1988. “On the Evaluation of Struc- bin, and J. E. Ledingham. New York: Springer-Verlag, 183-218.
tural Equation Models.” Journal of the Academy of Marketing Sci-
Drago, Robert and Geoffrey K. Turnbull. 1991. “Competition and Coop-
ence 16 (Spring): 77-94.
eration in the Workplace.” Journal of Economic Behavior and Orga-
Bandura, A. 1971. Social Learning Theory. New York: General Learning
nization 15 (May): 347-364.
Press.
Dubinsky, Alan J., Roy D. Howell, Thomas N. Ingram, and Danny
Baron, R. 1983. Behavior in Organizations. Newton, MA: Allyn and Ba-
Bellenger. 1986. “Sales Force Socialization.” Journal of Marketing
con.
50 (October): 192-207.
Becker, Thomas E. 1992. “Foci and Bases of Commitment: Are They
Distinctions Worth Making?” Academy of Management Journal , Masaaki Kotabe, Chae Un Lim, and William Wagner. 1997.
35:232-244. “The Impact of Values on Salespeople’s Job Responses: A Cross-Na-
Blau, Gary J. and Kimberly B. Boal. 1987. “Conceptualizing How Job In- tional Investigation.” Journal of Business Research 39 (3): 195-208.
volvement and Organizational Commitment Affect Turnover and Dwyer, F. Robert, Paul H. Schurr, and Sejo Oh. 1987. “Developing Buyer
Absenteeism.” Academy of Management Review 12 (2): 288-300. Seller Relationships.” Journal of Marketing 51 (April): 11-27.
Blau, P. 1974. On the Nature of Organizations. New York: John Wiley. Earley, P. Christopher. 1993. “East Meets West Meets Mideast: Further
Boorom, Michael, Jerry Goolsby, and Rosemary Ramsey. 1998. “Rela- Explorations of Collectivist and Individualistic Workgroups.” Acad-
tional Communications and Their Effects on Adaptiveness and Sales emy of Management Journal 36:319-348.
Performance.” Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science 26 Eby, Lillian T. and Gregory H. Dobbins. 1997. “Collectivist Orientation
(Winter): 16-30. in Teams: An Individual and Group Level Analysis.” Journal of Or-
Brewer, M. B. 1979. “In-Group Bias in the Minimal Intergroup Situation: ganizational Behavior 18:275-295.
A Cognitive-Motivational Analysis.” Psychological Bulletin 86:307- Eisenberg, N. and P. A. Miller. 1987. “The Relation of Empathy to
324. Prosocial and Related Behaviors.” Journal of Personality and Social
Brown, Steven P. and Robert A. Peterson. 1993. “Antecedents and Conse- Psychology 52:91-119.
quences of Salesperson Job Satisfaction: Meta-Analysis and Assess- Emerson, R. E. 1962. “Power-Dependence Relations.” American Socio-
ment of Causal Effects.” Journal of Marketing Research 30 logical Review 27:31-41.
(February): 63-77. Enz, Cathy A. 1988. “The Role of Value Congruity in Intraorganizational
Burke, Ronald J., Carol A. McKeen, and Katherine S. McKenna. 1990. Power.” Administrative Science Quarterly 33 (June): 284-304.
“Sex Differences and Cross-Sex Effects on Mentoring: Some Prelim- Feldman, Daniel C. and Hugh J. Arnold. 1983. Managing Individual and
inary Data.” Psychological Reports 67:1011-1023. Group Behavior in Organizations. Tokyo, Japan: McGraw-Hill.
Campion, Michael A., Gina J. Medsker, and A. Catherine Higgs. 1993. Fornell, Claes and David F. Larcker. 1981. “Evaluating Structural Equa-
“Relations Between Work Group Characteristics and Effectiveness: tion Models With Unobservable Variables and Measurement Error.”
Implications for Designing Effective Work Groups.” Personnel Psy- Journal of Marketing Research 18 (February): 39-50.
chology 46:823-850. Galbraith, J. R. and D. A. Nathanson. 1978. Strategic Implementation:
Cespedes, Frank V., Stephen X. Doyle, and Robert J. Freedman. 1989. The Role of Structure and Process. Dallas, TX: Business Publica-
“Teamwork for Today’s Selling.” Harvard Business Review (March- tions.
April):44-54. George, J. M. 1992. “Extrinsic and Intrinsic Origins of Perceived Social
Chatman, Jennifer A. 1991. “Matching People and Organizations: Selec- Loafing in Organizations.” Academy of Management Journal
tion and Socialization in Public Accounting Firms.” Administrative 35:191-202.
Science Quarterly 36 (September): 459-484.
Groves, David L. 1981. “Impact of Industrial Recreation Upon Job Satis-
 and Sigal G. Barsade. 1995. “Personality, Organizational Cul- faction and Productivity.” Journal of Environmental Systems 11:187-
ture, and Cooperation: Evidence From a Business Simulation.” Ad- 200.
ministrative Science Quarterly 40:423-443. Guzzo, R. A. and G. P. Shea. 1992. “Group Performance and Intergroup
Churchill, Gilbert A., Neil M. Ford, and Orville C. Walker Jr. 1976. “Or- Relations in Organizations.” In Handbook of Industrial and Organi-
ganizational Climate and Job Satisfaction in the Salesforce.” Journal zational Psychology. Eds. M. D. Dunnette and L. M. Hough. Palo
of Marketing Research 13 (November): 323-332. Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press, 269-313.
Cohen, Andy. 1996. “Managing National Account Programs.” Sales and
Hechter, M. 1987. Principles of Group Solidarity. Berkeley: University
Marketing Management (April):32-46.
of California Press.
Colman, A. 1982. Interpersonal Process in Close Relationships.
Wokingham, England: Van Nostrand. Heide, Jan B. and Anne S. Miner. 1992. “The Shadow of the Future: Ef-
fects of Anticipated Interaction and Frequency of Contact on Buyer-
Cook, John and Toby Wall. 1980. “New Work Attitude Measures of
Seller Cooperation.” Academy of Management Journal 35 (2): 265-
Trust, Organizational Commitment, and Personal Need Non-
291.
Fullfilment.” Journal of Occupational Psychology 53:39-52.
Cook, Karen S. and Richard M. Emerson. 1978. “Power, Equity, and Hofstede, Geert. 1980. Culture’s Consequences. Beverly Hills, CA:
Commitment in Exchange Networks.” American Sociological Re- Sage.
view 43 (October): 721-739. Hrebiniak, Lawrence G. and Joseph A. Alutto. 1972. “Personal and Role
Davis, Mark. 1983. “Measuring Individual Differences in Empathy: Evi- Related Factors in the Development of Organizational Commit-
dence for a Multidimensional Approach.” Journal of Personality and ment.” Administrative Science Quarterly 17 (3): 555-572.
Social Psychology 44:113-126. Hui, C. H. and Villareal M. J. 1989. “Individualism-Collectivism and
Deshpande, Rohit, John U. Farley, and Frederick E. Webster, Jr. 1993. Psychological Needs: Their Relationships in Two Cultures.” Journal
“Corporate Culture, Customer Orientation, and Innovativeness in of Cross-Cultural Psychology 20:310-323.
Japanese Firms: A Quadrant Analysis.” Journal of Marketing 57 Hunt, Shelby D. and Lawrence B. Chonko. 1984. “Marketing and
(January): 23-27. Machiavellianism.” Journal of Marketing 48 (Summer): 30-42.
Deutsch, Morton. 1949. “A Theory of Cooperation and Competition.” , , and Van R. Wood. 1985. “Organizational Commitment
Human Relations 2:129-152. and Marketing.” Journal of Marketing 49 (Winter): 112-126.
356 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

Hutt, Michael D., Wesley J. Johnston, and John R. Ronchento, Jr. 1985. McDonald, Roderick and Herbert W. Marsh. 1990. “Choosing a
“Selling Centers and Buying Centers: Formulating Strategic Ex- Multivariate Model: Noncentrality and Goodness of Fit.” Psycholog-
change Partners.” Journal of Personal Selling and Sales Management ical Bulletin 107 (2): 247-255.
5 (May): 33-40. Mintzberg, H. 1979. The Structure of Organizations. Englewood Cliffs,
John, George. 1984. “An Empirical Investigation of Some Antecedents of NJ: Prentice Hall.
Opportunism in a Marketing Channel.” Journal of Marketing Re- Moch, M. and S. E. Seashore. 1981. “How Norms Affect Behaviors in
search 21 (August): 278-289. and of Corporations.” In Handbook of Organizational Design, Vol. 1.
Johnston, Mark W., A. Parasuraman, Charles M. Futrell, and William C. Eds. P. C. Nystrom and W. H. Starbuck. New York: Oxford University
Black. 1990. “A Longitudinal Assessment of the Impact of Selected Press, 534-565.
Organizational Influences on Salespeople’s Organizational Commit- Moenart, R. K. and W. E. Souder. 1990. “An Analysis of the Use of
ment During Early Employment.” Journal of Marketing Research 17 Extrafunctional Information by R&D and Marketing Personnel: Re-
(August): 333-344. view and Model.” Journal of Product Innovation Management 7:91-
Jones, Gareth R. and Jennifer M. George. 1988. “The Experience and 107.
Evolution of Trust: Implications for Cooperation and Teamwork.” Mohr, Jakki and John R. Nevin. 1990. “Communication Strategies in
Academy of Management Review 23 (3): 531-546. Marketing Channels: A Theoretical Perspective.” Journal of Mar-
Jöreskog, Karl G. and Dag Sörbom. 1993. LISREL 8: A Guide to the Pro- keting 54 (October): 36-51.
gram and Applications. Chicago: Statistical Package for the Social Moon, Mark A. and Gary M. Armstrong. 1994. “Selling Teams: A Con-
Sciences. ceptual Framework and Research Agenda.” Journal of Personal
Kahn, Robert L., Donald M. Wolfe, Robert P. Quinn, J. Diedrick Snoek, Selling and Sales Management 14 (Winter): 17-30.
and Robert A. Rosenthal. 1964. Organizational Stress. New York: Moorman, Christine, Rojit Deshpande, and Gerald Zaltman. 1993. “Fac-
John Wiley. tors Affecting Trust in Market Research Relationships.” Journal of
Marketing 57 (January): 81-101.
Keller, R. T. and W. E. Holland. 1983. “Communicators and Innovators in
Morgan, Robert M. and Shelby D. Hunt. 1994. “The Commitment-Trust
Research and Development Organizations.” Academy of Manage-
Theory of Relationship Marketing.” Journal of Marketing 58 (July):
ment Journal 26:742-749.
20-38.
Kelley, Harold H. and John W. Thibaut. 1978. Interpersonal Relations: A Morrison, Donald. 1976. Multivariate Statistics. New York: McGraw-
Theory of Interdependence. New York: John Wiley. Hill.
Kidwell, Roland E. Jr. and Nathan Bennett. 1993. “Employee Propensity Mottaz, Clifford J. 1988. “Determinants of Organizational Commit-
to Withhold Effort: A Conceptual Model to Intersect Three Avenues ment.” Human Relations 41 (6): 467-482.
of Research.” Academy of Management Review 18 (3): 429-456. Mowday, Richard T., Richard M. Steers, and Lyman W. Porter. 1979.
Kohli, Ajay K. and Bernard J. Jaworski. 1994. “The Influence of Co- “The Measurement of Organizational Commitment.” Journal of Vo-
Worker Feedback on Salespeople.” Journal of Marketing 58 (4): 82- cational Behavior 14:224-227.
94. Murnighan, J. K. 1994. “Game Theory and Organizational Behavior.” In
Kumar, Nirmalya, Lisa K. Scheer, and Jan-Benedict E. M. Steenkamp. Research in Organizational Behavior. Eds. B. M. Staw and L. L.
1995a. “The Effects of Perceived Interdependence on Dealer Atti- Cummings. Greewich, CT: JAI, 323-347.
tudes.” Journal of Marketing Research 32 (August): 348-56. Netemeyer, Richard G., James S. Boles, Daryl O. McKee, and Robert
, , and . 1995b. “The Effects of Supplier Fairness McMurrian. 1997. “An Investigation Into the Antecedents of Organi-
on Vulnerable Resellers.” Journal of Marketing Research 32 (Febru- zational Citizenship Behaviors in a Personal Selling Context.” Jour-
ary): 54-65. nal of Marketing 61 (July): 85-98.
Larson, Carl E. and Frank M. J. LaFasto. 1989. Teamwork: What Must Go Nunnally, Jum C. 1978. Psychometric Theory. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Right/What Can Go Wrong. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Nyhan, Ronald C. 1999. “Increasing Affective Organizational Commit-
Larzelere, Robert E. and Ted L. Huston. 1980. “The Dyadic Trust Scale: ment in Public Organizations.” Review of Public Personnel Adminis-
Toward Understanding Interpersonal Trust in Close Relationships.” tration 19 (3): 58-70.
Journal of Marriage and the Family 42 (August): 595-604. Oliver, Richard, L. and Erin Anderson. 1994. “An Empirical Test of the
Laughlin, P. R. 1978. “Ability and Group Problem Solving.” Journal of Consequences of Behavior- and Outcome-Based Sales Control Sys-
Research and Development in Education 12:114-120. tems.” Journal of Marketing 58 (October): 53-67.
Loher, B. T., J. B. Vancouver, and S. Chajka. 1994. “Preferences and Re- O’Reilly, C. A. and Jennifer Chatman. 1986. “Organizational Commit-
actions to Teams.” Paper presented at the meeting of the Society for ment and Psychological Attachment: The Effects of Compliance,
Industrial and Organizational Psychology, April, Nashville, TN. Identification, and Internalization on Prosocial Behavior.” Journal of
Lu, Luo and Michael Argyle. 1991. “Happiness and Cooperation.” Per- Applied Psychology 71 (3): 492-499.
sonality and Individual Differences 12:1019-1030. Paulhus, D. 1983. “Sphere-Specific Measures of Perceived Control.”
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 44:1253-1265.
Lucas, George H., Jr., A. Parasuraman, Robert A. Davis, and Ben M.
Petersen, Trond. 1992. “Individual, Collective, and Systems Rationality
Enis. 1987. “An Empirical Study of Salesforce Turnover.” Journal of
in Workgroups: Dilemmas and Market-Type Solutions.” American
Marketing 51 (July): 34-59.
Journal of Sociology 98:469-510.
MacKenzie, Scott B., Philip M. Podsakoff, and Michael Ahearne. 1998. Pinto, Mary Beth, Jeffrey K. Pinto, and John E. Prescott. 1993. “Anteced-
“Some Possible Antecedents and Consequences of In-Role and ents and Consequences of Project Team Cross-Functional Coopera-
Extra-Role Salesperson Performance.” Journal of Marketing 62 tion.” Management Science 39 (October): 1281-1297.
(July): 87-98. Podsakoff, Phillip M., Scott B. MacKenzie, and William H. Bommer.
, , and Julie Beth Paine. 1999. “Do Citizenship Behaviors 1996. “Transformational Leader Behaviors and Substitutes for Lead-
Matter More for Managers Than for Salespeople?” Journal of the ership as Determinants of Employee Satisfaction, Commitment,
Academy of Marketing Science 27 (4): 396-410. Trust, and Organizational Citizenship Behaviors.” Journal of Man-
Mathieu, John E. and Dennis M. Zajac. 1990. “A Review and Meta-Anal- agement 22 (2): 259-298.
ysis of the Antecedents, Correlates, and Consequences of Organiza- , , July Beth Paine, and Daniel G. Bachrach. 2000. “Orga-
tional Commitment.” Psychological Bulletin 108 (2): 171-194. nizational Citizenship Behaviors: A Critical Review of the Theoreti-
Mayer, Roger C., James H. Davis, and David F. Schoorman. 1995. “An cal and Empirical Literature and Suggestions for Future Research.”
Integrative Model of Organizational Trust.” Academy of Manage- Journal of Management 26 (3): 513-552.
ment Review 20 (3): 709-734. Porter, Lyman W., Richard M. Steers, Richard T. Mowday, and P.
McAllister, Daniel J. 1995. “Affect- and Cognition-Based Trust as Foun- Boulian. 1974. “Organizational Commitment, Job Satisfaction, and
dations for Interpersonal Cooperation in Organizations.” Academy of Turnover Among Psychiatric Technicians.” Journal of Applied Psy-
Management Journal 38 (1): 24-59. chology 59:603-609.
McDonald, Gerald W. 1981. “Structural Exchange and Marital Interac- Pritchard, Robert D., Steven D. Jones, Philip L. Roth, Karla K. Stuebing,
tion.” Journal of Marriage and the Family (November):825-839. and Steven E. Ekeberg. 1988. “Effects of Group Feedback, Goal Set-
Yilmaz, Hunt / SALESPERSON COOPERATION 357

ting, and Incentives on Organizational Productivity.” Journal of Ap- Wagner, John A. 1995. “Studies of Individualism-Collectivism: Effects
plied Psychology 73 (2): 337-358. on Cooperation in Groups.” Academy of Management Journal 38 (1):
Pullins, E. Bolman, Leslie M. Fine, and Wendy L. Warren. 1996. “Iden- 152-172.
tifying Peer Mentors in the Sales Force: An Explanatory Investiga-
 and M. K. Moch. 1986. “Individualism-Collectivism: Concept
tion of Willingness and Ability.” Journal of the Academy of
and Measure.” Group and Organization Studies 11 (September):
Marketing Science 24 (2): 125-136.
280-304.
Reichers, A. E. 1985. “A Review and Reconceptualization of Organiza-
tional Commitment.” Academy of Management Review 10:465-476. Weitz, Barton and Kevin D. Bradford. 1999. “Personal Selling and Sales
. 1986. “Conflict and Organizational Commitments.” Journal of Management.” Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science 27 (2):
Applied Psychology 71:508-514. 241-254.
Ring, P. S. and A. Van de Ven. 1994. “Developmental Processes of Coop- Wiener J. Lyle and Tabitha A. Doescher. 1991. “A Framework for Pro-
erative Interorganizational Relationships.” Academy of Management moting Cooperation.” Journal of Marketing 55 (April): 38-47.
Review 19:90-118.
 and . 1994. “Cooperation and Expectations of Coopera-
Roberts, Brent. 1991. “The School Cooperativeness Scale.” Working Pa-
tion.” Journal of Public Policy and Marketing 13 (Fall): 259-270.
per. University of California, Berkeley.
Seabright, Paul. 1993. “Managing the Local Commons: Theoretical Is- Williamson, Oliver E. 1975. Markets and Hierarchies: Analysis and An-
sues in Incentive Design.” Journal of Economic Perspectives 7 (Fall): titrust Implications. New York: Free Press.
113-134.
Schletzer, V. 1965. “A Study of the Predictive Effectiveness of the Strong
Vocational Interest Blank for Job Satisfaction.” Unpublished doctoral
dissertation. University of Minnesota, Duluth. ABOUT THE AUTHORS
Shapiro, B. P. 1977. “Can Marketing and Manufacturing Co-Exist?”
Harvard Business Review 55:104-114.
Smith, J. Brock and Donald W. Barclay. 1993. “Team Selling Effective- Cengiz Yilmaz is an assistant professor of marketing at Gebze
ness: A Small Group Perspective.” Journal of Business-to-Business Institute of Technology, Turkey. He obtained his Ph.D. in market-
Marketing 1 (2): 3-31. ing from Texas Tech University in 1999. His research interests
 and . 1997. “The Effects of Organizational Differences focus on sales management, distribution channels and relation-
and Trust on the Effectiveness of Selling Partner Relationships.” ship marketing, and strategic issues concerning intra- and
Journal of Marketing 61 (January): 3-21.
Smith, Ken G., Susan J. Carroll, and Stephen J. Ashford. 1995. “Intra- interfirm aspects in marketing systems and their links with busi-
and Interorganizational Cooperation: Toward a Research Agenda.” ness performance. His research has been published in various
Academy of Management Journal 38 (1): 7-23. conference proceedings.
Spicer, M. W. 1985. “A Public Choice Approach to Motivating People in
Bureaucratic Organizations.” Academy of Management Review
10:518-526. Shelby D. Hunt is the J. B. Hoskins and P. W. Horn Professor of
Steiner, I. D. 1972. Group Processes and Productivity. Orlando, FL: Aca- Marketing at Texas Tech University, Lubbock, Texas. A past edi-
demic Press. tor of the Journal of Marketing (1985-1987), he is the author of
Thibaut, John W. and Harold H. Kelley. 1959. The Social Psychology of Modern Marketing Theory: Critical Issues in the Philosophy of
Groups. New York: John Wiley.
Thompson, J. D. 1967. Organizations in Action. New York: McGraw- Marketing Science (South-Western, 1991) and A General Theory
Hill. of Competition: Resources, Competences, Productivity, Eco-
Thorne, A. 1987. “The Press of Personality: A Study of Conversation Be- nomic Growth (Sage Publications, 2000). He has written numer-
tween Introverts and Extroverts.” Journal of Personality and Social ous articles on competitive theory, macromarketing, ethics,
Psychology 53:718-726.
Tjosvold, Dean. 1984. “Cooperation Theory and Organizations.” Human
channels of distribution, philosophy of science, and marketing
Relations 37 (9): 743-767. theory. Three of his Journal of Marketing articles, “The Nature
. 1986. “The Dynamics of Interdependence in Organizations.” and Scope of Marketing” (1976), “General Theories and Funda-
Human Relations 39 (6): 517-540. mental Explananda of Marketing” (1983), and “The Compara-
 and Yuan Tsao. 1989. “Productive Organizational Collaboration: tive Advantage Theory of Competition” (1995) (with Robert M.
The Roles of Values and Cooperation.” Journal of Organizational Be- Morgan) won the Harold H. Maynard Award for the “best article
havior 10 (2): 189-195.
Tompson, Holly B. and Jon M. Werner. 1997. “The Impact of Role Con- on marketing theory.” His 1985 Journal of Business Research ar-
flict/Facilitation on Core and Discretionary Behaviors: Testing a Me- ticle with Lawrence B. Chonko, “Ethics and Marketing Manage-
diated Model.” Journal of Management 23 (4): 583-601. ment,” received the 2000 Elsevier Science Exceptional Quality
Vancouver, J. B. and D. R. Ilgen. 1989. “Effects of Interpersonal Orienta- and High Scholarly Impact Award. His 1989 article, “Reification
tion and the Sex-Type of the Task on Choosing to Work Alone in
Groups.” Journal of Applied Psychology 74:927-934.
and Realism in Marketing: In Defense of Reason,” won the Jour-
Van De Ven, Andrew H., Andre L. Delbecq, and Richard Koenig. 1976. nal of Macromarketing Charles C. Slater Award. For his contri-
“Determination of Coordination Modes Within Organizations.” butions to theory and science in marketing, he received the 1986
American Journal of Sociology 41 (April): 322-338. Paul D. Converse Award from the American Marketing Associa-
Wageman, Ruth. 1995. “Interdependence and Group Effectiveness.” Ad- tion, the 1987 Outstanding Marketing Educator Award from the
ministrative Science Quarterly 40 (1): 145-180.
 and George Baker. 1997. “Incentives and Cooperation: The Joint Academy of Marketing Science, and the 1992 American Mar-
Effects of Task and Reward Interdependence on Group Perfor- keting Association/Richard D. Irwin Distinguished Marketing
mance.” Journal of Organizational Behavior 18:139-158. Educator Award.
JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY
Sarkar et al.
OF/ ALLIANCE
MARKETINGPERFORMANCE
SCIENCE FALL 2001

The Influence of Complementarity,


Compatibility, and Relationship
Capital on Alliance Performance
MB Sarkar
Raj Echambadi
University of Central Florida

S. Tamer Cavusgil
Michigan State University

Preet S. Aulakh
Temple University

Value creation through alliances requires the simulta- organizations to cope with the discontinuities created by a
neous pursuit of partners with similar characteristics on volatile, interdependent, and information-intensive global
certain dimensions and different characteristics on other economy. The quest for sustainable competitive advantage
dimensions. Partnering firms need to have different re- through competing and collaborating simultaneously in
source and capability profiles yet share similarities in global markets (Lado, Boyd, and Hanlon 1997) has pro-
their social institutions. In this article, the authors empiri- pelled the formation of collaborative relationships that
cally examine the impact of partner characteristics on the exhibit governance structures very distinct from tradi-
performance of alliances. In particular, they test hypothe- tional vertically integrated forms. However, along with the
ses related to both direct impact of partner characteristics proliferation of alliances has come the realization that
on alliance performance and indirect effects through rela- many alliances underperform and fail to deliver results rel-
tional capital aspects of the alliance. Empirical results ative to expectations or potential (Madhok and Tallman
based on a sample of alliances in the global construction 1998).
contracting industry suggest that complementarity in Recent research suggests that the success of both
partner resources and compatibility in cultural and opera- domestic and cross-border collaborations may be a func-
tional norms have different direct and indirect effects on tion of partner characteristics (Hitt, Dacin, Levitas,
alliance performance. Accordingly, organizational rou- Arregle, and Borza 2000; Madhok 1995; Saxton 1997).
tines aimed at partner selection need to be complemented However, as Hitt et al. (2000) note, further research is
by relationship management routines to maximize the po- required in this area of inquiry. In this article, we address
tential benefits from an alliance. how different types of interfirm diversity among partners
(Parkhe 1991) affect the performance of alliances. We sug-
gest that the issue of partner selection presents firms with a
potential paradox, wherein seemingly contradictory ele-
The rise of hybrid organizational forms, or interfirm ments need to coexist and be simultaneously achieved to
alliances (Borys and Jemison 1989), reflects attempts by convey a more illuminating “insight into truth than either
Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science.
can muster in its own right” (Slaate 1968:4). Specifically,
Volume 29, No. 4, pages 358-373. we suggest that collaborative value creation requires the
Copyright © 2001 by Academy of Marketing Science. pursuit of partners who possess similar characteristics on
Sarkar et al. / ALLIANCE PERFORMANCE 359

certain dimensions and dissimilar and/or complementary environment that facilitates the achievement of joint goals
characteristics on other dimensions. Value generated from and objectives (Aulakh et al. 1996; Parkhe 1993).
alliances is enhanced when partners have different Interactive theorists (Heide and Miner 1992), on the
resource and capability profiles yet share similarities in other hand, focus on the “pattern of interaction that facili-
their social institutions. These partner characteristics are tates and allows for the effective functioning of the alli-
important since they help in the formation of relationship ance on a day-to-day basis” and various sociopsychologi-
capital or the behavioral and sociopsychological aspects of cal factors that help create relationship capital (Cullen
an alliance that find expression in relational dynamics et al. 2000:224). Complementing the structural approach
such as mutual trust, commitment, and information by explicitly considering the sociopsychological dimen-
exchange (Cullen, Johnson, and Sakano, 2000; Heide and sions, this stream highlights behavioral issues relevant to
John 1992). the development and maintenance of relationships
Our empirical study is set in the global construction (Bradach and Eccles 1989; Johnson, Cullen, Sakano, and
contracting industry. This industry is particularly appro- Takenouchi 1996; Madhok 1995). The key premise here is
priate since project-based alliances play a crucial role in that cooperative behavior springs from the development of
this industry. The literature on interorganizational collabo- relational capital between partners, which is critical in
rations has been criticized for its relatively narrow focus transforming the potential value of an alliance into actual-
on equity-based joint ventures and for ignoring loosely ized collaborative economic rents (Madhok and Tallman
structured alliances where separate legal entities are not 1998).
formed (Cullen, Johnson, and Sakano 1995). In such alli- In essence, extant research housed in multiple disci-
ances, partner characteristics and relationship-based gov- plines and using different theoretical lenses suggests an
ernance mechanisms are likely to assume greater salience association between structural aspects of partners (their
as coordinating mechanisms since bureaucratic lines of diversity in resource profiles and social compatibility),
control are typically absent. In this regard, the construction sociopsychological issues (relationship capital), and
contracting industry offers an appropriate context. effective collaborations (performance of alliances). Some
The layout of the article is as follows. We start with a lit- past research has shown (Heide 1994; Johnson et al. 1996)
erature review and then present our case for considering that structural and relational aspects are related, suggest-
two types of interfirm diversity. Thereafter, we present our ing the possibility that structural aspects affect alliance
model and our hypotheses, describe data collection con- performance both directly and indirectly through the
text and procedures, and discuss our methods. We then test sociopsychological aspects of a relationship. However, the
our model and end with a Discussion and Implications interrelationships between these variables and how they
section. affect performance are less clear. For example, it is unclear
whether interfirm diversity affects performance, and if it
does, whether it influences performance directly, indi-
LITERATURE REVIEW rectly through relationship capital, or both. Furthermore,
do different aspects of partner characteristics have an
The importance of interfirm collaborations is reflected impact on different performance dimensions differently?
in a virtual explosion of interdisciplinary research on this We investigate this nexus between interfirm diversity
topic. Existing research has examined structural and and alliance performance. In our conceptualization, we
sociopsychological aspects of collaborations to better draw on Parkhe’s (1991) conceptualization of interfirm
understand issues related to performance of alliances diversity in terms of Type I diversity (complementary
(Aulakh, Kotabe, and Sahay 1996; Parkhe 1993). Struc- resources and capability profiles) and Type II diversity
tural aspects focus on the ex ante aspects of partnerships. (social dimensions). Type I diversity deals with the recip-
This includes investigating why firms enter into alliances rocal strengths of the partners and relates to differences in
(Hagedoorn 1993), partner selection criteria, and owner- their skills, resources, and capabilities that generate alli-
ship control issues. While this research stream provides ances in a search for synergy. The complementarity of
important insights into the structuring of partnerships resources, which is the raison d’être of any alliance, cre-
(Aulakh et al. 1996), an underlying premise here is that ates mutual interdependency and “facilitates the forma-
effective interorganizational alliances are associated with tion, development and collaborative effectiveness” of alli-
selection of appropriate partners since choosing partners ances (Parkhe 1991:580). On the other hand, alliances are
who possess necessary resources and with whom strategic “socially contrived mechanisms for collective action,
and economic incentives can be aligned is a critical deter- which are continually shaped and restructured by actions
minant of partnering success. This perspective suggests and symbolic interpretations of the parties involved”
that the resource-based interdependence between partner (Ring and Van de Ven 1994:96). Accordingly, Type II
firms, as well as their social compatibility, creates an diversity refers to interorganizational cultural and pro-
360 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

cessual differences between social actors participating in Our rationale is as follows: it has been noted that alliances
the alliance (Parkhe 1991). Dissimilarities between social provide a focal firm with two streams of economic rents.
actors can negatively affect the quality of interactions in a First, alliances create common or shared benefits that ac-
partnership and thus hinder the complex integration and crue collectively to alliance participants (Khanna 1998).
transformation of disparate pools of tacit know-how into Second, collaborations create rents indirectly, as when a
value creation. firm picks up skills from a specific partner and applies
In addition, given that “the production of a collective them to its operations in areas unrelated to the activities of
good is inextricably intertwined with the underlying the specific alliance. Conceptualized as private benefits,
dynamics of exchange” (Madhok and Tallman 1998:327), these accrue when externalities from an alliance generate
behavioral aspects of alliances need to be considered. In value in a firm’s operational domain that falls outside the
fact, the genesis of interfirm cooperation is based on the focal collaboration (Khanna, Gulati, and Nohria 1998).
premise that competitive advantage accrues to firms that The first measure, project performance, relates to the for-
can successfully transcend transaction-based exchange mer, while the latter (i.e., strategic performance) refers to
and develop long-term cooperative relationships. A grow- the private benefits that accrue to a focal firm through stra-
ing body of relationship marketing literature has con- tegic learning-related benefits.
cluded similarly. Researchers have questioned the domi-
nant paradigm of the discrete transaction and have posited Type I interfirm diversity: Resource complementarity.
that interfirm exchanges take place in a context of continu- Research indicates that resource complementarity is cru-
ity where relational constructs such as trust and commit- cial to collaborative success (Bleeke and Ernst 1991;
ment are key (J. Anderson and Narus 1990; Bucklin and Harrigan 1985). As noted by Johnson et al. (1996), re-
Sengupta 1993; Gundlach, Achrol, and Mentzer 1995; source complementarity involves both uniqueness and
Heide and John 1992; Morgan and Hunt 1994). Consistent symmetry. On one hand, complementarity determines the
with this literature, we believe that relational aspects medi- mix of unique and valuable resources available to achieve
ate the relationship between interfirm diversity and collab- strategic objectives (Killing 1983), thus enhancing com-
oration performance. petitive viability of the alliance. On the other, comple-
mentarity implies strategic symmetry, wherein a balanced
share of unique strengths creates partner interdependence
HYPOTHESES (Harrigan 1985). We conceptualize resource comple-
mentarity as the extent to which each partner brings in
In this section, we first articulate the two types of unique strengths and resources of value to the collabora-
interfirm diversity, namely, resource complementarity (or tion (Johnson et al. 1996).
Parkhe’s Type I interfirm diversity) and cultural and opera- From a resource-based view (RBV) (Wernerfelt 1984),
tional compatibility (or Parkhe’s Type II interfirm diver- differential firm performance is due to heterogeneity and
sity) and examine the rationale for their direct effects on imperfect mobility of resource and capabilities (Barney
performance.1 Thereafter, we examine the mediating 1991). The two perspectives within the RBV, namely, the
effects of relational-capital variables (mutual trust, recip- static and dynamic views (Lado et al. 1997), differ in their
rocal commitment, and bilateral information exchange) on explanation regarding how economic rents are generated
the relationship between interfirm diversity and perfor- and sustained. The static view emphasizes sustainability
mance. Our conceptual model is presented in Figure 1. as accruing from unique firm resources in a state of equi-
librium. For example, Barney’s (1991) argument that
Relationships Between Interfirm resources need to be rare, inimitable, valuable, and
Diversity and Performance nonsubstitutable for sustained competitive advantage is
rooted in the static view. On the other hand, arguing that
Performance. While researchers have used financial, changing environments reduce the rent-creating ability of
survival (Killing 1983), duration (Kogut 1988), and own- most resources, the dynamic RBV emphasizes flow and
ership instability (Gomes-Casseres 1987) as measures of the dynamic accumulation of capabilities, rather than
collaborative performance, Geringer and Hebert (1989) static resource stocks (Dierickx and Cool 1989). Together,
note the lack of consensus regarding an appropriate defini- they suggest that both stock and flow of resources and
tion and measure of the construct. Given the multi- capabilities are crucial to performance.
dimensionality of the concept and the empirical setting, Furthermore, it has been noted that many skills and
we use two perceptual measures of performance—one re- resources required for sustained competitiveness and sur-
lated to the economic performance of the venture, namely, vival are transorganizational in nature, in that they are resi-
project performance, and the other related to the strategic dent outside a focal firm’s boundaries and direct control
aspects of the relationship, namely, strategic performance. (Achrol 1997) and are thus accessible only through
Sarkar et al. / ALLIANCE PERFORMANCE 361

FIGURE 1
A Conceptual Model of the Role of Complementarity, Compatibility,
and Relationship Capital on Alliance Performance
Inter-firm Diversity/ Relationship Capital Performance
Compatibility

H1a(+), H2a(+), H3a(+)


Project
H4a(+) Performance
Resource
Complementarity H5a(+)
H6a(+) H7a(+), H8a(+),
(H1,H4) H9a(+)
Mutual
Trust

Cultural H4b(+) Reciprocal


Compatibility H5b(+) Commitment
(H2,H5) H6b(+)

Bilateral
Information
Operational Exchange
H4c(+)
Compatibility H5c(+) H7b(+), H8b(+),
(H3,H6) H6c(+) H9b(+)
Strategic
Performance

H1b(+), H2b(+), H3b(+)

collaborations. Therefore, extending the atomistic firm nonredundancy in knowledge bases. Accordingly, we
view that is prevalent in much of RBV literature, it is argue that the potential for partners to synergistically le-
argued that organizations that are able to access and use verage the pooled resources and capabilities in the market-
complementary transorganizational strategic assets2 place would increase with resource complementarity. In
(TSA) through alliances would realize strategic advan- other words, when partners bring in unique and valuable
tages in both the stock of resources that they possess and strengths and resources, both the learning aspects of the al-
the flow of learning that can enhance their capabilities liance, as well as the performance of the project for which
(Dyer and Singh 1998; Lado et al. 1997). the alliance has been created, are likely to be enhanced.
First, by pooling complementary resources and capa- Thus,
bilities, firms can initiate and perform competitively on
Hypothesis 1a-b: Resource complementarity between
projects that they could not have done alone (Harrigan
partners will be positively associated with (a) pro-
1985). Accessing complementary resources through mar- ject and (b) strategic performance.
ket mechanisms is not always feasible, nor is internal de-
velopment (Chung, Singh, and Lee 2000; Sarkar, Type II interfirm diversity: Cultural and operational
Echambadi, and Harrison 2001). For example, in the pres- compatibility. The effect of partner compatibility on creat-
ent context of the construction industry, no single com- ing value through alliances has been noted (Madhok
pany has the complete array of resources to individually 1995). Compatibility, or the congruence in organizational
develop and deliver projects and to fully absorb the magni- cultures and capabilities between alliance partners, influ-
tude of risk. Second, interactive learning opportunities that ences the extent to which partners are able to realize the
help firms add to their capabilities and know-how are synergistic potential of an alliance (Madhok and Tallman
likely to be greater in cases where there is diversity and 1998). While alliance formation is an outcome of per-
362 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

ceived recognition of potential benefits that can accrue if The Mediating Role of Relationship Capital
resources and capabilities are pooled, the actualization of
this collaborative potential is generated through the dy- Besides the direct impact of structural factors (resource
namic process of interaction and integration of the part- complementarity and partner compatibilities) on alliance
ners’ resource bases and the effectiveness with which the performance, there is evidence that partner characteristics
partners succeed in moving away from a market-based ex- indirectly affect performance through certain mediating
change toward a mutually oriented cooperative relation- behavioral variables. Researchers have argued and found
ship (Koza and Lewin 1998). empirical support for (a) the effect of relationship-capital
In this context, it has been noted that compatibility, or a variables on alliance outcomes (e.g., Aulakh et al. 1996;
similarity in outlook and objectives, rather than ownership Bradrach and Eccles 1989) and (b) links between partner
arrangements, makes for success in equity-based alliances characteristics and relationship capital (Cullen et al. 1995;
(Friedmann and Beguin 1971). Similar organizational val- Morgan and Hunt 1994; Stump and Heide 1996). The
ues reduce coordination costs between collaborating orga- sociopsychological aspects embodied in relationship capi-
nizations and serve as a means for behavioral control (Das tal are important since they act as coordinating mecha-
and Teng 1998) and expectation management (Chung nisms and determine the quality of the relationship in the
et al. 2000). On the other hand, incompatibility among collaboration. In fact, it has been suggested that interfirm
partners may lead to a counterproductive working rela- cooperation can lead to competitive advantage only when
tionship characterized by strife and suspicion. Social firms transcend transaction-based exchange and develop
incompatibility may lead to an inability on the part of the long-term relationships (Dyer and Singh 1998). We con-
partners to develop a harmonious relationship and thus sider three key aspects of relationship capital, namely,
negatively influence collaborative effectiveness (Sarkar, mutual trust, mutual commitment, and information
Cavusgil, and Evirgen 1997). For example, it has been exchange, which have been highlighted in the literature as
noted that cultural clash has caused many mergers and factors that differentiate relationship-based practices from
acquisitions to fail due to the inability of the two entities to arm’s-length exchange (Heide and John 1992; Morgan and
work seamlessly (Wilkof, Brown, and Selsky 1995). Hunt 1994).
Higher levels of stress result when organizations that are Mutual trust. Interfirm trust, which has been described
essentially incompatible in their values, norms, and capa- as a “fundamental relationship building block” (Wilson
bilities attempt to blend their organizational cultures in an 1995:337) and as a critical element of economic exchange
alliance (Das and Teng 1998). Organizational differences (Ring 1996), is argued to be essential to the development
hinder role socialization (Smith and Barclay 1997), thus of enduring alliances (Aulakh et al. 1996; Johnson et al.
making it more difficult for interfacing managers to work 1996). In economic exchange, trust implies a general ex-
together. pectation of good faith efforts by parties to honor commit-
Therefore, there appears theoretical and empirical sup- ments, to be honest in negotiations, and to decry
port behind the idea that organizational compatibility in opportunistic behavior (see Hosmer 1995). An extensive
various domains has a positive effect on alliance perfor- literature (see Ring 1996) points toward two issues rele-
mance. We conceptualize Type II diversity or organiza- vant to this study: (1) that interfirm trust needs to be char-
tional compatibility along two dimensions, namely, acterized by the “property of bilateral expectations”
cultural compatibility and operational compatibility. Cul- (Aulakh et al. 1996:1008) and mutuality (E. Anderson and
tural compatibility refers to the congruence in organiza- Weitz 1989) and (2) that trust has both cognitive and be-
tional philosophies, goals, and values. The second, havioral components (Moorman, Zaltman, and Deshpande
operational compatibility, addresses the extent of congru- 1992). Consistent with this literature, we conceptualize
ence in the partners’ procedural capabilities. Therefore, mutual trust in a partnership as the degree of confidence
while the first dimension addresses broad issues related to shared by the partners regarding each other’s integrity
organizational norms and value systems, operational com- (Aulakh et al. 1996).
patibility relates to status similarity on capability and
processual issues that assume salience in the context of a Reciprocal commitment. Commitment, or “an ex-
working relationship. Thus, we posit that both forms of change partner believing that an ongoing relationship with
compatibility will directly affect performance. another is so important as to warrant maximum efforts at
maintaining it . . . to ensure that it endures indefinitely”
Hypothesis 2a-b: Cultural compatibility of the partners (Morgan and Hunt 1994:23), is a critical element of rela-
will be positively associated with (a) project and (b) tionship capital (Madhok 1995). This “enduring desire to
strategic performance. maintain a relationship” (Moorman et al. 1992:316) in-
Hypothesis 3a-b: Operational compatibility of the part- volves a long-term orientation such that partners restrict
ners will be positively associated with (a) project their search for alternatives and forego better short-term
and (b) strategic performance. options in favor of strengthening an ongoing relationship
Sarkar et al. / ALLIANCE PERFORMANCE 363

(Dwyer, Schurr, and Oh 1987). These continuity expecta- the generation of relational norms (Macneil 1980). The
tions influence partners to make relationship-specific in- condition of dependence that results from Type I diversity,
vestments that, on one hand, demonstrate their reliability where both partners need each other’s resources and capa-
and commitment to their exchange partner, and on the bilities, represents a mutual safeguard and thus a joint
other, enhance the competitiveness of the alliance (E. An- incentive to create relationship capital in the collaboration
derson and Weitz 1992). Consistent with literature, we be- (Oliver 1990). The resulting strategic symmetry facilitates
lieve that it is not the act of commitment alone but also the formation and development of cooperative norms
rather the structure of commitment that fashions relation- (Madhok 1995).
ship quality (E. Anderson and Weitz 1992). Accordingly, Evans’s (1963) similarity hypothesis suggests that in a
we conceptualize reciprocal commitment as the degree to dyadic relationship, the degree of similarity is positively
which both partners are willing to invest requisite re- associated with favorable relationship outcomes. Similar-
sources into the alliance (Gulati, Khanna, and Nohria ity in values serves as a base for social relationships, which
1994). lie at the heart of social interaction processes. The resul-
tant bonds create stability in the relationship and greater
Bilateral information exchange. Collaborative commu-
levels of tolerance through a “social ‘glue’ [that helps] to
nication serves as a pseudointegrating device since it helps
tide over temporary periods of disequilibrium” (Madhok
align partners’ interests and values (Mohr, Fisher, and
1995:121). Type II diversity or organizational compatibil-
Nevin 1996). Acting as a bonding mechanism between
ity, by facilitating a sense of unity and congeniality in the
partners, the exchange of timely, quality, and participative
relationship, is thus likely to foster relational-capital-
communication is argued to be vital to successful collabo-
building behaviors among partners. This would be espe-
rations (Mohr and Nevin 1990). In fact, in the marketing
cially pertinent in international alliances where cultural
literature, it has been described as “the glue that holds to-
differences are likely to exist. Accordingly,
gether a channel relationship” (Mohr and Nevin 1990:36)
and that facilitates the realization of mutual benefits by al- Hypothesis 4a-c: Resource complementarity will be pos-
lowing exchange of necessary information and by reduc- itively associated with (a) mutual trust, (b) recipro-
ing misunderstandings and uncertainty (Dwyer et al. cal commitment, and (c) bilateral information
1987). We conceptualize reciprocal information exchange exchange.
in terms of formal and informal communication of mean- Hypothesis 5a-c: Cultural compatibility will be posi-
ingful and timely information (J. Anderson and Narus tively associated with (a) mutual trust, (b) reciprocal
1990). In doing so, we are consistent with existing litera- commitment, and (c) bilateral information ex-
ture that has conceptualized collaborative communication change.
in terms of its quality, extent, and participation (Mohr and Hypothesis 6a-c: Operational compatibility will be posi-
Spekman 1994). tively associated with (a) mutual trust, (b) reciprocal
commitment, and (c) bilateral information ex-
Relationship capital is likely to be fostered when part- change.
ners perceive a high level of complementarity and compat-
ibility. The principle of reciprocity implies that actions are Relationship Between Relational
contingent on the potential of rewarding reactions from Capital and Performance
others. Thus, in relationships where partners need each
other’s resources and where reciprocal needs exist, part- Existing literature identifies various interrelated ways
ners are less likely to resort to opportunism. The resource in which mutual trust affects interfirm exchanges. Mutual
interdependence created through Type I diversity is likely trust acts as a substitute for hierarchical governance and
to result in reciprocity and thus reduce incentives for assumes added significance where formal ownership-
opportunistic behavior, as both partners perceive value in based governance is absent (Dwyer et al. 1987). Trust al-
their relationship (Morgan and Hunt 1994; Stump and lows for bilateral governance through joint accomplish-
Heide 1996). Resource-interdependent partners are more ments, shared beliefs, and mutual concern (Heide 1994).
likely to be motivated to create relationship capital by Mutual trust also deters opportunistic behavior (Bradach
engaging in trustworthy acts that increase their vulnerabil- and Eccles 1989) in favor of long-term gains. The motiva-
ity to each other, signaling their expectations of continuity tion for opportunistic behavior is reduced because “behav-
and solidarity to the relationship by committing relation- ioral repertoires are biased toward cooperation” (Hill
ship-specific resources and maintaining open and 1990:511). Finally, “trust has efficiency implications, and
participative lines of communication. its potential cost reduction and value enhancing properties
Furthermore, symmetric dependence motivates both need to be recognized” (Madhok 1995:126). There is evi-
parties to jointly show forbearance (Williamson 1991) or dence that trust has important implications for market per-
flexibility in response to changing circumstances, which formance and efficiency (Aulakh et al. 1996; Bleeke and
in turn has the effect of preserving the relationship through Ernst 1991). This occurs because of reduced costs of mon-
364 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

itoring and the ability of partners to engage in the complex 1996) and thus positively affects collaboration perfor-
process of integrating their disparate tacit resources and mance (Badaracco 1991). Information asymmetry and
capabilities effectively when the relationship is character- participatory imbalance create an environment prone to
ized by mutual trust (Dyer and Singh 1998). Accordingly, opportunism and power imbalances, whereas shared
power and participative decision-making are characteris-
Hypothesis 7a-b: Mutual trust will be positively associ- tics of successful alliances (Bucklin and Sengupta 1993).
ated with (a) project and (b) strategic performance. In other words, participative and frequent exchange of in-
formation and maintaining open-door policies to each
The positive effect of commitment on collaborative other result from a willingness of the partners to create
performance has been widely reported (Badaracco 1991; transparency in the relationship.
Gundlach et al. 1995). Given that the competitive advan-
tage of any alliance derives from the mix of skills and other Hypothesis 9a-b: Bilateral information exchange will be
resources that it commands, it is likely that commitment of positively associated with (a) project and (b) strate-
resources by partners has a positive effect on financial per- gic performance.
formance. Furthermore, Gulati et al. (1994) argue that bi-
lateral commitment of resources moves alliances from
METHOD
win-lose situations to win-win situations, thus suggesting
that reciprocal commitment is likely to enhance partners’
perceptions of how successful the relationship has been. Sample Selection and Data Collection
Reciprocal commitment of inputs leads to stable long-
term relationships through aligning incentive structures The sampling frame included 561 firms in the interna-
and enhancing confidence in each other (Williamson tional construction contracting industry. Engineering
1985). By reducing the threat of opportunistic behavior News Record (ENR), a McGraw-Hill published weekly
and increasing the cost of dissolution, commitments by trade journal for the construction contracting industry,
both parties act as powerful signals of relationship quality. periodically collects and publishes industry data. The
Long-term relationships reduce search and start-up costs uniqueness of the construction industry lies in its charac-
of frequently dealing with new parties. Along with result- teristics. First, each construction product is different in
ing economies of learning costs and experience effects, terms of specifications, scope, and requirements. The
they also require simpler governance structures, monitor- potential to standardize complex construction projects is
ing systems, and “provide a host of efficiencies” limited due to the individualized preferences of the project
(Gundlach et al. 1995:80). Also, the “lock-in” effect of re- owners, dissimilarities of the project locations, and the
ciprocal commitment (Katz 1989) promotes behavior that advancement of technology. As a corollary, most construc-
ensures the continuance of the relationship since the quasi- tion firms tend to be specialized in certain product niches.
rents that are generated through the relationship would be Second, there is no centralized or fixed point of production
lost to both parties in the event of termination (Heide (Eccles 1981). The construction product is spatially bound
1994). Accordingly, to a single site, which happens to be both the point of pro-
duction and consumption of the product. Third, variability
Hypothesis 8a-b: Reciprocal commitment will be posi- in location leads to the requirement of resource mobility.
tively associated with (a) project and (b) strategic Equipment, vehicles, materials, and labor need to be
performance. mobile to be able to move from one site to another for dif-
ferent projects. Fourth, both the degree of competition and
It has been noted that healthy interfirm collaborations the competitive structure of this industry have dramati-
are characterized by open communication, accessibility, cally changed over recent years as U.S. dominance is
availability, information flows, and a sense of participa- threatened by firms from within the Triad and from the
tion and involvement in the relationship (Mohr and Nevin newly industrialized countries (NICs) and some develop-
1990; Mohr et al. 1996). These attributes create transpar- ing countries (Yates, Mukherjee, and Njos 1999).
ency in the relationship and signal a mutual willingness to International alliances are very common in this indus-
increase vulnerability to each other. Furthermore, commu- try, where the complexity of projects makes it imperative
nication facilitates the realization of mutual benefits by al- for firms to collaborate on various modules with other spe-
lowing exchange of necessary information and by cialist firms. Typically, the alliances are loosely structured
reducing misunderstandings and uncertainty (Dwyer et al. and not equity based, thus making it particularly appropri-
1987; Mohr and Nevin 1990). Mutual disclosure ensues ate in the wake of Cullen et al.’s (1995) critique that much
from a norm of information exchange (Heide and John of the collaboration literature focuses on equity-based
1992) and helps volitional compliance between partners. It joint ventures and ignores alliances where separate legal
highlights shared interests and common goals (Mohr et al. entities are not formed. The globalized nature of this
Sarkar et al. / ALLIANCE PERFORMANCE 365

industry, its reliance on international collaborations, and item scale that measured the perceived levels of similarity
its understudied nature motivated us to set the empirical and congruence in organizational norms and values
part of this study here. (Dwyer et al. 1987; Heide and John 1992; Morgan and
We followed a systematic approach toward pretesting, Hunt 1994) and mutual appreciation of each other’s goals
refining, and validating our scales. In-depth semistruc- and objectives. Operational compatibility was operation-
tured interviews, lasting about 1 hour each, were con- alized through a three-item scale that measured the level of
ducted with 12 experts from industry associations and con- congruence in the partners’ managerial skills, organiza-
struction contracting firms to get insights into industry tional procedures, and technical capabilities (Sarkar et al.
dynamics and to ensure that conceptual constructs were 1997; Wilson 1995).
grounded in reality. Following the process of adapting Relationship capital. Mutual trust was measured
existing measures and adapting developing new ones, we through a four-item scale that assessed the perceived level
pilot tested the survey instrument on these industry execu- of moral integrity, fairness, and dependability in the rela-
tives to eliminate ambiguous scale items. Using this feed- tionship. Reciprocal commitment was measured through a
back, a revised instrument was developed, and feedback three-item scale adapted from E. Anderson and Weitz
was obtained from academic experts regarding item clar- (1992) and the organizational-commitment literature. The
ity. The questionnaire was subsequently modified and scale measured the mutual willingness of each partner to
finalized through an iterative process. invest required resources into the relationship. Bilateral
We derived our sampling frame from the 1993 list of the information exchange was measured through a four-item
top 225 international contractors and the top 400 U.S. con- scale adapted from Heide and John (1992). This construct
tractors. Since some of the U.S. contractors were included tapped the extent to which partners exchanged and shared
in both lists, the total number of firms that emerged was information through face-to-face and mediated interaction.
561. Data were collected using a self-administered ques-
tionnaire prepared in English. The survey cover page elab- Performance. We developed a three-item scale for stra-
orated the fact that the survey was about nonequity collab- tegic performance and a four-item scale for project perfor-
orative ventures with other contractors, and not about mance. We measured strategic performance through items
relationships involving contractors and project owners. that assessed the degree to which the relationship met stra-
Furthermore, respondents were requested to answer rele- tegic and learning objectives. The latter were measured
vant questions in the context of a collaborative venture that through perceived profitability, efficiency, client satisfac-
they were most familiar with and that involved an interna- tion, and quality of the project.
tional partner. It was elaborated that the project should be
one that either has been recently completed or is nearing Measurement Model
completion. Sixty-eight usable questionnaires were
returned, making the effective response rate 12.3 percent. We used Partial Least Squares (PLS) version 3.0 to esti-
We received responses from firms in 18 countries other mate our causal model. PLS, also called “soft modeling”
than the United States.3 To assess nonresponse bias, the (Lohmöller 1989), estimates latent variables as exact lin-
responses were divided into two groups based on the date ear combinations of observed measures and therefore
on which they were received. The two groups of early and assumes that all measured variance is useful variance to be
late responders were then compared on their annual bill- explained. PLS makes minimal demands on sample size
ings (Armstrong and Overton 1977). The results indicate (Barclay and Smith 1997), thus making it especially
no significant difference between these groups. appropriate for testing structural models with relatively
smaller sample sizes.4
Measurement Although PLS estimates both factor loadings and struc-
tural paths simultaneously, we followed the procedure
All the items used to measure the constructs were advocated by Hulland (1999) in evaluating PLS models.
closed-ended with 5-point Likert-type scales of strongly The estimated model was analyzed and interpreted in two
agree to strongly disagree. stages: (a) the assessment and reliability of the measure-
ment model and (b) the testing of the structural model. We
Type I and II interfirm diversity. Resource comple- assessed the adequacy of the measurement model through
mentarity was measured through a three-item scale that examining individual-item reliabilities, the convergent
was adapted from J. Anderson and Narus (1990) into a pro- validity of the measures associated with each construct,
ject setting. The items tapped the level of resource interde- and assessing their discriminant validity.
pendence in the relationship by measuring the extent to We first assessed individual-item reliabilities by exam-
which both partners perceived the value of resources and ining loadings of the measures on their respective con-
capabilities that the other brought to the relationship. Cul- structs. A rule of thumb is to check for loadings of .70 or
tural compatibility was operationalized through a four- more (which implies a shared variance of 50% or greater
366 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

between the item and the construct). An examination of overall comprehensive causal model: interfirm diversity
the initial measurement model revealed that of the 28 leading to relationship capital, subsequently leading to
items, 20 had loadings greater than .7, 5 items had loadings performance. The direct effects of the various exogenous
greater than .65, and 3 items had loadings of less than .5. constructs on performance are given in the first column.
These 3 items with poor loadings were removed from sub- The indirect effects (reported in column 2) of interfirm
sequent analysis. Table 1 provides the final list of individ- diversity on performance through relationship-capital
ual items used in the analysis and their loadings. Overall, variables were computed using a hand-calculable signifi-
these statistics are above the cutoff suggested by Hulland cance test proposed by Baron and Kenny (1986).7
(1999) and indicate that all of our items demonstrate good
individual-item reliabilities.
Next, we focused on assessing the construct validity of RESULTS
our constructs by computing the composite reliabilities.
We used the internal consistency measure developed by Direct effects. An examination of the R2 values reveal
Fornell and Larcker (1981),5 who argue that their measure that the variance explained in endogenous constructs
of internal consistency is superior to Cronbach’s alpha ranges from .15 to .55. The results indicate that resource
since the loadings estimated within the causal model are complementarity is related to project performance (β =
used in its computation. The internal consistency values .22, p < .05) but not to strategic performance (β = .09, p >
for the constructs are reported in Table 2. All constructs .05). Hypothesis 1a is thus supported, but not Hypothesis
exhibit composite reliabilities of .7 or more, thus indicat- 1b. Results further indicate that while the direct effect of
ing that the reliabilities of all the constructs are adequate cultural compatibility on project performance is not statis-
(Hulland 1999). tically significant (β = .03, p > 0.05), the effect on strategic
Finally, to complete the psychometric assessment of performance is (β = .50, p < .05). Hypothesis 2b is thus
our model, we examined the discriminant validity, which supported, but not Hypothesis 2a.
represents the extent to which measures of a given con- Contrary to expectations, operational compatibility is
struct differ from measures of other constructs in the same not significantly related to project performance (β = .13,
model. Fornell and Larcker (1981) suggest the use of aver- p > .05), thus indicating lack of support for Hypothesis 3a.
age variance (i.e., the average variance shared between a Regarding the impact of operational compatibility on stra-
construct and its measures) extracted to assess discrim- tegic performance (β = –.25, p < .05), the path coefficient
inant validity.6 As shown in Table 2, the average variances is statistically significant; however, the sign is reversed,
extracted in all the constructs were all at least or greater thereby failing to support Hypothesis 3b.8
than .50, which is indicative of convergent validity Hypotheses 4a, 5a, and 6a respectively hypothesize that
(Barclay and Smith 1997). Also, the overall model pro- resource complementarity will positively affect mutual
vided reasonable evidence of discriminant validity in that trust, reciprocal commitment, and bilateral information
the variance shared between any two constructs was less exchange. The results reflect interesting differences in the
than the average variance extracted by the constructs, and impact of resource complementarity on various relationship-
all measures loaded higher on intended constructs than on capital constructs. Contrary to expectations, resource
other constructs (Hulland 1999). Overall, these statistics complementarity is not significantly related to trust (β =
indicate that the psychometric properties of the model are .07, p > .05) or bilateral information exchange (β = .01, p >
sufficiently strong to enable interpretation of structural 0.05), thereby failing to support Hypotheses 4a and 6a.
estimates. However, resource complementarity has a significant rela-
tionship with reciprocal commitment (β = .27, p < .05),
Structural Estimates supporting Hypothesis 5a.
Hypotheses 4b, 5b, and 6b respectively suggest that
Since PLS does not attempt to minimize residual item cultural compatibility enhances relationship capital. As
covariance, there is no summary statistic to measure the expected, cultural compatibility is significantly related to
overall fit of models as in the case of SEM techniques. mutual trust (β = .40, p < .05), reciprocal commitment (β =
Variance explained (R2) and the sign and significance of .42, p < .05), and bilateral information exchange (β = .39,
path coefficients are used to assess nomological validity. A p < .05), thus supporting all three hypotheses.
bootstrapping method of “sampling with replacement” Hypotheses 4c, 5c, and 6c respectively hypothesize the
was used to assess the statistical significance of the param- positive impact of operational compatibility on trust, com-
eter estimates. Standard errors were computed on the basis mitment, and bilateral information exchange. As
of 500 bootstrapping runs. Results of the structural model expected, operational compatibility is significantly related
are given in Tables 3 and 4. Table 3 provides the results of to trust (β = .37, p < .05), and commitment (β = .19, p <
the direct effects of interfirm diversity on relationship- .05), thereby supporting Hypotheses 4c and 5c. However,
capital constructs. Table 4 presents the results of the operational compatibility is not significantly related to
Sarkar et al. / ALLIANCE PERFORMANCE 367

TABLE 1
Measurement Model
Construct Itema Loading
Resource Both firms needed each other’s resources to accomplish their goals and responsibilities .66
Complementarity The resources contributed by both firms were significant in getting the bid .91
Resources brought into the venture by each firm were very valuable for the other .93
Cultural The organizational values and social norms prevalent in the two firms were congruent .77
Compatibility Executives from both firms involved in this project had compatible philosophies/approaches to business dealings .76
The goals and objectives of both firms were compatible with each other .86
b
The chemistry was right between the two firms —
Operational Technical capabilities of the two firms were compatible with each other .89
Compatibility The organizational procedures of the two firms were compatible .82
Employees of both firms had similar professional or trade skills .67
Mutual Trust Both firms were generally honest and truthful with each other .83
Both firms treated each other fairly and justly .80
Both firms found it necessary to be cautious in dealing with each other (R) .80
b
Relying on each other was risky for both firms (R) —
Reciprocal There was frequent communication between the two firms (e.g., visits to each other’s firms, meetings, written
Information and telephone communications) .81
Exchange Exchange of information in this relationship took place frequently and informally .91
Making contact with people from the other firm was hard for both firms (R) .69
b
Decisions regarding the project were made unanimously in joint meetings with managers from both firms —
Reciprocal Both firms were willing to dedicate whatever people and resources it took to make this project a success .83
Commitment Both firms provided experienced and capable people to the project .90
Both firms were committed to making this project a success .89
Strategic The collaboration provided a very effective medium of learning .68
Performance Collaborating with this partner was a wise business decision .91
Our strategic objectives going into the venture were achieved .85
Project The owner’s objectives (in terms of specifications, schedule, quality) were met .68
Performance A quality job was done on the project .80
Overall, the project was efficiently carried out .84
The venture was profitable for our firm .71

NOTE: (R) Indicates items that were reverse-coded.


a. Scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree).
b. Items that were deleted after initial tests.

TABLE 2
Internal Consistency, Square Roots of Average Variance Extracted, and Correlation Matrix
Construct Internal Consistency 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
1. Resource Complementarity .88 .84
2. Cultural Compatibility .84 .26 .80
3. Operational Compatibility .84 .11 .65 .80
4. Mutual Trust .85 .22 .66 .64 .81
5. Bilateral Information Exchange .85 .09 .38 .24 .31 .81
6. Reciprocal Commitment .91 .40 .61 .49 .48 .37 .87
7. Project Performance .86 .45 .54 .48 .53 .33 .67 .76
8. Strategic Performance .84 .29 .50 .17 .23 .38 .50 .38 .82

NOTE: The diagonal (in italics) shows the square root of the average variance extracted for each construct.

bilateral information exchange (β = –.02, p > .05), thereby thereby failing to support Hypothesis 7b. Commitment is
failing to support Hypothesis 6c. significantly related to both project performance (β = .39,
Hypotheses 7a and 7b state that trust will be positively p < .05) and strategic performance (β = .30, p < .05),
associated with both project performance and strategic thereby supporting Hypotheses 8a and 8b. Furthermore,
performance. Trust is significantly related to project per- results indicate that reciprocal information exchange is not
formance (β = .17, p < .05), thereby supporting Hypothesis 7a. significantly related to project performance (β = .07, p >
However, contrary to expectations, trust is not signifi- .05), thereby failing to support Hypothesis 9a. However,
cantly related to strategic performance (β = –.15, p > .05), reciprocal information exchange is positively related to
368 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

TABLE 3
Effect of Interfirm Diversity on Relationship Capital: Standardized PLSa Coefficients
Hypothesized Standardized
Dependent Variable Independent Variable H0 Sign Coefficient
2
Mutual trust (R = .51) Resource complementarity H4a + .07
Cultural compatibility H5a + .40***
Operational compatibility H6a + .37***
2
Reciprocal commitment (R = .45) Resource complementarity H4b + .27***
Cultural compatibility H5b + .42***
Operational compatibility H6b + .19***
2
Bilateral information exchange (R = .15) Resource complementarity H4c + .01
Cultural compatibility H5c + .39***
Operational compatibility H6c + –.02

NOTE: H = hypothesis.
a. PLS = Partial Least Squares.
*** Denotes significance at p < .05.

TABLE 4
Direct, Indirect, and Total Effects of Interfirm Diversity
on Performance: Standardized PLSa Coefficients
Standardized Coefficient
Dependent Variable Independent Variable H0 Hypothesized Sign Direct Indirectb Totalc
Project performance (R2 = .55) Interfirm Diversity
Resource complementarity H1a + .22*** .10 .32
Cultural compatibility H2a + .03 .22 .22
Operational compatibility H3a + .13 .13 .13
Relationship capital
Mutual trust H7a + .17*** NA —
Reciprocal commitment H8a + .39*** NA .41
Bilateral information exchange H9a + .07 NA —
2
Strategic performance (R = .41) Interfirm Diversity
Resource complementarity H1b + .09 .08 .08
Cultural compatibility H2b + .50*** .20 .70
Operational compatibility H3b + –.25*** .06 –.19
Relationship capital
Mutual trust H7b + –.15 NA —
Reciprocal commitment H8b + .30*** NA —
Bilateral information exchange H9b + .18*** NA .21

NOTE: NA = not applicable.


a. PLS = Partial Least Squares.
b. Only statistically significant indirect effects were included in the computation.
c. Only statistically significant effects (direct or indirect) were included.
*** Significant at p < .05.

strategic performance (β = .18, p < .05), thereby providing from .24 to .41. The increases for both project performance,
support for Hypothesis 9b. F(3, 61) = 4.52, Fcrit = 2.76, and strategic performance,
F(3, 61) = 6.20, Fcrit = 2.76, are significant at p < .05, thus
Indirect effects. Preliminary tests suggested that condi-
indicating that relationship-capital variables contribute
tions stipulated by Baron and Kenny (1986) for a media-
substantially to the explanatory power of the model.
tion model were satisfied,9 indicating that relationship-
Next, based on the approach suggested by Baron and
capital variables mediated the relationship between part-
Kenny (1986), we assessed the mediation by examining
ner characteristics and alliance performance. We examined
the size and significance of the indirect effects. Interest-
the contribution of the relationship-capital variables to the
ingly, for both performance constructs, all three antecedents—
explanatory power of the model. Specifically, we exam-
namely, resource complementarity, cultural compatibility,
ined the increase in R2s of the alliance performance con-
and operational compatibility—had significant indirect
structs when the relationship-capital variables were
effects. The standardized effect sizes were small to
included. The R2 of project performance increases from
medium (J. Cohen 1988), ranging from .06 to .22. Taken in
.45 to .55, while that of strategic performance increases
Sarkar et al. / ALLIANCE PERFORMANCE 369

tandem, the statistical significance of the increase in R2s diversity differentially affect performance and that models
and the significant indirect effects together indicate the that integrate both structural and relationship-capital vari-
important role of relationship capital in explaining the ables have greater explanatory power over the vexing
relationship between partner characteristics and perfor- question of alliance performance. We now summarize the
mance. Also, the presence of these indirect effects sug- main implications of what this research has found.
gests that any omission of these variables from a theoreti- With regard to complementarity, our analysis indicates
cal model could lead to an underestimation of the total that the synergy that results when alliance partners pool
effects of partner characteristics on performance. Spe- together complementary resources and capabilities
cifically, resource complementarity affects both project enhances performance. First, it enhances the economic
performance (β = .10) and strategic performance (β = .08), efficiency and qualitative effectiveness of the task being
cultural compatibility affects both project performance (β = jointly carried out both directly and indirectly. While the
.22) and strategic performance (β = .20), and operational direct effect is stronger, there is a substantive indirect
compatibility seems to influence both project performance effect, primarily through reciprocal commitment. It thus
(β = .13) and strategic performance (β = .06) through rela- appears that when firms can partner with firms that can
tionship capital.10 In summary, the results indicate the complement their weaknesses, not only is there a direct
importance of considering both direct and indirect effects effect on project performance, but it also has the added
on alliance performance, thus giving further credence to effect of increasing the commitment of each partner to the
the theoretical rationale behind integrating both structural relationship wherein they are willing to invest requisite
and relational perspectives into an explanation of alliance resources into the relationship to make it a success. This
performance. serves as a powerful signaling mechanism that reduces the
threat of opportunism, aligns incentive structures, and pro-
vides a host of efficiencies. Second, although comple-
DISCUSSION AND IMPLICATIONS mentarity does not have a direct effect on strategic perfor-
mance (i.e., a perception that the relationship is
Recent scholarship on international alliances has artic- competency enhancing and strategically beneficial), it
ulated the need for more research partner selection issues does have an indirect effect, although weak, through the
(Hitt et al. 2000), especially because of their impact on mediating effect of relationship-capital variables. Thus,
alliance performance (Johnson et al. 1996; Saxton 1997). Type I interfirm diversity appears to have important impli-
Although strategic alliances have proliferated throughout cations for performance. It enhances the efficiency and
the world, a substantial proportion of these underperform effectiveness of the joint task being performed, or the com-
(Madhok and Tallman 1998). In examining determinants mon benefits from the alliance, as well as more strategic
of alliance performance, we focus on a unique aspect asso- private benefits that a focal firm may take out from the
ciated with the characteristics of partners involved in an relationship. Furthermore, while its effect on the common
alliance, namely, interfirm diversity (Parkhe 1991). We benefits is primarily direct, its effect on private benefits is
suggest that performance is likely to be enhanced when mediated through relationship capital. This appears plau-
firms are able to manage the paradox involved in choosing sible since mere complementarity may not lead to learning
a firm that is different, yet similar. Complementary and knowledge transfer, which requires a certain depth of
resource and capability profiles enhance the value gener- interaction and relationship quality for tacit know-how to
ated in alliances, as do similarity in the social institutions be transferred.
of the partners. We therefore focus on three constructs The effects of Type II diversity, namely, cultural and
related to interfirm diversity. Drawing on Parkhe’s (1991) operational compatibility, on performance are intriguing,
work, we develop a multidimensional treatment of thus highlighting the dilemma and complexity surround-
resource complementarity, cultural compatibility, and ing partner selection issues. Cultural compatibility
operational compatibility. We integrate extant interna- enhances both project and strategic performance. It affects
tional alliance literature that has traditionally examined project performance indirectly through relationship-capi-
the structural and sociopsychological aspects of alliances tal variables and influences strategic performance both
separately and develop a theoretical framework that sug- directly and indirectly (with the direct effect being stron-
gests that the diversity-related characteristics of partners ger). Also, the total effect on strategic performance is
affect performance directly and indirectly through their stronger than on project performance, while relative to
effects on relationship capital or sociopsychological vari- complementarity, cultural compatibility has a stronger
ables that are the focus of interactive theorists (Cullen et al. effect on strategic performance than on project perfor-
2000; Heide and Miner 1992). Our results provide a mance. The results suggest that when partners in an alli-
unique contribution to the understanding of how partner ance share similar organizational cultures, they are likely
characteristics affect alliance-related performance to enjoy a better quality of relationship, which in turn will
because they suggest that various types of interfirm facilitate an effective intermingling of skills and
370 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

competencies and ensure that the project is efficiently and capital variables on alliance performance and thus high-
effectively carried out. On the other hand, the strategic light the importance of alliance management capabilities.
learning that a focal firm can achieve from its partner or the In other words, the performance-enhancing effect of com-
private rents that it generates from the focal alliance is patibility is enhanced even further when firms make con-
directly affected by the cultural congruence of the scious efforts to create relationship capital and embed the
partners. relationship within certain sociopsychological states
On the other hand, operational compatibility has a posi- through an interaction process designed to specifically
tive indirect effect on project performance, implying that improve the quality of the relationship.
compatibility in procedural capabilities enhances the qual- The findings from this study have important manage-
ity of the relationship and thus increases the efficiency and rial and theoretical implications. From a normative per-
effectiveness of the project execution. However, it has a spective, the results suggest the relative importance of var-
surprising negative direct impact on strategic perfor- ious aspects in choosing the appropriate alliance partner.
mance, which is partly mitigated by a weaker positive In particular, allying with firms with complementary
effect through the relational mediators. This counter- resources is likely to ensure success of the particular pro-
iintuitive result is intriguing in that it suggests the follow- ject (or shared benefits), while finding partners with simi-
ing: although common benefits accruing from an alliance lar cultural norms is more important to achieve strategic
are enhanced when levels of professional skills, technical private benefits. Thus, criteria used in partner choice can
capabilities, and operational procedures of two partners be guided by specific firm objectives in forging cross-bor-
converge, private benefits from the alliance may be ham- der alliances. From a theoretical perspective, our findings
pered. We offer a speculation based on the learning race clarify the relative importance and interrelationships
analogy of Hamel (1991). Operating at similar levels of between partner characteristics and relational aspects in
technology and skills (albeit the specific technology, explaining alliance performance. Much of the prior
skills, and capabilities may be different) is likely to research has generally considered these two aspects as
increase the absorptive capacity of a firm and enhance its alternative ways to improve alliance performance. Our
ability to recognize, assimilate, and commercialize exter- findings, by examining the direct and indirect effects of
nal information (W. Cohen and Levinthal 1990) from its partner characteristics on performance, highlight the need
partner. Accordingly, to protect itself from redundancy, a to examine both simultaneously.
focal firm may be wary of passing information and know- While the study makes important contributions to the
how that it considers critical to partners that possess high alliance literature, several potential limitations should be
levels of absorptive capacity. In response to the perceived noted. First, we undertook a cross-sectional approach to
threat, they may clamp down with procedures that limit data collection. This prohibits studying the temporal
transfer of information and know-how beyond what is aspects of a relationship. Second, we collected informa-
immediately relevant for project execution to their part- tion from only one side of the dyad. To what extent percep-
ners. Thus, although operational compatibility translates tions would have converged is unknown. Third, we used
into better project management and execution, it has a neg- single informants, a procedure less rigorous than the use of
ative effect on strategic performance. However, this multiple informants (Kumar, Stern, and Anderson, 1993).
intriguing result needs to be examined in future research. Fourth, our sample size is relatively small. Although the
Interestingly, this research suggests that different types of closed nature of the industry partly explains the reason
compatibility related to the social institutions of the part- behind the low response rates, the sampling frame is also
ners could have very different effects on various aspects of limited. The industry-specific nature of the study makes us
alliance performance. believe that at the very least, our data do not suffer from the
Furthermore, the indirect effects suggest the impor- wide heterogeneity that characterizes most survey data.
tance of the mediating relationship-capital variables. Several intriguing questions remain. For example, vari-
Taken individually, the results suggest that reciprocal ous contingencies may exist that moderate the relationship
commitment and mutual trust enhance the level of direct between various partner characteristics and performance
common benefits from an alliance, while reciprocal com- variables. Do complementarity, cultural compatibility, and
mitment and bilateral information exchange increase the operational compatibility always play an equal role? Are
private benefits that accrue from an alliance. At a more they compensatory? Is one or two more important in cer-
macro perspective, however, our research suggests that the tain aspects of the relationship than the other(s)? Do the
issue of partner compatibility, which is influenced by orga- proposed relationships hold in all conditions and contexts?
nizational routines related to partner selection, by itself can- The scope of our article and the small sample size limit our
not maximize benefits that can emerge from an alliance. ability to test these questions, and we leave it to future
The results indicate the strong impact of relationship- research to unravel these conundrums.
Sarkar et al. / ALLIANCE PERFORMANCE 371

NOTES Armstrong, J. Scott and Terry S. Overton. 1977. “Estimating


Nonresponse Bias in Mail Surveys.” Journal of Marketing Research
14 (August): 396-402.
1. Parkhe’s Type I and II diversities are related to differences be-
Aulakh, Preet S., Masaaki Kotabe, and Arvind Sahay. 1996. “Trust and
tween partner firms. In this study, we examine differences in resources Performance in Cross Border Marketing Partnerships: A Behavioral
(which we call resource complementarity) and differences in cultural and Approach.” Journal of International Business Studies 27 (5): 1005-
operational norms. However, since it is proposed that firms need to be 1032.
similar in terms of social norms, we conceptually operationalize Type II Badaracco, Joseph L. Jr. 1991. The Knowledge Link: How Firms Com-
diversity as reverse, that is, compatibility (or similarity in cultural and op- pete Through Strategic Alliances. Bostons: Harvard Business
erational norms). School.
2. Strategic assets are defined as “set of difficult to trade and imitate, Barclay, Donald and Brock Smith. 1997. The Effects of Organizational
scarce, appropriable, and specialized resources and capabilities that be- Differences and Trust on the Effectiveness of Selling Partner Rela-
tionships.” Journal of Marketing 61 (1): 3-21.
stow the firm competitive advantage” (Amit and Schoemaker 1993:36).
Barney, Jay B. 1991. “Firm Resources and Sustained Competitive Ad-
3. Details of respondent countries are available from authors on re- vantage.” Journal of Management 17 (1): 99-120.
quest. Baron, Reuben M. and David A. Kenny. 1986. “The Moderator-Mediator
4. Chin’s (1998) rule of thumb suggests that the sample size for a Variable Distinction in Social Psychological Research: Conceptual,
Partial Least Squares (PLS) study be equal to the larger of the following: Strategic and Statistical Considerations.” Journal of Personality and
(1) 10 times the scale with the largest number of formative (i.e., emer- Social Psychology 15 (6): 1173-1182.
gent) indicators or (2) 10 times the largest number of structural paths di- Bleeke, Joel and David Ernst. 1991. “The Way to Win in Cross-Border
rected at a particular construct in the structural model. These conditions Alliances.” Harvard Business Review 69 (6): 127-135.
are satisfied in our study. Borys, Bryan and David B. Jemison. 1989. “Hybrid Arrangements as
2 2 Strategic Alliances: Theoretical Issues in Organizational Combina-
5. Internal consistency = ((Σλyi) / ((Σλyi) + Σ var(εi)), where tions.” Academy of Management Review 14:234-249.
2
var(εi) = 1 – λyi . Bradach, Jeffrey L. and Robert G. Eccles. 1989. “Price, Authority, and
2 2
6. Average variance extracted = Σλyi / Σλyi + Σ var(εi) e var(εi) = 1 – Trust: From Ideal Types to Plural Forms.” American Review of Soci-
2
λyi . ology 15:97-118.
Bucklin, Louis P. and Sanjit Sengupta. 1993. “Organizing Successful Co-
7. Indirect effects that include three variables (X1 → X2 → X3) can
Marketing Alliances.” Journal of Marketing 57 (April): 32-46.
be tested as follows: a and b are the path coefficients for the direct effects Chin, Wynne W. 1998. “The Partial Least Square Approach to Structural
of X1 → X2 and X2 → X3, respectively. SEa and SEb are the standard er- Equation Modeling.” In Modern Methods for Business Research. Ed.
rors. The product ab represents the indirect effect of X1 on X3. The stan- G. A. Marcoulides. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 295-336.
2 2
dard error for the indirect effect is given as follows: SEab = sqrt [(b SEa + Chung, Seungwha, Harbir Singh, and Kyungmook Lee. 2000.
2 2 2 2
a SEb + SEa × SEb )]. “Complementarity, Status Similarity and Social Capital as Drivers of
8. We estimated multiple PLS models, by dropping different inde- Alliance Formation.” Strategic Management Journal 21:1-22.
pendent variables, to investigate the possibility that the negative influ- Cohen, Jacob. 1988. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sci-
ences. New York: Academic Press.
ence of operational compatibility on alliance performance may be
Cohen, Wesley M. and David A. Levinthal. 1990. “Absorptive Capacity:
attributed to suppressor effects. PLS results are robust and indicate that A New Perspective on Learning and Innovation.” Administrative Sci-
the impact of operational compatibility on alliance performance is indeed ence Quarterly 35 (September): 128-152.
negative when we control for the effects of other exogenous constructs. Cullen, John B., Jean L. Johnson, and Tomoaki Sakano. 1995. “Japanese
9. To establish that a mediation model exists, Baron and Kenny and Local Partner Commitment to IJVs: Psychological Conse-
(1986) stipulate that four conditions must hold: (1) the antecedent vari- quences of Outcomes and Investments in the IJV relationship.” Jour-
able must be related to the mediator, (2) the antecedent variable must be nal of International Business Studies 26 (1): 91-116.
related to the dependent variable, (3) the mediator must be related to the , , and . 2000. “Success Through Commitment and
dependent variable, and (4) the relationship between the antecedent and Trust: The Soft Side of Strategic Alliance Management.” Journal of
the dependent variable must be less in (3) than in (2). All conditions were World Business 35 (3): 223-240.
Das, Tarun K. and Bing-Sheng Teng. 1998. “Between Trust and Control:
satisfied. Tables 2, 3, and 4 show the results of all the steps.
Developing Confidence in Partner Cooperation in Alliances.” Acad-
10. The indirect effect of resource complementarity on a specific per- emy of Management Review 23 (3): 491-512.
formance variable equals the sum of statistically significant indirect ef- Dierickx, Ingemar and Karel Cool. 1989. “Asset Stock Accumulation
fects through trust, commitment, and reciprocal information exchange on and Sustainability of Competitive Advantage.” Management Science
that particular performance type. Similar computations were made for 35 (12): 1504-1513.
cultural and operational compatibility variables. Dwyer, Robert F., Paul H. Schurr, and Sejo Oh. 1987. “Developing
Buyer-Seller Relationships.” Journal of Marketing 51 (April): 11-27.
Dyer, Jeff H. and Harbir Singh. 1998. “The Relational View: Cooperative
Strategy and Sources of Inter-Organizational Competitive Advan-
REFERENCES tage.” The Academy of Management Review 23 (4): 660-679.
Eccles, Robert. 1981. “The Quasifirm in the Construction Industry.”
Achrol, Ravi S. 1997. “Changes in the Theory of Interorganizational Re- Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization 2:335-357.
lations in Marketing: Toward a Network Paradigm.” Journal of the Evans, F. B. 1963. “Selling as a Dyadic Relationship: A New Approach.”
Academy of Marketing Science 25:56-71. American Behavioral Scientist 6 (May): 76-79.
Amit, Raphael and Paul J. H. Schoemaker. 1993. “Strategic Assets and Fornell, Claes and David F. Larcker. 1981. “Evaluating Structural Equa-
Organizational Rent.” Strategic Management Journal 14 (1): 33-46. tion Models With Unobservable Variables and Measurement Error.”
Anderson, Erin and Barton Weitz. 1989. “Determinants of Continuity in Journal of Marketing Research 18 (1): 39-50.
Conventional Industrial Channels Dyads.” Marketing Science 8 Friedmann, Wolfgang G. and John-Pierre Beguin. 1971. Joint Interna-
(Fall): 310-323. tional Business Ventures in Developing Countries. New York: Co-
 and . 1992. “The Use of Pledges to Build and Sustain lumbia University Press.
Commitment in Distribution Channels.” Journal of Marketing Re- Geringer, J. Michael and Louis Hebert. 1989. “Control and Performance
search 29:18-34. of International Joint Ventures.” Journal of International Business
Anderson, James C. and James A. Narus. 1990. “A Model of Distributor Studies 20 (2): 235-254.
Firm and Manufacturer Firm Working Partnerships.” Journal of Mar- Gomes-Casseres, Benjamin. 1987. “Joint Venture Instability: Is It a
keting 54 (1): 42-58. Problem?” Columbia Journal of World Business 22 (2): 97-102.
372 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

Gulati, Ranjay, Tarun Khanna, and Nitin Nohria. 1994. “Unilateral Com-  and Robert Spekman. 1994. “Characteristics of Partnership Suc-
mitments and the Importance of Process in Alliances.” Sloan Man- cess: Partnership Attributes, Communication Behavior, and Conflict
agement Review 35 (3): 61-69. Resolution Techniques.” Strategic Management Journal 15 (2): 135-
Gundlach, Gregory T., Ravi S. Achrol, and John T. Mentzer. 1995. “The 152.
Structure of Commitment in Exchange.” Journal of Marketing 59 , Robert J. Fisher, and John R. Nevin. 1996. “Collaborative Com-
(January): 78-92. munication in Interfirm Relationships: Moderating Effects of Inte-
Hagedoorn, John. 1993. “Understanding the Rationale of Strategic Tech- gration and Control.” Journal of Marketing 60 (July): 103-115.
nology Partnering: Interorganizational Modes of Cooperation and Moorman, Christine, Gerald Zaltman, and Rohit Deshpande. 1992. “Re-
Sectoral Differences.” Strategic Management Journal 14 (5): 371- lationships Between Providers and Users of Market Research: The
385. Dynamics of Trust Within and Between Organizations.” Journal of
Hamel, Gary. 1991. “Competition for Competence and Interpartner Marketing Research 29 (3): 314-328.
Learning Within International Strategic Alliances.” Strategic Man- Morgan, Robert M. and Shelby D. Hunt. 1994. “The Commitment-Trust
agement Journal 20 (Special Issue): 83-103. Theory of Relationship Marketing.” Journal of Marketing 58 (3): 20-
Harrigan, Kathryn R. 1985. Strategies for Joint Ventures. Lexington, 38.
MA: Lexington Books. Oliver, Christine. 1990. “Determinants of Interorganizational Relation-
Heide, Jan B. 1994. “Interorganizational Governance in Marketing ships: Integration and Future Directions.” Academy of Management
Channels.” Journal of Marketing 58 (1): 71-85. Review 15:241-265.
 and George John. 1992. “Do Norms Matter in Marketing Rela- Parkhe, Arvind 1991. “Interfirm Diversity, Organizational Learning, and
tionships?” Journal of Marketing 56:32-44. Longevity in Global Strategic Alliances.” Journal of International
 and Anne S. Miner. 1992. “The Shadow of the Future: Effects of Business Studies 20:579-601.
Anticipated Interaction and Frequency of Contact on Buyer-Seller . 1993. “Strategic Alliance Structuring: A Game Theoretic and
Cooperation.” Academy of Management Journal 2:265-300. Transaction Cost Examination of Interfirm Cooperation.” Academy
Hill, Charles W. L. 1990. “Cooperation, Opportunism, and the Invisible of Management Journal 36 (4): 794-829.
Hand: Implications for Transaction Cost Theory.” Academy of Man- Ring, Peter Smith. 1996. “Fragile and Resilient Trust and Their Roles in
agement Review 15 (3): 500-513. Economic Exchange.” Business and Society 35 (2): 148-175.
Hitt, Michael A., Tina M. Dacin, Edward Levitas, Jean-Luc Arregle, and  and Andrew Van de Ven. 1994. “Developmental Processes of Co-
Anca Borza. 2000. “Partner Selection in Emerging and Developed operative Interorganizational Relationships.” Academy of Manage-
Market Context: Resource-Based and Organizational Learning Pro- ment Review 19 (1): 90-118.
spective.” Academy of Management Journal 43 (3): 449-467. Sarkar, Mitrabarun, S. Tamer Cavusgil, and Cuneyt Evirgen. 1997. “The
Hosmer, LaRue Tone. 1995. “Trust: The Connecting Link Between Or- Commitment-Trust Framework in International Collaborative Ven-
ganizational Theory and Philosophical Ethics.” Academy of Manage- tures.” In Cooperative Strategies: North American Perspectives. Eds.
ment Review 20 (2): 370-403. Paul W. Beamish and J. Peter Killing. San Francisco: The New
Hulland, John. 1999. “Use of Partial Least Squares (PLS) in Strategic Lexington Press, 255-285.
Management Research: A Review of Four Recent Studies.” Strategic , Raj Echambadi, and Jeffrey Harrison. 2001. “Alliance Entrepre-
Management Journal 20 (2): 195-204. neurship and Firm Market Performance.” Strategic Management
Johnson, Jean L., John B. Cullen, Tomoaki Sakano, and Hideyuki Journal 22 (6/7): 701-711.
Takenouchi. 1996. “Setting the Stage for Trust and Strategic Integra- Saxton, Todd. 1997. “The Effects of Partner and the Relationship Charac-
tion in Japanese-U.S. Cooperative Alliances.” Journal of Interna- teristics on Alliance Outcomes.” Academy of Management Journal
tional Business Studies 27 (5): 981-1004. 40:443-461.
Katz, Michael L. 1989. “Vertical Contractual Relations.” In Handbook of Slaate, H. A. 1968. The Pertinence of the Paradox. New York: Human-
Industrial Organizations, Vol 1. Eds. R. Schmalensee and R. D. ities Press.
Willig. Reading, NY: Elsevier, 655-721. Smith, J. Brook, and Donald W. Barclay. 1997. “The Effect of Organiza-
Khanna, Tarun. 1998. “The Scope of Alliances.” Organization Science 9 tional Differences and Trust on the Effectiveness of Selling Partner
(3): 340-355. Relationship.” Journal of Marketing 61 (1): 3-21.
, Ranjay Gulati, and Nitin Nohria. 1998. “The Dynamics of Stump, Rodney, and Jan B. Heide. 1996. “Controlling Supplier Oppor-
Learning Alliances: Competition, Cooperation, and Relative Scope.” tunism in Industrial Relationships.” Journal of Marketing Research
Strategic Management Journal 19 (3): 193-210. 33 (November): 431-441.
Killing, J. Peter. 1983. Strategies for Joint Venture Success. London: Wernerfelt, Birger. 1984. “A Resource-Based View of the Firm.” Strate-
Croom Helm. gic Management Journal 5 (2): 171-180.
Kogut, Bruce. 1988. “Joint Ventures: Theoretical and Empirical Perspec- Wilkof, M. V., D. W. Brown, and J. W. Selsky. 1995. “When Stories Are
tives.” Strategic Management Journal 9 (2): 319-332. Different: The Influence of Corporate Culture Mismatches in
Interorganizational Relations.” Journal of Applied Behavioral Sci-
Koza, Mitchell P. and Arie Y. Lewin. 1998. “The Co-Evolution of Strate-
ence 30:373-388.
gic Alliances.” Organization Science 9 (3): 255-264.
Williamson, Oliver E. 1985. The Economic Institutions of Capitalism.
Kumar, Nirmalya, Louis W. Stern, and James G. Anderson. 1993. “Con-
New York: Free Press.
ducting Interorganizational Research Using Key Informants.” Acad-
emy of Management Journal 36 (6): 1633-1651. . 1991. “Comparative Economic Organization: The Analysis of
Lado, Augustine A., Nancy G. Boyd, and Susan C. Hanlon. 1997. “Com- Discrete Structural Alternatives.” Administrative Science Quarterly
petition, Cooperation, and the Search for Economic Rents: A Syn- 26 (June): 269-296.
cretic Model.” Academy of Management Review 22 (1): 110-141. Wilson, David T. 1995. “An Integrated Model of Buyer-Seller Relation-
ships.” Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science 23 (4): 335-345.
Lohmöller, Jan-Bernd. 1989. Latent Variable Path Modeling With Partial
Yates, J. K., S. Mukherjee, and S. Njos. 1999. Anatomy of Construction
Least Squares. New York: Springer-Verlag.
Industry Competition in the Year 2000. Austin, TX: Construction In-
Macneil, Ian R. 1980. The New Social Contract. New Haven, CT: Yale
dustry Institute.
University Press.
Madhok, Anoop. 1995. “Revisiting Multinational Firms’ Tolerance for
Joint Ventures: A Trust-Based Approach.” Journal of International
Business Studies 26 (1): 117-138. ABOUT THE AUTHORS
 and Stephen B. Tallman. 1998. “Resources, Transactions, and
Rents: Managing Value Through Interfirm Collaborative Relation- MB Sarkar (Ph.D., Michigan State University) is an assistant
ships.” Organization Science 9 (3): 326-339.
Mohr, Jakki and John R. Nevin. 1990. “Communication Strategies in
professor of marketing at the University of Central Florida. His
Marketing Channels: A Theoretical Perspective.” Journal of Mar- current research includes strategic alliances, innovation and en-
keting 50 (October): 36-51. trepreneurship, knowledge management, and electronic markets.
Sarkar et al. / ALLIANCE PERFORMANCE 373

His research has been published in the Strategic Management tional resource center. His teaching, research, and administrative
Journal, the Journal of International Business Studies, and the activities have focused on international business and marketing.
Journal of Business Research, among others. His research has been published in the Journal of Marketing, the
Journal of Marketing Research, and the Journal of International
Raj Echambadi (Ph.D., University of Houston) is an assistant Business Studies, among others. His specific interests include the
professor of marketing at the University of Central Florida. His internationalization of the firm, global marketing strategy, and
current research interests include investigation of territorial loy- internationalization of business education. He was the founding
alty issues, management of innovations, and estimation issues editor of the Journal of International Marketing, now published
pertaining to structural equation modeling and Partial Least by the American Marketing Association.
Squares. His research has been published in the Strategic Man-
agement Journal, Multivariate Behavioral Research, and the Preet S. Aulakh (Ph.D., University of Texas at Austin) is an as-
Journal of Product Innovation Management. sociate professor of strategy and international business at the Fox
School of Business and Management, Temple University. His re-
S. Tamer Cavusgil (Ph.D., University of Wisconsin) is Univer- search focuses on international technology licensing, cross-bor-
sity Distinguished Faculty and serves as the John William der joint ventures and strategic alliances, and strategies of firms
Byington Endowed Chair in global marketing at Michigan State from developing economies. His research has been published in
University (MSU). He is also the executive director of MSU’s the Academy of Management Journal, the Journal of Marketing,
Center for International Business Education and Research, a na- and the Journal of International Business Studies.
JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY
Keaveney,OF
Parthasarathy
MARKETING / ONLINE
SCIENCE
SERVICES FALL 2001

Customer Switching Behavior in


Online Services: An Exploratory Study
of the Role of Selected Attitudinal,
Behavioral, and Demographic Factors
Susan M. Keaveney
Madhavan Parthasarathy
University of Colorado at Denver

With a quarter of a billion Internet users worldwide and commerce access (e.g., retail merchandise, travel), and
estimates of more than one-half billion people online by interactive services (e.g., e-mail, bulletin boards, chat
the year 2003, growth in the online services industry has rooms), have grown at phenomenal rates—some report-
been exponential. With this growth has come concern edly growing at rates of 200 percent per year (AOL.com
about customer “churn,” a concern that parallels issues of 2000; Prodigy.com 2000). Rapid growth has attracted the
customer switching behavior in services industries in gen- interest of the major Internet service providers (ISPs),
eral. This manuscript reports results of two field studies, such as IBM, AT&T’s Worldnet, WorldCom’s UUnet, or
conducted among two randomly selected samples of on- PSINet, whose original focus was dial-up Internet access.
line service users, that investigate the degree to which se- Industry sources observe that the major ISPs now compete
lected behavioral (information that customers used when for markets claimed by the traditional online service pro-
making the online service decision, their service usage), viders, with value-added and premium services (Borland
attitudinal (risk-taking propensity), and demographic (in- 1998).
come and education) factors are effective in discriminat- A key issue for online service providers as a result of
ing between continuers and switchers. The research in this increased competition is “churn,” or customer move-
Study 1 is replicated in Study 2 and extended to consider ment in and out of the marketplace. Some churn is online
additional attitudinal factors of satisfaction and involve- service discontinuance, where individuals try a service
ment. Implications for managers and researchers are (e.g., AOL) but subsequently decide to stop using the ser-
discussed. vice category (e.g., online services) altogether. For exam-
ple, one study reported that 9 million people in the United
States had tried using the Internet during 1997 but had dis-
continued use by 1998 (Kingsley and Anderson 1998).
During the past decade, the online service industry has Some of the churn is “customer service switching behav-
witnessed tremendous growth, much of it spurred by the ior” (Keaveney 1995), where customers continue to use
Internet revolution (cf. Cyberatlas.com 2000e; Datamonitor. the service category (e.g., online services) but switch from
com 1999). Online service providers such as America one service provider (e.g., Prodigy) to another (e.g., AOL).
Online (AOL), CompuServe, Prodigy, Delphi, and For example, a 1995 report stated that only 2.5 of 6.7 mil-
Microsoft Network (MSN), which offer content (e.g., lion online users were still with their original online ser-
news, weather, sports, health care, entertainment), features vice provider (Carl 1995), and a 1997 report stated that
(e.g., software downloads, financial research data), e- online service providers had lost 3 percent to 5 percent of
Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science.
their customers in a 90-day window (Levin 1997). More
Volume 29, No. 4, pages 374-390. recent reports state that 14 percent of Internet users indi-
Copyright © 2001 by Academy of Marketing Science. cated a “strong intent to switch ISPs in the next 12 months,”
Keaveney, Parthasarathy / ONLINE SERVICES 375

and an additional 22 percent said that they might consider Customer switching behavior can be particularly dam-
switching providers at some point (Cyberatlas.com aging for subscription- or membership-based service
2000c). While industry reports of numbers of Internet firms where customers commit to ongoing relationships
users, customer migration, and churn can be rather fluid in and services are continuously provided. For example,
this fast-changing market, it is clear that churn is an impor- insurance, banking, public utilities, health care, telecom-
tant issue for online service providers. munications, cable, and other subscription-based services
Concern about churn in the world of electronic com- depend on customers to pay all or part of their charges on a
merce parallels a general and widespread concern with fixed-fee continuous basis. Service firms in industries
customer retention and customer switching behavior in such as banking, health care, and telecommunications
services industries. Identification of “defection-prone” charge fixed fees for access and basic services, with addi-
customers is a “pivotal requirement for companies and a tional charges for increased usage. Others, like public util-
ripe area for research” (Zeithaml 2000:77). The purpose of ities, may estimate overall costs for high and low periods
this research is to begin to identify defection-prone cus- of usage and charge customers prorated but relatively sta-
tomers of online services. Study 1 explores the effective- ble periodic fees, with increased (decreased) billing for
ness of selected attitudinal, behavioral, and demographic higher-than-estimated (lower-than-estimated) average
variables in discriminating between switchers and use. Still others, like cable companies, health clubs, and
continuers of online services. Study 2 replicates Study 1 online services, charge a single periodic fee for access
and extends the research to include measures of customer with unlimited usage. For each of these service firms that
satisfaction and involvement. allocate fixed costs across large numbers of customers and
depend on receipt of fixed membership or access fees on a
continuous basis, customer switching behavior can have a
BACKGROUND: SERVICE SWITCHING particularly devastating effect on the bottom line.

Service Switching and the Firm Service Switching and the Customer

The benefits of customer retention and the costs of cus- Because of the arguments presented above, the identifi-
tomer switching behavior have received much attention in cation of those customers prone to service switching
the literature. A decrease in customer switching creates behavior is a high priority. Prior work has examined rea-
benefits on both sides of the income statement in the form sons for customer service switching (Bolton and
of higher revenues and lower costs and has been shown to Bronkhurst 1995; Keaveney 1995), differences in satisfac-
be effective from both defensive and offensive strategic tion and loyalty of service switchers versus stayers
marketing perspectives (Fornell and Wernerfelt 1987; (Ganesh, Arnold, and Reynolds 2000), cognitive models
Zeithaml 2000). Consider first the defensive strategic per- of service switching (Bansal and Taylor 1999), and pro-
spective: on the revenue side, studies have shown that con- cess models of customer service switching (Roos 1999).
tinuing customers purchase higher volumes at higher mar- Few studies to date have identified which, if any, customer
gins (Grant and Schlesinger 1995; Heskett, Sasser, and characteristics might be effective in predicting customer
Schlesinger 1997; Reichheld 1993, 1996; Reichheld and switching behavior.
Kenny 1990; Reichheld and Sasser 1990) and increase A substantial body of research has supported the rela-
their usage of a service even when prices increase (Bolton tionships of two attitudinal variables, satisfaction and ser-
and Lemon 1999). Firms whose customers report high lev- vice quality, to customers’ switching intentions (e.g.,
els of satisfaction also enjoy “insulating” effects on their Anderson 1994; Anderson and Sullivan 1993; Boulding,
reputations (Anderson and Sullivan 1993). On the cost Kalra, Staelin, and Zeithaml 1993; Cronin and Taylor
side, studies have shown that selling costs of serving con- 1992; Oliva, Oliver, and Bearden 1995; Reichheld and
tinuing customers are lower and operating efficiencies are Sasser 1990; Zeithaml, Berry, and Parasuraman 1996).
higher (Heskett et al. 1997; Reichheld and Sasser 1990). Fewer studies have examined these relationships in the
From an offensive strategic perspective, on the revenue context of actual switching behavior, but they too suggest
side, retained customers attract new customers through that dissatisfaction explains at least some customer
positive word of mouth, thereby increasing market share switching (Bansal and Taylor 1999; Bolton and
(Heskett et al. 1997; Reichheld and Sasser 1990). Con- Bronkhorst 1995; Keaveney 1995).
tinuing customers may also help the firm to sustain pre- Other research has supported the need to consider
mium pricing tactics and validate the firm’s good reputa- causes of service switching beyond dissatisfaction. In an
tion to new customers (Zeithaml 2000). On the cost side, exploratory study of customer switching behavior in ser-
the positive word of mouth associated with increased cus- vices industries, Keaveney (1995) identified eight major
tomer retention can lower marketing costs to acquire new causes. Some could be associated with feelings of dissatis-
customers (cf. Bolton and Bronkhorst 1995; Peters 1988). faction with the service (e.g., core service failures, failed
376 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

service encounters, poor service recoveries), but others Zeithaml, Berry, and Parasuraman 1993). Predictive
were extrinsic or situational factors (e.g., price, inconve- expectations affect consumers’ judgments of satisfaction
nience, ethics, competition, and involuntary situations). and service quality, which in turn affect key behavioral
Anderson (1996) reported the importance of price toler- outcomes such as repurchase, switching behavior, and loy-
ance in addition to satisfaction when predicting customer alty (Oliver 1997; Yi 1990; Zeithaml et al. 1996).
switching. Dabholkar and Walls (1999) found that out- Thus, the types of information that customers use when
come-related, extrinsic, and service process factors were making choice decisions will, directly or indirectly, affect
all important in predicting customers’ intentions to switch their subsequent assessments of the choice. Different
providers of an experiential service. Bolton and types of information sources, when each is typically used,
Bronkhurst (1995) found that customers of cellular ser- and its anticipated effects on predictive expectations, satis-
vices who had complained to the firm were more likely to faction, and switching behavior are discussed in the next
switch than customers who had not complained. Thus, it sections.
seems important to augment studies of satisfaction and
intentions—which, although quite important, have been External sources of information. External information
well studied—to include other attitudinal, behavioral, and search is a purposeful decision by the consumer to scan the
demographic variables. environment for new and decision-relevant information
In light of the preceding discussion, the research (Berning and Jacoby 1974; Moore and Lehman 1980).
reported in this article contributes to the services market- Consumers may engage in external information search by
ing literature in a number of ways: (1) this research begins reading about products or services in mass media, using
the important work of identifying and profiling defection- marketer-produced print information, and researching
prone customers; (2) this research examines the actual third-party reviews of products and services in articles and
switching behavior of online service customers, not just books. (Note that these sources are also characterized as
their self-reported repurchase intentions; (3) this study impersonal, as opposed to personal, information sources.)
goes beyond satisfaction and intentions to explore hereto- Some consumers are more likely than others to rely on
fore unexamined attitudinal, behavioral, and demographic external, impersonal sources of information when making
characteristics of customers as possible predictors of purchase decisions. In a study of purchasers of new auto-
switching behavior; and (4), this research focuses on cus- mobiles, Furse et al. (1984) identified two segments of
tomers of online services—a rapidly growing industry, consumers, representing 44 percent of respondents, that
and one seemingly plagued by churn. were characterized by their above-average use of external,
impersonal sources such as brochures, pamphlets, adver-
tisements, magazines, reviews, and ratings. Moreover,
CONCEPTUAL DEVELOPMENT some purchase situations are more (or less) likely to trig-
AND RESEARCH HYPOTHESES ger the desire for external, impersonal sources of informa-
tion. In a study of durable goods, consumers preferred
Informational Influence advertising and other external sources when they believed
they were capable of drawing their own conclusions about
A basic tenet of consumer behavior theory holds that product attributes and judging the merits of the product
when consumers make purchase decisions, they use differ- themselves (Houston 1979). Consumers also prefer exter-
ent types of information sources to help them identify pos- nal, impersonal sources more when choosing goods, but
sible alternatives, to evaluate those alternatives, and to less when choosing services (Murray 1991).
make choices. Information sources can be characterized in Consumers who seek external, impersonal sources are
a variety of ways—internal versus external, personal ver- interested in gaining factual, objective information about
sus impersonal, marketer-oriented versus third-party, and product or service attributes. A preponderance of factual
so forth. Research on consumer information search tells us information about attributes should lead to more accurate
that consumers differ in their preferences for types of predictive expectations about the future performance of
information (cf. Furse, Punj, and Stewart 1984). Prefer- the product or service (Boulding et al. 1993). Accurate
ences for different types of information sources have also predictive expectations mean an absence of discon-
been found to vary between goods versus services deci- firmation, as is demonstrated by the use of “accurate: it
sions (Murray 1991). was just as I thought” as the central or neutral value in
The consumer satisfaction/dissatisfaction literature scales measuring positive and/or negative disconfirmation
(CS/D) and the service quality literature both state that the (Oliver 1997:103). Absence of disconfirmation, combined
information used by buyers in their choice decisions has a with a positive valence of the predictive expectation for the
direct effect on the formation of predictive expectations of chosen good or service, means that the positive predictive
perceived performance (Boulding et al. 1993; Oliver 1997; expectation is confirmed by perceived performance,
Keaveney, Parthasarathy / ONLINE SERVICES 377

resulting in satisfaction. Barring any unpleasant surprises jective and evaluative, it does provide a semblance of
in performance, continued use by consumers should vicarious experience.
result. With a heuristic mode of processing, such as relying on
In the case of a thoughtful purchase decision, as the opinions or judgments of others, decisions are based on
described above, contradictory information from actual a more superficial assessment of information (Furse et al.
experience with a product or service may be cognitively 1984). Without the well-informed expectations that result
resisted and confirming evidence sought. “Deviation from from independent decision processing, consumers are vul-
an attitude that is supported by a complex cognitive struc- nerable to negative disconfirmation if services do not per-
ture is said to involve a high psychological cost to the indi- form for them in the way that their friends have described
vidual” (Pritchard, Havitz, and Howard 1999:335). (Oliver 1997). Moreover, we know that service consump-
Rethinking information in a complex informational tion can be highly individualized and difficult to compre-
schema is effortful, disruptive, and dissonant; consumers hend until personally experienced. Thus, consumers who
become committed to the decision they worked so hard to rely on vicarious experience to make service choices may
reach (Pritchard et al. 1999). Strong expectations can even risk disappointment and dissatisfaction if their personal
act to edit or filter perceived performance in such a way experiences differ from others’ experiences. When expec-
that consumers see what they expect to see and overlook tations are violated by contrary experience, motivation to
discrepant information (Hoch and Deighton 1989). Again, correct the problem—by switching services or discontinu-
barring any clear and unambiguous unpleasant surprises in ing use—is high (Hoch and Deighton 1989).
performance, consumers will be motivated to continue
their satisfied use of the product or service (Oliva et al. Hypothesis 2: Online service continuers were influenced
1995.) by interpersonal sources of information when mak-
ing their subscription decisions less than online ser-
Finally, evidence from the advertising literature pro-
vice switchers.
vides yet further support for this line of reasoning. When
consumers learn about a product and make product deci- Experiential sources of information. When faced with a
sions under the influence of advertising, the advertising in- purchase decision, consumers first engage in internal
formation may be said to prime or “transform the product search, examining information in memory about past ex-
experience” (Hoch and Deighton 1989). Unless perceived periences and product-relevant knowledge (Bettman
product performance is unambiguously negative, prior ex- 1979). The more perceived risk associated with the pur-
pectations based on advertising information predominate chase situation, as in the case of services, the more con-
and perceptions of performance are assimilated (Deighton sumers prefer their own observations and experiences as
1984). In summary, on the basis of theory and research in sources of information (Murray 1991).
the satisfaction, information-processing, experiential- Hoch and Deighton (1989) observed that “consumers
learning, and advertising literatures, we propose the fol- tend to grant special status to conclusions drawn from
lowing hypothesis: experience.” They theorized that motivation and involve-
ment in the decision tend to be higher, consumers have
Hypothesis 1: Online service continuers were influenced control over the pace and content of the learning, and
by external sources of information when making source credibility is high because the interests of source
their subscription decisions more than online ser-
and consumer are one. Research suggests that information
vice switchers.
from experience is likely to have a greater influence on
Interpersonal sources of information. Other customers behavior than information from other sources (Smith and
rely on word-of-mouth opinions of others more than on Swinyard 1982).
their own decision-making processes when making prod- Having already used a service, or having service-rele-
uct or service decisions. Consumers who have little prior vant knowledge in memory, means that the consumer not
experience with a product or service, who find the deci- only has personal knowledge about service attributes but
sion-making process difficult, or who have little confi- also has personal experience about how this service works
dence in their own abilities to judge the product or service for him or her and satisfies his or her needs. As a respon-
may prefer to ask advice from others perceived as knowl- dent in one major study of customer expectations of ser-
edgeable (Furse et al. 1984). Consumers also tend to use vice was quoted as saying, “My expectations are definitely
more, and to have more confidence in, interpersonal influenced by my past experience . . . my expectations are
sources of information when purchasing services than more realistic because of the knowledge I’ve gained”
when purchasing goods (Murray 1991). This is thought to (Zeithaml et al. 1993:10). Thus, predictive expectations
be because of the experiential nature of services, which based on experience are quite accurate and probabilities of
renders services difficult to evaluate prior to purchase and disconfirmation low, as long as subsequent experiences
use. While information from other individuals is both sub- are not surprisingly different.
378 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

Consumers learn from their experiences with products tive satisfaction; higher overall satisfaction in turn was
and services and update what they already know linked to increased subsequent usage.
(Boulding et al. 1993). Consumers who already have prior A third explanation is offered by theories of consumer
experience with a product or service, or who have product- learning. Because consumers act as though the cost of
related knowledge, learn quickly from experience (Hoch learning is greater than the cost of making a mistake (Hoch
and Deighton 1989). If what experience has to teach is un- and Deighton 1989), heavy users of a particular online ser-
ambiguously positive or negative, experienced consumers vice may feel that they have developed nontransferable,
will quickly adjust their subsequent evaluations in the provider-specific skills. Having invested time and energy
same direction. If, however, continued experience is only to become facile at one online service, consumers may be
slightly different from past experience, consumers will ei- unwilling to learn how to use an alternative product (Alba
ther ignore or assimilate the new information. In this case, and Hutchinson 1987; Hoch and Deighton 1989). Based
there is little to learn from experience and subsequent eval- on the above discussion,
uations will not change. Thus, barring clear and unex-
pected negative surprises in performance, we expect the Hypothesis 4a: Online service continuers use the online
following relationship to hold: service more frequently than online service
switchers.
Hypothesis 3: Online service continuers were influenced Hypothesis 4b: Online service continuers use the online
by experiential sources of information when making service more intensively (i.e., on average, their on-
their subscription decisions more than online ser- line sessions continue for longer periods of time)
vice switchers. than online service switchers.
Hypothesis 4c: Online service continuers exhibit higher
overall service usage than online service switchers.
Service Usage
Propensity for Risk-Taking Behavior
No studies to date have directly investigated the rela-
tionship between levels of service usage and switching We are aware of no studies that examine the direct rela-
behavior. However, a number of related studies lead us to tionship between propensity for risk-taking behavior and
hypothesize that heavier users of the service will be less actual service-switching behavior. However, there are a
likely to switch. First, the disconfirmation paradigm pre- number of reasons why we expect the two to be related.
dicts that frequent usage should provide customers with Both propensity for risk-taking behavior and brand-
relatively accurate and realistic performance expectations, switching behavior have been empirically shown to be cor-
thereby decreasing disconfirmation and increasing satis- related with a third, hypothetically upstream variable
faction and repurchase intentions (Anderson and Sullivan known as “optimum stimulation level” (Raju 1980). The-
1993). Results from the Swedish Customer Satisfaction ories of optimum stimulation level (OSL) suggest that
Barometer (SCSB) project indicate that higher levels of individuals prefer a certain level of stimulation, which
customer usage are associated with lower incidences of may be termed their “optimum stimulation level.” When
disconfirmation, somewhat higher levels of satisfaction, an individual’s stimulation level is below optimum, he or
and higher repurchase intentions (Anderson 1994). she will take action to raise it; when it is above optimum,
From an overall satisfaction perspective, when a cus- he or she will act to reduce it. One way in which consumers
tomer uses a service frequently or intensely, he or she raise or lower their optimum stimulation levels is through
develops a fairly strong and—as inferred by continued their behavior in the marketplace. Thus, individuals with
use—presumably positive attitude about it. A consumer higher OSLs reported that they had both higher risk-taking
with multiple positive service experiences will be increas- and brand-switching tendencies than individuals with
ingly insensitive to discrete instances of service failure and lower OSLs (Raju 1980). The question becomes, if the
may endure a number of problems before positive satisfac- optimum stimulation level is positively associated with a
tion is revised downward (Oliva et al. 1995; Oliver 1997). propensity for risk-taking behavior, and the optimum stim-
In a study of frequently purchased consumer goods, ulation level is positively associated with brand-switching
LaBarbera and Mazursky (1983) found that the longer the behavior, is a propensity for risk-taking behavior associ-
sequence of repeat purchase behavior, the more that expe- ated with brand- (or service-) switching behavior?
rience with the brand accounted for repurchase behavior Both theory and evidence in the services marketing lit-
and the lower the role of satisfaction. Bolton and Lemon erature suggest that customers perceive the selection and
(1999) found evidence of a dynamic relationship between purchase of services to be riskier than goods (Guseman
customers’ prior usage, cumulative satisfaction evalua- 1981; Murray 1991; Murray and Schlacter 1990). It would
tions, and subsequent usage: Customers with positive be reasonable to expect that individuals with higher risk-
evaluations of their prior usage experience higher cumula- taking propensities might be more likely than their lower-
Keaveney, Parthasarathy / ONLINE SERVICES 379

risk-taking counterparts to select and purchase these “risk- online services or at evaluating online service perfor-
ier” services (particularly if their stimulation levels were to mance.
fall below optimum levels). Let us suppose, for the sake of
argument, that the consumer views switching from one Hypothesis 6: Online service continuers have higher av-
service provider to another as analogous to selecting and erage income levels than online service switchers.
purchasing a new service—one must engage in a similar Hypothesis 7: Online service continuers have higher av-
process of selecting and choosing the new service pro- erage education levels than online service switchers.
vider. Like selecting and choosing services, service-
switching behavior can be seen as an inherently risky ac-
tivity. By extension, then, we might expect that individuals RESEARCH METHOD
with higher propensities for risk taking would be more
willing to engage in the activity of switching among ser- The Sample
vice providers should the need arise. Thus, we propose the
following: Sampling frame. To test these hypotheses, data were
collected from present and past subscribers of online ser-
Hypothesis 5: Online service continuers have a lower vices (e.g., America Online, Delphi, Prodigy, MSN, and
propensity for risk-taking behavior than online ser- CompuServe). A major national online service provider
vice switchers. provided a list of all customers who had ever subscribed to
the service since the company’s inception. The enormity
Demographics of this list necessitated reducing the sampling frame. A
computer program generated a random number between 1
Customers with higher incomes and education levels and 26, with the random number used to indicate a letter of
may be capable of developing sophisticated and probably the alphabet. The number 13, corresponding to the letter M
accurate estimates of what to expect from a service. For was selected. The company then generated a list of all cur-
example, customers with higher incomes may have experi- rent and former customers whose user names began with
ence with more frequent usage of services or usage of a the letter M. Thus, some people on the list were still sub-
greater variety of services. In the online world, higher- scribers, some had switched services, some had switched
income users tend to be more experienced “surfers” services a number of times, and some no longer subscribed
(Cyberatlas.com 2000b). Higher incomes also allow cus- to any online service.
tomers to afford to choose “buy” in potential “make-ver-
As the list consisted of more than 200,000 individuals,
sus-buy” service decisions, potentially giving them more
it was further reduced in size by a computer program writ-
experience with a wider variety of services.
ten to randomly select 40,000 records. After cleaning the
Higher education levels may also provide consumers
data by eliminating duplicate user records and records
with greater skills in forming hypotheses about future ser-
with missing data, a base list of 28,217 records was
vice performance. Because services are intangible, they
obtained. Each record contained the user name, date of ini-
can require somewhat more imagination, vision, or
tial subscription with the service, date of termination of the
hypothesizing to predict what actually using the service
service (if any), name, and address.
would be like. This is particularly true of services having
experiential or credence properties where, even after expe- Sample size and selection. Sample size was estimated
riencing the service, many consumers find evaluation of using both power analysis and consideration of recom-
service performance to be complex and difficult (Murray mended procedures for discriminant function analysis.
1991; Nelson 1970). Power analysis at an alpha level of .01 and a low-to-moder-
In contrast, lower income and less-educated service us- ate anticipated effect size of .35 requires a sample size of
ers may find that their expectations were ambiguous, their 200 respondents; a minimum alpha level of .10, holding
ability to learn from experience limited (Hoch and effect size constant, requires only 100 respondents,
Deighton 1989), their assessments uncertain, and their whereas a midrange alpha of .05 requires 130 respondents
evaluations of the service more vulnerable to instances of (Cohen and Cohen 1980). Discriminant function analysis
dissatisfaction. For example, the Internet’s older, less-edu- is sensitive to sample size as well; some experts suggest
cated, and lower income users tend to lack confidence in that 20 observations for each predictor variable are ideal
their computer skills (Cyberatlas.com 2000a). Older, less- (Hair, Anderson, Tatham, and Black 1995; Tabachnick
educated, and lower income computer users reported that and Fidell 1983). Since our study has eight independent
they felt doubtful about their computer proficiency signifi- variables plus one interaction term, this suggested a sam-
cantly more often than younger, more highly educated, ple of about 180 observations. Taken together, a sample
higher income users. Thus, less-educated, lower income size of around 200 respondents was deemed desirable.
customers may not be as skilled at forecasting the use of Based on a reasonably conservative estimate of a 20 per-
380 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

cent response rate for a consumer mail survey, 1,000 indi- study’s participants or a 20.5 percent response rate relative
viduals were sampled. to the initial sample. Of the 205 respondents, 72 returned
Records in the base list were sorted on the basis of pres- “continuer” and 133 returned “quitter” surveys.
ence or absence of a termination date to ensure that a mix Following Armstrong and Overton’s (1977) method for
of continuers and switchers would be sampled. Because a checking possible nonresponse bias, early respondents
relatively equal distribution between groups was desired (defined as those who responded within the first 2 weeks of
for the most robust estimation of the discriminant function the mailing date) were compared with later respondents on
(Tabachnick and Fidell 1983), 500 names were selected each of the measured variables. No significant differences
randomly from each group (with and without termination in means were found between the two groups, at p < .05, on
dates). any measured variables.

Data Collection Measures of Variables

In the first wave of the data collection process, the ini- Relative sources of informational influence. Sources of
tial sample of 1,000 was sent a postcard by mail and asked influence were conceptualized as the relative influence of
whether they would be willing to participate in the study. different sources of information on the consumers’ sub-
They were also asked (1) if they currently subscribed to a scription decisions. The concept was operationalized us-
paid computer online service; (2) whether they had ever, at ing three categories of consumer information adopted
any time in the past, subscribed to an online service for from Kotler (1997). External sources of information were
more than 3 months that they later dropped; and (3) their defined as heterophilous sources, or sources from outside
names and addresses. The 3-month adoption period was the customer’s social system, including mass media, ad-
specified in an effort to eliminate spurious switching vertising, articles, brochures, pamphlets, reviews, and
among trial offers and focus on service switching after a other impersonal sources. Thus, in this study, external
reasonable adoption-and-use period. One hundred forty- sources included both commercial sources and public
three postcards were returned by the post office as sources of information. Interpersonal sources of informa-
undeliverable. tion were defined as homophilous sources, or interper-
Of the 857 remaining postcards, 443 were completed sonal communication sources within the social system,
and returned. Of the 443 respondents, 125 reported that also known as word-of-mouth communications. Experien-
they were currently subscribed to an online service and tial sources of information were defined as personal expe-
had not stopped using any online service provider to which rience with the service or similar services, and general
they had subscribed for more than 3 months (“yes” to item service-relevant knowledge.
1 and “no” to item 2). This group, designated the As Kotler (1997) notes, “Of key interest to the marketer
“continuers,” was composed primarily of people who had are the major information sources to which the consumer
either (1) been continuously subscribed with the list pro- will turn and the relative influence each will have on the
vider or (2) tried the list provider but continuously sub- subsequent purchase decision [italics added]” (p. 193).
scribed with a different provider. Two hundred and eigh- Thus, respondents were asked to distribute 100 points
teen respondents reported that they had stopped using an among the three types of information sources according to
online service provider after 3 months at least once (“yes” how much each had influenced the online subscription
to item 2). This group, designated the “quitters” group, decision. Items for all variables are reported in the
was composed primarily of people whose most recent appendix.
online service switch was either (1) from the list provider
to a new online service provider, (2) between other online Usage level. The consumer’s overall usage of online
service providers, or (3) from another service provider to services was conceptualized as a function of two issues:
the list provider. frequency and intensity (Ram and Jung 1990). Frequency
Questionnaires were sent by mail to the 443 respon- of use was defined as the number of times that customers
dents who had agreed to participate in the study. The ques- engaged the online service, operationalized as the number
tionnaires differed somewhat, depending on the above of times they logged on during a specified time period. In-
classifications: the “continuer” questionnaire asked about tensity was defined as the depth of involvement in the on-
their current continuous subscription, while the “quitter” line activity, operationalized as the amount of time in
questionnaire focused on the online service provider that hours they interacted with the service each time they
they had most recently stopped using. Two reminder post- logged on.
card mailings were sent at 15-day intervals. At the end of Following Ram and Jung (1990), the product of the two
the 45-day survey period, a total of 205 questionnaires items was used to compute overall service usage. For
were returned, for a 46 percent response rate among the example, the consumer who used the service every day but
Keaveney, Parthasarathy / ONLINE SERVICES 381

for less than 15 minutes per occasion (the type of person TABLE 1
who only checks e-mail) would have lower overall service Characteristics of the Two Samples
usage than the consumer who used the service only once a Sample 1 Sample 2
week but spends more than 2 hours per occasion.
Variable Frequency Percentage Frequency Percentage
Propensity for risk-taking behavior. Propensity for Gender
risk-taking behavior is defined as the propensity to enjoy, Male 109 79.6 243 86.5
take advantage of, or otherwise seek new and potentially Female 25 18.2 38 13.5
Marital status
risky activities and experiences. The eight-item scale de-
Single 38 25.7 70 23.1
veloped by Raju (1980) was used. Reliability figures be- Married 85 57.4 172 56.8
tween .80 and .83 were reported in the original study; Divorced 24 16.2 61 20.1
reliability of the scale in the present study was .91. Widowed 1 0.7 0 0.0
Age
Income and education. In addition to the above, income < 20 years 4 2.7 5 1.7
and education were measured as categorical variables. 20-29 22 15.0 36 11.9
30-39 45 30.6 73 24.1
Nine categories, ranging from less than $20,000 to more
40-49 47 32.0 124 40.9
than $90,000, were used to measure income, while eight 50-59 16 10.9 43 14.2
categories from grade school to advanced degree were > 59 12 8.2 20 6.6
used to measure education. Income
≤ $20,000 1 0.7 6 2.0
$20,001-$30,000 13 8.8 34 11.3
DATA ANALYSIS AND RESULTS $30,001-$40,000 16 10.9 30 9.9
$40,001-$50,000 15 10.2 38 12.6
$50,001-$60,000 19 12.9 34 11.3
Categorization of Groups: $60,001-$70,000 16 10.9 29 9.6
Switchers Versus Continuers $70,001-$80,000 22 15.0 61 20.2
$80,001-$90,000 12 8.2 18 6.0
> $90,000 33 22.4 52 17.2
Further classification of the “quitter” respondents as
Education
“service discontinuers” or “service switchers” was con- Grade school 0 0 1 0.3
ducted based on more detailed information provided by Some high school 5 3.5 8 2.7
several items in the questionnaire. As discussed earlier, High school graduate 8 5.6 30 10.0
service discontinuers are those former customers who Trade/tech graduate 1 0.7 2 0.7
Some college 23 16.2 43 14.4
have stopped using a service altogether—in this case, they
College graduate 58 40.8 127 42.5
had stopped using online services. Service switchers are Advanced degree 47 33.1 88 29.4
those customers who stopped using the services of one ser-
vice provider and switched to become the customers of a
different service provider (Keaveney 1995). In the “quit-
and education and is more predominantly male than the
ter” questionnaire, respondents were asked to name the
global Internet population demographics reported at pub-
online service they had most recently stopped using and to
lication date (Cyberatlas.com 2000d).
provide the start and stop dates (month and year) of the
subscription. They were also asked to name the online ser-
vice provider to which they currently subscribed and the Discriminant Function Analysis and Results
start date (month and year) of the subscription. Based on
this analysis, 82 respondents had discontinued use of an To test the ability of the hypothesized variables to dis-
online service and were currently subscribed to a different criminate between switchers and continuers, two-group
online service provider. This group was classified as ser- discriminant function analysis was used. The independent
vice switchers. The remaining 51 respondents had discon- variables were external influence, interpersonal influence,
tinued use of an online service provider but had not sub- experiential influence, utilization rate (frequency, inten-
scribed with a new service. These service discontinuers sity, and overall), risk-taking behavior, income, and educa-
were not used in the present study because this group could tion. To check the validity of the models, a proportional
not be said to have switched from one service provider to a 50-50 split sample validation procedure was used. Results
different service provider. In summary, 69 usable are shown in Tables 2, 3, and 4. Results reported in the
continuer surveys and 82 switcher surveys were used in the tables are for the analysis sample, as recommended by
discriminant analysis. Hair et al. (1995).
Descriptive statistics for the sample are reported in The discriminant function was significant (Wilks’s
Table 1. The sample is somewhat higher in age, income, lambda = .66, p < .02), thereby confirming overall differ-
382 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

TABLE 2
Results of the Discriminant Analysis
Univariate F
Discriminant Loadings Study 2 Study 2
Study 1 Replication Extension
Study 2 Study 2
Hypothesis Attribute Study 1 Replication Extension Ratio p Ratio p Ratio p
1 External influence 0.371 0.255 0.225 4.67 .03 4.16 .04 4.16 .04
2 Interpersonal influence –0.670 –0.670 –0.593 15.24 .00 28.77 .00 28.77 .00
3 Experiential influence 0.361 0.564 0.499 4.43 .04 20.39 .00 20.39 .00
4a Usage-frequency 0.651 0.508 0.449 14.37 .00 16.55 .00 16.55 .00
4b Usage-intensity 0.223 0.145 0.128 1.69 .20 1.34 .25 1.34 .25
4c Usage-overall 0.414 0.252 0.223 5.82 .02 4.07 .05 4.07 .05
5 Risk-taking behavior 0.388 0.415 0.368 5.11 .03 11.07 .00 11.07 .00
6 Income 0.379 0.436 0.386 4.86 .03 12.22 .00 12.22 .00
7 Education 0.316 0.235 0.208 3.40 .07 3.55 .06 3.55 .06
8 Satisfaction — — 0.422 — — — — 14.60 .00
9 Involvement — — 0.269 — — — — 5.95 .02

ences in characteristics between switchers and continuers. from switchers by their higher levels of some types of ser-
Predictive accuracy of the discriminant function was vice usage. Online service continuers can be discriminated
assessed by comparing the hit ratios of the analysis from switchers by higher frequency of use (.65, p < .00)
(68.1%), holdout (62.7%), and overall (67.6%) samples and overall usage (.41, p < .02), but not by intensity of use
with chance. Because sample sizes in each of the groups (.22, p < .20).
are about equal, simple, proportional, and maximum Hypothesis 6, which predicted that online service
chance criteria are all about 50 percent. Hair et al. (1995) continuers had higher average income levels than switch-
recommended that classification accuracy should be at ers, was supported (.38, p < .03). Hypothesis 7 was sup-
least 125 percent of that achieved by chance, making the ported only at p < .07, indicating somewhat qualified sup-
desired cutoff for this study to be 62.5 percent. Hit ratios port that online service continuers have higher average
for each sample exceed the recommended value. Signifi- education levels than switchers (coefficient .32).
cance of the discriminatory power of the classification Hypothesis 5 proposed that online service continuers
matrix, when compared to chance, is indicated by Press’s would have a lower propensity for risk-taking behavior
Q statistic. Press’s Q-values were significant for the analy- than online service switchers. However, results indicated
sis (9.06, p < .01), holdout (4.31, p < .05), and overall that continuers had a higher propensity for risk-taking
(16.94, p < .01) samples. behavior than switchers (coefficient .39, p < .03). Group
Seven of the nine hypotheses had discriminant loadings means are reported in Table 5.
with a significance level of p < .05; an eighth was sup-
ported at p < .07. Each of the hypotheses about informa-
tional influence was supported at the p < .04 level or better. STUDY 2: REPLICATION AND EXTENSION
Hypothesis 1, which proposed that online service
continuers relied more on external sources of information Involvement and satisfaction are consistent themes
when making their subscription decisions than did online underlying hypothesis development in Study 1, but they
service switchers, was supported by a structure coefficient were neither explicitly measured nor tested. Study 2 repli-
of .37 (p < .03). Hypothesis 2, which proposed that online cates the first study in a new sample and extends the set of
service continuers relied less on interpersonal sources of independent variables to include measures of satisfaction
information when making the subscription decision than and involvement.
did switchers, was supported with a structure coefficient of
–.67 (p < .00). Hypothesis 3, which proposed that Conceptual Development
continuers relied more on experiential sources of informa- and Research Hypotheses
tion than did switchers, was supported by a structure coef-
ficient of .36 (p < .04). Satisfaction

Hypotheses 4a and 4c were supported, providing evi- Prior studies have found that dissatisfaction is associ-
dence that online service continuers can be discriminated ated with increased brand- (product-) switching behavior
Keaveney, Parthasarathy / ONLINE SERVICES 383

TABLE 3 TABLE 4
Classification Statistics Classification Results
Study 2 Study 2 Predicted Membership
Item Study 1 Replication Extension
Sample Type Actual Membership Continuers Switchers Total
Wilks’s lambda .66 (p = .02) .67 (p = .00) .61 (p = 0.00)
Study 1
Analysis Continuers 27 7 34
Percentage correctly
sample Switchers 15 20 35
classified
Holdout Continuers 22 10 32
Analysis sample 68.1 73.1 83.8
sample Switchers 15 20 35
Holdout sample 62.7 72.6 77.8
Complete Continuers 50 16 66
Complete sample 67.6 70.6 79.6
sample Switchers 28 42 70
Proportional chance
Classification ratio (%)
criterion (%) 50.0 50.0 50.0
Analysis sample = 68.1
Maximum chance
Holdout sample = 62.7
criterion (%) 50.7 50.8 50.8
Complete sample = 67.6
Press’s Q
Study 2 (replication)
Analysis sample 9.06 (p < .01) 27.69 (p < .01) 59.57 (p < .01)
Analysis Continuers 49 17 66
Holdout sample 4.31 (p < .05) 27.56 (p < .01) 41.67 (p < .01)
sample Switchers 18 46 64
Holdout Continuers 44 22 66
sample Switchers 15 54 69
Complete Continuers 87 45 132
(LaBarbera and Mazursky 1983), service-switching be- sample Switchers 33 100 133
havior (Bansal and Taylor 1999; Bolton and Bronkhorst Classification ratio (%)
Analysis sample = 73.1
1995; Keaveney 1995; Roos 1999), and both product- and
Holdout sample = 72.6
service-switching intentions (e.g., Anderson 1994; Ander- Complete sample = 70.6
son and Sullivan 1993; Boulding et al. 1993; Cronin and
Taylor 1992; Oliva et al. 1995; Oliver 1997; Reichheld and Study 2 (extension)
Sasser 1990; Yi 1990; Zeithaml et al. 1996). Based on Analysis Continuers 56 10 66
sample Switchers 11 53 64
prior work, we, too, expect the following:
Holdout Continuers 51 15 66
sample Switchers 15 54 69
Hypothesis 8: Online service continuers are more satis- Complete Continuers 97 35 132
fied with the service than online service switchers. sample Switchers 19 114 133
Classification ratio (%)
Involvement Analysis sample = 83.8
Holdout sample = 77.8
More highly involved consumers have been found to Complete sample = 79.6
report higher levels of positive disconfirmation and satis-
faction (Oliver and Bearden 1983; Richins and Bloch
1991). These higher levels of satisfaction tend to be endur-
ing. Involved consumers engage in greater prepurchase Hypothesis 9: Online service continuers experience
search (Beatty and Smith 1987) and greater deliberation in greater involvement with the service than online ser-
choice (Celsi and Olson 1988). Higher levels of involve- vice switchers.
ment have been associated with higher levels of commit-
ment to a decision and resistance to belief change (Prit- Research Method
chard et al. 1999).
Theories of cognitive consistency and assimilation pre- The research methods used for Study 1 were generally
dict that under conditions of high product and/or service followed, with a few modifications. The procedure is
involvement, satisfied customers will be motivated to pre- briefly reviewed here and changes noted.
serve their satisfied state across a wide range of perfor-
The Sample
mances, even when performance is somewhat discrepant.
Researchers have found that satisfied customers tolerate The sampling frame of 28,217 names provided by the
greater degrees of poor performance as their product and/ major online service provider and used in Study 1 was
or service involvement increases (Oliva et al. 1995). This once again used as the base list, after removing the records
perseverance may continue until negative performance in- used in Study 1. In an effort to increase the number of
formation is sufficiently discrepant from the cognitive responses, a randomly selected sample of 2,000 individu-
schema as to demand attention and subsequent reprocess- als was generated. The base list was sorted into quitters
ing (Fiske and Taylor 1984). Thus, and continuers based on the presence or absence of a
384 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

TABLE 5
Group Means by Attribute
Study 1 Study 2 Replication Study 2 Extension
Attribute Continuers Switchers Continuers Switchers Continuers Switchers
External influence 60.88 42.29 58.92 44.92 58.92 44.92
Interpersonal influence 13.09 43.57 12.80 44.29 12.80 44.29
Experiential influence 25.77 12.71 33.58 10.78 33.58 10.78
Usage-frequency 4.32 3.29 4.02 3.16 4.02 3.16
Usage-intensity 3.26 2.89 3.11 2.88 3.11 2.88
Usage 14.41 10.51 12.74 10.31 12.74 10.31
Risk-taking behavior 4.52 3.73 4.61 3.78 4.61 3.78
Income 6.26 5.06 6.59 5.25 6.59 5.25
Education 6.32 5.94 5.91 5.47 5.91 5.47
Satisfaction — — — — 4.63 3.73
Involvement — — — — 4.88 4.33

service termination date, and 1,000 individuals were ran- study, based on typical scales of satisfaction (see Yi 1990
domly selected from each group. or Oliver 1997 for excellent reviews). Items are shown in
Data Collection the appendix. Reliability for the satisfaction scale was .75.

The 2000 individuals were sent postcards asking for Product involvement. Product involvement was defined
their participation in the study. Once again, they were as interest and expertise in the product class, following
asked (1) if they currently subscribed to an online service, Zinkhan and Locander (1988). Their scale was selected
(2) whether they had ever stopped using an online service because, like the present study, it focused on customers’in-
provider after using it for at least 3 months, and (3) their volvement and interest in a technology-related area (i.e.,
names and addresses. calculators). The seven-item scale included measures of
One thousand one hundred and six postcards were com- involvement, interest, expertise, and experience with the
pleted and returned. Of the 1,106 respondents, 495 were product class. Reliability was .80 in this study, compared
sent “continuer” questionnaires and 611 were sent “quit- with .87 for Zinkhan and Locander’s (1988) study. Items
ter” questionnaires, following the classification system are shown in the appendix.
described in Study 1. One reminder mailing was sent 15
days following the initial mailing. At the end of the 30-day Data Analysis and Results
survey period, a total of 390 questionnaires were returned,
for a 35 percent response rate among the study’s partici-
Categorization of Groups
pants or a 19.5 percent response rate relative to the initial
postcard mailing. Of the 390 respondents, 136 returned Further classification of the “quitter” respondents as
“continuer” and 254 returned “quitter” surveys. service discontinuers or service switchers was conducted
Once again, to check for possible nonresponse bias, as in Study 1. Of 390 respondents, 136 were continuers.
early respondents (those who responded within the first 2 The remaining 254 quitter responses were composed of
weeks) were compared with later respondents on each of 172 service switchers and 82 service discontinuers. Again,
the measured variables (Armstrong and Overton 1977). service discontinuers were not used in the study. In sum-
No significant differences in means of any measured vari- mary, the analysis included 136 usable continuer surveys
ables were found between the two groups (p < .05). and 172 usable switcher surveys. Characteristics of the
Measures of Variables sample are reported in Table 1.

Measures of sources of informational influence, utili- Discriminant Function Analysis and Results
zation level, risk-taking propensity, income, and education
were the same as in Study 1. Reliability of the risk-taking Replication. The first phase of data analysis examined
propensity scale was computed again for Study 2; reliabil- whether the results of Study 1 were replicated in a different
ity was .93. Measures of satisfaction and involvement are sample. Following the procedures applied in Study 1, nine
discussed below. Items are reported in the appendix. variables were subjected to two-group discriminant func-
tion analysis to determine their effectiveness in discrimi-
Satisfaction. Satisfaction was defined as an overall cog- nating between switchers and continuers. Also as in
nitive and affective state of happiness and contentment Study 1, a proportional 50-50 split sample validation pro-
(Oliver 1997). Three items were developed for use in this cedure was used to check the classification accuracy of the
Keaveney, Parthasarathy / ONLINE SERVICES 385

discriminant function. As reported below and in the tables, p < .01), holdout (41.67, p < .01), and overall (93.02, p <
Study 2 replicates the results of Study 1. .01) samples.
The independent variables were (Hypothesis 1) exter- Both new hypotheses, Hypotheses 8 and 9, were sup-
nal informational influence; (Hypothesis 2) interpersonal ported at the p < .02 level or better. Hypothesis 8, which
informational influence; (Hypothesis 3) experiential proposed that online service continuers would be discrimi-
informational influence; (Hypotheses 4a, 4b, and 4c) fre- nated from online service switchers by higher levels of sat-
quency, intensity, and overall usage; (Hypothesis 5) pro- isfaction with the service, was supported by a structure
pensity for risk-taking behavior; (Hypothesis 6) income; coefficient of .42 (p < .00). Hypothesis 9 proposed that
and (Hypothesis 7) education. As before, the discriminant online service continuers would be discriminated from
function was significant (Wilks’s lambda = .67, p = .00), online service switchers by greater involvement with the
again confirming overall differences in characteristics service. Hypothesis 2 was supported with a structure coef-
between switchers and continuers. Hit ratios of the analy- ficient of .27 (p < .02). The significance of the other vari-
sis (73.1%), holdout (72.6%), and overall (70.6%) samples ables in the discriminant function equation did not change.
were well in excess of the 62.5 percent cutoff rate recom- Thus, the improvement in the classification accuracy of
mended for a 25 percent improvement over chance, again the discriminant function appears to be due entirely to the
confirming the predictive accuracy of the discriminant addition of satisfaction and involvement to the model.
function. Press’s Q-value was significant for the analysis Implications of Study 1 and Study 2 (replication and
(27.7, p < .01), holdout (27.6, p < .01), and overall (44.83, extension) for managers and researchers are discussed in
p < .01) samples. Discriminant function and classification the following sections.
results are shown in Tables 2, 3, and 4.
As in Study 1, continuers were discriminated from
DISCUSSION
switchers by their use of information sources. Continuers
were significantly more likely than switchers to use exter-
nal information sources (coefficient .26, p < .04), were sig- The goal of this research was to explore whether
nificantly less likely than switchers to use interpersonal selected attitudinal, behavioral, and demographic charac-
information sources (coefficient –.67, p < .00), and were teristics of consumers might be effective in discriminating
significantly more likely than switchers to use experiential between switchers and continuers of online services. Two
information sources (coefficient .56, p < .00) when making empirical studies, conducted in field settings, supported
the online subscription decision. Once subscribed, the general proposition that online service switchers have
continuers used the online service significantly more fre- characteristics that are identifiably and significantly dif-
quently than switchers (coefficient .51, p < .00) and exhib- ferent from those of online service continuers.
ited greater overall usage (coefficient .25, p < .05), but no
differences in the intensity of usage were detected (coeffi- Contributions of the Research
cient .15, p < .25). Continuers were again characterized by
significantly higher income (coefficient .44, p < .00) but The research contributes to the services marketing liter-
only somewhat higher education levels (coefficient .24, ature by identifying defection-prone customers and pro-
p < .06). The surprising finding that continuers have a sig- viding further explanation of customer service-switching
nificantly higher propensity for risk-taking behavior than behavior. In summary, the profile of an online service
switchers was also replicated (coefficient .42, p < .00). switcher is that of an individual who was influenced to
Group means are shown in Table 5. subscribe to the service through word of mouth, rather
than through research or previous experience; who used
Extension. Study 2 then extended the scope of Study 1 the service less; who was less satisfied and less involved
by including measures of satisfaction and involvement in with the service; and who had a lower income and educa-
the discriminant function analysis. As shown in Tables 2, tion level, as well as a lower propensity for taking risks.
3, and 4, classification results are improved with the addi- This research makes a significant contribution to the
tion of the two attitudinal variables. word-of-mouth literature (WOM) with the interesting
The discriminant function was once again significant finding that switchers were more likely to have relied on
(Wilks’s lambda = .61, p < .00). Hit ratios of the analysis WOM sources when making their subscription decisions.
(83.8%), holdout (77.8%), and overall (79.6%) samples Researchers in the services marketing area recognize the
were substantially improved and are now well above the importance of WOM communications in the context of
62.5 percent cutoff rate suggested for a 25 percent services (Murray 1991; Zeithaml 2000), with some sug-
improvement over chance. Classifications based on the gesting that WOM may be an even more important com-
discriminant function are now 67 percent, 56 percent, and munication tool for services firms than is advertising
59 percent improvements over chance, respectively. (Danaher and Rust 1996a, 1996b; Kordupleski, Rust, and
Press’s Q-values were significant for the analysis (59.57, Zahorik 1993; Murray 1991). The present research clearly
386 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

identifies some possible downside risks heretofore that consumers experienced satisfaction or dissatisfaction
unidentified in the literature. with the different information sources and that informa-
Some of our findings support, and contribute to, the tion satisfaction mediated the information-switching rela-
experiential learning literature (Hoch and Deighton 1989). tionship. This question provides another interesting area
First, continuers had more prior experience with the ser- for future research. Second, the research reported here pro-
vice or with related services than did switchers. Second, vides additional evidence of the behavioral outcomes
continuers had more experience with the service after pur- (here, customer service-switching and continuance behav-
chasing, in the form of increased usage, than did switchers. iors), of satisfaction and/or dissatisfaction, and service
We propose that these results are explained by experiential involvement.
learning theory, but testing of the explicit linkages is a
good topic for future research. Implications for Managers
Our findings that switchers had significantly lower ser-
vice usage than continuers also contributes to the rela- A major contribution of this article is that it empirically
tively new body of services research investigating the rela- identifies and profiles defection-prone customers of
tionships between service usage and customer satisfaction, online services that will enable managers to develop “early
repurchase intentions, and switching (cf. Anderson 1994; warning systems” to target potential switchers and take
Anderson and Sullivan 1993; Bolton and Bronkhurst action to retain them. One goal of this research was to
1995; Bolton and Lemon 1999). While the results reported select variables that were not only theoretically sound but
here provide support for the relationship between service managerially implementable. For example, results indi-
usage and continuance, future research should explore fur- cating that switchers and continuers could be identified by
ther explanations for the link. differences in levels of income and (to some degree) edu-
The findings of this study also extend services market- cation is a particularly useful finding since income and
ing literature in the area of consumer information-processing education are two of the more accessible demographic
theory. Specifically, services literature has revealed that segmentation variables. Service usage data in general
consumers prefer to use external, impersonal sources of should be available given good internal marketing infor-
information less when making purchasing decisions for mation systems, and online service usage data are readily
services than they do when making purchase decisions for available. Measuring the relative importance of different
goods (Murray 1991). The research reported here indi- types of information used when making the subscription
cates that there are repercussions to those preferences, that decision requires only the addition of a brief question at
is, lower use of external, impersonal sources of informa- the time of enrollment, a variation of the question already
tion when making an online service choice was related to asked by many firms (“How did you hear about us?”). Sat-
higher service switching. Services research has also isfaction is another variable already collected by many ser-
shown that consumers are less likely to make an outright vices firms. Items for the remaining variables, risk-taking
purchase or use direct observation or trial as information propensity and service involvement, could be added to
strategies when purchasing services relative to goods online companies’ data collection efforts.
(Murray 1991). The results of this study indicate that On the basis of our results, we recommend that manag-
reduced reliance on these experiential sources is associ- ers target customer retention strategies at both pre- and
ated with increased service-switching behavior. Hence, postpurchase stages of the consumer’s decision process.
service customers’ least preferred modes of information During prepurchase phases, marketers should increase
acquisition are, contrarily, the two modes best suited to customer involvement in the decision-making process,
service continuance. increase trial and other experiential opportunities, make
Finally, this study provides two new contributions to marketer-generated sources of information more accessi-
the satisfaction literature. First, and perhaps most interest- ble or more appealing, and generally help potential cus-
ingly, the study contributes to recent work on information tomers to engage in active learning about the service. Dur-
satisfaction. Spreng, MacKenzie, and Olshavsky (1996) ing postpurchases phases, marketing activities should be
found that customers’ overall satisfaction was composed designed to increase customer satisfaction, involvement,
of both product satisfaction and information satisfaction, and service usage (especially frequency), thereby reduc-
or satisfaction with the information provided to customers ing the likelihood of customer switching. In particular, ser-
as they were making their decisions. (Their study focused vice usage results were consistent with satisfaction the-
only on external, impersonal information sources.) We ory’s proposition that more incidences of satisfied usage
hypothesized that the differential effects of types of infor- should lead to the formation of attitudes that are resilient to
mation were due to the accuracy of predictive expecta- discrete incidences of service failure. Since service fail-
tions. An alternative explanation for our results might be ures are inevitable, service firms might find that
Keaveney, Parthasarathy / ONLINE SERVICES 387

encouraging more usage, greater familiarity, and more customer’s decision to purchase a risky service stimulated
experience with the service, both before and after the pur- increased information search (Murray 1991), which led to
chase decision, would lead their customers to form the making a more involved, or at least a more sound, deci-
somewhat more resilient positive attitudes evidenced by sion—qualities associated with continuance rather than
the continuers. switching behavior.
Finally, future research should examine not only reve-
Implications for Future Research nue generation but also profitability of service switchers
and continuers. Recent articles have begun to emphasize
The research reported here contributes another set of the importance of segmenting the “right” customers
findings to the study of customer service-switching behav- (Blattberg and Deighton 1996; Dowling and Uncles 1997;
ior. Much work remains. Some ideas for future research Jeffrey and Franco 1996). It will be important for service
are offered. managers to determine who among the potential switchers
The study was conducted among a field sample of for- should be targeted for retention.
mer and current customers of a major online service pro-
APPENDIX
vider. Future research should also examine the general-
izability of results across other subscription-based services Measurement Items
such as telecommunications, cable, insurance, public utili-
ties, health care, and other membership-based services. In Sources of Influence
addition, the study was concerned with the difference
between two broadly defined groups—switchers versus How much did each of these sources influence you to sub-
continuers—and did not further classify switchers by the scribe to [this/that]a service? Please make sure that the total
frequency of their switching behavior or the completeness equals 100 percent.
of the switch. Future research work might examine the dif-
1. [External sources of influence]: _____
ference between frequent and infrequent switchers (e.g., Articles, reviews, advertising, or other activities of
as characterizes some customers in the long-distance tele- the company
phone industry) and between complete and “partial” or
“incomplete” switchers (those customers who reduce uti- 2. [Interpersonal sources of influence]: _____
lization or transfer part, but not all, of their accounts). Opinions of friends, colleagues, relatives, or others
One limitation of discriminant function analysis is that,
like many multivariate analysis techniques, it permits one 3. [Experiential sources of influence]: _____
to determine the overall effects of a set of variables on the My own personal experience and general computer
knowledge
dependent variable but does not allow for examination of
Total: _____
relationships between the variables. The results can be
thought of as providing a broad-brush outline of the
domain—variables x, y, z are good predictors; variables a,
b, c are not—with the investigation of causal relationships Service Usage
the work of subsequent research stages. This research gen-
erates many interesting causal questions regarding the 1. Frequency: Typically, how often [do/did] you use this ser-
vice?
underlying mechanisms through which the various attitu-
dinal, behavior, and demographic variables ultimately _____ Less than once a month
affect switching behavior. _____ Once a month
The unexpected finding that continuers, rather than _____ Once a week
switchers, had higher risk-taking propensities was puz- _____ 2 to 3 times a week
zling and is a good topic for future investigation. As dis- _____ Most days
cussed when generating the hypothesis, both theory and
evidence suggest that customers perceive services to be 2. Intensity: Typically, how much time [do/did] you spend us-
ing the service each time you logged on?
riskier than goods. Perhaps the risk-taking aspect of the
choice was in the adoption of the risky new service, not in _____ Less than 15 minutes
switching from one service provider to another within a _____ 15 to 30 minutes
now-familiar service class. Another possibility, in the fast- _____ 31 minutes to 1 hour
changing world of technology, is that the riskier choice is _____ 1 to 2 hours
to remain loyal to a service provider in the face of many _____ More than 2 hours
new market entrants. Alternately, perhaps the risk-prone
388 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

Risk-Taking Scale (Raju 1980) 3. High school graduate


4. Trade/tech graduate
1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree 5. Some college
1. When I eat out, I like to try the most unusual items the res- 6. College graduate
taurant serves even if I am not sure I would like them. 7. Advanced degree
2. I am the kind of person who would try any new product
a. The brackets refer to [continuer/switcher] verisons of the surveys.
once. Continuers answered with regard to the service they currently subscribe
3. When I go to a restaurant, I find it safer to order dishes I am to; switchers answered with regard to the service they most recently
familiar with (reverse). switched from.
4. I am cautious in trying new/different products (reverse).
5. Even for an important date or dinner, I wouldn’t be wary of
trying a new or unfamiliar restaurant REFERENCES
6. I would rather stick to a brand I usually buy than try some-
thing I am not very sure of (reverse). Alba, Joseph W. and J. Wesley Hutchinson. 1987. “Dimensions of Con-
7. I never buy something I don’t know about at the risk of sumer Expertise.” Journal of Consumer Research 13 (March): 411-
454.
making a mistake (reverse). . 1994. “Cross-Category Variation in Customer Satisfaction and
Retention.” Marketing Letters 5 (1): 19-30.
. 1996. “Customer Satisfaction and Price Tolerance.” Marketing
Satisfaction Letters 7 (3): 265-274.
Anderson, Eugene W. and Mary Sullivan. 1993. “The Antecedents and
Consequences of Customer Satisfaction for Firms.” Marketing Sci-
1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree ence 12 (Spring): 125-143.
1. On the whole, I [am/was] satisfied with my experience with Armstrong, J. Scott and Terry S. Overton. 1977. “Estimating
[this/that] service. Nonresponse Bias in Mail Surveys.” Journal of Marketing Research
14 (August): 399-403.
2. Overall, my negative experience [outweighs/outweighed]
Bansal, Harvir S. and Shirley F. Taylor 1999. “The Service Provider
my positive experience with [this/that] service (reverse). Switching Model (SPSM): A Model of Consumer Switching Behav-
3. In general, I [am/was] happy with the service experience. ior in the Services Industry.” Journal of Service Research 2 (Novem-
ber): 200-218.
Beatty, Sharon E. and Scott M. Smith. 1987. “External Search Effort: An
Investigation Across Several Product Categories.” Journal of Con-
Interest /Involvement sumer Research 1:83-95.
Berning, Carol A. and Jacob Jacoby. 1974. “Patterns of Information Ac-
1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree quisition in New Product Purchases.” Journal of Consumer Research
1 (September): 18-22.
1. I am very interested in online services. Bettman, James R. 1979. An Information Processing Theory of Con-
2. My level of involvement with online services is high. sumer Choice. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
3. I am particularly engaged in the online service environ- Blattberg, Robert C. and John Deighton. 1996. “Manage Marketing by
ment. the Customer Equity Test.” Harvard Business Review 74 (July-Au-
4. I consider myself an expert on the online electronic envi- gust): 136-144.
Bolton, Ruth N. and Tina M. Bronkhorst. 1995. “The Relationship Be-
ronment. tween Customer Complaints to the Firm and Subsequent Exit Behav-
5. I consider myself an Internet expert. ior.” Advances in Consumer Research 22:94-100.
6. I purchase products from online vendors regularly.  and Katherine N. Lemon. 1999. “A Dynamic Model of Cus-
7. My level of expertise regarding personal computers is high. tomers’Usage of Services: Usage as an Antecedent and Consequence
of Satisfaction.” Journal of Marketing Research 36 (May): 171-186.
Boulding, William, Ajay Kalra, Richard Staelin, and Valarie A.
Zeithaml. 1993. “A Dynamic Process Model of Service Quality:
Household income From Expectations to Behavioral Intentions.” Journal of Marketing
Research 30 (February): 7-27.
Carl, Jeremy. 1995. “Online Service Users: Loyal as Alley Cats?” Web
1. $20,000 or less Week 1 (November).
2. $20,001-$30,000 Celsi, Richard L. and Jerry C. Olson. 1988. “The Role of Involvement in
3. $30,001-$40,000 Attention and Comprehension Processes.” Journal of Consumer Re-
4. $40,001-$50,000 search 15:210-224.
Cohen, Jacob and P. Cohen. 1980. Applied Multiple Regression/Correla-
5. $50,001-$60,000
tion Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. New York: Lawrence
6. $60,001-$70,000 Erlbaum.
7. $70,001-$80,000 Cronin, Joseph J. Jr. and Steven A. Taylor. 1992. “Measuring Service
8. $80,001-$90,000 Quality: A Reexamination and Extension.” Journal of Marketing 56
9. More than $90,000 (July): 55-68.
Dabholkar, Pratibha and Simon Walls. 1999. “Service Evaluation and
Switching Behavior for Experiential Services: An Empirical Test of
Gender Differences Within a Broader Conceptual Framework.” Jour-
Education nal of Consumer Satisfaction, Dissatisfaction, and Complaining Be-
havior 12:123-137.
Danaher, Peter J. and Roland T. Rust. 1996a. “Determining the Optimal
1. Grade school Return on Investment for an Advertising Campaign.” European Jour-
2. Some high school nal of Operational Research 95 (3): 511-521.
Keaveney, Parthasarathy / ONLINE SERVICES 389

 and . 1996b. “Indirect Financial Benefits From Service Oliver, Richard L. 1997. Satisfaction. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Quality.” Quality Management Journal 3 (2): 63-85.  and William O. Bearden. 1983. “The Role of Involvement in Sat-
Deighton, John. 1984. “The Interaction of Advertising and Evidence.” isfaction Processes.” In Advances in Consumer Research. Eds. Rich-
Journal of Consumer Research 11 (December): 763-770. ard P. Bagozzi and Alice M. Tybout. Ann Arbor, MI: Association for
Dowling, Grahame R. and Mark Uncles. 1997. “Do Customer Loyalty Consumer Research, 250-255.
Programs Really Work?” Sloan Management Review (Summer): 71- Peters, Thomas J. 1988. Thriving on Chaos. New York: Knopf.
82. Pritchard, Mark P., Mark E. Havitz, and Dennis R. Howard. 1999. “Ana-
Fiske, Susan T. and Shelley E. Taylor. 1984. Social Cognition. New York: lyzing the Commitment-Loyalty Link in Service Contexts.” Journal
Random House. of the Academy of Marketing Science 27 (3): 333-348.
Fornell, Claes and Birger Wernerfelt. 1987. “Defensive Marketing Strat- Raju, P. S. 1980. “Optimum Stimulation Level: Its Relationship to Per-
egy by Customer Complaint Management: A Theoretical Analysis.” sonality, Demographics, and Exploratory Behavior.” Journal of Con-
Journal of Marketing Research 24 (November): 337-346. sumer Research 7 (December): 272-282.
Furse, David H., Girish N. Punj, and David W. Stewart. 1984. “A Ram, S. and Hyung-Shik Jung. 1990. “The Conceptualization of Product
Typology of Individual Search Strategies Among Purchasers of New Usage.” Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science 18 (Winter):
Automobiles.” Journal of Consumer Research 10 (March): 417-431. 67-76.
Ganesh, Jaishankar, Mark J. Arnold, and Kristy E. Reynolds. 2000. “Un- Reichheld, Frederick. 1993. “Loyalty-Based Management.” Harvard
derstanding the Customer Base of Service Providers: An Examina- Business Review 71 (March-April): 64-74.
tion of the Differences Between Switchers and Stayers.” Journal of
. 1996. “Learning From Customer Defections.” Harvard Busi-
Marketing 64 (July): 65-87.
ness Review 74 (March): 56-69.
Grant, Alan W. and Leonard A. Schlesinger. 1995. “Realize Your Cus-
tomer’s Full Profit Potential.” Harvard Business Review 73 (Septem-  and David W. Kenny. 1990. “The Hidden Advantages of Cus-
ber-October): 59-72. tomer Retention.” Journal of Retail Banking 12 (4): 19-23.
Guseman, Dennis S. 1981. “Risk Perception and Risk Reduction in Con-  and W. Earl Sasser, Jr. 1990. “Zero Defections: Quality Comes to
sumer Services.” In Marketing of Services. Eds. James Donnelly and Services.” Harvard Business Review 68 (September-October): 105-
William George. Chicago: American Marketing Association, 200- 111.
204. Richins, Marsha L. and Peter H. Bloch. 1991. “Post-Purchase Product
Hair, Joseph F. Jr., Rolph E. Anderson, Ronald L. Tatham, and William C. Satisfaction: Incorporating the Effects of Involvement and Time.”
Black. 1995. Multivariate Data Analysis. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Journal of Business Research 23:145-158.
Prentice Hall. Roos, Inger. 1999. “Switching Processes in Customer Relationships.”
Heskett, James L., W. Earl Sasser Jr., and Leonard A. Schlesinger. 1997. Journal of Service Research 2 (1): 68-85.
The Service Profit Chain. New York: Free Press. Smith, Robert E. and William R. Swinyard. 1982. “Information Re-
Hoch, Stephen J. and John Deighton. 1989. “Managing What Consumers sponse Models: An Integrated Approach.” Journal of Marketing 46
Learn From Experience.” Journal of Marketing 53 (April): 1-20. (Winter): 81-93.
Houston, Michael J. 1979. “Consumer Evaluation of Product Informa- Spreng, Richard A., Scott B. MacKenzie, and Richard W. Olshavsky.
tion Sources.” In Current Issues and Research in Advertising. Eds. 1996. “A Reexamination or the Determinants of Consumer Satisfac-
James H. Leigh and Claude R. Martin, Jr. Ann Arbor: University of tion.” Journal of Marketing 60 (July): 15-32.
Michigan, 135-144. Tabachnik, Barbara G. and Linda S. Fidell. 1983. Using Multivariate Sta-
Jeffrey, Jaclyn R. and John J. Franco. 1996. “Identifying a Company’s tistics. New York: Harper & Row.
Most Profitable Customers: The First Step in Intelligent Customer Yi, Youjae. 1990. “A Critical Review of Consumer Satisfaction.” In Re-
Retention.” Information Strategy: The Executive’s Journal, Summer, view of Marketing 1990. Ed. Valarie A. Zeithaml. Chicago: American
pp. 15-21. Marketing Association, 68-123.
Keaveney, Susan M. 1995. “Customer Switching Behavior in Service In- Zeithaml, Valarie A. 2000. “Service Quality, Profitability, and the Eco-
dustries: An Exploratory Study.” Journal of Marketing 59 (2): 71-82. nomic Worth of Customers: What We Know and What We Need to
Kingsley, Paul and Terry Anderson. 1998. “Facing Life Without the Learn.” Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science 28 (1): 67-85.
Internet.” Internet Research: Electronic Networking Applications , Leonard L. Berry, and A. Parasuraman. 1993. “The Nature and
and Policy 8 (4): 303-312. Determinants of Customer Expectations of Service.” Journal of the
Kordupleski, Raymond E., Roland T. Rust, and Anthony J. Zahorik. Academy of Marketing Science 21 (Winter): 1-12.
1993. “Why Improving Quality Doesn’t Improve Quality (or What-
, , and . 1996. “The Behavioral Consequences of
ever Happened to Marketing?).” California Management Review 35
Service Quality.” Journal of Marketing 60 (April): 31-46.
(3): 82-95.
Zinkhan, George M. and William Locander. 1988. “ESSCA: A Multidi-
Kotler, Philip R. 1997. Marketing Management: Analysis, Planning, Im-
mensional Analysis Tool for Marketing Research.” Journal of the
plementation, and Control. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Academy of Marketing Science 16 (1): 36-46.
LaBarbera, Priscilla A. and David Mazursky. 1983. “A Longitudinal As-
sessment of Consumer Satisfaction/Dissatisfaction: The Dynamic
Aspect of the Cognitive Process.” Journal of Marketing Research 20 Internet Resources
(November): 393-404.
Levin, Rich. 1997. “The Ups and Downs of ISPs.” Information Week 657
AOL.com. 2000. “AOL: The World’s Leading Interactive Service” [On-
(November 17).
line]. Available: http://corp.aol.com/whoweare/who_brandsaol.html
Moore, William L. and Donald R. Lehman. 1980. “Individual Differ-
ences in Search Behavior for a Nondurable.” Journal of Consumer Borland, John. 1998. “Rivalry in ISP Market Heats Up” [Online]. Avail-
Research 7 (December): 296-307. able: http://www.techweb.com/news/story/TWB19980101S006.
Murray, Keith B. 1991. “A Test of Services Marketing Theory: Consumer Cyberatlas.com. 2000a. “Demographics of the Net Getting Older” [On-
Information Acquisition Activities.” Journal of Marketing 55 (Janu- line]. August 29. Available: http://cyberatlas.internet.com/big_pic-
ary): 10-25. ture/demographics/article/0,1323,5921_448131,00.html
 and John L. Schlacter. 1990. “The Impact of Services Versus . 2000b. “Lower Income Households Moving Online” [Online].
Goods on Consumers’ Assessments of Perceived Risk and Variabil- August 21. Available: http://cyberatlas.internet.com/big_picture/
ity.” Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science 18 (1): 51-65. demographics/article/0,1323,5901_442041,00.html
Nelson, Philip. 1970. “Information and Consumer Behavior.” Journal of . 2000c. “Smaller ISPs Threatening the Dominance of Big Boys”
Political Economy 78 (20): 311-329. [Online]. September 12. Available: http://cyberatlas.internet.com/
Oliva, Terence A., Richard L. Oliver, and William O. Bearden. 1995. big_picture/demographics/article/0,1323,5921_458021,00.html
“The Relationships Among Consumer Satisfaction, Involvement, . 2000d. “Web Remains a Man’s World Outside U.S.” [Online].
and Product Performance: A Catastrophe Theory Application.” Be- July 6. Available: http://cyberatlas.internet.com/big_picture/demo-
havioral Science 40:104-132. graphics/article/
390 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

Datamonitor, Inc. 1999. “Global ISP Equipment Market Will Be Worth chology and Marketing, the Journal of Marketing Channels, the
$15 Billion in 2003” [Online]. August 2. Available: http://www.
Journal of Promotion Management, the Journal of Marketing in
datamonitor.com/press/prtemplate.asp?id=tcpr08199902_1
Prodigy.com. 2000. “Corporate Profile” [Online]. Available: http:// Higher Education, and the Journal of Business and Psychology.
www.corporate-ir.net/ireye/ir_site.zhtml?ticker=prgy&script= In 1996, she received the American Marketing Association’s
2100&layout=9. SERVSIG award for the Best Services Article in 1995. She is co-
author, with Philip R. Cateora, of Marketing: An International
Perspective, published both in English and in Japanese transla-
ABOUT THE AUTHORS tion and was actively involved in the “internationalization” of
business schools in the United States.
Susan M. Keaveney (Ph.D., University of Colorado) is a profes-
sor of marketing at the University of Colorado at Denver. She Madhavan Parthasarathy (Ph.D., University of Nebraska at
teaches marketing management, international marketing, and Lincoln) is an assistant professor of marketing at the University
services marketing for the Graduate School of Business and Ex- of Colorado in Denver. He teaches Internet Marketing, New
ecutive MBA Programs and has received numerous awards for Product Development, Marketing Management, and Marketing
Excellence in Teaching. Prior to commencing her academic ca- Research for the Graduate School of Business. His research in-
reer, she worked in the retailing, financial services, and health terests focus on customer retention, switching and discontinu-
care industries. Her research in services marketing focuses on ance behavior, and the diffusion of new products. He has
customer retention and service switching and has been published published in such journals as Information Systems Research, In-
in such journals as the Journal of Marketing, the Journal of the ternational Marketing Review, the Journal of Macromarketing,
Academy of Marketing Science, the Journal of Retailing, Mar- and the Journal of Business and Industrial Marketing, among
keting Management, the Journal of Marketing Education, Psy- others.
JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY
Bush et al.OF
/ INTERCULTURAL
MARKETING SCIENCE
COMMUNICATION FALL 2001

Managing Culturally Diverse


Buyer-Seller Relationships: The Role of
Intercultural Disposition and Adaptive
Selling in Developing Intercultural
Communication Competence
Victoria D. Bush
Gregory M. Rose
Faye Gilbert
University of Mississippi

Thomas N. Ingram
Colorado State University

Given the increase in cultural diversity within marketing In recognition of an increasingly diverse workforce and
organizations as well as within current and potential cus- customer base in the United States, marketers have
tomer bases, possessing the appropriate communication acknowledged the importance of studying cultural differ-
skills becomes crucial to success in managing culturally ences to better understand, develop, and manage culturally
diverse relationships. Although marketing researchers diverse buyer-seller relationships. Webster (1992) stresses
have recognized the importance of adaptive selling behav- that marketers could benefit further by working with mem-
ior for successful buyer-seller relationships, the explora- bers from other disciplines such as cultural anthropology
tion of the intercultural aspects of these relationships has and sociology for a more in-depth understanding of the
only recently begun. This article examines how adaptive differences in values, beliefs, or decision-making pro-
selling behaviors and intercultural dispositions of market- cesses, among other constructs, that contribute to manag-
ing executives contribute to their perceived intercultural ing culturally diverse relationships (Deshpande and Web-
communication competence. Results show that in addition ster 1989; Montgomery 1991; Montgomery and Weiss
to being adaptive, the intercultural disposition of a mar- 1991; Webster and Deshpande 1991). Intercultural com-
keter is of key importance in developing intercultural com- munication is one such discipline that can contribute to the
munication competence. Theoretical and practical development of successful buyer-seller relationships.
implications for incorporating intercultural communica- The purpose of this article is to discuss and investigate
tion into the development of successful buyer-seller rela- the role that intercultural communication and adaptive
tionships are discussed. selling behavior play in developing the intercultural com-
munication competence of marketing executives. First,
relevant literature in marketing and buyer-seller relation-
ships concerning adaptive selling and communication is
Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science. discussed. Second, intercultural communication is intro-
Volume 29, No. 4, pages 391-404.
Copyright © 2001 by Academy of Marketing Science. duced with a discussion of intercultural disposition and
392 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

competence. Third, a model is developed and tested THEORETICAL FOUNDATIONS


among marketing executives. Finally, implications for
marketing managers and academic researchers are Adaptive Selling Behavior
discussed.
A genuine customer orientation requires that marketers
learn as much about their customers’ communication
BACKGROUND styles, behaviors, and needs as possible. Accordingly,
adaptive selling has been defined as “the altering of sales
Cultural diversity is defined as differences in age, gen- behaviors during a customer interaction or across cus-
der, race, nationality, and ability (Loden and Rosener tomer interactions based on perceived information about
1991). The proportion of diverse groups in the workforce the nature of the selling situation” (Weitz, Sujan, and
has been steadily increasing (Seal 1991). For example, in Sujan 1986:175).
personal selling, the growth rate of women, African Amer- Researchers have noted that adaptive selling can be
icans, Hispanics, and Asians has increased tremendously practiced in effective and ineffective ways (e.g., Spiro and
in recent years (Comer, Nicholls, and Vermillion 1998). In Weitz 1990; Weitz et al. 1986). Adaptive selling is only
addition, according to the American Advertising Federa- effective in the long term when benefits outweigh the costs
tion, the annual spending power of Asians, Hispanics, and of selecting and training salespeople to practice this
African Americans in the United States is approximately approach, such as in the development of a long-term
$1 trillion (Bertagnoli 2001). This increasingly diverse buyer-seller relationship. The effectiveness of adaptive
customer base is one of the top reasons for marketers to selling is moderated by (1) the variety of customer needs
value diversity whether it be in the United States or when and types encountered by the sellers, (2) the importance of
expanding into global markets (Bertagnoli 2001). Accord- the typical buying situation to the seller, (3) the resources
ing to Thansee Mustafa, chair of College Relations at provided by the company, and (4) the seller’s skills and
Johnson & Johnson, “A diverse workforce ensures the capabilities (Weitz et al. 1986).
innovative thought necessary within a company to remain Cultural diversity is relevant when considering the
competitive” (Bertagnoli 2001:9). potential impact of these four moderators. Adaptive sell-
Because of these trends, marketing managers are faced ing is thought to be more effective when encountering a
with two challenges. First, they will need to expend more heterogeneous customer base. With increases in ethnic
effort to recruit, retain, and manage culturally diverse mar- businesses, customers, and foreign opportunities, market-
keting executives. Second, to remain competitive, they ers have and will increasingly encounter a wide variety of
must recognize the diversity of their present and future customers from a cultural perspective.
customer base to develop and maintain successful buyer- The importance of the buying situation as discussed by
seller relationships. Weitz et al. (1986) relates to the economic significance,
Cultural diversity in buyer-seller relationships has only that is, size of the order. While larger orders may indeed be
recently been recognized as an important and under- worth the extra effort to practice adaptive selling, the
researched issue (Comer and Nicholls 2000; Comer et al. importance of an individual order may not completely
1998). Most of the existing research has focused on characterize the full significance of the buying situation.
women in the sales force (e.g., Comer and Jolson 1991). Recent emphasis on the value of a customer over an
Much less has been done on other groups such as African extended period of time, even a lifetime, suggests that the
Americans, Hispanics, and Asians (Comer and Nicholls significance of buying situations might be defined by
2000; Comer et al. 1998). Equally important is research order size in the short run or by the economic value of a
investigating culturally diverse customers. Some initial customer during an extended period of time. To build and
research has been done on similarities between buyers and sustain customer relationships during extended time hori-
sellers in terms of age and gender (Dwyer, Richard, and zons, marketers must recognize that as the workforce
Shepherd 1998; Kang and Hillary 1998). However, more becomes more diverse, so will the population of buyers
is needed on the impact of diverse communication styles in and purchasing agents. Thus, understanding cultural dif-
the buyer-seller interaction (Comer et al. 1998). Further- ferences among buyers and practicing adaptive selling are
more, cultural diversity research would be strengthened if expected to become even more crucial for success in the
it could be incorporated into an overall theoretical frame- future.
work. It is suggested here that the theoretical foundations With these changes in the cultural composition of the
of adaptive selling behavior and intercultural communica- workforce, companies are committing more resources to
tion can provide the basis for incorporating cultural diver- better understand the economic impact of culturally
sity issues into a viable stream of research for buyer-seller diverse workforces, to pursue diverse markets, and to
relationship management. select and train employees to be more effective with
Bush et al. / INTERCULTURAL COMMUNICATION 393

diverse customer bases (Wentling 2000). Finally, market- dispositions and traits, their communication strategy will
ing executives must have the capability to adapt to cultur- be more appropriate in a culturally diverse buyer-seller
ally diverse buyer-seller interactions. Hence, some interaction. Thus, the adaptive selling framework provides
research has recognized, to a limited extent, cultural diver- a bridge for incorporating intercultural communication
sity in the adaptive selling framework. For example, Levy into the marketing literature.
and Sharma (1994) found that age and gender interact and
have an impact on adaptive selling. However, little Intercultural Communication
research exists on the diversity of customers in the adap- in Buyer-Seller Relationships
tive selling framework.
Intercultural communication is
Communication in Buyer-
Seller Relationships a symbolic, interpretive, transactional, contextual
process in which the degree of difference between
Communication can be defined as occurring whenever people is large and important enough to create dis-
similar interpretations and expectations about what
meaning is attributed to behavior (Porter and Samovar
are regarded as competent behaviors that should be
1991). Communication has been recognized as an impor- used to create shared meanings. (Lustig and Koester
tant construct in effective buyer-seller and channel rela- 1993:58)
tionship management (Anderson and Narus 1990;
Boorom, Goolsby, and Ramsey 1998; Mohr and Nevin In other words, intercultural communication occurs when-
1990; Williams, Spiro, and Fine 1990). The role of com- ever a message in one culture must be processed in another
munication in relationship marketing has historically been culture (Porter and Samovar 1991). In the marketing con-
reduced to the assertion that “more communication” or text, intercultural communication occurs when a message
“improved communication” is needed. Mohr and Nevin from a buyer (seller) from one culture must be processed
(1990) state that this view is not only simplistic but also by a seller (buyer) from another (Bush and Ingram 1996).
inaccurate. For example, more communication in a rela- Research in intercultural communication has uncov-
tional situation where a high degree of conflict exists is not ered dispositions and skills that can affect buyer-seller
always the answer if that communication is in the form of interactions. It is posited here that these constructs can be
threats. Furthermore, simply stating that communication incorporated into a theoretical framework that integrates
needs improvement does not give marketers any input on cultural diversity into buyer-seller relationship manage-
how to improve communication (Bush and Ingram 2001). ment. Furthermore, previous research in the intercultural
Williams et al. (1990) addressed the need to investigate communication and education literature has focused pri-
buyer-seller relationships from a more detailed communi- marily on the intercultural dispositions of college stu-
cation perspective. Marketing researchers have investi- dents, military personnel, and the international relocation
gated a number of communication skills and their potential of corporate personnel (Bradford, Allen, and Beisser
effect on performance. Such abilities as gathering infor- 1997). To date, no research has specifically investigated
mation and asking questions (Schuster and Danes 1986), the intercultural disposition of marketing executives in
listening or the lack thereof (Ingram, Schwepker, and relation to the adaptive selling framework. Figure 1
Hutson 1992), and recognizing different communication depicts a model of intercultural communication and adap-
styles (Williams and Spiro 1985) have been advocated as tive selling. The components of the model are discussed as
critical to successful buyer-seller interactions. However, follows.
little research in this area has been conducted using adap-
tive selling behavior as a theoretical framework (e.g., Intercultural Disposition
Boorom et al. 1998).
An exception to this observation is a recent study by Of key importance to successful intercultural commu-
Boorom et al. (1998). These authors demonstrated that nication is to possess what Gudykunst, Wiseman, and
certain communication traits affect the adaptiveness of Hammer (1977) call a general “cross-cultural attitude” or
salespeople. Citing research conducted in the communica- intercultural disposition. An individual’s intercultural dis-
tion competence literature (e.g., Spitzberg and Cupach position has been described as the “core” of effective
1984), the authors state that “communication effectiveness intercultural communication. Gudykunst et al. (1977)
is viewed as arising from a set of dispositions and traits that developed a framework for intercultural disposition that
facilitate receiving, deciphering, and responding appropri- includes (1) the ability to empathize with people from
ately to information in dyadic interactions” (Boorom et al. other cultures, (2) being astute noncritical observers of
1998:17). It is suggested here that these dispositions and their own and other people’s behavior, (3) being less
traits become even more crucial in a culturally diverse set- ethnocentric, and (4) being accurate in perceiving differ-
ting. Specifically, if marketing executives possess such ences and similarities between the sojourner’s own culture
394 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

FIGURE 1
Intercultural Disposition, Adaptive Selling Behavior, and Perceived
Intercultural Communication Competence: A Conceptual Model

Intercultural Disposition Perceived Intercultural


H1 (+) Adaptive Selling H3 (+) Communication Competence
• Empathy
Behavior
• Worldmindedness • Ability to Deal With Stress

• Ethnocentrism • Interpersonal Relationships

• Attributional • Communication Styles


Complexity

H2 (+)

and the host culture. Researchers have conceptualized and ing an astute noncritical observer of one’s own and other
measured these characteristics in an attempt to predict people’s behavior. Thus, an individual must be receptive to
intercultural effectiveness or cross-cultural adaptation new ideas and ways of looking at the world—even enjoy-
(e.g., Cui and Van Den Berg 1991; Hammer, Gudykunst, ing new experiences. Worldmindedness refers to an indi-
and Wiseman 1978). Some of these capabilities have also vidual’s frame of reference, apart from knowledge about,
been investigated in the marketing literature (e.g., McBane or interest in, cultural diversity (Sampson and Smith
1995; Spiro and Weitz 1990) and are most relevant to the 1957). This “cosmopolitan” perspective toward the world
buyer-seller interaction. has been well documented in its ability to facilitate
intercultural communication (Wiseman, Hammer, and
Empathy. An empathic tendency refers to the capacity Nishida 1989) in a variety of settings such as study-abroad
to clearly project an interest in others and to obtain and re- programs (Carlson and Widaman 1988) and international
flect a reasonably complete and accurate sense of an- relocation (Bhawuk and Triandis 1996). Furthermore,
other’s thoughts, feelings, and experiences. Past research worldmindedness has been shown to increase knowledge
has shown that persons who tend to empathize with others of one’s own culture as well.
also tend to be more interculturally effective (Cleveland, In marketing, Weitz and Jap (1995) emphasize that
Mangone, and Adams 1960; Ruben 1976). A certain de- relationship development within channels of distribution
gree of empathy will allow an individual to further under- is affected by the manner in which intentions, expecta-
stand a member of another culture’s point of view. tions, and information are communicated. In a culturally
The importance of empathizing with others has been diverse interaction between a buyer and a seller, the seller
recognized in the marketing literature (e.g., Comer and must be receptive to the cultural background of the buyer
Drollinger 1999; McBane 1995; Spiro and Weitz 1990; (Bush and Ingram 1996; Honeycutt and Ford 1995). As
Weitz et al. 1986). In more specific terms, Davis (1983) Sharma, Shimp, and Shin (1995) state, “Individuals differ
proposed that empathy refers to the reaction of individuals in terms of their experience with and openness toward the
to the observed experiences of others. These observers can people, values, and artifacts of other cultures” (p. 28).
have a variety of empathetic reactions that consist of per- Thus, the marketing executive who is worldminded (i.e.,
spective taking, fantasy, empathetic concern, and personal an astute observer of his or her own culture as well as oth-
distress. The cognitive component of empathy, perspective ers) may be in a better position to develop a relationship
taking, has been the most widely accepted theoretical per- with a buyer from a different cultural background.
spective in the marketing literature (McBane 1995), and
conventional wisdom holds that this dimension should Ethnocentrism. The third aspect of intercultural dispo-
directly help the marketing executive involved in a buyer- sition that Gudykunst et al. (1977) refer to is being less
seller interaction (Ingram et al. 1992). Thus, the cognitive ethnocentric. Ethnocentrism is the tendency to identify
aspect of empathy may aid in understanding cultural with one’s in-group (ethnic, racial, cultural, etc.) and eval-
aspects of the buyer’s decision-making process. uate out-groups according to its standards (Gudykunst and
Kim 1984). A high level of ethnocentrism can distort the
Worldmindedness. According to Gudykunst et al. meaning of an individual’s behavior. In turn, a low level of
(1977), part of one’s intercultural disposition includes be- ethnocentrism, termed cultural relativity, has been identi-
Bush et al. / INTERCULTURAL COMMUNICATION 395

fied as a critical variable in facilitating effective intercul- Summary


tural communication (Porter and Samovar 1991).
The concept of ethnocentrism has been investigated in In summary, it is posited that a marketer must have the
the marketing literature from several perspectives. From a appropriate intercultural disposition—that is, empathy,
strategic perspective, marketers have looked at the effect worldmindedness, low ethnocentrism, and attributional
of ethnocentrism on global planning and decision-making. complexity—to effectively communicate with diverse
The ethnocentrism, polycentrism, regiocentrism, buyers. The central argument of adaptive selling is that the
geocentrism (EPRG) framework has served as a guiding ability to create and modify messages through interactive
force in explaining the structure and perspective of inter- communication with customers will result in better perfor-
national organizations (Shoham, Rose, and Albaum mance (Spiro and Weitz 1990; Sujan, Weitz, and Kumar
1995). Consumer behavior researchers have developed 1994; Weitz 1978; Weitz et al. 1986). Based on the previ-
and empirically tested the construct of consumer ous discussion of intercultural communication, it is pos-
ethnocentrism (e.g., Shimp and Sharma 1987). Here, ited that a key contributor to this adaptability, or “working
ethnocentrism results from the love for one’s own country, smarter,” in a culturally diverse interaction is the
implies an intention to not purchase foreign products intercultural disposition of a marketing executive. Thus,
based on moral concerns rather than economic issues, and the following is hypothesized (refer to Figure 1):
refers to a personal level of prejudice against imports.
Since the overall level of consumer ethnocentrism in a Hypothesis 1: There is a positive relationship between
social system is the aggregation of individual tendencies the intercultural disposition and adaptive selling be-
(e.g., Sharma et al. 1995), ethnocentric marketing execu- havior of marketing executives.
tives may not realize the implications of these tendencies
in a culturally diverse buyer-seller interaction. In such an Perceived Intercultural
interaction, the success of a culturally diverse interaction Communication Competence
between a buyer and a seller may be unknowingly inhib-
ited on the seller’s part by his or her ethnocentrism (Bush
Perceived intercultural communication competence is
and Ingram 1996).
an impression that message behavior is appropriate and
Attributional complexity. Finally, intercultural disposi- effective in a given context (Spitzberg 1991). Appropriate-
tion includes being accurate in perceiving differences and ness means that the valued rules and norms of either cul-
similarities between one’s own culture and the host culture ture in an interaction are not violated significantly. Thus,
(Gudykunst et al. 1977). Palmer and Pickett (1999) state similar to Williams et al.’s (1990) descriptions, appropri-
that before individuals “can take corrective action regard- ateness refers to the communication codes and rules that
ing a discrepancy between a referent and their perfor- are valued in each culture. Effectiveness refers to the
mance, they must first generate a theory as to why the accomplishment of the goal of the interaction.
discrepancy occurred” (p. 26). Since intercultural interac- Extensive research has been conducted in the field of
tions may involve more complex culturally embedded ver- intercultural communication to define the subdimensions
bal and nonverbal signals, individuals must be able to of perceived intercultural communication competence
accurately identify more potentially complex causes of the (e.g., Brislin and Yoshida 1994; Gudykunst 1994). These
interaction. Attributional complexity refers to an individ- include the ability to handle stress, to establish and main-
ual’s tendency to attribute complex causes to other’s be- tain a meaningful relationship, and to deal with different
haviors (Fletcher, Danilovics, Fernandez, Peterson, and communication styles (Hammer et al. 1978). Since a cul-
Reeder 1986). These authors suggest that when forming turally diverse experience can be composed of many new
causal attributions, “attributionally complex people tend or novel situations, an individual must be capable of han-
to notice and use information garnered from a behavioral dling the psychological stress that can result when trying
interaction to a greater extent than attributionally simple to cope with those differences. The ability to establish and
people” (Fletcher et al. 1986:876). This results, as sug- maintain a relationship is also an important factor when
gested here, in a more accurate explanation of another’s addressing someone perceived as culturally different.
behavior. This tendency has been shown to be associated Here, an individual must not only be comfortable in the
with lower anxiety and greater effectiveness in intercul- initial phases of relationship development but also be able
tural interactions (Gudykunst and Kim 1984). to maintain the relationship, such as in a long-term buyer-
Thus, if a person is adept or accurate at figuring out seller relationship. Finally, an individual must perceive
“what makes people tick,” that person may be better able to himself or herself as being able or comfortable with com-
communicate in an unfamiliar setting such as departing to municating with a culturally diverse person who may be
a foreign country (Stephan and Stephan 1992) or entering used to communicating in a different style and/or social
a culturally diverse market/sales territory. system.
396 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

Research has shown that the intercultural disposition of METHOD


an individual can affect the perceived intercultural com-
munication skills or competence of that individual in a cul- Sample
turally diverse interaction (cf. Abe and Wiseman 1983;
Wiseman et al. 1989). For example, Wiseman et al. (1989) The target population for this study was defined as
found that more ethnocentric individuals had less under- members of organizations involved in marketing interac-
standing of a Japanese host culture. In a review of the liter- tions with nonretail customers. Our goal was to recruit a
ature on intercultural effectiveness, Hannigan (1990) con- diverse sample of marketing executives across various
cluded that cross-cultural attitude (e.g., Gudykunst et al. industries in a major southern metropolitan city. Members
1977) is an important and widely researched factor in pre- of a regional sales and marketing association along with
dicting cross-cultural success—one of its outcomes being executive MBA students involved in marketing courses at
perceived intercultural communication competence. a major state university in the same city were solicited by
Based on the previous discussion, an individual’s the researchers. The letter stressed that participants be
intercultural disposition is a key contributor to the success marketing executives involved in the selling process with
of a culturally diverse interaction. This disposition can in- nonretail customers and that the issue of cultural diversity
fluence the perceived intercultural communication com- among their clientele was a real concern to them.
petence of the individual (refer to Figure 1). Thus, the A total of 30 letters were sent to marketing managers
following is hypothesized: and professors—serving as key contact points for their
organization or class. A total of six groups (20% response
rate) agreed to participate in the survey. Three groups con-
Hypothesis 2: There is a positive relationship between
the intercultural disposition of marketing executives sisted of marketing organizations (51 respondents, 42%)
and their perceived intercultural communication and three consisted of executive MBA marketing classes
competence. (71 respondents, 58%). In total, 122 respondents partici-
pated in the study. All respondents, including executive
MBAs, were employed in marketing organizations, had
Finally, the relationship between adaptive selling and
regular interactions with customers, and were concerned
intercultural communication competence is examined. In-
about cultural diversity in their clientele. Fifty-six percent
tuitively, if a marketing executive is adaptive to the buyer-
of the respondents listed sales as their occupation, another
seller interaction, then he or she should be competent in a
20 percent stated they were in marketing, and 24 percent
culturally diverse buyer-seller interaction (see Figure 1).
stated that they were in marketing management. The
In other words, an adaptive seller should naturally be able
majority of the participants in this study were male
to handle the stress of new situations, be able to establish
(69.7%), married (70.8%), and between 30 and 50 years of
and maintain a relationship with a buyer from a culturally
age. More than 80 percent of the respondents were Cauca-
diverse background, and be able to deal with different
sian and most of the respondents were college graduates
communication styles. Thus, adaptive selling behaviors
(85%).
should result in better intercultural communication skills.
The marketing executives in this study also represented
However, this relationship has not been empirically exam-
a broad spectrum of industries. Participants stated they
ined within the context of cultural diversity nor within the
were involved in such industries as health care products
marketing literature.
(20%), financial planning (18%), business consulting
Relevant research has been conducted in the inter- (15%), manufacturing (25%), transportation (7%), com-
cultural literature that may parallel the concept of adaptive puter technology (5%), and others (10%). In a recent
selling behavior and its impact on perceived intercultural worldwide survey conducted by Sales and Marketing
communication competence. Cleveland et al. (1960) Executives International (SMEI) in 1998, 14 percent of the
suggested that a sojourner to a foreign culture must pos- marketing executives surveyed stated they were in finance,
sess flexibility. This is characterized by the ability to 13 percent in manufacturing, 2 percent in computer tech-
change goals and methods for reaching goals as needed. nology, 2 percent in medical, and 1 percent in transporta-
This is similar to a seller being flexible in the selling tion (SMEI 2000). The marketing executives in the present
approach used (Weitz et al. 1986). Thus, the following is study are similar in representation to those in the SMEI
hypothesized: study in some categories (i.e., finance, manufacturing,
computer technology). However, our study also represents
Hypothesis 3: There is a positive relationship between the metropolitan location with a higher percentage of mar-
the adaptive selling behavior of marketing execu- keting executives in health care, manufacturing, and trans-
tives and their perceived intercultural communica- portation (U.S. Census Bureau 2000). In sum, our objec-
tion competence. tive was to purposely recruit marketing executives who
Bush et al. / INTERCULTURAL COMMUNICATION 397

were involved in culturally diverse marketing interactions .77 for this scale. For the present study, the seven items
across a variety of industries. consisting of perspective taking were measured using a 6-
To further validate the selection of participants, several point scale, ranging from 1 (describes me very well) to 6
questions regarding diversity issues were included in the (does not describe me well).
survey. Respondents were asked on a scale of 1 (strongly Worldmindedness. The Worldmindedness Scale
agree) to 6 (strongly disagree) their level of agreement to (Sampson and Smith 1957) consists of 32 items in total
the following statement: “The issue of cultural diversity in that measure one’s “frame of reference” toward the world
the workplace is not relevant to me and my organization.” on a variety of issues such as immigration, race relations,
The mean response was 5.01 on a 6-point scale, indicating economics, and education. Some researchers (e.g., Wise-
that respondents were indeed concerned about cultural man et al. 1989) suggest that six items of this scale are
diversity. Furthermore, respondents were asked how often more culture-general and thus more applicable to various
they interact with culturally diverse customers. Using a cross-cultural situations. A typical item is, for example, “It
scale of 1 (frequently), 2 (occasionally), 3 (rarely), and 4 would be better to be a citizen of the world than of any par-
(never), 87.6 percent of the respondents stated that they ticular nation” (p. 101). Wiseman et al. (1989) found that
frequently (53.3%) or occasionally (34.3%) interact with these items loaded on one factor. All six items were mea-
diverse customers. Thus, the relevance of diversity and fre- sured on a 6-point scale, ranging from 1 (strongly agree) to
quent or occasional interaction with diverse customers 6 (strongly disagree).
revealed that the sample was involved in diverse marketing
interactions.1 Ethnocentrism. Ethnocentrism was measured using
Because of the nature and content of this study, two pre- items developed by Stephan and Stephan (1992). This
cautionary steps were included in analyzing the data. First, scale includes such statements as “Americans have been
the measures of interest to this study have the potential for very generous in teaching other people how to do things in
socially desirable responses. To address this issue, a corre- more efficient ways” (p. 92). Reliability for this scale has
lation analysis between the constructs and social desirabil- previously been reported to be as high as α = .83. Two re-
ity (i.e., the shortened Marlow-Crowne Scale) was con- lated items were also included from Shimp and Sharma’s
ducted (Strahan and Gerbasi 1972). No significant (1987) revised Patriotism Scale, which was based on ear-
correlations at the .05 level were found. Second, the stabil- lier work by Levinson (1950). These items are the follow-
ity of the responses for this study was also assessed ing: “America may not be perfect, but the American way
between each company or class from which the data were has brought us about as close as human beings can get to a
collected. A series of one-way ANOVAs were conducted perfect society” and “the main threat to basic American in-
on the constructs of interest. No significant difference was stitutions this century has come from the infiltration of for-
found between the groups before the data were combined eign ideas, doctrines and agitators” (p. 287). These items
for analysis. were included to reflect the fact that the study is focused on
American respondents. All items from both scales were
Measures measured using a 6-point scale, ranging from 1 (strongly
agree) to 6 (strongly disagree).
To test the hypotheses generated for the present study, Attributional complexity. Five items were used from the
measures from the intercultural communication and mar- Fletcher et al. (1986) scale and consist of such statements
keting literature were used. Where possible, existing as “I really enjoy analyzing the reasons or causes for peo-
scales, or slightly modified versions of these scales, have ple’s behavior.” These items have been used in previous
been used to maintain scale integrity and enhance compar- studies measuring abilities before departing to a foreign
isons to other studies (e.g., Hensel and Bruner 1992). Spe- country (Stephan and Stephan 1992). In addition, these
cific scale items and their reliability in the present study five items were chosen because they exhibited the highest
are presented in the appendix. correlations with the overall scale in two previous studies
(Fletcher et al. 1986; Stephan and Stephan 1992).
Empathy. Davis (1980, 1983) developed and validated Attributional complexity was measured on a 6-point scale,
a multidimensional Interpersonal Reactivity Index that fo- ranging from 1 (describes me very well) to 6 (does not de-
cuses on empathetic tendencies. The seven-item Perspec- scribe me well).
tive Taking subscale measures the “tendency to adopt the
psychological point of view of others” (Davis 1983:114) Adaptive selling. Adaptive selling was measured using
and has demonstrated sufficient reliability among men the ADAPTS Scale developed by Spiro and Weitz (1990).
(α = .75) and women (α = .78). The Perspective Taking This scale measures the degree to which salespeople per-
scale has been used in previous marketing studies. For ex- ceive themselves as “working smarter” (Spiro and Weitz
ample, Spiro and Weitz (1990) achieved a reliability of α = 1990; Weitz 1978; Weitz et al. 1986). In other words,
398 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

ADAPTS measures the adaptability of salespeople to their In summary, the specific scales used in this study were
customers. The reliability of this scale has been well docu- based on previous research, demonstrate unidimen-
mented in the marketing literature with coefficient alphas sionality and multidimensionality where appropriate, and
ranging from .85 (Boorom et al. 1998, Spiro and Weitz show sufficient reliability.2
1990) to .88 (Sujan et al. 1994). Again, all items for the
present study were measured on a 6-point scale, ranging
from 1 (strongly agree) to 6 (strongly disagree). RESULTS
Previous research has found that adaptive selling is
composed of adaptive selling beliefs and adaptive selling Model Parameter Estimates
behaviors (Marks, Vorhies, and Badovick 1996). Since the and Goodness of Fit
theoretical foundation of our study focuses on inter-
cultural disposition (i.e., a trait) and the resulting skills All hypotheses were tested in a structural-equations
(i.e., behaviors) of adaptive selling and perceived inter- model (see Figure 2). A structural-equations analysis was
cultural communication competence, we decided to focus employed because it allows for the simultaneous estima-
on adaptive selling behaviors. Ten items were used to mea- tion of a series of interdependent relationships and allows
sure this scale with all model fit indices indicating that the for an assessment of the overall fit of a model (Hair,
scale was unidimensional (with Comparative Fit Index Anderson, Tatham, and Black 1992). The correlation
[CFI], Incremental Fit Index [IFI], and Goodness-of-Fit matrix for model estimation is presented in Table 1.
Index [GFI] above .90 for a one-factor model). Intercultural disposition and intercultural competence
were examined as multidimensional constructs. Empathy,
Perceived intercultural communication competence. worldmindedness, ethnocentrism, and attributional com-
The Intercultural Communication Competence Scale plexity were analyzed as indicators of intercultural dispo-
(Hammer et al. 1978) is a “culture-general” as opposed to sition, while the ability to deal with stress, to establish and
“culture-specific” method to assess perceived skills when maintain interpersonal relationships, and to manage dif-
communicating with different cultures (e.g., Harris and ferent communication styles were used as indicators of
Moran 1991). The scale consists of three dimensions: intercultural competence. Summated scales were used for
(1) ability to deal with psychological stress (five items), each of the indicators of intercultural disposition and
(2) ability to establish and maintain interpersonal relation- intercultural competence. Adaptive selling was repre-
ships (five items), and (3) ability to deal with different sented by a single-item indicator, with each of the 10 items
communication styles (two items). These dimensions have given equal weight in forming the summated scale. The
been used in previous studies (e.g., Cui and Van Den Berg error term of the indicator was set to one minus the reliabil-
1991; Hammer et al. 1978) and have been shown to load on ity multiplied by the observed variance to adjust for the
three factors. Respondents were asked to rate themselves measurement error in the construct (Price, Arnould, and
on each ability from 1 (high ability) to 6 (low ability). Abil- Tierney 1995; Shoham, Rose, and Kahle 1998).
ity to deal with psychological stress included questions re- Overall model fit was acceptable, as evidenced by a
garding the respondents’ self-rating on such issues as nonsignificant chi-square value (χ2 = 23.09 with 18
dealing with “frustration,” “pressure to conform,” “inter- degrees of freedom, p = .19), which indicates the lack of a
personal conflict,” and “anxiety” in communicating with significant discrepancy between the hypothesized model
different cultures. Ability to establish interpersonal rela- and the observed results. The GFI and Bollen’s IFI were
tionships included such items as the ability to “initiate in- also above .90 (GFI = .942, IFI = .934 ), further indicating
teraction with a stranger” and “develop satisfying an acceptable model fit (Bollen 1989).
interpersonal relationships with others.” The final dimen-
Hypothesis 1, which posited that intercultural disposi-
sion included the ability to deal with “different communi-
tion and adaptive selling would be positively related, was
cation styles” and to “accurately understand the feelings of
not supported. Intercultural disposition and adaptive sell-
another person.”
ing were not significantly related (γ = .301, t = 1.63, p >
The finalized questionnaire was pretested among 25 .05). Hypotheses 2 and 3, which directionally examine the
graduate business students. Adjustments were made to the influence of intercultural disposition and adaptive selling,
wording and length based on their suggestions. The order respectively, on intercultural competence, were supported.
sequence of some of the items was also revised. Both intercultural disposition (γ = .522, t = 1.79, p < .05)
The reliability of each measure was assessed via coeffi- and adaptive selling (β = .352, t = 2.03, p < .05) were posi-
cient alphas (Cronbach 1951). With the exception of tively related to intercultural competence in a one-tailed
worldmindedness (α = .63), which was included for con- test. Furthermore, 51 percent of the variance is explained
ceptual reasons despite its low reliability, the scales used in by intercultural communication competence. Thus,
this study were at or above the .70 level (Nunnally 1978). intercultural disposition and adaptive selling appear to be
Bush et al. / INTERCULTURAL COMMUNICATION 399

FIGURE 2
Intercultural Disposition, Adaptive Selling Behavior, and Perceived
Intercultural Communication Competence: Empirical Findings

Empathy .758 (2 F1
.13)
Intercultural .5
Disposition 22
9*
.29 (1
.7
9)

)
.34
Worldmindedness .301 (1.63)

(-1
F3

02
-.2 F2 Perceived
.352 (2.03)
4) Adaptive Intercultural
2.1
Ethnocentrism Selling Communication
3(

Competence
.53

Behavior

.46
5)
4*

2.9
.43

6(
5(
Attributional Complexity

2.6
.70

5)
Stress Interpersonal Communication
Relationships Styles

NOTE: All parameter estimates are standardized. t-values are given in parentheses.
*No t-values are reported for Worldmindedness and Stress because these items were used to set the metric for the construct.

TABLE 1
Correlation Matrix for Model Estimation
Scale 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
1. Empathy 1.0
2. Worldmindedness .19 1.0
3. Ethnocentrism –.14 –.23* 1.0
4. Attributional Complexity .40** .21 –.08 1.0
5. Adaptive Selling .25* .12 .10 .09 1.0
6. Stress .10 .04 –.15 .07 .12 1.0
7. Relationships .39** .10 –.05 .28** .35** .30** 1.0
8. Communication Style .21* .02 –.09 .10 .25* .41** .25* 1.0

* Significant at the .05 level. ** Significant at the .01 level.

relatively independent factors, both of which contribute to executives alone may not increase the effectiveness of a
intercultural competence. culturally diverse buyer-seller interaction. It should be rec-
ognized that the intercultural disposition or “cross-cul-
tural attitude” of marketing executives may also have an
IMPLICATIONS effect on this relationship. Marketers may perceive them-
selves as quite adaptive to their customers. However, while
The overall purpose of this study was to investigate the adaptive selling within a marketing executive’s own cul-
role of intercultural disposition and adaptive selling in ture has been shown to affect outcomes, this construct
developing perceived intercultural communication com- alone may not be sufficient to ensure successful
petence. The results show that intercultural disposition intercultural interactions.
and adaptive selling both affect the perceived intercultural The findings of our study have several implications for
communication competence of marketing executives. marketing managers. First, managers must be able to
However, adaptive selling and intercultural disposition recruit, retain, and manage culturally diverse marketing
seem to be separate unrelated constructs. Thus, programs executives. Second, managers must also recognize that
designed to increase the adaptiveness of marketing their existing employees will be interacting with more
400 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

diverse coworkers and customers. To address these two since “other variables such as allocentrism and dogmatism
issues, selection and training methods are needed that may be related to cultural ethnocentrism” (Sharma et al.
focus on recognizing and developing intercultural skills. 1995:35) and “cultural similarity, historical associations
The importance of training for adaptive selling has been between countries, and present political-economic rela-
recognized by academic researchers and practitioners. In tions between countries [and cultures] may moderate the
their framework for adaptive selling, Weitz et al. (1986) effect of consumer-ethnocentric tendencies” (p. 35), the
incorporated training as a key variable to influence the concept of intercultural disposition provides a plethora of
knowledge and skills of marketers to practice adaptive research opportunities. Furthermore, this study has taken a
selling. As such, practitioners have used methods of train- culture-general approach, whereas more investigation
ing that focus on adaptation. For example, the communica- needs to be done with specific cultures and ethnicities.
tion style matrix (Merrill and Reid 1981) has been used to Such issues as social distance, distrust, and culture shock
train marketers to classify customers on the basis of three when interacting with a specific cultural or ethnic group of
communication style dimensions: (1) responsiveness, buyers or sellers are viable avenues to pursue.
(2) assertiveness, and (3) flexibility. However, marketers The area of intercultural communication in marketing
must also be trained to recognize and interact with custom- is ripe for future research. One research path involves the
ers’ various intercultural communication styles. Given the identification of individuals who may or may not be com-
current trends of an increasingly diverse workforce, petent in dealing with culturally diverse individuals. In the
intercultural communication training may help marketers present study, intercultural disposition and competence
gain a competitive advantage in understanding culturally were investigated. Individuals who scored well on these
diverse markets. constructs may be better able to deal with cultural differ-
Selecting the appropriate intercultural training pro- ences. It would also be of interest to investigate how these
gram and recruiting new employees require methods for constructs relate to other variables such as relationship
assessing marketing executives’ intercultural disposition trust, performance, turnover, and so on. In other words, are
and perceived intercultural communication competence. certain individuals more predisposed to successful inter-
The measures used in this study can provide an initial start- action with culturally diverse individuals than others?
ing point in identifying individuals who possess these Such findings could affect the type of diversity recruitment
capabilities. Other methods of assessment include per- and training needed in marketing organizations.
sonal inventories, interview questions, and simulation Within the framework of adaptive selling, future
games that help identify the reactions of individuals to research could investigate how intercultural disposition
diverse cultures (cf. Bush and Ingram 2001; Cushner and and competence ultimately affect performance. His-
Brislin 1996; Shirts 1973). torically, however, research results that provide clear link-
ages between adaptive selling skills and performance out-
comes have been rare. Goolsby, Lagace, and Boorom
LIMITATIONS AND DIRECTIONS (1992) only found one relationship between the psycho-
FOR FUTURE RESEARCH logical adaptiveness trait of self-monitoring and perfor-
mance. Those authors concluded that “the relationship
As with any study, there are limitations to the results between adaptive selling and . . . performance is likely to
from this work. First, external validity may be somewhat be moderated by factors such as the nature of the sales situ-
lessened because of the use of marketing professionals ation and/or the nature of the product itself” (p. 62). When
from executive MBA courses and from actual corpora- the buyer-seller situation crosses cultural boundaries,
tions. However, individuals were solicited from a broad intercultural disposition may be one of the factors that has
base of industries who were concerned about the issue of mediated the effect of adaptiveness on subsequent
diversity in their clientele as well as within their own orga- performance.
nization. Second, the findings of this study are based on Another research path concerns the customer’s evalua-
self-reported data. However, the constructs of interest to tion of the marketing executive’s intercultural skills. Simi-
this study were not significantly correlated with social lar arguments can be found in the intercultural literature
desirability. Third, the reliability for the Worldmindedness that advocate investigating the host culture’s reactions to
Scale was relatively low, which may have attenuated some foreigners/sojourners into their culture (Gudykunst 1994).
of the relationships reported. Fourth, the results of this Thus, future research can be conducted by investigating
study could vary with such issues as individuals’ experi- buyers’ perceptions of sellers’ intercultural skills.
ence and travel abroad. While beyond the scope of the Investigating various types of training programs for
present study, future research could examine these issues. developing intercultural competence could also provide
The constructs selected for this study are not exhaustive helpful insights. Such programs as exposure to different
but rather serve as a starting point for relating adaptive ethnic neighborhoods (i.e., market territories), movies that
selling concepts to intercultural domains. For example, emphasize cultural differences, classes on cross-cultural
Bush et al. / INTERCULTURAL COMMUNICATION 401

communication, and simulation exercises could be investi- APPENDIX


gated and compared. Long-term study is also needed to Scale Items and Reliabilities for Study
track the overall effectiveness of such training programs
on such issues as performance, retention, and upward
Empathic Tendency—
mobility of culturally diverse employees within the mar- Perspective Taking ( = .71)
keting organization.
Finally, measures more specific to the marketing envi- Range: 1 (describes me very well) to 6 (does not describe me
ronment could be developed. Some of the measures used well)
in this study were borrowed from the educational and
cross-cultural communication disciplines. This may have 1. I believe that there are two sides to every question and
try to look at them both.
been the cause of one of the measures’ (i.e., worldminded-
2. I try to look at everybody’s side of a disagreement be-
ness) somewhat lower reliability. Insights from the present
fore I make a decision.
study can be used to develop better measures to gauge the
3. When I’m upset at someone, I usually try to “put my-
intercultural skills of marketing executives. self in his or her shoes’ for a while.
4. If I’m sure I’m right about something, I don’t waste
much time listening to other people’s arguments. (R)
CONCLUSIONS 5. I sometimes find it difficult to see things from the
“other guy’s” point of view. (R)
While marketing academics and practitioners recog- 6. I sometimes try to understand my friends better by
nize the importance of adapting to the customer, adapting imagining how this looks from their perspective.
to a culturally diverse customer is a relatively new issue. 7. Before criticizing somebody, I try to imagine how I
However, more traditional marketing functions are evolv- would feel if I were in their place.
ing toward relationship management. According to
Johnston, Lewin, and Spekman (1999), the
Worldmindedness ( = .63)
changing global economic and business environ-
ment is forcing firms to move increasingly closer to Range: 1 (strongly agree) to 6 (strongly disagree)
their exchange partners, to form international alli-
ances, and to participate in complex multifirm/mul- 1. It would be better to be a citizen of the world than of
tinational networks. . . . Indeed, many practitioners any particular nation.
believe that a company’s competitive prowess no 2. Our responsibility to people of other races ought to be
longer depends upon the company itself, but on its as great as our responsibility to people of our own
ability to establish and maintain high-quality busi- area.
ness partnerships. (P. 269) 3. Any healthy individual, regardless or race or religion,
should be allowed to live wherever he or she wants to
Thus, traditional forms of communication in marketing in the world.
must be viewed from an integrated marketing perspective, 4. Our schools should teach the history of the world
where highly personalized, targeted communication be- rather than of our own nation.
comes a key focus in the development of relationships. 5. Our country should permit the immigration of foreign
When these relationships cross cultural or ethnic bound- peoples even if it lowers our standard of living.
aries, the necessity of training for adaptive, intercultural
communication becomes more apparent.
In conclusion, successful relationship management Ethnocentrism ( = .74)
skills should be emphasized in today’s culturally diverse
environment. People possessing these skills will become Range: 1 (strongly agree) to 6 (strongly disagree)
valuable assets to the firm (Thorelli 1986; Webster 1992).
1. In many countries, people do not place a high value on
According to Gronroos (1999), “A true transition toward a human life—to them, life is cheap.
relationship marketing strategy requires a focus on 2. English should be accepted as the international lan-
resources and competencies in the relationship” (p. 334). guage of communication.
One of those resources are the communication competen- 3. Americans have been very generous in teaching other
cies exhibited by the marketing organization. As global people how to do things in more efficient ways.
competition and cultural diversity in the workplace con- 4. Primitive people have unsophisticated social and po-
tinue to increase, people possessing intercultural commu- litical systems.
nication competence will be of extreme importance in 5. Minority groups within a country should conform to
managing relationships that cross cultural boundaries. the customs and values of the majority.
402 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

Patriotism The following is a list of qualities that have been identified as


being associated with success in sales encounters with people
1. America may not be perfect, but the American way from different cultural backgrounds. Please rate your:
has brought us about as close as human beings can get
to a perfect society. Intercultural Communication Stress Skills (α = .82):
2. The main threat to basic American institutions during
this century has come from the infiltration of foreign 1. ability to deal with interpersonal conflict
ideas, doctrines, and agitators. 2. ability to deal with stress
3. ability to deal with social alienation
4. ability to deal with anxiety
5. ability to deal with communication misunderstand-
Attributional Complexity ( = .78) ings between yourself and others

Range: 1 (describes me very well) to 6 (does not describe me Intercultural Communication Relationship Skills (α = .75):
well)
1. ability to initiate interaction with a stranger
1. I really enjoy analyzing the reasons or causes of peo- 2. ability to develop satisfying interpersonal relation-
ple’s behavior. ships with others
2. I am very interested in how my own thinking works 3. ability to maintain satisfying interpersonal relation-
when I make judgments about people or attach causes ships with others
to their behavior. 4. ability to accurately understand the feelings of an-
3. To understand a person’s personality or behavior, I other person
have found it important to know how that person’s at- 5. ability to empathize with another person
titudes, beliefs, and character traits fit together.
4. I think a lot about the influences that society has on Intercultural Communication Style Skills (Kuder-Richardson =
other people. .73):
5. I enjoy learning about other cultures through reading
and movies. 1. ability to deal with different communication styles
2. ability to deal with different social systems
NOTE: (R) = reverse coded.
Adaptive Selling Behaviors ( = .88)

Range: 1 (strongly agree) to 6 (strongly disagree) NOTES

1. When I feel that my sales approach is not working, I 1. The model was also run excluding the 12.4 percent of respondents
can easily change to another approach. who indicated that they rarely (7.7%) or never (4.7%) interact with di-
2. I like to experiment with different sales approaches. verse customers. This analysis produced similar results to those using the
full sample with no changes in the significance of any of the paths and all
3. I am very flexible in the selling approach I use. fit indices suggesting an acceptable model fit.
4. I don’t change my approach from one customer to an- 2. All constructs were examined via factor loadings and item-to-total
other. (R) correlations. Any item-to-total correlation that fell below .30 was de-
5. I can easily use a wide variety of selling approaches. leted. Subsequent confirmatory factor analysis revealed that all first-
6. It is easy for me to modify my sales presentation if the order scales were unidimensional, with all constructs fitting adequately
situation calls for it. on a one-factor model. The Comparative Fit Index (CFI), the Incremental
Fit Index (IFI), and the Goodness-of-Fit Index (GFI) for all constructs in
7. Basically, I use the same approach with most custom-
this study were above .90 for a one-factor model. More details regarding
ers. (R) reliability and validity of the measures are available from the authors
8. I vary my sales style from situation to situation. upon request.
9. I try to understand how one customer differs from an-
other.
10. I feel confident that I can effectively change my REFERENCES
planned presentation when necessary.
Abe, H. and R. Wiseman. 1983. “A Cross-Cultural Confirmation of the
Dimensions of Intercultural Effectiveness.” International Journal of
Intercultural Relations 7 (1): 53-67.
Perceived Intercultural Anderson, J. and J. Narus. 1990. “A Model of Distributor Firm and Manu-
facturer Firm Working Relationships.” Journal of Marketing 54 (Jan-
Communication Competence uary): 42-58.
Bertagnoli, Lisa. 2001. “Culture Club: Executives Get Boost From Di-
Range: 1 (high ability) to 6 (low ability) versity Recognition.” Marketing News, January 29, p. 9.
Bush et al. / INTERCULTURAL COMMUNICATION 403

Bhawuk, Dharm P. S. and Harry C. Triandis. 1996. “The Role of Culture  and Y. Y. Kim. 1984. Communicating With Strangers: An Ap-
Theory in the Study of Culture and Intercultural Training.” In Hand- proach to Intercultural Communication. New York: Random House.
book of Intercultural Training. Eds. Dan Landis and Rabi Bhagat. , R. L. Wiseman, and M. R. Hammer. 1977. “Determinants of a
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, 17-34. Sojourner’s Attitudinal Satisfaction: A Path Model.” In Communica-
Bollen, Kenneth A. 1989. Structural Equations With Latent Variables. tion Yearbook. Ed. B. Ruben. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Pub-
New York: John Wiley. lishing, 415-425.
Boorom, Michael L., Jerry R. Goolsby, and Rosemary P. Ramsey. 1998. Hair, J. F., R. E. Anderson, R. L. Tatham, and W. C. Black. 1992.
“Relational Communication Traits and Their Effect on Adaptiveness Multivariate Data Analysis. New York: Macmillan.
and Sales Performance.” Journal of the Academy of Marketing Sci- Hammer, M., W. Gudykunst, and L. Wiseman. 1978. “Dimensions of
ence 26 (1): 16-30. Intercultural Effectiveness: An Exploratory Study.” International
Bradford, Lisa, Mike Allen, and Kevin Beisser. 1997. “An Evaluation and Journal of Intercultural Relations 2 (4): 383-393.
Meta-Analysis of Intercultural Communication Competence Re- Hannigan, Terence P. 1990. “Traits, Attitudes, and Skills That Are Re-
search.” Working Paper. University of Wisconsin–Milwaukee. lated to Intercultural Effectiveness and Their Implications for Cross-
Brislin, R. and T. Yoshida. 1994. Intercultural Communication Training: Cultural Training: A Review of the Literature.” International Journal
An Introduction. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. of Intercultural Relations 14:89-111.
Bush, Victoria D. and Thomas N. Ingram. 1996. “Adapting to Diverse Harris, Phillip R. and Robert T. Moran. 1991. Managing Cultural Differ-
Customers: A Training Matrix for International Marketers.” Indus- ences. Houston, TX: Gulf.
trial Marketing Management 25 (5): 373-383. Hensel, Paul and Gordon Bruner. 1992. “Scaling and Measurement:
 and . 2001. “Building and Assessing Cultural Diversity Multi-Item Scaled Measures in Sales Related Research.” Journal of
Skills: Implications for Sales Training.” Industrial Marketing Man- Personal Selling and Sales Management 12 (3): 77-82.
agement 30 (1): 65-76. Honeycutt, Earl and John Ford. 1995. “Guidelines for Managing an Inter-
Carlson, J. and K. Widaman. 1988. “The Effect of Study Abroad During national Sales Force.” Industrial Marketing Management 24 (2):
College on Attitudes Toward Other Cultures.” International Journal 135-144.
of Intercultural Relations 12:1-17. Ingram, Thomas N., Charles H. Schwepker, Jr., and Don Hutson. 1992.
Cleveland, H., G. Mangone, and J. Adams. 1960. The Overseas Ameri- “Why Salespeople Fail.” Industrial Marketing Management 21 (Au-
cans—A Report on Americans Abroad. New York: McGraw-Hill. gust): 225-230.
Comer, Lucette B. and Tanya Drollinger. 1999. “Active Empathetic Lis- Johnston, Wesley J., Jeffrey E. Lewin, and Robert E. Spekman. 1999.
tening and Selling Success: A Conceptual Framework.” Journal of “International Industrial Marketing Interactions: Dyadic and Net-
Personal Selling and Sales Management 19 (Winter): 15-29. work Perspectives.” Journal of Business Research 46 (November):
 and Marvin A. Jolson. 1991. “Perceptions of Gender Stereotypic 259-271.
Behavior: An Exploratory Study of Women in Selling.” Journal of Kang, Jikyeong and Julie Hillary. 1998. “Older Salespeople’s Role in Re-
Personal Selling and Sales Management 11 (Winter): 17-32. tail Sales Encounters.” Journal of Personal Selling and Sales Man-
agement 18 (Fall): 39-47.
 and J.A.F. Nicholls. 2000. “Communication Between Hispanic
Levinson, Daniel J. 1950. “The Study of Ethnocentric Ideology.” In The
Salespeople and Their Customers: A First Look.” Journal of Personal
Authoritarian Personality. Eds. T. W. Adorno, Else Frenkel-
Selling and Sales Management 20 (Summer): 121-127.
Brunswik, Daniel J. Levinson, and R. Nevitt Sanford. New York:
, , and Leslie J. Vermillion. 1998. “Diversity in the Sales Harper & Row, 108-163.
Force: Problems and Challenges.” Journal of Personal Selling and Levy, Michael and Arun Sharma. 1994. “Adaptive Selling: The Role of
Sales Management 18 (Fall): 1-20. Gender, Age, Sales Experience, and Education.” Journal of Business
Cronbach, L. 1951. “Coefficient Alpha and the Internal Structure of Research 31:39-47.
Tests.” Psychometrika 16 (September): 297-334. Loden, M. and J. Rosener. 1991. Workforce America! Managing Em-
Cui, G. and S. Van Den Berg. 1991. “Testing the Construct Validity of ployee Diversity as a Vital Resource. Homewood, IL: Irwin.
Intercultural Effectiveness.” International Journal of Intercultural Lustig, Myron and Jolene Koester. 1993. Intercultural Competence. New
Relations 15:227-241. York: HarperCollins.
Cushner, Kenneth and Richard W. Brislin. 1996. Intercultural Interac- Marks, Ronald, Douglas W. Vorhies, and Gordon J. Badovick. 1996. “A
tions: A Practical Guide. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Psychometric Evaluation of the ADAPTS Scale: A Critique and Rec-
Davis, M. H. 1980. “A Multidimensional Approach to Individual Differ- ommendations.” Journal of Personal Selling and Sales Management
ences in Empathy.” JSAS Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychol- 16 (4): 53-65.
ogy 10:85. McBane, Donald A. 1995. “Empathy and the Salesperson: A Multidi-
. 1983. “The Effects of Dispositional Empathy on Emotional Re- mensional Perspective.” Psychology and Marketing 12 (July): 349-
actions and Helping: A Multidimensional Approach.” Journal of Per- 370.
sonality 51:114-174. Merrill, David and Roger Reid. 1981. Personal Styles and Effective Per-
Deshpande, Rohit and Frederick Webster. 1989. “Organizational Culture formance. Radnor, PA: Chilton.
and Marketing: Defining the Research Agenda.” Journal of Mar- Mohr, J. and J. Nevin. 1990. “Communication Strategies in Marketing
keting 53 (January): 3-15. Channels: A Theoretical Perspective.” Journal of Marketing 54 (Oc-
Dwyer, Sean, Orlando Richard, and C. David Shepherd. 1998. “An Ex- tober): 36-51.
ploratory Study of Gender and Age Matching in the Salesperson- Montgomery, David. 1991. “Understanding the Japanese as Customers,
Prospective Customer Dyad: Testing Similarity-Performance Predic- Competitors, and Collaborators.” Japan and the World Economy 3
tions.” Journal of Personal Selling and Sales Management 18 (Fall): (1): 61-91.
55-69.  and Allan M. Weiss. 1991. “Managerial Preferences for Strategic
Fletcher, G., P. Danilovics, G. Fernandez, D. Peterson, and G. Reeder. Alliance Attributes.” Research Paper 1134. Stanford University,
1986. “Attributional Complexity: An Individual Differences Mea- Graduate School of Business, Stanford, CA.
sure.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 51:875-884. Nunnally, J. 1978. Psychometric Theory. 2d ed. New York: McGraw-
Goolsby, Jerry, Rosemary Lagace, and Michael Boorom. 1992. “Psycho- Hill.
logical Adaptiveness and Sales Performance.” Journal of Personal Palmer, Timothy and Gregory Pickett. 1999. “The Role of Mental
Selling and Sales Management 12 (Spring): 51-63. Models in Control Theory: Understanding Cognitive Factors Influ-
Gronroos, Christian. 1999. “Relationship Marketing: Challenges for the encing the Behaviors of Salespeople.” Journal of Marketing Theory
Organization.” Journal of Business Research 46 (November): 327- and Practice 7 (1): 17-29.
335. Porter, R. and L. Samovar. 1991. “Basic Principles of Intercultural Com-
Gudykunst, W. 1994. Bridging Differences: Effective Intergroup Com- munication.” In Intercultural Communication. Eds. L. Samovar and
munication. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. R. Porter. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth, 5-22.
404 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

Price, Linda L., Eric J. Arnould, and Patrick Tierney. 1995. “Going to Ex- , , and Leslie M. Fine. 1990. “The Customer-Salesperson
tremes: Managing Service Encounters and Assessing Provider Per- Dyad: An Interaction/Communication Model and Review.” Journal
formance.” Journal of Marketing 59 (2): 83-97. of Personal Selling and Sales Management 10 (Summer): 29-43.
Ruben, B. 1976. “Assessing Communication Competency for Intercul-
Wiseman, R. L., M. R. Hammer, and H. Nishida. 1989. “Predictors of
tural Adaptation.” Group and Organizational Studies 1:334-354.
Intercultural Communication Competence.” International Journal of
Sales and Marketing Executives International, Sales and Marketing Man-
Intercultural Relations 13:101-357.
agement Resource Center. 2000. “Sales and Marketing Compensa-
tion and Trends Survey” [Online]. Available: http://www.sell. org
Sampson, D. and H. Smith. 1957. “A Scale to Measure World-Minded
Attitudes.” Journal of Social Psychology 45:99-106. ABOUT THE AUTHORS
Schuster, Camille P. and Jeffrey E. Danes. 1986. “Asking Questions:
Some Characteristics of Successful Sales Encounters.” Journal of
Personal Selling and Sales Management 6 (May): 17-27. Victoria D. Bush (Ph.D., University of Memphis) is an associate
Seal, K. 1991. “Know thy Neighbor.” American Way, August 15, p. 34.
Sharma, Subhash, Terence Shimp, and Jeongshin Shin. 1995. “Consumer professor of marketing at the University of Mississippi. Her re-
Ethnocentrism: A Test of Antecedents and Moderators.” Journal of search has appeared in such journals as the Journal of Advertis-
the Academy of Marketing Science 23 (1): 26-37. ing, the Journal of Advertising Research, Industrial Marketing
Shimp, Terence A. and Subhash Sharma. 1987. “Consumer Management, the Journal of Public Policy and Marketing, the
Ethnocentrism: Construction and Validation of the CETSCALE.”
Journal of Marketing Research 24 (August): 280-289.
Journal of Business Ethics, and the Journal of Services Mar-
Shirts, Gary. 1973. BAFA BAFA. La Jolla, CA: Simili II. keting. Her research interests are in diversity, advertising, and
Shoham, Aviv, Gregory M. Rose, and Gerald Albaum. 1995. “Export ethics.
Motives, Psychological Distance, and the EPRG Framework.” Jour-
nal of Global Marketing 8 (3/4): 9-38.
, , and Lynn R. Kahle. 1998. “Risky Sports Participation: Gregory M. Rose (Ph.D., University of Oregon) is an associate
From Intention to Action.” Journal of the Academy of Marketing Sci- professor of marketing at the University of Mississippi. His re-
ence 26 (4): 307-321. search interests include consumer socialization and cross-cul-
Spiro, R. and B. Weitz. 1990. “Adaptive Selling: Conceptualization, tural consumer behavior. He has published or has forthcoming
Measurement and Nomological Validity.” Journal of Marketing Re-
search 27 (February): 61-69. articles in the Journal of Consumer Research, the Journal of the
Spitzberg, Brian H. 1991. “Intercultural Communication Competence” Academy of Marketing Science, the Journal of Business Re-
In Intercultural Communication. Eds. L. Samovar and R. Porter. search, the Journal of Consumer Psychology, the Journal of Ad-
Belmont, CA: Wadsworth, 353-365. vertising, the Journal of Marketing, and other journals and
 and William R. Cupach. 1984. Interpersonal Communication proceedings.
Competence. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Stephan, Cookie and Walter Stephan. 1992. “Reducing Intercultural
Anxiety Through Intercultural Contact.” International Journal of Faye Gilbert (Ph.D., University of North Texas) is an associate
Intercultural Relations 16:89-106. professor of marketing at the University of Mississippi. She has
Strahan, Robert and Kathleen Gerbasi. 1972. “Short, Homogeneous Ver-
sions of the Marlow-Crowne Social Desirability Scale.” Journal of published in the Journal of Business Research, Psychology and
Clinical Psychology 28:191-193. Marketing, the Journal of Health Care Marketing, the Journal of
Sujan, Harish, Barton Weitz, and Nirmalya Kumar. 1994. “Learning Ori- Research in Pharmaceutical Economics, the Journal of Applied
entation, Working Smart, and Effective Selling.” Journal of Mar- Business Research, the Journal of Marketing Management, the
keting 58 (July): 39-52.
Thorelli, Hans. 1986. “Networks: Between Markets and Hierarchies.”
Journal of Marketing Theory and Practice, and the Journal of
Strategic Management Journal 7:37-51. Marketing Education, among others. Her work emphasizes the
U.S. Census Bureau, CenStats DataWell. 2000. “County Business Pat- application of consumer behavior theory to health care and to
terns, Shelby County, TN, Major Industry Group” [Online]. Avail- channel relationships.
able: http://tier2.census.gov/cgi-win/cbp/sector.exe
Webster, Frederick. 1992. “The Changing Role of Marketing in the Cor-
poration.” Journal of Marketing 56 (4): 1-17. Thomas N. Ingram (Ph.D., Georgia State University) is a pro-
 and Rohit Deshpande. 1991. “Analyzing Corporate Cultures in fessor of marketing at Colorado State University. He has been
Approaching the Global Marketplace.” Report No. 90-111. Mar- honored as the Marketing Educator of the Year by Sales and Mar-
keting Science Institute, Cambridge, MA.
Weitz, Barton A. 1978. “The Relationship Between Salesperson Perfor-
keting Executives International (SMEI) and as a recipient of the
mance and Understanding of Customer Decision Making.” Journal Mu Kappa Tau National Marketing Honor Society Recognition
of Marketing Research 15 (November): 501-516. Award for Outstanding Scholarly Contributions to the Sales Dis-
, and Sandy Jap. 1995. “Relationship Marketing in Distribution cipline. He has served as the editor of the Journal of Personal
Channels.” Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science 23 (4): 305- Selling and Sales Management and is the current editor of the
320.
Journal of Marketing Theory and Practice. His primary research
, Harish Sujan, and Mita Sujan. 1986. “Knowledge, Motivation
and Adaptive Behavior: A Framework for Improving Selling Effec- is in personal selling and sales management. His work has ap-
tiveness.” Journal of Marketing 50 (October): 174-191. peared in the Journal of Marketing, the Journal of Marketing Re-
Wentling, Rose Mary. 2000. “Current Status of Diversity Initiatives in search, the Journal of Personal Selling and Sales Management,
Selected Multinational Corporations.” Human Resource Develop- and the Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, among
ment Quarterly 11 (1): 35-61.
Williams, Kaylene C. and Rosann L. Spiro. 1985. “Communication Style others. He is the coauthor of three textbooks: Professional
in the Salesperson-Customer Dyad.” Journal of Marketing Research Selling: A Trust-Based Approach, Sales Management: Analysis
22 (November): 434-442. and Decision-Making, and Marketing: Principles and Perspectives.
JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY
Summers /OF
RESEARCH
MARKETING
IN MARKETING
SCIENCE FALL 2001

Guidelines for Conducting Research


and Publishing in Marketing:
From Conceptualization Through
the Review Process
John O. Summers
Indiana University

A primary mission of institutions of higher learning is the Manuscript Acceptance Rates


generation and dissemination of knowledge. The low ac- at Leading Marketing Journals:
ceptance rates at the leading research journals in market- From Single Digit to Low Teens
ing, typically in the single digits to low teens, suggests the
need to increase the quality of the research manuscripts The acceptance rate at the leading research journals is
produced. This article presents a set of guidelines for re- currently averaging around 10 percent. Because editors
searchers aspiring to do scholarly research in marketing. are limited in the number of pages they can have in each
Discussed are issues such as developing the necessary re- issue, a journal’s acceptance rate is constrained by the
search skills, conceptualizing the study, constructing the number of manuscripts submitted and the average length
research design, writing the manuscript, and responding of the manuscripts accepted. Hence, as the overall quality
to reviewers. Also presented are the author’s personal ob- of the manuscripts received by a journal increases over
servations concerning the current state of research in time, its standards for acceptance also rise.
marketing. For most top journals, there isn’t a dramatic drop in
quality between the top 10 percent of manuscripts received
and the next best 10 percent, and most of the manuscripts
submitted to the leading journals are reasonably well-
This article is intended for doctoral students and those done. About 80 percent of the manuscripts submitted are
researchers who are beginning or are early in their careers rejected on the initial round of reviews. There are several
and would like to increase their journal acceptance rates. basic reasons for rejecting manuscripts reporting on em-
The experienced author with several major publications pirical studies. These include the following:
and years of reviewing experience will find little, if any-
thing, “new” to them. What follows are the author’s reflec- 1. The research questions being investigated are
not very interesting (e.g., studies that are mainly
tions on more than a quarter century of guiding doctoral
descriptive and lack theoretical implications).
students and reviewing for, and publishing in, some of the 2. The research, although well executed, does not
leading journals in marketing. The author’s remarks pri- appear to make a sufficiently large contribution
marily relate to research that involves the collection and to the literature (e.g., the study largely replicates
analysis of primary data (e.g., case studies, surveys, and past research with minor modifications).
experiments). Not addressed are such things as review 3. The conceptual framework is not well developed
papers, theory development not based on empirical (e.g., lacks precise conceptual definitions of the
research, and quantitative marketing models. constructs and/or compelling theoretical ratio-
nale for the hypotheses).
Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science. 4. The methodology is seriously flawed (e.g., the
Volume 29, No. 4, pages 405-415. sample is inappropriate for the research ques-
Copyright © 2001 by Academy of Marketing Science. tion, the validity of one or more key measures is
406 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

suspect, and/or the experiment lacks experimen- “accepting point of view” in reading the literature and
tal realism). focus on the conclusions of these studies, it will seem to
5. The writing is so confused that an invitation to them as if everything has been done, and they will feel dis-
revise and resubmit is considered unlikely to re- appointed that they had not thought to do these studies
sult in an acceptable manuscript. first. It is only when researchers look for flaws and/or limi-
tations in the research they read that they begin to develop
For a detailed discussion of the weaknesses in manu-
ideas for building on this research. For example, with
scripts cited by the reviewers of one leading journal along
regard to the conceptual framework, readers should con-
with some guideposts for authors, see Varadarajan (1996).
cern themselves with whether the conceptual definitions
To be published in a respected peer-reviewed journal, a
are sufficiently unambiguous and whether the theoretical
study must be judged as meeting the currently accepted
rationale provided for each of the hypotheses is convinc-
standards for scholarly research. Moreover, the study must
ing. With regard to survey research methodology, they
be judged as more worthy than others competing for the
might consider whether there is a serious problem with
same journal space. What should researchers do to
shared method variance and/or whether the measures used
increase the chances that their studies will make a signifi-
validly capture the constructs of interest. The limitations
cant contribution to marketing knowledge and be among
identified in existing research alert the researcher to
those that are eventually published by one of the leading
opportunities for making contributions to the research area
research journals? Answering this question is the major
of interest.
focus of this article.
Focus on Developing
SCHOLARLY RESEARCH ON Hypotheses to Be Tested
SUBSTANTIVE ISSUES IN MARKETING
As researchers start reading the literature, it is impor-
This section presents a set of 12 guidelines for research- tant that they begin thinking about identifying the hypoth-
ers aspiring to do scholarly research in marketing. These eses they might want to test. This will help them develop
guidelines deal with developing the necessary set of some structure for their conceptual frameworks and con-
research skills and the research process. struct boundaries for their empirical studies. This, in turn,
will allow them to determine which articles in their general
Develop a Broad Set area of interest are most central to the empirical study they
of Methodological Skills plan to design. In deciding what hypotheses to investigate
in the empirical study, thought should be given to the
Developing a broad set of methodological skills (e.g., potential contribution to the literature and the feasibility of
qualitative research methods, survey research methodol- developing a rigorous research design for testing them.
ogy, and experimental design) is critical to becoming a Researchers who fail to focus on developing hypotheses as
productive researcher. Those with a limited set of method- they review the literature often end up spending many
ological tools are restricted in what they can study and months or even a year reading the literature without having
what they can learn from their research. For example, identified a single hypothesis they want to test.
someone with weak or no training in qualitative research
methods is very limited with regard to developing Use the Literature to
grounded theory in his or her research area of interest, and Stimulate Your Thinking
researchers without a background in experimental design
are likely to use surveys to test causal hypotheses. It is critical that the existing literature be used to stimu-
Developing a broad set of methodological skills early in late one’s thinking beyond that of merely understanding
one’s career provides long-term benefits because one can what is covered in each of the individual articles reviewed.
rely on this same set of skills for many years. Many of the In this regard, researchers need to consider such things as
research techniques used today were developed several why different studies may have produced what seem to be
decades ago. For example, much of the most important conflicting results and what overall inferences one can
work on reliability and validity was published during the draw from the studies as a group. They also should concern
1950s and 1960s. themselves with how existing conceptual frameworks
might be improved. For example, have previous research-
Learn to Be a Critical ers overlooked important antecedents or consequences?
Reader of the Literature Have past studies failed to consider potential mediators or
moderators? Researchers must avoid allowing the litera-
It is important to become practiced in reading the litera- ture to constrain their thinking. One aid for doing this is for
ture in a critical manner. When researchers take an researchers to constantly ask themselves what they
Summers / RESEARCH IN MARKETING 407

personally believe about the phenomenon of interest. meaningful theoretical rationale for why Construct A
These are issues that researchers should concern them- should be related to Construct B if the exact meaning of
selves with as they are reviewing the literature rather than each of these two constructs has not been established.
only after all of the literature has been read. Moreover, it is impossible to develop a valid measure of a
construct that is not precisely defined.
Put It on Paper Avoid developing pseudodefinitions. Some authors
will talk about some Construct A being a result of or the
Researchers should write down their ideas as they occur cause of some other Construct B. However, one cannot
to them and maintain a file. Failure to immediately commit define a construct in terms of its antecedents or its conse-
one’s ideas to paper means that time will be wasted trying quences. Moreover, trying to do so means that the pro-
to rediscover old ideas, and some ideas may be lost forever. posed theoretical linkage between A and B would not be
The mere act of writing down their ideas often makes empirically testable (i.e., it could not be falsified); rather, it
researchers more aware of ambiguities in their thinking. would be true by definition. Another type of pseudodefini-
Frequently, arguments that seem so clear in their heads tion one finds in the literature involves merely giving
become unraveled when they write these down. This per- examples of what is included in a construct (e.g., Con-
mits researchers to identify the problems in their current struct A includes such things as . . . ). These pseudodefini-
thinking and work to resolve them. Finally, committing tions invariably provide an incomplete listing of the con-
one’s thoughts to writing makes it much easier to get con- struct’s content and fail to indicate what is not included in
structive feedback from others. the construct. The central role of constructs requires that
researchers make reasonably certain that their constructs
Don’t Work in Isolation are well defined before moving on to other aspects of their
conceptual framework or to their research designs.
It is difficult for most researchers to conceptualize a
tight research study without interacting with others, if for Evaluate the Hypotheses
no other reason than that it is difficult for people to evalu-
ate their own work. This is particularly true for less-experi- The hypotheses to be tested also need to be evaluated
enced researchers. Doctoral students who have infrequent before designing the empirical study.
interaction with their dissertation committees almost
always take a long time to complete their dissertations. It is • Are the hypotheses clearly written?
often the case that researchers clarify their own thoughts, • Is each of the hypotheses falsifiable?
identify problems with their conceptual framework, and • Do any of the hypotheses involve truism or tautolo-
discover new ideas solely as a result of communicating gies?
their current thinking to others. The mere process of orally • Are any of the hypotheses trivial in the sense that
explaining their thoughts to others forces researchers to others would be likely to question the methodology
examine their ideas more deeply. Hence, it is almost of any study that reported negative results?
always a mistake for researchers to wait until they feel their • Is the theoretical rationale provided for each hypoth-
conceptual frameworks are very well developed before esis compelling?
exposing them to others. Although almost anyone willing • Are there any additional theoretical arguments that
to listen and read what has been written can be helpful, par- would strengthen the conceptual support for the hy-
ticularly valuable are those who constantly ask for clarifi- potheses?
cation and question the researcher’s assumptions, concep- • Do the hypotheses to be tested represent a cohesive
tual definitions, and theoretical rationale. These inter- set?
actions are especially beneficial when researchers have
previously committed their ideas to writing. It is important for researchers to aggressively solicit
criticism of all aspects of their conceptual framework. It is
Develop Precise Conceptual only when continued exposure of the conceptual frame-
Definitions for the Constructs work to criticism ceases to uncover serious flaws and all
necessary revisions have been made that researchers
The conceptual definitions of the constructs of interest should move to the design phase. The time to revise the
warrant special attention. Constructs are the building conceptual framework is before the data are collected. Af-
blocks of theory. Without well-developed conceptual defi- ter the data are collected, researchers are severely re-
nitions for the constructs, it is impossible to develop a stricted by the available measures as to what changes they
coherent theory. For example, we cannot develop a can make in their conceptual frameworks.
408 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

Identify the Intended Contributions 2. improve the construct validity of the putative
causes and effects (e.g., through the develop-
At this point, it is important to make explicit the in- ment of improved manipulations of the inde-
pendent variables and/or the improvement of
tended contributions of the study and to evaluate them. multiple-item scales for the dependent vari-
The contributions of a study can be conceptual, empirical, ables);
or methodological in nature. Conceptual contributions 3. enhance statistical conclusion validity;
could involve such things as: 4. increase the experimental realism of the experi-
ment; and/or
1. improved conceptual definitions of the original 5. decrease the plausibility of demand artifacts.
constructs;
2. the identification and conceptual definition of Not infrequently, less-experienced researchers try to
additional constructs to be added to the concep- design their studies to contain many such contributions in
tual framework (e.g., additional dependent, in- an attempt to make certain that the overall contribution of
dependent, mediating, and/or moderator their research will be sufficiently high. Pursuit of this ap-
variables); proach is often associated with the risk of the researcher’s
3. the development of additional theoretical link- time and effort getting so spread out among many tasks
ages (i.e., research hypotheses) with their ac-
that every aspect of the study is poorly done. The impor-
companying rationale; and
4. the development of improved theoretical ratio- tant issue is not how many contributions a study will make
nale for existing linkages. but rather the significance of each contribution. One
should be concerned with such things as the degree to
Empirical contributions would include such things as: which a proposed contribution fills some important gap in
the literature. For example, a study could make a very sub-
1. testing a theoretical linkage between two con- stantial contribution by demonstrating that a previously
structs that has not previously been tested, unidentified moderator variable could explain what previ-
2. examining the effects of a potential moderator ously appeared to be conflicting results in past research.
variable on the nature of the relationship be- Feedback from successful researchers with a reputation
tween two constructs, for being candid is very helpful in pruning the list of in-
3. determining the degree to which a variable me- tended contributions to those likely to have the greatest im-
diates the relationship between two constructs, pact on the research area of interest.
and
4. investigating the psychometric properties of an
important scale. Designing the Empirical Study

When field studies are being used, methodological con- When the conceptual framework has been set and the
tributions might involve changes in the design of past stud- intended contributions of the study determined, it is time
ies that: to consider the details of the research design. Although
past research in an area can serve as a valuable guide, it is
1. reduce the potential problems with shared important to recognize that no study is without method-
method variance through the insightful use of ological shortcomings. One should always be cognizant of
multiple methods of measurement, the methodological weaknesses and/or limitations of pub-
2. increase the generalizability of the research lished research and attempt to overcome these limitations
through more appropriate sampling procedures, in one’s own work. For example, to the degree that previ-
3. allow the investigation of the plausibility of ous measures appear to lack content validity, consider-
“third-variable explanations” for the results of ation should be given to revising some of the items used in
past studies, and/or these scales and developing new items to add.
4. enhance the construct validity of key measures
through the use of refined multiple-item mea- The time for researchers to get critical feedback on their
sures and/or the use of measurement approaches research designs is before they collect their data. Although
that do not rely on self-reports. researchers can make some modifications to their concep-
tual frameworks (e.g., clarify conceptual definitions, pro-
With respect to laboratory experiments, methodologi- vide additional theoretical rationale for some of the
cal contributions might involve such things as modifica- hypotheses) even while their manuscripts are under review
tions in the experimental procedures that serve to: at a research journal, nothing can be done to improve the
research methodology once the data have been collected.
1. increase the internal, ecological, and/or external Moreover, if the data are seriously flawed, no amount of
validity of the experiment; rewriting of the manuscript can overcome this fact.
Summers / RESEARCH IN MARKETING 409

Experts on the particular research methods being used measured when the scales are reflective. When building
should be solicited to critique the research design before multiple-item, reflective scales, it is useful to administer
the data are collected. Moreover, they should be encour- the questionnaire to a small sample (e.g., approximately
aged to be as critical and detailed as they are when review- 30 participants) after the initial pretest has been conducted
ing manuscripts for a journal. and revisions made. This allows researchers to determine
if their items are producing the anticipated pattern of cor-
Pretesting Questionnaires relations. When this pattern is not achieved, the sample
correlation matrix can be used to identify problem items.
A rigorous pretest of the questionnaire can almost These items can then be revised or discarded based on a
always provide valuable information on how it might be careful analysis of the content of each item.
improved. Unfortunately, many pretests are not very rigor-
ous and only give the researcher a false sense of security. Pretesting Experiments
For example, when conducting a pretest of a question-
naire, many researchers will ask a small sample from the Experiments involving human subjects are even more
population of interest to complete the questionnaire and difficult to design and pretest than are surveys. When
when they are finished ask them if they noticed any prob- developing a new experimental design, it is critical that an
lems. If those in the pretest sample complete all items on extensive evaluation of the design be undertaken. In addi-
the questionnaire and do not report any problems with any tion to pretesting the measures, researchers need to be con-
of the items, these researchers conclude that the question- cerned with whether (1) the experiment has a sufficient
naire is without serious flaws. However, this conclusion is amount of experimental realism, (2) the experiment con-
seldom justified. Participants often mark responses to the tains demand artifacts, (3) the manipulations provide the
most confusing questionnaire items and never question intended variance in the independent variables, and (4) the
what these items were intended to measure. When asked manipulations might be causing unintended variance in
after completing a questionnaire whether any part of it was other variables that might have an impact on the dependent
confusing, participants typically say little, if anything, variables of interest. After evaluating their own initial
even when many of the questions are confusingly worded. experimental designs and making the necessary revisions,
There are several plausible reasons for this situation. First, researchers should ask one or two individuals with special
pretest participants may be constrained in the time and expertise in experimental design (e.g., those who routinely
thought they are willing and able to devote to filling out review manuscripts reporting experimental studies for the
questionnaires. Second, they may not be sufficiently leading research journals) to examine their experimental
skilled and/or experienced at detecting and articulating designs and materials and to comment on what they feel
problems with questionnaire items. Finally, they may be the weaknesses of the designs might be. After revising
reluctant to be critical, even when asked to. their designs, researchers should recruit three or four
Pretesting of the questionnaire is especially critical if insightful and articulate individuals to serve as initial pre-
new scales are being constructed or previous scales have test participants. These participants should be asked to
been significantly revised. To determine what pretest par- provide a verbal protocol as they proceed through the
ticipants really think about their questionnaires, research- experiment in a thoughtful manner. After all necessary
ers must be very aggressive in extracting this information. revisions have been made, a pretest using participants
For example, as the pretest participants complete the ques- from the population of interest should be conducted. The
tionnaire, the researcher might ask these participants primary purpose of this pretest is to collect manipulation
whether they can think of more than one way to interpret and confounding check measures. This will tell research-
what each item is asking and to report these interpreta- ers whether their manipulations are working as planned. If
tions. This should be done separately with each participant the dependent variables are assessed during this pretest,
one question at a time. The researcher might also ask these they should be measured after the manipulation and con-
participants to explain why they responded the way they founding checks. Given a sufficient sample size for the
did on each item. However, this approach will work only if pretest, it will not be necessary to include manipulation
the participants are perceptive and willing to devote a sig- and confounding checks in the main experiment.
nificant amount of time thinking about each item. One Unless a behavioral experiment largely replicates a past
insightful and articulate pretest participant who is commit- research design, failure to identify several significant
ted to providing constructive criticism is worth more than problems in the initial design is reason for concern. It is
20 reluctant pretest participants. rarely, if ever, the case that a newly developed research
Whenever feasible, it is a good idea to use multiple- design does not contain several serious methodological
item scales because these scales are usually more reliable problems. Hence, when the initial pretest does not reveal
than single-item scales and their reliability can be easily serious defects in the research design, the researcher
410 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

should strongly consider conducting a second, more rigor- Previous research has addressed several aspects of
ous pretest. ___________ : (1) ______________ (cite two to three
relevant articles), (2) _____________ (cite two to
three relevant articles), and (3) _____________ (cite
CRAFTING MANUSCRIPTS FOR two to three relevant articles).
SCHOLARLY JOURNALS IN MARKETING
The results of the studies cited need not be reviewed
When researchers do an excellent job of conceptualiz- when the current article focuses on different issues than
ing their studies, developing and executing their research those covered in the studies cited.
designs, and analyzing their data, the most difficult part of Then, researchers need to identify important gaps, in-
their work is behind them. Researchers need not be tal- consistencies, and/or controversies in the literature. This
ented or creative writers to report the results of well-con- serves to establish the need for additional research in the
ceptualized and executed studies. They only need to be topic area of interest. This task, like those that precede it,
organized, accurate, and concise in their writing. All well- can be achieved in a concise manner. For example, con-
written manuscripts have three characteristics in common: sider the following sample text:
(1) an introduction that “sells” the study; (2) tight logic,
clarity, and conciseness throughout all sections; and (3) a However, in addition, ___________________ en-
creative and insightful Discussion and Conclusions compasses several unexplored dimensions that
section. lately have attracted research attention in other dis-
ciplines (cite two to three relevant articles).
Introduction—Selling the Study Some of these unexplored ________ appear to be
important and worthy of investigation in the context
To convince readers of the importance of their studies, of _____________________________.
authors need to accomplish the following four goals in the An investigation of these issues is important be-
indicated order: cause ___________________________________.
Furthermore, previous empirical research has fo-
1. Establish the importance of the general area of cused primarily on ___________________ . Very
interest. little research has been done on ______________ .
2. Indicate in general terms what has been done in
this broad area. Finally, and most important, the researcher must pro-
3. Identify important gaps, inconsistencies, and/or vide a concise statement of the manuscript’s purposes, the
controversies in the relevant literature. contributions the manuscript makes to the literature. This
4. Provide a concise statement of the manuscript’s statement should follow logically from the text that identi-
purpose(s), the contributions the manuscript fies gaps, inconsistencies, and/or controversies in the liter-
makes to the literature. ature. For example, consider the following sample text:

The contributions noted should relate back to the gaps, In this study we seek to extend _______________
inconsistencies, and controversies identified earlier. by addressing the gaps in ________________ . The
In establishing the importance of the general area of in- study investigates the impact of four ___________ :
terest, one need not develop long and complicated argu- (1) __________ , (2) ___________ , (3) ____________ ,
ments or discuss the detailed results of several articles. and (4) _______________ . In addition, interrela-
Establishing the importance of the topic area can often be tionships among __________________________
accomplished rather quickly and easily as the following are examined.
sample text suggests:
Researchers should avoid trying to develop a long list
_____________ researchers have devoted consider- of contributions (conceptual, empirical, and methodologi-
able attention to developing and testing models of cal). Inevitably, several of these “contributions” will be of
___________________ (e.g., cite several promi- low importance and will divert the reader’s attention from
nent articles in the area).1 the major focus of the study. Researchers must make clear
what major contributions their studies make and explain
Next, the author should indicate in general terms what why these contributions are important. It is a mistake to as-
has been done in the broad area. A lot of journal space need sume that readers will decipher the importance of the study
not be devoted to achieving this goal. It is not expected or from a description of what was done. The failure to clearly
desirable that authors report the detailed findings of indi- specify the importance of the study in the introduction is
vidual studies. For example, consider the following sam- often the result of not having given enough thought to this
ple text: issue before the study was conducted.
Summers / RESEARCH IN MARKETING 411

Writing Quality Research methodology: “Data were obtained


through self-administered questionnaires from
Writing quality is often a reflection of the clarity of the _________________ in three ________________ .”
author’s thoughts. Overly vague ideas invariably lead to “A total of ______ usable responses were obtained
confused writing or the lack of any writing. It is generally for an overall response rate of ________ .”
the case that when authors have trouble writing, the prob- “ ________ was measured by an ___ item instru-
lem lies primarily with the clarity of their thoughts as ment based on the research of ______________
opposed to their ability to phrase their ideas properly. As (cite key article).”
such, authors should first question their understanding of
what they want to communicate when they are having dif- Each of the above passages contains a lot of informa-
ficulty writing. tion while using very few words.
The manuscript must be clearly written, concise, and Another way to keep the length of a research manu-
characterized by tight logic. When evaluating their own script reasonable is to be parsimonious in the use of refer-
writing, authors will often ask themselves whether the text ences. Often, two or at most three well-chosen references
is consistent with their ideas. This is far too low a standard will provide sufficient support for a position. Moreover,
to use because it does little, if anything, to ensure that the too many references may make the manuscript difficult to
reader will understand the author’s message. Instead, one read.
should adopt Stevenson’s standard: “Don’t write merely to Sections involving reviews of the literature deserve
be understood. Write so that you cannot possibly be special attention. It is unsatisfactory to provide a series of
misunderstood.” summaries of individual studies when reviewing past
Authors need to ask themselves whether it is possible to research. These consume journal space without adding
derive either unintended meaning or no meaning at all anything to our understanding of the literature. As Chur-
from what they have written. The aggressive search for chill and Perreault (1982) observe, a review should
alternative interpretations of one’s text is a key to identify- “advance the field by virtue of its insightful, integrative,
ing ambiguous and confusing passages. and critical evaluation of the state of work in a subject
Jargon, the specialized vocabulary of a discipline, can area.” A good review section will provide a synthesis of the
be useful by adding precision and conciseness to research- literature and make clear what is “known” with a fair
ers’ writings. However, it is frequently misused (overused) amount of certainty and where the gaps are.
in an attempt to make a manuscript appear more sophisti-
cated. Unfortunately, it typically achieves the opposite A Creative and Insightful
effect. All such terms should be defined where they first Discussion and Conclusions Section
appear unless their meaning is (1) invariant and (2) well-
known to most readers. The Discussion and Conclusions section is the last
Conciseness in writing is a virtue, particularly when thing readers see, and it can have a large impact on their
publishing in research journals. Since journal space is impressions of the research being reported. This section
scarce and costly, the contribution-to-length ratio is an im- should build on the Introduction section. In this regard, it
portant consideration in a journal’s decision as to whether needs to reaffirm the importance of the study by showing
or not to accept a manuscript for publication. While writ- how the study reported fits into the literature (e.g., what
ing in a succinct manner can be a daunting task for first- gaps in the literature it fills). The study’s contributions and
time authors, examining particularly well-written articles their importance should be made clear by communicating
in the target journal can be very helpful. For example, con- the study’s implications for theory and practice. To merely
sider the following passages that deal with conceptual def- summarize the empirical results is an inappropriate
initions, theoretical rationale for hypotheses, and research strategy.
methodology: It is important to clearly distinguish between conclu-
sions and speculation when writing the Discussion and
Conceptual definitions: “__________________ is Conclusions section. Conclusions must be clearly sup-
defined as _______________________________ .”
ported by the data. However, authors may have valuable,
(If borrowed, cite the source.)
informed speculation to share. As Churchill and Perreault
Rationale for hypotheses: “Considerable evidence
from previous research suggests that ___________ (1982) observe, “Good science and good ‘speculation’ are
_______________________ .” (Cite two to three not incompatible, but each should be clearly labeled so
key articles.) that the two are not confused” (p. 286). A few interesting
“Furthermore, _____________ (cite “leading ex- ideas can go a long way here. While the Discussion and
perts”) argue that _______________ , they hypothe- Conclusions section should not be dominated by specula-
size ___________________________ .” tion, authors should identify new issues raised by the
412 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

study’s findings and/or provide insightful (nonobvious) suggestions for major changes, authors should consider
directions for future research. soliciting critiques from a second set of colleagues
because it is unlikely that the first set of colleagues were
Self-Edit the Manuscript being sufficiently critical. Almost all of the approximately
10 percent of the manuscripts that are eventually accepted
The initial draft of even the most carefully prepared for publication at leading research journals are the subject
manuscript can always be significantly improved. As of substantial reviewer criticism and go through at least
such, the initial draft should be revised prior to submitting one major revision. Anyone who spends the time to give
the manuscript to others for their evaluation. It is difficult highly critical, constructive feedback to an author is doing
for authors to edit their own writing. In addition to the the author an enormous favor.
problem of being critical of one’s own work, authors know
what they wanted to communicate. This makes it difficult Responding to the Reviewers
for them to notice ambiguities and omissions in their
manuscripts. However, there are things writers can do to Authors are seldom pleased by the reviewers’ reactions
reduce these problems. Laying their manuscripts aside for to their manuscripts. After their initial reading of the
a few weeks reduces writers’ familiarity with their papers. reviewers’ comments, authors are frequently angered and/
This can help them develop a fresh perspective and be or depressed because they feel the reviewers have not
more open to changes. Another strategy involves analyz- fairly judged their work, some reviewers more so than oth-
ing the manuscript from the point of view of someone who ers. There is a natural tendency for authors to want to prove
knows little or nothing about the topic area. This would the most critical reviewers wrong, an approach that is dys-
include such things as checking to see whether the special- functional to the goal of getting their manuscripts pub-
ized terms have been clearly defined and whether the logic lished. Authors need to pause to recover from their initial
underlying each of the arguments made and the positions emotional reaction and develop a pragmatic approach to
taken are readily apparent. Finally, authors should ask dealing with the reviews. They need to keep in mind that
themselves whether their students would be likely to even the most critical reviewers are not vindictive and
understand most of what they have written. If not, the most of what they say is valid criticism. Reviewers for the
manuscript needs to be reworked. leading research journals tend to be very successful
researchers, and they typically spend from 1 to 2 days pre-
Solicit Critical Feedback paring their reviews for a single manuscript. The manu-
Before Submission script revision process must be guided by a careful consid-
eration of the suggestions and critical comments of the
“A colleague who will read what is written, then reviewers and the editor.
question its assumptions, ask what’s new, and quibble When, even after careful consideration, the specific
about its language is a person to be cultivated” (Markland content of a reviewer’s comment appears to be unjustified,
1983:142). authors should examine whether the comment is the prod-
Getting feedback from colleagues before a manuscript uct of some other problem with the manuscript. For exam-
is submitted to a journal can significantly increase the ple, authors may sometimes feel the reviewers are asking
chances of the manuscript being ultimately accepted for about issues already covered in their manuscripts or that
publication, but only if the feedback solicited is highly the reviewers do not understand what the authors are
critical and authors respond to this feedback in a positive doing. When this happens, it is best for authors to consider
fashion. Authors should select critics with extensive how they organized and explained things in their manu-
reviewing experience and ask them to treat their manu- script. It may be that the authors need to better communi-
scripts like they would if they had received these manu- cate what was done. Reviewers spend considerable time
scripts from a journal editor for review. It is not essential and effort reading each manuscript. If they are confused, it
that these critics be experts in the topic area of interest. A is likely that the journal’s readers will also be confused.
strong reviewer can usually provide excellent feedback on In addition to carefully studying the reviewers’ individ-
manuscripts dealing with a wide range of topics. The feed- ual comments, authors should look for trends in each
back writers receive from their colleagues on various reviewer’s comments. It may be that several of a reviewer’s
aspects of the manuscript (e.g., conceptual definitions, comments are all related to a single basic problem.
theoretical rationale, measurement of the constructs, and Reacting to the comments individually may not fix this
writing) can provide valuable guidance as to how authors problem and could even create additional problems by
might improve their manuscripts. producing a disjointed manuscript. Authors should also
Should the “reviews” received from an initial set of col- look for recurring themes across reviewers. Studying the
leagues contain few substantive criticisms and/or related comments as a group may give authors a better
Summers / RESEARCH IN MARKETING 413

understanding of the underlying problem and lead to a Lack of Theory-Building Research


stronger paper than would a piecemeal approach. More-
over, any shortcomings that are noted by more than one Marketing researchers have devoted little attention to
reviewer deserve special attention. theory-building research. It is difficult to think of many
Authors should try to respond to all of the reviewers’ empirical articles in marketing whose primary purpose is
comments in a positive fashion. It is always in the author’s to develop theory as opposed to merely introducing mar-
best interest to set a tone for courtesy when responding to keters to theories developed in other disciplines (e.g., psy-
reviewers. The accepted norm is professionalism and chology and sociology) and/or testing existing theories.
courteousness even when communicating disagreements As a discipline, marketing has become content with bor-
with the reviewers and the editor. rowing theory from other disciplines. Several factors may
contribute to this situation. First, most of our doctoral pro-
After making the necessary revisions to their manu- grams do not do a good job of teaching the qualitative
scripts and formulating their responses to the reviewers, research methods (e.g., conducting field interviews and
authors should prepare a thorough set of revision notes that case studies) that are essential to developing grounded the-
address both the major themes included in each review and ory.2 Many doctoral programs devote very little time to
the reviewers’ individual comments. A separate set of these methods even though one could argue that rigorous
responses should be prepared for each reviewer. The revi- qualitative research is more difficult to conduct, analyze,
sion notes are easiest for reviewers to follow when each of and report than are surveys or experiments. As a result,
their individual comments is followed by the authors’ most graduates are not skilled at theory-building research.
detailed responses. Second, many in our discipline appear to believe that qual-
itative research is inherently not as rigorous or prestigious
as quantitative research (e.g., surveys and experiments)
THE STATE OF RESEARCH IN MARKETING:
SOME PERSONAL OBSERVATIONS and, therefore, the results are difficult to publish. This
belief seems to be reinforced by the fact that few doctoral
dissertations are based on qualitative research, and one
While it is easy for an experienced reviewer to be criti- seldom sees a rigorous qualitative research study pub-
cal of any study, research in marketing has greatly lished in any of the leading research journals in marketing.
improved during the past two decades. Researchers are It may also be due, in part, to the negative reactions of
giving increased attention to providing a solid theoretical some researchers to those qualitative researchers who
base for their studies. Theories developed in other disci- seem to feel that their research findings do not need to be
plines have been widely used for this purpose. Purely objectively verifiable. For too many of the qualitative stud-
descriptive studies have all but disappeared. More thought ies published in the past two decades, it is difficult, if not
is also being given to how a given study fits into the exist- impossible, for other researchers to determine whether the
ing literature and what contribution it makes. Because authors’ conclusions are adequately supported by the data
today’s research studies are more theory based and tightly collected and/or to replicate the authors’ findings.
linked to the literature, the results of these individual stud-
ies are more easily generalized to other contexts.
Psychometric Properties of Measures
Today’s quantitative studies are more rigorously
designed than past research. More attention is being given The vast majority of authors’ claims regarding the con-
to the development and/or use of multiple-item measures vergent validity of their measures are unwarranted (i.e.,
of the central constructs and to providing evidence regard- maximally different methods of measurement are rarely
ing the psychometric properties of the measures used in used), tests for discriminant validity are typically very
the study, primarily internal-consistency measures of reli- weak, and test-retest reliability is rarely examined.3
ability (e.g., coefficient α). Greater attention is being paid Although authors often claim to have provided evidence
to selecting subjects that are appropriate for the research regarding the convergent validity of their measures, it is
question of interest. There is less reliance on college usually the case that they use the same interitem correla-
undergraduate student samples. Finally, the results of tions as evidence of both reliability and convergent valid-
today’s studies are less open to alternative interpretations ity. Furthermore, in many studies, it appears that the
than past studies. researchers have sacrificed the content validity of some of
However, there are areas that are in need of improve- their measures by deleting items in their initial scales to
ment. These include (1) theory building research; (2) claims develop unidimensional scales.4 Often, the remaining
regarding convergent and discriminant validity; (3) use of items reflect a much narrower construct than that origi-
single-source, self-report data; and (4) experimental nally contemplated. Researchers need to give more con-
realism. sideration to using formative scales (i.e., scales for which
414 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

the observed measures are considered to form the abstract cipline. Without them, no top research journal could oper-
unobserved construct) in those situations where attempts ate. Most reviewers are among the most prolific authors in
to develop unidimensional reflective scales (i.e., scales the field. They serve as reviewers because they want to
whose item scores are considered to be caused by, or help the discipline advance, because they feel they owe it
reflective of, the construct of interest) fail to result in mea- to their discipline, because of the prestige of being a mem-
sures with acceptable content validity. When this occurs, it ber of an editorial board, and/or because they enjoy the re-
is often the case that the construct is composed of several viewing process. How reviewers perform their jobs has a
different aspects or dimensions that are not highly huge impact on how manageable editors’positions and au-
correlated. thors’ tasks are likely to be. Below are some guidelines for
reviewers that help editors and/or authors fulfill their re-
Single-Source Self-Report Data sponsibilities.

A long-standing issue regarding studies employing sur- 1. Clearly identify all of the major problems with
veys is that many involve self-reports and/or key-infor- the manuscript that are within the reviewer’s ar-
mant reports from a single source.5 Data are never col- eas of expertise. Reviewers should avoid taking
lected from any other source, and the survey respondents strong positions on issues that are not within
their areas of expertise.
provide measures for both the independent and the
2. When making global evaluations (e.g., the writ-
dependent variables. The single-source issue is less of a ing is unclear, the theoretical rationale for the
concern when several of the variables are objective and/or hypotheses are weak, etc.), provide specific ex-
factual in nature (e.g., the respondent’s age and corporate amples supporting these evaluations.
profits as a percentage of sales) and, therefore, more likely 3. Indicate which problems are major and which
to be independently verifiable from other sources. How- are minor.
ever, when most or all of the measures involve summary 4. Indicate which flaws appear to be correctable
judgments of an attitudinal or perceptual nature, common and which are not.
method variance becomes a serious concern in interpreting 5. For correctable flaws, indicate what might be
the correlations between these measures. Another related done to fix them.
problem with single-source data involving self-reports 6. For uncorrectable flaws, indicate which should
be discussed in the Limitations section.
and/or key informants relates to the consistency motif. A
7. If the manuscript is considered to be potentially
great deal of past research on cognition and attitudes has publishable with revisions, clearly indicate what
shown that respondents have an urge to provide answers must be done to make the article acceptable.
that they feel are logically consistent. This creates prob- 8. When recommending rejection of an article,
lems because respondents will often have lay theories of specify the specific reasons (e.g., uncorrectable
how the variables of interest should be related. flaws). Provide a convincing argument as to why
these flaws justify rejecting the manuscript.
Experimental Realism 9. Be tactful in writing the Comments to the Au-
thors. Start these comments with some positive
statements about the manuscript. Avoid making
Perhaps the most frequent and serious problem with personal comments and using words with nega-
experiments in marketing is the lack of experimental real- tive connotations (e.g., naive and hopelessly
ism (i.e., the degree to which the experiment involves the confused).
participants, forces them to take it seriously, and has an 10. When not too time-consuming, direct the au-
impact on them). 6 Experiments that ask the participants to thors to articles or books that may be useful to
role-play without previously having had similar task- them in revising their manuscripts and/or de-
related experiences and/or for which there are no meaning- signing their next study. For example, if the the-
ful consequences for the participant tend to lack experi- oretical rationale provided for a hypothesis is
mental realism. In these situations, the respondents are most weak, cite previous research that might help the
likely to tell the experimenter what they feel is a reason- authors develop stronger rationale.
11. Avoid suggesting that the authors cite literature
able response. Unfortunately, participants are not always
that is only loosely related to the research issues
able to predict how they would behave in a given situation. of interest.
12. Avoid asking the authors to cite the reviewer’s
articles unless they are central to the research.
REVIEWING FOR SCHOLARLY 13. Be open to alternative paradigms for studying
JOURNALS IN MARKETING the research questions of interest.
14. Allow the authors some flexibility to write the
Although they are frequently the targets of authors’ an- article they want to write.
ger, reviewers provide an indispensable service to the dis- 15. Provide timely reviews (i.e., within 30 days).
Summers / RESEARCH IN MARKETING 415

SUMMARY sample text appropriate for a wide range of studies. This basic approach
can and should be used with other particularly well-written articles.
2. For an excellent discussion of building theories from case study re-
A major key to getting one’s research accepted for pub- search, see Eisenhardt (1989).
lication and dissemination in a leading journal is paying 3. For the most authoritative treatments of convergent and
careful attention to doing the best job possible at every step discriminant validity, see Campbell and Fisk (1959) and Campbell
of the research and publication process, starting with (1960).
developing the research idea through preparing the final 4. For an authoritative discussion of content validity, see Cronbach
(1971).
revision of the manuscript. The success of each step is 5. For an excellent discussion of the problems associated with single-
dependent on the steps that preceded it (e.g., it is impossi- source, self-report data, see Podsakoff and Organ (1986).
ble to develop valid measures of constructs without having 6. For an authoritative discussion of experimental realism, see
developed precise conceptual definitions of these con- Aronson and Carlsmith (1968).
structs). Hence, it is important for researchers to check the
adequacy of each completed aspect of their studies before
proceeding to the next stage. Too frequently, researchers REFERENCES
do not seek feedback from their colleagues until they have Aronson, Elliot and J. Merrill Carlsmith. 1968. “Experimentation in So-
written the first draft of their manuscript. Moreover, feed- cial Psychology.” In The Handbook of Social Psychology. 2nd ed.
back is only helpful when it is solicited from those with Vol. 2. Eds. Gardner Lindzey and Elliot Aronson. Reading, MA: Ad-
dison-Wesley, 1-79.
high levels of expertise, those providing the feedback are
Campbell, Donald. 1960. “Recommendations for APA Test Standards
motivated to be highly critical, and those receiving the Regarding Construct, Trait, or Discriminant Validity.” American Psy-
feedback are receptive to constructive criticism. Being chologist 15 (August): 546-553.
responsive to criticism is especially critical when going  and Donald W. Fisk. 1959. “Convergent and Discriminant Vali-
dation by the Multitrait-Multimethod Matrix.” Psychological Bulle-
through the review process at a major journal. Not infre- tin 56 (March): 81-105.
quently, a publishable study never gets in print because the Churchill, Gilbert A., Jr. and William D. Perreault, Jr. 1982. “JMR Edito-
author chooses to argue with the reviewers, ignores the rial Policies and Philosophy.” Journal of Marketing Research 19 (Au-
gust): 283-287.
reviewers’ comments, and/or otherwise fails to adequately Cronbach, L. J. 1971. “Test Validation.” In Educational Measurement. 2d
address the reviewers’ and editor’s concerns and incorpo- ed. Ed. R. L. Thorndike. Washington, DC: American Council on Edu-
rate their suggestions in the revised manuscript. cation, 443-507.
Eisenhardt, Kathleen M. 1989. “Building Theories From Case Study Re-
Research in marketing has improved greatly both con- search.” Academy of Management Review 14 (4): 532-550.
ceptually and methodologically during the past quarter Kohli, Ajay K. 1985. “Some Unexplored Supervisory Behaviors and
century. However, much remains to be done. Theory- Their Influence on Salespeople’s Role Clarity, Specific Self-Esteem,
Job Satisfaction, and Motivation.” Journal of Marketing Research 22
building research is lacking in marketing. Survey (November): 424-433.
researchers should reduce their reliance on single-source, Markland, Murry F. 1983. “Taking Criticism—And Using It.” Scholarly
self-report data and use maximally different methods Publishing: A Journal for Authors and Publishers 14 (February):
139-147.
when trying to assess convergent validity. Finally, experi- Podsakoff, Philip M. and Dennis W. Organ. 1986. “Self-Reports in Orga-
menters need to be more concerned with the experimental nizational Research: Problems and Prospects.” Journal of Manage-
realism of their studies. ment 12 (4): 531-544.
Varadarajan, P. Rajan. 1996. “From the Editor: Reflections on Research
and Publishing.” Journal of Marketing 60 (October): 3-6.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
ABOUT THE AUTHOR
The author thanks A. Parasuraman, Thomas Hustad,
Scott MacKenzie, Cheryl Jarvis, and the editor for their John O. Summers (Ph.D., Purdue University, 1968) is a profes-
constructive comments on previous drafts of this article. sor of marketing in the Kelley School of Business at Indiana Uni-
versity. His work has appeared in the Journal of Marketing
Research, the Journal of Marketing, the Journal of Consumer
NOTES Research, the Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, the
Journal of Business Research, the Journal of Business Adminis-
1. This sample text is based on material found in Kohli (1985), as are tration, and the Journal of Advertising Research. He served on
most all sample texts presented in this section. Basically, the verbiage the Editorial Review Board of the Journal of Marketing Research
specific to Kohli’s study was stripped from Kohli’s article to provide a from 1972 through 1998.
JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY
REVIEWSOF
OFMARKETING
BOOKS SCIENCE FALL 2001

Reviews of Books
Peggy Cunningham, Editor
Queen’s University, pcunningham@business.queensu.ca

This edition of JAMS features reviews of five eclectic ivity, Economic Growth. In this book, Hunt synthesizes
but important books that may represent must-reads for extant theories of competition to formulate what he terms
marketing academics, practitioners, and graduate stu- the resource-advantage theory of competition.
dents. The section begins with reviews of two books that I hope you enjoy reading these insightful reviews and
address questions of the social responsibility of business. the books they cover. While many of the books reviewed in
First is Jay M. Handleman’s review of the Handbook of this section in the past have been written by American
Marketing and Society edited by Paul N. Bloom and Greg- authors, we hope to have books written by authors from
ory T. Gundlach. This is an important collection of articles other countries included in this section soon. Thus, I
for those interested in interplay between marketing and encourage you to submit suggestions for books to be
society. Articles by top scholars in the field clearly show included in the future in this section. I also invite you to
how marketing can affect various aspects of consumer contact me if you are interested in reviewing a text in your
welfare. Next is Edwin R. Stafford and Cathy L. particular area of expertise.
Hartman’s review of NGOs Engaging With Business: A
World of Difference and a Difference to the World, written
by Simon Heap. This book shows how nongovernment Handbook of Marketing and Society
organizations (NGOs) are collaborating with the global
business community to address social and environmental Volume Editors: Paul N. Bloom and Gregory T.
problems through environmentally sustainable practices Gundlach
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, 2001, 543 pages, $94.95
and ethical codes of conduct.
(hardcover)
Marketing has long adopted and adapted theories and
methods originating in other disciplines. Alf H. Walle III’s It is hard not to notice the heightened interest by mar-
book, Rethinking Marketing: Qualitative Strategies and keting scholars and practitioners in the interplay between
Exotic Visions, is a new addition to this tradition. It is marketing and society. This handbook’s compilation of
reviewed by June Cotte. Both the author and the reviewer articles by some of the field’s top scholars represents a
stress that while new methods and insights can be gained timely effort by editors Paul N. Bloom and Gregory T.
from the humanities, simple appropriation of the methods Gundlach to provide a venue through which readers can
without true mastery is dangerous. assess where the field has been and where it is going. The
The last two books reviewed in this volume come from editors have organized the collection of articles around an
two marketing titans—Philip Kotler and Shelby D. Hunt. easy-to-follow framework that represents their view of
how marketing can affect various aspects of consumer
As A. Coskun Samli notes in his review of Kotler on Mar-
welfare. Their framework represents an effective structure
keting: How to Create, Win and Dominate Markets, Philip around which the literature within this growing field can
Kotler needs no introduction to either marketing practitio- be organized.
ners or marketing academics. Kotler on Marketing is much There are, no doubt, many challenges that the editors
more than an abbreviated version of Marketing Manage- and contributors faced in putting together this volume of
ment. It opens up new horizons in teaching and practicing work. However, there are two challenges that particularly
marketing. Robert A. Peterson and Ashutosh Prasad took stand out in my mind. First, the intended audience for this
on the task of reviewing Shelby D. Hunt’s A General The- handbook is quite diverse. Marketing practitioners, public
ory of Competition: Resources, Competences, Product- policymakers and regulators, and marketing scholars all
represent important potential target audiences for this
handbook. Providing a collection of work that all audi-
Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science.
Volume 29, No. 4, pages 416-423. ences will find valuable is a daunting task. The second
Copyright © 2001 by Academy of Marketing Science. challenge lies in attempting to do justice to the diversity of
REVIEWS OF BOOKS 417

scholarship that examines marketing’s impact on societal and 14) useful in detailing the role of such research and the
well-being. For me, this handbook succeeds on the first potential “land mines” that researchers might face if cross-
challenge but falls short on the second. examined.
First, dealing with the challenge of a diverse range of One area in which the handbook falls short, however, is
target audiences, this handbook provides a series of well- in its relatively narrow representation of what is otherwise
written articles that almost anyone who is interested in a wonderfully diverse field of scholarship. This handbook
understanding the interplay between marketing and soci- affords some issues numerous chapters that provide seem-
ety will find of at least some value. For example, the public ingly infinite refinement to the given topic, while other
policy practitioner will find this handbook useful with a important issues are either glossed over or not given a
number of chapters related to public policy and marketing voice at all.
issues. As a brief illustration of this point, chapter 2 by Nearly three-quarters of the chapters of this handbook
Gregory Gundlach is useful in thinking about how market- can be classified as dealing with primarily two issues. The
ing thought and research can be applied to consumer pro- first are regulatory issues such as antitrust, consumer pro-
tection and antitrust issues. Chapter 3, by Paul Bloom, tection, deceptive advertising regulations, product safety
Julie Edell, and Richard Staelin, is an excellent follow-up regulations, and other governmental remedies. The second
article that provides a useful set of criteria by which public is the effect of some specific (advertising, pricing, fran-
policymakers and regulators can assess the application of chising) or general aspect of marketing strategy on con-
academic research to regulatory and public policy deci- sumer welfare defined primarily from an economic
sions. Corporate marketers interested in public policy orientation.
issues will find Mary Jane Sheffet’s essay (chapter 6) Three very important topic areas in this field are only
important in thinking about antitrust issues in the context afforded the remaining one quarter of the handbook—
of developing marketing strategy. three chapters on social marketing, two chapters related to
For marketing academics, this handbook provides two corporate social responsibility, and only one chapter on
primary benefits. First, there are a number of chapters that consumer resistance (boycotting). Such a distribution of
provide a preliminary empirical examination (usually by chapters leaves three important gaps. First, social market-
way of descriptive statistics) and a literature review that ing and corporate social responsibility, which arguably
gives a solid grounding of the current state of knowledge represent two of the greatest growth areas of the field, are
for a given topic. For example, researchers interested in given very little attention, particularly when compared to
examining the effects of warnings such as product hazard issues such as warning labels, for instance. A second gap
warnings (chapter 15), food labels (chapter 16), and envi- relates to our understanding of “societal welfare.” While
ronmental claims (chapter 17) will find good literature understanding societal welfare from the marketing per-
reviews and preliminary descriptive statistics on these spective is a primary motivation underlying much of the
issues. Brenda Derby and Alan Levy (chapter 16) present work in the field, the reader is left without much of a sense
some preliminary descriptive statistics on the consumer of what is meant by this construct beyond the relatively
and marketplace impacts from the Nutrition Labeling and narrow definitions of consumer welfare defined primarily
Education Act that may provide the basis for more exten- from an economic perspective. This shortcoming directly
sive academic research. Robert Mayer, Linda Lewis, and stems from the third gap being that important streams of
Debra Scammon (chapter 17) provide a superb review of literature within the marketing and consumer behavior
the issues surrounding environmental marketing claims disciplines that are directly related to marketing and soci-
and then lay out the basis for interesting future research ety are given either no or very limited attention. In particu-
directions. Identifying future research directions repre- lar, critical theory (cf. Murray and Ozanne 1991) and
sents the second main benefit of this handbook for market- subsequent applications of this theoretical framework
ing academics. For example, Andrew Abela and Paul have been given no attention, even though such research
Farris (chapter 9) provide an excellent review of studies directly considers the interplay between marketing and
that have examined the impact of advertising on competi- aspects of societal welfare that go beyond consumer wel-
tion, prices, and consumer welfare. The authors clearly fare defined from the economic perspective. Furthermore,
outline the conflicting results from previous work on this the stream of research that examines various aspects of
topic, lay out some solid assumptions on which to conduct consumer resistance to marketing practice (cf. Klein 2000;
future research, and then provide specific research direc- Penaloza and Price 1993—as a small sample) is given no
tions for those interested in examining this topic. Most of attention except for the essay by Craig Smith (chapter 7)
the chapters in this handbook provide a section on future that examines consumer boycotts. The absence of atten-
research. tion given to these streams of research not only limits a
Marketing academics may find a couple of other “sec- broadening of our understanding of societal welfare but
ondary” benefits from this handbook. Some of the chap- also casts a blind eye to what represents great areas of
ters, particularly those that detail regulatory issues, may future research potential in the field of marketing and soci-
provide useful definitions and illustrations for classroom ety. The hope is that this edition of the handbook repre-
use. In addition, those researchers who might be called sents Volume 1 of what will be many future volumes that
upon to serve as expert witnesses by regulators and public provide readers with a more complete scope of research
policymakers will find three of the chapters (chapters 1, 2, that captures the interplay between marketing and society.
418 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

REFERENCES draw primarily from depth interviews of key participants.


Methodological detail is kept to a minimum so that the
Klein, Naomi. 2000. No Logo: Taking Aim at the Brand Bullies. Toronto, book will appeal to business and NGO practitioners, as
Canada: Knopf. well as academics and students.
Murray, Jeff B. and Julie L. Ozanne. 1991. “The Critical Imagination:
Emancipatory Interests in Consumer Research.” Journal of Con- Heap describes several win-win motivations driving
sumer Research 18 (2): 129-144. NGO-private sector collaboration. From the NGOs’ per-
Penaloza, Lisa and Linda Price. 1993. “Consumer Resistance: A Concep- spective, NGOs are growing disenchanted with govern-
tual Overview.” In Advances in Consumer Research. Vol. 20. Eds. ments as a provider and enforcer for social, ethical, and
Leigh McAlister and Michael L. Rothschild. Provo, UT: Association
for Consumer Research, 123-128.
environmental solutions to global business challenges.
NGOs see the power of markets and corporate access to
Jay M. Handelman supply chains and distribution channels to facilitate
McGill University change. As such, NGOs have added market-oriented tac-
tics, such as product endorsements and technology advo-
cacy to their arsenals of traditional protest and activism
strategies. The case histories illustrate the advantages and
NGOs Engaging With Business: A World of disadvantages of different tactics. Although the private
Difference and a Difference to the World sector has been slower to recognize the opportunities,
By Simon Heap businesses engaged in constructive relationships with
Oxford, UK: INTRAC, 2000, 309 pages, £15.95 NGOs can head off negative public confrontations, and
(paper) corporate social and environmental initiatives derived
from NGO collaboration enjoy enhanced public credibil-
ity. Heap describes the interesting case of Greenpeace’s
Increasingly, market globalization is shifting the bal-
endorsement and collaborative marketing of “green-
ance of power from national governments to multinational
freeze,” an ozone- and climate-safe refrigerant that helped
corporations. Because global businesses are able to oper-
ate and move people, capital, resources, and information German appliance manufacturers establish the technology
efficiently around the world, their collective influence on as an industry standard throughout Europe. Manufacturers
people’s lives and the planet requires an increased obliga- tapped into Greenpeace’s influential networks of support-
tion for social and environmental responsibility. This trend ers, consumers, scientists, government agencies, develop-
is changing how development, human rights, and environ- ment organizations, and other stakeholders to advance the
mental nongovernmental organizations (NGOs) address technology. Greenpeace even helped to broker joint ven-
the world’s social and environmental problems. NGOs are tures between its corporate partners and factories in devel-
recognizing the opportunities for leveraging the global oping nations to further diffuse greenfreeze. In short,
business community as collaborative partners rather than NGO–private sector collaboration can facilitate socially
as adversaries to advance environmentally sustainable and environmentally responsible global development to
practices and ethical codes of corporate conduct. As achieve both market and socioenvironmental goals.
NGOs pursue strategic relationships with the private sec- In practice, however, NGO-business partnerships face
tor, however, their new business development models and significant challenges, particularly in integrating profit
collaborative implications for industry are largely and socioenvironmental values (Crane 1998). Heap draws
underresearched (for exceptions in marketing, see the following conclusion from his analysis of a product en-
Drumwright, Cunningham, and Berger 2000; Milne, Iyer, dorsement partnership between the Rainforest Alliance
and Gooding-Williams 1996). Simon Heap’s book and Chiquita Brands in Cost Rica: while product endorse-
attempts to fill this gap in the literature by examining the ments provide a valuable signal to consumers who lack
changing nature of NGO–private sector relationships and environmental expertise, they can open the NGO to criti-
detailing several varied case histories of NGO–private sec- cism from stakeholders who are not involved in the en-
tor partnerships. Heap’s well-written book delivers much dorsement process and whose interests are not addressed
of what it promises. properly.
A historian by training, Heap is a researcher for the
International Non-Governmental Organisation Training If the NGO has a narrow set of objectives which do
and Research Centre (INTRAC) based in Oxford, United not cover all the major issues related to the product
Kingdom. His book is the fruit of a two-phased investiga- or service sector in question, then working on a one-
tion. The first was to explore and identify issues relevant to to-one basis is very risky and likely to prove an inef-
the emerging dynamics between NGOs and private sec- ficient programme in the long run. (P. 189)
tors, and the second was to study those issues empirically
across several NGO-business partnership case studies Close corporate ties can threaten NGO credibility should
conducted by INTRAC. Following this pattern, the first the partnership fail to meet its social or environmental
half of the book provides a comprehensive review of objectives (cf. Westley and Vredenburg 1991).
emerging literature on NGO-business collaboration, and For businesses, NGO collaboration can make corpora-
the second half details INTRAC’s case studies, which tions vulnerable when their operations and practices are
REVIEWS OF BOOKS 419

scrutinized by NGO activists. Most NGOs use both coop- Rowell 1996). Some research suggests that collaboration
erative “carrot” and traditional adversarial “stick” can only work when corporations perceive a credible
approaches in their advocacy campaigns, and NGOs are threat of confrontation should the business fail to act
not averse to working with companies on one issue and responsibly (cf. Harrison 1999).
protesting the same companies on another issue simulta- Indeed, marketing researchers should find the growing
neously. Partnering NGOs and businesses need to learn to trend of NGO-business collaboration as an important con-
“agree to disagree” when engaged in ongoing relation- text with which to investigate relationships, social market-
ships. Heap quotes Milliman, Clair, and Mitroff (1994) to ing, and image management. Given the depth and clarity
summarize how business managers and NGO campaign- of Heap’s case descriptions, marketing academics will
ers need to view one another in collaboration: “For corpo- find his book to be a useful starting point to explore how
rate officials, this means trying to understand the NGO-private sector relationships are affecting marketing
emotional and spiritual views of [NGOs]. For [NGOs], this practice.
means having the willingness to temper their idealism and In short, we would categorize the book as a good refer-
moral convictions to pursue cooperative, often compro- ence tracing the increasing demands for global corporate
mised, solutions” (p. 42). social responsibility and how NGOs are responding to the
Heap finds that environmental NGOs appear to be fur- trend. Relationship marketing scholars, in particular, may
ther along the learning curve with corporate collaboration find the book worthwhile for stimulating thought on how
than their human rights and development NGOs. This is to extend the concepts of trust, commitment, compatibil-
largely because industry is increasingly recognizing that ity, and conflict resolution into the unique context of
its long-term future is dependent on environmental and NGO–business collaboration. The book may be most suit-
resource sustainability; by contrast, industry has yet to able as a supplementary text for graduate-level courses
consider its long-term market or financial sustainability as related to sociology, political science, corporate environ-
being threatened by social inequity and domestic civil mentalism, stakeholder management, and natural resource
society. As such, while many NGOs may desire to work and environmental policy. Marketing instructors, how-
with the private sector, social- and development-oriented ever, may find the book’s illustrative case examples of
NGOs have more difficulty convincing businesses that NGO–business relationships useful for embellishing lec-
they are worthwhile partners. tures and class discussions on social marketing, ethics, and
The book’s key strengths lay in its chapters that over- globalization.
view past research and detail INTRAC’s primary research
of various NGO-business partnerships. Heap organizes
key managerial implications from past research for select-
REFERENCES
ing partners, managing relations, and strategically posi-
tioning such partnerships among stakeholders and broader
society. The descriptions and analyses of INTRAC’s pri- Crane, Andrew. 1998. “Exploring Green Alliances.” Journal of Mar-
keting Management 14 (August): 559-579.
mary research cover five NGO-private sector relationships
involving sustainable business practices (e.g., the Marine Drumwright, Minette E., Peggy H. Cunningham, and Ida E. Berger.
2000. “Social Alliances: Company/Nonprofit Collaboration.” Report
Stewardship Council) and Third World development (e.g., No. 00-101. Marketing Science Institute, Cambridge, MA.
Living Earth and Shell; Rainforest Alliance and Chiquita).
Harrison, Kathryn. 1999. “Talking With the Donkey: Cooperative Ap-
The cases provide good contextual detail to understand proaches to Environmental Protection.” Journal of Industrial Ecol-
each partnership’s processes, challenges, and outcomes. ogy 2 (3): 51-72.
Given the book’s breadth and depth, the four-page con- Livesey, Sharon. 1999. “McDonald’s and the Environmental Defense
clusion and summary chapter misses the opportunity to Fund: A Case Study of a Green Alliance.” Journal of Business Com-
draw significant parallels and themes between INTRAC’s munications 36 (January): 5-39.
detailed cases and research from other scholars to advance Milliman, John, Judith A. Clair, and Ian Mitroff. 1994. “Environmental
an integrated “process model” or a comprehensive set of Groups and Business Organizations: Conflict or Co-Operation?” So-
“lessons learned.” Such a synthesis could have also been ciety for Advanced Management Journal 59 (1991): 41-47.
used as an organizing framework to present the cases and Milne, George R., Easwar S. Iyer, and Sara Gooding-Williams. 1996.
improve the book’s overall contribution to understanding “Environmental Organization Alliance Relationships Within and
Across Nonprofit, Business, and Government Sectors.” Journal of
NGO-business collaboration. Moreover, Heap provides Public Policy and Marketing 15 (2): 203-215.
only brief discussion concerning how collaboration may
Rowell, Andrew. 1996. Green Backlash: Global Subversion of the Envi-
affect or undermine the effectiveness of more traditional ronmental Movement. New York: Routledge.
adversarial strategies used against business by other NGOs
Westley, Frances and Harri Vredenburg. 1991. “Strategic Bridging: The
who have not bought into dialogue or partnership. The Collaboration Between Environmentalists and Business in the Mar-
broader question of whether collaboration is more effec- keting of Green Products.” Journal of Applied Behavioral Science 27
tive than confrontation to bring corporations in line with (March): 65-90.
social responsibility and sustainability is not addressed
fully. Some critics and researchers have charged that Edwin R. Stafford
NGO-business dialogue and collaboration have fallen Cathy L. Hartman
short of their heralded promises (e.g., Livesey 1999; Utah State University
420 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

Rethinking Marketing: Qualitative Strategies literature to “cull wisdom” marketers can use. He links
and Exotic Visions themes in The Aeneid and The Virginian to modern adver-
tising and uses the New Testament to demonstrate local-
By Alf H. Walle III ized marketing strategies, niche marketing, international
Westport, CT: Quorum Books, 2001, 211 pages, franchises and multinationals, and positioning.
$67.50 In the second part of the book, Walle starts to point the
reader more toward the source material in the social sci-
The central question Walle addresses in his book is, ences and humanities. He makes the very valid point that
“Do marketers and consumer researchers have anything to some of the approaches he discusses are controversial and
gain from the humanities and qualitative social science?” come with intellectual baggage due to infighting within
Although I thoroughly enjoyed this book (more on that the humanities. (If any interested reader wants to see some
later), this question is immaterial. Most consumer examples of this infighting, simply check out Camille
researchers have already answered this question with a Paglia’s columns in Salon.com—read any reference to
resounding “yes, of course!” Evidence that they have is Foucault and you’ll see what I mean.) For an example of
clear in the Journal of Consumer Research, as well as else- the format of the book, consider the following. Walle fol-
where (e.g., Sherry 1995; Stern 1990, 1992). The con- lows the development of literary criticism from Aristotle
sumer researchers who answer “no, there is nothing to Freud to New Criticism and neo-Freudian Leslie
whatsoever to be gained from the humanities,” if indeed Fielder. He does this to set up his analysis of the Joe Camel
any of them still exist, would be hard-pressed to find ratio- character, which takes the form of compare/contrast/
nal reasons to justify their answers. Many researchers who destroy Derrida’s deconstructionist method (as practiced
would never use humanities perspectives or methods by Barbara Stern) to his own structural “myth and symbol”
likely still can see the value of more, and diverse, sources approach. (Although he stays relatively polite, it is obvi-
of information on consumers and marketing strategy. ous that Walle harbors little or no respect for Stern’s work;
Marketing managers may be a different story, but if the if you have a morbid, voyeuristic fascination with intellec-
plethora of media stories on the increasing use of anthro- tual carnage, be certain to read chapter 6.) Other methods
pological and other “qualitative social sciences” methods and approaches that Walle discusses include “American
within businesses is an indication, I doubt that they need Studies,” and Walle also makes an impassioned plea for
much convincing either (e.g., “Sending Ethnographers” more attention to be paid to Edward T. Hall’s work in lin-
2000; “Storytelling” 1997). Walle’s stated goal of inter- guistic anthropology. Because Hall’s work is the basis for
preting humanistic and qualitative social sciences perspec- some of my own work, I wholeheartedly concur.
tives so that managers can use them is laudable, although A main theme throughout the book is that marketers
perhaps slightly condescending. and consumer researchers should look to the humanities
Having said all that, let me explain why I did enjoy the for new methods and new insights. However, Walle cau-
book. Or perhaps I won’t yet, because I haven’t yet dealt tions that a simple appropriation of the methods without
with the book’s title. The title does an extreme disservice true mastery is dangerous. In demonstrating this point, he
to the book itself. First, the main “Rethinking Marketing” takes on the work of Barbara Stern and describes Morris
conjures up an image of a critical perspective on the field Holbrook’s (1997) analysis of Edward Bellamy’s work as
of marketing, which this book is not. In fact, my impres- fundamentally flawed—“The basic premise of this chap-
sion from this main title was that the book would point out ter is that Holbrook significantly misread Bellamy
all of the problems with marketing as a discipline, perhaps because he lacked substantive knowledge of the literary
with the goal of debunking some of the central tenets of heritage of the late nineteenth century; therefore, his work
current marketing thought, à la Stephen Brown (1995). is compromised” (p. 132). Personally, I have never seen
(This anticipation is partly why I initially agreed to review Morris Holbrook described in print as lacking in knowl-
Walle’s book.) It doesn’t. In fact, this book reaches into edge—I expect Walle should expect a lively reaction from
history and literature to show the seemingly universal Holbrook himself. Walle’s analysis of Bellamy shows par-
applicability of core marketing strategy issues such as tar- allels between nineteenth-century utopian literature and
geting, segmentation, globalized versus localized strate- George Fisk’s writings in marketing. Although I under-
gies, and so on to “more firmly establish links between the stand why Walle cautions us against uninformed appropri-
humanities/qualitative social sciences and the strategic ation, the somewhat depressing conclusion appears to be
orientation of contemporary marketing” (p. ix). Thus, the that unless you already have a Ph.D. in the humanities or
“rethinking marketing” in the title could more accurately are willing to spend years of your life retraining yourself,
be called “buttressing marketing.” A second disappoint- these methods aren’t for you. I should note that the author
ment in the title is the phrase “qualitative strategies.” Per- never says this, but I drew this conclusion.
haps the reader of this review would expect, as I did, that While pointing out how far he feels we as consumer and
this subtitle indicates the book would offer relatively con- marketing researchers have come, Walle also makes the
crete how-to suggestions for using qualitative methods to point that we have much further to go in our understanding
“rethink marketing.” It doesn’t. It does, however, offer and applications of humanities perspectives and methods.
broad suggestions for the sorts of strategies (in general) At the end of the book, he tries to explain that his aim is not
that could be used. For example, in Part 1, Walle uses a to attack what has already been done by groundbreakers
very broad humanities approach to studying great works in like Stern and Holbrook. However, whenever he does
REVIEWS OF BOOKS 421

mention a specific consumer researcher, he does more than profession. Kotler on Marketing is another notch in that
“point out certain limitations.” These attacks (yes, that’s outstanding tradition.
what I’d call them, contrary to his request that they not be This is a good book—well written and full of ideas. It is
seen as such) detract from the book. Walle could have composed of four parts: strategic marketing, tactical mar-
made his very eloquent argument about the need for eclec- keting, administrative marketing, and transformational
tic but informed use of the humanities in marketing with- marketing.
out the harsh polemical style he uses when talking about The first part is particularly dynamic. It articulates a
existing consumer research. well-known concept by marketing people quite ade-
Pointing out some of the limitations of a book is, I quately. By emphasizing marketing, a company can be
believe, part of the purpose of an informed review. Regard- very profitable. This implies the ability of that company to
less of the objections I have to certain parts of the book, it learn. Perhaps a vice president of learning is in the making.
was an extremely enjoyable read. If there are academicians This position may become more critical in the organiza-
who dread submerging themselves into difficult and com- tional structure than even a chief financial officer. What is
pletely new disciplines without a primer or a guidepost, learned here can be put to work to create and deliver better
then this is the book for you. It stimulates new thinking and consumer value. These values are created by “winning
points out “the way in,” while cautioning about the need practices.” These must be put in proper segment-position
for a much fuller immersion in the basic disciplines. The combinations. All of these identify where the company
analogy I drew is that this is like a map of a forest wilder- can create more value and can reflect this value in its brand
ness area without details like rivers and mountains. It equity position.
shows only the borders of the area, and outlines them very Part 2 gets down to more specific areas of market intel-
well, but does not provide the detail needed to survive in ligence, development, designing marketing mixes, creat-
the forest. The book is (I think) written for marketing man- ing customer loyalty, and delivering customer value,
agers; it is very accessible to any reader, but I do think that which in reality distinguishes marketing from selling and
the abstract nature and far-reaching implications of the makes it the essential competitive force in a market
book are likely more interesting to marketing academics. economy.
Having a taste for reading widely across disciplines Part 3 dwells upon administration or implementation of
myself, I highly recommend this “quick read” to others. marketing plans. Two very key concepts are essential here.
After all, who among the eclectic readers out there could First, marketing plans must be developed. There are a
fail to be intrigued by a book that links Milton Friedman
number of those, such as brand plans, product category
with Friedrich Nietzsche, interprets their work using the
plans, new product plans, market segment plans, and oth-
writings of William James, and that contains a section
ers. These marketing plans must have five key compo-
titled “Witches, Whores, and Ethics: Illusions or Reality?”
nents: situation analysis, marketing objectives and goals,
marketing strategy, marketing action plan, and marketing
REFERENCES controls. Second, these plans must be implemented by the
marketing organization. This organization is also in charge
Brown, Stephen. 1995. Postmodern Marketing. London: Routledge. of evaluation and control. Again, this is a good section
“Sending Ethnographers into New-SKU Jungle.” 2000. Brandweek, Sep- even though it deals mostly with the conventional wisdom.
tember 25, p. 32. Finally, transformational marketing (a new and a some-
Sherry, John F. 1995. “Marketing and Consumer Behavior: Into the what nebulous concept since marketing by definition is
Field.” In Contemporary Marketing and Consumer Behavior. Ed.
John F. Sherry. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, 3-44. transformational) is the last section that deals with primar-
Stern, Barbara B. 1990. “Literary Criticism and the History of Marketing ily electronic marketing, which in some ways is an oxymo-
Thought: A New Perspective on ‘Reading’ Marketing Thought.” ron in the sense that marketing is a personal value
Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science 18 (4): 329-336. generation, whereas electronic marketing at this point in
. 1992. “Historical and Personal Nostalgia in Advertising Text: time is more like communicating with the markets in an
The Fin de Siècle Effect.” Journal of Advertising 21 (4): 11-22.
“Storytelling: A New Way to Get Close to Your Customer.” 1997. For- impersonal or dehumanized manner. Granted, e-com-
tune, February 3, p. 102. merce has an important role to play; however, it is not quite
clear how it is generating additional consumer value.
June Cotte As for me, the first three parts are more interesting and
University of Western Ontario present more depth. The last section should have gone way
beyond the cyberspace marketing. Value marketing, rela-
tionship marketing, and mass customization are all reali-
Kotler on Marketing: How to Create, Win and ties and are likely to change or transform marketing to a
substantial extent.
Dominate Markets In some ways, Kotler on Marketing is a summary of
By Philip Kotler Marketing Management, the millenium edition, but it is a
New York: Free Press, 1999, 257 pages, $27.50 good summary. I certainly would give my right arm if my
weekend MBAs were to read this book before they start the
Philip Kotler single-handedly has done more for mar- course with me. In fact, I think this book was written with
keting education than most of us combined who are in this this market in mind, anyway.
422 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

Over the years, I have approached marketing manage- theory of competition. Specifically, through a series of
ment by analyzing four distinct approaches: negative mar- successive analyses, Hunt has attempted to reduce extant
keting, inactive marketing, reactive marketing, and theories of competition and “traditions,” including neo-
proactive marketing. The book can be placed between classic economic theory, differential advantage theory,
reactive and proactive; however, I consider it to be a little competence-based tradition, and economic sociology, to
bit short on proactivity. To be proactive, the firm must what he terms the resource-advantage theory of competi-
know, must detect, and react early to major market changes tion. Such reductionism, in the words of Causey (1977),
in its markets. There are at least three areas that need to be requires successfully explaining the laws found in the
connected to the contents of this book to make it more extant theories and traditions in terms of the laws found in
proactive. All three are related to markets and market resource-advantage theory. Has Hunt been successful? Is
changes. First, market turbulence or a more extreme ver- A General Theory of Competition a true tour de force, a
sion of it, chaos, necessitates understanding dramatic mar- quixotic attempt to slay neoclassic economics, both, or
ket changes and converting them into possible marketing neither?
opportunities. Thus, early detection of market changes is Shelby Hunt has produced, in A General Theory of
extremely important. Second, learning organizations and Competition, a tightly written (indeed, at times even
knowledge maps. Although at the beginning of the book an dense) tome that is simultaneously interesting, infor-
interesting discussion regarding learning organizations is mative, and important. According to Hunt, the monograph
presented, it does not go far enough as to determining how “develops the structure, foundations, and implications of
the learning and/or knowledge are distributed in the orga- the resource-advantage (‘R-A’) theory of competition.
nization and are connected to the organizational process This new theory, it is argued, is a general, interdisciplinary,
flow. If knowledge is not located in the critical areas where evolutionary, disequilibrium-provoking, process theory of
key decisions are made, the company cannot be quite competition” (p. xii). Much of what is contained in this
proactive. Finally, with the development of more and monograph reflects both a distillation and a synthesis of
more advanced software, data warehousing and data min- more than a dozen articles that Hunt has authored or
ing are critical tools that would facilitate the marketing coauthored during the past few years in journals found in
proactivity. such disparate disciplines as economics, management,
Perhaps one key point that I raise is related to the title of
and, of course, marketing. Thus, the substance of the
the book. Professor Kotler says, “how to create, win and
monograph is neither unique nor novel for those who have
dominate markets.” There is no problem with winning and
followed Hunt’s recent work. Even so, A General Theory
dominating markets, but I maintain that marketers do not
of Competition represents a prodigious scholarly effort,
have enough supernatural powers to create markets (how
one intended, in part, to challenge existing dogma regard-
we wish we did). We can tap into untapped markets, we
can articulate unsatisfied consumer needs, we can address ing “perfect” competition (hence the earlier reference to a
to tacit needs, we can stimulate demand, but we cannot quixotic attempt). Consequently, it is likely to be contro-
create needs and hence, we cannot create markets. The versial and subjected to a plethora of deconstruction as
book may give some marketing managers the wrong idea well as reconstruction endeavors.
that they could single-handedly create markets. A major strength of the book is that it articulates a clear
To summarize, this is a very good book, particularly if interpretation and integration of existing theories of com-
one is not familiar with Kotler’s monumental book, Mar- petition and related traditions. As such, it reminds the
keting Management. It will certainly open up new horizons reader of Hunt’s (1991) Modern Marketing Theory in its
in teaching and practicing marketing. It is only hoped that insightful yet concise treatment of oftentimes complex
it will be read by our colleagues who are not in marketing phenomena. This articulation alone makes A General The-
and know nothing about it but either by teaching or by ory of Competition worthwhile reading. Hunt possesses an
practicing business subjects they are influencing market- enviable knack for coalescing what often seem to be unre-
ing directly or indirectly. lated perspectives into a coherent and parsimonious
whole, and the frequent comparisons made between
A. Coskun Samli resource-advantage theory and extant theories and tradi-
University of North Florida tions are most enlightening (and often persuasive).
Perhaps the main “take-away” from the monograph is
the continually voiced allegation regarding the limitations
and failures of neoclassic economics to accurately account
A General Theory of Competition: Resources, for and predict real-world competition, especially the
Competences, Productivity, Economic Growth disequilibrating nature of competition. Economists and
By Shelby D. Hunt business pundits, though, are increasingly making this al-
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, 2000, 303 pages legation as well. For example, Noble laureate North (1995,
p. 7) opined that
Ceteris paribus, the understanding of any phenomenon
increases as an increasingly general and unifying theory of it is not clear where economics is going. But the di-
it evolves. In A General Theory of Competition, Shelby rection is suggested by two glaring shortcomings of
Hunt has attempted to construct a general and unifying neoclassic theory: it is a frictionless theory in a
REVIEWS OF BOOKS 423

world in which the frictions are where the action is, it possible to define competitors? If a firm obtains new
and it is static in a world in which dynamic change is management or a new name (essentially new resources), is
going on at an unprecedented rate. Remedying these it still the same firm?
defects requires that economics . . . modifies the un- Resource-advantage theory is offered as a general the-
realistic assumptions . . . and incorporates time. ory of competition, with neoclassic economic theory
being a special case. However, since resource-advantage
Moreover, many of the critical notions in the mono- theory seems to reject several tenets of neoclassic eco-
graph, such as the treatment of intangible assets as nomic theory, such as utility maximization by consumers,
resources, are already diffusing into the marketing litera- it is difficult to accept the conclusion that neoclassic eco-
ture, with Srivastava, Shervani, and Fahey’s (1998) nomic theory reduces to resource-advantage theory. The
award-winning conceptualization of market-based assets two theories would intuitively seem to be either incom-
being but one example. mensurate or rivals. In brief, to reiterate Hunt’s closing
By definition, A General Theory of Competition is a statement in the monograph, “There is still a lot of work to
work in progress. Thus, it is appropriate at this time to raise be done—a lot of work” (p. 259).
a few issues that need to be addressed, the most important
of which is whether resource-advantage theory qualifies as
a theory. According to Hunt (1991), a theory is “a system- REFERENCES
atically related set of statements, including some lawlike
generalizations, that is empirically testable” (p. 4). With- Causey, Robert L. 1977. Unity of Science. Boston, MA: D. Reidel.
out question, resource-advantage theory consists of sys- Hunt, Shelby D. 1991. Modern Marketing Theory: Critical Issues in the
tematically related statements, some of which can be Philosophy of Marketing Science. Cincinnati, OH: South-Western.
construed as laws. However, is resource-advantage theory North, Douglass C. 1995. “Economic Theory in a Dynamic Economic
empirically testable? As a general, unifying theory, World.” Business Economics 30 (January): 7-12.
resource advantage is at times necessarily nonspecific and Srivastava, Rajendra K., Tasadduq A. Shervani, and Liam Fahey. 1998.
“Market-Based Assets and Shareholder Value: A Framework for
even unnecessarily vague. (Indeed, chapter 5 can be inter- Analysis.” Journal of Marketing 62 (January): 2-18.
preted as implying that resource-advantage theory can be
broadly considered a theory of human behavior.) What Robert A. Peterson
constitutes, for example, “superior financial performance” University of Texas at Austin
or “constrained self interest seeking,” and how can these
constructs be subjected to rigorous empirical testing? If it Ashutosh Prasad
is not possible to adequately define an industry, how then is University of Texas at Dallas
JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY
MARKETING
OF MARKETING
AND THE LAW
SCIENCE FALL 2001

Marketing and the Law


Ann Morales Olazábal, Anita Cava, and René Sacasas, Editors
University of Miami

Trade Dress Protection Does Not Extend protected without such a filing, as long as it has acquired a
Expired Patent “secondary meaning.” That is, the design serves its pri-
mary purpose but also serves to identify the product with
TrafFix Devices, Inc. v. Marketing Displays, Inc., 121 the manufacturer. Trade dress infringement occurs when
S. Ct. 1255 (2001) another party adopts the same or a similar feature that is
likely to cause confusion for a potential purchaser.
When is it safe to copy an expired patent? Your com- Federal law clearly protects against trade dress
pany has waited a number of years for your competitor’s infringement. The Lanham Act requires the party seeking
profitable patent to expire. As the date draws near, you trade dress protection to establish (1) that the trade dress is
plan your strategy—how to lure customers away from the distinctive, either inherently or because of a “secondary
patent-holding company. You have pondered whether to meaning” in the marketplace; (2) that the trade dress of the
change the design, examined the various methods for mar- two competing products is confusingly similar; and (3) that
keting the item, explored pricing strategies, and attempted the feature of the trade dress is primarily nonfunctional.
to gauge your competitor’s reaction to the patent expira- Because the party claiming trade dress infringement must
tion and your launch of a similar product. prove each of these elements, failure to prove any one of
Once the patent expires, confident you are within your the three will result in a denial of trade dress protection.
rights to copy the patented feature, you begin selling the Whether a manufacturer can obtain protection from
copied product. To your dismay, the holder of the expired competition through a trade dress infringement claim is of
patent sues you for trade dress infringement under the extreme importance. Patents restrict competitors for a lim-
Lanham Act. How can this be? Can the patent holder, in ited time only, while trade dress is protected for as long as
effect, extend the life of the patent by claiming trade dress it is used.
protection? In the TrafFix case, the trial judge held that MDI had not
This is the scenario presented by TrafFix Devices, Inc. v. proven that consumers associated the dual-spring design’s
Marketing Displays, Inc., a recent United States Supreme look with MDI and so had not established the necessary
Court case. Marketing Displays, Inc. (MDI) manufactured “secondary meaning.” The lower court also found that
and marketed sign stands, which were built to stand erect MDI’s spring design would be afforded trade dress protec-
despite adverse wind conditions. The stands used a pat- tion only if the feature were shown to be nonfunctional.
ented dual-spring design visibly situated at the base of the Upon review, the Sixth Circuit Court of Appeals
sign. When the patents expired, TrafFix, a competitor, reversed, observing that in this case, it would take “little
began to sell sign stands with identical spring mecha- imagination” on the part of a competitor to hide the dual-
nisms. MDI sued TrafFix under the Lanham Act, claiming spring mechanism or to add an additional spring or two to
that TrafFix had infringed on its trade dress by copying the avoid infringing on MDI’s trade dress. Basing its holding
visible spring mechanism. MDI asserted that its spring on the availability of alternative designs, the appellate
stand design should be afforded trade dress protection court held that trade dress protection can be denied on
because it was visible and recognizable to buyers and users functionality grounds only when the exclusive use “puts
as being MDI’s product. competitors at a significant non-reputation-related
Trade dress is the total image or appearance of a prod- disadvantage.”
uct that serves to identify the product with its manufacturer The United States Supreme Court disagreed, rejecting
or source. It can include distinctive ways products are this standard. Focusing on the functionality requirement,
packaged, styled, shaped, or even their color. Although the justices unanimously concluded that the two expired
trade dress can be registered as a trademark with the U.S. patents on the dual-spring design were strong evidence
Patent and Trademark Office, the design can still be that MDI’s design is functional and therefore not eligible
for trade dress protection.
Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science. The appropriate way to protect a functional component
Volume 29, No. 4, pages 424-426. is by use of a patent. Congress has legislated a limited
Copyright © 2001 by Academy of Marketing Science. period of exclusivity for patents, and once that time has
MARKETING AND THE LAW 425

expired, the law allows competitors to copy the design. assured that they can copy the feature without fear of trade
This encourages innovation by rewarding the creator with dress infringement lawsuits.
a short-term monopoly to recoup development costs and to
increase profitability. Once the patents expire, further Linda Christiansen
restrictions from competition would impede the free mar- Indiana University Southeast
ket process. Congress did not intend that patent law be cir-
cumvented by the use of trade dress protection provided in
the Lanham Act. The Court pointed out that for it to decide Supreme Court Expands Federal Power to
otherwise would allow companies to use trade dress pro- Regulate the Availability and Use of Data
tection to effectively extend a patent indefinitely, creating
a monopoly situation and unacceptably hindering free Condon v. United States, 528 U.S. 141 (2000)
market competition.
The nonfunctional-feature requirement for trade dress In the era of Internet communication and e-commerce,
protection ensures that it will not be used to extend a pat- privacy of one’s personal data has emerged as a major con-
ent. The Court’s opinion noted that while MDI had relied cern among individuals and government regulators. Even
on the functionality of the design in the past, by this suit it the popular NBC television program The West Wing,
was seeking trade dress protection—which protects non- which has been known for mirroring current political
functional features—after the patent had expired. Not only issues, chimed in on the matter when one of the fictional
did MDI promote the functionality of the dual-spring White House staff members asserted that privacy would be
design as evidence to support the patent application, but the legal issue of the twenty-first century.
MDI also had successfully defended its exclusive right to Against this political and social backdrop, the United
the design under the patent, even when the competitor States Supreme Court recently articulated a broad state-
placed the springs in different positions than those in the ment of authority for Congress to regulate the availability
MDI patent. The focus taken at the time was on the overall and use of personal data. The Court’s decision in Reno v.
function of the springs, not the look of the mechanism. Condon and its potential impact for privacy regulation are
What lessons can a company take away from this case,
discussed herein.
either as the patent holder or the competitor? Advance
The constitutional “right of privacy” in the United
planning and preparation can determine the outcome of a
States does not provide consumers any protection against
trade dress infringement lawsuit. A company that may ulti-
use or abuse of personal data by private sector organiza-
mately claim trade dress infringement can strengthen its
tions. The U.S. Constitution only guarantees that it will
future case in several ways. The firm should seek federal
protect individuals or organizations from government
trademark registration of the trade dress feature/design, if intrusion, not from invasions by private sector parties,
it qualifies. Management should note that trademark regis- such as businesses or nonprofit organizations. Thus, any
tration does not necessarily guarantee trade dress protec- protection individuals or organizations enjoy from each
tion because of the possible differences in interpretations other comes through legislation passed by the states or
of this opinion by the U.S. Patent and Trademark Office as Congress. In the United States, the federal government can
opposed to court interpretations. only regulate those areas of national life that are specifi-
In anticipation of future trade dress battles, careful pat- cally enumerated in the Constitution. One such federal
ent application preparation is also necessary. The manu- enumerated power is the authority to regulate interstate
facturer should include descriptions of functional features commerce.
only, being careful to exclude ornamental, incidental, and Historically, the interstate commerce clause—and
arbitrary features. Instead, these features should be regis- Congress’s power to promulgate legislation thereunder—
tered for trade dress protection. has been interpreted quite broadly by the courts, especially
Moreover, to eventually succeed in demonstrating the in the twentieth century. As a matter of fact, under current
necessary secondary meaning (i.e., acquired distinctive- interpretations, all commercial enterprises, no matter how
ness), producers should regularly collect and preserve evi- small or localized, can be regulated by Congress because
dence to support possible future trade dress infringement they “affect” interstate commerce. Still, the regulated
claims. Consumer surveys, sales data, and advertising activity must be commercial, as opposed to purely charita-
expenses, as well as actual ads are examples of evidence to ble, recreational, or educational. Arguably, however, the
support acquired distinctiveness attributed to the feature in Supreme Court in Condon expressed a broader view of
question. congressional authority to regulate data privacy in tradi-
Finally, manufacturers should remember that even if tionally noncommercial settings.
the feature was not previously patented, a party seeking The genesis of the Condon case is the Driver’s Privacy
trade dress protection also will have to prove the Protection Act (DPPA), a federal law passed by Congress
nonfunctionality of the feature. in 1994. This statute regulates the states’ disclosure of per-
The TrafFix case makes it clear that patent holders can- sonal information contained in motor vehicle department
not extend expired patents by making a trade dress (DMV) records, including the driver’s photograph, social
infringement claim under the Lanham Act. Once the pat- security number, driver identification number, name,
ent on a functional feature expires, competitors can now be address, telephone number, and medical or disability
426 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

information. Prior to the passage of DPPA, such informa- As noted above, the Condon decision was important to
tion that drivers provided to their states to receive a driver’s many interest groups. Presumably, none of these court
license and to register their vehicles was widely sold to watchers anticipated that the Supreme Court would articu-
industry for surveys, marketing, and other purposes. The late a per se congressional power to regulate the use and
Supreme Court noted that, for example, the state of Wis- availability of data. Yet, that is the practical implication of
consin received almost $8 million annually from the sale the Court’s conclusion when it declared the DMV data “an
of this information. article of commerce.” The Court’s description of the DMV
The DPPA restricts the states’ right to sell DMV infor- data and its use by commercial and noncommercial parties
mation without the prior approval of the driver. Such a sys- “in the stream of interstate commerce” seem to apply to
tem of affirmative approval is described as an “opt in” any data that are systematically collected and reused by
system. Under DPPA, the states must ask the subjects if any organization. The commercial nature of the users no
they want their data included in the information disclosed, longer is important.
and the subjects must affirmatively agree. Presumably, in Moreover, the Court’s broad statement of authority
this era of increasing concern over privacy, not many citi- appears not limited to the sale of data, even though that was
zens would agree to this sale of their information. the activity at issue in Condon. For example, the Court
The DPPA does make exceptions for all state use of the stated that these particular data were “released” for mat-
information to carry out governmental functions. The stat- ters related to “interstate motoring.” Here, the Court seems
ute also allows for limited disclosure to industry for pur- to be saying that dissemination of data in relation to any
poses such as product recalls, performance monitoring, activity that can be characterized as “interstate” leads to
and research. The reuse or resale of information received the conclusion that such data are “an article of commerce.”
under these exceptions, however, is restricted. The statute Sale of the information is not required. As noted above,
imposes criminal penalties for intentional violators and even small, local activity has been declared “interstate” in
creates a civil action for drivers whose information is our modern jurisprudence. Regardless of whether the reg-
wrongfully disclosed. ulated entity is commercial, governmental, nonprofit, or
In Condon, the DPPA was challenged by South recreational, it appears Congress now can regulate any and
Carolina because its state disclosure provisions directly all of an entity’s uses of data because information “is an
contravened the federal mandate. South Carolina’s DMV article of commerce.”
records were available to anyone who filled out a request After Condon, the states will continue to be the focus of
form and who confirmed that the records would not be this congressional power. For example, a recent Senate bill
used for telephone solicitations. South Carolina law only proposed a unified tax collection scheme for states to
restricted use of driver information by way of a system impose sales tax obligations on Internet and other remote
commonly described as “opt out,” which requires drivers sellers. In crafting such a uniform state scheme, the bill
to proactively request that their information be excluded requires that the states protect consumer privacy in the tax
from disclosure. collection process. The states have long sought the ability
As the Condon challenge made its way through the to collect state taxes on these remote sales without disputes
courts, it was widely watched by the direct marketing over whether such processes impose undue burdens on
industry because motor vehicle information was com- interstate commerce. Now, as Congress contemplates pav-
monly purchased for targeted solicitations. Industry ing the way for states to accomplish this tax collection
observers were concerned about the impact the federal goal, it may also flex its power to impose on states the obli-
limitations would have on the availability of this oft-used gation to protect the corresponding data that will be gener-
information. State governments and states’ rights advo- ated in the process.
cates were interested in the case because it represented Thus, it appears Congress is taking its federalism vic-
issues of federalism versus states’ rights. tory in Condon and continuing to impose privacy
In deciding Condon, the United States Supreme Court protections on the states. Presumably, new legislation in
pointed out that the relevant data were used by both public all fields, such as education, health care, or “faith-based”
and private parties engaged in interstate commerce. As community services may also include privacy mandates
such, the Court declared this type of information to be “an regarding information obtained in the course of the regu-
article of commerce” and therefore that “its sale or release lated activity. Watch for privacy protection to be a compo-
into the interstate stream of business is sufficient to sup- nent of all new federal regulations.
port congressional regulation.” Thus, the Court held that
the DPPA is constitutional, and it follows that South Rita Marie Cain
Carolina’s or any other state’s sale of driver information University of Missouri at Kansas City
without drivers’ affirmative “opt in” consent violates
DPPA and could subject the states and other users thereof
to sanctions.
JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY
INDEX OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

INDEX

to

JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE

Volume 29

Number 1 (Winter 2001) pp. 1-112


Number 2 (Spring 2001) pp. 113-208
Number 3 (Summer 2001) pp. 209-332
Number 4 (Fall 2001) pp. 333-432
Authors: CARLSON, LES, RUSSELL N. LACZNIAK, and ANN
WALSH, “Socializing Children About Television: An
AULAKH, PREET S., see Sarkar, MB. Intergenerational Study,” 276.
BARRY, ANN MARIE, “How Advertising Works: The CARR, CHRIS, see Tietje, B.
Role of Research, edited by John Philip Jones” [Re- CAVUSGIL, S. TAMER, see Sarkar, MB.
views of Books], 103. CHALLAGALLA, GOUTAM, see Venkatesh, R.
BERTHON, PIERRE, LEYLAND F. PITT, and MICHAEL T.
CHRISTIANSEN, LINDA, “Trade Dress Protection Does
EWING, “Corollaries of the Collective: The Influence
Not Extend Expired Patent” [Marketing and the Law],
of Organizational Culture and Memory Development
424.
on Perceived Decision-Making Context,” 135.
CLARKE, IRVINE, III, and MARGARET OWENS,
BLODGETT, JEFFREY G., LONG-CHUAN LU,
“Internet Marketing: Metatags May Create Trademark
GREGORY M. ROSE, and SCOTT J. VITELL, “Ethi-
Liability” [Marketing and the Law], 204.
cal Sensitivity to Stakeholder Interests: A Cross-Cul-
tural Comparison” [Research Note], 190. COMPEAU, LARRY D., see Nicholson, C. Y.
BRAVO, SANDRA J., “Creative Strategy in Direct Mar- CORNWELL, T. BETTINA, “Advances in International
keting, 2d ed., by Susan K. Jones” [Reviews of Books], Marketing, edited by S. Tamer Cavusgil and Tage Koed
107. Madsen” [Reviews of Books], 318.
BURKE, DEBRA, “Put ‘Tony the Tiger’ in Your Tank?” COTTE, JUNE, “Rethinking Marketing: Qualitative
[Marketing and the Law], 203. Strategies and Exotic Visions, by Alf H. Walle III” [Re-
BURTON, SCOT, see Mason, K. views of Books], 420.
BUSH, VICTORIA D. GREGORY M. ROSE, FAYE COULTER, RON, “High Visibility: The Making and Mar-
GILBERT, and THOMAS N. INGRAM, “Managing keting of Professionals into Celebrities, by Irving Rein,
Culturally Diverse Buyer-Seller Relationships: The Philip Kotler, and Martin Stoller” [Reviews of Books],
Role of Intercultural Disposition and Adaptive Selling 105.
in Developing Intercultural Communication Compe- CRAVENS, DAVID W., see Grant, K.
tence,” 391. CURRAN, CATHARINE M., and MICHAEL R.
CAIN, RITA MARIE, “Supreme Court Expands Federal HYMAN, “Selling to Newly Emerging Markets, by
Power to Regulate the Availability and Use of Data” Russell R. Miller” [Reviews of Books], 324.
[Marketing and the Law], 425. DE R. BARONDES, ROYCE, “Excessive Access to Web
Information Can Be Tortious” [Marketing and the
Law], 327.
Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science.
DEL CASTILLO, DIAMELA, “Rumors & Lies: The Pa-
Volume 29, No. 4, pages 427-430. rameters of Liability for Commercial Speech” [Mar-
Copyright © 2001 by Academy of Marketing Science. keting and the Law], 328.
428 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

DICKSON, PETER R., PAUL W. FARRIS, and WILLEM and Attribute Judgments: The Role of Information Rel-
J.M.I. VERBEKE, “Dynamic Strategic Thinking,” evancy, Product Experience, and Attribute-Relationship
216. Schemata” [Research Note], 307.
ECHAMBADI, RAJ, see Sarkar, MB. MCCUBBINS, TIPTON F., “A Trade Dress Tale: Gang
ESTELAMI, HOOMAN, and DONALD R. LEHMANN, Abducts Princess, Enhances Ransom Through Internet,
“The Impact of Research Design on Consumer Price Court Approves” [Marketing and the Law], 109.
Recall Accuracy: An Integrative Review,” 36. MOHR, JAKKI J., see Low, G. S.
EWING, MICHAEL T., see Berthon, P. MONCRIEF, WILLIAM C., see Grant, K.
FARRIS, PAUL W., see Dickson, P. R. MOORE, MELISSA, see Liddell, P.
GILBERT, FAYE, see Bush, V. D. MOORE, ROBERT, see Liddell, P.
GRANT, KEN, DAVID W. CRAVENS, GEORGE S. NAKATA, CHERYL, and K. SIVAKUMAR, “Instituting
LOW, and WILLIAM C. MONCRIEF, “The Role of the Marketing Concept in a Multinational Setting: The
Satisfaction With Territory Design on the Motivation, Role of National Culture,” 255.
Attitudes, and Work Outcomes of Salespeople,” 165. NICHOLSON, CAROLYN Y., LARRY D. COMPEAU,
HANDELMAN, JAY M., “Handbook of Marketing and and RAJESH SETHI, “The Role of Interpersonal Lik-
Society, edited by Paul N. Bloom and Gregory T. ing in Building Trust in Long-Term Channel Relation-
Gundlach” [Reviews of Books], 416. ships,” 3.
HARTMAN, CATHY L., see Stafford, E. R. OWENS, MARGARET, see Clarke, I.
HENARD, DAVID H., see Szymanski, D. M. PARKER, RICK, “Personalities and Products: A Histori-
HULT, G. THOMAS M., “International Marketing Strat- cal Perspective on Advertising in America, by Edd
egy: Contemporary Readings, by Isobel Doole and Applegate” [Reviews of Books], 102.
Robin Lowe” [Reviews of Books], 322. PARTHASARATHY, MADHAVAN, see Keaveney, S. M.
HUNT, SHELBY D., see Yilmaz, C. PETERSON, ROBERT A., and ASHUTOSH PRASAD,
HYMAN, MICHAEL R., see Curran, C. M. “A General Theory of Competition: Resources, Compe-
INGRAM, THOMAS N., see Bush, V. D. tences, Productivity, Economic Growth, by Shelby D.
JASSAWALLA, AVAN R., see Sashittal, H. C. Hunt” [Reviews of Books], 422.
JENSEN, THOMAS, see Mason, K. PITT, LEYLAND F., see Berthon, P.
K EAV EN EY, SUSAN M., and MADH AVA N PODSAKOFF, PHILIP M., see MacKenzie, S. B.
PARTHASARATHY, “Customer Switching Behavior PRASAD, ASHUTOSH, see Peterson, R. A.
in Online Services: An Exploratory Study of the Role RICH, GREGORY A., see MacKenzie, S. B.
of Selected Attitudinal, Behavioral, and Demographic ROACH, DAVE, see Mason, K.
Factors,” 374. ROSE, GREGORY M., see Blodgett, J. G.
KEITH, JANET E., see Wagner, J. A. , see Bush, V. D.
KLEIN, NOREEN M., see Wagner, J. A. SACASAS, RENÉ, “The ‘Pizza Wars’” [Marketing and
KOHLI, AJAY K., see Venkatesh, R. the Law], 205.
LACZNIAK, RUSSELL N., see Carlson, L. SAMIEE, SAEED, “Globalization, Privatization, and
LEHMANN, DONALD R., see Estelami, H. Free Market Economy, edited by C. P. Rao” [Reviews
LEWIN, JEFFREY E., “The Effects of Downsizing on Or- of Books], 319.
ganizational Buying Behavior: An Empirical Investi- SAMLI, A. COSKUN, “Kotler on Marketing: How to
gation,” 151. Create, Win and Dominate Markets, by Philip Kotler”
LIDDELL, PEARSON, MELISSA MOORE, and ROB- [Reviews of Books], 420.
ERT MOORE, “Just Sign on the Electronic Line” SAMUELS, JEFFREY M., see Samuels, L. B.
[Marketing and the Law], 110. SAMUELS, LINDA B., and JEFFREY M. SAMUELS,
LOW, GEORGE S., see Grant, K. “Supreme Court Makes Protection of Product Design
, and JAKKI J. MOHR, “Factors Affecting the Use Trade Dress More Difficult” [Marketing and the Law],
of Information in the Evaluation of Marketing Commu- 108.
nications Productivity,” 70. SARKAR, MB, RAJ ECHAMBADI, S. TAMER
LU, LONG-CHUAN, see Blodgett, J. G. CAVUSGIL, and PREET S. AULAKH, “The Influence
MACKENZIE, SCOTT B., PHILIP M. PODSAKOFF, and of Complementarity, Compatibility, and Relationship
GREGORY A. RICH, “Transformational and Trans- Capital on Alliance Performance,” 358.
actional Leadership and Salesperson Performance,” SA SH I T TA L , H E MA N T C., and AVAN R .
115. JASSAWALLA, “Marketing Implementation in
MASON, KEVIN, THOMAS JENSEN, SCOT BUR- Smaller Organizations: Definition, Framework, and
TON, and DAVE ROACH, “The Accuracy of Brand Propositional Inventory,” 50.
INDEX 429

SETHI, RAJESH, see Nicholson, C. Y. Behavioral, and Demographic Factors,” Keaveney and
SIVAKUMAR, K., see Nakata, C. Parthasarathy, 374.
STAFFORD, EDWIN R., and CATHY L. HARTMAN, “Dynamic Strategic Thinking,” Dickson et al., 216.
“NGOs Engaging With Business: A World of Difference “The Effects of Downsizing on Organizational Buying
and a Difference to the World, by Simon Heap” [Re- Behavior: An Empirical Investigation,” Lewin, 151.
views of Books], 418. “Factors Affecting the Use of Information in the Evalua-
SUMMERS, JOHN O., “Guidelines for Conducting Re- tion of Marketing Communications Productivity,” Low
search and Publishing in Marketing: From Conceptual- and Mohr, 70.
ization Through the Review Process,” 405. “Guidelines for Conducting Research and Publishing in
SZYMANSKI, DAVID M., “Modality and Offering Ef- Marketing: From Conceptualization Through the Re-
fects in Sales Presentations for a Good Versus a Ser- view Process,” Summers, 405.
vice” [Research Note], 179. “Heterogeneity in Sales Districts: Beyond Individual-
Level Predictors of Satisfaction and Performance,”
, see Troy, L. C.
Venkatesh et al., 238.
, and DAVID H. HENARD, “Customer Satisfac- “The Impact of Research Design on Consumer Price Re-
tion: A Meta-Analysis of the Empirical Evidence,” 16. call Accuracy: An Integrative Review,” Estelami and
TIETJE, BRIAN, and CHRIS CARR, “The Latest in In- Lehmann, 36.
surance Liability Coverage Determinations: ‘Market “The Influence of Complementarity, Compatibility, and
Overflow’ Claims” [Marketing and the Law], 330. Relationship Capital on Alliance Performance,” Sarkar
TROY, LISA C., DAVID M. SZYMANSKI, and P. et al., 358.
RAJAN VARADARAJAN, “Generating New Product “Instituting the Marketing Concept in a Multinational Set-
Ideas: An Initial Investigation of the Role of Market In- ting: The Role of National Culture,” Nakata and
formation and Organizational Characteristics” [Re- Sivakumar, 255.
search Note], 89. “Managing Culturally Diverse Buyer-Seller Relation-
VARADARAJAN, P. RAJAN, see Troy, L. C. ships: The Role of Intercultural Disposition and Adap-
VENKATESH, R., GOUTAM CHALLAGALLA, and tive Selling in Developing Intercultural Communica-
AJAY K. KOHLI, “Heterogeneity in Sales Districts: tion Competence,” Bush et al., 391.
Beyond Individual-Level Predictors of Satisfaction and “Marketing Implementation in Smaller Organizations:
Performance,” 238. Definition, Framework, and Propositional Inventory,”
VERBEKE, WILLEM J.M.I., see Dickson, P. R. Sashittal and Jassawalla, 50.
VITELL, SCOTT J., see Blodgett, J. G. “The Role of Interpersonal Liking in Building Trust in
WAGNER, JUDY A., NOREEN M. KLEIN, and JANET E. Long-Term Channel Relationships,” Nicholson et al.,
KEITH, “Selling Strategies: The Effects of Suggesting 3.
a Decision Structure to Novice and Expert Buyers,” “The Role of Satisfaction With Territory Design on the
289. Motivation, Attitudes, and Work Outcomes of Sales-
WALSH, ANN, see Carlson, L. people,” Grant et al., 165.
“Salesperson Cooperation: The Influence of Relational,
WHITE, D. STEVEN, “Behind the Success and Failure of
Task, Organizational, and Personal Factors,” Yilmaz
U.S. Export Intermediaries: Transactions, Agents and
and Hunt, 335.
Resources, by Mike W. Peng” [Reviews of Books ],
“Selling Strategies: The Effects of Suggesting a Decision
323.
Structure to Novice and Expert Buyers,” Wagner et al.,
YILMAZ, CENGIZ, and SHELBY D. HUNT, “Salesper-
289.
son Cooperation: The Influence of Relational, Task,
“Socializing Children About Television: An Intergenera-
Organizational, and Personal Factors,” 335.
tional Study,” Carlson et al., 276.
“Transformational and Transactional Leadership and
Salesperson Performance,” MacKenzie et al., 115.
Articles:

“Corollaries of the Collective: The Influence of Organiza- Reviews of Books:


tional Culture and Memory Development on Perceived
Decision-Making Context,” Berthon et al., 135. “Advances in International Marketing, edited by S. Tamer
“Customer Satisfaction: A Meta-Analysis of the Empiri- Cavusgil and Tage Koed Madsen,” Cornwell, 318.
cal Evidence,” Szymanski and Henard, 16. “Behind the Success and Failure of U.S. Export Intermedi-
“Customer Switching Behavior in Online Services: An aries: Transactions, Agents and Resources, by Mike W.
Exploratory Study of the Role of Selected Attitudinal, Peng,” White, 323.
430 JOURNAL OF THE ACADEMY OF MARKETING SCIENCE FALL 2001

“Creative Strategy in Direct Marketing, 2d ed., by Susan “Internet Marketing: Metatags May Create Trademark Li-
K. Jones,” Bravo, 107. ability,” Clarke and Owens, 204.
“A General Theory of Competition: Resources, Compe- “Just Sign on the Electronic Line,” Liddell et al., 110.
tences, Productivity, Economic Growth, by Shelby D. “The Latest in Insurance Liability Coverage Determina-
Hunt,” Peterson and Prasad, 422. tions: ‘Market Overflow’Claims,” Tietje and Carr, 330.
“Globalization, Privatization, and Free Market Economy, “The ‘Pizza Wars,’” Sacasas, 205.
edited by C. P. Rao,” Samiee, 319. “Put ‘Tony the Tiger’ in Your Tank?” Burke, 203.
“Handbook of Marketing and Society, edited by Paul N. “Rumors & Lies: The Parameters of Liability for Com-
Bloom and Gregory T. Gundlach,” Handelman, 416. mercial Speech,” del Castillo, 328.
“High Visibility: The Making and Marketing of Profes- “Supreme Court Expands Federal Power to Regulate the
sionals into Celebrities, by Irving Rein, Philip Kotler, Availability and Use of Data,” Cain, 425.
and Martin Stoller,” Coulter, 105. “Supreme Court Makes Protection of Product Design
“How Advertising Works: The Role of Research, edited by Trade Dress More Difficult,” Samuels and Samules,
John Philip Jones,” Barry, 103. 108.
“International Marketing Strategy: Contemporary Read- “Trade Dress Protection Does Not Extend Expired Pat-
ings, by Isobel Doole and Robin Lowe,” Hult, 322. ent,” Christiansen, 424.
“Kotler on Marketing: How to Create, Win and Dominate “A Trade Dress Tale: Gang Abducts Princess, En-
Markets, by Philip Kotler,” Samli, 421. hances Ransom Through Internet, Court Approves,”
“NGOs Engaging With Business: A World of Difference McCubbins, 109.
and a Difference to the World, by Simon Heap,”
Stafford and Hartman, 418.
“Personalities and Products: A Historical Perspective on Research Notes:
Advertising in America, by Edd Applegate,” Parker,
102. “The Accuracy of Brand and Attribute Judgments: The
“Rethinking Marketing: Qualitative Strategies and Exotic Role of Information Relevancy, Product Experience,
Visions, by Alf H. Walle III,” Cotte, 420. and Attribute-Relationship Schemata,” Mason et al.,
“Selling to Newly Emerging Market, by Russell R. Miller,” 307.
Curran and Hyman, 324. “Ethical Sensitivity to Stakeholder Interests: A Cross-
Cultural Comparison,” Blodgett et al., 190.
“Generating New Product Ideas: An Initial Investigation
Marketing and the Law: of the Role of Market Information and Organizational
Characteristics,” Troy et al., 89.
“Excessive Access to Web Information Can Be Tortious,” “Modality and Offering Effects in Sales Presentations for
de R. Barondes, 327. a Good Versus a Service,” Szymanski, 179.

You might also like