You are on page 1of 21

S98 SUMMER 2012

Learner Attitudes, Perceptions,


and Beliefs in Language Learning
Pamela M. Wesely
University of Iowa

Abstract: This literature review examines recent research in the area of learner
attitudes, perceptions, and beliefs about language learning and about themselves as
language learners, together with the consequences of these perceptions on learning out-
comes. After an overview of relevant definitions of these complex concepts, the review
categorizes this research into three orientations: studies that have focused on how
learner attitudes, perceptions, and beliefs relate to their traits and characteristics; stud-
ies that have examined how these notions relate to the learning environment; and studies
that have looked at how these notions play out in the interaction between the learner
and the environment. The review concludes with suggestions related to research design
and research questions that would address current lacunae in the field.

Key words: language learning anxiety, learner attitudes, learner beliefs, learner per-
ceptions, motivation

Understanding language learners is a matter of examining a variety of evidence,


both observable and unobservable, about their learning of language. This review of
the literature is largely concerned with a certain subset of unobservable attributes
of language learners: that of their attitudes, perceptions, and beliefs about language
learning. In that these attributes are unobservable, the researchers who examine
them largely ask language learners to share what they think, and assume that these
thoughts are pertinent and important to understanding how languages are learned
and taught.
One can generally categorize the scholarly works in this review of the literature as
having one of three orientations: focusing more on the learner as the agent, and how
learners’ static, largely unchanging attitudes, perceptions, and beliefs relate to their
own demographic or identity characteristics (“trait” or “learner” studies); focusing
more on learners’ attitudes, perceptions, and beliefs as affected by their learning situ-
ation (“state” or “environmental” studies); and focusing on the interaction between
the learner and the learning environment (“dynamic” or “complexity” studies).
Numerous scholars have noted the distinction between trait and state orientations
in research on language learner attitudes, perceptions, and beliefs (see for instance
Barcelos, 2003a; Dörnyei, 2009a, 2009b; MacIntyre, 20071). The third orientation
has been suggested by Barcelos (2003a, 2003b) and others to characterize studies that

Pamela M. Wesely (PhD, University of Minnesota) is Assistant Professor of Foreign


Language and ESL Education, University of Iowa.
Foreign Language Annals, Vol. 45, Iss. S1, pp. S98–S117. © 2012 by American Council on the Teaching of Foreign Languages.
DOI: 10.111/j.1944-9720.2012.01181.x.
FOREIGN LANGUAGE ANNALS · VOL. 45, NO. S1 S99

focus on the dynamic, constantly negotiated, of the prevalence of survey research (e.g.,
embedded, and interconnected nature of Dörnyei, 1994; Oxford, 1994) about lan-
learners’ attitudes, perceptions, and beliefs.2 guage learner attitudes, perspectives, and
This review of the literature seeks to beliefs were published in the 1990s, the
examine research in the area of learners’ dominance of survey research has con-
attitudes, perceptions, and beliefs3 about tinued to the present day. These surveys
language learning and about themselves as have been frequently composed of Likert-
language learners, together with the con- scale items that reflected the components
sequences of these perceptions on learning of the construct being investigated; for
outcomes. This review further emphasizes instance, the participant’s agreement with
the last 10 years of research in foreign lan- the statement, “I think French people are
guage (FL) contexts (i.e., contexts in which good people” would be seen as a measure
the language being taught is not the major- of attitudes toward the target community.
ity language of the surrounding commu- Learners themselves have completed these
nity) in the United States. Earlier works surveys at some point before, during, and/
and works from related language learning or after their language learning experiences.
contexts are referenced as appropriate. They have been analyzed with everything
from simple frequency counts to structural
equation modeling (SEM). Mills, Pajares,
Foundations and Definitions and Herron (2006) suggested, in describ-
The groundwork for the inquiry into learner ing the limitations of their own study, that
attitudes, perceptions, and beliefs was laid for self-report instruments of this type hold an
the most part in the 1970s and 1980s, with inherent danger in that “participants some-
studies that emphasized defining and validat- times report what they believe is expected,
ing key concepts such that further research rather than their true beliefs.” The authors
could take place. Work like Bartley’s 1970 continued in stating that “the assurance of
article correlating attitude with attrition, confidentiality and anonymity” and using
Gardner’s (1985) exploration of the attitude- “valid empirical … measurements” did
dependent socio-educational model of lan- mitigate this problem (Mills et al., 2006,
guage learning, and Horwitz’s work with p. 285). This review of the literature, there-
anxiety (Horwitz, Horwitz, & Cope, 1986) fore, is positioned in part to assess the body
and learner beliefs (Horwitz, 1988) largely of survey-based research on attitudes, per-
focused on the task of operationalizing the ceptions, and beliefs, and to present some
target construct, crafting a survey from its of the alternative modes of inquiry that
primary identified components, and validat- have been put forth in the past 10 years.
ing that survey. Important instruments like A brief review of some of the general
the Foreign Language Attitude Scale (FLAS) definitions of the three concepts offers fur-
(Bartley, 1970), the Attitude/Motivation Test ther foundations for understanding the
Battery (AMTB) (Gardner, 1985), the Foreign issues at play in the current literature in
Language Classroom Anxiety Scale (FLCAS) the field. Learner attitudes have often been
(Horwitz et al., 1986), and the Beliefs and Atti- addressed in the literature in relation to
tudes Language Learning Inventory (BALLI) two different targets: attitudes toward the
(Horwitz, 1988) were the tools that were cre- learning situation (often encompassing the
ated, validated, or used in those studies. Other instructor as well as the instructional tech-
texts of the time that focused on individual niques used [Gardner, 2005]), and attitudes
learner differences, notably Spolsky (1989) toward the target community. Attitudes
and Skehan (1989), also depended on these toward the target community have been
instruments to define these concepts. addressed in recent work by Yashima (2009),
Although high-profile qualitative who developed the idea into the notion of
research (e.g. Norton, 1995) and critiques “international posture,” which relates to
S100 SUMMER 2012

how students see themselves as “connected ideas about the task of learning a second/
to the international community, [with] con- foreign language” (Barcelos & Kalaja, 2003,
cerns for international affairs and … a readi- p. 231). Yet more generally, Horwitz (1988)
ness to interact with people [from the target defined beliefs as “student opinions on a
culture(s)]” (Yashima, 2009, p. 146). variety of issues and controversies related
Learner perceptions, like learner atti- to language learning” (Horwitz, 1988,
tudes, have been commonly associated with p. 284). Ostensibly, these definitions allow
two targets in the literature: perceptions of for beliefs to be not just about the learning
themselves, and perceptions of the learning situation, but about the target community,
situation. Perceptions of themselves have language, and culture as well.
often been defined as how students under- Finally, two important related con-
stand and make sense of themselves and cepts, motivation and anxiety, have often
their own learning (Liskin-Gasparro, 1998; been placed in causal relationships with
Williams & Burden, 1999). Learner percep- learner attitudes, perceptions, and beliefs
tions of the learning situation have included in the literature, featuring prominently in
how students experience and understand many of the foundational texts mentioned
aspects of the classroom, like instructor above. Motivation has been closely con-
behaviors (Brown, 2009). It is important to nected with “the affective characteristics of
note that, although many researchers have the learner, referring to the direction and
chosen to focus on one perceptual target magnitude of learning behavior in terms
or the other, most have operated under the of the learner’s choice, intensity, and dura-
assumption that these two types of learner tion of learning” (Dörnyei, 2009a, p. 231).
perceptions are interrelated. As Gardner (2005) has attested, motiva-
Learner beliefs, although rarely distin- tion can be a way of understanding learner
guished formally from learner perceptions attitudes, perceptions, and beliefs, in that it
in the literature, have often been assumed offers “one parsimonious way of account-
to be more overarching and pervasive than ing for individual differences in second lan-
perceptions, which have tended to focus guage acquisition” (Gardner, 2005, p. 21).
on specific experiences. Learner beliefs Motivation may thus serve as a central con-
have included what learners think about struct that can guide and structure research.
themselves, about the learning situation, Another important concept is anxiety,
and about the target community. Beliefs which has been typically characterized as
that learners have about themselves have situation-specific anxiety (Horwitz, 2010).
often been related in the literature to the Aida (1994) explained that Horwitz and
notion of self-efficacy, or “the judgments her colleagues have conceptualized FL
[students] hold about their capability to anxiety as “a distinct complex of self-per-
organize and execute the courses of action ceptions, beliefs, feelings, and behaviors
required to master academic tasks” (Mills related to classroom language learning aris-
et al., 2007, p. 417; see also Bandura, 1997; ing from the uniqueness of the language
Graham, 2006). The notion of self-efficacy learning processes” (Horwitz et al., 1991,
rejects what Bandura has called the “crude” p. 31, as cited in Aida, 1994, p. 156). Simi-
idea that everything is externally controlled lar to motivation (which incidentally has
(Bandura, 1997, p. 23). A related type of included anxiety as a component in most
self-belief is language-learning self-con- models), anxiety has offered researchers
cept, or how students generally feel about a way to understand and contextualize
themselves as language learners (Mills learner attitudes, perceptions, and beliefs
et al., 2007, p. 423). Learner beliefs can be in a framework that is easily measured and
focused on more external targets as well; understood in the field. Works of research
Barcelos and Kalaja (2003) suggested that on the constructs of motivation and anxi-
beliefs comprise students’ “opinions and ety are referenced in this review particularly
FOREIGN LANGUAGE ANNALS · VOL. 45, NO. S1 S101

when they emphasize the components of focusing on specific aspects of the language
attitudes, perceptions, or beliefs that con- learning classroom: first language (L1)
tribute to the constructs. usage (Rolin-Ianziti & Varshney, 2008),
Ultimately, as this review of the litera- technology (Peters, Weinberg, & Sarma,
ture demonstrates, the importance of con- 2009), the native versus nonnative class
sistent and reliable definitions of learner instructor (Hertel & Sunderman, 2009),
attitudes, perceptions, and beliefs cannot be and, in the context of incoming freshmen in
underplayed. All too often, the body of litera- a university program, preferred classroom
ture and development of a dialogue has been activities (Mandell, 2002). Similarly, Price
hindered by unclear definitions or incon- and Gascoigne (2006) sought to describe
sistent application of guiding principles in more general attitudes from college students
examining a specific notion. Hopefully, this about the “importance of foreign language
review can contribute to the dialogue that study” and “postsecondary foreign lan-
can improve this present dynamic. guage requirements” (Price & Gascoigne,
2006, p. 386). Usually, these studies have
been interpretive in nature, depending on
The Trait or Learner Orientation the analysis of essays, interviews, and other
Studies with a “trait” or “learner” orienta- forms of data, but some, notably Peters
tion have focused on describing, measur- et al. (2009), have used surveys with Likert-
ing, and understanding learners’ attitudes, scale and rank-order questions to ascertain
perceptions, and beliefs about language student perceptions. Note that these stud-
learning in a manner that is independent ies, when considered together, do not create
of the learning context and other environ- a body of coherent findings about student
mental factors. Often, these scholars have perceptions; they represent isolated cases
depicted learner attitudes, perceptions, and of exploratory work that each contribute to
beliefs as unchanging and static. a different knowledge base about disparate
topics (e.g., L1 usage, technology) in lan-
guage learning.
Describing Learner Attitudes, A group of similar studies has focused
Perceptions, and Beliefs on tracing the relationship between learner
Some studies with the trait/learner orien- and instructor perceptions. Scholars have
tation have remained at the descriptive examined this relationship as it relates
level, refraining from linking learner atti- to target language use in the classroom
tudes, perceptions, and beliefs to outcomes, (Levine, 2003), learners’ self-expressive
although those implications were often speech (Yoshida, 2007), effective FL teach-
suggested. For instance, authors of studies ing (Brown, 2009), accents in the target
using both structural equation modeling language (Drewelow & Theobald, 2007),
(Csizér and Dörnyei, 2005) and qualitative and teaching strategy frequency (Bernaus
interviews (Graham, 2006) have provided & Gardner, 2008). These studies all found
detailed descriptions of motivation (in the that the learners’ perceptions did not match
former case) and self-efficacy (in the latter those of their instructors. Interestingly,
case). These researchers did not provide evi- researchers looking at English learners in
dence connecting the findings with learner Georgia, Eurasia, found that learners and
outcomes, but they did suggest, interest- instructors in that context were quite simi-
ingly, that the positive academic outcomes lar in their beliefs about language learn-
were expected, given the effort implied in ing. This suggests that differences between
highly motivated students or students with learners and instructors do not necessar-
high self-efficacy. ily always exist when researchers examine
Descriptions of student perceptions concepts other than perceptions (Polat,
have also been common in the field, often 2009). The research methods in these
S102 SUMMER 2012

studies differed slightly, with most using ing the students to comment on their confi-
some form of parallel survey administra- dence in completing specific tasks (see Mills
tion; in contrast, Polat (2009) added quali- et al., 2006). The findings in these studies
tative data, and Yoshida (2007) conducted a generally showed that more positive learner
purely ethnographic study. attitudes, perceptions, and beliefs were
This small body of work is an interest- associated with more positive outcomes of
ing area of inquiry, and it proves that the the types listed above (e.g., more enjoy-
investigation of learners’ attitudes, percep- ment, higher achievement, lower anxiety).
tions, and beliefs without necessarily con- However, some of the studies also raised
necting it to outcomes can be an important questions about how the interaction of
contribution to the discourse in the field. different attitudes, perceptions, and beliefs
might have influenced outcomes in differ-
ent ways, such as with motivation and anxi-
Connecting Learner Attitudes, ety (Graham, 2004; Yan & Horwitz, 2008).
Perceptions, and Beliefs With Learners’ attitudes, perceptions, and
Outcomes beliefs related to the process of language
The most abundant body of literature on atti- learning have been connected to outcomes
tudes, perceptions, and beliefs with a trait/ as well. In the 2000 study mentioned above,
learner orientation, however, has focused Donato et al. sought to “create a profile”
on how those concepts can be related to a where items including attitudes and self-
diverse array of outcomes. The attitudes, assessment could be related to individual
perceptions, and beliefs investigated in this achievement (Donato et al., 2000, p. 386).
type of study have been directed in one Cochran, McCallum, and Bell (2010) simi-
or more of three areas: learners looking at larly included students’ attributions for
themselves as language learners, learners their own success and their attitudes toward
looking at the process of language learning, learning a language, examining their rela-
or learners looking at the target community. tionship through the quantitative analysis of
Learners’ attitudes, perceptions, and Structural Equation Modeling with the stu-
beliefs about themselves as language learn- dents’ achievement. Other studies have been
ers have been associated with outcomes more focused on perceptions of the learning
like enjoyment (Brantmeier, 2005) and situation specifically, linking perceptions
achievement on proficiency measures or of corrective feedback with their desire
grades (Brantmeier, 2005; Donato, Tucker, to participate (Yoshida, 2008), perceptions
Wudthayagorn, & Igarashi, 2000; Graham, of certain instructional techniques with
2004; Mills et al., 2006, 2007). Similar stud- their choice of learning strategies (Mori &
ies have correlated learners’ perceptions of Shimizu, 2007), or perceptions of instruc-
themselves as language learners and their tors with their motivation or anxiety (Ewald,
levels of FL anxiety (Yan & Horwitz, 2008) 2007; Wesely, 2009). This emphasis on per-
and their extrinsic or intrinsic motivation ceptions is notable, and it suggests that there
(Noels, Clément, & Pelletier, 2001). Of is room for further research in this particular
these studies, Graham (2004) and Yan and trait/learner orientation that might further
Horwitz (2008) offered the only non-survey explore the relationship between these per-
data, in the form of interviews. Note that ceptions, learner attitudes, and beliefs.
several of these studies focused on the prin- In some studies, learner attitudes
ciple of self-assessment, where the learn- toward language learning have been seen
ers offered evaluations of their own ability as a contributing factor in achievement,
in language class, essentially indicating but only as mediated by other contribu-
their beliefs about themselves as learners tors. Aida (1994) connected learners’ atti-
(see Brantmeier, 2005). Other researchers tudes and fears about the class with their
looked at the concept of self-efficacy, ask- anxiety, which Aida then connected with
FOREIGN LANGUAGE ANNALS · VOL. 45, NO. S1 S103

their performance in class. Quantitative Cochran et al., 2010). This is a perspective


studies on motivation have been charac- that is currently lacking in educators’ under-
terized by this type of modeling of con- standing of learner attitudes, perceptions,
tributors to achievement (e.g., Bernaus & and beliefs.
Gardner, 2008; Hernández, 2006; Yashima,
2009; Yashima, Zenuk-Nishide, & Shimizu,
2004). For instance, Bernaus and Gardner
Addressing Learner Characteristics
(2008) connected students’ perceptions of With Their Attitudes, Perceptions,
teaching strategies (attitudes toward the and Beliefs
learning situation) with their motivation The third group of trait/learner-oriented
and their achievement in a large-scale sur- studies has emphasized how learner char-
vey. In other studies, the students’ attitudes acteristics can be connected with learner
toward the target community have been attitudes, perceptions, and beliefs. In most
seen as contributing to either their “integra- cases, these studies have examined a group
tive motivation” (Hernández, 2006) or their of individuals with a specific characteris-
“international posture” (Yashima, 2009; tic (e.g., heritage learners), or two groups
Yashima et al., 2004), which then in turn with contrasting characteristics in the same
influenced motivation in general, which environment (e.g., male/female, L1/second
led to achievement. This brief summary of language [L2] background), and explored
these quite complex studies, while neces- their perceptions of themselves and of
sarily simplified, allows for an important their language learning, often as related to
perspective on how these concepts have their learner characteristics. Some of these
been linked to outcomes. authors have linked the learner charac-
These trait/learner-oriented studies teristics to their attitudes, perceptions, or
that have connected learner attitudes, per- beliefs, and then in turn to outcomes. Note
ceptions, and beliefs to outcomes, in their also that these studies have represented a
correlational analysis and focus on relation- variety of research approaches; although
ships, have offered an elegance and clarity there have been some purely quantitative
to the field of literature. They have provided studies (e.g., Comanaru & Noels, 2009;
strong evidence of the importance of these Schmidt & Watanabe, 2001), most of the
concepts in any understanding of language studies have contained at least some quali-
education and the experience of the language tative elements, and many have been com-
learner. However, one important critique of pletely interpretive in nature.
these studies, as suggested in the aforemen- The literature on heritage learners has
tioned work by Donato et al. (2000), lies dominated this particular area of inquiry.
in the nature of correlational relationships. Case studies of heritage learners of German
Associating or correlating two learner attrib- (Dressler, 2010) and interviews with her-
utes, such as learner self-efficacy and high itage learners of Chinese (Wong & Xiao,
levels of proficiency, does not provide proof 2010) in a special issue of the Heritage
that self-efficacy produces high proficiency. Language Journal revealed that how some
The possibility cannot be ruled out that stu- university students viewed themselves as
dents who have higher proficiency, perhaps heritage learners had a strong relationship
as a result of an unknown or unmeasured with how they felt about their own linguis-
variable like access to better learning strat- tic expertise and investment. The connec-
egies or members of the target community, tion between heritage identity and linguistic
then have higher self-efficacy. Finally, schol- and cultural investment was supported by
ars should embrace when a relationship is a qualitative study by Bearse and de Jong
not found when expected, and they should (2008), which contrasted Spanish L1 stu-
allow themselves to explore and interpret dents and English L1 students in a two-way
the reasons (see for instance the study by high school language immersion program.
S104 SUMMER 2012

The authors found that Spanish L1 students & Padilla, 1998) to more neutral or equal
valued Spanish foremost for its connec- performance on attitudinal or motivational
tion to their families and their identity (see measures (Cortés, 2002; Heining-Boynton
also Comanaru & Noels, 2009; Husseinali, & Haitema, 2007). Other types of learner
2006). In all of these studies, the heritage characteristics, particularly characteristics
learners’ beliefs about what was valuable that identify learners with traditionally
in language learning related to their own underrepresented populations in FL, have
identities as heritage learners and their largely been disregarded in the recent lit-
self-concept. Scholars have also inquired erature. One exception to this is the work
into how learners’ identities as heritage by Moore (2005); in this study, she inves-
learners influenced their attitudes toward tigated in an exploratory way the reasons
classroom activities (Schmidt & Watanabe, why African Americans have commonly
2001; Weger-Guntharp, 2006) and the use had low enrollments in language classes
of the target language in the classroom at the college level. She found that the stu-
(Ducar, 2008). Out of these studies, only dents’ attitudes, perspectives, and beliefs
one (Schmidt & Watanabe, 2001) found no about language learning were largely posi-
relation between heritage background and tive, with low motivation resulting from a
pedagogical preferences, although it did lack of systematic access and encourage-
identify the fact that students were study- ment rather than other attitudinal factors.
ing the language of their ethnic heritage as Ultimately, these studies examining
a distinct motivational factor. The fact that how learner characteristics relate to their
this was the only large-scale survey study attitudes, perceptions, and beliefs have
(N = 2,089) of this cluster of studies sug- offered strong insights into learner iden-
gests that research methods might be at least tity and language learning, although some
some of the reason for this unusual finding. important critiques must also be men-
It should also be noted that, with the excep- tioned. As with the studies in the previous
tion of the Bearse and de Jong (2008) study, section, one must be careful to avoid equat-
as already noted, all of these studies took ing correlation and causation. In addition,
place at the university level. any examination of individuals with similar
Beyond heritage learners, groups characteristics must take care when looking
defined by other learner characteristics have at a group as a unique, identifiable, con-
also been investigated in the literature, but sistent category. Weger-Guntharp (2006)
much more infrequently. The differences suggested that scholars looking at herit-
between male and female learners of FLs age learners also consider “the complexity
have long been seen as important questions of individual backgrounds,” avoiding clear
to pursue (see Spolsky, 1989), but recent markers such as place of birth in catego-
published studies that focused on these top- rizing students as heritage or non-heritage
ics (Kissau, 2006; Kissau, Kolano, & Wang, (Weger-Guntharp, 2006, p. 39; see also Lee,
2010) have largely been outnumbered by 2005). Finally, any researcher purporting to
large-scale survey studies where gender look at groups of learners identified solely
was seen as one variable (see Cochran by one characteristic has a responsibility to
et al., 2010; Cortés, 2002; Heining-Boynton also consider the power differentials that
& Haitema, 2007; Shaaban & Ghaith, 2000; are implicated in any identification and rei-
Sung & Padilla, 1998) or as something that fication of difference in society.
had to be controlled for (Brantmeier, 2005). It is clear from this review that there
These large-scale survey studies have has been little serious, focused inquiry
revealed a variety of findings, ranging from about the attitudes, perceptions, and beliefs
strongly indicating more positive attitudes of many subgroups of FL learners. Moore’s
and valuing of FL study by girls (Cochran (2005) study of African American language
et al., 2010; Shaaban & Ghaith, 2000; Sung learners was exceptional, as was a dynamic/
FOREIGN LANGUAGE ANNALS · VOL. 45, NO. S1 S105

complexity study by Csizér, Kormos, & beliefs naturally suggests the adoption of
Sarkadi (2010) that looked at dyslexic learn- a comparative framework, where partici-
ers of FL. Other populations whose attitudes, pants are drawn from two or more learning
perceptions, and beliefs require investiga- settings and then compared. This section
tion include racial and ethnic minorities reviews studies that have compared learner
who are not heritage learners, learners in attitudes, perceptions, and beliefs among
English Language Learning programs who groups defined in two primary ways: dif-
also take FL, learners with differing sexual ferent languages and different classes of the
orientations or gender identities, learners same language.
living in poverty, and learners with develop- Comparing different languages in
mental or behavioral differences. Expand- learner attitudes, perceptions, and beliefs
ing the knowledge base in this manner is about language learning has rarely pro-
imperative, given the findings of the stud- duced consistent findings at the individual
ies outlined above about the importance of language level. However, some general
the connection between learner attitudes, themes have emerged in a close examination
perceptions, and beliefs, and learner success of these studies. One important theme was
and persistence in FL education. the difference in beliefs between learners of
commonly taught languages (i.e., Spanish,
French, and German) in the United States
The State or Environmental and learners of less commonly taught lan-
Level guages. One of the more comprehensive
…learner beliefs about foreign language studies on this topic was Horwitz’s 1999
learning are at least as diverse as the lan- meta-analysis in which she investigated
guages, levels, and institutions in which the cultural and situational influences on
the learners are studying and that teach- language learners’ beliefs, as evidenced by
ers and researchers cannot assume that the compilation of BALLI results from eight
beliefs identified in one group of learn- different studies. Notably, Horwitz found
ers are representative of the beliefs of that U.S. learners of Japanese were differ-
learners of different languages, at differ- ent in their beliefs from any other U.S. FL
ent levels, or at different kinds of insti- learners, in that they estimated a longer
tutions. (Rifkin, 2000, p. 407) time needed to learn the language, had a
As Rifkin indicated in his representation lower evaluation of their own abilities as
of his findings from a large-scale study on language learners, appreciated vocabulary
learner beliefs, looking at learner attitudes, and grammar instruction more, and had
perceptions, and beliefs cannot always be stronger beliefs that their knowledge of the
analyzed as independent from context. The language would help them find employ-
state/environmental orientation features ment (Horwitz, 1999). Similarly, Rivera and
studies that have examined how learning Matsuzawa (2007), in a study that served
situations influenced (or did not influence) as a part of a program evaluation, offered
certain learner attitudes, perceptions, and that students’ learning priorities and beliefs
beliefs. Plainly, the implication and assump- were very different, depending on whether
tion here is that learner attitudes, percep- they were learning a commonly or a less
tions, and beliefs can and do change. commonly taught language. These priori-
ties were largely based on factors related
to whether or not the students had some
Comparing Learner Attitudes, background knowledge of the language,
Perceptions, and Beliefs Across which was more common for the students
Different Environments learning the commonly taught languages
Understanding the role that environment (see also Brown, 2009; Loewen et al.,
plays in learner attitudes, perceptions, and 2009). Thomas (2010), in a questionnaire
S106 SUMMER 2012

study that asked U.S. university students to not been created in order to allow for more
rank their reasons for choosing a language, conclusions to be made in the space of this
offered evidence that learners’ general rea- review. This is certainly an area for further
sons for learning languages correlated with investigation.
the languages that they ended up taking; It is important to note here that all of
for instance, learners largely expressing the studies listed above shared the same
a desire to fulfill the requirement selected context: North American (primarily U.S.)
Spanish, while those with more of an inter- university FL classrooms. Other studies,
est in communication sought out Hebrew, like that of Donitsa-Schmidt, Inbar, and
Portuguese, Italian, Japanese, Chinese, or Shohamy (2004), have suggested that these
Korean. Learner attitudes, perceptions, and findings might not be applicable or as rele-
beliefs relating to the teaching of different vant to non-university or international con-
languages in the language classroom have texts. These researchers found, in a study
been vital issues for many stakeholders in of elementary and middle school Arabic
FL education in the United States, and fur- learners in Israeli schools, that learners who
ther investigations of the reasons behind studied spoken Arabic (rather than the lit-
some of these beliefs are warranted. erary form of Modern Standard Arabic) dis-
The second area of inquiry comparing played more positive attitudes toward the
learner attitudes, perceptions, and beliefs Arabic language, its culture, and its speak-
among groups has focused on learners ers, with more motivation to learn the lan-
attending different classes in the same lan- guage (Donitsa-Schmidt et al., 2004). Other
guage. Different classes can mean a number studies, like the Horwitz (1999) study cited
of things, including different sections of the above, have offered evidence that learner
same class (Gardner, Masgoret, Tennant, & attitudes, perceptions, beliefs, and related
Mihic, 2004), classes for students with dif- concepts differ considerably from country
ferent levels of proficiency (Brown, 2009; to country (see also Kouritzin, Piquemal, &
Kondo-Brown, 2001), and classes focus- Renaud, 2009). The investigation of learn-
ing on different modalities (Kim, 2009). ing situations other than the university
Although Gardner et al. (2004) found FL programs of North America is vital to
invariance in student attitudes among dif- creating a realistic and vivid picture of the
ferent sections of the same Canadian uni- true nature of environmental influences on
versity French classes, it was clear from learner attitudes, perceptions, and beliefs.
survey-based studies by Brown (2009) and
Kondo-Brown (2001), both in the U.S. uni-
versity context, that students in early lev- Examining How FL Programs Affect
els of language instruction had slightly less Learner Attitudes, Perceptions,
positive attitudes to language study than and Beliefs
students at more advanced levels. In addi- The insights that can arise from a com-
tion, at upper levels of study, students were parative framework are complemented
shown to engage more with the material and by another type of study featuring a state/
expect less directive instructor interaction environmental orientation: detailed exami-
(Brown, 2009; Kondo-Brown, 2001). In a nations of how one FL program of study
survey-based study on learner anxiety and affects (or does not affect) the attitudes,
motivation at a U.S. university, Kim (2009) perceptions, and beliefs of learners in that
found that motivation in classes focused on program. Attitudinal change over the course
different modalities (reading and conversa- of one FL class has not been shown to be
tion classes) stayed constant, while anxiety uniformly positive, with some research-
changed in the two circumstances. Unfortu- ers indicating no change (Gardner et al.,
nately, a comprehensive body of knowledge 2004) or negative change (Worth, 2008).
on the comparisons of different classes has Similar longitudinal studies have looked
FOREIGN LANGUAGE ANNALS · VOL. 45, NO. S1 S107

at change over many years, indicating that that participating in a FLES program cor-
early elementary experiences with language related with long-term attitudinal ben-
learning persisted with the learners and efits. Kennedy, Nelson, Odell, and Austin
influenced their positive (Heining-Boynton (2000), through a comparative quantita-
& Haitema, 2007) and negative (Nikolov, tive approach, similarly investigated ways
2001) attitudes later in life. in which elementary students experienced
Another cluster of studies has focused attitudinal changes as a result of studying
on investigating how specific teaching an FL. Students participating in the FLES
practices have been reflected (or not) in program were shown to have more positive
learner attitudes, perceptions, and beliefs attitudes about school, learning, language,
in that program, again usually focusing on culture, and self compared to their non-
university settings. This review has already FLES peers (Kennedy et al., 2000).
noted ample evidence that learners and SA programs are the third alternate set-
instructors often differ in their percep- ting that has appeared with increasing reg-
tions of aspects of the language classroom; ularity in the literature addressing learner
Dewey’s 2004 experimental design study attitudes, perceptions, and beliefs. Indeed,
suggested that when the instructors shared Wang (2010), in a review of the scholarship
their beliefs with their students about a on SA, highlighted the need for a “sociali-
specific instructional technique, the stu- zation perspective,” arguing that research-
dents’ beliefs tended to mirror those of ers need to pay more attention to students’
their instructor after the class had been perspectives and the identity-related issues
completed. Tutoring sessions focusing on that they face during their SA experiences
deeper understanding of language structure (Wang, 2010; see also Kinginger, 2003,
and characterizing language as learnable 2008). In a recent issue of Foreign Language
and regular (Matthews, 2010), inquiry- Annals, Allen (2010) examined the devel-
based programs on culture (Altstaedter & opment of language learning motivation
Jones, 2009), and project-based learning during SA abroad experiences, ultimately
(Mills, 2009) enhanced learner attitudes challenging the idea that SA experiences
and self-efficacy as well. The interventions automatically resulted in transformative
in these cases all produced positive results. learning for all learners (see also Alred &
Other types of learning settings beyond Byram, 2002, for a similar finding related
the traditional university classes inves- to intercultural competence). Although
tigated in the literature have included researchers have continued to argue that
programs for heritage learners, Foreign contact with the target community improves
Language in the Elementary School (FLES) attitudes and motivation (Dörnyei, Csizér,
programs, and study abroad (SA) programs. & Németh, 2006) and reduces anxiety
Programs for heritage learners have been (Aida, 1994), this work on SA programs
shown repeatedly to be beneficial to the has suggested that researchers must closely
improvement of student attitudes, when examine the nature of that contact in order
issues of the learners’ identities as heritage to provide an accurate depiction of the pro-
learners are also considered (see Mikulski, grams’ effects on learners’ attitudes, percep-
2006; Otcu, 2010). For example, Otcu tions, and beliefs (see also Yager, 1998).
(2010) used discourse analysis procedures To conclude, the authors of these stud-
to identify the fact that learning the Turkish ies with state/environmental orientations
language in a heritage school helped stu- have offered direct commentary on the way
dents construct a Turkish cultural identity that language educators teach, program, and
in the United States. Investigations of FLES structure FL classes, and have connected
programs have often followed the pattern them with the ways that learners under-
already seen in the study by Heining-Boyn- stand and process their language learning
ton and Haitema (2007), which showed experiences. Allowing for learner attitudes,
S108 SUMMER 2012

perceptions, and beliefs to be acknowl- ing a dynamic/complexity orientation, due


edged and addressed in FL programs has to the fact that they not only addressed
emerged as a theme, as has the necessity of both of these orientations but also allowed
incorporating these attitudes, perceptions, for interplay, change, and instability of the
and beliefs into how instructors plan their learner attitudes, perceptions, and beliefs.
classes. These researchers also cautioned There was, however, a small class of highly
that mere participation in a FL program will complex studies that did not fit those cri-
not guarantee positive effects on learner teria. The authors of these studies, many
attitudes, perceptions, and beliefs. This has of which have already been mentioned at
been shown most notably in the SA learning length in this review (Aida, 1994; Cortés,
situation. 2002; Schmidt & Watanabe, 2001; Sung
The gaps in the literature are still quite & Padilla, 1998), still identified learner
obvious. Given the fact that the learn- attitudes, perceptions, and beliefs as static
ing environment has been shown to be so entities but addressed them with both
important to learner attitudes, perceptions, a trait/learner orientation and a state/
and beliefs, there is clearly a mandate to environmental orientation. Methodologi-
continue to expand the type of programs cally, these studies were uniformly based
that are studied. Comparative studies in large-scale survey data, where many dif-
looking at learner attitudes, perceptions, ferent variables were identified, correlated,
and beliefs could take place in a variety and presented in the text of the study. A
of contexts beyond those described above, detailed example of such a study was Yang
including hybrid versus face-to-face learn- (2003), which examined learners’ attitude-
ing environments, or immersion versus dependent motivational orientations, then
non-immersion schools. Some of the above summarized all of the variables that had
university-situated studies comparing lan- statistically significant effects on those ori-
guage or level certainly merit replication entations, which included the language
in high school, middle school, adult educa- of study, gender, heritage learner status,
tion, and elementary programs. Indeed, it is requirement, and language proficiency.
truly stunning that there have been nearly Studies like this were difficult to synthe-
no studies on learner attitudes, perceptions, size in their entirety without extracting
and beliefs with the state/environmental sub-findings, which often felt like reducing
orientation at the high school level in the their complexity. As noted in the next sec-
United States, given the number of Ameri- tion, the dynamic/complexity orientation
cans who study at this level. Essentially, offers a promising way to build on this type
looking at learner attitudes, perceptions, of dual-orientation study.
and beliefs with a state/environmental ori-
entation necessitates a constant exploration
of new contexts, and that is the primary The Dynamic/Complexity
recommendation for future research in this Orientation
area. The dynamic/complexity orientation finds
its roots in the 1990s, in influential texts by
Norton (1995) and McKay and Wong (1996)
A Note on Dual-Orientation that explicitly adopted new perspectives on
Studies learners and their attitudes, perceptions,
When categorizing these studies into and beliefs. In part in reaction to Gardner’s
one orientation or another, the challenge concepts of instrumental and integrative
has become what to do with studies that motivation, Norton (2000) explained that
address both the trait/learner and the state/ she was seeking a more “complex relation-
environmental orientations. In some cases, ship between power, identity, and language
these studies were easily classifiable as hav- learning” in her work with English language
FOREIGN LANGUAGE ANNALS · VOL. 45, NO. S1 S109

learners (Norton, 2000, p. 10). McKay and England, questioned the participants about
Wong (1996) interpreted this as imagining how they knew that they were doing well
the learner as a “complex social being,” and in French class. In a detailed analysis,
the school as a “contestatory discursive site” the authors explained how the students
(McKay & Wong, 1996, p. 604; see also emphasized both their own perceptions of
Potowski, 2007). In more recent years, oth- their success and the perceptions of their
ers have embraced these ideas and reframed instructors and classmates as influences on
them. An example is Barcelos (2003b), their own impressions. Although this study
who stated that “beliefs about SLA should positioned itself as focused on students’
be investigated interactively and organi- “developing” conceptions of themselves as
cally, where beliefs and actions interconnect language learners, it is obvious from the
and interrelate with each other” (Barcelos, analysis that the emphasis was on the pre-
2003b, p. 196; see also Dörnyei, 2009a, cise mechanics of how those conceptions
2009b; Kinginger, 2008). Studies that have were formed through the interplay between
done this have been primarily rooted in the the environment and the learner. Ushioda’s
interpretive paradigm. 2001 study of French learners in Dublin,
Three early studies, conducted by through open-ended and semi-structured
Liskin-Gasparro (1998), Williams and interview techniques, traced learners’ con-
Burden (1999), and Ushioda (2001) pro- ceptions of their own motivation, their pat-
vide examples of the types of research terns of thinking, and their experiences.
implied by this orientation. The qualita- This study is notable in its emphasis on the
tive study by Liskin-Gasparro (1998), not role of experience as shaping learner atti-
explicitly situated as a dynamic study but tudes, perceptions, and beliefs; the notion
still very much operating on that level, of learner experience is often emphasized in
reported on students’ perceptions of their studies with this orientation as a process-
linguistic ability and learning experiences oriented aspect of learning.
in a summer university-level Spanish In more recent years, notably with the
immersion program. Through case studies publication of Beliefs about SLA (Kalaja &
of seven learners, Liskin-Gasparro demon- Barcelos, 2003) and Dörnyei and Ushioda’s
strated explicitly how the contextual factors Motivation, Language Identity, and the L2 Self
in the learning environment interacted with (2009), the discourse about learner atti-
the students’ perceptions of their progress tudes, perceptions, and beliefs in dynamic/
and performance. Presaging the findings complexity studies has gained more trac-
from the SA studies by Allen (2010) and tion and richness. In the former text, Kalaja
Yager (1998), Liskin-Gasparro found that presented a study that focused on using a
students had conflicting and ambiguous discursive approach to examine the words
beliefs about this special language program. of one test-taker as he reflected on test-
For instance, these students depicted the taking. The author identified four interpre-
immersion model as the best way to learn a tive repertoires in the test-taker’s accounts,
language, yet they also expressed the belief including “Mr. Hard Work,” “Mr. Skilled,”
that more formal grammar and vocabulary “Mr. Cool,” and “Mr. Chance,” to demon-
learning was the only way to achieve true strate the variability that can exist in rela-
accuracy and fluency in a language. In order tion to a student’s expectations of taking a
to reconcile these conflicting beliefs, the test in an FL. Explicitly avoiding a cause-
learners reacted to their immersion envi- and-effect relationship, she emphasized
ronment by constantly reimagining and that “expectations of success are situated,
reinterpreting their experiences as language constructed, and rhetorically organized”
learners (Liskin-Gasparro, 1998). Williams (Kalaja, 2003, p. 104). Kramsch (2003), in
and Burden (1999), in a qualitative study a piece in the same edited volume, focused
of early adolescent language learners in on how university language learners viewed
S110 SUMMER 2012

learning a language, as reflected through complexity orientation, and its strong link
their completion of this phrase: “Learn- with interpretive research, suggests that
ing a language is like …” Through her language educators may be entering a new
analysis, Kramsch argued that the learners’ phase of research on learner attitudes, per-
responses, though seeming to reflect sta- ceptions, and beliefs. New studies might
ble, personal beliefs, in fact reflected their be directed at characterizing the interplay
learning contexts quite closely, their “con- between learner and environment in unique
structed representations of themselves and environments and populations.
their experiences” (Kramsch, 2003, p. 125; Adopting a dynamic/complexity orien-
see also Dufva, 2003; Kramsch, 2009). In tation in looking at learner attitudes, per-
the 2009 volume by Dörnyei and Ushioda, ceptions, and beliefs, however, carries some
a mixed-methods study by Lyons (2009) risks. Barcelos and Kalaja (2003), though
demonstrated that French Legionnaires, focused on the discursive and varied aspects
required to speak French at all times, could of research on beliefs, clearly acknowledged
be characterized by a “fluid, complex, and in their volume that connecting beliefs to
context-dependent relationship between … both effects in language learning and causes
motivational orientations, anxiety, and their in instructor action are important aspects
low L2 achievement” (Lyons, 2009, p. 260). of research on the topic (Barcelos & Kalaja,
Power relations and group dynamics, Lyons 2003). In addition, research with a dynamic/
argued, constantly affected the Legion- complexity orientation must be accessible
naires’ experiences, consistently affecting to most scholars, both methodologically
how they viewed learning language and and theoretically. To wit, this research, as
their self-concept as language learners. evidenced by Lyons’s study (2009), has lent
A slow but growing appearance of itself well to a mixed methods approach.
related studies in the past few years indi- The first part of this study addressed con-
cates that the interest in understanding the structs already in the field with a validated
interplay between learner and environment instrument (Gardner’s AMTB [1985]), and
with a dynamic/complexity orientation is the second qualitative part focused on an
growing. Promisingly, these researchers interpretive investigation of how learner atti-
have explored contexts (e.g., online envi- tudes, perceptions, and beliefs developed in
ronments; Coryell & Clark, 2009) and the interplay between learner and environ-
learner populations (e.g., dyslexic learners; ment. The theoretical groundwork that has
Csizér et al., 2010) outside of the traditions been done by scholars like Horwitz (1988;
and contexts that this review has identified. Horwitz et al., 1986) and Gardner (1985) can
Even when these texts have been situated and should still be considered as researchers
outside of the scholarly discourse, they adopt a dynamic/complexity orientation.
have provided dynamic views of learner
attitudes, perceptions, and beliefs, as in
the book Language Crossings: Negotiating Views to the Future
the Self in a Multicultural World (Ogulnick, As the study of learner attitudes, percep-
2000), which featured short chapters by tions, and beliefs moves forward in our
individuals sharing their “linguistic auto- field, we need to balance the need to cre-
biographies,” the descriptions of their own ate a cohesive body of knowledge for the
language learning experiences in light of field with the independent pursuit of what
the social, cultural, and political contexts Oxford and Shearin (1994) have called
in which they occur. Kramsch’s The Multi- “intellectual paths” in examining these
lingual Subject (2009) similarly depended concepts. This is an argument for allow-
on learner narratives to illustrate how ing many voices to examine learner atti-
they understood their language learning tudes, perceptions, and beliefs in a number
experiences. The nature of the dynamic/ of ways, but also for asking these voices
FOREIGN LANGUAGE ANNALS · VOL. 45, NO. S1 S111

to speak to one another without getting that hones our knowledge and builds new
locked in an echo chamber that immedi- understandings.
ately discounts new paths of inquiry. As
such, here are my primary suggestions for Regarding Research Questions
our field, inspired by this review of the lit-
The following are some of the primary
erature, and with an eye to the next steps
research questions that have been suggested
that we should take together as a commu-
in this review of the literature:
nity of scholars. First, I summarize some
of the important considerations regarding 1. Is there any causal relationship between
designing and framing studies on these top- learner attitudes, perceptions, and beliefs
ics. Then, I suggest research questions that and outcomes like achievement or pro-
would target areas where additional inves- ficiency? Do more positive attitudes,
tigations are needed on the topic of learner perceptions, and beliefs truly contribute
attitudes, perceptions, and beliefs. significantly to making language learners
more successful in the classroom, or as
language learners in general? Although
Regarding Research Design correlations have been identified in the
The field of research on language learner research reviewed in this article, there
attitudes, perceptions, and beliefs is built has been a lack of clarity and few con-
on a foundation of well-defined constructs. clusive findings about the directionality
As MacIntyre, MacKinnon, and Clément of this relationship, particularly across
warned, scholars must avoid the prolifera- different contexts of study. This is not
tion of related concepts, what they called an easy question to study, but more con-
“the naming problem” (2009, p. 58). How- tributions to the knowledge base in this
ever, building on these foundational con- area are imperative in order to create the
cepts in new and different ways is necessary most effective FL instruction possible.
at this point in time. We would be well 2. Is there any long-term effect of partici-
advised to continue to diversify the research pation in a language program on learn-
methodologies used in these studies, with- ers’ attitudes, perceptions, and beliefs
out necessarily discarding the theoretical related to language learning? The body
frameworks. Scholars can allow for the of work tracing long-term effects of
uncoupling of survey instruments and their participation in language study is very
theoretical foundations, thus investigating small, and longitudinal studies are des-
the same concepts with the same compo- perately needed in order to advocate for
nents but different research methods. the importance of FL study at a variety
Furthermore, when examining differ- of levels to educators, policymakers,
ences among groups in terms of their atti- students, and parents.
tudes, perceptions, and beliefs, we must 3. How do subgroups of FL learners with
find ways to draw valuable information different learner characteristics vary in
from the proving of the null hypothesis. their attitudes, perceptions, and beliefs?
If we discover, for example, that there are As shown in this review, with the excep-
really very few differences between groups tion of heritage learners, very few
on the basis of their gender or the lan- subgroups of FL learners have been sys-
guages taught, we must find ways in either tematically represented in the research;
the initial design of the study or a revisiting FL learners are much more often rep-
of the data to discover explanations beyond resented as a homogeneous population
“there was no difference.” Incorporating with no variation in ethnicity, socio-
explanatory structures into our research, economic status, gender identity, and
such as follow-up interviews or member so forth. More studies that specifically
checks, can help create a body of literature address this issue are needed in order
S112 SUMMER 2012

for FL education to be shown as rele- Notes


vant to all. 1. MacIntyre (2007) in fact differentiated
4. What are the learner attitudes, percep- between “state,” “situation-specific,” and
tions, and beliefs about language learn- “trait” levels of abstraction rather than
ing in settings other than traditional just “state” and “trait,” but these two
university programs? The majority of the terms are more commonly agreed upon
studies represented in this review have as representing the primary dichotomy.
examined the attitudes, perceptions, and
beliefs of postsecondary learners, and 2. In cases where studies are not easily
communities outside of the university identifiable as state, trait, or dynamic
context (e.g., elementary, middle, and studies, I have categorized the studies
secondary language education, adult according to the primary intent and ori-
education) or with different program entation, when possible.
structures (e.g., hybrid courses, immer- 3. Although the original purpose of this
sion programs) have been addressed piece was to focus on learner attitudes
only rarely. The absence of research on and perceptions, I have found that the
high school FL learners in the United concept of learner beliefs is in conver-
States is particularly problematic, given sation with those other notions in the
the number of students across the coun- literature, yet is sufficiently distinct to
try that are enrolled at that level. merit inclusion as a third concept in the
5. Finally, how does the interplay between review.
the learner and the environment relate
to learner attitudes, perceptions, and
beliefs? This last question is a call to References
expand the body of work with a dynamic/ Aida, Y. (1994). Examination of Horwitz,
complexity orientation in order to seek Horwitz, and Cope’s construct of foreign lan-
out new ways of understanding these guage anxiety: The case of students of Japa-
nese. Modern Language Journal, 78, 155–168.
concepts.
Allen, H. W. (2010). Language-learning moti-
The scholarly community has crafted vation during short-term study abroad: An
rigorous, thoughtful, and enlightening activity theory perspective. Foreign Language
work about learner attitudes, perceptions, Annals, 43, 27–49.
and beliefs in language learning. These Alred, G., & Byram, M. (2002). Becoming an
new questions offer us an opportunity to intercultural mediator: A longitudinal study
continue to develop our scholarly knowl- of residence abroad. Journal of Multilingual
and Multicultural Development, 23, 339–352.
edge of these complex concepts. In this,
we position ourselves to understand the Altstaedter, L. L., & Jones, B. (2009). Moti-
language learner even more completely, an vating students’ foreign language and culture
acquisition through web-based inquiry. For-
understanding that will serve us well in the eign Language Annals, 42, 640–657.
implementation and expansion of high-
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exer-
quality FL programs across the country and cise of control. New York: W. H. Freeman and
the world. Company.
Barcelos, A. M. F. (2003a). Researching beliefs
Acknowledgments about SLA: A critical review. In P. Kalaja & A.
I would like to thank my ACTFL mentor, M. F. Barcelos (Eds.), Beliefs about SLA: New
research approaches (pp. 7–33). Boston: Klu-
Leo van Lier, as well as Eileen Glisan and wer Academic Publishers.
Richard Donato, for their feedback and sug-
Barcelos, A. M. F. (2003b). Teachers’ and stu-
gestions during my drafting of this manu- dents’ beliefs within a Deweyan framework:
script. I also thank Fatima Baig for her Conflict and influence. In P. Kalaja & A. M.
research assistance. F. Barcelos (Eds.), Beliefs about SLA: New
FOREIGN LANGUAGE ANNALS · VOL. 45, NO. S1 S113

research approaches (pp. 171–199). Boston: Csizér, K., Kormos, J., & Sarkadi, A. (2010).
Kluwer Academic Publishers. The dynamics of language learning attitudes
and motivation: Lessons from an interview
Barcelos, A. M. F., & Kalaja, P. (2003).
study of dyslexic language learners. Modern
Conclusion: Exploring possibilities for future
Language Journal, 94, 470–487.
research on beliefs about SLA. In P. Kalaja
& A. M. F. Barcelos (Eds.), Beliefs about Dewey, D. P. (2004). Connections between
SLA: New research approaches (pp. 231–238). teacher and student attitudes regarding
Boston: Kluwer Academic Publishers. script choice in first-year Japanese language
classrooms. Foreign Language Annals, 37,
Bartley, D. E. (1970). The importance of
567–577.
the attitude factor in language dropout: A
preliminary investigation of group and sex Donato, R., Tucker, G. R., Wudthayagorn, J.,
differences. Foreign Language Annals, 3, 383– & Igarashi, K. (2000). Converging evidence:
393. Attitudes, achievements, and instruction
in the later years of FLES. Foreign Language
Bearse, C., & de Jong, E. J. (2008). Cultural
Annals, 33, 377–393.
and linguistic investment: Adolescents in
a secondary two-way immersion program. Donitsa-Schmidt, S., Inbar, O., & Shohamy,
Equity and Excellence in Education, 41, 325– E. (2004). The effects of teaching spoken
340. Arabic on students’ attitudes and motivation
in Israel. Modern Language Journal, 88, 217–
Bernaus, M., & Gardner, R. C. (2008). Teacher
228.
motivation strategies, student perceptions,
student motivation, and English achievement. Dörnyei, Z. (1994). Understanding L2 moti-
Modern Language Journal, 92, 387–401. vation: On with the challenge! Modern Lan-
guage Journal, 78, 515–523.
Brantmeier, C. (2005). Nonlinguistic vari-
ables in advanced second language reading: Dörnyei, Z. (2009a). Individual differences:
Learners’ self-assessment and enjoyment. Interplay of learner characteristics and learn-
Foreign Language Annals, 38, 494–504. ing environment. Language Learning, 59, 230–
248.
Brown, A. V. (2009). Students’ and teach-
ers’ perceptions of effective foreign language Dörnyei, Z. (2009b). The psychology of second
teaching: A comparison of ideals. Modern Lan- language acquisition. New York: Oxford Uni-
guage Journal, 93, 46–60. versity Press.
Cochran, J. L., McCallum, R. S., & Bell, S. Dörnyei, Z., Csizér, K., & Németh, N. (2006).
M. (2010). Three A’s: How do attributions, Motivation, language attitudes and globalisa-
attitudes, and aptitude contribute to foreign tion: A Hungarian perspective. Buffalo, NY:
language learning? Foreign Language Annals, Multilingual Matters.
43, 566–582. Dörnyei, Z., & Ushioda, E. (2009). Motiva-
Comanaru, R., & Noels, K. A. (2009). Self- tion, language identities and the L2 self:
determination, motivation, and the learning Future research directions. In Z. Dörnyei & E.
of Chinese as a heritage language. Canadian Ushioda (Eds.), Motivation, language identity,
Modern Language Review, 66, 131–158. and the L2 self (pp. 350–356). Clevedon, UK:
Multilingual Matters.
Cortés, K. H. (2002). Youth and the study
of foreign languages: An investigation of Dressler, K. (2010). “There is no space for
attitudes. Foreign Language Annals, 35, 320– being German”: Portraits of willing and reluc-
332. tant heritage language learners of German.
Heritage Language Journal, 7, 1–21.
Coryell, J. E., & Clark, M. C. (2009). One
right way, intercultural participation, and lan- Drewelow, I., & Theobald, A. (2007). A com-
guage learning anxiety: A qualitative analysis parison of the attitudes of learners, instruc-
of adult online heritage and nonheritage lan- tors, and native French speakers about the
guage learners. Foreign Language Annals, 42, pronunciation of French: An exploratory
483–504. study. Foreign Language Annals, 40, 491–520.
Csizér, K., & Dörnyei, Z. (2005). The inter- Ducar, C. M. (2008). Student voices: The
nal structure of language learning motivation missing link in the Spanish heritage language
and its relationship with language choice and debate. Foreign Language Annals, 41, 415–433.
learning effort. Modern Language Journal, 89, Dufva, H. (2003). Beliefs in dialogue: A
19–36. Bakhtinian view. In P. Kalaja & A. M. F.
S114 SUMMER 2012

Barcelos (Eds.), Beliefs about SLA: New Horwitz, E. K., Horwitz, M. B., & Cope, J.
research approaches (pp. 131–151). Boston: (1991). Foreign language classroom anxi-
Kluwer Academic Publishers. ety. In E. K. Horwitz & D. J. Young (Eds.),
Language anxiety: From theory and research
Ewald, J. D. (2007). Foreign language learn-
to classroom implications (pp. 27–39). Engle-
ing anxiety in upper-level classes: Involving
wood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
students as researchers. Foreign Language
Annals, 40, 122–142. Husseinali, G. (2006). Who is studying Arabic
and why? A survey of Arabic students’ orien-
Gardner, R. C. (1985). Social psychology and
tations at a major university. Foreign Language
second language learning: The role of attitudes
Annals, 39, 395–412.
and motivation. Baltimore, MD: Edward Arnold.
Kalaja, P. (2003). Research on students’
Gardner, R. C. (2005, May). Integrative moti-
beliefs about SLA within a discursive
vation and second language acquisition. Paper
approach. In P. Kalaja & A. M. F. Barcelos
presented at the Canadian Association of
(Eds.), Beliefs about SLA: New research
Applied Linguistics/Canadian Linguistics
approaches (pp. 87–108). Boston: Kluwer
Association, London, Canada.
Academic Publishers.
Gardner, R. C., Masgoret, A.-M., Tennant, J.,
Kalaja, P., & Barcelos, A. M. F. (Eds). (2003).
& Mihic, L. (2004). Integrative motivation:
Beliefs about SLA: New research approaches.
Changes during a year-long intermediate-
Boston: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
level course. Language Learning, 54, 1–34.
Kennedy, T. J., Nelson, J. K., Odell, M. R. L.,
Graham, S. J. (2004). Giving up on modern
& Austin, L. K. (2000). The FLES attitudinal
foreign languages? Students’ perceptions of
inventory. Foreign Language Annals, 33, 278–
learning French. Modern Language Journal,
289.
88, 171–191.
Kim, S.-Y. (2009). Questioning the stability of
Graham, S. J. (2006). A study of students’
foreign language classroom anxiety and moti-
metacognitive beliefs about foreign language
vation across different classroom contexts.
study and their impact on learning. Foreign
Foreign Language Annals, 42, 138–157.
Language Annals, 39, 296–309.
Kinginger, C. (2003). Alice doesn’t live here
Heining-Boynton, A. L., & Haitema, T.
anymore: Foreign language learning and
(2007). A ten-year chronicle of student atti-
identity reconstruction. In A. Pavlenko &
tudes toward foreign language in the elemen-
A. Blackledge (Eds.), Negotiation of identities
tary school. Modern Language Journal, 91,
in multilingual contexts (pp. 219–242). Clev-
149–168.
edon, UK: Multilingual Matters.
Hernández, T. (2006). Integrative motivation
Kinginger, C. (2008). Language learning
as a predictor of success in the intermediate
in study abroad: Case studies of Americans
foreign language classroom. Foreign Language
in France [Monograph]. Modern Language
Annals, 39, 605–617.
Journal, 92.
Hertel, T. J., & Sunderman, G. (2009). Stu-
Kissau, S. (2006). Gender differences in
dent attitudes toward native and non-native
motivation to learn French. Canadian Modern
language instructors. Foreign Language
Language Review, 62, 401–422.
Annals, 42, 468–482.
Kissau, S. P., Kolano, L. Q., & Wang, C. (2010).
Horwitz, E. K. (1988). The beliefs about lan-
Perceptions of gender differences in high
guage learning of beginning university foreign
school students’ motivation to learn Spanish.
language students. Modern Language Journal,
Foreign Language Annals, 43, 703–721.
72, 283–294.
Kondo-Brown, K. (2001). Bilingual heritage
Horwitz, E. K. (1999). Cultural and situa-
students’ language contact and motivation.
tional influences on foreign language learners’
In Z. Dörnyei & R. Schmidt (Eds.), Motiva-
beliefs about language learning: A review of
tion and second language acquisition (Vol. 23,
BALLI studies. System, 27, 557–576.
pp. 433–459). Honolulu: University of Hawai’i,
Horwitz, E. K. (2010). Foreign and second lan- Second Language Teaching and Curriculum
guage anxiety. Language Teaching, 43, 154–167. Center.
Horwitz, E. K., Horwitz, M. B., & Cope, J. Kouritzin, S. G., Piquemal, N. A., & Renaud,
(1986). Foreign language classroom anxiety. R. D. (2009). An international comparison
Modern Language Journal, 70, 125–132. of socially constructed language learning
FOREIGN LANGUAGE ANNALS · VOL. 45, NO. S1 S115

motivation and beliefs. Foreign Language ment and agency in second-language learning
Annals, 42, 287–317. among Chinese adolescent immigrant
students. Harvard Educational Review, 66,
Kramsch, C. (2003). Metaphor and the sub-
577–608.
jective construction of beliefs. In P. Kalaja &
A. M. F. Barcelos (Eds.), Beliefs about SLA: Mikulski, A. M. (2006). Accent-uating rules
New research approaches (pp. 109–125). Bos- and relationships: Motivations, attitudes, and
ton: Kluwer Academic Publishers. goals in a Spanish for native speakers class.
Foreign Language Annals, 39, 660–682.
Kramsch, C. (2009). The multilingual subject.
New York: Oxford University Press. Mills, N. (2009). A Guide du Routard simula-
tion: Increasing self-efficacy in the Standards
Lee, J. S. (2005). Through the learners’
through project-based learning. Foreign Lan-
eyes: Reconceptualizing the heritage and non-
guage Annals, 42, 607–639.
heritage learner of the less commonly taught
languages. Foreign Language Annals, 38, 554– Mills, N., Pajares, F., & Herron, C. (2006).
563. A reevaluation of the role of anxiety: Self-
efficacy, anxiety, and their relation to reading
Levine, G. S. (2003). Student and instructor
and listening proficiency. Foreign Language
beliefs and attitudes about target language
Annals, 39, 276–295.
use, first language use, and anxiety: Report of
a questionnaire study. Modern Language Jour- Mills, N., Pajares, F., & Herron, C. (2007).
nal, 87, 343–364. Self-efficacy of college intermediate French
students: Relation to achievement and moti-
Liskin-Gasparro, J. E. (1998). Linguistic
vation. Language Learning, 57, 417–442.
development in an immersion context: How
advanced learners of Spanish perceive SLA. Moore, Z. (2005). African-American students’
Modern Language Journal, 82, 159–175. opinions about foreign language study: An
exploratory study of low enrollments at the
Loewen, S., Li, S., Fei, F., Thompson, A.,
college level. Foreign Language Annals, 38,
Nakatsukasa, K., Ahn, S., & Chen, X. (2009).
191–199.
Second language learners’ beliefs about gram-
mar instruction and error correction. Modern Mori, Y., & Shimizu, H. (2007). Japanese lan-
Language Journal, 93, 91–104. guage students’ attitudes toward Kanji and
their perceptions on Kanji learning strategies.
Lyons, Z. (2009). Imagined identity and the
Foreign Language Annals, 40, 472–490.
L2 self in the French Foreign Legion. In Z.
Dörnyei & E. Ushioda (Eds.), Motivation, lan- Nikolov, M. (2001). A study of unsuccessful
guage identity and the L2 self (pp. 248–273). language learners. In Z. Dörnyei & R. Schmidt
Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters. (Eds.), Motivation and second language acquisi-
tion (Vol. 23, pp. 149–169). Honolulu: Uni-
MacIntyre, P. D. (2007). Willingness to com-
versity of Hawai’i, Second Language Teaching
municate in the second language: Understand-
and Curriculum Center.
ing the decision to speak as a volitional process.
Modern Language Journal, 91, 564–576. Noels, K. A., Clément, R., & Pelletier, L. G.
(2001). Intrinsic, extrinsic, and integrative
MacIntyre, P. D., MacKinnon, S. P., & Clément,
orientations of French Canadian learners of
R. (2009). The baby, the bathwater, and
English. Canadian Modern Language Review,
the future of language learning motivation
57, 424–442.
research. In Z. Dörnyei & E. Ushioda (Eds.),
Motivation, language identity and the L2 self Norton, B. (1995). Social identity, investment,
(pp. 43–65). Clevedon, UK: Multilingual and language learning. TESOL Quarterly, 29,
Matters. 9–31.
Mandell, P. B. (2002). On the background and Norton, B. (2000). Identity and language learn-
motivation of students in a beginning Span- ing: Gender, ethnicity and educational change.
ish program. Foreign Language Annals, 35, New York: Pearson Education.
530–542.
Ogulnick, K. (Ed.). (2000). Language cross-
Matthews, P. H. (2010). Factors influencing ings: Negotiating the self in a multicultural
self-efficacy judgments of university students world. New York: Teachers College Press.
in foreign language tutoring. Modern Lan-
Otcu, B. (2010). Heritage language mainte-
guage Journal, 94, 618–635.
nance and cultural identity formation: The
McKay, S. L., & Wong, S.-L. C. (1996). Mul- case of a Turkish Saturday school in New York
tiple discourses, multiple identities: Invest- City. Heritage Language Journal, 7, 112–137.
S116 SUMMER 2012

Oxford, R. L. (1994). Where are we regarding Spolsky, B. (1989). Conditions for second lan-
language learning motivation? Modern Lan- guage learning: Introduction to a general theory.
guage Journal, 78, 512–514. New York: Oxford University Press.
Oxford, R. L., & Shearin, J. (1994). Language Sung, H., & Padilla, A. M. (1998). Student
learning motivation: Expanding the theoreti- motivation, parental attitudes, and involve-
cal framework. Modern Language Journal, 78, ment in the learning of Asian languages in
12–28. elementary and secondary schools. Modern
Peters, M., Weinberg, A., & Sarma, N. (2009). Language Journal, 82, 205–216.
To like or not to like! Student perceptions of Thomas, J. A. (2010). How do I satisfy the gen-
technological activities for learning French as eral education language requirement? Univer-
a second language at five Canadian universi- sity students’ attitudes toward language study.
ties. Canadian Modern Language Review, 65, Foreign Language Annals, 43, 531–551.
869–896.
Ushioda, E. (2001). Language learning at
Polat, N. (2009). Matches in beliefs between university: Exploring the role of motivational
teachers and students, and success in L2 thinking. In Z. Dörnyei & R. Schmidt (Eds.),
attainment: The Georgian example. Foreign Motivation and second language acquisition
Language Annals, 42, 229–249. (Vol. 23, pp. 93–125). Honolulu: University
Potowski, K. (2007). Language and identity in of Hawai’i, Second Language Teaching and
a dual immersion school. Buffalo, NY: Multilin- Curriculum Center.
gual Matters. Wang, C. (2010). Toward a second language
Price, J., & Gascoigne, C. (2006). Current socialization perspective: Issues in study abroad
perceptions and beliefs among incoming col- research. Foreign Language Annals, 43, 50–63.
lege students towards foreign language study Weger-Guntharp, H. (2006). Voices from the
and language requirements. Foreign Language margin: Developing a profile of Chinese her-
Annals, 39, 383–394. itage language learners in the FL classroom.
Rifkin, B. (2000). Revisiting beliefs about Heritage Language Journal, 4, 29–46. Retrieved
foreign language learning. Foreign Language February 17, 2012, from http://www.herit-
Annals, 33, 394–408. agelanguages.org
Rivera, G. M., & Matsuzawa, C. (2007). Wesely, P. M. (2009). The language learning
Multiple-language program assessment: motivation of early adolescent French immer-
Learners’ perspectives on first-year and sion graduates. Foreign Language Annals, 42,
second-year college foreign language pro- 270–286.
grams and their implications for program Williams, M., & Burden, R. L. (1999). Stu-
improvement. Foreign Language Annals, 40, dents developing conceptions of themselves
569–583. as language learners. Modern Language Jour-
Rolin-Ianziti, J., & Varshney, R. (2008). Stu- nal, 83, 193–201.
dents’ views regarding the use of the first Wong, K. F., & Xiao, Y. (2010). Diversity and
language: An exploratory study in a tertiary difference: Identity issues of Chinese heritage
context maximizing target language use. language learners from dialect backgrounds.
Canadian Modern Language Review, 65, 249– Heritage Language Journal, 7, 153–186.
273.
Worth, R. (2008). Before and after 101:
Schmidt, R., & Watanabe, Y. (2001). Motiva- Change in learners’ attitudes in the Italian
tion, strategy use, and pedagogical preferences as a foreign language classroom. In H. J.
in foreign language learning. In Z. Dörnyei & Siskin (Ed.), From thought to action: Exploring
R. Schmidt (Eds.), Motivation and second lan- beliefs and outcomes in the foreign language pro-
guage acquisition (Vol. 23, pp. 313–359). Hon- gram (pp. 155–185). Boston: Heinle.
olulu: University of Hawai’i, Second Language
Teaching and Curriculum Center. Yager, K. (1998). Learning Spanish in Mexico:
The effect of informal contact and student
Shaaban, K. A., & Ghaith, G. (2000). Student attitudes on language gain. Hispania, 81, 898–
motivation to learn English as a Foreign Lan- 913.
guage. Foreign Language Annals, 33, 632–644.
Yan, J. X., & Horwitz, E. K. (2008). Learn-
Skehan, P. (1989). Individual differences in ers’ perceptions of how anxiety interacts with
second-language learning. New York: Edward personal and instructional factors to influence
Arnold. their achievement in English: A qualitative
FOREIGN LANGUAGE ANNALS · VOL. 45, NO. S1 S117

analysis of EFL learners in China. Language Yashima, T., Zenuk-Nishide, L., & Shimizu, K.
Learning, 58, 151–183. (2004). The influence of attitudes and affect
on willingness to communicate and second
Yang, J. S. R. (2003). Motivational orienta-
language communication. Language Learning,
tions and selected learner variables of East
54, 119–152.
Asian language learners in the United States.
Foreign Language Annals, 36, 44–56. Yoshida, R. (2007). Perceptions of a learner’s
self-expressive speech by an instructor and the
Yashima, T. (2009). International posture and
learner. Foreign Language Annals, 40, 622–634.
the ideal L2 self in the Japanese EFL context.
In Z. Dörnyei & E. Ushioda (Eds.), Motiva- Yoshida, R. (2008). Learners’ perceptions of
tion, language identity and the L2 self (pp. 144– corrective feedback in pair work. Foreign Lan-
163). Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters. guage Annals, 41, 525–541.
Copyright of Foreign Language Annals is the property of Wiley-Blackwell and its content may not be copied or
emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission.
However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.

You might also like