You are on page 1of 12

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/44888174

Specificity of cognitive emotion regulation strategies: A transdiagnostic


examination

Article  in  Behaviour Research and Therapy · October 2010


DOI: 10.1016/j.brat.2010.06.002 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

432 5,521

2 authors, including:

Amelia Aldao
The Ohio State University
73 PUBLICATIONS   7,107 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Clinical Assessment of Physiological Processes (Comprehensive Assessment and Intervention Program) View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Amelia Aldao on 22 April 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached
copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research
and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution
and sharing with colleagues.
Other uses, including reproduction and distribution, or selling or
licensing copies, or posting to personal, institutional or third party
websites are prohibited.
In most cases authors are permitted to post their version of the
article (e.g. in Word or Tex form) to their personal website or
institutional repository. Authors requiring further information
regarding Elsevier’s archiving and manuscript policies are
encouraged to visit:
http://www.elsevier.com/copyright
Author's personal copy

Behaviour Research and Therapy 48 (2010) 974e983

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Behaviour Research and Therapy


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/brat

Specificity of cognitive emotion regulation strategies: A


transdiagnostic examination
Amelia Aldao*, Susan Nolen-Hoeksema 1
Department of Psychology, Yale University, 2 Hillhouse Ave, New Haven, CT 06520, USA

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Despite growing interest in the role of regulatory processes in clinical disorders, it is not clear whether
Received 8 April 2010 certain cognitive emotion regulation strategies play a more central role in psychopathology than others.
Received in revised form Similarly, little is known about whether these strategies have effects transdiagnostically. We examined
27 May 2010
the relationship between four cognitive emotion regulation strategies (rumination, thought suppression,
Accepted 4 June 2010
reappraisal, and problem-solving) and symptoms of three psychopathologies (depression, anxiety, and
eating disorders) in an undergraduate sample (N ¼ 252). Maladaptive strategies (rumination, suppres-
Keywords:
sion), compared to adaptive strategies (reappraisal, problem-solving), were more strongly associated
Emotion regulation
Strategies
with psychopathology and loaded more highly on a latent factor of cognitive emotion regulation. In
Transdiagnostic addition, this latent factor of cognitive emotion regulation was significantly associated with symptoms of
Eating all three disorders. Overall, these results suggest that the use of maladaptive strategies might play a more
Depression central role in psychopathology than the non-use of adaptive strategies and provide support of a trans-
Anxiety diagnostic view of cognitive emotion regulation.
Ó 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Introduction maladaptive). Stress and coping theories (Billings & Moos, 1981;
Carver, Scheier, & Weintraub, 1989; Folkman & Lazarus, 1986) and
Cognitive emotion regulation strategies are cognitive responses early cognitive-behavioral approaches to psychopathology (Beck,
to emotion-eliciting events that consciously or unconsciously 1976; Cooper, Russell, Skinner, Frone, & Mudar, 1982; D’Zurilla,
attempt to modify the magnitude and/or type of individuals’ 1988; Marlatt, Baer, Donovan, & Kivlahan, 1988) suggested that
emotional experience or the event itself (Campbell-Sills & Barlow, reappraisal and problem-solving should be adaptive across
2007; Harvey, Watkins, Mansell, & Shafran, 2004; Rottenberg & a variety of contexts. Reappraisal involves generating benign or
Gross, 2007; Thompson, 1994; Williams & Bargh, 2007). In recent positive interpretations of a stressful situation as a way of reducing
years, a substantial amount of work has been devoted to delin- distress (Gross, 1998). Cognitive theories put maladaptive appraisal
eating the relationships between dispositions to use certain strat- processes at the core of depression and anxiety (Beck, 1976; Clark,
egies and a variety of disorders, including depression (Garnefski & 1988; Salkovskis, 1998). More recently, Gross’s (1998) influential
Kraaij, 2006; Nolen-Hoeksema, Wisco, & Lyubomirsky, 2008), model of emotion regulation highlights reappraisal as a strategy
mania (Feldman, Joormann, & Johnson, 2008), generalized anxiety that results in positive emotional and physical responses to
disorder (Mennin, Holoway, Fresco, Moore, & Heimberg, 2007), emotion-eliciting stimuli. Cognitive-behavioral therapies for
post-traumatic stress disorder (Tull & Roemer, 2003), social anxiety depression and anxiety focus on teaching reappraisal skills (Beck,
disorder (Kashdan & Breen, 2008), and eating disorders (Nolen- Rush, Shaw, & Emery, 1979; Clark & Wells, 1995).
Hoeksema, Stice, Wade, & Bohon, 2007; Piran & Cormier, 2005). Problem-solving responses are conscious attempts to change
Overall, several cognitive emotion regulation strategies have a stressful situation or contain its consequences (Billings & Moos,
been argued to have negative associations with psychopathology 1981). Problem-solving measures can include cognitions directed
(i.e., adaptive) whereas others have been associated with the at solving a problem (e.g., brainstorming solutions, planning
etiology and maintenance of clinical disorders (i.e., be a course of action) or an orientation toward problem-solving as
a way of coping with stressful circumstances. Problem-solving
coping can have beneficial effects on emotions by modifying or
eliminating stressors. Low problem-solving orientation or poor
* Corresponding author. Tel.: þ1 (203) 432 7352.
E-mail addresses: amelia.aldao@yale.edu (A. Aldao), susan.nolen-hoeksema@
problem-solving skills have been associated with depression
yale.edu (S. Nolen-Hoeksema). (Billings & Moos, 1981; D’Zurilla, Chang, Nottingham, & Faccinni,
1
Tel.: þ1 (203) 432 0699. 1988), anxiety (Chang, Downey, & Salata, 2004; Kant, D’Zurilla, &

0005-7967/$ e see front matter Ó 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.brat.2010.06.002
Author's personal copy

A. Aldao, S. Nolen-Hoeksema / Behaviour Research and Therapy 48 (2010) 974e983 975

Maydeu-Olivares, 1997), and eating disorders (Van Boven & four cognitive emotion regulation strategies and symptoms of four
Espelage, 2006). Training in problem-solving skills is a compo- types of psychopathology: depression, anxiety, eating disorders,
nent of cognitive-behavioral therapies for all these disorders (Beck and substance use.2 When we collapsed all the symptom types
et al., 1979; Fairburn, Shafran, & Cooper, 1998; Marlatt et al., 1988). together, we found mixed evidence for specificity in the relation-
In contrast to these adaptive strategies, suppression of dis- ship between emotion regulation strategies and psychopathology:
tressing thoughts has long been seen as a maladaptive response to although we found that all four strategies were significantly asso-
a variety of stressors and a risk factor for psychopathology (Carver ciated with psychopathology, the maladaptive strategies of rumi-
et al., 1989; Folkman & Lazarus, 1980). Wenzlaff and Wegner (2000) nation and suppression were more strongly associated with
have produced a large body of research showing that attempts to symptoms than the adaptive strategies of reappraisal and problem-
voluntarily suppress unwanted thoughts result in an increased solving. Delineating which strategies have stronger associations
accessibility of the suppressed thought (Wegner & Erber, 1992; with psychopathology can help us identify the ones that play
Wegner, Schneider, Carter, & White, 1987). More germane to a more central role in the development, maintenance, and remis-
emotion regulation, suppression of emotional thoughts has been sion of various disorders. This has implications for prevention
shown to produce increases in sympathetic activation (Wegner, programs that focus on the development of cognitive emotion
Broome, & Blumberg, 1997; Wegner & Gold, 1995), and self-repor- regulation skills (e.g., Brackett & Katulak, 2006) as well as treat-
ted anxiety (Roemer & Borkovec, 1994), anxiety and depression ments that focus on teaching regulatory skills (Beck, 1976; Fairburn
(Borton, Markowitz, & Dieterich, 2005), and discomfort (Purdon & et al., 1998; Fairholme, Boisseau, Ellard, Ehrenreich, & Barlow, 2010;
Clark, 2001). Wegner et al. have also suggested that chronic Hayes, Strosahl, & Wilson, 1999; Roemer, Orsillo, & Salters-
suppression of emotionally evocative thoughts might prevent Pedneault, 2008). Specifically, if we can identify what strategies
habituation to emotional stimuli, and as such result in hypersen- are more protective against, or stronger risk factors for, psychopa-
sitivity to depression and anxiety-related thoughts and symptoms thology, we can ensure that these strategies are targeted in
(Wegner & Zanakos, 1994; Wenzlaff & Wegner, 2000). Indeed, prevention and intervention programs.
thought suppression has been associated with increased risk for Another important question regarding the relationship between
depression and anxiety in several studies (Purdon, 1999; Wenzlaff cognitive regulatory strategies and psychopathology involves
& Wegner, 2000). Similarly, emotion regulation models of eating whether these strategies are more strongly related to certain
disorders suggest that suppression of concerns leads to binge disorders than to others. Leading theorists have argued that diffi-
eating and then maladaptive compensatory behaviors (e.g., culties using cognitive emotion regulation strategies, including in
Heatherton & Baumeister, 1991; McCarthy, 1990; Polivy & Herman, rumination, thought suppression, reappraisal, and problem-
2002). In addition, thought suppression has been associated with solving, may be critical transdiagnostic factors underlying several
the frequency with which individuals with borderline personality forms of psychopathology (Ehring & Watkins, 2008; Fairburn,
disorders engage in self-harm as an emotion regulation mechanism Cooper, & Shafran, 2003; Gross & John, 2003; Harvey et al., 2004;
(Chapman, Specht, & Cellucci, 2005). Kring & Sloan, 2010; Mansell, Harvey, Watkins, & Shafran, 2009;
Another maladaptive strategy is rumination, the tendency to Moses & Barlow, 2006; Purdon, 1999; Rassin, Merckelbach, &
repetitively focus on the experience of negative emotion and its Muris, 2000). Unfortunately, most studies on cognitive emotion
causes and consequences (Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 2008; Trapnell & regulation have been disorder-specific, limiting tests of trans-
Campbell, 1999; Watkins, 2008). Although individuals report that diagnostic models. Identifying cognitive emotion regulation strat-
they engage in rumination to understand the sources of their egies that have transdiagnostic effects can inform the development
distress (Papageorgiou & Wells, 2003), experimental studies have of interventions targeting these strategies, thereby having
shown that rumination increases negative mood-congruent preventative and treatment effects across a range of disorders.
thinking, interferes with problem-solving and instrumental In our meta analysis, we found that the relationships between
behavior, and drives away social support (for a review see emotion regulation strategies were stronger for depression and
Lyubomirsky & Tkach, 2004). In turn, rumination prospectively anxiety than for eating and substance use disorders. These results
predicts symptoms and diagnoses of anxiety and depression (see suggest that, not surprisingly, mood and anxiety disorders might be
Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 2008). Some people who ruminate may more closely related to certain problems in cognitive emotion
turn to binge eating to escape their aversive self-awareness regulation than disorders in which mood disturbances are not as
(Heatherton & Baumeister, 1991), leading to symptoms and diag- central (see Garnefski, Kraaij, & van Etten, 2005) and that eating
noses of eating disorders (Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 2007). disorders are more consistently related to cognitive emotion
Thus, two cognitive emotion regulation strategies that have regulation strategies than substance use.
been widely theorized to be protective against psychopathology are Our meta analysis provided important clues as to the relative
reappraisal and problem-solving. Two strategies that have consis- strength of different cognitive emotion regulation strategies in
tently been argued to be associated with development and main- predicting psychopathology, and which types of psychopathology
tenance of psychopathology are thought suppression and these strategies were most related to. There are significant limita-
rumination. However, it is not clear whether strategies vary in the tions of the meta-analytic approach, however. Given the nature of
strength of their association to psychopathology. a meta-analytic review, we could only examine the relationships
In a recent meta analysis (Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema, & Schweizer, between individual strategies and disorders independently of one
2010), we examined the relationships between self-reports of these another, and thus could not simultaneously model the relationships
among all strategies and disorders (see Rosenthal & DiMatteo,
2001). This is problematic for several reasons. First, we could not
2
We actually examined two additional emotion regulation strategies in the meta test whether the any lack of specificity found in the relationship
analysis, namely avoidance and acceptance. Avoidance had a very similar pattern of between strategies and psychopathology could be attributed to
relationships to psychopathology as suppression, and indeed there appears to be shared variance among the strategies. In other words, we could not
considerable conceptual and measurement overlap between the two constructs test whether each strategy would still show significant associations
(Ottenbreit & Dobson, 2004). Thus, we did not include measures of avoidance in
this study. Acceptance was not significantly associated with psychopathology across
with psychopathology when examined in models that included
studies in the meta analysis; measures of this construct were not included in this other strategies. Additionally, we could not examine whether the
study. strategies would intercorrelate and potentially load onto latent
Author's personal copy

976 A. Aldao, S. Nolen-Hoeksema / Behaviour Research and Therapy 48 (2010) 974e983

factor of cognitive emotion regulation characterized by frequent 55.2% as Caucasian, 7.9% as Hispanic, .8% as Native American, and
use of maladaptive strategies and infrequent use of adaptive 8.3% as mixed or other.
strategies.
Second, the nature of univariate effect sizes did not allow us to
examine whether the relationships between regulatory strategies Materials
and specific disorders were reflecting transdiagnostic processes or
the shared variance resulting from symptom overlap among Participants completed the following measures
disorders. In particular, we could not test whether the relationship Emotion regulation measures. The Problem-Solving subscale of the
between eating disorders and regulatory strategies would be COPE (Carver et al., 1989) is a 16-item measure of the tendency to
maintained when accounting for the roles of depression or anxiety. take a problem-solving approach to stressful situations. It has four
That is, eating disorders might be related to regulatory strategies subscales: Active Coping, Planning, Suppression of Competing
because of their associations with distress (e.g., Macht, Haput, & Activities, and Restraint Coping. Items are scored on a 4-point scale
Ellgring, 2005; Polivy & Herman, 2002). Similarly, we could not with higher scores indicating greater use of problem-solving to
address the high comorbidity seen in depression and anxiety cope. The four subscales have good internal consistency in the
(Watson, 2009). In this respect, a better understanding of the literature (Carver et al., 1989; a’s .62e.80) and in this study (a’s
phenomenology of processes uniquely associated with each of .75e.91). We initially conducted analyses using the four separate
these disorders requires the isolation of the shared variance scales; the results across the scales were very similar to the results
between them. Although the interpretation of the residual variance using the total scale comprised of the average score across the four
that results when a covariate has been included in a multivariate scales. Thus, for parsimony, we report here the analyses using the
model is not without problems (see Miller & Chapman, 2001), it is total problem-solving score (a ¼ .90).
still a parsimonious and straightforward way to identify which The Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ; Gross & John, 2003)
variables are more strongly associated with anxiety and which with is a 10-item measure that assesses individual differences in the
depression (e.g., Aldao, Mennin, Linardatos, & Fresco, 2010; dispositional use of two emotion regulation strategies: cognitive
Joormann & Stoeber, 1999; Mennin, Heimberg, Turk, & Fresco, reappraisal and expressive suppression. Items are scored on a 7-
2005). point scale with higher scores indicating more use of a strategy. The
In the present investigation, we sought to address these limi- internal consistency for the Reappraisal subscale in this sample was
tations by examining the relationships among reappraisal, good (a ¼ .82).
problem-solving, thought suppression, and rumination, and their We chose not to use the expressive suppression subscale and
relationships to symptoms of depression, anxiety, and eating instead to use a measure of thought suppression (see below)
disorders in one large sample of young adults.3 Consistent with our because Gross and John (2003) have argued that expressive
previous results (Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema, et al., 2010), we tested suppression is a “form of response modulation that involves
the hypothesis that all the cognitive emotion regulation strategies inhibiting ongoing emotion-expressive behavior.that primarily
would show high intercorrelations and therefore fall on a latent modifies the behavioral aspect of the emotion response tenden-
factor of cognitive emotion regulation. Additionally, in line with the cies. and will not be helpful in reducing the experience of negative
results of our meta analysis, we predicted that the maladaptive emotion, which is not directly targeted by suppression.” (Gross &
strategies would show stronger associations with the latent factor John, 2003, p. 349). Indeed, in a line of experimental work, Gross
as well as with psychopathology. Lastly, we examined whether and colleagues have shown that expressive suppression does not
each of the three symptom types would continue to be associated modify the subjective experience of emotion (Gross, 1998, Gross &
with cognitive emotion regulation when interrelationships among John, 2003; Gross & Levenson, 1993; Richards & Gross, 2000) and
symptom types was taken into account. that its effects consist of cardiac rebounds (i.e., increased sympa-
thetic activation; Gross, 1998; Gross & John, 2003; Gross &
Levenson, 1993) and cognitive difficulties (i.e., memory problems;
Richards & Gross, 2000).4
Methods The Ruminative Response Scale (RRS; Treynor, Gonzalez, & Nolen-
Hoeksema, 2003) is a 22-item measure that assesses an individual’s
Participants tendency to engage in repetitive, ruminative behavior in response
to distress. Items are score on a 4-point scale, with higher scores
Participants were 252 undergraduate students at a private indicating more use of rumination. Treynor et al. (2003) have
university in the northeastern United States who completed removed those items with a strong content overlap with depression
a battery of self-report questionnaires administered online using and factor analyzed the remaining items to produce two subscales.
Survey Monkey. All of them received a $5 compensation (in the
form of an Amazon.com gift certificate) and they were also entered
4
into a lottery to win an iPod Touch (awarded to 1 in 30 partici- We calculated correlations between ERQ Suppression and the regulatory
strategies. It did not correlate with ERQ Reappraisal (r ¼ .01, p ¼ .91), which is in line
pants). They provided online consent according to the guidelines
with the CFA conducted by Gross and John (2003) in their initial validation of the
specified by our Human Subjects Committee. The mean age of the ERQ and with subsequent investigations examining both subscales (e.g., Fresco
sample was 18.44 (SD ¼ .66) and a little over half the sample et al., 2007; Magar, Phillips, & Hosie, 2008). In addition, it did not correlate with
(55.6%) identified as female. In terms of ethnic background, 5.6% Problem-Solving Total (r ¼ .06, p ¼ .32), RS Brooding (r ¼ .09, p ¼ .17), or RS
Pondering (r ¼ 06, p ¼ .35). However, it did correlate mildly with WBSI (r ¼ .13,
identified as African American, 22.2% as Asian/Asian American,
p < .05). This pattern of correlations suggests that ERQ suppression does not
correlate with strategies aimed at modifying internal experience, with the excep-
tion of thought suppression, which might share a common inhibitory process. In
3
We did not include analyses of substance-related symptoms in the analyses addition ERQ Suppression did not correlate with MASQ Anxious Arousal (r ¼ .06, ¼
here because our measures only assessed use (rather than abuse) and in this under- p ¼ .38) or EDE total (r ¼ .01, p ¼ .89) and it correlated positively with MASQ
age sample the rate of heavy drinking was low. In addition, our meta analysis of Anhedonic Depression (r ¼ .21, p < .01), thus suggesting a limited role in psycho-
previous studies (Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema, et al., 2010) showed that substance pathology symptoms in this normative sample, consistent with the arguments of
abuse was more weakly associated with emotion regulation than any of the other Gross and John (2003) that expressive suppression will not have a strong rela-
psychopathologies. tionship with the subjective experience of emotion.
Author's personal copy

A. Aldao, S. Nolen-Hoeksema / Behaviour Research and Therapy 48 (2010) 974e983 977

The Pondering subscale reflects attempts to analyze problems strategies were correlated with one another; 2) adaptive strategies
leading to depression (e.g., “analyze recent events to try to under- (i.e., reappraisal and problem-solving) showed the expected
stand why you are depressed;” “write down what you are thinking negative correlations with psychopathology, and maladaptive
about and analyze it”). The Brooding subscale reflects the moody strategies (i.e., brooding, pondering, and suppression) showed the
rumination at the core of Nolen-Hoeksema’s (1991) rumination expected positive correlations; 3) consistent with our previous
theory (e.g., think, “What am I doing to deserve this?” “think about meta analysis, the adaptive strategies showed weaker relationships
a recent situation, wishing it had gone better.”). The authors with psychopathology symptoms than the maladaptive strategies;
reported adequate reliability (a ¼ .77 and a ¼ .72 for Brooding and 4) surprisingly, problem-solving showed no association with eating
Pondering, respectively). In our sample the reliability was good symptoms. In order to explore this pattern of results at a multivar-
(a ¼ .81 and a ¼ .83 for Brooding and Pondering, respectively). iate level, we analyzed the relationships between cognitive
The White Bear Suppression Inventory (WBSI; Wegner & Zanakos, emotion regulation strategies and psychopathology using struc-
1994) is a 15-item measure designed to assess whether individuals tural equation modeling (SEM; Arbuckle, 2007).
have a tendency to suppress thoughts and experience thought
intrusion. Items are scored on a 5-point scale, with higher scores Structural equation modeling
indicating more thought suppression. The internal reliability for
this measure in previous studies has been good (a’s ranging from We ran SEM models using Maximum Likelihood estimation.
.87 to .89; Wegner & Zanakos, 1994). In the current sample, reli- Although SEM parameters estimated with this method tend to be
ability was also good (a ¼ .94). We utilized this measure (instead of robust to deviations from multivariate normality, standard errors
the ERQ Suppression) because of its focus on the regulation of and fit indices might still be biased (Bollen, 1989; West, Finch, &
internal experience. Curran, 1995). Therefore, we ran our models utilizing boot-
strapping, a procedure involving the drawing of multiple random
Psychopathology symptom measures. The Mood and Anxiety samples by sampling from the original sample with replacement
Symptom Questionnaire e Short Form (MASQ-SF; Watson & Clark, that results in more stable parameters, particularly with small
1991) is a 62-item measure that assesses symptoms that samples and samples where multivariate normality may not old
commonly occur in anxiety and mood disorders. Items are rated on (Byrne, 2010; Nevitt & Hancock, 2001; Preacher & Hayes, 2008). We
a 5-point scale, with higher scores indicating higher levels of obtained 1000 random samples.
anxiety and depression. The short form version of the questionnaire We evaluated model fit by examining several fit indices that are
consist of four subscales: General Distress: Anxious Symptoms based on different information, thus producing a comprehensive
(GDA) subscale, which consists of 11 items that reflect non-specific evaluation of our models. We examined: 1) chi square, which
symptoms of anxiety (e.g., “was unable to relax”), General Distress: should be non-significant (Hu & Bentler, 1998), but is overly sensi-
Depressive Symptoms (GDD) subscale, which consists of 12 items tive to sample size, thus resulting in a tendency to reject the null
that reflect non-specific depression symptoms (e.g., “felt sluggish hypothesis (Ulman, 2007); 2) chi square/df, which adjusts for
or tired”), Anxious Arousal (AA) subscale, which consists of 17 items sample size and should be lower than 3 or 2 (e.g., Mennin et al.,
that assess anxiety-specific symptoms (e.g., “startled easily”), and 2007; Ulman, 2007); 3) Tucker Lewis Index (TLI; Tucker & Lewis,
Anhedonic Depression (AD) subscale, which consists of 22 items 1973), which should be close to .95 (Hu & Bentler, 1999); 4)
that assess symptoms specific to depression (e.g., “felt withdrawn Comparative Fix Index (CFI; Bentler, 1990), which should be close
from other people”). In the present investigation, we only utilized to .95 (Hu & Bentler, 1999); 5) Root Mean Square Error of Approxi-
the AA and AD subscales, since they have lower overlap and are mation (RMSEA), which should have values close to .06 (Hu &
therefore likely to produce stronger empirical differentiation Bentler, 1999), with values between around .08 representing
between the highly overlapping constructs of anxiety and depres- reasonable errors of approximation (Browne & Cudeck, 1993); and
sion (in this sample, for AA and AD, r ¼ .29, p < .01, whereas for GDA 6) Hoelter’s Critical N (Hoelter, 1983), which measures the adequacy
and GDA, r ¼ .70, p < .01;). Watson et al. (1995) reported high levels of sample size, and should be higher than 200 (Hoelter, 1983).
of internal consistency in multiple samples. In our sample, the a’s
for AA and AD were .91 and .93, respectively. Measurement model
The Eating Disorders Examination-Questionnaire (Fairburn &
Beglin, 1995) is adapted from the Eating Disorder Examination We first tested a structural model with the cognitive emotion
(EDE; Fairburn & Cooper, 1993) and it assesses a broad range of regulation strategies loading onto a single latent factor of cognitive
symptoms of eating disorders. It contains probe items (i.e., yes/no emotion regulation. We did not correlate the variances between the
format) and items on a 7-point scale assessing behavioral features two measures coming from a same measure (i.e., RSQ Brooding and
of eating disorders, with higher scores indicating more psychopa- Pondering; Brown, 2006) because this resulted in Heywood cases
thology. The dimensional scores are utilized to calculate the EDE-Q (i.e., estimates of negative variance that prevent the model from
subscales: Restraint, Shape Concern, Weight Concern, and Eating running properly; Brown, 2006; Byrne, 2010). Surprisingly,
Concern. These subscales have shown good internal reliability (a’s problem-solving did not load onto the latent factor of cognitive
ranging for the subscales ranging from .78 to .93; Luce & Crowther, emotion regulation (p ¼ .73) and the overall fit of the model was
1999). We utilized the total score of the EDE-Q (e.g., Roefs & Jansen, poor, as evidenced by a large, significant chi square [c2 (5,
2002) in order to capture overall eating pathology. This score had N ¼ 252) ¼ 40.14, p < .01] and poor fit indices (chi square/df ¼ 8.03;
very good reliability in our sample (a ¼ 95). TLI ¼ .67; CFI ¼ .84; RMSEA ¼ .17; Hoelter’s Critical N ¼ 95).
Removal of problem-solving in a subsequent model resulted in
Results a good fit, as evidenced by both a non-significant chi square
(c2 ¼ [2, N ¼ 252] ¼ 5.91, p ¼ .05) and appropriate values in the
Univariate associations additional fit indices (chi square/df ¼ 2.96; TLI ¼ .94; CFI ¼ .98;
RMSEA ¼ .09; Hoelter’s Critical N ¼ 392). As Fig. 1 and Table 2
We first examined the univariate relationships among the indicate, brooding, pondering, suppression and reappraisal loaded
measures of cognitive emotion regulation strategies and symptoms significantly on the latent factor of cognitive emotion regulation.
of psychopathology (see Table 1). The results suggest that: 1) not all Three strategies (i.e., brooding, pondering, and suppression) had
Author's personal copy

978 A. Aldao, S. Nolen-Hoeksema / Behaviour Research and Therapy 48 (2010) 974e983

Table 1
Bivariate correlations between measures of emotion regulation and psychopathology.

2 3 4 5 6 7 8 Mean (SD) Range


ERQ Reappraisal .27** .18** .01 .05 .32** .20** .15* 4.5 (1.05) 1e7
PS total .02 .18** .10 .25** .17** 0 2.62 (.53) 1e4
RSQ Brooding .54** .55** .51** .37** .36** 2.14 (.73) 1e4
RSQ Pondering .39** .24** .16* .29** 2.21 (.79) 1e4
WBSI .36** .33** .25** 2.84 (.95) 1e5
MASQ Anhedonic Depression .29** .37** 2.72 (.65) 1e3.47
MASQ Anxious Arousal .27** 1.66 (.59) 1.10e4.67
EDE total 1.34 (1.19) 0e5.48

Note: ERQ: Emotion Regulation Questionnaire; PS: problem-solving; RSQ: Response Styles Questionnaire; WBSI: White Bear Suppression Inventory; MASQ: Mood and Anxiety
Symptoms Questionnaire; EDE: Eating Disorder Examination. *p < .05; **p < .01.

positive loadings, whereas reappraisal had a negative loading that to .37; see Table 3, Fig. 2). In addition, all three symptom measures
was very low in magnitude, yet significant. Removal of this indi- had significant regression weights onto the latent factor of cogni-
cator or creation of a second latent factor with the indicators of tive emotion regulation, suggesting that even when the overlap
reappraisal and problem-solving both resulted in Heywood cases between symptoms is taken into account, they are all correlated
and models that could not be run. Thus, we proceeded with the with cognitive emotion dysregulation (yet the coefficients were
latent factor that included brooding, pondering, suppression, and lager for depression symptoms, followed by anxiety symptoms and
reappraisal. We return to the statistical and conceptual implications then by eating symptoms).
of this issue in the discussion section.
Discussion
Structural model
A plethora of cognitive emotion regulation strategies has been
We examined a structural model, in which we evaluated the conceptualized and operationalized in recent decades (e.g., Billings
relationship between the latent factor of cognitive emotion regu- & Moos, 1981; Carver et al., 1989; Folkman & Lazarus, 1986;
lation and symptom measures (i.e., MASQ AD, MASQ AA, and EDE- Garnefski & Kraaij, 2006; Gross, 1998; Nolen-Hoeksema et al.,
Q). Given the high overlap between symptoms of measures of 2008; Wenzlaff & Wegner, 2000). These various strategies have
psychopathology (Watson, 2009), we allowed the MASQ AD, MASQ generally been studied for their individual contributions to
AA, and EDE-Q to correlate with each other. psychopathology; however, the studies that have compared several
The first version of the model produced a fair fit, with a signifi- different strategies in the same sample have found that they tend to
cant chi square [X (11, N ¼ 252) ¼ 41.22, p < .01] and additional fit intercorrelate (e.g., Garnefski, Kraaij, & Spinhoven, 2001; Hong,
indices slightly outside of the ranges for good fit (chi square/ 2007; Wenzlaff & Luxton, 2003), thus suggesting they might have
df ¼ 3.75, TLI ¼ .85, CFI ¼ .92, RMSEA ¼ .11, Hoelter’s Critical low specificity.
N ¼ 151). As a result, we examined the modification indices. In this study, we examined the relationships among four
Among the suggestions, one was the addition of a covariance path commonly studied cognitive emotion regulation strategies (i.e.,
between the error variance or ERQ reappraisal and that of the reappraisal, problem-solving, rumination, and suppression) and
latent factor of cognitive emotion regulation (M.I. ¼ 10.02, parameter found that three of them (reappraisal, suppression, and rumination,
change ¼ .08). Since this change would not result in the modifi- with its subcomponents of brooding and pondering) loaded on
cation of the conceptual model, we decided to add this path in a single latent factor of cognitive emotion regulation. These results
a second model. overall support a model of low specificity of regulatory strategies,
The second structural model produced good fit. The chi square with one exception: adaptive strategies appear to play a smaller
was still significant (10, N ¼ 252) ¼ 19.84, p < .05, but the rest of the role in cognitive emotion regulation than maladaptive strategies.
indices were now within the ranges considered to be indicative of Reappraisal loaded on the cognitive emotion regulation latent
good fit (chi square/df ¼ 1.98; TLI ¼ .95; CFI ¼ .97; RMSEA .06, factor, yet its loading was negative and extremely low. Its removal
Holter’s Critical N ¼ 294). In this model, as predicted, the symptom from the model substantially deteriorated it, generating Heywood
measures were significantly correlated with each other (r’s from .27 cases and preventing it from running. Similarly, alternative models
in which reappraisal and problem-solving loaded onto a different
latent factor of adaptive cognitive emotion regulation also resulted
in Heywood cases and extremely poor fit. This suggests that reap-
praisal might belong in this latent factor of cognitive emotion
regulation, yet its role in the process might be weaker. For example,
most individuals who tend to use maladaptive strategies such as
rumination and thought suppression may try to use reappraisal to
improve their emotional responses to stress, but their maladaptive
tendencies may be stronger than their ability to use reappraisal
successfully.
Although problem-solving was correlated with reappraisal and
the pondering subcomponent of rumination, it did not load onto
the latent cognitive emotion regulation factor in the structural
model. This suggests that problem-solving might be fundamentally
different from other regulatory strategies, as it is an attempt to
change a situation rather than an emotion. The adaptiveness of
Fig. 1. Measurement model: standardized coefficients. problem-solving may also depend even more on the context than
Author's personal copy

A. Aldao, S. Nolen-Hoeksema / Behaviour Research and Therapy 48 (2010) 974e983 979

Table 2
Measurement model coefficients.

Path Parameter Standard Standardized p-Value Bootstrap mean Bootstrap SE


estimate error estimate estimate bias estimate bias
Cognitive emot reg / ERQ Reappraisal .37 .15 .17 <.05 .001 .003
Cognitive emot reg / RSQ Brooding 1.41 .20 .90 <.01 .009 .002
Cognitive emot reg / RSQ Pondering 1.00 e .60 e .001 .002
Cognitive emot reg / WBSI total 1.22 .16 .61 <.01 .003 .002

Note: ERQ: Emotion Regulation Questionnaire; RSQ: Response Styles Questionnaire; WBSI: White Bear Suppression Inventory. There is p-value for RSQ Pondering, since its
loading was fixed to 1.

for other strategies. For example, problem-solving may not be an contextual adaptiveness of strategies would benefit from the use of
adaptive strategy when facing an uncontrollable situation in which experience sampling designs (e.g., Ebner-Priemer et al., 2007;
there is no problem to solve (e.g., Cheng, Hui, & Lam, 1999; Folkman Feldman-Barrett & Barett, 2001), the manipulation of several
& Lazarus, 1986). contextual factors within one experimental session, and the use of
Moreover, the bivariate correlations showed that problem- functional analyses as a framework (e.g., Ferster, 1973; Kohlenberg
solving was significantly correlated with depression and anxiety & Tsai, 1991; Linehan, 1993).
symptoms but not with eating symptoms (see Table 1). This finding Another possibility is that, once individuals start using malad-
was at odds with the results from our meta analysis, which sug- aptive strategies frequently, these quickly become their default way
gested a small-to-medium effect size between problem-solving and of managing affective states. As a result, they start using the
eating symptoms. A few explanations come to mind. First, the meta adaptive strategies more haphazardly, which would result in more
analysis results were calculated using only two effect sizes (one noise and therefore weaker correlations with psychopathology
from a clinical population and one from a normative population) so symptoms. This notion is consistent with models of emotion
this might have resulted in a spurious finding. Second, problem- regulation that stress flexibility in the use of strategies as a sign of
solving (and perhaps adaptive strategies at large) might have adaptive regulation (e.g., Bonanno, Papa, O’Neill, Westphal, &
a different relationship with psychopathology symptoms. Coifman, 2004; Coiffman & Bonanno, 2010; Cole, Martin, &
The notion that adaptive strategies might play a smaller role in Dennis, 2004; Gratz & Roemer, 2004). Future work on this respect
the cognitive emotion regulation process is consistent with the should focus on quantifying the ability to flexibly switch strategies
results from our meta analysis, in which we found that suppression, as the contingencies in the environment change.
rumination, and avoidance were more strongly associated with The latent cognitive emotion regulation factor did include both
psychopathology then problem-solving, reappraisal, and accep- the brooding and pondering subcomponents of rumination. Many
tance (Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema, et al., 2010). Adaptive strategies studies have confirmed that the brooding subcomponent is associ-
might have lower associations with psychopathology symptoms at ated with a variety of psychopathological symptoms (see reviews by
the dispositional level because their adaptiveness might be more Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema, et al., 2010; Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 2008).
context-dependent than the maladaptiveness of maladaptive The relationships between the pondering subcomponent and
strategies. For example, reappraisal might only be adaptive when psychopathology have been more mixed (see review by Nolen-
the situation can actually be reframed, whereas rumination is Hoeksema et al., 2008). The items on this subcomponent seem to
maladaptive most of the time (Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 2008). reflect attempts to understand and solve one’s problems (e.g.,
Indeed, recent work suggests that when reappraisal is deployed “analyze recent events to try to understand why you are depressed;”
later in the emotion regulation process, it results maladaptive “write down what you are thinking about and analyze it”). In this
outcomes, such as increased sympathetic activation (Sheppes, study, the pondering subcomponent was positively correlated with
Catran, & Meiran, 2009) and diminished self-control (Sheppes & problem-solving, but it was also positively correlated with brooding,
Meiran, 2008). In this respect, the temporal dynamics of the suppression, and all the symptom types (for similar patterns in other
deployment of emotion regulation strategies might be critical to studies, see review by Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 2008). Thus, although
the adaptiveness of strategies (Davidson, 1998; Thompson, 1994). In people may engage in pondering to try to understand and solve their
addition, recent work suggests that the relationship between problems, pondering is associated with a pattern of maladaptive
adaptive strategies and symptoms of anxiety and distress might be emotion regulation strategies and psychopathology symptoms.
moderated by gender (Zlomke & Hahn, 2010). Future work on the Some individuals may try to engage in pondering about their

Table 3
Structural model coefficients.

Path Parameter Standard Standardized p-Value Bootstrap mean Bootstrap SE


estimate error estimate estimate bias estimate bias
Regression paths
Cognitive emot reg / ERQ Reappraisal 1.19. .27 .53 <.01 .002 .004
Cognitive emot reg / RSQ Brooding 1.43 .17 .90 <.01 .001 .001
Cognitive emot reg / RSQ Pondering 1.00 e .59 e .001 .002
Cognitive emot reg / WBSI total 1.28 .17 .62 <.01 .000 .002
EDE total / cognitive emot reg .06 .02 .15 <.01 .000 .002
MASQ Anx Arousal / cognitive Emot Reg .19 .05 .24 <.01 .000 .002
MASQ Anhedonic Dep / cognitive emot reg .32 .05 .44 <.01 .002 .002

Correlations
EDE total 4 MASQ Anhedonic Dep. .37 <.01 .002 .002
EDE total 4 MASQ Anx Arousal .27 <.01 .001 .002
MASQ Anhedonic Dep 4 MASQ Anx Arousal .29 <.01 .001 .002

Note: ERQ: Emotion Regulation Questionnaire; RSQ: Response Styles Questionnaire; WBSI: White Bear Suppression Inventory; EDE: Eating Disorder Examination; MASQ:
Mood and Anxiety Symptom Questionnaire. There is no p-value for RSQ Pondering, since its loading was fixed to 1.
Author's personal copy

980 A. Aldao, S. Nolen-Hoeksema / Behaviour Research and Therapy 48 (2010) 974e983

An alternate explanation for the larger coefficients between


cognitive emotion regulation measures and depression and anxiety
symptoms is that eating disorder symptoms might themselves
regulatory mechanism utilized when experiencing high levels of
anxiety and depression (Polivy & Herman, 2002). Evidence sup-
porting this notion comes from findings showing that a history of
negative affect is predictor of future onset or exacerbation of
anorexic and bulimic symptoms (Burton, Stice, Bearman, & Rohde,
2007; Stice, Burton, & Shaw, 2004; Stice, Presnell, & Spangler,
2002). Further, Stice et al. (2002) have found that disordered
eating behaviors in response to negative emotions more strongly
predicted the development of bulimia nervosa in adolescent girls
than did disordered eating behaviors that were in response to
shape and weight concerns. Additionally, Macht et al. (2005) have
found that students reported engaging in eating behaviors to
distract themselves during periods of elevated stress.
Still, the results from this study indicate that eating disorder
symptoms are significantly related to cognitive emotion regulation
strategies, even after accounting for the relationships between
eating disorder symptoms and depressive and anxiety symptoms.
This suggests that cognitive emotion dysregulation has unique
relationships to eating disorder symptoms, even if this relationship
is smaller in magnitude than the one between cognitive emotion
dysregulation and depressive and anxiety symptoms. At the same
Fig. 2. Structural model: standardized coefficients.
time, it does not completely invalidate the notion that eating
disorders might be developed and/or maintain as strategies to
regulate negative affect. Future studies using longitudinal designs
problems but fall into rumination and/or suppression. Interventions should clarify the predictive relationships over time between
for such people may need to help them recognize when they are cognitive emotion regulation strategies, eating disorder symptoms,
moving from pondering into rumination or suppression and then and depressive and anxiety symptoms and directly compare the
prevent this, perhaps through concreteness training (i.e., having role of cognitive strategies across these disorders.
individuals focus on the concrete details of a situation rather than on An important goal for future research is to identify the mecha-
abstract evaluations of the situation). Watkins (2008) has shown nisms by which the latent cognitive emotion regulation factor might
that concreteness training can allow individuals to think about lead to more symptoms of depression, anxiety, and eating disorders.
important self-relevant situations without falling into rumination. Previous work on rumination, thought suppression, and reappraisal
The latent factor of cognitive emotion regulation, composed of provide some clues as to the mechanisms by which each of these
rumination (brooding and pondering), suppression, and reappraisal contributes to psychopathology (Gross & Thompson, 2007; Nolen-
was significantly related to all three types of psychopathology: Hoeksema et al., 2008; Purdon, 1999; Watkins, 2008; Wenzlaff &
depressive symptoms, anxiety symptoms, and eating disorders Wegner, 2000). The confluence of tendencies to both ruminate
symptoms. An initial examination of the standardized coefficients and suppress thoughts, and difficulties in reappraisal, may have
suggests that depressive symptoms had the largest coefficients, deleterious effects on the quality of thought and exacerbation of
followed by anxiety symptoms and then by eating symptoms. This mood that go beyond the effects of any of these regulatory deficits
is in line with the results of our meta analysis showing that the individually, however. For example, people who ruminate and do
individual cognitive emotion regulation strategies had stronger not try to reappraise may experience particularly rapid escalation of
relationships to depression and anxiety than to eating disorders catastrophizing thoughts and distressing emotions; if they are also
(Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema, et al., 2010) and with previous findings prone to suppression, they may turn to food, substances, or self-
suggesting that internalizing symptoms are more strongly associ- destructive behavior to escape from their thoughts and moods. As
ated with emotion regulation than non-internalizing symptoms a result, people with the confluence of regulatory problems indexed
(e.g., Garnefski et al., 2005). Although premature to draw conclu- by our latent cognitive emotion regulation factor may be especially
sions from this preliminary observation, the consistency between emotionally reactive to stressors and prone to escapist behaviors
our previous findings and the trends in this study lead us to spec- that may be self-destructive.
ulate on some explanations for the potential stronger relationships
between cognitive regulatory strategies of depression and anxiety. Limitations
The larger coefficients for depression and anxiety might be the
result of more overlap in item content between these measures A first limitation was the use of self-report measures for both
than with eating disorder measures. The rumination scale has been psychopathology and emotion regulation, thus resulting in shared
criticized for its item overlap with depression (e.g., “I think about method variance. In addition, self-reports of psychopathology
my feelings of sadness”). However, we used the version of the symptoms provide an incomplete picture of an individual’s
rumination questionnaire (i.e., the brooding and pondering pathology and should therefore be administered as part of a multi-
subscales) from which Treynor et al. (2003) removed items that method assessment including diagnostic interviews (e.g.,
most obviously overlapped with distress and found they were still Achenbach, Krukowski, Dumenci, & Ivanova, 2005; Meyer et al.,
strongly related to depressive and anxiety symptoms. Similarly, our 2001). To this end, structured clinical interviews could be used to
meta analysis found that the brooding subscale was as strongly assess psychopathology in future studies in order to: reduce shared
related to depression and anxiety as was the full rumination scale method variance, compensate for the biases resulting from self-
(Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema, et al., 2010). report measures, and provide a more precise delineation of
Author's personal copy

A. Aldao, S. Nolen-Hoeksema / Behaviour Research and Therapy 48 (2010) 974e983 981

individual disorders (in this paper, we focused on general symp- A latent factor of cognitive emotion regulation was significantly
toms of depression, anxiety, and eating disorders). Our meta associated with symptoms of all three disorders, suggesting it has
analysis found stronger relationships between several emotion transdiagnostic effects. These results encourage the development
regulation measures and psychopathology in clinically diagnosed of prevention and intervention programs focused on cognitive
samples than in general population samples (Aldao, Nolen- emotion regulation strategies, because such programs may have
Hoeksema, et al., 2010), underscoring the importance of assessing positive effects across a range of disorders.
psychopathology with a multi-method approach.
Additionally, assessing emotion regulation strategies by self-
report is problematic for several reasons, including the blurry lines Acknowledgements
between emotion generation and emotion regulation (Campos,
Frankel, & Camras, 2004; Cole et al., 2004) and reporting biases We would like to thank Blair Wisco for her assistance in data
(Robinson & Clore, 2002). In this respect, a comprehensive study of collection and her comments on this manuscript. We would also
regulatory strategies will require a multi-modal method of like to thank Kirsten Gilbert, Andrea Gold, Yael Levin, Brett
assessment capturing the subjective, physiological, and behavioral Marroquin, and Vera Vine for their insightful comments on this
emotional domains utilizing multiple measures of each domain. manuscript.
Future work examining the relationship between cognitive
emotion regulation strategies and psychopathology as assessed by
References
structured diagnostic interviews is needed to confirm the results
here. Achenbach, T. M., Krukowski, R. A., Dumenci, L., & Ivanova, M. Y. (2005). Assessment
A second limitation of the present investigation is that we of adult psychopathology: meta-analyses and implications of cross-informant
limited our scope to four cognitive regulatory strategies. We chose correlations. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 361e382.
Aldao, A., Mennin, D. S., Linardatos, E., & Fresco, D. M. (2010). Differential patterns of
these strategies because they are among the most commonly physical symptoms and subjective processes in generalized anxiety disorder
studied ones in the field and therefore the most representative of and unipolar depression. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 24, 250e259.
current conceptualizations of emotion regulation. However, these Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion regulation strategies
across psychopathology: a meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 30,
strategies have been largely studied within the context of mood 217e237.
and anxiety disorders and might not be as relevant to the main Arbuckle, J. (2007). Amos user’s guide. Chicago, Ill.: SPSS Inc.
affective processes to be regulated in other disorders (e.g., positive Beck, A. T. (1976). Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders. New York: Inter-
national University Press.
emotions resulting from the consumption of foods and other
Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive therapy of depression.
substances; elevated positive affect in mania). Future trans- New York: Guilford.
diagnostic work on regulatory strategies should take this issue into Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fit indices in structural models. Psychological
account and seek to identify process that might be germane to Bulletin, 107, 238e246.
Billings, A. G., & Moos, R. H. (1981). The role of coping responses and social
specific disorders. Additionally, in light of our surprising results on responses in attenuating the stress of life events. Journal of Behavioral Medicine,
problem-solving, future work should focus on carefully delineating 4, 139e157.
strategies theoretically and empirically, as there is substantial Bollen, K. A. (1989). Structural equations with latent variables. New York: Wiley.
Bonanno, G. A., Papa, A., O’Neill, K., Westphal, M., & Coifman, K. (2004). The
heterogeneity in the conceptualization of strategies. Lastly, it will importance of being flexible: the ability to both enhance and suppress
be important to more systematically examine the relationship emotional expression predicts long-term adjustment. Psychological Science, 15,
between strategies aimed at the modification of subjective versus 482e487.
Borton, J. L. S., Markowitz, L. J., & Dieterich, J. (2005). Effects of suppressing negative
expressive emotional processes. self-referent thoughts on mood and self-esteem. Journal of Social and Clinical
A third limitation of this study is its cross-sectional design, Psychology, 24, 172e190.
which did not allow for modeling of relationships among variables Brackett, M. A., & Katulak, N. A. (2006). Emotional intelligence in the classroom:
skill-based training for teachers and students. In J. Ciarrochi (Ed.), Applying
over time. Lastly, a fourth limitation stems from the nature of the
emotional intelligence: A practitioners guide (pp. 1e27). New York: Psychology
population we studied, as we utilized an undergraduate sample, Press.
therefore restricting the range of psychopathology and potentially Brown, T. A. (2006). Confirmatory factor analysis for applied research. New York:
Guilford.
also that of cognitive emotion regulation strategies. Indeed, the
Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In
models presented in this manuscript had good fit, but it could have K. A. Bollen, & J. S. Long (Eds.), Testing structural equation models (pp. 136e162).
certainly been higher. It therefore becomes critical to replicate the Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
structural findings in a wide range of samples varying in severity of Burton, E., Stice, E., Bearman, S. K., & Rohde, P. (2007). Experimental test of the
affect-regulation theory of bulimic symptoms and substance use: a randomized
the symptoms and pervasiveness of the emotion regulation deficits. trial. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 40, 27e36.
Byrne, B. M. (2010). Structural equation modeling with AMOS. New York: Taylor &
Conclusion Francis.
Campbell-Sills, L., & Barlow, D. H. (2007). Incorporating emotion regulation into
conceptualizations and treatments of anxiety and mood disorders. In J. J. Gross
We found mixed evidence for specificity in the relationship (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 542e559). New York: Guilford Press.
between multiple cognitive emotion regulation strategies and Campos, J. J., Frankel, C. B., & Camras, L. (2004). On the nature of emotion regulation.
Child Development, 75, 377e394.
psychopathologies. The adaptive strategy of reappraisal showed Carver, C. S., Scheier, M. F., & Weintraub, J. K. (1989). Assessing coping strategies:
a weaker association with a latent factor of cognitive emotion a theoretically based approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56,
regulation and with psychopathology symptoms than maladaptive 267e283.
Chang, E. C., Downey, C. A., & Salata, J. L. (2004). Social problem-solving and positive
strategies. Clinically, this might help account for the difficulties psychological functioning: looking at the positive side of problem-solving. In
teaching clients to incorporate adaptive strategies to their reper- E. C. Chang, T. J. D’Zurilla, & L. J. Sanna (Eds.), Social problem-solving: Theory,
toire; the maladaptive strategies of rumination and thought research, and training (pp. 99e116). Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association Press.
suppression may often overwhelm clients’ attempts to reappraise.
Chapman, A. L., Specht, M. W., & Cellucci, T. (2005). Borderline personality disorder
This suggests that interventions that directly focus on reducing and deliberate self-harm: does experiential avoidance play a role? Suicide and
rumination and thought suppression, such as teaching mindfulness Life-Threatening Behavior, 35, 388e399.
meditation practices, may be critical precursors to interventions to Cheng, C., Hui, W., & Lam, S. (1999). Coping style of individuals with functional
dyspepsia. Psychosomatic Medicine, 61, 789e795.
teach clients to use reappraisal more successfully (e.g., Segal, Clark, D. M. (1988). A cognitive model of panic disorder. In S. Rachman, & J. D. Maser
Williams, & Teasdale, 2002; Watkins et al., 2007). (Eds.), Panic: Psychological perspectives (pp. 71e89). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Author's personal copy

982 A. Aldao, S. Nolen-Hoeksema / Behaviour Research and Therapy 48 (2010) 974e983

Clark, D. M., & Wells, A. (1995). A cognitive model of social phobia. In Hong, R. Y. (2007). Worry and rumination: differential associations with anxious
R. G. Heimberg, M. R. Liebowitz, D. A. Hope, & F. R. Schneier (Eds.), Social phobia: and depressive symptoms and coping behaviors. Behaviour Research and
Diagnosis, assessment and treatment (pp. 69e93). New York: Gilford Press. Therapy, 45, 277e290.
Coiffman, K. G., & Bonanno. (2010). Emotion context sensitivity in adaptation and Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1998). Fit indices in covariance structure modeling: sensi-
recovery. In A. M. Kring, & D. M. Sloan (Eds.), Emotion regulation and psycho- tivity to underparameterized model misspecification. Psychological Methods, 3,
pathology: A transdiagnostic approach to etiology and treatment (pp. 157e173). 424e453.
New York: Gilford Press. Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indices in covariance structure
Cole, P. M., Martin, S. E., & Dennis, T. A. (2004). Emotion regulation as a scientific analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation
construct: methodological challenges and directions for child development Modeling, 6, 1e55.
research. Child Development, 75, 317e333. Joormann, J., & Stoeber, J. (1999). Somatic symptoms of generalized anxiety disorder
Cooper, M. L., Russell, M., Skinner, J. B., Frone, M. R., & Mudar, P. (1982). Stress and from the DSM-IV: associations with pathological worry and depression symp-
alcohol use: moderating effects of gender, coping, and alcohol expectancies. toms in a nonclinical sample. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 13, 491e503.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101, 139e152. Kant, G. L., D’Zurilla, T. J., & Maydeu-Olivares, A. (1997). Social problem-solving as
Davidson, R. J. (1998). Affective style and affective disorders: perspectives from a mediator of stress-related depression and anxiety in middle-aged and elderly
affective neuroscience. Cognition & Emotion, 12, 307e330. community residents. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 21, 73e96.
D’Zurilla, T. J. (1988). Problem-solving therapies. In K. S. Dobson (Ed.), Handbook of Kashdan, T. B., & Breen, W. E. (2008). Social anxiety and positive emotions:
cognitive-behavioral therapies (pp. 85e135). New York: Guilford. a prospective examination of a self-regulatory model with tendencies to suppress
D’Zurilla, T. J., Chang, E. C., Nottingham, E. J., & Faccinni, L. (1988). Social problem- or express emotions as a moderating variable. Behavior Therapy, 39, 1e12.
solving deficits and hopelessness, depression, and suicidal risk in college Kohlenberg, R. J., & Tsai, M. (1991). Functional analytic psychotherapy: Creating
students and psychiatric impatiens. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 54, intense and curative therapeutic relationships. New York: Plenum.
1091e1107. Kring, A. M., & Sloan, D. M. (2010). Emotion regulation in psychopathology: A trans-
Ebner-Priemer, U. W., Welch, S. S., Grossman, P., Reisch, T., Linehan, M. M., & Bohus, M. diagnostic approach to etiology and treatment. New York: Guilford.
(2007). Psychophysiological ambulatory assessment of affective dysregulation in Linehan, M. M. (1993). Cognitive-behavioral treatment of borderline personality
borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Research, 150, 265e275. disorder. New York: The Guilford Press.
Ehring, T., & Watkins, E. R. (2008). Repetitive negative thinking as a transdiagnostic Luce, K. H., & Crowther, J. H. (1999). The reliability of the eating disorder exami-
process. International Journal of Cognitive Therapy, 1, 192e205. nation self-report questionnaire version (EDE-Q). International Journal of Eating
Fairburn, C. G., & Beglin, S. J. (1995). Assessment of eating disorders: interview or Disorders, 25, 349e351.
self-report questionnaire? International Journal of Eating Disorders, 16, 363e370. Lyubomirsky, S., & Tkach, C. (2004). The consequences of dysphoric rumination. In
Fairburn, C. G., & Cooper, Z. (1993). The eating disorder examination. In C. Papageorgiou, & A. Wells (Eds.), Depressive rumination: Nature, theory, and
C. G. Fairburn, & G. T. Wilson (Eds.), Binge eating: Nature, assessment, and treatment (pp. 21e42). New York: Wiley.
treatment (12th ed.). New York: Guilford Press. Macht, M., Haput, C., & Ellgring, H. (2005). The perceived function of eating is
Fairburn, C. G., Cooper, Z., & Shafran, R. (2003). Cognitive behaviour therapy for changed during examination stress: a field study. Eating Behaviors, 6, 109e112.
eating disorders: a “transdiagnostic” theory and treatment. Behaviour Research Magar, E. C. E., Phillips, L. H., & Hosie, J. A. (2008). Self-regulation and risk-taking.
and Therapy, 41, 509e528. Personality and Individual Differences, 45, 153e159.
Fairburn, C. G., Shafran, R., & Cooper, Z. (1998). A cognitive behavioural theory of Mansell, W., Harvey, A., Watkins, E., & Shafran, R. (2009). Conceptual foundations of
anorexia nervosa. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37, 1e13. the transdiagnostic approach to CBT. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy: An
Fairholme, Boisseau, Ellard, Ehrenreich, & Barlow. (2010). Emotions, emotion International Quarterly, 23, 6e19.
regulation, and psychological treatment: a unified perspective. In A. M. Kring, & Marlatt, G. A., Baer, J. S., Donovan, D. M., & Kivlahan, D. R. (1988). Addictive
D. M. Sloan (Eds.), Emotion Regulation and Psychopathology: A transdiagnostic behaviors: etiology and treatment. Annual Review of Psychology, 39, 223e252.
approach to etiology and treatment (pp. 283e309). New York: Guilford. McCarthy, M. (1990). The thin ideal, depression and eating disorders in women.
Feldman, G. C., Joormann, J., & Johnson, S. L. (2008). Responses to positive affect: Behaviour Research and Therapy, 28, 205e215.
a self-report measure of rumination and dampening. Cognitive Therapy and Mennin, D. S., Heimberg, R. G., Turk, C. L., & Fresco, D. M. (2005). Preliminary
Research, 32, 507e525. evidence for an emotion regulation deficit model of generalized anxiety
Feldman-Barrett, L., & Barett, D. J. (2001). An introduction to computerized expe- disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43, 1281e1310.
rience sampling in psychology. Social Science Computer Review, 19, 175e185. Mennin, D. S., Holoway, R. M., Fresco, D. M., Moore, M. T., & Heimberg, R. G. (2007).
Ferster, C. B. (1973). A functional analysis of depression. American Psychologist, 28, Delineating components of emotion and its dysregulation in anxiety and mood
857e870. psychopathology. Behavior Therapy, 38, 284e302.
Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. S. (1980). An analysis of coping in a middle-aged Meyer, G. J., Finn, S. E., Eyde, L. D., Kay, G. G., Moreland, K. L., Dies, R. R., et al. (2001).
community sample. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 21, 219e239. Psychological testing and psychological assessment. American Psychologist, 56,
Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. S. (1986). Stress processes and depressive symptom- 128e156.
atology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 107e113. Miller, G. A., & Chapman, J. P. (2001). Misunderstanding analysis of covariance.
Fresco, D. M., Moore, M. T., van Dulmen, M. H. M., Segal, Z. V., Ma, S. H., Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 40e48.
Teasdale, J. D., et al. (2007). Initial psychometric properties of the experiences Moses, E. B., & Barlow, D. H. (2006). A new unified treatment approach for
questionnaire: validation of a self-report measure of decentering. Behavior emotional disorders based on emotion science. Current Directions in Psycho-
Therapy, 38, 234e246. logical Science, 15, 146e150.
Garnefski, N., & Kraaij, V. (2006). Relationships between cognitive emotion regu- Nevitt, J., & Hancock, G. R. (2001). Performance of bootstrapping approaches to
lation strategies and depressive symptoms: a comparative study of five specific model test statistics and parameter standard errors estimation in structural
samples. Personality and Individual Differences, 40, 1659e1669. equation modeling. Structural Equation Modeling, 8, 353e377.
Garnefski, N., Kraaij, V., & Spinhoven, P. (2001). Negative life events, cognitive Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their effect on the dura-
emotion regulation and emotional problems. Personality and Individual Differ- tion of depressive episodes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 569e582.
ences, 30, 1311e1327. Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Stice, E., Wade, E., & Bohon, C. (2007). Reciprocal relations
Garnefski, N., Kraaij, V., & van Etten, M. (2005). Specificity of relations between between rumination and bulimic, substance abuse, and depressive symptoms
adolescents’ cognitive emotion regulation strategies and internalizing and in female adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116, 198e207.
externalizing psychopathology. Journal of Adolescence, 28, 619e631. Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B. E., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Rethinking rumination.
Gratz, K. L., & Roemer, L. (2004). Multidimensional assessment of emotion regula- Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 400e424.
tion and dysregulation: development, factor structure, and initial validation of Ottenbreit, N. D., & Dobson, K. S. (2004). Avoidance and depression: the construc-
the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. Journal of Psychopathology and tion of the cognitive-behavioral avoidance scale. Behaviour Research and
Behavioral Assessment, 26, 41e54. Therapy, 42., 293e313.
Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: an integrative review. Papageorgiou, C., & Wells, A. (2003). An empirical test of a clinical metacognitive
Review of General Psychology, 2, 271e299. model of rumination and depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27,
Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation 261e273.
processes: implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Piran, N., & Cormier, H. C. (2005). The social construction of women and disordered
Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 348e362. eating patterns. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 52, 549e558.
Gross, J. J., & Levenson, R. W. (1993). Emotional suppression: physiology, self-report, Polivy, J., & Herman, C. P. (2002). Causes of eating disorders. Annual Review of
and expressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 970e986. Psychology, 53, 187e213.
Gross, J. J., & Thompson, R. A. (2007). Emotion regulation: conceptual foundations. Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for
In J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation. New York: Guilford Press. assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior
Harvey, A. G., Watkins, E., Mansell, W., & Shafran, R. (2004). Cognitive behavioural Research Methods, 40., 879e891.
processes across psychological disorders. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Purdon, C. (1999). Thought suppression and psychopathology. Behaviour Research
Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K. D., & Wilson, K. G. (1999). Acceptance and commitment and Therapy, 37, 1029e1054.
therapy: An experiential approach to behavior change. New York: Guilford Press. Purdon, C., & Clark, D. A. (2001). Suppression of obsession-like thoughts in
Heatherton, T. F., & Baumeister, R. F. (1991). Binge eating as escape from self- nonclinical individuals: impact on thought frequency, appraisal, and mood
awareness. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 86e108. state. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 39, 1163e1181.
Hoelter, J. W. (1983). The analysis of covariance structures: goodness-of-fit indices. Rassin, E., Merckelbach, H., & Muris, P. (2000). Paradoxical and less paradoxical effects
Sociological Methods Research, 11, 325e344. of thought suppression: a critical review. Clinical Psychology Review, 20, 973e995.
Author's personal copy

A. Aldao, S. Nolen-Hoeksema / Behaviour Research and Therapy 48 (2010) 974e983 983

Richards, J. M., & Gross, J. J. (2000). Emotion regulation and memory: the cognitive experiential avoidance. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment,
costs of keeping one’s cool. Personality Processes and Individual Differences, 79, 25, 147e154.
410e424. Ulman, J. B. (2007). Structural equation modeling. In B. G. Tabachnick, & L. S. Fidell
Robinson, M. D., & Clore, G. L. (2002). Episodic and semantic knowledge in (Eds.), Using multivariate statistics (pp. 676e780). New York: Allyn & Bacon.
emotional self-report: evidence for two judgment processes. Journal of Van Boven, A. M., & Espelage, D. L. (2006). Depressive symptoms, coping strategies,
Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 198e215. and disordered eating among college women. Journal of Counseling and Devel-
Roefs, A., & Jansen, A. (2002). Implicit and explicit attitudes towards high-fat foods opment, 84, 341e348.
in obesity. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 517e521. Watkins, E. (2008). Constructive and unconstructive repetitive thought. Psycho-
Roemer, L., & Borkovec, T. D. (1994). Effects of suppressing thoughts about logical Bulletin, 134, 163e206.
emotional material. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 467e474. Watkins, E., Scott, J., Wingrove, J., Rimes, K., Bathurst, N., Steiner, H., et al. (2007).
Roemer, L., Orsillo, S. M., & Salters-Pedneault, K. (2008). Efficacy of an acceptance- Rumination-focused cognitive behaviour therapy for residual depression: a case
based behavior therapy for generalized anxiety disorder: evaluation in series. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 2144e2154.
a randomized control trial. Journal of Counseling and Clinical Psychology, 76, Watson, D. (2009). Differentiating the mood and anxiety disorders: a quadripartite
1083e1089. model. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 5, 221e247.
Rosenthal, R., & DiMatteo, M. R. (2001). Meta-analysis: recent developments in Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1991). Self-versus peer ratings of specific emotional traits:
quantitative methods for literature reviews. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, evidence of convergent and discriminant validity. Journal of Personality and
59e82. Social Psychology, 60, 927e940.
Rottenberg, J., & Gross, J. J. (2007). When emotion goes wrong: realizing the promise of Watson, D., Weber, K., Assenheimer, J. S., Clark, L. A., Strauss, M. E., &
affective science. Clinical Psychology Science and Practice, 10, 227e232. McCormick, R. A. (1995). Testing a tripartite model: I. Evaluating the convergent
Salkovskis, P. M. (1998). Psychological approaches to the understanding of obses- and discriminant validity of anxiety and depression symptom scales. Journal of
sional problems. In R. Swinson (Ed.), Obsessive-compulsive disorder: Theory, Abnormal Psychology, 104, 3e14.
research, and treatment (pp. 33e50). New York: Guilford Press. Wegner, D. M., Broome, A., & Blumberg, S. J. (1997). Ironic effects of trying to relax
Segal, A., Williams, J. M. G., & Teasdale, J. D. (2002). Mindfulness-based cognitive under stress. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35, 11e21.
therapy for depression: A new approach to preventing relapse. New York: Guilford. Wegner, D. M., & Erber, R. (1992). The hyperaccessibility of suppressed thoughts.
Sheppes, G., Catran, E., & Meiran, N. (2009). Reappraisal (but not distraction) is Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 903e912.
going to make you sweat: physiological evidence for self-control effort. Inter- Wegner, D. M., & Gold, D. B. (1995). Fanning old flames: emotional and cognitive
national Journal of Psychophysiology, 71, 91e96. effects of suppressing thoughts of a past relationship. Journal of Personality and
Sheppes, G., & Meiran, N. (2008). Divergent cognitive costs for online forms of Social Psychology, 68, 782e792.
reappraisal and distraction. Emotion, 8, 870e874. Wegner, D. M., Schneider, D. J., Carter, S. R., & White, T. L. (1987). Paradoxical effects
Stice, E., Burton, E. M., & Shaw, H. (2004). Prospective relations between bulimic of thought suppression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 5e13.
pathology, depression, and substance abuse: unpacking comorbidity in Wegner, D. M., & Zanakos, S. (1994). Chronic thought suppression. Journal of
adolescent girls. Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology, 72, 62e71. Personality, 62, 615e640.
Stice, E., Presnell, K., & Spangler, D. (2002). Risk factors for binge eating onset in Wenzlaff, R. M., & Luxton, D. D. (2003). The role of thought suppression in
adolescent girls: a 2-year prospective investigation. Health Psychology, 21, depressive rumination. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27, 293e308.
131e138. Wenzlaff, R. M., & Wegner, D. M. (2000). Thought suppression. Annual Review of
Thompson, R. A. (1994). Emotion regulation: a theme in search of definition. Psychology, 51, 59e91.
Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59, 25e52. West, S. G., Finch, J. F., & Curran, P. J. (1995). Structural equation models with
Trapnell, P. D., & Campbell, J. D. (1999). Private self-consciousness and the five- nonnormal variables: problems and remedies. In R. H. Hoyle (Ed.), Structural
factor model of personality: distinguishing rumination from reflection. Journal equation modeling: Concepts, issues, and applications (pp. 56e75). Thousand
of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 284e304. Oakes, CA: Sage.
Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Rumination reconsidered: Williams, L. E., & Bargh, J. A. (2007). The nonconscious regulation of emotion. In
a psychometric analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27, 248e259. J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 429e445). New York:
Tucker, L. R., & Lewis, C. (1973). A reliability coefficient for maximum likelihood Guilford Press.
factor analysis. Psychometrika, 38, 1e10. Zlomke, K. R., & Hahn, K. S. (2010). Cognitive emotion regulation strategies: gender
Tull, M. T., & Roemer, L. (2003). Alternative explanations of emotional numbing of differences and associations to worry. Personality and Individual Differences, 48,
posttraumatic stress disorder: an examination of hyperarousal and 408e413.

View publication stats

You might also like