You are on page 1of 10

Developmental Science 17:1 (2014), pp 1–10 DOI: 10.1111/desc.

12093

PAPER
Parallel development of ERP and behavioural measurements of
visual segmentation
Carlijn van den Boomen,1,2 Victor A.F. Lamme3 and Chantal Kemner1,2,4
1. Department of Experimental Psychology, Helmholtz Institute, Utrecht, The Netherlands
2. Department of Developmental Psychology, Utrecht University, The Netherlands
3. Brain and Cognition, Department of Psychology, Faculty of Behavioral and Societal Sciences, University of Amsterdam,
The Netherlands
4. Rudolf Magnus Institute of Neuroscience, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, University Medical Centre, Utrecht,
The Netherlands

Abstract
Visual segmentation, a process in which elements are integrated into a form and segregated from the background, is known to
differ from adults at infancy. The further developmental trajectory of this process, and of the underlying brain mechanisms,
during childhood and adolescence is unknown. The aim of the study was to investigate the developmental trajectory of ERP
reflections of visual segmentation, and to relate this to behavioural performance. One hundred and eleven typically developing
children from 7 to 18 years of age were divided into six age groups. Each child performed two visual tasks. In a texture
segmentation task, the difference in event-related potential (ERP) response to homogeneous (no visual segmentation) and
checkered stimuli (visual segmentation) was investigated. In addition, behavioural performance on integration of elements into
contours was measured. Both behavioural and ERP measurements of visual segmentation differed from adults in 7–12 year-old
children. Behaviourally, young children were less able to integrate elements into a contour than older children. In addition, a
developmental change was present in the ERP pattern evoked by homogeneous versus checkered stimuli. The largest differences
in behaviour and ERPs were found between 7–8- and 9–10-, and between 11–12- and 13–14-year-old children, indicating the
strongest development between those age groups. Behavioural as well as ERP measurements at 13–14 years of age showed
similar results to those of adults. These results reveal that visual segmentation continues to develop until early puberty. Only by
13–14 years of age, children do integrate and segregate visual information as adults do. These results can be interpreted in terms
of functional connectivity within the visual cortex.

Introduction involved in the processing of basic features of small


elements, such as line orientation; higher-level visual areas
The pace of brain development differs between brain in processing more general information on global parts of
areas, and sensory areas, such as the visual cortex, show a an object, such as its shape (for a review, see Nassi &
strong development relatively early in life, although they Callaway, 2009). The full processing of an object relies on
continue to develop until at least early puberty (Gogtay, processes in both lower and higher areas and involves,
Giedd, Luck, Hayashi, Greenstein, Vaituzis, Nugent, among others, visual integration of local elements into a
Herman, Clasen, Toga, Rapoport & Thompson, 2004; form, detection of its borders, and segregation of this form
van den Boomen, van der Smagt & Kemner, 2012). This from its background, together leading to visual segmen-
development involves changes both within visual brain tation (also known as visual or texture segregation). This
areas, as well as in the connections between them, affecting process is required for perception of objects such as faces,
the flow of information between lower- and higher-level geometric forms and letters, and therefore plays an
areas of the visual hierarchy. Lower-level visual areas are important role in social and cognitive development.

Address for correspondence: Carlijn van den Boomen, Utrecht University, Department of Experimental Psychology, Heidelberglaan 2, Van Unnik
Building Room 16.17, 3584 CS Utrecht, The Netherlands; e-mail: C.vandenboomen@uu.nl

© 2013 John Wiley & Sons Ltd


2 Carlijn van den Boomen et al.

Lamme and Roelfsema proposed a model to explain


the role of different types of neural functional connec-
tivity in visual segmentation (Lamme, Super & Spe-
kreijse, 1998; Lamme & Roelfsema, 2000; Roelfsema,
Lamme, Spekreijse & Bosch, 2002). In the model, there
are two types of connectivity: the first is feedforward
connectivity distributing information from lower (i.e. V1)
to higher areas (i.e. V2 and higher). The second is
recurrent connectivity, which can be subdivided into Figure 2 Examples of contours with different relative noise
feedback connectivity from higher to lower areas and density (left D = 1.2; right D = 0.75).
horizontal connectivity within brain areas. Visual
segmentation is eventually established using recurrent Caputo & Casco, 1999; Scholte, Jolij, Fahrenfort &
connectivity. Lamme, 2008).
The model is supported by research using single-cell Behavioural investigations can also be informative on
recording in the monkey visual brain (e.g. Lamme, 1995; connectivity processes involved in segmentation. In the
Zipser, Lamme & Schiller, 1996; Lamme et al., 1998; well-established contour integration task (Kovacs &
Super, Spekreijse & Lamme, 2001). In these studies, the Julesz, 1993), the focus has been on the process of
contribution of different types of connectivity to the integration of visual elements, an important aspect of
processing of specific visual stimuli was investigated. segmentation. Stimuli in this task contain Gabor ele-
Typically, two types of stimuli are used: homogeneous ments forming a contour, which needs to be detected in a
and textured stimuli such as checkered ones (Figure 1). background with a varying amount of randomly posi-
The latter type contains a pattern that can be processed tioned and oriented Gabor elements (noise) (Figure 2,
by integrating the oriented line elements within each where contours are circular; Kovacs & Julesz, 1993;
check and segregating the different checks from each Kovacs, 2000; Hadad, Maurer & Lewis, 2010). Due to
other. It was revealed that recurrent signals from higher- the distance between Gabors, integration of elements
level visual areas back to V1 are necessary for the into a contour is proposed to depend on recurrent
successful segmentation of textured stimuli, but not for connections (e.g. Angelucci, Levitt, Walton, Hupe,
processing of homogeneous ones. This specific neural Bullier & Lund, 2002; Gilbert, 1998; Kovacs, Kozma,
signalling related to visual segmentation in monkeys is Feher & Benedek, 1999; Stettler, Das, Bennett & Gilbert,
typically expressed in the modulation of responses at 2002; Zhang & von der Heydt, 2010). The level of noise
latencies beyond 100 ms, i.e. relatively late compared to in which adults can detect the contour, providing insight
the signals related to feedforward processing (starting at into their recurrent connectivity, is well investigated
40 ms) (e.g. Lamme, 1995; Zipser et al., 1996; Lamme (Kovacs & Julesz, 1993; Hess, Hayes & Field, 2003).
et al., 1998; Super et al., 2001). Using the same para- Thus, both ERP and behavioural research have increased
digm, a similar modulation of activation was found in our understanding of recurrent connectivity in the adult
the event-related potential (ERP) of human adults, visual cortex.
expressed as a more negative peak for checkered than In children, behavioural research suggests that visual
homogeneous stimuli, also at somewhat longer latencies segmentation improves until 13 years of age (Sireteanu &
and largest at the occipital electrodes (e.g. Lamme, van Rieth, 1992; Kovacs, 2000; Hadad et al., 2010), but the
Dijk & Spekreijse, 1992; Bach & Meigen, 1992, 1998; specific changes in underlying mechanisms are less well
understood. ERPs evoked by textured stimuli have only
been investigated in infants (Arcand, Tremblay,
Vannasing, Ouimet, Roy, Fallaha, Lepore, Lassonde &
McKerral, 2007).1 In the first year of life, the modulation
of brain activity evoked by checkered versus homogeneous
stimuli changes with age, but still differs from adults at
1 year of age. It is unknown which developmental changes

1
This process was studied in 13-year-old children as well (Kemner,
Figure 1 Stimuli used in the texture segmentation task; Lamme, Kovacs & van Engeland, 2007), but not in a developmental
homogeneous (left) and checkered (right) stimuli. perspective since results were not compared to adults.

© 2013 John Wiley & Sons Ltd


Development of visual segmentation 3

in brain functioning related to visual segmentation are differences between groups on IQ, as measured using a
present after this age. shortened version of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for
The aim of the current study was to investigate the Children, revised Dutch version (WISC-RN). Average
developmental trajectory of ERP reflections of visual age and IQ of the groups are summarized in Table 1.
segmentation in children from 7 to 18 years of age in a Children were recruited from elementary and high
texture segmentation task using the homogeneous and schools in and around Utrecht and Someren (The
checkered stimuli (Figure 1), as described above. In Netherlands). The medical ethical committee of the
addition, we investigated behavioural performance using University Medical Center Utrecht approved the study,
the contour integration task, aiming to replicate the in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. All
previously reported developmental trajectory (Kovacs, parents or caretakers gave written informed consent
2000; Hadad et al., 2010), and investigate possible prior to participation, after explanation of the procedure.
improvements in this task after 13 years of age. Fur- Tasks and procedures are equal to those previously
thermore, our goal was to compare the developmental described in Kemner et al. (2007).
trajectories of behavioural and ERP measurements.
Stimuli in both tasks are thought to require recurrent
Contour integration task
connectivity, and we therefore hypothesize a parallel
developmental trajectory of both measurements. A card version of the contour integration task was used
Using the texture segmentation and contour integra- (Kovacs et al., 1999; Kovacs, 2000). A contour integra-
tion tasks, we will be able to uncover the developmental tion card contains multiple Gabor elements, forming a
trajectory of the neurocognitive mechanisms underlying closed circular figure (contour) embedded in a back-
visual segmentation. This is imperative to better appre- ground of randomly positioned and oriented distracters
ciate the biological basis of developmental transitions in (orientation; Figure 2). The contour had a random
processing of relatively complex visual information. In position to avoid stereotyped responding. Each Gabor
addition, increased knowledge of the typical develop- element had a carrier spatial frequency of 5 c/deg at a
mental trajectory of visual segmentation and recurrent viewing distance of 50 cm, and a luminance contrast of
connectivity can help in gaining insight into disorders about 95%, printed on white paper. All cards had fixed
such as autism spectrum disorder, schizophrenia, and inter-element spacing along the contour of 8 wavelengths
Williams syndrome where development of these visual (8k) of the carrier, center-to-center (1.6°). Participants
processes seems to go astray (Gervan, Gombos & were asked to identify the contour by pointing out the
Kovacs, 2012; Kemner et al., 2007;Silverstein, Keane, full path of the circle, which was scored as correct if they
Barch, Carter, Gold, Kovacs, MacDonald, Ragland & were able to indicate the total contour at the correct
Strauss, 2012; Vandenbroucke, Scholte, van Engeland, location and as fail if they either could not point out any
Lamme & Kemner, 2008, 2009). contour or pointed out noise (only one closed contour
was present per card). The difficulty of the task depends
on the relative noise density (D) of the stimulus. D is
Methods defined as the ratio of average noise spacing over
contour spacing, and varied between 0.5 and 1.2 in steps
of 0.05. A maximum of 15 cards per set was presented in
Participants
a decreasing order of D, and thus in an increasing order
A total of 111 children took part in the study, divided of difficulty. The last card on which the contour was
into six age groups: 7–8 years (n = 9), 9–10 years correctly identified defined the threshold. A simple
(n = 14), 11–12 years (n = 15), 13–14 years (n = 28), staircase procedure was applied to measure the thresh-
15–16 years (n = 30), and 17–18 years (n = 15). The last old, in which progression was allowed if a card was
group (17–18-year-olds) is also referred to as ‘adults’ missed, but a reversal occurred when the following card
throughout the results and discussion sections. All was missed as well. The task was terminated when the
children were male, and there were no significant contour on a specific card could not be detected twice.

Table 1 Average age in years and total IQ (standard error) per age group
Group 7–8 9–10 11–12 13–14 15–16 17–18
Age 8.3 (.23) 10.1 (.14) 11.9 (.15) 14.2 (.10) 16.0 (.10) 17.7 (.18)
IQ 112.3 (4.5) 112.7 (3.3) 112.3 (3.1) 104.9 (2.7) 109.5 (2.3) 106.4 (3.0)

© 2013 John Wiley & Sons Ltd


4 Carlijn van den Boomen et al.

Texture segmentation task eye-movements) (Gratton, Coles & Donchin, 1983).


Epochs with additional amplitude artifacts were removed.
To investigate the differential activation evoked by
These artifacts were defined by a voltage change of more
checkered (textured) versus homogeneous stimuli, a
than 50 lV per sampling point, a voltage difference of less
texture segmentation task was performed. Stimuli con-
than 3 lV per 200 ms, or amplitudes below 100 or above
sisted of 45- or 135-degree oriented black line segments,
100 lV. ERP were averaged separately for the checker-
randomly positioned on a white background (total 450
board and homogeneous stimuli. Next, subtracting the
line segments; 100% luminance contrast). The lines were
ERP evoked by the homogeneous from that evoked by the
organized either into homogeneous (eight different stim-
checkered stimuli created a difference wave that selectively
uli) or into checkered stimuli (also eight different stimuli;
extracts the neural signals related to image segmentation
see Figure 1 for an example) such that local modulations
while discarding any signals related to the processing of
are, on average, equally distributed between the two types
lower-level features such as the line segments and their
of stimuli, but that stimuli are presented in a homoge-
orientation. The electrode of interest was Oz, which was
neous or checkered fashion, thus modulating the amount
determined based on previous research (e.g. Lamme et al.,
of segmentation independently of the changes in orienta-
1992) and was confirmed by visual inspection of ERP
tion that occur on each transition from one stimulus to the
patterns in other electrodes (Figure 3) and topographies
next (see Lamme et al., 1992, for a full explanation of this
of the activity (Figure 4). These figures showed that for all
balancing of local modulation). Stimuli subtended
age groups the segmentation activity is present at the Oz
17.5 9 13 degrees of visual angle at a viewing distance
electrode, and that the pattern of segmentation is not
of 57 cm. Homogeneous and checkered stimuli were
substantially different from Oz at other electrodes (if
alternated every 500 ms (2 Hz). The basic sequence
present). All pre-processing steps were carried out using
consisted of the 16 stimuli presented in a fixed order.
Brain Vision Analyzer (Brain Products GmbH, Munich,
A total of 800 stimuli were presented (400 per condition)
Germany).
on a Dell Latitude D610 laptop. Participants performed
In the difference wave, two peaks were of interest for the
an oddball task on Pokemon cartoons presented at
analyses: first the typical texture negativity (TN;
random timepoints during the task. Stimuli were previ-
Figure 5), described in previous research. Secondly,
ously described by Lamme et al. (1992) and the task has
ERP waveforms of the younger children revealed an
previously been described by Kemner et al. (2007).
additional positive peak. This peak was present before the
TN, and is referred to as the texture positivity (TP;
EEG recordings and analyses Figure 5) in the current paper. The texture positivity was
semi-manually scored as the most positive point (global
Electroencephalographic activity was recorded from 32
maximum) directly preceding texture negativity (i.e. peak
electrodes using a Biosemi Active Two EEG system
was automatically detected, but manually corrected if the
(Biosemi, Amsterdam, The Netherlands). Electrodes
criterion of preceding the texture negativity was not
were positioned at standard EEG recording locations
satisfied). Scoring was performed semi-manually to pre-
according to the international 10/20 system. Vertical
vent mis-detection of possible positive peaks following the
EOG was recorded from electrodes placed above and
TN. Texture negativity was automatically scored as the
below the left eye, and horizontal EOG from electrodes
most negative point (global maximum) between 130 and
placed at the outer canthi of the eyes. An additional
190 ms post-stimulus. Peak amplitude and latency of
electrode was placed at the right mastoid for offline
both the TP and the TN were exported for further
re-referencing purposes. During recording, EEG was
analyses. Four one-way analyses of variance (ANOVA)
sampled at a rate of 2048 Hz. Two electrodes in the cap,
were carried out using PASW 18 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL,
the CMS (common Mode Sense) and DRL (driven Right
USA), with latency and amplitude of the TP and the TN
Leg) provided an ‘active ground’.
as the dependent variables, and age as the between-
Raw data were re-referenced offline to the right mastoid.
subjects factor. For all reported analyses, the alpha value
Then data were re-sampled to 512 Hz, and filtered with a
was set at .05 and all post-hoc analyses are comparisons of
high-pass filter of 1 Hz, a low-pass filter of 30 Hz and a
an age group with its following age group (e.g. 7–8- with
notch filter of 50 Hz. In order to compute ERPs, epochs of
9–10-year-old children), performed by univariate analyses
50 ms pre-stimulus (baseline) to 400 ms post-stimulus
with repeated contrasts. These tests were performed with
were extracted from the continuous data. Ocular artifacts
two-way analyses for ERPs, and one-way analyses for the
were removed from the EEG with a regression analysis
contour task due to the specific hypothesis of an
based on eye movements detected by vertical EOG
improvement with age (Hadad et al., 2010; Kovacs, 2000).
(blinks) and horizontal EOG electrodes (horizontal

© 2013 John Wiley & Sons Ltd


Development of visual segmentation 5

Figure 3 Grand averages of ERPs at occipital and parietal electrodes, evoked by checkerboard and homogeneous stimuli, and the
difference wave between stimuli, for the age groups investigated. Black and grey lines are placed at latency of P1 and N2 peaks,
respectively, in the oldest age group (17–18 years). The figure shows that the pattern of segmentation as represented in the difference
wave is not substantially different at other electrodes than Oz, which was chosen as electrode of interest for analyses.

107) = 8.3, p < .001). Younger children detected con-


tours in fewer cards as compared to older children. Post-
hoc analyses revealed a significant difference between
7–8- and 9–10- (contrast estimate = 1.5; p < .05), and
between 11–12- and 13–14-year-old children (contrast
estimate = 1.0; p < .05). Performance values (mea-
sured in D-threshold) are depicted in Figure 6e.

EEG

Figure 4 Activity maps representing the distribution of the Texture positivity


difference in brain activity between checkered and A main effect of age on latency of the texture positivity
homogeneous stimuli over the 32 EEG electrodes. Maps are (F(5, 105) = 8.0; p < .001) indicated that younger chil-
plotted per age group at the moment of maximum peak
dren showed longer latencies than older children. Post-
amplitude for the Texture Positivity (TP) and the Texture
hoc analyses revealed a significant difference between
Negativity (TN).
11–12- and 13–14-year-old children (contrast esti-
mate = 23.6; p < .001). Latency values are depicted in
Performance on the contour integration task was Figure 6a. However, the TP peak amplitude was close to
analysed in a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA), zero in 13–18-year-old children, which could affect peak
with number of correct-detected cards as dependent detection and thus latency analyses. To provide further
variable, and age as between-subjects factor. support for a latency change between 11–12 and
13–14 years, ERP peaks evoked by checkerboard and
homogeneous images, rather than difference waves, were
Results studied as well. The TP peak originated from the
difference in activity evoked by checkerboard versus
homogeneous stimuli at the P1 peak, which was larger
Contour integration test
than zero in all age groups (Figure 3). Additional
Analyses revealed a significant effect of age on the analyses were performed confirming an effect of age on
number of cards with correctly detected contours (F(5, latency of the P1 peak (checkerboard: F(5, 105) = 5.6;

© 2013 John Wiley & Sons Ltd


6 Carlijn van den Boomen et al.

(a) (b)

Figure 5 Difference waves of grand averages evoked by (c) (d)


checkerboard versus homogeneous stimuli for the age groups
investigated. Peaks of interest are the texture positivity (TP) and
the texture negativity (TN).

p < .001; homogeneous: F(5, 105) = 6.8; p < .001). Post-


hoc analyses revealed a significant difference between
11–12- and 13–14-year-old children (checkerboard: con-
trast estimate = 7.5; p < .05; homogeneous: contrast
estimate = 12.7; p < .001). Post-hoc analyses showed
that for the checkerboard stimuli, the P1 latency also
decreased between 7–8 and 9–10 years (contrast esti-
mate = 10.1; p < .05). (e)
A main effect of age on amplitude of the texture
positivity (F(5, 105) = 9.4, p < .001) indicated that
younger children showed larger amplitudes than older
children. Post-hoc analyses revealed a significant differ-
ence between 7–8- and 9–10-year-old children (contrast
estimate = 2.3; p < .001). Amplitude values are depicted
in Figure 6b.

Texture negativity
A main effect of age on latency of the texture negativity Figure 6 Average values per age group on (a) Texture
(F(5, 105) = 5.7, p < .001) indicated that younger chil- positivity amplitude; (b) Texture positivity latency; (c) Texture
dren showed longer latencies than older children. Post- negativity amplitude; (d) Texture negativity latency; (e)
hoc analyses revealed a significant difference between Contour integration performance. Asterisks show significant
11–12- and 13–14-year-old children (contrast esti- differences between age groups.
mate = 9.1; p < .05). Latency values are depicted in
Figure 6c. independent processes, and it is possible that the changes
A main effect of age on amplitude of the texture in latency and amplitude of the TP influence those of the
negativity (F(5, 105) = 3.0, p < .05) indicated that youn- TN. The age differences in the TN could therefore be due
ger children showed less negative amplitudes than older to differences in the TP. To examine whether this was the
children. Post-hoc analyses revealed a significant differ- case, we investigated whether the effects of age on the TN
ence between 7–8- and 9–10-year-old children (contrast latency and amplitude are still present when taking the
estimate = 2.7; p < .01). Amplitude values are depicted TP latency and amplitude as a covariate. Covariance
in Figure 6d. analyses (one-tailed testing) showed that the effects of
The current data are not suitable for determining with age on texture negativity amplitude were no longer
certainty whether changes in the TP and the TN reflect present when corrected for texture positivity amplitude

© 2013 John Wiley & Sons Ltd


Development of visual segmentation 7

(F(5, 104) = .67; p > .05). There was still a linear effect from 11–12 to 13–14 years of age, the latency of the TN
of age on texture negativity latency when corrected for was decreasing. Moreover, the results showed that in
texture positivity latency (Contrast estimate = 9.985; younger children the difference wave contained an
p < .05). additional positive difference (Textured Positivity, TP)
preceding the TN. This peak was most apparent in the
7–8-year-old children, and showed a large decrease in
Conclusion and discussion amplitude until 9–10 years of age, after which the
amplitude was close to zero lV. The latency of the TP
The current study aimed to investigate the development showed a large decrease from 11–12 to 13–14 years of
of ERP reflections of visual segmentation, a process that age. The TP and TN thus both show large changes in
is crucial to higher-order processes, to provide informa- amplitude from 7–8 to 9–10, and in latency from 11–12
tion on the development of specific neural mechanisms to 13–14 years of age, when the ERP pattern resembled
involved and relate this to behavioural performance. We that of adults. The ERP results imply that there are two
revealed a parallel development of both ERP and stages in development. The first is a transition from an
behaviour over childhood and adolescence. early-in-time process involved in visual segmentation in
More specifically, we measured the ability to behavio- younger children (represented by the TP) to a later-in-
urally detect a circle (contour) among increasing values time process in older children (represented by the TN).
of relative noise density (D), referred to as contour Subsequently, there is a general increase in speed of
integration. The lower the D-value, the more difficult it is processing, represented by the shorter latencies of the TP
to integrate contours. This task showed that the D-value and TN with increasing age.
at which a contour can be detected decreased with age, Based on ERP and behavioural measurements, no
with the largest changes from 7–8 to 9–10, and from solid conclusions can be drawn regarding the underlying
11–12 to 13–14 years of age. In the latter group, mechanisms of the developmental trajectory that is
performance was almost equal to adults. It should be revealed. We here propose a possible mechanism con-
noted that the absence of significant change between tributing to the observed changes. Although we did not
9–10- and 11–12-year-old children does not necessarily directly measure functional connectivity in the brain,
imply no change at all between these ages, and that for considering the processes theoretically involved in visual
instance a lack of power could have affected the results. segmentation we could hypothesize that the first devel-
Development until late childhood in this task is in opmental stage is related to increasing involvement of
accordance with previous findings (Kovacs et al., 1999; feedback processes with age, and the second to increased
Kovacs, 2000). Based on multiple studies using animals speed of processing due to myelinization (see discussion
and computational models (e.g. Angelucci et al., 2002; below).
Gilbert, 1998; Stettler et al., 2002; Zhang & von der With respect to the increasing role of feedback
Heydt, 2010) it has previously been proposed that processes in visual segmentation with age, we separately
contour integration among increasing noise levels (i.e. considered the possible role of horizontal and feedback
decreasing D-levels) requires increasing activation in connectivity. The former mediates detection of borders;
recurrent connections between neurons (e.g. Hess & the latter facilitates the grouping of elements of a figure
Field, 1999; Kovacs et al., 1999). The lower performance that is represented by the borders in higher areas such as
in young children thus implies an immaturity of recur- IT (Roelfsema et al., 2002). Horizontal and feedback
rent connectivity. connectivity are together involved in the generation of
In the texture segmentation task we investigated the the difference wave, and thus in the TP and TN peaks.
difference in brain activity evoked by checkered versus We speculate that each of these types of connectivity is
homogeneous stimuli in order to explore the specific more prominently involved in the generation of one of
developmental changes in neural mechanisms underlying the ERP peaks: horizontal connectivity in generation
visual segmentation, especially the maturation of recur- of the TP peak, and feedback connectivity mainly in that
rent connectivity. Visual segmentation is reflected in a of the TN peak. This distinction is based on the order of
negative peak (texture negativity or TN) in the so-called the peaks and processes involved: the TP, horizontal
difference wave between ERPs evoked by checkered connectivity, and border processing precede the TN,
versus homogeneous stimuli (e.g. Lamme et al., 1992; feedback connectivity, and grouping, respectively (Lam-
Bach & Meigen, 1992, 1998; Caputo & Casco, 1999; me, Rodriguez-Rodriguez & Spekreijse, 1999; Lamme &
Scholte et al., 2008). Results of the current study showed Roelfsema, 2000). Evidence for a functional distinction
a negative deflection in all age groups, with increasing between early and late segmentation-related activity also
negativity from 7–8 to 9–10 years of age. At a later age, comes from EEG studies in adults. In these studies,

© 2013 John Wiley & Sons Ltd


8 Carlijn van den Boomen et al.

stimuli with varying amounts of borders and groups or long-range connectivity increases with age. Thus, we
figures were presented. Results showed an early activity hypothesize that both feedback and long-range horizon-
related to border processing and later activity related to tal connectivity become increasingly involved during the
grouping (Caputo & Casco, 1999; Scholte et al., 2008). first developmental stage that was described in the
The ascription of the prominent involvement of hori- present study. The potential cause of the second devel-
zontal versus feedback connectivity in the generation of opmental stage, i.e. myelinization, could be generalized
the TP versus TN peaks implies a relative decrease of to explain the decreased thresholds in the contour
horizontal and increase of feedback connectivity integration task because myelinization would increase
involved during the first developmental stage between speed of processing in both feedback and long-range
7–8 and 9–10 years. A possible mechanism contributing horizontal connectivity, which might lead to faster and
to this change is synaptic pruning, resulting in a decrease possibly better or more efficient processing.
of horizontal connections. The second developmental Altogether, based on the proposed underlying processes
stage, characterized by decreasing latency, could possibly of visual segmentation we speculate that the developmen-
be related to increased myelinization of connections. tal changes in ERPs and behavioural performance
Myelinization results in increased speed of processing, between 7–8 and 9–10 years of age can be related to a
which is reflected in decreasing latencies of the ERP relatively increased involvement of long-range horizontal
peaks involved in visual segmentation. It has been and feedback connectivity as compared to short-range
previously shown that myelinization continues until late horizontal connectivity. This stage would reflect a qual-
childhood in the visual cortex (Paus, Collins, Evans, itative change in processes. The second stage, from 11–12
Leonard, Pike & Zijdenbos, 2001). to 13–14 years of age, characterized by decreasing latency
Two developmental stages were observed in the of ERP peaks and improved behavioural performance,
behavioural contour integration task as well. However, could be related to myelinization and would thus involve a
before generalizing the hypothesized changes in recur- quantitative change. These results match with the previ-
rent connectivity from the ERP to the behavioural task, ously reported decrease in short-range functional connec-
the specific types of connectivity possibly involved in tivity within, and increase in long-range functional
each of the tasks should be considered. For both tasks, connectivity between, areas over the whole brain during
the involvement of feedback connectivity has been infancy and childhood (e.g. Supekar, Musen & Menon,
suggested. Therefore, the proposed increasing role of 2009; Vakorin, Lippe & McIntosh, 2011). Based on the
feedback connectivity over development could underlie current results we speculate that these specific changes in
developmental changes in the ERP and behavioural connectivity also occur within the visual cortex, and are
tasks. However, a distinction has been made regarding specifically related to behavioural maturation. Future
the involvement of specific horizontal connections in research is required to investigate this hypothesis.
each of the tasks. Structurally, a subdivision can be made We here propose changes in specific recurrent connec-
into short- (<0.5 mm) and long-range (<5 mm) horizon- tions as underlying mechanisms of developmental dif-
tal connectivity (Gilbert & Wiesel, 1985; Rockland & ferences in visual segmentation, but acknowledge that
Lund, 1983; Wielaard & Sajda, 2006). Functionally, this mechanism cannot explain all the results of the
short-range connections are thought to be involved in current and previous studies.2 The proposed mechanisms
discriminating nearby stimuli with different orientations, of change, i.e. pruning and myelinization, cannot fully
whereas long-range connections are involved in connect- explain the observed changes in peak amplitude and
ing further-away stimuli with similar orientations (e.g. latency. Pruning of connections, as is now proposed to
Das & Gilbert, 1999; Wielaard & Sajda, 2006; Angelucci contribute to the decreasing TP peak, is not restricted to
et al., 2002). Contour integration probably relies mainly horizontal connections but occurs in feedback connec-
on long-range connections, which are increasingly tions as well. This would result in a decrease in the TN
required in larger distances between contour elements peak amplitude, which is not observed in the current
and in increasing noise levels (Hess & Field, 1999; data. Future research is required to investigate to what
Kovacs, 2000). The checkered stimuli in the ERP task extent pruning occurs in different neuronal connections
contain borders, which are plausibly processed using and whether this can explain the changes in TP and
short-range horizontal connectivity, although this has TN peaks. In addition, although myelinization in the
not been specifically investigated. Due to this distinction, occipital cortex continues throughout childhood (Paus
the decreasing role of horizontal connectivity, proposed
in the ERP task, cannot be generalized to the behavio-
ural contour integration task. Instead, the current results 2
An anonymous reviewer substantially contributed to the following
suggest that short-range connectivity decreases and discussion.

© 2013 John Wiley & Sons Ltd


Development of visual segmentation 9

et al., 2001), it is unlikely to change so considerably underlying visual segmentation between 7–8 and
between 11–12 and 13–14 years of age as to fully explain 13–14 years of age in the same children, and a more
the current results. Furthermore, the current interpreta- specific difference between two developmental stages is
tion does not fully match previous findings, in that it demonstrated by the ERP results. The underlying
implies at least a lower contribution of horizontal mechanisms, proposed to be specific aspects of recurrent
connections in older than younger children. These connectivity in the visual cortex, have matured at 13–14
connections are, however, also present and contributing years of age.
to segmentation in older children and adults (Lamme
et al., 1998). However, some changes in (long-range)
connectivity over childhood have been previously Acknowledgements
reported (Supekar et al., 2009; Vakorin et al., 2011)
and are proposed to affect segmentation processes The authors would like to thank Emmie van Schaffelaar
(Kovacs, 2000). Although it is very unlikely that for her contribution to the data collection, and Sascha
horizontal and feedback mechanisms function com- Morel and the lab groups of Chantal Kemner and Frans
pletely differently in younger children compared to Verstraten for discussion and comments. We also thank
adults, some changes in the behavioural outcome have two anonymous reviewers for their thoughtful com-
been proposed (Kovacs, 2000). Thus, it is still under ments. C. van den Boomen and C. Kemner are
debate to what extent and via which mechanisms supported by a VICI Grant from the Dutch Organiza-
connectivity develops throughout childhood in the visual tion for Scientific Research (NWO) granted to C.
cortex, and to what extent this can explain developmen- Kemner. V.A.F. Lamme is supported by an Advanced
tal changes in segmentation. Investigator Grant (DEFCON1) from the European
Moreover, other possible interpretations of ERP results Research Council (ERC).
and limitations in the comparison of tasks should be
noted. First, the presence of the TN in the youngest age
group, and the subsequent development in older children References
could be influenced by that of the TP. For example, the
negativity defined as the TN could in the youngest age Angelucci, A., Levitt, J.B., Walton, E.J.S., Hupe, J.-M., Bullier,
group represent a fading of activity in neurons generating J., & Lund, J.S. (2002). Circuits for local and global signal
integration in primary visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience,
the TP, which would lead to the conclusion that the TN is
22, 8633–8646.
absent. Another possibility could be that all changes
Arcand, C., Tremblay, E., Vannasing, P, Ouimet, C., Roy,
observed in the TN are due to that in the TP, and that M.-S., Fallaha, N., Lepore, F., Lassonde, M., & McKerral,
neurons generating the TN are thus as active in all age M. (2007). Development of visual texture segregation during
groups. Covariance analyses on developmental changes the first year of life: a high-density electrophysiological study.
observed in the TP and TN did however reject this last Experimental Brain Research, 180, 263–272.
possibility, showing that changes in the TN occur Bach, M., & Meigen, T. (1992). Electrophysiological correlates
independently of those in the TP. Overall, it can still be of texture segregation in the human visual evoked potential.
concluded that the TP is more prominent in younger Vision Research, 32 (3), 417–424.
children, whereas the TN does resemble more adult-like Bach, M., & Meigen, T. (1998). Electrophysiological correlates
characteristics in older children. Another limiting aspect of human texture segregation, an overview. Documenta
Ophthalmologica, 95, 335–347.
includes factors such as structural changes in cortical
Boon, M.Y., Suttle, C.M., & Dain, S.J. (2007). Transient VEP
folding that influence the position of neurons and thus
and psychophysical chromatic contrast thresholds in children
whether their activity can be recorded using EEG, which and adults. Vision Research, 47, 2124–2133.
could affect the peak amplitudes (Luck, 2005). Moreover, Caputo, G., & Casco, C. (1999). A visual evoked potential
in the comparison between ERP and behavioural results it correlate of global figure– ground segmentation. Vision
should be kept in mind that this represents a correlation Research, 39, 1597–1610.
rather than a causal relation and that other aspects, such as Gervan, P., Gombos, F., & Kovacs, I. (2012). Perceptual
attention and motivation, affect each of the measurements learning in Williams syndrome: looking beyond averages.
in a different way (for an extensive discussion, see Boon, PlosOne, 7 (7), e40282.
Suttle & Dain, 2007; van den Boomen et al., 2012). Gilbert, C.D. (1998). Adult cortical dynamics. Physiological
Therefore, the current results should be interpreted with Reviews, 78, 467–485.
Gilbert, C.D., & Wiesel, T.N. (1985). Intrinsic connectivity and
caution.
receptive field properties in visual cortex. Vision Research, 25,
In conclusion, the current study revealed a parallel
365–374.
development of behavioural and ERP measurements

© 2013 John Wiley & Sons Ltd


10 Carlijn van den Boomen et al.

Gogtay, N., Giedd, J.N., Luck, L., Hayashi, K.M., Greenstein, Rockland, K.S., & Lund, J.S. (1983). Intrinsic laminar lattice
D., Vaituzis, A.C., Nugent, T.F. III, Herman, D.H., Clasen, connections in primate visual cortex. Journal of Computa-
L.S., Toga, A.W., Rapoport, J.L., & Thompson, P.M. (2004). tional Neurology, 216, 303–318.
Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during Roelfsema, P.R., Lamme, V.A.F., Spekreijse, H., & Bosch, H.
childhood through early adulthood. Proceedings of the (2002). Figure–ground segregation in a recurrent network
National Academy of Sciences, USA, 101, 8174–8179. architecture. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 525–537.
Gratton, G., Coles, M.G.H., & Donchin, E. (1983). A new Scholte, H.S., Jolij, J., Fahrenfort, J.J., & Lamme, V.A.F. (2008).
method for off-line removal of ocular artifact. Electroen- Feedforward and recurrent processing in scene segmentation:
cephalographical Clinical Neurophysioly, 55, 468–484. electroencephalography and functional magnetic resonance
Hadad, B., Maurer, D., & Lewis, T.L. (2010). The effects of imaging. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 2097–2109.
spatial proximity and collinearity on contour integration in Silverstein, S.M., Keane, B.P., Barch, D.M., Carter, C.S., Gold,
adults and children. Vision Research, 50, 772–778. J.M., Kovacs, I., MacDonald, A. III, Ragland, J.D., &
Hess, R., & Field, D. (1999). Integration of contours: new Strauss, M.E. (2012). Optimization and validation of a visual
insights. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 480–486. integration test for schizophrenia research. Schrizophrenia
Hess, R.F., Hayes, A., & Field, D.J. (2003). Contour integra- Bulletin, 38 (1), 125–134.
tion and cortical processing. Journal of Physiology, 97, Sireteanu, R., & Rieth, C. (1992). Texture segregation in infants
105–119. and children. Behavioural Brain Research, 49 (1), 133–139.
Kemner, C., Lamme, V.A.F., Kovacs, I., & van Engeland, H. Stettler, D.D., Das, A., Bennett, J., & Gilbert, C.D. (2002).
(2007). Integrity of lateral and feedbackward connections in Lateral connectivity and contextual interactions in macaque
visual processing in children with pervasive developmental primary visual cortex. Neuron, 36, 739–750.
disorder. Neuropsychology, 45, 1293–1298. Supekar, K., Musen, M., & Menon, V. (2009). Development of
Kovacs, I. (2000). Human development of perceptual organi- large-scale functional brain networks in children. PLOS
zation. Vision Research, 40, 1301–1310. Biology, 7, e1000157.
Kovacs, I., & Julesz, B. (1993). A closed curve is much more Super, H., Spekreijse, H., & Lamme, V.A.F. (2001). Two
than an incomplete one: effect of closure in figure–ground distinct modes of sensory processing observed in monkey
segmentation. Proceedings of the National Academy of primary visual cortex (V1). Nature Neuroscience, 4, 304–310.
Sciences, USA, 90, 7495–7497. Vakorin, V.A., Lippe, S., & McIntosh, A.R. (2011). Variability
Kovacs, I., Kozma, P., Feher, A., & Benedek, G. (1999). Late of brain signals processed locally transforms into higher
maturation of visual spatial integration in humans. Proceed- connectivity with brain development. Journal of Neurosci-
ings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 96, 12204– ence, 31, 6405–6413.
12209. Van den Boomen, C., van der Smagt, M.J., & Kemner, C.
Lamme, V.A.F. (1995). The neurophysiology of figure–ground (2012). Keep your eyes on development: the behavioral and
segregation in primary visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, neurophysiological development of visual mechanisms
15, 1605–1615. underlying form processing. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 3, 16.
Lamme, V.A.F., Rodriguez-Rodriguez, V., & Spekreijse, H. Vandenbroucke, M.W.G., Scholte, H.S., Van Engeland, H.,
(1999). Separate processing dynamics for texture elements, Lamme, V.A.F., & Kemner, C. (2008). A neural substrate for
boundaries and surfaces in primary visual cortex of the atypical low-level visual processing in autism spectrum
macaque monkey. Cerebral Cortex, 9, 406–413. disorder. Brain, 131, 1013–1024.
Lamme, V.A.F., & Roelfsema, P.R. (2000). The distinct modes Vandenbroucke, M.W.G., Scholte, H.S., Van Engeland, H.,
of vision offered by feedforward and recurrent processing. Lamme, V.A.F., & Kemner, C. (2009). A new approach to the
Trends in Neurosciences, 23, 571–579. study of detail perception in Autism Spectrum Disorder
Lamme, V.A.F., Super, H., & Spekreijse, H. (1998). Feedfor- (ASD): investigating visual feedforward, horizontal and
ward, horizontal, and feedback processing in the visual feedback processing. Vision Research, 49, 1006–1016.
cortex. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 8, 529–535. Wielaard, J., & Sajda, P. (2006). Extraclassical receptive field
Lamme, V.A.F., van Dijk, B.W., & Spekreijse, H. (1992). phenomena and short-range connectivity in V1. Cerebral
Texture segregation is processed by primary visual cortex in Cortex, 16, 1531–1545.
man and monkey: evidence from VEP experiments. Vision Zhang, N.R., & von der Heydt, R. (2010). Analysis of the
Research, 32, 797–807. context integration mechanisms underlying figure–ground
Luck, S.J. (2005). An introduction to the event-related potential organization in the visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 30,
technique. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. 6482–6496.
Nassi, J.J., & Callaway, E.M. (2009). Parallel processing Zipser, K., Lamme, V.A.F., & Schiller, P.H. (1996). Contextual
strategies of the primate visual system. Nature Reviews modulation in primary visual cortex. Journal of Neurosci-
Neuroscience, 10, 360–372. ence, 16, 7376–7389.
Paus, T., Collins, D.L., Evans, A.C., Leonard, G., Pike, B., &
Zijdenbos, A. (2001). Maturation of white matter in the Received: 18 July 2012
human brain: a review of magnetic resonance studies. Brain Accepted: 9 May 2013
Research Bulletin, 54 (3), 255–266.

© 2013 John Wiley & Sons Ltd

You might also like