You are on page 1of 11

Tourism Management 33 (2012) 1270e1280

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Tourism Management
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/tourman

Designing persuasive destination websites: A mental imagery processing


perspective
Woojin Lee a, *, Ulrike Gretzel b,1
a
Tourism Development & Management Program, School of Community Resources & Development, Arizona State University, 411 N. Central Ave., Suite 550, Phoenix,
AZ 85004-0953, USA
b
Laboratory for Intelligent Systems in Tourism, Institute for Innovation in Business and Social Research, University of Wollongong, Northfields Ave., Wollongong, NSW 2522, Australia

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Existing research suggests that consumers’ choices of vacations may be significantly influenced by
Received 1 November 2010 mental imagery processing, which is considered to be high elaboration cognitive processing. This study
Accepted 30 October 2011 first aimed to identify imagery-eliciting Web site features, and second to test their influence on
persuasion-related outcomes such as attitude strength, confidence, and attitude resistance. A total of 252
Keywords: subjects participated in a Web-based experiment to examine the influence of selected Web site features
Website features
on individual imagery processing and its effect on consumers’ attitudes and expectations. The data was
Narrative text
analyzed primarily using a structural equation modeling methodology. Structural model results revealed
Pictures
Sound
that the mental imagery construct strongly influenced the measured communication effects. In terms of
Mental imagery inducing mental imagery processing, only pictures were found to make a significant difference. The
Attitude strength findings stress the importance of researching mental imagery processing in tourism and provide practical
Attitude confidence implications for the design of persuasive destination Web sites.
Attitude resistance Ó 2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Online-destination marketing

1. Introduction features of destination Web sites that can effectively persuade and
encourage users to elaborate on the portrayed advertising
Persuasive Web sites are those that can influence the attitudes messages so they can form resistant attitudes.
of Web site users (Morosan & Fesenmaier, 2007). Web sites’ influ- Recent studies have especially addressed the importance of
ence on product attitudes has been confirmed by empirical virtually transporting the consumers of travel information to the
research (Lee, Gretzel, & Law, 2010; Macias, 2003). Strong attitudes destination to support the formation of concrete expectations (Lee
that are resistant to change are especially important in the context et al., 2010; Rozier-Rich & Santos, 2010). It is indeed a major concern
of tourism destinations, for which online information is abundant of providers of tourism products to encourage quasi-trial experi-
(Xiang, Wöber, & Fesenmaier, 2008), competition for the attention ences to support travel decision-making processes (Stamboulis &
of consumers is fierce (Gretzel, Fesenmaier, Formica, & O’Leary, Skayannis, 2003). Since actual product trial is impossible, being
2006), and expectation formation is difficult due to the able to vividly imagine what a destination is like is the next best
complexity and experiential nature of the destination product alternative if the goal is to form concrete expectations (Goossens,
(Nelson, 1970). Consumers planning a vacation are also likely to 1995). Oh, Fiore, and Jeoung (2007) emphasize that destination
encounter traveler reviews and other forms of social media (Xiang choice is influenced by the mental images the tourist forms based
& Gretzel, 2010) that can challenge the brand image portrayed by on the expected experience at the destination. Further, Miller and
the official destination Web site. Further, research has found that Stoica (2003) have demonstrated that consumers’ choices of
users typically make very quick judgments about destination Web vacations may be significantly influenced by mental imagery
sites (Kim & Fesenmaier, 2008); therefore, there is a great practical processing.
need from the destination marketing point of view to identify those Mental imagery processing refers to high elaboration processing
and involves the representation of nonverbal information in
working memory (MacInnis & Price, 1987). When travelers engage
* Corresponding author. Tel.: þ1 602 496 1228; fax: þ1 602 496 0853.
E-mail addresses: Woojin.Lee.1@asu.edu (W. Lee), ugretzel@uow.edu.au
in mental imagery processing, they experience the destination in
(U. Gretzel). their mind’s eye. Such processing is believed to help in learning
1
Tel.: þ61 2 4221 4823; fax: þ61 2 4221 4210. about a product (Elliott, 1973). Research has also found that it helps

0261-5177/$ e see front matter Ó 2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.tourman.2011.10.012
W. Lee, U. Gretzel / Tourism Management 33 (2012) 1270e1280 1271

in the persuasion process. For instance, MacInnis and Price (1990) 2000). It is the latter that is of specific interest in the context of
have pointed out that mental imagery has a positive influence on persuasion. Delgadillo and Escalas (2004) identified two required
consumer attitudes and leads to greater confidence that events will structural features that distinguish narratives from other text
unfold as expected. Past research has revealed that imagery can be genres: chronology and causality. The first refers to events
stimulated by various external advertising elements such as described in the narrative as being organized with respect to
pictures, concrete words, sound effects, and instructions to imagine a temporal sequence. Second, text elements are structured in a way
(Babin & Burns, 1997; Bone & Ellen, 1992; Lutz & Lutz, 1977; that builds relationships among characters and objects, thus
MacInnis & Price, 1987; Miller & Marks, 1997). Recently, Lee et al. allowing for causal inference. Similarly, Stein and Albro (1997, p. 12)
(2010) identified sensory descriptions on destination Web sites as suggest that stories are comprised of “goal-directed action-
textual elements that encourage mental imagery processing. outcome sequences” called “episode schemata,” that is, they start
Building on this existing research, the study presented in this paper from initial events that result in a response from a character’s
tests the ability of textual, auditory, and pictorial features on physical condition, psychological state, or both. These responses
destination Web sites to foster strong and resistant attitudes by develop goals that lead to courses of action and certain outcomes
eliciting mental imagery processing. (Pennington & Hastie, 1986).
The specific structure of narratives influences the way in which
2. Theoretical background they are processed. According to Packer and Jordan (2001),
narratives allow the human mind to “collapse boundaries of space
2.1. Mental imagery processing and persuasion and time, drawing attention to previously undetected connections,
creating links between disparate ideas and elements (p. 174).”
Mental imagery processing can be conceptualized as “a mode of Some researchers claim that this is the case because the structure
information processing which includes sensory representations of narratives resembles the associative way the human mind
(images) in working memory that are used in the same way as stores knowledge (Schank & Abelson, 1995). They are also inher-
perceptions of external stimuli” (Goossens, 2000; p. 306). Similarly, ently entertaining (Brewer, 1988). Green and Brock (2002) stress
it is identified by Lutz and Lutz (1978) as “a mental event involving that mental imagery may be facilitated by narratives because they
visualization of a concept or relationship” (p. 611). Mental imagery allow for deep immersion and, thus, “transportation” into the
theory assumes that we mentally represent in our minds some- story. Transportation into a narrative world is conceptualized as
thing that was presented to us before by experience or that we a distinct mental process, which may mediate the impact of
imagine based on information available. According to Burns, narratives on beliefs, and may derive its force from individuals’
Biswas, and Babin (1993), this mental imagery can be experi- general ability to create vivid mental images (Escalas, 2004; Green
enced in different sensory modalities and can vary in terms of & Brock, 2000).
vividness and quantity. Research on imagery has provided strong Narrative processing has indeed been linked to persuasion.
evidence that elaborate imagery processing may positively influ- Padgett and Allen (1997) contend that narrative advertising would
ence decision outcomes (Babin & Burns, 1997; Bone & Ellen, 1990). be the most effective way to communicate, especially in the context
From the perspective of consumer research, a consumer’s of an experiential product such as tourism. This argument has also
mental image of a tourism product can be the main source of been made by Mattila (2000). Escalas (2007) finds that self-
information available to enhance expectations and facilitate referenced narratives enable consumers to generate a positive
purchasing decisions (Walters, Sparks, & Herington, 2007). When evaluation of an advertised product no matter what level of argu-
imagery is encouraged through vivid product information, it ment strength they include. When narrative ads are presented in
strongly influences consumers’ attitudinal judgments (McGill & the form of stories, potential customers are likely to envision
Anand, 1989). Most importantly, several researchers have found functional consequences and derive symbolic meanings to inter-
that attitudes formed based on mental imagery processing tend to pret the advertisement (Padgett & Allen, 1997). Phillips and
be stronger, more stable over time, and more resistant to persua- McQuarrie (2010) also point out that the greater the trans-
sion because of its high elaboration quality (Haugtvedt & Petty, portation into a story, the greater the belief that the world within
1992; Petty, Haugtvedt, & Smith, 1995). Considering these find- the story is true, since there will be less critical examination of the
ings, it is critically important for tourism marketers to understand ideas that were presented in the story world.
what types of stimuli on a destination Web site efficiently induce In the context of tourism, Gretzel (2006) suggests that “travel
mental imagery so that they can design persuasive sites that can stories help us understand and make meaning of our travel expe-
successfully compete in the tourism information space. riences and encourage us to relive and reflect on trips, as well as
integrate travel experiences with the rest of our experiences and
2.2. Narrative information processing knowledge” (p. 175). Adaval and Wyer (1998) found that vacations
at unfamiliar destinations were more positively evaluated by
While the impact of pictures on imagery processing has been consumers who were exposed to advertising information in
studied extensively in consumer research (e.g. Babin & Burns, 1997; a narrative format rather than in a list of attributes. According to
Babin, Burns, & Biswas, 1992; Burns et al., 1993; Paivio, 1971; research by Tussyadiah and Fesenmaier (2008), the narrative
Rossiter, 1978), little empirical research exists that deals with the structure of travel blogs allows readers to feel empathy and asso-
effects of text formats on mental imagery processing. Brewer ciate the experiences of the blogger with their own. A connection
(1988) claims that text genre can indeed be an important factor between the reading of travel narratives and narrative trans-
in encouraging mental imagery processing and Lee et al. (2010) also portation was established by Rozier-Rich and Santos (2010).
found textual qualities of destination Web sites to matter. Thus, While tourism-related print and TV advertising make extensive
there is a need to further examine the relations between textual use of sensory descriptions and narrative texts, destination Web
genre and mental imagery. sites often contain functional lists of, for example, attractions or
A text genre very common in tourism promotion is the narrative accommodation establishments (Gretzel & Fesenmaier, 2002). The
(Rozier-Rich & Santos, 2010). Narrative in a broad sense refers to question is whether including a greater amount of narrative text on
anything recounted or retold; in a narrower sense it describes destination Web sites will increase the persuasiveness of these Web
something that is told or recounted in the form of a story (Denning, sites.
1272 W. Lee, U. Gretzel / Tourism Management 33 (2012) 1270e1280

2.3. Processing pictures and sounds visual, verbal and auditory stimuli should therefore contribute to
their persuasiveness.
As mentioned above, pictures have been studied extensively in Along with multimedia learning theory and dual coding theory,
the context of mental imagery processing (Kisielius & Sternthal, media richness theory, which was initially posited by Daft and
1984; Rossiter, 1978; Shepard, 1967). In addition, Lutz and Lutz Lengel (1986), also asserts that multiple cues embedded in
(1977, 1978), Alesandrini and Sheikh (1983), and Rossiter and messages support information processing tasks. Rich media are
Percy (1983) investigated the effects of various types of pictures especially important in ambiguous communication contexts (Daft,
specifically on recall and attitude. Babin et al. (1992) found a picture Lengel, & Trevino, 1987; Dennis & Kinney, 1998). In the context of
superiority effect which demonstrated that visual information online consumer behavior, a recent study noted that the perceived
would be remembered over verbal information. In the context of richness of online stores may have a significant impact on
destination marketing, Olson, McAlexander, and Roberts (1986) consumers’ intentions to buy online (Brunelle & Lapierre, 2008).
found that pictures presented in destination advertisements have However, a priori statements of one medium being richer than
an influence on a consumer’s perception of the vacation experience another are inappropriate as they neglect the specific communi-
through the association of variety of pictures with certain types of cation context (Dennis & Valacich, 1999). Furthermore, D’Ambra
experiences. Furthermore, Miller and Stoica (2003) indicated that (1995) asserted that the measures provided by Daft et al. (1987)
photographic images of beach scenes effectively stimulated mental were highly unreliable and unidimensional. Due to these reasons,
imagery processing. More recently, Walters et al. (2007) asserted this study adopted dual coding theory and multimedia learning
that the presence of more concrete pictures affects the extent of theory as theoretical frameworks to guide the research.
elaboration and the quality of consumers’ consumption visions.
Adaval and Wyer (1998) noted that the addition of pictures to 2.4. Communication effects of mental imagery processing
a narrative format can encourage readers to imagine the sequence
of events, and can facilitate the construction of a representation to Mental imagery plays a significant role in mediating ad-evoked
be used as a basis for judgment. In a more recent study they feelings and attitudes (Bone & Ellen, 1990; 1992; Burns et al., 1993;
reconfirmed that a picture presented in combination with narrative Mitchell, 1986; Mitchell & Olson, 1981). Since mental imagery is
text can cause a verbal event description to be more vivid, help the self-generated cognitive processing, it can be expected to be more
recipients to enhance perceptual links between events, and personally relevant (Escalas, 2007). Thus, it may result in self-
increase the story’s coherence (Adaval, Isbell, & Wyer, 2007). On the generated persuasion and stronger attitudes (MacInnis &
other hand, Goossens (1995) claimed that using pictorial images Jaworski, 1989). Importantly, mental imagery can lead to self-sell
may smother individuals’ imagination as they become over-reliant and therefore greater belief that the formed attitude is correct
on the featured image as an information source rather than elab- (Tormala & Petty, 2004). The literature also infers stronger attitudes
orating on their own mentally created images. as a result of mental imagery processing from the fact that it
With regard to the effects of sound on mental imagery, several involves high elaboration processing (Haugtvedt & Petty, 1992;
researchers found that the inclusion of sound effects increased Petty et al., 1995). Petty and Krosnick (1995) noted that there are
mental imagery processing (Ferrington, 1994; Miller & Marks, 1992, two dimensions of strong attitudes: the first is related to the
1997). Lee et al. (2010), however, did not identify any significant persistence of an attitude (stability), which is the degree to which
sound effects. Interestingly, Frick (1984) discovered that if pro- an attitude remains unchanged over an extended period. The other
cessing was evoked visually with a set of items followed by a series is resistance: the attitude’s ability to withstand an attack. Research
of auditory items, recall was better than if both sets of items were by Petty and Cacioppo (1986) indicates that strong attitudes are
elicited by auditory or visual modes alone. With regard to mixing more likely to come to mind faster, persist over time, resist counter
sound and narrative text, Kerr (1999) claimed that sounds add persuasive attempts and guide behavior more than weak attitudes.
critical information to narrative text. Even more persuasively, Several researchers have also found a connection between attitude
Verhallen, Bus, and De Jong (2006) showed that stories accompa- certainty and resistance to persuasion (Bassilli, 1996; Krosnick &
nied by multimedia features including video, sounds and music Abelson, 1992; Pomerantz, Chaiken, & Tordesillas; 1995; Swan,
were able to stimulate people to construct meaning and gain Pelham, & Chidester, 1988; Tormala & Petty, 2002). Such resis-
a deeper understanding of the story line. Specifically, presenting tance to persuasion is of great importance in an online travel
the story with sounds can result in a listener’s greater ability to planning context, where new and potentially negative information
process the story and to construct a more coherent mental repre- about a destination is only a click away.
sentation of the story events (Verhallen et al., 2006). These findings
are consistent with dual coding theory. 3. Conceptual framework and hypotheses
Dual coding theory developed by Paivio (1971) points out that
people learn better when stimuli include related verbal and While there is strong theoretical and, in part, also empirical
pictorial information compared to verbal information alone or support for the positive effects of narrative, pictorial and auditory
pictorial information alone. He also indicated that information stimuli on mental imagery processing and for mental imagery
presented via the pictorial channel is more salient and better processing leading to stronger attitudes, no empirical study has so
remembered than information presented through the verbal far tested these relationships in a comprehensive model. A
channel (Paivio, 1991). Richardson (1999) also pointed out that comprehensive model is important to simultaneously test the
information is likely to be retrieved more accurately when it is effects and also to identify interaction effects. Further, most of the
encoded using dual codes rather than just one code. If one code is research on which the theories are based was conducted using
forgotten, the other code can still facilitate the retrieval of all the print advertising. In contrast, the current study seeks to examine
information. Lukosius (2004) claimed that the more codes (e.g. whether these assumptions hold true in the context of multimedia
picture, sound, text, touch, etc.) used, the better the recall. Multi- environments such as destination Web sites.
media learning theory developed by Mayer (1997) suggests that As illustrated in Fig. 1, specific Web site features (text informa-
multimedia presentations can foster focused attention and tion in narrative format, presence of picture and presence of sound)
immersion into the content and, consequently, can make it more are posited as predictor variables, while communication effects in
meaningful. The ability of destination Web sites to include both the form of attitude strength, attitude confidence, and attitude
W. Lee, U. Gretzel / Tourism Management 33 (2012) 1270e1280 1273

Website H5 Attitude strength and confidence effects generated through


Characteristics mental imagery processing significantly influence attitude
Narrative Text vs. resistance to change.
Expository Text Attitude Strength
H1 H4
& Confidence
Mental
Presence of Picture Imagery
H2 4. Research methodology
vs. No Picture Processing H5

Presence of Sound
H3
Attitude An experiment was designed to examine the influence of Web
vs. No Sound Resistance site design features including narrative texts, presence of pictures,
and sound effects on attitude strength, confidence and resistance,
with the assumption that this influence is mediated by mental
Fig. 1. Conceptual framework. imagery processing induced by these Web site elements.

4.1. Design
resistance are assigned as dependent variables. Mental imagery
serves as a mediator to accentuate or attenuate the effects of the The study used a 2 (narrative text vs. expository text)  2 (picture
predictor variables on the dependent constructs. Overall, the vs. no-picture)  2 (sound vs. no sound) full factorial between-
conceptual framework indicates that an imagery-eliciting Web site subjects design to determine the main and interaction effects of
feature is assumed to evoke mental imagery, which in turn is the influence of narrative content, pictures, and sounds on mental
expected to lead to stronger attitudes, i.e., attitudes that are more imagery processing and ultimately attitude strength, confidence and
resistant to counter-persuasion attacks. resistance. The Web site designed for the purpose of the experiment
As described in the literature review, narratives, pictures and showed a fictitious island destination. Only the homepage of the
sounds enrich information, foster immersion, and encourage Web site was visible, with the links indicated on the page not being
a deeper and more extensive processing of content. Therefore, the active to achieve controlled exposure to the text/picture/sound
following hypotheses are proposed: elements. There were a total of eight experimental conditions. The
contents and lengths of the text included on the sites were the same,
H1 Narrative text on a destination Web site has a positive influence only the text structure varied. Narrative text and expository text
on mental imagery processing. differ in that narrative text portrays a sequence of events, includes
H2 Pictures on a destination Web site positively affect mental protagonists, and connects parts of the text by means of suggesting
imagery processing. causal relationships (Wolfe, 2005). In contrast, expository text is
H3 The presence of sound on a destination Web site encourages descriptive, does not refer to protagonists, and does not seek to
mental imagery processing. connect the individual text elements (Fig. 2). The narrative text
condition is presented in Fig. 3. The Web site shown to the subjects
According to the cognitive theory of multimedia learning only included the logo and text in the no-picture condition. The
(Mayer, 1997), a story supplemented with multimedia such as picture condition (Fig. 3) included a series of four revolving pictures
pictures or sounds may foster a deeper understanding of infor- of island, beach, and ocean scenes. In the sound condition, the
mation. Additionally, research by Verhallen et al. (2006) suggests Website included ocean sounds (crashing waves and seagulls). All
that presenting pictures or sounds can increase the ability to conditions were pre-tested to ensure their appropriateness.
process a story and construct a more coherent mental representa-
tion of story events. Stories can uniquely incorporate pictures or 4.2. Measures
sounds to enhance perceptual links between events and to increase
the story’s coherence (Adaval et al., 2007). Thus, there is strong 4.2.1. Mental imagery
support for interaction effects to take place among the mental To measure the mental imagery construct, the present study
imagery inducing stimuli. adapted an imagery scale developed by Miller, Hadjimarcou, and
Miciak (2000). This mental imagery scale consists of four dimen-
H1a The positive impact of narrative text on imagery processing sions: 1) vividness; 2) quantity; 3) valence; and, 4) modality.
will be even greater when pictures are present. Quantity and modality were measured using seven-point rating
H1b The positive impact of narrative text on imagery processing scales ranging from 1 (Strongly disagree) to 7 (Strongly agree),
will be even greater when sounds are present. including three items for quantity and two items for modality
H1c The positive impact of narrative text on imagery processing (Table 1). Only taste and scent were tested as modalities as sound
will be the greatest when pictures and sounds are both and visual stimuli were provided in some of the conditions and
present. touch was not applicable in the context of the study. The vividness
H2a The positive impact of pictures on imagery processing will be and valence scales were measured using seven-point semantic
greater when sound is present. differential scales, including five items for vividness and five for
valence (Table 1). The items were preceded by the introductory
The reviewed literature suggests that mental imagery processing statement “The mental imagery I experienced was.”.
is a form of cognitive processing that is elaborate and focused and
very influential when it comes to attitude formation. Consequently, 4.2.2. Attitude scales
when customers surf destination Web sites, imagery processing As dependent variables, the current study explored attitude
allows them to form more concrete expectations and engage in strength, confidence, and resistance of attitude to counter-
greater information elaboration, which will likely instill confidence arguments. The scales for attitude strength and confidence were
and make their attitudes stronger and more resistant to change. adapted and modified from Bizer, Tormala, Rucker, and Petty (2006)
using seven-point rating scales (Table 1). Attitude strength was
H4 Mental imagery processing leads to positive communication measured with four items and confidence with two. In order to
effects, namely greater attitude strength and confidence. measure attitude resistance, subjects were exposed to a negative
1274 W. Lee, U. Gretzel / Tourism Management 33 (2012) 1270e1280

Fig. 2. Experimental condition: expository text only.

Fig. 3. Experimental condition: Narrative text, picture, no sound. **denotes significance at the .5 level.
W. Lee, U. Gretzel / Tourism Management 33 (2012) 1270e1280 1275

Table 1
Descriptive statistics for each construct.

Construct name Items in scale Mean SD Internal consistency

Item No. Item wording Cronbach’s Alpha


Quantity Q12a Many images came to my mind 5.23 1.34 .92
Q12b A lot of images came to my mind 4.96 1.45
Q12c I experienced various images in my mind 5.02 1.41
Modality Q12d I imagined tastes 3.38 1.71 .82
Q12e I imagined scents 3.72 1.85
Vividness Q13a VivideVague 2.77 1.49 .93
Q13b CleareUnclear 2.90 1.44
Q13c SharpeDull 3.34 1.60
Q13d IntenseeWeak 3.69 1.61
Q13e Well-definedeFuzzy 3.28 1.61
Valence Q13f PleasanteUnpleasant 2.20 1.21 .96
Q13g GoodeBad 2.37 1.26
Q13h NiceeAwful 2.38 1.30
Q13i LikableeNot likeable 2.21 1.23
Q13j PositiveeNot positive 2.08 1.16
Attitude strength-time 1 Q14 How good would the destination be for a pleasure trip? 6.12 1.02 .94
Q15 How positive would you say is your impression of the destination? 5.88 1.01
Q16 How much do you think you would like this destination? 6.02 1.05
Q17 How favorable is your impression of the destination? 5.86 1.05
Attitude confidence Q18 How certain are you of your attitude toward this destination? 5.40 1.23 .84
Q19 How sure are you that your current impression of the destination is correct? 4.85 1.39
Attitude strength-time 2 Q20 How good would the destination be for a pleasure trip? 3.68 1.48 .95
Q21 How positive would you say is your impression of the destination? 3.46 1.36
Q22 How much do you think you would like this destination? 3.72 1.54
Q23 How favorable is your impression of the destination? 3.47 1.32

destination review that contradicted the initial destination were asked to indicate their attitudes toward the destination again
description. Attitude strength was measured before as well as after after their exposure to the negative review. After completing the
the exposure to the reviews. Accordingly, an attitude resistance survey, all subjects were debriefed and compensated.
measure was produced by subtracting the attitude strength score of
each item in time two from attitude strength scores in time one. 4.5. Manipulation checks

4.3. Participants To make sure that the manipulations were reliable and effective,
this study conducted several manipulation checks during pre-tests by
This study intended to obtain a minimum of 25 responses per asking pre-test subjects about the believability of the Website and the
experimental cell; thus, at least 200 participants were needed. The destination review as well as the suitability of the texts, pictures and
goal of the study was to test a theoretical model rather than to the sound. More specifically, 15 graduate students and 20 students
describe occurrences of a phenomenon in a population; also, influ- from the same subject pool as the main study were involved in
ences of visual, textual and auditory stimuli on mental imagery a series of pre-tests to evaluate the validity and score reliability of the
processing are based on very basic cognitive processes in which scales used in this study as well as to test whether subjects could
social characteristics should not play a role. Thus, a student sample successfully distinguish the narrative text from the expository text;
was deemed to be appropriate. Problems arising from potential pre-testing determined that this was the case. During the main study,
gender differences were avoided by using random assignment to the manipulation checks aimed to make sure that the subjects actually
experimental conditions. A $3 gift card was given to each participant paid attention to the manipulations. First, all subjects in the picture
as an incentive to join the study. The recruitment effort resulted in conditions stated that they saw pictures. Second, all of those in the
252 responses. However, seven students could not complete the sound conditions indicated that they had heard sound.
survey due to technical problems, and six students completed the
survey but indicated the same response for every question. Conse- 4.6. Analysis
quently, a total of 239 subjects were included in the analysis.
The hypothesized relationships between independent, medi-
4.4. Procedure ator, and dependent measures were tested by means of structural
equation modeling (SEM) using Amos 5.0 (Arbuckle, 2003). To
When subjects arrived at the research lab, they were greeted by address problems with large sample size requirements, unreli-
a research assistant and informed that an online travel agency was ability, and non-normal or coarsely measured item-level data,
interested in developing a newly created destination Website. Next, parceling was used (Landis, Beal, & Tesluk, 2000). The idea of
they were seated at a computer and randomly assigned to an creating and using item parcels was originally introduced by Cattell
experimental condition based on the order of their appearance. The (1956), and further explored by Cattell and Burdsal (1975).
subjects were instructed to carefully look at the Website and read the Parceling is usually done by summing or averaging several items
vacation destination description provided. After being exposed to the that presumably measure the same construct (Meade & Kroustalis,
Website for 2 min, they were presented with an online, self- 2005). For the current study, a total of six parcels were created for
administered questionnaire asking them to evaluate the Website each dimension underlying a latent construct. First, under the
and rate their attitude toward the destination. The Web-based survey mental imagery construct, quantity, modality, vividness and
then presented a negative review of the destination. The participants valence parcels were created by averaging the item scores. In the
1276 W. Lee, U. Gretzel / Tourism Management 33 (2012) 1270e1280

context of the communication effect construct, attitude strength Table 3


and attitude confidence parcels were produced. However, attitude Model-fit indices for structural model.

resistance was measured using its four items separately since the Model-fit Statistics c2 df c2/df CFI GFI NFI RMSEA
construct consisted of only one dimension. 132.49 65 2.03 .93 .93 .89 .065

5. Results RMSEA values less than .08 are seen as indicating satisfactory fit
(Hu & Bentler, 1999).
5.1. Measurement model The squared multiple correlation coefficients (SMC; R2) for
indicator variables were assessed. SMCs lie between 0 and 1 (the
The originally proposed measurement model was estimated closer to 1, the more of the latent construct can be explained by the
using maximum-likelihood estimation. As suggested by Hu and model). The results reveal that the model does not explain mental
Bentler (1999) and Quintana and Maxwell (1999), several indices imagery (R2 ¼ .07) and attitude resistance (R2 ¼ .10) very well.
were used to assess goodness of fit for the model. The initial test of However, it explains a large portion of the communication effects
the measurement model resulted in poor model fit (Chi-Square/ construct (R2 ¼ .72).
df ¼ 4.1; CFI ¼ .95; NFI ¼ .93, RMSEA ¼ .10). In particular, the mental Fig. 4 shows the hypothesized structural model paths. Most of the
imagery construct exhibited very poor construct reliability, as indi- model paths yielded significant parameter estimates. However, in the
cated by the fact that the four parcel indicators loaded inappropri- context of the impact of multimedia stimuli (e.g., narrative text,
ately onto the latent construct. Additional analyses suggested that pictures, and sound) on mental imagery processing, only pictures
vividness and valence variables were causing the problems. There- showed a significant effect (b ¼ .23, p < .01). These results provide
fore, an alternative model was estimated that only included quantity support for Hypothesis 2, but not for Hypotheses 1 and 3. Addition-
and modality as dimensions of mental imagery processing. As dis- ally, the interaction effects were not significant. Thus, the results of
cussed by other researchers (Chin & Todd, 1995; Gefen, Straub, & the structural model did not support H1a, H1b, H1c, and H2a.
Boudreau, 2000; MacCallum, Roznowski, & Necowitz, 1992), the Most importantly, Fig. 4 reveals that the mental imagery
dropping of measurement items to improve model fit should be construct strongly influenced the communication effects construct
executed with caution as it can generate an overfitting of the model (b ¼ .85, p < .01), which is represented by attitude strength and
to the data. As recommended in the literature (MacCallum et al., attitude confidence. In other words, evoked mental imagery leads
1992), only a few model modifications were made to fix the rela- to positive communication effects, which supports Hypothesis 4. In
tively severe model-fit problems. Also, appropriateness from addition, the results presented in Fig. 4 show that attitude strength
a theoretical point of view was a major decision criterion. The overall and confidence have a significant impact on attitude resistance
model fit improved: Chi-Square/df ¼ 3.0, which is appropriate (Kline, (b ¼ .32, p < .01). This implies that the stronger the attitude the
1998); CFI ¼ .97; NFI ¼ .95; and RMSEA ¼ .09; consequently, the more resistant it is to negative stimuli. This finding supports
alternative model was adopted. The discriminant validity of the Hypothesis 5. In addition, this study confirmed full mediation of
constructs was tested by calculating the squared roots of average mental imagery processing as the modification indices for the
variance extracted (AVE). As shown in Table 2, all AVE scores were model did not suggest the need for modeling additional relation-
higher than the threshold of .5 as recommended by Fornell and ships among the constructs.
Larcker (1981) for all three constructs (mental imagery ¼ .65;
communication effect ¼ .84; attitude resistance ¼ .84). Second, the 6. Implications
convergent validity of the constructs was examined. All items had
factor loadings higher than .5, which suggests good convergent The purpose of this study was to generate insights into the nature
validity (Nunnally, 1967). Composite reliability was computed to of imagery-evoking Website features including narrative text,
assess the internal consistency of the constructs. As shown in Table 2, pictures, and sounds, and to explore the relationship between these
the composite reliability values of all of the constructs exceeded the Web site features and communication effects that are encouraged
minimum .60 suggested by Nunnally and Bernstein (1994). through mental imagery processing. The overarching goal was to see
whether certain Website features can induce changes in attitudes
5.2. Structural model that are resistant to subsequent persuasion attempts.

The overall model fit and the regression paths were estimated to 6.1. Theoretical implications
determine if the proposed model described the data well. As shown
in Table 3, the model fit is acceptable. The normalized chi-square Prior research has found that mental imagery processing can be
statistic indicated an adequate fit (Kline, 1998). Further, the rec- evoked by various external advertising stimuli (Babin & Burns,
ommended threshold is .9. for NFI, CFI and GFI (Brown, 2006), and 1997; Bone & Ellen, 1992; Lutz & Lutz, 1977; MacInnis & Price,

Table 2
Alternative measurement model analysis.

Latent variable Indicators Factor loadings Construct reliability Average variance extracted (AVE)
Mental imagery Quantity .72 .60 .65
Modality .58
Communication effect Attitude strength .96 .82 .84
Attitude confidence .69
Attitude resistance Attitude resistance 1 .76 .90 .84
Attitude resistance 2 .78
Attitude resistance 3 .86
Attitude resistance 4 .94
W. Lee, U. Gretzel / Tourism Management 33 (2012) 1270e1280 1277

Narrative
d
R2 = .72
Picture .07(ns) .95**
AS e
.23** d Communication
Sound Effects AC e
.00(ns) R2 = .07 .70**

.85**
Narrative× Picture
-.05(ns) .32**
Mental Imagery 2
AR1 e
.07(ns) R = .10
Picture × Sound .06 (ns)
.76**
AR2 e
Attitude .81**
Resistance
Narrative ×Sound -.07(ns) .88** AR3 e
.72** .58**
.92**
AR4 e
Narrative×Picture d
×Sound Q M

e e

Fig. 4. Hypothesized structural model.

1987; Miller & Marks, 1997). Most of this research has focused on to travel consumers, it will be ever more important to create
print advertising and has usually only included one stimulus, which persuasive Website designs that can help consumers learn about
does not allow for interaction effects to be tested. Mental imagery destinations and form strong attitudes about them. Moreover, in
research in the context of Websites has mainly dealt with the the context of successful tourism marketing, with the rocketing
effects of object interactivity (Macias, 2003; Schlosser, 2003) and amounts of consumer-generated contents that include personal
sensory information (Lee et al., 2010). Thus, the current study has stories, pictures, and sound available on tourism Websites,
contributed to mental imagery research by simultaneously testing a persuasive Website effectively instills confidence in consumers
three different stimuli in the context of a Website. Further, in the and helps them form attitudes that are more resistant to counter-
tourism field very little research has been conducted on the effects arguments. Also, given the many benefits for marketing that can
of features (Lee & Gretzel, 2008) and particularly pictures (Jeong & be assumed to result from imagery processing, it appears to be
Choi, 2004) included in destination Websites. Thus, the findings critical for online tourism marketing to be better informed about
certainly contribute to this body of literature as well. the effectiveness of imagery processing and the stimuli that can
The most important finding of the study is that mental imagery encourage it. When consumers evaluate experience products, they
processing indeed not only leads to very strong attitudes but also may have a higher degree of uncertainty compared to other phys-
greater confidence in attitudes. This issue had not been addressed ical products. Running a simulation-like scenario in one’s mind can
in the context of tourism nor in the context of online information facilitate the process of destination decision making and lead to
processing. In the present study, mental imagery processing was more confidence in the decision. While customers consume infor-
confirmed as an important element of persuasive communication mation on travel Websites, imagery processing enables them to
in the context of travel planning, even if the persuader is not engage in greater information elaboration and to form more certain
a human communicator or a traditional advertising medium but expectations (Goossens, 2000). Tourism marketers should make an
a Website. Consequently, more emphasis needs to be placed on effort to help travel planners to imagine what it would be like to
studying mental imagery processing in the context of tourism experience the destination, which can lead to positive attitudes and
marketing and travel decision-making as well as online commu- build confidence. Furthermore, the generated high levels of
nication environments. certainty foster resistance to subsequent negative persuasion,
With respect to attitude resistance, this study also successfully which is of high practical relevance given the likely exposure to
supports past research indicating that strong attitudes significantly social media content in travel information searches (Xiang &
influence attitude resistance (Haugtvedt & Petty, 1992; Petty et al., Gretzel, 2010).
1995). Based on these findings, it can be stated that once subjects These findings can help tourism marketers understand how
form strong attitudes and confidence in the destination through critical it is to invest in careful Website design and also to
mental imagery processing, they are more likely to resist negative evaluate the effectiveness of particular designs. The current
reviews of the destination. Given that very little research has been study especially emphasizes the importance of pictures. While
conducted regarding attitude resistance in the realm of tourism many tourism marketers intuitively know that they are impor-
marketing while the likelihood of tourism consumers being tant, this study shows that they can make a significant differ-
exposed to reviews is increasing, the findings of this study are an ence; thus, decisions to place pictures on Websites need to be
important contribution to tourism marketing research. made with an understanding that changes will affect the Web-
site’s persuasiveness. In general, Websites are often designed by
6.2. Practical implications practitioners based on past experience, existing examples, or
technological considerations. The research presented in this
With the growing amount of information available on the paper argues very strongly for a more scientific approach to
Internet and the increasing number of destination options available Website design.
1278 W. Lee, U. Gretzel / Tourism Management 33 (2012) 1270e1280

6.3. Limitations and future research Several issues need to be discussed with regard to the text
manipulation. First, all subjects were exposed to a text in either
The current study should be considered in light of its limitations. expository or narrative form. Thus, even though the findings of this
First, while the manipulations were extensively pre-tested there study revealed that the narrative versus expository text did not
always remains the question of whether they were strong enough. influence mental imagery processing, it is highly possible that the
Second, using just a beach destination for this experiment may information contained in the text by itself might have evoked
constrain the generalizability of the results found in this study. The mental imagery processing. The proposed structural model showed
use of Websites for a broader array of tourism products such as that the picture condition significantly affected mental imagery, but
hotels, events and festivals, and other destinations may generate indicated a small standardized coefficient b ¼ .23, which means
different results in terms of mental imagery processing. Third, the that the pictures had a small influence on mental imagery. Never-
use of student subjects was limiting in that students can be theless, many of the subjects reported mental imagery. It needs to
different from older and less educated individuals in terms of their be asked what generated this mental imagery. Indeed, both text
experience or proficiency in interpreting information (James & conditions contained a large number of sensory words. Previous
Sonner, 2001). However, given that the research aim of the research has found that the proper mixture of sensory information
current study was to focus on theoretical explanation rather than presented on Websites can strongly evoke mental imagery, which
the generalization of the study to a population, using student can greatly influence consumer attitudes and behavioral intentions
subjects was appropriate (Peterson, 2001; Sternthal, Tybout, & (Lee et al., 2010; Miller et al., 2000; Schlosser, 2003). Another aspect
Calder, 1994; Walters et al., 2007). Moreover, as is typical for that might have to be considered is the length of the text as it was
experiments, the context was artificially created and did not rather short to avoid burdening the study subjects. In real trip
represent a real travel planning scenario, which can influence planning contexts, individuals are highly motivated to process
motivations to process the information. Further, the problems in information and will probably consume more text, which might
measuring the vividness and valence of mental imagery processing make effects more visible.
suggest that a stable measure has yet to be developed to assess Of the stimuli included in the study, only pictures significantly
these dimensions. The lack of visible effects could have been due to affected mental imagery processing. The result is consistent with
only measuring the quantity and modality of the mental imagery. existing research (Babin et al., 1992; Hirschman & Solomon, 1984;
Future research in this area should be extended to include Kisielius & Sternthal, 1984; Paivio, 1971; Rossiter, 1978; Shepard,
a greater variety of potentially imagery-evoking Website features. 1967; Starch, 1966) that finds pictures to more efficiently evoke
For example, it may be interesting to investigate how interactive or mental imagery processing than other stimuli. However, the
customized features such as interactive maps or chatting with travel insignificant results for the other modalities are inconsistent with
experts on the Website influence mental imagery processing. On the research that claims that narrative texts and sounds support mental
other hand, presenting just pictures to subjects without any other imagery processing. More research is needed to examine whether
conditions, or pictures with narration in the form of audio instead of the specific context (i.e. destinations) is the reason for this or if
narrative text, or videos that combine moving pictures with narra- other types of manipulations are needed to evoke effects.
tive and sound may result in a different level of mental imagery In addition, the current study revealed that sound does not
processing. Also, varying the length of the narratives and the type influence mental imagery. Some researchers claim that when
and quality of sound might produce different results. Additionally, imagery processing and auditory perception compete for the same
while instructions to imagine have been widely tested for evoking resources, elaboration of information can be reduced (Unnava,
mental imagery processing, they have not been tested in online Agarwal, & Haugtvedt, 1996). In other words, they assert that
environments, and thus can be a subject for future research. Further, imagery generated by subjects selectively interfered with the
the experiment was conducted using traditional computer screens. processing of externally provided information (Unnava et al.,
However, Fogg, Booker, and Don (2004) assert that recently devel- 1996). For example, while conducting the experiment for this
oped mobile phones are more compelling platforms for narratives. study, subjects were informed that they would see the Website of
They claim that mobile phones are highly personal devices, thus an island destination. At that time, subjects might have generated
people are more likely to experience content from those phones in auditory imagery by themselves before they actually heard waves
a way that is more personal and influential than content from and seagull sounds. In this case, based on Unnava et al. (1996), the
a computer. If people are indeed more receptive to persuasive interference between self-generated auditory imagery and actual
messages through mobile phones, it will be valuable to study mental sounds can adversely affect cognitive elaboration. Not enough
imagery processing while using these other types of platforms, research currently exists to address this issue in a definitive
which are increasingly used in the context of travel and tourism. manner.

Appendix 1
Intercorrelations among proposed indicators.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9
1. AtRes1 e
2. AtRes2 .39** e
3. AtRes3 .66** .48** e
4. AtRes4 .65** -.36** .78** e
5. Quan .54** .39** .66** .70** e
6. Modality .40** .42** .56** .50** .66** e
7. Vividness .20** .03 .05 .14* .23** .01 e
8. Valence .19** .04 .12 .16* .30** .13* .72** e
9. AttiS .25** .24** .37** .28** .23** .17** .00 .01 e
10. AttiC .03 .11 .01 .01 .01 .02 .10 .05 .15*

Note. N ¼ 239. 1 ¼ AtRes(Attitude resistance),2 ¼ AtRes2, 3 ¼ AtRes3, 4 ¼ AtRes4, 5 ¼ Quantity,6 ¼ Modality, 7 ¼ Vividness, 8 ¼ Valence 9 ¼ Attitude Strength, 10 ¼ Attitude
Confidence.
*p < .05. **p < .01.
W. Lee, U. Gretzel / Tourism Management 33 (2012) 1270e1280 1279

References Green, M. C., & Brock, T. C. (2000). The role of transportation in the persua-
siveness of public narratives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
79(5), 701e721.
Adaval, R., Isbell, L. M., & Wyer, R. S. (2007). The impact of pictures on narrative- and
Green, M. C., & Brock, T. C. (2002). In the mind’s eye: transportation-imagery model
list-based impression formation: a process interference model. Journal of
of narrative persuasion. In M. C. Green, J. J. Strange, & T. C. Brock (Eds.), Narrative
Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 352e364.
impact: Social and cognitive foundations (pp. 315e341). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
Adaval, R., & Wyer, R. S., Jr. (1998). The role of narratives in consumer information
Erlbaum Associate, Inc.
processing. Journal of Consumer Research, 7, 207e245.
Gretzel, U. (2006). Narrative design for travel recommender systems. In
Alesandrini, K. L., & Sheikh, A. A. (1983). Research on imagery: implications for
D. R. Fesenmaier, H. Werthner, & K. Wöber (Eds.), Travel destination recom-
advertising. In A. A. Sheikh (Ed.), Imagery: Current theory, research and appli-
mendation systems: Behavioral foundations and applications (pp. 171e179).
cation (pp. 535e556). New York: John Wiley.
London: CAB International.
Arbuckle, J. L. (2003). AMOS user’s guide, version 5.0. Chicago, IL: SmallWaters Co.
Gretzel, U., & Fesenmaier, D. R. (2002). Building narrative logic into tourism infor-
Babin, L. A., & Burns, A. C. (1997). Effects of print ad pictures and copy containing
mation systems. IEEE Intelligent Systems59e61, November/December.
instructions to imagine on mental imagery that mediates attitudes. Journal of
Gretzel, U., Fesenmaier, D. R., Formica, S., & O’Leary, J. T. (2006). Searching for the
Advertising, 26(3), 33e44.
future: challenges faced by destination marketing organizations. Journal of
Babin, L. A., Burns, A. C., & Biswas, A. (1992). A framework providing direction for
Travel Research, 45(November), 116e126.
research on communications effects of mental imagery-evoking advertising
Haugtvedt, C. P., & Petty, R. E. (1992). Personality and persuasion: need for cognition
strategies. Advances in Consumer Research, 19, 621e628.
moderates and persistence and resistance of attitude changes. Journal of
Basilli, J. N. (1996). Meta-judgmental versus operative indexes of psychological
Personality and Social Psychology, 63(2), 308e319.
attributes: the case of measures of attitude strength. Journal of Personality and
Hirschman, E., & Solomon, M. (1984). Utilitarian, aesthetic and familiarity responses
Social Psychology, 71, 637e653.
to verbal versus visual advertisements. Advances in Consumer Research, 11,
Bizer, G. Y., Tormala, Z. L., Rucker, D. D., & Petty, R. E. (2006). Memory-based versus
426e443.
on-line processing: implications for attitude strength. Journal of Experimental
Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fix indexes in covariance struc-
Social Psychology, 42, 646e653.
tural analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation
Bone, P. F., & Ellen, P. S. (1990). The effect of imagery processing and imagery
Modeling, 6, 1e55.
content on behavioral intentions. Advances in Consumer Research, 17, 449e454.
James, W. L., & Sonner, B. S. (2001). Just say no to traditional student samples.
Bone, P. F., & Ellen, P. S. (1992). Generation and consequences of communication
Journal of Advertising Research, 41(October), 63e71.
evoked imagery. Journal of Consumer Research, 19, 93e104.
Jeong, M., & Choi, J. (2004). Effects of picture presentations on consumer’s behavioral
Brewer, W. F. (1988). Imagery and text genre. Text, 8, 431e439.
intentions on the web. Journal of Travel and Tourism Marketing, 17(2/3), 193e204.
Brown, T. A. (2006). Confirmatory factor analysis for applied research. New York: The
Kerr, B. (1999). Effective use of audio media in multimedia presentations. In:
Guildford Press.
Proceedings of the mid-south instructional technology conference, Murfrees-
Brunelle, E., & Lapierre, J. (2008). Testing media richness theory to explain
boro, TN, March 28e30.
consumers’ intentions of buying online. In: Proceedings of 10th international
Kim, H., & Fesenmaier, D. R. (2008). Persuasive design of destination web sites: an
conference of electronic commerce, Innsbruck, Austria.
analysis of first impression. Journal of Travel Research, 47(3), 3e13.
Burns, A. C., Biswas, A., & Babin, L. A. (1993). The operation of visual imagery as
Kisielius, J., & Sternthal, B. (1984). Detecting and explaining vividness effects in
a mediator of advertising effects. Journal of Advertising, 22(2), 71e85.
attitudinal judgments. Journal of Marketing Research, 21, 54e64.
Cattell, R. B. (1956). Personality and motivation structure and measurement. New
Kline, R. B. (1998). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. New York:
York: World Book Co.
Guilford Press.
Cattell, R. B., & Burdsal, C. A. (1975). The radial parcel double factoring design:
Krosnick, J. A., & Abelson, R. P. (1992). The case for measuring attitude strength in
a solution to the item-vs.-parcel controversy. Multivariate Behavioral Research,
surveys. In J. Tanur (Ed.), Questions about questions (pp. 177e203). New York:
10(2), 165e179.
Russell Sage.
Chin, W. W., & Todd, P. A. (1995). On the use, usefulness and ease of use of structural
Landis, R. S., Beal, D. J., & Tesluk, P. E. (2000). A comparison of approaches to forming
equation modeling in MIS research: a note of caution. MIS Quarterly, 19(2),
composite measures in structural equation models. Organizational Research
237e246.
Methods, 3(2), 186e207.
Daft, R. L., & Lengel, R. H. (1986). Organizational information requirements, media
Lee, W., & Gretzel, U. (2008). Use and perceived usefulness of destination website
richness and structural design. Management Science, 32, 554e571.
features. In T. Hara (Ed.), Proceedings of the 13th annual graduate education and
Daft, R. L., Lengel, R. H., & Trevino, L. K. (1987). Message equivocality, media
student research conference in hospitality and tourism (pp. 142e150), Orlando, FL,
selection and manager performance: implications for information systems. MIS
January 3e5, 2008.
Quarterly355e366, September.
Lee, W., Gretzel, U., & Law, R. (2010). Quasi-trial experiences through sensory
D’Ambra, J. (1995). A field study of information technology, task equivocality, media
information on destination web sites. Journal of Travel Research, 49(3), 310e322.
richness, and media preference. PhD Dissertation, University of New South
Lukosius, V. (2004). Mental imagery in advertisements: More than meets the eye?
Wales.
Boston, MA: American Marketing Association. August 6e9.
Delgadillo, Y., & Escalas, J. (2004). Narrative word-of-mouth communication:
Lutz, K. A., & Lutz, R. J. (1977). Effects of interactive imagery on learning: application
exploring memory and attitude effects of consumer storytelling. Advances in
to advertising. Journal of Applied Psychology, 62(4), 493e498.
Consumer Research, 31, 186e192.
Lutz, K. A., & Lutz, R. J. (1978). Imagery-eliciting strategies: review and implications
Denning, S. (2000). The springboard: How storytelling ignites action in knowledge-era
of research. Advances in Consumer Research, 5, 611e620.
organizations. Boston: Butterworth-Heinemann.
MacCallum, R. C., Roznowski, M., & Necowitz, L. B. (1992). Model modifications in
Dennis, A. R., & Kinney, S. T. (1998). Testing media richness theory in the new
covariances structure analysis: the problem of capitalization on chance.
media: the effects of cues, feedback, and task equivocality. Information System
Psychological Bulletin, 111(3), 490e504.
Research, 9(3), 256e274.
Macias, W. (2003). A preliminary structural equation model of comprehension and
Dennis, A.R., & Valacich, J.S. (1999). Rethinking media richness: towards a theory of
persuasion of interactive advertising brand web sites. Journal of Interactive
media synchronicity. In: Proceedings of the 32nd Hawaii international confer-
Advertising, 3(2), 36e48.
ence on systems sciences, 1e10.
MacInnis, D. J., & Jaworski, B. J. (1989). Information processing from advertisement:
Elliot, L. (1973). Imagery versus repetition encoding in short- and long-term
toward an integrative framework. Journal of Marketing, 53, 1e23.
memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 100, 270e276.
MacInnis, D. J., & Price, L. L. (1987). The role of imagery in information processing:
Escalas, J. E. (2004). Image yourself in the product: mental simulation, narrative
review and extensions. Journal of Consumer Research, 13, 473e491.
transportation, and persuasion. Journal of Advertising, 33(2), 37e48.
MacInnis, D. J., & Price, L. L. (1990). An exploratory study of the effects of imagery
Escalas, J. E. (2007). Self-referencing and persuasion: narrative transportation
processing and consumer experience on expectations and satisfactions.
versus analytical elaboration. Journal of Consumer Research, 33, 421e429.
Advances in Consumer Research, 17, 41e47.
Ferrington, G. (1994). Audio design: creating multi-sensory images for the mind.
Mattila, A. S. (2000). The role of narratives in the advertising of experiential
Journal of Visual Literacy, 14(1), 61e67.
services. Journal of Service Research, 3(1), 35e45.
Fogg, B. J., Booker, A., & Don, A. (2004). Persuading people via computer-based
Mayer, R. E. (1997). Multimedia learning: are we asking the right questions.
narratives. Persuasive Technology Lab, Stanford University. http://captology.
Educational Psychologist, 32, 1e19.
stanford.edu/ Available at:.
McGill, A. L., & Anand, P. (1989). The effect of imagery on information processing
Fornell, C. R., & Larcker, D. F. (1981). Structural equation models with Unobserved
strategy in a multi-attribute choice task. Marketing Letters, 1(1), 7e16.
variables and measurement Error. Journal of Marketing Research, 18, 39e50.
Meade, A. W., & Kroustalis, C. M. (2005). Problems of item parceling with CFA: Test
Frick, R. (1984). Using both an auditory and a visual short-term store to increase
of measurement invariance. Paper presented at the 20th Annual Conference of
digit span. Memory and Cognition, 12, 507e514.
the Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology, Los Angeles, CA.
Gefen, D., Straub, D. W., & Boudreau, M. C. (2000). Structural equation modeling and
Miller, D. W., Hadjimarcou, J., & Miciak, A. (2000). A scale for measuring
regression: guidelines for research practice. Communications of the Association
advertisement-evoked mental imagery. Journal of Marketing Communication, 6,
for Information Systems., 4(7), 1e70.
1e20.
Goossens, C. F. (1995). External information search: effects of tour brochures with
Miller, D. M., & Marks, L. J. (1992). Mental imagery and sound effects in radio
experiential information. Journal of Travel and Tourism Marketing, 3(3), 89e107.
commercials. Journal of Advertising, 21, 83e93.
Goossens, C. F. (2000). Tourism information and pleasure motivation. Annals of
Miller, D. M., & Marks, L. J. (1997). The effects of imagery-evoking radio advertising
Tourism Research, 27(2), 301e321.
strategies on affective responses. Psychology and Marketing, 14(4), 337e360.
1280 W. Lee, U. Gretzel / Tourism Management 33 (2012) 1270e1280

Miller, D. W., & Stoica, M. (2003). Comparing the effects of a photograph versus Rossiter, J. R. (1978). Visual imagery ability as a mediator of advertising response.
artistic renditions of a beach scene in a direct response print ad for a Caribbean Advances in Consumer Research, 5, 621e629.
resort island: a mental imagery perspective. Journal of Vacation Marketing, 10(1), Rossiter, J. R., & Percy, L. (1983). Visual communication in advertising. In R. J. Harris
11e21. (Ed.), Information processing research in advertising (pp. 83e125). Hillsdale, NJ:
Mitchell, A. A. (1986). The effect of verbal and visual components of advertisements Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
on brand attitudes and attitude toward the advertisement. Journal of Consumer Rozier-Rich, S., & Santos, C. A. (2010). Processing promotional travel narratives.
Research, 13(June), 12e24. Tourism Management, . doi:10.1016/j.tourman.2010.03.009.
Mitchell, A. A., & Olson, J. C. (1981). Are product attribute beliefs the only mediator Schank, R., & Abelson, R. (1995). Knowledge and memory: the real story. In
of advertising effects on brand attribute? Journal of Marketing Research, R. S. Wyer, Jr. (Ed.), Knowledge and memory: The real story. Advances in social
18(August), 318e332. cognition, Vol. 8 (pp. 1e85). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Morosan, C., & Fesenmaier, D. R. (2007). A conceptual framework of persuasive Schlosser, A. E. (2003). Experiencing products in the virtual world: the role of goal
architecture of tourism websites: propositions and implications. In M. Sigala, and imagery in influencing attitudes versus purchase intentions. Journal of
L. Mich, & J. Murphy (Eds.), Information and communication technologies in Consumer Research, 30(3), 184e198.
tourism 2007 (pp. 243e254). Vienna, Austria: Springer Verlag. Shepard, R. N. (1967). Recognition memory for words, sentences and pictures.
Nelson, P. (1970). Information and consumer behavior. Journal of Political Economy, Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 6, 156e163.
78(March/April), 311e329. Stamboulis, Y., & Skayannis, P. (2003). Innovation strategies and technology for
Nunnally, J. G. (1967). Psychometric theory (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill. experience-based tourism. Tourism Management, 24, 35e43.
Nunnally, J. C., & Bernstein, I. H. (1994). Psychometric theory. New York: McGraw- Starch, D. (1966). How does the shape of ads affect relationship? Media/Scope, 10,
Hill. 83e85.
Oh, H., Fiore, A. M., & Jeoung, M. Y. (2007). Measuring experience economy Stein, N. L., & Albro, E. R. (1997). Building complexity and coherence: children’s use
concepts: tourism applications. Journal of Travel Research, 46, 119e132. of goal-structured knowledge in telling stories. In M. Bamberg (Ed.), Narrative
Olson, J., McAlexander, J., & Roberts, S. (1986). The impact of the visual content of development: Six approaches, Vol. 1 (pp. 5e44). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
advertisements upon the perceived vacation experience. In W. Joseph, Associates.
L. Moutinho, & I. Vernon (Eds.), Tourism services marketing: Advances in theory Sternthal, B., Tybout, A., & Calder, B. (1994). Experimental design; generalization
and practice, Vol. 2 (pp. 260e269). Cleveland State University: American and theoretical explanation. In R. Bagozzi (Ed.), Principles of marketing research
Marketing Association. (pp. 195e223). Cambridge, MA: Blackwell Publishing.
Packer, R. K., & Jordan, K. (2001). Multimedia: From Wagner to virtual reality. New Swan, W. B., Pelham, B. W., & Chidester, T. R. (1988). Change through paradox: using
York: W. W. Norton. self-verification to alter beliefs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54,
Padgett, D., & Allen, D. (1997). Communicating experiences: a narrative approach to 268e273.
creating service brand image. Journal of Advertising, 26(4), 49e62. Tormala, Z. L., & Petty, R. E. (2002). What doesn’t kill me makes me stronger: the
Paivio, A. (1971). Imagery and verbal processes. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. effects of resisting persuasion on attitude certainty. Journal of Personality and
Paivio, A. (1991). Dual coding theory: retrospect and current status. Canadian Social Psychology, 83, 1298e1313.
Journal of Psychology, 45, 255e287. Tormala, Z. L., & Petty, R. E. (2004). Resistance to persuasion and attitude certainty:
Pennington, N., & Hastie, R. (1986). Evidence evaluation in complex decision the moderating role of elaboration. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 242e258. 30(11), 1446e1457.
Peterson, R. A. (2001). On the use of college students in social science research: Tussyadiah, I. P., & Fesenmaier, D. R. (2008). Marketing places through first-person
insights from a second-order meta-analysis. Journal of Consumer Research, stories-an analysis of Pensylvania roadtripper blog. Journal of Travel & Tourism
28(3), 450e461. Marketing, 25(3), 299e311.
Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion. Unnava, H. R., Agarwal, S., & Haugtvedt, C. P. (1996). Interactive effects of presen-
Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 19, 123e205. tation modality and message-generated imagery on recall of advertising
Petty, R. E., Haugtvedt, C. P., & Smith, S. M. (1995). Elaboration as a determinant of information. Journal of Consumer Research, 23, 81e88.
attitude strength: creating attitudes that are persistent, resistant, and predic- Verhallen, M. J. A. J., Bus, A. G., & De Jong, M. T. (2006). The promise of multimedia
tive of behavior. In R. E. Petty, & J. Krosnick (Eds.), Attitude strength: Antecedents stories for kindergarten children at risk. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(2),
and consequences (pp. 93e130). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 410e419.
Petty, R. E., & Krosnick, J. A. (1995). Attitude strength: Antecedents and consequences. Walters, G., Sparks, B., & Herington, C. (2007). The effectiveness of print advertising
Hillside, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. stimuli evoking elaborate consumption visions for potential travelers. Journal of
Phillips, B. J., & McQuarrie, E. F. (2010). Narrative and persuasion in fashion Travel Research, 46(3), 24e34.
advertising. Journal of Consumer Research, 37(October), 368e392. Wolfe, M. B. W. (2005). Memory for narrative and expository text: independent
Pomerantz, E. M., Chaiken, S., & Tordesillas, R. S. (1995). Attitude strength and influences of semantic associations and text organization. Journal of Experi-
resistance processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 408e419. mental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31(2), 359e364.
Quintana, S. M., & Maxwell, S. E. (1999). Implications of recent development in Xiang, Z., Wöber, K., & Fesenmaier, D. R. (2008). Representation of the online
structural equation modeling for counseling psychology. The Counseling tourism domain in search engines. Journal of Travel Research, 47(2), 137e150.
Psychologist, 27, 485e527. Xiang, Z., & Gretzel, U. (2010). Role of social media in online travel information
Richardson, J. T. E. (1999). Imagery. Hove, UK: Psychology Press. search. Tourism Management, 31(2), 179e188.

You might also like