You are on page 1of 12

TECHNIQUES FOR ASSESSING STANDARDIZATION IN ARTIFACT

ASSEMBLAGES: CAN WE SCALE MATERIAL VARIABILITY?

Jelmer W. Eerkens and Robert L. Bettinger

The study of artifact standardization is an important fine of archaeological inquiry that continues to be plagued by the lack of
an independent scale that would indicate what a highly variable or highly standardized assemblage should look like. Related
to this problem is the absence of a robust statistical technique for comparing variation between different kinds ofassemblages.
This paper addresses these issues. The Weber fraction for fine-length estimation describes the minimum difference that humans
can perceive through unaided visual inspection. This value is used to derive a constant for the coefficient of variation (CV =
1.7 percent) that represents the highest degree of standardization attainable through manual human production of artifacts.
Random data are used to define a second constant for the coefficient of variation that represents variation expected when pro-
duction is random (CV = 57.7 percent). These two constants can be used to assess the degree of standardization in artifllct
assemblages regardless ofkind. Our analysisfurther demonstrates that CV is an excellent measure of standardization and pro-
vides a robust statistical technique for comparing standardization in samples of artifacts.

El estudio de estandarizacion y variacion ha sido una importante y valiosa linea de intenis en los amilisis arqueolr5gicos. Sin
embargo, min persisten dos problemas que son el enfoque de este estudio. En primer lugar; faltan medidas independientes para
evaluar problemas de estandarizacion y variacion. En otros terminos, no hay nada que indica como se debe hacer una muestra
arqueologica bien estandardizada 0 bien variable. En segundo lugar; no existe una tecnica estadistica segura para hacer com-
paraciones cuantitativas. El 'Weberfi"action,' utilizado para la estimacion de una linea amplia describe la diferencia minima que
seres humanos pueden percibir con solo una inspeccion ocular. Este valor es utilizado para derivar una constante (CV = 1.7 pe r-
cent) que representa la variach5n minima obtenida a traves de la produccion manual de artefactos por seres humanos. Datos
aleatorios son utilizados para determinar una segunda constante que representa la variac ion esperada bajo condiciones aleato-
rias (CV = 57.7. percent). De este modo, estas dos constantes pueden estar utifizadas para determinar el grado de estandarizaci6n
en las colecciones de artefactos. Tambier!, este estudio proporciona una tecnica estadistica segura para comparar la estandarizaci6n
en muestras de artefactos.

T
he study and interpretation of artifact varia- 1995; Crown 1999; Longacre 1999; Longacre et aI.
tion is essential for understanding and 1988; Rice 1991; Rottlander 1966), but Iithics (Bet-
explaining the archaeological record. The tinger and Eerkens 1997, 1999; Chase 1991; Eerkens
most visible contribution of this research is taxo- 1997, 1998; Hayden and Gargett 1988; Torrence
nomic, the creation of schemes that divide material 1986), bone and antler (Dobres 1995), and textiles
culture into meaningful functional, temporal, and (Rowe 1978) are also well represented.
geographical categories. In recent years, however, Variation is useful for understanding such a broad
inquiry has increasingly shifted from developing tax- range of phenomena because it reflects the degree
0nomies to interpreting the variation that makes them of tolerance for deviation from a standard size, shape,
work. The study of variation and standardization has form, or method of construction. Higher tolerance
become commonplace across a broad range of sub- increases variability, while lower tolerance decreases
ject matters relevant to anthropological theory and variability leading to standardization. Standardiza-
culture history (see Rice 1991 for a review). Ceram- tion, then, is a relative measure of the degree to which
ics feature prominently in these studies (e.g., Arnold artifacts are made to be the same. Standardization is
1991; Blackman et a!. 1993; Costin and Hagstrum in turn related to the life cycle of the artifact type or

Jelmer W. Eerkens Ii! Department of Anthropology, University of California, Santa Barbara, CA 93106
Robert L. Bettinger Ii! Department of Anthropology, Universi ty of California, Davis, CA 95616

American Antiquity, 66(3), 200 I, pp. 493 -504


200 I by the Society for American Archaeology
ll,v(;n"hl''U

493
494 AMERICAN ANTIQUITY [Vol. 66, No.3, 2001]

class In such as nnXlllC- per error in the model prc)je(:tile


tion costs, consumer preferences, replication and Multiple factors would contribute to this error (Rice
learning behaviors, number of producers, concern 1991 :273 lists several), but a key source is what can
with quality, producer skill, and access to resources. be termed scalar error, stemming from errors in esti-
Unfortunately, the statistics of variation have not mating object size and translating mental images
kept pace with this growing interest in variation. into properly scaled physical objects. This error is
Although many approaches have been used, none is neither random nor absolute. It is limited by human
universally applicable, and, when the analysis pro- visual perception and motor skill and increases lin-
ceeds to interpretation, the emphasis is always on early with the magnitude or size of the intended end
qualitative rather than quantitative characterizations. product (e.g., Coren et a1. 1994). This makes it pos-
Studies of variation have employed a sophisticated sible to define a quantitative boundary for the least
range of measures (e.g., standard deviation, coeffi- amount of variation that can be expected under the
cient of variation, skewness, etc.), but nothing in the most rigorous kind of production.
theoretical or experimental literature provides an When humans attempt to estimate the size or mag-
independent standard for interpreting these mea- nitude of an object visually, without reference to an
sures. Nor is it possible, given the present situation, independent scale (i.e., without a ruler), they make
to compare the amount of variation observed between mistakes that grow larger in absolute size as the size
two artifact classes, for example, between ground of object increases; the larger the object, the larger
stone and chipped stone artifacts. In sum, the anthro- the absolute error in estimated size (Coren et a1.
pological study of variation lacks a robust statistical 1994:39-43; Kerst and Howard 1978; Teghtsoonian
approach. 1971). Similarly, when people attempt to make an
This paper addresses these issues on two counts. object from a mental image or model, they make mis-
First, it seeks to place observed artifact variation takes that increase in absolute size as template size
within a universal context by exploring theoretically increases. If a person makes 10 objects independently
derived guidelines or baseline values that can assist from the same mental image, both the range and stan-
interpretation. The upper baseline (highest degree of dard deviation of size of those 10 finished objects will
standardization) describes the minimum amount of increase as the template size increases. In Sh01t, error
metrical variation humans can generate without such and size are correlated; people make larger absolute
external aids as rulers. The lower baseline describes errors when making larger objects. More importantly,
the amount of variation that will occur when there is the rate at which error and intended size are corre-
no attempt at standardization at all, i.e., when pro- lated is linear. Such scaling error is frequently dis-
duction is random relative to a standard. We borrow cussed in the psychophysics literature (e.g., Algom
from psychology and statistics to derive these bound- 1992; Coren et 'al. 1994; Gescheider 1997; Miller
aries. Second, we present a statistical method for 1956; Stevens 1975) and has also been observed in
comparing variation between assemblages that is archaeological materials (e.g., Bettinger and Eerkens
applicable to cases where assemblages differ with 1997; Eerkens 1998; Shott 1997) and replication
respect to artifact class or attribute size. We argue that experiments (Eerkins 2(00).
under most circumstances coefficient of variation This phenomenon is a product of how the human
(CV) is a stable and reliable measure of variation. brain interprets, measures, and compares visual and
other sensory information. In the mid-1800s E. H.
Human Error and Weber Fractions Weber observed that the ability of individualsto dis-
Humans commit all kinds of errors when hand-craft- criminate between objects of different weight
ing such objects as ceramic pots and stone projec- depended on the mean weight of the objects involved
tile points. The kind of error we are interested in here (Coren et a!. 1994:39--43; Weber 1834). In lifting
is that which would result were one to show a skilled experiments Weber discovered that to be perceived
stone knapper a model projectile point, request 10 as differing in weight, heavy objects had to differ by
identical copies (to the best of hislher ability), and a greater absolute amount than lighter objects. Weber
allow the knapper to discard any specimens slhe also determined, however, that the relative ditler-
might regard as deviant. Observed variation in shape ence needed to make such distinctions remained rel-
or size of the 10 points would then represent knap- atively constant. Specifically, he found that two
REPORTS 495

had to differ more than about d",,,I,,,,, artifact it implil~s


(1/50) for a difference in weight to be detected, mean- scalar error divided by size wi II be constant in sets
ing that two large objects had to differ more in of handmade artifacts that are manufactured with-
absolute size than two small objects. Thus, unlike out rulers. This is convenient because archaeologists
rneehanical scales that deterrnine weight within an frequently express artifact variation in precisely this
invariant unit of error (e.g., ± .1 g), human apprecia- manner using the Coefficient of Variation (eV),
tion of heaviness is scaled relative to object weight defined as the sample standard deviation divided by
(see Jones 1986; Ross 1981, 1995; Stevens 1979 for the sample mean, which is often multiplied by 100
more recent work with weight). This value (2 per- and expressed as a percentage. Thus, the Weber frac-
cent) has come to be calleel the Weber's fraction for tion and CV both express variation scaled to magni-
heaviness (see also Norwich 1987; Ross 1997; Ross tude. Further, it is easy to convert the Weber fraction
and Gregory 1964; Teghtsoonian 1971). into CV form by using the notion of a uniform dis-
Human perception of length and area are simi- tribution.
larly scaled. The Weber fraction for perception of the A uniform distribution defines a range within
length of a line is similar to that for heaviness, about which all values are equally frequent or probable.
3 percent (Teghtsoonian 1971). This number varies This might be the case if one were randomly picking
slightly from person to person, but does not vary sig- numbers between 0 and X out of a hat, each number
nificantly by gender, age, or within an individual being represented once and having the same chance
over the course of time (VerriIlo 1981, 1982, 1983) of being drawn. Such a population is uniformly dis-
although remembered length seems to vary more tributed between 0 and X, with a mean of X/2. TIle
with increasing time (Kerst and Howard 1978, 1981, width of the range, then, is twice, or 200 percent of,
1984) and context (Hotopf et al. 1983; Pagano and the mean, running from X/2 - X/2 (= 0) to X/2 + X/2
Donahue 1999). In this respect the Weber fraction (= X). Regardless of the size of X, all such distribu-
for length perception is surprisingly constant over an tions have a CV of 57.7 percent (= l/-Y3).1 In com-
extremely wide range of sizes (Coren et a1. 1994; parison, Weber error should generate distributions
Laming 1997; Poulton 1989; see also Ross 1997). that are uniform but much more narrowly limited
Recent work with other aspects of vision, such as around the mean. As we have seen, acting alone,
color and contrast recognition, stereopsis, blur dis- Weber error will cause humans to produce collections
crimination, and depth perception, show similar mag- ofobjects whose range in size is 6 percent of the mean,
nitude and error-scaling properties, though the Weber i.e., from 97 percent of the mean to to3 percent of
Fraction value and the structure of the relationship the mean. Such a relationship might be expected if
can change (Howard and Rogers 1995; Mather 1997; subjects were asked to draw a line equal in length to
Schwartz 1999; Smallman et a1. 1996). a reference line and they did so without any additional
Thus, the ability of humans to perceive a ditTer- error due to motor-skill inaccuracy. When the line is
ence in the size of two objects, or between a mental drawn within +3 percent or -3 percent of the refer-
image of an object and the object itself, is limited by ence, subjects perceive the two as equal and stop
our sensory system. This difference must be at least drawing, though in reality the lines would differ by
3 percent. This does not apply when a physical stan- some finite amount. Since human perception is unable
dard, such as a ruler, is used as the method of mea- to discern smaller differences, values will be uni-
surement. In that context, the ability of a subject to formly distributed within these extremes (i.e., all val-
measure size or length is independent of absolute ues are equally likely)? Since the CV for unifonn
object size, turning instead on subject ability to dif- distributions whose range is 200 percent of the mean
ferentiate between marks on the ruler. With a ruler, is 1/{3 (= 57.7 percent), it follows that the CV for the
the error in measuring 10-cm objects and 1000-cm nalTower uniform distribution defined by the Weber
objects is the same. fraction (range = 6 percent of the mean) will be (6
percent I 200 percent) x UF3 1.7 percent. This is
Scaling and Artifact Variation essentially identical to the values obtained in psy-
That scalar error and object size are linearly and pos- chological experiments where subject estimates of
itively correlated in human perception of weight, line-segment length display a CVof 1.6 percent (Ogle
length, and area has several implications for under- 1950:231). We can produce the same result ernpiri-
496 AMERICAN ANTIQUITY [Vol. 66, No.3, 2001]


10
/

r / /
8 /
/r •
+
/
+ +/ Brooch Best-Fit
Random Uniform Lin~;/
+/+
6 (+
I + Data Set
/ Microlith Best-Fit
/
I-f +
/1 + x Weber fraction
4
t
I.. t- Random-Uniform #$

); Mesolithic Microlith
2
o Chaco Canyon Manos

• Bronze Age Brooches


o 10 30 40

Mean
]<igure 1. Mean-standard deviation relationships for three archaeological data sets and relationship to random data and
Weber fraction. Solid lines represent best-fit regression lines through relevant data points. Dashed lines represent best-fit
regression lines through data generated by Weber fraction and random-uniform data.

cally by repeatedly drawing random numbers from pendent standard. Of course, small errors in motor
uniform distributions whose means are different but skills and memory will introduce additional vari-
whose ranges are always from 97 percent of the mean ability in the manual production of artifacts (AIgam
to 103 percent of the mean. The lower line in Figure 1992; Kerst and Howm-d 1984; Moyer et al. 1978).
I presents such a simulation. Each of the 20 cases Eerkens (2000) suggests that CVs in the range of
shown represents a sample of200 numbers randomly 2.5--4.5 percent are more typical of the minimum
drawn from a unifo1TI1 distribution, producing a cor- error attainable by individuals in manual production
responding mean, standard deviation, and Cv. As without usc of external mlers. Similarly, Longacre
shown, the expected CV for each case is 1.7 percent. (1999) rep011s CV values in the range of 2-5 percent
The simulation obeys this expectation with CVs for aperture, circumference, and height for "stan-
t~tlIing very near 1.7 percent for each of the 20 cases. dardized" handmade pots, constmcted without the
The CVof 1.7 percent derived for the Weber frac- use of a mler by highly skilled Philippine special-
tion should represent the minimum amount of vari- ists. Quite clearly, these artifacts are highly stan-
ability attainable by humans for length dardized, approaching the Weber fraction CV, and
measurements. Variation below this threshold is not arc probably close to the minimum CV attainable in
possible given the visual perception capabilities of manual production.
most humans. Sets of artifacts that display CVs less It follows from all of the above that variation in
than 1.7 percent imply automation or usc of an inde- artifacts produced manually without the use of an
REPORTS

inctepen iclellt ruler be scaled and lin~ Inllel)erld(~nt Standards and the
early to the mean. Attributes that fail to show such
scaling imply an alternative mode of production. The CVs derived from the Weber fraction and the
Variation in random distributions is also scaled to uniform-random distribution provide two baseline
the mean. As we have just shown. any uniform dis- measures against which variability in artifact assem-
tribution of positive numbers, with a lower limit of blages can be compared. The uniform-random CV
0, an upper limit of X, and a mean of XI2, will have value (57.7 percent) does not involve the kind of psy-
a CV of 57.7 percent. A set of artifact attributes dis- chological limitation that gives rise to the Weber
tributed in such a manner would imply that variation fraction CV 0.7 percent). Nevertheless, it provides
within 100 percent on either side of the mean was a useful measurement to examine variability
within production standards; this as opposed to 3 per- encountered in archaeological situations. Variation
cent on either side of the mean defined by the Weber below 1.7 percent suggests use of a scale or exter-
fraction. Production where anything from 0 percent nal template to measure and manufacture artifacts
of the mean to 200 percent of the mean is tolerated and should be typical of settings where items are
would, indeed, be extreme, and clearly, humans do mechanically produced (i.e., perhaps from a mold
not produce artifacts in uniformly distributed ways or by a machine). Variation above 57.7 percent sug-
(again, see note 2). However, even a normally dis- gests intentional inflation of variation and may indi-
tributed variable with a CV of 57.7 percent displays cate situations where individual manufacturers are
nearly as many variates that fall more than half of actively trying to differentiate their products from
the mean from the mean as a uniform distribution those of others, thereby increasing variation. An
(39 percent against 50 percent); and the normally dis- intentional increase is necessarily implied because
tributed variable displays more variates that fall fur- variation is greater than would occur when produc~
ther than the mean from the mean than the uniform tion is completely random. Alternatively, such cases
distribution (8 percent against 0 percent). In short, might also describe situations where an archaeolo~
whether populations are normally or uniformly dis- gist has unknowingly lumped two or more discrete
tributed, CVs greater than or equal to 57.7 percent classes of artifacts into a single category, thereby
are derived from extremely variable populations in artificially increasing variation.
which approximately 40-50 percent of the variates Between these extremes a wide range of possi-
fall more than half the mean from the mean. bilities exist. Figure ] also shows mean-standard
As just noted, artisans producing material goods deviation relationships for several archaeological
are unlikely to be working with an arbitrary size collections including microliths from Mesolithic
interval ranging from an unspecified value of X all sites in Northern England, manos or milling hand-
the way down to O. In the real world, therefore, stones from Chaco Canyon, New Mexico, and
unstandardized assemblages should display CVs less Bronze Age safety pin brooches from Switzerland.
than 57.7 percent. Observed minimum and maxi- As Figure 1 shows, archaeological data often
mum values can be used to obtain a more conserv- show linear correlation between mean and standard
ative, empirical standard for random production in deviation (see Bettinger and Eerkens 1997 for a sim~
specific cases = ((A-B)/(A+B» x .577, where A is the ilar discussion for Great Basin projectile points).
maximum observed value and B is the minimum Lines running through the data indicate best-fit
observed value. This avoids the implication that regressions. However, the nature of the regression,
objects of zero length or zero size arc acceptable as measured by the slope, varies by collection.
when production is random (which is obviously not Steeper slopes denote collections characterized by
so), but risks the possibility that the observed max- less-standardized attributes (i.e., standard deviation
imum and minimum values underestimate the rIlle increases relatively sharply relative to size). For
limits of production tolerance. Because of the latter, example, Chaco Canyon manos show the least vari~
we prefer to usc the theoretically derived value (CV ation with increasing mean (see Carneron ]997),
57.7 percent) as the baseline standard for randorIl though they show more variation than the Weber
production, noting that under the proper conditions fraction for length (indicated by a dashed line near
important insights may be gained through the use of the bottom of Figure I). This suggests that of the three
an empirically determined standard. collections, the Chaco Canyon rnanos are the most
498 AMERICAN ANTIQUITY [Vol. 66, No 3,

bel'w(~en rnean standard de'llation


Eerkens I 1998) show greater variation on ative or non-linear.
average than Chaco Canyon manos, but less than In sum, where the mean-standard deviation rela-
Bronze Age brooches (see Doran and Hodson 1975), tionship is linear and positive, especially when the
which equal the variation expected under random regression line passes near the origin, CV will be the
conditions. We are reluctant to characterize the pro- more reliable measure of variation because it scales
duction of brooches as random, since each was care- standard deviation to the mean. When these condi-
fully made in a certain way. However, the high CVs tions hold, CV facilitates comparison of variation
suggest manufacturers were relatively unconcerned across different-sized attributes (i.e., large vs. small),
with conformance to a specific size. In this respect, as well as across attributes measured by different
the brooches represent a very unstandardized set of scales (i.e., centimeters vs. grams). Most metric
artifacts-at least with respect to size (see also Tor- attributes typically measured in artifact assemblages
rence 1986: 158-159 for examples of highly variable (e.g., length, width, thickness, diameter, and weight)
lithic data sets where CVs exceed 57.7 percent). meet these conditions. Provided the range of values
Another way to think of this is in terms of the is not excessive (i.e., not greater than 180 degrees),
intensity of constraints or forces acting to reduce angular data should also meet these criteria. For these
variation within a data set (perhaps how intensely reasons, CV should be the standard statistic in stud-
the data set has been winnowed or selected). The ies of variation.
strength of the regression as measured, for exam-
ple, by r2 , describes the consistency in standard- Quantifying and Measuring Standardization
ization within the data set from sample to sample. The CV is commonly used in other natural sciences
Collections with high goodness-of-fit values sug- such as medicine, biology, and psychology. Although
gest that the intensity of selection is roughly equal some archaeological studies have made qualitative
on all samples, while lower values imply that some comparisons of CVs (e.g., Arnold 1991; Benco 1988'
attributes or samples are more standardized than Longacre et al. 1988; Torrence 1986), quantitativ~
others. analyses with this statistic are notably absent. It has
Importantly, the figure demonstrates that stan- even been argued that it is not possible to test the sta-
dard deviation is inappropriate as a statistic to com- tistical significance of CV (e.g., Arnold and Nieves
pare standardization between samples, because it 1992; Blackman et al. 1993). This is not so. Statis-
fails to scale variation properly. Samples with smaller tical research provides several techniques for creat-
means will have smaller standard deviations simply ing confidence intervals and testing equality of CV
because their means are small, hence will appear (Bennett 1976; Doornbos and Dijkstra 1983; Gupta
more standardized. For example, consider two sam- and Ma 1996; Vangel 1996), some of which are
ples of random numbers drawn from uniform distri- robust to departures from normality (Feltz and Miller
butions, the first with a mean of 10.03 and standard 1996).
deviation 5.47 (n = 50) and a second with mean .96 Many archaeological studies rely on the F-ratio
and standard deviation .56 (n =50). These two sam- test to compare variation. However, as Kvamme et
ples represent two distinct points in the left-hand line aI. (1996) have pointed out, this test requires nor-
in Figure 1. Since both samples contain completely mality in the underlying sample populations, an
random numbers, neither is more standardized than assumption that does not hold in many archaeolog-
the other. However, any statistical test used to com- ical situations. Instead, they recommend use of alter-
pare standar~l deviation between these two samples, native homogeneity of variance (HOV) tests, such
including the F- Test and Brown-Forsyth test (see as the Brown-Forsyth test (Brown and Forsythe
below), would find statistically significant differ- 1974), that are robust to departures from normality
ences between the two. Such a test would wrongly (see Conover et aI. 1983 for a comparison and dis-
conclude that the second sample is more standard- cussion of over 50 llOV tests). Unfortunately, use of
ized than the first. A test comparing CVs, on the other HOV tests, even those that are robust to non-nor-
hanel, would find no difference, which is the desired mality, are of little use in studying variation unless
result (see below). CV is an inappropriate compara- the analyst is celiain that the means of the samples
tive Ineasure, however, when the relationship being compared are approximately equal. This is
REPORTS 499

Machine-Produced Items .1 .1 -.2 Eerkens 2000


Weber Fraction 1.6 1.6 1.7 Ogle 1950
Pots by specialized potters 4 2-6 Longacre 1999
Cut-outs from mental image 5 2.5 8 Eerkens n.d.
DunaAre Kou 10 8- 11 White and Thomas 1972
Chaco Canyon Manos 17 8- 35 Cameron 1997
English Mesolithic Microliths 19 5 39 Eerkens 1997, 1998
Great Basin projectile points 22 6 - 55 Bettinger and Eerkens 1997
Owens Valley Handstones 22 10 -32 This article
Random Uniform Data 58 50 65 This article
Stylistic elements on SW pots 66 46 84 Kantner 1999
Safety pin brooch attributes 74 25 - 113 Doran and Hodson 1975:224

because, as shown above, variance is often scaled to cally describes how far sample CVs lie from the esti-
the mean. A standard deviation of five indicates mate of the overall population CV Unlike the Brown-
something quite different in a sample with a mean Forsyth statistic, D'AD is simple to determine and can
of 10 (CV =50 percent) than in a sample with a mean be computed from summary statistics only (mean,
of 100 (CV =5 percent). Tests for HOY are not sen- standard deviation, and number of samples).
sitive to this, and only compare absolute measures
of variance. Unless variation is scale-independent or Examples
sample means are approximately equal, HOY tests How do archaeological samples stack up against the
should not be used in studies of artifact variation. CV boundary values of 1.7 percent and 57.7 percent
Tests comparing CV, on the other hand, are sen- presented earlier? Table 1 lists the average and the
sitive to differences in magnitude or mean. Moreover, range of CV values for various attributes on mater-
CV is a reliable statistic even at small sample sizes ial altifacts from a variety of studies. This sample
(Simpson 1947; Simpson et al. 1960). For this rea- is nonrandom and obviously incomplete, but repre-
son, the CV is appropriate for archaeological stud- sents a range of artifact and attribute types typically
ies comparing sample variation. Unfortunately the encountered by archaeologists. Obviously, there is
techniques have not yet been incorporated into pop- much variation in CVs across these data sets. Items
ular statistical packages (Reh and Scheffler 1996). made by a few people, such as Philippine pots (Lon-
Presented below is the formula for one such test gacre 1999) and Duna Are Kou stone tools (White
developed by Feltz and Miller (1996) that is rea- and Thomas 1972), are much more standardized
sonably robust to departures from normality and than generalized assemblages of microliths from
allows simultaneous comparison of CYs from k sam- England (Eerkens 1997, 1998) and projectile points
ple populations with unequal sample sizes. This sta- from the Great Basin (Eerkens and Bettinger 1997),
tistic is recommended for use in standardization and which were likely made by hundreds if not thou-
variation studies. sands of different flintknappers. Similarly, artifacts
typically considered functional, such as projectile
points from the Great Basin and manos from Chaco
D' AD Canyon (Cameron 1997) have much lower CV val-
ues than attributes typically considered stylistie,
such as line elements painted on Southwestern pots
In D'AD, k is the number of samples, j is an index (Kantner 1999) or Swiss Bronze Age safety pin
referring to the sample number, n) is the sample size brooches (Doran and Hodson 1975). CV values on
of the jth population, In) " = (n,) - 1), s) is the standard the latter often exceed 57 .7 percent, suggesting indi-
x
deviation of thejth population, and j is the mean of
2
viduals were resisting conformity to a central or
thejth population. D'AD is distributed as a X ran- ideal template.
dom variable with k- I degrees offreedom, and basi- The amoLlnt of variation in most of these cases is
500 AMERICAN ANTIQUITY [Vol. 66, No.3, 2001]

to 1·I),n!,.,,1

J.7 percent) and produce (at 2--4 percent), often by ify, while others, such as flaked stone, are less pre-
a f~lctor of 10 or more. This may stem from several dictable and controllable, and can only be modified
factors. First, people may accept visually detectable through further reduction of the artifact. Media that
variation (more than 1.7 percent) because within are more difficult to control are likely to have inflated
some margm an mtifact may be close enough to the CV values. Of course, standardization and design
ideal shape that spending more time modifying it is tolerances are relative to different media, technolo-
not worth the extra effort (i.e., to possibly obtain a gies, and intended artifact functions (Aldenderfer
small increase in performance). In other words, 1990; Bleed 1997: 100). Thus, CVs that might be
beyond some point, imperfect artifacts may still be considered standardized within a flaked-stone tech-
good enough. This concept has been refen'ed to else- nology producing projectile points may not be in a
where as design constraint or design tolerance clay technology producing pots, clay being easier
(Aldenderfer 1990; Bleed 1986, 1997). Items needed to shape. The study of each technology will need to
for exact or specialized work are likely to have high empirically derive CV values that represent w hat is
design constraints (low tolerance for deviation from called "standardized."
the optimal shape), and should display lower CVs Our point here is that there is a limit to how stan-
than less-specialized tools. dardized things can get, based on the human ability
Second, as we have seen, the number of people to differentiate size. In the example above, people
responsible for a set of artifacts may be important. are likely to see that, in an absolute sense, there is
Different people are likely to have slightly different more variability among projectile points than pots.
ideas and definitions of what constitutes an "ideal" However, the effort that it would take to make the
shape for a particular item. As such, samples of arti- projectile points as standardized as the pots through
facts that archaeologists typically compare when additional careful flaking may not be worth what-
studying variation may differ simply as a result of ever benefits might accrue. In this sense, we can
the number of manufacturers contributing to sam- compare standardization and variation between dif-
ples. For example, Eerkens (1997, 1998) has com- ferent technologies. However, the results might tell
pared Later Mesolithic microliths from generalized us more about the inherent difficulties in controlling
site contexts, likely representing numerous individ- different media than whether one technology is more
uals, with those from specialized "hoard" or "group" standardized, and that people were more careful or
find-spots representing the work of a single individ- concerned about it, than another.
uaL Not surprisingly, CVs from the latter are much Table 2 uses seven samples drawn from Table 1
smaller than the former. Routinization is likely to to illustrate the superiority of the D 'AD test over the
playa role here as well. Large numbers of artifacts F-ratio test. The F-ratio test (see Runyon and Haber
made over a short amount of time with a similar and 1988:324), which is occasionally used in archaeo-
wel1-remembered mental image wil1 have lower CV logical studies (e.g.,ArnoldI991 ;Arnold and Nieves
values than those made one at a time over a longer 1992; Longacre et aL 1988), examines the ratio of
period of time. squared sample standard deviations (sample vari-
Third, archaeologists may unknowingly group ances) to test equality of variance. Unlike D 'AD,
artifacts that were considered distinct by their mak- then, F-ratio does not incorporate sample mean. The
ers, thereby artificial1y increasing CV values. In other samples compared in Table 2 include attributes that
words, elevated CVs may be a product of the etic cat- are typically considered "functional" (microlith
egories archaeologists define, as opposed to the emic length and thickness, projectile-point length); attrib-
categories and restricted CVs manufacturers were utes typically considered "stylistic" (Swiss safety-
originally working with. Longacre et at (1988) has pin bow width, painted line width on black-on-white
recognized this problem in an ethnoarchaeological ceramics), as well as two sets of random data drawn
study of Kalinga pots, where inadvertent lumping of from uniform distributions with rneans of 10 and I.
mUltiple size classes of pots by archaeologists led to The UAD tests clearly demonstrate distinct differ-
artificially inflated values of variation. ences between the "functional" and "stylistic" attrib-
Finally, different raw rnaterials, such as clay and utes. The CVs of all functional samples are
stone, exhibit different forming properties. Some, stati stically distinct from all stylistic samples (i .e., p
REPORTS 501

In
and D'AD (Using C.'V) in Lower Left.

F-Ratio-~ Microlith Microlith DSN SWpot Brooch Random Random

Prestatyn microlith length 180.0 1.862 43.78 19 128 741


(x = 20.32; s = 4.83; n = 16) P = .000 P = .08 p .000 p = .07 P .25 p .000
Prestatyn microlith thick. 1.24 96.94 4. II 95.52 230.87 2.43
(x = 1.97; s .36; n 25) p = .27 P = .000 p= .000 p .000 p = .000 p =.01
Owens Valley DSN length 3.03 0.4 23.52 1.0 2.39 39.82
(x = 22.05; s = 3.54; n = 28) p = .08 p = .52 P = .000 p .47 p = .004 P = .000
SW pots line width 6.96 15.21 19.28 22.99 56.15 1.69
(x 1.12; s = .73; n = 190) p= .01 p = .000 p .000 p .000 p = .000 p= .02
Brooch bow width 6.71 14.42 24.84 0.97 2.44 38.9
(x = 6.57; s = 3.5; n = 30 ) p = .01 p= .000 p= .000 p = .32 p = .003 p .000
Random Data I 6.71 17.11 23.14 1.24 .012 95.1
(x = lO.03; s 5.47; n = 50) p = .0] p .000 p = .000 p = .27 P .91 p = .000
8.62 18.4 24.5 .244 .327 .3
P = .003 P = .000 p = .000 p = .62 P = .57 P = .58
thickness, DSN desert side-notched projectile point.

< .05). This is not true with the F-ratio tests, which the Weber Fraction can help in recognizing different
in several instances failed to distinguish (p > .05) a modes of artifact production and degrees of stan-
functional attribute from a stylistic one (e.g., dardization. Weber fractions also have implications
microlith and projectile-point length are statistically in symbolic archaeology because humans are lim-
indistinguishable from both brooch bow width and ited in their ability to view, interpret, and discrimi-
random data). Moreover, variation in the two random nate artifacts in the same way they are limited in their
data sets is statistically equivalent by the D'AD test ability to produce them in standardized form. Thus,
but statistically different according to the F-ratio. two potters using color and size of painted design
The results demonstrate that the F-ratio test is inad- elements to differentiate their products must make
equate for the task of comparing variation and eval- them different enough that they exceed the just-
uating degree of standardization. noticeable-difference (derived from the Weber frac-
tion) for color contrast and size. Even if we, as
Discussion and Conclusions archaeologists, can discriminate finer differences
Most archaeological studies of technology recog- using Munsell color charts, rulers, calipers, or Scan-
nize only the role of the physical and social envi- ning Electron Microscopes (SEM), prehistoric peo-
ronment in shaping material culture (Bleed 1997:98) ple may not have been able to.
by focusing on how a raw material is modified using Finally, we feel the research is of relevance to
various tools, and how different social and physical studies of artifact change and the evolution of tech-
processes influence the final product (Schiffer and nologies through time. The study demonstrates that
Skibo 1997). As Bleed (1997 :98) has discussed, the people are unable to differentiate subtle differences
human body has seldom been seen as part of this in the size of objects beyond a certain point. In the
process. As we hope to have made clear, the human transmission of cultural information these limits are
body, with all of its attendant sensory systems and just as applicable, affecting how accurately people
limitations, is a medium through which technology can copy from and learn from others, and how pre-
operates. Our abilities to see, feel, and modify mate- cisely artifact traits will be transmitted between peo-
rial items are limited and alfected not only by cul- ple. Although beyond the scope of this paper, it
ture, but by the physics and psychophysics of the should be possible to use the Weber fraction to make
human body as welL some predictions about the degree of drift expected
Understanding these limitations can help archae- in artifact populations through time, if people are
ologists to ask new questions from the material attempting to faithfully copy traits and are randomly
record. As we have shown above, the psychophysi- making small errors due to the limits of visual per-
cal lirnitations of size discrimination quantified by ception. These predictions could be tested against the
502 AMERICAN ANTIQUITY [Vol. 66, No.3,

arc:haeolo~;ie:al record to m artifacts and Klarich


skills in translating the abstract.
follow those expected under drift. If variation is less
than this value, other variation-minimizing forces References Cited
may be at work. Alternatively, if variation exceeds Aldenderfer, M. A.
this level, various variation-inflating forces may be 1990 Defining Lithics-Using Craft Specialists in Lowland
responsible. Maya Society through Microwear Analysis: Conceptual
Problems and Issues. In The Interpretative Possibilities of
In the last analysis, size-con-elated en-or toler- MicrowearStudies, edited by B. Graslund et aI., pp. 53-70.
ance is probably telling us something important about Societas Archaeologica Upsaliensis, Uppsala, Sweden.
the evolutionary setting in which humans evolved, AIgom, D.
1992 Memory Psychophysics: An Examination of Its Per-
specifically about the penalties suffered in matching ceptual and Cognitive Prospects. In Psychophysical
tool size to intended task or duplicating tools made Approaches to Cognition, edited by D. Algom, pp. 44 1-512.
by others. The evidence would suggest that errors Elsevier, New York.
Arnold, D. E., and A. L. Nieves
became, or were perceived as becoming, more costly 1992 Factors Affecting Ceramic Standardization. In Ceramic
as tool size decreased. In such a context small tools Production and Distribution, edited by G . .I. Bey III and C.
are specialized tools by definition. Alternatively, the A. Pool, pp. 93-113. Westview, Boulder, Colorado.
Arnold, P..I.
Weber fraction for estimating size may have evolved 1991 Dimensional Standardization and Production Scale in
in an altogether different context, perhaps foraging Mesoamerican Ceramics. Latin American Antiquity
where, as prey size decreases, absolute en-or in esti- 2:363-370.
Benco, N.
mating prey size increases return-rate variability, 1988 Morphological Standardization: An Approach to the
hence risk of resource shortfall. If so, one would Study of Craft Specialization. In A Pot for All Reasons:
expect to find evidence of size-con-elated error tol- Ceramic Ecology Revisited, edited by C. Kolb and L. Lackey,
pp. 57-72. Temple University, Philadelphia.
erance in a wide range of species other than humans. Bennett, B. M.
We are unaware of any animal studies of this phe- 1976 On an Approximate Test for Homogeneity of Coeffi-
nomenon, though these would clearly be worth pur- cients ofVariation. Contributions to Applied Statistics, edited
by W. .I. Ziegler, pp. 169-171. Birhauser Verlag, Stuttgart,
suing as would studies comparing size-con-elated Germany.
en-or between different hominid forms. Bettinger, R. L., and.l. W. Eerkens
In sum, we have presented evidence showing that 1997 Evolutionary Implications of Metrical Variation in Great
Basin Projectile Points. In Rediscovering Darwin: Evolu-
CV should be the standard statistic in studies of vari- tionary Theory and Archaeological Explanation, edited by
ation and have offered two baseline measures for C. M Barton and G. A. Clark, pp. 177·-191. Archaeological
placing observed CVs of length measurements along Papers of the American Anthropological Association, Arling-
ton, Virginia.
a continuum of variation from 1.7 percent, the limit 1999 Point Typologies, Social Transmission and the Intro-
of human ability to perceive a difference in size, to duction of Bow and Arrow Technology in the Great Basin.
57.7 percent, the variability expected when produc- American Antiquity 64:231-242.
Blackman,.l. M., P. B. Vandiver, and G. .1. Stein
tion is random or near-random and uniform (i.e., 1993 Standardization Hypothesis and Ceramic Mass Pro-
completely unstandardized). Using the CV, future duction: Technological, Compositional, and Metric Indexes
studies can use these baseline values to evaluate the of Craft Specialization at Tell Leilan, Syria. American Antiq-
uity 58:60-80.
degree of variation or standardization in independent Bleed, P.
sets of artifacts. The D 'AD test facilitates statistical 1986 The Optimal Design of Hunting Weapons: Maintain-
comparison of CVs from samples of artifacts of dif- ability or Reliability. American Antiquity 5 I :737-847.
1997 Content as Variability, Result as Selection: Toward a
fering size or magnitude to help evaluate degree of Behavioral Definition ofTechnology. In Rediscovering Dar-
standardization. We hope that further exploration of win: Evolutionary Theory and Archaeological txplanation,
the psychophysical literature will lead to a deeper edited by C. M. Barton and G. A. Clark, pp. 95-103. Archae-
ological Papers of the American Anthropological Associa-
understanding of how technology and the human tion, Arlington, Virginia.
body interact to create the material record, and vari- Brown, M. B., and A. B. Forsythe
ation therein, that we study. 1974 Robust Tests for the Equality of Variance. Journal of
the American Statistical Association 69:364-367.
Cameron, C. M.
Ao'(/u:'wl,?dgments:. Thanks to Mark Aldenderfer, Peter Bleed,
1997 An Analysis of Manos from Chaco Canyon, New Mex-
Mike Jochim, John Kantner, Dwight Read, Kevin Vaughn, and
ico. In Ceramics, Lithics, and Ornaments olChaco Canyon,
an anonymous revicwer for reading earlier drafts of this paper. Volume IlI, edited by .I. Mathien, pp. 997-1012. National
Their comments greatly helped to improve the final product. Park Service, Santa Fe, New Mexico.
We also kindly thank Tim Kohler for critique, suggestions, and Chase, P. G.
REPORTS 503

Symbols and PaleolIthIe /\rnraers: Santa


lion, and the Imposition of Arbitrary Form. Journal of Kerst, S. M., and 1. H. Howard, Jr.
An.thropological 10: 193-214. 1978 Memory Psychophysics for Visual Area and Length.
Conover, W. J., M. E. Johnson, and M. M. Johnson Memory & Cognition 6:327- 335.
1981 A Comparative Study ofTests for Homogeneity ofVari- 1981 Memory and Perception of Cartographic Information
al1Ces, with Applications to the Outer Continental Shelf Bid- for Familiar and Unfamiliar Environments. Human Factors
ding Data. Technonzetrics: 23, No.4, 351-361. 23:495-503.
Coren, S., L M. Ward, and J. T. Enns 1984 Magnitude Estimates of Perceived and Remembered
1994 Sensation and Perception. 4th ed. Harcourt Brace, Fort Length and Area. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society
Worth, Texas. 22:517-520.
Costin, C. L., and M. B. Hagstrum Kvamme, K. L, M. T. Stark, and W. A. Longacre
1995 Standardization, Labor Investment, Skill, and the Orga- 1996 Alternative Procedures for Assessing Standardization in
nization of Ceramic Production in Late Prehispanic High- Ceramic Assemblages. American Antiquity 61 : 116-126.
land Peru. American Antiquity 60:6 19·-639. Laming, D. R. .1.
Crown, P. L 1997 The Measurement of Sensation. Oxford University
1999 Socialization in American Southwest Pottery Decora- Press, Oxford.
tion. In Pottery and People, edited by J. M. Skibo and G. M. Longacre, W. A.
Feinman, pp. 25-43. University of Utah Press, Salt Lake City. 1999 Standardization and Specialization: What's the Link?
Dobres, M. 1n Pottery and People, edited by J. M. Skibo and G. M. Fein-
1995 Gender and Prehistoric Technology: On the Social man, pp. 44-58. University of Utah Press, Salt Lake City.
Agency of Technical Strategies. World Archaeology Longacre, W. A., K. L. Kvamme, and M. Kobayashi
27:25-49. 1988 Southwestern Pottery Standardization: An Ethnoar-
Doornbos, R., and J. B. Dijkstra chaeological View from the Philippines. Kiva 53: lO 1-112.
1983 A Multi Sample Test for the Equality of Coefficients of Mather, G.
Variation in Normal Populations. Communications In 1997 The Use of Image Blur as a Depth Cue. Perception
Statistics: Simulation and Computation 12: 147-158. 26: 1147-1158.
Doran, J. E., and F. R. Hodson Miller, G.
1975 Mathematics and Computers in Archaeology. Harvard 1996 The Magical Number Seven, Plus or Minus Two: Some
University, Cambridge, Massachusetts. Limits on Our Capacity for Processing Information. The
Eerkens, J.W. Psychological Review 63:81-97.
1997 Variability in Later Mesolithic Microliths of Northern Moyer, R. S., D. R. Bradley, M. H. Sorenson, J. C. Whiting, and
England. Lithic, 17/I8:51- 65. D. P. Mansfield
1998 Reliable and Maintainable Technologies: Artifact Stan- 1978 Psychophysical Functions for Perceived and Remem-
dardization and the Early to Later Mesolithic Transition in bered Size. Science 200:330-332.
Northern England. Lithic Technology 23:42-53. Norwich, K. H.
2000 Practice Makes Within 5% ofPeliect: The Role ofVisual 1983 On the Theory of Weber Fractions. Perception and Psy-
Perception, Motor Skills, and Human Memory in Artifact cllOphysics 42:286-298.
Variation and Standardization. Current Anthropology Ogle, K. N.
41:663-668. 1950 Researches in Binocular Vision. Saunders, Philadelphia.
Feltz, C. J., and G. E. Miller Pagano, C. c., and K. G. Donahue
1996 An Asymptotic Test for the Equality of Coefficients of 1999 Perceiving the Lengths of Rods Wielded in Different
Variation from K Populations. Statistics in Medicine Media. Perception and Psychophysics 61: 1336-1344.
15:647-658. Poulton, E. C.
Gescheider, G. A. 1989 Bias in Quantifying Judgments. Lawrence Erlbaum,
1997 Psychophysics: The Fundamentals. L. Erlbaum, Hills- Hove, England.
dale, New Jersey. Reh, W, and B. ScheWer
Gupta, R. c., and S. Ma 1996 Significance Tests and Confidence Intervals for Coef-
1996 Testing the Equality of Coefficients of Variation in K ficients of Variation. Computational Statistics and Data
Normal Populations. Communications in Statistics: Theory Analysis 22:449-452.
and Method 25: 115-132. Rice, P. M.
Hayden, B., and R. Gargett 1991 Specialization, Standardization, and Diversity: A Ret-
1988 Specialization in the Paleolithic. Lithic Technology rospective. In The Ceramic Legacy of Anna O. Shepard,
17:12-18. edited by R. L. Bishop and F. W Lange, pp. 257- 279. Uni-
lIotopf, W H. N., M. C. Hibberd, and S. A. Brown versity of Colorado, Boulder.
1983 Position in the Visual Field and Spatial Expansion. Per- Ross, H. E.
ception 12:469-476. 1981 How Important Are Changes in Body Weight for Mass
Howard,!. P, and B. J. Rogers Perception? Acta Astronautica 8: 1051-1058.
1995 Binocular Vision and Stereopsis. Oxford University 1995 Weber on Temperature and Weight Perception. In Fi'ch-
Press, Oxford. ner Day 95, editcd by C. A. Possamai, pp. 29-34. [nterna-
Jones, L A. tional Society for Psychophysics, Cassis, France.
1986 Perception of Force and Weight: Theory and Research. 1997 On the Possible Relations between Discriminability and
Psychological Bulletin 100:2942 Apparent Magnitude. British Journal oj'Mathematical and
Kantner, J. Statistical Psychology 50: 187-203.
1999 The Influence of Self-Interested Behavior on Sociopo- Ross, 11. E., and R. L. Gregory
litical Change: The Evolution of the Chaco Anasazi in the 1964 [s the Weber Fraction a Function or Physical or Per·
Prehistorie Ameriean Southwest. Unpublished Ph.D. dis- ceived Input? Quarterly Journal oj' Experimental Psychol·
sertation. Department of Anthropology, University of Cali- 16:116-122.
504 AMERICAN ANTIQUITY [Vol. 66, No.3, 20011

1986 Prodw.tion and Exchange of Stone nJOls. launbn!dt':e


etry 9:76--91. University Press, Cambridge.
Rowe,J. H. Vangel, M. G.
1978 Standardization in Inca Tapestry Tunics. In Junius B. 1996 Conildence Intervals for a Normal Coefilcient of Van-
Bird Pre-Columbian Textile Conference, edited by A. P. ation. American Statistician 15:21-26.
Rowe, E. P Benson. and A. Schaffer. pp 239-264. Dum- Verrillo, R. T.
barton Oaks, Washington DC 1981 Absolute Estimation of Line Length in Three Age
Runyon, R. P, and A. Haber Groups. Journal of Gerontology 36:625-627.
1988 Fundamentals ofBehavioral Statistics, 6th eel. Random 1982 Absolute Estimation of Line Length as a Function of
House, New York. Sex. Bulletin ofthe Psyehonomic Society 19:334-335.
SchwilliZ, S. H. 1983 Stability of Line-Length Estimates Using the Method
1999 Visual Perception. 2nd cd. Appleton and Lange, Stam- of Absolute Magnitude Estimation. Perception & Psy-
ford, Connecticut. chophysics 33:261-·265.
Schiffer, M. B., and J. M. Skibo Weber, E. H.
1997 Explanation of Artifact Variability. American Antiquity 1834 De Pulen, Resorptione, Auditu et Tactu: Annotationes
62:27-50. Anatomicae et Physiologicae. Kohler, Leipzig, Germany.
Shott, M. J. White, J. P., and D. H. Thomas
1997 Transmission Theory in the Study of Stone Tools: A 1972 What Mean These Stones'? Ethno-Taxonomic Models
Midwestern North American Example. In Rediscovering and Archaeological Interpretations in the New Guinea high-
Darwin: Evolutionary Theory and Archaeological Expla- lands. In Models in Archaeology, edited by D. L. Clarke, pp.
nation, edited by C M. Barton and G. A. Clark, pp. 193-204. 275-308. Methuen, London.
Archaeological Papers of the American Anthropological
Association, Arlington, Virginia. Notes
Simpson, G C.
1947 Note on the Measurement of Variability and on Rela- I. This ensues becuase the mean of a uniform distribution
tive Variability of Teeth of Fossil Mammals. American Jour- on [O,X] is X/2 and the standard deviation is X/~ 12. Thus, the
nal of Science 245:522-525. CV is 2/~ 12, or 1/~3, or 57 percent. Note that this value only
Simpson, G. C, A. Roe, and R. C Lewontin applies when the lower limit of the interval is set to O. When
1960 Quantitative Zoology. Harcourt, Brace, New York. the interval is more narrow, starting at a value greater than 0,
Smallman, H. S., D. 1. A. MacLeod, S. He, and R. W. Kentridge a smaller CV ensues.
1996 Fine Grain of the Neural Representation of Human
2. Normal distribution in human perception and manu-
Vision. Journal of Neuroscience 16: 1852-1859.
facture are more likely. However, modeling the Weber frac-
Stevens, J. C.
1979 Thermal Intensification of Touch Sensation: Further tion as a normal variable requires a priori definition of a
Extensions of the Weber Phenomenon. Sensory Processes standard deviation or error value, which is what we are try-
3:240-248. ing to model in the ilrst place. As such, a uniform distribution
Stevens, S. S. is used.
1975 Psychophysics: Introduction to Its Perceptual, Neural,
and Social Prospects. Wiley, New York.
Teghtsoonian, R.
1971 On the Exponents in Stevens' Law and the Constant in Received April 22, 1999; Revised March! 3, 2000; Accepted
Ekman's law. Psychological Review 78:71-80. July] 8, 2000.

You might also like