You are on page 1of 18

Journal of Occupational Health Psychology © 2014 American Psychological Association

2015, Vol. 20, No. 2, 172–189 1076-8998/15/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0038278

Please Respond ASAP: Workplace Telepressure and Employee Recovery


Larissa K. Barber and Alecia M. Santuzzi
Northern Illinois University

Organizations rely heavily on asynchronous message-based technologies (e.g., e-mail) for the purposes
of work-related communications. These technologies are primary means of knowledge transfer and
building social networks. As a by-product, workers might feel varying levels of preoccupations with and
urges for responding quickly to messages from clients, coworkers, or supervisors—an experience we
label as workplace telepressure. This experience can lead to fast response times and thus faster decisions
and other outcomes initially. However, research from the stress and recovery literature suggests that the
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

defining features of workplace telepressure interfere with needed work recovery time and stress-related
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

outcomes. The present set of studies defined and validated a new scale to measure telepressure. Study 1
tested an initial pool of items and found some support for a single-factor structure after problematic items
were removed. As expected, public self-consciousness, techno-overload, and response expectations were
moderately associated with telepressure in Study 1. Study 2 demonstrated that workplace telepressure
was distinct from other personal (job involvement, affective commitment) and work environment
(general and ICT work demands) factors and also predicted burnout (physical and cognitive), absentee-
ism, sleep quality, and e-mail responding beyond those factors. Implications for future research and
workplace practices are discussed.

Keywords: workplace telepressure, work recovery, information and communication technology, individ-
ual differences, social norms

Advances in technology have changed how we transfer infor- construct of workplace telepressure to conceptually represent the
mation and maintain relationships with others in our work envi- combination of preoccupation and urge to immediately respond to
ronment. Technology-mediated workplace interactions have sub- work-related ICT messages.
stantial benefits to productivity, including increasing perceptions Employees often feel the need to be continuously connected to
of more control over the timing and location of work and reducing the workplace through ICTs to meet the needs of supervisors,
perceived conflict between the work and home domains (Kossek, colleagues, and clients—a phenomenon called the autonomy par-
Lautsch, & Eaton, 2006; Mazmanian, Orlikowski, & Yates, 2013). adox (Mazmanian et al., 2013). The continuous connection to
Organizations are adopting more technology-based work arrange- one’s work is increasing employee stress by not allowing employ-
ments such as telecommuting and virtual teams (Cascio, 2000; ees to take a substantial break from work (Day, Scott, & Kelloway,
Townsend, DeMarie, & Hendrickson, 1998), resulting in an in- 2010; Olson-Buchanan & Boswell, 2006). For example, 44% of
creasing number of employees completing some or all of their Americans report checking their e-mail daily during vacation, and
work using information and communications technologies (ICTs). 54% do so even when home sick (American Psychological Asso-
Even onsite employees are relying on ICTs to facilitate interac- ciation, 2013). Thus, there appears to be a strong need to feel
tions with coworkers through e-mail communications rather than connected among employees who rely on ICT communications for
real-time, face-to-face interactions (Finn, 2006; Markus, 1994). work purposes. As noted above, we define workplace telepressure
However, the anticipated benefits to productivity from using as the combination of a strong urge to be responsive to people at
ICTs might be attenuated by mental and physical health costs to work through message-based ICTs with a preoccupation with
workers over the long term. The flexibility of ICT-based commu- quick response times. We believe the experience of telepressure
nications might blur the boundaries between work and recovery represents the psychological state that encourages continued con-
time. Even when not “working,” individuals might feel the need to nection to work activities.
respond to work-related messages. In this paper we introduce the The purpose of this research is to distinguish workplace tele-
pressure from other “connection-related” reactions to job demands,
explore potential personal and work environment predictors of
telepressure, and examine this construct’s relationships with stress,
This article was published Online First November 3, 2014. recovery, and e-mail responding outcomes. Workplace telepres-
Larissa K. Barber and Alecia M. Santuzzi, Department of Psychology, sure is a unique construct that can add to our understanding of why
Northern Illinois University.
employees might prioritize ICT communications during work time
We thank Dr. Nicholas Bowman and Dr. Sabine Sonnentag for their
assistance and their feedback on the initial workplace telepressure concep-
and tend to stay connected with coworkers via ICTs even during
tual definition and items. their nonwork time (e.g., Olson-Buchanan & Boswell, 2006; Park
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Larissa & Jex, 2011). In this article, we highlight the importance of
K. Barber, Department of Psychology, Northern Illinois University, studying workplace telepressure in organizations from an occupa-
DeKalb, IL, 60115. E-mail: lbarber@niu.edu tional health perspective. In Study 1, we present an initial valida-

172
WORKPLACE TELEPRESSURE 173

tion of a workplace telepressure measure and examine construct the motivational component of workaholism involves a combina-
validity with respect to other variables associated with general and tion of preoccupation and urge to stay connected to the work
ICT-specific work connection (workaholism, engagement, bound- environment. Different from workaholism, workplace telepressure
ary, and crossing), personality factors (extraversion, self- is broadly focused on maintaining social relationships and impres-
consciousness, self-monitoring), and work environment factors sions at work through computer-mediated communication. As
(technological work demands and norms). In Study 2, we link such, the focus is on responding to work-related messages rather
workplace telepressure to stress, recovery, and e-mail responding than actually performing work tasks.
outcomes beyond personal and work environment factors. Workplace telepressure is also separate from work engagement
(e.g., Schaufeli, Bakker, & Salanova, 2006), which is conceptual-
ized as high vigor (high energy while working), dedication (en-
Workplace Telepressure
thusiasm and perceived significance of one’s work), and absorp-
We conceptualize workplace telepressure as a single construct tion (full engrossment in work tasks) in one’s work. Employee
defined by thinking about ICT messages accompanied and an engagement is also positively associated with job demands, includ-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

overwhelming urge to respond. Asynchronous communication, ing sometimes working long hours (Schaufeli et al., 2009). Unlike
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

such as e-mail, is intended to add to response flexibility and workaholics, the increased psychological connection to one’s work
convenience by allowing workers long spans of uninterrupted time tends to be positive because engaged workers have more energy
on task to accomplish work goals or uninterrupted personal time and enthusiasm for their work in the form of high vigor. Like
for recovery. The core problem with workplace telepressure is that workaholism, work engagement has more to do with one’s actual
it has the potential to negate the intended advantage of asynchro- interaction with work tasks (and in this case, enjoying work) rather
nous communication technologies (e.g., e-mail) that provide work- than technologically facilitated social interactions in the work-
ers with flexibility and control over their response times. Work- place. Thus, telepressured employees, if they give in to their
place telepressure arises when employees start to view this mode responsive urges, might have the appearance of engagement or
of ICT use in the workplace as similar to synchronous forms, workaholism, despite not necessarily completing actual work tasks
which typically require an immediate response. For example, if an (i.e., they look busy with work-related tasks). Additionally, tele-
employee is asked in a synchronous, face-to-face conversation to pressured employees might overlap more with the work engage-
serve on a project team, the employee is expected to give some ment components of dedication and absorption in relation to work
response immediately. However, if the employee received an asyn- (staying connected and involved), rather than vigor (enthusiasm
chronous e-mail or voicemail with the request, the employee has for work), given that the latter entails intrinsic enjoyment as
some time to think about a response before actually replying to the opposed to just responsiveness to work demands.
message. If experiencing high levels of telepressure, the employee
will feel pressure to respond immediately to an asynchronous
ICT Connectivity
communication, similar to what is expected in a synchronous,
face-to-face request. This can lead employees to prioritize ICT Workplace telepressure is also different from connectivity be-
communications throughout the work day and fail to take neces- haviors specific to ICT use, such as ICT boundary creation and
sary recovery periods between tasks. Beyond designated work boundary crossing (Park & Jex, 2011). Boundary creation refers to
time, this shift in focus can transform asynchronous ICT use away self-imposed restrictions around ICT use for work purposes,
from a perception of “flexible work access” to “inescapable work.” whereas boundary crossing refers to using ICTs at home to per-
In the following paragraphs, we outline its conceptual distinction form work-related tasks (rather than the urge or preoccupation
from generalized work connectivity constructs (workaholism and with responding). Although workplace telepressure should be as-
engagement) and ICT-specific connectivity behaviors (work– sociated with these types of work– home boundary-blurring behav-
home ICT boundary creation and work– home ICT boundary iors (i.e., less boundary creation and more boundary crossing), it is
crossing). a psychological state rather than an ICT behavior. Given that it is
also specific to message-based communication, it should be more
closely associated with behaviors such as e-mail responding fre-
General Work Connectivity
quency rather than using ICTs to conduct work at home more
The relevance and value of the workplace telepressure construct generally. Employees experiencing higher levels of workplace
is contingent on it being distinct from other responses to work telepressure might be more likely to give in to their urges and
demands that result in greater connection to the work environment, engage in frequent e-mail responding behaviors.
both within and beyond the workplace. These responses are often
studied in terms of workaholism or work engagement— both of Personality/Trait Contributions to
which entail heightened connection to one’s work but differ on
Workplace Telepressure
motivational orientation (Van den Broeck et al., 2011). Workaho-
lism is often conceptualized as consisting of two components: the Workplace telepressure might be related to a number of personal
motivational component, representing a compulsion to think about factors of the employee, especially personality traits—psycholog-
and perform work tasks, and the behavioral component of working ical characteristics of the individual that are stable over time and
excessively (Schaufeli et al., 2009). Job demands are positively explain individuals’ behavior (i.e., Mount Barrick, Scullen, &
associated workaholism (Kanai & Wakabayashi, 2001), especially Rounds, 2005). Personality traits that might be positively associ-
with the behavioral component of working excessively (Schaufeli, ated with sensitivity to workplace telepressure include conscien-
Taris, & Van Rhenen, 2008). Similar to workplace telepressure, tiousness and extraversion. Conscientious workers tend to be more
174 BARBER AND SANTUZZI

responsive, reliable, and achievement-oriented. Extraverted work- (availability, response expectations, miscommunication over
ers tend to be more sociable and have a higher tendency to seek e-mail due to lack of face-to-face cues) and hassles related to
stimulation from others (see McCrae & John, 1992). The descrip- technological malfunctions (Day, Paquet, Scott, & Hambley,
tions of both personality traits suggest that behavior is responsive 2012). Workplace telepressure is a response to a specific type of
to external cues. For example, a highly conscientious person at work demand, namely message-based ICT demands. As such,
work is likely to respond quickly to an e-mail from a coworker due workplace telepressure should be more associated with perceptions
to a tendency to be responsive and diligent in pursuit of the of technological demands, such as technological overload, com-
achievement of work goals. A highly extraverted worker will feel pared with work overload more generally. Techno-overload refers
the need to respond quickly because they seek the social connec- to employee perceptions that the technology they use in their
tion. Note that neither of these personality traits are work specific; environment requires them to work faster and create more work
thus, individuals who are high in conscientiousness or extraversion than they can handle (Ragu-Nathan, Tarafdar, & Ragu-Nathan,
might also be more likely to respond quickly to messages from 2008).
others in nonwork situations as well (i.e., responsiveness to family
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

and friends).
Social Norms
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Workplace telepressure can also be conceived as a specific type


of impression management in technology-mediated interactions Similar to the power of social norms in other contexts, work-
and thus should be associated with general tendencies to monitor place telepressure might result from an effort to “fit in” with others
impressions around others, such as those driven by public self- at work by conforming to workgroup norms. Social influence
consciousness and self-monitoring (Scheier & Carver, 1985; Sny- theories suggest that our social environment motivates us to
der, 1974). The general concern about one’s standing in the work- change our behavior to match the behavior of others (e.g., Cialdini
place should lead workers to actively monitor the impressions that & Goldstein, 2004), including attempts to act according to percep-
coworkers have of them and use self-presentation strategies to tions of what is seen as the “correct behavior” in a given situation.
encourage those impressions to be positive. Thus, highly self- The perceived expectation to respond quickly can increase the urge
conscious workers should feel more compelled to respond to to respond quickly to asynchronous work communications whether
messages from coworkers to prevent negative impressions. perceived norms are descriptive (most people seem to respond
quickly) or prescriptive (the job explicitly requires that they re-
spond quickly; Forsyth, 2009). In other words, workplace telepres-
Work Environment Contributions to
sure should be high when the perceived norm is to respond
Workplace Telepressure quickly, regardless of whether such norms are explicit in organi-
Although we expect particular personality variables to make zational policies.
some workers slightly more predisposed to workplace telepressure Despite recent research focusing on telecommunications and
than others, workplace telepressure should have stronger relation- work–life boundary management, little is known about social
ships with job demands— especially related to ICT demands—and norms around technology use that might contribute to psycholog-
social norms around response behavior rather than individual ical states like workplace telepressure. One exception is a study
difference factors. that merely adapted a measure of personal preferences for work–
home segmentation (i.e., keeping work matters out of personal life)
into perceptions about work colleagues’ segmentation preference
Job and ICT Demands
(Park, Fritz, & Jex, 2011). Their results showed that the perceived
According to the job demands-resources model (JD-R) of stress (descriptive) segmentation norms predicted psychological detach-
(Demerouti, Bakker, Nachreiner, & Schaufeli, 2001; Bakker & ment from work, and the relationship was partially explained by
Demerouti, 2007), job demands are externally driven aspects of the the use of technology tools outside of work. More recent research
work environment that require sustained mental, physical, or emo- has also incorporated prescriptive norms into measures of ICT
tional exertion. Traditionally, job demands are conceptualized as demands (Day et al., 2012). Specifically, expectations regarding
amount of work to be done (i.e., workload; Karasek, 1979), as this response times, availability outside of work, and learning new
is the most common cost to psychological and physical resources technology are all forms of prescriptive norms around ICT use that
on the job. However, the JD-R model expands the conceptualiza- can arise in the workplace. Thus, workplace telepressure should be
tion of job demands to also include social demands that can lead to associated with perceived workplace norms around response times
employee stress. This includes factors such as “emotionally de- derived from descriptive (what most people do) and prescriptive
manding interactions with clients” or other social relationships (what is expected) information in the workplace environment.
(e.g., coworkers, supervisors; Bakker & Demerouti, 2007; p. 312),
which are often facilitated by ICT use at work. Study 1: Initial Scale Development and
Similar to the appraisal of an environmental stressor (Lazarus &
Construct Validity
Folkman, 1984), individuals might vary in their psychological
responses to a common demand or even the perception of the Workplace telepressure was defined as a combination of preoc-
demand itself. As such, workplace telepressure represents a poten- cupation with fast response times and the strong urge to respond to
tially maladaptive response to a particular type of work demand— asynchronous work-related messages. On the basis of this con-
messages via ICTs. ICTs pose a unique set of demands in the struct definition, the authors generated a pool of eight self-report
workplace that go beyond general work overload due to techno- items that covered both preoccupation and urge aspects of work-
logical demands. These can take the form of social demands place telepressure (see Appendix). We conceptualize telepressure
WORKPLACE TELEPRESSURE 175

as a narrow, cognitively oriented construct and thus avoided word- Method


ing that contained affective (anxious, worried, etc.) or behavioral
(i.e., responding to messages) information to ensure that items Participants and Procedure
referred only to thinking about and urges to respond to messages.
Participants were recruited through Amazon’s Mechanical Turk
The specific focus of the construct definition allowed us to create (MTurk). Survey data from MTurk users tends to be of comparable
items that were high in face validity and accounted for all aspects quality as other data sources and is more representative of U.S.
of the construct definition. We aimed to further reduce the pool of population characteristics than are undergraduate samples (Beh-
eight items to those that were useful and necessary to create a brief rend, Sharek, Meade, & Wiebe, 2011; Buhrmester, Kwang, &
measure that might be useful for both academic research and as a Gosling, 2011). A total of 404 individuals agreed to continue with
practical assessment tool in organizations. The measure instruc- the study after reading the recruitment statement approved by the
tions were written to be specific to work-related ICT messages but Institutional Review Board at the authors’ university. Access to the
broad in terms of who sent the communication (see Appendix for survey was limited to respondents in the United States, and par-
all items and response scale). The item development followed the ticipants received $0.50 for completing the survey. Only 380 met
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

assumptions for Likert scaling such that multiple items were the eligibility requirements to continue with the survey (measured
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

created to represent moderate statements that were accompanied through automatic disqualification items), including working
by a bipolar response scale (strongly disagree to strongly agree), part-time or full-time (15 hours or more per week) and comfort
and the average of the item scores should scale individuals on the with reading and understanding English. An additional 15 respon-
construct (DeVellis, 2012; Hinkin, 1998). dents were dropped for missing more than one of the three quality
Following standard procedures for self-report scale item devel- control check items, indicating lack of attention to the survey.
opment (DeVellis, 2012), items were reviewed by subject matter Thus, 354 final cases were used for data analysis in this study.
experts and then evaluated by a group of raters. After items were Respondents were primarily full-time workers (69.8%), men
developed by the authors, an expert in work recovery research and (60.4%), Caucasian (74.6%), and married (56.2%). Also, 28.4% of
the participants were telecommuters. The average tenure in the
an expert in communication research (both also had expertise and
organization was 6.5 years, and the average hours worked per
experience with test development) independently reviewed the
week was 37 (with an average of 5 hours telecommuting). A wide
item content and comprehension and provided suggestions for
variety of occupations were reported (e.g., individuals in educa-
revisions. A group of 17 undergraduate students who worked at
tion, food service, health care, and information technology posi-
least part-time and used ICTs for work purposes also reviewed the tions), with the most frequent occupations being engineers and
items for wording clarity and content relevance in relation to the employees in sales.
construct definition. The items were then distributed as part of a
larger Web-based survey that included measures of expected pre- Measures
dictors (personality and norms) and constructs with similar defi-
Conscientiousness and extraversion. Extraversion (␣ ⫽ .90)
nitions. The goals of this study were to test the items from the new
and conscientiousness (␣ ⫽ .86) were assessed with 10 items each
workplace telepressure measure with a sample from a relevant
from the International Personality Item Pool (NEO Domain; Gold-
population, identify any needed item revisions, and examine the
berg, 1999; Goldberg et al., 2006). Response options ranged from
relationship between workplace telepressure and existing personal
1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). The measure asks to
factors, work environment factors, and work connectivity con- what extent each statement describes the respondents’ typical
structs. Specifically, we expected the following relationships: behavior. An example extraversion item read “make friends eas-
ily,” and an example conscientiousness item read “make plans and
Hypothesis 1: Workplace telepressure is moderately and pos- stick to them.”
itively associated with personality factors, specifically (a) Public self-consciousness. Public self-consciousness (␣ ⫽
conscientiousness, (b) extraversion, and (c) impression .88) was measured by using the revised Self-Consciousness Scale
management. (Scheier & Carver, 1985). Response options range from 1
(strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Example items read “I’m
Hypothesis 2: Workplace telepressure is moderately and pos- concerned about the way I present myself” and “I usually worry
itively associated with work environment factors, specifically about making a good impression.”
(a) ICT demands techno-overload and (b) response norms. Self-monitoring. Self-monitoring (␣ ⫽ .75) was measured by
using the 18-item Self-Monitoring Scale (Gangestad & Snyder,
Hypothesis 3: Workplace telepressure is moderately and pos- 2000). Respondents are asked to indicate whether each statement
itively associated with other general work connectivity con- on this measure is true (1) or false (0) for them. Example items
structs, specifically (a) increased workaholism and (b) in- read, “I guess I put on a show to impress or entertain others” and
creased work engagement. “I’m not always the person I appear to be.”
Techno-overload. A relevant subscale from the technostress
Hypothesis 4: Workplace telepressure is moderately associ- measure (Ragu-Nathan et al., 2008) was chosen for this study:
ated with other ICT-specific connectivity behaviors, specifi- techno-overload (five items; ␣ ⫽ .85). An example item is, “I am
cally (a) less work– home ICT boundary creation, (b) more forced by this technology to do more work than I can handle.”
work– home ICT boundary crossing, and (c) more email Response options ranged from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly
work– home boundary crossing. agree).
176 BARBER AND SANTUZZI

Norms. The descriptive norm item was constructed on the factor solution. Relying on Kaiser’s rule for factor extraction, the
basis of instructions from Fishbein and Ajzen (2010). The resulting results supported a two-factor solution (50% variance explained).
item read, “Most people in my workgroup respond quickly.” The However, the second observed factor compromised only two items
prescriptive norm item was adapted from expectancy violation and, thus, did not provide sufficient evidence for a substantial
measure (Afifi & Metts, 1998). The item read, “I’m expected to factor (Nunnally & Bernstein, 1994). Moreover, the two items did
respond as soon as possible.” Although developed independently, not seem to identify an interpretable content area that uniquely
this item had similar wording to response expectations items from added to the telepressure construct. Thus, omitting the items would
the ICT demands measure (Day et al., 2012: “I am expected to improve the factor solution without negatively affecting the con-
respond to e-mail messages immediately”). Response options for tent validity of the telepressure measure. After omitting the two
both descriptive and prescriptive norm items ranged from 1 items, the remaining six items fit a single-factor solution with 52%
(strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). variance explained and all factor loadings ⬎.58.
Workaholism. Workaholism was measured with the Dutch Although confirming the factor structure required an additional
Workaholism Scale (Schaufeli et al., 2009), which contains a sample (see Study 2), we used the current data to conduct a
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

five-item working excessively subscale (“I seem to be in a hurry preliminary confirmatory factor analysis to identify alternative
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

and racing against the clock”; ␣ ⫽ .81) and a five-item working models to test in subsequent validation studies. Using AMOS
compulsively subscale (“I feel that there’s something inside me (Version 20), we examined the proposed workplace telepressure
that drives me to work hard”; ␣ ⫽ .73). Response options are on construct using the hypothesized one-factor model (Model 1) as
a 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree) scale. well as two alternative models: a model with two correlated
Work engagement. Work engagement was measured with the subdimensions (Model 2) and a model with two uncorrelated
nine-item Utrecht Work Engagement Scale (Shaufeli & Bakker, subdimensions (Model 3). For the two alternative models, the three
2004), consisting of vigor (three items; “At my work, I feel items that more closely related to preoccupation were used as
bursting with energy”), dedication (three items; “My job inspires indicators of one factor and the other three items related to urge
me”), and absorption (three items; “I feel happy when I am were indicators of the second factor. In the current data, Model 1
working intensely”) subscales. Response options were provided on (one-factor model) did not achieve acceptable fit, ␹2(9) ⫽ 67.70,
a seven-point scale ranging from 0 (never) to 6 (always). Because ␹2/df ⫽ 7.53, CFI ⫽ .937, TLI ⫽ .895, SRMR ⫽ .054, RMSEA ⫽
of the high intercorrelation found in this study among variables .136 (CI90% ⫽ [.107, .167]. Model 2 (correlated two-factor model)
(.70 to .81) and recommendation by the authors of the scale provided an acceptable fit to the data, ␹2(8) ⫽ 16.90, ␹2/df ⫽ 2.11,
development (Schaufeli & Bakker, 2003), the nine items were CFI ⫽ .990, TLI ⫽ .982, SRMR ⫽ .024, RMSEA ⫽ .056
combined into an overall measure of work engagement (␣ ⫽ .81)1.
(CI90% ⫽ [.016, .094], with the preoccupation and urge latent
Boundary creation and crossing. Boundary creation around
subdimensions having a very strong intercorrelation (r ⫽ .81). The
ICT use was measured with seven items adapted from Olson-
results for Model 3 (uncorrelated two-factor model) yielded unac-
Buchanan and Boswell (2006). Examples of boundaries include
ceptable fit, ␹2(9) ⫽ 225.15, ␹2/df ⫽ 25.02, CFI ⫽ .769, TLI ⫽
limiting amount of time or when ICTs are used (i.e., only until 7
.614, RMSEA ⫽ .261 (CI90% ⫽ [.232, .291]. Model 3 also had the
p.m.) and not using ICTs during weekends (␣ ⫽ .88). Response
lowest fit among the three (AIC ⫽ 249.15) compared with Model
options ranged from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree).
1 (91.70) and Model 2 (42.90), suggesting that neither subdimen-
Boundary crossing with ICTs was measured with two items that
sion showed strong measurement properties independent of the
focused on performing work tasks at home (Olson-Buchanan &
other one. Given that the fit of the two-factor uncorrelated model
Boswell, 2006; Park & Jex, 2011). The original scale consisted of
(Model 3) was unacceptable, it seems unlikely that workplace
four items, but two items referred to “thinking about work” (i.e.,
telepressure comprised two distinct subfactors. Although the two-
closer to psychological detachment) and were removed. The re-
factor correlated model (Model 2) showed adequate fit, the high
maining two items showed adequate internal consistency (␣ ⫽
interfactor correlation precludes the two labeled subfactors from
.88). The first question asked about how often one tries to arrange,
schedule, or perform job-related activities outside of normal work being treated as separate constructs; both subdimensions cannot be
hours using ICTs. The second question asked how often ICTs are used simultaneously in analyses because of collinearity. Moreover,
used to perform one’s job when one is at home during nonwork the items for workplace telepressure were created and tested to
hours. Response options ranged from 1 (never/almost never) to 5 reflect a unified construct. Using a subset of the items would create
(very often/almost always). Respondents were asked to report how deficiency if used to represent telepressure or as another related
often they typically respond to messages from work during work construct (e.g., preoccupation) outside of the context of workplace
hours. This item was created for this study, and response options telepressure. Taken together, the preliminary set of factor analysis
were on a five-point scale ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (almost results supports the use of workplace telepressure as a single-factor
constantly). construct. Note, however, that the preliminary analyses were con-
ducted on the six workplace telepressure items that were part of an
original pool of eight items. The preliminary results must be
Results
confirmed with a new sample and when administering only the six
items retained as a result of the revision process.
Exploratory Factor Analysis
For an initial examination of the original item pool, an explor- 1
Using the subscales, only dedication (r ⫽ .11) and absorption (r ⫽ .14)
atory factor analysis using principal axis factoring was conducted were significantly associated with telepressure; vigor was not (r ⫽ .03, p ⫽
to identify factor structure and items with poor contributions to the .538).
WORKPLACE TELEPRESSURE 177

Construct Validity uted 5.2% of variance, F(4, 349) ⫽ 4.82, p ⬍ .001, although only
public self-consciousness was a significant predictor (␤ ⫽ .12, p ⫽
Correlations between the revised six-item workplace telepres- .016). The work environment factors explained an additional
sure measure and all study variables are located in Table 1. In 14.7% of the variance in workplace telepressure beyond person-
partial support of Hypothesis 1, workplace telepressure was pos- ality, F(3, 346) ⫽ 21.19, p ⬍ .001. Both techno-overload (␤ ⫽ .25,
itively associated with conscientious and public consciousness, p ⬍ .001) and prescriptive norms (␤ ⫽ .27, p ⬍ .001), but not
with only modest correlations (.11 and .19, respectively). Extra- descriptive norms, were significant predictors. Comparisons be-
version and self-monitoring were not significantly associated with tween dependent coefficients (t tests) showed that both prescrip-
workplace telepressure. Supporting Hypothesis 2, workplace tele-
tive norms and techno-overload were significantly larger than the
pressure was positively associated with work environment de-
coefficient for public self-consciousness (p’s ⬍ .05, one-tailed).
mands of the work environment, specifically, techno-overload,
descriptive norms for responding, and prescriptive norms for re-
sponding. In support of Hypothesis 3, workplace telepressure was Discussion
associated with general work connectivity constructs such as
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

workaholism and work engagement. Finally, in support of Hypoth- The exploratory factor analysis results showed strong support
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

esis 4, workplace telepressure was associated with various opera- for the workplace telepressure measure being a one-factor model
tionalizations of ICT work connectivity: ICT work– home bound- after two problematic items were removed. Preliminary confirma-
ary creation, ICT work– home boundary crossing, and e-mail tory factor analysis results showed some support for the solution
responding in the evenings. The small to moderate correlations but also showed support for an alternative two-factor correlated
across all of these constructs (highest correlation ⫽ .34) supports model. Further examination of the results revealed a strong corre-
our expectation that workplace telepressure is distinct from these lation between factors and similar pattern of relationships across
constructs and that our operationalization of workplace telepres- subdimensions. Overall, the results suggest that the distinction
sure is not redundant with measures of constructs with conceptual between the factors might be too small to warrant treating work-
overlap. place telepressure as a two-factor measure. The comparison be-
tween the hypothesized one-factor model and alternative two-
factor correlated model was revisited in a formal confirmatory
Personal and Work Environment Predictors of
factor analysis with a second sample in Study 2.
Workplace Telepressure
Additionally, correlation results with work connectivity mea-
We conducted an exploratory hierarchical regression analyses to sures (both general and ICT-specific), personal factors, and work
determine the contribution of both personal and work environment environment factors indicated that workplace telepressure was not
factors to workplace telepressure experiences. All personality vari- redundant with conceptually similar constructs. Exploratory anal-
ables (conscientiousness, extraversion, public self-consciousness, yses suggested that public self-consciousness, techno-overload,
self-monitoring) were entered in Step 1, and work environment and prescriptive norms are the best predictors of workplace tele-
factors (techno-overload, descriptive, and prescriptive norms) pressure, with the work environment factors (techno-overload and
were entered in Step 2. The personality factors together contrib- prescriptive norms) having the strongest associations.

Table 1
Correlations and Means/Standard Deviations for Study 1 Variables

M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14

1. Telepressure 3.46 0.79 .86


2. Workaholism (excessive) 3.28 0.72 .22ⴱ .81
3. Workaholism (compulsive) 3.22 0.72 .23ⴱ .59ⴱ .73
4. Work engagement 3.32 1.09 .11ⴱ .31ⴱ .36ⴱ .81
5. ICT work–home boundary
creation 2.63 0.80 ⫺.18ⴱ ⫺.24ⴱ ⫺.30ⴱ ⫺.17ⴱ .81
6. ICT work–home boundary
crossing 2.88 0.99 .20ⴱ .39ⴱ .42ⴱ .34 ⫺.34ⴱ .88
7. E-mail work–home boundary
crossing 2.73 0.99 .25ⴱ .41ⴱ .41ⴱ .32ⴱ ⫺.44ⴱ .56ⴱ —
8. Conscientiousness 3.79 0.62 .11ⴱ .28ⴱ .32ⴱ .47ⴱ ⫺.16ⴱ .12ⴱ .16ⴱ .86
9. Extraversion 3.07 0.78 .02 .13ⴱ .15ⴱ .36ⴱ .08 .14ⴱ .13ⴱ .34ⴱ .90
10. Public self-consciousness 1.64 0.71 .19ⴱ .16ⴱ .17ⴱ .09 ⫺.02 .12ⴱ ⴱ
.11 ⫺.05 ⫺.05 .88
11. Self-monitoring 0.49 0.21 .01 .14ⴱ .09 .16ⴱ ⫺.11ⴱ .15ⴱ .09 ⫺.02 .51ⴱ .14ⴱ .75
12. Technostressors-overload 3.16 0.87 .31ⴱ .47ⴱ .29ⴱ .10 ⫺.09 .31ⴱ .25ⴱ .07 .05 .17ⴱ .04 .85
13. Norm-descriptive
ⴱ ⴱ ⴱ
(workgroup) 3.74 0.87 .15 .01 .03 .20 ⫺.05 ⫺.05 .06 .18 .03 .04 ⫺.04 .07 —
14. Norm-prescriptive
(expectations) 3.78 0.87 .34ⴱ .17ⴱ .23ⴱ .19ⴱ ⫺.15ⴱ .16ⴱ .20ⴱ .31ⴱ .11ⴱ ⫺.03 .14ⴱ .31ⴱ .31 —
Note. N ⫽ 354. Cronbach’s alpha is indicated in bold on the diagonal.

p ⬍ .05.
178 BARBER AND SANTUZZI

Study 2: Revised Scale Construct Validation and Study 1 (i.e., expectations guiding what is appropriate response
Criterion-Related Validity behavior in the organization). Additionally, the workload subscale
of the ICT demands measure is similar to the techno-overload
We conducted a second study to (a) confirm the factor structure measure in Study 1, as it relates to additional work requirements
results for the revised six-item measure by using a new sample, (b) arising from technology use. However, there are a few additional
explore additional personal (i.e., job involvement and affective subdimensions that might be related to workplace telepressure. For
commitment) and work environment factors (i.e., ICT work de- example, availability (expectations for contact outside of work)
mands) associated with workplace telepressure, and (c) examine and poor communication (misinterpreting e-mail tone) might be
the criterion-related validity for workplace telepressure with re- associated with workplace telepressure and lack of control of
spect to stress, recovery, and e-mail responding behavior out-
technology use. Technological hassles (e.g., dealing with computer
comes.
glitches) and monitoring of technology use (e.g., phone calls,
e-mail, Internet), and learning expectations (expectation to learn
Additional Personal and Work Environment Factors new technology) would seem less relevant to affecting message-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

based responding.
Although the results from Study 1 appear to suggest that per-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

sonal factors were not strong predictors of workplace telepressure


Hypothesis 6: Workplace telepressure is positively and mod-
(with the exception of a modest relationship with public self-
erately associated with general work demands and ICT de-
consciousness), this could be because we focused more on person-
mands relevant to message-based responding (response expec-
ality factors rather than psychological identification or attitudes
tations, availability, poor communication, and workload).
toward one’s work. Two particularly relevant factors that might be
associated with workplace telepressure are job involvement and
organizational commitment. Employee Stress, Recovery, and E-mail Behavior
Job involvement is described as “a cognitive or belief state of
psychological identification” with one’s work (Kanungo, 1982; p. Workplace telepressure might have critical occupational health
342). Individuals with high levels of job involvement are more implications because it has the potential to prolong employee work
likely to be intrinsically motivated to engage in work, which is stress both during designated work times and during nonwork
why they tend to have higher levels of both work engagement hours. Given that workplace telepressure should lead to prolonged
(Hallberg & Schaufeli, 2006) and workaholism (Aziz & Zickar, demand exposure via increased ICT use (Day, Scott, & Kelloway,
2006). Additionally, organizational commitment—particularly af- 2010; Olson-Buchanan & Boswell, 2006), it can contribute to the
fective commitment—might also play a role in how connected one health impairment process described by the JD-R model (Demer-
is to the work environment. Employees with higher levels of outi et al., 2001; Bakker & Demerouti, 2007). Thus, continued
affective commitment tend to have a high emotional attachment to exposure to these demands can exhaust these resources to the point
the organization because of shared values and interests (Allen & of poor psychological and physical health. This might take the
Meyer, 1990; Meyer & Allen, 1991) and thus are more likely to form of burnout (physical, cognitive, and emotional exhaustion;
exert cognitive effort on the job, attend work, and engage in Shirom & Melamed, 2006), presenteeism (engaging in work tasks
discretionary behaviors on the job (i.e., organizational citizenship under less than optimal health conditions; Koopman et al., 2002),
behaviors; Meyer, Stanley, Herscovitch, & Topolnytsky, 2002). and health-related absenteeism.
Both job involvement and affective commitment have been linked Job demands that are specific to ICT use have been linked with
with ICT use after work hours (Boswell & Olson-Buchanon, 2007) employee stress above and beyond regular work demands (Day et
and thus these might be important factors to distinguish from al., 2012). It is interesting to note that response expectations and
workplace telepressure. availability aspects of ICT demands were not good predictors of
strain and burnout in past research (Day et al., 2012), which
Hypothesis 5: Workplace telepressure is positively and mod- suggests employees might acknowledge prescriptive norms around
erately associated with (a) job involvement and (b) organiza-
ICT use without necessarily feeling the need to conform to them.
tional commitment.
In fact, the transactional model of stress proposes that the percep-
In Study 1, we also found that workplace telepressure was tion of a particular aspect of the work environment as a demand is
predicted by technological work demands (i.e., techno-overload) needed to affect stress-related outcomes (Day et al., 2010; Lazarus
and prescriptive norms around responding. However, we did not & Folkman, 1984). Thus, workplace telepressure might have a
examine work overload more broadly or a wider variety of ICT distinct relationship with stress outcomes that is not accounted for
demands. Thus, it is important to determine that workplace tele- by general job or specific ICT demands because it represents an
pressure is linked to ICT work demands, more specifically, rather individual’s internalization of ICT demands (i.e., response expec-
than just general perceptions of work demands. Day and col- tations and availability). Different from general work and ICT-
leagues (2012) recently developed a measure that assesses eight specific demands, workplace telepressure might directly lead to
distinct ICT demands that can arise from the work environment: stress because it requires the perception that one needs to adhere to
response expectations, availability, poor communication, hassles, the ICT demand requirements (or prescriptive norms). As such,
employee monitoring, learning expectations, and workload. The employees who are actually experiencing workplace telepressure
most relevant ICT demand to workplace telepressure should be (whether the actual ICT demands are there or not) might suffer
response expectations, which is conceptually similar to prescrip- psychological and physical costs due to the health impairment
tive norms around message-based responding that was assessed in process. In line with the JD-R model, we expect the following:
WORKPLACE TELEPRESSURE 179

Hypothesis 7: Workplace telepressure is positively associated Hypothesis 12: Workplace telepressure is positively associ-
with negative stress-related outcomes, namely (a) burnout, (b) ated with (a) responding frequency and negatively associated
presenteeism, and (c) absenteeism. with (b) e-mail response latency beyond personality and work
environment factors.
Hypothesis 8: Workplace telepressure is positively associated
with negative stress-related outcomes, namely (a) burnout, (b)
presenteeism, and (c) absenteeism beyond personality and Method
work environment factors.
Participants and Procedures
Workplace telepressure might also be associated with recovery
processes. The effort-recovery model (Meijman & Mulder, 1998) Study 2 participants were also recruited through Amazon’s
suggests that prolonged exertion in response to job demands cause Mechanical Turk approximately 1 year after Study 1. Participants
fatigue, and thus some period of recovery is needed to be able to who responded to the previous studies were excluded from partic-
avoid allostatic load (negative physiological effects because of ipation. A total of 343 individuals agreed to continue with the
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

heightened sympathetic nervous system response; McEwen & study after reading the recruitment statement. Access to the survey
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Stellar, 1993). Work recovery activities are critical for sustaining was limited to respondents in the United States who worked
employee health and performance, as they provide a reversal of the full-time, and participants received $1.00 for completing the sur-
stress process brought on by job demands. Given employees need vey. Only 326 met the eligibility requirements to continue with the
sufficient work recovery to be engaged and productive workers survey (measured through automatic disqualification items) and an
(Sonnentag & Fritz, 2007; Sonnentag, 2003), it is imperative that additional 23 respondents were dropped for missing more than one
organizations examine how telepressure might disrupt recovery (out of three) quality control check items, resulting in a final
processes. sample of 303 for data analysis. Respondents were primarily
Prolonged demand exposure due to frequent ICT use might lead full-time workers (76.6%), men (58.4%), Caucasian (78.5%), mar-
to problems with the following work recovery processes: psycho- ried (51.0%), and not telecommuters (79.9%).
logical detachment and restorative sleep. Psychological detach-
ment is considered a critical recovery strategy in the occupational
health literature. It refers to the strategy of not thinking about work
Measures
and work-related events (Sonnentag & Fritz, 2007). Use of ICTs to The six-item measure from Study 1 was used to assess work-
conduct work after hours and low work– home boundaries, which place telepressure. The same measures for Study 1 assessing
are conceptually related to workplace telepressure, are both asso- conscientiousness (Goldberg, 1999; Goldberg et al., 2006) and
ciated with reduced psychological detachment (Park et al., 2011; public self-consciousness (Scheier & Carver, 1985) also were
Sonnentag, Binnewies, & Mojza, 2010). A second form of recov- included. Additional measures are reported in the following para-
ery needed to sustain worker health is achieving restorative sleep. graphs.2
High quality sleep is needed to prevent fatigue and achieve optimal Job involvement. Job involvement was assessed using Ka-
cognitive functioning due to its role in restoring central nervous nungo’s (1982) 10-item Job Involvement Scale (␣ ⫽ .74). Exam-
system functions (Åkerstedt, Nilsson, & Kecklund, 2009). Work- ple items read, “I live, eat, and breathe my job” and “I consider my
place telepressure is expected to lead to cognitive and behavioral job to be very central to my existence.” Response options ranged
interference with sleep that might disrupt restorative aspects of from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree).
sleep. For example, individuals might sleep less because they are Affective commitment. Affective commitment was assessed
using that time to respond to work demands instead (e.g., Barnes, using Meyer and Allen’s (1997) revised six-item subscale from the
Wagner, & Ghumman, 2012). organizational commitment measure (␣ ⫽ .91). Example items
read, “I feel as if my organization’s problems are my own” and “I
Hypothesis 9: Workplace telepressure is negatively associated
do not feel like ‘part of the family’ at my organization” (reverse-
with (a) psychological detachment and (b) restorative sleep.
coded). Response options ranged from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5
Hypothesis 10: Workplace telepressure is negatively associ- (strongly agree).
ated with (a) psychological detachment and (b) restorative General work demands. General work demands were mea-
sleep beyond personality and work environment factors. sured by the five-item Quantitative Workload Inventory (Spector
& Jex, 1998; ␣ ⫽ .86). Example items include, “How often does
Finally, as mentioned previously, workplace telepressure is a
unique response to interpersonal work demand due to interpersonal 2
communication (Day et al., 2010). Employees experiencing higher Per a reviewer’s suggestion, a longer conscientiousness measure assessing
subfacets was also included (NEO P-R Facets; Goldberg, 1999; Goldberg et
levels of workplace telepressure might be more likely to give in to al., 2006), which consisted of the following dimensions: self-efficacy, order-
their urges and engage in frequent e-mail responding behaviors. liness, dutifulness, achievement-striving, self-discipline, and cautiousness.
Thus, this study also investigated e-mail response latency during However, none of these subdimensions were significantly associated with
work time (i.e., time to respond to e-mails) and frequency of e-mail telepressure. The same measure from Study 1 was retained for analyses for
consistency across studies. A 12-item “Dark Triad” measure (Jonason &
responding behavior in general.
Webster, 2010) was also included in this study, which examines the subclinical
traits of Machiavelliasm (“I tend to manipulate others to get my way”; ␣ ⫽
Hypothesis 11: Workplace telepressure is positively associ- .86), psychopathy (“I tend to be callous or insensitive”; ␣ ⫽ .86), and
ated with (a) responding frequency and negatively associated narcissism (“I tend to seek prestige or status”; ␣ ⫽ .88). However, none of
with (b) e-mail response latency. these variables were significantly associated with workplace telepressure.
180 BARBER AND SANTUZZI

your job require you to work very fast?” and “How often is there usual amount of sleep feeling tired and worn out (␣ ⫽ .84). Response
a great deal to be done?” Responses options ranged from 1 (less options range from 0 (never) to 5 (nearly every night), with higher
than once per month or never) to 5 (several times per day). scores indicating poorer sleep quality. Sleep consistency was mea-
ICT demands. ICT demands were measured using a 27-item sured with three items from the Sleep Hygiene Index (Mastin, Bryson,
measure developed by Day et al., (2012), which consists of eight & Corwyn, 2006, referring to going to bed at different times, getting
different subscales: response expectations (two items; ␣ ⫽ .86; out of bed at different times, and staying in bed longer than one should
e.g., “I am expected to respond to e-mail messages immediately”), (i.e., sleeping in) day to day. Response options ranged from 1 (never)
availability (four items; ␣ ⫽ .83; e.g., “Technology enables people to 5 (always) so that higher scores represented more sleep inconsis-
I work with to contact me at any time”), poor communication tency (␣ ⫽ .77)3.
(three items; ␣ ⫽ .84; e.g., “I have misinterpreted the tone of my Self-reported e-mail responding frequency and latency.
incoming e-mail messages”), lack of control (three items; ␣ ⫽ .77; Respondents were asked to report how often they typically respond to
e.g., “I am expected to respond to e-mail messages immediately”), messages from work in the following five situations: during work
hassles (five items; ␣ ⫽ .83; e.g., “My computer freezes”), mon- hours, during the evenings, during the weekend, during vacation days,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

itoring (four items; ␣ ⫽ .91; e.g., “My organization monitors my and during sick days. Response options were on a five-point scale
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

e-mails”), learning expectations (three items; ␣ ⫽ .72; e.g., “I am ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (almost constantly). These items were
expected to stay current with technological advances related to my created for this study. For ease of data presentation, these items were
work”), and workload (three items; ␣ ⫽ .83; e.g., “Technology combined into one measure, given their moderate to strong intercor-
creates more work for me”). Response options ranged from 0 relations (r ⫽ .23 to .82) acceptable internal consistency (␣ ⫽ .77).
(never) to 4 (almost always). An additional measure of e-mail response behavior during work
Burnout. Job burnout is characterized by physical, cognitive,
hours with more exact e-mail response time reporting was included
and emotional exhaustion resulting from chronic exposure to work
for this study. Respondents were asked to open their work e-mail
stress. The Shirom-Melamed Burnout Measure (Shirom &
account and answer questions regarding their last two work e-mails.
Melamed, 2006) was used to measure burnout experienced at work
This information allowed us to analyze specific data on response time
over the past month. This measure uses 14 items consisting of
latency rather than self-reported perceptions of general frequency in
three subdimensions: physical fatigue (e.g., “I feel tired”; ␣ ⫽ .95),
responding. For each e-mail, respondents were asked to report the
cognitive weariness (e.g., “My thinking process is slow”; ␣ ⫽ .96),
e-mail sender classification (e.g., supervisor, coworker, subordinate,
and emotional exhaustion (e.g., “I feel I’m unable to be sensitive
client), day e-mail was sent, time e-mail was sent, their reply day, their
to the needs of coworkers or customers”; ␣ ⫽ .94). Responses
options ranged from 1 (never or almost never) to 7 (always or reply time, and whether or not their reply was during regular work
almost always). hours (yes/no). One-hundred three individuals indicated they had their
Absenteeism and presenteeism. Whereas absenteeism de- work email account readily accessible and reported their last response
scribes an employee not being at work, presenteeism describes the act latency information. Of these individuals, only 96 reported enough
of engaging in work tasks under less than optimal health conditions data to calculate response latencies (i.e., seven had missing informa-
(Koopman et al., 2002). Absenteeism was measured using an item tion for either the send/reply date or time sections). Given that e-mail
from the Health and Work Performance Questionnaire (Kessler et al., response latencies showed a strong positive skew (2.89), this score
2003). This question asked the number of full work days missed were transformed using a logarithmic (base 10) transformation before
because of a physical or mental health issues. Presenteeism was conducting analyses.4
measured using the Stanford Presenteeism Scale (Koopman et al.,
2002). It contained six items asking to what extent health problems 3
Stress and recovery outcomes were tested in a pilot study with 313
interfered with productivity over the past month (e.g., “Because of MTurk respondents using the revised six-item measure. Telepressure was
health problems, I was unable to finish hard tasks in my work”; ␣ ⫽ associated with less psychological detachment (␣ ⫽ .78; r ⫽ –.25), but not
.97). Response options ranged from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 other recovery activities such as relaxation (␣ ⫽ .89; r ⫽ –.08, p ⫽ .239),
(strongly agree). mastery (␣ ⫽ .83; r ⫽ –.05, p ⫽ .362), or control (␣ ⫽ .83; r ⫽ –.01; p ⫽
.948). With respective to restorative sleep, telepressure was associated with
Psychological detachment. The Psychological Detachment poorer sleep quality (␣ ⫽ .84; r ⫽ .18) and more sleep inconsistency (␣ ⫽
subscale from Sonnentag and Fritz’s (2007) Recovery Experience .77; r ⫽ .19) but not sleep quantity (r ⫽ ⫺.07, p ⫽ .242). Telepressure was
Questionnaire was used to measure recovery. Respondents were also associated with increased physical (␣ ⫽ .95; r ⫽ .24), cognitive (␣ ⫽
asked to state their agreement with four statements (e.g., “I don’t think .96; r ⫽ .32), and emotional (␣ ⫽ .94; r ⫽ .18) burnout, and increased
presenteeism (␣ ⫽ .97; r ⫽ .22) and absenteeism (r ⫽ .14). Telepressure
about work at all”; ␣ ⫽ .78) regarding their free evenings (or nonwork was also related to more psychological strain (␣ ⫽ .67; r ⫽ .19) and less
time), with responses ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 satisfaction with work–life balance (␣ ⫽ .95; r ⫽ ⫺.19; Valcour, 2007).
(strongly agree). Because of survey length constraints for Study 2, we focused on burnout
Sleep recovery problems. Sleep recovery problems were mea- and presenteeism/absenteeism outcomes only.
4
sured in three different ways: sleep quantity, sleep quality, and sleep The e-mail responding frequency and latency measures were tested in
a pilot study with 228 MTurk respondents using the revised six-item
inconsistency. Respondents were asked how many hours they typi- measure. Telepressure was significantly associated with responding across
cally slept per night during a work week. Less sleep duration repre- the five different situations (r ⫽ .22 to .36) and the combined response
sents more recovery problems. Sleep quality was measured using a frequency measure (r ⫽ .35; ␣ ⫽ .84). Telepressure was also significantly
measure of chronic insomnia (Jenkins, Standton, Niemcryk, & Rose, associated with shorter response latency when respondents reported their
last (r ⫽ –.17) and second to last (r ⫽ –.16) e-mail response times for work
1988). This measure asks about four insomnia symptoms experienced purposes. Because of a high amount of positive skew (i.e., only a few
over the last month: trouble falling asleep, trouble staying asleep, individuals had very long response lags), response latencies were be
waking up several times during the night, and waking up after one’s transformed with a logarithmic (base 10) transformation for analyses.
WORKPLACE TELEPRESSURE 181

Results beyond personal and work environment factors, ⌬F(1, 288) ⫽


9.43, p ⬍ .05, ⌬R2 ⫽ .030. Thus, Hypothesis 8 was supported for
Confirmatory factor analyses. The hypothesized one-factor these stress outcomes but not emotional burnout and presenteeism.
model showed good fit with the exception of the RMSEA, ␹2(9) ⫽ Results for recovery outcomes are presented in Table 4. Hy-
57.14, ␹2/df ⫽ 6.35, TLI ⫽ .932, CFI ⫽ .959, SRMR ⫽ .036, pothesis 10 was only partially supported, as workplace telepressure
RMSEA ⫽ .133 (CI90% ⫽ [.101, .167]. The alternative two-factor did not predict additional variance in psychological detachment or
model for the six-item measure demonstrated good fit with the some aspects of restorative sleep (quantity and consistency) be-
data, also with the exception of RMSEA, ␹2(8) ⫽ 48.08, ␹2/df ⫽ yond personal and work environment factors. However, workplace
6.01, TLI ⫽ .936, CFI ⫽ .966, SRMR ⫽ .033, RMSEA ⫽ .129 telepressure did add incremental validity to the prediction of sleep
(CI90% ⫽ [.095, .165]). Although the one-factor solution had a quality, ⌬F(1, 288) ⫽ 4.27, p ⬍ .05, ⌬R2 ⫽ .012.
comparatively poorer fit than the two-factor model (one-factor Results for e-mail responding outcomes are presented in Table
AIC ⫽ 81.14; two-factor AIC ⫽ 74.08), the usefulness of two 5. Hypothesis 12 was only partially supported, as workplace tele-
distinct subfactors could not be supported. Similar to the prelim- pressure only added incremental validity to the prediction of
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

inary results in Study 1, the latent correlation between urge and frequency of e-mail responding, ⌬F(1, 288) ⫽ 7.19, p ⬍ .05,
preoccupation was very high (standardized estimate ⫽ .94), again
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

⌬R2 ⫽ .013, beyond personal and organizational factors. Work-


suggesting a high degree of overlap between the subdimensions. place telepressure did not predict additional variance in response
Given that the construct was conceptualized with a unified defi- latency with all variables. However, the personal and work envi-
nition and the alternative two-factor model did not yield distin- ronment factors also did not contribute in the prior steps, poten-
guishable subfactors, we find the hypothesized one-factor solution tially because the sample size was so small for response latency
to have the strongest support in this study. (N ⫽ 96). Thus, although there is theoretical justification that the
Construct validity. Bivariate associations among Study 2 personal and work environment factors in this study have relation-
variables are presented in Table 2. In support of Hypothesis 5, ships with e-mail response latency, this assumption was only
workplace telepressure was significantly associated with increased supported for three work environment factors in the bivariate
job involvement (r ⫽ .22) and affective commitment (r ⫽ .13). correlation results: ICT demands for response expectations, poor
Contrary to Study 1, conscientiousness was not significantly as- communication, and hassles. Thus, only these variables were used
sociated with workplace telepressure in this sample, but its signif- in a revised regression model in the first step, with the second step
icant relationship with public self-consciousness remained (r ⫽ including workplace telepressure (see revised model in Table 5). In
.22). In support of Hypothesis 6, workplace telepressure was this model, workplace telepressure did explain incremental vari-
associated with increased perceptions of work demands (r ⫽ .24), ance in response latency beyond ICT demands, ⌬F(1, 91) ⫽ 5.08,
and ICT demands that are relevant for message-based responding: p ⬍ .05, ⌬R2 ⫽ .045, with increased workplace telepressure
response expectations (r ⫽ .36), availability expectations (r ⫽ predicting shorter response latencies.
.28), poor communication (r ⫽ .11), and technological workload
(r ⫽ .20). Unexpectedly, workplace telepressure was also linked
with learning expectations (r ⫽ .18). Taken together, these modest Discussion
to moderate associations with these constructs suggest workplace On the basis of the results from Study 2, workplace telepressure
telepressure is distinct from personal factors (job involvement, is distinct from personal factors such as conscientiousness, self-
affective commitment) and work environment factors related to consciousness, job involvement, and affective commitment, as it
general and ICT-specific work demands. has only modest relationships with these constructs. Workplace
Criterion-related validity with stress, recovery, and e-mail telepressure is also distinct from general perceptions of work
responding outcomes. In partial support of Hypothesis 7, work- demands and ICT demands. Employees who report more ICT
place telepressure was associated with increased physical and demands around response expectations, availability, poor commu-
cognitive burnout as well as absenteeism. Workplace telepressure nication, technological workload, and learning expectations were
was not significantly associated with emotional burnout and pre- more likely to experience workplace telepressure, but the moderate
senteeism. Hypothesis 9 was also only partially supported. Work- correlations with these constructs suggest workplace telepressure
place telepressure was associated with less psychological detach- is not redundant with perceptions of work demands.
ment (r ⫽ ⫺.20) and sleep quality (r ⫽ .21) but not sleep quantity In terms of predicting outcomes, employees reporting increased
or sleep inconsistency. Finally, Hypothesis 11 was fully supported, levels of workplace telepressure also reported less psychological
as workplace telepressure was associated with more frequent detachment and poorer sleep quality (although not sleep quantity or
e-mail responding (r ⫽ .31) and shorter e-mail response times consistency). Also, more workplace telepressure was associated with
(r ⫽ ⫺.34). higher levels of burnout (physical and cognitive only) and health-
Hierarchical regression analyses were conducted to determine related absenteeism. Finally, employees experiencing more workplace
whether workplace telepressure demonstrated incremental predic- telepressure were more likely to respond more frequently to e-mails
tive validity for stress (Hypothesis 8), recovery (Hypothesis 10), and have shorter e-mail response times. Moreover, with the exception
and e-mail (Hypothesis 12) outcomes beyond personal and work of psychological detachment, workplace telepressure predicted phys-
environment factors. Results for stress outcomes are presented in ical/cognitive burnout, health-related absenteeism, sleep quality, and
Table 3. Workplace telepressure added incremental validity to the e-mail responding outcomes beyond personal and work environment
prediction of physical burnout, ⌬F(1, 288) ⫽ 14.48, p ⬍ .05, factors. Employees might feel that staying connected longer and
⌬R2 ⫽ .028, and cognitive burnout, ⌬F(1, 288) ⫽ 9.92, p ⬍ .05, responding quickly will be viewed as a sign of good performance.
⌬R2 ⫽ .021. Workplace telepressure also predicted absenteeism However, similar to other sources of work-related strain and burnout,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

182

Table 2
Descriptive Statistics and Correlations for Study 2 Variables

M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24

1. Telepressure 3.36 0.93 .91


2. Conscientiousness 3.94 0.59 .07 .87
3. Self-consciousness 1.62 0.78 .22ⴱ .01 .90
4. Job involvement 2.79 0.61 .19ⴱ .20ⴱ .12ⴱ .74
5. Affective commitment 4.34 1.51 .13ⴱ .42ⴱ .07 .58ⴱ .91
6. General workload 3.34 0.75 .24ⴱ .10 .22ⴱ .19ⴱ ⫺.06 .86
7. ICT-response 3.22 0.96 .36ⴱ .14ⴱ .30ⴱ .20ⴱ .09 .32ⴱ .85
8. ICT-available 3.30 0.87 .28ⴱ .21ⴱ .24ⴱ .34ⴱ .21ⴱ .42ⴱ .62ⴱ .83
9. ICT-Comm 1.85 0.72 .11ⴱ ⫺.32ⴱ .11 .11 ⫺.13ⴱ .11 .18ⴱ .15ⴱ .84
10. ICT-lack of control 2.56 0.92 .02 ⫺.37ⴱ ⫺.01 ⫺.24ⴱ ⫺.38ⴱ .13ⴱ .00 ⫺.14ⴱ .11 .77
11. ICT-hassles 1.96 0.63 .04 ⫺.36ⴱ .05 .01 ⫺.25ⴱ .08 .10 ⫺.01 .42ⴱ .18ⴱ .83
12. ICT-monitor 2.32 1.25 .11 ⫺.03 .06 ⫺.04 ⫺.16ⴱ .29ⴱ .20ⴱ .11 .17ⴱ .23ⴱ .23ⴱ .91
13. ICT-learning 3.13 0.88 .18ⴱ .27ⴱ .10 .25ⴱ .15ⴱ .26ⴱ .31ⴱ .41ⴱ .12ⴱ ⫺.22ⴱ .02 .23ⴱ .72
14. ICT-workload 2.66 0.98 .20ⴱ .05 .20ⴱ .26ⴱ .03 .49ⴱ .38ⴱ .44ⴱ .30ⴱ .05 .20ⴱ .26ⴱ .50ⴱ .83
15. Detachment 3.00 0.99 ⫺.20ⴱ ⫺.11 ⫺.16ⴱ ⫺.43ⴱ ⫺.24ⴱ ⫺.21ⴱ ⫺.27ⴱ ⫺.41ⴱ ⫺.12ⴱ ⫺.02 .01 ⫺.03 ⫺.17ⴱ ⫺.32ⴱ .89
16. Sleep quantity 6.77 1.03 ⫺.04 .04 ⫺.06 ⫺.10 .01 ⫺.05 .14ⴱ ⫺.23ⴱ ⫺.08 ⫺.06 ⫺.05 ⫺.05 .01 ⫺.09 .16ⴱ —
17. Sleep quality (poor) 2.81 1.72 .21ⴱ ⫺.15ⴱ .15ⴱ .07 ⫺.06 .14ⴱ .24ⴱ .25ⴱ .19ⴱ .17ⴱ .22ⴱ .13ⴱ .10 .15ⴱ ⫺.19ⴱ ⫺.44ⴱ .86
BARBER AND SANTUZZI

18. Sleep inconsistency 2.27 0.95 .10 ⫺.28ⴱ .05 .05 ⫺.08 .07 .03 .00 .18ⴱ .12ⴱ .20ⴱ .08 ⫺.07 .08 ⫺.08 ⫺.19ⴱ .37ⴱ .81
19. Burnout- physical 3.53 1.48 .19ⴱ ⫺.46ⴱ .10 ⫺.18ⴱ ⫺.38ⴱ .17ⴱ .11 .04 .23ⴱ .40ⴱ .33ⴱ .20ⴱ ⫺.10 .09 ⫺.03 ⫺.32ⴱ .54ⴱ .44ⴱ .95
20. Burnout-cognitive 2.81 1.32 .14ⴱ ⫺.54ⴱ .13 ⫺.09 ⫺.28ⴱ .04 ⫺.02 ⫺.03 .26ⴱ .33ⴱ .32ⴱ .14ⴱ ⫺.12ⴱ .03 .01 ⫺.25ⴱ .45ⴱ .41ⴱ .78ⴱ .96
21. Burnout-emotional 2.50 1.44 ⫺.01 ⫺.48ⴱ .04 ⫺.03 ⫺.34ⴱ .05 .03 .05 .33ⴱ .31ⴱ .33ⴱ .19ⴱ ⫺.03 .16ⴱ ⫺.01 ⫺.17ⴱ .27ⴱ .21ⴱ .53ⴱ .58ⴱ .96
22. Presenteeism 2.05 1.36 .11 ⫺.35ⴱ .03 .06 ⫺.19ⴱ .05 .14ⴱ .07 .27ⴱ .21ⴱ .23ⴱ .10 .02 .16ⴱ ⫺.15ⴱ ⫺.12ⴱ .31ⴱ .30ⴱ .44ⴱ .41ⴱ .37ⴱ .96
23. Absenteeism-health 0.35 0.89 .14ⴱ ⫺.10 ⫺.06 .04 ⫺.07 ⫺.07 .00 .01 .10 .04 .02 ⫺.06 ⫺.11 .01 ⫺.02 ⫺.05 .05 .14ⴱ .18ⴱ .18ⴱ .20ⴱ .42ⴱ —
24. E-mail response frequency 2.67 0.72 .31ⴱ .17ⴱ .14ⴱ .45ⴱ .31ⴱ .23ⴱ .43ⴱ .58ⴱ .19ⴱ ⫺.18ⴱ .01 ⫺.01 .28ⴱ .41ⴱ ⫺.50ⴱ ⫺.17ⴱ .15ⴱ .08 ⫺.05 ⫺.07 ⫺.07 .10 .03 —
25. E-mail response latency 40.05 66.46 ⫺.34ⴱ .03 ⫺.17 ⫺.03 ⫺.13 ⫺.05 ⫺.30ⴱ ⫺.13 ⫺.20ⴱ ⫺.01 ⫺.21ⴱ ⫺.09 ⫺.07 ⫺.11 ⫺.02 .04 ⫺.13 .09 ⫺.08 .01 .18 .02 .02 ⫺.22

Note. N ⫽ 303, except for e-mail response latency (N ⫽ 92). Raw means and standard deviations for e-mail response latencies in minutes are reported for better interpretation but reported correlations
are based on logarithmic (base 10) transformations. Cronbach’s alpha is indicated in bold on the diagonal.

p ⬍ .05.
WORKPLACE TELEPRESSURE 183

Table 3
Workplace Telepressure Predicting Study 2 Stress-Related Outcomes Beyond Personal and Work Environment Factors

Burnout Burnout Burnout


(Physical) (Cognitive) (Emotional) Presenteeism Absenteeism
␤ t ␤ t ␤ t ␤ t ␤ t

Step 1
Conscientiousness ⫺.31 ⫺5.33ⴱ ⫺.44 ⫺7.48ⴱ ⫺.32 ⫺5.32ⴱ ⫺.28 ⫺4.11ⴱ ⫺.03 ⫺.37
Public self-consciousness .03 .59 ⫺.11 2.15ⴱ .01 .25 ⫺.04 ⫺.67 ⫺.09 ⫺1.45
Job involvement ⫺.07 ⫺1.06 .02 .30 .15 2.40ⴱ .16 2.32 .14 1.81
Affective commitment ⫺.15 ⫺2.28ⴱ ⫺.05 ⫺.78 ⫺.24 ⫺3.49ⴱ ⫺.14 ⫺1.92 ⫺.17 ⫺2.10ⴱ
⌬F(4, 298), ⌬R2 27.90ⴱ .263 35.44ⴱ .322 29.75ⴱ .285 13.99ⴱ .158 1.94 .025
Step 2
General workload .08 1.34 ⫺0.02 ⫺.32 ⫺.11 ⫺1.84 ⫺.08 ⫺1.14 ⫺.15 ⫺2.06ⴱ
⫺0.12 ⫺1.92 ⫺.07 ⫺1.09 ⫺1.41 ⫺.36 ⫺.46
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ICT Demands-response expectations .01 .05 .10


2.38ⴱ
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ICT Demands-availability .12 1.85 .12 1.78 .16 .16 .20 .08 .95
ICT Demands-poor communication ⫺.01 ⫺.05 .03 .53 .10 1.68 .08 1.33 .08 1.14
ICT Demands-lack of control .17 3.20ⴱ .14 2.47ⴱ .13 2.22ⴱ 1.37 ⫺.01 ⫺.05
ICT Demands-hassles .13 2.36ⴱ .11 2.06ⴱ .07 1.15 .09 .36 ⫺.05 ⫺.76
ICT Demands-monitoring .05 .99 .06 1.24 .10 1.92 .02 .20 ⫺.04 ⫺.64
ICT Demands-learning expectations ⫺.05 ⫺.80 ⫺.01 ⫺.14 ⫺.01 ⫺.13 .01 .25 ⫺.16 ⫺2.25ⴱ
ICT Demands-workload ⫺.03 ⫺.47 ⫺.05 ⫺.84 .09 1.35 .02 1.12 .09 1.17
⌬F(9, 289), ⌬R2 4.73ⴱ .093 2.14ⴱ .042 3.44ⴱ .069 1.91 .047 1.97 .035
Step 3
Telepressure .18 3.67ⴱ .16 3.15ⴱ ⫺.02 ⫺.37 .07 1.21 .19 3.07ⴱ
⌬F(1, 288), ⌬R2 14.48ⴱ .028 9.92ⴱ .021 .13 .000 1.46 .004 9.43ⴱ .030
Full Model
F(14, 288), R2 13.39ⴱ .394 12.92ⴱ .386 11.31ⴱ .355 5.45ⴱ .209 2.04 .015
Note. Global model fit statistics are in italics.

p ⬍ .05.

the preoccupation and urge to respond quickly to work communica- misperceived when workers rely extensively on technology for
tions might backfire and yield negative, rather than positive, outcomes communication. For example, workers can carefully craft ICT
for the individual. messages that convey a strong norm for fast response times by
apologizing for delays in responses. A coworker who receives such
Overall Discussion messages might infer that she or he should be responding very
quickly. It is important that the recipient of the message can only
The main objectives of this series of studies were to (a) intro- observe the content of an ICT message, conveying a prescriptive
duce the construct of workplace telepressure and its important norm. ICT communications limit informative nonverbal cues that
implications for work and health outcomes and (b) develop and can clarify the actual urgency or importance of replying to the
validate a brief measure of workplace telepressure that can be used message. Therefore, prescriptive norms might be relatively more
in research and practice. The results from Study 1 supported the
salient as workers have more access to them through explicit
validation of a six-item measure. Study 2 confirmed the single-
organizational policies and formal performance feedback.
factor structure of the six-item measure as the most useful structure
Prescriptive norms are often learned through organizational culture,
with a new sample and showed support for workplace telepressure
including explicit organization policies and procedures (Cialdini, Ba-
predicting some stress, recovery, and e-mail responding outcomes
tor, & Guadagna, 1999). If prescriptive norms are the stronger influ-
beyond personal and work environment factors.
ence on workplace telepressure, the employing organization might
Study 1 also showed that personality factors appear to only have
modest correlations with workplace telepressure, whereas work have more control over those perceptions. Unlike descriptive norms
environment factors tended to have moderate correlations. Pre- among employees that arise out of habits, an organization has the
scriptive (but not descriptive) situational norms and techno- power to dictate changes to prescriptive organizational norms. Orga-
overload served as the best predictors of workplace telepressure, nizations should be able to address problems related to workplace
followed by public self-consciousness. Thus, workplace telepres- telepressure by developing workplace policies around ICT use and
sure seems less likely to be a manifestation of personality than a expectations or encouraging workgroups to collaboratively discuss
response to work environment demands. The fact that prescriptive and set response expectation agreements independently. Examples
norms emerged as better predictors of workplace telepressure than include specific guidelines regarding response times during the work
descriptive norms suggests that perceptions about what one should day or during weekends (e.g., within 48 hrs), training employees on
do might be more influential than perceptions of what others how to limit message responding and checking to certain times of day
actually do. Past research suggests that this should be true when or outlining “blackout” days or times when employees are not ex-
prescriptive norms are more salient than descriptive norms (Cial- pected to respond (after 6 PM and before 8 AM). In 2011, the German
dini, Kallgren, & Reno, 1991). Many descriptive norms can be company Volkswagen made news headlines when it restricted e-mail
184 BARBER AND SANTUZZI

Table 4
Workplace Telepressure Predicting Study 2 Recovery Outcomes Beyond Personal and Social/organizational Factors

Psychological
Detachment Sleep Quantity Sleep Quality Sleep Consistency
␤ t ␤ t ␤ t ␤ t

Step 1
Conscientiousness ⫺.04 ⫺.56 .03 .34 ⫺.11 ⫺1.63 ⫺.25 ⫺3.49ⴱ
Public self-consciousness ⫺.03 ⫺.63 ⫺.01 ⫺.24 .05 .90 .01 .17
Job involvement ⫺.32 ⫺4.84ⴱ ⫺.11 ⫺1.39 .01 .20 .08 1.06
Affective commitment ⫺.02 ⫺.29 .08 1.03 .00 .02 .01 .08
⌬F(4, 298), ⌬R2 18.06ⴱ .195 1.52 .020 4.29ⴱ .054 7.78ⴱ .095
Step 2
General workload .04 .61 .10 .10 ⫺.03 ⫺.38 .03 .45
⫺3.75ⴱ ⫺.02 ⫺.25
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ICT Demands-response expectations .03 .40 .01 .04 .58


⫺.29 ⫺4.02ⴱ ⫺.30 ⫺.15 2.99ⴱ
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ICT Demands-availability .23 .00 .05


ICT Demands-poor communication ⫺.03 ⫺.45 ⫺.01 ⫺.93 .02 .36 .04 .66
ICT Demands-lack of control ⫺.15 ⫺2.51ⴱ ⫺.06 .01 .15 2.37ⴱ ⫺.01 ⫺.10
ICT Demands-hassles .06 .95 .00 ⫺.68 .15 2.32ⴱ .08 1.15
ICT Demands-monitoring .02 .35 ⫺.04 1.64 .02 .28 .04 .70
ICT Demands-learning expectations .08 1.34 .12 ⫺.33 .05 .73 ⫺.09 ⫺1.31
ICT Demands-workload ⫺.17 ⫺2.47ⴱ ⫺.03 .25 ⫺.06 ⫺.84 .05 .67
⌬F(9, 289), ⌬R2 5.11ⴱ .111 2.33ⴱ .066 4.19ⴱ .109 .74 .020
Step 3
Telepressure ⫺.05 ⫺.85 .02 .35 .13 2.07ⴱ .09 1.49
⌬F(1, 288), ⌬R2 .73 .002 .06 .000 4.27ⴱ .012 2.23 .007
Full model
F(14, 288), R2 9.13ⴱ .307 1.95ⴱ .086 4.28ⴱ .176 2.85ⴱ .122
Note. Global model fit statistics are in italics.

p ⬍ .05.

access for organizational members on company-issued ICT devices behavior. The results from Study 2 found that higher workplace
during nonwork hours. Specifically, employees could only access telepressure is associated with stress-related outcomes such as
their work e-mail during work hours up to 30 min after their shift, physical and cognitive burnout and health-related absenteeism.
with access returning 30 min before their next shift (“VW turns off,” Although workplace telepressure does not seem to be associated
2011). A survey of over 300 U.S.-based and multinational organiza- with psychological detachment beyond personal and work envi-
tions found that 21% of companies reported instituting formal policies ronment factors or sleep quantity and consistency, telepressured
regarding ICT use outside of work hours, with 26% reporting infor- employees do report poorer sleep quality. Study 2 also indicated
mal policies (Society of Human Resource Management, 2012). Given that telepressured employees report that they respond more fre-
that recent research suggests that setting boundaries around ICT use is quency to e-mails and have shorter e-mail response times.
helpful for employee recovery and well-being (Barber & Jenkins, On the surface, faster response times might be interpreted as
2014; Olson-Buchanan & Boswell, 2006), more organizations should positive work performance such that information is being commu-
consider adopting technology use policies or providing training to nicated faster and progress toward organizational goals is acceler-
help employees set ICT use boundaries. ated. However, responsiveness might not map on to actual work
Having influence via prescriptive norms over workplace tele- productivity and might actually be linked to poorer work quality in
pressure also positions an organization to demonstrate support for an effort to maximize speed. Additionally, gains in communication
a healthy workplace and work-life balance. Given that prescriptive and information exchange might come at a cost to health and
norms seem to be more tied to workplace telepressure, an organi- well-being if workplace telepressure levels are high. Given that
zation might directly intervene when high levels of workplace Study 2 suggests workplace telepressure is associated with higher
telepressure and the associated risks to health and well-being burnout and absenteeism, as well as poorer sleep quality, employ-
emerge among employees. Communicating cultural values such as ers should ensure that employee health and well-being is not
family supportive work environments (i.e., organizational support compromised as a consequence of their high motivation to per-
for employees’ family lives; Allen, 2001; Thompson, Beauvais, & form. Organizations might want to emphasize that they value high
Lyness, 1999), might help employees engage in more recovery quality work that requires extended periods of uninterrupted time
time during their off-work hours instead of responding to work- rather than high responsiveness via message-based technologies.
related messages. Having managerial support for helping employ- Study 2 also suggests that technological demands of the work
ees set healthy limits on accessibility is a key factor that might help environment— especially related response expectations—seem to be
reduce workplace telepressure. significantly associated with workplace telepressure rather than over-
Also important to both employees and their employing organi- all workload. Thus, workplace telepressure appears to be a unique
zation is that higher levels of workplace telepressure are associated experience arising from a growing reliance on technological commu-
with some aspects of stress, recovery, and e-mail responding nication in the work environment. As such, organizations need to pay
WORKPLACE TELEPRESSURE 185

Table 5
Workplace Telepressure Predicting Study 2 E-mail Behavior Outcomes Beyond Personal and Work Environment Factors

E-mail
E-mail E-mail Responding
Responding Responding Latency
Frequency Latency (Modified)
␤ t ␤ t ␤ t

Step 1
Conscientiousness .01 .11 .12 .76
Public self-consciousness ⫺.06 ⫺1.31 ⫺.03 ⫺.23
Job involvement .19 3.36ⴱ ⫺.13 .98
Affective commitment .07 1.08 ⫺.27 ⫺1.95
⌬F(4, 298), ⌬R2 20.32ⴱ .214 ⌬F(4, 91), ⌬R2 1.56 .064
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Step 2
General workload ⫺.08 ⫺1.50 .02 .12
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ICT Demands-response expectations .08 1.36 ⫺.33 ⫺2.23ⴱ ⫺.19 ⫺1.76


ICT Demands-availability .38 6.15ⴱ .08 .54
ICT Demands-poor communication .07 1.36 ⫺.18 ⫺1.59 ⫺.14 ⫺1.34
ICT Demands-lack of control ⫺.06 ⫺1.19 .09 .71
ICT Demands-hassles ⫺.02 ⫺.40 ⫺.18 ⫺1.38 ⫺.13 ⫺1.20
ICT Demands-monitoring ⫺.07 ⫺1.36 ⫺.04 ⫺.30
ICT Demands-learning expectations ⫺.08 ⫺1.37 ⫺.03 ⫺.19
ICT Demands-workload .23 3.96ⴱ .13 .93
⌬F(9, 289), ⌬R2 14.64ⴱ .246 ⌬F(9, 82), ⌬R2 1.97 .166 ⌬F(3, 92), ⌬R2 5.26ⴱ .146
Step 3
Telepressure .13 2.68ⴱ ⫺.19 ⫺1.54 ⫺.24 ⫺2.25ⴱ
⌬F(1, 288), ⌬R2 7.19ⴱ .013 ⌬F(1, 81), ⌬R2 2.38 .022 ⌬F(1, 91), ⌬R2 5.08ⴱ .045
Full model
F(14, 288), R2 18.50ⴱ .473 F(14, 81), R2 1.95ⴱ .252 F(4, 91), R2 5.39ⴱ .192
Note. Model fit statistics are in italics.

p ⬍ .05.

more attention to how ICTs create additional demands beyond em- independent of ICT or customer responsiveness) might be less
ployees’ regular workload. Specifically, employee responses to these susceptible to workplace telepressure.
demands (i.e., workplace telepressure) can have an incrementally As expected, the present set of studies found only minimal
negative effect on employee health and well-being. evidence for a relationship between personal factors and work-
place telepressure. However, we only examined those personal
factors (e.g., conscientiousness) as direct predictors of workplace-
Limitations and Future Research specific telepressure and outcomes. Future research should exam-
Although the set of studies included individual differences and ine whether those individual differences have similar associations
outcomes that were most clearly implicated in the existing social with nonwork demands and nonwork-related telepressure (i.e.,
influence and occupational health literatures, several additional messages from family members and friends). Important to our
variables could be considered to be important to this examination. conclusions, such research should consider whether work-related
For example, the manner in which information about norms is and nonwork ICT messages both create additional burden and
conveyed might play a large role in how workers respond to those stress for some types of workers. For instance, highly conscien-
norms. If an organization has a strong social environment, descrip- tious and extraverted workers might experience an additional
tive norms might be transmitted and more salient than prescriptive burden from attempting to respond quickly to both work and
norms conveyed through formal policy and performance feedback. nonwork communications. Furthermore, how these individual dif-
Thus, our research did not find strong support for the role of ferences manifest with telepressure might be moderated by other
descriptive norms in workplace telepressure, but we were unable to factors, such as domain centrality (e.g., Edwards & Rothbard,
examine the strength or ambiguity of those norms from the work- 1999; Kossek Ruderman, Braddy, & Hannum, 2012). That is, the
ers’ perspectives. relationships among work demands, workplace telepressure, and
Future research should build on our findings related to work- stress outcomes might be stronger among individuals who define
place norms to examine whether specific job characteristics or their self-concept in relation to the work domain (highly work
organizational features yield more workplace telepressure than centric) as opposed to those who find their home domains more
other characteristics among employees. For example, fast response salient (highly family centric). Those with dual centricity (i.e.,
times are rewarded in client-oriented work such as in consulting Lobel & St. Clair, 1992), might suffer the most from telepressure
and sales. Workers in those positions likely are susceptible to high arising from trying to meet social communication demands in both
levels of workplace telepressure and the associated negative health work and home domains. Future research could extend our limited
consequences. Other jobs that have a different reward system (e.g., examination of individual differences to examine additional dis-
186 BARBER AND SANTUZZI

positional tendencies and moderating factors that might make they did not currently have access to their work email or did not
some employees more susceptible to workplace telepressure than wish to complete that information. This might have been partly
others. Workers who have a nigh need for cognitive closure driven by the longer time taken to complete the survey (with the
(Webster & Kruglanski, 1994), for example, might experience response latency questions at the end). In our pilot survey with this
stronger urges to complete ICT communications as they have a variable, we had an 82% response rate, most likely because this
lower tolerance for ambiguity and “loose ends.” survey only respondents an average of seven minutes to complete
Limiting generalizability across time, data from both studies (compared with the average of 20 min for Study 2). Fortunately,
were based on cross-sectional surveys. The decision was based in the workplace telepressure relationships with this outcome for
part on the construct definition for workplace telepressure indicat- Study 2 appeared to be consistent with the pilot study, but we
ing that the experience was situational. Thus, an employee’s report cannot rule out the potential of bias due to the high proportion of
of workplace telepressure, antecedent norms, and health recovery missing data. Second, relying on only the last work e-mail to
outcomes were collected from the same situation. This design was assess this measure is a small sample of behavior. This method was
limited to only a single, short-term experience reported in these chosen so that it would be less time-intrusive for participants to
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

studies. Future research using longitudinal designs should consider


complete. Finding a way to average e-mail response times over
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

the trajectory of workplace telepressure and whether it will have


more e-mails by using a more comprehensive yet less time-
consequences on both performance and health over a period of
intensive methodology (i.e., e-mail monitoring software that can
time on a job. Over short or moderate periods of time, workplace
protect content privacy) for future research would help to alleviate
telepressure might not negatively affect health outcomes. How-
this concern. Also, future studies can move beyond work e-mail
ever, long-term exposure to workplace telepressure might have
responding behavior to determine what effects workplace telepres-
cumulative effects on health through increased allostastic load
(i.e., negative physiological effects of chronic stress exposure; sure might have on work performance. As mentioned before, email
McEwen, 1998). This occurs because of the heighted “fight-or- responsiveness is not necessarily an indicator of better job perfor-
flight” stress response to workplace demands that employees mance—thus, explorations into how workplace telepressure might
might be continually exposed to because of the lack of sustained affect actual work quality and quantity would be useful to orga-
disconnection from work. Moreover, research should consider nizations.
whether experiences of workplace telepressure shape subse-
quent interpretations of ICT and other job demands, perhaps
Conclusions
contributing further to increased stress responses and negative
health consequences over the long term. For example, employ- Employee health has become a central concern to organizations
ees experiencing workplace telepressure might misinterpret am- in their efforts to sustain a competitive edge. Research suggests
biguous cues in an e-mail (“please respond ASAP”) as a need to that a healthy worker is a productive worker, as job stress leads to
respond in a shorter time frame than what was intended by the increased levels of absenteeism, turnover, and even medical ex-
email sender (i.e., the sender meant within the next work day penditures (Cooper & Cartwright, 1994; Goetzel, Guindon, Tur-
rather than immediately or within the hour). As such, workplace shen, Ozminkowski, 2001; Mills, Kessler, Cooper, & Sullivan,
telepressure might skew the perception of the social demands 2007). With the increased concern about employee well-being,
of the work environment toward escalating quick response more flexible work arrangements that rely on technology-mediated
expectations. Subsequent studies might want to examine the communications have become part of many job designs. Despite
specific time course in which workplace telepressure develops the positive health intentions of such flexibility, the increased
in relation to perceptions of work demands, including if these experiences of workplace telepressure among employees who use
effects are reciprocal. Additionally longitudinal research can
technology-based communications for work purposes might actu-
determine the time course in which workplace telepressure has
ally reduce control over work recovery (i.e., leading to an auton-
the most detrimental impact on recovery and health, as well as
omy paradox; Mazmanian et al., 2013). According to the results
how quickly these effects can be reversed.
from these studies, workplace telepressure involves a heightened
Also, future research should examine whether changes in work-
preoccupation and urge to respond quickly to ICT messages. More
place telepressure occur across work situations within the same
workplace telepressure is associated with poor physical and psy-
organization. Workplace telepressure might change over time be-
cause of various workplace characteristics, such as tenure within chological health in employees and is associated with more ICT
an organization or within a given workgroup. Newer employees demands in the work environment. Researchers and practitioners
might be more susceptible to workplace telepressure given their must be prepared to manage workplace telepressure issues among
desire to impress new colleagues with quick responsiveness or employees to ensure that technology-mediated communications
model their behavior around the current workgroup. Understand- yield their intended effects of improving employee autonomy,
ing the long-term trajectories and implications of workplace tele- well-being, and potentially performance. This includes helping
pressure can help organizations to determine whether different employees set boundaries around their technology use to preserve
interventions are required for telepressure-related problems that recovery time (Barber & Jenkins, 2014; Olson-Buchanan & Bo-
are newly emerging as compared with problems that have persisted swell, 2006), including setting organizational norms and policies
undetected for a period of time. around technological responsiveness that allows for sufficient re-
Finally, we had a few significant limitations regarding our covery. Otherwise, employees might become burned out, unfo-
measure of response latency. We had a high nonresponse rate on cused, and ineffective at work because of the burden of continuous
this particular variable; around 68% of the sample indicated that accessibility expectations.
WORKPLACE TELEPRESSURE 187

References comes: The moderating effect of organizational ICT support. Journal of


Occupational Health Psychology, 17, 473– 491. http://dx.doi.org/
Afifi, W. A., & Metts, S. (1998). Characteristics and consequences of 10.1037/a0029837
expectation violations in close relationships. Journal of Social and Day, A., Scott, N., & Kelloway, E. K. (2010). Information and communi-
Personal Relationships, 15(3), 365–392. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ cation technology: Implications for job stress and employee well-being.
0265407598153004
Research in Occupational Stress and Well-being, 8, 317–350. http://dx
Åkerstedt, T., Nilsson, P. M., & Kecklund, G. (2009). Sleep and recovery.
.doi.org/10.1108/S1479-3555(2010)0000008011
Research in Occupational Stress and Well-Being, 7, 205–247.
Demerouti, E., Bakker, A. B., Nachreiner, F., & Schaufeli, W. B. (2001).
Allen, N. J., & Meyer, J. P. (1990). The measurement and antecedents of
The job demands-resources model of burnout. Journal of Applied Psy-
affective, continuance, and normative commitment to the organization.
Journal of Occupational Psychology, 63, 1–18. http://dx.doi.org/ chology, 86, 499 –512. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.86.3.499
10.1111/j.2044-8325.1990.tb00506.x DeVellis, R. F. (2012). Scale development: Theory and applications (3rd
Allen, T. D. (2001). Family-supportive work environments: The role of ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
organizational perceptions. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 58, 414 – Edwards, J. R., & Rothbard, N. P. (1999). Work and family stress and
435. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jvbe.2000.1774 well-being: An examination of person-environment fit in the work and
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

American Psychological Association. (2013). Americans stay connected to family domains. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Pro-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

work on weekends, vacation, and even when out sick. American Psy- cesses, 77, 85–129. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/obhd.1998.2813
chological Association Press Room. Retrieved from http://www.apa Finn, J. (2006). An exploratory study of email use by direct service social
.org/news/press/releases/2013/09/connected-work.aspx workers. Journal of Technology in Human Services, 24, 1–20. http://dx
Aziz, S., & Zickar, M. J. (2006). A cluster analysis investigation of .doi.org/10.1300/J017v24n04_01
workaholism as a syndrome. Journal of Occupational Health Psychol- Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (2010). Predicting and changing behavior: The
ogy, 11, 52– 62. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1076-8998.11.1.52 reasoned action approach. New York: Taylor & Francis.
Bakker, A. B., & Demerouti, E. (2007). The job demands-resources model: Forsyth, D. R. (2009). Group dynamics. In H. T. Reis & S. Sprecher (Eds.),
State of the art. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 22, 309 –328. Encyclopedia of human relationships (pp. 145–148). Thousand Oaks,
http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/02683940710733115 CA: Sage. http://dx.doi.org/10.4135/9781412958479.n248
Barber, L. K., & Jenkins, J. S. (2014). Creating technological boundaries to Gangestad, S. W., & Snyder, M. (2000). Self-monitoring: Appraisal and
protect bedtime: Examining work– home boundary management, psy- reappraisal. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 530 –555. http://dx.doi.org/
chological detachment and sleep. Stress and Health, 30, 259 –264. 10.1037/0033-2909.126.4.530
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/smi.2536
Goetzel, R. Z., Guindon, A. M., Turshen, I. J., & Ozminkowski, R. J.
Barnes, C. M., Wagner, D. T., & Ghumman, S. (2012). Borrowing from
(2001). Health and productivity management: Establishing key perfor-
sleep to pay work and family: Expanding time-based conflict to the
mance measures, benchmarks, and best practices. Journal of Occupa-
broader nonwork domain. Personnel Psychology, 65, 789 – 819. http://
tional and Environmental Medicine, 43, 10 –17. http://dx.doi.org/
dx.doi.org/10.1111/peps.12002
Behrend, T. S., Sharek, D. J., Meade, A. W., & Wiebe, E. N. (2011). The 10.1097/00043764-200101000-00003
viability of crowdsourcing for survey research. Behavior Research Goldberg, L. R. (1999). A broad-bandwidth, public domain, personality
Methods, 43, 800 – 813. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13428-011-0081-0 inventory measuring the lower-level facets of several five-factor models.
Boswell, W. R., & Olson-Buchanan, J. B. (2007). The use of communi- In I. Mervielde, I. Deary, F. De Fruyt, & F. Ostendorf (Eds.), Personality
cation technologies after hours: The role of work attitudes and work-life Psychology in Europe (Vol. 7, pp. 7–28). Tilburg, the Netherlands:
conflict. Journal of Management, 33, 592– 610. http://dx.doi.org/ Tilburg University Press.
10.1177/0149206307302552 Goldberg, L. R., Johnson, J. A., Eber, H. W., Hogan, R., Ashton, M. C.,
Buhrmester, M., Kwang, T., & Gosling, S. D. (2011). Amazon’s Mechan- Cloninger, C. R., & Gough, H. C. (2006). The International Personality
ical Turk: A new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality data? Perspec- Item Pool and the future of public-domain personality measures. Journal
tives on Psychological Science, 6, 3–5. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ of Research in Personality, 40, 84 –96. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp
1745691610393980 .2005.08.007
Cascio, W. F. (2000). Managing a virtual workplace. The Academy of Hallberg, U. E., & Schaufeli, W. B. (2006). “Same same” but different?
Management Executive, 14, 81–90. Can work engagement be discriminated from job involvement and
Christian, M. S., Garza, A. S., & Slaughter, J. E. (2011). Work engage- organizational commitment? European Psychologist, 11, 119 –127.
ment: A quantitative review and test of its relations with task and http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040.11.2.119
contextual performance. Personnel Psychology, 64, 89 –136. http://dx Hinkin, T. R. (1998). A brief tutorial on the development of measures for
.doi.org/10.1111/j.1744-6570.2010.01203.x use in survey questionnaires. Organizational Research Methods, 1,
Cialdini, R. B., Bator, R. J., & Guadagno, R. E. (1999). Normative 104 –121. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/109442819800100106
influences in organizations. In L. Thompson, D. Messick, & J. Levine
Jenkins, C. D., Stanton, B.-A., Niemcryk, S. J., & Rose, R. M. (1988). A
(Eds.), Shared cognitions in organizations: The management of knowl-
scale for the estimation of sleep problems in clinical research. Journal of
edge. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Clinical Epidemiology, 41, 313–321. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0895-
Cialdini, R. B., & Goldstein, N. J. (2004). Social influence: Compliance
4356(88)90138-2
and conformity. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 591– 621. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.142015 Jonason, P. K., & Webster, G. D. (2010). The dirty dozen: A concise
Cialdini, R. B., Kallgren, C. A., & Reno, R. R. (1991). A focus theory of measure of the dark triad. Psychological Assessment, 22, 420 – 432.
normative conduct. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 24, http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0019265
201–234. Kanai, A., & Wakabayashi, M. (2001). Workaholism among Japanese
Cooper, C. L., & Cartwright, S. (1994). Healthy mind, healthy organiza- blue-collar employees. International Journal of Stress Management, 8,
tion—A Proactive approach to occupational stress. Human Relations, 129 –145. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1009529314121
47, 455– 471. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/001872679404700405 Kanungo, R. N. (1982). Measurement of job and work involvement.
Day, A., Paquet, S., Scott, N., & Hambley, L. (2012). Perceived informa- Journal of Applied Psychology, 67, 341–349. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
tion and communication technology (ICT) demands on employee out- 0021-9010.67.3.341
188 BARBER AND SANTUZZI

Karasek, R. A. (1979). Job demands, job decision latitude, and mental tivity. American Journal of Health Promotion, 22, 45–53. http://dx.doi
strain: Implications for job redesign. Administrative Science Quarterly, .org/10.4278/0890-1171-22.1.45
24, 285–308. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/2392498 Mount, M. K., Barrick, M. R., Scullen, S. M., & Rounds, J. (2005).
Kelloway, E. K., & Day, A. L. (2005). Building healthy workplaces: What Higher-order dimensions of the big five personality traits and the big six
we know so far. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 37, 223–235. vocational interest types. Personnel Psychology, 58, 447– 478. http://dx
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0087259 .doi.org/10.1111/j.1744-6570.2005.00468.x
Kessler, R. C., Barber, C., Beck, A., Berglund, P., Cleary, P. D., McKenas, Nunnally, J. C., & Bernstein, I. H. (1994). Psychometric theory. New York:
D., . . . Wang, P. (2003). The World Health Organization Health and McGraw-Hill.
Work Performance Questionnaire (HPQ). Journal of Occupational and Olson-Buchanan, J. B., & Boswell, W. R. (2006). Blurring boundaries:
Environmental Medicine, 45, 156 –174. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/01 Correlates of integration and segmentation between work and nonwork.
.jom.0000052967.43131.51 Journal of Vocational Behavior, 68, 432– 445. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
Koopman, C., Pelletier, K. R., Murray, J. F., Sharda, C. E., Berger, M. L., j.jvb.2005.10.006
Turpin, R. S., . . . Bendel, T. (2002). Stanford presenteeism scale: health Park, Y., Fritz, C., & Jex, S. M. (2011). Relationships between work– home
status and employee productivity. Journal of Occupational and Envi- segmentation and psychological detachment from work: The role of
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ronmental Medicine/American College of Occupational and Environ- communication technology use at home. Journal of Occupational
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

mental Medicine, 44(1), 14 –20. Health Psychology, 16, 457– 467. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0023594
Kossek, E. E., Lautsch, B. A., & Eaton, S. C. (2006). Telecommuting, Park, Y., & Jex, S. M. (2011). Work– home boundary management using
control, and boundary management: Correlates of policy use and prac- communication and information technology. International Journal of
tice, job control, and work–family effectiveness. Journal of Vocational Stress Management, 18, 133–152. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0022759
Behavior, 68, 347–367. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jvb.2005.07.002 Ragu-Nathan, T. S., Tarafdar, M., Ragu-Nathan, B. S., & Tu, Q. (2008).
Kossek, E. E., Ruderman, M. N., Braddy, P. W., & Hannum, K. M. (2012). The consequences of technostress for end users in organizations: Con-
Work-nonwork boundary management profiles: A person-centered ap- ceptual development and empirical validation. Information Systems Re-
proach. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 81, 112–128. http://dx.doi.org/ search, 19(4), 417– 433. http://dx.doi.org/10.1287/isre.1070.0165
10.1016/j.jvb.2012.04.003 Schaufeli, W. B., & Bakker, A. B. (2003). Test manual for the Utrecht
Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Psychological stress and the coping Work Engagement Scale. Unpublished manuscript, Utrecht University,
process. New York, NY: Springer. the Netherlands. Retrieved from http://www.schaufeli.com
Lobel, S., & St. Clair, L. (1992). Effects of family responsibilities, gender, Schaufeli, W. B., & Bakker, A. B. (2004). Job demands, job resources, and
and career identitysalience on performance outcomes. Academy of Man- their relationship with burnout and engagement: A multi-sample study.
agement Journal, 35, 1057–1069. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/256540 Journal of Organizational Behavior, 25, 293–315. http://dx.doi.org/
Markus, M. L. (1994). Electronic mail as the medium of managerial choice. 10.1002/job.248
Organization Science, 5, 502–527. http://dx.doi.org/10.1287/orsc.5.4 Schaufeli, W. B., Bakker, A. B., & Salanova, M. (2006). The measurement
.502 of work engagement with a short questionnaire A cross-national study.
Mastin, D. F., Bryson, J., & Corwyn, R. (2006). Assessment of sleep Educational and Psychological Measurement, 66(4), 701–716. http://dx
hygiene using the Sleep Hygiene Index. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, .doi.org/10.1177/0013164405282471
29(3), 223–227. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10865-006-9047-6 Schaufeli, W. B., Shimazu, A., & Taris, T. W. (2009). Being driven to
Mazmanian, M., Orlikowski, W. J., & Yates, J. (2013). The autonomy work excessively hard: The evaluation of a two-factor measure of
paradox: The implications of mobile email devices for knowledge pro- workaholism in the Netherlands and Japan. Cross-Cultural Research,
fessionals. Organization Science, 24, 1337–1357. http://dx.doi.org/ 43, 320 –348. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1069397109337239
10.1287/orsc.1120.0806 Schaufeli, W. B., Taris, T. W., & Van Rhenen, W. (2008). Workaholism,
McCrae, R. R., & John, O. P. (1992). An introduction to the five-factor burnout, and work engagement: Three of a kind or three different kinds
model and its applications. Journal of Personality, 60(2), 175–215. of employee well-being? Applied Psychology, 57(2), 173–203. http://dx
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1992.tb00970.x .doi.org/10.1111/j.1464-0597.2007.00285.x
McEwen, B. S. (1998). Stress, adaptation, and disease. Allostasis and Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1985). The Self-Consciousness Scale: A
allostatic load. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 840, revised version for use with general populations. Journal of Applied
33– 44. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.1998.tb09546.x Social Psychology, 15, 687– 699. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816
McEwen, B. S., & Stellar, E. (1993). Stress and the individual. Mecha- .1985.tb02268.x
nisms leading to disease. Archives of Internal Medicine, 153(18), 2093– Shirom, A., & Melamed, S. (2006). A comparison of the construct validity
2101. of two burnout measures in two groups of professionals. International
Meijman, T. F., & Mulder, G. (1998). Psychological aspects of workload. Journal of Stress Management, 13, 176 –200. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
In P. J. D. Drenth & H. Thierry (Eds.), Handbook of work and organi- 1072-5245.13.2.176
zational psychology: Work psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 5–33). Hove, Eng- Snyder, M. (1974). Self-monitoring of expressive behavior. Journal of
land: Psychology Press. Personality and Social Psychology, 30(4), 526 –537. http://dx.doi.org/
Meyer, J. P., & Allen, N. J. (1991). A three-component conceptualization 10.1037/h0037039
of organizational commitment. Human Resource Management Review, Society of Human Resource Management. (2012). Technology and its
1, 61– 89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/1053-4822(91)90011-Z impact on employees during nonwork hours. Survey findings. Retrieved
Meyer, J. P., & Allen, N. J. (1997). Commitment in the workplace. from http://www.shrm.org/research/surveyfindings/articles/pages/
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. technologyanditsimpactonemployeesduringnonworkinghours.aspx
Meyer, J. P., Stanley, D. J., Herscovitch, L., & Topolnytsky, L. (2002). Sonnentag, S. (2003). Recovery, work engagement, and proactive behav-
Affective, continuance, and normative commitment to the organization: ior: a new look at the interface between nonwork and work. The Journal
A meta-analysis of antecedents, correlates, and consequences. Journal of of Applied Psychology, 88(3), 518 –528.
Vocational Behavior, 61, 20 –52. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jvbe.2001 Sonnentag, S., Binnewies, C., & Mojza, E. J. (2010). Staying well and
.1842 engaged when demands are high: The role of psychological detach-
Mills, P. R., Kessler, R. C., Cooper, J., & Sullivan, S. (2007). Impact of a ment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95, 965–976. http://dx.doi.org/
health promotion program on employee health risks and work produc- 10.1037/a0020032
WORKPLACE TELEPRESSURE 189

Sonnentag, S., & Fritz, C. (2007). The Recovery Experience Question- Townsend, A. M., DeMarie, S. M., & Hendrickson, A. R. (1998).
naire: Development and validation of a measure for assessing recu- Virtual teams: Technology and the workplace of the future. The
peration and unwinding from work. Journal of Occupational Health Academy of Management Executive, 12, 17–29.
Psychology, 12, 204 –221. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1076-8998.12.3 Valcour, M. (2007). Work-based resources as moderators of the rela-
.204 tionship between work hours and satisfaction with work-family bal-
Spector, P. E., & Jex, S. M. (1998). Development of four self-report ance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 1512–1523.
measures of job stressors and strain: Interpersonal Conflict at Work Van den Broeck, A., Schreurs, B., De Witte, H., Vansteenkiste, M.,
Scale, Organizational Constraints Scale, Quantitative Workload In- Germeys, F., & Schaufeli, W. (2011). Understanding workaholics’
ventory, and Physical Symptoms Inventory. Journal of Occupational motivations: A self-determination perspective. Applied Psychology:
Health Psychology, 3(4), 356 –367. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1076- An International Review, 60, 600 – 621. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j
8998.3.4.356 .1464-0597.2011.00449.x
Thompson, C. A., Beauvais, L. L., & Lyness, K. S. (1999). When VW turns off out-of-hours email. (2011, December). BBC News. Re-
work–family benefits are not enough: The influence of work–family trieved from http://www.bbc.co.uk/news/technology-16314901
culture on benefit utilization, organizational attachment, and work– Webster, D. M., & Kruglanski, A. W. (1994). Individual differences in
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

family conflict. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 54, 392–415. http://dx.doi need for cognitive closure. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
.org/10.1006/jvbe.1998.1681 chology, 67, 1049 –1062.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Appendix
Workplace Telepressure Measure

Instructions 5. I can’t stop thinking about a message until I’ve


responded. (Preoccupation)
For the following questions, think about how you use technol-
ogy to communicate with people in your workplace. Specifically 6. I feel a strong need to respond to others immediately.
think about message-based technologies that allow you to control (Urge)
when you respond (email, text messages, voicemail, etc.). Please
rate how much you agree or disagree with the statements. 7. I have an overwhelming feeling to respond right at
When using message-based technology for work purposes . . . that moment when I receive a request from someone.
(Urge)
1. I’m concerned about keeping fast response times. (Pre-
occupation) 8. It’s difficult for me to resist responding to a message
right away. (Urge)
2. I often think about how I need to respond more quickly.
(Preoccupation) ⴱ
Bold items are included in final revised measure.
3. It’s hard for me to focus on other things when I
receive a message from someone. (Preoccupation)
Received November 26, 2013
4. I can concentrate better on other tasks once I’ve Revision received September 8, 2014
responded to my messages. (Preoccupation) Accepted September 15, 2014 䡲

You might also like