You are on page 1of 13

Tatiana Pasternak1-3 and William H.

Merigan"
Motion Perception following
Lesions of the Superior Temporal Departments of "Neurobiology and Anatomy, and
2
Sulcus in the Monkey Ophthalmology, and 'Center for Visual Science,
University of Rochester, Rochester, New York 14642

We examined the effect of bilateral ibotenic acid lesions, Physiological properties of neurons in cortical areas

Downloaded from http://cercor.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of Bath Library & Learning Centre on July 7, 2015
aimed at areas MT/MST in three macaques, on their MT and MST of the macaque monkey suggest an im-
perception of motion. The medial boundary of the le- portant role of these areas in the processing of visual
sions in the three monkeys was near the dorsal end of motion signals. Neurons in area MT have large re-
the STS, but the lesions extended different lengths ven- ceptive fields, many are directionally selective, and
tralry along the STS. The lesions extended the shortest some respond to an integrated direction of motion
distance ventrally in monkeys 1 and 2, covering most when stimulated with the motion of multiple, ori-
of MST but possibly sparing a portion of lateral MT. ented moving patterns (Zeki, 1974; Maunsell and Van
That in monkey 3 damaged all of MT and MST bilaterally Essen, 1983; Albright, 1984; Movshon et al., 1985;
and extended through most of FST. All three lesions Rodman and Albright, 1989). In addition, recent phys-
caused a temporary disruption, followed by at least iological evidence (Newsome et al., 1989; Britten et
partial recovery, of most motion thresholds. Permanent al., 1992) indicates that the sensitivity of MT neurons
effects of the lesions on visual sensitivity were graded to visual motion is similar to that of the psychophys-
with lesion extent Contrast sensitivity for detecting ically tested monkey. Additional evidence that MT
low-spatial-frequency (1 cycle/degree) drifting gratings may be important for motion perception is the dem-
over a wide range of drift rates, as well as for identifying onstration that microstimulation of small numbers of
their direction of motion, was slightly affected only in MT neurons can alter the visual choices of a monkey
monkey 3. Only monkeys 2 and 3 showed a deficit in toward the direction to which the stimulated neurons
discriminating stimulus speed, and the size of the loss are most sensitive (Salzman et al., 1992). Similarly,
was two- to fourfold. Discrimination of opposite direc- recordings from the adjacent area, MST, also suggest
tions of dot pattern motion, which required integration a role in motion processing, specifically in the analysis
of local motion signals, was mildly affected in monkeys of object motion and optic flow (Duffy and Wurtz,
2 and 3, and not affected in monkey 1. However, ad- 1991; Tanaka et al., 1993) and in pursuit eye move-
dition of directional noise to this discrimination caused ments (Komatsu and Wurtz, 1988).
the performance of all monkeys to be permanently dis- Lesions of MT and MST affect motion perception
rupted, especially that of monkeys 2 and 3. Finally, in a way that suggests a central role for these areas.
direction difference thresholds were elevated by a factor Lesions of areas MT (Newsome et al., 1985) and MST
of 2-4 after the lesions in all three monkeys. Many of (Dursteller and Wurtz, 1988) resulted in deficits in
these deficits were more pronounced during the first 2 smooth pursuit eye movements that suggested im-
months of testing following the lesion. paired perception of the velocity of motion, although
Thus, our results demonstrate that areas within dor- these effects were transient and recovered within a
sal STS make an important contribution to the perfor- week (Yamasaki and Wurtz, 1991). Newsome and Pare
mance of various motion perception tasks including the (1988) used dynamic random dots to examine the
discrimination of smaD differences in direction and speed, discrimination of motion direction. They reported def-
and the perception of global motion in the presence of icits in thresholds for discriminating opposite direc-
directional noise. The residual motion perception, even tions of motion that were severe on the day of the
in the monkey with virtually complete removal of areas lesion, but largely recovered after a week. When the
MT/MST, may suggest either that these tasks are nor- lesion was larger, and included both MT and a large
mally mediated in part by cortical areas outside of areas portion of MST, there was a twofold threshold ele-
MT and MST, or that the disrupted functions were par- vation, which appeared to be permanent.
tially assumed by other cortical areas after lesions. While the above work suggests some role for MT/
MST in motion perception, the limited and largely
transient deficits following lesions of these areas
question how essential they are. The basis for this
rapid recovery of function is an important and as yet
unresolved question. Related questions that formed
the basis of the present study are the possibilities (1)
that other motion-related capacities might be sub-
stantially and permanently affected by MT lesions and

Cerebral Cortex May/June 1994;4.247-259; 1O47-3211/94/M.OO


(2) that extensive lesions involving both MT and MST rect stimulus. Incorrect responses resulted in a 6 sec
would have even more dramatic effects on some mo- tone and no juice reward. To eliminate position bias,
tion-related thresholds. a correction procedure was used: three consecutive
incorrect responses on the same side resulted in a
trial being repeated until the animal made a correct
Materials and Methods response. Data from correction trials were discarded.
Responses were effective only after the stimulus was
Subjects present for 2 sec. In the detection task, the monkeys
Three adult monkeys were used. On weekdays, water viewed a uniform field and a moving grating of the
was restricted for a period of 23 hr before testing and same mean luminance, and were rewarded for choos-
they received their daily water ration, in the form of ing the target containing the grating. In the direction
fruit juice, during behavioral testing. On weekends, discrimination task, they viewed two identical grat-
the monkeys were not tested behaviorally and had ings moving at the same speed in opposite direction,
unrestricted access to water. Food was continually and chose the grating that moved rightward. In the
speed discrimination task, the monkeys were pre-

Downloaded from http://cercor.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of Bath Library & Learning Centre on July 7, 2015
available in the home cage, and their body weights
were monitored daily. The three monkeys (with orig- sented with two gratings of the same spatial frequency
inal numbers in parentheses) were labeled 1 (047), moving in the same direction at two different rates.
2 (854), and 3 (9103) in order of the completeness They were rewarded for choosing the target that moved
of their MT/MST lesions to facilitate clarity of de- faster.
scription of their behavioral deficits. Initially the monkeys were trained to detect and
to discriminate the direction of motion of a high-
Stimuli and Behavioral Procedures contrast, 1 cycle/degree grating, drifting at 2.24 Hz.
After they acquired both tasks (criterion: three con-
Grating Stimuli secutive 200 trial sessions >90% correct, or four con-
Stimuli. Stimuli and behavioral procedures were sim- secutive sessions >80% correct), they were trained
ilar to those described in Pasternak and Leinen (1986). to discriminate differences in speed. Thresholds were
The stimuli were sinusoidal, moving or stationary ver- measured with a staircase procedure in which three
tical gratings that were generated with an Innisfree consecutive correct responses resulted in a decrease
stimulus generator on a Hewlett-Packard 1332a os- of contrast (or for speed thresholds, a decrease in the
cilloscope screen (P-31 phosphor). The gratings ap- speed of the comparison stimulus), while each in-
peared behind two 6° diameter circular openings in correct response resulted in an increase of contrast
a white surround of the same mean luminance (75 or the difference in speed between the two compar-
cd/m 2 ). All gratings had a slow, smoothed onset (200 ison stimuli. A psychometric function was constructed
msec raised cosine), and they remained on until the from the results of all trials, and contrast and speed
first response after 2 sec. Contrast sensitivity functions thresholds were calculated by interpolation at the
for detecting gratings and discriminating their direc- stimulus value consistent with 75% correct. Contrast
tion of motion were measured for 1 cycle/degree grat- thresholds were converted into contrast sensitivity by:
ings at temporal frequencies from 2.24 Hz to 19 Hz. I/contrast threshold x 100, while speed thresholds
Difference thresholds for speed were measured over were converted into Weber fractions: increment in
a range of stimulus contrasts, with grating contrast speed/base speed (AV/V). Each testing session con-
defined as ( £ m - £„,„)/(£„„ + Z ^ ) , where L^ and sisted of 200 trials, and the monkeys were tested in
Zmta are the maximum and minimum luminance in the two consecutive sessions. Thus, contrast sensitivities
grating. The mean contrast values were 1.5%, 3%, 6%, for detection and direction discrimination for a given
12%, 24%, and 48%. To eliminate cues due to differ- stimulus were usually measured on the same day.
ences in apparent contrast between the standard and Spatial frequency discrimination. This function
comparison gratings, contrast was randomly varied was measured using stimuli identical to those used
±15% around the mean for each trial, except at the in other discriminations to see if the effects of MT/
highest contrast level (48%), where a single contrast MST lesions were specific to motion tasks. This could
value was used. Finally, spatial frequency difference be determined by comparing the smallest discrimin-
thresholds were measured with the same stimuli. All able difference in spatial frequency measured here to
measurements were performed at a distance of 35 cm. the smallest discriminable difference in speed mea-
The grating stimuli usually remained on until the sured in the motion tasks. In this task the monkey
response was made. discriminated between two stationary gratings of
Contrast sensitivity and speed discrimination. The identical contrast, that differed only in spatial fre-
monkeys were trained to perform three different tasks: quency. A base spatial frequency of 1 cycle/degree
to detect the presence of a moving grating, to dis- was always compared with a higher spatial frequency,
criminate its direction of motion, and to discriminate and the monkey was rewarded for responding to the
small differences in the speed of moving gratings. The higher frequency. Temporal onset of the gratings fol-
behavioral procedure was identical for all three tasks. lowed a cosine envelope of 200 msec rise time. A
The monkeys viewed the comparison stimuli binoc- staircase procedure, identical to that described above
ularly, and were rewarded for pushing one of two for speed discrimination, was used and threshold was
buttons that was located on the same side as the cor- taken at a value corresponding to 75% correct per-

248 Effects of MT/MST Lesions on Motion Thresholds • Pasternak and Merigan


formance. Measurements were performed at two con- continued testing is common, a large number of
trast levels: 12% and 50%. threshold determinations was performed until there
was no indication of further improvement. To reach
Dynamic Random Dot Stimuli stable performance it was often necessary to repeat
Stimuli. Dynamic random dot targets, used in the the testing 4-10 times. At least four threshold deter-
present study, were identical to those used recently minations contributed to the mean used as a baseline
by Pasternak et al. (1990). The stimuli, first intro- threshold estimate at each stimulus parameter. In all
duced by Williams and Sekuler (1984), consisted of tasks, the sequence of testing at each contrast level
dots repeatedly displaced with a direction of motion (speed discrimination), temporal frequency (contrast
chosen randomly from a uniform distribution of di- thresholds), range, step size, and so on, was irregular,
rections. The speed with which the dots were dis- with interleaving of different conditions.
placed was determined by the step size (Ax) and the
temporal interval between steps (At), and was either Postoperative Testing
5 or 15 degrees/sec. When the range of the distri- Since the present studies were designed to examine

Downloaded from http://cercor.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of Bath Library & Learning Centre on July 7, 2015
bution of directions was 360°, there was only local the permanent effects of lesions, rather than transient
random motion of individual dots, and no coherent deficits that occur immediately after the lesions (e.g.,
motion could be seen. However, when the distribu- Newsome and Pare, 1988), postoperative testing be-
tion was less than about 320-340°, the dots appeared gan between 3 and 7 d after the lesions and continued
to flow coherently in the direction of the mean of the over a period of 6 months. Nonetheless, in all three
distribution. The saliency of the stimulus was manip- animals we were able to assess the extent of early
ulated by varying the range of the direction distri- deficits on some of the tasks. The sequence of testing
bution (direction range) and/or the proportion of the following the lesions was as follows. In monkey 1,
dots moving in nonrandom directions (% motion sig- single threshold determinations were performed for
nal). all tasks during the first 2 weeks of the postoperative
The dot stimuli were displayed for 2 sec on two behavioral testing. After that, systematic measure-
adjacent regions of the screen of a Tektronix 606 os- ments of each visual function resumed. In all mon-
cilloscope (P-31 phosphor), placed at a distance of keys, we made at least three to five measurements of
35 cm from the observer, behind two immediately speed, contrast, direction, and range thresholds. In
adjacent 7° diameter circular openings in a white sur- cases when animals were unable to perform discrim-
round. Each stimulus contained 50 dots, the dot den- inations, attempts to measure thresholds were re-
sity in all experiments was 0.94 dots/degree 2 , and the peated several times. When there was any sign of im-
mean luminance of the display was 0.1 cd/m 2 . Each provement, the monkeys received additional testing
dot was 0.08° in diameter and its luminance was set to determine at what level performance would sta-
3.5 log units above detection threshold for human bilize. In monkey 2, range thresholds were measured
observers. Under these conditions there was no in- first, followed by contrast sensitivity testing at a single
dication of streaks produced by moving dots. temporal frequency (9 Hz), followed by direction dif-
ference threshold testing and speed discriminations.
Tasks. The monkeys were trained on two tasks in-
After these initial measurements, the monkey re-
volving random dots: discrimination of opposite di-
ceived extensive systematic testing on all tasks. In
rections, and discrimination of small differences in
monkey 3, contrast thresholds for detecting and dis-
direction. Thefirsttask was identical to that used with
criminating direction over a range of temporal fre-
gratings. The monkeys viewed two stimuli consisting
quencies were measured first. This was followed by
of rightward and leftward moving dots, and were re-
measurements of speed discriminations, and finally
warded for choosing the rightward motion. The
by range and direction thresholds for random dots.
broadest range of directions in the stimulus that al-
The performance on all tasks stabilized after the first
lowed the animals to perform the discrimination at
2 months of testing. Thresholds for all tasks were
75% correct was determined at several levels of mo-
rechecked during the last month of the experiment,
tion signal: 100%, 50%, 25%, 12%, and 6%. In addition,
6 months after the lesion.
a threshold for the limit of motion signal was mea-
sured with direction range held at 0°. In the second
task, one stimulus moved to the right and the other Ibotenic Acid Lesions
downward but less than 90° from rightward, the mon- Lesions were made by injecting ibotenic acid (10 /ig/
keys were rewarded for choosing rightward motion, lil) at multiple sites along the superior temporal sul-
and the minimal discriminable difference in direction cus (STS) in both hemispheres. Injections began near
was measured. Direction difference thresholds were the dorsal end of the STS and continued along the
measured for the following direction ranges: 0°, 90°, STS for about 8 mm. Individual injections were made
180°, and 270°. Other procedural details were iden- with about 2.5 mm separation anterior and posterior
tical to those described above for gratings. to the lumen of the STS along its dorsalmost 8 mm.
Injections were made with a Hamilton syringe and
Preoperative Testing their location based on direct visualization of the STS.
Preoperative testing was performed over a period of In monkey 1, 106 and 118 injections of 1.5 MI (total,
3-6 months for different monkeys. Since during the 3360 ng) were placed in the left and right hemi-
initial data collection improvement in thresholds with spheres, respectively. In monkey 2, 73 and 74 injec-

Cerebral Conex Mayflune 1994, V 4 N 3 249


Downloaded from http://cercor.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of Bath Library & Learning Centre on July 7, 2015
Rgore 1 . Representative Kstotogical section from which the intenic acid lesions were reconstructed. This is a photograph of a 40-jim-thick horizontal section of the brain of
monkey 1 stained for CO to illustrate the lesion location. In tfw photograph up is lateral and leh is anterior. The locations of three suld are indicated: lunate [LS]. superior
temporal [STS), and mtraparieal [IP). The lesion a located along both banks of the STS. Scale bar, 1 mm.

tions of 1.5 Ml (total, 2205 Mg) were made in the left sition to normal-appearing neuropil at the lesion edg-
and right hemispheres, and in monkey 3, 96 and 112 es as shown in Figure 1. This lesion was complete
injections of 1.5 MI (total, 3120 tig). Due to the length along both anterior and posterior banks of the STS,
of the injection procedure, the lesions in each hemi- except for a small region in which deeper layers of
sphere were made in separate surgeries a week apart, cortex survived. In instances when the lesion termi-
during which time visual thresholds were measured nated with a sulcus, we often found a small region of
for monkeys 1 and 3 to determine the effect of the damage to only superficial layers bordering the full
surgery itself (plus the unilateral lesion) on thresh- thickness lesion.
olds. The location and extent of complete and partial
lesions are illustrated on flattened 2-D maps of the
Histology cortical surface in Figure 2. Lesions were reconstruct-
At the conclusion of behavioral testing, each animal ed primarily from the cytochrome oxidase (CO) sec-
was killed with an overdose of barbiturate and per- tions, which clearly showed the extent of neuron loss.
fused with a phosphate buffer rinse followed by 4% Comparison with cresyl violet and myelin stains
paraformaldehyde fixative. A second rinse was used showed thinning of the cortical surface at lesion sites,
to removed excess fixative. The brain was removed, with substantial gliosis but no apparent damage to
blocked, and sectioned at a thickness of 32 or 40 pm. fibers. The lesions are shown, as in previous studies
Triplets of adjacent sections at intervals of 0.25 or 0.50 (e.g., Newsome and Pare, 1988), in relation to the
mm were stained for cytochrome oxidase (Wong-Rlley, borders and fundus of the STS. Also shown is the
1979), with cresyl violet, and for myelinated fibers secondary fundus of the STS, which is a reliable in-
(Gallyas, 1979). dicator of the location of lateral (central) MT (Van
Essen et al., 1981). Small regions along the lateral
Results border of the lesions in monkeys 1 and 2 showed the
myeloarchitecture that is characteristic of area MT in
Lesion Reconstruction unlesioned monkeys (Van Essen et al., 1981; Unger-
A representative histological section through one le- leider and Desimone, 1986), and these areas are shown
sion is shown in Figure 1. Lesions usually produced by solid lines in Figure 2.
a very uniform loss of neurons with an abrupt tran- Lesions in all three monkeys began near the medial

2(0 Effects of MT/MST Lesions on Motion Thresholds • PasternaV and Merigan


end of the STS but had a variable anterior-posterior Monkey
extent. The lesion in monkey 1 appears to have dam- 1
aged all of dorsal MT and MST, but to have partially
spared approximately one-fourth of ventral MT (cor-
responding to approximately the central 4° of the vi-
sual 6eld), as well as a portion of ventral MST. As
indicated by the shading, damage to superficial layers
extended to the estimated border of MT. The lesion
in monkey 2 again damaged all of dorsal MT and MST,
and extended slightly farther ventral than the lesion
in monkey 1, as can be seen from the length of the
secondary fundus of the STS (dotted line to the left
of the primary fundus of the STS) that was spared. A
small portion of the most ventral edge of MT was
partially spared (deeper layers) on both sides in this

Downloaded from http://cercor.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of Bath Library & Learning Centre on July 7, 2015
monkey, as well as part of ventral MST. This monkey
also had small lesions in the left hemisphere near the
anectant gyrus and in the interparietal sulcus. In mon-
key 3, the lesions in both hemispheres appear to cover
all of MT and MST, as well as a substantial part of FST.
There are also small lesions near the anectant gyrus
and in ventral cortex in the inferior occipital sulcus.
Thus, all three lesions involved large and slightly dif-
ferent portions of MT and MST.

Contrast Sensitivity: Detection and


Direction Discrimination
Preoperative (open circles) and stable postoperative
(solid circles and triangles) contrast sensitivity for
drifting gratings as a function of temporal frequency
is shown in Figure 3. Data collected shortly after the
lesion ("early") are shown by dashed-dotted lines
and will be described at the end of Results. The stable
postoperative data were collected during a period of Figure 2. Reconstnjctkm of lesions on a flattened representatm of visual cortex
about 4 weeks, several months after the lesion. Mon- (Van Essen and Maunseil, 1980). an approach that allows 3 f l information about
keys 1 and 2 showed no permanent deficit on either cortex, obtained from histtriogcat seaiore. to be displayed on a 2-0 map. The senrnds
task. The slightly higher postlesion sensitivity for some shown to the left of each conical map shows the location of the foveal portion of
V I . which contacts the rest of cortex at atong the borders, when are shown separated
conditions was most likely due to a gradual increase m this figure only IH*TITP of Qeometncal constrains. The rtoht hemisphere is shown
in sensitivity during the course of testing. Monkey 3 as h would appear in the brain, with righi bang approximately anterior. The left
showed a small sensitivity loss (less than twofold) for hemuphere is shown mirror reversed for ease of comparison. Dotted lines show the
detection, and a somewhat larger loss for direction location of the fundus of suta, and three of these, the inferior occtprtal [IDS), the
discrimination at all temporal frequencies. superior temporal [STS), and the tunate [LS], are labeled. The secondary fundus of
the STS is slightly posterior (left) of the primary fundus, and its ventral end can be
In addition to the measures for drifting gratings seen in monkeys 1 and 2. Area MT a located on the posterior bank of the STS, with
shown here, in monkey 3 we also measured contrast its center close to the dorsal limit of the secondary sulcus of the STS. Regions with
sensitivity for a 9 Hz counterphasemodulated grating the characteristic rrryeioarchitecture of MT that extended beyond complete lesions were
present only in monkeys 1 and 2 and are shown by a thm fm. Regions in which aO
of 1 cycle/degree spatial frequency. For this stimulus, layers of cortex were damaged are shown in A t a t white areas with the loss of only
postlesion contrast sensitivity remained 0.28 log units superficial cortical layers are shown by a tnken tkK, The plane ol section was
below that for a drifting grating. A similar greater horoontal for rmnkeys 1 and 2 and sagittal for monkey 3.
sensitivity for drifting than flickering gratings is com-
monly found in normal observers (Levinson and Sek-
uler, 1975; Pasternak, 1986). olds of the two monkeys with more extensive damage
to these areas were elevated about two- to fourfold,
Speed Discrimination although thresholds remained independent of con-
Pre- and postoperative speed discrimination are shown trast. While these two monkeys showed a reliable el-
as Weber fractions (speed increment/base speed) in evation of thresholds, they were still able to discrim-
Figure 4. Preoperatively, the monkeys could discrim- inate differences in speed of less than 40%.
inate 5-10% differences in speed over a broad range In these experiments variation in stimulus speed
of grating contrasts. Their performance was less ac- is confounded with variation in temporal frequency,
curate only at the lowest grating contrast (1.5%). Mon- making it unclear which stimulus feature was dis-
key 1, which had the most substantial sparing of por- criminated. To assess the possibility that monkey 1
tions of MT and MST, showed no permanent elevation discriminated temporal frequency rather than speed,
of speed threshold after the lesions, while the thresh- we compared the above discrimination to the dis-

Cerebral C o n e i May/June 1994, V 4 N 3 Ml


Detection Direction Discrimination
:
1 c/deg j ^ Jarion 1 c/deg
300 300

100 100

30 ^ 'early 30

10 *»" 10 .^early

3 3
Monkey 1 •*•
Monkey 1

Downloaded from http://cercor.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of Bath Library & Learning Centre on July 7, 2015
300 . lesion 300 lesion
•—"?
C/3 100 • intact 100
^ ^
early >j
a 30 30 A early
00
.4—>
10 • 10
8C 3 3
Monkey 2 Monkey 2
O
u
300
300

100 100

30 30

10 • 10
-early /'
3 3
Monkey 3 Monkey 3
10 20 10 20

Temporal Frequency (Hz)


Figure 3. Effects of MT/MST lestons on contrast sensitivity functions for detection [left) and direction discrimination [right). Preoperative (moo. open symbols) and postoperative
(teen) pufoimance is shown as a function of temporal frequency (drift rate) for the three monkeys. Thresholds measured 3-6 months after the bilateral lesions are shown with
solid syntols and dashed fines. Each date point e based on at least three threshold determinations. Error bars are ± 1 SEM. Where not w i d e , SEMs are smaSer than symbols.
Open tnatfes indicate single thresholds measured after the l i t e r a l injection of iboienit add. DaslHloned tines show the performance measured shortly (3-7 d) after the lesion
was placed.

crimination of temporal frequency using a counter- sults in Figure 6 show that the lesion had no reliable
phase flickering grating of the same spatial and tem- effect on spatial frequency discrimination at either of
poral frequency. These results are shown in Figure 5. the tested contrasts. Thresholds before and after the
Both before and after the lesion this monkey was less lesion were on the order of 6% irrespective of grating
accurate in discriminating differences in flicker rate contrast. Thus, although this monkey showed a three-
than differences in speed. Thus, it is unlikely that the fold loss in accuracy in the discrimination of small
speed discrimination was performed on the basis of differences in direction (see below), it showed no
apparent flicker rate. deficit in the discrimination of spatial frequency dif-
ferences.
Spatial Frequency Discrimination
Thresholds for discriminating differences in spatial Dynamic Random Dots: Discrimination of
frequency were examined with stationary gratings in Opposite Directions
monkey 1 to determine if deficits resulting from MT/ Preoperative and postoperative range thresholds, that
MST lesions were limited to motion thresholds. Re- is, the broadest range of directions in a dot display

262 Effects of MT/MST Lesions on Motion Thresholds • Pasternak and Merigan


Monkey 1
0.6
lc/dcg Intact Lesion
2.24 Hz
8"
3- 03
0.4
1"
I"
0.2 10 50 2 10 50

Grating Contrast (%)

intact R g u r a 6 . A companion of difference thresholds for speed and temporal frequency


Monkey 1 for monkey 1. Speed dierence thresholds were measured relative to a 1 cycle/degree
grating dnfting at 2.24 H2 (2.24 degrees/sec), whde temporal frequency difference
thresholds were measured in comparison wnh a 1 cycle/degree grating flickering in

Downloaded from http://cercor.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of Bath Library & Learning Centre on July 7, 2015
> U.8
counterphase at 2 24 H L Prenperatrve performance [ m a . open symbols) a shown
on the left while postoperative performance {lesion, solid symbols) is shown on the
<
right Both before and after the taston the monkey was less accurate m discnmnating

II 0.6

leskm
tempers! frequency differences than in discriminating differences m speed.

0.4
discriminate motion direction in the presence of sub-
Q stantial stimulus noise. However, in the absence of
0.2 noise (larger motion signal), the residual perfor-
"8 Monkey 2
mance on the direction task was only slightly below
normal, suggesting that the ability to integrate local
CO
motion signals was largely preserved after the lesion.
0.6 r

Dynamic Random Dots: Discrimination of


Small Differences in Direction
0.4 Direction difference thresholds were measured at two
stimulus speeds, 5 and 15 degrees/sec, as a function
of the range of directions in the stimulus. Preopera-
tively, the accuracy of direction discrimination de-
0.2 creased with an increasing range of directions, but
increased with stimulus speed. For example, at 5 de-
intact
Monkey 3
1 10 50
Spatial Frequency Discrimination

Contrast (%) 0.08 I c/deg


C stationary
R g u r a 4 . Effects of MT/MST lesions on speed discriminations. Preoperative [open (/a
symbols) and postoperative [solid symbols) speed discnmmatcn thresholds are shown 2
as a function of paling contrast It? the three monkeys. Ossh-dotted lines for monkeys
1 and 3 and a solid trimgfe for monkey 2 show the performance measured 7 or 30 -O 0.06
d after the bilateral lesion. o
VI

2 0.04
for which opposite directions of global motion could
s:
be discriminated, are shown in Figure 7. When all the 0)
dots in the distribution were directionally biased c
(100% signal), range thresholds reached 330-340°. 0.02
The thresholds decreased with percentage of signal
a
.0
and the monkeys were unable to perform the task
when the motion signal dropped below about 5%
(i.e., over 95% of dots moved in random directions).
The postoperative performance of all monkeys was
affected by the lesion, and the effect was greatest at
Grating Contrast (%)
low motion signal. While this effect was most pro- 6. The effect of MT/MST lesions on spatial frequency (fiscrrminatjon. Pre-
nounced in monkeys 2 and 3, there was a small deficit operative [light tars, BUsct) and postoperative (flEsDr bffs, lesion) difference thresholds
are shown for the spatial frequency of a stationary grating. Base spatial frequency
even in monkey 1, which may have had partial sparing was 1 cycle/degree. Each 4 M point represents at least three threshold detaminaiions.
of the central representation of MT. Thus, lesions of Error bars are ± 1 SEM. The thresholds were measured at two contrail revets. 5 1 %
areas MT and MST permanently affected the ability to and 1256-

Cerebral Cortei May/June 1994, V 4 N 3


360 ' 5 deg/sec 5 deg/sec

270
V
intactyj^
180 00
• early u
2-
90 / Aesion .
o Monkey 1 s>e 15 deg/sec

0 d I L 90

Monkey 1
10
360

Downloaded from http://cercor.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of Bath Library & Learning Centre on July 7, 2015
270 Monkey 2 Monkey 2
intact

180

90 10

0
Monkey 2 Monkey 3 Monkey 3

90 180 270 90 180 270

360 Direction Range (deg)


intact Direction difference thresholds as a ftnction of the range of directors in
270 the stimulus. The direction range ts 0° when all dots in the display move in parallel
in a smgte direction. The direction range is 180° when the dens are allowed to move
in directions ± 9 0 ° from rightward motion. Error bars are ± 1 SEM. Each data poim
180 includes at least three threshold deterrrinauons. The data labeled exiy for monkey 1
and connected by a dohdashed line are thresholds measured 14 d after the bilateral
lesions were placed. The open triangle indicates the direction threshold measured
within days of placement of the unilateral lesion. Speeds. 5 degrees/sec (Ar - 30
90 /'early msec; to - 0.15°) or 15 degrees/sec (Ar = 15 msec, to - 0225°); 100%
signal, task, discrimination of small direction differences (see inset).
0
Monkey 3
factor of 2-3, and this effect appeared to be largely
10 20 50 100 independent of the range of directions in the target,
except for monkey 1, which showed no deficit for the
Motion Signal (%) stimulus containing the broadest range of directions
(270°).
Rgnra 7 . Effects of MT/MST lesons on the abihy to integrate local directional
signals. Preopsratrve [imsa, open symbols) and postoperative [lesion, solid symbols) The above data were obtained with stimuli in which
range thresholds as a function of motion signal are shown for the three monkeys. The dot displacement was 0.15°. Since the maximal dis-
data labeled lesion were collected 3-6 months after placing the brtatera) STS lesion placement that allows the perception of motion, Dnal,
8Rd are means of ai teas three threshold determinations. Open trusses m the data has been used as a measure of the spatial scale of
for monkeys 1 and 3 show single thresholds measured within days of placement of
the unilateral lesion. The data shown by the doutashed a m (labeled esrty] are
motion mechanisms (Cleary and Braddick, 1990; Mor-
thresholds measured within the first 7 d (monkey 2). 14 d (monkey 1), and 45 d gan, 1992), we examined the effect of the size of
(monkey 3) following the lesion. Each data poim on these curves represents a single spatial displacement on motion thresholds in two
threshold determination. Speed, 5 degrees/sec (Ar = 30 msec to •=• 0.151; task, monkeys (Fig. 9). The temporal interval between dot
discrimination between rignrward and leftward motion (see inset). Prims at 0° range displacements was adjusted to maintain a constant
were measured by varying the proportion of randomly moving dots while keeping the
"s^jnaJ" dots at 0° range. speed of 5 degrees/sec. Before the lesion, direction
thresholds were greater for larger step sizes, and for
steps greater than 0.6-0.8° the monkey was unable to
perform the task. After the lesion, the variation of
grees/sec monkey 2 discriminated at 10° difference range threshold with step size was less pronounced,
in direction, while at 15 degrees/sec, thresholds im- resulting in relatively normal performance at larger
proved to about 5° (see Fig. 8). For all monkeys le- step sizes. The maximal step size for which a direction
sions of MT/MST permanently reduced the precision of 90° could be discriminated was 0.67-0.72°, which
with which they judged target direction by about a was at least as large as the prelesion maximum.

IU Effects of MT/MST Lesions on Motion Thresholds • Pasternak and Merigan


Transient Effects -a
Dot-dashed lines in all of the above figures show 5deg/sec
thresholds measured relatively shortly after the le-
sions. These data do not have the same reliability as
1 *>
the stable postlesion data, since the experiments were 30
not designed to examine short-term effects, and be- 8
cause early threshold measures were usually not re-
I
10
peatedly tested until they were stable. Moreover, the
early threshold measures had to be limited to a small
S
number of tests, and they were not performed in all
three animals. Nonetheless, these data, especially the
§
•a
Monkey 1
0.2 0.4
,§©,
0.6 0.8 1
Monkey 3
0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8
more complete early data available for monkey 1, give
Q Step Size (deg)
a clear impression of dramatic disruptions in thresh-
olds shortly after the lesions, with a gradual recovery Figure 9. Pro- snd postoperative direction difference thresholds as a function of the
A x (step sizel of individual dots for two of the monkeys. The direction thresholds

Downloaded from http://cercor.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of Bath Library & Learning Centre on July 7, 2015
of at least partial prelesion sensitivity. were measured at 5 degrees/sec, whrii was kept constam by adjusting the temporal
nterval. The largest step sue on each graph is the maximal spatial displacement
Contrast Sensitivity ( 0 _ ) , when was determined by varying the step see (and temporal interval) end
In all three animals, measurements of sensitivity for asking the monkeys to discnrraraie between nghtward and downward moving stimuli
detection and direction discrimination revealed some |90° difference m direction) The earr> postoperative data were collected 14 d after
the bilateral festoa Direction range, &. 100% signal.
transient depression of sensitivity. The most complete
data were obtained from monkey 1, for whom we were
able to measure complete functions for detection and
discrimination during the first 6 d after the lesions by less than 100°. The deficit in monkey 1 was less pro-
making determinations of detection and direction nounced, with range thresholds reduced to about 270°.
thresholds each day. Loss was more pronounced for With lower-percentage signal, early losses were even
direction discrimination than detection, suggesting more pronounced, and all monkeys were unable to
that these early transient effects were specific either perform the task with less than 20% motion signal.
to motion mechanisms or to the greater demands of With repeated testing (or the passage of time), per-
discrimination than detection tests. Interestingly, the formance at high levels of motion signal (50% and
pronounced effects at lower temporal frequencies de- 100% signal) improved nearly to preoperative levels,
creased within 3 d, with an eventual return to normal although performance with low motion signal re-
preoperative levels. A pronounced early loss in di- mained poor.
rection contrast sensitivity was also seen in monkey Data obtained in the interval between the first and
3, and again the deficit was most marked for slowly second lesions in monkeys 1 and 3 (Fig. 7, open tri-
moving gratings. The loss for monkey 3 at 7 d was angles) indicate that even unilateral lesions substan-
greater than that in monkey 1 at 6 d, and this differ- tially affected these thresholds. In both monkeys the
ence was consistent with the final thresholds, which effect of the unilateral lesion was comparable to the
showed loss only for monkey 3. transient effect of the complete lesions. Since the uni-
This early sensitivity loss was unlikely to be a sim- lateral lesion caused little or no effect on contrast
ple consequence of surgical trauma, since for two sensitivity for detection and direction discrimination
monkeys (monkeys 1 and 3) we tested sensitivity dur- in these same animals (open triangles in Fig. 3), these
ing the week between placement of the two unilateral deficits in global motion thresholds could reflect ei-
lesions, and found little, if any, loss in contrast thresh- ther a greater sensitivity of these measures to simple
olds (see open triangles in Fig. 3). Thus, even though surgical trauma or a dependence of these thresholds
the animals had received a unilateral MT/MST lesion, on a contribution from both visual fields.
there was little effect.
Discussion
Dynamic Random Dot Tests Our results show that large lesions of areas MT and
Transient losses on tests involving discrimination of MST produce permanent deficits in some tasks in-
opposite directions usually exceeded permanent def- volving motion discriminations. These effects includ-
icits by about a factor of 2, with the most consistent ed a reduction in the accuracy of speed and direction
early effects across monkeys seen on range thresholds. discrimination, and reduced ability to integrate local
Monkeys 2 and 3 were unable to discriminate direc- directional signals in the presence of directional noise.
tion above chance levels even 4-6 d after the lesions. However, the most striking feature of the present re-
Early data, collected during the first few weeks fol- sults was not the observed deficits, but rather the sub-
lowing the lesion, show a dramatic drop in range stantial preservation of motion discrimination. It was
thresholds in all monkeys even when all the dots in particularly impressive that the lesioned monkeys dis-
the display were directionally biased (100% signal). criminated direction of grating motion at essentially
Before the lesion both monkeys could discriminate contrast threshold, and when stimuli contained no
direction when the breadth of the direction distri- directional noise, they showed near-normal perfor-
bution was about 330°. However, after the lesions, mance on discriminations that require the integration
range thresholds in monkeys 2 and 3 decreased to of local motion vectors.

Cerebral Cortex May/June 1994, V 4 N 3 2W


Lesion Location and Extent results of Pasternak et al. (1989), who measured con-
Interpretation of the lesion effects requires both a trast sensitivity for detection and direction discrimi-
knowledge of which visual areas were damaged and nation in cats after ibotenic acid lesions of a cortical
which portions of the visual field were affected in each area possibly involved in motion analysis, the lateral
of the involved areas. These issues are particularly suprasylvian (LS) cortex. As in these monkeys, no loss
difficult for the conical areas studied in this experi- of sensitivity was found for either detection or direc-
ment. While area MT is retinotopically organized, its tion discrimination. They concluded that direction
relatively large receptive fields make it difficult to signals present in undamaged cortical areas may me-
interpret partial lesions. Area MST shows only mini- diate this behavior.
mal retinotopy and it has very large receptive fields A more severe deficit in contrast sensitivity for di-
(Saito et al., 1986), making interpretation of partial rection discrimination was found in a human patient
lesions even more difficult. In this study we attempted with an occipito-parietal cortical lesion (Hess et al.,
to make complete lesions of areas MT and MST, but 1989). This loss in sensitivity was more reminiscent
lesion reconstruction indicates sparing of deep but of that found in cats developmentally deprived of mo-

Downloaded from http://cercor.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of Bath Library & Learning Centre on July 7, 2015
not superficial layers of a portion of central MT in tion perception, which have a widespread loss of cor-
monkeys 1 and 2. On the other hand, the lesion in tical directionality (Pasternak et al., 1985; Pasternak
monkey 3 appeared complete, and the pattern of re- and Leinen, 1986), than in cats with LS lesions or
sults was qualitatively similar in all three monkeys. monkeys with lesions of MT and MST. One implica-
This similarity suggests that the regions of central MT tion of this analysis is that the human patient of Hess
in which there was sparing of only deep layers in et al. (1989) probably has a more widespread lesion
monkeys 1 and 2 may not have been functional. Al- than either the cats with LS lesions or the monkeys
ternatively, these regions of central MT may not have with MT/MST lesions.
been sufficient to prevent permanent loss of some
motion sensitivity. In any case, it does not appear Speed Discrimination
possible that the substantial survival of motion per- Two of the monkeys showed consistent and perma-
ception seen in this study was due to undamaged nent two- to fourfold losses in the accuracy of speed
regions of MT or MST. discrimination over a wide range of contrasts. How-
ever, postoperative performance remained indepen-
Detection and Direction Contrast Sensitivity dent of stimulus contrast, suggesting that, at least in
Intact sensitivity for detecting stationary and drifting some respects, mechanisms underlying the perfor-
gratings has previously been reported for monkeys mance retained their preoperative properties.
after MT lesions (Newsome and Pare, 1988), and hu- Since differences in grating speed are accompa-
mans with damage to parieto-occipital cortex (Hess nied by differences in temporal frequency, it was con-
et al., 1989; Plant et al., 1990). A similar preservation ceivable that the residual performance on speed dis-
of contrast sensitivity for detection has also been ob- crimination was based on flicker cues, rather than on
served in cats with lesions of the lateral suprasylvian speed per se. If this were the case, we would expect
cortex (Pasternak et al., 1989), an area often compared that the discrimination of flicker differences would
to area MT in the monkey (Payne, 1993). In these be at least as accurate as the discrimination of speeds.
studies, observers detected gratings at contrast levels However, previous reports (McKee et al., 1986; Pas-
close to, or only slightly above, normal thresholds. In ternak, 1987), and the data from one of the monkeys
the present study, monkeys 1 and 2 showed no per- in the present study, show that discrimination thresh-
manent loss of sensitivity, while monkey 3 showed a olds for temporal frequency are about a factor of 2-3
small but persistent decrease in sensitivity for both higher then speed thresholds. Moreover, it has been
thresholds. The small depression in sensitivity in demonstrated that the discrimination of flicker rates
monkey 3 may have been due to the larger extent of cannot be performed in the absence of directionally
its lesion, which appeared to include all of MT and selective velocity mechanisms (Pasternak, 1987). Thus,
MST as well as a portion of area FST. This decreased the residual performance of the monkeys appears to
sensitivity for drifting gratings was accompanied by a be based on differences in target speed.
parallel decrease in sensitivity for counterphase-mod- Although the lesions produced some loss in the
ulated gratings flickered in counterphase, such that accuracy of speed discrimination, the monkeys re-
the nearly twofold greater sensitivity for drifting grat- tained substantial ability to judge speed differences.
ings was maintained after the lesion. Superior sensi- Preservation of these speed discrimination abilities
tivity for drifting gratings suggests, as does other ev- after destruction of areas MT/MST, suggests that neu-
idence discussed below, that grating detection at rons outside the STS can perform some analysis of
contrast threshold was mediated by directionally se- stimulus speed, and that MT neurons do not make a
lective mechanisms (Watson et al., 1980; Pasternak, unique contribution to the analysis of speed.
1986).
Our results on speed discrimination address the
The largely preserved sensitivity for the direction issue of a special role for MT in the perception of
of image motion is similar to that found by Plant et velocity. The first evidence that MT might be involved
al. (1990) in a human patient with extrastriate lesions, in the processing of target velocity came from the
who was otherwise severely deficient in discriminat- study of Newsome et al. (1985), who showed that
ing speed differences. It is also consistent with the ibotenic acid lesions of MT produced transient defi-

158 Effects of MT/MST Lesions on Motion Thresholds • Pasternak and Merigan


cits in smooth pursuit eye movements, a result that directionally selective neurons in areas most likely
was interpreted as reflecting a loss of velocity infor- involved in local analysis of direction (e.g., VI). In-
mation. Effects of MST lesions on smooth pursuit were deed, our results showed very limited effects of MT/
similar in that the monkey was unable to match eye MST lesions on contrast sensitivity for direction, a
speed to target speed (Dursteller and Wurtz, 1988). function that may reflect local directionally selective
Subsequently, deficits in speed discrimination (like responses (Pasternak and Maunsell, 1992). It seems
those shown here) were reported in patients with more likely that the pooling of local directional re-
lesions of posterior occipitoparietal cortex (Hess et sponses was affected by the lesion, although the near-
al., 1989; Vaina, 1989; Plant et al., 1990). The impli- ly intact range thresholds we found in the absence of
cations of all of these findings remain somewhat un- directional noise suggest that at least some aspects of
clear. First, the eye movement deficits of Newsome pooling remained intact after MT/MST lesions. Com-
and colleagues and Dursteler et al. appeared more putational approaches to directional difference
transient than the speed deficits measured in this study. thresholds suggest shallower directional tuning func-
This difference could be due in pan to the variation tions or increased variability as likely causes of in-

Downloaded from http://cercor.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of Bath Library & Learning Centre on July 7, 2015
in lesion size and location in the different studies. creased direction thresholds (Wilson and Gelb, 1984).
Comparison of speed difference effects in the present These considerations would be consistent with either
macaque and previous human studies is even more an alteration in these properties in neurons mediating
difficult given the uncertain extent to the lesions in direction thresholds, or assumption of these functions
studies of humans, and the unclear relationship of by other neurons with less optimal directional tuning
macaque MT to human occipitoparietal cortex. and variability.
Preoperative values of D m measured in this study
Global Motion (0.7-0.8° for a target moving at 5 degrees/sec) are
To assess the role of MT in the integration of local close to the upper limit for dot displacement in di-
motion signals, we used dynamic random dots. MT rectionally selective neurons in area MT (Mikami et
neurons have several properties that make them par- al., 1986). Likewise, psychophysical measures of D^
ticularly well suited for integrating dot motion, in- as a function of eccentricity closely match the maxi-
cluding their large, directionally selective receptive mal spatial displacement for MT neurons at different
fields and the directionally integrative response of eccentricities (Baker and Braddick, 1985). These par-
many of its neurons to multiple directions of motion allels between the properties of MT neurons and the
(Movshon et al., 1985). Like Newsome and Pare psychophysical performance of humans have sug-
(1988), we found that the integration of local direc- gested that receptive fields of MT neurons could be
tional signals (the range threshold for dot motion) the substrate of the maximal spatial displacement for
was transiently disrupted by lesions. A similar inabil- direction discrimination. In this study, postoperative-
ity to perform motion tasks in the presence of noise ly reduced accuracy of directional judgments was less
has been reported for a "motion blind" patient by pronounced at larger spatial displacements, and there
Baker et al. (1991). Permanent range threshold def- was no detectable deficit at all at the largest step sizes.
icits at low levels of motion signal suggest either that In fact, the maximal spatial displacement (£>„„) for
these areas play a particularly important role in some direction discrimination was somewhat larger after
processing that enhances signal-to-noise ratios (e.g., the lesion. This increase in £>„„ suggests a postop-
summation) or simply that lesions may exacerbate erative increase in the spatial scale of mechanisms
noise levels in motion-sensitive areas. The possibility mediating the perception of direction (Baker and
that MT neurons may play some role in extracting Braddick, 1985).
motion signals from noise is supported by studies of
Britten and Newsome (Newsome et al., 1989; Britten Specificity of the Motion Deficit
et al., 1992), who have recently shown that cells in All three monkeys showed initial and often perma-
MT respond to the direction of motion in noisy targets nent deficits on a variety of discriminations of the
at signal-to-noise levels near psychophysical thresh- speed and direction of moving targets. On the other
old. Moreover, recent microstimulation experiments hand, we found almost no deficits on the detection
(Salzman et al., 1992) have shown that it is possible task, which did not require the use of motion cues.
to influence the monkey's ability to extract correlated It is possible that this difference simply reflects a
motion by stimulating individual neurons in MT. greater vulnerability of discrimination than detection
In addition to deficits in the discrimination of op- tasks, although this explanation is not supported by
posite directions for dots and gratings, deficits in the the intact ability of monkey 1 on a nonmotion dis-
discrimination of small direction differences were crimination: the spatial frequency of stationary grat-
found in all three monkeys. Since such discrimina- ings. Thus, it seems likely that the effects of MT/MST
tions are likely based on a comparison of the response lesions were probably limited to the domain of image
of local motion detectors, involving a pooling of re- motion. On the other hand, the effect of the MT/MST
sponse differences across directions (Wilson and Gelb, lesion on the spatial frequency discrimination was
1984; Watamaniuk et al., 1989), the direction deficit examined in only a single monkey. Since the lesion
could result from disruption of the response of local in monkey 1 was less extensive and the accompanying
directional detectors or the pooling of directional out- deficit in motion thresholds was less pronounced than
puts. It is unlikely that the MT/MST lesion affected that in the other two monkeys, the question of the

Cerebral Cortex May/June 1994, V 4 N 3 M7


specificity of MT/MST lesion effects needs to be fur- oping testing software. We also thank William Newsome and
ther explored. John Maunsell for comments on the manuscript.
Correspondence should be addressed to Tatiana Paster-
nak, Center for Visual Science, University of Rochester,
7*e Meaning of Transient Deficits Rochester, NY 14627.
The nature of transient effects is of great importance
in understanding the contribution of MT and MST to References
motion perception, since it is not clear from previous Albright TD (1984) Direction and orientation selectivity
studies whether the more profound earlier effects of of neurons in visual area MT of the macaque. J Neurosci
MT lesions are qualitatively different from permanent 42:1106-1130.
effects. Unfortunately, it is impossible to study the Baker CL.BraddickOJ (1985) Eccentricity-dependent scal-
detailed profile of MT effects at the same time as the ing of the limits for short range motion perception. Vision
Res 25:803-812.
detailed time course of transient effects, since such Britten KH, Shadlen MN, Newsome WT, MovshonJA (1992)
effects often last only a day or two. Thus, the present The analysis of visual motion: a comparison of neuronal
study was designed primarily to examine permanent and psychophysical performance. J Neurosci 12:4745-
effects of MT/MST lesions on a large number of visual 4765.

Downloaded from http://cercor.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of Bath Library & Learning Centre on July 7, 2015
functions, to set the stage for subsequent studies of Cleary R, Braddick OJ (1990) Direction discrimination for
band-pass filtered random dot kinematograms. Vision Res
the detailed time course of interesting effects. 30:303-316.
However, some early effects of lesions were ex- Duffy C.WurtzR (1991) Sensitivity of MST neurons to optic
amined in this study, and as has been previously ob- flow stimuli. I. A continuum of response selectivity to
large-field stimuli. J Neurophysiol 65:1329-1345.
served, they were more profound than permanent ef- Dursteller MR, Wurtz RH (1988) Pursuit and optokinetic
fects. At present we can only speculate about the deficits following lesions of cortical areas MT and MST.
relation of transient and permanent effects of MT/ 60:940-965.
MST lesions to the functional role of these areas. One Gallyas F (1979) Silver staining of myelin by means of
possibility is that these areas alone are central to mo- physical development. Neurol Res 1:203-209.
Hess RF, Baker CL, Zihl J (1989) The "motion-blind" pa-
tion processing in the normal monkey and that rapid tient: low-level spatial and temporal filters J Neurosci
recovery simply reflects the postlesion (plastic) as- 9:1628-1640.
sumption of motion processing by other cortical areas Komatsu H, Wurtz RH (1988) Relation of cortical areas MT
(Newsome and Pare, 1988). A second possibility is and MST to pursuit eye movements. I. Localization and
that motion processing normally involves a wide net- visual properties of neurons. J Neurophysiol 60:580-603.
Levinson E, Sekuler R (1975) The independence of chan-
work of many cortical areas, and that recovery time nels in human vision selective for direction of movement.
represents an adaptation of those not damaged. Fi- J Physiol (Lond) 250:347-366.
nally, it could be that many motion perception ca- Maunsell JHR, Van Essen DC (1983) Functional properties
pacities are not critically dependent on MT or MST, of neurons in the middle temporal area of the macaque
and that measured disruption simply represents an monkey. I. Selectivity for stimulus direction, speed and
orientations. J Neurophysiol 49:1148-1167.
increase in noise throughout all cortical areas. Clearer McKee S, Nakayama K, Silverman GH (1986) Precise ve-
evidence that deficits were highly specific to motion locity discrimination despite variations in temporal fre-
processing would argue against this possibility (see quency and contrast. Vision Res 26:609-619.
above). These issues may best be approached by a Mikami A, Newsome WT, Wurtz RH (1986) Motion selec-
careful comparison of the time course and nature of tivity in macaque visual cortex- spatio-temporal range of
directional Interactions in MTandVl.J Neurophysiol 55.
both neural and behavioral disruptions. 1328-1339.
Morgan MJ (1992) Spatial filtering precedes motion de-
tection. Nature 355:344-346.
Conclusions Movshon JA, Adelson EH, Gizzi MS, Newsome WT (1985)
Permanent deficits in the detection of motion in the The analysis of moving visual patterns. In: Pattern rec-
ognition mechanisms (Chagas C, Gattas R, Gross CG,
presence of directional noise and the discrimination eds), pp 117-151. Vatican City: Ponticifica Academia
of small differences in direction and speed provide Scientiarum.
strong support for a role of areas MT/MST in motion Newsome WT, Pare EB (1988) A selective impairment of
processing. On the other hand, since many aspects motion perception following lesions of the middle tem-
of motion perception survive damage to MT/MST, it poral visual area (MT). J Neurosci 8:2201-2211.
Newsome WT, Wurtz RH, Dursteler MR, Mikami A (1985)
is likely that intact areas outside ventral STS, perhaps Deficits in visual motion processing following Ibotenic
at lower cortical levels, can perform many of the op- acid lesions of the middle temporal visual area of the
erations required by the motion tasks of the present macaque monkey. J Neurosci 5:825-840.
study. It remains to be seen whether cortical reor- Newsome WT, Britten KH, Movshon JA, Shadlen M (1989)
ganization following the lesion contributes to the dis- Single neurons and the perception of visual motion. The
Woodlands, TX- Portfolio.
tribution of these capacities. Pasternak T (1986) The role of cortical directional selec-
tivity in detection of motion and flicker. Vision Res 26:
1187-1194.
Notes Pasternak T (1987) Discrimination of differences in speed
This work was supported by grants from the National Eye and flicker rate depends on directionally selective mech-
Institute, EY06175, EY05911, and EY01319 (Core Grant to anisms. Vision Res 27:1881-1890
the Center for Visual Science). John Maunsell participated Pasternak T, Leinen LJ (1986) Pattern and motion vision
in early stages of the experiment We thank Dawn Ferris, in cats with selective loss of cortical directional selectiv-
Walter Polashenski, Judy Tompkins, and Peter Vamvakias for ity. J Neurosci 6:938-945.
excellent technical assistance, and Emil Rainero for devel- Pasternak T, Maunsell JHR (1992) Spatiotemporal sensi-

258 Effects of MT/MST Lesions on Motion Thresholds • Pasternak and Merigan


tivity following lesions of area 18 in the cat. J Neurosci
12.4521-4529.
Pasternak T, Horn KM, Maunsell JHR (1989) Deficits in
speed discrimination following lesions of the lateral su-
prasylvian cortex in the cat. Visual Neurosci 3:365-375.
Pasternak T, Albano JE, Harvftt D (1990) The role of di-
rectionally selective neurons in the perception of global
motion. J Neurosci 10:3079-3086.
Pasternak T, Schumer RA, Gizzi MS, Movshon JA (1985)
Abolition of cortical directional selectivity impairs visual
behavior in cats. Exp Brain Res 61-214-217.
Payne BR (1993) Evidence for visual cortical area homo-
logs in cat and macaque monkey. Cereb Cortex 3:1-25
Plant G, Laxer KD, Barbaro NM, Nakayama K (1990) Im-
paired speed discrimination restricted to the hemifield
contralateral to an occipitoparietal surgical resection.
Soc Neurosci Abstr 16:5.

Downloaded from http://cercor.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of Bath Library & Learning Centre on July 7, 2015
Rodman HR, Albright TD (1989) Single-unit analysis of
pattern-motion selective properties in the middle tem-
poral visual area (MT). Exp Brain Res 75:53-64.
Saito HA, Yukie M, Tanaka K, Hikosaka K, Fukada Y, Iwai E
(1986) Integration of direction signals of image motion
in the superior temporal sulcus of the macaque monkey.
J Neurosci 6:145-157.
Salzman CD, Mirasugi CM, Britten KH, Newsome WT (1992)
Microstimulation of visual area MT: effects of direction
discrimination performance. J Neurosci 12:2331-2356.
Tanaka K, Sugita Y, Moriya M, Saito HA (1993) Analysis
of object motion in the ventral part of the medial superior
temporal area of the macaque visual cortex. J Neurophy-
siol 69:128-142.
Vaina LM (1989) Selective impairment of visual motion
interpretation following lesions of the right occipito-pa-
rietal area in humans Biol Cybern 61-347-359.
Van Essen D, Maunsell JHR (1980) Two-dimensional maps
of the cerebral cortex. J Comp Neurol 191:255-281.
Van Essen D, Maunsell JHR, BigsbyJL (1981) The middle
temporal visual area in the macaque: myeloarchitecrure,
connections, functional properties, and topographic or-
ganization. J Comp Neurol 199:293-326.
WatamaniukSNJ.SekulerR, Williams DW (1989) Direction
perception in complex dynamic displays, the integration
of direction information. Vision Res 29-47-60.
Watson AB, Thompson PG, Murphy BJ, NachmiasJ (1980)
Summation and discrimination of gratings moving in op-
posite directions. Vision Res 20:341-348.
Williams DW, Sekuler R (1984) Coherent global motion
perception from stochastic local motions. Vision Res 24.
55-62.
Wilson HR, Gelb DJ (1984) Modified line-element theory
for spatial-frequency and width discrimination. J Opt Soc
Am 1:124-131.
Wong-RileyM (1979) Changes in the visual system of mon-
ocularly sutured or enucleated cats demonstrable with
cytochrome oxidase histochemlstry. Brain Res 171:11-
28.
Yamasaki DS, Wurtz RH (1991) Recovery of function after
lesions In the superior temporal sulcus in the monkey.
J Neurophysiol 66:651-673.
Zeki SM (1974) Functional organization of a visual area in
the posterior bank of the superior temporal sulcus of the
rhesus monkey. J Physiol (Lond) 236:549-573.

Cerebral Cortex May/June 1994, V 4 N 3 259

You might also like