You are on page 1of 8

EJSP

SHORT PAPER

The effect of red color on perceived self-attractiveness


Anne Berthold*, Gerhard Reese† & Judith Martin‡
* University of Zürich, Zürich, Switzerland
† University of Koblenz-Landau, Landau, Germany
‡ Friedrich Schiller University of Jena, Jena, Germany

Correspondence Abstract
Anne Berthold, Social Psychology,
University of Zürich, Rämistrasse 71, 8006 Recent research showed that individuals are perceived as more attractive
Zürich, Switzerland. when presented with the color red. We seek to extend these findings by
E-mail: Anne.Berthold@googlemail.com studying the effects of red color on individuals’ perception of self-
attractiveness, rather than the attractiveness of others. Based on the color-
Received: 25 April 2016 in-context theory, we hypothesized that individuals would perceive
Accepted: 4 August 2016 themselves as more attractive under red chromatic conditions. In three ex-
periments, participants were asked to wear a red or a blue shirt and rated
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2238
their own attractiveness. As expected, participants in the red shirt condition
Keywords: red effect, attractiveness,
indicated a higher level of self-attractiveness than participants in the blue
status, self-perception condition. Moreover, the results showed that the self-perception red effect
was mediated by the individuals’ self-perceived sexual receptivity and self-
perceived status.

Attractiveness has always been important to women influences individuals’ self-perception. Specifically, we
and men. However, in light of the omnipresence of extend previous findings on the red effect by studying
the concept of perfect beauty in the media and also in the effects of red on individuals’ perception of self-
real life, it may be more difficult than ever to feel attrac- attractiveness.
tive. People are constantly trying to increase their attrac-
tiveness because they want potential partners to see Color-in-context Theory
them as a good catch. Meta-analytic evidence suggests
that attractiveness is a crucial predictor of the way Elliot and Maier (2012) specified the basic premises on
individuals are treated, not only in romantic but also color and psychological functioning. They assume that
in professional contexts (Langlois et al., 2000). Recent color meanings and effects depend on the respective
research has shown that a subtle factor—the color context. People’s reactions towards the color red, for ex-
red—has important consequences for attractiveness ample, can range from approach to avoidance as a result
judgments. Elliot and Niesta (2010) found that men of the varying meaning of the color. While red color has
consistently rated women as higher in attractiveness an appetitive meaning in affiliation contexts, one and
when a border of red framed their photographs or when the same hue can trigger an aversive reaction in
they were presented with a red shirt compared to other achievement contexts, because in this context red is
colors. This red effect was also found for women associated with danger and failure (Elliot & Maier,
perceiving men (Elliot et al., 2008; Roberts, Owen, & 2012). But why do such color associations exist in the
Havlicek, 2010). first place?
However, the research of Roberts et al. (2010) pro- According to the color-in-context theory, two sources
vides not only evidence for a red effect regarding the are responsible for associations of color and responses.
perception of unknown target persons; their data also On the one hand, it is assumed that societal learning
suggest that the color red may have an influence on (i.e., classical conditioning) can cause an association be-
the targets themselves. Roberts et al. (2010) compared tween a color and a reaction; the color red for example
perceivers’ attractiveness ratings of pictures displaying has symbolized love and carnal passion for centuries.
targets that wore a white or red shirt with those in On the other hand, color associations can be due to bio-
which the targets only appeared to be wearing white logically based predispositions (Elliot & Maier, 2012).
or red. They found a positive effect when the targets Red color, for example, represents a sexual signal that
were photographed in red that was independent of the might have evolved from our biological heritage. This
actually presented clothing color, and speculated that reasoning is supported by research showing that non-
the red effect in wearers “may be even stronger than human female primates exhibit red coloration for
perceiver effects” (p.360). In this paper, we aim to shed example as indicator of fertility (Elliot & Niesta, 2008).
more light on the question whether the color red indeed Thus, both societal learning and biologically based

European Journal of Social Psychology 47 (2017) 645–652 Copyright © 2017 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 645
Red color and self-attractiveness A. Berthold et al.

tendencies can jointly contribute to the development Additional support for our hypothesis on red and self-
and consolidation of a color association. Elliot and Maier attractiveness provides self-perception theory (Bem,
(2012) suspect that many color associations are origi- 1967). The theory states that people infer beliefs about
nally derived from an evolutionary engrained receptiv- themselves in part from observations of their own ap-
ity, but then are shaped by societal learning. Thus, it is pearance and behavior. Bem’s assumptions were con-
most likely that the meaning of the color red also has a firmed by Kellerman and Laird (1982), who found
biological background, because red coloration already that having people wear glasses influenced their self-
functions as a sexual signal for nonhuman species. perceptions regarding their performance and compe-
Nonetheless, societal learning seems to have turned tence. Thus, by manipulating the look of individuals, it
the originally pure sexual meaning of red color into a seems possible to influence their self-perception.
more romantic association: red equals love. Still, the
sexual undertone seems to play a major role, because
it has been demonstrated that under red chromatic Underlying Processes of the Red Effect in the
conditions targets were perceived as more sexually at- Affiliation Context
tractive but not as more likeable (Elliot & Niesta, 2008;
Elliot et al., 2010). According to the color-in-context theory, biologically
based predispositions are deemed responsible for the ap-
petitive meaning of the color red, as is visible in the ex-
Self-perception and Attractiveness ample that nonhuman primates use red as indicator of
fertility (Elliot & Maier, 2007; 2012; Elliot & Niesta,
Imagine somebody is repeatedly treated with more ap- 2008). Previous studies support the notion of biological
preciation when wearing red attire; what would you ex- parallels between nonhumans and humans, as they
pect to happen? Is it likely that this person starts to feel revealed a link between red color, attractiveness judg-
better when putting on something red? According to ment, and perceived sexual receptivity (Pazda, Elliot,
learning theorists, the answer would be ‘yes’. Whenever & Greitemeyer, 2012). Pazda et al. (2012) hypothesized
people constantly experience a positive reaction together that men would perceive a female target as more attrac-
with a specific stimulus, they learn to associate both tive under red chromatic conditions because they asso-
stimulus and reaction—even without being aware of ciate red with a higher sexual receptivity, which can
that. One way to develop such an association is be viewed as a proxy (or prerequisite) for fertility. Their
second-order conditioning (Pavlov, 1927). In our red- research provides empirical evidence that perceived
clothes example, the positive response (conditioned sexual receptivity indeed mediates the relation between
stimulus CS1 = others feel attracted and react positively) red and sexual attraction in men viewing women. Addi-
towards the person wearing red (unconditioned tional evidence for the fertility-hypothesis provide
stimulus = red color) is hypothesized to lead to a second Schwarz and Singer (2013), showing that red enhanced
positive response (CS2 = the red-wearer feels appreci- men’s attraction to young women, but not to meno-
ated). In short, information is transferred from one stim- pausal female targets. Moreover, there is recent re-
ulus to another (red color ➔ reaction of others ➔ search on female participants indicating that women
reaction of the self). A similar explanation is provided are (at least unconsciously) aware of the link between
in the concept of the spreading attitude effect, stating that red and fertility; it was found that female participants
valence can be transferred “to objects or events that are were more inclined to wear red clothes at the peak of
preassociated with the CS because of prior experimental their fertility (Beall & Tracy, 2013).
learning” (p. 920, Walther 2002). Thus, individuals learn Several further experiments have corroborated the
to associate red (clothes) with feeling attractive by reasoning that there is a positive effect of red on attrac-
experiencing the positive reactions of attracted others. tiveness judgments, which is caused by sexual receptiv-
This reasoning is fitting quite well to the assumption of ity expectations (Elliot, Greitemeyer, & Pazda, 2013;
color-in-context theory stating that societal learning Guéguen 2012; Niesta Kayser, Elliot, & Feltman, 2010;
(i.e., conditioning processes) causes color associations. Pazda et al. 2014; Young 2015). In accordance with
Bearing in mind that people are likely to associate these findings on red and the perceived attractiveness
(i.e., learn to associate) wearing red with being attrac- of others, we now propose that sexual receptivity also
tive, we now propose that the color red can heighten in- functions as mediator regarding the red effect on self-
dividuals’ self-attractiveness. As mentioned in the attractiveness. Judging oneself as more attractive, when
introduction, Roberts et al. (2010) already provide a the desire for a sexual intimacy is increased (through
hint for our hypothesis. Their data revealed a positive the color red), seems to be an efficient strategy to attract
effect when the targets were photographed in red that potential partners.
was independent of the actually presented clothing Other research suggests that status could also explain
color. Obviously the t-shirt color influenced how the the effect of red color on attractiveness ratings. For
targets (i.e., wearer) had felt and/or probably behaved example, nonhuman male primates use red color to
at the very moment when being photographed—other- demonstrate dominance and status (see Elliot et al.,
wise the participants’ ratings could not have differed as a 2010). Moreover, it was found that women perceived
function of photographed t-shirt color. male targets as more attractive under the chromatic

646 European Journal of Social Psychology 47 (2017) 645–652 Copyright © 2017 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
A. Berthold et al. Red color and self-attractiveness

Table 1. Self-attractiveness (means, standard deviations, and effect sizes) as function of gender and color condition (for all experiments separately and
across experiments)

Experiment 1 Experiment 2 Experiment 3 Overall (Exp1–3)

Female Male Female Male Female Male Female Male


(n = 61) (n = 13) (n = 42) (n = 8) (n = 30) (n = 26) (n = 133) (n = 47)
Red condition 4.59 (1.02) 4.92 (0.37) 4.78 (0.98) 5.13 (1.20) 5.41 (0.68) 5.45 (0.67) 4.82 (0.93) 5.23 (0.79)
Blue condition 4.28 (0.77) 4.08 (0.76) 4.42 (0.96) 4.26 (0.93) 4.57 (1.48) 4.66 (1.20) 4.44 (0.93) 4.43 (1.18)
2
ηp .03 .04 .05 .06 .08 .06 .03 .04
Hedges’ d .39 1.38 .36 .74 .79 .74 .46 .89
2
M = mean (SD = standard deviation), ηp = effect size partial eta square, Hedges’ d, Positive values of the bias-corrected effect size Hedges’ d indicate that
self-attractiveness was higher in the red than the blue condition.

condition of red because red enhanced the perception of with a red t-shirt and in the control condition with a
high status (Elliot et al., 2010; Stephen, Oldham, Perrett, blue t-shirt (S, M, or L). All participants were seated
& Barton, 2012). We therefore assume that the red in cubicles that were equipped with a mirror, and the
effect on self-appearance is also mediated by self- respective t-shirt was provided within a big envelope
perceived status. In other words, people most likely on the table. After putting on the shirt, participants
perceive themselves as more attractive, because of the completed the questionnaire assessing perceived self-
ostensibly increased self-status (which is inferred from attractiveness by 12 items (α = .89) rated on an
the red color). author-constructed scale from 1 = do not agree to 7 = do
In sum, given that the meaning of red has such genu- agree (e.g., “At the moment I consider myself attrac-
ine biological roots and is bolstered through societal tive.”). Finally, participants were thanked for their par-
learning, we argue that wearing red affects individuals’ ticipation and debriefed via e-mail.
self-attractiveness judgments, because the color shapes
their look and they are inclined to base inferences on
their appearance (Bem, 1967). Accordingly, the color Results and Discussion
red should be influential when inferring one’s sexual
receptivity and self-status. We assume that those two An ANOVA with perceived self-attractiveness as the
variables mediate the effect of color on self- dependent variable and color condition as the be-
attractiveness, as was found regarding the perception tween subjects factor revealed a significant effect of
of others (Elliot & Maier, 2012; 2014). We refrain from color1, F(1, 72) = 4.34, p = .04, ηp2 = .06. Participants
examining gender differences in detail, as for the time wearing a red shirt rated themselves as more attractive
being, we aim to establish the self-perception red effect (M = 4.76, SD = 1.10) than participants in the control
in general. condition wearing a blue shirt, (M = 4.18, SD = 1.25).
We included gender as additional factor in the analysis
Experiment 1 to test for potential gender differences. The results re-
vealed no interaction of color and gender F(1, 72)
In Experiment 1, we investigated whether the color red = .88, p = .35, indicating that female and male partici-
increases individuals’ perception of self-attractiveness. It pants did not differ regarding this self-perception red
was expected that individuals would perceive themselves effect (see also Table 1).
as more attractive when wearing a red versus blue shirt.
1
We conducted a short online test with the same self-attractiveness
Method scale as used in Experiment 1. Forty-two German-speaking individuals
took part in the online-study. They were asked to indicate their agree-
Seventy-four university students (psychology students ment with the attractiveness-items and then described the colors of
for the most part) from a large university in Eastern their actual clothes. Additionally, we assessed if they were dressed in
Germany participated in the experiment (61 women, the color red with a dichotomously structured item (yes/no). We calcu-
lated the mean perceived self-attractiveness of the participants, who did
13 men) with a mean age of 21.1 years (SD = 2.7). The
not wear red (n = 42) and compared this score with those of the red and
alleged aim of the study was to investigate the predict- blue condition of Experiment 1. There was no significant difference be-
ability of personality traits based on facial features. Par- tween the participants, who took part in the short online test (and who
ticipants were told that the procedure would require did not wear red, M = 4.16, SD = 0.91) and the participants in the blue
them to wear a plain ordinary t-shirt (no-name prod- condition of Exp.1, t(76) = .47, p = .64. Not surprisingly, self-
uct) provided by the experimenter so as to avoid other perceived attractiveness was higher in the red condition compared to
influences. The personality judgments were allegedly the online sample not wearing red, t(77) = -2.37, p = .02. Accordingly,
the significant difference between the blue and red condition of Exper-
made by the experimenters observing participants indi-
iment 1 is most likely due to an increase of self-attractiveness as a result
vidually through a window, while the participants were of wearing the color red. Put differently, it is rather unlikely that indi-
completing a questionnaire on personality traits. Partic- viduals decrease in self-perceived attractiveness as a consequence of
ipants were randomly assigned to one of two condi- wearing blue, as there is no difference between the online-sample
tions. In the red condition, participants were provided and the blue condition.

European Journal of Social Psychology 47 (2017) 645–652 Copyright © 2017 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 647
Red color and self-attractiveness A. Berthold et al.

6
Experiment 2

The aim of Experiment 2 was to test the stability of the

Self-attractiveness
red effect on self-attractiveness. Therefore, we con- 5
Blue
ducted an experiment including a distraction phase. It
Red
was hypothesized that perceived self-attractiveness,
measured before and after the distraction, would be
4
higher in the red condition and that this effect would
be stable over time. We expected that the effect would
still be found after diverting participants’ attention
(using a short video sequence), because whenever indi- 3
viduals rate their attractiveness they are likely to infer their Before distraction After distraction
estimation from their current appearance (Bem, 1967).
Fig. 1: Perceived self-attractiveness as a function of color condition and
Moreover, in Experiment 2, we included several
time
additional variables known to influence individuals’
self-evaluation. These variables were mood, body mass
index (BMI), and extraversion, which was found to be showed that the red effect neither interacted with gen-
strongly related to individuals’ self-esteem and self- der, F(1, 41) = .63, p = .43, nor with time, F(1, 41)
evaluation (e.g., Cheng & Furnham, 2003). Our aim = 1.79, p = .19, indicating that both women’s and
was to control for potential confounds that might inter- men’s self-images profited from wearing red
fere with the occurrence of the self-perception red effect. (Table 1). Analyzing the data without the control var-
iables also showed that self-attractiveness was higher
Method in the red condition (M = 4.93) than in the blue condi-
tion (M = 4.38), F(1, 50) = 2.30, p = .14, η2 = .04. How-
The sample and procedure were similar to that of ever, without the control variables, the effect was not
Experiment 1, except for the delay. Fifty participants significant. It is possible that not all participants
(42 women, 8 men) with a mean age of 21.7 years responded similar to our manipulation, and future re-
(SD = 3.0) rated their self-attractiveness on 11 items2 search should examine the role of extraversion, BMI,
before and after watching a 5-minute video (α = .92, and mood regarding the red effect.
α = .94). The video showed a scene of the U.S.– The first two experiments provide evidence that wear-
American film “Dog Day Afternoon” from the year ing a red shirt increases one’s self-perceived attractive-
1975 displaying a bank-hold up. Mirrors were present ness. One limitation of both studies, however, is that the
throughout the whole session. We assessed height and experimenter’s alleged personality judgment through
weight via open questions and thus calculated partici- the lab window allowed participant–experimenter inter-
pants BMI. Mood was assessed with one single item action. Not being blind to the hypotheses, the experi-
“How is your mood at the moment? Please cross the line menter could have influenced the participants
at the respective position.” (the two ending poles were unintentionally. A second issue that needs to be resolved
bad vs. good). The questionnaire contained seven extra- is the question of how the color red influences self-
version items (e.g., “I like to be at the center of action.”; perception. Based on previous research on the influence
based on the scale of Borkenau & Ostendorf, 2008), of red on the perception of others, we expected that self-
which were randomly distributed across the attractive- perceived sexual receptivity and status would mediate
ness items. the red effect (Elliot & Maier, 2012; 2014).

Results and Discussion Experiment 3

The attractiveness ratings were subjected to a 2 × 2 In Experiment 3, we examined the processes underly-
ANCOVA with color (red vs. blue) as the between- ing the self-perception red effect. Previous findings
subjects factor and time (before vs. after distraction) demonstrated that male participants associated female
as the within-subjects factor. We included the addi- targets wearing a red shirt with higher sexual receptivity
tional variables mentioned above as covariates. Again, resulting in higher attractiveness ratings (Pazda et al.
perceived self-attractiveness was higher in the red 2012; 2014; see also Guéguen, 2012). Regarding male
(M = 4.96, SD = 0.95) than the blue condition targets, Elliot et al. (2010) showed that female partici-
(M = 4.34, SD = 0.98), F(1, 41) = 4.42, p = .04, ηp2 = .10. pants perceived men with a red shirt as high in status
The effect was stable across time (see Figure 1, Red ef- and therefore rated these male targets as more attractive
fect t1before F(1,41) = 3.78, p = .06, ηp2 = .08; Red effect (see also Stephen et al., 2012). Accordingly, we included
t2afterfilm F(1, 41) = 4.79, p = .03, η2p ¼ :11). The analysis these two variables—sexual receptivity and status—as
possible mediators. Note that in our study on self-
2
The scale was identical to the one used in Experiment 1a except for perception (and not on perception by others), we ex-
one item that was excluded because participants (in Experiment 1) crit- pected that the increase in self-attractiveness when
icized the wording of this item. wearing red would be due to an increase in self-

648 European Journal of Social Psychology 47 (2017) 645–652 Copyright © 2017 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
A. Berthold et al. Red color and self-attractiveness

perceived sexual receptivity as well as due to an increase significant interaction of color and gender emerged
in self-perceived status. F(1, 52) < .01, p = .94.
We modified the experimental procedure to reduce
demand effects—due to the presence of an experi- Self-perceived sexual receptivity. As expected,
menter—that might have influenced the results of participants wearing red shirts (M = 4.49, SD = 0.95)
Experiments 1 and 2. Participants were asked to take a reported higher sexual receptiveness than partici-
snapshot of themselves. They were told that their snap- pants wearing blue (M = 3.94, SD = 1.10), F(1, 52)
shot would be given to experts who would judge the = 7.64, p < .01, ηp2 = .13. There was no interaction
participant’s personality on the basis of facial features with gender, F(1,52) = .23, p = .63, indicating that
(such as symmetry). both females and males responded similar to our
manipulation. Moreover, the analyses revealed a
Method main effect of gender, F(1, 52) = 8.70, p < .01,
ηp2 = .14, indicating that men (M = 4.55, SD = 1.00) re-
Fifty-six participants from a university in West ported higher sexual receptiveness than women
Germany (30 women, 26 men) with a mean age of (M = 3.95, SD = 1.02).
20.5 years (SD = 1.7) were told that their first task would
be to put on the “standardized” t-shirt and to take a Self-perceived self-status. Participants wearing
picture of themselves (headshot with shoulders). The red shirts reported higher status (M = 4.61, SD = 0.78)
experimenter explained to each participant that later than participants wearing blue shirts (M = 4.15,
on, the photo would be given to independent experts SD = 0.86), F(1,52) = 8.65, p < .01, ηp2 = .14. There
who would then allegedly rate their personality traits was no interaction of color and gender, F(1,52) < .03,
based on specific facial features. As in Experiments 1 again indicating that female and male participants did
and 2, participants were randomly assigned to one of not differ regarding the manipulation. Moreover,
the two conditions. In both conditions—the experimen- the results showed that men (M = 4.65, SD = 0.73)
tal (red shirt) and the control (blue shirt)—participants rated their status in general higher than did
were told that the “standardized” t-shirt served the women (M = 4.16, SD = 0.88), F(1, 52) = 9.36, p < .01,
purpose of preventing individual clothing styles from ηp2 = .15.
influencing the experts’ ratings. All participants were
given a questionnaire to fill out containing the following Mediation analysis. To investigate the mediating
variables rated on a scale from 1 = do not agree to roles of self-perceived sexual receptivity and per-
7 = agree: perceived self-status (5 items, “I would con- ceived self-status between color condition and self-
sider my current status as high”; “At the moment I am attractiveness, we examined confidence intervals
a person of high standing”; α = .76), sexual receptivity (CIs) with standard errors that were estimated via
(5 items, “At the moment I am in need of some physical bootstrapping (Hayes, 2013). We used Hayes PRO-
intimacy”; “I can imagine being sexually active today”, CESS macro for SPSS (Model 4) to estimate the
α = .74) and perceived self-attractiveness (4 items, “At 95% CIs of the indirect effects, using 10 000 boot-
the moment I am: attractive/good-looking/handsome/ strap re-samples to approximate the sampling distri-
sexy.” α = .92). butions of the conditional indirect effect of shirt
color on self-attractiveness ratings via sexual recep-
Results and Discussion tivity and status. Both indirect effects, tested to-
gether within one analysis, turned out to be
Perceived self-attractiveness. Again, participants significant: An indirect effect of color on self-
wearing a red shirt rated themselves as more attrac- attractiveness via sexual receptivity, B = 0.34, SE(B)
tive (M = 5.43, SD = 1.09) than participants in the con- = 0.17; 95% bias-corrected and accelerated CI BCa
trol condition wearing a blue shirt (M = 4.61, CI = [0.06, 0.62] and an indirect effect of color on
SD = 1.06), F(1, 52) = 8.06, p < .01, η2p ¼ :13 . Partici- self-attractiveness via self-status, B = 0.44, SE(B)
= 0.14; BCa CI = [0.09, 0.79] emerged. These results
pants’ gender3 did not influence the results; no
are consistent with our hypothesis that both self-

3
Both females and males showed a similar pattern regarding all three
4
dependent variables. Analyzing the data separately for males and fe- We tested alternative models with self-attractiveness as mediator. The
males indicated that the red effect on self-attractiveness was present indirect effect of color on self-perceived sexual receptivity via self-
2
for females, Ffemale = 4.54, p = .04, η = .08 and males Fmale = 3.58, attractiveness was significant, B = 0.56, SE(B) = 0.11; 95% BCa
2
p = .06, ηp = .06. Regarding self-perceived sexual receptivity, the effect CI = [0.33; 0.79], and the indirect effect of color on self-status via self-
2
was found for women Ffemale = 5.61, p = .02, ηp = .10, but was not pres- attractiveness was also significant B = 0.46, SE(B) = 0. 09; 95% BCa
ent for men (although a trend may be seen), Fmale = 2.46, p = .12. CI = [0.27; 0.63]. Thus, considering self-attractiveness as the mediator
p = .90. Finally regarding self-status, a similar pattern emerged for fe- seems to be a viable alternative, but as the presentation in the current
2
males, Ffemale = 5.06, p = .03, ηp = .09, and males, Fmale = 3.69, p = .06, paper is theoretically driven, we join the theorizing of previous research
2
ηp = .07. suggesting that attractiveness is the outcome variable.

European Journal of Social Psychology 47 (2017) 645–652 Copyright © 2017 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 649
Red color and self-attractiveness A. Berthold et al.

perceived sexual receptivity and status mediate the (CIfemale = 0.12, 0.81; CImale = 0.28, 1.50). Table 1 con-
relation between color and self-attractiveness4. Addi- tains all effect sizes along with the overall effect for fe-
tional mediation analyses examining gender differ- males and males6 separately.
ences5 resulted in similar conclusions except that
for the male sample, the mediation via self- General Discussion
perceived sexual receptivity did not reach a conven-
tional level of significance. The present research provides evidence for the influ-
We also calculated a mediation analyses (PROCESS ence of the color red on individuals’ self-perception.
macro, Model 8) testing for moderated mediation with Across three experiments, individuals rated their own
gender as moderator, the two mediators (sexual receptiv- attractiveness as higher when wearing red as com-
ity and status) and self-attractiveness as the outcome var- pared to wearing blue (d = 0.57, across all experi-
iable. The results indicate that gender differences ments). This red effect even endured a temporal
regarding both mediators are unlikely; BCa distraction (Experiment 2).
CIsex_rez = [0.62, 0.33], BCa CIstatus = [0.53, 0.39]; in Mediation analyses revealed that both self-perceived
other words, both indirect effects did not differ as a func- sexual receptivity and self-status independently medi-
tion of gender. ated the effect of color on self-attractiveness. Individ-
uals with a red shirt perceived themselves as more
attractive, because of an increase in their self-perceived
Meta-analytic Summary
sexual receptivity and also because of an increase in
perceived self-status. These findings are in line with
In order to gain a more precise estimate of the red effect
previous research, as it was shown that women were
regarding self-attractiveness, we calculated the average
perceived as more attractive by men because the color
effect size of our experiments weighted by the inverse
red signaled greater sexual receptivity (Pazda et al.,
of the variance (Johnson & Eagly, 2000). Combining ef-
2012) and men were seen as more attractive by
fect sizes across experiments (N = 180), resulted in an
women because red signaled higher status (Elliot
overall effect size of d = 0.57 (d = Hedge’s sample-size-
et al., 2010). Our data did not provide clear evidence
corrected effect size, which is a little smaller than
for gender differences; however, it could be fruitful to
Cohen’s effect size), representing a medium effect. The
disentangle whether the effect is subject to different
95% CI for the overall effect size, which does not in-
processes among men and women. Moreover, as our
clude zero, indicates that possible values for the effect
findings indicate that both mediation paths are possible
are between d = 0.27 and 0.87. We also calculated the
for women and men, it might be worthwhile to study
overall effect size for females and males separately,
if men perceive women under red chromatic condi-
obtaining effect sizes of dfemale = 0.46 and dmale = 0.89.
tions as more attractive because of an increase in status
5
(aside to the increased sexual receptivity). Likewise,
Additional Mediation Analysis—Female Sample: Using the same
red men could be seen as more attractive because of
bootstrapping parameters as for the complete sample, we tested if self-
perceived sexual receptivity would serve as mediator between color
an increase of sexual receptivity (next to status). From
condition and self-attractiveness for the female sample. As expected, a biological point of view, these alternative paths
the indirect effect of shirt color on self-attractiveness via sexual receptiv- would make sense, as for example Setchell and
ity was significant, B = 0.53, SE(B) = 0.28; 95% bias-corrected and accel- Wickings (2006) found that male mandrills prefer high
erated confidence interval BCa CI = [0.08; 1.18]. Thus, self-perceived rank female mandrills (which are more likely fertile
sexual receptivity mediated the relation between shirt color and self- and thus display red).
attractiveness among women. Moreover, we tested if self-status would
The present findings nicely corroborate recent
also serve as mediator between color condition and self-attractiveness
for the female sample. The indirect effect of shirt color on self-
evidence that red is important for the self-image.
attractiveness via self-status was also significant, B = 0.47, SE(B) Recently, Elliot and Pazda (2012) found that women
= 0.31; 95% BCa CI = [0.02; 1.26]. Thus, among women, the increase displayed red on their individual website, when they
in self-attractiveness in the red condition was due to both the increase were interested in sexual behavior. Also, women were
in self-perceived sexual receptivity as well as the increased self-status. more inclined to prefer red attire, when they expected
Additional Mediation Analysis—Male Sample: We found an indirect effect to meet an attractive man (Elliot et al., 2013; Prokop &
of shirt color on self-attractiveness ratings via perceived self-status,
Hromada, 2013). Farrelly, Slater, Elliott, Walden, and
B = 0.60, SE(B) = 0.31; 95% BCa CI = [0.09; 1.26] for the male sample.
Thus, self-perceived status mediated the relation between shirt color
Wetherell (2013) found that men chose red over other
and self-attractiveness among men. In addition, we tested if self- colors when their levels of testosterone were high,
perceived sexual receptivity would also mediate the effect of color con- indicating status and dominance.
dition on self-attractiveness. The indirect effect of color condition on Experiment 2 showed that the red effect persisted
self-attractiveness via self-perceived sexual receptivity was not even when individuals were distracted. Thus, there is
significant, B = 0.42, SE(B) = 0.32; 95% BCa CI = [-0.10; 1.17]. reason to believe that it is possible to heighten one’s
Altogether, the additional analyses for the male sample indicate that
own attractiveness by wearing a red (and, hopefully,
the effect of color on self-attractiveness is primarily due to the increase
in self-perceived status.
occasionally cleaned) shirt over a longer period. Subse-
6
Note that the current data suffer from low power regarding the male quent research should thus focus on the durability of
subsample. This, however, does not question the overall effect across these effects, testing both effects of wearing red over a
both gender (see also meta-analysis). longer period (e.g., across several experimental

650 European Journal of Social Psychology 47 (2017) 645–652 Copyright © 2017 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
A. Berthold et al. Red color and self-attractiveness

instances) and delayed effects of wearing red (e.g., Elliot, A. J., & Maier, M. A. (2014). Color psychology: Effects
several days after wearing a red shirt). of perceiving color on psychological functioning in
Finally, we need to acknowledge the possibility of humans. Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 95–120.
unintended negative consequences when applying the Elliot, A. J., & Niesta, D. (2008). Romantic red: Red enhances
color red in an experimental design such as ours. Under men’s attractions to women. Journal of Personality and Social
certain conditions, wearing red might lead to a disad- Psychology, 95, 1150–1164. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
vantageous increase in self-surveillance, and we know 0022-3514.95.5.1150
from self-objectification research that increasing the Elliot, A. J., Niesta Kayser, D., Greitemeyer, T., Lichtenfeld, S.,
Gramzow, R. H., Maier, M. A., & Liu, H. (2010). Red, rank
self-surveillance of one’s own physical appearance can
and romance in women viewing man. Journal of Experi-
trigger severe consequences like body guilt and eating
mental Psychology: General, 139, 399–417. http://dx.doi.
restraint (Calogero & Pina, 2011). Thus, future
org/10.1037/a0019689
research should examine under which conditions the
Elliot, A. J., & Pazda, A. D. (2012). Dressed for sex: Red as a
red effect might backlash. It might also be fruitful to
female sexual signal in humans. PLoS ONE, 7 e34607.
address the question of whether there are certain http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0034607
situations in which individuals avoid wearing red in Farrelly, D., Slater, R., Elliott, H., Walden, H., & Wetherell, M.
order to prevent sending wrong or unintended signals. (2013). Competitors who choose to be red have higher
However, when it comes to a desired romantic interac- testosterone levels. Psychological Science, 24, 2122–2234.
tion, we aim to emphasize that wearing red appears to http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797613482945
be a promising strategy to increase one’s attractiveness Guéguen, N. (2012). Color and women attractiveness: When
and to send the intended signals. red clothed women are perceived to have more intense
sexual intent. Journal of Social Psychology, 152, 261–65.
Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and
Acknowledgements conditional process analysis. New York: The Guilford Press.
Johnson, B. T., & Eagly, A. H. (2000). Quantitative synthesis
We thank Lisa D’Andola, Louise Burtin, Sina Franke, of social psychological research. In H. T. Resi, & C. M. Judd
Viktoria Fukazawa, Antonia Kaluza, Marietta (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in social and personality
Klingenberg, Marie Lange, Philipp Mariacher, Martin psychology (pp. 496–528). London: Cambridge University
Schönke, Julia Straube, Carolin Utecht, and Lena Press.
Sophie Zimmermann for their help with data collection. Kellerman, J. M., & Laird, J. D. (1982). The effect of
We also want to thank Johannes Ullrich and Sascha appearance on self-perceptions. Journal of Personality, 50,
Schwarz for their valuable comments on a previous 296–315. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1982.
version of the manuscript. tb00752.x
Langlois, J. H., Kalakanis, L., Rubenstein, A. J., Larson, A.,
Hallam, M., & Smoot, M. (2000). Maxims or myths of
References beauty? A meta-analytic and theoretical review. Psycholog-
ical Bulletin, 126, 390–423. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
Beall, A. T., & Tracy, J. L. (2013). Women are more likely to 0033-2909.126.3.390
wear red or pink at peak fertility. Psychological Science, 24, Niesta Kayser, D., Elliot, A. J., & Feltman, R. (2010). Red and
1837–1841. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0956797613476045 romantic behavior in men viewing women. European
Bem, D. J. (1967). Self-perception: An alternative Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 901–908. http://dx.doi.org/
interpretation of the cognitive dissonance phenomena. 10.1002/ejsp.757
Psychological Review, 74, 183–200. http://dx.doi.org/ Pavlov, I. P. (1927). Conditioned reflexes: An investigation of the
10.1037/h0024835 physiological activity of the cerebral cortex. Oxford, U.K.:
Borkenau, P., & Ostendorf, F. (2008). NEO-FFI—NEO-Fünf- Oxford University Press.
Faktoren-Inventar nach Costa und McCrae. Handanweisung. Pazda, A. D., Elliot, A. J., & Greitemeyer, T. (2012). Sexy red:
Göttingen: Hogrefe. Perceived sexual receptivity mediates the red-attraction re-
Calogero, R. M., & Pina, A. (2011). Body guilt: Preliminary lation in men viewing women. Journal of Experimental So-
evidence for a further subjective experience of self- cial Psychology, 48, 787–790. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.
objectification. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 35, jesp.2011.12.009
428–440. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0361684311408564 Pazda, A. D., Elliot, A. J., & Greitemeyer, T. (2014). Perceived
Cheng, H., & Furnham, A. (2003). Personality, self-esteem, sexual receptivity and fashionableness: Separate paths
and demographic predictions of happiness and depression. linking red and black to perceived attractiveness. Color
Personality and Individual Differences, 34, 921–942. Research and Application, 39, 208–212.
Elliot, A. J., Greitemeyer, T., & Pazda, A. D. (2013). Women’s Prokop, P., & Hromada, M. (2013). Women use red in order
use of red clothing as sexual signal in intersexual interac- to attract mates. Ethology, 119, 605–613.
tion. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49, 599–602. Roberts, S. C., Owen, R. C., & Havlicek, J. (2010).
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2012.10.001 Distinguishing between perceiver and wearer effects in
Elliot, A. J., & Maier, M. A. (2007). Color and psychological clothing color-associated attributions. Evolutionary Psychol-
functioning. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, ogy, 8, 350–364.
25. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00514.x Schwarz, S., & Singer, M. (2013). Romantic red revisited:
Elliot, A. J., & Maier, M. A. (2012). Color-in-context theory. Red enhances men’s attraction to young, but not
Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 61–125. menopausal women. Journal of Experimental Social

European Journal of Social Psychology 47 (2017) 645–652 Copyright © 2017 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 651
Red color and self-attractiveness A. Berthold et al.

Psychology, 49, 161–164. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j. Walther, E. (2002). Guilty by mere association: Evaluative
jesp.2012.08.004 conditioning and the spreading attitude effect. Journal of
Setchell, J. M., & Jean Wickings, E. (2006). Mate choice in Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 919–934. http://dx.
male mandrills (Mandrillus sphinx). Ethology, 112, 91–99. doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.82.6.919
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2006.01128.x Young, S. G. (2015). The effect of red on male perceptions of
Stephen, I. D., Oldham, F. H., Perrett, D. I., & Barton, R. A. female attractiveness: Moderation by baseline attractive-
(2012). Redness enhances perceived aggression, domi- ness of female faces. European Journal of Social Psychology.
nance and attractiveness in men’s faces. Evolutionary http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2098
Psychology, 10, 562–72.

652 European Journal of Social Psychology 47 (2017) 645–652 Copyright © 2017 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

You might also like