You are on page 1of 464

PRESCRIPTIONS FOR REDUCING THE PLANNING FALLACY: THE

OUTSIDE VIEW VERSUS UNPACKING

SITI ZUBAIDAH OTHMAN

BA Psychology (I/O Psychology), MSc Human Resources

This thesis is presented for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy

of

The University of Western Australia

UWA Business School

Management and Organizations

2009
Declaration

Prescriptions for reducing the planning fallacy: The outside view versus unpacking

This thesis is my own composition, all sources have been acknowledged and my

contribution is clearly identified in the thesis.

______________________________________________________________________

Siti Zubaidah Othman


Abstract

This thesis examines two theories of the planning fallacy, the inside / outside

view and the unpacking approach, and extends planning fallacy explanations to projects

involving Human Resource Information Systems (HRIS). This is the first empirical

research to compare these theories on the same tasks and in the same study.

The first study tested the two theories in a controlled experiment with 708

student participants working on small group project assignments. Individuals were

nested within 162 project groups, and data was collected on a weekly basis for eight

weeks. The hypotheses were tested using three-level hierarchical linear modeling

(HLM). Both approaches showed opposite effects on predictions made towards

completion time and outcome success, but had no effect on changes in confidence in

predictions of completion time and outcome success.

The second study explored whether the outside view, inside view or unpacking

planning approaches are commonly used by planners for 45 real HRIS projects in

organizations, and their effects on the accuracy of predictions about the success or

failure of the project in terms of project completion time, outcome achieved, and

satisfaction with the outcome. Hypotheses were tested using correlations, and results are

also presented using descriptive statistics. The reported extent of use of the unpacking

approach was positively related to the success of the project in terms of percentage of

outcome achieved, the satisfaction with the HRIS system, which includes the content,

format, accuracy, timeliness and ease of use of the system and the reported affective

response towards outcome success.

Recommendations for practice include support for the unpacking approach and

the use of work breakdown structure in project planning. Suggestions are made for

further research on differentiating the inside view and unpacking, and on the

i
undesirable effects of both the outside view and unpacking in increasing rather than

reducing optimistic bias for outcome success.

ii
Table of Contents

Abstract .............................................................................................................................. i
Table of Contents .............................................................................................................iii
Dedication ....................................................................................................................... vii
Acknowledgements ........................................................................................................viii
List of Figures .................................................................................................................. ix
List of Tables..................................................................................................................... x
List of Appendices .........................................................................................................xiii
CHAPTER 1 ..................................................................................................................... 1
INTRODUCTION ............................................................................................................ 1
1.1 Context and Significance ........................................................................................ 1
1.2 Scope and Aim of Research .................................................................................... 6
1.3 Organization of Chapters in the Thesis ................................................................... 8
CHAPTER 2 ................................................................................................................... 11
A REVIEW OF PROJECT SUCCESS, PROJECT PLANNING AND PLANNING
BIASES ........................................................................................................................... 11
2.1 Introduction ........................................................................................................... 11
2.2 The Characteristics of Projects.............................................................................. 11
2.3 The Concepts of Information System (IS) Project Success and Failure ............... 12
2.4 Past Research on Causes of Information System (IS) Project Failure .................. 14
2.5 The Role of Planning ............................................................................................ 16
2.6 The Planning Process: Issues and Challenges ....................................................... 17
2.7 Planning Under Uncertainty.................................................................................. 19
2.7.1 Biases in Completion Time Predictions ......................................................... 21
2.7.2 Biases in Outcome Predictions....................................................................... 28
2.7.3 Confidence in Predictions .............................................................................. 32
2.7.4 Biases in Affective Predictions ...................................................................... 35
2.8 Conclusion and Research Issues to be Addressed................................................. 38
CHAPTER 3 ................................................................................................................... 41
HYPOTHESES - STUDY 1............................................................................................ 41
3.1 Introduction ........................................................................................................... 41
3.2 Testing the Theories of the Planning Fallacy........................................................ 41
3.3 Theories of the Planning Fallacy and Outcome Prediction ................................... 42
3.4 Theories of the Planning Fallacy and Confidence in Predictions ......................... 43
3.5 Theories of the Planning Fallacy and Prediction of Affective Reactions towards
Outcome Success and Failure ..................................................................................... 44
3.6 Conclusion ............................................................................................................ 46
CHAPTER 4 ................................................................................................................... 48
METHOD – STUDY 1 ................................................................................................... 48
4.1 Introduction ........................................................................................................... 48
4.2 Research Design .................................................................................................... 48
4.3 Data Collection Process ........................................................................................ 49
4.4 Participants ............................................................................................................ 49
4.5 Experimental Materials ......................................................................................... 50
4.6 Experimental Procedure ........................................................................................ 52
4.7 Measures ............................................................................................................... 55
4.7.1 Time Completion Measure ............................................................................. 55
4.7.2 Outcome Success Measures ........................................................................... 55
4.7.3 Confidence Measures ..................................................................................... 56
4.7.4 Affective Response for Success and Failure Measures.................................. 56
4.8 Data Analysis Strategy .......................................................................................... 57
iii
4.8.1 Level of Analysis ........................................................................................... 57
4.8.2 Data Centering using Grand Means ............................................................... 58
4.8.3 Time Centering .............................................................................................. 59
4.9 Sample Size for Multilevel Modeling Analyses ................................................... 60
4.10 Data Cleaning...................................................................................................... 62
4.11 Conclusion .......................................................................................................... 62
CHAPTER 5 ................................................................................................................... 63
RESULTS – STUDY 1 ................................................................................................... 63
5.1 Introduction ........................................................................................................... 63
5.2 Demographic Characteristics of the Participants .................................................. 63
5. 3 Hypothesis Testing............................................................................................... 63
5.3.1 Completion Time Predictions ........................................................................ 64
5.3.1.1 Unconditional Three-level HLM Models ............................................... 67
5.3.1.2 Conditional Three-level Model for Completion Time Prediction Bias .. 71
5.3.2 Outcome Predictions ...................................................................................... 78
5.3.2.1 Unconditional Three-level Model ........................................................... 79
5.3.2.2 Conditional Three-level Model ............................................................... 81
5.3.3 Confidence in Predictions of Project Completion Time ................................ 87
5.3.3.1 Unconditional Three-level Model ........................................................... 88
5.3.3.2 Conditional Three-level Model ............................................................... 90
5.3.4 Confidence in Predictions of Project Outcome .............................................. 95
5.3.4.1 Unconditional Three-level Model ........................................................... 96
5.3.4.2 Conditional Three-level Model ............................................................... 98
5.3.5 Predictions of Affective Response towards Outcome Success .................... 103
5.3.5.1 Unconditional Three-level Model ......................................................... 105
5.3.5.2 Conditional Three-level Model ............................................................. 107
5.3.6 Predictions of Affective Response towards Outcome Failure ..................... 113
5.3.6.1 Unconditional Three-level Model ......................................................... 115
5.3.6.2 Conditional Three-level Model ............................................................. 117
5.4 Conclusion .......................................................................................................... 122
CHAPTER 6 ................................................................................................................. 124
DISCUSSION – STUDY 1 ........................................................................................... 124
6.1 Introduction ......................................................................................................... 124
6.2 Completion Time Predictions ............................................................................. 124
6.3 Outcome Predictions ........................................................................................... 127
6.4 Confidence in Predictions ................................................................................... 128
6.5 Predictions of Affective Response towards Outcome Success and Failure ........ 128
6.6 Study 1 Limitations ............................................................................................. 130
6.7 Conclusion .......................................................................................................... 131
CHAPTER 7 ................................................................................................................. 133
INTRODUCTION TO STUDY 2 AND DEVELOPMENT OF THE HYPOTHESES133
7.1 Introduction ......................................................................................................... 133
7.2 Background of the Second Study ........................................................................ 134
7.3 Research on HRIS Effectiveness ........................................................................ 138
7.4 Reasons for Unsuccessful HRIS Projects ........................................................... 141
7.4 Importance and Challenges of HRIS Project Planning ....................................... 145
7.5 The Planning Fallacy Theories and HRIS Projects: Extending Study 1 ............. 147
7.5.1 Project Completion Predictions ................................................................... 147
7.5.2 Project Outcome Prediction ......................................................................... 147
7.5.3 Predictions of Affective Response towards Outcome Success and Failure . 149
7.6 Conclusion .......................................................................................................... 150
CHAPTER 8 ................................................................................................................. 151

iv
METHOD – STUDY 2 ................................................................................................. 151
8.1 Introduction ......................................................................................................... 151
8.2 Sampling Design ................................................................................................. 151
8.2.1 The Participating Organizations................................................................... 151
8.2.2 The Participants ............................................................................................ 153
8.3 Survey Materials ................................................................................................. 154
8.4 Procedure............................................................................................................. 158
8.5 Research Measures .............................................................................................. 159
8.5.1 Planning Method Measures .......................................................................... 159
8.5.2 Project Completion Time Measures ............................................................. 161
8.5.3 Outcome Success Measures ......................................................................... 162
8.5.4 Measures of Affective Response to Success and Failure ............................. 165
8.5.5 Demographic Measures ................................................................................ 166
8.6 Data Analysis Strategy ........................................................................................ 166
8.6.1 Open-ended Questions ................................................................................. 167
8.7 Conclusion .......................................................................................................... 168
CHAPTER 9 ................................................................................................................. 169
RESULTS – STUDY 2 ................................................................................................. 169
9.1 Introduction ......................................................................................................... 169
9.2. Demographic Characteristics of the Participants ............................................... 169
9.3 HRIS Projects in the Survey ............................................................................... 170
9.4 Planning Methods and Project Outcomes ........................................................... 173
9.5 Correlation Analysis............................................................................................ 176
9.5.1 Bias in Project Completion Prediction ......................................................... 176
9.5.2 Bias in Project Outcome Prediction ............................................................. 179
9.5.3 Other Correlation Results............................................................................. 179
9.6 Hypothesis Testing .............................................................................................. 180
9.6.1 Project Completion Time Predictions .......................................................... 180
9.6.2 Project Outcome Predictions ........................................................................ 181
9.6.3 Predictions of Affective Response towards Outcome Success and Failure . 184
9.7 Conclusion .......................................................................................................... 186
CHAPTER 10 ............................................................................................................... 187
DISCUSSION – STUDY 2 ........................................................................................... 187
10.1 Introduction ....................................................................................................... 187
10.2 Project Completion Time Predictions ............................................................... 187
10.3 Project Outcome Predictions ............................................................................. 188
10.4 Predictions of Affective Reactions towards Outcome Success and Failure ..... 190
10.5 Study 2 Limitations ........................................................................................... 192
10.6 Conclusion ........................................................................................................ 193
CHAPTER 11 ............................................................................................................... 194
GENERAL DISCUSSION............................................................................................ 194
11.1 Introduction ....................................................................................................... 194
11.2 Theories of the Planning Fallacy....................................................................... 194
11.2.1 Completion Time Predictions .................................................................... 195
11.3 Extending the Explanations of the Planning Fallacy ........................................ 196
11.3.1 Outcome Predictions .................................................................................. 196
11.3.2 Confidence in Predictions .......................................................................... 197
11.3.3 Predictions of Affective Reactions towards Outcome Success and Failure
............................................................................................................................... 198
11.3.4 Further Theoretical Development .............................................................. 200
11.4 Implications for Practice ................................................................................... 200
11.5 Limitations and Directions for Future Research ............................................... 202

v
11.6 Conclusion ........................................................................................................ 204
REFERENCES.............................................................................................................. 206
APPENDICES .............................................................................................................. 222

vi
Dedication

To my mother, Aminah Kontak, my father, Othman Ali, my husband, Abdul Razak Abd

Manaf, and all my siblings, especially Suzana, Hamidah, Norhayati and Noraini.

vii
Acknowledgements

Without the dedication and support from these people, the completion of this

thesis would not have been possible. I would like to thank Universiti Utara Malaysia,

for sponsoring my study, and to UWA Business School for awarding me the ad-hoc

scholarship. I owe an immense debt to Dr. Catherine Lees, my main supervisor for all

the energy, care and enthusiasm she devoted to this effort. Her brilliant ideas,

suggestions and above all, her belief in my potential have made me feel confident and

gave me a strong focus from the very beginning of my study. Without her professional

guidance and support, I would not be where I am today. I am also deeply grateful to

Professor John Cordery, my second supervisor, for giving me invaluable support

throughout my candidature.

To my loving and supporting husband, Abdul Razak, my beloved parents,

Aminah and Othman, my parent in-laws, Abd Manaf and Mukhayani, my siblings, in-

laws and all my nephews and nieces, thank you for all your prayers, patience, support,

and word of encouragement for me to keep going till the final end of this journey.

I also would like to thank my colleague at Universiti Utara Malaysia, Dr

Khulida, Sharima, Jannatul, Safinas and my wonderful PhD friends Naomi, Karen,

Hairul, Nazlida, Hazarina, Aspalella, Norizan, Rusmawati, Nor Azlin, Nor Haniza,

Nashrah, Shamsuri and Norhisham Nordin for providing me with many discussions,

constructive comments and suggestions during this PhD journey.

Finally yet importantly, I would like to express my gratitude to all the lecturers

and students during the second semester session 2005 / 2006, in the Faculty of Human

and Social Development, Universiti Utara Malaysia, for participating in my first study.

A word of appreciation also goes to all HR staffs in organizations in Perak, Kedah,

Perlis, Penang and Kuala Lumpur, for their involvement in my second study. Without

their sincere participation, this study will not be as successful as today.

viii
List of Figures

Figure 5.1 Mean initial predicted and actual completion times. (n=708) 64

Figrue 5.2 Mean predicted and actual completion times for the eight 65
weeks project. (n=702)

Figure 5.3 Mean prediction bias of individuals’ project completion time


predictions for the four conditions. Bias is actual minus 66
predicted completion time. (n=708)

Figure 5.4 Mean prediction bias of individuals’ project outcome


predictions for the four conditions. Actual outcome (in marks) 78
minus predicted. (n=689)

Figure 5.5 Mean confidence in predictions of project completion time for


the four conditions. (n=708) 87

Figure 5.6 Mean confidence in predictions of project outcome for the four 95
conditions. (n=689)

Figure 5.7 Mean rating of predicted and experienced affective response 104
for success. (n=422)

Figure 5.8 Mean prediction bias of individuals’ affective response


towards project outcome success for the four conditions. 105
Actual response minus predicted response. (n=422)

Figure 5.9 Mean rating of predicted and experienced affective response 114
for failure. (n=130)

Figure 5.10 Mean prediction bias of individuals’ affective response


towards project outcome failure for the four conditions. Actual 115
response minus predicted response. (n=130)

ix
List of Tables

Table 5.1 Prediction bias for project completion time: A three-level


fully unconditional model 70

Table 5.2a Prediction bias for project completion time: Three-level


model of the effects of manipulations relative to the 74
packed (control) condition

Table 5.2b Prediction bias for project completion time: Three-level


model of the effects of manipulations relative to the 76
outside view condition

Table 5.3 Prediction bias for project outcome: A three-level fully


unconditional model 80

Table 5.4a Prediction bias for project outcome: Three-level model of


the effects of manipulations relative to the packed 83
(control) condition

Table 5.4b Prediction bias for project outcome: Three-level model of


the effects of manipulations relative to the outside view 85
condition

Table 5.5 Confidence in predictions of project completion time: A


three-level fully unconditional model 89

Table 5.6a Confidence in predictions of project completion time:


Three-level model of the effects of manipulations relative 91
to the packed (control) condition

Table 5.6b Confidence in predictions of project completion time:


Three-level model of the effects of manipulations relative 93
to the outside view condition

Table 5.7 Confidence in predictions of project outcome: A three-


level fully unconditional model 97

Table 5.8a Confidence in predictions of project outcome: Three-


level model of the effects of manipulations relative to the 99
packed (control) condition

Table 5.8b Confidence in predictions of project outcome: Three-


level model of the effects of manipulations relative to the 101
outside view condition

Table 5.9 Prediction bias for affective response towards project


outcome success: A three-level fully unconditional 106
model

x
Table 5.10a Prediction bias for affective response towards project
outcome success: Three-level model of the effects of 109
manipulations relative to the packed (control) condition

Table 5.10b Prediction bias for affective response towards project


outcome success: Three-level model of the effects of 111
manipulations relative to the outside view condition

Table 5.11 Prediction bias for affective response towards project


outcome failure: A three-level fully unconditional model 116

Table 5.12a Prediction bias for affective response towards project


outcome failure: Three-level model of the effects of 118
manipulations relative to the packed (control) condition

Table 5.12b Prediction bias for affective response towards project


outcome failure: Three-level model of the effects of 120
manipulations relative to the outside view condition

Table 7.1 Definitions of human resource information systems 134


(HRIS)

Table 7.2 Summary of potential values of human resource 137


information systems (HRIS)

Table 8.1 The four versions of the questionnaires 156

Table 8.2 The nine activity statements 160

Table 8.3 Descriptions of the three approaches 161

Table 8.4 Original and adapted versions of end-user satisfaction 165


items

Table 9.1 HR computer-based applications reported as presently 170


implemented in surveyed organizations

Table 9.2 Participants’ involvement in the project phases (all the 45 172
projects)

Table 9.3a Aspects of system that participants felt most satisfied 174
with

Table 9.3b Aspects of system that participants felt least satisfied 175
with

Table 9.4 Descriptive statistics, scale reliabilities, and correlations 177


of variables for completed projects

Table 9.5 The planning approach used and project completion time 181
(forced choice questions)

xi
Table 9.6 The planning approach used and mean reported
percentage of outcome achieved (standard deviations in 182
brackets)

Table 9.7 The planning approach used and mean predicted of


outcome achieved (standard deviations in brackets) 182

Table 9.8 The planning approach used and mean end-user


satisfaction (standard deviations in brackets) 183

Table 9.9 The planning approach used and mean of overall


satisfaction with the system (standard deviations in 184
brackets)

Table 9.10 Descriptive statistics of participants’ affective response


towards project outcome success and failure 185

Table 9.11 The planning approach used and mean predicted and 186
actual affective response towards project outcome
success (standard deviations in brackets)

xii
List of Appendices

APPENDIX A SAMPLE OF THE EXPERIMENTAL MATERIALS 223


(ENGLISH AND TRANSLATED VERSION) – STUDY 1

APPENDIX B EXPERIMENTAL DESIGN AND WEEKLY 320


RESPONDENTS

APPENDIX C RESULTS OF DEMOGRAPHIC CHARACTERISTICS 321


OF THE PARTICIPANTS – STUDY 1

APPENDIX D SAMPLE OF THREE-LEVEL HIERARCHICAL


REGRESSION EQUATIONS – STUDY 1 322

APPENDIX E RESULTS FOR CONDITIONAL THREE-LEVEL HLM


MODELS – STUDY 1

Appendix E-1 Prediction bias for project completion time: Three- 340
level model of the effects of manipulation relative
to the inside view condition

Appendix E-2 Prediction bias for project outcome: Three-level 342


model of the effects of manipulation relative to the
inside view condition

Appendix E-3 Confidence in predictions of project completion 344


time: Three-level model of the effects of
manipulation relative to the inside view condition

Appendix E-4 Confidence in predictions of project outcome: 346


Three-level model of the effects of manipulation
relative to the inside view condition

Appendix E-5 Prediction bias for affective response towards 348


outcome success: Three-level model of the effects
of manipulations relative to the inside view
condition

Appendix E-6 Prediction bias for affective response towards 350


outcome failure: Three-level model of the effects of
manipulations relative to the inside view condition

APPENDIX F LEGENDS FOR GRAPHICAL ANALYSIS RESULTS – 352


STUDY 1

APPENDIX G SAMPLE OF THE SURVEY MATERIALS – STUDY 2 354

APPENDIX H OPEN-ENDED QUESTION MATERIALS – STUDY 2 430

APPENDIX I DEMOGRAPHIC CHARACTERISTICS OF THE 444


PARTICIPANTS – STUDY 2

xiii
APPENDIX J DESCRIPTIVE STATISTICS OF PROJECTS IN THE 445
SURVEY – STUDY 2

APPENDIX K MEAN PREDICTION BIAS FOR PROJECT


COMPLETION TIME – STUDY 2

Appendix K-1 Mean prediction bias for completed projects 447

Appendix K-2 Mean prediction bias for project at the planning 448
stage

xiv
CHAPTER 1

INTRODUCTION

1.1 Context and Significance

Making accurate predictions about when a project can be completed, what a

project can deliver, and how successful it will be are important aspects of project

planning and project management. This research investigates how to improve the

accuracy of such predictions made during the planning stages of project management.

Overestimating task completion time and budget may result in failure to convince top

management to approve the project, but underestimating these things may lead to failure

to deliver the expected outcome within time and budget, even if the project is approved

(Morgenshtern, Raz, & Dvir, 2007). Making inaccurate completion time and outcome

estimates can have economic, social and personal costs including disputes, litigation,

and total abandonment of a project (Aibinu & Jagboro, 2002; Calisir & Gumussoy,

2005; Ewusi-Mensah & Przasnyski, 1991; Manavazhi & Adhikari, 2002; Sambasivan &

Soon, 2007).

In information systems (IS) development projects such as human resource

information systems (HRIS), inaccurate predictions could lead to a number of problems

such as failure to provide what the organization needs, inaccurate or insufficient data,

disappointing end-functionalities, and lack of interaction with other systems, which in

turn can lead to further problems. Thus, finding ways to improve the accuracy of project

outcome predictions will assist organizations to achieve greater economic certainty and

effectiveness in the use of their funds.

In practice, various methods and techniques have been tried to improve the

performance of project prediction, and these include among others network techniques

like the critical path model (CPM) and the program evaluation and review technique

1
(PERT) (Cleland & Ireland, 2002; Hardie, 2001; Lock, 2003); estimation by analogy,

parametric models such as the constructive cost model (COCOMO), expert estimation,

and artificial intelligence techniques (Hill, Thomas, & Allen, 2000; Hughes, 1996;

Morgenshtern et al., 2007); work breakdown structure (WBS); milestones schedules,

and bar chart or Gantt charts (Cleland & Ireland, 2002; Lock, 2003; Meredith & Mantel,

2006; Rosenau, 1998).

Despite the wide range of project planning techniques and methodologies being

introduced and utilized, evidence of prediction problems among IS projects continues to

appear. Cats-Baril and Thompson (1995) reported that 20 percent of all IT projects are

scrapped before completion, and 80 percent of the completed projects finished late, over

budget or failed to meet users’ expectations. In 1999, Ambler reported that since the

1980s the failure rate in the development of large-scale software projects was 85

percent. Keil, Mann and Rai (2000) found that 30 to 40 percent of IS projects showed

some degree of escalation, and in a sample of 76 projects analyzed, Calisir and

Gumussoy (2005) found that the average IT budget overrun was 19 percent and

schedule overrun 49 percent.

The formal methods for reducing prediction error in project management have

been widely discussed in the project management literature, but very little research has

been devoted to the behavioral aspects of the problem. Research into how project

managers make plans and decisions during project planning, and their impact on the

success of the project is still limited.

The main concern of this thesis is the role of human judgment in making

accurate project planning predictions, with particular application to human resource

information system (HRIS) projects. Even though formal planning tools can be useful,

McFarland (1981) argued that these tools contribute little to reducing uncertainty and

highlighting problems at the early stage of the planning. The planning tools do allow

2
project managers to structure the sequence of tasks, but it is very difficult to predict the

time, cost and technical performance simultaneously. As argued by Hill, Thomas and

Allen (2000), the effectiveness of project management techniques depends heavily on

accurate duration prediction. Making predictions or forecasts is essentially a human

task, and therefore, the way projects are run needs to be considered from a perspective

that includes the psychological and behavioral aspects of the problem.

Predictions are guesses regarding aspects of future events, made on the basis of

available knowledge. The accuracy of predictions about performance of a task, such as a

project, may be affected by the extent of uncertainties associated with the task to be

predicted. The uncertainties are often associated, as in the case of an IS project like

HRIS, with the definition of requirements, choice of technology available, and the

users’ needs. To make accurate predictions planners need to cope with many vague and

conflicting requirements, as well as diverse users’ skills and expectations. According to

Smith (2007), the uncertain nature of a project always leaves planners to deal with the

unknown, and subject to streams of unpredictable and unexplainable events that may

prevent project success.

Because of the uncertain nature of events that might delay or otherwise disrupt a

project, Lovallo and Kahneman (2003) believed that many planners tend to establish

what they think would be the “most likely” scenario and assume that its outcome is, in

fact, the most likely outcome. This phenomenon is known as the planning fallacy. Even

though the concept of the planning fallacy is relatively new to the project management

literature, it has been researched and documented in the psychology literature.

The planning fallacy has been described as “the tendency to hold a confident

belief that one’s own project will proceed as planned, even while knowing that the vast

majority of similar projects have run late” (Buehler, Griffin, & Ross, 1994, p.366).

According to Lovallo and Kahneman (2003), it is a tendency to make decisions based

3
on delusional optimism rather than on a rational weighting of gains, losses and

probabilities. In trying to complete tasks, many of us tend to be optimistic and

overconfident.

Two compelling but conflicting theories have been put forward in the literature

to explain the planning fallacy. One line of research, termed the inside-outside

approach, has shown that focusing on the outside perspective, that is, a broader context

that includes relevant past experiences, the experiences of others, and background

events, produces more accurate predictions regarding task completion time than using

the inside perspective, which involves focusing only on the project at hand to make the

prediction (Lovallo & Kahneman, 2003). In contrast, research on an approach termed

enumerating, or unpacking, has suggested that taking an inside perspective that

specifically involves thinking more about the task at hand and its separate components

also results in a reduction of the planning fallacy (Kruger & Evans, 2004). As theories

of the planning fallacy are contradictory, and logically lead to different prescriptions for

planning processes, understanding of the planning fallacy can be extended by testing the

two theories against each other in the same study, to investigate the strategy that is

better in reducing the planning fallacy. These two theories are tested against each other

for the first time in the experiment that forms Study 1 in this thesis.

Moreover, the planning fallacy has been observed in a wide variety of activities,

from novel laboratory tasks to large-scale industrial projects (Buehler et al., 1994;

Camerer & Lovallo, 1999; Kahneman & Lovallo, 1993; Lovallo & Kahneman, 2003),

but to date there has been no research to understand the planning fallacy with respect to

the detailed planning of specific types of projects, such as IS projects. For example,

projects involving human resource information systems (HRIS). There is evidence

showing that many HRIS projects have encountered problems during implementation

and fail to deliver the expected outcome, where the systems are said to have low

4
benefits, inaccurate or insufficient data, to be difficult to access, have disappointing end

functionalities, and a lack of interaction with other systems (Caplan, 2004; Kavanagh,

Gueutal, & Tannenbaum, 1990; Macy, 2004; Russell, 2006). Little is known about

whether issues in planning and development in HRIS projects contribute to these

failures. The second study reported in this thesis is a field study that applies theories of

the planning fallacy to a sample of HRIS projects and measures the extent to which their

outcomes fall short of expectations.

The data for both studies were collected in Malaysia. The rapid economic

development in Malaysia made it an ideal setting for this research as it provides a

context in which there is rapid change in the spread and adoption of IT. As Malaysia is

moving forward towards achieving a fully developed and industrialized country by the

year 2020, the effort of making science and technology an integral component of socio-

economic planning and development continues. During the Eighth Malaysia Plan (2001-

2005), efforts were on providing a stronger platform for the country’s transition towards

a knowledge-based economy by promoting information and communication

technologies (ICT) as a strategic driver to support and contribute directly to the growth

of the economy as well as to enhance quality of life. These efforts continue in the Ninth

Malaysia Plan (2006-2010), but with greater adoption and usage of ICT to allow greater

expansion of ICT-related industries and services.

A total of RM12.9 billion has been allocated for ICT-related programs and

projects during this plan, with a major portion of the allocation for computerization of

government ministries and agencies as well as for the supply and maintenance of

computers and Internet access. Providing adequate and reliable ICT infrastructure with

extensive capacity to support access and delivery of information remains a major focus

in this plan, and is seen as an effort to strengthen Malaysia’s position as a preferred

global location for ICT investment and as a market leader for ICT solutions.

5
The Malaysian government also acknowledges that having technology per se is

not sufficient if the human resources are not technologically fluent. Therefore, various

education, training, and skills development programs have been planned and expanded

to cover schools, pre-university levels, and the higher education institutions to keep up

with the demand for high quality, skilled and creative ICT personnel. It is expected that

the total ICT workforce will increase at a rate of 10.4 percent per year from 183,204 in

2005 to 300,000 in 2010 (EPU, 2006). At the same time, both public and private sectors

are also encouraged to provide a conducive environment for e-learning as a work

culture, which allows for knowledge sharing and knowledge application at the

workplace.

In summary, the Malaysian government has acknowledged the importance of IT

in meeting future challenges. Thus, IT projects have high strategic expectations

associated with them in the context of a developing economy such as Malaysia. This

research can make an effective contribution to our understanding of the best way to plan

for successful IT projects. This is a broader contribution that extends beyond the

Malaysian context. This study should benefit both scholars and practitioners regarding

methods for reducing prediction errors during project planning, and should also apply

beyond the HRIS context to project planning generally. A literature search reveals

limited empirical studies on the issue of judgmental biases in project management, and

none to my knowledge has been conducted on HRIS projects.

1.2 Scope and Aim of Research

The main focus of this study is twofold: first, to examine and compare two

theories of the planning fallacy - the inside / outside view and the unpacking approach,

and second, to extend the application of planning fallacy explanations to real projects in

organizations involving HRIS projects. Specifically, I aim to identify which approaches

are best utilized to reduce the planning fallacy, and how these approaches affect the

6
success of the project in terms of accuracy of predictions of project duration and

outcome.

The study was conducted in two phases using two different research designs.

The first study involved a longitudinal experiment with 162 student group projects

(involving 772 student participants). The second study involved a survey of 45 HRIS

projects in place in organizations.

The first study was experimental because this is the first empirical study to

examine and compare the effects of the inside / outside view and the unpacking

approach to the planning fallacy in a single study. The first study also compares these

theories for the first time on predictions other than time to completion of a project,

specifically on prediction of project outcome, on confidence in predictions of project

completion time and outcome, and on predictions of affective reactions to project

outcome success and failure.

It was also important for the first study to demonstrate that the planning fallacy

occurs in a Malaysian context and culture, and to show that previous effects of the

inside / outside views and unpacking approaches could be replicated in that context. The

sample for the first study was recruited from the Faculty of Human and Social

Development, Universiti Utara Malaysia. Students’ predictions about their projects were

collected on a weekly basis over a two-month period.

For the second study, which was a cross-sectional study, data were collected

from firms located in the four states of Peninsular Malaysia: Perak, Penang, Kedah and

Perlis. In selecting the research sample, there were no restrictions on the types of

industry, but the firms had to have an HRIS in place, and the respondents had to be

involved directly with the HRIS project planning. The aim for the second study is to

explore which of the planning approaches are commonly used by planners during HRIS

project planning, and to examine how the approach used during the planning process

7
relates to the accuracy of predictions about the success or failure of the project in terms

of project completion time and outcome.

1.3 Organization of Chapters in the Thesis

This chapter is the first of the eleven chapters in this thesis. Chapter 2 gives a

general review of the literature on project management in general and IT/IS projects in

particular. The concepts of project success and failure, and how these can be measured

are also presented. Discussion in Chapter 2 continues with an explanation of the role

and challenges as well as issues in project planning. The chapter concludes with a

discussion on biases that often influence the predictions made during planning.

Based on the literature reviewed in Chapter 2, Chapter 3 proposes testing

theories of the planning fallacy using predictions made during project planning. Two

theories of the planning fallacy, the inside versus outside view (Lovallo & Kahneman,

2003) and the unpacking approach (Kruger & Evans, 2004) are explored, leading to the

development of the research hypotheses for Study 1. Several propositions are made

regarding the effect of the inside/outside view and the unpacking approach on

predictions of project completion time and outcome, on the confidence in prediction of

project completion time and outcome, and on the prediction of affective reactions to

project outcome success and failure.

Chapter 4 describes the method for Study 1, namely the research design and

procedure. The chapter reports the selection of the participants, the experimental

materials used, and the experimental process and data collection procedure. Chapter 4

concludes with a brief outline of the strategies and procedures that were used to analyze

data collected from the experiment.

Chapter 5 presents the results from testing the research hypotheses for Study 1.

There are reports of the multilevel hierarchical linear modeling (HLM) analyses and

8
graphical analyses. The hypotheses were tested using HLM, and their results are

summarized in a number of tables to facilitate interpretation.

Chapter 6 discusses the interpretation of the research findings from Study 1. The

findings from this study are compared to those found in past research reviewed in

Chapter 2. New findings are also discussed. Chapter 6 concludes with a discussion on

the limitations of Study 1.

Chapter 7 presents an introduction of Study 2. Literature on Human Resource

Information Systems (HRIS) is reviewed, explaining their definition and purpose,

potential value and usage in the organization. The challenges of planning for an HRIS

system are also discussed. The chapter concludes with the development of the research

hypotheses for Study 2.

Chapter 8 describes the research method for Study 2. The chapter reports the

selection of the respondents, sample types and size, the development of the

questionnaire for the research, and the survey process and data collection procedure.

Chapter 8 ends with a brief description of the strategies and procedures that were used

to analyze data collected from the survey.

Chapter 9 reports the results for Study 2. There are reports of the descriptive

statistical analysis and bivariate correlation analysis. The results are summarized in a

number of tables to facilitate interpretation.

Chapter 10 discusses the interpretation of the research findings for Study 2. The

findings are compared to those found in the past research reviewed in Chapters 2 and 7.

New findings are also discussed. The chapter ends with a discussion on limitations of

Study 2.

Chapter 11, the final chapter, presents the general discussion and conclusion of

the two studies and their implications for both researchers and practitioners. Chapter 11

9
concludes with a discussion of the benefits of the two studies and some suggestions for

future research.

10
CHAPTER 2

A REVIEW OF PROJECT SUCCESS, PROJECT PLANNING AND

PLANNING BIASES

2.1 Introduction

This chapter sets out issues related to project planning as presented and

discussed in the project management and psychology literatures. These issues are

reviewed to provide theoretical foundation for the research. The chapter begins by

describing the characteristics of projects in general and information systems (IS)

projects in particular. Then, how project success and failure are commonly measured in

the literature is discussed, followed by findings from past studies on the causes of IS

project failure. The chapter then reviews the role of planning as well as issues and

challenges surrounding the planning process. Common biases associated with planning

are also discussed. The chapter concludes by highlighting areas of research to be

addressed.

2.2 The Characteristics of Projects

A project is a unique endeavor - a special task that has not been done before

(Dvir, Raz, & Shenhar, 2003). Projects have three-dimensional objectives –

performance specification, the time schedule and the cost budget; they involve

resources; are accomplished within an organization (Rosenau, 1998); and are designed

to attain specific results (Katagiri & Turner, 2004). Gemimo, Reich and Sauer (2007)

view projects as complex, multidimensional phenomena with many factors interacting

in their execution. Even though all projects are in a sense unique, Perminova,

Gustafsson and Wikstrom (2008) argued that projects do share some similarities. All

projects have some restriction in time, costs and scope as well as demand for quality,

and involve a high level of uncertainty.

11
With regards to information systems (IS) projects, some authors believe that IS

projects possess certain characteristics that make them different from other projects and

increase the chances of their failure. IS by definition is a “combination of computer

hardware, communication technology and software designed to handle information

related to one or more business processes” (Flowers, 1996, p.3). Examples include

among others accounting systems, personnel systems, and sales order processing

systems.

Ewusi-Mensah (1997) has identified three main characteristics of IS projects,

and these include a requirement for an intense collaboration of three groups of

stakeholders, namely IS staff, end-users, and management; inherent risks and

uncertainties that are difficult to assess at the start of the project; and their capital

intensive nature as IS become increasingly critical to the survival and well-being of

companies.

Because the practical management of IS projects is difficult and challenging, and

I will argue that their outcomes have often been disappointing, it is important to

consider how IS project success and failure can be measured.

2.3 The Concepts of Information System (IS) Project Success and Failure

Projects are more successful when criteria for judging success are defined and

agreed upon by all the participants (Wateridge, 1995, 1998). If the project team and

major stakeholders fail to understand or have differing views on what success

constitutes, the project will end up with unclear scope, inappropriate measurement,

specification changes, delays and other issues (Hartman & Ashrafi, 2002).

But what does project success or failure mean? Though many have written about

IS success and failure, there is no generally agreed definition of these terms. This is

because success or failure is subjective in nature and the definition often depends on

who is asked. A project can be a success for one party and a disaster for another.

12
Many authors have suggested time, cost and quality outcome as success criteria

(M. C. Jones & Harrison, 1996; Rai & Al-Hindi, 2000; Robey, Smith, & Vijayasarathy,

1993). However, Wateridge (1998) disagreed that using these three criteria (being on

time, within budget and to technical specification) is enough to define success, even if

time, cost and specifications are important for IS projects. He argued that there are

instances where these three criteria have not been met but the project is considered to be

successful. Success and failure, he argued, cannot be seen as “black and white,” because

a project may not always be seen as completely successful or a complete failure as

different people may see the project outcome differently.

Based on his study on the meaning of project success with project managers,

sponsors, users, and system analysts, Wateridge found that all parties agreed that

budget, timescale, and user requirements were important in judging the success of IS

projects. However, users were more concerned about the system meeting their needs

and being happy with the system, while project managers viewed commercial success

and meeting quality as more important criteria. In other words, project managers focus

on the project process, such as meeting the time and budget constraints, while users

focus on the project product, such as delivering a system that makes them happy.

Looking from the failure perspective, Yeo (2002) considered an IS project as a

failure if the system is under-utilized or abandoned. Flowers (1996, p.4) regards an IS

project as a failure if any of the following situations occur: 1) when the system as a

whole does not operate as expected and its overall performance is sub-optimal; 2) if, on

implementation, it does not perform as originally intended or if it is so user-hostile that

it is rejected by users and is under-utilized; 3) the cost of IS development exceeds the

benefits the system may bring throughout its useful life; or 4) the information system

development is abandoned due to either problems with complexity of the system or the

management of the project.

13
Lyytinen and Hirschheim (1987) classified the notion of IS failure into four

major categories: correspondence failure, process failure, interaction failure and

expectation failure. An IS project is considered failed if the system design objectives are

not met (correspondence failure); the IS cannot be developed within the allocated

budget and / or time (process failure); the level of end-user usage of the information

system is low (interaction failure); or the system is unable to meet its stakeholders’

requirements, expectations or values (expectation failure).

To summarize, project success and failure are difficult to define as they mean

different things to different people. Project managers may view success as the survival

of their project whereas users are more concerned whether the system meets their need

or not. Therefore, how success and failure are defined and who evaluates them, affects

the final judgment of success or failure. In the following section, causes of project

failure are reviewed.

2.4 Past Research on Causes of Information System (IS) Project Failure

According to Pinto and Mantel (1990), it is difficult to gain a complete

understanding of what causes a project to fail. The difficulties are due to factors such as

the difficulty for people to agree on how project failure is defined; much of the research

conducted has been based on theoretical concepts or anecdote; causes of failure may

vary by type of project being studied; and the causes may depend on the stage of project

life cycle where different stages might have different causes of failure.

A review of studies on IS project failure reveals that failure could be caused by a

combination of factors such as unrealistic expectations; lack of resources; uncooperative

customers; weak management of contractors (Brown & Jones, 1998); poor risk

management (Jiang & Klein, 1999; McFarlan, 1981); lack of user involvement; poor

organizational communication and information sharing (Peterson & Kim, 2003); lack of

general agreement on what the new system’s goals and objectives should be in order to

14
satisfy the organization’s requirements; a weak or problematic project team which is

caused by lack of leadership and active interaction among all the three IS project groups

– IS staff, end-users, and management; poor project management where there is lack of

a measurement system to measure progress and to identify risks; lack of experience and

knowledge of the relevant application; lack of senior management involvement where

there is no active participation in monitoring progress and in making decisions at critical

stages; and escalation of project cost and time to completion (Ewusi-Mensah, 1997;

Ewusi-Mensah & Przasnyski, 1991, 1994).

Though the causes for IS project failure portrayed in the literature are

multidimensional issues, Murray (2001) argued that many of the causes of project

failure are universal with the majority of the causes generally being similar from project

to project. The similarity is due to the nature of the project itself. Sharing a similar view,

Raymond and Bergeron (2008) also believe that all projects share a number of things in

common: all need to be managed, where they need to be planned, staffed, organized,

monitored, controlled and evaluated. In fact, planning has been mentioned repeatedly in

studies as being critical for project success (Aladwani, 2002; Morgenshtern et al., 2007;

Pinto & Prescott, 1990; Yeo, 2002).

Pinto and Prescott (1990) argued that a variety of project critical success factors

studied in the literature often fall into two distinct subgroups: one is related to initial

project planning and the other is concerned with subsequent tactical operationalization.

Project mission, top management support, schedules and client consultation for

instance, are all consistent with the role of planning whereas personnel, technical tasks,

client acceptance, monitoring and feedback, communication and trouble-shooting are

concerned with project development and operationalization once initial plans have been

determined. Based on their survey of 408 project managers, Pinto and Prescott found

15
that planning factors were of great relative importance for project success and continue

to drive tactics throughout the project life cycle.

The impact of planning on project success has also been illustrated in other

studies. Yeo (2002) found that a significant proportion of problems faced in information

system projects are related to project planning issues. Aladwani (2002) also found that

planning is a major contributor to IT project success, in that project planning is a

significant mediator of the project uncertainty-success link. Dvir, Raz and Shenhar

(2003) discovered that the amount of effort spent in defining the project’s goals and

functional requirements has a significant positive relationship with project success.

Planning at a detailed level (shorter activities, smaller tasks) has also been found to

contribute to the accuracy of estimates and reduces the size of estimation errors

(Morgenshtern et al., 2007).

In summary, planning is important in ensuring project success as numerous

decisions are made at this stage. Task duration, types and number of activities needed to

be carried out, and the amount of resources and budget are all determined and estimated

during the initiation and planning stage of the project. These estimations then provide a

basis for further planning, control and decision making. In the following section, the

importance and role of planning in projects are explained.

2.5 The Role of Planning

Planning is “the process of thinking through and making explicit the objectives,

goals, and strategies necessary to bring the project through its life cycle to a successful

termination when the project’s product, service or process takes its rightful place in the

execution of project owner strategies” (Cleland & Ireland, 2002, p.310).It is a rational

determination of how a project can be initiated, sustained and terminated.

In the project management literature, planning has been considered as an

essential element that determines the success or failure of a project. Although planning

16
does not guarantee project success and some believe that forward planning may

sometimes reduce the accuracy of time prediction (Buehler, Griffin, & MacDonald,

1997), lack of planning will probably guarantee failure (Dvir et al., 2003). As

highlighted in most project management literature, plans are useful, even necessary

because the complete absence of plans would make project completion impossible.

The main purpose of project planning is to reduce uncertainty (Dvir et al., 2003),

and to ensure that projects are delivered on time, within budget and to the quality

expected by the customer (Yeates, 1991). A plan is needed to coordinate and

communicate project tasks, to satisfy requirements imposed by others such as customers

or top management, to provide a basis for monitoring activity, and to help avoid

problems (Rosenau, 1998). Plans also help “to establish a set of directions in sufficient

detail to tell the project team what exactly must be done, when it must be done, and

what resources to use in order to produce the deliverables of the project successfully”

(Meredith & Mantel, 2006, p.235).

Because project planning is intended to determine whether a project is able to

accomplish the performance specifications on or before the time limit and within

budget, a detailed plan usually contains all the tasks to be done, the resources needed,

the methods to be followed in the project and its management and control, the tools to

be used in support of the methods, the work schedule, the budget, and an identification

of potential risks and contingencies (Lientz & Rea, 1998).

Though planning is considered an important element that determines the success

or failure of the project, it is also one of the most challenging activities. What makes

planning a challenging process is discussed next.

2.6 The Planning Process: Issues and Challenges

Planning processes “define and refine objectives and select the best of the

alternative courses of action to attain the objective that the project was undertaken to

17
address” (Zwikael & Sadeh, 2007, p.756). Maylor (2005) has identified four main

stages of planning processes namely identifying the elements of activities, determining

the sequence of the activities, estimating time and resources, and presenting the plan in

an understandable format. To have a good likelihood of survival, all phases must be

carefully planned out at the beginning of the project. This is because decisions made at

the initiation and planning stages are said to impact the organization more compared to

decisions at other times during the project management process (McCray, Purvis, &

McCray, 2002).

Because planning deals with the uncertainty of the project, Maylor (2005)

argued that estimation is the key part of planning. If estimation is considered to be the

important element in planning, perhaps the most common project-related problems arise

due to incorrect estimations for example, of the amount of effort that must go into the

project. Problems such as project overrun, unrealistic expectations, underestimation of

the project’s scope, and hardware and software budget short falls, which have often

been highlighted in the literature, may result from poor estimation.

The complexity and scale of the project has been found to be related to the

estimation error. In a study conducted by Morgenshtern, Raz and Dvir (2007), it was

found that more complex projects with higher uncertainty, as indicated by the project

size (duration, effort, and number of potential users), the complexity of the application

and the level of innovativeness needed, are more likely to entail larger error in

estimation of duration and effort.

Some researchers believe that the complexity of projects has made the intuitive

process of decision making more attractive rather than the rational (Bukszar, 1988;

McCray et al., 2002; McFarlan, 1981; Purvis, McCray, & Roberts, 2004; White &

Fortune, 2002). In a survey of 118 project managers and team members who were

directly and actively involved in IT projects, Purvis, McCray and Roberts (2004)

18
discovered that no models or formal methods were mentioned when constructing

estimates for project cost or durations. In fact, respondents did not deny using intuition

for critical decision making. Whilst many would expect less experienced managers to

utilize intuitive processes, Leybourne and Sadler-Smith (2006) found that more

experienced managers use intuition and improvisation more than do less experienced

managers.

The complexity and mixed success of formal planning tools may also explain

why people prefer the intuitive process. White and Fortune (2002) found only small

numbers of project management methods, tools and techniques are used, and

respondents reported drawbacks to the methods, tools and techniques they had used. In

another study, Raymond and Bergeron (2008) found that using formal techniques like a

project management information system (PMIS) does not lead to greater impacts on

project performance and the PMIS itself has no direct influence upon project success.

These empirical findings suggest that judgment under uncertainty continues to

play an essential role in project decisions especially in predicting events such as the

success of a project. However, judgment can be subject to biases, which may underlie

errors in predictions made during planning. The kinds of biases that have been found to

occur in planning processes are discussed next.

2.7 Planning Under Uncertainty

Though project planners realize that projects are risky undertakings that may not

always end as planned and have the tendency to suffer unexpected outcomes, many are

still overly optimistic in their predictions of the success of their projects. Evidence of

optimistic prediction bias in planning has been observed in projects from everyday

events such as completing major school assignments (Buehler et al., 1997; Buehler et

al., 1994; Newby-Clark, Ross, Buehler, Koehler, & Griffin, 2000) to large scale

19
industrial and commercial projects like the completion of hydroelectric dams and public

transportation systems (Hall, 1980; Schnaars, 1989).

The evidence for unrealistic optimism is considerable. People are found to

underestimate how long it will take them to accomplish tasks and projects (Buehler et

al., 1997; Buehler et al., 1994; Buehler, Griffin, & Ross, 2002; Byram, 1997; Newby-

Clark et al., 2000), overestimate the impact of future events on their future feelings

(Lam, McFarland, Ross, & Cheung, 2005), and overestimate how likely they are to

experience a wide variety of positive life events (Perloff & Fetzer, 1986; Weinstein,

1980). There is also evidence that people tend to shift from optimism in their personal

predictions across time relative to the event being predicted. Most people tend to be

very optimistic in the beginning but as the outcome draws near, they tend to abandon

their optimism by displaying realism or even pessimism (Gilovich, Kerr, & Mecdvec,

1993; Liberman & Trope, 1998; Shepperd, Findley-Klein, Kwavnick, Walker, & Perez,

2000; Shepperd, Grace, Cole, & Klein, 2005).

According to researchers, these distortions of reality occur because of errors in

the way people process information (Buehler & Griffin, 2003; Buehler et al., 1994;

Weinstein, 1980). Specifically, the lack of certain information needed to make accurate

future predictions tends to introduce this error. Human ability to process the information

is also found to be limited and bounded by various biases. These combinations of

information search and processing biases introduce significant error to human judgment.

Thus, predictions can be characterized by two general shortcomings: people tend to

make overly optimistic predictions, and people tend to be overly confident in their

predictions (Dunning, 2007).

In the next sections, several biases that relate to specific predictions and their

causes are discussed.

20
2.7.1 Biases in Completion Time Predictions

To plan and schedule a project, planners must predict the time needed to perform

the project-related activities. However, more often than not, planners tend to make

predictions that are overly optimistic, claiming that they need less time to complete

tasks than actually turns out to be the case. Even when they are aware that their past

projects took a longer time to finish, they still believe their current project will finish as

planned. This specific form of optimistic bias is known as the planning fallacy. The

Planning fallacy is where people stick to an optimistic completion prediction even while

knowing that similar projects have run late in the past (Buehler et al., 1994). Evidence

of the planning fallacy has been observed in many studies involving a wide variety of

tasks such as completing income tax forms, school assignments, and holiday shopping,

and in each case, a longer time was taken than initially predicted (e.g., Buehler et al.,

1997; Buehler et al., 1994).

One explanation that has been offered for the occurrence of the planning fallacy

is that people tend to focus on the specific task at hand and regard it as a unique event,

disassociated from previous similar tasks (Buehler & Griffin, 2003; Buehler et al., 1997;

Buehler et al., 1994, 2002). Lovallo and Kahneman (2003) note that in 1979 Kahneman

and Tversky suggested two types of information that people can use to predict task

completion time, singular information or distributional information. Singular

information, also termed the inside perspective or the case-based view, is information

related to the specific task at hand whereas distributional information, or the outside

perspective or the class-based view, is information about previous similar tasks.

Neglecting distributional information or the outside perspective when predicting task

completion time has been suggested as a possible cause of the planning fallacy, because

prediction that is based on the inside view, a view that portrays the progression of a

21
specific scenario from the present to the future, could lead people to neglect the vast

number of ways in which their plan may go wrong.

Lovallo and Kahneman (2003) further argued that if individuals view their task

in a broader context that includes relevant past experiences, the experiences of others

and background events that may influence progress, a more realistic prediction can be

made. This is because the outside, distributional, or class-based view focuses on the set

of “comparable instances and produces estimates that are less extremely optimistic than

the singular estimates” (Griffin & Buehler, 2005, p. 758). In other words, these

researchers have claimed that a more realistic prediction can be made if individuals

think more about past experiences similar to the event at hand, that is, if they make

more connection between past experiences and possible future experiences. The general

difference between these two approaches is then “whether individuals treat the target

task as a unique case or as an instance of a category of similar problems” (Buehler et al.,

2002, p. 253).

There are a number of findings that support this inside / outside explanation of

the planning fallacy. When people are encouraged to consider past similar experiences,

they tend to become more accurate in their prediction and the planning fallacy is

reduced (Buehler et al., 1994, 2002). According to Buehler and Griffin (2003) a detailed

plan such as a planner’s concrete step-by-step plan for implementation actually

heightens the tendency to make overly optimistic forecasts and projections. This is

because planning processes are tied more directly to stated predictions than to actual

behavior as the planner has greater control over a predicted completion time than over

actual completion time. These findings also indicate that people make overly optimistic

predictions, partly because they overweight their specific plans for a given future

project and underweight more general distributional information. For these reasons,

Buehler and Griffin (2003), like Lovallo and Kahneman (2003) suggested that

22
individuals who are seeking a more realistic forecast are advised to adopt an approach to

prediction that involves considering the distribution of completion times for related

projects or the views of natural, outside observers and give less weight to their own

plans.

Though the inside / outside view suggested that the planning fallacy occurs

because people neglect past relevant experience, Roy, Christenfeld and McKenzie

(2005) believe that underestimation is not due to people neglecting or ignoring accurate

past experiences, but to inaccurate recall, or what they termed “memory bias.” Memory

bias is based on the assumption that “people use memories of duration when making

predictions of future duration and that biases in these memories cause biases in

prediction” (Roy et al., 2005, p. 749). In other words, the incorrect prediction of future

duration is caused by the incorrect memories of past durations. They argue that people

rarely check the precise starting and ending time for particular tasks, and therefore

people base predictions on memories of past estimated duration instead of past actual

duration. Because people forget some events in the past, they remember previous tasks

as taking less time than they actually did, and so they tend to underestimate similar

future durations. Griffin and Buehler (2005, p.759) however, disagreed with Roy,

Christenfeld and McKenzie’s proposition. They argued that general beliefs about past

projects do not just depend on memory, but could be from observations of other

people’s experiences or from general knowledge.

On the other hand, Thomas, Newstead and Handley (2003) and Thomas and

Handley (2008) found that people tend to misestimate future task duration even when

they consider information about their previous task performance, and suggested an

anchoring and adjustment explanation. Anchoring has been known to be one of the

strongest and most prevalent of cognitive biases. Anchoring happens when people make

their best guess, and this anchors their thinking, which prevents them from imagining

23
circumstances that differ substantially from it. In their studies, they found that using

previous task duration as an anchor for prediction lead to misestimating of future task

duration: underestimation occurred with the shorter duration (low) anchor and

overestimation occurred with the longer (high) anchor (Thomas & Handley, 2008;

Thomas et al., 2003).

Another explanation for the occurrence of the planning fallacy which seems

conflict with the inside / outside view was offered by Kruger and Evans (2004). Kruger

and Evans (2004) argue that taking an inside perspective, which involves thinking more

about the specific task at hand, does not always result in a planning fallacy. Indeed, they

found that prompting individuals to think more about the specific task at hand

sometimes caused the planning fallacy to be reduced, not increased. Kruger and Evans

(2004) believe that the reason why people are unable to make accurate predictions

regarding task completion time is because they do not unpack those tasks into their

various subcomponents. The activity of enumerating and breaking down tasks into their

various subcomponents has been termed “unpacking” (Tversky & Koehler, 1994). The

idea of enumerating is to make the subcomponents of the task salient when making

estimations instead of looking at the task as a whole. By doing that, it should increase

the overall completion time estimate and, as a result, decrease the planning fallacy.

Kruger and Evans (2004) found that when individuals are induced to unpack

multifaceted tasks like preparing for a holiday, shopping, getting ready for a date,

formatting a document or preparing food, completion time estimates increased and as a

result, the planning fallacy was decreased.

Kruger and Evans (2004) based their explanations on the logic of support theory

(Rottenstreich & Tversky, 1997; Tversky & Koehler, 1994). Support theory is a

descriptive theory of subjective probability where the subjective probability of an event

depends on the explicitness of its description (Tversky & Koehler, 1994). “Like the

24
measured length of a coastline, which increases as a map becomes more detailed, the

perceived likelihood of an event increases as its description becomes more specific”

(Tversky & Koehler, 1994, p. 565). Rather than attaching probabilities to the event,

support theory attaches subjective probability to the event’s descriptions called

hypotheses. Each hypothesis has a support value, and to increase this support, the

hypothesis should be unpacked into its exclusive components. For example, when

people are asked to unpack a multifaceted category like “death due to natural causes”

into its subcomponents (e.g., death due to heart attack, cancer or other related natural

cause) the subjective probability of the event increases.

Like Kruger and Evans, Wilson, Wheatley, Meyers, Gilbert and Axsom (2000)

suggest that it is the way people think about the focal event that causes the planning

fallacy, not a failure to think about past experiences. According to this focalism

hypothesis, thinking more about non-focal events, even ones that will occure in the

future, such as other assignments they have that week, or how busy their life will be that

could impede their progress, should result in more realistic time completion predictions.

They believe that people could correct their prediction in this manner even if they do

not have any past experience with the task.

It is important to note that Kruger and Evans’s (2004) notion of enumerating

activities is in accordance with prescriptions on project planning in the project

management literature. In project planning, work breakdown structure (WBS) is used to

divide the overall project into work elements that represent singular work units,

assigned either to the organization or to an outside agency, such as a contractor (Cleland

& Ireland, 2002). Work breakdown structure is also known as “chunking” or

“unbundling” (Maylor, 2005). According to Maylor (2005), projects can be broken

down in several ways. The first level of breakdown is known as activity breakdown,

where activities to be undertaken are broken down into major groups. Functional

25
breakdown is the second form of breakdown, where the project is divided into its

functional areas. Another type of breakdown is known as physical grouping, where in

the example of IT projects, the hardware and the software issues are split. Apart from

breaking down tasks, Smith (2007) has introduced the concept of a “risk register”, a

schedule that lists all the hazards faced by a project, whatever their form, when dealing

with uncertainties.

The underlying philosophy of the work breakdown structure is to divide the

project into work packages, the smallest manageable work elements of the project, that

are assignable, and for which accountability can be expected. Also, it is a way of

dividing a project to ensure the completeness, compatibility, and continuity of all work

that is required for successful completion of the project. The work breakdown structure

helps for better planning as it provides necessary information to “assign responsibilities

within the team, allocate tasks to specific resources, estimate time against tasks to help

understand the overall timing within the project, create a budget for the project, provide

a structure for monitoring and reporting progress against budgets, predict spending to

the end of the project / milestones, and recognize and understand problems within the

plan” (R. Jones, 2007, p.89).

Even though unpacking seems similar to the idea of decomposition in judgment,

Kruger and Evans (2004) noted that unpacking and decomposition are different in terms

of their operationalization, underlying theory and predictions. Decomposition involves

breaking a category down literally and asking participants to make separate estimates

for each of several components of the category, which are then aggregated. Unpacking

involves breaking down the task only symbolically to change the way the task is

described or represented. Unlike decomposition, which involves combination of

multiple judgments, unpacking only involves a single prediction judgment.

Decomposition infers that breaking down a category into simple components ought to

26
reduce bias because it should be easier to make accurate predictions for smaller, simpler

components than for a large, complex aggregate. It is not based on any explanation for

the direction of prediction bias. In contrast, unpacking is based on support theory, and

infers only an increase in overall estimates, which may or may not translate into reduced

bias.

There is other evidence supporting the unpacking approach to reducing bias.

Sanna and Schwarz (2004) studied the impact of a thought listing task on the planning

fallacy. When people are asked to consider factors that could lead to failure or success,

their predictions depend on the number of failure or success components they are asked

to generate. For example, they argued that when people are asked to generate three ways

to succeed, they find this easy, and infer that success is therefore a likely outcome. But

when they are asked to think of 12 ways to succeed they find this difficult to bring to

mind and infer that success is unlikely. They believed that the ability to recall events

depends on the subjective experience of ease where subjective experience can override

the amount of information that comes to mind. The interaction of thought content and

accessibility experiences (how easy or difficult it is for thoughts to come to mind) also

influences task completion time predictions.

In their study, Sanna and Schwarz (2004) found that students who did not list

their thoughts were overly optimistic regarding their study completion time before an

exam. However, when students were asked to list their thoughts implying success (3

success thoughts or 12 failure thoughts) 28 days before the exam, the planning fallacy

did not increase but when students were asked to list their thoughts implying failure (3

failure thoughts or 12 success thoughts) at the same time, the planning fallacy was

significantly reduced, even though it did not completely eliminate the fallacy.

Thinking along a similar line, Russo and Schoemaker (1992) also suggested that

people underestimate future project duration because they have difficulty imagining all

27
the ways that events could unfold. Those scenarios that do come to mind are regarded as

most likely and therefore, people tend to assume that everything will go accordingly.

Similarly, Byram (1997) argued that people tend to underestimate because of the

difficulty of thinking of things that may go wrong during task performance. This is

known as availability bias, and for time prediction, the availability bias may account for

underestimation if people do not consider things that can go wrong.

In summary, the problem of overly optimistic prediction in task completion time

is often associated with the planning fallacy. I will focus on two conflicting

explanations of how to reduce the planning fallacy. The inside / outside view suggests

that the possible cause for the planning fallacy is because people focus on the specific

task at hand and neglect past distributional information. However, planning in real

project often involves planning in great details and utilizing the workbreakdown

structure which seems similar to focusing on the project at hand and the unpacking

approach. Both of these explanations have empirical support from the literature for

predictions of task completion time.

2.7.2 Biases in Outcome Predictions

Optimism is not limited to predictions of task completion time described by the

planning fallacy. Studies have shown that people in general display “optimistic bias” in

their predictions about future events and outcomes, believing that they are more likely

than others to experience positive events and less likely than others to experience

negative events (Shepperd et al., 2000; Weinstein, 1980). Weinstein (1980) found that

when student participants were asked to rate their likelihood of experiencing various

positive and negative life events, the average likelihood of responses given by

participants tended to be above average for positive events but below average for

negative events. People were also found optimistically biased for a wide variety of

events such as lung cancer (Lee, 1989; McKenna, Warburton, & Winwood, 1993),

28
unplanned pregnancy (Burger & Burns, 1988; Whitley & Hern, 1991), criminal

victimization (Perloff & Fetzer, 1986), illness (Weinstein, 1980, 1982, 1987), and

automobile accidents (McKenna et al., 1993). The optimism in personal predictions

may arise from errors in the way people process information or may be motivated by

self-enhancement needs. Several explanations have been advanced to account for the

optimistic biases in future outcome predictions.

Some authors regard optimistic bias as evidence of wishful thinking and the

illusion of control (Makridakis & Wheelwright, 1989; Weinstein, 1980). In wishful

thinking bias, people tend to exaggerate the likelihood of the outcomes that they prefer.

Weinstein (1980) believed that exaggerating the likelihood of the preferred outcomes

tends to produce positive affect, whereas underestimating the likelihood of positive

outcomes would produce negative affect. Therefore, Weinstein (1980) assumed that the

stronger the affect, the stronger the distortion of reality.

The illusion of control bias is the tendency for individuals to believe that they

are able to control or at least influence outcomes even when they clearly cannot. In most

cases, people fail to regard the possibility of uncontrollable occurrences that may hinder

their progress toward a goal. Instead of focusing on the uncertainty of the events, most

individuals tend to focus on the aspects that they believed they can control, and thus,

overestimate their own capabilities of achieving those outcomes. According to

Weinstein (1980), if individuals perceive an event can be controllable, they tend to

believe that there are steps that they could take to increase the likelihood of a desirable

outcome. Since it is easier to bring to mind their own action than the actions of others,

people make assumptions that the desired outcomes are more likely to happen to them

than to other people. Apart from focusing on the aspect that they can control, observing

an early sequence of success can also lead people to believe they have some control

over outcomes (Langer, 1975). Individuals are also found to regard risk as ca hallenge

29
to be met where by trying harder, people may believe they have more control than is

realistic over the tasks they are trying to predict (Lovallo & Kahneman, 2003).

Optimistic outcome prediction can also be explained by the representativeness

heuristic (Kahneman & Tversky, 1972; Tversky & Kahneman, 1974).

Representativeness refers to the process of judging the probability that an individual

case fits into a particular category by evaluating the degree to which the individual case

is representative of, or similar to the category, but ignoring the base rate of category

membership. For many events, people may have a stereotyped conception about the

kind of person to whom this event typically happens (Weinstein, 1980). The

representativeness heuristic suggests that if the persons do not see themselves fitting

with the stereotype, then they will conclude that the event would not happen to them.

Weinstein further argued that if stereotypes are defensive, the image of a person who

experiences positive events would overemphasize a person’s own characteristics, and

this tendency would exaggerate optimistic biases.

Another explanation is that people’s thoughts about the future are dominated by

goals and plans and rarely include considerations of failure or unpleasant episodes.

Newby-Clark and Ross (2003) found in their study that participants thoughts of the

future often included desirable goals such as graduation, gainful employment, marriage,

children, and exotic travel. Participants took longer to record negative future outcomes

than a pleasant future scenario, and participants were slower to generate negative future

events than to generate positive future events.

Optimistic outcome prediction is not just caused by having a thought about the

future; past personal experience may influence beliefs about the chances of

experiencing an event. Personal experience provides the material for individuals to

recall past occurrences of similar events and to imagine situations in which such events

30
could occur, leading to greater perceived probability through the mechanism of

“availability” (Tversky & Kahneman, 1973, 1974).

Though there is evidence of people being optimistically biased towards future

outcomes, there is also evidence showing that people tend to be very optimistic in the

beginning, but as the outcome draws near they tend to abandon their optimism by

displaying realism or even pessimism (Liberman & Trope, 1998; Shepperd et al., 2000;

Shepperd et al., 2005). Liberman and Trope (1998) found that individuals tend to be

more optimistic about distant future than near future outcomes.

There are several explanations for this shift from optimism suggested in the

literature. Optimism is believed to reduce as the outcome draws near because people

gain new information that may help them to see more clearly what is likely to happen

(Shepperd et al., 2005). In fact, some evidence suggests that people will abandon their

optimism, displaying realism or even pessimism as defensive mechanism, if they

anticipate that information or feedback might soon challenge their optimistic stance.

People are also found to shift away from optimism in response to a shift in their

understanding of the event from abstract to concrete (Liberman & Trope, 1998).

Abstract features are more likely to be used when explaining distant future than near

future events, because in many situations relevant information regarding the distant

future is unavailable or uncertain. As the outcome draws near, this optimism will soon

change when more concrete information has been gathered.

The role of affective information processing has also been suggested to be

responsible for this transition from optimism to pessimism in predictions. Schwarz and

Clore (1983) found that people used their momentary affective states as a source of

information when making various kinds of judgments. However, it was found that only

unpleasant affective states motivate people to seek explanations as opposed to

individuals in pleasant affective states. This is because the unpleasant affective state is

31
often associated with the deviation from the individual’s usual feelings and thus, more

attempts are generated to seek reasonable explanations. As highlighted by Gilovich,

Kerr and Medvec (1993), people might regard their level of anxiety about an impending

outcome as an important source of information about the outcomes’ status. That is, if

people feel anxious as the outcome draws near, it must be because they will do poorly.

Apart from gaining new information, Shepperd et al. (2005) proposed that

another category of explanation for this shift from optimism is because people brace for

a possible undesired outcome. People tend to display realism or even pessimism as a

way to ready themselves for unpleasant surprises or undesired outcomes and most

likely, to avoid the disappointment (Shepperd et al., 2000; Shepperd et al., 2005). Since

people are normally trying to avoid unpleasant feelings associated with unexpected

negative outcomes, they are motivated to make pessimistic predictions regarding future

outcomes as those outcomes draw nearer.

In conclusion, people tend to render predictions that are overly optimistic:

underestimate the amount of time needed to complete a task or over predict the

occurrence of positive events and under predict negative ones. In the following section,

people’s confidence in their predictions is discussed.

2.7.3 Confidence in Predictions

The terms confidence and optimism are both used in the literature. Both

optimism and confidence have been used to refer to a prediction of a successful

outcome. For example, Gilovich, Kerr and Medvec (1993) and Sanna and Schwarz

(2004) both use the concept of confidence as belief in a successful outcome, whereas

Shepperd, Findley-Klein, Kwavnick, Walker, & Perez (2000) refer to this as optimism.

In this study, the term confidence refers to the strength of belief or degree of conviction

in a prediction, regardless of the content of the prediction. Confidence indicates the

32
strength of belief that the prediction is correct. The term optimism is used to indicate the

level of success participants predict, that is, the content of the prediction.

Research has shown that people do not just make predictions that are overly

optimistic; they tend to be overconfident in the accuracy of their beliefs. When

participants were asked about how certain they were that they will finish by the time

that they had predicted, Buehler, Griffin and Ross (1994) found that participants were

quite confident that they would meet their predictions. Based on the scale of 0% (not at

all certain) to 100% (completely certain), participants rated their confidence in meeting

a predicted time and date at 74% for an academic project and 69.9% for non-academic

tasks. For each case, approximately 40% of the participants actually finished by the

predicted time.

Russo and Schoemaker (1992) explored the sources of optimism, referring to it

as overconfidence. They proposed four cognitive causes of overconfidence: availability,

anchoring, confirmation bias and hindsight. They believed that most people tend to be

optimistic in predictions, because they have difficulties in imagining all the ways events

could unfold. Normally, people tend to rely on data and scenarios that come easily to

hand and mind (availability bias). Those scenarios that do come to mind are regarded as

most likely and therefore, they tend to assume that everything will go accordingly.

Second, when people make their best guess, they tend to anchor their thinking, which

prevents them from imagining circumstances that differ substantially from it (anchoring

bias). Anchoring bias is persistent in many aspects of our daily lives, and is known to be

one of the strongest and most prevalent of cognitive biases. Third, people tend to lean

toward one perspective when making their predictions (confirmation bias) and seek

support for their initial view rather than looking for disconfirming evidence. Fourth,

people always believe that the world is more predictable than it really is (hindsight

bias), due to the failure to appreciate the full uncertainty that existed at the time. They

33
are rarely surprised by events that have already happened, even though they may have

had considerable difficulty in predicting the same events.

People are not always optimismistic, their optimism may change over time.

Sanna (1999) offers an explanation of temporal changes in optimism (Sanna used the

term confidence) based on the reciprocal influences between mental simulation and

affect. He proposed that affect can serve as both cause and consequence of simulations.

One reason why he believes people are more optimistic in success when they view a

performance from a distal perspective than from a proximal perspective is because

upward simulation (or upward counterfactual, simulations that are better than reality)

may increase as performance gets proximal. Since upward simulation is preparative,

people would likely have preparative thoughts as a performance gets closer. In other

words, as performance approaches, a person may think more about everything that still

requires doing, and this may decrease their confidence. Decrease in optimism may also

result from increased negative mood or anxiety. People may try to buffer themselves

from failure as performance approaches and upward simulation can generate negative

affect. Sanna (1999) found that change in optimism was related to both mental

simulation and affect, either increase in upward simulations, increase in negative affect

and anxiety, or both (given the reciprocal relation between simulation and affect).

In another study, Sanna and Schwarz (2004) examined the impact of thought

listing on change in optimism. They altered people’s change in optimism by

manipulating the number of failure or success components that they asked people to

generate. They found that asking students to generate thoughts implying success (3

success thoughts, 12 failure thoughts) 28 days before exam eliminated the otherwise

observed low optimism for proximal outcome and asking students to generate thoughts

implying failure (3 failure thoughts, 12 success thoughts) eliminated the otherwise

observed high optimism for distal outcomes.

34
2.7.4 Biases in Affective Predictions

People frequently think about how they will feel when particular events take

place. They may consider the kinds of emotions they will experience, the intensity of

their emotional reactions, and how long those feelings will last (Buehler, McFarland,

Spyropoulos, & Lam, 2007; Gilbert & Wilson, 2000; Wilson et al., 2000). Such

affective predictions are important because people base many decisions on them.

Decisions about whether to pursue a project or not for example, are based at least in

part, on predictions about how these decisions will make one feel. People may

reasonably choose events that they predict will produce positive feelings and avoid

those that might distress them.

Generally, people know exactly what will make them feel happy or sad. But,

knowing what one wants is not the same as knowing how much one will like it once it

happens. In fact, past studies on affective predictions (e.g., Lam et al., 2005; Sanna &

Schwarz, 2004) have shown that people often mispredict their own future feelings and

overestimate the impact of future events on their feelings. People always think that they

will be happier after a positive outcome and will be sadder after a negative outcome

even when the future outcome is similar to ones they have experienced in the past. But,

the results turn out not as they initially expected them to be.

The tendency for people to overestimate the power and persistence of emotional

reactions to events has been termed impact bias, which is one of the most prevalent

biases in affective predictions (Wilson & Gilbert, 2003). Impact bias has been observed

for affective prediction concerning a wide range of positive and negative events and

outcomes such as wining or losing sporting events, learning about exam grades,

receiving positive or negative feedback over an aptitude test, being granted or denied

tenure, and living in a pleasant or an unpleasant climate (Buehler & McFarland, 2001;

Gilbert, Pinel, Wilson, Blumberg, & Wheatley, 1998; Sanna & Schwarz, 2004; Schkade

35
& Kahneman, 1998; Van Dijk, 2009; Wilson, Meyers, & Gilbert, 2001; Wilson et al.,

2000). However, Finkenauer, Gallucci, Dijk and Pollman (2007) found that the bias is

more pronounced for negative affect than for positive affect. In their study, participants

highly overestimated their disappointment toward failing their driver’s license exam,

while they only slightly overestimated their happiness following success of the exam.

Focalism has been suggested to be one of the key sources of impact bias, at least

for positive events (Wilson et al., 2000). Focalism is where people focus intensely on

the event concerned and fail to consider other events that are likely to occur that could

also influence emotions. Because people are focusing too much on some events and too

little on other events, they fail to consider the consequences these other events might

bring about. Wilson, Wheatley, Meyers, Gilbert and Axsom (2000) found that when

college students were asked to consider a wider range of life events that would be

occurring at the same time as the event, they generated more moderate affective

predictions over an upcoming football game. Relatedly, Schkade and Kahneman (1998)

found that people incorrectly predicted that they would be happier living in California

than the Midwest when they focused on the salient differences in climate.

Another suggested cause of impact bias is related to the misconstrual problem in

predicting one’s reaction to events. While focalism involves neglecting other events that

will occur at the same time as the target event, construal involves imagining the target

event in a particular manner (Buehler & McFarland, 2001). Buehler and McFarland

(2001) argued that each of the future events can occur in many different ways regarding

the particular manner in which it unfolds, and each will evoke different emotional

reactions. Giving an example of getting an A in midterm, they argued that it can give

different meanings and consequences depending on the details of the situations and the

context in which it occurs.

36
According to Wilson and Gilbert (2003), to know how a future event will impact

one’s feelings one needs to bring to mind an accurate representation of that event. If the

person does not have experience with those events before, they need to construct a

representation of what the event is likely to entail. Because people often fail to

appreciate the fact that a future event may not occur exactly the way they imagine,

people are prone to errors in predicting how they will feel about those events.

Neglecting a set of past relevant experiences, that is, failing to adopt the outside

view, is also suggested as one of the reasons why people tend to exaggerate the

emotional impact of future events. In their study, Buehler and McFarland (2001) found

that participants who were asked to recall several of their past experiences with

Christmas generated more realistic positive affective predictions than those who focused

exclusively on the upcoming holidays.

The problems of mispredicted affective reactions to future events can also be

attributed to one’s memory of the events. If people have experienced the event before,

they can predict their feelings by recalling how they felt in the past. However, Robinson

and Clore (2002) note that emotional experiences are not stored in the memory in a

form that can be retrieved directly later. They further argued that because the details of

an experience fade over time, people depend more on their theories about how the event

will make them feel rather than on the actual experience. Poor recall and incorrect

theories have been said to lead to error in predicting how future events will affect one’s

feelings.

Accordingly, Morewedge, Gilbert and Wilson (2005) also believe that people

generally do not realize how, and how often, they think about the events that have

already happened, and therefore, mispredict their affective reactions to future events.

But, knowing how and how often one thinks about the event will not be enough if one

fails to imagine the event accurately. According to Morewedge et al. (2005), people

37
naturally depend on their memories of past events to generate their affective reactions

toward future events. However, people tend to remember more of the unusual events

than the more routine, everyday events. The tendency to remember the best and the

worst of times instead of the most typical of times has also been found to contribute to

impact bias. Morewedge, Gilbert and Wilson (2005) found that participants who were

asked to recall a single instance of an event, or to recall no events at all, made more

extreme predictions about the future than participants who were asked to recall several

events or to recall atypical events. However, the recall of events also involves the

subjective experience of ease of recall where subjective experience can override the

amount of information that comes to mind. Sanna and Schwarz (2004) found that the

impact bias was eliminated for reactions to failure when success is easy to recall and the

impact was also eliminated for reactions to success when failure is easy to recall.

2.8 Conclusion and Research Issues to be Addressed

The above literature review indicates that the process of planning and prediction

are important for any project. A considerable amount of money and effort is spent in

attempts to predict how long projects will take to complete and certainly, whether the

projects are possible to complete. However, the uncertain nature of projects often makes

planning, especially making predictions about the project’s future, a challenging

activity. Some researchers believe that the uncertain nature of project environments has

made intuitive decision making rather than rational decision unavoidable. But, intuitive

decision making is vulnerable to various temporal biases such as the planning fallacy,

confidence changes, and impact bias that may influence the accuracy of the predictions

made. Therefore, finding ways to neutralize these biases could generate significant

benefits for organizations.

Several researchers have suggested that the combination of information search

and processing biases tends to introduce significant error to human judgment which

38
often leads people to make overly optimistic predictions, and to be overly confident in

their predictions (Buehler & Griffin, 2003; Buehler et al., 1994; Dunning, 2007;

Weinstein, 1980). From the above discussions, Lovallo and Kahneman (2003) noted

that in 1979 Kahneman and Tversky have distinguished between two approaches to

intuitive predictions, which they label the inside and outside view. Focusing on the

outside view, that is, a broader context that includes relevant past experiences, the

experiences of others and background events, produces more accurate predictions

regarding task completion time than using the inside view, focusing only on the project

at hand to make the prediction Several prediction biases such as the tendency to make

overconfident and overly optimistic predictions of completion time can be understood

from the results of people’s tendency to favor the inside view, and underweight relevant

past experiences. In contrast, research on an approach termed enumerating or unpacking

has suggested that taking an inside perspective that specifically involves thinking more

about the task at hand and its components may also result in a reduction of the planning

fallacy (Kruger & Evans, 2004). If both the outside view and the unpacking approach

were able to reduce bias in completion time prediction, which of these methods would

produce a greater effect? A second issue is that the outside view and unpacking

approach have been used to explain the occurrence of the planning fallacy. Would their

prescriptions result in similar effects for other types of predictions like outcome and

affective reactions towards outcome success and failure, and on the confidence of those

predictions?

In this thesis I examine and compare the effects of the inside-outside and

unpacking approaches on task completion time prediction. There are practical reasons to

compare the effectiveness of these two methods for reducing the planning fallacy.

Detailed planning is unavoidable in project work. Indeed, the preparation of a complete

work breakdown structure is an essential part of project management (Cleland &

39
Ireland, 2002). If this detailed focus on the project at hand exacerbates the planning

fallacy, there is a conundrum that the more we know about a project, the worse our

predictions about it will be. In contrast, the enumerating approach of Kruger and Evans

(2004) accords with the work breakdown structure concept in project management. If

the enumerating or unpacking approach is effective in reducing the planning fallacy, it

could be utilized to improve prediction of project duration and outcomes.

The study was also designed to explore whether individuals exhibit an optimistic

bias when predicting a project’s outcome, and tested whether the explanations of the

planning fallacy can be applied to optimistic outcome predictions. Also, I examined

how the outside perspective and enumerating approach influence the confidence in

estimation and the prediction of affective reactions toward the success of the project’s

completion time and outcome.

In the next chapter, Chapter 3, hypotheses for Study 1 are proposed for project

completion time and outcome, for confidence in prediction and for prediction of

affective reactions towards outcome success or failure.

40
CHAPTER 3

HYPOTHESES - STUDY 1

3.1 Introduction

This chapter presents the twelve research hypotheses for Study 1, based on the

two dominant theoretical frameworks of the planning fallacy reviewed in Chapter 2.

The extension of these two theories to predictions other than time to completion of a

project, specifically on prediction of project outcome, confidence in prediction of

project completion time and outcome, and predictions of affective reactions to project

outcome success and failure are also considered and hypotheses are prepared.

3.2 Testing the Theories of the Planning Fallacy

Chapter 2 has reviewed two compelling, but conflicting explanations about the

occurrence of the planning fallacy. One line of research, the inside/outside view

proposes that neglecting distributional information or the outside perspective when

predicting task completion time is the cause of the planning fallacy, because prediction

that is based on the inside view, a view that portrays the progression of a specific

scenario from the present to the future, leads people to neglect the vast number of ways

in which their plan may go wrong. In contrast, the second line of research proposed that

taking an inside perspective that involves thinking more about the specific task at hand

does not always result in a planning fallacy, provided that those tasks are unpacked into

their various subcomponents.

There is an apparent contradiction between the inside / outside view and

unpacking approaches and the main aim of this study is to compare these as methods for

reducing the planning fallacy. These two theories have not been tested on the same task

before. It is possible that the conflicting results for these two explanations are due to the

different tasks with which they have been tested. Therefore, testing these two theories

41
on the same task allows for the outside perspective and the unpacking approach to be

compared. However, no hypothesis contrasting the outside perspective and the

unpacking approach was prepared in this study, as there was no basis on which to

hypothesize the existence or direction of differences between them, so the tests I

conduct to make comparisons between those approaches are exploratory. Hypothesis 1

was proposed to replicate the separate findings from previous studies on the inside /

outside perspective and the unpacking approach. It was also necessary to demonstrate

that the planning fallacy occurs in this research context, so that further hypotheses may

be tested.

H1.1a: People who make predictions regarding task completion time using the outside

perspective are more accurate than people who use the inside perspective.

H1.1b: People who make predictions regarding task completion time after unpacking

the task are more accurate than people who make a prediction for the task as a whole.

3.3 Theories of the Planning Fallacy and Outcome Prediction

Working from the two theoretical frameworks of the planning fallacy, I now

extend them to outcome prediction. First, the outside view suggests that outcome

predictions should be more accurate if individuals compare their chances of achieving

the predicted outcome with others, use their own past experiences as a basis to visualize

how the outcome would transpire or take into consideration the probability of the events

for the general population. In other words, gathering information from various sources

and viewing the future outcomes in a broader context should increase the chances of

making more accurate outcome predictions.

Second, support theory suggested that for an unpacked multifaceted task such as

a project, individuals should be able to make a more accurate outcome prediction

because the unpacking process changes the way the project is represented or described.

The process of enumerating helps people to better understand what they are supposed to

42
deliver in a project and thus, they should be able to make a more accurate outcome

prediction regarding whether they can deliver it or not.

Thus, the following hypotheses are proposed which are parallel with hypotheses

for prediction of completion time.

H1.2a: People who use the outside perspective are more accurate in their prediction of

project outcome success than people who use the inside perspective.

H1.2b: People who unpack the project components are more accurate in their prediction

of project outcome success than people who do not unpack.

3.4 Theories of the Planning Fallacy and Confidence in Predictions

Applying the theoretical analysis of the planning fallacy to the occurrence of

confidence changes, it is suggested that people who adopt an inside perspective or fail

to unpack the future event will demonstrate greater changes in their confidence. This is

because people who adopt the outside perspective should have more information in the

early stage of the prediction process than people who adopt the inside perspective, and

those who unpack the events early in the prediction process should also have more

information than those who do not. Therefore, the difference between the information

on hand early in the process and the information on hand later, as the outcome of event

draws near, will not be as great for these groups as for people who use the inside

approach or do not unpack. Their changes in confidence will also not be as great. Four

hypotheses are proposed to test the effect of inside / outside perspectives and unpacking

on confidence change in predictions of completion time and outcome success.

H1.3a: People who use the outside perspective will demonstrate smaller change in their

confidence of predicted completion time compared to those who use the inside

perspective.

H1.3b: People who unpack will demonstrate smaller change in their confidence of

predicted completion time compared to those who do not unpack.

43
H1.4a: People who use the outside perspective will demonstrate smaller change in their

confidence of predicted outcome compared to those who use the inside perspective.

H1.4b: People who unpack will demonstrate smaller change in their confidence of

predicted outcome as compared to those who do not unpack.

3.5 Theories of the Planning Fallacy and Prediction of Affective Reactions towards

Outcome Success and Failure

Research on the impact bias suggests that reasons for people to overestimate

their emotional reaction when making affective predictions can be attributed to people’s

failure to consider the consequences of other events that will influence their emotions

(Wilson & Gilbert, 2003; Wilson et al., 2000); the misconstrual problem that involves

imagining the target event in a particular manner, and neglecting a set of past

experiences (Buehler & McFarland, 2001) which seems to be consistent with the

explanation of inside / outside perspective on the planning fallacy.

Based on these explanations, I suggest that the reason why people tend to

overestimate the emotional reactions they will experience is because they frequently

adopt the inside perspective when making their affective predictions. Adopting the

inside perspective makes people prone to the focalism problem because the scenario-

based approach tends to lead people to neglect a vast majority of other events that might

influence their feelings. In contrast, if people adopt the outside perspective, a more

moderate affective prediction should be generated. This is because they have a set of

relevant past experiences as their reference, and taking consideration of how previous

events have influenced their feelings may actually help them to make a more accurate

prediction.

However, using the outside perspective can be cognitively demanding especially

when a number of different events must be evaluated within a short period of time,

making the process of connecting the past experiences and the specific prediction of

44
future events difficult. A person must first select an appropriate standard for

comparison, a similar past experience. In many cases, individuals find it difficult to

detect the similarities in their past experiences when there are various instances that

seem so different from each other. Besides, prediction by its very nature, focuses on the

future rather than the past and this may prevent individuals from looking backward

(Buehler et al., 2002).

Wilson, Wheatley and Meyers (2000) argued that people don’t have to think

about past experiences to correct their predictions of affective reactions, instead they

can think more about non-focal events, which refer to many other events that will

influence emotions. This may seem to contradict the inside/outside perspective, but

working with time to completion predictions, Kruger and Evans (2004) argued that if

the inside perspective pays attention to non-focal subcomponents of the task, people

will generate more accurate predictions.

Based on the above analysis, I explored the effects of the inside/outside

perspectives and the unpacking approach on accuracy of affective predictions. I

proposed hypotheses for affective prediction that parallel the hypotheses for prediction

of completion time. Separate hypotheses are necessary to deal with successful and

unsuccessful outcomes. A more accurate, that is, less extreme prediction of affective

reaction was expected to occur in the presence of the additional information provided by

the outside view or the unpacking procedure. Therefore, the following hypotheses are

proposed:

H1.5a: People who use the outside perspective will make more accurate predictions of

positive affective reactions towards outcome success than people who use the inside

perspective.

H1.5b: People who unpack will make more accurate predictions of positive affective

reactions towards outcome success than those who do not unpack.

45
H1.6a: People who use the outside perspective will make more accurate predictions of

negative affective reactions towards outcome failure than people who use the inside

perspective.

H1.6b: People who unpack will make more accurate predictions of negative affective

reactions towards outcome failure than those who do not unpack.

In organizational projects, planning and prediction are normally carried out not

by individuals, but by groups. Even if the project managers make the final prediction

alone, they usually interact with others to collect information, opinions and advice

(Heath & Gonzalez, 1995). This new information, which is not available to them prior

to making their initial prediction, could cause them to revise their decision making or

influence the degree of bias in their predictions. Since this study is examining the

planning predictions in a project, a group project is used. Data are collected on measures

of the confidence changes hypotheses which involve a number of time points as the

project deadline draws closer.

The design involved repeated weekly observations of prediction behavior,

among student participants in a group project over a period of time. Taking into

consideration the hierarchically nested data structures in this study, the repeated

measures (time at level 1) are nested within participants (level 2) and the participants in

turn are nested within project groups (level 3). The research hypotheses in this

longitudinal study are tested using Hierarchical Linear Models (HLM). Detailed

discussions on the multilevel analysis approach used in Study 1 are presented in the

next chapter, Chapter 4.

3.6 Conclusion

This chapter has focused on two theories of the planning fallacy namely the

inside / outside view and the unpacking approach, and the development of 12 research

hypotheses. It is proposed that the adoption of either the outside perspective or the

46
unpacking approach would generate a more accurate prediction of task completion time

and outcome, a smaller change in confidence in predictions of task completion time and

outcome, and a more accurate prediction of affective response towards outcome success

or failure.

The next chapter, Chapter 4, discusses the method used to test the hypotheses

presented in this chapter.

47
CHAPTER 4

METHOD – STUDY 1

4.1 Introduction

This chapter describes the research method for the first study, including the

design of the experiment, the data collection process, participants, the experimental

materials, the procedure involved and the research measures used in the study. The

chapter concludes with strategies for analyzing the data.

4.2 Research Design

The hypotheses stated in Chapter 3 were tested in a controlled experiment. The

experiment involved four conditions in which participants’ initial planning

considerations about their projects were manipulated with instructions that were

consistent with the inside view, outside view, unpacked or packed (control) approaches.

Responses were collected on eight measures – predicted project completion time,

predicted project outcome, confidence in prediction of project completion time,

confidence in prediction of the success of the outcome and predicted affective response

toward outcome success and failure and reported affective response towards outcome

success and failure.

As the design required experimental manipulation of the prediction and planning

procedure for a large number of projects, it was not practical to use projects in

organizations. To provide control of the environment I have used students’ group

project assignments. Using students’ project assignments made it possible to gather a

large amount of data, ensured that the projects would not be abandoned, and that the

duration of the projects fits within this study time. The eight weeks duration and small

group nature of the projects is a realistic parallel for small organizational projects.

Furthermore, these projects were important with real consequential outcomes that could

48
be expected to engender affective reactions in the participants, an aspect that is difficult

to achieve in laboratory studies.

4.3 Data Collection Process

Approval for this study was granted by both the Human Research Ethics

Committee of The University of Western Australia, and the Malaysia Economic

Planning Unit (EPU). The fieldwork was conducted from December 2005 until April

2006. During that period, the following activities were conducted.

The plan for recruitment of participants began in December before the second

semester session 2005 / 2006 started, in the Faculty of Human and Social Development,

Universiti Utara Malaysia. Lecturers were first contacted through email and the purpose

and nature of the study were explained. Through the first contact, twelve lecturers

agreed to allow their students to participate in the study. To ensure that no student

would be involved in the experiment twice, classes were selected for this study through

manually checking each of the twelve lecturers’ student lists. At the end of the process,

eleven classes conducted by ten different lecturers were chosen. The experiment was

conducted between January and April 2006. Detailed procedure of the experiment is

provided in Section 4.6.

4.4 Participants

A total of 799 undergraduate students from eleven classes agreed to participate.

At the conclusion of the experiment all participants were given a small gift for their

voluntary participation. Participants were working in one of 162 student project groups

(ranging from 3 to 10 members per group), but completed all the questionnaire

materials individually. The 162 project groups were randomly assigned to one of the

four between-participant conditions: inside view, outside view, unpacked and packed

(control), as described in detail in Section 4.6.

49
Twenty-three participants failed to continue with the experiment because they

dropped the relevant class and were not included in the final sample. Another four

students’ data were also excluded as the information given was not feasible. For

example, one participant reported that the project assignment would be completed

within a day, with the predicted date of completion given as the day after the experiment

was conducted. Therefore, data of 772 participants are potentially available for analysis.

4.5 Experimental Materials

Acknowledging the cultural differences and since teaching is carried out in both

English and Bahasa Malaysia at Universiti Utara Malaysia, all materials prepared for

the participants were provided in both English and Bahasa Malaysia. Participants were

given the choice between the two versions so that they could express their ideas freely.

The Bahasa Malaysia version was translated using back translation. I first translated the

English versions of the materials (which have been approved by the Human Research

Ethics Committee of The University of Western Australia) to Bahasa Malaysia as I am a

bilingual native speaker of Bahasa Malaysia. The Bahasa Malaysia versions were given

to two bilingual native speakers who then translated them back into English (working

independently). The final translated version was consistent with the original. Both

versions of the experimental materials are shown in Appendix A. All participants in this

study chose the Bahasa Malaysia version.

Throughout the experiment, each participant received the following

experimental materials: an information sheet, a consent form, experimental

questionnaire (with cover letter attached) for one of the four conditions [inside view,

outside view, unpacked or packed (control)], eight copies of a weekly diary form and a

short affective questionnaire. The information sheet, consent form, experimental

questionnaire and a first week’s diary were given on the first day of the experiment. A

weekly diary was then given on a weekly basis until week 8. A short affective

50
questionnaire was given on the last day of the experiment, after participants received

their project assignment marks.

The information sheet provides participants with information such as the

background and general aim of the study, what they would be asked to do as part of

their involvement in the study, and benefits gained by participating in the study. Contact

phone numbers were also provided in case participants have any questions or they need

further information pertaining to the study. The consent form assured participants that

their participation in this study was voluntary and confidential. They were advised that

they were free to withdraw at any time without reason and without prejudice.

The experimental questionnaire asked about participants’ predictions regarding

project completion time and project assignment marks that they anticipated receiving,

level of confidence regarding those predictions and their expected affective response

towards project success and failure should those occur. Before answering these

questions, the focus of participants’ thoughts was varied according to one of the four

conditions with instructions in the first section of the questionnaire.

The weekly diary entry asked participants to record any activities and group

meetings that were conducted in that particular week that were related to their project

assignments. Specific information such as date, start time and finish time for each of the

activities and matters discussed during group meetings were also asked. For the last

diary entry, participants were also asked to indicate their contribution and other group

members’ contributions towards the project assignment. The affective questionnaire on

the last day asked participants to rate their actual feelings regarding the marks that they

received. Each of the experimental materials had a coded ID number based on the

participants’ student number, to identify participants and their different classes and

group projects.

51
4.6 Experimental Procedure

The experiment was conducted during the weeks of the semester where students

are required to complete a project assignment. In each class, after lecturers had assigned

students to groups, students were asked to sit together in their own groups before the

first part of the experimental process took place. Then, I addressed the students by

introducing the experiment and invited them to participate in the experiment. Students

were provided with a sheet of information about the experiment and a consent form.

Students were invited to ask questions and then consent forms were collected. Each

participant was then given a questionnaire for one of the four conditions: inside view,

outside view, unpacked and packed (control). All members of a group received the

instructions for the same condition. The questionnaires for the four conditions were

randomly distributed among the groups in each class in the following manner. Group

one was given questionnaires with the inside view condition, group two was given

questionnaires with the outside view condition, group three was given questionnaires

with the unpacked condition and group four was given questionnaires with the packed

(control) condition. The process was repeated with the rest of the groups in the class.

The materials for the four conditions differed as follows.

Participants in the inside and outside perspective conditions were asked in the

written instructions to spend a few minutes considering their plans for completing the

project. The instructions were designed to vary the focus of their thoughts towards

either the outside or inside view. The instructions for the outside view were as follows

(the inside view version of the instructions is in parentheses):

Please take a few minutes to consider your plans for completing your project

assignment. Recall (think about) the kind of events that have happened (are likely to

happen) in your past (future) project assignment and those of people that you know

about (as you work on your project assignment). Recall (think about) all the problems

52
that have happened (are likely to happen) in the past (future), that have impeded (will

impede) your project progress and performance, and any aspects of the project or

situation in the past (future) that have facilitated (will facilitate) your project progress

and performance.

The next page of the questionnaire asked participants to “Please describe all the

problems that have happened (are likely to happen) in the past (future) that have

impeded (will impede) your project progress and performance.” Ample space was

provided on which participants could write their descriptions. The next page asked

participants to “Please describe any aspects of the projects or situations in the past

(future) that have facilitated (will facilitate) your past project (your project’s) progress

and performance.”

For participants in the unpacked and packed (control) conditions, the instructions

asked participants to “Please take a few minutes to consider your plans for completing

your project assignment.” On the next page, where they were asked to write down their

descriptions, participants in the unpacked condition were asked to do the following.

Participants in packed (control) condition did not go through this part of the procedure.

a) “Please list down components that are needed for project progress and

performance.”

And on the page after that, they were asked,

b) “Please list down each and every thing that you plan to do in completing this

project assignment.”

Participants in all conditions were then asked to “Try to predict as accurately and

realistically as possible when you will finish the project assignment and what marks you

anticipate to get.” Participants were also asked to indicate how confident or certain they

were that they would finish this project assignment by the date that they predicted and

how confident or certain they were that they would get the marks that they predicted.

53
Then, they were asked to indicate how they would feel after “a successful project” and

if the project were to “succeed” and how they would feel after “not a successful project”

and if “you do not succeed,” should that occur. Apart from answering prediction

questions, participants were also asked to fill in their demographic information: gender,

age, race and number of semesters that they have completed.

After participants finished answering the questions, they were supplied with a

prepared form (like a diary) on which they were to report their project progress for the

next week. Participants were requested to write down their student number as their

personal identification code and to record every occasion and the times (date, hour and

minutes; from beginning to end) when they worked on the project assignment. Apart

from reporting the progress of their project, participants were also requested to record

every meeting their group held outside of class time (if any, during that particular week)

and to report all the matters discussed during those meetings. The final questions on the

diary form were similar prediction questions to those on the initial questionnaire and

were to be answered at the end of the week.

Participants were assured that all the information given will remain confidential at

all times and will be used for the study only. It would not affect their project

assignment’s grade and would not be seen by their lecturer. Any identifying information

such as student number will be destroyed after the data analysis has been completed and

the data will be completely anonymous in the analysis and reports of the study. Each

week I went to every class in the study and personally collected their week’s diaries and

issued new ones, for the eight weeks of the projects. The last diary entry was collected

on the day the participants handed in their project assignments. Once the lecturers

finished marking the project assignments, the marks were given to me. The participants

collected their project assignment marks from me in person, and they were asked to fill

out a short questionnaire asking their feelings towards the marks they had received.

54
4.7 Measures

The major dependent constructs in this study were biases in the participants’

prediction of project completion time, and outcome success, confidence in prediction of

project’s completion time and outcome success, and their predicted and actual affective

reactions towards project success and failure.

4.7.1 Time Completion Measure

The actual completion time was measured by analyzing participants’ final

diaries. Based on the report written by the participants, the date and time that the

participants stated they finally finished the project was taken as the actual completion

time. The start time was the time of the class day when the project was distributed and

the experimental manipulation was conducted.

Prediction bias was indexed by the signed time difference between predicted and

actual completion dates, with the predicted subtracted from actual. If the predicted

completion time falls before the actual time completion, the difference is positive. If the

predicted completion time falls after the actual completion time the difference is

negative. In previous empirical studies, Buehler, Griffin and MacDonald (1997) and

Sanna and Schwarz (2004) have used a similar measure and subtracted the predicted

from the actual to operationalize time completion prediction accuracy measures.

4.7.2 Outcome Success Measures

There were two measures of predicted outcome success. Based on the points

allocated for the project assignment, participants were asked to predict their percentile

performance using a 13-point scale ranging from 40% to 100%. For the second

measure, participants were asked to state the exact marks they anticipated receiving (in

percentage) in a box provided. The actual project outcome was measured by the score

participants received from their respective lecturer converted to a percentage. Although

the projects were completed in groups, the marks included individual components that

55
ensured that individuals in a group did not all receive the same marks. Prediction bias

for the project outcome was indexed by the signed difference between predicted and

actual scores, with predicted subtracted from actual.

4.7.3 Confidence Measures

Confidence in predictions of completion time and outcome was assessed on a

weekly basis by asking participants to provide their confidence level by marking a 15

cm line, which had labeled anchors of absolutely impossible (0%), 50/50 chance (50%)

and completely certain (100%). For analysis, the response was measured from the 0%

end of the line and centimeters were converted to a number out of 100. A similar

measure was taken for confidence in the predicted assignment mark.

4.7.4 Affective Response for Success and Failure Measures

Measures for affective response were adapted from Sanna and Schwarz (2004).

In the questionnaire completed at the first meeting, but after the manipulations of the

four conditions, participants were asked to estimate on two 11-point scales how they

would feel after “a successful project” and if the project were to “succeed” (1 = not

good, 11 = very good). They were also asked to estimate how they would feel after “a

not successful project” and if the project “do not succeed” (1 = very bad, 11 = not bad).

Immediately after receiving the grades, participants were asked to answer a

dichotomous question on whether they viewed the mark they received for the project as

a successful project or not a successful project. If they answered successful they then

turned to two questions asking the degree to which they feel good (1 = not good, 11 =

very good), and if they answered not successful they turned to questions asking the

degree to which they feel bad (1 = not bad, 11 = very bad) about their obtained project

assignment marks. This scale is as described by Sanna and Schwarz (2004), and

following their practices, before conducting the analysis, the responses to feeling “bad”

were reverse coded.

56
4.8 Data Analysis Strategy

Multilevel regression analysis was used to test research hypotheses developed in

Chapter 3. The data consisted of repeated weekly observations obtained from

individuals working on group projects over a two-month period. The repeated measures

(time at level 1) are nested within 772 participants (level 2), and the participants in turn

are nested within 162 project groups (level 3). Multilevel modeling provides a way to

analyze nested data simultaneously at all levels. The HLM 6.02 program by

Raudenbush, Bryk, Cheong, Congdon and du Toit (2004) was used.

4.8.1 Level of Analysis

The first HLM analysis examined the intercept-only unconditional models with

only time (WEEK) included in the regression equations. Estimation of the unconditional

models allowed the investigation of the overall change trajectory across all participants

and groups. These include the estimation of initial status (G000) and the average

prediction bias rate per week (G100). The unconditional model also distributed the

variance components into participants within groups for initial status (R0) and weekly

prediction bias rates (R1) and between groups for mean initial status (U00) and mean

prediction bias rates (U10). These variance component estimates allowed the percentage

of variation that lies between groups for both the initial status and prediction rates to be

considered.

The next set of analyses examined the conditional model. The Level-1 model

remains the same as in the unconditional model. But now entered into the model at

Level-2 are participant-level variables such as the four conditions – inside, outside,

unpacked and packed (control), and participants’ demographic characteristics such as

age, gender, race and total number of semesters taken. At Level-3, the project group-

level variables such as group size, teacher, group mean for age, group mean for gender

and group mean for semester were entered. The manipulations in this study are entered

57
at the participant level (level-2) rather than at the project group level (level-3) because

the manipulations are applied to the individuals, through instructions read by the

individuals and responded to with predictions made by individuals.

The Type I error rate or alpha value in the multilevel regression analyses was set

at .10, which is consistent with what past multilevel authors have used (e.g., Krull &

MacKinnon, 2001; Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002; Singer & Willet, 2003).

4.8.2 Data Centering using Grand Means

Centering is an important consideration in multilevel models as it facilitates the

interpretation of the intercepts (Enders & Tofighi, 2007; Hofman & Gavin, 1998; Kreft

& de Leeuw, 1998; Kreft, de Leeuw, & Aiken, 1995; Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002).

Without centering (the raw score models), the intercepts can be interpreted as the

expected value of the dependent variable when the predictor variables are equal to zero.

For some predictor variables such as age, the value of zero is meaningless. Thus,

centering is critical in making the interpretation of the coefficient and variance

components more meaningful, especially in HLM, which focuses on explaining

variance in the intercept and regression coefficients.

Data that have a multilevel structure usually give researchers a range of

possibilities regarding the choice of reference values. For example, in this study, a

variable concerning participants (e.g. age, gender and semester) nested within group

projects can be centered by subtracting the grand mean of the variable or by subtracting

the group mean. With grand mean centering, each of the participant’s age, gender and

semester values is expressed as its deviation from the variable’s grand mean, which is

the overall mean of the variable of all participants in the sample under study. Grand

mean centering involves a linear transformation of the values for the centered variables,

and this will change the intercept but not the slope, thus, the intercept can be interpreted

as an adjusted mean (Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002). Since grand mean centering allows

58
consideration of an effect after partialling out or controlling for other effects, the

variance of the intercept is then the variance in the adjusted means.

The choice of centering depends on the model and research question under

investigation rather than on a statistical basis. Raudenbush and Bryk (2002) and Kreft,

de Leeuw and Aiken (1995) recommended the used of grand mean-centering as it

makes interpretation of intercepts clearer. Though using grand mean centering produces

nearly equivalent results with the raw metric scaling, Kreft, de Leeuw and Aiken (1995)

found that grand mean centered models provide a “computational advantage,” because

in most cases grand mean centering reduces the correlation between the intercept and

slope estimates across groups. Unlike grand mean centering, using group mean

centering produces results that differ from other approaches. Although group mean

centering can introduce complexity into model specification and interpretation,

Raudenbush and Bryk (2002), Kreft, de Leeuw and Aiken (1995), Hofmann and Gavin

(1998) note that group mean centering is useful when studying contextual effects.

In this longitudinal study, I am not investigating how groups (contexts) affect

individual participants’ predictions. The level-1 predictor (the WEEK variable) was

centered on day 1 when the experiment started and the manipulation was conducted.

When used, level-3 predictor (group size) was centered around its grand means. Level-2

predictors (age, gender and semester) were centered around their grand means.

4.8.3 Time Centering

As mentioned in Section 4.8.1, the data collected for this study consist of

repeated weekly observations where each participant is measured on an identical set of

occasions. Because the measurement occasions were equally and identically spaced for

all participants, the WEEK variable (at level-1) took on integer values between 0 (at the

initial assessment) and 8 (at the final assessment). According to Peugh and Enders

(2005) and Singer and Willet (2003) centering WEEK in this manner allows the

59
intercept to be interpreted as the true estimated initial status (i.e., the expected value of

the outcome variable when WEEK = 0).

4.9 Sample Size for Multilevel Modeling Analyses

In multilevel analysis, the maximum likelihood estimation methods assume that

the sample size is large (Hox, 2002). Thus, in this experimental study, 162 groups (from

772 individuals respondents), with three to ten participants per group, and with nine

waves of data were initially collected in the analysis to test the research hypotheses.

Since this is a longitudinal study, the presence of missing data is practically

unavoidable even if it is well-controlled. The missing data in longitudinal studies tends

to accumulate over time due to either item non-response, wave non-response or

participant drop out. Participants might respond on only a subset of the study variables,

miss a particular measurement wave or fail to follow-up or completely drop out from

the study (Hedeker & Gibbons, 1997). In this study, missing data occurred primarily

due to participants who failed to return their weekly diary for a particular week. The

experimental design and the weekly number of respondents involved in this study are

presented in Appendix B.

For this study, cases with missing data were not included in the analyses. Even

though the multi-level analysis procedure would permit the inclusion of cases with

missing values, it was considered that students who missed some weeks of classes

might not have all the necessary information to make informed predictions about the

project, compared to students who attended all classes.

From the 772 initial participant cases, 64 cases were deleted from the analysis.

This comprised 56 cases that contained missing data on at least one occasion, 6

participants who failed to return the diary on week eight indicating the actual

completion time, and 2 cases that were deleted because the project group had only one

group member remaining. The multilevel analysis cannot be conducted on groups with

60
only one member. Therefore, to test the hypotheses concerning completion time

prediction, data from 708 participants were used. Completion time predictions were

assessed until week 6 of the 8-week project. The seventh week data were not included in

the analysis as it was so close to the project submission deadline, by which time

participants knew they must finish the project.

For the outcome predictions, data from 689 participants were used to test the

hypotheses. Participants’ outcome predictions were measured until week 8 (final week)

of the project. During weeks 7 and 8, more respondents failed to hand in their diaries.

Therefore, out of 772 the initial participant cases, 83 cases were deleted with 79 cases

involving missing data on at least one occasion and 4 cases were deleted as the project

group had only one group member remaining.

For the confidence in prediction of project completion time and outcome, the

same number of cases was used as in testing the hypotheses for completion time and

outcome prediction. A total number of 708 and 689 participants were used respectively.

To test the hypotheses concerning the affective response towards project

outcome success and failure, 552 participant cases were used. At the end of the

experiment, where actual affective response was collected, 629 participants (from the

total of 772 students) took part. Of these 629 participants, 461 viewed their assignment

grade as a success, while 168 viewed their assignment grade as a failure. Thirty-nine

cases were deleted from the participants who viewed their assignment grade as a

success, of which 33 cases were deleted due to missing data on at least one occasion and

6 cases were deleted as only one group member remained. For participants who view

their assignment grade as a failure, 38 cases were deleted, comprising 15 cases deleted

due to missing data on at least on one occasion, and 23 cases deleted as only one group

member remained.

61
4.10 Data Cleaning

Prior to the primary analyses, the data were examined for data entry accuracy,

outliers, and distributional properties. Outliers were detected using both z-scores (with a

cut-off point of + 3SD) and Mahalanobis distance (a cut-off point of .001). At the end of

these procedures, 60 outliers were detected. Since there was no difference in the results

if the outliers were taken out or not, all the participant cases were used for further

analysis.

4.11 Conclusion

In this chapter, the method and the analysis strategy for the first study have been

described, including the design of the experiment, the data collection process, the

selection of the participants, the experimental procedure and the research measures.

This chapter also explains the adoption of the three-level modeling approach to test the

research hypotheses. The results of these tests are reported in the next chapter, Chapter

5.

62
CHAPTER 5

RESULTS – STUDY 1

5.1 Introduction

Chapter 5 reports results for the first study. The chapter begins with an overview

of the demographic characteristics of the participants in the experiment. It then reports

the results as graphs of mean responses and this is followed by the hierarchical linear

modeling (HLM) analyses, which test the hypotheses proposed in Chapter 3.

5.2 Demographic Characteristics of the Participants

78.6% of those who participated in this experiment were female. The average

age of the participants was 21 years old. Malays constituted 71.6% of the participants,

followed by 19.8% Malaysian Chinese, and 5.7% Malaysian Indians. The remaining

2.8% of the respondents reported that they were from other ethnic groups. On average,

participants in this experiment had completed 3 semesters of university study. The

detailed demographic characteristics of the participants are presented in Appendix C.

5. 3 Hypothesis Testing

Results for the first study are presented according to each category of the

dependent variables, starting with predictions made regarding task completion time,

followed by results for outcome predictions, confidence in predictions of completion

time, confidence in predictions of outcome, and predictions of affective response

towards outcome success and outcome failure. The results are first described through

graphical analysis, where means of responses over weeks of the experiment are plotted,

and then statistical analyses are presented.

63
5.3.1 Completion Time Predictions

Figure 5.1 plots means of completion time predictions made by 708 participants

on day 1 of the experiment and means for their actual project assignment completion

times. Standard error bars were not included in the graph because of the multilevel

nature of the data. Inspection of Figure 5.1 suggests that on the first day of the

experiment, participants in each of the four conditions predicted that they would take

less days to complete their project assignments than they actually did take, consistent

with the planning fallacy

56.00

55.00

54.00
Mean completion time (in days)

53.00

52.00 Inside
Outside
51.00
Unpacked
50.00 Control

49.00

48.00

47.00

46.00
Day 1 Actual
Time

Figure 5.1. Mean initial predicted and actual completion time for the four
conditions. (n=708)

Participants continued to make predictions each week. Figure 5.2 plots mean

completion time predictions made by 702 participants over the seven weeks of the

project and their actual completion times. There are fewer participants because some did

not complete all weeks. Inspection of the graph suggests that the predicted number of

64
days increases over time for all the four conditions. On average, participants in the

inside view and packed (control) conditions appear to be more optimistic about their

project completion time than participants in the unpacked and outside conditions.

56.00

55.00

54.00
Mean completion time (in days)

53.00

52.00 Inside
Outside
51.00
Unpacked
50.00 Control

49.00

48.00

47.00

46.00
day 1 week week week week week week week actual
1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Week

Figure 5.2. Mean predicted and actual completion time for the four conditions.
(n=702)

The hypotheses concerned the bias in predictions and not the actual predictions

made, as shown in Figures 5.1 and 5.2. Figure 5.3 plots mean prediction bias for the

completion time predictions made by 708 individuals over the first 6 weeks of the 8-

week project. The data for the seventh week were not included as it was so close to the

project submission deadline, by which time participants knew they must finish the

project. Completion time prediction bias was measured by subtracting predicted

completion time from actual completion time, so the positive numbers on the vertical

axis of Figure 5.3 indicate that participants underestimated how long the project would

take to complete, which is consistent with the planning fallacy. Inspection of Figure 5.3

65
suggests that the bias is less for the outside view than for the inside view, which is

consistent with Hypothesis 1.1a, and that the bias is less for the unpacked condition than

for the packed (control) condition, which is consistent with Hypothesis 1.1b. There

appears to be little difference between the outside view condition and the unpacked

condition. Inspection of the slopes of the graph suggests that the completion time

prediction bias was reduced as the project submission deadline drew closer. These

effects are tested in the following sections using multilevel models.

The results of the unconditional three-level HLM model are described first,

followed by the conditional three-level HLM model.

6.00

5.00
Mean predictions bias (in days)

4.00
Inside
Outside
3.00
Unpacked
Control
2.00

1.00

0.00
day 1 week 1 week 2 week 3 week 4 week 5 week 6
Weeks

Figure 5.3. Mean prediction bias of individuals’ project completion time


predictions for the four conditions. Bias is actual minus predicted completion time.
(n=708)

66
5.3.1.1 Unconditional Three-level HLM Models

In an initial analysis, the unconditional HLM model was estimated for the

completion time prediction bias dependent variable, with time (WEEK) as the only

predictor in the regression equation. The following are the equations for the

unconditional model which was adapted from Raudenbush and Bryk (2002, p.238).

Level-1 Model

Ytij = π0ij + π1ij(WEEK) + etij (1)

Level-2 Model

π0ij = β00j + r0ij (2a)

π1ij = β10j + r1ij (2b)

Level-3 Model

β00j = γ000 + u00j (3a)

β10j = γ100 + u10j (3b)

where

Ytij is the outcome at time t for participant i in group j;

π0ij is the initial status of participant ij, that is, the expected outcome for that participant

on the first day (when WEEK = 0);

π1ij is the prediction bias rate for participant ij during the weeks;

etij is a random “time effect”. These random time effects are assumed to be normally

distributed with mean of 0, and variance σ2;

β00j represents the mean initial status within group j;

γ000 is the overall mean initial status;

r0ij and r1ij are random “participant effects”. These random participant effects are

assumed to be normally distributed with mean of 0, and variance σ2;

β10j is the mean weekly prediction bias rate within group j;

γ100 is the overall mean weekly prediction bias rate;

67
u00j and u10j are the random “group effects”. These random group effects are assumed to

be normally distributed with mean of 0, and variance σ2.

The results for this unconditional HLM model are presented in Table 5.1. The

estimated initial status (G000) for completion time predictions bias was 4.400 and was

significant, indicating that there was bias in completion time predictions. The average

weekly prediction bias rate (G100) was estimated at -0.454 and was significant. This

indicates that the bias became smaller as the project completion date drew nearer. For

completion time prediction, the variance among participants within groups for initial

status and weekly prediction bias rates (i.e., R0 and R1) and the variance between

groups for mean initial status and mean weekly prediction bias rates (i.e., U00 and U10)

were statistically significant.

Based on these variance component estimates, the percentage of variation that

lies between groups for both initial status and weekly prediction bias rate can be

computed. The following formula was used to estimate the percentage of variation that

lies between groups for initial status and prediction bias rate. It was adapted from

Raudenbush and Bryk (2002, p.239).

% variance between groups on πpjk = τβpp / τβpp + τπpp (4)

where

p = 0, …, P, in this case, p = 0,1

πpjk is either the initial status (π0ij) or the prediction bias rate (π1ij)

τβpp is the random group effect (u00j or u10j)

τπpp is the random individual effect (r0ij or r1ij)

68
The results show that 16.03% of the variance in initial status for completion time

prediction bias lies between project groups. For completion time prediction bias rate,

18.14% of the variance is between groups. In summary, the unconditional model for

completion time predictions revealed significant variance of participants for initial

status and weekly prediction bias rates and between groups for mean initial status and

mean prediction bias rates. The following section reports results for conditional HLM

models that examined the effects of the four conditions [inside, outside, unpacked and

packed (control)] on participants’ predictions regarding task completion time.

69
Table 5.1
Prediction bias for project completion time: A three-level fully unconditional model

Prediction bias for project completion time


Fixed Effect Coefficient S.Error t-ratio p-value

For INTERCEPT1, P0
INTERCEPT2, B00
INTERCEPT3, (Average initial status),G000 4.400 0.302 14.566 0.000***
For Week slope, P1
INTERCEPT2, B10
INTERCEPT3, (Average weekly prediction bias rate), G100 -0.454 0.051 -8.943 0.000***

Random effect Variance Component df Chi-square p-value

Level-1 Effect (Week), E 10.950


Level-2
INTERCEPT1, (Individual initial status), R0 24.861 358 2094.508 0.000***
WEEK slope, (Individual weekly prediction bias rate), R1 0.521 358 803.215 0.000***
INTERCEPT1/INTERCEPT2,(Group mean status),U00 4.748 157 243.467 0.000***
WEEK / INTERCEPT2, (Group mean prediction bias rate),U10 0.116 157 235.897 0.000***

Level-1 Coefficient Percentage of Variance Between Groups


Initial status, P0 16.03
Prediction bias rate, P1 18.14

Deviance = 20455.174
Number of estimated parameters = 9
A three-level model with week = level - 1, individual participants = level - 2, and group = level - 3. Number of participants = 708 and groups = 158.
The dependent variable is measured as the difference between participants’ predicted and actual project completion time (in days).

p<0.10,*p <.05, **p<.01, ***p<.001

70
5.3.1.2 Conditional Three-level Model for Completion Time Prediction Bias

To test hypotheses 1.1a and 1.1b, the intercepts of the multilevel model were

tested to compare the effects of the outside and unpacking approaches against the inside

and packed (control) approaches respectively, for completion time predictions. These

four conditions were dummy coded and separate analyses were conducted with different

conditions coded as the reference condition. Since there were no strong theories on

which control variables should be included in the model, an exploratory procedure was

used. As suggested by Hox (2002), the procedure begins by adding the control variables

step by step. At each step, the results were inspected to see if the control variables were

significant and how much residual error remained. Fixed parameters were estimated

first before introducing random parameters.

The final full model that describes the effects of the four conditions [with

packed (control) as the reference condition] on completion time predictions is as

follows. Other versions of the equation with outside and inside conditions as the

reference conditions are presented in Appendix D. A three-level model with time

(WEEK) at level 1, and the three conditions (inside, outside and unpacked) at level 2 as

predictors of completion time predictions was estimated. The equation at level 3 had no

predictors. All the level-2 participants’ potential control variables (e.g. age, semester,

gender and race) were tested as were those at level 3 for the project groups (group size

and teacher), but they had no significant effects. Therefore, they were excluded from the

final model.

Level-1 Model
Ytij = P0 + P1*(WEEK) + E

Level-2 Model
P0 = B00 + B01*(Inside) + B02*(Outside) + B03*(Unpacked) + R0
P1 = B10 + B11*(Inside) + B12*(Outside) + B13*(Unpacked) + R1
71
Level-3 Model
B00 = G000 + U00
B01 = G010
B02 = G020
B03 = G030
B10 = G100 + U10
B11 = G110
B12 = G120
B13 = G130

where

Ytij is the outcome at time t for participant i in group j;

P0 is the initial status of participant ij, that is, the expected outcome for that participant

on the first day (when WEEK = 0);

P1 is the prediction bias rate for participant ij during the weeks;

E is a random “time effect”. These random time effects are assumed to be normally

distributed with mean of 0, and variance σ2;

B00 represents the mean initial status within group j;

G000 is the overall mean initial status;

R0 and R1 are random “participant effects”. These random participant effects are

assumed to be normally distributed with mean of 0, and variance σ2;

B10 is the mean weekly prediction bias rate within group j;

G100 is the overall mean weekly prediction bias rate;

U00 and U10 are the random “group effects”. These random group effects are assumed

to be normally distributed with mean of 0, and variance σ2.

72
The results for this three-level hierarchical regression for the completion time

bias data, with the packed (control) condition as the reference are summarized in Table

5.2a. The intercepts for initial status in Table 5.2a show that the outside view and

unpacked condition were both significantly less biased than the packed (control)

condition in their completion time predictions at the start of the project. This result for

the unpacked condition supports Hypothesis 1.1b. The inside view condition was not

significantly different to the packed (control) condition.

To test Hypothesis 1.1a, which compares the inside and outside view conditions,

and to explore whether there was a difference between the outside view and unpacked

conditions, an analysis of the same model was conducted with the outside view

condition as the reference. The results are reported in Table 5.2b. Though inspection of

Figure 5.1 suggests that the bias is less for the outside view than that for the inside view,

the intercepts for initial status in Table 5.2b were not significantly different between the

outside and the inside view condition, failing to provide statistical support for

Hypothesis 1.1a. The outside view condition was not significantly different to the

unpacked condition in the initial status intercept. Results with the inside view condition

as the reference are presented in Appendix E-1 on page 337-338.

73
Table 5.2a
Prediction bias for project completion time: Three-level model of the effects of manipulations relative to the packed (control) condition
Prediction bias for project completion time
Fixed effect Coefficient S.Error t-ratio p-value

Initial status: For INTERCEPT1, P0


INTERCEPT2, B00
INTERCEPT3, (Average initial status),G000 5.695 0.529 10.767 0.000***
For Inside (UP1), B01
INTERCEPT3, G010 -1.260 0.815 -1.545 0.123
For Outside (UP2), B02
INTERCEPT3, G020 -2.070 0.798 -2.593 0.010*
For Unpacked (UP3)
INTERCEPT3, G030 -1.904 0.792 -2.403 0.017*
Rate of change: For Week slope, P1
INTERCEPT2, B10
INTERCEPT3, (Average weekly prediction bias rate), G100 -0.552 0.106 -5.195 0.000***
For Inside (UP1), B11
INTERCEPT3, G110 -0.025 0.150 -0.167 0.868
For Outside (UP2), B12
INTERCEPT3, G120 0.239 0.139 1.720 0.086†
For Unpacked (UP3), B13
INTERCEPT3, G130 0.183 0.144 1.267 0.206

Random effect Variance Component df Chi-square p-value

Level-1 Effect (Week), E 10.950


Level-2 (student within groups)
INTERCEPT1, (Individual initial status), R0 24.958 355 2094.523 0.000***
WEEK slope, (Individual weekly prediction bias rate), R1 0.520 355 803.221 0.000***
Level-3 (between groups)
INTERCEPT1 / INTERCEPT2,(Group mean status),U00 3.889 157 228.716 0.000***
WEEK / INTERCEPT2, (Group mean prediction bias rate), U10 0.107 157 230.811 0.000***

74
Table 5.2a (Continued)

Random effect Variance Component df Chi-square p-value

Level-1 Coefficient Percentage of Variance Between Groups


Initial status, P0 13.48
Prediction bias rate, P1 17.06

Deviance = 20444.451
Number of estimated parameters = 15
A Three-level model with time = level-1, individual participants = level-2 and group = level-3. Number of participants = 708 and groups = 158
The dependent variable is measured as the difference between participants’ predicted and actual project completion time (in days).

p<0.10,*p <.05, **p<.01, ***p<.001

75
Table 5.2b
Prediction bias for project completion time: Three-level model of the effects of manipulations relative to the outside view condition
Prediction bias for project completion time
Fixed effect Coefficient S.Error t-ratio p-value

Initial status: For INTERCEPT1, P0


INTERCEPT2, B00
INTERCEPT3, (Average initial status),G000 3.625 0.598 6.060 0.000***
For Inside (IO1), B01
INTERCEPT3, G010 0.810 0.862 0.940 0.348
For Unpacked (IO2), B02
INTERCEPT3, G020 0.166 0.840 0.198 0.843
For Packed (IO3)
INTERCEPT3, G030 2.070 0.798 2.593 0.010*
Rate of change: For Week slope, P1
INTERCEPT2, B10
INTERCEPT3, (Average weekly prediction bias rate), G100 -0.313 0.089 -3.501 0.001**
For Inside (IO1), B11
INTERCEPT3, G110 -0.264 0.138 -1.913 0.056†
For Unpacked (IO2), B12
INTERCEPT3, G120 -0.056 0.132 -0.422 0.673
For Packed (IO3), B13
INTERCEPT3, G130 -0.239 0.139 -1.720 0.086†

Random effect Variance Component df Chi-square p-value

Level-1 Effect (Week), E 10.950


Level-2 (student within groups)
INTERCEPT1, (Individual initial status), R0 24.958 355 2094.523 0.000***
WEEK slope, (Individual weekly prediction bias rate), R1 0.520 355 803.221 0.000***
Level-3 (between groups)
INTERCEPT1 / INTERCEPT2,(Group mean status),U00 3.889 157 228.716 0.000***
WEEK / INTERCEPT2, (Group mean prediction bias rate), U10 0.107 157 230.811 0.000***

76
Table 5.2b (Continued)

Random effect Variance Component df Chi-square p-value

Level-1 Coefficient Percentage of Variance Between Groups


Initial status, P0 13.48
Prediction bias rate, P1 17.06

Deviance = 20444.451
Number of estimated parameters = 15
A Three-level model with time = level-1, individual participants = level-2 and group = level-3. Number of participants = 708 and groups = 158
The dependent variable is measured as the difference between participants’ predicted and actual project completion time (in days).

p<0.10,*p <.05, **p<.01, ***p<.001

77
5.3.2 Outcome Predictions

Figure 5.4 plots mean prediction bias for the outcome predictions made by

individuals over the entire eight weeks of the project. Outcome prediction bias was

measured by subtracting the predicted project mark from the actual project mark, so the

negative numbers on the vertical axis of Figure 5.4 indicate that, on average,

participants overestimated the marks they would receive for their projects. As for

completion time prediction, this bias is optimistic, but it is in the opposite direction to

the underestimation of completion time.

Importantly, the outside view and unpacked conditions again have similar

results, but while they were less biased than the inside view and packed conditions for

completion time predictions, they appear to be more biased for outcome predictions.

Therefore, inspection of Figure 5.4 suggests that Hypotheses 1.2a and 1.2b are not

supported, and actually in the wrong direction.

0.00
day 1 week week week week week week week week
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
-2.00
Mean prediction bias

-4.00
Inside
Outside
-6.00
Unpacked
Control
-8.00

-10.00

-12.00
Weeks

Figure 5.4. Mean prediction bias of individuals’ project outcome predictions for
the four conditions. Actual outcome (in marks) minus predicted. (n=689)

78
5.3.2.1 Unconditional Three-level Model

An unconditional HLM model was estimated for the outcome prediction bias dependent

variable, with time (WEEK) as the only predictor in the regression equations. The

results for this unconditional HLM model are presented in Table 5.3. For outcome

prediction bias, the estimated initial status (G000) was -7.778 and was significant,

indicating that bias did occur. The average weekly prediction bias rate (G100) was

estimated at -0.147, and was also significant, indicating that participants’ outcome

predictions became more biased as the end of the project drew nearer. The variance

among participants within groups for initial status and weekly prediction bias rates (i.e.,

R0 and R1), and the variance between groups for mean initial status and mean weekly

prediction bias rates (i.e., U00 and U10) were statistically significant. 69.52% of the

variance in initial status for outcome prediction bias lies between groups, and 14.98% of

the variance for the outcome prediction bias rate is between groups.

In short, the unconditional model for outcome predictions showed significant

variance of participants for initial status and weekly prediction bias rates and between

groups for mean initial status and mean prediction bias rates. Results for the conditional

HLM model are reported next.

79
Table 5.3
Prediction bias for project outcome: A three-level fully unconditional model

Prediction bias for project outcome


Fixed Effect Coefficient S.Error t-ratio p-value

For INTERCEPT1, P0
INTERCEPT2, B00
INTERCEPT3, (Average initial status),G000 -7.778 0.859 -9.059 0.000***
For Week slope, P1
INTERCEPT2, B10
INTERCEPT3, (Average weekly prediction bias rate), G100 -0.147 0.036 -4.079 0.000***

Random effect Variance Component df Chi-square p-value

Level-1 Effect (Week), E 10.105


Level-2
INTERCEPT1, (Individual initial status), R0 42.861 348 4261.302 0.000***
WEEK slope, (Individual weekly prediction bias rate), R1 0.297 348 951.367 0.000***
INTERCEPT1/INTERCEPT2,(Group mean status),U00 97.775 155 1233.888 0.000***
WEEK / INTERCEPT2, (Group mean prediction bias rate),U10 0.052 155 214.865 0.001**

Level-1 Coefficient Percentage of Variance Between Groups


Initial status, P0 69.52
Prediction bias rate, P1 14.98

Deviance = 26030.334
Number of estimated parameters = 9
A three-level model with week = level-1, individual participants = level -2, and group = level - 3. Number of participants = 689 and groups = 156.
The dependent variable is measured as the difference between participants’ predicted and actual project marks (in percentage).

p<0.10,*p <.05, **p<.01 ***p<.001

80
5.3.2.2 Conditional Three-level Model

Hypotheses 1.2a and 1.2b were tested by examining the intercepts of the

multilevel model to compare the effects of the outside and unpacked approaches against

the inside and packed (control) approaches for outcome prediction bias. These four

conditions were dummy coded and again separate analyses were conducted with

different conditions coded as the reference condition. Similar steps were taken with the

outcome predictions as for the completion time predictions, where the procedure begins

by adding the control variables step by step. At each step, the results were inspected to

see which control variables were significant and how much residual error remained.

Fixed parameters were estimated first before using random parameters.

The final full models that describe the effect of the four conditions [with packed

(control), outside and inside as the reference condition] on outcome predictions are

presented in Appendix D. For outcome predictions, I estimated a three-level model with

time (WEEK) as the only predictor at level 1, the three conditions (inside, outside and

unpacked) and semester as predictors at level 2, and teachers and group mean for

semester as predictors at level 3. Unlike the predictions of completion time, significant

effects of the teacher were found for the student predictions of their mark, and effect-

coded teacher variables are included in this analysis. In this three-level hierarchical

linear model, semester was grand mean centered.

81
Table 5.4a presents the results for the three-level hierarchical regressions with

the packed (control) condition as the reference. Initial status was not significantly

different from the packed condition for any of the experimental conditions. Thus, while

inspection of Figure 5.2 suggests that the unpacked condition was more biased than the

packed condition, the statistical analysis does not confirm this.

In an analysis of the same model with the outside view condition as the

reference (shown in Table 5.4b), the intercept for the inside view condition was

significantly different to that for the outside view condition, but not in the direction

stated in Hypothesis 1.2a. The intercept for the unpacked condition did not differ

significantly from the outside view condition. Results with the inside view condition as

the reference is presented in Appendix E-2 on page 339-340.

82
Table 5.4a
Prediction bias for project outcome: Three-level model of the effects of manipulations relative to the packed (control) condition
Prediction bias for project outcome
Fixed effect Coefficient S.Error t-ratio p-value

Initial status: For INTERCEPT1, P0


INTERCEPT2, B00
INTERCEPT3, (Average initial status),G000 -5.308 3.490 -1.521 0.130
TEACHER1(T1), G001 6.322 2.123 2.978 0.004**
TEACHER2(T2), G002 -9.667 2.852 -3.389 0.001**
TEACHER3(T3), G003 -12.551 4.309 -2.913 0.005**
TEACHER4(T4), G004 8.468 4.023 2.105 0.037*
TEACHER5(T5), G005 6.626 3.008 2.203 0.029*
TEACHER6(T6), G006 2.433 3.312 0.735 0.464
TEACHER7(T7), G007 -0.631 1.804 -0.350 0.727
TEACHER8(T8), G008 -10.376 2.253 -4.606 0.000***
TEACHER9(T9), G009 2.732 2.317 1.179 0.241
SEM_MEAN, G0010 -0.615 1.014 -0.606 0.545
For Inside (UP1), B01
INTERCEPT3, G010 1.999 1.752 1.141 0.255
For Outside (UP2), B02
INTERCEPT3, G020 -1.696 1.830 -0.927 0.355
For Unpacked (UP3), B03
INTERCEPT3, G030 -2.450 1.891 -1.296 0.196
For semester, B04
INTERCEPT3, G040 -2.055 0.461 -4.459 0.000***
Rate of change: For Week slope, P1
INTERCEPT2, B10
INTERCEPT3, (Average weekly prediction bias rate), G100 -0.147 0.036 -4.079 0.000***

Random effect Variance Component df Chi-square p-value

Level-1 Effect (Week), E 10.105


Level-2 (student within groups)

83
Table 5.4a (Continued)

Random effect Variance Component df Chi-square p-value


INTERCEPT1, (Individual initial status), R0 41.380 344 4124.282 0.000***
WEEK slope, (Individual weekly prediction bias rate), R1 0.297 348 951.367 0.000***
Level-3 (between groups)
INTERCEPT1 / INTERCEPT2,(Group mean status),U00 52.197 145 755.179 0.000***
WEEK / INTERCEPT2, (Group mean prediction bias rate), U10 0.052 155 214.727 0.001**

Level-1 Coefficient Percentage of Variance Between Groups


Initial status, P0 55.78
Prediction bias rate, P1 14.87

Deviance = 25933.896
Number of estimated parameters = 23

A Three-level model with time = level-1, individual participants = level-2 and group = level-3. Number of participants = 689 and groups = 156
The dependent variable is measured as the difference between participants’ predicted and actual project marks (in percentage).

p<0.10,*p <.05, **p<.01, ***p<.001

84
Table 5.4b
Prediction bias for project outcome: Three-level model of the effects of manipulations relative to the outside view condition
Prediction bias for project outcome
Fixed effect Coefficient S.Error t-ratio p-value

Initial status: For INTERCEPT1, P0


INTERCEPT2, B00
INTERCEPT3, (Average initial status),G000 -7.005 3.476 -2.015 0.045*
TEACHER1(T1), G001 6.322 2.123 2.978 0.004**
TEACHER2(T2), G002 -9.667 2.852 -3.389 0.001**
TEACHER3(T3), G003 -12.551 4.309 -2.913 0.005**
TEACHER4(T4), G004 8.468 4.023 2.105 0.037*
TEACHER5(T5), G005 6.626 3.008 2.203 0.029*
TEACHER6(T6), G006 2.433 3.312 0.735 0.464
TEACHER7(T7), G007 -0.631 1.804 -0.350 0.727
TEACHER8(T8), G008 -10.376 2.253 -4.606 0.000***
TEACHER9(T9), G009 2.732 2.317 1.179 0.241
SEM_MEAN, G0010 -0.615 1.014 -0.606 0.545
For Inside (IO1), B01
INTERCEPT3, G010 3.696 1.853 1.994 0.046*
For Unpacked (IO2), B02
INTERCEPT3, G020 -0.754 1.991 -0.379 0.705
For Packed (IO3), B03
INTERCEPT3, G030 1.696 1.830 0.927 0.355
For semester, B04
INTERCEPT3, G040 -2.055 0.461 -4.459 0.000***
Rate of change: For Week slope, P1
INTERCEPT2, B10
INTERCEPT3, (Average weekly prediction bias rate), G100 -0.147 0.036 -4.079 0.000***

Random effect Variance Component df Chi-square p-value

Level-1 Effect (Week), E 10.105


Level-2 (student within groups)

85
Table 5.4b (Continued)

Random effect Variance Component df Chi-square p-value


INTERCEPT1, (Individual initial status), R0 41.380 344 4124.282 0.000***
WEEK slope, (Individual weekly prediction bias rate), R1 0.297 348 951.367 0.000***
Level-3 (between groups)
INTERCEPT1 / INTERCEPT2,(Group mean status),U00 52.197 145 755.179 0.000***
WEEK / INTERCEPT2, (Group mean prediction bias rate), U10 0.052 155 214.727 0.001**

Level-1 Coefficient Percentage of Variance Between Groups


Initial status, P0 55.78
Prediction bias rate, P1 14.87

Deviance = 25933.896
Number of estimated parameters = 23

A Three-level model with time = level-1, individual participants = level-2 and group = level-3. Number of participants = 689 and groups = 156
The dependent variable is measured as the difference between participants’ predicted and actual project marks (in percentage).

p<0.10,*p <.05, **p<.01, ***p<.001

86
5.3.3 Confidence in Predictions of Project Completion Time

Figure 5.5 plots means of participants’ ratings of confidence in their predictions

of completion time in each of the four conditions over the first 6 weeks of the 8-week

project. As shown in Figure 5.5, there appears to be little change in confidence over

time for participants in each of the four conditions, contrary to Hypotheses 1.3a and

1.3b. Inspection of Figure 5.5 also suggests that participants in the outside view

condition were less confident in their predictions of completion time than participants in

the inside view condition. In contrast, participants in the unpacked condition appear to

be more confident in their predictions of completion time than participants in the packed

(control) condition. Once again there appears to be little difference between the outside

view condition and the unpacked conditions.

84.00
Mean confidence in predictions (in percentage)

82.00

80.00
Inside
Outside
78.00
Unpacked
Control
76.00

74.00

72.00
day 1 week 1 week 2 week 3 week 4 week 5 week 6
Weeks

Figure 5.5. Mean confidence in predictions of project completion time for the four
conditions. (n=708)

87
5.3.3.1 Unconditional Three-level Model

An unconditional multilevel model was estimated for the confidence in

predictions of completion time dependent variables, with time (WEEK) as the only

predictor in the regression equations. The results for this unconditional HLM model are

presented in Table 5.5. The estimated initial status (G000) for confidence in predictions

of completion time was 77.897. The average weekly confidence change rate (G100) was

estimated at 0.204, which was marginally significant. The variance among participants

within groups for initial status and weekly confidence change rates (i.e., R0 and R1) and

the variance components between groups for mean initial status and mean weekly

confidence change rates (i.e., U00 and U10) were statistically significant.

Regarding the percentage that lies between groups for both initial status and

confidence change rate, the results show that 20.67% of the variance in the initial status

for confidence in predictions of completion time lies between groups. 15.59% of the

variance for confidence change rate is between groups. In summary, the unconditional

models revealed significant variance of participants for initial status and weekly

confidence change rate and between groups for mean initial status and mean confidence

change rates. Results for the conditional HLM model are reported in the next section.

88
Table 5.5
Confidence in predictions of project completion time: A three-level fully unconditional model

Confidence in predictions of project completion time


Fixed Effect Coefficient S.Error t-ratio p-value

For INTERCEPT1, P0
INTERCEPT2, B00
INTERCEPT3, (Average initial status),G000 77.897 0.871 89.446 0.000***
For Week slope, P1
INTERCEPT2, B10
INTERCEPT3, (Average weekly confidence change rate), G100 0.204 0.116 1.757 0.080†

Random effect Variance Component df Chi-square p-value

Level-1 Effect (Week), E 63.493


Level-2
INTERCEPT1, (Individual initial status), R0 186.347 358 2640.614 0.000***
WEEK slope, (Individual weekly confidence change rate), R1 2.768 358 821.164 0.000***
INTERCEPT1/INTERCEPT2,(Group mean status),U00 48.554 157 272.232 0.000***
WEEK / INTERCEPT2, (Group mean confidence change rate),U10 0.512 157 199.396 0.012*

Level-1 Coefficient Percentage of Variance Between Groups


Initial status, P0 20.67
Confidence change rate, P1 15.59

Deviance = 27343.061
Number of estimated parameters = 9
A three-level model with week = level-1, individual participants = level -2, and group = level - 3. Number of participants = 708 and groups = 158.
The dependent variable is measured from the 0% end of the line and centimeters were converted to a number out of 100%.

p<0.10,*p <.05, **p<.01 ***p<.001

89
5.3.3.2 Conditional Three-level Model

Hypotheses 1.3a and 1.3b were tested by examining the slopes of the multilevel

model to compare the effects of the outside and unpacking approaches against the inside

and packed (control) approaches on changes to confidence in predictions of completion

time. I estimated a three-level model with time (WEEK) as the only predictor at level 1,

and the three conditions (inside, outside, and unpacked) as predictors at level 2. The

equation at level 3 had no predictors. As for the earlier analysis, similar steps have been

conducted by adding each of the participant control variables (e.g. age, semester, gender

and race) including the group size and teacher into the model. There were no significant

effects for those variables, and therefore, they were excluded from the final model. The

model equation for the effect of the three conditions on confidence in predictions of

completion time [with packed (control) as the reference condition] was the same as that

for completion time bias.

Table 5.6a presents the three-level hierarchical regression results for confidence

in predictions of completion time with the packed (control) condition as the reference.

The slopes for weekly confidence change rate were not significantly different from the

packed (control) condition for either the inside view condition or the unpacked

condition. This result for the unpacked condition does not support Hypothesis 1.3b. The

slope for the outside view condition was marginally significantly steeper than the

packed (control) condition.

In an analysis with the outside view condition as the reference (Table 5.6b), the

slopes for weekly confidence change rate were not significantly different from the

outside condition for the inside view condition or the unpacked condition. This result

for the outside condition does not support Hypothesis 1.3a. Results with the inside view

condition as the reference is presented in Appendix E-3 on page 341-342.

90
Table 5.6a
Confidence in predictions of project completion time: Three-level model of the effects of manipulations relative to the packed (control)
condition

Confidence in predictions of project completion time


Fixed effect Coefficient S.Error t-ratio p-value

Initial status: For INTERCEPT1, P0


INTERCEPT2, B00
INTERCEPT3, (Average initial status),G000 77.335 1.599 48.373 0.000***
For Inside (UP1), B01
INTERCEPT3, G010 2.623 2.424 1.082 0.280
For Outside (UP2), B02
INTERCEPT3, G020 -0.328 2.191 -0.150 0.882
For Unpacked (UP3)
INTERCEPT3, G030 -0.033 2.511 -0.013 0.990
Rate of change: For Week slope, P1
INTERCEPT2, B10
INTERCEPT3, (Average weekly confidence change rate), G100 -0.048 0.209 -0.228 0.820
For Inside (UP1), B11
INTERCEPT3, G110 0.219 0.283 0.774 0.439
For Outside (UP2), B12
INTERCEPT3, G120 0.468 0.278 1.680 0.093†
For Unpacked (UP3), B13
INTERCEPT3, G130 0.333 0.371 0.898 0.370

Random effect Variance Component df Chi-square p-value

Level-1 Effect (Week), E 63.493


Level-2 (Student within groups)
INTERCEPT1, (Individual initial status), R0 186.451 355 2640.617 0.000***
WEEK slope, (Individual weekly confidence change rate), R1 2.760 355 821.165 0.000***
Level-3 (between groups)
INTERCEPT1 / INTERCEPT2,(Group mean status),U00 46.956 157 268.369 0.000***
WEEK / INTERCEPT2, (Group mean confidence change rate), U10 0.494 157 197.163 0.016*

91
Table 5.6a (Continued)

Random effect Variance Component df Chi-square p-value

Level-1 Coefficient Percentage of Variance Between Groups


Initial status, P0 20.12
Confidence change rate, P1 15.19

Deviance = 27338.901
Number of estimated parameters = 15
A three-level model with week = level-1, individual participants = level -2, and group = level - 3. Number of participants = 708 and groups = 158.
The dependent variable is measured from the 0% end of the line and centimeters were converted to a number out of 100%.

p<0.10,*p <.05, **p<.01 ***p<.001

92
Table 5.6b
Confidence in predictions of project completion time: Three-level model of the effects of manipulations relative to the outside view condition

Confidence in predictions of project completion time


Fixed effect Coefficient S.Error t-ratio p-value

Initial status: For INTERCEPT1, P0


INTERCEPT2, B00
INTERCEPT3, (Average initial status),G000 77.008 1.498 51.391 0.000***
For Inside (IO1), B01
INTERCEPT3, G010 2.951 2.359 1.251 0.212
For Unpacked (IO2), B02
INTERCEPT3, G020 0.295 2.448 0.120 0.905
For Packed (IO3)
INTERCEPT3, G030 0.328 2.191 0.150 0.882
Rate of change: For Week slope, P1
INTERCEPT2, B10
INTERCEPT3, (Average weekly confidence change rate), G100 0.420 0.184 2.288 0.023*
For Inside (IO1), B11
INTERCEPT3, G110 -0.248 0.265 -0.937 0.350
For Unpacked (IO2), B12
INTERCEPT3, G120 -0.135 0.357 -0.378 0.706
For Packed (IO3), B13
INTERCEPT3, G130 -0.468 0.278 -1.680 0.093†

Random effect Variance Component df Chi-square p-value

Level-1 Effect (Week), E 63.493


Level-2 (Student within groups)
INTERCEPT1, (Individual initial status), R0 186.451 355 2640.617 0.000***
WEEK slope, (Individual weekly confidence change rate), R1 2.760 355 821.165 0.000***
Level-3 (between groups)
INTERCEPT1 / INTERCEPT2,(Group mean status),U00 46.956 157 268.369 0.000***
WEEK / INTERCEPT2, (Group mean confidence change rate), U10 0.494 157 197.163 0.016*

93
Table 5.6b (Continued)

Random effect Variance Component df Chi-square p-value

Level-1 Coefficient Percentage of Variance Between Groups


Initial status, P0 20.12
Confidence change rate, P1 15.19

Deviance = 27338.901
Number of estimated parameters = 15
A three-level model with week = level-1, individual participants = level -2, and group = level - 3. Number of participants = 708 and groups = 158.
The dependent variable is measured from the 0% end of the line and centimeters were converted to a number out of 100%.

p<0.10,*p <.05, **p<.01 ***p<.001

94
5.3.4 Confidence in Predictions of Project Outcome

Figure 5.6 plots means of participants’ confidence in their predictions of an

outcome for the assignment mark in each of the four conditions over the entire eight

weeks of the project. Inspection of Figure 5.6 suggests that there is some small increase

in confidence in predictions of outcome over time for all the four conditions. The

amount of change appears to be the same for the four conditions and this is not

consistent with Hypothesis 1.4a and 1.4b. However, on average, participants in the

outside view, inside view and unpacked conditions appear to be more confident than

participants in the packed (control) condition. There appears to be little difference

between the three manipulated conditions.

84.00
Mean confidence in predictions (in percentage

82.00

80.00
Inside
Outside
78.00
Unpacked
Control
76.00

74.00

72.00
day 1 week week week week week week week week
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
WEek

Figure 5.6. Mean confidence in predictions of project outcome for the four
conditions. (n=689)

95
5.3.4.1 Unconditional Three-level Model

An unconditional multilevel model was estimated for the confidence in

predictions of the outcome, with time (WEEK) as the only predictor in the regression

equations. The results for this unconditional HLM model are presented in Table 5.7.

The estimated initial status (G000) for confidence in predictions of outcome was

76.637. The average weekly confidence change rate (G100) was estimated at 0.313 and

was significant indicating that confidence in outcome predictions increased as the

project due date drew nearer. The variance among participants within groups for initial

status and weekly confidence change rates (i.e., R0 and R1) and the variance

components between groups for mean initial status and mean weekly confidence change

rates (i.e., U00 and U10) were statistically significant.

As for the percentage that lies between groups for both initial status and

confidence change rate, the results show that 13.87% of the variance in the initial status

for confidence in predictions of outcome lies between groups. 20.02% of the variance

for confidence change rate is between groups. In summary, the unconditional model for

confidence in predictions of outcome revealed significant variance among individuals

for initial status and weekly confidence change rate and between groups for mean initial

status and mean confidence change rates. The results for the conditional HLM model

are presented next.

96
Table 5.7
Confidence in predictions of project outcome: A three-level fully unconditional model

Confidence in predictions of project outcome


Fixed Effect Coefficient S.Error t-ratio p-value

For INTERCEPT1, P0
INTERCEPT2, B00
INTERCEPT3, (Average initial status),G000 76.637 0.809 94.695 0.000***
For Week slope, P1
INTERCEPT2, B10
INTERCEPT3, (Average weekly prediction bias rate), G100 0.313 0.084 3.718 0.000***

Random effect Variance Component df Chi-square p-value

Level-1 Effect (Week), E 61.640


Level-2
INTERCEPT1, (Individual initial status), R0 194.827 348 3334.193 0.000***
WEEK slope, (Individual weekly prediction bias rate), R1 1.336 348 809.823 0.000***
INTERCEPT1/INTERCEPT2,(Group mean status),U00 31.378 155 221.333 0.001**
WEEK / INTERCEPT2, (Group mean prediction bias rate),U10 0.334 155 223.308 0.000***

Level-1 Coefficient Percentage of Variance Between Groups


Initial status, P0 13.87
Prediction bias rate, P1 20.02

Deviance = 33761.008
Number of estimated parameters = 9
A three-level model with week = level-1, individual participants = level -2, and group = level - 3. Number of participants = 689 and groups = 156.
The dependent variable is measured from the 0% end of the line and centimeters were converted to a number out of 100%.

p<0.10,*p <.05, **p<.01 ***p<.001

97
5.3.4.2 Conditional Three-level Model

Hypotheses 1.4a and 1.4b were tested by examining the slopes of the multilevel

model to compare the effects of the outside and unpacking approaches against the inside

and packed (control) approaches on changes to confidence for predictions of outcome

success. Since adding each of the participants’ control variables (e.g. age, semester,

gender and race) including the group size and teacher gave no significant effects, I

estimated a similar three-level model as in the confidence in predictions of completion

time, with time (WEEK) as the only predictor at level 1, and the three conditions

(inside, outside and unpacked) as predictors at level 2. The equation at level 3 had no

predictors.

Results for this three-level hierarchical regression are summarized in Table 5.8a

with the packed (control) condition as the reference. The weekly confidence change rate

was not significantly different from the packed condition for any of the experimental

conditions. This result for the unpacked condition does not support Hypothesis 1.4b.

To test Hypothesis 1.4a, which compares the slopes for inside and outside view

conditions, and to explore whether there was a difference between the outside view and

unpacked conditions, an analysis was conducted with the outside condition as the

reference. The slopes of weekly confidence change rate (shown in Table 5.8b) were not

significantly different from the outside view condition for any of the experimental

conditions. Therefore, Hypothesis 1.4a is not supported. Results with the inside view

condition as the reference is presented in Appendix E-4 on page 343-344.

98
Table 5.8a
Confidence in predictions of project outcome: Three-level model of the effects of manipulations relative to the packed (control) condition

Confidence in predictions of project outcome


Fixed effect Coefficient S.Error t-ratio p-value

Initial status: For INTERCEPT1, P0


INTERCEPT2, B00
INTERCEPT3, (Average initial status),G000 75.580 1.307 57.809 0.000***
For Inside (UP1), B01
INTERCEPT3, G010 1.873 2.220 0.844 0.400
For Outside (UP2), B02
INTERCEPT3, G020 0.836 2.151 0.389 0.697
For Unpacked (UP3)
INTERCEPT3, G030 1.509 2.088 0.723 0.470
Rate of change: For Week slope, P1
INTERCEPT2, B10
INTERCEPT3, (Average weekly prediction bias rate), G100 0.301 0.121 2.484 0.014*
For Inside (UP1), B11
INTERCEPT3, G110 0.106 0.208 0.509 0.611
For Outside (UP2), B12
INTERCEPT3, G120 -0.074 0.201 -0.368 0.713
For Unpacked (UP3), B13
INTERCEPT3, G130 0.013 0.241 0.056 0.956

Random effect Variance Component df Chi-square p-value

Level-1 Effect (Week), E 61.640


Level-2 (student within groups)
INTERCEPT1, (Individual initial status), R0 194.804 345 3334.196 0.000***
WEEK slope, (Individual weekly prediction bias rate), R1 1.334 345 809.824 0.000***
Level-3 (between groups)
INTERCEPT1 / INTERCEPT2,(Group mean status),U00 30.967 155 220.288 0.001**
WEEK / INTERCEPT2, (Group mean prediction bias rate), U10 0.332 155 222.706 0.000***

99
Table 5.8a (Continued)

Random effect Variance Component df Chi-square p-value

Level-1 Coefficient Percentage of Variance Between Groups


Initial status, P0 13.72
Prediction bias rate, P1 19.93

Deviance = 33759.444
Number of estimated parameters = 15
A three-level model with week = level-1, individual participants = level -2, and group = level - 3. Number of participants = 689 and groups = 156.
The dependent variable is measured from the 0% end of the line and centimeters were converted to a number out of 100%.

p<0.10,*p <.05, **p<.01 ***p<.001

100
Table 5.8b
Confidence in predictions of project outcome: Three-level model of the effects of manipulations relative to the outside view condition

Confidence in predictions of project outcome


Fixed effect Coefficient S.Error t-ratio p-value

Initial status: For INTERCEPT1, P0


INTERCEPT2, B00
INTERCEPT3, (Average initial status),G000 76.416 1.708 44.734 0.000***
For Inside (IO1), B01
INTERCEPT3, G010 1.036 2.477 0.418 0.675
For Unpacked (IO2), B02
INTERCEPT3, G020 0.673 2.359 0.285 0.776
For Packed (IO3)
INTERCEPT3, G030 -0.836 2.151 -0.389 0.697
Rate of change: For Week slope, P1
INTERCEPT2, B10
INTERCEPT3, (Average weekly prediction bias rate), G100 0.227 0.161 1.412 0.160
For Inside (IO1), B11
INTERCEPT3, G110 0.180 0.233 0.772 0.441
For Unpacked (IO2), B12
INTERCEPT3, G120 0.088 0.263 0.333 0.739
For Packed (IO3), B13
INTERCEPT3, G130 0.074 0.201 0.368 0.713

Random effect Variance Component df Chi-square p-value

Level-1 Effect (Week), E 61.640


Level-2 (student within groups)
INTERCEPT1, (Individual initial status), R0 194.804 345 3334.196 0.000***
WEEK slope, (Individual weekly prediction bias rate), R1 1.334 345 809.824 0.000***
Level-3 (between groups)
INTERCEPT1 / INTERCEPT2,(Group mean status),U00 30.967 155 220.288 0.001**
WEEK / INTERCEPT2, (Group mean prediction bias rate), U10 0.332 155 222.706 0.000***

101
Table 5.8b (Continued)

Random effect Variance Component df Chi-square p-value

Level-1 Coefficient Percentage of Variance Between Groups


Initial status, P0 13.72
Prediction bias rate, P1 19.93

Deviance = 33759.444
Number of estimated parameters = 15
A three-level model with week = level-1, individual participants = level -2, and group = level - 3. Number of participants = 689 and groups = 156.
The dependent variable is measured from the 0% end of the line and centimeters were converted to a number out of 100%.

p<0.10,*p <.05, **p<.01 ***p<.001

102
5.3.5 Predictions of Affective Response towards Outcome Success

Figure 5.7 plots mean affective response for predicted and experienced success,

for the 422 participants who reported that they viewed their project outcome as a

success. Inspection of Figure 5.7 suggests that on the first day of the experiment,

participants in each of the four conditions predicted more positive feelings after success

than they actually experienced after receiving their grades.

A mixed between-within subjects analysis of variance was conducted to assess

the effect of the four conditions [inside view, outside view, unpacked and packed

(control)] on participant’s predictions of affective reactions towards outcome success,

across two time periods (on day 1 of the experiment and on the day grades were

received). There was a significant main effect for time, F (1,418) = 359.27, p < .001,

partial eta squared = .46, showing a reduction in positive feelings towards outcome

success across the two time periods. The main effect comparing the four conditions was

significant, F (3,418) = 5.04, p < .01, partial eta squared = .04, suggesting there is a

difference in the effect of the four conditions on the prediction of affective reactions

toward outcome success. The greatest difference is between the outside view condition

and the unpacked condition, with a more extreme affective prediction and response for

the outside condition. There was no significant interaction between the four conditions

and time. These results are further tested with a multilevel analysis.

103
Very good
11.00

10.00

9.00

8.00
Affective response

7.00 Inside
Outside
6.00
Unpacked
5.00 Control

4.00

3.00

2.00

1.00
Not good day 1 after receiving grades
Time

Figure 5.7. Mean rating of predicted and experienced affective response for
success. (n=422)

Figure 5.8 plots mean prediction bias for the affective response towards project

outcome success made by individuals over the entire eight weeks of the project.

Affective prediction bias was measured by subtracting predicted feelings from actually

experienced feelings, so the negative numbers on the vertical axis of Figure 5.8 indicate

that participants consistently overestimated their positive feelings to success as shown

in Figure 5.7. Inspection of Figure 5.8 suggests that the bias is less for the outside view

than for the inside view, which would be consistent with Hypothesis 1.5a, and that the

bias is less for the unpacked condition than for the packed (control) condition, which

would be consistent with Hypothesis 1.5b. Also, the outside view condition appears less

biased than the unpacked condition. These effects were tested statistically with

multilevel models.

104
0.00
day 1 week week week week week week week week
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8

Mean predictions bias

-1.00
Inside
Outside
Unpacked
Control
-2.00

-3.00
Week

Figure 5.8. Mean prediction bias of individuals’ affective response towards


outcome success for the four conditions. Actual response minus predicted
response. (n=422)

5.3.5.1 Unconditional Three-level Model

An unconditional multilevel model was estimated for predictions of affective

response towards outcome success, with time (WEEK) as the only predictor in the

regression equations. The results for this unconditional HLM model are presented in

Table 5.9. The estimated initial status (G000) for predictions of affective response

towards outcome success was -1.793 and was significant; supporting the ANOVA result

that bias occurred. The average weekly prediction bias rate (G100) was estimated at

0.023 and was significant, indicating that there was a small overall reduction in bias.

The variance among participants within groups for initial status and weekly prediction

bias rates (i.e., R0 and R1) and the variance components between groups for mean

initial status (i.e., U00) were statistically significant. But the variance between groups

for mean weekly prediction bias rate (i.e., U10) was marginally significant.

105
Table 5.9
Predictions bias for affective response towards project outcome success: A three-level fully unconditional model

Predictions bias for affective reactions towards project outcome success


Fixed Effect Coefficient S.Error t-ratio p-value

For INTERCEPT1, P0
INTERCEPT2, B00
INTERCEPT3, (Average initial status),G000 -1.793 0.144 -12.469 0.000***
For Week slope, P1
INTERCEPT2, B10
INTERCEPT3, (Average weekly prediction bias rate), G100 0.023 0.007 3.238 0.002**

Random effect Variance Component df Chi-square p-value

Level-1 Effect (Week), E 0.292


Level-2
INTERCEPT1, (Individual initial status), R0 2.698 189 4876.788 0.000***
WEEK slope, (Individual weekly prediction bias rate), R1 0.007 189 482.858 0.000***
INTERCEPT1/INTERCEPT2,(Group mean status),U00 1.219 115 250.059 0.000***
WEEK / INTERCEPT2, (Group mean prediction bias rate),U10 0.001 115 139.208 0.062†

Level-1 Coefficient Percentage of Variance Between Groups


Initial status, P0 31.13
Prediction bias rate, P1 14.10

Deviance = 6148.771
Number of estimated parameters = 9
A three-level model with week = level-1, individual participants = level -2, and group = level - 3. Number of participants = 422 and groups = 116.
The dependent variable is measured as the difference between participants’ predicted and actual feelings towards assignment marks.

p<0.10,*p <.05, **p<.01 ***p<.001

106
As for the percentage that lies between groups for both initial status and

prediction bias rate, the results show that 31.13% of the variance in the initial status for

predictions of affective response towards outcome success lies between groups. 14.10%

of the variance for prediction bias rate is between groups. In short, the unconditional

model for predictions of affective response towards outcome success revealed

significant variance of participants for initial status and weekly prediction bias rates and

between groups for mean initial status. The results for the conditional HLM model are

reported in the following section.

5.3.5.2 Conditional Three-level Model

To test Hypotheses 1.5a and 1.5b, the intercepts of the multilevel model were

examined to compare the effects of the outside and unpacking approaches against the

inside and packed (control) approaches on predictions of affective response towards

outcome success. The three-level hierarchical regression equation for the affective

prediction towards outcome success was estimated with time (WEEK) the only

predictor at level 1, and the three conditions (with packed as the reference condition) as

predictors at level 2 (at P0). The equation at level 3 had no predictors. All participant

control variables such as age, gender, semester and race were tested initially including

the group size and teacher, but the results showed no significant effects for those

variables. Thus, they were excluded from the model. The final full models that describe

the effects of the three conditions [with packed (control), outside and inside as the

reference condition] on affective reaction prediction bias are presented in Appendix D.

107
Table 5.10a summarizes the results of the three-level hierarchical regressions

with the packed (control) condition as the reference. The intercepts for initial status in

Table 5.10a show that the outside view was significantly less biased than the packed

(control) condition in affective predictions towards outcome success at the start of the

project. The unpacked and inside view condition, however, did not differ significantly

from the packed (control) condition. This result for the unpacked condition does not

support Hypothesis 1.5b.

In an analysis with the outside view condition as the reference (Table 5.10b),

initial status was significantly different from the outside view condition for the inside

view, and thus, the results provide some support for Hypothesis 1.5a. The unpacked

condition was marginally significantly different from the outside view condition.

Results with the inside view condition as the reference are presented in Appendix E-5

on page 345-346.

108
Table 5.10a
Prediction bias for affective response towards project outcome success: Three-level model of the effects of manipulations relative to the
packed (control) condition

Predictions bias for affective reactions towards project outcome success


Fixed effect Coefficient S.Error t-ratio p-value

Initial status: For INTERCEPT1, P0


INTERCEPT2, B00
INTERCEPT3, (Average initial status),G000 -2.168 0.258 -8.395 0.000***
For Inside (UP1), B01
INTERCEPT3, G010 0.282 0.382 0.738 0.461
For Outside (UP2), B02
INTERCEPT3, G020 1.053 0.349 3.013 0.003**
For Unpacked (UP3), B03
INTERCEPT3, G030 0.292 0.417 0.701 0.484
Rate of change: For Week slope, P1
INTERCEPT2, B10
INTERCEPT3, G100 0.023 0.007 3.241 0.002**

Random effect Variance Component df Chi-square p-value

Level-1 Effect (Week), E 0.292


Level-2 (student within groups)
INTERCEPT1, (Individual initial status), R0 2.688 186 4876.781 0.000***
WEEK slope, (Individual weekly prediction bias rate), R1 0.007 189 482.858 0.000***
Level-3 (between groups)
INTERCEPT1 / INTERCEPT2,(Group mean status), U00 1.104 115 236.716 0.000***
WEEK / INTERCEPT2, (Group mean prediction bias rate), U10 0.001 115 139.139 0.062†

109
Table 5.10a (Continued)

Level-1 Coefficient Percentage of Variance Between Groups


Initial status, P0 29.11
Prediction bias rate, P1 13.98

Deviance = 6141.417
Number of estimated parameters = 12

A Three-level model with time = level-1, individual participants = level-2 and group = level-3. Number of participants = 422 and groups = 116
The dependent variable is measured as the difference between participants’ predicted and actual feelings towards assignment marks.

p<0.10,*p <.05, **p<.01, ***p<.001

110
Table 5.10b
Prediction bias for affective response towards project outcome success: Three-level model of the effects of manipulations relative to the
outside view condition

Predictions bias for affective reactions towards project outcome success


Fixed effect Coefficient S.Error t-ratio p-value

Initial status: For INTERCEPT1, P0


INTERCEPT2, B00
INTERCEPT3, (Average initial status),G000 -1.115 0.237 -4.702 0.000***
For Inside (IO1), B01
INTERCEPT3, G010 -0.771 0.368 -2.097 0.037*
For Unpacked (IO2), B02
INTERCEPT3, G020 -0.760 0.404 -1.880 0.061†
For Packed (IO3), B03
INTERCEPT3, G030 -1.053 0.349 -3.013 0.003**
Rate of change: For Week slope, P1
INTERCEPT2, B10
INTERCEPT3, G100 0.023 0.007 3.241 0.002**

Random effect Variance Component df Chi-square p-value

Level-1 Effect (Week), E 0.292


Level-2 (student within groups)
INTERCEPT1, (Individual initial status), R0 2.688 186 4876.781 0.000***
WEEK slope, (Individual weekly prediction bias rate), R1 0.007 189 482.858 0.000***
Level-3 (between groups)
INTERCEPT1 / INTERCEPT2,(Group mean status), U00 1.104 115 236.716 0.000***
WEEK / INTERCEPT2, (Group mean prediction bias rate), U10 0.001 115 139.139 0.062†

111
Table 5.10b (Continued)

Level-1 Coefficient Percentage of Variance Between Groups


Initial status, P0 29.11
Prediction bias rate, P1 13.98

Deviance = 6141.417
Number of estimated parameters = 12

A Three-level model with time = level-1, individual participant = level-2 and group = level-3. Number of participants = 422 and groups = 116
The dependent variable is measured as the difference between participants’ predicted and actual feelings towards assignment marks.

p<0.10,*p <.05, **p<.01, ***p<.001

112
5.3.6 Predictions of Affective Response towards Outcome Failure

Mean predicted and experienced affective responses are plotted in Figure 5.9 for

the 130 participants who reported that they viewed their project outcome as a failure.

Inspection of Figure 5.9 suggests that on the first day of the experiment, participants in

each of the four conditions predicted that they would experience more negative feelings

after failure than were actually experienced after receiving grades.

A mixed between-within subjects analysis of variance was conducted to assess

the effect of the four conditions [inside view, outside view, unpacked and packed

(control)] on participant’s affective predictions towards outcome failure, across two

time periods (on day 1 of the experiment and on the day grades were received). There

was a substantial main effect for time, F (1,126) = 37.94, p < .0005, partial eta squared

= .23. The main effect comparing the four conditions was not significant, suggesting no

difference in the effect of the four conditions on the overall level of affective reactions

toward outcome failure. There was no significant interaction between the four

conditions and time.

113
Not bad
11.00

10.00

9.00

8.00
Affective response

7.00 Inside
Outside
6.00
Unpacked
5.00 Packed

4.00

3.00

2.00

1.00
Very bad day 1 after receiving grades
Time

Figure 5.9. Mean rating of predicted and experienced affective response for
failure. (n=130)

Figure 5.10 plots mean prediction bias for affective response towards project

outcome failure made by individuals over the entire eight weeks of the project.

Affective prediction bias was measured by subtracting predicted feelings of failure from

reported actually experienced feelings, so the positive numbers on the vertical axis of

Figure 5.10 indicate that, on average, participants overestimated the extent of their

negative feelings to failure. The graph also suggests that participants in the outside view

condition are less biased than those in the inside view condition, which is consistent

with Hypothesis 1.6a. But participants in the unpacked condition do not seem to be

more or less biased in their predictions of affective response towards outcome failure

than those in the packed (control) condition, which is inconsistent with Hypothesis 1.6b.

Participants in the outside view condition appear to be less biased than participants in

the other three conditions.

114
3.00

Mean predictions bias


2.00
Inside
Outside
Unpacked
Control
1.00

0.00
day 1 week week week week week week week week
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
Week

Figure 5.10. Mean prediction bias of individuals’ affective response towards


project outcome failure for the four conditions. Actual response minus predicted
response. (n=130)

5.3.6.1 Unconditional Three-level Model

An unconditional multilevel model was estimated for predictions of affective

response towards outcome failure, with time (WEEK) as the only predictor in the

regression equations. The results for this unconditional HLM model are presented in

Tables 5.11. The estimated initial status (G000) for predictions of affective response

towards outcome success was 1.971 and was significant. The average weekly prediction

bias rate (G100) was estimated at 0.055 and was also significant, suggesting a slight

increase in bias.

The variance among participants within groups for initial status and weekly

prediction bias rates (i.e., R0 and R1) and the variance components between groups for

mean initial status and mean weekly prediction bias rates (i.e., U00 and U10) were

statistically significant.

115
Table 5.11
Prediction bias for affective response towards project outcome failure: A three-level fully unconditional model

Predictions bias for affective reactions towards project outcome failure


Fixed Effect Coefficient S.Error t-ratio p-value

For INTERCEPT1, P0
INTERCEPT2, B00
INTERCEPT3, (Average initial status),G000 1.971 0.288 6.840 0.000***
For Week slope, P1
INTERCEPT2, B10
INTERCEPT3, (Average weekly prediction bias rate), G100 0.055 0.013 4.129 0.000***

Random effect Variance Component df Chi-square p-value

Level-1 Effect (Week), E 0.455


Level-2
INTERCEPT1, (Individual initial status), R0 4.385 66 1699.213 0.000***
WEEK slope, (Individual weekly prediction bias rate), R1 0.006 66 112.300 0.001**
INTERCEPT1/INTERCEPT2,(Group mean status),U00 1.709 43 84.826 0.000***
WEEK / INTERCEPT2, (Group mean prediction bias rate),U10 0.002 43 65.508 0.015*

Level-1 Coefficient Percentage of Variance Between Groups


Initial status, P0 28.05
Prediction bias rate, P1 31.09

Deviance = 2612.881
Number of estimated parameters = 9
A three-level model with week = level-1, individual participants = level -2, and group = level - 3. Number of participants = 130 and groups = 44.
The dependent variable is measured as the difference between participants’ predicted and actual feelings towards assignment marks.

p<0.10,*p <.05, **p<.01 ***p<.001

116
As for the percentage that lies between groups for both initial status and

prediction bias rate, the results show that 28.05% of the variance in the initial status for

predictions of affective response towards outcome failure lies between groups. The

results for prediction bias rate show that 31.09% of the variance is between groups. In

summary, the unconditional model for predictions of affective response towards

outcome failure revealed significant variance of participants for initial status and weekly

prediction bias rates and between groups for mean initial status. The results for the

conditional HLM model are presented next.

5.3.6.2 Conditional Three-level Model

Hypotheses 1.6a and 1.6b were tested by examining the intercepts of the

multilevel model to compare the effects of the outside and unpacking approaches

against the inside and packed (control) approaches respectively on predictions of

affective response towards outcome failure. Similar three-level hierarchical regression

equations as in estimating the predictions of affective response towards outcome

success were used, as adding the control variables into the model gave no significant

effects for those variables.

Results of the three-level hierarchical regression with packed (control) condition

as the reference are summarized in Table 5.12a. Initial status was not significantly

different from the packed condition for any of the experimental conditions. Therefore,

the result for the unpacked condition does not support Hypothesis 1.6b.

Similar results were obtained in an analysis with the outside view condition as

the reference (Table 5.12b). The initial status was not significantly different from the

outside view condition for any of the experimental conditions. Thus, Hypothesis 1.6a is

not supported. Results with the inside view condition as the reference are presented in

Appendix E-6 on page 347-348.

117
Table 5.12a
Prediction bias for affective response towards project outcome failure: Three-level model of the effects of manipulations relative to the packed
(control) condition

Predictions bias for affective reactions towards project outcome failure


Fixed effect Coefficient S.Error t-ratio p-value

Initial status: For INTERCEPT1, P0


INTERCEPT2, B00
INTERCEPT3, (Average initial status),G000 1.993 0.429 4.460 0.000***
For Inside (UP1), B01
INTERCEPT3, G010 -0.137 0.742 -0.185 0.854
For Outside (UP2), B02
INTERCEPT3, G020 -0.475 0.650 -0.731 0.467
For Unpacked (UP3), B03
INTERCEPT3, G030 0.424 0.526 0.806 0.422
Rate of change: For Week slope, P1
INTERCEPT2, B10
INTERCEPT3, G100 0.055 0.013 4.119 0.000***

Random effect Variance Component df Chi-square p-value

Level-1 Effect (Week), E 0.455


Level-2 (student within groups)
INTERCEPT1, (Individual initial status), R0 4.392 63 1699.207 0.000***
WEEK slope, (Individual weekly prediction bias rate), R1 0.006 66 112.300 0.001**
Level-3 (between groups)
INTERCEPT1 / INTERCEPT2,(Group mean status), U00 1.522 43 80.664 0.001**
WEEK / INTERCEPT2, (Group mean prediction bias rate), U10 0.002 43 65.863 0.014*

118
Table 5.12a (Continued)

Level-1 Coefficient Percentage of Variance Between Groups


Initial status, P0 25.73
Prediction bias rate, P1 31.74

Deviance = 2610.588
Number of estimated parameters = 12

A Three-level model with time = level-1, individual participants = level-2 and group = level-3. Number of participants = 130 and groups = 44
The dependent variable is measured as the difference between participants’ predicted and actual feelings towards assignment marks.

p<0.10,*p <.05, **p<.01, ***p<.001

119
Table 5.12b
Prediction bias for affective response towards project outcome failure: Three-level model of the effects of manipulations relative to the outside
view condition

Prediction bias for affective reactions towards project outcome failure


Fixed effect Coefficient S.Error t-ratio p-value

Initial status: For INTERCEPT1, P0


INTERCEPT2, B00
INTERCEPT3, (Average initial status),G000 1.518 0.532 2.850 0.007**
For Inside (IO1), B01
INTERCEPT3, G010 0.338 0.796 0.425 0.671
For Unpacked (IO2), B02
INTERCEPT3, G020 0.899 0.599 1.502 0.136
For Packed (IO3), B03
INTERCEPT3, G030 0.475 0.650 0.731 0.467
Rate of change: For Week slope, P1
INTERCEPT2, B10
INTERCEPT3, G100 0.055 0.013 4.119 0.000***

Random effect Variance Component df Chi-square p-value

Level-1 Effect (Week), E 0.455


Level-2 (student within groups)
INTERCEPT1, (Individual initial status), R0 4.392 63 1699.207 0.000***
WEEK slope, (Individual weekly prediction bias rate), R1 0.006 66 112.300 0.001***
Level-3 (between groups)
INTERCEPT1 / INTERCEPT2,(Group mean status), U00 1.522 43 80.664 0.001**
WEEK / INTERCEPT2, (Group mean prediction bias rate), U10 0.002 43 65.863 0.014*

120
Table 5.12b (Continued)

Level-1 Coefficient Percentage of Variance Between Groups


Initial status, P0 25.73
Prediction bias rate, P1 31.74

Deviance = 2610.588
Number of estimated parameters = 12

A Three-level model with time = level-1, individual participant = level-2 and group = level-3. Number of participants = 130 and groups = 44
The dependent variable is measured as the difference between participants’ predicted and actual feelings towards assignment marks.

p<0.10,*p <.05, **p<.01, ***p<.001

121
5.4 Conclusion

This chapter has presented a series of multilevel analyses to test the hypotheses

for Study 1, presented in Chapter 3. The results have also been described through

graphical analyses, where means of responses over weeks of the experiments are

plotted. The results of multilevel analyses show several significant effects of the outside

view and unpacking approaches on the predictions made regarding task completion time

and outcome, and on affective predictions towards outcome success.

There were no hypotheses on the difference between the outside view and the

unpacked conditions, but the exploratory analyses demonstrated some interesting

outcomes. There were similar results for the outside view and unpacked approach on all

except on one dependent measure. There was a marginally significant difference

between the outside view and the unpacked condition on predictions of affective

response towards outcome success.

The results from the graphical analyses suggest that participants in the outside

view and in the unpacking approach were less biased in their prediction of completion

time, but more biased in their prediction of outcome success than participants in the

inside view and in the packed (control) conditions. There was little change in

confidence in prediction of completion time, but there was a more substantial increase

in confidence in predictions of outcome over time for each of the four conditions. The

graphs indicate that participants in each of the four conditions predicted more positive

feelings after success than actually experienced after receiving grades, and the bias was

less for the outside view than for the inside view and was less for the unpacked

condition than for the packed (control) condition. Participants in each of the four

conditions also predicted they would experience more negative feelings after failure

than were actually experienced after receiving grades. There were no significant

122
differences between the conditions for impact bias for failure. These research findings

are discussed in the next chapter (Chapter 6).

123
CHAPTER 6

DISCUSSION – STUDY 1

6.1 Introduction

The aim of Study 1 was to test two theories of the planning fallacy and to extend

the theoretical explanations of the planning fallacy to domains other than time to

completion of a project, specifically to prediction of project outcome, to confidence in

prediction of project completion time and outcome, and to predictions of affective

reactions to project outcome success and failure. The discussion that follows is

organized around the hypotheses presented in Chapter 3.

6.2 Completion Time Predictions

Study 1 replicated the planning fallacy effect on task completion time prediction

for both the outside view and the unpacking approaches. As implied by the two theories

of the planning fallacy, participants who carry out behavior involved either in the

outside view prescription or the unpacking prescription make more accurate task

completion predictions than those who do not, that is, those in the control condition.

The inside/outside views approach claims that it is necessary to consider distributional

information about other projects (the outside view), as this gives information about the

likely duration of a project, and the likelihood of intervening events that could affect

project completion. According to that approach the inside view, focusing on the project

at hand, leads to neglect of relevant distributional information about obstacles to

completion. In contrast, the unpacking or enumerating approach does just that, it

focuses attention on the project at hand, requiring that all steps to be performed are

enumerated and considered in detail before making a prediction regarding completion

time. Unlike prior inside / outside view and unpacking approach research, these two

124
approaches were tested simultaneously on the same task verifying that these very

different prescriptions can both reduce the planning fallacy.

The outside view condition was significantly less biased than the control

condition, providing evidence that an outside view manipulation that only involves

recall is sufficient to reduce the planning fallacy. For the comparison of the inside and

outside views, although examination of Figure 5.1 suggests participants in the outside

view condition were less biased than participants in the inside view condition, this was

not confirmed by the statistical test of the intercepts. Possibly, this difference might

have reached significance if the instructions for the inside and outside views had been

more different. They were designed to be as similar as possible with only the minimal

word differences necessary to provide the manipulation.

The outside manipulation focused attention on past experiences. This

manipulation relies on recall. Participants were asked to recall the kind of events that

have happened in the past, which includes all the problems that have happened that have

impeded their project progress and performance. They were also asked to remember any

situation that has facilitated their project progress and performance. Participants in the

inside manipulation were also asked the same, but focused on the future. They were

asked to anticipate any problems that will impede their project progress and

performance and any situation that will facilitate their project progress and performance.

Buehler, Griffin and Ross (1994) suggest that asking participants to recall past

experiences before making predictions may not be enough to reduce optimistic

prediction unless they are also asked to describe how their past experiences could affect

their current predictions, which amounts to actively moderating their predictions.

Participants in the outside and inside conditions were not asked to do this.

More accurate predictions about project completion were also obtained when

participants were asked to unpack their task components as compared to those who do

125
not unpack the task. Prompting people to unpack a task into its subcomponents actually

changes the manner in which the task is represented. By describing tasks in more detail

through listing down components needed for a future project, and listing all the steps

that they planned to carry out before making the prediction, individuals get an

indication that the task would take a longer time, and thus, increase their time estimates.

In earlier planning fallacy research the importance of unpacking a task into its

subcomponents has been emphasized for its ability to enhance prediction accuracy of

completion time (Kruger & Evans, 2004). The current results provide support for the

unpacking effect on prediction accuracy.

Past research in separate studies has shown that manipulations of both the

outside view and enumerating (unpacked versus packed) reduce the planning fallacy.

Therefore, I was not able to predict which of these methods would produce a greater

effect. The findings that emerge from this study suggest that there was no difference

between them. Both approaches have similar effects and reduce the planning fallacy for

completion time prediction accuracy. It also indicates that for completion time

predictions in project planning applications, a detailed focus on the project at hand that

requires a full enumeration of the component tasks may be as accurate as taking an

outside approach to completion time prediction.

Results from this study also demonstrate that the inside view, which also focuses

on the project at hand, was not significantly different to the unpacked condition for

completion time predictions. This result leads to a question of whether the difference in

wording of instructions would have any implication on the planning fallacy. In this

study, participants in the inside view were asked to think about the kind of events that

would be likely to happen in the future, such as problems that will impede their project

progress and performance and to think about any aspect of the project or situations in

the future that will facilitate their project progress. On the other hand, participants in the

126
unpacked condition were asked to list down components that are needed for project

progress and performance and to list down each and every thing that they plan to do in

completing the project. While the current findings did not reach significance for the

affect of this slight difference of instructions on the planning fallacy, Figure 5.1

suggests there may be an effect, but no hypothesis was framed to compare the

unpacking approach and the inside view. If the slight difference in wording of

instructions contributes to the effect of increasing or decreasing the planning fallacy, it

may also have an implication for interpreting the mixed results of past research, and for

prescriptions for reducing the planning fallacy in practice. In the past, Kruger and Evans

(2004) have attempted to find the similarity between the inside view and the unpacking

approach by highlighting that if the inside view focuses on the “non-focal

subcomponents of the task, they would expect an increase in time estimates, and a

decreased planning fallacy” (p.596).

6.3 Outcome Predictions

It was hypothesized that the two approaches that have been found to reduce the

planning fallacy for completion time predictions could be extended to improve other

types of project related predictions, such as predicting the success of the project

outcome. The manipulations of both the outside view and the unpacking approach,

however, gave opposite effects to what was expected based on their effects on the

planning fallacy. I hypothesized that the two approaches should improve outcome

prediction accuracy, just as they do for completion time, because they contain more

information that can help individuals to make a more accurate outcome prediction. But,

the present study shows that inducing people to consider the outside view or

alternatively, unpacking the project task, led to more optimistic outcome predictions

compared to the control condition, increasing rather than reducing the optimistic bias

for outcome success. This is the first time this effect has been demonstrated. It is

127
difficult to find an explanation for this optimistically biased outcome prediction when

the outside view was adopted. Support theory can explain this phenomenon with respect

to unpacking. While enumerating all the component tasks expands the apparent size of

the project, and therefore increases predictions of how long it will take (reduce the

planning fallacy), enumeration will also increase availability and support occurrence of

the components, and therefore, support for the success of the overall project.

6.4 Confidence in Predictions

Evidence was also obtained on how strongly participants really believed in their

predictions, by assessing their confidence. The present study proposed and tested an

account of changes in confidence based on the two theories of the planning fallacy. It

was hypothesized that participants who adopt the outside view or the unpacking

approach would demonstrate smaller changes in confidence in predictions of project

completion time and outcome than those in the inside view or control conditions. The

results show no support for these two hypotheses. The statistical analyses suggest that

recalling problems of past projects, or alternatively, unpacking tasks does not produce

effects on the change in confidence over time. Participants showed a marginal increase

in confidence across time regarding their prediction of completion time, but showed an

increasing confidence in predictions of outcome success over time.

This study extends past research by showing that the outside view and

unpacking approach produce similar results for confidence in prediction of completion

time. Unfortunately, that result does nothing to distinguish between the competing

theoretical explanations on which these two approaches are based.

6.5 Predictions of Affective Response towards Outcome Success and Failure

For affective reactions I framed hypotheses only for predictions of reactions to

outcome, and did not propose that these would change over time differentially for the

four conditions. I argued that through the distributional information it provides, the

128
outside view should lead to a more realistic expectation for the outcome, and therefore,

a more accurate prediction of affective response to the outcome when it occurs,

compared to the inside view. As the enumerating or unpacking approach also reduces

the planning fallacy, I hypothesized that it also would lead to a more accurate prediction

of affective reaction than the packed (control) condition. The results support the

hypothesis for the outside view for affective predictions towards outcome success but

not for affective predictions towards outcome failure. However, the results failed to

support any of the hypotheses for the unpacking approach. Furthermore, while bias was

reduced for the outside view approach, the absolute level of affective prediction and

response to success was the most extreme in that condition.

Therefore, results from this study show that participants prompted to focus on

relevant past experiences improved affective prediction accuracy towards outcome

success, reducing impact bias for positive events. Prompting participants to focus on

past experiences may help them to consider that positive events can unfold in many

different ways, and thus, moderate the effects of temporal focus on affective prediction

towards outcome success. However, similar effects of recalling past experiences was

not found in affective predictions accuracy towards outcome failure. The findings imply

that encouraging people to consider past experiences may not always result in

improving accuracy of affective predictions, particularly, in predicting negative

reactions towards outcome failure. This may be due to difficulty to extract an

appropriate set of past experiences. This result should be considered cautiously as the

number of participants who viewed their project as a failure was not great.

Prompting participants to unpack project tasks into their subcomponents failed

to have a significant effect on the accuracy of the affective predictions towards outcome

success and failure. The statistical results of intercept suggest that both the unpacking

and pack (control) have similar impact bias. The failure of unpacking to reduce the

129
impact bias is consistent with Van Boven and Epley’s (2003) proposition that “more

detailed descriptions may make it easier to summon evidence that suggests more

extreme evaluation” (p.264). They based their proposition on support theory. In their

study, they found that participants prompted to unpack the category of events results in

more extreme affective predictions.

6.6 Study 1 Limitations

There are limitations in the design of this study that might influence the

interpretations and generalizations of these findings. These issues are discussed next.

The study was aimed at understanding processes involved in planning projects in

organizations, but student projects were used. These were real projects with real

outcomes and consequences, but it could be argued that the predictions made by the

students in an educational context do not have the same kinds of consequences as occur

for projects in business, such as HRIS projects. The ubiquity of the planning fallacy in

practice, however, suggests that the magnitude of consequences does not necessarily

improve accuracy of prediction. Indeed, the planning fallacy has been found to be

exacerbated by incentives (Buehler et al., 1997).

The student group projects were not fully free to vary the completion time

without constraint. This is like real business projects, as many real business projects are

also constraint by the deadline agreed in the contract. In software projects for example,

the process of completing and installing new systems or upgrading the existing system

must be conducted within a specified time to avoid distrupting business operations.

Business projects that fail to deliver as planned may face penalties in terms of financial

implications, and loss of future contracts. Besides, past studies on the planning fallacy

have also used student projects (Buehler & Griffin, 2003; Buehler et al., 1994).

Apart from that, the study used group projects, but the prediction was made

individually. This according to Heath and Gonzalez (1995) is known as “interactive

130
decision making” which is different from the “group decision making.” Interactive

decision making involves making decisions after interactions with others without

involving a consensual group procedure. In this study, participants do interact with their

group members during group meetings, and with their teachers, their seniors and other

friends from other groups in the process of completing their project assignments. I did

not further examine effects of interaction with others as a source of ongoing outside

view or unpacking information on predictions of task completion time and outcomes. A

study conducted by Heath and Gonzalez (1995) has revealed that the information

collected through the interaction process alone helps individuals to make decisions, but

it did not improve decision accuracy in their study.

No manipulation check was included in Study 1. If a manipulation check had

been conducted on day 1 of the experiment, after the manipulation, it may have affected

the behavior in subsequent weeks of the experiment. If a manipulation check was

conducted in week eight of the experiment, the participants’ memories of the

instructions may have faded through the lapse of time, and this would render the

manipulation check irrelevant to the study.

6.7 Conclusion

This study represents the first attempt to compare two theories of the planning

fallacy, the inside / outside view and the unpacking approach in the same study, and to

extend theoretical explanations of the planning fallacy to domains other than predictions

of time to completion of a project, specifically to outcome predictions, to confidence in

predictions and to predictions of affective reactions to project outcomes. In doing so,

these two theories were tested in an experimental study using students’ group project

assignments.

Given the findings and acknowledging the limitations of Study 1, several areas

of potential future research exist. This and the implications of these results for planning

131
fallacy research and practice are discussed in Chapter 11. A further study, which

follows, was conducted to understand processes involved in planning real projects in

organizations. In Study 2, planning approaches that are commonly used during Human

Resource Information System (HRIS) development projects are explored, and their

influence on the accuracy of predictions about the success or failure of the project in

terms of project completion time and outcome are examined. In Chapter 7, the literature

on HRIS is reviewed, with particular attention being paid to the role of planning on the

success of HRIS project implementation.

132
CHAPTER 7

INTRODUCTION TO STUDY 2 AND DEVELOPMENT OF THE

HYPOTHESES

7.1 Introduction

To understand processes involved in planning real projects in organizations, and

since to my knowledge, no study has been conducted on the planning fallacy with

Human Resource Information System (HRIS) projects, the second study was conducted

in an applied field environment involving HRIS development projects in place in

organizations in Malaysia. The aim was to explore which of the planning approaches are

commonly used by planners during HRIS project planning, and to examine how the

approach used during the planning process influences the accuracy of predictions about

the success or failure of the project in terms of project completion time and outcome.

Planners’ predictions regarding their affective response towards their project outcome

success or failure are also tested.

To examine the approaches used in the task of planning an HRIS development

project, it is necessary to understand the nature of human resource information systems,

and to examine whether predictions about the outcomes of those projects are successful,

it is necessary to understand the purposes the human resource information system are

intended to fulfill.

The structure of the remainder of this chapter is as follows. The discussion

begins with the definition and the potential value of HRIS. This is followed with

findings from past studies on HRIS effectiveness and possible reasons for the

unsuccessful outcome of HRIS projects. The discussion continues with HRIS planning

processes and challenges. The chapter ends with the development of the hypotheses for

the second study.

133
7.2 Background of the Second Study

An HRIS is a system for gathering and maintaining the data that describes the

human resources, transforming the data into information and then reporting the

information to users. Some of the HRIS definitions given in the literature are

summarized in Table 7.1. Some authors (Ceriello & Freeman, 1991; Hendrickson,

2003) suggest a broader concept of well functioning HRIS that includes people such as

managers, programmers, analysts, technical support, and users from both within human

resources and outside; policies and procedures that describe how to handle specific data

entry, transaction updates, report generation, system maintenance and related activities;

and data required to manage the HR function, and should not be limited to technical

parts of the system such as computer hardware and software.

Table 7.1
Definitions of human resource information systems (HRIS)

Definitions of Human Resource Information System Authors

“A system used to acquire, store, manipulate, analyze, retrieve and (Tannenbaum, 1990,
distribute pertinent information regarding an organization’s human p.27)
resources”.

“A computer-based technique of collecting, storing, maintaining (Targowski &


data and retrieving information about employees and their jobs”. Deshpande, 2001, p.44)

“The composite of data bases, computer applications, and hardware (Broderick & Boudreau,
and software that are used to collect, record, store, manage, deliver, 1992, p.17)
present, and manipulate data for Human Resources (HR)”.

“A total sum of all systems that store data, classify data and make it (Sadri & Chatterjee,
easily available to the decision maker in so far as the human 2003, p.86)
resource is concerned”.

“Any application of computers that an organization utilizes in order (Romm, Pliskin, &
to manage its workforce”. Weber, 1995, p.63)

“A functional database accessed on site or remotely, designed to (Tansley & Newell,


hold data on employees and to support HR activities such as 2007, p.350)
recruitment, selection, performance management, training and
development”.

134
Beside HRIS, terms like virtual HR(M), web-based HR, e-HR, e-HRM, HR intranet,

computer-based human resource management systems (CHRIS) and HR portals are also

widely used in the literature. Even though to some people e-HR is perceived as

equivalent to HRIS, some argue that there is a fundamental difference between e-HR

and HRIS. Ruel, Bondaruk and Looise (2004) for example, claimed that while the

implementation of HRIS is basically directed towards the HR department itself where

the users are mainly HR staff, the target group of e-HR is people outside of the HR

department such as employees and managers. Ruel, Bondaruk and Looise argue that the

difference between HRIS and e-HR can be identified “as the switch from the

automation of HR services towards technological support of information on HR

services” (p.365).

Strohmeier (2007) believes that a term such as HRIS is rather broad with respect to

technology application, whereas terms such as virtual HR(M), web-based HR, or e-HR

are all used with direct attention to main characteristics of the same phenomenon but

with narrower intentions. Strohmeier argues that e-HRM is more of the “application of

information technology for both networking and supporting at least two individuals or

collective actors in their shared performing of HR activities” (p.20).

The implementation of HRIS has been seen as an effort towards reducing costs

and improving the quality of HR services. Many are confident that by adopting IT into

HR, HR can deliver better services, build a far more accurate picture of their workforce

and produce more accurate information that can enable both executives and HR to make

better decisions, especially decisions that are related to the workforce. It has been

claimed that with IT, HR services can be delivered at a lower cost and in a consistent,

high quality and timely manner. IT makes it possible to increase transactions without

increasing resources, increase timeliness through processing power, increase

performance (e.g., accuracy, precision, completeness), and simplify processes

135
(Lengnick-Hall & Moritz, 2003). With the Internet, organizations can become more

collaborative, connected and responsive to the changing needs of the workforce, and

work arrangements can be modified so that work can be performed from many locations

at any time (Stone, Stone-Romero, & Lukaszewski, 2006).

Though the potential benefits of adopting IT into HR have been emphasized in

the literature, and these are summarized in Table 7.2, the reality shows that many

organizations are not implementing HRIS as effectively or as widely as expected. In

fact, there is evidence showing that many HRIS projects are not as successful as

expected and users are not as happy with the outcome as they expected they would be,

once the system is implemented. HRIS projects have been reported to be unable to

deliver the expected outcome, such as the system failed to provide what the organization

needs, have inaccurate or insufficient data, are difficult to access, have disappointing

end functionalities or are lacking the important functionalities, the logic of the HRM

administration did not match with the logic of the system, the information needed is not

available on time or it is out of date, or there is a lack of interaction with other systems

(Bondaruk & Ruel, 2008; Caplan, 2004; Chapman & Webster, 2003; Kavanagh et al.,

1990; Macy, 2004; Russell, 2006). It has also been claimed that implementation of

HRIS does not help in increasing accountability, making better decisions and better

communications and shifting the HR focus to strategic issues (Pearson, 2001). Past

studies have also shown that the effectiveness of using IT in HR was mixed, and many

were being underutilized. These are discussed next.

136
Table 7.2
Summary of potential values of human resource information systems (HRIS)

Applications Authors

On-line recruitment or e-recruitment


• enable to reach a wider range of qualified applicants,
(Baillie, 1996; Buckley, Minette,
including those in international markets;
Joy, & Michaels, 2004; Bussler &
• reduce recruitment costs; cut down the hiring cycle time; Davis, 2001-2002; Cappelli, 2001;
• attract the interest of highly competent individuals who are Cober, Brown, Levy, Cober, &
not currently searching for jobs that is, passive job-seeker; Keeping, 2003; Galanaki, 2002;
• facilitate the recruitment process for overseas operations; Greengard, 1998; Lin &
• address specific labor market niches; automate candidate Stasinskaya, 2002; Martinsons,
screening and tracking, manage a larger volume of 1997; Nink & Chieke, 2004;
resumes and applicant information; and Shand, 2000; Thaler-Carter,
• streamline the recruitment process 1998b; Zusman & Landis, 2002).

e-selection

• minimize costs and maximize the utilization of their (Kehoe, Dickter, Russell, & Sacco,
human capital 2005)

e-performance management
• simplifies the process of completing appraisal forms
(Cardy & Miller, 2003, 2005;
through the use of “canned” sentences and paragraph;
Stone et al., 2006)
• provide raters with online evaluation of the performance
where feedback from multiple raters can be received in a
timely manner;
• can track and compare unit performance where the data
gathered can be used to identify human resource problems,
highlight exceptional performance, uncover potential
errors and provide feedback to managers on the incidence
of such errors;
• gives a greater span of control without having to observe
employees’ behavior directly in the process of assessing
their performance;
• data related to number of work units completed, number of
key strokes, time spent on the task, and error rates can be
gathered
e-learning / computer-based learning / on-line learning /
distributed learning / web-based training
• provide consistent, worldwide training; reduce delivery
(Berry, 1993; Bussler & Davis,
cycle time;
2001-2002; Kotylar & Saks, 2001;
• increase learner convenience; reduce information overload; Welsh, Wanberg, Brown, &
improve tracking; Simmering, 2003)
• lower expenses; save travel cost and time spent away from
the office;
• able to practice using pre-recorded learning; can involved
with real time chat sessions;
• allows for more flexible management of one’s own growth
and learning;
• eliminate bureaucratic controls and procedures; and
• provide information storage for easily access and sharing
knowledge

137
7.3 Research on HRIS Effectiveness

A study conducted by Gardner, Lepak and Bartol (2003) on 1,969 HR

executives concluded that with more extensive use of IT, HR professionals are able to

provide increased information responsiveness, have more information autonomy and

have more external professional links. Though HR professionals spent less time on

routine tasks, they had to spend more time on IT related activities and on developing IT

related qualifications.

Studies concerning a more specific application such as the effectiveness of

online recruitment or e-recruiting have shown mixed results. While cost and efficiency

are among the advantages reported in using online recruitment, the quality of the

applicant pool has been seen as a potential disadvantage. In Jatuso and Sinar’s (2003)

study, the use of e-recruiting was found able to attract candidates with high levels of

drive, previous achievement and work experience. In terms of cost, results from two

case studies have shown how organizations that used an automated recruiting and

screening system reported substantial cost savings from reducing turnover, staffing cost

and increasing hiring process efficiencies (Buckley et al., 2004). Based on data

collected over a six-year period (December 1999 – June 2006) within the UK, Parry and

Tyson (2008) found not only that online recruitment reduced costs through reducing the

use of papers and reducing agency costs, but also reduced the time taken to hire

employees.

Even though e-recruiting is able to attract more applicants from a larger

geographical area, Chapman and Webster (2003) found that they are not always of

higher quality. McManus and Ferguson (2003) also found a similar outcome, where

candidates recruited through the internet are found to have less favorable background

characteristics than candidates recruited through traditional methods, particularly with

respect to employment stability (candidates are frequent job hoppers). Parry and Tyson

138
(2008) also reported similar findings where organizations that used online recruitment

did receive very large numbers of applications, but a high proportion of them were not

suitable for the position advertised.

In terms of the ability of online recruitment to reach a more diverse population,

studies have shown mixed feelings from the users. Though McManus and Ferguson

(2003) did not find any difference between using online recruitment and traditional

methods, other studies have shown opposite results. Respondents who were surveyed

and interviewed in Galanaki’s (2002) study reported feeling doubtful about the potential

of online recruitment to reach the “passive seeker”. Similar results were also shown in

Parry and Tyson’s (2008) study where users reported feeling doubtful about the ability

of online recruitment to get the kind of candidates that the organization required, to

target the passive job seekers, and to reach a diverse population. Apart from

recruitment, organizations also report mixed success in acquiring and implementing

technologies that support the application, screening and selection process (Chapman &

Webster, 2003). Surprisingly, Chapman and Webster found larger organizations report

having less success than smaller ones.

With regard to training, while potential benefits of e-learning discussed in the

literature are appealing, research on the effectiveness of e-learning shows little

difference between web-based training (WBT) and instructor-led training where scores

in both media show an exceptionally high level of learning (Coppola & Myre, 2002).

Results from interviews conducted with 22 individuals who have been involved with e-

learning efforts in SMEs found that the use of e-learning may not be equally effective

for everyone and for all types of courses (Welsh et al., 2003). Learners, with low

computer skills or with anxiety with the computers, may have difficulties using the

computer as a learning tool. Also, e-learning may be useful for training that emphasizes

cognitive learning outcomes that involve less complex knowledge and intellectual skill,

139
but not suitable for more advanced classes or for those requiring soft skills, and for

classes that teach skills with psychomotor components.

In summary, the mixed results of the effectiveness of using IT in HR processes

may indicate the ability of users to use the system which can be related to the extent and

understanding of functionality and support. Or it may indicate that the shift from using

the manual to a more advanced system is not as simple as hoped, with many

organizations underestimating the challenge of adopting technology solutions in HR

processes. The mixed results may also indicate a failure of the HR function to make

advanced used of the technology.

Based on interviews with HR groups in ten Fortune 500 companies, Broderick

and Boudreau (1992) found that the main focus of HRIS was on managing basic human

resource functions such as record keeping, payroll, compensation and benefits

administration. Other studies have also indicated that most organizations use their HRIS

as no more than an “electronic filing cabinet” for keeping relevant staff information like

age, gender, years of service, classification, qualification and previous work history and

processing routine administrative tasks, rather than facilitating a strategic focus for HR

within the organization (Bassett, Campbell, & Licciardi, 2003; Kinnie & Arthurs,

1996).

Ball (2001) found information in core administration, training and recruitment

was stored for administrative purposes rather than for analysis or decision support. In

the training and development area, HRIS has been used most frequently for monitoring

and administrative purposes such as to store course administration and evaluation

information. The budget control for training was also conducted manually and was kept

away from the HRIS. In the recruitment area, the system was used for tracking

applicants through the recruitment process, interview management and media response

analysis, rather than for more analytical tasks like skills matching.

140
A recent study conducted by Hussain, Wallace and Cornelius (2007) also

showed similar results in terms of HRIS usage. Their report was based on a survey

conducted on HR professionals from 450 organizations in the UK and in-depth semi-

structured interviews conducted with 11 senior organizational executives to whom the

HR professional reported. Slightly less than 50% of the companies they surveyed use

HRIS or other software to support strategic HR tasks, with differences in advanced task

usage or in strategic decision making between small to medium enterprise (SME) and

larger companies.

The problem of underutilized HRIS continues to be a fundamental challenge for

many in HR. Although past studies have made some attempts to consider reasons for

this low usage, there is still a need for an effective approach to increase the use of these

systems. Some of the reasons for unsuccessful HRIS projects, particularly one that

relates to underutilization of the system are discussed next.

7.4 Reasons for Unsuccessful HRIS Projects

Several reasons have been proposed for the low usage of Human Resource

Information Systems (HRIS) in the HR function, and this includes lack of financial

support, lack of top management support, the organizations’ size, human resource

strategy and users’ ability to use the system.

Caplan (2004) and Kovach and Cathcart (1999) for example, believed that lack

of financial support and interest from key people in the organization contribute to the

underutilization problems. According to Huo and Kearns (1992), the introduction of

HRIS always involves a substantial amount of money, and in many cases it is not easy

to justify. If HR tasks have been done well previously in a manual fashion, top

management may not see any good reason for investing in the computerization process,

unless HR people are able to demonstrate tangible return on investment. Apart from

financial support, top management support is related to the adoption of innovative

141
systems (Teo, Lim, & Fedric, 2007; Thong, 1999). Greater support from top

management not only implies getting greater resources for the development of HRIS

applications, but also overcoming user resistance to ensure a quicker pace of change and

more HRIS applications can be adopted.

Financial support also explains why larger organizations have more potential to

use HRIS than smaller organizations. Compared to larger organizations, smaller ones do

not have enough budget or resources to install all the applications needed to assist with

strategic decisions. A survey conducted on 500 companies in Hong Kong found that the

greatest barrier to the adoption of HRIS was insufficient financial support (Ngai & Wat,

2006). In reality, many of these HR systems are partially installed with restrictive

priorities having to be set, and often a simple, off-the-shelf software version is

implemented. Studies have shown that smaller organizations usually go for low cost and

low risk, more flexible software or in-house HRIS development (Ball, 2001; Thaler-

Carter, 1998a). In Hussain, Wallace and Cornelius’s (2007) study, larger companies

were found to have full computerization of strategic HR tasks as compared to small to

medium enterprises (SME).

Larger organizations also have a greater need for computerized HRIS as they

have more complicated tasks of coordination, where information processing

requirements are much higher compared to smaller firms (Thaler-Carter, 1998a). Based

on a postal survey to 470 respondents who were randomly selected from the Financial

Analysis Made Easy (FAME) database, Ball (2001) found that organizational size

determines whether an organization has an HRIS at all, whether it adopts certain

modules over others, and how information is used and analyzed. Different recruitment

practices have also been found between small and large organizations (Hausdorf &

Duncan, 2004). The differences were due to reasons such as larger organizations have

more job openings, and thus, hire larger numbers of candidates; larger organizations

142
need to deal with a greater number of candidates because they are well-established and

known in the marketplace; and larger organizations have more money for recruitment.

A survey conducted on 99 IT companies whose shares are traded in the London Stock

Exchange showed that the size of the organization was the most important factor in

determining the use of online recruitment (Galanaki, 2002). Teo, Lim and Fedric (2007)

also found the size of organizations is the most important discriminator between

adopters and non-adopters of HRIS, and the extent of HRIS adoption.

Broderick and Boudreau (1992) in contrast argue that the nature of HRIS usage

is determined by a firm’s human resources strategy. For example, in order to achieve a

cost leadership strategy, a transaction system is more likely to be developed to best

support more routine, high volume HR decisions with well-defined information needs

and outcomes. An expert system is more likely to be implemented if the strategy is to

achieve quality/ customer satisfaction, where emphasis is more on improving the

existing work methods, products/services and customer relations. Expert systems

possess a developmental focus that is suitable for distribution of knowledge and

experience. Conversely, a decision support style of system is developed if an innovation

strategy is to be achieved. This type of computer application allows users to pull

together information, analyze it and represent it in many forms, and also assists the user

with electronic memory aids and references.

Underutilization has also been attributed to users’ abilities to use HRIS, where

users are said to be unaware of the way an HRIS can be used, or they are lacking the

skill to retrieve information (Macy, 2004).

Even though a number of studies have been conducted to understand the reasons

for the underutilization of HRIS capabilities, one area that has received little attention in

the research of HRIS is the planning aspect of the system. Past studies on HRIS do not

include any attempt at studying decision making during project planning, and the

143
consequent impact on the level or type of usage of HRIS. Specifically, very little effort

has been devoted to examining the problem of a lower usage of the system from the

perspective of the HRIS project itself.

This apparent failure of HRIS projects to deliver systems that meet users’

expectations as planned may explain why the systems often fail to be utilized

strategically. Haines and Petit (1997) note that user satisfaction and system usage are

two common measures for system success. Even though usage may be voluntary, it also

depends on whether the system is perceived to be of value to the end user. They further

argue that one condition that can help explain the levels of user satisfaction and system

usage is in the computerization process itself as many decisions and conditions that

influence the final configuration of the system manifest in this process.

The importance of planning has also been highlighted by the 22 interviewees in

a study examining the successful implementation of e-learning in organizations (Welsh

et al., 2003). They believed that a successful e-learning implementation requires greater

planning effort especially when dealing with the training design issues such as whether

the employees can use the technology, how to ensure good design, and how to ensure

learner motivation; IT infrastructure issues such as whether the users have the

technology necessary to access content and whether the required hardware, software and

technical support are available; and change management issues that include how to

prepare users and training departments for the change and how to gain support from top

management.

In the HR practitioner literature, Ogier (2003) has pointed out that some of the

problems with HRIS implementation arise from a lack of proper planning and

resourcing. Because of the poor planning, managers are found to underestimate the

scope of IT projects, and fail to take into consideration that projects may take longer

than expected and cost more than budgeted.

144
Therefore, project planning may have a significant impact on the quality of the

HRIS that is delivered, and consequently on how well the HRIS is utilized after the

implementation process. In order to further understand the importance of planning in the

development of HRIS, its processes and challenges are discussed next.

7.4 Importance and Challenges of HRIS Project Planning

As in any other project, successful implementation of HRIS requires significant

planning and effort. This is because the overall system requirements and strategies need

to be developed at this stage such as the kind of data, analysis, security, reports, and

other features users need. Also, decisions must be made on whether there is a need for

new applications or just an upgrade to the existing ones by eliminating redundancies in

systems and data. Then the project team must decide whether the planned systems

should be developed internally or acquired from a vendor; whether the system should be

integrated with other systems or be one stand-alone system; whether the users need to

acquire extensive computer experience or none at all; and whether the users have access

to computer support or they have to function in relative isolation (Haines & Petit, 1997).

Apart from determining when the project should start and when it will end,

project teams are also required to estimate the resources needed to achieve their project

objectives such as hardware costs, vendor charges for software and facilities

requirements, consultant time, custom software development, documentation revisions,

and user training (Ceriello & Freeman, 1991). If the project team fails to plan carefully

and gather enough information during this stage, issues related to system design may

not be solved and the project teams may make the wrong kind of decision. These wrong

decisions will then be carried out in the next phase and finally, the wrong kind of

system will be implemented.

Even though planning is crucial for developing a successful HRIS, its process is

becoming increasingly complex as most of the technology associated with HRIS is new.

145
As in other development of information systems projects, HRIS are also becoming more

diversified in terms of size, application domain, and underlying technology. For that

reason, planning for the best technologies that match HR and business’s needs presents

a great challenge. As argued by Bassett (2003), it is not simply finding what the

technology can provide, it is more a matter of ensuring the information facilitated by

HRIS is what the business needs. Also, it is a matter of understanding what specific

aspects of IT support activities are needed for achieving HR and organization strategic

objectives. However, there is limited evidence concerning the kind of technologies

currently available in the HR area. This leaves many planners ignorant of what an HRIS

could or couldn’t do. Thus, many may have expectations of the system that are

unrealistic (Pearson, 2001).

The rapid advancement in HR technology and the increased diversity of HRIS

users and demands undoubtedly has made planning for HRIS projects more complex

and uncertain. Smith (2007) argues that the complexities and uncertainties of the project

leaves planners to make assumptions even though they are in a state of ignorance when

setting out the project plan. But, making accurate prediction is important for the success

of the project. Many of the unsuccessful aspect of IS projects already mentioned, such

as projects that finished late, over budget or failed to meet users’ expectations, indicate

inaccuracy in making predictions. Lovallo and Kahneman (2003) believe that a high

number of project failures are caused by the tendency to make planning decisions based

on delusional optimism rather than on a rational weighting of potential gains, losses and

probabilities. Though project planners know that their past projects have taken longer

than planned, many still believe that their next project will finish as planned. This

phenomenon is known as the planning fallacy, and it was described and explored in the

first study. In this second study, I propose that the planning fallacy is one of the reasons

that may contribute to HRIS project outcomes that are considered relatively

146
unsuccessful by those involved. In the following section, the hypotheses for the second

study are proposed.

7.5 The Planning Fallacy Theories and HRIS Projects: Extending Study 1

The planning fallacy has been introduced and explained in the previous chapters.

According to several researchers, the planning fallacy is the reason for many ill fated

projects.

7.5.1 Project Completion Predictions

Study 1 found that participants who were asked to take the unpacking approach,

and those who were asked to take the outside view approach to planning both made

more accurate predictions regarding project completion time than those who were in the

packed (control) conditions. However, there was no different in terms of accuracy in

prediction made regarding completion time between participants who take the inside

and the outside views. These hypotheses have not before been tested in a field

environment involving HRIS projects. Thus, similar hypotheses will be tested, but the

outside view and the unpacking approach will be tested against the inside view as it is

not realistic to include the control (packed) condition. It is therefore predicted that:

H2.1a: Predictions of task duration generated from an outside perspective are more

accurate than predictions generated from an inside perspective.

H2.1b: Predictions of task duration generated from the unpacking approach are more

accurate than predictions generated from an inside perspective.

7.5.2 Project Outcome Prediction

In Study 1, adopting the outside view and the unpacking approach gave an

opposite effect on the outcome prediction compared to their effect on completion time

prediction. Study 1 showed that inducing people to consider the outside view and the

unpacking approach led to more optimistic outcome prediction rather than reducing the

optimistic bias. However, the hypotheses tested in Study 1 have not been tested in a

147
field environment. IT projects like HRIS are very dynamic in nature, with technology

that evolves rapidly. Whether HRIS planners are optimistic or not in their outcome

prediction is still yet to be discovered. Therefore, similar hypotheses were also proposed

for Study 2.

H2.3a: Predictions of project outcome quality generated from an outside perspective are

more accurate than those generated from an inside perspective.

H2.3b: Predictions of project outcome quality generated from an unpacking approach

are more accurate than those generated from the inside perspective.

In Study 2, the outcome success of this project is tested as satisfaction with the

system implemented. According to DeLone and McLean (1992) and Ives and Olson

(1984), user satisfaction has been considered as one of the most important measures of

information systems success. Generally, systems that meet the needs of the user will

reinforce satisfaction of the system. Using technical quality to measure outcome success

would not be considered successful if it did not meet the needs of the users. The

hypotheses that I frame for Study 2 are to describe the relationship between the

planning approach used (the inside view, outside view or the unpacking approach) and

users’ satisfaction level. Therefore, another three hypotheses related to outcome success

are proposed to test whether the level of satisfaction with the HRIS project outcome is

related to the planning method adopted by the planners.

Specifically:

H2.4a: Planners who adopt the outside perspective are more satisfied with the HRIS

outcome than planners who adopt the inside perspective.

H2.4b: Planners who adopt the unpacking approach are more satisfied with the HRIS

outcome than planners who adopt the inside perspective.

148
7.5.3 Predictions of Affective Response towards Outcome Success and Failure

Research on affective predictions suggests that people often overestimate the

power and persistence of emotional reactions to events and this is known as impact bias

(Wilson & Gilbert, 2003). Wilson and Gilbert (2005) argued that “exaggerating the

impact of emotional events serves as a motivator, making people work hard to obtain

things that they predict will have large positive consequences and avoid things that they

predict will have large negative consequences” (p.134). If this is true, the impact bias

could be found when planning for a project as it serves a motivating function for

obtaining a successful project outcome and to avoid failures. As discussed earlier in this

chapter, HRIS users are not as happy with the system as they predicted they would be

once the system is implemented.

In Study 1, prompting participants to recall past experiences reduced the impact

bias for positive affect after outcome success but no effect was found when participants

were prompted to detail project task at hand. Both the outside view and unpacking

approach failed to have significant effect on reducing the impact bias for negative affect

after outcome failure. However, it is not known whether learning from past experience

(the outside view) or detailing the project at hand (the unpacking approach) will reduce

the impact bias in the context of HRIS projects in organizations. Therefore, similar

hypotheses were also proposed for Study 2, but in this study the outside view and the

unpacking approach will be tested against the inside view, because a control condition

is not available.

H2.5a: People who use the outside perspective will make more accurate predictions of

positive affective reactions towards outcome success than people who use the inside

perspective.

H2.5b: People who unpack will make more accurate predictions of positive affective

reactions towards outcome success than those who use the inside perspective.

149
H2.6a: People who use the outside perspective will make more accurate predictions of

negative affective reactions towards outcome failure than people who use the inside

perspective.

H2.6b: People who unpack will make more accurate predictions of negative affective

reactions towards outcome failure than those who use the inside perspective.

Changes in confidence tested in Study 1 cannot be tested in Study 2 as it

involves a cross-sectional study.

7.6 Conclusion

This chapter has presented an overview of the second study. Literature on HRIS

has been discussed especially as it relates to potential benefits and usage of the systems

in organizations. The aims of the second study were set out, and a series of hypotheses

were presented regarding the impact of the inside / outside view and unpacking

approach on the accuracy of HRIS project completion time and outcome success

predictions, and predictions of affective response towards outcome success and failure.

The following chapter, Chapter 8, describes the method of the second study.

150
CHAPTER 8

METHOD – STUDY 2

8.1 Introduction

Chapter 8 describes the method for the second study. In this chapter, the sample

design, survey materials used in the study, procedure for collecting data and the

research measures are described. The chapter ends with strategies for analyzing the data.

8.2 Sampling Design

The sampling frame for organizations in this study includes Malaysian HR

professionals who are employed in organizations located in the states of Perak, Kedah,

Perlis and Penang. The selection of firms was based on various sources with Federation

of Malaysian Manufacturers (FMM) Directory 2007 as the main reference. The FMM

Directory was used as many large companies are registered under this directory which is

relevant to the study. Also, a list of companies working cooperatively with Universiti

Utara Malaysia’s student practicum unit was used to select companies that are not

registered under the FMM Directory, but located within the same industrial estates

where the study was conducted. Only HR staff who were directly and actively involved

with the planning process of HRIS projects are relevant to this study.

8.2.1 The Participating Organizations

According to the FMM website, there are over 2000 manufacturing and

industrial services companies of varying sizes registered with FMM (Federation of

Malaysian Manufacturers, 2007). For practical reasons, only companies in the

geographical area of the states of Perak, Kedah, Perlis and Penang were included. These

states were chosen as they are among the states that have the most industrial estates in

Malaysia, and to make it practical for me to make repeated visits to each site to

distribute and collect the survey form on site. In total, there are 485 manufacturing and

151
12 services companies registered with FMM in these four states. From that list

companies were further selected based on their size and location.

Several studies (e.g. Ball, 2001; Galanaki, 2002; Teo et al., 2007; Thaler-Carter,

1998a) have shown that size plays a major role in decisions on whether an HRIS should

be implemented or not in an organization. The number of employees in the organization

also determines the need for computerization of HR functions. The larger the number of

the employees, the more HR functions needs to be automated. Following the definition

of small to medium enterprises (SME) given by the Malaysian National SME

Development Council (NSDC), manufacturing companies with a total number of

employees less than 50 and services companies with a total number of employees less

than 19 are considered small (NSDC, 2005). There were 117 manufacturing and 2

services companies that met this definition, and they were excluded from the list. Apart

from that, the location of the companies was also important for the purpose of

scheduling for appointments and making repeated visits to the companies, as the survey

was conducted on site. Thus, for reasons of difficult access to locations, another 204

manufacturing companies and 7 services companies were excluded from the list.

Companies (manufacturing and services) that are not registered in the FMM

Directory, but located within the industrial estates where the study was conducted, were

also included. Thus, another 98 companies were added to the list. This gives a total

number of 265 manufacturing and services companies, altogether. These companies

were arranged according to the four states, and the process of data collection proceeded

from one state to another, starting from the state of Perlis down to Perak.

Acknowledging the cultural context where the study was conducted, a referral or

networking technique was essential. This technique helps to build trust and rapport with

the potential respondents, and increases the chances of being entertained and getting

permission to conduct a study at the respective companies. Through this technique, 124

152
contact persons were identified at companies among the 265. As no contact could be

obtained at the remaining 141 companies, they were deleted from the list. Within the

period of data collection which began from the middle of September until middle of

December 2007, all the 124 HR managers or their representatives were contacted

personally by telephone.

A small number of firms in these four states, especially in the service industries

like banking, telecommunication, insurance and health indicated that the planning and

decision making processes were made by the HR staff at their Headquarters in Kuala

Lumpur. In this case, appointments were set up with the HR staff in Kuala Lumpur.

Among the industries that participated in this study were insurance, education,

hotel, banking and finance, health, construction, telecommunications, information

technology, electric and electronic products, petroleum, textiles, iron and steel products,

automotive parts and components, rubber products (i.e. gloves, tires), cement and

concrete products, and automobile.

8.2.2 The Participants

Out of 124 HR managers or their representatives from the organizations

contacted, 70 of them agreed to participate in the survey. Those who declined to

participate gave reasons such as being too busy with the year end budgeting, planning

and bonus payments; it was against their company’s policy to entertain any studies; they

did not use any HR computer systems as some of the HR functions such as payroll were

outsourced; they had just joined the company where the system already existed in the

company; the person in-charge of the HRIS planning had moved to another company;

and the company was undergoing major restructuring.

From the total of 70 who agreed to participate in the survey, 52 survey forms

were finally collected. Of these, 15 respondents from 9 organizations were at the

planning stage, and 37 respondents from 36 organizations indicated that their firm had

153
already completed their HRIS project. One of the challenges faced during the data

collection process was to get participation of the whole planning team, especially for the

completed projects. The majority of them had moved to other organizations, and some

were reluctant to participate, and recommended more senior team members to

participate in the survey. As there were insufficient respondents for multilevel analysis,

only one representative from each organization is included in the analyses that are

reported here. The representative was chosen based on their seniority in the

organization. All 45 respondents who participated in this study indicated that they were

one of the key decision makers with HRIS responsibility, and therefore would be in a

position to know about major decisions involving the HRIS project.

8.3 Survey Materials

All the survey materials were prepared in English, as professional-level workers

in Malaysia can and often do work in English. Two sets of questionnaires were

developed, one completed by participants who had already finished their HRIS project

development and the other completed by participants who were at the planning stage of

their HRIS project. Each participant in this survey received an information sheet, and

either an eight or a ten-page questionnaire (with cover letter attached). The survey

materials used in this study are shown in Appendix G.

For participants who had completed their HRIS project, the questionnaire asked

about the background of their project, their involvement in the project, methods used

during the planning process, the outcome of the project, their satisfaction level with the

systems and their overall feelings towards the project outcome. The ten-page

questionnaire consisted of five sections. Section 1 asked about the project background

information such as name and nature of the project, the date the project started and

ended, whether the project finished early, on time or late, and their involvement in the

project. Questions such as name and nature of the project, the date the project started

154
and ended, the number of days if the project finished early or late, and participants’

involvement in the project were open-ended questions.

Section 2 asked about the planning approach used by HR staff during the HRIS

project. There were 9 activity statements and 3 descriptions of approaches to project

planning. In Section 3 of the questionnaire, there were 2 items about the actual HRIS

project outcome, 12 items about end-user computing satisfaction and 1 item on

participants’ overall satisfaction with the project outcome. Two open-ended questions

were also included in this section asking participants to list the aspects of the system

that they were most satisfied and least satisfied with. In Section 4 there was a question

on whether participants view their project as a success or a failure. The final section of

the questionnaire, Section 5, sought the demographic characteristics of the participating

staff, their respective organizations and their HR systems.

The presentation order of the activity statements and the descriptions of planning

approaches were both counterbalanced. Four sets of questionnaires were prepared as

shown in Table 8.1. The first set was the original version. For the second set, the order

of the 9 activity statements (questions 6 to 14) in Section 2 of the questionnaire was

reversed. The order of the three descriptions of approaches in Section 2 remained the

same. The third set was the version with the original order of the 9 activity statements in

Section 2, but the order of the three descriptions of approaches was reversed. The last

set was the version with the order of the 9 activity statements as in the second version,

and the three descriptions of approaches were as in the third set of the questionnaire.

These four versions of the questionnaire were then arranged and distributed in such a

way that the first participant would get the first version of the questionnaire, the second

participant would get the second version, the third participant would get the third

version and the fourth participant would get the fourth version. The same procedure was

repeated with the fifth, sixth and the rest of the participants. As shown in the

155
questionnaire for the original version, in Appendix G on page 351, the names of the

planning approaches were not shown on the questionnaire, the approaches were

indicated by the letter A, B, and C. The names are used in Table 8.1 to refer to the

relevant description of the approaches.

Table 8.1
The four versions of the questionnaires

Version one Version two Version three Version four


(original version)

Order of Section 2 Order of Section 2 Order of Section 2 Order of Section 2


questions questions questions questions

9 activity statements 9 activity statements 9 activity statements 9 activity statements

Q6 Q14 Q6 Q14

Q7 Q13 Q7 Q13

Q8 Q12 Q8 Q12

Q9 Q11 Q9 Q11

Q10 Q10 Q10 Q10

Q11 Q9 Q11 Q9

Q12 Q8 Q12 Q8

Q13 Q7 Q13 Q7

Q14 Q6 Q14 Q6

Description of Description of Description of Description of


approaches approaches approaches approaches

A Inside A Inside A Unpacked A Unpacked

B Outside B Outside B Outside B Outside

C Unpacked C Unpacked C Inside C Inside

The questionnaire for participants who were at the planning stage asked about

the background of their HRIS project, their involvement in the project, methods used

during the planning process, their predicted outcome of the project and their predicted

156
feelings if the project was to succeed or to fail. In Section 1, participants were asked

about the name and nature of the project, the date the project would start and when it

was expected to finish, their prediction about when the project is likely to finish and

their involvement in the project. As for the completed project version, these questions

were open-ended questions.

The questions in Section 2 were similar to those for the completed project

version of the questionnaire. Counterbalancing of the activity statements and

descriptions of approaches was also used to control for any order effects. In Section 3 of

the questionnaire, there were 2 items about the predicted HRIS project outcome.

Questions in Sections 4 and 5 were similar to those in the completed project version

except that in Section 4, there was no question asking participants to indicate their view

on whether the project was a success or a failure. As for the completed project versions,

four sets of questionnaires were prepared and given to participants using a similar

process to that used for the completed project version.

All participants were informed and reminded (verbally and in writing) that their

participation was completely voluntary and that their responses would be treated with

the strictest confidentiality. They were not requested to identify themselves in that they

did not put their names on the survey forms. They could withdraw from the study, by

discontinuing the completion of the questionnaire, or by not returning it, at any time and

without prejudice. The completion and return of the questionnaire was taken as the

indication to the researcher that the participant agreed to participate. Each of the survey

forms had a coded ID to identify respondents only with respect to their different

organizations, to identify whether they were at the planning stage or already completed

the project and to differentiate between the four sets of questionnaires that were used.

157
8.4 Procedure

Potential organizations listed under the Federation of Malaysia Manufacturers

(FMM) Directory 2007 were contacted personally by telephone. Through the initial

telephone conversation, I introduced myself, explained the purpose of the call and

asked for an appointment with an HR manager or the person involved with the planning

of the HRIS, to conduct the survey. Once the respondent agreed to participate in the

study on behalf of the firm, a date was fixed at the respondent’s convenience.

During the survey sessions with respondents, I personally administered and

collected the completed questionnaire. Each respondent was first briefed about the

purpose and the nature of the survey. They were then provided with a sheet of

information about the survey and were invited to ask questions. Respondents were

assured that all the information given will remain confidential at all times and will be

used for the study only. They were not requested to identify themselves in that they do

not put their names on the survey forms. Respondents were allowed ample time to read

the information sheet to ensure that they understood the instructions and to decide if

they really wanted to participate in the survey. Before answering the survey questions,

each respondent was asked to choose a recently completed HRIS project or the one that

they were currently involved in and respond to the survey questions with respect to that

project. Respondents were then given 30 minutes to complete the forms. Each meeting

lasted between 30 and 60 minutes.

More time was given to those who could not complete the forms in 30 minutes

if they wished. Some participants did not have time to complete the questionnaire at

work, or preferred not to complete the questionnaire at work. These participants were

each given a pre-addressed and postage-paid envelope so they could post the

questionnaire back to me in their own time. There were also respondents who preferred

the survey form to be attached through their email and the completed form was returned

158
through email. For respondents who were not able to fill out the questionnaire during

the meeting, a follow-up telephone call reminder was used to remind respondents about

returning the questionnaire.

8.5 Research Measures

The major dependent measures in this study were bias in HRIS planners’

prediction of project completion time and project outcome success, and impact bias in

their predictions of affective response towards project success or failure.

8.5.1 Planning Method Measures

To explore planning methods used by the HRIS planners, two sets of questions

were prepared: the ratings of use of the three planning approaches and the forced choice

questions. To test the generalizability of Study 1 results to a real-world setting, the

wording used to develop the planning method measures in Study 2 was as similar as

possible to Study 1.

The questions regarding use of the three planning approaches involved 9 activity

statements, with 3 questions each to indicate the three approaches (the inside view, the

outside view and the unpacking approach). As shown in Table 8.2, these activity

statements were developed based on the manipulation instructions used in Study 1.

These nine activity statements were then arranged in a way that the first activity

statement represents the inside view, the second activity statement represents the

outside view and the third activity statement represents the unpacking approach, and the

process was repeated with the rest of the activity statements. Based on a seven-point

scale whereby, 1 = strongly disagree, and 7 = strongly agree, participants rated their

degree of agreement that the statement describes an activity they were doing in

accomplishing the project.

159
Table 8.2
The nine activity statements

Manipulation instructions used in Study 1 Nine activity statements used in Study 2

Inside view: Inside view:


Please take a few minutes to consider your plans 1. I think about the kind of events that are
for completing your project assignment. Think likely to happen in this project
about the kind of events that are likely to happen as
2. I think all the problems that are likely to
you work on your project assignment. Think about
happen in the future that will impede this
all the problems that are likely to happen in the
project’s progress and performance
future that will impede your project progress and
performance, and any aspects of the project or 3. I think about any aspects of this project or
situation in the future that will facilitate your situations in the future that will facilitate
project progress and performance. the project’s progress and performance

Outside view: Outside view:


Please take a few minutes to consider your plans 1. I recall the kinds of events that have
for completing your project assignment. Recall the happened in my past projects and those of
kind of events that have happened in your past other people that I know about
project assignment and those of other people that
2. I recall all the problems that have
you know about. Recall all the problems that have
happened in the past that have impeded
happened in the past that have impeded your
other project’s progress and performance
project progress and performance, and any aspect
of the project or situation in the past that have 3. I recall any aspects of project or situations
facilitated your project progress and performance. in the past that have facilitated other
projects’ progress and performance

Unpacking approach: Unpacking approach:


Please take a few minutes to consider your plans 1. I think about the components or elements
for completing your project assignment. Think that need to be included in the project
about the components that are needed for project
2. I breakdown the overall project into
progress and performance. Also, think about each
assignable work elements
and everything that you plan to do in completing
this project assignment. 3. I list each and every step that I am going
to take in order to complete the project.

The forced choice questions involved 3 descriptions of approaches to project

planning. The 3 descriptions were also developed based on the manipulation

instructions used in Study 1. These descriptions are presented in Table 8.3. Participants

were asked to indicate which among the three approaches they commonly used when

planning for their HRIS project.

160
Table 8.3
Descriptions of the threes approaches

Manipulations instructions used in Study 1 Descriptions of approaches used in Study 2

Inside view: Inside view:


Please take a few minutes to consider your plans When planning for an HRIS project, I focus on the
for completing your project assignment. Think kinds of events that are likely to happen. I take into
about the kind of events that are likely to happen as consideration all the problems that are likely to
you work on your project assignment. Think about happen in the future that will impede the project’s
all the problems that are likely to happen in the progress and performance, and any aspect of the
future that will impede your project progress and project or situations in the future that will facilitate
performance, and any aspects of the project or the project’s progress and performance.
situation in the future that will facilitate your
project progress and performance.

Outside view: Outside view:


Please take a few minutes to consider your plans When planning for an HRIS project, I recall the
for completing your project assignment. Recall the kinds of events that have happened in my past
kind of events that have happened in your past projects and those of other people that I know
project assignment and those of other people that about. I also recall all the problems that have
you know about. Recall all the problems that have happened in the past that have impeded my project
happened in the past that have impeded your progress and performance, and any aspects of the
project progress and performance, and any aspect project or situation in the past that have facilitated
of the project or situation in the past that have my project progress and performance. Apart from
facilitated your project progress and performance. learning from my past experience, I also consult
with other people who have experience in similar
projects.

Unpacking approach: Unpacking approach:


Please take a few minutes to consider your plans When planning for an HRIS, I think about the
for completing your project assignment. Think components that are needed for my project
about the components that are needed for project progress and performance. I also think about each
progress and performance. Also, think about each and everything that I plan to do in completing the
and everything that you plan to do in completing project. I breakdown all the tasks that need to be
this project assignment. carried out in the project into specific tasks.

8.5.2 Project Completion Time Measures

The predicted completion time is measured by taking the difference between the

answer given by participants to indicate when the project started, and when the project

is supposed to end, or did end. For completed projects, the actual completion time was

measured by the date stated by participants who already finished their HRIS project. In

a separate question, participants were further asked to state the total number of days by

which the project either falls short or exceeds the due date for completion.

161
8.5.3 Outcome Success Measures

Outcome success was measured in several ways. First, participants were asked

to indicate the extent to which the predicted or actual outcome of their project falls short

of expectations, meets expectations, or exceeds expectations. Second, by marking a 15

cm line, which had labeled anchors of 50%, 100% and 150%, participants were asked to

indicate their predicted or actual percentage of project outcome achieved. In business

projects, even though the outcome is claimed to meet the expectation, it may not mean

exactly 100% as planned. Thus, this scale was included to gain further understanding on

to what extent participants’ project outcomes really meet, or exceed their expectations.

The scale begins at 50% as most projects may be considered as a failure if the outcome

achieved was less than 50% from what was initially planned. In this study, 100%

indicates that the outcome achieved was exactly what was planned. The scale was

extended up to 150% as in certain situations, the outcome achieved do sometimes

exceed what was initially planned. For analysis, the response was measured from the

50% end of the line and centimeters were converted to a number relative to 100%.

Participants who had finished their HRIS project were asked to answer

additional questions related to their satisfaction with the system. To measure

participants’ satisfaction level towards specific components of the system, an

established 12-item measure of IT system user satisfaction from Doll and Torkzadeh

(1988) was adapted with permission. The scale was chosen because it is conceptualized

as “the affective attitude towards a specific computer application by someone who

interacts with the application directly” (Doll & Torkzadeh, 1988, p.261). Their end-user

computing satisfaction instrument has been shown to be both reliable and valid for

measuring end-user satisfaction involving different types of computer platform and

applications (Doll, Deng, Raghunathan, Torkzadeh, & Xia, 2004; Hendrickson,

Glorfeld, & Cronan, 1994). Several studies have reported that the scale has adequate

162
internal consistency (the Cronbach alphas for each factor ranging from .65 to .98 and for

the 12-item scale ranging from .88 to .96), test-retest reliability (correlation coefficient

for the 12-item scale ranging from .76 to .96), criterion-related validity (.76), and

construct validity (all items have significant loading on their corresponding factors

ranging from .62 to .94) (Doll & Torkzadeh, 1988; Doll, Xia, & Torkzadeh, 1994;

Hendrickson et al., 1994; McHaney & Cronan, 1998; McHaney, Hightower, & Pearson,

2002; McHaney, Hightower, & White, 1999; Torkzadeh & Doll, 1991). The 12 items

represent five underlying dimensions of end-user satisfaction: content, accuracy, format,

ease of use, and timeliness.

The 12 items were rephrased by changing the wording of the original version

from a question format to a statement format to suit the agree-disagree response scale

used for this study. In the past, some authors have also made some changes to the

original version of the Doll and Torkzadeh (1988) end-user satisfaction instrument.

Abdinnour-Helm, Chaparro and Farmer (2005) for example, made minor alterations in

the wording of the original end-user computer satisfaction items such as using past tense

for several items and using “site satisfaction” rather than “system satisfaction” where

appropriate to suit the specifics of Web-site user satisfaction. Otto, Nadjawi and Caron

(2000) replaced the “timeliness” construct with website “responsiveness” or download

time in measuring their web-user satisfaction.

Second, the response scale was changed from its original frequency scale to an

agreement scale taking into consideration the difficulty for participants to remember the

frequency of events that have happened. The original scale does not apply in this study

as some of the questions are not appropriate for the frequency scale. The question of

whether the HRIS is providing precise information, presenting output in a useful format,

providing with up-to-date information, or is easy to use or not, much depends on how

the system is being planned and designed initially. Problems that relate to the system are

163
often detected on the day it is implemented. If an effort is not made to correct them, the

problems will persist every time the system is used. Therefore, it is difficult to justify

measuring feelings of satisfaction towards the system using a frequency of occurrence

scale. In this study, each of the adapted questions asked how strongly the respondents

agreed or disagreed with the user satisfaction statements on a seven-point scale

whereby, 1 = strongly disagree, and 7 = strongly agree. For this measure, the higher the

score, the greater feelings of satisfaction.

Due to changes in the wording and response scale, the reliability of the new

adapted scale in this study was compared to previous studies. Coefficient alpha for the

36 participants for the adapted 12-item end-user satisfaction scale was .97. The

reliabilities (alpha) of the five sub-scales were: content = .93; accuracy = .89; format =

1.00, ease of use = .89 and timeliness = .92. The original and adapted versions of the 12

items are shown in Table 8.4.

Finally, in a separate question participants were asked to indicate their overall

feelings of satisfaction with the system by placing a single slash mark across a 15 cm

line, which had labeled anchors of not at all (0), fair (5) and extremely (10). For

analysis, the response was measured from the 0 end of the line and centimeters were

converted to a number out of 10. Participants were also asked to list aspects of the

system that they were most and least satisfied with (if any).

164
Table 8.4
Original and adapted versions of end-user satisfaction items

Original version Adapted version


Content
Does the system provide the precise The system provides the precise information
information you need? that I need.
Does the information content meet your The information content meets my needs.
needs?
Does the system provide reports that seem to The system provides reports that seem to be
be just about exactly what you need? just about exactly what I need.
Does the system provide sufficient The system provides sufficient information.
information?
Format
Do you think the output is presented in a The output is presented in a useful format.
useful format?
Is the information clear? The information is clear.
Accuracy
Is the system accurate? The system is accurate.
Are you satisfied with the accuracy of the I am satisfied with the accuracy of the system.
system?
Timeliness
Do you get the information you need in time? I get the information I need in time.
Does the system provide up-to-date The system provides up-to-date information.
information?
Ease of use
Is the system user friendly? The system is user friendly.
Is the system easy to use? The system is easy to use.

8.5.4 Measures of Affective Response to Success and Failure

Measures for affective response were adapted from Sanna and Schwarz (2004).

Participants who were at the planning stage of the HRIS project were asked to estimate

on two 11-point scales how they would feel after “a successful project” and if the

project were to “succeed” (1 = not good, 11 = very good). They were also asked to

estimate how they would feel after “not a successful project” and if the project “does

not succeed” (1 = very bad, 11 = not bad). Participants who had finished their HRIS

projects were asked to answer a dichotomous question on whether they viewed their

165
HRIS project as successful or not successful. If they answered successful they then

turned to two questions asking the degree to which they feel good (not good to very

good), and if they answered not successful they turned to questions asking the degree to

which they feel bad (very bad to not bad) about their HRIS project outcome.

8.5.5 Demographic Measures

A number of demographic variables were also measured for descriptive and

control purposes. These included job level, and the extent of experience with HRIS

systems and projects, which may be correlated with the level of expectations of project

outcomes, and with the accuracy of project completion predictions. It is necessary to

control such extraneous sources of variation in the analyses of the results. In Malaysia,

there is an interest in ethnicity, and acceptance of the results of this research in business

circles will require that a full description of the sample is possible, including the

breakdown of ethnic origins and gender of the participants. This information is also

necessary to show that the sample is representative and to ensure that generalizations to

the wider population of firms and employees can be made.

8.6 Data Analysis Strategy

Project data from 45 organizations were included in the analysis. Although the

final sample size for this study is fairly small in an absolute sense, it is nevertheless

comparable with past studies on projects in organizations. Ewusi-Mensah &

Parzasnyski (1991) for example, used 49 out of 566 questionnaires in their exploratory

study on project abandonment. Morgenshtern, Raz and Dvir (2007) based their analysis

on the final sample of 43 projects collected from 24 individuals to study factors that

affect duration and effort estimation errors in software development projects. In another

study, Hyvari (2006) used 25 respondents from among 368 individuals in 78 companies

to examine the success factors of project management in organizations.

166
Taking into consideration the small sample size, analyses were conducted using

descriptive statistics and hypotheses were tested using correlation analysis. The analyses

were conducted using SPSS (version 15) program for Windows. It was planned that if

the sample size of projects was larger, involving the whole project planning team in

each organization, factor analysis would be conducted to test the new adapted version of

the end-user satisfaction instruments and a multilevel regression analysis to test the

research hypotheses. However, the number of respondents was not sufficient for these

techniques.

8.6.1 Open-ended Questions

There were three open-ended questions in this survey. The first open-ended

question was regarding the nature of the participants’ involvement in the HRIS projects.

Responses from participants were first transcribed, before being given to two

individuals for the categorizing process (working independently). Each individual was

asked to sort the responses according to categories that I formulated based on Ceriello

and Freeman’s (1991) work. They suggested that steps in computerizing human

resources basically involve five phases of effort, namely system planning, system

design, vendor selection, system implementation and system maintenance and

evaluation. Thus, for the first open-ended question, these five categories were developed

for the sorting and coding process.

The second open-ended question was regarding the aspects of the system that

participants felt most satisfied with, and the third question was on the aspects of the

system that participants felt least satisfied with. A similar process was performed with

the same coders. The categories of answers for the second and third open-ended

questions were formulated based on Doll and Torkzadeh’s (1988) end-user satisfaction

constructs: content, format, accuracy, timeliness and ease of use. For answers that did

not fit with any of the categories given, they were recorded in the ‘others’ categories.

167
Therefore, six categories were developed for the sorting and coding process for the

second and third open-ended questions.

Each coder was given three instruction sheets with definitions for each category,

the three open-ended questions, participants’ responses for each of the questions and

three sets of coding sheets. For one coder, the process was started with the responses

regarding aspects of the system that participants felt most satisfied with. It then

followed by sorting responses regarding participants’ involvement in the HRIS project,

and finally with the responses regarding the aspects of the system that participants felt

least satisfied with. The other coder did these tasks in the reverse order.

Each coder took between 40 and 70 minutes to finish the whole task. Answers

from both individuals were then compared. For any answers that were not in agreement

for both individuals, consensus was sought. Both individuals were asked to discuss and

decide on the answers. It took approximately an hour to complete the second task. The

open-ended materials and the instructions for coding are presented in Appendix I.

8.7 Conclusion

This chapter has explained the research method and strategy of the second study.

It described how the sample of organizations was obtained, the selection of the

respondents, development of the questionnaire, the research materials, and the survey

procedure. This chapter also briefly explains the adoption of correlation analysis to test

the research hypotheses. The results of the second study are reported in Chapter 9.

168
CHAPTER 9

RESULTS – STUDY 2

9.1 Introduction

Chapter 9 reports results of the second study. The chapter begins by reporting

the demographic characteristics of the respondents and descriptions of the HRIS

projects involved. It then presents the bivariate relationships between the research

variables. The chapter concludes with the descriptive analysis of the findings.

9.2. Demographic Characteristics of the Participants

Detailed descriptive statistics of the participants’ demographic characteristics are

presented in Appendix I. It is noted that 62.2% of the 45 participants in this survey were

females. The average age of the participants was 37 years old. Malays constitute 80% of

the survey participants, followed by 11.1% Malaysian Chinese and 8.9% Malaysian

Indians. The majority of the participants in this survey (93.3%) had higher academic

qualifications of either a tertiary or diploma, first / professional degree or second degree

and above. The remainder of the respondents had either secondary education (11 years

of schooling) or certificate. Senior management staff such as executives, managers and

senior managers made up 80% of the total participants. The rest consisted of

administrative and other technical staff.

On average, the participants had been in their present position for 59 months,

almost 5 years, and had served their organization for 7.5 years. Participants’ average

experience as computer-based system users was 10.7 years, and average experience

with HRIS projects was 6 years.

169
The average number of employees in the organizations and in their HR

departments was 2161 and 21 people respectively. HRIS had been used by the

organizations for an average of 10 years. Computer-based HR system applications that

are presently implemented are summarized in Table 9.1.

Table 9.1
HR computer-based applications reported as presently implemented in surveyed
organizations

HR computer-based applications Total number of organizations


Payroll 43
Compensation and benefits 27
Training and career development 24
Performance appraisal 18
Recruitment and selection 15
Human resource planning 12
Time management 11
Employee relations 9
Employee database 8
Leave administration 7
Medical 3
Employee self-service (ESS) 2
Standard operating procedure (SOP) 1
Documentation and organization 1
Organization Management 1

In the next section, a brief description of the HRIS projects is presented.

9.3 HRIS Projects in the Survey

Results presented in this section are based on responses given in the open-ended

questions and descriptive statistics for selected relevant variables from the final sample

of 45 respondents. Most of the projects in the sample (66.7%) were projects to install a

new system. The remainder were projects to upgrade existing systems. Based on the

responses given by the participants, installing a new system in their context did not

mean that it was their first experience using HRIS. It was more about switching to

170
another software provider, changing the operating system used (e.g., from DOS to Web-

based system), or changing from a stand-alone system to a more advanced, integrated

system. Upgrading the old system referred to system maintenance where minor

modifications or adjustments to the system were made. For example, the system was

either upgraded from the old version to a newer version, or changing the calculation

setting in payroll and compensation modules when the government imposed a new

policy, which may affect for example, the calculation of employees’ salary, deduction

of employees’ provident fund (EPF), and employees’ social security organization

(SOCSO).

From the total of 45 projects, 36 projects had been completed and 9 projects

were still at the planning stage. Of the 36 projects completed, 29 were completed as

planned with respect to completion time, and the remaining 7 projects finished later

than planned. On average, the project was completed within 10 months, with the

shortest project completed within 1 month and the longest project completed in 48

months or 4 years. Of the 36 participants with completed projects, 32 of them viewed

their project as successful.

As for the 9 projects at the planning stage, 4 participants predicted that the

project will be completed as planned, 4 participants predicted that the project will finish

later than planned, and 1 participant predicted that the project will finish earlier than

planned. On average, these projects were scheduled to finish within 21 months, with the

shortest project scheduled to finish in 3 months and the longest in 42 months or 3.5

years.

When participants were asked about who initiated the idea to develop the

system, 91% indicated that the ideas were initiated internally by the organization.

Specifically, 40% of the ideas came from the HR department, 37.8% were initiated by

top management, 17.8% came from the IS department, 4.4% came from the accounts

171
and finance department and 2.2% came from the operation side. These percentages do

not total to 100 percent as six respondents indicated more than one source though they

were instructed to indicate only one. Most of the systems implemented were either

acquired from a vendor (46.7%) or through a combination of packaged and custom

software (37.8%), where a lower cost packaged program was purchased and some

modification was done to the package to suit the organization’s needs.

Regarding participants’ involvement in the project in terms of their role and

contribution, the coded responses indicate their involvement in the five phases of effort

during the project. Table 9.2 summarizes the responses given by participants.

Table 9.2
Participants’ involvement in the project phases (all the 45 projects)

Project phases Total number of participants Among the activities reported


coded as performing this phase
System Planning 18 Confirming the activities to be
carried out; determining the
requirements and specifications
of the systems

System Design 23 Researching, collecting and


drafting the system structure;
planning and designing new
screens, reports and modules

Vendor Selection 12 Contacting vendor; getting


quotation; evaluating
demonstration and selecting the
vendor

System 28 Monitoring the testing and the


Implementation transition process; reinstall data
into new system; check for
accuracy after data was
reinstalled; check for reporting
issues; solving problems that
arise during the transition process

System Maintenance 18 Getting feedback from the end-


and Evaluation users; identified problems with
the existing system; provide new
requirements to be included in
the upgrading of the system

172
The descriptive statistics of projects in the survey discussed in this section are

presented in Appendix J.

9.4 Planning Methods and Project Outcomes

All participants were asked a forced choice question regarding the kind of

approach they used when planning for HRIS. Of the 45 participants, 20 indicated that

they used the outside view, followed by the unpacking approach (17 participants) and

the inside view (8 participants). In terms of whether the project outcomes meet their

expectation or not, 30 of 36 participants who had completed their HRIS projects

indicated that their project outcomes did meet their expectations. The remainder

indicated that the project outcome fell short of their expectations (5 participants) or

exceeded their expectations (1 participant). Even though 30 participants stated that the

project outcomes did meet their expectations, only 6 reported that their outcomes met

100% of their expectations as planned. All 9 of the participants at the planning stage

predicted that their project outcomes will meet their expectations. However, when asked

about percentage of planned outcome they expected to be achieved, only 1 participant

predicted his / her project outcome will meet 100% as planned.

Participants who had completed the projects were further asked to list the

aspects of the system that they felt most and least (if any) satisfied with. Their responses

were coded using the categories of content, format, accuracy, timeliness and ease of use.

Table 9.3a and b summarizes the responses given by participants.

173
Table 9.3a
Aspects of system that participants felt most satisfied with
Aspects of the system Total number of participants Examples from among the
coded as responding to the responses reported
aspect of the system
Content 13 “Provides comprehensive,
precise and sufficient
information as needed; able to
generate basic information; able
to pull out information needed
for monthly report”
Format 14 “Allow to use a simpler report”
Accuracy 10 “Generate accurate report; the
information / data retrieved are
accurate; the system provides
accurate information”
Timeliness 8 “Able to retrieve and generate
reports in a timely manner; get
results in a faster manner; save
time; speed up the process of
getting employee information”
Ease of use 15 “Simple to use; easy to use; user
friendly; easy to update; easy for
tracking purposes; easy to
access”
Other aspects 7 “complete HR functions; the
system integrates with other
modules; the system provides
good file maintenance; the web-
based system allows employees
to access their own data which
helps in decreasing their
workload and saves time; the
system provides a sense of
internal control; and the system
was reliable and efficient”

174
Table 9.3b
Aspects of system that participants felt least satisfied with
Aspects of the system Total number of participants Examples from among the
coded as responding to the responses reported
aspect of the system
Content 12 “The system failed to generate
the kind of reports requested; the
system only provides the most
recent data and was not able to
retain the archive data, which
was needed for referencing and
checking”
Format 15 “The report generated does not
meet with external demand; the
amendments of the results need
to be done manually; unable to
breakdown the records as
needed”
Accuracy 0 None was reported
Timeliness 7 “The system was very slow in
processing especially when there
were many users concurrently on
the system; the system depends
too much on human input which
often delayed the process; it is a
batch processing, not real time
system”
Ease of use 6 “The system was not as user
friendly as expected; difficult to
use; very complicated especially
for aged staff and the non-IT
savvy”
Other aspects 6 “the system was not integrated
with the existing system, other
system or modules and outside
systems such as the bank auto-
pay system; the system was very
basic and simple where some
functions have to be carried out
manually, or it just serves a
transaction processing function
only and was not helping in
decision making; no helpdesk
facilities; very bad support
service; some of the HR
functions were either not
available or did not meet their
expectations; updating processes
have to be done manually; and
the system was not stable”

175
Regarding their overall satisfaction with the systems, twenty one participants

rated between good and extremely good (rated between 7.5 and 10 on 10 point scale),

and fifteen participants rated between fair and good (rated between 5 and 7.5 on 10

point scale). The average overall satisfaction was 7.17(SD=1.48).

The descriptive statistics of planning methods and expected project outcomes

summarized in this section are presented in Appendix J. The next section reports the

correlations between the research variables.

9.5 Correlation Analysis

Table 9.4 presents the means, standard deviations, and Pearson correlations of

variables for the 36 participants who had completed projects. The internal consistency

reliabilities (Cronbach’s Alpha) of the research measures are reported in parenthesis

along the diagonal of the correlation table. As shown in Table 9.4, the Cronbach’s

alphas for the three items in each of the planning approach measures were in a range

between .72 and .84. The five sub-scales of the 12 item end-user satisfaction scale

(content, accuracy, format, ease of use and timeliness) also have satisfactory reliability

values ranging from .89 to 1.00. It is noted that Cronbach’s alpha for satisfaction with

format was 1.00 and after checking the data, the answers to the two items did

correspond within the participants. The correlation analysis results were also used to

address the hypotheses for bias in predictions of task duration and outcome.

9.5.1 Bias in Project Completion Prediction

Bias in project completion prediction was not significantly correlated with the

extent of use of any of the three planning approaches, the inside view, the outside view,

and the unpacking approach.

176
Table 9.4
Descriptive statistics, scale reliabilities, and correlations of variables for completed projects

Variables N Mean S.D. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13


1. Gender 36 .61 .49
2. Age 33 37.30 6.58 .11
3. Malay 36 .78 .42 -.15 .13
4. Chinese 36 .14 .35 .16 -.45** -.75**
5. Indian 36 .08 .28 .03 .28 -.56** -.12
6. Education 36 3.75 1.08 -.24 -.37* .00 .02 -.02
7. Tenure in present position 36 60.17 60.46 .12 .52** -.08 -.21 .38* -.48**
8. Tenure 36 95.00 74.80 .30 .57** .11 -.14 .00 -.17 .28
9. Computer-based system experience 36 10.86 5.43 .02 .57** -.05 -.10 .20 -.03 .19 .46**
10. HRIS projects experience 36 75.33 54.17 .19 .32 .00 -.06 .07 .02 .11 .50** .63*
11. Inside – use rating 36 5.22 1.14 -.10 -.33 -.27 .18 .18 .26 -.28 -.03 -.17 -.20 (.83)
12. Outside – use rating 36 5.02 1.15 -.02 -.26 -.21 .09 .20 .12 -.24 -.02 .18 .16 .56** (.84)
13. Unpacked – use rating 36 5.59 .86 -.22 .03 .06 .00 -.09 .31 -.08 .02 .32 .02 .47** .33* (.72)
14. Project duration 36 10.81 9.68 -.20 .24 .23 -.22 -.08 .16 -.16 .30 .33* .18 -.12 -.02 .01
15. Bias in duration prediction 36 1.22 3.06 -.22 -.22 .15 -.08 -.12 .17 -.29 -.11 -.04 .05 .06 .17 -.02
16. Percentage of outcome achieved 36 89.44 16.84 .00 -.08 .35* -.02 -.50** .16 -.29 .14 -.06 -.07 .30 -.02 .35*
17. Satisfaction with content 36 5.48 1.06 -.08 .11 .13 -.15 -.02 .04 .13 .18 .11 -.13 .21 .05 .48**
18. Satisfaction with format 36 5.33 1.21 -.16 .03 .20 -.11 -.16 .17 .00 .18 .05 -.12 .34* .07 .56**
19. Satisfaction with accuracy 36 5.36 1.26 -.16 .02 .08 -.08 -.01 .06 .13 .16 -.02 -.20 .36* .10 .53**
20. Satisfaction with timeliness 36 5.19 1.42 -.05 .06 -.12 .09 .07 -.05 .23 .13 .02 -.20 .43* .18 .52**
21. Satisfaction with ease of use 36 5.25 1.19 -.12 -.03 .09 -.05 -.06 .01 .05 .06 -.13 -.33 .32 -.02 .45**
22. End-user satisfaction (12-item scale) 36 5.35 1.11 -.12 .05 .08 -.07 -.03 .05 .12 .16 .02 -.20 .34* .08 .54**
23. Overall satisfaction 36 7.17 1.48 -.20 .21 .29 -.13 -.28 .05 .01 .25 .00 -.15 .13 -.04 .50**
24. Feeling of success 32 8.30 1.53 -.11 .11 .13 -.08 -.09 -.18 .42* .20 -.07 -.15 .25 -.06 .40*

Note: Coefficient alpha reliability estimates are in parentheses on the diagonal of the correlation table
*Correlation is significant at p < 0.05 and **Correlation is significant at p< 0.01

177
Table 9.4 (Continued)
Descriptive statistics, scale reliabilities, and correlations of variables for completed projects

Variables 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24
14. Project duration
15. Bias in duration prediction .40*
16. Percentage of outcome achieved -.21 -.28
17. Satisfaction with content -.15 -.22 .48** (.93)
18. Satisfaction with format -.22 -.20 .64** .90** (1.00)
19. Satisfaction with accuracy -.19 -.13 .42* .90** .89** (.89)
20. Satisfaction with timeliness -.32 -.31 .42* .76** .73** .81** (.92)
21. Satisfaction with ease of use -.27 -.17 .46** .84** .78** .82** .84** (.89)
22. End-user satisfaction (12-item scale) -.24 -.23 .52** .96** .92** .95** .88** .92** (.97)
23. Overall satisfaction -.01 -.14 .46** .68** .67** .64** .69** .78** .74**
24. Feeling of success -.22 -.41* .58** .60** .61** .62** .75** .73** .71** .60**
Note: Coefficient alpha reliability estimates are in parentheses on the diagonal of the correlation table
*Correlation is significant at p < 0.05 and **Correlation is significant at p< 0.01

178
9.5.2 Bias in Project Outcome Prediction

Percentage of outcome achieved was significantly positively correlated with the

extent of use of the unpacking approach (r = .35, p<.01). This result indicates that

participants who report higher adoption of the unpacking approach also tend to report a

higher percentage of outcome achieved.

There were also significant positive correlations between end-user satisfaction

and both the extent of use of the inside view (r = .34, p<.05), and the extent of use of the

unpacking approach (r = .54, p<.01). Hence, the more participants report they adopted

the inside view or the unpacking approach, the more satisfied they were with the

system.

Table 9.4 also revealed significant positive relationships between all of the end-

user satisfaction components and the extent of use of the unpacking approach, with

correlation coefficients between .45 and .56. Also, there were significant positive

relationships between all except two of the end-user satisfaction components and the

inside view, with correlation coefficients between .34 and .43. These results imply that

the more participants adopted the unpacking approach, the more satisfied they were

with the content, format, accuracy, timeliness and ease of use of the system, and this

also applied to the inside view for some components.

Participants’ rating of overall satisfaction was significantly positively correlated

with the extent of use of the unpacking approach (r = .50, p<.01) but not with the

reported extent of use of the inside or outside views approaches, suggesting that the

more participants adopted the unpacking approach, the more satisfied they were with

the overall project outcome.

9.5.3 Other Correlation Results

Feelings that the project was a success were positively and significantly

correlated with the extent of use of the unpacking approach (r = .40, p<.05) but again

179
not with reported extent of use of the inside or outside views approaches. This suggests

that participants with higher adoption of the unpacking approach have higher feelings of

project success.

There were other significant correlations that are worth noting. Table 9.4 reveals

positive and significant relationships between reported use of the outside and inside

views (r = .56, p< .01), between unpacking and the inside view (r = .47, p<.01) and

between unpacking and the outside view (r = .33, p<.05). This suggests that the three

approaches were related, or it may reflect a common method variance component. There

are insufficient cases to test this explanation.

9.6 Hypothesis Testing

Two sets of questions explored the approaches used by the HRIS planners; the

ratings of use of the three planning approaches and the forced choice question. To test

hypotheses 2.1a, 2.1b, 2.2a and 2.2b, the three approaches are compared on the

dependent variable. But, with the small sample size it is not possible to conduct a

statistical test to compare the three approaches on the bias in completion time and

outcome. Results from the correlations and descriptive statistics were also taken into

consideration.

9.6.1 Project Completion Time Predictions

Table 9.5 presents the numbers of participants who reported that they used either

the inside view, outside view, or unpacking approach to planning when asked a forced

choice question. As shown in Table 9.5, it appears that the unpacking approach may be

related to finishing on time. From the total of 14 projects for which the respondents

chose the unpacking approach for the forced choice questions, only 1 project was

finished later than planned. However, this result is not significant using a Chi-squared

test of the contingency table.

180
Table 9.5
The planning approach used and project completion time (forced choice questions)

Project Planning approach used


completion time Total
Inside view Outside view Unpacking
On time 5 11 13 29
Late 2 4 1 7

Total 7 15 14 36

Out of 36 completed projects, 7 projects finished later than was planned. On

average, projects finished 6.28 months late or about 36.08% of the entire project

duration (see Appendix K-1). The biases are substantial.

Four participants at the planning stage predicted that their project would finish

later than planned (see Appendix K-2). On average, they predicted that their project will

take 5 months longer than expected or about 37% of the entire estimated project

duration. One participant believed that his / her project would finish 18 months earlier

than planned.

9.6.2 Project Outcome Predictions

For the completed projects, Table 9.6 presents the mean reported percentage of

outcome achieved for participants who reported that they used either the inside view,

outside view, or unpacking approach to planning for the forced choice question. As

shown in Table 9.6, it seems that the unpacking approach may be related to outcome

success. Out of 100 percent of outcome planned to achieve, on average 92.57 percent of

outcome was successfully achieved by those who adopted the unpacking approach.

However, the number of participants is too small for a statistical test of significance of

this effect.

181
Table 9.6
The planning approach used and mean reported percentage of outcome achieved
(standard deviations in brackets)

Planning approach used


Inside view (n=7) Outside view (n=15) Unpacking (n=14)
Mean percentage of
outcome achieved 87.00(13.08) 87.67(24.37) 92.57(5.24)

For the 9 participants who had not yet completed their project, Table 9.7

presents the mean predicted percentage of outcome expected to be achieved for those

who reported that they used either the outside view or the unpacking approach to

planning when asked the forced choice question. None reported using the inside view.

Results shown in Table 9.7 suggest that participants who adopt the unpacking approach

may be less optimistic regarding the outcome expected to be achieved than those who

adopt the outside view, but the number of participants is too small to establish this

difference statistically.

Table 9.7
The planning approach used and mean predicted percentage of outcome achieved
(standard deviations in brackets)

Planning approach used


Outside view (n=5) Unpacking (n=4)
Mean predicted percentage of
outcome achieved 88.00(10.37) 79.25(13.74)

In Hypothesis 2.4 I predicted that the level of satisfaction with the HRIS project

outcome is related to the planning approach adopted by the planners. Specifically, I

hypothesized that planners who adopt the outside view (H2.4a) or the unpacking

approach (H2.4b) will be more satisfied with the HRIS outcome than planners who

adopt the inside view. Table 9.8 presents the planning approach used and mean end-user

satisfaction. For the unpacking approach, the results in Table 9.8 support H2.4b, with

the direction of the difference in mean satisfaction consistent with that hypothesis. The
182
opposite occurred for the outside view, in that those who adopted the outside view were

less satisfied than those who used the inside view, so H2.4a was not supported.

Table 9.8
The planning approach used and mean end-user satisfaction (standard deviations
in brackets)

Mean end-user Planning approach used


satisfaction with
Inside view (n=7) Outside view (n=15) Unpacking (n=14)
The system (12-
items) 5.38(.90) 5.04(1.36) 5.67(.86)
Content 5.50(.84) 5.17(1.20) 5.80(.95)
Format 5.27(.74) 5.00(1.56) 5.72(.90)
Accuracy 5.36(1.14) 5.07(1.50) 5.68(1.01)
Timeliness 5.21(1.35) 4.90(1.76) 5.50(1.04)
Ease of use 5.43(.93) 4.93(1.44) 5.50(.98)

Table 9.9 shows the mean for the single question about overall satisfaction with

the system for the planning approach used. This was a different question in the

questionnaire, and is not the same as the first line in Table 9.8, which is for the 12-item

end-user satisfaction scale. Overall, participants who adopted the unpacking approach

appear to be more satisfied with the system than those who adopted the inside and

outside view. Out of a 10 point scale (with 0 not at all satisfied and 10 extremely

satisfied), on average participants who adopted the unpacking approach rated the system

between good and extremely good. Participants who adopted the outside view seem to

be less satisfied with the system and rated it between fair and good. Even though the

number of participants is small, the difference in mean overall satisfaction with the

system between those who chose the outside view and unpacking was marginally

significant difference by a two-tailed t-test for independent samples, t(27) = -1.91, p =

.068.

183
Table 9.9
The planning approach used and mean of overall satisfaction with the system
(standard deviations in brackets)

Planning approach used


Inside view (n=7) Outside view (n=15) Unpacking (n=14)
Mean of overall
satisfaction with the 7.21(1.16) 6.66(1.62) 7.71(1.35)
system

9.6.3 Predictions of Affective Response towards Outcome Success and Failure

Of the 36 participants who had completed their HRIS projects, 32 viewed their

projects as successful and 4 viewed their project as a failure. All four participants who

viewed the project as a failure reported that they adopted the outside view during the

planning process. The 9 participants who were at the planning stage indicated that they

adopted the outside and unpacking approaches when planning for the project. The size

of these groups is too small to statistically compare between the predicted and actual

feelings of success and failure of the participants in the inside view, outside view and

unpacking groups. Therefore, only descriptive analyses and qualitative comparisons are

used to describe the patterns of participants’ affective response towards project outcome

success and failure in this study.

Table 9.10 presents descriptive statistics of both the predicted and actual

affective response towards outcome success or failure. On average, predicted affective

response was higher than the actual affective response towards outcome success, and

predicted affective response was lower than the actual affective response towards

outcome failure.

184
Table 9.10
Descriptive statistics of participants’ affective response towards project outcome
success and failure

Descriptions Frequency Mean Std. Dev Median


Completed project
Actual affective response
Success 32 8.41 1.5 8.0
Failure 4 5.50 2.1 5.5
Total 36
Uncompleted project
Predicted affective response
Success 9 9.56 1.0 9.0
Failure 9 3.78 2.4 3.0
Total 9

Based on the descriptive statistics result shown in Table 9.10, in general

participants do overestimate the positive feelings they will experience upon success and

their negative feelings upon failure. This is consistent with the occurrence of impact

bias.

As no respondents for the uncompleted projects reported using the inside view,

only two groups could be compared (outside view and unpacking approach) for the

patterns of affective prediction and actual experience of positive feelings in response to

success. Table 9.11 presents means of the predicted and actual affective response

towards project outcome success for participants who reported using the outside view

and unpacking approaches. There is a smaller difference between the predicted and

actual affective response towards outcome success for those who used the unpacking

approach, than for those who used the outside view approach.

185
Table 9.11
The planning approach used and mean predicted and actual affective response
towards project outcome success (standard deviations in brackets)

Planning approach used


Outside view (n=5) Unpacking (n=4)
Mean predicted affective
response towards project 9.60 (1.14) 9.00 (0.82)
outcome success for
uncompleted project
Outside view (n=11) Unpacking (n=13)
Mean actual affective
response towards project 8.23 (1.89) 8.58(1.40)
outcome success for
completed project

9.7 Conclusion

This chapter described the demographic characteristics of the 45 participants, the

open-ended responses, and the results of the correlation and descriptive analyses. The

research hypotheses were considered in the light of those results. The results indicate

that those who adopt the unpacking approach during planning have projects that were

more likely to be completed on time as compared to those who adopt the inside or

outside view.

The results also imply that the unpacking approach is related to outcome

success. The more that participants adopted the unpacking approach the more satisfied

they were with the system, and this includes satisfaction with the content, format,

accuracy, timeliness and ease of use of the system.

As for the prediction of affective response towards outcome success and failure,

participants in general predicted more positive feelings after success and more negative

feeling after failure than were actually experienced. This effect was less for the

participants who used the unpacking approach, although the results cannot be tested for

significance. These research findings for Study 2 are discussed in the next chapter,

Chapter 10.

186
CHAPTER 10

DISCUSSION – STUDY 2

10.1 Introduction

This chapter discusses the findings of Study 2 in light of the literature reviewed

on human resource information systems (HRIS) and the planning fallacy, and the

hypotheses developed in Chapter 7. Study 2 was conducted with two purposes in mind:

First, in Study 2 I aimed to explore approaches commonly used by planners during

HRIS project planning; Second, I aimed to extend explanations of the planning fallacy

to HRIS projects. For Study 2, the two explanations of the planning fallacy were tested

and compared using data on actual projects where participants reflected on the projects

that they had handled or the one they presently handled. Study 2 provides identification

of planning approaches that are used during HRIS projects. The findings, as presented

in Chapter 9, are discussed in the sections below. A more general discussion of the

broader implications of both studies will follow in Chapter 11.

10.2 Project Completion Time Predictions

This is the first time that the planning fallacy has been studied and shown to

occur for HRIS projects. In Study 2, the completion time predictions were measured

using two sets of questions: the rating of use of the three planning approaches and the

forced choice questions. Results from the rating of use of the three planning approaches

shows no association between bias of the completion time and the extent of use of any

of the three planning approaches, the inside view, the outside view and the unpacking

approach.

Results from the forced choice questions, however, suggest that the planning

fallacy does occur for HRIS projects, with 7 of the 36 completed projects finishing later

than planned, and none finishing early. Furthermore, of the completed HRIS projects

187
that used the unpacking approach, only one finished later than planned, fewer than for

projects that used the inside and outside views. This provides support for Kruger and

Evans’s (2004) explanation based on support theory. According to support theory,

unpack multifaceted tasks into their various subcomponents would make the tasks seem

bigger, which would increase estimates of the time the task would require, thus reducing

the planning fallacy. This indicates that for completion time predictions, a detailed

focus on the project at hand that requires a full enumeration of the component tasks will

be at least as accurate as taking an outside view approach to completion time prediction.

For those who adopted the inside or the outside view, there was little difference

in terms of the numbers of projects that finished later than planned. One possibility for

this result is that though HRIS planners may attend to their past, they may fail to

incorporate this information into their predictions. This may be due to the difficulty in

finding suitable similar projects, with previous projects usually not similar enough to

the project under consideration.

Another possibility is that HRIS planners may not have the time to examine

other similar projects even if they know about them, especially when the decision has to

be made in a short period of time. Instead of spending time examining the experiences

of other similar projects, HRIS planners may prefer and use information that is available

and easy to apply.

10.3 Project Outcome Predictions

In Study 2, HRIS implementation success was assessed by reports of the

percentage of outcome achieved and by the level of satisfaction with the system. The

results show that there was an association between the extent of use of the unpacking

approach and the reported percentage of outcome achieved. By listing down and

detailing the specifications and requirements of the systems, HRIS planners would have

188
a clearer picture of what they are supposed to deliver. Also, the detailing process of

determining the requirements needed would make it easier for planners to communicate

with vendors in choosing systems that best suited their organization’s needs. In contrast,

the extent to which the outside view was used, recalling past similar projects, did not

relate to the percentage of outcome achieved. This is an important emerging outcome

worth noting even though there was no hypothesis prepared to test whether the actual

outcome of the HRIS project differed for the different planning approaches.

In terms of the level of satisfaction with the system, HRIS planners who adopt

the unpacking approach or the inside view were more satisfied with the system than

those who adopt the outside view. For those who adopted the unpacking approach, the

higher feelings of satisfaction were possibly due to the higher percentage of outcome

they achieved. For those who adopted the outside view, though they achieved as much

outcome as those who adopt the inside view, they were not as satisfied with the

outcome as those who adopted the inside view. One possible explanation for these

results is that HRIS planners who use the outside view anchor their expected outcome to

other peoples’ experience of their HRIS systems. As highlighted in Chapter 9, most of

the HRIS systems implemented were acquired from a vendor. Based on qualitative

analyses of participants’ responses regarding the sources of potential vendors, the most

common and easiest way of acquiring a vendor was through recommendation by friends

from other organizations. Though the HRIS systems used by their friends worked well

in their friends’ organizations, it might not be the case in the participants’ organization.

In other words, the system may be effective for one organization but not for the other.

For example, certain HRIS modules may be suitable for large companies, but became

complicated when used by smaller companies.

For planners who adopt the unpacking approach, though the potential vendors

were also recommended by their friends, they may not have depended on the experience

189
of their friends in preparing what they needed. They work more on detailing and listing

down their needs to be presented to the vendor, and this may explain the satisfaction

feelings towards the outcome that they achieved.

10.4 Predictions of Affective Reactions towards Outcome Success and Failure

The affective measures were included in this study because it was reported in the

literature that HRIS users were not as happy as they had predicted once the systems

were implemented. This may indicate that HRIS users overestimate the affective

reactions they would experience following the outcome of the HRIS projects; that is

they display an impact bias.

Unlike Study 1, HRIS planners’ actual and predicted affective response towards

outcome success or failure was not assessed within the same person or for the same

projects. HRIS planners who had completed their projects rated their actual affective

response towards the system they implemented, and whether it was a success or not,

which provided the actual affective response information. HRIS planners who were at

the planning stage predicted their future affective response towards both the projects’

outcome success and failure, which provided the predicted affective response

information.

In this study, actual affective response towards outcome success was positively

related to the ratings of use of the unpacking approach, (but not the rating of use of

either the outside or inside views). This suggests that the more planners adopted the

unpacking approach the happier they were with the system they implemented. This is

not surprising, because as discussed in the previous section, HRIS planners who

adopted the unpacking approach achieved more of what they planned for. This might

explain the high positive feelings reported by those who adopted the unpacking

approach.

190
Though the data of actual and predicted affective response toward success and

failure were gathered from different participants for different projects, the results

portray some similar patterns to those in Study 1. Descriptively, HRIS planners who

have completed their project actually experienced less extreme positive feelings after

success and less extreme negative feelings after failure than was predicted by

participants at the planning stage. This finding of impact bias is consistent with past

findings on prediction of affective responses (Sanna & Schwarz, 2004).

In Study 1, shown in the graph in Figure 5.6 on page 105 participants in the

outside view condition were less biased than the unpacked condition for predictions of

affective reactions to outcome success. In Study 2 however, the results in Table 9.11 on

page 184 show the impact bias for positive affective reactions following a successful

HRIS project was less for those who use the unpacking approach than for those who use

the outside view. This result from Study 2 suggests that describing a project in greater

detail may improve the affective predictions towards outcome success more than using

past relevant experiences. Detailing the tasks that need to be carried out and the kind of

system that needs to be delivered may change the manner in which the project is

described. With a detailed description, planners may be able to imagine mentally what

the outcome will be. Having a detailed description before a project has taken place may

make the possibility for success or failure become more visible, giving some indication

to planners of whether or not they can deliver the project outcome. This may moderate

planners’ predictions of, and their actual affective reactions towards the outcome

success of the project.

In contrast, the outside view depends on recalling past relevant experience to

make predictions of future affective reactions. Though HRIS planners may have

experienced past success, it may not be easy to choose the kind of past events that can

be considered as the most relevant to their future reactions. Even if they are able to

191
recall how they felt in the past, they may not be able to apply this knowledge to their

predictions about the future.

Another possible reason may be that planners recall and rely on those memories

that come most easily to mind. According to Morewedge et al.(2005), people tend to

remember more of the unusual events than the more routine, everyday events.

Depending on the memory of the best of times like best project success may have

heightened the impact bias and this may explain why planners who adopt the outside

view overestimate the impact of emotions of future events.

Since this study is unable to examine planners’ affective reactions towards

outcome failure, future research is needed to further investigate the impact bias for

negative affective reactions following unsuccessful HRIS projects.

10.5 Study 2 Limitations

The results discussed in this chapter are exploratory given that the number of

respondents who were prepared to share their experiences in planning for human

resource information systems (HRIS) was relatively small.

The small sample was related to challenges faced in obtaining suitable

organizations in which to conduct data gathering. Many organizations were reluctant to

participate as they were concerned about the kind of information they have to provide

and the amount of time they have to spend answering the survey. As this study sought

participants who had been involved directly with the planning and decision making

regarding the HRIS, many potential respondents were senior in their organizations and

their time was valuable.

Apart from that, respondent availability often became an issue. It was common

to find that HRIS planners or decision makers were no longer with an organization

because of organizational restructuring or professional career advancement. For those

who were still with the organization, they were often reluctant to participate and

192
recommended their more senior team members to participate in the survey. Thus, it was

very difficult to get the whole project team to participate in the study especially for

projects that have been completed. Even though the sample of respondents is small, the

diversity of organizations who actually responded to the survey lends some validity to

the results. Also, the sample size is typical of sample sizes in published research on

project planning.

Another limitation of this study is that some data provided in this study is from a

single source, except for affective predictions and reactions that are provided by

different people for different projects. This includes rating of extent use of the planning

approach, the prediction of completion time, the percentage of outcome planned to

achieve and the satisfaction level with the systems. In future research, it would be

desirable to have some outcomes success measure and a measure of the planning

methods used that are not provided by the same person.

Apart from that, Study 2 involves a correlational design. The correlational

nature of the design has not made it possible to comment on the causal nature of the

relationships, as the discovery of association only suggests the possibility of cause.

However, showing correlation can be a useful first step toward demonstrating causation.

In summary, while there are some limitations associated with the approach used

here and given the exploratory nature of the study, the results of this research provide

useful findings that should be of interest to both researchers and practitioners.

10.6 Conclusion

The current chapter has discussed results from Study 2 in light of the literature

and limitations. The next chapter, Chapter 11, will provide a general discussion of the

findings of both studies and present theoretical and practical implications, as well as

limitations of the methods. Finally, recommendations will be made regarding

applications, and areas of future research.

193
CHAPTER 11

GENERAL DISCUSSION

11.1 Introduction

This chapter presents the overall discussion for the thesis, incorporating both

studies. The two studies reported in this thesis elaborate and extend prior research on

the planning fallacy. This study is the first to compare the inside / outside view and the

unpacking approach on the planning fallacy in the same task; and on predictions other

than time to project completion, specifically prediction of project outcome, confidence

in prediction of project completion time and outcome, and prediction of affective

reactions to project outcome success and failure, thereby extending previous research in

important ways. This is also the first time that the planning fallacy has been studied and

shown to occur for HRIS projects. There are several contributions that can be drawn

from both of the studies.

11.2 Theories of the Planning Fallacy

An important aspect of this thesis was the test of two theories of the planning

fallacy to increase the understanding of their effects on predictions made during project

planning. What is apparent from these studies is that the comparison of these two

theories of the planning fallacy, the inside / outside view and unpacking approach

provides a new and very important basis for understanding how to improve prediction

accuracy when planning for a project.

The discussion that follows begins with the effect of the inside / outside view

and unpacking approach on prediction of completion time. This is followed by a

discussion on extending the explanations of the planning fallacy to other types of

predictions. The implications for practice, limitations and directions for future research

are then discussed.

194
11.2.1 Completion Time Predictions

Studies have shown that people are generally overly optimistic about how long it

will take to complete a project task in the future even while knowing that their past

projects have taken longer than planned (Buehler et al., 1997; Buehler et al., 1994;

Newby-Clark et al., 2000). Past studies have attempted to explain why people commit

this planning fallacy by offering two conflicting prescriptions: the inside / outside view

and the unpacking approach. The current study extends previous research by comparing

both of these approaches in a single study.

Results from Study 1 show that the outside view condition was significantly less

biased than the control condition, providing evidence that an outside view manipulation

that only involves recall is sufficient to reduce the planning fallacy. However, results for

the comparison of the inside and outside view do not support the hypothesis for the

outside view. Though a qualitative examination of Figure 5.1 suggests participants in

the outside view condition were less biased than participants in the inside view

condition, this was not confirmed by the test of the intercepts.

The results however, support the hypothesis for unpacking. More accurate

predictions about project completion time were obtained when participants were asked

to unpack their task components as compared to those who do not unpack their task.

Though they were subject to the planning fallacy, it is to a lesser extent than participants

in the packed (control) condition. This result supports prior research on the planning

fallacy, which found that prompting people to unpack specific tasks would decrease or

eliminate the planning fallacy (Kruger & Evans, 2004).

The findings of Study 2 revealed that HRIS planners who reported that they

adopted the unpacking approach when planning for the system tend to have more

projects completed on time as compared to those who adopted the inside or the outside

view.

195
The results from both studies support current planning practice of using work

breakdown structure. A more accurate completion time prediction can be achieved when

the multifaceted tasks are unpacked into subcomponents as the process makes tasks

seem bigger and more time consuming.

There are two important results from completion time predictions that emerge

from Study 1. Comparing these approaches on the same task shows no significant

difference between the outside view and the unpacking approach. Both approaches have

similar effects and reduce the planning fallacy for completion time prediction accuracy.

This indicates that for completion time predictions in project planning applications, a

detailed focus on the project at hand that requires a full enumeration of the component

tasks, may be as accurate as taking an outside approach to completion time prediction.

Second, the inside view, which also focuses on the project at hand, was not

significantly different to the unpacked conditions. This result leads to a question of

whether the difference in wording of instructions has implications for the planning

fallacy. If it does contribute to the effect of increasing or decreasing the planning

fallacy, it may also have an implication for interpreting the mixed results of past

research, and for prescriptions for reducing the planning fallacy in practice. Further

research is needed to examine the possible effects of differences in wording of

instructions on the planning fallacy.

11.3 Extending the Explanations of the Planning Fallacy

11.3.1 Outcome Predictions

In terms of explaining the outcome prediction, the effect of adopting the outside

perspective and unpacking approach has not been examined and compared in a single

study before. Therefore, the hypotheses proposed in Study 1 were parallel with

hypotheses for prediction of completion time. It was proposed that participants in the

outside view condition or the unpacked condition would have more accurate outcome

196
predictions as compared to participants in the inside view condition or the unpacked

(control) condition respectively.

Results from Study 1 indicated that prompting participants to focus on past

relevant experience or to unpack the task tends to increase not reduce the bias of

outcome predictions. However, Study 2 has shown that adopting the unpacking

approach is related to the actual percentage of outcome achieved, and HRIS planners

who unpacked were also more satisfied with their system as compared to HRIS planners

who adopted the inside or the outside view. The results indicate that when predicting for

outcomes of a real project, planning at greater detail by having proper identification of a

project’s activities helps planners to visualize the outcome that they plan to achieve and

thus, more accurate outcome prediction could be generated.

The mixed results of the effect of unpacking on outcome prediction may be

explained by the logic of support theory. Support theory suggests that the influence of

unpacking is related to the number of elements that are unpacked. Though the number

and the size of the constituent elements will increase time estimates, and thus decrease

the planning fallacy, it might also increase the subjective probability that the outcome

will be achieved, and thus increase the outcome prediction bias as shown in Study 1.

But, changing the manner in which the task is described or represented might also direct

people’s attention to important things in the project. The more detailed the description

of the project, the clearer the amount of the effort that needs to be put in to achieve that

outcome.

11.3.2 Confidence in Predictions

In this study, further evidence was also obtained on how much participants

really believe in their predictions towards project completion time and outcome success

by assessing their confidence. In previous research, Buehler, Griffin and Ross (1994)

found that participants were quite confident that they would meet their predictions;

197
confidence in meeting a predicted date and time was on average 74.1% for academic

project. The current study found a similar level of confidence in Study 1, and extends

this study on confidence in predictions by examining the effects of the inside / outside

view and the unpacking approach on the changes of confidence across time.

In Study 1, it was predicted that people who adopt the outside perspective or the

unpacking approach would have smaller changes in their confidence in the prediction

made towards completion time and outcome success as they have more information in

the early stage of the prediction process. However, results from Study 1 failed to

support any of the hypothesized effects of the outside view and unpacking approach on

changes in confidence. Participants showed stable confidence across time regarding

their prediction of completion time, but showed an increasing confidence in predictions

of outcome success.

The results suggest that recalling problems of past projects or alternatively,

unpacking tasks does not produce effects on the change in confidence over time though

the same mechanism reduced the planning fallacy and increased the bias in outcome

predictions. The study shows that participants consistently reported feeling high

confidence in their predictions regardless of being too optimistic in their predictions.

The same participants who confidently believed that they would complete the project

task before the required date and would achieve the grades they predicted, finished later

than expected and failed to get the grades that they expected. This study implies that

prompting people to focus on past experiences and even other people’s experiences or

alternatively, to unpack tasks into their subcomponents may reduce the bias in

prediction, but it may neither reduce nor increase their confidence in those predictions.

11.3.3 Predictions of Affective Reactions towards Outcome Success and Failure

The effects of the outside view and unpacking approach on affective prediction

accuracy are somewhat mixed. Results from Study 1 supported the hypothesized effect

198
of the outside view but not the unpacking approach on prediction of affective reactions

towards outcome success. Participants who were prompted to focus on relevant past

experiences predicted less positive affective reactions towards outcome success than

those prompted to focus on the inside view future scenario for the project at hand. This

finding supports previous research of the inside / outside views approach on affective

prediction, which found that participants who focused on the event itself and neglected

past experiences, anticipated stronger feelings than those who focused on relevant past

experiences (Buehler & McFarland, 2001).

Comparing the outside view and unpacking approach on the affective

predictions in Study 1 indicated that adopting the outside view generated more accurate

predictions than adopting unpacking. That is, participants prompted to focus on relevant

past experience were found to be more accurate in their prediction of affective reactions

towards outcome success than participants prompted to list down components of the

projects and steps needed to complete the project.

The results of Study 2 however, show the opposite. Though the HRIS planners

overestimated the emotions they would experience following the outcome of project

success just as the participants did in Study 1, the impact bias for positive affective

reactions following the successful HRIS project was reduced in association with the

extent of use of the unpacking approach, suggesting that describing the projects in

greater detail did reduce the impact bias.

As for predictions of negative affect after failure, Study 1 failed to support any

of the hypothesized effects of the outside view and unpacking approaches on predictions

of negative affect after failure.

When one considers findings of both studies, the results show that using either

prescription for reducing the planning fallacy may reduce the impact bias, at least for

positive affective reactions following the outcome success.

199
11.3.4 Further Theoretical Development

Results from this study show that the outside view and unpacking approaches

have similar effects on the planning fallacy. This leads to the question of whether the

approaches have different underlying mechanisms that can lead to smilar outcomes or

they actually have a same underlying mechanism. This thesis does not explore this

question, it remains for future research.

11.4 Implications for Practice

The current research findings have several implications for project managers.

The research results demonstrate that better predictions of completion time can be

achieved when each of the subcomponents of the project tasks are considered. Before

making predictions, project managers are encouraged to unpack project tasks into their

subcomponents and to list all the possible steps that are needed to accomplish the

project. Better predictions can be achieved when planners detail out tasks to be carried

out as detailed description may remind people of constituent elements they would not

have otherwise considered (Van Boven & Epley, 2003), and make it easier for

individuals to mentally simulate what an event will be like (Kahneman & Tversky,

1982).

This finding supports utilizing the work breakdown structure method when

planning for projects (Cleland & Ireland, 2002; R. Jones, 2007; Maylor, 2005).

However, it is important to note the difference between unpacking and decomposition in

terms of their operationalization, underlying theory and predictions. According to

Kruger and Evans (2004), decomposition involves actual breaking down a category

with separate estimates assigned to each of the components of the category, before it is

aggregated arithmetically. Unpacking involves breaking down a category symbolically

to change the way the task is described or represented. Unlike decomposition, which

involves combination of multiple judgments, unpacking only involves a single

200
prediction judgment. Decomposition infers that breaking down a category into simple

components ought to reduce bias because it should be easier to make accurate

predictions for smaller, simpler components than for a large, complex aggregate. But,

unpacking infers only an increase in overall estimates, which only translate into reduced

bias if there is an underestimation bias to start with.

Though there is a difference between the decomposition and unpacking, they

might be used together when planning for a project. It has been shown that prediction

problems among IS projects continues to occur even when various methods and

techniques including the workbreakdown structure are utilized. The findings of this

study show that the unpacking approach helps in improving completion time and

outcome predictions, which project managers could consider when utilizing the work

breakdown structure.

However, certain predictions may not be improved through adopting either the

outside view approach or the unpacking approach. It is discovered through this study

that prescriptions for reducing the planning fallacy have opposite effects on predictions

of project completion time and of project outcome success. This applies to both of the

existing methods for reducing the planning fallacy, the outside view approach and the

enumerating or unpacking approach. A message for managers is to beware of the

potential for project outcomes to fall short of expectations on outcome success measures

when these procedures to reduce the planning fallacy are used to reduce prediction bias

for completion times.

The study also demonstrated that people overestimate the emotions they would

experience following the outcome of project success. This might explain why people are

not as happy as they predicted they would be once the project outcome was delivered.

Though the potential impact of emotional events a around future project outcome may

serve a motivating function, which could make project managers work hard to obtain

201
success and to avoid failure, it may also create unnecessary anxiety about future

performance especially when overestimating the negative impact of an unsuccessful

project outcome. Again, the two existing methods for reducing the planning fallacy

have mixed effects on reducing impact bias. The results from Study 2 showed that if

project managers are to reduce the impact bias for positive affective reactions following

a successful HRIS project, describing the project in greater detail give greater benefits

than using past relevant experiences. However, no results can be provided whether

using the same approach will reduce impact bias for negative reactions following the

unsuccessful HRIS project.

In summary, the prescriptions discussed above are suggestive of the types of

actions that project managers can take, to help minimize the risk of underestimating

project completion time and overestimating project outcome success. It is hoped that

results from both studies will encourage new thinking not just among IS project

managers, but also other project managers. The research results reported in this study

suggest the need for a view of project management which includes a psychological and

behavioral perspective. Most studies in the past on IS problems have focused on

specific reasons for failure and suggested specific remedies such as risk management

techniques, user participation, and tools to represent system abstraction. But, project

success often depends on the validity and accuracy of project decisions made during

planning. Human judgment and decision-making is susceptible to various biases that

may influence the accuracy of the predictions made (Buehler et al., 1997; Buehler et al.,

1994; Byram, 1997; Newby-Clark et al., 2000). Practices designed to neutralize these

biases could generate significant benefits for organizations.

11.5 Limitations and Directions for Future Research

On the basis of current findings, several interesting questions remain. In

particular, the question arises of why these methods reduce optimistic bias for

202
completion time predictions, but exacerbate it for outcome success predictions. Are

there other types of predictions that fall between these? What is the underlying

mechanism, or are there multiple mechanisms? The outside view and the unpacking

approaches had some similar results in research, suggesting that they share an

explanatory mechanism, but they are based on very different arguments. The outside

view approach is based on enhancing the salience of base rate information to improve

prediction accuracy. The unpacking approach is based on exploiting people’s failure to

make the same judgments about a collection of subcomponents as they make about the

super category comprised of those components. Future research is needed either to

reconcile these explanations, or to show that there are conditions under which the

outside view and unpacking approaches do not lead to similar results.

Apart from that, this study used group projects, but the prediction was made

individually. In the first study, participants do interact with their group members during

group meetings, their lecturer, their seniors and other friends from other groups in the

process of completing their project assignments. But, I did not further examine the

effect of interaction with others and the adoption of the outside and unpacking approach

in predicting task completion time and outcome, as the main focus of study was more

on testing and comparing the two conflicting theories of the planning fallacy and

extending the explanation of the planning fallacy on the outcome prediction. This

provides another direction for future research.

There is also a need for future research to extend the exploration of the influence

of the outside view, inside view and unpacking approaches on planning and decision

making on other applications of project management apart from HRIS projects. Since

the study only compares the effects and tests whether there is a difference between the

two approaches, without taking into account the nature of the project involved, future

research should test these two approaches in various contexts to gain their effectiveness.

203
This study was not designed to examine the context in which the outside view approach

may be a more suitable method than the unpacking approach and vice versa in

predicting completion time, outcome, and affective reactions towards outcome success

and failure.

Though the first study is a longitudinal study, the second was cross-sectional as

it was not practical to a conduct a longitudinal study. A cross-sectional design is simple,

inexpensive, and allows for the collection of data in a relatively short period. Although

there are advantages to using a cross-sectional design, this method offers limited

information regarding how the whole planning process take place, starting from its

inception to the implementation stage. Perhaps, in the future, it may be worth

investigating the planning approach used when planning a project using a longitudinal

study.

11.6 Conclusion

The aim of this thesis was to investigate how to improve the accuracy of

predictions made during the planning stages of project management. The main concern

of this thesis is the role of human judgment in making accurate project planning

predictions, with particular applications to human resource information systems (HRIS)

projects. This thesis provides the first attempt to test the two compelling but conflicting

theories of the planning fallacy, the inside / outside view and the unpacking approach

against each other in a single study, and to demonstrate the differences and similarities

between these two approaches. The results indicate that the outside view and the

unpacking approach do affect the predictions made regarding the completion of a

project where the planning fallacy is reduced. When examining these two methods in a

real project environment of HRIS, it seems that the extent to which the unpacking

approach is used may be related to finishing on time and being satisfied with the

outcome, as well as the actual success of the project.

204
An important contribution made by this thesis is the comparison of the planning

fallacy theories for predictions other than time to completion of a project, specifically

prediction of project outcome, confidence in prediction of project completion time and

outcome, and predictions of affective reactions to project outcome success and failure.

Both approaches show opposite effects on predictions of completion time and outcome

success, but have no effect on changes in confidence in predictions of completion time

and outcome success. Testing these two methods in real projects involving HRIS,

suggests that the unpacking approach rather than the outside view is more effective in

predicting project outcome success and satisfaction, and in reducing the impact bias for

positive affective reactions following the success of an HRIS project.

It is hoped that through the examination of both the outside view and the

unpacking approach in reducing biases in prediction during project planning, a more

complete understanding of the effectiveness of these two methods will be achieved.

205
REFERENCES

Abdinnour-Helm, S. F., Chaparro, B. S., & Farmer, S. M. (2005). Using the end-user
computing satisfaction (EUCS) instrument to measure satisfaction with a web
site. Decision Sciences, 36(2), 341-364.

Aibinu, A. A., & Jagboro, G. O. (2002). The effects of construction delays on project
delivery in Nigerian construction industry. International Journal of Project
Management, 20, 593-599.

Aladwani, A. M. (2002). IT project uncertainty, planning and success: An empirical


investigation from Kuwait. Information Technology & People, 15(3), 210-226.

Baillie, J. (1996, August 29). Attracting employees who surf the internet. People
Management, 2, 46-47.

Ball, K. S. (2001). The use of human resource information systems: A survey.


Personnel Review, 30(6), 677-693.

Bassett, P., Campbell, G., & Licciardi, R. (2003). Tunnel vision: Limited use of human
resource information systems (HRIS): School of Management, Victoria
University of Technology.

Berry, W. E. (1993). HRIS can improve performance, empower and motivate


knowledge workers. Employment Relations Today, 297-303.

Bondaruk, T. V., & Ruel, H. J. M. (2008). HRM systems for successful information
technology implementation: Evidence from three case studies. European
Management Journal, 26, 153-165.

Broderick, R., & Boudreau, J. W. (1992). Human resource management information


technology and the competitive edge. Academy of Management Executive, 6(2),
7-17.

Brown, A. D., & Jones, M. R. (1998). Doomed to failure: Narratives of inevitability and
conspiracy in a failed IS project. Organization Studies, 19(1), 73-88.

Buckley, P., Minette, K., Joy, D., & Michaels, J. (2004). The use of an automated
employment recruiting and screening system for temporary professional
employees: A case study. Human Resource Management, 43(2 & 3), 233-241.

206
Buehler, R., & Griffin, D. (2003). Planning, personality, and prediction: The role of
future focus in optimistic time predictions. Organizational Behavior And Human
Decision Processes, 92(1-2), 80-90.

Buehler, R., Griffin, D., & MacDonald, H. (1997). The role of motivated reasoning in
optimistic time predictions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23(3),
238-247.

Buehler, R., Griffin, D., & Ross, M. (1994). Exploring the "planning fallacy": Why
people underestimate their task completion times. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 67(3), 366-381.

Buehler, R., Griffin, D., & Ross, M. (2002). Inside the planning fallacy: The causes and
consequences of optimistic time predictions. In T. Gilovich, D. Griffin & D.
Kahneman (Eds.), Heuristics and biases: The Psychology of Intuitive Judgment
(pp. 250-270). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Buehler, R., & McFarland, C. (2001). Intensity bias in affective forecasting: The role of
temporal focus. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27(11), 1480-1493.

Buehler, R., McFarland, C., Spyropoulos, V., & Lam, K. C. H. (2007). Motivated
prediction of future feelings: Effects of negative mood and mood orientation on
affective forecasts. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33(9), 1265-
1278.

Bukszar, E. (1988). Hindsight bias and strategic choice: Some problems in learning
from experience. Academy of Management Journal, 31(3), 628-641.

Burger, J. M., & Burns, L. (1988). The illusion of unique invulnerability and the use of
effective contraception. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 14(2), 264-
270.

Bussler, L., & Davis, E. (2001-2002). Information systems: The quiet revolution in
human resource management. Journal of Computer Information Systems, 17-20.

Byram, S. J. (1997). Cognitive and motivational factors influencing time prediction.


Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 3(3), 216-239.

Calisir, F., & Gumussoy, C. A. (2005). Determinants of budget overruns on IT projects.


Technovation, 25, 631-636.

Camerer, C., & Lovallo, D. (1999). Overconfidence and excess entry: An experimental
approach. The American Economic Review, 89(1), 306-318.

207
Caplan, J. (2004, March). eHR in Greater China: The future of HR takes flight. China
Staff, 10, 1-7.

Cappelli, P. (2001). Making the most of on-line recruiting. Harvard Business Review,
79, 139-146.

Cardy, R. L., & Miller, J. S. (2003). Technology implications for HRM. In D. L. Stone
(Ed.), Advances in human performance and cognitive engineering research (pp.
99-118). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

Cardy, R. L., & Miller, J. S. (2005). eHR and performance management: A


consideration of positive potential and the dark side. In H. G. Gueutal & D. L.
Stone (Eds.), The brave new world of eHR: Human resources management in
the digital age (pp. 138-165). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Cats-Baril, W., & Thompson, R. (1995). Managing information technology projects in


the public sector. Public Administration Review, 55(6), 559-566.

Ceriello, V. R., & Freeman, C. (1991). Human resource management systems:


Strategies, tactics and techniques. New York: Lexington Books.

Chapman, D. S., & Webster, J. (2003). The use of technologies in the recruiting,
screening, and selection processes for job candidates. International Journal of
Selection and Assessment, 11(2/3), 113-120.

Cleland, D. I., & Ireland, L. R. (2002). Project management: Strategic design and
implementation (4th ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.

Cober, R. T., Brown, D. J., Levy, P. E., Cober, A. B., & Keeping, L. M. (2003).
Organizational web sites: Web site content and style as determinants of
organizational attraction. International Journal of Selection and Assessment,
11(2/3), 158-169.

Coppola, N. W., & Myre, R. (2002). Corporate software training: Is web-based training
as effective as instructor-led training? IEEE Transactions on Professional
Communication, 45(3), 170-186.

DeLone, W. H., & McLean, E. R. (1992). Information systems success: The quest for
the dependent variable. Information Systems Research, 3(1), 60-95.

Doll, W. J., Deng, X., Raghunathan, T. S., Torkzadeh, G., & Xia, W. (2004). The
meaning and measurement of user satisfaction: A multigroup invariance analysis
of the end-user computing satisfaction instrument. Journal of Management
Information Systems, 21(1), 227-262.

208
Doll, W. J., & Torkzadeh, G. (1988). The measurement of end-user computing
satisfaction. MIS Quarterly, 12(2), 258-274.

Doll, W. J., Xia, W., & Torkzadeh, G. (1994). A confirmatory factor analysis of the
end-user computing satisfaction instrument. MIS Quarterly, 18(4), 453-461.

Dunning, D. (2007). Prediction: The inside view. In A. W. Kruglanski & E. T. Higgins


(Eds.), Social Psychology: Handbook of basic principles (2nd ed., pp. 69-90).
New York: The Guilford Press.

Dvir, D., Raz, T., & Shenhar, A. J. (2003). An empirical analysis of the relationship
between project planning and project success. International Journal of Project
Management, 21(2), 89-95.

Enders, C. K., & Tofighi, D. (2007). Centering predictor variables in cross-sectional


multilevel models: A new look at an old issue. Psychological Methods, 12(2),
121-138.

EPU. (2006). Ninth Malaysia Plan. Retrieved 28th August 2008. from
http://www.epu.jpm.my/RM9/html/english.htm.

Ewusi-Mensah, K. (1997). Critical issues in abandoned information systems


development projects. Communications of the ACM, 40(9), 74-80.

Ewusi-Mensah, K., & Przasnyski, Z. H. (1991). On Information systems project


abandonment: An exploratory study of organizational practices. MIS Quarterly,
15(1), 67-86.

Ewusi-Mensah, K., & Przasnyski, Z. H. (1994). Factors contributing to the


abandonment of information systems development projects. Journal of
Information Technology, 9, 185-201.

Federation of Malaysian Manufacturers. (2007). About FMM. Retrieved 5th April,


2007, from http://www.fmm.org.my/

Finkenauer, C., Gallucci, M., Dijk, W. W. v., & Pollmann, M. (2007). Investigating the
role of time in affective forecasting: Temporal influences on forecasting
accuracy. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33(8), 1152-1166.

Flowers, S. (1996). Software failure: Management failure: Amazing stories and


cautionary tales. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons.

209
Galanaki, E. (2002). The decision to recruit online: A descriptive study. Career
Development International, 7(4), 243-251.

Gardner, S. D., Lepak, D. P., & Bartol, K. M. (2003). Virtual HR: The impact of
information technology on the human resource professional. Journal of
Vocational Behavior, 63, 159-179.

Gemino, A., Reich, B. H., & Sauer, C. (2007). A temporal model of information
technology project performance. Journal of Management of Information
Systems, 24(3), 9-44.

Gilbert, D. T., Pinel, E. C., Wilson, T. D., Blumberg, S. J., & Wheatley, T. P. (1998).
Immune neglect: A source of durability bias in affective forecasting. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 75(3), 617-638.

Gilbert, D. T., & Wilson, T. D. (2000). Miswanting. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), Thinking and
feeling: The role of affect in social cognition (pp. 178-197). Cambridge,
England: Cambridge Press University.

Gilovich, T., Kerr, M., & Mecdvec, V. H. (1993). Effect of temporal perspective on
subjective confidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64(4),
552-560.

Greengard, S. (1998, August). Putting online recruiting to work. Workforce, 77, 73-77.

Griffin, D., & Buehler, R. (2005). Biases and fallacies, memories and predictions:
Comment on Roy, Christenfeld, and McKenzie (2005). Psychological Bulletin,
131(5), 757-760.

Haines, V. Y., & Petit, A. (1997). Conditions for successful human resource
information systems. Human Resource Management, 36(2), 261-275.

Hall, P. (1980). Great planning disasters. London: Weidenfeld & Nicolson.

Hardie, N. (2001). The prediction and control of project duration: A recursive model.
International Journal of Project Management, 19, 401-409.

Hartman, F., & Ashrafi, R. A. (2002). Project management in the information systems
and information technologies industries. Project Management Journal, 33(3), 5-
15.

210
Hausdorf, P. A., & Duncan, D. (2004). Firm size and Internet recruiting in Canada: A
preliminary investigation. Journal of Small Business Management, 42(3), 325-
334.

Heath, C., & Gonzalez, R. (1995). Interaction with others increases decision confidence
but not decision quality: Evidence against information collection views of
interactive decision making. Organizational Behavior And Human Decision
Processes, 61(3), 305-326.

Hedeker, D., & Gibbons, R. D. (1997). Application of random-effects pattern-mixture


models for missing data in longitudinal studies. Psychological Methods, 2(1),
64-78.

Hendrickson, A. R. (2003). Human resource information systems: Backbone technology


of contemporary human resources. Journal of Labor Research, 24(3), 381-395.

Hendrickson, A. R., Glorfeld, K., & Cronan, T. P. (1994). On the repeated test-retest
reliability of the end-user computing satisfaction instrument: A comment.
Decision Sciences, 25(4), 655-667.

Hill, J., Thomas, L. C., & Allen, D. E. (2000). Experts' estimates of task durations in
software development projects. International Journal of Project Management,
18, 13-21.

Hofman, D. A., & Gavin, M. B. (1998). Centering decisions in hierarchical linear


models: Implications for research in organizations. Journal of Management,
24(5), 623-641.

Hox, J. J. (2002). Multilevel analysis: Techniques and applications. New Jersey:


Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Publishers.

Hughes, R. T. (1996). Expert judgment as an estimating method. Information and


Software Technology, 38, 67-75.

Huo, Y. P., & Kearns, J. (1992). Optimizing the job-person match with computerized
human resource information system. Personnel Review, 21(2), 3-18.

Hussain, Z., Wallace, J., & Cornelius, N. E. (2007). The use and impact of human
resource information systems on human resource management professionals.
Information & Management, 44, 74-89.

Hyvari, I. (2006). Success of projects in different organizational conditions. Project


Management Journal, 37(4), 31-41.

211
Ives, B., & Olson, M. H. (1984). User involvement and MIS success: A review of
research. Management Science, 30(5), 586-603.

Jattuso, M. L., & Sinar, E. F. (2003). Source effects in Internet-based screening


procedures. International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 11(2/3), 137-
140.

Jiang, J. J., & Klein, G. (1999). Risks to different aspects of system success.
Information & Management, 36, 263-272.

Jones, M. C., & Harrison, A. W. (1996). IS project team performance: An empirical


assessment. Information and Management, 31(2), 57-65.

Jones, R. (2007). Project management survival: A practical guide to leading, managing


& delivering challenging projects. London: Kogan Page Limited.

Kahneman, D., & Lovallo, D. (1993). Timid choice and bold forecasts: A cognitive
perspective on risk taking. Management Science, 39(1), 17-31.

Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1972). Subjective probability: A judgment of


representative. Cognitive Psychology, 3, 430-454.

Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1982). The simulation heuristic. In D. Kahneman, P.


Slovic & A. Tversky (Eds.), Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases
(pp. 201-208). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Katagiri, M., & Turner, R. (Eds.). (2004). Goal directed project management: Effective
techniques and strategies (3rd ed.). London: Kogan Page.

Kavanagh, M. J., Gueutal, H. G., & Tannenbaum, S. I. (1990). Human resource


information systems: Development and application. USA: PWS-Kent.

Kehoe, J. F., Dickter, D. N., Russell, D. P., & Sacco, J. M. (2005). e-Selection. In H. G.
Gueutal & D. L. Stone (Eds.), The brave new world of eHR. San Francissco,
CA: Jossey-Bass.

Keil, M., Mann, J., & Rai, A. (2000). Why software projects escalate: An empirical
analysis and test of four theoretical models. MIS Quarterly, 24(4), 631-664.

Kinnie, N. J., & Arthurs, A. J. (1996). Personnel specialists' advanced use of


information technology: Evidence and explanations. Personnel Review, 25(3), 3-
19.

212
Kotylar, I., & Saks, A. M. (2001, October 22). Using technology for knowledge and
skill transfer. Canadian HR Reporter, 14, G9.

Kovach, K. A., & Cathcart, C. E. J. (1999). Human resource information systems


(HRIS): Providing business with rapid data access, information exchange and
strategic advantage. Public Personnel Management, 28(2), 275-282.

Kreft, I. G. G., & de Leeuw, J. (1998). Introducing multilevel modeling. London: Sage
Publications.

Kreft, I. G. G., de Leeuw, J., & Aiken, L. S. (1995). The effect of different forms of
centering in hierarchical linear models. Multivariate Behavioral Research,
30(1), 1-21.

Kruger, J., & Evans, M. (2004). If you don't want to be late, enumerate: Unpacking
reduces the planning fallacy. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40(5),
586-598.

Krull, J. L., & MacKinnon, D. P. (2001). Multilevel modeling of individual and group
level mediated effects. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 36(2), 249-277.

Lam, K. C. H., McFarland, C., Ross, M., & Cheung, I. (2005). Cultural differences in
affective forecasting: The role of focalism. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 31(9), 1296-1309.

Langer, E. J. (1975). The illusion of control. Journal of Personality and Social


Psychology, 32(2), 311-328.

Lee, C. (1989). Perceptions of immunity to disease in adult smokers. Journal of


Behavioral Medicine, 12(3), 267-277.

Lengnick-Hall, M. L., & Moritz, S. (2003). The impact of e-HR on the human resource
management function. Journal of Labor Research, 24(3), 365-380.

Leybourne, S., & Sadler-Smith, E. (2006). The role of intuition and improvisation in
project management. International Journal of Project Management, 24, 483-
492.

Liberman, N., & Trope, Y. (1998). The role of feasibility and desirability considerations
in near and distant future decisions: A test of temporal construal theory. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(1), 5-18.

213
Lientz, B. P., & Rea, K. P. (1998). Project management for the 21st century. San Diego:
Academic Press.

Lin, B., & Stasinskaya, V. S. (2002). Data warehousing management issues in online
recruiting. Human Systems Management, 21(1), 1-8.

Lock, D. (2003). Project management (8th ed.). Hampshire, England: Gower Publishing
Limited.

Lovallo, D., & Kahneman, D. (2003). Delusions of success: How optimism undermines
executives' decisions. Harvard Business Review, 81(7), 56-63.

Lyytinen, K., & Hirschheim, R. (1987). Information system failures: A survey and
classification of the empirical literature. In P. Zorkoczy (Ed.), Oxford surveys in
information technology (pp. 257-309). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Macy, J. (2004, September). Data disarray. HRMonthly, 36-38.

Makridakis, S., & Wheelwright, S. C. (1989). Forecasting methods for management


(5th ed.). NewYork: John Wiley & Sons.

Manavazhi, M. R., & Adhikari, D. K. (2002). Material and equipment procurement


delays in highway projects in Nepal. International Journal of Project
Management, 20, 627-632.

Martinsons, M. G. (1997). Human resource management applications of knowledge-


based systems. International Journal of Information Management, 17(1), 35-53.

Maylor, H. (2005). Project management (3rd ed.). England: Pearson Education Limited.

McCray, G. E., Purvis, R. L., & McCray, C. G. (2002). Project management under
uncertainty: The impact of heuristics and biases. Project Management Journal,
33(1), 49-57.

McFarlan, F. W. (1981). Portfolio approach to information systems. Harvard Business


Review, 59(5), 142-150.

McHaney, R., & Cronan, T. P. (1998). Computer simulation success: On the use of the
end-user computing satisfaction instrument: A comment. Decision Sciences,
29(2), 525-536.

214
McHaney, R., Hightower, R., & Pearson, J. (2002). A validation of the end-user
computing satisfaction instrument in Taiwan. Information & Management,
39(6), 503-511.

McHaney, R., Hightower, R., & White, D. (1999). EUCS test-retest reliability in
representational model decision support systems. Information & Management,
36(2), 109-119.

McKenna, F. P., Warburton, D. M., & Winwood, M. (1993). Exploring the limits of
optimism: The case of smokers' decision making. British Journal of Psychology,
84, 389-394.

McManus, M. A., & Ferguson, M. W. (2003). Biodata, personality, and demographic


differences of recruits from three sources. International Journal of Selection and
Assessment, 11(2/3), 175-183.

Meredith, J. R., & Mantel, S. M., Jr. (2006). Project management: A managerial
approach. Hoboken, New Jersey: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.

Morewedge, C. L., Gilbert, D. T., & Wilson, T. D. (2005). The least likely of times:
How remembering the past biases forecasts of the future. Psychological Science:
Research, Theory, & Application in Psychology and Related Sciences, 16(8),
626-630.

Morgenshtern, O., Raz, T., & Dvir, D. (2007). Factors affecting duration and effort
estimation errors in software development projects. Information and Software
Technology, 49, 827-837.

Murray, J. P. (2001). Recognizing the responsibility of a failed information technology


project as a shared failure. Information Systems Management, 18, 25-30.

Newby-Clark, I. R., & Ross, M. (2003). Conceiving the past and future. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 29(7), 807-818.

Newby-Clark, I. R., Ross, M., Buehler, R., Koehler, D. J., & Griffin, D. (2000). People
focus on optimistic scenarios and disregard pessimistic scenarios while
predicting task completion times. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied,
6(3), 171-182.

Ngai, E. W. T., & Wat, F. K. T. (2006). Human resource information systems: A review
and empirical analysis. Personnel Review, 35(3), 297-314.

Nink, C., & Chieke, R. K. (2004, August). Cyberspace recruiting: Useful strategies to
improve success. Corrections Today, 66, 104-108.

215
NSDC. (2005). Definitions for small and medium enterprises in Malaysia. Retrieved
3rd July, 2007, from
http://www.smeinfo.com.my/pdf/sme_definitions_ENGLISH.pdf

Ogier, J. (2003, September). Are you the tech wreck? The trouble with HRIS.
HRMonthly, 19-24.

Otto, J. R., Najdawi, M. K., & Caron, K. M. (2000). Web-user satisfaction: An


exploratory study. Journal of End User Computing, 12(4), 3-10.

Parry, E., & Tyson, S. (2008). An analysis of the use and success of online recruitment
methods in the UK. Human Resource Management Journal, 18(3), 257-274.

Pearson, G. (2001, October 22). HRMS does make a difference. Canadian HR


Reporter, 14, G1-2.

Perloff, L. S., & Fetzer, B. K. (1986). Self-other judgments and perceived vulnerability
to victimization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50(3), 502-510.

Perminova, O., Gustafsson, M., & Wikstrom, K. (2008). Defining uncertainty in


projects - a new perspective. International Journal of Project Management, 26,
73-79.

Peterson, D. K., & Kim, C. (2003). Perceptions on IS risks and failure types: A
comparison of designers from the United States, Japan and Korea. Journal of
Global Information Management, 11(3), 19-38.

Peugh, J. L., & Enders, C. K. (2005). Using the SPSS mixed procedure to fit cross-
sectional and longitudinal multilevel models. Educational and Psychological
Measurement, 65(5), 717-741.

Pinto, J. K., & Mantel, S. I., Jr. (1990). The causes of project failure. IEEE Transactions
on Engineering Management, 37(4).

Pinto, J. K., & Prescott, J. E. (1990). Planning and tactical factors in the project
implementation process. Journal of Management Studies, 27(3), 305-327.

Purvis, R. L., McCray, G. E., & Roberts, T. L. (2004). Heuristics and biases in
information systems project management. Engineering Management Journal,
16(2), 19-27.

216
Rai, A., & Al-Hindi, H. (2000). The effects of development process modeling and task
uncertainty on development quality performance. Information and Management,
37(6), 335-346.

Raudenbush, S. W., & Bryk, A. S. (2002). Hierarchical linear models: Applications


and data analysis methods (2nd ed.). Thousand Oak, CA: Sage Publications.

Raudenbush, S. W., Bryk, A. S., Cheong, Y. F., Congdon, R., & du Toit, M. (2004).
HLM6: Hierarchical linear and nonlinear modeling. Lincolnwood, IL:
Scientific Software International, Inc.

Raymond, L., & Bergeron, F. (2008). Project management information systems: An


empirical study of their impact on project managers and project success.
International Journal of Project Management, 26(2), 213-220.

Robey, D., Smith, L. A., & Vijayasarathy, L. R. (1993). Perceptions of conflict and
success in information systems development projects. Journal of Management
Information Systems, 10(1), 123-139.

Robinson, M. D., & Clore, G. L. (2002). Belief and feeling: Evidence for an
accessibility model of emotional self-report. Psychological Bulletin, 128(6),
934-960.

Romm, C. T., Pliskin, N., & Weber, Y. (1995). The relevance of organizational culture
to the implementation of human resources information systems. Asia Pacific
Journal of Human Resources, 33(2), 63-80.

Rosenau, M. D., Jr. (1998). Successful project management: Step-by-step approach with
practical examples (3rd ed.). New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.

Rottenstreich, Y., & Tversky, A. (1997). Unpacking, repacking and anchoring:


Advances in support theory. Psychological Review, 104(2), 406-415.

Roy, M. M., Christenfeld, N. J. S., & McKenzie, C. R. M. (2005). Underestimating the


duration of future events: Memory incorrectly used or memory bias?
Psychological Bulletin, 131(5), 738-756.

Ruel, H. J. M., Bondarouk, T. V., & Looise, J. K. (2004). E-HRM: Innovation or


irritation. An explorative empirical study in five large companies on web-based
HRM. Management Revue, 15(3), 364-380.

Russell, T. (2006, June 27). HR's rage against the machine. Human Resources, 16-17.

217
Russo, J. E., & Schoemaker, P. J. H. (1992). Managing overconfidence. MIT Sloan
Management Review, 33(2), 7-19.

Sadri, J., & Chatterjee, V. (2003). Building organisational character through HRIS.
International Journal of Human Resources Development and Management, 3(1),
84-98.

Sambasivan, M., & Soon, Y. W. (2007). Causes and effects of delays in Malaysian
construction industry. International Journal of Project Management, 25(5), 437-
542.

Sanna, L. J. (1999). Mental simulations, affect, and subjective confidence: Timing is


everything. Psychological Science, 10(4), 339-345.

Sanna, L. J., & Schwarz, N. (2004). Integrating temporal biases: The interplay of focal
thoughts and accessibility experiences. Psychological Science, 15(7), 474-481.

Schkade, D. A., & Kahneman, D. (1998). Does living in California make people happy?
A focusing illusion in judgment of life satisfaction. Psychological Science, 9(5),
340-346.

Schnaars, S. P. (1989). Megamistakes: Forecasting and the myth of rapid technological


change. New York: Free Press.

Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1983). Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being:
Informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 45(3), 513-523.

Shand, D. (2000, April 17). The passive/aggressive job seeker. Computerworld, 34, 50-
51.

Shepperd, J. A., Findley-Klein, C., Kwavnick, K. D., Walker, D., & Perez, S. (2000).
Bracing for loss. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(4), 620-634.

Shepperd, J. A., Grace, J., Cole, L. J., & Klein, C. (2005). Anxiety and outcome
predictions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31(2), 267-275.

Singer, J. D., & Willet, J. B. (2003). Applied longitudinal data analysis: Modeling
change and event occurrence. New York: Oxford University Press, Inc.

Smith, C. (2007). Making sense of project realities: Theory, practice and the pursuit of
performance. England: Gower Publishing Company.

218
Stone, D. L., Stone-Romero, E. F., & Lukaszewski, K. (2006). Factors affecting the
acceptance and effectiveness of electronic human resource systems. Human
Resource Management Review, 16, 229-244.

Strohmeier, S. (2007). Research in e-HRM: Review and implications. Human Resource


Management Review, 17, 19-37.

Tannenbaum, S. I. (1990). Human resource information systems: User group


implications. Journal of Systems Management, 41(1), 27-32.

Tansley, C., & Newell, S. (2007). Project social capital, leadership and trust: A study of
human resource information system development. Journal of Managerial
Psychology, 22(4), 350-368.

Targowski, A. S., & Deshpande, S. P. (2001). The utility and selection of an HRIS.
Advances in Competitiveness Research (ACR), 9(1), 42-56.

Teo, T. S. H., Lim, G. S., & Fedric, S. A. (2007). The adoption and diffusion of human
resources information systems in Singapore. Asia Pacific Journal of Human
Resources, 45(1), 44-62.

Thaler-Carter, R. E. (1998a, July). The HRIS in small companies: Tips for weighing the
options. HRMagazine, 43, 30-35.

Thaler-Carter, R. E. (1998b, November). Recruiting through the web: Better or just


bigger? HRMagazine, 43, 61-67.

Thomas, K. E., & Handley, S. J. (2008). Anchoring in time estimation. Acta


Psychologica, 127(1), 24-29.

Thomas, K. E., Newstead, S. E., & Handley, S. J. (2003). Exploring the time prediction
process: The effects of task experience and complexity on prediction accuracy.
Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17, 655-673.

Thong, J. Y. L. (1999). An integrated model of information systems adoption in small


businesses. Journal of Management Information Systems, 15(4), 187-214.

Torkzadeh, G., & Doll, W. J. (1991). Test-retest reliability of the end-user computing
satisfaction instrument. Decision Sciences, 22, 26-37.

Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1973). Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency
and probability. Cognitive Psychology, 5(2), 207-232.

219
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and
biases. Science, 185(4157), 1124-1131.

Tversky, A., & Koehler, D. J. (1994). Support theory: A nonextensional representation


of subjective probability. Psychological Review, 101, 547-567.

Van Boven, L., & Epley, N. (2003). The unpacking effect in evaluative judgments:
When the whole is less than the sum of its parts. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 39(3), 263-269.

Van Dijk, W. W. (2009). How do you feel? Affective forecasting and the impact bias in
track athletics. The journal of Social Psychology, 149(3), 243-248.

Wateridge, J. (1995). IT projects: A basis for success. International Journal of Project


Management, 13(3), 169-172.

Wateridge, J. (1998). How can IS/IT projects be measured for success? International
Journal of Project Management, 16(1), 59-63.

Weinstein, N. D. (1980). Unrealistic optimism about future life events. Journal of


Personality and Social Psychology, 39(5), 806-820.

Weinstein, N. D. (1982). Unrealistic optimism about susceptibility to health problems.


Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 5(4), 441-460.

Weinstein, N. D. (1987). Unrealistic optimism about susceptibility to health problems:


Conclusions from a community-wide sample. Journal of Behavioral Medicine,
10(5), 481-500.

Welsh, E. T., Wanberg, C. R., Brown, K. G., & Simmering, M. J. (2003). E-learning:
emerging uses, empirical results and future directions. International Journal of
Training and Development, 7(4), 245-258.

White, D., & Fortune, J. (2002). Current practice in project management - an empirical
study. International Journal of Project Management, 20(1), 1-11.

Whitley, B. E., Jr., & Hern, A. L. (1991). Perceptions of vulnerability to pregnancy and
the use of effective contraception. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
17(1), 104-110.

Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2003). Affective forecasting. In M. Zanna (Ed.),


Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 35, pp. 345-407). New York:
Academic Press.

220
Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2005). Affective forecasting: Knowing what to want.
Current Direction in Psychological Science, 14(3), 131-134.

Wilson, T. D., Meyers, J. M., & Gilbert, D. T. (2001). Lesson from the past: Do people
learn from experience that emotional reactions are short-lived? Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 27(12), 1648-1661.

Wilson, T. D., Wheatley, T., Meyers, J. M., Gilbert, D. T., & Axsom, D. (2000).
Focalism: A source of durability bias in affective forecasting. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 78(5), 821-836.

Yeates, D. (Ed.). (1991). Project management for information systems. London: Pitman
Publishing.

Yeo, K. T. (2002). Critical failure factors in information system projects. International


Journal of Project Management, 20, 241-246.

Zusman, R. R., & Landis, R. S. (2002). Applicant preferences for web-based versus
traditional job postings. Computers in Human Behavior, 18(3), 285-296.

Zwikael, O., & Sadeh, A. (2007). Planning effort as an effective risk management tool.
Journal of Operation Management, 25, 755-767.

221
APPENDICES

222
APPENDIX A

SAMPLE OF THE EXPERIMENTAL MATERIALS (ENGLISH AND


TRANSLATED VERSION) – STUDY 1

This appendix contains copies of the experimental materials provided to respondents,

namely the information sheet; consent form; cover letter; experimental questionnaire for

one of the four conditions: inside perspective, outside perspective, unpacked and packed

(control); a weekly diary entry; and a short affective questionnaire.

223
School of Economics and Commerce
Dr. Catherine D. Lees
Mail Bag 261
35 Stirling Highway, Crawley, WA 6009
Phone +61 8 6488 2877
Fax +61 8 6488 1055
Email Catherine.Lees@uwa.edu.au
Web www.biz.uwa.edu

Project Planning and Project Success

INFORMATION SHEET

Background of the study

It is known that many Human Resource Information Systems (HRIS) projects have
encountered problems during implementation and some were abandoned. Like many
other projects, HRIS projects were reported not to be able to finish on time, within
budget and more importantly, to deliver the expected outcome.

There has been no research on this phenomenon in the HRIS literature, although these
effects have been observed in a wide variety of activities. Also, most studies on project
planning have been conducted in Western countries. Carrying out the research in
Malaysia with cultural, economic, social and political differences from Western
countries will provide insight into how Malaysian people make decisions during project
planning. This may impact on the HRIS project planning and success in Malaysia in
ways that are as yet unknown.

Aims of the study

This study is conducted to investigate the best method of improving the planning in a
project. This is done by testing two theories of planning accuracy and extending
previous researchers’ explanations of project planning problems to the Malaysian
context.

Your involvement in this study

Participation in this study is totally voluntary and your responses will remain completely
confidential. The completion and return of the enclosed Consent Form is taken to
constitute your consent to participate in the study

You may withdraw from the study, by discontinuing the completion of the questionnaire,
at any time and without prejudice. You need not justify your decision to withdraw, and if
you choose to withdraw your responses will not be recorded.

Your participation in this study does not prejudice any right to compensation that you
may have under statute or common law.

After all the data have been collected, identifying information such as your name or
student ID number will be destroyed and the data will be completely anonymous in the
analyses and reports of the research. If the results from this study will be published,
only aggregate results will be reported and individual responses will not identifiable.

224
Risks

The researcher anticipates that this study will not involve any risk for you. Please be
assured that the researcher will give due care in providing support if you experience
any distress.

Benefits

As a result of participating in this study, you may feel empowered and feel a sense of
satisfaction because you have contributed to an important study that will benefit the
society. Findings from this study may have significant implications for human resource
management practices such as reducing forecasting errors during project planning and
provide them with avenues for improving HRIS implementation processes by
understanding the factors that might promote feasible and successful HRIS projects.

Experimental process

You will be invited to participate in an experiment with the Field Researcher. In the
beginning of the experiment, you will be randomly assigned to receive one of four
different sets of instructions that ask you to think about how you will complete your
project assignment. The Field Researcher will ask you to follow the instructions and to
fill out a short questionnaire about when you will finish your project assignment and
what mark you anticipate to get, how confident you are with your prediction and your
feelings towards the mark you receive. This will take approximately 30 minutes. Once
you have completed all the experimental materials, you will receive a form like a diary
where you are requested to write a weekly progress report. Your diary will be collected
on a weekly basis by the Field Researcher. As you hand in your report, you will receive
a new diary to write. The last diary entry will be collected on the day you hand in your
project assignment. Once your lecturer has finished marking your project assignment,
your mark will be given to the Field Researcher for you to collect. As you collect your
project assignment’s mark from the Field Researcher, you will be asked to fill out a
short questionnaire about your feelings towards the mark you receive. This will take
less than 10 minutes.

It is hoped that your participation in this study will provide invaluable information that
will help to guide human resource people and people involved in projects in knowing
which method is the best to reduce prediction error during project planning processes
and enable them to finish the project on time, within budget and deliver the outcome as
expected.

If you have any questions regarding this study, you may address them to the Field
Researcher Siti Zubaidah Othman or to the Chief Investigator, Dr. Catherine Lees at
the addresses below. A copy of this Information Sheet and Consent Form is provided
for your own records.

Dr. CATHERINE LEES SITI ZUBAIDAH OTHMAN


PhD Candidate
UWA Business School UWA Business School
The University of Western Australia The University of Western Australia
35 Stirling Highway, Crawley, 6009 35 Stirling Highway, Crawley, 6009
Phone: +61 8 6488 2877 Phone: +61 8 6488 3921
Fax: +61 8 6488 1055 Fax: +61 8 6488 1055
Email: Catherine.Lees@uwa.edu.au Email: othmas02@student.uwa.edu.au

225
Project planning and Project success

CONSENT FORM

I ________________________________________ have read the information


provided and any questions I have asked have been answered to my
satisfaction. I agree to participate in this activity, realising that I may withdraw at
any time without reason and without prejudice.

I understand that all information provided is treated as strictly confidential and


will not be released by the investigator unless required to by law. I have been
advised as to what data is being collected, what the purpose is, and what will be
done with the data upon completion of the research.

I agree that research data gathered for this study may be published provided
that my name or other identifying information is not used.

I understand that any future use that the University makes of the data is subject
to separate approval by the Research and Ethics Committees of The University
of Western Australia.

___________________________ __________________
Participant Date

The Human Research Ethics Committee at the University of Western Australia


requires that all participants are informed that, if they have any complaint
regarding the manner in which a research project is conducted, it may be given
to the researcher or, alternatively, to the Secretary, Human Research Ethics
Committee, Registrar’s Office, University of Western Australia, 35 Stirling
Highway, Crawley, WA 6009 (telephone number 6488-3703). All study
participants will be provided with a copy of the Information Sheet and Consent
Form for their personal records.

226
Project Planning and Project Success

Questionnaire

Dear Participant,

Thank you for agreeing to participate in this research on project planning.

We would appreciate it very much if you could answer the questions carefully as the
information you provide will influence the accuracy and the success of this research. It will
take no longer than 30 minutes to complete the questionnaire. All answers will be treated
with strict confidence and will be used for the purpose of the study only.

If you have any questions regarding this research, you may address them to Siti Zubaidah
Othman or to Dr. Catherine Lees at the contact details below.

Thank you for your cooperation and the time taken in answering this questionnaire.

Yours sincerely,

Dr. CATHERINE LEES SITI ZUBAIDAH OTHMAN


PhD Candidate
School of Economics & Commerce School of Economics & Commerce
The University of Western Australia The University of Western Australia
35 Stirling Highway, Crawley, 6009 35 Stirling Highway, Crawley, 6009
Phone: +61 8 6488 2877 Phone: +61 8 6488 3921
Fax: +61 8 6488 1055 Fax: +61 8 6488 1055
Email: Catherine.Lees@uwa.edu.au Email: othmas02@student.uwa.edu.au

Faculty of Human & Social Development


Universiti Utara Malaysia
Phone: +604-9283840
Fax: +604-9285754
Email: zubaidah@uum.edu.my

227
Project Planning and Project Success

SECTION 1:

Instructions: Please follow the instructions step by step. Do not look ahead until
you have completed each question.

Step 1: We would like you to write down your student number and today’s date. The
purpose of having your student number is that you will be receiving other materials in the
future. In order to ensure that you get the right materials, your student number will be
used.

Student # : ________________

Today’s Date : ________________

Step 2: After you have filled in your student number and today’s date, please read the
following instructions.

Please take a few minutes to consider your plans for completing your

project assignment. Think about the kind of events that are likely to

happen as you work on your project assignment. Think about all the

problems that are likely to happen in the future that will impede your

project progress and performance, and any aspects of the project or

situation in the future that will facilitate your project progress and

performance.

Step 3: Now, you can turn to the next page.

(A1) 228
Project Planning and Project Success

Step 4: To help you answering questions a through c, please focus on the project
assignment that you are going to complete for this class. Please use the space provided to
write your answer.

a. Please describe all the problems that are likely to happen in the future that will
impede your project progress and performance.

(A1) 229
Project Planning and Project Success

b. Please describe any aspects of the project or situation that will facilitate your
project progress and performance.

(A1) 230
Project Planning and Project Success

c. When do you start working on your project assignment? Please indicate your
predicted date and time even though you are still unsure about it at this stage.

Date: ______________

Time: _____________

Step 5: After you have finished writing down your plans for your project assignment, you
can now turn to page 5.

(A1) 231
Project Planning and Project Success

SECTION 2:

Instructions: Please answer all the questions below carefully.

Please predict as accurately and realistically as possible in questions 1 and 2:

1. When would you think you will complete your project assignment?

Completion Date: ___________

Completion Time: ___________

2. How many days do you think you will take to finish this project?

Number of days: ____________

Using the scale given below, please indicate how confident or certain you are of
the statement 3. Please make a mark (x) across the line to indicate your
confidence.

3. I will finish this project assignment by the date and time that I predicted.

Absolutely 50/50 Chance Completely


impossible (0%) certain (100%)

Please predict as accurately and realistically as possible in question 4:

4. Considering the total points for this project assignment as 100%, what percentage of
that mark do you anticipate to get for this project assignment? Please tick ( √ ) the
given box that represents your most appropriate answer.

            
40% 45% 50% 55% 60% 65% 70% 75% 80% 85% 90% 95% 100%

Please estimate the actual mark that you anticipate (in percentage).

(A1) 232
Project Planning and Project Success

Using the scale given below, please indicate how confident or certain you are of
the statement 5. Please make a mark (x) across the line to indicate your
confidence.

5. I will get the mark that I predicted.

Absolutely 50/50 Chance Completely


impossible (0%) certain (100%)

Questions 6 – 9 are about your feelings toward your project assignment outcome.
Using the following scale, please tick √( ) the given box that represents your
most appropriate answer.

6. How would you feel if you do well in this project assignment?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
not good very good

7. How would you feel if you succeed this project assignment?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
not good very good

8. How would you feel if you do poorly in this project assignment?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
very bad not bad

9. How would you feel if you do not succeed in this project assignment?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
very bad not bad

(A1) 233
Project Planning and Project Success

Please tick ( √ ) the given box OR fill in the blank that represents your most
appropriate answer.

1. I am:

 Male  Female

2. My age is ___________ years

3. My race is:

 Malay  Chinese  Indian  Other: ____________ (Please specify)

4. Since I started my course at Universiti Utara Malaysia, I have completed _____


semesters. (This does not include the current semester)

(A1) 234
Project Planning and Project Success

SECTION 1:

Instructions: Please follow the instructions step by step. Do not look ahead until
you have completed each question.

Step 1: We would like you to write down your student number and today’s date. The
purpose of having your student number is that you will be receiving other materials in the
future. In order to ensure that you get the right materials, your student number will be
used.

Student # : ________________

Today’s Date : ________________

Step 2: After you have filled in your student number and today’s date, please read the
following instruction.

Please take a few minutes to consider your plans for completing your

project assignment. Recall the kind of events that have happened in

your past project assignment and those of other people that you know

about. Recall all the problems that have happened in the past that

have impeded your project progress and performance, and any

aspects of the project or situation in the past that have facilitated your

project progress and performance.

Step 3: Now, you can turn to the next page.

(A2) 235
Project Planning and Project Success

Step 4: To help you answering questions a through c, you are encourage to recall as many
projects (either projects that you have done in the past or other people projects) as you
can. Please use the space provided to write your answer.

a. Please describe all the problems that have happened in the past that have
impeded your project progress and performance.

(A2) 236
Project Planning and Project Success

b. Please describe any aspects of the projects or situations in the past that have
facilitated your past project progress and performance.

(A2) 237
Project Planning and Project Success

c. When do you start working on your project assignment? Please indicate your
predicted date and time even though you are still unsure about it at this stage.

Date: ______________

Time: _____________

Step 5: After you have finished writing down your plans for your project assignment, you
can now turn to page 5.

(A2) 238
Project Planning and Project Success

SECTION 2:

Instructions: Please answer all the questions below carefully.

Please predict as accurately and realistically as possible in questions 1 and 2:

1. When would you think you will complete your project assignment?

Completion Date: ___________

Completion Time: ___________

2. How many days do you think you will take to finish this project?

Number of days: ____________

Using the scale given below, please indicate how confident or certain you are of
the statement 3. Please make a mark (x) across the line to indicate your
confidence.

3. I will finish this project assignment by the date and time that I predicted.

Absolutely 50/50 Chance Completely


impossible (0%) certain (100%)

Please predict as accurately and realistically as possible in question 4:

4. Considering the total point for this project assignment as 100%, what percentage of
that mark do you anticipate to get for this project assignment? Please tick ( √ ) the
given box that represents your most appropriate answer.

            
40% 45% 50% 55% 60% 65% 70% 75% 80% 85% 90% 95% 100%

Please estimate the actual mark that you anticipate (in percentage).

(A2) 239
Project Planning and Project Success

Using the scale given below, please indicate how confident or certain you are of
the statement 5. Please make a mark (x) across the line to indicate your
confidence.

5. I will get the mark that I predicted.

Absolutely 50/50 Chance Completely


impossible (0%) certain (100%)

Questions 6 – 9 are about your feelings toward your project assignment outcome.
Using the following scale, please tick√( ) the given box that represents your
most appropriate answer.

6. How would you feel if you do well in this project assignment?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
not good very good

7. How would you feel if you succeed this project assignment?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
not good very good

8. How would you feel if you do poorly in this project assignment?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
very bad not bad

9. How would you feel if you do not succeed in this project assignment?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
very bad not bad

(A2) 240
Project Planning and Project Success

Please tick (√ ) the given box OR fill in the blank that represents your most
appropriate answer.

1. I am:

 Male  Female

2. My age is ___________ years

3. My race is:

 Malay  Chinese  Indian  Other: ____________ (Please specify)

4. Since I start my course at Universiti Utara Malaysia, I have completed _____ semesters.
(This does not include the current semester)

(A2) 241
Project Planning and Project Success

SECTION 1:

Instructions: Please follow the instructions step by step. Do not look ahead until
you have completed each question.

Step 1: We would like you to write down your student number and today’s date. The
purpose of having your student number is that you will be receiving other materials in the
future. In order to ensure that you get the right materials, your student number will be
used.

Student # : ________________

Today’s Date : ________________

Step 2: After you have filled in your student number and today’s date, please read the
following instruction.

Please take a few minutes to consider your plans for completing your

project assignment. Think about the components that are needed for

project progress and performance. Also, think about each and every

thing that you plan to do in completing this project assignment.

Step 3: Now, you can turn to the next page.

(B1) 242
Project Planning and Project Success

Step 4: To help you answering questions a through c, please refer to your project
assignment that you are going to complete for this class. Please write your answer in the
space provided.

a. What are the components or elements that need to be included in this project
assignment?

(B1) 243
Project Planning and Project Success

b. Please describe in details each and every step that you are going to take in
order to complete this project assignment.

(B1) 244
Project Planning and Project Success

c. When do you start working on your project assignment? Please indicate your
predicted date and time even though you are still unsure about it at this stage.

Date: ______________

Time: _____________

Step 5: After you have listed all the components that are needed for this project
assignment and steps that you plan to take, you can now turn to page 5.

(B1) 245
Project Planning and Project Success

SECTION 2:

Instructions: Please answer all the questions below carefully.

Please predict as accurately and realistically as possible in questions 1 and 2:

1. When would you think you will complete your project assignment?

Completion Date: ___________

Completion Time: ___________

2. How many days do you think you will take to finish this project?

Number of days: ____________

Using the scale given below, please indicate how confident or certain you are of
the statement 3. Please make a mark (x) across the line to indicate your
confidence.

3. I will finish this project assignment by the date and time that I predicted.

Absolutely 50/50 Chance Completely


impossible (0%) certain (100%)

Please predict as accurately and realistically as possible in question 4:

4. Considering the total point for this project assignment as 100%, what percentage of
that mark do you anticipate to get for this project assignment? Please tick ( √ ) the
given box that represents your most appropriate answer.

            
40% 45% 50% 55% 60% 65% 70% 75% 80% 85% 90% 95% 100%

Please estimate the actual mark that you anticipate (in percentage).

(B1) 246
Project Planning and Project Success

Using the scale given below, please indicate how confident or certain you are of
the statement 5. Please make a mark (x) across the line to indicate your
confidence.

5. I will get the mark that I predicted.

Absolutely 50/50 Chance Completely


impossible (0%) certain (100%)

Questions 6 – 9 are about your feelings toward your project assignment outcome.
Using the following scale, please tick√( ) the given box that represents your
most appropriate answer.

6. How would you feel if you do well in this project assignment?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
not good very good

7. How would you feel if you succeed this project assignment?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
not good very good

8. How would you feel if you do poorly in this project assignment?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
very bad not bad

9. How would you feel if you do not succeed in this project assignment?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
very bad not bad

(B1) 247
Project Planning and Project Success

Please tick ( √ ) the given box OR fill in the blank that represents your most
appropriate answer.

1. I am:

 Male  Female

2. My age is ___________ years

3. My race is:

 Malay  Chinese  Indian  Other: ____________ (Please specify)

4. Since I start my course at Universiti Utara Malaysia, I have completed _____ semesters.
(This does not include the current semester)

(B1) 248
Project Planning and Project Success

SECTION 1:

Instructions: Please follow the instructions step by step. Do not look ahead until
you have completed each question.

Step 1: We would like you to write down your student number and today’s date. The
purpose of having your student number is that you will be receiving other materials in the
future. In order to ensure that you get the right materials, your student number will be
used.

Student # : ________________

Today’s Date : ________________

Step 2: After you filled in your student number and today’s date, please read the following
instruction.

Please take a few minutes to consider your plans for completing your

project assignment.

Step 3: Now, you can turn to the next page.

(B2) 249
Project Planning and Project Success

Step 4: If you need to write down your thoughts while reviewing your project assignment,
use the space provided below

a. You can write down your thoughts as you review your project assignment.

(B2) 250
Project Planning and Project Success

b. When do you start working on your project assignment? Please indicate your
predicted date and time even though you are still unsure about it at this stage.

Date: ______________

Time: _____________

Step 5: You can now turn to page 4.

(B2) 251
Project Planning and Project Success

SECTION 2:

Instructions: Please answer all the questions below carefully.

Please predict as accurately and realistically as possible in questions 1 - 2:

1. When would you think you will complete your project assignment?

Completion Date: ___________

Completion Time: ___________

2. How many days do you think you will take to finish this project?

Number of days: ____________

Using the scale given below, please indicate how confident or certain you are of
the statement 3. Please make a mark (x) across the line to indicate your
confidence.

3. I will finish this project assignment by the date and time that I predicted.

Absolutely 50/50 Chance Completely


impossible (0%) certain (100%)

Please predict as accurately and realistically as possible in question 4:

4. Considering the total point for this project assignment as 100%, what percentage of
that mark do you anticipate to get for this project assignment? Please tick ( √ ) the
given box that represents your most appropriate answer.

            
40% 45% 50% 55% 60% 65% 70% 75% 80% 85% 90% 95% 100%

Please estimate the actual mark that you anticipate (in percentage).

(B2) 252
Project Planning and Project Success

Using the scale given below, please indicate how confident or certain you are of
the statement 5. Please make a mark (x) across the line to indicate your
confidence.

5. I will get the mark that I predicted.

Absolutely 50/50 Chance Completely


impossible (0%) certain (100%)

Questions 6 – 9 are about your feelings toward your project assignment outcome.
Using the following scale, please tick√( ) the given box that represents your
most appropriate answer.

6. How would you feel if you do well in this project assignment?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
not good very good

7. How would you feel if you succeed this project assignment?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
not good very good

8. How would you feel if you do poorly in this project assignment?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
very bad not bad

9. How would you feel if you do not succeed in this project assignment?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
very bad not bad

(B2) 253
Project Planning and Project Success

Please tick (√ ) the given box OR fill in the blank that represents your most
appropriate answer.

1. I am:

 Male  Female

2. My age is ___________ years

3. My race is:

 Malay  Chinese  Indian  Other: ____________ (Please specify)

4. Since I start my course at Universiti Utara Malaysia, I have completed _____ semesters.
(This does not include the current semester)

(B2) 254
Project planning and project success

DIARY ENTRY

Instructions:

1. You are requested to make a weekly entry into your diary, explaining about your
project progress.

2. You need to write down your student number, name of the courses, class time
and date.

3. Please record every occasion and the times (date, hour and minute; from
beginning to end) when you have worked on the project assignment.

Notes:

All the information given will remain confidential at all times and will be used for the study
only.

THANK YOU FOR YOUR COOPERATION.

255
Student #: ______________ Course: ____________ Class Time: ______________ Date: ________ to _______

DIARY ENTRY WEEK ______


Date / Day Time Activities
Begin End

(G) 256
Student #: ______________ Course: ____________ Class Time: ______________ Date: ________ to _______

Date / Day Time Activities


Begin End

(G) 257
Student #: ______________ Course: ____________ Class Time: ______________ Date: ________ to _______

Please use the space provided to write your answer.

Meeting’s date & day Time Things being discussed


Begin End

(G) 258
Student #: ______________ Course: ____________ Class Time: ______________ Date: ________ to _______

Meeting’s date & day Time Things being discussed


Begin End

(G) 259
Student #: ______________ Course: ____________ Class Time: ______________ Date: ________ to _______

Please answer all the questions below carefully.

10. When would you think you will complete your project assignment?

Completion Date: ___________ Completion Time: _______________

11. How many days do you think you will take to finish this project?

Number of days: ____________

Using the scale given below, please indicate how confident or certain you are of the statement 3. Please make a mark (x) across the line to indicate your confidence.

12. I will finish this project assignment by the date and time that I predicted.

Absolutely 50/50 Chance Completely


impossible (0%) certain (100%)

13. Considering the total for this project assignment as 100%, what percentage of that mark do you anticipate to get from this project assignment? Please tick ( √ ) the given
box that represents your most appropriate answer.

            
40% 45% 50% 55% 60% 65% 70% 75% 80% 85% 90% 95% 100%

Please estimate the mark that you anticipate (in percentage).

(G) 260
Student #: ______________ Course: ____________ Class Time: ______________ Date: ________ to _______

Using the scale given below, please indicate how confident or certain you are of the statement 5. Please make a mark (x) across the line to indicate your confidence.

14. I will get the mark that I predicted.

Absolutely 50/50 Chance Completely


impossible (0%) certain (100%)
Questions 6 – 9 are about your feelings toward your project assignment outcome. Using the following scale, please tick√ ( ) the given box that represents your most
appropriate answer.

15. How would you feel if you do well in this project assignment?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
not good very good

16. How would you feel if you succeed this project assignment?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
not good very good

17. How would you feel if you do poorly in this project assignment?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
very bad not bad

18. How would you feel if you do not succeed in this project assignment?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
very bad not bad

(G) 261
Student #: ______________ Course: ____________ Class Time: ______________ Date: ________ to _______

LAST DIARY ENTRY


Date / Day Time Activities
Begin End

(G) 262
Student #: ______________ Course: ____________ Class Time: ______________ Date: ________ to _______

Date / Day Time Activities


Begin End

(G) 263
Student #: ______________ Course: ____________ Class Time: ______________ Date: ________ to _______

Please use the space provided to write your answer.

Meeting’s date & day Time Things being discussed


Begin End

(G) 264
Student #: ______________ Course: ____________ Class Time: ______________ Date: ________ to _______

Meeting’s date & day Time Things being discussed


Begin End

(G) 265
Student #: ______________ Course: ____________ Class Time: ______________ Date: ________ to _______

Please answer all the questions below carefully.

19. When would you think you will complete your project assignment?

Completion Date: ___________ Completion Time: _______________

20. How many days do you think you will take to finish this project?

Number of days: ____________

Using the scale given below, please indicate how confident or certain you are of the statement 3. Please make a mark (x) across the line to indicate your confidence.

21. I will finish this project assignment by the date and time that I predicted.

Absolutely 50/50 Chance Completely


impossible (0%) certain (100%)

22. Considering the total for this project assignment as 100%, what percentage of that mark do you anticipate to get from this project assignment? Please tick ( √ ) the given
box that represents your most appropriate answer.

            
40% 45% 50% 55% 60% 65% 70% 75% 80% 85% 90% 95% 100%

Please estimate the actual mark that you anticipate (in percentage).

(G) 266
Student #: ______________ Course: ____________ Class Time: ______________ Date: ________ to _______

Using the scale given below, please indicate how confident or certain you are of the statement 5. Please make a mark (x) across the line to indicate your confidence.

23. I will get the mark that I predicted.

Absolutely 50/50 Chance Completely


impossible (0%) certain (100%)

Questions 6 – 9 are about your feelings toward your project assignment outcome. Using the following scale, please tick (√ ) the given box that represents your most
appropriate answer.

24. How would you feel if you do well in this project assignment?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
not good very good

25. How would you feel if you succeed this project assignment?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
not good very good

26. How would you feel if you do poorly in this project assignment?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
very bad not bad

27. How would you feel if you do not succeed in this project assignment?

 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
very bad not bad

(G) 267
Student #: ______________ Course: ____________ Class Time: ______________ Date: ________ to _______

28. Your own contribution towards the project assignment (in percentage) :

29. The overall contribution of other members of your team towards the project assignment (in percentage):

30. Each of your team members’ contribution towards the project assignment (in percentage):
(Do not include yourself)

Name Contribution (in percentage)

__________________ ______________________

__________________ ______________________

__________________ ______________________

__________________ ______________________

__________________ ______________________

(G) 268
Project planning and Project success

Student #: ______________ Date: ______________

Instructions:

You have just received your mark for your project assignment. Please state your
view about the mark you received by answering question 1.

1. How do you view the mark you received for this project?

 A successful project  Not a successful project

If it is a successful project, please turn to page 2.

If it is not a successful project, please go to page 3.

269
Project planning and Project success

Instructions: Please answer questions 2 and 3.

Using the following scale, please tick√ () the given box that represents your
feelings toward your mark.

2. To what degree do you feel good about your mark after doing well in this project
assignment?

12 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11


Not good Very good

3. To what degree do you feel good about your mark after your success in this project
assignment?

12 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11


Not good Very good

THAT WAS THE FINAL QUESTION. THANK YOU FOR YOUR


PARTICIPATION.

270
Project planning and Project success

Instructions: Please answer questions 4 and 5.

Using the following scale, please tick√ () the given box that represents your
feelings toward your mark.

4. To what degree do you feel bad about your mark after doing poorly in this project
assignment?

12 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11


Not bad Very bad

5. To what degree do you feel bad about your mark after the unsuccessful project
assignment?

12 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11


Not bad Very bad

THAT WAS THE FINAL QUESTION. THANK YOU FOR YOUR


PARTICIPATION.

271
Perancangan Projek dan Kejayaan Projek

RISALAH MAKLUMAT
Latar belakang kajian

Seperti yang diketahui projek Sistem Maklumat Sumber Manusia (SMSM) menghadapi banyak masalah
ketika pelaksanaannya dan ada antaranya terhenti begitu sahaja. Sama seperti projek lain, projek SMSM
juga dilaporkan gagal disiapkan mengikut tempoh masa yang diberi, mengikut bajet yang diperuntukkan
dan yang paling utama, hasilnya tidak seperti yang diharapkan.

Penyelidikan mengenai fenomena ini masih lagi tiada dalam ulasan karya SMSM, walaupun kesannya
dapat dilihat dalam pelbagai jenis aktiviti. Selain daripada itu, kebanyakan kajian yang berkaitan dengan
perancangan projek dijalankan di negara-negara barat. Kajian seumpamanya di Malaysia, yang
mempunyai budaya, ekonomi, sosial dan politik yang berbeza daripada negara barat, akan dapat memberi
kefahaman baru tentang bagaimana orang Malaysia membuat keputusan ketika perancangan projek. Ini
mungkin akan memberi impak ke atas kejayaan dan perancangan projek SMSM di Malaysia dalam bentuk
yang masih belum diketahui lagi.

Matlamat kajian

Kajian ini dijalankan bagi mengkaji kaedah terbaik dalam memperbaiki perancangan projek. Ia dijalankan
dengan menguji dua teori berkenaan dengan ketepatan perancangan dan mengembangkan penerangan
daripada penyelidik terdahulu tentang masalah dalam perancangan projek ke dalam konteks Malaysia.

Penglibatan anda dalam kajian ini

Penyertaan dalam kajian ini adalah sukarela dan jawapan anda akan dianggap sebagai betul-betul sulit.
Borang kebenaran yang telah diisi dan dipulangkan akan dianggap sebagai tanda persetujuan anda untuk
menyertai kajian ini.

Anda boleh menarik balik penyertaan anda daripada kajian ini, dengan tidak meneruskan pengisian
borang soal selidik, pada bila-bila masa dan tanpa menjejaskan kedudukan anda. Anda tidak perlu
menyatakan alasan mengapa anda menarik balik penyertaan daripada kajian ini, dan sekiranya anda
memilih untuk berbuat demikian, jawapan anda tidak akan direkodkan

Penyertaan anda dalam kajian ini tidak akan menjejaskan apa-apa hak pampasan yang ada pada anda di
bawah statut atau common law.

Setelah semua data selesai dikumpul, maklumat diri seperti nama atau nombor pelajar akan dimusnahkan
dan tiada data pengenalan digunakan ketika penganalisaan dan penulisan laporan kajian. Sekiranya hasil
kajian ini diterbitkan, hanya hasil keseluruhan kajian akan dilaporkan dan jawapan secara individu tidak
akan didedahkan.

Risiko

Penyelidik mengandaikan bahawa kajian ini tidak melibatkan apa-apa risiko. Penyelidik memberi jaminan
bahawa bantuan akan diberikan sekiranya anda mengalami sebarang kesulitan.

272
Faedah

Hasil daripada penglibatan anda dalam kajian ini, anda mungkin akan berasa lebih berupaya dan
memperolehi kepuasan kerana anda telah memberi sumbangan terhadap satu kajian penting yang akan
memberi manfaat kepada masyarakat. Hasil kajian ini mungkin mempunyai implikasi yang signifikan
terhadap pengamalan pengurusan sumber manusia seperti mengurangkan kesilapan dalam membuat
ramalan ketika perancangan projek dan memberi jalan dalam memperbaiki proses pelaksanaan SMSM
dengan memahami faktor-faktor yang boleh mempromosikan kebolehlaksanaan dan kejayaan SMSM.

Proses Eksperimen

Anda akan dijemput untuk menyertai satu eksperimen oleh penyelidik lapangan. Pada permulaan
eksperimen, anda akan secara rawak menerima satu daripada empat set arahan yang meminta anda
untuk memikirkan bagaimana anda akan menyiapkan tugasan projek anda. Penyelidik lapangan akan
meminta anda untuk mengikut arahan yang diberi dan mengisi satu soal selidik yang pendek tentang
bilakah anda akan menyiapkan tugasan projek anda dan apakah markah yang anda andaikan akan
diterima, bagaimanakah tahap keyakinan anda terhadap ramalan yang diberi dan perasaan anda
terhadap markah yang bakal anda terima. Ini akan mengambil masa lebih kurang 30 minit. Apabila anda
selesai mengisi kesemua bahan eksperimen, anda akan menerima satu borang seperti diari yang
mengkehendaki anda untuk menulis laporan kemajuan secara mingguan. Diari anda akan dikutip secara
mingguan oleh penyelidik lapangan. Anda akan menerima diari yang baru setiap kali anda menghantar
laporan anda. Diari terakhir akan dikumpul pada hari anda menyerahkan tugasan projek. Setelah
pensyarah anda selesai memeriksa tugasan projek anda, markah anda akan diberikan kepada penyelidik
lapangan untuk anda ambil. Semasa anda mengambil markah tugasan projek anda daripada penyelidik
lapangan, anda akan diminta untuk mengisi satu soal selidik yang pendek yang mengkehendaki anda
untuk menyatakan perasaan anda terhadap markah yang diterima. Ini akan mengambil masa kurang
daripada 10 minit.

Adalah diharapkan dengan penyertaan anda dalam kajian ini, dapat memberikan maklumat yang berharga
yang boleh dijadikan panduan oleh pihak sumber manusia dan mereka yang terlibat dengan mana-mana
projek dalam mengetahui kaedah terbaik bagi mengurangkan kesilapan dalam membuat ramalan ketika
proses perancangan dan membolehkan mereka menyiapkan projek dalam tempoh yang ditetapkan,
mengikut bajet yang diperuntukkan dan dapat memberi hasil seperti yang diharapkan.

Sekiranya anda mempunyai apa-apa persoalan berkaitan dengan kajian ini, anda boleh kemukakannya
kepada penyelidik lapangan atau ketua penyelidik, Dr. Catherine Lees di alamat seperti di bawah. Satu
salinan Risalah maklumat dan Borang kebenaran akan diberikan untuk rekod peribadi anda.

Dr. CATHERINE LEES SITI ZUBAIDAH OTHMAN


Calon PhD
School of Economics & Commerce School of Economics & Commerce
The University of Western Australia The University of Western Australia
35 Stirling Highway, Crawley, 6009 35 Stirling Highway, Crawley, 6009
Tel: +61 8 6488 2877 Tel: +61 8 6488 3921
Faks: +61 8 6488 1055 Faks: +61 8 6488 1055
Email: Catherine.Lees@uwa.edu.au Email: othmas02@student.uwa.edu.au

Fakulti Pembangunan Sosial dan Manusia


Universiti Utara Malaysia
Tel: +604-9283840
Faks: +604-9285754
Email: zubaidah@uum.edu.my

273
Perancangan Projek dan Kejayaan Projek

BORANG KEBENARAN

Saya, ______________________________________ telah membaca maklumat yang disediakan


dan bagi setiap soalan yang saya ajukan, jawapan yang memuaskan telah diberikan. Saya
bersetuju untuk menyertai aktiviti ini, menyedari bahawa saya boleh menarik balik penyertaan
saya pada bila-bila masa tanpa sebab dan tanpa menjejaskan apa-apa hak pampasan yang ada
pada saya.

Saya faham bahawa semua maklumat yang saya berikan akan dianggap sebagai betul-betul sulit
dan tidak akan diberikan kepada mana-mana pihak lain melainkan jika dikehendaki berbuat
demikian oleh undang-undang. Saya telah diberitahu tentang jenis data yang akan dikumpul,
tujuannya dan tindakan yang akan diambil ke atas data tersebut apabila kajian selesai.

Saya bersetuju bahawa data penyelidikan yang dikumpul untuk kajian ini mungkin akan
diterbitkan dengan syarat tiada data pengenalan digunakan.

Saya faham bahawa apa-apa kegunaan data tersebut oleh Universiti pada masa depan adalah
tertakluk kepada kelulusan berasingan daripada Jawatankuasa Kajian dan Etika The Universiti of
Western Australia.

___________________________ __________________
Peserta Tarikh

Jawatankuasa Etika Kemanusiaan The University of Western Australia mengkehendaki supaya


semua peserta dimaklumkan bahawa jika mereka mempunyai apa-apa aduan, tentang cara
projek kajian ini dijalankan, ia boleh diberikan kepada penyelidik, atau sebagai pilihan lain,
kepada Secretary, Human Research Ethics Committee, Registrar’s Office, University of Western
Australia, 35 Stirling Highway, Crawley, WA 6009 (nombor telefon 6488-3703). Semua peserta
kajian akan diberikan salinan Risalah maklumat dan Borang kebenaran untuk rekod peribadi
mereka.

274
Perancangan projek dan Kejayaan projek

Soal selidik

Peserta yang dihormati,

Terima kasih di atas persetujuan anda untuk menyertai penyelidikan tentang


perancangan projek ini.

Kami amat menghargai sekiranya anda dapat menjawab soalan dengan berhati-hati
kerana maklumat yang anda beri akan mempengaruhi ketepatan dan kejayaan
penyelidikan ini. Ia akan mengambil masa tidak lebih daripada 30 minit untuk
menyiapkan soal selidik ini. Kesemua jawapan akan dianggap sebagai betul-betul sulit
dan hanya akan digunakan untuk tujuan kajian ini sahaja.

Sekiranya anda mempunyai apa-apa persoalan mengenai penyelidikan ini, anda boleh
kemukakan kepada Siti Zubaidah Othman atau Dr. Catherine Lees seperti alamat di
bawah.

Terima kasih di atas kerjasama yang diberi dan masa yang diambil untuk menjawab
soal selidik ini.

Yang benar,

Dr. CATHERINE LEES SITI ZUBAIDAH OTHMAN


Pelajar PhD
School of Economics & Commerce School of Economics & Commerce
The University of Western Australia The University of Western Australia
35 Stirling Highway, Crawley, 6009 35 Stirling Highway, Crawley, 6009
Tel: +61 8 6488 2877 Tel: +61 8 6488 3921
Faks: +61 8 6488 1055 Faks: +61 8 6488 1055
Email: Catherine.Lees@uwa.edu.au Email: othmas02@student.uwa.edu.au

Fakulti Pembangunan Sosial & Manusia


Universiti Utara Malaysia
Tel: +604-9283840
Faks: +604-9285754
Email: zubaidah@uum.edu.my

275
Perancangan projek dan kejayaan projek
BAHAGIAN 1:

Arahan: Sila ikut arahan langkah demi langkah. Anda tidak dibenarkan untuk
melihat soalan lain sehinggalah anda selesai menjawab setiap soalan.

Langkah 1: Kami ingin meminta anda untuk menulis nombor pelajar dan tarikh hari ini.
Tujuan kami mendapatkan nombor pelajar anda adalah kerana anda akan menerima bahan-
bahan lain di masa akan datang. Bagi memastikan anda mendapat bahan yang betul,
nombor pelajar anda akan digunakan.

# Pelajar : ________________

Tarikh hari ini : ________________

Langkah 2: Setelah anda menulis nombor pelajar dan tarikh hari ini, sila baca arahan di
bawah.

Sila ambil beberapa minit untuk memikirkan rancangan anda untuk

menyiapkan tugasan projek anda. Fikirkan tentang sebarang

peristiwa yang bakal berlaku semasa menyiapkan tugasan projek

anda. Fikirkan tentang semua masalah yang bakal berlaku di masa

akan datang yang boleh menghalang kemajuan dan prestasi projek

anda, dan aspek lain yang berkaitan dengan projek atau keadaan di

masa akan datang yang akan mempercepatkan kemajuan dan prestasi

projek anda.

Langkah 3: Anda boleh ke halaman sebelah sekarang.

(A1) 276
Perancangan projek dan kejayaan projek
Langkah 4: Bagi membantu anda menjawab soalan a hingga c, sila fokus pada tugasan
projek yang akan anda siapkan untuk kelas ini. Sila gunakan ruangan yang disediakan bagi
menulis jawapan anda.

a. Sila perihalkan semua masalah yang bakal berlaku di masa akan datang yang
boleh menghalang kemajuan dan prestasi projek anda.

(A1) 277
Perancangan projek dan kejayaan projek

b. Sila perihalkan apa-apa aspek yang berkaitan dengan projek atau keadaan
yang akan mempercepatkan kemajuan dan prestasi projek anda.

(A1) 278
Perancangan projek dan kejayaan projek

c. Bilakah anda akan mula membuat tugasan projek anda? Sila jangkakan tarikh
dan masa walaupun anda belum pasti tentangnya pada tahap ini.

Tarikh: ______________

Masa: _______________

Langkah 5: Setelah anda selesai menulis rancangan bagi tugasan projek ini, anda boleh ke
halaman 5 sekarang.

(A1) 279
Perancangan projek dan kejayaan projek
BAHAGIAN 2:

Arahan: Sila jawab semua soalan berikut dengan teliti.

Sila berikan jawapan dengan seberapa tepat dan realistik yang mungkin bagi
soalan 1 dan 2:

1. Bilakah anda fikir anda akan menyiapkan tugasan projek anda?

Tarikh Disiapkan: ___________

Masa Disiapkan: ___________

2. Berapa harikah anda fikir anda akan ambil untuk menyiapkan projek ini?

Jumlah hari: ____________

Dengan menggunakan skala di bawah, sila nyatakan sejauh mana anda yakin
atau pasti dengan kenyataan 3. Sila tanda (x) atas garisan berikut bagi
menggambarkan keyakinan anda.

3. Saya akan menyiapkan tugasan projek ini pada tarikh dan masa yang saya jangkakan.

Mustahil sama sekali Peluang 50/50 Seratus peratus


(0%) yakin (100%)

Sila berikan jawapan dengan seberapa tepat dan realistik yang mungkin bagi
soalan 4:

4. Katakan markah keseluruhan bagi tugasan projek ini ialah 100%, apakah peratus
markah yang anda jangka anda akan dapat daripada tugasan projek ini? Sila tanda ( √ )
pada kotak yang benar-benar menggambarkan jawapan anda.

            
40% 45% 50% 55% 60% 65% 70% 75% 80% 85% 90% 95% 100%

Sila buat jangkaan markah sebenar yang anda fikir anda akan peroleh (dalam peratus).

(A1) 280
Perancangan projek dan kejayaan projek

Dengan menggunakan skala di bawah, sila nyatakan sejauh mana anda yakin
atau pasti dengan kenyataan 5. Sila tanda (x) atas garisan berikut bagi
menggambarkan keyakinan anda.

5. Saya akan mendapat markah yang saya jangkakan.

Mustahil sama sekali Peluang 50/50 Seratus peratus


(0%) yakin (100%)

Soalan 6 – 9 adalah tentang perasaan anda terhadap hasil tugasan projek anda.
Dengan menggunakan skala berikut, sila tanda ( √ ) pada kotak yang benar -
benar menggambarkan jawapan anda.

6. Apakah perasaan anda jika anda cemerlang dalam tugasan projek ini?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
tidak amat
menyenangkan menyenangkan

7. Apakah perasaan anda jika anda berjaya dalam tugasan projek ini?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
tidak amat
menyenangkan menyenangkan

8. Apakah perasaan anda sekiranya anda tidak cemerlang dalam tugasan projek ini?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
amat teruk tidak teruk

9. Apakah perasaan anda jika anda tidak berjaya dalam tugasan projek ini?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
amat teruk tidak teruk

(A1) 281
Perancangan projek dan kejayaan projek

Sila tandakan ( √ ) pada kotak yang disediakan ATAU isikan tempat kosong bagi
menggambarkan jawapan anda yang paling sesuai.

1. Saya seorang:

 Lelaki  Perempuan

2. Umur saya ___________ tahun

3. Saya berbangsa:

 Melayu  Cina  India  Lain-lain: _____ (Sila nyatakan)

4. Semenjak saya memulakan kursus di Universiti Utara Malaysia, saya telah


menghabiskan _____ semester. (Tidak termasuk semester sekarang)

(A1) 282
Perancangan projek dan kejayaan projek

BAHAGIAN 1:

Arahan: Sila ikut arahan langkah demi langkah. Anda tidak dibenarkan untuk
melihat soalan lain sehinggalah anda selesai menjawab setiap soalan.

Langkah 1: Kami ingin meminta anda untuk menulis nombor pelajar dan tarikh hari ini.
Tujuan kami mendapatkan nombor pelajar anda adalah kerana anda akan menerima bahan-
bahan lain di masa akan datang. Bagi memastikan anda mendapat bahan yang betul,
nombor pelajar anda akan digunakan.

# Pelajar : ________________

Tarikh hari ini : ________________

Langkah 2: Setelah anda menulis nombor pelajar dan tarikh hari ini, sila baca arahan di
bawah.

Sila ambil beberapa minit untuk memikirkan rancangan anda untuk

menyiapkan tugasan projek anda. Ingat kembali semua peristiwa

yang berlaku semasa anda menyiapkan tugasan projek anda yang lalu

dan orang lain yang anda kenali. Ingat kembali semua masalah yang

telah berlaku yang menghalang kemajuan dan pretasi projek anda,

dan apa-apa aspek yang berkaitan dengan projek atau keadaan di

masa lalu yang telah mempercepat kemajuan dan prestasi tugasan

projek anda.

Langkah 3: Anda boleh ke halaman sebelah sekarang.

(A2) 283
Perancangan projek dan kejayaan projek

Langkah 4: Bagi membantu anda menjawab soalan a hingga c, anda digalakkan untuk
mengingat kembali seberapa banyak projek yang boleh (sama ada projek yang telah anda
lakukan di masa lalu atau projek orang lain). Sila gunakan ruang yang disediakan untuk
menulis jawapan anda.

a. Sila perihalkan semua masalah yang berlaku di masa lalu yang telah
menghalang kemajuan dan prestasi tugasan projek anda.

(A2) 284
Perancangan projek dan kejayaan projek

b. Sila perihalkan apa-apa aspek yang berkaitan dengan projek atau keadaan
yang telah mempercepat kemajuan dan prestasi projek anda di masa lalu.

(A2) 285
Perancangan projek dan kejayaan projek

c. Bilakah anda mula membuat tugasan projek anda? Sila jangkakan tarikh dan
masa walaupun anda belum pasti tentangnya pada tahap ini.

Tarikh: ______________

Masa: _______________

Langkah 5: Setelah anda selesai menulis rancangan bagi tugasan projek ini, anda boleh ke
halaman 5 sekarang

(A2) 286
Perancangan projek dan kejayaan projek

BAHAGIAN 2:

Arahan: Sila jawab semua soalan berikut dengan teliti.

Sila berikan jawapan dengan seberapa tepat dan realistik yang mungkin bagi
soalan 1 dan 2:

1. Bilakah anda fikir anda akan menyiapkan tugasan projek anda?

Tarikh Disiapkan: ___________

Masa Disiapkan: ___________

2. Berapa harikah anda fikir anda akan ambil untuk menyiapkan projek ini?

Jumlah hari: ____________

Dengan menggunakan skala di bawah, sila nyatakan sejauh mana anda yakin
atau pasti dengan kenyataan 3. Sila tanda (x) atas garisan berikut bagi
menggambarkan keyakinan anda.

3. Saya akan menyiapkan tugasan projek ini pada tarikh dan masa yang saya jangkakan.

Mustahil sama sekali Peluang 50/50 Seratus peratus


(0%) yakin (100%)

Sila berikan jawapan dengan seberapa tepat dan realistik yang mungkin bagi
soalan 4:

4. Katakan markah keseluruhan bagi tugasan projek ini ialah 100%, apakah peratus
markah yang anda jangka anda akan dapat daripada tugasan projek ini? Sila tanda √( )
pada kotak yang benar-benar menggambarkan jawapan anda.

            
40% 45% 50% 55% 60% 65% 70% 75% 80% 85% 90% 95% 100%

Sila buat jangkaan markah sebenar yang anda fikir anda akan peroleh (dalam peratus).

(A2) 287
Perancangan projek dan kejayaan projek

Dengan menggunakan skala di bawah, sila nyatakan sejauh mana anda yakin
atau pasti dengan kenyataan 5. Sila tanda (x) atas garisan berikut bagi
menggambarkan keyakinan anda.

5. Saya akan mendapat markah yang saya jangkakan.

Mustahil sama sekali Peluang 50/50 Seratus peratus


(0%) yakin (100%)

Soalan 6 – 9 adalah tentang perasaan anda terhadap hasil tugasan projek anda.
Dengan menggunakan skala berikut, sila tanda√ () pada kotak yang benar -
benar menggambarkan jawapan anda.

6. Apakah perasaan anda jika anda cemerlang dalam tugasan projek ini?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
tidak amat
menyenangkan menyenangkan

7. Apakah perasaan anda jika anda berjaya dalam tugasan projek ini?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
tidak amat
menyenangkan menyenangkan

8. Apakah perasaan anda jika anda tidak cemerlang dalam tugasan projek ini?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
amat teruk tidak teruk

9. Apkah perasaan anda jika anda tidak berjaya dalam tugasan projek ini?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
amat teruk tidak teruk

(A2) 288
Perancangan projek dan kejayaan projek

Sila tandakan ( √ ) pada kotak yang disediakan ATAU isikan tempat kosong bagi
menggambarkan jawapan anda yang paling sesuai.

1. Saya seorang:

 Lelaki  Perempuan

2. Umur saya ___________ tahun

3. Saya berbangsa:

 Melayu  Cina  India  Lain-lain: _____ (Sila nyatakan)

4. Semenjak saya memulakan kursus di Universiti Utara Malaysia, saya telah


menghabiskan _____ semester. (Tidak termasuk semester sekarang)

(A2) 289
Perancangan projek dan kejayaan projek

BAHAGIAN 1:

Arahan: Sila ikut arahan langkah demi langkah. Anda tidak dibenarkan untuk
melihat soalan lain sehinggalah anda selesai menjawab setiap soalan.

Langkah 1: Kami ingin meminta anda untuk menulis nombor pelajar dan tarikh hari ini.
Tujuan kami mendapatkan nombor pelajar anda adalah kerana anda akan menerima bahan-
bahan lain di masa akan datang. Bagi memastikan anda mendapat bahan yang betul,
nombor pelajar anda akan digunakan.

# Pelajar : ________________

Tarikh hari ini : ________________

Langkah 2: Setelah anda menulis nombor pelajar dan tarikh hari ini, sila baca arahan di
bawah.

Sila ambil beberapa minit untuk memikirkan rancangan anda untuk

menyiapkan tugasan projek anda. Fikirkan tentang komponen yang

diperlukan untuk kemajuan dan prestasi projek. Fikirkan juga setiap

perkara yang anda cadang untuk lakukan bagi menyiapkan tugasan

projek ini.

Langkah 3: Anda boleh ke halaman sebelah sekarang.

(B1) 290
Perancangan projek dan kejayaan projek

Langkah 4: Bagi membantu anda menjawab soalan a hingga c, sila rujuk projek yang akan
anda siapkan untuk kelas ini. Sila guna ruangan yang disediakan bagi menulis jawapan anda.

a. Apakah komponen atau elemen yang perlu dimasukkan dalam tugasan projek
ini?

(B1) 291
Perancangan projek dan kejayaan projek

b. Sila perihalkan dengan terperinci setiap langkah yang anda akan ambil untuk
menyiapkan tugasan projek ini.

(B1) 292
Perancangan projek dan kejayaan projek

c. Bilakah anda mula membuat tugasan projek anda? Sila jangkakan tarikh dan
masa walaupun anda belum pasti tentangnya pada tahap ini.

Tarikh: ______________

Masa: _______________

Langkah 5: Setelah anda selesai menyenaraikan semua komponen yang diperlukan untuk
projek ini dan langkah yang anda rancang untuk ambil, anda boleh ke halaman 5 sekarang.

(B1) 293
Perancangan projek dan kejayaan projek

BAHAGIAN 2:

Arahan: Sila jawab semua soalan berikut dengan teliti.

Sila berikan jawapan dengan seberapa tepat dan realistik yang mungkin bagi
soalan 1 dan 2:

1. Bilakah anda fikir anda akan menyiapkan tugasan projek anda?

Tarikh Disiapkan: ___________

Masa Disiapkan: ___________

2. Berapa harikah anda fikir anda akan ambil untuk menyiapkan projek ini?

Jumlah hari: ____________

Dengan menggunakan skala di bawah, sila nyatakan sejauh mana anda yakin
atau pasti dengan kenyataan 3. Sila tanda (x) atas garisan berikut bagi
menggambarkan keyakinan anda.

3. Saya akan menyiapkan tugasan projek ini pada tarikh dan masa yang saya jangkakan.

Mustahil sama sekali Peluang 50/50 Seratus peratus


(0%) yakin (100%)

Sila berikan jawapan dengan seberapa tepat dan realistik yang mungkin bagi
soalan 4:

4. Katakan markah keseluruhan bagi tugasan projek ini ialah 100%, apakah peratus
markah yang anda jangka anda akan dapat daripada tugasan projek ini? Sila tanda ( √ )
pada kotak yang benar-benar menggambarkan jawapan anda.

            
40% 45% 50% 55% 60% 65% 70% 75% 80% 85% 90% 95% 100%

Sila buat jangkaan markah sebenar yang anda fikir anda akan peroleh (dalam peratus).

(B1) 294
Perancangan projek dan kejayaan projek

Dengan menggunakan skala di bawah, sila nyatakan sejauh mana anda yakin
atau pasti dengan kenyataan 5. Sila tanda (x) atas garisan berikut bagi
menggambarkan keyakinan anda.

5. Saya akan mendapat markah yang saya jangkakan.

Mustahil sama sekali Peluang 50/50 Seratus peratus


(0%) yakin (100%)

Soalan 6 – 9 adalah tentang perasaan anda terhadap hasil tugasan projek anda.
Dengan menggunakan skala berikut, sila tanda ( √ ) pada kotak yang benar -
benar menggambarkan jawapan anda.

6. Apakah perasaan anda jika anda cemerlang dalam tugasan projek ini?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
tidak amat
menyenangkan menyenangkan

7. Apakah perasaan anda jika anda berjaya dalam tugasan projek ini?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
tidak amat
menyenangkan menyenangkan

8. Apakah perasaan anda jika anda tidak cemerlang dalam tugasan projek ini?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
amat teruk tidak teruk

9. Apakah perasaan anda jika anda tidak berjaya dalam tugasan projek ini?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
amat teruk tidak teruk

(B1) 295
Perancangan projek dan kejayaan projek

Sila tandakan ( √ ) pada kotak yang disediakan ATAU isikan tempat kosong bagi
menggambarkan jawapan anda yang paling sesuai.

1. Saya seorang:

 Lelaki  Perempuan

2. Umur saya ___________ tahun

3. Saya berbangsa:

 Melayu  Cina  India  Lain-lain: _____ (Sila nyatakan)

4. Semenjak saya memulakan kursus di Universiti Utara Malaysia, saya telah


menghabiskan _____ semester. (Tidak termasuk semester sekarang)

(B1) 296
Perancangan projek dan kejayaan projek

BAHAGIAN 1:

Arahan: Sila ikut arahan langkah demi langkah. Anda tidak dibenarkan untuk
melihat soalan lain sehinggalah anda selesai menjawab setiap soalan.

Langkah 1: Kami ingin meminta anda untuk menulis nombor pelajar dan tarikh hari ini.
Tujuan kami mendapatkan nombor pelajar anda adalah kerana anda akan menerima bahan-
bahan lain di masa akan datang. Bagi memastikan anda mendapat bahan yang betul,
nombor pelajar anda akan digunakan.

# Pelajar : ________________

Tarikh hari ini : ________________

Langkah 2: Setelah anda menulis nombor pelajar dan tarikh hari ini, sila baca arahan di
bawah.

Sila ambil beberapa minit untuk memikirkan rancangan anda untuk

menyiapkan tugasan projek anda.

Langkah 3: Anda boleh ke halaman sebelah sekarang.

(B2) 297
Perancangan projek dan kejayaan projek

Langkah 4: Jika anda perlu menulis sebarang perkara yang anda terfikir semasa menyemak
semula tugasan projek anda, sila guna ruangan yang disediakan di bawah.

a. Anda boleh menulis sebarang perkara yang anda terfikir semasa menyemak
semula tugasan projek anda.

(B2) 298
Perancangan projek dan kejayaan projek

b. Bilakah anda mula membuat tugasan projek anda? Sila jangkakan tarikh dan
masa walaupun anda belum pasti tentangnya pada tahap ini.

Tarikh: ______________

Masa: _______________

Langkah 5: Anda boleh ke halaman 4 sekarang.

(B2) 299
Perancangan projek dan kejayaan projek

BAHAGIAN 2:

Arahan: Sila jawab semua soalan berikut dengan teliti.

Sila berikan jawapan dengan seberapa tepat dan realistik yang mungkin bagi
soalan 1 dan 2:

1. Bilakah anda fikir anda akan menyiapkan tugasan projek anda?

Tarikh Disiapkan: ___________

Masa Disiapkan: ___________

2. Berapa harikah anda fikir anda akan ambil untuk menyiapkan projek ini?

Jumlah hari: ____________

Dengan menggunakan skala di bawah, sila nyatakan sejauh mana anda yakin
atau pasti dengan kenyataan 3. Sila tanda (x) atas garisan berikut bagi
menggambarkan keyakinan anda.

3. Saya akan menyiapkan tugasan projek ini pada tarikh dan masa yang saya jangkakan.

Mustahil sama sekali Peluang 50/50 Seratus peratus


(0%) yakin (100%)

Sila berikan jawapan dengan seberapa tepat dan realistik yang mungkin bagi
soalan 4:

4. Katakan markah keseluruhan bagi tugasan projek ini ialah 100%, apakah peratus
markah yang anda jangka anda akan dapat daripada tugasan projek ini? Sila tanda ( √ )
pada kotak yang benar-benar menggambarkan jawapan anda.

            
40% 45% 50% 55% 60% 65% 70% 75% 80% 85% 90% 95% 100%

Sila buat jangkaan markah sebenar yang anda fikir anda akan peroleh (dalam peratus).

(B2) 300
Perancangan projek dan kejayaan projek

Dengan menggunakan skala di bawah, sila nyatakan sejauh mana anda yakin
atau pasti dengan kenyataan 5. Sila tanda (x) atas garisan berikut bagi
menggambarkan keyakinan anda.

5. Saya akan mendapat markah yang saya jangkakan.

Mustahil sama sekali Peluang 50/50 Seratus peratus


(0%) yakin (100%)

Soalan 6 – 9 adalah tentang perasaan anda terhadap hasil tugasan projek anda.
Dengan menggunakan skala berikut, sila tanda√ () pada kotak yang benar -
benar menggambarkan jawapan anda.

6. Apakah perasaan anda jika anda cemerlang dalam tugasan projek ini?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
tidak amat
menyenangkan menyenangkan

7. Apakah perasaan anda jika anda berjaya dalam tugasan projek ini?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
tidak amat
menyenangkan menyenangkan

8. Apakah perasaan anda jika anda tidak cemerlang dalam tugasan projek ini?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
amat teruk tidak teruk

9. Apakah perasaan anda jika anda tidak berjaya dalam tugasan projek ini?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
amat teruk tidak teruk

(B2) 301
Perancangan projek dan kejayaan projek

Sila tandakan ( √ ) pada kotak yang disediakan ATAU isikan tempat kosong bagi
menggambarkan jawapan anda yang paling sesuai.

1. Saya seorang:

 Lelaki  Perempuan

2. Umur saya ___________ tahun

3. Saya berbangsa:

 Melayu  Cina  India  Lain-lain: _____ (Sila nyatakan)

4. Semenjak saya memulakan kursus di Universiti Utara Malaysia, saya telah


menghabiskan _____ semester. (Tidak termasuk semester sekarang)

(B2) 302
Perancangan projek dan kejayaan projek

MASUKAN DIARI

Arahan:

1. Anda diminta untuk membuat masukan secara mingguan ke dalam diari,


menerangkan tentang perkembangan projek anda.

2. Anda dikehendaki menulis nombor pelajar, nama kursus, masa kelas dan tarikh.

3. Sila rekodkan semua perkara yang dilakukan dan masa yang diambil (tarikh, jam dan
minit; dari mula hingga akhir) setiap kali anda melakukan tugasan projek.

Nota:

Semua maklumat yang diberi akan kekal sebagai betul-betul sulit pada sepanjang masa dan
akan hanya digunakan untuk tujuan kajian semata-mata.

TERIMA KASIH DI ATAS KERJASAMA ANDA.

303
# Pelajar: ____________ Kursus: ___________ Masa Kelas: ______________ Tarikh: ________ hingga __________

KEMASUKAN DIARI MINGGU ______


Tarikh / Hari Masa Aktiviti
Mula Tamat

(K) 304
# Pelajar: ____________ Kursus: ___________ Masa Kelas: ______________ Tarikh: ________ hingga __________

Tarikh / Hari Masa Aktiviti


Mula Tamat

(K) 305
# Pelajar: ____________ Kursus: ___________ Masa Kelas: ______________ Tarikh: ________ hingga __________

Sila gunakan ruangan yang disediakan untuk menulis jawapan anda.

Tarikh & hari Masa Perkara yang dibincangkan


perjumpaan Mula Tamat

(K) 306
# Pelajar: ____________ Kursus: ___________ Masa Kelas: ______________ Tarikh: ________ hingga __________

Tarikh & hari Masa Perkara yang dibincangkan


perjumpaan Mula Tamat

(K) 307
# Pelajar: ____________ Kursus: ___________ Masa Kelas: ______________ Tarikh: ________ hingga __________

Sila jawab semua soalan berikut dengan teliti.

31. Bilakah anda fikir anda akan menyiapkan tugasan projek ini?

Tarikh Disiapkan: ___________ Masa Disiapkan: _______________

32. Berapa harikah anda fikir anda akan ambil untuk menyiapkan projek ini?

Jumlah hari: ____________

Dengan menggunakan skala di bawah, sila nyatakan sejauh mana anda yakin atau pasti dengan kenyataan 3. Sila tandakan (x) atas garisan berikut bagi menggambarkan
keyakinan anda.

33. Saya akan menyiapkan tugasan projek ini pada tarikh dan masa yang saya jangkakan.

Mustahil sama sekali Peluang 50/50 Seratus peratus yakin


(0%) (100%)

34. Katakan markah keseluruhan bagi tugasan projek ini adalah 100%, apakah peratus markah yang anda jangka anda akan dapat daripada tugasan projek ini? Sila tanda ( √ )
pada kotak yang benar-benar menggambarkan jawapan anda.

            
40% 45% 50% 55% 60% 65% 70% 75% 80% 85% 90% 95% 100%

Sila buat jangkaan markah sebenar yang anda fikir anda akan peroleh (dalam peratus).

(K) 308
# Pelajar: ____________ Kursus: ___________ Masa Kelas: ______________ Tarikh: ________ hingga __________

Dengan menggunakan skala di bawah, sila nyatakan sejauh mana anda yakin atau pasti dengan kenyataan 5. Sila tanda (x) atas garisan berikut bagi menggambarkan
keyakinan anda.

35. Saya akan mendapat markah yang saya jangkakan.

Seratus peratus yakin


Mustahil sama sekali Peluang 50/50 (100%)
(0%)
Soalan 6 – 9 adalah tentang perasaan anda terhadap hasil tugasan projek anda. Dengan menggunakan skala berikut, sila tanda ( √ ) pada kotak yang benar -benar
menggambarkan jawapan anda.

36. Apakahkah perasaan anda jika anda cemerlang dalam tugasan projek ini?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
tidak menyenangkan amat menyenangkan

37. Apakah perasaan anda jika anda berjaya dalam tugasan projek ini?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
tidak menyenangkan amat menyenangkan

38. Apakah perasaan anda jika anda tidak cemerlang dalam tugasan projek ini?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
amat teruk tidak teruk

39. Apakah perasaan anda jika anda tidak berjaya dalam tugasan projek ini?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
amat teruk tidak teruk

(K) 309
# Pelajar: ______________ Kursus: ____________ Masa Kelas: ______________ Tarikh: ______ hingga _______

KEMASUKAN DIARI AKHIR


Tarikh / Hari Masa Aktiviti
Mula Tamat

310
(K)
# Pelajar: ______________ Kursus: ____________ Masa Kelas: ______________ Tarikh: ______ hingga _______

Tarikh / Hari Masa Aktiviti


Mula Tamat

311
(K)
# Pelajar: ______________ Kursus: ____________ Masa Kelas: ______________ Tarikh: ______ hingga _______

Sila gunakan ruangan yang disediakan untuk menulis jawapan anda.

Tarikh & hari Masa Perkara yang dibincangkan


perjumpaan Mula Tamat

312
(K)
# Pelajar: ______________ Kursus: ____________ Masa Kelas: ______________ Tarikh: ______ hingga _______

Tarikh & hari Masa Perkara yang dibincangkan


perjumpaan Mula Tamat

313
(K)
# Pelajar: ______________ Kursus: ____________ Masa Kelas: ______________ Tarikh: ______ hingga _______

Sila jawab semua soalan berikut dengan teliti.

40. Bilakah anda fikir anda akan menyiapkan tugasan projek anda?

Tarikh Disiapkan: ___________ Masa Disiapkan: _______________

41. Berapa harikah anda fikir anda akan ambil untuk menyiapkan projek ini?

Jumlah hari: ____________

Dengan menggunakan skala di bawah, sila nyatakan sejauh mana anda yakin atau pasti dengan kenyataan 3. Sila tandakan (x) atas garisan berikut bagi menggambarkan
keyakinan anda.

42. Saya akan menyiapkan tugasan projek ini pada tarikh dan masa yang saya jangkakan.

Mustahil sama sekali Peluang 50/50 Seratus peratus yakin


(0%) (100%)

43. Katakan markah keseluruhan tugasan projek ini ialah 100%, apakah peratus markah yang anda jangka anda akan dapat daripada tugasan projek ini? Sila tanda ( √ ) pada
kotak yang benar-benar dapat menggambarkan jawapan anda.

            
40% 45% 50% 55% 60% 65% 70% 75% 80% 85% 90% 95% 100%

Sila buat jangkaan markah sebenar yang anda fikir anda akan peroleh (dalam peratus).

314
(K)
# Pelajar: ______________ Kursus: ____________ Masa Kelas: ______________ Tarikh: ______ hingga _______

Dengan menggunakan skala di bawah, sila nyatakan sejauh mana anda yakin atau pasti dengan kenyataan 5. Sila tanda (x) atas garisan berikut bagi menggambarkan
keyakinan anda.

44. Saya akan mendapat markah yang saya jangkakan.

Mustahil sama sekali Peluang 50/50 Seratus peratus


(0%) yakin (100%)

Soalan 6 – 9 adalah tentang perasaan anda terhadap hasil tugasan projek anda. Dengan menggunakan skala berikut, sila tanda ( √ ) pada kotak yang benar -benar
menggambarkan jawapan anda.

45. Apakah perasaan anda jika anda cemerlang dalam tugasan projek ini?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
tidak menyenangkan amat menyenangkan

46. Apakah perasaan anda jika anda berjaya dalam tugasan projek ini?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
tidak menyenangkan amat menyenangkan

47. Apkahkah perasaan anda jika anda tidak cemerlang dalam tugasan projek ini?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
amat teruk tidak teruk

48. Apakah perasaan anda jika anda tidak berjaya dalam tugasan projek ini?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
amat teruk tidak teruk

315
(K)
# Pelajar: ______________ Kursus: ____________ Masa Kelas: ______________ Tarikh: ______ hingga _______

49. Sumbangan anda terhadap tugasan projek ini (dalam peratus) :

50. Sumbangan keseluruhan ahli lain dalam pasukan anda terhadap tugasan projek ini (dalam peratus):

51. Sumbangan setiap ahli pasukan anda terhadap tugasan projek ini (dalam peratus):
(Tidak termasuk diri anda)

Nama Sumbangan (dalam peratus)

__________________ ______________________

__________________ ______________________

__________________ ______________________

__________________ ______________________

__________________ ______________________

316
(K)
# Pelajar: ______________ Tarikh: ______________

Arahan:

Anda baru sahaja menerima markah bagi tugasan projek anda. Sila nyatakan
pandangan anda terhadap markah yang anda terima dengan menjawab soalan 1.

1. Apakah pandangan anda terhadap markah yang anda terima untuk projek ini?

 Projek ini satu projek yang berjaya  Projek ini satu projek yang tidak berjaya

Sekiranya projek ini ialah projek yang berjaya, sila ke halaman 2.

Sekiranya projek ini ialah projek yang tidak berjaya, sila ke halaman 3.

317
Arahan: Sila jawab soalan 2 dan 3.

Dengan menggunakan skala yang diberi, sila tanda ( √ ) pada kotak yang
disediakan bagi menggambarkan perasaan anda terhadap markah anda.

2. Setakat mana anda berasa senang dengan markah anda setelah melakukan tugasan
projek ini dengan baik?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
Tidak Amat
menyenangkan menyenangkan

3. Setakat mana anda berasa senang dengan markah anda setelah anda berjaya dalam
tugasan projek ini?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
Tidak Amat
menyenangkan menyenangkan

ITU ADALAH SOALAN YANG TERAKHIR. TERIMA KASIH DI ATAS


KERJASAMA ANDA.

318
Arahan: Sila jawab soalan 4 dan 5.

Dengan menggunakan skala yang diberi, sila tanda ( √ ) pada kotak yang
disediakan bagi menggambarkan perasaan anda terhadap markah anda.

4. Setakat mana anda berasa teruk dengan markah anda setelah anda tidak melakukan
tugasan projek dengan baik?

12 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11


Tidak teruk Amat teruk

5. Setakat mana anda berasa teruk dengan markah anda setelah anda tidak berjaya dalam
tugasan projek ini?

12 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11


Tidak teruk Amat teruk

ITU ADALAH SOALAN YANG TERAKHIR. TERIMA KASIH DI ATAS


KERJASAMA ANDA.

319
APPENDIX B

EXPERIMENTAL DESIGN AND WEEKLY RESPONDENTS –STUDY 1

Respondents at week

Manipulated 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
Condition

Inside (n = 193) 193 192 192 189 187 186 188 187 190

Outside (n = 184) 184 180 179 179 176 178 177 178 180

Unpacked (n = 199) 199 198 194 196 193 194 194 192 191

Packed (n = 196) 196 195 193 191 192 190 188 184 186

320
APPENDIX C

RESULTS OF DEMOGRAPHIC CHARACTERISTICS OF THE

PARTICIPANTS – STUDY 1

Descriptions Frequency % Mean Std. Dev Median Min. Max.

Gender
Male 165 21.4
Female 607 78.6
Total 772 100
Age
Total response 772 100 21.69 1.4 22 18 36
Ethnicity
Malay 553 71.6
Chinese 153 19.8
Indians 44 5.7
Others 22 2.8
Total 772 100
Semesters
Total response 772 100 2.87 1.7 3 1 8

321
APPENDIX D

SAMPLES OF THREE-LEVEL HIERARCHICAL REGRESSION EQUATIONS

– STUDY 1

This appendix contains samples of the equation that describe the effect of the four

conditions and students’ control variables on project completion time predictions,

project outcome success predictions, confidence in predictions of project completion

time, confidence in predictions of project outcome success, affective predictions

towards outcome success, and affective predictions towards outcome failure, with

outside and inside as the reference conditions.

322
A sample of three-level equations for project completion time predictions,
confidence in predictions of project completion time and confidence in predictions
of project outcome success

1a. Equations with the outside view as a reference condition

Level-1 Model
Ytij = P0 + P1*(WEEK) + E

Level-2 Model
P0 = B00 + B01*(Inside) + B02*(Unpacked) + B03*(Packed) + R0
P1 = B10 + B11*(Inside) + B12*(Unpacked) + B13*(Packed) + R1

Level-3 Model
B00 = G000 + U00
B01 = G010
B02 = G020
B03 = G030
B10 = G100 + U10
B11 = G110
B12 = G120
B13 = G130

where
Ytij is the outcome at time t for participant i in group j;

P0 is the initial status of participant ij, that is, the expected outcome for that participant

on the first day (when WEEK = 0);

P1 is the prediction bias rate for participant ij during the weeks;

E is a random “time effect”. These random time effects are assumed to be normally

distributed with mean of 0, and variance σ2;

B00 represents the mean initial status within group j;

G000 is the overall mean initial status;

323
R0 and R1 are random “participant effects”. These random participant effects are

assumed to be normally distributed with mean of 0, and variance σ2;

B10 is the mean weekly prediction bias rate within group j;

G100 is the overall mean weekly prediction bias rate;

U00 and U10 are the random “group effects”. These random group effects are assumed

to be normally distributed with mean of 0, and variance σ2.

324
1b. Equations with the inside view as a reference condition

Level-1 Model
Ytij = P0 + P1*(WEEK) + E

Level-2 Model
P0 = B00 + B01*(Outside) + B02*(Unpacked) + B03*(Packed) + R0
P1 = B10 + B11*(Outside) + B12*(Unpacked) + B13*(Packed) + R1

Level-3 Model
B00 = G000 + U00
B01 = G010
B02 = G020
B03 = G030
B10 = G100 + U10
B11 = G110
B12 = G120
B13 = G130

where

Ytij is the outcome at time t for participant i in group j;

P0 is the initial status of participant ij, that is, the expected outcome for that participant

on the first day (when WEEK = 0);

P1 is the prediction bias rate for participant ij during the weeks;

E is a random “time effect”. These random time effects are assumed to be normally

distributed with mean of 0, and variance σ2;

B00 represents the mean initial status within group j;

G000 is the overall mean initial status;

R0 and R1 are random “participant effects”. These random participant effects are

assumed to be normally distributed with mean of 0, and variance σ2;


325
B10 is the mean weekly prediction bias rate within group j;

G100 is the overall mean weekly prediction bias rate;

U00 and U10 are the random “group effects”. These random group effects are assumed

to be normally distributed with mean of 0, and variance σ2.

326
A sample of three-level equations with grand-mean centering for project outcome
predictions

2a. Equations with the packed (control) as a references condition

Level-1 Model
Ytij = P0 + P1*(WEEK) + E

Level-2 Model
P0 = B00 + B01*(Inside) + B02*(Outside) + B03*(Unpacked) +
B04*( SEMESTER − x SEMESTER ) + R0
P1 = B10 + R1

Level-3 Model
B00 = G000 + G001(Teacher1) + G002(Teacher2) + G003(Teacher3) +
G004(Teacher4) + G005(Teacher5) + G006(Teacher6) + G007(Teacher7) +
G008(Teacher8) + G009(Teacher9) + G0010(SEMESTER_MEAN) + U00
B01 = G010
B02 = G020
B03 = G030
B10 = G100 + U10

where

Ytij is the outcome at time t for participant i in group j;

P0 is the initial status of participant ij, that is, the expected outcome for that participant

on the first day (when WEEK = 0);

P1 is the prediction bias rate for participant ij during the weeks;

E is a random “time effect”. These random time effects are assumed to be normally

distributed with mean of 0, and variance σ2;

B00 represents the mean initial status within group j;

G000 is the overall mean initial status;

R0 and R1 are random “participant effects”. These random participant effects are

assumed to be normally distributed with mean of 0, and variance σ2;


327
B10 is the mean weekly prediction bias rate within group j;

G100 is the overall mean weekly prediction bias rate;

U00 and U10 are the random “group effects”. These random group effects are assumed

to be normally distributed with mean of 0, and variance σ2.

328
2b. Equations with the outside view as a reference condition

Level-1 Model
Ytij = P0 + P1*(WEEK) + E

Level-2 Model
P0 = B00 + B01*(Inside) + B02*(Unpacked) + B03*(Packed) +
B04*( SEMESTER − x SEMESTER ) + R0
P1 = B10 + R1

Level-3 Model
B00 = G000 + G001(Teacher1) + G002(Teacher2) + G003(Teacher3) +
G004(Teacher4) + G005(Teacher5) + G006(Teacher6) + G007(Teacher7) +
G008(Teacher8) + G009(Teacher9) + G0010(SEMESTER_MEAN) + U00
B01 = G010
B02 = G020
B03 = G030
B10 = G100 + U10

where

Ytij is the outcome at time t for participant i in group j;

P0 is the initial status of participant ij, that is, the expected outcome for that participant

on the first day (when WEEK = 0);

P1 is the prediction bias rate for participant ij during the weeks;

E is a random “time effect”. These random time effects are assumed to be normally

distributed with mean of 0, and variance σ2;

B00 represents the mean initial status within group j;

G000 is the overall mean initial status;

R0 and R1 are random “participant effects”. These random participant effects are

assumed to be normally distributed with mean of 0, and variance σ2;

B10 is the mean weekly prediction bias rate within group j;

329
G100 is the overall mean weekly prediction bias rate;

U00 and U10 are the random “group effects”. These random group effects are assumed

to be normally distributed with mean of 0, and variance σ2.

330
2c. Equations with the inside view as a reference condition

Level-1 Model
Ytij = P0 + P1*(WEEK) + E

Level-2 Model
P0 = B00 + B01*(Outside) + B02*(Unpacked) + B03*(Packed) +
B04*( SEMESTER − x SEMESTER ) + R0
P1 = B10 + R1

Level-3 Model
B00 = G000 + G001(Teacher1) + G002(Teacher2) + G003(Teacher3) +
G004(Teacher4) + G005(Teacher5) + G006(Teacher6) + G007(Teacher7) +
G008(Teacher8) + G009(Teacher9) + G0010(SEMESTER_MEAN) + U00
B01 = G010
B02 = G020
B03 = G030
B10 = G100 + U10

where

Ytij is the outcome at time t for participant i in group j;

P0 is the initial status of participant ij, that is, the expected outcome for that participant

on the first day (when WEEK = 0);

P1 is the prediction bias rate for participant ij during the weeks;

E is a random “time effect”. These random time effects are assumed to be normally

distributed with mean of 0, and variance σ2;

B00 represents the mean initial status within group j;

G000 is the overall mean initial status;

R0 and R1 are random “participant effects”. These random participant effects are

assumed to be normally distributed with mean of 0, and variance σ2;

331
B10 is the mean weekly prediction bias rate within group j;

G100 is the overall mean weekly prediction bias rate;

U00 and U10 are the random “group effects”. These random group effects are assumed

to be normally distributed with mean of 0, and variance σ2.

332
A sample of three-level equations for predictions of affective response towards
outcome success, and affective response towards outcome failure

3a. Equations with the packed (control) as a reference condition

Level-1 Model

Ytij = P0 + P1*(WEEK) + E

Level-2 Model
P0 = B00 +B01*(Inside) + B02*(Outside) + B03*(Unpacked) + R0
P1 = B10 + R1

Level-3 Model
B00 = G000 + U00
B01 = G010
B02 = G020
B03 = G030
B04 = G040
B05 = G050
B06 = G060
B10 = G100 + U10

where

Ytij is the outcome at time t for participant i in group j;

P0 is the initial status of participant ij, that is, the expected outcome for that participant

on the first day (when WEEK = 0);

P1 is the prediction bias rate for participant ij during the weeks;

E is a random “time effect”. These random time effects are assumed to be normally

distributed with mean of 0, and variance σ2;

B00 represents the mean initial status within group j;

G000 is the overall mean initial status;

333
R0 and R1 are random “participant effects”. These random participant effects are

assumed to be normally distributed with mean of 0, and variance σ2;

B10 is the mean weekly prediction bias rate within group j;

G100 is the overall mean weekly prediction bias rate;

U00 and U10 are the random “group effects”. These random group effects are assumed

to be normally distributed with mean of 0, and variance σ2.

334
3b. Equations with the outside view as a reference condition

Level-1 Model
Ytij = P0 + P1*(WEEK) + E

Level-2 Model
P0 = B00 + B01*(Inside) + B02*(Unpacked) + B03*(Packed) + R0
P1 = B10 + R1

Level-3 Model
B00 = G000 + U00
B01 = G010
B02 = G020
B03 = G030
B10 = G100 + U10

where

Ytij is the outcome at time t for participant i in group j;

P0 is the initial status of participant ij, that is, the expected outcome for that participant

on the first day (when WEEK = 0);

P1 is the prediction bias rate for participant ij during the weeks;

E is a random “time effect”. These random time effects are assumed to be normally

distributed with mean of 0, and variance σ2;

B00 represents the mean initial status within group j;

G000 is the overall mean initial status;

R0 and R1 are random “participant effects”. These random participant effects are

assumed to be normally distributed with mean of 0, and variance σ2;

B10 is the mean weekly prediction bias rate within group j;

G100 is the overall mean weekly prediction bias rate;

335
U00 and U10 are the random “group effects”. These random group effects are assumed

to be normally distributed with mean of 0, and variance σ2.

336
3c. Equations with the inside view as a reference condition

Level-1 Model
Ytij = P0 + P1*(WEEK) + E

Level-2 Model
P0 = B00 + B01*(Outside) + B02*(Unpacked) + B03*(Packed) + R0
P1 = B10 + R1

Level-3 Model
B00 = G000 + U00
B01 = G010
B02 = G020
B03 = G030
B10 = G100 + U10

where

Ytij is the outcome at time t for participant i in group j;

P0 is the initial status of participant ij, that is, the expected outcome for that participant

on the first day (when WEEK = 0);

P1 is the prediction bias rate for participant ij during the weeks;

E is a random “time effect”. These random time effects are assumed to be normally

distributed with mean of 0, and variance σ2;

B00 represents the mean initial status within group j;

G000 is the overall mean initial status;

R0 and R1 are random “participant effects”. These random participant effects are

assumed to be normally distributed with mean of 0, and variance σ2;

B10 is the mean weekly prediction bias rate within group j;

337
G100 is the overall mean weekly prediction bias rate;

U00 and U10 are the random “group effects”. These random group effects are assumed

to be normally distributed with mean of 0, and variance σ2.

338
APPENDIX E

RESULTS FOR CONDITIONAL THREE-LEVEL HLM MODELS – STUDY 1

This appendix contains six results of conditional three-level HLM models:

1. Prediction bias for project completion time


2. Prediction bias for project outcome
3. Confidence in predictions of project completion time
4. Confidence in predictions of project outcome
5. Prediction bias for affective response towards project outcome success
6. Prediction bias for affective response towards project outcome failure

339
Appendix E-1

Table 5.2c
Prediction bias for project completion time: Three-level model of the effects of manipulations relative to the inside view condition
Prediction bias for project completion time
Fixed effect Coefficient S.Error t-ratio p-value

Initial status: For INTERCEPT1, P0


INTERCEPT2, B00
INTERCEPT3, (Average initial status),G000 4.435 0.620 7.149 0.000***
For Outside (IIO1), B01
INTERCEPT3, G010 -0.810 0.862 -0.940 0.348
For Unpacked (IIO2), B02
INTERCEPT3, G020 -0.644 0.856 -0.752 0.452
For Packed (IIO3)
INTERCEPT3, G030 1.260 0.815 1.545 0.123
Rate of change: For Week slope, P1
INTERCEPT2, B10
INTERCEPT3, (Average weekly prediction bias rate), G100 -0.577 0.105 -5.492 0.000***
For Outside (IIO1), B11
INTERCEPT3, G110 0.264 0.138 1.913 0.056†
For Unpacked (IIO2), B12
INTERCEPT3, G120 0.208 0.144 1.449 0.148
For Packed (IIO3), B13
INTERCEPT3, G130 0.025 0.150 0.167 0.868

Random effect Variance Component df Chi-square p-value

Level-1 Effect (Week), E 10.950


Level-2 (student within groups)
INTERCEPT1, (Individual initial status), R0 24.958 355 2094.523 0.000***
WEEK slope, (Individual weekly prediction bias rate), R1 0.520 355 803.221 0.000***
Level-3 (between groups)
INTERCEPT1 / INTERCEPT2,(Group mean status),U00 3.889 157 228.716 0.000***
WEEK / INTERCEPT2, (Group mean prediction bias rate), U10 0.107 157 230.811 0.000***

340
Appendix E-1

TABLE 5.2c (Continued)


Random effect Variance Component df Chi-square p-value

Level-1 Coefficient Percentage of Variance Between Groups


Initial status, P0 13.48
Prediction bias rate, P1 17.06

Deviance = 20483.096
Number of estimated parameters = 15
A Three-level model with time = level-1, individual student = level-2 and group = level-3. Number of students = 708 and groups = 158
The dependent variable is measured as the difference between students’ predicted and actual project completion time (in days).

p<0.10,*p <.05, **p<.01, ***p<.001

341
Appendix E-2

Table 5.4c
Prediction bias for project outcome: Three-level model of the effects of manipulations relative to the inside view condition
Prediction bias for project outcome
Fixed effect Coefficient S.Error t-ratio p-value

Initial status: For INTERCEPT1, P0


INTERCEPT2, B00
INTERCEPT3, (Average initial status),G000 -3.309 3.831 -0.864 0.389
TEACHER1(T1), G001 6.322 2.123 2.978 0.004**
TEACHER2(T2), G002 -9.667 2.852 -3.389 0.001**
TEACHER3(T3), G003 -12.551 4.309 -2.913 0.005**
TEACHER4(T4), G004 8.468 4.023 2.105 0.037*
TEACHER5(T5), G005 6.626 3.008 2.203 0.029*
TEACHER6(T6), G006 2.433 3.312 0.735 0.464
TEACHER7(T7), G007 -0.631 1.804 -0.350 0.727
TEACHER8(T8), G008 -10.376 2.253 -4.606 0.000***
TEACHER9(T9), G009 2.732 2.317 1.179 0.241
SEM_MEAN, G0010 -0.615 1.014 -0.606 0.545
For Outside (IIO1), B01
INTERCEPT3, G010 -3.696 1.853 -1.994 0.046*
For Unpacked (IIO2), B02
INTERCEPT3, G020 -4.449 1.925 -2.311 0.021*
For Packed (IIO3), B03
INTERCEPT3, G030 -1.999 1.752 -1.141 0.255
For semester, B04
INTERCEPT3, G040 -2.055 0.461 -4.459 0.000***
Rate of change: For Week slope, P1
INTERCEPT2, B10
INTERCEPT3, (Average weekly prediction bias rate), G100 -0.147 0.036 -4.079 0.000***

Random effect Variance Component df Chi-square p-value

Level-1 Effect (Week), E 10.105


Level-2 (student within groups)

342
Appendix E-2

Table 5.4c (Continued)


Random effect Variance Component df Chi-square p-value
INTERCEPT1, (Individual initial status), R0 41.380 344 4124.282 0.000***
WEEK slope, (Individual weekly prediction bias rate), R1 0.297 348 951.367 0.000***
Level-3 (between groups)
INTERCEPT1 / INTERCEPT2,(Group mean status),U00 52.197 145 755.179 0.000***
WEEK / INTERCEPT2, (Group mean prediction bias rate), U10 0.052 155 214.727 0.001**

Level-1 Coefficient Percentage of Variance Between Groups


Initial status, P0 55.78
Prediction bias rate, P1 14.87

Deviance = 25933.896
Number of estimated parameters = 23

A Three-level model with time = level-1, individual student = level-2 and group = level-3. Number of students = 689 and groups = 156
The dependent variable is measured as the difference between students’ predicted and actual project completion time (in days).

p<0.10,*p <.05, **p<.01, ***p<.001

343
Appendix E-3

Table 5.6c
Confidence in predictions of project completion time: Three-level model of the effects of manipulations relative to the inside view condition

Confidence in predictions of project completion time


Fixed effect Coefficient S.Error t-ratio p-value

Initial status: For INTERCEPT1, P0


INTERCEPT2, B00
INTERCEPT3, (Average initial status),G000 79.958 1.822 43.879 0.000***
For Outside (IIO1), B01
INTERCEPT3, G010 -2.951 2.359 -1.251 0.212
For Unpacked (IIO2), B02
INTERCEPT3, G020 -2.656 2.659 -0.999 0.319
For Packed (IIO3)
INTERCEPT3, G030 -2.623 2.424 -1.082 0.280
Rate of change: For Week slope, P1
INTERCEPT2, B10
INTERCEPT3, (Average weekly prediction bias rate), G100 0.172 0.191 0.899 0.370
For Outside (IIO1), B11
INTERCEPT3, G110 0.248 0.265 0.937 0.350
For Unpacked (IIO2), B12
INTERCEPT3, G120 0.113 0.361 0.315 0.753
For Packed (IIO3), B13
INTERCEPT3, G130 -0.219 0.283 -0.774 0.439

Random effect Variance Component df Chi-square p-value

Level-1 Effect (Week), E 63.493


Level-2 (student within groups)
INTERCEPT1, (Individual initial status), R0 186.451 355 2640.617 0.000***
WEEK slope, (Individual weekly prediction bias rate), R1 2.760 355 821.165 0.000***
Level-3 (between groups)
INTERCEPT1 / INTERCEPT2,(Group mean status),U00 46.956 157 268.369 0.000***
WEEK / INTERCEPT2, (Group mean prediction bias rate), U10 0.494 157 197.163 0.016*

344
Appendix E-3

Table 5.6c (Continued)

Random effect Variance Component df Chi-square p-value

Level-1 Coefficient Percentage of Variance Between Groups


Initial status, P0 20.12
Prediction bias rate, P1 15.19

Deviance = 27338.901
Number of estimated parameters = 15
A three-level model with week = level-1, individual students = level -2, and group = level - 3. Number of students = 708 and groups = 158.
The dependent variable is measured from the 0% end of the line and centimeters were converted to a number out of 100%.

p<0.10,*p <.05, **p<.01 ***p<.001

345
Appendix E-4

Table 5.8c
Confidence in predictions of project outcome: Three-level model of the effects of manipulations relative to the inside view condition

Confidence in predictions of project outcome


Fixed effect Coefficient S.Error t-ratio p-value

Initial status: For INTERCEPT1, P0


INTERCEPT2, B00
INTERCEPT3, (Average initial status),G000 77.453 1.794 43.169 0.000***
For Outside (IIO1), B01
INTERCEPT3, G010 -1.036 2.477 -0.418 0.675
For Unpacked (IIO2), B02
INTERCEPT3, G020 -0.364 2.422 -0.150 0.881
For Packed (IIO3)
INTERCEPT3, G030 -1.873 2.220 -0.844 0.400
Rate of change: For Week slope, P1
INTERCEPT2, B10
INTERCEPT3, (Average weekly prediction bias rate), G100 0.407 0.169 2.407 0.017*
For Outside (IIO1), B11
INTERCEPT3, G110 -0.180 0.233 -0.772 0.441
For Unpacked (IIO2), B12
INTERCEPT3, G120 -0.092 0.268 -0.345 0.730
For Packed (IIO3), B13
INTERCEPT3, G130 -0.106 0.208 -0.509 0.611

Random effect Variance Component df Chi-square p-value

Level-1 Effect (Week), E 61.640


Level-2 (student within groups)
INTERCEPT1, (Individual initial status), R0 194.804 345 3334.196 0.000***
WEEK slope, (Individual weekly prediction bias rate), R1 1.334 345 809.824 0.000***
Level-3 (between groups)
INTERCEPT1 / INTERCEPT2,(Group mean status),U00 30.967 155 220.288 0.001**
WEEK / INTERCEPT2, (Group mean prediction bias rate), U10 0.332 155 222.70 0.000***

346
Appendix E-4

Table 5.8c (Continued)

Random effect Variance Component df Chi-square p-value

Level-1 Coefficient Percentage of Variance Between Groups


Initial status, P0 13.72
Prediction bias rate, P1 19.93

Deviance = 33759.444
Number of estimated parameters = 15
A three-level model with week = level-1, individual students = level -2, and group = level - 3. Number of students = 689 and groups = 156.
The dependent variable is measured from the 0% end of the line and centimeters were converted to a number out of 100%.

p<0.10,*p <.05, **p<.01 ***p<.001

347
Appendix E-5

Table 5.10c
Prediction bias for affective response towards project outcome success: Three-level model of the effects of manipulations relative to the inside
view condition

Prediction bias for affective reactions towards project outcome success


Fixed effect Coefficient S.Error t-ratio p-value

Initial status: For INTERCEPT1, P0


INTERCEPT2, B00
INTERCEPT3, (Average initial status),G000 -1.886 0.281 -6.617 0.000***
For Outside (IIO1), B01
INTERCEPT3, G010 0.771 0.368 2.097 0.037*
For Unpacked (IIO2), B02
INTERCEPT3, G020 0.010 0.432 0.025 0.981
For Packed (IIO3), B03
INTERCEPT3, G030 -0.282 0.381 -0.738 0.461
Rate of change: For Week slope, P1
INTERCEPT2, B10
INTERCEPT3, G100 0.023 0.007 3.241 0.002**

Random effect Variance Component df Chi-square p-value

Level-1 Effect (Week), E 0.292


Level-2 (student within groups)
INTERCEPT1, (Individual initial status), R0 2.688 186 4876.781 0.000***
WEEK slope, (Individual weekly prediction bias rate), R1 0.007 189 482.858 0.000***
Level-3 (between groups)
INTERCEPT1 / INTERCEPT2,(Group mean status), U00 1.104 115 236.716 0.000***
WEEK / INTERCEPT2, (Group mean prediction bias rate), U10 0.001 115 139.139 0.062†

348
Appendix E-5

Table 5.10c (Continued)

Level-1 Coefficient Percentage of Variance Between Groups


Initial status, P0 29.11
Prediction bias rate, P1 13.98

Deviance = 6141.417
Number of estimated parameters = 12

A Three-level model with time = level-1, individual student = level-2 and group = level-3. Number of students = 422 and groups = 116
The dependent variable is measured as the difference between students’ predicted and actual feelings towards assignment marks.

p<0.10,*p <.05, **p<.01, ***p<.001

349
Appendix E-6

Table 5.12c
Prediction bias for affective response towards project outcome failure: Three-level model of the effects of manipulations relative to the inside
view condition

Prediction bias for affective reactions towards project outcome failure


Fixed effect Coefficient S.Error t-ratio p-value

Initial status: For INTERCEPT1, P0


INTERCEPT2, B00
INTERCEPT3, (Average initial status),G000 1.856 0.635 2.922 0.006**
For Outside (IIO1), B01
INTERCEPT3, G010 -0.338 0.796 -0.425 0.671
For Unpacked (IIO2), B02
INTERCEPT3, G020 0.561 0.698 0.804 0.423
For Packed (IIO3), B03
INTERCEPT3, G030 0.137 0.742 0.185 0.854
Rate of change: For Week slope, P1
INTERCEPT2, B10
INTERCEPT3, G100 0.055 0.013 4.119 0.000***

Random effect Variance Component df Chi-square p-value

Level-1 Effect (Week), E 0.455


Level-2 (student within groups)
INTERCEPT1, (Individual initial status), R0 4.392 63 1699.207 0.000***
WEEK slope, (Individual weekly prediction bias rate), R1 0.006 66 112.300 0.001**
Level-3 (between groups)
INTERCEPT1 / INTERCEPT2,(Group mean status), U00 1.522 43 80.664 0.001**
WEEK / INTERCEPT2, (Group mean prediction bias rate), U10 0.002 43 65.863 0.014*

350
Appendix E-6

Table 5.12c (Continued)

Level-1 Coefficient Percentage of Variance Between Groups


Initial status, P0 25.73
Prediction bias rate, P1 31.74

Deviance = 2610.588
Number of estimated parameters = 12

A Three-level model with time = level-1, individual student = level-2 and group = level-3. Number of students = 130 and groups = 44
The dependent variable is measured as the difference between students’ predicted and actual feelings towards assignment marks.

p<0.10,*p <.05, **p<.01, ***p<.001

351
APPENDIX F

LEGENDS FOR GRAPHICAL ANALYSIS RESULTS – STUDY 1

1a. Mean predictions bias for completion time for the four conditions

day 1 week 1 week 2 week 3 week 4 week 5 week 6


Inside 4.97 4.41 3.96 3.41 2.90 2.70 2.30
Outside 3.67 3.25 2.66 2.50 2.31 2.18 1.58
Unpacked 4.07 3.58 3.22 2.57 2.40 2.04 1.56
Packed 5.62 4.43 4.35 3.55 3.56 2.77 2.44

1b. Mean predictions bias for project outcome for the four conditions

day 1 week 1 week 2 week 3 week 4 week 5 week 6 week 7 week 8


Inside -5.29 -6.16 -6.68 -6.45 -6.94 -6.84 -6.86 -7.03 -7.38
Outside -8.32 -8.66 -9.33 -9.30 -9.30 -9.55 -9.48 -9.78 -9.92
Unpacked -9.55 -9.49 -9.56 -9.92 -9.92 -9.96 -10.42 -10.65 -11.07
Packed -6.86 -6.20 -6.16 -6.57 -6.57 -6.60 -6.85 -7.39 -8.13

1c. Mean confidence in predictions of project completion time for the four
conditions

day 1 week 1 week 2 week 3 week 4 week 5 week 6


Inside 81.42 82.57 81.22 81.10 81.87 82.56 82.93
Outside 78.98 78.70 79.12 79.44 80.06 80.79 80.28
Unpacked 80.01 78.94 79.87 80.55 80.12 80.65 81.23
Packed 77.25 76.00 76.52 77.00 76.54 76.90 76.30

1d. Mean confidence in predictions of project outcome for the four conditions

day 1 week 1 week 2 week 3 week 4 week 5 week 6 week 7 week 8


Inside 76.74 77.36 78.72 78.86 79.96 79.78 80.50 80.88 82.24
Outside 77.28 77.75 78.15 78.49 77.71 78.37 79.14 79.65 80.20
Unpacked 76.59 78.18 79.17 80.26 78.47 78.96 80.51 80.31 80.84
Packed 74.41 76.04 74.62 75.52 76.15 75.83 76.13 77.69 78.10

352
1e. Mean predictions bias for affective response towards project outcome success
for the four conditions

day 1 week 1 week 2 week 3 week 4 week 5 week 6 week 7 week 8


Inside -1.79 -1.56 -1.55 -1.55 -1.45 -1.49 -1.45 -1.53 -1.61
Outside -1.55 -1.25 -1.22 -1.21 -1.19 -1.17 -1.19 -1.17 -1.22
Unpacked -2.08 -1.84 -1.72 -1.80 -1.67 -1.77 -1.82 -1.77 -1.81
Packed -2.17 -2.00 -2.04 -1.96 -1.89 -1.85 -1.89 -1.93 -2.02

1f. Mean predictions bias for affective response towards project outcome failure
for the four conditions

day 1 week 1 week 2 week 3 week 4 week 5 week 6 week 7 week 8


Inside 1.90 2.14 2.12 2.26 2.48 2.38 2.05 2.48 2.50
Outside 1.03 0.96 1.32 1.28 1.45 1.38 1.42 1.41 1.46
Unpacked 1.97 2.04 2.20 2.30 2.28 2.20 2.32 2.28 2.50
Packed 1.23 2.16 2.18 2.14 1.98 2.41 2.18 2.20 2.20

353
APPENDIX G

SAMPLE OF THE SURVEY MATERIALS – STUDY 2

This appendix contains copies of the survey materials provided to respondents in the

second study, namely the information sheet, the cover letter; and the two sets of

questionnaire (with four different versions each) - one completed by participants who

have already finished their HRIS project, and the other completed by participants who

were at the planning stage of their HRIS project.

354
UWA Business School
Dr. Catherine D. Lees
Mail Bag 261
35 Stirling Highway, Crawley, WA 6009
Phone +61 8 6488 2877
Fax +61 8 6488 1055
Email Catherine.Lees@uwa.edu.au
Web www.biz.uwa.edu

HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

INFORMATION SHEET

Background of the study

Like other IT projects, it is known that Human Resource Information Systems (HRIS)
projects are difficult to develop and that planning plays an important part.

There has been no research to understand this process for HRIS projects to date,
although varied project outcomes have been observed in a wide variety of activities.
Also, most studies on project planning have been conducted in Western countries.
Carrying out the research in Malaysia with cultural, economic, social and political
differences from Western countries will provide insight into how Malaysian people make
decisions during project planning. This may impact on the HRIS project planning and
success in Malaysia in ways that are as yet unknown.

Aims of the study

This study is conducted to investigate the best method of improving the planning in an
HRIS project. This is done by testing theories of planning and extending previous
researchers’ explanations of project planning problems to the Malaysian context.

Your involvement in this study

Participation in this study is totally voluntary and your responses will remain completely
confidential. The completion and return of the enclosed questionnaire is taken to
constitute your consent to participate in the study

You may withdraw from the study, by discontinuing the completion of the questionnaire,
or by not returning it, at any time and without prejudice. You need not justify your
decision to withdraw, and if you choose to withdraw your responses will not be
recorded.

Your participation in this study does not prejudice any right to compensation that you
may have under statute or common law.

You are not asked to provide your name, and your responses will be completely
anonymous in the analyses and reports of the research. If the results from this study
are published, only aggregate results will be reported and individual responses will not
be identifiable.

355
Risks

The researchers anticipate that this study will not involve any risk for you. Please be
assured that the researchers will answer all your questions in full.

Benefits

As a result of participating in this study, you may feel empowered and feel a sense of
satisfaction because you have contributed to an important study that will benefit the
society. Findings from this study may have significant implications for human resource
management practices such as improving project planning and HRIS implementation
processes by understanding the factors that might promote feasible and successful
HRIS projects.

Survey process

You will be invited to participate in a survey. In the beginning of the survey session, you
will be briefed about the purpose and the nature of the survey. You are then provided
with a sheet of information and you will be invited to ask questions. You will be given
instructions and asked to fill out a short questionnaire about methods you use during
HRIS project planning, about the project completion time and the project outcome, and
your feelings towards the level of success of the HRIS project. This will take
approximately 30 minutes.

It is hoped that your participation in this study will provide valuable information that will
help to guide human resource professionals and others involved in projects regarding
the best method for planning processes, and enable them to deliver good project
outcomes.

If you have any questions regarding this study, you may address them to Siti Zubaidah
Othman or to the Chief Investigator, Dr. Catherine Lees at the addresses below. A copy
of this Information Sheet is provided for your own records.

Dr. CATHERINE LEES SITI ZUBAIDAH OTHMAN


PhD Candidate
UWA Business School UWA Business School
The University of Western Australia The University of Western Australia
35 Stirling Highway, Crawley, 6009 35 Stirling Highway, Crawley, 6009
Phone: +61 8 6488 2877 Phone: +61 8 6488 3669
Fax: +61 8 6488 1055 Fax: +61 8 6488 1055
Email: Catherine.Lees@uwa.edu.au Email: othmas02@student.uwa.edu.au

356
HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

Questionnaire

Dear Participant,

Thank you for agreeing to participate in this research on project planning.

We would appreciate it very much if you could answer the questions carefully as the
information you provide will influence the accuracy and the success of this research. It will
take no longer than 30 minutes to complete the questionnaire. All answers will be treated
with strict confidence and will be used for the purpose of the study only.

If you have any questions regarding this research, you may address them to Siti Zubaidah
Othman or to Dr. Catherine Lees at the contact details below.

Thank you for your cooperation and the time taken in answering this questionnaire.

Yours sincerely,

Dr. CATHERINE LEES SITI ZUBAIDAH OTHMAN


PhD Candidate
UWA Business School UWA Business School
The University of Western Australia The University of Western Australia
35 Stirling Highway, Crawley, 6009 35 Stirling Highway, Crawley, 6009
Phone: +61 8 6488 2877 Phone: +61 8 6488 3669
Fax: +61 8 6488 1055 Fax: +61 8 6488 1055
Email: Catherine.Lees@uwa.edu.au Email: othmas02@student.uwa.edu.au

357
HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

SECTION 1:

Instructions:

We would like you to briefly describe about the HRIS project that you have been
involved in. Please answer questions 1 to 5 as accurately as possible.

1. Name and nature of the project (e.g. new HRIS, upgrading payroll system, adding a
new module)

______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________

2. The project was completed on? _______________

3. When was the project started? ________________

4. Was the project finished by the due date? Please tick (√ ) one of the following.

The project was completed -

 Early  On time  Late

If you responded EARLY, please state how early:

Number of days before the due date: __________ months _________ days

If you responded LATE, please state how late:

Number of days after the due date: ___________ months _________ days

5. Please describe your involvement in the project in terms of your role and your
contribution.

______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________

Completed project (R: ___ O: ___)1 358


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

SECTION 2:

Instructions:

To answer questions 6 to 14, please think about the project that you have mentioned in
questions 1 to 5. Please read carefully each of the activity statements below, and
indicate how much you agree that the statement describes an activity you did in
accomplishing the project.

AS I WORK ON THIS PROJECT:

6. I think about the kind of events that are likely to happen in this project.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

7. I recall the kinds of events that have happened in my past projects and those of other
people that I know about.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

8. I think about the components or elements that need to be included in the project.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

9. I think about all the problems that are likely to happen in the future that will impede
this project’s progress and performance.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

10. I recall all the problems that have happened in the past that have impeded other
projects’ progress and performance.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

11. I breakdown the overall project into assignable work elements.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

Completed project (R: ___ O: ___)1 359


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

12. I think about any aspects of this project or situations in the future that will facilitate
the project’s progress and performance.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

13. I recall any aspects of projects or situations in the past that have facilitated other
projects’ progress and performance.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

14. I list each and every step that I am going to take in order to complete the project.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

Completed project (R: ___ O: ___)1 360


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

Instructions:

Below are three descriptions of approaches that can be used when planning for a
project.

APPROACH A
When planning for an HRIS project, I focus on the kinds of events that are likely
to happen. I take into consideration all the problems that are likely to happen in
the future that will impede the project’s progress and performance, and any
aspect of the project or situations in the future that will facilitate the project’s
progress and performance.

APPROACH B
When planning for an HRIS project, I recall the kinds of events that have
happened in my past projects and those of other people that I know about. I also
recall all the problems that have happened in the past that have impeded my
project progress and performance, and any aspects of the project or situation in
the past that have facilitated my project progress and performance. Apart from
learning from my past experience, I also consult with other people who have
experience in similar projects.

APPROACH C
When planning for an HRIS project, I think about the components that are
needed for my project progress and performance. I also think about each and
everything that I plan to do in completing the project. I break down all the tasks
that need to be carried out in the project into specific tasks.

15. Please indicate which of the above best describes the approach you used:

A B C

Completed project (R: ___ O: ___)1 361


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

SECTION 3:

16. The project outcome:

 Falls short of expectations

 Meets expectations

 Exceeds expectations

17. Considering that 100% is the outcome you planned to achieve, please indicate (in
percentage terms) the extent to which the HRIS project outcome meets your
expectations. Please make a mark across the line to indicate your answer.

50% 100% 150%

For the next set of questions, please think about the same HRIS project. Using
the following scale, please tick √( ) the given box that represents your most
appropriate answer.

18. The output is presented in a useful format.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

19. I am satisfied with the accuracy of the system.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

20. The information is clear.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

Completed project (R: ___ O: ___)1 362


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

21. The system is accurate.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

22. The system provides sufficient information.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

23. The system provides up-to-date information.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

24. I get the information I need in time.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

25. The system provides reports that seem to be just about exactly what I need.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

26. The system is easy to use.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

27. The system is user friendly.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

Completed project (R: ___ O: ___)1 363


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

28. The system provides the precise information I need.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

29. The information content meets my needs.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

For the next question, please think about the same HRIS project. Using the
following scale, please place a single slash mark across the line in a position that
represents your personal opinion in answer to the question.

30. Overall, how satisfied are you with the system?

0 5 10
Not at all Poor Fair Good Extremely

31. What aspects of the system, if any, are you MOST satisfied with?

______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________

32. What aspect of the systems, if any, are you LEAST satisfied with?

______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
Completed project (R: ___ O: ___)1 364
HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

SECTION 4:

Questions 33 – 37 are about your feelings toward your HRIS project outcome.
Using the following scale, please tick ( √ ) the given box that represents your most
appropriate answer.

33. How do you view your HRIS project outcome?

 A successful project  Not a successful project

If it is A SUCCESSFUL PROJECT, please answer questions 34 AND 35.

If it is NOT A SUCCESSFUL PROJECT, please answer question 36 AND 37.

34. To what degree do you feel good about your HRIS outcome after this project has
done well?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
Not good very good

35. To what degree do you feel good about your HRIS outcome after the success of this
project?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
Not good very good

36. To what degree do you feel bad about your HRIS outcome after this project has
done poorly?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
Very bad not bad

37. To what degree do you feel bad about your HRIS outcome after the unsuccessful
project?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
Very bad not bad

Completed project (R: ___ O: ___)1 365


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

SECTION 5:

Please tick ( √ ) the given box or fill in the blank that represents your answer:

1. I am:

 Male  Female

2. My age is _________ years.

3. My ethnic origin is:

 Malay  Chinese  Indian  Other: ___________ (Please stated)

4. My education level:

 Secondary Education Certificate  Diploma

 First / Professional Degree  Second Degree and Above

5. My position in this organization: _________________

6. Number of years in present position: _______________

7. Number of years with present organization: ___________

8. Number of years experience as a computer-based system user: _________________

9. Number of years experience with HRIS projects: ___________________________

10. Estimate of total number of employees working in this organization: ____________

11. Estimate of total number of employees in your Human Resource Department: _____

12. Estimate of number of years of HRIS usage by your organization: ______________

Completed project (R: ___ O: ___)1 366


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

13. The idea for the system described in question 1 came from (check the most
important source):

________ Sources internal to the organization (specify which):

______ Top management of the organization

______ Information system department

______ HR department

______Other (please specify)__________________________________

________ Sources external to the organization (specify which):

______ Customers

______ Suppliers

______ Observation of competitors

______Other (please specify) _________________________________

14. Check the HR computer-based applications presently implemented by your


organization:

 Payroll operation  Training and career development

 Compensation and benefits  Human resource planning

 Performance appraisal Recruitment and selection

 Employee / labor relations  Others: (please specify) __________________

15. The system described in question 1 has been:

 Developed internally  Acquired from vendor  Both of these

16. If acquired from vendor, please state vendor’s company name (Optional question):

__________________________________________________________________

Thank you for taking the time to complete this survey.

Completed project (R: ___ O: ___)1 367


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success
SECTION 1:

Instructions:

We would like you to briefly describe about the HRIS project that you have been
involved in. Please answer questions 1 to 5 as accurately as possible.

1. Name and nature of the project (e.g. new HRIS, upgrading payroll system, adding a
new module)

______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________

2. The project was completed on? _______________

3. When was the project started? ________________

4. Was the project finished by the due date? Please tick (√ ) one of the following.

The project was completed -

 Early  On time  Late

If you responded EARLY, please state how early:

Number of days before the due date: __________ months _________ days

If you responded LATE, please state how late:

Number of days after the due date: ___________ months _________ days

5. Please describe your involvement in the project in terms of your role and your
contribution.

______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________

Completed project (R: ___ O: ___)2 368


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

SECTION 2:

Instructions:

To answer questions 6 to 14, please think about the project that you have mentioned in
questions 1 to 5. Please read carefully each of the activity statements below, and
indicate how much you agree that the statement describes an activity you did in
accomplishing the project.

AS I WORK ON THIS PROJECT:

6. I list each and every step that I am going to take in order to complete the project.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

7. I recall any aspects of projects or situations in the past that have facilitated other
projects’ progress and performance.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

8. I think about any aspects of this project or situations in the future that will facilitate
the project’s progress and performance.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

9. I breakdown the overall project into assignable work elements.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

10. I recall all the problems that have happened in the past that have impeded other
projects’ progress and performance.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

11. I think about all the problems that are likely to happen in the future that will impede
the project’s progress and performance.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree
Completed project (R: ___ O: ___)2 369
HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

12. I think about the components or elements that need to be included in the project.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

13. I recall the kinds of events that have happened in my past projects and those of other
people that I know about.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

14. I think about the kind of events that are likely to happen in this project.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

Completed project (R: ___ O: ___)2 370


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

Instructions:

Below are three descriptions of approaches that can be used when planning for a
project.

APPROACH A
When planning for an HRIS project, I focus on the kinds of events that are likely
to happen. I take into consideration all the problems that are likely to happen in
the future that will impede the project’s progress and performance, and any
aspect of the project or situations in the future that will facilitate the project’s
progress and performance.

APPROACH B
When planning for an HRIS project, I recall the kinds of events that have
happened in my past projects and those of other people that I know about. I also
recall all the problems that have happened in the past that have impeded my
project progress and performance, and any aspects of the project or situation in
the past that have facilitated my project progress and performance. Apart from
learning from my past experience, I also consult with other people who have
experience in similar projects.

APPROACH C
When planning for an HRIS project, I think about the components that are
needed for my project progress and performance. I also think about each and
everything that I plan to do in completing the project. I break down all the tasks
that need to be carried out in the project into specific tasks.

15. Please indicate which of the above best describes the approach you used:

A B C

Completed project (R: ___ O: ___)2 371


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

SECTION 3:

16. The project outcome:

 Falls short of expectations

 Meets expectations

 Exceeds expectations

17. Considering that 100% is the outcome you planned to achieve, please indicate (in
percentage terms) the extent to which the HRIS project outcome meets your
expectations. Please make a mark across the line to indicate your answer.

50% 100% 150%

For the next set of questions, please think about the same HRIS project. Using
the following scale, please tick ( √ ) the given box that represents your most
appropriate answer.

18. The output is presented in a useful format.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

19. I am satisfied with the accuracy of the system.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

20. The information is clear.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

Completed project (R: ___ O: ___)2 372


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

21. The system is accurate.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

22. The system provides sufficient information.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

23. The system provides up-to-date information.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

24. I get the information I need in time.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

25. The system provides reports that seem to be just about exactly what I need.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

26. The system is easy to use.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

27. The system is user friendly.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

Completed project (R: ___ O: ___)2 373


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success
28. The system provides the precise information I need.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

29. The information content meets my needs.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

For the next question, please think about the same HRIS project. Using the
following scale, please place a single slash mark across the line in a position that
represents your personal opinion in answer to the question.

30. Overall, how satisfied are you with the system?

0 5 10
Not at all Poor Fair Good Extremely

31. What aspects of the system, if any, are you MOST satisfied with?

______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________

32. What aspect of the systems, if any, are you LEAST satisfied with?

______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________

Completed project (R: ___ O: ___)2 374


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success
SECTION 4:

Questions 33 – 37 are about your feelings toward your HRIS project outcome.
Using the following scale, please tick ( √ ) the given box that represents your most
appropriate answer.

33. How do you view your HRIS project outcome?

 A successful project  Not a successful project

If it is A SUCCESSFUL PROJECT, please answer questions 34 AND 35.

If it is NOT A SUCCESSFUL PROJECT, please answer question 36 AND 37.

34. To what degree do you feel good about your HRIS outcome after this project has
done well?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
Not good very good

35. To what degree do you feel good about your HRIS outcome after the success of this
project?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
Not good very good

36. To what degree do you feel bad about your HRIS outcome after this project has
done poorly?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
Very bad not bad

37. To what degree do you feel bad about your HRIS outcome after the unsuccessful
project?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
Very bad not bad

Completed project (R: ___ O: ___)2 375


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success
SECTION 5:

Please tick ( √ ) the given box or fill in the blank that represents your answer:

1. I am:

 Male  Female

2. My age is _________ years.

3. My ethnic origin is:

 Malay  Chinese  Indian  Other: ___________ (Please stated)

4. My education level:

 Secondary Education Certificate  Diploma

 First / Professional Degree  Second Degree and Above

5. My position in this organization: _________________

6. Number of years in present position: _______________

7. Number of years with present organization: ___________

8. Number of years experience as a computer-based system user: _________________

9. Number of years experience with HRIS projects: ___________________________

10. Estimate of total number of employees working in this organization: ____________

11. Estimate of total number of employees in your Human Resource Department: _____

12. Estimate of number of years of HRIS usage by your organization: ______________

Completed project (R: ___ O: ___)2 376


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success
13. The idea for the system described in question 1 came from (check the most
important source):

________ Sources internal to the organization (specify which):

______ Top management of the organization

______ Information system department

______ HR department

______Other (please specify)__________________________________

________ Sources external to the organization (specify which):

______ Customers

______ Suppliers

______ Observation of competitors

______Other (please specify) _________________________________

14. Check the HR computer-based applications presently implemented by your


organization:

 Payroll operation  Training and career development

 Compensation and benefits  Human resource planning

 Performance appraisal Recruitment and selection

 Employee / labor relations  Others: (please specify) __________________

15. The system described in question 1 has been:

 Developed internally  Acquired from vendor  Both of these

If acquired from vendor, please state vendor’s company name (Optional question):

__________________________________________________________________

Thank you for taking the time to complete this survey.

Completed project (R: ___ O: ___)2 377


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

SECTION 1:

Instructions:

We would like you to briefly describe about the HRIS project that you have been
involved in. Please answer questions 1 to 5 as accurately as possible.

1. Name and nature of the project (e.g. new HRIS, upgrading payroll system, adding a
new module)

______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________

2. The project was completed on? _______________

3. When was the project started? ________________

4. Was the project finished by the due date? Please tick (√ ) one of the following.

The project was completed -

 Early  On time  Late

If you responded EARLY, please state how early:

Number of days before the due date: __________ months _________ days

If you responded LATE, please state how late:

Number of days after the due date: ___________ months _________ days

5. Please describe your involvement in the project in terms of your role and your
contribution.

______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
Completed project (R: ___ O: ___)3 378
HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

SECTION 2:

Instructions:

To answer questions 6 to 14, please think about the project that you have mentioned in
questions 1 to 5. Please read carefully each of the activity statements below, and
indicate how much you agree that the statement describes an activity you did in
accomplishing the project.

AS I WORK ON THIS PROJECT:

6. I think about the kind of events that are likely to happen in this project.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

7. I recall the kinds of events that have happened in my past projects and those of other
people that I know about.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

8. I think about the components or elements that need to be included in the project.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

9. I think about all the problems that are likely to happen in the future that will impede
this project’s progress and performance.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

10. I recall all the problems that have happened in the past that have impeded other
projects’ progress and performance.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

11. I breakdown the overall project into assignable work elements.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

Completed project (R: ___ O: ___)3 379


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

12. I think about any aspects of this project or situations in the future that will facilitate
the project’s progress and performance.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

13. I recall any aspects of projects or situations in the past that have facilitated other
projects’ progress and performance.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

14. I list each and every step that I am going to take in order to complete the project.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

Completed project (R: ___ O: ___)3 380


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

Instructions:

Below are three descriptions of approaches that can be used when planning for a
project.

APPROACH A
When planning for an HRIS project, I think about the components that are
needed for my project progress and performance. I also think about each and
everything that I plan to do in completing the project. I break down all the tasks
that need to be carried out in the project into specific tasks.

APPROACH B
When planning for an HRIS project, I recall the kinds of events that have
happened in my past projects and those of other people that I know about. I also
recall all the problems that have happened in the past that have impeded my
project progress and performance, and any aspects of the project or situation in
the past that have facilitated my project progress and performance. Apart from
learning from my past experience, I also consult with other people who have
experience in similar projects.

APPROACH C
When planning for an HRIS project, I focus on the kinds of events that are likely
to happen. I take into consideration all the problems that are likely to happen in
the future that will impede the project’s progress and performance, and any
aspect of the project or situations in the future that will facilitate the project’s
progress and performance.

15. Please indicate which of the above best describes the approach you used:

A B C

Completed project (R: ___ O: ___)3 381


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

SECTION 3:

16. The project outcome:

 Falls short of expectations

 Meets expectations

 Exceeds expectations

17. Considering that 100% is the outcome you planned to achieve, please indicate (in
percentage terms) the extent to which the HRIS project outcome meets your
expectations. Please make a mark across the line to indicate your answer.

50% 100% 150%

For the next set of questions, please think about the same HRIS project. Using
the following scale, please tick ( √ ) the given box that represents your most
appropriate answer.

18. The output is presented in a useful format.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

19. I am satisfied with the accuracy of the system.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

20. The information is clear.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

Completed project (R: ___ O: ___)3 382


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

21. The system is accurate.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

22. The system provides sufficient information.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

23. The system provides up-to-date information.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

24. I get the information I need in time.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

25. The system provides reports that seem to be just about exactly what I need.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

26. The system is easy to use.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

27. The system is user friendly.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

Completed project (R: ___ O: ___)3 383


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

28. The system provides the precise information I need.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

29. The information content meets my needs.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

For the next question, please think about the same HRIS project. Using the
following scale, please place a single slash mark across the line in a position that
represents your personal opinion in answer to the question.

30. Overall, how satisfied are you with the system?

0 5 10
Not at all Poor Fair Good Extremely

31. What aspects of the system, if any, are you MOST satisfied with?

______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________

32. What aspect of the systems, if any, are you LEAST satisfied with?

______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
Completed project (R: ___ O: ___)3 384
HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

SECTION 4:

Questions 33 – 37 are about your feelings toward your HRIS project outcome.
Using the following scale, please tick ( √ ) the given box that represents your most
appropriate answer.

33. How do you view your HRIS project outcome?

 A successful project  Not a successful project

If it is A SUCCESSFUL PROJECT, please answer questions 34 AND 35.

If it is NOT A SUCCESSFUL PROJECT, please answer question 36 AND 37.

34. To what degree do you feel good about your HRIS outcome after this project has
done well?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
Not good very good

35. To what degree do you feel good about your HRIS outcome after the success of this
project?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
Not good very good

36. To what degree do you feel bad about your HRIS outcome after this project has
done poorly?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
Very bad not bad

37. To what degree do you feel bad about your HRIS outcome after the unsuccessful
project?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
Very bad not bad

Completed project (R: ___ O: ___)3 385


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

SECTION 5:

Please tick ( √ ) the given box or fill in the blank that represents your answer:

1. I am:

 Male  Female

2. My age is _________ years.

3. My ethnic origin is:

 Malay  Chinese  Indian  Other: ___________ (Please stated)

4. My education level:

 Secondary Education Certificate  Diploma

 First / Professional Degree  Second Degree and Above

5. My position in this organization: _________________

6. Number of years in present position: _______________

7. Number of years with present organization: ___________

8. Number of years experience as a computer-based system user: _________________

9. Number of years experience with HRIS projects: ___________________________

10. Estimate of total number of employees working in this organization: ____________

11. Estimate of total number of employees in your Human Resource Department: _____

12. Estimate of number of years of HRIS usage by your organization: ______________

Completed project (R: ___ O: ___)3 386


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

13. The idea for the system described in question 1 came from (check the most
important source):

________ Sources internal to the organization (specify which):

______ Top management of the organization

______ Information system department

______ HR department

______Other (please specify)__________________________________

________ Sources external to the organization (specify which):

______ Customers

______ Suppliers

______ Observation of competitors

______Other (please specify) _________________________________

14. Check the HR computer-based applications presently implemented by your


organization:

 Payroll operation  Training and career development

 Compensation and benefits  Human resource planning

 Performance appraisal Recruitment and selection

 Employee / labor relations  Others: (please specify) __________________

15. The system described in question 1 has been:

 Developed internally  Acquired from vendor  Both of these

If acquired from vendor, please state vendor’s company name (Optional question):

__________________________________________________________________

Thank you for taking the time to complete this survey.

Completed project (R: ___ O: ___)3 387


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

SECTION 1:

Instructions:

We would like you to briefly describe about the HRIS project that you have been
involved in. Please answer questions 1 to 5 as accurately as possible.

1. Name and nature of the project (e.g. new HRIS, upgrading payroll system, adding a
new module)

______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________

2. The project was completed on? _______________

3. When was the project started? ________________

4. Was the project finished by the due date? Please tick (√ ) one of the following.

The project was completed -

 Early  On time  Late

If you responded EARLY, please state how early:

Number of days before the due date: __________ months _________ days

If you responded LATE, please state how late:

Number of days after the due date: ___________ months _________ days

5. Please describe your involvement in the project in terms of your role and your
contribution.

______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
Completed project (R: ___ O: ___)4 388
HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

SECTION 2:

Instructions:

To answer questions 6 to 14, please think about the project that you have mentioned in
questions 1 to 5. Please read carefully each of the activity statements below, and
indicate how much you agree that the statement describes an activity you did in
accomplishing the project.

AS I WORK ON THIS PROJECT:

6. I list each and every step that I am going to take in order to complete the project.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

7. I recall any aspects of projects or situations in the past that have facilitated other
projects’ progress and performance.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

8. I think about any aspects of this project or situations in the future that will facilitate
the project’s progress and performance.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

9. I breakdown the overall project into assignable work elements.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

10. I recall all the problems that have happened in the past that have impeded other
projects’ progress and performance.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

11. I think about all the problems that are likely to happen in the future that will impede
the project’s progress and performance.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree
Completed project (R: ___ O: ___)4 389
HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

12. I think about the components or elements that need to be included in the project.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

13. I recall the kinds of events that have happened in my past projects and those of other
people that I know about.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

14. I think about the kind of events that are likely to happen in this project.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

Completed project (R: ___ O: ___)4 390


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

Instructions:

Below are three descriptions of approaches that can be used when planning for a
project.

APPROACH A
When planning for an HRIS project, I think about the components that are
needed for my project progress and performance. I also think about each and
everything that I plan to do in completing the project. I break down all the tasks
that need to be carried out in the project into specific tasks.

APPROACH B
When planning for an HRIS project, I recall the kinds of events that have
happened in my past projects and those of other people that I know about. I also
recall all the problems that have happened in the past that have impeded my
project progress and performance, and any aspects of the project or situation in
the past that have facilitated my project progress and performance. Apart from
learning from my past experience, I also consult with other people who have
experience in similar projects.

APPROACH C
When planning for an HRIS project, I focus on the kinds of events that are likely
to happen. I take into consideration all the problems that are likely to happen in
the future that will impede the project’s progress and performance, and any
aspect of the project or situations in the future that will facilitate the project’s
progress and performance.

15. Please indicate which of the above best describes the approach you used:

A B C

Completed project (R: ___ O: ___)4 391


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

SECTION 3:

16. The project outcome:

 Falls short of expectations

 Meets expectations

 Exceeds expectations

17. Considering that 100% is the outcome you planned to achieve, please indicate (in
percentage terms) the extent to which the HRIS project outcome meets your
expectations. Please make a mark across the line to indicate your answer.

50% 100% 150%

For the next set of questions, please think about the same HRIS project. Using
the following scale, please tick ( √ ) the given box that represents your most
appropriate answer.

18. The output is presented in a useful format.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

19. I am satisfied with the accuracy of the system.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

20. The information is clear.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

Completed project (R: ___ O: ___)4 392


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

21. The system is accurate.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

22. The system provides sufficient information.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

23. The system provides up-to-date information.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

24. I get the information I need in time.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

25. The system provides reports that seem to be just about exactly what I need.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

26. The system is easy to use.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

27. The system is user friendly.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

Completed project (R: ___ O: ___)4 393


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

28. The system provides the precise information I need.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

29. The information content meets my needs.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

For the next question, please think about the same HRIS project. Using the
following scale, please place a single slash mark across the line in a position that
represents your personal opinion in answer to the question.

30. Overall, how satisfied are you with the system?

0 5 10
Not at all Poor Fair Good Extremely

31. What aspects of the system, if any, are you MOST satisfied with?

______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________

32. What aspect of the systems, if any, are you LEAST satisfied with?

______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
Completed project (R: ___ O: ___)4 394
HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

SECTION 4:

Questions 33 – 37 are about your feelings toward your HRIS project outcome.
Using the following scale, please tick ( √ ) the given box that represents your most
appropriate answer.

33. How do you view your HRIS project outcome?

 A successful project  Not a successful project

If it is A SUCCESSFUL PROJECT, please answer questions 34 AND 35.

If it is NOT A SUCCESSFUL PROJECT, please answer question 36 AND 37.

34. To what degree do you feel good about your HRIS outcome after this project has
done well?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
Not good very good

35. To what degree do you feel good about your HRIS outcome after the success of this
project?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
Not good very good

36. To what degree do you feel bad about your HRIS outcome after this project has
done poorly?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
Very bad not bad

37. To what degree do you feel bad about your HRIS outcome after the unsuccessful
project?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
Very bad not bad

Completed project (R: ___ O: ___)4 395


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

SECTION 5:

Please tick ( √ ) the given box or fill in the blank that represents your answer:

1. I am:

 Male  Female

2. My age is _________ years.

3. My ethnic origin is:

 Malay  Chinese  Indian  Other: ___________ (Please stated)

4. My education level:

 Secondary Education Certificate  Diploma

 First / Professional Degree  Second Degree and Above

5. My position in this organization: _________________

6. Number of years in present position: _______________

7. Number of years with present organization: ___________

8. Number of years experience as a computer-based system user: _________________

9. Number of years experience with HRIS projects: ___________________________

10. Estimate of total number of employees working in this organization: ____________

11. Estimate of total number of employees in your Human Resource Department: _____

12. Estimate of number of years of HRIS usage by your organization: ______________

Completed project (R: ___ O: ___)4 396


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

13. The idea for the system described in question 1 came from (check the most
important source):

________ Sources internal to the organization (specify which):

______ Top management of the organization

______ Information system department

______ HR department

______Other (please specify)__________________________________

________ Sources external to the organization (specify which):

______ Customers

______ Suppliers

______ Observation of competitors

______Other (please specify) _________________________________

14. Check the HR computer-based applications presently implemented by your


organization:

 Payroll operation  Training and career development

 Compensation and benefits  Human resource planning

 Performance appraisal Recruitment and selection

 Employee / labor relations  Others: (please specify) __________________

15. The system described in question 1 has been:

 Developed internally  Acquired from vendor  Both of these

If acquired from vendor, please state vendor’s company name (Optional question):

__________________________________________________________________

Thank you for taking the time to complete this survey.

Completed project (R: ___ O: ___)4 397


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

SECTION 1:

Instructions:

We would like you to briefly describe about the HRIS project that you are currently
involved in. Please answer questions 1 to 5 as accurately as possible.

1. Name and nature of the project (e.g. new HRIS, upgrading payroll system, adding a
new module)

______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________

2. The project is scheduled to be completed on _______________

3. The project started (or will start) on _______________

4. I predict that this project will finish on ______________

5. Please describe your involvement in the project in terms of your role and your
contribution.

______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
At planning stage (R: __ O: __)1 398
HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

SECTION 2:

Instructions:

To answer questions 6 to 14, please think about the project that you have mentioned in
questions 1 to 5. Please read carefully each of the activity statements below, and
indicate how much you agree that the statement describes an activity you are doing in
accomplishing the project.

AS I WORK ON THIS PROJECT:

6. I think about the kind of events that are likely to happen in this project.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

7. I recall the kinds of events that have happened in my past projects and those of other
people that I know about.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

8. I think about the components or elements that need to be included in the project.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

9. I think about all the problems that are likely to happen in the future that will impede
the project’s progress and performance.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

10. I recall all the problems that have happened in the past that have impeded other
projects’ progress and performance.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

11. I breakdown the overall project into assignable work elements.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

At planning stage (R: __ O: __)1 399


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

12. I think about any aspects of this project or situations in the future that will facilitate
the project’s progress and performance.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

13. I recall any aspects of projects or situations in the past that have facilitated other
projects’ progress and performance.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

14. I list each and every step that I am going to take in order to complete the project.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

At planning stage (R: __ O: __)1 400


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

Instructions:

Below are three descriptions of approaches that can be used when planning for a
project.

APPROACH A
When planning for an HRIS project, I focus on the kinds of events that are likely
to happen. I take into the consideration all the problems that are likely to happen
in the future that will impede the project’s progress and performance, and any
aspects of the project or situations in the future that will facilitate the project’s
progress and performance.

APPROACH B
When planning for an HRIS project, I recall the kinds of events that have
happened in my past projects and those of other people that I know about. I also
recall all the problems that have happened in the past that have impeded my
project progress and performance, and any aspects of the project or situations in
the past that have facilitated my project progress and performance. Apart from
learning from my past experience, I also consult with other people who have
experience in similar projects.

APPROACH C
When planning for an HRIS project, I think about the components that are
needed for my project’s progress and performance. I also think about each and
everything that I plan to do in completing the project. I break down all the tasks
that need to be carried out in the project into specific tasks.

15. Please indicate which of the above best describes the approach you are using:

A B C

At planning stage (R: __ O: __)1 401


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

SECTION 3:

16. The project outcome will:

 Fall short of expectations

 Meet expectations

 Exceed expectations

17. Considering that 100% is the outcome you plan to achieve, please indicate (in
percentage terms) the extent to which the HRIS project outcome will meet your
expectations. Please make a mark across the line to indicate your answer.

50% 100% 150%

At planning stage (R: __ O: __)1 402


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

SECTION 4:

Questions 18 – 21 are about your feelings toward your HRIS project outcome.
Using the following scale, please tick√( ) the given box that represents your
most appropriate answer.

18. How would you feel if this project does well?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
Not good very good

19. How would you feel if this project succeeds?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
Not good very good

20. How would you feel if this project does poorly?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
Very bad not bad

21. How would you feel if this project does not succeed?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
Very bad not bad

At planning stage (R: __ O: __)1 403


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

SECTION 5:

Please tick ( √ ) the given box or fill in the blank that represents your answer:

1. I am:

 Male  Female

2. My age is _________ years.

3. My ethnic origin is:

 Malay  Chinese  Indian  Other: ___________ (Please stated)

4. My education level:

 Secondary Education  Certificate  Diploma

 First / Professional Degree  Second Degree and Above

5. My position in this organization: _________________

6. Number of years in present position: _______________

7. Number of years with present organization: ___________

8. Number of years experience as a computer-based system user: _________________

9. Number of years experience with HRIS projects: ___________________________

10. Estimate of total number of employees working in this organization: ____________

11. Estimate of total number of employees in your Human Resource Department: _____

12. Estimate of number of years of HRIS usage by your organization: _____________

At planning stage (R: __ O: __)1 404


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

13. The idea for the system described in question 1 came from (check the most
important source):

________ Sources internal to the organization (specify which):

______ Top management of the organization

______ Information system department

______ HR department

______ Other (please specify) _________________________________

________ Sources external to the organization (specify which):

______ Customers

______ Suppliers

______ Observation of competitors

______ Other (please specify) _________________________________

14. Check the HR computer-based applications presently implemented by your


organization:

 Payroll operation  Training and career development

 Compensation and benefits  Human resource planning

 Performance appraisal Recruitment and selection

 Employee / labor relations  Other (please specify) __________________

15. The system proposed in question 1 will be:

 Developed internally  Acquired from vendor  Both of these

If acquired from vendor, please state vendor’s company name (Optional question):

________________________________________________________________

Thank you for taking the time to complete this survey.

At planning stage (R: __ O: __)1 405


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

SECTION 1:

Instructions:

We would like you to briefly describe about the HRIS project that you are currently
involved in. Please answer questions 1 to 5 as accurately as possible.

1. Name and nature of the project (e.g. new HRIS, upgrading payroll system, adding a
new module)

______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________

2. The project is scheduled to be completed on _______________

3. The project started (or will start) on _______________

4. I predict that this project will finish on ______________

5. Please describe your involvement in the project in terms of your role and your
contribution.

______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
At planning stage (R: __ O: __)2 406
HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

SECTION 2:

Instructions:

To answer questions 6 to 14, please think about the project that you have mentioned in
questions 1 to 5. Please read carefully each of the activity statements below, and
indicate how much you agree that the statement describes an activity you are doing in
accomplishing the project.

AS I WORK ON THIS PROJECT:

6. I list each and every step that I am going to take in order to complete the project.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

7. I recall any aspects of projects or situations in the past that have facilitated other
projects’ progress and performance.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

8. I think about any aspects of this project or situations in the future that will facilitate
the project’s progress and performance.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

9. I breakdown the overall project into assignable work elements.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

10. I recall all the problems that have happened in the past that have impeded other
projects’ progress and performance.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

11. I think about all the problems that are likely to happen in the future that will impede
the project’s progress and performance.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

At planning stage (R: __ O: __)2 407


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

12. I think about the components or elements that need to be included in the project.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

13. I recall the kind of events that have happened in my past projects and those of other
people that I know about.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

14. I think about the kind of events that are likely to happen in this project.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

At planning stage (R: __ O: __)2 408


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

Instructions:

Below are three descriptions of approaches that can be used when planning for a
project.

APPROACH A
When planning for an HRIS project, I focus on the kinds of events that are likely
to happen. I take into the consideration all the problems that are likely to happen
in the future that will impede the project’s progress and performance, and any
aspects of the project or situations in the future that will facilitate the project’s
progress and performance.

APPROACH B
When planning for an HRIS project, I recall the kinds of events that have
happened in my past projects and those of other people that I know about. I also
recall all the problems that have happened in the past that have impeded my
project progress and performance, and any aspects of the project or situations in
the past that have facilitated my project progress and performance. Apart from
learning from my past experience, I also consult with other people who have
experience in similar projects.

APPROACH C
When planning for an HRIS project, I think about the components that are
needed for my project’s progress and performance. I also think about each and
everything that I plan to do in completing the project. I break down all the tasks
that need to be carried out in the project into specific tasks.

15. Please indicate which of the above best describes the approach you are using:

A B C

At planning stage (R: __ O: __)2 409


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

SECTION 3:

16. The project outcome will:

 Fall short of expectations

 Meet expectations

 Exceed expectations

17. Considering that 100% is the outcome you plan to achieve, please indicate (in
percentage terms) the extent to which the HRIS project outcome will meet your
expectations. Please make a mark across the line to indicate your answer.

50% 100% 150%

At planning stage (R: __ O: __)2 410


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

SECTION 4:

Questions 18 – 21 are about your feelings toward your HRIS project outcome.
Using the following scale, please tick√( ) the given box that represents your
most appropriate answer.

18. How would you feel if this project does well?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
Not good very good

19. How would you feel if this project succeeds?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
Not good very good

20. How would you feel if this project does poorly?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
Very bad not bad

21. How would you feel if this project does not succeed?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
Very bad not bad

At planning stage (R: __ O: __)2 411


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

SECTION 5:

Please tick ( √ ) the given box or fill in the blank that represents your answer:

1. I am:

 Male  Female

2. My age is _________ years.

3. My ethnic origin is:

 Malay  Chinese  Indian  Other: ___________ (Please stated)

4. My education level:

 Secondary Education  Certificate  Diploma

 First / Professional Degree  Second Degree and Above

5. My position in this organization: _________________

6. Number of years in present position: _______________

7. Number of years with present organization: ___________

8. Number of years experience as a computer-based system user: _________________

9. Number of years experience with HRIS projects: ___________________________

10. Estimate of total number of employees working in this organization: ____________

11. Estimate of total number of employees in your Human Resource Department: _____

12. Estimate of number of years of HRIS usage by your organization: _____________

At planning stage (R: __ O: __)2 412


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

13. The idea for the system described in question 1 came from (check the most
important source):

________ Sources internal to the organization (specify which):

______ Top management of the organization

______ Information system department

______ HR department

______ Other (please specify) _________________________________

________ Sources external to the organization (specify which):

______ Customers

______ Suppliers

______ Observation of competitors

______ Other (please specify) _________________________________

14. Check the HR computer-based applications presently implemented by your


organization:

 Payroll operation  Training and career development

 Compensation and benefits  Human resource planning

 Performance appraisal Recruitment and selection

 Employee / labor relations  Other (please specify) __________________

15. The system proposed in question 1 will be:

 Developed internally  Acquired from vendor  Both of these

If acquired from vendor, please state vendor’s company name (Optional question):

________________________________________________________________

Thank you for taking the time to complete this survey.

At planning stage (R: __ O: __)2 413


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

SECTION 1:

Instructions:

We would like you to briefly describe about the HRIS project that you are currently
involved in. Please answer questions 1 to 5 as accurately as possible.

1. Name and nature of the project (e.g. new HRIS, upgrading payroll system, adding a
new module)

______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________

2. The project is scheduled to be completed on _______________

3. The project started (or will start) on _______________

4. I predict that this project will finish on ______________

5. Please describe your involvement in the project in terms of your role and your
contribution.

______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
At planning stage (R: __ O: __)3 414
HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

SECTION 2:

Instructions:

To answer questions 6 to 14, please think about the project that you have mentioned in
questions 1 to 5. Please read carefully each of the activity statements below, and
indicate how much you agree that the statement describes an activity you are doing in
accomplishing the project.

AS I WORK ON THIS PROJECT:

6. I list each and every step that I am going to take in order to complete the project.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

7. I recall any aspects of projects or situations in the past that have facilitated other
projects’ progress and performance.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

8. I think about any aspects of this project or situations in the future that will facilitate
the project’s progress and performance.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

9. I breakdown the overall project into assignable work elements.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

10. I recall all the problems that have happened in the past that have impeded other
projects’ progress and performance.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

11. I think about all the problems that are likely to happen in the future that will impede
the project’s progress and performance.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

At planning stage (R: __ O: __)3 415


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

12. I think about the components or elements that need to be included in the project.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

13. I recall the kind of events that have happened in my past projects and those of other
people that I know about.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

14. I think about the kind of events that are likely to happen in this project.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

At planning stage (R: __ O: __)3 416


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

Instructions:

Below are three descriptions of approaches that can be used when planning for a
project.

APPROACH A
When planning for an HRIS project, I think about the components that are
needed for my project’s progress and performance. I also think about each and
everything that I plan to do in completing the project. I break down all the tasks
that need to be carried out in the project into specific tasks.

APPROACH B
When planning for an HRIS project, I recall the kinds of events that have
happened in my past projects and those of other people that I know about. I also
recall all the problems that have happened in the past that have impeded my
project progress and performance, and any aspects of the project or situations in
the past that have facilitated my project progress and performance. Apart from
learning from my past experience, I also consult with other people who have
experience in similar projects.

APPROACH C
When planning for an HRIS project, I focus on the kinds of events that are likely
to happen. I take into the consideration all the problems that are likely to happen
in the future that will impede the project’s progress and performance, and any
aspects of the project or situations in the future that will facilitate the project’s
progress and performance.

15. Please indicate which of the above best describes the approach you are using:

A B C

At planning stage (R: __ O: __)3 417


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

SECTION 3:

16. The project outcome will:

 Fall short of expectations

 Meet expectations

 Exceed expectations

17. Considering that 100% is the outcome you plan to achieve, please indicate (in
percentage terms) the extent to which the HRIS project outcome will meet your
expectations. Please make a mark across the line to indicate your answer.

50% 100% 150%

At planning stage (R: __ O: __)3 418


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

SECTION 4:

Questions 18 – 21 are about your feelings toward your HRIS project outcome.
Using the following scale, please tick√( ) the given box that represents your
most appropriate answer.

18. How would you feel if this project does well?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
Not good very good

19. How would you feel if this project succeeds?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
Not good very good

20. How would you feel if this project does poorly?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
Very bad not bad

21. How would you feel if this project does not succeed?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
Very bad not bad

At planning stage (R: __ O: __)3 419


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

SECTION 5:

Please tick ( √ ) the given box or fill in the blank that represents your answer:

1. I am:

 Male  Female

2. My age is _________ years.

3. My ethnic origin is:

 Malay  Chinese  Indian  Other: ___________ (Please stated)

4. My education level:

 Secondary Education  Certificate  Diploma

 First / Professional Degree  Second Degree and Above

5. My position in this organization: _________________

6. Number of years in present position: _______________

7. Number of years with present organization: ___________

8. Number of years experience as a computer-based system user: _________________

9. Number of years experience with HRIS projects: ___________________________

10. Estimate of total number of employees working in this organization: ____________

11. Estimate of total number of employees in your Human Resource Department: _____

12. Estimate of number of years of HRIS usage by your organization: _____________

At planning stage (R: __ O: __)3 420


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

13. The idea for the system described in question 1 came from (check the most
important source):

________ Sources internal to the organization (specify which):

______ Top management of the organization

______ Information system department

______ HR department

______ Other (please specify) _________________________________

________ Sources external to the organization (specify which):

______ Customers

______ Suppliers

______ Observation of competitors

______ Other (please specify) _________________________________

14. Check the HR computer-based applications presently implemented by your


organization:

 Payroll operation  Training and career development

 Compensation and benefits  Human resource planning

 Performance appraisal Recruitment and selection

 Employee / labor relations  Other (please specify) __________________

15. The system proposed in question 1 will be:

 Developed internally  Acquired from vendor  Both of these

If acquired from vendor, please state vendor’s company name (Optional question):

________________________________________________________________

Thank you for taking the time to complete this survey.

At planning stage (R: __ O: __)3 421


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success
SECTION 1:

Instructions:

We would like you to briefly describe about the HRIS project that you are currently
involved in. Please answer questions 1 to 5 as accurately as possible.

1. Name and nature of the project (e.g. new HRIS, upgrading payroll system, adding a
new module)

______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________

2. The project is scheduled to be completed on _______________

3. The project started (or will start) on _______________

4. I predict that this project will finish on ______________

5. Please describe your involvement in the project in terms of your role and your
contribution.

______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________

At planning stage (R: __ O: __)4 422


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success
SECTION 2:

Instructions:

To answer questions 6 to 14, please think about the project that you have mentioned in
questions 1 to 5. Please read carefully each of the activity statements below, and
indicate how much you agree that the statement describes an activity you are doing in
accomplishing the project.

AS I WORK ON THIS PROJECT:

6. I list each and every step that I am going to take in order to complete the project.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

7. I recall any aspects of projects or situations in the past that have facilitated other
projects’ progress and performance.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

8. I think about any aspects of this project or situations in the future that will facilitate
the project’s progress and performance.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

9. I breakdown the overall project into assignable work elements.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

10. I recall all the problems that have happened in the past that have impeded other
projects’ progress and performance.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

11. I think about all the problems that are likely to happen in the future that will impede
the project’s progress and performance.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

At planning stage (R: __ O: __)4 423


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success
12. I think about the components or elements that need to be included in the project.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

13. I recall the kind of events that have happened in my past projects and those of other
people that I know about.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

14. I think about the kind of events that are likely to happen in this project.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Strongly Strongly
Disagree Agree

At planning stage (R: __ O: __)4 424


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success
Instructions:

Below are three descriptions of approaches that can be used when planning for a
project.

APPROACH A
When planning for an HRIS project, I think about the components that are
needed for my project’s progress and performance. I also think about each and
everything that I plan to do in completing the project. I break down all the tasks
that need to be carried out in the project into specific tasks.

APPROACH B
When planning for an HRIS project, I recall the kinds of events that have
happened in my past projects and those of other people that I know about. I also
recall all the problems that have happened in the past that have impeded my
project progress and performance, and any aspects of the project or situations in
the past that have facilitated my project progress and performance. Apart from
learning from my past experience, I also consult with other people who have
experience in similar projects.

APPROACH C
When planning for an HRIS project, I focus on the kinds of events that are likely
to happen. I take into the consideration all the problems that are likely to happen
in the future that will impede the project’s progress and performance, and any
aspects of the project or situations in the future that will facilitate the project’s
progress and performance.

15. Please indicate which of the above best describes the approach you are using:

A B C

At planning stage (R: __ O: __)4 425


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

SECTION 3:

16. The project outcome will:

 Fall short of expectations

 Meet expectations

 Exceed expectations

17. Considering that 100% is the outcome you plan to achieve, please indicate (in
percentage terms) the extent to which the HRIS project outcome will meet your
expectations. Please make a mark across the line to indicate your answer.

50% 100% 150%

At planning stage (R: __ O: __)4 426


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

SECTION 4:

Questions 18 – 21 are about your feelings toward your HRIS project outcome.
Using the following scale, please tick√( ) the given box that represents your
most appropriate answer.

18. How would you feel if this project does well?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
Not good very good

19. How would you feel if this project succeeds?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
Not good very good

20. How would you feel if this project does poorly?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
Very bad not bad

21. How would you feel if this project does not succeed?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
Very bad not bad

At planning stage (R: __ O: __)4 427


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success

SECTION 5:

Please tick ( √ ) the given box or fill in the blank that represents your answer:

1. I am:

 Male  Female

2. My age is _________ years.

3. My ethnic origin is:

 Malay  Chinese  Indian  Other: ___________ (Please stated)

4. My education level:

 Secondary Education  Certificate  Diploma

 First / Professional Degree  Second Degree and Above

5. My position in this organization: _________________

6. Number of years in present position: _______________

7. Number of years with present organization: ___________

8. Number of years experience as a computer-based system user: _________________

9. Number of years experience with HRIS projects: ___________________________

10. Estimate of total number of employees working in this organization: ____________

11. Estimate of total number of employees in your Human Resource Department: _____

12. Estimate of number of years of HRIS usage by your organization: _____________

At planning stage (R: __ O: __)4 428


HRIS Project Planning and Project Success
13. The idea for the system described in question 1 came from (check the most
important source):

________ Sources internal to the organization (specify which):

______ Top management of the organization

______ Information system department

______ HR department

______ Other (please specify) _________________________________

________ Sources external to the organization (specify which):

______ Customers

______ Suppliers

______ Observation of competitors

______ Other (please specify) _________________________________

14. Check the HR computer-based applications presently implemented by your


organization:

 Payroll operation  Training and career development

 Compensation and benefits  Human resource planning

 Performance appraisal Recruitment and selection

 Employee / labor relations  Other (please specify) __________________

15. The system proposed in question 1 will be:

 Developed internally  Acquired from vendor  Both of these

If acquired from vendor, please state vendor’s company name (Optional question):

________________________________________________________________

Thank you for taking the time to complete this survey.

At planning stage (R: __ O: __)4 429


APPENDIX H

OPEN-ENDED QUESTION MATERIALS – STUDY 2

This appendix contains copies of the open-ended materials provided to two independent

individuals for sorting processes: two sets of instruction, three open-ended questions

and three sets of answer sheet.

430
OPEN-ENDED QUESTIONS

Instruction:

Please sort the answer as best as you can into five categories of heading. You can use

the ‘other’ category if you think the answer does not fit with the five categories

mentioned. You are allowed to go back and revise your answer as many times as you

like until you are satisfied with your answer. Please indicate at the end of the answer

sheet, the amount of time you spend to finish this task.

To guide you with the sorting process, below are definitions for each category:

System Planning – often a two-step process – requirement definition and feasibility

analysis. Requirement definition involves several types of investigation to determine

what kinds of data, analysis, security, reports, and other features users need. Feasibility

analysis estimates the resources required to achieve those objectives. It includes

hardware cost, vendor charges for software, facilities requirements, consultant time,

custom software development, documentation revisions and user training.

System Design – the design process has three components: (1) define data needs by

designing data structure, content and control; (2) select, create or adapt software that

will store, process, and provide those data as required; and (3) select hardware on which

that software will function properly or locate software that will run on hardware already

in place.

Vendor Selection – entails identifying potential vendors, communicating with them

clearly and thoroughly, and evaluating their proposals.

431
System Implementation – is a process of bringing the HRIS from a design state to an

operational state. It usually involves a number of overlapping processes such as

implementation planning, policy and procedure development, project team training,

installation, modification, interfaces, conversion, user training, controlled testing and

parallel testing.

System Maintenance – occurs throughout a systems’ life cycle and include any work

done on the system after delivery and operational acceptance. There are three categories

of maintenance: (1) corrective maintenance – fixing problems that prevent the system

from working the way designers and users intended it to work; (2) adaptive maintenance

– modifications to HRIS in responses to changes in technology, government regulations

or external forces such as fixes or new system releases from the vendor; and (3)

perfective maintenance – modifying the system to respond to changes and requests from

users and technicians.

432
Please describe your involvement in the project in terms of your role and your

contribution.

433
INVOLVEMENT IN THE PROJECT

Respondent System planning System Design Vendor Selection System System Maintenance
Implementation and Evaluation
Respondent 1
Respondent 2
Respondent 3
Respondent 4
Respondent 5
Respondent 6
Respondent 7
Respondent 8
Respondent 9
Respondent 10
Respondent 11
Respondent 12
Respondent 13
Respondent 14
Respondent 15
Respondent 16
Respondent 17
Respondent 18

434
Respondent System planning System Design Vendor Selection System System Maintenance
Implementation and Evaluation
Respondent 19
Respondent 20
Respondent 21
Respondent 22
Respondent 23
Respondent 24
Respondent 25
Respondent 26
Respondent 27
Respondent 28
Respondent 29
Respondent 30
Respondent 31
Respondent 32
Respondent 33
Respondent 34
Respondent 35
Respondent 36
Respondent 37
Respondent 38

435
Respondent System planning System Design Vendor Selection System System Maintenance
Implementation and Evaluation
Respondent 39
Respondent 40
Respondent 41
Respondent 42
Respondent 43
Respondent 44
Respondent 45

How long it takes you to finish this task? _______________

436
OPEN-ENDED QUESTIONS

Instruction:

Please sort the answer as best as you can into five categories of heading. You can use

the ‘other’ category if you think the answer does not fit with the five categories

mentioned. You are allowed to go back and revise your answer as many times as you

like until you are satisfied with your answer. Please indicate at the end of the answer

sheet, the amount of time you spend to finish this task.

To guide you with the sorting process, below are definitions for each category:

Content – is referring to the information or report generated by the system, whether the

information or reports are precise, sufficient and exactly as one needed.

Accuracy – is referring to the correctness of the output information.

Format – is referring to the way the output is being presented, whether it is clear and in

a useful format.

Ease of use – is referring to the easiness and user-friendliness of using the application.

Timeliness – is referring to how fast you can get the information you need and whether

the system provides up-to-date information.

437
What aspects of the system, if any, are you MOST satisfied with?

438
What aspects of the system, if any, are you LEAST satisfied with?

439
MOST SATISFIED

Respondent Content Format Accuracy Ease of use Timeliness Others


Respondent 1
Respondent 2
Respondent 3
Respondent 4
Respondent 5
Respondent 6
Respondent 7
Respondent 8
Respondent 9
Respondent 10
Respondent 11
Respondent 12
Respondent 13
Respondent 14
Respondent 15
Respondent 16
Respondent 17
Respondent 18
Respondent 19

440
Respondent Content Format Accuracy Ease of use Timeliness Others
Respondent 20
Respondent 21
Respondent 22
Respondent 23
Respondent 24
Respondent 25
Respondent 26
Respondent 27
Respondent 28
Respondent 29
Respondent 30
Respondent 31
Respondent 32
Respondent 33
Respondent 34
Respondent 35
Respondent 36

How long it takes you to finish this task? ______________

441
LEAST SATISFIED

Respondent Content Format Accuracy Ease of use Timeliness Others


Respondent 1
Respondent 2
Respondent 3
Respondent 4
Respondent 5
Respondent 6
Respondent 7
Respondent 8
Respondent 9
Respondent 10
Respondent 11
Respondent 12
Respondent 13
Respondent 14
Respondent 15
Respondent 16
Respondent 17
Respondent 18
Respondent 19

442
Respondent Content Format Accuracy Ease of use Timeliness Others
Respondent 20
Respondent 21
Respondent 22
Respondent 23
Respondent 24
Respondent 25
Respondent 26
Respondent 27
Respondent 28
Respondent 29
Respondent 30
Respondent 31
Respondent 32
Respondent 33
Respondent 34
Respondent 35
Respondent 36

How long it takes you to finish this task? ____________

443
APPENDIX I
Demographic characteristics of the participants from forty-five organizations –
Study 2
Descriptions Frequency % Mean Std. Dev Median Min. Max.
Gender
Male 17 37.8
Female 28 62.2
Total 45 100
Age
Total Response 42 93.3 37.14 6.2 37.0 27.0 49.0
Ethnicity
Malay 36 80.0
Chinese 5 11.1
Indians 4 8.9
Total 45 100
Academic Qualification
Secondary education 2 4.4
Certificate 1 2.2
Diploma 15 33.3
First/Professional Degree 15 33.3
Second degree and above 12 26.7
Total 45 100
Job Designation
Senior Manager 3 6.7
Manager 18 40.0
Assistant Manager 4 8.9
Senior Executive 2 4.4
Executive 9 20.0
Administrative staff / officer 3 6.7
Assist. Admin. staff/ officer 2 4.4
Others 4 8.9
Total 45 100
No. of Mnths in present Position
Total Response 45 100 59.4 62.3 36.0 1 240
No. of Mnths in Current Org
Total Response 45 100 89.8 76.1 72.0 1 336
No of Yrs as system user
Total Response 45 100 10.7 5.0 10.0 1 24
No. of Mnths in HRIS Project
Total Response 45 100 72.8 50.8 60.0 0 204
No. of Employees in Org
Total Response 45 100 2160.8 4816.2 750.0 30 25000
No. of Employee in HR Dept
Total Response 45 100 21.2 27.2 12.0 1 150
No. of Yrs of HRIS usage in Org
Total Response 44 97.8 10.07 7.2 8.5 1 30
HRIS presently Implemented
Payroll 42 93.3
Compensation and Benefits 27 60.0
Performance Appraisal 18 40.0
Employee Relations 9 20.0
Training and Career Development 24 53.3
Human Resource Planning 12 26.7
Recruitment and Selection 15 33.3
Time Management 11 24.4
Medical 3 6.7
Leave Administration 7 15.6
Employee Database 8 17.8
SOP 1 2.2
Documentation 1 2.2
Organization Management 1 2.2
Employee Self Service (ESS) 2 4.4

444
APPENDIX J

DESCRIPTIVE STATISTICS OF PROJECTS IN THE SURVEY – STUDY 2

Descriptions Frequency % Mean Std. Median Min. Max.


Dev
Nature of the project
Installing new system 30 66.7
Upgrading the existing system 15 33.3
Total 45 100
Project status
Completed 36 80.0
At the planning stage 9 20.0
Total 45 100
Project success
Successful 32 88.9
Not successful 4 11.1
Total 36 100
Actual project completion time
On time 29 64.4
Late 7 15.6
Total 36 100
Predicted completion time
Early 1 11.1
On time 4 44.4
Late 4 44.4
Total 9 100
Actual project duration (in mths)
Total response 36 100 10.5 9.4 9.50 1 48
Predicted project duration(in mths)
Total response 9 100 21.7 14.4 24.0 3 42
Idea to develop the system
Internally 41 91.1
Top Management 17 37.8
IS Department 8 17.8
HR Department 18 40.0
Account Department 2 4.4
Operation Side 1 2.2
Finance Department 2 4.4
Externally 4 8.9
Suppliers 2 4.4
Observation of Competitors 1 2.2
State Secretary 1 2.2
Development of the System
Develop Internally 7 15.6
Acquired from Vendor 21 46.7
Both of these 17 37.8
Total 45 100
Planning method used
Inside view 8 17.8
Outside view 20 44.4
Unpacking 17 37.8
Total 45 100
Completed project outcome
Falls short of expectation 5 13.9
Meets the expectation 30 83.3
Exceeds expectation 1 2.8
Total 36 100

445
APPENDIX K

MEAN PREDICTION BIAS FOR PROJECT COMPLETION TIME – STUDY 2

This appendix contains mean prediction bias for completed projects and projects at the

planning stage for Study 2.

446
Appendix K-1

Mean prediction bias for completed projects

Completed Projects Projects duration (in Total months exceeded % exceeded over
months) entire project duration
7 36 6 16.67
16 12 3 25
19 24 12 50
21 12 3 25
27 11 2 18.18
33 19 12 63.16
34 11 6 54.55

Average 17.86 6.28 36.08

447
Appendix K-2

Mean prediction bias for projects at the planning stage

Project at the Estimated projects duration as Total months predicted % predicted to exceed
Planning planned (in months) to exceed over entire estimated
Stage project duration
1 25 12 48.00
2 3 2 66.67
6 12 3 25.00
7 36 3 8.33

Average 19.00 5.00 37.00

448

You might also like