You are on page 1of 13

897122

research-article2020
ASMXXX10.1177/1073191119897122AssessmentRivera et al.

Article
Assessment

Regression-Based Normative Data for


1­–13
© The Author(s) 2020
Article reuse guidelines:
Children From Latin America: Phonological sagepub.com/journals-permissions
DOI: 10.1177/1073191119897122
https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191119897122

Verbal Fluency Letters M, R, and P journals.sagepub.com/home/asm

Diego Rivera1 , Laiene Olabarrieta-Landa1, Wim Van der Elst2,


Isabel Gonzalez3, Rosario Ferrer-Cascales4, Ana Ivis Peñalver Guia5,
Alberto Rodriguez-Lorenzana6, Javier Galarza-del-Angel7 ,
María José Irías Escher8, and Juan Carlos Arango-Lasprilla9,10 ,11

Abstract
This study is part of a larger project to generate norms for letter verbal fluency test (VFT) in 3,284 children from
nine Latin American countries. The letter VFT (letters M, R, and P) was administered and multiple linear regressions,
including age, age2, MPE (mean parental education), MPE2, sex, and interactions were used as predictors. Results showed
significant differences across countries for all scores. Age affected scores linearly except for Ecuador (P-letter), in which
a quadratic effect was found. Scores increased linearly as a function of MPE, with the exception of Mexico (R-letter), in
which a quadratic effect was found. Age by MPE (M- and P-letters), and age by MPE2 (R-letter) interactions were found
in Mexico. Sex had an impact on letter R in Cuba, and letter P in Ecuador and Paraguay. Age2 by sex interaction was
found in Ecuador (P-letter). These norms will be useful for clinical neuropsychologists in these countries to evaluate
their patients’ verbal fluency.

Keywords
regression models, normative data, letter verbal fluency test, Spanish-speaking populations, children

Verbal fluency tasks (VFTs) are widely used in the evalua- Garcia et al., 2012). Verbal fluency, particularly letter flu-
tion of adults’ cognitive functioning and have gained ency tasks, has shown to be a sensitive assessment tool to
increased interest in the neuropsychological assessment of detect several neurocognitive conditions in children, includ-
the pediatric population. Letter VFT is one type of task that ing dyslexia (Malloy-Diniz et al., 2007), dysgraphia
requires the individual to produce, in 60 seconds, as many
words as possible that start with a given letter (A, F, M, P, 1
Departamento de Ciencias de la Salud, Universidad Pública de Navarra,
R, S; Al-Ghatani, Obonsawin, & Al Moutaery, 2009). Pamplona, España
2
This relatively simple, brief, and easy-to-administer task The Janssen Pharmaceutical Companies of Johnson & Johnson, Beerse,
Belgium
requires several higher order functions. These abilities are 3
Albizu University, Miami, FL, USA
related to the executive function, which is thought to be an 4
University of Alicante, Alicante, Spain
umbrella term for several cognitive skills including work- 5
National Institute of Neurology and Neurosurgery INN, Havana, La
ing memory (not repeating the already stated word), atten- Habana, Cuba
6
tion (simple and sustained), inhibition (blocking irrelevant Universidad de Las Américas, Quito, Pichincha, Ecuador
7
Universidad Autónoma de Baja California, Mexicali, Baja California, Mexico
information that comes to mind), and mental switching and 8
Universidad Nacional Autónoma de Honduras, Tegucigalpa, Francisco
flexibility (phonemic clusters; Delis, Kaplan, & Kramer, Morazán, Honduras
2001; Oliviera, Mograbi, Andrade, & Charchat-Fichman, 9
Biocruces Bizkaia Health Research Institute, Barakaldo, Spain
10
2016). When compared with semantic fluency, letter flu- IKERBASQUE, Basque Foundation for Science, Bilbao, Spain
11
ency has been shown to have an added executive demand. Department of Cell Biology and Histology, University of the Basque
Country (UPV/EHU), Leioa, Spain
As letter fluency is more strategic, it needs more cognitive
organization, requiring the individual to consider more than Corresponding Author:
one set of categories (Garcia et al., 2012; McCarthy & Juan Carlos Arango-Lasprilla, Grupo de Psicología y Salud, Biocruces
Bizkaia Health Research Institute, Hospital Universitario Cruces,
Warrington, 1990; Riva, Nichelli, & Devoti, 2000). IKERBASQUE, Basque Foundation for Science, Plaza de Cruces s/n.,
VFT performance has been shown to be a good indicator Barakaldo, Bizkaia 48903, Spain.
of the integrity of the frontal lobe (Al-Ghatani et al., 2009; Email: jcalasprilla@gmail.com
2 Assessment 00(0)

(Sandler et al., 1992), dyscalculia (Sundheim & Voeller, Cognitive development and the approach to neuropsycho-
2004), attention deficit disorder (Garcia et al., 2012; logical assessments seem to be different across cultures
Willcutt, Pennington, Olson, Chhabildas, & Hulslander, (Olabarrieta-Landa, Rivera, et al., 2017; Oliviera et al.,
2005), traumatic brain injury (Goldstrohm & Arffa, 2005), 2016). Additionally, verbal fluency performance of lower
stroke (Hirsch, 1993), Tourette’s syndrome (Eddy, Rickards, socioeconomic groups was found to be impacted by the
& Cavanna, 2012), and autism (Begeer et al., 2013). teacher’s qualification and the availability of libraries
In children, the ability to organize and retrieve phonemic (Ardila et al., 2005). Based on these findings, the develop-
information is believed to develop slowly, given the devel- ment of verbal fluency normative data should take into con-
opment and late maturation of the frontal lobe (Riva et al., sideration the performance of children with similar
2000). Since this ability is still developing in children and sociodemographic factors (Olabarrieta-Landa, Rivera,
adolescents, several authors have highlighted the impor- et al., 2017; Tallberg et al., 2011).
tance of considering age as a stratification variable in nor- Unfortunately, only a handful of studies have estab-
mative data (Lezak, Howieson, & Loring, 2004; Riva et al., lished normative data for Spanish-speaking children and
2000; Strauss, Sherman, & Spreen, 2006). Research has adolescents in Latin American countries, such as Colombia
shown the amount of words that are produced in letter VFTs (Beltran Dulcey & Solis-Uribe, 2012), Argentina (Butman,
progressively increase with age. It has been suggested that Allegri, Harris, & Drake, 2000; Marino & Alderete, 2010),
the maximum development of those skills, and as a result of and Mexico (Lozano & Ostrosky-Solis, 2006; Nieto et al.,
peak performance, does not occur until adolescence or early 2008). Most of these studies have several limitations, in
adulthood (Klenberg, Korkman, & Lahti-Nuuttila, 2001). particular small sample sizes. Recently Olabarrieta-Landa,
Another similarly imperative variable in the development Rivera, et al. (2017) conducted a methodologically sound
of normative data for children is educational level, which normative study in nine Latin American countries. In the
has been shown to be strongly related to VFT performance latter study, normative data for letter VFTs using the let-
(Acevedo et al., 2000; Olabarrieta-Landa, Rivera, et al., ters F, A, and S were derived based on a large sample size
2017). Educational level appears to be relevant because of (N = 4,373).
factors such as increased experience with pencil-and-paper The development of appropriate norms for Spanish
tasks (Ponton et al., 1996) and greater vocabulary knowl- speakers is imperative as professionals continue using
edge (Ruff, Light, Parker, & Levin, 1997). An interesting norms based on Western and European populations.
finding that should be considered in the development of nor- Moreover, the letters typically used with English speakers
mative data for children is that not only the child’s education (e.g., F, A, S) have been shown to be less than optimal for
but also higher parental educational level is related to the Spanish speakers. Nonetheless, these continue to be used in
child’s improved performance in fluency tasks (Ardila, VFT assessments (Olabarrieta-Landa, Torre, López-
Rosselli, Matute, & Guajardo, 2005; Olabarrieta-Landa, Mugartza, Bialystok, & Arango-Lasprilla, 2017). The
Rivera, et al., 2017; Van der Elst, Hurks, Wassenberg, Meijs, amount of words produced with letters F, A, S in Spanish
& Jolles, 2011). Gender differences in VFT performance speakers has been shown to be less than in English, since
have also been evaluated, but its impact is typically insig- the Spanish language has less words that start with those
nificant (Malloy-Diniz et al., 2007; Nieto, Galtier, Barroso, letters (Strauss et al., 2006). Additionally, using the letter S
& Espinosa, 2008; Olabarrieta-Landa, Rivera, et al., 2017; increases the complexity of the task for children who are in
Ruffieux et al., 2009; Van der Elst et al., 2011). the process of developing their language abilities, as the let-
Until now, most of the letter VFT normative data were ter S, Z, and C have the same phoneme /S/ in some Spanish-
established in samples in the United States and Canada speaking countries (e.g., Mexico; Nieto et al., 2008). Based
(Gladsjo et al., 1999; Lucas et al., 1998; Mitrushina, 2005; on this, some authors have used the letter M instead of S
Strauss et al., 2006; Tombaugh, Kozak, & Rees, 1999). In (Garcia et al., 2012; Nieto et al., 2008). Yet others have
the English language, the most commonly used letters in decided to use a combination of the letters M, R, and P,
letter VFT in the United States and Canada are F, A, S which seems to be more appropriate for Spanish speakers
(Anderson, Northam, Hendy, & Wrenall, 2001; Delis et al., (Artiola, Hermosillo, Heaton, & Pardee, 1999; Butman
2001; Mitrushina, 2005; Tombaugh et al., 1999). Although et al., 2000; Garcia et al., 2012; Marino & Alderete, 2010;
scarce, other countries have developed normative data for Nieto et al., 2008).
letter fluency including Italy (Riva et al., 2000), Brazil Even though steps have been taken, the limited norma-
(Martins, Mograbi, Andrade Gabrig, & Charchat-Fichman, tive data currently available are outdated. To complicate
2016), Sweden (Tallberg, Carlsson, & Lieberman, 2011), things further, most of the available normative data have a
Netherlands (Van der Elst et al., 2011), and Hong Kong small statistical power due to their small sample size, which
(Lee, Yuen, & Chan, 2002). makes it harder to generalize the results. Therefore, the cur-
Important factors that have often been overlooked in ver- rent study was developed to provide innovative normative
bal fluency normative studies are culture and language. data for Spanish-speaking children using the letters M, R, P,
Rivera et al. 3

and mean VFT total score for letter fluency, as these three Fihn, & Bradley, 1998) for participants aged 12 years and
letters are believed to be the most appropriate ones for the older, and (j) using psychoactive substances such as heroin,
Spanish lexicon. This study is part of a larger project with a barbiturates, amphetamines, methamphetamines, or cocaine
sample of N = 3,284 to generate normative data for a series in the last 6 months for participants aged 12 years and older.
of neuropsychological measures in healthy subjects aged 6
to 17 years from nine countries in Latin America (Rivera &
Procedure
Arango-Lasprilla, 2017).
Prior to initiating the assessment, informed consent was
requested for all parents/guardians and children 12 years of
Method age and older. In the case of children younger than 12 years,
an assent signature was demanded. The neuropsychological
Participants battery, which lasted approximately 120 minutes, was
The original sample consisted of N = 3,370 healthy chil- administered individually in a single day to the participants
dren and adolescents aged 6 to 17 years from nine Latin at schools and/or universities. All test participants were vol-
American countries (Chile, Cuba, Ecuador, Guatemala, unteers who did not receive financial compensation for par-
Honduras, Mexico, Paraguay, Peru, and Puerto Rico). ticipation. Data collection began in January 2016 and
Nonproportionate quota sampling was used, looking for a finished in May 2017. More details can be found in Rivera
symmetric distribution for strata of age, sex, and type of and Arango-Lasprilla (2017).
school (public or private) for each country. For Cuba, type
of school was not considered in the sampling procedure as
all education is public (Gasperini, 2000). A total of n = 64
Measures
participants were excluded from the analyses because there The participants were required to produce as many words as
was no information regarding the educational level of their possible in 1 minute beginning with the letters M, R, and P.
parents, and n = 22 were excluded because of incomplete The VFTs were always administered in the same order, that
information, yielding a final sample of 3,284 participants. is, first letter M, subsequently letter R, and finally letter P.
The final sample size per country ranged between n = 192 For administration and scoring, the guidelines of
and n = 919. The demographic characteristics of the test Olabarrieta-Landa, Rivera, et al. (2017) and Olabarrieta-
participants (age, sex, parent education, and type of school) Landa, Torre, et al. (2017) were used.
by country can be found in Table 1.
All participants met the following inclusion criteria: (a)
being between the ages of 6 and 17 years, (b) being born
Statistical Analyses
and currently living in the country where the protocol was Exploratory Data Analysis. To determine significant differ-
administered, (c) Spanish as primary language, (d) having ences between countries on the performance of each of the
an intelligence quotient (IQ) of ≥80 according to the test of VFT scores, three analyses of covariance (ANCOVA) mod-
nonverbal intelligence (TONI-2; Brown, Sherbenou, & els were fitted. Country was used as a fixed-effect factor in
Johnsen, 2009), (e) having a score of <19 on the Children’s the model, with age, mean parental education (MPE), and
Depression Inventory (CDI; Kovacs, 1992), and (f) being sex as covariates. MPE was calculated using the mean year
enrolled in a regular private or public school. of education for both parents/guardians. If a child had a
Participants were excluded based on the following crite- single parent/guardian, the MPE was calculated using her or
ria: (a) having a history of a central nervous system disease his years of education. Benjamini–Hochberg correction
associated with neuropsychological problems (e.g., epi- (Benjamini & Hochberg, 1995) was used to conduct the
lepsy), (b) having a history of alcohol abuse and/or con- pairwise comparisons of the VFT test scores across coun-
sumption of psychotropic substances, (c) having some type tries. Furthermore, Pearson correlations between the VFT
of active or uncontrolled systemic disease associated with scores (including M, R, P total scores, and mean VFT total
cognitive impairment (e.g., diabetes mellitus), (d) having a score) and the sociodemographic (age, MPE, and sex) vari-
history of psychiatric illness (e.g., major depression), (e) ables were computed. Mean VFT total score was calculated
having severe sensory deficits (e.g., loss of vision) that by summing M, R, and P total scores, and dividing it by 3:
affect the performance on the tests, (f) currently taking psy- [(M + R + P)/3].
chiatric or other medications that could alter cognitive per-
formance, (g) having intellectual or learning disabilities or The Effects of Demographic Variables and the Derivation of
other neurodevelopmental disorders, (h) having a history of Normative Data.  The letters M, R, P, and mean VFT total
pre-, peri-, or postnatal problems (e.g., hypoxia), (i) having scores were computed separately. The effects of demo-
a score of >5 on the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification graphic variables on VFT scores were evaluated by means
Test–Consumption (AUDIT-C; Bush, Kivlahan, McDonell, of multiple linear regression analyses. For each country,
4 Assessment 00(0)

Table 1.  Sample Distribution by Country, Age, MPE, Sex, and Type of School.

Age (Years) MPE Sex Type of school

Girl Boy Public Private


Age range
Country (Years) n M (SD) M (SD) n n n n
Chile 6 to 8 94 7.0 (.8) 13.1 (3.1) 47 47 46 48
  9 to 11 98 10.0 (.8) 12.5 (3.1) 50 48 49 49
  12 to 14 94 13.0 (.8) 12.0 (2.7) 46 48 47 47
  15 to 17 95 16.0 (.8) 11.8 (3.1) 48 47 49 46
  Total 381 11.5 (3.4) 12.4 (3.0) 191 (50.1%) 190 (49.9%) 191 (50.1%) 190 (49.9%)
Cuba 6 to 8 96 7.0 (.8) 15.9 (1.9) 48 48 96 0
  9 to 11 94 10.0 (.8) 15.9 (2.0) 46 48 94 0
  12 to 14 96 13.0 (.8) 16.5 (1.5) 48 48 96 0
  15 to 17 95 16.0 (.8) 16.4 (1.6) 48 47 95 0
  Total 381 11.5 (3.5) 16.2 (1.8) 190 (49.9%) 191 (50.1%) 381 (100%) 0 (0%)
Ecuador 6 to 8 79 7.0 (.8) 14.9 (3.4) 42 37 41 38
  9 to 11 73 9.9 (.8) 14.1 (3.7) 43 30 40 33
  12 to 14 75 13.0 (.8) 14.3 (3.2) 49 26 36 39
  15 to 17 70 16.0 (.8) 14.5 (4.1) 39 31 38 32
  Total 297 11.4 (3.4) 14.4 (3.6) 173 (58.2%) 124 (41.8%) 155 (52.2%) 142 (47.8%)
Guatemala 6 to 8 34 7.3 (1.0) 12.4 (4.1) 14 20 12 22
  9 to 11 94 9.9 (.8) 9.0 (3.4) 41 53 68 26
  12 to 14 52 12.4 (.7) 10.2 (4.1) 24 28 30 22
  15 to 17 22 15.6 (.7) 14.0 (3.9) 15 7 1 21
  Total 202 10.7 (2.5) 10.4 (4.1) 94 (46.5%) 108 (53.5%) 111 (55.0%) 91 (45.0%)
Honduras 6 to 8 70 7.0 (.8) 13.2 (3.6) 36 34 47 23
  9 to 11 96 10.0 (.8) 12.6 (3.6) 48 48 49 47
  12 to 14 70 13.0 (.9) 12.0 (3.9) 42 28 38 32
  15 to 17 56 15.9 (.9) 13.7 (3.3) 33 23 18 38
  Total 292 11.2 (3.2) 12.8 (3.7) 159 (54.5%) 133 (45.5%) 152 (52.1%) 140 (47.9%)
Mexico 6 to 8 247 6.9 (.8) 13.3 (4.1) 128 119 144 103
  9 to 11 229 10.0 (.8) 13.4 (4.0) 114 115 143 86
  12 to 14 218 13.0 (.8) 13.7 (3.9) 114 104 123 95
  15 to 17 225 16.0 (.8) 12.1 (3.5) 118 107 153 72
  Total 919 11.4 (3.5) 13.1 (3.9) 474 (51.6%) 445 (48.4%) 563 (61.3%) 356 (38.7%)
Paraguay 6 to 8 74 7.1 (.8) 13.7 (2.6) 38 36 35 39
  9 to 11 76 10.0 (.8) 13.9 (3.2) 50 26 42 34
  12 to 14 63 13.0 (.8) 14.8 (2.6) 31 32 25 38
  15 to 17 84 16.0 (.8) 13.9 (3.0) 41 43 36 48
  Total 297 11.6 (3.5) 14.1 (2.9) 160 (53.9%) 137 (46.1%) 138 (46.5%) 159 (53.5%)
Peru 6 to 8 66 7.1 (.8) 12.7 (2.3) 32 34 25 41
  9 to 11 76 9.9 (.7) 12.5 (2.4) 33 43 34 42
  12 to 14 96 13.1 (.8) 12.2 (2.5) 47 49 56 40
  15 to 17 85 16.0 (.7) 12.6 (2.5) 48 37 57 28
  Total 323 11.9 (3.3) 12.5 (2.4) 160 (49.5%) 163 (50.5%) 172 (53.3%) 151 (46.7%)
Puerto Rico 6 to 8 46 7.1 (.9) 14.6 (2.5) 29 17 32 14
  9 to 11 31 9.8 (.8) 14.7 (2.1) 18 13 16 15
  12 to 14 54 13.4 (.8) 14.7 (2.6) 28 26 31 23
  15 to 17 61 16.0 (.8) 14.5 (2.6) 32 29 39 22
  Total 192 12.1 (3.6) 14.6 (2.5) 107 (55.7%) 85 (44.3%) 118 (61.5%) 74 (38.5%)

Note. MPE = mean parental education; M = mean; SD = standard deviation.


Rivera et al. 5

separate regression models were fitted to establish country-


specific normative data. The full regression models included
( )
1. The expected test score Yi computed based
on the fixed-effect parameter estimate of
as predictors: age, age2, MPE, MPE2, sex, and all two-way the established final regression model:
interactions between these variables. Age and MPE were Yi = B0 + B1 X 1i + B2 X 2i +  + Bk X ki .
centered (= calendar age in years—mean age in the sample 2. To obtain the residual value ( ei ) , the model-pre-
by country; MPE in years—mean education in the sample dicted value (Yi ) is subtracted from the raw score of
by country) before computing the quadratic age and educa- the neuropsychological test (Yi ) , that is, ei = Yi − Yi .
tion terms to avoid multicollinearity (Kutner, Nachtsheim, 3. Using the residual standard deviation ( SDe ) value
Neter, & Li, 2005). Squared terms of age and years of edu- provided by the regression model, the residual is
cation were added in the full model to allow for quadratic standardized: zi = ei / SDe .
effects between these independent variables and the letter 4. Finally, using the standard normal cumulative distri-
scores (i.e., nonlinear relations between age/MPE and the bution function, the exact percentile value corre-
VLT test scores). Sex was dummy coded as boy = 1 and sponding to the zi -score previously calculated is
girl = 0. The full regression model can be formally obtained (if the model assumption of normality of the
described as 2 residuals was met in the normative sample), or via the
   
yi = B0 + B1 ⋅  Age − x Ageby  + B2 ⋅  Age − x Ageby  empirical cumulative distribution function of the stan-
 country i  country i dardized residuals (if the standardized residuals were
not normally distributed in the normative sample).
( ) ( )
2
+ B3 ⋅ MPE − xMPE by country + B4 ⋅ MPE − xMPE .by country
i i
+ B5 ⋅ Sexi + Bk ⋅ Interactionsi + εi , with the subscript i refer- This four-step process was applied to the letters M, R, P,
ring to the participants (i = {1, 2, . . ., ni }, and ni the number and mean VFT total scores separately for each country.
of participants in a country), the subscript k referring to the
regression parameters (k = {1, 2, . . ., K}, and K = the num- Preliminary Estimates of Reliability.  The reliability of each of
ber of fixed effects in the model including the intercept), and the VFT scores for the entire subsample was calculated
the term Interactionsi being a shorthand notation to refer to through the test–retest method, which evaluates the tem-
all two-way interactions between the fixed effects. The poral stability of the test scores. The reliability was calcu-
model assumes that the residuals εi are normally distributed lated with two-way mixed intraclass correlation
(
with mean 0 and variance σε2 , that is, εi ∼ N 0, σε2 . ) coefficients ( ICCtest − restest ). The intraclass correlation coef-
ficient is frequently used to report test–retest reliability
Independent variables that were not statistically signifi-
cant (using an alpha level of 0.05/13 independent variables (Weir, 2005). The subsample used to calculate the test–
≈ 0.003) in the full multiple regression model (i.e., the retest reliability consisted of 166 participants from Chile
model shown above that includes all independent variables (n = 32; 19.9%), Cuba (n = 38; 23.6%), Ecuador (n = 16;
and their two-way interactions) were removed in a step- 9.9%), and Mexico (n = 75; 46.6%). Of those, 64.0%
wise hierarchical way. In the model-building procedure, were boys and the average age was 10.9 (SD = 3.7) years.
predictors were not removed as long as they were included The average time elapsed between the two test administra-
in a higher order term in the model (Aiken & West, 1991). tions was 76.5 (SD = 10.4; range= 360, 126) days. All
For all multiple linear regression models, the following analyses were performed using R 3.4.2 for Window (R
assumptions were evaluated: (a) multicollinearity (evalu- Development Core Team, 2017) and SPSS version 23
ated by computing the variance inflation factor [VIF], (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY).
which should not be greater than 10, and by computing the
collinearity tolerance values which should not be greater Results
than 1); (b) homoscedasticity (evaluated by grouping the
participants into quartiles of the predicted test scores and
Exploratory Data Analysis
applying the Levene test on the residuals); (c) normality of Positive correlations were found between age and all VFT
the standardized residuals (evaluated by conducting the scores (rs ≥ .33; ps < .01). Moreover, correlations were
Kolmogorov–Smirnov test); and (d) the existence of influ- found between MPE and (a) all VFT scores in Cuba (rs ≥
ential values (evaluated by computing the maximum .11; ps < .05) and Mexico (rs ≥ .09; ps < .01), and (b) let-
Cook’s distance values and relating them to an F ( p, n − p ) ters M and P (rs ≥ .16; ps < .05) in Guatemala. Sex was
distribution, for details see Kutner et al. (2005). correlated with letter R (Cuba; r = −.16; p < .01), and letter
Using the final regression model obtained at the end of P (Guatemala [r = −.14; p < .05], Mexico [r = −.07; p <
the stepwise procedure, normative data were adjusted for .05], and Paraguay [r = −.21; p < .01]).
demographic variables. This was established by means of a The intercorrelation between letters M, R, and P scores
four-step procedure (Rivera et al., 2019; Van Breukelen & across countries were also significantly positive (rs ≥ .622
Vlaeyen, 2005): to 0.806; ps < .001). In view of the high correlations
6 Assessment 00(0)

Table 2.  Post Hoc Analysis of the Mean Differences Between Countries in Each VFT Score.

Mean difference (I − J)

(I) Country (J) Country Letter M Letter R Letter P Letter mean VFT
Chile Cuba −.46 −1.46* −.22 −.72*
  Ecuador −.17 −.59* −.10 −.29
  Guatemala −.81* −1.13* −.58 −.84*
  Honduras .16 .003 .23 .13
  Mexico −.46* −.63* .05 −.35*
  Paraguay −.79* −.44 −.45 −.56*
  Peru −1.33* −1.59* −1.28* −1.40*
  Puerto Rico .80* .70* 1.09* .86*
Cuba Ecuador .28 .88* .12 .43
  Guatemala −.36 .33 −.36 −.12
  Honduras .62* 1.46* .45 .85*
  Mexico −.01 .83* .27 .37*
  Paraguay −.34 1.03* −.23 .15
  Peru −.88* −.12 −1.06* −.68*
  Puerto Rico 1.26* 2.17* 1.31* 1.57*
Ecuador Guatemala −.64* −.54 −.48 −.55*
  Honduras .34 .59* .34 .42
  Mexico −.29 −.04 .16 −.06
  Paraguay −.62* .15 −.35 −.27
  Peru −1.16* −1.00* −1.17* −1.11*
  Puerto Rico .98* 1.29* 1.20* 1.14*
Guatemala Honduras .97* 1.13* .81* .97*
  Mexico .35 .50 .63* .49*
  Paraguay .02 .69* .13 .28
  Peru −.52 −.45 −.70* −.56*
  Puerto Rico 1.61* 1.83* 1.67* 1.70*
Honduras Mexico −.62* −.63* −.18 −.48*
  Paraguay −.96* −.43 −.69* −.69*
  Peru −1.50* −1.58* −1.51* −1.53*
  Puerto Rico .64* .70* .86* .72*
Mexico Paraguay −.33 .19 −.51* −.22
  Peru −.87* −.96* −1.33* −1.05*
  Puerto Rico 1.27* 1.33* 1.04* 1.20*
Paraguay Peru −.54* −1.15* −.82* −.84*
  Puerto Rico 1.60* 1.14* 1.55* 1.42*
Peru Puerto Rico 2.14* 2.29* 2.37* 2.25*

Note. Asterisk (*) indicates significant difference according to Benjamini–Hochberg correction. Each comparison is significant if the observed p is
smaller than a critical value defined as Pcrit = ( j / k )α .

between all VFT scores, a mean VFT total score ([M + R + pair-comparisons were found in letter M, 25 in letter R, 19
P]/3) was calculated and normative data were provided for in letter P, and 26 in mean VFT (see Table 2).
this overall test score summary metric.
ANCOVAs showed significant differences across the
Model Assumptions
nine countries for the letter M VFT score (F(8, 3272) = 10.793,
p < .001, partial η2 = .026), the letter R VFT score (F(8, 3265) The assumptions of multiple linear regression analysis
= 15.762, p < .001, partial η2 = .037), the letter P VFT were met for most final models. There was no multicol-
score (F(8, 3264) = 9.663, p < .001, partial η2 = .023), and linearity, that is, the VIF values in all final models were at
the mean VFT total score (F(8, 3264) = 9.663, p < .001, par- most 3.264 (letter P in Ecuador), and thus, well below the
tial η2 = .023). According to Benjamini–Hochberg correc- threshold value = 10 that is indicative for multicollinear-
tion, from the 36 total pair-comparisons, 22 significant ity. Collinearity tolerance values did not exceed the value
Rivera et al. 7

of 1. There were no influential cases in any of the models letter P scores ranged from 10.8% (Guatemala) to 43.4%
considered (the maximum Cook’s distance value was (Chile).
0.10; relating this value to an F(2, 290) distribution yields a
percentile value of 10, which is well below the threshold Mean VFT Total Score.  For all nine countries, the final multiple
percentile value = 50 that is indicative for the presence of linear regression models of the mean VFT total score were
influential cases). The Levene test suggested no homosce- significant (Table 3). In all countries, the mean VFT total
dasticity in the majority of the final models, except for the scores increased linearly as a function of age. In Mexico, a
models of Chile (letter R), Cuba (letters M and R), significant interaction was found between age and MPE, so
Ecuador (all scores), Guatemala (all scores), Honduras that MPE influence on children and adolescents’ performance
(letter P), Paraguay (letter R), and Puerto Rico (letters M became more evident while they got older, with huge dis-
and P). Standardized residuals for all but one of the mod- crepancy between 5 and 20 MPE, starting from 12 years old.
els (letter P in Cuba) were normally distributed (all ps of The participant’s sex affected the mean VFT total scores for
the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test > .01) so the standard nor- Cuba and Paraguay, such that girls achieved higher scores
mal cumulative distribution was used to convert z scores than boys. The amount of variance explained by these predic-
2
to percentiles. For letter P in Cuba, the empirical cumula- tors (adjusted R ) in the mean VFT total scores ranged from
tive distribution function of the standardized residual was 23.0% (Guatemala) to 51.9% (Honduras).
used.
Calculator of Normative Data.  The four-step normative pro-
cedure explained above allows to determine an exact per-
The Effects of Demographic Variables centile value for a VFT test scores by hand. However, this
Letter M. For all nine countries the final multiple linear method can be prone to human error, so to enhance user-
regression models for letter M scores were significant (see friendliness, the authors created a calculator in Microsoft
Table 3). The letter M score increased linearly as a func- Excel that conducts all required computations. The clinician
tion of age in all countries. In Mexico, MPE influence on simply fills-in the country, raw VFT scores and demo-
letter M score, so that children and adolescents with higher graphic characteristics of the tested child (i.e., age, MPE,
MPE obtained higher scores (see Figure 1A). Sex was not and sex), and the software automatically computes the stan-
related to letter M scores in any country. The amount of dardized residuals and their corresponding exact percentile
2
variance (adjusted R ) explained by these predictors in the values (see Figure 2). For example, if a 10-year-old Para-
letter M scores ranged from 24.9% (Guatemala) to 50.5% guayan boy, whose parents have an MPE of 10 years of edu-
(Honduras). cation, scored 11 on the letter P, it belongs to the 90th
percentile (z score = 1.305). This tool is freely available for
Letter R.  The final multiple linear regression models for let- all users and may be downloaded at https://neuropsycholo-
ter R VFT scores of all nine countries were significant gylearning.com/datos-normativos-archivos-descargables/.
(Table 3). Letter R scores increased linearly as a function of
age in all countries. In Mexico, a significant interaction was
Preliminary Estimates of Reliability
found between age and MPE, that is, the increase in letter R
scores as a function of age was more pronounced for chil- The interclass coefficient of the VFT score for the subsam-
dren with a higher MPE (i.e., larger slope for children with ple were 0.853 (95% confidence interval [CI] = [0.799,
a higher MPE; see Figure 1B). The participant’s sex affected 0.892]; p < .001) for the letter M score, 0.849 (95% CI =
letter R scores for Cuba, such that girls achieved higher [0.794, 0.889]; p < .001) for the letter R score, 0.863 (95%
scores than boys. The amount of variance explained by CI = [0.813, 0.899]; p < .001) for the letter P score, and
2
these predictors (adjusted R ) ranged from 17.8% (Guate- 0.906 (95% CI = [0.872, 0.931]; p < .001) for the mean
mala) to 41.4% (Peru). VFT total score.

Letter P.  In all nine countries, final multiple linear regres-


Discussion
sion models for letter P scores were significant (Table 3). In
all countries letter P scores increased linearly as a function The main aim of the present study was to establish the nor-
of age. In Mexico, a significant interaction was found mal range of performance for letters M, R, P, and mean VFT
between age and MPE, so that the MPE influence on chil- total score in a large sample of children aged 6 to 17 years
dren and adolescents’ performance became more evident from nine countries in Latin America. As a first step in the
while they got older (see Figure 1C). The participant’s sex normative procedure, the influence of demographic vari-
affected letter P scores for Paraguay, such that girls achieved ables on test performance was evaluated. Age was consis-
higher scores than boys (see Figure 1D). The amount of tently found to be the most important predictor of VFT
variance explained by these predictors (adjusted R 2 ) in the performance (Table 3), with older children outperforming
8
Table 3.  Final Multiple Linear Regression Models for VFT Scores.

Chile Cuba Ecuador Guatemala Honduras Mexico Paraguay Peru Puerto Rico

Letter B (β) B (β) B (β) B (β) B (β) B (β) B (β) B (β) B (β)
M (Intercept) 7.969** 8.842** 8.332** 7.978** 7.665** 8.446** 9.040** 9.603** 7.876**
  Age .741 (.648)** .730 (.643)** .694 (.637)** .670 (.503)** .839 (.712)** .715 (.637) ** .675 (.550)** .843 (.635)** .566 (.573)**
  MPE — — — — — .155 (.155)** — — —
  Adjusted R2 .419 .412 .403 .249 .505 .411 .300 .401 .325
R (Intercept) 7.15** 9.735** 7.935** 7.52** 7.02** 7.849** 7.86** 9.018** 7.129**
  Age .674 (.600)** .785 (.621)** .705 (.644)** .531 (.427)** .678 (.618)** .676 (.611)** .517 (.484)** .839 (.645)** .548 (.582)**
  MPE — — — — — .19 (.193)** — — —
  Sex −1.393 (−.159) — — — — — — —
  Age × MPE — — — — — .039 (.138)** — — —
  Adjusted R2 .359 .409 .413 .178 .380 .386 .231 .414 .335
P (Intercept) 9.132** 9.757** 9.438** 8.893** 8.754** 9.122** 1.872** 1.718** 8.717**
  Age .847 (.660)** .796 (.630)** .759 (.641)** .442 (.336)** .768 (.645)** .744 (.633)** .638 (.497)** .870 (.629)** .633 (.581)**
  MPE — — — — — .158 (.151)** — — —
  Sex — — — — — — −2.175 — —
(−.242)**
  Age × MPE — — — — — .024 (.081)* — — —
  Adjusted R2 .434 .395 .413 .108 .414 .400 .286 .394 .335
Mean VFT (Intercept) 8.084** 9.625** 8.568** 8.130** 7.813** 8.481** 9.516** 9.780** 7.933**
  Age .754 (.699)** .769 (.693)** .719 (.710) ** .548 (.484) ** .762 (.722)** .714 (.693)** .615 (.557)** .851 (.685)** .582 (.644)**
  MPE — — — — — .170 (.185)** — — —
  Sex — −.817 (−.107)** — — — — −1.285 — —
(−.167)**
  Age × MPE — — — — — .028 (.105)** — — —
  Adjusted R2 .488 .490 .502 .230 .519 .484 .323 .467 .412

Note. VFT = verbal fluency test; MPE = mean parental education; B = Beta unstandardized; β = Beta standardized. “—” indicates independent variable no significant in the regression model.
*p = .002. **p < .001.
Rivera et al. 9

Figure 1.  Predicted mean letter scores as a function of (A) mean parental education (MPE) for the letter M from the Mexican
sample (A); age and MPE interaction for the letter R (B) and the letter P (C) from the Mexican sample; and sex for the letter P from
Paraguayan sample (D).

their younger counterparts. Interestingly, the relation (Olabarrieta-Landa, Rivera, et al., 2017; Van der Elst et al.,
between age and test performance was linear for all three 2011). In pediatric populations, neurodevelopmental
VFT scores in all nine countries. This means that the yearly aspects are essential to understand cognitive performance.
average change in the test scores remains constant over the Verbal fluency, especially letter fluency task, requires the
entire age range considered. Specifically, the cognitive abil- development of executive functions and the frontal lobe,
ities assessed by verbal fluency tests seem to develop at a both keeping their processes of maturation until young
constant rate during the entire age range considered in this adulthood (Klenberg et al., 2001; Korkman, Kemp, & Kirk,
study (6-17 years). Sex and MPE also had a significant 2001; Malloy-Diniz et al., 2007). Thus, it is not surprising
effect on some VFT scores, but the effects of these indepen- that age has such an impact on this task. Moreover, letter
dent variables were not consistent across countries. Overall, verbal fluency requires greater organizational and strategic
the correlations between MPE/sex and the three VFT scores skills than semantic verbal fluency, and these abilities are
were low and never exceeded r = .22. Pearson univariate thought to develop slowly in children (Riva et al., 2000).
correlations showed weak relations that disappeared at the Literature suggests that the environment in which chil-
final multivariate regression models (i.e., Cuba with all let- dren grow up may influence their future cognitive develop-
ters vs. MPE, and Guatemala with sex in letter P). ment, especially MPE (Meador et al., 2011; Schady, 2011),
These results are in line with previous studies where age and parents’ occupation (Dubow, Boxer, & Huesmann,
was the most important and substantial influencer on letter 2009). MPE is thought to help in the more advanced vocab-
verbal fluency (e.g., Ruffieux et al., 2009; Tallberg et al., ulary development of children through the mediation of
2011), while the impact of MPE and sex tends to be minor responsive and clear communication with the child
10 Assessment 00(0)

Figure 2.  Calculator in Microsoft Excel.

(Sohr-Preston et al., 2013). Finally, sex influence was very vocabulary level is similar among both groups (Friesen,
small compared with age impact, and even though discrep- Luo, Luk, & Bialystok, 2015; Luo, Luk, & Bialystok,
ancies can be found in the literature regarding its influence, 2010). Also, according to Manly, Touradji, Tang, and Stern
sex differences in verbal abilities appear to be almost non- (2003), quality of education is a stronger predictor of cog-
existent (Hyde & Linn, 1988). nitive performance than years of education (variable that
Importantly, even though different countries in Latin has been considered in this study). In Latin America,
America are comparable in terms of language spoken and countries differ according to socioeconomic and education
general cultural background, there were major differences systems (e.g., Puerto Rico vs. Cuba) that may impact qual-
in VFT performance (see Table 2). For example, the age-, ity of education, and thus, cognitive performance.
sex-, and MPE-corrected letter M score of 8 equaled 30th Finally, as Strauss et al. (2006) pointed out, the selection
and 52th percentiles for Peru and Puerto Rico, respec- of letters during this task will determine its difficulty.
tively. A typical inhabitant of Peru thus has 22 centiles of Looking at final multiple linear regressions of letters M, R,
difference compared with his or her counterpart from and P, their intercepts remain, in general, higher compared
Puerto Rico. This finding clearly stresses the importance with the constants of letters F, A, and S published by
of having country-specific normative data for Latin Olabarrieta-Landa, Rivera, et al. (2017) with Spanish-
American countries. Several variables that may impact speaking children from the same Latin American countries.
letter verbal fluency, which were not assessed in this study, For example, for Ecuador, the constant of letter S was 7.361
could explain these differences in scores among countries. (Olabarrieta-Landa, Rivera, et al., 2017) while the constant
For instance, language-related competences (e.g., vocabu- of letter M in the current study was 8.332. Hence, children
lary level, reading, and writing) have been associated with produce more words if a combination of the letters M, R,
better performance in letter verbal fluency (Kochhann, and P is used. This supports the idea that those letters are
Holz, Beber, Chaves, & Fonseca, 2018; Olabarrieta- more appropriate to be used with Spanish speakers. In addi-
Landa, Rivera, et al., 2017; Olabarrieta-Landa, Torre, tion, MRP letters showed high temporal consistency (r ≥
et al., 2017). In countries with bilingual population, such .849), according to cutoff points proposed by Strauss et al.
as Puerto Rico, bilingualism might be an important vari- (2006) for intraclass test–retest correlation coefficients.
able since it has been also related to letter verbal fluency,
with no clear results: While some studies have reported
Limitations
that monolinguals outperform bilingual participants on
letter verbal fluency (Bialystok, Craik, & Luk, 2008; Several limitations of the current study can be given. First,
Gollan, Montoya, & Werner, 2002), others have found that participants in this study were native Spanish speakers and
bilingual participants outperform monolinguals when no data were collected on bilingualism. As stated above,
Rivera et al. 11

several studies have shown how verbal fluency test perfor- References
mance may be influenced by this variable (Bialystok et al., Acevedo, A., Loewenstein, D. A., Barker, W. W., Harwood, D.
2008; Friesen et al., 2015; Luo et al., 2010). Second, the G., Luis, C., Bravo, M., . . . Duara, R. (2000). Category flu-
results of this study cannot be generalized to other Latin ency test: Normative data for English- and Spanish-speaking
American countries that were not included in this study elderly. Journal of the International Neuropsychological
(such as Venezuela and Uruguay) or other countries where Society, 6, 760-769.
people from these same countries are living (such as Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing
United States of America). Third, even though in Mexico and interpreting interactions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
the sample was collected from several geographic areas of Al-Ghatani, A. M., Obonsawin, M. C., & Al Moutaery, K.
R. (2009). Normative data for the two equivalent forms
the country, for the rest of the countries of the study, only
of the Arabic verbal fluency test. Pan Arab Journal of
people from a specific geographic area were recruited. Neuropsychology, 13(2), 57-65.
Last, normative data were generated using healthy chil- Anderson, V., Northam, E., Hendy, J., & Wrenall, J. (2001).
dren and adolescents. Future studies should include clini- Developmental Neuropsychology: A clinical approach. New
cal samples (e.g., children with attention deficit/ York, NY: Psychology Press.
hyperactivity disorder, brain injury, epilepsy) to obtain Ardila, A., Rosselli, M., Matute, E., & Guajardo, G. (2005). The
proper cutoff points to calculate the sensitivity and speci- influence of parents’ educational level on the development of
ficity of VFT. executive functions. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28,
539-560.
Artiola, L., Hermosillo, D., Heaton, R., & Pardee, R. E. (1999).
Conclusion Manual de normas y procedimientos para la bateria neu-
ropsicologica en espanol [Manual of norms and procedures
The current study offers verbal fluency test norms across
for neuropsychological battery in Spanish]. Tucson, AZ:
nine countries in Latin America with 3,284 healthy chil- m Press.
dren and adolescents aged 6 to 17 years. Most normative Begeer, S., Wierda, M., Scheeren, A. M., Teunisse, J. P., Koot, H.
data studies have focused on adult populations, so to M., & Geurts, H. M. (2013). Verbal fluency in children with
date, this represents the largest normative data study of autism spectrum disorders: Clustering and switching strate-
neuropsychological tests for children and adolescents in gies. Autism, 18, 1014-1018. doi:10.1177/1362361313500381
the world. Beltran Dulcey, C., & Solis-Uribe, G. (2012). Evaluación
In addition, for the creation of the normative data, more Neuropsicológica en adolescentes: Normas para población de
up-to-date approaches have been used, such as multiple Bucaramanga [Neuropsychological Assessment in adolescents:
regressions and residual values. Moreover, age and MPE Norms for Bucaramanga population]. Revista Neuropsicología,
were introduced into the model as continuous variables. Neuropsiquiatría y Neurociencias, 12(2), 77-93.
Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false
Therefore, these norms are expected to be useful for those
discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to mul-
who work in neuropsychological evaluation of children and tiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series
adolescents in these countries. B (Methodological), 57, 289-300. doi:10.1111/j.2517-6161
.1995.tb02031.x
Author’s Note Bialystok, E., Craik, F., & Luk, G. (2008). Cognitive control and
Isabel Gonzalez is now affiliated with University of Colorado lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. Journal of
School of Medicine, Aurora, Colorado. Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition,
34, 859-873.
Brown, L., Sherbenou, R. J., & Johnsen, S. K. (2009). Test of non-
Declaration of Conflicting Interests verbal intelligence. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect Bush, K., Kivlahan, D. R., McDonell, M. B., Fihn, S. D., &
to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. Bradley, K. A. (1998). The AUDIT alcohol consumption
questions (AUDIT-C): An effective brief screening test
Funding for problem drinking. Archives of Internal Medicine, 158,
1789-1795.
The author(s) received no financial support for the research, Butman, J., Allegri, R. F., Harris, P., & Drake, M. (2000).
authorship, and/or publication of this article. Fluencia verbal en español. Datos normativos en Argentina
[Verbal fluency in Spanish. Normative data from Argentina].
ORCID iDs Medicina, 60, 561-564.
Diego Rivera https://orcid.org/0000-0001-7477-1893 Delis, D. C., Kaplan, E., & Kramer, J. H. (2001). Delis-Kaplan
Executive Function System (DKEFS), Examiner’s manual.
Javier Galarza-del-Angel https://orcid.org/0000-0001-5865 San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
-5198 Dubow, E. F., Boxer, P., & Huesmann, L. R. (2009). Long-term
Juan Carlos Arango Lasprilla https://orcid.org/0000-0002 effects of parents’ education on children’s educational and
-7184-8311 occupational success: Mediation by family interactions, child
12 Assessment 00(0)

aggression, and teenage aspirations. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., & Loring, D. W. (2004).
55, 224-249. Neuropsychological assessment (4th ed.). New York, NY:
Eddy, C. M., Rickards, H. E., & Cavanna, A. E. (2012). Executive Oxford University Press.
functions in uncomplicated Tourette syndrome. Psychiatry Lozano, A., & Ostrosky-Solis, F. (2006). Efecto de la edad y la
Research, 200, 46-48. escolaridad en la fluidez verbal semántica: Datos normativos
Friesen, D. C., Luo, L., Luk, G., & Bialystok, E. (2015). Proficiency en población hispanohablante [Effect of age and education
and control in verbal fluency performance across the lifespan on semantic verbal fluency: Normative data for Spanish-
for monolinguals and bilinguals. Language, Cognition and speaking population]. Revista Mexicana de Psicología, 23(1),
Neuroscience, 30, 238-250. 37-44.
Garcia, E., Rodriguez, C., Martin, R., Jimenez, J. E., Hernandez, Lucas, J. A., Ivnik, R. J., Smith, G. E., Bohac, D. L., Tangalos, E.
S., & Dıaz, A. (2012). Test de fluidez verbal: Datos normati- G., Graff-Radford, N. R., & Petersen, R. C. (1998). Mayo’s
vos y desarrollo evolutivo en el alumnado de primaria [Verbal older Americans normative studies: Category fluency norms.
Fluency Test: normative data and evolutionary development Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 20,
in elementary students]. European Journal of Education and 194-200.
Psychology, 5, 53-64. Luo, L., Luk, G., & Bialystok, E. (2010). Effect of language pro-
Gasperini, L. (2000). The Cuban education system: Lessons and ficiency and executive control on verbal fluency performance
dilemmas. Washington, DC: World Bank. in bilinguals. Cognition, 114, 29-41. doi:10.1016/j.cogni-
Gladsjo, J. A., Shuman, C. C., Evans, J. D., Peavy, G. M., Miller, tion.2009.08.014
S. W., & Heaton, R. K. (1999). Norms for letter and category Malloy-Diniz, L. F., Bentes, R. C., Figueiredo, P. M., Brandãao-
fluency: Demographic corrections for age, education, and eth- Bretas, D., da Costa-Abrantes, S., Parizzi, A. M., .  .  . Salgado,
nicity. Assessment, 6, 147-178. J. V. (2007). Normalización de una batería de tests para
Goldstrohm, S. L., & Arffa, S. (2005). Preschool children with evaluar las habilidades de comprensión del lenguaje, fluidez
mild to moderate traumatic brain injury: An explora- verbal y denominación en niños brasileños de 7 a 10 años:
tion of immediate and post-acute morbidity. Archives of Resultados preliminares [Standardization of a battery of
Clinical Neuropsychology, 20, 675-695. doi:10.1016/j. tests to assess language comprehension skills, verbal fluency
acn.2005.02.005 and denomination in Brazilian children aged 7 to 10 years:
Gollan, T. H., Montoya, R. I., & Werner, G. A. (2002). Preliminary results]. Revista de Neurología, 44, 275-280.
Semantic and letter fluency in Spanish-English bilin- Manly, J. J., Touradji, P., Tang, M., & Stern, Y. (2003). Literacy
guals. Neuropsychology, 16, 562–576. doi:10.1037/0894- and memory decline among ethnically diverse elders. Journal
4105.16.4.562 of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 25, 680-690.
Hirsch, K. W. (1993). Age of acquisition and naming. In F. J. doi:10.1076/jcen.25.5.680.14579
Stachowiak (Ed.), Developments in the assessment and reha- Marino, J., & Alderete, A. M. (2010). Valores normativos de prue-
bilitation of brain-damaged patients (pp. 225-232). Tübingen, bas de fluidez verbal, categoriales, fonológicas, gramatica-
Germany: Gunter Narr Verlag. les y combinadas y análisis comparativo de la capacidad de
Hyde, J. S., & Linn, M. C. (1988). Gender differences in verbal iniciación [Normative values of verbal, categorical, phono-
ability: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 104, 53-69. logical, grammatical and combined fluency tests and compar-
Klenberg, L., Korkman, M., & Lahti-Nuuttila, P. (2001). ative analysis of initiation ability]. Revista Neuropsicología,
Differential development of attention and executive Neuropsiquiatría y Neurociencias, 10(1), 79-93.
functions in 3- to 12-year-old Finnish children. Developmen- Martins, R., Mograbi, D. C., Andrade Gabrig, I., & Charchat-
tal Neuropsychology, 20, 407-428. doi:10.1207/s15326942dn Fichman, H. (2016). Normative data and evidence of valid-
2001_6 ity for the Rey auditory verbal learning test, verbal fluency
Kochhann, R., Holz, M. R., Beber, B. C., Chaves, M. L. F., & test, and Stroop test with Brazilian children. Psychology &
Fonseca, R. P. (2018). Reading and writing habits as a predic- Neuroscience, 9, 54-67. doi:10.1037/pne0000041
tor of verbal fluency in elders. Psychology & Neuroscience, McCarthy, R. A., & Warrington, E. K. (1990). Cognitive neuropsy-
11, 39-49. chology: A clinical introduction. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
Korkman, M., Kemp, S. L., & Kirk, U. (2001). Effects of age on Meador, K. J., Baker, G. A., Browning, N., Clayton-Smith, J.,
neurocognitive measures of children ages 5 to 12: A cross- Cohen, M. J., Kalayjian, L. A., . . . Loring, D. W. (2011).
sectional study of 800 children from the United States. Relationship of child IQ to parental IQ and education in
Developmental Neuropsychology, 20, 331-354. doi:10.1207/ children with fetal antiepileptic drug exposure. Epilepsy &
s15326942dn2001_2 Behavior, 21, 147-152.
Kovacs, M. (1992). The children’s depression inventory manual. Mitrushina, M. (2005). Handbook of normative data for neuro-
Toronto, Ontario, Canada: Multi-Health Systems. psychological assessment. New York, NY: Oxford University
Kutner, M. H., Nachtsheim, C. J., Neter, J., & Li, W. (2005). Press.
Applied linear statistical models (5th ed.). New York, NY: Nieto, A., Galtier, I., Barroso, J., & Espinosa, G. (2008). Fluencia
McGraw-Hill. verbal en niños españoles en edad escolar: Estudio norma-
Lee, T. M. C., Yuen, K. S. L., & Chan, C. C. H. (2002). Normative tivo piloto y análisis de las estrategias organizativas [Verbal
data for neuropsychological measures of fluency, attention, fluency in Spanish children of school age: Pilot normative
and memory measures for Hong Kong Chinese. Journal of study and analysis of organizational strategies]. Revista de
Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 24, 615-632. Neurología, 46, 2-6.
Rivera et al. 13

Olabarrieta-Landa, L., Rivera, D., Ferrer-Cascales, R., Rodríguez of writing disorders in middle childhood. Developmental
Lorenzana, A., Galarza-del-Angel, J., Peñalver Guia, A. I., Behavioral Pediatrics, 13, 17-23.
. . .Arango-Lasprilla, J. C. (2017). Verbal fluency tests: Schady, N. (2011). Parents’ education, mothers’ vocabulary, and
Normative data for Spanish Speaking pediatric population. cognitive development in early childhood: Longitudinal evi-
Neurorehabilitation, 41, 673-686. dence from Ecuador. American Journal of Public Health,
Olabarrieta-Landa, L., Torre, E. L., López-Mugartza, J. C., 101, 2299-2307.
Bialystok, E., & Arango-Lasprilla, J. C. (2017). Verbal fluency Sohr-Preston, S. L., Scaramella, L. V., Martin, M. J., Neppl, T. K.,
tests: Developing a new model of administration and scoring Ontai, L., & Conger, R. (2013). Parental socioeconomic sta-
for Spanish language. NeuroRehabilitation, 41, 539-565. tus, communication, and children’s vocabulary development:
Oliviera, R. M., Mograbi, D. C., Andrade, I., & Charchat- A third-generation test of the family investment model. Child
Fichman, H. (2016). Normative data and evidence of validity Development, 84, 1046-1062.
for the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, Verbal Fluency Strauss, E., Sherman, E., & Spreen, O. (2006). A compendium
Test, and Stroop Test with Brazilian children. Psychology & of neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms, and
Neuroscience, 9, 54-67. commentary (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Oxford University
Ponton, M. O., Satz, P., Herrera, L., Ortiz, F., Urrutia, C., Young, R., Press.
D’Elia, L. F., . . . Namerow, N. (1996). Normative data strati- Sundheim, S. T., & Voeller, K. K. (2004). Psychiatric impli-
fied by age and education for the Neuropsychological Screening cations of language disorders and learning disabilities:
Battery for Hispanics (NeSBHIS): Initial report. Journal of the Risks and management. Journal of Child Neurology, 19,
International Neuropsychological Society, 2, 96-104. 814-826.
R Development Core Team. (2017). R: A language and environ- Tallberg, I. M., Carlsson, S., & Lieberman, M. (2011). Children’s
ment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation word fluency strategies. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology,
for Statistical Computing. Retrieved from http://www.R-proj- 52, 35-42.
ect.org/ Tombaugh, T. N., Kozak, J., & Rees, L. (1999). Normative data
Riva, D., Nichelli, F., & Devoti, M. (2000). Developmental aspects stratified by age and education for two measures of ver-
of verbal fluency and confrontation naming in children. Brain bal fluency: FAS and animal naming. Archives of Clinical
and Language, 71, 267-284. Neuropsychology, 14, 167-177.
Rivera, D., & Arango-Lasprilla, J. C. (2017). Methodology for the Van Breukelen, G. J., & Vlaeyen, J. W. (2005). Norming clinical
development of normative data for Spanish-speaking pediat- questionnaires with multiple regression: The Pain Cognition
ric populations. NeuroRehabilitation, 41, 581-592. List. Psychological Assessment, 17, 336-344.
Rivera, D., Olabarrieta-Landa, L., Van der Elst, W., Gonzalez, Van der Elst, W., Hurks, P., Wassenberg, R., Meijs, C., & Jolles,
I., Rodríguez-Agudelo, Y., Aguayo, A. A., . . . Arango- J. (2011). Animal verbal fluency and design fluency in school
Lasprilla, J. C. (2019). Normative data for verbal fluency aged children: Effects of age, sex, and mean level of parental
in healthy Latin American adults: Letter M, and fruits and education, and regression-based normative data. Journal of
occupations categories. Neuropsychology, 33, 287-300. Clinical & Experimental Neuropsychology, 33, 1005-1015.
doi:10.1037/neu0000518 doi:10.1080/13803395.2011.589509
Ruff, R. M., Light, R. H., Parker, S. B., & Levin, H. S. (1997). Weir, J. P. (2005). Quantifying test-retest reliability using the
The psychological construct of word fluency. Brain and intraclass correlation coefficient and the SEM. Journal of
Language, 57, 394-405. Strength and Conditioning Research, 19, 231-240. doi:10.
Ruffieux, N., Njamnshi, A. K., Mayer, E., Sztajzel, R., Eta, S. C., 1519/15184.1
Doh, R. F., . . . Hauert, C. A. (2009). Neuropsychology in Willcutt, E. G., Pennington, B. F., Olson, R. K., Chhabildas, N.,
Cameroon: First normative data for cognitive tests among & Hulslander, J. (2005). Neuropsychological analysis of
school-aged children. Child Neuropsychology, 16, 1-19. comorbidity between reading disability and attention defi-
Sandler, A. D., Watson, T. E., Footo, M., Levine, M. D., Coleman, cit hyperactivity disorder: In search of the common deficit.
W. L., & Hooper, S. R. (1992). Neurodevelopmental study Developmental Neuropsychology, 27, 35-78.

You might also like