You are on page 1of 8

Journal of Affective Disorders 210 (2017) 241–248

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Affective Disorders


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jad

Research paper

Cognitive control dysfunction in emotion dysregulation and MARK


psychopathology of major depression (MD): Evidence from transcranial
brain stimulation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC)

Mohammad Ali Salehinejada,b, , Elham Ghanavaic, Reza Rostamia, Vahid Nejatid
a
Department of Psychology, University of Tehran, Tehran, Iran
b
Department of Psychology, University of Kansas, Lawrence, KS, USA
c
Department of Psychology, Islamic Azad University, Science & Research Branch, Tehran, Iran
d
Institute for Cognitive & Brain Sciences, Shahid Beheshti University, Tehran, Iran

A R T I C L E I N F O A BS T RAC T

Keywords: Background: Previous studies showed that MD is associated with a variety of cognitive deficits and
Major depression (MD) executive dysfunctions which can persist even in remitted states. However, the role of cognitive impairments
Executive functions in MD psychopathology and treatment is not fully understood. This article aims to discuss how executive
Cognition functions central components (e.g., Working memory and attention) mediate MD psychopathology considering
Emotion
the role of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dLPFC) and present findings of a brain stimulation experiment to
tDCS
support this notion.
Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC)
Methods: The effect of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) of the dLPFC on enhancing cognitive
control functions was investigated. Twenty-four patients with MD (Experimental group=12, Control group=12)
received 10 sessions of tDCS (2 mA for 30 min) over 10 consecutive days. The experimental group received
active stimulation and the control group received sham stimulation. Participant's performance on cognitive
functions (PAL, SRM, RVP and CRT from CANTAB) and their depression scores were assessed before and after
tDCS.
Results: Results showed that brain stimulation of the dLPFC improved executive dysfunction in patients and a
significant improvement on depression scores was also observed suggesting that cognitive control dysfunction
may be a mediator in emotional dysregulation and psychopathology of MD.
Limitations: No follow-up investigation was done in this study which does not allow to infer long-term effect of
tDCS. Low-focality of tDCS might have stimulated adjacent areas too.
Conclusion: Cognitive components, namely cognitive control dysfunction, play role in MD psychopathology as
they are involved in emotion dysregulation in MD. The amount of contribution of cognitive components in MD
psychopathology is however, an open question. tDCS can be used as an intervention to improve cognitive
dysfunction in MD.

1. Background cognitive deficits might persist beyond the acute stages of illness in
MD (Bora et al., 2013). In addition to cognition, emotion is also
As one of the most incapatiating and prevalent conditions world- impaired in MD in terms of emotion dysregulation and sustained
wide, major depression (MD) is characterized by altered cognitive and negative affects that are suggested to be central to MD psychopathology
emotional functioning (Diener et al., 2012) marked with various (Gotlib and Joormann, 2010). Although studies have shown an
emotional and cognitive impairments (Davidson et al., 2002; interaction between cognition and emotion in MD, the way they
Marazziti et al., 2010; Nezhad et al., 2011). A large number of studies interact has been subject to controversy and most studies prioritized
show that MD is associated with cognitive deficits in several domains emotional components over cognitive components in MD psycho-
including different types of memory, attention, executive functions pathology.
(EF), perception and psychomotor skills (Marazziti et al., 2010; Rock Previous studies suggest that cognitive components have substan-
et al., 2014; Wagner et al., 2012) and studies even suggest that tial implications in MD psychopathology. These studies suggest that


Correspondence to: Institute for Cognitive & Brain Sciences, Shahid Beheshti University, Daneshjoo Blvd, Velenjak, Tehran, Iran.
E-mail address: salehinejadmohammadali@gmail.com (M.A. Salehinejad).

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2016.12.036
Received 1 July 2016; Received in revised form 8 November 2016; Accepted 17 December 2016
Available online 31 December 2016
0165-0327/ © 2016 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
M.A. Salehinejad et al. Journal of Affective Disorders 210 (2017) 241–248

biased cognitive processing is related to emotion dysregulation in MD Grafman, 2009; Long et al., 2015; Ochsner and Gross, 2005; Roiser
(Gotlib and Joormann, 2010; Nitschke et al., 2004) which is aligned and Sahakian, 2013). Studies showed that depressed individuals have
with cognitive theories of depression (e.g., Beck et al., 1987; Ellis and poor WM (Rock et al., 2014; Rose and Ebmeier, 2006; Wang et al.,
Grieger, 1986). However, empirical studies about the biased cognitive 2015) and impaired attentional processing (Clark et al., 2005; Maalouf
functions in MD yielded mixed results (Gotlib and Joormann, 2010). et al., 2010) leading to a biased processing of information.
In this study, we first discuss how cognition and emotion are It has been shown that there is an imbalance of function between
interrelated with a particular focus on role of the prefrontal cortex the right and left dLPFC in depression known as the “imbalance
(PFC). We will also argue how cognitive impairments, specifically the hypothesis of MD” (Grimm et al., 2008; Nitsche et al., 2009a; Plewnia
PFC-related executive dysfunction, are related to emotion dysregula- et al., 2015). According to this notion there is a relative hypoactivity
tion and psychopathology of MD and how they interact with emotional and hyperactivity in the left and right dLPFC respectively in MD
components. We then present findings of a brain stimulation experi- (Grimm et al., 2008; Nitschke et al., 2004). This imbalance of function
ment to support the above notion. in the dLPFC is associated with impaired WM (Fregni et al., 2005;
Nitschke et al., 2004) and attention (Sánchez-Navarro et al., 2014;
1.1. PFC, cognitive functions and emotion Schafer and Moore, 2011) in MD leading to affective and attentional
biases toward negative information, which in turn, reinforces depres-
The PFC is a collection of cortical regions that have interconnec- sive symptoms (Plewnia et al., 2015).
tions with almost all cortical and subcortical structures, involved in This imbalance of function influences emotional processing in some
affect and cognition (Miller and Cohen, 2001). According to Miller and ways one of which is the inability to initiate and maintain attention
Cohen (2001), cognitive control or goal-directed action is the primary toward emotional materials specifically toward neutral and positive
function of the PFC. To fulfil a goal-directed action we need to keep materials (Sánchez-Navarro et al., 2014). Depressed individuals with
task-relevant information active and inhibit task-irrelevant informa- hypoactivity in the l-dLPFC have difficulties in affect-guided planning
tion. Working memory (WM) and attentional control are necessary for and stopping automatic responses that preserve negative affects and
keeping task-relevant information active as well as manipulating that attitudes (Davidson et al., 2002). In addition, they are less sensitive to
information to accomplish behavioural goals (Gazzaniga et al., 2014). rewards and positive affects but more responsive to punishment and
Lateral regions of the PFC especially the left dorsolateral PFC (l- negative affects as a result of cognitive control dysfunction (Must et al.,
dLPFC) are suggested to be responsible for these PFC-related cognitive 2006). Biased self-referential processing (Lemogne et al., 2010) and
functions (Miller and Cohen, 2001; Ochsner and Gross, 2005). impaired strategy utilization (Hertel, 2000) are other cognitive com-
The PFC is not only important for cognitive control functions but ponents in MD associated with the decreased dLPFC activity. There is
also for affective states and emotional processing (Davidson, 2002). also a hyperactivity in the right PFC specially the r-dLPFC that is
The critical role of the PFC in goal-directed behaviours explains why it associated with negative memory bias (Grimm et al., 2008; Nitschke
is important for regulation of mood and emotion. In situations where et al., 2004). These findings support the notion that impaired proces-
mood and emotion are involved, the arousal of emotion is usually sing of emotional information in MD which is related to abnormalities
inconsistent with other goals that have already been expressed in the PFC, especially dLPFC, leads to failure in appropriate goal-
(Davidson et al., 2002) and the PFC is required to signal to other directed responses to emotional stimuli.
brain regions to produce an appropriate response. For example in In sum, neuroimaging studies indicate an imbalanced cortical
experience of loss, the availability of continuous sadness may not be activity in the dLPFC regions that are associated with cognitive
adaptive to a person's goals. In such case the PFC is required to impairment and emotional difficulties in MD indicating that cognition
produce adaptive emotion-based decision making in service of the and emotion interact in MD psychopathology. Indeed, negative affects
goals. and attitudes and emotional dysregulation may occur because the PFC
Cognitive regulation of emotions is one of the cognitive mechan- fails to guide appropriate response in emotional situations to serve the
isms through which the PFC is involved in emotional processing. person's goal. This implies that emotional dysregulation, which is
Cognitive regulation of emotions is a major emotion regulatory strategy suggested to be central in MD psychopathology (Davidson et al., 2002),
that regulates emotional responsivity through cognitive control might be dependent on impaired cognitive control of the PFC which
(Ochsner and Gross, 2005). In contrast to behavioural regulation, may suggest that cognitive impairment rather than emotional impair-
cognitive regulation moderates unpleasant experience, neutralizes ment is primary in MD psychopathology. This also suggests that by
negative experience and might decrease physiological arousal (Gross, improving cognitive control functions in MD, we can expect an overall
2002; Salehinejad et al., 2017). Attentional control, a PFC-related improvement in negative emotions and affects of depressed indivi-
function, is a major type of cognitive regulation and is often referred to duals.
“selective aspect of information processing”, enabling us to focus on
goal-relevant information and ignore goal-irrelevant ones (Ochsner 1.3. Brain stimulation studies
and Gross, 2005). It facilitates cognitive appraisal of emotions by
assessing the significance of stimuli to current goals (Scherer et al., Recent studies highlighted the importance of noninvasive brain
2001) and then channelling appropriate emotional response (Kalisch stimulation as a mean of modulating cortical excitability (Bestmann
et al., 2006). In sum, studies indicate that the lateral PFC regions are et al., 2015; Parkin et al., 2015). Transcranial direct current stimula-
related to control processes over cognitive functions as well as tion (tDCS) is a neuromodulation technique in which a weak direct
emotions. current, applied on the scalp, reaches the brain and induces shifts in
membrane resting potentials and modulates cortical excitability
1.2. PFC and cognitive-emotional abnormalities in MD (Nitsche et al., 2009a). Anodal stimulation increases cortical excit-
ability, whereas cathodal stimulation has a reverse effect (Utz et al.,
MD is marked with structural and functional abnormalities in the 2010). Neuromodulation studies have shown that anodal tDCS over the
brain especially in the PFC regions (Price and Drevets, 2012; Ye et al., l-dLPFC improves WM, attention and emotional functioning in MD
2012) which are related to cognitive and emotional impairments (Ferrucci et al., 2009; Loo et al., 2012; Salehinejad et al., 2015).
(Davidson et al., 2002; Diener et al., 2012; Drevets and Furey, 2009). Moreover, the neuropsychological characterization of the l-dLPFC
Neuroimaging studies suggest that the lateral PFC regions are more hypoactivity and r-dLPFC hyperactivity and its association with MD
implicated in cognitive dysfunction whereas the medial PFC regions are psychopathology is poorly understood (Grimm et al., 2008).
more associated with control of emotions and affects (Koenigs and Based on the findings suggesting an asymmetry of function in the

242
M.A. Salehinejad et al. Journal of Affective Disorders 210 (2017) 241–248

left and right dLPFC, we ran a tDCS experiment with a specific montage
to see whether modulating cortical activity in the dLPFC would
improve cognitive control functions, namely the WM and attentional
control in MD; and whether such improvement in cognitive control
leads to an improvement in negative emotions and affect presumably
through increased cognitive control over emotion and emotional
regulation. The l-dLPFC was selected as the main site of anodal
stimulation, which is hypothesized to increase cortical activity in l-
dLPFC; and the r-dLPFC was selected as the main site of cathodal
stimulation, which is suggested to decrease cortical activity in r-dLPFC.
We applied this specific tDCS montage based on the imbalance
hypothesis in MD (Grimm et al., 2008), that takes into account both
Fig. 1. Experiment procedure in experimental (red) and control (blue) groups.
the left and right dLFPC which is also the suggested montage for
tDCS=transcranial direct current stimulation; PAL=Paired Associates Learning;
studying cognitive effects of tDCS in MD (Brunoni et al., 2016). SRM=Spatial Recognition Memory; RVP=Rapid Visual Information Processing;
CRT=Choice Reaction Time. (For interpretation of the references to color in this figure
2. Methods legend, the reader is referred to the web version of this article.)

2.1. Participants
beginning of tDCS sessions as baseline and once right after 10th tDCS
We enrolled twenty four patients aged from 18 to 41 years session. All patients were blind to the type of tDCS delivered in each
diagnosed with unipolar, non-psychotic MD according to DSM-IV session. Participants in the control group were assigned to other
criteria confirmed by the participant's scores on the Beck Depression therapeutic protocols by end of the study (See Fig. 1).
Inventory (BDI) (Beck et al., 1961) and the Hamilton Depression
Rating Scale (HDRS) (Hamilton, 1960). Demographic data are shown 2.3. tDCS protocol
in Table 1. The patients were recruited from the local university Atieh
Mental Health Clinic. Inclusion criteria were: (1) not to be on The tDCS device in use was “ActivaDose Iontophoresis” manufac-
antidepressant or other psychotropic medications during the study tured by Activa Tek, battery-driven with a 9-volt battery as its source.
(2) moderate to severe depression score based on BDI (scores close to Electrical direct current of 2 mA generated by the stimulator was
29 and higher); (3) HDRS score of at least 20 (scored by an experienced applied through a pair of saline-soaked sponge electrodes with size of
psychiatrist); (4) MD diagnosis based on a clinical interview by an 35 cm2 (7×5) for 20 min (with 15 s ramp up and 15 s ramp down).
experienced psychiatrist according to the DSM-IV criteria. Exclusion Anode electrode was positioned over area F3 (l-dLPFC) and cathode
criteria were other axis I and any axis II (personality and develop- electrode was positioned over F4 (r-dLPFC) according to the 10–20
mental) disorders. The study was conducted according to the EEG International System. This specific montage is the suggested
Declaration of Helsinki ethical standards and approved by the local montage for studying cognitive effects of the tDCS in depression (e.g.,
Institutional Review Board and the Ethical Committee. Patients gave Brunoni et al., 2016).
their informed consent before participation and were free to withdraw
from study at any phase. 2.3.1. Cognitive measurement
WM and attention, as components of cognitive control, were
2.2. Materials and procedure assessed using the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated
Battery (CANTAB) (Robbins et al., 1994). CANTAB is designed with
Participants were randomly assigned in two groups of experimental a significant focus on neuropsychological functions subserved by the
(N=12) and control (N=12). The experimental group received 20-min frontal lobe regions that mediate motor, cognitive and behavioural
active stimulation per day for 10 consecutive days and the control functions (Fray et al., 1996). It's been widely used in studies investigat-
group received 20-min sham stimulation. During sham condition, ing cognitive functions in neuropsychological diseases including MD
electrical current was ramped up for 30 s to generate the same (Clark et al., 2009; Egerhazi et al., 2013; Owen et al., 1995; Sahakian
sensation as active condition for the participant in the sham group et al., 1990). One reason to use CANTAB for cognitive measurement
and then turned off (Palm et al., 2013). A side-effect survey was done was its sensitivity to cognitive functions especially in studies involving
after tDCS sessions; all participants tolerated the tDCS treatment well passage of electrical current on the frontal and temporal regions
and no adverse effects were reported. Participants completed the (Falconer et al., 2010). Moreover, brain stimulation studies showed
measures (CANTAB tests for measuring WM and attention and BDI that tDCS interventions are task specific especially during anodal
and HDRS for measuring mood and depressive scores) once before stimulation of the l-dLPFC (Pope et al., 2015). It is suggested that

Table 1
Demographic variables of participants.

Experimentalgroup Controlgroup

Sample size (n) 12 12


Age – Mean (SD) 26.8 (7.1) 25.5 (4.6)
Gender – Male (female) 5 (7) 4 (8)
Marital Status – Single (married) 8 (4) 7 (5)
Antidepressant use during last 6 months (n) 10 11
Education Diploma 7 6
BA 3 3
MA or higher 2 3

M=Mean; SD=Standard Deviation; BA=Bachelor of Arts; MA=Master of Arts.

243
M.A. Salehinejad et al. Journal of Affective Disorders 210 (2017) 241–248

anodal tDCS over the l-dLPFC would affect performance when a 3. Results
cognitive task is attentionally demanding and especially difficult to
perform, and perhaps when it engages specific WM operations (Pope Overview of data showed different performance on the tasks in both
et al., 2015). The battery used in the present study was cognitively groups (Table 2). Results showed that anodal l-dLPFC / cathodal r-
demanding and specific to frontal functions. dLPFC tDCS had significant effects on impaired WM and attention
A four-test CANTAB battery (30–32 min duration) selected from performance of patients. First of all, the effect of tDCS on PAL test was
CANTAB memory tests, attention tests and depression battery was investigated. ANOVA results for memory score indicated a significant
used in this study. Paired Associates Learning (PAL) and Spatial main effect of tDCS (F=44.83, p < 0.1) and group (F=5.60, p < 0.2) but
Recognition Memory (SRM) were selected as measures of WM and no significant interaction effect was observed (F=2.47, p=0.13).
Rapid Visual Information Processing (RVP) and Choice Reaction Time ANOVA results of error scores also indicated a significant main effect
(CRT) were selected as attention measures. It is of note that we only of tDCS (F=39.93, p < 0.01), a significant main effect of group (F=6.20,
included tests of WM and attention, as two major components of p < 0.02) and a significant interaction between the two factors
cognitive control, in this battery since we are primarily interested in (F=10.74, p < 0.01). These results show that tDCS significantly im-
studying cognitive control functions in MD. proved visual WM of patients compared to their baseline performance
The PAL test assesses visual WM by displaying boxes on the screen and effect of tDCS on memory performance depends on group factor.
that one or more of them will contain a pattern. The patterns are then ANOVA results of the SRM also showed a significant main effect of
displayed in the middle of the screen, one at a time, and the participant tDCS (F=58.87, p < 0.01) and group (F=5.04, p < 0.03) on memory
must touch the box where the pattern was originally located. This test scores. A significant interaction was also found (F=10.58, p < 0.01)
lasts around 10 min and performance on it is assessed in terms of the indicating that patients who received active tDCS performed signifi-
latency and memory scores. The SRM is a test of visual spatial cantly better than the ones who received sham stimulation. The same
recognition memory in a 2-choice forced discrimination paradigm. pattern was also observed for latency scores of participants on SRM
Participants are presented with a white square, which appears in (Table 3) ( Fig. 2).
sequence at five different locations on the screen. In the recognition We also evaluated whether tDCS could improve performance on
phase, participants see a series of five pairs of squares, one of which is attentional functions as a major component of cognitive control.
in a place previously seen in the presentation phase. The other square ANOVA results of the RVP test showed a significant interaction effect
is in a location not seen in the presentation phase. It takes about 5 min (F=7.31, p < 0.01) indicating that patients who received active tDCS
to complete and the performance is assessed in terms of latency and performed significantly better on the attention task than the ones
accuracy. The RVP is a sensitive measure of sustained visual attention received sham stimulation. The main effects of tDCS (F=69.91, p <
(Robbins et al., 1994) and presents participants a white box in the 0.01) and group (F=5.52, p < 0.03) in RVP scores were also significant.
center of the computer screen, inside which digits from 2 to 9 appear in Attentional performance was also measured using the CRT which
a pseudo-random order at the rate of 100 digits per minute. requires less attentional loading compared to the RVP. Results showed
Participants are asked to detect target sequences of digits and register significant main effect of tDCS for all output measures including
responses using the press pad. It takes about 10 min to complete and latency time (F=14.41, p < 0.01), percentage of correct (F=122.96, p
the output measure include latency which is a good indicator of < 0.01) and number of errors (F=34.77, p < 0.01). However, no
sustained attentional function. Finally, the CRT is a 2-choice reaction significant interaction or significant main effect of group was observed
time test during which participants should press the left hand button if except for the main effect of group in correct responses (F=16.62, p <
the stimulus is displayed on the left hand side of the screen, and the 0.01) (Table 4). The differed difficulty level of RVP and CRT tests could
right hand button if the stimulus is displayed on the right hand side of possibly be one reason why tDCS had more robust effect on RVP
the screen. It takes about 6 min to complete and output measures performance as studies showed that tDCS is more effective in tasks that
include latency, accuracy and number errors. require more cognitive loading (Brunoni et al., 2016) (Fig. 3).
In addition to cognitive control functions, participant's scores on
the BDI and HDRS were significantly improved implying that cognitive
2.4. Emotional and mood assessment
control improvement was a mediator of changes in emotional dysre-
gulation and negative mood of participants although a causal relation-
To assess emotional functioning and mood we used two well-known
ship cannot be drawn here (Fig. 4). ANOVA results showed a significant
depression inventories that measure depressive symptoms and mood:
interaction effect (F=21.81, p < 0.01), a significant main effect of tDCS
the BDI and HDRS. The BDI is a self-reported twenty-one questions
(F=162.61, p < 0.01) but non-significant main effect of group (F=0.25,
inventory about how the subject has been feeling in the last week and
p=0.61) on BDI scores. ANOVA result of HDRS also show the same
each question has four answers ranging in intensity. The HDRS is a
pattern with significant interaction effect (F=18.79, p < 0.01), a sig-
multiple items questionnaire designed for measuring adult depression
nificant main effect of tDCS (F=157.01, p < 0.01) but a non-significant
and is administrated by health care professional which is currently the
main effect of group (F=1.56, p=0.22). This shows that tDCS signifi-
most common depression measure used worldwide (Marijnissen et al.,
cantly reduced depressive symptoms in patients who received active
2002). Both measures are designed to indicate presence of depressive
stimulation.
symptoms in a past number of days.
4. Discussion
2.5. Statistical analysis
We discussed MD psychopathology from a cognitive neuroscience
The data was analyzed using PASW Statistics-SPSS 21.0. To point of view focusing on the role of the dLPFC in cognitive and
examine effects of tDCS on cognitive functions we employed a 2×2 emotional impairments. Cognitive and emotional functions are two
Mixed ANOVA with tDCS condition (pre-stimulation/post-stimulation) major areas that are severely impaired in MD but the way they interact
as within-subject factor, group (active/sham) as between-subject factor has been subject of controversy. Previous neuroimaging studies
and scores on CANTAB as dependent variables. A similar 2×2 Mixed suggested the dLPFC and the vMPFC as regions involved in cognitive
ANOVA was used for measuring effects of tDCS on mood. Our analyses and emotional processing in MD respectively (Diener et al., 2012;
of variance (ANOVA) met linear assumptions and Leven's test was used Koenigs and Grafman, 2009; Long et al., 2015; Roiser and Sahakian,
to examine homogeneity of variances. A significance level of P < 0.05 2013). Brain stimulation studies also showed cognitive and emotional
was used for all statistical comparisons. improvements in MD after modulating cortical activity in the PFC

244
M.A. Salehinejad et al. Journal of Affective Disorders 210 (2017) 241–248

Table 2
Means and SDs of cognitive tasks and depression measures.

Task Experimental pre-stimulation Experimental post-stimulation Control pre-stimulation Control post-stimulation


M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD)

PAL (memory scores) 12.67 (1.46) 15.86 (2.32) 11.78 (1.72) 13.75( (1.61)
PAL (error) 18.91 (2.83) 14.78 (2.34) 19.55 (2.05) 18.24 (1.72)
SRM (% correct) 60.65 (5.17) 67.41 (7.94) 58.38 (2.61) 61.11 (2.77)
SRM (latency)a 3233.36 (355.64) 2391.31 (691.40) 3271.46 (405.67) 3102.53 (394.27)
RVP (latency)a 468.09 (50.20) 400.60 (53.57) 492.01 (34.57) 457.50 (37.41)
CRT (latency)a 465.65 (37.54) 433.13 (46.70) 476.05 (35.86) 460.66 (34.75)
CRT (% correct) 88.91 (4.58) 95.08 (3.57) 77.66 (9.17) 82.91 (9.28)
CRT (error) 7.58 (2.84) 5.33 (2.42) 8.75 (1.13) 7.50 (2.06)
BDI 33.66 (6.45) 17.50 (4.29 28.58 (2.64) 21.08 (2.39)
HDRS 24.58 (2.57) 12.75 (3.36) 22.56 (1.92) 16.83 (2.62)

PAL=Paired Associates Learning; SRM=Spatial Recognition Memory; RVP=Rapid Visual Information Processing; CRT=Choice Reaction Time; M=Mean; SD=Standard Deviation.
a
Values marked by (a) are in ms.

Table 3 Table 4
ANOVA results of tDCS effects on CANTAB memory tests. ANOVA results of tDCS effects on CANTAB attention tests.

Cognitive functions df Mean square F Significance Eta squared Cognitive functions df Mean square F Significance Eta squared

PAL (memory RVP (latency)


scores) tDCS 1.22 31212.01 69.91 .01 0.76
tDCS 1.22 79.67 44.83 .01 0.67 Group 1.22 19594.01 5.52 .03 0.20
Group 1.22 26.85 5.60 .02 0.20 tDCS*Group 1.22 3263.70 7.31 .01 0.25
tDCS*Group 1.22 4.40 2.47 .13 0.11
CRT (latency)
PAL (error) tDCS 1.22 6888.02 14.41 .01 0.40
tDCS 1.22 88.89 39.93 .01 0.64 Group 1.22 4317.19 1.68 .21 0.71
Group 1.22 50.34 6.20 .02 0.22 tDCS*Group 1.22 880.65 1.84 .18 0.77
tDCS*Group 1.22 23.91 10.74 .01 0.32
CRT (% correct)
SRM (% correct) tDCS 1.22 391.02 122.96 .01 0.85
tDCS 1.22 292.17 58.87 .01 0.72 Group 1.22 1645.02 16.62 .01 0.43
Group 1.22 238.39 5.04 .03 0.18 tDCS*Group 1.22 2.521 0.793 .38 0.34
tDCS*Group 1.22 52.56 10.58 .01 0.32
CRT (error)
SRM (latency) tDCS 1.22 36.750 34.77 .01 0.61
tDCS 1.22 3321680.94 25.44 .01 0.53 Group 1.22 33.33 3.82 .06 0.15
Group 1.22 1824804.10 5.51 .03 0.20 tDCS*Group 1.22 3.01 2.83 .10 0.11
tDCS*Group 1.22 1472550.96 11.27 .01 0.33
RVP=Rapid Visual Information Processing; CRT=Choice Reaction Time; Transcranial
PAL=Paired Associates Learning; SRM=Spatial Recognition Memory; tDCS=Transcra- Direct Current Stimulation; Significant results are highlighted (p≤0.05) in bold.
nial Direct Current Stimulation; Significant results are highlighted (p≤0.05) in bold.

Fig. 2. Memory scores (PAL on the left, SRM on the right) after tDCS in both groups. PAL=Paired Associates Learning; SRM=Spatial Recognition Memory; MS=Memory Score;
Corr=Number of Corrects; E=Experimental Group; C=Control Group.

245
M.A. Salehinejad et al. Journal of Affective Disorders 210 (2017) 241–248

negative affects and emotion dysregulation in MD are associated with


impaired cognitive control that are related to functional and structural
abnormalities in the dLPFC. Therefore, by improving cognitive control
functions, such as WM and attention, it is not surprising to expect
emotion regulation and therapeutic effects in MD.
Results of the present study support the above notion. Based on the
“imbalance hypothesis of MD” (Grimm et al., 2008) and neurocognitive
model of depression characterized by dLPFC hypoactivity (Plewnia
et al., 2015), we applied a specific tDCS montage to increase and
decrease cortical activity in the left and right dLPFC respectively in
order to improve WM and attention and see how such improvement
affects depressive symptoms. Results showed a significant improve-
ment in WM and attention scores of participants after dLPFC tDCS. A
significant improvement was also observed in mood scores of patients
Fig. 3. Attention scores after tDCS in both groups. RVP=Rapid Visual Information which we think is related to improved cognitive control deficits in MD.
Processing; CRT=Choice Reaction Time; E=Experimental Group; C=Control Group. An improved WM helps to better direct and guide responses to
emotional materials by maintaining all emotional information and
not just the negative one, online and available; improved attentional
control after dLPFC tDCS also enables a depressed patient to initiate
and maintain attention toward negative and positive emotional stimuli
in an unbiased way. Consequently, a functional cognitive control allows
depressed people to process emotional and neutral stimuli unbiasedly
(Plewnia et al., 2015) and enables them to utilize good-enough strategy
in emotional situations (e.g., withdrawal form negative affects). Such
improved cognitive control also modulates hypersensitivity to negative
emotions as a result of unbiased attentional processing of emotional
materials (Ochsner and Gross, 2005). This is also aligned with studies
showing that people who have more left-sided frontal activation are
better able to voluntarily suppress negative emotion (Jackson et al.,
2000). This study therefore suggests that emotional dysregulation, is
significantly associated with dysfunctional cognitive control of the PFC
and by improving cognitive control functions we can expect an
improvement in depression symptoms.
Fig. 4. MD scores after tDCS in both groups. BDI=Beck Depression Inventory; What our study found is considerable from several points: first of
HDRS=Hamilton Depression Rating Scale; E=Experimental Group; C=Control Group. all, it supported the notion that the interaction between emotion and
cognition in depression psychopathology is at least partly mediated by
cognitive components. That is, the emotional dysregulation in depres-
sion might result from impaired cognitive control functions caused by
regions specifically the dLPFC (Brunoni and Vanderhasselt, 2014; abnormalities in the dLPFC and supports the cognitive dysfunction
Ferrucci et al., 2009; Salehinejad et al., 2015; Shiozawa et al., 2014). treatment in MD. However, it should be noted that the present study
However, the role of cognitive control improvement in emotional cannot claim a causal role of cognitive components in emotional
regulation of MD has not been studied enough. dysregulation observed in MD as we could not control for other
Understanding functional mechanisms of the PFC regions allows us possible mediating variables. The findings rather suggest that cognitive
to realize how cognition and emotion interact in MD. MD is character- control components are considerably associated with emotional dysre-
ized by emotional dysregulation as hallmark feature of depression gulation in MD. Although numerous studies reported therapeutic
(Davidson et al., 2002). It appears that there are several cognitive effects in depression after modulating cortical activity (Kalu et al.,
mechanisms underlying emotion dysregulation in MD. One such 2012; Nitsche et al., 2009b), few of them emphasized on the role of
mechanism is biased cognitive processing resulting from deficits in cognitive functions in both psychopathology and treatment of depres-
WM and inhibitory processes (Gotlib and Joormann, 2010). In sion (Hammar and Årdal, 2009).
situations with abnormalities in the PFC like MD, these cognitive A recent tDCS study investigated whether cognitive effects of tDCS
control components are impaired and negatively affect processing of are associated with changes in depressive symptoms or not (Brunoni
emotional information. Inability to initiate and maintain attention et al., 2016). Although the study found robust depression improvement
toward both positive and negative emotional materials (Sánchez- but no beneficial or deleterious cognitive effects of tDCS was reported.
Navarro et al., 2014; Duque and Vázquez, 2015), impaired strategy Results of Brunoni et al. (2016) study should be considered from
utilization in confronting emotional stimuli (Hertel, 2000), hypersen- several aspects: first of all, their sample included moderately depressed
sitivity to negative events and affects (Gotlib and Joormann, 2010), and individuals with normal cognitive scores at baseline while tDCS effects
negative self-referential processing (Lemogne et al., 2010) are some are shown to be more effective in populations with more severe
emotional consequences of impaired cognitive control in MD. cognitive deficits (Hill et al., 2016). Secondly, neurpsychological
The other PFC-driven mechanism involved in MD is cognitive assessments were measured after 6 weeks of treatment and it is
regulation of emotions which is suggested to modulate emotional possible that cognitive improvement and the emotional regulation
responsiveness (Ochsner and Gross, 2005). Cognitive regulation mod- associated with it occurred immediately after or during the daily tDCS
erates unpleasant experience, neutralizes negative experience and phase. Finally, the tasks used in the study were not sensitive or specific
might decrease physiological arousal (Gross, 2002; Salehinejad et al., enough to detect cognitive changes (Brunoni et al., 2016) and the
2017) and attentional control, as a function of the PFC, is one of the authors recommended using computerized cognitive tasks that are
major types of cognitive regulation (Ochsner and Gross, 2005). In sum, sensitive to the dLPFC activity and collecting both accuracy and
reaction time in future studies. In our study however, we applied a

246
M.A. Salehinejad et al. Journal of Affective Disorders 210 (2017) 241–248

computerized battery sensitive to frontal lobe functions which was Declaration of conflicting interests
attentionaly demanding and sensitive to passage of electrical current
on the frontal lobe (Falconer et al., 2010). We also measured both The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect
accuracy and reaction time of patients in cognitive tasks. to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
In addition to cognitive control dysfunction and its association with
MD psychopathology, our study suggests a tDCS montage for treating Funding
cognitive deficits in depression. Almost all studies evaluating tDCS
effects on WM and attention in MD, applied anodal stimulation over This research did not receive any specific grant from funding
the l-dLPFC while applying cathodal stimulation over the right supra- agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.
orbital area. These studies mostly emphasized on improving WM not
necessarily as a way to improve emotional regulation in MD (Fregni Acknowledgments
et al., 2006; Loo et al., 2012). However, our study proposes that
application of anodal tDCS over the l-dLPFC concurrently with We appreciate anonymous reviewers for their helpful and insightful
cathodal tDCS over the r-dLPFC, modulates imbalanced cortical comments. We also thank patients for participating in the study.
activity in the dLPFC and thus improves cognitive control components,
which is supposed to improve emotional dysregulation in MD. This References
specific tDCS montage is based on the “imbalance hypothesis of MD”
according to which there is a hyperactivity in the contralateral regions Beck, A.T., Brown, G., Steer, R.A., Eidelson, J.I., Riskind, J.H., 1987. Differentiating
of the l-dLPFC (Grimm et al., 2008). Cathodal stimulation over the r- anxiety and depression: a test of the cognitive content-specificity hypothesis. J.
Abnorm. Psychol. 96, 179–183.
dLPFC would theoretically decrease the r-dLPFC activity and thus Beck, A.T., Ward, C., Mendelson, M., 1961. Beck depression inventory (BDI). Arch. Gen.
facilitates cognitive enhancement (Brunoni et al., 2016). Psychiatry 4, 561–571.
It is noteworthy that the present study cannot suggest a causal Bestmann, S., de Berker, A.O., Bonaiuto, J., 2015. Understanding the behavioural
consequences of noninvasive brain stimulation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 19, 13–20.
relationship between cognitive components and MD psychopathology Bora, E., Harrison, B.J., Yücel, M., Pantelis, C., 2013. Cognitive impairment in euthymic
despite the fact that the tDCS montage used in this study was a major depressive disorder: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Med. 43, 2017–2026.
cognitive-enhancing montage based on the PFC-related cognitive Brunoni, Vanderhasselt, M.-A., 2014. Working memory improvement with non-invasive
brain stimulation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex: a systematic review and meta-
dysfunctions and is the suggested montage for cognitive treatment of
analysis. Brain Cogn. 86, 1–9.
MD (Brunoni et al., 2016). For example, it is possible that cognitive Brunoni, A.R., Tortella, G., Benseñor, I.M., Lotufo, P.A., Carvalho, A.F., Fregni, F., 2016.
dysfunction is improved by reduction in depression symptoms. The Cognitive effects of transcranial direct current stimulation in depression: results
from the SELECT-TDCS trial and insights for further clinical trials. J. Affect. Disord.
methodology and analysis used in this study cannot suggest such causal
202, 46–52.
relationship and to draw such conclusion, future studies need to Clark, L., Chamberlain, S.R., Sahakian, B.J., 2009. Neurocognitive mechanisms in
replicate the findings by controlling emotional components. The depression: implications for treatment. Annu. Rev. Neurosci., 57–74.
findings of the present study would at most suggest that cognitive Clark, L., Kempton, M.J., Scarnà, A., Grasby, P.M., Goodwin, G.M., 2005. Sustained
attention-deficit confirmed in euthymic bipolar disorder but not in first-degree
components are significantly involved in psychopathology and emo- relatives of bipolar patients or euthymic unipolar depression. Biol. Psychiatry 57,
tional dysregulation in MD which is consistent with the well-known 183–187.
cognitive theories of depression and the “imbalance hypothesis” of MD Duque, A., Vázquez, C., 2015. Double attention bias for positive and negative emotional
faces in clinical depression: Evidence from an eye-tracking study. J. Behav. Ther.
and If this is the case, then a cognitive conceptualization of MD Exp. Psy. 46, 107–114.
psychopathology may be more benefitial to MD patients and research- D’Urso, G., Mantovani, A., Micillo, M., Priori, A., Muscettola, G., 2013. Transcranial
ers in this field.. direct current stimulation and cognitive-behavioral therapy: evidence of a synergistic
effect in treatment-resistant depression. Brain Stimul.: Basic Transl. Clin. Res.
Future studies are recommended to consider several points our Neuromodul. 6, 465–467.
study lacked. In term of study design it would be better to add a follow- Davidson, Pizzagalli, D., Nitschke, J.B., Putnam, K., 2002. Depression: perspectives from
up investigation of tDCS effects on dependent variables to see if there is affective neuroscience. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 53, 545–574.
Davidson, R.J., 2002. Anxiety and affective style: role of prefrontal cortex and amygdala.
long-term effect of tDCS on cognitive deficits and mood. Some studies Biol. Psychiatry 51, 68–80.
suggest that cognitive improvement would occur immediately after or Diener, C., Kuehner, C., Brusniak, W., Ubl, B., Wessa, M., Flor, H., 2012. A meta-analysis
during the early tDCS phase and fade away afterwards (Brunoni et al., of neurofunctional imaging studies of emotion and cognition in major depression.
NeuroImage 61, 677–685.
2016). Secondly, it is suggested to use more sensitive emotional
Drevets, W.C., Furey, M.L., 2009. Depression and the brain. In: Larry, R.S. (Ed.),
measurement in addition to the BDI and HDRS that measures Encyclopedia of Neuroscience. Academic Press, Oxford, 459–470.
emotional functions specially the tasks involving emotional biases such Egerhazi, A., Balla, P., Ritzl, A., Varga, Z., Frecska, E., Berecz, R., 2013. Automated
as emotional Stroop or using emotional faces in the "dot probe" neuropsychological test battery in depression–preliminary data.
Neuropsychopharmacol. Hung. 15, 5–11.
paradigm. Neuropsychological investigation of negativity bias would Ellis, A.E., Grieger, R.M., 1986. Handbook of Rational-emotive Therapy 2. Springer
also be helpful in future neuromodulation studies. Low-focality of tDCS Publishing Co., New York, NY, US.
could also affects adjacent areas and hinder montage specificity. Falconer, D.W., Cleland, J., Fielding, S., Reid, I.C., 2010. Using the Cambridge
neuropsychological test automated battery (CANTAB) to assess the cognitive impact
Our results also provide supporting document for application of the of electroconvulsive therapy on visual and visuospatial memory. Psychol. Med. 40,
tDCS, as a method of cognitive enhancement (Parkin et al., 2015), in 1017–1025.
the field of cognitive rehabilitation that attempts to apply new Ferrucci, R., Bortolomasi, M., Vergari, M., Tadini, L., Salvoro, B., Giacopuzzi, M.,
Barbieri, S., Priori, A., 2009. Transcranial direct current stimulation in severe, drug-
techniques for improving cognitive impairments (Ponsford, 2004). resistant major depression. J. Affect. Disord. 118, 215–219.
However, this study is not to suggest tDCS as the best treatment Fray, P., Robbins, T., Sahakian, B., 1996. Neuorpsychiatyric applications of CANTAB.
option for MD. Instead it suggests that cognitive improvement would Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 11, 329–336.
Fregni, F., Boggio, P., Nitsche, M., Bermpohl, F., Antal, A., Feredoes, E., Marcolin, M.,
be a key factor in MD treatment. Lastly, given the improving effects of Rigonatti, S., Silva, M.A., Paulus, W., Pascual-Leone, A., 2005. Anodal transcranial
tDCS on cognitive deficits in MD, electrical stimulation of the brain direct current stimulation of prefrontal cortex enhances working memory. Exp. Brain
could be used as supplemental technique to maximize therapeutic Res. 166, 23–30.
Fregni, F., Boggio, P.S., Nitsche, M.A., Rigonatti, S.P., Pascual-Leone, A., 2006. Cognitive
effect of psychological interventions in disorders marked with signifi-
effects of repeated sessions of transcranial direct current stimulation in patients with
cant cognitive deficits such as MD (D’Urso et al., 2013) which could be depression. Depress. Anxiety 23, 482–484.
an interesting future research avenue. Gazzaniga, M.S., Ivry, R.B., Mangun, G.R., 2014. Cognitive Neuroscience: The Biology of

247
M.A. Salehinejad et al. Journal of Affective Disorders 210 (2017) 241–248

Mind fourth ed.. W. W. Norton, New York, NY. short-term recognition memory and learning after temporal lobe excisions, frontal
Gotlib, I.H., Joormann, J., 2010. Cognition and depression: current status and future lobe excisions or amygdalo-hippocampectomy in man. Neuropsychologia 33, 1–24.
directions. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 6, 285–312. Palm, U., Reisinger, E., Keeser, D., Kuo, M.-F., Pogarell, O., Leicht, G., Mulert, C.,
Grimm, S., Beck, J., Schuepbach, D., Hell, D., Boesiger, P., Bermpohl, F., Niehaus, L., Nitsche, M.A., Padberg, F., 2013. Evaluation of sham transcranial direct current
Boeker, H., Northoff, G., 2008. Imbalance between left and right dorsolateral stimulation for randomized, placebo-controlled clinical trials. Brain Stimulation 6,
prefrontal cortex in major depression is linked to negative emotional judgment: an 690–695.
fMRI study in severe major depressive disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 63, 369–376. Parkin, Beth L., Ekhtiari, H., Walsh, Vincent, F., 2015. Non-invasive human brain
Gross, J.J., 2002. Emotion regulation: affective, cognitive, and social consequences. stimulation in cognitive neuroscience: a primer. Neuron 87, 932–945.
Psychophysiology 39, 281–291. Plewnia, C., Schroeder, P.A., Wolkenstein, L., 2015. Targeting the biased brain: non-
Hammar, Å., Årdal, G., 2009. Cognitive functioning in major depression – a summary. invasive brain stimulation to ameliorate cognitive control. Lancet Psychiatry 2,
Front. Hum. Neurosci., 3. 351–356.
Hamilton, M., 1960. A rating scale for depression. J. Neurol. Neurosurg., Psychiatry 23, Ponsford, J., 2004. Cognitive and behavioral rehabilitation: From neurobiology to clinical
56. practice. Guilford Press.
Hertel, P.T., 2000. The cognitive-initiative account of depression-related impairments in Pope, P.A., Brenton, J.W., Miall, R.C., 2015. Task-specific facilitation of cognition by
memory. In: Medin, D.L. (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation 39. anodal transcranial direct current stimulation of the prefrontal cortex. Cereb. Cortex
Academic Press, New York, 47–71. 25, 4551–4558.
Hill, A.T., Fitzgerald, P.B., Hoy, K.E., 2016. Effects of anodal transcranial direct current Price, J.L., Drevets, W.C., 2012. Neural circuits underlying the pathophysiology of mood
stimulation on working memory: a systematic review and meta-analysis of findings disorders. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 61–71.
from healthy and neuropsychiatric populations. Brain Stimul. 9, 197–208. Robbins, T.W., James, M., Owen, A.M., Sahakian, B.J., McInnes, L., Rabbitt, P., 1994.
Jackson, D.C., Malmstadt, J.R., Larson, C.L., Davidson, R.J., 2000. Suppression and Cambridge neuropsychological test automated battery (CANTAB): a factor analytic
enhancement of emotional responses to unpleasant pictures. Psychophysiology 37, study of a large sample of normal elderly volunteers. Dement. Geriatr. Cogn. Disord.
515–522. 5, 266–281.
Kalisch, R., Wiech, K., Critchley, H.D., Dolan, R.J., 2006. Levels of appraisal: a medial Rock, P.L., Roiser, J.P., Riedel, W.J., Blackwell, A.D., 2014. Cognitive impairment in
prefrontal role in high-level appraisal of emotional material. NeuroImage 30, depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol. Med. 44, 2029–2040.
1458–1466. Roiser, J.P., Sahakian, B.J., 2013. Hot and cold cognition in depression. CNS Spectr. 18,
Kalu, U., Sexton, C., Loo, C., Ebmeier, K., 2012. Transcranial direct current stimulation 139–149.
in the treatment of major depression: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Med. 42, 1791–1800. Rose, E., Ebmeier, K., 2006. Pattern of impaired working memory during major
Koenigs, M., Grafman, J., 2009. The functional neuroanatomy of depression: distinct depression. J. Affect. Disord. 90, 149–161.
roles for ventromedial and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Behav. Brain Res. 201, Sahakian, B.J., Downes, J.J., Eagger, S., Everden, J.L., Levy, R., Philpot, M.P., Roberts,
239–243. A.C., Robbins, T.W., 1990. Sparing of attentional relative to mnemonic function in a
Lemogne, C., Mayberg, H., Bergouignan, L., Volle, E., Delaveau, P., Lehéricy, S., Allilaire, subgroup of patients with dementia of the Alzheimer type. Neuropsychologia 28,
J.-F., Fossati, P., 2010. Self-referential processing and the prefrontal cortex over the 1197–1213.
course of depression: a pilot study. J. Affect. Disord. 124, 196–201. Salehinejad, M.A., Nejati, V., Derakhshan, M., 2017. Neural correlates of trait resiliency:
Long, Z., Duan, X., Wang, Y., Liu, F., Zeng, L., Zhao, J.-p., Chen, H., 2015. Disrupted evidence from electrical stimulation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dLPFC)
structural connectivity network in treatment-naive depression. Prog. Neuro- and orbitofrontal cortex (OFC). Person. Ind. Diff. 106, 209–216.
Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 56, 18–26. Salehinejad, M.A., Rostami, R., Ghanavati, E., 2015. Transcranial direct current
Loo, C.K., Alonzo, A., Martin, D., Mitchell, P.B., Galvez, V., Sachdev, P., 2012. stimulation of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of major depression: improving visual
Transcranial direct current stimulation for depression: 3-week, randomised, sham- working memory, reducing depressive symptoms. NeuroRegulation 2, 37–49.
controlled trial. Br. J. Psychiatry 200, 52–59. Sánchez-Navarro, J.P., Driscoll, D., Anderson, S.W., Tranel, D., Bechara, A., Buchanan,
Maalouf, F.T., Klein, C., Clark, L., Sahakian, B.J., LaBarbara, E.J., Versace, A., Hassel, S., T.W., 2014. Alterations of attention and emotional processing following childhood-
Almeida, J.R., Phillips, M.L., 2010. Impaired sustained attention and executive onset damage to the prefrontal cortex. Behav. Neurosci. 128, 1.
dysfunction: bipolar disorder versus depression-specific markers of affective Schafer, R.J., Moore, T., 2011. Selective attention from voluntary control of neurons in
disorders. Neuropsychologia 48, 1862–1868. prefrontal cortex. Science 332, 1568–1571.
Marazziti, D., Consoli, G., Picchetti, M., Carlini, M., Faravelli, L., 2010. Cognitive Scherer, K.R., Schorr, A., Johnstone, T., 2001. Appraisal Processes in Emotion: Theory,
impairment in major depression. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 626, 83–86. Methods, Research. Oxford University Press, New York, NY, US.
Marijnissen, G., Tuinier, S., Sijben, A.E.S., Verhoeven, W.M.A., 2002. The temperament Shiozawa, P., Fregni, F., Benseñor, I.M., Lotufo, P.A., Berlim, M.T., Daskalakis, J.Z.,
and character inventory in major depression. J. Affect. Disord. 70, 219–223. Cordeiro, Q., Brunoni, A.R., 2014. Transcranial direct current stimulation for major
Miller, E.K., Cohen, J.D., 2001. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu depression: an updated systematic review and meta-analysis. Int. J.
Rev. Neurosci. 24, 167–202. Neuropsychopharmacol. 17, 1443–1452.
Must, A., Szabó, Z., Bódi, N., Szász, A., Janka, Z., Kéri, S., 2006. Sensitivity to reward and Utz, K.S., Dimova, V., Oppenländer, K., Kerkhoff, G., 2010. Electrified minds:
punishment and the prefrontal cortex in major depression. J. Affect. Disord. 90, transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) and galvanic vestibular stimulation
209–215. (GVS) as methods of non-invasive brain stimulation in neuropsychology—a review of
Nezhad, M.A., Khodapanahi, M.K., Yekta, M., Mahmoodikahriz, B., Ostadghafour, S., current data and future implications. Neuropsychologia 48, 2789–2810.
2011. Defense styles in internalizing and externalizing disorders. Procedia - Soc. Wagner, S., Doering, B., Helmreich, I., Lieb, K., Tadić, A., 2012. A meta-analysis of
Behav. Sci. 30, 236–241. executive dysfunctions in unipolar major depressive disorder without psychotic
Nitsche, Boggio, Fregni, Pascual-Leone, 2009a. Treatment of depression with symptoms and their changes during antidepressant treatment. Acta Psychiatr. Scand.
transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS): a review. Exp. Neurol. 219, 14–19. 125, 281–292.
Nitsche, M.A., Boggio, P.S., Fregni, F., Pascual-Leone, A., 2009b. Treatment of Wang, X.-L., Du, M.-Y., Chen, T.-L., Chen, Z.-Q., Huang, X.-Q., Luo, Y., Zhao, Y.-J.,
depression with transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS): a review. Exp. Kumar, P., Gong, Q.-Y., 2015. Neural correlates during working memory processing
Neurol. 219, 14–19. in major depressive disorder. Prog. Neuro-Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 56,
Nitschke, J.B., Heller, W., Etienne, M.A., Miller, G.A., 2004. Prefrontal cortex activity 101–108.
differentiates processes affecting memory in depression. Biol. Psychol. 67, 125–143. Ye, T., Peng, J., Nie, B., Gao, J., Liu, J., Li, Y., Wang, G., Ma, X., Li, K., Shan, B., 2012.
Ochsner, K.N., Gross, J.J., 2005. The cognitive control of emotion. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, Altered functional connectivity of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in first-episode
242–249. patients with major depressive disorder. Eur. J. Radiol. 81, 4035–4040.
Owen, A.M., Sahakian, B.J., Semple, J., Polkey, C.E., Robbins, T.W., 1995. Visuo-spatial

248

You might also like