You are on page 1of 12

CHILD

Celano et
MALTREATMENT
al. / ATTRIBUTION
/ FEBRUARY
RETRAINING
2002

Attribution Retraining With Sexually


Abused Children: Review of Techniques

Marianne Celano
Ann Hazzard
Emory University School of Medicine
Susan K. Campbell
Georgia Center for Children
Claudia B. Lang
Emory University School of Medicine

Clinicians increasingly use empirically based cognitive- be- sion and anxiety (e.g., Kendall & Treadwell, 1996;
havioral techniques in their treatment of child victims of sexual Lewinsohn, Clarke, Rohde, Hops, & Seeley, 1996). Attri-
abuse. Attribution retraining is often a primary component of bution retraining has recently been adapted for use with
this work, and it involves various techniques aimed at decreas- sexually abused children, given the increasing acceptance
ing abuse-related self-blame and encouraging the child to at- of models that include victims’ cognitive appraisals
tribute responsibility for the abuse to the perpetrator. This among those variables that influence the effects of abuse
article reviews literature that highlights the complexity of self on mental health (e.g., Finkelhor & Browne, 1985;
and other blame for sexually abused children in terms of devel- Newberger & De Vos, 1988; Spaccarelli & Kim, 1995).
opmental status, the multifaceted nature and interrelationships Although early anecdotal literature suggested that self-
of abuse-specific attributions, and the psychological effects of blame1 was common among all sexually abused children,
self-blame and perpetrator blame. A review of written attribu- recent empirical findings suggest gender and age-based
tion retraining techniques developed by diverse authors for use variability in children’s attributions. For example,
with sexually abused children and their nonoffending parents is Hunter, Goodwin, and Wilson (1992) found gender dif-
provided, including written and verbal techniques and tech- ferences in interrelationships between different types of
niques using games and the arts. The relative utility of different attributions (e.g., family blame and perpetrator blame,
approaches with children of various stages of development is self-blame and perpetrator blame). In addition, Hunter et
discussed, along with the need for empirical research regarding al. (1992) found self-blame for sexual abuse to be more
the effectiveness of these techniques. prevalent among adolescents and adults than among child
victims in a nonpsychiatric sample. Using a clinical sam-

I n recent years, increasing attention has been given to at-


tribution retraining techniques (also called cognitive re-
structuring) in which the therapist encourages the client
to modify maladaptive beliefs, often by using logical anal-
ysis to demonstrate how the beliefs are irrational (Finch,
Nelson, & Ott, 1993). These techniques are commonly
implemented in empirically supported cognitive-behav-
ioral treatments for children and adolescents with depres-
Authors’ Note: Reprint requests may be sent to Marianne Celano,
Ph.D., Department of Psychiatry & Behavioral Sciences, Emory Univer-
CHILD MALTREATMENT, Vol. 7, No. 1, February 2002 64-75 C sity School of Medicine, Box 26064, Grady Health System, Atlanta,
© 2002 Sage Publications H GA, 30335. E-mail: mcelano@emory.edu

64
Celano et al. / ATTRIBUTION RETRAINING 65

ple and a different methodology for assessing attributions, among sexually abused children (Feiring, Taska, & Chen,
Hazzard, Celano, Gould, Lawry, and Webb (1995) found 2002, this issue), abuse-specific self-blame attributions
that younger children blamed themselves more for the were found to be related to self-reported depressive symp-
abuse than older children among a sample of 8- to 13-year- toms, PTSD symptoms, and poor self-esteem at study
old girls. entry and 1 year later, even after controlling for age, gen-
Developmental theory and empirical literature in der, and number of abuse events.
social cognition suggest developmental variation in social Despite the empirical support for the negative effects of
cognitive processes related to attributions. For example, self-attributions, some authors have suggested that spe-
preschoolers’ attributions are influenced by their ten- cific, behavioral self-blame may be adaptive in some cir-
dency to overattribute intentionality, underattribute cumstances (e.g., Lamb, 1986; Shapiro, 1995). For exam-
behavior to stable personality traits, and evaluate people ple, the child may have made errors in judgment that
and actions according to their outcomes (Miller & Aloise, increased the likelihood of abuse, suggesting that some
1989). However, it is not well understood how these internal attributions of responsibility may be appropriate.
developmental factors affect young children’s attributions Thus, it is important to distinguish between those self-
of responsibility for sexual abuse. Development also attributions that stem from cognitive distortions and
affects the extent to which children can understand and those that are the natural result of children’s failure to
reliably report self-blame. The interchangeable use of the meet adults’ legitimate expectations (Celano, 1992). A
terms blame and responsibility in the treatment literature related dilemma is the presumed positive relationship
(Dalenberg & Jacobs, 1994) may obscure young children’s between perpetrator blame and feelings of powerlessness,
self-attributions. That is, younger children may be reluc- often called “the attributional dilemma” (Shapiro, 1989).
tant to admit blame, as this term carries negative connota- Thus, attempts to discourage victims’ self-blame may have
tions, whereas older children may more readily perceive the unintended effect of increasing children’s perceived
that blame refers to the broader concept of causal respon- helplessness and concomitant anxiety.
sibility (Dalenberg & Jacobs, 1994). A related assumption in the clinical literature is that
One assumption embodied by the anecdotal literature perpetrator blame has positive effects for the child victim.
on child sexual abuse is that self-blame and perpetrator However, Chaffin et al. (1997) found that a coping style
blame are two extremes on a single continuum. However, characterized by blaming others and expression of anger
some empirical findings challenge the assumption of an was associated with the greatest number of behavioral
inverse relationship between self-blame and perpetrator problems. This finding raises questions about how clini-
blame. For example, Hunter and colleagues (1992) did cians should encourage appropriate attributions of blame
not find an inverse relationship between self-blame and to the perpetrator without promoting expressions of rage
perpetrator blame for male victims. In addition, Chaffin, that may exacerbate acting-out behavior. In addition,
Wherry, and Dykman (1997) did not find internalized encouragement of perpetrator blame is a delicate matter
coping style (blaming and isolating self, remaining quiet in cases in which the perpetrator is important to the child.
and passive about the problem) and angry coping style Several authors have pointed out that anger toward an
(blaming others and expression of anger) to be negatively offender who is a cherished parent figure can be very pain-
correlated in a sample of sexually abused children. ful, and some children may cling to positive internal
The empirical literature is supportive of the early clini- images of a perpetrator-caregiver (Friedrich, 1990;
cal assumption that victims’ self-blame has negative Shapiro, 1995).
effects. Studies assessing child victims’ attributions of The empirical and clinical literature have emphasized
responsibility for their abuse have found internal attribu- self-blame and perpetrator blame more than blame of fam-
tions to be related to low self-esteem (Morrow, 1991), ily members (e.g., nonoffending caregivers). Among a
higher levels of depression (Morrow, 1991; Spaccarelli sample of women survivors, McMillen and Zuravin
& Fuchs, 1997), and post-traumatic stress disorder (1997) found both self-blame and nonperpetrator family
(PTSD) symptoms (Wolfe, Sas, & Wekerle, 1994). blame to be independently associated with adverse out-
Chaffin and colleagues (1997) found an internalized cop- comes, suggesting that clinicians should broaden their
ing style to be significantly related to child-reported assessments of child victims’ attributions to include
PTSD hyperarousal and guilt symptoms. Mannarino and nonperpetrator family blame. In their factor analysis of
colleagues (Mannarino & Cohen, 1996; Mannarino, abuse-specific attribution items, Feiring et al. (2002)
Cohen, & Berman, 1994) found that negative abuse- identified a factor (External-Mom) that was positively
related attributions and perceptions were strongly related related to the factor measuring internal-global abuse-spe-
to symptom formation in sexually abused children at dis- cific attributions. The impact of parent blame is not well
closure and follow-up. In a longitudinal study of the rela- understood but probably depends at least in part on the
tionship between attributions for abuse and adjustment child’s developmental status (i.e., dependence on the par-

CHILD MALTREATMENT / FEBRUARY 2002


66 Celano et al. / ATTRIBUTION RETRAINING

ent), the circumstances of the abuse, and the relationship retraining techniques, we searched psychological and
between the parent and the perpetrator. medical databases for material on attributions of sexually
Even internal attributions of responsibility can benefit abused children. Articles, workbooks, or texts including a
from further differentiation. Celano (1992) offered a detailed description of an attribution retraining tech-
typology of child victims’ internal attributions including nique were included.
responsibility for failing to recognize abuse, for participa-
tion in the abuse, for failing to seek help, for failing to
ATTRIBUTION RETRAINING TECHNIQUES
avoid or control the abuse, for pleasure gained, for failure
to protect siblings, for failure to protect oneself, and for Verbal and Written Techniques
the family reaction to disclosure. Children may make dif-
ferent attributions for various aspects of the abuse; for Many techniques primarily involve verbal discussion
example, a girl may blame the offender for the abusive act of thoughts and feelings or written materials that are used
but blame herself for failing to disclose the abuse. In gen- to stimulate discussion or record thoughts. The tech-
eral, the empirical findings on attributions of responsibil- niques summarized in this section use primarily verbal/
ity for sexual abuse suggest that child victims’ attributions written formats although some may include visual illustra-
are more complicated and differentiated than was once tions as secondary aspects to the material.
thought and may require more sophisticated and differen- Cognitive-behavioral challenges of negative thoughts.
tiated treatment responses. Deblinger and colleagues (Deblinger & Heflin, 1996;
Advances in theoretical development have informed Deblinger, McLeer, & Henry, 1990) pioneered the appli-
the treatment of sexually abused children’s attributions. cation of cognitive-behavioral principles to the treatment
Abuse-specific treatment has been guided by cognitive- of sexually abused children, using a PTSD conceptualiza-
behavioral theory (Deblinger & Heflin, 1996), the four- tion of abuse effects. They present the cognitive coping
factor traumagenic dynamics model (Finkelhor & triangle model to clients, to illustrate that thoughts, feel-
Browne, 1985), and life-span developmental theory ings, and behaviors are interrelated. Negative thoughts
(Newberger & De Vos, 1988). These theories are more may lead to a variety of negative affects about the abuse
alike than they are different in their emphasis on the role situation (e.g., anger, anxiety, guilt). The authors provide
of victims’ attributions in influencing the effects of abuse several detailed examples of how a therapist might elicit
on the child’s functioning. The life-span developmental specific thoughts contributing to guilty feelings (e.g.,
theory is unique in its emphasis on the time dimension What are the thoughts going through your head when you
over which the child’s recovery (including attributions) are feeling guilty?) and then use the Socratic method to
evolves and on the reciprocal interaction of child behav- dispute these thoughts. For example, for a girl who blames
ior, cognitive processes, and environmental response. herself because she wore short, baby doll pajamas, the
In addition, general theoretical models linking attribu- therapist normalizes the child’s behavior by asking if other
tions for negative events to consequences have been children her age wear that type of pajama and whether
applied to sexually abused children’s attributions. they were abused. The therapist encourages an alternative
According to the learned helplessness model (Abramson, attribution that wearing the pajamas did not really cause
Seligman, & Teasdale, 1978; Seligman et al., 1984), nega- the abuse but was an excuse used by the perpetrator.
tive outcomes, such as depression, accrue when children’s With preschoolers, Cohen and Mannarino (1993) rec-
attributions for negative events are internal, stable, and ommend first presenting the child with brief descriptions
global. According to the compensatory model (Brickman of positive and negative situations unrelated to sexual
et al., 1982), positive outcomes are most likely when the abuse (e.g., you leave your bike in the driveway and your
individual does not take responsibility for the origin of the father hits it with his car) and then describing a sexually
problem but takes full responsibility for problem solution abusive situation. For each situation, the child tells who
(i.e., coping with effects of the problem and avoiding he or she thinks is responsible for making the event occur.
recurrence). If the child makes an inappropriate attribution, the thera-
Treatments designed for sexually abused children usu- pist is encouraged to (a) ask what the person he or she
ally include techniques designed to reduce self-blame and named did to make the event happen, (b) ask if anyone
help child victims attribute responsibility for the abuse to else did anything to contribute to it, and (c) offer alterna-
the offender. These techniques vary in the degree to tive explanations for why the event occurred. Finally, the
which they address the complexities and controversies therapist follows this procedure related to the child’s own
inherent in attributional training. The purpose of this abuse. Because of young children’s egocentric view of cau-
article is to review attribution retraining techniques sality and limited logical reasoning skills, Cohen and
developed for use by clinicians treating sexually abused Mannarino (1993) note that the therapist may need to
children (see Table 1). To locate written attribution make more direct statements such as “I don’t think it was

CHILD MALTREATMENT / FEBRUARY 2002


Celano et al. / ATTRIBUTION RETRAINING 67

TABLE 1: Attribution Retraining Techniques for Sexually Abused Children

Name of Activity Authors Target Age Modality

Verbal techniques
Cognitive coping triangle Deblinger et al., 1990 Latency Individual
Challenging negative thoughts Cohen & Mannarino, 1993 Preschool Individual
“Why List” Celano et al., 1991 Latency Individual
“Guilt-Free Kids” Crisci et al., 1997 Latency Individual/group
List of blaming beliefs Munson & Riskin, 1995 Teens Individual
Story metaphors
“Kit’s Story” Crisci et al., 1997 Preschool Individual/group
“Sugar for Diabetics” role-play James, 1989 Latency Individual
“Jay’s Story” Karp & Butler, 1996a Latency Individual/group
“Why Me?” Celano et al., 1991 Latency Individual
Affirmation techniques
Children’s books Sweet, 1981 4 to 12 Individual/group
Workbook sheets Spinal-Robinson & Wickham, 1992 Latency, teens Individual/group
Boulden & Boulden, 1994 Latency Individual/group
Stowell & Dietzel, 1982 Latency Individual/group
Expressive affirmations James, 1989 All ages Individual
Games and the arts
“Why Pie” Celano et al., 1991 Latency Individual
“Responsibility Ruler” Mannarino, personal communication Latency, teens Individual
“Innocence of Childhood” Crisci et al., 1997 Latency, teens Individual/group
Draw self and perpetrator Wieland, 1997 Latency Individual
“Red Riding Hood” role-play Crisci et al., 1997 Preschool Individual/group
“Best Friends Role-Play” Deblinger & Heflin, 1996 Latency Individual
Gestalt chair technique Wieland, 1997 Latency, teens Individual
“Time of Confusion” song Celano et al., 1991 Latency Individual
“Hat Game” Hansen et al., 1998 Latency Group
“Guilt Trip Game” Crisci et al., 1997 Latency Group
Offender-oriented techniques
Explanation of offender blame Berliner & Wheeler, 1987
Letter to the offender Celano et al., 1991 Latency Individual
Mandell & Damon, 1989 Latency Group
Spinal-Robinson & Wickham, 1992 All ages Individual
Lindon & Nourse, 1994 Teens Individual
“Hero Declaration” Allen, 1995 All ages Group
“Truth and Lies” Allen, 1995 4 to 12 Group
“Talk Back” cartoon bubbles Crisci et al., 1997 Teens Individual/group
Courtroom role-play Pearce & Pezzot-Pearce, 1997 Latency, teens Individual

that child’s fault” or “I think the grown-up should have Celano, Hazzard, and colleagues developed an eight-
known better.” session curriculum based on Finkelhor and Browne’s
Lists of attributions and suggested challenges. Several au- (1985) traumagenic dynamics conceptualization of abuse
thors have compiled more comprehensive lists of possible sequelae. The curriculum includes two sessions of activi-
reasons that children may blame themselves for the occur- ties related to self-blame, including the “Why List,” which
rence of abuse, for delayed disclosure, and/or for family lists 17 possible self-blaming attributions from which the
disruption following the abuse. These authors generally child is asked to choose any thoughts he or she may have
advise clinicians to spend ample time eliciting specific at- (Celano, Hazzard, Simmons, & Webb, 1991; Hazzard,
tributions and gradually discussing alternative view- 1995). Among the possibilities are: because the person
points, cautioning that premature reassurance that abuse picked me to abuse, because I liked getting affection,
is never the child’s fault may interfere with disclosure and because I accepted gifts, because I didn’t obey a parental
reframing of negative attributions. rule, and because some of the ways the person touched me

CHILD MALTREATMENT / FEBRUARY 2002


68 Celano et al. / ATTRIBUTION RETRAINING

felt good to my body. Therapists are provided with sug- as, “how do you think Kit felt when Tom Cat told her to
gested approaches to counter each of the self-blaming keep what happened a secret?”
attributions on the list. For example, the therapist might James (1989) combines the story metaphor technique
encourage a child to question whether the offender really with role-playing. After acting out a story involving a girl
picked the child because of personal characteristics or with diabetes whose parents are told never to give her
because of convenience. Children who made an error in sugar (“Sugar for Diabetics” role-play), the children
judgment that may have increased their risk are encour- answer questions about responsibility when the father
aged to acknowledge their mistake (e.g., make a situa- gives the child a candy bar that makes her very sick.
tional attribution) but are discouraged from assigning Children are asked, “Who did the wrong thing? What if
global internal blame: “Even if you make a mistake, no the little girl didn’t make the father stop? What if she
one has the right to abuse you.” For self-blaming attribu- enjoyed the candy?” The story illustrates that no matter
tions related to the victim’s sexual arousal, the child can what the child did or felt, the adult made the mistake by
be informed that sexual arousal is an automatic response doing something he knew was wrong for the child.
of the body, similar to automatic laughing or twitching in Karp and Butler (1996a) developed a workbook with
response to tickling. an accompanying clinician’s guide (Karp & Butler,
Crisci, Lay, and Lowenstein (1997) use similar tech- 1996b). The workbook includes “Jay’s Story” about a
niques in their “Guilt-Free Kids” activity, one of a number child abused by his older brother. After reading the story,
of creative therapeutic activities for sexually abused chil- children are asked why Jay thought the abuse was his fault
and what the brother did to make Jay think that the
dren included in their book. In this activity, children are
touching was OK; this activity is followed by eliciting and
introduced to a comic book with cartoon characters that
disputing the child’s own self-blaming attributions.
have been sexually abused. Each character is depicted
making a guilty statement, such as “I feel to blame because “Why Me?” is a fill-in-the-blank story in which a girl is
my parents split up after I told” or “I feel guilty because I abused and wonders why this happened to her (Celano et
al., 1991). This third-person format allows elicitation of
really miss the abuser.” Following a discussion of the many
the child’s probable attributions for his or her own abuse
ways guilt feelings occur, children are asked to help the
in a less threatening format. There are four versions of the
cartoon characters “tell it like it is” by writing a guilt-free
story: stranger rape and stepfather molestation versions
response to each one of the character’s guilty statements.
for both boys and girls.
Activities with older children generally involve
encouraging child survivors to develop their own list of Affirmation techniques. Most of the techniques dis-
things they should or should not have done, which they cussed involve eliciting specific reasons for self-blaming
(or others) believe may have contributed to the abuse. attributions and offering alternative perspectives to chil-
Munson and Riskin’s (1995) workbook normalizes guilty dren. Other authors simply affirm to the child that abuse is
feelings in introductory paragraphs by offering self-blam- never the child’s fault (e.g., Corder, Haizlip, & DeBoer,
ing comments the authors have heard from other chil- 1990), often using books (e.g., Something Happened to Me
dren. Teens are then given the opportunity to write their by Sweet, 1981) or workbook sheets such as Cartwheels
own blaming beliefs and statements made by the perpetra- (Spinal-Robinson & Wickham, 1992), Secrets That Hurt
tor or others that have contributed to self-blaming attri- (Boulden & Boulden, 1994), and My Very Own Book
butions. The authors then offer a checklist of ambivalent About Me (Stowell & Dietzel, 1982; Stowell, Dietzel, &
feelings that adolescents may experience (e.g., liking the Gleason, 2000). James (1989) asserts that therapists
gifts or money, loving the abuser) to which they respond should quickly and clearly tell children that the abuse was
with educational reframes. not their fault. She acknowledges to them that it may take
a while to believe this statement, however, and she ex-
Story metaphors. A number of authors have used story plores any self-blaming thoughts the children express.
metaphors to elicit and challenge self-blaming attribu- James has developed several expressive affirmation activi-
tions. For example, Crisci et al. (1997) describe a story- ties that encourage bold declarations of “It’s not my fault!”
telling activity called “Kit’s Story”, particularly (INMF). For example, she encourages children to yell the
appropriate for younger children. In the story, a cat named phrase out of the therapist’s office window, has children or
Kit is sexually abused by Tom Cat, the big barn cat next families make INMF banners for display in the home, wear
door. Kit shares her feelings of responsibility and guilt INMF badges, and create INMF dances and prayers. James
with her friend, the Wise Old Owl, who helps her sort out (1989) theorizes that imprinting the message in this way
her confusing feelings. After reading the story, the thera- creates a circumstance in which “the child first tells the
pist engages children in dialogue by asking questions such universe, then learns to tell himself” (p. 70).

CHILD MALTREATMENT / FEBRUARY 2002


Celano et al. / ATTRIBUTION RETRAINING 69

Techniques Using Games and the Arts his or her “best friend” and to try to convince the best
friend/therapist that his or her negative thoughts are not
A number of techniques have been developed that use true.
visual stimuli, music, drama, or games to address chil-
Wieland (1997) suggests using the Gestalt chair tech-
dren’s attributions. Although these techniques generally
nique for a role-play between two conflicting internal
involve verbal explanation as well, they may be more
parts of a child. For example, a child’s “now-self” may be
appealing and developmentally appropriate for children.
angry and blaming of the abused self for not telling sooner.
Visual techniques. The “Why Pie” is a drawing of a pie The child moves between two chairs during a role-played
that the child is instructed to divide into various-sized conversation between these two aspects of himself or her-
pieces, with each different-colored piece representing self. The therapist assists the abused self in expressing
how much he or she blames a given person (Celano et al., what made it hard to tell or the nonverbal ways he or she
1991). This technique allows one to assess the relative had tried to tell but no one had paid attention.
amount of blame to self and other individuals (including
but not limited to the offender). Mannarino and col- Musical techniques. Celano et al. (1991) developed a
leagues (personal communication, May 2000) use a simi- rap song, “Time of Confusion.” The song is available on
lar technique called the “Responsibility Ruler,” in which tape with their curriculum and may be especially appeal-
children measure out the relative responsibility of differ- ing to African American clients. The song’s lyrics address
ent individuals for their abuse. issues such as (a) bad things sometimes happen to good
In the “Innocence of Childhood” activity (Crisci at al., people; (b) wanting to be liked is normal, and sometimes
1997), therapists ask children to bring pictures of them- others take advantage of that feeling to trick you, (c) say-
selves taken at the age when they were abused. Looking at ing yes to one activity doesn’t mean you said yes to every-
the pictures, therapists elicit discussions geared toward thing, such as sex.
identifying how vulnerable and trusting the children
Games. Project SAFE is a 12-session treatment proto-
were. This is followed by discussions aimed at increasing
col for latency-aged children and nonoffending parents
their understanding of how the offender misused their
that uses a parallel group format (Hansen, Hecht, & Futa,
trust and exploited their vulnerability.
1998). The “Hat Game” is used to elicit and address self-
In her description of techniques based on a psycho-
blame and stigmatization issues. A hat is filled with ques-
dynamic trauma-internalization model, Wieland (1997)
tions such as “Has your body changed since the abuse?”
recommends having the child draw a picture of himself or
“Have you felt like you could have stopped it?” and “Do
herself at the age of abuse and a proportionally sized pic-
you think it was your fault?” The hat is passed from one
ture of the perpetrator. This activity emphasizes the size
person to the next, with each child reading and respond-
and power differential between victim and perpetrator
ing to a question.
and helps the child recognize the realistic limitations of
The “Guilt Trip Game” (Crisci et al., 1997) is a board
his or her ability to “fight back.”
game particularly effective in engaging resistant latency
Drama/role-plays. In an activity for younger children and preteen children. In this game, group members sit in a
called “Little Red Riding Hood,” therapists introduce the circle with the “Guilt Trip Game” board placed in the
concept of “being tricked” by performing a magic trick, middle. First, a toy car is placed on the “start” square.
followed by a discussion on why we like tricks and how Then, the leader distributes “guilt eraser” cards among the
they work (Crisci et al., 1997). They then read the story of players. The first player moves the toy car along to the first
Little Red Riding Hood, which is followed by a discussion square of the board and reads the situation aloud to the
on what trick confused Red Riding Hood and what trick group (e.g., A kid kept the abuse a secret for a long time).
frightened her. Therapists relate the concept of tricks to The leader then asks the player, “What thoughts would a
sexual abuse, explaining the difference between the fun child have to make him feel guilty about having kept the
tricks in a magic show and the scary or confusing tricks in abuse a secret?” Once the player responds, the leader asks,
secret touching. Using a puppet show to dramatize the “Who has a guilt eraser card that will challenge this child’s
story of Red Riding Hood may make this technique even feelings of guilt?” An example of an appropriate guilt
more appealing to young children. eraser card could be “Abusers trick kids into keeping the
Deblinger and Helfin (1996) use a “Best Friends Role- abuse a secret.” The player with this card is asked to read it
Play” as a mechanism to help children counter aloud and explain why he or she thinks it challenges the
maladaptive cognitions. The therapist plays the role of a guilt. Through a fun, interactive, and familiar modality,
child who is having negative abuse-related thoughts. The therapists can assess the children’s perception of responsi-
child client is encouraged to pretend that the therapist is bility, challenge cognitive distortions related to feelings of

CHILD MALTREATMENT / FEBRUARY 2002


70 Celano et al. / ATTRIBUTION RETRAINING

guilt, and reinforce the concept that children are not to posts the truth, to the cheers of the group, and places a
blame for sexual abuse. cardboard block between the truth and lie posters to
chants of “build a wall!” When the brick is in place, the
Offender-Oriented Techniques
group applauds and in unison pronounces the child a hero.
A number of authors focus on discussion or explana- Crisci et al. (1997) describe a similar activity designed
tion of offenders’ motivations as a means of clarifying for young teens (“Talk Back” cartoon bubbles). The
responsibility. Although self-blame and offender blame leader explains that the guilty feelings children have are
may not necessarily be inversely related, most authors often generated by an abuser’s statements, which are
assume that increasing appropriate offender blame is designed to absolve his own feelings of responsibility.
facilitative of positive child outcomes. For example, Ber- Therapists hold up a cartoon with an example of what an
liner and Wheeler (1987) suggest directly explaining that abuser might say to a child. Children are then encouraged
“the offender has a problem. He wants to be sexual with to deconstruct the statement by examining why the
children, something which most grown-ups don’t want. abuser might make the statement, how the victim might
And he tells himself it’s OK even though he knows it’s feel, and what the actual truth is. Children are then given
wrong.” a set of cartoon bubble sheets with offender statements.
One of the most common techniques, recommended They choose the ones that apply to them and then gener-
by many authors, is a “Letter to the Offender,” generally ate “talk back” statements for the victim bubbles.
not to be mailed (e.g., Celano et al., 1991; Mandell & Pearce and Pezzot-Pearce (1997) suggest using a court-
Damon, 1989; Spinal-Robinson & Wickham, 1992). Fill- room role-play to elicit and challenge the child’s attribu-
in-the-blank formats are often used with younger clients tions. The therapist initially portrays the judge, and the
whereas an unstructured format may be more appropriate child is asked to be the defense attorney for the perpetra-
with adolescents (Lindon & Nourse, 1994). The purpose tor. The judge asks the child-attorney to explain the per-
of this activity is to facilitate expression of feelings about petrator’s behavior. If the child blames the abuse inappro-
the perpetrator as well as elicit the child’s abuse-related priately on himself or herself, the judge can challenge the
attributions. attribution. Reversing roles for a subsequent role-play is
One session of “Heroes Great and Small,” a 12-week recommended to help empower the child and solidify
support group for children (Allen, 1995), is devoted to an appropriate attributions of responsibility to the offender.
evening of art and role-play involving thoughts about the
perpetrator. Each child is assigned an adult volunteer, Techniques With Parents
with whom he or she draws a picture of a perpetrator (not
Many structured treatment programs for sexually
necessarily the person who abused them) and then write
abused youth include parallel treatment protocols for
down things they would like to say to the perpetrator. In
their parents because parental attributions, emotional
front of the group, the child then says to his or her partner
reactions, and support have been found to be strongly
those things the child would like to say to the person who
related to child outcome (e.g., Cohen & Mannarino,
abused them. The session ends with a “Hero Declaration,”
1998; Everson, Hunter, Runyon, Edelsohn, & Coulter,
in which children place fault with the perpetrator and
1989; Hazzard et al., 1995). The therapist is typically
acknowledge their bravery and hopefulness about
urged to address both inappropriate parental self-blame
healing.
and potential child blame.
Allen (1995) uses dramatic techniques in an addi-
tional group exercise entitled “Truth and Lies.” Children Self-blame. Inappropriate parental self-blame can lead
are again paired with an adult partner, with whom they to immobilizing guilt and inappropriate models of self-
generate a list of things they were told by the perpetrator, blame for the child. To address parent self-blame, most au-
and a list of the truths of their experience. Adult partners thors rely primarily on verbal cognitive-behavioral tech-
are provided with sample lists of lies to facilitate the pro- niques. Deblinger and Heflin (1996) encourage therapists
cess (e.g., you made me do it, you wanted it, you deserved to elicit self-blaming attributions such as “Why didn’t I
it). When the exercise is complete, group leaders bring the recognize this was happening?” or “I must be a bad
children to a section of the room where a large poster with mother.” They encourage the therapist to provide educa-
the word danger hangs, alongside one poster with the word tional information to facilitate alternative cognitions. For
truth and one with the word Lies and a collection of card- example, the therapist can let parents know that even ex-
board boxes. Children are told that by knowing the truth, perts cannot identify perpetrators based on everyday be-
they can build a safety wall between themselves and per- havior and that abuse is very difficult to detect because of
petrators. Each child comes to the front with his or her the secrecy and pressure on the child not to tell. Some au-
adult partner, reads a lie, and posts it on the Lie poster. thors use techniques that parallel those used with chil-
The group boos and yells, “lies.” The child then reads and dren; for example, Deblinger and Heflin (1996) also use

CHILD MALTREATMENT / FEBRUARY 2002


Celano et al. / ATTRIBUTION RETRAINING 71

their “Best Friends Role-Play” technique with parents, are designed for use with latency-aged girls. There are
and Hansen et al. (1998) uses a parallel “Hat Game” with fewer attribution techniques developed or adapted for use
parents. with boys, preschoolers, adolescents, or children from
Cohen and Mannarino (1993) and Celano et al. racial and ethnic minority groups. In addition, more tech-
(1991) note that in some cases, parents may have contrib- niques are needed for nonoffending parents, especially
uted to circumstances that made the child’s abuse more given the strong influence of parent support on child out-
likely, and acceptance of some responsibility may be comes (Cohen & Mannarino, 1996a, 1998) as well as the
appropriate. In some cases, parents may consider ways of value of parents’ involvement in abuse-specific treatment
making restitution to the child. Wieland (1997) notes (Saywitz, Mannarino, Berliner, & Cohen, 2000).
that it may be appropriate for parents to apologize for There is also scant attention to the role of broader cul-
problems in the parent-child relationship that made it tural factors in the development, reinforcement, and
hard for the child to disclose immediately, for not creating treatment of children’s self-attributions of responsibility
a safe enough family situation, or for not recognizing the for sexual abuse, despite evidence that variables related to
child’s nonverbal ways of telling. In other cases, it may be ethnicity influence the psychological functioning of sexu-
helpful to assist parents in distinguishing differences ally abused girls (Sanders-Phillips, Moisan, Wadlington,
between feelings of regret (e.g., for an unforeseeable bad Morgan, & English, 1995). Furthermore, children’s attri-
choice of babysitter) from feelings of responsibility butions about themselves and others often are embedded
(Mannarino, personal communication, May 2000). within a social context of race, gender, or socioeconomic
status discrimination. Wyatt (1990) described how multi-
Child blame. To address potential child blame, ple dimensions of victimization, including institutional
Deblinger and Heflin (1996) recommend asking parents racism, parallel or amplify the sequelae of sexual abuse for
to identify ways that the perpetrator engaged their child in ethnic minority children. For example, a child may inter-
abuse. They offer a list of ways that perpetrators engage nalize subtle forms of racism, leading to low self-worth.
children to facilitate psychoeducation and discussion Thus, attempts to discourage self-blame among ethnic
(e.g., encouraging the child to think that the activity was minority children should be integrated with efforts to
a game or an appropriate expression of affection, taking build or reinforce a strong positive ethnic identity. Given
advantage of the child’s eagerness to please or respect for the cultural variability in family structure as well as indi-
authority). Many parents are most frustrated by the child’s viduals’ appraisals of and responses to specific traumatic
delayed disclosure; Deblinger and Heflin encourage par- events, attribution training should be tailored to the
ents to consider from the child’s point of view the many child’s and family’s cultural group(s).
reasons not to tell. Again, a list of reasons children may Although this review has selectively considered attri-
not tell is provided to assist with reframing (e.g., they bution techniques in isolation, virtually all authors
don’t have appropriate language skills, they don’t recog- describe these techniques within the context of a multi-
nize the activity as inappropriate, they are embarrassed, faceted treatment program in which attribution training
they fear getting in trouble or getting the perpetrator in is an important but probably not sufficient component.
trouble, they don’t want to disrupt family relationships, For example, several programs explicitly target affective
they are afraid of perpetrator threats). symptoms (i.e., those associated with anxiety and depres-
sion) in addition to attribution retraining. In this vein,
DISCUSSION further clinical and empirical examination of the recipro-
cal relationship between mood and attributions would be
A diverse array of attribution retraining techniques helpful. Research is needed to determine the relative
have been developed or adapted for use in treatment of impact of attribution retraining versus pharmacological
sexually abused children. Clinical efforts to discourage or treatment on depression in sexually abused children.
reframe children’s self-attributions of responsibility for All of the attribution techniques reviewed aim to dis-
the abuse have become more sophisticated since the early courage self-blame and promote attributions of responsi-
days of sex abuse treatment, when clinicians tended sim- bility toward the perpetrator; however, the techniques
ply to affirm that the abuse was not the child’s fault. Cur- vary along a number of dimensions, raising issues for the
rent attribution retraining techniques explore differenti- clinician to consider in selecting an appropriate tech-
ated self-attributions of responsibility, offer several nique for a given child. First, the reviewed techniques dif-
vehicles for discouraging self-blame (e.g., story meta- fer regarding the degree of cueing used to elicit children’s
phors, games, direct discussion), and employ creative attributions. At one end of the continuum are techniques
nonverbal components (e.g., visual illustrations or music) that provide written or verbal self-attributions for the
to make them more appealing to children. Despite the child to consider (e.g., “Why List,” “Guilt-Free Kids”); at
proliferation of attribution retraining techniques, most the other end are techniques that use only open-ended

CHILD MALTREATMENT / FEBRUARY 2002


72 Celano et al. / ATTRIBUTION RETRAINING

questions or tasks to elicit attributions (e.g., cognitive in attribution retraining are (a) consistency with attribu-
coping triangle, “Why Pie”). Story metaphors (e.g., “Why tion theories, (b) the potentially increased efficacy
Me?” “Jay’s Story”) provide some cueing in the story nar- associated with the encouragement of alternative attribu-
rative, but the clinician may not always ask children to tions tailored to a specific child and abuse situation, and
share their attributions about their own abuse. The heter- (c) minimization of helplessness by encouraging the child
ogeneity in the extent of cueing offers clinicians many to attribute responsibility to specific controllable factors
choices in selecting a technique appropriate for a given (Shapiro, 1995).
child. Story metaphors may be a less threatening way of Most of the offender-oriented attribution techniques
exploring the child’s attributions than direct questions. reviewed encourage fairly specific abuse-related attribu-
However, clinicians should use caution in implementing tions, assuming that these attributions promote positive
story metaphors with young children before they have had outcomes for the child victim. However, most authors
a forensic interview, as the child may confuse details of his have not explicitly cautioned clinicians about potential
or her abuse with those described in the story narrative. dangers of encouraging global negative attributions or
Techniques with considerable cueing may be helpful expressions of rage toward the perpetrator. As noted pre-
for children with high levels of defensiveness, poor ver- viously, global negative attributions about an offender
bal expression skills, and differentiated self-attributions of may be contraindicated when the offender is emotionally
responsibility (i.e., in incest cases). A potential drawback important to the child victim, especially if family reunifi-
of techniques employing cueing is the possibility that such cation is a goal of treatment. Instead, children should be
techniques could increase guilt in suggestible or concrete- encouraged to attribute abuse to specific, unstable, and
thinking children. controllable factors within the perpetrator (Shapiro,
The techniques also vary with respect to the specificity 1995) and should be helped to accept ambivalent feelings
of self-attribution retraining. Some (e.g., affirmation toward him or her (Cohen & Mannarino, 1993;
techniques) vest clinicians with the responsibility for Deblinger & Heflin, 1996). However, processing and
making a global statement such as “what happened to you accepting ambivalent feelings may be particularly difficult
is not your own fault,” whereas others require the commu- for preschoolers, who tend to perceive others in global
nication of specific reframing messages tailored to specific terms such as “all good” or “all bad” (Saltz & Medow,
self-attributions (e.g., cognitive coping triangle, “Why 1971).
List,” “Guilt Trip Game”). Direct communication that Another disadvantage of global attributions of blame
the abuse is not the child’s fault may be most appropriate toward the perpetrator is the role such attributions may
for younger children, given their tendency to make attri- play in the potential process from victim to victimizer
butions of responsibility based on whether the behavior is (Ryan, 1989). In encouraging children to globally blame
viewed as good or bad and to rely on authority figures for the perpetrator, therapists might inadvertently reinforce
moral guidance. In addition, young children may not have the child’s feelings of persecution, which may provide per-
the cognitive reasoning skills to engage in the logical ceived justification for retaliatory aggression (Celano,
analysis of their attributions required by specific attribu- 1992) or identification with the aggressor (Ryan, 1989).
tion retraining techniques (Friedrich, 1996). On the Thus, clinicians should not encourage expression of rage
other hand, a global reassurance of blamelessness may be toward the perpetrator, especially for children with poor
perceived by the child as a prohibition against sharing impulse control, feelings of persecution, or parents who
self-attributions (Celano, 1992). have difficulty teaching the children to take responsibility
Older children and adolescents may benefit more from for their actions. Therapists should also discourage rumi-
techniques that use specific attribution retraining tech- native anger and generalization of anger to others. For
niques. In some cases, children who blame themselves for example, an adolescent can be told that becoming “stuck”
the abuse are encouraged to make unstable and specific in anger affirms the offender’s power, while getting
self-attributions, consistent with the reformulated “unstuck” reduces the offender’s control over his or her
learned helplessness model (Abramson et al., 1978; life.
Seligman et al., 1984). For example, a girl who blames Despite the increasing focus on therapeutic techniques
herself because she mistakenly let the perpetrator (a to challenge sexually abused children’s attributions, there
neighborhood acquaintance) into the house while her have been few published treatment studies using attribu-
mother was gone can be reminded that her mistake is only tions as an outcome or a predictor variable. Several studies
a small and impermanent aspect of herself. The therapist have found that cognitive-behavioral treatments (CBT)
might say, “You let him in because you didn’t know any for sexually abused children result in better outcomes
better; lots of kids would have made the same mistake. than nondirective supportive therapy (e.g., Cohen &
Now that you are older and know more, you won’t make Mannarino, 1996b, 1998; Deblinger, Lippmann, & Steer,
that mistake again.” The primary advantages of specificity 1996), which may be due to the emphasis in CBT on

CHILD MALTREATMENT / FEBRUARY 2002


Celano et al. / ATTRIBUTION RETRAINING 73

addressing and correcting distorted attributions about the and perpetrator-blame, and whether situationally specific
sexual abuse. Celano, Hazzard, Webb, and McCall (1996) self-attributions are adaptive in some circumstances.
used self-blame as an outcome measure in a study examin-
ing the efficacy of a structured treatment program based NOTE
on Finkelhor and Browne’s (1985) traumagenic dynamics
model as compared to an unstructured control interven- 1. The terms self-blame and internal attribution of responsibility
are used interchangeably in this manuscript. Similarly, perpetra-
tion. Both treatments decreased self-blame among low-
tor blame and attributions of responsibility toward the perpetrator are
income, African American girls; the parallel structured
considered synonymous.
intervention for nonoffending parents was more effective
than the comparison intervention in decreasing parent
self-blame. Recently, Cohen and Mannarino (1996a, REFERENCES
1998, 2000) have assessed the extent to which abuse- Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M.E.P., & Teasdale, J. (1978). Learned
related attributions and parent support predict differential helplessness in humans: Critique and reformulation. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 87, 49-74.
response to treatment. In their analysis of factors predict- Allen, S. (1995). Heroes great and small: Volume 1. Unpublished treat-
ing treatment outcome for sexually abused preschool chil- ment manual. (Available from Heroes Great and Small, P.O. Box
705, Armuchee, GA 30105)
dren, they found that parental emotional distress was the Berliner, L., & Wheeler, J. R. (1987). Treating the effects of sexual abuse
strongest familial predictor of child outcomes at on children. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 2, 415-434.
posttreatment (Cohen & Mannarino, 1996a), but paren- Boulden, J., & Boulden, J. (1994). Secrets that hurt. Weaverville, CA:
Boulden Publishing.
tal support (operationally defined as high support and low Brickman, P., Rabinowitz, V. C., Karuza, J., Coates, D., Cohen, E., &
blame of the child) became a stronger predictor of out- Kidder, L. (1982). Models of helping and coping. American Psycholo-
come at the 6- and 12-month follow-up assessments gist, 37, 368-384.
Celano, M. P. (1992). A developmental model of victims’ internal attri-
(Cohen & Mannarino, 1998). In their treatment out- butions of responsibility for sexual abuse. Journal of Interpersonal Vio-
come study of 7- to 14-year-old sexually abused children, lence, 7, 57-69.
Cohen and Mannarino (2000) found that negative abuse- Celano, M., Hazzard, A., Simmons, M., & Webb, C. (1991). Recovering
from Abuse project. Unpublished treatment manual. (Available from
related attributions and excessive family flexibility signifi- Ann Hazzard, Box 26065, Grady Hospital, 80 Butler Street, Atlanta,
cantly predicted posttreatment trauma symptoms. Unfor- GA 30335)
tunately, these studies have not employed the statistical Celano, M., Hazzard, A., Webb, C., & McCall, C. (1996). Treatment of
traumagenic beliefs among sexually abused girls and their mothers:
strategies recommended for testing mediated or moder- An evaluation study. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 24(1), 1-
ated effects (Holmbeck, 1997). Thus, assertions that chil- 17.
dren’s attributions about the abuse mediate or moderate Chaffin, M., Wherry, J. N., & Dykman, R. (1997). School age children’s
coping with sexual abuse: Abuse stresses and symptoms associated
treatment outcome await empirical validation. with four coping strategies. Child Abuse & Neglect, 21, 227-240.
Thus, there is little empirical information about the Cohen, J., & Mannarino, A. (1993). A treatment model for sexually
overall effectiveness of attribution retraining techniques abused preschoolers. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 8(1), 115-131.
Cohen, J., & Mannarino, A. P. (1996a). Factors that mediate treatment
with sexually abused children and their parents to guide outcome of sexually abused preschool children. Journal of the Ameri-
clinicians in their interventions. There is much need for can Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 35, 1402-1410.
additional treatment research that assesses sexually Cohen, J., & Mannarino, A. (1996b). A treatment outcome study for
sexually abused children: Initial findings. Journal of the American
abused children’s attributions as a mediating, moderating, Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 35, 42-50.
or outcome variable. Further research could also address Cohen, J., & Mannarino, A. (1998). Factors that mediate treatment out-
differential treatment effectiveness according to chil- come of sexually abused preschool children: Six and 12-month fol-
low-up. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psy-
dren’s developmental stage, treatment format, or treat- chiatry, 37, 44-51.
ment modality. For example, studies could address the fol- Cohen, J. A., & Mannarino, A. P. (2000). Predictors of treatment out-
lowing questions: Are direct affirmations that “It’s not come in sexually abused children. Child Abuse & Neglect, 24, 983-
994.
your fault” more effective than specific cognitive restruc- Corder, B., Haizlip, T., & DeBoer, P. (1990). A pilot study for a struc-
turing techniques with children age 7 and younger? Are tured, time-limited therapy group for sexually abused pre-adolescent
attribution retraining exercises presented in group for- children. Child Abuse and Neglect, 14, 143-251.
Crisci, G., Lay, M., & Lowenstein, L. (1997). Paper dolls and paper air-
mats more effective than those presented individually? planes. Charlotte NC: Kidsrights.
Are techniques using arts or game modalities more effec- Dalenberg, C. J., & Jacobs, D. A. (1994). Attributional analyses of child
tive or well accepted than verbal/written techniques? sexual abuse episodes: Empirical and clinical issues. Journal of Child
Sexual Abuse, 3, 37-50.
Continued research on the relationship between parental Deblinger, E., & Heflin, A. (1996). Treating sexually abused children and
and child attributions is also needed. Finally, single-sub- their nonoffending parents: A cognitive behavioral approach. Thousand
ject and qualitative research designs may be helpful in Oaks, CA: Sage.
Deblinger, E., Lippmann, J., & Steer, R. (1996). Sexually abused chil-
investigating some of the more subtle issues in this area, dren suffering posttraumatic stress symptoms: Initial treatment out-
such as the impact of wording (e.g., blame versus responsi- come findings. Child Maltreatment, 1, 310-321.
bility), the relationship between self-blame, family-blame,

CHILD MALTREATMENT / FEBRUARY 2002


74 Celano et al. / ATTRIBUTION RETRAINING

Deblinger, E., McLeer, S., & Henry, D. (1990). Cognitive behavioral Morrow, K. B. (1991). Attributions of female adolescent incest victims
treatment for sexually abused children suffering post-traumatic regarding their molestation. Child Abuse & Neglect, 15, 477-483.
stress: Preliminary findings. Journal of the American Academy of Child Munson, L., & Riskin, K. (1995). In their own words: A sexual abuse work-
and Adolescent Psychiatry, 29, 748-752. book for teenage girls. Washington, DC: Child Welfare League of
Everson, M. D., Hunter, W. M., Runyon, D. K., Edelsohn, G. A., & America, Inc.
Coulter, M. L. (1989). Maternal support following disclosure of Newberger, C. M., & De Vos, E. (1988). Abuse and victimization: A life-
incest. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 59, 197-207. s pa n de ve l opm e n t a l pe r s pe c t i ve . A m e r i c a n J o u r n a l o f
Feiring, C., Taska, L., & Chen, K. (2002). Trying to understand why Orthopsychiatry, 58, 505-511.
horrible things happen: Attribution, shame, and symptom develop- Pearce, J., & Pezzot-Pearce, T. (1997). Psychotherapy of abused and
ment following sexual abuse. Child Maltreatment, 7, 26-41. neglected children. New York: Guilford.
Finch, A. J., Nelson, W. M., & Ott, E. S. (Eds.). (1993). Cognitive-behav- Ryan, G. (1989). Victim to victimizer: Rethinking victim treatment.
ioral procedures with children and adolescents. Boston: Allyn & Bacon. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 4, 325-341.
Finkelhor, D., & Browne, A. (1985). The traumatic impact of child sex- Saltz, E., & Medow, M. L. (1971). Concept conservation in children:
ual abuse: A conceptualization. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, The dependence of belief systems on semantic representation. Child
55, 530-541. Development, 42, 1533-1542.
Friedrich, W. N. (1990). Psychotherapy of sexually abused children and their Sanders-Phillips, K., Moisan, P. A., Wadlington, S., Morgan, S., & Eng-
families. New York: Norton. lish, K. (1995). Ethnic differences in psychological functioning
Friedrich, W. N. (1996). Clinical considerations of empirical treatment among Black and Latino sexually abused girls. Child Abuse & Neglect,
studies of abused children. Child Maltreatment, 1, 343-347. 19, 691-706.
Hansen, D. J., Hecht, D. B., & Futa, K. (1998). Child sexual abuse. In V. Saywitz, K. J., Mannarino, A. P., Berliner, L., & Cohen, J. A. (2000).
Van Hasselt & M. Hersen (Eds.), Handbook of psychological treatment Treatment for sexually abused children and adolescents. American
protocols for children and adolescents (The LEA series in personality Psychologist, 55, 1040-1049.
and clinical psychology) (pp. 153-178). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Seligman, M., Peterson, C., Kaslow, N., Tanenbaum, R., Alloy, L., &
Erlbaum. Abramson, L. (1984). Attributional style and depressive symptoms
Hazzard, A. (1995). Structured treatment and prevention activities for among children. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93, 235-238.
sexually abused children. Directions in Clinical Psychology, 5, 1-10. Shapiro, J. P. (1989). Self-blame versus helplessness in sexually abused
Hazzard, A., Celano, M., Gould, J., Lawry, S., & Webb, C. (1995). Pre- children: An attributional analysis with treatment recommenda-
dicting symptomatology and self-blame among child sex abuse vic- tions. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 8, 442-455.
tims. Child Abuse and Neglect, 19(6), 707-714. Shapiro, J. P. (1995). Attribution-based treatment of self-blame and
Holmbeck, G. N. (1997). Toward terminological, conceptual, and sta- helplessness in sexually abused children. Psychotherapy, 32, 581-591.
tistical clarity in the study of mediators and moderators: Examples Spaccarelli, S., & Fuchs, C. (1997). Variability in symptom expression
from the child-clinical and pediatric psychology literatures. Journal among sexually abused girls: Developing multivariate models. Jour-
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65, 599-610. nal of Clinical Child Psychology, 26, 24-35.
Hunter, J. A., Goodwin, D. W., & Wilson, R. J. (1992). Attributions of Spaccarelli, S., & Kim, S. (1995). Resilience criteria and factors associ-
blame in child sexual abuse victims: An analysis of age and gender ated with resilience in sexually abused girls. Child Abuse & Neglect,
influences. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, 1, 75-89. 19, 1171-1182.
James, B. (1989). Treating traumatized children: New insights and creative Spinal-Robinson, P., & Wickham, R. (1992). Cartwheels: A workbook
interventions. New York: Free Press. for children who have been sexually abused. Notre Dame, IN: Jalice.
Karp, C., & Butler, T. (1996a). Activity book for treatment strategies for Stowell, J., & Dietzel, M. (1982). My very own book about me. Spokane:
abused children: From victim to survivor. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Lutheran Social Services of Washington.
Karp, C., & Butler, T. (1996b). Treatment strategies for abused children: Stowell, J., Dietzel, M., & Gleason, B. B. (2000). My very own book about
From victim to survivor. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. me! A personal safety book. Spokane: Lutheran Social Services of
Kendall, P., & Treadwell, K. (1996). Cognitive-behavioral treatment Washington.
for childhood anxiety disorders. In E. Hibbs & P. Jensen (Eds.), Sweet, P. (1981). Something happened to me. Racine, WI: Mother Cour-
Psychosocial treatments for child and adolescent disorders: Empirically age Press.
based strategies for clinical practice. Washington, DC: American Psy- Wieland, S. (1997). Hearing the internal trauma: Working with children and
chological Association. adolescents who have been sexually abused. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Lamb, S. (1986). Treating sexually abused children: Issues of blame and Wolfe, D. A., Sas, L., & Wekerle, C. (1994). Factors associated with the
responsibility. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 56, 303-307. development of posttraumatic stress disorder among child victims of
Lewinsohn, P., Clarke, G., Rohde, P., Hops, H., & Seeley, J. (1996). A sexual abuse. Child Abuse & Neglect, 18, 37-50.
course in coping: A cognitive-behavioral approach to the treatment Wyatt, G. E. (1990). Sexual abuse of ethnic minority children: Iden-
of adolescent depression. In E. Hibbs & P. Jensen (Eds.), Psychosocial tifying dimensions of victimization. Professional Psychology: Research
treatments for child and adolescent disorders: Empirically based strategies and Practice, 21, 338-343.
for clinical practice. Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association.
Marianne Celano, Ph.D., is a licensed clinical psychologist on the
Lindon, J., & Nourse, C. A. (1994). A multi-dimensional model of
groupwork for adolescent girls who have been sexually abused. Child faculty in the Department Of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences at
Abuse and Neglect, 18, 341-348. Emory University School of Medicine. She has participated in an
Mandell, J., & Damon, L. (1989). Group treatment for sexually abused chil- empirical outcome study of a structured treatment program for sexu-
dren. New York: Guilford. ally abused girls and their nonoffending mothers. She has also con-
Mannarino, A. P., & Cohen, J. A. (1996). Abuse-related attributions ducted regional and national training workshops on treating sexually
and perceptions, general attributions, and locus of control in sexu- abused children and their families. She has published several articles on
ally abused girls. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 11, 162-180. child sexual abuse, cultural issues in professional practice, and pediat-
Mannarino, A. P., Cohen, J. A., & Berman, S. R. (1994). The Children’s ric asthma.
Attributions and Perceptions Scale: A new measure of sexual abuse-
related factors. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 23, 204-211.
McMillen, C., & Zuravin, A. (1997). Attributions of blame and respon- Ann Hazzard, Ph.D., is a licensed clinical psychologist on the fac-
sibility for child sexual abuse and adult adjustment. Journal of Inter- ulty in pediatrics at Emory University School of Medicine. She has
personal Violence, 12, 30-48. directed a federally funded clinical research project on a sexual abuse
Miller, P. H., & Aloise, P. A. (1989). Young children’s understanding of prevention curriculum and a state-funded program evaluation of a
the psychological causes of behavior: A review. Child Development, structured treatment program for sexually abused girls and
60, 257-285.

CHILD MALTREATMENT / FEBRUARY 2002


Celano et al. / ATTRIBUTION RETRAINING 75

nonoffending mothers. She has published numerous articles related to Claudia B. Lang is a licensed clinical psychologist in private prac-
abuse in professional journals and provides training and supervision tice and at the Marcus Institute for Development and Learning at
about sexual abuse sequelae and treatment, play therapy with chil- Emory University. From 1995 to 1997, she was the clinical director
dren, and cognitive-behavioral treatment approaches with families. of the Sexual Abuse Trauma Clinic at the University of Miami School
of Medicine. Currently, she performs evaluations and therapy for
Susan K. Campbell is a licensed psychologist who has studied and children with developmental disabilities, learning disabilities, attach-
worked in child sexual advocacy center settings and is currently clini- ment disorders, and sexual abuse. She has published in the areas of
cal director of the Georgia Center for Children, which serves sexually maternal depression, attachment theory, autism, and sexual abuse.
abused children and child homicide witnesses in Atlanta. She oversees
the clinical, forensic, and training components of this program. She is
involved in research efforts with local universities and provides agency
consultation, training, and expert testimony on a statewide level.

CHILD MALTREATMENT / FEBRUARY 2002

You might also like