You are on page 1of 10

Handbook of Clinical Neurology, Vol.

167 (3rd series)


Geriatric Neurology
S.T. DeKosky and S. Asthana, Editors
https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-804766-8.00011-X
Copyright © 2019 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved

Chapter 11

Cognitive reserve
YAAKOV STERN1* AND DANIEL BARULLI2
1
Departments of Neurology, Psychiatry and Taub Institute, Columbia University College of Physicians and Surgeons,
New York, NY, United States
2
Department of Psychology, Columbia University, New York, NY, United States

Abstract
Cognitive reserve is a latent construct theorized to account for the discrepancy between observed brain
deterioration and ultimate clinical outcomes. This review outlines the theoretical development of the
reserve concept and presents major trends within epidemiological and neuroimaging research literatures
in support of such a construct. Particular focus is placed on the implications for cognitive aging and
dementia.

Age-related dementia and normal cognitive decline


THE RESERVE CONCEPT
can be explained with several theories, all of which have
Disparity is frequently reported between an individual’s alternative but possibly interactive etiologic hypotheses.
level of brain pathology, such as disease burden as with These include the vascular hypothesis of dementia, the
Alzheimer’s disease (AD), and their discernible function inflammatory hypothesis, toxin build up within the brain,
on neuropsychologic instruments or on activities of daily and even psychosocial factors (Qiu and Fratiglioni,
living. This disparity has been explained by the concept 2011). Through observational studies, we can say with
of cognitive reserve (CR) (Stern, 2002; Barulli and Stern, some confidence that mixed brain pathologies, such as
2013). This chapter discusses epidemiologic evidence neurodegenerative and vascular pathologies, are respon-
for CR, controversies over its measurement, and the sible for a sizeable amount of dementia cases (Schneider
nature of reserve. Although most of the research being et al., 2007). A number of specific risk factors responsi-
conducted on CR is centered on cognitive decline and ble for dementia have been brought to light, with age
dementia (and this review focuses on those processes), being the most important one: approximately 70% of
it is important to realize that CR is not a mechanism all dementia cases occur in people who are 75 years of
solely compensatory to aging and age-related maladies. age or older (Fratiglioni et al., 2000). Over time, the accu-
Prevention or amelioration of cognitive impairment due mulation of risk factors can affect dementia susceptibil-
to multiple sclerosis (Sumowski et al., 2009), Parkinson’s ity, which makes longitudinal studies of all of these
disease (Farinpour et al., 2003), and HIV-related dementia risk factors as well as protective factors simultaneously,
(Kesler et al., 2003) has been shown to be facilitated by a necessity to fully understand models of dementia
lifelong exposure to CR proxies. It has also been shown (Kivipelto et al., 2006). As Qiu and Fratiglioni (2011)
to play a role in the recovery from traumatic brain injuries state, these considerations suggest that a life course
(Schneider et al., 2014) and stroke (Robertson and Murre, model of dementia may be a stronger method of studying
1999). CR has also shown to be protective against pathol- this form of chronic disorder, which is often character-
ogy that is traditionally vascular, such as white matter ized by very long (decades) latent periods. This set of fac-
hyperintensities (Brickman et al., 2009). tors, which differentially position some individuals than

*Correspondence to: Yaakov Stern, Ph.D., Professor of Neuropsychology, Columbia University College of Physicians and
Surgeons, 622 W 168th St, PH18-321, New York, NY 10032, United States. Tel: +1-212-342-1350, Fax: +1-212-342-1838,
E-mail: ys11@columbia.edu
182 Y. STERN AND D. BARULLI
others for greater levels of cognitive impairment, is the (Qiu et al., 2001). Education at higher levels has been
first issue that needs to be kept in mind while considering shown to correlate with lower levels of cognitive decline
not just cognitive aging, but the processes meant to and lower risk of dementia (Meng and D’Arcy, 2012).
compensate for it, such as CR. This protection is strongly supported in the literature on
The development of the reserve theory hinges on reserve, being found by several research teams (Albert
the ability to understand complex interactions among et al., 1995; Butler et al., 1996; Chodosh et al., 2002;
risk and protective lifestyle factors. Reserve proposes Christensen et al., 1997; Lyketsos et al., 1999; Farmer
that certain environmental factors can explain and et al., 1995; Zahodne et al., 2014).
predict why some individuals seem to respond more
favorably in the presence of pathology than others. In
MEASURING RESERVE AND ITS
a pioneering postmortem study of cognitively normal
ALTERNATIVES
women, Katzman et al. (1989) discovered advanced
Alzheimer’s pathology in 10 of these women; this was The measurement of CR is controversial because of
accounted for by the “brain reserve” hypothesis, which the vast array of evidence for a protective set of lifestyle
proposed that larger brains would be more durable to factors as well as the theoretical nature of CR. As dis-
pathology, effectively not allowing the effects of cussed in this chapter, the first theoretical difference
dementia to make themselves clinically or behaviorally drawn when considering the epidemiologic data is the
manifest (Katzman, 1993). The rationale behind this distinction between passive and active concept, with
concept is that the larger the brain, the larger the number the former being more closely related to brain reserve
of neurons and synapses, and hence the greater the (Satz et al., 2011) and the latter being more in relation
threshold to be reached before pathologic changes make to CR (Stern, 2002). If reserve was simply a quantifiable
themselves present behaviorally. The brain reserve (BR) metric that was able to be measured within the brain
theory is present in several modified forms today. Studies itself, then response to pathology would likely not be
indicate that roughly 25%–32% of individuals who at highly modified by life-long exposure factors, such as
autopsy show plaques and neurofibrillary tangles charac- the ones discussed later. However, we know this is not
teristic of AD are not diagnosed with dementia in their the case. CR proposes that the relationships between
lifetime (Ince, 2001; Riley et al., 2002; Roe et al., the numerous lifetime exposure factors can lead to a brain
2007). More nuanced structural variables have been pro- that is much more resilient with respect to the presence of
posed to explain this observation: one suggested metric damage and pathology, and knowledge of how many fac-
that can help predict the rate of cognitive decline in tors are present in an individual’s life is useful to predict
response to certain pathologies is the combined number to a given level of pathology (Stern, 2002). A further
of pyramidal neurons (Sachdev and Valenzuela, 2009), major distinction must be drawn between CR and the
which can effectively complement other measures such newer notion of brain maintenance (BM). Brain mainte-
as the circumference of one’s head, which has been nance can be defined as the theory that lifestyle factors
linked to the risk of dementia (Mortimer et al., 2003). give an individual protection from the development of
Recently cortical fractal dimensionality or the fractal pathology itself, as opposed to protection given against
structure of the cortical surface has been proposed as the manifestation of harmful cognitive decline in the
another advanced metric for quantifying a reserve-like presence of increasing pathologic burden (Nyberg
variable (Whalley et al., 2016). The associations among et al., 2012). While brain maintenance may be viewed
these variables, however, is affected by other variables as internally protective, CR does not prevent the preor-
within the model, such as genetic factors that may put dained, biologic factors from spurring on dementia; it
one at greater or lesser risk for dementia (Graves acts as a complementary mechanism, which modifies
et al., 2001). how the brain responds to said factors, once they are pre-
The development of the concept of CR, in contrast, can sent. Discerning differences between these two theories
be attributed to the subsequent observation of protective is challenging, seeing as they both assume that similar
effects of experiential, rather than biometric, factors. BR variables, such as education and complex mental activi-
and CR differ in that BR is viewed as a predominantly ties, are responsible for granting protection. Valenzuela
passive model wherein once some biologic threshold of et al. (2008) discovered less hippocampal atrophy over
disease burden or neural loss has been reached, cognitive a 3-year period within individuals who engaged in com-
decline is inevitable; and CR is viewed as an active process plex mental activities over their lifetimes, consistent with
where such thresholds can be manipulated with certain the brain maintenance model. However, most of featured
lifetime experiences (Stern, 2002). Education seems to research shows greater support for the compensation
have a profound, protective effect on long-term cognition model CR, than the protection model, BM (e.g.,
and is one of the most extensively studied factors Steffener et al., 2014). Assessing outcomes in a clinical
COGNITIVE RESERVE 183
setting, Brayne et al. (2010) found that education had Nithianantharajah and Hannan (2009) argue that a strong
an indirect effect on the neurodegenerative and vas- correlation between environmental enrichment and CR is
cular pathologies. The pathologies were not prevented present, and understanding one could shed light on the
directly. Thus, it seems that brain maintenance may be other. Environmental enrichment, however, can be more
the most applicable to normal aging, where lifestyle easily studied using more invasive experimental manip-
and physical activity can, evidently, affect the mainte- ulations in animal models. It can, therefore, potentially
nance of the brain itself. However, some evidence has help us understand how the how mechanisms of CR
shown that AD and other brain pathology may be directly develop and operate over the course of one’s life and
impacted by lifestyle. Therefore, CR and brain mainte- even across generations. Such an epigenetic implemen-
nance may complement one another and be differentially tation of CR can be seen in the increase of long-term
applicable depending on the type of brain deterioration potentiation within babies of mothers exposed to envi-
present. Indeed, recent work directly analyzing the two ronmental enrichment, even when the offspring are not
concepts (Habeck et al., 2016) has found that while the (Arai et al., 2009).
two measures can both be correlated with the same prox- In addition, the two theories exhibit different mecha-
ies (e.g., education and verbal IQ), they themselves can nisms as explanations of observations. CR suggests that
remain unassociated. cognitive responses play a role in the brain’s response
While the CR theory does not seek to exclude brain over a long period of time to certain types of exposure
related structural measures, it assumes that prediction variables outlined later, while the short-term response
of dementia susceptibility is possible, independent of is present to address challenges once the pathology is
physical or biologic measurements, unlike brain reserve. present, in the form of functional activation changes
However, as we gain greater understanding of the micro- (Stern, 2009). Stern suggested neural reserve and neural
structural foundations and functional networks of the compensation are characteristic of the neural implemen-
brain through the use of tools such as MRI-based Diffu- tation of CR. Neural reserve proposes that differences
sion Tensor Imaging (DTI), brain reserve and CR in the efficiency, capacity, or flexibility of cognitive
become increasingly interrelated, as there is usually a networks are a result of lifelong exposures. This may
neurophysiologic correlate of the environmental expo- account for the noticeable differences between individ-
sures of a given individual (Barulli and Stern, 2013). uals with high and low CR; the former have networks
Additionally, there are increasingly fewer distinctions which exhibit greater capacity (i.e., a larger range of acti-
between the two theories, as evidence of environmental vation, due to the response to a neural challenge, such
exposures such as exercise having neuroplastic modifi- as performing a difficult task) and/or greater efficiency
cations to brain structure accumulate (Whalley et al., (i.e., at the exact same level of activation as measured
2004). Structural brain measures have also been recently with the Blood Oxygen Level Dependent (BOLD) sig-
linked to active compensation present in CR. Piras and nal), greater performance (possibly due to more direct
colleagues (2011), for instance, have observed differ- and less repetitive synaptic wiring within said networks
ences in the microstructure of the hippocampus, in rela- or in a more expedient hemodynamic response). This
tion to education. Midlife physical activity also seems to hypothesis is supported empirically, with recent findings
protect against dementia risk (Rovio et al., 2005). Addi- demonstrating lower levels of activation with similar
tionally, some success has been demonstrated with aero- performance during a working memory task in those
bic exercise intervention with individuals already living with a higher CR, measured with education and premor-
with dementia; depression has been shown to be reduced, bid IQ (Sole-Padulles et al., 2009).
as well as primary neuropsychiatric symptoms (Paillard Another proposed mechanism of reserve is known as
et al., 2015). One suggestion for the mechanism neural compensation. This is where individuals afflicted
behind this type of intervention ties strongly into existing by pathology must use another cognitive network to
lines of animal research within enriched environments compensate for the loss of a primary task-related cogni-
(Nithianantharajah and Hannan, 2009). As reviewed tive network once it has become too damaged (Stern,
by Brown et al. (2003) and Van Praag et al. (2005), pro- 2009). In general, neural compensation is difficult to
motion of neurogenesis in areas of the hippocampus such interpret because additional functional magnetic reso-
as the dentate gyrus leads to new neurons playing an nance imaging (fMRI) activity can be associated with
active role in pattern integration, spawning new task- both improved and diminished performance. This is con-
related networks. This was found to be present in rodents sistent with what we know now of the wide array of
when placed in stimulating environments and made to neural networks (Boyle et al., 2008; Steffener and
exercise (Deng et al., 2010). Environmental enrichment Stern, 2012). Findings from fMRI provide this mecha-
could also serve as a factor that prevents or slows down nism with a vast amount of empirical evidence, in which
the progression of AD pathology (Lazarov et al., 2005). studies have found areas of additional activation in
184 Y. STERN AND D. BARULLI
older adults but not younger adults and have interpreted statistical variance. This is distinct from broader defini-
these as compensatory activation (see Morcom and tions of cognitive functions. Measures such as these
Johnson, 2015 for a review). Without activating these display congruent and discriminant validity, which sug-
secondary networks, in theory, older subjects would gests that their lumping is appropriate and, thus, may be a
not be able to perform tasks as well as younger, healthier good way to estimate reserve. This proxy variable
subjects. There are, however, reports of compensatory approach has yielded many standardized ways to esti-
networks being associated with poorer performance mate lifetime exposures which are thought be in line with
(Craik, 2006; Steffener and Stern, 2012). It is fair to CR (Harrison et al., 2015). Valenzuela and Sachdev
assume, in these situations, that the compensatory net- (2007) for instance, developed the “Lifetime Experi-
work is utilized in the place of damaged primary ences Questionnaire” to convey academic, social, occu-
networks and, thus, is not as effective. The alternative pational, and leisure mental stimulation throughout
network acts as a cane to a pair of legs; it allows one lifetime. In addition to showing validity and reliability,
to walk, but not nearly as well. it has been useful in weeding out individuals that will
An alternative way to approach CR is to find cognitive experience cognitive decline over the next 18 months.
processes that might underlie it. CR, for example, could Problematically, in a meta-review of operationalizations
be associated with one’s ability to figure out and use of CR, Harrison et al. (2015) found a notable lack of
alternative cognitive processes and strategies (Barulli consensus among research teams as to the definition
and Stern, 2013; Barulli et al., 2013). This stance on and agreed proxies of CR. There certainly is no “gold
CR is heavily cognitive-oriented and is in tandem with standard” CR composite, and there is high variation
results of factor analyses of CR proxies and neuropsy- between groups and across studies, but proxies have been
chologic variables. These studies tend to find high a generally effective approach with regard to making
levels of overlap between cognitive control components predictions about the conversion of dementia.
of executive function factors and CR (Siedlecki et al., A different view was expressed in a review by Satz
2009). Similar theories of CR propose that it is primarily et al. (2011), stating that while reserve is successful based
brought about by working memory ability (Sandry and on the fact that the proxies it uses are found to modulate
Sumowski, 2014), can be obtained by the long-term effects of pathology on cognition, it is unsuccessful as an
deployment and enhancement of the noradrenergic sys- explanatory construct because of the narrow amount of
tem, and because of this, is mainly a function of attention support for the a priori grouping of these indicators, such
(Robertson, 2013) or is essentially a product of decision- as education, socioeconomic status, or IQ, together.
making factors through the lifespan (Boyle et al., 2013). Additionally, reserve is often touted as dissociable from
Ideally, research will be able to find ways to augment constructs, such as a construct of general intelligence (g),
and utilize CR through understanding it in both the cog- which themselves are often used to predict disease
nitive and neurophysiologic terms. The noradrenergic outcomes (Gottfredson, 2004; Gottfredson and Deary,
theory of reserve recently received not only theoretical 2004). Satz argues that few studies show the necessary
support but also demonstrated methodological utility; dual validity among the reserve proxies or the discrimi-
following up on observations that neuronal density in nant validity that should exist between such concepts and
the locus coeruleus (a major source of norepinephrine) supposedly dissociable factors. He argues reserve is
was inversely associated with dementia risk, Clewett more likely a 2-factor model (brain and CR) or even a
et al. (2015) reported that signal intensity in this region 4-factor model (where “reserve” decays into individual
using neuromelanin-sensitive MRI is positively associ- factors such as processing resources, executive function,
ated with CR proxies such as education and verbal complex mental activities, and g) rather than a model uti-
intelligence. These authors argue that these results point lizing a single latent factor. Jones et al. (2011) also voice
to the first bona fide biomarker of CR. these concerns and argue that prior to utilizing the reserve
Putting aside the ontologic considerations about CR, concept, certain a priori decisions must be explicated.
the question becomes how it is best measured, as numer- One of these decisions is whether to handle reserve as
ous exposure variables have been associated with it. a reflective (proxies for reserve each occur individually
Proxy variables seem to be the most dominant way to because of the underlying latent variable that is usually
approach this task, using variables such as premorbid antecedent to them but do not affect one another) or as
IQ, education, or occupational history. These approaches a formative latent variable (proxies affect one another,
can be problematic since reserve is thought to be a cumu- but collectively contribute to an emergent reserve con-
lative process based on experiences that occur in both struct). The former model would work well with a
early and late life (Richards and Sacker, 2003). But fixed genetic interpretation of disease risk, whereas
Siedlecki et al. (2009) found that within structural equa- the latter raises questions about a single reserve vari-
tion models, IQ and exposure variables share common able’s construct validity, but is consistent with the
COGNITIVE RESERVE 185
theory of CR. Another concern voiced by Jones and measure that is beyond proxy variables. Stern et al.
others is the close relationship between socioeconomic (2008), for example, were able to find an overarching
status (SES) and standard reserve proxies. Low SES is neural network that was utilized during a common visual
powerfully associated with disease, and with proxies for and verbal delayed match-to-sample test, whose expres-
CR correlating highly with SES, it is not surprising that sion differed as a function of CR within a group of young
the correlation is used to predict health outcomes. The adults, and was independent of task performance. Iden-
argument is present in animal research (van Praag et al., tification of these networks is of utmost importance to
2000), where enriched environment paradigm critics researchers. Unlike proxy measures, this measure of
note that enrichment may not be protective, but the reserve would be much more direct. Furthermore, it
depravity of a standard housing condition could affect would require making fewer assumptions about the
and exaggerate study outcomes. Indeed, this perspec- convergent and discriminant validity of reserve proxies.
tive seems to conform well with recent findings that Because this measure is modifiable by lifetime experi-
parental education and income are major determinants ences but does not clearly fluctuate, it would capture
of brain volumes in children from infancy through ado- the essence of reserve, such as the previously discussed
lescence (Noble et al., 2015). As Whalley et al. (2016) CR measure. Lastly, many of the extant theoretical ques-
speculate, this could reflect any number of possible tions about the nature of the mechanisms of reserve
causal sequences, including not only less access to edu- would be settled if a measure like this was identified. For-
cational resources but also more biologic deprivations tunately, extensive research in the field of neuroimaging
such as poor nutrition or increased psychosocial stress. is underway, utilizing a vast spectrum of cognitive tasks,
Regardless of the cause, one study by the same authors measures of brain structure and demographics, fMRI
even found that childhood SES was predictive of function, performance outcomes, and traditional proxy
adult hippocampal volume even after controlling for measures of CR (e.g., Stern et al., 2014).
education, gender, adult SES, and intelligence at age
11 (Whalley et al., 2016).
FACTORS THAT IMPART RESERVE
Reed et al. (2010), with respect to quantifying reserve
that doesn’t rely on proxies, have taken a different Studies indicate that control of risk factors such as hyper-
approach—a technique wherein they broke down the tension, abstinence from smoking, and regular physical
variance of episodic memory performance and denoted activity may effectively slow the progression of demen-
CR as the residual variance after subtracting the variance tia, and even counter the risk of dementia in the first place
accounted for by variables like age, sex, and ethnicity, (Flicker et al., 2010). Although these may seem like fea-
as well as by structural brain measures and education. sible preventive measures, there is still controversy sur-
The leftover variance should, in theory, correspond to rounding the establishment of a universal approach to
“reserve” in a more basic sense or maintain function prevent cognitive decline (Williams et al., 2010). Identi-
despite the predictions of pathology and other confounding fication of these gradual steps can be crucial to learning
demographics. Indeed, this calculated version of CR paral- to cope with the public health crisis of Alzheimer’s and
leled with word reading scores and seemed to slightly dementia. Estimates show that almost half of AD cases
decrease the rate of conversion from mild cognitive impair- may result from risk factors that would have been possi-
ment (MCI) to dementia, as well as the rate of decline in the ble to modify. Three million cases of Alzheimer’s could
executive abilities of individuals. Moreover, in individuals have been prevented with a reduction of just 10%–25%
with high levels of CR, baseline brain status was less pre- of these risk factors, according to some estimates (Barnes
dictive. We should expect this, if this is a truly pure mea- and Yaffe, 2011). There is evidence that delaying the
sure of reserve. Interesting and potentially useful, this onset of the disease by just 1 year could be enough to
measure of reserve could work well if we are willing to reduce the global prevalence of the disease by as many
accept the varying nature of the measure, given its reliance as 9.2 million in 2050 (Brookmeyer et al., 2007). Failure
on varying tasks and utilization of certain brain measure. in identifying working interventions can be attributed in
However, if researchers can discover more substantial cog- part to the common approach of analyzing only a single
nitive and neural mechanisms for reserve and demonstrate avenue of prevention, as opposed to analyzing the myr-
the validity of the construct using a method like Structural iad factors that play a part in dementia (Mangialasche
Equation Modeling (SEM), then a realistic interpretation et al., 2012).
like the one discussed later seems more plausible. Education has been proven to be such a powerful, pro-
A CR network is a collection of networks that could tective force that it lowers the risk of dementia by about
mediate among sets of lifetime variables that are thought 50% in humans with the ApoE-4 allele, the strongest
to give reserve and the compensatory mechanism seen in known genetic risk factor for AD that is not a mutation
mental task performance. This is a potential for a CR (Wang et al., 2011). In an early epidemiologic study of
186 Y. STERN AND D. BARULLI
the relation of education and occupation to dementia risk, cognitive outcomes (Scarmeas et al., 2001) and have
a longitudinal sample consisting of 593 nondemented been shown to protect against the onset of dementia
elderly was followed for up to 4 years, and 106 converted (Akbaraly et al., 2009). Engagement in intellectually or
to dementia; 101 of these dementia cases were AD. Edu- socially stimulating activities has been shown to reduce
cation played a crucial role in the conversion rates of risk of developing dementia by 30% (Akbaraly et al.,
this study, with the conversion being 2.2 times greater 2009). In addition, studies have shown that living with
in those with less than 8 years of education as compared a partner in midlife decreases risks of dementia later in
to those with more education. It was also 2.25 times life (Håkansson et al., 2009). Another study showed that
higher in those with a low level of occupational attain- with a greater number of leisure activities, the likelihood
ment compared to high, and the risk increased to 2.87 of dementia developing decreased (Scarmeas et al.,
when the factors were considered together, suggesting 2003). While the ideal time to engage in the aforemen-
the profound effect that the two variables have collabo- tioned activities is still up for debate, the effectiveness
ratively (Stern et al., 1994). of the said activities is certain and has shown to delay
Stern et al. (1992) also used Xenon blood flow imag- the onset of dementia (Paillard-Borg et al., 2009). One
ing to investigate how education might moderate the form of cognitive stimulation, bilingualism, has garnered
effect of AD pathology on clinical disease severity. special empirical attention. Craik et al. (2010) investi-
When patients were matched for overall disease severity, gated the medical records of 211 patients who were
parietotemporal cerebral blood circulation was lower in elderly and had AD. Of these 211, 102 were bilingual
individuals with higher education. This suggests that and showed a dementia onset 5.1 years later than those
the pathology of their AD was more advanced, but out of the group who were monolingual. Using CT scans,
that those with higher education were in some way neuroimaging has also shown that bilinguals show much
compensating for it. Additionally, negative correlations more AD pathology than elderly individuals who are
have been found between cerebral metabolism in the pre- monolingual on average, when matched for cognitive
frontal, premotor, and left superior parietal areas and function (Schweizer et al., 2012). This again suggests
higher education, after controlling for dementia severity. that the bilinguals are compensating for a greater under-
This again hints at a compensatory role for additional lying disease burden. However, some findings still
regional activation when disease severity increases suggest that studies that show that bilingualism delays
(Alexander et al., 1997). Later studies have confirmed the dementing process were cross sectional and relied
using positron emission tomography (PET) imaging that too much on estimations of dementia duration. One inci-
patients with a greater level of education exhibit more dence study did not find that bilingualism had any
profound AD-related blood flow decline (Garibotto protective effect (Zahodne et al., 2014).
et al., 2008; Kemppainen et al., 2008; Oh et al., 2015), Premorbid IQ also has been shown to act as a shield
suggesting that their brains are showing resilience and against the onset of dementia (Schmand et al., 1997).
protection in the face of the deleterious nature of the Rentz et al. (2010) found that within a group of cogni-
disease, because of their levels of education. The over- tively normal people, when memory scores were
arching message of these studies is that highly educated adjusted based on IQ, they showed correlation with cere-
people are less likely to be diagnosed with dementia and bral perfusion in areas of the brain that were vulnerable
higher education results in better cognitive function to the early pathology of AD. Greater pathology was
(Bennett et al., 2003) when brain pathology is at a con- observed, despite intact memory, in those with higher
stant, even while controlling for pathology (Roe et al., IQ. These individuals also exhibited higher cognitive
2007). These findings do not dismiss the concept of decline over the next three years. This decline is charac-
brain reserve. However, they do stress the need for a teristic of CR, which theorizes that once symptoms are
more active version of compensation, rather than just present, dementia progresses at faster rates in people with
the presence of a larger brain to begin with or an higher CR (Stern et al., 1999; Scarmeas et al., 2006; Hall
increased number of neurons. et al., 2007). Valenzuela and Sachdev (2005), in a meta-
Slower rates of cognitive decline have been attributed analysis of 33 studies over the course of 7 years, found
to other factors, outside of education. Earlier, a study by that 25 of 33 studies displayed substantial protective
Stern and colleagues indicated a negative correlation effects through variables ranging from education to
between occupational status and signs of dementia in late stimulating leisure activities. An overall decreased risk
life (Stern et al., 1994, 1995). Karp et al. (2004) later dis- of dementia at 46% was observed in individuals who
covered that mentally strenuous or challenging activities engaged in the widest array of these activities. Although
at work, during midlife, substantially reduced dementia this can be observed as beneficial, the relationships that
risk. Beneficial results have been shown to arise from each of the factors has with one another need to be taken
stimulating leisure activities as well with respect to into account, simultaneously, to paint the most accurate
COGNITIVE RESERVE 187
picture. For example, one study found that the protective Bennett DA, Wilson RS, Schneider JA et al. (2003). Education
effects of education became a noncontributing factor modifies the relation of AD pathology to level of cognitive
for preventing cognitive decline, after controlling for function in older persons. Neurology 60: 1909–1915.
late-life leisure activities (Hall et al., 2009). This shows Boyle PA, Wilson RS, Schneider JA et al. (2008). Processing
resources reduce the effect of Alzheimer pathology on
the impact on research that investigating risk factors in
other cognitive systems. Neurology 70: 1534–1542.
isolation can have.
Boyle PA et al. (2013). Much of late life cognitive decline is
not due to common neurodegenerative pathologies. Ann
Neurol 74: 478–489. https://doi.org/10.1002/ana.23964.
CONCLUSION Brayne C et al. (2010). Education, the brain and dementia: neu-
roprotection or compensation? EClipSE Collaborative
Understanding the full spectrum of risk and protective
Members. Brain 133: 2210–2216.
factors not only allows us to understand dementia and
Brickman AM, Siedlecki KL, Muraskin J et al. (2009). White
reserve from an epidemiologic stance, but it is also crit- matter hyperintensities and cognition: testing the reserve
ical to helping develop CR-based interventions. Recent hypothesis. Neurobiol Aging 32: 1588–1598.
work suggests that differential levels of CR can affect Brookmeyer R, Johnson E, Ziegler-Graham K et al. (2007).
the efficacy of cognitive training programs in demented Forecasting the global burden of Alzheimer’s disease.
adults; Mondini et al. (2016) showed that older adults Alzheimers Dement 3: 186–191.
with lower levels of CR benefitted from cognitive train- Brown J, Cooper-Kuhn CM, Kempermann G et al. (2003).
ing more intensely than older adults with high CR, which Enriched environment and physical activity stimulate
could reflect the fact that high CR may be masking an hippocampal but not olfactory bulb neurogenesis. Eur
already highly advanced disease state. Alternatively, J Neurosci 17: 2042–2046.
Butler SM, Ashford JW, Snowdon DA (1996). Age, education,
findings such as this may support the environmental
and changes in the Mini-Mental State Exam scores of older
enrichment critics; those with lower CR may simply
women: findings from the Nun Study. J Am Geriatr Soc 44:
have been cognitively “impoverished” prior to the train- 675–681.
ing regimen and so actually have room for improvement, Chodosh J, Reuben DB, Albert MS et al. (2002). Predicting
while those with high CR do not. Regardless, findings cognitive impairment in high-functioning community-
such as these point to the relevance of understanding dwelling older persons: MacArthur studies of successful
CR for not just epidemiologic understanding, but aging. J Am Geriatr Soc 50: 1051–1060.
pragmatic implementation as well. Christensen H, Korten AE, Jorm AF et al. (1997). Education
and decline in cognitive performance: compensatory but
not protective. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 12: 323–330.
REFERENCES Clewett D, Lee TH, Greening S et al. (2015). Neuromelanin
marks the spot: identifying a locus coeruleus biomarker of
Akbaraly TN, Portet F, Fustinoni S et al. (2009). Leisure activ- cognitive reserve in healthy aging. Neurobiol Aging 30: 1e10.
ities and the risk of dementia in the elderly results from the Craik FI (2006). Brain–behavior relations across the lifespan: a
Three-City Study. Neurology 73: 854–861. commentary. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 30: 885–892.
Albert MS, Jones K, Savage CR et al. (1995). Predictors of Craik FI, Bialystok E, Freedman M (2010). Delaying the onset
cognitive change in older persons: MacArthur studies of of Alzheimer disease bilingualism as a form of cognitive
successful aging. Psychol Aging 10: 578. reserve. Neurology 75: 1726–1729.
Alexander GE, Furey ML, Grady CL et al. (1997). Association Deng W, Aimone JB, Gage FH (2010). New neurons and new
of premorbid intellectual function with cerebral metabo- memories: how does adult hippocampal neurogenesis affect
lism in Alzheimer’s disease: implications for the cognitive learning and memory? Nat Rev Neurosci 11: 339–350.
reserve hypothesis. Am J Psychiatry 154: 165–172. Farinpour R, Miller EN, Satz P et al. (2003). Psychosocial
Arai JA, Li S, Hartley DM et al. (2009). Transgenerational risk factors of HIV morbidity and mortality: findings from
rescue of a genetic defect in long-term potentiation and the multicenter AIDS cohort study (MACS). J Clin Exp
memory formation by juvenile enrichment. J Neurosci Neuropsychol 25: 654–670.
29: 1496–1502. Farmer ME, Kittner SJ, Rae DS et al. (1995). Education and
Barnes DE, Yaffe K (2011). The projected effect of risk factor change in cognitive function: the epidemiologic catchment
reduction on Alzheimer’s disease prevalence. Lancet area study. Ann Epidemiol 5: 1–7.
Neurol 10: 819–828. Flicker L, Liu-Ambrose T, Kramer AF (2010). Why so nega-
Barulli D, Stern Y (2013). Efficiency, capacity, compensation, tive about preventing cognitive decline and dementia? The
maintenance, plasticity: emerging concepts in cognitive jury has already come to the verdict for physical activity
reserve. Trends Cogn Sci 17: 502–509. https://doi.org/ and smoking cessation. Br J Sports Med 45: 465–467.
10.1016/j.tics.2013.08.012. Fratiglioni L, Launer LJ, Andersen K et al. (2000). Incidence
Barulli DJ et al. (2013). The influence of cognitive reserve on of dementia and major subtypes in Europe: a collaborative
strategy selection in normal aging. J Int Neuropsychol Soc study of population-based cohorts. Neurologic Diseases in
19: 1–4. the Elderly Research Group. Neurology 54: S10–S15.
188 Y. STERN AND D. BARULLI
Garibotto V, Borroni B, Kalbe E et al. (2008). Education and Lyketsos CG, Chen LS, Anthony JC (1999). Cognitive decline
occupation as proxies for reserve in aMCI converters and in adulthood: an 11.5-year follow-up of the Baltimore epi-
AD FDG-PET evidence. Neurology 71: 1342–1349. demiologic catchment area study. Am J Psychiatry 156:
Gottfredson LS (2004). Intelligence: is it the epidemiologists’ 58–65.
elusive "fundamental cause" of social class inequalities in Mangialasche F, Kivipelto M, Solomon A et al. (2012).
health? J Pers Soc Psychol 86: 174. Dementia prevention: current epidemiological evidence
Gottfredson LS, Deary IJ (2004). Intelligence predicts health and future perspective. Alzheimers Res Ther 4: 6. https://
and longevity, but why? Curr Dir Psychol Sci 13: 1–4. doi.org/10.1186/alzrt104.
Graves AB et al. (2001). Head circumference and incident Meng X, D’Arcy C (2012). Education and dementia in the con-
Alzheimer’s disease: modification by apolipoprotein E. text of the cognitive reserve hypothesis: a systematic
Neurology 57: 1453–1460. review with meta-analyses and qualitative analyses.
Habeck C, Razlighi Q, Gazes Y et al. (2016). Cognitive reserve PLoS One 7: e38268.
and brain maintenance: orthogonal concepts in theory and Mondini S, Madella I, Zangrossi A et al. (2016). Cognitive
practice. Cereb Cortex 27 (8): 3962–3969. Reserve in Dementia: implications for cognitive training.
Håkansson K, Rovio S, Helkala EL et al. (2009). Association Front Aging Neurosci 8.
between mid-life marital status and cognitive function Morcom AM, Johnson W (2015). Neural reorganization and
in later life: population based cohort study. BMJ 339: compensation in aging. J Cogn Neurosci.
b2462. Mortimer JA et al. (2003). Head circumference, education and
Hall CB, Derby C, LeValley A et al. (2007). Education delays risk of dementia: findings from the Nun Study. J Clin Exp
accelerated decline on a memory test in persons who Neuropsychol 25: 671–679.
develop dementia. Neurology 69: 1657–1664. Nithianantharajah J, Hannan AJ (2009). The neurobiology of
Hall CB, Lipton RB, Sliwinski M et al. (2009). Cognitive brain and cognitive reserve: mental and physical activity
activities delay onset of memory decline in persons who as modulators of brain disorders. Prog Neurobiol 89:
develop dementia. Neurology 73: 356–361. 369–382.
Harrison SL, Sajjad A, Bramer WM et al. (2015). Exploring Noble KG, Houston SM, Brito NH et al. (2015). Family
strategies to operationalize cognitive reserve: a systematic income, parental education and brain structure in children
review of reviews. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 37 (3): and adolescents. Nat Neurosci 18: 773–778.
253–264. Nyberg L et al. (2012). Memory aging and brain maintenance.
Ince PG (2001). Pathological correlates of late-onset dementia Trends Cogn Sci 16: 292–305.
in a multicenter community-based population in England Oh H, Steffener J, Razlighi QR et al. (2015). Ab-related hyper-
and Wales. Lancet 357: 169–175. activation in frontoparietal control regions in cognitively
Jones RN et al. (2011). Conceptual and measurement challenges normal elderly. Neurobiol Aging 36: 3247–3254.
in research on cognitive reserve. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 17: Paillard T, Rolland Y, de Souto Barreto P (2015). Protective
593–601. effects of physical exercise in Alzheimer’s disease and
Karp A, Kåreholt I, Qiu C et al. (2004). Relation of education Parkinson’s disease: a narrative review. J Clin Neurol 11:
and occupation-based socioeconomic status to incident 212–219.
Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Epidemiol 159: 175–183. Paillard-Borg S, Fratiglioni L, Winblad B et al. (2009). Leisure
Katzman R (1993). Education and the prevalence of dementia activities in late life in relation to dementia risk: principal
and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 43: 13–20. component analysis. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 28:
Katzman R, Aronson M, Fuld P et al. (1989). Development of 136–144.
dementing illnesses in an 80-year-old volunteer cohort. Piras F, Cherubini A, Caltagirone C et al. (2011). Education
Ann Neurol 25: 317–324. mediates microstructural changes in bilateral hippocam-
Kemppainen NM, Aalto S, Karrasch M et al. (2008). Cognitive pus. Hum Brain Mapp 32: 282–289.
reserve hypothesis: Pittsburgh compound B and fluoro- Qiu C, Fratiglioni L (2011). Epidemiology of the dementias.
deoxyglucose positron emission tomography in relation In: P McNamara (Ed.), Dementia history and incidence,
to education in mild Alzheimer’s disease. Ann Neurol vol. 1. ABC-CLIO Press, Inc., Santa Barbara, CA.
63: 112–118. Qiu C, B€ackman L, Winblad B et al. (2001). The influence of
Kesler SR, Adams HF, Blasey CM et al. (2003). Premorbid education on clinically diagnosed dementia incidence and
intellectual functioning, education, and brain size in trau- mortality data from the Kungsholmen Project. Arch Neurol
matic brain injury: an investigation of the cognitive reserve 58: 2034–2039.
hypothesis. Appl Neuropsychol 10: 153–162. Reed BR, Mungas D, Farias ST et al. (2010). Measuring cog-
Kivipelto M, Ngandu T, Laatikainen T et al. (2006). Risk score nitive reserve based on the decomposition of episodic
for the prediction of dementia risk in 20 years among mid- memory variance. Brain 133 (8): 2196–2209.
dle aged people: a longitudinal, population-based study. Rentz DM, Locascio JJ, Becker JA et al. (2010). Cognition,
Lancet Neurol 5: 735–741. reserve, and amyloid deposition in normal aging. Ann
Lazarov O, Robinson J, Tang YP et al. (2005). Environmental Neurol 67: 353–364.
enrichment reduces Ab levels and amyloid deposition in Richards M, Sacker A (2003). Lifetime antecedents of cogni-
transgenic mice. Cell 120: 701–713. tive reserve. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 25: 614–624.
COGNITIVE RESERVE 189
Riley KP, Snowdon DA, Markesbery WR (2002). Alzheimer’s Steffener J, Barulli D, Habeck C et al. (2014). The role of edu-
neurofibrillary pathology and the spectrum of cognitive cation and verbal abilities in altering the effect of age-related
function: findings from the Nun Study. Ann Neurol 51: gray matter differences on cognition. PLoS One 9: e91196.
567–577. Stern Y (2002). What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research
Robertson IH (2013). A noradrenergic theory of cognitive application of the reserve concept. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 8:
reserve: implications for Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol 448–460.
Aging 34: 298–308. Stern Y (2009). Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia 47:
Robertson IH, Murre JM (1999). Rehabilitation of brain dam- 2015–2028.
age: brain plasticity and principles of guided recovery. Stern Y et al. (1992). Inverse relationship between education
Psychol Bull 125: 544. and parietotemporal perfusion deficit in Alzheimer’s
Roe CM, Xiong C, Miller JP et al. (2007). Education and disease. Ann Neurol 32: 371–375.
Alzheimer disease without dementia support for the cogni- Stern Y et al. (1994). Influence of education and occupation
tive reserve hypothesis. Neurology 68: 223–228. on the incidence of Alzheimer’s disease. JAMA 271:
Rovio S, Kareholt I, Helkala EL et al. (2005). Leisure-time 1004–1010.
physical activity at midlife and the risk of dementia and Stern Y, Alexander GE, Prohovnik I et al. (1995). Relationship
Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet Neurol 4: 705–711. between lifetime occupation and parietal flow implications
Sachdev PS, Valenzuela M (2009). Brain and cognitive for a reserve against Alzheimer’s disease pathology.
reserve. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 17: 175–178. Neurology 45: 55–60.
Sandry J, Sumowski JF (2014). Working memory mediates the Stern Y et al. (1999). Rate of memory decline in AD is related
relationship between intellectual enrichment and long-term to education and occupation: cognitive reserve? Neurology
memory in multiple sclerosis: an exploratory analysis of 53: 1942–1957.
cognitive reserve. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 20: 868–872. Stern Y, Zarahn E, Habeck C et al. (2008). A common neural
Satz P et al. (2011). Brain and cognitive reserve: mediator(s) and network for cognitive reserve in verbal and object
construct validity, a critique. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 33: working memory in young but not old. Cereb Cortex 18:
121–130. 959–967.
Scarmeas N et al. (2001). Influence of leisure activity on the inci- Stern Y, Habeck C, Steffener J et al. (2014). The reference
dence of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 57: 2236–2242. ability neural network study: motivation, design, and initial
Scarmeas N, Zarahn E, Anderson KE et al. (2003). Association feasibility analyses. NeuroImage 103: 139–151.
of life activities with cerebral blood flow in Alzheimer Sumowski JF et al. (2009). Cognitive reserve protects against
disease: implications for the cognitive reserve hypothesis. cognitive dysfunction in multiple sclerosis. J Clin Exp
Arch Neurol 60: 359–365. Neuropsychol 31: 913–926.
Scarmeas N, Stern Y, Tang MX et al. (2006). Mediterranean Valenzuela MJ, Sachdev P (2005). Brain reserve and
diet and risk for Alzheimer’s disease. Ann Neurol 59: dementia: a systematic review. Psychol Med 25: 1–14.
912–921. Valenzuela MJ, Sachdev P (2007). Assessment of complex
Schmand B, Smit JH, Geerlings MI et al. (1997). The effects of mental activity across the lifespan: development of the life-
intelligence and education on the development of demen- time of experiences questionnaire (LEQ). Psychol Med 37:
tia. A test of the brain reserve hypothesis. Psychol Med 1015–1026.
27: 1337–1344. Valenzuela MJ et al. (2008). Lifespan mental activity predicts
Schneider JA, Arvanitakis Z, Bang W et al. (2007). Mixed brain diminished rate of hippocampal atrophy. PLoS One 3:
pathologies account for most dementia cases in community- e2598.
dwelling older persons. Neurology 69: 2197–2204. van Praag H et al. (2000). Neural consequences of environ-
Schneider EB, Sur S, Raymont V et al. (2014). Functional mental enrichment. Nat Rev Neurosci 1: 191–198.
recovery after moderate/severe traumatic brain injury: a Van Praag H, Shubert T, Zhao C et al. (2005). Exercise
role for cognitive reserve? Neurology 82 (18): 1636–1642. enhances learning and hippocampal neurogenesis in aged
Schweizer TA, Ware J, Fischer CE et al. (2012). Bilingualism mice. J Neurosci 25: 8680–8685.
as a contributor to cognitive reserve: evidence from Wang HX, Gustafson D, Kivipelto M et al. (2011). Education
brain atrophy in Alzheimer’s disease. Cortex 48 (8): halves the risk of dementia due to apolipoprotein E4 allele:
991–996. a collaborative study from the Swedish brain power initia-
Siedlecki KL, Stern Y, Reuben A et al. (2009). Construct valid- tive. Neurobiol Aging 33 (5): 1007-e1.
ity of cognitive reserve in a multiethnic cohort: the northern Whalley LJ, Deary IJ, Appleton CL et al. (2004). Cognitive
Manhattan Study. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 15: 558–569. reserve and the neurobiology of cognitive aging. Ageing
Sole-Padulles C, Bartres-Faz D, Junque C et al. (2009). Brain Res Rev 3: 369–382.
structure and function related to cognitive reserve vari- Whalley LJ, Staff RT, Fox HC et al. (2016). Cerebral correlates
ables in normal aging, mild cognitive impairment and of cognitive reserve. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging 247:
Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging 30: 1114–1124. 65–70.
Steffener J, Stern Y (2012). Exploring the neural basis of cog- Williams JW, Plassman BL, Burke J et al. (2010). Preventing
nitive reserve in aging. Biochim Biophys Acta 1822: Alzheimer’s disease and cognitive decline. Evidence
467–473. report/technology assessment no. 193. (Prepared by the
190 Y. STERN AND D. BARULLI
Duke evidence-based practice center under contract no. psychological symptoms in people with dementia: a
HHSA 290-2007-10066-I), Agency for Healthcare systematic review and meta-analysis. Ageing Res Rev
Research and Quality, Rockville, MD. AHRQ Publication 24: 274–285.
No. 10-E005. Manly JJ, Touradji P, Tang MX et al. (2003). Literacy and
Zahodne LB, Schofield PW, Farrell MT et al. (2014). memory decline among ethnically diverse elders. J Clin
Bilingualism does not alter cognitive decline or dementia risk Exp Neuropsychol 25: 680–690.
among Spanish-speaking immigrants. Neuropsychology Mielke MM, Zandi PP, Sjogren M et al. (2005). High total
28: 238. cholesterol levels in late life associated with a reduced
risk of dementia. Neurology 64: 1689–1695.
FURTHER READING Ownby RL, Crocco E, Acevedo A et al. (2006). Depression and
risk for Alzheimer disease: systematic review, meta-analysis,
Atti AR, Palmer K, Volpato S et al. (2008). Late-life body mass and metaregression analysis. Arch Gen Psychiatry 63:
index and dementia incidence: nine-year follow-up data from 530–538.
the Kungsholmen Project. J Am Geriatr Soc 56: 111–116. Qiu C, Winblad B, Fratiglioni L (2005). The age-dependent
Barnes DE, Covinsky KE, Whitmer RA et al. (2009). relation of blood pressure to cognitive function and demen-
Predicting risk of dementia in older adults: the late-life tia. Lancet Neurol 4: 487–499.
dementia risk index. Neurology 73: 173–179. Qiu C, Xu W, Fratiglioni L (2010). Vascular and psychosocial
de Souto Barreto P, Demougeot L, Pillard F et al. (2015). factors in Alzheimer’s disease: epidemiological evidence
Exercise training for managing behavioral and toward intervention. J Alzheimers Dis 20: 689–697.

You might also like