You are on page 1of 10

Personality and Individual Differences 113 (2017) 209–218

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Personality and Individual Differences

journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid

Task conflict leads to relationship conflict when employees are low in


trait self-control: Implications for employee strain☆

Nerina L. Jimmieson a,⁎, Michelle K. Tucker a, Jack L. Campbell b


a
School of Management, Queensland University of Technology, Australia
b
School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, Australia

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: This research advances our theoretical understanding of interpersonal conflict in the workplace by examining the
Received 13 December 2016 process underpinning the escalation of task conflict to relationship conflict with one's supervisor and the moder-
Received in revised form 13 March 2017 ating effect of employee trait self-control (TSC) to predict employee strain. Study 1 with 160 employees from the
Accepted 14 March 2017
healthcare sector showed that the positive effect of task conflict on relationship conflict was significant for low
Available online 21 March 2017
TSC employees and non-existent for high TSC employees. Study 2 with 106 local council employees used a
Keywords:
time-ordered research design (time lag of 10 months) and replicated the TSC moderation effect in the context
Trait self-control of an indirect positive effect of task conflict (through relationship conflict) on psychological strain, job burnout,
Task conflict and turnover intentions, but not job dissatisfaction. Theoretical and practical implications are discussed in
Relationship conflict regards to the role of individual differences in TSC in supervisor-employee interpersonal relations.
Employee strain © 2017 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction (TSC). TSC can be viewed as a personal resource that can be drawn
upon to regulate responses to conflict, minimize conflict escalation,
One set of psychosocial risk factors in the workplace is stress arising and subsequent stress reactions. As such, we focus on the distinction be-
from interpersonal conflict, defined as a process in which an individual tween task conflict and relationship conflict with one's supervisor and
perceives that something he or she cares about, for instance interests examine the extent to which employee TSC helps to minimize escala-
and goals, are being blocked, opposed, or affected in a negative manner tion, thus having implications for employee strain.
by another (Wall & Callister, 1995). Interpersonal conflict in the work-
place has been categorized as a social stressor and is considered to be 1.1. Types of interpersonal conflict
a common and detrimental source of stress for employees in regards
to their physical and psychological functioning (De Dreu & Beersma, Interpersonal conflict may be either task-oriented or relationship-
2005; Spector & Bruk-Lee, 2008). Such a process sits within the stress- oriented in nature (Jehn, 1994, 1995). Task conflict is largely based on
or-strain framework underpinning many theories of stress, such as the non-relationship issues and pertains to disagreements regarding the
cognitive-phenomenological model of stress and coping proposed by work to be undertaken (Giebels & Janssen, 2005), and may involve dif-
Lazarus and Folkman (1984). ferences in views about policies and procedures, disputes regarding al-
In the context of the conflict escalation model that specifies that con- location and distribution of resources, or disagreements in judgments
flicts can strengthen and deepen into more extreme forms of conflict and interpretation of facts (De Dreu & Van de Vliert, 1997; De Dreu &
over time and one such reason being contentious reactions and behav- Weingart, 2003). Task conflict has sometimes been shown to be func-
iors from one of the parties encouraging or fostering those of the other tional for teams and organizations because it is thought to enable better
(Glasl, 1982), we draw on self-control theory (Carver & Scheier, 1981) decision-making as this type of conflict encourages greater cognitive
and investigate the moderating role of employees' trait self-control understanding of the issue being considered, and is thought to stimulate
critical-thinking and prevent premature consensus. For example, the
meta-analysis of 116 team studies by de Wit, Greer, and Jehn (2012)
☆ Author noteAn Australian Research Council Future Fellowship (FT110100083) showed that task conflict was good for team performance (but only
awarded to the first author funded this research. The funder had no involvement in any when process conflict and relationship conflict were controlled for). An-
parts of the study.
⁎ Corresponding author at: School of Management, Queensland University of
other meta-analysis of 45 team-level studies showed that task conflict
Technology, Brisbane, Queensland 4000, Australia. had positive implications for team performance (DeChurch, Mesmer-
E-mail address: n.jimmieson@qut.edu.au (N.L. Jimmieson). Magnus, & Doty, 2013).

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.03.035
0191-8869/© 2017 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
210 N.L. Jimmieson et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 113 (2017) 209–218

However, task conflict also can be an impediment to performance 1.3. Conflict escalation and employee strain
because it adds to cognitive load redirecting resources away from the
task at hand (Carnevale & Probst, 1998; van Woerkom & van Engen, Over and above performance implications, both task conflict and re-
2009). The 2003 meta-analysis by De Dreu and Weingart revealed lationship conflict can be upsetting and stressful to the recipient. First,
that task conflict had a negative relationship with performance, as did conflict brings obstruction in one's goal directed actions (Bruk-Lee &
the more recent 2013 meta-analysis by O'Neill, Allen, and Hastings Spector, 2012; Spector & Bruk-Lee, 2008) and this may trigger thoughts
(2013). Overall, the current understanding is that the extent to which and feelings that act as prerequisites for a stress response. Second, con-
task conflict is good for performance depends on a host of contingen- flict elicits negative emotions that can undermine one's sense of identi-
cies, including the presence of curvilinear effects, the extent to which re- fication and self-worth and self-esteem, and also similarities and social
lationship conflict also exists, and moderators that capture team belonging with others, and subsequently, be related to physiological
characteristics (trust and norms for conflict) and individual differences and psychological strain (De Dreu, van Dierendonck, & De
in demographics (education and skills levels) and traits, such as open- Best-Waldhober, 2003, Frone, 2000, Spector & Bruk-Lee, 2008). Indeed,
ness to experience (Loughry & Amason, 2014). multiple meta-analyses have demonstrated that relationship conflict
Relationship conflict, on the other hand, refers to conflict that stems has robust negative implications for physical health (Nixon, Mazzola,
from opposing or incompatible differences in interpersonal style/attri- Bauer, Krueger, & Spector, 2011) and attitudinal outcomes for em-
butes, preferences/personal taste, attitudes, and values (De Dreu & ployees and/or teams (De Dreu & Weingart, 2003; de Wit et al., 2012;
Van de Vliert, 1997; Pinkley, 1990; Yang & Mossholder, 2004). Relation- DeChurch et al., 2013).
ship conflict is thought to inhibit group processes and reduce the fre- Task conflict also has been associated with poor health outcomes
quency and quality of communications. Relationship conflict impairs such as psychological strain (Dijkstra, Beersma, & Cornelissen, 2012;
information processing as individuals spend more cognitive time on Dijkstra, Beersma, & Evers, 2011; Guerra, Martínez, Munduate, &
each other than on the task. As such, multiple meta-analyses have Medina, 2005, Sample 1; Sonnentag, Unger, & Nägel, 2013), as well as
found that relationship conflict impairs performance (DeChurch et al., negative attitudes such as job dissatisfaction (De Dreu & Weingart,
2013; De Dreu & Weingart, 2003; de Ent, Baumeister, & Tice, 2015; 2003; de Wit et al., 2012; Guerra et al., 2005, Sample 1). However,
O'Neill et al., 2013). other empirical research has shown that task conflict is good for team
attitudinal outcomes (DeChurch et al., 2013); whereas other studies
have shown that task conflict was not predictive of anger emotions
1.2. Task to relationship conflict escalation (Meier, Gross, Spector, & Semmer, 2013), psychological strain (Guerra
et al., 2005, Sample 2), emotional exhaustion (Giebels & Janssen,
One negative outcome of task conflict is relationship conflict. Task 2005), and job dissatisfaction (Guerra et al., 2005, Sample 2), once rela-
conflict can harm relationships (Jehn, 1997) and is therefore viewed tionship conflict was controlled for. Irrespective of relationship conflict,
as an antecedent to relationship conflict (Curseu & Schruijer, 2010). Giebels and Janssen showed that task conflict was unrelated to turnover
Such a proposal recognizes that interpersonal conflict is dynamic in intentions and absenteeism, and Meier et al. showed no association for
nature and escalates over time. According to Glasl's (1982) conflict somatic complaints. Some have speculated that, during task conflict,
escalation model, when conflict arises during the undertaking of a employees attribute disputes to external characteristics (e.g., situation
task, there is the potential for that conflict to increase in its duration or task) that are short-term and controllable which bypasses threats
and become intensified, with individuals' positions and differences to the self and the resultant negative emotional and stress reactions
becoming polarized as time goes on. As individuals experience (Bruk-Lee & Spector, 2012; De Dreu & Gelfand, 2008).
heightened negative reactions to conflict events, subsequent person- In response to these mixed results, it has been suggested that an in-
al issues can arise. direct relationship exists such that task conflict leads to relationship
As reviewed by Simons and Peterson (2000), task conflict escalates conflict that in turn has implications for employee strain. Two studies
into relationship conflict because of cognitive misattributions or the concluded this to be the case in the prediction of psychological strain
use of inappropriate emotional and behavioral reactions. First, some in a sample of 82 employees in a medical department (Friedman, Tidd,
people interpret disagreements about the task as personal attacks and Currall, & Tsai, 2000) and for psychological strain and job dissatisfaction
come to dislike people who present task-related criticism. The tendency in a sample of 169 Spanish hotel workers (Medina, Munduate, Dorado,
to internalize the cause of task conflict occurs as a result of individuals' Martínez, & Guerra, 2005). In both studies, task conflict did not have a
information processing biases and self-fulfilling prophecies (Fiske & direct effect on employee strain when relationship conflict was in the
Taylor, 1991). Over time, continued challenges to individuals' opinions model. Although these studies provided initial support for an indirect
may strengthen such biases and self-fulfilling prophecies, which lead in- effects model of task conflict on employee strain through relationship
dividuals to increasingly appraise their interpersonal interactions as conflict, both used a cross-sectional research design, limiting causal di-
threatening. Second, individuals debating a work task may intentionally rection of the hypothesized relationships. As such, we incorporate the
or unintentionally respond to each other in a harsh and aggressive man- element of time into the research design and hypothesize that:
ner. This may signal disrespect and dislike to the opposing individual,
triggering threat appraisals and negative emotions, allowing relation- Hypothesis 1. Task conflict at Time 1 will have a positive indirect effect
ship conflict to emerge. on employee strain at Time 2 through relationship conflict at Time 2.
The fact that task conflict and relationship conflict go hand in hand is
evidenced by a review of correlations by Simons and Peterson (2000)
and the later meta-analysis by de Wit et al. (2012) showed that task 1.4. Preventing conflict escalation
conflict is correlated with relationship conflict (0.54). However, it
should be noted that much of this research is cross-sectional. Some ex- Even though the above studies show that task conflict has the poten-
ceptions include the studies by Curseu and colleagues. Curseu and tial to increase relationship conflict, task conflict does not always lead to
Schruijer (2010, Study 2) showed that task conflict at the beginning of relationship conflict over time (Bono, Boles, Judge, & Lauver, 2002;
the semester predicted end-of-semester assessments of relationship Greer, Jehn, & Manniz, 2008; Guenter et al., 2016), leading to a search
conflict in student teams. In a multi-level analysis also with student for moderating variables. For example, Simons and Peterson (2000)
teams, Pluut and Curşeu (2013) observed a significant correlation be- found that, in top management teams, high pre-existing trust prevented
tween Time 1 individual-level task conflict and Time 2 group-level rela- escalation and the use of raised voices promoted escalation. Curseu and
tionship conflict, as reported 2 weeks later. Schruijer (2010, Study 1) also showed that trust weakened the positive
N.L. Jimmieson et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 113 (2017) 209–218 211

association between task conflict and relationship conflict in a sample of Increased interest in TSC was ignited by the meta-analysis finding
student teams reflecting on their semester together. Tidd, McIntyre, and (de Ridder et al., 2011) that TSC had a stronger impact on automatic be-
Friedman (2004) found that, when one's role was ambiguous, individ- haviors than conscious behaviors. It has since been shown that high TSC
uals were less inclined to interpret task conflict as relational because it individuals automatically and with little conscious intent or effort select
was considered to be arising from the work itself, an interactive effect the desired behavioral response that aligns with their long-term goals, a
that also was present at high rather than low trust. Bai, Han, and process thought to be a reflection of their well-formed habits (Galla &
Harms (2016) showed that political climate was the trigger for task to Duckworth, 2015; Gillebaart & de Ridder, 2015). Gillebaart and de
relationship escalation in teams. Longitudinal studies also have shown Ridder proposed that high TSC individuals rely on two habitual tenden-
support for moderators. Pluut and Curşeu (2013) found that Time 1 in- cies to maintain goal pursuit. First, high TSC individuals automatically
dividual-level task conflict predicted Time 2 group-level relationship avoid situations or experiences that elicit response conflict. Second, if
conflict when the team was low in the use of problem-focused coping high TSC individuals do experience response conflict, they automatically
strategies, whereas high problem-focused coping strategies reduced employ strategies to down-regulate the experience (Gillebaart & de
the relationship between conflict types over time. Guenter et al. showed Ridder, 2015). These habits ensure that goal pursuit is not impeded by
that task conflict in teams relates to growth in relationship conflict only changes in motivation, lapses of attention, stress, or effortful inhibition
when team performance was perceived as problematic. (Galla & Duckworth, 2015).
Yang and Mossholder (2004) argue in their model of decoupling task To date, no research has investigated the relationship between TSC
conflict and relationship conflict that collective emotional intelligence is and interpersonal conflict in the work context. Nevertheless, the
a core team competence that determines how the two conflict types op- meta-analysis by de Ridder et al. (2011) explored the relationship be-
erate together. Several studies have since found that the emotion man- tween TSC and a range of life outcomes and showed that TSC was a pos-
agement skills of the team are a moderator. Curseu, Boros, and itive predictor of interpersonal relationships and personal adjustment.
Oerlemans (2012) found that the positive association between task con- This conclusion also aligns with earlier research demonstrating that
flict and relationship conflict was less marked when emotion manage- high TSC is associated with better interpersonal relationships
ment was rated as high by students performing group activities, an (Tangney et al., 2004). Overall, such research suggests that TSC has an
effect more prominent for the long-term teams compared to the important role in mitigating the escalation of task conflict into relation-
short-term teams. van den Berg, Curseu, and Meeus (2014) showed ship conflict. During encounters of task conflict, high TSC employees
that the positive association between the two conflict types was no lon- should have responses that minimize or prevent inappropriate cogni-
ger present at high emotion management in an individual-level analysis tive, emotional, and behavioral responses to task conflict. These tenden-
of 94 employees across 23 IS-development teams. Accordingly, in con- cies ensure that the focus remains on task-oriented goal attainment and
sidering which individual traits determine an employee's ability to ac- minimizes the likelihood that task conflict will escalate into one about
cept task conflict with confidence and manage the emotions of the relationships. Therefore, we hypothesize that:
situation so that escalate to relationship conflict is minimized, we
focus on employees' TSC. Hypothesis 2. TSC will moderate the direct positive effect of task con-
flict at Time 1 on relationship conflict at Time 2, such that the effect
will be stronger for employees low in TSC and weaker for employees
1.5. Moderating role of trait self-control high in TSC.
Following on from this proposition, we seek to understand the ex-
Self-control is a core feature of the self and considered to be essential tent to which TSC also moderates the conflict escalation process in the
for achieving happiness and life success. It refers to how capable an in- prediction of employee strain. In this respect, individuals high in TSC
dividual is in overriding, interrupting, or changing thoughts, emotions, who appropriately manage task conflict and minimize conflict escala-
impulses, and behavioral tendencies and refraining from acting on tion to relationship conflict should be protected, or buffered, from the
them in situations that would otherwise interfere with desired out- negative effects of the stress-inducing consequences of relationship
comes (Tangney, Baumeister, & Boone, 2004). Individuals are regularly conflict. Based on this rationale, we hypothesize that:
exposed to self-control dilemmas and such dilemmas create unresolved
conflict. This conflict, also known as response conflict, is experienced as Hypothesis 3. TSC will act as a first-stage moderator of the positive in-
a result of competing urges and desires between short-term rewards direct effect of task conflict at Time 1 on employee strain at Time 2
and long-term goals. High TSC individuals tend to avoid or experience through relationship conflict at Time 2, such that the relationship will
less response conflict situations in their lives (Ent et al., 2015; be stronger for employees low in TSC and weaker for employees high
Hofmann, Baumeister, Forster, & Vohs, 2012; Hofmann, Luhmann, in TSC.
Fisher, Vohs, & Baumeister, 2014) and when they do, they are better
at managing, or balancing, their response to achieve an optimal out-
come (Hofmann et al., 2014).
Over the past 30 years, scholars have conducted an extensive 1.6. The present research
amount of research on self-control (see de Ridder, Lensvelt-Mulders,
Finkenauer, Stok, & Baumeister, 2011, for a meta-analysis), with Although task conflict has been heavily studied in groups and teams,
scholars taking both state- and trait-based views on self-control. In ac- it has not been extensively studied within vertical dyads. Thus, we con-
cord with the Limited Strength Model of Self-Control (Muraven & sider task conflict and its consequences in supervisor-subordinate
Baumeister, 2000), the state-based perspective considers the enactment dyads from a subordinate's perspective. This decision was influenced
of self-control to be a conscious process that draws on a limited resource by previous research (Frone, 2000; McMillan, Chen, Richard, & Bhuian,
that is effortful and fatiguing. This ego depletion is thought to compro- 2012; Xin & Pelled, 2003) that has highlighted the importance of the su-
mise continued capacity to engage in self-control over time. The trait- pervisor-subordinate relationship for employee adjustment, and called
based perspective considers self-control as a dispositional difference be- for further research into the dynamics of supervisor-subordinate con-
tween individuals. As reviewed by Olson (1995), TSC is a superordinate flict. Study 1 was intended to provide an initial test of the hypothesized
trait dimension that is underscored by, for example, dimensions of the task conflict × TSC interaction on relationship conflict in an employee
Big 5, such as restraint of uncooperative, hostile, and aggressive behav- sample and Study 2 aimed to replicate this interaction in the context
iors (Agreeableness), irresponsible, careless behaviors (Conscientious- of moderated mediation using a time-ordered research design, also
ness), and negative, distressing emotions (Emotional Stability). with an employee sample.
212 N.L. Jimmieson et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 113 (2017) 209–218

2. Study 1 Table 2
Study 1 hierarchical regression analysis (n = 160).

2.1. Procedure and participants Supervisor relationship conflict B (SE) at step in which entered

Step 1
An online survey was distributed to 1066 employees of an Australian Supervisor task conflict 0.40⁎⁎⁎ (0.05)
disability and aged care support service, and 225 were returned, giving a Trait self-control −0.30⁎⁎ (0.11)
response rate of 21.1%. Listwise deletion occurred at the item-level, R2 0.35⁎⁎⁎ [0.23; 0.44]
F 41.57⁎⁎⁎
prior to calculation of the composite scales. Thus, the final useable sam-
ple was 160 employees. This sample comprised 10.6% men and 88.1% Step 2
women (1.3% missing). The average age of participants was Supervisor task conflict × trait self-control −0.44⁎⁎⁎ (0.10)
ΔR2 due to two-way interaction term 0.07⁎⁎⁎ [0.03; 0.21]
44.85 years (SD = 11.05), ranging from 20 to 68 years. In terms of occu-
ΔF due to two-way interaction term 19.05⁎⁎⁎
pations, 85.6% were carers, 10.0% were professionals, and 3.1% were ad-
ministration/clerical workers (and 1.3% other). Employees had been B = unstandardized regression coefficient; SE = standard error; 95% confidence interval
for effect size estimate in square brackets.
working with their supervisor for an average of 2.31 years (SD = ⁎⁎ p b 0.01.
2.72), ranging from 1 month to 12.42 years. ⁎⁎⁎ p b 0.001.

2.2. Measures
conditional effect of supervisor task conflict on supervisor relationship
TSC was assessed with the 13-item Brief Self-Control Scale devel- conflict. When TSC was 0.362 or less, which was true for 75.6% of the
oped by Tangney et al. (2004). The scale is designed to capture both re- sample, the relationship between supervisor task conflict and supervi-
straint (e.g., “People would say that I have iron self-discipline”) and sor relationship conflict was positive and significant. When TSC was
impulse control (e.g., “I often act without thinking through all of the al- higher than 0.362 (24.4% of the sample), the effect of supervisor task
ternatives”). The rating scale ranged from (1) “not at all like me” to (5) conflict on supervisor relationship conflict was not significant. Overall,
“very much like me”. Nine items were reverse-scored so that higher the results supported Hypothesis 2 because the expected positive rela-
scores reflected higher TSC. Supervisor task (e.g., “Do you and your su- tionship between the two conflict types became weaker as a function
pervisor disagree about the work being done?”) and relationship (e.g., of TSC, becoming non-existent for employees high in TSC.
“Are personality conflicts evident between you and your supervisor?”)
conflict were each measured with four items adapted from Jehn
(1995), with supervisor as the referent. Responses options ranged 3. Study 2
from (1) “never” to (7) “always”. All scales had acceptable Cronbach
(1951) alpha coefficients (see Table 1). 3.1. Research design

2.3. Results The full model incorporating employee strain was tested using a
time-ordered research design with a time lag of 10 months. Supervisor
Table 1 presents the means, standard deviations, and correlations. A relationship conflict was measured at both time points in order to en-
hierarchical regression analysis was conducted using mean-centred sure baseline levels of supervisor relationship conflict could be
predictors (see Table 2). Sex, age, and supervisor tenure were not in- accounted for, allowing for a more accurate test of the predictive effect
cluded as covariates as these were not related to the focal variables of task conflict on relationship conflict with one's supervisor over time.
(see Table 1). Step 1 was significant, R2 = 0.35, p b 0.001, 95% CI [0.23, In addition, Time 1 employee strain was measured and results involving
0.44], with supervisor task conflict a significant positive predictor of su- the prediction of Time 2 employee strain were checked for robustness in
pervisor relationship conflict, B = 0.40, p b 0.001, and trait self-control a light of baseline levels.
significant negative predictor of supervisor relationship conflict, B =
−0.30, p b 0.01. At Step 2, the interaction between supervisor task con-
flict and TSC on supervisor relationship conflict was significant, B = 3.2. Procedure
− 0.44, p b 0.001, and the addition of this interaction term accounted
for a significant increment in variance, ΔR2 = 0.07, p b 0.001, 95% CI Data were from a sample of employees working for a local council in
[0.03, 0.21]. Australia that provides a range of facilities and services to ratepayers.
The interaction was probed with simple slopes and presented in Surveys were distributed electronically via an email that contained an
Fig. 1. The positive relationship between the two conflict types was sig- online link, and also in person as paper copies at employee training ses-
nificant at low TSC, b = 0.54, t = 9.22, p b 0.001, and medium TSC, b = sions and toolbox talks. Employees who received paper copies were
0.31, t = 5.76, p b 0.001, but not significant at high TSC, b = 0.08, t = provided with reply paid envelopes to post their survey back to the re-
0.93, p = 0.356 (see Fig. 1). The Johnson-Neyman Technique (Johnson searchers. Both methods of survey distribution were used at Time 1 and
& Fey, 1950) was used to estimate the regions of significance for the Time 2.

Table 1
Study 1 descriptive data and inter-correlations (n = 160).

Variables M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6

1. Sex – – –
2. Age (years) 44.85 11.05 0.04 –
3. Tenure with supervisor (years) 2.31 2.72 0.06 0.20⁎ –
4. Trait self-control 3.68 0.52 0.07 0.09 −0.09 (0.78)
5. Supervisor task conflict 1.82 1.10 0.01 −0.06 0.10 −0.26⁎⁎⁎ (0.93)
6. Supervisor relationship conflict 1.38 0.86 0.04 −0.08 −0.09 −0.31⁎⁎⁎ 0.56⁎⁎⁎ (0.96)

Cronbach's (1951) alpha coefficients for multi-item scales are in parentheses along main diagonal.
⁎ p b 0.05.
⁎⁎⁎ p b 0.001.
N.L. Jimmieson et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 113 (2017) 209–218 213

Fig. 1. Relationship between supervisor task conflict and supervisor relationship conflict at levels of trait self-control in Study 1.

3.3. Participants 3.4. Measures

At Time 1, 1047 surveys were distributed and 534 (51.0%) were TSC, supervisor task conflict, and supervisor relationship conflict
returned. Of these, 44 respondents did not complete the anonymous were assessed as per Study 1. Psychological distress was measured
identification code needed to match their responses with Time 2, thus using Goldberg's (1972) 12-item General Health Questionnaire (e.g.,
leaving 490 respondents with the potential for a Time 2 follow-up. At “Over the past 4 weeks, how often have you been feeling unhappy or de-
Time 2, 134 respondents could be matched with their Time 1 survey pressed?”), assessed on a 7-point scale from (1) “never” to (7) “always”.
using the anonymous identification code. t-tests revealed no significant Six of these items were reverse-scored. Job burnout was measured using
differences between the matched sample and the valid cases from Time Shirom and Melamed's (2006) 14-item scale (e.g., “I have no energy for
1 on the focal and demographic variables. going to the work in the morning”), assessed on a 7-point scale from (1)
Prior to finalizing the sample for analyses, 12 matched cases were “never or almost never” to (7) “always or almost always”. Job dissatis-
excluded because the respondents indicated that they had changed su- faction was measured using Warr's (1991) 3-item scale (e.g., “I am dis-
pervisor between Time 1 and Time 2, leaving 122 cases. Listwise dele- satisfied with my job”), assessed on a 7-point scale from (1) “strongly
tion occurred at the item-level, prior to calculation of the composite disagree” to (7) “strongly agree”. Turnover intentions were measured
scales. Thus, the final useable sample was 106 employees. using a single item asking employees if they seriously believe that, in
This sample comprised 42.5% men and 57.5% women. Mean age at the future that they would resign because of a stress-related problem,
Time 1 was 44.10 years (SD = 12.52), ranging from 20 to 71 years. Par- assessed on a 7-point scale from (1) “extremely unlikely” to (7) “ex-
ticipants held either full-time (92.5%) or part-time/casual (7.5%) posi- tremely likely”. All multi-item scales had acceptable Cronbach (1951)
tions. Employees held positions across a range of occupations, the alpha coefficients (see Table 3).
majority being clerical/administrative workers (50.0%), professionals
(23.6%), managers (13.2%), and technical, trades, or laborers (6.6%). A 3.5. Data analysis overview
small proportion held other job types (0.9%) and 5.7% did not indicate
their occupation. At Time 1, employees had been working with their su- Table 3 presents the means, standard deviations, and correlations.
pervisor for an average of 2.70 years (SD = 3.03), ranging from 1 month Given that sex, age, and supervisor tenure had no significant associa-
to 18 years. tions with the focal variables, they were not included as covariates in

Table 3
Study 2 descriptive data and inter-correlations (n = 106).

Variables M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11

1. Sex – – –
2. Age (years) 44.10 12.52 0.02 –
3. Tenure with supervisor (years) 2.70 3.03 −0.04 0.19 –
4. T1 trait self-control 3.58 0.52 0.01 0.07 0.06 (0.76)
5. T1 supervisor task conflict 2.34 1.10 −0.14 −0.07 0.05 −0.30⁎⁎ (0.95)
6. T1 supervisor relationship conflict 1.74 1.17 −0.14 −0.13 0.03 −0.25⁎ 0.80⁎⁎⁎ (0.98)
7. T2 supervisor relationship conflict 1.67 1.12 −0.04 −0.09 −0.09 −0.29⁎⁎ 0.58⁎⁎⁎ 0.61⁎⁎⁎ (0.98)
8. T2 psychological strain 2.42 0.95 0.01 −0.11 0.02 −0.27⁎⁎ 0.41⁎⁎⁎ 0.38⁎⁎⁎ 0.53⁎⁎⁎ (0.91)
9. T2 job burnout 2.82 1.22 −0.04 −0.08 0.08 −0.31⁎⁎⁎ 0.41⁎⁎⁎ 0.36⁎⁎⁎ 0.44⁎⁎⁎ 0.77⁎⁎⁎ (0.97)
10. T2 job dissatisfaction 2.60 1.37 −0.17 −0.07 −0.08 −0.08 0.33⁎⁎⁎ 0.23⁎ 0.30⁎⁎ 0.64⁎⁎⁎ 0.57⁎⁎⁎ (0.94)
11. T2 turnover intentions 2.08 1.71 −0.06 −0.07 −0.11 −0.22⁎ 0.46⁎⁎⁎ 0.41⁎⁎⁎ 0.56⁎⁎⁎ 0.68⁎⁎⁎ 0.52⁎⁎⁎ 0.61⁎⁎⁎ –

Cronbach's (1951) alpha coefficients for multi-item scales are in parentheses along main diagonal.
⁎ p b 0.05.
⁎⁎ p b 0.01.
⁎⁎⁎ p b 0.001.
214 N.L. Jimmieson et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 113 (2017) 209–218

Table 4
Simple mediation results for T1 supervisor task conflict on T2 employee strain through T2 supervisor relationship conflict (n = 106).

Model 1: T2 supervisor Model 2: T2 psychological Model 2: T2 job burnout Model 2: T2 job Model 2: T2 turnover
relationship conflict strain dissatisfaction intentions

Overall model summary R2 (MSE) F R2 (MSE) F R2 (MSE) F R2 (MSE) F R2 (MSE) F


0.40 (0.77) 33.78⁎⁎⁎ 0.29 (0.66) 13.99⁎⁎⁎ 0.23 (1.15) 10.05⁎⁎⁎ 0.14 (1.67) 5.30⁎⁎ 0.34 (1.98) 17.72⁎⁎⁎

Control variable B (SE) 95% CI B (SE) 95% CI B (SE) 95% CI B (SE) 95% CI B (SE) 95% CI
T1 supervisor relationship 0.38 (0.12)⁎⁎ 0.13; 0.62 −0.03 (0.12) −0.26; 0.21 −0.04 (0.16) −0.36; 0.27 −0.19 (0.19) −0.56; 0.19 −0.09 (0.21) −0.50; 0.32
conflict

Independent variables
T1 supervisor task conflict 0.28 (0.13)⁎ 0.02; 0.54 0.15 (0.12) −0.09; 0.40 0.30 (0.17) −0.03; 0.63 0.42 (0.20)⁎ 0.03; 0.81 0.37 (0.21) −0.06; 0.79
T2 supervisor relationship 0.38 (0.09)⁎⁎⁎ 0.19; 0.56 0.33 (0.12)⁎⁎ 0.09; 0.57 0.25 (0.15) −0.04; 0.54 0.71 (0.16)⁎⁎⁎ 0.39; 1.02
conflict

Indirect effect Effect (SE) 95% CI Effect (SE) 95% CI Effect (SE) 95% CI Effect (SE) 95% CI
0.10 (0.07) −0.00; 0.27 0.09 (0.07) −0.00; 0.27 0.07 (0.07) −0.01; 0.29 0.20 (0.15) −0.03; 0.60

MSE = mean squared error; B = unstandardized regression coefficient; SE = standard error; CI = confidence interval; 95% confidence interval is significant for results in bold.
⁎ p b 0.05.
⁎⁎ p b 0.01.
⁎⁎⁎ p b 0.001.

the subsequent analyses. Hayes' (2013) PROCESS macro for SPSS was 3.7. Simple moderation results
used to test the hypotheses (see Tables 4, 5, & 6). Simple mediation
analyses were conducted to test the indirect effects of Time 1 supervisor The results for the moderating role of TSC on conflict escalation are
task conflict on each of the Time 2 employee strain variables through presented in Table 5. In accord with Hypothesis 2, TSC moderated the
Time 2 supervisor relationship conflict (Hypothesis 1). The conditional relationship between Time 1 supervisor task conflict and Time 2 super-
effect of TSC on the relationship between the two supervisor conflict visor relationship conflict, B = − 0.41, p = 0.003, 95% CI [− 0.68,
types was assessed with simple moderation (Hypothesis 2). Last, the − 0.14], and accounted for a significant proportion of variance in the
Stage 1 moderated mediation effect of TSC on the indirect relationship overall model, ΔR2 = 0.05, p b 0.01, 95% CI [0.01, 0.20]. Simple slopes
between Time 1 supervisor task conflict and Time 2 employee strain analysis was conducted 1 SD below the mean, at the mean, and 1 SD
through Time 2 supervisor relationship conflict (Hypothesis 3) was above the mean. For those low in TSC, Time 1 supervisor task conflict
assessed. All analyses controlled for Time 1 supervisor relationship con- had a significant positive relationship with Time 2 supervisor relation-
flict. The analyses used 10,000 bootstrapping resamples to create an ap- ship conflict, b = 0.39, p = 0.005, noting that Time 1 supervisor rela-
proximation of the sampling distribution, and 95% confidence intervals tionship was controlled for. In contrast, supervisor task conflict at
(CIs) were used to determine significance. To preserve degrees of free- Time 1 did not lead to supervisor relationship conflict at Time 2 for
dom, baseline employee strain was not controlled for, but when results those with high, b = − 0.03, p = 0.836, and medium, b = 0.18, p =
change as a result of their inclusion, these have been footnoted. 0.162, levels of TSC. Overall, this finding replicates the interaction ob-
served in Study 1. The Johnson-Neyman Technique (Johnson & Fey,
1950) revealed that, when TSC was − 0.172 or lower, which was true
3.6. Simple mediation results for 38.7% of the sample, the relationship between Time 1 supervisor
task conflict and Time 2 supervisor relationship conflict was positive
3.6.1. Direct effects and significant. When TSC was higher than −0.172 (61.3% of the sam-
As shown in Table 4, although not hypothesized, Time 1 supervisor ple), the effect of Time 1 supervisor task conflict on Time 2 supervisor
task conflict had a significant positive direct effect on Time 2 job dissat- relationship conflict was not significant.
isfaction (B = 0.42, p = 0.034), but did not have a significant direct ef-
fect on the remaining Time 2 employee strain variables. As expected as
3.8. Moderated mediation results
part of Hypothesis 1, a significant positive direct effect was identified
between Time 1 supervisor task conflict and Time 2 supervisor relation-
Conditional indirect effects were examined to test for moderated
ship conflict, B = 0.28, p = 0.037. Time 2 supervisor relationship con-
mediation (Hypothesis 3). As shown in Table 6, Hayes' (2015) index
flict had a significant positive direct effect on three of the four Time 2
of moderated mediation (IMM) was significant for: psychological strain
employee strain variables, including psychological strain, B = 0.38, p
(IMM = −0.16, SE = 0.08, 95% CI [−0.32, −0.02]); job burnout (IMM
= 0.000, job burnout, B = 0.33, p = 0.008, and turnover intentions, B
= − 0.14, SE = 0.07, 95% CI [− 0.30, − 0.02]); turnover intention
= 0.71, p = 0.000.
(IMM = −0.29, SE = 0.16, 95% CI [−0.67, −0.03]). The IMM for job dis-
satisfaction was not significant (IMM = − 0.10, SE = 0.08, 95% CI
[−0.32, 0.00]).1 The significant positive indirect effects on psychological
3.6.2. Indirect effects
strain, job burnout, and turnover intentions then were examined at
As shown in Table 4, Hypothesis 1 was not supported, as Time 1 su-
three values of TSC: 1 SD below the mean, at the mean, and 1 SD
pervisor task conflict did not have a significant indirect effect through
above the mean. In each case, the positive indirect effect was significant
Time 2 supervisor relationship conflict on Time 2 employee strain: psy-
chological strain (indirect effect = 0.10, SE = 0.07, 95% CI [−0.00, 0.27]);
job burnout (indirect effect = 0.09, SE = 0.07, 95% CI [−0.00, 0.27]); job
dissatisfaction (indirect effect = 0.07, SE = 0.07, 95% CI [−0.01, 0.29]);
1
turnover intentions (indirect effect = 0.20, SE = 0.15, 95% CI [− 0.03, When controlling for the relevant Time 1 dependent variable, the IMM remained sig-
nificant for psychological strain (IMM = −0.15, SE = 0.08, 95% CI [−0.32, −0.02]), job
0.60]). Rather, as demonstrated below, the anticipated positive indirect burnout (IMM = −0.14, SE = 0.07, 95% CI [−0.31, − 0.01]), and turnover intentions
effect was contingent on employees' TSC (with the exception of job (IMM = −0.28, SE = 0.16, 95% CI [−0.64, −0.03]). The IMM for job dissatisfaction was
dissatisfaction). not significant (IMM = −0.13, SE = 0.09, 95% CI [−0.38, 0.00]).
N.L. Jimmieson et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 113 (2017) 209–218 215

Table 5 4.1. Theoretical and practical implications


Moderation of T1 trait self-control on the relationship between T1 supervisor task conflict
and T2 supervisor relationship conflict (n = 106).
These findings shed light on the mixed results seen in the inter-
T2 supervisor relationship conflict personal conflict literature about the effects of task conflict on em-
Overall model summary 95% CI ployee strain. Our findings suggest that supervisor task conflict
R2 (MSE) 0.46 (0.70)⁎⁎⁎ 0.30; 0.55 does not have a direct effect on employee strain but happens
F 21.50⁎⁎⁎ through supervisor relationship conflict but such a process is condi-
tional on an individual's level of TSC. Employees with high levels of
B (SE) 95% CI
Control variable TSC make use of self-regulation strategies that prevent misattribu-
T1 supervisor relationship conflict 0.34 (0.12)⁎⁎ 0.10; 0.57 tion and inappropriate responses to supervisor task conflict. This
stops supervisor task conflict from escalating into supervisor rela-
Independent variables
T1 supervisor task conflict 0.18 (0.13) −0.07; 0.44 tionship conflict and in turn associated detriments to their
T1 trait self-control −0.28 (0.17) −0.61; 0.05 adjustment.
T1 supervisor task conflict × trait self-control −0.41 (0.14)⁎⁎ −0.68; −0.14 It was originally hypothesized that conflict escalation would be
ΔR2 due to two-way interaction term 0.05⁎⁎ 0.01; 0.20 weaker for high TSC individuals but our results showed that they were
Conditional effects able to bypass conflict escalation altogether. The results were not sug-
−1 SD T1 trait self-control 0.39 (0.14)⁎⁎ 0.12; 0.67 gestive that high TSC individuals are sufficiently skilled to turn a high
Mean T1 trait self-control 0.18 (0.13) −0.07; 0.44 task conflict situation into lower levels of relationship conflict. Thus,
+1 SD T1 trait self-control −0.03 (0.16) −0.34; 0.28
an interesting avenue for future research would be to identify the per-
MSE = mean squared error; B = unstandardized regression coefficient; SE = standard sonal resources and skills that foster situations in which task conflict
error; CI = confidence interval; SD = standard deviation; 95% confidence interval is signif-
goes on to strengthen relationships.
icant for results in bold.
⁎⁎ p b 0.01. Results point to the importance of considering TSC in the stressor-
⁎⁎⁎ p b 0.001. strain process in a work context. First, the significant negative relation-
ships between TSC and both types of conflict are indicative of support
for employees with low TSC and not significant for high TSC and medi- for the exposure hypothesis; that individuals low in TSC actually create
um TSC.2 more interpersonal conflict for themselves. Such results are in line with
other trait studies showing that individuals high in negative affect per-
4. General discussion ceive more interpersonal stressors (Bolger & Zuckerman, 2005). Second,
it is interesting to note that TSC (Time 1) had prospective negative di-
This paper explored the mediating and moderating processes that rect effects on Time 2 psychological strain, job burnout, turnover inten-
underpin the relationship between interpersonal conflict types with tions, and job dissatisfaction, confirming it is not effortful or depleting in
one's supervisor, individual differences, and employee strain. First, the long-term to engage in such habitual tendencies. Such findings are
there was evidence of escalation in light of the fact that supervisor in line with Hofmann et al. (2014) who demonstrated that high TSC is
task conflict at Time 1 predicted supervisor relationship conflict at important for affective well-being and life satisfaction. Although TSC is
Time 2, controlling for Time 1 supervisor relationship conflict 10 months just one of many possible traits that could be examined in interpersonal
prior. As to be expected, supervisor relationship conflict at Time 2 was conflict situations, it is a construct that is well-suited to situations that
related to higher psychological strain, job burnout, and stress-related require one to exercise self-control over appropriate emotional, cogni-
turnover intentions at Time 2, but not job dissatisfaction, suggesting tive, and behavioral reactions in the work setting, one that is
that the implications are more salient for health outcomes than job atti- underscored by agreeableness, conscientiousness, and emotional stabil-
tudes. It is interesting to note that Time 1 supervisor task conflict was ity (Olson, 1995). Nevertheless, future research could explore more spe-
not conducive to Time 2 job satisfaction. Time 1 supervisor task conflict cific self-regulation strategies that are amenable to training and
was unrelated to the Time 2 health outcomes and nor did it have a sim- development such as those captured by emotional intelligence and its
ple indirect effect (through supervisor relationship conflict) on the em- sub-facets, such as emotion recognition and management in the self
ployee strain outcomes (Hypothesis 1). and others. Indeed, in a review of factors that facilitate successful re-
Rather, support was shown for Hypothesis 2, as employees' levels of sponses to workplace conflict, emotion regulation was pointed out as
TSC moderated the relationship between supervisor task conflict and an effective workplace skill to have in high-conflict situations (Oore,
supervisor relationship conflict across both studies. In Study 1, the pos- Leiter, & LeBlanc, 2015).
itive effect of supervisor task conflict on supervisor relationship conflict This research could be used to broaden the agenda of employee self-
was evident at low and medium levels of TSC and non-existent at high development programs. Although TSC is exercised in a habitual manner,
TSC. In Study 2, the same pattern was evident but the analyses also individuals also engage in acts of self-control in a conscious manner.
showed that escalation did not happen for those with medium TSC. Previous research has shown that self-control can be strengthened via
Hypothesis 3 was supported, as TSC moderated the positive indirect ef- conscious deliberation such as practice (Muraven, Baumeister, & Tice,
fects of supervisor task conflict (through supervisor relationship con- 1999) and implementation intentions (Webb & Sheeran, 2003) and
flict) on psychological strain, job burnout, and turnover intentions in also via unconscious deliberation such as high-level construal-depen-
the same fashion. dent evaluative associations (Fujita & Han, 1999). Incorporating self-
control development training into workplaces using these techniques
could help to reduce the escalation of interpersonal conflict to one
about relationships and enable effort to be maintained on the task.
Such training would be applicable to both supervisor-subordinate
2
When controlling for psychological strain at Time 1, the conditional indirect effect at dyads and also horizontal and team-based workplace relationships. In
low TSC was not significant (conditional indirect effect = 0.13, SE = 0.08, 95% CI [−0.00, particular, results from Study 2 suggest that an employee just needs to
0.32]) but the direction of the slopes was consistent. When controlling for job burnout be reasonably good at TSC in conflict situations with their supervisor
at Time 1, the conditional indirect effect at low TSC remained significant (conditional indi- to see benefits in terms of both relationship management and their
rect effect = 0.13, SE = 08, 95% CI [0.01, 0.34]). When controlling for turnover intentions at
Time 1, the conditional indirect effect at low TSC was not significant (conditional indirect
own health and well-being. Thus, workplace training that achieves a
effect = 0.25, SE = 0.18, 95% CI [− 0.03, 0.71]) but the direction of the slopes was modest improvement in employees' self-regulation skills will have
consistent. value.
216 N.L. Jimmieson et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 113 (2017) 209–218

Table 6
Moderated mediation results and conditional indirect effects (n = 106).

Model 1: T2 supervisor Model 2: T2 psychological Model 2: T2 job burnout Model 2: T2 job Model 2: T2 turnover
relationship conflict strain dissatisfaction intentions

Overall model R2 (MSE) F R2 (MSE) F R2 (MSE) F R2 (MSE) F R2 (MSE) F


summary 0.46 (0.70) 21.50⁎⁎⁎ 0.29 (0.66) 13.99⁎⁎⁎ 0.23 (1.18) 10.05⁎⁎⁎ 0.14 (1.67) 5.30⁎⁎ 0.34 (1.98) 17.72⁎⁎⁎

Control variable B (SE) 95% CI B (SE) 95% CI B (SE) 95% CI B (SE) 95% CI B (SE) 95% CI
T1 supervisor −0.34 (0.12)⁎⁎ 0.10; 0.57 −0.03 (0.12) −0.26; 0.21 −0.04 (0.16) −0.36; 0.27 −0.19 (0.19) −0.56; 0.19 −0.09 (0.21) −0.50; 0.32
relationship conflict

Independent variables
T1 supervisor task 0.18 (0.13) −0.07; 0.44 0.15 (0.12) −0.09; 0.40 0.30 (0.17) −0.03; 0.63 0.42 (0.20)⁎ 0.03; 0.81 0.37 (0.21) −0.06; 0.79
conflict (STC)
T1 trait self-control −0.28 (0.17) −0.61; 0.05 −0.28 (0.17) −0.61; 0.05 −0.28 (0.17) −0.61; 0.05 −0.28 (17) −0.61; 0.05 −0.28 (0.17) −0.61; 0.05
(TSC)
T1 STC × T1 TSC −0.41 (0.14)⁎⁎ −0.68; −0.14
T2 supervisor 0.38 (0.09)⁎⁎⁎ 0.19; 0.56 0.33 (0.12)⁎⁎ 0.09; 0.57 0.25 (0.15) −0.04; 0.54 0.71 (0.16)⁎⁎⁎ 0.39; 1.02
relationship conflict

Index of Moderated Effect (SE) 95% CI Effect (SE) 95% CI Effect (SE) 95% CI Effect (SE) 95% CI
Mediation −0.16 (0.08) −0.32; −0.02 −0.14 (0.07) −0.30; −0.02 −0.10 (0.08) −0.32; 0.00 −0.29 (0.16) −0.67; −0.03

Conditional indirect Effect (SE) 95% CI Effect (SE) 95% CI Effect (SE) 95% CI
effects
−1 SD T1 trait 0.15 (0.08) 0.01; 0.33 0.13 (0.08) 0.01; 0.33 0.28 (0.18) 0.01; 0.74
self-control
Mean T1 trait 0.07 (0.06) −0.04; 0.20 0.06 (0.05) −0.03; 0.19 0.13 (0.12) −0.07; 0.43
self-control
+1 SD T1 trait −0.01 (0.06) −0.15; 0.09 −0.01 (0.05) −0.15; 0.07 −0.02 (0.11) −0.28; 0.17
self-control

MSE = mean squared error; B = unstandardized regression coefficient; SE = standard error; CI = confidence interval; SD = standard deviation; 95% confidence interval is significant for
results in bold.
⁎ p b 0.05.
⁎⁎ p b 0.01.
⁎⁎⁎ p b 0.001.

4.2. Limitations and future research evidence shows that even though relationship conflict is lower in prev-
alence than task conflict, as one comparison, it has much stronger and
Several methodological limitations should be acknowledged. Inter- more consistent negative implications for employee positive affect and
personal conflict between an employee and their supervisor was mea- job attitudes (de Wit et al., 2012). Importantly, the interaction was rep-
sured via self-report, thus assessing the employee's perspective only. licated across two different samples of employees, affording greater
However, even if these ratings do not reflect supervisors' experiences confidence in the results and their significance and meaningfulness
of conflict, they provide insight into employees' perceptions of the con- (Kirk, 1996; Thompson, 1997).
flict and their associated stress reactions. Nevertheless, future research The research design was limited to a 2-wave design that precluded
should make use of 360-degree designs to capture a multi-source per- the testing of true longitudinal effects and also conflict cycles in which
spective of the conflict situation. Such a design would acknowledge it is possible that relationship conflict goes on to trigger retaliatory be-
that the supervisor is not passive in the process. Indeed, Bono et al. haviors that are task-oriented in nature (Jehn, Chadwick, & Thatcher,
(2002) found that the Big 5 traits of the dyad partner (in this case, room- 1997). Such an idea is in line with the sabotage hypothesis in which re-
mates living in residential halls) influenced the actor's experience of re- lationship conflict motivates people to undermine other's task perfor-
lationship conflict. For example, roommates of extroverted and mance, although considered to be much less common as people tend
conscientious individuals reported more relationship conflict. In addi- to know when task conflict is relational in origin (Jehn, 1997). It also
tion, results depended on the mean level within a pair and also differ- is important to acknowledge that it is possible that the two types of con-
ence effects. In the work context, Camps, Stouten, and Euwema flict may co-exist with no real causal ordering. As such, future research
(2016) showed that supervisors' reported level of conscientiousness should consider shorter and more frequent time intervals than the 10-
predicted employees' experience of abusive supervision. month one used here in order to better tease apart their relationship
There are several methodological limitations that need to be ac- and unpack dynamic processes.
knowledged, including the small sample size, potential for sampling Future research should test these hypotheses in the context of con-
bias due to low response rates, and the fact that the mean values for su- flict within teams and colleagues. Indeed, much of the literature on
pervisor relationship conflict were low in both studies, suggesting it was task conflict and relationship conflict has been from this perspective.
not an overly prevalent stressor for employees, all of which limit statis- Nevertheless, the focus on conflict between supervisors and subordi-
tical power but also might lead to spurious findings. It is important to nates in the present research represents an important contribution in
acknowledge that the effect size estimates for the supervisor task con- light of research in the colleague context that has identified low-level
flict x trait self-control interaction on relationship conflict (ΔR2 = 0.07 conflicts as a precursor to more extreme forms of relationship conflict.
in Study 1; ΔR2 = 0.05 in Study 2) were small but nevertheless did For instance, Baillen et al. (2016) showed that relationship conflict me-
reach Ferguson's (2009) recommended minimum effect size diated the association between task conflict and being a target of bully-
representing a practically significant effect for social science data of ing. By better understanding the process that underpins early forms of
0.04. It is argued that the detection of even a small increase in supervisor interpersonal conflict in vertical dyads, it is possible to identify avenues
relationship conflict as a function of high supervisor task conflict and through which such conflict can be defused or avoided before becoming
low TSC is a valuable finding, given that exposure to interpersonal a form of abusive supervision and significantly threatening the health
forms of conflict is especially detrimental to employees. Meta-analytic and well-being of employees.
N.L. Jimmieson et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 113 (2017) 209–218 217

Last, it would be interesting to explore the extent to which TSC is the Handbook of work and health psychology (pp. 495–516). Chichester, UK: John Wiley
& Sons.
mechanism behind the use of adaptive conflict handling management DeChurch, L. A., Mesmer-Magnus, J. R., & Doty, D. (2013). Moving beyond relationship and
styles and strategies such as collaboration and problem-solving. Indeed, task conflict: Toward a process-state perspective. Journal of Applied Psychology, 98,
as noted by De Dreu and Beersma (2005), conflict management is not 559–578. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0032896.
Dijkstra, M. T. M., Beersma, B., & Evers, A. (2011). Reducing conflict-related employee
simply a personality characteristic. Dijkstra et al. (2011) found that in- strain: The benefits of an internal locus of control and a problem-solving conflict
ternal locus of control explained the moderating effect of problem-solv- management strategy. Work and Stress, 25, 167–184. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
ing on the relationship between task/relationship conflict and 02678373.2011.593344.
Dijkstra, M. T. M., Beersma, B., & Cornelissen, R. A. W. M. (2012). The emergence of the
psychological strain. Dijkstra et al. (2012) found a similar pattern of re- Activity Reduces Conflict Associated Strain (ARCAS) model: A test of a conditional
sults for organization-based self-esteem as a predictor of the stress- mediation model of workplace conflict and employee strain. Journal of Occupational
buffering effect of active conflict handling strategies in the context of Health Psychology, 17, 365–375. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0028550.
Ent, M. R., Baumeister, R. F., & Tice, D. M. (2015). Trait self-control and the avoidance of
task conflict and psychological strain. As such, further research incorpo-
temptation. Personality and Individual Differences, 74, 12–15. http://dx.doi.org/10.
rating both personality characteristics and conflict handling styles in the 1016/j.paid.2014.09.031.
context of task to relationship conflict escalation is needed. Ferguson, C. J. (2009). An effect size primer: A guide for clinicians and researchers.
Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 40, 532–538. http://dx.doi.org/10.
1037/a0015808.
4.3. Conclusion Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. (1991). Social cognition. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Friedman, R. A., Tidd, S. T., Currall, S. C., & Tsai, J. C. (2000). What goes around comes
around: The impact of personal conflict style on work conflict and stress.
In conclusion, this research advances our theoretical understanding International Journal of Conflict Management, 11, 32–55. http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/
of interpersonal conflict in the workplace by examining the process un- eb022834.
derpinning the escalation of task conflict to relationship conflict with Frone, M. R. (2000). Interpersonal conflict at work and psychological outcomes: Testing a
model among young workers. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 5, 246–255.
one's supervisor and the moderating effect of TSC to predict employee http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1076-8998.5.2.246.
strain. Results showed that high and even medium levels of TSC are pro- Fujita, K., & Han, H. A. (1999). Moving beyond deliberative control of impulses: The effect
tective in conflict situations with one's supervisor, and provides new av- of construal levels on evaluative associations in self-control conflicts. Psychological
Science, 20, 799–804. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02372.x.
enues to continue the examination of TSC in task to relationship conflict Galla, B. M., & Duckworth, A. L. (2015). More than resisting temptation: Beneficial habits
escalation in the broader context of occupational stress. mediate the relationship between self-control and positive life outcomes. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 109, 508–525. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
pspp0000026.
References Giebels, E., & Janssen, O. (2005). Conflict stress and reduced well-being at work: The buff-
ering effect of third-party help. European Journal of Work and Organizational
Bai, Y., Han, G. H., & Harms, P. D. (2016). Team conflict mediates the effects of organiza- Psychology, 14, 137–155. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13594320444000236.
tional politics on employee performance: A cross-level analysis in China. Journal of Gillebaart, M., & de Ridder, D. T. D. (2015). Effortless self-control: A novel perspective on
Business Ethics, 139, 95–109. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10551-105-2604-6. response conflict strategies in trait self-control. Social and Personality Psychology
Baillen, E., Camps, J., Van den Broeck, A., Stouten, J., Godderis, L., Sercu, M., & de Whitte, H. Compass, 9, 88–99. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12160.
(2016). An eye for an eye will make the whole world blind: Conflict escalation into Glasl, F. (1982). The process of conflict escalation and role of third parties. In G. B. J.
workplace bullying and the role of distributive conflict behavior. Journal of Business Bomers, & R. B. Peterson (Eds.), Conflict management and industrial relations
Ethics, 137, 415–429. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10551-105-2563-y. (pp. 119–140). Boston: Kluwer-Nijhoff.
van den Berg, W., Curseu, P. L., & Meeus, M. T. H. (2014). Emotion regulation and conflict Goldberg, D. P. (1972). The detection of psychiatric illness by questionnaire. London: Oxford
transformation in multi-team systems. International Journal of Conflict Management, University Press.
25, 171–188. http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/JCMA-05-2012-0038. Greer, L. L., Jehn, K. A., & Manniz, E. A. (2008). Conflict transformation: A longitudinal in-
Bolger, N., & Zuckerman, A. (2005). A framework for studying personality in the stress vestigation of the relationships between different types of intragroup conflict and the
process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 890–902. http://dx.doi.org/ moderating role of conflict resolution. Small Group Research, 39, 278–302. http://dx.
10.1037/0022-3514.69.5.890. doi.org/10.1177/1046496408317793.
Bono, J. E., Boles, T. L., Judge, T. A., & Lauver, K. J. (2002). The role of personality in task and Guenter, H., van Emmerik, H., Schreurs, B., Kuypers, T., van Iterson, A., & Notelaers, G.
relationship conflict. Journal of Personality, 70, 311–344. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/ (2016). When task conflict becomes personal: The impact of perceived task perfor-
1467-6494.05007. mance. Small Group Research, 47, 569–604. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
Bruk-Lee, V., & Spector, P. E. (2012). Interpersonal conflict and stress at work. In A. Rossi, 1046496416667816.
P. Perrewe, & J. Meurs (Eds.), Coping and prevention (pp. 3–22). Charlotte, NC: Infor- Guerra, J. M., Martínez, I., Munduate, L., & Medina, F. J. (2005). A contingency perspective
mation Age Publishing. on the study of the consequences of conflict types: The role of organizational culture.
Camps, J., Stouten, J., & Euwema, M. (2016). The relation between supervisors' Big Five European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 14, 157–176. http://dx.doi.
personality traits and employees' experience of abusive supervision. Frontiers in org/10.1080/13594320444000245.
Psychology, 7, 1–11. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00112. Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis:
Carnevale, P. J., & Probst, T. (1998). Social values and social conflict in creative problem A regression-based approach. New York: Guilford Press.
solving and categorization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, Hayes, A. F. (2015). An index and test of moderated mediation. Multivariate Behavioral
1300–1309. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.5.1300. Research, 50, 1–22. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00273171.2014.962683.
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1981). Attention and self-regulation: A control-theory ap- Hofmann, W., Baumeister, R. F., Forster, G., & Vohs, K. D. (2012). Everyday temptations:
proach to human behavior. New York: Springer-Verlag. An experience sampling study of desire, conflict, and self-control. Journal of
Cronbach, L. J. (1951). Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests. Psychometrika, Personality and Social Psychology, 102, 1318–1335. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
16, 297–334. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/bf02310555. a0026545.
Curseu, P. L., & Schruijer, S. G. L. (2010). Does conflict shatter trust or does trust obliterate Hofmann, W., Luhmann, M., Fisher, R. R., Vohs, K. D., & Baumeister, R. F. (2014). Yes, but
conflict?: Revisiting the relationships between team diversity, conflict, and trust. are they happy?: Effects of trait self-control on affective well-being and life satisfac-
Group Dynamics: Theory, Research and Practice, 14, 66–79. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ tion. Journal of Personality, 82, 265–277. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12050.
a0017104. Jehn, K. A. (1994). Enhancing effectiveness: An investigation of advantages and disadvan-
Curseu, P. L., Boros, S., & Oerlemans, L. A. G. (2012). Task and relationship conflict in short- tages of value-based intragroup conflict. International Journal of Conflict Management,
term and long-term groups: The critical role of emotion regulation. International 5, 223–238. http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/eb022744.
Journal of Conflict Management, 23, 97–107. http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/ Jehn, K. A. (1995). A multimethod examination of the benefits and detriments of
10444061211199331. intragroup conflict. Administrative Science Quarterly, 40, 256–282. http://dx.doi.org/
De Dreu, C. K. W., & Beersma, B. (2005). Conflict in organizations: Beyond effectiveness 10.1177/1558689810380920.
and performance. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 14, Jehn, K. A. (1997). Qualitative analysis of conflict types and dimensions in organizational
105–117. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13594320444000227. groups. Administrative Science Quarterly, 42, 530–557. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/
De Dreu, C. K. W., & de Vliert, Van (1997). Using conflict in organizations. London: Sage. 2393737.
De Dreu, C. K. W., & Gelfand, M. J. (2008). Conflict in the workplace: Sources, functions, Jehn, K. A., Chadwick, C., & Thatcher, S. M. B. (1997). To agree or not to agree: The effects
and dynamics across multiple levels of analysis. In C. K. W. De Dreu, & M. J. Gelfand of value congruence, individual demographic dissimilarity, and conflict on
(Eds.), The psychology of conflict and conflict management in organizations workgroup outcomes. International Journal of Conflict Management, 8, 287–305.
(pp. 3–54). New York: Taylor & Francis Group/Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/eb022799.
De Dreu, C. K. W., & Weingart, L. R. (2003). Task versus relationship conflict, team perfor- Johnson, P. O., & Fey, L. C. (1950). The Johnson-Neyman Technique: Its theory and appli-
mance, and team member satisfaction: A meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, cation. Psychometrika, 15, 349–367. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF02288864.
88, 741–749. http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/APBPP.2002.7516590. Kirk, R. E. (1996). Practical significance: A concept whose time has come.
De Dreu, C. K. W., van Dierendonck, D., & De Best-Waldhober, M. (2003). Conflict at work Educational and Psychological Measurement, 56, 746–749. http://dx.doi.org/10.
and individual wellbeing. In M. Schabracq, J. A. M. Winnubst, & C. L. Cooper (Eds.), 1177/0013164496056005002.
218 N.L. Jimmieson et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 113 (2017) 209–218

Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York: Springer. Shirom, A., & Melamed, S. (2006). A comparison of the construct validity of two burnout
Loughry, M. L., & Amason, A. C. (2014). Why won't task conflict cooperate?: Deciphering measures in two groups of professionals. International Journal of Stress Management,
stubborn results. International Journal of Conflict Management, 25, 333–358. http://dx. 13, 176–200. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1072-5245.13.2.176.
doi.org/10.1108/IJCMA-01-2014-0005. Simons, T. L., & Peterson, R. S. (2000). Task conflict and relationship conflict in top man-
McMillan, A., Chen, H., Richard, O. C., & Bhuian, S. N. (2012). A mediation model of task agement teams: The pivotal role of intragroup trust. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85,
conflict in vertical dyads: Linking organizational culture, subordinate values, and sub- 102–111. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0021-9010.85.1.102.
ordinate outcomes. International Journal of Conflict Management, 23, 307–332. http:// Sonnentag, S., Unger, D., & Nägel, I. J. (2013). Workplace conflict and employee well-
dx.doi.org/10.1108/10444061211248994. being: The moderating role of detachment from work during off-job time.
Medina, F. J., Munduate, L., Dorado, M. A., Martínez, I., & Guerra, J. M. (2005). Types of International Journal of Conflict Management, 24, 166–183. http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/
intragroup conflict and affective reactions. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 20, 10444061311316780.
219–230. http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/02683940510589019. Spector, P. E., & Bruk-Lee, V. (2008). Conflict, health, and well-being. In C. K. W. De Dreu, &
Meier, L. L., Gross, S., Spector, P. E., & Semmer, N. K. (2013). Relationship and task conflict M. J. Gelfand (Eds.), The psychology of conflict and conflict management in organizations
at work: Interactive short-term effects on angry mood and somatic complaints. (pp. 267–288). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 18, 144–156. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ Tangney, J. P., Baumeister, R. F., & Boone, A. L. (2004). High self-control predicts good ad-
a0032090. justment, less pathology, better grades, and interpersonal success. Journal of
Muraven, M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Self-regulation and depletion of limited re- Personality, 72, 271–324. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00263.x.
sources: Does self-control resemble a muscle? Psychological Bulletin, 126, 247–259. Thompson, B. (1997). Editorial policies regarding statistical significance tests: Further
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.126.2.247. comments. Educational Researcher, 26, 29–32. http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/
Muraven, M., Baumeister, R. F., & Tice, D. M. (1999). Longitudinal improvement of self- 0013189X026005029.
regulation through practice: Building self-control strength through repeated exercise. Tidd, S. T., McIntyre, H. H., & Friedman, R. A. (2004). The importance of role ambiguity and
Journal of Social Psychology, 139, 446–457. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/ trust in conflict perception: Unpacking the task conflict to relationship conflict link-
00224549909598404. age. International Journal of Conflict Management, 15, 364–380. http://dx.doi.org/10.
Nixon, A. E., Mazzola, J. J., Bauer, J., Krueger, J. R., & Spector, P. E. (2011). Can work make 1108/eb022918.
you sick?: A meta-analysis of the relationships between job stressors and physical Wall, J., & Callister, R. (1995). Conflicts and its management. Journal of Management, 21,
symptoms. Work and Stress, 25, 1–22. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02678373.2011. 515–538. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/014920639502100306.
569175. Warr, P. B. (1991). Mental health, well-being, and job satisfaction. In B. Hesketh, & A.
Olson, K. R. (1995). Engagement and self-control: Superordinate dimensions of Big Five Adams (Eds.), Psychological perspectives on occupational health and rehabilitation
traits. Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 1689–1700. http://dx.doi.org/10. (pp. 143–165). London: Harcourt Brace.
1016/j.paid.2004.11003. Webb, T. L., & Sheeran, P. (2003). Can implementation intentions help to overcome ego-
O'Neill, T. A., Allen, N. J., & Hastings, S. E. (2013). Examining the “pros” and “cons” of team depletion? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 279–286. http://dx.doi.org/
conflict: A team-level meta-analysis of task, relationship, and process conflict. Human 10.1016/S0022-1031(02)00527-9.
Performance, 26, 236–260. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08959285.2013.795573. de Wit, F. R. C., Greer, L. L., & Jehn, K. A. (2012). The paradox of intragroup conflict: A
Oore, D. G., Leiter, M. P., & LeBlanc, D. E. (2015). Individual and organizational factors pro- meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 97, 360–390. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
moting successful responses to workplace conflict. Canadian Psychology, 56, 301–310. a0024844.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/cap0000032. van Woerkom, M., & van Engen, M. L. (2009). Learning from conflicts?: The relations be-
Pinkley, R. L. (1990). Dimensions of conflict frame: Disputant interpretations of conflict. tween task and relationship conflicts, team learning, and task performance. European
Journal of Applied Psychology, 75, 117–126. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/256687. Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 18, 381–404. http://dx.doi.org/10.
Pluut, H., & Curşeu, P. L. (2013). Perceptions of intragroup conflict: The effect of coping 1080/13594320802569514.
strategies on conflict transformation and escalation. Group Processes & Intergroup Xin, K. R., & Pelled, L. H. (2003). Supervisor-subordinate conflict and perceptions of lead-
Relations, 16, 412–425. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1368430212453633. ership behavior: A field study. The Leadership Quarterly, 14, 25–40. http://dx.doi.org/
de Ridder, D. T. D., Lensvelt-Mulders, G., Finkenauer, C., Stok, F. M., & Baumeister, R. F. 10.1016/S1048-9843(02)00185-6.
(2011). Taking stock of self-control: A meta-analysis of how trait self-control relates Yang, J. X., & Mossholder, K. W. (2004). Decoupling task and relationship conflict: The role
to a wide range of behaviors. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 16, 76–99. of intra-group emotional processing. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 25, 589–605.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1088868311418749. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/job.258.

You might also like