You are on page 1of 10

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/261950190

Pomiankowski A, Iwasa Y, Nee S. The evolution of costly mate preferences. I.


Fisher and biased mutation. Evolution 45: 1422-1430

Article  in  Evolution · September 1991


DOI: 10.2307/2409889

CITATIONS READS

344 472

3 authors, including:

Andrew Pomiankowski Yoh Iwasa


University College London Kyushu University
240 PUBLICATIONS   11,068 CITATIONS    446 PUBLICATIONS   21,500 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Female Sneak Copulation View project

evolution of cooperation View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Andrew Pomiankowski on 31 October 2017.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Evolution,45(6), 1991, pp. 1422-1430

THE EVOLUTION OF COSTLY MATE PREFERENCES


I. FISHER AND BIASED MUTATION

ANDIREwPOMIANKOWSKII,
ABRG, Departmentof Zoology, Oxford,OX] 3PS, UK
YOH IWASA,
Departmentof Biology,Faculty of Science, Kyushu University,
Fukuoka 812, JAPAN
AND

SEAN NEE
AFRC UnitofEcology and Behaviour,Departmentof Zoology, Oxford,OX] 3PS, UK

Abstract.-Fisher's runawayprocessis thestandardexplanation of theevolutionofexaggerated


femalepreferences. Butmathematical formulationsofFisher'sprocess(haploidand additivedip-
loid) showit cannotcause stableexaggeration
iffemalepreferencecarriesa cost.At equilibrium
femalefitness mustbe maximized.Our analysisshowsthatevolutionary stableexaggerationof
femalepreference can be achievedifmutationpressureon themalecharacter is biased,thatis,
mutation has a directional
effect.
Atthisequilibriumfemalefitness
is notmaximized. We discuss
thereasonsand evidenceforbelievingthatmutationpressureis typically biased.Our analysis
highlightsthepreviously unacknowledgedimportance ofbiasedmutation forsexualselection.
Keywords.
-Mate choice,mutation
pressure,
runaway
process,sexualcharacters,
sexyson.

ReceivedSeptember
24, 1990. AcceptedFebruary
25, 1991.
Male matingbehavior ofteninvolves the genesfortheiroffspring; in the second view,
display of extravagantornaments accom- theyare lookingforgood viabilitygenes.
panied by loud and complex songs and the A keyquestionto pose is underwhatcon-
releaseofvolatile secretions.To understand ditions will these forceslead to the estab-
the evolution of these exaggeratedsecond- lishmentof costlysexual preferences.This
arysexual characterswe need to explainhow is a crucial question not only because there
femalesdevelop sexual preferencesforsuch is much evidence that there are costs as-
traits.Two theorieshave been proposed for sociated with mate choice (Pomiankowski,
cases where the benefitsof preferenceare 1987) but because in previousstudiesit has
heritable.The first, originatingwithDarwin been shownthatchoice costsdistinguishbe-
(1871) and Fisher(1915, 1930), suggeststhat tween the two theoriesof sexual selection.
theadvantageofpreference lies in thechoice All currentgeneralformulationsof Fisher's
of mates who will fatherattractivesons. If process show that female fitnessmust be
femaleson average prefera particularmale maximized at a stable evolutionaryequilib-
trait,thenmales withthattraithave a mat- rium.Fisher's processcannot cause the sta-
ing advantage and, if the traitis heritable, ble evolution of costly female preferences.
femaleswith the preferencehave male off- This is true whetherthe costs incurredin
springsimilarlyadorned who also enjoy a matechoice affectfemaleviability(Pomian-
matingadvantage. The second view, force- kowski, 1987; Bulmer, 1989b) or fecundity
fully advocated by Zahavi (1975) in the (Kirkpatrick,1985). In contrast,models of
"handicap" principle,arguesthattheprime the "handicap" principlehave shown that
benefitof choice lies in increased offspring it can lead to the evolution of costlyfemale
survival.In thisview, male ornamentspro- preferences (Andersson,1986; Pomiankow-
vide thefemalewithinformationabout her- ski, 1987). Raised offspringviability can
itable male quality. In the firstview, then, compensate forchoice costs, so that at the
femalesare looking forgood attractiveness evolutionaryequilibrium female fitnessis
not maximized.
' Presentaddress:Departmentof Geneticsand Bi- This paper setsout to investigatetheevo-
ometry,University CollegeLondon,London NW1 lution of costly female mate choice. We
2ME, UK. presentquantitativegeneticmodels of both

1422

This content downloaded from 128.40.80.65 on Wed, 25 Feb 2015 10:27:45 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
FISHER AND BIASED MUTATION 1423

Fisher's process and in the companion pa- beingexpressedonlyin femalesand thecho-


per (Iwasa et al., 1991) of the "handicap" sen ornamenttrait(t) only in males. The
principleof sexual selection.First,we con- fitnessof a male, Wm(t;t; p), is a function
firmthatin the simplestmodels of Fisher's of both his individualphenotype(t) and the
process, costlyfemale preferencesmay in- populationmean values of the male trait(t)
crease temporarilyby runawaybut at equi- and female preference(p)-separation of
libriumtheyare alwayseliminated.We then these differentvariables being indicatedby
show that this qualitative resultis not ge- a semi-colon.A female'sfitnessis unaffect-
neric; it disappears when slightmodifica- ed by the distributionof male phenotypes
tions are made to the model. In particular, and depends solely on her individual phe-
we show that if mutationhas a directional notype.These fitnessfunctionsmeasurethe
bias on the male trait then costly female average survivalto thebreedingseason and
preferences are present at evolutionary mating success of a given male or female
equilibrium.Reasons whymutationbias is phenotyperelativeto others.The effectof
particularlystrongin exaggeratedand elab- selectionis calculated by takingpartialde-
orate ornamentaltraitsare explored in the rivatives of the log fitnessfunctionswith
discussion. respectto theindividualtraits,evaluated at
the population means, t = t and p = p.
Two TRAIT MODEL G,and Gpare thevariancesoftheadditive
Consider a population withtwo sex-lim- geneticvalues of the male traitt and female
ited quantitativetraits,a male matingor- preferencep. Btpis the covariance of the
namentt and a femalematingpreference p. additivegeneticvalues of thetwo. In sexual
Both traits have an autosomal, polygenic selectionthegeneticcovariancemainlyaris-
basis. The phenotypicvalue of each traitis es fromnonrandommating.We simplyas-
statisticallydecomposed into an additive sume that the values of Gt, Gp and Bp are
geneticcomponentand an "error"termdue positive constants. The covariance BP is
to environmentalvariationand dominance positive because femaleswithlargervalues
(Bulmer,1980). As our goal is to make qual- of p tend to mate with males with larger
itative points as clearlyand easily as pos- values of t. Althoughthese geneticparam-
sible, we have chosen to model only the etersmaychangewithtimedue to selection,
evolutionoftheadditivegeneticeffects. This mutation,and recombination,we make a
we do using a novel, computationallysim- constancyassumptionbecause as yet there
ple, quantitativegenetic formulation.The is no tractablemethod of calculatingthese
main conditionrequiredis weak selection; parametersforthequestionswe wantto ad-
thatis, thefitnessfunctionschangelittleover dress (see, Barton and Turelli, 1987; Bul-
the range of phenotypicvariation in the mer, 1989a). It is unlikelythatour conclu-
population. A fulljustificationof the tech- sions willbe compromisedby morerealistic
nique is givenin thecompanionpaper (Iwa- models when these become available. This
saetal., 1991). view is supportedby similar resultsbeing
The evolution of the mean values of two found in major gene models of sexual se-
sex-limitedtraitsin a population with dis- lection,in which the variance and covari-
crete, nonoverlappinggenerationswill be ance termsare freeto evolve.
modeled as the productof the additive ge- UnderlyingEquation (1) and similarfor-
netic variance-covariancematrix and the mulationsis an importantassumptionthat
selectionvector, has rarelybeen emphasized.Mutationcaus-
es a shiftin the additive geneticvalues of
a In W,,(t; t,p theloci contributing to each trait.However,
(AtA_ 1 Gt Btp At upward and downward changes are as-
kAp! 2kBtP Gp a in Wf(p) sumed to occur equally frequently and with
similar magnitude. Summed over all loci
and averaged over all individuals in the
(1) population, mutationproduces no net di-
The factorof l/2 indicates the sex-limited rectional force on the mean phenotypes.
nature of the fitnessfunctions;choice (p) Hence, Equation (1) has no mutationterm.

This content downloaded from 128.40.80.65 on Wed, 25 Feb 2015 10:27:45 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
1424 ANDREW POMIANKOWSKI ET AL.

As explained later,modificationof this as- When thereare no costs to choice and b = 0


sumptioncauses a radicalchangeto theevo- the two lines are identical, specifyingthe
lutionaryoutcome of sexual selection. classic straightline of equilibriap = (2c/a)T
By specifyingthe fitnessfunctionsWm(t; (Lande, 1981). The line of equilibrium is
t,p) and WJXp) we can calculate the change stable (more exactly,an attractinglimitset,
in the mean values of the ornament and Guckenheimerand Holmes, 1983) if the
preference.Male fitnessis the product of slope of the evolutionarytrajectoryis less
survivorshipand matingsuccess,and in the than the slope of the line of equilibria (B,,/
standardmodel (Lande, 1981) corresponds G, < 2c/a).
to, But if choice is costlyand b > 0 the two
Wm(t; t, p) = ea(t0-bect2
isoclines intersectat theorigin(Fig. 1). There
(2)
is only a singleequilibriumat peq = 0, the
Males are polygamousand can mate many point where female fitnessis maximized.
times. For positive values of preference, This example suggeststhatmate preference
matingsuccessincreasesexponentiallywith cannot be costly at the evolutionaryequi-
ornamentsize t, the rate coefficientbeing librium. The resultis due to the fact that
proportionalto p, the average strengthof when At = Ap = 0 in Equation (1) the prod-
the femalemate preference.We furtheras- uct of the geneticvariance-covariancema-
sume thatfemalesassess males withrespect trix(G) and the selectionvector mustbe a
to the mean male phenotype(t). Females zero vector. If both traitsremain indepen-
with positive preferenceprefermales with dently geneticallyvariable then the vari-
largerthanaverageornaments,and females ance-covariance matrix is nondegenerate
with negativepreferenceprefermales with and the determinantof G is nonzero, so at
smaller than average ornaments.Females equilibriumtheselectionvectormustbe zero
withzero preferencemate at random. This (0 ln Wf(p)/op= 0). This means thatfemale
model of femalepreferenceis similarto the fitnessis at a maximum at equilibrium(for
"psychophysical" model used by Lande sensible fitnessfunctions).
(1981). In contrast,survival chances de- This general conclusion states that at
crease as ornamentsize deviates fromthe equilibriumfemalematepreferenceevolves
optimal male phenotype,whichis arbitrar- to one maximizingher lifetimereproduc-
ily set at t = 0. tive success. Effectsof mate choice on off-
Females have to spend some time or en- springattractiondo not influencethe evo-
ergy,or riskpredationengagingin mate dis- lutionaryequilibriumat all. The male trait
crimination.We assume that this cost to evolves to thelevel thatmaximizeshis own
femalefecundity or survivalis an increasing lifetime reproductive success given the
functionofthestrength offemalepreference equilibrium strengthof female mate pref-
and is independentof the distributionof erence. We note that,forconvenience,the
male phenotypes. optimum femalepreferencewas set at p =
= (3) 0, which correspondsto random mating.If
Wf(p) eb-p2
we had taken some othervalue forthe op-
For conveniencewe assume costs are min- timum female preference,say p > 0, then
imized when femalesmate at random (i.e., the equilibriummale characterwould have
p = 0). The cost coefficients a, b and c are deviatedfromitsnaturalselectionoptimum
small positive constants.From these defi- by an amount reflecting the strengthof fe-
nitionsthechangein mean values are found male preference(Kirkpatrick,1987).
by substitutionin Equation (1),
G
At = (ap - 2ct) - Btp(bp), (4a)
MUTATION BIAS CAN ESTABLISH
B COSTLY CHOICE
= 2P (ap - 2ct) - Gp(bp). (4b)
2
The previous resultimplies that Fisher's
By settingAT= 0 and Ap = 0 we can define processcannotexplainthestableevolution-
two isoclines that intersectat equilibrium. ary exaggerationof costly female prefer-

This content downloaded from 128.40.80.65 on Wed, 25 Feb 2015 10:27:45 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
FISHER AND BIASED MUTATION 1425

5
-0

FIG. 1. Evolution of the male trait(r) and female


FIG. 2. Costlyfemalepreference evolveswhenthe
preference(p) when preferencesare costly.Even with maletraitis subjectto mutationbias. At equilibrium
a slightcost, b = 0.0125, preferencealways evolves to thecostsoffemalepreference areoffset bythemating
an equilibrium at which female fitnessis maximized advantageof havingmoreattractive male offspring.
(p = 0). Only transientevolution of costly mate pref- Parameter valuesas in Figure1 withu = 0.005.
erenceoccurs. Parametervalues a = 0.5, c = 0.5, Gt=
GP = 0.2, Btp= 0.06.

ences. However, this requiresthatthe evo- / In Wm(t; t, p)


lution of preferenceis exactly, not just
approximatelydescribed by Equation (1). kAP!2\2BtpGp di n Wfp)
This requirementcan be easilytransgressed.
An example that we will concentrateupon
ap /
is biased mutation,where the net effectof
mutation is nonzero. There are two types ( ? ) (5)
of mutationbias thatare importantin sex-
ual selection.It is well knownthatmutation Substituting Equations (2) and (3) for
is biased on viability,most mutationsre- Wm(t;t,p) and Wf(p) respectivelyand set-
ducingviability.We will considerthiseffect tingAt = Ap = 0 again gives two isoclines,
in thecompanion paper (Iwasa et al., 1991). but these now intersectat a nonzero equi-
In addition,mutationpressureon morpho- librium(Fig. 2),
logical characters,particularlyexaggerated t - u(aBtp - 2bGp) (6a)
secondary sexual traits,is likely to be bi- teq - (a
-2bc(GtGp B2t)
ased. Primarilythis is because random al-
terationsto a complex systemare morelike- uBtp (6b)
lyto reduceitsperformance thanto improve
it (forreasonssee discussion).We now show
thatthe incorporationof a directionalbias If there is no mutational bias (u = 0) the
in mutation acting on the male trait can equilibriumis at teq= Peq =0, and female
easilyresultin theevolutionofcostlyfemale fitnessis maximized. However, withbiased
mate preference. mutation (u > 0) female fitnessdoes not
Mutationcan be simplyincorporatedinto reach its maximum at equilibrium.Rather
thestandardquantitativegeneticmodel. Let the equilibrium strengthof female prefer-
u be the per generationreductionin the ad- ence lies beyond the natural selection op-
ditivegeneticvalue of male traitdue to mu- timum, increasingwith the degree of mu-
tation. Now the change in mean values is, tationbias (u) and decreasingwiththecosts

This content downloaded from 128.40.80.65 on Wed, 25 Feb 2015 10:27:45 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
1426 ANDREW POMIANKOWSKI ET AL.

of mate preference(b). At equilibrium,di- whetherand to what degreemutationis bi-


rectselectionon femalemate preferencere- ased. But it is at once obvious on logical
mains negative; that is, female choice is groundsthat if mutationis unbiased given
costly.This cost is offsetbytheindirectben- one definitionand scale of measurement
efitof mating with the males bearing ex- then it mustbe biased formany otherdef-
aggeratedornamentaltraitsand so having initions and scales. If mutation does not
more attractivesons. Interestingly, ieq is in- alter the mean of a characteron an arith-
dependentof the efficiency of mate prefer- metic scale then it must alter it on a loga-
ence (a) or of the cost of the male trait(c). rithmicscale. If we define the color of a
In contrast,the equilibriumlevel of male crypticcaterpillarin termsof saturationwe
trait,teq, depends on these extra quantities mayfindthatmutationis unbiased.But then
which affectmale reproductivesuccess. At we know that mutationis biased if we re-
equilibrium,selectionon the male traitre- definecolor in termsofthedegreeofcrypsis,
mains positive; that is, males with larger the match to the background.
ornaments enjoy greatertotal fitness,be- What mattersfor sexual selection is the
cause the benefitof highermating success definitionand scale of femalepreference,a
exceeds thecost of lowersurvival.Selection subject about which littleis known. How-
constantlyfavorsgreaterexaggerationin the ever, femalepreferenceoftenappears to be
male trait.But this forceis checked by bi- forexaggeratedand intricatelydesignedor-
ased mutationand the correlatedeffectof namental characters(Darwin, 1871; Triv-
selectionon femalepreference, bothofwhich ers, 1985). There are two relatedreasons for
tend to reduce the mean value of the male expectinga pronounced mutation bias in
trait. At equilibrium these forces are bal- such characters.First,a greatlyexaggerated
anced. traitis likelyto be at the physiologicallimit
This indicates that costly female prefer- of its production. For example, calling in
ence can evolve if the preferredmale char- frogsand toads is known to be one of the
acteris subjectto directionalmutationbias. most, if not the most, energeticallycostly
We would like to stressthatthisresultholds behaviors that the male performs(Ryan,
irrespective ofanyassociationsthemale trait 1988). Probably it is extremelydifficult for
has with general viability. The reason for these frogsand toads to call any better(be
the establishmentof costly female prefer- it louder, longer,more frequently).So, it is
ence lies purelyin the Fisherianbenefitthat likelythatmanymutationsin genesaffecting
she gains fromhavingmore attractivemale callingwilldecreasecallingaptitudeand, so,
offspring.In nature, it may be that traits mutation will be biased. Second, many
subject to significantmutation bias also charactersused in matingshow greatintri-
stronglyreflectbiased mutation on viabil- cacy and designof structureand coloration,
ity. However, it is importantto separate forexample,the"eye" on thepeacock's tail.
thesetwo distinctforcesforcleartheoretical To getsuch perfectionprobablyrequiresthe
analysis. It is also importantthatthese two very exact action of a large number of en-
forces be separatelyassessed in empirical zymaticpathways.There will be manyways
studies. of disruptingsuch pathways but very few
ways of improvingthem. So, again muta-
MUTATIONBIAs tion in characterswith very intricateand
The preceding investigationshows that specificdesigns is likelyto be biased.
Fisher's process can cause stable exaggera- Anotherway of thinkingabout mutation
tionoffemalemate preferenceseven ifpref- bias is to relateit to the historyof selection
erence is not a neutralcharacter.But to as- on the traitin question. Mukai (1964) pre-
sess the importanceof biased mutationon dicted an equal frequencyof positive and
the male traitwe need to estimateits likely negativemutations,and hence no mutation
value in nature. To do this we need to be bias in characterssubjectto weak selection.
aware thatthe measurementof bias is scale In contrast,he predictedthatcharactersre-
dependent. Given a particulardefinitionof centlysubjectto directionalselectionshould
a characterand given a particularscale of show a negative correlationbetween bias
measurement,it is an empirical question and the directionof selection.For example,

This content downloaded from 128.40.80.65 on Wed, 25 Feb 2015 10:27:45 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
FISHER AND BIASED MUTATION 1427

we mayexpectlittleor no bias in Drosophila if mutationbias on the preferredmale trait


bristlenumber because this traithas been is similar in strengthto mutation bias on
shown to be under weak stabilizingselec- viability. In practice these two biases will
tion, but if bristle number was increased be intimatelyrelated. The bias reportedin
throughartificialselection then mutation induced mutationstudies is oftenin traits
bias towardslower numbersmightwell ap- that are probably closely related to fitness
pear. If this is the case then the mutation (e.g., Ohnishi in Crow and Simmons, 1983)
term,u, mightmorerealisticallybe replaced and P-element induced mutations in D.
by u It -t,t I in Equation (5). That is, weak melanogaster at genetic loci controlling
mutationbias close to the naturalselection quantitativecharactersfrequently have del-
optimum, but strongmutation bias away eteriouseffects on viabilityas well (Mackay,
fromthe optimum. Many sexual traitswill 1986). It is largelyforanalyticalclaritythat
have been recentlysubjectto directionalse- we have examined mutation on the male
lectiondue to femalemate preference, lead- trait under the assumption that mutants
ing to exaggerationaway fromthe viability have no pleiotropiceffectson other traits.
optimum. We expect these traitsto be par- Mutations thataffectthe male traitand fit-
ticularlysusceptibleto mutationbias. ness in other respectsare examples of so-
Mutation studies provide some evidence called "good genes," and are analyzed in a
fortheoccurrenceof mutationbias in quan- three trait model in the companion paper
titative traits. Unfortunatelymost studies (Iwasa et al., 1991). At the level of a single
of mutation on polygeniccharactershave geneticlocus it will not be possible to apply
concentratedon measuringchanges in the this distinctionbecause some alleles affect-
genetic variance not the mean. However, ingthe matingornamentwill have no pleio-
measurementsof changesto the mean were tropiceffects and otherswillhave significant
reportedin several radiationbiologyexper- pleiotropiceffectson viabilitytraits.How-
iments carriedout in the 1960s and 1970s ever, it may be possible, at the level of a
using X-irradiationor EMS (ethylmethane traitto make such a distinctionand findout
sulphate) treatmentto artificiallyraise the thefractionof mutantsin thattraitthatalso
mutationrate. These studiescommonlyre- have pleiotropic effectson other viability
porta decreasein thephenotypicmean after traits.This will potentiallyallow a gauge of
irradiation(Yamada and Kitagawa, 1963; the relative importance of these different
Abrams and Frey, 1964; Gaul, 1965; Doll, typesof mutationaleffect.
1972; Chakrabati, 1975; Ohnishi in Crow
and Simmons, 1983). More recentlyLai and SEXY SON
Mackay (1990) have used the P-M hybrid The findingsreportedhere also apply to
dysgenesis system of Drosophila melano- "sexy son" models of sexual selection.The
gaster to look at transposable element in- ''sexy son" hypothesis is derived from
duced mutationsin quantitativecharacters. Weatherhead and Robertson's (1979) sug-
They found that average stemopleuraland gestion that female preferenceevolves for
abdominal bristlenumbers were lower for males that are attractiveeven if theygive
P strain X chromosomes from dysgenic less assistance in raising offspringand so
crosses that sufferhigh mutationrates be- lower fecundity.The assumptions of the
cause of elevated P-element transposition. "sexy son" hypothesisdifferfromthe two
There is also some evidence fromsponta- traitmodel examined here only in that fe-
neous mutationaccumulation experiments male mate choice affectsherfecundityrath-
that the effectof mutationon bristlescore er than her viability.
is negativein D. melanogaster(T. Mackay, Fecunditycosts are more complicated to
pers. comm.), thougha similarexperiment model because theydepend on assumptions
by Mukai and coworkerson ADH activity about the distributionof male phenotypes
failedto show any bias (Mukai et al., 1984). and how the numberof femalesmated to a
Althoughthese reportssuggestthat mu- male affectsfecundity.However,in thesim-
tationbias on traitsexists,theydo not allow plest case of "unlimited male reproductive
an estimation of its magnitudein natural potential" (Kirkpatrick,1985), fecundityis
populations.It would be interesting to know a constantforeach male phenotypeand fe-

This content downloaded from 128.40.80.65 on Wed, 25 Feb 2015 10:27:45 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
1428 ANDREW POMIANKOWSKI ET AL.

cunditycosts act in an identical mannerto ditive models do conformto the sexy son
viabilitycosts: females with strongerpref- hypothesis,but it seems unlikelythat this
erences mate with males having largeror- is common.
naments and so have lower fecundity.If
thereis mutationbias on themale traitthen
a stable equilibriumwill be foundat which DIscusSION
losses in fecundityare offsetby the advan- The female'srolein matinghas oftenbeen
tage of bearing more attractiveoffspring. overlooked or femaleshave been relegated
Whether this is a strongenough force to to supposedly passive partners (Wallace,
account for the proposed examples of the 1891; Mayr,1972), wherepassivityhas been
"sexy son" is unclear.But it is clear thatthe taken to mean that females have simple,
strongargumentsforrejectionof the "sexy fixedpreferences, whichmales have evolved
son" hypothesis(Kirkpatrick,1985) need to to exploit (Parker, 1983; Arak, 1983). In a
be qualified. similar vein, much of the influentialwork
Our re-establishmentof the "sexy son" on the evolution of female choice has as-
hypothesisdiffers fromthatrecentlyputfor- sumed thatpreferenceis a neutraltraitthat
wardby Curtsingerand Heisler (1988) using is not subject to direct selective pressures
a two locus diploid model that explicitly (Lande, 1981; Kirkpatrick,1982). In these
considersnonadditivegeneticaffects.Curt- models preferenceevolves only as a corre-
singerand Heisler (1988) claim thatdiploid latedresponseto directselectionon themale
models permita stable evolutionaryequi- trait. The main result reported in these
libriumtrade-off betweenthefecundity costs models is that any strengthof femalepref-
of choice and the advantage of matingwith erencecan evolve (i.e., a lineofequilibria)-
more attractivemales. However, a reanaly- the evolutionaryoutcome of sexual selec-
sis by Kirkpatrick(1988) has shown that tion beinghighlyindeterminateand greatly
the resultsof diploid models are consistent affectedby random forces,such as genetic
with those found previously in haploid drift.
models in two importantrespects. Either We believe thatthe considerablebody of
thereis a stableequilibriumat whichfemale interpretation based on theassumptionthat
fecunditylies at a maximum (local or glob- preferenceis a neutralcharacteris mislead-
al), or this equilibriumis unstableand run- ing.First,thereis littleevidence thatfemale
away increasespreferenceand themale trait preferenceis not subject to selection. Al-
indefinitely (or untilgeneticvariance is ex- though there are few unambiguous mea-
hausted).Curtsinger and Heisler(1989) have sures, many observations stronglysuggest
questioned this conclusion and highlighted thatcostlymate choice has evolved (review
the complexities found in diploid models Pomiankowski,1987). For example,choosy
withnon-additiveeffects. female sandflieshave to vigorouslyresist
In our view,Kirkpatrick'sconclusionsare unwantedmale matingattempts,a behavior
generally valid for haploid and diploid that reduces fertility at low densities (En-
models withoutmutationbias. Though the gelhardet al., 1989); femalesage grousedo
evolutionarydynamics in diploid models not mate withthe firstmale theyencounter
are more complex, especially due to over- but spend several days assessingmales, of-
dominant selection maintaining genetic ten at different leks,beforechoosinga mate
variation, it appears that stable equilibria (Bradburyand Gibson, 1983; Johnsonand
occur only when femalefitnessis at a max- Boyce, 1990) and femalewhiteibises seek-
imum. This contrastswith our demonstra- ing extra-paircopulations risk greaternest
tion, in a quantitativegenetic model with parasitismand predation(Frederick,1987).
mutationbias on the male trait,that there In each of these cases female mate prefer-
are stable equilibria at whichfemalefitness ences do not appear to have evolved to the
is not maximized. However, because Curt- least costlyform.Females could minimize
singerand Heisler's (1988) resultsand Kirk- time,energy,and predationrisksby,forex-
patrick's(1988) reanalysisare based on sim- ample, accepting the firstmale they en-
ulation alone, the possibilityremains that counter or by avoiding extra pair copula-
some parametervalues in diploid nonad- tions. But theydo not.

This content downloaded from 128.40.80.65 on Wed, 25 Feb 2015 10:27:45 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
FISHER AND BIASED MUTATION 1429

Second,directselectionon femalepref- causeofexaggeration evenwhenchoicecar-


erencesradicallyalters the evolutionary riesa cost.
equilibriafoundin modelsof sexualselec- The importance ofmutationon thepre-
tion.Principally,thelineofequilibriais de- ferredmale traithas been sporadically al-
stroyedand it is replacedby a singleequi- luded to before(e.g., Parker,1983) and
librium. Thisresultholdswithbothviability clearlyanalyzedin a two locus modelby
andfecundity costsevenifthecostsofchoice Bulmer(1989b),thoughhedidnotconsider
areminute.Although thisfactwasacknowl- whether mutation biascouldsupportcostly
edgedearlyon (Lande, 1981), the impor- choice.We expectthatbiased mutationis
tance of choice costs overridingFisher's commonin nature.Bias is especiallylikely
runaway processhasonlyrecently beenfully in traitsthathave recently been subjectto
stated(Kirkpatrick, 1985; Pomiankowski, directionalselectionand evolvedcompli-
1987;Bulmer,1989b).Runawaycan cause catedstructures or exaggeration, as is true
transitory increasesin femalepreference ofmanysecondary sexualcharacters. Itmay
awayfromthefitness maximum, butat the also be thecase thatotherforcesact in a
evolutionaryequilibrium,female fitness similarfashionto biasedmutation. For in-
mustbe maximized.Thiscounter-intuitivestance,migration pressure mayhavebiased
resultappearstoshowthatFisherianmating effects ifthereis selectivediffusion ofpar-
advantages, via attractivesons,cannotoff- ticular phenotypes (SegerandTrivers, 1986).
set naturalselectionagainstchoice.It im- Another bias thatis important in sexualse-
plies that Fisher'srunawayprocessonly lectionis mutationbias on overallfitness.
causes stableexaggeration if femalepref- We haveexplicitly notconsidered thishere
erencesare not costlyat all. to allow clearanalysisof Fisher'sprocess
We have demonstrated thatthisconclu- distinct fromthe"handicap"principle. The
sion holdsonlyundertheparticular setof importance ofmutation bias on viability is
assumptions implicitin thestandardquan- consideredin thecompanionpaper(Iwasa
titativegeneticformula,Equation(1). An et al., 1991) wherewe also discusstherel-
important assumptionis thattheexpecta- ativeimportance ofthesetwoforcesin sex-
tionofvariousindependent suchas ual selection.
effects,
mutation and migration arezeroforall val-
ues of theaveragemale ornamentand fe- LrrERATuRE CrTED
male preference traits.If thisassumption ABRAMS,R., AND K. J. FREY. 1964. Variation in
holdsthentheonlyevolutionary equilibri- quantitativecharactersof oats (Avena sativa) after
various mutagentreatments.Crop Science 4:163-
um lies at thepointwherefemalefitness is 168.
maximized.However,ifthisassumption is ANDERSSON, M. B. 1986. Evolution of condition-de-
violatedthe standardformulais onlyan pendent sex ornaments and mating preferences:
approximation and theevolutionary equi- Sexual selectionbased on viabilitydifferences.
Evo-
libriummaynotmaximizefemalefitness. ARAK, lution 40:804-816.
A. 1983. Male-male competitionand mate
We have shownthatif thereis biased choice in anuran amphibians, p. 181-210. In P.
mutationon the male ornament,female Bateson(ed.), Mate Choice. CambridgeUniv. Press,
choicecan be costlyat equilibrium. Biased Cambridge,UK.
mutation tendstocreatean excessofpoorly BARTON, N., AND M. TURELLI. 1987. Adaptive land-
scapes, geneticdistanceand the evolutionof quan-
ornamented males.Although theyare sub- titativecharacters.Genet. Res. Camb. 49:157-173.
ject to weakerviabilityselection,theseless BRADBURY,J. W., AND R. M. GIBSON. 1983. Leks
wellornamented malesareat a netselective and mate choice, pp. 109-140. In P. Bateson (ed.),
disadvantage becausetheyare unattractive Mate Choice. CambridgeUniv. Press,Cambridge,
as mates.So, it remainsbeneficialforfe- UK.
BULMER,M. 1980. The Mathematical Theory of
malesto matewithmoreattractive males Quantitative Genetics. Clarendon Press, Oxford,
evenifchoiceis costly.At equilibrium the UK.
costof matechoiceis exactlybalancedby 1989a. Maintenanceofgeneticvariabilityby
theFisherianbenefit ofmatingwitha more mutation-selection balance: A child'sguidethrough
thejungle. Genome 31:761-767.
attractivemateandbearingmoreattractive 1989b. The structuralinstabilityof sexual
sons.The importance oftheresultis thatit selectionmodels. Theor. Popul. Biol. 36:195-206.
establishesFisher'sprocessesas a plausible CHAKRABARTI,S. N. 1975. Effect ofcombinedX-ray

This content downloaded from 128.40.80.65 on Wed, 25 Feb 2015 10:27:45 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
1430 ANDREW POMIANKOWSKI ET AL.

and diethylsulphate treatmenton mutation fre- 82. In J. W. Bradburyand M. Andersson (eds.),


quencies in rice (Oryza sativa L.) Radiation Biol. Sexual Selection: Testing the Alternatives.Wiley,
15:417-421. N.Y.
CROW,J.F.,AND M. J.SIMMONS.1983. Themutation 1988. Consistencyin geneticmodels of the
load in Drosophila,pp. 2-35. In M. Ashburner,H. sexyson: Replyto Curtsinger and Heisler.Am. Nat.
L. Carson,and J.N. Thompson (eds.), The Genetics 132:609-610.
and BiologyofDrosophila,Vol. 3. Academic Press, LAi, C., AND T. MACKAY. 1990. Hybrid dysgenesis-
N.Y. induced quantitativevariation on the X chromo-
CURTSINGER, J.W.,AND I. L. HEISLER. 1988. A dip- some of Drosophila melanogaster.Genetics 124:
loid "sexy son" model. Am. Nat. 125:788-810. 627-636.
1989. On theconsistencyofsexy-sonmodels: LANDE, R. 1981. Models of speciation by sexual se-
A replyto Kirkpatrick.Am. Nat. 134:978-981. lectionon polygenictraits.Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. 78:
DARwIN,C. R. 1871. The Descent of Man and Se- 3721-3725.
lection in Relation to Sex. JohnMurray,London, MACKAY,T. 1986. Transposableelement-inducedfit-
UK. ness mutationsin Drosophila melanogaster.Genet.
DOLL, H. 1972. Variation in proteinquantityand Res. Camb. 48:77-87.
quality induced in barley by EMS treatment,pp. MAYR,E. 1972. Sexual selectionand natural selec-
331-342. In Induced mutationsand plantimprove- tion, pp. 87-104. In B. Campbell (ed.), Sexual Se-
ment,IAEA. Vienna. lection and the Descent of Man. Aldine, Chicago,
ENGELHARD, G., S. P. FOSTER, AND T. H. DAY. 1989. IL.
Genetic differencesin mating success and female MuKAI,T. 1964. Polygenicmutationaffecting quan-
choice in seaweed flies(Coelopafrigida). Heredity titativecharacterof Drosophila melanogaster,pp.
62:123-131. 13-29. In Mutations in QuantitativeTraits, Pro-
FISHER, R. A. 1915. The evolution of sexual prefer- ceedingsGamma Field Symposium 3. Ministryof
ence. Eugenics Review 7:115-123. Agriculture,Japan.
1930. The Genetical Theory of Natural Se- MuKAI,T., K. HARADA,AND H. YOSHIMARU. 1984.
lection.Clarendon Press, Oxford,UK. Spontaneous mutations modifyingthe activityof
FREDERICK, P. 1987. Extrapaircopulationsin the alcohol dehydrogenase(ADH) in Drosopila mela-
matingsystemof White Ibises (Eudocimus albus). nogaster.Genetics 106:73-84.
Behaviour 100: 170-201. PARKER, G. A. 1983. Mate quality and matingde-
GAUL, H. 1965. The concept of macro- and micro- cision, pp. 141-166. In P. Bateson (ed.), Mate
mutationand resultsof induced micro-mutations Choice. CambridgeUniv. Press, Cambridge,UK.
in barley.Radiation Biology 5 (suppl.): 407-428. PoMLArNKowsKI, A. 1987. The costs of choice in sex-
GUCKENHEIMER, J.,AND P. HOLMES. 1983. Nonlinear ual selection.J. Theor. Biol. 128:195-218.
Oscillations,Dynamical Systemsand Bifurcations RYAN, M. J. 1988. Energy,calling,and selection.Am.
of Vector Fields. Springer-Verlag, N.Y. Zool. 28:885-898.
JOHNSON, L. L., AND M. S. BOYCE. 1991. Female SEGER, J.,ANDR. TRIVERS. 1986. Asymmetryin the
choice of males with low parasite loads in sage evolutionoffemalematingpreferences. Nature319:
grouse. In J. E. Loye, C. Van Ripper, and M. Zuk 771-773.
(eds.), Ecology, Behavior and Evolution of Bird- TRIVERS, R. L. 1985. Social Evolution. Benjamin/
Parasite Interactions.Univ. of Chicago Press,Chi- CummingsPublishingCo., Menlo Park,California.
cago. In press. WALLACE, A. R. 1891. Natural SelectionandTropical
IWASA, Y., A. POMANKOwSKI, AND S. NEE. 1991. The Nature. Macmillan, London, UK.
evolutionofcostlymate preferencesII. The "hand- WEATHERHEAD, P. L., AND R. J. ROBERTSON. 1979.
icap" principle.Evolution 45:1431-1442. Offspring qualityand the polygyny threshold:"The
KIRKPATRICK, M. 1982. Sexual selectionand theevo- sexy son hypothesis."Am. Nat. 113:201-208.
lution of femalechoice. Evolution 36:1-12. YAMADA, Y., AND 0. KITAGAWA. 1963. Doublingdose
1985. The evolution of female choice and forpolygenicmutationin Drosophila melanogaster.
male parental investmentin polygynousspecies: Jpn.J. Genetics 36:76-83.
The demise of the "sexy son." Am. Nat. 125:788- ZAHAVI,A. 1975. Mate selection-a selection for a
810. handicap. J. Theor. Biol. 53:205-214.
1987. The ecological forcesactingon female
matingpreferencesin polygynousanimals, pp. 67- Corresponding Editor:A. G. Clark

This content downloaded from 128.40.80.65 on Wed, 25 Feb 2015 10:27:45 AM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
View publication stats

You might also like