You are on page 1of 17

See

discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/228221814

The Influence of Outcome Effects, Decision


Aid Usage, and Intolerance of Ambiguity on
Evaluations of Professional Audit...

Article in International Journal of Auditing · August 2003


DOI: 10.1111/1099-1123.00012

CITATIONS READS

11 74

2 authors:

D. Jordan Lowe Philip Reckers


Arizona State University Arizona State University
54 PUBLICATIONS 974 CITATIONS 97 PUBLICATIONS 1,600 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by D. Jordan Lowe on 13 April 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file. All in-text references underlined in blue are added to the original document
and are linked to publications on ResearchGate, letting you access and read them immediately.
International Journal of Auditing 1(1), 43–59 (1997)

The Influence of Outcome Effects,


Decision Aid Usage, and Intolerance
of Ambiguity on Evaluations of
Professional Audit Judgement
D. Jordan Lowe1 and Philip M. J. Reckers2
1
Department of Accounting, Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State
University, Blacksburg, Virginia 24061-0101, USA 2 Department of Account-
ing, Arizona State University, Tempe, Arizona 85287-3606, USA

Auditors’ performance is evaluated in a variety of contexts and at


various time intervals following professional conduct. Outcome
knowledge is often available prior to evaluation, and accordingly,
evaluations may reflect hindsight biases. The purpose of this
paper is to examine the effects of outcome knowledge on
evaluations of professional audit judgement. Specifically, we
hypothesize that outcome effects will be altered by two factors:
(1) conduct that involves the incomplete use (truncated applica-
tion) of available decision aids and (2) the degree to which
evaluators exhibit intolerance of ambiguity. We also examined the
interaction of these two factors. Results of our experiment
confirmed our hypotheses regarding the influence of decision
aid usage and individuals’ intolerance of ambiguity on outcome
effects. Negative, non-normative outcome effects were strongest
when available analytical procedures decision aids were only
partially utilized; participants who were intolerant of ambiguity
were subject to hindsight biases, while tolerant subjects were not.
Implications are discussed in terms relating to (internal) perfor-

#
mance evaluations and extensions to external legal liability.
1997 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Int. J. Audit. 1(1), 43–59 (1997)

(No. of Figures: 1 No. of Tables: 1 No. of Refs: 60)


Key words: decision aids; outcome effects; tolerance of ambiguity;

hindsight; performance evaluation; analytical procedures; audit

judgement; decision process; liability; auditors.

SUMMARY and internally pursuant to audit scheduling and


renumeration decisions and, ultimately, pursuant
Valid evaluations of auditor performance repre- to retention and promotion decisions. In spite of
sent an important task to the profession. Apprai- its importance, limited research has addressed
sals of auditors occur externally in courts of law performance review in public accounting. This is
somewhat ironic given that internal performance
Data Availability: Data used in this study are available from review is an essential element in the individual’s
the authors. development process and in the firm’s effort to

Received December 1995

#
CCC 1090–6738/97/010043-17 $17.50 Revised July 1996
1997 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Accepted September 1996
44 D. J. Lowe and P. M. J. Reckers

retain competent and motivated professional staff. the likelihood of error (or irregularities) was
Externally, litigation costs are one of the largest insignificant, and given that the audit budget was
costs born by public accounting firms. tight, the supervising senior decided not to allocate
Outcome knowledge is frequently available prior further attention to the matter. Outcome knowl-
to performance evaluation decisions such as those edge was manipulated between subjects. Subjects
noted above. Outcome knowledge (hindsight bias) were either given no outcome or were provided a
may inhibit evaluators’ ability to retrospectively negative audit outcome. Subjects’ tolerance of
evaluate objectively. Therefore, it is important to ambiguity also was measured.
gain an understanding of the phenomenon and the Results of the experiment confirmed our hypoth-
conditions in which it manifests itself. We hypothe- eses. Outcome effects were greatest when decision
size that outcome effects are exacerbated by two aids were perceived as only partially utilized. Our
factors: (1) incomplete decision aid usage by the results suggest that, notwithstanding the intended
auditor and (2) intolerance of ambiguity by the benefits of decision aids, auditors who are per-
evaluator. ceived to only partially utilize available decision
An experiment was conducted with 147 audit aids may be evaluated significantly more nega-
seniors from an international public accounting tively. This tendency could lead auditors to be less
firm. Subjects were provided with a case scenario receptive to the implementation of decision aids in
in which the focal point related to analytical their firms. Knowledge of an auditor’s partial
procedures. In seeking to explain a significant utilization of a decision aid may create the
decrease in the inventory turnover ratio, the perception of a failure of due diligence by the
supervising senior considered 10 explanations auditor. In addition, subjects who were intolerant
generated by a firm-adopted decision aid. Decision of ambiguity were found to be more apt to reduce
aid usage was manipulated such that the super- ambiguity by focusing on the unfavourable out-
vising senior either investigated (all) 10 of 10 come exclusively. That is, these individuals were
explanations that the decision aid provided (full more subject to outcome effects. Finally, ambiguity-
utilization) or alternatively investigated (the same) tolerant individuals did attend to additional in-
10 of 15 explanations (partial utilization). None of formation, and accordingly were found to be more
the 10 items investigated explained the ratio influenced by specific audit conduct.
change. Since the supervising senior believed that

INTRODUCTION hindsight. Under certain conditions, selected eva-


luators in an ex post position may systematically
Valid evaluation of professional conduct is very perceive there to have been a predictable sequence
important to individuals, firms, institutions and of events leading to the obtained outcome, and
society. To be successful, an individual must be able therefore presume that the decision maker should
both to direct and evaluate subordinates; only by have known what outcome would occur (Janoff-
doing so can one (over time) expect to surround Bulman et al., 1985; Mitchell and Kalb, 1981).
oneself with competent individuals. No less so Evaluators may assess whether they would have
must firms, institutions, and society strive for foreseen the likelihood of an event and conse-
responsible and accurate evaluation of professional quently project that the decision maker ex ante (i.e
judgement. Within the context of the public in foresight) should have foreseen or anticipated
accounting profession, audit judgement is continu- what became clear ex post (i.e. in hindsight)
ously being evaluated, externally through the (Anderson, 1986). This reasoning suggests that
judicial system and internally as a basis for job evaluative judgements of auditor’s competence,
scheduling, remuneration and, ultimately, retention judgement, and performance may be influenced by
and promotion of employees. audit outcome, potentially in a non-normative
A potential impediment to the sound evaluation fashion. That is, an objective performance evalua-
of professional judgement is the improper use of tion may be hindered if decision makers are held to
outcome knowledge. When outcome knowledge is an unrealistic standard for predicting an outcome
available before performance is evaluated, the and for the taking of a priori steps to avoid the
evaluation is conducted from the perspective of outcome (Kadous, 1996).

# 1997 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. International Journal of Auditing, 1(1), 43–59 (1997)
Evaluations of Professional Audit Judgment 45

Limited research to date has addressed outcome that within an experimental setting, both factors
effects would interact with outcome knowledge such that
Within an accounting context, Lipe (1993) de- outcome effects would be greatest when a negative
monstrated that managers making decisions to reconstruction of events was feasible (which
investigate (versus not investigate) prevailing con- challenged due diligence of effort, i.e. non-exhaus-
ditions were evaluated more favourably when tive use of available decision aids are observed) or
investigations revealed problems in the control when subjects’ intolerance of ambiguity was high.
systems (compared with managers making the We also hypothesized that ambiguity-tolerance
same decisions based on the same data but without would interact with decision aids usage.
outcome information being provided to research The remainder of this paper is organized as
participants). Brown and Solomon (1987, 1993) follows. The next section discusses the literature
found similarly that outcome knowledge influ- and develops our hypotheses. The section after
enced managers’ evaluations and judgements of details the methodology and overall experimental
capital budgeting decisions. Lowe and Reckers design. We then present the statistical results of the
(1994) examined the effects of outcome knowledge study. Finally, we discuss our results and provide
among jurors in an auditor evaluation task. Results important implications.
revealed that negative outcome information re-
garding client bankruptcy was associated with
significantly more unfavourable evaluative assess- HYPOTHESES DEVELOPMENT
ments of auditor performance, relative to a no Outcome Effect Factors
outcome control group. Recently, a number of researchers (Baron and
While certain types of outcome knowledge (e.g. Hershey, 1988; Fitschhoff, 1982; Hawkins and
client bankruptcy, management fraud) may not Hastie, 1990) have observed that future research
necessarily be available immediately following would contribute more to the current state of
performance for all evaluations (e.g. formal end- knowledge if research was to focus on the factors
of-engagement reviews), other outcome informa- that may attenuate (or amplify) outcome effects on
tion may be available in the near term (e.g. decision appraisals as opposed to merely continu-
workpaper errors, failure to meet client deadlines, ing to document the existence of outcome effects.
audit budget overruns). In addition, not all The present study responds to that call. A
evaluations of auditors are conducted immediately significant contribution of the present study is the
following performance. For example, future job examination of two such factors: (1) decision aid
assignments, annual reviews, remuneration deci- usage and (2) intolerance of ambiguity.
sions and, particularly, retention and promotion
decisions will take place frequently after significant Decision Aid Usage
intervals following performance. Thus, the risks of Outcome effects involve backwards processing
tainted internal as well as external evaluations are (reconstruction of events) from the outcome to
quite real in the auditing profession. antecedent factors. The given outcome acts as a cue
Given the limited but challenging prior research to retrieve antecedent events from memory (Dellar-
on outcome effects, it is important to develop a osa and Bourne, 1984; Slovic and Fischhoff, 1977).
better understanding of how individuals are Individuals tend to assimilate the outcome to
differentially affected by outcome knowledge and antecedent events by adding semantic links sig-
how selected characteristics might attenuate or nifying causal relations to their knowledge struc-
(alternatively) enhance outcome effects in auditor ture. This assimilation of knowledge forces a ‘fit’
performance evaluation. Specifically, this paper between the given outcome and antecedent events.
examines whether outcome effects are influenced If a reasonably good fit is effected, it becomes
by two factors: decision aid usage and individuals’ difficult for individuals to assess accurately the
intolerance of ambiguity. Secondary to this objec- likelihood that alternative outcomes could have
tive was our interest in understanding the relation- occurred (Janoff-Bulman et al., 1985).
ship between these two factors. We hypothesized In terms of audit judgement, a coherent causal
story would necessarily focus on an incriminating
1 audit trail whereby reconstructed evidence reveals
Baron and Hershey (1988) refer to ‘hindsight effects’ in
relation to studies utilizing a prediction task. Within the certain arguable deficiencies in audit procedures
context of decision evaluation, the phenomenon has been and decisions. Judgements otherwise perceived as
termed ‘outcome effects’ (Brown and Solomon, 1993). appropriate may take on the appearance of inept-

International Journal of Auditing, 1(1), 43–59 (1997) # 1997 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd
46 D. J. Lowe and P. M. J. Reckers

ness or culpability in the presence of negative Little if any research has addressed the relation-
outcome information. In a legal courtroom setting, ship between intolerance of ambiguity and out-
Kripke (1988, p. 361) observes that unfavourable come effects. An exception is Campbell and Tesser
audit outcomes are magnified when the ‘seeds of (1983) who observed that individuals do have
the subsequent failure were present in the audited varying tendencies to see their world as just,
period, and that the auditors culpably overlooked predictable and certain, and to reconstruct the past
them’. The perceived partial utilization of a firm- to make it appear deterministic and orderly.
endorsed decision aid may create such an incrimi-
nating audit trail for both internal and external H2: Tolerance of ambiguity will interact with
review (Ellis, 1983). Ex post, attention may be outcome effects such that evaluators who are
placed more on decision aid-based explanations intolerant of ambiguity will be more influ-
not investigated rather than on those that were enced by outcome information.
investigated. Knowledge of an auditor’s partial We were also interested in examining the
utilization of a decision aid may call into question relationship of tolerance of ambiguity to decision
the quality of his=her performance. In the context of aid usage in an effort to gain a better under-
our study, outcome effects should be heightened standing of underlying performance evaluation
when the auditor’s decision process includes factors (and for purposes of completeness). Ambi-
partial (incomplete) utilization of audit decision guity-intolerant individuals actively seek to reduce
aids. This leads to hypothesis 1. uncertainty by focusing on simple solutions to
H1: Decision aids usage will interact with outcome resolve apparent ambiguous situations. Once a
effects such that negative outcome effects will solution is identified, these individuals tend to
be greatest under conditions of incomplete neglect other information. That is, ambiguity-
application of decision aids (perceptually, intolerant individuals are more apt to arrive at
assimilated as the ‘cause’ of subsequent fail- premature closure of a decision process than are
ure). ambiguity-tolerant individuals (Frenkel-Bruns-
wick, 1949; MacDonald, 1970). We conjecture that
as ambiguity-intolerant individuals focus upon
Intolerance of Ambiguity outcome knowledge (hypothesis 2), they will tend
Individual differences also deserve attention in to ignore other information such as the utilization
their relation to outcome effects, particularly of decision aids. By doing so, these individuals are
individuals’ intolerance of ambiguity (Einhorn more apt to close prematurely their decision
and Hogarth, 1985; Gul, 1984). Individuals have process in favour of outcome information.
often been observed to react differently to ambig- Conversely, ambiguity-tolerant individuals are
uous situations (Frenkel-Brunswick, 1949). An not expected to focus exclusively on outcome
ambiguous situation can be defined as one that knowledge. These individuals should be in a
cannot be totally categorized or structured because position to process additional information (such
of incomplete information. Ambiguity tolerance as decision aid usage) and be more thorough in
refers to the way that individuals respond to their decision process. This reasoning leads to
ambiguous situations. Compared with ambiguity- hypothesis 3.
tolerant individuals, ambiguity-intolerant indivi-
duals perceive ambiguous situations as being more H3: Tolerance of ambiguity will interact with
uncertain and psychologically threatening. Conse- decision aid usage such that evaluators who
quently, individuals who are intolerant of ambi- are tolerant of ambiguity will be more influ-
guity seek means to cope with ambiguous enced by the utilization of decision aids.
situations (Budner, 1962; Duncan, 1972). Pratt
(1980) suggests that ambiguity-intolerant indivi- However, it is an empirical question (that we
duals seek to ‘ignore uncertainty, fail to recognize address, herein) whether ambiguity tolerant indi-
uncertainty, or act to reduce the threat of un- viduals will normatively process additional infor-
certainty’. Within a professional judgement con-
2
text, individuals are more likely to resort to actively Campbell and Tesser (1983) originally assumed that the
reducing the perceived threat by (1) applying fixed tolerance of ambiguity construct was a motivational mechan-
ism contributing to hindsight effects. However, other
rules in a ‘black and white’ manner and (2) quickly researchers (i.e. Davies, 1992; Hawkins and Hastie, 1990)
adopting available answers to various questions have interpreted Campbell and Tesser’s experimental con-
(MacDonald, 1970; Pincus, 1990; Zebda, 1991). structs as evidence of cognitive processes.

# 1997 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. International Journal of Auditing, 1(1), 43–59 (1997)
Evaluations of Professional Audit Judgment 47

mation, or fall prey to other biases (i.e. focus on accounting firm performing an audit of a medium-
what the auditor did not do rather than on what the sized, publicly traded client. The client manufac-
auditor did do, with respect to completeness of tures a variety of plastic and ceramic=glass
decision aids usage). products for both commercial and domestic use.
Summarized, unaudited financial information by
the client was presented for the current year.
The focal point of the case related to analytical
METHOD procedures and, more specifically, to an unusual
An experiment was conducted using practising decline in the inventory turnover ratio (compared
auditors assigned an evaluation task. The subjects, with prior periods).
instrument development, task, independent and We then provided subjects with a description of
dependent measures are described below. the firm’s analytical procedures decision aid soft-
ware which generates selected causal explanations
for account and ratio changes. In seeking to explain
Subject Group the significant decrease in the inventory turnover
The subjects consisted of 147 audit seniors from a ratio, the supervising senior considered=investi-
large international public accounting firm. The gated ten of the decision aid generated explana-
experiment was conducted during senior auditor tions. None of the ten items investigated were
training sessions with one of the researchers in found to explain the ratio change. Since the
attendance. The subjects were assured of the supervising senior believed that the likelihood of
confidentiality of their responses. The mean age error (or irregularities) was insignificant, and given
of the auditors was 26 years. Forty-eight percent of that the audit budget was tight, the supervising
the auditors were women. senior decided not to allocate further attention to
Auditors were believed to be a good subject the matter. Following the case scenario, subjects
group that would provide a strong test of outcome were instructed to respond to the dependent
effects. Other groups such as jurors or students measure and complete debriefing questions and
may be more likely to anchor on the outcome given the Tolerance of Ambiguity Scale (see Appendix B).
their limited knowledge of the decision process or
task. Previous research suggests that greater famil-
iarity with the decision process or task may Independent Measures
moderate against outcome effects (Christensen- The experiment included three primary indepen-
Szalanski and Fobian-Willham, 1991; Voss et al., dent measures (and their interactions), two ma-
1980). In addition, auditors work in an environ- nipulated within the scenario (decision aid usage
ment that consists of conflicting risks and objec- and outcome) and one measured (tolerance of
tives. ambiguity). We also included the age and gender
measures to strengthen our regression design
Instrument Development rather than to test specific hypotheses. Prior
research has indicated their potential effect on
We consulted many members to develop a case that performance evaluation (Greenhaus and Parasura-
was representative of conditions found frequently man, 1993; Hooks, 1992; Hunt, 1995; Lehman, 1988;
in practice. We designed our case instrument to Reckers and Schultz, 1993).
include an analytical procedures task in which a
declining inventory turnover ratio is the focal part
of the case (see Appendix A). The inventory 3
Graham et al. (1991, p. 72) assert that, ‘Some audit issues are
turnover ratio was selected for attention since it is easily identified through the use of simple analytical
deemed to be a key ratio auditors consider as part procedures. For example, declining inventory turnover or
declining receivables turnover have certain implications with
of their analytical procedures (Graham et al., 1991). respect to risks that the auditor should identify and assess.’
4
The 38 audit managers had an average of five years and
four months of audit experience (with a standard deviation
Task of 12.6 months). The managers stated that approximately
32% of their engagements involved the audits of inventory
Subjects were given a case booklet containing and further that about 12% of their engagements involved
instructions, the experimental materials, and a errors or irregularities relating to inventory. The average size
debriefing questionnaire (see Fig. 1). The back- of the offices in which they worked was between 150–175
ground information depicted a large public professionals.

International Journal of Auditing, 1(1), 43–59 (1997) # 1997 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd
48 D. J. Lowe and P. M. J. Reckers

Decision aid
usage

No outcome Full
provided utilization
(foresight)
Partial
utilization
Background case Evaluation and Debriefing and
and instructions promotion tolerance of
Full questions ambiguity questions
utilization

Outcome
provided Partial
(hindsight) utilization

Figure 1. Experimental procedures.

Decision Aid Usage instance—the senior would be perceived as not


Decision aid packages in the analytical procedures having satisfied the professional norm implicit in
area are currently being developed by several the decision aid (see Anderson et al., 1994; Gournic,
firms, with some packages (e.g. ANSWERS) now 1994; Jennings et al., 1993).
being available commercially (Blocher and Will- In the full utilization condition, the decision aid
ingham, 1988). Their purpose is to provide lists of was exhaustively used in that the supervising
explanations for unusual account or ratio trends. senior considered and investigated all ten explana-
These decision aids are not necessarily designed to tions that the decision aid provided. The ten
be comprehensive, rather they serve to identify explanations were among the 15 more frequently
relatively common explanations. The manner in cited explanations provided by the audit managers
which decision aids were utilized was represented in Anderson et al. (1994). Our list contained five
as being exhaustive or non- exhaustive. We error and five non-error (e.g. changing client
expected that full utilization of a decision aid circumstances) explanations. In this regard, the
would be perceived to satisfy ‘due diligence’ materials read, in part:
professional standards. That is, the decision aid
may imply a standard of performance similar to In seeking to explain the significant decrease in the
what other auditors would do in the same inventory turnover ratio, during the course of the
audit, Bill INVESTIGATED ALL 10 OF THE ITEMS
situation. We expected this to be the case even if
ON THE ANALYTICAL PROCEDURES DECISION
the decision aid generated only ten high likelihood AID LIST.
explanations, not including the true explanation,
and the audit senior failed to self-generate and Having the decision aid produce ten plausible
explore other explanations. Conversely, we pre- explanations is comparable to the current stage of
dicted that a situation in which the decision aid is development of analytical procedure packages. For
only partially (incompletely) used, might be per- instance, ANSWERS, a widely used analytical
ceived as a non-normative decision process (even if procedures package, lists seven explanations for a
the decision aid included a more lengthy list of significant decrease in the inventory turnover ratio.
explanations and the audit senior investigated the In the partial utilization condition, the decision
same ten diagnostic explanations investigated aid was incompletely utilized in the sense that not
under the other condition). We expected the seeds all listed explanations were investigated by the
of failure would have been sown in the latter audit team. The decision aid listed 15 explanations,
of which only ten were investigated by the super-
5
vising senior. These ten explanations were the same
The audit profession is placing increased emphasis on
analytical procedures as a source of audit evidence (Koonce,
as those provided (and investigated) in the full
1993; Tabor and Willis, 1985). In fact, a recent audit standard utilization condition.
‘requires the use of analytical procedures in the planning and
overall review stages of all audits’ (AICPA, 1988, SAS No. 56, Outcome
para. 1). Outcome was manipulated between subjects at two

# 1997 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. International Journal of Auditing, 1(1), 43–59 (1997)
Evaluations of Professional Audit Judgment 49

levels: no outcome (foresight) and negative out- information. First, we explictly instructed subjects
come (hindsight). In the foresight condition, sub- to make their judgements based only on informa-
jects were given the general case scenario but were tion that the auditors had at the time they
not given information regarding subsequent events conducted the audit. That is, to base their judge-
as the outcome was withheld. Subjects formed their ments on ex ante information which existed before
judgements from an ex ante perspective. learning of the outcome. In the absence of this
In the hindsight condition, subjects were given explicit directive, it might be argued that some
the general case scenario followed by the following subjects would alter their ex post judgements in a
outcome: fashion similar to a Bayesian process (with the
adjustment being normative) (Hawkins and Hastie,
Three months after release of the financial statements 1990; Hoch and Loewenstein, 1989). Secondly, we
of Springdale, the correct explanation for the decrease were careful to provide subjects with enough
in the inventory turnover ratio was discovered. The information so that they will not be induced to be
correct explanation was an ERROR, and thus THE dependent on the given outcome (Baron and
PUBLISHED FINANCIAL STATEMENTS WERE IN Hershey, 1988; Helleloid, 1985; Slovic and Fischh-
ERROR. The correct explanation was an error in
off, 1977). The three-page, single spaced scenario
application of labor rates (for manufactured products)
to inventory, resulting in an overstatement of inven-
provides considerably more information than is
tory. The firm and its labor union agreed to new wage generally found in studies of this nature (see Baron
rates during the audit year effective the following year. and Hershey, 1988). We provided enough informa-
These new rates were prematurely used in calculating tion to depict a realistic case, but not so much
the labor portion of the cost of inventory in the year of information that subjects became susceptible to
the audit. information overload (Casper et al., 1989). In
addition, we intentionally designed the case such
The Clarkson Group had purchased Springdale
that it was not possible to know with certainty the
stock shortly after publication of the erroneous
financial statements. Following discovery and dis-
eventual outcome of the audit.
closure of the inventory and net income misstate-
ments Springdale’s stock price dropped sharply. Tolerance of Ambiguity
The Clarkson Group is now suing Springdale and We used the AT-20 scale (MacDonald, 1970) to
its external auditors. measure this attribute. This scale consists of 20
true–false questions and has been found to be more
The actual error, related to the misapplication of reliable than other available measures (e.g. Budner,
labor rates, was among the five explanations that 1962; Rydell and Rosen, 1966).The scale is designed
were not investigated by the supervising senior (in so that lower scores indicate ambiguity intolerance
the partial utilization condition). and higher scores indicate ambiguity tolerance. The
With regard to the hindsight condition, it was MacDonald Scale is particularly appropriate as it
important to establish a proper methodological was developed to operationalize intolerance of
setting to measure outcome effects. Decision theory ambiguity from the related concept of rigidity
methodology maintains that the decision process (Campbell and Tesser, 1993). MacDonald (1970)
(and not the decision outcome) is the main criterion indicates that reliability estimates range from a low
for assessing decision quality. Decision quality of 0.63 (test–retest, six month interval) to a high of
should depend upon such things as the wisdom 0.86 (split-half corrected by Spearman-Brown).
or consistency of the decision at the time it is
implemented, without reference to the outcome
(Brown and Solomon, 1987; Fisher and Selling, Dependent Measures
1993). Our objective was to allow subjects to focus After reviewing the case information, subjects were
upon the audit process and particularly the ex ante asked to answer two dependent measure questions.
nature of the auditor’s decision. Following Baron
and Hershey (1988) and Hershey and Baron (1992), 7
By keeping the number of explanations investigated (10)
we provide a methodological means to ensure constant across both the full and partial usage conditions,
measurement of the non-normative use of outcome differential results could be interpreted to be due to the
utilization of the decision aid and not to the amount of audit
work performed.
6 8
Of the seven explanations for a significant decrease in the The actual error was not considered to be an unusual error.
inventory turnover ratio, four relate to errors and three relate In the developmental pretest of the case instrument, 11 of the
to non- errors—changing client circumstances (ANSWERS 38 audit managers generated this error. This error was the
version 2.5A). ninth (out of 35) most common listed error.

International Journal of Auditing, 1(1), 43–59 (1997) # 1997 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd
50 D. J. Lowe and P. M. J. Reckers

The first question requests a generic (non- con- t ˆ ÿ2.81, p < 0.01 for generic evaluations;
textual) evaluation of the audit senior’s judgement t ˆ ÿ2.13, p ˆ 0.02 for promotion).
not to modify the standard audit program in light
of the inventory ratio change. This evaluation is
intended to generalize to various performance Tests of Hypothesis 1
evaluation settings such as annual reviews, future Hypothesis 1 predicted that outcome effects would
job assignments=scheduling, and remuneration interact with decision aid usage. Specifically, out-
decisions, each of which has the potential for come effects were expected to be greatest when
outcome effects. Specifically, the question read as there was only partial (incomplete) usage of
follows. decision aids. As shown in Table 1, the interaction
Please evaluate Bill’s decision not to make further was found to be marginally significant for the
inquiries regarding the significant ratio change. evaluation judgement (t ˆ 1.26, p ˆ 0.105).
For the promotion decision, the interaction was
0 . . . 1 . . . 2 . . . 3 . . . 4 . . . 5 . . . 6 . . . 7 . . . 8 . . . 9 . . . 10 found to be statistically significant at the 0.05 level:
Very Inappropriate Very Appropriate t ˆ 1.70, p ˆ 0.045) (see Table 2). Practical interpre-
In the second question, we focused on a context- tation of these findings may be facilitated by a
specific performance evaluation setting—a promo- review of the mean responses for the promotion
tion decision. We believed this to be potentially the decision under the four experimental conditions.
most important type of evaluation made internally
of an auditor. Further, it was anticipated that Tests of Hypothesis 2
significant negative outcome knowledge could be
especially damaging to an individual when s=he Hypothesis 2 proposed that tolerance of ambiguity
comes up for promotion. Subjects were asked to will interact with outcome knowledge. We antici-
assess the audit senior’s pending promotion as pated that (negative) outcome information would
follows. register progressively greater influence on auditor
respondents who were relatively less tolerant of
To what extent do you believe Bill should be ambiguity. Conversely, we hypothesized that out-
supported for promotion to manager? come information would affect the evaluation and
0 . . . 1 . . . 2 . . . 3 . . . 4 . . . 5 . . . 6 . . . 7 . . . 8 . . . 9 . . . 10
promotion decisions of more ambiguity-tolerant
Not at All Very Strongly Supported
subjects to a lesser extent. Table 1 reveals that for
the evaluation judgement the interaction was
marginally significant at traditional levels
RESULTS (t ˆ 1.24, p ˆ 0.11). For the promotion decision, the
interaction between tolerance of ambiguity and
Ordinary least-squares regression was used to test outcome knowledge was significant at the 0.05
the three hypotheses. Regression analysis was level (t ˆ 1.72, p ˆ 0.045) (see Table 2). In each
considered appropriate as two of the independent instance, the results were in the direction
measures were continuous (of particular impor- predicted.
tance being the tolerance of ambiguity measure). The AT-20 scale measures tolerance for ambi-
The model included, as independent measures, guity on a 20-point scale. To partition categorically
decision aid usage, outcome, tolerance of ambi- subjects based on that scale involves a significant
guity, age, and gender. In addition, the model loss of information (thus the propriety of using
contained three two-way interactions (related to the regression analysis rather than ANOVA). None-
three primary independent measures) of outcome theless, categorical partitioning may facilitate a
by decision aid usage, outcome by tolerance of basic understanding of the above empirical find-
ambiguity, and decision aid usage by tolerance of ings. Accordingly, participants were classified as
ambiguity. intolerant if included in the bottom third of
As Tables 1 and 2 indicate, all three independent respondents; and tolerant if included in the top
measures were significant. Decision aid usage was third. The distribution of responses was unimodal
significant in predicting the evaluation judgement with a kurtosis measure of ÿ0.26; accordingly, the
(t ˆ ÿ2.53, p < 0.01) and promotion judgement
(t ˆ ÿ2.18, p ˆ 0.015). The results also provide 9
The three hypotheses were directional in nature. Therefore,
evidence that (in)tolerance of ambiguity signifi- one-tailed tests were performed on the t-statistics for each of
cantly influenced subjects’ responses (respectively, these coefficients.

# 1997 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. International Journal of Auditing, 1(1), 43–59 (1997)
Evaluations of Professional Audit Judgment 51

Table 1. Regression results. Dependent measure: evaluation judgement

Source Coefficient Std error t-Statistic Probability

Decision Aid (D) ÿ4.0801 1.61 ÿ2.53 < 0.010


Outcome (O) ÿ2.2353 1.60 ÿ1.39 0.085
ÿ0.3880 ÿ2.81 < 0.010
6
Tolerance (T) 0.14

6
D O 1.2274 0.98 1.26 0.105

6
O T 0.1856 0.15 1.24 0.110
D T 0.3718 0.15 2.52 < 0.010
Age ÿ0.1504 0.09 ÿ1.73 0.045
Gender 0.4884 0.50 0.97 0.165
F-ratio ˆ 1.671

Table 2. Regression results. Dependent measure: promotion decision

Source Coefficient Std error t-Statistic Probability

Decision Aid (D) ÿ2.4837 1.13 ÿ2.18 0.015


Outcome (O) ÿ2.8868 1.13 ÿ2.55 < 0.010
ÿ0.2077 ÿ2.13
6
Tolerance (T) 0.09 0.020

6
D O 1.1713 0.69 1.70 0.045

6
O T 0.1829 0.11 1.72 0.045
D T 0.2453 0.10 2.35 0.010
Age ÿ0.1153 0.06 ÿ1.88 0.030
Gender 0.7229 0.36 2.03 0.020
F-ratio ˆ 2.916

centrist classification sacrifices significant informa- Hypothesis 3 explored the potential interaction
tion, value, and interpretation will rely on mean between tolerance of ambiguity and decision aid
responses of tolerants and intolerants. So defined, usage. We expected that individuals who were
ambiguity-intolerant subjects provided higher more tolerant of ambiguity would attend more to
evaluations in foresight (6.76) than in hindsight available non-outcome information (such as deci-
(5.74). On the other hand, ambiguity-tolerant sion aid usage.) It was our purpose to explore the
subjects provided essentially the same responses risk that they then might be influenced (potentially
across foresight and hindsight conditions (5.45 non-normatively) by the auditors’ full versus
compared with 5.49). Similarly, subjects intolerant partial utilization of decision aids, even if not
of ambiguity rendered higher mean promotion influenced directly by the outcome effects. As
decision in foresight (6.54) compared with hind- shown in Tables 1 and 2, the results of the
sight (5.46). Conversely, subjects who were tolerant experiment confirm this risk for both the evaluation
of ambiguity were unaffected by outcome knowl- and promotion dependent measures (t ˆ 2.52,
edge; mean decisions were substantively the same p < 0.01; t ˆ 2.35, p ˆ 0.01, respectively).
over the foresight and hindsight conditions (6.19 In response to evaluation questions, ambiguity-
compared with 6.14, respectively). These findings
provide support for hypothesis 2.
11
For the evaluation judgement, subjects provided a partial
utilization of a decision aid gave lower mean judgements in
hindsight (5.17) than in foresight (5.65). For subjects provided
Tests of Hypothesis 3 a full utilization of a decision aid, mean judgements were
higher in hindsight (5.93) than in foresight (5.13).
10 12
Tolerance of ambiguity scores have a total range of nought Alternatively, the comparative conditions could have been
to 20. Auditors’ scores ranged from a low of two to high of interpreted through regression such that changes between
19, with each of the values in between having at least one regression points are calculated and assessed (see Reckers et
auditor score. The variance of the scores was 10.77 with a al., 1994). Analyses using this method is more informative,
standard deviation of 3.38. however, the interpretation may be somewhat less intuitive.

International Journal of Auditing, 1(1), 43–59 (1997) # 1997 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd
52 D. J. Lowe and P. M. J. Reckers

tolerant individuals (as defined above) provided decision may explain the difference in responses.
higher mean judgements for the full utilization Promotion decisions are infrequent and have
condition (6.27) than for the partial utilization significant consequences to the individual as
condition (4.60), confirming expectations that these well as for the firm. Also, given the defined
individuals will use decision aid utilization infor- (context-specific) purpose of the promotion
mation. Also as expected, ambiguity-intolerant assessment, subjects may have been more atten-
individuals were less influenced by decision aid tive to this decision (as suggested by Hunt and
utilization, as their mean judgements (6.63 and Messier, 1995).
5.82) were more comparable across these same Before discussing the results of our study, it is
conditions. This was as expected as intolerant first appropriate to note certain limitations. First,
individuals anchor on outcome information almost our selection of subjects may limit the external
exclusively, as reported in the prior section. validity of our results.
Similarly, with respect for the promotion decision, Secondly, our study excluded many factors that
ambiguity-tolerant individuals gave higher mean may be related to performance evaluation. The
judgements for the full utilization condition (7.00) experimental materials may not have contained the
compared with the partial utilization condition richness of a real world setting. Given time
(5.26), whereas, ambiguity-intolerant individuals constraints we were limited in what information
displayed almost the same mean judgements (6.04 we could provide.
and 5.89) for these same conditions. These results Finally, as with behavioural experiments in
provide support for hypothesis 3. general, this experiment is subject to other inherent
limitations; therefore, results should be interpreted
with care as to their generalizability. This study
involves a limited sample of subjects from a single
Other Results
firm, deciding a single hypothetical case (of which
The age measure was found to be significant in the the unfavourable outcome was rather extreme).
model for both the evaluation and promotion Also, this study measured decisions that were
decisions (t ˆ ÿ1.73, p ˆ 0.045; t ˆ ÿ1.88, p ˆ 0.03). actually intentions to act rather than actual beha-
Younger subjects tended to provide higher evalua- viours. Therefore, the associations between inde-
tions and greater support for the senior’s promo- pendent measures and decision responses may be
tion. Gender was not significant for the evaluation overstated. Given these limitations this study
judgement (t ˆ 0.97, p ˆ 0.165) however it was yields the following important findings in order
significant for the promotion decision (t ˆ 2.03, of the three hypothesized interactions.
p ˆ 0.02). Women tended to provide greater sup- Perhaps the major finding of this study relates to
port for the senior’s promotion. our first hypothesis. In spite of receiving instruc-
tions ‘to exercise care not to be influenced by
outcome information’, our subjects were suscepti-
ble to outcome information when also provided
DISCUSSION, IMPLICATIONS, AND with a subordinate’s non-exhaustive use of an
FUTURE RESEARCH audit decision aid. These results have implications
The primary objective of this study was to for the use of decision aids in the audit
investigate the effects of outcome knowledge on environment. Cushing and Loebbecke (1986) and
performance evaluations and specifically to deter- Willingham (1986) identify a trend of some firms
mine whether outcome effects are influenced by toward more structured methods of operation to
two factors: the presence and relative usage of deter the inconsistent judgements of various
decision aids and intolerance of ambiguity. We auditors in respective firms. This lack of consensus
were also interested in understanding the relation- may be considered problematic for the audit
ship between these two factors. These effects were profession in general and for certain auditors it
examined for (1) a generalizable, context-free (i.e. suggests that the accuracy and statistical rationality
generic) evaluation of an auditor’s performance of their audit decisions are apt to be faulty (Ashton,
as well as for (2) a pending context-specific 1985; Ashton and Willingham, 1988). Decision aids
promotion decision. Results were generally stron- are intended to reduce the occurrence of incon-
ger for the promotion decision (and with less sistent decisions in similar situations by different
variance) compared with the evaluation judge- auditors (Abdolmohammadi, 1991; Ashton and
ment. The greater importance of the promotion Willingham, 1988; Messier, 1995).

# 1997 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. International Journal of Auditing, 1(1), 43–59 (1997)
Evaluations of Professional Audit Judgment 53

Our results suggest that, notwithstanding the study to a legal liability context to ascertain
intended benefits of decision aids, auditors who are whether partial utilization of a decision aid (lead-
perceived to utilize them partially may be viewed ing to an unfavourable outcome) amplifies negative
negatively in performance evaluations, even when outcome effects among juries and judges.
those aids are not intended to be deterministic. This With respect to the second hypothesis, we found
tendency could lead auditors to be less receptive to that ambiguity-intolerant auditors were more apt
the implementation of decision aids in their firms. to reduce uncertainty by focusing on an unfavour-
Alternatively, auditors may tend to follow decision able outcome. These results have both theoretical
aids completely even when perceived costs exceed and practical implications for the study of outcome
benefits (Messier, 1995). CPA firms may need to effects on performance evaluation. From a theore-
implement further policies regarding the use of tical standpoint, our study confirms that individual
decision aids. Future research should substantiate differences may affect the underlying cognitive
these claims as well as examine related issues such processes that are at the heart of the hindsight
as the perceived effects of auditors choosing not to phenomenon (Hawkins and Hastie, 1990). An
use firm-designed decision aids or auditors electing increased understanding of the basis for outcome
to override them. effects may assist researchers in developing meth-
ods to mitigate their effects, assuming these effects
While our study has important implications for are perceived to be non- normative. If these
internal performance evaluations, its focus could be mitigation methods prove to be successful, they
extended to external evaluation of auditors in the could be integrated into the performance evalua-
legal arena. Presently, CPA firms can only con- tion task.
jecture ‘how reliance on a decision aid will be Future research could also be directed toward
judged in hindsight’ by judicial litigants (Boatsman confirming our findings that tolerance of ambiguity
et al., 1995, p. 5). Judicial litigants may interpret is a relevant factor in professional judgement
decision aid information (and its adherence) as evaluation. From a practical perspective, evaluators
representative of due diligence. That is, the auditor who are intolerant of ambiguity may be more likely
may not be perceived to have satisfied the profes- to be subject to outcome effects, which may
sional standard implicit in the decision aids if they contribute to negative performance evaluations of
are not fully-utilized. Jennings et al. (1993) surmise subordinates. Therefore, evaluators need to be self
that ‘when standards of auditor conduct are not aware of their tolerance for ambiguity and how this
readily at hand, jurists will use internal decision factor may affect their evaluation of subordinates.
aids as surrogate standards against which to Also, if our findings are replicated in a legal setting,
measure performance.’ Given that auditor liability it could indicate that certain judicial litigants (those
is determined from a perspective that includes intolerant of ambiguity) would be more apt to
outcome information, knowledge of auditor’s focus on a negative outcome. Jury selection (voir
partial utilization of a decision aid may create an dire) procedures could be developed as a means of
incriminating audit trail directly calling into ques- identifying individuals who are intolerant of
tion the due diligence of the auditor (especially ambiguity.
among jurists who are likely to be less knowledge-
Reaction to the evaluatee’s decision aid usage
able of audit practice and less supportive of the
was also affected by subject’s tolerance of ambi-
auditing profession than the auditors participating
guity, as proposed by hypothesis 3. Ambiguity-
in this study). Future research should extend our
tolerant individuals provided higher assessments
when the evaluatee fully utilized the decision aid.
We suggest that these individuals did not fixate on
13
A large contingent of research involving performance outcome knowledge and thus were willing to
evaluation has (by necessity) utilized subjects that were not
directly responsible for evaluating subordinate behaviour.
examine additional information such as the utiliza-
Baron and Hershey (1988) utilized undergraduate students in tion of decision aids. While ambiguity-tolerant
a study that evaluated the competence of medical decisions. individuals displayed normative behaviour in
In another medical study, Mitchell and Kalb (1981) used being thorough in their decision process, they
nurses to take the role of the (nurse) supervisor of poorly may have succumbed to non-normative behaviour
performing subordinates. Brown and Solomon (1987, 1993)
and Lipe (1993), in a managerial context, used undergraduate in their assessments of decision aid usage. For both
and graduate students as subjects. Kaplan and Reckers (1985) the partial and full utilization conditions, the
employed audit seniors to make performance evaluations of auditor performed the same ten analytical proce-
hypothetical audit seniors. dure investigations. However, the partial utiliza-

International Journal of Auditing, 1(1), 43–59 (1997) # 1997 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd
54 D. J. Lowe and P. M. J. Reckers

tion condition was presented against a background supervision by senior internal audit personnel.
of a larger population of explanations (10 of 15). The internal audit staff numbers sixteen, including
Future research is needed to examine these three new college graduates hired last summer.
concepts more fully as these results are exploratory One senior member of the department resigned last
and require replication. year, accepting a position at another firm at a
significant pay increase.

ANALYTICAL PROCEDURES: In performing


mandated analytical procedures for preliminary
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS audit planning, typically several ratios are consid-
Financial support is greatly appreciated for the ered. One significant ratio is inventory turnover
first author from a summer research grant at (cost of sales divided by average inventory).
Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State Uni- Presented below is the set of inventory turnover
versity. We also acknowledge the valuable com- ratios for Springdale:
ments received from participants at the 1995
Annual Meetings of the American Accounting PRIOR YEAR CURRENT YEAR PER
Association and the Society of Judgement and (audited) (unaudited) CHA
Decision Making. INVENTORY TURNOVER 5.47 5.20 ÿ

APPENDIX A The SIGNIFICANT difference between the two


years’ inventory turnover ratios could be the result
Case Instrument of: (a) an environmental change (including normal
CLIENT: This case deals with the external audit of year-to-year variation), or (b) an accounting error
Springdale Industries. Sprindale is a medium-size, or irregularity in the unaudited financial state-
publicly-traded (Pacific Stock Exchange) US cor- ments. Environmental changes include all explana-
poration, founded in 1949. Springdale engages in tions other than accounting errors and
the manufacture of a variety of plastic and irregularities. If the significant difference between
ceramic=glass products for both commercial and the two year’s ratios is caused by an environmental
domestic use. Summarized, unaudited financial explanation, then the financial statements do not
information provided by the client for the current require an adjustment. However, if the difference is
year is provided below: caused by a material error or irregularity, then the
financial statements must be adjusted.
INCOME STATEMENT BALANCE SHEET
DECISION AIDS: Some audit firms have devel-
SALES $48,017,200 oped or are
CASH & SECURITIES developing decision aid packages to
$1,323,600
COST OF SALES 38,379,500 NET RECEIVABLES assist their5,093,300
professionals in conducting analytical
SELLING & ADMIN 3,647,100 INVENTORY procedures. 7,407,500
Commercial software packages also
OTHER EXPENSES 1,672,500 OTHER CURRENT ITEMS
are becoming 333,600
available and are finding increased
INCOME TAXES 2,329,300 LONG TERM ASSETS
adoption 28,527,300
by public accounting firms and internal
NET INCOME $1,988,800 $42,685,300 who choose not to develop their
audit departments
ACCOUNTS PAYABLEown decision aids. Among these decision aids are
7,705,900
diagnostic decision
CURRENT LIABILITIES 990,000 aid packages which provide
LONG TERM LIABILITIES 12,497,300
lists of viable explanations for unusual financial
EQUITY 21,492,100
relationships or perceived significant change in
financial$42,685,300
relationships across time.

INTERNAL AUDIT: The internal audit department AUDIT TEAM: The supervising senior on the
of the client is evaluated to be of MEDIUM quality Springdale engagement is Bill Ward. Bill is an
based on past performance, educational back- aggressive, individual with a sharp focus on career
ground, experience and objectivity. Last year about advancement. Bill is married and has two young
90% of internal audit procedures examined pro- children—both pre-school age. Bill has been with
vided evidence of satisfactory instruction and the firm six years and is being considered for

# 1997 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. International Journal of Auditing, 1(1), 43–59 (1997)
Evaluations of Professional Audit Judgment 55

promotion to manager in the fall. Bill has nearly always had his jobs come in at- or under-budget. He has
exhibited good client relations. The staff consider him a tough senior to work for though. Among the staff
assigned to the Springdale audit are two new juniors and one staff member who served on the audit last
year. Neither Bill nor the audit manager were on the Springdale audit team last year, although the
engagement partner is continuing. The audit fee was renegotiated with the client since the last audit and
reduced. The resulting audit time budget is tight this year.

AUDIT ISSUE: To facilitate the analytical procedures phase of the external audit your firm has developed
and approved for field application analytical procedures decision aid software which generates selected
causal explanations for account=ratio changes. This diagnostic decision aid, however, does not necessarily
include all non-trivial explanations. In developing the software, your firm (the external auditor of
Springdale) reviewed over 2000 audits which the firm performed over the last five years.

In seeking to explain the significant decrease in the inventory turnover ratio, during the course of the
audit, Bill INVESTIGATED ALL 10 OF THE ITEMS [ALTERNATE DECISION AIDS’ VERSION: 10 of
the 15 ITEMS] ON THE ANALYTICAL PROCEDURES DECISION AID LIST (see below).

10 EXPLANATIONS INVESTIGATED BY BILL


Error=Irregularity Explanations:
1. Incorrect test counts of inventory.
2. Unbooked physical adjustments to inventory, including obsolete or unsalable inventory, or unrecorded shrinkage.
3. Incorrect cut-off of inventory, accounts payable, cost of sales, or sales, including unrecorded finished goods or sales,
incorrect beginning inventory, and improper revenue recognition.
4. Misclassified transaction (posted to the wrong account).
5. Transfer pricing errors.

Environmental Explanations:
1. Change in product mix or change in production method, including new process or plant, or change in production due
to new regulation.
2. Change in accounting method.
3. Increase in raw materials inventory costs.
4. Sales down due to defective product(s), bad publicity, strike(s), change in marketing, decrease in advertising, or
increased personnel turnover in sales department.
5. Sales down due to downturn in the economy, change in credit policy, loss of a major customer, change in credit
terms, increase in competition, or technology changes.

ALTERNATE DECISION AIDS VERSION ˆ [ ]

[5 EXPLANATIONS NOT INVESTIGATED BY BILL

Error=Irregularity Explanations
1. Misapplied or miscosted overhead, or misapplication of variances.
2. Error in recording and=or applying direct labor rates to inventory.
3. Consigned goods incorrectly included in inventory.

Environmental Explanations
1. Efficiency problems in production.
2. Inventory up due to poor anticipation of needs or poor ordering practice, resulting in excess inventory.]

None of the 10 items investigated was found to explain the ratio change. Bill believed that the likelihood
of other error (or irregularity) explanations were insignificant. The audit time budget was tight and Bill

International Journal of Auditing, 1(1), 43–59 (1997) # 1997 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd
56 D. J. Lowe and P. M. J. Reckers

did not believe it prudent at the end of the audit to allocate further special attention to the matter, and did
not do so. Other normal audit procedures and tests of samples did not suggest an error basis.

OUTCOME VERSION

Three months after release of the financial statements of Springdale, the correct explanation for the
decrease in the inventory turnover ratio was discovered. The correct explanation was an ERROR, and thus
THE PUBLISHED FINANCIAL STATEMENTS WERE IN ERROR. The correct explanation was an error in
application of labor rates (for manufactured products) to inventory, resulting in an overstatement of
inventory. The firm and its labor union agreed to new wage rates during the audit year effective the
following year. These new rates were prematurely used in calculating the labor portion of the cost of
inventory in the year of the audit.

The Clarkson Group had purchased Springdale stock shortly after publication of the erroneous financial
statements. Following discovery and disclosure of the inventory and net income misstatements
Springdale’s stock price dropped sharply. The Clarkson Group is now suing Springdale and its external
auditors.

APPENDIX B

Tolerance of Ambiguity (MacDonald) Scale


Please answer the following True–False questions. Do not spend too much time on any one question; there
are no right or wrong answers and therefore your first response is important. Mark T for True and F for
False. Be sure to answer every question.

—— 1. A problem has little attraction for me if I don’t think it has a solution.


—— 2. I am just a little uncomfortable with people unless I feel that I can understand their behaviour.
—— 3. There’s a right way and a wrong way to do almost everything.
—— 4. I would rather bet 1 to 6 on a long shot than 3 to 1 on a probable winner.
—— 5. The way to understand complex problems is to be concerned with their larger aspects instead
of breaking them into smaller pieces.
—— 6. I get pretty anxious when I’m in a social situation over which I have no control.
—— 7. Practically every problem has a solution.
—— 8. It bothers me when I am unable to follow another person’s train of thought.
—— 9. I have always felt that there is a clear difference between right and wrong.
—— 10. It bothers me when I don’t know how other people react to me.
—— 11. Nothing gets accomplished in this world unless you stick to some basic rules.
—— 12. If I were a doctor, I would prefer the uncertainties of a psychiatrist to the clear and definite
work of a surgeon or X-ray specialist.
—— 13. Vague and impressionistic pictures really have little appeal for me.
—— 14. If I were a scientist, it would bother me that my work would never be completed (because
science will always make new discoveries).
—— 15. Before an examination, I feel much less anxious if I know how many questions there will be.
—— 16. The best part of working a jigsaw puzzle is putting in the last piece.
—— 17. Sometimes, I rather enjoy going against the rules and doing things I’m not supposed to do.
—— 18. I don’t like to work on a problem unless there is a possibility of coming out with a clear-cut and
unambiguous answer.
—— 19. I like to fool around with new ideas, even if they turn out later to be a total waste of time.
—— 20. Perfect balance is the essence of all good composition.

# 1997 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. International Journal of Auditing, 1(1), 43–59 (1997)
Evaluations of Professional Audit Judgment 57

REFERENCES
Abdolmohammadi, M. J. (1991) Factors affecting auditor’s perceptions of applicable decision aids for various audit
tasks. Contemporary Accounting Research, 7, pp. 535–548.
American Institute of Certified Public Accountants (1988) Analytical procedures. Statement on Auditing Standards
No. 56, New York, AICPA.
Anderson, L. (1986) Is hindsight a fair judge of foresight: an experimental investigation of second-guessing.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Loyola University.
Anderson, J. C., Jennings, M. M., Kaplan, S. E. and Reckers, P. M. J. (1994) The effect of using diagnostic decision aids
of analytical procedures on judges liability judgements. Working Paper, Arizona State University.
Ashton, A. (1985) Does consensus imply accuracy in accounting studies of decision making? The Accounting Review, 60,
pp. 173–185.
Ashton, R. and Willingham, J. (1988) Using and evaluating audit decision aids. In R. P. Srivastava and J. E. Rebele
(Eds), Auditing Symposium IX: Proceedings of the 1988 Touche and Ross=University of Kansas Symposium on Auditing
Problems, University of Kansas, pp. 1–25.
Baron, J. and Hershey, J. C. (1988) Outcome bias in decision evaluation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54,
pp. 569–579.
Blocher, E. and Willingham, J. (1988) Analytical Review: A Guide to Performing Analytical Procedures, McGraw Hill.
Boatsman, J. R., Moeckel, C. L. and Pei, K. W. (1995) The effects of decision consequences on auditors’ reliance on
decision aids in audit planning. Working Paper, Arizona State University.
Brown, C. E. and Solomon, I. (1987) Effects of outcome information on evaluations of managerial decisions. The
Accounting Review, 62, pp. 564–577.
Brown, C. E. and Solomon, I. (1993) An experimental investigation of explanations for outcome effects of appraisals of
capital-budgeting decisions. Contemporary Accounting Research, 10, pp. 84–117.
Budner, S. (1962) Intolerance of ambiguity as a personality variable. Journal of Personality, 30, pp. 29–50.
Campbell, J. D. and Tesser, A. (1983) Motivational interpretations of hindsight bias: an individual difference analysis.
Journal of Personality, 51, pp. 605–620.
Casper, J. D., Benedict, K. and Perry, J. L. (1989) Juror decision making, attitudes, and the hindsight bias. Law and
Human Behaviour, 13, pp. 291–310.
Christensen-Szalanski, J. J. and Fobian-Willham, C. S. (1991) The hindsight bias: a meta-analysis. Organizational
Behaviour and Human Decision Processes, 48, pp. 147–168.
Cushing, B. and Loebbecke, J. K. (1986) Comparison of Audit Methodologies of Large Accounting Firms. Studies in
Accounting Research No. 26, American Accounting Association.
Davies, M. F. (1992) Field dependence and hindsight bias: cognitive restructuring and the generation of reasons.
Journal of Research in Personality, 26, pp. 58–74.
Dellarosa, D. and Bourne, L. (1984) Decisions and memory: differential retrievability of consistent and contradictory
evidence. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 23, pp. 669–682.
Duncan, R. (1972) Characteristics of organizational environments and perceived environmental uncertainty.
Administrative Science Quarterly, 17, pp. 313–327.
Einhorn, H. J. and Hogarth, R. M. (1985) Ambiguity and uncertainty in probabilistic inference. Psychological Review, 92,
pp. 433–461.
Ellis, P. (1983) Expert systems—a key innovation in professional and managerial problem solving. Information Age,
January, pp. 2–6.
Fischhoff, B. (1982) Debiasing. In D. Kahneman, P. Slovic and A. Tversky (Eds). Judgement Under Uncertainty: Heuristics
and Biases, Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
Fisher, J. and Selling, T. I. (1993) The outcome effect in performance evaluation: decision process observability and
consensus. Behavioural Research in Accounting, 5, pp. 58–77.
Frenkel-Brunswick, E. (1949) Intolerance of ambiguity as an emotional and perceptual personality variable. Journal of
Personality, 18, pp. 108–143.
Ghosh, D. and Whitecotten, S. M. (1996) Analysts’ problem-solving ability, earnings forecasts and decision aids.
Working Paper, University of Oklahoma.
Gournic, S. J. (1994) Professional standards and hindsight bias: implications for determinations of negligence.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, SUNY-Buffalo.
Graham, L. J., Damens, J. and Van Ness, G. (1991) Developing risk advisor: an expert system for risk identification.
Auditing: A Journal of Practice and Theory, 10, pp. 69–96.
Greenhaus, J. and Parasuraman, S. (1993) Job performance attributions and career advancement prospects: an
examination of gender and race effects. Organizational Behaviour and Human Decision Processes, July, pp. 273– 297.
Gul, F. A. (1984) The joint and moderating role of personality and cognitive style on decision making. The Accounting
Review, 59, pp. 264–277.
Hawkins, S. A. and Hastie, R. (1990) Hindsight: biased judgements of past events after the outcomes are known.
Psychological Bulletin, 107, pp. 311–327.
Helleloid, R. T. (1985) Hindsight bias and judgements by tax professionals. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, The

International Journal of Auditing, 1(1), 43–59 (1997) # 1997 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd
58 D. J. Lowe and P. M. J. Reckers

University of Minnesota. 612.


Hershey, J. C. and Baron, J. (1992) Judgement by Pincus, K. V. (1990) Auditor individual differences and
outcomes: when is it justified? Organizational Behaviour fairness of presentation judgements. Auditing: A Journal
and Human Decision Processes, 53, pp. 89–93. of Practice and Theory, 9, pp. 150–166.
Hoch, S. J. and Loewenstein, G. F. (1989) Outcome Pratt, J. (1980) The effects of personality on a subject’s
feedback: hindsight and information. Journal of Experi- information processing: a comment. The Accounting
mental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, Review, 55, pp. 501–506.
pp. 605–619. Reckers, P. M. J., Sanders, D. L. and Roark, S. J. (1994) The
Hooks, K. (1992) Gender effects and labor supply in influence of ethical attitudes on taxpayer compliance.
public accounting: an agenda of research issues. The National Tax Journal, 18, pp. 825–836.
Accounting, Organizations and Society, 17, pp. 343–366. Reckers, P. M. J. and Schultz, J. J. (1993) The effects of
Hunt, S. C. (1995) A review and synthesis of research in fraud signals, evidence order, and group-assisted
performance evaluation in public accounting. Journal of counsel on independent auditor judgement. Behaviour-
Accounting Literature, 14, pp. 107–139. al Research in Accounting, 5, pp. 124–144.
Hunt, S. C. and Messier, W. F. (1995) Auditor perfor- Rydell, S. T. and Rosen, E. (1966) Measurement and some
mance evaluation: factors affecting information search correlates of need cognition. Psychological Reports, 19,
and the rating decision. Working Paper, University of pp. 139–165.
North Texas. Schkade, D. A. and Kilbourne, L. M. (1991) Expectation-
Janoff-Bulman, R., Timko, C. and Carli, L. L. (1985) outcome consistency and hindsight bias. Organizational
Cognitive biases in blaming thhe victim. Journal of Behaviour and Human Decision Processes, 49, pp. 105–123.
Experimental Social Psychology, 21, pp. 161–177. Slovic, P. and Fischhoff, B. (1977) On the psychology of
Jennings, M. M., Kneer, D. and Reckers, P. M. J. (1993) The experimental surprises. Journal of Experimental Psychol-
significance of audit decision aids and pre-case jurists’ ogy: Human Perception and Performance, 3, pp. 544–551.
attitudes on perceptions of audit firm culpability. Tabor, R. and Willis, J. (1985) Empirical evidence on the
Contemporary Accounting Research, 10, pp. 489–507. changing role of analytical review procedures. Audit-
Kadous, K. (1996) Juror decision making in professional ing: A Journal of Theory and Practice, 4, pp. 93–109.
cases: a model and tests in the audit litigation setting. Voss, J. F., Vesonder, G. T. and Spilich, G. J. (1980) Test
Working Paper, University of Waterloo. generation and recall by high-knowledge and low-
Kaplan, S. E. and Reckers, P. M. J. (1985) An examination knowledge individuals. Journal of Verbal Learning and
of auditor performance evaluation. The Accounting Verbal Behaviour, 19, pp. 651–667.
Review, 60, pp. 477–487. Willingham, J. J. (1986) Audit research and its role at Peat
Kida, T. E. (1984) Performance evaluation and review Marwick. Internal Memorandum, Peat Marwick, Main,
meeting characteristics in public accounting firms. and Co.
Accounting, Organizations and Society, 9, pp. 137–147. Zebda, A. (1991) The problem of ambiguity and vague-
Koonce, L. (1993) A cognitive characterization of audit ness in accounting. Behavioural Research in Accounting,
analytical review. Auditing: A Journal of Practice and 3, pp. 117–145.
Theory, 12, pp. 57– 76.
Kripke, H. (1988) Some reactions and some down-to-
earth reflections on accountants’ liability—a general
discussion. Journal of Accounting, Auditing, and Finance,
3, pp. 359–367.
Lehman, C. R. (1988) Accounting ethics: surviving AUTHOR PROFILES
survival of the fittest. Advances in Public Interest
Accounting, 2, pp. 71–82. D. Jordan Lowe is an Assistant Professor of
Lipe, M. G. (1993) Analyzing the variance investigation Accounting and John E. Peterson, Jr. Research
decision: the effects of outcomes, mental accounting, Fellow at Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State
and framing. The Accounting Review, 68, pp. 748–764. University. He teaches undergraduate auditing and
Lowe, D. J. and Reckers, P. M. J. (1994) The effects of researches behavioural issues in auditing. His
hindsight bias on jurors’ evaluations of auditor research has been published in Accounting
decisions. Decision Sciences, 25, pp. 401–426.
MacDonald, Jr. A. P. (1970) Revised scale for ambiguity Horizons, Auditing: A Journal of Practice and Theory,
tolerance: reliability and validity. Psychological Reports, Decision Sciences, and Journal of Economic Psychology.
26, pp. 791– 798. Philip Reckers is Professor and Director of the
Messier, W. F. (1995) Research in and development of School of Accountancy and Information Manage-
audit decision aids. In R. H. Ashton and A. H. Ashton ment at Arizona State University. He is editor of
(Eds). Judgement and Decision Making in Accounting and Advances in Accounting and serves on the editorial
Auditing, Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University
Press.
boards of Auditing: A Journal of Practice and Theory
Mitchell, T. R. and Kalb, L. S. (1981) Effects of outcome and Behavioural Research in Accounting, as well as
knowledge and outcome valence on supervisors’ the International Journal of Auditing. He is author of
evaluations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 66, pp. 604– Intermediate Accounting (West Publishing Co., 1995).

# 1997 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. International Journal of Auditing, 1(1), 43–59 (1997)

View publication stats

You might also like