You are on page 1of 9

J Autism Dev Disord (2010) 40:1179–1187

DOI 10.1007/s10803-010-0977-z

ORIGINAL PAPER

Dysfunctional Attitudes and Perfectionism and Their Relationship


to Anxious and Depressive Symptoms in Boys with Autism
Spectrum Disorders
Rebecca Greenaway • Patricia Howlin

Published online: 25 February 2010


Ó Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2010

Abstract In spite of increasing interest in cognitive Introduction


behaviour therapy for emotional disorders in children with
autism spectrum disorders (ASD), little research has There is an increasing literature demonstrating higher
explored the relevance of the cognitive model in this levels of anxious and depressive symptoms in children with
population. This study explores dysfunctional attitudes and autism spectrum disorders (ASD) compared to typically
perfectionism in boys with ASD and the relationship with developing children (e.g. Farrugia and Hudson 2006;
anxious and depressive symptoms. Compared to a typically Meyer et al. 2006) and children with other developmental
developing group (n = 42), boys with ASD (n = 41) or learning disabilities (e.g. Brereton et al. 2006; Gillott
endorsed more dysfunctional attitudes and reported higher et al. 2001). Severe depression and anxiety are clinically
emotional symptoms. The relationship between emotional debilitating disorders that have a substantial impact on
and cognitive variables was weak in both groups, although young people (Kessler et al. 1995; Van Ameringen et al.
in the ASD group dysfunctional attitudes were significantly 2003). Anxious and depressive symptoms are also likely to
associated with reported obsessive–compulsive symptoms. exacerbate core ASD symptoms such as social withdrawal
Reasons for elevated dysfunctional attitudes in the ASD and repetitive behaviours (Ghaziuddin et al. 2002). For
group are discussed and the roles of cognitive inflexibility example, increased anxiety is thought to amplify social
and social impairments are explored. inappropriateness, repetitive questioning, slowness, ritual-
ized behaviour, aggression, social withdrawal and irrita-
Keywords Autism spectrum disorders (ASD)  bility (Tantam 2003).
Anxiety  Depression  Dysfunctional attitudes  In spite of the high prevalence of anxiety and depression
Perfectionism  Perseveration in individuals with ASD, there has been little research into
interventions for these comorbid disorders (Anderson and
Morris 2006; Ghaziuddin et al. 2002). Cognitive behaviour
therapy (CBT) is recommended by the National Institute
for Health and Clinical Excellence (2005) for the treatment
of depression in young people and there is increasing
The work reported here was completed by the first author in partial evidence of the value of CBT for treating anxiety disorders
fulfilment of the requirements for the award of Doctorate in Clinical in typically developing young people (James et al. 2005).
Psychology. Whilst there are currently no published studies of CBT for
depression in young people with ASD, there has been a
R. Greenaway  P. Howlin
Department of Psychology, Institute of Psychiatry, recent proliferation in studies investigating the effective-
King’s College London, London, UK ness of CBT for anxiety in this population (Chalfant et al.
2007; Ooi et al. 2008; Reaven et al. 2009; Sofronoff et al.
R. Greenaway (&)
2005; White et al. 2009; Wood et al. 2009). These studies
The Wolfson Neurodisability Service, Great Ormond Street
Hospital, 10th Floor, Nurses Homes, London WC1N 3JH, UK have tended to report significant improvements in anxiety
e-mail: r.greenaway@ich.ucl.ac.uk compared to control groups. However, the extent and

123
1180 J Autism Dev Disord (2010) 40:1179–1187

nature of the cognitive components in these interventions is correlate with depression and anxiety in typically develop-
not clear. Thus, although most of these studies include ing children (Hewitt et al. 2002) and can be addressed in
educational (e.g. affect recognition) and behavioural (e.g. CBT (Antony and Swinson 1998).
relaxation and exposure) components, the few that do To our knowledge, there are no published studies of
mention cognitive restructuring do not elaborate further on dysfunctional attitudes and perfectionism in people with
how this was carried out, nor do they discuss the cognitions ASD although anecdotally they are often described as
that were addressed. Furthermore, studies that have demonstrating perfectionism (e.g. Bolton et al. 1994). It
explored factors relating to depression and anxiety in ASD has also been suggested that the cognitive inflexibility
have tended to focus on social rather than cognitive factors often observed in these individuals might cause them to
(e.g. Bellini 2004; Green et al. 2000). Therefore, little hold too strongly to non-functional beliefs (Gaus 2007).
research has explored the relevance of the cognitive model Inflexibility has also been linked to perfectionism in the
to anxiety and depression in children with ASD. As a result typical adult literature (e.g. Shafran and Mansell 2001).
there is currently limited theoretical basis for the use of Studies exploring the cognitive profile in ASD have often
cognitive techniques in psychological interventions for reported greater cognitive inflexibility (Hill 2004) and
comorbid emotional disorders in children with ASD. therefore we might expect higher levels of dysfunctional
In recent years, a small number of studies exploring the and perfectionistic attitudes in boys with ASD.
relationship between cognitive variables and depression and
anxiety in children with ASD has been published (Barnhill Aims of the Study
and Myles 2001; Farrugia and Hudson 2006; Hedley and
Young 2006; Meyer et al. 2006). For example, Meyer et al. The principal aim of this study was to explore whether
(2006) investigated social-cognitive factors related to dysfunctional and/or perfectionistic attitudes are elevated
emotional and behavioural difficulties in young people with in children with ASD compared to typically developing
Asperger syndrome. They found parent-reported (but not children and to explore whether these are related to anxious
self-reported) child anxiety was associated with social and depressive symptoms. A secondary aim of the study
encoding errors and hostile intent attributions. These studies was to explore whether dysfunctional and/or perfectionistic
offer preliminary evidence that cognitive factors may be attitudes are related to cognitive inflexibility.
important in understanding anxiety and depression in indi- The following hypotheses were explored:
viduals with ASD and may have significant implications for
successful intervention. However, the focus has tended to Primary Hypotheses
be on social cognition and attributional styles rather than
deeper level dysfunctional beliefs that have formed the core 1. Children with ASD will show higher levels of anxious
of Beck’s (1967) cognitive model. and depressive symptoms compared to typically
Dysfunctional attitudes are typically excessively rigid developing children.
and perfectionistic, for example ‘‘if I do not succeed at 2. Children with ASD will show higher levels of
everything I am a complete failure’’. Research with typically dysfunctional and perfectionistic beliefs compared to
developing young people has found evidence that high typically developing children.
levels of dysfunctional attitudes are a risk factor for sub- 3. Dysfunctional attitudes and clinical perfectionism will
sequent depressive symptoms (e.g. D’Alessandro and Bur- be associated with anxious and depressive symptoms
ton 2006; Lewinsohn et al. 2001). One form of dysfunctional in both groups.
belief that might be particularly pertinent to individuals with
ASD is clinical perfectionism. In relation to obsessive
Secondary Hypothesis
compulsive disorder in typically developing individuals,
perfectionism has been defined as ‘‘the tendency to believe
4. Cognitive inflexibility will relate to higher levels of
there is a perfect solution to every problem, that doing
dysfunctional and perfectionistic attitudes.
something perfectly (i.e. mistake-free) is not only possible,
but also necessary, and that even minor mistakes will have
serious consequences’’ (Obsessive Compulsive Cognitions
Working Group 1997, p. 678). Shafran et al. (2002) offer a Methods
cognitive–behavioural definition of perfectionism ‘‘the
overdependence of self-evaluation on the determined pur- Participants
suit of personally demanding, self-imposed, standards in at
least one highly salient domain, despite adverse conse- The ASD group consisted of 41 boys aged 11–14 years
quences’’ (p. 778). Perfectionist beliefs have been found to (range 11.5–14.8 years; mean 13.2 years). All had an IQ

123
J Autism Dev Disord (2010) 40:1179–1187 1181

above 80 (range 80–125, mean 100.0) and had previously high-functioning autism and Asperger syndrome (e.g.
received a diagnosis of Asperger syndrome or high-func- Chalfant et al. 2007; Sofronoff et al. 2005). In the current
tioning autism from child psychiatrists, psychologists or study, internal consistency for the SCAS was good in both
paediatricians. To ensure the validity of the initial diag- groups (child-report; ASD, a = .84; TD, a = .92; parent-
nosis, parents of participants in the ASD group completed report; ASD, a = .93; TD, a = .88). Internal consistency
the Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ; Berument was lower for the subscale scores, although predominantly
et al. 1999; Rutter et al. 2003). A cut-off of 15 is suggested acceptable. The physical injury subscale had especially
as optimal for differentiating pervasive developmental poor internal consistency (ASD, a = .18; TD, a = .40),
disorder (PDD) and non-PDD diagnoses (Berument et al. therefore this subscale score was not entered into statistical
1999). In the current study SCQ scores in the ASD group analyses.
ranged from 18 to 37, with a mean of 25.84. This is similar The Children’s Depression Inventory (CDI; Kovacs
to the mean (24.23) reported by Berument et al. for a group 1992) is one of the most widely used and best-studied self-
of high-functioning young people with autism. report measures of childhood depression and has been used
The control group consisted of 42 typically developing as a measure of depression in young people with Asperger
boys (TD group) group-matched on age (range 11.8–14.4, syndrome (e.g. Barnhill and Myles 2001; Hedley and
mean 13.2 years) and intellectual functioning (Full Scale Young 2006). The young people completed the self-report
IQ on Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence range short-form (CDI-S) and parents completed the parent-ver-
85–133, mean 103.9). The SCQ was not given to parents of sion (CDI-P; Kovacs 1992). In the present study, coeffi-
young people in the TD group because of concerns that the cient alpha was acceptable for the CDI-S (ASD, a = .75;
questions would be inappropriate for typically developing TD, a = .72) and good for the CDI-P (ASD, a = .85; TD,
children of this age and might dissuade parents and their a = .83).
children from completing the other measures. Boys with The Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale for Children (DAS-C,
ASD were recruited via special schools as many higher D’Alessandro and Burton 2006) is an adaptation of the
ability children with a formal diagnosis of ASD in the UK Dysfunctional Attitude Scale (Weissman and Beck 1978)
continue to be educated in specialist provision and educa- and is one of the most extensively used measures of cog-
tion in such settings does not imply significant behavioural nitive vulnerability relating to depression. Internal reli-
or learning difficulties. The typically developing boys were ability in the current study was good (ASD, a = .90; TD,
recruited via mainstream schools. In both groups, teachers a = .85).
were asked to confirm that all the boys were working The Child and Adolescent Perfectionism Scale (CAPS;
within the average range for their age group and were not Flett et al. 2000) is a self-report measure of perfectionism
suspected of having any degree of intellectual impairment. with two subscales: self-oriented perfectionism and
The groups did not differ significantly in age (t = .13, socially-prescribed perfectionism. In the current study,
df = 81, p = .899) or Full Scale IQ (t = -1.70, df = 80, alpha coefficients indicated good internal reliability (ASD,
p = .093). a = .82; TD, a = .86).

Measures Cognitive Flexibility

Intellectual Ability The Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST; Heaton 1981) is
the most extensively used measure of cognitive flexibility
All the young people completed the Wechsler Abbreviated in autism and has been found to be highly reliable over
Scale of Intelligence (WASI) (Psychological Corporation time in this population (Ozonoff 1995). The shorter Wis-
1999; two-subtest version). consin Card Sorting Test (WCST-64; Kongs et al. 2000)
was employed in this study. Participants are instructed to
Questionnaires match a series of 64 cards to a target according to a non-
explicit sorting rule which changes during the course of the
The boys also completed the self-report Spence Children’s test. The standard score for perseverative errors was the
Anxiety Scale (SCAS, Spence 1997), whilst their parents measure of interest in the current study.
completed the parent version of this scale (SCAS-P, Nauta
et al. 2004). This yields a total anxiety score, as well as six Procedure
subscale scores: generalised anxiety disorder, obsessive
compulsive disorder, specific phobia, panic and agora- Written consent was obtained from all participants and
phobia, separation anxiety disorder and social anxiety. The their parents. Participants completed the assessment in two
scale has been previously used with young people with 30-min sessions at their school; young people in both

123
1182 J Autism Dev Disord (2010) 40:1179–1187

groups completed the WASI and WCST and four ques- on socially-prescribed perfectionism (U = 486.0, padj =
tionnaires (SCAS, CDI-S, DAS-C and CAPS). To control .086, d = .46). However, following Hochberg adjustment,
for reading difficulties, all instructions and questionnaire this difference no longer met significance.
items were presented verbally as well as visually. Parents
were posted questionnaire packs containing the SCAS-P Dysfunctional Attitudes and Clinical Perfectionism will be
and the CDI-P. Additionally, parents of children in the Associated with Anxious and Depressive Symptoms in Both
ASD group were also asked to complete the SCQ. Groups

Correlations between dysfunctional and perfectionistic


Results attitudes and child-reported anxious and depressive symp-
toms are shown in Table 2. The only significant correlation
Preliminary Analyses in the ASD group was between obsessive compulsive
symptoms and dysfunctional attitudes (rho = .42, df = 39,
Several of the factors were skewed in distribution and padj = .036). When partial correlation was used to control
outliers were present. Non-parametric, two-tailed analyses for age and IQ the strength of the association increased
were employed and corrections for multiple comparisons (rho = .52, df = 37, p = .001). Thus, those children
and correlations were made using Hochberg’s Multiple reporting higher obsessive compulsive symptoms endorsed
Test Procedure (Hochberg 1988). Statistical adjustments more dysfunctional attitudes. There was also a medium-
were made according to the number of tests per scale and sized association between measures of obsessive compul-
are denoted by padj in the text. The significance level was sive symptoms and perfectionism in the ASD group,
set at p \ .05. Strength of associations were interpreted although this no longer met significance following Hoch-
according to the criteria of Cohen (1992; r = .10, small; berg adjustment.
r = .30, medium; r = .50, large). Unless stated, correla- In the TD group there were medium-sized correlations
tions were not substantially affected by partialling out age between measures of perfectionism and panic/agoraphobia
and IQ. There was no significant relationship between and between perfectionism and separation anxiety, but
child- and parent-report on the CDI (ASD, rho = .007; again these no longer met significance following adjust-
TD, rho = .138) or the SCAS (ASD, rho = .180; TD, ment for multiple correlations.
rho = .251), suggesting that each type of report may pro- Overall, the correlations between emotional and cogni-
vide unique information. Thus scores from both sources tive measures tended to be positive but weak. Dysfunc-
were employed separately in the following analyses. tional attitudes and child-reported depression did not
correlate significantly in either group. Similarly, there were
Primary Hypotheses no significant associations between perfectionism or dys-
functional attitudes and parent-reported anxious and
Children with ASD will Show Higher Levels of Anxious and depressive symptoms.
Depressive Symptoms Compared to Typically Developing
Children Secondary Hypothesis

The ASD group scored significantly higher than the TD Cognitive Inflexibility will Relate to Higher Levels of
group on child- and parent-reported SCAS and CDI total Dysfunctional and Perfectionistic Attitudes
scores, see Table 1. The ASD group also scored signifi-
cantly higher on all SCAS subscale scores, except child- In line with previous research, the ASD group made more
reported social anxiety (U = 763.0, ns). perseverative errors on the WCST than the TD group
(U = 584.5, p = .012, d = -.58). The CAPS, DAS-C and
Children with ASD will Show Higher Levels of WCST perseveration standard scores did not violate
Dysfunctional and Perfectionistic Beliefs Compared to assumptions of normality, thus Pearson’s product-moment
Typically Developing Children correlation was used in these analyses. In the ASD group,
those children who were more perseverative on the WCST
The ASD group scored higher than the TD group on the endorsed more perfectionistic beliefs (r = -.403, df = 38,
DAS-C and CAPS although this difference only reached padj = .020) and dysfunctional attitudes (r = -.327,
significance for the former. Further analysis of the sub- df = 39, padj = .037). After controlling for age and IQ
scales from the CAPS indicated that whilst both groups effects, there remained a significant correlation between
scored similarly on self-oriented perfectionism, the ASD perseveration and perfectionism (r = -.320, df = 37, p =
group tended to rate themselves higher than the TD group .050), but not between perseveration and dysfunctional

123
J Autism Dev Disord (2010) 40:1179–1187 1183

Table 1 Group differences on emotional and cognitive measures


ASD group TD group Group difference
Median 25th–75th quartile Median 25th–75th quartile Statistic padj

SCAS child 25.0 20.0–38.0 13.0 10.0–21.0 u = 388 \.001


SCAS parent 31.0 20.0–48.0 9.5 5.0–17.8 u = 167 \.001
CDI-S child 49.0 43.0–53.0 43.0 43.0–49.0 u = 648 =.049
CDI parent 57.0 52.0–66.0 44.0 39.0–49.0 u = 200 \.001
DAS-C 52.0 41.0–66.5 44.5 39.8–54.3 u = 599 =.034
CAPS self 32.5 26.3–36.0 33.0 28.0–37.0 u = 760 =.457
CAPS social 26.0 20.5–29.8 22.5 17.0–28.0 u = 623 =.086
WCST pers 101.0 86.0–116.0 110.0 96.0–135.0 u = 585 =.012
Note: SCAS, Spence Children’s Anxiety Scale, total score (parent and child report shown); CDI-S, Children’s Depression Inventory, short form.
CDI parent, Children’s Depression Inventory, parent form; DAS-C, Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale for Children; CAPS, Child and Adolescent
Perfectionism Scale (self-oriented and socially-prescribed subscale scores shown); WCST pers, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test standard score for
perseverative errors

Table 2 Correlations between dysfunctional attitudes/perfectionism and child-reported anxious and depressive symptoms
ASD group (n = 41) TD group (n = 42)
Dysfunctional attitudes Perfectionism Dysfunctional attitudes Perfectionism

Spearman r
CDI-S .21 .16 .17 -.01
SCAS .21 .12 .09 .24
Obsessive–compulsive .42** .34* .06 .30
Panic/agoraphobia .11 .02 .18 .38*
Social phobia -.07 .01 .26 .05
Separation anxiety .16 .07 .13 .31*
Generalised anxiety .06 -.03 .02 .08
Note: CDI-S, Children’s Depression Inventory, short form; SCAS, Spence Children’s Anxiety Scale
* Significant at the p \ .05 level prior to adjustment
** Significant at the p \ .05 level after Hochberg adjustment

attitudes (r = -.176, df = 37, p = .289). In the typically on the social reciprocal interaction domain of the SCQ and
developing group perseveration did not correlate sig- socially-prescribed perfectionism on the CAPS was
nificantly with perfectionism (r = -.054, df = 40, p = examined. Those children with ASD whose parents rated
.736) nor dysfunctional attitudes (r = -.028, df = 40, them as having more severe social interaction impairments
p = .860). on the SCQ endorsed more socially-prescribed perfec-
tionistic beliefs (rho = .33, df = 35, p = .050).
Additional Research Question

Does Higher Socially-Prescribed Perfectionism in ASD Discussion


Relate to Impairments in Reciprocal Social Interaction in
ASD? The study aimed to explore dysfunctional attitudes and
perfectionism and their relationship to anxious and
It was interesting to find that whilst both groups scored depressive symptoms in boys with ASD. Consistent with
similarly on self-oriented perfectionism, the ASD group previous research, scores on measures of anxious and
scored higher than the TD group on socially-prescribed depressive symptoms were higher in the ASD group
perfectionism. Therefore, an additional research question compared to the TD group. The ASD group also endorsed
was explored—whether higher socially-prescribed perfec- more dysfunctional and socially-prescribed perfectionistic
tionism in the ASD group might relate to social interaction beliefs than the TD group. In the ASD group, children who
impairments in ASD. The relationship between the score endorsed more dysfunctional attitudes also reported higher

123
1184 J Autism Dev Disord (2010) 40:1179–1187

levels of obsessive–compulsive symptoms. Overall, the However, whilst some of the items on the scales used relate
correlations between cognitive distortions and emotional to social performance, the majority cover more general
measures were weak and inconsistent, although there was perfectionistic performance standards and rigid ideas about
no evidence to indicate that the cognitive measures were the world. Thus, it is unlikely that the elevated scores in
more strongly related to anxiety and depression in the TD dysfunctional attitudes and socially-prescribed perfection-
group compared to the ASD group. ism can be ascribed entirely to social failure. Moreover, the
The ASD group endorsed more dysfunctional attitudes boys with ASD in this study all attended specialist provi-
than the TD group. There was also a greater tendency for sion and thus are likely to have been in more ‘‘ASD
the ASD group to endorse statements indicating that others friendly’’ and hence less stressful environments than many
set high standards for them, whilst both groups showed other children with this condition.
similar levels of self-oriented perfectionism. The evidence In contrast to some other studies of typically developing
also indicated that cognitive inflexibility was associated children (e.g. Lewinsohn et al. 2001), the relationship
with greater endorsement of these beliefs in the ASD between reported emotional symptoms and cognitive dis-
group. As there is no published research on perfectionism tortions was generally weak. In particular there was no
or dysfunctional attitudes in individuals with ASD, it is significant correlation between scores for dysfunctional
only possible to speculate about these findings. Cognitive attitudes and depression in either group. The only signifi-
inflexibility might lead to rules being applied in an overly cant correlation between child-reported anxiety and dys-
rigid way by children with ASD, even in the face of con- functional attitudes was for the obsessive–compulsive
tradictory evidence. Thus, cognitive inflexibility might subscale in the ASD group. Dysfunctional attitudes have
explain, in part, the higher levels of dysfunctional and been theoretically linked to depression rather than anxiety
perfectionistic attitudes in the ASD group. Furthermore, and few studies have examined the relationship between
difficulty interpreting social information might lead indi- dysfunctional attitudes and anxiety disorders. Although not
viduals with ASD to overestimate others’ expectations. theoretically related, evidence in the typical adult literature
There was some evidence for this in the present study as a reported a relationship between obsessive compulsive dis-
higher score for impairments in reciprocal social interac- order (OCD) and dysfunctional attitudes (Vogel et al.
tion in the ASD group was associated with higher socially- 2000). Therefore, it is possible that dysfunctional attitudes
prescribed perfectionism. A further possibility is that the play an important role in OCD in both typical and ASD
pragmatic language deficits that are typically observed in populations.
individuals with ASD (Baron-Cohen 1988), might lead
children to interpret comments about others’ expectations Limitations and Future Research
(e.g. ‘‘I want you to give 110%’’) in an over-literal way.
Pragmatic language skills were not, however, assessed in The weak or absent relationships between emotional and
the current study. cognitive factors may be due to a small sample. A study of
Thus far, dysfunctional attitudes and clinical perfec- dysfunctional attitudes among 94 depressed adolescent
tionism have been considered to be disadvantageous cog- outpatients found that whilst dysfunctional attitudes were
nitive styles. However, it is also possible that these traits associated with more severe depression, cognitive distor-
have advantages for children with autism, for example, in tions were not universal across the depressed group
simplifying social expectations and offering a framework (Marton and Kutcher 1995); thus the association may only
for social understanding. Furthermore, it should be ques- be consistently detected among larger sample sizes. A
tioned whether these higher scores on measures of dys- further reason why few significant relationships between
functional attitudes and socially-prescribed perfectionism cognitive and emotional factors were identified might be
actually represent distorted thinking or whether they reflect the use of a community rather than a clinic sample. In the
the harsh social reality faced by many individuals with typically developing group there was only a limited range
ASD. It could be argued that people do, in fact, set overly of depressive symptomatology reported, with the majority
high standards for individuals with ASD who are of normal scoring in or below the average range for depressive
or above average IQ, and that the consequences of not symptoms.
meeting these expectations can be very negative. For There were differences between the special schools
example, many children with ASD are expected to attend where the young people with ASD were placed and the
mainstream school, a highly complex and demanding mainstream schools from where the typically developing
social environment, and to interact successfully with their children were recruited. All but one of the special schools
typically developing and socially much more sophisticated was independent (although pupils were typically funded by
peers. If their attempts at social interaction fail this can their LEA) whilst all mainstream schools were state-fun-
frequently lead to rejection and ridicule (Attwood 1998). ded. The special schools were smaller than the mainstream

123
J Autism Dev Disord (2010) 40:1179–1187 1185

schools and, unlike the mainstream schools, all but one had Children with autism often present with different fears
boarding provision. These differences in the schools compared to typically developing children (Howlin 1998)
attended by the two groups is likely to impact on their day- and there may be overlap between symptoms of depression
to-day lives and the findings, particularly for the ASD and features of ASD (e.g. social withdrawal, impaired sleep
group, might not be replicated in pupils attending less patterns) that might be difficult to tease apart when relying
specialized (and hence less supportive) integrated provi- solely on questionnaire measures. It is also important to
sion. The schools were also all in predominantly white note that children with ASD may have difficulty reporting
middle-class areas and the study aimed to target a fairly their emotions, or fully understanding the concepts mea-
homogenous sample, which limits the generalisability of sured by standard questionnaires. The boys with ASD in
the results. the current study appeared to have little difficulty com-
Detailed diagnostic assessments were not conducted on pleting the self-report anxiety and depression measures,
the boys in the ASD group; therefore classification was although we have no data on whether their interpretation of
based on previous diagnoses given by a variety of clinical the questions is the same as for typically developing peers.
teams. The SCQ was, however, used as an additional Future studies may benefit from the use of clinical inter-
screening tool in the ASD group and all scored within the views to assess emotional symptoms and the clinical
clinical range for PDD. No ASD assessments were used in impact of these.
the TD group, so we cannot be sure that any of the boys in
this group did not have an undiagnosed ASD. However, Strengths of the Study
neither parents nor teachers reported any suspected neu-
rodevelopmental difficulties in the TD group. In spite of these limitations, the study also benefits from
A further limitation of the study is the use of typically several strengths. Firstly, it is unique in measuring dys-
developing boys as a comparison group as, by nature of the functional attitudes and perfectionism in relation to
condition, development in boys with ASD is likely to fol- depression and anxiety in children with ASD. Secondly, the
low a different trajectory. A typically developing com- participants comprised a well-defined group of boys with
parison group was chosen because CBT techniques for ASD who were closely matched with the typically devel-
emotional disorders in ASD are extrapolated from the oping comparison group. Diagnoses from school reports
typically developing literature. In future studies in this area were validated by parental screening questionnaire and
the inclusion of a comparison group with a different neu- level of intellectual functioning was established by both
rodevelopmental disorder may provide additional infor- teacher report and an IQ test. Finally, a further strength of
mation, although, of course, how to select the most the study was the use of both child- and parent-report
appropriate comparison group remains an issue of debate. emotion measures.
Whilst efforts were made to keep the groups as
homogenous and as closely matched as possible, there Clinical Implications
remained variability in IQ scores within each group, with
some participants scoring in the low average and some in This study provides further evidence of higher than normal
the above average range. Furthermore, the ASD group levels of anxious and depressive symptoms in a community
contained boys who had been diagnosed with Asperger sample of children with ASD, with the data supported by
syndrome or high-functioning autism. No attempt was both child- and parent-report. This highlights the necessity
made to explore subgroups within the ASD group owing to of developing appropriate interventions for anxious and
the relatively small group size and because the ASD depressive disorders in ASD populations. There is also a
diagnoses came from a range of clinical teams. In addition, need to determine the accuracy of standardised measures of
it is now increasingly argued that there is insufficient emotional distress in this group and at the very least
research evidence reliably to distinguish between these two research and clinical work with high-functioning children
(e.g. Sanders 2009). with ASD should collect both child- and parent-report to
It is also important to note that the correlational nature gain a fuller understanding of the individual’s symptoms.
of the study means that causal interpretations cannot be The ASD group tended to show inflated socially-pre-
made; there are no data on the directionality of the rela- scribed perfectionism, which in turn was associated with
tionship or the influence of other potential variables. Future reported impairments in reciprocal social interaction.
studies would benefit from the use of prospective designs Given the potentially negative impact of perfectionism
to develop a better understanding of the factors that make on psychopathology (Shafran and Mansell 2001) and
individuals with ASD vulnerable to anxiety and depression. functioning (e.g. procrastination and obsessional slow-
Finally, the questionnaires used in this study have not ness), adults may need to exercise caution in the use
been standardised for use with individuals with ASD. of achievement-related comments, which although to

123
1186 J Autism Dev Disord (2010) 40:1179–1187

typically developing children may be encouraging, by related to OCD symptoms in the ASD group and there was
children with ASD may be interpreted either too rigidly or evidence that cognitive inflexibility and social impairments
literally (e.g. ‘‘you must always do your best’’). in ASD might relate to some aspects of distorted beliefs.
The current study suggests that OCD symptoms in Further research is needed to demonstrate the relevance of
children with ASD are associated with cognitive distor- cognitive models to the treatment of emotional problems in
tions. Consequently, this suggests that psychological young people with ASD and to investigate the active
interventions for OCD in children with ASD may be more components of CBT for these individuals.
effective if they tackle these distorted cognitions. There is
also some evidence that perfectionism can interfere with
treatment outcome when treating typical adults with emo- References
tional disorders (e.g. Blatt et al. 1998) and hence inter-
ventions that aim to ameliorate dysfunctional and Anderson, S., & Morris, J. (2006). Cognitive behaviour therapy for
perfectionistic beliefs may also enhance therapeutic pro- people with Asperger syndrome. Behavioural and Cognitive
gress and relapse prevention in children with ASD. Psychotherapy, 34, 293–303.
Antony, M. M., & Swinson, R. P. (1998). When perfect isn’t good
There was no evidence to indicate that cognitive dis- enough: strategies for coping with perfectionism. Oakland, CA:
tortions are less relevant to anxious and depressive symp- New Harbinger Publications.
toms in children with ASD than in typically developing Attwood, T. (1998). Asperger’s syndrome: A guide for parents and
children. However, the weak and inconsistent correlations professionals. London: Jessica Kingsley.
Barnhill, G. P., & Myles, B. S. (2001). Attributional style and
in both groups suggest that there are many other important depression in adolescents with Asperger syndrome. Journal of
factors involved and that cognitive distortions may play a Positive Behavior Interventions, 3, 175–182.
larger role in emotional symptoms for some young people Baron-Cohen, S. (1988). Social and pragmatic deficits in autism:
than others. In clinical practice, detailed assessment and Cognitive or affective? Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders, 18, 379–402.
individualised formulation is important to understand the Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: clinical, experimental and theoret-
factors relevant to the individual. Bolton (2004) suggests ical aspects. New York: Harper and Row.
that when conducting CBT with children, a pragmatic Bellini, S. (2004). Social skill deficits and anxiety in high-functioning
approach should be taken, whereby cognitive distortions adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Focus on Autism
and Other Developmental Disabilities, 19, 78–86.
are assessed and if they are present, then they should be a Berument, S. K., Rutter, M., Lord, C., Pickles, A., & Bailey, A.
target for therapy; if not then other approaches should be (1999). Autism screening questionnaire: Diagnostic validity.
taken, regardless of the age of the child. British Journal of Psychiatry, 175, 444–451.
Finally, there was evidence that cognitive deficits might Blatt, S. J., Zuroff, D. C., Bondi, C. M., Sanislow, C. A., & Pilkonis,
P. A. (1998). When and how perfectionism impedes the brief
underlie cognitive distortions in children with ASD, as treatment of depression: Further analyses of the National
perseveration was related to perfectionism and measures of Institute of Mental Health Treatment of Depression Collabora-
reciprocal social interaction skills were related to socially- tive Research Program. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
prescribed perfectionism in the ASD group. As cognitive Psychology, 66, 423–428.
Bolton, D. (2004). Cognitive behaviour therapy for children and
inflexibility and social cognitive deficits also relate to the adolescents: some theoretical and developmental issues. In P. J.
core diagnostic triad of ASD, this may suggest that cog- Graham (Ed.), Cognitive-behaviour therapy for children and
nitive distortions will be more resistant to change in these families (2nd ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
individuals. This indicates that CBT with these individuals Bolton, P., Macdonald, H., Pickles, A., Rios, P., Goode, S., Crowson,
M., et al. (1994). A case-control family history study of autism.
might be more effective if cognitive deficits are also Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 877–900.
addressed. For example, interventions may include reha- Brereton, A. V., Tonge, B. J., & Einfeld, S. L. (2006). Psychopa-
bilitation techniques aimed at reducing cognitive inflexi- thology in children and adolescents with autism compared to
bility (e.g. Young and Freyslinger 1995) or improving young people with intellectual disability. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 36, 863–870.
social understanding (e.g. Howlin et al. 1999). Chalfant, A. M., Rapee, R., & Carroll, L. (2007). Treating anxiety
disorders in children with high functioning autism spectrum
disorders: A controlled trial. Journal of Autism and Develop-
Conclusions mental Disorders, 37, 1842–1857.
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112,
155–159.
To our knowledge, this is the first study to investigate D’Alessandro, D. U., & Burton, K. D. (2006). Development and
perfectionism and dysfunctional attitudes in young people validation of the Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale for children:
with ASD. The study revealed elevated scores on measures Tests of Beck’s cognitive diathesis-stress theory of depression,
of its causal mediation component, and of developmental effects.
of anxious and depressive symptoms and cognitive dis- Cognitive Therapy and Research, 30, 335–353.
tortions in children with ASD, indicating that this is an Farrugia, S., & Hudson, J. (2006). Anxiety in adolescents with
important area of study. Dysfunctional attitudes were Asperger syndrome: negative thoughts, behavioral problems, and

123
J Autism Dev Disord (2010) 40:1179–1187 1187

life interference. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental clinic and normal sample. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42,
Disabities, 21, 25–35. 813–839.
Flett, G. L., Hewitt, P. L., Boucher, D. J., Davidson, L. A., & Munro, Obsessive Compulsive Cognitions Working Group. (1997). Cognitive
Y. (2000). The Child-Adolescent Perfectionism Scale: develop- assessment of obsessive compulsive disorder. Behaviour
ment, validation, and association with adjustment. Manuscript Research and Therapy, 35, 667–681.
submitted for publication. Ooi, Y. P., Lam, C. M., Sung, M., Tan, W. T. S., Goh, T. J., Fung, D.
Gaus, V. L. (2007). Cognitive-behavioral therapy for adult Asperger S. S., et al. (2008). Effects of cognitive-behavioural therapy on
syndrome. New York: The Guildford Press. anxiety for children with high-functioning autistic spectrum
Ghaziuddin, M., Ghaziuddin, N., & Greden, J. (2002). Depression in disorders. Singapore Medical Journal, 49, 215–220.
persons with autism: Implications for research and clinical care. Ozonoff, S. (1995). Reliability and validity of the Wisconsin Card
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32, 299–306. Sorting Test in studies of autism. Neuropsychology, 9, 491–500.
Gillott, A., Furniss, F., & Walter, A. (2001). Anxiety in high- Psychological Corporation. (1999). Wechsler abbreviated scale of
functioning children with autism. Autism, 4, 117–132. intelligence. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment.
Green, J., Gilchrist, A., Burton, D., & Cox, A. (2000). Social and Reaven, J. A., Blakeley-Smith, A., Nichols, S., Dasari, M., Flanigan,
psychiatric functioning in adolescents with Asperger syndrome E., & Hepburn, S. (2009). Cognitive-behavioral group treatment
compared with conduct disorder. Journal of Autism and Devel- for anxiety symptoms in children with high-functioning autism
opmental Disorders, 30, 279–293. spectrum disorders: A pilot study. Focus on Autism and Other
Heaton, R. K. (1981). Wisconsin Card Sorting Test manual. Odessa, Developmental Disabilities, 24, 27–37.
FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc. Rutter, M., Bailey, A., & Lord, C. (2003). SCQ: Social Communi-
Hedley, D., & Young, R. (2006). Social comparison processes and cation Questionnaire. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological
depressive symptoms in children and adolescents with Asperger Services.
syndrome. Autism, 10, 139–153. Sanders, J. L. (2009). Qualitative or quantitative differences between
Hewitt, P. L., Caelian, C. F., Flett, G. L., Sherry, S. B., Collins, L., & Asperger’s disorder and autism? Historical considerations.
Flynn, C. A. (2002). Perfectionism in children: Associations with Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 1560–
depression, anxiety, and anger. Personality and Individual 1567.
Differences, 32, 1049–1061. Shafran, R., Cooper, Z., & Fairburn, C. G. (2002). Clinical
Hill, E. L. (2004). Executive dysfunction in autism. Trends in perfectionism: A cognitive-behavioural analysis. Behaviour
Cognitive Sciences, 8, 26–32. Research and Therapy, 40, 773–791.
Hochberg, Y. (1988). A sharper Bonferroni procedure for multiple Shafran, R., & Mansell, W. (2001). Perfectionism and psychopathol-
tests of significance. Biometrika, 75, 800–803. ogy: A review of research and treatment. Clinical Psychology
Howlin, P. (1998). Practitioner review: Psychological and educational Review, 21, 879–906.
treatments for autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychi- Sofronoff, K., Attwood, T., & Hinton, S. (2005). A randomized
atry, 39, 307–322. controlled trial of a CBT intervention for anxiety in children with
Howlin, P., Baron-Cohen, S., & Hadwin, J. (1999). Teaching children Asperger syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry
with autism how to mind read: A practical guide for teachers and Allied Disciplines, 46, 1152–1160.
and parents. Chichester: Wiley. Spence, S. H. (1997). Structure of anxiety symptoms among children:
James, A., Soler, A., & Weatherall, R. (2005). Cognitive behavioural A confirmatory factor-analytic study. Journal of Abnormal
therapy for anxiety disorders in children and adolescents. The Psychology, 106, 280–297.
Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, 4, CD004690. Tantam, D. (2003). The challenge of adolescents and adults with
Kessler, R. C., Foster, C. L., Saunders, W. B., & Stang, P. E. (1995). Asperger syndrome. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of
Social consequences of psychiatric disorders, I: Educational North America, 12, 143–163.
attainment. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 1026–1032. Van Ameringen, M., Mancini, C., & Farvolden, P. (2003). The impact
Kongs, S. K., Thompson, L. L., Iverson, G. L., & Heaton, R. K. of anxiety disorders on educational achievement. Journal of
(2000). Wisconsin Card Sorting Test-64 card version (WCST- Anxiety Disorders, 17, 561–571.
64). Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Vogel, P. A., Stiles, T. C., & Nordahl, H. M. (2000). Cognitive
Kovacs, M. K. (1992). Children’s depression inventory-short form. personality styles in OCD outpatients compared to depressed
New York: Multi-Health Systems Inc. outpatients and health controls. Behavioural and Cognitive
Lewinsohn, P. M., Joiner, T. E., & Rohde, P. (2001). Evaluation of Psychotherapy, 28, 247–258.
cognitive diathesis stress models in predicting major depressive Weissman, A. N., & Beck, A. T. (1978). Development and validation
disorder in adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, of the Dysfunctional Attitude Scale: A preliminary investigation.
203–215. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American
Marton, P., & Kutcher, S. (1995). The prevalence of cognitive Educational Research Association, Toronto, Canada.
distortion in depressed adolescents. Journal of Psychiatry & White, S. W., Ollendick, T., Scahill, L., Oswald, D., & Albano, A. M.
Neuroscience, 20, 33–38. (2009). Preliminary efficacy of a cognitive-behavioral treatment
Meyer, J. A., Mundy, P., Van Hecke, A. V., & Durocher, J. S. program for anxious youth with autism spectrum disorders.
(2006). Social attribution processes and comorbid psychiatric Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 1652–
symptoms in children with Asperger syndrome. Autism, 10, 1662.
383–402. Wood, J. J., Drahota, A., Sze, K., Har, K., Chiu, A., & Langer, D. A.
National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence. (2005). (2009). Cognitive behavioral therapy for anxiety in children with
Depression in children and young people. http://guidance.nice. autism spectrum disorders: A randomized, controlled trial.
org.uk/CG28/niceguidance/pdf/English. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50(3), 224–234.
Nauta, M., Scholing, A., Rapee, R., Abbott, M., & Spence, S. (2004). Young, D., & Freyslinger, M. (1995). Scaffolded instruction and the
Development of a parent report measure of children’s anxiety: remediation of Wisconsin Card Sorting Test deficits in chronic
Psychometric properties and comparison with child report in a schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 16, 199–207.

123

You might also like