You are on page 1of 12

Journal of Child and Family Studies (2020) 29:82–93

https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-019-01582-7

ORIGINAL PAPER

The Feasibility and Effectiveness of Mindful Yoga for Preschoolers


Exposed to High Levels of Trauma
1
Rachel A. Razza ●
Rachel Uveges Linsner1 Dessa Bergen-Cico2 Emily Carlson3 Staceyann Reid1
● ● ●

Published online: 21 September 2019


© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2019

Abstract
Objectives Although mindfulness-based interventions with children have been increasingly used as a strategy to promote
social-emotional learning, the exploration of these practices among preschoolers in limited. This pilot study examined the
feasibility and effectiveness of an 8-week mindful yoga program for preschoolers living in communities with high levels of
trauma. Of particular interest was whether participation in the intervention was associated with gains in children’s self-
regulation of attention and behavior. A related objective was to examine the acceptability and need for trauma-informed
professional development for teaching staff.
1234567890();,:
1234567890();,:

Methods Five classrooms (n = 89 children) were randomly assigned to the intervention and control conditions; three
classrooms participated during the fall and two classrooms served as wait-list controls who received the intervention in the
spring.
Results Analyses revealed significant increases in children’s behavioral and attention regulation during the time that they
participated in the intervention. An unexpected potentially influential finding was the high prevalence of posttraumatic stress
among staff; the majority of staff experienced were above the threshold for civilian PTSD.
Conclusions Mindful yoga may be a promising strategy to promote behavioral regulation and attention regulation among
economically disadvantaged preschoolers. Staff may also benefit from trauma-informed practices given their high rate of
exposure to traumatic events and above-average levels of PTSD.
Keywords Mindfulness Yoga At-risk Preschoolers Trauma
● ● ● ●

Early childhood trauma is a mounting public health con- families, 78% of preschoolers and 66% of parents reported
cern, as approximately 25% of children in the United States exposure to at least one incident of community violence
either witness or experience a traumatic event before the age (Shahinfar et al. 2000).
of 4 years (Briggs-Gowan et al. 2010; National Center for Children exposed to trauma in the home or community
Mental Health Promotion and Youth Violence Prevention can be neurologically and psychologically affected in ways
2012). Unfortunately, these rates are even higher among that negatively impact cognitive function, emotional reg-
ethnic minority children and those who live in dis- ulation, and increase their risks for depression, anxiety, and
advantaged neighborhoods (Finkelhor et al. 2013; Sacks substance abuse (Greenberg 2006; Kim and Cicchetti 2010;
and Murphey 2018). For example, in a study of Head Start Mejia et al. 2006). Relatedly, these children are at increased
risk for deficits in academic achievement and social com-
petence (see Chu and Lieberman 2010, for a review).
Research conducted within the community where the pre-
* Rachel A. Razza sent study was implemented, found significant associations
rrazza@syr.edu
between community gun violence, academic failure, and
1
Department of Human Development and Family Science, school suspensions among children in elementary schools
Syracuse University, 144D White Hall, Syracuse, NY 13244, USA within high gun shot areas indicating that community vio-
2
Department of Public Health, Syracuse University, 144D White lence negatively impacts cognitive and behavioral factors
Hall, Syracuse, NY 13244, USA pertinent to educational attainment (Bergen-Cico et al.
3
Department of Social Work, Syracuse University, 144D White 2018). Thus, there is growing interest in developing effec-
Hall, Syracuse, NY 13244, USA tive school-based primary prevention programs for children
Journal of Child and Family Studies (2020) 29:82–93 83

that cultivate habits of mind and behavior to foster resi- (Blair and Raver 2015; Sanders et al. 2006; Shonkoff et al.
lience, health, and well-being in the face of everyday 2009). The rapid growth in these skills across the preschool
adversity (Shonkoff et al. 2009). Self-regulation is one key years mirrors significant brain development, particularly in
protective factor highlighted by extant prevention research, the prefrontal cortex, an area associated with higher-order
as it represents a critical asset for healthy development and thinking and behavior regulation (Diamond 2002; Mor-
school success (Blair and Diamond 2008; McClelland et al. iguchi and Hiraki 2011), and marks a critical turning point
2013; Moffitt et al. 2011). in development (McClelland et al. 2018). Mindfulness
Self-regulation refers to the ability to monitor and control programs for preschoolers are particularly appealing
our attention, emotion, and behavior to fit situational because they target these early self-regulatory processes that
demands and includes a suite of specific skills that support contribute to both the cognitive and socioemotional
learning and social interaction, including focused attention domains of school-readiness and thus have the potential to
and inhibitory control (Blair and Dennis 2010; Rothbart and significantly impact children’s developmental trajectories
Bates 2006). Extant literature suggests that early self- (Diamond and Lee 2011; Thierry et al. 2016; Willis and
regulatory skills serve as the foundation for children’s Dinehart 2014). Moreover, with an emphasis on refining
behavioral and academic competence, and thus serve as an concentration or attention and strengthening impulse control
important target of intervention, particularly during early within a nonjudgmental framework, mindfulness interven-
childhood when children are acquiring these foundational tions may impact both the top-down (cognitive response
skills (Blair and Razza 2007; Greenberg 2006; McClelland engaging the prefrontal cortex characterized by a measured
et al. 2018). Promoting self-regulation is particularly critical response) and bottom-up (stress/emotional reactivity to
among disadvantaged children, as they routinely exhibit sensory input driven by the amygdala) processes involved
dysregulation, and subsequently experience higher levels of in self-regulation (Gard et al. 2014; Ochsner et al. 2009;
problem behavior and lower levels of academic achievement Zelazo and Lyons 2012). Thus, these programs may be
(Blair and Raver 2012; Buckner et al. 2009; Hackman and particularly powerful for children with a history of chronic
Farah 2009). One strategy for enhancing self-regulation that stress or trauma, as they are at increased risk for deficits in
is rooted in, and reflective of, a trauma-informed approach executive function (e.g., working memory, inhibitory con-
are mindfulness-based practices in early childhood education trol, attention flexibility), as well as compromised physio-
(Holmes et al. 2015; McConnico et al. 2016). logical functioning (e.g., elevated cortisol levels, reduced
Mindfulness-based practices, particularly mediation and immune response) (Blair and Raver 2012; Evans and Kim
yoga, use movement, thinking, and breathing techniques to 2013; Miller et al. 2011; Telles et al. 2012).
increase attention, concentration, flexibility, and reduce stress Yoga, defined as a set of practices that include physical
(Cook-Cottone 2017; Greenberg and Harris 2012). More postures, breathing techniques, meditation, and relaxation
specifically, mindfulness is defined as “paying attention to (Hagan and Nayar 2014), represents one mindfulness-based
what’s happening in the present moment in the mind, body, technique that has been successfully adapted to be devel-
and external environment, with an attitude of curiosity and opmentally appropriate for young children (Cook-Cottone
kindness” (Mindfulness All-Party Parliamentary Group 2017; Kaiser-Greenland 2010; Roeser and Peck 2009). In
[MAPP] 2015, p. 13). Thus, while there is variability across yoga, movements are enacted to strengthen, stretch and
exercises, a shared goal of mindfulness-based practice is to relax the body, including the nervous and emotional sys-
enhance our attention to, and acceptance of, present moment tems, while breath is used to restore attention to the present
experiences (Bishop et al. 2004; Cook-Cottone 2017). moment and balance to the body and mind (Cook-Cottone
Mindfulness-based programs are increasingly popular among 2015; Khalsa 2004). The use of movement and somatic
school-aged children, and there is a growing body of evidence experiences is common in early childhood education and
to support their effectiveness in promoting the regulation of practices like yoga, that involve sensory-motor coordina-
emotions, attention, and behavior among children and youth tion, are thought to reinforce the brain-body connection and
(Felver et al. 2016; Waters et al. 2015; Zenner et al. 2014). neurological underpinnings of self-regulation (Diamond and
Although fewer in number, the results of a recent review Lee 2011; Williams 2018). Moreover, these practices
suggest that mindfulness-based programs also hold promise facilitate embodied self-regulation, a concept delineated by
for young children, with benefits found in the domains of self- Cook-Cottone (2017) in her mindful and yogic self as an
regulation, social-emotional competence, and physical effective learner (MY-SEL) model that delinates how
development (Erwin and Robinson 2016). mindfulness practices and yoga contribute to student
Early childhood is viewed as an optimal time for redu- engagement in the classroom. Specifically, embodied self-
cing self-regulatory deficits, as the underlying develop- regulation requires mindful awareness of our thoughts,
mental processes, including attention and executive feelings, and, physical sensations and includes key pro-
functions, are highly malleable during this sensitive period cesses that support the mind-body-behavior connection
84 Journal of Child and Family Studies (2020) 29:82–93

such as inquiry into our inner experiences and choice of our health compared with the broader population, with 24% of the
actions (Cook-Cottone 2015, 2017). Consequently, yoga is sample meeting the criteria for depression (Whitaker et al.
considered an important tool and educational strategy for 2013). Unfortunately, these adverse outcomes also have nega-
promoting social-emotional learning (SEL; Butzer et al. tive implications for classroom quality, as high levels of teacher
2016; Cook-Cottone 2017). stress and depressive symptoms are associated with lower
The self-regulatory benefits of secular practice for older emotional climates and reduced social-emotional competence
children, including enhanced academic engagement and among preschoolers (Friedman-Krauss et al. 2014a; Jennings
psychological well-being, are supported by recent reviews of 2015; Roberts et al. 2016). Collectively, these findings are
yoga in the schools research (Khalsa and Butzer 2016; consistent with the Prosocial Classroom theoretical model
Serwacki and Cook-Cottone 2012). As the reviews point (Jennings and Greenberg 2009), which posits that classroom
out, however, the findings are considered preliminary given quality is influenced by the bidirectional associations between
the low methodological quality (e.g., lack of randomized the teacher’s social-emotional competence and the child’s
control, limited follow-up data, self-report measures, follow- social-emotional competence. Moreover, there is evidence that
up data) and limited intervention description among many of mindfulness training for preschool teachers was associated with
the early studies. Moreover, most of the research on yoga decreased reports of challenging behaviors and negative social
has focused on programs at the elementary-school level or interactions with peers and increased compliance among stu-
above. Thus, research on the feasibility and effectiveness of dents (Singh et al. 2013).
yoga interventions among young children is still in its This pilot study extends the scope of successful
infancy. There is evidence, however, that mindful yoga, mindfulness-based interventions for young children by
defined as mindfulness-based programs that include simple infusing yoga into the daily physical/gross motor activities
breathing and yoga poses, can be effective in enhancing self- for children in the center while also assessing the needs of
regulation among preschool children (Adair and Bhaskaran their teachers. The first aim was to examine the benefits of
2010; Flook et al. 2015; Poehlmann-Tynan et al. 2016; an 8-week mindful yoga program for preschoolers’ self-
Razza et al. 2015). For example, meditation and simple yoga regulation. The evaluation used random assignment with
postures integrated across the year by classroom teachers in two intervention cohorts and included direct assessments of
both Montessori (Adair and Bhaskaran 2010) and public attention and behavioral regulation at three time-points.
(Razza et al. 2015) preschools has been associated with Based on previous research (Flook et al. 2015; Razza et al.
benefits in both attentional and behavioral regulation. 2015), we expected children in the intervention to demon-
Moreover, the self-regulatory benefits of yoga are supported strate significant gains in self-regulation. We also expected
in recent qualitative work investigating young children’s children in the first cohort of the intervention to maintain
experiences of yoga in the classroom. Specifically, emo- their self-regulatory skills at the 3-month post-test, although
tional regulation and cognitive function, along with physical this hypothesis was more tentative given the limited
benefits, emerged as themes reflected in the narratives of research on the sustainability of self-regulatory gains within
preschoolers in one study (Stapp and Wolff 2019). Another the context of mindfulness-based interventions for children
theme that emerged in similar work was children’s auton- (Poehlmann-Tynan et al. 2016; Thierry et al. 2016). A
omy and how somatic experiences promote awareness of second aim was to document the level of trauma and stress
their inner and outer-selves and a sense of independence that among the teaching staff and examine the acceptability of
supports behavioral regulation (Buono 2019). Thus, it is professional development workshops that focused on
feeble that mindful yoga can be infused into classrooms via a trauma-informed practice and mindfulness-based stress
trauma-informed early education framework that addresses reduction. In line with prior work with Head Start teachers
the needs of the children and their teachers. (Friedman-Krauss et al. 2014b; Zhai et al. 2011), we
Early childhood educators experience multiple job stressors, expected elevated levels of stress and trauma within this
including inadequate compensation and resources, reduced sample. We also anticipated that teachers would find the
autonomy, and low confidence in managing children’s behavior professional development workshops useful and indicate
(Hall-Kenyon et al. 2014; Hammarberg and Hagekull 2002; interest in additional seminars.
Leana et al. 2009). These challenges, particularly behavioral
management concerns, are exacerbated for preschool teachers
who serve children from disadvantaged neighborhoods (Fried- Method
man-Krauss et al. 2014b; Li-Grining et al. 2010; Zhai et al.
2011). Not surprisingly, these teachers report high levels of Participants
emotional exhaustion and burnout (McGinty et al. 2008).
Moreover, a recent poll of Head Start teachers in Pennsylvania The study included 89 children aged 3 to 5 years (M = 4.1
found that they were at greater-risk of poor physical and mental years, SD = 0.37; 50% male) from five preschool
Journal of Child and Family Studies (2020) 29:82–93 85

classrooms within an urban Head Start center in a mid-sized the program or in its evaluation; children provided verbal
city in the northeastern region of the United States. The consent at the time of each assessment.
center serves an ethnically diverse community of children The five preschool classrooms were randomly assigned
who were predominantly African American (74%) and to the intervention or wait-list control condition. Three
mixed-race (14.3%). Per the Federal regulations, at least classrooms participated in the intervention in the fall; two
90% of families enrolled in Head Start must be at or below classrooms served as the wait-list control and received the
130% of the federal poverty level, with priority given to intervention the following spring. Thus, two rounds of
families at or below 100% of the federal poverty level the mindful yoga intervention were administered across the
(http://www.benefits.gov/benefits/benefit-details/616). This school year. All children were individually assessed three
Head Start center serves a particularly disadvantaged times (fall, winter, spring) across the school year. The child
population, as one third of all city residents live below the assessments were conducted by trained research assistants
federal poverty level; among families with children under in a quiet area inside the classroom, or in the hall directly
age 18, 43% fall below the poverty level and 54% are outside the classroom. The battery of assessments are
female-headed (United States Census Bureau 2016). The described in detail in the measures section and included two
high school graduation rate in the community studied here tasks tapping self-regulation that were administered in a
is about 50% while 52% of their public schools have been fixed order to maximize interest and minimize bias related
designated as failing by New York State (New York State to variation in administration, as is typical in individual
Education Department 2015.) Moreover, these children difference designs (Carlson et al. 2004; Clark et al. 2013).
reside in a city that had the highest per capita murder rate in The self-regulation battery took approximately 20 min to
all of New York state in 2013; and the rate of posttraumatic complete and children received a sticker after each session.
stress among citizens in the community where this study The families also received a child yoga book after the
was conducted is 51% (Lane et al. 2017). completion of the study as an incentive.
Study participants also included 23 teaching staff who The teaching staff were informed of the professional
attended at least one of the two professional development development workshops via the Center Director and a letter
workshops. The staff included eight head teachers, seven distributed by the researchers. Staff who attended either
teaching assistants, and eight teacher aides from the pre- session were eligible to participate in the research study and
school classrooms. The staff was primarily female (91% complete a teacher survey. Informed consent was obtained
female, 9% male) between the ages of 26 and 40 years at the workshops; there were no refusals to participate. The
(39%). The majority of the staff members were white (74%) survey included a demographic questionnaire and two self-
with the remaining staff members identifying as Black report measures tapping trauma and stress. It was admi-
(13%), Asian/Pacific Islander (9%), or Hispanic (4%). nistered at the start of the workshop and took approximately
Sixty-five percent of the staff members were employed at 15 min to complete. Teachers received a gift card for their
the center for five years or less. The majority of staff participation. Participants also completed a satisfaction
completed a postsecondary degree, including an Associate’s survey following each of the two workshops.
degree (13%), a Bachelor’s degree (30%), or a Master’s
degree (13%). The remaining staff either completed high Mindful yoga intervention
school (or equivalent; 21%) or some college (21%).
The mindful yoga program was delivered by a certified
Procedure child yoga instructor who visited the center twice a week for
8-weeks. The sessions were conducted in the center’s
Parents of the five preschool classrooms in the center were gymnasium and delivered to each classroom independently.
notified about both the mindful yoga program and the The yoga instructor taught three consecutive classes in the
opportunity to participate in its evaluation via packets fall and two in the spring. The mindful yoga program was
mailed directly to their homes from the Head Start center. reflective of the YogaKids curriculum that treats yoga as a
Given that the mindful yoga program was considered part of medium for learning and integrates movement with other
the center’s preschool curriculum, parental informed con- elements including music, anatomy, language, and ecology
sent was not required for children to participate in the yoga (Wenig 2003). Each 25-minute session began with a cen-
program. Initially, families were recruited via active par- tering activity (e.g., feather breathing, mindful listening to
ental consent. Given the low recruitment rate at T1 (44%), the singing bowl), progressed through a set of child-
at the request of the Head Start Center, the process was centered yoga poses (e.g., downward dog, cat/cow), and
changed to passive parental concent. This procedural concluded with a brief relaxation activity (e.g., deep-belly
change was approved by the University’s IRB. There were breathing with a ball, lying on the back and watching a
no instances of parental refusal for children to participate in rubber duck rise and fall on their bellies). The same lesson
86 Journal of Child and Family Studies (2020) 29:82–93

plan was used for all of the classes, but thematic content the child received a score of 0 (incorrect), 1 (self-correct), or
differed slightly between the fall and spring cohorts, given 2 (correct). Scores were summed across trials to reflect a
the integration of relevant events (e.g., holidays, seasons). total correct response ranging from 0 to 60.
The instructor incorporated music, books, and props (e.g., Children’s attention regulation was assessed using the
stuffed animals, rainbow scarves) into all of the classes. Attention Sustained task (AST) from the Leiter International
Performance Scale-Revised (Roid and Miller 1997). Chil-
Teacher professional development dren were shown a picture of a variety of objects scattered
throughout a page. There was a target object at the top of
Teachers and teaching staff attended the yoga sessions and the page and children were asked to put a line through as
participated to their comfort level. They also attended one many of the objects matching the target as possible without
of two, two-hour child-yoga workshops led by the chil- accidentally crossing out any other objects. Children’s
dren’s yoga instructor. The intent of these workshops was to performance across four timed trials were averaged to yield
connect with the teachers in the intervention classrooms scores of attentional focus and inhibition. The adjusted total
prior to the start of the program. The instructor discussed correct score, which reflects the number of correct responses
the benefits of children’s yoga, particularly within the (cross-outs of objects matching the target) minus the num-
context of trauma, and led the teachers through a brief class ber of incorrect responses (cross-outs of objects not
that mirrored the child session. She also modeled simple matching the target), was used in analyses. The score was
breathing techniques that could be integrated into their standardized against a national norming sample with a mean
classrooms. Two trauma-informed workshops for preschool of 10 (SD = 3). The task has high internal reliability (α =
teaching staff were conducted at the center during a two- 0.83) for young children and good test-retest reliability (r =
hour time reserved for professional development. In the first 0.85) (Roid and Miller 1997).
seminar, a certified mindfulness instructor presented an
experiential mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) Teacher survey
workshop. The purpose of this workshop was to introduce
teachers to mindfulness techniques (e.g., breath meditation, The Life Events Checklist for DSM-5 (LEC-5; Weathers
body scan) that they could use for self-care and well-being. et al. 2013) is a self-report measure intended to screen for
In the second seminar, a licensed marriage and family potentially traumatic life events in an individual’s lifetime.
therapist presented strategies for treating complex trauma The list includes 16 events, such as a natural disaster (e.g.,
using the Collaborative Change Model (CCM; Barrett and flood, hurricane, tornado, earthquake), that potentially
Stone Fish 2014). More specifically, the therapist educated increase a person’s risk for personal distress or Post-
the staff on the effects of trauma and provided tools to help Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD). For each item, the
cope with symptoms of trauma that decrease hope and respondent indicated their level of exposure to the event on
resilience. The intent of this session was to increase the a 5-point nominal scale (i.e., happened to me; witnessed it;
teachers’ knowledge of trauma-informed practice and pro- learned about it; not sure; does not apply) that allowed for
mote its application in their professional lives. the selection of multiple levels of exposure to the same
event. The scale does not yield a composite score. For this
Measures paper, the percentages of staff who experienced, witnessed,
or learned about the events were calculated to represent
Assessments of children’s self-regulation levels of risk within the sample.
The PTSD Checklist (PCL—Civilian version; Weathers
Children’s self-regulation was assessed via a two tasks. The et al. 1994) was used to assess the teachers’ self-report of
Head Toes Knees and Shoulders task (HTKS; Ponitz et al. DMS-IV symptoms of PTSD. The 17-item measure asks
2009) measures children’s behavioral regulation using a about the severity of symptoms over the last month in
structured observation that requires children to perform the relation to generic stressful experiences. For example,
opposite of a dominant response (i.e., touch their heads respondents are asked to rate how much they are bothered
when the interviewer says “touch your toes”) to four dif- by problems (e.g., repeated, disturbing memories, thoughts,
ferent oral commands. The measure has demonstrated both or images of a stressful experience from the past), using a 5-
construct and predictive validity in diverse samples (Dun- point Likert scale (1 = not at all; 2 = a little bit; 3 = mod-
can et al. 2017; McClelland et al. 2014; Wanless et al. erately, 4 = quite a bit; 5 = extremely). A total symptom
2011). Internal reliability for the current sample was similar severity scores (range 17–85) was obtained by summing the
to levels reported in previous research (α = 0.83 to 0.87). scores from each of the 17 items. The scale has demon-
The HTKS task includes three phases, each with 10 trials, strated both convergent and discriminant validity in non-
which become progressively more difficult. For each trial, clinical samples (Conybeare et al. 2012). Suggested PLC
Journal of Child and Family Studies (2020) 29:82–93 87

Table 1 Summary of main


Measure Fall cohort Spring cohort
effects for repeated-measures
ANOVA models examining M (SD) M (SD) N F p-value Partial η2
self-regulation
Behavioral regulation (HTKS)
Pretest 1.25 (3.34) 20 4.47 (11.41) 17
Post-test 6.30 (12.07) 20 10.35 (15.10) 17 11.30 0.002 0.24
Attention regulation (AST)
Pretest 7.31 (3.86) 16 8.70 (3.25) 23
Post-test 9.81 (3.47) 16 10.09 (2.84) 23 14.23 0.001 0.28
The fall intervention cohort participated in the program between T1 and T2; the spring cohort participated in
the program between T2 and T3. The F statistic represents the within-subject effect of time
HTKS Head Toes Knees Shoulder task, AST Attention Sustained task

cut-point scores within civilian settings are 30–35, where follow-up/T3 (n = 78); the notable increase at post-test was
the estimated prevalence of PTDS is 15% or below. a result of parent consent changing from active to passive
consent. Almost half (49.4%) of the children participated in
Data Analyses the post-test and follow up only; 32 children (36.0%) par-
ticipated at all three time-points. There was variation in
Our original intent to compare change between the inter- completion across the tasks due to unexpected events, such
vention and wait-list control groups was limited given as a fire drill or dismissal. Results from independent t-tests
issues with data collection. The revised analyses model the indicate that the two groups did not differ significantly
change in time associated with participation in the inter- across self-regulation measures at baseline T1. Similarly,
vention (T1 and T2 scores for the fall intervention group; the mean pre-test scores for the fall intervention group did
T2 and T3 scores for the spring intervention group). Spe- not differ significantly from the mean pre-test scores for the
cifically, the effects of the program on children’s self- spring intervention group.
regulation were examined using repeated-measures analysis The effect of the intervention on children’s self-
of variance (ANOVA) models. The within-subject variable regulation was examined via a series of repeated mea-
(time) had two levels (pre-test and post-test) and the sures ANOVAs; the main effects are summarized in
between-subjects variable reflected group (fall or spring). Table 1. The model examining behavioral regulation
The partial eta-squared statistic was used as an estimate of (HTKS) revealed a significant effect of time, F(1, 35) =
effect size, with η2 = 0.02 suggesting a small effect, η2 = 11.30, p < 0.05, partial η2 = 0.24. Post hoc tests using the
0.09 signifying a moderate effect, and η2 = 0.25 represent- Bonferroni correction revealed that the intervention eli-
ing a large effect (Cohen 1988). Additional analyses cited a significant increase (mean difference = 5.47, p <
included independent t-tests comparing the change scores 0.01) in behavioral regulation from pre-test to post-test.
between the intervention and wait-list control group The interaction between time and group was not sig-
between T1 and T2 (intervention effect) as well as com- nificant, F(1, 35) = 0.10, ns, suggesting that the change
paring the pre-test score across both groups (T1 for the fall from pre-test to post-test was similar across the fall and
intervention with T2 for the spring intervention). A paired- spring intervention cohorts. These patterns are reflected in
sample t-test for the fall intervention group was also con- Fig. 1. Additional analyses examining change scores
ducted to test whether there was significant change in mean between T1 and T2 only found that the fall intervention
scores during the follow-up period (T2 to T3). Teacher data cohort experienced significantly greater growth over that
were analyzed via descriptive statistics representing demo- time period compared with the waitlist control group,
graphic information, trauma, stress, and satisfaction with the t(22) = 2.32, p < 0.05. We also examined the change from
workshops. post-test (T2) to follow-up (T3) for the fall intervention
cohort using a paired-samples t-test. Although the scores
increased from post-test (M = 6.41, SD = 14.41) to
Results follow-up (M = 8.50, SD = 13.60), the change was not
significant, t(22) = 1.97, p = 0.06. Thus, behavioral reg-
Although 89 children participated in the research, sample ulation was responsive to the mindful yoga practice and
size varied across time-points and outcomes. The following that the effects of the program were maintained across the
number of children participated in the child assessments 3-month follow-up period for the children in the fall
over time: pre-test/T1 (n = 39), post-test/T2 (n = 81), and cohort.
88 Journal of Child and Family Studies (2020) 29:82–93

Fig. 1 Change in behavioral regulation (HTKS) score by intervention Fig. 2 Change in attention regulation (AST) score by intervention
group. The fall cohort participated in the program from T1 to T2 and group. The fall cohort participated in the program from T1 to T2 and
the spring cohort group participated in the program from T2 to T3 the spring cohort group participated in the program from T2 to T3

A similar series of repeated measures ANOVA models mean number of events experienced was 2.91 (SD = 2.25),
were conducted to examine change in attention regulation with a range of 0–9. Thirty-five percent experienced sexual
(AST) over time. The model examining attention regulation assault, 9% experienced physical assault, and 9% experi-
revealed a significant effect of time, F(1, 37) = 14. 23, p < enced assault with a weapon. Ninety-one percent of the staff
0.01, partial η2 = 0.28. Post hoc tests using the Bonferroni participants had witnessed multiple traumatic events, with
correction revealed that the intervention elicited a sig- 22% witnessing physical assault, 22% witnessing assault
nificant increase (mean difference = 1.95, p < 0.01) in with a weapon, and 4% witnessing sexual assault. The mean
attention regulation from pre-test to post-test. The interac- number of events witnessed was 2.30 (SD = 2.51), with a
tion between time and group was not significant, F(1, 37) = range of 0 to 9 events. The mean score on the PCL was
1.16, ns, indicating that the change from was similar across 34.38 (SD = 12.74). Fifty-seven percent of the staff mem-
the fall and spring groups (see Fig. 2). The changes scores bers scored higher than the 30 cut-point on the PCL, while
between T1 and T2 also suggest that the fall intervention 83% scored higher than a 35 on the PCL. The results from
cohort experienced greater growth over that time period independent t-tests indicated that trauma and stress among
compared with the waitlist control group, t(24) = 1.99, p = teachers was similar across the intervention and control
0.06. The results of a paired samples t-test indicated positive classrooms.
and significant change in attention regulation for partici- The feedback from staff on the workshops suggested that
pants in the fall intervention group from post-test (M = they found the trainings relevant and were interested in
9.31, SD = 3.66) to follow-up (M = 10.81, SD = 2.12), t = future opportunities. Sixty-one percent of the staff reported
2.65, p < 0.05. In sum, the intervention appeared to promote having none-to-little prior experience with MBSR practices
attention regulation and continued growth was supported and 63% reported having none-to-little prior training in
during follow-up for the children in the fall cohort. trauma-informed practice techniques. In terms of the MBSR
In total, 23 teachers or teaching staff completed the workshop, the majority of staff (66.7%) found the training
teacher survey. The staff sample included eight head tea- to be quite a bit or very relevant to their professional life;
chers, seven teaching assistants, and eight teacher aides. 50% indicated that they would use the MBSR practices that
The staff was primarily female (91%) and between the ages they learned to address needs in their professional lives.
of 26 and 40 years (39%). The majority of the staff mem- Similarly, the majority (66.7%) of staff were quite or very
bers who participated in the trainings were White (74%) satisfied with this training and reported that they would be
with the remaining staff members identifying as Black moderately or more interested in attending a future work-
(13%), Asian/Pacific Islander (9%), or Hispanic (4%). shop on mindfulness-based practice. Fifty-two percent of
Sixty-five percent of the staff members were employed at staff found the training on the CCM model to be at least
the center for five years or less. The majority (44%) of staff quite a bit relevant to their professional life and 63%
members completed high school and/or had some college reported that they would use materials from the training to
experience; 30% received their Bachelor’s degree and 13% address needs in their professional lives. Sixty-eight percent
earned a Master’s degree. of staff were satisfied with this training quite a bit and
Almost all of the staff participants (95.7%) reported 68.5% said that they would attend a future workshops that
experiencing multiple traumatic events on the TLEQ. The focused on trauma.
Journal of Child and Family Studies (2020) 29:82–93 89

Discussion associated with later phases of practice (Chiesa et al. 2011).


Thus, similar to how we can learn more about the specificity
Overall, the results of this pilot study suggest that the in the associations between mindfulness and EF, more
mindful yoga intervention was both feasible and effective sophisticated measures would allow us to better identify the
for Head Start preschoolers. The 8-week program was attentional mechanisms supported by mindful yoga practice
easily integrated into the preschool curriculum and was in young children.
successfully implemented twice over the school year for In addition to bringing mindful yoga to children, the
two independent cohorts of children in the same childcare results of the present study also highlight the need for
center. The findings suggest that the intervention promoted mindfulness-based programming for the teachers who serve
behavioral regulation and attention regulation among eco- and guide them. Notably, almost all of the staff (95.7%)
nomically disadvantaged preschoolers. Moreover, the reported experiencing multiple traumatic events in their
trauma-informed professional development workshops were lives. Moreover, there was a high prevalence of post-
well received by staff, the majority of whom experienced traumatic stress (57% with a PCL > 30) in this sample of
multiple traumatic events and were above the civilian teachers, which is consistent with previous literature citing
threshold for PTSD. indices of poor mental health among Head Start teachers
The current study advanced our understanding of how compared to the general population (Whitaker et al. 2013;
mindful yoga can promote self-regulatory skills among at- Whitaker et al. 2014). Thus, exposure to trauma is clearly a
risk children. As expected, both cohorts of children relevant risk factor for these teachers, which left unad-
demonstrated a significant increase in behavioral regulation dressed, may result in lower quality of teacher-child inter-
while participating in the intervention. Moreover, children actions and reduced emotional climate (Friedman-Krauss
in the fall intervention group maintained their skill level et al. 2014a; Jennings 2015). It is noteworthy that the 57%
over the three-month follow-up. These findings are con- rate of PTSD among the teachers is slightly higher, yet
sistent with the results of other mindfulness-based inter- similar to the rate among the community (51%) where this
vention studies with young children that have demonstrated study was conducted (Lane et al. 2017). Another study
benefits in behavioral regulation using the HTKS task found that the neighborhoods where the families of the
(Poehlmann-Tynan et al. 2016; Razza et al. 2015) and children attending the school in this study live have among
support self-regulation as a mechanism through which th highest rates of gun violence in the city (Bergen-Cico
mindfulness influences early academic and social compe- et al. 2018). Together these factors illustrate the prevalence
tence (Razza et al. 2015; Tang et al. 2012). Given that the of PTSD among the adults in these children’s lives and the
HTKS measure taps all three facets of executive function pervasive gun violence in the children’s neighborhoods.
(EF), future studies could include tasks from the NIH The high level of acceptance of the trauma-informed and
Toolbox that tap individual components to gain insight into mindfulness-based professional development trainings
the specific facets of EF that are targeted via mindfulness among this sample suggests that they may be particularly
during preschool. posed to participate in, and benefit from, additional pro-
Similarly, the mindful yoga intervention was associated gramming to reduce stress and improve wellbeing. Future
with change in preschoolers’ attention regulation, which is replication with more diverse samples of educators is nee-
also compatible with previous research detecting an effect ded, however, to determine the generalizability of the tea-
of mindfulness-based practice on young children’s focused chers’ perceptions of the acceptability of the professional
attention (Poehlmann-Tynan et al. 2016). Thus, during the development. Other classroom-based interventions targeting
time that they were in the intervention, children experienced teachers’ job stressors, such as the Chicago School Readi-
an increase in their ability to sustain their attention, and ness Project (CSRP; Zhai et al. 2011), have been successful
continued growth were reported for children in the fall in promoting teachers’ perceived job control and work-
intervention group across the follow-up period. These pat- related resources. Similarly, initiatives to enhance trauma-
terns are exciting and should be addressed via a more informed practice among this population of teachers, such
comprehensive attention battery, such as the child version as the Head Start Trauma Smart program (HSTS; Holmes
of the Attention Network Task (ANT; Rueda et al. 2004). et al. 2015), are associated with enhanced emotional climate
The ANT taps the three individual networks of attention and emotional support in classrooms. Thus, ideally, future
(i.e., alerting or attentional readiness, orienting or selective initiatives should include both teacher- and child-level
attention, and executive control or conflict attention), and components aimed at reducing the impact of stress and
provides scores for each attentional component (Posner and promoting resilience within the Head Start community. One
Peterson 1990). The adult literature suggests that mind- relevant and well-established intervention for teachers that
fulness may initially improve the executive control network could be included in a comprehensive initiative is the
as well as orienting, whereas changes in alerting may be Cultivating Awareness and Resilience in Education (CARE;
90 Journal of Child and Family Studies (2020) 29:82–93

Jennings 2016) program, which provides teachers with practice to teachers over time should be explored. Possible
training in emotion regulation, mindfulness and stress strategies include formal MBSR training or programs like
management, and care and listening practices. Benefits of CARE, which help teachers build and sustain a personal
CARE include reduced burnout and stress for teachers and a practice, as well as workshops and online resources that
more positive emotional climate for the classroom (Jennings share techniques for infusing child-based practices into their
et al. 2017; Sharp and Jennings 2016). classrooms. Indeed, there is evidence that even a few
minutes of mindful yoga a day can enhance self-regulation
Limitations and Future Research Directions and other positive outcomes among students (Adair and
Bhaskaran 2010; Bergen-Cico et al. 2015; Erwin and
One limitation of the current study was the small analytic Robinson 2016).
sample. The substantial missing data, particularly at T1, Finally, although our program did not include a parent
limited our analytic options. While we were able to detect component, a comprehensive approach to addressing
between-group differences in the change scores between the trauma and stress in communities should target children,
intervention and wait-list control groups between T1 and families, and teachers. Prior studies have indicated that
T2, the repeated measures ANOVAs used pooled data mindfulness programs can improve the health and well-
across both intervention cohorts to model within-group being of both children and parents, particularly in popula-
change. Thus, although the results reflect large effect sizes, tions subjected to trauma and/or stress (Coatsworth et al.
we cannot rule out change due to other factors, such as 2010; Costello and Lawler 2014; Jee et al. 2015). Similarly,
maturation or emotional climate of the classroom (Raver there is evidence that family-based programs that focus on
2012). Future studies with larger sample sizes and class- attention training for children and parenting practices for
room observations are needed to clarify the specific effect of caregivers can influence the neural systems mediating
mindful yoga programs. Due to the small sample size, we selective attention in lower SES preschool children and
were also unable to examine potential moderators, such as decrease parenting stress within families (Neville et al.
children’s initial levels of self-regulation, which have been 2013). Thus, future work should explore multi-tiered
shown to be associated with the effectiveness of similar mindfulness-based intervention strategies that promote
interventions (Flook et al. 2010, 2015; Razza et al. 2015). resilience and enhance wellbeing across both home and
Another limitation was the restricted battery of self- school settings.
regulation tasks. Ideally, to elucidate the mechanisms
through which mindfulness promotes self-regulation, stu- Authors’ Contributions R.A.R. Designed and implemented the pro-
dies would include multiple tasks assessing specific aspects gram evaluation, assisted with data analyses, and wrote the paper. R.U.
L. served as a project manager and was responsible for scheduling data
of EF and attention, as well as biological markers of stress.
collection, interviewing children, entering the data, and contributing to
For example, a 10-week classroom-based yoga intervention data analysis. D.B.C. Collaborated on the design of the study, assisted
was associated with decreases in baseline cortisol levels with measure preparation and IRB applications, contributed to the
among second-grade students (Butzer et al. 2015). More- analytic plan, and reviewed the manuscript and revisions. E.C.
Assisted with data collection and entry. S.R. Assisted with data
over, the present study did not examine academic and
analyses.
behavioral outcomes associated with mindfulness and self-
regulation, such as achievement and social-emotional
competence (Thierry et al. 2016). These data are impor- Compliance with Ethical Standards
tant for understanding how mindful yoga may influence the
Conflict of Interest The authors declare that they have no conflict of
academic or social-emotional trajectories of young children. interest.
Additional concerns include the generalizability of the
findings and the sustainability of the effects of the inter- Ethical Approval All procedures performed in studies involving
vention. Specifically, although the intervention was repli- human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of
Syracuse University and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its
cated across two cohorts of children, it was implemented by
later amendments or comparable ethical standards. Approval for this
a single child yoga instructor. Thus, it is possible that there study was obtained from the Syracuse University Internal
were characteristics of the instructor, such as personality or Review Board.
experience teaching, that influenced the children’s engage-
ment in the program. Thus, future replications should Informed Consent Informed consent was obtained from all individual
participants included in the study. Parental consent included both
examine the fidelity and effectiveness of the mindful yoga active and passive consent. All children provided verbal assent to
intervention across multiple instructors. Sustainability, of participate. Teachers provided active consent.
both the practice and the effects, is also a concern, parti-
cularly for interventions implemented by personnel outside Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to
the school system. Thus, options for transferring the jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Journal of Child and Family Studies (2020) 29:82–93 91

References Chu, A. T., & Lieberman, A. F. (2010). Clinical implications of


traumatic stress from birth to age five. Annual Review of Clinical
Adair, J. K., & Bhaskaran, L. (2010). Meditation, rangoli, and eating Psychology, 6, 469–494.
on the floor: practices from an urban preschool in Bangalore, Clark, C. A. C., Sheffield, T. D., Wiebe, S. A., & Espy, K. A. (2013).
India. Young Children, 65, 48–55. Longitudinal associations between executive control and devel-
Barrett, M. J., & Stone Fish, L. (2014). Treating complex trauma: a oping mathematical competence in preschool boys and girls.
relational blueprint for collaboration and change. New York, Child Development, 84, 662–677.
NY: Routledge. Coatsworth, J. D., Duncan, L. G., Greenberg, M. T., & Nix, R. L.
Bergen-Cico, D., Lane, S., Keefe, R., Larsen, D., Salaam Jennings-Bey, (2010). Changing parents’ mindfulness, child management skills,
N., Panasci, A., Jennings-Bey, T., & Rubinstein, R. (2018). Com- and relationship quality with their youth: Results from a rando-
munity gun violence as a social determinant of elementary school mized pilot intervention trial. Journal of Child and Family Stu-
achievement. Journal of Social Work in Public Health, 33, 439–448. dies, 19, 203–217.
Bergen-Cico, D., Razza, R., & Timmins, A. (2015). Fostering mind- Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sci-
fulness and self-regulation through curriculum infusion of ences. 2nd ed New York, NY: Academic Press.
mindful yoga: a pilot study of efficacy and feasibility. Journal of Conybeare, D., Behar, E., Solomon, A., Newman, M. G., & Borkovec,
Child and Family Studies, 24, 3448–3461. T. D. (2012). The PTSD Checklist—Civilian version: reliability,
Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., Carlson, L., Anderson, N. D., validity, and factor structure in a nonclinical sample. Journal of
Carmody, J., & Devins, G. (2004). Mindfulness: a proposed Clinical Psychology, 68, 699–713.
operational definition. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, Cook-Cottone, C. P. (2015). Mindfulness and yoga for self-regulation:
11, 230–241. a primer for mental health professionals. New York, NY:
Blair, C., & Dennis, T. (2010). An optimal balance: emotion-cognition Springer Publishing.
integration in context. In S. Calkins & M. Bell (Eds), Child Cook-Cottone, C. P. (2017). Mindfulness and yoga in schools: a guide
development at the intersection of cognition and emotion (pp. for teachers and practitioners. New York, NY: Springer Pub-
17–36). Washington DC: American Psychological Association. lishing Company, LLC.
Blair, C., & Diamond, A. (2008). Biological processes in prevention Costello, E., & Lawler, M. (2014). An exploratory study of the effects
and intervention: the promotion of self-regulation as a means of of mindfulness on perceived levels of stress among school-
preventing school failure. Development and Psychopathology, children from lower socioeconomic backgrounds. International
20, 899–911. Journal of Emotional Education, 6, 21–39.
Blair, C., & Raver, C. (2015). School readiness and self-regulation: a Diamond, A. (2002). Normal development of prefrontal cortex from
developmental psychobiological approach. Annual Review of birth to young adulthood: cognitive functions, anatomy, and
Psychology, 66, 711–731. biochemistry. In D. T. Stuss & R. T. Knight (Eds), Principles of
Blair, C., & Raver, C. C. (2012). Child development in the context of frontal lobe function (pp. 466–503). London, England: Oxford
adversity: experiential canalization of brain and behavior. University Press.
American Psychologist, 67, 309–318. Diamond, A., & Lee, K. (2011). Interventions shown to aid executive
Blair, C., & Razza, R. P. (2007). Relating effortful control, executive function development in children 4 to 12 years old. Science, 333,
function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and 959–964.
literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Development, 78, 647–663. Duncan, R. J., McClelland, M. M., & Acock, A. C. (2017). Relations
Briggs-Gowan, M. J., Ford, J. D., Fraleigh, L., McCarthy, K., & between executive function, behavioral regulation, and achieve-
Carter, A. S. (2010). Prevalence of exposure to potentially trau- ment: moderation by family income. Journal of Applied Devel-
matic events in a healthy birth cohort of very young children in opmental Psychology, 49, 21–30.
the northeastern United States. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 23, Erwin, E. J., & Robinson, K. A. (2016). The joy of being: making way
725–733. for young children’s natural mindfulness. Early Child Develop-
Buckner, J. C., Mezzacappa, E., & Beardslee, W. R. (2009). Self- ment and Care, 186, 268–286.
regulation and its relations to adaptive functioning in low income Evans, G. W., & Kim, P. (2013). Childhood poverty, chronic Stress, self-
youths. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 79, 19–30. regulation, and coping. Child Development Perspectives, 7, 43–48.
Buono, A. (2019). Interweaving a mindfully somatic pedagogy into an Felver, J. C., Celis-de Hoyos, C. E., Tezanos, K., & Singh, N. N.
early childhood classroom. Pedagogies: An International Jour- (2016). A systematic review of mindfulness-based interventions
nal, 14, 150–168. for youth in school settings. Mindfulness, 7, 34–45.
Butzer, B., Bury, D., Telles, S., & Khalsa, S. B. S. (2016). Imple- Finkelhor, D., Turner, H., Shattuck, A., & Hamby, S. L. (2013).
menting yoga within the school curriculum: a scientific rationale Violence, crime, and abuse exposure in a national sample of
for improving social-emotional learning & positive student out- children and youth: An update. Pediatrics, 167, 614–621.
comes. Journal of Children’s Services, 11, 3–24. Flook, L., Goldberg, S. B., Pinger, L., & Davidson, R. J. (2015).
Butzer, B., Day, D., Potts, A., Ryan, C., Coulombe, S., Davies, B., Promoting prosocial behavior and self-regulatory skills in pre-
Weidknecht, K., Ebert, M., Flynn, L., & Khalsa, S. B. S. (2015). school children through a mindfulness-based kindness curricu-
Effects of a classroom-based yoga intervention on cortisol and lum. Developmental Psychology, 51, 44–51.
behavior in second- and third-grade students: a pilot study. Flook, L., Smalley, S. L., Kitil, M. J., Galla, B. M., Kaiser-Greenland,
Journal of Evidence-Based Complementary & Alternative Medi- S., Locke, J., & Kasari, C. (2010). Effects of mindful awareness
cine, 20, 41–49. practices on executive functions in elementary school children.
Carlson, S. M., Moses, L. J., & Claxton, L. J. (2004). Individual Journal of Applied School Psychology, 26, 70–95.
differences in executive functioning and theory of mind: an Friedman-Krauss, A. H., Raver, C. C., Morris, P. A., & Jones, S. M.
investigation of inhibitory control and planning ability. Journal of (2014a). The role of classroom-level child behavior problems in
Experimental Child Psychology, 87, 299–319. predicting preschool teacher stress and classroom emotional cli-
Chiesa, A., Calati, R., & Serretti, A. (2011). Does mindfulness training mate. Early Education and Development, 25, 530–552.
improve cognitive abilities? A systematic review of neuropsycho- Friedman-Krauss, A. H., Raver, C. C., Neuspiel, J. M., & Kinsel, J.
logical findings. Clinical Psychology Review, 31, 449–464. (2014b). Child behavior problems, teacher executive functions,
92 Journal of Child and Family Studies (2020) 29:82–93

and teacher stress in Head Start classrooms. Early Education and Leana, C. R., Appelbaum, E., & Shevchuk, I. (2009). Work process
Development, 25, 681–702. and quality of care: encouraging positive job crafting in childcare
Gard, T., Noggle, J. J., Park, C. L., Vago, D. R., & Wilson, A. (2014). classrooms. Academy of Management Journal, 52, 1169–1192.
Potential self-regulatory mechanisms of yoga for psychological Li-Grining, C., Raver, C. C., Champion, K., Sardin, L., Metzger, M.,
health. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 1–20. & Jones, S. M. (2010). Understanding and improving classroom
Greenberg, M. T. (2006). Promoting resilience in children and youth: emotional climate and behavior management in the “real world”:
preventive interventions and their interface with neuroscience. the role of Head Start teachers’ psychosocial stressors. Early
Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 1094, 139–150. Education and Development, 21, 65–94.
Greenberg, M. T., & Harris, A. R. (2012). Nurturing mindfulness in McClelland, M. M., Acock, A. C., Piccinin, A., Rhea, S. A., & Stal-
children and youth: current state of research. Child Development lings, M. C. (2013). Relations between preschool attention span-
Perspectives, 6, 161–166. persistence and age 25 educational outcomes. Early Childhood
Hackman, D. A., & Farah, M. J. (2009). Socioeconomic status and the Research Quarterly, 28, 314–324.
developing brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 65–73. McClelland, M. M., Cameron, C. E., Duncan, R., Bowles, R. P.,
Hagan, I., & Nayar, U. (2014). Yoga for children and young people’s Acock, A., Miao, A., & Pratt, M. E. (2014). Predictors of early
mental health and well-being: research review and reflections on growth in academic achievement: the head-toes-knees-shoulders
mental health potential of yoga. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 5, 1–6. task. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 599.
Hall-Kenyon, K. M., Bullough, R. V., MacKay, K. L., & Marshall, E. McClelland, M. M., Geldhof, J., Morrison, F., Gestdottier, S.,
(2014). Preschool teacher well-being: a review of the literature. Cameron, C., Bowers, E., … Grammer, J. (2018). Self-regulation.
Early Childhood Education Journal, 42, 153–162. In N. Halfon, C. B. Forrest, R. M. Lerner, & E. M. Faustman
Hammarberg, A., & Hagekull, B. (2002). The relation between pre- (Eds.) Handbook of life course health development. Cham,
school teachers’ classroom experiences and their perceived con- Switzerland: Springer.
trol over child behaviour. Early Child Development and Care, McConnico, N., Boynton-Jarrett, R., Bailey, C., & Nandi, M. (2016).
172, 625–634. A framework for trauma-sensitive schools: Infusing trauma-
Holmes, C., Levy, M., Smith, A., Pinne, S., & Neese, P. (2015). A informed practice into early childhood education systems. ZERO
model for creating a supportive trauma-informed culture for TO THREE, 36(5), 36–44.
children in preschool settings. Journal of Child and Family Stu- McGinty, A. S., Justice, L., & Rimm-Kaufman, S. E. (2008). Sense of
dies, 24, 1650–1659. school community for preschool teachers serving at-risk children.
Jee, S. H., Couderc, J. P., Swanson, D., Gallegos, A., Hilliard, C., Early Education and Development, 19, 361–384.
Blumkin, A., & Heinert, S. (2015). A pilot randomized trial Mejia, R., Kliewer, W., & Williams, L. (2006). Domestic violence
teaching mindfulness-based stress reduction to traumatized youth exposure in Colombian adolescents: pathways to violent and
in foster care. Complementary Therapies in Clinical Practice, 21, prosocial behavior. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 19, 257–267.
201–209. Miller, G. E., Chen, E., & Parker, K. J. (2011). Psychological stress in
Jennings, P. A. (2015). Early childhood teachers’ well-being, mind- childhood and susceptibility to the chronic diseases of aging:
fulness and self-compassion in relation to classroom quality and moving toward a model of behavioral and biological mechan-
attitudes towards challenging students. Mindfulness, 6, 732–743. isms. Psychological Bulletin, 137, 959–997.
Jennings, P. A. (2016). CARE for teachers: a mindfulness-based Mindfulness All-Party Parliamentary Group (MAPP). (2015). Mindful
approach to promoting teachers’ social and emotional compe- nation U.K. The mindfulness initiative. Retrieved from
tence and well-being. In K. Schonert-Reichl & R. Roeser (Eds), http://www.themindfulnessinitiative.org.uk/publications/mindful-
Handbook of mindfulness in education. Mindfulness in beha- nation-uk-report
vioral health (pp. 1650–1659). New York, NY: Springer. Moffitt, T. E., Arseneault, L., Belsky, D., Dickson, N., Hancox, R. J.,
Jennings, P. A., Brown, J. L., Frank, J., Doyle, S., Oh, Y., Davis, R., & Harrington, H., & Caspi, A. (2011). A gradient of childhood self-
Greenberg, M. (2017). Impacts of the CARE for teachers program control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proceedings of
on teachers’ social and emotional competence and classroom the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of
interactions. Journal of Educational Pyschology, 109, America, 108, 2693–2698.
1010–1028. Moriguchi, Y., & Hiraki, K. (2011). Longitudinal development of
Jennings, P. A., & Greenberg, M. (2009). The prosocial classroom: teacher prefrontal function during early childhood. Developmental Cog-
social and emotional competence in relation to child and classroom nitive Neuroscience, 1, 153–162.
outcomes. Review of Educational Research, 79, 491–525. National Center for Mental Health Promotion and Youth Violence
Kaiser-Greenland, S. (2010). The mindful child: how to help your kid Prevention. (2012). Childhood trauma and its effect on health
manage stress and become happier, kinder, and more compas- development. Retrieved from http://www.promoteprevent.org/
sionate. New York, NY: Free Press. sites/www.promoteprevent.org/files/resources/childhood%20tra
Khalsa, S. B. (2004). Yoga as a therapeutic intervention: a bibliometric uma_brief_in_final.pdf.
analysis of published research studies. Indian Journal of Phy- Neville, H. J., Stevens, C., Pakulak, E., Bell, T. A., Fanning, J., Klein,
siology and Pharmacology, 48, 269–285. S., & Isbell, E. (2013). Family-based training program improves
Khalsa, S. B., & Butzer, B. (2016). Yoga in school settings: a research brain function, cognition, and behavior in lower socioeconomic
review. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1373, status preschoolers. Proceedings of the National Academy of
45–55. Sciences of the United States of America, 110, 12138–12143.
Kim, J., & Cicchetti, D. (2010). Longitudinal pathways linking child New York State Education Department. (2015). Syracuse city school
maltreatment, emotion regulation, peer relations, and psycho- district 2012. Albany, NY: New York State Education Department.
pathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, https://reportcards.nysed.gov/schools.php?district=
706–716. 800000040902&year=2011.
Lane, S., Rubinstein, R., Bergen-Cico, D., Jennings-Bey, T., Stone Ochsner, K. N., Ray, R. R., Hughes, B., McRae, K., Cooper, J. C.,
Fish, L., Larsen, D., & Robinson, J. A. (2017). Neighborhood Weber, J., & Gross, J. J. (2009). Bottom-up and top-down pro-
trauma due to violence: a multilevel analysis. Journal of Health cesses in emotion generation: common and distinct neural
Care for the Poor and Underserved, 28, 446–462. mechanisms. Psychological Science, 20, 1322–1331.
Journal of Child and Family Studies (2020) 29:82–93 93

Poehlmann-Tynan, J., Vigna, A. B., Weymouth, L. A., Gerstein, E. D., Stapp, A. C., & Wolff, K. (2019). Young children’s experiences with
Burnson, C., Zabransky, M., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (2016). A pilot yoga in an early childhood setting. Early Child Development and
study of contemplative practices with economically dis- Care, 189, 1397–1410.
advantaged preschoolers: children’s empathetic and self- Tang, Y., Yang, L., Leve, L. D., & Harold, G. T. (2012). Improving
regulatory behaviors. Mindfulness, 7, 46–58. executive function and its neurobiological mechanisms through a
Ponitz, C. C., McClelland, M. M., Matthews, J. M., & Morrison, F. J. mindfulness-based intervention. Child Development Perspectives,
(2009). A structured observation of behavioral self-regulation and 6, 361–366.
its contribution to kindergarten outcomes. Developmental Psy- Telles, S., Singh, N., & Balkrishna, A. (2012). Managing mental
chology, 45, 605–619. health disorders resulting from trauma through yoga: a review.
Posner, M. I., & Peterson, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the Depression Research and Treatment, 2012, 1–9.
human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 25–42. Thierry, K. L., Bryant, H. L., Nobles, S. S., & Norris, K. S. (2016).
Razza, R. A., Bergen-Cico, D., & Raymond, K. (2015). Enhancing Two-year impact of a mindfulness-based program on pre-
preschoolers’ self-regulation via mindful yoga. Journal of Child schoolers’ self-regulation and academic performance. Early
and Family Studies, 24, 372–385. Education and Development, 27, 805–821.
Raver, C. C. (2012). Low-income children’s self-regulation in the United States Census Bureau. (2016). American fact finder: commu-
classroom: scientific inquiry for social change. The American nity facts. Washington, DC: USA http://factfinder.census.gov/fa
Psychologist, 67, 681–689. ces/nav/jsf/pages/index.xhtml.
Roberts, A., Lo-Casale-Crouch, J., Hamre, B., & DeCoster, J. (2016). Wanless, S. B., McClelland, M. M., Acock, A., Ponitz, C. C., Son, S.
Exploring teachers’ depressive symptoms, interaction quality, and H., Lan, X., & Li, S. (2011). Measuring behavioral regulation in
children’s social-emotional development in Head Start. Early four societies. Psychological Assessment, 23, 364–378.
Education and Development, 27, 642–654. Waters, L., Barsky, A., Ridd, A., & Allen, K. (2015). Contemplative
Roeser, R. W., & Peck, S. C. (2009). An education in awareness: self, education: a systematic, evidence-based review of the effect of
motivation and self-regulation in contemplative perspective. meditation interventions in schools. Educational Psychology
Educational Psychologist, 44, 119–136. Review, 27, 103–134.
Roid, G. H., & Miller, L. J. (1997). Leiter International Performance Weathers, F. W., Blake, D. D., Schnurr, P. P., Kaloupek, D. G., Marx,
Scale-revised. Wood Dale, IL: Stoelting. B. P., & Keane, T. M. (2013). The life events checklist for DSM-
Rothbart, M. K., & Bates, J. E. (2006). Temperament. In N. Eisenberg, 5 (LEC-5). Instrument available from the National Center for
W. Damon, R. M. Lerner, N. E. Eisenberg, W. E. Damon & R. PTSD at www.ptsd.va.gov.
M. E. Lerner (Eds), Handbook of child psychology: vol. 3, social, Weathers, F. W., Litz, B. T., Huska, J. A., & Keane, T. M. (1994).
emotional, and personality development. 6th ed. (pp. 99–166). PTSD Checklist—Civilian version. Boston: National Center for
Hoboken, NJ, US: Wiley. PTSD, Behavioral Science Division.
Rueda, M. R., Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Halparin, J. D., Gruber, D. B., Wenig, M. (2003). YogaKids: educating the whole child through yoga.
Lercari, L. P., & Posner, M. I. (2004). Development of attentional New York, NY: Stewart, Tabori, and Chang.
networks in childhood. Neuropsychologia, 42, 1029–1040. Whitaker, R. C., Becker, B. D., Herman, A. N., & Gooze, R. A.
Sacks, V., & Murphey, D. (2018). The prevalence of adverse childhood (2013). The physical and mental health of Head Start staff: the
experiences, nationally, by state, and by ethnicity. Retrieved from Pennsylvania Head Start staff wellness survey, 2012. Preventing
https://www.childtrends.org/publications/prevalence-adverse- Chronic Disease, 10, 1–9.
childhood-experiences-nationally-state-race-ethnicity/. Whitaker, R. C., Dearth-Wesley, T., Gooze, R. A., Becker, B. D.,
Sanders, L. D., Stevens, C., Coch, D., & Neville, H. J. (2006). Gallagher, K. C., & McEwen, B. S. (2014). Adverse childhood
Selective auditory attention in 3- to 5-year-old children: an event- experiences, dispositional mindfulness, and adult health. Pre-
related potential study. Neuropsychologia, 44, 2126–2138. ventive Medicine, 67, 147–153.
Serwacki, M., & Cook-Cottone, C. P. (2012). Yoga in the schools: a Williams, K. E. (2018). Moving to the beat: using music, rhythm,
systematic review of the literature. International Journal of Yoga and movement to enhance self-regulation in early childhood
Therapy, 22, 101–110. classrooms. International Journal of Early Childhood, 50,
Shahinfar, A., Fox, N. A., & Leavitt, L. A. (2000). Preschool children’s 85–100.
exposure to violence: relation of behavior problems to parent and Willis, E., & Dinehart, L. H. (2014). Contemplative practices in early
child reports. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 70, 115–125. childhood: implications for self-regulation skills and school
Sharp, J. E., & Jennings, P. A. (2016). Strengthening teacher presence readiness. Early Child Development and Care, 184, 487–499.
through mindfulness: what educators say about the Cultivating Zelazo, P. D., & Lyons, K. E. (2012). The potential benefits of
Awareness and Resilience in Education (CARE) program. mindfulness training in early childhood: a developmental social
Mindfulness, 7, 209–218. cognitive neuroscience perspective. Child Development Per-
Shonkoff, J. P., Boyce, W. T., & McEwen, B. S. (2009). Neuroscience, spectives, 6, 154–160.
molecular biology, and the childhood roots of health disparities: Zenner, C., Herrnleben-Kurz, S., & Walach, H. (2014). Mindfulness-
building a new framework for health promotion and disease preven- based interventions in schools: a systematic review and meta-
tion. Journal of the American Medical Association, 301, 2252–2259. analysis. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1–20.
Singh, N. N., Lancioni, G. E., Winton, A. S., Karazsia, B. T., & Singh, Zhai, F., Raver, C. C., & Li-Grining, C. (2011). Classroom-based
J. (2013). Mindfulness training for teachers changes the behavior interventions and teachers’ perceived job stressors and con-
of their preschool students. Research in Human Development, 10, fidence: evidence from a randomized trial in Head Start settings.
211–233. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 26, 442–452.

You might also like