You are on page 1of 13

Psychological Research (2019) 83:1519–1530

https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1007-6

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Training and transfer effects of interference control training


in children and young adults
Xin Zhao1 · Lina Jia1 

Received: 26 December 2017 / Accepted: 1 April 2018 / Published online: 24 April 2018
© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2018

Abstract
Many studies have examined transfer of working memory (WM) training improvements to non-trained cognitive tasks,
with largely disappointing results. Interference control has been suggested to be a central feature of WM. However, studies
examining transfer effects of a training program exclusively and directly targeting interference control are lacking. Forty-one
10‒12 year-old children and 47 19‒24 year-old adults were assigned to an adaptive interference control training or active
control condition. Transfer of training effects to tasks measuring interference control, response inhibition, WM updating,
task-switching, and non-verbal fluid intelligence were assessed during a 3-month follow-up session and/or an immediate
post-training session. Substantial evidence of training improvements and a positive transfer effect to a non-trained interfer-
ence control task were observed for both age groups. Marginal evidence for beneficial transfer of training effects for the
trained compared to non-trained participants was found for a WM task for both age groups, and for the children for another
interference control task and a response inhibition task. However, these transfer effects were absent during the 3-month
follow-up measurement. These results suggest some potential for interference control training programs to enhance aspects of
cognitive functioning, with some evidence for a more wide-spread, but short-lived, transfer for children compared to adults.

Introduction recent reviews). Relatively few studies explicitly focused


on training other aspects of executive functioning, such as
The issue of the trainability of cognitive functions in gen- response inhibition (e.g., Enge, Behnke, Fleischhauer, Küt-
eral, and higher-order cognitive processes implied in execu- tler, Kliegel, & Strobel, 2014; Thorell, Lindqvist, Bergman
tive functions (EFs) in particular, have received considerable Nutley, Bohlin, and Klingberg, 2009), and task switching
attention since the 1980s. EFs are related to many daily-life (e.g., Grönholm-Nyman et  al., 2017; Karbach & Kray,
cognitive skills and psychological well-being (Diamond, 2009; Minear, & Shah, 2008; Pereg, Shahar, & Meiran,
2013). They enable the display of goal-directed behavior 2013; Zinke, Einert, Pfenning, & Kliegel, 2012). By and
in the face of distracting information and are decomposable large, these studies suggest that training improves perfor-
into a limited number of more basic processes, such as work- mance on the trained task and on similar non-trained tasks
ing memory monitoring and updating (WM), inhibition of (called near-transfer effects). However, at least for healthy
prepotent responses, and task switching (Miyake, Friedman, participants and when using appropriate control groups, the
Emerson, Witzki, Howerter, & Wager, 2000). evidence of training-induced beneficial transfer to dissimi-
The majority of studies examining whether EFs and gen- lar non-trained tasks is not convincing (far-transfer effects,
eral intelligence of healthy and clinical populations can be Dougherty, Hamovitz, & Tidwell, 2016; Soveri et al., 2017).
improved through training adopted training tasks specifi- Moreover, even if near- or far-transfer effects are found, it
cally targeting WM (see Melby-Lervåg, Redick, & Hulme, is far from conclusive that these effects are due to a genuine
2016; Soveri, Antfolk, Karlsson, Salo, & Laine, 2017, for improvement of the trained cognitive function(s) or instead
reflect the (learned) use of some relatively task-specific,
strategy-based approach (e.g., Shipstead, Redick, & Engle,
* Lina Jia
psyjialn@163.com 2012).
As indicated above, training programs targeting inhibition
1
Behavior Rehabilitation Training Research Institution, are relatively few and did not reveal convincing evidence
School of Psychology, Northwest Normal University, 967 for (strong) transfer effects (Enge et al., 2014; Thorell et al.,
East Anning Road, Lanzhou 730070, China

13
Vol.:(0123456789)

1520 Psychological Research (2019) 83:1519–1530

2009). One feature of these studies is that they primarily task-switching, and general intelligence. Moreover, we used
used response or behavioural inhibition tasks for training. two different age groups: a group of 10–12 year-old children
However, behavioural inhibition is but one of other types and a group of young adults. The age range for the children
or components of inhibition that have been suggested in the was based on previous evidence that interference control, as
literature (e.g., Nigg, 2000). Specifically, more ‘cognitive’ trained in the present study by the Stroop-like task, shows a
types of inhibition (hereafter referred to using the more gen- developmental spurt between about 10 and 13 years of age
eral term of cognitive inhibition), such as the suppression of (Brocki & Bohlin, 2004). We reasoned that training within
distracting or proactively interfering information, have been the 10–12 years of age range might be associated with the
found to be correlated with, but separable from, response strongest training effects, boosting potential development-
inhibition (Stahl et al., 2014). Importantly, cognitive inhibi- based enhanced plasticity. Given the previous evidence of
tion capabilities have been found to show considerable over- larger transfer benefits of cognitive training programs for
lap with WM capacity (Jonides & Nee, 2006). This overlap children compared to young adults (e.g., Karbach & Unger,
prompts the question whether a training specifically focus- 2014; Zhao, Chen, & Maes, 2016), we expected that any
sing on this central element of WM would lead to stronger near- or far-transfer benefits (if present) would be larger for
transfer effects than those observed for response inhibition the children than adults.
training programs, and for that matter, also for WM-training
protocols that do not explicitly train cognitive inhibition.
Notably, training studies addressing cognitive inhibi- Method
tion are even rarer than response inhibition training studies.
To our knowledge, cognitive inhibition training has until Participants
now only been realized in the framework of WM updat-
ing training tasks, such as the n-back task. The participant The study included 52 healthy undergraduate students from
in an n-back task has to monitor whether the identity of Northwest Normal University (begin data collection: May
the current item of sequentially presented items is identical 2016), and 47 fifth graders from an elementary school in
to that presented n trials before. Involvement of proactive Gansu province (begin data acquisition: April 2017). The
interference has been manipulated in this task by varying the adults and children were mostly (adults: 96%; children: 98%)
number of ‘lure’ trials. These are the trials in which a recent from the Han population. The participants were randomly
stimulus, which is not at position n but close to it, is identical assigned to a training and a control group. Further details
to the current stimulus. On such trials, relative to trials in for the adult sample are as follows. Three participants from
which identical previous stimuli are relatively far removed the training group did not complete the training program;
from the current stimulus, the participant has to inhibit the the data for two participants from the control group were
tendency evoked by the familiarity of the recent item to iden- not complete because of an internship elsewhere. Their data
tify the current stimulus as a target stimulus, also called the were discarded. Hence, the adult training group consisted
control of proactive interference. Oelhafen et al. (2013) and of 13 women and 10 men (M = 20.39 years, SD = 1.31,
Loosli et al. (2016) manipulated the number of lure trials range 19–24); the control group included 16 women and 8
and did not find differential transfer effects to other EF tasks: men (M = 20.92, SD = 0.93, range 20–24). The groups did
there was no convincing evidence of transfer, regardless of not significantly differ in age, sex distribution, and hand-
interference control demand in the training task. However, edness. Based on self-reports, all participants had normal
these studies used a very specific and arguably somewhat or corrected-to-normal vision, were not colour-blind, and
indirect way of training interference control, based on the had no history of psychiatric or neurological disease. The
suppression of familiarity-evoked responses. The question students were paid 8 RMB (about €1) for each training ses-
remains whether a training protocol that implies a more sion. All students had signed an informed consent form and
direct training of interference control, using a task that is received small presents for their participation in the pre-and
derived from a very commonly used interference control post-training, and 3-month follow-up (F3) assessment ses-
task, would yield more promising results. sions. Further details for the child sample are as follows. The
Against this background, the purpose of the present study elementary school was contacted via the Education Bureau
was to evaluate training and transfer effects of repeatedly of Lanzhou city and the children had their primary caretak-
performing a Stroop-like task (MacLeod, 1991), using an ers sign informed consent forms. Participants were randomly
active control group as comparison. The task entailed the assigned to either the training or control group according to
consistent suppression of responses that were automati- the last two digits of the child’s student number. The train-
cally triggered by the identity of visual stimuli. Transfer ing or transfer data of six children (three from each group)
benefits were assessed using non-trained tasks measuring were incomplete for various reasons, such as transference
interference control, response inhibition, working memory, to another school, participation in sports competitions, or

13
Psychological Research (2019) 83:1519–1530 1521

sick leave. The data from these children were not included types, excluding trials with a RT < 200 ms and an incorrect
in the data analysis. Therefore, the training group included response. For statistical analysis, we used an interference
9 girls and 12 boys (M = 10.48 years, SD = 0.60, range index representing the difference in mean RT on incongruent
10–12), and the control group consisted of 10 girls and 10 and congruent trials. A high score reflects weak interference
boys (M = 10.65 years, SD = 0.67, range 10–12). One child control.
from each of the control and training groups did not perform
the transfer tests during the F3 session. During this session, Flanker task
one child from the control group did not complete the go/
no-go task and two children from the training group failed to The Flanker task was used to obtain an additional index of
complete the switching task. Based on medical reports and interference control, also implicating a near-transfer task.
teachers’ information, all children had normal or corrected- Stimuli consisted of five arrows presented in the center of
to-normal vision, were not colour-blind, and had no history the screen. The central arrow was the target; the four non-
of psychiatric or neurological disease. The children from center arrows were distractors. Trials were either congruent
the training group received some pencils as remuneration. (→→→→→ or ←←←←←) or incongruent (→→←→→
All children received a small present (pencil, ruler, etc.) for or ←←→←←). Participants had to make a response in
their participation in the assessment sessions. There was no accordance with the direction of the middle arrow by press-
significant difference between the two groups in age, sex ing “J” and “F” on the keyboard if the arrow was pointing to
distribution, and handedness. The study was approved by the the right and left, respectively. On each trial, a fixation cross
local ethics committee and all experimental manipulations was first presented for 500 ms, followed by a blank screen
were performed in accordance with the approved guidelines. that was presented for a variable duration between 300 and
500 ms. The stimulus was presented for 1500 ms (response
Pre‑ and post‑training, and F3 tests window), followed by a 1000-ms blank screen. The next
trial started immediately thereafter. After 16 practice trials,
We employed the following six transfer tasks, which were the main task was initiated, which comprised four blocks of
all programmed using Eprime software. 32 trials each (16 incongruent trials), with a possible break
between blocks if the participant wished to have a break. The
Stroop color–word interference task order of trials was random and the test lasted about 15 min.
The dependent measure was the mean RT on the different
This task was used as near-transfer task. The participant had trial types, based on RTs > 200 ms and trials with a correct
to indicate the color in which Chinese characters (Hanzi) or response. We computed an interference index by subtract-
the symbols “####” were printed as quickly and accurately ing the mean RT on congruent trials from the mean RT on
as possible, by pressing “F” for red and “J” for green on a incongruent trials. A high score reflects weak interference
standard keyboard. The corresponding keys were covered by control.
a piece of red and green paper. The Hanzi represented the
colors red or green and were printed in either red or green. Go/no‑go task
The task contained congruent, incongruent, and neutral tri-
als. Congruent trials consisted of the Hanzi referring to the A go/no-go task was used to assess response inhibition
word “red” printed in red and the word “green” printed in (far-transfer task). The task consisted of practice trials and
green. On incongruent trials the word “red” was printed in four blocks of 100 experimental trials each. Each practice
green and the word “green” was printed in red. On neu- trial commenced with a white fixation point presented for
tral trials, the symbols “####” were either printed in red or 1000 ms in the center of the screen against a black back-
green. Each trial commenced with a fixation cross that was ground, followed by a 600-ms presentation of either the
presented for 500 ms, followed by a 1000-ms blank screen. letter X or Y. Thereafter, a blank screen was presented
Thereafter, the target stimulus (colored Hanzi or symbols) for 1000 ms. The next trial began immediately thereafter,
was presented for 1500 ms (response window). A blank implicating a response window of 1600 ms. The partici-
screen was subsequently presented for a variable duration pant was instructed to respond to each X by pressing “J” on
between 600 and 1000 ms. The next trial started immediately a standard computer keyboard and to refrain from respond-
thereafter. The task contained 18 practice trials and three ing upon presentation of the letter Y. The participant was
blocks of 36 experimental trials each; the participant could asked to perform the task as quickly and accurately as
have a break between blocks. Each block consisted of 12 possible. The participant continued with the experimental
congruent, 12 incongruent, and 12 neutral trials. The order phase of the task after reaching an accuracy level of ≥ 85%
of trials was random and the test lasted about 15 min. The during a block of 20 trials. Each trial of the following
dependent measure was the mean RT on the different trial two experimental blocks was identical to the trials in the

13

1522 Psychological Research (2019) 83:1519–1530

practice phase. For the adult participants, each 100-trial Switching task
block consisted of 75% trials with the letter X (go trials)
and 25% trials with the letter Y (no-go trials). Also, based This task measured the ability to flexibly switch between
on a pilot study, for the children we choose a 50% go-50% tasks (far-transfer task). A series of digits (from 1 to 9 except
no-go trials distribution to achieve pre-training accuracy 5) colored in red or blue were presented in the center of the
rates that were comparable to those for the adults. The screen. Participants had to either make a magnitude or par-
order of trials was random. During the next two 100-trial ity judgment for each digit, depending on the digit’s color.
blocks (which were preceded by one or more blocks of In task A, participants judged whether the current red digit
practice trials until the participant reached an accuracy was larger than 5 or not (magnitude task), and to respond by
level of ≥ 85%), the participant had to press “J” to the pressing “A” if the digit was < 5, and by pressing “L” if the
letter Y and not respond to the letter X. In these blocks, digit was > 5. In task B, participants had to judge whether
the percentage of Y and X trials was 75 and 25%, respec- the blue digit was odd or even (parity task). They had to
tively, for the adults and 50 and 50% for the children. The respond by pressing “A” if the digit was odd and “L” if the
participant could have a break between trial blocks and the digit was even. Participants first performed two single-task
task lasted approximately 15 min. The dependent measure (task A and task B) practice blocks until they reached an
from this test was the difference score: proportion of hits accuracy level of 75%. Subsequently, they performed 20
(correct response to go stimuli) minus proportion of false experimental blocks: 10 single-task blocks consisting of 8
alarms (incorrect response to no-go stimuli). A high score trials each, and 10 mixed-task blocks, each consisting of
represents a strong inhibition capacity. 17 trials. During mixed-task blocks, the participant had to
switch between tasks A and B every second trial. The order
of blocks was random with the constraint that two single
Numerical WM updating: easy and difficult task and two mixed-task blocks were grouped together. Each
trial commenced with a fixation cross that was presented
Two tasks were used to measure WM ability (far-transfer for 1400 ms, followed by the target. The target was presented
tasks). On each trial of the easy task, a series of single until the participant responded and the next fixation cross
digits, ranging from 0 to 9, were presented consecu- was presented 25 ms thereafter. The test lasted about 20 min.
tively in the center of the screen. The length of the digit Based on RTs > 200 ms and RTs < 4000 ms of trials with a
sequences varied and consisted of 5, 7, 9, or 11 digits. The correct response, for each participant we first computed the
four trial types, defined by sequence length, appeared an median RT on switch and non-switch trials of the mixed-
equal number of times and were presented in a random task blocks. These RTs were used to compute the dependent
order. Participants were instructed to sequentially remem- measure: median RT on switch trials minus mean median
ber the final three numbers presented. For example, if the RT on non-switch trials. Hence, a high score reflects a poor
presented digits were 7–6–3–1–4–5–8, the participants switching ability.
should have remembered 7–76–763–631–314–145–458.
After presentation of the last digit of the sequence, a blank Raven’s progressive matrices test
bar was presented on the screen and the participant had
to enter the last three digits presented (i.e., 4–5–8) using Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices (RSPM; Raven,
the keyboard. Each trial commenced with a fixation cross, Raven, & Court, 2000) test and Raven’s Advanced Progres-
which was presented for 500 milliseconds. Subsequently, sive Matrices test (RAPM; Raven, Court, & Raven, 1977)
the digits were shown for 1750 ms, followed by a blank were used as measure of non-verbal intelligence for the chil-
screen that was presented for a random time between 800 dren and adults, respectively. These tests consist of a series
and 1200 ms. The task included three trial blocks. The of incomplete designs. The participant is asked to select
first consisted of eight practice trials. The second and third the correct part to complete the designs from a number of
blocks constituted the formal test and each block contained options printed underneath. For the children, the 60-item
12 trials. The difficult WM updating task was identical to RSPM was split into two equal parts. They performed the
the easy task except that the digits were shown for only even- and odd-numbered items in the pre- and post-training
750 ms. Each participant finished the easy task prior to the test, respectively. The adults completed 18 even-numbered
difficult task. The dependent measure was the total number problems from the RAPM before training and 18 odd-num-
of points acquired during the task, with one point assigned bered problems during the post-training session. The maxi-
for each correct digit correctly put in the correct serial mum time allowed to work on the task was 20 min for both
position, implicating a maximum score of 72 points. This the children and adults. The dependent measure was the pro-
number was converted to proportion correct responses. portion of correctly solved problems. It must be noted that
the RSPM and RAPM are not directly comparable, thereby

13
Psychological Research (2019) 83:1519–1530 1523

complicating the interpretation of possible age group dif- and accuracy of their responses. Mean RTs on correct tri-
ferences. However, we were primarily interested in perfor- als, based on the three training tasks, were used as outcome
mance differences in the pre- vs. post-training assessment measure.
as a function of training condition within each age group.
Procedure
Computerized training program
All participants completed the pre-training tests in the fol-
A Stroop-like task was used for training. The task included lowing order and with approximately 24 h in between: the
three types of picture pairs: scenery pictures (sun and moon; Stroop task (Day 1), Flanker task (Day 2), go/no-go task
8 different pictures of each category), cartoon characters (Day 3), WM updating tasks (Day 4) and the RAVEN (Day
(boy and girl; 8 different pictures of each category) and 5). Participants on Saturday and Sunday have rest, so task
fruit pictures (big watermelon and small watermelon; 1 pic- was put off until next Monday for switching task. Adults
ture of each type). The task consisted of three blocks of 80 were divided into three groups in pre-test, about 18 people
experimental trials each on each session. One type of picture per group and children were divided into two groups, about
pair was presented during each block, with a fixed order of 21 people per group. The training group (23 adults or 21
picture-type blocks on each session: sun/moon, followed by children) then completed the adaptive Stroop-like training
boy/girl, followed by big/small. Within each block, pictures consisting of one 240-trial session on each of 20 successive
were presented in a random order with the constraint that weekdays. The training was performed under supervision
each category (e.g., sun or moon, boy or girl, were presented of teacher (for the child sample) and/or a lab assistant in
on 50% of the trials). Each trial started with a 500-ms fixa- one group in a laboratory room (for the adults) or in the
tion cross, followed by a blank screen that was presented school’s computer room (children). The daily training time
for a variable duration between 400 and 600 ms. The target was approximately 20 min. The control groups (both the
stimulus (picture) was then presented for 2000 ms, followed children and adults, 20 children or 24 adults) made sand
by a 1000-ms blank screen. The next trial started imme- paintings at the same time and in the same environment as
diately thereafter. Participants could have a short break held for the training group. Sand paining is part of the Bud-
between trial blocks. The task lasted about 20 min. Task dhist tradition and involves the individual to evenly spread
difficulty was adapted by decreasing or increasing the inter- fine colored sand on a sticky drawing paper containing tem-
stimulus interval (ISI, measured between stimulus onset plates. These templates, consisting of complex geometric
and onset of the next trial) for each type of 80-trial block, patterns, have to be completed stepwise. Completion of the
starting with an ISI of 3000 ms during the first training ses- painting requires much patience, endurance, and focused
sion, depending on the participant’s percentage of correct attention. After completion of the training or control pro-
responses on that type of trial block during the preceding gram, all participants performed the same tests, in the same
session. For example, for the first block (sun/moon), if the order and with the same inter-session interval as used in the
percentage of correct responses on this block in the previ- pre-training test sessions (post-training assessment). When
ous session was < 60%, the ISI would increase with 200 ms. obtaining a significant transfer effect for a given task, a fol-
If the percentage of correct responses on the previous cor- low-up measurement was planned to be performed approxi-
responding block was > 90%, the ISI would be reduced by mately 3 months after the post-training assessment.
200 ms. Finally, in case the percentage of correct responses
ranged from 60 to 90%, the ISI would remain the same. The Statistical analysis
same procedure was maintained for each of the other two
trial-type blocks (boy/girl and big/small). The “F” key on Prior to performing the main analyses, we first tested, for
the response keyboard was labeled with the word “day”, each age group separately, whether there was a difference
“J” with “night”, “D” with “boy”, “K” with “girl”, “S” with between the trained and control participants on any of
“big”, and “L” with “small”. The participant was instructed the pre-training transfer tests, which was not the case (all
to respond as quickly as possible to each stimulus by press- ps > 0.05). We used a repeated measures analysis of variance
ing the key corresponding to the “wrong” exemplar of the (ANOVA) to evaluate the training RT data, with group (chil-
category to which the picture belonged. For example, upon dren vs. adults) as between-subjects factor and training ses-
presentation of a moon picture, the participant had to press sion (1–20) as within-subject factor. Pre- and post-training
the key corresponding to “day”, and when shown a drawing task performance was analyzed using a group (children vs.
of a boy, the correct response was “girl”. After 10 days of adults), training condition (training vs. control), and ses-
training, the labels of the keys were switched so that now sion (pre- vs. post-training) ANOVA, using the dependent
“F” was labeled with “night”, “J” with “day”, etc. After each measure from each task. An identical ANOVA was used
trial block, participants received feedback about mean speed to analyze pre-training and F3 assessment differences as a

13

1524 Psychological Research (2019) 83:1519–1530

function of group and training condition. It is important to 800


note that identical analyses using a dependent measure for
the Stroop, Flanker, and Switching tasks that was based on 700
accuracy rather than RT data did not reveal any significant

Mean RT (ms)
effects of critical interest. This absence of effects was also 600
partly due to the fact that the overall accuracy (proportion
of trials with a correct response) was rather high, at least for 500
the Stroop (M 0.93) and Flanker (M 0.95) tasks. Significant
interactions were followed up by simple main effect analyses 400 Adults
and analysis of covariance (ANCOVA). An alpha level of Children
p < 0.05 was adopted throughout. Partial eta-squared (ηp2) 300
was used as an effect size estimate. In addition to the tradi- 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21
tional ANCOVAs, testing the significance of a hypothesis of Training Session
post-training test performance difference between training
conditions after controlling for pre-training performance, in Fig. 1  Mean (+ SEM) RT on each of the 20 training sessions for the
the case of a significant result we also performed a Bayes- children and adults
ian ANCOVA on the transfer test data using Bayes factors
(BF). These tests, which were performed using JASP (Com-
puter software; Version 0.8.0.1), enable a quantification of Transfer effects
the strength of the evidence for the alternative compared
to the null hypothesis, with a ­BF10 larger than 3 indicating Table 1 shows the groups’ mean score on the performance
substantial evidence, and a B ­ F10 between 1 and 3 indicating measure of the different transfer tasks during the pre- and
only marginal evidence in favor of the alternative hypothesis post-training sessions, and F3 session (if performed).
relative to a null model. In our case, the alternative model Details of the overall ANOVAs on the pre- and post-train-
consisted of the model explaining post-training task perfor- ing performance measures are presented in Table 2.
mance on the basis of training condition and pre-training
task performance. The null model incorporated only the pre-
training scores. Stroop task

ANOVA using the Stroop interference score on the pre-


and post-training sessions revealed significant main ses-
Results sion and training condition × session interaction effects.
The interaction effect reflected a significantly lower inter-
Training ference score at post-compared to pre-training test for the
trained participants, F(1, 49) = 20.35, p < 0.001, η p2 =
Figure 1 displays the mean RT for the adults and children 0.32, but not control participants, F < 1. Moreover, also
on each of the training sessions. ANOVA (see Table 1, for when controlling for the pre-training performance differ-
details) revealed a main effect of group, reflecting overall ence between the training and control groups, the trained
faster RTs for the adults than the children. The main effect participants had a lower interference score during the post-
of session reflected the general decrease in RTs across ses- training test than the control participants, ANCOVA, F(1,
sions for both groups. Follow-up analyses of the group × ses- 85) = 6.93, p = 0.01, η p2 = 0.08, ­B F 10 = 4.14. ANOVA
sion interaction revealed significantly shorter RTs for the using the pre-training and F3 data revealed a signifi-
adults compared to the children on sessions 1–3 and 9–12, cant main session effect, F(1, 80) = 6.74, p = 0.01, η p2
ps < 0.02, but not on each of sessions 4–8, ps > 0.06. A nota- = 0.08, and a training condition × session interaction,
ble feature was a significant increase in RT from session 10 F(1, 80) = 4.33, p = 0.04, η p2 = 0.05. This interaction
to 11 in the children, p < 0.001, which was likely caused by reflected a significantly lower interference score at the
the change in response-to-key mapping initiated on session F3 than the pre-training test for the trained participants,
11. F(1, 41) = 9.80, p = 0.003, η p2 = 0.19, but not the con-
trols, F < 1. However, when controlling for the pre-training
performance difference between the training condition
groups, the difference between trained and control par-
ticipants on post-training test performance was not statisti-
cally significant, ANCOVA, F < 1. These results suggest

13
Psychological Research (2019) 83:1519–1530 1525

Table 1  Descriptive statistics (mean and SEM) for the groups’ pre- and post-training and 3-month follow-up (F3) test performance measures
Task (dependent measure) Session Children trained Children control Adults trained Adults control

Stroop (interference score in ms) Pre-training 67.72 (13.51) 35.06 (18.28) 45.06 (13.83) 23.24 (9.15)
Post-training 9.07 (10.42) 38.49 (16.07) 4.60 (6.02) 28.79 (11.52)
F3 20.80 (7.30) 35.89 (12.76) 20.13 (8.47) 16.24 (7.19)
Flanker (interference score in ms) Pre-training 44.96 (5.02) 33.33 (6.09) 16.88 (3.29) 14.93 (3.11)
Post-training 8.20 (3.87) 20.99 (5.45) 15.98 (2.21) 11.25 (3.76)
F3 19.40 (4.52) 28.82 (9.66)
Go/no-go (hits-FA) Pre-training 0.80 (0.04) 0.82 (0.02) 0.87 (0.01) 0.87 (0.02)
Post-training 0.87 (0.02) 0.79 (0.05) 0.85 (0.01) 0.84 (0.02)
F3 0.86 (0.04) 0.81 (0.04)
WM-easy (proportion correct) Pre-training 0.88 (0.01) 0.83 (0.02) 0.97 (0.01) 0.97 (0.01)
Post-training 0.91 (0.01) 0.84 (0.03) 0.97 (0.01) 0.97 (0.01)
WM-difficult (proportion correct) Pre-training 0.85 (0.02) 0.84 (0.03) 0.92 (0.01) 0.95 (0.01)
Post-training 0.90 (0.03) 0.81 (0.04) 0.94 (0.01) 0.94 (0.01)
F3 0.94 (0.01) 0.96 (0.01)
Switching (switch cost, ms) Pre-training 296.48 (57.72) 327.33 (77.25) 295.00 (48.46) 188.98 (31.22)
Post-training 159.17 (62.44) 347.15 (56.90) 162.50 (28.55) 130.31 (18.34)
Raven (proportion correct) Pre-training 0.65 (0.02) 0.61(0.04) 0.56 (0.04) 0.65 (0.02)
Post-training 0.76 (0.02) 0.68 (0.03) 0.68 (0.04) 0.70 (0.03)

substantial evidence for a short-lived positive transfer The interaction was due to a smaller interference score for
effect of the interference control training on interference the F3 compared to pre-training test session for the trained
control as measured with the Stroop task. children, F(1, 19) = 12.56, p = 0.002 ηp2 = 0.40, but not the
control children, F < 1. However, the difference between
Flanker task training conditions on the F3 follow-up test was not sig-
nificant both with and without controlling for pre-training
ANOVA of the Flanker task data revealed a significant task performance, ANCOVAs, ps > 0.12. These results sug-
main effect of group and session, in addition to group × ses- gest marginal evidence for a short-lived transfer of training
sion, training condition × session, and group × training benefits to interference control capacity as measured by the
condition × session interaction effects. The latter interac- flanker task for the children but not adults.
tion was examined further with a training condition × ses-
sion ANOVA for each age group separately. For the adults, Go/no‑go task
this analysis did not reveal any significant effects, Fs < 1.
However, for the child sample, a significant training con- ANOVA did not reveal any significant effects. However,
dition × session interaction was found, F(1, 39) = 6.82, the three-way interaction was at trend level, motivating a
p = 0.013, ηp2 = 0.15, in addition to a main effect of ses- training condition × session ANOVA separately for each age
sion, F(1, 39) = 27.59, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.41. The interac- group. For the adults, this analysis only revealed a significant
tion reflected a significantly lower interference score on the session effect, F(1, 45) = 5.97, p = 0.02, ηp2 = 0.12, reflecting
post- compared to pre-training test for the trained children, a lower difference score for the post- than pre-training test,
F(1, 20) = 37.44, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.65, but not the control other Fs < 1. For the children, the analysis revealed an inter-
children, p = 0.10. Moreover, the difference in post-training action effect at trend level, F(1, 39) = 4.01, p = 0.052, ηp2 =
task performance between the trained and non-trained chil- 0.09, other Fs < 1. This interaction reflected a difference at
dren was significant after controlling for pre-training task trend level between post- vs. pre-training performance for
performance, ANCOVA, F(1, 38) = 4.57, p = 0.039, ηp2 = the trained children, F(1, 20) = 3.91, p = 0.062, ηp2 = 0.16,
0.11, However, the evidence for the model incorporating but not the controls, F < 1. Moreover, after controlling for
the training condition factor was only marginal, ­BF10 = 1.75. the difference in pre-training performance, the difference
ANOVA using the pre-training and F3 data from the children in post-training performance between the trained and non-
revealed a significant training condition × session interac- trained children was significant, ANCOVA, F(1, 38) = 4.26,
tion, F(1, 37) = 4.44, p = 0.04, ηp2 = 0.11, in addition to a p = 0.046, ηp2 = 0.10, ­BF10 = 1.50. However, analysis con-
main session effect, F(1, 37) = 8.07, p = 0.007, ηp2 = 0.18. trasting pre-training and 3-month follow-up test performance

13

1526 Psychological Research (2019) 83:1519–1530

Table 2  Results of statistical Task (dependent measure) Factor dfs F p η2


analyses (ANOVAs) of training
and transfer test data Training (RT) Group 1, 42 18.84 < 0.001 0.31
Session 19, 798 56.69 < 0.001 0.57
Group × session 19, 798 6.37 < 0.001 0.13
Stroop (interference score in ms) Group 1, 84 1.63 0.21 0.02
Training condition (TC) 1, 84 0.00 0.98 0.00
Session 1, 84 7.52 0.007 0.08
Group × TC 1, 84 0.02 0.88 0.00
Group × session 1, 84 0.38 0.54 0.01
TC × session 1, 84 10.81 0.001 0.11
Group × TC × session 1, 84 0.24 0.63 0.00
Flanker (interference score in ms) Group 1, 84 14.22 < 0.001 0.15
Training condition (TC) 1, 84 0.18 0.67 0.00
Session 1, 84 26.04 < 0.001 0.24
Group × TC 1, 84 0.37 0.54 0.00
Group × session 1, 84 17.92 < 0.001 0.18
TC × session 1, 84 4.24 0.04 0.05
Group × TC × session 1, 84 6.68 0.01 0.07
Go/no-go (hits-FA) Group 1, 84 2.68 0.11 0.03
Training condition (TC) 1, 84 0.40 0.53 0.01
Session 1, 84 0.14 0.71 0.00
Group × TC 1, 84 0.34 0.56 0.00
Group × session 1, 84 3.92 0.051 0.05
TC × session 1, 84 3.83 0.054 0.04
Group × TC × session 1, 84 2.97 0.088 0.03
WM updating-easy (proportion correct) Group 1, 84 53.01 < 0.001 1.0
Training condition (TC) 1, 84 4.97 0.03 0.06
Session 1, 84 1.33 0.25 0.02
Group × TC 1, 84 5.17 0.03 0.06
Group × session 1, 84 1.87 0.18 0.02
TC × session 1, 84 0.45 0.51 0.01
Group × TC × session 1, 84 0.26 0.61 0.00
WM updating-difficult (proportion correct) Group 1, 84 23.11 < 0.001 0.22
Training condition (TC) 1, 84 0.87 0.35 0.01
Session 1, 84 0.57 0.45 0.01
Group × TC 1, 84 2.86 0.10 0.03
Group × session 1, 84 0.01 0.94 0.00
TC × session 1, 84 4.50 0.04 0.05
Group × TC × session 1, 84 0.94 0.34 0.01
Switching (switch cost in ms) Group 1, 83 4.73 0.03 0.05
Training condition (TC) 1, 83 0.22 0.64 0.00
Session 1, 83 6.97 0.01 0.08
Group × TC 1, 83 5.10 0.03 0.06
Group × session 1, 83 0.36 0.55 0.00
TC × session 1, 83 3.86 0.053 0.04
Group × TC × session 1, 83 0.59 0.45 0.01

13
Psychological Research (2019) 83:1519–1530 1527

Table 2  (continued) Task (dependent measure) Factor dfs F p η2

Raven (proportion correct) Group 1, 84 0.90 0.35 0.01


Training condition (TC) 1, 84 0.00 0.97 0.00
Session 1, 84 44.93 < 0.001 0.35
Group × TC 1, 84 4.29 0.04 0.05
Group × session 1, 84 0.19 0.67 0.00
TC × session 1, 84 4.06 0.047 0.05
Group × TC × session 1, 84 0.46 0.50 0.01

p values in bold are significant using an α = 0.05 level

failed to reveal any significant effects, Fs < 1. These results post-compared to pre-training session for the trained par-
suggest marginal evidence for a short-lived positive transfer ticipants, F(1, 42) = 18.53, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.31, but not
of the training to go/no-go task performance for the children the control participants, F < 1. However, the difference
but not adults. between training groups was significant for neither the pre-
nor post-training session, ps > 0.16, and ANCOVA also
WM updating tasks failed to reveal a significant difference in post-training task
performance between the trained and control participants
The data from one adult participant for the difficult WM when controlling for pre-training performance differences,
task were lost. ANOVA using the data from the easy task p = 0.072. These analyses suggest no reliable training ben-
did not reveal any significant results of interest. However, efits for switching task performance.
ANOVA on the data from the difficult test revealed a sig-
nificant training condition × session interaction effect, in Raven
addition to a main group effect. The interaction reflected a
significant performance increase from pre- to post-training ANOVA using the Raven scores revealed a significant train-
for the trained participants, F(1, 43) = 8.89, p = 0.005, ηp2 = ing condition × session interaction, next to a significant main
0.17, but not control participants, F < 1. The training group effect of session and a group × training condition interaction.
difference in post-training task performance was significant However, simple main effect analyses investigating the criti-
after controlling for pre-training task performance differ- cal training context × session interaction revealed a signifi-
ences, ANCOVA, F(1, 85) = 3.98, p = 0.049, ηp2 = 0.05, cant increase from pre- to post-training task performance
although the evidence supporting the model assuming a for both trained and non-trained participants, ps ≤ 0.001,
group difference was only marginal, B ­ F10 = 1.21. The adult and the difference between conditions was significant for
participants also performed the difficult task at the 3-month neither assessment session, Fs < 1. Moreover, the difference
follow-up test. A training condition × session ANOVA using between training conditions on the post-training test was also
the pre-training and F3 data from the adult participants did not significant after controlling for pre-training performance
not reveal any significant effects, maximum p = 0.06, for the differences, ANCOVA, p = 0.07. These results suggest no
non-critical main effect of training condition: if anything, reliable beneficial training effect on general IQ as measured
overall the control participants performed better than the by the Raven test.
trained participants. These results suggest marginal evidence
for a short-term benefit of the interference control training
on WM performance as measured by a running memory Discussion
task.
The present study examined the effect of 20 sessions of
Switching task adaptive interference training, using a Stroop-like task in
both children and adults. We measured performance on the
Analysis of the data from the switching task only revealed trained task and on tasks measuring transfer effects to per-
significant main effects for the group and session factors formance on a variety of cognitive tasks. In the course of
and the group × session interaction, which were not relevant training, both the children and adults improved performance
for the question of differential transfer benefits. The rel- on the trained task, as indicated by decreasing RTs. Moreo-
evant training condition × session interaction just failed to ver, the trained participants from both age groups displayed
be significant and follow-up analyses targeting this interac- substantial evidence of beneficial transfer of the training to
tion only revealed significantly better performance on the a task measuring interference control using a different type

13

1528 Psychological Research (2019) 83:1519–1530

of stimuli (words rather than pictures), relative to the par- response interference. This suggests that what had been
ticipants from an active control group. Marginal evidence learned during training, and used during the Stroop transfer
for beneficial transfer to a WM updating task was also found task, concerned both learning to ignore irrelevant distractors
for both age groups. Finally, marginal evidence for positive consisting of the concrete identity of the visual or verbal
transfer was obtained for the children but not adults to a stimuli, and to suppress the prepotent response activated by
task measuring interference control using peripheral distrac- those stimuli.
tors, and a go/no-go task. However, all evidence of positive Concerning the other task measuring interference con-
transfer for the adults and/or children was restricted to a trol, the Flanker task, we only found marginal evidence of
post-training assessment that was performed immediately transfer. Moreover, this evidence only applied to the child
after training. A follow-up assessment 3 months after the sample. This more limited transfer suggests the involvement
post-training assessment did not reveal a difference between of at least partly different processes in Stroop and Flanker
the training conditions for any of the two age groups for any tasks. One option concerns the involvement of different
of the transfer tasks. types of response-related interference. Specifically, in this
The stimuli used for training were chosen to, at least context, it may be noted that the Stroop task requires the
initially, automatically trigger a response due to prepotent, suppression of a prepotent response, whereas the Flanker
‘over trained’ stimulus–response associations. The decreas- task implicates the simultaneous activation of two equipo-
ing RTs across training sessions suggest that the participants tent responses. This difference may also be related to the dif-
gradually learned to ignore the interfering information pro- ferential involvement of response inhibition in the two types
vided by the identity of the visual stimulus and to inhibit of task, with a stronger involvement in Stroop than Flanker
the incorrect prepotent response that was associated with tasks (Stahl et al., 2014). In any case, the somewhat stronger
it, to generate the required, task-appropriate response (see evidence for transfer in the children compared to adults is in
also Stahl et al. 2014, for evidence of the involvement of accordance with other evidence, suggesting generally larger
both interference control and response inhibition in Stroop transfer effects for the former than latter age group (Karbach
tasks). Arguably, this description fits more to a conceptu- & Unger, 2014). Moreover, it may be consistent with the
alization of the task implicating interference control rather evidence of a developmental trajectory of EFs in general,
than (resisting) proactive interference. This, in turn, would and inhibitory processes in particular, that extents well into
implicate that the present training was not directly relevant adolescence (e.g., Best & Miller, 2010). A process that is
to the issue of the role of proactive interference in the (lack still developing may show larger training-induced plasticity
of) success of previous WM-training studies that was dis- than a matured one.
cussed in the introduction. However, it is important to note The involvement of response inhibition in Stroop-like
that although interference control and proactive inhibition tasks may also be the source of the marginal evidence for
have been shown to be separable constructs, they are still transfer of training benefits to go/no-go task performance
strongly correlated (Stahl et al., 2014). It is also noteworthy that we, again, found for the children but not the adults. In
that the pattern of decreasing RTs speaks to the task pro- other words, by training the Stroop-like task, the children
moting the learning of yet other “automatic” (“reversed”) might also have improved their response inhibition abilities,
stimulus-key press response associations. If this were the which marginally benefitted their performance on the go/
case, one would have expected the change in mapping of no-go task, a standard response inhibition task.
stimuli to response keys that was introduced halfway the A notable finding was the marginal evidence that we
training program to result in a recovery of the initial long found in both age groups for training-mediated beneficial
RTs. However, apart from a relatively small and temporary effects on the WM task, suggesting common processes
increase in RT observed in the children from session 10 to involved in the training and WM tasks. One likely common
session 11, the continued short RTs suggest a maintained processes entails interference control (Szmalec, Verbruggen,
involvement of controlled rather than automatic processes. Kemps, & Vandierendonck, 2011). In both types of task,
The standard Stroop task used as near-transfer task also target information must be protected against interference
requires the participant to inhibit the prepotent response that (either proactive or otherwise) coming from simultaneously
is triggered by the color word. Therefore, the differential present (in the Stroop task) or preceding (in the running
pre- to post-training performance improvement effect seen memory task) stimuli. Another common potential element
for the Stroop task as a function of training condition sug- concerns goal maintenance (e.g., Kane & Engle, 2003): in
gests identical processes involved in the trained and Stroop both types of task, participants must keep remembering the
tasks. As already partly indicated above, Stahl et al. (2014) “rules” of the task.
found Stroop task interference, expressed as RT difference We did not find any evidence for training-induced trans-
score, to be associated with other measures of interference fer effects for the switching task. Although this task may
control and response inhibition, but not with measures of share certain processes with the trained task, such as goal

13
Psychological Research (2019) 83:1519–1530 1529

maintenance and inhibitory processes, the lack of transfer of interference. In this sense, our control group might be
is consistent with previous studies suggesting that the task argued to be more conservative compared to more traditional
mainly involves a separate component of executive func- control groups. However, future research should test these
tioning compared to different types of WM and (response) speculations using a non-adaptive variant of the interference
inhibition tasks (Miyake et al., 2000; Miyake & Friedman, control training in an additional control group. A final asset
2012). We also failed to find transfer to performance on the of the present study is the use of Bayesian statistics in addi-
Raven tests. This failure does not seem to be due to ceiling tion to the traditional null hypothesis significance testing
effects and is in accordance with the abundance of previous (NHST). These statistics enabled us to estimate the strength
cognitive training studies yielding no significant transfer to of the evidence for a model assuming greater pre- to post-
general intelligence, at least not in non-clinical populations training improvements for the trained compared to control
(e.g., Melby-Lervåg et al., 2016). participants (alternative model). These additional analyses
revealed that, although NHST revealed significant effects,
Strengths and limitations the evidence for the alternative model was in fact very weak
for all but one of the transfer tests.
We used an adaptive training protocol that consisted of only
one type of task. Such approach ensures that training spe-
cifically targets one cognitive process, or a limited num-
ber of cognitive processes, and that evidence of transfer Conclusions
can be ascribed to more specific cognitive processes than
would have been the case when using a battery of different The present study was the first to assess training and transfer
types of training tasks. We also used a relatively varied set effects of a training program that exclusively targeted inter-
of assessment instruments that covered important aspects ference control, using 10–12-year-old children and young
of executive functioning. However, to measure transfer the adults in a trained and an active control group. The trained
use of more than one task to tap a specific EF component, participants from both age groups improved their perfor-
thereby reducing the influence of task-specific aspects, mance across the 20 training sessions. More importantly,
would have been preferred. A further strength was that the this training benefit transferred to performance on a near-
sample sizes were relatively large. A post hoc power analy- transfer interference control task using verbal rather than
sis using G*Power 3 (Faul, Erdfelder, Lang, & Buchner, pictorial stimuli, as evaluated in comparison with the non-
2007) in fact revealed an achieved power of 1 − β = 0.88 to trained participants. However, this beneficial transfer effect
detect a critical training condition × session interaction with was no longer present after 3 months. Some evidence was
an intermediate effect size (Cohen’s d = 0.50) in a repeated obtained for training-induced benefits for participants from
measures analysis, given a total sample size of 41 (the N both age groups regarding performance on a task measur-
for the child sample), an α value of 0.05, two groups, and ing WM updating ability. However, this evidence was only
two measurements (based on a total sample size of N = 88— very weak and no beneficial effects whatsoever were present
adults and children combined, the corresponding power was at the 3-month follow-up measurement. Specifically for the
even 1 − β = 1.0). However, the assumption of an intermedi- children there was also evidence, albeit again very weak, of a
ate effect size could be argued to be somewhat unrealistic, beneficial transfer effect to another interference control task
especially for the far-transfer and follow-up measurements. implicating the inhibition of attention to peripheral rather
Reducing the to-be expected effect size by half (Cohen’s than central stimuli and to a standard response inhibition
d = 0.25) would in fact implicate an achieved power of only task. However, here too, there was no evidence whatsoever
0.64 (with the full sample of 88 participants). Hence, the for any of these training benefits to be maintained 3 months
present study could have been underpowered, especially in after the training. The collective results at best suggest some
detecting far-transfer effects. very limited potential for interference control training to
Our active control group was also used in a previous temporarily improve performance on tasks putatively involv-
study but might have been a bit unusual. The control task ing interference control, with this potential being somewhat
was carefully chosen so as to equate the training and con- stronger for children than adults. The limited transfer effects,
trol groups on a number of important aspects: experience and the short-lived character of those effects that were found,
with environmental factors associated with the study, being are in line with the general cognitive training literature that
involved in an engaging and motivating task, and believing so far failed to reveal strong evidence for the potential and
that one was involved in some treatment program, thereby usefulness of process-based cognitive training programs to
reducing potential differences in expectancy effects. Making improve non-trained and/or more general and daily-life cog-
sand paintings might further be argued to involve focused nitive functions (Melby-Lervåg & Hulme, 2016; Shipstead,
attention and the inhibition of internal and external sources Hicks, & Engle, 2012).

13

1530 Psychological Research (2019) 83:1519–1530

Acknowledgements  We thank Joseph H.R. Maes for the help with Loosli, S. V., Falquez, R., Unterrainer, J. M., Weiller, C., Rahm, B., &
analysis and for feedback on the manuscript. This work was supported Kaller, C. P. (2016). Training of resistance to proactive interference
by Grants to XZ by the National Natural Science Foundation of China and working memory in older adults: A randomized double-blind
(31560283). study. International Psychogeriatrics, 28, 453–467. https​://doi.
org/10.1017/S1041​61021​50015​19.
MacLeod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop effect:
Compliance with ethical standards  An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 163‒203.
Melby-Lervåg, M., Redick, T. S., & Hulme, C. (2016). Working memory
Conflict of interest  Author Xin Zhao declares that he has no conflict training does not improve performance on measures of intelligence
of interest. Author Lina Jia declares that she has no conflict of interest. or other measures of “far transfer”: Evidence from a meta-analytic
review. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11, 512–534. https​
Ethical statement  This study was funded by 31560283. The manuscript ://doi.org/10.1177/17456​91616​63561​2.
does not contain clinical studies or patient data. Studies were approved Minear, M., & Shah, P. (2008). Training and transfer effects in task
by the local ethics committee and all experimental manipulations were switching. Memory and Cognition, 36, 1470–1483. https​://doi.
performed in accordance with the approved guidelines. org/10.3758/MC.336.8.1470.
Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of
Informed consent  All human participants had signed an informed individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclu-
consent form. The school was contacted via the Education Bureau sions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 8‒14. https​
of Lanzhou city and the children had their primary caretakers sign ://doi.org/10.1177/09637​21411​42945​8.
informed consent forms. Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter,
A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive
functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A
References latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100. https​://
doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0734.
Best, J. R., & Miller, P. H. (2010). A developmental perspective on execu- Nigg, J. T. (2000). On inhibition/disinhibition in developmental psycho-
tive function. Child Development, 81, 1641‒1660. https​://doi.org/1 pathology: Views from cognitive and personality psychology and a
0.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01499​.x. working inhibition taxonomy. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 220‒246.
Brocki, K. C., & Bohlin, G. (2004). Executive functions in children aged https​://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.126.2.220.
6 to 13: A dimensional and developmental study. Developmental Oelhafen, S., Nikolaidis, A., Padovani, T., Blaser, D., Koenig, T., & Per-
Neuropsychology, 26, 571–593. https​://doi.org/10.1207/s1532​6942d​ rig, W. J. (2013). Increased parietal activity after training of inter-
n2602​_3. ference control. Neuropsychologia, 51, 2781–2790. https​://doi.
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychol- org/10.1016/j.neuro​psych​ologi​a.2013.08.012.
ogy, 64, 135–168. https​://doi.org/10.1146/annur​ev-psych​-11301​ Pereg, M., Shahar, N., & Meiran, N. (2013). Task switching effects are
1-14375​0. mediated by working-memory management. Intelligence, 41, 467–
Dougherty, M. R., Hamovitz, T., & Tidwell, J. W. (2016). Reevaluating 478. https​://doi.org/10.1016/j.intel​l.2013.06.009.
the effectiveness of n-back training on transfer through the Bayesian Raven, J., Raven, J. C., & Court, J. H. (2000). Standard progressive matri-
lens: Support for the null. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 23, ces. Oxford: Psychology Press.
306–316. https​://doi.org/10.3758/s1342​3-015-0865-9. Raven, J. C., Court, J. H., & Raven, J. (1977). Manual for Raven’s pro-
Enge, S., Behnke, A., Fleischhauer, M., Küttler, L., Kliegel, M., & Stro- gressive matrices and vocabulary scales. London: H. K. Lewis.
bel, A. (2014). No evidence for true training and transfer effects Shipstead, Z., Redick, T. S., & Engle, R. W. (2012). Is working memory
after inhibitory control training in young healthy adults. Journal training effective? Psychological Bulletin, 138, 628‒654. https​://doi.
of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition, 40, org/10.1037/a0027​473.
987–1001. https​://doi.org/10.1037/a0036​165. Soveri, A., Antfolk, J., Karlsson, L., Salo, B., & Laine, M. (2017). Work-
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: ing memory training revisited: A multi-level meta-analysis of n-back
A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behav- training studies. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 24, 1077–1096.
ioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, https​://doi.org/10.3758/s1342​3-016-1217-0.
175–1191. https​://doi.org/10.3758/BF031​93146​. Stahl, C., Voss, A., Schmitz, F., Nuszbaum, M., Tüscher, O., Lieb, K., &
Grönholm-Nyman, P., Soveri, A., Rinne, J. O., Ek, E., Nyholm, A., Neely, Klauer, K. C. (2014). Behavioral components of impulsivity. Jour-
S., A., & Laine, M. (2017). Limited effects of set shifting training in nal of Experimental Psychology General, 143, 850–886. https​://doi.
healthy older adults. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 9, 69. https​:// org/10.1037/a0033​981.
doi.org/10.3389/fnagi​.2017.00069​. Szmalec, A., Verbruggen, F., Kemps, E., & Vandierendonck, A. (2011).
Jonides, J., & Nee, D. E. (2006). Brain mechanisms of proactive interfer- Control of interference during working memory updating. Journal
ence in working memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 139, 181–193. of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance,
https​://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuro​scien​ce.2005.06.042. 37, 137–151. https​://doi.org/10.1037/a0020​365.
Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2003). Working-memory capacity Thorell, L. B., Lindqvist, S., Nutley, B., Bohlin, S., G., & Klingberg,
and the control of attention: The contributions of goal neglect, T. (2009). Training and transfer effects of executive functions in
response competition, and task set to Stroop interference. Jour- preschool children. Developmental Science, 12, 106–113. https​://
nal of Experimental Psychology General, 132, 47–70. https​://doi. doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00745​.x.
org/10.1037/0096-3445.132.1.47. Zhao, X., Chen, L., & Maes, J. H. R. (2016). Training and transfer effects
Karbach, J., & Kray, J. (2009). How useful is executive control train- of response inhibition training in children and adults. Developmental
ing? Age differences in near and far transfer of task-switching train- Science. https​://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12511​.
ing. Developmental Science, 12, 978‒990. https​://doi.org/10.111 Zinke, K., Einert, M., Pfennig, L., & Kliegel, M. (2012). Plasticity of
1/j.1467-7687.2009.00846​.x. executive control through task switching training in adolescents.
Karbach, J., & Unger, K. (2014). Executive control training from middle Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 41. https​://doi.org/10.3389/
childhood to adolescence. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, Article 390. fnhum​.2012.00041​.
https​://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg​.2014.00390​.

13
Psychological Research is a copyright of Springer, 2019. All Rights Reserved.

You might also like