You are on page 1of 11

In Praise of Difficult People: A Portrait of the Committed Whistleblower

Author(s): Philip H. Jos, Mark E. Tompkins and Steven W. Hays


Source: Public Administration Review , Nov. - Dec., 1989, Vol. 49, No. 6 (Nov. - Dec.,
1989), pp. 552-561
Published by: Wiley on behalf of the American Society for Public Administration

Stable URL: https://www.jstor.org/stable/976577

JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide
range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and
facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at
https://about.jstor.org/terms

Wiley and American Society for Public Administration are collaborating with JSTOR to
digitize, preserve and extend access to Public Administration Review

This content downloaded from


203.124.40.244 on Mon, 18 Oct 2021 04:42:22 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
552

In Praise of Difficult People:


A Portrait of the Committed
_Whistleblower
Philip H. Jos, College of Charleston
Mark E. Tompkins, University of
South Carolina
Steven W. Hays, University of
South Carolina

Critics of large organizations have long regarded prin- Ideally, studies of whistleblowing would incorporate
cipled individual dissent, including "whistleblowing," as measures of employee attitudes and conduct taken before
important for insuring accountability in otherwise unre- wrongdoing is observed, externally validated observations
sponsive bureaucracies. It is widely reported that whistle- of malfeasance, externally validated observations of
blowing is a costly act for the whistleblower, which many employee responses and evolving attitudes, and an
regard as further evidence of the need for improved legal account of the organization's response. Such a study
would be very costly and may not be feasible. Absent
protections and greater external control over the organiza-
such comprehensive studies, efforts to understand whistle-
tions involved. The conflict engendered by internal dis-
blowing must involve a multimethod strategy (as advocat-
sent involves high stakes for the organization as well. In
ed by Near and Miceli, 1985), which employs less robust
addition to challenging several societal taboos (don't be a
methods whose weaknesses can be addressed through the
"stool pigeon," never air "dirty laundry" in public), blow-
use of several complementary studies. In this case, two
ing the whistle often pits loyalty to one's clients or col- methodological approaches are valuable: broadly based
leagues against loyalty to the public and includes an accu- employee surveys and "capture samples" of populations of
sation that one's superiors and/or fellow employees have known whistleblowers.
neglected or abused the public trust (Bok, 1980). As a
Broadly distributed surveys of all employees in an
result, whistleblowing represents one of the most threaten-
organization can be useful in developing understanding of
ing forms of organizational dissent, likely to prompt con-
how often wrongdoing is acknowledged and reported (but
siderable hostility and various forms of organizational
not necessarily all that is observed) and the frequency of
retaliation.
organizational retaliation, where it is acknowledged
Knowledge of whistleblowing is generally anecdotal, (MSPB, 1981, 1983; Miceli and Near, 1985; Near and
however, limiting understanding of those who sound these Miceli, 1986). Such surveys, however, neglect those who
alarms, their motivations, and their subsequent fate. This have been dismissed or forced out of the organization as a
study addresses several of the more fundamental concerns result of their whistleblowing effort, i.e. committed
raised by the legislative debate and academic research whistleblowers who have persisted in the face of substan-
regarding whistleblowing. First, what becomes of those tial opposition and despite strong retaliation. Moreover,
existing employee surveys have not been designed to
who choose to blow the whistle? Is retaliation as severe as
assess individual personality characteristics, decision mak-
many have suggested? Second, what motivates whistle-
ing styles, or the moral beliefs of the respondents. Under-
blowers? What is it that prompts these few individuals to
standing the motivations and perceptions of actual whistle-
risk ostracism, career sanctions, and other forms of retri-
blowers is crucial for developing a more comprehensive
bution while most of their colleagues remain complacently
model of whistleblowing behavior (Dozier and Miceli,
silent?l
1985; Miceli and Near, 1985).

The authors analyze the results of a survey questionnaire completed by 161 whistleblowers, 80 percent of whom
were or are government employees. They examine the consequences of whistleblowing and explore the organiza-
tional position and personal attributes of whistleblowers. The authors report severe retaliation against this group
of whistleblowers and find that they are in many ways exceptional and tend to exhibit a distinctive approach to
moral issues and decision making.

NOVEMBER/DECEMBER 1989

This content downloaded from


203.124.40.244 on Mon, 18 Oct 2021 04:42:22 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
IN PRAISE OF DIFFICULT PEOPLE 553

With few exceptions (e.g., Soeken and Soeken, 1987), survey of this kind. In all, 161 surveys were returned
however, in-depth research on committed whistleblowers (producing a response rate of 56 percent).3 This response
has been limited to case studies (Peters and Branch, 1972; rate is also higher than the rates reported in all of the simi-
Nader et al., 1972; Weisband and Franck, 1975; Perrucci lar studies reviewed.4
et al., 1980; Glazer, 1983; Glazer and Glazer, 1986, 1989; Fifty-five percent of the respondents worked for the
Chalk, 1988). Case materials are limited by the "unique- federal government, 9 percent for state government, 10
ness paradox" (Martin et al., 1983), the bias toward view- percent for counties and cities, 5 percent for quasi-public
ing personal experience in an organization as unique, even agencies, and 20 percent for private firms. Many of these
if these experiences are repeated in many other settings people held positions with significant responsibilities in
accessible only to an outside analyst. The analysis of their organizations, but their responsibilities and experi-
these case materials is also confounded by their anecdotal ences varied widely. As a result, this group of respondents
nature, which can create a "good stories" bias. Cases may provides no evidence that committed whistleblowers are
be chosen as especially important exemplars or because drawn disproportionately from any one role or rank in
they involve especially serious wrongdoing. Do these organizations.
cases typify the experience of committed whistleblowers?2
While this group is surely not representative of all
This study is based on a survey, conducted under condi- those who observe and report organizational wrongdoing,
tions of anonymity by a university-based group, of the it does provide a more detailed picture of the experiences
largest group of committed whistleblowers yet contacted of a large group of strongly committed whistleblowers.
regarding their experiences. The analysis explores and The study involves only those who chose to contact an
extends the insights developed by Glazer and Glazer identified whistleblower support group or another whistle-
(1989) and by Soeken and Soeken (1987), in studies based blower; thus it treats a group of people who have for
on smaller populations. These earlier studies raise many some reason been dissatisfied with the formal mechanisms
questions: What sorts of people engage in whistleblow- of protest or appeal and, perhaps as a result, have general-
ing? Does whistleblowing have any impact on the organi- ly paid a particularly heavy price for their dissent. The
zations involved-or is it a futile gesture? Does whistle- fact that this study focuses only on those who have blown
blowing carry the high cost that many argue it involves? the whistle and become dissatisfied with the response to
Do whistleblowers regret their actions when retaliation is their allegations, and who chose to respond to the survey,
severe? limits its value for drawing conclusions about the experi-
ences of all whistleblowers. The focus of this study on
committed whistleblowers also precludes direct investiga-
Survey Procedures and the Nature of the Group tion of the factors influencing those who consider whistle-
blowing, but reject it (since it excludes them from the pool
Unless problems inherent in the independent identifica- of respondents). These respondents can shed light, how-
tion of whistleblowing can be overcome, research will ever, on their own motivations, understandings, and
continue to rely on the reports of those who seek legal, experiences, and, since they represent those who persisted
political, and emotional support. In an effort to reduce thein the face of serious opposition, it is especially important
problems associated with self selection, this analysis was for students of whistleblowing to understand the perspec-
not confined to particularly well-publicized cases of tive of these committed whistleblowers. Many of these
whistleblowing or to a single list kept by a particular indi-
respondents, moreover, are excluded from studies of
vidual. Instead, six groups of people were contacted whowhistleblowing by current employees, since a majority of
were identified as whistleblowers. The first three groups the respondents have lost or changed their jobs as the
consisted of 213 individuals who had in one way or anoth- result of their experiences.
er identified themselves as whistleblowers to one of two
whistleblower support groups (the Government Account-
ability Project and the Coalition Against Government Consequences of Blowing the Whistle
Waste) or with a prominent whistleblower correspondent.
Various case studies, first-hand reports, and survey data
An additional 47 people were identified through another
have indicated that many forms of retaliation are used
whistleblower's list of contacts and were included in the
against whistleblowers, including bureaucratic isolation,
study. Another group of 52 people whose cases were still
character assassination, and dismissal.S The responses of
sensitive to disclosure were identified by the Government
these committed whistleblowers will not resolve the ongo-
Accountability Project and incorporated into the study ing debate over the extent of retaliation against whistle-
under conditions which assured their anonymity. Finally, blowers, nor can they be used to evaluate the effectiveness
an additional 17 people were identified by other respon- of agencies with formal responsibilities for protecting
dents and contacted directly. In all but a few unusual whistleblowers. However, a large group of respondents
cases, an initial mailing was followed by three follow-up who have experienced particularly severe retaliation was
mailings (lasting over about a four-month period in each contacted in this research. Their responses provide addi-
case) as a part of the survey process. The survey was con- tional evidence of the overwhelming personal and profes-
ducted between November of 1987 and September of sional hardship that some whistleblowers endure and of
1988, and it reached 329 people, more than any previous the need for legal protections for this group.

NOVEMBER/DECEMBER 1989

This content downloaded from


203.124.40.244 on Mon, 18 Oct 2021 04:42:22 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
554 PUBLIC ADMINISTRATION REVIEW

Table 1 Table 2
Most Serious Form of Retaliation Resources Used by Whistleblowers
Experienced By Whistleblowers* and Their Helpfulness

Form of Retaliation Percentage Resource Percentage of Respondents Average Helpfulness


Using Resource Rating
All Federal Employees

Loss of Job 62 59 Family 84 5.0


Other
Job Responsibilities or
Whistleblowers 50 5.0
Salary Reduced 11 15
Government
Harassment, Transfer 18 19
Accountability
Job Responsibilities Project 47 4.6
Changed 2 4
Psychological
Work More Closely Counseling 31 4.3
Monitored 1 1
Legal Advice 79 3.9
No Retaliation 5 1
Medical
Consultation 26 3.8
* This reports only the most serious form of retali
enced by the respondents. Most whistleblowers
Relatives 32 3.4 who
severe forms of retaliation, such as the loss of their j
Coworkers 78 3.3
rienced lesser forms of retaliation as well.
Congressional
Committees 45 3.1
Findings
Professional
As Table 1 indicates, retaliation against those respond- Organizations 41 2.6
ing was especially severe. Approximately 60 percent of Home State
the respondents reported losing their jobs. They were Congressperson 55 2.5
either fired or forced to resign or retire. Retaliation Merit System
against public employees was almost as severe as in the Protection Board 31 1.9
private sector. Of the 32 private sector whistleblowers Internal Ombudsman 20 1.7
included in this study, 69 percent lost their jobs, while 59 Office of Special
percent who worked in public or quasi-public agencies Counsel 31 1.4
reported losing their jobs.
*Respondents are ask
Respondents who were employees of the United States on a scale ranging fr
government were extremely dissatisfied with the perfor- 4 means 'somewhat
mance of the Merit Systems Protection Board (MSPB) and are reported.

the Office of Special Counsel (OSC) (see Table 2). port groups can h
Indeed, the three groups rated as the least helpful by to the them. This exp
respondents are all executive branch organizations of the K. William O'Con
United States government whose official responsibilities opinion as a priva
include handling complaints of waste, fraud, and abuse. "I'd say that unless you're independently wealthy, don't
Some caution is indicated, however, in interpreting do thisit. Don't put your head up, because it will get blown
result, since this study is focused on those who viewed off"the
(Washington Post, 7/17/84).
initial response to their allegations as unsatisfactory. The
respondents' dissatisfaction may reflect poorly on the This research confirms that the committed whistleblow-
OSC's investigatory procedures, or it may indicate that er faces protracted legal battles, often waged at consider-
their complaints did not meet the difficult burden of proof able personal expense. Over half of those contacted
established by existing law. A 1985 study prompted by reported that the controversy over their actions lasted
the controversy regarding the performance of the OSC 6 more than two years. A majority of those responding
found that 99 percent of all whistleblower complaints are reported spending their own money (an average of
closed by the OSC without initiating disciplinary or cor- $28,166) in defending themselves. Of those who reported
rective action (U.S. GAO, 1985). Some have attributed receiving financial help from other groups, the average
this record to the fact that the OSC is not an independent amount spent was $42,504.
agency and may lack incentive to pursue investigations of Harassment, loss of job, and legal entanglements also
other executive branch agencies (Devine and Aplin, 1986). take a personal toll. Thirty one percent of the respondents
Others have suggested that the OSC's record of dismissing sought psychiatric counseling, and 26 percent consulted
the vast majority of complaints after an initial screening ismedical personnel. A majority of respondents reported
reasonable given the legal obstacles to proving retaliation significant disruption in their family lives as well.
(U.S. GAO, 1985, pp. 20-26).
Despite the heavy price these people paid, when they
In either case, most whistleblowers are left to fend for were asked, "If you knew what the results of your whistle-
themselves in an environment where whistleblower sup- blowing would be, before you attempted to report these

NOVEMBER/DECEMBER 1989

This content downloaded from


203.124.40.244 on Mon, 18 Oct 2021 04:42:22 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
IN PRAISE OF DIFFICULT PEOPLE 555

Table 3 For the most part, employee surveys have emphasized


issue characteristics and organizational power relation-
Reported Effects of Whistleblower Actions on the
ships. There is reason to believe that position in the hier-
Organization*
archy, tenure in the organization, and alternative employ-
Changes Within Organization Percentage ment opportunities may help to explain whistleblowing,
but a number of theoretical and methodological problems
Total Reporting Changes Within Organization 62
make the exploration of these issues difficult. A bureau-
Managerial Changes 37
People Transferred/Replaced/Not Reappointed 37 cratic account of organization, for example, might predict
Personnel Practices 24 that younger employees with few years of service would
Departmental Reorganization 17 be more likely to blow the whistle insofar as they have not
Safety Improvements Made 11
yet been rendered uncritical functionaries (e.g., Hummel,
Policies Changed 32
1982). However, this approach makes the questionable
External Investigations Percentage assumption that public organizations are "fully rational-
ized" in the Weberian sense (Jos, 1988). Moreover, as
Total Reporting External Investigation 51
Hacker (1978) observes, younger employees may be espe-
Outside Agency (e.g. FBI, EPA, NRC) 31
Congressional Hearing/Investigation Held 28 cially concerned with career advancement and therefore be
Criminal Investigation 22 less likely to blow the whistle and jeopardize their future
Indictments Resulted 11 (a possibility that illustrates the unresolved interplay
Convictions Obtained 12
between individual personality and motivation and organi-
*Respondents could report more zational socialization).
than one Miceli's
effect.and Near's (1985) analy-
The total rep
changes within the organization and the total reporting external investi- sis of MSPB survey data supports this latter view, but fur-
gations thus includes those reporting more than one example of each out- ther progress in assessing organizational influences
come
depends on a more complete specification of the power
incident(s), would you have done what you did?", an over- relationship between the individual and the organization.
whelming majority (81 percent) reported that they would Extending such a "power-dependency framework" (Miceli
do it again. An even larger proportion (87 percent) indi- and Near, 1985) requires attention to how individuals per-
cated that they would blow the whistle again if presented ceive their power within the organization, the likelihood of
with a similar situation in the future. retaliation and their own ability to secure other employ-
ment. Similarly, assessing the influence of situational vari-
Part of the explanation of this rather striking result may
ables such as the seriousness and relative clarity of the
be that the respondents feel a need to rationalize their
events which might occasion whistleblowing (e.g., "Is my
actions. It would be difficult to acknowledge a mistake in
superior really propositioning women in the office or is it
judgment given the extraordinary hardships that they (and
just good-natured teasing?" "How serious a moral or legal
their families) have endured. On the other hand, at least
problem is this?") requires an account of how individuals
according to the respondents, their actions were not futile
come to perceive wrongdoing as clearly objectionable and
gestures that brought about unnecessary personal suffer-
serious enough to warrant blowing the whistle. Thus, as
ing. Table 3 shows that 51 percent reported that their
noted earlier, assessing the motivations and personality
actions prompted some form of external investigation of
characteristics of whistleblowers is of great importance to
the organization. Sixty-two percent of the respondents
any comprehensive account of whistleblowing.
saw evidence of changes within the organization.7
Given the limited empirical research on whistleblow-
While after-the-fact rationalizations may account for
ers, it is appropriate to use caution and to explore compet-
some of the zeal with which whistleblowers defend their
ing explanations (Graham, 1985; Dozier and Miceli,
actions, their willingness to risk their careers and subject
1985). A number of theoretical perspectives on the moti-
themselves to harassment and intimidation is extraordi-
vations and personal characteristics of whistleblowers may
nary. What kind of employee is willing to take these risks
prove helpful. Kohlberg's (1981, 1984) account of an
and make these kinds of sacrifices when so many others
invariant and age-related process of moral development
are not?
would imply that whistleblowers are particularly adept at
"higher level" moral reasoning, a trait that Dozier and

Who Blows the Whistle and Why? Miceli (1985), following Rushton (1980), associate with
altruism. Braybeck (1984) offers experimental evidence
Several noteworthy attempts have been made to model that whistleblowing behavior is more common among
the whistleblowing process and to account for the factors those who have reached higher levels of moral reasoning,
which may influence the choices made by those who but this has not been confirmed among a population of
observe organizational wrongdoing (Near and Miceli, actual whistleblowers. Moreover, significant doubt
1985; Graham, 1985). Three sets of interrelated variables remains regarding the construct validity of tests of moral
might influence decisions of whether and how to blow the development (Emler et al., 1983; Kurtines and Greif,
whistle: particular characteristics of the issue at hand, the 1974; Nassi et al., 1983). Such measures may overstate
employee's power relationship to the organization, and the the importance of cognitive reasoning abilities. Acquiring
employee's personal characteristics and motivations. the sensitivity to recognize moral issues in the workplace

NOVEMBER/DECEMBER 1989

This content downloaded from


203.124.40.244 on Mon, 18 Oct 2021 04:42:22 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
556 PUBLIC ADMINISTRATION REVIEW

may require strengths of character that are not primarily Table 4


cognitive in nature (Gilligan, 1982; Benhabib, 1988; Jos, Personal Characteristics
1988).
and Organizational Responsibilities
Alternatively, Dozier and Miceli (1985) make the case of Whistleblowers
that whistleblowing is appropriately viewed as "prosocial"
Personal Characteristics Percentage
behavior, that is behavior that involves both egoistic and
altruistic motives. Perhaps the most significant implica- Male 78
tion of this view is that it would lead one to expect some White 90
balancing of advantages and disadvantages (for oneself Received at least Bachelor's Degree in college 62
Ever married 82
and for others) on the part of the whistleblower. This kind
Have children 80
of rationality is quite different than the Kohlbergian ideal Worked for organization for more than three years 67
of deliberation on rational moral principles. Miceli's and Worked in job where incident that prompted
Near's employee survey finds "some evidence of a 'sub- whistleblowing took place for more than three years 54

jectively rational' decision process, in the sense used by


Organizational Responsibility Percentage
March and Simon (1958), whereby observers of wrongdo-
ing weigh costs and benefits ... [which] ... suggests that, Managerial 48
to the extent organizations and individuals can influence Managerial Support 9
Other Professional Services 20
these costs and benefits, the level and nature of whistle-
Technical Services 18
blowing activity will change " (Miceli and Near, 1985, p. Political Liaison 4
542). In this view the level of moral reasoning ability is Support Services 5
less important than the array of incentives and disincen- (e.g., secretarial)
Other 19
tives experienced by observers of wrongdoing. Dozier
and Miceli (1985) suggest that bystander intervention
studies (especially Schwartz, 1970) may provide insight Findings
into conditions which will affect the perception of these
Consistent with the findings of an earlier survey
incentives or disincentives.
(Soeken and Soeken, 1987), the group surveyed in this
Dozier and Miceli (1985) acknowledge that it is not study is overwhelmingly male, white, and relatively well
clear that whistleblowers engage in this kind of calcula- educated (see Table 4). The underrepresentation of
tion. Studies done on small samples of whistleblowers women and minorities probably has more to do with their
(Near and Jensen, 1983) have found no relationship underrepresentation in positions of responsibility than
between comprehensiveness of retaliation and the willing- with gender differences. Almost half of the respondents
ness to blow the whistle again, and more or less stable per- reported that they held managerial positions. The majority
sonal characteristics (e.g., "locus of control" and feelings of this group exercised significant discretion on the job
of personal efficacy) may condition how the whistleblow- (50 percent reported that they were "allowed to decide
er weighs his or her decision. In addition, at least one what I would do and how I would do it") and occupied the
experimental study (Fritzsche and Becker, 1984) found kinds of managerial or professional positions which would
that whistleblowers were far less likely to employ a give them knowledge of significant policy decisions.
decision-making strategy based on calculating costs and Whistleblowers in this group do not appear to be concen-
benefits as is assumed by the prosocial account of whistle- trated in any particular position in the organizational hier-
blowing. Instead, they are far more likely to rely on moral archy nor do they share common organizational histories.
theories that emphasize rights. Still, Dozier and Miceli
To explore the motivation and decision-making styles
remain appropriately skeptical, given the paucity of empir-
of respondents, this study employed the Ethics Position
ical research, and reluctant to conclude that whistleblow-
Questionnaire, developed by Forsyth (1980) and first used
ers do not calculate the costs and benefits of their actions.
with whistleblowers by Soeken and Soeken (1987). The
Their account suggests that both egoistic calculations of
scale is designed to measure differences in the extent to
costs and benefits and altruistic motives should be consid-
which individuals accept or reject the idea that there are
ered in studying whistleblowing behavior.
universal moral rules. Of the whistleblowers responding,
Are whistleblowers typical employees who happen to 74 percent had an average score of at least "slightly dis-
confront especially egregious cases of malfeasance and agree" on the EPQ Relativism scale (meaning that they
conclude that they have enough power within the organi- rejected relativistic claims about morals). This indicates
zation to put an end to organizational wrongdoing without that the large majority expressed support for the idea that
sacrificing their careers? Do they weigh costs and bene- universal moral rules exist that ought to guide one's judg-
fits? Do those who blow the whistle exhibit a set of per- ments. [In contrast, the average response of the college
sonality characteristics distinct from other employees? students used by Forsyth to establish his norms was more
Until the methodological obstacles to a comprehensive positive than the response "slightly agree" (1980, p. 179).]
study of the sort described earlier are overcome, an In addition, 58 percent of the respondents not only
organi-
expressed support for universal moral rules but responded
zational and psychological profile of committed whistle-
that such rules ought to apply without exception.
blowers can offer some clues.

NOVEMBER/DECEMBER 1989

This content downloaded from


203.124.40.244 on Mon, 18 Oct 2021 04:42:22 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
IN PRAISE OF DIFFICULT PEOPLE 557

This finding is consistent with many case studies and the image they present to others through "self monitor-
with the experiences of those who work with whistleblow- ing." Thus, the "high self-monitoring" individual is keen-
ers. Those who provide legal and psychological help to ly attentive to the nature of particular social settings. They
whistleblowers often describe them as intensely commit- are flexible in their presentation of self, and in different
ted and uncompromising, often to the point of being rather social settings often act like very different people. They
rigid. Ironically, these are people who are often the most "tend to define their identities in terms of characteristics of
intensely committed to the organization's goals (Glazer, the situations in which they find themselves" (Snyder,
1983). Far from being politically radical or marginal, they 1979, p. 101). When confronted with an occasion for
are often described as patriotic and very traditional. Louis choice, the high self-monitoring person is cognitively and
Clark, Director of the Government Accountability Project, behaviorally guided by the question "Who does this situa-
notes that whistleblowers are usually not "children of the tion want me to be and how can I be this person" (ibid., p.
1960's, but children of the 1950's." They exhibit little of 102). By contrast, low self-monitoring individuals show
the cynicism and disillusionment that often go along with little behavioral differences across a variety of situations
political activism or dissent. They are, if anything, too since they are guided by internal beliefs and values ("who
trusting of the organization's willingness to respond to am I and how can I be me in this situation").
their concerns. To pursue these expectations further, The overwhelming majority of whistleblowers contact-
Berkowitz's and Lutterman's (1968) "social responsibili- ed are apparently uninterested in regulating their behavior
ty" scale was used to assess the degree to which respon- to conform to particular situations. Ninety-one percent of
dents embrace traditional values. A person who scores the respondents surveyed scored low on the self-monitor-
high on the "socially responsible personality" scale ing scale.8 They are unlikely to look to others or to
"believes in finishing tasks he has started, and in doingaspectsas of the situation for cues to appropriate behavior.
well as possible the jobs assigned to him. More than this, Instead, their behavior is consistent across situations
he indicates he wants to meet his obligations: he says becausehe they rely on their own attitudes and beliefs, which
sticks to the duties given him even though temptations include a strong endorsement of universal moral standards
might come along; he is strongly opposed to letting his as a guide.
friends down, and strongly in favor of working for theThis is not to say that whistleblowers necessarily feel
good of the team rather than for his own good; he insists good about themselves or that they are confident that their
people should vote, participate in community activities; actions will succeed. The respondents scored somewhat
and not cheat on their income taxes" (ibid., pp. 170-171). lower than the norm on Rosenberg's (1965) measure of
Seventy-three percent of the whistleblowers contacted self-esteem.
in This is consistent with Leventhal's finding
this survey had an average score of stronger than "agree" that those with low self-esteem were least likely to be
on the five-point ("strongly agree" to "strongly disagree") influenced by a communication that aroused fear, while
SRS scale. These are individuals who take their obliga- those who had a good opinion of themselves were more
tions seriously. likely to change their behaviors (Leventhal, 1970). Also,
the scores of the respondents on Rotter's (1966) "locus of
It is also clear that those who are willing to blow the
control" scale indicate a slight tendency to attribute the
whistle are not only committed to certain values but that
course of events to external sources such as luck and the
they are capable of acting on this sense of obligation even
power of others rather than to their own actions. (These
when there are strong organizational and situational pres-
last two findings, however, are more likely to reflect their
sures to the contrary. As Gary Carbone, Director of the
recent experiences than is true of the earlier measures.)
GAO Fraud Hotline, observes, those who use the hotline
are usually not privy to inside information that is hidden These findings are generally consistent with earlier
from their coworkers. Typically, wrongdoing is known to research (Near and Jensen, 1983; Near, Miceli, and
virtually everyone in the workplace but few will report Jensen, 1983; Fritzsche and Becker, 1984) which found
such behavior. Similarly, according to a 1981 MSPB little evidence of the kinds of cost/benefit calculations
study, less than one-third of those federal employees who implied as a step in the decision-making process by
observed organizational wrongdoing reported the incident. "prosocial" accounts of whistleblowing. While none of
When those who reported the incident only to coworkers the studies can claim generalizability, the present research
and those who reported the incident because it was a regu- casts further doubt on this theoretical perspective. To the
lar part of their job are excluded, the percentage of those extent that this study has reached a distinctive group of
reporting wrongdoing drops to 20 percent (U.S. MSPB, especially "difficult people," the findings suggest that
1981; Miceli and Near, 1985). employee surveys will fail to take account of the commit-
ted whistleblower if they assume that those who observe
This suggests that committed whistleblowers may be organizational wrongdoing will act only after considering
far less responsive to social cues which define "appropri- the seriousness of the issue, their power vis-a-vis the orga-
ate" behavior than most people. Snyder (1974, 1979) nization, alternative employment opportunities, etc. The
argues that there are systematic differences among people decisions made by those contacted in this research seem to
in their responsiveness to social cues. Respondents were have less to do with weighing costs and benefits than with
asked to complete Snyder's scale of "self-monitoring" in strong commitments to moral principle and resistance to
this study (Snyder and Gangestad, 1986). The scale was social pressure or manipulation. At least some whistle-
developed to detect differences in the extent to which peo- blowers, as Ralph Nader has claimed, seem to be "born,
ple are willing and able to exercise conscious control over not made."

NOVEMBER/DECEMBER 1989

This content downloaded from


203.124.40.244 on Mon, 18 Oct 2021 04:42:22 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
558 PUBLIC ADMINISTRATION REVIEW

Conclusion administrative wrongdoing.10 While this study suggests


that some people will undertake whistleblowing whatever
This survey found evidence of severe retaliation among
the costs, this does not permit a determination of how
the 161 whistleblowers who responded. The majority of
many other employees will be deterred by significant
those responding reported that they had lost their jobs and
sanctions. Insofar as employees themselves are best able
an even larger proportion said that they had been harassed
to scrutinize government performance at close range, pun-
or transferred and faced reductions in salary and job
ishing or intimidating those who dissent not only violates
responsibilities. For those able to procure legal represen-
the rights of the individual employee but compromises an
tation, the costs were often overwhelming and personal
important check on the abuse of the public interest. This
hardship was common.
is so both because some employees are less likely to alert
While these respondents varied in their organizational others and because the less credible the threat of public
tenure and experience, they did tend to share a belief in disclosure by one's fellow administrators, the less likely it
absolute moral standards, a strong sense of individual is that officials will resist the temptations of corruption or
responsibility, and a fierce commitment to upholding scrutinize their conduct in light of broader expectations
moral principles. These respondents generally do not look and standards.11
to aspects of particular situations or to others for cues to
The vulnerability of whistleblowers under the current
appropriate behavior.
legal framework and the importance of ensuring adminis-
These committed whistleblowers may not be represen- trative accountability to ethical and legal standards is
tative of all those who blow the whistle or all those who being more widely recognized. On April 10, 1989, Presi-
experience wrongdoing in an organizational setting. This dent Bush signed a whistleblower protection bill, similar
study involves only those who chose to contact a whistle- to one pocket vetoed by Ronald Reagan in 1988. The Act
blower support group; thus it treats a group of people who makes the OSC an independent agency, specifies that its
have for some reason been dissatisfied with the formal mandate includes protecting whistleblowers, and gives the
mechanisms of protest or appeal and, perhaps as a result, OSC the authority to issue a 45-day stay prohibiting an
have generally paid a particularly heavy price for their dis- agency from demoting or firing a worker who has filed a
sent. Whether or not these respondents are representative, complaint. The new law also modifies the existing burden
their experiences have important implications for organi- of proof by requiring only that the whistleblower show
zational leadership and for bureaucratic accountability. that his whistleblowing was a "contributing" factor in his
dismissal or harassment, rather than a "significant" or
First, the people studied here are unlikely to be dissuad-
"predominant" factor.12
ed from reporting wrongdoing by the subtleties of organi-
zational socialization and peer pressure or by more obvi- Although too late to affect the respondents in this study,
ous threats of reprisal. Furthermore, once wrongdoing has these legislative initiatives provide tacit recognition of the
been reported, even swift and severe retaliation may not difficulties that some whistleblowers have encountered.
deter this particular kind of employee. This is because Whether or not these measures stimulate more whistle-
these whistleblowers do not appear to be rational calcula- blowing activity or better protect those who blow the
tors weighing the various costs and benefits of dissent. If whistle, this study suggests that at least some committed
they were, they would not blow the whistle at all.9 From individuals will continue to call attention to wrongdoing
the narrow perspective of means-ends rationality, whistle- with little regard for the personal consequences.
blowing is irrational. It may often be principled and
admirable, but the consequences for the whistleblower can
be devastating and the personal rewards, although not Philip H. Jos is an Assistant Professor of Political Sci-
insubstantial, are uncertain. This is why so many of those ence at the College of Charleston in Charleston, South
who represent whistleblowers feel an obligation to engage Carolina. He is the author of an essay entitled "Moral
in "reality therapy" with their clients, urging them to Autonomy and the Modem Organization" (Polity, Winter
acknowledge the probable consequences. Many of those 1988).
surveyed also advised others to be realistic about what Mark E. Tompkins is an Associate Professor of Gov-
they will face. ernment and International Studies at the University of
South Carolina. His recent research has been reported in
Second, the kind of severe retaliation this group report-
ed only heightens concern over the adequacy of existing journals such as The Journal of Politics, The American
legal protections in both the private and public sectors. Journal of Epidemiology, and Coastal Management.
Retaliation against those who report wrongdoing is not Steven W. Hays is a Professor in the Department of
only a threat to the individual dissenter but to bureaucraticGovernment and International Studies at the University of
accountability. For, while alternative mechanisms of South Carolina. His recent books include Personnel
accountability are available, their effectiveness depends on Management in the Public Sector and Managing the Pub-
individuals and groups willing and able to alert them to lic Organization.

NOVEMBER/DECEMBER 1989

This content downloaded from


203.124.40.244 on Mon, 18 Oct 2021 04:42:22 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
IN PRAISE OF DIFFICULT PEOPLE 559

Notes

The authors are 4. For example, Soeken and Soeken


grateful to (1987)Don
contacted 233 whistle-
Aplin
ment Accountability Project and to Janice Rio of the Committee Against blowers and received 87 completed questionnaires, for a response
Government Waste. Their assistance and cooperation were essential to rate of 40 percent.
the completion of this research project. The authors also thank Bill Bush 5. For an account of such strategies, see Devine and Aplin (1988).
and the many respondents who shared their experiences with us. The Also, recent efforts have been made to explain organizational retali-
research was conducted with the support of the Institute for Public ation (Parmerlee et al., 1982; Near and Jensen, 1983; Near and
Affairs and Policy Studies at the College of Charleston and the Bureau of Miceli, 1986). Parmerlee et al. observed that organizations seem to
Governmental Research and Service at the University of South Carolina. retaliate against whistleblowers who are relatively powerless in
Hank Veleker served as research assistant. their relationships with their employers, but that "employers also
seem to retaliate strongly against more valued whistleblowers who
1. While some suggest that the incidence of whistleblowing is increas- are rather powerful" (1982, p. 30). Near's and Miceli's (1986) find-
ing (Ewing, 1983), it clearly remains extraordinary behavior. ings suggest that retaliation may have less to do with the whistle-
MSPB data suggests that the percentage of federal employees who blower's lack of power than with the degree of damage the organi-
observed some type of wrongdoing and chose not to report it was zation expects the complaint to cause. The retaliation experienced
approximately 70 percent in 1981 and 1983 (U. S. MPSB, 1984). by the respondents in this analysis, however, was so uniformly
2. The most recent and most impressive report of interviews with severe that it was not possible to distinguish among these compet-
whistleblowers has been conducted by Glazer and Glazer (1989). ing accounts.
Their work treats 64 whistleblowers, following many of their cases 6. As the authors of the 1985 GAO report pointed out, "In its 6-year
over the entire period of the controversy, albeit through the eyes of history, OSC has been the object of criticism from federal employ-
the whistleblowers and their records. Still, their report remains gen- ee representatives, GAO, and the Congress. OSC has been
erally anecdotal. For example, in a fascinating chapter entitled "The described as administratively inept, ineffective in prosecuting vio-
Power of Belief Systems for Ethical Registers," they report on three lations, and of little benefit to federal employee complainants such
cases involving strongly religious whistleblowers whose convic- as whistleblowers alleging management reprisals for their disclo
tions apparently sustained them though protracted and costly con- sures" (U. S. GAO, 1985, p. 6).
troversies. They offer little systematic evidence that would allow 7. When asked to give advice to other whistleblowers, many respon-
one to determine whether these cases involving people with strong dents emphasize the need to be aggressive and contact some exter-
convictions are typical. nal agent early on in the process. However, among the respondents
3. Janice Rio of the Committee Against Government Waste and in this study, the efficacy of whistleblowing does not appear to be
William Bush of Huntsville, Alabama, contributed to List #1. In the related to whether or not the whistleblower contacted an external
case of List #2, the staff at GAP did the actual mailing of the sur- agent immediately or whether they used internal channels first.
veys; letters to them were not personally addressed as they were in 8. Based on an extensive series of validating studies, Snyder and
all other cases of these people whose cases were still sensitive. List Gangestad (1985, p. 437) report a scale score which divides "high"
#3 was provided by Janice Rio at the CAGW and consisted of those self-monitors from low self-monitors-this value, a score of 11,
who had corresponded with Marie Rhaggianti, a whistleblower was used.
whose case was widely publicized, and indicated that they had sim- 9. Recall that most of these people report that they would do it again,
ilar experiences. List #4 involved people who were identified by knowing the consequences, and that they would do it in the future
other respondents as potential participants in the survey (although in a new situation. Even allowing for retrospective justifications of
they had not necessarily contacted one of our primary whistleblow- their answers, the results seem overwhelming on this count.
er support organizations). The table below provides more detailed 10. As McCubbins and Schwartz (1984) observe, Congress does not
information on the sample and response rates. ignore its oversight responsibilities but exhibits a preference for
"fire-alarm" oversight rather than "police-patrol" oversight. Instead
Responses to Survey of examining a sample of administrative decisions, looking for vio-
List Total Excluded* Declined to Number Response lations of legislative goals, Congress establishes a system of rules,
Participate** Participating Rate procedures, and informal practices that enable individual citizens
and organized interest groups to examine individual decisions
#1 213 40 9 90 58% (sometimes in retrospect), to charge executive agencies with violat-
ing congressional goals, and to seek remedies from agencies,
#2 52 0 0 28 54% courts, and Congress itself" (p. 166). The attention of the press is
similarly episodic. The whistleblowers who responded to this
#3 47 3 1 36 82% inquiry were often frustrated in their attempts to attract and main-
tain press attention to their allegations.
#4 17 0 1 7 41% 11. Experimental work by Tetlock (1985) has shown that demands for
accountability can, under certain conditions, "motivate people to
All become more vigilant, thorough, and self-critical information pro-
Lists 329 43 12 161 56% cessors" by placing subjects in a "self-critical mental set in which
they actively try to anticipate the objections or counter-arguments
*Includes bad addresses, deceased
that might bepersons, and
raised to their positions. those
As a result, who
subjects pay
ed that they were mistakenly included
close attention in
to the evidence, are this
careful to refrain sample.
from judgment T
excluded when computing the response rate. on the basis of incomplete information, and make persistent efforts
to integrate contradictory or inconsistent information into their
**Includes those who objected to responding and those who overall impression of the evidence" (Tetlock and Kim, 1987, pp.
expressed a desire to participate but decluned on advice of coun- 706, 701). Accountability may do more than motivate thought, it
sel. These were included when computing the response rate. may function as a "social brake on judgmental biases that occur in
less reflective moments" (ibid., p. 708). It is not clear what kind of

NOVEMBER/DECEMBER 1989

This content downloaded from


203.124.40.244 on Mon, 18 Oct 2021 04:42:22 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
560 PUBLIC ADMINISTRATION REVIEW

accountability demands produce this result, however. Under some consideration in 1989. It is expected that the "Uniform Health and
conditions, demands for accountability may have no effect or even Safety Whistleblower Protection Act" introduced in 1988 will be
inhibit judgment [see also Romzek and Dubnick's (1987) discus- reintroduced this session. The bill would have increased the statute
sion of various "accountability systems" and their effect on the of limitations on reporting violations, required more timely action
decision to launch the space shuttle Challenger]. on employee complaints, and strengthened the Occupational Safety
12. A measure aimed at strengthening protections for private employ- and Health Administration's (OSHA) employee protection provi-
ees reporting health and safety violations was also receiving serious sions.

References

Seyla Benhabib, "Judgment and the Moral Foundations of Politics in tonss" in L. Berkowitz ed., Advances in Experimental Social Psy-
Arendt's Thought," Political Theory, vol. 16 (February 1988), pp. chology, vol. 5 (New York: Academic Press, 1970), pp. 119-186.
29-51. James G. March and Herbert Simon, Organizations (New York: Wiley,
Leon Berkowitz, and K. Lutterman, "The Traditional Socially Responsi- 1958).
ble Personality," Public Opinion Quarterly, vol. 32 (Summer Joanne Martin, Martha S. Feldman, Mary Jo Hatch, and Sim B. Sitkin,
1968), pp. 169-185. "The Uniqueness Paradox in Organizational Stories," Adninistra-
Sissela Bok, "Whistleblowing and Professional Responsibilities," tive Science Quarterly, vol. 28 (September 1983), pp. 438-453.
Ethics Teaching in Higher Education, D. Callahan and S. Bok, eds. Mathew D. McCubbins and Thomas Schwartz, "Congressional Oversight
(New York: Plenum Press, 1980), pp. 169-194. Overlooked: Police Patrols Versus Fire Alarms," American Jour-
Mary M. Braybeck, "Ethical Characteristics of Whistle Blowers," Jour- nal of Political Science vol. 28 (February 1984), pp. 165-179.
nal of Research in Personality, vol. 18 (March 1984), pp. 41-53. Marcia P. Miceli and Janet P. Near, "Characteristics of Organizational
Rosemary Chalk, "Making the World Safe For Whistleblowers," Tech- Climate and Perceived Wrongdoing Associated With Whistleblow-
nology Review, vol. 91 (January 1988), pp. 48-57. ing Decisions," Personnel Psychology, vol. 38 (Autumn 1985), pp.
Thomas M. Devine and Donald G. Aplin, "Whistleblower Protection: 525-544.
The Gap Between Law and Reality," Howard Law Journal, vol. 31, Ralph Nader, P. Petkas, and K. Blackwell, Whistleblowing (New York:
no. 2 (1988), pp. 223-240. Grossman, 1972).
Janelle Brinker Dozier and Marcia P. Miceli, "Potential Predictors of A. J. Nassi, S. I. Abramowitz, and J. E. Youmans, "Moral Development
Whistleblowing: A Prosocial Behavior Perspective," Academy of and Politics a Decade Later: A Replication and Extension," Jour-
Management Review, vol. 10 (October 1985), pp. 823-836. nal of Personality and Social Psychology, vol. 45 (November
N. Emler, S. Renwick, and B. Malone, "The Relationship Between Moral 1983), pp. 1127-1135.
Reasoning and Political Orientation," Journal of Personality and Janet P. Near and T. C. Jensen, "The Whistleblowing Process: Retaliation
Social Psychology, vol. 45 (November 1983), pp. 1073-1080. and Perceived Effectiveness," Work and Occupations, vol. 10
David Ewing, Do It My Way or You're Fired! (New York: John Wiley (February 1983), pp. 3-28.
and Sons, 1983). Janet P. Near and Marcia P. Miceli, "Organizational Dissidence: The
Donelson R. Forsyth, "A Taxonomy of Ethical Ideologies," Journal of Case of Whistleblowing," Journal of Business Ethics, vol. 4
Personality and Social Psychology, vol. 39 (July 1980), pp. 175- (February 1985), pp. 1-16.
184. Janet P. Near and Marcia P. Miceli, "Retaliation Against Whistleblowers:
D. J. Fritzsche and H. Becker, "Linking Management Behavior to Ethical Predictors and Effects," Journal of Applied Psychology, vol. 71
Philosophy: An Empirical Investigation," Academy of Manage- (February 1986), pp. 137-145.
ment Journal, vol. 27 (March 1984), pp. 166-175. Janet P. Near, Marcia P. Miceli, and T. C. Jensen, "Variables Associated
Carol Gilligan, In a Different Voice, (Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univer- with the Whistleblowing Process" (Working paper 83-11, College
sity Press, 1982). of Administrative Science, Ohio State University, 1983).
Myron Glazer, "Ten Whistleblowers and How They Fared," The Hast- Marcia Parmerlee, Janet P. Near, and Tamilia Jensen, "Correlates of
ings Center Report, vol. 13 (December 1983), pp. 3341. Whistleblower's Perceptions of Organizational Retaliation,"
Myron Glazer and Penina Glazer, "Whistleblowing," Psychology Today, Administrative Science Quarterly, vol. 27 (March 1982), pp. 17-34.
vol. 20 (August 1986), pp. 3643. R. Perrucci, R. Anderson, D. Schendel, and L. Trachtman, Divided Loy-
Myron Glazer and Penina Glazer, The Whistleblowers: Exposing Cor- alties (West Lafayette, IN: Purdue University Press, 1980).
ruption in Government and Industry (New York: Basic Books, C. Peters and T. Branch, Whistleblowing: Dissent in the Public Interest
1989). (New York: Praeger, 1972).
Jill W. Graham, "Principled Organizational Dissent: A Theoretical Barbara Romzek and Melvin J. Dubnick, "Accountability in the Public
Essay," Research in Organizational Behavior, vol. 8 (1985), pp. 1- Sector: Lessons From the Challenger Tragedy," Public Adminis-
52. tration Review, vol. 47 (May/June 1987), pp. 227-238.
Andrew Hacker, "Loyalty and the Whistleblower," Across the Board, Morris Rosenberg, Society and the Aqolescent Self-Image (Princeton, NJ:
vol. 15 (November 1978), pp. 4-9, 67. Princeton University Press, 1965).
Ralph P. Hummel, The Bureaucratic Experience (2d ed.) (New York: St.
Julian B. Rotter, "Generalized Expectancies For Internal Versus External
Martins Press, 1982). Control of Reinforcement," Psychological Monographs, vol. 80,
Philip H. Jos, "Moral Autonomy and the Modem Organization," Polity, no. I (whole no. 609), (1966).
vol. 21 (Winter 1988), pp. 321-343. J. E. Rushton, Altruism: Socialization and Society (Englewood Cliffs,
Lawrence Kohlberg, Essays in Moral Development, vol. 1 (San Francis- NJ, Prentice-Hall, 1980).
co: Harper and Row, 1981). S. H. Schwartz, "Elicitation of Moral Obligation and Self-Sacrificing
Lawrence Kohlberg, Essays in Moral Development, vol. 2 (San Francis- Behavior: An Experimental Study of Volunteering To Be a Bone
co: Harper and Row, 1984). Marrow Donor," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
William Kurtines and Esther B. Greif, "The Development of Moral vol. 15 (August 1970), pp. 283-293.
Thought: Review and Evaluation of Kohlberg's Approach," Psy- Mark Snyder, "The Self-Monitoring of Expressive Behavior," Journal
chological Bulletin, vol. 81 (August 1974), pp. 453 - 470. of Personality and Social Psychology, vol. 30 (October 1974), pp.
H. Leventhal, "Findings and Theory in the Study of Fear Communica- 526 - 537.

NOVEMBER/DECEMBER 1989

This content downloaded from


203.124.40.244 on Mon, 18 Oct 2021 04:42:22 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
IN PRAISE OF DIFCULT PEOPLE 561

Mark Snyder, "Self-Monitoring Processes," in Leonard Berkowitz, ed., U.S. GAO: 1985).
Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, vol. 12 (New York: U. S. Merit System Protection Board, Office of Merit Systems Review
Academic Press, 1979), pp. 85-128. and Studies, "Blowing the Whistle in the Federal Government: A
Mark Snyder and Steven Gangestad, "On the Nature of Self-Monitonng: Comparative Analysis of 1980 and 1983 Survey Findings" (Wash-
Matters of Assessment, Matters of Validity," Journal of Personal- ington: U. S. MPSB, 1984).
ity and Social Psychology, vol. 51 (July 1986), pp. 125-139. U. S. Merit System Protection Board, Office of Merit Systems Review
Karen L Soeken and Donald R. Soeken, "A Survey of Whistleblowers: and Studies, "Breaking Trust: Prohibited Personnel Practices in the
Their Stressors and Coping Strategies" (unpublished manuscript, Federal Service" (Washington: U.S. MSPB, 1982).
1987). U.S. Merit System Protection Board, Office of Merit Systems Review
Philip E. Tetlock, "Accountability: The Neglected Social Context of and Studies, "Do Federal Employees Face Reprisal for Reporting
Judgment and Choice," Research in Organizational Behavior, vol. Fraud, Waste, and Mismanagement?" (Washington:U.S. MSPB,
7 (1985), pp. 297-332. 1981).
Philip E. Tetlock and Jae II Kim, "Accountability and Judgment Process- Washington Post, "Friendly Advice: Don't Put Your Head Up" (July 17,
es in a Personality Prediction Task," Journal of Personality and 1984), p. A17.
Social Psychology, vol. 52 (April 1987), pp. 700-709. Edward Weisband and T. Franck, Resignation in Protest, (New York:
U. S. General Accounting Office, "Whistleblower Complaints Rarely Grossman, 1975).
Qualify For Office of Special Counsel Protection" (Washington:

I National Institute of Justice The annual Research Program Plan will be available
starting about October 1. It tells you how to apply
for research grants in I or more of 15 scheduled

NU Research Plan programs, listed here with application deadlines and


names and telephone numbers of program managers.

for 1990 For your free copy of the plan, write or call:
National Institute of Justice/NCJRS
Box 6000
Rockville, MD 20850
Office of Crime Prevention Victims of Crime 800-851-3420 or 301-251-5500
and Criminal Justice Research Richard Titus, 202-724-7686
Cycle 1: February 2, 1990
Apprehension, Prosecution, and Cycle 2: May 25, 1990
Ethnographies of Fellowship Programs
Adjudication of Criminal Offenders Property Offenders
Bernard Auchter, 202-724-2952 White Collar and Organized Winifred Reed, 202-724-7636 Visiting Fellowships
Cycle 1: January 19, 1990 Crime
Single cycle: April 20, 1990 Richard Rau, 202-724-7631
Cycle 2: May 11, 1990 Lois Mock, 202-724-7684
Single cycle: February 16, 1990
Cycle 1: February 16, 1990
Forensic Sciences and Criminal
Drug Testing in Community Cycle 2: June 8, 1990 Justice Technology Graduate Research Fellowships
Corrections
Richard Rau, 202-724-7631 Rosemary Murphy, 202-724-7636
Doris MacKenzie, 202-724-7460
Single cycle: March 16, 1990 Richard Laymon, 202-724-7631
Single cycle: March 30, 1990 Center for Crime Single cycle: February 16, 1990
Control Research Offender Classification and
Public Safety and Security
Prediction of Criminal Behavior Summer Research Fellowships
George Shollenberger, 202-724-2956
Criminal Careers and the Richard Laymon, 202-724-7631 Winifred Reed, 202-724-7636
Cycle 1: January 26, 1990
Control of Crime Cycle 1: January 12, 1990 Single cycle: February 2, 1990
Cycle 2: May 18, 1990
Winifred Reed, 202-724-7636 Cycle 2: May 2, 1990
Single cycle: February 23, 1990
Punishment and Control
Violence Prevention and
of Offenders
Drugs, Alcohol, and Crime Control Note: Funding of these
Voncile Gowdy, 202-724-2951
Bernard Gropper, 202-724-7631 Richard Rau, 202-724-7631 Programs is contingent
Cycle 1: February 9, 1990 upon appropriations by
Cycle 1: January 10, 1990 Cycle 1: January 19, 1990
Cycle 2: June 1, 1990 the US. Congress.
Cycle 2: May 9, 1990 Cycle 2: April 27, 1990

NOVEMBER/DECEMBER 1989

This content downloaded from


203.124.40.244 on Mon, 18 Oct 2021 04:42:22 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms

You might also like