You are on page 1of 8

Available online at www.sciencedirect.

com

ScienceDirect

How different are cultural and economic ideology?


Christopher D Johnston and Trent Ollerenshaw

While a single left-right dimension is often used for elites, many In this paper, we review recent research on the extent to
scholars have found it useful to distinguish mass political which cultural and economic ideology can be distin-
ideology along two dimensions: an ‘economic’ dimension guished within the mass public. We address this question
concerning issues of redistribution, regulation, and social in two, related ways. First, we examine the empirical
insurance, and a ‘cultural’ (or ‘social’) dimension concerning association between cultural and economic ideology in
issues of national boundaries and traditional morality. While public opinion data. How strong (and in what direction) is
economic and cultural ideology do not reduce to a single left- the correlation between these dimensions in the mass
right dimension, they are often moderately — and sometimes public?1 Second, we consider the extent to which cultural
strongly — correlated. These correlations vary in magnitude and economic ideology share common psychological
and direction across individuals and countries. The association antecedents. How much overlap is there in the psycho-
of these dimensions is due, in part, to shared antecedents in logical sources of preference structure within each
psychological needs for security and certainty. However, these domain?2
needs explain more variance in cultural than economic
ideology, and their relationship with the latter varies across The recent literature suggests there is no single answer
individuals and countries. Traits related to empathy, to either of these questions that is accurate for all people
compassion, and agreeableness are an additional source of in all places. Rather, the relationship of cultural to
variation in mass ideology and are especially important to economic ideology, as well as the determinants of orien-
orientations toward inequality and thus to economic ideology. tations along these two dimensions, is conditioned by the
characteristics of citizens and the contexts in which they
Address reside.
Duke University, United States
Shared and unshared variance in economic
Corresponding author: Johnston, Christopher D (cdj19@duke.edu) and cultural ideology
We begin with a review of the association between
economic and cultural ideology in the mass public. It is
Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 2020, 34:94–101 important at the outset to emphasize that our concern is
This review comes from a themed issue on Emotion, motivation, with ideology,such as it is, and not specific policy prefer-
personality and social sciences *Political Ideologies* ences. Consistent with seminal work on public opinion,
Edited by John T Jost, Eran Halperin and Kristin Laurin recent research finds modest correlations among policy
For a complete overview see the Issue and the Editorial
preferences (r < 0.20, on average), and instability in these
preferences across time (r < 0.50 over a four year period),
Available online 13th February 2020
though both consistency and stability increase as a func-
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2020.01.008 tion of political sophistication [1].
2352-1546/ã 2020 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
We see little conflict between these findings and the
research we review below. First, political sophistication
is indeed a crucial moderator of the association between
economic and cultural ideology. Second, our concern is
not with specific policy preferences, but with the more
Introduction
Scholars often describe mass political ideology along two 1
We refer to correlations between economic and cultural ideology as
dimensions: an ‘economic’ dimension concerning issues ‘positive’ when preferences align with contemporary U.S. liberalism-
of redistribution, regulation, and social insurance, and a conservatism (i.e. anti-redistributive preferences positively correlate
with traditional values) and ‘negative’ in the opposite case (i.e. anti-
‘cultural’ (or ‘social’) dimension concerning issues of
redistributive preferences negatively correlate with traditional values).
national boundaries and traditional morality. Ideology 2
It is important to note at the outset that we do not review the
is what is latent, or shared, among the preferences within literature on estimating (and interpreting) the dimensionality of mass
each domain, and thus the source of the covariance among ideology empirically. While this is an interesting and important issue,
them. But is this as far as mass political ideology extends space limitations require a narrower focus. This article assumes that it is
useful to talk about a subset of mass preferences in terms of latent
or is it useful to posit a superordinate left-right dimension ‘economic’ and ‘cultural’ orientations. This assumption can (and should)
that binds cultural and economic preferences together? be critically evaluated, but it is not our charge to do so in this short
How different are cultural and economic ideology? article.

Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 2020, 34:94–101 www.sciencedirect.com


How different are cultural and economic ideology? Johnston and Ollerenshaw 95

abstract orientations that link them within domains. Even example, cross-domain constraint is increasing from the
if policy preferences are rather modestly correlated, there mid-20th Century to the present [11]. This is likely due,
is still some structure that needs to be explained. Our in part, to the growing clarity of elite cues as a result of
review concerns the relationship between, and the deter- party polarization during this period [12–14].
minants of, the broader orientations that give rise to the
structure that we do observe. It is also important to Recent research also finds moderators at the individual
remember that the question of ideological constraint — level. A particularly robust finding in the Western context
the extent to which specific preferences are shaped by is that the association between cultural and economic
broader ideological orientations — is distinct from the ideology increases as a function of political engagement.
question of correlation among ideological dimensions. Citizens who know and care about politics display stron-
For example, recent work in China finds that overall ger correlations than citizens who largely tune out [8,11
levels of ideological constraint are lower than in the U. ,15,16]; though some recent work suggests this engage-
S. and Europe, but the three latent dimensions of ideol- ment gap is smaller for self-placements on economic and
ogy that provide this minimal structure are correlated at cultural dimensions, for which the correlation is generally
nearly 1.0 [2]. large [7,17]. At the highest levels of engagement, ide-
ology may effectively collapse to a single dimension.
Turning to the focal question, recent work suggests that Among delegates to U.S. major party conventions, for
there is a great deal of variation in the size and the nature example, correlations between economic and cultural
of the association of economic and cultural ideology [e.g. preferences are close to 1.0 [9], and for Western European
Refs. 2,3,4,5]. If we consider the several dozen nations parties there is a strong relationship between expert
with available data in the World Values Survey, the placements on the economic and cultural (‘TAN-GAL’)
average within-nation correlation between indicators of dimensions [18].
economic and cultural ideology is close to zero [5]. Yet
the mean obscures many statistically significant, small to There are at least two mechanisms that account for the
moderate correlations within nations and these are moderating role of political engagement. First, citizens
roughly equally distributed in terms of direction.3,4 That whose preferences better align with the structure of
is, with a diverse set of countries, negative correlations elite conflict at a given point in time may feel stronger
between cultural and economic views are about as likely about their political options and thus participate at
as positive correlations. In studies of the United States — higher rates [19–23]. Second, engaged citizens may
the most closely examined context — the estimated change their issue positions to better match those of
correlation between these two dimensions in recent dec- their favored party and politicians [24,25]. It is likely
ades is positive in sign and typically moderate to large in that both are operative to at least some degree [26–28].
magnitude [6,7,8,9,10]. A recent study also finds a large, For example, Layman et al. find that policy activists who
positive relationship between these dimensions in the become committed partisans tend to bring preferences
United Kingdom [7]. less central to their political identity into alignment
with their party’s platform [29].
Several factors appear to be influential in shaping the size
and direction of the relationship between economic and To summarize, economic and cultural forms of ideology
cultural ideology across countries. In nations not under are clearly distinguishable in the mass public. At the same
communist rule during the Cold War, with high levels of time, they are correlated at substantively important levels
human development, and with low levels of traditional in many places and one recent study with a diverse set of
sexual morality, leftist economic ideology tends to be countries suggests that within-country correlations are
positively correlated with cultural liberalism. However, in about as likely to be negative as positive. This suggests
post-communist nations, and those with lower levels of common factors shaping ideology across dimensions, but
development and higher levels of traditional morality, the also that the specific factors in play — or perhaps the way
correlation tends to reverse direction [2,5]. Within- in which these factors operate — varies across citizens and
country factors may also matter. In the U.S. case, for contexts.

3
This research relies on the limited number of indicators available in
the World Values Survey and does not combine them into scales of
Psychological determinants of cultural and
economic and cultural conservatism. Unreliable measures of the dimen- economic ideology
sions may attenuate the size of the relationship within countries toward In this section we first review recent work that explains
zero (from either the positive or negative direction). More comparative the shared variance between cultural and economic ide-
research is needed on this question. ology: why are they correlated to the extent, and in the
4
When using qualitative terms to discuss magnitudes, we refer to
correlations as ‘low’, ‘small’, or ‘weak’ when they are less than 0.20;
way, that they are? We then examine research that
‘medium’ or ‘moderate’ when they are between 0.20 and 0.50; and ‘high’ explains the unshared variance: why do economic and
or ‘large’ when they are greater than or equal to 0.50. cultural ideology move, in part, independently?

www.sciencedirect.com Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 2020, 34:94–101


96 Emotion, motivation, personality and social sciences *Political Ideologies*

Explanations for shared variance between economic examinations of this interaction and more research is
and cultural ideology needed, especially in contexts outside the U.S.
Perhaps the most influential framework for understanding
the covariation of economic and cultural ideology is Jost One of the few comparative projects in the literature finds
et al.’ theory of political conservatism as motivated social that, averaging across a diverse set of countries, needs for
cognition [30,31]. They argue that rightist views on both security and certainty weakly promote left-wing eco-
dimensions are rooted in psychological needs for security nomic views. However, the relationship varies substan-
and certainty because both are system-justifying ideologies. tially at the country level and political context may
That is, both economic and cultural conservatisms ratio- explain some of this variation [68]. What the authors term
nalize status quo values, institutions, and socioeconomic country-level ‘ideological constraint’ — the extent to
hierarchies and thus provide stability in one’s environ- which economic and cultural ideology are positively
ment and closure on normative questions [7,31,32]. In related within a given country — reduces the size of
turn, these needs drive not only cultural traditionalism, the relationship between needs and leftist economic
but also an ideology that justifies market economies and views. At the highest levels of ideological constraint, this
the inequalities they produce [7] (though with impor- relationship flips sign such that needs for certainty and
tant exceptions [e.g. Eastern Europe; 33]). For example, security promote rightist economic ideology. Further,
Azevedo et al. find that economic conservatism is moder- context interacts with individual-level political engage-
ately correlated with both right-wing authoritarianism and ment such that security and certainty needs promote
general system justification tendencies in the U.S. and the rightist economic ideology primarily among engaged citi-
U.K. [7]. A very large literature has accumulated since zens in ideologically constrained countries. While an
Jost et al.’ seminal paper in 2003, and this work is sum- important initial contribution, much more comparative
marized in several recent meta-analyses [32,34,35]. Over- research is needed on this topic as this work uses only a
all, these find strong support for the claim that epistemic single, relatively unreliable measure of epistemic and
and existential needs promote rightist ideology. existential needs.

However, these meta-analyses largely sidestep the ques- One possible explanation for this pattern is that, in
tion of whether epistemic and existential needs shape countries with high levels of ideological constraint, elite
ideology in similar ways across the economic and cultural discourse constructs the meaning of the economic domain
domains. Recent reviews that more directly address this in a way that ties it more closely to cultural ideology, but
question find that needs for security and certainty (and politically engaged citizens are more likely to be exposed
related variables) have, on average, a stronger relationship to such discourse. For example, this might involve fram-
to cultural ideology than economic ideology [36 ing rightist economic policy in system-justifying terms or
,37,38,39]. Recent empirical examples illustrating this simply pairing rightist economic views with symbols that
asymmetry include the effects of feelings of physical strongly resonate with needs for security and certainty for
safety [40], need for cognitive closure [8,41,42], openness cultural reasons, such as ‘conservative’ or ‘Republican’ in
to experience [43], disgust sensitivity [44], need for the U.S. [39,69]. Indeed, recent experimental work
cognition [8], and analytic cognitive style [42,45–48]. demonstrates that partisan and cultural cues strengthen
Moreover, research is mixed with respect to the direction the relationship between needs for certainty and security
of the relationship in the economic domain. Some and economic ideology in the U.S. [39,70].
research finds that epistemic and existential needs (and
related variables) promote leftist economic views [49–58] In sum, while there is strong support for the hypothesis
while other work finds such needs promote rightist views that needs for security and certainty are related to rightist
[59–65]. ideology in a general sense, a growing body of work
suggests the relationship is stronger for cultural than
Recent research also finds substantial variation in the rela- economic ideology. Moreover, recent studies find varia-
tionship of security and certainty needs with economic tion in the relationship with economic ideology across
ideology across individual-level and country-level vari- both political engagement and political context. Overall,
ables. At the individual level, political engagement is par- however, there is a dearth of comparative research and
ticularly influential. In several recent studies, mostly with this is an area ripe for further investigation.
data from the U.S., needs for certainty and security consis-
tently promote rightist economic ideology among politi- Explanations for unshared variance between cultural
cally interested and knowledgeable citizens [39,66,67]. As and economic ideology
political engagement decreases, however, this association Even in countries where there is a moderate to strong
moves toward zero and, in many cases, eventually flips sign association of cultural and economic ideology, most of the
such that needs for certainty and security promote leftist variance in these two orientations is unshared — that is,
economic views at the lowest levels of engagement. To- despite substantively meaningful associations between
date, however, there are relatively few empirical dimensions, mass ideology remains multidimensional.

Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 2020, 34:94–101 www.sciencedirect.com


How different are cultural and economic ideology? Johnston and Ollerenshaw 97

Interestingly, much work in psychology suggests that (at moderately associated with concerns with maintaining
least) two dimensions are useful in explaining the social order. Conversely, RWA is moderately to strongly
psychological antecedents of sociopolitical attitudes: related to concerns with maintaining social order, but
‘One dimension has been labeled authoritarianism, social only weakly (negatively) associated with care and fairness
conservatism, or traditionalism, at one pole, versus open- [81–84].5 Research also demonstrates that both SDO and
ness, autonomy, liberalism, or personal freedom at the low levels of agreeableness explain unique variance
other pole. The other has been labeled economic conser- (above and beyond RWA and other personality traits)
vatism, power, hierarchy, and inequality at one pole, in support for politicians and parties that cross-cut the
versus egalitarianism, humanitarianism, social welfare, traditional left-right cleavage, such as Donald Trump in
and social concern at the other pole’ [71; see also Ref. 72]. the United States and populist parties in Western Europe
[85–88], though one study suggests the relationship of
Duckitt et al.’ dual-process model of ideology and preju- agreeableness to populist attitudes may actually be posi-
dice attempts to unify this body of work within a single tive [89].
theory [73,74]. They argue for two primary dimensions
underpinning sociopolitical attitudes: right-wing authori- Research on core human values and ideology, in the
tarianism (RWA) and social dominance orientation context of Schwartz value theory (SVT) [90], also sup-
(SDO). While RWA and SDO are typically moderately, ports the dual-process approach. Within SVT, basic
positively correlated, Duckitt et al. argue that they are values can be mapped to a two-dimensional space. The
primarily rooted in distinct psychological antecedents. first dimension is anchored by ‘self-transcendence’ (e.g.
Low dispositional openness, and a view that the world benevolence and universalism) at one end and ‘self-
is a threatening and dangerous place, drive an ideology enhancement’ (e.g. power and achievement) at the other,
emphasizing traditionalism, submission to established while the second dimension is anchored by ‘conservation’
authority, and aggression toward social deviants (i.e. high (e.g. tradition, conformity, and security) at one end and
RWA). In contrast, low levels of dispositional agreeable- ‘openness to change’ (e.g. self-direction and stimulation)
ness, compassion, and empathy, and a view that the world at the other. These two dimensions are moderately cor-
is a zero-sum ‘competitive jungle,’ drive an ideology related with SDO and RWA, respectively [71,91] and
seeking advantageous inequality for the self and the research finds that the ‘self-transcendence’ dimension is
in-group (i.e. high SDO). more closely tied to economic ideology, while the
‘conservation’ dimension is more closely tied to cultural
RWA and SDO are not intended to be conceptually ideology [92–94].6
equivalent to cultural and economic ideology, respec-
tively. Nevertheless, concerns with traditional values In sum, a large body of research suggests two dimensions
and social deviance are more relevant to cultural than are useful for capturing the psychological antecedents of
economic issues, which suggests that threat sensitivity sociopolitical attitudes: a dimension related to agreeable-
and openness should be more powerful in explaining ness, compassion, and empathy is primarily associated
cultural than economic ideology. This is supported by with orientations toward equality and inequality, while a
research reviewed in the previous section. In contrast, dimension related to threat sensitivity and uncertainty
concerns with equality and inequality are more relevant aversion is primarily associated with orientations toward
to economic debates. This suggests that agreeableness, traditionalism and social deviance. Consistent with this
compassion, and empathy may be more important in framework, recent work suggests that the latter variables
explaining economic than cultural ideology. Recent are more important in explaining cultural ideology, while
research provides some support for this prediction. Agree- the former are more important to economic ideology.
ableness has a weak-to-moderate relationship with leftist Thus, the dual-process framework provides a plausible
economic preferences (especially in highly developed psychological basis for the multidimensional structure of
countries), while its relationship to cultural views is less mass ideology.
consistent and often (though not always) weaker
[43,59,62,75,76,77]. While there is less research on the Conclusion
topic, empathy may promote leftist economic preferences Economic and cultural ideology do not reduce to a single
under some conditions, though recent work suggests it left-right dimension, but they are not orthogonal. This
interacts with citizens’ political predispositions in com- suggests both shared psychological antecedents as well as
plex ways [78,79,80]. Overall, the relationship of these differences in the roots of ideology across dimensions.
traits to economic versus cultural ideology is a topic that
5
requires additional research. This asymmetry is even more pronounced in multiple regression and
structural equation models where RWA and SDO are included
simultaneously.
Also consistent with the dual-process framework, SDO is 6
Though this research also suggests that in non-post-communist
moderately (negatively) related to concerns with caring Western nations political values show a tendency to collapse toward a
for others and ensuring fair outcomes, but only weakly to single left-right dimension.

www.sciencedirect.com Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 2020, 34:94–101


98 Emotion, motivation, personality and social sciences *Political Ideologies*

Consistent with seminal work in the field, variables Belief Systems in Mass Publics using more recent data from the American
National Elections Studies. The authors find (contrary to some narratives
related to needs for security and certainty partly explain in the literature) that Converse’s claims stand the test of time: few citizens
the positive association of rightist cultural and economic use ideological concepts in describing their political likes and dislikes,
static constraint among policy preferences remains low, and overtime
ideology in the U.S. and Western Europe. Moreover, the stability of preferences is low in both absolute terms and relative to
impact of these needs is larger for politically engaged partisan identity. As Converse argued, only a small subset of especially
‘sophisticated’ citizens evinces high levels of ideological thinking. The
citizens — especially in the economic domain — which third part of the book considers a puzzle: if the mass public is largely
tracks the more general finding that economic and cul- ‘innocent of ideology,’ why do so many people identify as liberal or
conservative? The authors propose that ideological identification is better
tural ideology are correlated more strongly among politi- understood as an extension of group identities than abstract ideological
cally active citizens. Importantly, however, these two thinking.
forms of ideology are sometimes negatively correlated 2. Pan J, Xu Y: China’s ideological spectrum. J Politics 2018,
and needs for certainty and security may help to explain  80:254-273
This article examines political ideology in Chinese mass politics. Using
this pattern as well. While these needs consistently data from the zuobiao survey, conducted online between 2012 and 2014,
promote rightist cultural preferences, they often promote the authors explore the extent to which individual preferences are con-
strained by more abstract ideological principles, as well as the relation-
leftist economic preferences, and this is particularly true of ship among those principles. First, they find relatively low levels of
politically unengaged citizens and countries with less ideological constraint relative to the U.S.: latent ideological dimensions
ideologically constrained political systems. account for less variance in specific preferences in the Chinese compared
to the U.S. mass public. Nonetheless, there is structure to Chinese mass
preferences, and a three-dimensional model appears to best fit the data.
Finally, much variance in cultural and economic ideology The authors conceptualize these dimensions as tapping preferences over
the inclusivity of political institutions, market economies combined with
remains unshared and this is reflected in research that non-traditional values, and nationalism. Strikingly, despite the superior fit
finds asymmetries in the predictive power of psychologi- of the three-dimensional model, they find that these three ideological
dimensions are correlated at close to 1.0. This suggests that, for practical
cal antecedents across dimensions. Variables related to purposes, they collapse to a single dimension which we might concep-
security and certainty needs are more influential in shap- tualize as political, economic, and social liberalism (in the classical, not
American, sense).
ing cultural ideology than economic ideology. Variables
related to agreeableness, compassion, and empathy, by 3. Laméris MD, Jong-A-Pin R, Garretsen H: On the measurement of
voter ideology. Eur J Political Econ 2018, 55:417-432.
contrast, appear more powerful in the economic domain,
though more research is needed on this question. This 4. Lefkofridi Z, Wagner M, Willmann JE: Left-authoritarians and
policy representation in Western Europe: electoral choice
pattern mirrors to some extent long-standing work in across ideological dimensions. West Eur Politics 2014, 37:65-
psychology that identifies two broad dimensions under- 90.
pinning sociopolitical attitudes, one related to orienta- 5. Malka A, Lelkes Y, Soto CJ: Are cultural and economic
tions toward social deviance and traditional morality and  conservatism positively correlated? A large-scale cross-
national test. Br J Political Sci 2019, 49:1045-1069
the other orientations toward equality and inequality. This paper examines the association between economic and cultural
ideology across around 100 nations included in the World Values Survey
between the late-1980s and 2014 (a total of 229 nation-years). There are
Conflict of interest statement several interesting results from this paper. First, if we average the within-
Nothing declared. nation correlations between economic and cultural conservatism, the two
dimensions are negatively related, in the sense that leftist economic
preferences predict rightist cultural preferences, though this average
References and recommended reading correlation is quite small. However, looking at the within-nation correla-
Papers of particular interest, published within the period of review, tions individually reveals many statistically and substantively meaningful
have been highlighted as: associations which are about as likely to be negative as positive. Thus,
there is substantial cross-country heterogeneity in the association of
 of special interest these ideological dimensions. They find that this variation is explained
 of outstanding interest in part by post-communist status (makes the correlation more negative),
extent of human development (makes the correlation less negative), and
extent of moral traditionalism (makes the correlation more negative).
Third, the authors find that individual-level political engagement also
moderates this relationship in several cases, such that the relationship
CRediT authorship contribution statement between economic and cultural ideology is most likely to be positive
Christopher D Johnston: Conceptualization, Investiga- among politically engaged citizens in non-post-communist countries with
high levels of human development and low levels of moral traditionalism.
tion, Writing - original draft, Writing - review & editing. Finally, the authors argue that the general pattern of rightist cultural
Trent Ollerenshaw: Conceptualization, Investigation, ideology paired with leftist economic ideology is partly explained by their
common roots in needs for security and certainty. While these needs
Writing - original draft, Writing - review & editing. increase cultural conservatism, as predicted by past work, they promote
leftist economic ideology, on average.
6. Bartels LM: Partisanship in the Trump era. J Politics 2018,
References 80:1483-1494.
7. Azevedo F et al.: Neoliberal ideology and the justification of
1. Kinder DR, Kalmoe NP: Neither Liberal Nor Conservative:  inequality in capitalist societies: why social and economic
 Ideological Innocence in the American Public. University of dimensions of ideology are intertwined. J Social Issues 2019,
Chicago Press; 2017 75:49-88
This is a book-length examination of the extent and nature of ideological This article reconsiders several results in the recent literature, including
thinking in the American mass public. It is roughly divided into three parts, the claim that citizens are ‘innocent of ideology,’ that cultural and
and all of which are valuable. The first provides a concise and highly economic ideology are largely independent, and that various forms of
readable overview of the debate over Philip Converse’s seminal work in ‘authoritarianism’ are positively related to leftist economic ideology. The
the 1960s and 1970s on mass belief systems. The second reexamines authors also make an extended argument for why economic and cultural
Converse’s primary empirical claims from his 1964 paper The Nature of ideology should be closely related. Specifically, they argue that cultural

Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 2020, 34:94–101 www.sciencedirect.com


How different are cultural and economic ideology? Johnston and Ollerenshaw 99

conservatism and ‘neoliberalism’ in the economic sphere are system- higher among high knowledge citizens. Further, the variance attributable
justifying ideologies that serve similar purposes in perpetuating existing to ideology doubles if we allow for two dimensions rather than one.
social and economic inequalities. This also explains why they should find
common roots in personality traits and beliefs related to authoritarianism. 17. Klar S: A multidimensional study of ideological preferences
Empirically, the article examines four high-quality samples, three from the and priorities among the american public. Public Opin Q 2014,
U.S. in 2016 and one from the U.K. in 2014 and 2015. The article reports 78:344-359.
several important results. First, using several different operationalizations
of each, the authors find substantial correlations between economic and 18. Bakker R et al.: Measuring party positions in Europe: The
cultural ideology, for both low and high sophisticates, such that cultural Chapel Hill expert survey trend file, 1999–2010. Party Politics
conservatism tends to be positively related to support for laissez-faire 2015, 21:143-152.
capitalism. They also find that both dimensions are strongly correlated
with ideological self-placements, suggesting ideological coherence in the 19. Iyengar S et al.: The origins and consequences of affective
mass public. Examining the psychological antecedents of these dimen- polarization in the United States. Annu Rev Political Sci 2019,
sions, they find that both rightist economic and cultural ideology are 22:129-146.
predicted by general system justification and rightist authoritarianism,
20. Federico CM, Fisher EL, Deason G: The authoritarian left
and that neoliberal ideology is predicted (among both high and low
withdraws from politics: ideological asymmetry in the
sophisticates) by social dominance orientation, rightist authoritarianism,
relationship between authoritarianism and political
general system justification, economic system-justification, and gender
engagement. J Politics 2017, 79:1010-1023.
system justification. These results support the argument that both eco-
nomic and cultural ideology are determined, in large part, by needs to 21. Carmines EG, Ensley MJ, Wagner MW: Issue preferences, civic
rationalize status quo inequalities of various forms. engagement, and the transformation of American politics.
Facing the Challenge of Democracy: Explorations in the Analysis of
8. Feldman S, Johnston C: Understanding the determinants of
Public Opinion and Political Participation. 2011:329-354.
political ideology: implications of structural complexity.
Political Psychol 2014, 35:337-358. 22. Bougher LD: The correlates of discord: identity, issue
alignment, and political hostility in polarized America. Political
9. Lupton RN, Myers WM, Thornton JR: Political sophistication and
Behav 2017, 39:731-762.
the dimensionality of elite and mass attitudes, 1980 2004. J
Politics 2015, 77:368-380. 23. Webster SW, Abramowitz AI: The ideological foundations of
affective polarization in the US electorate. Am Politics Res
10. Treier S, Hillygus DS: The nature of political ideology in the
2017, 45:621-647.
contemporary electorate. Public Opin Q 2009, 73:679-703.
24. Barber M, Pope JC: Does party trump ideology? Disentangling
11. Freeze M, Montgomery JM: Static stability and evolving
party and ideology in America. Am Political Sci Rev 2019,
 constraint: preference stability and ideological structure in the
113:38-54.
mass public. Am Politics Res 2016, 44:415-447
As the title suggests, the authors explore how two of Converse’s empirical 25. Lenz GS: Follow the Leader?: How Voters Respond to Politicians’
claims hold up over time: the extent of preference stability in the mass Policies and Performance. University of Chicago Press; 2013.
public and the extent of static constraint. Using panel data from the
American National Elections Study (two datasets from the 1970s and 26. Carsey TM, Layman GC: Changing sides or changing minds?
1990s, respectively) and The American Panel Study (TAPS; from the Party identification and policy preferences in the American
2010s) as a means of correcting for measurement error, they find a electorate. Am J Political Sci 2006, 50:464-477.
substantial degree of preference stability over time for a wide array of
issues, in some cases approaching that of partisanship. Of particular 27. Tesler M: Priming predispositions and changing policy
interest to the present review, they find substantial correlations between positions: an account of when mass opinion is primed or
error-cleaned economic and cultural preferences and also find that inter- changed. Am J Political Sci 2015, 59:806-824.
domain constraint is increasing over time. For example, in TAPS, gay
marriage preferences are correlated at about 0.40 with preferences on the 28. Schonfeld B, Winter-Levy S: Policy Voting or Partisanship in the
minimum wage and 0.60 with preferences on government health insur- United Kingdom? Quasi-Experimental Evidence from Brexit.
ance. The authors confirm prior research in finding greater stability and Quasi-Experimental Evidence from Brexit (March 28, 2019). 2019.
(especially) constraint among high sophisticates relative to low sophis-
ticates. Critically, however, they also find substantial stability and con- 29. Layman GC et al.: Activists and conflict extension in American
straint among the latter group. party politics. Am Political Sci Rev 2010, 104:324-346.

12. Abramowitz A: The Disappearing Center: Engaged Citizens, 30. Jost JT, Federico CM, Napier JL: Political ideology: Its structure,
Polarization, and American Democracy. Yale University Press; functions, and elective affinities. Annu Rev Psychol 2009,
2010. 60:307-337.

13. Druckman JN, Peterson E, Slothuus R: How elite partisan 31. Jost JT et al.: Political conservatism as motivated social
polarization affects public opinion formation. Am Political Sci cognition. Psychol Bull 2003, 129:339.
Rev 2013, 107:57-79.
32. Jost JT et al.: The politics of fear: is there an ideological
14. McCarty N: Polarization: What Everyone Needs to KnowJ. Oxford asymmetry in existential motivation? Soc Cogn 2017, 35:324-
University Press; 2019. 353.

15. Lönnqvist J-E, Kivikangas M: Economic Attitudes, social 33. Thorisdottir H et al.: Psychological needs and values underlying
attitudes and their psychological underpinnings – a study of left-right political orientation: cross-national evidence from
the finish political elite. Front Psychol 2019, 10. Eastern and Western Europe. Public Opin Q 2007, 71:175-203.

16. Lauderdale BE, Hanretty C, Vivyan N: Decomposing public 34. Jost JT: Ideological asymmetries and the essence of political
 opinion variation into ideology, idiosyncrasy, and instability. J psychology. Political Psychol 2017, 38:167-208.
Politics 2018, 80:707-712
This short article uses two-wave panel data in the U.S. (with a one-month 35. Stern C, Sterling J, Jost JT: Getting closure on conservatism, or
gap between waves), and an innovative statistical model, to decompose the politics of epistemic and existential motivation. The
the variance in mass policy preferences into three components: ideology Motivation-cognition Interface. Routledge; 2017:74-105.
(variation across respondents that is constant across both time and
issues), instability (variation within respondents across time that is spe- 36. Federico CM, Malka A: The contingent, contextual nature of the
cific to issues), and idiosyncrasy (variation across respondents that is relationship between needs for security and certainty and
specific to a given issue and constant across time). The authors find that political preferences: evidence and implications. Political
about one-seventh of the total variance is attributable to ideology, three- Psychol 2018, 39:3-48.
sevenths to instability, and three-sevenths to idiosyncrasy. However, they
37. Crawford JT: Are conservatives more sensitive to threat than
find that the variance attributable to instability is higher among citizens
liberals? It depends on how we define threat and
low in political knowledge and the variance attributable to ideology is
conservatism. Social Cogn 2017, 35:354-373.

www.sciencedirect.com Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 2020, 34:94–101


100 Emotion, motivation, personality and social sciences *Political Ideologies*

38. Hibbing JR, Smith KB, Alford JR: Differences in negativity bias 55. Johnston CD: Dispositional sources of economic
underlie variations in political ideology. Behav Brain Sci 2014, protectionism. Public Opin Q 2013, 77:574-585.
37:297-350.
56. Kam CD, Estes BA: Disgust sensitivity and public demand for
39. Johnston CD, Lavine HG, Federico CM: Open Versus Closed: protection. J Politics 2016, 78:481-496.
 Personality, Identity, and the Politics of Redistribution. Cambridge
University Press; 2017 57. Nilsson A, Erlandsson A, Västfjäll D: The complex relation
In this book, the authors propose a theory linking economic ideology in the between receptivity to pseudo-profound bullshit and political
American mass public to personality factors related to low levels of open- ideology. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 2019. p. 0146167219830415.
ness, such as the need for cognitive closure and conservation values. They
58. Petrescu DC, Parkinson B: Incidental disgust increases
argue that such factors predispose people to leftist economic ideology
adherence to left-wing economic attitudes. Soc Justice Res
because of the security and certainty enhancing function served by redis-
2014, 27:464-486.
tribution, social welfare, and government regulation of markets. Importantly;
however, elite packaging of rightist economic policies with cultural con- 59. Clifford S, Jewell RM, Waggoner PD: Are samples drawn from
servatism reverses the sign of this relationship among citizens attentive to Mechanical Turk valid for research on political ideology? Res
elite discourse, for whom economic ideology becomes inextricable from the Politics 2015, 2 2053168015622072.
broader cultural and political symbols of political conflict in the United
States. The upshot is that economic ideology among politically engaged 60. Pedersen WS, Muftuler LT, Larson CL: Conservatism and the
citizens is, to a large extent, a reflection of deeper cultural conflicts, as well neural circuitry of threat: economic conservatism predicts
as the personality factors that underpin these conflicts. The authors provide greater amygdala–BNST connectivity during periods of threat
support for this theory in several ways. First, they show that the relationship vs safety. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2018, 13:43-51.
of these traits to leftist economic preferences is positive among the poli-
tically unengaged and negative among the engaged. Second, they show 61. Everett JAC: The 12 item social and economic conservatism
that these traits predict rightist political identification and information scale (SECS). PLoS One 2013, 8 e82131.
seeking, but this relationship is much stronger among engaged citizens.
62. Gerber AS et al.: Personality and political attitudes:
Third, they show experimentally that the provision of partisan, ideological,
relationships across issue domains and political contexts. Am
and cultural cues increases the relationship of these traits to rightist
Political Sci Rev 2010, 104:111-133.
economic preferences, but primarily among engaged citizens.
63. Hennes EP et al.: Not all ideologies are created equal:
40. Napier JL et al.: Superheroes for change: physical safety epistemic, existential, and relational needs predict system-
promotes socially (but not economically) progressive attitudes justifying attitudes. Soc Cogn 2012, 30:669-688.
among conservatives. Eur J Soc Psychol 2018, 48:187-195.
64. Mondak JJ: Personality and the Foundations of Political Behavior.
41. Federico CM, Ergun D, Hunt C: Opposition to equality and Cambridge University Press; 2010.
support for tradition as mediators of the relationship between
epistemic motivation and system-justifying identifications. 65. Sterling J, Jost JT, Pennycook G: Are neoliberals more
Group Processes Intergroup Relat 2014, 17:524-541. susceptible to bullshit? Judgment Decis Making 2016, 11.
42. Yılmaz O, Sarıbay SA: An attempt to clarify the link between 66. Johnston CD: Authoritarianism, affective polarization, and
cognitive style and political ideology: A non-western replication economic ideology. Political Psychol 2018, 39:219-238.
and extension. 2016.
67. Jedinger A, Burger AM: The role of right-wing authoritarianism
43. Fatke M: Personality traits and political ideology: a first global and political sophistication in shaping attitudes toward
assessment. Political Psychol 2017, 38:881-899. redistribution. Eur J Soc Psychol 2019, 49:560-573.
44. Terrizzi JA Jr, Shook NJ, Ventis WL: Disgust: a predictor of 68. Malka A et al.: Do needs for security and certainty predict
social conservatism and prejudicial attitudes toward cultural and economic conservatism? A cross-national
homosexuals. Pers Individ Diff 2010, 49:587-592. analysis. J Pers Soc Psychol 2014, 106:1031-1051.
45. Yilmaz O, Adil Saribay S, Iyer R: Are neo-liberals more intuitive? 69. Malka A, Soto CJ: Rigidity of the economic right? Menu-
Undetected libertarians confound the relation between independent and menu-dependent influences of
analytic cognitive style and economic conservatism. Curr psychological dispositions on political attitudes. Curr Direct
Psychol 2019 https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-019-0130-x. Psychol Sci 2015, 24:137-142.
46. Deppe KD et al.: Reflective liberals and intuitive conservatives: 70. Johnston CD, Wronski J: Personality dispositions and political
a look at the cognitive reflection test and ideology. Judgment preferences across hard and easy issues. Political Psychol
Decis Making 2015, 10. 2015, 36:35-53.
47. Yilmaz O, Saribay SA: Lower levels of resistance to change (but 71. Duckitt J, Sibley CG: Personality, ideology, prejudice, and
not opposition to equality) is related to analytic cognitive style. politics: a dual-process motivational model. J Pers 2010,
Soc Psychol 2018, 49:65-75. 78:1861-1894.
48. Yılmaz O, Sarıbay SA: The relationship between cognitive style and 72. Miles A, Vaisey S: Morality and politics: comparing alternate
political orientation depends on the measures used. 2017. theories. Soc Sci Res 2015, 53:252-269.
49. Carl N: Verbal intelligence is correlated with socially and 73. Duckitt J: A dual-process cognitive-motivational theory of
economically liberal beliefs. Intelligence 2014, 44:142-148. ideology and prejudice. Advances in Experimental Social
Psychology. Elsevier; 2001:41-113.
50. Jedinger A, Burger AM: The ideological foundations of
economic protectionism: authoritarianism, social dominance 74. Duckitt J, Sibley CG: The dual process motivational model of
orientation, and the moderating role of political involvement. ideology and prejudice. The Cambridge Handbook of the
Poltical Psychol 2020 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/pops.12627. Psychology of Prejudice. 2017:188-221.

51. Lewis GJ, Bates TC: Higher levels of childhood intelligence 75. Aichholzer J, Danner D, Rammstedt B: Facets of personality and
predict increased support for economic conservatism in “ideological asymmetries. J Res Pers 2018, 77:90-100.
adulthood. Intelligence 2018, 70:36-41.
76. Bakker BN: Personality traits, income, and economic ideology.
52. Arikan G, Sekercioglu E: Authoritarian predispositions and Political Psychol 2017, 38:1025-1041.
attitudes towards redistribution. Political Psychol 2019, 0.
77. Bakker BN, Lelkes Y: Selling ourselves short? How abbreviated
53. Ludeke SG, Rasmussen SHR: Different political systems  measures of personality change the way we think about
suppress or facilitate the impact of intelligence on how you vote: personality and politics. J Politics 2018, 80:1311-1325
a comparison of the U.S. and Denmark. Intelligence 2018, 70:1-6. This article makes the simple but important point that the push for
representative samples in the study of personality and politics has had
54. Carl N: Cognitive ability and political beliefs in the United the unintended side-effect of attenuating the observed relationship
States. Pers Individual Diff 2015, 83:245-248. between traits and political attitudes and behavior, on average. The

Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 2020, 34:94–101 www.sciencedirect.com


How different are cultural and economic ideology? Johnston and Ollerenshaw 101

reason is that researchers using representative samples tend to cut costs 83. Kugler M, Jost JT, Noorbaloochi S: Another look at moral
by using abbreviated personality instruments that have poor reliability. foundations theory: do authoritarianism and social dominance
Empirically, the authors provide a stark demonstration of the implications orientation explain liberal-conservative differences in “Moral”
with examples taken from literature on the need for cognition and political intuitions? Soc Justice Res 2014, 27:413-431.
decision making and from the literature on the Big Five personality traits
and political ideology. 84. Milojev P et al.: Right-wing authoritarianism and social
dominance orientation predict different moral signatures. Soc
78. Feldman S, Huddy L, Wronski J, Lown P: The interplay of Justice Res 2014, 27:149-174.
empathy and individualism in support for social welfare
policies. Political Psychol 2019 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/ 85. Choma BL, Hanoch Y: Cognitive ability and authoritarianism:
pops.12620. understanding support for Trump and Clinton. Pers Individual
Diff 2017, 106:287-291.
79. Lucas BJ, Kteily NS: (Anti-)egalitarianism differentially predicts
 empathy for members of advantaged versus disadvantaged 86. Bakker BN, Rooduijn M, Schumacher G: The psychological roots
groups. J Pers Soc Psychol 2018, 114:665-692 of populist voting: evidence from the United States, the
This paper examines the relationship between egalitarianism and empa- Netherlands and Germany. Eur J Political Res 2016, 55:302-320.
thy. Previous research suggests that egalitarianism and related con-
structs (e.g. low social dominance orientation) are associated with higher 87. Pettigrew TF: Social psychological perspectives on Trump
levels of empathy. In contrast, the authors propose a conditional theory in supporters. J Soc Political Psychol 2017, 5:107-116.
which egalitarianism is associated with higher empathy for disadvan-
taged targets but lower levels of empathy for advantaged targets. They 88. Womick J et al.: Group-based dominance and authoritarian
argue that egalitarianism shapes perceptions of harm differently for the aggression predict support for Donald Trump in the 2016 US
two types of targets and that these differential perceptions mediate the presidential election. Soc Psychol Pers Sci 2018.
effect of egalitarianism on empathy. Across eight studies, with varied 1948550618778290.
sampling frames and a variety of stimulus materials, they find strong
support for this theory. The authors find that the dynamic holds regardless 89. Fatke M: The personality of populists: how the Big Five traits
of the respondent’s own social status. They also examine perceived value relate to populist attitudes. Pers Individual Diff 2019, 139:138-151.
conflict with the target and perceived deservingness of the target as
alternative mediating pathways and find that perceptions of harm are the 90. Schwartz SH et al.: Refining the theory of basic individual
primary mediator of the effect of egalitarianism. values. J Pers Soc Psychol 2012, 103:663.

80. Sparkman DJ, Eidelman S, Till DF: Ingroup and outgroup 91. Feather NT, McKee IR: Values, right-wing authoritarianism,
interconnectedness predict and promote political ideology social dominance orientation, and ambivalent attitudes
through empathy. Group Processes Intergroup Relations toward women. J Appl Soc Psychol 2012, 42:2479-2504.
2019. p. 1368430218819794.
92. Goren P et al.: A unified theory of value-based reasoning and
81. Federico CM et al.: Mapping the connections between politics US public opinion. Political Behav 2016, 38:977-997.
and morality: the multiple sociopolitical orientations involved
in moral intuition. Political Psychol 2013, 34:589-610. 93. Goren P: On voter competence. Oxford University Press; 2013.

82. Sidanius J et al.: You’re inferior and not worth our concern: the 94. Schwartz SH et al.: Basic Personal values underlie and give
interface between empathy and social dominance orientation. coherence to political values: a cross national study in
J Pers 2013, 81:313-323. 15 countries. Political Behav 2014, 36:899-930.

www.sciencedirect.com Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 2020, 34:94–101

You might also like