You are on page 1of 24

Neuroscience Insights

Is there any gender in different genres of the art of Music ?

Journal: Neuroscience Insights

Manuscript ID EXN-22-0045

Manuscript Type: Perspective

Date Submitted by the


07-Sep-2022
Fo
Author:

Complete List of Authors: GARCIA-FALGUERAS, ALICIA; Biblioteca Nacional de Espana, Psychology


Research; Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience
rP

Neuroanatomy, gender, music, classical music, fado, flamenco, genre,


Keywords:
sulcus, temporal lobe, motor cortex, guitar, viola, rhythm, mandoline
ee

Music is an art form and cultural activity whose language, the sounds
and silences, is organized in time with logic and sensitivity. it seems not
wrong to affirm the art form of classical music does not have a clear
gender distinction and it might be mainly and ideally focus to intellectual
rR

purposes. Music as a whole is the result of an ancestral non verbal, non


violent and international mode of human expression and communication.
Musicality and music implies two different sides of the same coin, where
the former is based on the human capacities to produce the further. We
ev

propose that if there is any allusion or reminder about the gender in


Abstract: music might happen most probably in the folk non always written pagan
or secular music with lyric or voice. When differences between genders
iew

occurs in classical music it is usually due to empirical differences in


amplitude of records for sound waves (different voices in choirs). On the
other hand, folk and cultural music such as Fado or Flamenco are more
gender specific. In this text we will explore some neurological basis of
music in the brain that might have themselves some gender
neuroanatomical or functional differences. We will get into three different
genres of music (Classical music, Fado and Flamenco) to elucidate some
possible explanations about these gender differences.

https://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/exn
Page 1 of 23 Neuroscience Insights

1
2
3
4
5
Is there any gender in different genres of
6
7
8
the art of Music ?
9
10
11 Alicia Garcia-Falgueras1, 2
12
1Biblioteca Nacional de España, Paseo de Recoletos 20-22, 28001, Madrid
13
14 2Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience, Meibergdreef 47, 1105 BA Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
15
16
17 Email address:
18
19 algarfal@hotmail.com
20
21
Fo

22
23 Abstract: Music is an art form and cultural activity whose language, the
24
rP

25 sounds and silences, is organized in time with logic and sensitivity. it seems
26
27 not wrong to affirm the art form of classical music does not have a clear
28
gender distinction and it might be mainly and ideally focus to intellectual
ee

29
30 purposes. Music as a whole is the result of an ancestral non verbal, non
31
rR

32 violent and international mode of human expression and communication.


33
34 Musicality and music implies two different sides of the same coin, where
35
ev

the former is based on the human capacities to produce the further. We


36
37 propose that if there is any allusion or reminder about the gender in music
38
iew

39 might happen most probably in the folk non always written pagan or secular
40
41
music with lyric or voice. When differences between genders occurs in
42 classical music it is usually due to empirical differences in amplitude of
43
44 records for sound waves (different voices in choirs). On the other hand, folk
45
46 and cultural music such as Fado or Flamenco are more gender specific. In
47 this text we will explore some neurological basis of music in the brain that
48
49 might have themselves some gender neuroanatomical or functional
50
51 differences. We will get into three different genres of music (Classical
52
53
music, Fado and Flamenco) to elucidate some possible explanations about
54 these gender differences.
55
56
57
58 Keywords: gender, music, classical music, flamenco, fado, genre,
59
sulcus, temporal lobe, motor cortex, guitar, mandoline, viola, rhythm,
60

1
https://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/exn
Neuroscience Insights Page 2 of 23

1
2
3
4
5
6 1. Introduction
7
8 Accordingly to the definition of “music as an art form and cultural
9
10 activity whose language, the sounds and silences, is organized in time with
11
12 logic and sensitivity”, there are two different and separated sources where
13
music emerges: the art form and the cultural activity. The former involves
14
15 the written classical style, the later implies that the folk has not always
16
17 written pagan or secular music with lyric or voice. A huge gap have
18
19 always existed between these two musical activities, increased by religious
20 arguments, different dancing styles, type of audience which it is directed,
21
Fo

22 percussion grade within the music, musical intentionality (higher and pure
23
24 communication vs fun and entertainment), etc. Honorable exceptions
rP

25
26
happened for these two categories, such as the Gregorian chant, sacred
27 written songs sung by choirs of religious men and boys or the sung poems
28
ee

29 written by the dissenting young clerics the Goliards, some of them have
30
31 been reinterpreted by Carl Orff of the manuscript poems written in 1230
rR

32
Carmina Burana in 1936.
33
34 Another distinctive definition could be the difference between
35
ev

36 musicality and music. The first one implies a natural and inherent
37
38 spontaneous human trait based upon the musical brain and its different
iew

39 functions (biology and cognition). The second one, music, is the result of
40
41 that musicality developed inside a social and a cultural environment [1, 2].
42
43 For Darwin, music had no survival benefits, but it offered a means of
44
45
impressing potential partners. It is very noticeable the social value that
46 music has as a social glue. Music and musicality might be considered as an
47
48 embodied language which provide a manner to the brain for communicating
49
50 with the environment (what the ensemble is playing) without losing the own
51
musical track [2]. Advances in molecular technologies and twin studies have
52
53 reported familiar clusters of specific genes for amusia, absolute pitch or
54
55 pitch perception [3]. Recording in raw acoustic signals (wavelet transform
56
57 signals) and brain signals (electroencephalographies, EEG) have also been
58 used to study the synchrony between the sound of guitars playing in duet
59
60 with the brain signals of their players [4].

2
https://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/exn
Page 3 of 23 Neuroscience Insights

1
2
3 Despite the fact many kinds of music have been done under the
4
5 effects of drugs or alcohol, there is not prove or evidence that music had any
6
7 psychedelic effects per se. Aggressively might be the same, an state of mind
8
expressed through music, but not caused by its simple auditory action.
9
10 While people is listening or playing music, violence is far away from their
11
12 minds. Under specific circumstances, music and its loudness has been used
13
14 as a tool for tortures and abuses in Guantanamo jails. However, music is
15 generally a more friendly speech, like Mozart´s compositions: he was
16
17 unable to swear or curse through music [5]. Music as a mind game, is
18
19 mostly funny and smiley created to amuse and whoopee as a reward.
20
21
Musical performance involves several complex and different acts
Fo

22 (symmetry among hemispheres, motor coordination, concentration on the


23
24 sounds, etc), whether constructed for artistic music or construed for other
rP

25
26 music and each of them implies different specific brain areas related to the
27
ear [6]. In nature, birds use sounds to communicate (in many songbirds
28
ee

29 species only male sing) and their auditory cells in their brains are hormone
30
31 sensitive and depending on the breeding season. Those auditory cortical
rR

32
33 neurons are shaped by learning [7].
34 Depending on specific qualities of the melody, as its tender nature,
35
ev

36 flexibly rather than emphatically constructed, some listeners might consider


37
38 specific melodies as typically constructed by feminine or masculine
iew

39
composers. The number of female composers or conductors is very few,
40
41 probably due to a confidence matter, related to the different number of
42
43 professional chances or the amount of unreasonable obstacles for each
44
45 gender [8].
46 Some Dutch musicians such as Antonia Brico and Frieda Belinfante
47
48 became conductors after passing many turbulent obstacles, uncountable
49
50 recklessness. Brico moved to the USA before becoming a conductor, while
51
52
Belinfante did the same after she became a conductor. The knowledge of the
53 written piece of music, the ability of giving instructions to musicians about
54
55 when to start, how change intensity or tempo, the capacity of controlling
56
57 emotions caused by music, leadership preparedness etc, might not be
58
different between man and woman. Not in vain, some female classical
59
60 music composers chose to give their pieces and merits to men, for being

3
https://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/exn
Neuroscience Insights Page 4 of 23

1
2
3 someway listened in their own constructions by an audience, no mattered
4
5 copyrights or labels. That is the case of Josephine Lang who gave her songs
6
7 to Robert Schumann or Maria Anna Mozart, who did the same with her well
8
known and reputed brother, or Felix Mendelssohn, who obtained his elder
9
10 sister Fanny´s compositions, Opuses 8 and 9, as his own pieces [9, 10, 11].
11
12 In these cases, not under a pseudonymous, but deleting their whole identity
13
14 from their works. And nobody noticed, proving the gender of the composer
15 for classical music is not identifiable from the musical content of a
16
17 composition itself. That was also empirically proved after checking the
18
19 opinion of 71 listener of classical musical extracts: they did not find
20
21
watermarks that significantly reveals the accurate sex of a composer [11].
Fo

22 Gender in classical music is difficult to perceive.


23
24 And the same is true for the construed dimension of artistic music.
rP

25
26 After analyzing the performance of several interpreters, 69 well trained
27
musically listeners were not able to accurately distinguish the gender of the
28
ee

29 performing artist or interpreter [12]. Measuring some parameters during the


30
31 playing by 54 pianists (10 women and 44 men) of the song “Arlequin” from
rR

32
33 Schumann´s “Carnaval”, op. 9, (tempo Vivo or, Vivace, lively and fast, 156-
34 176 beats per minute, bpm for both genders) they found a small but
35
ev

36 statistically significant difference in the speed interpretation of the tempo.


37
38 While women performed at 165 bpm men did it at 180 bpm [13]. This funny
iew

39
piece of music by Schumann is made in three different structures: one and
40
41 two parts that are very rhythmical with a clear beat. However, part three is
42
43 more melodic and singing, with a clear ritardando written in the music sheet.
44
45 Difference in genders might only be pointing out a different interpretation of
46 that ritardando (more or less elongation in time) and have nothing to do
47
48 with skills or abilities in performance.
49
50 Curiously a different gender expressions of gender social roles has
51
52
been described between musicians: men classical musicians show a higher
53 female-typical gender role behavior while the opposite is true for female
54
55 classical musicians, compared both to non-musician people [14]. After
56
57 checking steroids levels in voice musicians, Nieschlang and colleagues
58
found androgens might shorten and estrogens might extend life expectancy
59
60 in singer women, while in singer men, the opposite might be true [15].

4
https://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/exn
Page 5 of 23 Neuroscience Insights

1
2
3 Hormones are very present in classical music [16], however performances
4
5 between genders is not remarkable different.
6
7 Musical practice and listening for both genders involves cerebral
8
benefits. It also protects brain against declination of certain cognitive
9
10 domains which happens with aging (auditory perception) [5, 17]. When
11
12 music involves human voices together with instruments, the gender of the
13
14 singing is more palpable, corporeal or tangible. The human voice is a feature
15 which each person possess and it gives information about his or her gender,
16
17 mood, origin, etc. [18].
18
19 Speaking about body movement, during watching dance, the occipital
20
21
temporal cortex (left fusiform gyrus) is significantly activated in male and
Fo

22 female dancers, compared to non dancers. Remarkable is the fact that the
23
24 expertise in body movements of professional dancers and athletes of both
rP

25
26 sexes carries an increase in symmetry or a reduction in the hemispherical
27
asymmetry in the sensorimotor and visual areas compared with non experts
28
ee

29 (Figure 1) [19].
30
31
rR

32
33
34
35
ev

36
37
38
iew

39
40
41
42
43
Figure 1. Topographic maps of the voltage distribution over the scalp while watching
44
45 dance performance. Dancer group (left side of the picture) showed bilaterality negative
46
values of voltage over the occipito-temporal area (blue areas), while non dancers (right
47
48 side of the picture) only showed that negativity in right hemispherere. With permission,
49
Orlandi and Proverbio, 2019.
50
51
52 Related to body balance in professional dancers, it was described a
53
54
gender difference in the time of body stabilization after landing from a
55 vertical jump: female dancers presented a higher rate of ankle sprain. In the
56
57 last few years. Speed and dynamics of dance performance have increased,
58
59 raising the risk of injuries and accordingly to the performance level [20]. That
60
might have an effect over the time to equilibrium, being prevalence of

5
https://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/exn
Neuroscience Insights Page 6 of 23

1
2
3 stabilization higher in girls, in both directions (anterior-posterior and medial-
4
5 lateral axis) compared to male dancers [20, 21].
6
7 Music and language processing occurs across a number of discrete
8
modules in the brain, some of them overlap while others remain distinct.
9
10 During singing instrumental and singing music, the areas of the brain that are
11
12 activated are located in the superior temporal sulcus. These areas are
13
14 considerable larger during musical listening compared to activation that
15 occurs during speech with no music (Figure 2) [22].
16
17
18
19
20
21
Fo

22
23
24
rP

25
26
27
28
ee

29
30
31
rR

32
33
34
35
ev

36
37
38
iew

39
40
41
42
43
44
45 Figure 2. Visual representation of the clusters of activation while listening instrumental
46 music (green bar), singing song (red bar) and language speech (grey bar). Three of them
47
are located in the sulcus area, over the temporal lobe. However, those sounds which carry
48
49 music in their spread, are occupying a significantly larger area in the sulcus (green and
50 red bars) than speech. With permission Whitehead and Armony, 2018.
51
52
53 Some years ago a neuroanatomical study of the human auditory cortex
54
55
showed that cytoarchitectonically there is a gender differences in the amount of
56 neurons in this area around the superior temporal gyrus, including the
57
58 previously mentioned, superior temporal sulcus [23]. Analyzing Nissl stained
59
60 sections with mean thickness of 25 µm: women presented higher amount of

6
https://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/exn
Page 7 of 23 Neuroscience Insights

1
2
3 neurons in layers II and IV (not in III, V or VI ones). Cell packing density in
4
5 both hemispheres of cortical temporal gyrus was 11% greater in females brains
6
7 compared to male, suggesting the idea the number of granular cells were
8
similar between genders, but differences did exist because of a different packed
9
10 mechanism. That 11% difference was explained as one possible cause of a
11
12 mechanical compression or geometric consequence of smaller volume in total
13
14 brain in females [23]. A similar results was found in rat brain: Reid and Juraska
15 found only in layer IV a sex difference in number of neurons, having male rats
16
17 the bigger amount [24] .
18
19 During the experience of musical rhythm, both active production and
20
21
passive perception, the cortical auditory and motor are connected and
Fo

22 activated together with a number of subcortical and different structures such


23
24 as basal ganglia, cerebellum, supplementary motor areas, premotor cortex,
rP

25
26 temporal and parietal cortices. However, the nature of the music and those
27
songs with defined or non-defined clear beats (percussion) might have a
28
ee

29 different effect over this brain activation [25]. Metabolically gender


30
31 differences have been described for biological rhythms, which are regulated
rR

32
33 via hypothalamus, occurring because of the gender differences in its
34 anatomy, physiology and pathophysiology which sustain circadian rhythms
35
ev

36 [26]. Circadian rhythm have been widely proved to be different among


37
38 genders and that difference in metabolisms, not necessarily involved in
iew

39
healthy terms, might also have some implication with music, beats and
40
41 sounds interpretation, especially in folkloric non classical pieces of music.
42
43 Some neuroanatomical gender differences have been found in the
44
45 cerebellum [27, 28] as well as in the temporo-parietal region [29] those that
46 might be involved in the rhythmical production and perception of the music.
47
48
49
2. Genres in Music
50
51
52 Fado is the most internationally known expression of Portuguese music. One
53
54
of the best definitions of Fado was offered by the Fado singer Amália
55 Rodrigues (1920-1999), who is considered the artistic ambassadress of Fados in
56
57 Portugal: "Fado is a very mysterious thing, you have to feel it and you have to
58
59 be born with the anguished side of the people, to feel like someone who has
60
neither ambitions nor desires, a person... as if one did not exist. That person is

7
https://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/exn
Neuroscience Insights Page 8 of 23

1
2
3 me and that's why I was born to sing fado. " It is generally sung by a single
4
5 person, accompanied by string instruments like the "viola", the Portuguese
6
7 guitar, mandolin or rajäo although there are adaptations to other instruments
8
such as piano or dulzaina (Figure 3) [30]. Percussion is not used or very rarely
9
10 [31]. Fado takes its name from the Portuguese word "fate", which is
11
12 synonymous with the Latin word "factum", which means destiny). The main
13
14 theme is the Romance, the myth and the cry of the homeless sailors who sail
15 the seas are recurring themes. However, there are also cheerful and danceable
16
17 fados such as the Fado Batido or the “A Rianxeira”.
18
19
20
21
Fo

22
23
24
rP

25
26
27
28
ee

29
30
31
rR

32
33
34
35 Figure 3. Representative example of a Fado handwritten sheet music for pìano and voice,
ev

36 by the Spanish composer and Professor in the Royal Conservatorium of Madrid, Fernando
37
38 Moraleda Bellver. Tempo is similar to Fado (4/4) and rhythmically it also oscillates like
iew

39 waves of the sea.


40
41
42 Although the most notorious singers of this genre are and have been
43
women, there are also quality male performers such as Alfredo Marceneiro
44
45 and Fernando Farinha or the more recent fadistas Carlos do Carmo and
46
47 Camané [31, 32, 33, 34]. On the other hand, the musicians (nearly always
48
49 string musicians), are always men. The rhythm of Fados is rocking and
50 pendulous, reminding the waves of the sea (Figure 3). It surrounds the
51
52 listener and take him/her to a shared mood trip.
53
54 These songs expressed in taverns (Figure 4), voiced the bad
55
56
moments of life through singing, the tragic sense of life, the melancholy, the
57 nostalgia (named “saudade” or “moriña”) and small stories of the daily life
58
59 of the humble neighborhoods. But, especially and more traditionally, the
60

8
https://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/exn
Page 9 of 23 Neuroscience Insights

1
2
3 fatalism and the frustration of sailor’s lives, the uncertainty of the sea and
4
5 the danger of each crossing to earn the sustenance (fado do marinheiro),
6
7 [33]. It is not known with certainty, but there is evidence that this nostalgic
8
and deep music with guitars of the time, were originated eight centuries ago,
9
10 when the Arabs lived in the hill of the Saint George Castle of Lisbon
11
12 (century XIV). The existence of the Fado is verified since 1838 (19th
13
14 century), although the mystery of its origins has not yet been unveiled [34].
15 Since 2011 the Fado, popular urban singing of Portugal, was named by
16
17 UNESCO as part of the Representative List of Intangible Cultural Heritage
18
19 of Humanity (6.COM 13.39).
20
21
Fo

22
23
24
rP

25
26
27
28
ee

29
30
31
rR

32
33
34
35
ev

36
37
38 Figure 4. Painting of oil on canvas by Viktor Schivert in 1900, showing a typical scene in a
iew

39
40 tavern of early Fados, the lady singer and the musician playing a mandoline.
41
42 Within the genre of Fado sub genres are distinguished such as
43
44 Coimbra Fado, The traditional Fado of Coimbra, is related to the academic
45
46
traditions of the University of Coimbra. It is exclusively sung by men. Both
47 the singers and the musicians wear black cloaks and cassocks. The themes
48
49 refer to student loves or the city. The style emphasizes the instrumental
50
51 component. The best known of the fados of Coimbra is "Coimbra é uma
52 canção” (Coimbra is a song), that had a remarkable success in all Europe.
53
54 Interpreted by students from the bourgeois or aristocratic classes of the
55
56 nineteenth century, the Fado of Coimbra has always been characterized by a
57
58
higher style, with a raised and more erudite character, but with the same
59 romantic aura. Unrecorded interpreters of this genre would be Hilário Costa
60

9
https://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/exn
Neuroscience Insights Page 10 of 23

1
2
3 Alves (a medical student at the University of Coimbra; 1864-1896), whose
4
5 poems in songs were referred to erudite themes related to the human
6
7 condition itself.
8
The Flamenco on the other hand, is a musical genre traditionally
9
10 from the South of Spain closely related to the Gypsies culture of Romani
11
12 (Roma) ethnicity which tries to express the sorrow and pain of many
13
14 centuries of marginalization, poverty and persecution but also the cheerful
15 point of view to cope with miseries (Figure 5). Transmission takes place
16
17 within dynasties, families, social groups and Flamenco clubs, all of which
18
19 are key players in its preservation and dissemination, because Flamenco
20
21
music has traditionally not been written down. It is the product of cross-
Fo

22 hybridization, which happened at the beginning of the XX century, between


23
24 the agrarian culture, urban folklore and popular festivals. For Roma
rP

25
26 ethnicity communities and in certain regions of Spain flamenco music is
27
acquired as a mother tongue. The music and songs are related to their
28
ee

29 experiences in family, labor or social context [35]. This music usually took
30
31 place in specific places (“colmao” or “tabanco”) and one of its peculiarities
rR

32
33 is its ability to sublimate the sorrow, pities, hardships or sacrifices which are
34 link to traditional jobs in the South of Spain (fishing, hunting, harvesting,
35
ev

36 mining, salt flats) and only show with the music the happiness and joy of the
37
38 active hard-working person. This music expression is able to transcend
iew

39
domestic difficulties to make common familiar and social troubles more
40
41 bearable, unifying feeling inside whole and entire communities [35].
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59 Figure 5. Oil on canvas named “El Jaleo” by John Singer Sargent in 1882 showing a
60 typical Spanish scene of a woman dancer, several guitar players (“tocaores”) and several

10
https://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/exn
Page 11 of 23 Neuroscience Insights

1
2
3 singers (“cantaores”), at the end of XIX century, performing in a colmao or tabanco.
4
5 On 16 November 2010, UNESCO declared flamenco one of the
6
7 Masterpieces of the Oral and Intangible Heritage of Humanity (5.COM
8
9 6.39). Like mostly all the vocabulary and words for definitions this genre
10 uses, they are sui generis, that is, with very pronounced intra cultural traits
11
12 and sometimes very different between distinct but close regions. Sometimes
13
14 they are incorrect in terms of Spanish grammar or vocabulary (“naide”
15
16
instead of “nadie”, “dirme” instead of “dime”, “curpita” instead of “culpita”,
17 “cormigo” instead of “conmigo”, “muchachaco” instead of “muchacho” no
18
19 use of verbs, etc).
20
21 It is performed in three dimensions: dance, singing and Spanish
Fo

22
23 guitar although in some cases contemporary flamenco expression is
24
dispensing with Spanish guitar or any other stringed instruments. While the
rP

25
26 two formers (dancing and singing) can be successfully expressed both by
27
28 men or women, the musicians and guitars are mainly only played by men.
ee

29
30 Voices in flamenco use to be located in high register or falsetto and in this
31
way, gender vocalization in flamenco sometime is undistinguishable or very
rR

32
33 similar inside the soprano register (i.e. that might be, in occasions, the case
34
35 between the woman folklore singer Isabel Pantoja and her man cousin
ev

36
37 folklore singer Chiquetete).
38
iew

39 In the academic line of purity, the poet Federico Garcia Lorca and
40
41 the composer Manuel de Falla had the idea of “concurso de cante jondo” in
42
Granada, organized in 1921 and celebrated in 1922, which entitle the
43
44 Flamenco folklore genre for the first time to have the recognition of Jondo
45
46 and international recognition, attempting to protect their deepest artistic
47
48 roots such as a high art (alte-arte). Some years later, in 1928, Falla started to
49 suffer pain in his eyes which affected his compositions [6].
50
51
52
One of the strongest and most original points of the Flamenco is its
53 original rhythm, not always inside the academic and orthodox canons
54
55 established by the metronome [36]. Moreover, Flamenco combines three
56
57 different artistic expressions: voice, guitar and dance, each of them having
58
their own rhythmical patterns and sometimes going in parallel but in
59
60 different sequential times (very often the song accompanies the guitar, the

11
https://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/exn
Neuroscience Insights Page 12 of 23

1
2
3 voice walks over the instrument without a rhythmic schema). In some cases
4
5 improvisation might happen, as a spontaneous musical expression such as in
6
7 Jazz. Although the most characteristic musical time for Flamenco is of 12
8
beats divided into two different times: 6/8 and 2/4 (3 + 3 + 2 + 2 + 2) and
9
10 sometimes 12/8. Songs usually change the tonality at least once and
11
12 compass is also changeable according to lyrics or the structures of the songs.
13
14 There are usually three structures: I) an instrumental introduction, II) the
15 core of the song, III) the refrain and IV) the conclusion, which are repeated
16
17 twice each of them and twice again the total song, except the conclusion.
18
19 Where on those pieces of time the emphasis is focused makes completely
20
21 different songs and styles (palos). The rhythm of “Alegrías”
Fo

22
23 (characteristically marked as “tiriti, tran, tran, tran ….”) is one of the most
24
recognizable and well established and it combines this 12 beats with another
rP

25
26 12 introductory beats (1, 2, 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8, 9, 10// 1, 2, 3, 1, 2, 3, 1, 2, 1,
27
28 2, 1, 2). That might be reason why the Flamenco singer Camarón de la Isla
ee

29
30 (1958-1992) used to say at the beginning of his performances “I am starting
31 with singing Alegrías and, later on you, the audience, will let me know”
rR

32
33 (figure 6).
34
35
ev

36
37
38
iew

39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
Figure 6. Representative examples of a fragment flamenco written music, a Tanguillo from
57
58 Cádiz. It is noticeable the beat of this piece is changing from a binary rhythm to a tertiary,
59 finishing in a binary one (introductory instrumental and part of the melody in 4/4, while rest
60

12
https://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/exn
Page 13 of 23 Neuroscience Insights

1
2
3 song composition is made at 6/8 and the end at 4/4). Total of 12 hits subdivision (6/8 and
4
4/4). Adapted to piano by the author, Alicia Garcia-Falgueras.
5
6
7 The rhythm in flamenco music is very elastic, not always inside the
8
9 conventional scheme of rhythmical framework [36]. Elastic in the meaning
10
of being able to change its shape but after a while, returns to its original.
11
12 This surprising expression causes constant tension which resolves and
13
14 relieves in a sense and provokes reaction on the listeners who follow the
15
16 cadence, expressing themselves sometimes saying words such as “olé” or
17 “arsa” at the end of a rhythm group. Those words were originally
18
19 instructions coming from the dance incentives clapping, because “olé”
20
21 comes from the word ola (wave of the sea), while “arsa” comes from alza
Fo

22
23
(raise), meaning both to make body movements in dance [37].
24
rP

25
26
3. Conclusions
27
28 After this brief revision it seems not wrong to affirm the art form of
ee

29
30 classical music does not have a clear gender distinction and it might be
31 mainly and ideally focus to intellectual purposes. When differences between
rR

32
33 genders occurs in classical music it is usually due to empirical differences in
34
35 amplitude of records for sound waves (different voices in choirs). On the
ev

36
37
other hand, folk and cultural music such as Fado or Flamenco are more
38 gender specific. When singing to love, labors or common troubles happens
iew

39
40 that references to gender differences are more often. In these traditional paths
41
42 of music (Fado and Flamenco) the string instruments are always played by
43
male musicians, but singing and dancing in Flamenco, might be performed by
44
45 both men or women, being sometimes indistinguishable. The outfit of each
46
47 gender during flamenco performances are usually very different and visually
48
49 distinguishable. Accordingly to these differences, brain neuroanatomy might
50 also have distinctions between women and men musicians, however, very
51
52 little research has been done in the field. The brain areas associate to each
53
54 activity involved in folk music have been proved to be different accordingly
55
56
to professionalism, but gender was not analyzed: those brain areas related
57 superior temrpoal gyrus and the superior temporal sulcus to watch and
58
59 understand the movement of dance are more activated symmetrically in
60

13
https://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/exn
Neuroscience Insights Page 14 of 23

1
2
3 professional and while listening or singing songs. Further research on the
4
5 brain basis of gender differences would be recommended to a better
6
7 understanding of the reasons of those palpable sex differences in folk music
8
in order to improve and amplify the beauty of expression that resides in these
9
10 traditional sounds. New areas for research and opportunities to musically
11
12 explore gender differences for future research would be recommended.
13
14
15 Credits of Images
16
17
18
19 Photographs presented in this work come from files with free
20
21
license in the public domain as Wikipedia Commons. In cases where
Fo

22 pictures come with an authorship from a published paper, that has been
23
24 properly quoted and permission to the correspondence author were asked.
rP

25
26
27 Acknowledgements
28
ee

29
We would like to thank to Prof. Swaab at the Netherlands Institute
30
31 for Neuroscience for his counselling, supportive reading and ideas provision.
rR

32
33 Also to the Centro Andaluz del Flamenco in Xerez (Cadiz) and to the
34
35 Biblioteca Nacional de España (Madrid) for providing the scores and music
ev

36 sheets to study. To the Museu do Fado in Lisboa, for its webpage which
37
38 provides information and musical resources.
iew

39
40
41
42
43 References
44
45 [1] Honing H, ten Cate C, Peretz I, Trehub SE. (2015) Without it no music:
46
47 cognition, biology and evolution of musicality. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B
48
49
Biol Sci 370:20140088.
50
51 [2] Dell'Anna A, Leman M, Berti A. (2021): Musical Interaction Reveals Music
52
53 as Embodied Language. Front Neurosci. 15:667838.
54
55 [3] Gingras B, Honing H, Peretz I, Trainor LJ, Fisher SE. (2015): Defining the
56
57 biological bases of individual differences in musicality. Philos Trans R Soc
58
Lond B Biol Sci. 370: 20140092.
59
60

14
https://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/exn
Page 15 of 23 Neuroscience Insights

1
2
3 [4] Müller V, Lindenberger U. (2019): Dynamic Orchestration of Brains and
4
5 Instruments During Free Guitar Improvisation. Front Integr Neurosci.
6
7 4;13:50.
8
9 [5] Swaab, D. (2019): Our Creative Brains. Atlas Contact.
10
11
12
[6] García-Falgueras A, Swaab DF. (2021): The Spanish Composer Manuel de
13 Falla and His Eyes: The Musical Brain. Neurosci Insights. 16: 1- 10.
14
15
16 [7] Brenowitz EA, Remage-Healey L. (2016): It takes a seasoned bird to be a
17 good listener: communication between the sexes. Curr Opin Neurobiol.
18
19 38:12-7.
20
21
Andrew, K. (2012, February 8): Why are there so few women composers?
Fo

[8]
22
23 The Guardian.
24
rP

25
26
[9] Citron, M. J. (1987): Fanny Mendelssohn Hensel (1805-1847). An Historical
27 Anthology of Music by Women. Blomington., IN: University of Indiana
28
ee

29 Press.
30
31
[10] Gates, E. (2007): Fanny Mendelssohn Hensel: A life of Music within
rR

32
33 domestic limits. A Journal of women in Music. 5: 1-16.
34
35
ev

36
[11] Sergeant, D.C., Himonides, E. (2016): Gender and Music Composition: A
37 Study of Music, and the Gendering of Meanings. Front Psychol. 1;7:411.
38
iew

39
40 [12] Sergeant, D.C., Himonides, E. (2014): Gender and the performance of music.
41 Front Psychol. 5: 276.
42
43
44 [13] Lehmann, A. (2011). “Expressive variants in the opening of Robert
45
Schumann’s Arlequin (from Carnaval, op 9): 54 pianists’ interpretations of a
46
47 metrical ambiguity. Cited by Kopiez in Music and the Mind, Essays in
48
49 Honour of John Sloboda, eds I. Deliège and J. W . Davidson (Oxford
50
51 University Press).
52
53 [14] Hassler M. (1991): Testosterone and musical talent. Exp Clin Endocrinol.
54
55 98: 89-98.
56
57 [15] Nieschlag E, Kramer U, Nieschlag S. (2003): Androgens shorten the
58
59 longevity of women: sopranos last longer. Exp Clin Endocrinol Diabetes.
60

15
https://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/exn
Neuroscience Insights Page 16 of 23

1
2
3 11(4):230-1.
4
5
6 [16] Garcia-Falgueras, A. (2019): Music and Hormones. Encyclopedia of
7 Evolutionary Psychological Science, Shackelford, Weekes-Shackelford eds.
8
9 Switzerland.
10
11
12
[17] Román-Caballero R, Arnedo M, Triviño M, Lupiáñez J. (2018): Musical
13 practice as an enhancer of cognitive function in healthy aging - A systematic
14
15 review and meta-analysis. PLoS One. 13:e0207957.
16
17 [18] Garcia-Falgueras, A. (2019): Psychology of the Human Voice. Psychol.
18
19 Behav. Sci. Int. J. 11 (2): 555808.
20
21
Orlandi A, Proverbio AM. (2019): Bilateral engagement of the occipito-
Fo

[19]
22
23 temporal cortex in response to dance kinematics in experts. Sci Rep.
24
30:1000.
rP

25
26
27 [20] Wanke EM, Schreiter J, Groneberg DA, Weisser B. (2018): Muscular
28
ee

29 imbalances and balance capability in dance. J Occup Med Toxicol. 3;13:36


30
31
[21] Pappas E, Kremenic I, Liederbach M, Orishimo KF, Hagins M. (2011):
rR

32
33 Time to stability differences between male and female dancers after landing
34
35 from a jump on flat and inclined floors. Clin J Sport Med. 21: 325-9.
ev

36
37 [22] Whitehead JC, Armony JL. (2018): Singing in the brain: Neural
38
iew

39 representation of music and voice as revealed by fMRI. Hum Brain Mapp.


40
41 39: 4913-4924.
42
43 [23] Witelson SF, Glezer II, Kigar DL. (1995): Women have greater density of
44
45 neurons in posterior temporal cortex. J Neurosci. 15: 3418-28.
46
47 [24] Reid SN, Juraska JM. (1992): Sex differences in neuron number in the
48
49 binocular area of the rat visual cortex. J Comp Neurol. 321: 448-55.
50
51 [25] Kasdan AV, Burgess AN, Pizzagalli F, Scartozzi A, Chern A, Kotz SA,
52
53 Wilson SM, Gordon RL. (2022): Identifying a brain network for musical
54
55 rhythm: A functional neuroimaging meta-analysis and systematic review.
56
57
Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 136: 1-21.
58
59 [26] Nicolaides NC, Chrousos GP. (2020) Sex differences in circadian endocrine
60

16
https://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/exn
Page 17 of 23 Neuroscience Insights

1
2
3 rhythms: Clinical implications. Eur J Neurosci. 52: 2575-2585.
4
5
6 [27] da Veiga M, Prates JC. (1993) The sulci of the cuneus of the human
7 cerebrum. Ital J Anat Embryol. 98: 41-57.
8
9
10 [28] Nguon K, Ladd B, Baxter MG, Sajdel-Sulkowska EM. (2005): Sexual
11
12
dimorphism in cerebellar structure, function, and response to environmental
13 perturbations. Prog Brain Res. 148:341-51.
14
15
16 [29] Witelson SF. (1991): Neural sexual mosaicism: sexual differentiation of the
17 human temporo-parietal region for functional asymmetry.
18
19 Psychoneuroendocrinology. 16:131-53.
20
21
Ribeiro-Castela, L. P. (2011): A guitarra Portuguesa e a Canção de Coimbra.
Fo

[30]
22
23 Facultade de Letras. Universidade de Coimbra.
24
rP

25
26
[31] Widmann-Miguel, E. (2014): Fado. Expresión musical portuguesa. IberInfo.
27 Buenos Aires.
28
ee

29
30 [32] LeVeque, S. N. (2015): El Fado portugués. De Lisboa a Coimbra, del Mar
31
hasta la tierra. Universidad de Oviedo. Patrimonio Musical.
rR

32
33
34 [33] Pérez López, PJ (2012): Fado, saudade y tragedia. Dispitatio Philosophical
35
ev

36
Research Bulletin, 1: 97-111.
37
38 [34] Rocha-Pinto, J. A. V. C. (2012): Analise e sintese de Musica atraves de
iew

39
40 Sistemas Computacionais. Instituto Superior de Engenharia do Porto.
41
42 [35] Casas-Mas A, Pozo JI, Montero I. (2022): Oral Tradition as Context for
43
44 Learning Music From 4E Cognition Compared With Literacy Cultures. Case
45
Studies of Flamenco Guitar Apprenticeship. Front Psychol. 13:733615.
46
47
48 [36] Berlanga, M. A. (2014): La originalidad musical del Flamenco: el compás.
49
50 Sinfonía Virtual, Revista de Música y Reflexión Musical, 26: 1- m21.
51
52 [37] Falla, M. (1910): Seguidille nº 3 to Madam Claude Debussy. Rouart Lerolle
53
54 & Cir. Editors (score).
55
56
57
58
59
60

17
https://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/exn
Neuroscience Insights Page 18 of 23

1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
Fo
20
21
22
23
rP

24
25
26
ee

27
28
29
rR

30
31 Fig. 1. Topographic maps of the voltage distribution over the scalp while watching dance performance.
32 Dancer group (left side of the picture) showed bilaterality negative values of voltage over the occipito-
temporal area (blue areas), while non dancers (right side of the picture) only showed that negativity in right
ev

33
hemispherere. With permission, Orlandi and Proverbio, 2019.
34
35 254x190mm (96 x 96 DPI)
36
iew

37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60 https://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/exn
Page 19 of 23 Neuroscience Insights

1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
Fo
20
21
22
23
rP

24
25
26
ee

27
28
29
rR

30
31
32
ev

33
34
35
36
iew

37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45 Fig. 2.- Visual representation of the clusters of activation while listening instrumental music (green bar),
46 singing song (red bar) and language speech (grey bar). Three of them are located in the sulcus area, over
47 the temporal lobe. However, those sounds which carry music in their spread, are occupying a significantly
larger area in the sulcus (green and red bars) than speech. With permission Whitehead and Armony, 2018).
48
49 190x254mm (96 x 96 DPI)
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60 https://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/exn
Neuroscience Insights Page 20 of 23

1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
Fo
20
21
22
23
rP

24
25
26
ee

27
28
29
rR

30
31 Fig. 3.- Representative example of a Fado handwritten sheet music for piano and voice, by the Spanish
32 composer and Professor in the Royal Conservatorium of Madrid, Fernando Moraleda Bellver. Tempo is
typically as Fado (4/4) and rhythmically it also oscillates like waves of the sea.
ev

33
34 254x190mm (96 x 96 DPI)
35
36
iew

37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60 https://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/exn
Page 21 of 23 Neuroscience Insights

1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
Fo
20
21
22
23
rP

24
25
26
ee

27
28
29
rR

30
31
32
ev

33
34
35
36
iew

37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45 Fig. 4.- Painting of oil on canvas by Viktor Schivert in 1900, showing a typical scene in a tavern of early
46 Fados, the lady singer and the musician playing a mandoline.
47
190x254mm (96 x 96 DPI)
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60 https://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/exn
Neuroscience Insights Page 22 of 23

1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
Fo
20
21
22
23
rP

24
25
26
ee

27
28
Fig. 5. Oil on canvas named “El Jaleo” by John Singer Sargent in 1882 showing a typical Spanish scene of a
29
woman dancer, several guitar players (“tocaores”) and several singers (“cantaores”), at the end of XIX
rR

30 century, performing in a colmao or tabanco.


31
32 282x190mm (72 x 72 DPI)
ev

33
34
35
36
iew

37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60 https://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/exn
Page 23 of 23 Neuroscience Insights

1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
Fo
20
21
22
23
rP

24
25
26
ee

27
28
29
rR

30
31
32
ev

33
34
35
36
iew

37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45 Figure 6.- Representative examples of flamenco written music, a Tanguillo from Cádiz. It is noticeable the
46 beat of this piece is changing from a binary rhythm to a tertiary, finishing in a binary one (introductory
47 instrumental and part of the melody in 4/4, while rest song composition is made at 6/8 and the end at 4/4).
Total of 12 hits subdivision (6/8 and 4/4). Adapted to piano by the author, Alicia Garcia-Falgueras.
48
49 190x254mm (96 x 96 DPI)
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60 https://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/exn

You might also like