You are on page 1of 15

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/3076753

Culture and Consumer Responses to Web Download Time: A Four-Continent


Study of Mono and Polychronism

Article  in  IEEE Transactions on Engineering Management · March 2003


DOI: 10.1109/TEM.2002.808262 · Source: IEEE Xplore

CITATIONS READS
105 778

3 authors:

Gregory M Rose J. Roberto Evaristo


Washington State University 3M
47 PUBLICATIONS   3,234 CITATIONS    103 PUBLICATIONS   3,281 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Detmar Straub
Temple University
240 PUBLICATIONS   56,559 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Price Thresholds View project

All projects View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Detmar Straub on 30 April 2013.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON ENGINEERING MANAGEMENT, VOL. 50, NO. 1, FEBRUARY 2003 31

Culture and Consumer Responses to Web Download


Time: A Four-Continent Study of Mono and
Polychronism
Gregory M. Rose, Roberto Evaristo, and Detmar Straub

Abstract—Most e-commerce sites would like to include as much heavier firm investments in e-commerce (EC) technology [3].
relevant and sales-inducing content on their pages as possible. Culture is undoubtedly a crucial issue in determining how firms
Unfortunately, resulting download delays may lead to consumer will tailor their appeal to e-consumers, and it is a particularly
frustration and a negative attitude toward the product or service
displayed. But is frustration with download delay a universal important factor in situations where technological limitations
problem or is it culture-specific? How should firms view this are impeding the growth of the Web.
problem? These are the primary research questions investigated What are the key technological challenges that firms will have
in the current study. An experiment conducted on four continents to address in order to fully exploit the inherent advantages of the
was selected as an appropriate research method to answer those Web channel, especially with respect to cultural issues? One of
questions. Country sites were selected for differing cultural senses
of time and how this might affect individual responses to down- the most important is download delay [4], [5]. There are good
load delay. Our sample included the U.S. and Finnish cultures reasons to believe that the wait time that customers face when re-
representing monochronic cultures and Egyptian and Peruvian questing a web page from a server is one of the most serious im-
cultures representing polychronic cultures. Consistent with the pediments to rapid development of online business [4]. In fact,
proposed hypotheses, subjects from polychronic cultures were Khosrowpour and Herman’s Delphi study identified download
significantly less concerned with download delays than subjects
in monochronic cultures. Similarly, perceived wait times varied time as the single greatest problem for “the overall utilization
significantly between the mono- and polychronic groups. Practical and management of Web-enabled technologies” [5, p. 1].
insights derived from this study enable specific suggestions on What are the crucial decision variables for managers? Which
customization of web page content richness as well as infrastruc- technological choices should be applied to result in faster down-
ture requirements based on the cultural identity of the intended loads for their customers? How might both of these vary by cul-
e-Consumer. Moreover, results suggest theoretical implications for
future research. ture?
First, firms have little to no control over the technology
Index Terms—Culture, e-commerce (EC), download delay, through which a consumer accesses the firm’s websites.
information systems (IS), information technology (IT), perceived
wait time (PWT), web and online businesses. Narrow-band Internet connections on the consumer side mean
that files sent by e-retailers cannot be downloaded quickly
even if they arrive at the client end of the infrastructure in a
I. INTRODUCTION timely fashion. This so-called client-side bottleneck cannot
be eliminated by content providers and may only be resolved
T HE ABILITY to effectively address the needs of cus-
tomers through web technology has begun to differentiate
service industries in the last decade [1]. Since many services
over time as broadband penetrates the world’s markets [6],
[7]. Other limitations include the geographical location of the
have historically provided poor “service,” the Web offers one server relative to the client, and even how aggressively content
more way that forward-looking firms can expand their set of is cached on the consumer’s machine or on an intervening
available channels and further outdistance competitors. Within proxy server.
a relatively short period of time, this electronic channel has Yet, managers currently do have some power to combat this
changed from a novelty to a mainstream tactic for firms [2]. It impediment. Specifically, the firm’s server-side technology and
seems inevitable that increasingly busy consumers pressured content is under their control. While sizable investments in
by time will opt for convenient technology-based solutions, industrial-strength servers, better compression algorithms, and
and that such customer demand, therefore, will require even server-side software can reduce the delay in sending content
out to the consumer, server speed alone cannot eliminate delay.
All other factors being the same, the more effective approach is
Manuscript received September 15, 2001; revised April 15, 2002. Review of reducing the amount of content on a web page. Lean content,
this manuscript was arranged by Special Issue Editors M. G. Martinsons and R. including smaller (or nonexistent) multimedia and applet files,
Davison. is the only way to reliably provide faster downloads through
G. M. Rose is with the College of Business and Electronics, Washington State
University, Vancouver, WA 98686 USA (e-mail: roseg@vancouver.wsu.edu). narrow-band, client-side connections. Reducing delay, however,
R. Evaristo is with the Information and Decision Sciences Department, Uni- has serious repercussions, as desired content is sacrificed.
versity of Illinois, Chicago, Chicago, IL 60607 USA (e-mail: evaristo@uic.edu). Is the only solution for managers to limit client-side delay
D. Straub is with the Computer Information Systems Department, Georgia
State University, Atlanta, GA 30303 USA (e-mail: dstraub@gsu.edu). to “dummy-down” their web pages to the lowest common
Digital Object Identifier 10.1109/TEM.2002.808262 denominator of acceptable download delay? When is lean
0018-9391/03$17.00 © 2003 IEEE

Authorized licensed use limited to: IEEE Xplore. Downloaded on February 24, 2009 at 09:26 from IEEE Xplore. Restrictions apply.
32 IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON ENGINEERING MANAGEMENT, VOL. 50, NO. 1, FEBRUARY 2003

content a sensible strategy? Exactly how much of a capital a product when the delivery of information is much slower than
investment needs to be made in rapid-response servers? Is e-customers expect?
millisecond response acceptable or does it have to be timed For an e-consumer, delays in downloading Web pages may
in nanoseconds? be due either to: 1) client processing limitations or 2) band-
One perspective on this question is to examine the current width limitations on the client-side; 3) intermediary infrastruc-
online infrastructure, especially on the client side. At the ture slow downs; and/or 4) server-side limitations. Although the
present time, even advanced technology countries like the U.S. firm can deal with server-side issues, the other three limitations
and those in Northern Europe can be characterized as having a are largely beyond its control. Particularly troublesome is the
“slow” residential and small business infrastructure. Roughly bandwidth used to connect to websites since most e-consumers
90%–95% of users in 2001 in these areas were connected to are equipped with very slow connections.
the Internet via narrow-band connections with throughput rates Historically, increased response time has been perceived neg-
of 56 kb/s or less through dial-up modems [8], [9]. Further, atively in American studies of systems users [16], [17]. One can
slow connections are even more characteristic of emerging argue that the impact of the download time of web page con-
economies [8], [9]. tent is analogous. Indeed, recent research has shown that in the
Given that slow connections are now commonplace and likely U.S., download time has a negative impact on e-consumer web
to persist for some time to come, [6], [7], [10] are there any in- page and brand attitude formation, [14] can lead to aborted page
dications about how to select a reasonable level of investment loads, [12], and can reduce user satisfaction, perceived ease of
based on client predilections? Anecdotes in the practitioner lit- use, and intentions to use web applications [13]. These find-
erature indicate that customers are frustrated when pages take a ings likely offer some explanation for why more than 65% of
long time to download [11]. Empirical work has also uncovered e-shoppers abandon a sale before check out [18]. Moreover,
a relationship between delays and negative reactions such as: other studies suggest that negative attitudes are subsequently
increased intentions to abort EC web pages before they finish transferred to objects associated with such delay leading to dis-
loading [12]; more negative user satisfaction, perceived ease of satisfaction with such objects as a software application [16] or
use, and intentions to use Web applications [13]; and more neg- a person’s job [17]. To address such negative attitudes, many
ative attitudes toward brands included in a web page associated e-tailers have eliminated bandwidth-hungry content in order to
with the delays [14]. However, such studies have only begun reduce download times [19].
to provide insight into exactly where the problems lie and with However, studies-to-date (reported previously) were con-
which customer base. ducted only in the U.S. Therefore, as a set, they raise the
It is very likely, for instance, that certain groups of consumers possibility that non-U.S. e-consumers may have different
will not react as adversely as others. From an international per- perceptions regarding download time. This is a nontrivial issue
spective, culture has been found to play a key role in determining in that EC is increasingly becoming a global phenomenon,
what individuals perceive to be a “long” or a “short” time [15]. with Internet access being equally divided between English and
Are the download delays reported in the practitioner press of non-English speakers [20]. Considering the relatively small
the U.S. and European countries an equally serious problem in penetration in countries like China and, therefore, the potential
countries where “time” is not so inextricably associated with for explosive growth, this ratio is likely to be tipped even fur-
money? ther toward non-English websites. In fact, Wood [21] suggests
The research question explored in the current cross-cultural that Chinese will become the number one web language by
study is whether differing views of time have an impact on cus- 2007, a notion that was even featured recently in an Accenture
tomer reactions to download delay. Our empirical approach in- (formerly Andersen Consulting) media advertisement in the
volved gathering data from four countries located on four con- U.S.
tinents, each having a different cultural attitude toward time. Hofstede [22] has suggested that management theories that
We found strong support for culture as a significant factor in apply to the U.S. may be ethnocentric and not apply well to
perceptions of download delay. Among the managerial implica- other cultures. Indeed, although some models explaining com-
tions of this finding is that culture-specific web pages can be de- puter behavior in U.S. or European countries do apply to general
signed to capitalize on the tolerance of certain cultures to longer PC use in other cultures, [23] other research has been unable to
downloads. This will enable managers to target their message to so characterize such technology transfer between nations [24].
these groups through richer content. Likewise, results indicate Similarly, there is support for the notion of culture-specific soft-
that, depending on the targeted end user, firms can spend less on ware. For example, software in China provides a culture-specific
server technologies and still be viable e-service providers. graphical user interface to Microsoft applications [2]. However,
research has also found that some cultural differences lead to
identical outcomes, albeit for different reasons. Specifically, dif-
II. LITERATURE REVIEW AND HYPOTHESES fering motivations have been identified for the use of identical
software applications across cultural groups [25]. Therefore, it
One of the motivations behind the EC phenomenon is access seems reasonable to view contradictory findings to date as evi-
to an even larger amount of information in a shorter time than is dence that culture in-and-of-itself may not be the only, or even
possible in a traditional bricks-and-mortar setting. The conun- the decisive factor in explaining all systems outcomes. What this
drum is that the very same promise that makes EC appealing, means is that, while culture may suppress or strengthen negative
i.e., more in less time, can create severe negative perceptions of responses, it will not eliminate them.

Authorized licensed use limited to: IEEE Xplore. Downloaded on February 24, 2009 at 09:26 from IEEE Xplore. Restrictions apply.
ROSE et al.: CULTURE AND CONSUMER RESPONSES TO WEB DOWNLOAD TIME 33

While negative impacts of delay on attitudes toward delay to multitasking). Therefore, delays in a main task may be
are found outside of web-based services and systems [26]–[28], potentially played down.
this relationship needs to be validated in EC environments. In a manner similar to Hall, Nydell [35] observes these differ-
Likewise, the work to date has not validated this relationship ences in chronism between polychronic Arabs and monochronic
within polychronic cultures. However, since studies investigating “Westerners”
task delays have found attitude toward delay is consistently “…among Arabs, time is not as fixed and rigidly seg-
negative both in computer systems and services, [26]–[28] we mented as it tends to be among Westerners. It flows from
posit that: past to present to future, and Arabs flow with it. Social oc-
: Increases in download delay lead to a negative impact casions and even appointments need not have fixed begin-
on attitudes toward delay, irrespective of culture. ning or endings. Arabs are thus much more relaxed about
the timing of events than they are about other aspects of
A. Cultural Sense of Time their lives [35, p. 27].
In sum, it appears that different regions of the world can be clas-
One key cultural variable related to download delay is how sified as being predominantly poly- or monochronic in culture.
people perceive time. According to Hall, [29] perceptions of Further, these cultural differences correspond with a predictably
time in different cultures are either monochronic or polychronic. more relaxed (polychronic) or less relaxed (monochronic) rela-
Cultures that perceive events as unfolding in a linear fashion are tionship with delays. Based on this line of reasoning, we posit
said to be monochronic, a perspective common in most Euro- that:
pean/North American cultures. Monochronic cultures tend to be : Effect of download delay on attitude toward down-
task oriented; they value promptness and typically do not change load delay is less pronounced with polychronics than with
plans at the last minute [30]. Members of such cultures work on monochronics.
one issue at a time, and, therefore, delays in one task imply that Differences between mono- and polychronic cultures should
others are, per force, delayed. not be limited to delay attitudes. Research shows that per-
“Monochronic people, like Scandinavians and North ceived wait time (PWT) is subjective and varies depending
Americans, prefer to do one thing at a time. For instance, on circumstances, attitudes, and environment [26], [36], [37].
they do not book several meetings at the same time. Time While no studies to date have measured differences in PWT
is compartmentalized: there’s time for everything and between mono- and polychronic cultures, it seems likely that
everything has its time [31, p. 89].” perceptions of passage of time would vary by culture. If cultural
Corroborating this, Hall [32, p. 6] suggests that “Americans differences exist with regards to the perceived passage of time,
think of time as a road [that] has segments or compartments evidence of cultural differences in attitude toward delay (as
that are to be kept discrete.” Similarly, northern European Fin- posited in ) would be further validated as well. Likewise,
land “is an extremely monochronic time-oriented culture” [33, this finding would be very important to systems developers
p. 151]. as many applications are designed expressly with reduction
On the other hand, events in polychronic cultures are not per- in PWT in mind [38]. Cultural differences in perceived delay
ceived as being tightly coupled to antecedents; they simply tran- would suggest new research into the predictors and outcome
spire. Examples of polychronic cultures include Latin American of PWT is needed for systems built for users of different
and Middle Eastern countries [15, p. 17] as well as Southern Eu- cultural chronisms.
ropean cultures [15, p. 150]. Although events to polychronics The ways individuals in cultural groups tackle tasks and tol-
unfold in complex ways, these events are seen as occurring erate task-induced delays suggest that they may perceive wait
mostly in parallel. Hall [34] suggests that polychronic groups time duration differently. Similarly, relationships with time vary
tend to be involved in several tasks and processes concurrently, significantly based on cultural chronism. Our last hypothesis is
like jugglers. This may be due to the high extent of personal in- a logical extension of these concepts:
volvement within social groups. Tella [31] suggests that: : Monochronics experience different levels of per-
ceived delay time than polychronics.
“…polychronic people[s], represented by South Ameri-
No specific directional expectation was developed for this hy-
cans or Southern Europeans, tend to do several things at the
pothesis on PWT. The aforementioned literature finds that ac-
same time or in parallel time segments. They do not think
tual delay has been shown to predict perceived delay, but not
it odd to answer their phones while having a videoconfer-
the predicted direction. Measured PWTs have been found to be
ence with foreign partners, or talk to a passing student by
greater than actual treatment times in some experimental groups
leaving the other videoconference partners to simply wait”
and less than actual delay in others (see [26], [36], [39] for ex-
[31, p. 89].
amples). Therefore, we limit our hypothesis to one of differ-
In fact, polychronic peoples are more likely to change plans
ences between cultures.
and focus on relationships rather than tasks [30]. Some of
the implications that may be gleaned, therefore, are that for
polychronic peoples, this perception of nonlinearity of time III. METHODOLOGY
implies less urgency. This may be the case partly because one
could turn his or her attention from a main task to another Consistent with nearly all prior delay and computer response
task being performed in a rough block of time (analogous time studies, laboratory experiments were conducted to test the

Authorized licensed use limited to: IEEE Xplore. Downloaded on February 24, 2009 at 09:26 from IEEE Xplore. Restrictions apply.
34 IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON ENGINEERING MANAGEMENT, VOL. 50, NO. 1, FEBRUARY 2003

Fig. 1. Sample screen shot of the browser artifact.

research hypotheses. Experiments have traditionally been used mock browser did not tax the performance of a basic test ma-
to test for impacts of delay in service settings, [26], [40] tra- chine. In the prior studies, [12], [41], [43] browser artifacts were
ditional computer systems; [17], [28] and web-based systems loaded onto university computer lab machines. As in Rose, et al.
[12], [13], [41]. Experiments are also appropriate in download [12], Rose and Straub [41], and Rose and Straub [43], the current
delay impact studies because the hypotheses imply a causal study was conducted with students enrolled in university infor-
model [42]. mation systems (IS) courses (third and fourth year undergradu-
For the experiment, validated artifacts, measures, and exper- ates, and masters students, including M.B.A. students) in com-
imental procedures were faithfully adopted from Rose, et al. puter labs. These groups were appropriate because they were
[12], Rose and Straub, [41], and Rose and Straub [43]. For de- technically savvy and relatively young adults (average age of
tailed information on the validation process, please see these participants was 26 years). This profile is consistent with e-con-
papers. In these studies, a mock web browser controlled the en- sumers as a whole [44]. Moreover, student subjects have been
tire experience of the subject. The browser replicated the look shown to predict results similar to “general” consumers [45] in
and feel of Microsoft’s Internet Explorer (as a screen shot of situations like those being investigated here.
the artifact in Fig. 1 shows). Although totally self-contained lo- The repeated measures design in this study copes with the
cally on an individual PC (i.e., not on the Internet or on a local high cost in time and effort of setting up and conducting the
area network), the mock browser appears to be retrieving and experiment and the relative scarcity of appropriate subjects. In
loading web pages from the Internet while, in fact, it is actu- spite of the fact that these problems were exacerbated when the
ally simulating and controlling the download times. Based on study was conducted in universities other than the U.S. institu-
observations and pilot experiments, the artifact is built to sim- tions where the researchers teach, this repeated measures design
ulate realistic variations of delay times and associated cues in allowed the capture of sufficient data for comparison across the
the browser in order to increase the sense of realism. Exit inter- four countries.
views with over 100 test subjects in a variety of settings confirm The browser artifact that allowed subjects to navigate through
that the mock browser and web pages appeared to be authentic four different EC web pages presented multiple page views.
to test subjects who believed they were examining actual web The pages represented mock e-retailers selling a variety of
pages on the Internet.1 camera films and VHS videotapes and supplies, with actual
Conducting the experiment required a lab of computers, with web pages and brand cue data adopted as templates for the
one PC for each subject. Although the hardware was different experimental pages. Each page varied in trivial ways from other
across the different sites, this created no noticeable differences pages so as not to arouse suspicion [46]. Based on pilot studies
in how the subjects viewed the software since, in pretests, the documented in [47] it was determined that as many as five
treatment pages could be observed (the first four pages shown
1To ensure that the interface as perceived across cultures was not a confound, were each unique and the fifth was a repeat) without making
we looked at typical catalog applications in many countries and discovered that subjects wary about the reality of the experience. Likewise,
the interface chosen is a format that existed in most countries. The design was with the instrument items being limited in number, the study
simple and functional, and, therefore, avoided graphics preferences of one type
or another. Moreover, the questions asked to test the hypotheses were not related could be conducted using five page views without excessive
to attitude toward the interface, but to functionality. subject fatigue.

Authorized licensed use limited to: IEEE Xplore. Downloaded on February 24, 2009 at 09:26 from IEEE Xplore. Restrictions apply.
ROSE et al.: CULTURE AND CONSUMER RESPONSES TO WEB DOWNLOAD TIME 35

Fig. 2. Browser artifact showing status bar change.

Four international locations were selected to run the ex- were set at 15-s increments, for a total of six possible treatment
periments. The criteria for choice of location were twofold: levels. As noted, subjects were exposed to five page views and
First, we wanted to compare monochronic and polychronic each of the five pages had a different delay treatment level. To
cultures. Second, we wanted to ensure that the results were increase the reality of the treatment, the status bar showed the
not otherwise culturally-idiosyncratic, and, thus, selected two loading time as well (as seen in Fig. 2). In that treatment, delay
countries for each of these views of time. In addition to the levels were randomly assigned to subjects, the study demon-
monochronic culture of the U.S., we conducted an experiment strates strong internal validity.
in Finland, which is also a monochronic country.2 For poly- Subjects were given an instruction and questionnaire packet.
chronism, we selected Egypt and Peru. The classification of The packet contained an overview of the study, an opt-out
monochronic versus polychronic nations was based on Hall clause, and a login code for the browser (Appendix A and B
[15], [32]. Thus, we were able to compare results across four include excerpts from the packet). The login code determined
countries and four continents. the experimental treatment to which the subject was exposed.
For these cultures, we did not expect other national differ- Instructions indicated how the users should navigate the web
ences like lack of economic development to affect results in that browser as well as how subjects should let each page load
the subjects were drawn from a population base that was not af- completely before answering post-test instrument items. After
fected in this experiment by that characteristic.3 In other words, viewing an individual web page, subjects were instructed to
all subjects were similar with respect to economic and working answer questions related to that page.
status and access to computers. They differ, if the literature is All subjects were enrolled in courses conducted in English.
correct, on chronism, which is why we manipulated download Written materials were in English, and, without exception, com-
delay to test our predictions about their responses. prehension of written English ranged from good to very good.
Identical experiments were conducted in each culture. All For these reasons, the mock browser was not translated, but kept
subjects were enrolled in an IS course. Personal computers at in English in all four countries. In fact, the browser included
the universities all used an identical experimental artifact, and common and simple terms that subjects would come across fre-
the experimental software randomly assigned delay times and quently in their forays on the Web. The questionnaire was also
treatment pages. Treatment delays ranged from 15 to 90 s and kept in English for the Finnish and Egyptian samples because
of their level of familiarity with sophisticated English texts.4
2It is clear that countries do not always equate with “culture.” Nevertheless,
The first item was for attitude toward delay, adopted from
we used this as a rough approximation in lieu of more definitive work
on how to specify an individual’s “culture.” prior download time studies [12], [41], [43]. This measure was
3Research colleagues in Finland, Peru, and Egypt helped us assess possible a variation of the one adopted by Hui and Tse [48]. Attitude
confounds such as number of disconnected internet calls, actual speed, etc. In- toward delay was ranked on a four-point scale: from “not
ternet access in Finland was similar to that of the U.S., if anything the Internet
was more available to the subjects. In Peru, graduate students paid $20 000 4It needs to be noted that in Peru, instruction was accompanied by simulta-
(USD) annual fees to the Business School in a country where per capita income neous translation from English to Spanish, which was used by perhaps one third
is around $2400 and, therefore, the subjects had internet access speeds and so- of the class. Similarly, some of the more complex teaching materials were trans-
lutions not necessarily available to the rest of the population. This would make lated to Spanish to ensure that even the small percentage of students who were
their situation more comparable to their counterpart shoppers in monochronic not completely fluent in English would have no problems with comprehension.
cultures, since actual access speed or other internet access characteristics appear For this reason, we decided to have the questionnaire translated to Spanish (see
not to be that different. In Egypt, subjects were working adults who had access Appendix B), something done in Peru by bilingual native speakers of Spanish.
to relatively high speed lines at work and in the university laboratories so the The Spanish version was then back translated into English by one of the authors,
experience of the subjects was unlikely to be a factor in their responses. and final adjustments made.

Authorized licensed use limited to: IEEE Xplore. Downloaded on February 24, 2009 at 09:26 from IEEE Xplore. Restrictions apply.
36 IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON ENGINEERING MANAGEMENT, VOL. 50, NO. 1, FEBRUARY 2003

TABLE I TABLE III


OVERVIEW OF FINDINGS REGRESSION MODEL SUMMARY: TEST OF IMPACTS OF DOWNLOAD DELAY AND
CULTURAL CHRONISM ON DELAY ATTITUDE

be Web savvy. Demographic differences were not significant in


TABLE II the model tests detailed. 6
DESCRIPTIVE STATISTICS
V. HYPOTHESIS TESTS
The critical question in our study, of course, was whether
there would be statistically significant variations in the attitude
toward download delay across all cultures considered sepa-
rately. To test this, chronism (dummy coded as 0,1) and actual
download delay (see Table III) were regressed on attitude
toward download delay. As Table II shows, both variables were
significant ( ). The negative value for the download
delay suggests that the longer the download delay, the more
negative the subjects’ attitudes, holding chronism constant.
This provides strong support for .
significant delay,” to “acceptable delay,” to “excessive but still The significance ( ) of cultural chronism implies
tolerable delay,” to “intolerable delay.” This measure of delay that is also supported, and there are, indeed, cultural differ-
attitude has been shown to predict e-consumer intentions to ences (as represented in this case by chronism) on attitude to-
abort EC web page loads, a very serious concern for e-retailers ward delay. Since cultural chronism was coded as a dummy vari-
[12]. able (“1” for polychronism, “0” for monochronism), the posi-
The second construct was PWT. Subjects were asked to in- tive regression beta indicates that polychronic subjects have a
dicate how long in seconds they perceived the download time more positive attitude toward delay than monochronic subjects,
delay to be for the page they viewed. An additional measure of as predicted by .
attitude toward the web graphics was interleaved with attitude Table IV presents the ANOVA run used to test . PWT
toward delay and PWT questions to counteract methods bias, was found to be significantly different between monochronic
and hopefully avoid fatigue and hypothesis-guessing [49]. and polychronic cultures ( ). This very
interesting finding called for further analysis to see where the
IV. DATA ANALYSIS exact differences lie. Therefore, we examined actual delay and
PWTs individually, for all four cultures (Table V).
A. Overview and Demographic Data As can be seen in Table V, PWTs did, indeed, vary among
Results of the data analyses, detailed in the following, are the cultural experimental groups. Likewise, these differences
summarized in Table I. appear to vary greatly between, but very little within the cul-
Sample sizes 5 and demographic information, including home tural chronic groups. Monochronic cultures had average PWTs
Internet connection speeds, are shown in Table II. As can be of 37.88 and 35.45 for the U.S. and Finland, respectively. In the
seen, subjects are fairly similar in age and all groups appear to polychronic groups, the times were 47.53 and 45.96 for Egypt
and Peru.
The average actual delay treatment for all four sample groups
5Sample sizes were appropriate for conducting multivariate regression anal- was 52.39 s. These results indicate that all subject groups un-
ysis and ANOVA tests. For regression analysis, sample sizes should be 15–20 derestimated actual download delay, but monochronic culture
times the number of independent variables [50]. In this study, the sample size groups underestimated by 14.05 s on average for the Americans
of 595 would allow for up to 40 independent variables to be included and still
allow for generalizability. The tests conducted here had no more than 14 inde-
pendent variables when all of the demographic questions were included. The
primary tests included only two independent variables each and would have re- 6One difference between groups appears to be in home Internet connection
quired a sample of only 30 for generalizability. Similarly, ANOVA tests should speed. To see if this variable could be confounding results, it was included in a
ideally be run with samples larger than 60 for tests of this type to have appro- separate set of regression runs. In each case, home connection speed had no sig-
priate power levels [50]. The assumptions of the techniques employed were also nificant relationship with the outcome variable (p-values for connection speed
met or can be ascertained to be high in statistical conclusion validity. Regarding were in excess of 0.27 in the tests). Likewise, demographic variables as control
multicollinearity, tests of the independent variables download delay and cultural variables were examined in separate runs. While number of Internet hours was
chronism demonstrate the factors are not significantly correlated (VIF scores significant at the 0.05 level, it did not change the significance levels of the pri-
= 1.0, Pearson p-values = 0 824
: ). These findings were expected because the mary variables of interest. Further, the adjusted R values for the models varied
delay treatment was randomly generated and assigned. Per assumptions of equal + 0
only by = 0.01. Similar results were found for the attitude toward graphics
cell sizes and homogeneity of variances, multivariate analyses have been shown measure. For reasons of parsimony and lack of theoretical grounding, these vari-
to be robust to violations of these assumptions [51]. ables were omitted from the final reporting of results.

Authorized licensed use limited to: IEEE Xplore. Downloaded on February 24, 2009 at 09:26 from IEEE Xplore. Restrictions apply.
ROSE et al.: CULTURE AND CONSUMER RESPONSES TO WEB DOWNLOAD TIME 37

TABLE IV
ANOVA MODEL SUMMARY: DIFFERENCES IN PERCEIVED WAIT TIME BETWEEN MONOCHRONIC AND POLYCHRONIC CULTURES

Predictor: CULTURAL CHRONISM


Dependent Variable: PERCEIVED WAIT TIME
3 Indicates Significance at the .05 Level

TABLE V TABLE VI
DESCRIPTIVE STATISTICS OF AVERAGE ACTUAL DELAY AND PERCEIVED REGRESSION MODEL SUMMARY FOR COMBINED EXPERIMENTS:
DELAY IN ALL FOUR CULTURES DIFFERENCES IN PERCEIVED WAIT TIME BETWEEN MONOCHRONIC AND
POLYCHRONIC CULTURES

Dependent Variable: PERCEIVED WAIT TIME


Overall F = 142:054 Adjusted R = :330 Total df: 574
Overall Regression p-value = :000
Indicates Significance at the .05 Level
and 17.19 s for the Finns. By contrast, Egyptians only underes-
timated the actual delay by 4.18 s and Peruvians by 7.25 s.
These facts suggested that post hoc analysis would be tests of reported modem speed home connection speeds on
useful for further insights into the differences in PWT between both dependent variables (PWT, ; delay attitude,
monochronic and polychronic cultures. Table VI presents the ) found their influence to be insignificant, while the
regression results. Again, both predictor variables were signifi- significance of download delay and cultural chronism were
cant ( ). This significant finding for cultural chronism still significant ( for each). This result strengthens our
suggests that in polychronic cultures there is a perception of argument that cultural differences, rather than other behavioral
longer wait times than in monochronic cultures, in spite of or experiential characteristics of a given group, affect their
experimentally identical actual delays. attitude or perception of delay in download times.
What does this mean? Subjects from polychronic cultures be- While there are differences between cultures, within-chro-
lieved that they waited longer than their monochronic counter- nism groups’ PWTs do not appear to vary significantly. An
parts did, even though both groups waited the same amount of ANOVA test of differences between the U.S. and Finnish
time. Yet, considering the previous hypothesis tests, polychronic groups found no difference in PWT for the two chronic groups
subjects also held a more positive attitude toward delay com- ( -value ) and an ANOVA between Egyptian and
pared to members of the monochronic groups. In sum, it appears Peruvian subjects likewise found no significant differences
that polychronics feel better about waiting and have more pos- ( -value ). It appears that within-chronism groups
itive attitudes toward the delay even though they believe they passage of time is viewed in a similar fashion. However, as
have waited significantly longer than those in the monochronic noted earlier, there is a significant difference between attitudes
group have. toward delay for these monochronic and polychronic groups
(see Table III). In sum, these findings lend additional evidence
VI. TESTING OF POTENTIAL CONFOUNDS to the theoretical proposition that cultural differences impact
attitude toward download time but that these differences
To be assured that potential confounds such as user expecta-
are minimal within cultures similar in chronism, even when
tions are not affecting results, we examined these statistically. It
members of these cultures are separated by thousands of miles.
was important to account for the possibility that instead of in-
herent cultural differences, monochronic users differ from poly-
chronics with respect to acceptance of response times because VII. IMPLICATIONS FOR PRACTICE, THEORY, AND FUTURE
RESEARCH DIRECTIONS
they are accustomed to faster Internet connectivity speeds.
To test for the impacts of expectations of delay, analyses of Results of this research have several important implications,
differences in home Internet connection speeds were conducted. for both theory and practice. The four primary findings and as-
The logic behind these tests was that people accustomed to sociated suggestions are summarized in Table VII. Naturally,
routinely waiting longer (i.e., if their home modem speeds these possible interpretations are the result of a single study and
are slower) might have better attitudes toward waiting than as such will need to be supported with future work.
those who are accustomed to waiting less for page loads (i.e., First, it is clear that our basic finding about cultural influence
those with faster home Internet connection speeds). Regression on IT/web usage agrees with the limited prior work in this area.

Authorized licensed use limited to: IEEE Xplore. Downloaded on February 24, 2009 at 09:26 from IEEE Xplore. Restrictions apply.
38 IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON ENGINEERING MANAGEMENT, VOL. 50, NO. 1, FEBRUARY 2003

TABLE VII
SUMMARY OF PRIMARY FINDINGS AND SUGGESTIONS

Indeed, the current study suggests that there are good reasons portant because it validates this relationship within the world of
to differentially customize content for cultures. Although there web-based systems and extends prior monochronic findings to
is a robust literature on how interface factors such as language, polychronic cultures. However, there are differences as to how
currency, colors, and other potential faux pas [52] need to be much actual delay affects attitudes toward delay among different
fitted to culture, we believe that the present study is able to take cultures, and we will draw implications from this finding, some
such recommendations one step further. In fact, because some of which are counterintuitive to some extent with respect to the
more intuitively obvious customizations have already been im- extant literature.
plemented across the world (e.g., see Davis and Grimes [53]), Polychronic cultures are typically more comfortable than
our results may suggest critical yet-to-be-enacted changes in monochronic cultures in performing more than one task or
commercial web approaches as well as spark further research process in a given time block. We found, as expected, that this
to refine the recommendations. particular response of polychronics results in a lesser concern
First, it is clear that our basic finding about cultural influence with longer download times than among the monochronic
on IT/web usage agrees with the limited prior work in this area. groups. What we did not predict was an interesting side finding.
Indeed, the current study suggests that there are good reasons Polychronic people PWT to be considerably longer than the
to differentially customize content for cultures. Although there monochronic group.
is a robust literature on how interface factors such as language, This outcome is especially interesting because people in the
currency, colors, and other potential faux pas [52] need to be experimental treatment were not as free as they would have been
fitted to culture, we believe that the present study is able to take in their natural settings to be involved in other physical tasks
such recommendations one step further. In fact, because some during the same block of time. Yet, even when unable to freely
more intuitively obvious customizations have already been im- multitask, polychronic subjects still reported significantly lower
plemented across the world (e.g., see Davis and Grimes [53]), concern about longer downloads (with even longer PWTs) than
our results may suggest critical yet-to-be-enacted changes in monochronics. This finding may point to an even more basic
commercial web approaches as well as spark further research structural issue. Relatively speaking, regardless of how many
to refine the recommendations. other tasks polychronics are involved in at a particular moment,
Armed with knowledge of this study, what should managers it appears that they more willingly accept long downloads. In
in charge of websites and web designers do with respect to other words, they seem to have been “trained” by their culture
cultural chronism and downloading? Specific implications for to tolerate longer waits, so even in the absence of alternate ways
customization can be derived from how people perceive delays to use their time, delays are less of a nuisance than they are for
in download time as well as their attitude toward such delay. monochronic groups.
As predicted, we found that, irrespective of culture, all peoples Future research may ascertain whether, in the presence of
would like to have download delays reduced. This finding is im- other tasks, polychronic peoples may be even less resistant to

Authorized licensed use limited to: IEEE Xplore. Downloaded on February 24, 2009 at 09:26 from IEEE Xplore. Restrictions apply.
ROSE et al.: CULTURE AND CONSUMER RESPONSES TO WEB DOWNLOAD TIME 39

longer downloads and ascertain whether outcomes from delay but English-speaking cultures (e.g., black South Africans). So,
are even less prominent when polychronics are free to multitask it would need to be combined with top-level domains. Likewise,
naturally. If polychronics viewing web pages outside of a labo- minority situations like native English-speaking polychronic
ratory setting are indeed less negatively impacted by delay, then Peruvians would not be covered. Future research could identify
even more desirable content can be included in pages for these the relevancy and profitability of catering to overtly minority
groups than is currently indicated by the present analyses. Such groups and would need to find whether language combined
analysis should include monochronics in their natural environ- with national domain is indeed a stable culture identifier with
ment to allow them to multitask as well. Furthermore, it may be respect to polychronism or monochronism.
interesting to ask subjects their perceptions on waiting. A separate issue arises with localized EC sites. In principle,
The ultimate design objective is to adjust extent of content they would have the benefit of appropriate knowledge and
to the needs and wants of specific cultures, even if that implies would build a site culturally well suited to the needs of intended
offering a much larger content set (pictures, video, sound, etc.) customers. Unfortunately, though, such sites are sometimes
with their associated longer delays. People may still choose to influenced by or modeled after English-language sites. As
use or not to use such content, but longer download times nec- a result, even in polychronic cultures, such sites may have
essarily associated with richer content would be less likely to embedded monochronic principles. Therefore, even bringing
generate negative reactions from polychronic users. Future re- the distinctions discovered in this paper to the attention of Web
search must be conducted to understand how best to utilize the designers could have considerable practical implications. Other
additional degrees of freedom developers have when creating studies could add to what we now tentatively believe about
systems for polychronic users. The data here offer no specific content-richness and download time. It would be interesting to
guidelines as to what delay-increasing characteristics should be investigate, for example, the proportion of locally built sites
added back to applications for a given polychronic audience. that “fit” their local culture, and whether that leads to higher
Our study does suggest that when building systems for poly- site approval ratings and sales.
chronics, there are additional degrees of freedom available to Another concept dealing with localization concerns the ex-
increase wait times. Future research can identify what should be tent of appropriate investment of e-business resources. For poly-
done with these extra degrees of freedom and in what ways de- chronics, it appears that fewer resources need to be spent for fast
velopers should exchange greater delays for functionality when servers on the server side, expensive broadband connections,
building systems for polychronics. At a theoretical level, this and responsive applications. In brief, in order to have an ac-
endeavor would enhance knowledge in contributing disciplines ceptably responsive site, pages for polychronic cultures require
such as computer science and human computer interface studies. smaller investments. The implication of this lower cost barrier is
In order to take full advantage of these findings, a multina- that limited-resources organizations selling in polychronic cul-
tional company must be able to identify the culture to which tures may still be able to afford a viable web presence. This lim-
the customer belongs so that a given site offered may be ap- itation on cost structures may be critical for organizations in de-
propriately customized. An initial approach is to associate web veloping countries where income levels are low and bandwidth
presentation language with cultural chronism. In this way, Eng- and computer equipment costs are higher than in the developed
lish content would assume monochronism, whereas romance world. Generally accepted e-business notions in the U.S. about
languages could be geared to polychronic audience. Although needing the fastest servers connected to fat Internet pipes do not
crude, it may provide a higher level of fit than is current, and appear to apply equally to polychronic cultures.
firms can go beyond this algorithm to offer an alternative to While these findings indicate that polychronics are more tol-
people who come to their site. erant of delay than monochronics, managers and software de-
Currently, some firms like Amazon.com have completely sep- signers should be careful when interpreting these results. These
arate websites for different countries, with language, prices and findings do not imply that polychronics should be punished with
even functionality appropriately chosen. Similarly, others such unnecessary delay. Outcomes of this experiment allow us to
as AOL.com offer different default languages and pricing based better understand the relative cost of this impediment within
on originating internet provider (IP) address, but the defaults each culture so that a culturally sensitive cost/benefit analysis
can be overridden. These are valid solutions. It may be that a can be conducted in the design or implementation of applica-
combined approach would work best, addressing even the situ- tions. These findings indicate that polychronics simply do not
ation of an American expatriate living, say, in Lima, Peru. Since have the same level of dissatisfaction with increased delay when
computer analysis of the IP address can identify the geograph- compared with monochronics. However, results clearly demon-
ical area from which the request for a web page originates, the strate that neither likes to wait.
downloaded page can be offered in the major local tongue (such In an ideal world, full functionality with near-zero delay
as Spanish, in this example) with an option to display in English. would be optimal across both cultures. However, given that
Since Spanish, a romance language, is associated with poly- web applications routinely require tradeoffs between delay and
chronic cultures, that set of pages could include richer content as other functionality, [19] and that applications are routinely
deemed desirable by the content provider for that target group. built with less functionality than desired, [54] these findings
In contrast, if the customer requests content in English, which indicate that polychronics would more willingly sacrifice speed
typifies many monochronic cultures, content could be reduced for other functionality when compared with monochronic
and a set of content-restricted pages with faster downloads users. In contrast, it appears monochronics would see more
could be served. This solution still does not cover polychronic value in eliminating delay at the expense of other desirable

Authorized licensed use limited to: IEEE Xplore. Downloaded on February 24, 2009 at 09:26 from IEEE Xplore. Restrictions apply.
40 IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON ENGINEERING MANAGEMENT, VOL. 50, NO. 1, FEBRUARY 2003

functionality when compared to their polychronic counterparts. tially nonrepresentative, something to be considered as well in a
In some instances in the developing world, where the cost follow-up study. Ideally, a follow-up study should cross subjects
of entry in providing web-based computing is so high, this from either a less developed monochronic culture, a well-devel-
inherent tolerance for waiting might make it possible to provide oped polychronic culture or a combination of both, with par-
acceptable e-services where identical configurations would ticular care about the representativeness of the sample make-up
be unacceptable in the monochronic world. Therefore, these with regard to the whole population.
findings may open polychronic cultures to more services than In addition to extending this research into other nations
would have been thought possible based on monochronic and cultural groups, future research should investigate cultural
preferences. differences for causes of PWT. These studies might lend
The results of this study also have implications regarding the insight into the ways polychronic and monochronic individuals
validity of the PWT construct for measure of Web application respond to web-based delay and suggest possible design
success. Historically, PWT has been a measure of convenience choices for website developers. Although the browser artifact
for researchers and corporations trying to identify the impacts of did include a status bar as a potential PWT feedback mecha-
delay in services and software. The appeal of this measure is that nism, it is possible that future work will be needed to test the
a consumer or client can be asked for their perception of delay outcomes and antecedents for PWT under varied conditions.
(which is relatively simple to do). Managers or researchers do An additional possibility may be to manipulate displays of
not need to physically capture the actual passage of time (which expected download time to identify variations in the PWT with
can be very difficult to do outside of a controlled laboratory all its implications.
setting). Similarly, additional investigations are needed to understand
While this measure has been deemed acceptable outside of the impacts of PWT when compared to actual delay impacts. Re-
Web applications, it appears that PWT could be a fickle mea- sults from such studies would allow researchers and businesses
sure of delay for web services and may not be an appropriate to better understand which construct or constructs should be
surrogate. The significant but low explained variance between captured to measure and predict web application success. More-
actual and perceived delay in this study ( ) brings over, these impacts and predictors need to differ by culture. In
into question its usefulness as a surrogate. In addition, the sig- fact, future research should also investigate whether differences
nificance ( ) of cultural chronism in Table VI indicates in chronism, delay attitude, and PWT translates into changes on
that PWT is even less appropriate as a substitute for actual delay success variables such as the actual buying decision.
when one tries to generalize across cultures.
These findings support the recommendation in Rose and
VIII. CONCLUSION
Straub [41] that, when testing for delay impacts, PWT should
be used in combination with actual delay (as they seem to This research sheds light on differences in delay attitudes
measure different aspects of waiting). Furthermore, additional and perceptions of web-based delay in monochronic and poly-
delay constructs, such as attribution, [55] should be included. chronic cultures. Results indicate that culture has a major im-
Each measure may lead to different outcomes and call for pact on how e-consumers respond to websites. Managers who
alternate preventive remedies. can tune their websites to general cultural dimensions, like sense
While the experiment conducted here does provide a good of time, will have a freer rein to include rich content for poly-
deal of insight, it does have limitations that suggest future re- chronic cultures. On the other hand, monochronic cultures ex-
search. One of the potential limitations in this study relates to perience problems with long delays in general, and managers
the choice of countries. Although our study compares two poly- need to ensure that server-side delays are minimal and that web
chronic with two monochronic countries and the subjects within pages are lean for fast downloading.
the four groups were very similar in real wealth and access to In the previous section, we discussed several other important
infrastructure, it also happened that the two polychronic nations theoretical and practical implications of our work. In a nutshell,
were developing whereas the two monochronic countries were a major implication of our findings and the likely follow-up
developed. This difference in national development creates an research suggests possible additions to the theoretical body of
alternate explanation for the differences other than culture. The work on attitude toward delay, PWT and their respective im-
counter argument is twofold: 1) that some polychronic cultures pacts on e-retailing and web-based system success as moder-
are, in fact, in economically developed countries (for instance, ated by culture. Furthermore, there are pragmatic implications,
France and Italy), and that 2) in the selected polychronic cul- related not only to acceptable potential differences in EC infra-
tures, the researchers engaged subjects who were economically structure in monochronic versus polychronic cultures but also
advantaged. The Peruvian sample was from an exclusive busi- in the amount and type of content made available to users from
ness school that has above-average income subjects and, there- various cultures.
fore, subjects with better connectivity, almost like a highly de-
veloped neighborhood of Peru (something easily noticeable by
APPENDIX A
high broadband penetration in the sample). In Egypt, the sample
RESEARCH INSTRUMENT (ENGLISH VERSION)
was working adults, the vast majority of whom had reasonable
bandwidth connections to the Internet at work and who had a Thank you for agreeing to participate.
state-of-the-art computer lab at school. Therefore, in both cases Part of this class session will be devoted to a research study.
the sample reflected a small stratum of the population, poten- Results of this research will be used understand attitudes toward

Authorized licensed use limited to: IEEE Xplore. Downloaded on February 24, 2009 at 09:26 from IEEE Xplore. Restrictions apply.
ROSE et al.: CULTURE AND CONSUMER RESPONSES TO WEB DOWNLOAD TIME 41

delay and graphics in Web pages. The activity conducted today (let page download…)
will involve viewing a series of a Web pages and answering For Web Page #1
some related questions. 1. Please indicate your attitude toward the download time
Final results from this research can be obtained by contacting delay for the page you just viewed (choose one):
[name withheld]. Please ask the research administrator for con- [ ] Intolerable delay
tact information if you are interested. [ ] Excessive but still tolerable delay
The total approximate time for participating in this research [ ] Acceptable delay
is 20 minutes. [ ] Not significant delay
Note: Please read the following statements and then sign (Next step: please open this questionnaire to the next
on the line below if you wish to participate. If you have any sheet…)
questions, please ask the research administrator. For Web Page #2
• I have freely volunteered to participate in this research 2. Please indicate how long you perceive the download
• I have been informed in advance what my task(s) will be time delay was for the page you just viewed: ——seconds
and what procedures will be followed Instructions for question #3: Please review the graphics
• I have been given the opportunity to ask questions, and contained in this Web page. Indicate your attitude toward the
have had my questions answered to my satisfaction graphics contained in the page you just viewed by checking
• I am aware that I have the right to withdraw consent and one box of the scale below. The more strongly you identify
to discontinue participation at any time, without prejudice with one anchor word on a scale, the more near that word you
to my future treatment. check a box on that line.
• My signature below may be taken as affirmation of all of 3. Your attitude toward the graphics contained in the Web
the above statements; it was given prior to my participation page you just viewed is:
in this research. Dislike very much -[ ]-[ ]-[ ]-[ ]-[ ]-[ ]-[ ]- Like very much
• I understand that all responses to this questionnaire are (Next step: Please click on the yellow “Home” button in the
strictly confidential; only summary findings will be re- browser to return to the home page.
ported. When that page has finished loading, open this question-
Signature:——— naire to the next sheet…)
(Next Step: Please enter in your login code (below) in the
password box. This will start the special, customized Web II. PERSONAL INFORMATION
browser used for this research.
When done, please open this questionnaire to the next Please answer the following questions. Note that all re-
sheet and follow the directions…) sponses to this questionnaire are strictly confidential; only
LOGIN CODE-[ ] summary findings will be reported.
16. What is your sex? Male [ ] or Female [ ]
I. WEB PAGE EVALUATION 17. What is your marital status? Married [ ] or Not Mar-
ried [ ]
A. Web Page Section Instructions
18. What is your age? —— Years (optional)
The following section asks that you view a series of five Web 19. How many years of post-high school education do
pages and answer some related questions. you have? —— Years
The Web pages contain content referring to brands of video 20. Are you currently employed full-time? Yes [ ] or No
cassettes and photographic film. Included among these are []
brands with which you are likely familiar: Kodak brand film 21. How many years of working experience you have?
and JVC brand video cassettes. Also included are two relatively —— Years
new brands with which you are likely not familiar: FilmTech 22. Are you a [NATION NAME HERE] citizen: Yes [ ]
brand film and SCS brand video cassettes. or No [ ]
In the following section, before answering any questions 23. Do you generally view shopping as a source of plea-
about a specific page, please make sure the Web page you are sure (choose one)? Yes [ ] Neutral [ ] No [ ]
evaluating has completely downloaded. Once the Web page has 24. How many hours in a week do you spend on the
fully downloaded, please proceed as indicated. Internet? —— Hours
(Next step: Please click on the yellow “Home” button in the 25. How often do you make purchases or gather infor-
browser. This will open the home page for this research which mation for shopping over the Internet? —— Per Month
contains hyperlinks to the product pages you are to evaluate. 26. If you have Internet connection at home, please in-
When that page has finished loading, please open this ques- dicate your connection speed: 33.6k or slower [ ] 56k [ ]
tionnaire to the next sheet to begin this study…) High Speed (Cable Modem or DSL, etc.)[ ]
Note: this is only a subset of the research instrument: The
actual instrument contained five iterations of the questions in
APPENDIX B
Section I below
RESEARCH INSTRUMENT (SPANISH VERSION)
(Next step: please go to the first Web page available from the
home page by clicking on the hyperlink for “Web Page #1”. Gracias por participar.

Authorized licensed use limited to: IEEE Xplore. Downloaded on February 24, 2009 at 09:26 from IEEE Xplore. Restrictions apply.
42 IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON ENGINEERING MANAGEMENT, VOL. 50, NO. 1, FEBRUARY 2003

Parte de esta sesión de clase estaŕ dedicada a un estudio de que la página web que usted está evaluando haya bajado del In-
investigación. Los resultados de esta investigación serán uti- ternet por completo. Una vez que la página Web se haya descar-
lizados para comprender las actitudes hacia el retraso en la pre- gado por completo, por favor proceda según se indica.
sentación de gráficos en las páginas Web. La actividad a realizar (Siguiente paso: Por favor haga un click en el botón amar-
el día de hoy incluirá el ver una serie de páginas Web y luego se illo “Home” (“Inicio”) del navegador (browser). Esto abrirá
procederá a contestar algunas preguntas relacionadas a éstas. la página web de inicio para esta investigación la cual con-
Los resultados finales de esta investigación se pueden obtener tiene hiperenlaces a las páginas de los productos que usted va
contactando a [name withheld]. Por favor, solicite a la persona a evaluar.
que administra la investigación información si es que usted está Cuando esa página haya terminado de descargarse, por
interesado. favor abra este cuestionario en la página siguiente para dar
El tiempo total aproximado para participar en esta investi- inicio al estudio…)
gación es de 20 minutos. Note: this is only a subset of the research instrument: The
Nota: Por favor lea la siguiente declaración y luego firme actual instrument contained five iterations of the questions in
en la línea de abajo si es que usted desea participar. Si tu- Section I below
viera alguna pregunta, por favor hágaselo saber al admin- (Siguiente paso: por favor diríjase a la primera página web
istrador de la investigación. disponible desde la página de inicio; esto lo hace haciendo un
• Yo participo voluntariamente en esta investigación click en el hiperenlace para la “Web Page #1” (“Página Web
• Se me ha informado por adelantado cuál serál mi #1”).
tarea(s) y qué procedimientos se deben seguir (permita que la página se descargue…)
• Se me ha dado la oportunidad de hacer preguntas y se Para la Página Web #1
han contestado mis preguntas a mi total satisfacción 1. Por favor indicar su actitud hacia el tiempo de demora
• Soy consciente que tengo el derecho de retirar mi para la descarga de la página web que usted acaba de observar
consentimiento y retirar mi participación en cualquier (elija una):
momento, sin ningún perjuicio en el tratamiento [ ] Retraso Intolerable
futuro hacia mí. [ ] Excesivo pero aún es un retraso tolerable
• Mi firma en la parte inferior puede ser tomada como [ ] Retraso aceptable
la afirmación de todo lo establecido anteriormente en [ ] El retraso no es significativo
este documento; esta firma se ha hecho antes de iniciar (Siguiente paso: por favor abra este cuestionario en la sigu-
mi participación en esta investigación. iente página…)
• Doy por entendido que todas las respuestas a este cues-
tionario son estrictamente confidenciales; sólo se re- B. Para la Página Web #1 (cont.)
portarán los hallazgos resumidos.
2. Por favor indique cuánto tiempo percibió usted que fue
Firma:———
el tiempo de demora para la descarga de la página web que
(Siguiente paso: Por favor ingrese su código de login
acaba de ver: —— segundos
(abajo) en el espacio para el password. Esto iniciará el web
Instrucciones para la pregunta #3: Por favor revise los grá-
browser (navegador del web) especial, configurado a medida,
ficos que contiene esta página Web. Indique su actitud hacia los
que se utilizará en esta investigacián.
gráficos que contiene la página Web que acaba de ver marcando
Al finalizar, por favor abra este cuestionario en la siguiente
una opción en la escala a continuación. Ponga su marca tan cerca
página y siga las instrucciones . . .)
de la frase en un extremo (izquierdo o derecho) según usted se
CODIGO DE LOGIN-[——]
sienta identificado con la frase.
3. Su actitud hacia los gráficos que contiene la página Web
III. EVALUACIÓN DE LA PáGINA WEB que usted acaba de ver es:Me disgusta mucho -[ ]-[ ]-[ ]-[ ]-[
]-[ ]-[ ]- Me gusta mucho
A. Instrucciones de la Sección de la Página Web (Siguiente paso: Por favor haga un click en el botón amar-
en la sección a continuación se le pedirá que usted vea una illo “Home” (“Inicio”) del navegador (browser) para regresar
serie de cinco páginas Web y conteste algunas preguntas al re- a la página de inicio.
specto. Cuando esa página haya terminado de descargarse, por
Las páginas Web tienen contenidos relacionados a marcas de favor abra este cuestionario en la página siguiente…)
video cassettes y rollos de película para fotografía. Entre estas
marcas hay algunas con las que usted está ya familiarizado: la IV. INFORMACIÓN PERSONAL
película fotográfica de marca Kodak y los video cassettes de
marca JVC. También están incluidas dos marcas relativamente Por favor conteste a las siguientes preguntas. Considere que
nuevas con las que usted probablemente no está familiarizado: todas las respuestas a este cuestionario son estrictamente con-
La película fotográfica de marca FilmTech y los video cassettes fidenciales; sólo se reportarán los resultados de manera re-
de marca SCS. sumida.
En la sección siguiente, antes de contestar cualquier pre- 16. Sexo: Masculino [ ] o Femenino [ ]
gunta acerca de una página específica, por favor asegúrese de 17. Estado civil: Casado [ ] o No Casado [ ]

Authorized licensed use limited to: IEEE Xplore. Downloaded on February 24, 2009 at 09:26 from IEEE Xplore. Restrictions apply.
ROSE et al.: CULTURE AND CONSUMER RESPONSES TO WEB DOWNLOAD TIME 43

18. Su edad: —— Años (opcional) [19] B. Tedeschi. (1999) Seeking ways to cut the web-page wait. NY
19. ¿Cuántos años tiene usted de educación posterior a Times [Online]http://www.nytimes.com/library/tech/99/06/cyber/com-
merce/14commerce.html
la secundaria? —— Años [20] Anonymous. (1999) Global Internet Statistics [On-
20. ¿En la actualidad usted tiene empleo a tiempo com- line]http://www.glreach.com/globstats/
pleto? Si [ ] o No [ ] [21] C. Wood, “The future: Will it work? Amid smart robots and wireless
wonders, some see a dark side,” Macleans.ca, Aug. 21, 2000.
21. ¿Cuántos años de experiencia de trabajo tiene [22] G. Hofstede, “Cultural constraints in management theories,” Acad.
usted? —— Años Manage. Exec., vol. 7, pp. 81–94, 1993.
22. ¿Tiene usted nacionalidad peruana?: Si [ ] o No [ ]. [23] G. Rose and D. Straub, “Predicting general IT use: Applying TAM
to the Arabic world,” J. Global Inform. Manage., vol. 6, pp. 39–46,
23. ¿ Usualmente usted considera que el ir de compras 1998.
es una fuente de placer (elija una)? Si [ ] Neutral [ ] No [24] D. W. Straub, K. Loch, and C. Hill, “Transfer of information technology
[] to developing countries: A test of cultural influence modeling in the Arab
world,” J. Global Inform. Manage., vol. 9, pp. 6–28, 2001.
24. ¿Cuántas horas por semana pasa usted en Internet? [25] R. O’Keefe, M. Cole, P. Chau, A. Massey, M. Montoya-Weiss, and M.
—— Horas Perry, “From the user interface to the consumer interface: Results from
25. ¿Quě tan a menudo realiza compras o busca infor- a global experiment,” Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud., vol. 53, pp. 611–628,
2000.
mación para realizar compras en el Internet? —— Por [26] J.-C. Chebat and P. Filiatrault, “The impact of waiting in line on con-
Mes sumers,” Int. J. Bank Market., vol. 11, pp. 35–40, 1993.
26. Si usted tiene conexión a Internet en la casa, por [27] M. M. Davis and M. J. Maggard, “An analysis of customer satisfaction
with waiting times in a two-stage service process,” J. Oper. Manage.,
favor indicar la velocidad de conexión:33.6k o más lento vol. 9, pp. 324–334, 1990.
[ ] 56k [ ] Alta Velocidad (Cable Modem o DSL, etc.)[ ] [28] J. L. Guynes, “Impact of system response time on state anxiety,”
Commun. ACM, vol. 31, pp. 342–347, 1988.
[29] E. T. Hall, The Silent Language. New York: Anchor, 1973.
REFERENCES [30] A. Bluedorn, C. F. Kaufman, and P. M. Lane, “How many things do
you like to do at once? An introduction to monochronic and polychronic
[1] A. Parasuraman, “Understanding and leveraging the role of customer time,” Acad. Manage. Exec., vol. 6, pp. 17–26, 1992.
service in external, interactive and internal marketing,” in Proc. Fron- [31] S. Tella, “Achronos: Reflections on timeless time, media and commu-
tiers in Services Conf., Nashville, TN, 1996. nication,” in Media, Mediation, Time and Communication: Emphases
[2] D. Amor, The E-Business (R)evloution. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Pren- in Network-Based Media Education, S. Tella, Ed. Helsinki, Finland:
tice Hall, 2000. Media Education Center, Dept. Teacher Education, Univ. of Helsinki,
[3] M. L. Meuter, A. L. Ostrom, R. I. Roundtree, and M. J. Bitner, “Self- 2000, vol. 9, pp. 83–100.
service technologies: Understanding customer satisfaction with tech- [32] E. T. Hall, The Silent Language, 2nd ed. New York: Anchor, 1990.
nology-based service encounters,” J. Market., vol. 64, pp. 50–64, 2000. [33] M. Karpinen-Shetta, “Cultural analysis of working time in Japan and
[4] G. Rose, H. Khoo, and D. Straub, “Current technological impediments Finland,” Loisir & Société/Leisure and Society, vol. 19, pp. 151–167,
to business-to-Consumer electronic commerce,” Commun. AIS, vol. 1, 1996.
pp. 1–74, 1999. [34] E. T. Hall, The Hidden Dimension. New York: Doubleday, 1966.
[5] M. Khosrowpour and N. Herman, “Web-enabled technologies assess- [35] M. K. Nydell, Understanding Arabs: A Guide for
ment and management: Critical issues, challenges and trends,” in Man- Westerners. Yarmouth, ME: Intercultural, 1987.
aging Web-Enabled Technologies in Organizations: A Global Perspec- [36] D. Leiser, D. Shinar, and J. Meyer, “Time estimation of computer “Wait”
tive, M. Khosrowpour, Ed. Hershey, PA: Idea Group, 2000, pp. 1–22. message displays computer systems: Interfacing with the user,” in Proc.
[6] C. Platt, “The future will be fast but not free,” in Wired, 2001, vol. 9, p. Human Factors Perspectives on Human-Computer Interaction: Selec-
120. tions From the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meet-
[7] (2001) “Shakeout in fiber optics”. NY Times [Online]http://www.ny- ings 1983–1994, G. Perlman, G. K. Green, and M. S. Wogalter, Eds.,
times.com/2001/07/29/opinion/29SUN2.html Santa Monica, CA, 1995, pp. 210–214.
[8] (2001) “U.K. behind broadband race”. BBC [37] G. Tom, M. Burns, and Y. Zeng, “Your life on hold: The effect of tele-
News [Online]http://news.bbc.co.uk/hi/english/busi- phone waiting time on customer perception,” J. Direct Market., vol. 11,
ness/newsid_1258000/1258445.stm pp. 25–31, 1997.
[9] (2001) “Korea leads world in broadband usage”. NetValue World- [38] B. Shneiderman, Designing the User Interface: Strategies for
wide [Online]http://www.netvalue.com/corp/presse/index_frame.htm? Effective Human-Computer Interaction, 3rd ed. Reading, MA:
fichier=cp0028.htm Addison-Wesley, 1998.
[10] L. Trager, “Study: 56K growing faster than broadband,” in Inter@ctive [39] (2001) UIEtips E-mail Newsletter [Online]. Available:
Week, 2000. http://world.std.com~uieweb/truth.htm
[11] D. Lake. (2001) “Quick and easy”. Ind. Stand. [Online]http://www.the- [40] J.-C. Chebat, P. Filiatrault, C. Gelinas-Chebat, and A. Vaninsky, “Impact
standard.com/article/display/0,1151,22342,00.html of waiting attribution and consumer’s mood on perceived quality,” J.
[12] G. Rose, J. Lees, and M. Meuter, “A refined view of download time Bus. Res., vol. 34, pp. 191–196, 1995.
impacts on e-consumer attitudes and patronage intentions toward e-re- [41] G. Rose and D. Straub, “The effect of download time on consumer atti-
tailers,” Int. J. Media Manage., vol. 3, pp. 105–111, 2001. tude toward the e-service retailer,” E-Service J., vol. 1, pp. 55–76, 2001.
[13] J. Hoxmeier and C. DiCesare, “System response time and user satis- [42] E. F. Stone, Research Methods in Organizational Behavior. Glenview,
faction: An experimental study of browser-based applications,” in Proc. IL: Scott, Foresman, 1978.
6th Americas Conf. Information Systems, Long Beach, CA, 2000, pp. [43] G. Rose and D. Straub, The Impact of Download Time on Brand
140–145. Attitudes in B2C E-Commerce: The Download Time Brand Impact
[14] G. Rose and D. Straub, “The effect of download time on e-Commerce: Model. Altanta, GA: Georgia State Univ., 2001.
The download time brand impact model,” in Proc. 9th Workshop on In- [44] Anonymous, “GVU’s 9th WWW user survey general
formation Technologies and Systems, Charlotte, N.C., 1999. demographics summary,” Georgia Tech. Res. Corporation,
[15] E. T. Hall, Beyond Culture, 2nd ed. New York: Anchor, 1989. http://www.gvu.gatech.edu/user_surveys/survey-1998-04/re-
[16] J. B. Wirtz and J. E. Bateson, “An experimental investigation of Halo ports/1998-04-General.html, 1998.
effects in satisfaction measures of service attributes,” Int. J. Serv. Ind. [45] S. Durvasula, S. C. Mehta, J. C. Andrews, and S. Lysonski, “Adver-
Manage., vol. 6, no. 3, pp. 84–102, 1995. tising beliefs and attitudes: Are students and general consumers indeed
[17] R. E. Barber and H. C. J. Lucas, “System response time, operator pro- different?,” J. Asian Bus., vol. 13, pp. 71–84, 1997.
ductivity and job satisfaction,” Commun. ACM, vol. 26, pp. 972–986, [46] M. T. Orne, “On the social psychology of the psychological experiment:
1983. With particular reference to demand characteristics and their implica-
[18] J. Rewick, “Clinching the holiday e-sale,” Wall Street J., Oct. 9, 2000. tions,” Amer. Psych., vol. 17, pp. 776–783, 1962.

Authorized licensed use limited to: IEEE Xplore. Downloaded on February 24, 2009 at 09:26 from IEEE Xplore. Restrictions apply.
44 IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON ENGINEERING MANAGEMENT, VOL. 50, NO. 1, FEBRUARY 2003

[47] D. Straub and G. Rose, “Measuring inhibitors and responses to B2C Roberto Evaristo received the Ph.D. degree in management information sys-
e-commerce: An IS-marketing perspective,” Georgia State Univ., At- tems from the University of Minnesota, Minneapolis.
lanta, GA, 2001. He is currently an Assistant Professor in the Information and Decision Sci-
[48] M. K. Hui and D. K. Tse, “What to tell consumers in waits of different ences Department, University of Illinois, Chicago. He is involved in several
lengths: An integrative model of service evaluation,” J. Market., vol. 60, projects related to the management of distributed projects in virtual organiza-
pp. 81–90, 1996. tions, with work done in Japan, the U.S., and Europe. He has published in jour-
[49] T. D. Cook and D. T. Campbell, Quasi Experimentation: Design and nals including Communications of the ACM, International Journal of Project
Analytical Issues for Field Settings. Shokie, IL: Rand McNally, 1979. Management, Database, Journal of Global Information Management, Euro-
[50] J. F. Hair Jr, R. E. Anderson, R. L. Tatham, and W. C. Black, Multivariate pean Management Journal, Journal of Organizational Computing and Elec-
Data Analysis With Readings, 4th ed ed. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Pren- tronic Commerce, and elsewhere. and serves on the Editorial Review Boards
tice-Hall, 1995. of the Journal of Global Information Management and the Journal of Global
[51] H. R. Lindman, Anova in complex experimental designs. San Fran- Information Technology Management.
cisco, CA: W. H. Freeman, 1974.
[52] A. Al-Arjani, “Impact of cultural issues on the scheduling of housing
maintenance in a saudi arabian urban project,” Int. J. Project Manage.,
vol. 13, pp. 373–382, 1995.
[53] M. E. Davis and J. D. Grimes, “Creating global software: Text handling
and localization in taligent’s commonpoint application system,” IBM
Syst. J., vol. 35, pp. 227–243, 1996.
[54] B. Tedeschi. (2000) “E-Commerce report: Web sites redesign Detmar Straub received the Ph.D. degree in english
home pages”. NY Times [Online]. Available: http://www.ny- from The Pennsylvania State University, University
times.com/2000/10/30/technology/30ECOMMERCE.html Park, and the D.B.A. in management information sys-
[55] S. Taylor, “The effects of filled waiting time and service provider control tems from Indiana University, Bloomington.
over the delay on evaluations of service,” J. Acad. Market. Sci., vol. 23, He is currently the J. Mack Robinson Dis-
pp. 38–48, 1995. tinguished Professor of Information Systems at
Georgia State University, Atlanta. He has con-
ducted research in Net-enhanced organizations,
computer security, technological innovation, and
Gregory M. Rose received the Ph.D. degree in computer information systems international information technology with over 100
from Georgia State University, Atlanta. publications in journals including Management
After receiving Ph.D. degree, he was a Postdoctoral Fellow at the University Science, Information Systems Research, MIS Quarterly, Organization Science,
of Jyvaskyla, Jyvaskyla, Finland. He is currently an Assistant Professor with the Journal of MIS, Journal of AIS, Journal of Global Information Management,
College of Business and Economics, Washington State University, Vancouver. Communications of the ACM, Information & Management, Communications of
His research interests include the development, design, adoption, and use of the AIS, Academy of Management Executive, and Sloan Management Review.
web-based systems across various cultures in the Americas, Europe, and Africa. He is currently an Associate Editor for Management Science and Information
He has published in such journals as Accounting, Management and Informa- Systems Research. He was formerly an Editor-in-Chief of DATA BASE for
tion Technologies, Information Systems Journal, Journal of Global Information Advances in Information Systems, a Senior Editor for Information Systems
Management, Electronic Markets, and Communications of the AIS. He serves on Research (Special Issue on e-Commerce Metrics), and an Associate Editor for
the Editorial Review Board of the Journal of Global Information Management. MIS Quarterly.

Authorized
View publication stats licensed use limited to: IEEE Xplore. Downloaded on February 24, 2009 at 09:26 from IEEE Xplore. Restrictions apply.

You might also like