You are on page 1of 2

Two Views of the Same News Find Opposite Biases

By Shankar Vedantam
Washington Post Staff Writer
Monday, July 24, 2006; A02

You could be forgiven for thinking the television images in the experiment were from 2006. They were
really from 1982: Israeli forces were clashing with Arab militants in Lebanon. The world was watching,
charges were flying, and the air was thick with grievance, hurt and outrage.

There was only one thing on which pro-Israeli and pro-Arab audiences agreed. Both were certain that
media coverage in the United States was hopelessly biased in favor of the other side.

The endlessly recursive conflict in the Middle East provides any number of instructive morals about
human nature, but it also offers a psychological window into the world of partisan behavior. Israel's 1982
war in Lebanon sparked some of the earliest experiments into why people reach dramatically different
conclusions about the same events.

The results say a lot about partisan behavior in general -- why Republicans and Democrats love to hate
each other, for example, or why Coke and Pepsi fans clash. Sadly, the results also say a lot about the
newest conflicts between Israel and its enemies in Lebanon and the Palestinian territories, and why news
organizations are being besieged with angry complaints from both sides.

Partisans, it turns out, don't just arrive at different conclusions; they see entirely different worlds . In one
especially telling experiment, researchers showed 144 observers six television news segments about
Israel's 1982 war with Lebanon.

Pro-Arab viewers heard 42 references that painted Israel in a positive light and 26 references that painted
Israel unfavorably.

Pro-Israeli viewers, who watched the very same clips, spotted 16 references that painted Israel positively
and 57 references that painted Israel negatively.

Both groups were certain they were right and that the other side didn't know what it was talking about.

The tendency to see bias in the news -- now the raison d'etre of much of the blogosphere -- is such a
reliable indicator of partisan thinking that researchers coined a term, "hostile media effect," to describe the
sincere belief among partisans that news reports are painting them in the worst possible light.

Were pro-Israeli and pro-Arab viewers who were especially knowledgeable about the conflict immune
from such distortions? Amazingly, it turned out to be exactly the opposite, Stanford psychologist Lee D.
Ross said. The best-informed partisans were the most likely to see bias against their side.

Ross thinks this is because partisans often feel the news lacks context. Instead of just showing a missile
killing civilians, in other words, partisans on both sides want the news to explain the history of events that
prompted -- and could have justified -- the missile. The more knowledgeable people are, the more context
they find missing.

Even more curious, the hostile media effect seems to apply only to news sources that strive for balance.
News reports from obviously biased sources usually draw fewer charges of bias. Partisans, it turns out,
find it easier to countenance obvious propaganda than news accounts that explore both sides.

"If I think the world is black, and you think the world is white, and someone comes along and says it is
gray, we will both think that person is biased," Ross said.
The experiment, of course, did not address whether news reports were in fact biased -- who would decide?
-- or how the media ought to cover conflicts. Partisans argue that assigning equal weight to both sides is
wrong when one side (theirs) is right. In any event, psychologists such as Ross are less interested in rating
the news or in which side is right than in the curiosities of human perception: Why are partisans
invariably blind to how news coverage might help their side?

If someone says several nice things about you and one derogatory thing, what sticks in your mind? People
who are deeply invested in one side are quicker to spot and remember aspects of the news that hurt than
they are to see aspects that help, said Richard Perloff, a Cleveland State University political
communication researcher.

Perloff elicited the same clashing perceptions of bias from pro-Israeli and pro-Arab audiences when he
showed them news clips with equal amounts of violence.

Ross and Perloff both found that what partisans worry about the most is the impact of the news on neutral
observers. But the data suggest such worry is misplaced. Neutral observers are better than partisans at
seeing flaws and virtues on both sides. Partisans, it turns out, are particularly susceptible to the general
human belief that other people are susceptible to propaganda.

"When you are persuaded by something, you don't think it is propaganda," Ross said. "Israelis know they
see the world the way they do because they are Israelis, and Arabs, too. The difference is people think in
their case, their special identities are a source of enlightenment, whereas other people's source of
enlightenment is a source of bias."

Next Monday: What brain-imaging research shows about partisan behavior.

Staff writer Shankar Vedantam will be online today at 3 p.m. athttp://www.washingtonpost.com/scienceto


answer questions about human behavior and evolution.

© 2006 The Washington Post Company

You might also like