You are on page 1of 25

MATHEMATICS ANXIETY AND HUMOR 1

Humor Effects on Mathematics Anxiety and Mathematics Achievement

Maria Socha

Missouri Southern State University

HNRS 400: Research Seminar

Casey Cole

December 2, 2021
MATHEMATICS ANXIETY AND HUMOR 2

Humor Effects on Mathematics Anxiety and Mathematics Achievement

In education, research based practices are everything. There are always new

studies coming out regarding best practices and how to implement them effectively for

the benefit of students. In the area of education, thousands of scholarly articles have

been and continue to be published each year. As children reflect the future of where

society is headed, it is imperative to do the best we can to provide effective services for

students. Although mathematics anxiety has been affecting students and adults for

many years, it was not until the past few decades when we started to really study

mathematics anxiety (Ashcraft, 2002; Barroso, 2021; Chinn, 2009; Devine, 2012;

Furner, 2002). Though researchers still have not pinpointed causes of mathematics

anxiety in students, what we have learned about what it is and how it affects our

teachers and students in educational and in real world settings. This study aims to

explore the effectiveness of one strategy in relieving student mathematics anxiety

levels.

When reading through the bodies of literature already written, much information

is available about mathematics anxiety. Research shows effects of mathematics anxiety

on students, teachers, classrooms, test scores, and child well-being among other things

(Ashcraft, 2002). Many studies have been performed to show links between

mathematics anxiety and test scores showing the inverse relationship between these

two factors (Tobias, 1993; Harari et al., 2013; Devine et al., 2012) . Additionally, much

information is available regarding humor studies ((Darling and Civikly, 1986; Bakar,

2019; Nicoleta, 2018; (Shahid and Ghazal, 2019). This has shown the effects of humor

physically and emotionally, as well as the variety of ways that students and teachers
MATHEMATICS ANXIETY AND HUMOR 3

perceive humor and how it is used in the classroom. A gap in the previous research,

however, is how mathematics anxiety and humor interact with one another in the

classroom. In this study, I hope to add to the previous bodies of literature by determining

if there is any significant relationship between humor and mathematics anxiety as well

as humor and mathematics performance in the classroom when used as a method of

teaching style.

Mathematics anxiety, also known as a phobia of mathematics, affects students

and adults of all ages (Ashcraft, 2019). This phenomena is having negative effects on

students academically, socially, emotionally, and even physiologically (Lyons and

Beilock, 2012). Over the years, numbers of students with mathematics anxiety have

been on the rise and this trend does not seem to be slowing down (Ashcraft, 2019). The

significance of the proposed experiment involves finding ways to ease student

mathematics anxiety in order to help students feel and perform better. This study

intends to bridge the gap between areas of study that have already been highly

researched. Mathematics anxiety is known to have negative effects on mathematics

performance (Ashcraft and Krause, 2007; Devine et al., 2012; Barroso et al., 2021;

Haiyan, 2009). On the flip side, humor has been shown to have positive effects on

general anxiety throughout research as a coping mechanism or way to relieve

symptoms of anxiety (Tagalidou et al., 2019). Teachers in the educational field have

historically used humor as a method of teaching adopted into their teaching style for

purposes of relationships and classroom community more than as means of teaching

content (Shahid and Ghazal, 2019).. Since this has proved to be true, theoretically, by

using humor as a method of teaching practice in the classroom, student mathematics


MATHEMATICS ANXIETY AND HUMOR 4

anxiety should be reduced and consequently, student mathematics performance should

increase.

Mathematics Anxiety

When studying how humor may affect levels of mathematics anxiety, it is first

important to establish what exactly this phenomenon is. Mathematics anxiety is

commonly defined as feelings of tension, apprehension, or fear related to working with

mathematical problems or manipulation of numbers (Tobias, 1993). While the feelings

and symptoms that individuals with mathematics anxiety face are real, mathematics

anxiety is not a disorder that is officially diagnosed. Mathematics anxiety is a

widespread problem that affects individuals of all ages across the globe (Tobias, 1993).

Ashcraft (2002) argues that highly math-anxious individuals demonstrate characteristics

including a strong tendency to avoid math. This avoidance of mathematics can lead to

underperformance in mathematics competence due to lack of exposure. According to

Ashcraft (2002), mathematics anxiety notably differentiates mathematics anxiety from

both general anxiety and test anxiety. These forms of anxiety can absolutely be isolated

or can be presented in combination with one another. While general anxiety is a

diagnosable disorder, mathematics anxiety is characterized more as people who

experience feelings of stress and tension when faced with math-related situations.

Ashcraft (2002) explains that individuals with high levels of math anxiety also generally

scores high on other anxiety measurements. In a 2002 study by Ashcraft, findings found

that the strongest interrelationship with mathematics anxiety was with test anxiety which

demonstrated a .52 correlation. This shows that if the proposed study does reveal an

effect of humor on mathematics anxiety, it will likely also aid in test anxiety. Additional
MATHEMATICS ANXIETY AND HUMOR 5

research should be done to determine humor aids in relief of general test anxiety as well

as mathematics. Research shows that despite overlaps between mathematics and test

anxiety, the two prove to be separate phenomenons. Ashcraft (2002) and Skaalvik

(1997) agree that mathematics anxiety in research can be defined as avoidance

oriented feelings of fear, tension, and apprehension that can occur when engaging with

mathematics in school or other settings. Supporting the notion regarding negative

effects of mathematics anxiety, Ashcraft and Krause (2007) demonstrated that the

performance on a standardized mathematics achievement test varies as a function of

mathematics anxiety. They argued that the preoccupation with one’s mathematics fears

and anxieties is resource demanding. It interferes with the working memory of

math-anxious individuals and lowers the capacity to concentrate on mathematics

problems. A study of a large sample of undergraduate students revealed that these

negative emotions including anxiety were highly predictive of lower self-regulation skills

in students.

Prevalence of Mathematics Anxiety

According to Kennedy and Tipps (1998), an increasing number of studies are

revealing that mathematics anxiety begins in the early school years. However, although

it begins in these early years, mathematics anxiety widely exists in middle school,

college, and adult life (Harari et al. 2013; Jackson and Leffingwell 1999). Research has

estimated that at least 93% of Americans from kindergarten through college and around

50% of British adults experience varying degrees of mathematics anxiety in their lifetime

(Chinn 2009; Furner and Duffy 2002). Again in the US, an estimated 25% of four-year

college students and up to 80% of students who attend community colleges experience
MATHEMATICS ANXIETY AND HUMOR 6

moderate to severe mathematics anxiety levels (Wang and Lu, 2006). Another study

that yet again proves that mathematics anxiety is prevalent was a Chinese study by

Wang and Lu (2006). This study showed that 69% of junior high school students

experience moderate and more serious mathematics anxiety (Wang and Lu, 2006).

Studies have shown that mathematics anxiety affects students of a variety of grades

and stages of life. Additionally, mathematics anxiety does not just occur within the

borders of one nation but occurs universally in educational facilities and beyond. In a

society that runs on the basis of basic literacy and mathematics skills, it is easy to

understand how an issue that is this prevalent needs to be addressed. The present

study will be performed with hopes of finding a way to ease levels of mathematics

anxiety to hopefully improve the negative effects discussed below in order to help

students and adults alike across the globe.

Effects of Mathematics Anxiety

From mathematics avoidance to poor test scores and student relationships with

math, mathematics anxiety is shown to have many negative effects on students.

Mathematics anxiety has been negatively correlated with mathematics performance in

many studies (Ashcraft and Krause, 2007; Devine et al., 2012; Barroso et al., 2021;

Haiyan, 2009). A study by Chinn (2009) shows that mathematics anxiety has a negative

effect on learning attitudes. The tension and apprehension that students feel around

mathematics stimuli hurts their motivation and excitement to learn. Additionally, students

who face mathematics anxiety are less likely to enroll in courses that involve

mathematical content (Meece et al.,1990). Working memory (Ashcraft and Krause,

2007), and metacognition (Legg and Locker Jr., 2009), relating to mathematics
MATHEMATICS ANXIETY AND HUMOR 7

performance are also decreased when individuals are faced with mathematics anxiety.

Not only are the negative effects we see students face with mathematics presented in

the classroom, but these symptoms are appearing in students’ normal lives (Maloney

and Beilock, 2012). This makes students more likely to struggle with financial decisions,

choosing a career, and even day-to-day activities like reading cash register data

(Maloney and Beilock, 2012).

In addition to struggles academically, students with high levels of math anxiety

experience symptoms that affect them physically as well (Lyons and Beilock, 2012). In a

study by Lyons and Beilock (2012), data shows that when anticipating an upcoming

math task, the higher the participant’s anxiety, the more the participant demonstrates an

increase in the regions associated with visceral threat. Additionally, in the bilateral

dorsal-posterior insula region, the region associated with the feeling of pain, there is an

increase in brain activity (Lyons and Beilock, 2012). In other words, for individuals with

high levels of math anxiety, the brain’s neural response to math stimuli sparks a similar

reaction as it would with experiencing physical pain. In the study, participants were

asked to complete a word task and a math task for the experiment while using fMRI to

track and monitor neural data (Lyons and Beilock, 2012). Findings from this study show

that the regions of the brain that are activated when people feel physical pain are the

same regions of an HMA individual anticipating math (Lyons and Beilock, 2012).

Relating to the present study, finding a method to relieve math anxiety for students will

not only help them academically, but physically and emotionally as well by avoiding this

form of neural response to mathematics.

Humor
MATHEMATICS ANXIETY AND HUMOR 8

History of Humor in Education

Historically, in the last decade, research on humor in educational settings has

focused on four main areas (Darling and Civikly, 1986). The areas include general

positive effects of humor on learning and in teaching styles, humor as an aid in memory

retention and comprehension of content, humor as a method of relieving test or

performance anxiety, and the effects of humor on student perceptions in the classroom

(Darling and Civikly, 1986). Though there are a variety of subjects covered in this

research, an area of literature that is lacking is between the relationship of humor with

mathematics anxiety, which is the aim of the present study.

Effects of Humor in Teaching

Humor has been shown to have psychological, physical and social benefits and

has proved to be useful in educational contexts over and over again in the last sixty

years (Unsal et al., 2018). In a study by Shahid and Ghazal (2019), results from 100

student participants show that the use of humor in teaching with aims to make students

laugh has positive effects on student learning. Rather than focusing on student

performance, this article supports my ideas by showing that humor helps improve

teaching effectiveness in terms of motivation, engagement, anxiety reduction,

stimulation of thoughts, interest in studies, and fostering positive teacher-student

relationships. In the study, a cross-sectional design compared teachers who use humor

and teachers who do not (Shahid and Ghazal, 2019). Each student rated two teachers

choosing one who they think uses humor in class lectures and the other who does not,

and filled out a questionnaire on each as well (Shahid and Ghazal, 2019). When

analyzing the data collected, student responses generally rated teachers more effective
MATHEMATICS ANXIETY AND HUMOR 9

who were perceived to use humor in their teaching style as opposed to teachers who

did not (Shahid and Ghazal, 2019). These results show a positive relationship between

teacher’s use of humor and student’s motivation levels, anxiety reduction, class

engagement, teacher-student relationships, and teacher effectiveness overall (Shahid

and Ghazal, 2019). This provides evidence support for the benefits of using humor as a

method of relieving math anxiety in the classroom. Since research shows that

mathematics anxiety causes student motivation to decline (Ashcraft and Krause, 2007),

this study provides evidence to counter that by implementing humor into teaching styles

(Shahid and Ghazal, 2019).

Bakar’s 2019 study shows other benefits to using humor in education. According

to Bakar (2019), humor assists teachers in gaining and retaining student’s attention and

enhances student learning. Humor helps to maintain a teacher’s good reputation, to

build strong teacher-student relationships, and foster positive emotions towards a

learning subject (Bakar, 2019). Humor has proved to benefit students in the classroom

both socially, emotionally, and academically when used in a manner that aligns with

learning content.

Benefits of Laughter

Using humor as a method of teaching has two goals: sparking student laughter,

and teaching students. Since humor stimulus aims to bring about laughter for students,

it is important to understand what effects laughter has on students that could relate to

mathematics anxiety and its consequences. Nicoleta’s 2018 study aimed to investigate

the immediate effects of laughter on self-acceptance, anxiety, and anger by having

volunteer and anonymous participants watch a situational comedy show. The 17


MATHEMATICS ANXIETY AND HUMOR 10

participants were divided into a control and an experimental group. Findings from this

study show that the participants in the experimental group, after having watched and

laughed at a comedy, were more self-acceptant, less likely to express anger, and most

importantly to the present study, were less inclined to feel thoughts of anxiety (Nicoleta,

2018). If students are feeling more confident in themselves, they may be less likely to

present mathematics avoidant behaviors. In addition to simulated laughter, integrative

laughter is another method used to ease student anxiety. Another study concluded that

adding an integrative laughter therapy session to standard treatment in a day hospital

for individuals with addictive disorders yielded a greater improvement in self-esteem,

anxiety, and happiness compared to standard treatment alone (Francisco et al., 2019).

This shows the effects of laughter on trait anxiety and demonstrates the idea that

laughter proves to have benefits on general anxiety, leading to the idea that humor will

help reduce mathematics anxiety when used in schools.

Using humor in the classroom might be viewed as a risky behavior for a teacher

as it could be hit or miss in the sense that students will value the humor used. However,

a 2011 study by Mora-Ripoll explores the health benefits of simulated laughter. This

study in particular focuses on how simulated laughter techniques can be easily

implemented in traditional clinical settings for the medical field. Since these practices

proved successful in this context, it is beneficial to study potential benefits in the field of

education as well. Results of the analysis of literature show that laughter has shown

different physiological and psychological benefits with very limited adverse effects.

There is limited literature, but there is evidence to suggest that simulated laughter also

has effects on certain aspects of health, though further well-designed research is


MATHEMATICS ANXIETY AND HUMOR 11

warranted. This research shows the possibility of benefits of laughter that is simulated

(artificial rather than genuine). While the human mind is able to make a distinction

between stimulated and spontaneous laughter, the human body cannot (Mora-Ripoll,

2011). Regardless of whether laughter is stimulated or spontaneous, literature shows

that health-related outcomes are produced (Mora-Ripoll, 2011). This supports the idea

that even if students in the classroom do not find the humor stimulus to be humorous,

forced/fake laughter that is simulated is still shown to have positive effects on the body.

Effective Use of Humor in the Classroom

When using humor in the classroom, not just any humor should be used. Studies

show that students respond to some types of humor better than others (Bakar, 2019;

Darling and Civikly, 1986). Research shows that the general consensus among

students is that the use of humor is profoundly beneficial in education (Unsal et al.,

2018). However, humor in the classroom can help students or hurt them depending on

whether it is perceived or understood well or not (Darling and Civikly, 1986). One way to

effectively implement humor into education is by using it in a manner that is both

appropriate and relevant (Bakar, 2019). Humor is considered relevant when it is usefully

applicable to the topic, concept, or theories discussed in the course content (Bakar,

2019). Students prefer teachers to use humor that is relevant to the learning content as

opposed to unrelated humor or no humor at all (Bakar, 2019). In the present study, the

humor stimulus used will be relevant in accordance with this Bakar (2019) study and will

be appropriate to the age level and context of the classroom.

Hypotheses
MATHEMATICS ANXIETY AND HUMOR 12

Hypothesis 1: Students taught by a teacher using humor will have reduced mathematics

anxiety compared to a class taught with the absence of humor.

Hypothesis 2: Students taught by a teacher using humor will have increased

mathematics performance compared to a class taught with the absence of humor.

Methods

Participants

For this study, 40 students will be recruited to participate. All of these students

will be 12th grade students attending Pea Ridge High School in Pea Ridge, Arkansas.

The participants of this study will be recruited from 2 different virtual/online Algebra 3

classes. Although participants have not had the same mathematics teacher in years

prior, all students in the study will have taken Algebra 1 and Algebra 2 as prerequisites

for entry into this mathematics course. The typical age range of participants will be

17-18 years old.

Materials

For course delivery, I will use two Powerpoints. These Powerpoints will cover the

topic of substitution in Algebra 3 and will be identical with the exception of five

math-related humorous images acting as the independent variable in the humor group.

In accordance with Bakar 2019, these five cartoon images will be relevant and

appropriate. To measure mathematics anxiety, I will use The Abbreviated

mathematicsAnxiety Scale (AMAS; Hopko et al., 2003). Items in this scale will have

response options from 1 (not anxious at all) to 5 (very anxious) where higher summed

scores will indicate a greater level of mathematics anxiety. To measure mathematics

achievement, students will be given a 10-item assessment over the material covered in
MATHEMATICS ANXIETY AND HUMOR 13

the lecture (substitution). Items on this assessment will include 5 multiple choice, 2

true/false, and 3 open-response styled questions.

Design

In order to determine if humor might have an effect on mathematics anxiety or on

mathematics performance, I will conduct a quantitative experimental study with a

pre-test post-test design. My experiment will have two groups- the group that is exposed

to a humor stimulus will act as the experimental group, and the group that is not

exposed to humor will act as the control group. The independent variable for this study

will be humor.

For the purposes of this study, the operational definition of humor will be five

mathematics-related cartoon images created with the intention of being funny. There will

be two dependent variables for this study. The first dependent variable that I will

observe is mathematics anxiety. To measure this, I will use students’ scores on the

AMAS (Hopko et al., 2003). My second dependent variable is mathematics

achievement. This will be measured by students’ scores on the assigned 10-item

assessment to be completed after the lecture. All participants will be randomly assigned

to one of the two classrooms/groups (i.e., humorous or standard).

Procedure

After being randomly assigned to their group, all students will participate in an

Algebra 3 class in their designated group. Participants will be told that they will (1) take

a pre-test to measure individual levels of mathematics anxiety, (2) watch their

mathematics lecture video in a whole-group setting, (3) complete a brief mathematics

achievement assessment, and (4) complete a post-test to again measure their levels of
MATHEMATICS ANXIETY AND HUMOR 14

mathematics anxiety. In this Algebra 3 course, class material will be presented virtually

in a format that uses Powerpoint presentations with a voiceover. The voiceover will be

identical for each of the two groups. The form of humor manipulation used in the lecture

will consist of 5 relevant and appropriate math-related cartoon images. The Powerpoints

for each group will be 3 identical with the exception of the cartoon images in the humor

group. In the humor group, the Powerpoint will begin and end with a cartoon image to

prime the effect of humor and to likely have more of an impact on participants due to

primacy recency effects. In the humor group, each of the five humorous images will be

shown for eight seconds.

I chose to use a virtual method of class delivery for this study to eliminate

possible differences in the presentation of information including time, speed, content

and duration of the lesson. This method ensures that students all receive a lesson that

is similar except for the use of humor in the experimental group. Each classroom will

have a supervisory teacher who plays the Powerpoint and runs the experiment (passing

out and collecting pre and post-tests and mathematics achievement assessment).The

class period will be approximately 60 minutes. The Powerpoint video lecture portion of

the lesson will last 25 minutes in the standard group and 25 minutes 40 seconds in the

humor group.

Following the completion of the lesson, students will be given a brief, 10 item

mathematics assessment. Ten minutes at the beginning and end of the class will be

reserved for students to complete the AMAS (Hopko et al., 2003). The classrooms for

each group will be next door to each other and participants will attend the same

classroom at the same time that they would each day for Algebra 3 class for the sake of
MATHEMATICS ANXIETY AND HUMOR 15

consistency with their typical daily routine. Both groups will participate in the study at the

same time as one another to reduce effects of extraneous variables.

Statistical Analysis

Since mathematics anxiety will be measured using a pre and a post-test, but

mathematics achievement will only be assessed at the conclusion of the lesson, I will

use two separate analyses in order to process and analyze my data. Firstly, regarding

mathematics anxiety, I will use a repeated measures ANOVA statistics function in order

to analyze the data sets from the humor group and from the non-humor group. This will

help me to compare the mean differences in mathematics anxiety levels from the humor

group to the non-humor group. Additionally, within each group, it will help me to

compare the differences of levels in the pre-test as compared to the post-test of each

group for each participant. I predict there should be a greater mean difference between

the pre and post-test in the humor group than the non-humor group. That is, I expect

their math anxiety levels to have a more significant change and to be lower than the

non-humor group. I expect this to occur because I anticipate the humor stimulus to

cause a change in the participants' math anxiety measures that I don’t anticipate

occuring in the group without the humor stimulus. In the humor group, post

measurement, I expect their scores to be lower than the non-humor group.

Secondly, mathematics achievement will be analyzed using a separate ANOVA

function. This will tell the differences in the mean scores of each group on the

mathematics assessment and will help to determine if the group with humor learned

more than the group without it. After data is collected and analyzed, each of the two

ANOVA’s will be used to either reject or fail to reject the null hypotheses.
MATHEMATICS ANXIETY AND HUMOR 16

Present literature shows that mathematics anxiety has negative consequences

on students and their mathematics performance. Conversely, humor and laughter have

positive effects on students' motivation, confidence, and anxiety levels. A decrease in

general anxiety can be transferred to a decrease in mathematics anxiety. With all that

said, then hypothetically, using humor in the classroom should reduce students'

mathematics anxiety and increase their mathematics performance. While the literature

shows much research in the areas of mathematics anxiety, mathematics performance,

and humor, this study aims to further these areas. This study will make connections by

looking at the way these variables interact with one another through an experimental

based research project. The pre-test post test design will allow for comparing

mathematics scores and mathematics anxiety levels and comparing how they interact in

the humor group and the non-humor group. If my prediction that humor will decrease

mathematics anxiety and thus raise mathematics performance proves to be true, this

will open the door to a possible strategy to help students who face mathematics anxiety.

This study could be repeated and exposed to a larger population of participants to better

understand how humor influences mathematics anxiety and mathematics performance.

Additional experiments in this area would need to be conducted in order to fully

understand the way these variables interact with one another to best serve students in

the field of mathematics.


MATHEMATICS ANXIETY AND HUMOR 17

Appendix

1.

2.
MATHEMATICS ANXIETY AND HUMOR 18

3.

4.
MATHEMATICS ANXIETY AND HUMOR 19

5.

6.
MATHEMATICS ANXIETY AND HUMOR 20

7.

8.

9.
MATHEMATICS ANXIETY AND HUMOR 21

10.
MATHEMATICS ANXIETY AND HUMOR 22

References

Ann L. Darling, & Jean M. Civikly. (1986). The Effect of Teacher Humor on Student

Perceptions of Classroom Communicative Climate. The Journal of Classroom

Interaction, 22(1), 24–30.

Ashcraft, M. H. (2002). Math anxiety: Personal, educational, and cognitive

consequences. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 181–185.

Ashcraft, M.H., Krause, J.A. Working memory, math performance, and math anxiety.

Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 14, 243–248 (2007).

https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03194059

Bakar, F. (2019). Appropriate and relevant humour in the university classroom: insights

from teachers and students. European Journal of Humour Research, 7(4),

137–152. https://doi.org/10.7592/EJHR2019.7.4.bakar

Barroso, C., Ganley, C. M., McGraw, A. L., Geer, E. A., Hart, S. A., & Daucourt, M. C.

(2021). A meta-analysis of the relation between math anxiety and math

achievement. Psychological Bulletin, 147(2), 134–168.

https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000307

Chinn, S. (2009). Mathematics anxiety in secondary students in England. Dyslexia

15(1), 61–68. https://doi.org/10.1002/dys.381.

Devine, A., Fawcett, K., Szűcs, D., & Dowker, A. (2012). Gender differences in

mathematics anxiety and the relation to mathematics performance while

controlling for test anxiety. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 8(33), 2–9.

https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-8-33.
MATHEMATICS ANXIETY AND HUMOR 23

Francisco, Cristina Torres, Sandra De Andrés, Ana Millet, M. Teresa Ricart, Elvira

Hernández-Martínez-Esparza, Mercedes Abades, & Joan Trujols. (2019).

Effectiveness of Integrative Laughter Therapy to Reduce Anxiety, Improve

Self-Esteem and Increase Happiness: A Naturalistic Study at a Day Hospital for

Addictive Disorders. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public

Health, 16(21), 4194. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph16214194

Furner, J. M., & Duffy, M. L. (2002). Equity for All Students in the New Millennium:

Disabling Math Anxiety. Intervention in School and Clinic, 38, 67-74.

http;//dx.doi.org/10.1177/10534512020380020101

Haiyan Bai, LihShing Wang, Wei Pan, & Frey, M. (2009). Measuring Mathematics

Anxiety: Psychometric Analysis of a Bidimensional Affective Scale. Journal of

Instructional Psychology, 36(3), 185–193.

Harari, R. R., Vukovic, R. K., & Bailey, S. P. (2013). Mathematics anxiety in young

children: An exploratory study. The Journal of Experimental Education, 81(4),

538–555.

Hopko, D. R., Mahadevan, R., Bare, R. L., & Hunt, M. K. (2003). The Abbreviated

Math Anxiety Scale (AMAS): Construction, Validity, and Reliability. Assessment,

10(2), 178–182. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191103010002008

Jackson, C. D., & Leffingwell, R. J. (1999). The role of instructors in creating math

anxiety in students from kindergarten through college. The Mathematics Teacher,

92(7), 583–586. https://doi.org/10.2307/27971118.

Kennedy, L. M., & Tipps, S. (1988). Guiding children’s learning of mathematics (5th ed.).

Belmont: Wadsworth.
MATHEMATICS ANXIETY AND HUMOR 24

Legg, A. M., & Locker Jr, L. (2009). Math performance and its relationship to math

anxiety and metacognition. North American Journal of Psychology, 11(3),

471–486.

Lyons IM, Beilock SL (2012) When Math Hurts: Math Anxiety Predicts Pain Network

Activation in Anticipation of Doing Math. PLoS ONE 7(10): e48076.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0048076

Maloney, E. A., & Beilock, S. L. (2012). Math anxiety: Who has it, why it develops, and

how to guard against it. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(8), 404–406.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.06.008.

Meece, J. L., Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. S. (1990). Predictors of math anxiety and its

influence on young adolescents' course enrollment intentions and performance in

mathematics. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82(1), 60–70.

https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.82.1.60.

Mora-Ripoll, R. (2011). Potential health benefits of simulated laughter: A narrative

review of the literature and recommendations for future research.

Complementary Therapies in Medicine, 19(3), 170–177.

Nicoleta, Răban-Motounu. (2018). Laughter and Well-Being. Current Trends in Natural

Sciences, 7(14), 266–271.

Shahid, I., & Ghazal, S. (2019). Humor as a tool to Teaching Effectiveness. Journal of

Behavioural Sciences, 29(1), 25–37.

Skaalvik, E. M. (2018). Mathematics anxiety and coping strategies among middle school

students: relations with students’ achievement goal orientations and level of


MATHEMATICS ANXIETY AND HUMOR 25

performance. Social Psychology of Education, 21(3), 709–723.

https://doi.org/10.1007/s11218-018-9433-2

Tagalidou, N., Distlberger, E., Loderer, V., & Laireiter, A.-R. (2019). Efficacy and

feasibility of a humor training for people suffering from depression, anxiety, and

adjustment disorder: a randomized controlled trial. BMC Psychiatry, 19(1), 93.

https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-019-2075-x

Tobias, S. (1993). Overcoming math anxiety revised and expanded. New York: Norton.

Unsal, S., Agcam, R., & Aydemir, M. (2018). Humour-Based Learning: From the Lens of

Adolescents. REMIE - Multidisciplinary Journal of Educational Research, 8(1),

29–55.

Wang, J., & Lu, J. (2006). 初中生数学焦虑的调查及其调控研究 [A survey and

adjustment research about math anxiety of junior middle school students].

Psychology Science, 29(3), 605–608.

https://doi.org/10.16719/j.cnki.1671-6981.2006.03.020.

You might also like