You are on page 1of 10

Accelerat ing t he world's research.

Misophonia: A new mental disorder


Steven Taylor

Cite this paper Downloaded from Academia.edu 

Get the citation in MLA, APA, or Chicago styles

Related papers Download a PDF Pack of t he best relat ed papers 

A brief course of cognit ive behavioural t herapy for t he t reat ment of misophonia: a case exa…
Cryst al Dehle, Rosemary Bernst ein

Treat ing t he Init ial Physical Reflex of Misophonia wit h t he Neural Repat t erning Technique: A Count erc…
T homas H Dozier

Misophonia: physiological invest igat ions and case descript ions


romke rouw, Miren Edelst ein
Medical Hypotheses 103 (2017) 109–117

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Medical Hypotheses
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/mehy

Misophonia: A new mental disorder?


Steven Taylor ⇑
University of British Columbia, Canada

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Misophonia, a phenomenon first described in the audiology literature, is characterized by intense emo-
Received 6 March 2017 tional reactions (e.g., anger, rage, anxiety, disgust) in response to highly specific sounds, particularly
Accepted 1 May 2017 sounds of human origin such as oral or nasal noises made by other people (e.g., chewing, sniffing, slurp-
ing, lip smacking). Misophonia is not listed in any of the contemporary psychiatric classification systems.
Some investigators have argued that misophonia should be regarded as a new mental disorder, falling
Keywords: within the spectrum of obsessive-compulsive related disorders. Other researchers have disputed this
Misophonia
claim. The purpose of this article is to critically examine the proposition that misophonia should be clas-
Selective sensory intolerance
Selective sound sensitivity syndrome
sified as a new mental disorder. The clinical and research literature on misophonia was examined and
Nosology considered in the context of the broader literature on what constitutes a mental disorder. There have
Psychiatric classification been growing concerns that diagnostic systems such as DSM-5 tend to over-pathologize ordinary quirks
and eccentricities. Accordingly, solid evidence is required for proposing a new psychiatric disorder. The
available evidence suggests that (a) misophonia meets many of the general criteria for a mental disorder
and has some evidence of clinical utility as a diagnostic construct, but (b) the nature and boundaries of
the syndrome are unclear; for example, in some cases misophonia might be simply one feature of a
broader pattern of sensory intolerance, and (c) considerably more research is required, particularly work
concerning diagnostic validity, before misophonia, defined as either as a disorder or as a key feature of
some broader syndrome of sensory intolerance, should be considered as a diagnostic construct in the psy-
chiatric nomenclature. A research roadmap is proposed for the systematic evaluation as to whether miso-
phonia should be considered for future editions of DSM or ICD.
Ó 2017 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Introduction The question of whether misophonia, or some syndrome in


which misophonia is a prominent feature, should be classified as
We live in an era in which the major psychiatric classification a mental disorder is important for several reasons. The recognition
systems, such as the successive editions of DSM, have been steadily of misophonia as a distinct mental disorder, if indeed it is a disor-
expanding in the number of phenomena that are considered to be der, could facilitate recognition of the condition to health-care pro-
mental disorders. This has led to a growing concern that common- viders, raise public awareness, provide information and validation
place quirks, eccentricities, or problems of living are becoming to sufferers (i.e., the positive effects of labeling), and could facili-
over-pathologized and over-diagnosed as mental disorders [1–3]. tate research and treatment. A disadvantage in classifying miso-
Accordingly, there is good reason to be skeptical when some new phonia as a mental disorder is the possibility of stigmatizing and
psychiatric disorder is proposed. The purpose of this article is to over-pathologizing possibly benign eccentricities (i.e., negative
critically examine the evidence for a purportedly new mental dis- effects of labeling). Accordingly, it is important to carefully con-
order, misophonia, characterized by marked distress from hearing sider whether misophonia meets criteria for a mental disorder,
particular sounds. Misophonia is not listed in any of the major psy- and whether there is sufficient evidence for clearly specifying its
chiatric classification systems and, until recently, has received little essential features and delineating the boundaries of any syndrome
attention from psychiatric researchers, having been described of which it might be a part.
almost exclusively in the audiology literature. This article begins by defining misophonia and related concepts,
and by distinguishing it from other phenomena, particularly
phenomena arising from dysfunctions of the primary auditory
⇑ Address: Department of Psychiatry, University of British Columbia, 2255 system. This is followed by a review of the common features of
Wesbrook Mall, Vancouver, BC V6T 2A1, Canada misophonia, as described by case studies, case series, and larger
E-mail address: Steven.Taylor@ubc.ca

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.mehy.2017.05.003
0306-9877/Ó 2017 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
110 S. Taylor / Medical Hypotheses 103 (2017) 109–117

investigations. Theories of misophonia are also discussed. Such patterns in terms of the nature of distressing stimuli and common
theories are relevant to the question of whether misophonia is a reactions to those stimuli. In the sections appearing below, these
mental disorder because if it is such a disorder, then it should arise 19 studies will be collectively referred to as the clinical studies.
from a psychobiological dysfunction as opposed to some other In addition to these studies, another relevant investigation [8]
(e.g., purely audiological) dysfunction. General considerations for included 318 cases of misophonia, but provided little descriptive
diagnosing mental disorders are considered followed by a critical information about the sample. What relevant information that
review of the proposals regarding the diagnostic classification of was provided is discussed later in this article. Another potentially
misophonia. Finally, a roadmap for further research is discussed, relevant study [28] reported secondary analyses on a sample that
including research strategies for evaluating the clinical utility of was described in an earlier investigation [15]. An analogue study
misophonia, or some variant thereof, as a mental disorder. of college students was also identified [29]. Here, questionnaires
assessing misophonia and other phenomena were administered
to 483 college students.
Definition of concepts
In the following sections the results from the 19 clinical studies
and other investigations are summarized. Emphasis is on the clin-
Originally described in the audiology literature, misophonia
ical studies of misophonia patients although the findings from the
(literally ‘‘hatred of sound”) refers to a strong dislike of sounds—
analogue study are also relevant.
particularly oral and nasal sounds produced by other people—
accompanied by unusually intense, distressing emotional reactions
Triggering sounds
[4]. Misophonia has also been called ‘‘selective sound sensitivity
syndrome” and ‘‘soft sound sensitivity syndrome” [5]. The latter
Almost all of the distressing triggering sounds reported by peo-
term emphasizes the finding that some of the distressing sounds
ple with misophonia involved oral or nasal sounds produced by
in misophonia are soft rather than loud.
other people, and were typically more distressing when the sounds
Misophonia can be distinguished from hyperacusis and tinnitus.
where produced by familiar others (e.g., family members) than by
Although there is some confusion in the literature regarding the
strangers. The sounds could be loud or soft. The following were the
precise definition of these concepts [6], hyperacusis is commonly
most commonly reported distressing sounds: Chewing (especially
defined as a reduced tolerance of loud sounds. Hyperacusis is an
with mouth open), crunching of foods, slurping, swallowing, lip
auditory disorder where sounds of normal volume are perceived
smacking, sniffing, throat clearing, and heavy breathing. Other
as uncomfortably loud [7]. Unlike misophonia, in hyperacusis the
sounds were also sometimes reported as distressing: Pen clicking,
sound’s meaning and the context in which it occurs is irrelevant
dogs barking, babies crying, bus brakes, noisy neighbors, cutlery
[8]. The perception of excessive loudness in hyperacusis depends
clinking on plates, clinking glasses, keyboard typing, finger tap-
only on the physical characteristics of the sound (i.e., its spectrum
ping, high-pitched or loud voices, the sound of nail clipping, and
and intensity).
the sound of footsteps. Once an offending sound was noticed, peo-
Tinnitus refers to the perception of a sound (commonly ringing
ple with misophonia typically reported difficulty distracting them-
or hissing) in one or both ears, or seeming to originate from inside
selves from the sound. Sounds such as bird song, running water,
the head, in the absence of an external acoustic source [9]. In com-
the ocean, and rain were rarely reported as distressing in these
parison to tinnitus, misophonia is characterized by intolerance of
studies and in other clinical accounts [8].
highly specific sounds from an external source. Although they are
Consistent with the findings of clinical studies, an analogue
distinguishable phenomena, misophonia, tinnitus, and hyperacusis
study using college students [29] found that oral and nasal sounds
can co-occur, particularly in samples obtained from audiology clin-
produced by others tended to be the most distressing, in addition
ics [5,6].
to repetitive pen or foot tapping. For a minority of participants
(4%), distressing sounds included particular consonants or vowels
Features of misophonia (e.g., ‘‘k” sounds) and 15% were distressed by particular environ-
mental sounds (e.g., clock ticking, refrigerator humming).
Empirical studies
Visual triggers
Descriptive data for the present article were obtained from pub-
lished case studies, case series, and experimental investigations Some people with misophonia also reported that non-auditory
reporting data on cases classified as having misophonia, as identi- stimuli were distressing. These included visual cues to eating; for
fied in searches of PsychInfo and Medline up to March 1, 2017, example, the sight of lips moving or seeing someone chewing
using the search terms ‘‘misophonia”, ‘‘selective sound sensitivity” gum, even though the chewing noise was inaudible. Unpleasant
and ‘‘soft sound sensitivity syndrome”. Reference lists of source sounds tend to be rated as most aversive when accompanied by
articles were also searched, along with review articles. an image corresponding to the sound (e.g., watching a person
A total of 19 misophonia clinical investigations (case studies or chewing loudly while also hearing the chewing sounds) [30].
case series) with relevant data were identified, for a total of N = 247 Accordingly, visual information (e.g., watching someone chewing
cases [5,10–27]. Studies were from the United States (10 studies), with their mouth open) appears to be an amplifying factor for
the United Kingdom (3 studies), Australia (2 studies), Brazil (2 misophonia. A number of misophonic people also reported that
studies), Canada (1 study), and the Netherlands (1 study). The specific movements unrelated to sounds, made by other people,
studies ranged from single case investigations to studies of larger were distressing; for example, the sight of someone jiggling or
groups of individuals, recruited from audiology clinics or from swinging their legs, shaking their foot, or twirling their hair.
mental health settings in which the person was seeking treatment
for misophonia or for some other clinical problem (e.g., obsessive- Emotional reactions
compulsive disorder). None of the cases were diagnosed with aut-
ism spectrum disorder. The relevance of the latter is discussed The most common emotional reactions were anger and irrita-
later. Given the way in which the data were reported, it was not tion, with these emotions being particularly intense when the
possible to extract statistical information (e.g., percentages) for sufferer believed that the person making the offending noise was
the relevant clinical variables. However, it was possible to identify doing so deliberately or was being inconsiderate or had poor
S. Taylor / Medical Hypotheses 103 (2017) 109–117 111

manners. Some people experienced intense rage in response to the Psychiatric comorbidity
triggering sounds. Anxiety and panic were also common reactions,
and some people reported feeling disgust in response to the The clinical studies described a range of findings. A number of
triggering stimuli. Given these emotional reactions it is not studies reported no comorbidity whereas other studies, particu-
surprising that the triggering sounds were associated with larly those drawn from psychiatric settings, reported cases of miso-
elevated autonomic arousal, as indicated by heightened skin phonia comorbid with a number of disorders, including anxiety
conductance responses [24]. and mood disorders, obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD),
obsessive-compulsive personality disorder, tic disorders, and
attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). In contrast, for a
Behavioral responses sample drawn from an audiology clinic [8], the prevalence of psy-
chiatric comorbidity was low; ‘‘we have seen 318 misophonic
Common behavioral reactions involved avoidance or escape patients . . . all evaluated by physicians, and in only 7 cases (2.2%)
from the triggering stimuli. For example, some people with miso- did patients exhibit psychiatric problems” (p. 381). Little further
phonia avoided having meals with family members and avoided information was presented in that report, making it difficult to
places where other people would be eating (e.g., cafeterias). The interpret the findings. For example, it is not known whether the
use of sound-dampening methods was also reported (e.g., earplugs, physicians were psychiatrists or whether a structured diagnostic
listening to music on headphones, or the use of an electric fan to interview was used. It is also unclear why the patients, presenting
generate white noise). to an audiology clinic, were selected for an assessment of mental
Some people with misophonia confronted the person making disorders by a physician.
the offending noise, such as by glaring at the individual or by ver- Wu et al.’s (2014) analogue study of college students [29] found
bal confrontation (e.g., berating, yelling, or, in the case of children that misophonia was correlated with obsessive-compulsive (OC)
and adolescents, tantrums). This led, in some cases, to family symptoms, anxiety, and depressive symptoms, along with general
accommodation in which family members made efforts to avoid sensory sensitivities (e.g., distress triggered by innocuous tactile,
producing the offending sounds or ensured that they did not eat auditory, visual, gustatory, or olfactory stimuli).
in front of the person. Some cases of physical aggression were Clearly more research is needed to better understand the psy-
reported (e.g., throwing objects). To illustrate, in Schröder et al.’s chopathologic correlates of misophonia. At present, all that can
[15] sample of 42 misophonic patients, 29% became verbally be concluded is that misophonia seemingly can occur as a stand-
aggressive on occasion in response to triggers, 17% directed phys- alone clinical condition but also can be associated with a range
ical aggression toward objects, and 12% had hit a partner who had of psychiatric symptoms and disorders.
produced an offending noise.
A number of people with misophonia reported that they delib-
erately mimicked the offending sound as a means of coping. By Demographics, onset, and course
producing the offending sound themself, the person was unable
to hear the same sound produced by others (e.g., eating foods in Based on data from the 19 clinical studies, the cases were
synchrony with another person). Given that the self-production approximately balanced in terms of gender (55% female) and were
the sound was typically not distressing, mimicry apparently served either adult or pediatric cases at the time of assessment. The ana-
to mask the offending sound made by others, at least in some logue study found that the severity of misophonia symptoms was
cases. In other cases, people with misophonia mimicked the unrelated to age and ethnicity [29]. Onset of misophonia was typ-
sounds as a means of retaliation against the offending person pro- ically in childhood or adolescence. Some people with misophonia
ducing the sounds [24]. reported an early triggering event (e.g., noises of family members
eating triggering feelings of disgust). However, it is not clear
whether such events were etiologically significant as triggers or
Insight were simply the earliest recollections of misophonic distress.
The course of misophonia varied. Some sufferers reported that
People with misophonia were typically aware that their reac- the problem abated over time, others reported no change over
tions were excessive [15,24]. They regarded their loss of control time, and still others reported that it had gradually worsened in
(e.g., angry outbursts) as being excessive and morally unacceptable terms of an increasing aversiveness to triggering sounds and a
[15], and yet they also regarded the person producing the offend- growing number of triggers.
ing noises as being rude, inconsiderate, and acting inappropriately
[24].
Prevalence

Functional impairment Little is known about the prevalence of misophonia. Some com-
mentators claimed that it is relatively common [31]. This is consis-
People with misophonia reported strained relationships with tent with the many websites and social media support fora
housemates and family members, and other forms of strained devoted to misophonia. The analogue study reported that 20% of
interpersonal relationships, especially in regard to eating. Social participants had ‘‘clinically significant” misophonia symptoms
isolation was also reported in some cases as a consequence of [29]. That is, symptoms causing significant interference in their
avoiding triggering stimuli. Concentration difficulties and distress lives. The high proportion might have been due to some degree
at work and school settings were described, including dropping of self-selection in participants choosing to enrol in a study on
out of school in order to avoid distressing sounds. Similar findings misophonia. Other estimates, based on extrapolations from cases
were reported in the analogue study [29]. For the latter, the sever- presenting to audiology clinics, suggest a point prevalence of about
ity of misophonia, as measured by a questionnaire, was associated 3% in the general population [32]. Population-based epidemiologic
with impairments in work, school-related functioning, social func- studies are needed in order to draw firm conclusions about the life-
tioning, and family and home functioning. time and point prevalence of misophonia.
112 S. Taylor / Medical Hypotheses 103 (2017) 109–117

Etiological hypotheses is a complex concept that refers to the rapid (20–200 Hz) fluctua-
tions in frequency intensity. It is less clear why the scraping and
If misophonia is to be classified as a mental disorder, then this screeching sounds associated with roughness are widely perceived
carries the assumption that the disorder arises from some type of as unpleasant.
psychobiological dysfunction. Accordingly, etiological hypotheses Is misophonia is simply some kind of exaggerated response to
are relevant as to how misophonia should be classified. inherently aversive sounds; that is, sounds characterized by some
combination of loudness, sharpness, and roughness? In other
Audiological versus psychiatric disorder words, are people with misophonia simply those individuals who
have especially aversive reactions to the sound of nails scraping
If misophonia is to be classified as a disorder, should it be clas- a chalkboard? Some of the distressing sounds reported by people
sified as an audiological or a psychiatric disorder? Misophonia does with misophonia are consistent with this possibility, such as
not have any relation to hearing thresholds; it can present in peo- sounds of eating popcorn, crunching on toast, and gum snapping,
ple with normal hearing [4] and can occur in cases of hearing loss which have rough and sharp properties. Other lines of evidence
[8]. Although misophonia can be comorbid with tinnitus and suggest that misophonia is more than simply a heightened sensi-
hyperacusis, it often occurs in the absence of any identifiable tivity to sounds that are loud, sharp, or rough. As illustrated by
pathology to the peripheral or central auditory system [33]. Given the clinical examples presented earlier in this article, misophonia
the highly specific nature of the disturbing sounds (e.g., distur- is not limited to an aversion to sounds that are loud, sharp, or
bance specific to soft chewing sounds made by family members), rough; some people report that they are distressed only by highly
misophonia seems unlikely to arise from some simple disturbance specific sounds, including soft sounds such as the sound of slurping
to the auditory system. According to Jastreboff and Jastreboff [8], and lip smacking.
who coined the term misophonia and pioneered the original work
on the topic: Heightened disgust sensitivity and misophonia

The proposed mechanisms of misophonia involve a high level of


Some of the distressing sounds for people with misophonia are
activation of the limbic and autonomic nervous systems due to
disgust related; for example, chewing, lip smacking, belching,
enhanced functional connections between the auditory system
There are individual differences in disgust sensitivity; that is, some
and other systems in the brain (mainly the limbic and auto-
people have more aversive reactions than other people in response
nomic nervous systems for specific patterns of sound only) . . .
to disgust related stimuli [42]. Accordingly, this suggests that
At the same time the auditory system functions normally. (p.
heightened disgust might play a role in misophonia. Disgust sensi-
383)
tivity is indeed correlated with misophonia and might be a con-
tributing factor [43]. However, disgust sensitivity is in itself
Consistent with this proposal, neuroimaging research suggests insufficient to explain misophonia in general, because many people
that the amygdala plays a role in modulating the auditory cortex with misophonia do not have elevated disgust sensitivity [43].
[34]. Anatomical regions implicated in misophonia are also conjec-
tured to include structures that are activated by highly processed Learned associations and interpretations
sounds; that is, located after auditory information has been sub-
jected to considerable processing and selection, such as the inferior The importance of psychological factors in misophonia has been
temporal lobe [35]. suggested in several investigations [6,15,26,44]. Misophonia is
The intensity of the misophonic emotion reaction is said to characterized as a negative reaction to sounds with a specific pat-
depend ‘‘predominantly on a patient’s previous experience with a tern and meaning to the person; the reactions to the sound are
given sound (or type of sounds), the context in which sound is pre- thought to depend on non-auditory factors such as the person’s
sented, as well as a patient’s psychological profile” ([36], p. 27). previous evaluation of that sound and the context in which the
In summary, the available evidence and current conjectures sound is presented, and beliefs about social decorum regarding
suggest that misophonia is better regarded as a psychiatric prob- eating-related sounds [6]. Cultural factors may play a role in shap-
lem rather than a simple audiological problem. ing the meaning attributed to sounds. The slurping of foods, for
example, is considered a sign of bad manners in Western cultures
Is misophonia simply an exaggerated response to inherently aversive but is socially acceptable in Asian cultures [45].
sounds? Reid et al. [26] argued that ‘‘misophonia may be better concep-
tualized as a psychological condition rather than a pure sensory
Some sounds are widely regarded as pleasant (e.g., the sound of sensitivity when . . . clear behavioral triggers, cognitive distortions,
rain or the roar of the ocean) whereas other sounds are widely con- and operantly reinforced maladaptive behaviors exist” (p. 7). From
sidered to be aversive (e.g., screaming infants, squeaking Styro- this perspective, a person’s interpretation of the meaning of partic-
foam packaging, or screeching nails on a chalkboard) [37–39]. ular sounds is important (e.g., ‘‘Eating with your mouth open is bad
For a wide range of everyday sounds, people show considerable manners and a sign that you are a disgusting pig”). Interpretations
agreement as to what constitutes an annoying sound. Three prop- influence the person’s emotional reactions (e.g., anger) and behav-
erties characterize sounds that are widely regarded as annoying: ioral responses (avoidance, escape, confrontation). Moral judg-
Loudness, sharpness, and roughness [40]. People differ in their toler- ments, possibly fueled by heightened disgust sensitivity, may be
ance for loud sounds, but extremely loud sounds are almost uni- relevant in triggering distressing responses to some nasal and mas-
versally perceived as aversive, and are damaging to the auditory ticatory stimuli. It is less clear why other particular stimuli would
system. The sharpness of a sound refers to the proportion of sound be distressing, such as the sound of breathing or the sounds of key-
energy at high-frequencies, with sharper sounds (2–4 kHz) being board typing.
generally perceived to be less pleasant. The perceptually annoying Schröder et al.’s [15] patients commonly reported that the onset
effects of sharpness are thought to be because the ear is particu- of the problem was associated with a profound disgust of hearing
larly sensitive to sounds in the range of 2–4 kHz and can be dam- family members eating during childhood. This lead Schröder and
aged by loud sounds at in this frequency range [41]. Roughness, colleagues to speculate that ‘‘a process of recurrent conditioning
which is a characteristic of many scraping and screeching sounds, following these repetitive annoying events that eventually results
S. Taylor / Medical Hypotheses 103 (2017) 109–117 113

in misophonic symptoms” (p. 4). It is unclear, however, as to why investigation to establish the diagnostic validity of a given disorder.
such events would lead to distress. A more typical response to Here, the goal was to identify homogeneous clinical groups:
recurrent minor stressors, as demonstrated clinically and in exper-
imental learning studies, is for habituation to occur [46]. In other 1. Clinical description of the disorder, which might be a single fea-
words, recurrent exposure to the sounds of family members eating ture or combination of features that defines a given disorder.
should lead to less rather than more distress. Demographics, age of onset, and chronicity can be used to
define the clinical picture more precisely.
Comment 2. Laboratory studies, including biological and psychological stud-
ies, can be used to define a disorder more precisely (e.g., neu-
It appears unlikely that misophonia arises from some simple ropsychological tests can be used to diagnose neurocognitive
dysfunction in the primary auditory system. Higher-level auditory disorders). The ultimate goal is to develop laboratory tests that
processing seems important, in which the meaning of the acoustic can be used to diagnose psychiatric disorders. Few such tests
stimulus is important. This has led several investigators to argue are currently available for the disorders listed in ICD-10 and
that psychobiological factors play an important role. This is consis- DSM-5.
tent with the view that misophonia, or some syndrome in which 3. Distinction from other disorders (e.g., evidence that a given dis-
misophonia is a prominent feature, should be considered as a pos- order can reliably distinguished from other disorders).
sible mental disorder. 4. Followup studies to determine whether outcome is homoge-
neous or heterogeneous (e.g., do some cases of a given disorder
resolve over time while other cases show a deteriorating
Diagnostic considerations
pattern?)
5. Family studies. Here, the assumption is that ‘‘the finding of an
There are several important considerations in deciding whether
increased prevalence of the same disorder among the close rel-
or not a particular diagnostic construct should be included in a
atives of the original patients strongly indicates that one is deal-
diagnostic classification system.
ing with a valid entity” ([50], p. 984).

General criteria for defining mental disorders: ICD-10 and DSM-5 Despite over four decades of research, it has proven difficult to
establish the diagnostic validity of many diagnostic categories in
According to ICD-10, a mental disorder defines the existence of ICD and DSM [51]. Some diagnoses might not refer to real entities
a clinically recognizable constellation of signs or symptoms that but yet still serve a pragmatic, clinically useful function [52]. An
tends to be associated with distress and interference in personal alternative to ICD and DSM has been recently proposed, called
functioning [47]. DSM-5 expands on this definition to specify three the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC; [53]). The goal here is to iden-
cardinal features of a mental disorder [48]: (a) clinically significant tify and investigate the mechanisms of basic systems, circuits, and
signs and symptoms (i.e., clinically significant disturbances in cog- processes that may underlie various types of psychopathology
nition, emotion, or behavior), (b) evidence suggesting that the (e.g., brain reward systems, threat processing systems).
signs and symptoms arise from some type of psychobiological dys-
function, and (c) the signs and symptoms are associated with sig-
nificant distress or disability (e.g., social or occupational). Diagnostic considerations for misophonia

Clinical utility RDoC and misophonia

The developers of modern classification systems have stressed If the promise of RDoC is fulfilled and it becomes viable as a
the importance of clinical utility. For example, the introductory clinical diagnostic system, the most likely result is that misophonia
text in DSM–5 [48] stresses that clinical utility was one of the main (and perhaps many other disorders classified in ICD and DSM)
considerations in making changes to the fourth edition of the DSM. would not be diagnostic classifications. Instead the diagnosis
That is, a diagnosis should imply (a) a prognosis if the disorder is would be based on the underlying mechanisms; for example, a
not treated (e.g., is the disorder likely to be transient or chronic?), diagnostic formulation might be ‘‘Mr. A. presents with intense dis-
(b) implications about treatment plans (e.g., what types of treat- tress when he hears other people chewing. Biometric assessment
ments will or will not be appropriate?), and (c) imply information reveals dysregulation in brain circuits x, y, and z.”
about likely outcomes of treatment. That is, a diagnostic label Currently, the relative merits of RDoC over ICD and DSM remain
should convey information to clinicians about what the present to be demonstrated, and RDoC presently exists as a research frame-
problems are (or aren’t) and how they might best be treated. Diag- work under construction, not a clinical tool. Although RDoC might
nostic classifications also have clinical utility if they facilitate com- eventually prove useful for understanding misophonia, for the time
munication among users of the system (professionals, researchers, being a classification scheme based on clinical descriptions (i.e., a
patients, families, administrators, etc.), and facilitate the conceptu- system based on ICD or DSM) is clinically more useful for identify-
alization and understanding of psychiatric disorders [49]. ing and treating misophonia.

Diagnostic validity Schröder’s diagnostic criteria for misophonia

An issue of ongoing debate is whether the current diagnostic Using an approach to diagnosis based on clinical description,
categories in ICD-10 and DSM-5 describe etiologic entities that Schröder et al. [15] proposed that misophonia should be regarded
exist in nature or whether they simply describe patterns of signs as a new, discrete psychiatric disorder. ‘‘Discrete” refers to the pro-
and symptoms for which a given pattern might or might not have posal that misophonia be classified as a stand-alone disorder and
a specific etiology. Various criteria have been proposed for validat- not simply an associated feature of some other disorder. This claim
ing psychiatric diagnostic classifications. Among the most influen- was based on the finding that in Schröder et al.’s sample of 42
tial systems for establishing diagnostic validity was proposed by patients, misophonia was distressing, associated with functional
Robins and Guze in 1970 [50]. They proposed five types of impairment, could occur in the absence of other forms of
114 S. Taylor / Medical Hypotheses 103 (2017) 109–117

psychopathology, and was not attributable to psychiatric disorders Breadth of syndrome


described in DSM and ICD. Schröder’s diagnostic criteria for miso-
phonia are listed in Table 1. Note that these criteria exclude cases If misophonia is regarded as a key feature of some clinical psy-
in which misophonia can be attributed to some other disorder, chiatric syndrome, then the question arises as to the breadth of the
such as autism spectrum disorder, which can be associated with syndrome. Is misophonia the only form of sensory intolerance, as
various forms of sensory intolerance [54]. implied by the Schröder’s criteria, or is it part of a broader pattern
Schröder’s diagnostic criteria were proposed in order to ‘‘offi- of intolerance for sensory stimuli (in people who do not have an
cially” recognize patients with the disorder, to improve identifica- autism spectrum disorder)?
tion of misophonia by health care professionals, and to encourage In a recent study we used latent class analysis to investigate
scientific research. A further reason for defining misophonia as a whether misophonia, sampled from people in the general popula-
distinct mental disorder was that if it was diagnosed according tion, represents a distinct category [43]. As part of this research we
to the current classificatory systems, the resulting diagnosis would conducted a preliminary investigation as to the breadth of the syn-
be vague and uninformative. In DSM-5, for example, misophonia drome. A statistical method known as latent class modeling was
would most likely be diagnosed as Other Specified Mental Disorder implemented, using variables assessing intolerance of sounds
(code 300.9). This vague diagnostic category applies to ‘‘presenta- (e.g., chewing) and intolerance of tactile sensations (e.g., sticky or
tions in which symptoms characteristic of a mental disorder that greasy substances, or tactile sensations from clothing tags). The
cause clinically significant distress or impairment in social, occupa- sample (N = 534) was drawn from the general population. Results
tional, or other important areas of functioning predominate but do indicated two classes of individuals; the first class (n = 150) con-
not meet the full criteria for any specific mental disorder” ([48], p. sisted of people who were intolerant of both auditory and tactile
708). stimuli, and the second, larger class (n = 384) consisted of people
who were relatively undisturbed by these stimuli. Sensory-
intolerant individuals, compared to those who were comparatively
sensory tolerant, had greater scores on indices of general psy-
Criticisms of Schröder’s criteria
chopathology, more severe OC symptoms, a higher likelihood of
meeting caseness criteria for OCD, a greater tendency to report
Schröder’s criteria have been subject to several criticisms:
OC-related phenomena (e.g., a greater frequency of tics), higher
scores on measures of disgust sensitivity, and greater impairment
1. The triggering sounds need not be of human origin, as discussed
on indices of social and occupational functioning. Sensory-
earlier in this article; the sounds can sometimes be produced by
intolerant individuals had significantly higher scores on OC
animals (e.g., dogs barking) or by machines [31].
symptoms even after controlling for general psychopathology.
2. The criteria overemphasize anger as an emotional reaction.
The categorical nature of misophonia (i.e., present vs. absent) is
Anxiety and panic are often reported [21], as noted above.
consistent with the prototypic conceptualization of mental
3. Only a fraction of people with misophonia experience a ‘‘pro-
disorders as diagnostic categories. Overall, the findings provided
found sense of loss of self-control”; most report considerable
suggestive evidence for a sensory intolerance syndrome in which
distress at the triggering sounds and try to cope by means of
misophonia is a cardinal feature. Although the sensory intolerant
escape or avoidance. Aggressive outbursts are more commonly
latent class combined two types of sensory intolerance, both types
found in younger samples, such as children or adolescents [31].
of intolerance were associated with OC symptoms. The findings are
4. Although the person may recognize that his or her emotional
consistent with Schröder’s et al.’s [15] proposal that misophonia be
reaction is excessive, some people with misophonia regard their
classified as an OC-related (spectrum) disorder. The latter is a
reactions are justified because they believe that the person
group of disorders that include OCD, hoarding disorder, body
making the offending sound is behaving in a rude or disrespect-
dysmorphic disorder, trichotillomania, and other disorders [48].
ful manner [31].
However, our results also suggest that misophonia is not so
5. Schröder’s criteria may have been based on an unrepresentative
narrowly defined as in the Schröder criteria; the result suggest that
of people with misophonia. Their sample, recruited from a psy-
misophonia is part of a broader pattern of sensory intolerance.
chiatric clinic, had a high degree of psychiatric comorbidity
Further research is needed to firmly establish the boundaries of
(e.g., 52% had comorbid obsessive-compulsive personality dis-
the syndrome.
order). In comparison, such comorbidity is reportedly rare in
A related issue is whether misophonia is different from Sensory
audiological clinics. For example, Jastreboff and Jastreboff [55]
Over-Responsivity (SOR). The latter is a clinical condition seen in
reported that only 5% of their 184 misophonia patients had
childhood and is associated with autism spectrum disorder [56].
comorbid psychopathology.
The question is whether misophonia in non-autistic children and

Table 1
Schröder et al.’s (2013) diagnostic criteria for misophonia.

A. The presence or anticipation of a specific sound, produced by a human being (e.g. eating sounds, breathing sounds), provokes an impulsive aversive physical reaction
which starts with irritation or disgust that instantaneously becomes anger
B. This anger initiates a profound sense of loss of self-control with rare but potentially aggressive outbursts
C. The person recognizes that the anger or disgust is excessive, unreasonable, or out of proportion to the circumstances or the provoking stressor
D. The individual tends to avoid the misophonic situation, or if he/she does not avoid it, endures encounters with the misophonic sound situation with intense
discomfort, anger or disgust
E. The individual’s anger, disgust or avoidance causes significant distress (i.e. it bothers the person that he or she has the anger or disgust) or significant interference in
the person’s day-to-day life. For example, the anger or disgust may make it difficult for the person to perform important tasks at work, meet new friends, attend
classes, or interact with others
F. The person’s anger, disgust, and avoidance are not better explained by another disorder, such as obsessive-compulsive disorder (e.g. disgust in someone with an
obsession about contamination) or post-traumatic stress disorder (e.g. avoidance of stimuli associated with a trauma related to threatened death, serious injury or
threat to the physical integrity of self or others)

Source: Schröder et al. (2013), doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0054706.t002.


S. Taylor / Medical Hypotheses 103 (2017) 109–117 115

adults is part of a SOR syndrome. SOR is characterized by intense sufferer, the naming of the problem as ‘‘misophonia” may help suf-
distress (e.g., irritability or anger outbursts) by sensory stimula- ferers to better understand their problem, and with this insight
tion, such as tactile stimuli (e.g., certain fabrics or tags in clothing) they may be better able to cope with, or tolerate, triggering sounds.
and particular auditory stimuli (e.g., sirens) [57]. Clinically signifi-
For me, one of the most frustrating aspects of misophonia is
cant SOR (i.e., at a severity in which it interferes with daily activi-
what I call the ‘‘incredulity factor.” For years, I could not believe
ties), is reportedly found in 5–21% of school-age children [58,59].
that my friends and relatives were not getting as upset at what I
SOR, like misophonia, can be comorbid with OCD [60] but also
considered rude behaviors. They were getting frustrated with
can be a stand-alone disorder [59]. Problems with the concept of
me for focusing on sounds they did not really hear. One of the
SOR have been described, including the lack of firmly established
advantages of defining misophonia is that it reminds those with
criteria for diagnosis and differential diagnosis, and concerns about
the condition that only a small percentage of the population—
its diagnostic validity [59,61].
no one knows the exact number—is affected. As one commenta-
It is not clear whether the trigger stimuli associated with SOR
tor wrote online, ‘‘I had to learn this is MY problem, not the
are the same as those of misophonia (i.e., oral or nasal sounds). A
problem of other people.”
problem is that many studies have described sensory over-
[63]
responsivity only in general terms, without providing sufficient
information to determine whether participants with SOR were dis-
tressed by the oral and nasal triggering sounds that typify miso- To have clinical utility, a diagnosis of misophonia should imply
phonia. However, prototypic descriptions of SOR suggest a some form of treatment. So far this issue has been discussed only
general hypersensitivity to loud sounds, rather than a sensitivity in a poorly described report of an uncontrolled trial. The treatment
to the often soft sounds associated with misophonia. In this regard, involved counseling (education) about the nature of misophonia
SOR resembles the concept of ‘‘sensory-processing sensitivity”, in combined with systematic exposure to triggering sounds (i.e., a
which highly sensitive people become overaroused and over- form of desensitization therapy) [55]. In an open trial of 184 miso-
whelmed by sensory inputs such as strong smells, loud noises, phonia patients, 83% were rated as being significantly improved
bright lights, and strong tastes [62]. Further research is needed to after receiving this form of therapy [8]. Unfortunately, little infor-
address the distinction between SOR and misophonia. mation was provided about the methodology of the open trial. No
information was presented on the outcome measures, on the crite-
Diagnostic threshold ria for defining a patient as being ‘‘significantly improved”, or
about whether the assessors were blind as to whether patients
The case studies indicate that misophonia ranges in severity were being assessed before or after they had received treatment.
from mild annoyance or distress about triggering sounds, to Despite the limited information provided in the report of the open
intense anger or panic, sometimes accompanied by verbal or phys- trial, the results are encouraging and provide a justification for
ical aggression. Regardless of the specific criteria for diagnosing conducting randomized controlled treatment studies.
misophonia, the question arises as to the threshold of severity that
should be used to diagnose misophonia as a clinically significant Summary
condition. Lenient thresholds for conditions such as, for example,
social anxiety disorder have been criticized because they over- Theory and research suggests that misophonia, or some syn-
pathologize commonplace problems of living (e.g., (2)). Accord- drome in which misophonia is a key feature, may represent a
ingly, before misophonia can become a serious candidate as a cat- new mental disorder. At this stage, however, there are several
egory of mental disorder there needs to be empirical studies of important areas of uncertainty, which make it premature to pro-
severity thresholds; for example, is there an empirically definable pose diagnostic criteria. Important areas of uncertainty involve
threshold (as determined, for example, by latent class analysis) the essential features (e.g., key emotional reactions to triggers),
for which people who are above a particular severity of misophonia syndromal breadth, and severity threshold of a syndrome in which
are at heightened risk for adverse events such as job loss, social iso- misophonia is a key feature. Other issues concerning diagnostic
lation, or contact with the legal system as a result of angry out- validity and clinical utility also need to be further investigated.
bursts in response to triggering sounds?
Diagnostic threshold can be defined in a variety of ways. Empir- A roadmap for future research
ical research is needed to determine the most useful criteria for
defining a threshold, using criteria such as the above-mentioned Development and refinement of assessment instruments
adverse events. For misophonia, the diagnostic threshold could
be defined in terms of any or all of the following: (a) the severity In order to establish whether misophonia is a stand-alone disor-
of the emotional response (e.g., anger, rage, panic, intense disgust), der or part of a broader syndrome of sensory intolerance, it is
(b) severity of behavioral reactions (e.g., complete avoidance of important to develop reliable, valid measures of misophonia and
social situations in which people are eating, or aggressive behav- other forms of sensory intolerance. Questionnaire measures have
ioral responses to people making the triggering sounds), or (c) been developed for research purposes [29,43], and a structured
number of situations or stimuli that trigger distress (e.g., distress clinical interview has been developed, adapted from a measure of
evoked only by family members eating vs. distress evoked by a OC symptoms [15]. The psychometric properties of these instru-
wide range of sounds). ments have yet to be firmly established. Most measures have
focused fairly narrowly on misophonia, without providing a
Clinical utility of misophonia as a diagnosis detailed assessment of other kinds of sensory intolerance. To
accomplish the task of determining the breadth of a syndrome in
Regardless of how it is precisely defined, there is anecdotal evi- which misophonia might play a part, it is important to develop
dence that misophonia may have clinical utility as a diagnostic reliable, valid measures of many different forms of sensory intoler-
construct. Sufferers may find it reassuring that they are not alone ance, including auditory and tactile intolerance, along with visual
in their problems with triggering sounds, that this problem is intolerance (e.g., distress evoked by witnessing someone fidget-
known to at least some clinicians, and that the problem has a ing), and olfactory intolerance (e.g., distress evoked by common-
name. As suggested by the following personal account from one place odors).
116 S. Taylor / Medical Hypotheses 103 (2017) 109–117

Future studies of diagnostic validity targets misophonic symptoms, such as the intervention consisting
of counseling (education) and graduated exposure developed by
The diagnostic validity of misophonia, based on Robins and Jastreboff and Jastreboff [55].
Guze’s five types of studies [50], remains to be investigated. The A diagnosis of misophonia would still have clinical utility even
following sections discuss each of these as they apply to misopho- if treatment research was to show that misophonia is simply one of
nia. They are relevant to establishing diagnostic validity, if a DSM- many distressing clinical conditions that respond to generic treat-
like system is used, and are relevant for understanding etiology, ments such as selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) or
which is relevant to RDoC. transdiagnostic cognitive-behavior therapy (TCBT). SSRIs are useful
in treating a range of different emotional disorders [65]. Similarly,
Clinical description: studies of syndrome boundary and breadth TCBT has been shown to be efficacious for treating a range of emo-
With the development of reliable and valid assessment instru- tional disorders. Here, patients with ostensibly different mental
ments, researchers can delineate the breadth and nature of a syn- disorders can be efficaciously treated with the same basic
drome in which misophonia is a key feature. That is, it will be cognitive-behavioral protocol [66]. If misophonia can be success-
possible to determine whether misophonia is a discrete clinical fully ameliorated with these generic, nonspecific therapies, then
entity or whether it is part of a broader syndrome of sensory intol- misophonia might still have clinical utility, similar to the way in
erance. Statistical methods, such as latent class analysis, can be which the diagnoses for some infections (e.g., pharyngitis) have
used to investigate this issue, as was done in our preliminary clinical utility even though the corresponding treatment is non-
investigation of this issue [43]. specific (e.g., broad-spectrum antibiotics). Randomized controlled
Studies are also required to investigate the psychopathological trials are needed to investigate the treatment of misophonia,
and demographic correlates of misophonia. The prevalence of including comparisons of treatments that are specific to misopho-
misophonia remains unknown and it is also not know whether nia versus generic or transdiagnostic interventions.
the prevalence varies with age, gender, or cultural background.
Given the evidence of cultural differences concerning the social Conclusion
acceptability of oral sounds (e.g., sounds of food slurping), the
question arises as to whether misophonia is a relatively culture- Misophonia meets many of the general criteria for a mental dis-
bound syndrome, being more common in Western cultures. order and there is preliminary evidence as to its clinical utility.
Schröder et al. [15] proposed that misophonia be regarded as a
Laboratory studies new diagnostic category, related to OC disorders. A review of the
The role of audiological testing also requires further investiga- evidence indicates that this proposal is premature, and that
tion. Misophonia can be comorbid with tinnitus and hyperacusis Schröder’s proposed diagnostic criteria overstate the importance
[8]. The question remains as to whether there are subtypes of of anger, to the relative neglect of other emotional reactions, and
misophonia (i.e., with or without hearing impairment) and fail to adequately capture the clinical phenomena that fall under
whether there are important implications in terms of etiology the rubric of misophonia, as described in the empirical literature.
and treatment. Before misophonia can be serious considered as a possible new
mental disorder, several important uncertainties need to be
Distinction from other disorders resolved, including questions concerning the syndromal bound-
As noted above, research is needed to determine how misopho- aries and breadth of a disorder in which misophonia is a key fea-
nia is related to, or differs from, SOR. ture, and the question of whether misophonia is merely a feature
of some broader syndrome such as SOR. The diagnostic validity
Followup and family studies and clinical utility of a diagnosis of misophonia also require further
Little is known about the pattern (or patterns) of long-term out- investigation. Before adding yet another putative disorder to the
come of misophonia. Research is required to investigate this issue ever-growing list of psychiatric diagnoses, it is important to care-
and to investigate patterns of family aggregation. If misophonia is fully consider whether such an addition is warranted.
an OC-spectrum disorder, as suggested by Schröder and colleagues
[15], then behavioral-genetic (twin) studies should show that
misophonia and OC symptoms have genetic factors in common. Declaration of interests
Behavioral-genetic studies have found that the major types of OC
symptoms (e.g., washing, checking) have genetic and environmen- The author declares no conflict of interest.
tal etiological factors in common with one another, and that these
factors may also be common to other OC-like phenomena [64]. References
Accordingly, the question arises as to whether misophonia is
[1] Horwitz AV, Wakefield JC. The loss of sadness: how psychiatry transformed
shaped by the same genetic and environmental etiologic factors normal sorrow into depressive disorder. New York: Oxford University Press;
that also influence OC symptoms. If this is found to be the case, 2007.
then it would support Schröder et al.’s conjecture that misophonia [2] Lane C. Shyness: how normal behavior became a sickness. New Haven, CT: Yale
University Press; 2008.
is an OC-related disorder. [3] McNally RJ. What is mental illness? Cambridge. MA: Belknap Press; 2012.
[4] Jastreboff MM, Jastreboff PJ. Decreased sound tolerance and tinnitus retraining
Studies of clinical utility therapy (TRT). Aust NZ J Audiol 2002;24:74–81.
[5] Bruxner G. ‘‘Mastication rage”: a review of misophonia—An under-recognised
symptom of psychiatric relevance? Aust Psychiat 2016;24:195–7.
Further research is needed to firmly establish the clinical utility [6] Aazh H, McFerran D, Salvi R, et al. Insights from the First International
of misophonia as a diagnostic construct. Anecdotal evidence sug- Conference on Hyperacusis: Causes, evaluation, diagnosis and treatment. Noise
Health 2014;16:123–6.
gests that labeling a person’s problems as ‘‘misophonia” might
[7] Sheldrake J, Diehl PU, Schaette R. Audiometric characteristics of hyperacusis
have a positive effect, but this needs to be empirically established. patients. Front Neurol 2015;6. 105.
Clinical utility can also be investigated by conducting randomized, [8] Jastreboff PJ, Jastreboff MM. Decreased sound tolerance: hyperacusis,
controlled treatment studies. The clinical utility of a diagnosis of misophonia, diplacousis, and polyacousis. Handbook Clin Neurol
2015;129:375–87.
misophonia would be demonstrated, for example, by showing that [9] Hayes SH, Radziwon KE, Stolzberg DJ, et al. Behavioral models of tinnitus and
it is most effectively treatment by an intervention that specifically hyperacusis in animals. Front Neurol 2014;5:179.
S. Taylor / Medical Hypotheses 103 (2017) 109–117 117

[10] Dozier TH. Counterconditioning treatment for misophonia. Clin Case Studies [38] Ely DJ. Aversiveness without pain: potentiation of imaginal and auditory
2015;14:374–87. effects of blackboard screeches. Bull Psychonomic Soc 1975;6:295–6.
[11] Hazen EP, Reichert EL, Piacentini JC, et al. Case series: sensory intolerance as a [39] Halpren DL, Blake R, Hillenbrand J. Psychoacoustics of a chilling sound. Percept
primary symptom of pediatric OCD. Ann Clin Psychiatry 2008;20:199–203. Psychophys 1986;39:77–80.
[12] Webber TA, Johnson PL, Storch EA. Pediatric misophonia with comorbid [40] McDermott JH. Auditory preferences and aesthetics: Music, voices, and
obsessive–compulsive spectrum disorders. Gen Hosp Psychiatry 2014;36: everyday sounds. In: R. J. Dolan, Sharot T, editors. Neuroscience of
e1–2. preference and choice. London: Elsevier; 2012. p. 227–56.
[13] Veale D. A compelling desire for deafness. J Deaf Stud Deaf Ed [41] Henoch MA, Chesky K. Ear canal resonance as a risk factor in music-induced
2006;11:369–72. hearing loss. Med Probs Perf Artists 1999;14:103–6.
[14] Schwartz P, Leyendecker J, Conlon M. Hyperacusis and misophonia: the lesser- [42] Olatunji BO, Broman-Fulks JJ. A taxometric study of the latent structure of
known siblings of tinnitus. Minnesota Med 2011;94:42–3. disgust sensitivity: converging evidence for dimensionality. Psychol Assess
[15] Schröder A, Vulink N, Denys D. Misophonia: diagnostic criteria for a new 2007;19:437–48.
psychiatric disorder. PLoS ONE 2013;8(1):e54706. [43] Taylor S, Conelea CA, McKay D, et al. Sensory intolerance: latent structure and
[16] Neal M, Cavanna AE. Selective sound sensitivity syndrome (misophonia) in a psychopathologic correlates. Compr Psychiatry 2014;55:1279–84.
patient with Tourette syndrome. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2013;25. [44] Webber TA, Storch EA. Toward a theoretical model of misophonia. Gen Hosp
E01-E. Psychiatry 2015;37:369–70.
[17] McGuire JF, Wu MS, Storch EA. Cognitive-behavioral therapy for 2 youths with [45] Kushner B. Slurp! A social and cultural history of Ramen – Japan’s favorite
misophonia. J Clin Psychiatry 2015;76:573–4. noodle soup. The Netherlands: Brill; 2012.
[18] Kumar S, Hancock O, Cope T, et al. Misophonia: a disorder of emotion [46] Bouton ME. Learning and behavior. MA: Sinauer; 2007.
processing of sounds. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2014;85:1 (Article e3). [47] WHO. ICD-10 classification of mental and behavioural disorder: Clinical
[19] Kuehn S. Misophonia: An exploration of the experience, symptoms, and descriptions and diagnostic guidelines. Geneva: Author; 1992.
treatment of selective sound sensitivity. Doctoral dissertation: John F. [48] American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
Kennedy University; 2016. disorders. 5th ed. Arlington, VA: Author; 2013.
[20] Kluckow H, Telfer J, Abraham S. Should we screen for misophonia in patients [49] Keeley JW, Reed GM, Roberts MC, et al. Developing a science of clinical utility
with eating disorders? A report of three cases. Int J Eating Disord in diagnostic classification systems field study strategies for ICD-11 mental
2014;47:558–61. and behavioral disorders. Am Psychol 2016;71:3–16.
[21] Johnson PL, Webber TA, Wu MS, et al. When selective audiovisual stimuli [50] Robins E, Guze SB. Establishment of diagnostic validity in psychiatric illness:
become unbearable: a case series on pediatric misophonia. Neuropsychiatry its application to schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 1970;126:983–7.
2013;3:569–75. [51] Zachar P, Jablensky A. The concept of validation in psychiatry and psychology.
[22] Hadjipavlou G, Baer S, Lau A, et al. Selective sound intolerance and emotional In: P. Zachar, D. St. Stoyanov, M. Aragona, et al., editors. Alternative
distress: what every clinician should hear. Psychosom Med 2008;70:739–40. perspectives on psychiatric validation. New York: Oxford University Press;
[23] Ferreira GM, Harrison BJ, Fontenelle LF. Hatred of sounds: misophonic disorder 2015. p. 3–24.
or just an underreported psychiatric symptom? Ann Clin Psychiatry [52] Rodrigues AC, Banzato CE. Reality and utility unbound: An argument for dual-
2013;25:271–4. track nosologic validation. In: P. Zachar, D. St. Stoyanov, M. Aragona, et al.,
[24] Edelstein M, Brang D, Rouw R, et al. Misophonia: Physiological investigations editors. Alternative perspectives on psychiatric validation. New York: Oxford
and case descriptions. Frontiers Hum Neurosci 2013;7:1–11. University Press; 2015. p. 47–59.
[25] Bernstein RE, Angell KL, Dehle CM. A brief course of cognitive behavioural [53] Cuthbert BN. Research domain criteria: toward future psychiatric nosologies.
therapy for the treatment of misophonia: a case example. Cog Beh Ther 2013;6 Dialogues Clin Neurosci 2015;17:89–97.
(e10):1–13. [54] Reynolds S, Lane SJ. Diagnostic validity of sensory over-responsivity: a review
[26] Reid AM, Guzick AG, Gernand A, et al. Intensive cognitive-behavioral therapy of the literature and case reports. J Autism Dev Disord 2008;38:516–29.
for comorbid misophonic and obsessive-compulsive symptoms: a systematic [55] Jastreboff PJ, Jastreboff MM. Treatments for decreased sound tolerance
case study. J Obsessive-Compulsive Rel Disord 2016;10:1–9. (hyperacusis and misophonia). Sem Hearing 2014;35:105–20.
[27] Prado-Barreto VM, Salvatori R, Santos RC, et al. Hearing status in adult [56] Robertson AE, Simmons DR. The relationship between sensory sensitivity and
individuals with lifetime, untreated isolated growth hormone deficiency. autistic traits in the general population. J Autism Dev Disord 2013;43:775–84.
Otolaryngology 2014;150:464–71. [57] Miller LJ, Anzalone ME, Lane SJ, et al. Concept evolution in sensory integration:
[28] Schröder A, van Diepen R, Mazaheri A, et al. Diminished n1 auditory evoked a proposed nosology for diagnosis. Am J Occup Ther 2007;61:135–40.
potentials to oddball stimuli in misophonia patients. Front Beh Neurosci [58] Ben-Sasson A, Carter AS, Briggs-Gowan MJ. The development of sensory over-
2014;8:123. responsivity from infancy to elementary school. J Abnorm Child Psychol
[29] Wu MS, Lewin AB, Murphy TK, et al. Misophonia: incidence, phenomenology, 2010;38:1193–202.
and clinical correlates in an undergraduate student sample. J Clin Psychol [59] Carter AS, Ben-Sasson A, Briggs-Gowan MJ. Sensory over-responsivity,
2014;70:994–1007. psychopathology, and family impairment in school-aged children. J Am Acad
[30] Cox TJ. The effect of visual stimuli on the horribleness of awful sounds. Appl Child Adol Psychiatry 2011;50:1210–9.
Acoust 2008;69:691–703. [60] Lewin AB, Wu MS, Murphy TK, et al. Sensory over-responsivity in pediatric
[31] Cavanna AE, Seri S. Misophonia: current perspectives. Neuropsychiatr Dis obsessive compulsive disorder. J Psychopathol Beh Assess 2015;37:134–43.
Treat 2015;11:2117–23. [61] Rogers C, Luby J. Sensory over-responsivity: a diagnosis whose time has come?
[32] McFerran D. Misophonia and phonophobia. In: Baguley DM, Fagelson M, J Am Acad Child Adol Psychiatry 2011;50:1205–7.
Baguley DM, et al., editors. Tinnitus: clinical and research perspectives. San [62] Aron EN, Aron A. Sensory-processing sensitivity and its relation to introversion
Diego: Plural Publishing; 2016. p. 245–59. and emotionality. J Pers Soc Psychol 1997;73:345–68.
[33] Duddy DF, Oeding KA. Misophonia: an overview. Sem Hearing 2014;35:84–91. [63] Lerner B. Please stop making that noise. New York Times. 2015;http://well.
[34] Kumar S, von Kriegstein K, Friston K, et al. Features versus feelings: dissociable blogs.nytimes.com/2015/02/23/please-stop-making-that-noise/ (retrieved
representations of the acoustic features and valence of aversive sounds. J September 2, 2016).
Neurosci 2012;32:14184–92. [64] Taylor S, Asmundson GJG, Jang KL. Etiology of obsessive–compulsive
[35] Møller AR. Misophonia, phonophobia, and ‘‘exploding head” syndrome. In: symptoms and obsessive–compulsive personality traits: common genes,
Møller AR, Langguth B, De Ridder D, et al., editors. Textbook of mostly different environments. Depress Anxiety 2011;28:863–9.
tinnitus. NY: Springer; 2011. p. 25–7. [65] Taylor S, Stein MB. The future of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs)
[36] Jastreboff PJ, Jastreboff MM. Tinnitus retraining therapy: a different view on in psychiatric treatment. Med Hypoth 2006;66:14–21.
tinnitus. J Oto-Rhino-Laryngology Rel Specialties 2006;68:23–9. [66] Newby JM, McKinnon A, Kuyken W, et al. Systematic review and meta-analysis
[37] Boyd GF. Auditory irritants and impalpable pain. J Gen Psychol of transdiagnostic psychological treatments for anxiety and depressive
1959;60:149–54. disorders in adulthood. Clin Psychol Rev 2015;40:91–110.

You might also like