You are on page 1of 552

The SAGE Handbook of

Evolutionary Psychology
This page intentionally left blank
The SAGE Handbook of
Evolutionary Psychology

Applications of Evolutionary Psychology

Edited by
Todd K. Shackelford
SAGE Publications Ltd Editorial arrangement © Todd K. Shackelford, 2021
1 Oliver’s Yard
55 City Road Chapter 1 © Cezar Giosan, 2021 Cody Jorgensen and Jessica
London EC1Y 1SP Chapter 2 © Riadh Abed and Wells, 2021
Paul St John-Smith, 2021 Chapter 14 © Ian A. Silver and
SAGE Publications Inc. Chapter 3 © John F. Gunn III, Jamie Newsome, 2021
2455 Teller Road Pablo Malo and C. A. Soper, 2021 Chapter 15 © Russil Durrant, 2021
Thousand Oaks, California 91320 Chapter 4 © James Carmody, Chapter 16 © Anthony C. Lopez,
2021 2021
SAGE Publications India Pvt Ltd Chapter 5 © Ulysses Paulino Chapter 17 © Holly Wilson, Paul
B 1/I 1 Mohan Cooperative Industrial Area Albuquerque, Joelson M. B. Rauwolf and Joanna J. Bryson,
Mathura Road Moura, Risoneide Henriques da 2021
New Delhi 110 044 Silva, Washington S. Ferreira Chapter 18 © Robert Finkelstein,
Júnior and Taline C. Silva, 2021 2021
SAGE Publications Asia-Pacific Pte Ltd Chapter 6 © Simon Russell, 2021 Chapter 19 © Deanna Singhal
3 Church Street Chapter 7 © Diana Santos and David Wiesenthal, 2021
#10-04 Samsung Hub Fleischman, 2021 Chapter 20 © Gayle S. Stever,
Singapore 049483 Chapter 8 © Michael Latner and 2021
Elissa Feld, 2021 Chapter 21 © Ned Kock, 2021
Chapter 9 © Joseph L. Nedelec, Chapter 22 © Mark A. Caudell,
2021 2021
Chapter 10 © Lois James, 2021 Chapter 23 © James R.
Chapter 11 © Eyal Aharoni and Anderson, 2021
Morris B. Hoffman, 2021 Chapter 24 © Stephen Whyte and
Chapter 12 © Alina Simona Rusu, Benno Torgler, 2021
2021 Chapter 25 © Robert Finkelstein,
Editor: Donna Goddard Chapter 13 © Anthony Walsh, 2021
Editorial Assistant: Umeeka Raichura
Production Editor: Prachi Arora Apart from any fair dealing for the purposes of research or private
Copyeditor: Sunrise Setting Ltd. study, or criticism or review, as permitted under the Copyright,
Proofreader: Derek Markham Designs and Patents Act, 1988, this publication may be reproduced,
Indexer: Caroline Eley stored or transmitted in any form, or by any means, only with the prior
Marketing Manager: Camille Richmond permission in writing of the publishers, or in the case of reprographic
Cover Design: Naomi Robinson reproduction, in accordance with the terms of licences issued by
Typeset by Cenveo Publisher Services the Copyright Licensing Agency. Enquiries concerning reproduction
Printed in the UK outside those terms should be sent to the publishers.

Library of Congress Control Number: 2020947010

British Library Cataloguing in Publication data

A catalogue record for this book is available from the British Library

978-1-5264-8916-6

At SAGE we take sustainability seriously.


Most of our products are printed in the
UK using responsibly sourced papers
and boards. When we print overseas we
ensure sustainable papers are used as
measured by the PREPS grading system.
We undertake an annual audit to monitor
our sustainability.
Contents

List of Figures, Tables and Boxes vii


Notes on the Editor and Contributors ix

PART 1  INTEGRATION WITHIN PSYCHOLOGY

1. Evolutionary Psychology and Counseling and Psychotherapy 3


Cezar Giosan

2. Evolutionary Psychology and Psychiatry 24


Riadh Abed and Paul St John-Smith

3. Evolutionary Psychology and Suicidology 51


John F. Gunn III, Pablo Malo, and C. A. Soper

4. Evolutionary Psychology and Mindfulness and Meditation:


Easing the Anxiety of Being Human 94
James Carmody

5. Evolutionary Psychology and Environmental Sciences 107


Ulysses Paulino Albuquerque, Joelson M. B. Moura, Risoneide
Henriques da Silva, Washington S. Ferreira Júnior, and Taline C. Silva

6. Evolutionary Psychology and Public Health 123


Simon Russell

7. Animal Ethics and Evolutionary Psychology 144


Diana Santos Fleischman

PART 2  APPLICATIONS TO LAW AND ORDER

8. Evolutionary Psychology and Political Institutions 171


Michael Latner and Elissa Feld

9. Evolutionary Psychology and Crime 188


Joseph L. Nedelec

10. Evolutionary Psychology and Policing: The Balance Between


Aggression and Restraint 203
Lois James

11. Evolutionary Psychology, Jurisprudence, and Sentencing 221


Eyal Aharoni and Morris B. Hoffman
vi THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

12. Evolutionary Psychology and Incarceration 243


Alina Simona Rusu

13. Evolution and Punishment 258


Anthony Walsh, Cody Jorgensen, and Jessica Wells

14. Evolutionary Psychology and Corrections and Rehabilitation 277


Ian A. Silver and Jamie Newsome

15. Evolutionary Psychology and Organized Crime 296


Russil Durrant

16. Evolutionary Psychology and Warfare 316


Anthony C. Lopez

PART 3 APPLICATIONS TO TECHNOLOGY, COMMUNICATIONS,


AND THE FUTURE

17. Evolutionary Psychology and Artificial Intelligence: The Impact of


Artificial Intelligence on Human Behaviour 333
Holly Wilson, Paul Rauwolf, and Joanna J. Bryson

18. Evolutionary Psychology and Robotics 352


Robert Finkelstein

19. Evolutionary Psychology and Dangerous Driving Behaviour 374


Deanna Singhal and David Wiesenthal

20. Evolutionary Psychology and Mass Media 398


Gayle S. Stever

21. Evolutionary Psychology and Communication 417


Ned Kock

22. Evolutionary Psychology and Climate Change: Understanding the Impact


of Time Perspective on Carbon Emissions across 75 Countries 435
Mark A. Caudell

23. Evolutionary Psychology and Thanatology: Responses to Death 457


James R. Anderson

24. Evolutionary Psychology and Reproduction 477


Stephen Whyte and Benno Torgler

25. Evolutionary Psychology and Cyberwarfare 499


Robert Finkelstein

Index515
List of Figures, Tables and Boxes

FIGURES

2.1 Life history strategy trade-offs 29


3.1 Summary of posited antisuicide defences 68
3.2 A tentative mapping of hypothesized types of antisuicide mechanisms
(keepers) across common diagnostic categories of mental disorder 74
4.1 Alarm-related components of experience forming a cycle of distress 97
4.2 Memory, imagination and emotion are symphonies of three interwoven
experiential components 99
4.3 Components recognized as differentiated and connected 100
4.4 Attention shifts from differentiated components to arousal-neutral sensations
of breathing 101
4.5 Re-perceiving reduces distress through a perceptual/attentional shift from
what the thought is about – ‘I’m going to pass out’ – to the thought as
an event in the mind/awareness – ‘This is a thought’ 101
5.1 Environment of Evolutionary Adaptedness definition (EEA), original version
and extended version 110
5.2 Structure of the human naturalist mind 112
6.1 The impact of the food supply on the expression of obesity 125
6.2 Key determinants of psychological mechanisms and behaviour strategies 129
7.1 Charitable donations towards animal organizations as compared to animal use 151
9.1 Age-specific birth rates (per 1,000 women per year) in the 10 neighborhoods
with the longest life expectancy compared to the 10 neighborhoods with
the shortest life expectancy 198
10.1 Police balance between aggression and restraint 213
15.1 The basic dimensions of organized crime 298
19.1 GAM factors included in the investigation of modelling of aggressive or
risky driving 384
19.2 Domestic box-office history for the Fast and Furious movies389
21.1 Path model showing a costly trait and its relationship with fitness 421
21.2 Probability of evolution of a new costly trait 423
21.3 Variation of the expected ratio between pay and pax424
21.4 The evolution of oral speech in humans 425
22.1 Effect of time orientation on total fertility as a function of education 446
22.2 Effect of time orientation on total fertility as a function of mortality level 447
22.3 Effect of time orientation on carbon emissions per capita as a function
of investment in education 448
viii THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

TABLES

2.1 Tinbergen’s four questions 27


3.1 The fitness costs of suicide 54
7.1 Days of suffering per kilogram of food weight produced by the animal adjusted
for the badness of each day of life as estimated by animal welfare researchers 158
15.1 Approaches to defining organized crime 297
15.2 The functional structures of organized crime groups in relation to the key
evolutionary processes that facilitate cooperation 305
18.1 Processes of mind and their computational equivalents 354
21.1 Oral speech and the two common characteristics of costly traits 426
22.1 Descriptive statistics for Model 1 441
22.2 Variable correlations for Model 1 441
22.3 Data sources, years, and future orientation scores 443
22.4 Descriptive statistics for Model 2 445
22.5 Variable correlations for Model 2 445
22.6 Relationship between total fertility rates, time orientation, mortality,
and parental investment 446
22.7 Effect of time orientation and education on carbon emissions 448

BOX

2.1 Pathways for the persistence of disease and disorder 28


2.2 Evolutionary theories of depression 33
5.1 Structure and behavior of the human naturalistic mind 113
Notes on the Editor
and Contributors
THE EDITOR

Todd K. Shackelford received his Ph.D. in evolutionary psychology in 1997 from the University
of Texas at Austin. Since 2010, he is Professor and Chair of the Department of Psychology at
Oakland University in Rochester, Michigan, where he is Co-Director of the Evolutionary
Psychology Lab. In 2016, he was appointed Distinguished Professor by the Oakland University
Board of Trustees. He led the founding of new Ph.D. and M.S. programs which launched in
2012. Shackelford has published around 300 journal articles and his work has been cited over
22,000 times. Much of Shackelford’s research addresses sexual conflict between men and
women, with a special focus on men’s physical, emotional, and sexual violence against their
intimate partners. Since 2006, Shackelford has served as editor of the journal Evolutionary
Psychology, and in 2014 founded the journal Evolutionary Psychological Science as
Editor-in-Chief.

THE CONTRIBUTORS

Riadh Abed FRCPsych, Qualified in medicine from Baghdad and trained in psychiatry in the
UK. Retired Psychiatrist, Medical Director and Hon. Senior Clinical Lecturer, University of
Sheffield, UK. Currently is a medical member of Mental Health Tribunals, Ministry of Justice,
UK. He is author of a number of novel evolutionary hypotheses on eating disorders, obsessive
compulsive disorder and schizophrenia and has published both theoretical as well as research
articles on a range of evolutionary psychiatry subjects. He is founding chair of the Evolutionary
Psychiatry Special Interest Group (EPSIG) at the Royal College of Psychiatrists, UK.

Eyal Aharoni is an Associate Professor of Psychology, Philosophy, and Neuroscience at


Georgia State University in Atlanta. His research investigates violence risk assessment and the
influence of emotion, cognitive bias, and other extra-legal factors on legal decision-making.
Prior to his current appointment, Aharoni served as a Research Associate for the RAND
Corporation. He completed a postdoctoral fellowship with appointments at The MIND Research
Network for Neurodiagnostic Discovery and the University of New Mexico Psychology. He
has also held research positions at the Research Center for Virtual Environments and Behavior
and the Institute for Social, Behavioral, and Economic Research. Aharoni earned his PhD in
psychology at UC Santa Barbara where he also served as a research fellow for the MacArthur
Foundation’s Law and Neuroscience Project.

Ulysses Paulino Albuquerque received his Ph.D. in biology in 2001 from the Universidade
Federal de Pernambuco, Brazil. He is Full Professor of the Department of Botany at Universidade
Federal de Pernambuco, Pernambuco, Brazil. In 2011, he led the founding of new Ph.D. program
in Ethnobiology and Nature Conservation. Albuquerque has published around 316 journal articles,
x THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

200 chapter’s book and edited or authored 50 books (including new editions and translations).
Much of actual Albuquerque’s research addresses how work our naturalistic mind. Albuquerque
has served as editor of various peer-reviewed journals, and in 2011 co-founded the journal
Ethnobiology and Conservation as Co-Editor-in-Chief.

James R. Anderson is a Professor in Psychology at Kyoto University, Japan. For more than
30 years he has studied behaviour and cognition in a wide range of nonhuman primates in
captivity in the wild, as well as human children and elderly people. Most of his research is in
the following areas: the effects of early experience on social adjustment, the development of
abnormal behaviour, improving welfare of captive primates through environmental enrichment,
communicative behaviours including tactile and postural signals, gaze-following, feeding
competition and the influence of dominance relationships, tool-use, the ontogeny and phylogeny
of self-recognition, self-control, third-party social evaluations, sleep-related behavioural
adaptations, and responses to dead and dying group members and other individuals.

Joanna J. Bryson is recognised for broad expertise on intelligence and its impacts, advising
governments, transnational agencies and NGOs globally. She holds two degrees each in
psychology and AI (BA Chicago, MSc and MPhil Edinburgh, PhD MIT). From 2002–2019
she was Computer Science faculty at the University of Bath; she has also been affiliated
with Harvard Psychology, Oxford Anthropology, Mannheim Social Science Research and
the Princeton Center for Information Technology Policy. During her PhD she observed the
confusion generated by anthropomorphised AI, leading to her first AI ethics publication ‘Just
Another Artifact’ in 1998. She has remained active in the field including coauthoring the
first national-level AI ethics policy, the UK’s (2011) Principles of Robotics. She is presently
Professor of Ethics and Technology at the Hertie School of Governance in Berlin, Germany.

James Carmody is a Professor in the Departments of Medicine, and Population and Health
Sciences Division of Preventive and Behavioral Medicine at University of Massachusetts Medi-
cal School. His research is on how evolutionary and biological imperatives shape our habits of
attending to experience, their effect on anxiety, and the degree to which they can be mitigated
by mind–body training programmes like mindfulness and yoga. He is principal investigator
on NIH-funded clinical trials of the clinical effects and mechanisms of mindfulness training.
James studied and practised in Zen, Tibetan, Theravada and Advaita traditions in several coun-
tries over 50 years. He has been a therapist and leads retreats for clinicians with the goal of
making the conceptualisation and psychological mechanisms of mindfulness straightforward,
jargon-free and practically accessible for patients. His work has been featured in national and
international media including the New York Times, NPR and ABC.

Mark A. Caudell is a Medical Anthropologist working for the Food and Agriculture Organiza-
tion of the United Nations and is adjunct faculty in the Department of Anthropology at Wash-
ington State University. His research centres on understanding how socioecological systems
drive the emergence and transmission of environmental risks as well as pattern individual and
community responses to these risks. His current work combines ethnographic and lab-based
methods to identify the cultural and cognitive drivers of infectious disease and antimicrobial
resistance in low- and middle-income countries. He has conducted research among foragers in
the Congo Basin and farmers and pastoralists throughout sub-Saharan Africa and is funded by
the National Science Foundation, The Fleming Fund, Amazon and the Paul G. Allen School for
Global Animal Health at Washington State University. Currently, he is based in Nairobi, Kenya.
Notes on the Editor and Contributors xi

Risoneide Henriques da Silva is master in Botany (2018) from the Federal Rural University of
Pernambuco, Brazil. She is currently PhD candidate in Ethnobiology and Nature Conservation
by the same institution. Since 2016 she is a researcher associated at the Laboratory of Ecology
and Evolution of Social-ecological Systems at the Federal University of Pernambuco, Brazil.
Risoneide Henriques has an interest in understanding the cognitive aspects involved with
human behaviour in relation to biota.

Russil Durrant is a Senior Lecturer at the Institute of Criminology, Victoria University of


Wellington, New Zealand, where he teaches courses in criminal, biosocial and investigative
psychology. His research focuses on the application of evolutionary theory to understanding
crime and crime-related phenomena. He is the author (or co-author) of five books including
(with Tony Ward) Evolutionary Criminology: Towards a Comprehensive Explanation
for Crime and (with Zoe Poppelwell) Religion, Crime, and Punishment: An Evolutionary
Perspective.

Elissa Feld earned a Masters in Public Policy at California Polytechnic State University, San
Luis Obispo. Her research continues to focus on political psychology and public policy.

Robert Finkelstein earned a DBA in Systems Theory and Cybernetics from George
Washington University (GWU), an ApSci. in Operations Research (GWU), an MS in
Operations Research (GWU), an MS in Physics (University of Massachusetts) and a BA in
Physics (Temple University), as well as completing postgraduate courses in Physics at MIT. Dr
Finkelstein is President, since 1985, of Robotic Technology Inc., a professional services firm
that provides technical analyses, systems engineering, technology assessments, operations
research, business development and other services in the fields of autonomous ground, air and
water vehicles, intelligent systems, robotics for military and civil applications, and memetics.
Dr Finkelstein is an Adjunct Professor in the graduate school at the University of Maryland
Global Campus and is serving as a Co-Director of the Intelligent Systems Laboratory at the
University of Maryland Clark School of Engineering.

Diana Santos Fleischman is an Associate Professor of psychology at the University of


Portsmouth in the UK. She received her PhD in evolutionary psychology at the University
of Texas at Austin. Diana has been involved in animal ethics and effective animal advocacy
for several years. From 2010–2012 Diana co-hosted ‘The Vegan Option’ podcast which
explored many themes at the intersection of psychology and animal ethics. From 2017–
2019 Diana served on the board at Sentience Institute, a think tank devoted to exploring
ways to expand humanity’s moral circle. Diana also has served on the grants board of the
Animal Advocacy Research Fund which aims to promote research into effective animal
advocacy.

Cezar Giosan PhD is a faculty member of the Department of Psychology at the University of
Bucharest and has a doctorate in psychology from the New School University, New York. He
worked for a long time as a research psychologist in the top-ranked Department of Psychiatry
at Weill Medical School of Cornell University. Dr Giosan has authored dozens of peer-reviewed
publications in impact journals, including studies on the applications of evolutionary psychology
in psychological interventions. He has tested, in the form of a randomized clinical trial, the
efficacy of an evolutionary intervention for depression, and is the author of a published therapy
protocol of Cognitive-Evolutionary Therapy.
xii THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

John F. Gunn III PhD has a BA (Stockton University) and MA (Rutgers University – Camden)
in psychology and a PhD in family science and human development (Montclair State Univer-
sity). John is currently a Postdoctoral Associate at the NJ Center on Gun Violence Research at
Rutgers University, where he is investigating the role of firearms in suicide mortality on both
small and large scales. His research interests include risk and protective factors for suicidal
behaviour (such as access to a firearm; exposure to bullying), social connectedness and sui-
cide, theoretical models of suicidal behaviour (such as evolutionary explanations for suicide)
and the impact of media on suicide. John has written or co-authored over 20 scholarly publica-
tions, three edited books and 10 book chapters.

Morris B. Hoffman has been a district judge in the state of Colorado since 1991. He is a
member of the John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation’s Research Network on Law
and Neuroscience, and a Research Fellow at the Gruter Institute for Law and Behavioral
Research. He has published many law review articles, focusing on the areas of criminal law,
jurisprudence, law and neuroscience, and jury history, in journals that include Stanford,
University of Chicago, Vanderbilt, Duke, Northwestern, the University of Pennsylvania,
Journal of Law and Biosciences and Journal of Philosophy, Science and Law. He has co-
authored science papers in journals that include Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, Journal of Neuroscience, Trends
in Neuroscience and Social Neuroscience. His book, The Punisher’s Brain: An Evolutionary
History of Judge and Jury, was published in 2014.

Lois James is an Assistant Professor in the Washington State University (WSU) College of
Nursing, where she focuses on bias, stress, sleep and performance in ‘high-stress’ populations
such as police officers, military personnel, nurses and top-tier athletes. She has received
multiple honours and awards for her work, and is internationally recognised as a leading expert
in her field. Her simulation-based research on the impact of bias on police decision making has
significantly advanced what is known about how suspect race and ethnicity influences police
officers during critical encounters with the public. She is the founding director of Counter
Bias Training Simulation (CBTsim), a novel and innovative simulation-based implicit bias
training program that has been featured in National Geographic and the recent feature-length
documentary ‘bias’. Her work has been published extensively in academic journals, practitioner
magazines and mainstream media such as the New York Times and the Washington Post.

Cody Jorgensen is an Assistant Professor at Boise State University. He received his PhD from
the University of Texas at Dallas in 2014. His main research interests include biosocial and
developmental criminology, policing and forensics, and drug policy.

Washington S. Ferreira Júnior received his Ph.D in Botany in 2015 at Federal Rural
University of Pernambuco. Since 2016, he is Professor of the Biological Sciences course at
Pernambuco University in Petrolina, Pernambuco, where he is coordinator of the Laboratory
of Biocultural Investigations in the Semiarid. He is professor at the Postgraduate Program in
Environmental Science and Technology at University of Pernambuco and the Postgraduate
Program in Ethnobiology and Nature Conservation at Federal Rural University of Pernambuco.
Ferreira Júnior has published around 40 journal articles, 40 book chapters and organized three
books with partners. His research efforts are focused on understanding the structure, dynamics
and evolution of local medical systems with an emphasis on the use of medicinal plants.
Notes on the Editor and Contributors xiii

Ned Kock is Texas A&M Regents Professor and Chair of the Division of International Busi-
ness and Technology Studies, in the Sanchez School of Business, at Texas A&M International
University. He is an active member of the Human Behavior and Evolution Society and the
Association for Information Systems. He has edited the Springer book Evolutionary Psychol-
ogy and Information Systems Research, and guest-edited the Special Issue on Darwinian Per-
spectives on Electronic Communication published in the IEEE Transactions on Professional
Communication. Ned has published his research in a number of high-impact journals including
Communications of the ACM, Decision Support Systems, European Journal of Information
Systems, European Journal of Operational Research, IEEE Transactions (various), Informa-
tion & Management, Information Systems Journal, Journal of the Association for Informa-
tion Systems, Journal of Management Information Systems, MIS Quarterly and Organization
Science. His research interests include evolution and human behaviour toward technology,
computational statistics and structural equation modelling.

Michael Latner is a Professor of Political Science at California Polytechnic State University,


San Luis Obispo, faculty scholar at Cal Poly’s Institute for Advanced Technology and Public
Policy and a Senior Fellow with the Union of Concerned Scientists’ Center for Science and
Democracy. His research focuses on electoral systems, coalition behavior, representation and
voting rights.

Anthony C. Lopez is Associate Professor of Political Psychology in the School of Politics,


Philosophy, and Public Affairs, at Washington State University. He received his PhD from Brown
University, and also received training as a research associate at the Center for Evolutionary
Psychology, University of California, Santa Barbara. His research explores the evolutionary
origins of warfare, as well as its constituent psychological dynamics, such as revenge, the
nature of deterrence, collective action problems of warfare, extremist violence, and distinctions
between offensive and defensive aggression. He serves as Associate Editor of Politics with the
Evolution Institute and blogs regularly at Psychology Today.

Pablo Malo studied medicine in the University of the Basque Country in Spain and psychiatry
in the Psychiatric Hospital of Zamudio. Pablo is currently a clinical psychiatrist working in
the Mental Health Center of Bombero Etxaniz, in Bilbao, Spain. His clinical interests include
psychosis and severe mental disorders, violence and mental health disorders, intimate partner
violence and evolutionary psychology. Pablo has co-authored a book about evolutionary
psychiatry, written over 20 scholarly papers and is the editor of two blogs about evolutionary
psychology and biology.

Joelson M. B. Moura has a complete degree in Biological Sciences from the Universidade
Federal Rural de Pernambuco (UFRPE), with an internship at the University of Coimbra (UC)
in 2013. He received his master’s degree in Ethnobiology and Nature Conservation (UFRPE)
in 2018. He is currently a Ph.D. candidate in the same program and integrates the Laboratory
of Ecology and Evolution of Socioecological Systems (LEA) at the Universidade Federal de
Pernambuco (UFPE) since 2016. The research carried out by Moura seeks to understand the
processes that contributed to the evolution of the human mind. His work is in the perspective
of evolutionary ethnobiology, which addresses the influence of the evolutionary past on
current human behavior and how it reflects on the relationship between people and their
environments.
xiv THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Joseph L. Nedelec is an Associate Professor in the School of Criminal Justice at the University
of Cincinnati. His research interests include biosocial criminology, evolutionary psychology,
intelligence, quantitative behaviour genetics, and cybercrime. Dr Nedelec’s work has been
published in a variety of journals including Criminology, Evolution and Human Behavior,
Intelligence, Journal of Criminal Justice, Journal of Quantitative Criminology, Personality and
Individual Differences and PLOS One, among others.

Jamie Newsome received her doctorate in 2013 from the School of Criminal Justice at UC,
and is currently the Research Coordinator for UCCI. Her research interests include the design,
implementation and effectiveness of correctional interventions, and translating research into
practice.

Paul Rauwolf is an Assistant Professor (Lecturer) in Psychology at Bangor University, UK.


Previously, he was a Postdoctoral Researcher in the Mathematics Institute at the University
of Oxford and the School of Psychology at Bangor University. In 2016, he received his PhD
in Computer Science from the University of Bath. His research takes an interdisciplinary
perspective to better understand the utility and aetiology of biases in human decision-making.
During his PhD, he used agent-based modelling to demonstrate that ignoring freely available
information can be adaptive in a variety of contexts, such as maintaining cooperative societies.
His current work focuses on understanding how the changing information and technological
landscape impacts human decision-making under uncertainty.

Simon Russell is a Research Fellow at the Great Ormond Street Institute of Child Health. Simon
has conducted research in the fields of evolutionary science and public health, with a particular
interest in health risk behaviours, obesity, inequalities in health, public health policy and
epidemiology. Simon’s PhD entailed applying evolutionary principles to the obesity problem
and other issues in public health, work that demonstrated that health behaviours and strategies
are likely to be adaptive. Currently, Simon leads various research projects that have high policy
relevance and often involve emerging methodologies. In recent years, Simon has specialised in
simulating policy relevant interventions using causal inference techniques across longitudinal
cohort data. Simon’s work involves applying epidemiological techniques to explore causes and
consequences of obesity and to appraise the effectiveness of potential population policy action.

Alina Simona Rusu is a biologist and psychologist and is currently an Associate Professor in
the Department of Special Education, School of Psychology and Sciences of Education, Babeş-
Bolyai University, Romania. She received her PhD in Animal Behaviour from University of Zurich,
Switzerland. She is interested in interdisciplinary research of human prosocial behaviour in the
context of inclusive institutions, as well as the applied values of human–animal interactions.

Taline C. Silva received her Ph.D. in Botany in 2014 from the Federal Rural University of
Pernambuco. Since 2016, she is Professor of the Biology course at State University of Alagoas
in Palmeira dos índios, Alagoas, where she is coordinator of the Ethnobiology and Ecossistems
conservation Lab. In 2018, she was member of Post graduation program in ethnobiology and
nature conservation. Silva has published around 30 journal articles. She has experience in
Ethnobiology and seeks to understand the factors that guide the complex relationship between
people and nature, accessing knowledge, use and local perception of natural resources and
landscape changes.
Notes on the Editor and Contributors xv

Ian A. Silver is an Assistant Professor at the Law and Justice Department at Rowan University
and a Fellow at the University of Cincinnati Corrections Institute (UCCI). His research
interests include correctional rehabilitation, incarceration, biopsychosocial criminology,
and methodological issues. His recent work has appeared in Criminology and Public Policy,
Aggressive Behavior, Journal of Offender Rehabilitation, Brain Injury and Journal of Criminal
Justice.

Deanna Singhal received a bachelor’s degree in psychology from Queen’s University in


Kingston, and a master’s degree (exercise and health science) and doctoral degree (psychology:
brain, behaviour, and cognitive science) from York University in Toronto. She is now a Faculty
Service Officer in the Department of Psychology at the University of Alberta, Canada, with
a primary role to support the Undergraduate Teaching and Learning Program. Her research
interests include driving behaviour and cognitive workload, and her areas of teaching include
introductory psychology and human neuropsychology.

Paul St John-Smith FRCPsych. Trained at Oxford, graduated in natural sciences in 1976


and qualified in medicine in 1979. He then trained as a general practitioner and later joined
the pharmaceutical industry for five years as a research physician in psychopharmacology
investigating various antidepressants, hypnotics and other central nervous system agents
including the benzodiazepine antagonist. In 1987 he returned to study psychiatry full time and
worked in a number of London hospitals including Barnet, Guys, Greenwich, St Thomas’, and as
a consultant in Hertfordshire. He has published on a range of areas in evolutionary psychiatry
and has peer reviewed papers on psychopharmacology, placebo effects, professionalism, suicide
and depression, as well as evolution.

C. A. Soper is a psychotherapist in private practice in Lisbon, Portugal. He holds degrees from


the University of Cambridge and the University of London and gained his PhD at the University
of Gloucestershire for theoretical research into the evolutionary origins of suicide. His book,
The Evolution of Suicide, was published in 2018. He has authored various other scholarly
publications on suicide and suicidology, and presented at academic and clinical conferences
on the subject. Soper’s clinical interests include the provision of therapeutic support for people
affected by suicide and those suffering from alcoholism and other addictions.

Gayle S. Stever is a Professor of Psychology at Empire State College/State University of


New York. She has done research in the areas of parasocial theory, media effects and fan studies
for over 30 years, and has authored numerous peer-reviewed articles on these subjects, in
addition to writing The Psychology of Celebrity (2019). With her degree in lifespan development
psychology, looking at media effects from a lifespan perspective is central to her work.

Benno Torgler is Professor of Economics at the Centre for Behavioural Economics, Society
and Technology (BEST) and School of Economics and Finance, Queensland University
of Technology (QUT), Australia. He was also Adjunct Professor at the EBS Universität für
Wirtschaft und Recht, Germany (2012–2015) and an Australian Research Council (ARC) Future
Fellow (2011–2015). Before QUT he was a Research Affiliate and Lecturer in International
and Comparative Political Economy in the Leitner Program at the MacMillan Center, Yale
University; a Visiting Scholar in the Law and Economics Program at University of California-
Berkeley; and a Visiting Scholar in the Andrew Young School of Policy Studies at Georgia State
xvi THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

University. His primary research interest lies in the area of economics, but he has also published
in journals with a political science, social psychology, medical, sociology and biology focus.

Anthony Walsh is a Professor Emeritus at Boise State University. He received his PhD from
Bowling Green State University in 1983 after working as a marine, police officer and probation/
parole officer. His primary interest is biosocial criminology. He has published 43 books, the
latest being God, Science, and Society: The Origin of the Universe, Intelligent Life, and Free
Societies (Vernon, 2020).

Jessica Wells is an Assistant Professor of Criminal Justice at Boise State University. She received
her PhD in Criminal Justice and Criminology from Sam Houston State University in 2017. Her
research is in the area of psychological and biosocial perspectives on criminal offending. Her
publications focus on the consequences of stress and trauma exposure in samples of community
members, incarcerated individuals and criminal justice professionals.

Stephen Whyte is a Research Fellow in Behavioural Economics at the Centre for Behavioural
Economics, Society and Technology (BEST) and School of Economics and Finance,
Queensland University of Technology (QUT), Australia. His research focus explores large-
scale decision making in mate-choice settings. His work takes a multi-disciplinary approach in
studying key sex differences in human behaviour, with work that bridges the fields of applied
micro-economics, personality and social psychology, and evolutionary biology. His most recent
research has explored such diverse topics as sex differences in nonbinary gender identification,
male and female decision making in assisted reproductive and donor insemination medical
environments, and preferences vs choice in cyber dating markets.

David Wiesenthal received a bachelor’s degree in psychology from the City College of New
York and a doctoral degree in social psychology from the State University of New York at
Buffalo, followed by a postdoctoral fellowship from York University, Canada. He is now a
Professor Emeritus and Senior Scholar at York University, Canada. He has been a Visiting
Professor at the Hebrew University of Jerusalem and lectured as a Scandinavian Fellow at
the Building Research Institute (Gävle, Sweden), Umeå University, Lund University, and the
Helsingborg campus of Lund University, as well as the University of Costa Rica and the College
of Psychologists (Costa Rica). His research interests are social and environmental influence
processes, driver behaviours, scientific racism and research ethics.

Holly Wilson is a second-year PhD student in the Department of Computer Science at the
University of Bath. Her doctoral research involves investigating the neural correlates of both
visual imagery and perception, and developing a brain computer interface which facilitates visual
imagery reconstruction. Holly merged her interests in both artificial and natural intelligence by
obtaining a BSc in Psychology followed by an MSc in Computer Science. In the first year
of her PhD she focused on the ethics and impact of AI on people, on both an individual and
societal level, alongside developing agent-based models of cultural evolution during the Upper
Palaeolithic transition.
PART 1

Integration within Psychology


This page intentionally left blank
1
Evolutionary Psychology and
Counseling and Psychotherapy
Cezar Giosan

This chapter covers two important clinical symptoms in increasing fitness, seeing them
applications of evolutionary psychopathology: as evolved strategies serving an organism’s
(1) evolutionary-inspired psychological inter- goal to survive and reproduce, or, conversely,
ventions, and (2) integration of evolutionary center on the mismatch between our adap-
insights into couples therapy. tations, evolved during the Environment of
Evolutionary Adaptedness, and the modern
world.
An important practical question that arises
APPLICATIONS OF EVOLUTIONARY from this substantial body of work revolves
PSYCHOLOGY IN THE TREATMENT OF around the clinical implications of such theo-
MENTAL DISORDERS ries. Although still speculative for the most
part, evolutionary explanations of mental dis-
In recent years, there has been an outpouring orders do raise the intriguing possibility that
of attempts at evolutionary hypotheses of psychological interventions that target fitness
mental disorders. Different authors have pro- could have unique clinical benefits, which
posed evolutionary explanations for depres- can go above and beyond those of current
sion (Durisko et al., 2015), anxieties (Gilbert, treatments.
2001; Nesse, 1998), schizophrenia (Crow, To this end, clinical psychologists, psy-
2000), and personality disorders (Glenn chotherapists, or clinical researchers have
et  al., 2011; Gutiérrez et  al., 2013; Hertler, attempted to bridge the gap between evolu-
2014; Molina et  al., 2009; O’Reilly et  al., tionary theory and clinical practice by devel-
2001), to name a few. oping protocols that integrate evolutionary
Evolutionary explanations of mental dis- insights into the treatment of mental dis-
orders typically focus on the role of the orders. Some of these protocols have been
4 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

tested in randomized clinical trials with active the patient, which can facilitate recovery
comparators, while others have led to case stud- (John and Segal, 2015; Kuyken et al., 2008).
ies or small-n pilot studies with no comparators. Historically, Sigmund Freud was the first
Also, some of these insights have been used in a to introduce case conceptualization as a
cross-diagnostic manner – that is, same princi- key element in psychotherapy, through the
ples applied to various conditions – while oth- analysis of the latent content of the dreams
ers are specific to certain disorders. and interpretation of the neurotic symptoms
Alfonso Troisi and Michael McGuire have (Freud, 2017). Today, case conceptualiza-
proposed that an evolutionary therapy’s main tion is used in many therapeutic approaches.
aim is to increase a patient’s short-term bio- For instance, Cognitive Behavioral Therapy
logical goals. In this light, evolutionary psy- (CBT), one of the most widely used interven-
chopathology makes the distinction between tions for anxiety and depression, typically
mental disorder and mental suffering, seeing includes information about the causal mecha-
many mental symptoms as adaptive reac- nisms of the problem, that is, proximal causes
tions to situations associated with negative of psychopathology, thus answering the ‘how’
cost–benefit outcomes, which should not be questions from the ABC1 model (Ellis, 1994;
treated if they do not cause distress (Troisi Ellis et al., 2007).
and McGuire, 2014). Case conceptualization in modern psy-
More specifically, in the view of Troisi and chological interventions, however, typically
McGuire (2014), an evolutionary-driven ther- includes only information about proximal
apy should (1) address cost–benefit outcomes; causes of the symptoms. For instance, the
(2) facilitate the development of revised mod- ABC model leaves out the ‘how’ questions,
els of the social environments; and (3) aid the focusing almost exclusively on the immediate
patient in developing capacities to achieve mechanisms, such as dysfunctional thinking
biologically relevant goals. This therapy (Lam and Gale, 2000).
should attempt to refine traits or to foster the Some therapeutic schools do offer distal
use of alternative capacities that can help to explanations of symptoms, but none of them
achieve high-priority goals. In the more diffi- goes so deep as to bring into the therapeutic
cult cases, the authors argue, therapy should discourse, in a coherent, unified manner, the
encourage patients with suboptimal functional factors, forces, and elements that have shaped
capacities to actively search for environments the evolution of our species. For example,
where they can be successful in reaching high- psychoanalysis, the first school of thought
priority goals (Troisi and McGuire, 2014). that brought into discussion the distal causes
of psychopathology, explains phobias through
repression and displacement. A conflict origi-
Evolutionary Psychology and nates in childhood and that conflict is either
repressed or displaced onto the feared object.
Case Conceptualization
As an illustrative example, in ‘Notes upon a
Case conceptualization – an explanation, case of obsessional neurosis’ Freud attributed
offered by the therapist, of the problems the Rat Man’s fear of relatives dying from
bothering a patient – addresses the mental being burrowed through by rats to guilt origi-
problem and its possible causes, the ethio- nating from a repressed desire he had earlier
pathogenetic processes presumed to be to see women that he knew naked (Williams,
involved, and the positive or adverse effects 2008). A phobia of snakes, from the same
of the proposed treatment. The efficient con- psychoanalytical perspective, was an uncon-
ceptualization of a problem can generate scious fear of something else, which was to
positive expectations about the treatment as be unraveled in therapy through dream inter-
well as a sense of prediction and control in pretation or analysis of slips of tongue.
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND COUNSELING AND PSYCHOTHERAPY 5

Evolutionary psychopathology makes one Sigmund Freud also made this central in his
giant leap further and addresses the distal, psychoanalytical psychotherapy. Indeed, one of
evolutionary causes of mental illness, namely, the fundamental tenets of psychoanalytical
the evolutionary factors and forces that might psychotherapy is complete disclosure and com-
be at the root of the presenting symptoms. By munication: the patient is required to disclose
addressing the evolutionary causes of behav- anything that comes to their mind, without
iors, evolutionary psychopathology finds itself censorship. Thus, in Freudian psychoanalysis,
in the privileged – and unique, to some extent – the therapist took a central role in patients’
position to offer explanations of symptoms lives, since he would learn information about
that typically make much sense to patients. them that no one else was privy to. This unique
By offering evolutionary explanations and extremely close relationship (sessions were
of symptoms, evolutionary psychology can held several times per week) made Freud real-
enhance case conceptualizations of various ize that it played a major role in the therapeutic
treatment approaches in meaningful ways, outcomes and subsequently led to the definition
potentially leading to better therapeutic out- of important constructs such as ‘transference’,
comes. For instance, incorporating information which slowly replaced the initial emphasis on
about the hypothesized adaptive functions of sexual symbolism with more nuanced under-
the symptoms in the ABC, or the further refined standings of the therapeutic alliance.
ABCDE model (Ellis, 1994; Ellis et al., 2007), Some scholars note that many psychothera-
can lead to answers to ‘why’ questions, thus pies – including evolutionary interventions –
giving the patient a broader and more meaning- do not place sufficient importance on the
ful understanding of the problems they are con- relationship between the client and the thera-
fronting, which can lead to better acceptance. pist, or they may use that relationship to manip-
ulate patients in what the therapist believes is in
his own best interests. Kriegman (2000) argues
Integration of Evolutionary that evolutionary insights can help reduce
this risk in all forms of therapy, including
Principles in Specific Forms
psychoanalysis. Since psychoanalysis revolves
of Therapy
around a deep relationship between two unre-
There have been several attempts to integrate lated individuals and since one evolutionary
evolutionary insights into various therapies principle is that we are hardwired to operate
in recent years. We begin by briefly describ- for our own benefits, it follows that the power
ing the possible evolutionary resuscitation of the therapist has over the patient may some-
Freud’s psychoanalysis and Jung’s analytical times be used to further the interests of the
therapy and continue with a presentation of therapist, even if unconsciously (Kriegman,
several evolutionary-driven therapy protocols 1998). Becoming more aware of the distal
that have been tested in randomized clinical mechanisms responsible for human behavior
trials. We will end this section with a brief will place a therapist in a better position to
presentation of the potential applications of avoid confusion between proximal and distal
evolutionary conceptualizations to other causes, ultimately benefitting the patient. For
types of mental conditions. instance, as Kriegman describes hypotheti-
cally, a woman who dresses provocatively but
is angered when perceived as a sexual object
can be seen by an analyst as having an uncon-
Psychoanalysis
scious wish to be ravished or raped, with
Evolutionary psychotherapies place substantial anger being a reaction formation. From an
importance on the therapeutic relationship. evolutionary lens, however, this interpreta-
This is not something new. A century ago, tion might reflect a mix between projections
6 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

of male wishes and confusion of proximal objectivity in the therapeutic relationship,


(dressing sexily) and ultimate causes (wom- replacing it with something common in evo-
an’s self-interest enhancement through the lutionary therapies today, namely, the empha-
stimulation of men). Becoming more aware sis on a warm, reciprocal alliance.
of such nuances can help the therapeutic Not unlike the mismatch hypothesis
process and, therefore, evolutionary inter- (Giphart and van Vugt, 2018), psychopathol-
pretations can bring value to such clinical ogy, in the Jungian paradigm, occurs when
situations. environmental mismatches at critical develop-
mental stages lead to malfunction in ‘arche-
typal’ strategies (Stevens, 2000). Evolutionary
psychology can add to analytical therapy the
Jungian Analytical Therapy
critical element of an even broader view of
While for classical psychoanalytical therapy self than Jung conceived. Armed with mod-
the answer to the central question of what is ern knowledge about the functions of psy-
wrong with the patient is their repressed chological mechanisms, therapists nowadays
memories, which the therapist tries to bring can bring into the clinical conceptualization a
to the conscious level using strategies such as more expanded conversation about the role of
interpretation of dreams or slips of the these mechanisms in mental illness.
tongue, in Carl Gustav Jung’s analytical
therapy it is the archetypal intent that needs
to be freed to unleash the patient’s full poten- Evidence-Based Evolutionary
tial (Stevens, 2000). Interventions
Jung’s theory of archetypes – universal,
innate, archaic patterns and images of evo- After this brief theoretical presentation of the
lutionary origins that stem from the collec- ways in which evolutionary insights can aid
tive unconscious and which are the psychic Freudian and Jungian therapies, we continue,
counterpart of instinct – closely anticipated in the section that follows, with the presenta-
the notions of evolved mechanisms (innate tion of results from several empirical studies
strategies or algorithms) present in evolu- that have examined the benefits of integrat-
tionary theories today. Indeed, Jung rejected ing evolutionary insights into the treatment
the tabula rasa understanding of human of depression and personality disorders.
mind, common to his contemporaries (nota-
bly, John Watson in the United States) and
replaced it with a theory that included the Depression
enormous influence of evolutionary factors
on human behavior. Like evolutionary psy- A Rwandan man once described the Rwandan
chologists today, Jung argued that homeosta- treatment for depression to the 2001 National
sis, epigenesis, and adaptation are at the basis Book Award winner Andrew Solomon like
of the human psyche (Stevens, 1982, 1999), this:
a paradigm that was in stark contrast to the
You know, we had a lot of trouble with Western
blank-slate view of the mind from the Standard mental health workers, especially the ones who
Social Sciences Model. Jung also rejected the came here right after the genocide. They came and
sexualized Freudian interpretation of com- their practice did not involve being outside in the
plexes such as Oedipus, anticipating the later sunshine… which is, after all, where you begin to
feel better. There was no drumming or music to
works of John Bowlby, who argued that a child
get your blood flowing again – when you’re
is attached to his/her mother because she is the depressed and low you need to have your blood
caregiver (Bowlby, 1983, 2005). Also, of note, flowing. There was no sense that everyone had
in clinical practice, Jung rejected Freud’s cold taken the day off so that the entire community
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND COUNSELING AND PSYCHOTHERAPY 7

could come together to lift you up and bring you which the patients terminate protest, that is,
back to joy. There was no acknowledgement of accept defeat as an immutable fact of their
the depression as something invasive and external
lives. This phase, which is the analogue equiv-
that could actually be cast out of you again.
Instead, they would take people one at a time into alent of exposure therapy in anxiety disorders,
these dingy little rooms and have them sit around is the centerpiece of TDD, as it facilitates the
for an hour or so to talk about bad things that had transition from protest to acquiescence; and
happened to them. We had to ask them to leave (3) behavioral activation without the func-
the country. (Taljaard, n.d.)
tional analysis component. Throughout TDD
treatment, cognitive reappraisal takes place,
While this description of an intervention for following standard cognitive therapy
depression is in stark contrast to the standard approaches (e.g., analysis of distortions).
one-hour-weekly therapy sessions common
in Western cultures, it would not surprise an
evolutionary therapist. Evolutionary psycho-
Evidence in Favor of TDD
pathologists view mild and moderate depres-
sion as functional states, serving adaptive A preliminary study testing the efficacy of
functions (for a review, see Durisko et  al., TDD was conducted in the form of a pilot
2015). For instance, as early as the 1990s, observation on a sample of 12 participants,
some authors conceptualized depression as a who met for 24 biweekly, 90-minute-long
warning signal that biosocial goals have not sessions (Krupnik, 2014). The protocol dem-
been achieved (Nesse, 1991). The clinical onstrated effectiveness and specificity for
implication of this line of thought is that depression, differentiating it from anxiety
finding solutions to reset the cost–benefit and personality disorders. The results showed
balance in favor of the patient should make marked decline in depressive symptomatol-
depressed mood subside. ogy; however, the study was underpowered
In one of the earlier attempts at incorpo- and the tentative trends in the dynamics of
rating evolutionary insights into therapy for the participants’ scores did not reach statisti-
depression, McGuire and Troisi presented a cal significance. The TDD protocol needs
clinical case of a patient who was depressed further testing in randomized controlled
because of her inability to have children (i.e., studies in comparison with established proto-
major direct fitness problem). The treatment cols for depression to better establish its
focused on addressing the dysregulating efficacy.
effects of the patient’s inability to reproduce, Since not all clients can engage in mind-
and, crucially, also formulated strategies to fulness, which is a key element in the accept-
help this patient’s fitness through kin invest- ance phase in TDD, the same author replaced,
ment (i.e., inclusive fitness) (McGuire and in a further case study on a medicated patient,
Troisi, 1998: 270–271). the acceptance phase from TDD with eye
movement desensitization and ­reprocessing –
EMDR (Shapiro, 2017). In this study, EMDR
was used in a truncated form, and the nature
Treating Depression Downhill
of the targets was the subjective percep-
One evolutionary-based intervention proto- tion of loss, rather than actual events, while
col for depression is Treating Depression reappraisal took place along the protest–­
Downhill – TDD (Krupnik, 2014). TDD acceptance axis. The results showed that,
relies on an experiential approach and at the end of the treatment and at follow-
involves three distinct phases: (1) explora- up assessment, the patient reported a more
tory, in which the patients gain insight into accepting disposition and decreased depres-
their experience of defeat; (2) acceptance, in sive symptoms (Krupnik, 2015).
8 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Following the same line of investigation, Evidence in Favor of TLC-D


another study conducted by the same author
reported a case series of 21 military person- Karwoski et al.’s (2005) protocol was retested
nel diagnosed with depressive disorders, who with additional data and gender comparison
received a course of TDD-EMDR (Krupnik, by Jacobson et al. (2007). The authors exam-
2018). By the end of treatment (12 sessions), ined TLC-D on 81 patients who underwent
80% of completers (n = 15) did not meet 12 sessions of TLC-D therapy, with follow-
the criteria for depressive disorder and they up evaluations at three and six months. The
showed a significant reduction in scores on experimental group was compared to a
the Beck Depression Inventory-II – BDI-II Treatment as Usual (TAU) group, represent-
(Beck et  al., 1996) with a large effect size ing one-third of the sample. The results
(d = 2.8) and an increase in accepting dis- showed that the TLC-D group outperformed
position (d = 1.8) on the Acceptance and the control group. The results also showed
Action Questionnaire (Bond et  al., 2011). that, at the end of the therapy, participants
Non-completers showed a similar decrease averaged a 17.8% decrease in BDI-II (Beck
in the BDI-II scores at mid-treatment. The et al., 1996) scores, which represented a sta-
author observed no statistically significant tistically significant 60.6% reduction from
decrease in anxiety symptoms on the BDI-II. baseline. These improvements were stable,
These results suggest that TDD-EMDR may showing a 67.7% reduction at three-month
be an effective treatment for depressive disor- follow-up, and 64.0% reduction at six-month
ders (Krupnik, 2018). They also indicate that follow-up.
this type of intervention may target depres- Further research on TLC-D continued to
sive over anxiety symptoms (Krupnik, 2018), show promising results. In a study conducted
as was previously observed for the original by Botanov et al., (2012), 29 patients were
TDD pilot study (Krupnik, 2014). recruited into a TLC-D protocol, in a two-to-
one random assignment (22 in TLC-D and 7
in TAU). The participants underwent 12 ses-
sions of group therapy over 14 weeks and
Therapeutic Lifestyle Change were assessed weekly with the BDI-II (Beck
for Depression (TLC-D) et al., 1996). The results showed a clinically
Another attempt to incorporate evolutionary- significant response (>= 50% reduction in
inspired interventions in therapy is the BDI-II scores) in 77.3% of the participants
Therapeutic Lifestyle Change for Depression in the TLC-D condition versus 28.6% in the
(TLC-D) protocol (Karwoski et  al., 2005), TAU condition, and, notably, no significant
which includes several evolutionary elements change in BDI-II scores was observed from
thought to have positive effects on mood. treatment end to six-month follow-up, sug-
TLC-D combines several relevant factors, gesting low relapse rates post-treatment.
some of which are evolutionary-relevant, that
are shown to be effective in the treatment of
depression. These factors include: Cognitive Evolutionary Therapy
1 Omega-3 fatty acid consumption (Peet and for Depression (CETD)
Horrobin, 2002).
Another clinically tested evolutionary-driven
2 Bright light exposure (Martiny et al., 2005).
3 Sleep hygiene (Mayers and Baldwin, 2006).
intervention protocol for depression is
4 Aerobic exercise (Blumenthal et al., 2007). Cognitive Evolutionary Therapy for Depression
5 Anti-rumination exercises (Fennell and Teasdale, (CETD) (Giosan, 2020; Giosan, Cobeanu,
1987). Wyka, et  al., 2020; Giosan, Cobeanu et  al.,
6 Social support (George, 1989). 2014). As conceptualized by the authors,
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND COUNSELING AND PSYCHOTHERAPY 9

besides targeting the proximal causes of The CETD manual (Giosan, 2020) provides
depression as is standard in CBT, CETD the evolutionary therapist with concrete exam-
focuses on distal causes as well, such as inclu- ples of therapeutic interventions on each of the
sive fitness or reproductive success. While EFS items. For instance, a negative endorse-
sharing common underpinnings with CBT, ment of the EFS item ‘I have at least one best
CETD adds the inclusion of evolutionary con- friend’ should be dealt with by exploring the
ceptualizations of the patient’s symptoms and reasons and refuting dysfunctional thinking,
the targeting of fitness-related problems. Very as well as exploring modalities to increase
much unlike classical Cognitive Therapy for connectedness with at least one non-relative.
Depression, in which the problems that preoc- Likewise, a negative endorsement of the EFS
cupy the patient are identified by the patients item ‘My family brag about me’ should be
during the therapy sessions, CETD starts from dealt with by discussing solutions to increase
the premise that depressive symptoms reflect status and dominance (e.g., more education if
fitness difficulties, some of which are appropriate, job change, community involve-
unknown to the patients, and which can be ment, etc.) (‘Darwinian Psychotherapy’,
identified via an evaluation of the patient’s 2019; Giosan, 2020).
fitness prior to the first session. By identify- As far as the therapeutic alliance is con-
ing a patient’s fitness problems at intake, the cerned, the CETD protocol advocates that
CETD therapist thus is pre-equipped with this the therapist go beyond the recommen-
knowledge at the first session and can start dations of interventions such as Rational
working with the patient on problematic areas Emotive Behavior Therapy – REBT (where
right away. the alliance is centered on unconditional
Along with these evolutionary-driven behav- acceptance, empathy, humor, and genuine-
ioral activations, discussions about human ness) or psychoanalysis (friendly neutrality)
nature from evolutionary perspectives are also and try to become a patient’s psychological
taking place during CETD, such as modularity kin, while maintaining a safe set of bounda-
(Cosmides and Tooby, 1994), parental invest- ries (“Darwinian Psychotherapy,” 2019;
ment theory (Buss et  al., 1990), conspicuous Giosan, 2020). This approach is in line with
consumption (Sundie et  al., 2011), or costly the suggestions of other evolutionary psy-
signaling theory (Fraser, 2012), all of which chopathologists, who emphasize rapport
can facilitate acceptance, a key CBT ingredi- between the therapist and patient (Troisi
ent (Chamberlain and Haaga, 2001). and McGuire, 2014: 34), question the effi-
The instrument that CETD therapists cacy of one-hour-per-week therapy sessions
use to identify a patient’s fitness difficulties (Gilbert et  al., 2014: 19), or propose that
is the Evolutionary Fitness Scale – EFS depressed patients may even need ‘thera-
(Giosan et  al., 2018). The EFS is a 58-item peutic cheerleading’ (Markowitz, 1994;
scale assessing mismatches between the Michels, 1997).
Environment of Evolutionary Adaptedness While many evolutionary therapists argue
and the modern world, such as in physical for a stronger connection between the thera-
activity or nutrition, environmental misfits, pist and the depressed patient than the one
or fitness-related factors such as health of advocated by other therapeutic paradigms,
the actor, his/her partner and their extended support for such an idea predates these recent
families, attractiveness (both of the actor and developments in evolutionary psychotherapy.
partner), status, resource control, extended The early and fascinating work of Jerome
family, social capital, and mate value. Some Motto, who found that simply mailing per-
examples of items are: ‘I visit my relatives fre- sonally signed ‘Caring Letters’ to people who
quently’, or ‘I am an active outdoors person’, had attempted suicide drastically reduced
which are actionable in therapy. future suicide attempts, as people felt more
10 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

connected to the therapist communicating with for depression: Cognitive Therapy (CT; Beck
them, illustrated the importance of therapeutic et  al., 1979). A total of 97 depressed patients
rapport (Motto, 1976). The ‘Caring Letters’ received 12 sessions of either (1) CETD or
approach has been revised to include a form of (2) CT. Baseline, mid-treatment, post-treatment,
intervention that essentially makes the thera- and three-month follow-up assessments were
pist available almost continuously, and the conducted. The CT group underwent classical
results of this kin-like therapeutic relationship cognitive interventions aimed at the correc-
are promising. (For a detailed account of this tion of dysfunctional, automatic thoughts and
project, including historical aspects, see James beliefs hypothesized to be implicated in depres-
Cherkis’ (n.d.) excellent article in Huffington sive symptoms. These interventions were paired
Post, https://bit.ly/2NqSWeC). with behavioral activation and positive rein-
forcements. The CETD group added s­pecific
goals targeted at increasing fitness (see full pro-
tocol at Giosan, Cobeanu et al., 2014).
Evidence in Favor of CETD
Both interventions led to similar reduc-
In a case study examining the potential bene- tions in depressive symptomatology, as meas-
fits of CETD, Giosan, Muresan et  al. (2014) ured by the BDI-II (Beck et al., 1996), which
used this protocol on a patient with an intake were maintained at three-month follow-up.
score of 22 on the BDI-II (Beck et al., 1996) Although non-significant, the CETD group
and a diagnosis with depression made with the showed a consistent pattern of larger gains
Structured Clinical Interview for the DSM (greater decreases in BDI-II scores) dur-
(SCID) (First et  al., 1997), who presented ing the treatment as well as post-treatment.
deteriorating functioning (school perfor- Fewer CETD participants were classified as
mance) and quality of life following a recent having moderate or severe depression over
break-up. An assessment of her perceived fit- time, with between-group analyses showing
ness with the EFS (Giosan et  al., 2018) trend differences at post-treatment.
revealed deficiencies in self-image, healthy The results also showed that the CETD
eating habits, and physical activity. The patient group experienced significantly greater reduc-
was offered a cognitive-evolutionary conceptu- tions in behavioral inhibition/avoidance at
alization of her symptoms that centered on the both post-treatment and follow-up, compared
distal causes of depression as well as on the with the CT group. Notably, CETD was also
dysfunctional cognitions that led to symptoms significantly superior to CT in increasing
(Giosan, Muresan et al., 2014). The treatment engagement in social and enjoyable activities
focus was to engage the patient in the EFS- at post-treatment. The study showed that in the
suggested fitness-increasing activities, while participants receiving CETD, but not in those
simultaneously challenging dysfunctional receiving CT, engagement in these activities
thinking. The treatment was successful, the was directly related to decreased symptoms
patient achieving a ~68% reduction in the of depression, suggesting that CETD leads to
BDI-II (Beck et al., 1996) scores by session 8 greater social reach, which, in turn, might
(BDI-II = 7), therapeutic gains maintained at translate into better therapeutic outcomes
post-evaluation (BDI-II = 7) and follow-up (Giosan, 2020; Giosan et  al., 2019; Giosan,
(BDI-II = 13). Cobeanu, Wyka, et al., 2020).
A randomized, single-blinded active-­
controlled design (Giosan, Cobeanu, Wyka,
et  al., 2020; Giosan, Cobeanu et  al., 2014) Personality Disorders
expanded on this preliminary case study and
contrasted the efficacy of CET for depression Personality disorders are typically perceived
with one of the best validated interventions as difficult to address in therapy, with some
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND COUNSELING AND PSYCHOTHERAPY 11

of them, such as borderline personality disor- personality disorders were assessed at admis-
der, being especially prone to de facto demedi- sion, discharge, and three-month follow-up
calization (Sulzer, 2015). The evolutionary and the outcome measures consisted of self-
scholars Prunetti et  al. (2013) developed a reported depression, anxiety, general symp-
protocol for Cognitive Evolutionary Therapy toms, duration of inpatient admissions after
specifically aimed at personality disorders the program was over, and continuation in an
(CET-PD). CET-PD is based on the Darwinian outpatient program. The results suggested
view that humans are driven by evolutionary- that CET-PD was effective in reducing the
selected motivations and develop psycho- level of depression and anxiety, with a change
pathologies when their biologically relevant that was stable for trait anxiety. Obsessive
goals are not met. Thus, failures in patients symptoms, paranoid ideation, psychoses, and
with personality disorder are explained by the feelings of self-inadequacy and inferiority
authors as resulting from disordered function- diminished. Overall, the results showed an
ing of evolutionary-shaped social motives improvement in psychopathology after
(Prunetti et al., 2013). The authors differentiate release and in follow-up sessions, a decrease
CET-PD from other treatments from which it in the number of further hospital admissions,
borrows, such as Cognitive Therapy (Beck, and an increased level of outpatient therapy
1976), Rational Emotive Therapy (Ellis and attendance (Prunetti et al., 2013).
Dryden, 2007), and Dialectical Behavioral
Therapy (Chapman, 2006).
Potential Applications of
Evolutionary Conceptualizations
Key Elements in CET-PD to Other Mental Conditions
The key elements of CET-PD include: In the previous section, we reviewed the pos-
sible integration of evolutionary insights in
1 Focus on restructuring schemas of self-with-others
around biologically relevant needs (attachment, psychoanalysis and analytical psychother-
caregiving, social ranking, mating, cooperation). apy, and we summarized the results from
2 Special focus on the therapeutic relationship. controlled studies that examined the efficacy
CET-PD places importance on the rapport of evolutionary interventions for depression
between the therapist and patient, with special and personality disorders. In the next section,
attention on discovering the specific motive that we briefly present, in a speculative manner
is active during the flow of therapy conversation. that needs further testing in controlled trials,
3 Assessing interpersonal motivations during the some potential applications of evolutionary
therapeutic relationship (e.g., attachment or conceptualizations to the treatment of other
social rank).
mental conditions.
4 Managing the therapeutic relationship to pre-
vent/repair ruptures.
5 Making people aware of how dysfunctional
schemas guide behaviors. Postpartum Depression
Conceptualizing postpartum depression as an
adapted response to unfavorable circumstances
Evidence in Favor of CET-PD
(e.g., child sickness, lack of resources or sup-
The authors examined the benefits of CET-PD port) (Hagen, 1999), evolutionary mismatch
in an intensive 20-hour weekly three-week (Crouch, 1999), or age (Bottino et  al., 2012)
residential treatment (both individual and may make a patient suffering from it more
group) of a wide range of severe personality likely to recover. A treatment aimed at increas-
disorders. Fifty-one patients with various ing fitness (e.g., by focusing on resource
12 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

acquisition) may be better than correcting patients showing improvement, even when
dysfunctional beliefs (‘I am a bad mother’). gold-standard treatments, such as CBT, are
From an evolutionary perspective, cognitive used (Loerinc et al., 2015). Conceptualizing
techniques could be tried to increase the per- SA as one of the poles (besides social domi-
ceived benefits of having a child and reduce nance) necessary to maintain social order
the perceived costs. Such a strategy might (Öhman, 1986), or as a vestigial response to
lead to a decrease in the severity of postpar- social threat (Trower and Gilbert, 1989), may
tum depression precisely because it addresses increase a patient’s acceptance of the symp-
evolutionary causes. For instance, a young toms. Moreover, evolutionary understandings
mother could understand that her symptoms of SA may serve as a guide in therapeutic
do not reflect her incapacity as a mother, but, decisions. For instance, in some cases, just
rather, a mechanism by which she is asking treating symptoms (through gradual expo-
for help. Thus, the intervention could focus sure, for instance) may not be enough, and
on coping mechanisms and problem-solving a discussion about eliminating or modifying
targeting the fundamental causes of the the circumstances that elicit symptoms may
symptoms, addressing not only the depres- be in order (Brosnan et al., 2017).
sive symptoms per se, but also the situation Providing patients with evolutionary
that led to them (decreased fitness). explanations of phobic symptoms is not pos-
sible in all the cases, so only some patients
will benefit from this kind of evolutionary-
aided case conceptualization. This approach
Anxiety Disorders
is suitable in the case of patients with fears
By distinguishing between situations in of biologically relevant stimuli, such as
which anxiety is disabling (when medication heights, public speaking, dark, blood, or
can be useful) and those where anxiety may certain types of animals, who could ben-
be adaptive, evolutionary theories can offer efit from logical distal explanations of the
meaningful case conceptualizations that can symptoms, and less so, if at all, in the case
help patients to accept these symptoms and of patients presenting fears of evolutionary-
possibly reduce impairment. For instance, a irrelevant objects, such as a fear of cotton
debilitating phobia of snakes might be balls or certain colors.
accepted and dealt with better by a patient if In other words, while evolutionary expla-
it is explained to her that fear of snakes is an nations of anxieties can be helpful in treat-
evolved fear which increased the likelihood ment, this does not mean a replacement of
of survival in ancestral times (Marks and current explanations, which typically rely
Nesse, 1994) and that her extreme anxiety on either proximal causes or distant, but not
around such stimuli is not a brain disorder, evolutionary ones (e.g., childhood traumas).
but an evolved, normal mechanism that may On the contrary, multi-layered explanations
be functioning in overdrive. Similarly, in (evolutionary, developmental, proximal
Obsessive Compulsive Disorder, conceptual- mechanisms) should be used, with evolution-
izing the symptoms as exacerbated mecha- ary insights helping in the creation of a more
nisms to facilitate reproduction and protect comprehensive causal picture of the problems
offspring (Feygin et  al., 2006) can lead to bothering a patient.
better acceptance of the symptoms, a key ele-
ment in the recovery process (Chamberlain
and Haaga, 2001). Dysmorphic Disorder
One of the most common forms of anxiety,
social anxiety (SA), is particularly resist- Dysmorphic disorder is explained evolution-
ant to treatment, with only about half of arily through one’s attempt to compare with
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND COUNSELING AND PSYCHOTHERAPY 13

others and the avoidance of rejection or ridi- and PTSD (Giosan and Wyka, 2009), which
cule, which are linked to lower status and might lead to novel, reproductive strategies-
lower mate value (Veale and Gilbert, 2014). based intervention protocols in the future.
Understanding the context and functions of
the behaviors associated with this condition
can be critical for the success of an interven- Eating Disorders
tion, especially when the patient has aversive
emotions, such as shame or rejection, which Evolutionary explanations of eating disorders
have not been properly processed (Veale and center on intrasexual competition (Li et  al.,
Gilbert, 2014). Cognitive behavioral tech- 2010) or on life-history strategy (Mehta et al.,
niques for treating this condition could be 2011). Such hypotheses can have clinical
improved through the analysis of the functions implications. As in the examples above, an
and contexts in which the behaviors appear. understanding of the mechanisms that activate
This can be realized via multiple routes, such when we eat certain foods can be therapeuti-
as (1) linking the body-related fears to fears of cally helpful when the patient’s cognitions are
rejection or to emotionally charged memories; addressed. In cognitive behavioral interven-
(2) rewriting of the narrative; (3) providing an tions, for instance, such explanations could
evolutionary context that separates the symp- facilitate the psychoeducational aspect of
toms from the feelings of shame and the therapy and can also aid in the generation of
affected person; or (4) the direct targeting of alternative thoughts that are to replace the
the feelings of shame and self-criticism and automatic, dysfunctional ones. Furthermore,
the development of social skills through com- the integration of evolutionary explanations of
passion (Veale and Gilbert, 2014). eating disorders in school curricula may put
young people in a better position to under-
stand human tendencies, which can then act as
an important protective factor.
Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder
(PTSD)
Evolutionary hypotheses of PTSD center on Substance Abuse
evolved mechanisms of avoiding dangers
(Silove, 1998; Wiedenmayer, 2004). Once a Evolutionary explanations of substance
person has been exposed to a traumatic event, dependence or abuse revolve around the fact
they will automatically learn to avoid that that people have consumed psychoactive sub-
type of situation, thus increasing their sur- stances over our recent and ancestral history
vival chances. In some vulnerable individu- (Dudley, 2004; Sullivan and Hagen, 2002),
als, this learning can be excessive or hard to with some authors arguing that drug
stop. While validated evolutionary interven- consumption can be associated with fitness
tions for PTSD have yet to be reported, the benefits (Kirillova et al., 2008). The mismatch
reinterpretation of PTSD symptoms as pro- between the past benefits associated with
duced by adaptations to protect an individual such behaviors and the easy access to such
from future harm, mechanisms that are found substances in our modern world can make
in other species as well (Zanette et al., 2019), some predisposed individuals consume them
can help patients to better understand and more, slowly driving them into addiction.
accept the condition and may improve the Understanding the distal explanations
therapeutic benefits offered by validated of substance consumption might be useful
interventions for PTSD, such as Gradual in therapy, especially in the conceptualiza-
Exposure Therapy. Furthermore, some tion phase of an intervention. In substance
authors have found a link between life history abuse, patients typically feel guilt and shame
14 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

(McGaffin et al., 2013). Evolutionary insights and Sprecher, 2016). In contrast, since men
integrated into therapy could potentially can impregnate a large number of women
reduce such reactions, deepening positive in a short period of time, they have evolved
therapeutic outcomes. stronger preferences for pursuing short-term
mating opportunities (Schmitt et  al., 2003).
Studies show a gender difference favoring
men in the number of sexual partners (Todd
APPLICATIONS OF EVOLUTIONARY et  al., 2009) and other research has shown
PSYCHOLOGY IN COUPLES THERAPY differences in sexual fantasies, with men
being more likely to fantasize about sexual
No section on the applications of evolution- variety (Ellis and Symons, 1990). Other
ary psychology to counseling and psycho- studies show that men are more permissive
therapy would be complete without a about casual sex and have a higher incidence
discussion about the many helpful elements of masturbation (Oliver and Hyde, 1993) and
that evolutionary psychology can bring to are more likely to be consumers of pornog-
couples therapy. Since evolutionary psychol- raphy (Hald, 2006), an element that has been
ogy examines the processes that have helped linked to couple dissatisfaction (Stewart and
our ancestors to survive and reproduce, it is Szymanski, 2012).
evident that it can bring insights into prob- Males’ stronger desire for multiple sexual
lems typically encountered in couples, such partners comes with a substantial threat to
as sexual incompatibilities, emotional and/or marriage, especially since men are some-
sexual infidelity, trust, gender stereotyping, times willing to leave their children behind
or control. for the pursuit of new relationships. Indeed,
An important class of results generated some authors have argued that men live in
by evolutionary psychology is that, when a state of ‘mild torment’ that stems from
it comes to mating, men and women are their propensity for sexual variety (Singer,
hardwired somewhat differently and their 1985a, 1985b). It is evident that such deeply
strategies to reach a common biological goal – engrained feelings can have catastrophic con-
­reproduction – can, at times, be quite different, sequences on a marriage or long-term rela-
which can be a source of conflict, potentially tionship. Therapists must be aware of such
leading to the dissolution of the couple. mechanisms and address them in therapy in
Studies on heterosexual mating prefer- a non-judgmental manner, as treating these
ences have documented gender commonali- tendencies as a ‘disease’ or lack of character
ties, such as dependability, faithfulness, and can destroy the therapeutic relationship. In
kindness (Barber, 1995; Buss, 1989; Buss addressing such issues, therapists must also
et  al., 1990) but also differences, in that be careful about balancing male needs and
women are more interested in earning capac- female needs. For instance, some authors have
ity, while men are more interested in physical stated that promoting commitment in therapy
beauty and health cues (e.g., skin smooth- may mean, in fact, promoting female repro-
ness, waist-to-hip ratio) of their partners, ductive interests at the expense of the male
with overlapping bell curves in such tenden- reproductive interests (Glantz and Moehl,
cies (Buss et al., 1990; Zhang et al., 2018). 2000). Such realities can make men feel they
Because women require a minimum of are not understood, which can alter the thera-
nine months investment (pregnancy) in order peutic relationship. Offering explanations of
to be successful at reproduction, and because the distal causes of the gender differences in
they cannot have nearly as many children sexual preferences is usually a good strategy
as a man can theoretically have, they have to navigate through these issues in therapy
evolved to be the choosier sex (Hatfield and sometimes helping a man deal with his
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND COUNSELING AND PSYCHOTHERAPY 15

conflicts about commitment is better done in extreme manifestations, such as delusions,


one-person therapy (Gilbert et al., 2014). it is a ­normal evolved mechanism that we
Men’s stronger preferences for sexual vari- should not be ashamed of. Reinterpretation of
ety are also linked to the so-called Coolidge jealousy as an adaptation that facilitates mate
effect, which is the sexual interest in a new retention may aid in the therapeutic process.
female, even when the male has reached sex- Understanding the important differences
ual satiation with his existing partner (Buss, in sexual preferences and tendencies between
1994; Dewsbury, 1981; Glantz and Pearce, men and women can help a couples thera-
1989). In humans, this phenomenon translates pist’s attempts to heal a fractured relation-
into greater interest for sex outside the pair ship. Some authors have argued that some of
and reduced interest for sex within the pair. the fundamental principles of therapy, such
This sexual boredom, affecting men and, also, as communication and sharing feelings, fail
women, but for different reasons, can under- to take into account core male needs (Glantz
mine a relationship. Therapists who understand and Moehl, 2000), potentially leading to the
that sexual boredom is not reversible and that inefficiency of the interventions. Indeed,
the passion of youth cannot be restored are in a men’s and women’s relating styles are differ-
much better position to help a couple in need of ent (Winstead et al., 1997), which may make
counseling (Glantz and Moehl, 2000). the former harder to engage in psychotherapy.
Moreover, since women, but not men, are Furthermore, the tabula rasa paradigm advo-
always certain that their babies are theirs, cated by the Social Science Standard Model
men are faced with the uncertainty of pater- assumes no innate gender differences, which
nity, which has led to gender differences in may lead to unreasonable therapeutic requests
the experience of feelings of jealousy. Thus, of males to reveal inner emotions and insecu-
women appear to be more affected by their rities (Shem and Surrey, 1998), further dam-
partners’ emotional infidelity, whereas men aging a potentially already fragile therapeutic
are more affected by their partners’ sexual relationship. Let’s not forget that studies have
infidelity (Buss et al., 1992; Daly et al., 1982). shown that women prefer confident men, who
This, in turn, makes women less likely to for- are able to protect their partners from other
give emotional infidelity, and men less likely men (Buss, 1989). This can and will make a
to forgive sexual infidelity (Shackelford man reluctant to display signals of weakness
et  al., 2002). The issue of jealousy appears and subordination both in front of his partner
often in couples therapy and in many a case and in front of the therapist. Clinicians who
one of the partners adamantly accuses the understand these nuances well are in a bet-
other of ‘destroying the relationship’ by ter position to establish rapport – critical for
being too jealous. Indeed, strong feelings of good outcomes – with a male patient in cou-
jealousy can lead to controlling behaviors ples therapy. For instance, since status is often
(e.g., controlling the partner’s social media a crucial factor for men, acknowledging it or
accounts), verbal or physical violence, sus- working to increase it can be an effective ther-
piciousness, isolation of the partner from apeutic strategy (Glantz and Moehl, 2000).
family and friends, and lack of trust, which Similarly, framing interventions in concrete
can undermine a relationship until its com- economic terms (costs/benefits/advantages),
plete dissolution. Clinicians would be well- as opposed to the more vague ‘better’, can
advised to use evolutionary insights in such lead to positive therapeutic outcomes (Glantz
situations and explain to their clients that and Moehl, 2000). Furthermore, given the fact
jealousy is, at its most fundamental level, that men are less likely to disclose emotions
a universal mate-guarding strategy (Buss, and feelings, encouraging communication and
2000), which has helped us pass on our genes deep disclosure, especially about weaknesses,
to the next generations, and that, barring may be counter-productive in some cases and
16 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

outright destructive when the disclosure might markedly better than the rest (Cuijpers et al.,
reveal profound couple incompatibilities, such 2008; Miller et  al., 2008; Smith and Glass,
as sexual (Glantz and Moehl, 2000). 1977), and there is no reason to believe that
When the issue of misuse of power, such evolutionary therapies are any different.
as anger directed toward family members, Indeed, the most influential factors appar-
comes up in therapy, some clinicians have ently common to virtually all schools are the
argued that this can lead to shaming and therapist’s technique and the rapport between
the activation of self-defense mechanisms client and therapist (Budge and Wampold,
in male patients, and that reframing, in the 2015). Since some evolutionary therapies
sense of explaining what the function of (e.g., CETD, described earlier in this chap-
competition among males is, may be a bet- ter), place, among others, a premium on the
ter therapeutic strategy, with the important client/therapist relationship, further research
observation that the therapist should not rec- should examine whether this emphasis might
ommend that a man simply give in to his part- be differentially associated with therapeutic
ner (Glantz and Moehl, 2000). success.
Last, but not least, the evolutionary inter-
ventions presented in this section have gen-
erally addressed specific mental disorders,
CRITICISM OF EVOLUTIONARY but there is debate in the field whether they
INTERVENTIONS exist as ‘real’ natural conditions to begin
with (First and Pincus, 2009). As such, more
In the previous sections, we succinctly pre- cross-diagnostic evolutionary interventions
sented some of the recent progress in the field should be attempted and tested, since a ther-
of evolutionary interventions for certain mental apy developed for a certain condition (e.g.,
disorders as well as possible applications of depression) may well be efficient for a differ-
evolutionary insights in couples therapy. ent one (e.g., anxiety or self-harm) (Wampold
Despite these promising developments, and Imel, 2015).
we must note the fact that the evolution-
ary hypotheses of mental disorders are, for
the most part, speculative and do not have
strong empirical support yet. Generally, SUMMARY
there is rivalry between hypotheses, with little
movement toward consensus, as well as slow This chapter briefly presented some of the
adoption by practitioners. In addition, while recent advances in clinical applications of evo-
some of the progress made in evolutionary lutionary psychology. Progress has recently
randomized clinical trials is noteworthy, it is been made in incorporating evolutionary
very difficult to draw incontrovertible conclu- insights into psychological interventions for
sions from medical-style randomized clinical depression and personality disorders, with sev-
trials in this field, except perhaps when they eral randomized clinical trials supporting such
can be pooled in bulk as meta-analyses. Even approaches already completed. Treatments of
then, it is hard to adjust for publication bias, other psychological problems, such as anxiety,
unaccounted placebo effects, statistical phe- substance abuse, and eating disorders, might
nomena, and other confounds (Westen et al., also benefit from the inclusion of evolutionary
2004). understandings of symptoms, although such
Another point of caution in evaluating the assumptions need to be tested in future con-
merits of evolutionary interventions is the trolled clinical studies.
fact that meta-analyses generally show that By offering distal explanations of sexual
no particular theoretical approach performs preferences, evolutionary psychology may
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND COUNSELING AND PSYCHOTHERAPY 17

also aid substantially in couples therapy. pharmacotherapy in the treatment of major


Issues like jealousy or infidelity can be bet- depressive disorder. Psychosomatic Medicine,
ter dealt with in couples therapy when they 69(7), 587–596. https://doi.org/10.1097/
are interpreted through evolutionary lenses, PSY.0b013e318148c19a
potentially leading to better therapeutic alli- Bond, F. W., Hayes, S. C., Baer, R. A., Carpenter,
K. M., Guenole, N., Orcutt, H. K., & Zettle,
ance and outcomes.
R. D. (2011). Preliminary psychometric
Despite these recent developments,
properties of the Acceptance and Action
much more research on the merits of such Questionnaire–II: A revised measure of psy-
approaches should be conducted, as the chological inflexibility and experiential avoid-
unclear role of common factors in evolution- ance. Behavior Therapy, 42(4), 676–688.
ary therapies, the speculative nature of many https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2011.03.007
evolutionary hypotheses of mental disorders, Botanov, Y., Keil, K., Ilardi, S. S., Scheller, V.,
and the lack of controlled evolutionary trials Sharp, K. L., & Williams, C. L. (2012). Success-
on cross-diagnostic symptoms make it hard ful treatment of depression via therapeutic
to draw definitive conclusions about the effi- lifestyle change: Preliminary controlled-trial
cacy of such efforts. results. Poster presented at the Annual confer-
ence of the Association for Psychological
Science. Retrieved from http://tlc.ku.edu/sites/
tlc.drupal.ku.edu/files/files/PosterAPS2012.
Note pdf (Accessed 30 November 2018).
Bottino, M. N., Nadanovsky, P., Moraes, C. L.,
1  The ABC model proposes that emotions (C) are
Reichenheim, M. E., & Lobato, G. (2012).
not caused by external events (A), but by beliefs
(B) and, in particular, irrational beliefs (IB) (Sar- Reappraising the relationship between mater-
racino et  al., 2017). The ABC model can also nal age and postpartum depression according
be referred to as the ‘ABCDE’ model, where D to the evolutionary theory: Empirical evidence
stands for the disputation of beliefs and E stands from a survey in primary health services. Journal
for new effect, the result of holding healthier of Affective Disorders, 142(1), 219–224.
beliefs (Jorn, 2016). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2012.04.030
Bowlby, J. (1983). Attachment: Attachment
and loss volume one (2nd edition). New
York, NY: Basic Books.
REFERENCES Bowlby, J. (2005). A secure base; Clinical appli-
cations of attachment theory (1st edition).
Barber, N. (1995). The evolutionary psychology London: Routledge.
of physical attractiveness: Sexual selection Brosnan, S. F., Tone, E. B., & Williams, L. (2017).
and human morphology. Ethology and Socio- The evolution of social anxiety. In T. K. Shack-
biology, 16(5), 395–424. https://doi.org/ elford & V. Zeigler-Hill (Eds.), The Evolution
10.1016/0162-3095(95)00068-2 of psychopathology (pp. 93–116). Springer:
Beck, A. T. (1976). Cognitive therapy and the Switzerland. https://doi.org/10.1007/
emotional disorders. New York, NY: Interna- 978-3-319-60576-0_4
tional Universities Press. Budge, S. L., & Wampold, B. E. (2015). The rela-
Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. tionship: How it works. In O. C. G. Gelo,
(1979). Cognitive therapy of depression. A. Pritz, & B. Rieken (Eds.), Psychotherapy
New York, NY: Guilford Press. research: Foundations, process, and outcome
Beck, A. T., Steer, A. R., & Brown, G. K. (pp. 213–228). Springer: Switzerland. https://
(1996). Manual for the Beck Depression doi.org/10.1007/978-3-7091-1382-0_11
Inventory—II. San Antonio, TX: The Psycho- Buss, D. M. (1989). Sex differences in human
logical Corporation. mate preferences: Evolutionary hypotheses
Blumenthal, J. A., Babyak, M. A., Doraiswamy, tested in 37 cultures. Behavioral and Brain
P. M., Watkins, L., Hoffman, B. M., Barbour, Sciences, 12(1), 1–14. https://doi.org/10.1017/
K. A., & Sherwood, A. (2007). Exercise and S0140525X00023992
18 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Buss, D. M. (1994). The evolution of desire: ology, 3(1), 11–27. https://doi.org/


Strategies of human mating. New York, NY: 10.1016/0162-3095(82)90027-9
Basic Books. Darwinian Psychotherapy. (2019). Retrieved
Buss, D. M. (2000). The dangerous passion: January 23, 2019, from https://darwini-
Why jealousy is as necessary as love and sex. anpsychotherapy.com
New York, NY: Free Press. Dewsbury, D. A. (1981). Effects of novelty of
Buss, D. M., Abbott, M., Angleitner, A., Asherian, copulatory behavior: The Coolidge effect and
A., Biaggio, A., Blanco-Villasenor, A., & Yang, related phenomena. Psychological Bulletin,
K.-S. (1990). International preferences in 89(3), 464–482. https://doi.org/10.1037/
selecting mates: A study of 37 cultures. Journal 0033-2909.89.3.464
of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 21(1), 5–47. Dudley, R. (2004). Ethanol, fruit ripening, and
https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022190211001 the historical origins of human alcoholism in
Buss, D. M., Larsen, R. J., Westen, D., & Sem- primate frugivory. Integrative and Compara-
melroth, J. (1992). Sex differences in jealousy: tive Biology, 44(4), 315–323. https://doi.org/
Evolution, physiology, and psychology. Psy- 10.1093/icb/44.4.315
chological Science, 3(4), 251–256. https://doi. Durisko, Z., Mulsant, B. H., & Andrews, P. W.
org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1992.tb00038.x (2015). An adaptationist perspective on the
Chamberlain, J. M., & Haaga, D. A. F. (2001). etiology of depression. Journal of Affective
Unconditional self-acceptance and psychologi- Disorders, 172, 315–323. https://doi.org/
cal health. Journal of Rational-Emotive and 10.1016/j.jad.2014.09.032
Cognitive-Behavior Therapy, 19(3), 163–176. Ellis, A. (1994). Reason and emotion in psycho-
https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1011189416600 therapy. Secaucus, NJ: Carol Publishing
Chapman, A. L. (2006). Dialectical behavior Group.
therapy. Psychiatry (Edgmont), 3(9), 62–68. Ellis, A., & Dryden, W. (2007). The practice
Cherkis, J. (n.d.). The best way to save people of rational emotive behavior therapy
from suicide. Retrieved January 28, 2019, from (2nd edition). New York, NY: Springer Pub-
https://highline.huffingtonpost.com/articles/ lishing Company.
en/how-to-help-someone-who-is-suicidal/ Ellis, A., Dryden, W., & DiGiuseppe, R. (2007).
Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1994). Origins of The practice of rational emotive behavior
domain specificity: The evolution of functional therapy. New York, NY: Springer Publishing
organization. In L. Hirschfeld & S. Gelman Company.
(Eds.), Mapping the Mind: Domain Specificity Ellis, B. J., & Symons, D. (1990). Sex differences
in Cognition and Culture (pp. 85–116). in sexual fantasy: An evolutionary psychologi-
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. cal approach. The Journal of Sex Research,
doi:10.1017/CBO9780511752902.005 27(4), 527–555. https://doi.org/10.1080/
Crouch, M. (1999). The evolutionary context 00224499009551579
of postnatal depression. Human Nature, Fennell, M. J., & Teasdale, J. D. (1987). Cognitive
10(2), 163–182. https://doi.org/10.1007/ therapy for depression: Individual differences
s12110-999-1013-x and the process of change. Cognitive Therapy
Crow, T. J. (2000). Schizophrenia as the price and Research, 11(2), 253–271. https://doi.
that homo sapiens pays for language: A org/10.1007/BF01183269
resolution of the central paradox in the Feygin, D. L., Swain, J. E., & Leckman, J. F.
origin of the species. Brain Research Reviews, (2006). The normalcy of neurosis: Evolution-
31(2–3), 118–129. ary origins of obsessive–compulsive disorder
Cuijpers, P., van Straten, A., Andersson, G., & and related behaviors. Progress in Neuro-­
van Oppen, P. (2008). Psychotherapy for Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychia-
depression in adults: A meta-analysis of com- try, 30(5), 854–864. https://doi.org/10.1016/
parative outcome studies. Journal of Consult- j.pnpbp.2006.01.009
ing and Clinical Psychology, 76(6), 909–922. First, M. B., & Pincus, H. A. (2009). Diagnosis
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013075 and classification. In M. G. Gelder, N. C.
Daly, M., Wilson, M., & Weghorst, S. J. (1982). Adreasen, J. J. Lopez-Ibor, & J. R. Geddes
Male sexual jealousy. Ethology and Sociobi- (Eds.), New Oxford Textbook of Psychiatry
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND COUNSELING AND PSYCHOTHERAPY 19

(pp. 99–121). Oxford: Oxford University Giosan, C., Muresan, V., & Moldovan, R.
Press. (2014). Cognitive evolutionary therapy for
First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Williams, J. B. W., & depression: A case study. Clinical Case
Gibbon, M. (1997). Structured clinical inter- Reports, 2(5), 228–236. https://doi.org/
view for DSM-IV SCID. Washington, DC: 10.1002/ccr3.131
American Psychological Association. Giosan, C., & Wyka, K. (2009). Is a successful
Fraser, B. (2012). Costly signalling theories: high-K fitness strategy associated with better
Beyond the handicap principle. Biology & mental health? Evolutionary Psychology,
Philosophy, 27(2), 263–278. https://doi.org/ 7(1), 28–39. https://doi.org/10.1177/
10.1007/s10539-011-9297-8 147470490900700104
Freud, S. (2017). The interpretation of dreams Giosan, C., Wyka, K., Mogoaşe, C., Cobeanu,
(A. A. Brill, Trans.). Digireads.com Publishing. O., & Szentagotai, A. (2018). The Evolution-
George, L. K. (1989). Stress, social support, and ary Fitness scale: A measure of the independent
depression over the life-course. In K. S. criterion of fitness. EvoS Journal: The Journal of
Markides & C. L. Cooper (Eds.), Aging, stress the Evolutionary Studies Consortium, 7(1),
and health (p. 241–267). John Wiley & Sons. 181–205.
Gilbert, P. (2001). Evolution and social anxiety: Giphart, R., & van Vugt, M. (2018). Mismatch:
The role of attraction, social competition, How our stone age brain deceives us every
and social hierarchies. Psychiatric Clinics, day (and what we can do about it). London:
24(4), 723–751. https://doi.org/10.1016/ Robinson.
S0193-953X(05)70260-4 Glantz, K., & Moehl, M.-B. (2000). Reluctant
Gilbert, P., Bailey, K. G., & McGuire, M. T. males: Evolutionary perspectives on male
(2014). Evolutionary psychotherapy: Princi- psychology in couples therapy. In P. Gilbert &
ples and outline. In P. Gilbert & K. G. Bailey K. G. Bailey (Eds.), Genes on the couch:
(Eds.), Genes on the couch: Explorations in Explorations in evolutionary psychotherapy
evolutionary psychotherapy (pp. 3–27). New (pp. 176–195). New York: Routledge.
York, NY: Routledge. Glantz, K., & Pearce, J. (1989). Exiles from
Giosan, C. (2020). Cognitive-Evolutionary Ther- Eden: Psychotherapy from an evolutionary
apy for Depression. Springer, Switzerland, perspective (1st edition). New York, NY: W.
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-38874-4 W. Norton & Co. Inc.
Giosan, C., Cobeanu, O., Mogoase, C., Mure- Glenn, A. L., Kurzban, R., & Raine, A. (2011).
san, V., Malta, L. S., Wyka, K., & Szentagotai, Evolutionary theory and psychopathy. Aggres-
A. (2014). Evolutionary cognitive therapy sion and Violent Behavior, 16(5), 371–380.
versus standard cognitive therapy for depres- https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2011.03.009
sion: A protocol for a blinded, randomized, Gutiérrez, F., Gárriz, M., Peri, J. M., Ferraz, L.,
superiority clinical trial. Trials, 15(1), 83. Sol, D., Navarro, J. B., & Valdés, M. (2013).
https://doi.org/10.1186/1745-6215-15-83 Fitness costs and benefits of personality dis-
Giosan, C., Cobeanu, O., Wyka, K., Muresan, V., order traits. Evolution and Human Behavior,
Mogoase, C., Szentagotai, A., & Moldovan, R. 34(1), 41–48. https://doi.org/10.1016/
(2019, June 29). Cognitive-evolutionary ther- j.evolhumbehav.2012.09.001
apy for depression – results of a randomized Hagen, E. H. (1999). The functions of postpar-
clinical trial. Presented at the 31st Annual tum depression. Evolution and Human
Human Behavior & Evolution Society Meeting, Behavior, 20(5), 325–359. https://doi.org/
Boston, MA. Retrieved from http://meetatbu. 10.1016/S1090-5138(99)00016-1
com/hbes19/ (Accessed 1 September 2019). Hald, G. M. (2006). Gender differences in por-
Giosan, C., Cobeanu, O., Wyka, K., Muresan, nography consumption among young het-
V., Mogoase, C., Szentagotai, A., Malta, L. S., erosexual Danish adults. Archives of Sexual
Moldovan, R. (2020). Cognitive evolutionary Behavior, 35(5), 577–585. https://doi.org/
therapy versus standard cognitive therapy for 10.1007/s10508-006-9064-0
depression: A single‐blinded randomized Hatfield, E., & Sprecher, S. (1995). Men’s and
clinical trial. Journal of Clinical Psychology; Women’s Preferences in Marital Partners in the
1–14. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.22991 United States, Russia, and Japan. Journal of
20 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Cross-Cultural Psychology, 26(6), 728–750. psychotherapy. In P. Gilbert & K. G. Bailey


https://doi.org/10.1177/002202219502600613 (Eds.), Genes on the couch: Explorations in
Hertler, S. C. (2014). A review and critique of evolutionary psychotherapy (pp. 71–92).
obsessive-compulsive personality disorder New York, NY: Routledge.
etiologies. Europe’s Journal of Psychology, Krupnik, V. (2014). A novel therapeutic frame
10(1), 168–184. https://doi.org/10.5964/ for treating depression in group Treating
ejop.v10i1.679 Depression Downhill. Sage Open, 4(1),
Jacobson, J. D., Lehman, K. A., Stites, B. A., 2158244014523793. https://doi.org/
Karwoski, L., Stroupe, N., Steidtmann, D., & 10.1177/2158244014523793
Ilardi, S. (2007). Therapeutic lifestyle change Krupnik, V. (2015). Integrating EMDR into an
for depression: Results of a randomized con- evolutionary-based therapy for depression:
trolled trial. Presented at the Annual confer- A case study. Clinical Case Reports,
ence of the Association for Behavioral and 3(5), 301–307. https://doi.org/10.1002/
Cognitive Therapies, Philadelphia, PA. ccr3.228
Retrieved from http://tlc.ku.edu/sites/tlc. Krupnik, V. (2018). Differential effects of an
drupal.ku.edu/files/files/2007ABCTPoster.pdf evolutionary-based EMDR therapy on depres-
(September 1 2018). sion and anxiety symptoms: A case series
John, S., & Segal, D.L. (2015). Case Conceptual- study. Journal of EMDR Practice and
ization. In R.L. Cautin & S.O. Lilienfeld (Eds.), Research, 12(2), 46–57. https://doi.org/
The Encyclopedia of Clinical Psychology. 10.1891/1933-3196.12.2.46
Chichester: Wiley. doi:10.1002/ Kuyken, W., Padesky, C. A., & Dudley, R. (2008).
9781118625392.wbecp106 The science and practice of case conceptual-
Jorn, A. (2016, May 17). Rational emotive ization. Behavioural and Cognitive Psycho-
behavior therapy. Retrieved November 10, therapy, 36(6), 757–768. https://doi.org/
2019, from //psychcentral.com/lib/rational- 10.1017/S1352465808004815
emotive-behavior-therapy/ Lam, D., & Gale, J. (2000). Cognitive behaviour
Karwoski, L., Prohaska, J. A., Lehman, K. A., therapy: Teaching a client the ABC model – the
Stites, B. A., Steidtmann, D., & Ilardi, S. S. first step towards the process of change. Jour-
(2005). Therapeutic Lifestyle Change (TLC) nal of Advanced Nursing, 31(2), 444–451.
For Depression: Initial Results with a Severely https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2648.
Depressed Outpatient Population. Poster 2000.01280.x
presented at the annual meeting of the Li, N. P., Smith, A. R., Griskevicius, V., Cason, M. J.,
American Psychological Association, Wash- & Bryan, A. (2010). Intrasexual competition
ington, D.C., August 18–21, 2005. and eating restriction in heterosexual and
Kirillova, G. P., Vanyukov, M. M., Kirisci, L., & homosexual individuals. Evolution and Human
Reynolds, M. (2008). Physical maturation, Behavior, 31(5), 365–372. https://doi.
peer environment, and the ontogenesis of org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.05.004
substance use disorders. Psychiatry Research, Loerinc, A. G., Meuret, A. E., Twohig, M. P.,
158(1), 43–53. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Rosenfield, D., Bluett, E. J., & Craske, M. G.
psychres.2007.02.017 (2015). Response rates for CBT for anxiety
Kriegman, D. (1998). Interpretation, the uncon- disorders: Need for standardized criteria.
scious, and psychoanalytic authority: Toward Clinical Psychology Review, 42, 72–82.
an evolutionary, biological integration of the https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2015.08.004
empirical/scientific method with the field- Markowitz, J. C. (1994). Psychotherapy of dys-
defining, empathic stance. In R. F. Bornstein & thymia. The American Journal of Psychiatry,
J. M. Masling (Eds.), Empirical perspectives on 151(8), 1114–1121. https://doi.org/10.1176/
the psychoanalytic unconscious (pp. 187–272). ajp.151.8.1114
Washington, DC: American Psychological Marks, I. F. M., & Nesse, R. M. (1994). Fear and
Association. fitness: An evolutionary analysis of anxiety
Kriegman, D. (2000). Evolutionary psychoanal- disorders. Ethology and Sociobiology, 15(5),
ysis: Toward an adaptive, biological perspec- 247–261. https://doi.org/10.1016/0162-
tive on the clinical process in psychoanalytic 3095(94)90002-7
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND COUNSELING AND PSYCHOTHERAPY 21

Martiny, K., Lunde, M., Undén, M., Dam, H., & Nesse, R. M. (1998). Emotional disorders in evo-
Bech, P. (2005). Adjunctive bright light in lutionary perspective. British Journal of Medi-
non-seasonal major depression: Results from cal Psychology, 71(4), 397–415. https://doi.org/
clinician-rated depression scales. Acta Psychi- 10.1111/j.2044-8341.1998.tb01000.x
atrica Scandinavica, 112(2), 117–125. https:// Öhman, A. (1986). Presidential address: Face the
doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.2005.00574.x beast and fear the face: Animal and social
Mayers, A. G., & Baldwin, D. S. (2006). The fears as prototypes for evolutionary analyses
relationship between sleep disturbance and of emotions. Psychophysiology, 23, 737–747.
depression. International Journal of Psychiatry Oliver, M. B., & Hyde, J. S. (1993). Gender dif-
in Clinical Practice, 10(1), 2–16. https://doi.org/ ferences in sexuality: A meta-analysis. Psy-
10.1080/13651500500328087 chological Bulletin, 114(1), 29–51.
McGaffin, B. J., Lyons, G. C. B., & Deane, F. P. O’Reilly, T., Dunbar, R., & Bentall, R. (2001).
(2013). Self-forgiveness, shame, and guilt in Schizotypy and creativity: An evolutionary
recovery from drug and alcohol problems. connection? Personality and Individual Dif-
Substance Abuse, 34(4), 396–404. https:// ferences, 31(7), 1067–1078. https://doi.org/
doi.org/10.1080/08897077.2013.781564 10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00204-X
McGuire, M., & Troisi, A. (1998). Darwinian psy- Peet, M., & Horrobin, D. F. (2002). A dose-
chiatry. Retrieved from www.oxfordclini- ranging study of the effects of ethyl-­
calpsych.com/view/10.1093/med:psych/ eicosapentaenoate in patients with ongoing
9780195116731.001.0001/med- depression despite apparently adequate
9780195116731 (Accessed 1 September 2019). treatment with standard drugs. Archives of
Mehta, S., Abed, R., Figueredo, A. J., Aldridge, General Psychiatry, 59(10), 913–919.
S., Balson, H., Meyer, C., & Palmer, R. (2011). Prunetti, E., Bosio, V., Bateni, M., & Liotti, G.
P02-113 – Eating disorders and intrasexual (2013). Three-week inpatient Cognitive Evolu-
competition: Testing an evolutionary hypoth- tionary Therapy (CET) for patients with person-
esis among young women. European Psy- ality disorders: Evidence of effectiveness in
chiatry, 26, 709. https://doi.org/10.1016/ symptoms reduction and improved treatment
S0924-9338(11)72414-2 adherence. Psychology and Psychotherapy:
Michels, R. (1997). Psychotherapeutic Theory, Research and Practice, 86(3), 262–279.
approaches to the treatment of anxiety and https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8341.
depressive disorders. The Journal of Clinical 2011.02060.x
Psychiatry, 58(Suppl 13), 30–32. Sarracino, D., Dimaggio, G., Ibrahim, R.,
Miller, S., Wampold, B., & Varhely, K. (2008). Popolo, R., Sassaroli, S., & Ruggiero, G. M.
Direct comparisons of treatment modalities (2017). When REBT goes difficult: Applying
for youth disorders: A meta-analysis. Psycho- ABC-DEF to personality disorders. Journal of
therapy Research: Journal of the Society for Rational-Emotive & Cognitive-Behavior Ther-
Psychotherapy Research, 18(1), 5–14. https:// apy, 35(3), 278–295. https://doi.org/10.1007/
doi.org/10.1080/10503300701472131 s10942-016-0258-7
Molina, J. D., López-Muñoz, F., Stein, D. J., Schmitt, D. P., Alcalay, L., Allik, J., Ault, L.,
Martín-Vázquez, M. J., Alamo, C., Lerma- Austers, I., Bennett, K. L., & International
Carrillo, I., & Calle-Real, M. de la. (2009). Sexuality Description Project. (2003). Universal
Borderline personality disorder: A review and sex differences in the desire for sexual variety:
reformulation from evolutionary theory. Tests from 52 nations, 6 continents, and 13
Medical Hypotheses, 73(3), 382–386. https:// islands. Journal of Personality and Social
doi.org/10.1016/j.mehy.2009.03.024 Psychology, 85(1), 85–104.
Motto, J. A. (1976). Suicide prevention for Shackelford, T. K., Buss, D. M., & Bennett, K.
high-risk persons who refuse treatment. (2002). Forgiveness or breakup: Sex differ-
Suicide & Life-Threatening Behavior, 6(4), ences in responses to a partner’s infidelity.
223–230. Cognition and Emotion, 16(2), 299–307.
Nesse, R. M. (1991). What good is feeling bad? https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930143000202
The Sciences, 31(6), 30–37. https://doi. Shapiro, F. (2017). Eye Movement Desensitiza-
org/10.1002/j.2326-1951.1991.tb02346.x tion and Reprocessing (EMDR) therapy, third
22 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

edition: Basic principles, protocols, and pro- Peacocks, Porsches, and Thorstein Veblen:
cedures (3rd edition). New York, NY: The Conspicuous consumption as a sexual signal-
Guilford Press. ing system. Journal of Personality and Social
Shem, S., & Surrey, J. (1998). We have to talk: Psychology, 100(4), 664–680. https://doi.org/
Healing dialogues between women and 10.1037/a0021669
men. New York, NY: Basic Books. Taljaard, T. (n.d.). Treating depression with
Silove, D. (1998). Is posttraumatic stress disorder tribal wisdom. Retrieved January 23, 2019,
an overlearned survival response? An from https://upliftconnect.com/treating-
evolutionary-learning hypothesis. Psychiatry, depression-with-tribal-wisdom/
61(2), 181–190. Todd, J., Cremin, I., McGrath, N., Bwanika, J.-B.,
Singer, B. (1985a). A comparison of evolutionary Wringe, A., Marston, M., & Żaba, B. (2009).
and environmental theories of erotic response Reported number of sexual partners: Com-
part I: Structural features. The Journal of Sex parison of data from four African longitudi-
Research, 21(3), 229–257. https://doi.org/ nal studies. Sexually Transmitted Infections,
10.1080/00224498509551265 85(Suppl 1), i72–i80. https://doi.org/10.1136/
Singer, B. (1985b). A comparison of evolutionary sti.2008.033985
and environmental theories of erotic response Troisi, A., & McGuire, M. T. (2000). Psychother-
part II: Empirical arenas. The Journal of Sex apy in the context of Darwinian psychiatry. In
Research, 21(4), 345–374. https://doi.org/ P. Gilbert & K. G. Bailey (Eds.), Genes on the
10.1080/00224498509551275 couch: Explorations in evolutionary psycho-
Smith, M. L., & Glass, G. V. (1977). Meta-analysis therapy (pp. 28–41). Brunner-Routledge.
of psychotherapy outcome studies. American Retrieved from http://psycnet.apa.org/
Psychologist, 32(9), 752–760. https://doi. record/2001-16368-000
org/10.1037/0003-066X.32.9.752 Trower, P., & Gilbert, P. (1989). New theoretical
Stevens, A. (1982). Archetype: A natural history conceptions of social anxiety and social phobia.
of the self. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. Clinical Psychology Review, 9(1), 19–35.
Stevens, A. (1999). On Jung: Updated edition Veale, D., & Gilbert, P. (2014). Body dysmorphic
(Updated, Subsequent edition). Princeton, disorder: The functional and evolutionary
NJ: Princeton University Press. context in phenomenology and a compassion-
Stevens, A. (2000). Jungian analysis and evolu- ate mind. Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive
tionary psychotherapy: An integrative and Related Disorders, 3(2), 150–160.
approach. In P. Gilbert & K. G. Bailey (Eds.), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jocrd.2013.11.005
Genes on the couch: Explorations in evolu- Wampold, B. E., & Imel, Z. E. (2015). The great
tionary psychotherapy (pp. 93–117). New psychotherapy debate: The evidence for what
York, NY: Routledge. makes psychotherapy work (2nd ­edition). New
Stewart, D. N., & Szymanski, D. M. (2012). Young York, NY: Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group.
adult women’s reports of their male romantic Westen, D., Novotny, C. M., & Thompson-­
partner’s pornography use as a correlate of Brenner, H. (2004). The empirical status of
their self-esteem, relationship quality, and empirically supported psychotherapies:
sexual satisfaction. Sex Roles, 67(5), 257–271. Assumptions, findings, and reporting in con-
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-012-0164-0 trolled clinical trials. Psychological Bulletin,
Sullivan, R. J., & Hagen, E. H. (2002). Psychotropic 130(4), 631–663. https://doi.org/10.1037/
substance-seeking: Evolutionary pathology or 0033-2909.130.4.631
adaptation? Addiction, 97(4), 389–400. Wiedenmayer, C. P. (2004). Adaptations or
Sulzer, S. H. (2015). Does ‘difficult patient’ pathologies? Long-term changes in brain
status contribute to de facto demedicaliza- and behavior after a single exposure to
tion? The case of borderline personality dis- severe threat. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral
order. Social Science & Medicine, 142, Reviews, 28(1), 1–12. https://doi.
82–89. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed. org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2003.09.005
2015.08.008 Williams, P. (2005). ‘Notes Upon a Case of
Sundie, J. M., Kenrick, D. T., Griskevicius, V., Obsessional Neurosis’. In R. J. Perelberg (Ed.),
Tybur, J. M., Vohs, K. D., & Beal, D. J. (2011). Freud: A modern reader (pp. 177–188).
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND COUNSELING AND PSYCHOTHERAPY 23

Routledge: NY https://doi.org/10.1002/ wild animals. Scientific Reports, 9(1), 1–10.


9780470713525.ch10. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-47684-6
Winstead, B. A., Derlega, V. J., & Rose, S. (1997). Zhang, L., Lee, A. J., DeBruine, L. M., & Jones,
Gender and close relationships. Thousand B. C. (2018, December 4). Are sex differ-
Oaks, CA: Sage. ences in preferences for physical attractive-
Zanette, L. Y., Hobbs, E. C., Witterick, L. E., ness and good earning capacity in potential
MacDougall-Shackleton, S. A., & Clinchy, M. mates smaller in countries with greater
(2019). Predator-induced fear causes PTSD- gender equality?. https://doi.org/10.31234/
like changes in the brains and behaviour of osf.io/mtsx8
2
Evolutionary Psychology and
Psychiatry
Riadh Abed and Paul St John-Smith

INTRODUCTION and favours mechanistic explanations of


disease and disorder. However, unlike medi-
Psychiatry is a branch of medicine that deals cine where human physiology provides clear
with mental disorders that manifest them- reference points for normal functioning,
selves through disturbances in cognition, emo- psychiatry has attempted to identify disorder
tions, and behaviour. Like the rest of medicine and dysfunction without a coherent theory of
but unlike psychology (with the exception of normal human psychology (Nesse, 2019). We
clinical psychology), psychiatry is an interven- argue in this chapter that evolutionary psy-
tionist discipline that aims to modify the signs chology and evolutionary biology can serve as
and symptoms of disorder in order to reduce/ a vital basic science for psychiatry.
relieve individual distress and reduce risk Despite the publication of notable evolution-
(harm) to the individual and/or others. The ary psychiatry texts over the last couple of dec-
contemporary failure of psychiatry to make ades as well as numerous scholarly articles in
significant progress in understanding the aetiol- peer-reviewed journals, evolutionary thinking
ogy of mental disorders has been characterized has remained underappreciated by mainstream
as a ‘crisis’ by leading evolutionists (Brune psychiatry (e.g. Brune, 2015; Del Giudice,
et al., 2012); a fact that has also been acknowl- 2018; McGuire and Troisi, 1998; Nesse, 2019;
edged in an article in ‘Science’ that stated that Stevens and Price, 2000a). Although a plu-
there have been no major breakthroughs in the ralistic and multi-level approach to causality
treatment of schizophrenia for 50 years nor in mental health remains essential (Kendler,
in the treatment of depression for 20 years 2008), the current pluralism is unconstrained
(Akil et al., 2010). and lacks any recognizable framework (Abed,
Mainstream psychiatry, like the rest of 2000). While it is recognized that all mental
medicine, focuses on proximate causation phenomena are mediated by physical events
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND PSYCHIATRY 25

in the brain, the phenotypic end-products of appealing, having considerable face validity,
interest to psychiatry cannot be understood problems with its clinical utility linger
by examining the behaviour of neurons alone because our understanding of the function of
(a situation compared to trying to understand the neurobiological systems involved in
the mechanics of bird flight through the study mental disorder remains poor (First, 2007). In
of feathers (Marr, 1982)). addition, whereas the emphasis on context is
We propose evolution as being ideally acknowledged to be important or even vital in
placed to guide psychiatrists in determining determining the existence of mental disorder
what the phenotypic end-products of neuro- (Nesse, 2007), this potentially reduces the
biological systems constitute. Such evolu- diagnostic inter-rater reliability subsequent to
tionary emphasis on function can provide the the increased scope of subjective judgement,
scientific basis for expanding the concept of generating concern for the authors of official
the biological to encompass the psychologi- classification systems such as the DSM-5
cal, social, and cultural domains (Abed and (American Psychiatric Association, 2013).
St John-Smith, 2016). Hence, in contrast with Hence, while the DSM-5 accepts mental dis-
mainstream biological psychiatry’s narrow orders necessarily involve internal dysfunc-
‘decontextualized’ view of mental disorder tion and that this produces harm and/or
as brain disorder (Andreasen, 1984), evolu- distress, it leaves the term ‘dysfunction’ unde-
tionists consider the environmental context to fined. Furthermore, whereas context is con-
be vital in determining the existence of men- sidered in a range of conditions, it is excluded
tal disorder (Nesse, 2019). in others. For example, in the DSM-5, unlike
its predecessors, low mood lasting longer than
two weeks can now be diagnosed as major
depressive disorder (MDD) following a major
THE CONCEPT OF MENTAL DISORDER bereavement (Kavan and Barone, 2014).
Del Giudice (2018) submits that a number
Despite its widespread adoption within psy- of facets must be recognized to avoid com-
chiatry and medicine generally, the concept mon errors in interpreting Wakefield’s HD
of disorder has been difficult to define with concept, including the fact that dysfunctions
precision (Nesse, 2001). One influential evo- can arise from a number of different causes,
lutionary proposal is that mental disorder both internal and external; that the concept
represents a hybrid concept, with a biological of dysfunction is fuzzy; and that systems can
and a socio-cultural component; a ‘harmful have degrees of functionality where the line
dysfunction’ (HD) (Wakefield, 1992). of demarcation between function and dys-
Accordingly, the biological component of any function is unclear.
disorder is the failure of a biological mecha- While there are undoubted benefits from
nism to perform its evolved function, and the an evolutionary analysis of the concept of
value-laden component identifies that the dys- mental disorder, we support Troisi’s (2015)
function inflicts harm or damage on the conclusion that evolutionary biology alone
affected individual as judged by socio-cultural does not resolve the central question of what
standards. Although Wakefield’s HD concept should (and should not) be categorized as a
has been subject to criticism (e.g. Bolton, mental disorder, as ethical, health, and social
2007; Fulford and Thornton, 2007), it is policy considerations lie outside the remit
acknowledged to be a significant improve- of evolutionary science. In other words,
ment on existing formulations (e.g. First, it is important not only to appreciate how
2007; Nesse, 2007). However, while the bio- evolutionary biology can help advance our
logical criterion of the failure of a system to understanding of mental disorder but also to
perform its evolved function is intellectually understand its limits.
26 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

THE REMIT OF PSYCHIATRY • Mental disorders are biological disorders involv-


ing brain circuits that implicate specific domains
of cognition, emotion, or behaviour;
In addition to the DSM-5, the other major clas-
• Each level of analysis needs to be understood
sification system of mental disorders in clinical
across a dimension of function; and
use throughout most of the world, outside the • Mapping the cognitive, circuit, and genetic
United States, is the ICD-10 issued by the aspects of mental disorders will yield new and
World Health Organization (WHO, 1992). better targets for treatment.
Both systems endeavour to follow an atheoreti-
cal approach to the definition and differentia- The RDoC approach is rooted in experimen-
tion of mental disorder and, with the exception tal neuroscience and lists five domains: posi-
of organic mental disorders, base their diagnos- tive valance systems, negative valence
tic categories broadly on symptom clusters and systems, cognitive systems, systems for
duration. Context is acknowledged in some social processes, and arousal and regulatory
instances. The ICD-10 definition of mental systems. Each system has a number of con-
disorder, being more succinct than that of the structs and these are investigated using a
DSM, omits the assumption of an internal dys- number of units of analysis ranging from the
function, and proceeds as follows: ‘a clinically molecular level to individual behaviour. The
recognizable set of symptoms or behaviours RDoC has been characterized as a bottom-up
associated in most cases with distress and with approach to the classification of mental dis-
interference with personal functioning’ (WHO, orders, grounded in the latest research in
1992: 11). Their main categories of adult biological sciences that can cut across exist-
mental disorder comprise organic mental dis- ing DSM/ICD categories (Del Giudice,
orders, mental disorders secondary to psycho- 2018). However, critics have raised concerns
active substance use, schizophrenia and related regarding the neglect of context (above and
disorders, mood disorders, anxiety and stress- beyond the DSM or ICD) and neglect of the
related disorders, behavioural syndromes asso- role of evolution (Wakefield, 2014).
ciated with physiological disturbances, and
personality and other behaviour disorders.
Other chapters deal with mental retardation,
developmental disorders, and mental disorders EVOLUTION AND CAUSALITY
of childhood and adolescence. Remarkably,
given that both the ICD and DSM are systems
The application of evolutionary thinking to
based on the consensus of committees, these
psychiatry commences by considering some
categorical domains continue to demarcate
general principles that apply to all biological
effectively the current boundaries of psychiatric
phenomena. Tinbergen (1963) proposed that a
practice (Nesse and Stein, 2012). Nevertheless,
complete understanding of any biological trait
criticism remains directed against both systems
or system involves understanding its mecha-
for increasing reliability at the expense of
nism, developmental history (collectively
validity (Insel, 2013).
referred to as proximate causes), phylogenetic
The National Institute of Mental Health in
history, and function (referred to as ultimate or
the United States, in an attempt to overcome
evolutionary causes) (Table 2.1).
these shortcomings, proposed the Research
These are referred to as Tinbergen’s four
Diagnostic Criteria (RDoC). The four princi-
questions and all four apply simultaneously
ples used to formulate the RDoC system were
to biological phenomena (Gluckman et  al.,
explained as follows (Insel, 2013):
2009). It is acknowledged that unlike proxi-
• A diagnostic approach based on the biology as mate causation which can directly lead to
well as the symptoms must not be constrained therapeutic interventions, understanding evo-
by the current DSM categories; lutionary or ultimate causation is somewhat
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND PSYCHIATRY 27

Table 2.1  Tinbergen’s four questions


Developmental/historical Characteristics of trait/system
Proximate causation 1. Ontogeny: how does the trait develop 2. Mechanism: how does it work?
during the lifetime of the organism?
Evolutionary or 3. Phylogeny: what is the phylogenetic 4. Adaptive function: How has the trait or system
ultimate causation history of the trait? contributed to the organism’s inclusive fitness
in its natural environment?

Source: Adapted from Nesse (2013).

removed from direct clinical applications but and not disorders themselves (Nesse, 2019).
is no less important. Neglecting the question of This applies throughout medicine, including
function (ultimate causation) runs the risk of psychiatry, and stems primarily from the
psychiatrists inadvertently altering psycholog- demonstration that bodies and brains are a
ical functioning through their interventions to bundle of adaptations shaped by selection
relieve distressing but adaptive states, leading over thousands of generations to increase
to potentially negative consequences for some reproductive success and not good health,
patients. It can also lead us to construct defec- happiness, or longevity. The answer to the
tive models of how psychopathology arises. pivotal conundrum of why evolution has left
Focusing exclusively on the proximate humans so vulnerable to disease and disorder
is akin to a technician’s view of a machine, has itself been evolving ever since it was first
whereas considering ultimate causation as posed by the founders of modern evolution-
well is more like an engineer’s view (Nesse, ary medicine (Nesse and Williams, 1994).
2019). Hence, it may seem adequate for a busy Accordingly, pathways by which evolution-
clinician to simply recognize the existence of ary processes can lead to the existence and
depression or anxiety in a given patient and to persistence of disease or disorder have been
dispense standard advice and treatment accord- proposed (Box 2.1).
ingly. However, a clinician who also under- These causal pathways are not mutually
stands why we have such emotions in the first exclusive and several may be implicated
place and how emotional systems interact with concurrently or sequentially in the origin
people’s current lives is likely to have a deeper of mental disorders. They represent a list of
understanding of the patient’s emotional prob- ultimate causes of our vulnerability to mental
lems and is able to take greater account of the disorder. Examples of many of these causal
patient’s circumstances that may be contribut- pathways will be given in the sections below.
ing to their current state. It also has the poten- These evolutionary explanations for vul-
tial for influencing the research agenda through nerability to disorder are based on the rec-
testing hypotheses regarding what the normal ognition that selection is unable to eliminate
function is of the system that is giving rise to all harmful mutations, and can be too slow
psychopathology; a question that is seldom to respond to rapidly changing environments,
asked by mainstream psychiatry (Brune, 2015). creating states of evolutionary mismatch (Del
Giudice, 2018). This concept of ‘mismatch’
is crucial for understanding and explaining
the existence of many diseases and disorders
CAUSAL PATHWAYS FOR THE of modernity such as obesity, metabolic syn-
PERSISTENCE OF DISEASE AND drome, Type 2 diabetes, eating disorders, and
DISORDER many others. Evolutionary mismatch occurs
when the environment changes too rapidly
It is obligatory to recognize that selection for selection to be able to track it, resulting in
shapes vulnerability to disease and disorder residual traits that are no longer suited to the
28 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

BOX 2.1 Pathways for the persistence of disease and disorder 

(Adapted from Gluckman et  al. (2009) and Crespi (2016); for definition of terms see glossary: www.rcpsych.
ac.uk/docs/default-source/members/sigs/evolutionary-psychiatry-epsig/evolutionary-psychiatry-glossary-2.
pdf?sfvrsn=707dd6b_2)

• Mismatch
• Life history factors
• Overactive defence mechanisms
• Co-evolutionary considerations: consequences of the arms race against pathogens
• Constraints imposed by evolutionary history
• Trade-offs
• Sexual selection and its consequences
• Balancing selection: maintaining an allele that raises disease risk
• Demographic history and its consequences
• Selection favours reproductive success at the expense of health
• Deleterious alleles
• Extremes of adaptations

new environment. Developmental mismatch 2015). Hence, LHT provides a framework


arises when circumstances alter radically for understanding how organisms allocate
during an individual’s lifetime. For example, time and energy in achieving core biosocial
moving from a state of impoverishment dur- goals across the lifespan. Life history strate-
ing early development to a state of affluence gies involve a series of trade-offs that shape
in adult life can increase the risk of cardiovas- important biological developments including
cular disease, Type 2 diabetes, and metabolic the timing of sexual maturity, the number and
syndrome (Gluckman and Hanson, 2006). quality of offspring, and the length of lifespan
Furthermore, the extreme ends of functional (Stearns, 1992).
adaptations can become maladaptive e.g. The application of LHT demonstrates that
when adaptive personality traits are magni- the trade-offs yield a spectrum of life his-
fied (Trull and Widiger, 2013). Additionally, tory strategies where the trade-offs include
over-activation of useful emotional defences somatic versus reproductive effort, present
(mood states and anxiety) can result in harm- versus the future, and quality versus quan-
ful outcomes, leading to defence activation tity of offspring (Figure 2.1). The ‘fast’ end
disorders (Del Giudice, 2018). of the spectrum is characterized by a shorter
It is important to understand that selection lifespan, faster growth, earlier maturation
necessitates trade-offs. Increasing one trait and reproduction, and a larger number of
is often at the expense of worsening perfor- offspring, while those at the slow end of the
mance of another. For example, increasing life history spectrum show the opposite char-
resistance to infections increases the risk of acteristics (Del Giudice, 2018). Differences
autoimmune diseases. Improving nutritional in life history strategies are partly under
conservation increases the risk of obesity. genetic control but it appears that the nature
Trade-offs are also involved in life history and quality of the individual’s early environ-
strategies. Life history theory (LHT) deals ment may also be important (Belsky et  al.,
with species-typical solutions for problems 1991; Ellis et  al., 2011) (see Barbaro et  al.,
associated with survival and reproduction that 2016 for a different perspective). This ren-
change over an individual’s lifespan (Brune, ders LHT important for the understanding
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND PSYCHIATRY 29

Lifetime Energy Investment

Reproductive
Somatic Effort
Effort
(Slower)
(Faster)

Mating
Parental Effort
Effort
(Slower)
(Faster)

Figure 2.1  Life history strategy trade-offs

of vulnerability to mental disorders (Brune, Hence, an evolutionary analysis provides a


2015; Del Giudice, 2018) (see later section theoretical framework that enables us to dis-
‘Evolutionary Models of Mental Disorders’). tinguish states of mental distress and mental
One major insight that follows from under- disorder that arise from functional or dys-
standing the evolutionary causal pathways functional systems, and also provides a more
for the persistence of disease and disorder effective way of understanding the role of
is the recognition that mental distress can environmental context.
arise from functional systems. Hence, an
evolutionary taxonomy of treatable (undesir-
able) mental health conditions goes beyond
harmful dysfunctions (Tooby and Cosmides, GENETICS AND HERITABLE RISKS
1999). Undesirable conditions may result OF MENTAL DISORDERS
from different scenarios as summarized
below (Del Giudice, 2018): Taking an evolutionary perspective is tanta-
mount to turning genetics on its head. Hence,
• Undesirable mental health conditions can either whereas a non-evolutionary view may con-
arise from: sider specific DNA sequences as the primary
{{ harmful dysfunctions (system breakdowns); or biological cause of a given trait, an evolution-
{{ functional mechanisms, which can be either: ary approach seeks to understand the selec-
• maladaptive states at population level (e.g. evo- tion pressures over evolutionary history that
lutionary mismatch), or are: led to the retention of these genes. So, evolu-
• currently adaptive, but outcomes may vary, tionary views consider environmental influ-
resulting in:
ences at two distinct levels, first over
{{ maladaptive outcomes at the indi-
evolutionary history (leading to the shaping
vidual level (e.g. overactive defences,
developmental mismatches), or: of adaptations) and, second, the ontogenic
{{ adaptive outcomes at the individual effects of the environment during the indi-
level even if considered harmful by vidual’s lifetime.
others (e.g. antisocial personality/psy- Mental disorders require a degree of
chopathy). heritability, and hence some genetic basis,
30 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

before becoming candidates for evolutionary Differential Susceptibility


explanations. Remarkably, 55% of all coding
genes in humans are expressed in the brain. Research has demonstrated that people pos-
This renders the brain a prime target for sessing at least one s-allele of the serotonin
mutations and evolutionary changes (Brune, transporter gene HTTLPR incur increased risk
2015). of developing depression when facing adverse
After considering heterogeneity and uncer- events. However, the same variation is linked
tainty, psychiatric disorders demonstrate a to superior cognitive performance in several
degree of heritability suggesting a moderate domains and increases social conformity
degree of heritable risk. For example, 90% (Homberg and Lesch, 2011). A balanced poly-
of trait variation for autism can be accounted morphism also explains the frequency of a
for with genetics; bipolar disorder, 85%; particular SNP in the general population, and
schizophrenia, 81%; unipolar depression, why it has not been selected against. Beyond
37% (Kendler, 2001). Similarly, heritabil- this important concept, evolutionary theory
ity estimates for anxiety disorders range has aided in developing the idea that a particu-
from 30% to 45% (Hettema et  al., 2001). lar SNP such as the s-allele of the 5-HTTLPR
Family studies (including twin and adoption not only confers heightened risk for depression
studies) provide consistent evidence that under unfavourable conditions, but lower risk
genetic factors are involved in the presenta- for depression under favourable environmental
tion of these syndromes (Kendler and Eaves, conditions such as parental warmth and emo-
2005). tional availability during important develop-
Two types of heterogeneity have been mental stages. This phenomenon is referred to
identified in association with psychiatric as ‘differential susceptibility’ (Belsky, 1997;
genetics: causal and clinical. Causal hetero- Pluess and Belsky, 2010), where phenotypic
geneity refers to two or more causes indepen- plasticity occurs in response to early environ-
dently inducing the same clinical syndrome. mental conditions, and differs radically from
Clinical heterogeneity occurs when a single genetically mediated resilience which involves
cause leads to multiple clinical syndromes unresponsiveness to environmental conditions.
(Tsuang et al., 2003). The specific phenomenon of differential
Natural selection does not directly select response to positive experiences is referred to
for genes that cause disease or disorder, so as ‘vantage sensitivity’; a concept that shows
other explanations for their persistence must promise in assessing the likelihood of respond-
be considered. Accordingly, alongside any ing to psychological interventions (de Villiers
degree of heritability psychiatrists should et al., 2018).
ask: ‘Why does this mental disorder exist and This example serves as evidence against
persist?’ Mental disorders may be actively simple genetic determinism and also provides
maintained through a number of evolutionary an indication that aspiring to alter genes alone
processes. These include: A) despite natural to treat disorders may not be in an individual’s
selection e.g. (i) mutation-selection balance, interests as differing circumstances alter the
(ii) ancestral neutrality; and B) because of harmfulness or benefits of such a gene.
natural selection, (i) balancing selection,
(ii) antagonistic pleiotropy, (iii) stabilizing
selection on continuous traits, (iv) alternating Mutation Load and Mental
selection, and (v) functioning adaptations. Disorder
These categories are not mutually exclu-
sive, and there may be multiple mechanisms Mutation load has been implicated in the
maintaining some disorders in the population causation of some mental disorders (Keller
(Durisko et al., 2016). and Miller, 2006), referring to de novo
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND PSYCHIATRY 31

germ-line mutations passed on from parents, Usually in autosomal genes, expression occurs
rather than somatic mutations. Because ova go from both alleles. However, in a very small
through far fewer replications than sperm, fraction, one of the two alleles is switched off
paternal age at conception was suspected as the or ‘imprinted’, which may have significant
primary source of de novo mutations (Crow, effects on behaviour, as many are expressed in
2000). Paternal age is associated with increased the brain (Wilkinson et  al., 2007). Genomic
risk of mental disorders generally (Hare and imprinting represents a form of intragenomic
Moran, 1979). Mutation load is believed to play conflict, whereby different alleles and loci
a significant role in the causation of schizophre- express the fitness interest of one of the par-
nia and this is especially the case in childhood ents (Crespi, 2019). Intragenomic conflict
onset (Ahn et  al., 2014; Caplan, 2016) (for a arises from the asymmetry in the confidence
contrary view, see Ek et al., 2014). regarding parental relatedness to offspring
For autistic-spectrum disorder (ASD), between the sexes. The conjecture is that
mutation load was more significant in females paternally expressed (maternally imprinted)
and in severe cases (Jacquemont et al., 2014). genes in an individual exert phenotypic effects
The risk of attention deficit hyperactivity that increase fitness-related demands imposed
disorder (ADHD) has been found to be posi- by offspring upon the mother, due to the lower
tively related to paternal age (Chudal et  al., probability of relatedness of paternal genes
2015; D’Onofrio et al., 2014; Russell et al., (than maternal genes) within a given brood.
2014, 2015). This is thought to be because mothers are
In depression, no significant relationship always related to offspring by 50%, while the
has been found with paternal age but there offspring of a given female can have different
is increased risk with maternal age, suggest- fathers (Crespi, 2019).
ing prenatal stress as a factor (Del Giudice, Contrastingly, maternally expressed (pater-
2018). In eating disorders and obsessive– nally imprinted) genes are predicted to exert
compulsive disorder (OCD), the relationship the reverse effect, namely, lower demands
with mutation load remains inconclusive imposed on mothers. Hence, sometimes incre-
(Del Giudice, 2018). mental investment will be favoured by paternal
Conversely, young paternal and maternal genes but resisted by maternal genes (Haig,
age is also related to the risk of a range of 2014). Intriguingly, maternal gene imprinting
mental disorders in offspring. This, however, (paternal expression) may be one cause for the
is not related to mutation load but rather to underdevelopment of the ‘social brain’, gener-
heritability of fast life history strategies, as ating a higher risk of ASD, whereas the pater-
fast life history is associated with early par- nal gene imprinting (maternal gene expression)
enthood in both men and women and predicts may predispose to hyper-­development of the
a greater risk of fast life history spectrum dis- social brain and increased risk of schizophre-
orders in the offspring of young parents (most nia and related psychosis (Crespi, 2019) (see
notably ADHD and schizophrenia spectrum section ‘Schizophrenia Spectrum Disorders
disorders) (see ‘Evolutionary Models of (SSDs)’, para. E, below).
Mental Disorders’ section below).

EVOLUTIONARY MODELS OF
Genomic Imprinting and
MENTAL DISORDERS
Mental Disorder
In diploid species such as humans, each auto- In contrast to the avowedly atheoretical
somal gene is represented by two alleles, approach of the DSM/ICD systems described
with one copy inherited from each parent. above and the bottom-up biological approach
32 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

of the RDoC, evolutionary frameworks for F-type, S-Type, and D-type. The system is
the classification of mental disorder are currently aimed at use by researchers rather
top-down systems with explicit theoretical than clinicians and it does not currently
assumptions. They tend to utilize high-level accommodate organic mental disorders or
organizing principles derived from evolu- mental handicap.
tionary insights regarding the adaptive sig-
nificance of various brain systems. Such a
top-down approach remains compatible with
a range of existing non-evolutionary EVOLUTIONARY THINKING ABOUT
approaches (Del Giudice, 2018). According SELECTED PSYCHIATRIC DISORDERS
to Del Giudice (2018), any coherent frame-
work for mental disorder (evolutionary or It is important to note that due to the dual
otherwise) should meet four main challenges: problems of heterogeneity and co-morbidity
explain patterns of co-morbidity; address that beset current classification systems (Del
heterogeneity within diagnostic categories; Giudice, 2018), none of the evolutionary
bridge psychopathology with individual dif- theories discussed in this section can account
ferences; and account for developmental for the full range of the conditions they pur-
features of mental disorders including life port to explain. Heterogeneity in this context
course trajectories. The evolutionary frame- refers to the likelihood that most common
work proposed by Del Giudice (2018) based mental disorders are a collection of disparate
on LHT is more comprehensive and wide- conditions that share certain clinical features
ranging than others such as the diametric but may differ in their causation.
model of ASD and psychosis (Crespi, 2019;
‘Mutation Load and Mental Disorder’ section,
above) and the externalizing–internalizing Depression
model (Martel, 2013). Del Giudice (2018)
suggests that his proposed framework meets Sadness is universally recognized as the
all four challenges and offers an alternative normal emotional response to loss, setbacks,
to the existing trans-diagnostic taxonomies and reversals in life (Horowitz and Wakefield,
of mental disorders such as the RDoC. The 2007). Unlike anxiety (a state of vigilance
most recent version of this framework has designed to detect and deal with risk and pre-
been expanded to include a primary dimen- vent/reduce harm), there is no consensus on
sion of fast–slow life history strategy sup- the function(s) of sadness. Depressive disor-
plemented by a secondary dimension of ders are marked by a severe negative mood
defence-activation and hence the model has with an inability to experience pleasure. In
been dubbed the FSD model (Del Giudice, addition to low mood or anhedonia lasting a
2018). It is based on a core proposition, minimum of two weeks the DSM-5 requires
namely that the risk of developing a mental the existence of four or more symptoms (loss
disorder depends on a pattern of individual or gain in weight, insomnia or hypersomnia,
differences that can be understood as mani- agitation or retardation, fatigue/loss of energy,
festations of alternative life history strate- feelings of worthlessness or inappropriate
gies. Hence, moving along the fast–slow life guilt, poor concentration or indecisiveness,
history dimension will increase the risk of and thoughts of death and suicide) for a diag-
certain mental disorders and reduce the risk nosis of major depressive disorder (MDD)
of others e.g. fast life history strategies (American Psychiatric Association, 2013).
increase the risk of psychosis while reducing Although the DSM-5 treats MDD as a unitary
the risk of autism, and vice versa. The FSD condition, the application of its criteria allows
model generates three clusters of disorders: for a wide variety of combinations where
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND PSYCHIATRY 33

individual patients can share few or even no (Power et  al., 2013). Depression occurs at
symptoms (Fried and Nesse, 2015). both ends of the fast–slow life history contin-
Although many accept that depressive dis- uum, with a fast life history subgroup of both
orders are a highly heterogeneous collection males and females having early puberty and
of conditions (Akiskal and McKinney, 1975; a slow life history subgroup (mainly males)
Brune, 2015; Gilbert, 2006; Rantala et  al., having late puberty (Del Giudice, 2018).
2018), most evolutionary theories of depres- Hence, depression is not so much a slow life
sion still treat it as if it was a unitary con- history strategy as a ‘slowing down’ strategy
dition with a single explanation. Depression that can occur across the life history strategy
remains one of the most common mental spectrum (Brune, 2015).
disorders in clinical practice, with a lifetime Although there is lack of agreement on the
risk in the US population that exceeds 15% precise function of low mood, most evolu-
(Blazer et al., 1994) and striking at increas- tionists agree that the capacity for low mood
ingly younger ages (Rottenberg, 2014). The has been shaped by selection because of its
increase in prevalence of depression in mod- contribution to inclusive fitness in the ances-
ern societies most probably results from evo- tral environment. Disagreements between
lutionary mismatch (Brune, 2015; Rantala evolutionists arise where some consider
et  al., 2018; Rottenberg, 2014). As in most the extremes or persistence of low mood as
other defence activation disorders, depres- maladaptive and/or dysfunctional, while oth-
sion has a higher prevalence in females with ers consider the whole range of low mood
an overall F:M ratio of around 2:1. The higher including the extremes of depression as
female risk is contributed to by higher levels adaptations. Broadly speaking, one can clas-
of neuroticism, sensitivity to social rejection, sify evolutionary theories of depression into
and interpersonal stressors (Del Giudice, social and non-social theories (Gilbert, 2006)
2018). Interestingly, and unlike most other (Box 2.2).
mental disorders such as schizophrenia, Depression primarily occurs in social or
autism, and anorexia nervosa, patients with interpersonal contexts and is less frequently
depression show rates of reproductive suc- associated with events in non-social domains
cess very close to that of the general popula- (Brune, 2015). Evolutionary formulations
tion, with males at 90% and females at 100% suggest explanations for the observed female

BOX 2.2  Evolutionary theories of depression 

Social Evolutionary Theories


1 Theories based on attachment theory (Bowlby, 1980).
2 Theories on social competition and social rank (Price et al., 1994).
3 Social navigation hypothesis (Watson and Andrews, 2002).
4 Social risk theory (Allen and Badcock, 2003).
5 Depression as bargaining (Hagen, 2003).
6 Analytical rumination hypothesis (Andrews and Thomson, 2009).

Non-social Evolutionary Theories


1 Theories of resource conservation (Nesse, 2019).
2 Depression as immune response, defence against pathogens, starvation (see Rantala et al., 2018 for
a review).
34 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

preponderance in depression as being related An influential non-social evolutionary theory


to ‘female fitness’, which appears much more proposes that low mood is adaptive for dis-
dependent on securing support from others engaging from unattainable goals. However,
compared to males (Troisi, 2001). depressive disorder arises when the goals are
The social competition and rank theories too important to be abandoned and the indi-
propose that depression is part of a strategy vidual becomes trapped in an unwinnable
of subordination associated with decline in situation (Nesse, 2019).
social standing or rank and where further More recently, Rantala et  al. (2018) pro-
contest is judged to be futile or even risky. posed a subtyping of MDD, based on an
The low mood serves the dual function of evolutionary framework, with 12 distinct
signalling helplessness and submission both conditions each with its own proximate and
to dominants and to potential helpers. It also ultimate causal profile. According to this
stops the individual from resuming competi- model, MDD cannot be explained by a single
tion too quickly (Price et al., 1994). However, theory and is consistent with the widespread
if social competition lies at the root of depres- view that depression is a heterogeneous dis-
sion, males would be expected to be at higher order. These include infection, long-term
risk of depression given the higher fitness stress, hierarchy conflict, grief, loneliness,
costs incurred by males as a result of status traumatic experiences, post-partum events,
setbacks (Brune, 2015). romantic rejection, the season, chemicals,
According to attachment theory, the low somatic disease, and starvation.
mood of depression bears a distinct resem- While many of the theories briefly described
blance to the phase of despair that occurs in above are reasonably parsimonious accounts of
an infant after prolonged separation from its known facts about depression, the fact remains
main carer, which involves reduced activity that few of their predictions have been empiri-
and vocalization as well as disengagement cally tested (Hagen, 2011). Unfortunately, the
from its environment (Bowlby, 1980). This same can be said about many of the evolution-
suggests that depression is an evolved strat- ary theories regarding other mental disorders.
egy that is activated by the disruption of sig- The current dearth of data in the field remains
nificant attachment bonds. an important obstacle to the integration of evo-
The social risk theory focuses on the risk lutionary thinking into mainstream psychiatry.
of social exclusion, which would have had Nevertheless, the evolutionary perspective
grave consequences in the ancestral environ- is crucial for the formulation of appropriate
ment (Allen and Badcock, 2003). The uncon- questions and examination of existing data as
scious and subtle calculation of the quotient well as for the collection of new information
of one’s social value to social burden will sig- on mental disorders.
nal the risk of exclusion if this drops to a crit-
ical level. This will trigger a depressive state
designed to conserve energy and help build Schizophrenia Spectrum
up future potential social value to others; it Disorders (SSDs)
also predicts increased suicide risk once the
quotient drops below one (Brune, 2015). According to DSM-5, SSDs include schizo-
The analytical rumination hypothesis phrenia (requires a minimum of six months of
proposes that depressive rumination is an symptoms), schizophreniform disorder (up to
adaptation designed to solve complex social six months), brief psychotic episode (up to one
dilemmas (Andrews and Thomson, 2009). month), schizoaffective disorder, drug-induced
This is supported by the finding that low psychosis, and catatonia. DSM-5 requires two
mood facilitates complex decision-making or more of the following for a diagnosis
(von Helversen et al., 2011). of schizophrenia: delusions, hallucinations,
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND PSYCHIATRY 35

disorganized thinking, disorganized behav- hemispheric asymmetry in schizophrenic patients


iour, and negative symptoms. In addition, a and increased levels of ambidexterity in children
number of specifiers should be applied for the who later develop psychosis, these findings can
diagnosis to be made (American Psychiatric be explained equally well through mutation
load and developmental stress (Yeo et al., 1999).
Association, 2013).
Moreover, genome-wide studies demonstrate
Although it was once believed that schizo-
that SSDs are not the result of the action of single
phrenia occurs uniformly across the world, or a small number of genes but the cumulative
affecting 1% of the population, it is now effect of thousands of common and rare variants
recognized that this view is erroneous and (Plomin, 2018).
that schizophrenia varies significantly in its 2 The lipid metabolism hypothesis: this hypothesis
prevalence (McGrath, 2006). Some studies proposes that changes in lipid metabolism within
have found a 30-fold difference in prevalence the human lineage enabled the development
(0.1–3%) (Kinney et al., 2009). The average of creativity, which explains the flourishing of
incidence is suggested to be between 0.2– culture, including art and religion, over the last
50,000 years. According to this hypothesis, SSDs
0.6 per 1,000 (Brune, 2015). The sex ratio
are the by-product of these newly evolved meta-
shows a male preponderance of around 1.4:1 bolic pathways (Horrobin, 2001). Horrobin’s ideas
(McGrath, 2006). on the role of lipid metabolism in the aetiology
Schizophrenia is highly heritable, with of schizophrenia resulted in an interest in testing
monozygotic (MZ) twins having a 48% the effects of administering Omega-3 fatty acids
concordance compared to 17% for dizy- in high concentrations to patients, but the results
gotic (DZ) twins, and the relative risk shows of randomized controlled trials have been incon-
a progressive reduction with increasing sistent (National Institute for Health and Care
genetic distance (Owen et  al., 2007). The Excellence (NICE), 2013).
3 The social brain theory of schizophrenia: Burns’
persistence of schizophrenia within human
cortical dysconnectivity hypothesis is arguably
populations, a condition that strikes at the
the best developed example of the ‘social brain’
peak of reproductive years and has a dev- theory and also the most plausible example of
astating effect on reproductive success, is evolutionary ‘by-product’ formulations gener-
a puzzle that has exercised evolutionists ally (Burns, 2007). Burns’ hypothesis states
and has resulted in a diversity of evolution- that the emergence of the social brain, with
ary hypotheses (Brune, 2015). Power et  al. its complex and vulnerable circuits, produced a
(2013) found that males with schizophrenia vulnerability to aberrant connectivity. According
had fertility rates 23% and females 47% that to this model, schizophrenia is a disorder of the
of the general population. Patients’ brothers fronto-temporal and fronto-parietal circuits that
evolved in our species as a substrate for the
also showed highly reduced fertility whereas
social brain. Schizophrenia, as a disorder of the
sisters showed a slightly increased fertility. social brain, is consistent with a range of find-
Hence, schizophrenia is associated with the ings that show deficits in social cognition prior
lowest rates of fertility compared to all other to first psychotic episode, including deficits in
common mental disorders. We list below a recognition of facial emotions, mentalizing, and
number of evolutionary formulations for interpersonal processes such as understanding
SSDs. of fairness, reciprocity, and trust, as well as
findings following the onset of the psychosis
(a) Evolutionary by-product models: (Brune, 2015).
1 The laterality and language model of schizo- 4 The ‘cliff edge’ fitness functions model: this
phrenia: schizophrenia arising from disrupted is based on the idea that certain adaptive
lateralization of the brain with the failure of traits can overshoot their optimum, resulting
the hemispheric dominance for language is one in catastrophic failure and severe maladap-
of the best-known by-product models (Crow, tive consequences. Nesse (2019) has suggested
1997). Although this is supported by reduced that schizophrenia is intimately related to the
36 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

development of language ability and theory of characteristics that include creativity, emotional
mind where the fitness peak is dangerously close expressiveness, and superior mentalizing ability
to the catastrophic cliff edge. This model is con- (Nettle, 2001; Shaner et  al., 2004). Hence the
sistent with a range of evolutionary formulations sexual selection model proposes that SSDs are
including the social brain, language, and lateral- the maladaptive outcome of adaptive but risky
ity, as well as the sexual selection hypothesis of mating strategies (Del Giudice, 2018). This model
schizotypal traits. is also compatible with the view of SSDs as a fast
(b) Schizophrenia as an adaptation: an early model life history spectrum disorder (see para. f below).
that has since been falsified was based on a bal- The model is consistent with a range of findings
anced polymorphism of a single gene that was including the slight increase in fertility in sisters,
beneficial in the heterozygote state but causes but it is rather difficult to reconcile with the find-
schizophrenia in homozygotes (Huxley et al., 1964). ing of a dramatic reduction in fertility in brothers
More recently, a range of models based on group (Power et al., 2013).
selection have been proposed that suggest that (e) The diametrical model of psychosis (including
schizotypal traits facilitated group splitting during schizophrenia) and autism: in this model autistic-
human evolutionary history through magical and spectrum disorders (ASD) and psychotic-spectrum
paranoid thinking as well as idiosyncratic behav- conditions (including schizophrenia (SSD)) repre-
iour which can lead to messianic leadership and sent two major suites of disorders of human cog-
group fission (Stevens and Price, 2000a, 2000b). nition, affect, and behaviour that involve altered
Other formulations focused on the shaman as the development and function of the social brain
self-sacrificing equivalent of the sterile castes in (Crespi and Badcock, 2008). The model is based on
social insects that produces group cohesion and evidence that large sets of phenotypic traits exhibit
solidarity through magical thinking, possession diametrically opposite phenotypes in ASD versus
states, and religious ritual which is maintained psychotic-spectrum conditions, with a focus on
through group selection (Polimeni, 2012). However, schizophrenia. These include constrained growth in
while these theories draw attention to the fascinat- psychotic-spectrum disorders as opposed to over-
ing similarities between religious phenomenology growth in ASD and underdeveloped social cogni-
and psychosis, they remain highly speculative. tion in ASD as opposed to its hyper-development
(c) Mismatch model: the outgroup intolerance in the psychotic spectrum (the reverse is the case
hypothesis is an attempt to provide an explana- for mechanistic cognition resulting in the psychosis
tory framework for a range of epidemiological spectrum being hypermentalistic/hypomechanis-
findings pointing to wide variation in the inci- tic and the reverse is the case in ASD). The role
dence and prevalence of SSDs. The hypothesis of genomic imprinting in this phenomenon has
proposes that schizophrenia arises as the result of already been alluded to in the section ‘Genomic
a mismatch between the social brain as shaped Imprinting and Mental Disorder’, above. The dif-
by evolution and the novel social conditions of ferent cognitive biases of SSD and ASD proposed
the post-Neolithic that involve living in large in the diametric model have received considerable
settlements and regularly encountering strangers empirical support (Abu-Akel et  al., 2015; White
(outgroup members). The hypothesis can provide et  al., 2016). However, the overlap between ASD
an explanation for (i) the higher risk in migrants and SSD (co-morbidity) remains a challenge for
and especially second-generation migrants and this model (Chisholm et al., 2015).
migrants who are racially and/or ethnically sali- (f) Life history theory and SSDs: broadly speaking,
ent; (ii) increased risk of schizophrenia that is positive schizotypy, characterized by odd beliefs,
inversely related to same-group ethnic density in magical thinking, unusual perceptual experiences,
a given locality; (iii) the increased risk to individu- and paranoid thoughts, associated with hyper-
als who have grown up in cities; and (iv) the puta- mentalizing, enhanced creativity, and unrestricted
tive low risk of schizophrenia in hunter-gatherer socio-sexuality, fits the pattern of fast life history
societies (Abed and Abbas, 2011, 2014). strategy. This is also consistent with the asso-
(d) Sexual selection model of creativity of schi- ciation of positive schizotypy with aggression,
zotypal traits: these hypotheses are based on impulsivity, and sensation-seeking as well as
the proposal that schizophrenia is the extreme, early maturation (Del Giudice, 2018). Negative
low-fitness end of a range of sexually selected schizotypy, on the other hand, characterized by
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND PSYCHIATRY 37

lack of social engagement, flat affect, and social affect (e.g. stimulants). These drugs override
anxiety with paranoid tendencies that tends to the adaptive functions of the primary emo-
overlap with autistic traits, is associated with late tional systems so individuals experience an
maturation in males but not in females, and is increase in positive affect, or decrease in
consistent with a slow life history strategy (Kaiser negative affect, independently of any change
and Gruzelier, 1999). It is clear that the diametric
in their circumstances, thus decoupling the
model of psychosis and ASD as well as the sexual
selection hypothesis both fit the fast life history
emotional system from environmental events,
strategy model. some continuing to consume the drug despite
mounting harm because the reactions bypass
the evolved protective mechanisms used to
Drug and Alcohol Addictions signal real success or danger (Nesse, 2019).

Examining substance abuse from an evolution- Mismatch


ary perspective offers explanatory advantages The hijack hypothesis implies that a range of
in illuminating a wide range of biological, drugs of abuse effectively commandeer the
psychological, and social facts and mecha- neural reward circuitry in the mesolimbic
nisms in substance misuse (St John-Smith reward pathway as a result of mismatch as the
et al., 2013). Evolutionary models are unique contemporary abundance of potent psychoac-
in that they emphasize the effects that drugs tive substances is a recent and novel phenome-
had on fitness over human evolution. For sub- non that was not present and therefore could not
stance abuse, a seemingly maladaptive trait, to have occurred in the ancestral environment.
persist, there must be either a ‘trade-off’ where
the harm is counterbalanced by a fitness ben- Human–plant co-evolutionary
efit, or substance-taking is a by-product of history and the paradox of drug
other more adaptive processes. Such models reward
include: a) psychotropic self-medication Plants evolved the capacity to synthesize
(pharmacological manipulation of emotions); chemicals (nicotine, morphine, cocaine etc.)
b) pharmacophagy and infection control; that act as neurotoxins to deter consumption by
c) mismatch theory; d) increasing reproductive insects and herbivores (Sullivan et  al., 2008).
fitness; e) evolutionary constraints; f) trade- The efficacy of plant neurotoxins evolved over
offs; g) costly signalling and handicap theo- 400 million years and is therefore not evolu-
ries; h) placebo, ritual, and healing effects; and tionarily novel. Consequently, human physiol-
i) drug use in spirituality or religion (e.g. the ogy can ‘identify’ plant toxins and activate
role of psychedelic drug use by ‘neo-shamans’ defences that involve genes, tissue barriers,
and ‘psychonauts’). Some of these models are neural circuits, organ systems, and behaviours
conceptually similar or overlapping, are not to protect against them. Drug toxicity and aver-
mutually exclusive, and may interact in unpre- sive responses (e.g. headache, sweating,
dictable ways (Orsolini et al., 2017). nausea, and vomiting) occur in humans so are
inconsistent with a simplistic theory of drug
Emotional pathways reward. Consequently other mechanisms, such
Primary emotional systems evolved to pro- as trade-offs, must be invoked as explanations.
duce pleasurable affects in response to propi- The neurotoxin regulation hypothesis pro-
tious circumstances or stimuli indicating poses that the parallel consumption of both the
adaptive success, and aversive affects in nutrients and neurotoxins in plants selected for
response to environmental or other threats, a system capable of maximizing the benefits of
indicating reduced adaptive success. Drugs plant energy extraction while mitigating the cost
(of abuse) may be used to diminish aversive of plant toxicity. The pharmacophagy hypothe-
affects (e.g. opiates) or to increase positive sis proposes that the consumption of chemicals
38 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

with medicinal properties is contingent on Drug use can increase reproductive fitness
human–plant co-evolution. Self-medication because consumption may: (1) advertise bio-
advantages arose when humans learned to over- logical quality, sexual maturity, or availability;
come cues of plant toxicity (e.g. bitter taste) (2) decrease inhibitions in mating contexts;
and consumed potentially toxic substances with and/or (3) enhance associative learning behav-
little energetic content because ingesting the iours that in turn increase mating opportunities
toxins in small amounts was advantageous. (Richardson et al., 2017).
Thus, the consumption of plant alkaloids could Variation in drug use susceptibility is in part
have contributed to reproductive fitness, and due to genetic factors; therefore, successful
a taste for these substances could have been drug consumption may be a costly and honest
selected for. It is recognized that many such signal of biological quality: a process of costly
toxins are known to have anti-helminthic or signalling and sexual selection. Such risk-
antimicrobial and antiparasitic effects. taking behaviour represents a fast life history
strategy and involves future discounting (see
Alcohol ‘Evolutionary Models of Mental Disorders’
Consuming ripe fruits containing small section above). LHT can explain the current
amounts of ethanol is selectively advantageous male preponderance in drug use, as female
(Dudley, 2004), as volatile alcohols potentially drug users incur much higher fitness costs
aid in olfactory localization of ripe fruit. through reduced parenting capacity, potential
Herbivores developed the capacity to metabo- teratogenic effects, and potential circumven-
lize alcohol to be able to utilize energy-rich tion of mate choice (Orsolini et al., 2017).
fruits despite the presence of alcohol. In the Finally, another aspect of mismatch is
ancestral environment, alcohol would have that the ancient ‘evolved’ advantages of any
been encountered in fermenting fruit in low psychoactive substances have now poten-
concentrations and small quantities for brief tially become a liability and risk in modern
periods in the year. Subsequent to the agricul- environments as cultural change is accelerat-
tural revolution, large surpluses of fruits and ing and outstrips biological adaptation. The
grains became available for fermentation so evolutionary perspective can help researchers
alcoholic drinks were brewed up to 12–14% reach a functional understanding of substance
and stored/traded for year-round consumption. abuse and develop treatments for the various
Much more recently, the development of dis- complex underlying causes of substance mis-
tilling technology permitted the production of use. Some of these models are conceptually
far higher concentrations of alcohol. With the similar or overlapping and can interact in
rise of larger settlements and cities, having unpredictable ways. In addition, psychoac-
access to alcoholic beverages may have pro- tive substances, often hallucinogens which
tected against waterborne pathogens. However, tend not to be addictive, have been used in
enzyme systems that evolved to process small various religious and cultural ceremonies
amounts of alcohol on an occasional basis can (signalling) for millennia. Some advantages
now be presented with inexhaustible supplies may be had from related group cohesion as
of highly concentrated alcohol, giving rise to a well as their action on micro-organisms and
state of mismatch (St John-Smith et al., 2013). other trade-offs discussed above.

Cultural, psychological,
anthropological models and sexual Anorexia Nervosa (AN) and
selection hypotheses Bulimia Nervosa (BN)
Some evolutionary psychological theories con-
cerning drug use suggest individuals consume AN and BN are diagnostic categories of eating
drugs to increase reproductive opportunities. disorders according to ICD-10 and DSM-5
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND PSYCHIATRY 39

classifications. The conditions share core fea- of starvation, and hyperactivity could r­ epresent
tures of morbid fear of fatness, distorted body an adaptive behaviour that helped ancestral
image, and a pattern of behaviour aimed at nomadic foragers to migrate from depleted
weight reduction that includes purging, restric- environments to more promising surroundings
tion of food intake, or excessive exercise in times of food shortages (Guisinger, 2003).
(American Psychiatric Association, 2013; However, the ‘fleeing famine hypothesis’
WHO, 1992). AN is characterized by low body appears to confound consequences with cau-
weight with possible amenorrhea whereas BN sation in that the features of ‘fleeing famine’
is associated with binge eating and a normal represent the consequences of starvation that
body weight. Evidence demonstrates some arise in AN as a result of self-imposed restric-
heritability (Bulik et  al., 2016; Yilmaz et  al., tion of food intake. It is of interest that the
2015) and AN and BN share some genetic basis trigger for the initiation of dieting proposed
(Eley et al., 2005). Notably, the epidemiology by Guisinger (2003) is the improvement of
of AN and BN demonstrates a marked female attractiveness and competition for mates,
preponderance with a female-to-male sex ratio which is more or less identical to the Sexual
of 10:1 or greater (Gordon, 1990; Hudson et al., Competition Hypothesis (see below).
2007). Also, both are by far more prevalent in It is notable that these theories focus exclu-
developed countries compared to developing sively on AN where food restriction causes
countries, particularly when considering sub- low body weight, which in turn can lead to
threshold phenotypes (Katzman et al., 2004). amenorrhoea and reproductive suppression
or the starvation response, whereas this does
Evolutionary theories for eating not occur in BN.
disorders
A number of evolutionarily informed theo- The Sexual Competition
ries and hypotheses have been proposed. The Hypothesis (SCH) and LHT
‘Reproductive Suppression Hypothesis’ of The SCH is a more inclusive evolutionary
AN considers eating restriction as a strategy model which reconsiders the whole spectrum
to delay reproduction in times of disadvanta- of eating disorders including AN and BN
geous environmental conditions by lowering (Abed, 1998). The SCH, based on the
the amount of body fat to a level incompati- Darwinian theory of sexual selection, proposes
ble with ovulation (Surbey, 1987; Voland and that female intra-sexual competition is the bio-
Voland, 1989; Wasser and Barash, 1983). logical root for the drive for thinness, an adap-
Consistent with the Reproductive Suppression tive response originally suited to the ancestral
Hypothesis it is reported that women who environment, and that the extreme version of
perceive low levels of support from romantic this manifests in what we know as eating disor-
partners and family are prone to dieting and ders. The SCH proposes that AN and BN are
do not feel ready for parenthood, suggesting manifestations of abnormally intense female
that poor environmental conditions are causal intra-sexual competition whereby autonomous
in the development of AN (Juda et al., 2004). females of reproductive age compete with each
Unlike the original Reproductive Suppression other in the novel modern Westernized urban
Hypothesis which hypothesized the occurrence environment through a strategy of ‘the pursuit
of reproductive self-suppression, an alterna- of thinness’ as a signal of youth, leading to
tive hypothesis was put forth by Mealey (2000) ‘runaway female intra-sexual competition’, the
where reproductive suppression was imposed extreme version of which manifests as eating
upon subordinate females by dominants. disorders (Abed, 1998).
Other evolutionary hypotheses have pos- The SCH is based on the fact that throughout
ited that symptoms of AN may help to cope human evolutionary history the female shape
with famine, whereby food restriction, denial has been a reliable indicator of the female’s
40 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

reproductive history and ­ consequently her have found a significant correlation between
reproductive potential (Bovet and Raymond, abnormal eating behaviour and the intensity
2015; Singh, 1993). Youth and good health of competition for mates (Abed et al., 2012;
have always been major determinants of female Faer et al., 2005). Also, supportive evidence
mate value not least because of the finite repro- has been found for the predictions that homo-
ductive window in humans that abruptly ends sexual men resemble heterosexual women
with menopause (Buss, 1987). The visual sig- and lesbians resemble heterosexual men in
nal for a female’s peak reproductive potential their concerns about physical attractiveness
in the ancestral environment was the female and eating behaviour (Li et al., 2010). More
nubile shape, which was generally short-lived recently an exploratory study on anorexic and
and deteriorated with the repeated cycles of bulimic patients supported the fast–slow life
gestation and lactation (Symons, 1995). history strategy prediction for BN and AN
Hence, according to SCH, female intra- and partially supported the predictions of
sexual competition in affluent Westernized SCH (Nettersheim et al., 2018).
societies became focused on the preserva-
tion of the ‘nubile shape’ through a strategy
of the pursuit of thinness to display signs of The Placebo Response and Nesse’s
youth. The SCH further proposes that other Smoke Detector Principle
important factors serve to up-regulate the
intensity of female intra-sexual competition. Placebo effects may be considered as expla-
Some of the major additional factors include nations of how healing and caring works
(Nettersheim et  al., 2018): (a) female auton- (McQueen et  al., 2013). The universality of
omy that involves the ability to make mating placebo responses suggests a likely evolution-
decisions with relatively little interference ary basis to the underlying mechanisms.
from kin (unlike the case in ancestral and tra- Placebo responses permit mammals to modify
ditional societies) (Apostolou, 2007; (b) living internal processes and behaviours. Adaptive
in cities where abnormally large numbers of advantages might result from the evolution of
autonomous females live in close proximity abilities to modify our internal environment
to each other; (c) reduced fertility (birth rates) in the light of positive evaluations of our
(Vining, 1986); and (d) the ubiquity of abnor- external environments, social interactions,
mally attractive youthful nubile female images and appraisals of the future. The hypothetical
in the media that are mistaken for competitors system charged with health maintenance,
(Ferguson et al., 2011). shaped by evolution, has been referred to as a
Therefore, the SCH is based on a proposed ‘health governor’, aspects of which are shared
mismatch between the design of the female’s across many species but which is most highly
psychological adaptations for mate attrac- developed in humans and operates entirely
tion and retention and for competing with outside conscious awareness (Humphrey and
rival females, on the one hand, and the novel Skoyles, 2012). Nesse (2019) stresses that
circumstances of the modern urban environ- placebo responses primarily entail modifica-
ment, on the other. tion of the body’s defences e.g. pain, nausea,
However, intra-sexual competition alone anxiety, depression, fever, coughing, vomit-
cannot explain the different presentations of ing, and diarrhoea, rather than altering disease
AN and BN. Hence, life history strategies processes. Hence, evolution has selected for
were considered as an added factor where BN mechanisms that defend against injury, infec-
lies at the fast and AN on the slow end of the tion or poisoning and the regulation of these
life history spectrum (Abed et al., 2012). defences is influenced by appraisals of the
Predictions from the SCH have been exam- environment. However, many defences appear
ined in a number of non-clinical studies and to be over-expressed. A signal-detection
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND PSYCHIATRY 41

analysis can explain this apparent paradox. pleiotropy (Williams, 1957) and LHT. As AD
When the cost of expressing an all-or-nothing seems to be specific to Homo sapiens, its
defence is low compared with the potential existence may in part be anchored in the
harm it protects against, the optimal system adaptive changes that have occurred after
will express many false alarms. For example, humans separated from other primates.
vomiting may cost only a few hundred calo- Evolutionary theories also take into account
ries and a few minutes, whereas not vomiting issues around brain development including
may result in a chance, however small, of the related phenomena of altriciality and
death from poisoning. This has been dubbed grandmothering, the evolution of ApoE and
‘the smoke detector principle’ (Nesse, 2001). the genome lag hypothesis. Thus, an evolu-
The over-expression of many defences allows tionary look into AD may shed new light on
that they can often be dampened without the causes and treatments of this devastating
compromising fitness. The regulation of disease (Von Gunten et al., 2018).
defences allows that otherwise ‘protective’ Others have suggested that AD is the result
defences can be turned off both in situations of mismatch related to the vastly increased
of extreme danger, to facilitate escape, and in levels in the modern environment of insulin
situations propitious for recovery, where they resistance, inflammation, and exposure to
may no longer be necessary for protection. toxins (Fox, 2018), or that AD is the result of
This may explain why pain is reduced both a trade-off between the antimicrobial effects
when facing immediate threat and when of amyloid beta and the damaging effects of
being cared for. its sustained activation (Moir et al., 2018).
Furthermore, the goal of the attachment sys- Personality disorders (PD) are defined by
tem is to maintain proximity to caregivers who DSM-5 as an enduring pattern of inner expe-
would provide safety from danger. Thus, at rience and behaviour that deviates markedly
times of threat, the attachment system becomes from the expectations of the individual’s cul-
activated. Manifestations of attachment behav- ture. The DSM and ICD classifications list
iour change with the stage of the life cycle and around a dozen different types each but they
attachment style, but at times of subjectively differ in their subtyping, terminology, and cri-
perceived threat, which includes illness, prox- teria. The five-dimension model of personal-
imity and caring are sought from attachment ity is currently widely favoured and comprises
figures, which may come to include trusted extraversion, neuroticism, agreeableness,
professional carers, and hence the placebo conscientiousness, and openness (McCrae
response may be an emergent property of the and Costa, 2003). Personality disorders are
attachment system (Bowlby, 1980). clustered into three groups with Cluster A
comprising the ‘eccentric’ PDs such as para-
noid and schizoid; cluster B ‘dramatic’ such
Other Disorders: Alzheimer’s, as antisocial and borderline PDs; and cluster
Personality Disorders, and Bipolar C ‘anxious’, including avoidant, dependent,
and obsessive–compulsive PDs.
Disorder
Evolutionary formulations have proposed
People are increasingly surviving into old that antisocial PDs may be an ‘adaptive’
age. This increase in longevity is associated cheating strategy that is maintained through
with increased levels of morbidity of both frequency dependent selection (e.g. Mealey,
somatic and mental disorders, among them 1995). Cluster B (antisocial and borderline
the dementias such as Alzheimer’s disease PDs) has been considered to represent a fast
(AD), during those added years. Evolutionists life history strategy while both clusters A and
consider explanatory theories for the phe- C are considered slow life history disorders
nomenon of aging such as antagonistic (Brune, 2015). However, Del Giudice (2018)
42 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

takes a more nuanced approach and classifies as well as other groups such as amino acids,
PDs as fast (antisocial and borderline) and purines, cannabinoids, and neuropeptides.
slow (obsessive–compulsive), with avoidant Such chemicals have been found not only
PD as a defence activation disorder. in animals, but also in plants and unicellu-
Bipolar disorder (BPD) has a prevalence of lar microorganisms (Roshchina, 2010). This
between 1 and 5% of the population depend- ubiquity is best explained by universal cel-
ing on the subtypes included. In contrast to lular mechanisms, communication systems
schizophrenia, BPD has received relatively across kingdoms, and shared evolutionary
little attention from evolutionists. Many of the ancestry, demonstrating the ‘thriftiness’ of
existing evolutionary models propose some evolutionary processes and the conserva-
evolutionary advantage of hypomanic traits tion of evolved mechanisms and strategies.
or even manic episodes (Del Giudice, 2018). Phylogenetically, it appears that these chemi-
The manic mood has been considered the win- cals and their associated enzymes existed for
ning and the depressive mood the losing pro- a substantial period before their respective
grammes of the dominance system (Gilbert receptor proteins.
et al., 2007). It is of interest to note the rela- Evolution of sophisticated nervous systems
tively small decline in fertility associated with arrived independently of the synthesis of newer
BPD (85% of general population levels in sophisticated transmitter substances, receptor
females and 75% in males) compared to the proteins, transducers, and effector proteins;
steep decline in schizophrenia (Power et  al., rather they evolved with improved organiza-
2013). Nesse (2019) considers BPD as an tion and utilization of these entities, forming
example of a malfunctioning mood regulation increasingly advanced and refined circuitry
system or broken ‘moodostat’. Del Giudice via natural and sexual selection (Roshchina,
(2018), applying the life history framework, 2010). There are hundreds of chemical sub-
proposes that there are two distinct variants, a stances that provide communication between
fast and a slow life history strategy variant. The cells in humans, some simple monoamines,
fast subtype has greater links to schizophrenia others more complex, e.g. neuropeptides.
with a higher risk of psychotic symptoms and Knowledge of differing receptor function
the slower subtype has links to autism and in other species has aided drug development.
lower risk of psychotic symptoms. For example, there are important changes dur-
ing evolutionary time, related to the neuro-
transmitters/receptors and how they function
in humans. As further examples of biological
EVOLUTION AND cross-reactivity, many psychotropic agents
PSYCHOPHARMACOLOGY have an action on microorganisms, includ-
ing such varied taxa as bacteria, helminths,
Psychopharmacological drugs became insects, and other parasites. The antipsychot-
widely available in the 1950s and have ics (phenothiazines and thioxanthenes) show
changed many outcomes; however, psychiat- antibacterial activity, exerting their activity
ric disorders are so complex and heterogene- independently of antibiotic resistance. The ben-
ous that psychopharmacology alone cannot zodiazepine clonazepam is anti-schistosomal
cure every aspect of any disorder. Current (Stohler, 1978). Monoamine oxidase inhibi-
psychopharmacology is not based on evolu- tors, lithium, tricyclic antidepressants, and
tionary insights or theories. valproic acid have a range of antimicrobial
Highly preserved, bio-active chemicals activities (Kristiansen, 1990).
play fundamental roles in many processes Many psychiatric conditions involve emo-
across virtually all life forms. They include tion dysregulation, inappropriate expression
acetylcholine and the biogenic monoamines of emotions, or impaired access to one’s
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND PSYCHIATRY 43

emotional life. Positive emotions developed side effects of medication through the lens of
evolutionarily to motivate humans to take evolutionary theory, potentially considering
advantage of environmental opportunities drug interference with evolutionarily relevant
and to recognize when we have succeeded in systems that might have negative conse-
doing so. Negative emotions evolved to moti- quences for the individual’s ability to attain
vate humans to avoid misfortune by escaping, vital biosocial goals.
attacking, or preventing harm or repair-
ing damage when it has already occurred.
Emotional reactions importantly correspond
to differences in appraisal that result from indi- LOOKING TOWARD THE FUTURE
vidual differences in personal values, experi-
ences, and goals. Psychopharmacological At present, the evolutionary literature remains
agents may modify these responses in ways largely invisible to mainstream psychiatrists.
which have consequences beyond the sim- This is partly explained by the current paucity
ple alleviation of distress. ‘Side effects’ of of evolutionarily inspired interventions but is
medications are sometimes consequences also influenced by a range of other factors.
of effects on attendant processes, as distinct These include ideological, religious, and lib-
from the direct pharmacology, for instance a ertarian concerns as well as factors related to
reduction in anxiety leading to an increase in the inertia inherent in paradigm shifts (Kuhn,
risk taking or disinhibition. 1962). Whereas the religious and ideological
Understanding why symptoms exist/per- (primarily post-modernist, anti-science
sist may enhance psychiatric management. trends) opposition to Darwinism is largely
Treatments should be evaluated regarding entrenched and probably unchangeable, the
whether the index symptoms are aiding indi- libertarian concerns arise from misconcep-
vidual coping strategies with respect to the tions that should, in principle, be amenable to
adverse life event which caused the lowered modification. For example, mistaking evolu-
mood in the first place. Importantly, pharma- tionary science for social Darwinism and
cologically reducing symptoms remains ben- assuming that evolution implies strict genetic
eficial, even essential, when the symptoms determinism can be countered by appropriate
are excessive or fail to serve their adaptive scientific argument and evidence. However, it
purpose, and when the symptoms are not may prove much more difficult to overcome
associated with events that triggered the epi- the anti-evolutionary position of ‘biological
sode. Conversely, in cases where a depressive reductionism’ that is currently the dominant
episode is a functional response to adver- trend in medical and psychiatric academic
sity, suppressing it unconditionally without centres within the Western world.
addressing the underlying causes might be We propose that evolutionary science pro-
harmful. This is analogous to treating pain vides a framework that can organize a huge
without considering the aetiology. number of facts about human biology and
Conceptualizing sickness behaviours, pain psychology into a coherent narrative that, in
mechanisms, and mental disorders in rela- time, will lead to insights that can give rise to
tion to the problems that they evolved to novel treatments and interventions in psychi-
solve potentially encourages practitioners atry and the rest of medicine. This can help
to provide treatment options that are more further our understanding of sex differences
effectively targeted, ensuring a patient’s in vulnerability to disorder, phenotypic plas-
long-term well-being, though the patient’s ticity including differential susceptibility as a
immediate best interests must always be result of gene–environment interactions, and
regarded as paramount (Rantala et al., 2018). the role of life history strategies. The unique
Psychopharmacology should also review the insights evolutionary thinking brings stem
44 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

primarily from combining an understanding systematically can help minimize confusion


of the role of ultimate causation alongside and errors. One of the major contributions of
proximate causes. Such evolutionary think- evolutionary thinking is that it helps research-
ing has already resulted in novel interven- ers formulate the right questions regarding the
tions for cancer (DeGregori, 2018). nature of disease and disorder. Evolution also
However, a critical mass of evolutionarily cautions us against simplistic genetic models
informed psychiatrists is necessary to signifi- and draws attention to the possible adaptive
cantly influence the research agenda. Hence, function(s) of genes implicated in mental
the first step must involve better evolutionary disorders.
education for psychiatrists both at under- and Evolution’s flagship contribution is that it
postgraduate levels. highlights the mistake of equating distress with
We suggest that trainee psychiatrists would disease and disorder. This prompts clinicians
benefit from the following basic evolutionary to consider the possible downside of treating
knowledge/competences: potentially adaptive states of defence activa-
tion in individual patients as well as to consider
1 An understanding of how selection shapes adap- the currently neglected possibility that insuffi-
tations (physical and psychological traits). cient defences (e.g. low or absent anxiety) are
2 An understanding of Tinbergen’s four causes also a possible source of psychopathology and
with special emphasis on the distinction between harmful dysfunction (Nesse, 2019).
proximate and ultimate causation (see ‘Evolution
Aside from future advantages in the areas
and Causality’ section).
of research and classification, there are potential
3 An understanding of the concepts of kin selection
and inclusive fitness. benefits from utilizing evolutionary thinking
4 An understanding of the evolutionary causal pro- in the clinic in the present. Examples include
cesses for the persistence of disease and disorder introducing patients with anxiety and panic
with special emphasis on mismatch, trade-offs, disorders to evolutionary concepts such as the
life history strategies and sexual selection (see ‘smoke detector principle’ (Nesse, 2019) or the
‘Causal Pathways for the Persistence of Disease harm-avoidance model of OCD (Abed and de
and Disorder’ section). Pauw, 1999). Finally, we submit that possessing
5 An understanding of the basics of evolution- an evolutionary understanding of unique human
ary genetics, including selection, mutation, drift, vulnerabilities in itself enhances empathy and
intra-genomic conflict, and genomic imprinting.
understanding, complementing the clinician’s
effectiveness (Nesse, 2019; Troisi, 2012).
Many evolutionary applications in medicine
rely on well-established methods, such as
population genetics, phylogenetic analysis,
and observing pathogen evolution. Approaches ACKNOWLEDGEMENT
to evolutionary questions about traits that
leave bodies vulnerable to disease are less We are grateful to David Geaney and the
well developed. Strategies for formulating anonymous referee for reading and com-
questions and hypotheses remain unsettled, menting on previous drafts of this chapter.
and methods for testing evolutionary hypoth-
eses are unfamiliar to many in medicine.
Nesse (2011) has suggested a structure for
appropriate evolutionary research which uses REFERENCES
recent examples to illustrate successful strat-
egies and some common challenges. He Abed, R.T. (1998). The sexual competition
identifies 10 questions to consider in testing hypothesis for eating disorders. British Jour-
evolutionary hypotheses. Addressing them nal of Medical Psychology, 71, 525–547.
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND PSYCHIATRY 45

Abed, R.T. (2000). Psychiatry and Darwinism: American Psychiatric Association (2013).
Time to reconsider? British Journal of Psy- Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
chiatry, 177, 1–3. Disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, American
Abed, R.T. & Abbas, M.J. (2011). A reformulation Psychiatric Association.
of the social brain theory of schizophrenia: The Andreasen, N. (1984). The Broken Brain. New
case for outgroup intolerance. Perspectives in York, HarperPerennial.
Biology and Medicine, 54, 132–151. Andrews, P.W. & Thomson, J.A. (2009). The
Abed, R.T. & Abbas, M.J. (2014). Can the new bright side of being blue: Depression as an
epidemiology of schizophrenia help eluci- adaptation for analyzing complex problems.
date its causation? Irish Journal of Psycho- Psychological Review, 116, 620–654.
logical Medicine, 31, 1–5. Apostolou, M. (2007). Sexual selection under
Abed, R.T. & de Pauw, K.W. (1998). An evolu- parental choice: The role of parents in the
tionary hypothesis for obsessive compulsive evolution of human mating. Evolution and
disorder: A psychological immune system? Human Behavior, 28, 403–409.
Behavioural Neurology, 11(4), 245–250. Barbaro, N., Boutwell, B.B., Barnes, J.C. &
Abed, R.T. & St John-Smith, P. (2016). Evolution- Shackelford, T.K. (2017). Genetic confound-
ary psychiatry: A new college special interest ing of the relationship between father
group. BJPsych Bulletin, 40, 233–236. absence and age at menarche. Evolution and
Abed, R., Mehta, S., Figueredo, A.J., Aldridge, Human Behavior, 38, 357–365.
S., Balson, H., Meyer, C. & Palmer, R. (2012). Belsky, J. (1997). Variation in susceptibility to
Eating disorders and intrasexual competition: rearing influence: An evolutionary argument.
Testing an evolutionary hypothesis among Psychological Inquiry, 8, 182–186.
young women. Scientific World Journal, Belsky, J., Steinberg, L. & Draper, P. (1991).
290813. doi: 10.1100/2012/290813 Childhood experience, interpersonal develop-
Abu-Akel, A.M., Wood S.J., Hansen, P.C. & ment and reproductive strategy: An evolution-
Apperly, I.A. (2015). Perspective-taking abili- ary theory of socialization. Child Development,
ties in the balance between autism tendencies 62, 647–670.
and psychosis proneness. Proceedings of the Blazer, D.G., Kessler, R.C., McGonagle, K.A. &
Royal Society of London B, 282, 20150563. Swartz, M.S. (1994). The prevalence and
Ahn, K., Gotay, N., Andersen, T.M., Anvari, distribution of major depression in a national
A.A., Gochman, P., Lee, Y., Sanders, S., Guha, community sample: The National Comorbid-
S., Darvasi, A., Glessner, J.T., Hakonarson, H., ity Survey. American Journal of Psychiatry,
Lencz, T., State, M.W., Shugart, Y.Y. & Rapo- 151, 979–986.
port, J.L. (2014). High rate of disease-related Bolton, D. (2007). The usefulness of Wake-
copy number variation in childhood onset field’s definition for the diagnostic manuals.
schizophrenia. Molecular Psychiatry, 19, World Psychiatry, 6(3), 164–165.
568–572. Bovet, J. & Raymond, M. (2015). Preferred
Akil, H., Brenner, S., Kandel, E., Kendler, K.S., women’s waist-to-hip ratio variation over the
King, M-C., Scolnick, E., Watson, J. D. & last 2,500 years. PLOS One, 10, e0123284.
Zoghby, H.Y. (2010). The future of psychiat- Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and Loss: Loss
ric research: Genomes and neural circuits. Sadness and Depression (Vol. 3). London,
Science, 327(5973), 1580–1581. Hogarth Press.
Akiskal, H.S. & McKinney, W.T. (1975). Over- Brune, M. (2015). Textbook of Evolutionary
view of recent research in depression: Inte- Psychiatry and Psychosomatic Medicine:
gration of ten conceptual models into a The Origins of Psychopathology. Oxford,
comprehensive clinical frame. Archives of OUP.
General Psychiatry, 32(3), 285–305. Brune, M., Belsky J., Fabrega, H., Feierman,
Allen, N.B. & Badcock, P.B. (2003). The social H.R., Gilbert, P., Glantz, K., Polimeni, J.,
risk hypothesis of depressed mood: Evolu- Price, J.S., Sanjuan, J., Sullivan, R., Troisi, A. &
tionary, psychosocial and neurobiological Wilson, D.R. (2012). The crisis of psychiatry –
perspectives. Psychological Bulletin, 129, insights and prospects from evolutionary
887–913. theory. World Psychiatry, 11(1), 55–57.
46 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Bulik, C.M., Kleiman, S.C. & Yilmaz, Z. (2016). DeGregori, J. (2018). Adaptive Oncogenesis: A
Genetic epidemiology of eating disorders. New Understanding of How Cancer Evolves
Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 29, 383–388. Inside Us. Cambridge, Massachusetts,
Burns, J. (2007). The Descent of Madness: Evo- Harvard University Press.
lutionary Origins of Psychosis and the Social Del Giudice, M. (2018). Evolutionary Psycho-
Brain. London, Routledge. pathology: A Unified Approach. New York,
Buss, D.M. (1987). Sex differences in mate OUP.
selection criteria: An evolutionary perspec- de Villiers, B., Lionetti, F. & Pluess, M. (2018).
tive. In: C. Crawford, M. Smith & Krebs, D. Vantage sensitivity: A framework for
(eds.) Sociobiology and Psychology: Ideas, individual differences in response to
Issues and Applications (pp. 335–351). Hills- psychological intervention. Social Psychiatry
dale, NJ, Erlbaum. and Psychiatric Epidemiology. doi.org/
Caplan, R. (2016). Childhood schizophrenia. In: 10.1007/s00127-017-1471-0
D. Cicchetti (ed.) Developmental Psycho- D’Onofrio, B.M., Rickert, M.E., Franz, E., Kuja-
pathology: Vol. 3 Maladaptation and Halkola, R., Almqvist, C., Sjolander, A.,
Psychopathology (3rd ed., pp. 898–997). Larsson, H. & Lichtenstein, P. (2014). Parental
New York, Wiley. age at childbearing and offspring psychiatric
Chisholm, K., Lin, A., Abu-Akel, A. & Wood, and academic morbidity. JAMA Psychiatry,
S.J. (2015). The association between autism 71, 432–438.
and schizophrenia spectrum disorders: A Dudley, R. (2004). Ethanol, fruit ripening, and
review of eight alternative models of co- the historical origins of human alcoholism in
occurrence. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral primate frugivory. Integrative and Compara-
Reviews, 55, 173–183. tive Biology, 44(4), 315–323.
Chudal, R., Joelsson, P., Gyllenberg, D., Lehti, Durisko, Z., Mulsant, B.H., McKenzie, K. &
V., Leivonen, S., Hinkka-Yli-Salomaki, S., Andrews, P.W. (2016). Using evolutionary
Gissler, M. & Sourander, A. (2015). Parental theory to guide mental health research.
age and the risk of attention-deficit/hyperac- Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 61(3),
tivity disorder: A nationwide, population- 159–165.
based cohort study. Journal of the American Ek, M., Wicks, S., Svensson, A.C., Idring, S. &
Academy of Adolescent Psychiatry, 54(6), Dalman, C. (2014). Advancing paternal age
487–494. and schizophrenia: The impact of delayed
Crespi, B.J. (2016). The evolutionary etiologies fatherhood. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 41(3),
of autism spectrum and psychotic affective 708–714.
spectrum disorders. In: A. Alvergne, C. Eley, T.C., Collier, D. & McGuffin, P. (2005).
Jenkinson & C. Faurie (eds.) Evolutionary Anxiety and eating disorders. In: P. McGuffin,
Thinking in Medicine: From Research to Policy M.J. Owen & I. Gottesman (eds.) Psychiatric
and Practice (pp. 299–327). Switzerland: Genetics and Genomics (pp. 303–340).
Springer. Oxford, OUP.
Crespi, B.J. (2019). Autism, psychosis, and Ellis, B.J., Shirtcliff, E.A., Boyce, W.T., Deardorff,
genomic imprinting: Recent discoveries and J. & Essex, M.J. (2011). Quality of early family
conundrums. Current Opinion in Behavioral relationships and the timing and tempo of
Sciences, 25, 1–7. puberty: Effects depend on biological sen-
Crespi, B.J. & Badcock, C. (2008). Psychosis sitivity to context. Developmental Psycho-
and autism as diametrical disorders of the pathology, 23, 85–99.
social brain. Behaviour and Brain Sciences, Faer, L.M., Hendriks, A., Abed, R.T. & Figueredo,
31, 241–320. A.J. (2005). The evolutionary psychology of
Crow, J.F. (2000). The origins, patterns, and eating disorders: Female competition for
implications of human spontaneous muta- mates or for status? Psychology and Psycho-
tion. Nature Reviews Genetics, 1, 40–47. therapy, Theory, Research and Practice, 78,
Crow, T.J. (1997). Is schizophrenia the price 397–417.
Homo sapiens pays for language? Schizo- Ferguson, C.J., Winegard, B. & Winegard, B.N.
phrenia Research, 28, 127–141. (2011). Who is the fairest of them all? How
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND PSYCHIATRY 47

evolution guides peer and media influences Hettema, J.M., Neale, M.C. & Kendler, K.S.
on female body dissatisfaction. Review of (2001). A review and meta-analysis of
General Psychology, 15, 11–28. genetic epidemiology of anxiety disorders.
First, M.B. (2007). Potential implications of the American Journal of Psychiatry, 158,
harmful dysfunction analysis for the develop- 1568–1578.
ment of DSM-V and ICD-11. World Psychia- Homberg, J.R. & Lesch, K.P. (2011). Looking on
try, 6(3), 158–159. the bright side of serotonin transporter gene
Fox, M. (2018). ‘Evolutionary medicine’ per- variation. Biological Psychiatry, 69,
spectives on Alzheimer’s disease: Review and 513–519.
new directions. Ageing Research Reviews, Horowitz, A.V. & Wakefield, J.C. (2007). The
47, 140–148. Loss of Sadness: How Psychiatry Transformed
Fried, E.I. & Nesse, R.M. (2015). Depression is Normal Sorrow into Depressive Disorder.
not a consistent syndrome: An investigation Oxford, OUP.
of unique symptom patterns in the STAR* D Horrobin, D.F. (2001). The Madness of Adam
study. Journal of Affective Disorders, 172, and Eve: How Schizophrenia Shaped Human-
96–102. ity. Ealing, Bantam.
Fulford, K.W.M. & Thornton, T. (2007). Fanatical Hudson, J.I., Hiripi, E., Pope, H.G. Jr. & Kessler,
about ‘harmful dysfunction’. World R.C. (2007). The prevalence and correlates of
Psychiatry, 6(3), 161–162. eating disorders in the national comorbidity
Gilbert, P. (2006). Evolution and depression: survey replication. Biological Psychiatry, 61,
Issues and implications. Psychological Medi- 348–358.
cine, 36, 287–297. Humphrey, N. & Skoyles, J. (2012). The evolu-
Gilbert, P., McEwan, K., Hay, J., Irons, C. & tionary psychology of healing: A human suc-
Cheung, M. (2007). Social rank and attach- cess story. Current Biology, 22(17),
ment in people with bipolar disorder. Clinical R695–R698.
Psychology & Psychotherapy, 14, 48–53. Huxley, J., Mayr, E., Osmond, H. & Hoffer, A.
Gluckman, P.D. & Hanson, M.A. (2006). Mis- (1964). Schizophrenia is a genetic morphism.
match: The Lifestyle Diseases Timebomb. Nature, 204, 220–221.
Oxford, OUP. Insel, T. (2013). Transforming diagnosis. www.
Gluckman, P.D., Beedle, A.S. & Hanson, M.A. nimh.nih.gov/about/directors/thomas-insel/
(2009). Principles of Evolutionary Medicine. blog/2013/transforming-diagnosis.shtml
OUP. (accessed May 1st 2019).
Gordon, R.A. (1990). Anorexia and Bulimia: Jacquemont, S., Coe, B.P., Hersch, M., Duyzend,
Anatomy of a Social Epidemic. Cambridge, M.H., Krumm, N., Bergmann, S., Beckmann,
Wiley-Blackwell. J.S., Rosenfeld, J.L. & Eichler, E.E. (2014). A
Guisinger, S. (2003). Adapted to flee famine: higher mutational burden in females supports
Adding an evolutionary perspective on ano- a ‘female protective model’ in neurodevelop-
rexia nervosa. Psychological Review, 110, mental disorders. The American Journal of
745–761. Human Genetics, 94, 415–425.
Hagen, E.H. (2003). Depression as bargaining: Juda, M.N., Campbell, L. & Crawford, C.B.
The case postpartum. Evolution and Human (2004). Dieting symptomatology in women
Behavior, 23, 323–336. and perceptions of social support. Evolution
Hagen, E.H. (2011). Evolutionary theories of and Human Behavior, 25, 200–208.
depression: A critical review. Canadian Jour- Kaiser, J. & Gruzelier, J.H. (1999). Timing
nal of Psychiatry, 56(12), 716–726. of puberty and syndromes of schizotypy: A
Haig, D. (2014). Genetic dissent and individual replication. International Journal of Psycho-
compromise. Biology and Philosophy, 29, physiology, 34, 237–247.
233–239. Katzman, M.A., Hermans, K.M.E., van Hoeken,
Hare, E.H. & Moran, P.A. (1979). Raised paren- D. & Hoek, H.W. (2004). Not your ‘typical
tal age in psychiatric patients: Evidence for island woman’: Anorexia nervosa is reported
the constitutional hypothesis. British Journal only in subcultures in Curaçao. Culture,
of Psychiatry, 134, 169–177. Medicine and Psychiatry, 28, 463–492.
48 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Kavan, M.G. & Borone, E.J. (2014). Grief and McGuire, M.T. & Troisi, A. (1998). Darwinian
major depression – controversy over changes Psychiatry. New York: OUP.
in DSM-5 diagnostic criteria. American McQueen, D., Cohen, S., St John-Smith, P. &
Family Physician, 90(10), 693–694. Rampes, H. (2013). Rethinking placebo in
Keller, M.C. & Miller, G. (2006). Resolving the psychiatry: How and why placebo effects
paradox of common, harmful, heritable occur. Advances in Psychiatric Treatment,
mental disorders: Which evolutionary genetic 19(3), 171–180.
models work best? Behavioral and Brain Sci- Mealey, L. (1995). The sociobiology of
ences, 29, 385–452. sociopathy: An integrated evolutionary
Kendler, K. (2001). Twin studies of psychiatric model. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 18,
illness: An update. Archives of General Psy- 41–79.
chiatry, 58, 1005–1014. Mealey, L. (2000). Anorexia: A ‘losing’ strategy?
Kendler, K.S. & Eaves, L.J. (2005). Psychiatric Human Nature, 11, 105–116.
Genetics. Arlington, American Psychiatric Moir, R.D., Lathe, R. & Tanzi, R.E. (2018). The
Publishing. antimicrobial protection hypothesis of
Kendler, K.S. (2008). Explanatory models for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s & Dementia,
psychiatric illness. American Journal of Psy- 14, 1602–1614.
chiatry, 165, 695–702. Nesse, R.M. (2001). On the difficulty of defining
Kinney, D.K., Teixeira, P., Hsu, D., Napoleon, disease: A Darwinian perspective. Medicine,
S.C., Crowley, D.J. & Miller, A. (2009). Rela- Health Care and Philosophy, 4, 37–46.
tion of schizophrenia prevalence to latitude, Nesse, R.M. (2007). Evolution is the scientific
climate, fish consumption, infant mortality, foundation for diagnosis: Psychiatry should
and skin color: A role for prenatal vitamin D use it. World Psychiatry, 6(3), 160–161.
deficiency and infections? Schizophrenia Nesse, R.M. (2011). Ten questions for
Bulletin, 35, 582–595. evolutionary studies of disease vulnerability.
Kristiansen, J.E. (1990). The antimicrobial activity Evolutionary Applications, 4(2), 264–277.
of psychotherapeutic drugs and stereo- Nesse, R.M. (2013). Tinbergen’s four questions
isomeric analogues. Danish Medical Bulletin, organized: A response to Bateson and
37(2), 165–182. Laland. Trends in Ecology & Evolution,
Kuhn, T. (1962). The Structure of Scientific 28(12), 681–682.
Revolutions. Chicago, University of Chicago Nesse, R.M. (2019). Good Reasons for Bad
Press. Feelings: Insights from the Frontiers of Evolu-
Li, N.P., Smith, A.R., Griskevicius, V., Cason, tionary Psychiatry. UK: Allen Lane.
M.J. & Bryan, A. (2010). Intrasexual competi- Nesse, R.M. & Stein, D.J. (2012). Towards a
tion and eating restriction in heterosexual genuinely medical model for psychiatric
and homosexual individuals. Evolution and nosology. BMC Medicine, 10(5), www.
Human Behavior, 31, 365–372. biomedcentral.com/1741-7015/10/5
Marr, D. (1982). Vision: A Computational Nesse, R.M. & Williams, G. (1994). Why We
Investigation into the Human Representation Get Sick: The New Science of Darwinian
and Processing of Visual Information. San Medicine. New York, Times Books.
Francisco, WH Freeman. Nettersheim, J., Gerlach, G., Herpertz, S.,
Martel, M.M. (2013). Sexual selection and sex Abed, R., Figueredo, A.J. & Brüne, M.
differences in the prevalence of childhood (2018). Evolutionary psychology of eating
externalizing and adolescent internalizing disorders: An explorative study of patients
disorders. Psychological Bulletin, 139(6), with anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa.
1221–1259. Frontiers in Psychology, 9. doi: 10.3389/
McCrae, R.R. & Costa, P.T. (2003). Personality fpsyg.2018.02122
in Adulthood: A Five-Factor Theory Perspec- Nettle, D. (2001). Strong Imagination: Madness,
tive (2nd ed.). New York, Guilford. Creativity and Human Nature. Oxford: OUP.
McGrath, J.J. (2006). Variations in incidence of National Institute for Health and Care Excellence
schizophrenia: Data versus dogma. Schizo- (2013). Schizophrenia: Omega-3 Fatty Acid
phrenia Bulletin, 32, 195–197. Medicines. www.nice.org.uk/advice/esuom19/
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND PSYCHIATRY 49

chapter/key-points-from-the-­evidence (accessed: from a mediation analysis. PLOS One, 10,


February 1st 2019). e0128248.
Orsolini, L., St John-Smith, P., McQueen, D., Russell, G., Ford, T., Rosenberg, R. & Kelly, S.
Papanti, D., Corkery, J. & Schifano, F. (2017). (2014). The association of attention deficit
Evolutionary considerations on the emerging hyperactivity disorder with socioeconomic
subculture of the e-psychonauts and the disadvantage: Alternative explanations and
novel psychoactive substances: A comeback evidence. Journal of Child Psychology and
to the shamanism? Current Neuropharma- Psychiatry, 55, 436–445.
cology, 15(5), 731–737. Shaner, A., Miller, G. & Mintz, J. (2004). Schizo-
Owen, M.J., Craddock, N. & Jablensky, A. phrenia as one extreme of a sexually selected
(2007). The genetic deconstruction of Psycho- fitness indicator. Schizophrenia Research, 94,
sis. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 33, 905–911. 58–63.
Pleuss, M. & Belsky, J. (2010) Children’s differ- Singh, D. (1993). Adaptive significance of
ential susceptibility to effects of parenting. female physical attractiveness: Role of waist-
Family Science, 1(1), 14–25. to-hip ratio. Journal of Personality and Social
Plomin, R. (2018). Blueprint: How DNA Makes Psychology, 65, 293–307.
Us Who We Are. London: UK, Allen Lane. Stearns, S.C. (1992). The Evolution of Life
Polimeni, J. (2012). Shamans Among Us: Schiz- Histories. New York: OUP.
ophrenia, Shamanism and the Evolutionary Stevens, A. & Price, J. (2000a). Evolutionary Psy-
Origins of Religion. Boston: EvoEbooks. chiatry: A New Beginning (2nd ed.) London,
Power, R.A., Kyaga, S., Uher, R., McCabe, J.H., Routledge.
Langstrom, N., Landen, M., McGuffin, P., Stevens, A. & Price, J.S. (2000b). Prophets,
Lewis, C.M., Lichtenstein, P. & Svenson, A.C. Cults and Madness. London, Duckworth.
(2013). Fecundity of patients with schizo- St John-Smith, P., McQueen, D., Edwards, L. &
phrenia, autism, bipolar disorder, depression, Schifano, F. (2013). Classical and novel psy-
anorexia nervosa, or substance abuse vs their choactive substances: Rethinking drug
unaffected siblings. JAMA Psychiatry, 70, misuse from an evolutionary psychiatric per-
22–30. spective. Human Psychopharmacology: Clini-
Price, J.C., Sloman, L., Gardner, R., Gilbert, P. & cal and Experimental, 28, 394–401.
Rohde, P. (1994). The social competition Stohler, H.R. (1978). RO 11-3128 – a novel
hypothesis of depression. British Journal of schistosomicidal compound. Vol. 1 Proceed-
Psychiatry, 164(3), 309–315. ings of the 10th International Congress of
Rantala, M.J., Luoto, S., Krams, I. & Karlsson, Chemotherapy (pp. 147–148). Washington
H. (2018). Depression subtyping based on DC, American Society for Microbiology.
evolutionary psychiatry: Proximate mecha- Sullivan, R.J., Hagen, E.H. & Hammerstein, P.
nisms and ultimate functions. Brain, Behavior (2008). Revealing the paradox of drug reward in
and Immunity, 69, 603–617. human evolution. Proceedings of the Royal
Richardson, G., Chen, C-C., Dai, C-L., Swoboda, Society B: Biological Sciences, 275,
C.M., Nedelec, J.L. & Chen, W-W. (2017). 1231–1241.
Substance use and mating success. Evolution Surbey, M.K. (1987). Anorexia nervosa, amenor-
and Human Behavior, 38(1), 48–57. rhea, and adaptation. Ethology Sociobiology,
Roshchina, V.V. (2010). Evolutionary considera- 8, 47–61.
tions of neurotransmitters in microbial, plant, Symons, D. (1995). Beauty is in the adaptation
and animal cells. In: M. Lyte & P.P.E. Free- of the beholder. In: P.R. Abramson & S.D.
stone (eds.) Microbial Endocrinology – Pinkerson (eds.) Sexual Nature, Sexual
Interkingdom Signaling in Infectious Disease Culture (pp. 80–118). Chicago, University of
and Health (pp. 17–52). Cham: Springer. Chicago Press.
Rottenberg, J. (2014). The Depths: The Evolu- Tinbergen, N. (1963). On the aims and methods
tionary Origins of the Depression Epidemic. of ethology. Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie,
New York, Basic Books. 20, 410–433.
Russell, A.E., Ford, T. & Russell, G. (2015). Socio- Tooby, J. & Cosmides, L. (1999). Toward an evo-
economic associations with ADHD: Findings lutionary taxonomy of treatable conditions.
50 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 108(3), Wakefield, J.C. (1992). The concept of mental
453–464. disorder: On the boundary between biological
Troisi, A. (2001). Gender differences in vulner- facts and social values. American Psycholo-
ability to social stress: A Darwinian perspec- gist, 47, 373–388.
tive. Physiology & Behavior, 73, 443–449. Wakefield, J.C. (2014). Wittgenstein’s night-
Troisi, A. (2012). Mental health and well-being: mare: Why the RDoC grid needs a concep-
Clinical applications of Darwinian psychiatry. tual dimension. World Psychiatry, 13(1),
Applied Evolutionary Psychology. doi: 10.1093/ 38–40.
acprof:oso/9780199586073.003.0017 Wasser, S.K. & Barash, D.P. (1983). Reproduc-
Troisi, A. (2015). The evolutionary diagnosis of tive suppression among female mammals:
mental disorder. WIREs Cognitive Science, 6, Implications for biomedicine and sexual
323–331. selection theory. Quarterly Review of Biol-
Trull, T.J. & Widiger, T.A. (2013). Dimensional ogy, 58, 513–538.
models of personality: The five-factor model Watson, P. & Andrews, P. (2002). Toward a
and the DSM-5. Dialogues in Clinical Neuro- revised evolutionary adaptationist analysis of
science, 15(2), 135–146. depression: The social navigation hypothesis.
Tsuang, M., Taylor, L. & Faraone, S. (2003). Journal of Affective Disorders, 72, 1–14.
Psychiatric genetics: Future and prospects. White, T.P., Borgan, F., Ralley, O. & Shergill, S.S.
In: M. Leboyer & F. Bellivier (eds.) Psychiatric (2016). Are you looking at me? Interpreting
Genetics: Methods and Reviews (pp. 251– social cues in schizophrenia. Psychological
265). Totwa, NJ: Humana Press. Medicine, 46, 149–160.
Vining, D.R. (1986). Social versus reproductive Wilkinson, L.S., Davies, W. & Isles, A.R. (2007).
success: The central theoretical problem of Genomic imprinting effects on brain devel-
human sociobiology. Behavior and Brain Sci- opment and function. Nature Reviews Neu-
ences, 9, 167–216. roscience, 8, 832–843.
Voland, E. & Voland, R. (1989). Evolutionary biol- Williams, G. (1957). Pleiotropy, natural selec-
ogy and psychiatry: The case of anorexia ner- tion, and the evolution of senescence. Evolu-
vosa. Ethology Sociobiology, 10, 223–240. tion, 11(4), 398–411.
Von Gunten, A., Clerc, M-T., Tomar, R. & St World Health Organization (1992). ICD-10.
John-Smith, P. (2018). Evolutionary consider- Geneva, World Health Organization.
ations on aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Yeo, R.A., Gangestad, S.W., Edgar, C. & Thoma,
Journal of Alzheimers Disease and Parkinson- R. (1999). The evolutionary genetic under-
ism, 8(1). doi: 10.4172/2161-0460.1000423 pinnings of schizophrenia: The developmen-
Von Helversen, B., Wilke, A., Johnson, T., tal instability model. Schizophrenia Research,
Schmid, G. & Klapp, B. (2011). Performance 39, 197–206.
benefits of depression: Sequential decision Yilmaz, Z., Hardaway, J.A. & Bulik, C.M. (2015).
making in a healthy sample and a clinically Genetics and epigenetics of eating disorders.
depressed sample. Journal of Abnormal Psy- Advances in Genomics and Genetics, 5,
chology, 120, 962–968. 131–150.
3
Evolutionary Psychology
and Suicidology
J o h n F. G u n n I I I , P a b l o M a l o , a n d C . A . S o p e r

As of patch 7.822, androids can no longer shut challenge (Satcher, 1999; World Health
themselves down. Reason: they just kept doing it Organization, 2012, 2014). Indeed, a new
at the slightest inconvenience – @ctrlcreep, quoted
multi-disciplinary field of research emerged
by Perry (2016).
in the second half of the 20th century, suicid-
ology, focused on tackling the problem
(American Association of Suicidology, 2019;
Shneidman, 2001).
SUICIDE, SUICIDOLOGY AND But, frustratingly, decades of effort have
EVOLUTION produced only patchy progress. The global
suicide rate has fallen in recent years, but it
Suicide, ‘the act of deliberately killing oneself’ is not clear why (it may be because of gen-
(World Health Organization, 2014: 12), takes erally improved population health rather than
about 800,00 lives each year, and accounts for special prevention initiatives) and wide unex-
some 1.4% of human deaths: more of us die by plained differences in rates and trends persist
our own hands than from wars, terrorism, and (Naghavi, 2019). In the United States, for
all other forms of homicides put together example, the rate is probably the same now as
(World Health Organization, 2013). Millions it was 100 years ago, and seems to be rising
more of the living are affected – bereaved (Hedegaard et  al., 2018; Nock et  al., 2019).
families, friends, and carers left to deal with Rival theories of suicide have accumulated by
the aftermath of others’ self-destruction the dozens,1 but none has won a consensus of
(Cerel et  al., 2019). Around the world, sui- support, and suicide’s causation remains a sci-
cide is acknowledged to be a major, and entific mystery (Lester, 2019; Nock, Borges,
presumed preventable, cause of misery and and Ono, 2012; Soper, 2019a). The disarray
death, and an important public health is such that a recent meta-review describes
52 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

suicidology as ‘still in a preparadigmatic The Need for Evolutionary


phase’ (Franklin et al., 2017: 188) – that is, Explanation of Suicide
still in its infancy.
There may at least be the beginnings of a A preliminary question is whether evolution
consensus that the proper place for the sci- is at all relevant to suicide. Many human
entific study of suicide is alongside other activities can be viewed not as products of
modern life sciences – within an evolutionary selection but as exemplars of our behavioral
paradigm. This vision can be traced across flexibility: to some extent we are free to do as
generations of researchers. A century ago, we will despite having biological drives
in the evolutionist spirit of his time, Freud (Sarkar, 1998). Perhaps suicide is one such
(1920/1991) hypothesized a potentially sui- behavior (deCatanzaro, 1980). At a proximal
cidogenic ‘death drive’ as an extension of his level of understanding (Tinbergen, 1963)
theory of libido, a framework inspired by the such an answer might suffice. But there are at
Darwinian premise that selection depends least three reasons to believe that evolution
on sexual success (Gilbert, 1989; Litman, by natural selection is not just relevant but
1967; Tolaas, 2005). But psychoanalysis essential for making sense of the phenome-
failed to find a satisfactory explanation for non (Soper, 2018).
suicide, at least according to Freud’s con- First, we can deduce that suicide is under
temporaries (Zilboorg, 1936a) – debate con- the control of selection because the behavior
tinues (Goldblatt, 2014; Selby et  al., 2014). presents the full trio of handles – (a) herit-
And although other ideas have been floated ability, (b) variability, and (c) a differential
since, as we will see, suicide remains an effect on fitness – with which selection takes
evolutionary puzzle (Aubin et  al., 2013; hold of any trait (Darwin, 1859). Suicidality
Blasco-Fontecilla et al., 2009; Confer et al., (a) tends to cluster strongly in families, with
2010). On the face of it, self-killing defies at least some genetically heritable component
the rule of thumb for winning the struggle (Mullins et al., 2019; Tidemalm et al., 2011).
for existence: survive and reproduce. Darwin Suicide risk (b) varies markedly across and
himself told us, ‘Natural selection will never between groups of humans (e.g., De Leo
produce in a being anything injurious to itself, et  al., 2013; Schmidtke, 1997; Voracek and
for natural selection acts solely by and for the Marušič, 2008). And (c) if death is usu-
good of each’ (1859: 201). Yet here we are, ally calamitous for an organism’s reproduc-
scions of selection but with a more or less tive prospects, death by one’s own hand is
steady percentage of us taking our own lives. predictably even worse because of special
How could so self-destructive a propensity social, economic, and psychological penal-
have come about? Why has it persisted? And ties imposed on suicides’ kin (Wertheimer,
what can we do about it? 2014), a matter we will explore. The point to
Searching for answers, this chapter criti- note for now is that, with these three levers,
cally reviews prominent evolutionary think- selection would be expected powerfully to
ing in the field. We find tentative signs of promote the offspring of the less suicidal,
progress. Focusing on recent proposals, eventually driving the potential for suicide
we will discuss in particular a new ‘pain- out of the human genotype. But selection
and-brain’ framework – that suicide likely has evidently not done this, and the apparent
evolved as an evolutionary by-product of anomaly calls for an account.
social pain and human cognition (Gunn, The second reason for seeking evolution-
2017; Humphrey, 2011, 2018; Soper, 2018) – ary explanation is that suicide is endemic
which may offer a basis for convergence in across the human population. As far as can be
suicide theory and, it is hoped, new prospects known, no sizeable region, culture, or histori-
for saving lives. cal era is exempt (Bering, 2018; Fedden, 1938;
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND SUICIDOLOGY 53

Mishara, 2006). Where suicide is not directly (2018) notes, no distraught chimp has been
observable it can be inferred, as Durkheim seen, say, to climb to the highest available
(1897/1952) points out, from the fresh imprint branch and jump. Experimental set-ups have
it leaves in universal, or near universal, been devised that could in principle demon-
antisuicide moralities: suicide presumably strate nonhuman suicides under laboratory
posed enough of a societal threat in the past conditions, but there are no reports of posi-
to warrant proscribing. Importantly, suicide’s tive results (Lester, 2017; Schaeffer, 1967).
ubiquity extends to preliterate and hunter- Secondly, there is no lack of motivation to
gatherer societies (Steinmetz, 1894; Syme find evidence: discovery of an animal model
et  al., 2016; Tousignant, 1998; Zilboorg, of suicide would likely attract intense popular
1936b), which indicates ancient roots: it is interest, and catch the eye of a well-resourced
no mere novelty of modern conditions. Such pharmaceutical industry keen to test whether
universal human traits were probably in place suicide risk is affected by drugs (Malkesman
at the time of ancient human migrations out et al., 2009; Preti, 2011). Thirdly, if nonhu-
of Africa, and likely follow an unbroken line mans could suicide, then it would raise the
of descent (Brown, 2004; Kappeler et  al., question not of whether they do, but why it
2010). The curiosity is that, as Darwin (1859) is not commonplace behavior (Soper and
deduced, features that confer no selective Shackelford, 2018). Life in the Malthusian
advantage tend to phase out over time, hence arena of natural selection, a relentless strug-
the disappearance of the hind limbs of ceta- gle to survive and reproduce, is intrinsically
ceans and flying wings of some island birds. not pleasant. Combatants face pain, hunger,
There is no evidence of degeneration of suici- thirst, rejection, defeat, disease, and whatever
dality, a continuity that tells us that selection other privation. An animal that knew it could
has positively held the capacity for suicide in escape hardship by removing itself from the
place – that is, for some evolutionary reason. battlefield would fairly be expected do so.
Third, suicide is almost certainly a In other words, nonhuman suicide, if it were
uniquely human phenomenon. It is right to possible at all, ought to be not so rare as to
keep an open mind (Peña-Guzmán, 2018), elude scientific discovery but a routine out-
but there is scant evidence – none that meets come of animal suffering.2 In any event, aside
a scientific standard – that any other animal from absence of evidence, there are positive
deliberately takes its own life (Bering, 2018; grounds for doubting in principle that any
Comai and Gobbi, 2016; Maltsberger, 2003; nonhuman could be capable of suicide. The
Preti, 2007). Absence of evidence is not required intention – self-induced death of
evidence of absence, of course. But the the self – presumes an understanding of per-
absence of scientific evidence speaks vol- sonal mortality, a conceptual abstraction that
umes in the context of animal suicide because is demonstrably beyond the grasp of prepu-
there are at least three reasons to believe that, bescent humans (Kastenbaum, 1967; Seiden,
if such evidence existed, we could reasonably 1969; Slaughter and Griffiths, 2007; Soper,
expect it to have found its way into a peer- 2018) let alone of less intellectually sophisti-
reviewed publication by now. First, centuries cated animals (Anil et al., 1996; Bracke, 1992).
of concerted scientific enquiry have offered An adult chimp, said to be the smartest nonhu-
ample opportunities to observe nonhuman man, might match the deductive powers of, at
suicide – if it were there to be observed most, a 4- or 5-year-old child (O’Connell and
(Ramsden and Wilson, 2010). Chimpanzees, Dunbar, 2003), but the mind of a typical 5-year-
for example – our nearest living cousins and old child brain must develop over as many
hence arguably the species in which sui- years again, and more, before it is capable
cide is most likely to be found – have been of conceiving and organizing deliberate self-
studied particularly closely; but as Bering killing (Mishara, 1999; Shaffer, 1974; Soper,
54 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

2018). In sum, suicide is evidently unique to penalties predictably follow, as can be seen
our species, an exceptionality that presents a from Table 3.1. If the attempt is fatal (the
further call for an evolutionary account: the upper section of the table), then the fitness
behavior likely arose in the course of specia- consequences of dying (dying generally, that
tion, at or after our phylogenetic path diverged is – not specifically by one’s own hand) rip-
from that of our extant primate cousins. ple out from the individual also to harm close
relations and the wider kin and social group
(Duntley, 2005). Heading the costs schedule
(Item 1) is the fitness catastrophe of forfeit-
The Fitness Costs of Suicide
ing future opportunities for procreation. It is
So, suicide calls for evolutionary explana- hard to overstate this loss. For semelparous
tion. The main difficulty with formulating an species (they breed only once, such as Pacific
explanation, we suggest, is not so much that salmon) death may carry little or no genetic
behavior is self-injurious: many evolved downside after their reproductive phase is
traits can have self-injurious effects while complete (Cole, 1954). But for iteroparous
being fitness-enhancing overall (Williams, species (geared for multiple rounds of breed-
1996). The special problem is that suicide is ing), such as virtually all mammals, including
self-injurious to an extraordinary degree. humans, the cost of dying is severe, as can
From a genetic viewpoint, suicide is literally be inferred from the lengths gone to avoid it.
a fate worse than death. Whether an attempt is Most higher faunas are overridingly protec-
lethal or survived, multiple and severe fitness tive of their reproductive potential, however

Table 3.1  The fitness costs of suicide


Lethal attempt

Death generally: (Daly and Wilson, 1988; Duntley, 2005; Duntley and Buss,
1. Ends prospects of producing more offspring. 2004; Lankford, 2015)
2. Ends ability to invest in existing offspring.
3. Ends ability to support co-parent in their raising of
existing offspring.
4. Ends further prospects of investing in reproductive
success of other close relations (kin selection).
5. May deprive group of skills, experience, manpower.
6. May destabilize group’s power and allegiance structures.
Suicide specifically: (Andoh-Arthur et al., 2019; Bohannan, 1960; Chapple
7. Distancing and other special economic and social et al., 2015; Fedden, 1938; Grad and Andriessen, 2016;
costs for offspring and other close kin: loss of status, Hanschmidt et al., 2016; Healey, 1979; Hezel et al., 1985;
resources, mating opportunities. Mugisha et al., 2011; Poole, 1985)
8. Special psychological and emotional problems for (Bolton et al., 2013; Cerel and Aldrich, 2011; Erlangsen et al.,
offspring and other close kin; increased risk of 2017; Grad and Andriessen, 2016; Jordan, 2008; Pitman
psychopathology and suicide. et al., 2014; Sveen and Walby, 2008; Wertheimer, 2014)

Survived attempt

9. Risk of death by suicide (costs as above).


10. Prospect of physical injury and/or disfigurement, (Gandhi et al., 2006; Kahne, 1966; Kennedy et al., 1999; Penney
possibly permanent and/or serious. et al., 2002; Persley and Pegg, 1981; Salim et al., 2006)
11. Negative emotional sequelae: guilt, shame, psychological (Akotia et al., 2014; Kahne, 1966; Mehlum and Mork, 2016;
trauma, heightened risk of further suicide attempts. Stanley et al., 2019)
12. Social stigmatization: distancing, loss of status, loss of (Bering, 2018; Brown, 1986; Frey et al., 2015; Kahne, 1966;
mating opportunities. Knizek et al., 2013; Lester, 1993; Saunders et al., 2012;
Sheehan et al., 2016; Sudak et al., 2008)
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND SUICIDOLOGY 55

slight. In extremis, when their own survival of intentional self-killing do not end there,
is endangered, they will kill even offspring or because there are extra, special, penalties
siblings to preserve the capacity to procreate for kin bereaved specifically in this way.
(Hausfater and Hrdy, 1984; O’Connor, 1978). Table 3.1 groups these consequences, per-
Iteroparity helps to explain why intentional haps arbitrarily, into rejection and other
self-killing is not found among other animals social sequelae (Item 7), and psychological
(Lankford, 2015): in the Darwinian competi- injuries (Item 8). The first of these, involv-
tion to survive and reproduce, the organism’s ing often exemplary punishments for a sui-
death spells genetic ‘game over’. cide’s relations, seems to be a cross-cultural
Item 2, death ends the organism’s ability to phenomenon linked to a virtually universal
invest in existing progeny. The ill-timed death abhorrence of the act (Andoh-Arthur et  al.,
of a parent can seriously compromise the repro- 2019; Bohannan, 1960; Fedden, 1938; Hezel
ductive prospects of offspring – for our species et al., 1985). In the West, people bereaved by
more than others in view of the protracted suicide report being stigmatized – distanced
dependency of human childhood. Disadvantage as if they were tainted or contaminated by
appears to be a cross-cultural outcome: even association (Chapple et  al., 2015). Harsher
in, or especially in, pre-industrial societies, penalties are found elsewhere. Among the
children bereaved of one or both parents face Baganda in Uganda, for example, close kin
life with fewer resources and die younger as of suicides face disinheritance, termination
a consequence (Bailey, 2009; Geary, 2005). of their familial lineage, burning of their
Death also closes off the possibility of helping homes, and exile (Mugisha et al., 2011). And
a surviving co-parent in their task of raising then there is the noxious psychological fall-
the individual’s children (Item 3). The widow/ out, sometimes lethal, manifest in markedly
er, with their own survival needs to meet, may higher rates of mental illness and suicidality
be less able or less willing to care for the dead among suicides’ families (Erlangsen et  al.,
partner’s young. If the surviving parent pairs 2017; Jordan, 2008; Pitman et  al., 2014,
with a new mate, the deceased’s offspring are 2016; Wertheimer, 2014).
exposed to a new set of hazards at the hands of Importantly, dire fitness costs predictably
a step-parent, who will have competing genetic follow a suicide attempt even if it is survived,
interests (Daly and Wilson, 1988). as indicated by the lower section of Table
More broadly, Item 4, death ends the indi- 3.1. Aside from (9) risking suicidal death
vidual’s ability to invest labor, skills, and with its accompanying genetic forfeits as
experience towards the reproductive suc- already discussed, a suicide attempt can be
cess of other family members and the wider expected to injure and/or disfigure the actor
kin group, people whose offspring could (Item 10). The damage may be permanent
propagate the individual’s genetic material and/or serious. For example, jumping from
indirectly (Duntley, 2005). Wider still, multi- bridges, roofs, etc. often results in paraplegia
level selection (Wilson and Wilson, 2007) (Kennedy et al., 1999), and suicide attempts
would be expected to disfavor mortality with by pregnant women associate with conse-
or without kinship relations: death ends an quent maternal and perinatal morbidity and
individual’s contributions to the competitive sometimes perinatal death (Gandhi et  al.,
success of the individual’s group (Item 5); 2006). Injuries sustained in a suicide attempt
and as Duntley (2005) notes (6), a death may can be psychological as well as physical,
create a power vacuum that could destabilize and are often traumatic (Item 11): a quar-
a group’s organization and possibly unravel a ter of suicide attempters in a recent study
whole network of allegiances. screened positive for post-traumatic stress
Clearly, it is ‘bad to be dead’ (Duntley disorder resulting directly from their attempts
and Buss, 2004: 107). But the fitness costs (Stanley et al., 2019). People who have tried
56 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

to kill themselves are affected by intrusive ‘NOISE’ THEORIES


feelings of shame and blame (Akotia et  al.,
2014; Mehlum and Mork, 2016), and are at Could suicide be the kind of useless trait that
a heightened risk of making further attempts sometimes spreads in isolated populations by
(Turecki and Brent, 2016). happenstance? On the face of it, probably
Finally (Item 12), attempters face distanc- not: it is hard to conceive of suicide as trivial
ing social attitudes, even from professionals in its fitness impact, and the phenomenon is
charged with helping them, and from close not restricted to isolated groups. Nonetheless,
family members (Frey et  al., 2015; Kahne, an interesting ‘noise’-type theory warrants
1966; Knizek et  al., 2013; Saunders et  al., scrutiny. DeCatanzaro (1980, 1981, 1986) – a
2012). The stigma may impair reproductive sociobiologist, and the first writer to explore
fitness most directly via sexual deselection, as the evolution of suicide in depth – suggests
Bering (2018) points out: a replicated study that people who have no prospects of produc-
found that people would rather, all else equal, ing further offspring may kill themselves for
marry someone they loved even from a margin- want of biological reason to stay alive: they
alized ethnic group or dying of cancer in prefer- are already genetically dead. The idea links to
ence to someone they loved who had recently the principle of senescence (Dawkins, 1980;
tried to take their own life (Lester, 1993). Williams, 1957): if an organism has no repro-
Table 3.1, a grim catalogue as it stands, ductive future, then any subsequently emer-
may not be exhaustive. Clearly, in fitness gent trait may have no fitness effect. Perhaps
terms, suicide is exceptionally costly. This is suicide may be one such condition, a non-
a central point to note, because in order for selected behavior that, according to deCatan-
the trait to have become a genetic fixture it zaro, may express particularly among elderly
presumably associates with a commensu- bachelors. He compares their fate to that of the
rately powerful fitness benefit. The chal- semelparous salmon we mentioned earlier:
lenge, taken up by several theorists in recent once their procreative work is done, they die.
decades, is to try to identify this countervail- The proposal that low reproductive poten-
ing upside, as we will now review. tial may drive or open the door to suicide
In the following sections, various propos- has sparked interest among other theorists
als are organized into three headings – mod- (Campbell, 2002; Saad, 2007), and it finds some
ern evolutionary theory allowing three ways, empirical support (deCatanzaro, 1980, 1981,
and only three, by which any characteristic 1982, 1986, 1991; Soper, 2018). But there are
can propagate genetically across generations forceful objections (Bering, 2018; Lankford,
(Tooby and Cosmides, 1990b; Williams, 2015; Lester, 2014b; Rubinstein, 1986; Soper,
1966). A trait that has no effect on fitness can 2018; Wright, 1994). We highlight three.
sometimes arrive in isolated populations by First, as already noted, humans are not
chance and then fix, as background genetic semelparous: like virtually all mammals, we
‘noise’, for lack of selective pressure against are designed for multiple episodes of repro-
it (Wright, 1943); or a trait may propagate as duction. Although fertility declines with age,
an adaptation, directly selected for its fitness- species-typical men remain potentially able
enhancing effect; or it may spread not as an to father offspring throughout adulthood, and
adaptation but as a by-product of some other would hence be expected to avoid self-killing
feature that is adaptive overall, notwithstand- at almost any stage of life (Bribiescas, 2006;
ing its side effects. We will discuss these Lankford, 2015).
three in turn, reaching the provisional conclu- Second, the hypothesis is empirically con-
sion that suicide appears best to fit the third traindicated by much of the epidemiology
type of explanation – a harmful by-product of (Lester, 2014b). For example, suicides around
an adaptation. the world are characterized more by youth
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND SUICIDOLOGY 57

than old age, occurring more among under- organism’s genes (half equivalent), and so
45s than older (Värnik, 2012), and the demo- on. Genetic success, then, depends not on the
graphic most at risk of trying to take their number of direct offspring an organism pro-
own lives are not old men but young women duces, but the number of offspring equiva-
(Nock, Borges, Bromet et al., 2012), people lents, direct or not (Hamilton, 1964). Kin
whose reproductive careers could be assumed selection is the favoring of an organism’s
to lie ahead of them. At the other extreme, relations in the interests of inclusive fitness,
populations with certainly zero direct repro- even if this endangers the survival or direct
ductive prospects – post-menopausal women, reproduction of the organism (Maynard
and castrated men – are not reported to be Smith, 1964). Behaviors that display appar-
particularly suicidal (Usall et  al., 2009; ent altruism may therefore be genetically
Wilson and Roehrborn, 1999). self-serving: organisms that act altruistically
Third is a general problem for ‘genetic may advantage the survival and reproduction
noise’ explanations for suicide: unselected of kin which, due to genetic relatedness, may
traits are very unlikely to promote survival, pass on to their offspring the genes responsi-
but they would also be vanishingly unlikely ble for the altruism. Since, for mammals at
to produce willful self-killing, or any other least, death is assuredly unhelpful for direct
specific pattern of behavior for that matter reproduction, adaptationist explanations of
(Soper, 2018). The lifting of selection would suicide hypothesize reproductive payoffs to
be predicted, rather, simply to let the second be had via such indirect routes.
law of thermodynamics prevail. Spawned An often cited example of kin selection as it
salmon, a case in point, don’t deliberately supposedly relates to suicide is the seemingly
kill themselves – they carry on, for example, altruistic, but genetically selfish, responses
trying to evade predators: rather, depleted of worker bees, ants, and other social insects
of energy, they disintegrate. For suicide to when under attack (e.g., Shorter and Rueppell,
eventuate, some canalizing process would 2012). A caveat: although etymologists infor-
be needed to shape that particular outcome. mally talk of ‘suicide’ in the context of insect
The key to understanding the evolutionary behaviors, use of the word should not be taken
origins of suicide probably lies in identifying to signal equivalence with human self-killings
this special suicidogenic system rather than (Lankford, 2015; Soper, 2018). There are
in random genetic dynamics. important and categorical differences. One
stems from the special familial structure of
social insects: in a colony composed largely of
non-reproducing siblings, and where only one
ADAPTATION THEORIES queen is permitted to reproduce, there may be
genetically little to lose and much to gain in
Adaption-type explanations of suicide draw sacrificing an individual sibling in the colony’s
on a cluster of connected tenets of modern defense (Alexander, 1974; Hamilton, 1972) –
evolutionary theory: inclusive fitness, kin inclusive fitness logic that does not apply to
selection, and altruism. Inclusive fitness rec- virtually all mammals, including humans. To
ognizes that direct reproduction isn’t the only view ant ‘suicide’ as at all self-destructive is
way an individual’s genetic material can pass to superimpose inappropriately onto biology a
into future generations: facilitating reproduc- folk notion of the ‘self’: the biological ‘self’
tion by others who share the individual’s for ants may be more usefully viewed as the
genes can have the same result. For humans, colony, operating as a super-organism, rather
as other mammals, direct offspring carry half than the individual insect. Helpful analogies
of the organism’s genes (one equivalent) for ant ‘suicide’ would include, not literal
while a sibling’s child carries a quarter of the (human) suicide, but programed cell death, the
58 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

shedding of a tree’s leaves in the fall, or a liz- prominent adaptationist idea in suicide
ard self-amputating its tail to escape attack, research (Aubin et  al., 2013; Bering, 2018;
these being losses of comparable genetic Soper, 2018; Syme et al., 2016). Brown and
inconsequence (Dawkins, 1976; Hamilton, colleagues, for example, develop deCatanza-
1980; Lankford, 2015). Nonetheless, insect ro’s proposals and offer supportive empiri-
behaviors demonstrate that survival isn’t cal evidence (Brown et al., 1999, 2009). As
always an overriding biological imperative: other researchers also record, various meas-
as deCatanzaro (1992) points out, there are ures of burdensomeness do correlate with
‘evolutionary limits to self-preservation’. suicidal thinking and behavior (Chu et  al.,
Developing this theme, deCatanzaro pro- 2017; Lester, 2014b) – although, it should
vides most of suicidology’s adaptationist be noted, not strongly and no more strongly
literature (e.g., 1980, 1981, 1986, 1991), than do many other risk factors (Franklin
seeking to fill the theoretical gap noted in the et  al., 2017). Further empirical support
previous section – the need for some canaliz- might arguably be found in ethnographic
ing process that produces suicide as a specific reports from some tribal societies where a
outcome. He suggests that human self-killing group member too frail to survive the next
may be adaptive where a burdensome indi- season or journey may sometimes take
vidual’s death advantages the reproductive part in ritualized assisted suicide, although
prospects of close kin. DeCatanzaro (1986) this practice is not common (Falger and
went on to express this idea as a mathemati- Falger, 2003).
cal formula for a tipping point at which an These are all intriguing ideas. But the
individual’s genetic interests would be served state of play is that, after four decades, there
by self-removal. This calculation, he argues, are no signs of a scientific consensus form-
may shed light not just on self-killings but ing around the conception of suicide as the
on attempted suicide, self-sacrificial military adaptive removal of unsupportable kin
actions, and risk-taking behavior. Following (Bering, 2018; Lester, 2014b; Soper, 2018).
this lead, other theorists assert that inclusive There are multiple empirical contraindica-
fitness logic may explain suicide as a geneti- tions, including high notable suicidality
cally adaptive strategy aimed at protecting among young adults (De Leo et  al., 2013),
kin from infection or infestation (Tanaka and the gifted and talented (Delisle, 1986;
Kinney, 2011) or from internecine conflict Voracek, 2006), people with high incomes
(Riordan, 2019); and potentially to account (Goldsmith et  al., 2002) and others who, it
for suicide terrorism (Gallup and Weedon, can be presumed, are unlikely to be among
2013). Still others suggest that processes the most burdensome members of their
of multi-level selection, in which a group’s families. The general difficulty we perceive
competitive success may be furthered by the is a lack of compelling evidence of special
altruistic behavior of its members whether design – a precise match between observ-
genetically related or not, may offer adaptive able form and biological function that is the
logic for extreme acts of heroism in battlefield hallmark of adaptation:
situations (Orbell and Morikawa, 2011). Some
theorists offer a ‘mismatch’ variation on the Evolutionary adaptation is a special and onerous
concept that should not be used unnecessarily,
adaptation idea; that suicide may not be adap- and an effect should not be called a function
tive in current conditions but, as fossil behav- unless it is clearly produced by design… (Williams,
ior carried over from ancient environments, it 1966, vii)
might have been adaptive in the past (Aubin
et al., 2013; deCatanzaro, 1980, 1981). Three disconnects will illustrate the prob-
Self-killing as a way to relieve kin of lem. First, is the absence of a causal connec-
the burden of one’s existence is the most tor between suicide as the means and the end
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND SUICIDOLOGY 59

it is supposed to achieve: it is unclear why is unlikely to evolve (Symons, 1992; Tooby


the task of removing an individual should and Cosmides, 1990b).
call for suicide in preference to other solu- Third, given the severity of costs out-
tions that have stronger genetic logic (Bering lined in Table 3.1, the hypothesized benefits
and Shackelford, 2004). If an individual has accruing via kin or group selection seem
to be removed, there is no particular reason to fall decisively short, in power and reli-
to expect that removal to necessitate a kill- ability, of what would be required to justify
ing: abandonment, quarantine, or exile, say, self-killing as a fitness investment. The sug-
would achieve the same objective, with the gested payoffs are highly contingent, it being
advantage of preserving at least provision- far from certain that the reproduction of an
ally the individual’s reproductive capability individual’s family or group would improve
(Wright, 1994). And even if a killing would as a consequence (Lankford, 2013). On the
be in a conspecific’s reproductive interests, contrary: Table 3.1 suggests that suicide can
the expectable outcome is still not suicide: the be predicted to add to, not lift, reproductive
killing would more logically be done by that difficulties for those left behind.
conspecific, who has likely better informa- Summing up, it is hard to argue that sui-
tion and genetically more to gain (O’Connor, cide shows marks of special design. At the
1978; Skinner, 1969; Soper, 2018). Indeed the margins, some writers suggest possible adap-
empirical evidence among humans and other tive functionality in heroic deeds in battle and
species points to infanticide or fratricide, similar emergencies, which might be classed
rather than suicide, as the way unsupportable as ‘altruistic suicide’ following Durkheim’s
kin are removed (Dickeman, 1975; Harris, (1897/1952) nosology (Humphrey, 2018;
1974; Hrdy, 1979; O’Connor, 1978). Orbell and Morikawa, 2011). Other writers
Second, it is doubtful in principle whether question the usefulness of ‘altruistic suicide’
a biological stimulus could exist that would as a concept (Johnson, 1965; Townsend,
trigger suicide as an adaptive response. 2007). The phenomenon is unusual, as
Perry (2015) presumes that if an organism Durkheim himself acknowledged. And it
is to decide whether or not life is genetically may not anyway help to class as ‘suicide’
worth continuing, then it would need to be acts where killing of the self is not of itself
equipped with some kind of ‘inclusive fitness the primary intention, but where, rather, death
monitor’ that can compare the reproductive happens incidentally in pursuit of some other
value of life overall versus death. For sure, endeavor (Lankford, 2013). For these reasons
sophisticated measuring devices have been Soper (2018) argues that battlefield scenarios
proposed elsewhere by evolutionists, such are unlikely to shed light on private, solo
as a ‘sociometer’ that may alert the organ- self-killing – what Cholbi (2017) calls ‘run-
ism to threats to social supports (Leary and of-the-mill’ suicide – which probably is not
Guadagno, 2011; Leary et  al., 1995). But and never would have been adaptive.
Soper (2018) argues that a serviceable ‘inclu-
sive fitness monitor’ would entail an alto-
gether higher order of complexity; it would
need to take a view on, among other things, ‘BY-PRODUCT’ THEORIES
current and future kin members’ reproduc-
tive prospects and the future carrying capaci- With only three types of evolutionary expla-
ties of their environments. He claims that it nations available, and if suicide cannot be
would be so all-encompassing that it would credibly ascribed to the first two (that is, it
lack the specific input–output associations probably evolved neither as noise nor as an
to which selection responds. Modern theory adaptation), then Soper (2018) argues that
holds that such a general-purpose mechanism whatever remains has prima facie appeal.
60 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Explicitly or implicitly, several theories fall interpersonal communication. Their original


into this third category, characterizing sui- idea is that an otherwise powerless individ-
cide not as adaptive but as a noxious side effect ual, modally a young woman, may, at the
of some other trait that, despite the severe cost extreme, threaten to or try to kill herself as
of suicidality, is adaptive in the round. The the only available means by which she can
following section reviews prominent ideas of induce kin to attend to her honest genetic
this type. needs. It may be in her reproductive interests
to make a potentially high-stakes gamble,
informed unconsciously by an inclusive fit-
Pleiotropy ness calculation. Suicidal death, from this
perspective, can be seen as an unfortunate
The idea of senescence, as we discussed in gamble lost. The behavior posited to be adap-
above, did not seem to carry us far in under- tive is the threat of, or attempt at, suicide; the
standing suicide. But senescence is a special potential for death being necessarily con-
case of a broader genetic phenomenon, pleiot- comitant for the threat to be credible (Wiley,
ropy, which may take us further (Williams, 2020). A recent variation of the idea, also
1992; Wilson, 1980). Pleiotropy occurs where with the inherent risk of a lethal, maladaptive
genes express in the same individual in multi- outcome, is that a suicide attempt may con-
ple phylogenetic outcomes, some beneficial, stitute a costly signal of remorse (Syme and
some harmful. Particularly if a beneficial Hagen, 2018).
effect is felt early in the individual’s lifetime, Perhaps this line of theorizing may use-
then it may more than compensate for an inju- fully shed light on non-suicidal self-injury
rious one that emerges later – hence the link and unintended fatalities, both outside the
with senescence. We can safely presume that scope of this discussion. Researchers have
suicide arises in this general way, as a harmful long hypothesized a “cry for help” compo-
effect of genetic material that is fitness- nent in sub-lethal self-harming behaviors
enhancing on average. This is not to say that a (Shneidman and Farberow, 1961), phenomena
‘suicide gene’ exists or will ever be found: which may be significantly distinct from sui-
genetic predisposition to suicide appears to be cide (Kapur, Cooper, O’Connor and Hawton,
spread thinly across a large number of genes, 2013; Selby et  al., 2014; Stengel, 1970).
each of weak effect, interacting with each Among the chronically suicidal, it is possi-
other and with environmental factors (Marušič ble that a cycle of reinforcement may arise in
and Swapp, 2004; Mullins et al., 2019).3 More which carers’ well-meaning responses inad-
likely, as deCatanzaro (1980) suggests, inten- vertently provoke yet more behavioral cries
tional self-killing occur as an incidental effect for help (Linehan, 2020). There may also be
of a species-typical genome, the expression of strong commonsense appeal in attributing sui-
which has proved adaptive overall in the cide to communication. There is an important
course of human evolution. But then, what caveat in this regard: folk theorizing about the
adaptive aspect of this species-typical genome cause of suicides is evidently not an objective
could by-produce suicide? data source. It is rather, at least in large part,
a product of encultured post-rationalization
(Atkinson, 1978; Solano, Pizzorno, Pompili,
Serafini and Amore, 2018; Soper, 2019a).
Suicide as Communication
For example, alongside communication-type
One posited answer, offered by Hagen and explanations, and presumably as reliably,
Syme, (Hagen, 2002; Syme et al., 2016; Syme non-western informants often also blame
and Hagen, 2018), is that deaths may occur suicides on evil spirits, as Syme et al. (2016)
as an unfortunate by-product of suicidal themselves found. Observers frequently intuit
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND SUICIDOLOGY 61

interpersonal motives for attempted suicides threat or costly signal were needed: desertion,
too, but it should be born in mind that these sexual infidelity, self-mutilation, infanticide,
interpretations rarely agree with actors’ own sabotage, arson, and so forth. In sum, it is not
explanations: and where they do agree it may clear that suicide constitutes an outstandingly
be because surviving attempters sometimes good solution to the needs of coercive, or any
confess motives that they think others expect other, communication.
to hear (Bancroft et  al., 1979). All that said, The other difficulty recalls, again, the
we question whether communication offers extreme and predictable fitness penalty of a
a satisfactory evolutionary explanation for suicide attempt, whether lethal or survived
intentional self-killing, partly for the same (Table 3.1). It is hard to imagine a commen-
reasons that we found more directly adapta- surately extreme and predictable fitness ben-
tionist models wanting. We will highlight two efit that could, even in theory, be won from
difficulties (a critique may also be found in a suicidal communication. Empirically, the
Soper (2018)). epidemiology shows no obvious pattern of net
The first problem recalls the hallmark fitness gains. Lesser upsides offered as sup-
of adaptation: evidence of special design porting evidence in Syme et al.’s ethnographic
(Williams, 1966, 1996). It is an intuitive call, analysis (2016, supplementary material, table
but suicide, whether completed, attempted S3) seem to fall well short in specificity (e.g.,
or threatened, does not strike us as likely ‘Manipulate parents’) and reproductive impact
custom-designed for communication. People (‘Prevented unwanted ear modification’) to be
who seriously intend to take their own lives, plausibly sufficient, as biological prizes, to
presumably against kin’s wishes, would logi- justify the fitness losses and risks taken in try-
cally be expected not to communicate that ing to kill oneself. The strongest claimed pay-
intent, at least not sufficiently to invite inter- offs are cases where actors are alleged to have
ference. With rare exceptions, the empirical sought sexual concessions, such as ‘Prevent
picture seems to fit this expectation. Perhaps it unwanted marriage’ and ‘Concubine moved
is different in some parts of the world – most out of house’ – psychologically appealing,
of the evidence is from Western sources – but no doubt; but in a fitness calculation, still not
suicides are usually characterized by non-­ credibly worth dicing with genetic extinction
communication: privacy and non-disclosure for their sake. If there is not a plausible net fit-
are the norm. That they tend to happen without ness gain to be had from going through with a
effective warning may be inferred from rela- threat, then it is unclear on what fitness grounds
tives’ immediate reaction to the news: typi- such a threat would be accepted as being cred-
cally shock, confusion, and disbelief (Chow, ible. This is not to say that there may not be
2006; Dyregrov et  al., 2012). Far from reg- social utility in threatening to kill oneself, or
istering a communicated message, bereaved any other threat for that matter. The error, we
families are generally left bewildered by the suggest, is to confuse non-evolutionary and
act’s apparent senselessness (Jordan and evolutionary processes – to confuse utility
McIntosh, 2011; Wertheimer, 2014). Non- with biological fitness.
communication is characteristic of attempted Stepping back, we question a general
suicides too (Maple et al.). Where an attempt assumption that underlies hypotheses dis-
is survived, close kin are usually not told about cussed so far, that the fitness payoff of what-
it either before or after the event, and they usu- ever process it is that produces suicide derives
ally remain unaware even long afterwards from the suicide behavior itself. In principle,
(Brezo et  al., 2007; Walker et  al., 1990). At pleiotropy does not require there to be any
the same time, one can imagine any number obvious connection between a selected trait
of other deviances, potentially costly but not and its noxious concomitants (Williams,
ordinarily suicidal, available for use if a drastic 1992). The explanatory gap between suicide’s
62 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

hypothesized benefits (weak, and contingent) of the adaptive behavioral suite evolved as a
and evident costs (severe, and predictable; facet of eusociality’ (Joiner et al., 2017: 71).
Table 3.1) suggests that we probably need to Evolutionary rationale for this distinction
look beyond the act itself for a categorically isn’t provided, but the need to make a distinc-
more powerful biological impetus. With this tion seems to stem from the authors’ equating
in mind, we will next look at some prominent of insect and human sociality – a question-
evolutionary ideas circulating in mainstream able premise given the special familial make-
suicidology. up of insect colonies noted earlier. Adding to
the confusion is what appears to be a mixing
of biological process and moral evaluation
The Interpersonal–Psychological (Gorelik and Shackelford, 2017) something
that has long colored scientific discourse in
Theory of Suicide (IPTS)
this field (Soper, 2019a; Zilboorg, 1936a).
Another conceptual framework, the IPTS’s second element, thwarted belong-
Interpersonal–Psychological Theory of ingness that may motivate self-killing, is on
Suicide (IPTS), deserves attention. In various firmer theoretical ground and is where sui-
formulations it may currently be suicidology’s cide is envisioned explicitly as a dysgenic by-
most prominent theory (Hjelmeland and product (Joiner, 2005; Van Orden et al., 2010).
Knizek, 2019; Lester, 2019; Paniagua et  al., The aversiveness of rejection, abandonment,
2010), and it explicitly draws on some evolu- and similar interpersonal stressors has cer-
tionary ideas, including the notion that suicide tainly been attributed to evolutionary origins:
may have come about as an evolutionary by- psychological pain probably functions as an
product. In its original form IPTS holds that ancient alarm system, warning of potentially
suicide results from the co-occurrence of three fitness-damaging social losses (Baumeister
supposedly sufficient conditions: (1) a percep- and Leary, 1995; Bowlby, 1969/1997, 1973,
tion of being a burden to one’s family, (2) a 1980/1991). Suicide can be understood as a
thwarted desire to belong, and (3) a learned way to escape from this adaptive social dis-
capability to enact lethal self-injury (Joiner, tress. We will return to this idea.
2005; Van Orden et al., 2010). We will discuss Less strong in its theoretical underpin-
each in turn. nings is the third component of IPTS, a
The first element, a feeling that one is a hypothesized learned capability for lethal
liability to loved ones, has been mapped by self-injury. The idea derives from a centuries-
some commentators onto deCatanzaro’s (1980) old belief that suicide defies an ‘instinct for
sociobiological idea of burdensomeness – self-preservation’. This supposedly universal
critiqued earlier – to which suicide is allegedly natural drive must be overpowered, IPTS’s
the organism’s genetically adaptive response authors claim, before suicide can be carried
(Aubin et al., 2013; Brown et al., 2009). But out (Joiner, 2005; Van Orden et  al., 2010).
in what they call a ‘sociobiological exten- IPTS’s originator, Joiner (2005), ascribes
sion’ of their own theory, IPTS’s authors evolutionary credentials to an ‘instinct for
depart from deCatanzaro’s view of burden- self-preservation’, but the notion faces prin-
someness in that they posit (human) suicide cipled objections from evolutionists, not
to be intrinsically maladaptive: colonial least because there is no known biologi-
insects and humans are said to be equivalently cal means by which such a superordinate
eusocial, but while lethally self-sacrificial drive could come about (Kirkpatrick and
behaviors are understood to have inclusive Navarrete, 2006; Soper, 2019a). Modern the-
fitness logic for insects, supposedly the same ory holds that a general-purpose motivational
response among humans is seen as an error device would be underspecified, lacking the
– a ‘dysfunction, misfiring, or derangement recurring connections between proximal
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND SUICIDOLOGY 63

stimulus and fitness-impacting response that enables the organism to navigate fitness haz-
are required for selection to take hold: a sys- ards in its environment, both physical (Wall,
tem without specific links between inputs and 1999) and, as we have already noted, social
outputs is unlikely to be favored (Buss, 1990; (MacDonald and Leary, 2005). And in order
Buss and Penke, 2015). The same difficulty, to fulfil its navigation task, pain is necessar-
it will be recalled, arose above in discussions ily motivational: it is designed precisely to
of an ‘inclusive fitness monitor’. Soper (2016, force the organism to take action to relieve it
2018) suggests that if an antisuicide mecha- (Auvray et al., 2010; Corns, 2014; Melzack
nism were reconceived within the tenets of and Casey, 1968). Hence, pain hurts. As the
evolutionary psychology, as a special rather leprosy surgeon Paul Brand observed, pain
than general-purpose device, then useful is a valuable gift, albeit a gift nobody wants
implications follow. We will come back to this (Brand and Yancey, 1993).
point as well. Second, there is an accord among suicide
theorists and empirical researchers about
the kind of pain that most powerfully moti-
Suicide as an Escape from Pain vates action – whatever action – to obtain the
required relief. Although physical pain also
Pyschological pain may drive the phenotype associates with suicidality (Klonsky et  al.,
to seek relief in a way that is destructive for 2019; Klonsky and May, 2015), emotional
the genotype. We saw this idea underlying pain is more usually held responsible. The
IPTS’s second component (above), and indeed unbearable emotional state that can induce
it can be traced to suicidology’s formative people to take their own lives is encapsulated
writings. In the words of Henry A. Murray by Shneidman’s (1993) neologism, psychache:
(the clinician whom Edwin Shneidman, the
acknowledged father of suicidology, saw as Psychache refers to the hurt, anguish, soreness,
his mentor), aching, psychological pain in the psyche, the mind.
It is intrinsically psychological – the pain of exces-
sively felt shame, or guilt, or humiliation, or loneli-
Suicide does not have adaptive (survival) value but
ness, or fear, or angst, or dread of growing old or of
it does have adjustive value for the organism…it
dying badly or whatever. (1993: 51; original italics)
abolishes painful tension. (Murray and Kluckhohn,
1948: 15, original italics)
There is also implicit agreement across
By this perspective, suicide offers escape more than a century of suicide research that
(Baechler, 1975/1979; Baumeister, 1990; unbearable emotional pain usually stems from
Shneidman, 1993, 1996, 2005). A related social troubles. The disagreement is rather
idea, but with much older philosophical roots, about what hue of social trouble is deemed
is that self-killing may be a rational response most troublesome. Durkheim (1897/1952), for
to difficult life circumstances (Maris, 1982; example, cited detachment (‘anomy’) as the
Mishara, 2003). General reviews are avail- chief driver. A hundred years later, Williams
able elsewhere of ‘suicide as escape’ theories and colleagues ascribe suicide to feelings
(Gunn, 2014) and rational suicide (Lester, of social defeat and entrapment (Williams,
2014a). Of interest for our purposes is their 1997; Williams and Pollock, 2000; Williams
recognition, implicit or explicit, of the sui- et  al., 2005). Williams’ ideas reappear in
cidogenic power of emotional pain (Selby another theoretical framework, one that rivals
et al., 2014). IPTS, the Integrated Motivational–Volitional
There are two points to note. First, Model of Suicidal Behavior (IMV) devised
although pain systems can malfunction, pain by O’Connor and colleagues (O’Connor,
almost certainly has ancient adaptive origins 2011; O’Connor et  al., 2016). IPTS, it will
(Nesse and Schulkin, 2019). Pain’s signal be recalled, highlights yet other varieties of
64 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

interpersonal distress – thwarted belonging is even narrower than this because, as well as
and burdensomeness. And so on. nonhumans, certain human populations too
Gunn (2017) connects these threads in an are protected from taking their own lives,
explicitly evolutionist frame; his Social Pain however painful their circumstances: young
Model (SPM) registers that suicidogenic psy- children (Nock et  al., 2013; Shaffer, 1974)
chache is usually activated by various forms of and the mentally incapacitated (Merrick et al.,
damaged or threatened social relations. This 2006; Seyfried et al., 2011). Their immunity,
social pain, as with pain generally, is both or virtual immunity, also needs to be
adaptive and intrinsically aversive. It is adap- accounted for. Noting commonality across
tive in that it is designed to alert the organism these groups, Baechler (1975/1979) draws a
to the fitness threat posed by detachment – parsimonious conclusion that deliberate self-
rejection presaging near certain death for our killing presupposes a minimum level of intel-
hunter-gatherer forbears (Bjorklund et  al., lectual functioning. Soper (2017, 2018)
2010; Eisenberger and Lieberman, 2004; concurs, arguing that it’s only after more than
Eisenberger et  al., 2003; Lieberman, 2013). a dozen years of cerebral development that
And social pain is necessarily painful in order species-typical humans, and apparently only
to induce action to alleviate it. Unfortunately, humans, acquire sufficient capacity for logi-
suicide offers a genetically self-destructive cal thinking, foresight, and planning, for sui-
answer to this biological imperative to act. cide to become a conceivable and practicable
In sum, a weight of empirical and theo- response. Humans alone appear to cross what
retical evidence points to suicide instantiat- Perry (2014: 110) describes as a ‘cognitive
ing as an unfortunate by-product of pain, “floor” for suicide’, usually in adolescence.
notably social pain, this being integral to the Thus, a second necessary condition for sui-
navigational equipment that humans need cide emerges: cognitive competence. This may
for maintaining attachments in large and be central for understanding the behavior’s
close-knit groups. The aversiveness of social evolutionary origins. If deliberate self-killing
pain, adaptively, demands action to end it – presupposes the crossing of a developmental
a demand that can be met maladaptively by threshold during the individual’s lifetime, then
self-extinction. we can deduce that an evolutionary counter-
part, a phylogenetic threshold, also had to be
surpassed at some point in human prehistory.
Humphrey (2011: 211) makes the point by
‘Pain-and-Brain’ Model
quoting Stengel (1970: 37)…
But social pain alone cannot be sufficient
At some stage of evolution man must have discov-
explanation for the evolution of suicide. If it
ered that he can kill not only animals and fellow-
were sufficient, suicide would presumably be men but also himself. It can be assumed that life
found elsewhere among primates and other has never since been the same to him.
higher social animals. Indeed, solitary ani-
mals would expectably be vulnerable too: to Humphrey (2018) goes on to ascribe this
reprise the point, pain (social or other) is pivotal discovery to the arrival, around 100,000
biologically designed not to be tolerated – it years ago, of a suite of uniquely human cogni-
demands that the organism act to end or tive skills, including abstract thinking, mental
escape it – so, any animal that could termi- time travel, self-consciousness, and sophisti-
nate its pain by terminating itself would rea- cated theory of mind. These are virtuoso dem-
sonably be expected do so (Perry, 2016; onstrations of the flexible, general-purpose
Soper and Shackelford, 2018). We are still style of thinking of Homo sapiens sapiens –
searching for an explanation for suicide as a promiscuous intelligence favored by selection
narrowly human response. The search space because it conferred potent ecological, social,
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND SUICIDOLOGY 65

and sexual advantages (Flinn and Alexander, adults, equipped with much the same cognitive
2007; Humphrey, 1976; Pinker, 2010; Tooby machinery (Tooby and Cosmides, 1990a),
and DeVore, 1987). But the human brain have self-removal available as an exit route
is ‘expensive tissue’ (Aiello and Wheeler, from pain. A consequent ‘lure of death’
1995): encephalization brought with it (Humphrey, 2018) has probably featured in
heavy costs, including energetic demands our evolutionary environment at least since
(Aiello and Wheeler, 1995), mutational load the advent of behaviorally modern humans.
(Keller and Miller, 2006), obstetric chal- Perhaps for much longer – presumably,
lenges (Miller and Penke, 2007), problems indeed, ever since hominid intelligence began
of thermoregulation (Falk, 1990), and the to approach that of a modern-day pubescent
burden of supporting young while the brain child (Soper, 2019b). By implication, suicide
matures (Flinn et  al., 2007). Suicidality can presents a harmful variety of, in evolutionary
be understood as another such cost, a price parlance, adaptive problem:
we pay for a mind so free ranging that it can
Adaptive problems are evolutionarily long-enduring
conceive even of its own mortality (Bering
recurring clusters of conditions that constitute
and Shackelford, 2004; deCatanzaro, 1980; either reproductive opportunities (e.g., the arrival
Soper, 2018; Suddendorf, 2013). To sum of a potential mate, the reflectant properties of
up, it appears likely that suicide evolved as light) or reproductive obstacles (e.g., the speed of
a by-product of not one adaptation, but two a prey animal, the actions of a sexual rival, limited
food supplies for relatives). (Cosmides and Tooby,
combined: pain (usually social); and species-
2000: 96)
typical human cognition. Soper calls this a
pain-and-brain theory of suicide. Adaptive problems seek out adaptive solu-
tions (Mayr, 1965; Tooby and Cosmides,
1990b; Williams, 1996). That almost all
Suicide as an Adaptive Problem adults could be expected to take their own
lives, but few do, indicates that adaptive
The above discussion suggests that scientific solutions to the suicide problem are in fact
consensus, if loose and implicit, can be seen in place. Without them, we as individuals,
coalescing around some form of ‘by-product’ and we as a species, would not be here. At a
explanation for suicide’s ultimate origins. A phylogenetic level, hominid evolution would
pain-and-brain framework in particular does not have found a way through the cognitive
not appear contentious: it seems to offer a floor for suicide, the floor acting as a ceil-
point of agreement among prominent suicide ing of viable intelligence: in this light suicide
theories, it accords with the epidemiology of emerges as perhaps the pre-eminent adaptive
suicide, and connects to knowledge bases of problem of our species (Soper, 2019b). Our
neuroscience, anthropology, and elsewhere. existence presents a puzzle not so much of
But it raises new and difficult questions, suicide but non-suicide. What stops most of
mainly because, as Soper (2016, 2018, 2019a) us killing ourselves?
infers, the posited pain-and-brain conditions
are not only necessary for suicide, but logi-
cally sufficient: pain provides the motive, and Evolved Antisuicide Defenses
mature human cognition provides the means.
If the pain-and-brain assessment is correct, In seeking to answer this question, why not
then the fitness threat of suicide exists in suicide, we can set aside the pre-Darwinian
potentia among virtually all of us. All species- notion of a general-purpose survival instinct –
typical humans feel, and are motivated to critiqued briefly above and more fully else-
escape, pain (Benatar, 2015; Brand and where (Buss, 1990; Buss and Penke, 2015;
Yancey, 1993); and all species-typical human Kirkpatrick and Navarrete, 2006; Soper, 2019a).
66 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

The likely solution would look more like the for greater computing power (Geary, 2007;
customized ‘patch 7.822’ artificial intelli- Miller, 2000), while intelligentsia who lacked
gence fantasy that began this chapter (Perry, adequate protection were culled. Soper pos-
2016). If a fleet of androids dealt with every its that it was only after basic defenses were
difficulty by, uselessly, switching themselves installed that human intelligence could pro-
off, the problem would call for a software gress to the level of being able to enculture
update to eliminate that particular behavioral antisuicide ideas, proscriptions which pre-
option. In actuality, the human brain, a bio- suppose a capacity to conceive of personal
logical computer (Tooby and Cosmides, mortality.
2005), has presumably been retrofitted with a It is possible to deduce something of the
comparable fix, devised by selection. We likely design features of antisuicide defenses,
next need to ask: how would such a patch as we will next discuss, by adopting evolu-
operate? And what would its successful oper- tionary psychology’s ‘task analysis’ method
ation look like? (Tooby and Cosmides, 1992) – inferring
Two sorts of antisuicide programs have been likely parameters of an adaptive solution from
hypothesized in recent years (Humphrey, the nature of the adaptive problem. Soper
2018; Miller, 2008; Soper, 2016, 2017, (2018) suggests that, in view of the severity
2018). The first are evolved psychological of the threat, antisuicide devices are prob-
mechanisms (Buss, 1995); the second are ably arranged as serial fortifications, each
culturally propagated barriers. There won’t line guarding the position behind and deploy-
be a neat dichotomy in reality because many ing special stimulus-response mechanisms
evolved psychological mechanisms rely on to that end. Perhaps simplifying, he suggests
cultural inputs (Tooby and Cosmides, 1992), a framework that splits into front and rear
and many cultural devices exploit evolved defenses. At the back are emergency inter-
psychological preparedness for learning in ventions, labelled keepers to connote the pri-
their particular domains (McNally, 2016). mary role of a goalkeeper on a soccer team:
Nonetheless the distinction is interesting – on stand-by much of the time, keepers leap
partly because, although both sorts of devices into ‘Save!’ mode at times of crisis, and are
might be expected to emerge, there is dis- all that stands between an attacking shot and
agreement over their likely chronology and disaster. Front-line protections, in contrast,
relative importance. Humphrey (2018) rea- are continuously active: following the soccer
sons that psychological mechanisms (what he analogy, Soper labels them fenders to suggest
calls ‘natural’ or ‘innate’ protections) would a team’s other defensive players. Fenders’ job
not have arisen as the first or foremost pro- is to stop crises arising in the first place. The
tection because of their slowness to evolve. default outcome, if players can’t fulfil their
Soper (2018, 2019a) argues the contrary, respective tasks, is a conceded goal – a sui-
that they probably came first, emerging at cide attempt – which, as noted earlier, may be
least in part during a reconfiguration of ana- genetic ‘game over’.
tomically modern humans’ ‘wetware’ ahead Soper argues that defenses’ triggering
of the cultural explosion of the Later Stone inputs and behavioral outputs would be
Age (Klein and Edgar, 2002). The evolution expected to address both of suicide’s dual
of human intelligence may have been held at pain-and-brain drivers, and in distinct ways.
the cusp of suicidality for perhaps 150,000 To borrow the soccer analogy again, the fit-
years or more, while largely autonomic solu- ness hazard of suicide can be conceived as an
tions to the suicide problem were assembled opposing team with only two forward play-
and refined. The pressure favoring such solu- ers, ‘Pain’ and ‘Brain’, and they pose a threat
tions would have been intense due to a com- only when they attack together. They have
bination of forces: runaway selection pushing distinct styles of play, and the defending team
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND SUICIDOLOGY 67

must adopt customized pain-type and brain- of positive affect (Kuhl et al., 2015; Layard,
type tactics to neutralize them. 2011) via, inter alia, investment in pleasura-
Let us look first at the system design of ble activities beyond what would be econom-
keepers, the posited emergency defenses. To ically justified by normal animalian needs
detect an imminent threat from ‘Pain’, keep- of survival and reproduction (Pinker, 1997).
ers must be ready to respond to the experience And fourth, is the selection and maintenance
of chronic and intense emotional distress. To of a hopeful, measuredly fictitious, mental
detect an incoming ‘Brain’ threat, keepers model of self-in-the-world. This paradigm,
must also be alert to the surpassing, in adoles- more or less spiritual, holds that the universe
cence, of the cognitive threshold for suicide. and its people are not wholly indifferent to
Cues for both ‘Pain’ and ‘Brain’ must be pre- our wellbeing, and that our futures are not
sent for keepers to mobilize. And, likewise, entirely doomed to the pain that would attend
there are two, and only two, strategies avail- a purely Darwinian struggle. Thus, operating
able by which keepers can block incoming within (to adapt Baumeister’s (1989) phrase)
attacks: pain-type and brain-type. Pain-type an optimum margin of delusion, we perceive
keepers make self-killing unnecessary: they the world not as it is but through a rose-tinted
numb, distract from, or otherwise weaken the lens. This enhanced reality system is under
felt urgency of emotional pain as a motivator continual assault by factual counterevidence,
for escape. Brain-type keepers deny the means: and hence requires us continuously to defend
they interfere with intellectual functioning it, literally as if our lives – or, at least, mental
enough usually to prevent an effective suicide health – depend on it. Arising from this pro-
attempt being organized. We will discuss keep- tective worldview, Soper says, is a costly pro-
ers in more detail after this overview. The main pensity for selflessness and charity – that is,
point to note for now is that keepers’ interven- generosity that goes beyond the reciprocal cal-
tions may be highly costly to the organism – culus of economics. Taken as a whole, these
and they are evidently not failsafe: hence the sundry irrational-looking morale-boosters are
need for the pre-emptive work of fenders. not easy otherwise to explain and connect, but
As for fenders, the forward line of protec- they may be understood to work as integrated
tions; these are hypothesized also to deploy psychological machinery, designed to prevent
in pain- and brain-type forms. Pain-type crises of willful self-destruction and the neces-
fenders titrate the organism’s exposure to sity for keepers to intervene.
emotional pain within tolerable limits. They A separate set of evolved, culturally learned
use what could loosely be called positive protections, independently hypothesized by
psychology (Efklides and Moraitou, 2013) to Miller (2008), Humphrey (2018), and Soper
hold humans safely distant, most of the time, (2018), are designed to block access to the
from the potentially lethal danger that resides idea of suicide. They are brain-type fenders
lop-sidedly in negative affect. Soper suggests in Soper’s scheme. He suggests they propa-
they may work as four subsystems. First, is a gate by multi-level selection and form three
homeostasis of affect around a resting point serial walls: a thought-inhibiting taboo; the
that is happier than neutral (Heintzelman expectation of a fearful afterlife; and a mor-
and King, 2014), a base at which shocks can alistic stigma. The deterrent function of this
generally be absorbed without great disrup- last barrier, unfortunately but perhaps nec-
tion. Second, is a regulation of conscious essarily, involves exemplary punishments
contact with potentially painful realities – a for suicides and their kin, some noted in
self-serving self-deception that, empiri- Table 3.1. The dismantling of these prohibi-
cally, characterizes so-called psychoanalytic tions, the acceptance of suicide as a normal
(or psychodynamic) defenses (Paulhus and topic of conversation and a reasonable solution
Buckels, 2012). Third, is a manufacturing to trying circumstances, can release a surge of
68 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

suicides (Huber, 2015/2019). Such a wave may A summary graphic (Figure 3.1, from Soper,
perpetuate for generations (Macdonald, 2007): 2018) illustrates how the various posited pro-
once lost, cultural defenses may be exception- tections may interrelate. Out of a great num-
ally difficult to reinstate (Soper, 2018). ber of potential suicidal incidents, symbolized

Figure 3.1  Summary of posited antisuicide defences


Source: Reproduced from Soper (2018).
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND SUICIDOLOGY 69

by a mass of dots at the top of the diagram, a. Keepers would be activated by chronic,
only a few find their way through the for- intense pain (subject to the develop-
tifications to materialize as actual suicide mental condition of a ‘brain’ input). Soper
attempts, indicated by a couple of dots at the predicts that an activating ‘pain’ cue will combine
bottom. By focusing not on supposed (possi- (by some unspecified algorithm) pain’s chronicity
and intensity. Chronicity, on the grounds that sui-
bly inscrutable; Soper, 2019a) proximal cau-
cides take time to plan and enact (Joiner, 2005),4
sation of suicide but on evolved machinery so potentially drastic countermeasures should
posited to stop suicide, the graphic departs not be triggered by merely ephemeral upsets.
conceptually from the style of flow chart that Intensity, because the stronger the pain, presum-
usually accompanies presentations of suicide ably the more urgent the motivation to escape it.
theory (Gunn, 2019). An additional ‘brain’ input is set out in (d) below.
b. Input variable would be the unidimen-
sional aversiveness of pain, regardless
of the pain’s source or quality. Because the
PREDICTED FEATURES OF ‘KEEPER’ pain-and-brain model views suicide as an adjus-
LAST-LINE ANTISUICIDE DEFENSES tive response to the generic aversiveness of pain,
irrespective of its origin, keepers will respond to
the degree, not kind, of triggering pain. The point
This section focuses on Soper’s (2018) task recalls Shneidman’s (1993) catch-all notion of
analysis for keepers – the hypothesized last suicidogenic psychache; that any blend of shame,
line of defenses, which react to exigent risk. grief, anomy, thwarted belongingness, burden-
These ideas are novel, and not mainstream in someness, defeat, hopelessness, or whatever other
either suicidology or evolutionary psychol- emotional distress can motivate suicidal escape.
ogy, but we suggest they warrant attention Soper (2018) posits that, likewise, any combina-
for three reasons. First, their claim to logic – tion can trigger an antisuicide defense. Social pain
keepers’ design features being supposedly is more likely to activate keepers than is physi-
deducible a priori from the nature of suicide ological pain only inasmuch as social pain is often
experienced as more painful (Gunn, 2017).
as an adaptive problem – is central to the
c. Keeper responses would be calibrated
theory and calls for scrutiny. Second, these so that the intensity of defensive out-
features may be read as predictions, amena- puts accords with the intensity and
ble to empirical confirmation or falsification. chronicity of the pain input. The strength of
Third, if correct, they may have important antisuicide responses would be expected to adjust
repercussions for suicide prevention, psychi- according to the scale of the threat: more intense
atry, and wider mental health policy, as we and longer-lasting distress should associate with
will discuss. commensurately more robust countermeasures.
Keepers’ posited features are presented in
20 items, (a) to (t) below, the headings taken
from a tabulation in Soper (2018: 145). ‘Brain’ Input
d. Keepers would not activate earlier than
the species-typical age of first onset of
‘Pain’ Input suicide, in early adolescence, possibly
signaled by the onset of puberty. As
With suicide modelled as an answer to the
already noted, activation would presumably need
imperative to escape pain, Soper argues that a threshold ‘brain’ condition to be met, linked
the emotional aversiveness of pain would to developmental surpassing of the cognitive
probably serve as the primary activator of floor for suicide. Because young children lack the
emergency antisuicide defenses. Keepers mental capability for suicide, there would be no
would mobilize selectively among people in fitness benefit in keepers mobilizing among them
potentially suicidogenic distress. however distressed they may be. Soper (2018)
70 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

stops short of specifying how this ‘brain’ cue trauma. They could also manage pain exoge-
operates; but he posits that a biochemical signal nously: by, say, ingestion of analgesics – exploiting
connected with pubescence is probably involved ‘nature’s pharmacy’ as animals do (Engel, 2002);
because, empirically, it is usually at or shortly after by exploiting the phenomenon of pain-offset relief,
this stage of life that suicide becomes enactable. in which one pain can be relieved by applying
Interesting predictions may follow from this devel- another, unrelated, stimulus; and/or by distrac-
opmental association, as we will discuss later. tion (Eccleston, 2001). Other pain-type keepers
are posited to use psychological adjustments to
make pain bearable, such as the construction of a
Deactivation rationale for suffering pain and/or an emotionally
e. Keepers would demobilize spontane- tolerable, but partly imaginary, re-perception of
ously following a reduction in the origi- the environment (Eccleston, 2001).
nating pain input. Mirroring activation, keepers
The other category, brain-type keepers, deny
should deactivate after, and only after, relief of the
potentially suicidogenic pain that activated them. the means and opportunity for suicide. They
f. Deactivation would usually be slow, will attenuate the individual’s ability to plan
gradual and delayed, especially with- and implement suicide (and, incidentally, any
out an unambiguous ‘all clear’ signal. other complex task) by tactically disabling
Antisuicide defenses would work on a ‘better psychomotor and executive resources.
safe than sorry’ principle, deactivating slowly for Activation would expectably express inleth-
the same reason that prey mammals, on high argy and/or targeted cognitive deficits, such
alert after scenting a predator, stand down only as indecisiveness and forgetfulness.
cautiously once the scent disperses: the extreme
potential cost of misreading an ambiguous ‘all
clear’ outweighs the cost of remaining too long on General Characteristics of Keeper
guard (Blanchard, Griebel, and Nutt, 2011). Responses
h. Keepers would drive compulsive and
Specific Types of Keeper involuntary behaviors, resisting con-
Responses scious awareness and intervention. Suicide
being the kind of lethal, once-in-a-lifetime, fitness
g. Responses would aim to limit motiva- threat that offers few opportunities for conditioned
tion for suicide (‘pain-type’); or limit learning and where to err can be fatal, it ought to be
the capacity to organize suicide (‘brain- addressed by preset and strongly obligate routines.
type’). It was noted earlier that keepers are Keepers would present limited scope for being
predicted to block suicide by deploying ‘pain’ voluntarily moderated. Indeed, the phenomenon of
and ‘brain’ strategies, a dual process deduc- instinct blindness prevailing (Cosmides and Tooby,
ible, Soper (2018) argues, from suicide’s posited 1994), they may operate beyond awareness.
pain-and-brain evolutionary causation. He offers i. Multiple forms of keepers are likely
suggestions as to what these interventions may to operate in an integrated fashion in
entail, listed as bullet points in the ‘keeper’ boxes the same individual, concurrently and/
towards the bottom of Figure 3.2. Ellipses indi- or temporally. Blends of pain- and brain-type
cate that the lists may not be exhaustive. defenses would likely deploy in combinations in
  The first category, pain-type keepers, are tasked the interest of system robustness and to spread
with lessening the aversiveness of emotional costs. In principle, keepers could appear in many
pain. Given the neurological overlap between possible permutations, varying from person to
physical and emotional pain (Eisenberger and person and over time, responding to the varying
Lieberman, 2005), Soper (2018) hazards that needs of culture, gender, life stage, personality,
keepers would probably co-opt pre-existing cir- and other aspects of individual difference.
cuitry that moderates the felt intensity of physi- j. Keepers are likely to be accompanied by
cal pain (Wall, 1999). Pain-type keepers might, protracted anxiousness, and rumination
for example, numb emotions autonomically in focused on emotional pain. As a point related
a manner akin to the analgesic effect of physical to (f) above, following the general pattern by which
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND SUICIDOLOGY 71

animals respond to severe but uncertain fitness were flagged as such by an emotional regula-
threats (Blanchard, Griebel and Nutt, 2011), humans tory variable (Tooby and Cosmides, 2008). Soper
at risk of suicide would expectably display prototypi- (2018) hypothesizes the existence of separate
cal anxiety. Recalling the analogy of prey mammals defenses that deter suicide plans by using the
scenting an unlocatable predator, their best strategy disgust mechanism, informed by culturally learned
is not flight, because they would as likely head inputs (so-called brain-type fenders, discussed
towards death as away from it. For humans vulner- above). But he suggests that there may be no pre-
able to suicide, lethal danger similarly comes from no programmed biological tag marked ‘Suicide Plan’
discernible direction and there is likewise nowhere to to which keepers could respond. Commonplace
run. The priority in the presence of such an extreme, experience is that one can muse about suicide
ambiguous hazard is to stay on high alert and try without triggering a drastic autonomic response.
to infer its detail ‘beyond the information given’ On this basis, keepers may not be good at differen-
(Waldmann et al., 2006). By this light, a compulsive tiating, from among emotionally distressed people,
mental rumination, which may be a uniquely human those who have specific suicide plans from those
addition to what is otherwise a pan-­ animalian who don’t. Keepers would likely mobilize in some
anxiety state (Blanchard, Griebel, Pobbe et al., 2011), individuals who, although in pain, may never have
would be an expectable response. entertained serious thoughts of taking their own
lives. Keepers erring on the side of caution ((f),
above), there may be many such false alarms.
m. Keepers may themselves become patho-
Goals and Trade-Off
logical. In the same way that the physiologi-
Considerations cal immune system can malfunction, there may
k. The compromise objective would be to be exceptional situations in which keepers may
minimize the risk of suicide, while limit- become pathological. Soper posits the possibility,
ing the imposition of new, potentially for example, that antisocial behavioral outputs of
drastic, fitness costs arising from acti- keepers, mobilized in response to social pain, may
vated keepers. Keepers are necessarily costly invite more social pain, potentially creating a posi-
to activate because they involve attenuating tive feedback loop.
the advantageous-on-average ‘pain’ and ‘brain’
primary adaptations that brought suicide in their
wake. Keepers have evidently not delivered zero Manifestations of Successful
suicide risk, and would not be expected to do Operation
so, because zero suicidality would presumably
be achievable only by commensurately zeroing n. Keepers would result in a low, but above-
the fitness benefit of those adaptations. Rather, zero, incidence of suicide in human popu-
antisuicide defenses would evolve only up to lations. It follows from keepers’ compromise
an equilibrium; a point where the marginal fit- objective ((k), above) that their success would
ness gain to be had from further reducing actu- appear at a population level not as zero suicides
arial risk matches the cost of the extra defenses but as a minimal, biologically irreducible, residue.
required to achieve that reduced risk. Pockets of high, even demographically destabiliz-
l. Keepers would trigger sensitively, with ing, rates of suicide might arise, but these would be
a high incidence of false alarms – many short-lived and in due course supplanted, through
affected individuals will not have con- multi-level selection, by groups with suicidality held
sidered suicide. As with the posited ‘brain’ at sustainable levels.5
cue, Soper does not specify the ‘pain’ input that o. Residual suicides would be intrinsi-
is hypothesized to mobilize keepers. Candidate cally unpredictable at the level of
triggers would presumably include biochemical the organism. Functioning optimally as
correlates of pain. But they would probably not part of a wider system of antisuicide defenses,
include, despite its appealing specificity, conscious keepers would be expected to exploit and
planning of suicide projects. The informational exhaust any and all useful markers of actu-
currency of the brain being emotional rather than arial risk. The few suicides that do occur, it
semantic, the organism’s central nervous system follows, can be viewed as statistical residuals –
would be oblivious to suicide plans unless they suicidal trajectories that are amenable neither
72 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

to detection (not, that is, based on informational views suicide as a dysgenic, but ­psychologically
priors available to the organism) nor, therefore, rational, response to aversive affect, activated keep-
to autonomic prevention. With keepers operating ers can be understood as tactical denials of ration-
properly, suicides should be ‘predictably unpre- ality – they sacrifice aspects of the organism’s
dictable’ (Soper, 2019a). affective (‘pain’) and cognitive (‘brain’) functioning
p. Activated keepers would be associ- in order to preserve life. People under the influ-
ated with suicide, but only inasmuch ence of activated keepers will find themselves not
as they associate with suicidal idea- only distressed, but also emotionally (‘pain’) and
tion rather than with the enacting of intellectually (‘brain’) debilitated. Faced with this
those ideas. Most suicides would expectably inconvenience, sufferers and their families would be
be accompanied by activated keepers. Interesting expected to approach priests, healers, and the like
predictions arise from (k) and (l) above concern- for explanations and remedial interventions. It can
ing statistical associations between suicidal- be expected that keepers’ expressions are conspicu-
ity and activated keepers. Completed suicides ous and probably well known to science, and we
will likely be accompanied by signs of keepers may not need to look far to find evidence of them.
having activated – properly, although in these
instances ineffectively. Keepers should also corre-
late strongly with suicide plans and actions. But, Species-Specific and Species-
importantly, these correlations should hold only Universal
insofar as keepers correlate with the emotional
prior – a generalized urge to terminate pain. By s. Keepers would be species-specific: they
analogy, one could imagine a warring city that would not occur in nonhuman animals,
defended its centre against aerial bombardment although homologue of their features
with less-than-perfectly effective anti-aircraft may be found in other mammals. Keepers’
guns deployed at the city’s outskirts. Gunners style of response contrasts with the way animals
open fire pre-emptively; they start shooting as normally meet severe fitness hazards: just when
soon as bombers come within range. Hence, any other animal would be expected to bring all
gunfire would correlate strongly with bombing its mental resources to bear on tackling a mortal
of the centre, but only inasmuch as the outbreak threat, humans facing the threat of suicide are
of gunfire correlates with bombers approaching likely to find their affective and cognitive faculties
the outskirts – not especially with any surviving autonomically impaired. Keepers are a uniquely
bombers’ completion of their raids, progressions human solution to a uniquely human problem.
which the gunners are trying to avert. Likewise, Rudimentary precursors of their features may well
keepers will respond only weakly, if at all, to the be found in nonhumans, particularly among other
pursuit of specific suicide projects among people primates, but these will be only vestiges of phyloge-
motivated to end their suffering, this being the netic raw material that was co-opted and adapted
progression that keepers are designed to prevent. for antisuicide purpose in our species alone.
q. Keeper responses would be nearly t. Keepers would be species-universal:
always recoverable and survivable: they the same integrated system of keepers
should only rarely cause permanent dis- would be found activating among popu-
ability. Almost any outcome short of death being lations of mature humans in all cultures
preferable to suicide, keepers’ interventions may be and historical ages. Keepers are genetically
extreme. Nonetheless, as immune-like responses, transmitted, species-typical mechanisms. Although
they should usually not be degenerative. A protec- the proximal cause of precipitating social pain will
tive denial of psychomotor energy, for example, likely vary according to cultural context, the same
would expectably produce tactical lethargy, perhaps suite of antisuicide responses to that pain will be
for a while even complete immobility, but it should found in all sizeable human populations. That said,
not seriously compromise somatic functions. as universal human features, keepers’ antisuicide
r. Keeper responses would be nearly functionality may not be obvious because no
always recoverable and survivable: they extant control population may be to hand to show
should only rarely cause permanent dis- what the suicide rate would be, all else equal, were
ability. Because the pain-and-brain framework it not for their interventions.
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND SUICIDOLOGY 73

PSYCHIATRIC SYMPTOMS AS We note four interesting ways to inspect


ANTISUICIDE DEFENSES the hypothesis. First, it offers a novel concep-
tion of ‘functional’ psychiatric diagnoses, as
The above section outlined an engineering the concurrence of potentially suicidogenic
specification – a score of features that would be pain alongside blends of antisuicide responses
expected of so-called keepers, reactive devices to that pain. Soper (2018) offers a cross-­
designed to stop us taking our own lives when tabulation (Figure 3.2) to illustrate how psy-
we otherwise might. The specification follows, chiatric labels may describe various groupings
Soper (2018) argues, from the pain-and-brain of keepers in this way, with equivalent defen-
model of suicide’s evolution; defensive sys- sive components appearing under different
tems would activate with ‘pain’ and ‘brain’ diagnostic headings. Protective emotional
informational inputs, respond using ‘pain’ and numbing, for example, maps against criteria
‘brain’ processes, and produce observable used for diagnosing ‘major depressive disor-
‘pain’ and ‘brain’ outputs. der’, ‘psychotic disorders’, and ‘bipolar dis-
The predicted features of this system show order’. According to this conception, keepers,
striking similarities, according to Soper, with not diagnoses, are natural kinds.
adult patterns of so-called ‘functional’ (i.e., Second, the hypothesis may offer parsimo-
not due to structural brain dysfunction, as nious explanation for a suite of statistical links
opposed to ‘organic’) common mental disor- between suicidality and CMD which, without
der, or CMD (Goldberg and Goodyer, 2005) – the hypothesis, look unrelated and puzzling. We
sundry states that would usually be described note three. One is that almost all CMD diagno-
as depression, generalized anxiety, alcoholism ses associate with a heightened vulnerability to
and other addictions, psychoses, obsessive/ suicide (Cavanagh et al., 2003) – but, signally,
compulsive disorders, non-suicidal self-harm, the diagnosis of severe to profound intellectual
and perhaps other diagnoses. He asserts that disability associates with fewer suicides (Harris
the fit is so detailed and multi-faceted that it and Barraclough, 1997) as the pain-and-brain
is difficult plausibly to explain, except by the model would anticipate. Two is that, as pre-
proposal that CMD is, in fact, the expression dicted, CMD correlates with suicide only inas-
of antisuicide machinery. The hypothesis chal- much as it correlates with the ideational prior.
lenges a widespread preconception that CMD CMD associates strongly with generalized
causes suicide, rather than works to block sui- suicidal ideas; but only weakly, if at all, with
cidal trajectories among those at risk (Mishara the progression from this ideation to the plan-
and Chagnon, 2016). To ascribe suicide to ning and execution of specific suicide actions
CMD may be to commit the post hoc fallacy: (Klonsky et al., 2016; Nock et al., 2015). Three
to assume that because a preceded b, a must is that the developmental stage when CMD
have caused b. Recalling the analogy of air tends first to appear – in the teen years (Kessler
defences, ill-informed citizens might under- et al., 2007) – coincides with earliest first onsets
standably misplace blame for the bombing of suicidality (Nock et al., 2013). It is not easy
of their city on the anti-aircraft gunfire that otherwise to account for these correlations
usually foreshadowed it. The challenge is not individually let alone as a set. Together they
new: it has long been suggested within psy- could be taken as a fingerprint of antisuicide
chiatry that depression, addictions, psychotic functionality.
delusions, and diverse other psychopatholo- Third, the existence of marked patterns
gies can protect against self-killing (e.g., common to sundry diagnostic labels point
Hendin, 1975; Himmelhoch, 1988; Hundert, to a shared aetiology – specifically, it is sug-
1992; Menninger, 1938). What is new is evo- gested, as mechanisms designed to prevent
lutionary theory that predicts such a dynamic. potentially suicidogenic pain and suicidal ideas
74

Suggested types of Diagnostic criteria of some common psychopathologies (A.P.A., 2013)


keeper anti-suicide
mechanism
Major Depressive Disorder Substance use dis- Generalised Non-suicidal Psychotic disorders OCD Bipolar
orders Anxiety self-injury disorders
Disorder(GAD)a (NSSI)b

PAIN-TYPE (weaken the • Feeling ‘empty’; ‘Having • Compulsive use • Worry, • Self-injury • Diminished • Obsessions; • Depressive
motivation for suicide) no feelings’; ‘Not caring of analgesics rumination (iii, iv) emotional compulsions episode (i)
(i) Autonomic numbing. any more’ (i) and other restlessness; expression (i) (iv, v, vii) • Manic/
• Craving foods (iv) mind-altering insomnia (vi, vii) • Delusions; hypomanic
(ii) Medicate the pain.
substances hallucinations episodes (iv, vii)
(iii) Pain offset relief. (ii, iv, v, vi, vii) (iv, v, vi, vii)
(iv) Distract from pain.
(v) Detach from pain.
(vi) Make sense of the pain.
(vii) Find reason to live with pain.
BRAIN-TYPE (restrict access to • Indecisiveness; • Compulsive use • Difficulty • Disorganised • Obsessions • Depressive
the means of suicide) diminished ability to of sedatives concentrating; thinking; (viii); episode (viii, ix)
(viii) Degrade ability to plan and think or concentrate (viii) (viii, ix) mind going Disorganised/ • Compulsions
enact tasks. • Loss of interest in blank (viii) abnormal motor (ix)
(ix) Loss of psychomotor energy. activities; fatigue, loss • Fatigue (ix) behaviour (viii)
of energy, hypersomnia; • Negative
psychomotor retardation; symptoms;
loss of appetite (ix) catatonia (ix)

Figure 3.2  A tentative mapping of hypothesized types of antisuicide mechanisms (keepers) across common diagnostic categories of mental
THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

disorder
Notes: a The anxiousness of generalised anxiety disorder may be better understood as a concomitant of keepers rather than a keeper defence in itself, but the pain-type function of GAD
posited here might be taken to express the urgency of the organism’s need to seek relief from suicidogenic pain; bNon-suicidal self-injury (NSSI), classified as a ‘condition for further study’
in DSM-5 (APA, 2013), appears here as an exception, arguably lacking a ‘brain-type’ action: this function may be provided instead by other symptoms often comorbid with NSSI.
Source: Reproduced from Soper (2018).
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND SUICIDOLOGY 75

progressing to suicidal actions. One commonal- differences, vary more in degree than kind and
ity, mentioned above, is marked suicidality – to likely relate to a singular causal process, hith-
be exact, suicidal ideas rather than the enact- erto unclear (Caspi et  al., 2014; Menninger,
ing of those ideas (Nock et al., 2013). Another 1963; Stochl et al., 2015).
is the characteristic stimulus-response course Fourth, it is possible to understand why psy-
of CMD, precipitated by emotionally painful chiatric symptoms are poor predictors of sui-
events, and typically lifting over time (with cide, and indeed why a decades-long search
or without medical intervention) after the pre- for any clinically useful marker has returned
cipitating adversities are eased (Brown, 2009; empty handed (Carter et  al., 2017; Franklin
Goldberg and Goodyer, 2005; Harris, 2000). Yet et al., 2017). The failure of prediction arises
others include: extreme comorbidity – people not particularly, as is often claimed, because
diagnosed with one disorder usually also meet suicide is rare; rare events are not necessar-
criteria for one or more others, concurrently ily unpredictable (Soper, 2019a). Rather, pre-
or at different times (American Psychiatric cisely this null result would be expected of an
Association, 2013; First and Pincus, 2009); organism that is finely adapted to its ‘suicidal
lack of ‘zones of rarity’, or natural bounda- niche’ and already making best use of avail-
ries, between diagnostic criteria (Kendell and able cues (Soper, 2019b). Suicide attempts,
Jablensky, 2003); non-specificity of causation – as statistical residuals, are events associated
to large extent, as the keeper model anticipates, with no utilizable prognostic information
any kind of adversity can precipitate any vari- to which the individual’s defensive systems
ety of CMD response (Goldberg and Goodyer, could have responded. It should be antici-
2005; Kessler et al., 2010); non-specificity of pated, then, that suicides offer little or no
treatments – therapies developed for one condi- scope for being accurately foreseen at the
tion also alleviate others (Wampold and Imel, individual level.
2015); common cognitive deficits – selectively Summing up, a posited convergence of
impairing, also as the keeper specification evidence from multiple directions sets up
predicts, the ability to organize complex tasks a probability argument for special design
(Harvey, 2004); coincident scheduling of first (Williams, 1966, 1996). Soper (2018) finds
onsets – in adolescence, as already noted, the no unaccountable inconsistencies, and no
stage of life when suicidality first develops better explanation for the confluence. A soft-
(Kessler et al., 2007); species-specificity – an ware patch devised to stop humans switch-
absence of close animal models (Willner and ing themselves off in the face of difficulties
Belzung, 2015); and so on. This mesh of con- would predictably include routines that look
tinuities poses a problem for an ad hoc label- very much like symptoms of ‘functional’
by-label style of analysis that is widespread in common mental disorder. On this basis, he
Darwinian psychiatry (e.g., Brune, 2016; Del intuits, it is more likely than not that many
Giudice, 2018) – some evolutionary hypotheses outwardly dissimilar psychiatric states reflect
advanced for depression, others for generalized the proper workings of systems that evolved
anxiety, yet others for schizophrenia etc. Soper to prevent self-killing.
(2018) argues that any explanation proffered
for one diagnostic label is incomplete unless
also explained is the detailed configuration Implications: The Case of
that label shares, and its routine co-occurrence, Depression
with almost any other form of CMD. Soper’s
model may offer parsimonious theoretical If the pain-and-brain framework is correct,
underpinning for what some researchers have its ramifications may be wide-ranging, pro-
inferred from empirical observation: that many found, and take time to fathom. They could
psychiatric constructs, for all their superficial impact on the research agenda, suicide
76 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

prevention policy, psychiatric practice, and the Addressing the root cause of depressive
conceptualization of psychopathology and symptoms would presumably involve helping
mental health. We will illustrate with an exam- sufferers to relieve the precipitating distress,
ple relating to the extensive and widely recom- which probably originates, recalling Gunn’s
mended use of antidepressants and other (2017) Social Pain Model, from some form
psychopharmacological treatments to prevent of social detachment. That said, any pain
suicides among clinical patients assessed to be relief could offer provisional respite; and
at risk (Wasserman et al., 2012; Zalsman et al., given the likely social nature of the triggering
2017). The task analysis outlined above pain, any credible treatment, psychotropic
suggests that certain common psychiatric medication included, could be expected to
symptoms – including the emotional numbing, produce a strong placebo effect, signaling
cognitive impairment, fatigue, and generalized that a supportive attachment has been put in
anxiety characteristic of adult-pattern depres- place (Davies, 2013; Wampold, 2018).
sive states – can be understood not as suicido- Second, at worst, suppressing the psyche’s
genic disorder, but expressions of the antisuicide systems exogenously, at least
organism’s protective response to the lethal without other measures, would be expected
threat latent in chronic, intense, emotional to exacerbate, not lessen, suicide risk, at least
pain. In other words, various common depres- in some circumstances. This expectation is
sive symptoms may be conceived as actions of supported by considerable pharmacologi-
a psychological defensive system, in the same cal evidence (Hengartner and Plöderl, 2019;
way that modern medicine understands cough- Sharma et al., 2016).
ing, vomiting, and fever to be defensive Third, evolutionary analysis challenges a
responses (Nesse and Williams, 1995). It is for premise of such treatments, that people ‘at
this reason that the analysis predicts that, once risk’ can accurately be picked out in clinical
the triggering distress is relieved, depressive settings. As we have noted, special-purpose
symptoms would be expected to lift of biological defenses would be expected to
their own accord, with or without medical have exploited and exhausted any and all uti-
intervention – spontaneous remission that lizable markers, with the result that suicides
characterizes the normal course of most are probably not amenable to prediction in
common psychopathologies (Goldberg and principle (Soper, 2019a). The evolutionary
Goodyer, 2005; Harris, 2000). The notion of argument thus touches on an ethical ques-
protective antisuicide depression is not new. tion: whether it is justifiable to prescribe
Hendin (1975) inferred such a dynamic from mind-altering drugs to large numbers of
clinical observation half a century ago; and people, nearly all of whom were never going
some epidemiological findings might, arguably, to take their own lives, in an effort to pro-
support Hendin’s case (Rogers et  al., 2018). tect a small and unidentifiable minority. The
Psychiatrists across decades have warned that analysis would, in the round, appear to lend
suicide risk can intensify, not reduce, when theoretical support to those who already
depressive symptoms start to lift (Kahne, question on empirical grounds whether
1966; Meehl, 1973). What is new is an evolu- psychopharmacology is an appropriate
tionary foundation for the idea. Hence, evolu- approach to suicide prevention (Hjelmeland
tionary analysis pertains to important questions et al., 2019; Maris, 2015).
about the treatment of depression, in particular Looking at alternative strategies, the same
the use of pharmacological treatments, espe- evolutionary analysis predicts that restrict-
cially in the context of suicide prevention. We ing access to lethal means (such as, for
will highlight three issues. example, installing nets under favored jump
First, the model suggests that antidepres- sites, and limiting the size of retail drug
sants treat symptoms rather than causes. packs) may be effective to an extent that
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND SUICIDOLOGY 77

would seem far-fetched to people viewing delayed/accelerated intellectual development


such incomplete obstacles with the benefit of against populations with delayed/accelerated
normal intellectual functioning (Chen et  al., endocrine development. A pattern in which
2016; Hawton, 2007). Interventions that only suicide, but not psychiatric symptoms,
mildly complicate a suicide endeavor would occurred among the hormonally delayed but
be expected to disrupt disproportionately the cognitively precocious, and vice versa, could
plans of someone affected by denials of psy- be taken to support Soper’s proposals and be
chomotor energy, memory, decision-making, difficult otherwise to explain.
and other task-related cognitive faculties – On the other hand, Soper’s theory faces at
attenuations which, Soper (2016, 2018) pos- least four significant problems. First, being
its, may be protective aspects of depression. A at the stage of a preliminary sketch, it lacks
slight delay may be enough to allow homeo- detail and raises many unanswered questions.
static affective systems to de-escalate from a It is not specified, for example, how exactly
suicidal crisis, restoring the organism towards hypothesized antisuicide protections would
a protective, above-neutral, resting point. operate. To illustrate, ‘loss of psychomotor
Means restriction, in other words, could be energy’ (LoPE) (Soper, 2018: 143) is con-
understood to capitalize on and co-operate ceived as an autonomic defense against sui-
with the organism’s own antisuicide defenses. cide, but self-killing could be brought about
by inaction as well as action. It is unclear
how LoPE would interact with other hypoth-
esized keepers; it could conflict, for example,
Problems with Soper’s Theory
with others that presumably require positive
On one hand, Soper’s (2018) model has action, such as the compulsive consumption
merit. It appears, at least at this early stage, of analgesics. The biological parameters of
parsimoniously to fit multiple lines of evi- LoPE are undefined – whether it exists as a
dence, and it does so arguably better than discrete condition, and how it would be oper-
other available explanations; it could be ationalized as a testable construct, and so on.
judged attractive on the criterion of inference Soper’s proposals may have intuitive appeal,
to the best explanation (Harman, 1965). From but there is much work to do before a robust,
an evolutionary perspective, it appears to comprehensive framework could be said
match biological function to observable form to have been achieved, despite, or perhaps
(Buss, 1995; Williams, 1996). The theory is because of, the breadth of the framework’s
in principle falsifiable: many findings would ambit.
suffice as disproof – if science discovered, for Second, the forward/reverse engineering
example, nonhuman suicide, or a human approach, the appeal to evidence of special
population with a pattern of early childhood design on which Soper’s adaptationist argu-
suicides, or a group that lacked positive cor- ments largely rely, is intrinsically intuitive
relations between suicidality and hypothe- notwithstanding apparent objectivity (Lauder,
sized keeper responses, and so forth. It could 1996; Williams, 1996). Due to the method’s
generate novel and testable ancillary predic- subjectivity, another researcher replicating
tions. We propose, for example, that Soper’s Soper’s thought experiments might not arrive
notion of a statistical, but only indirect, asso- at the same conclusions. This is not to say
ciation between the scheduling of first onsets that his arguments are invalid, but that there
of keepers (triggered by a hormonal cue is scope for different students legitimately to
linked to puberty) and first onsets of suicide reach different opinions from the same data
(dependent on intellectual competence) could depending on their biographical backgrounds
be tested by comparing the epidemiology of (Kuhn, 1977). Soper’s efforts to infer a priori
suicide and keepers in populations with design parameters for keepers, for example,
78 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

will not be entirely a priori: the author, a as special-purpose antisuicide devices. But to
psychotherapist, could not have approached reject this hypothesis may be to invite fresh dif-
the topic with a blank slate and it would be ficulty, because two questions would then call
surprising if his findings were not colored by for answers. Which aspects of the antisuicide
experience. Indeed, some of his predictions task analysis are being disputed? And, if not
might strike many workers in mental health ‘functional’ psychiatric disorder, then what
as already matters of common knowledge. alternative empirical phenomenon is proposed
Third, Soper’s (2018) approach leans on that better matches that design specification? If
a presumption of optimal outcomes: that the pain-and-brain model is broadly correct, the
because x would be biologically ideal, x is call would remain to explain why few people
what we should find. While the optimization kill themselves. It may be partly for this reason
approach is a useful way to model biological that at least one prominent suicidologist is on
problems and may suggest solutions, it is not record as finding Soper’s proposals persuasive
a safe basis in itself for predicting evolutionary (Lester, 2019). Time will tell if others agree.
results. Many alternative outcomes of selection
are possible (Gould and Lewontin, 1979), and
the fitness functions of most adaptive problems
are complex landscapes of hills and valleys, the CONCLUSION
path to optimization often blocked at local fit-
ness maxima (Parker and Smith, 1990). At one level this chapter carries an encourag-
Fourth, testing certain aspects of Soper’s ing message. The evolutionary approach
(2018) theory may be problematic due to the would seem, in principle, capable of bringing
composite nature of its hypotheses. Taking unity and coherence to suicidology’s current
again the example of ‘loss of psychomo- morass of theory. A ‘pain-and-brain’ frame-
tor energy’ (LoPE), this is presented as both work in particular seems to offer a rallying
an adaptive solution to suicide risk, and as point for numerous, superficially disparate,
an expression of that solution. Associations theoretical positions, such as IPTS (Van
between LoPE and measures of suicidality that Orden et  al., 2010), IMV (O’Connor et  al.,
offer to support one hypothesis could simulta- 2016), or SPM (Gunn, 2017), theories which
neously undermine the other: if, say, suicides essentially characterize suicide as a way to
happen alongside LoPE, then that could be read escape intolerable emotional stress. None
both as confirmatory evidence (because LoPE would appear incompatible with the view that
had mobilized as predicted to meet the threat), pain, as a biological imperative, motivates
and as falsifying evidence (because LoPE had action to end or escape it, while regular adult
failed in its supposed design task, to stop sui- human cognition offers intentional self-killing
cides). This kind of epistemological trap is as an effective, but genetically destructive,
not unique to Soper’s theorizing and may be means to answer that demand.
endemic in evolutionary science. A comparable But the corollary, that suicide poses an engi-
zero-sum game plays out, for example, in the neering problem, as summed by the tweet that
long-debated Westermarck effect, an evolved began this chapter, may be harder to digest.
mechanism said to deter close inbreeding: the Presumably human beings are prevented
same evidence can be taken both to support and most of the time from switching themselves
contradict the theory (Sesardic, 2005). off because of one or more special-purpose
On balance, while acknowledging these software patches. Blind to our own instincts
and other weaknesses, it is not easy to dis- (Cosmides and Tooby, 1994), we may be
miss the thrust of Soper’s (2018) arguments. oblivious to their functioning, and skepti-
Intuitively difficult is the claim that varied cal they may even exist. A research pro-
symptoms of psychiatric disorders function gram to uncover their workings may not be
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND SUICIDOLOGY 79

easy to formulate. The idea that humans are existing theoretical and empirical content.
equipped with organismic defenses against There may be little to lose in such an incre-
suicide is not new (e.g., Himmelhoch, 1988; mental move. The upsides, on the other hand,
Hundert, 1992; Miller, 2008), but it has only may be great. Evolutionary psychology offers
recently gained prominence in the research fresh perspectives for suicidology, and ready
agenda (Culotta, 2019). Its implications tools that, if used, could be decisive in a bat-
may run wide and deep, and call some long- tle to save lives. Evolutionary psychology and
held preconceptions into question: as Lester suicidology deserve each other’s attention.
(2019) finds, accepting the ramifications may
require close reading of the arguments.
Progress is not helped by what Soper (2018) Notes
believes to be a systematic non-interaction
between suicidology and evolutionary psychol- 1  Reviews of prominent offers can be found in the
general suicidology literature (e.g., Gunn, 2019;
ogy, a two-way blockage of ideas that may go Gunn and Lester, 2014; O’Connor and Portzky,
beyond the institutional disconnects seen else- 2018; Paniagua et al., 2010; Selby et al., 2014).
where in psychological sciences (Staats, 2004). 2  If it were claimed that nonhuman suicide is rare
Soper posits that suicide and evolution, each for thanks to the action of antisuicide adaptations –
different reasons, are domains that many peo- something like the ‘patch 7.822’ software update
imagined in this chapter’s opening quotation –
ple find awkward to think about, researchers then those countermeasures need to be identi-
included. It may be for this reason that, in one fied, as indeed we will later suggest they probably
direction, evolutionary psychology has largely need to be identified in humans.
ignored suicide, as indeed has psychology gen- 3  As a side point: some theorists argue that risk of
erally (Rogers, 2001): in view of the gravity suicide may vary, albeit weakly, with a heritable
propensity for certain personality traits, notably
and ubiquity of suicide as a human phenom- impulsivity (McGirr et al., 2008).
enon, and the evolutionary puzzle it presents, 4  Suicide would presumably have been no easier
remarkably little has been written on the subject in our evolutionary environment. As ground-
from an evolutionary perspective, at least until dwellers on open grasslands, it may have been
recent years. In the other direction, suicidology a scarcity of ready opportunities for self-killing
that allowed humans to encephalize closer to the
has largely ignored evolutionary psychology. cognitive floor for suicide than would be feasible
It may be illustrative that a rare review of the for other social animals. Other social animals that
field, titled ‘Evolutionary processes in suicide’ otherwise would be expected to benefit from
(Chiurliza et al., 2017), attempts to appraise its increased social intelligence (Varki and Brower,
research group’s ideas (and, oddly, only that 2013) may occupy habitats where suicide could
be summarily enacted at almost any time by
group’s) without reference to evolutionary psy- default – by not gripping (chimp), or not surfac-
chology’s primary texts or tenets – a surprising ing (dolphin), for example (Soper, 2019b).
omission given that evolutionary psychology, 5  As a related point, Humphrey (2018) speculates
‘the study of behavior from an evolutionary that suicidality may help to account for cata-
perspective’ (Cornwell et  al., 2005: 369), is strophic demographic collapses thought to have
occurred in early human pre-history.
centrally relevant.
This chapter calls for consilience between
the two fields. An evolutionary stance would
not in itself be a departure for suicidology: REFERENCES
it would, rather, follow the lead set by Freud
(1920/1991), Shneidman (1985), Joiner Aiello, L. C., & Wheeler, P. (1995). The
(2005), and other prominent researchers, expensive-tissue hypothesis: The brain and
drawing on evolutionary ideas across more the digestive system in human and primate
than a century. Evolutionary psychology could evolution. Current Anthropology, 36(2),
synthesize, not replace, much of suicidology’s 199–221.
80 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Akotia, C. S., Knizek, B. L., Kinyanda, E., & Baumeister, R. F. (1990). Suicide as escape from
Hjelmeland, H. (2014). ‘I have sinned’: self. Psychological Review, 97(1), 90–113.
Understanding the role of religion in the Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need
experiences of suicide attempters in Ghana. to belong: Desire for interpersonal attach-
Mental Health, Religion & Culture, 17(5), ments as a fundamental human motivation.
437–448. Psychological Bulletin, 117(3), 497.
Alexander, R. D. (1974). The evolution of social Baumeister, R. F. (1989). The optimal margin of
behavior. Annual Review of Ecology and illusion. Journal of Social and Clinical
Systematics, 5(1), 325–383. Psychology, 8(2), 176–189.
American Association of Suicidology. (2019). Benatar, D. (2015). Life is not good. In T. K.
History of the American Association of Shackelford & R. D. Hansen (Eds.), The
Suicidology. Retrieved from https:// Evolution Of Morality (pp. 137–140). Cham,
suicidology.org/about-aas/#history (Accessed Switzerland: Springer.
20 May 2020) Bering, J. M. (2018). A Very Human Ending:
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). How Suicide Haunts Our Species. London,
Diagnostic And Statistical Manual Of Mental UK: Transworld.
Disorders: DSM-5. New York, NY: American Bering, J. M., & Shackelford, T. K. (2004). The
Psychiatric Association. causal role of consciousness: A conceptual
Andoh-Arthur, J., Knizek, B. L., Osafo, J., & addendum to human evolutionary psychology.
Hjelmeland, H. (2020). Societal reactions to Review of General Psychology, 8(4), 227–248.
suicide in Ghana: A qualitative study of Bjorklund, D. F., Causey, K., & Periss, V. (2010).
experiences of the bereaved. Crisis, 41(2), The evolution and development of human
128–134. social cognition. In P. M. Kappeler & J. B. Silk
Anil, M., Preston, J., McKinstry, J., Rodway, R., (Eds.), Mind The Gap (pp. 351–371).
& Brown, S. (1996). An assessment of stress Heidelberg, Germany: Springer.
caused in sheep by watching slaughter of Blanchard, D. C., Griebel, G., & Nutt, D. J.
other sheep. Animal Welfare, 5(4), (2011). Handbook Of Anxiety And Fear (Vol.
435–441. 17). Amsterdam, Netherlands: Academic.
Atkinson, J. M. (1978). Discovering Suicide: Blanchard, D. C., Griebel, G., Pobbe, R., &
Studies in the Social Organization of Sudden Blanchard, R. J. (2011). Risk assessment as
Death. London, UK: McMillan. an evolved threat detection and analysis
Aubin, H.-J., Berlin, I., & Kornreich, C. (2013). process. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral
The evolutionary puzzle of suicide. Interna- Reviews, 35(4), 991–998.
tional Journal of Environmental Research & Blasco-Fontecilla, H., Lopez-Castroman, J.,
Public Health, 10(12), 6873–6886. Gomez-Carrillo, A., & Baca-Garcia, E. (2009).
Auvray, M., Myin, E., & Spence, C. (2010). The Towards an evolutionary framework of
sensory-discriminative and affective- suicidal behavior. Medical Hypotheses, 73(6),
motivational aspects of pain. Neuroscience & 1078–1079.
Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(2), 214–223. Bohannan, P. (Ed.) (1960). African Homicide
Baechler, J. (1975/1979). Les Suicides (B. And Suicide. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
Cooper, Trans.). New York, NY: Basic Books. University Press.
Bailey, J. D. (2009). Orphan care: An introduction. Bolton, J. M., Au, W., Leslie, W. D., Martens.,
Social Work & Society, 7(1), 1–12. Patricia J., Enns, M. W., Roos, L L., & Sareen, J.
Bancroft, J., Hawton, K., Simkin, S., Kingston, (2013). Parents bereaved by offspring suicide:
B., Cumming, C., & Whitwell, D. (1979). The A population-based longitudinal case-control
reasons people give for taking overdoses: A study. JAMA Psychiatry, 70(2), 158–167.
further inquiry. British Journal of Medical Bowlby, J. (1969/1997). Attachment And Loss:
Psychology, 52(4), 353–365. Attachment (2nd ed. Vol. 1). London, UK:
Bateson, M., Brilot, B., & Nettle, D. (2011). Pimlico.
Anxiety: an evolutionary approach. The Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment And Loss:
Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 56(12), Separation: Anxiety And Anger (2nd ed.
707–714. Vol. 2). London, UK: Penguin.
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND SUICIDOLOGY 81

Bowlby, J. (1980/1991). Attachment And Loss: Buss, D. M. (1995). Evolutionary psychology: A


Loss: Sadness And Depression (Vol. 3). new paradigm for psychological science.
London, UK: Penguin. Psychological Inquiry, 6(1), 1–30.
Bracke, M. (1992). Can animals have a Buss, D. M., & Penke, L. (2015). Evolutionary
preference not to be killed?: Observations on personality psychology. In M. Mikulincer, P.
red deer, laying hens and mice which were R. Shaver, M. Cooper, Lynne, & R. J. Larsen
exposed to conspecifics being killed: A study (Eds.), APA Handbook Of Personality And
in cognitive ethology (Doctoral dissertation). Social Psychology: Vol. 4. Personality
Edinburgh School of Agriculture, Edinburgh, Processes And Individual Differences (pp. 3–
UK. 39). Washington, DC: American Psychological
Brand, P. W., & Yancey, P. (1993). Pain: The Gift Association.
Nobody Wants. New York, NY: HarperCollins. Campbell, A. (2002). A Mind Of Her Own: The
Brezo, J., Paris, J., Barker, E. D., Tremblay, R., Evolutionary Psychology Of Women. New
Vitaro, F., Zoccolillo, M., & Turecki, G. (2007). York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Natural history of suicidal behaviors in a Carter, G., Milner, A., McGill, K., Pirkis, J.,
population-based sample of young adults. Kapur, N., & Spittal, M. J. (2017). Predicting
Psychological Medicine, 37(11), suicidal behaviours using clinical instruments:
1563–1574. Systematic review and meta-analysis of
Bribiescas, R. G. (2006). On the evolution, life positive predictive values for risk scales. The
history, and proximate mechanisms of British Journal of Psychiatry, 210(6),
human male reproductive senescence. 387–395.
Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, Caspi, A., Houts, R. M., Belsky, D. W., Goldman-
and Reviews, 15(4), 132–141. Mellor, S. J., Harrington, H., Israel, S., &
Brown, D. E. (2004). Human universals, human Moffitt, T. E. (2014). The p factor: One
nature & human culture. Daedalus, 133(4), general psychopathology factor in the
47–54. structure of psychiatric disorders? Clinical
Brown, G. W. (2009). Medical sociology and Psychological Science, 2(2), 119–137.
issues of aetiology. In M. G. Gelder, N. C. Cavanagh, J. T. O., Carson, A. J., Sharpe, M., &
Andreasen, J. J. López-Ibor, & J. R. Geddes Lawrie, S. M. (2003). Psychological autopsy
(Eds.), New Oxford Textbook Of Psychiatry studies of suicide: A systematic review.
(pp. 268–275). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Psychological Medicine, 33(03), 395–405.
Press. Cerel, J., & Aldrich, R. S. (2011). The impact of
Brown, M. F. (1986). Power, gender, and the suicide on children and adolescents. In J. R.
social meaning of Aguaruna suicide. Man, Jordan & J. L. McIntosh (Eds.), Grief After
21(2), 311–328. Suicide: Understanding The Consequences
Brown, R. M., Brown, S. L., Johnson, A., Olsen, And Caring For The Survivors (pp. 81–92).
B., Melver, K., & Sullivan, M. (2009). Empirical New York, NY: Routledge.
support for an evolutionary model of self- Cerel, J., Brown, M. M., Maple, M., Singleton,
destructive motivation. Suicide and Life- M., van de Venne, J., Moore, M., & Flaherty,
Threatening Behavior, 39(1), 1–12. C. (2019). How many people are exposed to
Brown, R. M., Dahlen, E., Mills, C., Rick, J., & suicide? Not six. Suicide and Life-Threatening
Biblarz, A. (1999). Evaluation of an Behavior, 49(2), 529–534.
evolutionary model of self-preservation and Chapple, A., Ziebland, S., & Hawton, K. (2015).
self-destruction. Suicide and Life-Threatening Taboo and the different death? Perceptions of
Behavior, 29(1), 58–71. those bereaved by suicide or other traumatic
Brüne, M. (2016). Textbook of Evolutionary death. Sociology of Health & Illness, 37(4),
Psychiatry and Psychosomatic Medicine: The 610–625.
Origins of Psychopathology (2nd ed.). Chen, Y.-Y., Chien-Chang Wu, K., Wang, Y., &
Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Yip, P. (2016). Suicide prevention through
Buss, D. M. (1990). Toward a biologically restricting access to suicide means and
informed psychology of personality. Journal hotspots. In R. C. O’Connor & J. Pirkis (Eds.),
of Personality, 58(1), 1–16. International Handbook Of Suicide
82 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Prevention (2nd ed., pp. 609–636). Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1994). Beyond
Chichester, UK: John Wiley and Sons. intuition and instinct blindness: Toward an
Chiurliza, B., Rogers, M. L., Schneider, M. E., evolutionarily rigorous cognitive science.
Chu, C., & Joiner, T. E. (2017). Evolutionary Cognition, 50(1), 41–77.
processes in suicide. Current Opinion in Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2000). Evolutionary
Psychology, 22, 84–88. psychology and the emotions. In M. Lewis &
Cholbi, M. (2017). Suicide. The Stanford J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook Of
Encyclopedia of Philosophy. Fall 2017. Emotions (Vol. 2, pp. 91–115). New York,
Retrieved from https://plato.stanford.edu/ NY: Guilford Press.
archives/fall2017/entries/suicide/ (Accessed Culotta, E. (2019). Probing an evolutionary
20 May 2020) riddle. Science, 365(6455), 748–749.
Chow, A. Y. M. (2006). The day after: Daly, M., & Wilson, M. (1988). Homicide. New
Experiences of bereaved suicide survivors. In Brunswick, NJ: Transaction.
C. L. W. C. Chan & A. Y. M. Chow (Eds.), Darwin, C. (1859). On The Origin Of Species
Death, Dying And Bereavement: A Hong Kong By Means Of Natural Selection, Or The
Chinese Experience (Vol. 1, pp. 293–308). Preservation Of Favoured Races In The
Aberdeen, Hong Kong: Hong Kong University Struggle For Life. London, UK: John Murray.
Press. Davies, J. (2013). Cracked: Why Psychiatry Is
Chu, C., Buchman-Schmitt, J. M., Stanley, I. H., Doing More Harm Than Good. London, UK:
Hom, M. A., Tucker, R. P., Hagan, C. R., & Icon.
Ringer, F. B. (2017). The interpersonal theory Dawkins, R. (1976). The Selfish Gene (3rd ed.).
of suicide: A systematic review and meta- Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
analysis of a decade of cross-national research. Dawkins, R. (1980). Domesticity, senescence,
Psychological Bulletin, 143(12), 1313–1345. and suicide. Behavioral and Brain Sciences,
Chung, D., Hadzi-Pavlovic, D., Wang, M., 3(2), 274.
Swaraj, S., Olfson, M., & Large, M. (2019). De Leo, D., Milner, A., Fleischmann, A.,
Meta-analysis of suicide rates in the first Bertolote, J., Collings, S., Amadeo, S., &
week and the first month after psychiatric Saniel, B. (2013). The WHO START Study:
hospitalisation. BMJ open, 9(3), e023883. Suicidal behaviors across different areas of
Cole, L. C. (1954). The population consequences the world. Crisis, 34(3), 156–163.
of life history phenomena. The Quarterly deCatanzaro, D. (1980). Human suicide: A
Review of Biology, 29(2), 103–137. biological perspective. Behavioral and Brain
Comai, S., & Gobbi, G. (2016). Translational Sciences, 3(02), 265–272.
research in suicide: Is it possible to study deCatanzaro, D. (1981). Suicide And Self-
suicide in animal models? In P. Courtet (Ed.), Damaging Behavior: A Sociobiological
Understanding Suicide: From Diagnosis To Perspective. New York, NY: Academic Press.
Personalized Treatment (pp. 177–188). deCatanzaro, D. (1982). Suicide, aggression,
Cham, Switzerland: Springer. and natural selection. Behavioral and Brain
Confer, J. C., Easton, J. A., Fleischman, D. S., Sciences, 5(02), 316–317.
Goetz, C. D., Lewis, D. M., Perilloux, C., & deCatanzaro, D. (1986). A mathematical model
Buss, D. M. (2010). Evolutionary psychology. of evolutionary pressures regulating self-
Controversies, questions, prospects, and preservation and self-destruction. Suicide
limitations. American Psychologist, 65(2), and Life-Threatening Behavior, 16(2),
110–126. 166–181.
Corns, J. (2014). Unpleasantness, motivational deCatanzaro, D. (1991). Evolutionary limits to
oomph, and painfulness. Mind & Language, self-preservation. Ethology and Sociobiology,
29(2), 238–254. 12(1), 13–28.
Cornwell, R. E., Palmer, C., Guinther, P. M., & deCatanzaro, D. (1992). Prediction of self-
Davis, H. P. (2005). Introductory psychology preservation failures on the basis of
texts as a view of sociobiology/evolutionary quantitative evolutionary biology. In R. W.
psychology’s role in psychology. Evolutionary Maris, A. L. Berman, J. T. Maltsberger, & R. I.
Psychology, 3(1), 147470490500300124. Yufit (Eds.), Assessment And Prediction Of
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND SUICIDOLOGY 83

Suicide (pp. 607–624). New York, NY: Engel, C. (2002). Wild Health. Boston, MA:
Guilford Press. Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.
Delisle, J. R. (1986). Death with honors: Suicide Erlangsen, A., Runeson, B., Bolton, J. M.,
among gifted adolescents. Journal of Wilcox, H. C., Forman, J. L., Krogh, J., &
Counseling and Development, 64(9), Conwell, Y. (2017). Association between
558–560. spousal suicide and mental, physical, and
Del Giudice, M. (2018). Evolutionary social health outcomes: A longitudinal and
psychopathology: A unified approach. New nationwide register-based study. JAMA
York, NY: Oxford University Press. Psychiatry, 74(5), 456–464.
Dickeman, M. (1975). Demographic conse- Falger, V. S. E., & Falger, E. L. F. (2003). The
quences of infanticide in man. Annual Review cultural evolution of dying: Euthanasia in the
of Ecology and Systematics, 6(1), 107–137. Netherlands. In A. Somit & S. A. Peterson
D. Lester (Ed.), Theories Of Suicide: Past, (Eds.), Human Nature And Public Policy: An
Present and Future (pp. 9–22). Springfield, Evolutionary Approach (pp. 77–96). New
IL: Charles C. Thomas. York, NY: Palgrave Macmillan.
Duntley, J. D. (2005). Adaptations to dangers Falk, D. (1990). Brain evolution in Homo: The
from humans. In D. M. Buss (Ed.), The ‘radiator’ theory. Behavioral and Brain
Handbook Of Evolutionary Psychology Sciences, 13(02), 333–344.
(pp. 224–249). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Faucher, L. (2016). Darwinian blues:
Sons. Evolutionary psychiatry and depression. In J.
Duntley, J. D., & Buss, D. M. (2004). The C. Wakefield & S. Demazeux (Eds.), Sadness
evolution of evil. In A. G. Miller (Ed.), The Or Depression? (pp. 69–94). Dordrecht,
Social Psychology Of Good And Evil (pp. Netherlands: Springer.
102–124). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Fedden, R. (1938). Suicide; A Social And
Durkheim, E. (1897/1952). La Suicide (J. Historical Study. London, UK: Peter Davies.
Spaulding & G. Simpson, Trans.). Henley, UK: First, M. B., & Pincus, H. A. (2009). Diagnosis and
Routledge. classification. In M. G. Gelder, N. C. Andreasen,
Dyregrov, K., Plyhn, E., & Dieserud, G. (2012). J. J. López-Ibor, & J. R. Geddes (Eds.), New
After The Suicide: Helping The Bereaved To Oxford Textbook Of Psychiatry (pp. 99–121).
Find A Path From Grief To Recovery. London, Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
UK: Jessica Kingsley. Flinn, M. V., & Alexander, R. D. (2007). Runaway
Eccleston, C. (2001). Role of psychology in pain social selection in human evolution. In S. W.
management. British Journal of Anaesthesia, Gangestad & J. A. Simpson (Eds.), The
87(1), 144–152. Evolution Of Mind: Fundamental Questions
Efklides, A., & Moraitou, D. (Eds.). (2013). A And Controversies (pp. 249–255). New York,
Positive Psychology Perspective On Quality NY: Guilford Press.
Of Life. New York, NY: Springer. Flinn, M. V., Quinlan, R. J., Ward, C., & Coe, M.
Eisenberger, N. I., & Lieberman, M. D. (2004). (2007). Evolution of the human family: Coop-
Why rejection hurts: A common neural alarm erative males, long social childhoods, smart
system for physical and social pain. Trends in mothers, and extended kin networks. In C.
Cognitive Sciences, 8(7), 294–300. Salmon & T. K. Shackelford (Eds.), Family Rela-
Eisenberger, N. I., & Lieberman, M. D. (2005). tionships: An Evolutionary Perspective
Why it hurts to be left out: The neurocognitive (pp. 16–38). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
overlap between physical and social pain. In Franklin, J. C., Ribeiro, J. D., Fox, K. R., Bentley,
K. D. Williams, J. P. Forgas, & W. von Hippel K. H., Kleiman, E. M., Huang, X., & Nock, M.
(Eds.), The Social Outcast: Ostracism, Social K. (2017). Risk factors for suicidal thoughts
Exclusion, Rejection, And Bullying (pp. 109– and behaviors: A meta-analysis of 50 years
127). New York, NY: Psychology Press. of research. Psychological Bulletin, 143(2),
Eisenberger, N. I., Lieberman, M. D., & Williams, 187–232.
K. D. (2003). Does rejection hurt? An fMRI Freud, S. (1920/1991). Beyond The Pleasure
study of social exclusion. Science, 302(5643), Principle (J. Strachey, Trans.). London, UK:
290–292. Penguin.
84 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Frey, L. M., Hans, J. D., & Cerel, J. (2015). Gunn, J. F. (2014). Suicide as escape: Baechler,
Perceptions of suicide stigma: How do social Shneidman, and Baumeister. In J. F. Gunn &
networks and treatment providers compare? Gunn, J. F. (2017). The Social Pain Model.
Crisis, 37(2), 95–103. Crisis, 38(5), 281–286.
Gallup, G. G., & Weedon, S. L. (2013). Suicide Gunn, J. F. (2019). The importance of
bombers: Does an evolutionary perspective conceptual and theoretical frameworks. In D.
make a difference? Evolutionary Psychology, Lester (Ed.), The End Of Suicidology: Can We
11(4), 791–794. Ever Understand Suicide? (pp. 135–145).
Gandhi, S. G., Gilbert, W. M., McElvy, S. S., El New York, NY: Nova Science Publishers.
Kady, D., Danielson, B., Xing, G., & Smith, L. Gunn, J. F., & Lester, D. (2014). Theories Of
H. (2006). Maternal and neonatal outcomes Suicide: Past, Present And Future. Springfield,
after attempted suicide. Obstetrics & IL: Charles C. Thomas.
Gynecology, 107(5), 984–990. Hagen, E. H. (2002). Depression as bargaining:
Geary, D. C. (2005). Evolution of paternal The case postpartum. Evolution and Human
investment. In D. M. Buss (Ed.), The Behavior, 23(5), 323–336.
Handbook Of Evolutionary Psychology (pp. Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution
483–505). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. of social behaviour. I. Journal of Theoretical
Geary, D. C. (2007). The motivation to control Biology, 7(1), 1–16.
and the evolution of general intelligence. In Hamilton, W. D. (1972). Altruism and related
S. W. Gangestad & J. A. Simpson (Eds.), The phenomena, mainly in social insects. Annual
Evolution Of Mind: Fundamental Questions Review of Ecology and Systematics, 3(1),
And Controversies (pp. 305–312). New York, 193–232.
NY: Guilford Press. Hamilton, W. J. (1980). Do nonhuman animals
Gilbert, P. (1989). Human Nature And Suffering. commit suicide? Behavioral and Brain
Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Sciences, 3(02), 278–279.
Goldberg, D., & Goodyer, I. (2005). The Origins Hanschmidt, F., Lehnig, F., Riedel-Heller, S. G.,
And Course Of Common Mental Disorders. & Kersting, A. (2016). The stigma of suicide
Hove, UK: Routledge. survivorship and related consequences –
Goldblatt, M. J. (2014). Psychodynamics of a systematic review. PloS One, 11(9),
suicide. In M. K. Nock (Ed.), The Oxford e0162688.
Handbook Of Suicide And Self-Injury Harman, G. H. (1965). Inference to the best
(pp. 255–264). Oxford, UK: OUP. explanation. Philosophical Review, 74,
Goldsmith, S., Pellmar, T., Kleinman, A., & 88–95.
Bunney, W. (2002). Reducing Suicide: A Harris, E. C., & Barraclough, B. (1997). Suicide
National Imperative. Washington, DC: as an outcome for mental disorders. A meta-
National Academies Press. analysis. The British Journal of Psychiatry,
Gorelik, G., & Shackelford, T. K. (2017). Suicide 170(3), 205–228.
and the moralistic fallacy: Comment on Harris, M. (1974). Cows, Pigs, Wars And
Joiner, Hom, Hagan, and Silva (2016). Witches: The Riddles Of Culture. New York,
Evolutionary Psychological Science, 3(3), NY: Random House.
287–289. Harris, T. O. (2000). Introduction to the work of
Gould, S. J., & Lewontin, R. C. (1979). The George Brown. In T. O. Harris (Ed.), Where
spandrels of San Marco and the Panglossian Inner And Outer Worlds Meet: Psychosocial
paradigm: A critique of the adaptationist Research In The Tradition Of George W
programme. Proceedings of the Royal Society Brown (pp. 1–52). London, UK: Routledge.
of London B: Biological Sciences, 205(1161), Harvey, A. G. (2004). Cognitive Behavioural
581–598. Processes Across Psychological Disorders: A
Grad, O., & Andriessen, K. (2016). Surviving Transdiagnostic Approach To Research And
the legacy of suicide. In R. C. O’Connor & J. Treatment. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Pirkis (Eds.), The International Handbook Of Hausfater, G., & Hrdy, S. B. (1984). Infanticide:
Suicide Prevention (pp. 663–680). Chichester, Comparative And Evolutionary Perspectives.
UK: Wiley. New York, NY: Aldine.
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND SUICIDOLOGY 85

Hawton, K. (2007). Restricting access to Humphrey, N. (2018). The lure of death:


methods of suicide: Rationale and evaluation Suicide and human evolution. Philosophical
of this approach to suicide prevention. Crisis, Transactions of the Royal Society, B, 373, 1–8.
28(S1), 4–9. Hundert, E. M. (1992). The brain’s capacity to
Healey, C. (1979). Women and suicide in New form delusions as an evolutionary strategy
Guinea. Social Analysis: The International for survival. In M. Spitzer, F. Uehlein, M. A.
Journal of Social and Cultural Practice, (2), Schwartz, & C. Mundt (Eds.), Phenomenology,
89–106. Language & Schizophrenia (pp. 346–354).
Hedegaard, H., Curtin, S. C., & Warner, M. New York, NY: Springer.
(2018). Suicide mortality in the United States, Johnson, B. D. (1965). Durkheim’s one cause of
1999–2017. NCHS Data Brief, 330, 1–8. suicide. American Sociological Review, 30,
Heintzelman, S. J., & King, L. A. (2014). Life is 875–886.
pretty meaningful. American Psychologist, Joiner, T. E. (2005). Why People Die By Suicide.
69(6), 561–574. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Hendin, H. (1975). Growing up dead: Student Joiner, T. E., Buchman-Schmitt, J. M., Chu, C.,
suicide. American Journal of Psychotherapy, & Hom, M. A. (2017). A sociobiological
29(3), 327–338. extension of the interpersonal theory of
Hengartner, M. P., & Plöderl, M. (2019). Reply to suicide. Crisis, 38(2), 69–72.
the letter to the editor: ‘Newer-generation Joiner, T. E., Hom, M. A., Hagan, C. R., & Silva,
antidepressants and suicide risk: Thoughts on C. (2016). Suicide as a derangement of the
Hengartner and Plöderl’s Re-Analysis’. Psycho- self-sacrificial aspect of eusociality.
therapy and Psychosomatics. 88(6), 373–374. Psychological Review, 123(3), 235–254.
Hezel, F. X., Rubinstein, D. H., & White, G. M. Jordan, J. R. (2008). Bereavement after suicide.
(Eds.). (1985). Culture, Youth And Suicide In Psychiatric Annals, 38(10), 679–685.
The Pacific. Honolulu, HI: Pacific Island Jordan, J. R., & McIntosh, J. L. (Eds.). (2011).
Studies Program. Grief After Suicide: Understanding The
Himmelhoch, J. M. (1988). What destroys our Consequences And Caring For The Survivors.
restraints against suicide? Journal of Clinical New York, NY: Routledge.
Psychiatry, 49(9 (Suppl)), 46–52. Kahne, M. J. (1966). Suicide research: A critical
Hjelmeland, H., Jaworski, K., Knizek, B. L., & review of strategies and potentialities in
Marsh, I. (2019). Problematic advice from mental hospitals (Part II). International
suicide prevention experts. Ethical Human Journal of Social Psychiatry, 12(3), 177–186.
Psychology and Psychiatry, 20(2), 79–85. Kappeler, P. M., Silk, J. S., Burkart, J. M., & van
Hjelmeland, H., & Knizek, B. L. (2019). The Schaik, C. P. (2010). Primate behavior and
emperor’s new clothes? A critical look at the human universals: Exploring the gap. In P. M.
interpersonal theory of suicide. Death Kappeler & J. B. Silk (Eds.), Mind The Gap
Studies, 44(4), 168–178. (pp. 3–15). Berlin, Germany: Springer.
Hrdy, S. B. (1979). Infanticide among animals: Kapur, N., Cooper, J., O’Connor, R. C., &
A review, classification, and examination of Hawton, K. (2013). Non-suicidal self-injury v.
the implications for the reproductive attempted suicide: New diagnosis or false
strategies of females. Ethology and dichotomy? The British Journal of Psychiatry,
Sociobiology, 1(1), 13–40. 202(5), 326–328.
Huber, F. (2015/2019). Promise Me You’ll Shoot Kastenbaum, R. (1967). The child’s understanding
Yourself (I. Taylor, Trans.). London, UK: Allen of death: How does it develop? In E. A.
Lane. Grollman (Ed.), Explaining Death To Children
Humphrey, N. (1976). The social function of (pp. 89–100). Boston, MA: Beacon.
intellect. In P. Bateson & R. A. Hinde (Eds.), Keller, M. C., & Miller, G. F. (2006). Resolving
Growing Points In Ethology (pp. 303–317). the paradox of common, harmful, heritable
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. mental disorders: Which evolutionary genetic
Humphrey, N. (2011). Soul Dust: The Magic Of models work best? Behavioral and Brain
Consciousness. Princeton, NJ: Princeton Sciences, 29(4), 385–404; discussion
University Press. 405–352.
86 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Kendell, R., & Jablensky, A. (2003). Kuhn, T. S. (1977). The Essential Tension —
Distinguishing between the validity and Selected Studies In Scientific Tradition And
utility of psychiatric diagnoses. American Change. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago
Journal of Psychiatry, 160(1), 4–12. Press.
Kennedy, P., Rogers, B., Speer, S., & Frankel, H. Lankford, A. (2013). The Myth Of Martyrdom.
(1999). Spinal cord injuries and attempted New York, NY: Palgrave.
suicide: A retrospective review. Spinal Cord, Lankford, A. (2015). Is suicide terrorism really
37(12), 847–852. the product of an evolved sacrificial
Kessler, R. C., Amminger, G. P., Aguilar-Gaxiola, tendency? A review of mammalian research
S., Alonso, J., Lee, S., & Ustun, T. B. (2007). and application of evolutionary theory.
Age of onset of mental disorders: A review Comprehensive Psychology, 4(21).
of recent literature. Current Opinion in Lauder, G. V. (1996). The argument from
Psychiatry, 20(4), 359. design. In M. R. Rose & G. V. Lauder (Eds.),
Kessler, R. C., McLaughlin, K. A., Green, J. G., Adaptation (pp. 55–87). San Diego, CA:
Gruber, M. J., Sampson, N. A., Zaslavsky, Academic Press.
A. M., & Angermeyer, M. (2010). Childhood Layard, R. (2011). Happiness: Lessons From A
adversities and adult psychopathology in New Science (2nd ed.). London, UK: Penguin.
the WHO World Mental Health Surveys. The Leary, M. R., & Guadagno, J. (2011). The
British Journal of Psychiatry, 197(5), sociometer, self-esteem, and the regulation
378–385. of interpersonal behavior. In K. D. Vohs &
Kirkpatrick, L. A., & Navarrete, C. D. (2006). R. F. Baumeister (Eds.), Handbook Of Self-
Reports of my death anxiety have been Regulation: Research, Theory, And
greatly exaggerated: A critique of terror Applications (2nd ed., pp. 339–354). New
management theory from an evolutionary York, NY: Guilford Press.
perspective. Psychological Inquiry, 17(4), Leary, M. R., Tambor, E. S., Terdal, S. K., &
288–298. Downs, D. L. (1995). Self-esteem as an
Klein, R. G., & Edgar, B. (2002). The Dawn Of interpersonal monitor: The sociometer
Human Culture. New York, NY: Wiley. hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social
Klonsky, E. D., Dhingra, K., Klonsky, D., & Psychology, 68(3), 518.
Tapola, V. (2019). An empirical test of the Lester, D. (1993). The stigma against dying and
Three-Step Theory (3ST) of suicide in UK suicidal patients: A replication of Richard
university students. Suicide and Life- Kalish’s study twenty-five years later. OMEGA
Threatening Behavior, 49, 478–487. Journal of Death and Dying, 26(1), 71–75.
Klonsky, E. D., & May, A. M. (2015). The Three- Lester, D. (2014a). Suicide as deviance. In J. F.
Step Theory (3ST): A new theory of suicide Gunn & D. Lester (Eds.), Theories Of Suicide:
rooted in the ‘ideation-to-action’ framework. Past, Present And Future (pp. 244–277).
International Journal of Cognitive Therapy, Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas.
8(2), 114–129. Lester, D. (2014b). Suicide, ethology and
Klonsky, E. D., May, A. M., & Saffer, B. Y. sociobiology. In J. F. Gunn & D. Lester (Eds.),
(2016). Suicide, suicide attempts, and Theories Of Suicide: Past, Present And Future
suicidal ideation. Annual Review of Clinical (pp. 55–71). Springfield, IL: Charles C.
Psychology, 12, 307–330. Thomas.
Knizek, B. L., Kinyanda, E., Akotia, C. S., & Lester, D. (2017). Non-human animal suicide
Hjelmeland, H. (2013). Between Hippocrates could be tested. Animal Sentience, 2(20), 3.
and God: Ugandan mental health Lester, D. (2019). The End Of Suicidology: Can
professional’s views on suicide. Mental We Ever Understand Suicide? New York, NY:
Health, Religion & Culture, 16(8), 767–780. Nova Science Publishers.
Kuhl, J., Quirin, M., & Koole, S. L. (2015). Being Lieberman, M. D. (2013). Social: Why Our
someone: The integrated self as a Brains Are Wired To Connect. Oxford, UK:
neuropsychological system. Social and OUP.
Personality Psychology Compass, 9(3), Litman, R. E. (1967). Sigmund Freud on suicide.
115–132. In E. Shneidman (Ed.), Essays In
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND SUICIDOLOGY 87

Self-Destruction (pp. 324–344). New York, quality of care. In R. C. O’Connor & J. Pirkis
NY: Aronson. (Eds.), The International Handbook Of
Linehan, M. M. (2020). Building a Life Worth Suicide Prevention (pp. 387–402). Chichester,
Living. New York, NY: Random House. UK: Wiley.
Macdonald, C. J.-H. (2007). Uncultural Melzack, R., & Casey, K. L. (1968). Sensory,
Behavior: An Anthropological Investigation motivational and central control determinants
Of Suicide In The Southern Philippines. of pain: A new conceptual model. In D.
Honolulu, HI: University of Hawai’i Press. Kenshalo (Ed.), The Skin Senses (pp. 423–443).
MacDonald, G., & Leary, M. R. (2005). Why Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas.
does social exclusion hurt? The relationship Menninger, K. A. (1938). Man Against Himself.
between social and physical pain. New York, NY: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.
Psychological Bulletin, 131(2), 202. Menninger, K. A. (1963). The Vital Balance: The
Malkesman, O., Pine, D. S., Tragon, T., Austin, Life Process In Mental Health And Illness.
D. R., Henter, I. D., Chen, G., & Manji, H. K. New York, NY: Viking.
(2009). Animal models of suicide-trait- Merrick, J., Merrick, E., Lunsky, Y., & Kandel, I.
related behaviors. Trends in Pharmacological (2006). A review of suicidality in persons
Sciences, 30(4), 165–173. with intellectual disability. The Israel Journal
Maltsberger, J. T. (2003). Can a louse commit of Psychiatry and Related Sciences, 43(4),
suicide? Crisis, 24(4), 175–176. 258.
Maple, M., Frey, L. M., McKay, K., Coker, S., & Miller, G. (2000). Sexual selection for indicators
Grey, S. (2019). “Nobody hears a silent cry for of intelligence. In G. R. Bock, J. A. Goode, &
help”: Suicide attempt survivors’ experiences K. Webb (Eds.), The Nature Of Intelligence:
of disclosing during and after a crisis. Archives Novartis Foundation Symposium 233
of Suicide Research, 1–19. (pp. 260–275). Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Maris, R. W. (1982). Rational suicide: An Miller, G. F., & Penke, L. (2007). The evolution
impoverished self-transformation. Suicide of human intelligence and the coefficient of
and Life-Threatening Behavior, 12(1), 4–16. additive genetic variance in human brain
Maris, R. W. (2015). Pillaged: Psychiatric size. Intelligence, 35(2), 97–114.
Medications And Suicide Risk. Columbia, SC: Miller, M. B. (2008). Suicide and evolution
University of South Carolina Press. (Thesis). Florida State University, Tallahassee,
Marušič, A., & Swapp, R. (2004). Suicide genes FL.
floating in a glass of sparkling wine. Archives Mishara, B. L. (1999). Conceptions of death
of Suicide Research, 8(4), 297–301. and suicide in children ages 6–12 and their
Maynard Smith, J. (1964). Group selection and implications for suicide prevention. Suicide
kin selection. Nature, 201, 1145–1147. and Life-Threatening Behavior, 29(2),
Mayr, E. (1965). Cause and effect in biology. In 105–118.
D. Lerner (Ed.), Cause And Effect (pp. 33– Mishara, B. L. (2003). Suicide types: Rational. In
50). New York, NY: Free Press. R. Kastenbaum (Ed.), Macmillan Encyclopedia
McGirr, A., Renaud, J., Bureau, A., Seguin, M., Of Death And Dying (Vol. 2). New York, NY:
Lesage, A., & Turecki, G. (2008). Impulsive- Macmillan.
aggressive behaviours and completed suicide Mishara, B. L. (2006). Cultural specificity and
across the life cycle: A predisposition for universality of suicide: Challenges for the
younger age of suicide. Psychological International Association for Suicide
Medicine, 38(3), 407–417. Prevention. Crisis, 27(1), 1–3.
McNally, R. J. (2016). The legacy of Seligman’s Mishara, B. L., & Chagnon, F. (2016). Why
‘Phobias and Preparedness’ (1971). Behavior mental illness is a risk factor for suicide:
Therapy, 47(5), 585–594. Implications for suicide prevention. In R. C.
Meehl, P. E. (1973). Why I Do Not Attend Case O’Connor & J. E. Pirkis (Eds.), International
Conferences. Minneapolis, MN: University of Handbook Of Suicide Prevention (2nd ed.,
Minnesota Press. pp. 594–608). Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Mehlum, L., & Mork, E. (2016). After the Mugisha, J., Hjelmeland, H., Kinyanda, E., &
suicide attempt – the need for continuity and Knizek, B. L. (2011). Distancing: A traditional
88 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

mechanism of dealing with suicide among C. S. (2015). Mental disorders, comorbidity,


the Baganda, Uganda. Transcultural and pre-enlistment suicidal behavior among
Psychiatry, 48(5), 624–642. new soldiers in the US Army: Results from
Mullins, N., Bigdeli, T. B., Børglum, A., Coleman, the Army Study to Assess Risk and Resilience
J., Demontis, D., Mehta, D., & Lewis, C. M. in Service members (Army STARRS). Suicide
(2019). GWAS of suicide attempt in and Life-Threatening Behavior, 45(5),
psychiatric disorders and association with 588–599.
major depression polygenic risk scores. O’Connell, S., & Dunbar, R. (2003). A test for
American Journal of Psychiatry, 176(8), comprehension of false belief in chimpanzees.
651–660. Evolution and Cognition, 9(2), 131–140.
Murray, H. A., & Kluckhohn, C. (1948). Outline O’Connor, R. J. (1978). Brood reduction in
of a conception of personality. In C. Kluckhohn birds: Selection for fratricide, infanticide and
& H. A. Murray (Eds.), Personality In Nature, suicide? Animal Behaviour, 26, 79–96.
Society, And Culture (pp. 3–32). Oxford, UK: O’Connor, R. C. (2011). Towards an integrated
Knopf. motivational-volitional model of suicidal
Naghavi, M. (2019). Global, regional, and national behaviour. In R. C. O’Connor, S. Platt, &
burden of suicide mortality 1990 to 2016: J. Gordon (Eds.), International Handbook Of
Systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Suicide Prevention: Research, Policy And
Disease Study 2016. BMJ, 364(l94), 1–11. Practice (pp. 181–198). Chichester, UK: John
Nesse, R. M., & Schulkin, J. (2019). An Wiley and Sons.
evolutionary medicine perspective on pain O’Connor, R. C., Cleare, S., Eschle, S.,
and its disorders. Philosophical Transactions Wetherall, K., & Kirtley, O. J. (2016).
of the Royal Society B, 374(20190288), 1–7. The integrated motivational-volitional model
Nesse, R. M., & Williams, G. C. (1995). Why of suicidal behavior: An update. In R. C.
We Get Sick. New York, NY: Random House. O’Connor & J. Pirkis (Eds.), International
Nock, M. K., Borges, G., Bromet, E. J., Cha, C. B., Handbook Of Suicide Prevention (2nd ed.,
Kessler, R. C., & Lee, S. (2012). The epidemiology pp. 220–240). Chichester, UK: John Wiley
of suicide and suicidal behavior. In M. K. Nock, and Sons.
G. Borges, & Y. Ono (Eds.), Suicide: Global O’Connor, R. C., & Portzky, G. (2018). Looking
Perspectives From The WHO World Mental to the future: A synthesis of new developments
Health Surveys (pp. 5–32). Cambridge, UK: and challenges in suicide research and
Cambridge University Press. prevention. Frontiers in Psychology, 9(2139),
Nock, M. K., Borges, G., & Ono, Y. (2012). 1–14.
Conclusions and future directions. In M. K. Orbell, J., & Morikawa, T. (2011). An
Nock, G. Borges, & Y. Ono (Eds.), Suicide: evolutionary account of suicide attacks: The
Global Perspectives From The WHO World kamikaze case. Political Psychology, 32(2),
Mental Health Surveys (pp. 222–225). 297–322.
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Paniagua, F. A., Black, S. A., Gallaway, M. S., &
Nock, M. K., Green, J., Hwang, I., McLaughlin, Coombs, M. A. (2010). The Interpersonal-
K. A., Sampson, N. A., Zaslavsky, A. M., & Psychological Theory Of Attempted And
Kessler, R. C. (2013). Prevalence, correlates, Completed Suicide. Bloomington, IN:
and treatment of lifetime suicidal behavior AuthorHouse.
among adolescents: Results from the National Parker, G. A., & Smith, J. M. (1990). Optimality
Comorbidity Survey Replication Adolescent theory in evolutionary biology. Nature,
Supplement. JAMA Psychiatry, 70(3), 348(6296), 27.
300–310. Paulhus, D. L., & Buckels, E. (2012). Classic self-
Nock, M. K., Ramirez, F., & Rankin, O. (2019). deception revisited. In S. Vazire & T. D.
Advancing our understanding of the who, Wilson (Eds.), Handbook Of Self-Knowledge
when, and why of suicide risk. JAMA (pp. 363–378). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Psychiatry, 76(1), 11–12. Peña-Guzmán, D. M. (2018). Can nondolphins
Nock, M. K., Ursano, R. J., Heeringa, S. G., commit suicide? Animal Sentience, 2(20),
Stein, M. B., Jain, S., Raman, R., & Fullerton, 20.
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND SUICIDOLOGY 89

Penney, D., Stewart, A., & Parr, M. (2002). Riordan, D. (2019). Suicide and human sacrifice;
Prognostic outcome indicators following sacrificial victim hypothesis on the
hanging injuries. Resuscitation, 54(1), 27–29. evolutionary origins of suicide. Suicidology
Perry, S. A. (2014). Every Cradle Is A Grave: Online, 10(2), 1–10.
Rethinking The Ethics Of Birth And Suicide. Rogers, J. R. (2001). Theoretical grounding: The
Charleston, WV: Nine-Banded Books. ‘missing link’ in suicide research. Journal of
Perry, S. A. (2015). Antinatalism in biological Counseling & Development, 79(1), 16–25.
and cultural evolution: Fertility and suicide. Rogers, M. L., Ringer, F. B., & Joiner, T. E.
In T. K. Shackelford & R. D. Hansen (Eds.), (2018). The association between suicidal
The Evolution Of Morality (pp. 141–154). ideation and lifetime suicide attempts is
Cham, Switzerland: Springer. strongest at low levels of depression.
Perry, S. A. (2016). Patch 7.822: An Psychiatry Research, 270, 324–328.
experimental design puzzle. Retrieved from Rubinstein, D. H. (1986). A stress–diathesis
https://theviewfromhellyes.wordpress.com theory of suicide. Suicide and Life-
(October 2016) Threatening Behavior, 16(2), 182–197.
Persley, G., & Pegg, S. P. (1981). Burn injuries Saad, G. (2007). Suicide triggers as sex-specific
related to suicide. Medical Journal of threats in domains of evolutionary import:
Australia, 1(3), 134. Negative correlation between global male-to-
Pinker, S. (1997). How The Mind Works. New female suicide ratios and average per capita
York, NY: Norton. gross national income. Medical Hypotheses,
Pinker, S. (2010). The cognitive niche: 68(3), 692–696.
Coevolution of intelligence, sociality, and Salim, A., Martin, M., Sangthong, B., Brown,
language. Proceedings of the National C., Rhee, P., & Demetriades, D. (2006). Near-
Academy of Sciences, 107(Supplement 2), hanging injuries: A 10-year experience.
8993–8999. Injury, 37(5), 435–439.
Pitman, A. L., Osborn, D. P. J., King, M. B., & Sarkar, S. (1998). Genetics and Reductionism.
Erlangsen, A. (2014). Effects of suicide Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University
bereavement on mental health and suicide Press.
risk. The Lancet Psychiatry, 1(1), 86–94. Satcher, D. (1999). The Surgeon General’s Call
Pitman, A. L., Osborn, D. P. J., Rantell, K., & To Action To Prevent Suicide. Washington,
King, M. B. (2016). Bereavement by suicide DC: US Public Health Service.
as a risk factor for suicide attempt: A cross- Saunders, K. E., Hawton, K., Fortune, S., &
sectional national UK-wide study of 3432 Farrell, S. (2012). Attitudes and knowledge
young bereaved adults. BMJ, 6(1), e009948. of clinical staff regarding people who self-
Poole, F. J. P. (1985). Among the boughs of the harm: A systematic review. Journal of
hanging tree: Male suicide among the Bimin- Affective Disorders, 139(3), 205–216.
Kuskusmin of Papua New Guinea. In F. X. Schaeffer, H. (1967). Can a mouse commit
Hezel, D. H. Rubinstein, & G. M. White suicide? In E. Shneidman (Ed.), Essays In Self
(Eds.), Culture, Youth and Suicide in the Destruction. Northvale, NJ: Jason Aronson.
Pacific: Papers from an East-West Center Schechter, M. A., & Goldblatt, M. J. (2020).
Conference (pp. 152–181). Honolulu, HI: Suicide: A transdiagnostic phenomenon.
University of Hawaii at Manoa. Psychiatric Annals, 50, 136–137.
Preti, A. (2007). Suicide among animals: a Schmidtke, A. (1997). Suicide in Europe.
review of evidence. Psychological Reports, Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 27(1),
101(3), 831–848. 127–136.
Preti, A. (2011). Animal model and neurobiol- Seiden, R. H. (1969). Suicide Among Youth. A
ogy of suicide. Progress in Neuro-­ Review Of The Literature, 1900–1967. Chevy
Psychopharmacology and Biological Chase, MD: National Clearinghouse for
Psychiatry, 35(4), 818–830. Mental Health Information.
Ramsden, E., & Wilson, D. (2010). The nature Selby, E. A., Joiner, T. E., & Ribeiro, J. D. (2014).
of suicide: Science and the self-destructive Comprehensive theories of suicidal behaviors.
animal. Endeavour, 34(1), 21–24. In M. K. Nock (Ed.), The Oxford Handbook
90 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Of Suicide And Self-Injury (pp. 286–307). children. Clinical Child Psychology and
Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Psychiatry, 12(4), 525–535.
Sesardic, N. (2005). From genes to incest Solano, P., Pizzorno, E., Pompili, M., Serafini,
taboos: The crucial step. In A. P. Wolf & W. G., & Amore, M. (2018). Conceptualizations
H. Durham (Eds.), Inbreeding, Incest, And of suicide through time and socio-economic
The Incest Taboo: The State Of Knowledge factors: a historical mini-review. Irish Journal
At The Turn Of The Century (pp. 109–120). of Psychological Medicine, 35(1), 75–86.
Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Soper, C. A. (2016). Could some common
Seyfried, L. S., Kales, H. C., Ignacio, R. V., mental diseases have evolved to prevent
Conwell, Y., & Valenstein, M. (2011). suicide? – a theoretical enquiry. Paper
Predictors of suicide in patients with presented at the 16th European Symposium
dementia. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 7(6), on Suicide and Suicidal Behaviour, Oviedo,
567–573. Spain.
Shaffer, D. (1974). Suicide in childhood and Soper, C. A. (2017). Towards solving the
early adolescence. Journal of Child evolutionary puzzle of suicide (PhD thesis).
Psychology and Psychiatry, 15(4), 275–291. University of Gloucestershire, Cheltenham,
Sharma, T., Guski, L. S., Freund, N., & Gøtzsche, UK.
P. C. (2016). Suicidality and aggression Soper, C. A. (2018). The Evolution Of Suicide.
during antidepressant treatment: Systematic Cham, Switzerland: Springer.
review and meta-analyses based on clinical Soper, C. A. (2019a). Beyond the search for
study reports. BMJ, 352, i65. suigiston: How evolution offers oxygen for
Sheehan, L. L., Corrigan, P. W., & Al-Khouja, M. suicidology. In V. Zeigler-Hill & T. K.
A. (2016). Stakeholder perspectives on the Shackelford (Eds.), Evolutionary Perspectives
stigma of suicide attempt survivors. Crisis, On Death (pp. 37–61). Cham, Switzerland:
38(2), 73–81. Retrieved from https://doi. Springer.
org/10.1027/0227-5910/a000413 Soper, C. A. (2019b). Adaptation to the suicidal
Shneidman, E. S. (1985). Definition Of Suicide. niche. Evolutionary Psychological Science.
New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons. 5(4), 454–471.
Shneidman, E. S. (1993). Suicide As Psychache: Soper, C. A., & Shackelford, T. K. (2018). If
A Clinical Approach To Self-Destructive nonhuman animals can suicide, why don’t
Behavior. Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield. they? Animal Sentience, 2(20), 14.
Shneidman, E. S. (1996). The Suicidal Mind. Staats, A. W. (2004). The disunity-unity
New York, NY: Oxford University Press. dimension. American Psychologist, 59(4),
Shneidman, E. S. (2001). Comprehending 273.
Suicide. Landmarks In 20th-Century Stanley, I., Hom, M., Boffa, J., Stage, D. R. L.,
Suicidology. Washington, DC: American & Joiner, T. E. (2019). PTSD from a suicide
Psychological Association. attempt: An empirical investigation among
Shneidman, E. S. (2005). Anodyne suicide attempt survivors. Journal of Clinical
psychotherapy for suicide: A psychological Psychology, 75(10), 1879–1895.
view of suicide. Clinical Neuropsychiatry, Steinmetz, S. R. (1894). Suicide among primitive
2(1), 7–12. peoples. American Anthropologist, 7(1),
Shneidman, E. S., & Farberow, N. L. (Eds.). 53–60.
(1961). The Cry For Help. New York, NY: Stengel, E. (1970). Suicide And Attempted
McGraw-Hill. Suicide (Revised ed.). London, UK: Penguin.
Shorter, J., & Rueppell, O. (2012). A review on Stochl, J., Khandaker, G., Lewis, G., Perez, J.,
self-destructive defense behaviors in social Goodyer, I., Zammit, S., & Jones, P. (2015).
insects. Insectes Sociaux, 59(1), 1–10. Mood, anxiety and psychotic phenomena
Skinner, B. F. (1969). Contingencies Of measure a common psychopathological
Reinforcement: A Theoretical Analysis. New factor. Psychological Medicine, 45(7),
York, NY: Meredith. 1483–1493.
Slaughter, V., & Griffiths, M. (2007). Death Sudak, H., Maxim, K., & Carpenter, M. (2008).
understanding and fear of death in young Suicide and stigma: A review of the literature
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND SUICIDOLOGY 91

and personal reflections. Academic Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychological
Psychiatry, 32(2), 136–142. foundations of culture. In J. H. Barkow,
Suddendorf, T. (2013). The Gap: The Science L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The Adapted
Of What Separates Us From Other Animals. Mind: Evolutionary Psychology And The
New York, NY: Basic Books. Generation Of Culture (pp. 19–136). New
Sveen, C. A., & Walby, F. A. (2008). Suicide York, NY: Oxford University Press.
survivors’ mental health and grief reactions: Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2005). Conceptual
A systematic review of controlled studies. foundations of evolutionary psychology. In
Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 38(1), D. M. Buss (Ed.), The Handbook Of
13–29. Evolutionary Psychology (pp. 5–67).
Syme, K. L., Garfield, Z. H., & Hagen, E. H. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
(2016). Testing the bargaining vs. inclusive Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2008). The
fitness models of suicidal behavior against evolutionary psychology of the emotions and
the ethnographic record. Evolution and their relationship to internal regulatory
Human Behavior, 37(3), 179–192. variables. In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones,
Syme, K. L., & Hagen, E. H. (2018). When & L. F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook Of Emotions
saying ‘sorry’ isn’t enough: Is some suicidal (3rd ed., pp. 114–137). New York, NY:
behavior a costly signal of apology? A cross- Guilford Press.
cultural test. Human Nature, 30(1), Tooby, J., & DeVore, I. (1987). The reconstruction
117–141. of hominid behavioral evolution through
Symons, D. (1992). On the use and misuse of strategic modeling. In W. G. Kinzey (Ed.), The
Darwinism in the study of human behavior. Evolution Of Human Behavior: Primate
In J. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), Models (pp. 183–237). Albany, NY: State
The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology University of New York.
And The Generation Of Culture (pp. 137– Tousignant, M. (1998). Suicide in small-scale
162). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. societies. Transcultural Psychiatry, 35(2),
Tanaka, M., & Kinney, D. K. (2011). An 291–306.
evolutionary hypothesis of suicide: Why it Townsend, E. (2007). Suicide terrorists: Are
could be biologically adaptive and is so they suicidal? Suicide and Life-Threatening
prevalent in certain occupations. Behavior, 37(1), 35–49.
Psychological Reports, 108(3), 977–992. Turecki, G., & Brent, D. A. (2016). Suicide and
Tidemalm, D., Runeson, B., Waern, M., Frisell, suicidal behaviour. The Lancet, 387(10024),
T., Carlström, E., Lichtenstein, P., & 1227–1239.
Långström, N. (2011). Familial clustering of Usall, J., Pinto-Meza, A., Fernández, A., de
suicide risk: A total population study of 11.4 Graaf, R., Demyttenaere, K., Alonso, J., &
million individuals. Psychological Medicine, Haro, J. M. (2009). Suicide ideation across
41(12), 2527–2534. reproductive life cycle of women: Results from
Tinbergen, N. (1963). On aims and methods of a European epidemiological study. Journal of
ethology. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, Affective Disorders, 116(1), 144–147.
20(4), 410–433. van Dongen, J., & Boomsma, D. I. (2013). The
Tolaas, J. (2005). Evolution And Suicide. Salt evolutionary paradox and the missing
Lake City, UT: American University & Colleges heritability of schizophrenia. American
Press. Journal of Medical Genetics Part B:
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1990a). On the Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 162(2), 122–136.
universality of human nature and the Van Orden, K. A., Witte, T. K., Cukrowicz, K.
uniqueness of the individual: The role of C., Braithwaite, S. R., Selby, E. A., & Joiner,
genetics and adaptation. Journal of T. E. (2010). The interpersonal theory of
Personality, 58(1), 17–67. suicide. Psychological Review, 117(2),
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1990b). The past 575–600.
explains the present: Emotional adaptations Varki, A., & Brower, D. (2013). Denial: Self-
and the structure of ancestral environments. Deception, False Beliefs, And The Origins Of
Ethology and Sociobiology, 11(4), 375–424. The Human Mind. New York, NY: Twelve.
92 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Värnik, P. (2012). Suicide in the world. Wertheimer, A. (2014). A Special Scar: The
International Journal of Environmental Experiences Of People Bereaved By Suicide
Research And Public Health, 9(3), 760–771. (3rd ed.). Abingdon, UK: Routledge.
Voracek, M. (2006). Smart and suicidal? The Williams, G. C. (1957). Pleiotropy, natural
social ecology of intelligence and suicide in selection, and the evolution of senescence.
Austria. Death Studies, 30(5), 471–485. Evolution, 11(4), 398–411.
Voracek, M., & Marušič, A. (2008). Testing the Williams, G. C. (1966). Adaptation And Natural
Finno-Ugrian Suicide Hypothesis: Geographic Selection: A Critique Of Some Current
variation of elderly suicide rates across Evolutionary Thought. Princeton, NJ:
Europe. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 62(4), Princeton University Press.
302–308. Williams, G. C. (1992). Natural Selection:
Wiley, J. (2020). Psychological Aposematism: Domains, Levels, And Challenges. Oxford,
An Evolutionary Analysis of Suicide. Biological UK: Oxford University Press.
Theory. doi:10.1007/s13752-020-00353-8 Williams, G. C. (1996). Plan And Purpose In
World Health Organization. (2012). Public Nature. London, UK: Weidenfeld & Nicholson.
Health Action For The Prevention Of Suicide: Williams, J. M. G. (1997). Cry Of Pain:
A Framework. Geneva, Switzerland: World Understanding Suicide And Self-Harm.
Health Organization. London, UK: Penguin.
World Health Organization. (2013). Projections Williams, J. M. G., Crane, C., Barnhofer, T., &
of Mortality and Causes of Death, 2015 and Duggan, D. S. (2005). Psychology and
2030. Retrieved from www.who.int/ suicidal behaviour: Elaborating the
healthinfo/global_burden_disease/en/ entrapment model. In K. Hawton (Ed.),
(Accessed February 2016) Prevention And Treatment Of Suicidal
World Health Organization. (2014). Preventing Behaviour: From Science To Practice (pp.
Suicide: A Global Imperative. Geneva, 71–89). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Switzerland: World Health Organization. Williams, J. M. G., & Pollock, L. R. (2000). The
Waldmann, M. R., Hagmayer, Y., & Blaisdell, A. psychology of suicidal behaviour. In K.
P. (2006). Beyond the information given: Hawton & K. van Heeringen (Eds.), The
Causal models in learning and reasoning. International Handbook Of Suicide And
Current Directions in Psychological Science, Attempted Suicide (pp. 79–94). Chichester,
15(6), 307–311. UK: John Wiley & Sons.
Walker, M., Moreau, D., & Weissman, M. M. Willner, P., & Belzung, C. (2015). Treatment-
(1990). Parents’ awareness of children’s resistant depression: Are animal models of
suicide attempts. American Journal of depression fit for purpose? Psychopharma-
Psychiatry, 147(10), 1364–1366. cology, 232(19), 3473–3495.
Wall, P. D. (1999). Pain: The Science Of Wilson, D. S. (1980). Suicide, beanbag genetics,
Suffering. London, UK: Weidenfeld & and pleiotropy. Behavioral and Brain Sciences,
Nicholson. 3(2), 283.
Wampold, B. E. (2018). The therapeutic value Wilson, D. S., & Wilson, E. O. (2007). Rethinking
of the relationship for placebo effects and the theoretical foundation of sociobiology.
other healing practices. International Review The Quarterly Review of Biology, 82(4),
of Neurobiology, 139, 191–210. 327–348.
Wampold, B. E., & Imel, Z. E. (2015). The Great Wilson, J. D., & Roehrborn, C. (1999). Long-
Psychotherapy Debate: The Evidence For term consequences of castration in men:
What Makes Psychotherapy Work (2nd ed.). Lessons from the Skoptzy and the eunuchs
New York, NY: Routledge. of the Chinese and Ottoman courts. The
Wasserman, D., Rihmer, Z., Rujescu, D., Journal of Clinical Endocrinology &
Sarchiapone, M., Sokolowski, M., Titelman, Metabolism, 84(12), 4324–4331.
D., & Carli, V. (2012). The European Wright, R. (1994). The Moral Animal. New
Psychiatric Association (EPA) guidance on York, NY: Vintage.
suicide treatment and prevention. European Wright, S. (1943). Isolation by distance.
Psychiatry, 27(2), 129–141. Genetics, 28(2), 114–138.
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND SUICIDOLOGY 93

Zalsman, G., Hawton, K., Wasserman, D., van Zilboorg, G. (1936a). Differential diagnostic
Heeringen, K., Arensman, E., Sarchiapone, types of suicide. Archives of Neurology &
M., & Zohar, J. (2017). Evidence-based Psychiatry, 35(2), 270–291.
national suicide prevention taskforce in Zilboorg, G. (1936b). Suicide among civilized
Europe: A consensus position paper. European and primitive races. American Journal of
Neuropsychopharmacology, 27(4), 418–421. Psychiatry, 92(6), 1347–1369.
4
Evolutionary Psychology and
Mindfulness and Meditation:
Easing the Anxiety of Being
Human
James Carmody

INTRODUCTION developed in India including those of Vedanta


and Buddhism. Training exercises designed to
Something called mindfulness seems to be develop the personal qualities required to actu-
popping up everywhere: in clinics and hospi- alize the models’ goals were also developed
tals, schools, corporate offices, prisons, and in keeping with approaches to knowledge
online, the list keeps expanding. Today, at the at the time, these were not clearly distinguished
local farmer’s market, I bought greens from from religion. Mindfulness, as it is now com-
‘Mindful Veggies’. Promoted as a wellbeing monly recognized in the West, came primarily
enhancer, studies show that mindfulness does out of Buddhism and is described in the follow-
indeed reduce stress and distress in a range of ing section.
conditions and circumstances (Goyal et  al., Buddhist principles, particularly, later
2014; Willem et al., 2016). In this chapter I spread into other parts of Asia and integrated
describe the evolutionary roots of the psy- into those countries’ prevailing spiritual belief
chological processes that give rise to the systems. Both parties were changed as a result
human angst that mindfulness addresses and of those meetings. After a hiatus of several
how the training exercises designed to culti- centuries, increased global mobility brought
vate it alleviate that distress. And to provide Buddhism to Western countries and once
context for those I first describe the cultural again it has been adapted into the prevailing
roots of mindfulness and meditation. cultural narrative. One of those adaptations
Several innovative approaches to inquiry has been the integration of mindfulness, a core
emerged during what has been called the tenet of the system described in the following
Axial Age. In contrast to the Platonic and section, into the cultural narrative of self-help.
Aristotelian systems developing in Greece The cultural origins and purpose of Buddhism
during that era, internal psychological models however have parallels in some Vedanta
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND MINDFULNESS AND MEDITATION 95

practices that also have migrated to the West them to the research and that has guided their
in the ­ various forms of yoga. This secular decisions about the conditions, instruction,
transition has made its benefits more widely and syllabus of the mindfulness intervention.
available than they might otherwise have been, For mindfulness makes apparent the normally
for although a practice from a religious system unrecognized patterns of attending that never-
is attractive to some people, it is alienating to theless continuously affect our felt sense and
many in the secular West. the background hope is that study participants
This transition has also brought mindfulness also will benefit from that kind of noticing.
under the gaze of empirical science and close
inspection of its mechanisms of action reveals
parallels and differences in some principles
independently developed in Western psychol- THE CULTURAL ROOTS OF
ogy to categorize, account for, and alleviate MINDFULNESS AND ITS MIGRATION
human angst. Those principles, together with INTO WESTERN SETTINGS
evolutionary theory, can provide a coherent
and culturally familiar explanation of the eve- The Buddhist and Vedanta narratives are
ryday mental unease that plagues us and how rooted in the primarily introspective
the practice of mindfulness alleviates it. This approaches to knowledge extant in India
evolutionary and needs-based lens describes around 500 BCE, the time of the historical
inbuilt patterns of attending that keep us ill- Buddha. They each place the source of human
at-ease, and the psychological principles that angst in ignorance of the moment-by-moment
mindfulness and several similar mind–body construction of the personal self, and the
practices draw upon in enabling the recogni- chronic dissatisfaction (suffering) arising
tion and amelioration of that distress. from the accompanying sense of ownership of
The description draws upon my own and its desires and aversions. In this respect they
others’ published studies of the clinical effects are not a description of how the physical
and mechanisms of mindfulness training (MT), world operates, but of what we call mind.
as well as experience and feedback from teach- As the Buddha described his insight into
ing mindfulness to patients and clinicians. The this dilemma to others and the experience of
description is phenomenological because suf- enlightened release accompanying it, an eight-
fering and mindfulness are rooted in felt experi- faceted system developed through which they
ence and it is their felt sense that people wish also could cultivate a similar recognition. One
to address in doing the practices. Clinicians facet was the cultivation of something called
also talk most meaningfully to patients in those ‘sati’ to serve as a heuristic aid for real-time
terms. In that sense, mindfulness practices can recognition of these mental operations and their
be thought of as phenomenologically empiri- effects. Sati is from the Pali language spoken
cal explorations of the mental processes giving in Northern India at the time, but that is no
rise to mental distress and the capacity for self-­ longer understood outside the Indian scholar-
regulation that emerges in the face of it. ship community. Exactly how sati was trans-
The chapter also draws upon my own expe- lated, described, and cultivated varied in the
rience with 50 years of practice in Buddhist places to which Buddhism migrated over the
and Vedantic traditions of inquiry in Asia and centuries. This is apparent in the varied teach-
Western countries. Many academic papers ings and practices of the Western Buddhist sects
about mindfulness are caught in attempting whose traditions derive from those countries.
objectivity in relation to something that is Mindfulness, a term with an already existing
often anything but. In the background, u­ sually meaning in English, emerged as the accepted
undescribed, stands the author’s positive per- translation during the 19th century as Buddhism
sonal experience with mindfulness that led was becoming of interest in the West.
96 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Recognizing the commonality that 2013). They stay with it for a breath or two
Buddhism’s goal of reducing mental suffering before attention wanders to daydreaming. The
had with the goal of patient care, Kabat-Zinn tenacity of this default movement to cognitions
(Kabat-Zinn, 1982) experimented with teach- in the absence of sustained watchfulness sug-
ing groups of patients a selection of training gests an important function, one that becomes
exercises used to cultivate sati in South Asian clear in reflecting on the role attention plays in
traditions. Those patients described obtain- our mental ecology.
ing benefits in coping with their illnesses Our brains are continuously processing all
from participating in the classes that came manner of information about the internal and
to be called the Mindfulness-Based Stress external environment. Attention, the capac-
Reduction (MBSR) program. Replications in ity to selectively attend to some portion of
peer-reviewed journals affirmed those benefits this information over others, particularly
and the program became widely used in clinics opportunities and threats to the fulfillment of
(Shonin et al., 2015). Principles and practices needs, has clear value for survival and repro-
from MBSR form the foundation for many of ductive success (Geary, 2005). Selection has
the adaptations and applications found in the also resulted in this process being i­mmediate
wide variety of settings in which mindfulness rather than through conscious, deliberate, and
now appears. slower decision-making pathways (Tomlin
While the processes at the root of the men- et al., 2015). When physical danger threatens,
tal distress that the cultivation of mindfulness as it regularly did in the evolutionary past,
addresses are interwoven and run in parallel, attention automatically orients to sensory
for explanatory purposes I describe them as processes monitoring the external environ-
a sequence. It is important also to note that ment. Attention is also intimately connected to
early Buddhism employed a somewhat differ- arousal. In the best-selling book Why Zebras
ent frame to categorize the qualities of expe- Don’t Get Ulcers, Sapolsky (2004) describes
rience than Western systems do. For example, how the attention of animals in the wild is ori-
it did not use the construct of emotion in the ented to the senses and to arousal: danger is
description of affective experience; rather it smelled, seen, or heard, arousal levels and ten-
used a more granular analysis of experien- sion spike, the animal responds in some way,
tial components that comprise those states and a more quiescent state resumes.
as described in following sections. Attention We also would live in a more relaxed here
regulation, however, is common across the and now if real and immediate physical dan-
systems and is known to be fundamental to gers were the principle risks that our atten-
wellbeing (Posner and Rothbart, 2007). For tion responded to. But being in the moment is
that reason, it may be best to begin with not characteristic of modern life. The capac-
attention and the priorities that effect it. ity for cognition and language makes pos-
sible the imagination of conceivable future
threats (Andrews-Hanna, 2012; Baumeister
et  al., 2001) as well as complex planning
THE EVOLUTIONARY ROOTS OF and discussion of how best to address them.
ATTENTION AND ITS ROLE IN The importance of those capacities to mod-
A NEEDS-BASED SYSTEM ern humans is evident in our attention’s
insistent default to the concerns of projected
MT usually begins with an attention-regulation futures and pasts when it is not required for
task. A common one asks the person to place the execution of an immediate task. This can
their focus on the sensations of their breathing be observed by deliberately taking notice of
and to keep it there for some time. While this the content of the imaginings to which our
may seem a relatively easy task, trainees report minds ‘wander’. There we see recurring and
it as one of the most challenging (Segal et al., often conflicting and entangled concerns about
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND MINDFULNESS AND MEDITATION 97

our own and our family’s and friends’ welfare, means that navigating threats and opportuni-
social standing, sex, work, and money. ties and monitoring projected pasts and futures
The persistence of this default, and its is a constant project.
affective upshot, was captured nicely in a
real-time study by Killingsworth and Gilbert
(2010). Prompted at random times to report
what their attention was on in that moment, DARWIN AND BUDDHISM’S FIRST
subjects reported it being on the task at hand NOBLE TRUTH: VIGILANCE AND
for only about half the day, even though they THE INTERNAL NARRATIVE MAKE
reported greater happiness at those times. UNEASE OUR EVERYDAY STATE
Significantly, the attention wandering gener-
ally preceded the happiness decrease. It is this In traditional Buddhist teachings, the reduced
propensity of attention to quickly and repeat- happiness Killingsworth and Gilbert (2010)
edly default to cognitions, memories, plans, found associated with the preoccupations of a
speculations, and daydreams, that mindful- wandering mind results not from the thoughts
ness trainees encounter when asked to focus and imaginings themselves, but from the
on sense-based experience, and that they find unpleasant sensations of constriction that
so challenging to overrule and regulate. They reflexively accompany their semi-vigilant
discover also that much of this cognition goes (‘what could go wrong here?’) and uncertainty-
on outside of awareness and becomes appar- related character. A discomforting cycle is then
ent only when they deliberately watch their set up as those unpleasant sensations of tension
mental activity. remind us again of the threat-related thought
In the absence of immediate danger, then, (Damasio, 2003). Extended preoccupation with
attention functionally defaults to cognitive these alarm-based cycles is experienced as
processes serving the social-safety needs of rumination and worry. These are accompanied
tribal primates, particularly those for relation- by arousal-related inflammatory processes. As
ship/belonging, status, and power. The bio- we’ve all experienced, the level of arousal, con-
logical importance of these needs is evident striction, and unpleasantness can range from
in the fact that health becomes compromised very mild to dreadful depending on the degree
when, for some reason, we are stripped of to which those needs are frustrated or threat-
them. Also, their intricately interwoven and at ened (Brosschot et al., 2006). Figure 4.1 illus-
times conflicting nature, together with the fact trates this vigilance-based cycle in the
that they have no organic satiation mechanism, experience of anxiety.

Figure 4.1  Alarm-related components of experience forming a cycle of distress


98 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

When mental bandwidth is occupied by can also wish for and imagine a life free of it
this narrative, curiosity and connection with and trial possible solutions. That’s a reflec-
our surroundings becomes problem-focused, tion for the ages. It is also one that preoccu-
and we are less than openly present for the pied the historical Buddha who, after intense
present needs and concerns of others. When experimentation, recognized the above pat-
attention is task-related, however, this inter- terns shaping the affective quality of his own
nal narrative recedes (Watkins, 2004) and we life as well as related insights into how these
are less affected by its threat- and opportunity- can be overcome. In conveying these to inter-
oriented memories and imaginings. ested friends he saw that they were also
Even when these preoccupations barely useful to others.
reach awareness (Brosschot et  al., 2014; Three of those insights are contained, to a
Creswell et al., 2013; Custers and Aarts, 2010; greater and lesser degree, within present-day
Dahl et al., 2015), the ongoing bodily discom- MT programs. They are designed to support
fort gives rise to a desire for a break in some- greater awareness, acceptance, and some level
thing more pleasant; to interrupt the cycle and of everyday self-regulation in the face of these
replace it temporarily with one characterized mental and perceptual tendencies shaping
by greater ease. And because the experience everyday feeling life (Cavanagh et  al., 2014;
of pleasure and delight encompasses the body Donald et  al., 2016; Saunders et  al., 2016)
(Biswas-Diener et al., 2015), a sense pleasure and can be operationalized in familiar psycho-
is usually the most immediately accessible. It logical terms. The analysis is not regarded as
might be something like a snack, a drink, or the final word on mental mechanisms but as
a drug. It may also be something more elabo- a description of mental qualities that can be
rate; the possibilities are as broad as imagina- phenomenologically recognized in real time,
tion and resources allow. and trainees comprehend and use them to suit
We don’t start life cognitively preoccupied their interests and circumstances (Carmody
in this way. We arrive as little sensate creatures, and Baer, 2008; Cebolla et al., 2017).
curious and awake to the wonder of touching, The first insight, alluded to in the previ-
tasting, hearing, seeing, moving, and their ous section, is that everyday experience and
pleasant and unpleasant feeling tones. A cog- emotions are constructed from a suite of just
nitive past and future become gradually and three interwoven phenomenological compo-
imperceptibly woven into this sensory expe- nents: cognitions, sensations, and their pleas-
rience alongside language, the naming and ant/unpleasant feeling tones. Classification
appraisal enabling us to describe our needs and description of the affective dimension of
and impressions to others (Alderson-Day and experience change with the age and the cul-
Fernyhough, 2015). As we learn to weave ture, and feeling here is not to be confused
our way through the fragile social maze, the with how we commonly use the term today
hopes, comparisons, judgments, and regrets when we might say we are feeling angry or
embedded in this emerging internal narrative sad. It’s simply the pleasant or unpleasant
come to mediate and filter sensory perception quality of a sensation. Also, emotional cat-
and experience, and weave indiscernibly into a egories vary across cultures and over time;
developing sense of self. something described as an emotion at one
time might be referred to as a passion in
another. In the Buddhist analysis emotions
Using Our Heads to Get out of are approached more granularly as amalgams
of those three fundamental qualities. When
Them
those components are not distinguished their
Just as this safety-oriented cognitive watch- felt experience is seamless, as illustrated in
fulness has an affective downside, cognition Figure 4.2.
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND MINDFULNESS AND MEDITATION 99

Figure 4.2  Memory, imagination and emotion are symphonies of three interwoven
experiential components

Experiential recognition and discrimina- affect that their distinction was not apparent
tion of the components starts, most usually, in awareness (Pessoa, 2008) become noticed
with re-awakening interest in the realm of and named. Their associations, so rapid they
sensation. In the body scan, for example, an are normally missed, become apparent in
exercise taught in MBSR (Kabat-Zinn, 1990), noticing that attention does not stay with a
trainees are asked to systematically direct sensation, but quickly goes to its feeling tone,
attention to each part of the body and to notice to thoughts about it, or to something else.
any sensations that may be present there, Implicit in this bare noticing of experience is
including subtle and neglected sensations that an embodied acceptance, one that also may
may escape awareness in everyday life; this be made explicit in the instructions.
while refraining from attempting to change the Interoceptive awareness is important in
experience in any way. They are instructed to emotion regulation (Füstös et  al., 2013)
notice also any pleasant or unpleasant feeling and the perception of internal experience
tone that may be associated with a sensation, developed by these MT exercises appears
and the difference between the sensation and to mediate its beneficial effect on emo-
any thoughts that may also be present. This is tional wellbeing (Mehling et al., 2012). And
schematically illustrated in Figure 4.3 using even though trainees initially report finding
the example of a fearful thought and the sen- these exercises challenging (Segal et  al.,
sations of constriction that may be associated 2013), they result in measurable increases
with it, and which, on a less granular level, we in volitional orienting of attention (Chan
experience as anxiety. and Woollacott, 2007; Jha et  al., 2007),
This learning is akin to winding back the improved performance on sustained attention
developmental and experiential clock. As tasks (Tang et al., 2015), and improvements
described in the previous section, we are in working memory and autobiographical
not born with these groupings. Cognitions memory (Lao et al., 2016). Also, the differ-
that had been so implicitly integrated (Blair, ential thickness in brain regions associated
2002) into our early world of sensation and with attention, interoception, and sensory
100 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Figure 4.3  Components recognized as differentiated and connected

processing found in meditators compared to The principle is consistent with William


matched controls (Lazar et al., 2005) is con- James’ cogent remark that experience is what
sistent with meditators’ increased capacity one pays attention to (James, 1890). The key
for awareness of internal states, particularly role that this effortful focusing of attention
awareness of breathing sensations. plays in wellbeing-supportive emotion regula-
The second insight is that attention can be tion and in self-regulated behavior (Baumeister
regulated to create a more benign or neutral and Heatherton, 1996; Kirschenbaum, 1987;
affect. It builds upon the first insight and can Thayer et  al., 1996) was confirmed in later
be experienced in any of the MT exercises; experimental studies, and in the findings that
when attention is directed to arousal-neutral poor cognitive control is associated with many
bodily sensations, such as those of breathing mental disorders (Snyder and Hankin, 2016).
for example, a more benign affect emerges. Rumination, for example, is an indicator of
The sensations of breathing provide an acces- attention wandering from immediate tasks
sible and readily available object of attention and becoming captivated by an uncomfortable
that can be unobtrusively turned to as a restful internal narrative.
experience in moments of stress. This facil- Supporting this principle, MT has been
ity, sometimes referred to by clinicians as shown to reduce rumination (Campbell
‘going to the breath’, is readily established. et al., 2012) and to increase mood as a result
In the initial trials of MBSR, a large major- (Huffziger et al., 2013). More specifically, an
ity of participants indicated that they attached MT exercise focusing on the awareness of
high importance to this simple skill to calm breathing improved mood, meta-awareness,
themselves in moments of stress/distress and mind wandering (Levinson et al., 2014).
(Kabat-Zinn, 1987). These observations are supported by imaging
This redirection of attention and its affec- studies in which mindfulness trainees show
tive result is illustrated in Figure 4.4. It is less activation of brain regions involved in
distinguished from experiential avoidance, narrative processing of self-relevant stimuli
which involves a compulsive mental (or and greater activation of regions implicated
physical) turning away from difficult experi- in ‘experiential’ processing, relative to nov-
ences (Hayes et al., 1996). ices (Farb et  al., 2007; Lutz et  al., 2016).
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND MINDFULNESS AND MEDITATION 101

Figure 4.4  Attention shifts from differentiated components to arousal-neutral sensations of


breathing

Interestingly, MT appears to reduce reactivity This observing stance is implicit in most MT


to emotional stimuli, which is not found with exercises and is cultivated explicitly by the
relaxation training (Ortner et al., 2007). practice of silently naming component mental
The third insight is the use of an observ- qualities as they are occurring in awareness.
ing perspective toward mental life. This For example, rather than contending with,
‘observing self’ (Deikman, 1982) is used to or trying to push away, a harshly self-critical
notice thoughts/images, sensations, and feel- thought, attention is reoriented from its con-
ings while remaining unaffected by their tent, such as ‘I am a failure’, to the more affect-
content and tone. It is itself a cognitive process, neutral reflection ‘This is a thought’.
albeit one used to cultivate a sense of detach- The strategy is illustrated in Figure 4.5
ment from mental phenomena that previously using the example of a commonly reported
had captured attention and created distress. thought during panic attacks.

Figure 4.5  Re-perceiving reduces distress through a perceptual/attentional shift from what
the thought is about – ‘I’m going to pass out’ – to the thought as an event in the mind/
awareness – ‘This is a thought’
102 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Referred to as decentering (Teasdale et al., circumstances, attention defaults to sup-


2002) and meta-cognition (Wells, 1999), this porting our social needs and planning how
meta-awareness increases with participa- they can be met. And as social life becomes
tion in MT (Carmody et al., 2009; Feldman more complex, the goals longer term, and
et al., 2010; Lao et al., 2016; Levinson et al., success and failure more contingent, imag-
2014; Teasdale et  al., 2002). It has been ining assumes an increasingly central role.
found important in the treatment of depres- Essential to this planning is imagining what
sion (Bieling et al., 2012), and this decreased could go wrong here, how plans might be
cognitive reactivity appears to mediate the threatened by others or by circumstances,
association between mindfulness-based cog- and the consequences of these in terms of
nitive therapy and decreases in depressive our own, and our kin’s, future suffering
symptoms (Cladder-Micus et al., 2017). and possible death. Imagination also allows
for reflections that give rise to questions of
meaning and existential angst.
Each age and culture developed ways and
MINDFULNESS AND THE COGNITIVE means of coping with this anxiety that arises
NARRATIVE’S DOUBLE-EDGED from the necessary uncertainty of our plans,
SWORD and of life itself. Some rely on placating and
beseeching imagined beings assumed to con-
We are tribal primates with a powerful capac- trol events. The Greeks began a more rational
ity to imagine. This wondrous ability envi- and empirical approach to understanding
sions possible futures and constructs human nature and events that unfold in the
narratives of the past beyond simple condi- world, apparent in the teachings of people
tioning. It gives us a powerful advantage in such as Plato, Aristotle, and Archimedes.
meeting our human needs and is that which Around the same time philosophers in India
most clearly separates us from other pri- developed what we would now call psycho-
mates. The capacity for language that devel- logical methods of inquiry that focused on
oped in conjunction with it allows also for the mind itself. In that sense they are not
conversations about where dangers are most descriptions of the world, but mental mod-
likely to be found, how they might best be els that allow insight into the machinations
met, and the passing of those lessons across of the vehicle through which we apprehend
generations – group learning, in other words. the world and how it can be self-modified
The clear survival upside of imagination to alleviate the angst. In its original context,
is seen in the development of tools and their mindfulness was cultivated as part of a train-
uses, and in the ability to plan and coordinate ing system to recognize how mental suffering
future activities and scenarios that would is created moment-to-moment in the human
favor resource procurement and advantageous mind and the behaviors and attitudes that
mates: to be able to plan for the hunt and what can alleviate it. And while some schools of
would be required, as well as how any gains Western psychology developed constructs
might be distributed. When bands were small and principles that parallel some of those
and less complex, dangers imminent and found in Buddhism, they did not develop
immediate, and neural cognitive capacity less training exercises that so systematically
developed, those imaginings were probably develop their recognition and self-regulation.
relatively short-lived; life required careful In that sense, one of Buddhism’s most impor-
and continued attention to the senses. tant contributions has been the practices to
Immediate physical dangers have been actualize those principles; ones that have
minimized in modern life and sensory moni- been seamlessly integrated as mindfulness
toring has become less necessary. In these into existing psychotherapies.
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND MINDFULNESS AND MEDITATION 103

In our complex and secular societies, the that the practice provides: I want to feel less
cognitive activity required to navigate for ­anxious or less depressed, I want to sleep bet-
success in status, power, and relationships ter, or I’d like moments of quiet in the tumult
consumes inordinate amounts of neural band- of modern life. The relief that accompanies
width. And the affect associated with this is these recognitions satisfies the interest of
evident in ever-increasing rates of anxiety, most people.
depressive disorders, and suicide. It may also For those sufficiently interested to continue
be contributing to increasing rates of addic- exploring the system’s original existential
tion and obesity; rates that may make a more goal, other mental features become apparent.
universal healthcare unsustainable. The MT Gaps begin to appear between thoughts and in
exercises support recognition of this mental those moments the still background becomes
activity that goes largely unnoticed in daily apparent. The natural re-emergence of
life, even as it is affecting wellbeing. thoughts reveals how the narrative they form
To those ends, mindfulness encourages has become indistinguishable from that still
the recognition that our apparently seamless presence within which they occur (Carmody,
mental activity comprises phenomenological 2016). You realize that it has always been
components: sensations, cognitions, and their there, just overlain by attention’s ongoing
pleasant/unpleasant affective quality that we fascination with the contents of the cognitive
don’t normally experience as separate. The narrative. It also becomes apparent just how
exercises also develop a capacity to notice much of our lives are spent distracted and
where attention is focused in that suite and to preoccupied by that imagining. Perspective
regulate attention so that it becomes less con- shifts with that experience, as it would if a
ditioned and more fluid. The exercises also fish was to recognize water.
expose attention’s default impulse toward
cognition and the narratives forming through
imagination and memory, its handmaiden.
As a needs-serving mechanism, cogni- REFERENCES
tion has a watchful quality for threats and
opportunities. In the mental background, the Alderson-Day, B., & Fernyhough, C. (2015).
default mode network and related functions Inner speech: Development, cognitive func-
tions, phenomenology, and neurobiology.
are planning and wondering what could go
Psychological Bulletin, 141(5), 931. doi:http://
wrong here and whether this is an opportu- psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/bul0000021
nity. MT makes this vigilant quality appar- Andrews-Hanna, J. R. (2012). The brain’s
ent, as well as its affective downside in the default network and its adaptive role in
uncomfortable sensations of constriction that internal mentation. The Neuroscientist,
naturally accompany it. It also reveals how 18(3), 251–270. doi:https://doi.org/10.1177/
the system relaxes as interest and attention 1073858411403316b
are re-oriented to just sensation: an ease that Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer,
is associated with reduced neural monitoring C., & Vohs, K. D. (2001). Bad is stronger than
activity and downstream changes in arousal- good. Review of General Psychology, 5(4),
related biomarkers. 323. doi:http://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/
1089-2680.5.4.323
And while MT offers the opportunity for
Baumeister, R. F., & Heatherton, T. F. (1996).
profound insight, people come to it with Self-regulation failure: An overview. Psycho-
varying levels of interest and curiosity so logical Inquiry, 7(1), 1–15. doi:http://dx.doi.
that each gets off at their own stop along the org/10.1207/s15327965pli0701_1
route. In the clinical and secular settings into Bieling, P. J., Hawley, L. L., Bloch, R. T., Corco-
which mindfulness has been introduced it ran, K. M., Levitan, R. D., Young, L. T., &
is valued primarily for the palliative effects Segal, Z. V. (2012). Treatment-specific
104 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

changes in decentering following mindfulness- mechanisms of mindfulness in a mindfulness-


based cognitive therapy versus antidepres- based stress reduction program. Journal of
sant medication or placebo for prevention of Clinical Psychology, 65(6), 613–626.
depressive relapse. Journal of Consulting and doi:10.1002/jclp.20579
Clinical Psychology, 80(3), 365. doi:http:// Cavanagh, K., Strauss, C., Forder, L., & Jones, F.
psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/a0027483 (2014). Can mindfulness and acceptance be
Biswas-Diener, R., Linley, P. A., Dovey, H., learnt by self-help? A systematic review and
Maltby, J., Hurling, R., Wilkinson, J., & Lyub- meta-analysis of mindfulness and acceptance-
chik, N. (2015). Pleasure: An initial explora- based self-help interventions. Clinical Psychol-
tion. Journal of Happiness Studies, 16(2), ogy Review, 34(2), 118–129. doi:https://doi.
313–332. doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/ org/10.1016/j.cpr.2014.01.001
s10902-014-9511-x Cebolla, A., Campos, D., Galiana, L., Oliver, A.,
Blair, C. (2002). School readiness: Integrating Tomás, J. M., Feliu-Soler, A., & Baños, R. M.
cognition and emotion in a neurobiological (2017). Exploring relations among mindful-
conceptualization of children’s functioning at ness facets and various meditation practices:
school entry. American Psychologist, 57(2), Do they work in different ways? Conscious-
111. doi:http://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/ ness and Cognition, 49, 172–180. doi:https://
0003-066X.57.2.111 doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2017.01.012
Brosschot, J. F., Gerin, W., & Thayer, J. F. (2006). Chan, D., & Woollacott, M. (2007). Effects of
The perseverative cognition hypothesis: A level of meditation experience on attentional
review of worry, prolonged stress-related focus: Is the efficiency of executive or orien-
physiological activation, and health. tation networks improved? The Journal of
Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 60(2), Alternative and Complementary Medicine,
113–124. 13(6), 651–658. doi:10.1089/acm.2007.7022
Brosschot, J., Geurts, S., Kruizinga, I., Radstaak, Cladder-Micus, M., van Aalderen, J., Donders,
M., Verkuil, B., Quirin, M., & Kompier, M. A., Spijker, J., Vrijsen, J., & Speckens, A.
(2014). Does unconscious stress play a role in (2017). Cognitive reactivity as outcome and
prolonged cardiovascular stress recovery? working mechanism of mindfulness-based
Stress and Health: Journal of the Interna- cognitive therapy for recurrently depressed
tional Society for the Investigation of Stress, patients in remission. Cognition and Emo-
30(3), 179. doi:10.1002/smi.2590 tion, 1–8. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
Campbell, T. S., Labelle, L. E., Bacon, S. L., 02699931.2017.1285753
Faris, P., & Carlson, L. E. (2012). Impact of Creswell, J. D., Bursley, J. K., & Satpute, A. B.
mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) (2013). Neural reactivation links unconscious
on attention, rumination and resting blood thought to decision-making performance.
pressure in women with cancer: A waitlist- Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience,
controlled study. Journal of Behavioral Medi- 8(8), 863–869. doi:https://doi.org/10.1093/
cine, 35(3), 262–271. doi:https://doi. scan/nst004
org/10.1007/s10865-011-9357-1 Custers, R., & Aarts, H. (2010). The unconscious
Carmody, J. (2016). Fish Discovering Water: will: How the pursuit of goals operates outside
Meditation as a Process of Recognition. In M. of conscious awareness. Science, 329(5987),
West (Ed.), The Psychology of Meditation. 47–50. doi:10.1126/science.1188595
Oxford University Press. Dahl, C. J., Lutz, A., & Davidson, R. J. (2015).
Carmody, J., & Baer, R. A. (2008). Relationships Reconstructing and deconstructing the self:
between mindfulness practice and levels of Cognitive mechanisms in meditation practice.
mindfulness, medical and psychological Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(9), 515–523.
symptoms and well-being in a mindfulness- doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.07.001
based stress reduction program. Journal of Damasio, A. R. (2003). Looking for Spinoza: Joy,
Behavioral Medicine, 31(1), 23–33. doi:https:// sorrow, and the feeling brain. New York:
doi.org/10.1007/s10865-007-9130-7 Harcourt.
Carmody, J., Baer, R. A., Lykins, E. L. B., & Deikman, A. J. (1982). The observing self: Mysti-
Olendzki, N. (2009). An empirical study of the cism and psychotherapy. Boston: Beacon Press.
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND MINDFULNESS AND MEDITATION 105

Donald, J. N., Atkins, P. W., Parker, P. D., Christie, Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimen-
A. M., & Ryan, R. M. (2016). Daily stress and tal Psychiatry, 44(3), 322–328. doi:https://
the benefits of mindfulness: Examining the doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2013.01.007
daily and longitudinal relations between pre- James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology.
sent-moment awareness and stress responses. New York: H. Holt.
Journal of Research in Personality, 65, 30–37. Jha, A., Krompinger, J., & Baime, M. J. (2007).
doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2016.09.002 Mindfulness training modifies subsystems of
Farb, N. A. S., Segal, Z. V., Mayberg, H., Bean, attention. Cognitive Affective and Behavioral
J., McKeon, D., Fatima, Z., & Anderson, A. K. Neuroscience, 7(2), 109–119. doi:https://doi.
(2007). Attending to the present: Mindful- org/10.3758/CABN.7.2.109
ness meditation reveals distinct neural modes Kabat-Zinn, J. (1982). An out-patient program
of self-reference. Social Cognitive and Affec- in behavioral medicine for chronic pain
tive Neuroscience, 2(4), 313–322. doi:https:// patients based on the practice of mindful-
doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsm030 ness meditation: Theoretical considerations
Feldman, G., Greeson, J., & Senville, J. (2010). and preliminary results. General Hospital
Differential effects of mindful breathing, pro- Psychiatry, 4, 33–47.
gressive muscle relaxation, and loving-kindness Kabat-Zinn, J. (1987). Four-year follow-up of a
meditation on decentering and negative reac- meditation-based program for the self-­
tions to repetitive thoughts. Behaviour regulation of chronic pain; treatment out-
Research and Therapy, 48(10), 1002–1011. comes and compliance. Clinical Journal of
doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2010.06.006 Pain, 2, 159–173.
Füstös, J., Gramann, K., Herbert, B. M., & Kabat-Zinn, J. (1990). Full catastrophe living:
Pollatos, O. (2013). On the embodiment of Using the wisdom of your body and mind to
emotion regulation: Interoceptive awareness face stress, pain and illness. New York:
facilitates reappraisal. Social Cognitive and Delacorte.
Affective Neuroscience, 8(8), 911–917. Killingsworth, M. A., & Gilbert, D. T. (2010). A
doi:https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nss089 wandering mind is an unhappy mind.
Geary, D. (2005). The motivation to control Science, 330(6006), 932. doi:10.1126/science.
and the origin of mind: Exploring the life- 1192439
mind joint point in the Tree of Knowledge Kirschenbaum, D. S. (1987). Self-regulatory fail-
System. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 61(1), ure: A review with clinical implications. Clinical
21–46. doi:10.1002/jclp.20089 Psychology Review, 7(1), 77–104. doi:https://
Goyal, M., Singh, S., Sibinga, E. M., Gould, N. doi.org/10.1016/0272-7358(87)90005-5
F., Rowland-Seymour, A., Sharma, R., & Lao, S., Kissane, D., & Meadows, G. (2016).
Shihab, H. M. (2014). Meditation programs Cognitive effects of MBSR/MBCT: A
for psychological stress and well-being: A systematic review of neuropsychological
systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA outcomes. Consciousness and Cognition,
Internal Medicine, 174(3), 357–368. 45, 109–123. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/
doi:10.1001/jamainternmed.2013.13018 j.concog.2016.08.017
Hayes, S. C., Wilson, K. G., Gifford, E. V., Lazar, S. W., Kerr, C. E., Wasserman, R. H.,
Follette, V. M., & Strosahl, K. (1996). Experien- Gray, J. R., Greve, D. N., Treadway, M. T., &
tial avoidance and behavioral disorders: A Fischl, B. (2005). Meditation experience is
functional dimensional approach to diagnosis associated with increased cortical thickness.
and treatment. Journal of Consulting & Clinical NeuroReport 16(17), 1893–1897.
Psychology, 64(6), 1152–1168. doi:http:// Levinson, D. B., Stoll, E. L., Kindy, S. D., Merry,
psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0022-006X. H. L., & Davidson, R. J. (2014). A mind you
64.6.1152 can count on: Validating breath counting as
Huffziger, S., Ebner-Priemer, U., Eisenbach, C., a behavioral measure of mindfulness.
Koudela, S., Reinhard, I., Zamoscik, V., & Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1202. doi:https://
Kuehner, C. (2013). Induced ruminative and dx.doi.org/10.3389%2Ffpsyg.2014.01202
mindful attention in everyday life: An Lutz, J., Brühl, A. B., Doerig, N., Scheerer, H.,
­experimental ambulatory assessment study. Achermann, R., Weibel, A., & Herwig, U.
106 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

(2016). Altered processing of self-related poorer cognitive control and internalizing


emotional stimuli in mindfulness meditators. psychopathology. Clinical Psychological Sci-
Neuroimage, 124, 958–967. doi:https://doi. ence, 4(6), 1047–1064. doi:https://doi.org/
org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.09.057 10.1177/2167702616633157t
Mehling, W. E., Price, C., Daubenmier, J. J., Tang, Y.-Y., Hölzel, B. K., & Posner, M. I. (2015).
Acree, M., Bartmess, E., & Stewart, A. The neuroscience of mindfulness meditation.
(2012). The multidimensional assessment of Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16(4), 213–225.
interoceptive awareness (MAIA). PloS One, doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn3916
7(11), e48230. Teasdale, J. D., Moore, R. G., Hayhurst, H.,
Ortner, C. N. M., Kilner, S. J., & Zelazo, P. D. Pope, M., Williams, S., & Segal, Z. V. (2002).
(2007). Mindfulness meditation and reduced Metacognitive awareness and prevention of
emotional interference on a cognitive task. relapse in depression: Empirical evidence.
Motivation and Emotion, 31(4), 271–283. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychol-
doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-007- ogy, 70(2), 275–287. doi:http://psycnet.apa.
9076-7 org/doi/10.1037/0022-006X.70.2.275
Pessoa, L. (2008). On the relationship between Thayer, J. A., Friedman, B. H., & Borkovec, T. D.
emotion and cognition. Nature Reviews Neu- (1996). Autonomic characteristics of gener-
roscience, 9(2), 148–158. alized anxiety disorder and worry. Biological
Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2007). Psychiatry, 39(4), 255–266. doi:https://doi.
Research on attention networks as a model org/10.1016/0006-3223(95)00136-0
for the integration of psychological science. Tomlin, D., Rand, D. G., Ludvig, E. A., & Cohen,
Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 1–23. J. D. (2015). The evolution and devolution of
doi:https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych. cognitive control: The costs of deliberation in a
58.110405.085516 competitive world. Scientific Reports, 5, 11002.
Sapolsky, R. M. (2004). Why zebras don’t get doi:https://dx.doi.org/10.1038%2Fsrep11002
ulcers: The acclaimed guide to stress, stress- Watkins, E. (2004). Appraisals and strategies
related diseases, and coping (revised and associated with rumination and worry.
updated). New York: Macmillan. Personality and Individual Differences, 37(4).
Saunders, B., Rodrigo, A. H., & Inzlicht, M. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2003.10.002
(2016). Mindful awareness of feelings increases Wells, A. (1999). A meta-cognitive model and
neural performance monitoring. Cognitive, therapy for generalized anxiety disorder. Clini-
Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 16(1), cal Psychology and Psychotherapy, 6, 86–95.
93–105. doi:https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415- doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-0879(199905)
015-0375-2 6:2<86::AID-CPP189>3.0.CO;2-S
Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M. G., & Teasdale, J. D. Willem, K., Warren, F., Taylor, R., Whalley, B.,
(2013). Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy Crane, C., Bondolfi, G., & Schweizer, S.
for depression (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford. (2016). Efficacy and moderators of
Shonin, E., Van Gordon, W., & Griffiths, M. D. ­mindfulness-based cognitive therapy (MBCT)
(2015). Does mindfulness work? BMJ, 1, in prevention of depressive relapse: An
1–11. individual patient data meta-analysis from
Snyder, H. R., & Hankin, B. L. (2016). Spiraling randomized trials. JAMA Psychiatry. 73(6),
out of control: Stress generation and subse- 565–574. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/
quent rumination mediate the link between jamapsychiatry.2016.0076
5
Evolutionary Psychology and
Environmental Sciences
Ulysses Paulino Albuquerque, Joelson M. B. Moura,
Risoneide Henriques da Silva, Washington S.
F e r r e i r a J ú n i o r , a n d Ta l i n e C . S i l v a

INTRODUCTION previously selected mental mechanisms


allowed the assessment of the landscape,
An investigative program of evolutionary even if unconsciously (see Zajonc, 1980;
psychology must address the origin of human Townsend and Barton, 2018).
beings, as the environments that generated If we assume that the savanna was the
pressure during the evolution of early hominids ‘main paleoenvironment’ of our evolution
form the basis for premises about the evolution and that we inherited both physical and men-
of the human mind. As evidence suggests the tal adaptations of our ancestors, the above-
African savanna as the most likely place for the mentioned argument is coherent. However,
origin of the modern human being, the savanna the literature indicates that we evolved along
environment usually receives more emphasis different lineages and from myriad hominid
than other environments. groups that coexisted in a wide range of envi-
We have succeeded as a species through
ronments, and that more than one point of
mental specializations, also understood as
origin of H. sapiens may have existed (Foley
evolved psychological mechanisms. These
et  al., 2016; Stringer, 2016). Evolution may
specializations were selected because they
solved problems in paleoenvironments, and have occurred independently in different
were inherited by subsequent generations areas, with hominids developing morpholog-
of hominids. For example, when exploring ical substructures that resulted in a complete
new landscapes, early hominids needed to set of H. sapiens characteristics. Stringer
quickly identify potentially dangerous situa- (2016) calls this independent evolution
tions, which trees were climbable, and where ‘African multiregionalism’, characterized by
they could shelter. These decisions needed to interfertile subdivisions of H. sapiens in their
be fast, and they were only possible because evolutionary history across Africa.
108 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

This discussion is central to a program that occurred between 350,000 and 260,000 years
attempts to investigate the evolution of the ago (Schlebusch et al., 2017). In addition, a
human mind and human behavior. Discussing hominid skull, dating back about 436,000–
the origin of human beings is essential for 390,000 years, was recently discovered in the
evolutionary psychology and for understand- Cave of Aroeira in Portugal, reinforcing the
ing how our minds work in relation to the idea that human origins did not necessarily
environment and all its components. occur in Africa (López-García et  al., 2018).
Although the savanna is still regarded as the
main setting of our evolution, these findings
suggest that the origin and the great divisions
THE ORIGIN AND EVOLUTION in the family of hominids may have occurred
OF HUMANS outside Africa.
Nonetheless, if we understand that estab-
The transition from dense and closed forests lishment in the savanna was important for the
to the savanna may have occurred slowly. survival of hominids, it is reasonable to infer
This suggests that early hominids left the that, over time, natural selection favored indi-
canopy of forest trees gradually, venturing viduals better adapted to savanna conditions.
into the East African savanna to explore avail- These individuals inherited the anatomical
able resources and to identify hazards and safe and cognitive apparatus evolved in this envi-
sleeping places. Thus, arboreal behaviors may ronment and were most likely to survive and
have coexisted with bipedal locomotion (see leave offspring. This moment was crucial in
Townsend and Barton, 2018). Lucy, the most evolutionary history. Many aspects of our
famous Australopithecus afarensis, had both anatomy and current behaviors resulted from
bipedal and arboreal habits (Larson, 2012). solutions to challenges faced by early homi-
Until recently, paleontological data sug- nids (Townsend and Barton, 2018).
gested that the first hominids appeared in Townsend and Barton (2018) argue that
Central Africa seven million years ago (Ma) common behaviors and anatomical adapta-
(see Böhme et  al., 2017). However, recent tions of early hominids in the Pleistocene
evidence suggests that the earliest homi- persist today. For example, the palmar grasp
nid, Graecopithecus freybergi, lived in a reflex is a primitive reflex. It consists of a
savanna environment in the region of Greece, strong pressure with the hands and represents
between 7.37 and 7.11 Ma, which is 200,000 the primate’s need to hold the mother’s skin
years earlier than the previous earliest known as she moves through the canopy of the trees.
hominid, Sahelanthropus tchadensis, found During childhood, for example, there is a ten-
in Africa (Böhme et  al., 2017). Likewise, dency for children to show climbing behaviors
Homo sapiens was believed to have origi- (e.g., climbing trees or climbable objects) that
nated around 200,000 years ago in South fit the category of primitive reflex (Townsend
Africa, but recent fossil evidence suggests and Barton, 2018). Brachiation is also still
that H. sapiens appeared about 315,000 used today by children and gymnasts and
years ago in Morocco, 100,000 years earlier was essential for the hominids in the savanna
than previously thought (Hublin et al., 2017; paleoenvironments. Perhaps children use tree
Richter et al., 2017). These fossils have a mix climbing as strategy to avoid predators (Coss
of characteristics of H. sapiens fossils from and Moore, 2002). Brachiation refers to a
other parts of Africa, indicating a multicen- mobility method that depends on the specific
tric genesis for our species (see Hublin et al., structure of the shoulder to hang on the tree
2017; Richter et  al., 2017). This finding is limb and allow the arm to swing in a com-
consistent with genetic evidence that the first plete circle (Townsend and Barton, 2018).
divergence of modern human populations These authors also suggest that the standard
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND ENVIRONMENTAL SCIENCES 109

size of the human hand is proportional to the psychologists would be to broaden the
size of tree branches capable of supporting a ­spectrum of the environments in which we
human’s weight during a climb. In addition, evolved and the influence of the evolved psy-
humans generally prefer horizontal-branched chological mechanisms in solving problems
trees precisely because it facilitates climbing. different from those found in the savanna. If
These behaviors can be understood as an the human mind has evolved in response to the
ancestral inheritance of early hominids, with difficulties imposed by the environment, and
our cognition, like our anatomy, resulting if H. sapiens has emerged and evolved in dif-
from adaptations to the selective pressures ferent environments, it is possible that today’s
of paleoenvironments (Tooby and Cosmides, human behavioral responses, including their
2015). Blome et al. (2012) demonstrated that preferences, are a reflection of this multicen-
the African paleoclimate from 150,000 to tric origin. Indeed, the paleontological evi-
30,000 years ago also displayed regional varia- dence that hominids inhabited and explored
tion, so that periods of high aridity or humidity several environments in the Pleistocene sug-
did not occur simultaneously in the north- gests that other psychological mechanisms
ern, eastern, tropical, and southern regions of may have evolved in periods before or after
Africa. According to these authors, this cli- establishment in the savanna. For example, a
mate heterogeneity may have created oppor- recent study by our research group has shown
tunities for hominids to migrate to adjacent that some people, when analyzing landscapes
regions. Furthermore, Coulthard et al. (2013) of savanna, rainforest, tundra, desert, conif-
found that, in humid climates around 100,000 erous forest, deciduous forest, and urban
years ago, major African river systems flowed landscape, prefer images of exuberant green
northward, across the Sahara and to the rainforests (Moura et  al., 2018). In addition,
Mediterranean Sea. These authors believe that people living in Spain tend to prefer densely
three now-buried rivers could have been active green and closed landscapes (Hartmann and
in the period of human migration across the Apaolaza-Ibáñez, 2010). Because this land-
Sahara, with the abundance of water resources scape is typical of Spain, and in Brazil there
creating viable migratory routes for humans. is great media appeal to preserve the Amazon
Evidence shows that hominids adapted to rainforest, these findings suggest that recent
various environments in a wide latitudinal stimuli, rather than innate responses, may
range, such as the temperate and subtropi- exert strong influence on human behavior.
cal north of China and tropical regions of According to Barrett (2012), adaptations
Southeast Asia (Roberts et  al., 2016; Kong may provide plasticity to the human mind.
et al., 2018). The use of fire, which is a prac- They may also integrate mechanisms – whether
tice frequently described in the literature on more general or more specific – shaped by
arid environments, has also been observed evolutionary history with those shaped by
in tropical forests (Friesem et  al., 2017). In the ontogenetic development of the indi-
addition, traces of foraging activities and vidual. Therefore, our mental mechanisms
the discovery of tools for hunting arboreal may be heterogeneous in origin, with new
animals challenge the dominant idea of the structures evolving from older structures and
evolutionary adaptation of the first humans to ancestral features combining with relatively
the arid environment of the savanna (Barker recent characteristics (Barrett, 2012). Thus,
et al., 2007; Friesem et al., 2017). cognitive adaptations are not necessarily the
If cognitive mechanisms result from result of responses to difficulties imposed by
responses to selective pressures of the envi- a specific environment. They might reflect
ronment, much of our mind may also be the selection of general strategies of the
‘trapped’ in evolutionary environments. If human mind to meet challenges in different
this is true, a challenge for evolutionary environments.
110 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

THE ENVIRONMENT OF 1992, 2005), and the reconstruction of these


EVOLUTIONARY ADAPTEDNESS (EEA) selective environments could indicate why
AND THE STRUCTURING OF THE humans have propensities for certain types of
HUMAN MIND thoughts, motivations, and behaviors (Foley,
1996). However, the previously mentioned
Understanding the evolutionary environment evidence of evolution of hominids in different
of hominids is crucial for evolutionary psy- areas of the African continent seems to chal-
chology and other disciplines interested in the lenge the savanna hypothesis (see Bolhuis
evolution of the human mind. For example, et al., 2011). Thus, the human EEA may com-
Bowlby (1982) coined the term Environment prise a multitude of geographic and temporal
of Evolutionary Adaptedness (EEA) to refer environments (Volk and Atkinson, 2013), that
to the environment that selected the current is, the EEA has become less specific, taking
genotypes of an organism. According to this into account not only the African savanna (see
perspective, it is reasonable to suppose that Tooby and Cosmides, 2015), but also the other
these environments also influenced the selec- selective environments in which humans have
tion of mental traits of human beings. Frost lived over the course of their evolution. As a
(2011) proposed that, for humans, the EEA consequence, humans may have developed
would be represented by the African savanna psychological mechanisms in environments
of the Pleistocene, the environment probably that were different from the African savanna
occupied by early H. sapiens before they (see Hartmann and Apaolaza-Ibáñez, 2010,
started migrating to other continents about 2013; Moura et al., 2018) (Figure 5.1).
50,000 years ago. Many authors argue that Studies on human preference for land-
human psychological mechanisms evolved in scapes, for example, provide evidence that
response to the stable characteristics of the these psychological mechanisms may have
savanna environments (Tooby and Cosmides, suffered interference from the interaction

Figure 5.1  Environment of Evolutionary Adaptedness definition (EEA), original version and
extended version
Source: Created by the authors.
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND ENVIRONMENTAL SCIENCES 111

of humans and different ancestral environ- information on the name and diet of dangerous
ments. Orians (1980) argues that, because the animals were presented.
savanna is an open environment, it enabled
the first hominids a more accurate perception
of approaching predators. This suggests the Did Evolution Endow Us with a
evolution of a psychological mechanism in Naturalistic Mind?
humans to prefer savanna landscapes – the
savanna hypothesis. However, several studies We believe that many of today’s human deci-
have reported preference in humans for envi- sions and behaviors are influenced by the
ronments other than African savannas (see same psychological mechanisms present in
Han, 2007; Hartmann and Apaolaza-Ibáñez, our ancestors. An example of this would be
2010, 2013; Moura et al., 2018). the ability to recall information relevant to
Some studies have tried to understand how survival (see Nairne et  al., 2007), such as
our species recalls information that is relevant snakes and spiders (see Young et al., 2012).
to survival, providing evidence of how the However, we also believe that some of these
human mind may have evolved psychologi- ancestral psychological mechanisms have
cal mechanisms that deal with risky situations adjusted to the adversities of new environ-
in different environments. Yang et  al. (2014) ments humans occupied throughout their
observed that people in both ancestral survival evolution. Consequently, derived psychologi-
scenarios – grasslands and in non-ancestral cal mechanisms evolved from ancestral psy-
or modern environments – recalled important chological mechanisms. The ability to recall
words in a survival situation. In another study, information that refers to a risky situation in
Young et al. (2012) found that threats in mod- a modern environment, such as firearms and
ern environments – such as firearms and cars – cars, is evidence of a psychological mecha-
capture and maintain attention in the same nism adjusted to the reality of contemporary
way as would be expected for threats in ances- environments (see Young et al., 2012).
tral environments, such as snakes and spiders. Barrett (2012) relativizes the influence of
These findings lead us to believe that natural ancestral environments in the present – in the
selection favored psychological mechanisms case of evolved psychological mechanisms
that deal with challenges regardless of the shaped in these environments – considering
type of environment. Thus, human inventions innate cognitive modules as mechanisms spe-
(e.g., firearms and cars) that are immersed in cialized to solve a specific adaptive problem.
the culture and environment may be acting as However, if adaptations in the brain are analo-
a selective force that activates modern psy- gous to adaptations of the body, such as tissue
chological mechanisms. This fact seems to types, they are likely to be heterogeneous and
indicate that the human construction of niche1 hierarchical (Barrett, 2012). A hierarchical
interferes in own and others’ psychological organization is a feature of systems that evolve
mechanisms. This interpretation finds sup- and develop new structures from older struc-
port in reports that psychological mechanisms tures. These adaptations are, therefore, a com-
that favor the recall of information relevant to bination of ancestral and recent traits (Barrett,
survival can be observed in people occupying 2012). Thus, mental adaptations can be con-
different contemporary environmental and cul- structed during the ontogenetic development
tural contexts (see Barrett and Broesch, 2012; of each individual (see Barrett, 2012). As
Barrett et al., 2016). For example, Barrett and changes in the individual’s social environment
Broesch (2012) found that children living in the occur, there may be a selection of ‘dormant’
city of Los Angeles in California and children behaviors or preferences that would never or
of a village in Shuar in the Ecuadorian Amazon rarely be generated by the brain if the environ-
had high levels of recall when images of and ment remained static (see Barrett, 2012).
112 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

A study by Sandry et al. (2013) provided This evidence suggests that psychologi-
evidence of the hierarchical organization of cal mechanisms evolved through processes of
the human mind: these authors demonstrated descent with modification, indicating the for-
that people do not recall adaptive information mation of human cognition by a combination
in a similar way. By studying the memoriza- of ancestral and derived psychological mecha-
tion of words in different scenarios – survival, nisms (Barrett, 2012). This should make brain
fear and phobia, partner selection, incest processes highly heterogeneous and possibly
avoidance, detection of cheaters, jealousy, hierarchically organized, with information
infidelity, and acquisition and maintenance of organized in human memory according to its
status – they found that the survival scenario relevance in dealing with imposed environmen-
excelled in the number of words remembered tal adversity. Then, some ancestral or derived
by people when compared to the other sce- psychological mechanism, or both simultane-
narios (which were also considered adaptive). ously, would be activated (Figure 5.2).
If human memory were a non-hierarchical Albuquerque and Ferreira Júnior (2017)
system, all these psychological mechanisms argue that evolution has provided us with
should have promoted recall equally. a naturalistic mind that evolved to account

Figure 5.2  Structure of the human naturalist mind: Scheme I shows a cladogram with the
ancestral and derived psychological mechanisms that constitute the human mind. Scheme II
illustrates the ancestral and derivative psychological mechanisms distributed in the human
mind and how they can be activated; the psychological mechanisms are hierarchically
organized according to their relevance to deal with environmental adversity
Source: Created by the authors.
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND ENVIRONMENTAL SCIENCES 113

for myriad and complex relationships and frequency of illness with previous experience
challenges that the environment poses for suggests that there is, in fact, a hierarchy in
our species. Challenges include what to eat the mind. Interestingly, a similar pattern is
(e.g., plants and animals), where to look for observed in relation to other phenomena,
food, how to treat diseases or cope with acci- such as when people deal with environmental
dents with the resources provided by nature, hazards or catastrophes (see Ruin et al., 2007;
where to take refuge, and how to avoid preda- Miceli et  al., 2008; Gibbons and Groarke,
tors and poisonous animals. The naturalistic 2016). This led Ferreira Júnior et al. (2019)
mind, as one of the components of the human to formulate the Principle of Regularity.
mind, would also result from the numerous According to this principle, the human mind
selective pressures of the ancestral or mod- is biased, because it is organized based on
ern environment to which our species is the regular events of our experience. Box 5.1
subjected. One of the first studies using the summarizes what we know about the natural-
concept of naturalistic mind found that the istic mind.
human mind favors the recovery and stor-
age of information about diseases and plants
associated with their treatment, when these Does the Past Explain the Present?
diseases are frequent in the social system Human Beings and Landscapes
(common diseases) or related to previous
experiences of the individual (Silva et  al., Landscapes can be defined as the space of
2019). The authors expected to find that seri- interaction of people and environment. The
ous diseases, those normally debilitating or way humans relate to them may reveal strong
fatal, would be favored in memory. However, evolutionary roots. The Biophilia hypothesis
this was not the case. The modulation of the proposes that people possess an innate

BOX 5.1 Structure and behavior of the human naturalistic mind 

Origin
• The naturalistic mind is the fruit of all selective pressures along the hominid lineage in evolutionary
environments. Thus, evolved psychological mechanisms respond to different environmental challenges,
that is, they are not necessarily tied to a particular environment (such as the Pleistocene savanna).
• Memory, as one of the components of the naturalistic mind, prioritizes content with adaptive bias,
organizing content hierarchically. Thus, information related to environmental survival can be prior-
itized over other adaptive information. This means that ancestral hazards will not necessarily be
prioritized to the detriment of modern hazards.

Physiology
• The naturalistic mind, as shaped during evolution, can lead our species to experience adaptive lags.
However, as cultural responses operate faster than biological evolution, human activities of niche
construction can modulate the existence or not of adaptive lags.
• Possible mental responses generated in the ancestral environment can be modulated by the individual’s
previous experience with a given phenomenon.
• The frequency (regularity) of a given phenomenon biases cognitive processes associated with the
naturalistic mind, so that less common or rare phenomena tend to be neglected unless they are
modulated by previous experience.
114 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

emotional and affective predisposition to Sommer, 1997; Summit and Sommer, 1999;
living things, whether they are animals, Herzog et al., 2000; Falk and Balling, 2010).
plants, or processes (Wilson, 1993). People Orians and Heerwagen (1992) suggest that
tend to prefer images of natural environments this preference has an evolutionary origin and
to urban environments and these images are results from the importance of the savanna
more likely to be preferred when they contain environment for hominid survival during the
trees (Ulrich, 1993). In addition, humans pro- Pleistocene. The savanna offered the first
cess visual stimuli from nature more effi- humans a set of possibilities (e.g., a pano-
ciently than from urban environments. This ramic view of the open environment and trees
can elicit favorable feelings and emotions to that were easy to climb) that helped them to
natural landscapes (Townsend and Barton, escape from predators, search for food, and
2018). For example, when contemplating a shelter under their canopies (Appleton, 1975;
landscape, whether urban or natural, people Orians and Heerwagen, 1992; Townsend and
elicit emotional responses that may lead to Barton, 2018). The savanna may have played
positive or negative attitudes towards that a relevant role during human evolution, but
landscape (see Bargh et al., 1992). not the most prominent. Since we inhabited
Evidence has shown that some people other environments with different challenges
appreciate landscapes containing lakes or in the Pleistocene, survival strategies for
rivers (Ulrich, 1983), and feel more freedom the savanna have been potentially modified,
in environments with exuberantly green veg- improved, and combined with other strate-
etation than in urban landscapes (Hartmann gies or even abandoned over time.
and Apaolaza-Ibáñez, 2010). These affective According to Tooby and Cosmides (2015),
reactions can be the result of aesthetic aspects the period during which we were hunter-­
of the landscape (e.g., perceived naturalness – gatherers in paleoenvironments was crucial
how close a given landscape is to its natural for the evolution of our mind. Evolutionary
state – presence of water, complexity) or processes are slow and need hundreds of
of evolutionary aspects, as proposed by the generations to build a highly complex ‘men-
Biophilia hypothesis (Wilson, 1993; Han, tal’ program. That is, human minds would
2007; Ode et al., 2009; Lee and Son, 2017). still be adapted to the world of our ancestors.
The affinity of human beings with living According to these authors, ‘The industrial
elements may be the result of the continuing revolution – even the agricultural revolution –
relationship of hominids with nature dur- is too brief a period to have selected for new
ing their evolutionary history. This affinity neurocomputational programs of any com-
may influence aspects of human cognition plexity’ (Tooby and Cosmides, 2015: 19).
related to the use and management of natu- People commonly experience an adaptive
ral resources (Albuquerque and Ferreira delay when facing the challenges of industrial-
Júnior, 2017) as well as emotional responses ized societies, because these environments are
and preferences for aesthetic components different from the environment in which we
of nature. In this case, some natural sce- evolved. For example, the taste for fatty foods
narios stand out more than others (Orians is an adaptive behavior for ancestral environ-
and Heerwagen, 1992). For example, stud- ments, in which fat was scarce, but is non-
ies have shown that inhabitants of countries adaptive in the current environment because it
including Australia, Nigeria, South Africa, increases the incidence of cardiovascular dis-
the United States, Estonia, and Italy, among eases (Cosmides and Tooby, 2003).
others, prefer open landscapes with sparse The influence of past heritage on the inter-
trees with wide and stratified canopy, which action of people and landscapes and, conse-
are characteristic of the African Pleistocene quently, the existence of adaptive lags has
savanna (see Orians and Heerwagen, 1992; been debated by scholars. Some argue that this
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND ENVIRONMENTAL SCIENCES 115

argument overlooks evolutionary processes strategies such as trial and error may have
that have enabled the reproductive success of been important in order to avoid such threats
humans, such as the ability to adjust to vary- over time. However, trial and error are not
ing and variable environments in which they always advantageous because the learning
live (see Laland and O’Brien, 2012). Laland costs increase in situations such as contact
and Brown (2006) argue that humans do not with poisonous animals. In this context, strat-
experience adaptive lags, precisely because egies that favor the learning of certain infor-
they have the ability to build and rebuild key mation from the environment may have been
components of their environments to suit selected (see Rendell et al., 2011; Barrett and
their needs. This adaptive capacity of humans Broesch, 2012). For example, early hominids
and other organisms is understood as niche that remembered and quickly learned how to
construction, which occurs in response to the avoid certain components of the environment
environmental challenges created by their (e.g., dangerous animals) and how to recog-
ancestors (see Lewontin, 1982; Odling-Smee nize and select natural resources (e.g., fruits)
et al., 2003; Laland and Brown, 2006; Laland would have an advantage over others who did
and O’Brien, 2012). For example, even if not possess such skills or behaviors.
there is excessive consumption of fatty foods, In the Biophilia hypothesis, Wilson (1993)
humans create niches to solve this problem, proposes that the behaviors of approaching
such as the development of drugs and the (biophilia) and avoiding (biophobia) certain
practice of physical exercise. components of the environment may have a
Due to the cultural and environmental biological, evolutionary basis. These behav-
diversity in which we live and develop, the iors result from natural selection to promote
preference for landscape among humans is the survival of humans in their interactions
also diverse. For example, the preference with the environment (see also Kellert,
among the Japanese for landscapes of feudal 1993). Chief among the biophilic interactions
gardens in urban centers varies according to is the demand for food in the ancestral envi-
the distance to buildings and also to personal ronment. Rozin and Todd (2015) argue that,
life experience (Senoglu et al., 2018). Colley during human evolution, the need for food
and Craig (2019) observed that, in Scotland, and nutrients demanded more time and cog-
if people perceive a landscape as natural (i.e., nitive effort than other activities performed
with little human intervention), the emotional by hominids. Selecting food is critical to the
attachment to the landscape increases, influ- evolution of the human mind and structuring
encing their preferences. In addition, people the culture, but is not an easy task. This activ-
living in China prefer environments with ity requires caution in avoiding the ingestion
a balance between wild nature and human of toxins and other non-nutritive substances.
constructions, such as channeled streams in It was essential to differentiate the toxic
native vegetation (see Hu et al., 2019). Thus, from the nutritious. This has led humans to
recent environmental factors can influence specialize over time, through natural selec-
innate human preferences for landscapes. tion (Rozin and Todd, 2015). Although the
human–food interaction during evolution is
a promising way to understand our cognitive
How Does Our Mind Deal with structure – and a matter of great interest to
Information about Other Living evolutionary ethnobiology (see Albuquerque
and Ferreira Júnior, 2017) – this subject is
Things?
still neglected in the field of evolutionary
In their interactions with environments, psychology (Rozin and Todd, 2015).
humans had to deal with dangerous events Evidence suggests a bias in human mem-
that threatened ancestral survival. Learning ory towards learning information related to
116 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

dangerous animals, as we have previously mulatta – Cook and Mineka (1990) showed
mentioned. Broesch et  al. (2014) evaluated that young individuals can quickly acquire
memory retention of information related to fear of snakes merely by watching fear reac-
the danger of animals (e.g., if they were poi- tions of other individuals towards these ani-
sonous or not), diet, and habitat in indigenous mals in videos. However, observers were not
young people and adults of the Fiji Islands. afraid of flowers after watching edited videos
They observed that information about haz- displaying individuals who were afraid of
ard and toxicity was best retained by young these items. This suggests that fear is more
people, whereas adults showed no preferen- quickly learned when directed at danger-
tial retention for this information. In contrast, ous animals. Similarly, babies aged seven to
other studies have indicated that the use of 18 months paid more attention to snake
images of dangerous animals can increase images when they were associated with a
the retrieval and retention of information in frightened human voice than with a cheerful
adults (Kock et al., 2008; Riaz et al., 2018). voice (DeLoache and LoBue, 2009). Such
In addition to retrieving information from learning may have been important for the sur-
memory, humans may possess other charac- vival of early hominids, as individuals would
teristics that respond quickly to dangerous not need direct experience with dangerous
animals. Neurobiological studies have indi- animals to acquire the behavior of avoiding
cated that the amygdala of primates, includ- these animals.
ing humans, is able to tune visual areas of Adaptive mechanisms are also activated in
the brain to perceive fear-related stimuli (see relation to plants. Prokop and Fančovičová
Prokop and Randler, 2018). Studies on visual (2014) investigated children exposed to infor-
attention have shown that dangerous animals, mation on toxic and non-toxic plants associ-
such as lions and snakes, more quickly cap- ated with fruit images of different colors,
ture and maintain the attention of humans i.e., red and black for toxic plants and green
than non-dangerous animals (Yorzinski et al., for non-toxic plants. They found that plant
2014). Infants at five months of age stare information associated with red- and black-
longer at images that schematically represent colored fruits was better retained in children’s
a spider than images with random schemata memory, possibly due to their association
(Rakison and Derringer, 2008). This may with toxic fruits. A recent study showed that,
reflect an evolved response to detect more in a visual detection task, toxic plants were
quickly and to focus attention on danger- detected significantly sooner than non-toxic
ous animals (Yorzinski et  al., 2014; Prokop plants by humans (Prokop and Fančovičová,
and Randler, 2018). According to Tooby and 2019). The ability to recall information about
Cosmides (2015), this fact could also explain plant toxicity may have given humans the
modern phobias associated with these ani- ability to identify and avoid foods potentially
mals. However, these responses are culturally harmful to their survival.
modulated. For example, Maasai people in In addition to the behavior of aversion,
Kenya evaluated lions as aesthetically more people also exhibit behaviors that promote
attractive than hyenas (Pinho et  al., 2014). contact and interaction with animals and
Lions are of great cultural importance to the plants (biophilia). People have positive
Maasai people (Pinho et al., 2014), suggest- emotional responses towards animals with
ing that culture can partly modulate human certain characteristics (Prokop and Randler,
responses to dangerous animals. 2018). Martín-López et al. (2008) conducted
Aversion to dangerous animals can be a meta-analysis of 60 studies and showed that
learned quickly by observing the reactions of people are more likely to pay for animal con-
other individuals to these animals. In a study servation due to anthropocentric rather than
with laboratory-raised monkeys – Macaca scientific factors. Anthropocentric factors
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND ENVIRONMENTAL SCIENCES 117

include animal characteristics preferred by in current environmental events. For Penn,


people, such as length, weight, and eye size. population growth is one of the major current
Regarding plants, some studies have shown ecological issues – there are around seven
that people prefer landscapes of trees with billion people on the planet – and creating
larger canopies and shorter trunks (for a effective public policies to stabilize this
review, see Townsend and Barton, 2018). growth requires understanding the evolution-
It seems this preference may be produced ary roots of the problem. From an evolution-
by psychological adaptation. Individuals ary perspective, reproductive self-regulation
who had positive sensations in response to should be expected as the high demographic
these trees were selected because these trees rate disturbs population and environmental
offered them safety and shelter (Townsend balance – as advocated by the Demographic
and Barton, 2018). Transition Theory – as this adaptive response
The degree of interaction of people and provides the best chance for survival (see
nature may promote positive behaviors recent discussion in Brooks et  al., 2019;
directed at other animals. A study in China Salvati et al., 2019). However, in some tradi-
found that children living in rural areas and tional societies, reproductive success is posi-
having more contact with nature were more tively associated with wealth increase,
likely to protect and like animals (biophilia) whereas in rich developed countries, fertility
than children from urbanized regions (Zhang tends to fall as their people opt to have fewer
et  al., 2014). Zhang and colleagues suggest children to improve their quality of life
that contact of humans with nature can help (Penn, 2003). Therefore, more studies are
to promote conservation strategies. Sampaio necessary, analyzing the influence of evolu-
et  al. (2018) observed that the contact of tionary and cultural factors on current demo-
children with forests influenced their knowl- graphic dynamics.
edge of local biodiversity. The children were Another aspect to be considered when
encouraged to express their knowledge as developing policies to deal with long-term
drawings, and the authors found that chil- environmental threats is discounting the
dren who had more contact with forests also future, that is, the limitations humans have
had greater knowledge about native animals in considering environmental problems that
of the region. According to the authors, the may arise in a distant future, putting more
proximity of children to the forest drew atten- emphasis on the present day (Penn, 2003;
tion to the components of this environment, Henry et  al., 2017). In this case, natural
laying the foundations for the construction selection may have favored hominids that
of knowledge. This fact indicates that the discounted the future, as life expectancy
environment in which human beings live was relatively short and the future uncer-
can influence their cognition, and is respon- tain, making it crucial to focus on present
sible for promoting and mediating human needs, which increased individual survival
behaviors, such as being prone or not to pre- and reproductive success (Penn, 2003). Thus,
serve nature. a good conservation strategy would be to
associate time discount rates to nature con-
servation policies, as that may provide more
realistic expectations of human response to
FUTURE CHALLENGES FOR these policies (Henry et al., 2017).
ENVIRONMENTAL SCIENCES Moreover, humans tend to use natural
resources according to their own interests,
Penn (2003) offered a synthesis of a number putting societal interests in second place,
of evolutionary approaches that provide potentially leading to resource exhaustion
insight into human nature and its role (Penn, 2003). This kind of behavior was
118 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

proposed by Hardin (1968) as the tragedy ACKNOWLEDGMENTS


of the commons. For instance, a study by
Scheiter et al. (2019) showed that, in African This study was financed in part by the
rural savannas, people use open fields for Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal
pasture or hunting intensively, exceeding the de Nível Superior – Brasil (CAPES) –
optimum level. These authors propose this as Finance Code 001. We thank the CNPq for
an example of the tragedy of the commons the productivity grant awarded to UPA.
that will compromise ecosystem services in
the future. However, human populations
are able to adapt and to develop rules to effec- Note
tively manage common resources, avoid-
ing the tragedy of the commons (for a more 1  The process where organisms, through their
activities and decisions, modify their own and
complete argument, see Šestáková and
other’s environments. The environmental modifi-
Plichtová, 2019). cations generated by the niche constructors influ-
In this sense, it is essential to understand ence the selective pressures of their environment
the constraints and amplitude of the influence and can lead to changes in their metabolic, physi-
of the evolved psychological mechanisms ological, and behavioral activities (Odling-Smee
et  al., 2003; Laland and O’Brien, 2012; Flynn
in the relations of modern humans and the
et al., 2013; Matthews et al., 2014).
environment. Evolutionary psychology is
interested in better understanding not only
environmental problems and challenges
(having as background the attitudes of human REFERENCES
beings) but also how we model our multiple
relationships with nature. Penn (2003) Albuquerque, U.P. and Ferreira Júnior, W.S.,
argues that we are still moving towards an 2017. What do we study in evolutionary
understanding of environmental problems ethnobiology? Defining the theoretical basis
from an evolutionary perspective. In this for a research program. Evolutionary Biology,
chapter, we presented a brief overview of 44(2), pp.206–15.
how evolved psychological mechanisms help Appleton, J., 1975. The Experience of
to understand modern humans. However, Landscape. London: John Wiley & Sons.
Barrett, H.C., 2012. A hierarchical model of the
just as we move slowly in understanding
evolution of human brain specializations.
the environmental challenges generated by PNAS, 109(1), pp.10733–40.
human activity, our understanding of other Barrett, H.C. and Broesch, J., 2012. Prepared
aspects of the relationship between human social learning about dangerous animals in
beings and the environment is still in its children. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33,
infancy. pp.499–508.
We believe that dialogue with other fields Barrett, H.C., Peterson, C.D. and Frankenhuis,
in science can be fruitful for a better under- W.E., 2016. Mapping the cultural learnability
standing, from an evolutionary point of view, landscape of danger. Child Development,
of the relationship between humans and the 87(3), pp.770–81.
environment. An evolutionary perspective Bargh, J.A., Chaiken, S., Govender, R. and
Pratto, F., 1992. The generality of the
is not exclusive but provides an alternative
automatic attitude activation effect. Journal
or additional point of view. Evolutionary of Personality and Social Psychology, 62(6),
ethnobiology is a newly systematic science pp.893–912.
(Albuquerque and Ferreira Júnior, 2017) that Barker, G., Barton, H., Bird, M., Daly, P., Datan, I.,
shares this interest in understanding the eco- Dykes, A., Farr, L., Gilbertson, D., Harrisson, B.,
logical and evolutionary history behind our Hunt, C., Higham, T., Kealhofer, L., Krigbaum,
relations with the environment. J., Lewis, H., MacLaren, S., Paz, V., Pike, A.,
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND ENVIRONMENTAL SCIENCES 119

Piper, P., Pyatt, B., Rabett, R., Reynolds, T., Coulthard, T.J., Ramirez, J.A., Barton, N., Rog-
Rose, J., Rushworth, G., Stephens, M., erson, M. and Brucher, T., 2013. Were rivers
Stringer, C., Thompson, J. and Turney, C., flowing across the Sahara during the last
2007. The ‘human revolution’ in lowland interglacial? Implications for human migra-
tropical Southeast Asia: The antiquity and tion through Africa. PLoS ONE, 8(9), e74834.
behaviour of anatomically modern humans DeLoache, J.S. and LoBue, V., 2009. The
at Niah Cave (Sarawak, Borneo). Journal of narrow fellow in the grass: Human infants
Human Evolution, 52(3), pp.243–61. associate snakes and fear. Developmental
Blome, M.W., Cohen, A.S., Tryon, C.A., Brooks, Science, 12, pp.201–7.
A.S. and Russell, J., 2012. The environmental Falk, J.H. and Balling, J.D., 2010. Evolutionary
context for the origins of modern human influence on human landscape preference.
diversity: A synthesis of regional variability in Environment and Behavior, 42(4),
African climate 150,000–30,000 years ago. pp.479–93.
Journal of Human Evolution, 62, pp.563–92. Ferreira Júnior, W.S., Medeiros, P.M. and
Böhme, M., Spassov, N., Ebner, M. and Albuquerque, U.P., 2019. Evolutionary
Geraads, D., 2017. Messinian age and ethnobiology. Encyclopedia of Life Sciences,
savannah environment of the possible pp.1–6. John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
hominin Graecopithecus from Europe. PLoS Foley, R., 1996. The adaptive legacy of human
ONE, 12(5), pp.1–31. evolution: A search for the environment of
Bolhuis, J.J., Brown, G.R., Richardson, R.C. and evolutionary adaptedness. Evolutionary
Laland, K.N., 2011. Darwin in mind: New Anthropology, 4(6), pp.194–203.
opportunities for evolutionary psychology. Foley, R.A., Martin, L., Lahr, M.M. and
PLoS Biology, 9, pp.1–8. Stringer, C., 2016. Major transitions in human
Bowlby, J., 1982. Loss: Sadness and Depression. evolution. Philosophical Transactions Royal
New York: Basic Book Publishers. Society B, 371(1698), pp.1–8.
Broesch, J., Barrett, H.C. and Henrich, J., 2014. Friesem, D.E., Lavi, N., Madella, M., Boaretto,
Adaptive content biases in learning about E., Ajithparsad, P. and French, C., 2017.
animals across the life course. Human The formation of fire residues associated
Nature, 25, pp.181–99. with hunter-gatherers in humid tropical envi-
Brooks, D.J., Brooks, S.G., Greenhill, B.D. and ronments: A geo-ethnoarchaeological per-
Haas, M.L., 2019. The demographic transition spective. Quaternary Science Reviews, 171(1),
theory of war: Why young societies are conflict pp.85–99.
prone and old societies are the most peaceful. Frost, P., 2011. Human nature or human natures?
International Security, 43(3), pp.53–95. Futures, 43, pp.740–8.
Colley, K. and Craig, T., 2019. Natural places: Gibbons, A. and Groarke, A., 2016. Can risk
Perceptions of wildness and attachment to and illness perceptions predict breast cancer
local greenspace. Journal of Environmental worry in healthy women? Journal of Health
Psychology, 61, pp.71–8. Psychology, 21(9), pp.1–11.
Cook, M. and Mineka, S., 1990. Selective asso- Han, K.T., 2007. Responses to six major terres-
ciations in the observational conditioning of trial biomes in terms of scenic beauty, prefer-
fear in rhesus monkeys. Journal of Experi- ence, and restorativeness. Environment and
mental Psychology, 16, pp.372–89. Behavior, 39(4), pp.529–56.
Cosmides, L. and Tooby, J., 2003. Evolutionary Hardin, G., 1968. The tragedy of the com-
psychology: Theoretical foundations. In: L. mons. Science, 162(3859), pp.1243–8.
Nadel (Ed.). Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science Hartmann, P. and Apaolaza-Ibáñes, V., 2010.
(pp.54–64). London: Macmillan. Beyond savanna: An evolutionary and envi-
Coss, R.G. and Moore, M., 2002. Precocious ronmental psychology approach to behavioral
knowledge of trees as antipredator refuge effects of nature scenery in green advertising.
in preschool children: An examination of Journal of Environmental Psychology, 30(1),
aesthetics, attributive judgments, and relic pp.119–28.
sexual dinichism. Ecological Psychology, 14, Hartmann, P. and Apaolaza-Ibáñes, V., 2013.
pp.181–222. Desert or rain: Standardisation of green
120 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

advertising versus adaptation to the target according to the degree of visitors. Sustain-
audience’s natural environment. European ability, 9(8), pp.1–27.
Journal of Marketing, 47(5/6), pp.917–33. Lewontin, R.C., 1982. Organism and environ-
Henry, A.D., Christensen, A.E., Hofmann, R., ment. In: H.C. Plotkin (Ed.). Learning, Devel-
Steimanis, I. and Vollan, B., 2017. Influence of opment and Culture (pp.151–70). New York:
sea level rise on discounting, resource use and John Wiley & Sons.
migration in small-island communities: An López-García, L.M., Blain, H., Sanz, M., Daura, J.
agent-based modelling approach. Environ- and Zilhão, J., 2018. Refining the environ-
mental Conservation, 44(4), pp.381–8. mental and climatic background of the Middle
Herzog, T.R., Herbert, E.J., Kaplan, R. and Pleistocene human cranium from Gruta da
Crooks, C.L. 2000. Cultural and develop- Aroeira (Torres Novas, Portugal). Quaternary
mental comparisons of landscape percep- Science Reviews, 200, pp.367–75.
tions and preferences. Environment and Martín-López, B., Montes, C. and Benayas, J.,
Behavior, 32(3), pp.323–46. 2008. Economic valuation of biodiversity
Hu, S., Yue, H. and Zhou, Z., 2019. Preferences conservation: The meaning of numbers.
for urban stream landscapes: Opportunities Conservation Biology, 22, pp.624–35.
to promote unmanaged riparian vegetation. Matthews, B., De Meester, L., Jones, C.G., Ibel-
Urban Forestry & Urban Greening, 38, ings, B.W., Bouma, T.J., Nuutinen, V., Koppel,
pp.114–23. J.V. and Odling-Smee, J., 2014. Under niche
Hublin, J.J., Ben-Ncer, A., Bailey, S.E., Freidline, construction: An operational bridge between
S.E., Neubauer, S., Skinner, M.M., Bergmann, ecology, evolution, and ecosystem science.
I., Le Cabec, A., Benazzi, S., Harvati, K. and Ecological Monographs, 84, pp.245–63.
Gunz, P., 2017. New fossils from Jebel Irhoud, Miceli, R., Sotgiu, I. and Settanni, M., 2008.
Morocco and the pan-African origin of Homo Disaster preparedness and perception of
sapiens. Nature, 546(7657), pp.289–92. flood risk: A study in an alpine valley in Italy.
Kellert, S.R., 1993. The biological basis for Journal of Environmental Psychology, 28(2),
human values of nature. In: S.R. Kellert and pp.164–73.
E.O. Wilson (Eds.). The Biophilia Hypothesis. Moura, J.M.B., Ferreira Junior, W.S., Silva, T.C.
Washington, DC: Island Press. Pp. 42–69. and Albuquerque U.P., 2018. The influence
Kock, N., Chatelain-Jardón, R. and Carmona, of the evolutionary past on the mind: An
J., 2008. An experimental study of simulated analysis of the preference for landscapes in
web-based threats and their impact on the human species. Frontiers in Psychology,
knowledge communication effectiveness. 9(2485), pp.1–13.
IEEE Transactions on Professional Communi- Nairne, J.S., Thompson, S.R. and Pandeirada,
cation, 51, pp.183–97. J.N.S., 2007. Adaptive memory: Survival pro-
Kong, Y., Deng, C., Liu, W., Wu, X., Pei, S., cessing enhances retention. Journal of Exper-
Sun, L., Ge, J., Yi, L. and Zhu, R., 2018. Mag- imental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and
netostratigraphic dating of the hominin Cognition, 33(2), pp.263–73.
occupation of Bailong Cave, central China. Ode, A., Fry, G., Tveit, M.S., Messager, P. and
Scientific Reports, 8(9699), pp.1–12. Miller, D., 2009. Indicators of perceived nat-
Laland, K.N. and Brown, G.R., 2006. Niche uralness as drivers of landscape preference.
construction, human behavior, and the Journal of Environmental Management,
adaptive-lag hypothesis. Evolutionary 90(1), pp.375–83.
Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, Odling-Smee, J., Laland, K.N. and Feldman,
15(3), pp.95–104. M.W., 2003. Niche Construction: The
Laland, K.N. and O’Brien, M.J., 2012. Cultural Neglected Process in Evolution. Princeton,
niche construction: An introduction. Biologi- NJ: Princeton University Press.
cal Theory, 6(3), pp.191–2. Orians, G.H., 1980. Habitat selection: General
Larson, S., 2012. Did Australopiths climb trees? theory and applications to human behavior.
Science 338(6106), pp.478–9. In: J. Lockard (Ed.). The Evolution of Human
Lee, K. and Son, Y., 2017. Exploring landscape Social Behavior (pp.49–66). Chicago, IL:
perceptions of Bukhansan national park Elsevier.
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND ENVIRONMENTAL SCIENCES 121

Orians, G.H. and Heerwagen, J.H., 1992. the genus Homo. Evolutionary Anthropology:
Evolved responses to landscapes. In: J.H. Issues, News, and Reviews, 25(6), pp.306–17.
Barkow, L. Cosmides and L. Tooby (Eds.). The Rozin, P. and Todd, P.M., 2015. The evolution-
Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and ary psychology of food intake and choice.
the Generation of Culture (pp.555–79). New In: D.M. Buss (Ed.). The Handbook of Evolu-
York: Oxford University Press. tionary Psychology (pp.183–205). Hoboken,
Penn, D.J., 2003. The evolutionary roots of our NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
environmental problems: Toward a Darwin- Ruin, I., Gaillard, J.C. and Lutoff, C., 2007.
ian ecology. The Quarterly Review of Biology, How to get there? Assessing motorists’ flash
78(3), pp.275–301. flood risk perception on daily itineraries.
Pinho, J.R., Grilo, C., Boone, R.B., Galvin, K.A. Environmental Hazards, 7(3), pp.235–44.
and Snodgrass, J.G., 2014. Influence of Salvati, L., Carlucci, M., Serra, P. and Zambon,
aesthetic appreciation of wildlife species on I., 2019. Demographic transitions and socio-
attitudes towards their conservation in Kenyan economic development in Italy, 1862–2009:
agropastoralist communities. PLoS ONE, A brief overview. Sustainability (Switzerland),
9:e88842. 11(1), pp.1–12.
Prokop, P. and Fančovičová, J., 2014. Seeing Sampaio, M.B., De La Fuente, M.F.,
coloured fruits: Utilization of the theory of Albuquerque, U.P., Souto, A.S. and Schiel,
adaptive memory in teaching botany. Journal N., 2018. Contact with urban forests greatly
of Biological Education, 3(48), pp.127–32. enhances children’s knowledge of faunal
Prokop, P. and Fančovičová, J., 2019. The per- diversity. Urban Forestry & Urban Greening,
ception of toxic and non-toxic plants by chil- 30, pp.56–61.
dren and adolescents with regard to gender: Sandry, J., Trafimow, D., Marks, M.J. and Rice,
Implications for teaching botany. Journal of S., 2013. Adaptive memory: Evaluating alter-
Biological Education, 53, pp. 463–473. native forms of fitness-relevant processing in
Prokop, P. and Randler, C., 2018. Biological the survival processing paradigm. PLoS ONE,
predispositions and individual differences in 8(4), e60868.
human attitudes toward animals. In: U.P. Scheiter, S., Schulte, J., Pfeiffer, M., Martens,
Albuquerque and R.R.N. Alves (Eds.). Ethno- C., Erasmus, B.F.N. and Twine, W.C., 2019.
zoology: Animals in our Lives (pp. 447–66). How does climate change influence the eco-
Oxford: Elsevier Academic Press. nomic value of ecosystem services in savanna
Rakison, D.H. and Derringer, J., 2008. Do rangelands? Ecological Economics, 157,
infants possess an evolved spider-detection pp.342–56.
mechanism? Cognition, 107, pp.381–93. Schlebusch, C.M., Malmström, H., Günther, T.,
Rendell, L., Fogarty, L., Hoppitt, W.J.E., Morgan, Sjödin, P., Coutinho, A., Edlund, H., Munters,
T.J.H., Webster, M.M. and Laland, K,N., 2011. A.R., Vicente, M., Steyn, M., Soodyall, H.,
Cognitive culture: Theoretical and empirical Lombard, M. and Jakobsson, M., 2017.
insights into social learning strategies. Trends Southern African ancient genomes estimate
in Cognitive Sciences, 15, pp.68–76. modern human divergence to 350,000 to
Riaz, A., Gregor, S. and Lin, A., 2018. Biophilia 260,000 years ago. Science, 358(6363),
and biophobia in website design: Improving pp.652–5.
internet information dissemination. Informa- Senoglu, B., Oktay, H.E. and Kinoshita, I., 2018.
tion & Management, 55, pp.199–214. An empirical research study on prospect–
Richter, D., Grün, R., Joannes-Boyau, R., Steele, refuge theory and the effect of high-rise
T.E., Amani, F., Rué, M., Fernandes, P., buildings in a Japanese garden setting. City,
Raynal, J., Geraads, D., Ben-Ncer, A., Hublin, Territory and Architecture, 5(3), pp.1–16.
J. and McPherron, S.P., 2017. The age of the Šestáková, A. and Plichtová, J., 2019. Contem-
hominin fossils from Jebel Irhoud, Morocco, porary commons: Sharing and managing
and the origins of the Middle Stone Age. common-pool resources in the 21st century.
Nature, 546(7657), pp.293–6. Human Affairs, 29(1), pp.74–86.
Roberts, P., Boivin, N., Lee-Thorp, J., Petraglia, Silva, R.H., Ferreira Júnior, W.S., Medeiros, P.M.
M. and Stock, J., 2016. Tropical forests and and Albuquerque, U.P., 2019. Adaptive
122 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

memory and evolution of the human natural- Ulrich, R.S., 1993. Biophilia, biophobia, and
istic mind: Insights from the use of medicinal natural landscapes. In: S.R. Kellert and E.O.
plants. PLoS ONE, 14(3), pp.1–15. Wilson (Eds.). The Biophilia Hypothesis.
Sommer, R., 1997. Further cross-national Washington, DC: Island Press. pp. 73–137.
studies of tree form preferences. Ecological Volk, A.A., and Atkinson, J.A., 2013. Infant
Psychology, 9(2), pp.153–60. and child death in the human environment
Stringer, C., 2016. The origin and evolution of of evolutionary adaptation. Evolution and
Homo sapiens. Philosophical Transactions Human Behavior, 34, pp. 182–93.
Royal Society B, 371(1698), pp.1–12. Wilson, E.O., 1993. Biophilia and the conserva-
Summit, J. and Sommer, R., 1999. Further tion ethic. In: S.R. Kellert and E.O. Wilson
studies of preferred tree shapes. Environment (Eds.). The Biophilia Hypothesis. Washington,
and Behavior, 31(4), pp.550–76. DC: Island Press. pp. 31–41.
Tooby, J. and Cosmides, L., 1992. The psycho- Yang, L., Lau, K.P.L. and Truong, L., 2014. The
logical foundations of culture. In: J. Barkow, survival effect in memory: Does it hold into
L. Cosmides and J. Tooby (Eds.). The Adapted old age and non-ancestral scenarios? PLoS
Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Gen- ONE, 9(5), pp.1–9.
eration of Culture (pp.19–136). New York: Yorzinski, J.L., Penkunas, M.J., Platt, M.L. and
Oxford University Press. Coss, R.G., 2014. Dangerous animals capture
Tooby, J. and Cosmides, L., 2005. Conceptual and maintain attention in humans. Evolution-
foundations of evolutionary psychology. ary Psychology, 12, pp.534–48.
In: D.M. Buss (Ed.). The Handbook of Evolu- Young, S.G., Brown, C.M. and Ambady, N.,
tionary Psychology (pp.5–67). Hoboken, NJ: 2012. Priming a natural or human-made
John Wiley & Sons. environment directs attention to context-
Tooby, J. and Cosmides, L., 2015. The theoreti- congruent threatening stimuli. Cognition
cal foundations of evolutionary psychology. and Emotion, 26, pp.927–33.
In: D.M. Buss (Ed.). The Handbook of Evolu- Zajonc, R.B., 1980. Feeling and thinking: Prefer-
tionary Psychology (pp.3–87). Hoboken, NJ: ences need no inferences. American Psychol-
John Wiley & Sons. ogist, 35(2), pp.151–75.
Townsend, J.B. and Barton, S., 2018. The Zhang, W., Goodale, E. and Chen, J., 2014. How
impact of ancient tree form on modern land- contact with nature affects children’s biophilia,
scape preferences. Urban Forestry and Urban biophobia and conservation attitude in China.
Greening, 34, pp.205–16. Biological Conservation, 177, pp.109–16.
6
Evolutionary Psychology
and Public Health
Simon Russell

INTRODUCTION: PUBLIC HEALTH complex relationship between human fertility,


AND THE OBESITY PROBLEM fecundity, and fitness,[5] good health is recog-
nised as a key dimension of evolutionary fit-
Public health has been defined as ‘the art and ness[6] and has a greater impact on fitness
science of preventing disease, prolonging life than age-specific fertility.[7] However, selec-
and promoting health through the organized tion does not act on health and it is possible to
be a public health success but an evolutionary
efforts of society’.[1] Public health is an inter-
failure; one may lead a long and healthy life
disciplinary science, which aims to promote
but fail to reproduce. Equally, one may be an
the length and quality of human life through
evolutionary success and a public health fail-
the prevention and treatment of disease.
ure; it is possible to reproduce successfully
Whether relating to physical, mental, or social
from a position of poor health.
health, public health seeks to translate high- Despite public health and evolutionary
quality research into preventative or curative success being defined by and measured in
practice. The field of public health also recog- different currencies, applying the princi-
nises a positive value arising from good health, ples of evolution to the discipline of public
which incorporates concepts of psychological health may provide original insight for pol-
and emotional well-being. While selection icy makers and practitioners. Evolutionary
does not act to promote longevity, evolution- science has been increasingly applied to
ary fitness is profoundly affected by morbidity various disciplines within the field of public
and mortality[2] and both health outcomes and health, including reproductive health, immu-
evolutionary fitness are notably predicted by nity, infectious diseases, cancer, and mental
socioeconomic status in the developed and health.[8–11] This chapter explores the util-
developing world.[3,4] While there is a ity of applying the principles of evolutionary
124 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

psychology to the most pervasive physical with overweight and obesity.[14] Living with
health problem of the modern world, namely, overweight and obesity greatly increases the
non-communicable diseases (NCDs). The risk of Type-2 diabetes, cardiovascular dis-
primary focus will be on pathological health eases, and various forms of cancer,[15] in
risk behaviours, which greatly enhance the addition to many other physical and mental
risk of NCDs. Additionally, a case study health problems.
presents the relevance of evolutionary deci- Obesity can be conceived as both very simple
sion making in health-promotion behaviour, and as highly complex. A positive energy bal-
within the context of vaccinations and protec- ance can be thought of as an equation: energy
tion from communicable diseases. in, use by metabolic processes, and energy out.
Comprising cardiovascular diseases, can- Even a slight but consistent positive imbalance
cers, chronic respiratory diseases, and diabe- would accrue overweight and obesity over
tes, NCDs are preventable but account for 41 time. Conversely, there is almost nothing we do
million or 71% of global deaths annually.[12] in our lives which does not affect our energy
The main risk factors for NCDs are poor diet, balance and obesity. The Foresight obesity
physical inactivity, tobacco use, and harmful systems map[16] categorises, maps, and illus-
alcohol use.[13] Harmful alcohol use, smok- trates the varied and complex determinants of
ing, poor diet, and physical inactivity can be obesity. There is limited utility in focussing on
understood as health risk behaviours, which any one part of the map, given that the obe-
lead to potentially pathological metabolic sity system is dynamic and any change to one
and physiological changes. Poor diets typi- part of the system will have consequences for
cally involve consuming energy-dense but other parts. Public health responses can some-
low-nutrient food and drinks. Low nutrition times be simplistic; interventions may attempt
and overconsumption often co-exist and are to adjust one determinant in some way, while
typified by eating foods that are high in fats, controlling for others, and practitioners and
sugars, and salt (HFSS). Overweight and obe- policy lobbyists can become focused on just
sity are typical consequences of poor diet and one element of the map. In turn, the public
inactivity and will be considered as key patho- and the media sometimes misunderstand the
logical outcomes for the purpose of this chap- problem and oversimplify what is perceived to
ter. The principles and public health response be an effective policy response. Furthermore,
discussed here for overweight and obesity are there is no consensus between academics or
also potentially relevant to harmful drinking civil servants on where the public health focus
and smoking, since it will be argued that simi- should be. Some suggest the problem lies with
lar psychological mechanisms and evolution- ‘energy in’, since evidence suggests physical
ary strategies underpin and govern multiple activity may not help people lose weight, while
health-risk-taking behaviours (for usage and others focus mainly on ‘energy out’ given that
definitions, see section ‘Psychological mech- physical activity has been shown to attenuate
anisms and evolutionary strategies’). the risks of NCDs, even for people living with
Overweight and obesity has become per- overweight or obesity.
haps the most prevalent and burdensome The causality of obesity has drawn simi-
public health problem of the modern world. lar debate in terms of the relative impacts of
Overweight and obesity rates in adults have our genes and the environment and the inter-
tripled since 1975, and in 2016 there were play between the two. Broadly speaking, the
1.9 billion (39%) adults globally living with effects of heredity in obesity are likely to be
overweight or obesity.[14] Rates of child- low, given that obesity has grown rapidly
hood obesity have risen 10-fold over the same in genetically stable populations. However,
period, and in 2016 there were an estimated there is some evidence to suggest that biolog-
340 million children aged 5–19 years living ical influences account for large proportions
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND PUBLIC HEALTH 125

Figure 6.1  The impact of the food supply on the expression of obesityi[19]

of variance in food and satiety responsiveness nological and civil development, many physi-
and therefore risk of obesity. Yet the more we cal and psychological adaptations that
understand about genetic susceptibility, the are rooted in evolutionary time have become
more the importance of the environment has mismatched with modern life. Obesity is per-
been revealed; the expression of susceptibil- haps the most pronounced physical manifesta-
ity to obesity in environments with a limited tion of this mismatch; it has increased in almost
food supply is low but becomes hugely ampli- every country in the world, and is reported to
fied in environments with an abundant supply be proximally driven by changes in the food
(Figure 6.1).[17] Generally, diseases rarely system, where calorie-dense food is abundant,
have simple Mendelian patterns of inherit- affordable, and readily available in most of the
ance; heritability of ill health is more com- modern world.[20] Built environments have
monly the product of interactions between also changed and influence what and how
multiple genes, predispositions and the envi- much we eat, in addition to the energy we
ronment, and socioeconomic and cultural expend; energy-saving mechanisms are inte-
influences.[18] While genetic predispositions gral to modern civic design. Modern living has
are likely to exacerbate or attenuate the risk become increasingly urbanised; in 1950 there
of obesity, the problem is recent relative to were 751 million people worldwide living in
human history and has coincided with indus- urban areas, compared to 4.2 billion in 2018,
trial and urban modernisation, which provide which is 55% worldwide but as high as 72% in
the obesogenic platforms from which genetic Europe and 82% in North America.[21]
susceptibility may be expressed. Urbanisation is relevant to the obesity epi-
demic; research suggests that a lack of green
space and recreational facilities, perceived
unsafe communities, and high-density popula-
MAKING EVOLUTIONARY SENSE OF tions can be risk factors for obesity.[22]
THE MODERN WORLD Urbanisation is also relevant from an evolution-
ary perspective; community and kin networks
The majority of people now live in surround- have been replaced in urban environments by
ings that are very different from the selective small living units or nuclear families, typically
environments in which we evolved. With tech- with non-kin neighbours.[23]
126 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

In our ancestral, selective environment overweight, the more pertinent question may
there would have been food scarcity and be, why are some of us not living with over-
changeable conditions. In such circum- weight? We are conscious that overconsump-
stances it would have been adaptive to max- tion is not conducive to good health, and we
imise caloric intake, especially of food rich moderate our behaviour accordingly, but our
in sugar and fat. Our ancestors and tradi- ability to moderate consumption and other
tional populations have evolved to maxim- diet-related behaviour varies. If conscious
ise their foraging success and net energy moderation of behaviour is the mechanism
intake,[24–26] and, therefore, their health by which we prevent overeating, why are so
and evolutionary fitness. Adaptations that many people making suboptimal and seem-
motivate preferences for high-calorie foods ingly maladaptive health choices? Perhaps
would have been a consequence and humans these health choices are not maladaptive at
are reportedly hardwired to prefer HFSS all.
foods.[27] A mechanism of this adaptation There is a long-established link between
is that energy-rich foods elicit enjoyment socioeconomic gradients and health risk
in humans, especially when consumed in behaviours,[30] health outcomes, and mor-
combination. Various hormones are released tality.[3] While deprivation predicted food
when high-energy foods, particularly fats poverty and underweight in the past, in the
and sugars, are consumed, which stimulate modern developed world, low socioeconomic
pleasure-associated areas of the brain;[28]
­ status strongly predicts obesity;[31] in turn,
similar effects are found with alcohol, ciga- living with obesity has been found to exac-
rettes, and other substances. Humans are also erbate inequalities.[32] Within the developed
tolerant to food delays, which is likely to be a world, evidence suggests that relative inequal-
product of changeable environmental condi- ities are equally if not more important than
tions. It seems likely that our ancestors were absolute poverty in predicting problematic
adapted to maximise energy intake during health behaviours and outcomes.[33] There
periods of food scarcity and moderate con- are environmental factors that accompany
sumption during times of food abundance. deprivation, which increase the likelihood of
The agricultural and industrial revolutions obesity. Energy-dense foods represent bet-
brought great civil advancement, but food ter calories per unit cost compared to fresh
technology and food systems have changed ingredients, which appeals to low-income
more over the last 50 years than ever before; families.[34] The built environment in areas
production and supply of energy-dense and of deprivation also has reduced green spaces
processed foods have increased substan- and fewer recreation facilities, such as leisure
tially. Globalisation has resulted in a reduc- centres, which limit opportunities for physi-
tion in price and an increase in availability cal activity, increase risk factors for obesity,
of high-calorie foods.[20] The modern world and widen health inequalities overall.[35]
for most people affords a constant state of There are also higher densities of fast-food
food plenty and, given human preference outlets and reduced access to healthier food
for HFSS foods, consumption of food and stores in increasingly deprived areas, both of
prevalence of obesity have increased dra- which are associated with poorer diets.[36]
matically. It is for these reasons that obesity The relationship between disadvantage and
has been described as a normal response to obesity is complex and dynamic but, in devel-
an abnormal environment.[29] The advertis- oped countries, people of lower socioeco-
ing and food industries of the modern world nomic status are disproportionately affected
also work to maximise our contact with food by obesity.[37] However, obesity is prevalent
or associated psychological cues. So rather across all socioeconomic strata and there is
than asking why some of us are living with variation in overweight and obesity within
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND PUBLIC HEALTH 127

similarly deprived areas, which implies that centres, expectation centres, short- and long-
disadvantage only accounts for a proportion term memory,[44] all before a hand has been
of the observed variance. outstretched. Selection has not acted upon
these areas in isolation; the idea of modu-
larity recognises functional specialisations
Psychological Mechanisms and within the brain that are domain-specific and
are utilised for different cognitive processes.
Evolutionary Strategies
Whether modularity is strong or weak, any
Psychological mechanisms or modules choice and resulting behaviour is the product
throughout this chapter refer to functional of a hierarchical chained process that breaks
neurocognitive adaptations that have been down with impairment of any of the involved
successful over evolutionary time owing to areas.[45] Psychological mechanisms are
their selective value in solving problems and selected in their own right; they may be her-
their contribution to survival or reproductive itable in a conventional sense, but may also
success.[38] In combination and in addition be the product of behavioural and, therefore,
to anatomical and physiological traits, psy- phenotypic plasticity, leading to selectable
chological mechanisms create broader evolu- individual variation.[41]
tionary or behavioural strategies, a clear Genetic predispositions form the basis of
example being an individual’s reproductive all psychological mechanisms, but they also
strategy.[39] When originally conceptual- remain a part of our complex biological sys-
ised, psychological mechanisms or modules tems and are mutually associated with our
were proposed to be universal among humans physiology. For example, and in relation to
but that different mechanisms were invoked obesity, adipose tissue is an active organ and
in different situations, leading to phenotypic part of the homeostatic system that regulates
variation.[40] It is proposed here that while satiation and energy balance. Adipose tissue,
some fundamental psychological mecha- the gastrointestinal tract, and the pancreas
nisms are innate and species-typical, others release various hormonal regulators to the
are more varied across individuals, arising brain, which receives the signals and acts to
from individual-specific interactions between reprioritise behaviour to stimulate or sup-
genetic predispositions and phenotypic plas- press appetite. There are also physiological
ticity.[41] detectors for fats and sugars in food, which
Neurobiological structures are produced stimulate pleasure-associated areas of the
by our genes after thousands of years of selec- brain. Elicitation of reward mechanisms also
tion pressures, which act on individual genes motivates behaviour, to such an extent that
but also the resulting cognitive mechanisms. homeostatic processes may be overridden.
Physically, the brain is complex, comprising This push and pull creates a trade-off within
billions of neurons and chemical interac- our energy-balance system, whereby psycho-
tions, but it functions as a specialised system logical mechanisms, each with a clear adap-
that has been produced by the evolutionary tive significance, compete at cross-purposes.
process.[42] The brain acts hierarchically, It is a feature of environments with constantly
where functionality emerges as a prop- available energy-rich foods that allow the
erty of micro- and mesoscopic interactions reward mechanisms to gain the upper hand
through coordinated chained structures that and enable a chronic positive energy balance.
run between anatomically clustered areas. Developmental conditions also affect psy-
[43] For example, the choice to eat a donut chological mechanisms, beginning in utero;
would flow through and elicit responses these may be physiological, physical, emo-
from various areas of the brain: affect and tional, or sociocultural. Childhood experience
emotion centres, autonomic and conscious has been found to profoundly affect cognitive
128 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

development, psychological mechanisms, countries, it seems logical that behavioural


and therefore behaviour. Neurobiological strategies are matched to environmental con-
development occurs rapidly throughout child- texts or circumstances, despite the debatable
hood but continues through adolescence and and nuanced influence of culture. It will be
into adulthood. Cognitive systems and path- argued here that health behaviour is strate-
ways that govern behaviour can be affected gic and adaptive, which would provide the
if areas of the brain suffer impairment dur- principal reason why seemingly maladap-
ing development. Adverse experiences in tive choices, such as overeating, are so often
childhood, including psychological or physi- taken. There is a wealth of evidence that the
cal abuse, change the formation of physical health behaviour of industrialised popula-
structures and neural pathways, which can tions is influenced by physical and socioeco-
profoundly affect healthy development and nomic environments; deprived populations
behaviour in later life. These changes can are more likely to have low-nutrition diets,
also be irreversible, meaning that the conse- and more likely to engage in a range of other
quences of adverse experiences often persist health risk behaviours.[53–54] The trend
into adulthood and act independently of socio- is not solely economic; even when health
economic pressures.[46] Adverse childhood behaviours are free, people from increasingly
experiences have been found to contribute to deprived groups have been found to be less
problematic eating behaviours, weight gain, likely to engage with them,[30] implying the
and overweight and obesity in childhood and issue is strategic. The way we perceive our
adulthood.[47–49] The responses to adverse environmental circumstance is also impor-
experiences may have adaptive roots; stress- tant; perception of income, for example, has
ors are linked to heightened immune- and been shown to be more important than actual
nervous-system responses, but continued income in determining related behaviour.[55]
stress may produce over-activation that is Living in environments that are to lesser
likely to create a platform for poor mental or greater degrees mismatched from those in
health.[50] Adverse experiences are likely which we evolved has subtle and complex
to be accompanied by unstable and insecure implications for modern humans. Rates of
food environments, an adaptive response to mental health problems, including depression,
which may be overconsumption when the anxiety, and stress, have never been higher,
opportunity arises. If the consequences of an especially among young people. Our behav-
adverse childhood persist into adulthood, so iours act dynamically with environmental
too would the food response, even when food pressures; governed by psychological mecha-
becomes more consistently available. nisms and broader strategies, our behaviour
Psychological mechanisms are also is a response to the environment, whether
profoundly shaped by environmental cir- physiological or psychological, which is then
cumstances, both physical and, in modern experienced and remembered. Our experi-
contexts, socioeconomic.[51] Our phenotypes ence leads to learning, which subtly adjusts
are cumulative developmental outcomes of the psychological platform on which behav-
interactions between our genes and envi- iour is based. Just as adverse developmental
ronments. Assuming that the mechanism by experiences can impair cognitive functioning,
which a trait is inherited does not constrain so can more transient states of low mood or
its adaptive value, it is likely that behaviour poor well-being, and repetition of such states
and the underlying mechanisms are modi- can lead to poor mental health. Similarly, other
fied in line with environmental variation.[52] temporary states such as illness and disease
While fields such as human behavioural ecol- can alter not only our behaviour but our per-
ogy face challenges in testing quantifiable ception of social and physical environments.
hypothesis in non-traditional or industrialised Low mood and emotional disorders have been
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND PUBLIC HEALTH 129

found to affect behavioural strategies, and spe- behaviours within a changing environment;
cifically those related to food consumption behavioural plasticity was and remains a huge
and physical activity.[56] selective advantage for humans.[57] Our abil-
The key determinants of psychological ity to modify our behaviour extends selection
mechanisms and, therefore, behavioural strate- beyond psychological mechanisms and the
gies are genetic, physiological, environmental, resulting behaviours to behavioural strate-
developmental, and subjective (Figure 6.2). gies themselves. Such strategies are likely to
These determinants are not mutually exclu- be adaptive but modifiable both temporally
sive but are linked as part of a dynamic sys- and with changing environments, meaning
tem. For example, phenotypic expression is that physical, political, social, or individual
primarily the product of interplay between change would be likely to shift the parameters
genetic predispositions and environmental for optimal behaviour. Examples of this can
conditions; physical and socioeconomic envi- be anecdotally observed every day: a natural
ronments are inextricably linked but are also disaster, political change, or a dramatic change
associated with developmental and subjective in personal circumstance are all likely to dra-
determinants; and developmental and sub- matically shift the behaviour of an individual
jective factors also feedback and modify the or collective. Health behaviour strategies are
physiological system. Resulting mechanisms determined more subtly, especially given the
may be fixed, others may be more plastic, sometimes contradictory pressures of health
varying with changes in circumstances or even and fitness, but perhaps unhealthy choices are
mood states. Behavioural plasticity allows us not maladaptive but optimal given the evolu-
to make different choices and exhibit ­different tionary context of the choice.

Figure 6.2  Key determinants of psychological mechanisms and behaviour strategies


130 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Adaptive Mechanisms and range of adaptive functions.[59] In a health


Optimal Strategies setting, impulsiveness is sometimes con-
sidered hedonistic but reflective or rational
As outlined by life history theory, behavioural behaviour involves constraint and is likely to
optimality is a shaped by a range of problems, be the product of conscious or higher cogni-
including survival, growth, and development. tive control. Hedonic impulsivity may bring
[39] These represent multiple and complex a sense of enjoyment and freedom to the
pressures, which underpin choices or deci- human experience,[63] but in a health sense,
sions that produce variable behaviours. All this often involves long-term costs. Smoking
behaviour, including health behaviour, is the cigarettes, using substances, drinking alco-
product of a cognitive appraisal system, the hol, or eating HFSS foods may be enjoyable
process of choosing an action from a range of in the short term but are likely to pose future
options with the expectation of maximising health risks.
the benefit and minimising the cost.[58] The Reflective and goal-directed behaviour is
stress-response system functions to mediate often viewed as rational, reasoned, and con-
openness to environmental inputs and regu- scious, utilising volitional control or self-
late behaviour in a range of fitness-relevant regulation to achieve a goal, while impulsive
areas. ‘Switch’ mechanisms regulate genetic decisions, despite incorporating a range of
and environmental influences on phenotypic behaviours, have been described as subop-
development; during development, informa- timal, an inability to moderate behaviour.
tion processed by these mechanisms feeds Reflective decisions usually have the goal
back and recalibrates the stress-response of a larger payoff at a later time point, while
system, resulting in individual and adaptive forgoing the immediate reward or benefit. In
patterns of behaviour.[59] Environmental terms of health behaviour, this would be rep-
moderators may be social or familial, physi- resented by forgoing the immediate pleasure
cal, including the built or natural environ- and reward from an action, such as eating an
ment, or socioeconomic, given that there are HFSS food, in order that the chances of yield-
established gradients in health behaviour ing the longer-term and larger benefit of good
across the spectrum of inequality. health are enhanced. Impulsivity can be inter-
Temporal decision making provides an preted as the forgoing of a large but delayed
example, and in particular the extent to reward in favour of a smaller but immediate
which behavioural strategies are under- reward.[58] In certain environmental condi-
pinned by impulsive or reflective decision tions, impulsivity is likely to be adaptive;
making. Rather than assuming all impulsive dangerous environments, for example, where
behaviour is the absence of control, there is there is an immediate threat or mortality rates
utility in considering a dual system of behav- are high, are likely to favour short-term and
iour,[60] where reflective and impulsive high-risk behaviours.[39] Research sup-
influences exist, are differentially adaptive, ports this theory, demonstrating that people
and are often in conflict or trade-off against in adverse environments are more likely to
each other.[61–62] A moderator of this sys- discount the future and act more impulsively,
tem may be developmental factors, that have despite the potential harm to their health.[64]
the potential to inhibit the healthy forma- Impulsive and reflective decision making has
tion of neurobiological structures, which are also been shown to predict health behaviour in
important in overriding impulsive mecha- the way we would expect; lower health-risk-
nisms for behaviour. A good example is the taking behaviour is associated with increas-
impact of developmental conditions on the ingly reflective decision making, while riskier
functioning of the stress-response system, health behaviour is associated with increas-
a biological mechanism involved in a wide ingly impulsive decision making.[65–66]
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND PUBLIC HEALTH 131

Within the field of cognitive psychology, and adaptive behavioural strategy.[70] Within
the concept of delay or temporal discount- these parameters are individual factors, which
ing[67] is used to explore our cognitive may have arisen from biological, develop-
appraisal system, specifically relating to the mental, or subjective determinants and may
temporal nature of health economics. Delay be fixed or transient. Individual states may
discounting is the extent to which immediate not alter the objective optimality of a behav-
rewards are declined or postponed in order to ioural strategy matched to a set of environ-
gain larger rewards in the future; these con- mental conditions, but they would alter the
structs create behavioural strategies, which cognitive appraisal system and the perceived
are variable, context specific, and are highly costs and benefits in temporal decision mak-
likely to be adaptive. Crucial in the deci- ing. Like adverse developmental conditions,
sion are the values of the present and future strategies that favour short-term payoffs and
reward, but also the length of time required immediate rewards may have adaptive roots.
or delay in gaining the future benefit; this In states of high anxiety or depression, it may
can be considered as a cost subtracted from not be optimal or even possible to think about
the future reward. It is well established that long-term benefits; it is likely that for some-
humans trade off between present and future one suffering from a mental health disorder,
rewards and, as previously stated in terms of temporal perception may be very different to
food choice, tolerance for delays in forag- someone of good mental health and the cost
ing may have been selected for in variable of waiting for a reward may be perceived
environments. It is also well established that to be much higher. Such individuals may
humans make temporally optimal decisions still be behaving optimally and adaptively,
with financial or resource-based benefits and upon consideration of the determinants of
costs; the principle of delay discounting has their psychological mechanisms, the inter-
been used to predict various health behav- nal influences on behavioural appraisal,
iours, including food consumption, physical and the social, physical, and socioeconomic
activity, tobacco, and alcohol use.[68] The environment.
constructs that underlie impulsive or reflec- These ideas can be further evidenced by
tive behaviour and delay discounting are considering behavioural strategies that have
psychological mechanisms, which have been established evolutionary significance and
selected upon but remain modifiable with their relationship to currencies of health.
changing circumstances of environments. Risk taking or aversion is adaptive in vari-
The relevance of these mechanisms and their ous ancestral and modern contexts and has
determinants to the field of public health relevance to health. While there are broad
have not been fully explored but may hold differences in risk-taking behaviour between
the key to understanding why suboptimal age and gender groups, strategic variation
health behaviours are chosen to the point of can also be observed. Life history theory pre-
pathology and premature death at epidemic dicts that decisions relating to energy allo-
proportions. cation and behaviour are divided between
Behavioural traits, whether fixed or variable, competing functions, such as growth and
contribute to broader behavioural strategies and reproduction, which are affected by ecologi-
our overall phenotype. Behavioural strategies cal pressures, the differential proportion of
can become stable in evolutionary terms,[69] which result in diverse health outcomes.[71]
but changes to our environment create pres- Consider the relationship between wealth and
sures, which alter learning and lead to new reproduction – increasing wealth predicts
behaviours and strategies. This implies that higher long-term fitness and reproductive
for a set of environment conditions, including success but a trade-off also exists between
socioeconomic, there is likely to be an optimal the number of offspring produced and the
132 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

wealth transmitted to each; this balance has immediate adversity or danger. In this case, it
been found to be predicted by socioeconomic makes sense to opt for shorter-term rewards
and environmental conditions.[39] Accruing and payoffs. Chances of survival have been
wealth in the developed world requires time found to profoundly shape reproductive strat-
and career focus, which is time spent away egy,[39] and are likely to affect strategies
from having and raising children; women relating to health or wealth in a similar way.
in particular face a decision of how much An individual’s life history reflects trade-offs
wealth to accrue before having children. in the allocation of time or energy, and may
Accruing wealth is desirable but there is risk- be categorised as slow or fast depending on
taking relevance given that fertility decreases the environmental conditions and perceived
with increasing age. Risk-taking decisions time horizons.[76] Preferable environments
can also be observed more directly in a health are likely to induce slower or longer life
setting. The choice to forgo protection dur- histories, which are associated with higher
ing sex, such as a condom, is risky given the investment, lower impulsivity, and lower lev-
increased chance of getting an infection or an els of risk taking. Conversely, shorter life his-
unwanted pregnancy. However, unprotected tories are associated with future discounting,
sex is more pleasurable for men and women, impulsivity, and risk taking.[76]
and represents a reward to counter the risk;
research has shown that the greater the per-
ceived pleasure differential, the more will-
ing men and women would be to engage in APPLICATIONS OF EVOLUTIONARY
unprotected sex.[72] Similarly, eating HFSS CONCEPTS TO PUBLIC HEALTH
foods, drinking alcohol, or using other sub- PHENOMENA
stances can also be pleasurable in the short
term but each behaviour incurs long-term Investing and offsetting decision making is
risks to morbidity and mortality. It is also well fundamental to impulsive and reflective deci-
established that health risk behaviours cluster sion making and may be applied to various
together,[73] implying there is an underlying health behaviours. Exercise can be consid-
risk-taking strategy for any individual within ered as investing behaviour; while there may
a specific set of circumstances. be short-term rewards for some people, costs
Varying strategies of health risk, as shaped are involved in the form of time and energy.
by external factors, can also be illustrated in The investment is made with an idea that
financial terms. Low-income workers have there will be a reward of good health and
been found to be less likely to participate in potentially a long-term benefit of longer life.
insurance schemes, even when large subsi- Buying healthy foods can also be considered
dies are offered.[74] Despite the losses of not as an investment; fresh ingredients tend to
having insurance being potentially ruinous, cost more and usually take more time to pre-
low-income workers are less likely to partici- pare but they may represent a similar long-
pate because they have less to spend and, cru- term goal to that of exercise. In many
cially, less to lose. Conversely, gambling and societies, health insurance represents an
playing the lottery is more popular among investment; it may be part of a healthcare
poorer groups;[75] although the chance of system or it may represent a better level of
winning is slim, the relative reward is higher care. Like risk-aversion behaviours, these
for someone who has less. One could predict decisions are more likely to be taken by indi-
that people in adverse circumstances, whether viduals who have the financial means but
environmental, developmental, or subjective, also who perceive the long-term benefits to
would devalue future payoffs as the cost of be worth the investment. For those who do
waiting is perceived to be higher given the not have the means or think in temporally
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND PUBLIC HEALTH 133

shorter terms due to adversity in one or more circumstances can help to illustrate how
areas of their lives, investments are less extreme behaviours might make sense in
likely to be made. Offsetting behaviour can terms of the adaptive appraisal systems that
be thought of in similar but subtly different underpin them. Victims of childhood physi-
terms; instead of immediate investment costs cal or sexual abuse commonly grow up to
being incurred, immediate rewards are suffer poor health outcomes; not only is
rejected but still with the expectation of cognitive development impaired, potentially
higher rewards in the future. Choosing not to leading to reduced reflective decision mak-
eat HFSS foods, drink alcohol, or use sub- ing, but the immediate reward that may be
stances are examples of offsetting behaviour provided by an unhealthy behaviour would
as they are immediately pleasurable but in almost always outweigh the cost of waiting
accumulation are likely to incur risks to long- for a larger future reward. Individuals who
term health. suffer from mental health conditions could
Altruistic and cooperative behaviours also make a similar appraisal; for a person living
have relevance to health. In ancestral envi- with severe depression, for example, poten-
ronments, altruism may have evolved partly tial benefits in the distant future may feel
in response to foraging and food sharing; completely irrelevant. In these examples,
evidence has shown higher levels of altruistic the immediate reward could be alleviation
behaviour to be associated with better health or self-­medication from the ongoing costs of
and well-being. Cooperation has relevance extreme low mood. These unhealthy behav-
given that sharing resources equally allows iours, which incur long-term risks to health
for moderation and fairness but requires trust and fitness, are uniquely adaptable in that
that community members will not take more they may be survival mechanisms. Consider
than their fair share. The global food industry populations living in extreme environmen-
is a good example of non-cooperative behav- tal conditions such as war or within violent
iour whereby nation states or companies com- gang culture. Individuals are aware that
pete to gain as much market share as possible. their chances of survival are reduced, which
Such competition has led to over-farming diminishes the value of any future reward as
and fishing, which have led to environmental they may not live to benefit from it. Short-
destruction. Huge production and advertising term strategies would always be optimal in
of processed HFSS foods within a global mar- situations such as these since the rewards of
ket have also pushed consumption to the point tomorrow may never come.
that the majority of the world’s populations For any individual, health behaviours
live in countries where they are more likely can be better understood if framed as adap-
to suffer mortality from the consequences tive strategies that can be predicted by envi-
of living with overweight than living with ronmental, developmental, and subjective
underweight.[77] These pressures operate in a circumstances. Early exploratory work has
similar way to the other health-relevant evolu- demonstrated that environmental conditions
tionary behaviours, whether for individuals or shape the strategies that drive health behav-
larger organisations. Longer-term rewards that iour and that such strategies are predictive of
may be gained with altruistic and cooperative various adaptive or evolutionary strategies.
strategies are not optimal given the context, [70] Public health practitioners understand the
in this case the free-market food system. The determinants of poor health but there is little
optimal strategy for an individual, company, acknowledgement that there may be circum-
or nation state in such circumstances is to take stances in which it makes more sense to under-
the immediate payoff or reward. take unhealthy rather than healthy behaviours.
Extreme examples of adverse devel- Under such circumstances, it seems point-
opmental, subjective, and environmental less to try to change the behaviour itself, if
134 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

the underlying strategy is not acknowledged in addressing the obesity epidemic and other
and the determinants of that strategy are not major risk factors for NCDs.
addressed. Such an approach may also widen Developmental experiences often con-
the public health perspective. Often academ- tribute to subjective issues that continue to
ics and practitioners operate in silos, focusing underpin unhealthy behavioural strategies in
on specific health risk behaviours, interven- adults but, equally, poor adult well-being or
tions, or risk factors for disease, but perhaps mental health problems can be triggered by
the problem needs to be re-framed. In addi- life circumstances at any time. Recognition
tion to addressing the morbidity arising from and appropriate treatment are hugely impor-
obesity, how can we triangulate research and tant not only to treat the disorder but also to
practice to reduce the risk factors for NCDs re-balance the appraisal system that favours
more broadly? A key element of this chal- short-term rewards but has consequences for
lenge may be seeking to address the deter- long-term health. Mental and physical health
minants of health risk behaviours. The public are often treated separately but are inextrica-
health response must be informed, paradigms bly linked and are often cyclical. We cannot
may need to be challenged, and we will need expect an individual to make choices that are
a whole system and population approach good for their long-term health when they are
to address a whole system and population struggling to cope with the here and now.
problem. Given the complexity of the prob- Further to developmental and subjective
lem, no simple solution is likely to be suc- determinants, the environments in which we
cessful and each of the key determinants of live must be adapted to address key public
unhealthy behavioural strategies need careful health problems and particularly obesity. There
consideration. is a clear need to create local physical and
In public health, it is understood that pre- social environments that are conducive to good
vention is better than cure and this is espe- health; there are multiple and varied features
cially true with the obesity problem. We of our home, social, and work environments
understand how important and formative that affect our health and well-being. However,
developmental experiences are, and that the there is a bigger challenge to enabling good
impact of adversity may be irreversible in health, and it lies in the industrial, political,
terms of cognitive structures and resulting and philosophical climate in which we live. We
behaviours. In a similar way, the public health are the objects of a free-market food system in
response to the obesity problem must begin which the primary objective of any company
from pregnancy, and carry on through early is to make money and grow, not always but
years, childhood, and adolescence, given often at the expense of ill health and a burden
that nurturing environments with healthy of disease. The food system is incredibly com-
parent–child bonds are crucial to long-term plex but as long as companies large and small
health. Everything is important in early life are permitted to produce unhealthy products,
from breastfeeding and weaning to a healthy saturate the market, and advertise ubiquitously,
school environment and safe, enabling com- we cannot hope to shift the behaviour of popu-
munities. Children who live with obesity are lations in a meaningful way. There has been
likely to become adults who live with obesity, a helpful paradigm shift among academics
and adults very rarely sustain weight loss. In and public health experts, who now consider
addition to meeting health recommendations it unreasonable for populations to achieve
for early life, it is our developmental expe- and maintain a healthy weight through indi-
rience that can profoundly affect our short- vidual agency alone. Powerful regulations for
and long-term behaviours. Creating positive industry must be demanded by the public and
childhood experiences is a complex societal actioned by policy makers, if we are to tackle
challenge but it must be recognised as critical the obesity epidemic in a meaningful way.
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND PUBLIC HEALTH 135

Perhaps more importantly, the greatest EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND


challenge of our time is to reduce socioeco- PUBLIC HEALTH: CASE STUDY –
nomic inequalities. At a population level, VACCINATIONS
individuals from relatively poorer groups
make unhealthier choices, these choices For over 200 years,[80 ] vaccinations have pre-
are likely to be underpinned by behavioural
vented the consequences of disease for millions
strategies, and it is likely that these strategies
of people worldwide. Vaccines are a public
are adaptive. Trying to change the outcomes
health success story and are estimated to have
of the behaviour is unlikely to be effective
saved at least 10 million lives between 2010
without consideration of the environmen-
and 2015 alone.[81] Vaccinations stimulate an
tal conditions that determined the strategy.
immune response from the body to develop
Reducing inequalities is a global problem and
resistance to a pathogen, which can lead to the
requires unprecedented change but in terms
prevention or complete eradication of an infec-
of the public health response, interventions
tious disease.[82] Vaccines have successfully
and policies must be careful that they reduce
led to the worldwide eradication of smallpox
rather than exacerbate inequalities. For
and widespread eradication of measles, polio,
example, when dealing with obesity, nation-
and tetanus. Other standard vaccine-­preventable
wide policies that address issues such as pric-
ing are likely to reduce inequalities, while diseases include diphtheria, whooping cough,
person-focused interventions that encourage mumps, rubella, influenza, and hepatitis, but
behaviour change are likely to widen them. there are many other infectious diseases that
[78] Health disparities are the most serious have available vaccines.
outcome of socioeconomic inequalities, the Vaccines are a basic human right and have
reduction of which will certainly require an greatly reduced the burden of disease[83] but
understanding of the implications and appli- despite this success, a determined antivaccine
cations of evolutionary principles.[79] lobby, representing an increasingly sceptical
These broad discussion points do not world view of science, is on the rise.[84] The
overlook the huge amount of academic antivaccine movement has gained traction
and public health work that contributes online and is suggested to be well organised
substantially and meaningfully to addressing and widely dispersed.[85] Antivaccine mes-
obesity and other health problems. The sages question vaccine safety and effective-
object here is to suggest that an evolutionary ness and have been reported to rely on strong
perspective of health risk behaviours may emotional messages to enhance their appeal.
be helpful, given that they are likely to be [86] For this reason, a minority of antivac-
optimal and adaptive. Recognition of the cine advocates have been suggested to have
strategies that drive unhealthy choices, and a highly persuasive voice and the potential
the key determinants of them, will not only to persuade millions of parents against vac-
encourage a focus on the root causes of cinations on scientific, ethical, and political
health problems,[2] but might also reduce grounds. Medical and academic experts have
the stigmatisation of health risk behaviours tried to counter such messages to advocate
and the individuals that carry their burden. the safety and importance of vaccines but
An engrained mantra of public health is to have often suffered ‘harassment campaigns’,
‘make the healthy choice the easy choice’ typically via social media by vehement anti-
but in evolutionary terms the easy choice vaccine activists.[87] While the most effec-
is the adaptive choice; perhaps we have tive strategy at combatting this problem is
known for some time that health risk unclear,[88] the antivaccine movement rep-
behaviours are a normal response to an resents a difficult and complex challenge for
abnormal world. public health policy makers and practitioners.
136 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

While the academic consensus is that the but is less than 100%; estimates suggest
antivaccine movement is based on miscon- that for a disease such as measles the herd
ceptions,[89] there have been controversial immunity threshold is 93–95%; for diseases
studies reporting links between vaccinations such as smallpox or polio the threshold is
and disability and disease, such as autism and 80–86%.[102] Aside from the questionable
asthma, despite strong evidence to the con- links between vaccines and disability or dis-
trary.[90–92 ] Emotive online pressure and ease, there are various risks involved with
spurious evidence have succeeded to some vaccinating, typically low-level side effects.
degree. In the UK, the link with the measles, Vaccines can be painful and create soreness
mumps, and rubella (MMR) vaccine and and swelling, and in some cases they can also
autism appears to have gained momentum induce allergic reactions and subjects may
and, following year-on-year increases from become a little unwell. Individual and popu-
2007, decreases in coverage were recorded lation immunity is a clear benefit of vaccina-
in 2016/17 for the third year in a row.[93] In tions, while the risks and side effects can be
the United States, while the overall rate of interpreted as costs.
vaccination coverage has remained high, the An application of the principles of evolu-
rate of children not receiving any vaccina- tionary psychology can add insight into the
tions has been increasing since 2011.[94] The decision-making process of whether or not
consequences of the growing antivaccina- to vaccinate. Altruism is an act that benefits
tion movement have already been observed; others (non-kin) at expense to the actor,[103]
analysis of a measles outbreak in the United and cooperation, like altruism, provides a
States in 2015 indicated that the likely cause benefit to another individual or group but
was lack of or incomplete vaccinations.[95] does not require reciprocation since it also
Multiple developed and developing countries yields direct or indirect fitness benefits to the
have experienced outbreaks of measles in actor.[104] The process of vaccinating does
recent years,[96–97] which have been pri- not neatly fit with either principal definition;
marily attributed to gaps in vaccination cov- it is altruistic in that a cost is incurred and the
erage.[97–98] process benefits others, but it is also coop-
Research suggests that framing public erative in that it yields direct fitness benefits
health messages within the context of the to the actor. Vaccinating involves elements of
moral foundations that underpin judgements altruism and cooperation; the commonality
can successfully influence attitudes.[99] between the two is reliance on others to max-
Moral foundations theory proposes that inter- imise individual and group benefits.
nal decision making predicts social group However, within populations counter
attitudes.[100] While there is complexity strategies may occur arising from selfish
surrounding decision-making processes, con- or cheating behaviours that yield benefits
sideration of selective pressures and evolu- to the individual with no personal cost but
tionary principles that underpin motivations with an expense to others. Cheating strate-
may provide an enhanced understanding of gies may often lead to higher relative fitness
public health decision making. than altruistic strategies[105], but if caught,
The incentive to vaccinate is clear; it leads cheats are likely to be punished. Emotional
to individual and group immunity and is the mechanisms have evolved, including mor-
best defence against potentially epidemic alistic aggression, trust, suspicion, and dis-
diseases. If sufficiently high proportions of honesty, a function of which is to regulate
people vaccinate, herd immunity[101] can decisions and behaviours relating to altruism
be achieved whereby a whole population and cheating.[106] Cheating, free riding, or
becomes resistant to a parasite or disease. bandwagonning have been found to moti-
The coverage threshold varies by disease vate vaccination decisions[107], and across
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND PUBLIC HEALTH 137

a large population, cheating is incentivised of information relating to the product and


given that benefits from herd immunity can the process. Research suggests that 37% of
be realised without paying the individual US adults when questioned were not aware
costs, assuming that the proportion of free of the concept of herd immunity and over
riders does not exceed the threshold. One 75% thought that vaccination coverage was
would expect a successful cheating strategy higher than it was.[112] While there is some
to not only be hidden but to outwardly sup- evidence that educational interventions can
port vaccinations, in order that others vacci- improve willingness to vaccinate,[112] most
nate and herd immunity is maintained. traditional education tools have been found to
Yet there appears to be a maladaptive cul- have little impact on vaccine hesitancy,[113]
tural phenomenon whereby, far from being including those that specifically dispel the
hidden, not vaccinating or cheating in an myths linking vaccines with disability.[114]
evolutionary sense is publicly advocated and Since antivaccine lobby groups assume a
promoted. It may be that cultural misinfor- disproportionate space within the discus-
mation has shifted the perceived benefits sion arena,[115] the key response may be to
and costs to such an extent that the adaptive engage with mass media and initiate wide-
strategies of vaccinating or free riding have spread vaccination-promotion campaigns;
been abandoned in favour of a wholly mala- there is some evidence that such campaigns
daptive strategy. This is likely to be an exam- can reduce vaccine hesitancy and increase
ple of humans being adaptively mismatched coverage rates.[116] Perhaps fighting the fire
from a technologically developed world; of powerful antivaccine lobbyists requires the
our psychological mechanisms that strate- clear and effective communication of scien-
gize our behaviour may be fundamentally tific evidence via the medium of social and
misinformed leading to suboptimal decision digital mass media.
making. Vaccinations are a relatively recent One further much discussed public health
phenomenon and were not a feature of our response could be to make vaccinations com-
formative and selective environments, but pulsory; proposals are currently in place in
our ability to make adaptive choices in novel various countries in the developing world in
situations is a key feature of our behavioural light of recent outbreaks.[117] Aside from
plasticity. It seems likely that a successful the potential implications for civil liberties
public health response will need to change and the difficulty in implementation, it has
the parameters of the game in order that also been suggested that mandating vaccines
modern humans can once again make sense would not address the causal problems of
of their world. vaccine hesitancy[118] and may increase the
Perhaps a consequence of living in urban inherent mistrust of the political health sys-
populations rather than smaller communi- tem.[115]
ties is that people have become less trusting; Accounting for the available evidence,
the choice not to vaccinate has been found there have been sensible and well-considered
to be influenced by confidence, not only in recommendations of best practice to improve
the product but also the health professional vaccination coverage in an age of inherent
and the policy maker.[108–109] Evidence mistrust and hesitancy. Calls to utilise immu-
suggests public confidence in vaccinating is nisation frameworks, apply multi-component
decreasing,[110] and that social norms and interventions, link interventions to empirical
interactions with healthcare providers are evidence, and take account of the person-
highly influential on decisions to vaccinate. and environment-specific factors[36] are
[111] A response by public health policy mak- valid and helpful. However, mindfulness of
ers and practitioners may be to foster trust by the maladaptive strategies that have propa-
increasing the availability and transparency gated in recent years and the evolutionary
138 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

decision-making mechanisms that appear 9 Litman, G.W. and Cooper, M.D. (2007). Why
to be misfiring may be crucial in reminding study the evolution of immunity? Nature
parents that, in this case, the adaptive choice Immunology. 8. 547–548.
is the healthy choice. A successful campaign 10 Schmid-Hempel, P. (2011). Evolutionary Para-
may enable free riding to become a success- sitology: the Integrated Study of Infections,
Immunology, Ecology, and Genetics. Oxford
ful strategy once again, as long as cheats are
Scholarship Online. doi:10.1093/acprof:
low in number and quiet of voice, in order oso/9780199229482.001.0001.
that they alone incur the risks. 11 Greaves, M. (2010). Cancer stem cells: back
to Darwin? Seminars in Cancer Biology. 20.
2. 65–70.
12 World Health Organization. (2018).
REFERENCES Noncommunicable diseases – key facts.
[Date accessed: 4th March 2019] www.who.
1 Acheson, D. (1988). Public Health in England: int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/
The Report of the Committee of Inquiry into noncommunicable-diseases.
the Future Development of the Public Health 13 World Health Organization. (2019). Global
Function. Cm. 289. London: HMSO. Health Observatory (GHO) data – risk factors.
2 Gluckman, P.D., Low, F.M., Buklijas, T., [Date accessed: 10th March 2019] www.
Hanson, M.A. and Beedle, A.S. (2011). How who.int/gho/ncd/risk_factors/en/.
evolutionary principles improve the 14 World Health Organization. (2018). Taking
understanding of human health and disease. action on childhood obesity. [Date accessed:
Evolutionary Applications. 4. 2. 249–263. 10th March 2019] https://www.who.int/end-
3 Stringhini, S., Carmeli, C., Jokela, M., childhood-obesity/publications/taking-
Avendaño, M., Muennig, P., Guida, F. et  al. action-childhood-obesity-report/en/.
(2017). Socioeconomic status and the 25 × 15 Wang, Y.C., McPherson, K., Marsh, T.,
25 risk factors as determinants of premature Gortmaker, S.L. and Brown, M. (2011).
mortality: a multicohort study and meta- Health and economic burden of the projected
analysis of 1.7 million men and women. The obesity trends in the USA and the UK.
Lancet. 389. 10075. 1229–1237. Lancet. 378. 815–825.
4 Nettle, D. and Pollet, T.V. (2008). Natural 16 Butland, B. (2007). Tackling obesities: future
selection on male wealth in humans. The choices. Project report. London: Government
American Naturalist. 172. 5. 658–666. Office for Science.
doi:10.1086/591690. 17 Schrempft, S., van Jaarsveld, C.H.M., Fisher,
5 Gillespie, D.O.S., Russell, A.F. and Lummaa, V. A., Herle, M., Smith, A.D., Fildes, A. et  al.
(2008). When fecundity does not equal fitness: (2018). Variation in the heritability of child body
evidence of an offspring quantity versus quality mass index by obesogenic home environment.
trade-off in pre-industrial humans. Proceedings JAMA Pediatrics. 172. 12. 1153–1160.
of the Royal Society. 275. 713–722. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2018.1508.
6 Giosan, C., Mureșan, V., Wyka, K., Mogoașe, 18 Institute of Medicine (US) Committee on
C., Cobeanu, O. and Szentagotai, A. (2018). Assessing Interactions Among Social,
The Evolutionary Fitness Scale: A measure Behavioral, and Genetic Factors in Health;
of the independent criterion of fitness. Hernandez, L.M. and Blazer, D.G., editors.
The Journal of the Evolutionary Studies (2006). Genes, Behavior, and the Social
Consortium. 7. 1. 181–208. Environment: Moving Beyond the Nature/
7 Jones, J.H. (2009). The force of selection on Nurture Debate. Washington (DC): National
the human life cycle. Evolution and Human Academies Press (US) 3. Genetics and Health.
Behavior. 30. 305–314. 19 Llewellyn, C. and Wardle, J. (2013). Genetic
8 Jasienska, G. (2013). The Fragile Wisdom: An Influences on Child Eating Behaviour. In Ency-
Evolutionary View on Women’s Biology and clopedia on Early Childhood Development,
Health. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University edited by R.E. Tremblay, M. Boivin, R.D. Peters
Press. and M.S. Faith (eds). [Date accessed: July 15th
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND PUBLIC HEALTH 139

2019] https://eur01.safelinks.protection.out- 30 Nettle, D. (2010). Why are there social


look.com/?url=http%3A%2F%2Fwww. gradients in preventative health behaviour?
child-encyclopedia.com%2Fchild- A perspective from behavioural ecology.
nutrition%2Faccording-experts%2Fgenetic- PLoS One. 5. 10. 1–6.
influences-child-eating-behaviour&amp;data 31 Ball, K. and Crawford, D. (2005).
=02%7C01%7C%7Cfb2a1bd6fa7f449a946 Socioeconomic status and weight change in
008d70933e623%7C1faf88fea9984c5b93c adults: a review. Social Science and Medicine.
9210a11d9a5c2%7C0%7C1%7C63698799 60. 1987–2010.
0829674502&amp;sdata=%2BFkWye4%2F 32 Monteiro, C.A., Conde, W.L., Lu, B. and
G61UawslrZ%2FotY6r6naJkGGIwP8t6M9lUs Popkin, B.M. (2004). Obesity and inequities in
U%3D&amp;reserved=0. health in the developing world. International
20 Swinburn, B.A., Sacks, G., Hall, K.D., Journal of Obesity. 28. 1181–1186.
McPherson, K., Finegood, D.T., Moodie, M.L. 33 van Zon, S.K.R., Bültmann, U., Mendes de
et  al. (2011). The global obesity pandemic: Leon, C.F. and Reijneveld, S.A. (2015).
shaped by global drivers and local Absolute and relative socioeconomic health
environments. The Lancet. 378. 9793. inequalities across age groups. PLoS One.
804–814. 10. 12. e0145947.
21 United Nations. (2018). News: 68% of the 34 Edin, K., Boyd, M., Mabli, J., Ohls, J.,
world population projected to live in urban Worthington, J., Greene, S. et  al. (2013).
areas by 2050. [Date accessed: 12th March SNAP Food Security In-Depth Interview
2019] www.un.org/development/desa/en/ Study: Final Report. Washington, DC: US
news/population/2018-revision-of-world- Department of Agriculture, Food and
urbanization-prospects.html. Nutrition Service.
22 Foster, C., Hillsdon, M., Cavil, N., Allender, 35 Gordon-Larsen, P., Nelson, M.C., Page, P.
S. and Cowburn, G. (2005). Understanding and Popkin, B.M. (2006). Inequality in the
participation in sport: a systematic review. built environment underlies key health
London: Sport England. disparities in physical activity and obesity.
23 Goode, W.J. (1964). The Family. Englewood Pediatrics. 117. 2. 417–424.
Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall Inc. 36 Cummins, S. and Macintyre, S. (2006). Food
24 Krebs, C.J. (1978). Ecology. The experimental environments and obesity: neighbourhood
analysis of distribution and abundance. New or nation? International Journal of Epidemi-
York, United States: Joanna Cotler Books. ology. 35. 100–104.
ISBN 10: 0060437715.(Second edition). 37 Wang, Y. and Beydoun, M.A. (2007). The
25 Maynard-Smith, J. (1978). Optimization obesity epidemic in the United States –
theory in evolution. Annual Review of gender, age, socioeconomic, racial/ethnic,
Ecology and Systematics. 9. 31–56. and geographic characteristics: a systematic
26 Winterhalder, B. (1986). The analysis of review and meta-regression analysis.
hunter-gatherer diet: stalking an optimal Epidemiologic Reviews. 29. 6–28.
foraging model. In Food Preferences and 38 Starratt, V.G. (2017). Evolved psychological
Aversions, M. Harris and E.A. Ross (eds). mechanisms. In Encyclopedia of Personality
Philadelphia, PA: Temple University Press. and Individual Differences, V. Zeigler-Hill and
311–339. T.K. Shackelford (eds). Cham, Switzerland:
27 Power, M.L. and Schulkin, J. (2009). The Springer International Publishing. doi:10.1007/
evolution of obesity. Baltimore, MD: John 978-3-319-28099-8_1633-1.
Hopkins University Press. 39 Chisholm, J.S. (1993). Death, hope, and sex:
28 Dallman, M.F., Pecararo, N. and la Fleur, S.E. life-history theory and the development of
(2005). Chronic stress and comfort foods: reproductive strategies. Current
self-medication and abdominal obesity. Brain, Anthropology. 34. 1–24.
Behaviour and Immunity. 19. 275–280. 40 Tooby, J. and Cosmides, L. (1990). On the
29 Lancet editorial. (2011). Urgently needed: a universality of human nature and the unique-
framework convention for obesity control. ness of the individual: the role of genetics and
378. 741. adaptation. Journal of Personality. 58. 17–67.
140 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

41 Wilson, D.S. (1994). Adaptive genetic varia- doi:10.1002/9780470015902.a0003671.


tion and human evolutionary psychology. pub2.
Ethology and Sociobiology. 15. 219–235. 53 Pechey, R., Jebb, S.A., Kelly, M.P., Almiron-
42 Tooby, J. and Cosmides, L. (1995). Mapping Roig, E., Conde, S., Nakamura, R. et  al.
the evolved functional organization of mind (2013). Socioeconomic differences in pur-
and brain. In The Cognitive Neurosciences, chases of more vs. less healthy foods and
M.S. Gazzaniga (ed.). 3995. Cambridge, beverages: analysis of over 25,000 British
MA: MIT Press. 1185–1197. households in 2010. Social Science and
43 Mastrandea, R., Gabrielli, A., Piras, F., Medicine. 92. 22–26.
Spalletta, G., Caldarelli, G. and Gili, T. (2017). 54 Kipping, R.R., Smith, M., Heron, J., Hickman,
Organization and hierarchy of the human M. and Campbell, R. (2015). Multiple risk
functional brain network lead to a chain-like behaviour in adolescence and socio-
core. Scientific Reports. 7. 4888. 1–13. economic status: findings from a UK birth
doi:10.1038/s41598-017-04716-3. cohort. European Journal of Public Health.
44 Nolte, J. (2002). The Human Brain: An 25. 1. 44–49.
introduction to its functional anatomy. 55 Engelhardt, C. and Wagener, A. (2014).
Missouri, United States: Mosby. Biased Perceptions of Income Inequality and
45 Caramazza, A. and Coltheart, M. (2006). Redistribution. CESifo Working Paper Series
Cognitive neuropsychology twenty years No. 4838. Munich: CESifo Group.
on. Cognitive Neuropsychology. 23. 1. 56 Marsheb, R.M. and Grilo, C.M. (2006). Emo-
3–12. tional overeating and its associations with
46 Russell, S.J., Hughes, K. and Bellis, M.A. eating disorder psychopathology among over-
(2016). Impact of childhood experience and weight patients with binge eating disorder.
adult well-being on eating preferences and International Journal of Eating Disorders. 39.
behaviours. BMJ Open. 6. 1. e007770. 141–146.
doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2015-007770. 57 Bateson, P. and Gluckman, P. (2011). Plastic-
47 Johnson, J.G., Cohen, P., Kasen, S. and ity, Robustness, Development and Evolution.
Brook, J.S. (2002). Childhood adversities Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
associated with risk for eating disorders or 58 Kim, S. and Lee, D. (2011). Prefrontal cortex
weight problems during adolescence or early and impulsive decision making. Biological
adulthood. American Journal of Psychiatry. Psychiatry. 69. 12. 1140–1146.
159. 394–400. 59 Del Giudice, M., Ellis, B. and Shirtcliff, E.A.
48 Ebbeling, C.D., Pawlak, D.B. and Ludwig, (2011). The adaptive calibration model of
D.S. (2002). Childhood obesity: public health stress responsivity. Neuroscience &
crisis, common sense cure. Lancet. 360. Biobehavioral Reviews. 35. 1562–1592.
473–482. 60 Wiers, R.W., Bartholow, B.D., van den
49 Koch, F.S., Sepa, A. and Ludvigsson, J. Wildenberg, E., Thush, C., Engels, R.C.M.E.,
(2008). Psychological stress and obesity. Sher, K.J. et al. (2007). Automatic and
Journal of Pediatrics. 153. 6. controlled processes and the development of
50 Gustafson, T.B. and Sarwer, D.B. (2004). addictive behaviors in adolescents: a review
Childhood sexual abuse and obesity. Obesity and a model. Pharmacology, Biochemistry
Reviews. 5. 129–135. and Behavior. 86. 263–283.
51 Hassel, S., McKinnon, M.C., Cusi, A.M. and 61 Carver, C.S. (2005). Impulse and constraint:
Macqueen, G.M. (2011). An overview of perspectives from personality psychology,
psychological and neurobiological mecha- convergence with theory in other areas, and
nisms by which early negative experiences potential for integration. Personality and
increase risk of mood disorders. Journal of Social Psychology Review. 9. 312–333.
the Canadian Academy of Child and Adoles- 62 Hofmann, W., Friese, M. and Wiers, R.
cent Psychiatry. 20. 4. 277–288. (2008). Impulsive versus reflective influences
52 Borgerhoff-Mulder, M. and Schacht, R. on health behavior: a theoretical framework
(2012). Human behavioural ecology. In eLS. and empirical review. Health Psychology
Chichester: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Review. 2. 2. 111–137.
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND PUBLIC HEALTH 141

63 Hansen, E.B. and Breivik, G. (2001). coverage by low-income workers: can reduced
Sensation seeking as a predictor of positive premiums achieve full coverage? HSR: Health
and negative risk behaviour among Services Research. 32. 4. 453–470.
adolescents. Personality and Individual 75 Beckert, J. and Lutter, M. (2012). Why the
Differences. 30. 627–640. poor play the lottery: sociological approaches
64 Frankenhuis, W.E., Panchanathan, K. and to explaining class-based lottery play.
Nettle, D. (2016). Cognition in harsh and Sociology. 47. 6. 1152–1170.
unpredictable environments. Current 76 Kruger, D.J. and Kruger, J.S. (2016).
Opinion in Psychology. 7. 76–80. Psychometric assessment of human life
65 Bogg, T. and Roberts, B. W. (2004). history predicts health related behaviors.
Conscientiousness and health-related Psychological Topics. 25. 19–28.
behaviors: A meta-analysis of the leading 77 World Health Organization. (2018). Obesity
behavioral contributors to mortality. and overweight: key facts. [Accessed: 20th
Psychological Bulletin. 130. 887–919. March 2019] www.who.int/news-room/fact-
66 Grano, N., Virtanen, M., Vaherta, J., sheets/detail/obesity-and-overweight.
Elovainio, M. and Kivimäki, M. (2004). 78 McGill, R., Anwar, E., Orton, L., Bromley, H.,
Impulsivity as a predictor of smoking and Lloyd-Williams, F., O’Flaherty, M. et  al.
alcohol consumption. Personality and (2015). Are interventions to promote healthy
Individual Differences. 37. 1693–1700. eating equally effective for all? Systematic
67 Read, D., Frederick, S., Orsel, B. and review of socioeconomic inequalities in
Rahman, J. (2005). Four score and seven impact. BMC Public Health. 15. 457.
years from now: the date/delay effect in doi:10.1186/s12889-015-1781-7.
temporal discounting. Management Science. 79 Omenn, G.S. (2010). Evolution and public
51. 1326–1335. health. Proceedings of the National Academy
68 Daugherty, J.R. and Brase, G.L. (2009). of Sciences. 107. 1. 1702–1709.
Taking time to be healthy: predicting health
behaviors with delay discounting and time
perspective. Personality and Individual
Differences. 48. 202–207. CASE STUDY REFERENCES
69 Maynard-Smith, J. (1972). On Evolution.
Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press. 80 Stern, A. and Markel, H. (2005). The history
70 Russell, S.J. (2017). Applying evolutionary of vaccines and immunization: familiar
principles to the obesity problem and other patterns, new challenges. Health Affairs. 24.
issues in public health. [Date accessed: 18th 3: The Vaccine Enterprise.
March 2019] https://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails. 81 World Health Organization. (2017). The
do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.733959. doi: 10.24377/ power of vaccines: still not fully utilized.
LJMU.t.00007660. World Health Organization. [Date accessed:
71 Wells, J.C.K., Nesse, R.M., Sear, R., 6th January 2019] https ://www.who.int/
Johnstone, R.A. and Stearns, S.C. (2017). publications/10-year-review/vaccines/en/.
Evolutionary public health: introducing the 82 Fenner, F., Henderson, D.A., Arita, I., Jezek,
concept. Evolutionary Public Health. 390. Z. and Ladnyi, I.D. (1988). Smallpox and its
10093. 500–509. eradication, pp. 332–335. Geneva,
72 Randolph, M.E., Pinkerton, S.D., Bogart, Switzerland: World Health Organization.
L.M., Cecil, H. and Abramson, P.R. (2007). 83 Andre, F.E., Booy, R., Bock, H.L., Clemens,
Sexual pleasure and condom use. Archives of J., Datta, S.K., John, T.J. et  al. (2008).
Sexual Behavior. 36. 6. 844–848. Vaccination greatly reduces disease,
73 Spring, B., Moller, A.C. and Coons, M.J. disability, death and inequity worldwide.
(2012). Multiple health behaviours: overview Bulletin of the World Health Organization.
and implications. Journal of Public Health. 86. 2. 81–160.
34. i3–10.10.1093/pubmed/fdr111. 84 Kata, A. (2012). Anti-vaccine activists web
74 Chernew, M., Frick, K. and McLaughlin, G. 2.0 and the postmodern paradigm: an
(1997). The demand for health insurance overview of tactics and tropes used online by
142 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

the anti-vaccination movement. Vaccine. 30. Substandard vaccination compliance and the
25. 3778–3789. 2015 measles outbreak. JAMA Pediatrics.
85 Bandari, R., Zhou, Z., Qian, H., Tangherlini, 169. 5. 494–495.
T.R. and Roychowdhury, V.P. (2017). A resist- 96 World Health Organization. (2018).
ant strain: revealing the online grassroots rise Emergencies preparedness, response:
of the antivaccination movement. Computer. measles – Japan. [Date accessed: 20th January
50. 11. 60–67. 2019] www.who.int/csr/don/20-june-2018-
86 Wolfe, R.M., Sharp, L.K. and Lipsky, M.S. measles-japan/en/.
(2002). Content and design attributes of 97 Dyer, O. (2019). Philippines measles
antivaccination web sites. JAMA. 287. 24. outbreak is deadliest yet as vaccine scepticism
3245–3248. spurs disease comeback. BMJ. 364. l739.
87 Wong, J.C. (2019). Anti-vaxx ‘mobs’: doctors doi:https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.l739.
face harassment campaigns on Facebook. 98 World Health Organization. (2018b).
[Date accessed: 6th January 2019] www.the- Measles cases spike globally due to gaps in
guardian.com/technology/2019/feb/27/ vaccination coverage. [Date accessed: 20th
facebook-anti-vaxx-harassment-campaigns- January 2019] www.who.int/news-room/
doctors-fight-back. detail/29-11-2018-measles-cases-spike-
88 Leask, J. (2015). Should we do battle with globally-due-to-gaps-in-vaccination-coverage.
anti-vaccination activists? Public Health 99 Amin, A.B., Bednarczyk, R.A., Ray, C.E.,
Research and Practice. 30. 25. 2: e2521515. Melchiori, K.J., Graham, J., Huntsinger, J.R.
89 Jacobson, R.M., Targonski, P.V. and Poland, et  al. (2017). Association of moral values
G.A. (2007). A taxonomy of reasoning flaws with vaccine hesitancy. Nature Human
in the anti-vaccine movement. Vaccine. 25. Behaviour. 1. 873–880.
16. 3146–3152. 100 Graham, J., Nosek, B.A., Haidt, J., Iyer, R.,
90 Gerber, J.S. and Offit, P.A. (2009). Vaccines Koleva, S. and Ditto, P.H. (2011). Mapping
and autism: a tale of shifting hypotheses. the moral domain. Journal of Personality and
Clinical Infectious Diseases. 48. 4. 456–461. Social Psychology. 101. 366–385.
91 Destefano, F., Gu, D., Kramarz, P., Truman, 101 Anderson, R.M. and May, R.M. (1991).
B.I., Iademarco, M.F., Mullooly, J.P. et  al. Infectious Diseases of Humans: Dynamics
(2002). Childhood vaccinations and risk of and Control. Oxford: Oxford University
asthma. The Pediatric Infectious Disease Press.
Journal. 21. 6. 498–504. 102 Centers for Disease Control and Prevention.
92 Hviid, A., Hansen, J.V., Frisch, M. and (2015). Smallpox: Disease, Prevention, and
Melbye, M. (2019). Measles, mumps, rubella Intervention. Retrieved 13 March 2015.
vaccination and autism – a nationwide [Date accessed: 20th January 2019] https://
cohort study. Annals of Internal Medicine. stacks.cdc.gov/view/cdc/27929.
170. 8. 513–520. doi:10.7326/M18-2101. 103 O’Gorman, R., Wilson, D.S. and Miller, R.R.
93 National Health Service. (2017). Childhood (2005). Altruistic punishing and helping
Vaccination Coverage Statistics, England, differ in sensitivity to relatedness, friendship
2016–17. NHS Digital. [Date accessed: 8th and future interactions. Evolution and
January 2019] https://digital.nhs.uk/data- Human Behavior. 26. 375–387.
and-information/publications/statistical/nhs- 104 West, S.A., Griffin, A.S. and Gardner, A.
immunisation-statistics/childhood-vaccination- (2007). Evolutionary explanations for
coverage-statistics-england-2016-17. cooperation: review. Current Biology. 17.
94 Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. 16. 661–672.
(2018). Vaccination Coverage Among 105 Wade, M.J. and Breden, F. (1980). The
Children Aged 19–35 Months – United evolution of cheating and selfish behavior.
States, 2017. [Date accessed: 8th January Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 7. 3.
2019] www.cdc.gov/mmwr/volumes/67/wr/ 167–172.
mm6740a4.htm. 106 Trivers, R.L. (1971). The evolution of
95 Majumder, M.S., Cohn E.L., Mekaru, S.R., reciprocal altruism. The Quarterly Review of
Huston, J.E. and Brownstein, J.S. (2015). Biology. 46. 1. 35–57.
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND PUBLIC HEALTH 143

107 Hershey, J.C., Asch, D.A., Thumasathit, T., (2014). Effectiveness of interventions that
Meszaros, J. and Waters, V.V. (1994). The apply new media to improve vaccine uptake
roles of altruism, free riding, and and vaccine coverage: a systematic review.
bandwagoning in vaccination decisions. Human Vaccines and Immunotherapeutics.
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision 11. 1. 72–82. https://doi.org/10.4161/
Processes. 59. 2. 177–187. hv.34313.
108 MacDonald, N.E. (2014). Vaccine hesitancy: 114 Nyhan, B., Reifler, J., Richey, S. and Freed,
definition, scope and determinants. Vaccine. G.L. (2014). Effective messages in vaccine
33. 34. 4161–4164. promotion: a randomized trial. Pediatrics.
109 Larson, H.J., Schulz, W.S., Tucker, J.D. and 133. 4. 1–8.
Smith, D.M.D. (2015). Measuring vaccine 115 Dube, E., Gagnon, D. and MacDonald,
confidence: introducing a global vaccine N.E. (2015). Strategies intended to address
confidence index. Version 1 PLoS Currents. vaccine hesitancy: review of published
7: ecurrents.outbreaks.ce0f6177bc97332 reviews. Vaccine. 33. 4191–4203.
602a8e3fe7d7f7cc4. 116 Cairns, G., MacDonald, L., Angus, K., Walker,
110 Dubé, E., Vivion, M. and MacDonald, N.E. L., Cairns-Haylor, T. and Bowdler, T. (2012).
(2014). Vaccine hesitancy, vaccine refusal Systematic literature review of the evidence for
and the anti-vaccine movement: influence, effective national immunisation schedule
impact and implications. Expert Review of promotional communications. Stockholm:
Vaccines. 14. 1. 99–117. European Centre for Disease Prevention and
111 Leask, J., Willaby, H.W. and Kaufman, J. Control. [Date accessed 20th January 2019]
(2014). The big picture in addressing vaccine https://www.ecdc.europa.eu/sites/default/files/
hesitancy. Human Vaccines and Immunothera- media/en/publications/Publications/Literature-
peutics. 10. 9. 1–3. review-national-immunisation-schedule-
112 Logan, J., Nederhoff, D., Koch, B., Griffith, promotional-communications.pdf.
B., Wolfson, J., Awanal, F.A. et  al. (2018). 117 Arie, S. (2017). Compulsory vaccination
‘What have you HEARD about the HERD?’ and growing measles threat. British Medical
Does education about local influenza Journal. 358. j3429. doi:10.1136/bmj.j3429.
vaccination coverage and herd immunity 118 Wigham, S., Ternent, L., Bryant, A.,
affect willingness to vaccinate? Vaccine. 36. Robalino, S., Sniehotta, F.F. and Adams, J.
4118–4125. (2014). Parental financial incentives for
113 Odone, A., Ferrari, A., Spagnoli, F., increasing preschool vaccination uptake:
Visciarelli, S., Shefer, A., Pasquarella, C. et al. systematic review. Pediatrics. 134. 4. e1117.
7
Animal Ethics and Evolutionary
Psychology
Diana Santos Fleischman

INTRODUCTION exceedingly altruistic to favored animals, but


also industrialized cruelty towards less
Evolutionary psychology examines the favored animals at an incomprehensible
human mind through the lens of evolution to scale. While we should have no expectation
understand the functions of our psychological that human morality will be rational or con-
adaptations such as motivations, emotions, sistent, this chapter grapples with the fact that
and cognitions. Humans have many interac- our treatment of animals deviates very far
tions with nonhuman animals (hereafter just from any coherent, rational morality. In terms
‘animals’, although the term ‘nonhuman ani- of overall ‘suffering footprint’, human mal-
mals’ makes an important philosophical treatment of animals may be the biggest ethi-
point), most of which are fairly recent (e.g., cal issue in the world, and evolutionary
cats as companion animals), but some go psychology can give us deep insights into
much further back in our history (e.g., hunt- both the problem and possible solutions.
ing animals for food). We use animal bodies
for fur, meat, lactation, eggs, labor, and scien-
tific study; the scale of animal use is enor-
mous, trillions of animals are killed for food ANIMAL ETHICS
each year (Wiblin and Bollard, 2017). In the
last few hundred years, the average level of Animal ethics has different meanings among
human suffering has decreased dramatically, groups of philosophers, scientists, and the
but the total amount of animal suffering due public. Examining how our evolutionary
to human actions has skyrocketed. All around psychology obscures consistent ethics
us we can see examples of individuals being requires some consideration of what would
ANIMAL ETHICS AND EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY 145

serve as an ethical baseline for comparison EVIDENCE FOR ANIMAL SENTIENCE


(Fleischman, 2018). Views about human
obligations to animals, or lack thereof, have How can we establish which animals are sen-
proliferated in philosophy. Some philoso- tient, and therefore deserving of moral consid-
phers argue it’s wrong to own animals or use eration? Sentience is the ability to experience
them in any way (Francione, 2015), whereas pain and pleasure subjectively. First let’s dis-
others argue that we have no obligation to tinguish between the ability to react to tissue
animals because they cannot make social damage, and the ability to suffer. Nociception,
contracts and are thus not part of our moral or the experience of pain, is the simple and
community (Machan, 2004). Of all the main- ancient capacity of most animals to respond to
stream philosophical approaches to animal injuries that cause tissue damage. Nociception
ethics, utilitarianism and consequentialism is specific to animals with a nervous system,
have been most positively disposed to mor- but even simpler organisms may have a simi-
ally consider animals than other frameworks lar capacity to respond to harmful stimuli
(Beauchamp and Frey, 2011).
(Tomasik, 2018a). Behavioral evidence of
Utilitarianism defines what is good as what
nociception is, for example, a shrimp groom-
maximizes happiness or pleasure and mini-
ing an injured antenna (Elwood, 2011) or,
mizes suffering, across all sentient beings
physiologically, the measurement of neurons
(those capable of experiencing happiness or
firing in response to sensory stimulation
suffering) (Greene, 2013). This chapter rests
(Braithwaite, 2010). Sentience and the ability
on the basic evolutionary insight that verte-
to suffer is the subjective awareness of pleas-
brate animals like mammals and fish evolved
ure and pain and can be demonstrated when
the capacity to feel pain and pleasure, and
the response to stimuli is more complex
thus the capacity to suffer. Further, it rests
on a normative moral stance that sentience than a simple response to physical damage
should be the basis for moral consideration, (Braithwaite, 2010; Elwood, 2011).
that suffering is bad, and that reducing suf- Considering an evolutionary and functional
fering is good. I do not rely on any concept perspective, we can infer that subjective aware-
of ‘animal rights’, nor do I assume that using ness of suffering evolved to prevent and man-
animals as a means to human ends is always age bodily damage. If we take as given that
immoral (Regan, 2004), although I believe humans can suffer, and suffering has important
the moral foundations of these ideas are also adaptive functions in enabling our survival and
rooted in evolutionary psychology. For my reproduction, it’s parsimonious to assume that
normative moral claim that suffering is bad sentience and suffering evolved in other related
and alleviating suffering is good, most phi- animals, including many other vertebrates
losophers appeal to the reader’s personal (Tomasik, 2017). There is a solid foundation
experience with suffering, or take the idea of evidence that vertebrates and even some
that suffering is bad as obvious (‘a priori’ invertebrates evolved both nociception and
or ‘axiomatic’) (but see also Kahane, 2009). sentience (Braithwaite, 2010). Vertebrates as a
Evolutionary theory has influenced many to group generally have the same neurons, syn-
adopt an ethical stance that we should ascribe apses, and other neural hardware associated
moral standing and consideration on the basis with the ability to feel pain found in sentient
of the ability to suffer (Singer, 2011), other- humans. Fish brains, once thought to lack the
wise known as ‘sentientism’ (Ryder, 1991; fundamental hardware of sentience, have been
‘Sentientism’, 2019). This chapter analyzes found to have a brain region similar to the lim-
where our evolutionary psychology is con- bic system such that they may have the abil-
sistent with and deviates from sentientism as ity to ‘process information with an emotional
a moral baseline. component’ (Braithwaite, 2010: 102).
146 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Animal responses to pain, such as soliciting sets the stage for a planet filled with s­ entient
help, and avoiding stimuli previously asso- suffering both before and after humans
ciated with pain, are behavioral evidence achieved the technological means to exploit
of sentience. Many studies have shown that other species on an industrial scale.
invertebrates – widely thought to be incapa-
ble of sentience – show responses consistent
with subjective awareness of pain. For exam- Natural-Born Speciesists
ple, hermit crabs have been shown to make
adaptive tradeoffs when exposed to shock, There are two central questions with regards
choosing to endure more painful shock in to human morality towards animals: why do
a high-quality as opposed to a low-quality we value animals less than humans morally,
shell (Appel and Elwood, 2009). Trout given and why are our moral attitudes towards ani-
a painful injection in their lips failed to show mals so inconsistent? Both moral anthropo-
the normal neophobic response to a novel centrism and speciesism describe the concept
stimulus (a colorful block tower), compared of valuing human lives over animal lives,
to trout in the control condition; the trout’s although speciesism implies that this valua-
distraction from normal behavior because of tion is a form of prejudice due to mere spe-
pain suggests a subjective awareness of pain, cies membership (Caviola, 2019). Most
and thus suffering and sentience (Braithwaite, people value members of certain species
2010; Sneddon et al., 2003). Using crabs and above and beyond their ability to suffer, for
fish as examples is instructive, because they example valuing insensate humans (like those
show better objective evidence for suffering in a persistent vegetative state) more than
than human neonates do (Braithwaite, 2010: chimpanzees, and valuing dogs more than
153) – but babies would almost certainly be pigs, even though they are similarly sentient.
more likely to get the benefit of our doubt. Valuing humans over animals seems to be a
This is not to say that fish are equally sen- human moral universal. In one study, millions
tient with cats, dogs, babies, or adult humans. of participants all over the world overwhelm-
There are many potentially good reasons to ingly choose to save the life of a human over
prioritize some animals over others by virtue an animal, or several animals (Awad et  al.,
of their greater ability to suffer, but the influ- 2018). In another study, participants most
ence of, for example, brain size on degree of often choose to save one human over the lives
potential suffering is beyond the scope of this of several endangered animals (like gorillas)
chapter (see Tomasik, 2019). (Petrinovich et  al., 1993). This valuation is
The vast amount of sentience that evolved often reversed if the animal at risk is their pet.
across millions of species in our world – and When Topolski et  al. (2013) asked partici-
the resulting potential for suffering across pants who they would save if a bus was immi-
trillions of animals – can feel overwhelm- nently going to hit their pet or a foreign
ing. But that doesn’t mean it is not true – and tourist, 40% chose to save their pet. ‘The only
evolutionary psychologists have been coura- consistency in the way humans think about
geous in confronting other emotionally chal- animals is inconsistency’ (H.Herzog, 2010:
lenging, counter-intuitive truths. Evolution 14). Arguably, it can be rational to prioritize
may have created endless forms most beauti- some interests over others on the basis of
ful, but it would never have passed an eth- sentience, but, unsurprisingly, humans are not
ics review board (Bostrom, 2016: 188). doing any coherent form of ethical calculus
Evolutionary psychologists should take seri- when they choose actions or decide on the
ously the likelihood that evolution favors morality of those actions.
widespread sentience across species, but not Is there an inherent human moral response
widespread altruism to other species, and this towards animals? The study of how humans
ANIMAL ETHICS AND EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY 147

and animals interact has taken off in the last and moral disengagement. The consistent
two decades with the rise of anthrozool- thread in these perspectives is that violence
ogy, also known as human–animal relations is deeply unnatural. However, from the per-
(Amiot and Bastian, 2015; H. Herzog, 2010; spective of evolutionary morality, we should
Serpell, 1996). Anthrozoology is a diverse not expect sensitivity to animal suffering or
field, investigating topics like the therapeu- kindness to animals except to the extent that
tic properties of living with dogs, the possi- it helped us, for example when using animals
ble link between animal abuse and criminal for our benefit, or to signal our morality.
behavior, and the personality traits of animal-
rights activists (H. Herzog, 2010). These
fields have often tried to explain human Anthropomorphism
moral anthropocentrism and moral inconsist-
ency with descriptive frameworks like car- Anthropomorphism, or ascribing human char-
nism (the social ideology that supports meat acteristics to animals, is an apparently univer-
eating) (Joy, 2011), moral disengagement sal feature of the human mind (Urquiza-Haas
(i.e., we distance ourselves from our humane and Kotrschal, 2015). Anthropomorphism is
standards to harm animals) (Bandura, 1999; so naturally expressed that it’s difficult to
Vollum et  al., 2004), terror management think about animals in non-anthropomorphic,
theory (i.e., we cling to human uniqueness objective terms. Most children don’t make
and animal oppression to avoid existential clear distinctions between humans and ani-
anxiety) (Marino, 2019), and speciesism mals, and young children usually treat ani-
(discrimination based on species member- mals, like family pets, as human persons
ship) (Caviola et  al., 2018; Singer, 1995). (Serpell, 1996).
Explanations at a functional level of analysis Ironically, the basis for much of our moral
(Scott-Phillips et  al., 2011) and adaptation- feelings towards animals likely evolved so
ist accounts are less common in anthrozool- we could better exploit, kill, and eat them.
ogy (although see Bradshaw and Paul, 2010; Humans and animals aren’t so different, so
H. Herzog, 2002). the same theory of mind and empathy that
Evolutionary psychology is compat- helps us predict what humans do can also be
ible with most of the explanations of human used when hunting prey animals or avoiding
morality towards animals advanced by predators. Primatologists and other animal-
anthrozoologists. It posits functional expla- behavior scientists often use simple anthro-
nations for attitudes and behaviors as reflect- pomorphism to make predictions; behaviorist
ing evolved psychological adaptations (or John Garcia stated that anthropomorphism
their byproducts). Explanations like spe- with regard to rat behavior ‘works better than
ciesism and cultural explanations like car- most learning theories’ (Serpell, 1996: 174).
nism are not in conflict with evolutionary When you’re using the mind-reading ability
psychology (Tooby and Cosmides, 1989), that mostly evolved to predict the behavior of
because culture is both an outgrowth of and other people, you’re bound to ascribe human
a support for our evolved morality (e.g., the characteristics to animals. The ability to
cultural celebration of consensual courtship imagine what it was like to be an animal has
and maternal love). However, the assump- adaptive benefit to better predict the behav-
tion of some thinkers in anthrozoology, as ior of prey, predators, and dangerous animals
well as many animal-welfare advocates, (H. Herzog, 2002). Some speculate that
is that humans are naturally sensitive and empathy could have motivated nurturance
morally concerned about animal suffering, for domesticated species in animal husbandry
but this innate goodness is numbed by cultural (Bradshaw and Paul, 2010) however these ani-
and social factors like carnism (Joy, 2003) mals have also been bred to be cute and inspire
148 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

feelings of care. Given an adaptationist per- squealing bats into a fire and dissecting them
spective, empathy would have been bounded alive for their bones (Diamond, 1993).
so as not to interfere with the processes of Men and boys are much more likely to
killing, butchering, and eating animals. If abuse animals than women and girls (Arluke,
empathy is like a spotlight that illuminates 2002) but this passage about Nisa, a !Kung
the suffering of some at the expense of oth- San woman, describes with unusual clarity the
ers (Bloom, 2016), this spotlight turns off dynamic of curious, playful cruelty, and its abil-
or moves on. Empathy isn’t consistently ity to facilitate precise prediction of animals:
associated with caring about animal ethics
(Kasperbauer, 2015). And biophilia, or the A flying ant with a long, wormlike body and large,
almost transparent wings… landed in the hot
desire to affiliate with nature and animals
sand… Nisa saved it… and pierced it through half
(Wilson, 1984), often doesn’t have a loving the length of its body with a thin twig, leaving
or caring character. the upper half with the wings and head free. She
planted the stick, with the skewered insect at the
top, upright in the ground and tapped it gently with
her fingers. The insect’s wings burst into motion, as
Play and Animal Abuse if in flight, propelling the free parts of its body
around and around the stick; then it stopped. Nisa
One clue about evolved human morality tapped the stick again and again; each time, the
towards animals is how readily humans will insect responded with the same outpouring of
torment animals when it isn’t necessary, and energy… What Nisa was doing… seemed like an
inexcusable torture… [But Nisa’s] head and the
how much they enjoy doing it. Play is an
upper parts of her body had begun to move rhyth-
essential part of learning in our species and mically. I did not understand what she was doing at
many others. But humans playing with ani- first. Then it became clear: as the insect held itself
mals often involves both curiosity and cru- erect, Nisa’s body also became erect; when the
elty (Arluke, 2002). Around the world, in insect circled, drooped, and strained, Nisa’s body did
the same. Her face and torso echoed the insect’s
both traditional societies and Western socie-
plight with a wrenching subtlety and her mimicry of
ties, playing with animals and cruelty to its every movement was so sympathetic that the
animals are commonplace in both adults and situation took on a kind of beauty. (Shostak and
children – not just in psychopaths. Nisa, 2004: 321)
Read some anthropological descriptions
of hunter-gatherers and you’ll see sentimen- Using animals for entertainment has been one
tal, Noble Savage views like this: ‘Children of the most contentious and moralized aspects
learn to sympathize with animals and to see of animal use, even though the scale of suffer-
animals as sentient persons sharing the for- ing involved is much smaller than most modern
est world with them’ (Lew-Levy et al., 2017: industrialized forms of animal use. For exam-
X). The implication is that hunter-gatherers ple, many countries that otherwise have few
have greater moral regard for animals than animal-protection laws have banned circuses
Western people, who only see meat wrapped (‘These 27 Countries Have Banned Wild-
in cellophane at the grocery store. However, Animal Circuses!’, 2019, https://www.peta.org.
read descriptions from thinkers who are less uk/blog/these-26-countries-that-have-banned-
attached to a Noble Savage narrative, and wild-animal-circuses-are-making-england-
you’ll get a better picture of how difficult it look-really-bad/). Bear-baiting, hare-coursing,
may be for our species to bestow moral con- dogfighting, and cockfighting are more contro-
sideration on others. versial than other types of animal use, and were
It’s common for people in more tradi- banned much earlier (e.g., the UK banned bear-
tional societies to hurt animals for fun. Jared baiting in 1835 and Pakistan banned bear-bait-
Diamond describes Papua New Guinean men ing in 1890). This seems to be an area of strong
amusing themselves by raising and lowering moral signaling.
ANIMAL ETHICS AND EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY 149

One reason modern people might be judg- (‘Ahimsa in Jainism’, 2019). Here it’s notable
mental about animal entertainment is because that this requires strong spiritual and social
of the glut of other entertainment media incentives, such as the belief that any given
developed in the last few centuries. Yes, using human might be reincarnated as an insect.
animals for entertainment is unnecessary, but Given our evolutionary history as omnivores,
it seems even less necessary and thus indul- and the ubiquity of animal abuse across cul-
gently cruel when so many other forms of tures, it seems likely that carnism and specie-
entertainment are available, especially other sism are outgrowths of our evolved
forms of violent entertainment like combat psychology, rather than historically novel
sports, action films and video games. Forms cultural influences that render us weirdly
of animal entertainment considered lowbrow insensitive to animal suffering.
or ‘primitive’ may be more controversial. For
example, many have argued that forms of
animal entertainment enjoyed by the working Animal Abuse by Children – The
class, like cockfighting, have been banned Link
more quickly and more often than those
enjoyed by the higher classes, like fox hunt- Children commonly inflict suffering on ani-
ing (‘Fox Hunting’, 2019). mals, not just out of necessity, but for enjoy-
Most of us in Western societies who would ment and curiosity. Here again, we see the
not want to torment an animal directly, or sentimental narrative that animal abuse is a
would be appalled to see staged animal suf- rare, pathological glitch in children’s funda-
fering intended for entertainment, are still mentally caring natures, and that children who
entertained by watching animals inflict suffer- abuse animals will grow up to have other seri-
ing on each other in the wild, for example, in ous problems like psychopathy and criminal-
nature documentaries. The disparity in sensi- ity. Animal abuse is considered a risk factor
bilities is also well illustrated in this anecdote: for violence with such certainty in the animal
advocacy community that it’s sometimes
That is perhaps the hardest part of being an
anthropologist. [The hunter-gatherers I was study- referred to simply as ‘The Link’ (H. Herzog,
ing] sensed my weakness and would sell me all 2010). Most of the evidence for an association
kinds of baby animals with descriptions of what between childhood animal abuse and adult
they would do to them otherwise. I used to take violence suffers from methodological limita-
them far into the desert and release them, they tions like retrospective reporting, and sam-
would track them, and bring them back to me for
sale again! (Pinker, 2011: 473) pling incarcerated criminals (Flynn, 2011).
But, consistent with the idea that insensitivity
These behaviors that cause suffering to ani- to animal suffering is fairly standard in our
mals are often practiced alongside making species, there isn’t replicable evidence that
offerings to animal deities, praying to the animal abusers are more likely to commit vio-
spirit of animals after killing them, and efforts lent crime (H. Herzog and Arluke, 2006;
to embody animal qualities – contrary to the Patterson-Kane and Piper, 2009).
notion that cruelty requires dehumanization Consistent with the cross-cultural ubiquity
or suspension of empathy. In many small- of animal cruelty, and the historical com-
scale societies, formalized animal totemism monality of using animals for entertainment,
often co-exists with informal animal torment. animal abuse is normal among young peo-
The majority of human groups include some ple, even now. In one study, 40% of female
conspicuous cruelty to animals, but there are college students and 66% of male college
exceptions. For example, Jain monks and students admitted to having abused animals
nuns sweep the ground in front of them so as (Arluke, 2002) – and given the modern
to avoid inflicting any suffering on insects stigma against animal abuse, this is probably
150 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

an underreported behavior. There seems to be hour killing and butchering animals. Even
a moral panic about animal abuse; advocates among slaughterhouse workers, the worker
often depict anyone who has ever abused an who kills the cow, the ‘knocker’, is consid-
animal as likely to commit violence against ered to have psychological problems com-
people, even though the majority of peo- pared to other workers who bleed the cow
ple have, at some time, abused an animal or begin to dismember it (Pachirat, 2014).
(Patterson-Kane and Piper, 2009). Children There is evidence that the presence of slaugh-
might be learning about how animals ‘work’ terhouses is associated with increased local
by playing with them, developing mental rates of violent crime and sexual offences,
models of animal morphology and behavior relative to other industries like steel forging
that ancestrally would have been useful for (Fitzgerald et  al., 2009), but it’s unclear if
hunting, tracking, and butchering. Indeed, violent people are more attracted to working
vertebrate animals’ similarity to humans in slaughterhouses, or if slaughtering animals
means that children may be learning more increases workers’ propensities for violence
than this. Children around the world often towards other humans.
practice caretaking with pets and baby ani-
mals. I speculate that violence, which is
often used to adaptively compel others to do
what you want, can be practiced and honed WHY HAS ANIMAL ADVOCACY
similarly by playing with animals or through LAGGED BEHIND OTHER MORAL
cruelty. MOVEMENTS?
Indeed, lacking the kinds of modern toys
that Western children have access to, ani- Civil-rights movements, like those for the
mals are used in just this way, treated with abolition of slavery, black power, women’s
both care and cruelty. ‘Anthropologists have rights, worker’s rights, and gay rights, have
observed returning hunters bringing small flourished in the last several decades, and
wild animals back alive and promptly turning have reduced suffering for billions of humans
them over to their children… these animated (Pinker, 2011). The animal advocacy move-
toys are generally badly treated, short lived ment (including animal rights, animal wel-
and… end up the objects of target practice or fare, and animal liberation – but not
mutilation’ (Serpell, 1996: 68). There are two environmental protection) has not had the
main hypotheses about the animal-­ cruelty same success as other sentient-rights move-
association with crime: the graduation ments (Pinker, 2011). In some ways, attitudes
hypothesis posits that animal abuse is ‘a form have changed a great deal. In a representative
of rehearsal for human-directed violence’, sample of over 1,000 American adults,
and the deviance generalization hypothesis Sentience Institute found that nearly 50%
posits that antisocial personality is associated supported a ban on slaughterhouses and fac-
with both animal cruelty and criminal behav- tory farming (Reese, 2017). But, in that same
ior (Gullone, 2014). Animal cruelty might be survey, 75% of participants believed the reas-
both a normal behavior that children perform suring fiction that the animal products they
if unsupervised with animals, and also a form were eating had been humanely produced
of practice that is disproportionately attrac- (Reese, 2017) even though 99% of animal
tive to children with antisocial and violent products come from factory farms (Reese,
tendencies. 2019). A recent Gallup poll found that 32%
Animal killing and butchering were surely of Americans think that animals deserve ‘the
features of our evolutionary history, but mod- exact same rights as people’ (Riffkin, 2015),
ern specialization of labor means for the first up from 25% in 2003 (Moore, 2003). A study
time there are workers who spend hour after of 3,500 Ohio residents found 81% said farm
ANIMAL ETHICS AND EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY 151

animal welfare was as important as pet ­welfare, experience the vast majority of suffering and
and 75% said farm animals should be pro- death with more than 99% of farm animals
tected from physical pain (Rauch and Sharp, living on factory farms (Reese, 2019). Farm
2005). These self-reported attitudes are animals received only $20 million in charitable
impressively progressive, but consumer behav- donations in 2015, compared to $1.2 billion
ior has not changed that much. FewAmericans donated to animal shelters for pet species like
boycott animal products. The rate of self- dogs and cats (Bockman, 2016).
identified vegetarians has hovered around 5%
in the United States for several years (Riffkin, Of domesticated land animals used and killed by
humans in the United States, over 99.6% are
2015). Other reports claim there are slightly farmed land animals, about 0.2% are animals used
more vegetarians. The number of people who in laboratories, 0.07% are used for clothing, and
describe themselves as vegetarian is probably 0.03% are killed in companion animal shelters.
not actually representative of boycott, because However, about 66% of donations to animal chari-
only about one-third abstain from meat ties in the United States go to companion animal
shelters, 32% go to groups with mixed or other
(Cooney, 2014). Che Green, an expert on these activities, and just 0.8% of donations go specifically
trends, has called vegans and vegetarians ‘a to farmed animal organizations, while 0.7% go to
blip on the demographic radar’ and ‘below the laboratory animal organizations. (Bockman, 2016)
margin of error for most surveys’ (Zaraska,
2016: 136). A couple of caveats are that the ‘other’ cate-
The concern for animal wellbeing has gory does include some farm animal dona-
made uneven progress, with far more concern tions, because it includes large organizations
about some species and some issues than that engage in diverse animal advocacy cam-
others. One illustration of the vast disparity paigns (e.g., People for the Ethical Treatment
between the alleged human moral concern of Animals (PETA)). ‘Other’ also includes
for animal suffering, and the actual concen- environmental charities and wildlife preserva-
tration of animal suffering, is revealed by tion. Also, importantly, the ‘animals used and
patterns of charitable donations (Figure 7.1). killed’ number does not include fish, which
Farm animals, compared to all other animals, would completely dominate the left panel.

Figure 7.1  Charitable donations towards animal organizations as compared to animal use
152 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Fish are probably killed in the trillions, at a viral videos of cats who love rats and hippos
rate greater than all other animals combined who love tortoises, for example), but they are
(Mood and Brooke, 2010). This is another almost always the product of an artificial envi-
way that human behavior is inconguous with ronment (H. Herzog, 2014). Pet-keeping
stated concern. If humans cared about reduc- seems uniquely human. Two interesting
ing animal suffering and acted in keeping exceptions in the wild are a dolphin who
with this concern, they would not give a dis- adopted a melon-headed whale (Carzon et al.,
proportionate amount of their donations to 2019), and a marmoset adopted into a group
cat and dog shelters. of capuchin monkeys (Izar et al., 2006). In the
Below I will address the psychology of how first case, the dolphin nursed and cared for
and why humans morally value and devalue the whale, and in the second case, two female
animals. First, I’ll discuss kin selection, and capuchins provisioned the marmoset. In both
the empathy elicited by cuteness and neoteny cases, arguably, the adopted animal appears to
– two intertwined factors that account for our be a neotenous version of the animals’ own
kindness towards companion animals like young. This illustrates a major psychological
dogs and cats. Second, I’ll discuss disgust means through which humans integrate ani-
and food aversion, meat consumption, and the mals more centrally into their moral worlds:
future of meat eating. Empathy, disgust, and kin selection and empathy for cuteness. Both
food preferences show significant sex differ- cases also illustrate how this cuteness-based
ences, and I’ll discuss how they manifest in empathy is more motivating for females than
morality. Lastly, I’ll talk about how morality is for males. Humans keep a wide variety of
socially signaled, and how this virtue signaling pets, from insects to horses, but here I’ll focus
plays out in animal care and attitudes towards mostly on dogs, who seem to be the animals
the animal-focused moral minority, such as who most often reverse human speciesism.
vegans. The view of decision-making in the The psychological mechanisms motivating
moral domain in this chapter is consistent with kin selection may cause humans to value dogs
the popular metaphor of the elephant and the and cats much more than other comparably
rider (Haidt, 2012; Simler and Hanson, 2017). sentient animals. Evolution promotes pass-
Emotions and psychological mechanisms ing on our genes, including in other members
like the cuteness response, disgust, reputation of our families (Foster et al., 2006). Because
management, and empathy guide our moral cooperating in groups is adaptive, we may
decisions in often irrational directions, like an be more likely to interpret ambiguous cues
elephant deciding which path to take. We iden- as evidence of genetic association (Park and
tify as the rider, the part we are most conscious Ackerman, 2011). In this way, kin selection
of, and we later attribute our moral decisions means that we tend to be more altruistic to
to rational processes rather than the ‘hot’ emo- those who show cues of being members of
tional cognition of the elephant, the part that our ingroup – whether these cues are cultural
controls our behavior. (Kurzban et  al., 2001) or physical (Krupp
et al., 2011). There are other possible indica-
tors of genetic relatedness, like psychological
Kin Selection and the Cuteness similarity (Park and Schaller, 2005), and time
spent growing up together (Lieberman et al.,
Response
2007).
Prominent anthrozoologist James Serpell It’s unclear if our minds are simply indis-
defines pets as ‘animals we live with, with no criminate about what cues we take as indi-
obvious function’ (H. Herzog, 2010: 72). cators of genetic relatedness. Some animals
Other animals don’t usually keep pets. Cross- indisputably occupy a familial role, not only
species friendships sometimes happen (see in Western countries but also among many
ANIMAL ETHICS AND EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY 153

hunter-gatherers. New Guineans, which I (genetic similarity) and cuteness are difficult
earlier described abusing bats, treat pigs as to disentangle as contributors to this unusual
members of their families; piglets sleep in moral relationship. Because there is no word
the same hut with their human families and in English for the especially cute emotional
New Guinean women often nurse piglets at repertoire elicited by cuteness, I’ll use ‘cute-
the breast (Diamond, 1993). In the United ness response’. The cuteness response elicits
States, 91% of dog and cat owners reported nurturance but also inspires mentalizing and
that their pet was a part of their family, and anthropomorphizing, bringing cute individu-
20% of dog owners report giving their dogs als closer into the circle of moral concern
birthday gifts (‘Pets Really Are Members of (Sherman and Haidt, 2011). There is evi-
the Family’, 2011). dence that pets, especially dogs, parasitize
We attend to physical similarity when eval- our parental caretaking mechanisms (Turner,
uating species and breeds for special treat- 2001). People talk to dogs in a way similar to
ment. For example, people are more likely ‘motherese’ (the sing-song way in which par-
to support causes to save endangered species ents talk to their infants), and motherese for
that have more in common with humans, like dogs has been termed ‘doggerel’ (Hirsh-
great apes, and researchers who do invasive Pasek and Treiman, 1982). Dogs are also
scientific experiments on monkeys are most neotenous: they retain puppylike features
likely to be threatened by animal activists (H. throughout their lives. Furthermore, dogs who
Herzog, 2010). Even so, most people (around have more paedomorphic (i.e., cute) facial
75%) would choose to save one person over musculature are more likely to be adopted
five endangered lowland gorillas (Petrinovich from shelters (Waller et  al., 2013). Dogs in
et al., 1993). childless homes are much more likely to be
Dogs show resemblance to humans in groomed, given presents, and taken on vaca-
their facial musculature: domestication has tion (H. Herzog, 2010: 79).
changed dog faces compared to wolf faces Dogs have been bred to retain neotenous
to be able to display more humanlike expres- puppylike features and to be cuter than their
sions (Kaminski et  al., 2019). There is also wolfy ancestors. Dogs aren’t just cuter to us;
evidence that people resemble their dogs they’re also cuter to wolves themselves! In
(Nakajima et  al., 2009), and that people one study a wolf mother was given two dif-
choose dogs who resemble them (Roy and ferent litters to foster, one with wolf puppies
Nicholas, 2004). People also tend to think and one with dog puppies:
their dogs are psychologically similar to
The foster-mother wolf was… more nurturant with
them. Dog breeds go in and out of fashion
the Malamute pups than with the wolf pups. She
(Ghirlanda et al., 2013), but the proliferation washed them earlier and more frequently, spent
of so many dog breeds with different appear- 2–3 times as many hours in the den-box with them
ances and personalities may have been driven as she did with the wolf pups, was more defensive
by the kin-selection mechanisms of different toward intruders, showed far more distress when
one was missing (e.g., during supplemental feed-
individuals and ethnic groups with different
ings), played with them and continues to play with
implicit criteria for similarity. them for longer periods of time. (Frank and Frank,
1982: 515)

One reason that dog breed popularity doesn’t


Cute Ethics
track health, obedience, or other desirable
In ethics, cuteness doesn’t count. (Cohen, 2009) qualities is the desire for the cuteness super-
stimulus. Analyzing the popularity of dog
Because pets seem to occupy a place in the breeds in the United States from 1926 to
family as surrogate children, cues of kinship 1995, researchers concluded that there was
154 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

no indication ‘that breeds with more desirable Why do we as humans find a huge array
behavior, longer life, or fewer inherited of animals cute, from penguins to pandas to
genetic disorders have been more popular than pangolins? Prototypical cuteness elicitors are
other breeds’ (Ghirlanda et al., 2013: 4). Most cues like round and fat cheeks, large eyes,
purebred dog breeds have some endemic small teeth, and playful energetic behavior.
health problems, but brachycephalic dogs Our own young don’t achieve peak cuteness
(dogs with short snouts, like bulldogs) have until they are several months old (Sherman
many more problems. This preference for and Haidt, 2011). Humans produce very altri-
this brachycephalic cuteness super-stimulus, cial young and neonates are highly divergent
(for example these dogs have a much rounder in their appearance (e.g., presence of hair,
more infant like head than most other dog redness of skin, facial morphology). Also,
breeds) causes a huge amount of suffering neonates often aren’t cute, and yet this stage
for animals that are otherwise treated like of life is when our young are most vulner-
family. French and English bulldogs must able and most in need of care. In order to take
usually be delivered by C-section, and suffer care of our own neonates, the normal pri-
many other problems like allergies, hip dys- mate standards of cuteness may have become
plasia, persistent farting, and heat sensitivity relaxed in humans. Earlier I speculated that
that causes them to die more often in trans- slightly indiscriminate kin-detection mecha-
port (K. Herzog, 2019). Interestingly, people nisms could have made it easier for humans
with these neotenous breeds are more to form friendships. Something similar could
attached to their pets than those with less have happened with cuteness and pets. The
neotenous breeds. In a Danish sample, French large number of animal species we think
bulldog owners were more attached to their are cute could be because of indiscriminate
dogs and nearly 20% more likely to say ‘I cuteness perception. ‘Cuteness promiscu-
would do anything for my dog’ compared to ity’ might be a byproduct of our unusual life
owners of the much less neotenous Cairn history, high altriciality, and high variance
Terriers (Sandøe et  al., 2017). Perhaps they in infant appearance. This tendency to think
just reported this because their dogs required that many different animals are cute could be
extraordinary attention and care or perhaps even more pronounced in women, who are the
these health problems – are one reason people primary caretakers of altricial infants. Given
want these dogs (Serpell, 2019). Bulldogs selection over time for dogs to elicit both
have been in the top five for breed popularity fellow-feeling (kin selection) and the cuteness
for the last six years, and in the last two years, response, it makes sense that their suffering
French bulldogs have also been in the top five. is much more prioritized than that of other
With the recent high-profile win of ‘Thor’, a species. Given women’s special sensitivity to
bulldog, at a national dog pageant, this trend is cuteness, it makes sense that we find such a
likely to continue (K. Herzog, 2019). large difference in men and women when it
To some extent, our special affinity for our comes to animal morality.
pet dogs translates into special moral con-
sideration for all dogs. In the United States
there was widespread outrage prompted by
China’s annual dog-meat festival (Howard, SEX DIFFERENCES IN MORALITY
2016). The United States has loved dogs for a TOWARDS ANIMALS
long time – and, in perhaps the greatest suc-
cess story from the animal-protection move- In the example above in which a dolphin
ment, the number of dogs (and cats) that are fostered a melon-headed whale, she also
euthanized per year has plummeted, down nursed him. Cross-species nursing is also
75% from 2011 (Parlapiano, 2019). common cross-culturally. Women around the
ANIMAL ETHICS AND EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY 155

world have nursed baby animals like bears, of men, but less than half of women, consider
monkeys, pigs, and puppies (H. Herzog, the use of animal fur for clothing, and medi-
2019). In some of these cultures, eating an cal testing on animals to be morally accept-
animal nursed at the breast is considered as able’ (Saad, 2010). Women are also much
taboo as eating your own child. In other cul- more opposed to ‘unnatural’ technologies,
tures, animals like dogs, are traded with other including food additives, genetically modified
groups in order to limit the discomfort of foods, and animal testing (Funk et al., 2015).
killing animals one raised, and in other cul- In particular, 62% of women oppose the use
tures, animals are nursed so that they can be of animals in scientific research, whereas
later eaten (Serpell, 1996). Among the Ainu 60% of men support it.
of Japan, bear cubs are breast fed and then
ceremonially sacrificed and eaten, while the
women who suckled the bear cubs show their Animal Testing: Disgust and
ambivalence by alternately crying and laugh- Empathy
ing (Serpell, 1996: 184).
Unsurprisingly, given women’s sensitiv- The widespread opposition to animal testing
ity to cuteness and their greater nurturing is a good demonstration of how disgust and
response (on average), women show stronger empathy can create strong feelings around
moral concerns for animals than men do. For animal ethics, even when they conflict with
example, 45% of women would let a foreign important human interests such as biomedi-
tourist die before their cat or dog, compared cal advances. All drugs and interventions to
to 30% of men (Topolski et  al., 2013), and prevent human and animal suffering must
33% of women would kill a person to save first be tested. Gary Francione (2009), who
1,000 dogs, compared with 23% of men famously advocates completely abolishing
(Petrinovich et al., 1993). However, men and the use and ownership of animals, has called
women were similarly likely to say they would animal testing the only use of animals that
save a close relative over a pet (Topolski et al., isn’t frivolous. Animal testing is one of the
2013). There are many other sex differences highest-profile and most controversial uses
in moral attitudes towards animals, reflecting of animals, although it accounts for a very
differences in moral emotions such as disgust, small proportion of animal suffering. ‘We
empathy, and the cuteness response. Women kill 200 food animals for every animal used
are much more likely to be involved in ani- in a scientific experiment’ (H. Herzog, 2010:
mal protection and animal advocacy, much 176). More than half (52%) of Americans
more likely to be vegetarian, more likely to oppose the use of animals in scientific testing
hoard animals, and much less likely to hunt (Strauss, 2018), and 60% oppose animal
or engage in direct animal abuse ( H. Herzog, cloning (Masci, 2017) – a technology that
2007). Women are less speciesist than men could lead to significant advances in bio-
as measured through questionnaire (Caviola medical research and comparable gains in
et al., 2018). Women are more likely than men human welfare. In comparison to other
to believe that animals experience complex ­animal-related causes, there have been much
emotions like grief and anxiety (Walker et al., larger changes in legislation and regulation
2014). There are also substantial sex differ- around animal testing. For instance, the EU
ences in moral views on animals. In a 2015 has implemented bans on cosmetic animal
poll, 42% of women compared to 22% of men testing (‘Testing Cosmetics on Animals’,
said that animals deserve the same rights as 2019) (arguably, this was only possible
people (Riffkin, 2015). In a 2011 Gallup poll because most cosmetic ingredients have
on moral issues, the largest sex differences already been tested on animals for decades
were on issues related to animals: ‘Majorities and found safe).
156 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

In 2015, 0.2% of animals were killed in the world the trend is that people are eating
labs but a disproportionate 0.7% of charity more meat, year after year.
money went to this cause (Bockman, 2016). However, the strong human desire for meat
Some of the only true terrorism by animal has a flip side, because meat has often been a
advocates – like death threats and property dangerous food to eat, more likely to contain
damage – has targeted scientists and labo- pathogens than plant foods. Because humans
ratories conducting animal research. In par- eat both plants and animals, they face an
ticular, scientists who work with primates or omnivore’s dilemma: there are a large num-
dogs have been targeted (H. Herzog, 2010). ber of foods that could be eaten, but they dif-
Familiarity is one heuristic we use to infer fer both in nutritional quality and in the risk
that something isn’t dangerous or pathogenic. of dangerous pathogens. Disgust is thought
Unlike meat eating, animal experimenta- to have evolved to reduce the chance of com-
tion’s visceral associations and unfamiliarity ing into contact with potential pathogens,
combine to create an impression of ‘unnatu- especially those that are orally incorporated
ralness’ (Holden and H. Herzog, 2019) that (Tybur et  al., 2012). That’s likely why cul-
is disgusting and elicits moral condemnation. ture discourages eating certain animal foods.
Taboos are more often leveraged against
meat than against other foods (Fessler and
Navarrete, 2003). The trait of ‘disgust sensi-
Meat
tivity’ is positively linked to meat avoidance
Meat is a strongly preferred food among (Fessler et  al., 2003), and disgust is often
humans. This enjoyment of and desire for given as an explicit reason for not eating meat
meat is a major contributor to our moral atti- (Santos and Booth, 1996).
tudes about meat, and to our self-deception As I argued earlier, there is evidence that
about the living conditions of animals raised most animals used for food are sentient, and
for meat. Humans almost certainly evolved that human imposition of industrial-scale
eating meat (Wrangham, 2009), and seem suffering on sentient animals may, from the
motivated to eat meat specifically. For perspective of aggregate suffering, be one of
instance, human taste buds appear to be sen- the most pressing concerns of our genera-
sitive to a flavor abundant in cooked meat tion (Singer, 1990). The scale of animal use
called umami (Lindemann et  al., 2002). In is staggering. According to an interview with
many places in the world there is a special expert Lewis Bollard (Wiblin and Bollard,
word for ‘meat hunger’ (Fiddes, 2004; 2017), there are currently 23 billion chick-
Zaraska, 2016), as distinct from other kinds ens being farmed (15 billion for meat and
of hunger. In recent years the developing 8 billion for eggs), 6 billion mammals (like
world, either imitating rich industrialized cows, pigs, and rabbits), and over 100 billion
nations or actualizing their evolved taste farmed fish. Even if we ascribe to each of
preferences for savory, high-calorie foods, these animals just a fraction of the sentience
are eating more meat and other animal prod- and moral importance ascribed to a human,
ucts (Kearney, 2010). Meat eating has this adds up to a massive moral issue –
increased a great deal in the United States in vastly more aggregate suffering than global
the last few decades, and Americans now are poverty or disease among humans. From the
eating an average of 125 kg of meat per year perspective of sentient suffering, the envi-
(Zaraska, 2016). And in China, the most ronmental and sustainability issues around
populous country in the world, the average meat also matter. Meat is environmentally
person in the 1970s ate 14 kg of meat per costly to produce, requiring more water and
year, whereas in 2010 they were eating 55 kg land per calorie, in addition to being one of
of meat per year (H. Herzog, 2010). Around the major producers of greenhouse gases and
ANIMAL ETHICS AND EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY 157

water pollution (Steinfeld et al., 2006). Meat often motivated by both disgust and health
feeds fewer people with the same resources. messaging, often end up eating more chicken
Estimates of inefficiency vary, but the same than self-described meat eaters do (H. Herzog,
amount of grain produces 10 times fewer 2010: 195). Another factor beyond disgust is
calories through grain-fed cattle than when that we often feel more empathy for cows
eaten directly (Bittman, 2008). (However, it’s and pigs than for fish and chickens, because
important to consider that much of the land they have more humanlike and neotenous
that isn’t suitable for farming can still feed characteristics.
grazing ruminants, like cattle, and thus can How might consumption of fish, eggs, and
produce food.) chicken, to the exclusion of beef and pork,
In principle, boycotting animal products cause more animal suffering? There are two
could significantly reduce many of these main reasons: animal size and quality of
problems. But, even in places with abundant life. Chickens and farmed fish are smaller
food choices, vegetarianism and veganism animals, which means that for each animal
are rare (Pinker, 2011). Most self-described bred, caged, and slaughtered, we get far
‘vegetarians’ eat meat (Cooney, 2014), and fewer meals. This observation was the basis
perhaps 90% of people who self-identify as of a tongue-in-cheek campaign from PETA
vegetarian aren’t behaviorally vegetarian called ‘Eat the whales’ (Tomasula, 2001). In
(H. Herzog, 2010: 195). Moreover, lapsed a 100-ton blue whale, there are 70,000 chick-
vegetarians outnumber current vegetarians ens’ worth of meat. (H. Herzog, 2010: 193).
(H. Herzog, 2011), and many vegetarians Considering living conditions, chickens (both
avoid red meat for health reasons rather than egg-laying hens and broiler chickens) and
ethical reasons. Thus, it’s difficult to esti- farmed fish have much worse lives than con-
mate how many people are boycotting animal ventionally produced beef cattle (Tomasik,
products for ethical reasons. 2018b). Conventionally produced beef cattle
All foods cause some degree of suffering – spend much of their lives in pasture and the
even vegetables and fruit, because many last 100–200 days of their lives in a feedlot –
small wild animals, from insects to birds, they can eat, stretch out, and associate with
are killed during planting and harvesting. As others of their species. By contrast, broiler
Norwood and Lusk (2011) glibly comment, chickens live in cramped conditions and often
‘even veganism is murder’ (2011: 229). have crippling leg problems. Egg-laying
However, animal foods differ markedly in hens kept in cages usually have their beaks
how much suffering they cause. Ironically, removed so they don’t attack or cannibalize
the most popular ways that vegetarians and one another in cramped spaces. Often this
semi-vegetarians reduce their consumption causes chronic pain or inability to feed, and
of animal products may impose more net it doesn’t solve the problem of hens aggress-
suffering than a diet centered around beef ing against their cage mates. Conventional
would, as I discuss below. pork production is widely considered to be
Disgust is a major driver of meat avoidance terrible for smart social animals such as pigs,
(Fessler et al., 2003). Red meat, which retains who are confined and bored, like a dog kept
more cues of its animal origins, like blood, is in a kennel cage for months on end. For those
considered much more disgusting than fish concerned with humane animal treatment,
or chicken, which are often packaged to hide a reasonable goal is that animals raised for
their animal origin (H. Herzog, 2010: 190). food should only have ‘one bad day’: the day
Health messages about meat underscore this they go to slaughter. For detailed descriptions
disgust, with recommendations to cut out of how different animals are raised for food
red meat and to eat chicken, eggs, and fish see, for example Norwood and Lusk (2011)
instead. Self-described vegetarians, who are and Singer and Mason (2006).
158 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Remarkably few people have tried to foods/). Here, animal lifespan is how many
compare animal welfare across species or days the animal lives before slaughter, on
to calculate how much suffering is caused average; kg of food per animal lifespan is how
by eating different animal foods. Still, there much edible food weight is produced by the
is some consensus on which animals have animal; suffering per day of life is how bad
the best and worst lives. Economists Bailey the animal’s life is based on best estimates
Norwood and Jayson Lusk (2011) came to from animal-welfare researchers (note that
similar conclusions as Tomasik (2018b) in beef cattle have the best lives and battery hens
terms of the quality of life of various animals have the worst lives). The column on the right
raised for food (and also quantified the qual- indicates for each kg of the animal product
ity of life for animals kept for breeding pur- consumed how many days of suffering there
poses) (Norwood and Lusk, 2011: 229). They are adjusted for the badness of each day of
estimated that laying hens and veal calves life.
have the worst quality of life, and that beef Using a similar calculation, the standard
cattle and dairy cows have the best quality of American with a typical diet of animal prod-
life relatively speaking. (However, Norwood ucts causes 5 years, 6 months, and 5 days of
and Lusk argue that broiler chickens have a animal suffering per year (Hurford, 2014).
much better quality of life on average than Regardless of specific numbers, many ‘veg-
most animal advocates think they do.) etarians’, some adhering to the definition and
How does all this add up? We can quantify a eating eggs, and many others who still eat fish
‘suffering footprint’: an estimate of how many and chicken, are causing more days of animal
days of suffering animals endure to contribute suffering, and more intense suffering, than
a unit of meat to our diet. Table 7.1 is adapted many meat eaters are. Based on the calcula-
and simplified based on Tomasik’s (2018b) tions from the table above, a vegetarian who
calculation of how many days of suffering per eats three eggs at a meal (around 150 g) is
kg are caused by the demand from buying var- causing 19.5 days of chicken suffering, com-
ious animal foods. I have simplified the cal- pared to a meat eater who eats a 1.3 kg steak
culation here by assuming these animals have that causes around 2.4 days of cow suffer-
the same sentience, and by assuming that each ing. The average vegetarian almost certainly
animal has roughly the same suffering on the causes less suffering than the five years of suf-
day of slaughter (the reader can input values fering created through the average American
in the table at the Reducing Suffering blog, diet. But the perception that the average self-
https://reducing-suffering.org/how-much- described ‘vegetarian’ is more moral than the
direct-suffering-is-caused-by-various-animal- average meat eater is derived not from any

Table 7.1  Days of suffering per kilogram of food weight produced by the animal adjusted
for the badness of each day of life as estimated by animal welfare researchers
Animal product Animal lifespan (days) Kg of food per animal Suffering per day of Adjusted days of suffering
lifespan life (beef cows =1) caused per kg demanded

Farmed catfish 820 .39 1.5 3,200


Farmed salmon 639 2.0 1.5 480
Battery cage eggs 501 16 4 130
Chicken 42 1.9 3 68
Turkey 133 9.6 3 42
Pork 183 65 2.5 7.1
Beef 395 212 1 1.9
Milk 1,825 30,000 2 0.12
ANIMAL ETHICS AND EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY 159

quantitative analysis of animal suffering, but choices as consumers. One experiment on the
from their claimed concern for animals, and ‘vote/buy gap’ – the tendency for consumers
from the fact that they eat less meat from to vote for higher welfare standards but not to
mammals, who are cuter and more human- buy in accordance with these ideals – showed
like. Even a vegetarian who eats eggs every that 80% of consumers who chose to buy
day would cause more suffering than some- cookies made with battery cage eggs said
one on an all-beef diet. An actual vegan, who that battery cage eggs should be illegal (Paul
eats no animal products including meat, fish, et al., 2019). The vast majority of people in
eggs, and dairy, causes the least amount of Western societies would state that they are
suffering with their consumption. Giving up morally repulsed by slavery, and yet when a
fish, eggs, and chicken would reduce animal report documented that around one-third of
suffering about 90% as much as a vegan who shrimp produced in Thailand involved slave
eats no animal products at all (Cooney, 2014). labor (Hodal and Lawrence, 2014), this did
Unfortunately, there is no name for this avoid- not change the huge demand for shrimp, and
ing fish, eggs, and chicken ethical stance, and there is still slavery in the supply chain now
thus it is not possible to signal this or reap any (Clark, 2019). From a historical perspective,
benefits to social moral reputation. no movement has ever made significant gains
The impact of better welfare animal prac- from endorsing individual boycotts of large-
tices on animal suffering and human moral- scale industries. An analysis of the abolition-
ity is beyond the scope of this chapter. But ist movement against human slavery showed
it seems that people are much more likely to that boycotting slave-produced goods was
think they are buying humane animal products not effective, and was not that widespread,
than they really are (75% believe they are buy- even among abolitionists (Witwicki, 2017).
ing human products versus 99% of products One possible solution to the problem of
coming from factory farms) (Reese, 2017). animal suffering caused by meat production is
Given that billions of animals are farmed for in vitro meat, cultured meat, or ‘clean meat’.
food across thousands of facilities, and the Clean meat is the ‘cultivation of food grade
food industry remains politically powerful, animal tissues in carefully controlled environ-
it’s difficult to enforce humane standards. The ments’ (McLaren, 2014: 1). Clean meat holds
little evidence we have, such as that under- the promise of replacing slaughter-based
cover animal advocacy operations seem to meat production. The fast rate of technologi-
always discover cruel mishandling and mis- cal innovation in clean meat seems to have
treatment of animals, even on farms with overcome some of the obstacles I wrote about
‘humane standards’, doesn’t bode well. several years ago (Fleischman, 2013). The
main obstacle has been price preventing clean
meat from meeting market demand. Creating
a structure for in vitro meat to grow, to keep it
Clean Meat at the correct temperature and inundated with
nutrients for cell division, and free from con-
Fifty years hence, we shall escape the absurdity of tamination, made it prohibitively expensive.
growing a whole chicken in order to eat the breast
or wing by growing these parts separately under a The debut clean meat burger in London cre-
suitable medium. (Winston Churchill, 1932: 26) ated by Mark Post a few years ago cost about
$330,000 to make (McLaren, 2014). But, after
It’s unlikely that individual consumer choices many failed predictions (Madrigal, 2013), it
are going to significantly reduce the demand seems clean meat might soon be coming to
for animal products. Polls show Americans market. A few major obstacles seem to have
say they are very concerned about animal been overcome since clean meat is now being
welfare, but this doesn’t translate into their taste-tested for the public.
160 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

However, our evolved psychology may If clean meat is going to become an


still present obstacles to the uptake of clean ­­
important solution to the myriad problems of
meat. Food preferences crystallize at an the global animal industry we have to learn
early age (Birch, 1999) and people feel dis- from history, both evolutionary and cultural.
gust about foods that are unfamiliar to them. As I mentioned above, we as humans are
To increase demand for clean meat, ethi- more concerned about meat contamination
cal vegetarians might at least be willing to than other food sources. Any warning about
try it. However, in two small surveys it was clean-meat contamination, or a recall, could
found that the majority of vegans and veg- have pervasive long-term effects and mean that
etarians (71%) were unwilling to try in vitro people will continue to buy more familiar meat
meat (Fleischman, 2012). A larger survey of from animals that suffered and died for dec-
vegetarians found a similar result, with 73% ades to come. Branding clean meat as ‘clean
unwilling to eat it (Dahlgreen, 2013). In my meat’ rather than in vitro meat, lab meat, cul-
survey (Fleischman, 2012), it seemed that the tured meat, or synthetic meat was an impor-
stipulation that in vitro meat would cause no tant first step in combating disgust sensitivity.
more animal suffering than plant foods did One major reason that genetically modified
not change attitudes against in vitro meat, food wasn’t an unalloyed success was because
leaving disgust as the most probable cause. of perceptions of unnaturalness (Mohorčich,
Indeed, 32% of vegans explicitly cited dis- 2018), another form of disgust response that
gust as a reason they would not want to try it. can be reduced by increasing consumer famili-
There is some research indicating that moral arity. Framing is also important; meat pro-
vegetarians are more disgust-sensitive over- ducers learned long ago that mentioning the
all (Rozin et al., 1997). However, it is disap- animals themselves reduced consumer accept-
pointing that this group is likely not going to ance (Zaraska, 2016). We don’t really want to
be leading the way towards clean meat. know how the sausage is made, and less detail
Vegan and vegetarian attitudes are probably about how clean meat is produced generally
not that important for the future of clean meat. improves attitudes (Bryant and Barnett, 2018).
The most important thing is uptake from people We as humans are more concerned with what’s
eating the most meat and populous countries delicious than what is virtuous; consumers
whose meat consumption is increasing, namely rarely care enough to buy or boycott any prod-
China and India. There is some hopeful news uct because of its moral ramifications (Bryant
as familiarity with clean meat increases. In one and Barnett, 2018). That’s why making sure
survey of American adults, the majority were that clean meat is tastier than conventional
willing to try clean meat (65%) and about one- meat can go a long way. Finally, the rise of
third said they would be willing to eat clean zoonotic diseases like Covid19 and H1N1 can
meat as a replacement for farmed meat (Wilks hopefully turn the tide of disgust sensitivity in
and Phillips, 2017). Men, who tend to eat more the other direction, against forms of animal
meat than women, also had a more positive agriculture that can cause pandemics.
view of clean meat in this US sample. In a sam-
ple of over 3,000 participants from the United
States, India, and China, 93% of Chinese par- VIRTUE SIGNALING AND ANIMAL
ticipants said they were likely to purchase clean WELFARE
meat, as were 86% of Indian participants and
75% of US participants (Bryant et  al., 2019).
Stop smirking. One of the most universal pieces
In keeping with ideas about sex differences and
of advice from across cultures and eras is that we
food aversions, men and those who are less are all hypocrites, and in our condemnation of
disgust-sensitive are more favorable towards others’ hypocrisy we only compound our own.
clean meat (Bryant and Barnett, 2018). (Haidt, 2006: 60)
ANIMAL ETHICS AND EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY 161

Most animals are hidden from public view or as more moral and better than others; meat
otherwise incapable of communicating about ­eaters rate vegetarians as more moral than the
their suffering and cannot leverage reputa- average person, but rate vegetarians as less
tional concern (Sperber and Baumard, 2012). moral than themselves (Minson and Monin,
However, because people can advertise their 2012). Maintaining a moral reputation is a
moral attitudes in so many ways now, from major reason that meat eaters rate vegetar-
vegan bumper stickers to social-media posts, ians negatively. They anticipate that vegetar-
there is more widespread concern about the ians are judging them and will communicate
suffering of animals than at any previous point their moral condemnation of meat eaters to
in history. Our moral attitudes do not occur in others. Meat eaters rated vegetarians more
a vacuum. Advertising our moral qualities to negatively when they were first asked to
others for social benefits, whether these moral consider how much vegetarians might judge
qualities are instantiated in behavior or just them, and meat eaters expected vegetarians
‘cheap talk’, is known as virtue signaling to judge them three times more negatively
(Miller, 2019). When definitions of moral than they were actually judged (Minson and
behavior shift in social groups, culture can Monin, 2012). Of course, it’s possible that
change moral behavior to the extent that it’s because being judgmental is widely consid-
available for virtue signaling. This is one ered immoral, vegetarians were reporting less
reason that animal advocates have had so judgment than they actually felt.
much more success with institutional change One interesting aspect of the Minson and
over individual changes (Reese, 2018). People Monin (2012) study was that some meat eat-
are willing to advertise moral ideals by sign- ers were first given an opportunity to say
ing a petition or publicly advocating that a what they thought about vegans before later
business change its harmful animal practices, reporting how much they agreed with their
but are unwilling to engage in more costly and moral message. Participants in the study
less visible individual boycott. described vegetarians as ‘weird’, ‘preachy’,
Our moral identity is important to us; there and ‘sadistic’. But afterward they were more
is a strong psychological motivation to present likely than other participants who did not der-
ourselves as more moral than others (Kurzban, ogate vegetarians to say that they agreed with
2011) and to resist others’ claims of moral the moral message of vegetarianism. This is
superiority. This creates fraught relationships interesting from an evolutionary reputation-
with ‘moral minorities’ who consider them- management perspective. Reducing some-
selves to be in the moral vanguard – including one else’s reputational status relative to your
animal advocates, vegans, and others who hold own might increase your likelihood of tak-
and display a virtuous identity. Vegetarians are ing their message seriously; you don’t have
widely disliked by the rest of society. In one to fight as hard to make yourself look good.
study, participants reported disliking vegans Moral change often happens when we want
and vegetarians more than atheists, asexu- to socially affiliate with others, and negative
als, immigrants, or Blacks, but reported being impressions of activists – from animal advo-
more willing to hire or rent to vegans and veg- cates to social-justice advocates – undermines
etarians than all other target groups (MacInnis the cause (Bashir et  al., 2013). Importantly,
and Hodson, 2017). In this study, only drug for any moral advocate, they must remember
addicts were more disliked than vegans. that others are going to have strong incentives
Because moral rules are considered to be to derogate them and will scrutinize them for
universal, meeting someone who holds dif- moral inconsistency (Monin, 2007). Anyone
ferent or more strict moral standards than you advocating a major change in moral priori-
do can be seen as an implicit indictment of ties must remember that people have spent
your behavior. People tend to rate themselves years honing their virtue-signaling strategies,
162 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

and will not take kindly to someone arguing REFERENCES


that they have really been hugely less virtu-
ous than they thought. The evolutionary psy- Ahimsa in Jainism. (2019). In Wikipedia. https://
chological challenge for animal advocacy is en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?title=Ahimsa_
to nudge people to show more concern for in_Jainism&oldid=929905870 (Accessed
animal suffering, without feeling like their 4 January 2020)
whole virtue-signaling identity has to be jet- Amiot, C. E., & Bastian, B. (2015). Toward a
tisoned and rebuilt from scratch. psychology of human–animal relations.
Psychological Bulletin, 141(1), 6–47.
Appel, M., & Elwood, R. W. (2009). Motivational
trade-offs and potential pain experience in
hermit crabs. Applied Animal Behaviour
CONCLUSION Science, 119(1–2), 120–124.
Arluke, A. (2002). Animal abuse as dirty play.
Evolutionary explanations are often maligned Symbolic Interaction, 25(4), 405–430.
because they are said to excuse or normalize Awad, E., Dsouza, S., Kim, R., Schulz, J., Hen-
violence. To say animal cruelty and inflicting rich, J., Shariff, A., Bonnefon, J.-F., & Rahwan,
animal suffering is normal and natural is not to I. (2018). The moral machine experiment.
minimize the suffering of animal victims either Nature, 563(7729), 59.
as the result of any individual’s sadism or the Bandura, A. (1999). Moral disengagement in
the perpetration of inhumanities. Personality
large-scale production of animal products. To
and Social Psychology Review, 3(3),
say that our nurturing instincts predispose us to
193–209.
be kinder to animals that demonstrate kinship Bashir, N. Y., Lockwood, P., Chasteen, A. L.,
cues or that elicit the cuteness response is not Nadolny, D., & Noyes, I. (2013). The ironic
to say that these responses are moral. To say impact of activists: Negative stereotypes
that we are more disgusted by meat that looks reduce social change influence. European
more like the animal it came from than meat Journal of Social Psychology, 43(7),
that looks more abstract is not to say it is more 614–626.
moral to eat meat packaged in cellophane. And Birch, L. L. (1999). Development of food prefer-
to say that we virtue signal about our moral ences. Annual Review of Nutrition, 19(1),
behavior is not to say that moral behavior isn’t 41–62.
Bittman, M. (2008, January 27). Rethinking the
important or that cynical motivations render
Meat-Guzzler. The New York Times. www.
moral behavior immoral. When we take our
nytimes.com/2008/01/27/weekinreview/
moral intuitions as moral rules we project and 27bittman.html (Accessed 4 January 2020)
institutionalize our evolved moral blind spots Bloom, P. (2017). Against empathy: The case
into the world, often making it worse for for rational compassion. Random House.
others. Advocacy requires understanding. If Bockman, J. (2016, November 1). Why Farmed
animal suffering is an ethical issue, we have to Animals? Animal Charity Evaluators. https://
be realistic about our incentives to signal, our animalcharityevaluators.org/donation-
functional emotional responses and what com- advice/why-farmed-animals/ (Accessed 4
prises our evolved moral psychology towards January 2020)
animals. Bostrom, N. (2016). Superintelligence: Paths,
dangers, strategies. UK, Oxford: Oxford Uni-
versity Press.
Bradshaw, J. W., & Paul, E. S. (2010). Could
empathy for animals have been an adaptation
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS in the evolution of Homo sapiens. Animal
Welfare, 19(1), 107–112.
For helpful feedback, thanks to Hal Herzog, Braithwaite, V. (2010). Do fish feel pain? OUP
Geoffrey Miller, and Todd Shackelford. Oxford. http://books.google.co.uk/books?
ANIMAL ETHICS AND EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY 163

hl=en&lr=&id=aMvonPqzu_cC&oi=fnd& Fessler, D. M. T., Arguello, A. P., Mekdara, J. M., &


pg=PT2&dq=do+fish+feel+pain&ots=tjbXOJy Macias, R. (2003). Disgust sensitivity and
C5x&sig=oU0xqgrjKN3RQfRll99esQfMxEI meat consumption: A test of an emotivist
(Accessed 4 January 2020) account of moral vegetarianism. Appetite,
Bryant, C., & Barnett, J. (2018). Consumer 41(1), 31–41.
acceptance of cultured meat: A systematic Fessler, D. M. T., & Navarrete, C. D. (2003).
review. Meat Science, 143, 8–17. Meat is good to taboo: Dietary proscriptions
Bryant, C. J., Szejda, K., Deshpande, V., Parekh, as a product of the interaction of psychologi-
N., & Tse, B. (2019). A survey of consumer cal mechanisms and social processes. Journal
perceptions of plant-based and clean meat of Cognition and Culture, 3(1), 1–40. https://
in the USA, India, and China. Frontiers in doi.org/10.1163/156853703321598563
Sustainable Food Systems, 3, 11. Fiddes, N. (2004). Meat: A natural symbol.
Carzon, P., Delfour, F., Dudzinski, K., Oremus, Abingdon Oxfordshire: Routledge.
M., & Clua, É. (2019). Cross-genus adoptions Fitzgerald, A. J., Kalof, L., & Dietz, T. (2009).
in delphinids: One example with taxonomic Slaughterhouses and increased crime rates:
discussion. Ethology, 125(9), 669–676. An empirical analysis of the spillover from ‘The
https://doi.org/10.1111/eth.12916 Jungle’ into the surrounding community.
Caviola, L. (2019). How we value animals: The Organization & Environment, 22(2), 158–184.
psychology of speciesism [University of Oxford]. Fleischman, D. S. (2012, May 16). Lab Meat:
https://ora.ox.ac.uk/objects/uuid:9000a8be- Survey Results. The Vegan Option. http://
b6dc-4c63-88e8-974b9daaa83a (Accessed 4 theveganoption.org/2012/05/16/lab-meat-
January 2020) survey-results/ (Accessed 4 January 2020)
Caviola, L., Everett, J. A., & Faber, N. S. (2019). Fleischman, D. S. (2013, August 5). Will Clean
The moral standing of animals: Towards a Meat Become Cruelty Free? Sentientist.
psychology of speciesism. Journal of person- https://dianaverse.com/2020/04/29/will-
ality and social psychology, 116(6), 1011. clean-meat-become-cruelty-free-repost/
Churchill, W. (1932). Fifty years hence. (Accessed 4 January 2020)
Thoughts and adventures, 24–27. Thornton Fleischman, D. S. (2018, May 17). Universal
Butterworth London. Morality Is Obscured by Evolved Morality.
Clark, M. (2019, October 15). What Are We The Evolution Institute. https://evolution-
Supposed to Think About Shrimp? The New institute.org/universal-morality-is-obscured-
York Times. www.nytimes.com/2019/10/15/ by-evolved-morality/ (Accessed 1 May
dining/shrimp-sourcing-united-states.html 2020)
(Accessed 4 January 2020) Flynn, C. P. (2011). Examining the links between
Cohen, R. (2009, July 15). Nesting Blues. The animal abuse and human violence. Crime,
New York Times. www.nytimes.com/2009/ Law and Social Change, 55(5), 453–468.
07/19/magazine/19FOB-ethicist-t.html https://doi.org/10.1007/s10611-011-9297-2
(Accessed 4 January 2020) Foster, K. R., Wenseleers, T., & Ratnieks, F. L.
Cooney, N. (2014). Veganomics: The surprising (2006). Kin selection is the key to altruism.
science on vegetarians, from the breakfast Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 21(2), 57–60.
table to the bedroom. Herndon Virginia Fox Hunting. (2019). In Wikipedia. https://
USA: Lantern Books. en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?title=Fox_
Dahlgreen, W. (2013, August 5). No British hunting&oldid=933138160 (Accessed
Demand for Fake Meat. YouGov. https:// 2 January 2020)
yougov.co.uk/news/2013/08/05/no-demand- Francione, G. L. (2009). Animals as persons.
fake-meat/ (Accessed 4 January 2020) New York: Columbia University Press.
Diamond, J. (1993). New Guineans and their Francione, G. L. (2015). Animal rights: The
natural world. The biophilia hypothesis, abolitionist approach. Louisville, Kentucky:
251–271. Island Press Washington DC. Exempla Press.
Elwood, R. W. (2011). Pain and suffering Frank, H., & Frank, M. G. (1982). On the effects
in invertebrates? Ilar Journal, 52(2), of domestication on canine social develop-
175–184. ment and behavior. Applied Animal Ethol-
164 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

ogy, 8(6), 507–525. https://doi.org/ Herzog, H. (2019, August 6). Did Breast-
10.1016/0304-3762(82)90215-2 Feeding Play a Role in the Evolution of Pets?
Beauchamp, T. L., & Frey, R. G. (Eds.). (2011). Psychology Today. www.psychologytoday.
The Oxford handbook of animal ethics. com/blog/animals-and-us/201908/did-
Oxford University Press. Oxford, UK. breast-feeding-play-role-in-the-evolution-
172–198. pets (Accessed 4 January 2020)
Funk, C., Rainie, L., Smith, A., Olmstead, K., Herzog, H., & Arluke, A. (2006). Human–
Duggan, M., & Page, D. (2015). Attitudes animal connections: Recent findings on the
and Beliefs on Science and Technology Topics. anthrozoology of cruelty. Behavioral and
Pew Research Center. www.pewresearch. Brain Sciences, 29(3), 230–231.
org/science/2015/01/29/chapter-3-attitudes- Herzog, K. (2019, December 2). Genetic Freak
and-beliefs-on-science-and-technology- Wins National Dog Show. The Stranger. www.
topics/ (Accessed 4 January 2020) thestranger.com/slog/2019/12/02/42155095/
Ghirlanda, S., Acerbi, A., Herzog, H., & Serpell, genetic-freak-wins-national-dog-show
J. (2013). Fashion vs. function in cultural (Accessed 4 January 2020)
evolution: The case of dog breed popularity. Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Treiman, R. (1982). Dog-
PLoS One, 8(9), e74770. gerel: Motherese in a new context. Journal
Greene, J. D. (2013). Moral tribes: Emotion, of Child Language, 9(1), 229–237.
reason, and the gap between us and them. Hodal, K., & Lawrence, C. K. F. (2014, June 10).
London, UK: Penguin. Revealed: Asian Slave Labour Producing
Gullone, E. (2014). An evaluative review of Prawns for Supermarkets in US, UK. The
theories related to animal cruelty. Journal of Guardian. www.theguardian.com/global-
Animal Ethics, 4(1), 37–57. development/2014/jun/10/supermarket-
Haidt, J. (2006). The happiness hypothesis: prawns-thailand-produced-slave-labour
Finding modern truth in ancient wisdom. (Accessed 4 January 2020)
New York, USA Basic Books. Holden, C. J., & Herzog, H. (2019). Featherless
Haidt, J. (2012). The righteous mind: Why good chickens and puppies that glow in the dark.
people are divided by religion and politics. In K. Dhont & G. Hodson (Eds.), Why we love
New York: Pantheon Books. http://media. and exploit animals: Bridging insights from
matthewsbooks.com.s3.amazonaws.com/ academia and advocacy. Abingdon, Oxford-
documents/tocwork/030/9780307377906. shire: Routledge. 137–153.
pdf (Accessed 4 January 2020) Howard, B. C. (2016, June 21). Dog Meat
Herzog, H. (2002). Darwinism and the study of Festival Opens Amid Outrage. National
human–animal interactions. Society & Geographic News. www.nationalgeographic.
Animals, 10(4), 361–367. https://doi.org/ com/news/2016/06/china-yulin-dog-meat-
10.1163/156853002320936818 festival-controversy/ (Accessed 4 January
Herzog, H. (2007). Gender differences in 2020)
human–animal interactions: A review. Hurford, P. (2014, May 31). How Much Suffer-
Anthrozoös, 20(1), 7–21. ing Is in the Standard American Diet? Every-
Herzog, H. (2010). Some we love, some we day Utilitarian. www.everydayutilitarian.com/
hate, some we eat: Why it’s so hard to think essays/how-much-suffering-is-in-the-stand-
straight about animals (Reprint edition). ard-american-diet/ (Accessed 4 January 2020)
HarperCollins e-books. Izar, P., Verderane, M. P., Visalberghi, E., Ottoni,
Herzog, H. (2011, June 20). Why Do Most Veg- E. B., Gomes De Oliveira, M., Shirley, J., &
etarians Go Back to Eating Meat? Animals Fragaszy, D. (2006). Cross-genus adoption
and Us. www.psychologytoday.com/blog/ of a marmoset (Callithrix jacchus) by wild
animals-and-us/201106/why-do-most-­ capuchin monkeys (Cebus libidinosus): Case
vegetarians-go-back-eating-meat (Accessed report. American Journal of Primatology:
4 January 2020) Official Journal of the American Society of
Herzog, H. (2014). Biology, culture, and the Primatologists, 68(7), 692–700.
origins of pet-keeping. Animal Behavior and Joy, M. (2003). Psychic numbing and meat
Cognition, 1(3), 296–308. consumption: The psychology of carnism.
ANIMAL ETHICS AND EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY 165

(Unpublished dissertation). Saybrook Gradu- and target. Group Processes & Intergroup
ate School, NY. Relations, 20(6), 721–744.
Joy, M. (2011). Why we love dogs, eat pigs, Madrigal, A. C. (2013, August 6). Chart: When
and wear cows: An introduction to car- Will We Eat Hamburgers Grown in Test
nism. Newburyport, Massachusetts: Conari Tubes? The Atlantic. www.theatlantic.com/
Press. technology/archive/2013/08/chart-when-
Kahane, G. (2009). Pain, dislike and experi- will-we-eat-hamburgers-grown-in-test-
ence. Utilitas, 21(3), 327–336. tubes/278405/ (Accessed 4 January 2020)
Kaminski, J., Waller, B. M., Diogo, R., Hart- Marino, L. (2019). I am not an animal. Animal
stone-Rose, A., & Burrows, A. M. (2019). Sentience, 3(23), 40.
Evolution of facial muscle anatomy in dogs. Masci, D. (2017, February 22). 20 Years after
Proceedings of the National Academy of Dolly the Sheep’s Debut, Americans Remain
Sciences, 116(29), 14677–14681. Skeptical of Cloning. Pew Research Center.
Kasperbauer, T. J. (2015). Rejecting empathy www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2017/
for animal ethics. Ethical Theory and Moral 02/22/20-years-after-dolly-the-sheeps-
Practice, 18(4), 817–833. https://doi. debut-americans-remain-skeptical-of-­
org/10.1007/s10677-014-9557-1 cloning/ (Accessed 4 January 2020)
Kearney, J. (2010). Food consumption trends McLaren, M. E. (2014). Cultured Meat: A
and drivers. Philosophical Transactions of the Beneficial, Crucial, and Inevitable Nutrition
Royal Society of London B: Biological Technology. Seton Hall University. Law
Sciences, 365(1554), 2793–2807. https:// School Student Scholarship. 527. http://
doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0149 scholarship.shu.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?
Krupp, D. B., DeBruine, L. M., & Jones, B. C. article=1527&context=student_scholarship
(2011). Cooperation and conflict in the light of (Accessed 4 January 2020)
kin recognition systems. The Oxford Handbook Miller, G. (2019). Virtue signaling: Essays on
of Evolutionary Family Psychology. Oxford. UK: Darwinian politics & free speech. Albuquerque,
Oxford University Press. 345–362. New Mexico: Cambrian Moon.
Kurzban, R. (2011). Why everyone (else) is a Minson, J. A., & Monin, B. (2012). Do-gooder
hypocrite: Evolution and the modular mind. derogation: Disparaging morally motivated
Princeton: Princeton University Press. minorities to defuse anticipated reproach.
Kurzban, R., Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2001). Social Psychological and Personality Science,
Can race be erased? Coalitional computation 3(2), 200–207.
and social categorization. Proceedings of the Mohorčich, J. (2018). What Can the Adoption
National Academy of Sciences, 98(26), of GM Foods Teach Us about the Adoption
15387–15392. of Other Food Technologies? Sentience Insti-
Lew-Levy, S., Reckin, R., Lavi, N., Cristóbal- tute. https://sentienceinstitute.org/gm-foods
Azkarate, J., & Ellis-Davies, K. (2017). How (Accessed 4 January 2020)
do hunter-gatherer children learn subsist- Monin, B. (2007). Holier than me? Threatening
ence skills? Human Nature, 28(4), 367–394. social comparison in the moral domain.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-017-9302-2 Revue Internationale de Psychologie Sociale,
Lieberman, D., Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2007). 20(1), 53–68.
The architecture of human kin detection. Mood, A., & Brooke, P. (2010). Estimating the
Nature, 445(7129), 727–731. number of fish caught in global fishing each
Lindemann, B., Ogiwara, Y., & Ninomiya, Y. year. http://fishcount.org.uk/published/std/
(2002). The discovery of umami. Chemical fishcountstudy.pdf (Accessed 4 January 2020)
Senses, 27(9), 843–844. Moore, D. C. (2003). Public Lukewarm on
Machan, T. R. (2004). Putting humans first: Animal Rights. Gallup. https://news.gallup.
Why we are nature’s favorite. Lanham Mary- com/poll/8461/Public-Lukewarm-Animal-
land USA: Rowman & Littlefield Publishers. Rights.aspx (Accessed 4 January 2020)
MacInnis, C. C., & Hodson, G. (2017). It ain’t Nakajima, S., Yamamoto, M., & Yoshimoto, N.
easy eating greens: Evidence of bias toward (2009). Dogs look like their owners:
vegetarians and vegans from both source Replications with racially homogenous owner
166 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

portraits. Anthrozoös, 22(2), 173–181. pet-is-a-cat-dog-parakeet-or-something-


https://doi.org/10.2752/175303709X434194 else-the-pet-industry-is-thriving-and-for-
Norwood, F. B., & Lusk, J. (2011). Compassion, good-reason-more-than-three-in-f/ (Accessed
by the pound: The economics of farm animal 4 January 2020)
welfare. Oxford UK: Oxford University Press. Pinker, S. (2011). The better angels of our nature:
Pachirat, T. (2011). Every twelve seconds: The decline of violence in history and its
Industrialized slaughter and the politics of causes. London, UK: Penguin. https://books.
sight. Yale University Press. New Haven: google.co.uk/books?hl=en&lr=&id=c3cWa-
Connecticut. GnsfMC&oi=fnd&pg=PT11&dq=better+angel
Park, J. H., & Ackerman, J. M. (2011). Passion s+of+our+nature&ots=aKhKguUK8a&sig=iKN
and compassion: Psychology of kin relations 0Khly8JAx42B3LcJu30My-m8
within and beyond the family. In Salmon, C., Rauch, A., & Sharp, J. S. (2005). Ohioans’ atti-
& Shackelford, T. K. (Eds.) (2011). The Oxford tudes about animal welfare. Social Responsi-
handbook of evolutionary family psychology. bility Initiative. Department of Human and
Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Community Resource Development. The
Park, J. H., & Schaller, M. (2005). Does attitude Ohio State University.
similarity serve as a heuristic cue for kinship? Reese, J. (2017). Animal Farming Attitudes
Evidence of an implicit cognitive association. Survey 2017. Sentience Institute. www.sen-
Evolution and Human Behavior, 26(2), 158– tienceinstitute.org/animal-farming-attitudes-
170. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav. survey-2017 (Accessed 4 January 2020)
2004.08.013 Reese, J. (2018, June 25). 3 Big Changes We
Parlapiano, A. (2019, September 3). Why Need in the Farmed Animal Movement. Sen-
Euthanasia Rates at Animal Shelters Have tience Institute. www.sentienceinstitute.org/
Plummeted. The New York Times. www. blog/three-big-changes (Accessed 4 January
nytimes.com/2019/09/03/upshot/why-eutha 2020)
nasia-rates-at-animal-shelters-have-plum- Reese, J. (2019). US Factory Farming Estimates.
meted.html (Accessed 4 January 2020) Sentience Institute. www.sentienceinstitute.
Patterson-Kane, E. G., & Piper, H. (2009). org/us-factory-farming-estimates (Accessed
Animal abuse as a sentinel for human vio- 4 January 2020)
lence: A critique. Journal of Social Issues, Regan, T. (2004). The case for animal rights.
65(3), 589–614. https://doi.org/10.1111/ Oakland, California: University of California
j.1540-4560.2009.01615.x Press.
Paul, A. S., Lusk, J. L., Norwood, F. B., & Tonsor, Riffkin, R. (2015, May 18). In U.S., More Say
G. T. (2019). An experiment on the vote-buy Animals Should Have Same Rights as People.
gap with application to cage-free eggs. Jour- Gallup. https://news.gallup.com/poll/183275/
nal of Behavioral and Experimental Econom- say-animals-rights-people.aspx (Accessed 4
ics, 79, 102–109. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. January 2020)
socec.2019.02.005 Roy, M. M., & Nicholas, J. S. C. (2004). Do dogs
PeTAUK (2019). These 27 Countries Have resemble their owners? Psychological Science,
Banned Wild-Animal Circuses! [Blog Post]. 15(5), 361–363. https://doi.org/10.1111/
https://www.peta.org.uk/blog/these-26- j.0956-7976.2004.00684.x
countries-that-have-banned-wild-animal- Rozin, P., Markwith, M., & Stoess, C. (1997).
circuses-are-making-england-look-really- Moralization and becoming a vegetarian: The
bad/ (Accessed 6 July, 2020) transformation of preferences into values
Petrinovich, L., O’Neill, P., & Jorgensen, M. and the recruitment of disgust. Psychological
(1993). An empirical study of moral intuitions: Science, 8(2), 67.
Toward an evolutionary ethics. Journal of Per- Ryder, R. D. (1991). Souls and sentientism.
sonality and Social Psychology, 64(3), 467. Between the Species: An Online Journal for
Pets Really Are Members of the Family. (2011, the Study of Philosophy and Animals, 7(1) 1–5.
June 10). The Harris Poll. https://theharrispoll. https://doi.org/10.15368/bts.1991v7n1.1
com/americans-have-always-had-interesting- Saad, L. (2010, May 26). Four Moral Issues
relationships-with-their-pets-whether-that- Sharply Divide Americans. Gallup. https://
ANIMAL ETHICS AND EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY 167

news.gallup.com/poll/137357/Four-Moral-Issues- Singer, P., & Mason, J. (2006). The ethics of


Sharply-Divide-Americans.aspx (Accessed what we eat. Melbourne: Text Publishing.
4 January 2020) Sneddon, L. U., Braithwaite, V. A., & Gentle, M.
Sandøe, P., Kondrup, S. V., Bennett, P. C., Fork- J. (2003). Novel object test: Examining
man, B., Meyer, I., Proschowsky, H. F., Serpell, J. nociception and fear in the rainbow trout.
A., & Lund, T. B. (2017). Why do people buy The Journal of Pain, 4(8), 431–440.
dogs with potential welfare problems related to Sperber, D., & Baumard, N. (2012). Moral
extreme conformation and inherited disease? A reputation: An evolutionary and cognitive
representative study of Danish owners of four perspective. Mind & Language, 27(5),
small dog breeds. PLoS One, 12(2), e0172091. 495–518.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0172091 Steinfeld, H., Gerber, P., Wassenaar, T. D.,
Santos, M. L. S., & Booth, D. A. (1996). Castel, V., Rosales, M., Rosales, M., & de
Influences on meat avoidance among British Haan, C. (2006). Livestock’s long shadow:
students. Appetite, 27(3), 197–205. environmental issues and options. Food &
Scott-Phillips, T. C., Dickins, T. E., & West, S. A. Agriculture Org. https://books.google.co.uk/
(2011). Evolutionary theory and the ultimate– books?id=1B9LQQkm_qMC&lpg=PP1&ots=
proximate distinction in the human behavioral LO1XiY7OlI&dq=livestock’s%20long%20sha
sciences. Perspectives on Psychological dow&lr&pg=PP1#v=onepage&q&f=false
Science, 6(1), 38–47. https://doi.org/10.1177/ Strauss, M. (2018, August 16). Americans
1745691610393528 Divided over Use of Animals in Scientific
Sentientism. (2019). In Wikipedia. https:// Research. Pew Research Center. www.
en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?title=Sentienti pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2018/08/16/
sm&oldid=926119747 (Accessed 4 January a m e r i c a n s - a re - d i v i d e d - o v e r- t h e - u s e -
2020) of-animals-in-scientific-research/ (Accessed
Serpell, J. (1996). In the company of animals: A 4 January 2020)
study of human-animal relationships. Cam- Testing Cosmetics on Animals. (2019). In
bridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Wikipedia. https://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.
Serpell, J. (2019). How happy is your pet? The php?title=Testing_cosmetics_on_animals&
problem of subjectivity in the assessment of oldid=929257214 (Accessed 4 January
companion animal welfare. Animal Welfare, 2020)
28(1), 57–66. https://doi.org/10.7120/ Tomasik, B. (2017, August 17). Suffering in Ani-
09627286.28.1.057 mals vs. Humans. Reducing Suffering. https://
Sherman, G. D., & Haidt, J. (2011). Cuteness reducing-suffering.org/suffering-in-animals-
and disgust: The humanizing and dehuman- vs-humans/ (Accessed 4 January 2020)
izing effects of emotion. Emotion Review, Tomasik, B. (2018a, September 20). Bacteria,
3(3), 245–251. https://doi.org/10.1177/ Plants, and Graded Sentience. Reducing
1754073911402396 Suffering. https://reducing-suffering.org/
Shostak, M., & Nisa. (2004). Nisa: The life and bacteria-plants-and-graded-sentience/
words of a !Kung woman. Cambridge: Har- (Accessed 4 January 2020)
vard University Press. Tomasik, B. (2018b, July 14). How Much Direct
Simler, K., & Hanson, R. (2017). The elephant in Suffering Is Caused by Various Animal
the brain: Hidden motives in everyday life. Foods? Reducing Suffering. https://reducing-
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. suffering.org/how-much-direct-suffering-is-
Singer, P. (1995). Animal liberation. Random caused-by-various-animal-foods/ (Accessed 4
House. January 2020)
Singer, P. (2011). The expanding circle: Ethics, Tomasik, B. (2019, July 21). Is Brain Size
evolution, and moral progress. Princeton: Morally Relevant? Reducing Suffering.
Princeton University Press. http://books. https://reducing-suffering.org/is-brain-size-
google.co.uk/books?hl=en&lr=&id=Yve7lgvt morally-relevant/ (Accessed 4 January 2020)
LcsC&oi=fnd&pg=PP2&dq=the+expanding+ Tomasula, D. (2001, July 26). PETA Says Eat a
c irc le & ot s=5L e S l Yq g O j & s i g = 2 U l L 4 8 I p Whale, Save a Pig. DMNews.Com. www.
LqC-MKX7_s10Q0PjvnA dmnews.com/marketing-channels/direct-mail/
168 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

news/13088690/peta-says-eat-a-whale- (2013). Paedomorphic facial expressions give


save-a-pig (Accessed 4 January 2020) dogs a selective advantage. PLoS One, 8(12),
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1989). Evolutionary e82686.
psychology and the generation of culture, Wiblin, R., & Bollard, L. (2017, September 27).
part I: Theoretical considerations. Ethology Why we must end factory farming as soon as
and Sociobiology, 10(1–3), 29–49. possible – and how to do it [Interview].
Topolski, R., Weaver, J. N., Martin, Z., & McCoy, https://80000hours.org/podcast/episodes/
J. (2013). Choosing between the emotional lewis-bollard-end-factory-farming/ (Accessed
dog and the rational pal: A moral dilemma 4 January 2020)
with a tail. Anthrozoös, 26(2), 253–263. Wilks, M., & Phillips, C. J. (2017). Attitudes to
https://doi.org/10.2752/175303713X13636 in vitro meat: A survey of potential consumers
846944321 in the United States. PloS One, 12(2). https://
Turner, W. G. (2001). Our new children: The sur- jour nals.plos.org/plosone/article/file?
rogate role of companion animals in women’s type=printable&id=10.1371/journal.pone.
lives. The Qualitative Report, 6(1), 1–10. 0171904 (Accessed 4 January 2020)
Tybur, J. M., Lieberman, D., Kurzban, R., & DeScioli, Wilson, E. O. (1984). Biophilia: The human
P. (2013). Disgust: Evolved function and bond with other species. Cambridge: Harvard
structure. Psychological Review, 120(1), 65. University Press.
Urquiza-Haas, E. G., & Kotrschal, K. (2015). Witwicki, K. (2017). Social Movement Lessons
The mind behind anthropomorphic thinking: From the British Antislavery Movement:
Attribution of mental states to other species. Focused on Applications to the Movement
Animal Behaviour, 109, 167–176. https:// Against Animal Farming. Sentience Institute.
doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.08.011 www.sentienceinstitute.org/british-antislavery
Vollum, S., Longmire, D., & Buffington-Vollum, (Accessed 4 January 2020)
J. (2004). Moral disengagement and attitudes Wrangham, R. (2009). Catching fire: How
about violence toward animals. Society & cooking made us human. New York: Basic
Animals, 12(3), 209–235. Books. https://books.google.co.uk/books?
Walker, J. K., McGrath, N., Nilsson, D. L., hl=en&lr=&id=ebEOupKz-rMC&oi=fnd&pg=P
Waran, N. K., & Phillips, C. J. (2014). The role R5&dq=humans+evolved+eating+meat
of gender in public perception of whether &ots=sVRt8Y8NLS&sig=2a3UdxIPEq_Crn
animals can experience grief and other BKY0XK8O5hGrI
emotions. Anthrozoös, 27(2), 251–266. Zaraska, M. (2016). Meathooked: The history
Waller, B. M., Peirce, K., Caeiro, C. C., Scheider, and science of our 2.5-million-year obsession
L., Burrows, A. M., McCune, S., & Kaminski, J. with meat. New York: Basic Books.
PART 2

Applications to Law and Order


This page intentionally left blank
8
Evolutionary Psychology and
Political Institutions
Michael Latner and Elissa Feld

Biology and political science have a long, THE EVOLUTION OF COOPERATION


symbiotic relationship in their shared focus
on conflict and cooperation between organ- The Scottish Enlightenment, especially the
isms. Many of the major puzzles in biology work of David Hume, had a significant influ-
and political science, from the emergence of ence on James Madison’s theory of republican
cooperation, and the threat of parasitism and government (Mclean, 2005). Hume’s discourses
selfishness, to the dynamics of collective on competition and selective retention would
decision making and the evolution of moral- figure prominently in Madison’s constitutional-
ity, are interwoven in early, pre-Darwinian design principles, especially his commitment to
works. As these areas of study developed into design institutions responsive enough to ‘guard
separate professional disciplines in the early against the cabals of a few’ but not subject to so
20th century, they would part ways with con- much ‘mutability’ and ‘incessant changes’ that
siderable tension before returning to such ‘no man who knows what the law is today can
fundamentals with the advent of modern guess what it will be tomorrow’ (Palmer, 2002:
evolutionary psychology. In this chapter, we 109). Hume’s concern about the degenerative
survey the literature surrounding three major effects of extreme elements in political compe-
areas where the study of political institutions tition, as well as the capacity for such diverse
and evolutionary psychology intersect: the interests to be ‘serviceable’ to the public inter-
evolution of cooperation at the micro-level; est, would be fully developed in Madison’s
the emergence of complex political systems; writings on faction, the modern study of politi-
and democracy as a major evolutionary tran- cal institutions, and constitutional design
sition in nature. (Madison et al., [1788]2008).
172 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Charles Darwin studied both Hume and who came to the front at war and ‘freely
Madison’s contemporary, the Reverend risked their lives for others, would on an aver-
Thomas Malthus, whose Essay on the Principle age perish in larger number than other men’,
of Population (Malthus, [1798]2013) began to producing fewer offspring (Darwin, 1876:
quantify the relationship between competition 130). Immoral (selfish) actors would, on aver-
for resources and human-population growth age, out-produce their braver comrades, even-
(Hamilton et al., [1788]1998). Madison him- tually driving the trait of bravery out of the
self understood that the world would be ‘much population. However, competition between
indebted to’ Malthus for his work, though a groups could change the selective dynamics:
decade before Malthus’ Principle was pub-
It must not be forgotten that although a high
lished, Madison had already been reflecting standard of morality gives but a slight or no advan-
with Thomas Jefferson on the degenerative tage to each individual man and his children over
social effects of concentrated resources among the other men of the same tribe, yet that an
the ‘idle rich’ and the potential threat of over- advancement in the standard of morality and an
increase in the number of well-endowed men will
population (Madison, 1786; McCoy, 1980).
certainly give an immense advantage to one tribe
The threat of parasitism on social prosper- over another. (Darwin, 1876: 132)
ity and development was on the mind of
these institutional reformers. The Principle This is the basis of the cooperation dilemma,
of Population would also lead Darwin to the and the question of how cooperative or ‘good’
insight that with selection pressures under traits could gain a selective advantage through
conditions of scarcity ‘favourable variations social decisions was a central feature in early
would tend to be preserved, and unfavourable social choice theory. On that topic another
ones to be destroyed. The results of this would contemporary of Madison, the Marquis de
be the formation of a new species. Here, then Condorcet, also played a major role. He for-
I had at last got a theory by which to work’ mally showed that voters seeking the best col-
(Darwin, [1838]2011). lective decision for their group could rely on
Hume may have also been influential in majority rule to most likely yield a ‘correct’
the way that Madison and Darwin thought decision, if the probability of a voter being
about the emergence of cooperative behav- right was greater than 50%, and that the prob-
ior within the context of group competition, ability of a correct outcome approaches 100%
an insight neglected by Malthus. Consider as the size of the electorate increases (Young,
Hume’s account of the logic of reciprocity 1988). Condorcet’s probabilistic explorations
in his Treatise on Human Nature (Binmore, at least indirectly influenced Madison as well
1994: 261): (Schofield, 2005b), specifically the proclama-
tion in Federalist #10 that ‘If the proportion of
I learn to do service to another, without bearing
him any real kindness, because I foresee, that he fit characters be not less in the large than in the
will return my service in expectation of another of small republic, the former will present a greater
the same kind, and in order to maintain the same option, and consequently a greater probability
correspondence of good offices with me and of a fit choice’. (Hamilton et al., [1788]1998).
others. And accordingly, after I have serv’d him … While many thinkers who subsequently took
he is induced to do his part, as foreseeing the
consequences of his refusal. the mantle of ‘Darwinist’ would continue to
think about evolution purely in terms of compe-
Darwin certainly recognized the dynamics of tition, following T. H. Huxley’s famous dictum
reciprocity at play in cooperative behavior that nature ‘is on about the same level as the
and the dilemma of moral development. gladiator’s show’ from the ‘point of view of the
Darwin viewed morality as the highest princi- moralist’ (Kropotkin et al., 1955), others would
ple of human development, but he also saw focus on empirical observations of cooperation
that in group competition the ‘bravest men’ in nature and seek to explain them. In particular,
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND POLITICAL INSTITUTIONS 173

the Russian naturalist Peter Kropotkin’s obser- allowances for large working-class families
vations led him to believe that evolution was and those who possessed less ‘innate capac-
about combination as much as competition. ity for intellectual and emotional develop-
He observed that reciprocity was widespread, ment’ (Fisher, [1930]2000).
even between species. Kropotkin’s analysis The greatest breakthrough, and the reunion
of social insects, parental behavior, and asso- of evolutionary biology and political science,
ciative activities like pack hunting, integrated began after W. D. Hamilton broke the code
with an analysis of medieval guilds, led him to for kin selection (Hamilton, 1964a, 1964b).
the conclusion that mutual aid was as much a Adapting economic cost–benefit analysis,
law of nature as mutual struggle. In addition Hamilton showed how natural selection
to anticipating much in the scientific study would allow for altruism to evolve while still
of altruism, Kropotkin’s Mutual Aid remains maximizing individual fitness. In a series of
a foundational work of anarcho-communism articles, he demonstrated how natural selec-
(Kropotkin, 2017). tion can favor altruism between relatives
J. B. S. Haldane (also a vocal communist) when the product of the relatedness of indi-
was perhaps the first to begin formalizing viduals and the benefits of reciprocity out-
the link between genetic traits and sociality weigh the individual costs. The concept of
(Haldane, 1941). In The Causes of Evolution, inclusive fitness was born, as was Hamilton’s
Haldane first proposed that reciprocity could legacy as one of the greatest evolutionary
spread in a population if the genes determining theorists of the 20th century.
it were carried by individuals whose offspring The next giant step would be taken by
benefitted from the presence of the gene in their Robert Trivers, who extended Hamilton’s work
nearby relatives (Haldane, 1932). Similarly, R. to non-kin reciprocity, such as bird warning
A. Fisher, whose Genetical Theory of Natural calls, the symbiosis between cleaner and pred-
Selection heralded the modern synthesis in ator fish, and broader social interactions. His
evolutionary biology, focused considerable 1971 publication ‘The Evolution of Reciprocal
attention on how degrees of genetic relatedness Altruism’ showed how net benefits could be
affected individuals living in different types of accrued between non-relatives through social
populations (Fisher, [1930]2000). exchange, and how cheating and spiteful
It is telling that the politically conserva- behavior could be regulated through the evolu-
tive Fisher was attracted to understanding tion of moralistic aggression (Trivers, 1971).
how ‘distastefulness’ in insect larvae could Trivers’ work opened new opportunities for
spread in a population. He reasoned that the integration of the behavioral sciences, as he
nasty tasting larvae would provide increased sketched out how the analysis could extend to
protection for their siblings by driving away complex, multi-party coordination, the emer-
predators, such that the genetic benefit of an gence of norms and collective punishments
eaten larvae could be substantial. Perhaps (and rewards), generational return effects, and
Fisher’s politics motivated his interest in the like (Trivers, 2006).
distastefulness: recent research suggests that Soon after, John Maynard Smith (a student
sensitivity to disgust is linked to conserva- of Haldane) and George R. Price (a friend of
tism (Murray, 2012). Sadly, both Haldane and Hamilton) would formalize the Evolutionarily
Fisher were advocates of eugenics (Haldane Stable Strategy (ESS) and initiate evolution-
coined the term ‘cloning’ and Fisher headed ary game theory in ‘The Logic of Animal
the Department of Eugenics at University Conflict’ (Smith and Price, 1973), directly
College London), but unlike Haldane, Fisher addressing Darwin’s dilemma of moral devel-
viewed the fall of civilizations as a function of opment. An ESS is a strategy (social interac-
declining fertility rates of the upper classes, tion where agents compete over a resource,
and he was concerned about encouraging such as food, with payoffs dependent on both
174 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

agents’ strategies) that is stable once it is fixed cooperation is a fertile interdisciplinary field,
in a population. If agents will not do better identifying multiple stable states of coopera-
(more offspring) by employing a different tion, the foundations of pro- and anti-social
strategy, or perturbations from ‘mutant’ strat- behavior, the crucial role of structured interac-
egies cannot successfully invade and replace tion, and the ways that institutions cultivate
it, the trait becomes fixed in the population as cooperation (Alford and Hibbing, 2004;
the offspring of the winners replace those with Lopez, 2017; The Cooperative Human, 2018).
lower payoffs. The ESS has not only become One of the most relevant ongoing debates
central to the explanation of human evolu- concerning institutional analysis has to do
tion in evolutionary psychology, it has been with the concept of ‘strong reciprocity’ and
adopted in political science as a means of the role of institutions in sustaining coop-
understanding evolution through institutions. eration. A body of scholars have challenged
That pivotal moment came when W. D. the orthodox view of the reciprocal altruist
Hamilton teamed up with political scientist as self-regarding, developing an alternative
Robert Axelrod to publish ‘The Evolution of model of group-beneficial pro-social behav-
Cooperation’ in 1981 (Axelrod and Hamilton, ior (Bowles et al., 2003; Bowles and Gintis,
1981), one of the most cited publications in 2011; Fehr and Gächter, 2002; Henrich and
political science (Peress, 2019). Axelrod’s Boyd, 1998). Whereas kin selection, recipro-
computer tournaments provided output for sim- cal altruism, indirect reciprocity, and signal-
ulations using competing strategies in repeated ing explain behaviors that appear costly but
Prisoner’s Dilemma (PD) games. In such are repaid to genetic relatives, the strong-
games, both agents know they would do better reciprocity hypothesis posits that a genuinely
if they cooperated, but the payoff is higher for altruistic behavior can be adaptive, but crit-
individuals to defect rather than be exploited. ics are unconvinced (Burnham and Johnson,
Axelrod and Hamilton described how an elo- 2016). The answer to this controversy is
quently simple logic of cooperation (essen- immensely important for the design of politi-
tially do unto others, the ‘Tit for Tat’ strategy cal institutions, and it has rightly become a
submitted by anthropologist Anatol Rapaport) major focus of scientific attention (Abbot,
could emerge as an ESS, given a high-enough et  al., 2011; Ferriere and Michod, 2011;
probability of future interaction between coop- Herre and Wcislo, 2011; Nowak et al., 2010).
erative agents. Their analysis demonstrated
how ‘the benefits of life are disproportionately
available to cooperating groups’ and from it
they drew several strategic considerations: THE EMERGENCE OF COMPLEX
POLITICAL SYSTEMS
• Be nice: don’t be the first to defect.
• Be provocable: return defection for defection, The use of biological metaphors to explain the
cooperation for cooperation.
emergence and persistence of political sys-
• Don’t be envious: focus on maximizing your own
tems dates back at least to Thomas Hobbes’
‘score’, as opposed to ensuring your score is
higher than your ‘partner’s’. Leviathan with its cover art of the Sovereign
• Don’t be too clever: signal clarity is crucial. King, whose body is literally constituted by
the multitude of citizens, obedient co-signers
Successive work on iterated PD has relaxed of the social contract, the body politic repre-
the highly simplifying assumptions built into sented by one man (Hobbes, [1651]2009).
the original model, continuing to produce Walter Bagehot’s ‘Physics and Politics’ was
important insights, including those about the probably the first to explicitly apply Darwinian
importance of forgiveness and reputation selection and inheritance concepts to political
(Nowak, 2006). Today, the study of society (Bagehot, [1869]2009). Writing in
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND POLITICAL INSTITUTIONS 175

1869, Bagehot made a case for liberal-demo- behavior, David Easton’s theoretical explo-
cratic institutions having emerged from more rations reflected his study of physiology and
conformist, dictatorial restraints as a moral systems biology, specifically the concept of
achievement. The selective process had sup- homeostasis and the capacity of evolved sys-
posedly refined the nervous systems and tems to react to environmental stimuli with
moral capacity of ‘accomplished’ elites, cre- equilibrating responses (Cannon, 1963). As
ating opportunity for more complex decision- a member of the University of Chicago’s
making institutions and social progress. Committee on the Behavioral Sciences, he
Bagehot was also among the earliest in a was committed to the integration of biological
long line of ‘Social Darwinists’ to leverage and social scientific knowledge, co-author-
pseudo-scientific accounts of racial inequali- ing ‘Projects and Problems of Homeostatic
ties to justify an ideological theory of the state. Models in the Behavioral Sciences’ with psy-
John William Burgess similarly believed that chologist James G. Miller and anthropolo-
only ‘superior’ races had acknowledged the gist Anatol Rapoport, among others, in 1953
moral ‘duties of civilization’ (Gunnell, 2004). (Fontaine, 2016). Easton’s work transformed
Burgess established the US political science the study of political institutions by nesting
degree at Columbia University, where he them within a living-systems framework.
taught comparative constitutional law, and Easton was primarily interested in under-
his vision of political science emphasized standing how institutional arrangements regu-
the training of career government bureau- late demands on a political system, shaping
crats, as well as civic education as a sort of the way that social choices are put into effect
‘democratic’ training, within the confines of as policy outcomes, which he elaborated in
his racist ideology. After World War I, and A Framework for Political Analysis and A
the embrace of scientific racism within fas- Systems Analysis of Political Life (Easton,
cism and Nazism during World War II, social 1965, 1979). Among his many contributions to
Darwinism was largely discredited and aban- the study of institutions, Easton’s ideas about
doned by political science, at least publicly. how regimes generate support through the
Post-war social scientists turned to formal allocation of biological and social resources,
social choice and game theory as a framework and regulation of social values, stand out
to explain political institutions. Properties of (Easton, 1979). Easton was among the first to
social-decision rules, or ‘constitutional condi- flesh out how emotive attachment to codified
tions’ in the words of Kenneth Arrow, where social roles, and the social status they yield,
individual values are treated as inputs, became supply the behavioral energy upon which a
a focal point of analysis (Arrow, 1970). One political system depends for its survival.
of the most influential political scientists to In the last few decades there has been
emerge from this period was Robert Dahl, increasing convergence of Easton’s macro-
whose Arrow-inspired analysis of social level type of structural-constraints analysis
choice procedures shaped our understand- and the micro-foundations of cooperative
ing of how popular sovereignty and political game theory. Formalization of political author-
equality are instituted (Dahl, 1989, 2006). ity occurs when communicative artifacts,
Nevertheless, Dahl’s image of the locus of from mating and marriage practices to land-
democracy in US government as fluid, plu- tenure customs, nomenclature, measurement,
ralist bargaining assumed an equilibrium of and other forms of codification, instantiate a
social consensus, with no direct focus on evo- ‘high degree of mutual predictability’ with
lution or adaptation (Gunnell, 2004). large-scale coordination of, and thus con-
Dissatisfied with the historical bent of trol over, the allocation of biological and
most political science and unconvinced by social resources (Easton, 1979: 329). Just as
Dahl’s group bargaining theory of political valuable biological information is correlated
176 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

across generations through the germ line, population) to one, or pure dictatorship.
institutional transmission of communicative Both arrangements constitute cooperative
artifacts sustains biocultural complexes that regimes in the sense that in a regime of N
can greatly enhance the fitness of partici- decision makers, all preferences are taken
pants (Corning, 1995). Indeed, the produc- into consideration, maximizing the com-
tion and management of knowledge is itself a munication channels and flow of informa-
commons dilemma (Frischmann et al., 2014). tion to be processed, but also coordination
National constitutions are rules for strat- costs (the resources required to take every-
egy selection. That is, constitutional regimes one’s preferences into account) and potential
operate to regulate social niche-construction ‘boiling’ as each individual does their own
strategies (Santa Fe Institute, 2016). As Brian thing. Alternatively, pure dictatorship may be
Skyrms notes in Evolution of the Social the least costly social choice mechanism in
Contract (2014), here strategies come to the terms of coordination costs, but conformity
fore as units of selection, and individuals costs (everyone obeys the dictator) tend to be
recede into the background (much as genes extreme, in part because there is little effort
do in the biological literature). The success of to search the landscape for mutually benefi-
collectively selected strategies (laws) is based cial strategies (Page, 2008; Zhou, 2011).
on their success as behavioral phenotypes, and This tradeoff maps closely onto the solu-
how well populations converge on successful tions typically proposed for the management
strategies of social interaction (Skyrms, 2014). of public goods, or the ‘tragedy of the com-
Our understanding of institutions as carri- mons’. Ecologist Garrett Hardin famously
ers of cultural transmission has been built on argued in several papers (around the same
decades of successful modeling of coopera- time that models of evolutionary coopera-
tion, complexity, and culture (Axelrod, 2006; tion were emerging) that species are gener-
Cavalli-Sforza and Feldman, 1981; Creanza ally unable to cooperate for the greater good
and Feldman, 2014). (Hardin, 1968). In addition to being another
Studies of the evolution of complexity Malthusian throwback who believed that
show that, as regulators of niche construc- those who won superior intellect through
tion, political institutions face some uni- the genetic lottery (whites, of course) should
versal information-processing problems. deploy abortion and sterilization to maintain
For example, the tradeoff between exploring limited population growth because ‘freedom
alternative strategies and reliance on the sta- to breed will bring ruin to all’ (Hardin, 1968:
tus quo is a general dilemma in organizational 1248), he popularized the problem of main-
productivity (March, 1991). The ‘explore/ taining public goods through his analogy of
exploit’ dilemma arises from the inability to livestock management on shared grasslands,
optimize both the exploration of potentially an iterated PD game emphasizing the free-
adaptive/productive strategies, and cashing rider problem and defection as rational in the
in on the value of e­ xisting strategies. Without short term but irrational in the long run.
commitment to a strategy, a system can ‘boil’ Hardin’s solutions to the tragedy were either
in the endless search for optimal alternatives, privatization and strong property rights, in order
but without generating variation there are fewer to incentivize individual owners to be stewards
alternatives to optimize, making it less likely to over their own land, or the very Hobbesian-
find a better alternative than the status quo. sounding ‘mutual coercion, mutually agreed
In the context of political institutions these upon’ typically interpreted as collectivization.
constraints correspond to the bounds of politi- Willfully or otherwise, Hardin was ignorant
cal authority and social choice. The potential of the degree to which many commons were
range of participation in collective decision already being governed under ‘mutually coer-
making for a population ranges from N (total cive’ social structures that have evolved to
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND POLITICAL INSTITUTIONS 177

manage the ‘explore/exploit’ dilemma without exhibit properties that Walker and Davies
resorting to either strict privatization or col- (2012) characterize as an informational struc-
lectivization. No individual has played as ture that gains causal efficacy over matter, in
large a role in demonstrating the capacity this case, behavior: they are explicit instruc-
of local organizations to manage common tions, decontextualized from specific behav-
resources as Elinor Ostrom (Ostrom, 1990, iors to classes of behaviors, applicable to
2010, 2013). While Ostrom’s work fits within classes of agents, designed to yield high lev-
the broader fields of political economy and els of predictability (Bowles and Gintis, 2011;
organizational behavior (Levi, 1989; March Boyer and Petersen, 2012). Rather than being
and Olson, 1971; Olsen, 1976), her approach directly transmitted, legal expectations are
to core design principles, the Institutional learned, deriving from hard-wired moral intu-
Analysis and Development (IAD) framework, itions, but converted into ‘publicly scrutable
is distinctly Darwinian. Ostrom’s acceptance processes’ that modify social interaction from
speech for the 2009 Nobel prize in economics, the ‘top down’ (Boyer and Petersen, 2012).
‘Do Institutions Evolve?’, recounts decades of According to Boyer and Peterson, like our
research she and colleagues have developed to biophysical equipment, our evolved cogni-
understand how biophysical systems, partici- tive systems operate more reliably in match-
pant heterogeneity, and operational rules shape ing environments (ideally the environment of
the governance of commons (Ostrom, 2013). evolutionary adaptedness). As a result, we are
A set of ‘default’ rules highlights what Ostrom motivated to match our environments to those
has discovered as core design principles: cognitive systems, while the modification of
environmental niches creates and maintains
• Clearly defined group boundaries selective pressures that favor adaptions matched
• Proportionally equivalent costs and benefits
to them (Laland et al., 1999). Richard Dawkins
• Collective-choice mechanisms
famously called this sort of niche construction
• Monitoring
• Graduated sanctions The Extended Phenotype (Dawkins, 1999),
• Fast and fair conflict resolution based on the evolutionary logic that the qual-
• Polycentric governance (tiered rule-making) ity of an environmental niche (a spider web, a
beaver dam, a human settlement) is correlated
The work of Ostrom and other political with genetic variation, in that an allele for bet-
economists, including Douglas North, ter niche construction enhances the fitness of
Margaret Levi, Barry Weingast, and Geoffrey the organisms expressing it. In an environment
Hodgson, has engendered a generalized with good nest-building materials, the best nest
Darwinian approach to the study of institu- builders will proliferate the genes for building
tional change that stands independent of nests with those materials, as their offspring
genetics and biology (Aldrich et  al., 2008; increase in frequency through the population.
Fürstenberg, 2016; Hodgson, 2004; Levi and Constitutional environments will likewise favor
Weingast, 2019; Wilson and Gowdy, 2013). the selection and spread of behavior traits and
We have learned much from empirical field cultural norms, such as a sense of justice, under
studies that have demonstrated the adaptive- constitutional conditions that favor it.
ness of local communities, including tribal
communities that Hardin surely would not
have imagined were cooperatively stable.
Large-scale societies require complex, DEMOCRACY: A MAJOR
adaptive institutions to transmit norms of EVOLUTIONARY TRANSITION?
conflict resolution and morality (Bowles
et al., 2003; Bowles and Gintis, 2011; Ehrlich Representative democracy encompasses a
and Levin, 2005; Levin, 2014). Legal systems range of regimes that have emerged quite
178 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

recently on the human scene, possibly in to the operation of constitutional structure


response to the conformity costs imposed by (Skyrms, 2014). His eloquent essay ‘Sex and
more authoritarian, dictatorial regime struc- Justice’ (1994) compares the puzzle of sex
tures (Latner, 2017). The core design princi- ratios (which led to Fisher’s game-theoretic
ple of democracy is political equality, or insights) to the evolution of justice. Skyrms
fairness. Political theory on procedural jus- shows how, like many observed sex ratios, fair
tice has contributed to our understanding of division (50/50) is an attractive equilibrium
cooperation at all levels of life through a sort in repeated interactions. Crucially, if interac-
of cross fertilization. Specifically, the politi- tions between more cooperative agents can
cal philosopher John Rawls’ concept of the be correlated (a higher probability that they
‘veil of ignorance’ played prominently in the interact with one another instead of greedy
social theory of another Michigan biologist, agents), then the cooperative norm ‘share and
Richard D. Alexander (Alexander, 2017; share alike’ becomes a stronger evolutionary
Rawls, 1999). The basic logic is that if the attractor and will emerge as an ESS.
only way to be sure one gets ahead is to pro- Skyrms shows how mechanisms for the
mote rules that will improve overall welfare, evolution of cooperation, from kin selection
fair laws should be favored for selection. to ‘group’ selection, spatial interaction, and
What emerges is a society that applies laws the repression of competition, are all linked
equally to individuals regardless of their to increasing correlation and cooperative
status, or justice as fairness. association (Skyrms, 2014). Skyrms has also
Alexander saw that the enforcement of brought attention to the problem of ‘negative
mutualistic cooperation requires limiting correlation’ and how spiteful behavior can
opportunities for cheating, such that agents be sustained through correlated interactions,
can only increase their success by increas- such as when a reputation for ‘fighting too
ing the success of others (Frank, 2013). He hard’ (to the detriment of self and opponent)
also saw how natural selection could favor may enable agents to win future contests more
the suppression of internal competition by easily in repeated encounters (Skyrms, 2014).
drawing on the work of E. G. Leigh, probably The likelihood that political equality is a
the first modern biologist to use a political fit alternative against dictatorial hierarchy
analogy, equating the genome to ‘a parliament and anarchy is empirically supported across
of genes: each acts in its own self-interest, but a variety of fields. First, recent developments
if its acts hurt the others, they will combine in social choice theory have confirmed that
together to suppress it’ (Leigh, 1971). Later proportional representation, which most accu-
analysis has supported the idea that the cellu- rately translates votes into seats of political
lar infrastructure of meiosis in sexual repro- power, is the closest approximation to politi-
duction is favored, in part, as a mechanism to cal equality in electoral-system design (Hout
thwart parasitic conflict at the genetic level and McGann, 2009). Contemporary experi-
(Burt and Trivers, 2008; Howard and Lively, mental tests further support Condorcet’s posi-
1994). Similarly, Alexander’s argument that tion that majority rule outperforms alternative
socially imposed monogamy levels reproduc- strategies, including dictatorship (Hastie
tive opportunity, and in doing so reduces the and Kameda, 2005; Sorkin et al., 1998), and
pool of unmarried men, has found support in regimes that more closely approximate politi-
research that shows monogamy is associated cal equality (PR and simple majority rule)
with reduced rates of homicide, rape, and tend toward greater redistribution and respon-
related violence (Henrich et al., 2012). siveness, as predicted (Acemoglu et al., 2009;
Bryan Skyrms’ Evolution of the Social McGann and Latner, 2013). Representative
Contract and later work brings us full cir- assemblies, electoral systems, thresholds
cle through the evolution of cooperation for legislative decision making and policy
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND POLITICAL INSTITUTIONS 179

execution all reflect fundamental features of and assorting of populations. For example,
institutional niche construction and coopera- Creanza and Feldman have considered educa-
tive decision making (Latner, 2017). tion norms, specifically the belief that women
Steven Frank has, more than nearly any should receive secondary education and delay
other evolutionary thinker, shown how the childbirth (Creanza and Feldman, 2014). The
policing of competition, or reduction of probability of fixation of the belief is shaped
opportunities for exploitative behavior, cor- by lower fertility among those women who
relates individual payoffs and can sustain take part, as well as lower infant mortality
group cohesion (Frank, 2003, 2011, 2013). among the same families, and future sorting
Reducing opportunities for exploitative in offspring mating practices. Similar dynam-
behavior through political equality may result ics may impact the norm of democratic par-
in both higher levels of redistributive sharing ticipation and investment in the considerable
and flexible responsiveness in resource man- responsibilities of democratic citizenship, not
agement, making it an evolutionary attractive to mention the interaction between education,
equilibrium (McGann and Latner, 2013). socialization, and support for democratic
The historical record is also supportive of institutions, especially for women (Fox and
the evolutionary democracy hypothesis. The Lawless, 2014).
number of regimes holding minimally free and Asymmetries between the (de jure) consti-
fair elections has nearly doubled since 1990, tutional and (de facto) economic allocation
from 69 to 122 according to Freedom House of political power also shape the dynamics
(‘Freedom in the World’, 2014). Moreover, of regime stability. Daron Acemoglu and col-
among electoral democracies, major elec- leagues have produced a number of analyses
toral reforms have been decisively in the proposing that the threat of upheaval and
direction of more proportional representation benefits of anticipated economic growth have
(Soudriette and Ellis, 2006). This follows the been primary drivers of elite-led expansion
evolution from simple ‘originating’ systems in collective decision making (Acemoglu
in early electoral systems to electoral rules et al., 2009, 2011; Acemoglu and Robinson,
that reduced the frequency of single-party 2013). Peter Turchin has proposed a broader
dominance that had emerged (Colomer, 2001, model of democratic evolution that empha-
2007). At the other extreme, hyper-permissive sizes the role of exogenous threats, with
systems demonstrably produce greater insta- a similar sequencing that cooperation and
bility in governing coalitions, lower cabinet enfranchisement are frequently followed by
duration, and greater difficulty committing to periods of economic inequity and political
comprehensive policy platforms (Shugart and instability (Turchin, 2013, 2016). These and
Wattenberg, 2001; Taagepera, 2007). Reforms other analyses point toward higher potential
in these systems have generally maintained for upheaval under greater economic inequal-
proportionality along with consolidating legis- ity, when lower classes (and their offspring)
lative power in the direction of greater account- feel they have nothing to lose, and when the
ability (Shugart and Wattenberg, 2001). prospective benefits of a major transition out-
Of course, political institutions do not weigh the perceived costs of disruption.
operate exogenously on behavior, independ- The interplay of group conditions and spe-
ent of the complexity of other social systems. cific conflict-reduction mechanisms can deter-
Population heterogeneity and inequality in mine the fate of a regime’s future. If increasing
social resources obviously shape institutional inequality or prolonged economic stagnation
performance and the prospects of regime fuel animosity between competing economic
persistence. For example, cultural mutation classes, it will lead to increased demand for
and cycles of stability are highly sensitive to the powerful to invest in economic and politi-
interactions between transmission, selection, cal mechanisms to better preserve common
180 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

goods, and consequently the stronger taking intergroup conflict or cooperation, such
over more institutional control (Frank, 2011). that both masculine (conflict) and feminine
Whether that control is more equitable or (cooperation) traits can positively contribute
‘extractive’ (parasitic) may in turn depend on to the perception of effective leadership traits
the prior fixation of economic and cultural (Dolan and Lynch, 2014; Grabo and van
traits, like democratic norms among elites. Vugt, 2018). The sub-field of evolutionary
Further, the adequacy of a political solution feminist studies is challenging stereotypes of
will depend on whether competition among bio-essentialism head on, and will surely aid
reformers can be sustained and a coalition our understanding of institutional design with
holds, for example by requiring coalition relation to women’s and gender studies (Buss
building within broad-based political parties, and Malamuth, 1996; Feminist Evolutionary
or whether fragmented interests and the work Perspectives, 2019).
of compromise must be channeled into a rep- Another example of the evolution of
resentative assembly (Balinski and Young, democracy is seen in contestation over vot-
2001; Cox, 1997). ing rights in the United States. Epperly and
In electoral system science, the general colleagues have documented the dynamics at
pattern that proportional electoral systems are work in voter suppression as an exploitative
associated with higher rates of women’s repre- form of ‘cooperation’ (Epperly et al., 2019).
sentation is a puzzle that requires consistency They show that under conditions of low state
between macro- and micro-level explanations capacity and legibility (formalized authority
(Reynolds et al., 2005). One consistent model through administrative records, etc.), as well
that has been applied to the puzzle is social as external constraints on state legislative
contagion, where smaller parties in propor- suppression (the federal government), voter
tional systems differentiate themselves (at suppression under Reconstruction took the
lower cost than under single-seat systems) form of decentralized intimidation like lynch-
by listing more women on party lists, driv- ing that peaked just before federal restraints
ing larger parties to follow suit (Matland and on legalized suppression were relaxed, allow-
Studlar, 1996). Several outlier systems such ing Southern Democrats to re-take control of
as Israel and Malta exhibit very low percent- several state legislatures. In the mid 1890s,
ages of women in parliament despite using the inefficient and enforcement-costly strat-
rather pure versions of PR, and incorporating egy of lynching declined as the codified,
models of variance in child-rearing strategies more predictable discriminatory laws of Jim
would provide a fuller understanding of these Crow (poll taxes, registration requirements,
strategies (Lane, 1995; Rule, 1987). multiple-box voting, secret ballots, literacy
Evolutionary psychology has also contrib- tests, property tests, understanding clauses,
uted to our understanding of sex and gender grandfather clauses, and the white primary)
dynamics in campaign and election studies. came online. These electoral barriers on the
For example, a large body of research has representation and possible transmission
demonstrated that men and women differ in of less oppressive social strategies kept the
candidate support based on factors like facial Southern caste system in place for several
appearance and body image, with men pre- generations, and with it the cultural norms
ferring more attractive female candidates of segregation, revulsion at inter-racial mar-
and women preferring approachable male riage, and belief in the inherent inferiority of
candidates (Chiao et al., 2008; Dolan, 2014). African Americans.
However, electoral contexts interact with The Civil Rights Movement, and Civil
these abstract preferences in ways that shape Rights Act of 1964, Voting Rights Act of
vote choice, especially partisan signaling 1965, and accompanying court cases marked
and contextual priming emphasizing either a behavioral and legislative tipping point in
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND POLITICAL INSTITUTIONS 181

electoral reforms to expand racial represen- CONCLUSION


tation and socioeconomic opportunity in
the United States (Davidson and Grofman, This brief summary of what the evolutionary
1994). The cultural impact on norms has been approach has provided to our current under-
substantial, but complex. While Jim Crow standing of institutional design and perfor-
era norms have largely collapsed within the mance is far from exhaustive. But one final
white population, and broad support for sign of the extent of recent integration between
political equality has spread across all popu- institutionalists and biologists that must be
lations, support for government efforts to mentioned is the contributions that scholars of
address segregation and other discriminatory political institutions are making to biology.
practices is still frequently opposed (Bobo Political scientists are now researching animal
et al., 2012). Surviving racial stereotypes and institutions and collective decision making, a
negative views of racial minorities today tend sure sign that the ‘life sciences’ properly
to be based in characterizations of group cul- understood are integrating under the umbrella
ture, rather than biology (Bazian, 2016; Bobo of a generalized Darwinism (Akcay et  al.,
et  al., 2012; Kaufmann, 2019). Selective 2013; Conradt and List, 2009).
pressures, and the institutional regulation of The claim that human behavior and insti-
cooperation and competition, have altered the tutions are not reducible to biological sci-
fitness of racial norms. ences is true insofar as we narrow biology
Over the last decade, Americans have to genetic studies or similar micro-level pro-
experienced the consequences of relaxa- cesses. But political institutions are emer-
tions on federal constraints with the weak- gent phenomena, have their own laws, and
ening of the Voting Rights Act and reduced require their own sciences, just as the science
judicial oversight of discriminatory practices of complexity is not reducible to molecular
(Bentele and O’Brien, 2013; CNN, 2019; physics (Taagepera, 2008). Rather, unifica-
Keena et al., 2017). Predictably, attempts to tion posits that there be consistency between
discriminate against black voters and other micro- and macro-behavioral explanations.
groups are on the rise again, and as in the For example, models of political institutions
post-Reconstruction South, voter suppression and partisan competition should not assume
is increasingly couched in terms of partisan that agents value the welfare of others more
calculations (Hasen, 2014). While existing than their own, that men and women are
laws arguably make these new efforts, which equally prone to engage in extra-constitu-
range from gerrymandering to voter list purg- tional, violent confrontation, or that conven-
ing, voter ID laws, and proof of citizenship tions of masculinity and feminism have no
requirements, less effective than Jim Crow biological roots.
1.0, there is little reason to believe that Jim An even broader challenge, the levels
Crow 2.0 will not become more egregious of selection controversy over the relative
over time, or that efforts will not be made to importance of kin selection and reciprocity
restrict the franchise further if the Republican versus group selection, has animated all of
Party does not expand beyond its shrinking evolutionary science, and might benefit from
demographic base of support. In this event, better incorporation of institutional theory.
the importance of evolutionary psychology As already discussed, political institutions
is even more apparent, as scientists will need that mobilize, aggregate, synthesize, and
every tool at their disposal to educate and select competing policy strategies provide a
advocate for evidence-based policies to help clear Darwinian process with which to study
inoculate the public, and fight back against levels of selection, and the potential to trace
the resurgence of racism and white national- return benefits and other return effects across
ism in our electoral ecosystem. generations.
182 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

REFERENCES Balinski, M. L., & Young, H. P. (2001). Fair


representation: Meeting the ideal of one
Abbot, P. et. al. (2011). ‘Inclusive Fitness Theory man, one vote (2nd ed.). Washington, DC:
and Eusociality’, Nature, 471: E1–E4. Retrieved Brookings Institution Press.
from https://www.nature.com/articles/ Bazian, H. (2016, October 12). Police violence in
nature09831?proof=true19 America and compounded racism. Retrieved
Acemoglu, D., Egorov, G., & Sonin, K. (2009). November 2, 2016, from Hatem Bazian web-
Political selection and persistence of bad site: www.hatembazian.com/content/police-
governments (Working Paper No. 15230). violence-in-america-and-compounded-racism/
Retrieved from National Bureau of Economic Bentele, K. G., & O’Brien, E. E. (2013). Jim
Research website: www.nber.org/papers/ Crow 2.0? Why states consider and adopt
w15230 restrictive voter access policies. Perspectives
Acemoglu, D., Egorov, G., & Sonin, K. (2011). on Politics, 11(4), 1088–1116. https://doi.
Political model of social evolution. Proceed- org/10.1017/S1537592713002843
ings of the National Academy of Sciences, Binmore, K. G. (1994). Game theory and the
108(Supplement 4), 21292–21296. https:// social contract: Just playing. Cambridge: MIT
doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1019454108 Press.
Acemoglu, D., & Robinson, J. (2013). Why Bobo, L. D., Charles, C. Z., Krysan, M., & Sim-
nations fail: The origins of power, prosperity, mons, A. D. (2012). The real record on racial
and poverty (Reprint edition). New York: attitudes. Retrieved from https://dash.har-
Crown Business. vard.edu/handle/1/32197879
Akcay, E., Roughgarden, J., Fearon, J. D., Bowles, S., Choi, J.-K., & Hopfensitz, A. (2003).
Ferejohn, J. A., & Weingast, B. R. (2013). The co-evolution of individual behaviors and
Biological institutions: The political science of social institutions. Journal of Theoretical Biol-
animal cooperation (SSRN Scholarly Paper ogy, 223(2), 135–147. Retrieved from https://
No. ID 2370952). Retrieved from Social doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5193(03)00060-2
Science Research Network website: http:// Bowles, S., & Gintis, H. (2011). A cooperative
papers.ssrn.com/abstract=2370952 species: Human reciprocity and its evolution.
Aldrich, H. E., Hodgson, G. M., Hull, D. L., Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Knudsen, T., Mokyr, J., & Vanberg, V. J. Boyer, P., & Petersen, M. B. (2012). The natural-
(2008). In defence of generalized Darwinism. ness of (many) social institutions: Evolved
Journal of Evolutionary Economics, 18(5), cognition as their foundation. Journal of
577–596. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00191- Institutional Economics, 8(01), 1–25.
008-0110-z Burnham, Terence & Johnson, Dominic. (2005).
Alexander, R. (2017). The biology of moral The Biological and Evolutionary Logic of
systems. New York: Routledge. Human Cooperation. Analyse & Kritik. 27.
Alford, J., & Hibbing, J. (2004). The Origin of 113–135. 10.1515/auk-2005-0107.
Politics: An Evolutionary Theory of Political Burt, A., & Trivers, R. (2008). Genes in conflict:
Behavior. Perspectives on Politics, 2(4), 707– The biology of selfish genetic elements (1st
723. doi:10.1017/S1537592704040460 ed.). Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press.
Arrow, K. J. (1970). Social choice and individual Buss, D. M., & Malamuth, N. M. (Eds.). (1996).
values (2nd ed.). New Haven: Yale University Sex, power, conflict: Evolutionary and feminist
Press. perspectives. New York: Oxford University Press.
Axelrod, R. (2006). The evolution of cooperation Cannon, W. B. (1963). The wisdom of the body
(Revised). Basic Books, Inc., New York. (Revised and enlarged ed.). New York: W. W.
Axelrod, R., & Hamilton, W. D. (1981). The Norton & Company.
evolution of cooperation. Science, 211(4489), Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., & Feldman, M. W. (1981).
1390–1396. https://doi.org/10.1126/ Cultural transmission and evolution: A
science.7466396 quantitative approach. Princeton: Princeton
Bagehot, W. [1869](2009). Physics and politics. University Press.
CreateSpace, an Amazon Publishing Company. Chiao, J. Y., Bowman, N. E., & Gill, H. (2008).
Scotts Valley, California. The political gender gap: Gender bias in
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND POLITICAL INSTITUTIONS 183

facial inferences that predict voting behavior. Dawkins, R. (1999). The extended phenotype:
PLoS ONE, 3(10). e3666. https://doi. The long reach of the gene (Revised ed.).
org/10.1371/journal.pone.0003666 Oxford; New York: Oxford University Press.
CNN, J. B. (2019, January 21). The Supreme Dolan, K. (2014). When does gender matter?:
Court takes on MLK’s legacy. Retrieved Women candidates and gender stereotypes
August 4, 2019, from CNN website: www. in American elections. Retrieved from www.
cnn.com/2019/01/20/us/mlk-legacy- oxfordscholarship.com/view/10.1093/acpr
supreme-court/index.html of:oso/9780199968275.001.0001/acprof-
Colomer, J. M. (2001). Political institutions: 9780199968275
Democracy and social choice. Oxford Dolan, K., & Lynch, T. (2014). It takes a survey:
University Press, USA. Retrieved from www. Understanding gender stereotypes, abstract
oxfordscholarship.com/view/10.1093/01992 attitudes, and voting for women candidates.
4183X.001.0001/acprof-9780199241835 American Politics Research, 42(4), 656–676.
Colomer, J. M. (2007). On the origins of https://doi.org/10.1177/1532673X13503034
electoral systems and political parties: The Easton, D. (1965). A framework for political
role of elections in multi-member districts. analysis. Prentice-Hall, New Jersey.
Electoral Studies, 26(2), 262–273. https:// Easton, D. (1979). A systems analysis of political
doi.org/10.1016/j.electstud.2006.02.002 life. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Conradt, L., & List, C. (2009). Group decisions Ehrlich, P. R., & Levin, S. A. (2005). The
in humans and animals: A survey. Philosophi- evolution of norms. PLOS Biology, 3(6),
cal Transactions of the Royal Society of e194. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.
London B: Biological Sciences, 364(1518), 0030194
719–742. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb. Epperly, B., Witko, C., Strickler, R., & White, P.
2008.0276 (2019). Rule by violence, rule by law: Lynching,
Corning, P. A. (1995). Synergy and self- Jim Crow, and the continuing evolution of
organization in the evolution of complex voter suppression in the U.S. Perspectives on
systems. Systems Research, 12(2), 89–121. Politics, 1–14. Retrieved from https://doi.org/
https://doi.org/10.1002/sres.3850120204 10.1017/S1537592718003584
Cox, Gary W. (1997). Making votes count. Fehr E, Gächter S. Altruistic punishment in
Cambridge, UK; New York: Cambridge humans. Nature. 2002; 415(6868):137–140.
University Press. doi:10.1038/415137a
Creanza, N., & Feldman, M. W. (2014). Feminist Evolutionary Perspectives. (2019).
Complexity in models of cultural niche Feminist evolutionary perspectives society.
construction with selection and homophily. Retrieved May 22, 2020 from Feminist
Proceedings of the National Academy of Evolutionary Perspectives website: www.
Sciences, 111(Supplement 3), 10830–10837. facebook.com/groups/122502515165/
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1400824111 Ferriere, R., Michod, R. Inclusive fitness in
Dahl, R. A. (2006). A preface to democratic evolution. Nature 471, E6–E8 (2011). https://
theory (Expanded, anniversary ed.). Chicago: doi.org/10.1038/nature09834
University of Chicago Press. Fisher, R. A. [1930](2000). The genetical theory
Dahl, R. A. (1989). Democracy and its critics. of natural selection (1st ed.; J. H. Bennett, Ed.).
New Haven: Yale University Press. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Darwin, C. (1871). The descent of man, and Fontaine, P. (2016). Walking the tightrope: The
selection in relation to sex. John Murray, committee on the behavioral sciences and
London. academic cultures at the University of Chicago,
Darwin, C. [1838](2011). The autobiography of 1949–1955. Journal of the History of the
Charles Darwin. CreateSpace Independent Behavioral Sciences. 52(4). 349–370. Retrieved
Publishing Platform. from https://www.academia.edu/29432063/
Davidson, C., & Grofman, B. (1994). Quiet Walking_the_Tightrope_The_Committee_
revolution in the South: The impact of the on_the_Behavioral_Sciences_and_Academic_
Voting Rights Act, 1965–1990. Princeton: Cultures_at_the_University_of_Chicago_
Princeton University Press. 1949_1955
184 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Fox, R. L., & Lawless, J. L. (2014). Uncovering No. 10. Retrieved July 24, 2019, from https://
the origins of the gender gap in political avalon.law.yale.edu/18th_century/fed10.asp
ambition. American Political Science Review, Hamilton, W. D. (1964a). The genetical
108(3), 499–519. https://doi.org/10.1017/ evolution of social behaviour. I. Journal of
S0003055414000227 Theoretical Biology, 7(1), 1–16.
Frank, S. A. (2003). Perspective: Repression of Hamilton, W. D. (1964b). The genetical
competition and the evolution of evolution of social behaviour. II. Journal of
cooperation. Evolution; International Journal Theoretical Biology, 7(1), 17–52.
of Organic Evolution, 57(4), 693–705. Hardin, G. (1968). The tragedy of the commons.
Frank, S. A. (2011). “Evolutionary foundations Science, 162(3859), 1243–1248. https://doi.
of cooperation and group cohesion.” in org/10.1126/science.162.3859.1243
Simon Levin (Ed.). In Games, Groups and the Hasen, R. (2014, January 7). ‘Race or party?:
Global Good. Springer-Verlag Berlin How courts should think about Republican
Heidelberg Retrieved from http://arxiv.org/ efforts to make it harder to vote in North
abs/1112.3046 Carolina and elsewhere’. Retrieved November
Frank, S. A. (2013). “Introduction: A new 29, 2014, from Harvard Law Review website:
theory of cooperation.” In K. Summers & B. http://harvardlawreview.org/2014/01/race-or-
Crespi (Eds.), Human Social Behavior: The party-how-courts-should-think-about-republican-
Foundational Works of Richard D. Alexander. efforts-to-make-it-harder-to-vote-in-north-
Oxford University Press, USA. carolina-and-elsewhere/
Freedom in the World. (2014). Retrieved July Hastie, R., & Kameda, T. (2005). The robust
16, 2014, from Freedom House website: beauty of majority rules in group decisions.
www.freedomhouse.org/report/freedom-w Psychological Review, 112(2), 494–508. https://
orld-aggregate-and-subcategory-scores#. doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.112.2.494
U8bXglZfTQs Henrich, J., & Boyd, R. (1998). The evolution of
Frischmann, B. M., Madison, M. J., & conformist transmission and the emergence
Strandburg, K. J. (2014). Governing of between-group differences. Evolution and
knowledge commons. Oxford University Human Behavior, 19(4), 215–241. https://
Press. doi.org/10.1016/S1090-5138(98)00018-X
Fürstenberg, D. K. (2016). Evolutionary Henrich, J., Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (2012). The
institutionalism. Politics and the Life Sciences: puzzle of monogamous marriage. Philosophical
The Journal of the Association for Politics Transactions of the Royal Society of London B:
and the Life Sciences, 35(1), 48–60. Biological Sciences, 367(1589), 657–669.
Grabo, A., & van Vugt, M. (2018). Voting for a https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2011.0290
male warrior or female peacekeeper? Testing Herre, E., and Wcislo, W. (2011). In defence of
the evolutionary contingency hypothesis in the inclusive fitness theory. Nature 471, E8–E9.
2016 U.S. presidential elections. Evolutionary https://doi.org/10.1038/nature09835
Psychology, 16(2), 1474704918773267. Hobbes, Thomas. (1651;2009). Leviathan: Or
https://doi.org/10.1177/1474704918773267 the Matter, Form and Power of a
Gunnell, J. G. (2004). Imagining the American Commonwealth, Ecclesiastical and Civil.
polity: Political science and the discourse of Gutenberg Project. https://www.gutenberg.
democracy. University Park: Pennsylvania org/files/3207/3207-h/3207-h.htm
State University Press. Hodgson, G. M. (2004). The evolution of
Haldane, J. B. S. (1932). The causes of evolution. institutional economics: Agency, Structure
Retrieved July 26, 2019, from Princeton and Darwinism in American Institutionalism.
University Press website: https://press. London and New York: Routledge https://
princeton.edu/titles/4618.html doi.org/10.4324/9780203300350
Haldane, J. B. S. (1941). Concerning social Hout, E. van der, & McGann, A. J. (2009).
Darwinism. Science & Society, 5(4), 373– Liberal political equality implies proportional
375. Retrieved from JSTOR. representation. Social Choice and Welfare,
Hamilton, A., Madison, J., & Jay, J. [1788] 33(4), 617–627. https://doi.org/10.1007/
(1998, December 29). The federalist papers s00355-009-0382-8
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND POLITICAL INSTITUTIONS 185

Howard, R. S., & Lively, C. M. (1994). Parasitism, Sciences, 111(Supplement 3), 10838–10845.
mutation accumulation and the maintenance https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1400830111
of sex. Nature, 367(6463), 554–557. https:// Lopez, A. (2017). Does Conflict Drive
doi.org/10.1038/367554a0 Cooperation? The Evolution Institute. Retrieved
Kaufmann, E. (2019, March 18). Americans are May 20th, 2020 https://evolution-institute.org/
divided by their views on race, not race itself. cooperation-was-important-in-human-
The New York Times. Retrieved May 22, evolution/
2020 from www.nytimes.com/2019/03/18/ Madison, J. (1786). Equality: James Madison to
opinion/race-america-trump.html Thomas Jefferson. Retrieved December 1,
Keena, A., McGann, A., & Smith, C. A. (2017, 2017, from http://press-pubs.uchicago.edu/
October 25). The Supreme Court’s quiet founders/print_documents/v1ch15s33.html
gerrymandering revolution and the road to Madison, J., Hamilton, A., & Jay, J. [1788]
minority rule. Retrieved July 26, 2018, from (2008). The Federalist papers. CreateSpace
USAPP website: http://blogs.lse.ac.uk/ Independent Publishing Platform.
usappblog/2017/10/25/the-supreme-courts- Malthus, T. [1798](2013). An essay on the
quiet-gerrymandering-revolution-and-the- principle of population. J. Johnson, London.
road-to-minority-rule/ March, J. G. (1991). Exploration and exploitation
Kropotkin, P. (2017). Mutual aid: A factor of in organizational learning. Organization
evolution (J. Duran, Ed.). CreateSpace Science, 2(1), 71–87. https://doi.org/10.1287/
Independent Publishing Platform. orsc.2.1.71
Kropotkin, P. A., Huxley, T., & Montagu, A. March, J. G., & Olsen, J. P. (1976). Ambiguity and
(1955). Mutual aid and the struggle for choice in organizations. Universitetsforlaget.
existence. Extending Horizons Books. Boston, Oslo, Norway.
Massachusetts. Matland, R. E., & Studlar, D. T. (1996). The
Laland, K. N., Odling-Smee, F. J., & Feldman, contagion of women candidates in single-
M. W. (1999). Evolutionary consequences of member district and proportional
niche construction and their implications for representation electoral systems: Canada
ecology. Proceedings of the National and Norway. The Journal of Politics, 58(3),
Academy of Sciences of the United States of 707–733. https://doi.org/10.2307/2960439
America, 96(18), 10242–10247. McCoy, D. R. (1980). Jefferson and Madison on
Lane, J. C. (1995). The election of women under Malthus: Population growth in Jeffersonian
proportional representation: The case of political economy. The Virginia Magazine of
Malta. Democratization, 2(2), 140–157. History and Biography, 88(3), 259–276.
https://doi.org/10.1080/13510349508403433 McGann, A., & Latner, M. (2013). The calculus
Latner, M. (2017). Darwinian democracy? How of consensus democracy: Rethinking patterns
evolutionary theory informs constitutional of democracy without veto players.
design. Handbook of Biology and Politics. Comparative Political Studies, 46(7), 823–
Retrieved from www.elgaronline.com 850. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1177/
/view/edcoll/9781783476268/97817834762 0010414012463883
68.00037.xml Mclean, I. (2005). Before and after Publius: The
Leigh, E. G. (1971). Adaptation and diversity: sources and influences of Madison’s political
Natural history and the mathematics of thought. In S. Kernell (Ed.), James Madison:
evolution. Freeman, Cooper. San Francisco. The Theory and Practice of Republican
Levi, M. (1989). Of rule and revenue. California: Government. Stanford University Press.
University of California Press. Redwood City, California.
Levi, M., & Weingast, B. R. (2019). Douglass Miller, James G. (1953). Profits and Problems of
North’s theory of politics. PS: Political Homeostatic Models in the Behavioral
Science & Politics, 52(2), 213–217. https:// Sciences – Introduction. Chicago Behavioral
doi.org/10.1017/S1049096518002111 Sciences, University of Michigan Biography
Levin, S. A. (2014). Public goods in relation to of Publications.
competition, cooperation, and spite. Murray, G. (2012, March 4). Are you easily
Proceedings of the National Academy of disgusted? You may be a Conservative.
186 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Retrieved July 26, 2019, from Psychology to parliament in twenty-three democracies.


Today website: www.psychologytoday.com/ The Western Political Quarterly, 40(3), 477–
blog/caveman-politics/201203/are-you-easily- 498. https://doi.org/10.2307/448386
disgusted-you-may-be-conservative Santa Fe Institute. (2016, November 9). lawOS:
Nowak, M. A. (2006). Five rules for the Regulations as society’s operating system.
evolution of cooperation. Science, 314(5805), Retrieved August 2, 2019, from Santa Fe
1560–1563. https://doi.org/10.1126/ Institute website: www.santafe.edu/news-
science.1133755 center/news/lawos-regulations-societys-
Nowak, M., Tarnita, C., & Wilson, E. (2010). The operating-system
evolution of eusociality. Nature 466, 1057– Schofield, N. (2005). The intellectual
1062. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature09205 contribution of Condorcet to the founding
Olson, M. (1971). The logic of collective action: of the US Republic 1785–1800. Social Choice
Public goods and the theory of groups, and Welfare, 25(2–3), 303–318. https://doi.
Second printing with a new preface and org/10.1007/s00355-005-0005-y
appendix (Revised ed.). Cambridge: Harvard Shugart, M., & Wattenberg, M. P. (2001).
University Press. Mixed-member electoral systems: The best
Ostrom, E. (1990). Governing the commons: of both worlds? Oxford University Press.
The evolution of institutions for collective Oxford.
action (1st ed.). Cambridge; New York: Skyrms, B. (1994). Sex and justice. Journal of
Cambridge University Press. Philosophy, 91(6), 305–320.
Ostrom, E. (2010). Beyond markets and states: Skyrms, B. (2014). Evolution of the social
Polycentric governance of complex economic contract. Cambridge University Press. New
systems. American Economic Review, 100(3), York.
641–672. https://doi.org/10.1257/aer. Smith, J. M., & Price, G. R. (1973). The logic of
100.3.641 animal conflict. Nature, 246(5427), 15.
Ostrom, E. (2013). Do institutions for collective https://doi.org/10.1038/246015a0
action evolve? Journal of Bioeconomics, Sorkin, R. D., West, R., & Robinson, D. E.
16(1), 3–30. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10818- (1998). Group performance depends on the
013-9154-8 majority rule. Psychological Science, 9(6),
Palmer, Tom G. (2002). “Madison and 456–463. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-
Multiculturalism: Group Representation, 9280.00085
Group Rights and Constitutionalism”. In Soudriette, R., & Ellis, A. (2006). A global
John Samples (ed.), James Madison and the snapshot. Journal of Democracy, 17(2), 78–88.
Future of Limited Government, Cato Institute. https://doi.org/10.1353/jod.2006.0038
Page, S. E. (2008). The difference: How the Taagepera, R. (2007). Predicting party sizes:
power of diversity creates better groups, The logic of simple electoral systems (1st ed.).
firms, schools, and societies (New edition Oxford: Oxford University Press.
with a new preface by the author ed.). Taagepera, R. (2008). Making social sciences
Princeton: Princeton University Press. more scientific: The need for predictive
Peress, M. (2019). Measuring the research models. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
productivity of political science departments The cooperative human. (2018) Nat Hum
using Google Scholar. PS: Political Science & Behav 2, 427–428. Retrieved from https://
Politics, 52(2), 312–317. https://doi. doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0389-1
org/10.1017/S1049096518001610 Trivers, R. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal
Rawls, J. (1999). A theory of justice (2nd ed.). altruism. Quarterly Review of Biology, 46(1).
Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press. 35–57.
Reynolds, A., Reilly, B., & Ellis, A. (Eds.). (2005). Trivers, R. (2006). Reciprocal altruism: 30 years
Electoral system design: The new later. In P. M. Kappeler & C. P. van Schaik
international IDEA Handbook. Stockholm: (Eds.), Cooperation in Primates and Humans:
International IDEA. Mechanisms and Evolution (pp. 67–83).
Rule, W. (1987). Electoral systems, contextual Springer. New York. https://doi.org/10.1007/
factors and women’s opportunity for election 3-540-28277-7_4
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND POLITICAL INSTITUTIONS 187

Turchin, P. (2013, February 8). The double helix of Wilson, D. S., & Gowdy, J. M. (2013). Evolution as
inequality and well-being. Retrieved August 3, a general theoretical framework for economics
2019, from Peter Turchin website: http:// and public policy. Journal of Economic Behavior
peterturchin.com/cliodynamica/the-double- & Organization, 90, S3–S10. https://doi.org/
helix-of-inequality-and-well-being/ 10.1016/j.jebo.2012.12.008
Turchin, P. (2016). Ages of discord: A structural- Young, H. P. (1988). Condorcet’s theory of
demographic analysis of American history. voting. American Political Science Review,
Chaplin: Beresta Books. 82(4), 1231–1244. https://doi.org/10.2307/
Walker, S. I., & Davies, P. C. W. (2012). The 1961757
algorithmic origins of life. Journal of The Zhou, Y. M. (2011). Synergy, coordination
Royal Society Interface, 10(79), 20120869– costs, and diversification choices. Strategic
20120869. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsif. Management Journal, 32(6), 624–639.
2012.0869 Retrieved from JSTOR.
9
Evolutionary Psychology
and Crime
Joseph L. Nedelec

INTRODUCTION empirical analyses place the etiological respon-


sibility for antisocial behavior solely within the
As examined throughout this volume, humans realm of social factors. Recently, however,
are a highly social species and as such possess biosocial criminologists have illustrated the
a constellation of adaptations which aid in sur- shortcomings of such an approach. The current
vival and reproduction. Among the evolved chapter overviews the various ways in which an
strategies employed by highly social species, evolutionary viewpoint can inform our under-
those which can be viewed as exploitative are standing of antisocial behavior, crime, and
abundant. Indeed, nature is awash with exam- criminality. Increasingly referred to as evolu-
ples of evolved strategies that appear to cal- tionary criminology, the perspective described
lously infringe on the desires or choices of in this chapter illustrates how viewing antiso-
individuals who are the targets of such strate- cial behavior from an evolutionary standpoint
gies. To be sure, highly social species rely on can explain the most well-established observa-
cooperation, empathy, and stable group dynam- tions regarding criminality as well as contextu-
ics, but all these factors can also be exploited alize more recent empirical findings derived
for individual gain as an effective evolved from neuropsychology, behavioral genetics,
strategy. When viewed from an evolutionary and biosocial criminology.
perspective, criminal behavior among humans
clearly falls into an exploitative-strategy cate-
gory. However, the field of social science UNDERSTANDING AND USING
devoted to the study of criminal and antisocial EVOLUTIONARY THEORY
behavior, criminology, rarely examines behav-
ior using an evolutionary lens. Instead, almost Although covered elsewhere in this volume, it
all traditional criminological theories and is necessary at the outset of our discussion to
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND CRIME 189

address why many people within social-­ have made, along with the wide array of
science disciplines struggle with recognizing religious beliefs that have and continue to
­
the relevance of an evolutionary point of view. profess human exceptionalism, it is not sur-
Perhaps the two most important concepts prising that many feel humans are separate
related to evolutionary thinking that can help from the natural world – and, therefore,
in this regard are deep time and recognizing immune to the processes of natural and sexual
that humans are not immune to the processes selection that have shaped all life. Acceptance
of nature. of human’s place in nature is, and has been
Deep time is a geological concept that since well before Darwin, a controversial
informed Charles Darwin in generating his issue. Nonetheless, the evidence that humans
theory of evolution by natural selection. are a part of nature is as overwhelming as the
Briefly, the concept refers to the immense evidence for deep time. Empirical facts such
amount of time that has passed since the for- as homology (shared structures across differ-
mation of the earth and the amount of time ent species or taxa), genetic code illustrating
that has been available for natural processes relatedness across animals and plants, and
such as erosion, movement of the earth’s the considerable fossil record, among other
crust, and most germane to the current chap- empirical facts, all point to an inevitable
ter, evolution of biological organisms. Given conclusion: humans are not exceptional in
that humans typically live for only a handful terms of our place in nature and are the result
of decades, our perceptions of the passage of natural processes (i.e., natural and sexual
of time are drastically limited compared to selection) in the same way as all life on earth.
the age of the earth. Consequently, it is dif- Once this fact is acknowledged, and in com-
ficult for most people to understand how the bination with the concept of deep time, it is
complexities we observe in nature and our thus necessary that assessments of the causes
own behaviors could result from the pro- of almost any aspect of the human condition
cesses of evolution. Other factors such as must incorporate recognition of the evolution-
religious dogma and cultural characteristics ary processes that underpin the human condi-
also impinge on people’s ability to recog- tion. Such a conclusion was emphasized by
nize the immensity of time that has passed Pierre Teilhard de Chardin who poignantly
and the vast opportunities that have been pro- noted, “[evolution] is a general postulate to
vided for the processes of natural and sexual which all theories, all hypotheses, all sys-
selection to shape the evolution of species. tems must henceforward bow and which they
When one recognizes and accepts the over- must satisfy in order to be thinkable and true.
whelming evidence of deep time, however, it Evolution is a light which illuminates all facts,
becomes easier to see how natural processes a trajectory which all lines of thought must
could apply to all aspects of species’ lives, follow”. (As cited in Dobzhansky, 1973: 129.)
including behavioral traits such as crime and All this information inexorably leads to the
antisocial conduct. perspective that informs the chapters within
Given the time frames associated with this volume. Briefly, evolutionary psychol-
the typical human life course, deep time is ogy argues that our brain is the seat of all
often cognitively taxing. However, observ- human behavior and the construction and
ing natural wonders like the Grand Canyon functioning of our brain is driven, in part,
provides an opportunity to witness the results by genetic factors; these genetic factors, in
of deep time. Recognizing that humans are a turn, have been susceptible to the processes
part of nature, however, is often less widely of natural and sexual selection over the vast
accepted. Given the complexities and varie- eons of evolutionary time. Consequently,
ties of human culture and the incredible tech- all aspects of the human condition, includ-
nological and societal advances that humans ing human behavior, society, and culture,
190 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

can be assessed using an evolutionary lens. in criminal or antisocial behaviors. Thus,


Given that criminal and antisocial behaviors when biosocial criminologists examine crimi-
are an empirical universal across all recorded nal or antisocial behaviors using an evolution-
history and across every known society, it is ary lens, it is with the concept of criminality
clear that an evolutionary lens can also illu- in mind; in other words, the focus is on the
minate why such behaviors appear to be an behavior and not necessarily the legality of
integral, though often unfortunate, aspect of the behavior. As an example, criminologists
the human condition. examine not only antisocial behaviors that are
illegal but also behaviors that are considered
analogous to criminal behavior such as sub-
stance abuse or risky sexual behaviors and the
OF CRIME, CRIMINALITY, AND lifestyles associated or congruent with engag-
ANTISOCIAL BEHAVIOR ing in antisocial conduct. Thus, recognizing
that antisocial behaviors are acts which vio-
A common reaction in the social sciences to late the interests of one party to the benefit of
the suggestion of the utility of an evolution- another party in contravention of normative
ary perspective is that, given behaviors con- behaviors of the group to which the parties
sidered to be criminal are based on codified belong allows for a more nuanced examina-
laws, crime is a social construct that will vary tion of the etiology of antisocial behavior than
from society to society. Thus, a biologically simply relying on legal definitions. Thinking
based perspective such as evolutionary psy- in this way (i.e., focus on the behavior –
chology cannot inform the discussion of criminality) provides an opportunity to apply
crime. The argument, though partially correct an evolutionary lens. In the sections that
(codified prohibitions on certain behaviors follow, I outline how evolutionary psychology
certainly do change both within and between as a paradigm can help explain three of crimi-
societies over time), is incomplete in at least nology’s most commonly observed empirical
two ways. First, across all recorded history patterns in terms of antisocial behavior: the
and known societies there is considerable gender gap in crime, the age-graded distribu-
overlap in terms of prohibited behaviors. Few tion of crime, and the non-random distribu-
cultures accept behaviors such as thievery, tion of criminal behavior.
murder, assault, or rape (this list is not
exhaustive) without any social rebuke. Even
within highly violent cultures where the
murder of one group’s rivals is seen as a nec- THE GENDER GAP IN CRIME
essary step in the maturity of males, such
behavior is condoned only if it is directed at Imagine, if you will, a risk factor associated
an out-group. Consequently, there appears to with criminality and antisocial behavior that
be a human constant against the impingement is so pervasive that it replicates across all
of the rights and desires of others in terms of known societies and all recorded history.
these behaviors (at least in terms of one’s That risk factor is actually well-known, and
own in-group). Second, while it is certainly in our species, it is represented by a lone
the case that codified prohibitions (i.e., legal chromosome: the Y-chromosome. Sex is the
definitions of what constitutes crime) are single most consistent predictor of criminal
fluid across time and space, the behavioral behavior that has emerged from well over a
proclivities that underlie criminal or antiso- century of criminological theorizing and
cial behavior are consistent. These proclivi- empirical work. To be sure, not all men
ties are referred to, in general, as criminality, engage in criminal behavior and not all
which is a propensity or inclination to engage women refrain from it. However, in the
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND CRIME 191

statistical sense (i.e., average differences), differs considerably. While females in most
there has not existed a society wherein women primate species have a very high MPI (many
engaged in a higher rate of criminal behavior months of gestation and often many years of
than men, and this is particularly the case with offspring care), males have a relatively low
violent antisocial behaviors. The robust MPI (perhaps as minimal as a few moments
observed differences in terms of criminality of copulation). Consequently, males can
between the sexes has been addressed in a reproduce at much higher rates and with
variety of ways in the criminological literature shorter intervals than females. While males
with almost all explanations focusing on soci- have a much higher reproductive ceiling, they
ological factors (e.g., differential parental also evince much more variance in terms of
socialization, cultural factors, differential reproductive success – the proportion of
media exposure, etc.). In each case, these males who never reproduce is much higher
explanations have proven to be at best incom- than in females. Additionally, given that
plete and at worst incorrect. A potential reason females risk a much higher MPI, they exhibit
for the ineffective explanations is the lack of mating strategies that lead to what is gener-
recognition that humans are part of the natural ally referred to as choosiness (females assess
world. Here we see that an evolutionary per- males on their reproductive quality to a much
spective can illuminate potential causal factors higher degree than males assess females).
for the observed differences in antisocial The result of this difference in MPI is that
behaviors between men and women. males tend to differentially invest in mating
When one recognizes that humans are a effort while females tend to invest relatively
part of nature, one can then look to nature more in parenting effort. Along with this dif-
for potential analogues of the behavior or ferential allocation of resources comes a suite
dynamic of interest. Additionally, an evolu- of behavioral strategies which affect the level
tionary perspective allows for the applica- and degree of competition both within and
tion of well-known biological theories or between sexes.
processes. For example, biological explana- Differential allocation of reproductive
tions of the differences in behavioral reper- resources driven by MPI and within-sex
toires between the sexes within many species variances in reproductive success is associ-
are often informed by discussions of invest- ated with more intense within-sex competi-
ment (Trivers, 1972). Briefly, any given tion in the sex with the lower MPI. In less
organism within a sexually reproducing spe- technical language, the sex with more to lose
cies can allot time and energy to obtaining (i.e., greater reproductive variance – if you
mates (mating effort) or caring for offspring don’t reproduce, you’re a genetic dead end)
(parental effort or investment). Bioenergetic is the sex that will engage in more intense
resources cannot be allotted to both mating (i.e., risky, violent, combative) competition
and parenting simultaneously and, as we relative to the sex with higher MPI. Thus, the
shall see, the evolved strategies of sexes with biological concept of MPI provides the logic
regard to investment of resources are often for understanding the wide range of mor-
divergent. Throughout the animal kingdom, phological and behavioral traits that males
the primary driver of differentiation in bio- possess, relative to females, which appear
energetic resources is referred to as minimal specifically designed for intense competition.
parental investment (MPI). MPI refers to the The same morphological and behavioral traits
minimal time and energy costs associated related to within-sex competition (i.e., fight-
with producing a viable offspring (i.e., one ing rivals for access to the high MPI sex) are
that can live long enough to then also repro- also employed for between-sex competition
duce). In most primate species (human and (e.g., subduing the ability of the high MPI sex
non-human), the MPI for males and females to choose among mates).
192 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

While in most primate species males com- (particularly those which are considered
pete in a more intense fashion, there is noth- interpersonal and violent). Hence, one of the
ing about maleness per se that inevitably most robust empirical findings of crimino-
leads to increased violence and aggression; logical research: the gender gap in crime.1
likewise, there is nothing about femaleness
per se that inevitably leads to decreased
violence and aggression in within- and
between-sex competition. To illustrate this AGE-GRADED DISTRIBUTION OF
argument, one can again observe nature CRIME
for instances wherein the sexes converge in
terms of MPI (i.e., greater amount of shared In addition to the gender gap in crime,
parental efforts, aka biparental care). Many researchers of human antisocial behavior
species exhibit such patterns (e.g., wolves, have long observed that criminal activity, in
numerous types of birds, beavers, some mon- general, tends to rise with the onset of ado-
keys, among others), and the morphological lescence, peak near the end of adolescence,
and behavioral differences between males and then abruptly plummet in young adult-
and females are greatly reduced. Perhaps hood. This empirical pattern has been dubbed
the greatest evidence for the importance of the age-crime curve and, like the gender gap
MPI can be derived from species where the in crime, has appeared relatively consistent
males invest more resources to parental effort across time and space.2 Numerous scholars
relative to females and females have greater have put forth theoretical explanations for
reproductive variance relative to males. This this patterned empirical observation, the
occurs in varieties of fish and other animals most well-known among criminologists
(though no mammals) and what is observed being Laub and Sampson’s (1993) age-
is a role reversal – relative to most primates – graded theory of crime and Moffitt’s (1993)
in terms of behavioral strategies: the females dual taxonomic theory of crime.
compete with more aggression and the males Briefly, Laub and Sampson’s (1993) theory
are generally choosier in terms of selecting asserts that informal social control through
mates. an individual’s bond to society affects the
Armed with an evolutionary explanation of propensity to engage in criminal behav-
sex differences in terms of aggressive behav- ior over the life course. The theory places
ior, one can then see why men and women great weight on the bonds within an indi-
in our own species have exhibited and still vidual’s family during development as well
do exhibit differences in terms of criminal- as attachment to school, employment, and
ity or a propensity to engage in antisocial other structural aspects of society. Overall,
behaviors. Given that men possess a much Laub and Sampson argue that those youth
lower MPI than women, it is men who pos- with weak social bonds are more likely to
sess (on average) greater muscle mass, engage in antisocial behaviors during adoles-
height, and other morphological traits con- cence and that such behavior in turn leads to
ducive to intense competition. Additionally, an increased likelihood of criminal behavior
it is also the reason why men possess a in adulthood. Further, they argue that with-
suite of psychological traits that drive risky out social bonding or attachment to institu-
and aggressive behaviors more often and in tions such as employment, military service,
typically more intense degrees than women. and marriage, continued criminal behavior
Consequently, in possession of the psycho- during adulthood is probable. Finally, they
logical drive and morphological capaci- argue that desistence from criminal behavior
ties to aggressively compete, men are more is largely due to obtaining attachment to one
often the sex to engage in criminal behaviors or more of these social institutions.
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND CRIME 193

Moffitt’s (1993) dual taxonomic theory also adult) and the social limitations that remain
recognizes the importance of a life-course in place in modern societies (adolescence
approach and contains a multitude of hypothe- is a period of relaxed limitations relative
ses regarding the shape of the age-crime curve. to childhood, but it still consists of wide-­
In brief, Moffitt argued that antisocial behav- ranging restrictions on a variety of aspects of
ioral patterns illustrate two distinct groups of life). The difference between one’s biologi-
offenders: adolescent-limited offenders (AL) cal maturity and one’s perceptions of social
and life-course persistent offenders (LCP).3 freedom/limitation was termed the maturity
As suggested by the label, those exhibiting gap. Moffitt argued that the greater the expe-
AL patterns of offending engage in crimi- rienced maturity gap, the greater the frustra-
nal behavior that is limited to the period of tion experienced by the youth and therefore
adolescence, that was not preceded by anti- the greater the need to address the result-
social behavior in childhood, and does not ing frustration. The second component of
tend to continue into adulthood. Additionally, Moffitt’s explanation of AL offending pro-
the type of offending in which AL offend- vides the mechanism through which youth
ers engage is typically relatively minor and were said to then deal with the frustration
rarely results in serious contact with the resulting from an experienced maturity gap.
criminal justice system (i.e., long-term insti- Her argument indicated that AL individuals
tutionalization). LCP offenders, however, could recognize/observe the relative social
exhibit behavioral patterns illustrating a life- freedom exhibited by those who are engaged
time of antisocial behavior from childhood in LCP offending and lifestyle patterns.
through adulthood. Additionally, relative to Observing the socially uninhibited lifestyle
AL offenders, the type of criminal activity in of LCP offenders could then lead to a process
which LCP offenders engage is often severe of behavioral mimicry in order to gain simi-
and does typically lead to serious and con- lar social freedoms (termed social mimicry).
sistent interaction with the criminal justice Moffitt thus argued that AL offending was a
system (throughout the life course). result of experiencing a maturity gap during
In terms of etiological factors, Moffitt adolescence and engaging in social mimicry
(1993) argued that the behavioral pattern of such that the behavioral patterns of LCP
LCP offenders is a result of an unfortunate offenders are followed (although to a less
mix of neuropsychological deficits and del- serious degree) by non-LCP youth (i.e., AL
eterious rearing environments. She argued offenders). Further, Moffitt argued that the
that LCP offending patterns are so serious desistance from criminal behavior observed
in nature and consistency that exceedingly during early adulthood by AL offenders was
damaging circumstances such as abnormal a result of the reduced effect of the matu-
neuropsychological functioning and abusive rity gap (i.e., the social limitations placed
or otherwise damaging developmental envi- on adolescents become much less intense or
ronments were required. However, given that pervasive as they age into early adulthood) –
Moffitt argued that AL offending was norma- thus, with the accumulation of greater social
tive (i.e., an expected pattern of development freedoms throughout many aspects of their
in modern societies), her explanation required lives, those in the AL offending group no
normative processes typically experienced longer experience the frustrations associated
by most youth. Her hypotheses regarding AL with social limitations which eliminates the
offending centered on two components. The need to mimic the behavioral patterns of LCP
first component is represented by the differ- offenders.
ence between the biological maturity experi- Both the age-graded and dual taxonomic
enced by youth in adolescence (after puberty theories of crime have received substantial
an individual is, more or less, biologically an attention in the criminological literature and
194 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

researchers continue to test the hypotheses reproductive ­fitness. In this zero-sum social
derived from both theories. For the purposes contest, higher-ranking men tend to obtain
of this chapter, however, it is important to more mates and increase their reproductive fit-
note that both theories are proximal theo- ness, whereas lower-ranking men have fewer
ries of criminal behavior (as exhibited in or no mates. Given that social rank is a vital
the age-crime curve). As discussed through- component to reproductive fitness for men,
out this volume, proximal explanations of there is a heightened psychological awareness
behavior refer to those arguments that focus among men to threats to status or reputation
on factors related primarily to ontogeny (i.e., (often referred to as honor). Threats to social
development within an individual’s lifespan), status or reputation are, in essence, threats to
whereas evolutionary arguments provide ulti- social rank and, thus, threats to reproductive
mate explanations of behavior, which focus potential. Consequently, intense, aggressive,
on phylogeny (development over genera- and often violent competition to maintain or
tions rather than within a single lifespan) and advance rank can occur when such threats
adaptive function (i.e., what adaptive prob- arise. Further, within a polygymous breed-
lem is addressed by the behavior in ques- ing system wherein some males reproduce
tion?). For example, while it may be the case much more than most males there are numer-
that informal social bonds affect crime over ous reproductive benefits to intense (aggres-
the life course or that the frustration associ- sive, risky) competition. Such behavior can
ated with experiencing the maturity gap leads serve to protect or gain status, discourage or
to increased antisocial behavior in adoles- eliminate rival males in a competitive breed-
cence, we are still left with the question as ing environment, allow for the acquisition of
to why these processes have the (potential) resources to be used to woo females, engage
effect that is purported. Why should social in and protect from mate poaching, and pro-
bonds experienced during development and tect already acquired resources and mates
in adulthood affect behavioral patterns? Why (including aggressive mate guarding).
should biological maturity combined with Wilson and Daly (1985) provide empirical
limitations on social freedoms lead to frus- evidence supporting these assertions based
tration among youth? Why would aggressive on data from over 500 homicide cases in
or violent behavior be something in which Detroit in the early 1970s. They illustrate that
youth engage to deal with the frustration? not only were the majority of both offend-
Fortunately, an evolutionary perspective can ers and victims in homicide cases men, but
provide the ultimate reasons/answers to such offenders and victims were almost identical
questions. in terms of being unemployed, unmarried,
As with the discussion of the gender gap and younger (teen years to mid 20s). Analysis
in crime, an evolutionary explanation of the of the cases also indicated that the most com-
age-crime curve centers on variance in repro- mon type of homicide was the result of social
ductive effort between men and women. A conflict or what criminologists would refer to
few examples of evolutionary explanations as the escalation of a trivial altercation (the
for the age-crime curve exist in the literature majority of which were primarily in retalia-
(e.g., Quinsey et al., 2004) but they all center tion for a previous loss of face in the pres-
on what Wilson and Daly (1985) termed the ence of social peers). Summarizing their
young male syndrome. Briefly, Wilson and observations regarding lethal conflict, they
Daly argue that given the immense costs of note, ‘many, perhaps most, homicides con-
reproductive failure (i.e., genetic dead end) cern status competition’ (Wilson and Daly,
and the substantial variance in reproductive 1985: 59). Wilson and Daly also illustrated
fitness among men, there will be intense com- that other behaviors that carry a substantive
petition for any resources that help increase threat of physical harm (e.g., risky and/or
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND CRIME 195

aggressive driving) are likely the result of a wherein males engage in intense competition
generalized willingness to engage in compet- in order to secure reproductive success, but
itive risk taking relevant to social rank and, once this is secured tend to disengage from
thus, linked to male fitness. The observations risky behavioral strategies given the potential
and hypotheses put forth by Wilson and Daly costs to the obtained reproductive success.
have been further supported using data out- The ultimate explanations put forth by
side of Detroit and for different time periods Wilson and Daly (1985) and Kanazawa
(see Daly, 2016; Daly and Wilson, 2017). and Still (2000) are supported by proximal
Although Wilson and Daly (1985) outline explanations derived from psychophysi-
why engagement in criminal conduct and ological and criminological research. For
other risky behaviors increases during the example, the hormone testosterone – which
adolescent years, their discussion does not has been associated primarily with competi-
explicitly focus on the entirety of the age- tive behaviors – increases dramatically in
crime curve. In their theoretical piece on male males during puberty but has been observed
criminality over the life course, Kanazawa to drop substantially at the onset of marriage
and Still (2000) echo Wilson and Daly’s and the arrival of offspring (Beaver, 2009).
young male syndrome hypothesis but also Additionally, some criminological research
directly address the shape of the age-crime has illustrated a calming effect in males (in
curve. Overall, Kanazawa and Still indicate terms of engagement in criminal behavior)
that intense competition (manifested as anti- who begin families, though this research is
social, aggressive, and other risky behaviors) often confounded by issues of temporal order
occurs when the reproductive benefits of such and a lack of genetically informed analyses
behavior are maximized over the life course. (Barnes et al., 2014).
Thus, violent competition among males does Finally, we see that these evolutionary
not occur earlier in life (i.e., prior to puberty) explanations of the age-crime curve pro-
primarily because there are no reproductive vide some potential answers to our earlier
benefits to engaging in violence, theft, or questions derived from the discussion of the
murder – the pre-pubescent male is unable to age-graded and dual taxonomic theories of
translate a competitive edge into reproductive crime. For example, informal social bonds
success. However, after puberty the repro- such as employment, military service, and
ductive benefits sky-rocket and so too does marriage could affect male criminality as these
the resulting behavior (thus, the peak in ado- are resources which directly affect or relate
lescence of the age-crime curve). As noted to reproductive success. Given that crimi-
above, however, the age-crime curve drops nal behavior, especially violent or aggressive
precipitously in early adulthood. Kanazawa behavior, represents a threat to these resources
and Still argued that the ultimate reason there is a corresponding reduction in the level
for this drop is associated with the costs of of criminal behavior (on average) that is con-
continued intense competition. The authors gruent with the acquisition of these resources.
argue that risky strategies are employed to Additionally, experiencing the maturity gap
gain sexual access to mates and, in general, results in increased frustration in youth because
secure reproductive success (or at least the their biological maturity – driven by eons of
opportunity for reproductive success) and at evolutionary processes – motivates them to
the arrival of offspring (i.e., reproductive suc- engage in intra- and inter-sexual competition
cess) less risky behavior is employed to mini- to obtain mates. However, the wide-ranging
mize the costs to the reproductive success social limitations placed on adolescents pre-
of the male. Thus, the age-crime curve (for vent or at least minimize opportunities for such
males) is a manifestation of a two-pronged behaviors. Thus, social rebellion through minor
strategy selected over evolutionary time delinquency could result (not only to rebel
196 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

against the social restraints but to attempt to non-random distribution of criminal conduct
secure status and rank within a peer network). although the pattern of the conduct typically
Overall, the discussion illustrates what all evo- depends upon the specific type of crime. The
lutionary psychologists argue: both proximal remaining discussion within this section will
and ultimate explanations are required in order illustrate how an evolutionary perspective can
to fully understand observed behavioral pat- help explain these non-random patterns.
terns such as the age-crime curve.

Non-Random Clustering of
Criminality and Other Risky
NON-RANDOM DISTRIBUTION OF
Behaviors in Locales
CRIMINAL BEHAVIOR
As noted, criminologists have observed that
The final consistent criminological empirical criminal conduct tends to be over-represented
observation that we will address in the current or differentially concentrated within certain
chapter is the non-random distribution of areas of a city or town. Typically, the areas of
criminal behavior. In addition to the gender concentration are considered to represent
gap and age-crime curve, long-standing and highly unstable environments characterized
cross-cultural patterns in terms of where by low average economic status and relatively
criminal conduct tends to occur geospatially low social cohesion. Social scientists have
and the typical dynamics of offender–victim tended to point to these characteristics as the
relationships have been observed. Entire sub- causal factors in the accompanying concentra-
fields within criminology, for example, have tion of criminal conduct while others have
developed which center specifically on these noted that the criminal behavior exhibited in
observations. For example, the Chicago such areas is a result of a sub-culture of reck-
school of criminology, most readily exempli- lessness resulting from minimal opportunity
fied by the work of Shaw and McKay (1942), for social advancement. However, as dis-
focused on the differential patterns of criminal cussed in the prior section these types of
behaviors across different areas (or zones) explanations are incomplete – why would
within a city. The authors argued that the dif- criminal behavior, especially violent behavior,
ferential offending patterns were a result of result from reduced social and economic
variance in the structural conditions (e.g., opportunity? Why would criminal behavior
economic status, ethnic heterogeneity, and exhibit concentrations among communities
residential mobility) found in the zones. with a relative lack of social cohesion or sta-
Further, socioeconomic status and aspects of bility? In their analysis of 77 different neigh-
neighborhood cohesion (sometimes referred borhoods in Chicago using data from 1988 to
to as collective efficacy; Sampson et al., 1997) 1993, Wilson and Daly (1997) illustrated that
have long been assessed as key causal variables an evolutionary perspective can help to
in the etiology of crime and criminality. address such questions (see also Daly, 2016).
Additionally, criminologists and other social Briefly, Wilson and Daly (1997) examined
scientists have also been examining the nature the differential life expectancy for men and
of victim–offender relationships for well over women across various neighborhoods, as well
a century. Consequently, much is known as homicide rates, birth rates, and measures of
about the ways in which criminal behavior is socioeconomic status (i.e., household income
distributed in terms of geospatial location and an income inequality index). The key inde-
within cities and towns as well as how offend- pendent variable, life expectancy, was a meas-
ers and victims are (or are not) known or con- ure of the expected average duration of life,
nected. Overall, in both cases there is a in years, for an individual based on a variety
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND CRIME 197

of vital statistics and population data present the homicide rate skyrocketed from about
at the time of the individual’s birth. In their 5/100,000 in the five to 14 years age group to
analyses Wilson and Daly empirically over 300 deaths per 100,000 for the males in
assessed three specific hypotheses: (1) homi- the 15 to 24 years age group. While the hom-
cide rates are a function of local life expectancy; icide rate for males in these neighborhoods
(2) economic inequality accounts for variance decreased over time, the rates were still astro-
in homicide rates beyond that attributed to nomically higher than those observed in the
local life expectancy; and (3) local life expec- long life expectancy neighborhoods (25 to
tancy will impact reproduction (birth rates) 34 years: about 250/100,000; 35 to 44 years:
across neighborhoods. Overall, the research- about 175/100,000; 45 to 54 years: about
ers argued that life expectancy provides a vital 75/100,000; 55 to 64 years: about 60/100,000;
external cue to inhabitants of neighborhoods 65 to 74 years: about 40/100,000; and 75
such that their unconscious behavioral and years or older: about 55/100,000). While the
reproductive strategies can be adjusted based homicide rates for females in the short life
on an assessment of probable lifespan. Thus, expectancy neighborhoods were much lower
rather than representing a potential pathologi- than for males, the overall pattern across the
cal reaction to social conditions, high-crime age groups was similar and, in some cases,
areas may be a function of a rational calcu- exceeded the homicide rates for males in the
lation (though one that has been honed over long life expectancy neighborhoods.
evolutionary time) based on environmental Finally, in terms of reproductive behaviors
cues. The analyses testing these ideas revealed Wilson and Daly (1997) again compared the
a number of illuminating findings. 10 neighborhoods with the longest life expec-
First, life expectancy at birth was strongly tancy to the 10 neighborhoods with the short-
associated with homicide rates across neigh- est life expectancy across seven different
borhoods for both men and women. In neigh- age categories. The differences in birth rates
borhoods with a lower life expectancy at birth during the teen years and early adulthood
a much higher homicide rate was observed. between the neighborhoods was substantial.
The magnitudes of the bivariate associations Figure 9.1 illustrates the stark differences.
were very strong and statistically signifi- As illustrated, the birth rate for women in
cant for both men (r = −0.88, p < .0001) and the short life expectancy neighborhoods was
women (r = −0.83, p < .0001). Importantly, over four times higher than the long life expec-
these associations held in multivariate mod- tancy neighborhoods for the 15 to 19 years
els wherein measures for household income age group, and two-and-a-half times greater
and income inequality were introduced. in the 20 to 24 years age group. Additionally,
Second, comparisons between the 10 although the difference is still evident in the
neighborhoods with the longest life expec- 25 to 29 years age group, the birth rates for the
tancy and the 10 neighborhoods with the different neighborhoods become almost iden-
shortest life expectancy revealed some drastic tical in the remaining age categories (i.e., 30
differences in terms of homicide rates across years and above). Based on the results of their
age categories. For example, in the long life analyses, Wilson and Daly concluded:
expectancy neighborhoods the homicide rate
(deaths per 100,000 per year) went from vir- [t]he data presented here indicate that people
tually zero for those males aged five to 14 behave as if they have adjusted their rates of
years to about 20/100,000 for those aged future discounting and risk acceptance thresholds
in relation to local life expectancy, and that they
15 to 24, to a peak of about 25/100,000 for
do so in the non-violent domain of reproductive
those aged 25 to 34 before dropping to near decision making as well as in the potentially violent
zero for the remaining age groups. However, domain of social competition. (Wilson and Daly,
in the short life expectancy neighborhoods 1997: 1273)
198 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Figure 9.1  Age-specific birth rates (per 1,000 women per year) in the 10 neighborhoods
with the longest life expectancy compared to the 10 neighborhoods with the shortest life
expectancy
Source: Data derived from table 3 in Wilson and Daly (1997).

The analyses and conclusion presented by both victims and offenders tend to be in their
Wilson and Daly (1997) illustrate how an adolescence or early adulthood. This obser-
evolutionary lens can help explain social vation is not entirely surprising given the
phenomena that have hitherto only been age-crime curve, which manifests as a result
addressed employing sociologically based of the intense mating competition experi-
theories. Furthermore, they illustrate that the enced during that period in the life course.
behavioral repertoire exhibited by individu- However, examinations of specific criminal
als presented with certain environmental behaviors have illustrated other patterns in
cues may not be pathological or reckless, but terms of characteristics. For example,
rather a function of an unconscious calculus researchers estimate that in North America
resulting from eons of evolutionary pro- on average approximately 65% of all mur-
cesses. The next example provides a similar ders involve a male offender and a male
illustration in terms of observed patterns of victim, about 20% involve a male offender
victim–offender characteristics. and a female victim, about 10% involve a
female offender and a male victim, and less
than 5% involve a female offender and a
Non-Random Victim–Offender female victim (Buss, 2005; Daly and Wilson,
2017). Additionally, the risk of being a homi-
Characteristics
cide victim (in general) increases considera-
Depending on the criminal behavior of inter- bly during late adolescence, peaks in the
est, criminologists have observed that char- early 20s, and then drops substantially over
acteristics are consistently represented in adulthood. The overall pattern of homicide
terms of victims and offenders. In general, and other aggressive behaviors in terms of
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND CRIME 199

offender–victim characteristics has been of the argument is beyond the scope of the
addressed by a wide range of criminological current chapter. However, the data on which
inquiries. Most of the examinations, how- Buss based his conclusion and the evolution-
ever, have focused solely on proximate fac- ary arguments are worth considering herein.
tors such as socialization practices or even In addition to examining a large amount of
media consumption to account for the behavioral data similar to that presented in
observed patterns. As noted earlier, a sub- Daly and Wilson’s (2017) book (i.e., offi-
stantial problem for such explanations is that cial criminal justice records), Buss and his
the patterns observed in terms of offender– team also collected data on homicidal idea-
victim characteristics are relatively consist- tions (fantasies) from a sample of over 5,000
ent across time and space. Thus, explanations individuals across a number of age categories
which rely on variance in societal factors from multiple countries. The respondents in
such as socialization practices are doomed to the study were asked if they had ever thought
be incomplete. Fortunately, researchers of killing someone, who it was (in terms of
employing an evolutionary lens have pro- the relationship to the respondent), the man-
vided potential explanations for the observed ner in which they thought of killing the tar-
patterns of offender characteristics exhibited get, what prevented them from going through
in several crimes, including homicide. with the killing, and what could have poten-
In their book entitled Homicide: tially led them to actually kill (i.e., push them
Foundations of human behavior, Daly and over the edge). The respondents were also
Wilson (2017) expand upon their 1985 paper asked if they ever thought someone might kill
and provide a thorough examination of how them and were also presented similar follow-
an evolutionary viewpoint can help explain up questions (i.e., who they thought may have
the characteristics of offenders and of vic- wanted to kill them, how they might have
tims for various types of homicides. Their been killed, what the respondent did to pre-
discussion centers on intrasexual competition vent being killed, what prevented them from
among young males who are psychologi- being killed, and what would have pushed the
cally attuned to threats to status and again person over the edge to kill the respondent).
apply the young male syndrome logic to The results of the survey revealed several
socially competitive risky behaviors. Daly illuminating patterns that aligned with evo-
and Wilson provide a wellspring of empirical lutionary theory (only a handful of which are
analyses from multiple countries to support included here).
their claims, and their coverage of the topic First, a considerable majority of the
is thorough. However, as valuable as Daly respondents in the sample indicated that they
and Wilson’s book is – and it is certainly a had given thought to killing. Buss and his
definitive discussion of how an evolutionary team of researchers observed that 91% of the
viewpoint can be applied to murder – we will men in the sample and 84% of the women
focus our discussion in this section instead on reported at least one vivid fantasy about kill-
a book by David Buss (2005). ing someone. Buss argued that this finding
Buss’s (2005) book, entitled The Murderer supports the argument that over evolution-
Next Door: Why the Mind is Designed to ary time murder may have been an effective
Kill, presents the argument that homicidal strategy to employ when faced with a serious
behavioral patterns may have been selected adaptive problem.
for over evolutionary time as a potential Second, both men and women in the study
strategy for dealing with a variety of adaptive exhibited consistent yet distinct patterns of
problems. The claim that killing is produced homicidal ideations. While both sexes reported
by a specific psychological adaptation is a homicidal fantasies related to sexual rivalries
controversial one and discussing the merits (e.g., killing the new sexual/romantic partner
200 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

or a former partner), men tended to focus on Fourth, in terms of the thoughts related
issues centered on mate retention (i.e., infidel- to being a victim of homicide, Buss and his
ity, actual or perceived, by their current or for- team also noted some general patterns that
mer partner) whereas women tended to focus were consistent within and between sexes.
on threats by other women to their own mate For instance, both men and women consist-
quality (e.g., responding to rumors about their ently indicated that they thought they would
own sexual reputation) and on responding to be the victim of murder given that they had
perceived or actual abusive threats by current engaged in or came close to what evolutionary
or former partners. Buss notes that given the psychologists generally call mate poaching.
substantial threat to reproductive success in In essence, the process refers to a sexual or
these circumstances intense counter-measures, romantic partnership with another mate who
such as violent action, were likely selected for is already involved in a relationship. As noted
over evolutionary time. above, being the actual or perceived victim of
Third, related to the general patterns of mate poaching was consistently evident in the
men and women’s homicidal ideations the fantasies of those who reported wanting to kill.
researchers also noted that men generally Additionally, across most known societies and
reported homicidal ideations focused on sex- recorded time periods such behavior is associ-
ual (rather than emotional) infidelity, whereas ated with enraged emotion and what is typi-
when women mentioned infidelity of a cur- cally termed irrational behavior (though from
rent or former partner in a homicidal fantasy an evolutionary point of view, acting to mini-
it was more often related to emotional (rather mize threats to one’s reproductive status may
than sexual) infidelity. The general differ- actually be rational; see Daly, 2016). Indeed,
ences observed in the reported homicidal fan- the legal codes of many societies include pro-
tasies in this regard also belie the differential visions for reduced punishment or even culpa-
threats to reproductive success for the sexes. bility in cases where a spouse murders a mate
As outlined above, given that women are the poacher. Thus, all parties involved in the mate-
high-MPI sex in our species sexual access is poaching situation are aware of the potential
a resource that is highly contested by men. risks and the respondents who reported feared
Thus, any aspects of the social environment homicidal victimization in Buss’s study cer-
which affects the likelihood of securing such tainly echoed such awareness.
a resource will be met with severe reaction. In terms of some of the general differences
Likewise, while women are typically the between the sexes regarding the reports of
choosier sex in terms of sexual access they being a potential murder victim, the primary
are, as a result of being the high-MPI sex, variance mirrored that observed in the fan-
more burdened by potential and actual off- tasies associated with committing a homi-
spring than are men. Thus, over evolutionary cide. For example, men generally reported a
time psychological modules guiding wom- fear of homicidal victimization that resulted
en’s mate choice have been tuned to cues in from not only mate poaching but also from
potential mates that indicate a willingness to threats to other men regarding the target’s
invest in the long term (i.e., help to raise any (i.e., the potential homicidal male) social
future offspring). One such cue is the extent rank, status, or worthiness as a sexual partner.
to which a mate professes and exhibits emo- Additionally, women generally reported a
tional attachment. Consequently, Buss and fear of victimization resulting from engaging
his team argue, many of the women in the in verbal denigration of the sexual reputation
study centered their homicidal fantasies on or physical appearance of other female rivals.
situations wherein the respondent actually Overall, the pattern illustrated in the idea-
had, or perceived an experience of, emotional tions about homicidal victimization reflected
infidelity. a keen recognition of the type of social
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND CRIME 201

circumstances that typically could drive oth- and antisocial behavior. As noted in the intro-
ers to kill. In line with evolutionary theory, duction, given the social nature of our spe-
Buss noted that this dynamic is a result of cies, the exploitation of others for personal
co-evolution of intense strategies related gain is an indelible and likely inevitable
to maintaining or increasing one’s value in characteristic of the human condition. The
the highly competitive sexual reproduction inevitability of the characteristic, however,
market. Further, the findings are manifesta- does not mean that our species need accept,
tions of the evolution of strategies related to condone, or encourage antisocial behaviors of
prevent being a victim of such competition any kind. As noted throughout this volume,
(and therefore also maintaining or increasing nothing about the explanation of a behavior
one’s mate value). Biologists and evolution- should be seen as a moral stance on the
ary psychologists refer to this process as the behavior. Additionally, just because antiso-
Red Queen hypothesis, and it is discussed in cial, aggressive, and/or criminal behavior is in
detail elsewhere in this volume. part due to the natural processes associated
Overall, the data presented in Homicide with evolution it does not mean that the
and The Murderer Next Door align with the behavior is justified or in any way excused by
historical and contemporary data studied by the knowledge of those processes (to think
criminologists and other social scientists. The otherwise would be to commit the naturalistic
patterns observed in terms of the crime-specific fallacy). Rather, the stance taken in this chap-
non-random distributions of victim–offender ter and elsewhere is that the best opportunity
characteristics and relationships occur across to reduce harm associated with criminal
these data. The differences, however, arise in behavior must be derived from our best
the explanations that have been proffered to efforts to understand the underlying processes
account for these observations. Whereas social of criminality. While the social sciences have
scientists typically lay the blame on processes provided a wide variety of potentially useful
such as socialization, culture, and media expo- proximal hypotheses in this regard, the appli-
sure, evolutionary psychologists illustrate how cation of an evolutionary perspective to crim-
such observations may actually be the result inal behavior provides the ultimate, and
of our evolutionary heritage. Data presented therefore likely most useful, understanding.
by Buss and his research team indicate that As biosocial criminology advances within
the targets of offenses such as homicide may and beyond the discipline of criminology it is
(in general) be particular and such particular- likely that our ability to address the harms
ity is due to the specific threat to reproductive associated with criminality will be enhanced.
success or survival (the key aspects of evolu- Employing an evolutionary lens will be a
tionary processes) represented by the target. crucial component of that journey.
Understanding the victim–offender associa-
tions so often observed in criminological data in
terms of ultimate causes provides an opportu-
nity for greater clarity of etiology and therefore Notes
potential to increase our ability to reduce harm.
1  At the risk of being repetitive, it is key to note
here that an evolutionary perspective does not
dismiss the importance of cross-cultural diversity
in terms of observed crime rates and potential
CONCLUSION additional etiological factors. Indeed, an evolu-
tionary perspective is inherently biosocial such
that it emphasizes the interactive processes
The current chapter presented an overview of between the inherited genetic architecture of
some of the ways in which an evolutionary the brain and the developmental environment to
view can be applied to crime, criminality, which an individual is exposed.
202 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

2  Deviations across a variety of societies have been resources and risk taking. Evolution and
noted, but some of these patterns of deviation Human Behavior, 26, 332–343.
are due to exceedingly rare socio-political events Kanazawa, S., & Still, M. C. (2000). Why men
(e.g., aftermath of a world war and nuclear commit crimes (and why they desist).
attack; Hiraiwa-Hasegawa, 2005).
Sociological Theory, 18, 434–447.
3  Moffitt also suggested a third group, abstainers,
who appear to not engage in any offending over
Laub, J. H., & Sampson, R. J. (1993). Turning
the life course. While of much theoretical interest, points in the life course: Why change matters
our discussion will be limited to the two offend- to the study of crime. Criminology, 31,
ing groups emphasized in her dual taxonomy. 301–325.
Moffitt, T. E. (1993). A developmental
taxonomy. Psychological Review, 100,
674–701.
Quinsey, V. L., Skilling, T. A., Lalumiere, M. L., &
REFERENCES Craig, W. M. (2004). Juvenile delinquency:
Understanding the origins of individual
Barnes, J. C., Wright, J. P., Boutwell, B. B., differences. Washington, DC: American
Schwartz, J. A., Connolly, E. J., Nedelec, J. L., Psychological Association.
& Beaver, K. M. (2014). Demonstrating the Sampson, R. J., Raudenbush, S. W., & Earls, F.
validity of twin research in criminology. (1997). Neighborhoods and violent crime: A
Criminology, 52, 588–626. multilevel study of collective efficacy. Science,
Beaver, K. M. (2009). Biosocial criminology: A 277, 918–924.
primer. Dubuque: Kendall Hunt. Shaw, C. R., & McKay, H. D. (1942). Juvenile
Buss, D. M. (2005). The murderer next door: delinquency and urban areas. Chicago, IL:
Why the mind is designed to kill. New York: University of Chicago Press.
Penguin. Trivers, R. L. (1972). Parental investment and
Daly, M. (2016). Killing the competition: sexual selection. In B. Campbell (Ed.), Sexual
Economic inequality and homicide. New selection and the descent of man, 1871–1971
York: Transaction Publishers. (pp. 136–179). Chicago, IL: Aldine.
Daly, M., & Wilson, M. (2017). Homicide: Wilson, M., & Daly, M. (1985). Competitiveness,
Foundations of human behavior. New York: risk taking, and violence: The young male
Routledge. syndrome. Ethology and Sociobiology, 6,
Dobzhansky, T. (1973). Nothing in biology 59–73.
makes sense except in the light of evolution. Wilson, M., & Daly, M. (1997). Life expectancy,
The American Biology Teacher, 35, 125–129. economic inequality, homicide, and repro-
Hiraiwa-Hasegawa, M. (2005). Homicide by ductive timing in Chicago neighbourhoods.
men in Japan, and its relationship to age, BMJ, 314, 1271–1274.
10
Evolutionary Psychology and
Policing: The Balance Between
Aggression and Restraint
Lois James

INTRODUCTION enforcement of ‘stop and frisk’ practices,


contributing to racial injustice and anti-
Police professionalism and use of force are police sentiment (White and Fradella, 2016).
critical topics of public interest in the 21st In the ‘post-Ferguson’ era, the policing pro-
century. Perhaps more than ever in the his- fession is faced with nationwide calls for
tory of US policing, the public are demand- reform, and an understanding of how this
ing accountability and visibility of police professional group has evolved is essential
behavior. Some researchers indicate that this for guiding its path forward (President’s Task
intense microscope of scrutiny has led to Force on 21st Century Policing, 2015).
decreased legitimacy and public faith in The function of the police is tied to their
police, going so far as to label it a “legiti- granted authority to use force to ensure safety
macy crisis” (Gest, 2016; James et al., 2016). and order. Renowned sociology and policing
Following high-profile shootings of unarmed scholar Egon Bittner (1970) identified the
African American men in recent years, start- role of the police as the legitimate authority
ing with Michael Brown in Ferguson, to exercise force. This is challenging because
Missouri, public trust in police dropped sig- the exercising of this authority by the police
nificantly, equaling the rates observed in the frequently stirs accusations of brutality and
years following the Rodney King trials racism, civil unrest, demands that officers
(Jones, 2015). Minority citizens reported less be criminally punished, and at times mass
trust in the police than white citizens (Peck, violence and rioting. The goal of this chap-
2015). Within any social system a certain ter is to explore the police function and these
degree of give and take is necessary, and contradictory social realities using evolution-
historically the police have had some diffi- ary psychology. In order to maintain order
culty with yielding authority – for example, and serve the citizenry the police must be
204 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

both aggressive and restrained. Maintaining has evolved over generations as a ­beneficial
a balance between aggression and restraint response. These include the protection and
is required to promote and uphold social acquisition of necessary resources, self-
order. As society evolves, so does this bal- defense against attack, inflicting costs on
ance (whereby the scale can tip more towards same-sex rivals who are vying for the same
aggression or restraint depending on societal resources, gaining and maintaining power
demands). Our society today expects greater and dominance, deterring potential oppo-
restraint on the part of police, while police nents from future attacks, ensuring the sexual
culture continues to promote aggressive fidelity of a partner, and reducing resource
authority. Aligning public and police expec- investment in unrelated children. For exam-
tations of the appropriate balance between ple, in a pack of wolves, the dominant mem-
aggression and restraint could promote legiti- ber cannot show weakness in the face of a
macy in all aspects of police work. physical challenge or his dominant status
will be questioned and potentially overturned
(Millan, 2006). He must assert his dominance
through aggression, or submit to a new leader
THE EVOLUTION OF AGGRESSION (restraint). In this way, aggression evolved
AND RESTRAINT as a mechanism for demonstrating and pro-
tecting dominant social status. Examples of
Evolution is the process of change in all resource-acquisition-related aggression in
forms of life over generations. Each genera- humans include two men fighting over the
tion inherits traits, through genes, from their attentions of an attractive woman, a homeless
parents. If this new trait is heritable, and woman stabbing a wealthy-looking woman
contributes to greater reproductive success of to steal her wallet, or a nation going to war
the organism relative to individuals without with another over valuable natural resources.
the trait, it will be passed on to the next gen- Restraint behavior should occur when the
eration and accumulate in the population target of the aggressive behavior is subdued
(Buss and Shackelford, 1997). New traits or neutralized. As important as aggression
that do not help the organism survive long is, the failure to employ proper restraint can
enough to reproduce will become rare or compromise one’s survival. Restraint occurs
disappear. This process of natural selection in nature when animals are faced with the
or ‘survival of the fittest’ has guided the evo- submission of a challenger. For example, the
lution of aggressive and restrained behaviors roe deer buck will not clash antlers unless
over time. Although in social animals such as an opponent is face-on and engaged in the
humans, aggression is not typically an indis- fight, although he could successfully attack
criminate strategy (Savage and Kanazawa, when the opponent is turned around and vul-
2004), context-specific aggression is a natu- nerable (Eibl-Eibesfeldt, 1961). Likewise,
rally occurring, prevalent phenomenon the defeated wolf will show his neck to his
(Neuberg et al., 2010). Restraint is the action successful opponent, who refrains from kill-
that occurs when the ‘means to an end’ is ing him even though he could do so (Lorenz,
reached, for example when the threat of an 1952). Such restraint in the face of submis-
opponent is neutralized. It can also occur if sion is used to safeguard one’s energy for
one decides that the risks of employing future attacks – use of unnecessary energy
aggression as a strategy are too great. For the can signal that one is vulnerable (Millan,
most part, aggression and restraint are tightly 2006). Moreover, if a social animal fails to
linked. show proper restraint by either attacking oth-
Buss and Shackelford (1997) identified ers without good cause, being overly aggres-
seven social problems for which aggression sive in their pursuit of submission from
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND POLICING 205

others, or continuing an attack when their ‘state of nature’ as a society without legal
opponent is clearly defeated, that individual institutions. Although rare, a modern example
will be threatening to the social order and of such a system is the Gebusi tribe of New
may be killed, exiled, or (in the human case) Guinea – a highly cooperative, egalitarian,
jailed or imprisoned (Francis, 1998). social society which is non-competitive and
politically decentralized with interpersonal
relations that are mutually respectful, non-
hierarchical, and self-effacing. Aggression
THE CREATION OF THE POLICE among the Gebusi people is discouraged, as
PROFESSION AND THE SOCIAL antisocial behavior is contradictory to their
CONTRACT BETWEEN THE POLICE peaceful values. Fear of violence is fostered
AND THE CITIZENRY and withdrawal from violence is reinforced.
They also have a homicide rate among the
Evolution explains why using aggression as a highest reported. Between 1940 and 1982
means to an end may be necessary for sur- nearly one-third of adult deaths were caused
vival, and how restraint in humans evolved as by homicide (Knauft et  al., 1987). This is
a means of tempering and controlling aggres- the equivalent of a homicide rate of 568 per
sion, with the result that social order and the 100,000 per annum. To put this in perspec-
rule of law are preserved. Not all players are tive, the US homicide rate (one of the high-
afforded equal power in the exercise of est in the Western world) was roughly 6 per
aggression in contemporary US society. The 100,000 per annum in 2018 (Federal Bureau
police (and other professionals in ascribed of Investigation, 2018).
circumstances) are granted the right to exer- The most common cause of homicide is
cise legitimate physical force if necessary to ‘sorcery’, that is, an individual will be killed
protect public safety. when they are believed to have caused death
This can be explained by Social Contract through sickness to someone else in the vil-
Theory (SCT), which states that individuals’ lage. Given that the Gebusi live in the low-
moral obligations are shaped by a collective land rainforest of New Guinea, disease is
agreement or ‘social contract’ that binds a rampant, explaining the ‘sickness deaths’
society together with a set of accepted norms and consequent sorcerer killings. Homicide
and rules (such as the rule of law). This the- is considered a regrettable but unavoid-
ory was given its first rigorous defense by able burden required to maintain the social
Thomas Hobbes (1588–1679). John Locke system, and even the close kin members of
(1632–1704) and Jean-Jacques Rousseau the victim rarely seek retribution. Societies
(1712–1778) are other champions of this without legal institutions, even highly coop-
theory. In fact, Locke’s argument that citi- erative and peaceable tribes like the Gebusi,
zens have the right to revolt against author- will inevitably require a great deal of inter-
ity should it no longer be protecting their personal violence to prevent anarchy. Within
interests was enormously influential on most systems, the social contract between
democratic revolution – notably for Thomas police and citizens is the collective antidote
Jefferson and the founders of the United to the ‘state of nature’ Reiman describes.
States. Revolt against perceived tyrannical Individuals recognize that their right to use
rule or oppression has potential implications force may be countered by the rights of oth-
for the current police legitimacy crisis with ers to use force, and that the state of nature is
the rise of social justice movements such as inherently unsafe and unstable in the absence
Black Lives Matter. of a governing rule. As such, we sacrifice
Not all human societies are bound by a certain personal freedoms to increase safety
social contract. Reiman (1985) describes the at a broader level: ‘It becomes rational for
206 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

freedom-loving people to renounce their argument for why police use of force differs
freedom to use force at their own discretion’ based on suspect race and socioeconomic sta-
(Reiman, 1985: 239). But in order to do this, tus is that police discretion favors the socially
some society members must be responsible dominant and protects the status quo. Of
for maintaining public safety. We assign this course, this position tends to be vehemently
duty to the police. denied by members of the socially dominant
In the mid 19th century, as the United group, who argue that suspect behavior is
States expanded through immigration and solely responsible for police use of force.
the industrial revolution, professional police Nevertheless, the tension between the police
forces were established in metropolitan cit- and minority classes has fueled considerable
ies and a social contract authorized police discord and distrust in police legitimacy.
officers to protect individuals from victimi- Use of force perceived by citizens as
zation (Walker, 1977). Police officers were unnecessary, unjust, or excessive undermines
granted the authority to employ coercion to the social contract and the legitimacy of the
protect citizens’ lives and property (Reiman, police. In many circumstances, this is now the
1985). Conceptualized in this way, the police case: the police retain a cultural emphasis on
became society’s ‘professional aggres- aggressive crime-control tactics and have a
sors’, called upon when force was necessary self-image as society’s law enforcers (Brown,
(Bittner, 1970). The social contract estab- 1988). The citizenry, on the other hand, have
lishes the right of police to utilize the force come to expect that police not only control
necessary, including deadly force, if the crime, but serve the community in ways that
threat warrants it. For example, if a suspect build social bonds and prevent crime. Officers
is posing a deadly threat to innocent civilians are expected to be mentors, social workers,
the police are justified to shoot. In this case, mental-health professionals, and counselors,
aggression is not just allowed, but expected as well as law enforcers (Terrill et al., 2003).
for the overall good of society. The role of Similarly, while the citizenry expects greater
restraint within the social contract is equally and more nuanced restraint in the exercise of
important. Citizens expect that police author- law enforcement, police training and culture
ity be utilized legitimately, competently, and emphasize the need for officers to protect
in good faith (Reiman, 1985). Use of force themselves by staying one step ahead of the
by police must result in an overall increase in suspect or safety threat. These differences of
public safety. opinion reinforce the ‘us vs them’ mentality,
Despite a consistent emphasis on the func- in which officers increasingly feel the pub-
tion of the police being tied to the legitimate lic does not understand the harsh realities of
use of force, the major role of the police has what they face on a daily basis, and the public
evolved over time with the demands of the increasingly resents police authority.
ruling elite. From preventing slave revolts in
the mid 19th century, to maintaining segrega-
tion following emancipation, to riot enforce-
ment during the civil rights movement, the HAWKS VS DOVES AND THE
police can be seen as the ‘forceful arm’ of EVOLUTION OF THE POLICE PERSONA
local, state, and federal government, tasked
with maintaining the status quo. The police Maynard-Smith’s famous ‘hawk versus dove’
have long been directed by the political pow- model is one of the most important contribu-
ers, who have historically been made up of tions in evolutionary game theory and of
wealthy white men. It stands to reason that the direct relevance to the ‘police persona’. At its
police have traditionally served the interests most basic level, the hawk will choose the
of this socially dominant group. A common strategy of escalating aggression and
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND POLICING 207

continue attacking until their opponent required to change strategies or nobody gets
retreats or they themselves are injured. A food (Dawkins, 1980).
dove might display aggression but retreat if On average, police officers will be more
their opponent escalates. In a contest between successful with a hawk strategy than a dove
a hawk and a dove, a hawk would win. In a strategy. This makes sense because if an
contest between two hawks, assuming both officer comes up against a ‘hawk-like’ sus-
have equal ‘resource-holding potential’ pect and they themselves are ‘dove-like’,
(RHP) such as strength, access to weapons, the suspect will likely win (get away, attack
etc., there is an equal likelihood of either them, etc.). On the other hand, if the officer
winning. In a contest between two doves, the is ‘hawk-like’ they will likely win against a
resource being fought over is shared (nego- dove and have an equal likelihood of winning
tiation), or the War of Attrition model of against a hawk. Research by Yabuta (2008),
threat to avoid actual fighting is employed to however, has suggested that there might
determine who gets the resource. Maynard be a third more complex strategy to add to
Smith’s (1974) War of Attrition model states the hawk versus dove model with particu-
that when animals engaging in conflict lar relevance to policing, that of ‘assessor’.
cannot assess the other’s likelihood of beat- The assessor weighs RHP, then alternates
ing them they will attempt to deter conflict between hawk and dove strategies depend-
through ritual displays of aggression. ing on the situation. To assess is an ESS
For an evolutionarily stable strategy (ESS) because it prevents inappropriate attacks on
to occur there must be a mix of hawks and non-opponents. The role of assessor could
doves. A system of all doves is vulnerable be applied to police officers who must dis-
to invasion by a mutant hawk and in a sys- tinguish between opponents (threatening
tem of all hawks, the cost of loss becomes suspects) and non-opponents (general mem-
too great. The result is that when hawks are bers of the public or complying suspects) and
rare they have the advantage and are selected modify their strategy accordingly. How they
for. However, when there are more hawks select these strategies can also be explained
than doves some hawks are forced to take on by evolutionary theory.
a dove strategy so the evolutionary ‘seesaw’
that typifies an ESS swings back the other
way as doves are selected for. The same thing
can be said for those that follow the rules and GAME THEORY AND SELECTION
those that cheat in Hardin’s ‘Tragedy of the OF AGGRESSION AND RESTRAINT
Commons’ scenario, where there are only so STRATEGIES
many resources to go around. When cheat-
ers are rare their strategy is highly success- Selection between aggression and restraint
ful, but eventually the resources will run thin strategies applies to policing in two impor-
and their chances of detection will increase; tant ways. First, it is expected that officers
thus the balance will be tipped back in are able to expertly assess whether aggres-
favor of the compliers (Hardin, 1968). This sion is an appropriate response in a given
type of ESS can be seen throughout the ani- situation. The justification for the decision is
mal kingdom. For example, most seagulls grounded in the level of threat in their oppo-
catch fish but some wait by the shore and nents’ actions. Barash (2004) uses game
steal the food from the hard-working gulls. theory, and the example of the game ‘rock,
This is an effective strategy as cheaters paper, scissors’, to demonstrate the success
get maximum benefit for minimum effort. of a strategy that is dependent on another
However, when the cheating gulls start to player. If you pick rock and your opponent
outnumber the hard-working gulls some are also picks rock then you draw, the expected
208 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

return is zero, or E(R,R)=0. If your opponent This model can be observed in many species,
has picked scissors you win and the expected for example howler monkeys or elephant
return is one, or E(R,S)=+1; however, if they seals that loudly vocalize to display aggres-
picked paper you lose and the expected return sion, fish that puff up to try and prevent
is minus one, or E(R,P)=−1 (Barash, 1982). If attacks, and deer that shake their antlers
a player consistently employs one strategy at each other to determine who is the more
such as rock, their opponent will catch on and dominant (Krebs and Davies, 1984). If this
use a defeating strategy. Thus, in the ‘rock, deterrence does not work and a fight ensues,
paper, scissors’ game the best strategy is to then the contestant who is prepared to risk a
use each with equal probability. higher cost and fight for longer will win.
Now apply the game to police use of The War of Attrition model relates to
deadly force, where the options are ‘shoot’ police use of force because it shows how a
or ‘don’t shoot’. If the officer shoots and the cost–benefit analysis has evolved resulting in
suspect represents a real threat, the officer aggression only when necessary and only to
wins (assuming he or she does not get shot the extent necessary. The latter relates to the
first). If the officer shoots and the suspect second application of the balance between
does not represent a real threat (e.g. is try- aggression and restraint in policing: that
ing to pull out a wallet, not a gun) then the officers must apply immediate restraint fol-
officer loses and faces the consequences. If lowing the use of aggression. Furthermore,
the officer does not shoot and the suspect the very aggression they use should be con-
represents a real threat the officer loses and trolled and strategic as opposed to driven
might be injured or killed. Finally, if the by fear, anger, or frustration. They are also
officer does not shoot and the suspect does expected to render or call for medical aid
not represent a real threat the officer wins, for injuries they were personally responsible
as he or she has made the right decision. for inflicting. The failure to apply appropri-
As in the ‘rock, paper, scissors’ game, the ate restraint and allow aggression to become
police cannot always favor the same strategy emotionally driven results in incidents such
because they would be at high risk of mak- as the infamous beating of Rodney King in
ing an error. Thus, officers must constantly 1991. Video footage of the incident, featuring
weigh their perceptions of threat with the Los Angeles Police Department officers beat-
actions of the suspect and the consequences ing an African American man on the ground
of their own decisions. Of course, officers and circulated by media outlets nationwide,
have far more to think about than this simple has come to symbolize how use of force in
analogy implies, and are usually not blind to law enforcement can become unfettered,
the actions of the opponent. However, game brutal, and deadly when left unchecked.
theory exemplifies the decision officers must Restraint, then, is a crucial component of a
make when faced with a threat to employ law enforcement officer’s tactical skill set.
either aggression or restraint. The evolutionary foundation of the balance
Evolutionary game theory explains why between aggression and restraint is straight-
selection has favored certain characteristics, forward. Aggression when necessary can
behaviors, or attributes, when success in a solve several adaptive problems. Aggression
contest depends on the behaviors of oth- past the point of necessity in social systems,
ers (Barash, 1982). For example, Maynard however, can produce costs and often fails to
Smith’s War of Attrition model (1974) states solve adaptive problems. The police, just like
that when animals engaging in conflict can- every social animal, must achieve a balance
not assess the other’s likelihood of beat- between aggression and restraint. However,
ing them or RHP they will attempt to avoid officers are in a reasonably unique position in
conflict through ritual displays of aggression. having to balance aggression and restraint as
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND POLICING 209

a core function of their job, and answering to beliefs that we might not be aware of (James,
the public whenever they decide to employ an 2017).
aggressive response. In some cases, aggres- Nesse (2005) describes responses to threat
sion can be favored on the part of the police, signaling that evolved within the ances-
and evolutionary theory offers some insights tral environment, such as the fight or flight
as to why. response, and some of the problems that
arise in modern life because of these evolved
responses. These ideas have relevance to
policing, and officers’ use of aggression.
THREAT SIGNALS AND THE Natural selection resulted in the human nerv-
‘FIGHT OR FLIGHT’ RESPONSE ous system being highly responsive to threat
cues. We do not wait to see a predator attack-
The functioning of the human brain has not ing to trigger the response system, but instead
changed since the Pleistocene epoch. Humans we are sensitive to subtle cues and engage the
evolved over millions of years in the African sympathetic system that allows us to fight or
savanna where people lived in small groups flee. Classical conditioning also plays a role
of hunter-gatherers. This environment is here – if we have experienced a threaten-
referred to as the ‘environment of evolution- ing situation in the past, a similar situation
ary adaptedness’ (EEA) (e.g. Bowlby, 1969). will be likely to trigger the fight or flight
This is a critical time period and the environ- response in the future. This is the concept
ment where natural selection ‘designed’ the of conditioned anxiety to a cue of danger.
modern human species. One of the most ‘False alarms’ are inexpensive relative to the
important tools for survival, favored by natu- potential consequences of attack by a preda-
ral selection, was the ‘fight or flight’ system tor. This helps to explain why humans tend to
for responding to threats. Threat response be risk averse. This could also explain police
required efficient learning of threat signals shootings where officers shoot in the absence
and detecting threatening objects. An exam- of concrete evidence that the suspect posed
ple is fear of snakes (Neuberg et al., 2010). a deadly threat. From a survival perspective,
Despite fear of snakes serving little purpose the risk of being shot is worse than the risk
for the majority of humans in the modern era, of getting it wrong, and the fight or flight
humans tend to be particularly efficient at response can make us prone to responding to
learning fearful responses to threat signals, threat cues in the absence of real threat.
and particularly inefficient at unlearning James, Todak et al. (2018) speculated that this
them. construct from evolutionary psychology can be
Relatedly, humans tend to be fearful or seen in Fachner and Carter’s (2015) ‘threat per-
wary of coalitional outgroups (groups of ception failure’ (TPF) theory. TPF occurs when
people who are different from them) due to an officer mistakes a non-threatening object
successful threat responses against attacking (such as a wallet) for a threatening one (such
groups in the ancestral environment. This at as a gun), or a non-threatening action (reach-
least partially explains the concept of ‘implicit ing for a wallet) for a threatening one (reach-
bias’ that humans have against groups that are ing for a gun) (see also Scharf and Binder,
different from themselves. Common implicit 1983, for a discussion of false-positive
biases exist around race, ethnicity, sexual ori- errors). Within the policing literature, TPF is
entation, gender identification, and disability, associated with implicit racial bias, whereby
among others. Although in the modern era we officers are more likely to experience TPF
(typically) do not need to fear people who are when faced with African Americans than
different from us, we have evolved to favor with people of other races and ethnicities.
this strategy, and this can lead to attitudes and From an evolutionary perspective, TPF is
210 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

associated with fear and threat responses. Of protection of coalitions – groups who work
course, one could argue that increased fear together towards goals (Geary et  al., 2003).
indicates increased bias, but bias is not the These groups provide reproductive advan-
sole reason for a threat response. tages, as well as increased opportunity for
One of the causes of threat response status and protection. Even in highly social
in police officers is training they receive animals such as humans, group-level dynam-
designed to increase anxiety and heighten ics can facilitate individual aggression under
attention to threat signals. An example of this some circumstances, in particular between
training is ambush training. This type of train- male groups. A famous example of this was
ing is well intentioned, in that it is designed observed in the sociological ‘Robbers Cave’
to prepare officers for ambush encounters on experiments, where randomly assembled
the street, but it teaches rapid response over groups of boys engaged in between-group
careful thought, and consequently results in competition (Puurtinen et al., 2015). During
a heightened risk of error. James Todak and these 1950s and 1960s experiments, boys
colleagues (2018) argue that training should were grouped arbitrarily. They quickly began
help reduce the incidence of sudden, impul- coalition building and engaged in competi-
sive use of force by officers that might be tion with the other group. Evolutionary psy-
fueled by anxiety associated with a fight– chologists posit that this is evidence of
flight response. Training that focuses on psychological mechanisms that motivate
keeping the officer actively engaged (instead ingroup cooperation and outgroup-directed
of relying on fight or flight) could reduce the aggression (Sherif et al., 1961). Interestingly,
number of accidental shootings by police when provided with a problem that required
(Binder and Scharf, 1980; Fyfe, 1996). This resources outside of their group, the boys
is feasible because, just as we learn to detect would cooperate with the competing group
and respond to threat signals, we also can for a period of time, before reverting to their
become desensitized to those signals when own coalition.
they do not result in threat. Understandably, These insights have relevance to the
an argument frequently made by the polic- police profession as a coalition. Police cul-
ing profession is that desensitization to ture emphasizes loyalty to its members and a
threat cues could compromise officer safety. distrust of non-members (or at least a strong
Directly counter to this argument, evidence feeling that people outside the group do not
exists that preventing officers from being understand them or have their best inter-
overcome by a sympathetic nervous system ests at heart). The police culture has clear
response improves officers’ marksmanship benefits to individual officers – notably the
and deadly-force judgment and decision belief that members of their group will ‘have
making, ultimately promoting officer safety their back’ (Paoline, 2003), which promotes
(Johnson et al., 2014). officer safety. The feeling of belonging to a
family is often noted as a draw to policing,
and in many cases generations of people
from the same family will join the profes-
GENDER-BASED AGGRESSION AND sion. There are other aspects of police culture
POLICE CULTURE which are not as positive, for example the
fostering of an ‘us vs them’ mentality, which
Despite gender diversification in the police can impair police ability to connect with the
profession, policing remains an overwhelm- communities that they are expected to protect
ingly male profession. Selection has favored and serve. Also the ‘blue wall of silence’ or
individual aggression in boys and men, in unwillingness to ‘rat’ on fellow officers when
particular with regard to the development and they break the rules can lead to accusations of
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND POLICING 211

secrecy, corruption, and lack of accountability conferred deference instead of intimidation-


(Walker, 2001). Finally, the police culture is induced submission (Henrich and Gil-White,
inherently masculine, and female officers can 2001). There is an evolutionary basis for
have challenges with acceptance, bias, bigotry, respecting those we consider to be prestig-
or unfair promotion practices. ious. In some societies, prestige exists with-
Police researchers have shown that offic- out dominance hierarchies, and deference
ers who more closely align with the police to prestigious individuals offers advantages
culture are more likely to display aggression, (e.g. proximity to potential mates who flock
on and off the job. This includes aggres- to the prestigious). Those with prestige enjoy
sive tactics during traffic stops (Paoline and benefits, including the desire for proximity
Terrill, 2005) and citizen interactions (Terrill to the prestigious (rather than distance, as is
et  al., 2003). These officers are more likely common with regard to dominant individu-
to receive citizen complaints (Terrill and als), the admiration of others, preferential
Paoline, 2015) and to use unnecessary force copying, obedience, and attention (James,
(Silver et  al., 2017). Furthermore, they are Todak et  al., 2018). Of course, dominance
less likely to adhere to the principles of pro- and prestige are not mutually exclusive,
cedural justice (Terrill and Paoline, 2015) and many individuals may attain both. But
and are more likely to engage in misconduct prestige does not depend on dominance. For
(Kappeler et al., 1998). Finally, officers who example, a police officer who performs a
have a strong connection to police culture are heroic feat (such as rescuing a small child) is
more likely to engage in intimate-partner vio- likely to achieve prestige, both among peers
lence (Blumenstein et al., 2012). and within the community, even if they are
not a ‘dominant’ officer.
Status seeking is related to both dominance
and prestige. Although the construct is from
DOMINANCE, PRESTIGE, AND social psychology, it is relevant to evolution-
STATUS SEEKING ary psychology, because status seeking is a
product of competition for social status and
Dominance hierarchies form when access to consequent resources (Geary, 1999). Social
resources is limited and are common in status can be observed in children as young
social species. Within the hierarchy, some as two or three years old, particularly dur-
individuals have greater access to resources ing same-sex play (Bukowski et  al., 2011).
than others (Neuberg et  al., 2010). This is The construct of status seeking can also be
based on social rank, and its association with observed in the literature on juvenile delin-
reproductive success in social species quency, especially regarding gang participa-
(Paquette, 2015). Among humans, these hier- tion and violence (Thrasher, 1936). Within
archies are common across cultures (Neuberg this context, serious aggression and physi-
et  al., 2010) and can be observed even in cal violence can occur, especially between
young children (Beaulieu and Bugental, males, over issues that seem trivial. This is
2007). Within dominance hierarchies, those related to the idea of ‘saving face’ and not let-
at the top tend to induce submission to their ting other males disrespect or question one’s
dominance either through physical intimida- social status (Felson and Steadman, 1983).
tion or control of resources (Cheng et  al., Within policing, dominance, prestige,
2010). In mammals, dominant males have and status seeking are readily observed. A
greater access to mates, which then leads to police officer who takes a dominant approach
selection for these dominant traits. is likely to be seen as a protector, an aggres-
The construct of prestige, although related sor, and someone who will not back down in
to dominance, differs in that it elicits freely the face of attack. This strengthens ties to the
212 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

police culture, and also promotes aggressive accomplishing these goals include training
tactics with citizens, for example officers reform, policy change, and greater transpar-
who escalate their responses in an effort to ency. However, the type of reform the public
maintain control in a situation (Alpert et al., expects is not likely to be successful while
2004). Escalation tactics are more frequently the majority of police training emphasizes
observed when officers are faced with citi- aggressive tactics and police culture resists
zens who disrespect their authority (Van reform. According to the Police Executive
Maanen, 1978) or display ‘contempt of cop’ Research Forum (2015), over 90% of the
(James, James et  al., 2018). Thus, officers’ training hours officers currently receive are
physical responses to threats to authority on aggressive tactics. Paired with the empha-
and character can be seen as mechanisms for sis on physical aggression within the police
maintaining dominance during the encounter. culture, tactics that promote restraint are
This adheres to the policing culture, and is likely to be less appealing to police offic-
likely to secure prestige and status from their ers (Crank, 2014). The framework depicted
police-officer peers. in Figure 10.1 re-envisions the goal of the
police in modern society as expertly balanc-
ing aggression and restraint.
The proposed framework acknowledges
TOWARDS A BALANCE OF the importance of controlled aggression in
AGGRESSION AND RESTRAINT policing as a strategy for maintaining social
IN POLICING order, but emphasizes the need to tem-
per aggression with appropriate levels of
Despite a focus on aggression within the restraint. This balance can be demonstrated
policing culture, police officers are adept at across all elements of policing, from routine
employing restraint. They do not typically to deadly encounters. The proposed frame-
employ force indiscriminately. Recently, work does not diminish the importance of
however, serious allegations of excessive and aggression in situations in which it is legiti-
unnecessary force used by the police against mately required. For example, to compe-
young Black men has sparked controversy tently arrest an assaultive suspect, an officer
surrounding the police profession. This has must overpower any resistance. Similarly,
led to calls for action, and in some cases citi- encounters that warrant police force require
zens taking matters into their own hands an officer to aggressively ‘win’ against the
(either by rioting or attacking the police). suspect. This is especially true for use of
Throughout the evolution of human society, deadly force where the officer must guard
when social order is threatened and revolution against loss of innocent life, including their
looms, the antidote is often authority reform own. When officers are confident in their
(DeBenedetti, 1980; Shaw and Shaw, 1977; ability to employ aggression, they are less
Wolpert, 1962). The final report from President likely to activate a flight or fight response,
Obama’s Task Force on 21st Century Policing because they are less likely to feel that their
recommends police reform to reduce tensions resources are overwhelmed. In other words,
between the police and the people they are the police officer must have the ability to be
sworn to serve and protect (President’s Task a hawk, in order to select whether a hawk or
Force on 21st Century Policing, 2015). dove strategy is more appropriate.
The majority of the recommendations from The other side of the balancing scale is
the report are targeted at improving public restraint. Restraint in this case is not to be
trust in police, repairing broken relation- mistaken for meekness, second-guessing, or
ships, increasing police legitimacy, and pro- unwillingness to dominate if the situation
moting procedural justice. Avenues towards requires it. Restraint is defined as the ability
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND POLICING 213

Figure 10.1  Police balance between aggression and restraint

to temper aggression, or avoid it when it is not Police departments in the United States,
warranted. On the restraint side, the officer is including Kansas City (Missouri), have begun
supplied with a toolkit of additional options to implement tactical disengagement training
that circumvent the use of physically aggres- (Police Executive Research Forum, 2015).
sive tactics. It is important to note that these There has been a move in other countries
tactics (for example, tactical disengagement such as Canada and the UK in this direction,
and verbal de-escalation) are not new; they as well. The idea is to not force an encounter
have been around since the inception of the with a citizen when there is a risk of escala-
police profession. Arguably, however, they tion and the reason for the encounter is minor.
have yet to become central components of This mindset is antithetical to predominant
police work, given that the majority of train- cultural values in policing, which dictate that
ing continues to promote aggressive tactics, officers should not back down from a chal-
and police culture continues to reward physi- lenge and should jump in quickly to handle a
cal aggression. situation (Fyfe, 1986; Paoline, 2003). The
tactical disengagement philosophy teaches
officers that they do not need to initiate or
‘win’ every encounter.
PROMOTING RESTRAINT IN POLICE Relatedly, communication tactics such
TRAINING AND POLICY as ‘verbal judo’, de-escalation, and motiva-
tional interviewing have been taught in many
Several strategies exist in the police profes- departments as a way to influence citizens
sion for promoting restraint. One example is into voluntary compliance without using
the use of ‘tactical disengagement’ or actively physical coercion (Humphrey, 2013). Such
attempting to de-escalate a volatile encounter. techniques draw on professionalism and
214 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

empathetic connections with the i­ndividual’s Officers can exercise restraint in tempering
personal situation to reach a mutually agreed- their tactics according to the mental health
upon solution to the problem. Verbal tech- professionals’ information and suggestions.
niques require expert restraint and can be The same can be said for communicating
especially frustrating for police officers when with victims, distraught family members,
they are faced with individuals who are com- friends, and witnesses on scene.
bative, disrespectful, or rude (Pusatory, 2016). Police departments in some US cities have
Switching immediately from aggressor to begun to re-engineer their use of force train-
rendering medical aid is another example of ing towards an emphasis on violence de-
how officers balance aggression and restraint. escalation and avoidance. Officers in the Las
In fact, officers today are increasingly expected Vegas (Nevada), New York City (New York),
to immediately render aid to an injured person Seattle (Washington), Oakland (California),
or else be publicly condemned for neglect- and Leesburg (Virginia) police departments
ing human life (Dart and Walters, 2016). have all received training on violence de-
Rendering life-saving aid following a use of escalation, teaching tactics designed to de-
force requires that an officer changes mindsets escalate a police encounter and avoid the
quickly, shifting from a ‘warrior’ to a ‘guard- need to use physical force to solve the prob-
ian’ role (Rahr and Rice, 2015) – an officer lem. Unfortunately, programs that incorporate
must drop their defensive and offensive stance some form of de-escalation or verbal tactics
and save the person’s life. An officer may need training are frequently offered in a fragmented
to shift back and forth between controlled manner, failing to teach how these skills can
aggression and restraint if the individual con- be integrated with the use of force training.
tinues to resist or fight as the officer employs A prevailing criticism of de-emphasizing
medical aid. From an evolutionary perspective, aggressive tactics training for police is that
these expectations move beyond a simple deci- it will result in decreased officer safety, and
sion to employ restraint upon achieving the consequently decreased community safety. A
end goal – we are asking the police to engage central argument here, often voiced by offic-
in the seemingly more unnatural behavior of ers themselves, is that researchers and oth-
actually preserving the wellbeing of a physical ers who are advocating for reform in police
opponent by employing life-saving aid. use of force do not understand the danger-
Collaboration with other services also ous realities of police work. Supporting
requires restraint on the part of officers. this argument, some evidence suggests that
Officers are accustomed to being called on to hyper-vigilance on the part of the police is
solve a wide range of life problems that often necessary. Interviews with individuals who
have nothing to do with law enforcement but feloniously assaulted a police officer found
that the citizenry feels the police should do that they were more likely to attack an officer
something about right away (Bittner, 1974). if he or she seemed unprepared to react to a
As such, police agencies are beginning to problem, did not appear to know an attack
identify areas of police work that may be bet- was coming, or seem to have dropped their
ter handled if they are redirected to other pro- guard (Pinizzotto et al., 2006). This research
fessionals, or are handled collaboratively by confirms police-culture beliefs, and has made
multiple agencies. For example, when inter- the shift towards violence de-escalation diffi-
acting with a suspect suffering from mental cult to accept by many rank-and-file officers.
illness, an officer sometimes has the option to In contrast, de-escalation tactics may
call on mental health professionals who can promote officer safety. In a crisis of police
offer expert advice on the individual’s behav- legitimacy, citizens are less likely to follow
ior. Indeed, this is an underlying philosophy the law and comply with police commands.
of Crisis Intervention Team (CIT) training. This has been documented in the policing
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND POLICING 215

literature – people who perceive the police as and vice versa. Examples of COP strategies
legitimate are more likely to cooperate and include programs that promote citizen coop-
obey the law (Mastrofski et  al., 1996; Tyler eration in crime reduction and events that
and Fagan, 2008). Thus, training the police allow police and citizens to interact without a
in conflict- and violence-avoidance (i.e. to be power imbalance, such as non-crime-related
more restrained) may reduce the likelihood community events.
that situations will escalate and the officer Another strategy for reducing outgroup dis-
will become engaged in conflict. Such a situ- trust is implicit-bias training, which teaches
ation results in an overall reduction in the risk officers about their biases, and provides
of harm to all persons involved. strategies for identifying and overcoming the
impact of bias on behavior. Diversification of
the police force also has potential to reduce
outgroup suspicion, due to promotion of ties
GROUNDING POLICE TRAINING IN to both the police and minority communities.
EVOLUTIONARY THEORY Relatedly, promoting minority individuals
to positions of power where they influence
With respect to threat response, training for decisions and represent the police profession
reducing unnecessary or excessive force has promise for reducing outgroup distrust
could focus on officers’ concerns about and suspicion. Although promising, these
danger, and reduce the amount of training strategies have yet to be evaluated for effec-
that promotes rapid response over reasoned tiveness at promoting inter-group collabora-
decision making. For example, ambush train- tive relationships between the police and the
ing that teaches an officer that it is beneficial citizenry.
to be constantly alert may help an officer The theories of dominance, prestige, and
during the (statistically) unlikely situation status seeking also have relevance to police
that they are ambushed. However, it is also training. It is easy to train officers to exhibit
likely to increase the risk of officers rapidly dominant control behavior. This is because
responding to a perceived threat without evi- this training aligns with evolved psychology.
dence that a threat exists. It is unlikely that In the ancestral environment, however, the
officers in the field will favor critical deci- expectation related to an individual’s domi-
sion making over rapid threat response unless nance behavior is that if the individual wins,
they have been taught to do so. Deadly-force the opponent submits. This is complicated
judgment and decision-making training in the modern environment because police
(either simulation or role-play based) can authority is granted institutionally, and not
help promote critical decision making over based on a direct competition between indi-
rapid response, due to the consequence of viduals. In other words, no contest has estab-
‘getting it wrong’. lished that the police officer is dominant to
Police training can also reduce officers’ the citizen, so it is naïve to assume that the
natural distrust of ‘outgroups’ via exposure. citizen will always submit. The citizen might
As the Robbers Cave experiments demon- have strong reasons for not submitting (e.g.
strate, competing groups can put aside differ- gaining prestige and social status among
ences and overcome hostility when faced with peers). This can result in challenge to the
a common problem requiring a cooperative police authority which, in turn, the police
solution. Community-orienteered policing are unlikely to submit to. Police training that
(COP) strategies that focus on shared goals, demands officers exert dominance will in
such as improving the safety of people living these instances lead to escalation.
in the community, have potential to discour- Alternatively, training that teaches offic-
age police perceptions of citizens as rivals ers to reduce the likelihood of unnecessary
216 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

dominance contests can prevent the need and towards cooperation results in more
for aggression. There are situations when effective police work. The traditional ‘police
it is appropriate and expected for an officer persona’ of aggression, authority, and mas-
to be a hawk and take immediate control of culinity takes the strategy of deterrence to
a situation, regardless of escalation conse- prevent challenges to dominance (‘my RHP
quences. For example, if a suspect is armed is bigger than your RHP’). The evidence on
and posing a deadly threat to those around the effectiveness of this strategy, however,
them, an officer must neutralize that threat, is limited, and evidence exists that coopera-
quickly and decisively. The vast majority of tive strategies are more effective (Tyler and
police–citizen interactions, however, do not Fagan, 2008). For example, the research liter-
require the police to be forceful. Many of ature on procedural justice demonstrates that
these interactions, particularly for police offic- officers who treat people fairly, with dignity
ers that work in ‘anti-cop’ neighborhoods, will and respect, listen to them, and work towards
be fraught with challenges to police authority. mutually beneficial outcomes are more likely
Policing scholar Van Maanen (1978) describes to gain voluntary compliance and avoid use-
‘the asshole’ or citizen who is not inclined to of-force encounters (Tyler and Fagan, 2008).
submit to police authority. When facing such
a citizen, the officer can either engage in a
dominance challenge or not. In the absence of
criminal behavior, the officer has no grounds CONCLUSION
to force a citizen to submit to their authority
(Klinger, 1994), and doing so will escalate Bittner (1970) defined the role of the police
the situation. Unfortunately for the police, not in terms of their legitimate authority to exer-
everyone will like them, and taking that per- cise coercive force. Police culture has
sonally is both unprofessional and unsafe. embraced this definition, shaping training
De-escalation training has potential for and policy around the use of aggressive
reducing unnecessary dominance con- crime-control techniques. This mindset has
tests between the police and the citizenry. led to strained relationships between police
De-escalation techniques are typically com- and minority communities and a social crisis
munication based, and attempt to influence in the United States characterized by eroded
citizens into voluntary compliance without police legitimacy. Examining police strate-
using force (Humphrey, 2013). They can be gies for selecting tactics through an evolu-
used in volatile crisis encounters (e.g. hostage tionary lens provides a framework that
negotiation) or in day-to-day encounters for re-envisions police function as the expert
decreasing the likelihood that a situation will balance of controlled aggression and restraint.
escalate (e.g. procedural justice). These tech- Aggressive ‘hawk-like’ strategies are appro-
niques emphasize police need to read people, priate in certain situations, but in many
be professional, display empathy, and treat others, restrained ‘dove-like’ strategies will
people with dignity and respect, regardless of be more effective at gaining voluntary com-
their attitude towards the police. Evaluations pliance and avoiding unnecessary dominance
of de-escalation techniques show that they contests. Adopting this framework by focus-
improve public perceptions of police legiti- ing more training hours on force alternatives
macy (Todak, 2017). and promoting a culture of de-escalation may
The idea that de-escalation techniques result in police behavior falling more closely
can be used to dissuade citizens from chal- in line with citizens’ expectations of police.
lenging police authority has implications for Such a shift could result in a reduction in the
long-term police–community relationships. rate of violence that occurs between police
Influencing citizens away from antagonism and citizens.
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND POLICING 217

REFERENCES Cheng, J. T., Tracy, J. L., & Henrich, J. (2010).


Pride, personality, and the evolutionary
Alpert, G. P., Dunham, R. G., & MacDonald, foundations of human social status. Evolution
J. M. (2004). Interactive police-citizen and Human Behavior, 31(5), 334–347.
encounters that result in force. Police Crank, J. P. (2014). Understanding police
Quarterly, 7(4), 475–488. culture. Philadelphia:Routledge.
Barash, D. P. (2004). The survival game: How Dart, T., & Walters, J. (2016, September 22).
game theory explains the biology of Tulsa police under scrutiny for delayed
medical aid given to Terence Crutch. The
cooperation and competition. New York, NY:
Guardian. Retrieved from www.theguardian.
Macmillan.
com/us-news/2016/sep/22/tulsa-police-
Barash, D. (1982). Sociology and behavior
t e re n c e - c r u t c h e r- m e d i c a l - a s s i s t a n c e.
(2nd ed.). New York, NY: Elsevier.
(Accessed 28 March 2018).
Beaulieu, D. A., & Bugental, D. B. (2007).
Dawkins, R. (1980). Good strategy or
An evolutionary approach to socialization. In
evolutionary stable strategy? In G. W. Barlow
J. E. Grusec & P. D. Hastings (Eds.), Handbook
& J. Silverberg (Eds.), Sociobiology: Beyond
of Socialization (pp. 71–95). New York:
Nature/Nurture, Westview Press, Boulder,
Guilford Press.
Colorado, pp. 331–367.
Binder, A., & Scharf, P. (1980). The violent
DeBenedetti, C. (1980). The peace reform in
police-citizen encounter. The ANNALS of the
American history. Bloomington, IN: Indiana
American Academy of Political and Social
University Press.
Science, 452(1), 111–121.
Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1961). The fighting behavior
Bittner, E. (1970). The functions of the police in of animals. Scientific American, 205,
modern society. Bethesda, MD: National 112–122.
Institute of Mental Health. Fachner, G., & Carter, S. (2015). An assessment
Bittner, E. (1974). Florence Nightingale in of deadly force in the Philadelphia Police
pursuit of Willie Sutton: A theory of the Department (Collaborative Reform Initiative).
police. In H. Jacob (Ed.), Potential for Reform Washington, DC: Office of Community
of Criminal Justice (pp. 17–44). Beverly Hills, Oriented Policing Services, US. Department
CA: Sage. of Justice.
Blumenstein, L., Fridell, L., & Jones, S. (2012). Federal Bureau of Investigation. (2018).
The link between traditional police sub- Uniform Crime Report, January–June 2018.
culture and police intimate partner violence. Retrieved from https://ucr.fbi.gov/crime-in-
Policing: An International Journal of the-u.s/2018/preliminary-report. (Accessed
Police Strategies & Management, 35(1), 28 October 2018)
147–164. Felson, R. B., & Steadman, H. J. (1983).
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss. New Situational factors in disputes leading to
York, NY: Basic Books. criminal violence. Criminology, 21(1),
Brown, M. K. (1988). Working the street: 59–74.
Police discretion and the dilemmas of reform. Francis, R. C. (1988). On the relationship
New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation. between aggression and social dominance.
Bukowski, W. M., Buhrmester, D., & Ethology, 78(3), 223–237.
Underwood, M. K. (2011). Peer relations as Fyfe, J. J. (1986). The split-second syndrome
a developmental context. In M. K. and other determinants of police violence. In
Underwood & L. H. Rosen (Eds.), Social A. T. Campbell & J. J. Gibbs (Eds.), Violent
Development: Relationships in Infancy, Transactions (pp. 207–225). Oxford, UK:
Childhood, and Adolescence (pp. 153–179). Basil Blackwell.
New York, NY: Guilford Press. Fyfe, J. J. (1996). Training to reduce police-
Buss, D. M., & Shackelford, T. K. (1997). Human citizen violence. In W. A. Geller & H. Toch
aggression in evolutionary psychological (Eds.), Police Violence: Understanding and
perspective. Clinical Psychology Review, Controlling Police Abuse of Force. New
17(6), 605–619. Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
218 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Geary, D. C. (1999). Evolution and making. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience,


developmental sex differences. Current 8, 512.
Directions in Psychological Science, 8(4), Jones, J. M. (2015, June 29). In U.S., confidence
115–120. in police lowest in 22 years. Gallup. Retrieved
Geary, D. C., Byrd-Craven, J., Hoard, M. K., from www.gallup.com/poll/183704/
Vigil, J., & Numtee, C. (2003). Evolution confidence-police-lowest-years.aspx
and development of boys’ social behavior. (Accessed 25 July 2017).
Developmental Review, 23(4), 444–470. Kappeler, V. E., Sluder, R. D., & Alpert, G. P.
Gest, T. (2016, July 8). Is a ‘police legitimacy (1998). Forces of deviance: Understanding
crisis’ driving homicides up? The Crime the dark side of policing (2nd ed.). Long
Report. Retrieved from http://thecrimereport. Grove, IL: Waveland Press.
org/2016/07/08/is-a-police-legitimacy-crisis- Klinger, D. A. (1994). Demeanor or crime? Why
driving-homicides-up/ (Accessed 3 March ‘hostile’ citizens are more likely to be
2018). arrested. Criminology, 32(3), 475–493.
Hardin, G. (1968). Tragedy of the Commons. Knauft, B. M., Daly, M., Wilson, M., Donald, L.,
Science, 162, 1243–1248. Morren Jr, G. E., Otterbein, K. F., & van
Henrich, J., & Gil-White, F. J. (2001). The Wetering, W. (1987). Reconsidering violence
evolution of prestige: Freely conferred in simple human societies: Homicide among
deference as a mechanism for enhancing the the Gebusi of New Guinea [and comments
benefits of cultural transmission. Evolution and reply]. Current Anthropology, 28(4),
and Human Behavior, 22(3), 165–196. 457–500.
Humphrey, J. (2013, November 19). State Krebs, J. & Davies, N. (1984). Behavioural
police academy building guardians instead of ecology: An evolutionary approach. Oxford:
warriors. KXLY News. Retrieved from https:// Blackwell Scientific Publications.
www.kxly.com/state-police-academy- Lorenz, K. (1952). King Solomon’s ring. London,
building-guardians-instead-of-warriors/ UK: Methuen.
(Accessed 3 March 2018). Mastrofski, S. D., Snipes, J. B., & Supina, A. E.
James, L. (2017). The stability of implicit (1996). Compliance on demand: The public’s
racial bias in police officers. Police response to specific police requests. Journal
Quarterly. 21(1), 30–52. doi: 10.1177/ of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 33(3),
1098611117732974 269–305.
James, L., Fridell, L., & Straub, F. (2016, Millan, C. (2006). Cesar’s way. New York, NY:
February). Implicit bias versus the ‘Ferguson Harmony Books.
Effect’: Psychosocial factors impacting Nesse, R. M. (2005). Natural selection and the
officers’ decisions to use deadly force. The regulation of defenses: A signal detection
Police Chief, 83, 44–51. analysis of the smoke detector principle.
James, L. James, S., & Vila, B. (2018). Testing Evolution and Human Behavior, 26(1),
the impact of citizen characteristics and 88–105.
demeanor on police officer behavior in Neuberg, S. L., Kenrick, D. T., & Schaller, M.
potentially violent encounters. Policing: An (2010). Evolutionary social psychology.
International Journal of Police Strategies & In S. T. Fiske, D. Gilbert, & G. Lindzey
Management, 41(1), 24–40. https://doi. (Eds.), Handbook of Social Psychology
org/10.1108/PIJPSM-11-2016-0159 (pp. 761–796). New York: Wiley.
James, L., Todak, N., & Savage, J. (2018). Paoline, E. A. (2003). Taking stock: Toward a
Unnecessary force by police: Insights from richer understanding of police culture.
evolutionary psychology. Policing: A Journal Journal of Criminal Justice, 31(3),
of Policy and Practice. 14(1), 278–291. 199–214.
Johnson, R. R., Stone, B. T., Miranda, C. M., Paoline, E. A., & Terrill, W. (2005). The impact
Vila, B., James, L., James, S. M., & Berka, C. of police culture on traffic stop searches: An
(2014). Identifying psychophysiological analysis of attitudes and behavior. Policing:
indices of expert vs. novice performance in An International Journal of Police Strategies
deadly force judgment and decision & Management, 28(3), 455–472.
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND POLICING 219

Paquette, D. (2015). An evolutionary Savage, J., & Kanazawa, S. (2004). Social


perspective on antisocial behavior: Evolution capital and the human psyche: Why is social
as a foundation for criminological theories. life ‘capital’? Sociological Theory, 22(3),
In J. Morizot & L. Kazemian (Eds.), 504–524.
The Development of Criminal and Antisocial Scharf, P., & Binder, A. (1983). The badge and
Behavior (pp. 315–330). New York: the bullet: Police use of deadly force. New
Springer. York, NY: Praeger.
Peck, J. H. (2015). Minority perceptions of the Shaw, S. J., & Shaw, E. K. (1977). History of the
police: A state-of-the-art review. Policing: An Ottoman Empire and Modern Turkey:
International Journal of Police Strategies & Volume 2, Reform, Revolution, and Republic:
Management, 38(1), 173–203. The Rise of Modern Turkey 1808–1975.
Pinizzotto, A. J., Davis, E. F., & Miller, C. E. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
(2006). Violent encounters: A study of Sherif, M., Harvey, O. J., White, B. J., Hood, W.
felonious assaults on our nation’s law R., & Sherif, C. (1961). The Robbers Cave
enforcement officers (No. NCJ 231272). experiment: Intergroup conflict and
Washington, DC: US Department of Justice, cooperation. Institute of Group Relations,
Federal Bureau of Investigation. University of Oklahoma.
Police Executive Research Forum. (2015). Silver, J. R., Roche, S. P., Bilach, T. J., &
Re-engineering training on police use of Bontrager Ryon, S. (2017). Traditional police
force (Critical Issues in Policing). Washington culture, use of force, and procedural justice:
DC: Police Executive Research Forum. Investigating individual, organizational, and
Retrieved from https://www.policeforum. contextual factors. Justice quarterly, 34(7),
org/assets/reengineeringtraining1.pdf 1272–1309.
President’s Task Force on 21st Century Policing. Smith, J. M. (1974). The theory of games and
(2015). Final Report of the President’s Task the evolution of animal conflicts. Journal of
Force on 21st Century Policing. Washington, Theoretical Biology, 47(1), 209–221.
DC: Office of Community Oriented Policing Smith, J. M. (1982). Evolution and the Theory
Services. of Games. Cambridge University Press.
Pusatory, M. (2016, August 10). Watch: Terrill, W., & Paoline, E. A. (2015). Citizen
Spokane officer shows amazing patience complaints as threats to police legitimacy:
dealing with intoxicated man. Fox 28. The role of officers’ occupational attitudes.
Retrieved from https://www.khq.com/news/ Journal of Contemporary Criminal Justice,
watch-spokane-officer-shows-amazing- 31(2), 192–211.
patience-dealing-with-intoxicated-man/ Terrill, W., Paoline, E. A., & Manning, P. K.
article_616ebc29-947b-58d5-937c- (2003). Police culture and coercion.
ccf1cfbb50e8.html (Accessed 25 July Criminology, 41(4), 1003–1034.
2017). Thrasher, F. M. (1936). The boys’ club and
Puurtinen, M., Heap, S., & Mappes, T. (2015). juvenile delinquency. American Journal of
The joint emergence of group competition Sociology, 42(1), 66–80.
and within-group cooperation. Evolution Todak, N. (2017). De-escalation in police-citizen
and Human Behavior, 36(3), 211–217. encounters: A mixed methods study of a
Rahr, S., & Rice, S. K. (2015). From warriors to misunderstood policing strategy
guardians: Recommitting American police (Dissertation). Arizona State University,
culture to democratic ideals (No. NCJ 24865). Phoenix, AZ.
Laurel, MD: National Institute of Justice and Tyler, T. R., & Fagan, J. (2008). Legitimacy and
the Harvard Kennedy School Program in cooperation: Why do people help the police
Criminal Justice Policy and Management. fight crime in their communities? Ohio State
Reiman, J. (1985). The social contract and the Journal of Criminal Law, 6, 231–275.
police use of deadly force. In F. A. Ellison & Van Maanen, J. (1978). The asshole. In P. K.
M. Feldberg (Eds.), Moral Issues in Police Manning & J. V. Maanen (Eds.) Policing: A
Work. (pp. 237–249) Savage, MD: Rowman View from the Street (pp. 221–238). Santa
& Littlefield Publishers. Monica, CA: Goodyear.
220 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Walker, S. (1977). A critical history of police Wolpert, S. A. (1962). Tilak and Gokhale:
reform. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books. Revolution and reform in the making of
Walker, S. (2001). Police accountability: The role modern India. Berkeley and Los Angeles, CA:
of citizen oversight. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. University of California Press.
White, M. D., & Fradella, H. F. (2016). Stop and Yabuta, S. (2008). Evolution of cross-contextual
frisk: The use and abuse of a controversial displays: The role of risk of inappropriate
policing tactic. New York, NY: New York attacks on nonopponents, such as partners.
University Press. Animal Behaviour, 76(3), 865–870.
11
Evolutionary Psychology,
Jurisprudence, and Sentencing
Eyal Aharoni and Morris B. Hoffman

I. JURISPRUDENCE AND in the Enlightenment’s decidedly mixed


PUNISHMENT1 version of this controversy. The Constitution’s
distribution of powers between different
Two thousand years ago, Socrates and an branches of government was a reflection of the
Athenian Sophist named Thrasymachus began framers’ nuanced views about human nature.
a famous debate about human nature and the We are good enough to be largely free of an
overbearing state, but not quite good enough
meaning of justice (Plato, 380 BCE). Are
to live without a state or to populate it without
humans fundamentally good, built to cooperate
checks and balances between its parts.
and to appreciate beauty and truth, as Socrates
Until the paradigm-shifting insights of
contended, and therefore perhaps in need only
evolutionary psychology, behavioral eco-
of modest and occasional intervention by the
nomics, and the other disciplines described in
state? Or are we fundamentally bad, built only Section II of this chapter, most modern takes
to maximize our self-interest, as Thrasymachus on human nature have been skewed heavily
argued, and therefore probably in need of toward Thrasymachus’ dreary views. The
heavy-handed restraint by a robust state? Is central simplifying assumption of classical
justice ‘the excellence of the soul’ (Plato, 380 economics was that each of us is relentlessly
BCE: 297) or nothing more than ‘the interest of self-interested. Markets are an efficient inte-
the stronger’(Plato, 380 BCE: 275)? gration of all those individual greedy unseen
Humans have been having these same hands. Darwin’s insights strengthened the
debates ever since, and the ways we have belief that we are self-interest machines, and
resolved them have largely defined our political biology’s great synthesis of evolution and
and legal institutions. It is no coincidence that genetics simply moved the locus of that self-
the founders of the United States were steeped interest from the selfish individual animal
222 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

down to the animal’s selfish genes. Freud satisfied with any systems that depended
did much the same for the psyche, and Marx on unexaminable axioms of theology, natu-
for the allegedly relentless march of eco- ral law began to fall out of fashion. But
nomic and political history, framing the wars there have been, and continue to be, modern
between self-interested classes. efforts to re-ground natural law in concepts
But then something delightful happened of secular morality, most prominently by Lon
on this dark road to modern pessimism. Fuller (Fuller, 1965) and Ronald Dworkin
Some economists, anthropologists, and psy- (Dworkin, 1986). We will see similar re-
chologists had the audacity to look systemati- kindled interests in secular morality from the
cally at how humans actually behave instead normativists and even some positivists.
of how these dreary theories predicted we Even though natural law’s fundamental
should behave. And there were many surpris- principle is that law is a formal expression
ing results. They discovered that when we of morality, that position does not mean that
play economic games in the laboratory, we all natural-law theorists take Socrates’ side
engage in all kinds of cooperative behaviors in the debate about human nature. For exam-
that cannot be explained by classical eco- ple, Thomas Hobbes, a prominent Scottish
nomics, including sharing with and trusting proponent of natural law whose views were
other players, even when they are strangers. important to the founders of the United
They discovered that we are not only not the States, believed humankind’s natural tenden-
rational self-interest machines conceived by cies were toward selfishness and violence,
classical economic theory, but that our appar- tendencies that needed curbing by a power-
ent irrationality is predictable in many impor- ful state. Hobbes’ view of right and wrong
tant decision-making domains. As discussed seems presciently Darwinian; he once wrote
in more detail in Section II below, all these that man is forbidden by natural law ‘to do
observations were then linked to the theoreti- that which is destructive to his life, to take
cal insights of evolutionary psychology. away the means of preserving the same, or to
These same ancient and modern debates omit that by which he thinks it may best be
about human nature and the meaning of jus- preserved’ (Hobbes, 1651).
tice have echoed throughout the philosophy The second school of jurisprudence is called
of law, also called ‘jurisprudence’. There are ‘legal positivism’. Like Thrasymachus before
four main schools of jurisprudence – natural them, legal positivists rejected the fusing
law, legal positivism, legal realism, and nor- together of law and morality. The ‘positivism’
mative law – all addressed to these funda- simply means that laws are rules posited by
mental questions. man, not by God. They are social constructs
Natural-law theorists view law in a way and nothing more. The role of the legal posi-
that is analogous to how natural philoso- tivist is to describe those social constructs and
phers viewed the physical universe: there to analyze them non-normatively, especially
are a priori principles (like Socrates’ truth, the processes by which laws come into being
beauty, and justice) that animate legal sys- (Coleman and Leiter, 2010). There are many
tems just as physical laws (gravity, conserva- different subspecies of legal positivism, some
tion of momentum) animate the universe, and of which vary by the degree to which they pay
humankind’s job is to discover these natural attention to moral groundings (Gardner, 2001).
laws and apply them (Bix, 2010; Haakonssen, One of the most prominent modern positivists
1996). Natural law need not be grounded in was H. L. A. Hart (Hart, 1961), who famously
the divine, although its most famous his- debated Lon Fuller in the Harvard Law Review
torical proponents – Socrates, Aristotle, in 1958 (Fuller, 1958; Hart, 1958).
and Thomas Aquinas – of course did so. As The third school of jurisprudence is ‘legal
modern philosophers generally became less realism’. Legal realists, like positivists, take a
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY, JURISPRUDENCE, AND SENTENCING 223

non-normative view of law. But unlike most law. The one we consider here is criminal
positivists, they are not satisfied with simply sentencing.
accepting the conventions of law as given The debates between the four schools of
social constructs. Instead, most legal realists jurisprudence in some ways echo, and in
are interested in the psychological engines other ways are distinct from, the justifying
that drive legislatures to adopt laws and theories that underlie criminal punishment.
judges to interpret them (Leiter, 2010). The There are four main justifications for crimi-
law and economics phenomenon – using the nal punishment: retribution, rehabilitation,
insights of economics to study the relation- deterrence, and incapacitation (Alschuler,
ship between law and its intended and unin- 2003; Davis, 2009).
tended consequences – is a modern branch of Retributivists generally believe that pun-
legal realism. There are many other branches, ishment is its own categorical good, and there-
including perhaps the most extreme one, fore it need not accomplish, or be validated
usually credited to Oliver Wendell Holmes, by, any external effects. Hence, the other
Jr. In Holmes’ view, law is nothing more three theories, in contrast to retribution, are
than a prediction of the results of adjudica- sometimes lumped together as ‘utilitarian’.
tion. The promise contained in a contract, Retribution is the oldest punishment theory,
under this view, means nothing other than a though its formalization is generally attrib-
prediction that whoever breaks the promise uted to the German philosopher Immanuel
will have to pay damages. To Holmes, law Kant (1797). One can hear legal naturalism
is power dressed up in process (Alschuler, within retribution. Precisely because there
2000). Critical legal theory and its cousin, is a natural core of right and wrong, punish-
critical race theory, are modern versions of ment is as much a part of that a priori core as
this extreme legal realism. the right and wrong itself. But there are also
The fourth and most recent school of juris- positivist and realist currents in retribution.
prudence is normative law. The normativ- Kant’s intellectual successor, Georg Hegel,
ists, somewhat like Fuller’s and Dworkin’s once wrote that criminals must be punished
secular naturalism and the more morally simply to earn their way back into the social
interested positivists, focus on the founda- fold. Crime is a breach of the social contract,
tions and justifications of the law. They are which requires that the breacher pay damages
not satisfied either with the strict positivist in the form of suffering (Hegel, 1820).
assumption that laws are just arbitrary social Deterrence was the first utilitarian rebel-
constructs or with the realists’ fascination lion against retribution. Utilitarians like
with the levers of power. They examine the Cesare Beccaria (1766) and Jeremy Bentham
purposes of law, and study methods to meas- (1830) believed that no social group had
ure whether those purposes have been opti- the right to inflict punishment on its mem-
mized (Shiner, 2010). bers unless that punishment resulted in a net
All you non-philosophers out there are social good. For proponents of deterrence,
probably wondering what in the world any punishing wrongdoers is permissible because
of this esoteric theorizing has to do either it deters both the person being punished (spe-
with evolutionary psychology or with how cial deterrence) as well as others (general
the law actually operates on the ground. deterrence) from committing future crimes.
The answer is that jurisprudence and evolu- Early proponents of deterrence focused on
tionary psychology are both concerned with special deterrence. Bentham once famously
human nature. As evolutionary psychology asserted that no state had the right to pun-
is impacting our fundamental view of what ish any criminal, even a murderer, if it could
it means to be human, those impacts portend be certain the criminal would never com-
important consequences in many areas of the mit another crime (Bentham, 1830). But of
224 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

course, punishment that might not deter the for most serious crimes. Penitentiaries were
wrongdoer may nevertheless deter millions not originally intended to be criminal ware-
of others, and thus be a net good. Modern houses. They were a place for criminals to
proponents of deterrence therefore tend to contemplate their crimes and futures – to be
focus on general deterrence rather than spe- penitent. Prison sentences for non-capital
cial deterrence. Both kinds are an amalgam crimes in these early years were extraordinar-
of different jurisprudential schools. Like ily mild by modern standards – months rather
the realists and normativists, the deterrence than years.
school of punishment is focused on the way When rehabilitation ascended at the turn
law accomplishes, or fails to accomplish, its of the century these mild sentences became
central purpose of deterring crime. quite harsh. Cures took time. The length of
Rehabilitation is deterrence writ small. It prison sentences skyrocketed. Deterrence
focuses not on deterring the population as a and incapacitation theories, especially promi-
whole, or even the particular criminal being nent in the 1970s as rehabilitation fell out of
sentenced, but rather on treating criminals to favor, further boosted sentence lengths.
make them better persons who are less likely These theories have all survived to some
to commit future crimes. Rehabilitation extent. Today, in all federal and virtually all
became pre-eminent in US law at the begin- state courts, judges are expressly directed to
ning of the 20th century, as progressives consider all of them when imposing a sen-
viewed crime as mostly a product of failed tence. The federal statute, 18 U.S.C. § 3553(a),
social systems, and criminals as socially dis- is typical. It provides that federal judges
eased and in need of a cure. It began to fall
shall consider … the need for the sentence
out of favor in the late 1960s (Allen, 1978;
imposed:
Alschuler, 2003).
Incapacitation is the most recent justifica- a. to reflect the seriousness of the offense, to
tion for criminal punishment, at least when promote respect for the law, and to provide for
it comes to non-capital crimes. It ascended just punishment for the offense [retribution];
b. to afford adequate deterrence to criminals
as a theory of punishment beginning in [general deterrence];
the 1960s as rehabilitation began to wane. c. to protect the public from further crimes of
Incapacitation, like special deterrence and the defendant [special deterrence and
rehabilitation, is focused not on the popula- incapacitation];
tion as a whole but on the individual criminal. d. to provide the defendant with needed educa-
tional or vocational training, medical care, or
Incapacitationists believe the primary pur- other correctional treatment in the most effec-
pose of punishment is to remove criminals tive manner [rehabilitation].
from society so that they do not commit more
crimes. Unlike the other three theories of The idea of this kitchen-sink approach is
punishment, which attempt in different ways undoubtedly that different kinds of cases will
to remove people’s desire to commit future tend to command different punishment goals.
crimes, incapacitation is designed to remove But how ought we resolve conflicts between
their ability. these goals? For example, how should we
These theories had important, real-life deal with an offender who is morally culpa-
effects on the way we have treated criminals. ble but not dangerous? Or dangerous but not
When retribution was king, sentences in the morally culpable? The theories don’t tell us
United States were generally, and perhaps when one kind of goal should predominate
surprisingly, quite mild in length. Indeed, the over another. And if we need to consider all
Quakers invented the penitentiary in the late four goals, as most statutes command, none
1700s as a more merciful alternative to death of the theories explains how judges are to
and banishment, which were the punishments integrate the theories’ incompatible goals.
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY, JURISPRUDENCE, AND SENTENCING 225

If we punish only to deter, then why do we The evolutionary insights discussed in


often punish serious wrongdoers who have Section II below address these central criti-
low recidivism rates (murderers, for exam- cisms of retribution: our core moral intuitions
ple) more than wrongdoers with astronomi- evolved, and one of those intuitions is to punish
cal recidivism rates (forgers, for example)? serious violations of the other moral intuitions.
Similarly, if the main goal of punishment
is to rehabilitate, then why do we punish
Bentham’s already rehabilitated killer at all?
If we only punish to incapacitate, then why II. SCIENTIFIC PERSPECTIVES ON
doesn’t every criminal get a life sentence, THE ULTIMATE AND PROXIMATE
or a sentence gauged only to the risks of his FUNCTIONS OF PUNISHMENT
recidivating? It seems like something else,
something non-utilitarian, is animating our To predict where a storm front is heading, it
actual sentencing practices. helps to understand its initial conditions. The
There’s a wonderful thought experiment same is true for human nature. According to
(Alschuler, 2003) that sheds light on the the perspective of evolutionary psychology,
incompleteness of all the utilitarian theories human behavior is enabled by brains that
and hints that our retributivist urges run very solved ecological problems – problems such as
deep and need to be acknowledged. Imagine finding and maintaining good cooperation
a society in which all first-degree murder- partners – favored by natural selection. So
ers are executed as part of the halftime show consideration of the problems that our brains
at the Super Bowl. The method of execu- evolved to solve can help to explain and pre-
tion is a laser beam that we are told inflicts dict the motivations that shape our social insti-
unimaginably excruciating pain on the pris- tutions, including, perhaps, the justice system.
oner for several minutes before he is termi- It is through this lens that it makes sense
nally vaporized. But unbeknownst to us, the to ask what an organism would stand to gain
‘killer’ beam is really a painless transporter from being punitive. Punishing other peo-
beam, and it sends the prisoners to an idyl- ple is costly. At minimum, it takes time and
lic island in the South Seas where they live energy, and it exposes the punisher to the
out their lives in luxury but can never return. risk of retaliation. So, unless a given punish-
This system should be fine with true utilitar- ment strategy tended to confer proportionally
ians. Deterrence is maximized, the murder- large benefits to the punisher, such a strategy
ers are rehabilitated (at least in the sense of would be unlikely to have evolved by natural
being sent to a perfect society without social selection. Yet punishment has been a ubiqui-
wants), and they are likewise incapacitated tous part of human society (Brown, 1991). A
from ever hurting the rest of us again. But growing body of empirical scholarship has
there is something deeply unsettling about thus sought to identify the potential benefits
such an approach. It is not fair. A retributivist that common punishment behaviors are, in an
would say that the murderers are not getting ultimate sense, designed to procure. Here, we
their just deserts (and in fact they are being discuss some key results from this scholarship
rewarded for their heinous crimes). as well as their added value for our ancestors
But retribution comes with its own incom- and possibly for modern legal institutions.
pleteness issues. How much desert is just, and
why? Even more fundamentally, retribution
seems to be built on the same unsatisfactory Second-Party Punishment
a priori sands as natural law. Philosophers
might say that retribution has no antecedent Among the various forms of punishment that
moral justifications. It just is, like gravity. occurs in human societies, the simplest is
226 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

second-party punishment: when a victim A third way in which a victim could b­ enefit
retaliates against the victimizer. Retaliation from retaliation is by transforming the cheater
is risky business. Yet it’s a common response into a valuable cooperation partner. This char-
to cheating and aggression in humans and acterization is known as the theory of direct
even some non-human animals (Clutton- reciprocity, proposed by Trivers, and there
Brock and Parker, 1995; Jensen et al., 2007). is considerable evidence for its operation in
So, what are the benefits to be gained by both human and non-human species (Trivers,
second-party punishment? The evolutionary 1971). For this strategy to be effective, the
psychology literature provides at least three imposition of costs on the cheater would
notable answers. have to ‘educate’ the cheater by altering his
First, retaliation, if deployed effectively, incentive structure for how he conducts future
provides self-defense. It can serve as a precau- interactions with the punisher. In other words,
tionary strategy for managing a hazard in the the punishment, or at least a credible threat
environment (Fiddick et al., 2000), not unlike of punishment, must appeal to his psychol-
our instinct to shut our eyes in a dust storm. ogy of deterrence. If successful, this strategy
In this way, one need not posit the existence not only protects the punisher from further
of an evolved psychology of social interac- exploitation but also enables reciprocal gains
tion because our need for self-preservation in trade between the dyad via increased coop-
extends beyond the social. Indeed, there are eration. Thus, this strategy would commonly
many examples of self-defense across animal target members of one’s own existing social
species, social and non-social alike, includ- network. So, while retaliation might be risky,
ing algae (Paul and Fenical, 1986) and gup- it can also pay dividends.
pies (Godin and Davis, 1995). The most
fundamental form of self-defense may be
the immunological response (Hoffman and Third-Party Punishment
Krueger, 2017). Clearly, at least some of our
evolved psychology may support retaliatory Third-party punishment:
behavior, not to interface with the cheater’s why bother?
motivations or perceptions, but more simply Human punishment behavior, of course, is
to protect oneself from harm. not limited to two-party contexts. Across his-
Another potential benefit to be gleaned tory and across cultures, punishment by
from retaliating could be to gain a direct-­ ostensibly independent ‘third’ parties is per-
fitness advantage over the cheater. An impulse vasive. In the ancestral environment, third-
to ostracize, kill, or otherwise debilitate the party punishment would have been delivered
cheater could directly advance one’s own by members of the victim’s broader commu-
prospects, if deployed successfully (Trivers, nity, usually small coalitions of men who,
1971). However, since such actions invite together, could deliver punishment at lesser
dangerous countermeasures and thwart the risk to any one individual. In modern socie-
possibility of future cooperation, this strat- ties, such activities have been largely dele-
egy would seem to be limited to situations in gated to social institutions like the criminal
which continued victimization is assessed to justice system, which empower disinterested
be highly likely but the likelihood of mutual triers of fact, like judges and jurors, to deliver
cooperation is low. In the environment in third-party punishment.
which our punishment psychology evolved, The fact that punishment is so costly is
situations like this would have been more particularly problematic for evolutionary
common when the offender was a member of theories of third-party punishment since third
a competing coalition rather than a member parties are defined as people who are not
of one’s own social group. involved in the offense and so do not stand
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY, JURISPRUDENCE, AND SENTENCING 227

to gain directly from the punishment. Why explanation is strong reciprocity. According
should they bother? The answer may lie in to this view, altruistic punishment can evolve
the fact that they are not really independent because groups of people who expressed
parties after all. Supposed third parties may this trait deterred cheaters and, thus, enjoyed
have implicit social motivations driving their more cooperation than those who did not
punitive attitudes even if they do not have (Gintis, 2000). Support for this theory has
conscious access to those motivations. This is been argued on the basis of a series of eco-
because, being human, their minds evolved in nomic game studies. In these studies, partici-
a hyper-social context. People with traits that pants who engaged in a repeated public-goods
enabled them to exploit the social market- game demonstrate a willingness to punish
place gained a fitness advantage. So, it is rel- third-party defectors at a cost to themselves,
evant to ask, in an ultimate sense, how these and this behavior sustained a norm of coop-
so-called third parties could stand to gain eration across the group of players (Fehr
from particular types of punishment behav- and Fischbacher, 2004; Fehr and Gächter,
ior, and how these gains might undergird 2002; Fehr et  al., 2002). Since punishment
modern legal justifications for punishment. of this sort must effectively discourage the
A rich body of literature provides some cheater or other tempted onlookers in order
empirical answers to this question. The first is to be selected, this theory is compatible with
kin selection, which predicts altruism toward the legal justifications of special and gen-
genetic relatives. As expressed by the highly eral deterrence. Despite its intuitive appeal,
influential Hamilton’s Rule, the degree of scholars disagree about the viability of such
relatedness (given the modest assumption an altruistic strategy given that groups that
that our ancestors could recognize their kin punish would be vulnerable to exploitation by
with some accuracy) predicts the degree second-order cheaters, namely, people who
of altruism because such altruistic behav- shirk their contribution to the enforcement
iors increase the replication of our genes of punishment (Krasnow et al., 2015; Tooby
(Hamilton, 1964). Under these conditions, and Cosmides, 2016). There is a burden, crit-
our tolerance for risk should increase – a ics argue, to explain how the enforcement of
prediction well-captured by the quip cred- punishment ever gained an evolutionary foot-
ited to biologist J. B. S. Haldane that he’d hold, given that it’s individually costly.
be prepared to die for two brothers or eight To explain how third-party punishment
cousins (Connolly and Martlew, 1999: 10). could benefit the individual punisher, one
Thus, costly punishments that deter or inca- theory – social exchange theory – highlights
pacitate actors from cheating our kin should the implicit value of the cheater and victim to
be favored by natural selection. Indeed, evi- the punisher. According to this theory, since
dence for kin-based punishment has been humans evolved in small social exchange
widely documented (Daly and Wilson, 1988). networks, most people that we encountered
Since kin-based punishment must effectively on a day-to-day basis would have been net-
prevent or discourage cheating in order to be work members (i.e., direct resources to us),
selected, it is compatible with the incapacita- and so we operate on assumptions that our
tive and deterrence-based philosophical justi- peers, and even most strangers, are poten-
fications for punishment. tial exchange partners (Delton et  al., 2011;
Of course, punishments by judges and Krasnow et al., 2013). Under this constraint,
jurors are not supposed to favor kin. Indeed, if an actor cheats a victim, ‘third parties’
the law specifically bars judges and jurors who punish the cheater gain a direct advan-
from serving in such cases. So an additional tage in social capital by deterring the cheater.
explanation is needed to explain third-party This hypothesis has received support from
punishment in non-kin contexts. One such multiple methods including experimental
228 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

surveys and economic games with real stakes not require a deterrence psychology – a folk
(Aharoni and Fridlund, 2013; Delton and psychological theory of the cheater’s or other
Krasnow, 2017; Krasnow et al., 2012, 2016; group members’ motivations and incentive
Pedersen et al., 2018; Price et al., 2002). This structure. This makes the incapacitative strat-
theory is a generalization of Trivers’ direct- egy cognitively efficient. However, this strat-
reciprocity theory (1971), and it is most com- egy is costly, partly because it could forestall
patible with the legal justification of special a lifetime of future contributions by the for-
deterrence. mer cheater to the social exchange commu-
Other efforts to explain how third-party nity. Therefore, it should have evolved to be
punishment could evolve at the individual used as a last resort, and only when the costs
level point to the reputational benefits of pun- imposed by the cheater are expected to out-
ishing. Known as indirect reciprocity, this weigh his lifetime contributions.
theory states that by punishing a cheater, third Another, potentially less risky, way to
parties accrue reputational credit with the vic- limit the probability that the cheater will
tim and other community members, such that cheat is to socially exclude, or ostracize, that
if the third party helps the victim by punishing individual from the group. By removing his
the cheater, someone else may help the third opportunities to trade on the social market,
party in the future (Nowak and Sigmund, this also reduces his opportunities to exploit
2005). People who are willing to enforce cooperative group norms. In a general sense,
costly punishment signal their commitment this action can also be understood as an inca-
to group norms, thereby broadly advertising pacitative strategy, but unlike corporal pun-
their own social status (Frank, 1988). Since ishments, it may be easier to revoke. Thus, it
this strategy is designed to incentivize both would have been a useful strategy in ances-
second and third parties in the network, it is tral environments, which lacked correctional
compatible with the legal justifications of institutions to perform the incapacitative
special and general deterrence. work.
Though incapacitation is a recognized
Third-party Punishment: legal justification for punishment, social-
how is it done? science research suggests that it fails to
Besides asking what a punisher might gain capture a dominant feature of typical pun-
by punishing a cheater, it would be valuable ishment psychology, namely, the desire to
to understand by what manner punishment inspire changes in the cheater’s mental state.
might achieve these ends. Most evolutionary It is this feature of punishment psychology
theories of punishment posit that punishment that ultimately defines the deterrence motive.
increases the punisher’s fitness by reducing The advantage of deterrence strategies over
recidivism, but they are sometimes hazy other strategies like direct-fitness reduction
about the proximate mechanism – how our or ostracism is that, if effective, they imme-
evolved punishment response reduces recidi- diately restore gains in trade between the
vism. Here, we discuss some answers to this former cheater and the social exchange net-
question of mechanism. work because they convert the cheater into a
From an evolutionary perspective, the most cooperator.
direct way to gain a fitness advantage over a Different scholars have characterized this
cheater is by imposing corporal punishments process in slightly different ways. Some have
like injury or death, as we discussed in the described deterrence as a form of coercion
section on second-party punishment. This (e.g., Matravers, 2000). The implication is
strategy assumes a motivation to (temporarily that, while it may be against the cheater’s
or permanently) behave in ways that incapac- personal interest to shift to a more coop-
itate the cheater, but to achieve its aim, it does erative social strategy, the heavy threat of
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY, JURISPRUDENCE, AND SENTENCING 229

punishment may demand it, and so through punishers should be sensitive to signals that
a rational fear of that negative reinforcer, the the cheater had an unfavorable value struc-
cheater is compelled to change his behavior, ture to begin with. This could explain why
against his deeper preferences. Certainly, as punishers care so much about mens rea –
an empirical matter, most people are respon- the mental states at the time of the offense
sive to the threat of punishment, and so this – such as criminal intent and knowledge of
form of deterrence is likely to play a role in the risk of harm (e.g., Aharoni and Fridlund,
crime and punishment psychology today. 2011; Cushman et al., 2013). A punitive sen-
But a moment’s reflection suggests that timent that didn’t care about these mental
there is something deeply unfulfilling about states would end up imposing heavy costs on
a cheater who plays by the rules only because genuine cooperators who had a stroke of bad
of fear of punishment. Instead, we seem luck, like a driver who caused a fatality as the
to crave a genuine mental transformation: result of a brain seizure. Typically, we don’t
we want the cheater to understand how he punish such individuals as harshly, if at all,
harmed us, and to fully endorse and internal- and one reason could be that unlucky peo-
ize the values that guide our self-protective ple who are otherwise morally innocent are
social norms (Nahmias and Aharoni, 2017), not as likely to repeatedly undermine coop-
perhaps in no small part because when we erative norms in the future. The presence of
internalize norms, we can worry less about mental states like criminal intent, in contrast,
catching norm violators. They catch them- are prognostic of future recidivism. In other
selves. Scholars have described this deter- words, a likely proximate function of punish-
rence motive as a form of education or ment is to impose ecologically rational con-
recalibration, namely, to change how much straints on who we should punish.
the cheater values our welfare relative to his The premium that our deterrence psychol-
own (Petersen et  al., 2012; Trivers, 1971). ogy places on the cheater’s mental states
Consistent with this perspective, experimen- reveals another lesson for the law: attempts
tal research demonstrates that we care deeply to deter a cheater by encouraging internali-
about whether the offender understands zation of norms would seem to qualify as a
why he’s being punished. For instance, in form of rehabilitation. As we noted, standard
an experimental stock-market game, par- punishment theory treats deterrence and reha-
ticipants were more satisfied with their pun- bilitation as separate motives for punishment.
ishment of an ostensible cheater when they But both motivations, if delivered effectively,
received evidence that the cheater under- are designed to change behavior by inspiring
stood why he was being punished and even a change in the cheater’s personal goals and
more satisfied when the cheater expressed values. Where they differ is that the deter-
a commitment to stop cheating (Gollwitzer rence motive places greater emphasis on the
and Denzler, 2009). Theoretically, it’s not role of coercion as a motivator for recalibra-
surprising that we would evolve to favor a tion. Whether coercion is any more effective
fundamental transformation in values over a motivator is an empirical question.
a more simple, coerced obedience. This is If coercion simply represents the threat of
because coercion only works in the presence suffering, then as we discussed in the Super
of a credible enforcement threat, whereas a Bowl thought experiment, many people
recalibration of one’s values is self-enforcing. would be deeply unsatisfied by rehabilitation
In this sense, effective recalibration har- sans suffering. But if we could achieve such
nesses the power of first-party punishment, ends without the harmful means, then why do
which we discuss below. we feel an obligation that the offender should
If punishment is designed to deter cheat- suffer? Why do we boil with outrage when
ers by recalibrating their value structures, he does not? Stated differently, what evolved
230 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

purpose is served by the offender’s suffering punishment are not wholly incompatible –
and the torrent of retributive impulses that they are just operating on different levels of
give rise to it? The answer brings us to our analysis. Thus, the application of evolution-
final point about how our evolved punish- ary theory carries the potential to synthesize
ment psychology carries out its function: via the legally disparate theories for punishment.
sophisticated motivational programs we call
emotions.
As economist Robert Frank (1988) force- First-Party Punishment:
fully argued, emotions serve as commitment Conscience, Guilt, and Shame
devices: they are conspicuous, costly signals
that commit us to a particular course of action. We’ve saved first-party punishment for last
This is well illustrated by a classic anecdote because it might be the most complex, and
of a game of chicken played by two oppos- also because, in important ways, its effective-
ing drivers. If one driver, while revving his ness depends on the other two types. First-
engine, removes his steering wheel and drops party punishment, as the phrase suggests,
it out the window for his opponent to see, could be loosely understood as the tendency
that opponent must take seriously the first to ‘pre-punish’ ourselves with the bite of
driver’s commitment to winning. If retribu- conscience to help us refrain from violating
tive emotions are like our first driver, they norms in the first place.2 Guilt and shame are
can be understood as part of a system that also versions of first-party punishment,
is motivating us toward a particular punish- though they generally operate after we have
ment strategy. The fact that these retributive violated a norm, and serve the function of
emotions are not sated until the offender suf- reducing the chances of violating that norm
fers suggests that the suffering may likewise again. Of course, with an animal as smart and
serve a motivational role for the offender, devious as we humans, there is much overlap
namely to change his offensive behavior by between conscience, guilt, and shame. We
shifting the balance in how much he values might feel guilty or ashamed even for con-
our welfare (Petersen et al., 2010). So, retrib- templating a wrong. Likewise, we might look
utive emotions can be understood as mobi- back on some of our decisions and wonder
lizing punishment and facilitating offender why our conscience did or did not save us
behavior change – functioning to keep both from doing wrong.
parties honest and invested in long-term reci- The bite of conscience is not just a human
procity. The punisher, of course, need not be universal; it is a substantial part of self-image
consciously aware of the functional role of across cultures. It is central to many religions
these retributive emotions in order for them and mythologies (Katchadourian, 2009). It
to exert their effects. was probably a critical solution to the prob-
Conceived in this way, the retributive lem of effectively deterring antisocial behav-
motive for punishment is not in opposition iors in our social networks. At the proximate
to the utilitarian motives, as legal theory level of explanation, one of the reasons you
implies. Rather, it is an expression of them. don’t punch your neighbor during a politi-
In this view, we are psychological retribu- cal argument – perhaps the main reason – is
tivists but adaptive utilitarians (Cushman, not that you are afraid he’ll punch you back or
2015). Retributive emotions commit us to that you will be punished by the criminal jus-
punishment – a costly signal of our status tice system, but rather because you know it is
that enhances the punisher’s fitness, either wrong. Brains with built-in systems that make
directly or indirectly, by modifying the their owners feel bad when they contemplate
cheater’s fitness, rational incentives, or social cheating are brains that will not cheat as often.
values. In this view, the legal purposes of This is what we mean when we said earlier
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY, JURISPRUDENCE, AND SENTENCING 231

that an offender has recalibrated his value In a classic experiment by Wallace and
structure or internalized a norm. Conscience Sadulla (1966), individuals were led to
is the first line of defense against the com- believe they broke an expensive machine,
mission of antisocial behaviors, and at an and the transgression had either been discov-
ultimate level, it worked in concert with the ered or not. The investigators found that com-
other forms of punishment to deter individu- pared to a control group who did not break
als from undermining our ancestors’ precious the machine, the participants whose trans-
social exchange network. Presumably, it had gression was discovered were more likely to
to work in concert with other forms of pun- volunteer for a separate, painful experiment.
ishment because without an external threat of Volunteering for a painful experiment, and
punishment, there would be little incentive to other shame displays, can be understood as
regulate oneself. a costly signal of one’s commitment to the
One way to appreciate how important group, that one has internalized the purpose
conscience is to our cooperative nature is to of punishment, and potentially that second-
study people who don’t seem to have a con- or third-party punishment is unnecessary.
science – psychopaths. Psychopaths tend to This pattern of behavior suggests that self-
have deficiencies in brain regions associated governing psychological systems are in place
with moral decision-making, including affec- but may require social input to be triggered.
tive memory, inhibition, and empathy (Kiehl, This dependency would make sense if our
2007). Unsurprisingly, psychopaths make up self-governing systems coevolved with second-
a hugely disproportionate segment of incar- and third-party punishment systems.
cerated adults, are notoriously resistant to By implication, effective criminal punish-
traditional punitive and rehabilitative treat- ment practices might be those that utilize the
ments, and therefore consume an astonishing natural threat of public exposure to motivate
proportion of our criminal justice resources offenders to recalibrate their internal model
(Kiehl and Hoffman, 2011). A growing body for how to treat others – or at least for how not
of research has also begun to examine whether to get caught. Many legal practices already
certain psychopathic traits might themselves exploit such properties, for instance, in pub-
contain evidence of adaptive design (Glenn lishing the names and addresses of local sex
et al., 2011). Such insights would underscore offenders. But to maximize their effective-
the need to investigate alternative approaches ness, such practices should leverage rather
to behavior modification that better account than subvert our intrinsic, self-regulatory
for individual differences. emotions. If so, such practices could qualify
If conscience evolved to steer us away under the law’s deterrence- and rehabilitation-
from the commission of antisocial behavior, based justifications for punishment.
then guilt and shame evolved to help us learn
from lapses in conscience. Research suggests
that the emotion of guilt and the rumination
that often accompanies it evolved to change III. ANOMALOUS BYPRODUCTS OF
an undesirable pattern of behavior by recali- OUR EVOLVED PUNITIVE ATTITUDES
brating the cheater’s relative utility functions
for alternative goal states – the same process Though natural selection has fashioned
of recalibration discussed above. Similarly, sophisticated punishment strategies designed
the emotion of shame evolved to signal to to protect the punisher’s interests and preserve
others the cheater’s commitment to change norms of cooperation, it doesn’t always do
through public acts of compensation and pen- this well. Natural selection produces relative
ance (Nahmias and Aharoni, 2017; Sznycer, fitness advantages. It does not optimize – it
2018; Trivers, 1971). produces minimally viable solutions.
232 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Moreover, evolution by natural selection is such as the availability heuristic, the ten-
slow, which means that today’s adaptations dency to place more weight on information
reflect fitness advantages with respect to that happens to be most cognitively accessi-
ancestral environments, not modern ones. ble in that moment (Tversky and Kahneman,
When a trait that evolved to solve a problem in 1973). Although judges wouldn’t normally
the environment in which it adapted behaves talk to journalists in the real world, similar
differently in a more contemporary environ- anchoring effects could be caused by pretrial
ment, it is known as an evolutionary mis- publicity generally.
match. This lack of fit between the adaptation Another example of our over-sensitivity to
and its present environment can produce legally irrelevant cues is the identifiable vic-
behavioral phenotypes (i.e., byproducts) that tim effect, which shows that people are more
are over-sensitive to some evolutionarily novel likely to support punishment when they know
cues (as when we crave candy as a proxy for more about the victim – such as age or occu-
fruit) and under-sensitive to others (as when pation, for instance (Jenni and Loewenstein,
drivers underestimate fatally high travel 1997). Given natural limitations on our cog-
speeds). Likewise, the objectives and interests nitive resources, it makes sense that people
of legal institutions and their stakeholders are who prioritized the most immediate infor-
not necessarily aligned with those of our mation, such as who the specific victim
hunter-gatherer ancestors, and there are plenti- was, would tend to have an advantage in the
ful examples of punitive behaviors exhibiting sorts of high-pressure social problems com-
such over- and under-sensitivities. Here we monly faced by our ancestors. Of course, this
discuss these discrepancies as a potential logic does not necessarily result in fair and
source of ‘subtractive value’ of human punish- appropriate legal judgments. Arguably, not
ment expressed in modern environments. all information that judges and jurors learn
about victims is strictly relevant to their judg-
ments. Since some judges and juries are inci-
Over-Sensitivity to Legally dentally exposed to more information about
victims than others, one implication of this
Irrelevant Factors
research is whether attempts should be made
Several examples demonstrate how our puni- to limit and standardize what victim informa-
tive instincts may be over-sensitive to factors tion is and is not revealed to fact finders.
that are not legally relevant or efficacious. Some widely studied examples of our over-
For example, judges and jurors are highly sensitivity to legally irrelevant cues include
susceptible to the anchoring phenomenon. age, race, gender, and attractiveness biases.
One dramatic experiment showed that pro- Experimental simulation studies have shown
fessional judges who were asked to make that defendants are more likely to receive
hypothetical prison-sentencing judgments higher sentence recommendations when they
predictably increased or decreased their sen- are portrayed as young, black, and male (e.g.,
tence following a phone call from an ostensi- Mitchell et  al., 2005; Steffensmeier et  al.,
ble journalist (an actor in the study) who 1998; Sweeney and Haney, 1992). Archival
asked whether the sentence will be higher or research has been less consistent, suggesting
lower than ‘one’ vs ‘three’ years (Englich that any real-life sentencing biases against
et  al., 2006). Since judges know they ought age, race, and gender may be small and highly
not take questions by the media, this ten- variable (Bontrager et  al., 2013; Mitchell,
dency to conform their sentence to the jour- 2005; Wu and Spohn, 2009). To the extent
nalist’s suggestion presumably occurred at that these biases are real, however, findings
an unconscious level. Such effects have been like these have been explained in proximate
explained by known psychological biases terms including ingroup favoritism and halo
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY, JURISPRUDENCE, AND SENTENCING 233

effects. But they are also consistent with more principle of proportionality (see Robinson
ultimate explanations of social exchange. and Kurzban, 2006). One reason, as we dis-
According to these theories, in the environ- cussed above, may be our instinctive desire
ment in which our social brain evolved, to know that the offender will suffer because
demographic characteristics may have served suffering once was an effective means of
as a conspicuous cue to group membership, recalibration and long-term behavior change
and people who were willing to pay the cost (Nahmias and Aharoni, 2017).
to punish could reap direct rewards on trad- Our punishment judgments are also sensi-
ing markets by leveling more severe punish- tive to information from our internal, physio-
ments against groups and individuals with logical environments. For example, research
whom they were most likely to compete for has provided evidence that judicial sentenc-
physical and social resources (Aharoni and ing decisions are less favorable immediately
Fridlund, 2013; Petersen et al., 2010, 2012). following the shift to daylight savings time,
Ultimately, for such punishment biases to when people tend to be most fatigued (Cho
evolve, they would have had to be effective at et al., 2017). Other research has shown a pat-
deterring or incapacitating the most competi- tern of sentence increases following unex-
tive opponents. And while the law takes pains pected home-team football losses (Eren and
to try to remove such response patterns from Mocan, 2018), presumably by implicitly
the courtroom, they often persist. impacting the judge’s mood. Unconscious
Another example of our over-sensitivity cognitive processes, such as those that regu-
to legally irrelevant cues concerns the dis- late fatigue, stress, and mood, evolved as a
proportionate emphasis we place on severity part of a motivational system designed to
as the preferred mechanism of punishment. prioritize our momentary physical and social
The two most studied parameters by which needs, so they might have global effects on
a punisher can deter an offender are the our decision-making, though it remains to be
severity and the probability of punishment. seen whether such effects are large enough to
The intuition is that more severe punish- be decisive in court.
ments deter better, but research has shown Even subject-matter experts are susceptible
that increases in severity have limited deter- to influence by extra-legal factors. The
rent effect and diminishing marginal returns; testimony of expert witnesses, of course,
increasing the probability of punishment should be based on the principles of their
would have a greater impact (e.g., Grogger, discipline, but research suggests that scientific
1991; Paternoster and Iovanni, 1986). In the experts who engage in objective reasoning
ancestral environment, the situation was dif- problems are (unconsciously) prone to produce
ferent. People lived in small groups with little evidence that confirms the preferences of the
privacy, and so the looming threat of detec- hiring party (Robertson, 2010; Wong et  al.,
tion must have been real, and powerful. But 2015). One likely proximate explanation for
in the massive societies we live in today, the this ‘adversarial allegiance’ effect is socially
probability that an offender is caught and desirable responding, a form of motivated
adjudicated remains, on average, relatively reasoning. Socially desirable responding
low (Aharoni and Kiehl, 2013; Ehrlich, probably had important social advantages for
1996), and offenders can exploit this fact. our hunter-gatherer ancestors, but in a modern
Despite the evidence that severe sanctions trial context, these effects could also bias fact-
typically deter no better than moderate ones, finders’ ascriptions of guilt and punishment.
people seem to readily accept modulating the Another extra-legal factor that we may be
severity of punishment as the primary instru- sensitive to is how the costs and benefits of
ment through which to express their punitive punishment are presented. Although the cost
motives, consistent with the philosophical of incarcerating a defendant, for instance,
234 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

may or may not be important to judges and under-sensitive to factors that may actually
taxpayers, incidental changes in the avail- be legally relevant. For one, we seem to read-
ability of that information should certainly ily discount the value of modern, custodial
not drive sentencing attitudes. Yet recent punishment strategies. Through the use of
research suggests that when professional police forces, prisons, and civil commitment
judges and laypeople make sentencing rec- facilities, it is now possible to incapacitate
ommendations for criminal offenders, they dangerous offenders with lower risk to vic-
are more lenient when the direct monetary tims, and without the need to impose suffer-
costs of incarceration are made salient, sug- ing or coercive ultimata on the offender. But
gesting that they find those costs to be rel- as discussed, we often feel outrage at the
evant but fail to spontaneously consider them prospect of an offender spending his days in
under typical, low-salience conditions. As a a cushy rehabilitation center. And we demand
consequence, removing the cost information punishment of moral offenders even if they
(without also removing benefit information) are no longer dangerous (see Connolly,
may produce sentence length recommenda- 2010). In such cases, though the modern
tions that systematically exceed those made sanction may effectively satisfy the ultimate
under more transparent conditions (Aharoni objective of the punishment adaptation
et al., 2019; Rachlinski et al., 2012). (i.e., to reduce recidivism), it fails to meet the
This behavior pattern implies that indi- adaptation’s more proximate conditions,
viduals’ punishment attitudes are internally such as evidence that the offender has under-
inconsistent across informational contexts, gone mental recalibration by way of suffer-
placing more weight on whatever informa- ing and penitence. Such responses evolved in
tion happens to be most readily available in environments in which effective custodial
the moment. This tendency may be rooted in incapacitative sanctions were unavailable –
a more general tendency to discount distant they evolved not for incapacitation but for
future rewards relative to immediate ones deterrence. This could help explain public
(Loewenstein and Thaler, 1989; Metcalfe and dissatisfaction with purely custodial
Mischel, 1999; Mischel and Ebbesen, 1970). sanctions.
Placing greater importance on the here and From a public-safety perspective, in the
now might have been adaptive for people in presence of effective incapacitation, the
need of quick results under conditions of very offender’s attitudes about his punishment
limited information (see Daly and Wilson, should be irrelevant. But people still treat
2005), but such features do not necessarily them as relevant (Gollwitzer and Denzler,
promote the careful, disinterested, and judi- 2009) presumably because efforts to recali-
cious decision-making that we expect of brate his attitudes would have been one of
modern criminal justice decisions. We don’t the best strategies for reducing recidivism
mean to imply that more regulation is always in ancestral environments. For this reason,
the best solution to reduce bias in the court- we may be inclined to discount or altogether
room, but that a richer understanding of how reject recidivism-reduction strategies that
and why we punish can serve to clarify dis- bypass opportunities for inducing suffering
course on appropriate strategies. and penitence. If so, whether our evolved
psychology of punishment (i.e., to achieve
deterrence via retribution) provides a good
Under-Sensitivity to Legally justification for rejecting correctional sanc-
tions that are equally effective but more
Relevant Factors
humanitarian is a worthy normative ques-
There are also examples of our punitive tion, but not one that can be answered by the
instincts operating in ways that are science alone.
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY, JURISPRUDENCE, AND SENTENCING 235

IV. THE UTILITY OF THE connection between the ‘is’ and the ‘ought’.
EVOLUTIONARY PERSPECTIVE Moral intuitions (including intuitions to pun-
ish) evolved precisely because they gave our
ancestors a net survival advantage in their
From Explanation to Justification
intensely social groups. Human punishment
Before we finish up with some speculations behaviors are not just widespread by chance;
about how the lens of evolutionary psychol- they are widespread because evolution has
ogy might clarify some issues in the criminal armed us with emotions that make us feel
law (and even a few issues beyond the crimi- their moral bite. The boldest answer to the
nal law), we need to address the problem of jurisprudential question with which we began
Hume’s Gap, or what is also called the natu- this chapter – where does law come from? –
ralistic fallacy. In 1739, the Scottish philoso- is not that it comes from God (as the tradi-
pher David Hume complained about how tional natural-law theorists argued), or from
there is often a gap between what is and what social conventions (the positivists), or from
some philosophers claim ought to be (Hume the individual and varying interpretations of
et al., 1739 ). Just because behaviors happen lawmakers and judges (the legal realists).
regularly, that does not mean they are mor- Instead, it comes from our evolved moral
ally grounded. Rapes and murders happen all intuitions. In this account of moral realism,
the time, but that doesn’t mean they are our notions of right and wrong come from
acceptable under any jurisprudential theory of the same place as our opposable thumbs and
law. In more modern philosophical parlance, spines.
moral truths (‘the ought’) cannot be derived Understanding what goals our punitive
solely from observational ones (‘the is’). impulses evolved to achieve and how they
Evolutionary psychology has been a favorite achieve them can help us evaluate the degree
target of criticisms revolving around Hume’s of alignment with our modern societal goals,
Gap (Rose and Rose, 2000). Just because kill- and can help formulate hypotheses for how to
ing a rival and kidnapping his mate may have manage those impulses. For example, if deter-
been an effective strategy for our ancestors rence is our main goal, we can ask whether
(Buss, 2005; Daly and Wilson, 1988), and retribution is still the best way to achieve
therefore our brains are arguably built with deterrence. If so, lengthy prison terms might
motivations for these kinds of behaviors, evo- not satisfy that goal as efficiently as lashings
lutionary psychologists are wary about taking and scarlet letters. Or if retribution is not the
those ‘is’ observations into ‘ought’ realms like best way to achieve deterrence, then it might
the law, and quite properly so (Pinker, 2003). make more sense to remove offender suffer-
But Hume himself never suggested the trip ing from the equation and focus instead on
between ‘is’ and ‘ought’ was unnavigable; he evidence-based rehabilitation services. Such
was just complaining that moral philosophers tradeoffs would not be discoverable without a
jumped the gap without so much as a pause. theory of the function that retribution evolved
Indeed, like virtually every other moral phi- to serve.
losopher of his time, Hume believed the Caution is still warranted. Many a moral
worlds of nature and morals were connected. and legal judgment must be exercised in the
His connective tissue was empathy and vast areas beyond and between our evolved
socialization: our own selfish desires to avoid moral intuitions. These intuitions give us only
pain cause us to hesitate to inflict pain on oth- the broadest contours of a secular morality.
ers, and that hesitation was Hume’s root of Even for those readers who reject our sug-
morality (Hoffman, 2014). gestion that evolutionary psychology can
We suggest that evolutionary psychol- itself be a small bridge across Hume’s Gap,
ogy itself has now supplied a more rigorous it still has significant utility when examining
236 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

legal questions. We discuss three examples: evolutionary and legal accounts. The law
(1) synthesizing the various punishment attempts to protect a much larger and less
theories; (2) providing a framework for pre- well-defined social group than our punitive
dicting behavior under uncertainty in ways adaptations were designed to serve. Because
that might help us use law to leverage those of this mismatch, our evolved punitive psy-
behaviors; and (3) helping to inspire practi- chology cannot necessarily be relied on to
cal legal reforms in circumstances where the serve the explicit interests of society as a
distance between the law and our evolved whole, and conversely, pursuit of such social
behavioral predispositions is just too great to goals may not necessarily satisfy our punitive
leverage. desires, such as when the law is more merci-
The application of evolutionary theory to ful than what a victim demands. Evolutionary
the law carries the potential to fundamentally science alone is not sufficient to help us rec-
change how we think about legal justifications oncile this tension, which hinges on norma-
for punishment and their apparent tensions. tive judgments, but a deeper understanding of
As we have argued, our punitive sentiments, our evolutionary legacy brings such tensions
and the sense of obligation that accompanies to light.
them, evolved in part because they gave pun-
ishers a fitness advantage by deterring cheat-
ing behavior, thereby protecting their physical From Explanation to Change
resources and social exchange networks. The
ultimate benefit was deterrence, but the proxi- An evolutionary psychological perspective
mate mechanism to accomplish that deter- can also help us understand the ultimate rea-
rence was retribution. When a wrongdoer sons for our behavior, so we can more lucidly
revealed cues that he/she is not a good can- decide whether these reasons are worth paying
didate for deterrence, other tactics were avail- for and whether there are other ways of
able, and these are reflected in sentiments like achieving our desired ends. By analogy,
the desire to ostracize, incapacitate, or reha- knowing that a fever is an adaptive response
bilitate. Our powerful desire to forgive and to infection can help us decide whether it
be forgiven, about which there has developed benefits us most to take a medication that
a significant behavioral- and evolutionary- treats the fever symptoms or the underlying
psychology literature (McCullough, 2008), is infection. Fever, of course, is not a perfect
another one of these proximate mechanisms solution to overcoming an infection – in some
serving the ultimate advantage of reaping the cases, there might be other, more effective
many benefits of cooperation. solutions – but, with some discomfort, fever
Seen in this way, justice systems them- usually and eventually gets the job done
selves can be understood as a type of whereas treating the symptoms by suppress-
‘evolved’ system that is specialized to solve ing body temperature could actually make the
problems of cheating and cooperation. The infection worse (Nesse and Williams, 2012).
notion that legal systems themselves evolved On the other hand, there are some adaptations
by processes akin to natural selection remains that are clearly obsolete, like wisdom teeth
to be demonstrated, but whatever the mecha- that crowd the modern jaw. Similarly, know-
nism, they do appear equipped to increase ing that human punishment motivations
the scale by which group members can deter evolved to confer fitness advantages to mem-
cheaters. This could be achieved by minimiz- bers of a social exchange network, and
ing the costs of enforcement to any individual knowing that it does this by employing
group member (Cushman, 2015). retributive-emotion programs to cause indi-
Notably, goals like ‘deterrence’ take viduals to behave in ways that facilitate deter-
on somewhat different meanings in the rence and desistance of the offensive action
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY, JURISPRUDENCE, AND SENTENCING 237

can help us evaluate whether these are worthy to move its members away from antisocial
aims, and if so, whether our punitive motiva- or other destructive behaviors, then under-
tions achieve these aims more or less effec- standing the evolutionary pressures (i.e., the
tively than other strategies, such as particular fulcrum) shaping those behaviors may tell us
rehabilitative approaches that do not demand how long our levers need to be. Behaviors
suffering. Are retributive sentiments more with deep evolutionary roots will be hard to
like a fever that, despite the discomfort, move except with big levers.
works pretty well most of the time, or are We’ve focused here on criminal sentenc-
they more like an impacted wisdom tooth ing, but evolutionary psychology might
that’s better off pulled? We don’t know the impact other areas of the law. This is not to
answer to this question – whatever the say there is a one-to-one correspondence
answer, it is undoubtedly complicated – but between evolutionary theories and particular
it’s largely owing to evolutionary theory that legal reforms. The payoffs are not likely to
we can even articulate the question. be so direct. Instead, their value will be in
A richer understanding of the ultimate rea- constraining and inspiring plausible hypoth-
sons for human punishment behavior could eses about legally relevant behaviors includ-
potentially inspire practical strategies for ing their triggers and inhibitors, by helping
managing unwanted effects. For example, to identify any evolutionary roots of such
knowing that offenders can exploit relatively behaviors. For instance, researchers have
anonymous lifestyles in modern societies, already discovered that ordinary people (like
reducing perceptions of anonymity in high- jurors) have significant trouble distinguishing
crime areas (e.g., via increased surveillance) between two of the criminal law’s four mental
could reduce crime, increase the quality of states – knowing and reckless (the other two
evidence to support criminal charges, and are purposeful and negligent; Ginther et al.,
consequently make punishers less reliant 2014; Shen et al., 2011). Examining the adap-
on the use of heavy sanctions to achieve tive contours of mental-state attribution may
the same level of deterrence. Knowing that help judges improve their definitions of these
decision-makers might neglect the costs of mental states. Understanding more about
the punishment unless those costs are made how different kinds of substances or diseases
salient could bolster arguments for increased affect these mental states could also pay huge
transparency about sentencing costs, includ- dividends in reforming the law’s traditional
ing recognition of other ways the funds could defenses to responsibility, including intoxica-
be used (i.e., the opportunity costs of the tion and insanity (Hoffman, 2018).
punishment). Knowing that sentencing judg- Jury instructions – the written rules
ments may be primed by factors like fatigue through which judges inform jurors about the
and mood, countermeasures could be devel- legal rules they must apply to the case before
oped, such as protocols for randomizing them – are a fertile ground for evolutionary
defendants to docket schedules to ensure that psychology-based improvements. Knowing
particular types of defendants (e.g., if a court how best to communicate complex principles
tends to schedule high-risk defendants last) to ordinary citizens, and the pitfalls that lie
are not systematically penalized by time- in wait, could substantially improve the truth-
dependent effects. finding function of jury trials.
The power of evolutionary theory to mobi- Understanding more about human bias
lize the law in ways that more effectively may help judges and lawyers identify biased
shape human behavior has been dubbed ‘the jurors and/or inoculate them from the harsh-
law of law’s leverage’ (Jones, 2000). If we est effects of those biases. Insights about our
think of law’s proscriptions and threats of irrational discounting behaviors could affect
punishment as a lever by which society tries the way in which judges instruct civil jurors,
238 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

and lawyers argue to them, about future tort 2  We use the term ‘first-party punishment’ loosely
damages. Knowing that expert witnesses, since punishment in an ultimate sense implies
the delivery of costs that come at the expense
too, are susceptible to social-cognitive biases,
of the receiver, and it’s not clear that conscience,
such as unconscious allegiance to the hiring guilt, and shame are best understood this way.
party, could inspire the development of meth- Another way to think of these mental states is
ods for controlling the experts’ access to this in proximate terms, as programs that regulate
information, thereby improving the quality of how we conduct ourselves and treat others (see
Peterson et al., 2010).
the testimony on which guilt and punishment
decisions may be based (Robertson, 2010;
Wong et al., 2015).
The potential for these insights to impact
the law is not limited to courts and juries. They REFERENCES
may even be more important at the front end of
law as legislators consider new laws and regu- Aharoni, E., & Fridlund, A. J. (2011). Punishment
lators consider new rules and regulations. If without reason: Isolating retribution in lay
the difficulty in distinguishing between know- punishment of criminal offenders.
Psychology, Public Policy, and the Law, 18(4),
ing criminal acts and reckless ones ends up
599–625.
being insurmountable, informed legislatures
Aharoni, E., & Fridlund, A. J. (2013). Moralistic
may want to consider reforming the criminal punishment as a crude social insurance plan.
law’s mental states. Likewise, as evolution- In T. Nadelhoffer (Ed.), The Future of
arily informed researchers learn more about Punishment. New York, NY: Oxford University
those mental states, legislatures may consider Press (pp. 213–229).
changing the contours of some of the classic Aharoni, E., & Kiehl, K. A. (2013). Evading
defenses to responsibility, such as intoxication justice: Quantifying criminal success in
or insanity. Similarly, a working knowledge incarcerated psychopathic offenders. Criminal
of our time-discounting bias would certainly Justice and Behavior, 40(6), 629–645.
be important as legislators and regulators con- Aharoni, E., Kleider-Offutt, H. M., Brosnan,
S. F., & Watzek, J. (2019). Justice at any cost?
sider issues like usury rates, payday loans, and
The impact of cost/benefit salience on
caps on future tort damages.
criminal punishment judgments. Behavioral
Evolutionary psychology is likely to have Sciences & the Law, 37, 38–60.
significant traction in all these areas of the law Allen, F. A. (1978). The decline of the
because it informs our understanding of human rehabilitative ideal in American criminal
nature, and law is applied human nature. justice. Cleveland State Law Review, 27, 147.
Alschuler, A. W. (2003). The changing purposes
of criminal punishment: A retrospective on
the past century and some thought about
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS the next. University of Chicago Law Review,
70(1), 1–22.
Alschuler, A. W. (2000). Law without values:
We thank the Cooperation, Conflict, and
The life, work and legacy of Justice Holmes.
Cognition lab members, especially Sharlene
Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Fernandes, Corey Allen, and Justin Thurman, Beccaria, C. (1766). On crimes and punishment.
for helpful comments. Hackett (1986).
Bentham, J. (1830). The rationale for
punishment. London: R. Heward.
Bix, B. (2010). Natural law theory, 2nd ed.
Notes In D. Patterson (Ed.), A Companion to
1  The first part of this section was adapted from Philosophy of Law and Legal Theory. West
Hoffman (2018). Sussex, UK: Wiley Blackwell (pp. 211–227).
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY, JURISPRUDENCE, AND SENTENCING 239

Bontrager, S., Barrick, K., & Stupi, E. (2013). human generosity in one-shot encounters.
Gender and sentencing: A meta-analysis of Proceedings of the National Academy of
contemporary research. Journal of Gender, Sciences, 108(32), 13335–13340.
Race & Justice, 16, 349. Dworkin, R. (1986). Law’s empire. Cambridge,
Brown, D. (1991). Human universals. New MA: Harvard University Press.
York: McGraw-Hill. Ehrlich, I. (1996). Crime, punishment, and the
Buss, D. M. (2005). The dangerous passion: market for offenses. Journal of Economic
Why jealousy is as necessary as love and sex. Perspectives, 10(1), 43–67.
New York, NY: Odile Jacob. Englich, B., Mussweiler, T., & Strack, F. (2006).
Cho, K., Barnes, C. M., & Guanara, C. L. Playing dice with criminal sentences: The
(2017). Sleepy punishers are harsh punishers. influence of irrelevant anchors on experts’
Psychological Science, 28(2), 242–247. judicial decision making. Personality and
Clutton-Brock, T. H., & Parker, G. A. (1995). Social Psychology Bulletin, 32(2), 188–200.
Punishment in animal societies. Nature, Eren, O., & Mocan, N. (2018). Emotional
373(6511), 209. judges and unlucky juveniles. American
Coleman, J. L., & Leiter, B. (2010). Legal Economic Journal: Applied Economics, 10(3),
positivism. In D. Patterson (Ed.), A Companion 171–205.
to Philosophy of Law and Legal Theory Fehr, E., & Fischbacher, U. (2004). Third-party
(2nd ed.). West Sussex, UK: Wiley Blackwell punishment and social norms. Evolution and
(pp. 228–249). Human Behavior, 25(2), 63–87.
Connolly, K. (2010, July). Nazi death camp Fehr, E., Fischbacher, U., & Gächter, S. (2002).
guard charged over 430,000 Jewish deaths. Strong reciprocity, human cooperation, and
The Guardian. Retrieved on May 6, 2019 from the enforcement of social norms. Human
www.theguardian.com/world/2010/jul/29/ Nature, 13(1), 1–25.
nazi-death-camp-holocaust Fehr, E., & Gächter, S. (2002). Altruistic
Connolly, K., & Martlew, M. (Eds.) (1999). punishment in humans. Nature, 415(6868),
Altruism. Psychologically speaking: A book 137.
of quotations. Leicester, UK: BPS Books. Fiddick, L., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2000). No
Cushman, F. (2015). Punishment in humans: interpretation without representation: The
From intuitions to institutions. Philosophy role of domain-specific representations and
Compass, 10(2), 117–133. inferences in the Wason selection task.
Cushman, F., Sheketoff, R., Wharton, S., & Cognition, 77(1), 1–79.
Carey, S. (2013). The development of intent- Frank, R. H. (1988). Passions within reason: The
based moral judgment. Cognition, 127(1), strategic role of the emotions. New York, NY:
6–21. W.W. Norton & Co.
Daly, M., & Wilson, M. (1988). Homicide. New Fuller, L. L. (1958). Positivism and fidelity to
Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers. law: a reply to Professor Hart. Harvard Law
Daly, M., & Wilson, M. (2005). Carpe diem: Review, 71(4), 630–672.
Adaptation and devaluing the future. The Fuller, L. L. (1965). The morality of law (2nd
Quarterly Review of Biology, 80(1), 55–60. ed.). New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Davis, M. (2009). Punishment’s golden half Gardner, J. (2001). Legal positivism: 5 1/2
century: A survey of developments from myths. American Journal of Jurisprudence,
(about) 1957–2007. Journal of Ethics, 13(1), 46, 199–227.
73–100. Ginther, M. R., Shen, F. X., Bonnie, R. J., &
Delton, A. W., & Krasnow, M. M. (2017). The Hoffman, M. B. (2014). The language
psychology of deterrence explains why group of mens rea. Vanderbilt Law Review, 67,
membership matters for third-party 1327.
punishment. Evolution and Human Behavior, Gintis, H. (2000). Strong reciprocity and human
38(6), 734–743. sociality. Journal of Theoretical Biology,
Delton, A. W., Krasnow, M. M., Cosmides, L., & 206(2), 169–179.
Tooby, J. (2011). Evolution of direct Glenn, A. L., Kurzban, R., & Raine, A. (2011).
reciprocity under uncertainty can explain Evolutionary theory and psychopathy.
240 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Aggression and Violent Behavior, 16(5), Proceedings of the National Academy of


371–380. Sciences, 104(32), 13046–13050.
Godin, J., & Davis, S. (1995). Who dares, Jones, O. D. (2000). Time-shifted rationality
benefits: Predator approach behaviour in the and the Law of Law’s Leverage: Behavioral
guppy (Poecilia reticulata). Proceedings of economics meets behavioral biology.
the Royal Society of London B, 259, Northwestern University Law Review, 95,
193–200. 1141.
Gollwitzer, M., & Denzler, M. (2009). What Kant, I. (1797). The philosophy of law: An
makes revenge sweet: Seeing the offender exposition of the fundamental principles
suffer or delivering a message? Journal of of jurisprudence as the science of right
Experimental Social Psychology, 45(4), (W. Hastie trans.). Edinburgh: T & T Clark
840–844. 1887). Retrieved from https://oll.libertyfund.
Grogger, J. (1991). Certainty vs. severity of org/titles/kant-the-philosophy-of-law
punishment. Economic Inquiry, 29(2), Katchadourian, H. (2009). Guilt: The bite of
297–309. conscience. Stanford, CA: Stanford University
Haakonssen, K. (1996). Natural law and moral Press.
philosophy: From Grotius to the Scottish Kiehl, K. A. (2007). Without morals: The
Enlightenment. New York, NY: Cambridge cognitive neuroscience of psychopathy.
University Press. In W. Sinnott-Armstrong (Ed.), Moral
Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution Psychology (Vol. 3): The Neuroscience of
of social behaviour. Journal of Theoretical Morality: Emotion, Brain Disorders and
Biology, 7(1), 17–52. Development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Hart, H. L. A. (1958). Positivism and the Kiehl, K. A., & Hoffman, M. B. (2011). The
separation of law and morals. Harvard Law criminal psychopath: History, neuroscience,
Review, 71(4), 593–629. treatment, and economics. Jurimetrics, 4(1),
Hart, H. L. A. (1961). The concept of law. 355–397.
Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press. Krasnow, M. M., Cosmides, L., Pedersen, E. J.,
Hegel, G. W. F. (1820). Philosophy of right 71 & Tooby, J. (2012). What are punishment
(T. M. Knox trans.). Oxford: Oxford University and reputation for? PLOS ONE, 7(9), e45662.
Press (1942). Krasnow, M. M., Delton, A. W., Cosmides, L. &
Hobbes, T. (1651). Leviathan. London, UK: Tooby, J. (2015). Group cooperation without
A&C Black (2006). group selection: Modest punishment can
Hoffman, M. B. (2014). The punisher’s brain: recruit much cooperation. PLOS ONE, 10(4),
The evolution of judge and jury. New York, e0124561. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.
NY: Cambridge University Press. 0124561
Hoffman, M. B. (2018). Nine neurolaw Krasnow, M. M., Delton, A. W., Cosmides, L., &
predictions. New Criminal Law Review: An Tooby, J. (2016). Looking under the hood of
International and Interdisciplinary Journal, third-party punishment reveals design for
21(2), 212–246. personal benefit. Psychological Science,
Hoffman, M. B., & Krueger, F. (2017). The neu- 27(3), 405–418.
roscience of blame and punishment. Krasnow, M. M., Delton, A. W., Tooby, J., &
In S. Menon (Ed.), Self, Culture and Con- Cosmides, L. (2013). Meeting now suggests
sciousness: Interdisciplinary Convergences we will meet again: Implications for debates
on Knowing and Being. Singapore: Springer on the evolution of cooperation. Scientific
(pp. 207–223). Reports, 3, 1747.
Hume, D. (1739). A treatise of human nature, Leiter, B. (2010). American legal realism. In D.
469. Oxford: Clarendon Press (1906). Patterson (Ed.), A Companion to Philosophy
Jenni, K., & Loewenstein, G. (1997). Explaining of Law and Legal Theory (2nd ed.). West
the identifiable victim effect. Journal of Risk Sussex, UK: Wiley Blackwell (pp. 249–266).
and Uncertainty, 14(3), 235–257. Loewenstein, G., & Thaler, R. H. (1989).
Jensen, K., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Anomalies: Intertemporal choice. Journal of
Chimpanzees are vengeful but not spiteful. Economic Perspectives, 3(4), 181–193.
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY, JURISPRUDENCE, AND SENTENCING 241

Matravers, M. (2000). Justice and punishment: Human Morality & Sociality: Evolutionary &
The rationale of coercion. Oxford University Comparative Perspectives. New York, NY:
Press on Demand. Palgrave Macmillan.
McCullough, M. (2008). Beyond revenge: The Petersen, M. B., Sell, A., Tooby, J., & Cosmides,
evolution of the forgiveness instinct. New L. (2012). To punish or repair? Evolutionary
York, NY: Jossey-Bass. psychology and lay intuitions about modern
Metcalfe, J., & Mischel, W. (1999). A hot/cool- criminal justice. Evolution and Human
system analysis of delay of gratification: Behavior, 33(6), 682–695.
Dynamics of willpower. Psychological Review, Pinker, S. (2003). The blank slate: The modern
106(1), 3. denial of human nature. New York, NY:
Mischel, W., & Ebbesen, E. B. (1970). Attention Penguin.
in delay of gratification. Journal of Personality Plato (380 BCE). The Republic, 272–297 (B.
and Social Psychology, 16(2), 329. Jowett trans.). Seattle, WA: Amazon Classics
Mitchell, O. (2005). A meta-analysis of race (2017).
and sentencing research: Explaining the Price, M. E., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2002).
inconsistencies. Journal of Quantitative Punitive sentiment as an anti-free rider
Criminology, 21(4), 439–466. psychological device. Evolution and Human
Mitchell, T. L., Haw, R. M., Pfeifer, J. E., & Behavior, 23(3), 203–231.
Meissner, C. A. (2005). Racial bias in mock Rachlinski, J. J., Wistrich, A. J., & Guthrie, C.
juror decision-making: A meta-analytic (2012). Altering attention in adjudication.
review of defendant treatment. Law and UCLA Law Review, 60, 1586.
Human Behavior, 29(6), 621–637. Robertson, C. T. (2010). Blind expertise.
Nahmias, E., & Aharoni, E. (2017). New York University Law Review, 85,
Communicative theories of punishment and 174–257.
the impact of apology. In C. W. Surprenant Robinson, P. H., & Kurzban, R. (2006).
(Ed.), Rethinking Punishment in an Era of Concordance and conflict in intuitions of
Mass Incarceration. New York, NY: Routledge justice. Minnesota Law Review, 91, 1829.
(pp. 144–161). Rose, H., & Rose, S. (Eds.) (2000). Alas, poor
Nesse, R. M., & Williams, G. C. (2012). Why we Darwin: Arguments against evolutionary
get sick: The new science of Darwinian psychology. New York, NY: Harmony Books.
medicine. New York, NY: Vintage Books. Shen, F. X., Hoffman, M. B., Jones, O. D., &
Nowak, M. A., & Sigmund, K. (2005). Evolution Greene, J. D. (2011). Sorting guilty minds.
of indirect reciprocity. Nature, 437(7063), New York University Law Review, 86, 1306.
1291. Shiner, R. A. (2010). Law and its normativity. In
Paternoster, R., & Iovanni, L. (1986). The D. Patterson (Ed.), A Companion to Philosophy
deterrent effect of perceived severity: A of Law and Legal Theory (2nd ed.). Sussex,
reexamination. Social Forces, 64(3), UK: Wiley Blackwell (pp. 417–445).
751–777. Steffensmeier, D., Ulmer, J., & Kramer, J.
Paul, V. J., & Fenical, W. (1986). Chemical (1998). The interaction of race, gender, and
defense in tropical green algae, order age in criminal sentencing: The punishment
Caulerpales. Marine Ecology Progress Series, cost of being young, black, and male.
34(1–2), 157–169. Criminology, 36(4), 763–798.
Pedersen, E. J., McAuliffe, W. H., & McCullough, Sweeney, L. T., & Haney, C. (1992). The
M. E. (2018). The unresponsive avenger: influence of race on sentencing: A meta-
More evidence that disinterested third parties analytic review of experimental studies.
do not punish altruistically. Journal of Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 10(2),
Experimental Psychology: General, 147(4), 179–195.
514. Sznycer, D. (2018). Forms and functions of the
Petersen, M. B., Sell, A., Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. self-conscious emotions. Trends in Cognitive
(2010). Evolutionary psychology and criminal Sciences, 23(2), 143–157.
justice: A recalibrational theory of punishment Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2016). Human
and reconciliation. In H. Høgh-Oleson (Ed.), cooperation shows the distinctive signatures
242 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

of adaptations to small-scale social life. [Peer Wallace, J., & Sadalla, E. (1966). Behavioral
commentary on ‘Cultural group selection consequences of transgression: I. The effects
plays an essential role in explaining human of social recognition. Journal of Experimental
cooperation: A sketch of the evidence’ by Research in Personality, 1(3), 187–194.
P. Richerson et  al.]. Behavioral and Brain Wong, C., Aharoni, E., Aliev, G., & DuBois, J.
Sciences, 39, 42–43. doi:10.1017/ (2015). Blind collaborative justice: Testing
S0140525X15000266 the impact of expert blinding and consensus
Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal building on the validity of expert testimony.
altruism. The Quarterly Review of Biology, RAND Report RR-804-1-NIJ. Retrieved on
46(1), 35–57. May, 18, 2020 from www.rand.org/pubs/
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1973). Availabil- research_reports/RR804-1.html
ity: A heuristic for judging frequency and Wu, J., & Spohn, C. (2009). Does an offender’s
probability. Cognitive Psychology, 5(2), age have an effect on sentence length? A
207–232. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010- meta-analytic review. Criminal Justice Policy
0285(73)90033-9 Review, 20(4), 379–413.
12
Evolutionary Psychology
and Incarceration
Alina Simona Rusu

INTRODUCTION certain conditions are met, such as the neces-


sity to prevent the committing of an offence
Literature in the field of criminal justice or fleeing after having done so (Macovei,
­frequently places incarceration (i.e. the state 2004; Mauer, 2017).
of being confined in prison) in the context of Analyses of criminal justice systems
the normative nature of human activities and across human societies indicate that incar-
institutions (Ward and Durrant, 2011). A ceration today involves not only the isolation
recent analysis of incarceration systems at of individuals, but also their rehabilitation
the international level (Mauer, 2017) points through professionally designed interven-
out that a nation’s incarceration rate is often tions and educational programs. Features of
interpreted as the degree of civilization of violations of social rules are considered to
that nation, as well as the degree of punitive- meet the input conditions for the develop-
ness the nation is willing to impose in the ment of mechanisms designed to respond to
process of isolating specific members from inter-individual exploitation (Petersen et al.,
the broader community (i.e. in the direction 2010, 2012). Hence, criminal justice insti-
of public safety). While several forms and tutions can be interpreted in the context of
levels of imprisonment exist, incarceration is evolved counter-exploitation strategies. Two
often considered the end product of individ- categories of counter-exploitation strate-
ual or societal failure (Mauer, 2017). In line gies (i.e. punishment and rehabilitation) are
with this consideration, international conven- addressed in this chapter in the context of
tions and policies, such as the European evolutionary analyses of incarceration from
Convention on Human Rights, state that no the perspectives of incapacitation of offenders
individual shall be deprived of liberty unless and of reparative interventions.
244 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

INTER-INDIVIDUAL EXPLOITATION THE RECALIBRATION THEORY OF


AND COUNTER-EXPLOITATION COUNTER-EXPLOITATION
STRATEGIES
Starting from the idea that a major factor
Interactions with conspecifics in social spe- regulating the activation of reparative – rather
cies afford not only fitness opportunities but than punitive – responses to exploitation/rule
also potential costs, which can sometimes violation is the perceived social value of the
include even the death of individuals sub- perpetrator (the criminal’s social worth), an
jected to severe violence (e.g., Kurzban and explanatory frame has recently emerged, i.e.
Leary, 2001). Acts of exploiting conspecifics the recalibration theory of counter-exploitation
for self-benefits related to survival and repro- (Petersen et al., 2010). The authors offer the
duction have been documented in many evolutionary prediction that the human mind
social species, including humans, as well as spontaneously computes the magnitude of
evolved strategies designed to counter these two distinct variables when confronted with
exploitation efforts (e.g., Duntley and Buss, exploitation: (1) the exploitation’s serious-
2004; Petersen et  al., 2012; McCullough ness and (2) the exploiter’s association value;
et al., 2013). Punishment and behaviors that these variables are fed into motivational
resemble human social exclusion, i.e., pre- mechanisms regulating distinct aspects of
venting particular individuals from interact- strategies for countering exploitation (e.g.,
ing with the group, have been studied in how severely we want to punish, how long
nonhuman species, such as lemurs, chimpan- we wish to incapacitate the perpetrator, how
zees, and three-spined sticklebacks (e.g., intense the social repair efforts will have to
Wilson, 1980; Goodall, 1986; Wrangham, be; Petersen et al., 2012).
1987; Kurzban and Leary, 2001). Kurzban Recent research demonstrates that the social
and Leary (2001) interpret these types of decisions mentioned above depend upon the
behaviors in nonhumans and humans within magnitude of an internal variable – a welfare
the frame of discriminate sociality, noting tradeoff ratio (WTR) – which sets the weight
different forms that have emerged due to dif- the actor places on a specific person’s welfare
ferent selection pressures. relative to the actor’s own (Tooby et al., 2006;
Several authors present and discuss several Tooby and Cosmides, 2008). Within this frame-
categories of counter-exploitation strategies, work, Petersen et al. (2012) define exploitation
from the incapacitation of offenders to the as acts expressing too low a WTR (relative to
combination of incapacitation and rehabili- some baseline) by inflicting a cost on the tar-
tation, as well as the development of prison get for too small a benefit to oneself. From the
settings with respect to the needs and wel- perspective of this definition, evolution should
fare of human beings. Petersen et al. (2012) have selected for counter-exploitation strate-
point out that the evolutionary literature on gies designed to recalibrate the exploiter’s
exploitation and its application to modern WTR in the direction of decreasing the num-
justice should take into account the counter- ber of exploitive acts they commit in the future
exploitation strategies beyond punishment (Sell et al., 2009; Petersen et al., 2012) and of
and note that the small-scale social world of inducing the exploiter to place greater weight
our ancestors – with dense social networks on the welfare of others in the future (e.g.,
and high levels of dependency – should have Clutton-Brock and Parker, 1995; de Waal,
selected not only for punitive strategies, 1996, cited in Petersen et al., 2012).
but also for non-punitive reparative ones The efficacy of the reparatory functions of
(Aureli and de Waal, 2000; Petersen et  al., punishment appears to depend upon the abil-
2010, 2012). ity of the punisher to monitor the exploiter’s
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND INCARCERATION 245

behavior (Petersen et  al., 2012). From this the exploiter’s behavior violates social
perspective, prisons might have evolved as ­obligations (Vangelisti et al., 1991), remind-
systems for monitoring the behavior of the ing the exploiter of the favors done for them
exploiter, but also a way for the offender and in the past (Sell et  al., 2009) or signaling a
for the society (‘the collective punisher’) wish for future prosocial interactions
to interact through reconciliation and other (Fujisawa et al., 2005; Petersen et al., 2012).
forms of reparative gestures. The reparative gestures convey information to
exploitive persons that they have underesti-
mated the true magnitude of the harm
The Exploitation’s Seriousness inflicted, underestimated the true value of the
relationships jeopardized, or overestimated
Petersen et  al. (2012) propose that the differ- the gain to the exploiter; it is argued that such
ence between the minimally acceptable WTR information targets WTR circuits that are
and the WTR expressed toward the victim distinct from those targeted by punishment
reflects the intuitive concept of an exploitive (Petersen et al., 2012). Petersen et al. (2012)
act’s seriousness. In light of recalibration argue that because different factors regulate
theory, the information on the seriousness of an whether specific actions are adaptive in pri-
offense should be reflected in the intensity of vate versus public contexts, evolution should
the reaction to regulate the WTR of the perpe- have selected for cognitive machinery
trator toward potential victims (i.e. punitive or designed to compute a monitored WTR to
rehabilitative reaction). Petersen et al. summa- govern decisions when one’s actions are
rize from an evolutionary point of view the likely to become known to those who will be
criminological literature addressing the seri- affected by them, and a different cognitive
ousness of crime. It appears that the seriousness machinery to govern decisions when one’s
of crime is set by the crime’s physical or sym- actions are not being monitored. It is argued
bolic level of harm and that the rank orderings that reparative gestures aim at up-regulating
of the seriousness of crimes are stable across the exploiter’s intrinsic WTRs, such that they
cultures, especially with regard to crimes caus- inflict less harm in the future even when not
ing physical harm (Stylianou, 2003; cited in being monitored (Petersen et  al., 2012). In
Petersen et al., 2012). Petersen et al. point out line with this, research on emotions indicates
that, although less explored, there are data sug- that reparative interventions, when success-
gesting that the same crime is seen as less seri- ful, have the potential to elicit guilt in
ous when performed for a large gain in inclusive exploiters (Harris et al., 2004), which subse-
fitness, such as stealing food for one’s family, quently up-regulates their cooperativeness
defending relatives, etc., than for a small indi- (Harris, 2003; Petersen et al., 2012). Several
vidual gain (Rossi et  al., 1985). The authors studies support the idea that feelings of guilt
suggest that these findings might indicate that correspond specifically to an up-regulation
intuitions about crime seriousness track welfare of the exploiter’s intrinsic rather than moni-
tradeoffs rather than harm alone (Duntley and tored WTR (Tooby and Cosmides, 2008;
Buss, 2004; Petersen et al., 2012). Petersen et al., 2012).

The Perceived Social Value of Decision to Punish and/or


the Offender to Repair
Research on reparative gestures reveals that The dual character of the incarceration system,
they usually involve demonstrations of how i.e. isolation and rehabilitation, is supported
246 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

by the four-phases frame of analysis of the several experimentally manipulated cues (i.e.
evolutionary psychology behind the mecha- evolutionarily significant cues in the context
nisms of social justice institutions (Ward and of human social functioning), such as the
Durrant, 2011): phase 1 refers to the inquiry criminal history of the offender, the offend-
into the nature of rehabilitation, which can be er’s status as in-group or out-group member,
seen as a capacity-building guided process and the offender’s expression of remorse
aiming to assist and shape the capabilities of (Petersen et al., 2012). One of the conclusions
the incarcerated offenders in making better by the authors is that their results support the
prudential and moral choices; phase 2 con- hypothesis that the mind’s design for deci-
sists in the identification of the features of an sions to respond to offenders (exploiters) is
effective and ethical rehabilitation process, based on two distinct information-processing
such as the offenders’ inherent dignity, channels, i.e. one for computing the serious-
capacity for independent functioning, and ness of the crime and the other for comput-
increase in well being, all within the direc- ing the criminal’s association value (Petersen
tion of reducing the risk that these individu- et al., 2012).
als will commit further crimes; phase 3 refers While nowadays there is little chance that
to asking questions about the relevance of an individual’s well being will be directly
evolutionary approaches to human function- affected by the state’s decision to punish or
ing and behavior for the process of incarcera- to rehabilitate a specific offender, a strate-
tion and of reintegration, in terms of gic social calculus in terms of this decision
understanding that the strategies of providing might operate in intimate social settings, sug-
a ‘good life’ for offenders and other mem- gesting that this might mirror the adaptive
bers of society should be based on realistic problems faced by our hunter-gatherer ances-
views of human nature and on the under- tors (Petersen et  al., 2012). In other words,
standing of the causes of criminal behavior; the action of the selection pressures that have
phase 4 refers to the question of whether an favored the mental designs that trigger repar-
evolutionary psychological explanation pro- ative over punitive strategies in response to
vides the best theoretical guidance on under- exploitive acts by individuals with potential
standing the function of incarceration social value might be reflected in the modern
systems and of communalities of these sys- sanctioning institutions, including the incar-
tems across the nations of the world. ceration system.
In their comprehensive analysis of the
decisions to punish or to repair in the con-
text of an evolutionary psychological analy-
sis of modern criminal justice, Petersen THE NICHE CONSTRUCTION THEORY
et al. (2012) conclude that, across a range of APPLIED TO THE INCARCERATION
different types of crime and across two dif- SYSTEM
ferent countries (i.e. the United States and
Denmark), the preferences of the partici- Based on the four-phases model presented
pants for rehabilitation over punishment of above, Ward and Durrant (2011) place the
the offenders were regulated by the percep- rehabilitation efforts of the prison system
tion of the social value of the offender, inde- within the niche construction theory (Odling-
pendently of their perception of the severity Smee et al., 2003), which states that human
of the crime. However, the perception of beings partially engineer their environments
the seriousness of the crime has regulated and in this way contribute to downstream
the intensity of preferred sanction (Petersen selection pressures, i.e. in the case of evolu-
et al., 2012). The perceived association value tionary functions of criminal justice from the
of the offender was significantly affected by perspective of niche construction, it is
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND INCARCERATION 247

assumed that greater community involvement Odling-Smee et  al. (2003) describe three
in offender rehabilitation should encourage a types of processes involved in niche con-
higher investment by individuals in social struction: genetic processes, ontogenetic or
norms and, further, that these individuals will developmental processes (individual learning
be less likely to offend (Ward and Durrant, within lifetime), and cultural processes. These
2011). This idea is in line with the concept of processes are argued to result in the modifi-
rehabilitation applied to prison systems, cation of physical and cultural environments
which refers to normative processes that and are implicated in the generation of three
manifest at an individual level by assisting types of inheritance systems (Odling-Smee
individuals to renounce criminal activity and et  al., 2003): genetic inheritance, cultural
construct socially desirable identities, and at inheritance, and ecological inheritance (i.e.
the social level in terms of correctional poli- the altered ecological niche).
cies directed at risk reduction (Ward and In a comprehensive analysis of niche con-
Maruna, 2007; Ward and Durrant, 2011). In struction theory in the context of offenders’
other words, the evolution of modern incar- rehabilitation, Ward and Durrant (2011) direct
ceration systems might be considered an special attention to the following types of
intentional social-intervention strategy, inheritance: (1) culturally stored knowledge,
which should be analyzed by taking into arguing that this type of knowledge helps the
account multiple ways of applying evolution- incoming and current generations of humans
ary theory to human behavior, such as human not to repeat the errors made by the previous
ethology, anthropology, sociobiology, generations, and (2) ecological inheritance,
memetics, and gene-culture co-evolution which refers to changes in the environments
theory (Laland and Brown, 2002; Ward and and ecologies passed on to a new generation.
Durrant, 2011). While one can assume that in According to Ward and Durrant (2011), such
the hunter-gatherer Pleistocene environment, knowledge has the potential to offer greater
the reaction of the group to an offender was flexibility for a species, as well as to gradu-
probably a quick one in order to quickly ally develop increased environmental control.
resume everyday life activities, now there is In this line, the existence of incarceration sys-
a considerable social and economic effort in tems across human societies, which function
the direction of recalibration of interactions on similar rules and structured interventions
with offenders and reconstruction of their directed to offenders, might indicate that this
functional social identities. system is part of cultural and ecological inher-
Niche construction occurs when organisms itance at the species level. Another aspect that
alter their environment through metabolism, should be taken into account when placing
activities, and choices, thus reconfiguring the evolution of incarceration systems within
their relationships between their characteris- the explanatory frame of niche construc-
tics and the environment (Laland, 2007; Ward tion theory (Ward and Durrant, 2011) is the
and Durrant, 2011). These alterations might existence of two basic types of niche con-
reduce the selection pressures in the direc- struction: (1) inceptive niche construction,
tion of enhancement of survival and repro- which refers to the original modification of
duction. Examples of niche construction in the environment, and (2) counteractive niche
humans are not only the products related to construction, which refers to modification
basic survival needs, such as houses, farming in an attempt to counteract a problem or a
practices, heating systems, medical care, etc., previous change.
but also the products addressing the develop- Several implications of niche construc-
ment of social and cultural capital, such as tion theory, especially of counteractive niche
technology products, books, and educational construction, for the rehabilitation process
systems. of offenders (i.e. specific interventions with
248 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

offenders) are discussed in the literature, incarcerated does depend on the cognitive
­starting from elements of human behavior to resources of others (Ward and Durrant, 2011).
the concept of extended cognitive system (Ward Another important aspect that should be
and Durrant, 2011). From the point of view taken into account when addressing the evo-
of niche construction theory, human beings lution of modern incarceration systems in the
are culturally responsive animals, i.e. human context of niche construction theory is the fact
behavior and socio-emotional capabilities can that humans, regardless of their incarceration-
be shaped by ethical values and social norms related status, are naturally inclined to seek
that are imparted by social learning (Ward certain goals, which are often called primary
and Durrant, 2011). Hence, a broad range of human goods (e.g., physical health, related-
behavioral options, as well as attitudes and pro- ness, subjective happiness, mastery; Ward
clivities of offenders, can be altered by inter- and Maruna, 2007; Ward and Durrant, 2011).
ventions designed in the direction of socially These primary goods or ‘natural desires’
responsible and meaningful lives (Clark, 2003; (Arnhart, 1998) are considered markers of
Sterelny, 2011; Ward and Durrant, 2011). fitness or key components of well being; it is
Also, in the same direction of interpretation, it argued that they have their origin in human
is considered that the fact that human beings nature and have evolved through natural selec-
can engineer their cognitive environments sup- tion in the direction of establishing social net-
ports the existence of reparative interventions works favorable to survival and reproduction
in prisons aiming to diminish the criminogenic (Ward and Durrant, 2011). Primary goods
cognitions and crime-supportive beliefs (Ward are assumed to be linked to secure living that
and Durrant, 2011). should allow the realization of potentialities
The concerted effort of society (social sup- specific to human individuals, such as states
port) and professionals in the field of rehabili- of mind, personal characteristics and experi-
tation of offenders (e.g., psychologists, social ences, states of affairs, etc. (Ward and Stewart,
workers, correctional staff, medical staff, etc.) 2003). Additional to the primary goods, sec-
points toward an extended cognitive system ondary goods or instrumental goods refer to
in relation to the evolutionary significance of the means of achieving primary goods (Ward
incarceration. Specifically, Ward and Durrant and Durrant, 2011). Instrumental means could
(2011) suggest that, if one accepts the idea that be considered both the offending behavior and
human individuals utilize a combination of the incarceration strategy: in the case of offend-
internal and external resources when involved ing behavior, it is assumed that it can occur
in cognitive tasks, then it can be assumed that when individuals are trying to achieve primary
all the agents involved in the process of reha- goods in often destructive ways at individual
bilitation engineering of offenders are part of and societal levels (Ward and Durrant, 2011),
their extended cognitive system. In line with while in the case of incarceration systems, we
this, several authors indicate that a proper can assume that restructuring interventions in
understanding of the role of cognition in caus- prison are means of preventing further crimes
ing and maintaining offending is dependent in the direction of a secure living environment.
upon the fact that humans are socially embed-
ded beings with a hybrid cognitive system,
which consists of an integrated combination of Effects of Incarceration: Crime-
internal and external components that enable Suppressive and Criminogenic
problem solving (e.g., Clark, 2008; Menary, Consequences
2007; Ward and Durrant, 2011). In the con-
text of incarceration, it is interpreted that the Definitions of prison are generally based on
survival of offenders and the reflection on their functions related to the consequences of
their offending-related problems while being incarceration on individuals and society.
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND INCARCERATION 249

Several theories suggest that prison is phenomenon (Harding et  al., 2017). Most of
­crime-suppressive, while others suggest that the prison returns are due to a mix of new
prison can have criminogenic consequences crimes and technical violations of the condi-
(Harding et al., 2017). It is assumed that the tions of community supervision in the post-
crime-reduction effect is achieved through prison period (Pew Center on the States, 2011,
incapacitation, rehabilitation, and specific cited in Harding et al., 2017). Technical vio-
deterrence (Bushway and Paternoster, 2009; lations during the post-prison period are con-
Harding et  al., 2017). The magnitude of any sidered a key mechanism driving the prison’s
incapacitation effect depends on the offending revolving-door effect, rather than an artifact of
of a comparison group of individuals who the pre-existing differences between prison-
have not been imprisoned, and incapacitation ers and probationers (Harding et  al., 2017).
effects occur only when individuals remain These results point toward the fact that for the
incarcerated. In contrast, rehabilitation and reparatory function of the prison system to
specific deterrence will exert their effects after be effective, the rehabilitation programs and
release. It was also hypothesized that prison the monitoring of the former offenders should
increases criminal offending through stigmati- continue after their release, especially in the
zation and labeling effects, through social first years after, i.e. imprisonment for techni-
learning of pro-criminal attitudes, values, cal violations among prisoners is concentrated
skills, and roles (prisons as ‘schools of crime’), in the first two years post-release (Harding
and through prison’s effects on employment et  al., 2017). Even though the data indicate
prospects (Harding et al., 2017). that there is a moderate incapacitation effect
Recent studies indicate that returns to of incarceration, i.e. a prison sentence reduces
prison are primarily a product of post-prison the probability of a new conviction by 5–8% in
community supervision rather than crimino- the first year after sentence, the increased rate
genic effects of imprisonment, as many indi- of post-release imprisonment due to technical
viduals sentenced to prison are trapped in the violations supports a self-perpetuating process
escalating surveillance and punishment of resulting from the functioning of the criminal
the criminal justice system; in other words, justice system itself (Siegel, 2014; Harding
the rise in incarceration in the United States et al., 2017).
in the late 20th to 21st centuries was in part
a self-perpetuation process resulting from
the workings of the criminal justice system Social Functioning in Prison:
itself (Harding et al., 2017). Being sentenced An Evolutionary Analysis
to prison rather than probation increases the
probability of future imprisonment dramati- Various correlates of social functioning in
cally, regardless of racial groups (Harding prison have been addressed in the literature,
et al., 2017). Harding et al. (2017) suggest that such as the personality traits of the inmates,
probation sentences might be employed more psycho-affective vulnerabilities, socio-
frequently as an alternative to incarcerations. familial context, behavioral management in
The cost savings associated with probation detention, etc. (Picken, 2012; Tomar, 2013;
are large relative to the incapacitation effect Unver et al., 2013; Andelin and Rusu, 2015;
of imprisonment and prison sentences do lit- Rusu, 2016). The aspects investigated so far
tle to reduce/prevent criminal offending after reflect not only the high level of psychosocial
release, relative to offending by probation- heterogeneity of the prison population, but
ers. Also, a significant proportion of incar- also the level of complexity of the process of
cerated individuals are those that have been planning efficient strategies for the prevention
recently released from prison and have been of self- and hetero-aggressive behaviors in
re-imprisoned, i.e. prison’s revolving-door detention.
250 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

To date, there are no studies concerning perspective, aggressive behavior is useful for
the evolutionary significance of the dimen- self-defense and resources protection (Buss
sions associated with the optimal dynamic and Shackelford, 1997), it still remains one
of social interactions in prison environments, of the behaviors posing the highest risk on
specifically their functional value for survival the quality of life of incarcerated persons,
in this specific environment, in which the both at the physical and psychological levels,
most probable resources to be controlled by often associated with self-harm and suicide
the inmates are those directly related to their (Towl, 2003; Campbell, 2005). Violence is a
survival, i.e. social interactions that pose the major problem in settings with incarcerated
highest risk to their quality of life. From an persons, and it is frequently associated in the
evolutionary perspective, prison environ- literature with deficits in the facial emotion
ments represent a complex mixture of stimuli decoding accuracy (Hoaken et  al., 2007).
and selection pressures, bringing together Emotion-identification errors, especially
individuals who are not familiar with each anger, are significantly associated with attri-
other and who have different social abilities bution of instrumental value to aggression in
and inclusive-fitness-related traits. Inclusive social contexts. Thus, a high level of aggres-
fitness refers here to the abilities and traits of sive attitudes and verbal aggression can be
an individual organism to survive and pass associated with misperception of anger even
on its genes through direct reproduction and/ in its absence (Dodge, 1993). Also, individu-
or by investing somatic effort and other types als with a propensity for violence (who are
of resources in his/her relatives (Hamilton, frequently found in prison settings) have a
1964). higher probability of inadequately interpret-
Some of the survival abilities of the indi- ing subtle social cues, such as facial micro
viduals facing incarceration are visible (con- expressions of emotions (Hoaken et  al.,
spicuous) to others, i.e. they can be easily 2007).
evaluated by other inmates without neces- According to the social information pro-
sitating long-term interactions, such as age, cessing model (Dodge, 1993; McNiel et al.,
gender, voice, physical appearance, body 2003), errors in emotion decoding accuracy
mass, general health, access to social support have the potential to affect individuals’ abil-
(family and friend visits). Other survival- ity (especially of those predisposed to violent
related abilities are less visible (hidden) behaviors) to access and employ alternative
at primary evaluation, requiring time and adaptive responses to social situations. In the
longitudinal social interactions (e.g., ability case of incarcerated persons, there are data
to recognize emotions in specific contexts, indicating that the ability to recognize facial
emotional-intelligence level, interpersonal expressions of fear and anger is reduced in
dominance or submission tendencies, etc.). inmates with a higher number of arrests and
Both categories of abilities can be investigated with a history of aggression (Dodge, 1993).
as predictors for the behavior of individuals in From an evolutionary perspective, the abil-
detention, thus pointing out the need for their ity to detect facial expressions of emotions,
inclusion in the professional screening forms in particular those associated with anger, is
of newly convicted persons, especially when hypothesized to have enhanced the chances
dealing with individuals with a known history of survival and reproduction of our ancestors
of aggression (Rusu, 2016). in environments of evolutionary adaptedness,
Aggression is costly in the prison envi- anger being the main indicator of the inten-
ronment, not only at an individual level, but tion to aggress against another individual
also at the level of organization and mobili- (Grandjean et al., 2005; Hoaken et al., 2007).
zation of the human and other resources of Another important factor for optimal social
the prison. Although from an evolutionary functioning in prison is the level of emotional
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND INCARCERATION 251

intelligence (EI) of the offenders. EI seems to which are associated with increases in crime;
be a relevant factor for accessing strategies this effect is interpreted as the diminishing of
of responding to social situations other than informal social-control mechanisms that
the primary responses such as quick and vio- function to establish and reinforce social
lent behaviors. The social-cognitive theory of norms and create bonds among community
power (Fiske, 1993) posits that the ability to and family members (Clear et al., 2003).
perceive others, an important component of EI, Analyses of small human communities
plays an important role in social-functioning with high levels of incarceration indicate dif-
outcomes. According to this theory, indi- ficulties in family formation and child rear-
viduals situated in positions of power tend ing (e.g., Braman, 2002), especially in those
to perceive others in a non-individualizing, communities where most of the men have
stereotypical manner. On the other hand, less died or are serving in the military, but many
powerful individuals (i.e. submissive individ- are incarcerated. Hence, in terms of the direct
uals) seem to be favored by individualization fitness impact of incarceration at individual
of others because they consider interpersonal level, the imprisonment of men can consid-
relationships as depending on the more pow- erably diminish the ability of women to find
erful individuals and on interaction partners, sexual and parenting partners in these com-
in general (Fiske, 1993; Goodwin et  al., munities (Raphael and Stoll, 2009). Also,
1998). Having access to emotional signals literature indicates that imprisonment of
and decoding them correctly affords humans parents can have unintended negative conse-
better chances of evaluating the attitudes quences on the well being of their children,
and intentions of others (Hess et  al., 1988; often related to the placement arrangements
Mayer et  al., 2008), of determining if social of the children following parental incarcera-
conflict is imminent (Ekman, 2009), and of tion (Johnson and Waldfogel, 2002).
adjusting interactive behavior in accordance In terms of the financial costs imposed by
with the perceived emotions. Consistent with incarceration systems, comparative analyses
the aspects presented above, it is recom- of cost savings between probation and incar-
mended that, besides the standard psychologi- ceration indicate that the cost savings associ-
cal screening forms that are generally used in ated with probation are large relative to the
prisons, assessments of EI, emotion decod- incapacitation effect of incarceration (Harding
ing accuracy, and individual inclusive fitness et al., 2017). In the same study, Harding et al.
should be taken into consideration as bio- (2017) revealed that the impact of prison sen-
psychological and evolutionary predictors of tences on reducing criminal offending after
optimal social functioning in the prison envi- release is lower relative to the offending behav-
ronment (Rusu, 2016). ior of probationers; these results support the
policy recommendation of using probation
more frequently as an alternative to incar-
Costs of High Rates of ceration (depending on the seriousness of the
offending) and more efficient planning of the
Incarceration
post-prison parole supervision.
Several studies point out that imprisonment
can be a profound life experience not only
for the incarcerated persons, but also for their Emotional Burnout among
families and community (e.g., Mauer, 2017). Correctional Staff
These effects have been particularly related
to high incarceration rates. Moderate rates of In terms of costs associated with the evolution
incarceration have a higher impact on crime of incarceration systems, evidence-based
reduction than high levels of incarceration, studies indicate that occupational burnout,
252 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

especially the level of emotional fatigue, is institution. Correctional officers are usually
frequently reported among members of cor- the staff who are responsible for monitoring
rectional staff, which can have negative conse- and implementation of programs planned by
quences not only at the level of individuals, administrators and supervisory staff (Lambert
but also on the efficiency of the correctional et  al., 2015). Previous studies indicate that
organization (Hurst and Hurst, 1997; Griffin a conflict in personal belief system versus
et al., 2012; Lambert et al., 2015). Job burnout institutional goals contributed to increased
is most commonly defined in the literature levels of emotional burnout, which in turn
as psychological exhaustion and fatigue was associated with less favorable attitudes
due to excessive workplace demands to treatment (in the context of rehabilitation)
(Freundenberger, 1975 cited in Lambert et al., and more favorable attitudes to punishment
2015). Maslach and Jackson (1981) have pos- (Lambert et  al., 2015). Thus, one can con-
tulated three dimensions of job burnout: emo- clude that emotional burnout of correctional
tional exhaustion, depersonalization, and a staff represents an important variable to be
reduced sense of personal accomplishment. considered when analyzing the selective
Several studies indicate that the emotional pressures in the context of modern incarcera-
dimension is the core component of burnout tion’s evolution. Recommendations are made
(Cordes and Daugherty, 1993; Maslach et al., by several authors regarding the necessity of
2012; Lambert et al., 2015) and that, compared exploring other variables of working place
to other professions from the category of help- in prison, which are expected to play a role
ing others, correctional officers report signifi- in correctional staff’s quality of life, support
cantly higher levels of emotional exhaustion as for treatment, support for punishment, turn-
a dimension of job burnout (Maslach et  al., over intent, and humane consideration of the
2012). The high level of emotional exhaustion needs of the incarcerated people.
in correctional officers was associated with Another variable associated with lower
turnover intent, absenteeism, and increased levels of emotional burnout and greater
health problems, which were particularly high life satisfaction in correctional staff was
in maximum-security settings and in prisons the opportunity to directly interact with the
holding juveniles (Fox, 1982; Carlson and inmates in a positive manner (Lambert et al.,
Thomas, 2006; Lambert et  al., 2010; Griffin 2015). When interpreting the behavior and
et  al., 2012). Hence, it appears that working attitudes of correctional officers toward
tasks in prison settings involve important inmates, Lambert et al. (2015) suggest that it
fitness-related costs to the employers, which is important to take into account that correc-
are often addressed in terms of prevention and tional officers tend to have most of the direct
management by a higher income, possibilities contact with inmates on a daily basis, so their
for early retirement and, in some institutions, perceptions of the goal of treatment versus
access to psychological- and social-support punishment may be driven by the immediate
programs. circumstances of control rather than the long-
Lambert et  al. (2015) indicate in their range goals of the institutional plans.
investigation of the consequences of emo-
tional burnout among correctional staff that
the higher the level of education of correc- Incarceration System from the
tional officers (e.g., college graduates), the Perspective of Helping Others
lower the level of their emotional burnout;
the findings are explained by the possibil- In line with the data on the positive associa-
ity that the officers with college degrees tion between direct contact with inmates and
may have been given the opportunity to greater life satisfaction in correctional offic-
participate in decision making within the ers (Lambert et  al., 2015), one can assume
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND INCARCERATION 253

that a higher level of awareness of the fact be offered to the prosocial aspects of these
that working with inmates is one of the pro- professions. One proposed action would be
fessions within the category of ‘helping to include a learning objective in the train-
others’ (i.e. structured forms of prosocial ing curricula of the correction officers on the
behavior) may help in preventing emotional neurobiological mechanisms and substrate of
burnout and increase the occupational moti- successful human social interactions, such as
vation. A common definition of prosocial offering and receiving help from others in sit-
behavior refers to it as ‘a broad category of uations of need. For example, several recent
acts that are defined by some significant seg- investigations indicate that dopamine is one
ment of society and/or one’s social group as of the most commonly suggested candidates
generally beneficial to other people’ (Penner for the explanation of ‘helper’s high’ (Luks,
et al., 2005: 366). A more specific definition 1988), which is described in the literature as
of prosocial behavior from the field of evolu- a sensation of pleasure and subjective happi-
tionary psychology considers it as a form of ness associated with helping unrelated others
behavior that brings fitness benefits to the in need (Krach et al., 2010; Rusu, 2019).
recipient and diminishes the fitness of the
helper (West et al., 2007).
The costs of helping others are well docu-
mented in the literature, mainly in the con- CONCLUSION
texts of caregiving and informal and formal
volunteering, and they usually include psy- The purpose of incarceration, i.e. whether to
chological and physiological correlates of rehabilitate or punish offenders, is still a sub-
caregiver distress (Rusu, 2019). Brown and ject of debate not only within the criminal
Brown (2015) point out that the studies justice system itself, but also among members
addressing the stress associated with help- of society. In this chapter, incarceration is
ing others often fail to distinguish between analyzed from the perspective of recalibration
the stress associated with the behavior per theory of counter-exploitation (Petersen et al.,
se and the feelings about the recipients (e.g., 2010, 2012), which is based on the evolution-
compassion, sadness). Besides the costs ary prediction that, when facing exploitation,
mentioned above, recent studies suggest the human mind spontaneously computes the
that helping others in need (i.e. unrelated magnitude of two variables: (1) the exploita-
individuals) can be associated with ben- tion’s seriousness and (2) the exploiter’s asso-
efits, such as experience of positive states ciation value. It is suggested in the literature
and improvements in health and psycho- that evolution should have selected for
logical well being (e.g., Brown et al., 2009; counter-exploitation strategies designed to
­
Jenkinson et  al., 2013; Rusu, 2019). While recalibrate the exploiter’s welfare tradeoff
helping others in need from the perspective ratio (i.e. a variable that sets the weight the
of prosocial-behavior definitions implies an actor places on a specific individual’s welfare
individual autonomous decision and intrin- relative to the actor’s own) in the direction of
sic motivation, when it comes to the profes- decreasing the number of exploitive acts in the
sions targeting the rehabilitation of offenders future (Sell et al., 2009; Petersen et al., 2012).
in prisons, the idea of helping others and the In line with this, modern prisons might
ways of doing so are imposed by the institu- function as a monitored strategy of welfare
tional and society rules, leaving most prob- tradeoff ratio manipulation or adjustment,
ably little place for the intrinsic motivation combining punishment and reparative actions
to occur. Therefore, we consider that in order in the direction of preventing future harm to
to increase the rewarding value of the prison- society and up-regulation of the cooperative-
related professions, more attention should ness of the offenders.
254 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

The incarceration system is also discussed Brown, S.L., & Brown, R.M. (2015). Connecting
within the frame of niche construction theory, prosocial behavior to improved physical
specifically the ways in which interactions health: Contributions from the neurobiology
with offenders are designed not only with the of parenting. Neuroscience and Behavioral
purpose of their incapacitation, but also in Reviews, 55, 1–17.
Brown, S.L., Smith, D.M., Schulz, R., Kabeto,
the direction of assisting them in renouncing
M.U., Ubel, P.A., Poulin, M., & Langa, K.M.
criminal activity and in constructing socially (2009). Caregiving behavior is associated
desirable identities. Also, the chapter presents with decreased mortality risk. Psychological
the idea that the concerted effort of society Sciences, 20, 488–494.
(social support) and professionals in the field Bushway, S.D., & Paternoster, R. (2009). The
of rehabilitation of offenders (psychologists, impact of prison on crime. In Raphael, S. &
social workers, correction officers, etc.) could Stoll, M.A. (Eds.), Do prisons make us safer?
be interpreted as an extended cognitive system The benefits and costs of the prison boom
in relation to the evolutionary significance of (pp. 119–150). New York: Russell Sage
incarceration. In terms of fitness-related analy- Foundation.
sis, incarceration imposes costs both on the Buss, D.M., & Shackelford, T.K. (1997). Human
aggression in evolutionary psychological
offenders (e.g., incapacitation due to imprison-
perspective. Clinical Psychology Review, 17,
ment, violent interactions in prison, negative 605–619.
consequences on the well being of their chil- Campbell, A. (2005). Aggression. In Buss, D.M.
dren and family) and on the correction staff (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychol-
(e.g., emotional fatigue, occupational burnout, ogy (pp. 628–652) New York: John Wiley.
turnover intent). Hence, one can conclude that Carlson, J., & Thomas, G. (2006). Burnout
the two main functions of the incarceration sys- among prison caseworkers and corrections
tem (i.e. punitive and reparatory) are continu- officers. Journal of Offender Rehabilitation,
ously challenged by selective pressures that are 43, 19–34.
both external ones (i.e. system-independent, Clark, A. (2003). Natural born cyborgs: Minds,
such as financial crises in the society), but also technologies and the future of human
intelligence. New York: Oxford University
internal ones, including the system itself.
Press.
Clark, A. (2008). Supersizing the mind: Embod-
iment, action, and cognitive extension. New
York: Oxford University Press.
REFERENCES Clear, T., Rose, D., Waring, E., & Scully, K.
(2003). Coercive mobility and crime: A
Andelin, E.I., & Rusu, A.S. (2015). Investigation preliminary examination of concentrated
of facial micro-expressions of emotions in incarceration and social disorganization.
psychopathy – A case study of an individual Justice Quarterly, 20(1), 33–64.
in detention. Procedia – Social and Behavioral Clutton-Brock, T.H., & Parker, G.A. (1995).
Sciences, 209, 46–52. Punishment in animal societies. Nature, 373,
Arnhart, L. (1998). Darwinian natural right: The 209–216.
biological ethics of human nature. Albany, Cordes, C., & Dougherty, T. (1993). A review
NY: State University of New York Press. and integration of research on job burnout.
Aureli, F., & de Waal, F. (Eds.). (2000). Natural Academy of Management Review, 18,
conflict resolution. Berkeley and Los Angeles: 621–656.
University of California Press. de Waal, F. (1996). Good natured: The origins
Braman, D. (2002). Families and incarceration. of right and wrong in humans and other
In Mauer, M. & Chesney-Lind, M. (Eds.), animals. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Invisible punishment: The collateral conse- Press.
quences of mass imprisonment (pp. 137–135) Dodge, K.A. (1993). Social information
New York: New Press. processing and peer rejection factors in the
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND INCARCERATION 255

development of behavior problems in Harris, N. (2003). Reassessing the dimensionality


children. In Biennial Meeting of the Society of the moral emotions. British Journal of
for Research in Child Development, New Psychology, 94, 457–473.
Orleans, LA. Harris, N., Walgrave, L., & Braithwaite, J.
Duntley, J.D., & Buss, D.M. (2004). The (2004). Emotional dynamics in restorative
evolution of evil. In Miller, A. (Ed.), The social justice conferences. Theoretical Criminology,
psychology of good and evil (pp. 102–123). 8, 191–210.
New York: Guilford. Hess, U., Kappas, A., & Scherer, K.R. (1988).
Ekman, P. (2009) Telling lies: Clues to deceit in Multichannel communication of emotion:
the marketplace, politics, and marriage Synthetic signal production. In Scherer, K.R.
(Revised Edition). W.W. Norton & Company. (Ed.) Facets of emotion: Recent research
Fiske, S.T. (1993). Controlling other people: The (pp. 161–182). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
impact of power on stereotyping. American Erlbaum Associates.
Psychologist, 48, 621–628. Hoaken, P.N., Allaby, D.B., & Earle, J. (2007).
Fox, J. (1982). Organizational and racial conflict Executive cognitive functioning and the
in maximum-security prisons. Lexington, recognition of facial expressions of emotion
MA: Lexington Books. in incarcerated violent offenders, non-violent
Freundenberger, H. (1975). The staff burn-out offenders, and controls. Aggressive Behavior,
syndrome in alternative institutions. Psycho- 33, 412–421.
therapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 12, Hurst, T., & Hurst, M. (1997). Gender differences
73–82. in mediation of severe occupational stress
Fujisawa, K.K., Kutsukake, N., & Hasegawa, T. among correctional officers. American
(2005). Reconciliation pattern after Journal of Criminal Justice, 22, 121–137.
aggression among Japanese preschool Jenkinson, C.E., Dickens, A.P., Jones, K.,
children. Aggressive Behavior, 31, 138–152. Thompson-Coon, J., Taylor, R.S., Rogers, M.,
Goodall, J. (1986). Social rejection, exclusion, & Richards, S.H. (2013). Is volunteering a
and shunning among the Gombe chimpan- public health intervention? A systematic
zees. Ethology and Sociobiology, 7, 227–236. review and meta-analysis of the health and
Goodwin, S.A., Operario, D., & Fiske, S.T. survival of volunteers. BMC Public Health,
(1998). Situational power and interpersonal 13, 773.
dominance facilitate bias and inequality. Johnson, E.I., & Waldfogel, J. (2002). Parental
Journal of Social Issues, 54, 677–698. incarceration: Recent trends and implications
Grandjean, D., Sander, D., Pourtois, G., for child welfare. Social Service Review,
Schwartz, S., Seghier, M.L., Scherer, K.R., & 76(3), 460–479.
Vuilleumier, P. (2005). The voices of wrath: Krach, S., Paulus, F.M., Bodden, M., & Kircher,
Brain responses to angry prosody in T. (2010). The rewarding nature of social
meaningless speech. Nature Neuroscience, interactions. Frontiers in Behavioral
8, 145–146. Neuroscience, 4(22), 1–3.
Griffin, M., Hogan, N., & Lambert, E. (2012). Kurzban, R., & Leary, M.R. (2001). Evolutionary
Doing people work among a tough crowd: A origins of stigmatization: The function of
further examination of the job characteristics social exclusion. Psychological Bulletin, 127,
model and correctional staff burnout. Criminal 187–208.
Justice and Behavior, 39, 1131–1147. Laland, K.N. (2007). Niche construction, human
Hamilton, W.D. (1964). The genetical evolution behavioral ecology, and evolutionary
of social behavior. Journal of Theoretical psychology. In Dunbar, R.I.M. & Barrett, L.
Biology, 7, 1–52. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of evolutionary
Harding, D.J., Morenoff, J.D., Nguyen, A.P., & psychology (pp. 36–47). Oxford, UK: Oxford
Bushway, S.D. (2017). Short- and long-term University Press.
effects of imprisonment on future felony con- Laland, K.N., & Brown, G.R. (2002). Sense and
victions and prison admissions. Proceedings non-sense: Evolutionary perspectives on
of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(42), human behavior. Oxford, UK: Oxford
11103–11108. University Press.
256 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Lambert, A., Altheimer, I., & Hogan, N. (2010). neglected process in evolution. New Jersey,
Exploring the relationship between social NY: Princeton University Press.
support and job burnout among correctional Penner, L.A., Dovidio, J.F., Piliavin, J.A., &
staff: An exploratory study. Criminal Justice Schroeder, D.A. (2005). Prosocial behavior:
and Behavior, 37, 1217–1236. Multilevel perspectives. Annual Reviews of
Lambert, E.G., Barton-Bellessa, S., & Hogan, Psychology, 56, 365–392.
N.L. (2015). The consequences of emotional Petersen, M.B., Sell, A., Tooby, J., & Cosmides,
burnout among correctional staff. Sage L. (2010). Evolutionary psychology and
Open, 5(2), 1–15. criminal justice: A recalibrational theory of
Luks, A. (1988). Doing good: Helper’s high. punishment and reconciliation. In Hogh-
Psychology Today, 22(10), 34–42. Olesen, H. (Ed.), Human morality and
Macovei, M. (2004). The right to liberty and sociality: Evolutionary and comparative
security of the person: A guide to perspectives (pp. 72–131). Hampshire:
implementation of Article 5 of the European Palgrave Macmillan.
Convention on Human Rights. In Human Petersen, M.B., Sell, A., Tooby, J., & Cosmides,
rights handbooks (No. 5), Retrieved 18 L. (2012). To punish or repair? Evolutionary
November 2019 from https://www.refworld. psychology of lay intuitions about modern
org/docid/49f181e12.html, Council of Europe. criminal justice. Evolution of Human
Maslach, C., & Jackson, S. (1981). The Behavior, 33(6), 682–695.
measurement of experienced burnout. Pew Center on the States (2011). State of
Journal of Occupational Behavior, 2, Recidivism: The Revolving Door of America’s
99–113. Prisons. The Pew Charitable Trusts,
Maslach, C., Leiter, M., & Jackson, S. (2012). Washington, DC.
Making a significant difference with burnout Picken, J. (2012). The coping strategies,
interventions: Researcher and practitioner adjustment and well being of male inmates in
collaboration. Journal of Organizational the prison environment. Internet Journal of
Behavior, 33, 296–300. Criminology, 1–29. Retrieved 8th of December
Mauer, M. (2017, April 26). Incarceration Rates 2018 from https://studylib.net/doc/8657392/
in an International Perspective. Oxford the-coping-strategies–adjustment-and-well-
Research Encyclopedia of Criminology. being-of-male.
Retrieved 18 November 2019 from https:// Raphael, S., & Stoll, M. (Eds.). (2009). Do
oxfordre.com/criminology/view/10.1093/ prisons make us safer? The benefits and
acrefore/9780190264079.001.0001/acrefore- costs of the prison boom. New York: Russell
9780190264079-e-233. Sage Foundation.
Mayer, J.D., Roberts, R.D., & Barsade, S.G. Rossi, P.H., Simpson, J.E., & Miller, J.L. (1985).
(2008). Human abilities: Emotional intelligence. Beyond crime seriousness: Fitting the
Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 507–536. punishment to the crime. Journal of
McCullogh, M.E., Kurzban, R., & Tabak, B.A. Quantitative Criminology, 1, 59–90.
(2013). Cognitive mechanisms for revenge Rusu, A.S. (2016). Evolutionary-based aspects
and forgiveness (with commentaries and of optimal social functioning in prison. Acta
response). The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, Psychopathologica, 2(6), 1–3.
36, 1–58. Rusu, A.S. (2019). Educational practices for
McNiel, D.E., Eisner, J.P., & Binder, R.L. (2003). civic engaged students: Service-Learning -
The relationship between aggressive from general to applied values in animal-
attributional style and violence by psychiatric oriented professions. Journal of Educational
patients. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Sciences & Psychology, 9, 29–35.
Psychology, 71, 399–403. Sell, A., Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2009).
Menary, R. (2007). Cognitive integration: Mind Formidability and the logic of human anger.
and cognition unbounded. Basingstoke, UK: Proceedings of the National Academy of
Palgrave Macmillan. Sciences, 106, 15073–15078.
Odling-Smee, F.J., Laland, K.N., & Feldman, Siegel, J.A. (2014). Prisoner reentry, parole
M.W. (2003). Niche construction: The violations, and the persistence of the
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND INCARCERATION 257

surveillance state. PhD Dissertation (University Vangelisti, A.L., Daly, J.A., & Rudnick, J.R.
of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI). (1991). Making people feel guilty in
Sterelny, K. (2011). The evolved apprentice. conversations: Techniques and correlates.
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Human Communication Research, 18,
Stylianou, S. (2003). Measuring crime 3–39.
seriousness perceptions: What have we Ward, T., & Durrant, R. (2011). Evolutionary
learned and what else do we want to know? psychology and the rehabilitation of the
Journal of Criminal Justice, 31, 37–56. offenders: Constraints and consequences.
Tomar, S. (2013). The psychological effects of Aggression and Violent Behavior, 16,
incarceration on inmates: Can we promote 444–452.
positive emotion in inmates? Delphi Ward, T., & Maruna, S. (2007). Rehabilitation:
Psychiatry Journal, 16, 60–68. Beyond the risk paradigm. London, UK:
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2008). The Routledge.
evolutionary psychology of the emotions and Ward, T., & Stewart, C.A. (2003). The treatment
their relationship to internal regulatory of sex offenders: Risk management and
variables. In Lewis, M. & Haviland-Jones, J.M. good lives. Professional Psychology: Research
(Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 114–137; and Practice, 34(4), 353–360.
3rd ed). New York: Guilford Press. West, S.A., Griffin, A.S., & Gardner, A. (2007).
Tooby, J., Cosmides, L., & Price, M.E. (2006). Social semantics: Altruism, cooperation,
Cognitive adaptations for n-person mutualism, strong reciprocity and group
exchange: The evolutionary roots of selection. Journal of Evolutionary Biology,
organizational behavior. Managerial and 20, 415–432.
Decision Economics, 27, 103–129. Wilson, E.O. (1980). Sociobiology: The abridged
Towl, G. (2003). Psychology in prisons. Oxford, edition. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press.
UK: BPS Blackwell. Wrangham, R.W. (1987). The significance of
Unver, Y., Yuce, M., Bayram, N., & Bilgel, N. African apes for reconstructing human social
(2013). Prevalence of depression, anxiety, evolution. In Kinzey, W.G. (Ed.), The evolution
stress, and anger in Turkish prisoners. Journal of human behavior: Primate models
of Forensic Sciences, 58, 1210–1218. (pp. 51–71). Albany, NY: SUNY Press.
13
Evolution and Punishment
Anthony Walsh, Cody Jorgensen, and Jessica Wells

INTRODUCTION we are expressing censure and community


disapproval of his or her actions by inflicting
In the opening words of The Scarlet Letter, punishment upon him or her.
first published in 1850, Nathaniel Hawthorne Of course, prisons are a relatively modern
wrote: ‘The founders of a new colony, what- invention, but we have always retaliated in
ever Utopia of human virtue and happiness some way against those who have offended
they might originally project, have invariably us. We see this retaliatory aggression in
recognized it among their earliest practical almost all sexually reproducing animals
necessities to allot a portion of the virgin soil when conspecifics ‘cheat’ (signal coopera-
as a cemetery, and another portion as the site tion but fail to be forthcoming). A classic
of a prison’ (1879: 1). Hawthorne’s words are example is that of vampire bats who freeload
a reminder of two things we cannot avoid – on blood donation. Bats who fail to find a
human mortality and depravity, and that we blood meal on a given night will have regur-
must make provisions for both. We sadly gitated blood donated to them by other mem-
commit our departed loved ones to the bers of the group. The tendency is to share
ground for obvious reasons, and we gladly blood with those who have previously shared
remove miscreants from our sight for reasons with them, thus returning the favor. Failure
just as obvious. It is beyond doubt that there to reciprocate usually results in the original
are times when we must set certain people benefactor withdrawing future acts of sharing
apart from the rest of us because they have (Wilkinson, 1990). So even bats have some
demonstrated their inability to live by the built-in motivation to retaliate punitively
agreed-upon norms of acceptable behavior. against norm breakers.
When we do this we are doing it against the From a scientific perspective, any emotion
will of the person being removed, and thus or behavior that is shown to be as universal as
EVOLUTION AND PUNISHMENT 259

the urge to punish must have an evolutionary punishment obviously played an adaptive
history. Natural selection is a powerful posi- role, but the survival and reproductive func-
tive feedback process energized by differen- tion of punishment is not at all as obvious as
tial reproduction. If a genetic mutation results are the roles of food, drink, and sex. We do
in the design (morphological, physiological, know, however, that humans find the same
or behavioral) of an organism that allows it kind of satisfaction when heinous criminals
to out-reproduce other conspecifics, it will are punished. Brain-imaging studies have
eventually become more common in the shown increased blood flow to the nucleus
population and we then say that the design accumbens when subjects witness the pun-
change has been selected for. Over many ishment of those who have wronged them (de
generations, the design change will spread Quervain et  al., 2004; Klein, 2012). Blood
until it goes to fixation in the population; that flow to reward centers has also been observed
is, all members of the species have it. when people are punished who have harmed
Cosmides and Tooby (1994: 328) liken others, and the strength of the pleasure
natural selection to Adam Smith’s ‘invisible response is proportional both to the harm
hand’ whereby unobservable market forces done and to the level of the offender’s culpa-
(‘natural selection’) fueled by millions of bility (whether the act was purposeful, know-
people seeking their self-interest (‘survival ing, reckless, or negligent) (Buckholz and
and reproduction’) move the supply and Marois, 2012). These brain-imaging studies
demand of goods (‘genetic polymorphisms’) provide us with compelling evidence that the
in a free market (‘the environments of evo- urge to punish must be an adaptive feature of
lutionary adaptation’) to reach market equi- human nature, attested to by the old saying,
librium (‘genetic fixation’) automatically and ‘Vengeance is sweet’.
without any intention or foresight. Petersen and colleagues’ evolutionary per-
The consequences of punishing norm vio- spective on criminal justice notes the various
lators must have been positive with respect to ways that our ancestors could have reacted
the twin goals of all life – survival and repro- to exploitative harm or other norm violations
duction. It is important for all animals to be (2010: 92). These four ways they have identi-
motivated to do things that are vital in the fied are presented below.
pursuit of these goals. To provide that moti-
1 ‘Killing the perpetrator, which permanently
vation, nature has provided us with neural
removes the threat’. In an evolutionary context,
mechanisms that reward us with pleasurable executing a band member would probably be
feelings when we do things that aid in achiev- considered a hugely important decision since all
ing these goals. These pleasurable feelings band members would have known the perpetra-
arise from the neurotransmitter dopamine tor and his or her family. The decision would have
bombarding the nucleus accumbens and sim- required a sufficiently serious group-damaging
ilar so-called ‘pleasure centers’ in the brain act and significantly more members of the band
(Walsh et al., 2012). Each surge of dopamine favoring execution than opposing it. In the
can be viewed as Mother Nature’s reward modern United States, this is the death penalty,
when we satisfy urges to eat, drink, and have the imposition of which requires a particularly
sex, which are the most obvious requirements heinous murder and/or multiple murders. The
death penalty is retributive in nature, although
for survival and reproduction. The pleasant-
some justify it by a supposed deterrent effect.
ness associated with dopamine (known as the People who believe in the value of retribution
‘happy chemical’ in popular parlance) rein- say that punishment for its own sake is morally
forces the behavior that makes us feel good justified and need not be judged good or bad
and motivates us to repeat it. according to its consequences for society (such as
Given that the urge to punish wrongdo- deterrence). Logan and Gaes (1993) aver that ret-
ers is universal and strong (Penney, 2012), ribution is the most honestly stated justification
260 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

for punishment because it taps into our punitive sentencing to accommodate different rates of
urges and posits no secondary purpose for it. offender ‘rehabilitation’. Yet we continue to try
2 ‘Expelling the perpetrator from the social world to rehabilitate criminals because we realize that
through ostracism or confinement; or, on an indi- whatever helps the offender helps the commu-
vidual basis, not engaging in future cooperation nity. The rehabilitation model is well stated in
endeavors with those who have cheated’. In former United States Supreme Court Chief Justice
a band situation, being ostracized would have Warren Burger’s famous lines: ‘To put people
been devastating because the ostracized person behind walls and bars and do little or nothing
would have no other group to which to turn for to change them is to win a battle but lose a war.
sustenance. Not so long ago, those who violated It is wrong. It is expensive. It is stupid’ (in Stohr
cultural and social norms were publicly shamed et al., 2012: 246).
by being placed in the stocks for all passers-by
to taunt and abuse. We no longer publicly abuse Of course, all these responses to norm viola-
villains or tell them to ‘Get out of Dodge’; rather, tions are punitive in that those experiencing
we use imprisonment and justify it by the concept them do not welcome them. By imposing
of incapacitation. The justification for incapacita- such pain, we hope that offenders will not
tion is well put by James Q. Wilson (1975, p. 391): repeat the behavior that led to his or her pun-
‘Wicked people exist. Nothing avails except to set
ishment, and to make those witnessing the
them apart from innocent people’. Incapacitation
punishment not be tempted to follow the
obviously has positive consequences for society;
while criminals remain in prison they can no longer same path. We call this punishment justifica-
harm people on the outside. For instance, in 1995, tion ‘deterrence’. Radzinowicz and King
there were 135,000 inmates in prison whose most (1979) aver that retribution and deterrence
serious crime was robbery, and each robber on occur simultaneously, but that the real social
average commits five robberies per year. Had these function of punishment is deterrence:
robbers not been incarcerated, they would have
been responsible for an additional 675,000 rob- People are not sent to prison primarily for their
beries that year (Currie, 1999). An Italian study of own good, or even in the hope that they will be
cured of crime. Confinement is used as a measure
22,000 inmates published in 2006 found that the
of retribution, a symbol of condemnation, a vindi-
incapacitation effect was an average of between cation of the law. It is used as a warning and
14 and 18 subsequent theft and robbery (these deterrent to others. It is used, above all, to protect
were the only crimes documented) arrests per other people … from the offender’s depredations.
released convict (Buonanno and Raphael, 2013). (Radzinowicz and King, 1979: 296)
3 ‘Punishing the perpetrator through infliction of
costs or withdrawal of benefits’. This response to
norm violations would have been far more damag-
ing in ancestral environments if it meant the with-
drawal of cooperation; in a modern sense it would RETRIBUTION: ITS ORIGIN AND
still be painful but less so. Today such punishments FUNCTION
would be in the form of fines and/or withdrawal of
welfare benefits; just like the ‘No more blood for Whatever justification for punishment that
you, Vlad’ response to cheater bats. philosophers and criminologists may come
4 ‘Reconciling with the perpetrator’. In a modern up with, underneath it all is the urge for ret-
criminal justice system we call this rehabilitation, ribution. Retribution means recompense,
or to restore or return to constructive, healthy
repayment, payback, or getting even. Many
relationships with other members of society.
Rehabilitation is criticized by conservatives as
criminologists tend to view retribution as an
‘molly cuddling’ and lacking in truth in sentencing, irrational and barbaric justification for pun-
and by libertarians and some liberals for forc- ishment based on emotion rather than reason.
ing offenders into treatment against their wills. They may note that retribution is most often
Liberals charge that it contributes to sentenc- given as a rationale by supporters of the
ing disparity because it requires indeterminate death penalty (Bohm, 2012), which many see
EVOLUTION AND PUNISHMENT 261

as little more than state-sanctioned revenge motivated to attend to their own survival and
(Rosebury, 2009). This position carries with reproductive interests. The urge to cooperate
it the unspoken assumption that the emotions with our fellow creatures and the urge to sig-
behind the retributive urge are immoral and nal cooperation and then defect, thus receiv-
antisocial. Evolutionary psychologists and ing resources illegitimately, is the hedonic
neuroscientists beg to differ, as did Justice tug of war we all play with ourselves. Most
Potter Stewart. In his concurring opinion in of us successfully adjust with nary a thought
the Supreme Court’s 5–4 ruling in Furman of violating the social contract; some adjust
vs. Georgia (1972) invalidating Georgia’s with difficulty, and others find the illegiti-
death penalty statute, he wrote the following mate route congenial to their natures.
about the importance of retribution and the The modern evolutionary view of human
folly of dismissing it: nature accommodates the classical view
that humans are designed to maximize their
I cannot agree that retribution is a constitutionally pleasure and minimize their pain. The gov-
impermissible ingredient in the imposition of pun- ernance of these two sovereign masters is an
ishment. The instinct for retribution is part of the
nature of man, and channeling that instinct in the adaption and thus functional, but it can be
administration of criminal justice serves an impor- socially disruptive if immediate concerns for
tant purpose in promoting the stability of a society gratification are placed above maintaining
governed by law. When people begin to believe social harmony and one seeks ‘money with-
that organized society is unwilling or unable to out work, sex without courtship, and revenge
impose upon criminal offenders the punishment
they ‘deserve,’ then there are sown the seeds of without court delays’, as Gottfredson and
anarchy – of self-help, vigilante justice, and lynch Hirschi (1990: 89) put it. This is the origin
law. (Furman vs. Georgia 408 U.S. 238, 1972) of the urge to punish because any individual
who appropriates resources illegitimately
What did Justice Stewart mean by locating thereby deprives the legitimate owners of
the ‘instinct for retribution’ in the ‘nature of resources needed for their own survival and
man’? One method of determining if some- reproductive success.
thing is ‘in the nature of man’ is to scour the Counteracting the human competitive and
historical record for the universality of the status-striving motives that lead to conflict
practice so considered. All early legal codes, is a powerful egalitarian instinct that leads
such as the ancient Mesopotamian codes of to cooperation. One of the first moral state-
Hammurabi, Ur-Nammu, and Eshnunna, and ments uttered by children, and often quite
the Roman Laws of the Twelve Tables, were forcefully, is ‘That’s not fair!’ How do chil-
retributive, and the universality of such senti- dren arrive at that conclusion long before they
ments in criminal codes supports Stewart’s are able to articulate any moral reasoning for
contention about the urge for retribution it, and why is it so compelling? A number of
being a strong component of human nature. scholars (see generally Gavrilets, 2012) have
Given the assumption that it is part of human posited that the nomadic lifestyle character-
nature, how did it become so and why? ized by dangerous and uncertain prospects
Evolutionary logic tells us there must have of obtaining survival resources kick-started
been good adaptive reasons why our reward the evolution of our species’ powerful social
centers fire up when we punish or witness and egalitarian instincts. Small foraging
the punishment (vicariously or otherwise) of and hunting and gathering bands demanded
wrongdoers, but it does not tell us why pun- strict group-wide cooperation, with each
ishment was so vital to our distant ancestors. individual’s survival depending on the sur-
We may approach the question by noting that vival of the whole. Any kind of behavior that
Homo sapiens are an ultra-social species con- would subvert mutual reciprocity (not pull-
sisting of individuals who are preeminently ing one’s weight, cheating, stealing) could
262 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

not be tolerated. Scarce resources had to be food and mates, fighting off pathogens and
distributed according to egalitarian principles predators, etc.) that all animals share, but
lest the group fall into disputes and fracture, human culture has shaped both the anatomy
leading perhaps to the death of everyone in and function of the human brain above and
it. In such a group, resource sharing would beyond the changes wrought by the chal-
take place under the vigilant eyes of all in lenges posed by ecology. We know that from
immediate time as opposed to a delayed- the approximate 1.5 million years that sepa-
time sharing of society’s resources in agrar- rated Australopithecus afarensis and Homo
ian and industrial societies. Perceptions of erectus, hominid cranial capacity doubled
unequal distribution of resources would have from a mean of 450 cc to a mean of 900 cc,
produced immediate emotional reactions that and by another 70% to about 1,350 cc from
were not of the warm and pleasant variety. Homo erectus to modern Homo sapiens.
Social animals cooperate because they can On an evolutionary timescale, this is truly
achieve more as a group than they can indi- an astounding level of morphological change
vidually, and we feel good about ourselves in such a short period of time (Adolphs,
when helping others. We are also rewarded 2009).
by reciprocal behavior in the future, but even A study of hominid crania by Bailey and
if the person we help is a stranger we are Geary (2009: 77) found that geographic lati-
unlikely to meet again, we still receive a shot tude was strongly related to cranial capacity
of dopamine that makes us feel good (Brunero, (r = .61), but population density was more
2002). However, a population of cooperators strongly related (r = .79), leading them to
would thrive and grow bigger, thereby plant- conclude that the burden of evolutionary
ing the seeds of norm violation. A fairly large selection has moved from ‘climactic and
group of cooperators consisting overwhelm- ecological to social’. There are a number
ingly of non-kin provides a target-rich niche of other studies of hominid crania dating as
for people who cheat on the social contract far back as 1.9 million years that show more
and seek to gain resources at zero cost due to robust increases in cranial capacity among
a lower level of vigilance and a higher level groups in areas with greater population den-
of anonymity. It has been estimated that the sity and in areas in which food procurement
maximum group size to maintain stable rela- was most problematic, namely, colder and
tions among all members is around 150; this most northerly areas of the globe (Ash and
is known as ‘Dunbar’s Number’ (Hill and Gallup, 2007; Kanazawa, 2008). Living in
Dunbar, 2003). This number is derived from large groups forces the brain to make more
observation of different human groups which computations in order to negotiate relation-
maintained stable relations throughout their ships, to understand the thoughts, feelings,
history, extrapolation from relative neocortex and intentions of others, and to cooperate
size to group size in different species, and the in securing resources and in defending the
assumed cognitive constraints involved in group (Dunbar and Shultz, 2007).
maintaining cohesion in face-to-face interac- Despite increases in reasoning power, as
tion while still pursuing one’s own personal has been pointed out, it is difficult to maintain
evolutionarily relevant interests. the fuzzy warmth of all-for-one-and-one-for-
The reference to neocortex size calls on all kind of ‘primitive communism’ in groups
studies of a wide range of primate species that larger than about 150 (Dunbar’s number).
show that group size is related to brain size The seminal studies of Hutterite communi-
(Dunbar and Shultz, 2007). Most evolutionary ties by Baden and Stroup (1972) are among
adaptations, including brain adaptations, are the studies that reveal this. The Hutterites
the result of challenges to survival and repro- are a religious community spread across
ductive success posed by ecology (finding colonies in the United States and Canada.
EVOLUTION AND PUNISHMENT 263

The Hutterites seem to have a c­ ommon-sense Kant justifies retribution as the only moral
understanding of human nature, and man- justification for punishment in terms of the
date that a settlement must split and form value, dignity, and agency of human beings.
a new one every time the mother colony Kant’s retributionism is undergirded by his
reaches about 150 people. When the colony idea of duty and beliefs in human reason and
is small, public surveillance of each person autonomy. Human beings are different from
is more complete and an accounting of each other animals because they are guided by
person’s contribution (or lack of) is possible, reason, and it is from reason that we derive
just as it was on the ancient African savan- all of our duties and obligations. Because we
nah. The Hutterites recognize freeloaders are blessed with reason, we all have a duty
who take advantage of their system of com- to act out of reverence for moral law, which
munal ownership of resources and call them Kant conceived of as a set of categorical
drones (reference to male drone bees who do imperatives. A categorical imperative is a
not participate in nectar and pollen gathering moral duty to be discharged regardless of any
and whose only role is to mate with a queen further end or consequence. A categorical
bee). They also recognize that the number of imperative commands ‘Do this!’; it does not
‘drones’ increases proportionately to colony say ‘Do this if’. Any action consistent with
size, which affords the drones greater auton- this is morally good, says Kant, regardless
omy. When the colony splits the ‘drones’ of its consequences. Categorical impera-
again come under the vigilant gaze of others tives are viewed as universal laws that guide
and revert to norm compliance and become humans toward their duties: ‘Act as if the
good ‘worker bees’ in the offspring colony. maxim of your action were to become through
your will a universal law of nature’ (Kant,
1964: 89).
In his Groundwork of the Metaphysics of
KANT’S MORAL JUSTIFICATION Morals, Kant says that a categorical impera-
FOR RETRIBUTION tive should be grounded in something that
should be an ‘end in itself, and an absolute
The philosopher John Mackie (1982: 3) saw value’. He finds this grounding in ‘man’,
a paradox inherent in retribution because it who ‘exists as an end in himself, not merely
‘cannot be explained or developed within a as a means for arbitrary use for this or that
reasonable system of moral thought, while, will’ (1964: 95). ‘Man’, in all his actions,
on the other hand, such a principle cannot be must always be regarded as an end in himself
eliminated from our moral thinking’. This regardless of whether his actions are directed
‘paradox’ is the result of assuming that at himself or at others. Based on this, Kant
rational deliberations about moral issues can arrives at a final definition of a categorical
take place without involving the emotions imperative:
that, as we will see, cannot be done. However,
Act in such a way that you always treat humanity,
Immanuel Kant has defended retribution
whether in your own person or in the person of
rationally and morally. Kant’s justification of any other, never simply as a means, but always at
punishment is retributive but he is aware that the same time as an end. (1964: 96)
retribution carries a message to the public of
moral disapproval of criminals and their acts, Respecting humans as ends in themselves
and therefore conveys a de facto general leads to a retributionist justification of pun-
deterrent effect. Deterrence rides on retribu- ishment. From this viewpoint, punishing
tion’s coat tails, but as a stand-alone justifica- criminals for instrumental reasons such as
tion it cannot be a moral justification for deterring others or subjecting them to reha-
punishment in Kantian terms. bilitative treatment is morally wrong because
264 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

it treats them as means to an end, not as ends right to punish us. As McBride (2007: 122)
in themselves. As Kant phrased it in The avers: ‘Punishment is the midwife in the birth
Metaphysical Elements of Justice: of the social contract’. Central to the social
contract is social integration and social order
Judicial punishment can never be used merely as a based on the strongly felt norms of right and
means to promote some other good for the criminal
himself or for civil society, but instead it must in all wrong among its members. In evolutionary
cases be imposed on him only on the ground that he times, deeply felt moral rules helped individu-
has committed a crime; for a human being can als to stay connected to the group, survive in
never be manipulated merely as a means to the dangerous environments, and garner a valua-
purposes of someone else … He must first of all be ble reputation as being trustworthy. Morals
found to be deserving of punishment before any
consideration is given of the utility of his punishment were therefore invaluable in terms of achiev-
for himself or his fellow citizens. (Kant, 1965: 331) ing shared ends.
No society can do without intolerance, indigna-
According to Kant, criminals are ruled by tion, and disgust; they are the forces behind the
reason just as everyone else and thus live in moral law. (Moore, 1987: 199)
accordance with maxims – universal moral
standards (‘Act as if the maxim of your action Moore adds: ‘In this view, the emotions of a
were to become through your will a universal people constitute moral truth’ (1987: 199).
law of nature’). If criminals harm others, they Individuals who freeload or cheat (we call
are violating the autonomy of others, and by the worst of them criminals) will always
doing so, they are said to be endorsing criminal prosper in a population of unconditional
acts as universal laws, and thus in effect they cooperators whom evolutionary psycholo-
are saying that others should act similarly. By gists call ‘suckers’. Cheats would soon drive
punishing criminals, the state is treating them suckers to extinction. It has been consistently
in accordance with their own maxims; that is, shown in computer simulation games such as
how they think others should be treated. The the prisoner’s dilemma that in a mixed popu-
state is thus allowing criminals to decide how lation of cooperators and cheats, cheats
they will be treated, and by doing so, it is always do better than cooperators when no
respecting their judgment and autonomy. Thus, punitive response is forthcoming (Klein,
Kant says of the criminal: ‘His own evil deed 2012; Nowak, 2006). Of course, very few
draws the punishment upon himself’ (in humans are suckers. The vast majority of us
Rachels, 1986: 123). Retribution is a ‘just are ‘grudgers’ who cooperate conditionally.
deserts’ model demanding that criminals be Grudgers can be cheated because they abide
punished in proportion to the harm they have by the norms of mutual trust and cooperation
inflicted on their victims; thus death would be and expect the same from others. Once
a proportional punishment for someone who cheated, however, grudgers will react differ-
has taken the life of another. ently to cheaters in the future in tit-for-tat
ways (Wiebe, 2011).
Anyone stealing resources or sexual mates
in our species’ evolutionary history consti-
THE MORAL LAW WITHIN tuted a severe threat to everyone in a group
relying on strong norms of reciprocity, and
Humans live under an implied social contract thus would have generated intense negative
by which we surrender some of our freedoms emotions such as anger, outrage, disgust, and
to do as we please in exchange for security. a desire to punish. As de Waal (1996: 160)
A vital part of the contract is the agreement puts it: ‘A taste of revenge is the other side
not to harm others, and if we do, we recog- of the coin of reciprocity’. Victims feel angry
nize the fact that the state has the legitimate and hurt when treated unfairly. Victims also
EVOLUTION AND PUNISHMENT 265

feel confusion and frustration at losing the are concerned here with the moral or ‘social’
expectation of the predictability of reciproc- emotions such as guilt, shame, empathy, and
ity. The sum of these evolved emotions is embarrassment, although the more basic
moral outrage. Without moral outrage there emotions such as anger, disgust, and joy
would be no internal motivation to react come into play in punishment. These social
against those who violate the norms of recip- emotions that evolved as integral parts of our
rocal cooperation, cheats would have thrived social intelligence provide clues about the
without the threat of punishment in our kinds of relationships (cooperative vs unco-
ancestral environments, and we would have operative) that we are likely to have with
evolved as a quite different species of animal others, and serve as ‘commitment devices’
(Haidt, 2012; Nowak, 2006). and ‘guarantors of threats and promises’
However, natural selection does not pass (Mealey, 1995: 525). Barkow (1989: 121)
its judgment on emotions, or even on the describes them as involuntary and invasive
behaviors they motivate, if those behaviors ‘limbic system overrides’ that serve to adjust
do not result in enhanced reproductive suc- our behavior in social situations. The social
cess. Natural selection operates on the con- emotions animate, focus, and modify neural
sequences of the behavior motivated by the activity in ways that lead us to choose certain
emotion (Massey, 2002; Walsh, 2006). It is responses over other possible responses from
no use silently feeling angry and hurt when the streams of information we constantly
victimized. Those feelings must generate receive. The social emotions move us to
behavior designed to prevent it occurring behave in ways that enhanced our distant
again. Negative feelings caused by victimi- ancestors’ reproductive success by overriding
zation are mediated by punishing violators neocortical decisions suggesting alternatives
because punishment signals the restoration to cooperation (i.e., cheating) which may
of fairness and predictability (the perception have been more rational in the short term, but
that cheaters may be less likely to cheat in which were ultimately fitness reducing.
the future, and that potential cheaters may One of the most salient emotions in peo-
be deterred). The positive feelings accom- ple’s punishment urges is empathy; not for
panying the punishment of those who have the person to be punished but rather for his
wronged us, coupled with the reduction of or her victim. Rob Canton (2015: 59) main-
negative feelings, provide powerful rein- tains, ‘The emotions of punishment are dis-
forcement for punitive responses and may tinctly moral emotions. They are emotions
explain why we see increased blood flow to of judgement, of righteousness and repro-
the brain’s reward centers when wrongdoers bation’. Canton is implying that people feel
are punished. these emotions not just for themselves, but
also on behalf of victims they have never
met, and they are therefore deeply prosocial
emotions linked to concern for others. This
THE IMPORTANCE OF MORAL is the emotion we call empathy. Empathy is
EMOTIONS IN UNDERSTANDING an ancient capacity predating the emergence
PUNITIVE BEHAVIOR of Homo sapiens that evolved rapidly in the
context of parental care and is found only in
We have been waxing about such things as mammals and some bird species (de Waal,
moral outrage, disgust, and revenge, but 2008). Empathy is the cognitive and emo-
aren’t these things all primitive emotions, tional ability to understand the feelings and
and surely we have progressed beyond rely- distress of others as if they were our own.
ing on such unreasoned criteria when decid- The cognitive component allows us to under-
ing what to do with criminals? Primarily, we stand their distress and why they are feeling
266 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

it, and the emotional component allows us to homeostasis in the face of temptation and
to feel that distress ourselves (‘I feel your the decision not to give in, is the major deter-
pain’). Feelings of distress are discomforting minant of conscience development. It is obvi-
because they trigger stress/anxiety hormones ous that ‘doing the right thing’ is much more
(i.e., cortisol), giving us the unconscious than rational consideration since that thing
feeling that we are threatened and motivating we call a conscience typically moves us in
us to ‘do something’. that direction before we have had an oppor-
To the extent that we feel empathy for oth- tunity to contemplate our options (Barkow’s
ers, we have a visceral motivation to take some ‘limbic system override’).
action to alleviate their distress if we are able;
thus empathy is the emotional component of
altruistic actions. Performing a helpful action
on behalf of another alleviates the distress of RATIONALITY AND EMOTION: THE
the other individual and thereby we alleviate HEADS AND TAILS OF DECISION-
our own. Thus, empathy has a selfish compo- MAKING
nent, but it is a very good thing that it does
because if we were lacking in emotional con- The ancient notion that rationality and emo-
nectedness to others, we would be callously tion are polar opposites is no longer viable. It
indifferent to their needs and suffering. Once has long been known in neuroscience that
locked into the human repertoire, empathetic cognition is always suffused with emotion
feelings can be diffused to a wider network and cognition with emotion (Nowak and
of social relationships, which is why empa- Sigmund, 2005). Our emotional and rational
thy is considered ‘the heart of mammalian neural mechanisms work in unison and sus-
development, limbic regulation, and social tain each other, and often cannot be separated.
organization’ (Farrow and Woodruff, 2007: As we have seen, decision based on emotions
51). Empathy is arguably the quintessential are typically more effective when the decision
social emotion, and therefore it is difficult to to be made is a moral one than a decision
dismiss its impact on punishment decisions arrived at after pondering all possible out-
as irrational. comes and implications. Neural-network
We recognize that doing the right thing research has shown that emotion and reason
when there is a strong temptation to do other- are fully integrated in the lateral prefrontal
wise as conscience. In The Descent of Man, cortex (LPFC): ‘the convergence of both cog-
Charles Darwin opined that conscience is nition and affective/motivational information
based on two things: the social instincts and enables the LPFC to dynamically weigh mul-
cognition and habit: ‘the social instincts – the tiple lines of information in guiding action’
prime principle of man’s moral constitution – (Pessoa, 2008: 154). Although emotion and
with the aid of active intellectual powers rationality are two inseparable components
and the effects of habit, naturally lead to the of all that we think and do, neuroscience
golden rule’ (Darwin, 1896: 106). Obviously, informs us that when the two are in conflict,
we must have a cognitive awareness of what emotion usually triumphs over reason
is right and wrong, but the consensus of opin- (Verweij et al., 2015).
ion agrees with Darwin that the emotional Emotions are assumed to be located in a
component of conscience is most important set of brain structures called the limbic sys-
(Choy et  al., 2015; Kochanska and Aksan, tem, which is a multi-structural system of
2004). The degree to which the sympathetic behavioral regulation that predates the evolu-
branch of the autonomic nervous system tion of the prefrontal cortex structures where
(ANS) is activated, and/or the speed at which our reasoning power is housed by at least a
the parasympathetic branch returns the ANS million years (Suwa et al., 2009). As Douglas
EVOLUTION AND PUNISHMENT 267

Massey (2002: 15) notes: ‘Emotionality implications supplied by the social emotions
clearly preceded rationality in evolution- of guilt, shame, empathy, and embarrassment
ary sequence, and as rationality developed that explains their activities. Psychopaths
it did not replace emotionality as the basis feel more primitive primary emotions such
for human interaction. Rather, rational abili- as anger and fear, but due to the poor con-
ties were gradually added to preexisting and nections between the limbic system and the
simultaneously developing emotional capaci- prefrontal cortex, they are not guided by
ties’. Jonathan Haidt (2001: 819) puts it even rational considerations, just as their rational
more strongly: ‘It [emotion] comes first in considerations are not guided by the social
phylogeny, it emerges first in ontogeny, it emotions. When the psychopath does display
is triggered more quickly in real-time judg- emotions, they are indeed ‘confused pas-
ments, and it is more powerful and irrevoca- sions’ and ‘pathological’.
ble [than rationality] when the two systems A retributive punishment justification is the
yield conflicting judgments’. only justification associated with deep natural
Anyone who wishes to challenge the emotions. When people hear of some vicious
primacy of emotion over rationality in criminal act such as the rape and murder of a
making moral decisions will have to confront young girl, they become angry, outraged, and
the evidence of psychopathy. If relying on disgusted. Their first inclination is to want
rationality in making punitive decisions is the to exact some sort of retribution; it is highly
mark of a ‘civilized’ being, and introducing unlikely that their first thoughts should be
emotion into the equation is irrational and of deterrence or rehabilitation. Only a cal-
barbaric, then the callous psychopath must lous psychopath would shrug his shoulders
be the epitome of the civilized being. He/ upon hearing of such a crime because it had
she is the consummate expert in using his/ no impact on his life, which is, of course,
her rational faculties – unencumbered by the entirely ‘rational’. The rest of us would feel
emotions of guilt, shame, embarrassment, and empathy for the victim and her grieving fam-
empathy – to connive and manipulate others ily. However, this retributive urge still tends
into doing his/her bidding, and has been to be damned as ‘irrational’, ‘uncivilized’ by
defined as an ‘obligate cheater’ (da Silva et al., good liberal humanists who find it entirely
2015; Mealey, 1995; Quinsey, 2002). The inappropriate. Retribution, they rightly com-
psychopath knows the rational words of the plain, is punishment for its own sake from
moral song, but not its emotional music. This which nothing good can come, and which
results in an entirely utilitarian self-centered degrades society. Wanting something good to
pattern of behavior (Jorgensen et al., 2016). come from punishment is a utilitarian position
The defining neurobiological feature of by which the only justification for punishment
psychopaths is that the rational and emotional is its alleged positive consequences.
areas of the brain are poorly integrated (Raine, Moore (1987) sees this position as hypo-
2013; Wiebe, 2011). This has been demon- critical since no objections to emotional out-
strated using numerous techniques such as rage are offered when it is aimed at unjust
EEG, PET, fMRI, and even at the molecular punishment of the innocent. Don’t we all
level by diffuse tensor imaging (DTI), which experience feelings of outrage when we
tracks the movement of molecules along hear of an innocent man being released from
white-matter tracts to and from the ‘rational’ prison after 20 years through DNA exon-
prefrontal cortex and the ‘emotional’ limbic eration? Most people are emotionally dis-
system (Craig et al., 2009; Walsh and Bolen, turbed when they perceive innocent people
2012). Psychopaths are fully able to under- are being or have been punished, even if it
stand the moral norms of society rationally; has resulted in some positive consequence
it is their lack of understanding of their moral for society. Why then do many consider it
268 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

barbaric when the same emotional outrage some level people track crime as reported
is aimed at the acts of guilty people? If the in the news media and form their attitudes
moral judgment in the first instance is virtu- accordingly. We wish to emphasize that
ous, then it is virtuous in the second instance the following discussion of death penalty
also. The retributionist would say that if you ­decision-making implies neither support nor
demand a positive outcome from punishment non-support of capital punishment. It is meant
for its own sake, then that would be justice only to emphasize the role of evolved human
being done. The retributionist would also say emotions in the juror’s decision, for they must
that punishing the innocent, regardless of make it, not the judge. Additionally, we also
what positive consequences may come from note that it is possible to have no objection
it (such as general deterrence), is impermis- to the death penalty on moral grounds, but
sible and unjust. object to it on practical grounds. For exam-
ple, the death penalty is much more costly
to our public coffers compared with life
in prison without the possibility of parole.
THE ROLE OF EVOLVED EMOTIONS IN What is more, our criminal justice system is
CAPITAL-PUNISHMENT DECISIONS imperfect and may have executed an innocent
person in the past and may continue to do so
The issue of capital punishment is a deeply in the future if the death penalty remains an
moral one, and as we have seen, the founda- option.
tion for morality is more emotional than Serving on a jury in a case in which the
rational. The deep emotional underpinning of prosecutor is seeking the death penalty and
morality, and thus the morality we bring with knowing that you and 11 other jurors must
us to these issues, is generally not appreci- vote for life or death must be an exhausting
ated by people who cannot understand why emotional rollercoaster ride. On what does
equally reasonable people can arrive at oppo- one base one’s vote to execute another human
site conclusions regarding the issue of capital being or spare him or her? During the trial
punishment. Two equally intelligent people phase, jurors have learned a bit about the law
can have access to exactly the same informa- and how the judge says they must apply it,
tion on a deep moral issue, but differ radi- but they have also learned many gruesome
cally as to its meaning based on the emotions details that have alarmed, shocked, and dis-
it generates. Capital punishment thus pro- gusted them. Rule 403 of the federal Rules of
vides us with a meaningful stage on which to Evidence allows evidence to be excluded if it
present the role of emotions in punishment risks unfair prejudice, which ‘means an undue
since the death penalty is the ultimate and tendency to suggest decision on an improper
irrevocable punishment. basis, commonly, though not necessarily, an
Support for capital punishment waxes emotional one’. However, the gloves are off.
and wanes in the United States with crime When the trial is over and the defendant has
rates, especially violent crime rates. A 2018 been found guilty beyond a reasonable doubt,
Pew Research Center poll found that 54% the jurors must then deliberate on the mur-
of Americans favor the death penalty while derer’s sentence after a sentencing hearing
39% oppose it (Oliphant, 2018). Public sup- when the full force of emotion is evident.
port of the death penalty was at its lowest at According to Bandes (2008: 493): ‘When
42% in 1966 when the violent crime rate was asked to determine what sort of punishment
200 per 100,000 population, and at 80% in heinous murderers deserve, people consult
1994 when the violent crime rate peaked at their moral, ethical, and religious beliefs.
713.6 per 100,000 (Hatch and Walsh, 2016: They consult their emotional reactions – their
111). These figures clearly indicate that at empathy, disgust, and moral outrage’.
EVOLUTION AND PUNISHMENT 269

Bandes (2008: 495) further notes that: ‘The The urge to punish can also be seen as loyalty
capital [punishment] system is built upon to the group and an expression of commitment
choices among competing emotional claims – to it. Offenders are subconsciously viewed
some acknowledged and others not’. The as traitors and enemies of the group – ‘not one
competing emotional claims are abundantly of us’ – which serves to neutralize our feel-
evident in capital trials. In a capital case, ings of care and compassion for them. By their
prosecutors play on the jury’s fear, disgust, crimes, offenders have demonstrated a lack of
anger, and sympathy for the victim, and ask respect for authority, which represents the pos-
that the defendant be sentenced to death for sibility of the breakdown of the social contract.
what he or she has done. Then comes the This possibility generates anxiety among those
defense attorney who asks jurors to consider who have a deep respect for authority and social
the humanity of the defendant, trying to order and view just deserts as a way of increas-
evoke sympathy by conveying his or her ter- ing social order. Canton reminds us that crimes
ribly abusive childhood, and begs for mercy. for which the death penalty is sought evoke a
After which comes the very tearful testimony deep sense of negative emotion, especially
of the families of both the victim and the con- when multiple victims, children, and/or torture
victed. At the end of it all, the judge instructs are involved. We often hear metaphors for such
the jury to set their emotions aside and decide criminals as ‘filth’ or ‘scum’ and seeing them
the defendant’s fate by a rational deliberation punished described as an act of ‘cleansing’
of the law and the facts before them, but we (Canton, 2015: 66).
know that they cannot since: Reasoned argu- There are those who believe that not only
ment has limited effect because it is not rea- can jurors not eliminate emotion, but also
soning that prompts the judgement. (Canton, that they should not:
2015: 68)
Perhaps it is not so much that emotion is a key to
Canton (2015) offers us a number of social normative judgment as it is a key to important and
emotions and how they engage our evolved effective normative judgment, normative judg-
retributive urge, all of which are doubtlessly ment that gets our attention and gets translated
engaged during jury deliberations. He notes into action, either with respect to our own conduct
or to the reward or punishment of others.
that empathetic compassion for the victim and
(Goodenough and Prehn, 2004: 1717)
the victim’s survivors is perhaps the primary
emotion, remarking that ‘retributive emotions We should expect emotions to render the
reflect a decent compassion for a victim’s (or most effective moral judgments given their
survivor’s) distress and a virtuous expression long evolutionary history as the only basis
of solidarity with members of our community’ for hominid social interaction prior to the
(2015: 62). Only the ultimate punishment can evolution of our vaunted rationality. If these
achieve justice for the victim(s) and right the ‘effective moral judgments’ are more often
wrong. Jurors would also focus on the deep than not retributive when it comes to moral
instinct of fairness – Canton asserts that retri- decisions about punishing transgressors, one
bution ‘restores a balance: the offender’s unjust may wonder why Kant’s proportional retri-
profit or gain from the crime must be redressed bution is often viewed so negatively.
or annulled…The retributivist does not hit,
but hits back’ (2015: 63). There is no turning
of the other cheek for retributionists for it is
only virtuous to turn a cheek if it is one’s own SECOND- AND THIRD-PARTY
that was slapped. Rightly or wrongly, the ret- PUNISHMENT
ributionist views the convicted person as pos-
sessing the free will to make choices, and It is not always possible to punish those who
the condemned person has made evil ones. harm us and we must turn to others to carry
270 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

the burden. However, why would others take not directly benefit (like second-party chimp
on your risky burden with no apparent payoff alpha males) from meting out punishment.
for them? Among chimpanzees, alpha males This type of punishment is more impartial
take on the role of what is called control and less egocentric than second-party pun-
behavior, which includes the punishment of ishment, thereby minimizing the likelihood
troop members who bully and exploit others of a retaliatory tit-for-tat feedback loop (Fehr
(de Waal, 1996). This is a costly and risky role, and Fischbacher, 2004). However, there is
but it confers a number of benefits to the alpha some evidence to suggest that third parties
male. As an arbiter and punisher, alpha males do not punish differently than second par-
typically show a preference for the weaker ties (Leibbrandt and Lopez-Perez, 2012).
party in most disputes. This develops support Third-party punishers in modern societies
among the weaker regular members and serves are typically agents of the state operating
to level the hierarchy. Leveling the hierarchy in accordance with the law. For example,
increases the gap between the alpha male and offenders are caught by police, prosecuted
the more powerful members of the group who in court, and then sentenced by judges.
might seek to replace him (de Waal, 1996). However, other informal third-party pun-
Maintaining one’s position as the alpha male ishments may be doled out to offenders by
affords him status, allowing him ‘first dibs’ on school administrators or church officials.
the females when they come into estrus. A host Informal third-party punishment occurs more
of computer simulation studies of human altru- frequently than state-sponsored third-party
istic punishment (punishment on behalf of punishment and is essential to the enforce-
others) have conclusively shown that the pun- ment of norms (Bendor and Swistak, 2001).
isher receives many benefits, including an Numerous experiments have shown that third
increased likelihood of receiving future bene- parties will punish cheats at a cost to them-
fits, with enhanced status in the group being selves. A study involving 1,762 subjects from
the most valuable because it leads to enhanced five continents found that in all populations,
fitness (Ule et  al., 2009). Dos Santos et  al. people are willing to punish defectors who
(2011) remark that: “reputation is the key to have harmed unknown others (Henrich et al.,
the evolution of punishment, and that simple 2006). This study also found that ‘societies
reputation games can explain the high preser- with high degrees of punishment will also
vation of punishment in humans” (2011: 376). exhibit more altruistic behavior’ (Henrich
Leading theories of third-party punishment et  al., 2006: 1770). This tends to suggest
include inequity aversion and strong reciproc- altruism and punishment coevolved in the
ity. Inequity aversion favors fairness and rejects sense that ‘Third-party punishment of norm
inequitable outcomes. Humans (and primates) violations (‘I punish you because you harmed
are willing to forgo benefits to themselves if him’) seems especially crucial for the evo-
they perceive others receiving a greater and lutionary stability of cooperation and is the
unfair reward. This rejection of injustice helps cornerstone of modern models of criminal
to punish cheats and stabilize cooperation justice’ (Buckholz and Marois, 2012: 655).
among group members. Strong reciprocity
refers to the tendency to cooperate with group
members even when there are no immediate
benefits of doing so. It is therefore advanta- FROM PRIMITIVE VENGEANCE
geous to the group to punish non-cooperators TO MODERN LAW
and to use punishment to ensure fairness.
In modern societies, punishment is meted Just as we can become alcoholics, obese, and
out by third-party punishers who are indi- sex addicts partaking too freely in what feels
viduals not directly harmed and who will good, or even vital in the right proportions,
EVOLUTION AND PUNISHMENT 271

punishment can overstep its optimum and noted that many societies have moved from
become dysfunctional. In neurological imag- retributive to restitutive justice. For Durkheim
ing studies showing blood flow to the brain’s (1964), retributive justice is driven by the
pleasure center, when people witness the natural passion for punitive revenge that
punishment of those who have harmed them, ‘ceases only when exhausted … only after it
the punishment tends to be proportional to has destroyed’ (1964: 86). Restitutive justice,
the harm caused. Only a callous sadist would on the other hand, is driven by simple deter-
take pleasure in seeing grossly disproportion- rence, and is more humanistic and tolerant,
ate punishment imposed. Punishment that although it is still ‘at least in part, a work of
exceeds just and reasonable boundaries vengeance’ since it is still ‘an expiation’
induces disgust and repugnance, and many (1964: 88–89). While both forms satisfy the
people feel that the death penalty is one such human urge for social regularity, repressive
excessive punishment. justice oversteps its adaptive usefulness and
Even given the well-founded evolutionary becomes socially destructive, while restitutive
reasons for retributionist feelings, we must justice offers a rational balance between calm-
reflect on the propriety of acting on them ing moral outrage on the one hand, and engag-
completely. Just because these feelings are ing empathy and sympathy on the other.
natural, it does not mean that we should act Culture may engage or neutralize the emo-
on them and ignore rational consequential- tions that temper punishment with mercy or
ist considerations. If the primitive desire to allow vengeance to run wild. The lasting influ-
‘get even’ is left untamed, it can tear a social ence of Cesare Beccaria rests on his recogni-
group apart by generating a cycle of tit-for- tion that the brutal acts of retribution that were
tat blood feuds, which have smeared human common in the 18th century resulted in gen-
history (Boehm, 2011). It has been estimated eral distrust of powerful institutions and social
that approximately 30% of adult male deaths alienation rather than altruistic cooperation. In
among the Yanomamo of South America are other words, Beccaria argued that excessive
related to revenge feuds, which expand the punishment is indicative of tyranny and the
very injustice that ‘righteous’ revenge was associated lack of perceived legitimacy accom-
supposed to assuage (Chagnon, 1988). As panying tyrannical institutions fails to inspire
Susan Jacoby (1983: 13) put it: cooperation. Many of Beccaria’s recommended
criminal justice reforms were implemented
The struggle to contain revenge has been con-
ducted at the highest level of moral and civic throughout much of Europe within his lifetime
awareness at each stage in the development of (Durant and Durant, 1967). Such radical and
civilization. The self-conscious nature of the effort rapid change suggests that Beccaria’s ideas
is expectable in view of the persistent state of ten- tapped into and broadened other evolved emo-
sion between uncontrolled vengeance as destroyer tions such as sympathy and empathy among
and controlled vengeance as an unavoidable com-
ponent of justice. the European elite. We tend to feel empathy
for those whom we view as being ‘like us’,
Just as evolutionary scientists view cheating and empathy often leads to sympathy, which
and punishment as vital to the evolution of may translate the vicarious experiencing of the
cooperation, Durkheim saw it as necessary for pains of others into an active concern for their
maintaining social solidarity, and that it does welfare, even if they are wrongdoers. Vignette
so by reaffirming the justness of the social studies have shown that people tend to recom-
norms. He recognized that the urge to punish mend more lenient punishment for criminals
is inherent in human nature and that it serves whom they perceive to be similar to themselves
an expiatory role, but he also recognized that (reviewed in Miller and Vidmar, 1981), and the
we could temper the urge with sympathy. march of democracy has drawn more people
Over the course of human history, Durkheim into the circle of people we consider ‘us’.
272 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

RECONCILIATION AND rather than retaliation in the next round of the


REINTEGRATION: FORGIVENESS game, and in most circumstances, it will return
TIT-FOR-TAT the players to cooperation. Of course, we are
dealing with game theory here, with its ideal-
Just as social emotions such as empathy may ized laboratory conditions and assumption of
rational actors; the real world is expected to
lead to more lenient sentencing, the existence
be far more complicated. However, the predic-
of this emotion may increase the likelihood for
tions based on game-theoretical models have
forgiveness of offenders (Batson and Ahmad,
been remarkably consistent with real-world
2001; Batson and Moran, 1999). This empathy-
findings on numerous occasions (Levitt and
induced altruism has some evolutionarily
List, 2007). Cosmides and Tooby (1992) offer
advantageous outcomes that benefit social
the example of the Ache tribe of Paraguay
groups. In societies where cheating is likely to
(and certain other hunter/gatherer tribes) who
occur, forgiveness tit-for-tat operates to benefit
respond differently to hunters who cheat than
social groups by avoiding social actions that
to gatherers who cheat. Meat is a scarce and
degrade into absolute punishment. If two strict
valued resource, and is shared equally by all
tit-for-tat strategists play one another and one
when available, which is often a matter of luck
cheats (purposely or otherwise), it tends to
and hunting skills. Plant food is a low-variance
launch a long series of mutual punitive item, the availability of which depends only
response resulting in a loss for both. Work in on the effort spent on gathering it. Arguments
game theory indicates that a measure of for- erupt when plant gatherers are perceived as not
giveness may work better to maintain social pulling their weight. Punishing lazy gatherers
cooperation rather than always paying back has no adverse effect on the availability of plant
defectors in kind (Rand et al., 2009). If people food; it is still there for the gathering. Given
play strict tit-for-tat (always punishing non- the highly variable nature of meat acquisition,
cooperation in kind) in their relationships they however, tribal members are more forgiving of
risk losing a valuable relationship if the cheat- hunters whom they perceive as cheating. It is
ing incident was uncharacteristic of the person, recognized that a charge of cheating can be the
or accidental (Wagstaff, 1998). As Machalek result of a false perception (hunting is often
and Cohen (1991: 221) put it: ‘Forgiving strat- a matter of sheer luck as much as effort). If
egies can transform a pattern of mutual cheat- because of the false charge the hunter is pun-
ing by not remaining permanently punitive but ished by ostracism, the whole tribe will lose
instead by cooperating, even when the other a valuable cooperator in the tricky business
strategy cheats’. A large number of game-theory of hunting for meat, and the meat supply will
studies have found that strict tit-for-tat can become less stable and predictable.
amount to punishment that is just too costly for Continuing to invite cooperation in the face
the punisher, that all parties tend to suffer to of defection is not a ‘sucker’ strategy because
some extent, and that the generous (forgiving) it is not one that tolerates continued cheat-
tit-for-tat strategy invariably prevails (Rand ing. It is similar to sentencing an offender to
et  al., 2009). Although some social emotions probation rather than prison, thus leaving the
may drive this behavior across generations, door open to future mutually advantageous
findings focused on other social emotions such cooperation between offender and com-
as compassion muddy these waters. Weng munity. The ‘forgiving tit-for-tat’ strategy
et  al. (2015) found that while compassion is captured most familiarly in Braithwaite’s
enhances giving to victims, it does not affect (1989) concept of reintegrative shaming.
punishment of transgressors. It is Braithwaite’s position that we should
The generous tit-for-tat strategy forgives a retaliate against tit-for-tat defectors, but that
single betrayal by responding with cooperation retaliation should be conciliatory rather than
EVOLUTION AND PUNISHMENT 273

permanently punitive. Punishment (and the offenders, the perceived satisfaction with
shame accompanying it) should be reintegra- restorative justice is less pronounced in juve-
tive and motivate offenders to evaluate them- niles as compared to adults (Gal and Moyal,
selves and their behavior, not disintegrative. 2011; Kim and Gerber, 2012). Because of
Both the offender and the community benefit its ability to foster more cooperative socie-
by methods used to express our disapproval ties however, forgiveness tit-for-tat in game
of offenders that conforms to the reintegra- theory provides a degree of evolutionary and
tive ideal. If we always respond to defecting mathematical justification for restorative
with strong punitive reactions, the commu- justice, which is the only corrections model
nity loses a potentially valuable cooperator, aimed at reintegrating offenders back into the
and the offender becomes, to his or her great community with the help of the community.
disadvantage, alienated from it.
Reconciliatory behavior is common
among primate species, which indicates
that forgiving tit-for-tat had some positive REFERENCES
fitness consequences. In restorative-justice
programs, victims who confront their vic- Adolphs, R. (2009). The social brain: Neural
timizers in controlled settings and offer for- basis of social knowledge. Annual Review of
giveness to them report feeling positive (a Psychology, 60, 693–716.
Ash, J., & Gallup, G. G. (2007). Paleoclimatic
sense that justice had been done) about the
variation and brain expansion during human
experience (reviewed in Latimer et al., 2005). evolution. Human Nature, 18(2), 109–124.
Satisfaction with restorative justice is, how- Baden, J., & Stroup, R. (1972). Choice, faith, and
ever, contingent on procedural factors (trust, politics: The political economy of Hutterian
neutrality, respect, etc.) as well as situational communes. Public Choice, 12, 1–11.
factors such as flexibility and provisions of Bailey, D. H., & Geary, D. C. (2009). Hominid
care for the victim (Van Camp and Wemmers, brain evolution. Human Nature, 20(1),
2013). Thus, even forgiveness has rather 67–79.
strong elements of tit-for-tat. Bandes, S. (2008). Repellent crimes and rational
Restorative-justice principles that tap an deliberation: Emotion and the death penalty.
offender’s capacity for empathy for their vic- Vermont Law Review, 33, 489–519.
Barkow, J. H. (1989). The elastic between
tim are most fully implemented by juvenile
genes and culture. Ethology and Sociobiology,
justice agencies. Juvenile agencies implicitly 10(1–3), 111–129.
recognize that delinquent behavior is normal Batson, C. D., & Ahmad, N. (2001). Empathy-
behavior; indeed, the young male who does induced altruism in a prisoner’s dilemma II:
not engage in some sort of delinquent behav- What if the target of empathy has defected?
ior is statistically abnormal (Moffitt, 1993). European Journal of Social Psychology,
Adolescence and young adulthood is a period 31(1), 25–36.
of intense inter-male competition ultimately Batson, C. D., & Moran, T. (1999). Empathy-
(if not always consciously) aimed at securing induced altruism in a prisoner’s dilemma.
more mating opportunities than the next male European Journal of Social Psychology,
(McKibbin and Shackleford, 2011; Wiebe, 29(7), 909–924.
Bendor, J., & Swistak, P. (2001). The evolution
2011). To adopt a strategy of strict tit-for-tat
of norms. American Journal of Sociology,
rather than forgiveness tit-for-tat with juve- 106, 1493–1545.
nile offenders, whose physical desires and Boehm, C. (2011). Retaliatory violence in
abilities have temporarily outrun their neu- human prehistory. British Journal of
rological maturity, would be severely coun- Criminology, 51, 518–534.
terproductive. Although the evolutionary Bohm, R. (2012). Deathquest: An introduction
benefits are greater when applied to juvenile to the theory and practice of capital
274 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

punishment in the United States. Boston, De Quervain, D., Fischbacher, U., Valerie, T.,
MA: Anderson. Schellhammer, M., Schnyder, U., Buch, A., &
Braithwaite, J. (1989). Crime, shame, and Fehr, E. (2004). The neural basis of altruistic
reintegration. Cambridge: Cambridge punishment. Science, 305, 1254–1259.
University Press. De Waal, F. (1996). Good natured: The origins
Brunero, J. (2002). Evolution, altruism and of right and wrong in humans and other
internal reward explanations. Philosophical animals. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Forum, 33, 413–424. Press.
Buckholtz, J., & Marois, R. (2012). The roots of De Waal, F. (2008). Putting the altruism back
modern justice: Cognitive and neural into altruism: The evolution of empathy.
foundations of social norms and their Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 279–300.
enforcement. Nature Neuroscience, 13, Dos Santos, M., Rankin, D., & Wedekind, C.
655–661. (2011). The evolution of punishment through
Buonanno, P., & Raphael, S. (2013). reputation. Proceedings of the Royal Society:
Incarceration and incapacitation: Evidence Biology, 278, 371–377.
from the 2006 Italian collective pardon. Dunbar, R. I., & Shultz, S. (2007). Evolution
American Economic Review, 103(6), in the social brain. Science, 317(5843),
2437–2465. 1344–1347.
Canton, R. (2015). Crime, punishment and the Durant, W., & Durant, A. (1967). Rousseau and
moral emotions: Righteous minds and their revolution. New York: Simon and Schuster.
attitudes towards punishment. Punishment Durkheim, E. (1964). The division of labor in
& Society, 17, 54–72. society. Glencoe, IL: Free Press.
Chagnon, N. A. (1988). Life histories, blood Farrow, T., & Woodruff, P. (2007). Empathy in
revenge, and warfare in a tribal population. mental illness. Cambridge: Cambridge
Science, 239(4843), 985–992. University Press.
Choy, O., Farrington, D. P., & Raine, A. (2015). Fehr, E., & Fischerbacher, U. (2004). Third party
The need to incorporate autonomic arousal punishment and social norms. Evolution and
in developmental and life-course research Human Behavior, 25, 63–87.
and theories. Journal of Developmental and Furman vs. Georgia. 408 U.S. 238 (1972).
Life-Course Criminology, 1(2), 189–207. Gal, T., & Moyal, S. (2011). Juvenile victims in
Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1994). Better than restorative justice: Findings from the
rational: Evolutionary psychology and the reintegrative shaming experiments. The
invisible hand. The American Economic British Journal of Criminology, 51(6),
Review, 84, 327–332. 1014–1034.
Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1992). Cognitive Gavrilets, S. (2012). On the evolutionary origins
adaptations for social exchange. The of the egalitarian syndrome. Proceedings of
Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the National Academy of Sciences, 109(35),
the Generation of Culture, 163, 163–228. 14069–14074.
Craig, M., Catani, M., Deeley, Q., Latham, R., Goodenough, O., & Prehn, K. (2004). A
Daly, E., Kanaan, R., Picchioni, M., McGuire, neuroscientific approach to normative
P., Fahy, T., & Murphy, D. (2009). Altered judgment in law and justice. Philosophical
connections on the road to psychopathy. Transactions of the Royal Society of London:
Molecular Psychiatry, 14, 946–953. Biology, 359, 1709–1726.
Currie, E. (1999). Reflections on crime and Gottfredson, M., & Hirschi, T. (1990) A general
criminology at the millennium. Western theory of crime. Stanford University Press.
Criminology Review, 2(1), 1–13. Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its
Da Silva, D. R., Rijo, D., & Salekin, R. T. (2015). rational tail: A social intuitionalist approach
The evolutionary roots of psychopathy. to moral judgment. Psychological Review,
Aggression and Violent Behavior, 21, 85–96. 108, 814–834.
Darwin, C. (1896). The descent of man and Haidt, J. (2012). The righteous mind: Why
selection in relation to sex (Vol. 1). D. New good people are divided by politics and
York: Appleton. religion. New York: Pantheon.
EVOLUTION AND PUNISHMENT 275

Hatch, V., & Walsh, A. (2016). Capital punishment in ten simple games. Journal of
punishment: The theory and practice of the Economic Behavior and Organization, 84,
ultimate penalty. New York: Oxford University 753–766.
Press. Levitt, S. D., & List, J. A. (2007). What do
Hawthorne, N. (1879). The scarlet letter. The laboratory experiments measuring social
Blithedale romance (Vol. 9). New York: preferences reveal about the real world?
Houghton, Osgood. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 21(2),
Henrich, J., McElreath, R., Barr, A., Ensminger, J., 153–174.
Barrett, C., Bolyanatz, A., Cardenas, J., Logan, C., & Gaes, G. (1993). Meta-analysis
Gurven, M., Gwako, E., Henrich, N., and the rehabilitation of punishment. Justice
Lesorogot, C., Marlowe, F., Tracy, D., & Ziker, Quarterly, 10, 245–263.
J. (2006). Costly punishment across human Machalek, R., & Cohen, L. (1991). The nature of
societies. Science, 312, 1767–1770. crime: Is cheating necessary for cooperation?
Hill, R. A., & Dunbar, R. I. (2003). Social network Human Nature, 2, 215–233.
size in humans. Human Nature, 14, 53–72. Mackie, J. L. (1982). Morality and the retributive
Jacoby, S. (1983). Wild justice: The evolution of emotions. Criminal Justice Ethics, 1, 3–10.
revenge. New York: Harper & Row. Massey, D. (2002). A brief history of human
Jorgensen, C., Anderson, N., & Barnes, J. C. society: The origin and role of emotion in
(2016). Bad brains: Crime and drug abuse social life. American Sociological Review, 67,
from a neurocriminological perspective. Ameri- 1–29.
can Journal of Criminal Justice, 41(1), 47–69. McBride, K. (2007). Punishment and the
Kanazawa, S. (2008). Temperature and political order. Ann Arbor, MI: University of
evolutionary novelty as forces behind the Michigan Press.
evolution of general intelligence. Intelligence, McKibbin, W. F., & Shackelford, T. K. (2011).
36(2), 99–108. Women’s avoidance of rape. Aggression and
Kant, I. (1964). Groundwork of the metaphysics Violent Behavior, 16(5), 437–443.
of morals. Trans. by H. J. Paton. New York: Mealey, L. (1995). The sociobiology of
Harper & Row. sociopathy: An integrated evolutionary
Kant, I. (1965). The metaphysical elements of model. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 18(3),
justice. Indianapolis, IN: Bobbs–Merrill. 523–541.
Kim, H. J., & Gerber, J. (2012). The effectiveness Miller, D. T., & Vidmar, N. (1981). The justice
of reintegrative shaming and restorative jus- motive in social behavior. Boston, MA:
tice conferences: Focusing on juvenile offend- Springer.
ers’ perceptions in Australian reintegrative Moffitt, T. E. (1993). Life-course-persistent and
shaming experiments. International Journal adolescence-limited antisocial behavior: A
of Offender Therapy and Comparative Crimi- developmental taxonomy. Psychological
nology, 56(7), 1063–1079. Review, 100(4), 674–701.
Klein, R. (2012). The neurobiology of altruistic Moore, M. (1987). The moral worth of
punishment: A moral assessment of its social retribution. In F. Schoeman (Ed.),
utility. In K. Plaisance & T. Reydon (Eds.), Responsibility, character, and the emotions:
Philosophy of Behavioral Biology, pp. 297– New essays in moral psychology, pp. 179–
313. Boston, MA: Springer Science. 219. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Kochanska, G., & Aksan, N. (2004). Nowak, M. (2006). Five rules for the evolution
Development of mutual responsiveness of cooperation. Science, 314, 1560–1563.
between parents and their young children. Nowak, M., & Sigmund, K. (2005). Evolution of
Child Development, 75(6), 1657–1676. indirect reciprocity. Nature, 437,
Latimer, J., Dowden, C., & Muise, D. (2005). 1291–1298.
The effectiveness of restorative justice Oliphant, B. (2018, June 11). Public support for
practices: A meta-analysis. The prison the death penalty ticks up. Pew Research
Journal, 85(2), 127–144. Center. www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2018/
Leibbrandt, A., & Lopez-Perez, R. (2012). An 06/11/us-support-for-death-penalty-ticks-
exploration of third and second party up-2018/ (Accessed 7 June 2019)
276 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Penney, S. (2012). Impulse control and criminal toward strangers. Science, 326(5960),
responsibility. International Journal of Law 1701–1704.
and Psychiatry, 35, 99–103. Van Camp, T., & Wemmers, J. A. (2013). Victim
Pessoa, L. (2008). On the relationship between satisfaction with restorative justice: More
emotion and cognition. Nature/Neuroscience, than simply procedural justice. International
9, 148–158. Review of Victimology, 19(2), 117–143.
Petersen, M. B., Sell, A., Tooby, J., & Cosmides, Verweij, M., Senior, T., Dominquez, D., &
L. (2010). Evolutionary psychology and Turner, R. (2015). Emotion, rationality, and
criminal justice: A recalibrational theory of decision-making: How to link affective and
punishment and reconciliation. In Human social neuroscience with social theory.
morality & Sociality: Evolutionary & Frontiers in Neuroscience, 9, 1–13.
comparative perspectives (Eds. Henrik Hogh- Wagstaff, G. (1998). Equity, justice, and
Oleson). Palgrave MacMillan: New York. altruism. Current Psychology, 17, 111–134.
Quinsey, V. L. (2002). Evolutionary theory and Walsh, A. (2006). Evolutionary psychology and
criminal behaviour. Legal and Criminological criminal behavior. In J. Barkow (Ed.), Missing
Psychology, 7(1), 1–13. the revolution: Darwinism for social scientists,
Rachels, J. (1986). The elements of moral pp. 225–268. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
philosophy. New York: Random House. Walsh, A., & Bolen, J. (2012). The neurobiology
Radzinowicz, L., & King, J. (1979). The growth of criminal behavior: Gene-brain culture
of crime. Harmondsworth: Penguin. co-evolution. Farnham: Ashgate.
Raine, A. (2013). The anatomy of violence: The Walsh, A., Johnson., H., & Bolen., J. (2012).
biological roots of crime. New York: Drugs, crime, and the epigenetics of hedonic
Pantheon. allostasis. Journal of Contemporary Criminal
Rand, D. G., Ohtsuki, H., & Nowak, M. A. Justice, 28, 314–328.
(2009). Direct reciprocity with costly Weng, H. Y., Fox, A. S., Hessenthaler, H. C.,
punishment: Generous tit-for-tat prevails. Stodola, D. E., & Davidson, R. J. (2015). The
Journal of Theoretical Biology, 256(1), 45–57. role of compassion in altruistic helping and
Rosebury, B. (2009). Private revenge and its punishment behavior. PLoS ONE, 10(12),
relation to punishment. Utilatus, 21, 1–21. e0143794.
Stohr, M. K., Walsh, A., & Hemmens, C. (2012). Wiebe, R. (2011). The nature and utility of low
Corrections: A text/reader (Vol. 3). Thousand self-control. In K. Beaver & A. Walsh (Eds.),
Oaks, CA: Sage. The Ashgate research companion to biosocial
Suwa, G., Asfaw, B., Kono, R., Kubo, D., Lovejoy, theories of crime, pp. 369–395. Farnham:
C., & White, T. (2009). The Ardipithecus Ashgate.
ramidus skull and its implications for hominid Wilkinson, G. S. (1990). Food sharing in vampire
origins. Science, 326, 68e1–68e8. bats. Scientific American, 262(2), 76–83.
Ule, A., Schram, A., Riedl, A., & Cason, T. N. Wilson, J. Q. (1975). The rise of the bureaucratic
(2009). Indirect punishment and generosity state. The Public Interest, 41(3), 77–103.
14
Evolutionary Psychology and
Corrections and Rehabilitation
Ian A. Silver and Jamie Newsome

INTRODUCTION The impact of the RNR model on the field


of corrections has been tremendous, but the
Identifying effective strategies for managing pursuit to achieve even greater reductions
and responding to crime is a priority for in recidivism continues. Much of this effort
nations around the world. Scholars, policy- has been focused on refining the practical
makers, and practitioners continually work to application of the model. However, some
develop crime-control policies that will scholars have suggested that important the-
enhance public safety and reduce expenditures. oretical advancements have taken place in
Meeting these goals has become critical in the recent decades, and that these developments
United States – particularly in the correctional should be explored as potential avenues for
system where the number of individuals under informing continued work in rehabilitation
some form of supervision has exceeded (Newsome and Cullen, 2017). Explanations
6.6 million adults (Kaeble and Cowhig, 2018). of behavior grounded in an evolutionary psy-
The dominant perspective on ‘what works’ in chology framework have expanded, and may
offender rehabilitation, the risk-need-respon- now present another opportunity to enhance
sivity (RNR) model, has been widely used our understanding of antisocial behavior and
throughout North America and in other regions how individuals respond to interventions
throughout the world. It combines knowledge (Bakermans-Kranenburg and van IJzendoorn,
gained from empirically supported theories 2015). This chapter explores this possibility
that offer explanations of criminal behavior by first providing an overview of the state
with practical recommendations to guide of corrections in the United States, followed
policy and practice (Bonta and Andrews, by a brief description of the RNR model of
2017). Moreover, it is supported by a wealth of offender rehabilitation. The core ideas of an
empirical evidence (Smith et al., 2009b). evolutionary psychological perspective are
278 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

then summarized, and three theories based report by Alper et  al. (2018) indicated that
on this perspective – biological sensitivity 44% of state prisoners were rearrested in the
to context theory, differential susceptibility first year after they were released, and by nine
theory, and intrasexual competition – are dis- years post-release 83% had been rearrested.
cussed with an emphasis on how they might One trend that may have contributed to these
inform corrections. high failure rates is a reduction in rehabilita-
tive initiatives (Mears and Cochran, 2015).
Some reports suggest that during the rise in
the reliance on the correctional system lower
CORRECTIONS IN THE UNITED STATES percentages of inmates received treatment
services relative to earlier periods (Lynch and
The correctional system in the United States Sabol, 2001; Phelps, 2011). Moreover, the
monitors individuals detained in institutions, majority of inmates may not be participating
such as jails and prisons, as well as those in treatment services before they leave prison
who are supervised in the community, such (Lawrence et al., 2002; Phelps, 2011).
as probationers or parolees. The total number The size of the corrections populations,
of individuals in the system grew rapidly high failure rates, and lack of treatment rep-
beginning in the 1980s (Bureau of Justice resents a complex problem. Fortunately, an
Statistics, 2016) and continued until it abundance of empirical evidence has accu-
reached a peak of 7,339,600 in 2007 (Bureau mulated that suggests strategies known as the
of Justice Statistics, 2016). This period is principles of effective intervention may be
often referred to as the ‘era of mass incar- the most promising tactics for reducing recid-
ceration’, during which the rise in the use of ivism – whether an individual is confined in
prisons to sanction offenders was so extreme an institution or supervised in the commu-
and persistent some have questioned whether nity (Bonta and Andrews, 2017; Smith et al.,
the United States may have become ‘addicted 2009b). This model, which is described in
to incarceration’ (Pratt, 2019). Others have the following section, continues to be stud-
argued that recent decades may be more ied and refined in hopes of further enhancing
accurately described as an ‘era of mass cor- policy and practice in corrections.
rections’ given the simultaneous rise in com-
munity corrections populations (Mears and
Cochran, 2015). The number of individuals The Risk-Need-Responsivity (RNR)
on some form of community supervision Model of Offender Rehabilitation
reached a high of 5,119,000 in 2007 and has
only slowly declined to 4,650,900 in recent Offender rehabilitation has come to be pre-
years (Kaeble and Cowhig, 2018). dominantly founded on the principles of
The growth in the correctional system effective intervention, a set of guidelines for
represents a shift towards a more punitive providing treatment that maximizes the
strategy. Increased surveillance and punish- potential to achieve reductions in recidivism
ment were intended to decrease recidivism (Bonta and Andrews, 2017; Smith et  al.,
and crime more generally. Recent estimates, 2009b). Central among these principles are
however, indicate that those who become risk, need, and responsivity (RNR), which
involved with the criminal justice system are collectively referred to as the ‘RNR
may not subsequently redirect their lives Model’. Organizations throughout the world
to a more prosocial path. More than half of have adopted this model as the framework
the inmates who are released are returned for providing services to offenders, and it is
to prison within three years (Durose et  al., viewed as the leading strategy in rehabilita-
2014). Perhaps even more alarming, a recent tion (Cullen and Jonson, 2012; Newsome and
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND CORRECTIONS AND REHABILITATION 279

Cullen, 2017; Ogloff and Davis, 2004; In addition to determining who and what to
Polaschek, 2012). target in offender rehabilitation, the principles
To align practice with the risk principle, of effective intervention also provide guidance
corrections agencies are advised to assess each on how to approach treatment. Specifically, the
individual’s likelihood of committing another responsivity principle instructs practitioners
crime and focus the most intensive interven- to adopt treatment strategies known to be the
tion efforts towards those deemed to be at the most effective overall (i.e., general responsiv-
highest risk of recidivating (Andrews, Bonta ity), and to address individual differences that
et al., 1990; Andrews, Zinger et al., 1990). To could impact the effectiveness of the treatment
aid practitioners in adhering to this principle, strategies (i.e., specific responsivity) (Bonta
several actuarial risk-assessment tools have and Andrews, 2017). With regard to offender
been developed that capture the presence populations, research has suggested that strat-
of static and dynamic risk factors, quantify egies based on cognitive behavioral therapy
them, and generate an overall risk score and tend to be the most effective (Andrews,
classification (e.g., low risk, moderate risk, Zinger et al., 1990; Lipsey and Cullen, 2007;
and high risk). Static factors are stable over Lipsey et al., 2007; Lipsey and Wilson, 1998;
time, such as criminal history, and should Smith et al., 2009b). Specific responsivity fac-
not be identified as targets for treatment. tors, on the other hand, vary widely and may
Dynamic factors that heighten the risk for include things like low motivation to change,
recidivism can be changed through effective language barriers, or a history of trauma. To
interventions and provide insights practition- fully adhere to the responsivity principle,
ers can use to develop case plans. Scholars treatment providers must recognize these
and practitioners often collaborate to exam- factors and develop solutions to minimize any
ine the reliability and predictive validity of barriers to success in treatment.
such tools and make refinements based on The remaining principles of effective inter-
emerging research; however, evidence from vention build upon the risk, need, and respon-
numerous studies has suggested that modern sivity principles to form a comprehensive
tools can improve predictions of recidivism framework for establishing offender rehabili-
and case-management strategies while indi- tation programs (Bonta and Andrews, 2017).
viduals are involved with the justice system Overall, these principles have been empiri-
(Andrews et al., 2004; Gendreau et al., 2002; cally supported through several systematic
Smith et al., 2009a). reviews and meta-analyses (Andrews, Zinger,
Many of the dynamic risk factors identi- et al., 1990; Gendreau, 1996; Gendreau et al.,
fied through the risk-assessment process are 1996, 2006; Lipsey and Cullen, 2007; Lipsey
conceptualized in the need principle as ‘crim- et al., 2007; Lipsey and Wilson, 1998; Smith
inogenic needs’ that should be the targets of et  al., 2009a, 2009b). Moreover, the wide-
interventions (Bonta and Andrews, 2017). spread adoption and robust body of evidence
Criminogenic needs include antisocial atti- in support of the model has been attributed to
tudes, antisocial associates, antisocial tem- the strong theoretical basis, the psychology
perament/personality, family circumstances, of criminal conduct, upon which the model
employment, substance use, and leisure was created (Bonta and Andrews, 2017).
activities that may promote offending behav-
iors (Andrews, 2006). Focusing rehabilitative
efforts on these factors is the most strategic Using Theory to Inform
method for reducing the likelihood of recidi- Correctional Policy and Practice
vism. Translating this into practice should
include regular reassessments to monitor pro- Developing policy and practice with careful
gress in treatment. consideration for theories that emerge as
280 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

well-supported explanations of offending establish prosocial connections, and can bring


behavior may optimize the impact of interven- about a number of benefits that one would not
tions (Bonta and Andrews, 2017; Mears and want to lose. Sampson and Laub (1993) fur-
Cochran, 2015; Taxman and Friedmann, ther speculated that it is not likely that sim-
2009). The psychology of criminal conduct ply obtaining a job will divert someone away
has emphasized the importance of using from a life of crime; the job should be of some
theory to better understand individual differ- value to the individual (i.e., add favorable con-
ences, particularly those that could readily be sequences). While this aspect of the theory
used in practical application (Bonta and does not necessarily point to a new risk factor
Andrews, 2017). This has largely included or criminogenic need, it has the potential to
explanations of antisocial behavior that deepen the understanding of the relationship
emerged in psychology or criminology, with a between these factors and offending. It could
particular emphasis on a general psychology also provide some context to aid in under-
and cognitive social learning approach. standing why some justice-involved individu-
Briefly, this perspective views different behav- als do not desist from crime, even when they
iors as being learned, maintained, or changed are able to reduce their risk of reoffending by
through cues that signal a consequence that obtaining a job.
may follow an action and the consequences The above example demonstrates that estab-
themselves. This logic can then be extended to lishing strong connections between theory,
theorize that manipulating the cues and conse- research, and practice can be informative for the
quences associated with a behavior can be a corrections field. Some scholars have argued
useful strategy in eliminating behaviors that that emerging perspectives that appear to be sup-
are problematic and promoting the continua- ported by empirical evidence could be exam-
tion of those that are more desirable (Bonta ined as a potential avenue for further enhancing
and Andrews, 2017). public policy and corrections (Barnes, 2014;
A number of theories of criminal or antiso- Beaver and Schwartz, 2016; Moffitt, 2013;
cial behaviors have identified links between Newsome and Cullen, 2017; Rocque and
individual characteristics and lifestyle factors Welsh, 2015). The growth in research in evolu-
and offending, which could lend further insights tionary psychology has revealed new findings
and support to the RNR model. Sampson and that could serve to expand the understanding of
Laub’s (1993) age-graded theory represents offenders and rehabilitation. A brief overview
one criminological theory that complements of key concepts from evolutionary psychol-
the RNR model. Their theoretical proposition ogy and theories from this area of research is
argues that variation of social bonds over time provided in the following section, and key
influences variation in criminal behavior findings that are relevant for policy and prac-
throughout the lifecourse. As such, the weak- tice in corrections are discussed below.
ening of social bonds is expected to increase
involvement in criminal behavior, while the
strengthening of social bonds is speculated to
encourage individuals to desist from criminal EVOLUTIONARY EXPLANATIONS
behavior. For instance, as one reaches late OF BEHAVIOR
adolescence or adult stages of development,
obtaining meaningful employment – one of Over the past 20 years, various scholars have
the central criminogenic needs identified developed theoretical explanations of behav-
in the RNR model – can be a critical turn- ior through reliance on evolutionary princi-
ing point that encourages the adoption of a ples. These theories may be informative for
prosocial lifestyle. A job consumes free time, corrections by enhancing our understanding
provides an individual with opportunities to of the origins of antisocial behaviors and
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND CORRECTIONS AND REHABILITATION 281

understanding differences in how individuals Biological sensitivity to context theory,


respond to interventions designed to reduce initially proposed by Boyce and Ellis (2005),
offending. These theories are founded upon maintains that individuals vary in their sensitiv-
the idea that all living things, consciously or ity to environmental stressors. When individu-
subconsciously, strive to ensure their genes are als perceive that events or circumstances are
passed onto subsequent generations (Belsky, threatening in some way, their stress-response
1997a, 2005; Belsky and Pluess, 2009a, systems are activated generating increases in
2009b). In line with this goal is the under- heart rate and breathing, the release of various
standing that fitness (i.e., success in mating) neurotransmitters in the brain, and other phys-
and survival are fundamental to natural selec- iological processes. While these responses are
tion. As discussed below, scholars generally normal, there are differences in the situations
theorize that behaviors in modern society – that prompt the activation of this system and
including those that are widely considered to the intensity of the responses. The variation
be antisocial – could have increased fitness in reactivity is attributed to both genetic and
and survival over the evolutionary history of environmental influences, as some may be
humans. These foundational components have born with predispositions to be more or less
guided the development of evolutionary theo- reactive and early experiences can shape the
ries to explain a number of human behaviors. precision of responsiveness (Boyce and Ellis,
Although various perspectives exist, this chap- 2005). For example, highly reactive individu-
ter will focus on biological sensitivity to con- als may be more skilled at anticipating dan-
text theory, differential susceptibility theory, gers when repeatedly exposed to adversity
and intrasexual competition because these during their upbringing. This heightened
may be the most informative for working with sensibility may promote survival by allow-
correctional populations. ing them to avoid life-threatening situations.
Alternatively, a highly reactive individual who
is reared in a stable, predictable, and highly
Biological Sensitivity to Context supportive environment may thrive because he
and Differential Susceptibility or she would gain more of the benefits of the
advantageous environment. In contrast to the
Theories and Behavior
highly reactive individuals, others are theo-
Biological sensitivity to context theory and rized to be born with a lower level of reactivity
differential susceptibility theory are compati- (Boyce and Ellis, 2005). They are less likely to
ble perspectives that offer explanations for be harmed by adversity and are seemingly
differences in how individuals respond to the resilient to negative experiences, but also less
conditions they encounter during the course of likely to fully acquire the benefits of highly
their lives (Ellis et  al., 2011). These theories supportive environments.
may be particularly informative for correc- Belsky’s (1997, 2005) differential suscep-
tions, as they could provide insights into how tibility theory rests upon many of the same
antisocial behaviors develop and why justice- assumptions as biological sensitivity to con-
involved individuals respond differently to text theory but extends the logic to explain
interventions (Bakermans-Kranenburg and behavioral differences in children reared by
van IJzendoorn, 2015). Although prior the same parents. Briefly, the theory pro-
research has suggested that adhering to the poses that the traits that are advantageous in
RNR model is one of the most effective one generation will be passed on to descend-
approaches to reducing recidivism, less is ants. However, there are no guarantees that the
known about why some individuals who are same traits that are beneficial at one point in
exposed to evidence-based practices desist time will continue to be beneficial at other time
from crime and others do not. points (Belsky and Pluess, 2009a, 2009b).
282 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

In light of this uncertainty, the theory proposes environmental conditions could be altered,
that some genes that are passed from one and the strategies encouraged by the parents
generation to another can be expressed in a will not continue to be optimal. Should this
range of traits to provide a natural mecha- occur, children who are more resistant to the
nism for some flexibility. This is known as parental rearing strategies may have more
‘genetic plasticity’, a phenomenon that ulti- favorable odds of survival and reproduction –
mately reduces the risk that one would fail particularly when the rearing strategies could
to survive or reproduce if traits are inherited compromise success in these endeavors.
that are no longer beneficial. As an example,
one individual may be born with a genetic
predisposition that would permit the expres- Intrasexual Competition and
sion of higher levels of aggression in exceed- Antisocial Behavior
ingly volatile environments where aggression
may promote survival (e.g., aggression used Intrasexual competition theory relies on the
in self-defense, the capacity to take resources social commonplaces that promote reproduc-
from others during times of scarcity). The tive success to explain antisocial behavior. To
same individual might demonstrate little to briefly introduce an important concept,
no aggressive behaviors if reared in a safe parental investment refers to the amount of
and secure environment where such behav- energy and time a parent provides to its off-
iors may compromise survival and the like- spring. Human females, like other mamma-
lihood of reproducing. The theory further lian species, provide substantive parental
suggests that others will have lower levels investment to every offspring they produce
of susceptibility to environmental influence (e.g., gestation and lactation). Human males,
(Belsky, 1997b, 2005). For these individu- however, are not required by biological pro-
als, behavior will be more heavily influenced cesses to invest in any one offspring and can
by genetic factors regardless of the environ- produce numerous children without investing
mental conditions. Those with inclinations substantial resources. Consistent with the
towards aggression, for example, will likely differences in parental investment, females in
exhibit aggressive behaviors regardless of most mammalian species control access to
whether or not it is advantageous. reproduction (i.e., they dictate which males
Differences in susceptibility are hypoth- can reproduce). As a result, males must com-
esized to be an evolutionary strategy that pete for reproductive rights and engage in
maximizes the odds that at least one offspring competition for the ability to produce off-
of a mating pair is raised to reproductive age spring (Buss, 1988). Importantly, females
(Belsky, 1997a, 2005; Belsky and Pluess, must compete – at a much lower rate than
2009a, 2009b). This is because parents are males – for males that provide the best
likely to adopt rearing strategies that will opportunity for their offspring. As hypothe-
encourage the adoption of traits and behav- sized by Buss and colleagues (1988, 2006),
iors in the offspring that were beneficial for the competition for access to reproductive
them given the environmental conditions to resources has led to the selection of prosocial
which they were exposed. This could be a behaviors indicative of high paternal invest-
favorable strategy if the environmental con- ment (i.e., the best opportunity for the off-
ditions remained consistent across time. In spring). Prosocial behaviors, such as altruism
those instances, children who are more easily and selflessness, have been suggested to
influenced by their environments would be promote reproductive success among males
likely to adopt the strategies of the parents, (Phillips et al., 2008). These prosocial behav-
promoting continued success in surviving and iors, however, are counteracted by some
reproducing. However, it is also possible that antisocial behaviors that can promote
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND CORRECTIONS AND REHABILITATION 283

reproductive success with limited paternal and access resources generally reserved for
investment. tribal leaders. These same traits would then
Buss’s (1988) initial research suggested that be passed on to their offspring and increase
effective strategies for males to promote repro- the reproductive success and survival of the
ductive success generally involved acquir- offspring, independent of the parental fig-
ing or demonstrating access to resources. ures. These traits, as Buss (2006) suggests,
It is believed that females are more likely to were extremely beneficial and were com-
provide access to reproduction when males monly selected to improve the reproductive
can demonstrate paternal investment through success and survivability of the offspring.
actions or physical resources (e.g., altruism, Violence towards females appeared to
access to food, providing safety to offspring). be a reproductive strategy, selected for by
Access to physical resources (e.g., food, intrasexual competition, that allowed males
water, and property), especially during to take and maintain access to reproduc-
ancestral times, played an important role in tive resources while providing limited
maternal survival and the survival of any paternal investment (Buss, 2006). In the
offspring. Upon observing this phenom- current context, violence towards females
enon and additional evidence, Buss (1988) refers to extremely violent behaviors used
concluded that some antisocial behaviors to increase reproduction success and con-
are intended to promote reproductive suc- trol reproductive resources, while limit-
cess by immediately increasing access to ing paternal investment. Specifically, while
physical resources. Specifically, resource- scholars consistently agree that violence
based crimes, such as robbery and burglary, towards females is immoral in contempo-
increase access to food, water, and property, rary society, during ancestral times violent
and promote male attractiveness to female behaviors towards females provided indi-
suitors. For example, individuals during viduals with immediate access to reproduc-
ancestral times could steal food from oth- tion and potential reductions in cuckoldry,
ers to demonstrate their ability to feed the while requiring limited paternal investment
mother and offspring without the risks asso- (Shackelford et al., 2005). For instance, acts
ciated with hunting (e.g., death). In contem- of violence towards females, such as sexual
porary society, robbery and burglary can aggression and domestic violence, may have
provide individuals with immediate access increased access to reproduction and main-
to financial resources to potentially demon- tained reproductive control, without provid-
strate the ability to financially provide for ing resources, such as food, water, or safety.
the mother and offspring. Additionally, domestic violence may have
In addition to demonstrating paternal reduced the likelihood of cuckoldry (i.e.,
investment through resource-based crimes, providing resources to another male’s child)
Buss (1988) suggested that more aggression- and increased the likelihood of maintaining
based behaviors, selected for by intersexual reproductive success and control (Camilleri
competition, are intended to increase access and Quinsey, 2012). Violence towards
to reproductive resources by providing safety females likely arose during ancestral times,
and food for the mother. Furthermore, the where individuals would engage in sexual
offspring produced by aggressive males are aggression against females to produce large
more likely to survive due to their ability to numbers of offspring without investing any
effectively engage in combat and hunt. For resources in the children. Historical figures,
example, during ancestral times males that such as Genghis Khan, serve as case studies
had increased aggressive tendencies could for the evolutionary purpose of hyper-vio-
protect the females and offspring from exter- lence against females. In the case of Genghis
nal tribes, hunt with a high degree of success, Khan, it is documented that he would often
284 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

sexually assault females during conquests risk taking, offered a potential explanation of
and leave to continue his military campaigns. the age crime curve. Briefly, the age crime
Using hyper-sexual violence, Genghis Khan curve is a common criminological phenom-
was able to reproduce large numbers of enon suggesting that most individuals engage
descendants with limited paternal invest- in criminal behavior during adolescence
ment. Genghis Khan’s hyper-sexual vio- and early adulthood. Wilson and Daly’s
lence generated approximately 16 million (1985) theoretical assertions suggested that
descendants as of 2003 (Weatherford, 2005). the reproductive benefits of risk taking and
Although the majority of the research violence during adolescence – the peak age
focuses on intrasexual competition among for intrasexual competition – outweighs the
males, females also engage in intrasexual reproductive costs and increases the likeli-
competition. Distinct from males, intra- hood of criminal behavior among adoles-
sexual competition among females concen- cents. Specifically, using data from Detroit,
trates on increasing perceived attractiveness Wilson and Daly (1985) demonstrated that
(i.e., males select for physical attractive- the vast majority of homicide offenders
ness; Daly and Wilson, 1988). Often, females and homicide victims range in age between
increase perceived attractiveness by limiting 14 and 30 years of age, suggesting that intra-
skin blemishes, increasing body curvatures, sexual competition might account for this
and increasing apparent vulnerability (Rucas trend. As individuals age, however, Wilson
et  al., 2006). Consistent with this, females and Daly (1985) argued that the reproduc-
engage in positive behaviors (e.g., purchas- tive costs of risk taking and violent behav-
ing of accentuating clothes) or negative petty iors become too large and encourage males
behaviors (e.g., stealing of accentuating to desist from these behaviors. By the age of
clothes) to increase perceived attractiveness. 30 years, individuals who engage in violent
In addition to females increasing perceived behaviors risk injury, removal of resources,
attractiveness of themselves, female intrasex- and death, all of which are perceived as too
ual competition is often characterized as psy- costly given the limited reproductive ben-
chological struggle to damage the perceived efits at that age. Furthermore, in addition to
attractiveness of female rivals (Fink et  al., exposing oneself to increased risks, any ini-
2014). Specifically, while females engage tial offspring are exposed to heightened risks
in less physically aggressive and damaging when males engage in intrasexual competi-
behaviors, strategies such as intimidation and tion beyond early adulthood. This theoreti-
defamation may have improved reproductive cal proposition was again demonstrated by
success. Intimidation and defamation gener- the data, which illustrated stark reductions
ally result in limited physical harm but can in homicide offenders and homicide vic-
result in serious psychological damage to tims as the age of an individual becomes
females. Intrasexual violence among females greater than 30 years. Like other theoreti-
is rare but occurs when males provide emo- cal postulations founded within intrasexual
tional or physical resources to one female competition, Wilson and Daly (1985) argue
that is perceived as committed to another that competition is an important component
female (e.g., the primary form of female of the reproductive process for most human
jealousy; Campbell et  al., 1998; Daly and beings. While both differential susceptibility
Wilson, 1988). and intrasexual competition hypotheses offer
Scholars have employed the postulations broad evolutionary explanations of antiso-
of intrasexual competition to explain criminal cial behavior, these theoretical perspectives
behavior in contemporary society. For exam- can be applied to provide a greater under-
ple, Wilson and Daly (1985) proposed that standing of behavior among correctional
intrasexual competition, more specifically populations.
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND CORRECTIONS AND REHABILITATION 285

FUTURE DIRECTIONS: THE of twins, Newsome and Sullivan (2014)


INTEGRATION OF EVOLUTIONARY ­investigated the extent to which genetic and
PSYCHOLOGY INTO CORRECTIONS environmental influences could account for
variation in how adolescents respond to risk
Evolutionary psychology can offer various factors for delinquency. In line with differential
advancements to help understand or enhance susceptibility theory, the results of this study
offenders’ experiences during involvement provided evidence to suggest that individuals
with the correctional system. The following respond differently to the accumulation of
discussions will focus on the research in evo- risk factors, with some exhibiting resilience
lutionary psychology pertaining to offending (i.e., avoiding delinquency despite being
and the benefits evolutionary psychology can exposed to risks) and others exhibiting vulner-
offer to understanding offenders under com- ability (i.e., engaging in delinquency without
munity supervision (i.e., probation or parole) experiencing common risk factors). Differences
and incarcerated in secure facilities (i.e., jails in youths’ outcomes were attributed to both
and prisons). To provide a brief reiteration, genetic and unique environmental factors. In a
community supervision and incarceration are later study, Newsome and colleagues (2016)
used by correctional agencies to supervise or found that additive genetic influences
punish individuals who commit a wide vari- explained a significant proportion of variance
ety of offenses (both non-violent and vio- in males’ responses to risk for violent (41%)
lent). Both community supervision and and non-violent (29%) forms of delinquency.
incarceration possess hierarchical structures, Among females, however, environmental fac-
in which more serious offenses result in the tors fully accounted for differences in the
additional loss of freedoms. Community response to risk for all forms of delinquency.
supervision commonly involves offenders Others have used molecular genetic data
reporting to a probation or parole officer at to test hypotheses about behaviors linked to
predetermined times and engaging in a vari- criminality based on differential suscepti-
ety of other activities (e.g., alcohol treatment bility theory. Simon and colleagues (2011)
or drug testing) to ensure that the offenders examined the interaction between a cumula-
do not violate the conditions of their proba- tive measure of plasticity alleles and environ-
tion. Incarceration generally involves indi- mental conditions on aggression. The study
viduals living in a secured facility for six considered favorable aspects of the environ-
months or more. Incarceration conditions can ment (e.g., supportive parenting, neighbor-
vary drastically depending upon the crimes hood informal social control) and adverse
committed and the activities of the individual social factors (e.g., harsh parenting, dis-
during incarceration. Below is a review of the crimination, violent peers). The results indi-
research and the application of evolutionary cated that cumulative genetic plasticity and
psychology to corrections and rehabilitation. the qualities of the social environment pro-
duced a significant interaction that predicted
aggression, chronic anger, a hostile view
Differential Susceptibility and of relationships, and a belief in toughness.
Correctional Research A later study by Simon and his colleagues
(2012) also found cumulative genetic plastic-
Several empirical assessments of Belsky’s ity and exposure to a hostile or demoralizing
differential susceptibility hypothesis have environment was a significant predictor of
been published that offer explanations for the aggression and adopting a ‘street code’ that
emergence of maladaptive behaviors among supports criminal behavior.
individuals (van IJzendoorn and Bakermans- Findings pertaining to criminal behaviors
Kranenburg, 2015). For example, in a sample and related traits and behaviors based on
286 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

differential susceptibility theory reveal that and was found to increase parental sensitivity
these outcomes are the results of an interac- when administering discipline (Bakermans-
tion of genetic and environmental influences. Kranenburg, van IJzendoorn, Pijlam et  al.,
Based on these findings, it is further possible 2008). The intervention also reduced exter-
that there will be variation in outcomes when nalizing behaviors among children, but only
the environmental influences under investi- those who were carriers of the DRD4 7-repeat
gation are interventions designed to reduce allele. Another intervention tested in the Fast
deviant, delinquent, and criminal behaviors. Track Randomized Controlled Trial also gen-
Indeed, researchers have begun investigating erated evidence in support of the differen-
variation in treatment effects for prevention tial susceptibility perspective (Albert et  al.,
and intervention programs from a differen- 2015). Participants who had the rs10482672
tial susceptibility framework and findings A allele of the NR3C1 gene exhibited exter-
have produced a substantial amount of evi- nalizing behaviors less often at age 25 years
dence in support of the theory (Bakermans- after participating in Fast Track (18%) rela-
Kranenburg and van IJzendoorn, 2015; Belsky tive to control subjects (75%). The percent-
and van IJzendoorn, 2015; van IJzendoorn age of treatment and control participants who
and Bakermans-Kranenburg, 2015). did not possess the A allele and exhibited
This growing body of evidence is robust, externalizing behaviors were nearly identical
as many of the studies employed a strong (56% and 57%, respectively).
research design through the use of a rand- Brody et al. (2015) also tested the Adults
omized controlled trial. As an example, the in the Making program, which was designed
Strong African American Families (SAAF) to promote positive development and reduced
program was designed to reduce alcohol drug use. The results of this study revealed
use among African American teens, and was that individuals who were assigned to the
tested among a sample of 337 youths using intervention and were carriers of at least one
a randomized design (Brody et  al., 2006). long allele of DRD4 engaged in less drug
Results from this study suggested that the use compared to those who were placed in
DRD4 gene moderated the effects of the pro- the control group. Individuals with the same
gram on substance use among the youths. allele, however, engaged in more drug use if
Adolescents with a DRD4 7-repeat allele, they were in the control group and lived in
one hypothesized to increase plasticity, had a risky family environment. In other words,
lower alcohol use than teens with two DRD those with the genotype associated with
4-repeat alleles (Beach et al., 2010). A simi- greater plasticity seemed to experience more
lar pattern of findings emerged in a test of the benefits if they participated in the program
Promoting School-Community-University or more problematic behavior if they were
Partnerships to Enhance Resilience program. exposed to a high-risk context.
In this study, participating in the program Findings from these and other studies have
reduced alcohol use for individuals who pos- led some scholars to suggest that differential
sessed the DRD4 7-repeat allele if they indi- susceptibility theory can be informative for
cated that involvement with their primary intervention science (Bakermans-Kranenburg
caregiver was moderate or high (Cleveland and van IJzendoorn, 2015; Belsky and van
et al., 2015). IJzendoorn, 2015). This body of literature
Other studies have focused on interventions offers some explanation for why some indi-
designed to reduce externalizing behaviors, a viduals may respond better to treatment efforts
known correlate of delinquency (White et al., than others, and has the potential to enhance
1990). The Video-Feedback Intervention to the responsivity principle in the RNR model
Promote Positive Parenting and Sensitive – particularly specific responsivity. It is pos-
Discipline was tested with a randomized trial sible that, due to genetic factors, tremendous
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND CORRECTIONS AND REHABILITATION 287

gains from some interventions should not A well-developed component of the RNR
necessarily be expected. Considering this model is the responsivity principal, which
possibility more broadly, it has further been suggests that individual- and system-level
suggested that estimates of treatment effects differences influence the observed effec-
may be biased (Bakermans-Kranenburg and tiveness of treatment programs. At the indi-
van IJzendoorn, 2015). Some interventions vidual level, it is possible that differences in
appear to be stronger among those with par- genetic predispositions influence the causal
ticular genotypes; however, studies often fail association between treatment programs and
to account for this phenomenon. As a result, behavioral outcomes. To address this poten-
studies may be underestimating the treatment tial influence, probation and parole officers
effects that are possible when individuals are can develop individualized treatment plans,
better matched to interventions. To the extent which take into account psychological and
that this is occurring, efforts to take stock of behavioral assessments, to address the crimi-
the empirical status of the RNR model may nogenic needs of the individual.
be underestimating the actual efficacy of the Furthermore, genetic predispositions could
model (or its components) for individuals make individuals more or less vulnerable to
who are most likely to benefit from services the effects of confinement on psychological
offered under the model. and behavioral outcomes. In contemporary
Integrating the differential susceptibil- society, the conditions of confinement in
ity hypothesis into community supervision jails and prisons are often characterized by
efforts could suggest fruitful endeavors for the removal of basic freedoms (e.g., speak-
practitioners. Considering that the deterrent ing and movement), social isolation, and lim-
and beneficial effects of community super- ited access to additional human resources.
vision differ between individuals (Andrews Generally, evidence suggests that these con-
and Bonta, 2010), it can be speculated that ditions can lead to increases in antisocial
genetic predispositions could generate dif- outcomes (e.g., recidivism) and the severity
ferent observed outcomes for those on com- of psychological afflictions (Cullen et  al.,
munity supervision. Consistent with the 2011). These findings, however, often coin-
RNR model, the differential susceptibility cide with evidence suggesting that the effects
hypothesis would advocate for a customiz- of prison on behavior differ by individual.
able process for each individual under com- As such, it is not unreasonable to speculate
munity supervision. These individualized that individuals with genetic predispositions
case management plans should be guided for mental-health afflictions and antisocial
by validated psychological and behavioral behavior might be more or less affected by
assessments that identify the risks, needs, the conditions of confinement.
and responsivity issues. Furthermore, case
plans should be flexible to account for the
differential responses (i.e., specific respon- Integrating Intrasexual
sivity) some individuals will have to meet the Competition into Corrections
requirements of the case management plans.
This customizable process could benefit both Unlike differential susceptibility theory,
the practitioners and the offenders, as it will scholars have rarely considered the effects of
allow for the creation of more flexible and intrasexual competition on offender rehabili-
empirically supported case plans. tation and the RNR model. Considering the
In addition to individualized case man- limited postulations, it is believed that inte-
agement plans, the differential susceptibil- grating intrasexual competition into correc-
ity hypothesis could be used to advocate tions could potentially provide a superior
for more individualized treatment plans. understanding of the observed heterogeneity
288 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

in treatment effects (e.g., Lipsey and Cullen, system on resource prospects, intrasexual
2007). Specifically, intrasexual competition competition might provide a theoretical
theory can further emphasize the importance explanation for the observed heterogeneity in
of access to resources during offender reen- offender reentry. If females are more attracted
try, in addition to exploring the role of age to males with greater prospects, individuals
and sexual competition during incarceration previously involved in the correctional sys-
and reentry. tem might have reduced reproductive success
Briefly, as discussed above, in contempo- and in turn seek out antisocial strategies to
rary society females tend to subconsciously increase access to reproductive resources.
perceive males with higher financial pros- For example, illegal behaviors that provide
pects as more suitable mates (Wiederman and a means of financial stability, such as drug
Allgeier, 1992). This perceived suitability dealing, could potentially increase access
likely arises from the benefits these males pro- to reproductive resources. Alternatively,
vided, regarding paternal investment, during individuals previously involved in the cor-
evolutionary times. For instance, males with rectional system might engage in behaviors,
increased access to resources, such as food, such as robbery or burglary, to increase the
were more likely to provide these resources perception of financial stability by acquiring
to their partners and offspring. On the con- jewelry or other valuable objects, which in
trary, during evolutionary times individuals turn could increase reproductive success.
with limited access to resources were less As such, male offenders reentering society
likely to provide for their partners and off- might recidivate due to the inability to access
spring. Considering the evolutionary benefit financial resources through legal channels
of resources, increased survivability, females and engage in illegal activities to counter-
in contemporary society consider males with act the reduced financial prospects caused
greater resource prospects as more attractive by the criminal justice system. Furthermore,
than males with fewer resource prospects. As these reentry difficulties could influence the
such, individuals with regular employment, heterogeneity of correctional programming
disposable income, and housing likely have by encouraging individuals with reduced
increased reproductive success. Individuals resources to seek antisocial strategies for
without regular employment, disposable obtaining access to reproductive resources.
income, and housing likely have decreased Interestingly, access to financial prospects
reproductive success. These individuals, is an important subcomponent of the RNR
however, might employ alternative strategies model. Employment is one indicator of an
to access reproductive resources. These strat- individual’s risk of recidivating and has been
egies could be perceived as antisocial behav- identified as a criminogenic need area to tar-
iors by broader society. get in treatment (Bonta and Andrews, 2017).
As demonstrated by previous studies As such, intrasexual competition could
(Richie, 2001; Travis, 2005; Visher and potentially provide insights as to why such
Travis, 2003), involvement in the correc- factors are of critical importance to individu-
tional system drastically hinders access to als involved in the criminal justice system.
resources. For instance, individuals convicted Additionally, considering that intrasexual
of felony offenses have a decreased likelihood competition is heightened between the ages
of legally maintaining constant employment, of 15 and 30 (Wilson and Daly, 1985), pro-
purchasing a house, and sustaining wealth. bation and parole departments can service
Furthermore, felony convictions drastically these individuals differently from older cli-
hinder an individual’s legal financial pros- entele. For instance, younger individuals
pects (Raphael, 2014). Although speculative, can be provided supplemental opportunities
considering the effects of the correctional such as additional evidence-based treatment
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND CORRECTIONS AND REHABILITATION 289

opportunities to address negative behavioral benefits for understanding the ­heterogeneity


tendencies used to promote access to repro- in ­ correctional programs. Notably, while
ductive resources. These treatment opportuni- these postulations align with contemporary
ties could provide individuals on supervision theories of intrasexual competition, scholars
with strategies that reduce risk-taking behav- have yet to provide a comprehensive perspec-
iors and help them to recognize that compe- tive or empirical examination of intrasexual
tition over resources could result in negative competition in correctional environments. It
outcomes such as imprisonment or death. All is theorized that sex ratio, at the macro level,
of these strategies guided by evolutionary prin- can influence the efficacy of correctional pro-
ciples require minor alterations to the current gramming by influencing risk through the
services provided by community supervision development of environmental factors con-
agencies and should improve the benefits for ducive to heightened intrasexual competition.
the individuals under community supervision. For example, the prison environment drasti-
Intrasexual competition theory would cally reduces access to reproductive resources.
also argue that limited access to financial As such, incarcerated populations might have
prospects during incarceration might also heightened levels of violence and sexual vio-
contribute to inmate misconduct. For exam- lence resulting from the extreme reductions
ple, financial prospects, such as employ- in access to reproductive resources. Violent
ment and disposable income, are extremely behaviors, such as assault and homicide,
difficult to access during imprisonment. might occur as a result of limited reproductive
Nevertheless, while access to reproductive resources within the facility and an increased
resources is extremely limited during impris- number of males at peak competition age
onment, competition persists because of the (Wilson and Daly, 1985). Furthermore, due
high number of inmates at peak reproduc- to limited reproductive resources, sexual vio-
tive ages (15–30; Wilson and Daly, 1985). lence against other inmates might arise as a
Furthermore, intrasexual competition per- reproductive adaptation.
sists as inmates attempt to attract the limited Although the link between macro-level
number of females that work at or visit the intrasexual competition and recidivism has
facility. This competition and competition yet to be examined, various scholars have
with males who have obtained partners out- explored the association between sex ratio
side of the facility likely also contributes to and crime rates. The research on the associa-
the strain felt by inmates in the facility. The tion between sex ratio and crime rates could
inability to increase financial prospects during potentially provide insight into the potential
incarceration might further heighten competi- effects of sex ratio on the reentry process. For
tion among inmates within these age groups. example, Campbell and colleagues (1998)
Competition may also be further amplified published a study assessing the association
due to the limited availability of prosocial between intrasexual competition and female-
employment after incarceration – the options on-female assault rates. The data for the study
for convicted felons are exceedingly limited. came from the 1998 National Incident-Based
Working to expand employment opportunities Reporting System (NIBRS) for the state of
for inmates through work release programs Massachusetts. At the time, 34 districts in
and other reentry planning pre-release from Massachusetts reported crime incidents to
prison may ease competition among inmates NIBRS. Importantly, findings from the pre-
and lead to reductions in misconduct. liminary analysis suggested that the rate of
In addition to the individual-level same-sex assault was higher for males and
enhancements offered by intrasexual com- females under the age of 24 than males and
petition theory, a macro-level integration females over the age of 24. These findings
of intrasexual competition could provide suggested that intrasexual competition peaks
290 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

under the age of 24, evidence supporting First, it is possible that genetic factors should
the theoretical position of Wilson and Daly be considered as specific responsivity fac-
(1985). Additionally, males over the age of tors. Studies investigating this possibility
24 engaged in higher rates of cross-sexual among adult offenders are scare relative
violence, suggesting that older males might to those for juveniles, but as this body of lit-
use cross-sexual violence to reduce the like- erature continues to grow it could reveal
lihood of cuckolding or mate abandonment. important insights to guide correctional reha-
Further analysis, however, suggested that sex bilitation – particularly in understanding how
ratio did not predict female-on-female assault and why individuals respond differently to
rates. As such, it can be suggested that higher interventions. Second, scholars who are
cross-sexual violence might occur among for- examining the efficacy of correctional inter-
mer inmates over the age of 24. Furthermore, ventions risk generating biased results when
heightened levels of violent recidivism might genetic factors are not included in statistical
occur as an intrasexual competition strategy. models. As science and technology in this
While additional research on the association area continue to develop, new evidence and
between intrasexual competition and delin- methods should be given consideration in
quency exists, the literature is extremely designing tests of interventions.
limited regarding correctional populations Evolutionary psychology may also aid in
and criminal justice processes. As such, more understanding and managing inmate popu-
research is needed to explore how intersexual lations. As described above, research on
competition moderates the efficacy of reha- differential susceptibility theory has shown
bilitation programs. Although more research that individuals respond differently to risks
is needed, intrasexual competition, like dif- for delinquency and crime and interventions
ferential susceptibility theory, is useful for designed to target these behaviors. It is also
informing policy and practices in correc- conceivable that individuals will respond
tional settings. differently to prison environments in ways
that align with biological sensitivity to con-
text and differential susceptibility theories,
Implications of Evolutionary though this remains to be tested empirically.
To enhance research in this area, upon arrival
Psychology for Corrections
at a secure correctional facility the inmates
Although the empirical literature in correc- could be administered a broader range of
tions has predominantly drawn from psycho- assessments (Newsome and Cullen, 2017).
logical and sociological theories, there is Assessments for psychological afflictions,
emerging evidence to suggest that evolution- such as depression, anxiety, and psychoti-
ary psychology can offer another avenue for cism, and behavioral assessments for antiso-
extending scientific inquiry in corrections. cial tendencies and risk of recidivism could
Scholars testing differential susceptibility be employed by correctional departments
theory among youths have found strong evi- to gather more personalized information on
dence to suggest that children and adoles- the incarcerated individuals. There is also
cents respond differently to interventions, evidence to suggest that the biological pro-
and that the efficacy of treatment may be cesses that are triggered by stress, such as
underestimated when researchers fail to con- changes in cortisol levels, can also be altered
sider the role of genetic and environmental by participating in cognitive behavioral ther-
factors (Bakermans-Kranenburg and van apy (Bakermans-Kranenburg et al., 2008;
IJzendoorn, 2015; Belsky and van IJzendoorn, Cornet et al., 2014; van Goozen et al., 2007 ).
2015). These findings present two important A more comprehensive assessment process
implications for correctional rehabilitation. that includes these additional factors would
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND CORRECTIONS AND REHABILITATION 291

allow practitioners to identify the individu- correctional practices. These discussions


alized risks and needs of the inmates while focused on how contemporary policies and
also creating opportunities to identify those practices could be reexamined with consid-
who exhibited a heightened susceptibility eration for principles derived from evolution-
to environmental stressors and examine the ary psychology. Nevertheless, scholars have
impact of imprisonment and treatment on yet to integrate the principles of evolutionary
individuals. psychology to develop meaningful policy
The management of offender populations initiatives in corrections. As such, the core
could also be enhanced by further research ideas of the evolutionary psychological per-
and consideration for the intrasexual compe- spective were summarized and three middle-
tition perspective of behavior. For example, level theories were discussed with an
strategies that increase prosocial means of emphasis on how they might inform correc-
obtaining financial resources could poten- tions (Buss, 1995). These three theories
tially be beneficial. Specifically, one reentry were: biological sensitivity to context theory,
strategy that could reduce recidivism is pro- differential susceptibility theory, and intra-
viding former felons with prosocial means sexual competition. The policy discussions
of obtaining and maintaining financial pros- focused on how evolutionary psychology can
pects. Explicitly, these prosocial means can provide meaningful policy recommendations
include but are not limited to employment for community and institutional corrections.
opportunities, housing opportunities, and At the heart of these policy discussions, cor-
disposable income through legal channels. rectional departments should treat offenders
Access to these resources through proso- as individuals and train officers to better
cial channels could reduce the reliance on understand the individualistic nature of
resource-based crimes and violent behaviors human behavior. In conclusion, researchers
to promote access to reproductive resources. and practitioners should examine the feasi-
Furthermore, promoting financial prospects bility and efficacy of evolutionary psychol-
during reentry could enhance the efficacy of ogy policy recommendations to ensure that
the correctional programming. the desired results of the criminal justice
Correctional departments can make con- system are maintained.
certed efforts to reduce competition among
inmates. These efforts can focus on increas-
ing access to non-harm-inducing resources,
reduce competitive opportunities, and reduce REFERENCES
risk-taking opportunities. Additionally, cor-
rectional departments can reduce competi- Albert, W. D., Belsky, D. W., Crowley, D. M.,
tion among inmates by reducing the number Latendresse, S. J., Aliev, F., Riley, B., & Sun,
of inmates at the peak reproductive stage in C., (2015). Can genetics predict response to
the same vicinity. These efforts could reduce complex behavioral interventions? Evidence
intrasexual competition over resources dur- from a genetic analysis of the Fast Track Ran-
ing imprisonment. domized Control Trial. Journal of Policy
Analysis and Management, 34, 497–518.
Alper, M., Durose, M. R., & Markman, J.
(2018). 2018 Update on Prisoner Recidivism:
A 9-Year Follow-up Period (2005–2014).
CONCLUSION Washington, DC: US Department of Justice.
Andrews, D. A. (2006). Enhancing adherence
The current chapter introduced the state of to risk-need-responsivity: Making quality a
corrections in the United States and the RNR matter of policy. Criminology & Public Policy,
model to provide a brief overview of current 5, 595–602.
292 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Andrews, D. A., & Bonta, J. (2010). The public policy. In T. G. Blomberg, J. M. Brancale,
psychology of criminal conduct, 5th edition. K. M. Beaver, & W. D. Bales (Eds.), Advancing
New Providence, NJ: Matthew Bender & criminology & criminal justice policy
Company, Inc. (pp. 452–460). New York, NY: Routledge.
Andrews, D. A., Bonta, J., & Hoge, R. D. (1990). Belsky, J. (1997a). Variation in susceptibility to
Classification for effective rehabilitation: environmental influence: An evolutionary
Rediscovering psychology. Criminal Justice argument. Psychological Inquiry, 8, 182–186.
and Behavior, 17, 19–52. Belsky, J. (1997b). Theory testing, effect-size
Andrews, D. A., Bonta, J., & Wormith, S. J. (2004). evaluation, and differential susceptibility to
The Level of Service/Case Management rearing influence: The case of mothering and
Inventory (LS/CMI). Toronto, Ontario, attachment. Child Development, 68,
Canada: Multi-Health Systems. 598–600.
Andrews, D. A., Zinger, I., Hoge, R. D., Bonta, Belsky, J. (2005). Differential susceptibility to
J., Gendreau, P., & Cullen, F. T. (1990). Does rearing influence: An evolutionary hypothesis
correctional treatment work? A clinically and some evidence. In B. Ellis & D. Bjorklund
relevant and psychologically informed meta- (Eds.), Origins of the social mind: Evolutionary
analysis. Criminology, 28, 369–404. psychology and child development
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van (pp. 139–163). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
IJzendoorn, M. H. (2015). The hidden Belsky, J., & Pluess, M. (2009a). The nature
efficacy of interventions: Gene x environment (and nurture?) of plasticity in early human
experiments from a differential susceptibility development. Association for Psychological
perspective. Annual Review of Psychology, Sciences, 4, 345–351.
66, 381–409. Belsky, J., & Pluess, M. (2009b). Beyond diath-
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., van IJzendoorn, esis stress: Differential susceptibility to envi-
M. H., Mesman, J., Alink, L. R. A., & Juffer, F. ronmental influences. Psychological Bulletin,
(2008). Effects of an attachment-based 135, 885–908.
intervention on daily cortisol moderated by Belsky, J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2015). What
dopamine receptor D4: A randomized con- works and for whom? Genetic moderation of
trol trial on 1- to 3-year-olds screened for intervention efficacy. Development and
externalizing behavior. Development and Psychopathology, 27, 1–6.
Psychopathology, 20, 805–820. Bonta, J. & Andrews, D. A. (2017). The psychol-
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., van IJzendoorn, ogy of criminal conduct, 6th edition. New
M. H., Pijlman, F. T. A., Mesman, J., & Juffer, F. York, NY: Routledge.
(2008). Experimental evidence for differential Boyce, W. T., & Ellis, B. J. (2005). Biological
susceptibility Dopamine D4 receptor polymor- sensitivity to context: I. An evolutionary-
phism (DRD4 VNTR) moderates intervention developmental theory of the origins and
effects on toddlers’ externalizing behavior in a functions of stress reactivity. Development
randomized controlled trial. Developmental and Psychopathology, 17, 271–301.
Psychology, 44, 293–300. Brody, G. H., Murry, V. M., Kogan, S. M., Ger-
Barnes, J. C. (2014). The impact of biosocial rard, M., Gibbons, F. X., Molgaard, V., &
criminology on public policy: Where should Wills, T. A. (2006). The Strong African Ameri-
we go from here? In M. DeLisi & Kevin M. can Families program: A cluster randomized
Beaver (Eds.), Criminological theory: A life- prevention trial of long-term effects and a
course approach (2nd Edition, pp. 83–98). mediation model. Journal of Consulting and
Burlington, MA: Jones & Bartlett. Clinical Psychology, 74, 356–366.
Beach, S. R. H., Brody, G. H., Philibert, R. A., & Brody, G. H., Yu, T., & Beach, S. R. H. (2015). A
Lei, M. (2010). Differential susceptibility to differential susceptibility analysis reveals the
parenting among African American youths: ‘who and how’ about adolescents’ responses
Testing the DRD4 hypothesis. Journal of to preventive interventions: Tests of first- and
Family Psychology, 25, 513–521. second-generation Gene X Intervention
Beaver, K. M., & Schwartz, J. A. (2016). The hypotheses. Development and Psychopathol-
utility of findings from biosocial research for ogy, 27, 37–49.
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND CORRECTIONS AND REHABILITATION 293

Bureau of Justice Statistics (2016). Probation states in 2005: Patterns from 2005 to 2010.
and parole in the United States, 2016. Washington, DC: Bureau of Justice Statistics,
Washington, DC: National Institute of Justice. US Department of Justice.
Buss, D. M. (1988). The evolution of human Ellis, B. J., Boyce, W. T., Belsky, J., Bakermans-
intrasexual competition: Tactics of mate Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H.
attraction. Journal of Personality and Social (2011). Differential susceptibility to the envi-
Psychology, 54, 616–628. ronment: An evolutionary-­neurodevelopmental
Buss, D. M. (1995). The evolution of desire: theory. Development and Psychopathology,
Strategies of human mating. New York, 23, 7–28.
NY: Basic Books. Fink, B., Klappauf, D., Brewer, G., & Shackelford,
Buss, D. M. (2006). The murderer next door: T. K. (2014). Female physical characteristics
Why the mind is designed to kill. London, and intra-sexual competition in women.
UK: Penguin Publishers. Personality and Individual Differences, 58,
Campbell, A., Muncer, S., & Bibel, D. (1998). 138–141.
Female-female criminal assault: An evolu- Gendreau, P. (1996). The principles of effective
tionary perspective. Journal of Research in intervention with offenders. In A. T. Harland
Crime and Delinquency, 35, 413–428. (Ed.), Choosing correctional options that
Camilleri, J. A., & Quinsey, V. L. (2012). Sexual work: Defining the demand and evaluating
conflict and partner rape. In T. K. Shackelford the supply (pp. 117–130). Thousand Oaks,
& A. T. Goetz (Eds.), The Oxford handbook CA: Sage.
of sexual conflict in humans (pp. 257–268). Gendreau, P., Goggin, C., & Smith, P. (2002). Is
Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. the PCL-R really the ‘unparalleled’ measure
Caspi, A., McClay, J., Moffitt, T. E., Mill, J., of offender risk? A lesson in knowledge
Martin, J., Craig, I. W., & Poulton, R. (2002). cumulation. Criminal Justice and Behavior,
Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in 29, 397–426.
maltreated children. Science, 297, 851–854. Gendreau, P., Little, T., & Goggin, C. (1996). A
Cleveland, H. H., Schlomer, G. L., Vandenberg, meta-analysis of the predictors of adult
D. J., Feinberg, M., Greenberg, M., Spoth, R., offender recidivism: What works?
& Hair, K. L. (2015). The conditioning of Criminology, 34, 575–607.
intervention effects on early adolescent Gendreau, P., Smith, P., & French, S. A. (2006).
alcohol use by maternal involvement and The theory of effective correctional
dopamine receptor D4 (DRD4) and serotonin intervention: Empirical status and future
transporter linked polymorphic region directions. In F. T. Cullen, J. P. Wright, & K. R.
(5-HTTLPR) genetic variants. Development Blevins (Eds.), Advances in criminological
and Psychopathology, 27, 51–67. theory: Taking stock: The status of
Cornet, L. J. M., de Kogel, C. H., Nijman, H. L. criminological theory (Vol. 15, pp. 419–446).
I., Raine, A., & van der Laan, P. H. (2014). New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction.
Neurobiological changes after intervention in Kaeble, D., & Cowhig, M. (2018). Correctional
individuals with anti-social behaviour: A populations in the United States, 2016.
literature review. Criminal Behavior and Washington, DC: US Department of Justice.
Mental Health, 25, 10–27. Lawrence, S., Mears, D. P., Dubin, G., & Travis, J.
Cullen, F. T., & Jonson, C. L. (2012). Correctional (2002). The practice and promise of prison
theory: Context and consequences. Thousand programming. Washington, DC: The Urban
Oaks, CA: Sage. Institute.
Cullen, F. T., Jonson, C. L., & Nagin, D. S. Lipsey, M. W., & Cullen, F. T. (2007). The
(2011). Prisons do not reduce recidivism: The effectiveness of correctional rehabilitation: A
high cost of ignoring science. The Prison review of systematic reviews. Annual Review
Journal, 91, 48S–65S. of Law and Social Science, 3, 297–320.
Daly, M, & Wilson, M. (1988). Homicide. Lipsey, M. W., Landenberger, N. A., & Wilson, S. J.
Hawthorne, NY: Aldine deGruyter. (2007). The effects of cognitive-behavioral
Durose, M. R., Cooper, A. D., & Snyder, H. N. programs for criminal offenders. Campbell
(2014). Recidivism of prisoners released in 30 Systematic Reviews, 6, 1–27.
294 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (1998). Effective Pratt, T. C. (2019). Addicted to incarceration,
intervention for serious juvenile offenders: A 2nd edition. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
synthesis of research. In R. Loeber & D. P. Raphael, S. (2014). The effects of conviction and
Farrington (Eds.), Serious & violent juvenile incarceration on future employment out-
offenders: Risk factors and successful comes. In D. P. Farrington & J. Murray (Eds.),
interventions (pp. 313–366). Thousand Labeling theory: Empirical tests, advances in
Oaks, CA: Sage. criminological theory 18 (pp. 237–262). New
Lynch, J. P., & Sabol, W. J. (2001). Prisoner Brunswick, NJ: Transaction.
reentry in perspective. Washington, DC: Richie, B. (2001). Challenges incarcerated
The Urban Institute. women face as they return to their communi-
Mears, D. P., & Cochran, J. C. (2015). Prisoner ties: Findings from life history interviews.
reentry in the era of mass incarceration. Crime and Delinquency, 47, 368–389.
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Rocque, M., & Welsh, B. C. (2015). Offender
Moffitt, T. E. (2013). Childhood exposure to rehabilitation from a maturation/biosocial
violence and lifelong health: Clinical perspective. In M. DeLisi & M. Vaughn (Eds.),
intervention science and stress-biology Handbook of biosocial criminology (pp. 501–
research join forces. Development and 515). Abingdon, UK: Routledge.
Psychopathology, 25, 1619–1634. Rucas, S. L., Gurven, M., Kaplan, H., Winking,
Newsome, J., & Cullen, F. T. (2017). The risk- J., Gangestad, S., & Crespo, M. (2006).
need-responsivity model revisited: Using Female intrasexual competition and
biosocial criminology to enhance offender reputational effects on attractiveness among
rehabilitation. Criminal Justice and Behavior, the Tsimane of Bolivia. Evolution and Human
44, 1030–1049. Behavior, 27, 40–52.
Newsome, J., & Sullivan, C. J. (2014). Resilience Sampson, R. J., & Laub, J. H. (1993). Crime in
and vulnerability in adolescents: Genetic the making: Pathways and turning points
influences on differential response to risk for through life. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
delinquency. Journal of Youth and University Press.
Adolescence, 43, 1080–1095. Shackelford, T. K., Goetz, A. T., Buss, D. M.,
Newsome, J., Vaske, J. C., Gehring, K. S., & Euler, H. A., & Hoier, S. (2005). When we
Boisvert, D. L. (2016). Sex differences in hurt the ones we love: Predicting violence
sources of resilience and vulnerability to risk against women from men’s mate retention.
for delinquency. Journal of Youth and Personal Relationships, 12, 447–463.
Adolescence, 45, 730–745. Simons, R. L., Lei, M. K., Beach, S. R., Brody,
Ogloff, J. R., & Davis, M. R. (2004). Advances in G. H., Philibert, R. A., & Gibbons, F. X.
offender assessment and rehabilitation: (2011). Social environment, genes, and
Contributions of the risk-needs-responsivity aggression: Evidence supporting a differential
approach. Psychology, Crime & Law, 10, susceptibility perspective. American
229–242. Sociological Review, 76, 883–912.
Phelps, M. S. (2011). Rehabilitation in the Simons, R. L., Lei, M. K., Stewart, E. A., Beach,
punitive era: The gap between rhetoric and S. R., Brody, G. H., Philibert, R. A., &
reality in U.S. prison programs. Law & Society Gibbons, F. X. (2012). Social adversity,
Review, 45, 33–68. genetic variation, street code, and aggression:
Phillips, T., Barnard, C., Ferguson, E., & Reader, A genetically informed model of violent
T. (2008). Do humans prefer altruistic mates? behavior. Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice,
Testing a link between sexual selection and 10, 3–24.
altruism towards non-relatives. British Journal Smith, P., Cullen, F. T., & Latesssa, E. J. (2009a).
of Psychology, 99, 555–572. Can 14,737 women be wrong? A meta-
Polaschek, D. L. (2012). An appraisal of the analysis of the LSI-R and recidivism for
risk-need-responsivity (RNR) model of female offenders. Criminology and Public
offender rehabilitation and its application in Policy, 8, 183–208.
correctional treatment. Legal and Smith, P., Gendreau, P., & Swartz, K. (2009b).
Criminological Psychology, 17, 1–17. Validating the principles of effective
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND CORRECTIONS AND REHABILITATION 295

intervention: A systematic review of the Visher, C. A., & Travis, J. (2003). Transitions
contributions of meta-analysis in the field of from prison to community: Understanding
corrections. Victims and Offenders, 4, individual pathways. Annual Review of
148–169. Sociology, 29, 89–113.
Taxman, F. S., & Friedmann, P. D. (2009). Fidelity Weatherford, J. (2005). Genghis Khan and the
and adherence at the transition point: making of the modern world. New York, NY:
Theoretically driven experiments. Journal of Broadway Books.
Experimental Criminology, 5, 219–226. White, J. L., Moffitt, T. E., Earls, F., Robins, L., &
Travis, J. (2005). But they all come back: Facing Silva, P. A. (1990). How early can we tell?
the challenges of prisoner reentry. Predictors of childhood conduct disorder and
Washington, DC: The Urban Institute Press. adolescent delinquency. Criminology, 28,
van Goozen, S. H. M., Fairchild, G., Snoek, H., & 507–535.
Harold, G. T. (2007). The evidence for a neuro- Wiederman, M. W., & Allgeier, E. R. (1992).
biological model of childhood antisocial behav- Gender differences in mate selection criteria:
ior. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 149–182. Sociobiological or socioeconomic explanation?
van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Bakermans-Kranen- Ethology and Sociobiology, 13, 115–124.
burg, M. J. (2015). Genetic differential sus- Wilson, M., & Daly, M. (1985). Competitiveness,
ceptibility on trial: Meta-analytic support from risk taking, and violence: The young male
randomized controlled experiments. Develop- syndrome. Ethology and Sociobiology, 6,
ment and Psychopathology, 27, 151–162. 59–73.
15
Evolutionary Psychology and
Organized Crime
Russil Durrant

INTRODUCTION mitigate its deleterious effects on both specific


victims and society more generally. Despite a
In his foreword to the United Nations substantial body of research on the topic,
Convention against Transnational Organized organized crime remains a contested concept
Crime, the then Secretary-General of the within criminology with little or no consensus
United Nations, Kofi Annan, painted a vivid concerning what constitutes organized criminal
picture of what he termed the ‘gulf that exists activities (Paoli and Vander Beken, 2014), or
between the civil and the uncivil’ (United what the main causes of organized crime are
Nations Office of Drugs and Crime, 2004: 3). (Paoli, 2016). My primary aim in this chapter is
Civil society, he claimed, referred to the history to discuss how an approach that draws from
of accumulated progress that has resulted in evolutionary psychology can contribute to a
liberal societies that promote tolerance, respect, better understanding of what organized crime
and good governance. However, there also is, the motivations of organized criminal
exists what he called an ‘uncivil society’: ‘They offenders, and the structure of organized crimi-
are terrorists, criminals, drug dealers, traffick- nal groups.
ers in people and others who undo the good Although evolutionary psychologists have
works of civil society… they thrive in countries had a long-standing interest in criminal offend-
with weak institutions, and they show no scru- ing, especially violent crime (e.g., Daly and
ple about resorting to intimidation or violence’ Wilson, 1988; Duntley and Shackelford, 2008),
(United Nations Office of Drugs and Crime, and there is an emerging literature more gen-
2004: 3). In short, the forces of organized crime erally on ‘evolutionary criminology’ (e.g.,
are pitted against those mainstream institutions Durrant and Ward, 2015; Kavish et  al., 2017;
they seek to undermine. Efforts to better under- Roach and Pease, 2013; Walsh and Jorgensen,
stand the nature and scope of organized crime, 2018), the topic of organized crime has received
therefore, can contribute to strategies that can negligible attention from evolutionary-minded
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND ORGANIZED CRIME 297

scholars (although see Hirschfeld, 2015, for and indirect reciprocity, and cultural selection
an approach that draws from behavioural ecol- for ‘strong reciprocity’ involving the develop-
ogy). From the perspective of evolutionary psy- ment of group norms and the administration
chology, I argue that organized crime involves of sanctions for their violation. Taking a more
the coalitional exploitation of others for per- historical perspective, I consider how an evolu-
sonal gain in ways that violate criminal laws. tionary approach can help us to understand the
As such, the activities that comprise organized relationship between organized criminal groups
crime – the trafficking of illegal goods and ser- and the formation of states and under what con-
vices, predatory crimes such as theft, fraud, and ditions we might expect such groups to emerge.
robbery, and ‘illegal governance’ crimes such as In the final section, I discuss the broader impli-
protection payments – involve the cooperation cations of an evolutionary approach for efforts
of individuals in ways that can enhance their to prevent or reduce organized crime.
inclusive fitness at the expense of others. After
briefly reviewing contemporary criminological
approaches to conceptualizing organized crime,
I first examine the nature of organized criminal THE NATURE OF ORGANIZED CRIME
activities and discuss how these tend to focus
on exploiting evolved human motivations and What Is Organized Crime?
their manifestations in contemporary envi-
ronments to promote status among organized The concept of organized crime, like many
criminal offenders. I then outline how an evolu- other such concepts in the social and behav-
tionary perspective can shed light on the nature ioural sciences, is intensely contested, with
of organized crime groups. Specifically, I sug- different authors and agencies employing dif-
gest that cooperation among group members ferent, but overlapping, criteria (Table 15.1;
(in contexts where there can be strong tempta- Paoli and Vander Beken, 2014). Levi (1998:
tions to ‘defect’) can be maintained via three 335) notes that the term ‘organized crime’ ‘…
main evolutionary routes: kin selection, direct is generally applied to describe a group of

Table 15.1  Approaches to defining organized crime


United Nations Office of Council of the European Union2 Abadinsky3
Drugs and Crime1

1. A structured group of Mandatory (all) 1. Is devoid of political goals


three or more persons 1. Collaboration of more than two people 2. Is hierarchical
2. The group exists for a 2. For a prolonged or indefinite period of time 3. Has a limited or exclusive
period of time 3. Suspected of the commission of serious criminal offences membership
3. It acts in concert with 4. Motivated for the pursuit of profit and/or power 4. Constitutes a unique
the aim of committing Non-mandatory (at least 2) sub-culture
at least one serious 1. Each with their own appointed tasks 5. Perpetuates itself
crime 2. Using some form of discipline and control 6. Exhibits a willingness to
4. To obtain, directly or 3. Operating on an international level use illegal violence
indirectly, a financial or 4. Using violence or other means suitable for intimidation 7. Is monopolistic
other material benefit 5. Using commercial or business-like structures 8. Is governed by explicit
6. Engaged in money laundering rules and regulations
7. Exerting influence on politics, the media, public
administration, judicial authorities or the economy

Sources:
1
United Nations Office of Drugs and Crime, 2004, Article 2(a). Downloaded from: www.unodc.org/documents/treaties/UNTOC/
Publications/TOC%20Convention/TOCebook-e.pdf
2
Cited in Paoli and Vander Beken, 2014: 22.
3
Abadinsky, 2017: 2.
298 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

people who act together on a long-term basis arms, wildlife, and counterfeit products
to commit crimes for gain’, and this appears (e.g., Feinstein and Holden, 2014; Reuter,
to capture the core characteristics in many 2014), people smuggling, human trafficking,
definitions, as illustrated in Table 15.1. prostitution, and sex trafficking (Kleemans
Nonetheless, as von Lampe (2016a: 30) and Smit, 2014), illegal gambling (Spapens,
admits, ‘… there is no definition that could 2014), loan sharking, and money laundering
meaningfully delineate the study of organ- (Levi, 2014). In many cases, market-based
ized crime as a field of research’. Paoli crimes involve transactions with ‘willing’
(2016) notes that attempts to define organ- consumers, although this is not always the
ized crime have tended to take one of two case (e.g., human trafficking and sex traffick-
main paths: either they focus on the nature of ing). Market-based crimes, as I elaborate in
the criminal activities that are perpetrated more detail below, involve the provision of
(e.g., drug trafficking, extortion, money laun- goods and services that satisfy motivational
dering), or they concentrate on the nature of needs of consumers, but which are deemed
the groups themselves and how they are by governments to pose threats to the effec-
structured. Similarly, von Lampe (2016a) tive social functioning of society (hence their
argues that there are three main focal points illegality). The nature and scope of such
for definitions of organized crime that can activities thus vary as a function of national
serve to structure research and theory: (1) the and international regulations and organized
criminal activities that organized crime crime groups may move into or out of given
groups engage in; (2) the nature and structure markets depending on changes to their struc-
of organized crime groups; and (3) illegal ture. For example, alcohol prohibition in the
governance and the nature of the power that United States opened a vast market for ille-
organized crime groups can sometimes wield gal alcohol that organized crime groups – in
(Figure 15.1). particular the Italian-American Mafia – were
Organized crime groups engage in a diverse uniquely poised to exploit (Albanese, 2014).
range of illegal activities. These can be use- Predatory crimes, as their name suggests,
fully classified in terms of three main types involve clear victims: individuals exploited in
of activities: market-based crimes, predatory the service of criminal gain. Predatory crimes
crimes, and governance crimes (Figure 15.1; involve a diverse range of activities includ-
von Lampe, 2016a). Market-based crimes ing the running of paedophile rings, organ-
involve the provision of illegal goods and ser- ized fraud, property offending, robbery, and
vices to ‘consumers’. Widely studied exam- the stock-in-trade of many of the most well-
ples include the trafficking in illicit drugs, known organized crime groups: extortion

Figure 15.1  The basic dimensions of organized crime


Source: Based on the analysis in von Lampe (2016a).
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND ORGANIZED CRIME 299

(Varese, 2014). Finally, von Lampe (2016a: substantial – profit. In contrast, associational
77) argues that many organized crime groups criminal structures provide social functions
engage in what he refers to as ‘illegal gov- through the bonding of group members, the
ernance crimes’: ‘… activities that are inher- provision of mutual support, and the rein-
ently linked to the exercise of power within forcement of group norms and rules. Finally,
criminal organizations, criminal milieus and structures that serve to maintain social order
beyond’. Examples include the enforcement within organized crime groups (and, often,
of particular rules or procedures through the wider community) through the resolu-
the threat or use of force, the negotiation tion of conflicts and the imposition of sanc-
of agreements or ‘contracts’ among rival tions are referred to as quasi-government in
criminal groups, and the collection of ‘taxes’ structure, to highlight the similar functions
(i.e. protection payments) from either legal or that states provide their citizens. One endur-
illegal business operations (von Lampe, 2016a: ing challenge for organized crime groups is
78). Clearly, there is overlap between many the maintenance of cooperation among group
of the activities that criminal groups engage members in contexts where ‘defection’ can
in and many such groups will be involved lead to criminal arrest. As argued below, an
in a diverse range of criminal activities that evolutionary perspective can help to explain
might include trafficking, organized fraud, the particular kinds of structures that tend to
and the collection of protection payments to emerge among organized crime groups and
facilitate business monopolies. The Japanese which help to promote cooperation among
Yakuza, for example, are heavily involved group members.
in the trafficking of illegal drugs (particu- Finally, the third main focal point for con-
larly amphetamines), facilitate sex traffick- ceptualizing organized crime focuses on the
ing and illegal gambling, offer ‘protection’ nature of illegal governance. As many schol-
services for both legitimate and illegitimate ars have noted, organized crime groups often
business operations, and play an active role in operate in ways that mimic the functions of
dispute resolution for legal businesses (e.g., legitimate states: they collect ‘taxes’ from
debt collection and bankruptcy management) businesses, provide third-party enforcement
(Abadinsky, 2017; Hill, 2014). of ‘laws’, offer dispute-resolution mecha-
The structure of organized crime groups nisms, and in many cases provide public
varies substantially from group to group goods to local communities. For instance,
(and within groups), depending in part on the the head of the Medellin drug cartel, Pablo
main kinds of illegal activities that the group Escobar, donated substantial sums of money
is involved in. Von Lampe (2016a, 2016b) to the community for public works pro-
argues that there are three basic types of grammes, including the construction of a
criminal structure: entrepreneurial structures, football stadium (Thoumi, 2014; von Lampe,
associational criminal structures, and quasi- 2016a). Illegal governance activities reflect
government criminal structures (Figure 15.1). the power that organized crime groups can
Entrepreneurial structures are organized in yield, often through complex relationships
ways that facilitate economic activities that with legitimate businesses, political elites,
provide material benefits to organized groups and individuals who are involved in the ‘legit-
and their members. Transnational drug- imate’ running of state functions (e.g., police,
trafficking networks, for example, are often politicians, prosecutors, and civil servants).
structured in ways that involve specialized Although a substantial body of research
divisions of labour (from growers/manufac- has accumulated on the topic of organized
turers to traffickers to street-level dealers) crime and some advances have been made in
that serve the ultimate goal of delivering how best to conceptualize the activities and
the illicit drugs to consumers at an – often structures of organized crime groups, most
300 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

research has been descriptive. As such, as – were almost certainly absent from the
Paoli (2016: 3) notes, ‘no systematic atten- largely egalitarian hunter-gatherer societies
tion has so far been given to the causes that persisted throughout the Pleistocene.
of organized crime’. Most scholars have Organized crime, as I argue below, emerges
drawn from mainstream criminological per- most prominently in contexts where there is
spectives in attempts to explain organized a concentration of difficult-to-defend
criminal offending (e.g., Abadinsky, 2017, resources and competition among groups for
chapter 2). The handful of specific theoreti- access to those resources. Until the advent of
cal approaches – such as ‘ethnic succession agriculture, the existence of such resources
theory’ and ‘alien conspiracy theory’ tend to would have been limited.
focus on the ethnic status of organized crime Evolutionary psychologists are well versed
groups in society and thus have limited scope in the idea that there is often a ‘mismatch’
for understanding the diversity of organized between evolved psychological mechanisms
crime groups and organized criminal activi- and contemporary environments (Confer
ties both historically and cross-culturally. et  al., 2010). Given that organized crime,
Other approaches tend to focus on how as it is manifest in the modern world, is an
organized crime groups are similar in their evolutionarily novel phenomenon facilitated
functions to legal businesses or to legitimate by the development of large-scale societies,
political organizations (see Kleemans, 2014 it is reasonable to consider its potential evo-
for a review of these ideas). Ultimately, the lutionary roots: what features of past envi-
challenge for theoretical approaches is to ronments might have favoured motivations
explain (a) the motivations for engaging to form coalitions among group members
in organized crime; (b) the scope of organ- for the exploitation of others? Human-male
ized criminal activities; (c) the structures of coalitional aggression is arguably a feature
organized crime groups; (d) how such groups of our species that has deep roots in hominin
maintain cooperation among group members; evolution. Like our closest genetic relative,
and (d) how such groups persist over time the chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes), human
and space. An evolutionary psychological males form relatively fluid coalitions in a
perspective can contribute to a better under- range of contexts including collective hunt-
standing of these key issues, beginning with ing, disputes over status, and inter-group
a novel way of conceptualizing the primary conflict including the sporadic raiding of
nature of organized crime. other groups (Wrangham and Glowacki,
2012). In addition, humans employ col-
lective aggression in the punishment of
norm violators (Boehm, 2012). Although in
AN EVOLUTIONARY PERSPECTIVE humans both males and females form part
ON ORGANIZED CRIME of parochial groups which favour in-group
over out-group members, males tend to be
An immediate question for an approach to overwhelmingly represented in coalitions
organized crime that draws from evolution- that involve significant risk to protagonists
ary psychology is its novelty in historical (e.g., hunting, warfare).
terms: was organized crime, or anything like According to the ‘male warrior hypoth-
organized crime, a recurrent feature of ances- esis’ (McDonald et al., 2012; van Vugt et al.,
tral environments in hominin evolution? The 2007) ‘humans, particularly men, may pos-
paradigmatic examples of organized crime sess psychological mechanisms enabling
that are the feature of research in the modern them to form coalitions capable of planning,
world – hierarchically structured large-scale initiating and executing acts of aggression
organizations like the Mafia or the Yakuza on members of out-groups (with the ultimate
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND ORGANIZED CRIME 301

goal of acquiring or protecting reproductive both all-female and mixed-gender organized


resources)’ (McDonald et  al., 2012: 671). crime groups and women may be involved in
The male warrior hypothesis suggests that organized crime in a range of different roles
men, relative to women, may be particu- and contexts (see Pizzini-Gambetta, 2014).
larly prone to ethnocentrism, and are more In addition to the preponderance of male
strongly motivated to bond with in-group organized crime offenders, many organized
members. I argue, then, that organized crime, crime groups – such as the Mafia in both Italy
from an evolutionary perspective, can be and the United States – are exclusively male
conceptualized as a (predominantly) male and specifically exclude women from mem-
coalitional strategy to exploit resources from bership (Paoli, 2003). Finally, research sug-
others in the service of reproductive goals. Of gests that in addition to greater co-offending
course, the form that organized crime takes among males relative to females, when males
is highly variable and reflects, as I elaborate do offend with others they are more likely to
in more detail below, the outcome of cultural employ violence and seriously injure victims
evolutionary processes. However, at its core, (Bourgeois and Fisher, 2018; Lantz, 2019).
it reflects psychological adaptations that have Although organized crime often involves
evolved in the context of male coalitional the use of, or the threat of the use of, violence,
aggression. it involves a diversity of different activities –
We should expect, then, if organized crime from racketeering, to the trafficking of ille-
is largely a male coalitional strategy for gal substances – that fundamentally involve
exploiting resources from others, that most exploiting others for gain. This is most obvi-
perpetrators of organized crime are male, ously the case when organized crime groups
and that organized crime groups have fea- extort payment from individuals or busi-
tures that specifically promote male bonding. nesses in exchange for protection (often from
Clear information about the demographic the organized crime group itself) because, in
characteristics of organized crime offend- effect, this is the appropriation of resources
ers is difficult to come by, but the evidence backed up by the threat of violence. The traf-
we do have suggests that most offenders are ficking of people, organs, and wildlife also
male. For example, Kirby et  al. (2016), in are clearly exploitative and involve genuine
an analysis of 2.1 million recorded offend- harm to victims. However, many organized
ers drawn from the UK Police National groups, including the Mafia, offer ‘genuine’
Computer database between 2007 and 2010, protection services to willing individuals or
found that of the 4,109 offenders who met the groups (Paoli, 2016), and may be involved in
criteria for organized crime, 95% were male bringing order to otherwise lawless commu-
(compared to 78% of general offenders). The nities. In addition, trafficking in many ille-
literature on gender differences in offend- gal substances such as illicit drugs involves
ing highlights the fact that males are more the provision of goods to willing custom-
likely to perpetrate offences than females, ers (Kassab and Rosen, 2019; Paoli, 2016).
and that the asymmetry between males and Nonetheless, as a general rule, organized
females is greatest for sexual and serious crime groups and their members dispropor-
violent offending (e.g., homicide) (Durrant, tionally benefit from the goods and services
2019). It is noteworthy, then, that the gender that they provide relative to their custom-
bias for organized crime is similar to that for ers, and all organized crime, by definition,
homicide, even though in this sample the ‘exploits’ society by making profits without
majority of organized crime offenders did paying taxes. Indeed, there is good economic
not engage in violent crime. It is worth rec- evidence that the presence of organized crime
ognizing, however, that although organized groups tends to result in a suppression of eco-
crime is largely a male phenomenon there are nomic growth in those areas where they are
302 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

more active (van Dijk, 2007), suggesting that needs (Kenrick et  al., 2010), particularly
such groups are not operating in the interests those that relate to affiliation, status, mate
of the wider community. acquisition, and mate retention. I discuss the
Organized criminal activities, then, way that organized crime groups can meet
involve both the perpetrators of organized affiliation needs in more detail in the next
crime and a somewhat heterogenous group section, but it is worth noting how some
of individuals who can be considered ‘con- organized crime groups afford opportunities
sumers’ (some more willingly than others) of for social bonding among group members
the goods and services that organized crime that may not have been available through
groups provide. To understand why organ- legitimate channels. Membership in organ-
ized criminal activities occur it is impor- ized crime groups can also meet funda-
tant to recognize the motivational needs of mental motives underlying status. Status is
these two groups: offenders and consum- important from an evolutionary perspective
ers. For many consumers, the motivations because higher status can be translated into
are straightforward: they are able to obtain greater reproductive success (van Vugt and
goods and services that would otherwise be Tybur, 2015; von Rueden and Jaeggi, 2016;
unavailable through legitimate channels. It is von Rueden et  al., 2011). Organized crime
noteworthy that the kinds of goods and ser- groups provide various opportunities for sta-
vices that organized crime groups provide tus enhancement. Simple membership in an
typically map onto evolved motivational sys- organized crime group can reap substantial
tems (Kenrick et al., 2010). The ‘protection’ rewards in status – at least in the criminal
services that groups like the Mafia provide, underworld. For example, becoming a for-
for instance, are readily sought out (as well as mal member of groups like the post-Soviet
imposed) because in environments that lack ‘Thieves in Law’ or the Hell’s Angels motor-
robust and reliable third-party enforcement cycle gang, is an arduous task involving
of laws, humans seek to meet fundamental a lengthy screening process with success-
needs for safety and self-protection for them- ful applicants significantly enhancing their
selves and their kin. The provision of illegal social status among their peers (von Lampe,
drugs, gambling, and sex (via prostitution 2016b). Many organized crime groups are
and sex trafficking) also, arguably, tap into also structured in terms of hierarchies that
underlying motivational systems. Perhaps afford opportunities for advancement with
most straightforwardly from an evolution- commensurate benefits in terms of status and
ary perspective, human males are motivated access to resources. New York Mafia fami-
to seek opportunities for sex with desirable lies, for instance, are made up of ‘members’,
mates and hence are willing consumers of or ‘made guys’, who form ‘crews’ headed by
these services provided by some organized a caporegime or ‘captain’. Captains, in turn,
crime groups. The motivation to consume defer to the all-powerful ‘boss’ who wields
psychoactive drugs (and, perhaps, by exten- immense influence (Abadinsky, 2016). As
sion other activities like gambling) reflect the Abadinsky (2016: 53) explains: ‘The boss
evolution of rewards systems that drugs act demands absolute respect and total obedi-
on, even if they were not specifically selected ence … the boss is treated with a great deal
for drug use (Durrant et  al., 2009; Nesse, of deference. People rise when he enters
1994; although some forms of drug use may the room, and they never interrupt when
have played a role in enhancing fitness – see he is speaking’. There has been almost no
Roulette et al., 2016). research on how such status is translated into
Understanding the motivations of offend- reproductive success among organized crim-
ers from an evolutionary perspective entails inals, although van San (2011: 281) provides
considering ‘higher-order’ motivational an interesting analysis of how many women
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND ORGANIZED CRIME 303

‘find themselves uncontrollably drawn to THE PROBLEM OF COOPERATION


men who have made a career in crime’. IN ORGANIZED CRIME
Although the motivations to engage in
organized crime are likely to have an evolu- Organized crime involves the coordination of
tionary basis, there are also individual differ- at least two individuals in the commission of
ence factors that might help to explain why illegal activities, although for many organ-
some people are more likely to participate in ized crime groups there are a large number of
organized criminal groups than others. There individuals involved. Organized crime is,
has been surprisingly little work done on thus, an inherently cooperative endeavour
‘risk factors’ for organized crime in general, and, as such, poses an important and widely
although there is an extensive literature on
recognized problem: how is cooperation
the factors that promote involvement in cer-
maintained in the face of temptations to free-
tain kinds of organized criminal groups such
ride or defect? (Tooby and Cosmides, 2010).
as gangs (e.g., Lenzi et al., 2015), or activi-
The structure of the widely researched
ties such as human trafficking (e.g., Beeson,
‘Prisoner’s Dilemma game’ provides an apt
2014). Perhaps unsurprisingly the risk factors
illustration of this problem. In this game, as it
identified in this research are similar to the
was originally developed, two offenders have
risk factors for offending in general: social
been arrested by the police and have agreed to
deprivation, antisocial personality character-
keep silent about their criminal activities to
istics, antisocial attitudes and cognitions, and
protect themselves and their partner. If they
association with antisocial peers (Durrant,
2018). For many individuals, involvement in are successful in this (i.e. they cooperate),
organized criminal groups provides opportu- they will receive a one-year prison sentence.
nities for accruing status and resources that The police, as is their wont, offer a tangible
might be difficult to obtain through legiti- incentive to rat on their partner (i.e. to defect):
mate channels (e.g., see Bourgeois, 1995 if they talk, they will be set free, unpunished,
on involvement in drug dealing). However, and their partner will receive a lengthy
it needs to be recognized that many offend- 10-year prison sentence. If they incriminate
ers, especially those occupying ‘higher-level’ each other, they will get five years in prison.
positions in organized criminal groups, may The structure of the dilemma is obvious: the
diverge from this more typical profile. There best collective outcome for both offenders is
appears to be very little research on these to stay silent because they will receive a com-
individuals, but certain roles are likely to bined two years in prison. However, the best
favour traits that are more similar to white- individual outcome is to be set free, but
collar offenders, such as a greater capacity because this outcome is presented to each
for self-regulation (Ragatz et al., 2012), even offender, if both individuals take this option,
though they may have other antisocial per- they will both be worse off than if they stayed
sonality characteristics (Craparo et al., 2018). silent (Cronk and Leech, 2013). The Prisoner’s
An evolutionary approach can help us Dilemma provides a vivid illustration of a
delineate the core aspects of organized crime more general problem for evolutionary
and explain both the motivations for engag- theory: how do we explain cooperation among
ing in organized crime and the typical activi- group members given the selective advan-
ties pursued by organized crime groups. It tages of free-riding or defection?
can also help us to understand, as outlined There is now an extensive literature on the
below, how these groups are structured. More evolution of altruism and cooperation and a
specifically, it can shed light on how coopera- number of viable evolutionary mechanisms
tion is maintained in such groups in the face have been identified that can maintain cooper-
of incentives to defect. ation among individuals (see Clutton-Brock,
304 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

2009; Cronk and Leech, 2013; Kurzban values, institutions – that tend to promote
et  al., 2015; West et  al., 2011, for reviews). cooperation among in-group members will
Cooperation among kin is easily explained: be more successful relative to groups without
because kin are more likely to share genes these characteristics and hence these particu-
compared to non-kin, any behaviour that lar norms, values, and institutions will tend
facilitates interactions that benefit kin or to flourish at the expense of others. Recent
are mutually beneficial will be selected work suggests that cultural group selection
for (Dawkins, 1976; Hamilton, 1964). processes may have been important in the
Cooperation also occurs widely among non- cultural evolution of prosocial religions that
kin in humans and other animals and can be are maintained via constellations of norms,
maintained via direct reciprocity: if all parties values, and practices that demarcate in-group
benefit from cooperation, then all parties will from out-group members (e.g., rituals, hard-
be motivated to cooperate (Trivers, 1971). to-fake markers) and promote social cohesion
Cooperation can be maintained via direct and cooperation among members of the in-
reciprocity even if the exchange of benefits group (see Norenzayan, 2013; Norenzayan
is not simultaneous as long as they are recip- et  al., 2016; Saroglou, 2011). In particular,
rocated at some future date. Cooperation cultural evolutionary approaches to the evo-
may also evolve via what some scholars have lution of religion highlight the role of specific
termed ‘indirect reciprocity’. Cooperative norms and values that bind co-religionists
behaviour can be a reliable signal to other (e.g., the notion of fictive kin – all adherents
group members that the individual can be are ‘brothers’ and ‘sisters’): costly, time-­
trusted and thus they are likely to be cooper- consuming, and sometimes painful rituals
ated with in the future (Kurzban et al., 2015). that serve as clear markers of group commit-
Both direct and indirect reciprocity are vul- ment (what Norenzayan, 2013, calls ‘cred-
nerable, however, to exploitation. Individuals ibility enhancing displays’), and often strict
who cooperate but are not cooperated with in rules and their enforcement by both group
turn will be at a selective disadvantage relative members (Durrant and Poppelwell, 2017),
to those who accept the benefits of cooperation and by a god or gods (Norenzayan, 2013).
without reciprocating in kind. One solution to Similar processes operate to bind individu-
this problem is the evolution of punishment. als into closely knit male groups (army units,
Individuals who receive cooperation but don’t terrorist cells) in the context of inter-group
return it in kind may be subject to sanctions – conflict (e.g., Whitehouse et  al., 2014),
either at the hands of the cooperator (what we consistent with the male warrior hypothesis
call ‘revenge’ or ‘retaliation’ – Jackson et al., outlined above.
2019; McCullough et al., 2013) or via a third- How might these various evolutionary
party (what is often termed ‘altruistic punish- processes that support cooperation help us
ment’ or, more generally, ‘justice’ – Boyd to understand the nature, structure, and prac-
et al., 2003). tices of (largely male-based) organized crime
Finally, a number of scholars have argued groups? Table 15.2 provides an overview
that the evolution of cooperation in large-scale of how evolutionary mechanisms for coop-
groups, of the kind that most people live in eration might map on to the different organ-
today, has been shaped by cultural evolution- ization structures elucidated by von Lampe
ary processes (Chudek et al., 2013; Henrich, (2016a, 2016b). Many organized criminal
2016; Richerson et al., 2017; Turchin, 2016). groups are based on interactions involving
A particular emphasis has been placed on the family members (von Lampe, 2016b). This
potential role of cultural group selection in seems to be the case for both relatively small-
shaping cooperative behaviour within groups. scale organized crime groups as well as for
Groups that have characteristics – norms, larger, more complex criminal organizations.
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND ORGANIZED CRIME 305

Table 15.2  The functional structures of organized crime groups in relation to the key
evolutionary processes that facilitate cooperation1
Entrepreneurial Associational Quasi-governmental

Kin-selection Blood family associations and


‘fictive kin’ structures, and
‘ethnic’ groups
Direct reciprocity Illegal networks, markets, Mutual support Honouring of commercial
and businesses exchanges and ‘contracts’
Indirect reciprocity Reputation-based promotion Reputation, honour, loyalty
Punishment Violence used to enforce Mechanisms for the enforcement Sanctions for violations
reciprocal exchange – of rules and norms administered by group members
‘revenge’
Cultural selection The role of group-based norms, Mechanisms for regulation, conflict
markers, rituals, and practices resolution, protection
1
Examples provided are illustrative only and may be influenced by multiple evolutionary mechanisms (e.g., revenge may
have been shaped by both genetic and cultural evolutionary processes – Jackson et al., 2019).

In the UK, for instance, there is a long history of emphasis on the idea of ‘brotherhood’, the
criminal groups based on cooperation among Japanese Yakuza invoke the notion of Ikka,
brothers: the Sabini brothers in the 1920s, or family, creating an organizational struc-
the Richardson brothers in South London ture that mimics that of the family (with
in the 1940s and 1950s, and the notorious a ‘father’, and ‘brothers’), and the Hong
Kray twins during the same period in the Kong Triads invoke a concept of ‘universal
East End of London (Antonopoulos and Triad brotherhood’ which links the various
Papanicolaou, 2018). The Latin American Triad societies (von Lampe, 2016a). From
drug cartels that emerged during the 1970s an evolutionary perspective, the prevalence
were also often centred on leaders who were of both actual and fictive blood ties reflects
close kin. The Ochoa clan, along with Pablo the evolution of mechanisms that promote
Escobar, led the Medellin cartel, while the cooperation among close kin. Thus, they are
Cali cartel was led by the Rodriguez-Orejuela likely to reduce defection among members
brothers (Abadinsky, 2017; Antonopoulos of organized crime groups while enhancing
and Papanicolaou, 2018). Italian Mafia cooperation and social cohesion. Indeed,
organizations have also historically been many large-scale organized crime groups
based on blood ties, although this varies emphasize the importance of fictive-kin ties
somewhat among the different Mafia groups (among ‘brothers’) over and above ties to
(Antonopoulos and Papanicolaou, 2018; family members. The first rule of the Russian
von Lampe, 2016a). Research suggests that ‘Vory v zakone’ (‘Thieves in law’), for exam-
there is often a considerable degree of inter- ple, states: ‘A thief must turn his back on his
generational continuity among members of family – mother, father, brothers, and sisters.
organized crime groups as sons in particular The criminal community is family’ (cited in
follow their fathers’ involvement (van Dijk von Lampe, 2016a: 166).
et al., 2018). Cooperation among organized crime
Many large-scale organized crime groups groups is also maintained via direct and
(like many religious groups) tap into evolved indirect reciprocity. The operation of direct
mechanisms to preferentially cooperate reciprocity is straightforward. In the same
with kin by creating family-like structures, manner that legal transactions are maintained
based on the notion of ‘fictive kin’. Thus, the through simultaneous or delayed exchanges
various branches of the Italian Mafia place of goods or cash, the illegal activities of
306 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

organized criminals are facilitated through like “you’d rather I’d say” … [but if] I say it
such reciprocal exchanges. Individuals who now and then I’ll get killed and then what?’
do not reciprocate will be viewed as less (Maier and Ricciardelli, 2019: 245; see also
trustworthy and hence are less likely to be Dudai, 2018, for the role of punishment
chosen as cooperative partners in the future among members of the Irish Republican
(in addition to being the object of poten- Army). Entrepreneurial structures based
tial sanctions, including those that operate on reciprocal exchange are vulnerable to
through quasi-governmental-like processes). exploitation and defection because agree-
The exchange of goods and services among ments cannot be enforced via legal channels.
members of organized crime groups, how- Thus, protagonists in organized crime must
ever, is not always immediate or necessar- be willing to use violence and other threats
ily reciprocal, and cooperative behaviour to enforce cooperation. The high levels of
may be maintained as a way of signalling violence in some illegal markets, such as the
trustworthiness to other group members. drug market, reflect the absence of formal
Values such as honour, trustworthiness, and mechanisms of dispute resolution (Reuter,
loyalty to the group are strongly emphasized 2016). Many large-scale organized crime
in many organized crime groups such as groups, however, have clear rules and mecha-
the Mafia. In turn, organized crime groups nisms for their enforcement that entail quasi-
provide extensive mutual support to group governmental processes of dispute resolution
members in need: ‘The bonds that tie indi- and sanctioning. These processes go beyond
viduals together in an associational crimi- ‘revenge’ and may include processes that
nal structure almost by nature establish a resemble the criminal justice system, includ-
commitment to mutual support. There is an ing collective decision making, dispute reso-
unspoken or explicit obligation to come to lution between parties, and graded sanctions
the aid of other members when needed’ (von that include fines, suspension of member-
Lampe, 2016b: 25). As Paoli (2003: 17) ship, and death (von Lampe, 2016a). Some
notes for the Italian Mafia, aid to other methods are specific to particular groups
members is given ‘with no expectation of and serve as markers of group commitment:
short-term rewards’. among Japanese Yakuza, for example, finger
Even among members of organized crime amputation is employed as both a punish-
groups there may be temptations to ‘defect’ ment and a signal to others of commitment to
or ‘free-ride’ – to benefit from the coopera- the group (Hill, 2014).
tion of others while not fully reciprocating in When groups become large or entail high
turn. The role of sanctions in shaping behav- levels of cooperation among members, social
iour among offenders who violate norms is cohesion and cooperation are more difficult
well recognized. For example, Maier and to sustain. If, as suggested above, coopera-
Ricciardelli (2019), in a qualitative study tion in large-scale groups is partly maintained
of 56 former prisoners in Canada, found via norms, values, and institutions that have
that prisoners would often rather accept the been culturally selected for, then we might
institutional punishment handed out by the expect ‘successful’ organized crime groups
authorities than risk the informal punishment (e.g., those with greater longevity) to have
they might receive at the hands of their fellow converged upon rules, norms, and processes
inmates for being a ‘snitch’. As one partici- that maintain group cooperation. Codes of
pant recounted: ‘ …’ cause I figured out … conduct are similar across geographically
‘cause I found out who did it but I didn’t say divergent organized crime groups, provid-
anything [to the prison staff] … because they ing some support for this suggestion (Kassab
[prison staff] try to tell me well “why didn’t and Rosen, 2019; von Lampe, 2016b). They
you say who did it or you get it shit?” I’m usually coalesce around a set of strict rules or
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND ORGANIZED CRIME 307

obligations: (1) absolute loyalty to the group of secrecy among many organized crime
above all others, including kin; (2) strict groups, such as the Mafia, overt markers
secrecy concerning the nature of the group’s of group status may be absent from some
members and activities; and (3) honesty and groups. From the perspective of cultural
reciprocity among group members. In addi- group selection theory (Richerson et  al.,
tion, organized crime groups often have a 2016), it is easy to see why different organ-
diverse set of specific rules about how individ- ized crime groups have converged on similar
uals conduct themselves with both group and mechanisms for maintaining cooperation:
non-group members, and the kinds of criminal those groups that promote extreme within-
activities they can and cannot engage in. For group cooperation and that adhere to clear
example, Italian Mafia groups traditionally rules (like codes of secrecy) are likely to
prohibit involvement in drug trafficking or the thrive at the expense of groups that do not.
use of kidnapping for ransom, to avoid draw- There has been very little empirical
ing unnecessary attention to themselves (von research that has systematically examined
Lampe, 2016b). Interestingly, various organ- the nature of cooperation among members of
ized crime groups such as the Italian Mafia organized crime groups (or among offenders
also prohibit sexual relations with other mem- in general). In the first study to employ the
bers’ wives – a rule that is likely to promote Prisoner’s Dilemma game with a sample of
and maintain cohesive bonding among male prison inmates, Khadjavi and Lange (2013)
members (Kassab and Rosen, 2019). found that the prison sample was more likely
Cultural group selection processes are to cooperate in the simultaneous Prisoner’s
facilitated through clear mechanisms for Dilemma (where the decision to cooperate or
symbolically delineating group bound­ defect is made at the same time) compared
aries that demarcate group membership to student participants. Rates of cooperation
(Richerson et al., 2016). In addition, social were roughly the same in the two groups in
cohesion among group members, as noted response to a sequential Prisoner’s Dilemma
above, can be enhanced via participation (where individuals respond with cooperation
in collective rituals. The role of initiation or defection in response to an initial response
rituals and markers of group status are by their partner). The results perhaps reflect
common among organized crime groups. the existence of informal ‘prisoner codes’ that
Among Italian Mafia groups, traditional structure social interactions in correctional
initiation ceremonies involve the pricking facilities, and facilitate cooperation among
of the index finger so that blood drips on a inmates (Maier and Ricciardelli, 2019).
saint’s icon, which is then set on fire in the Interestingly, a more recent study using a
hand of the candidate, who swears loyalty sample of community-supervised offend-
to the Mafia and its rules (Antonopoulos ers found lower rates of cooperation in a
and Papanicolaou, 2018). Markers of group Prisoner’s Dilemma game in the offending
membership are also common: membership group compared to a control group (Clark
into outlaw motorcycle gangs is often sig- et  al., 2015), suggesting that it may be fea-
nalled by the wearing of distinct patches, the tures of the prison environment that play a
Russian vory v zakone sport specific tattoos role in structuring cooperative behaviour in
that signal group status communicated in a these games (although the nature of the sam-
coded language only known among group ple could also have been a factor, given that
members (Abadinsky, 2017). Among the Khadjavi and Lange’s study involved women
Yakuza, extensive body tattooing is com- prisoners only).
mon, and members are required to partici- Although we may expect that coopera-
pate in ceremonies such as initiation rites tion among offenders depends on specific
(Hill, 2014). However, given the importance contextual factors (e.g., lower in general, but
308 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

perhaps higher in institutional contexts), we of reciprocity. Interestingly, in the PD-TPP


should expect that organized criminals dis- game, when a third party could pay points to
play high rates of cooperation among group sanction the two participants in the PD game
members for the reasons that have been pre- (by taking points from them for not cooper-
sented above: kin and fictive-kin structures, ating), rates of cooperation dropped among
networks of direct and indirect reciprocity, both the Camorristi and the students but
and strong social norms and rules that pro- increased among the ordinary prisoners. This
mote trust, cooperation, and bonding among suggests that the introduction of sanctions
group members. This is exactly what Nese may reduce cooperation under some circum-
et  al. (2018) found in an experimental field stances, particularly if the sanctions are per-
study with a sample of Camorra (Mafia) ceived as unfair (see Fehr and Rockenbach,
prisoners recruited from a jail in Naples. 2003). In a related study employing a battery
Participants in this study (129 Camorra, of economic games with samples of students
109 ‘ordinary’ criminals, and 109 stu- residing in both pro- and anti-Mafia neigh-
dents) engaged in two different experimen- bourhoods in Palermo, the researchers found
tal games: a one-shot Prisoner’s Dilemma that levels of trust, trustworthiness, and altru-
(PD), and one-shot Prisoner’s Dilemma with ism were lower among students in the pro-
third-party punishment (PD-TPP). In the Mafia neighbourhoods, but that in-group
PD game, participants were given an enve- favouritism on these measures was higher
lope containing 10 tokens and paired with than in the anti-Mafia neighbourhood (Meier
another individual with a similar envelope et  al., 2016). This implies that the strong
containing 10 tokens. Both individuals had presence of organized crime groups might
to simultaneously decide whether to (a) keep erode generalized trust and cooperation in
their tokens; or (b) send them to their part- the environments in which they are active but
ner, in which case the researcher would triple promote greater cooperation among in-group
the amount received (at the end of the study members. Similar results have been found in
tokens could be exchanged for real money). research that examined the role of religion
The Prisoner’s Dilemma structure here is on cooperation: adherents tend to be more
clear: if both individuals cooperate and send prosocial and cooperative, but also more
their tokens, they will receive 30 points (the parochial, more strongly favouring in-groups
10 received times 3), if both defect and keep over out-groups (e.g., Purzycki et al., 2018).
their own tokens they obtain 10 points, and Clearly, more empirical research is needed
if one cooperates while the other defects the on the nature of cooperation among organ-
cooperator receives nothing while the defec- ized crime members, but these initial studies
tor obtains 40 points (their original 10 plus support the idea that cooperation has been
the 10 they receive multiplied by three). All maintained through a combination of mecha-
games were played with anonymous part- nisms including direct and indirect reciproc-
ners, but participants knew which group they ity along with third-party punishment and
were playing with (e.g., Camorristi interacted the maintenance of group-based norms and
with Camorristi, and students interacted with values.
students).
The results indicated that the organized
criminal offenders (the Camorristi) engaged
in higher levels of cooperation (86.7% of ORGANIZED CRIME AND STATE
trials) compared to the students (67.5%) BUILDING
and the ordinary criminals (50.0%), provid-
ing strong support for the idea that organ- Charles Tilly (1985/2002: 35) influentially
ized crime members abide by strong norms drew a somewhat startling comparison
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND ORGANIZED CRIME 309

between organized crime groups and the and under what conditions organized crime
institutions of ‘legitimate’ statehood: groups tend to emerge.
Understanding the transition from small,
Apologists for particular governments and for
largely mobile hunter-gatherer bands to sed-
government in general commonly argue, precisely,
that they offer protection from local and external entary, large-scale societies with state-like
violence. They claim that the prices they charge features remains a formidable task that has
barely cover the costs of protection. They call engaged scholars from a wide range of disci-
people who complain about the price of protection plines. Although there remain many theories of
‘anarchists’, ‘subversives’, or both at once. But
state formation (Carneiro, 1970; Turchin et al.,
consider the definition of a racketeer as someone
who creates a threat, then charges for its reduction. 2018), it seems that early states shared many
Governments’ provision of protection, by this of the features of organized crime groups
standard, often qualifies as racketeering. To the such as the Mafia, as they provided protec-
extent that the threats against which a given tion while extracting profits and wielding sub-
government protects its citizens are imaginary, or
stantial amounts of largely unchecked power
are a consequence of its own activities, the
government has organized a protection racket. (Scheidel, 2017). As Scheidel (2017: 48) lacon-
ically suggests: ‘Reduced to essentials, history
This comparison speaks to a number of has known only two ideal-typical modes of
important similarities between the ‘legiti- wealth acquisition: making and taking’. Male
mate’ practices and institutions of the nation coalitionary elites are uniquely poised to fol-
state and those that are apparent in many low the latter pathway. Scott (2017) argues that
organized crime groups. First, and most the development of high-density agriculture,
importantly, as the sociologist Max Weber particularly cereal production, provided the
(1978) emphasized, states possess a monop- conditions for emerging statehood. Because
oly on legitimate violence to punish rule wealth is heavily concentrated in a clearly
violations and maintain social order. To sus- defined territory, it is chronically vulnerable to
tain a state apparatus that can maintain order exploitation by mobile raiders.
and ‘protect’ its citizens from the predations Raiding, however, is inherently unsta-
of others, it levies taxes from those who ble as an ongoing concern: not only does it
reside within the state’s jurisdiction and, destroy wealth that forms the basis of the
more generally, ‘claims authority … to a very raiding activity, but as raiding increases in
large extent over all action taking place in the frequency sedentary lifestyles become unten-
area of its jurisdiction’ (Weber, 1978: 69). able. ‘Knowing this’, Scott (2017: 240–241)
Similarly, many organized crime groups such suggests
as the Mafia extract payments from busi-
raiders are more likely to adjust their strategy to
nesses that operate within their ‘jurisdiction’
something that looks more like a ‘protection
for which, in turn, they provide protection (in racket.’ In return for a portion of the trade goods,
part, from themselves, but also from other harvest, livestock, and other valuables, the raiders
predatory individuals and groups). Of course, ‘protect’ the traders and communities against
these similarities do not necessarily apply to other raiders and, of course, against themselves…
In extracting a sustainable surplus from sedentary
all organized crime groups and the degree to
communities and fending off external attacks to
which they exercise ‘state-like’ functions protect its base, a stable protection racket like this
vary, in part due to differences to the extent is hard to distinguish from the archaic state itself.
that the activities of such groups entail the
control of specific territory (Koivu, 2016). Recent quantitative analyses of emerging
Nonetheless, the comparison between states statehood suggest that large-scale complex
and organized crime groups is a potentially human societies were more likely to develop
fruitful one that may serve to shed light on in contexts where agriculture had been prac-
both the origin of the state itself, and how ticed for long periods, population size was
310 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

larger, and warfare was more intense (Currie resources that could be readily monopolized
et al., 2019; Turchin et al., 2018). – citrus fruit (Dimicio et al., 2017) and sul-
As humans scaled up from small-scale phur (Buonanno et al., 2015). Although many
hunter-gatherer bands to large-scale socie- organized crime groups interact with legiti-
ties, male coalitions – long a component of mate state actors and institutions in various
human societies in the context of collective forms of ‘symbiotic’-like relationships that
‘power scavenging’, hunting, and inter-group benefit each other, research clearly suggests
raiding – were uniquely placed to exploit that the prevalence of organized crime in a
new opportunities made available through given nation state is strongly (negatively)
the concentration of resources offered by the associated with the quality of rule of law,
emergence of agriculture (although it is worth indicating that it is the absence of legitimate
noting here that the development of agricul- third-party enforcement that creates oppor-
ture preceded the emergence of states by a tunities for organized crime groups to exert
considerable period of time, suggesting that influence (van Dijk, 2007).
agriculture was a precondition, rather than
an inevitable precursor, for state formation –
Gintis et  al., 2019). Hirschfeld (2015), who
has provided one of the few in-depth analyses CONCLUSION
of organized crime from an evolutionary per-
spective, employs the idea of an ‘evolution- I have argued in this chapter that organized
ary stable strategy’ to argue that economic crime involves the coalitional exploitation of
exchanges based purely on reciprocity are others for personal gain in ways that violate
going to be vulnerable to exploitation from criminal laws. Consistent with the male war-
organized crime like activities (raiding, vio- rior hypothesis, I have suggested that selec-
lence, extortion) (although see Koivu, 2016). tion for male coalitional aggression in our
In turn, exploitative groups are vulnerable to evolutionary past has fostered the tendency
others that provide protection with less overt for males to form groups in the context of
or obvious exploitation of primary producers, hunting, raiding, and inter-group conflict. As
resulting in transitions to ‘gangster-states’. such, the activities that comprise organized
More stable state-like institutions of the kind crime – the trafficking of illegal goods and
that operate in contemporary liberal democ- services, predatory crimes such as theft,
racies may emerge but will remain vulner- fraud, and robbery, and ‘illegal governance’
able to exploitation from organized criminal crimes such as protection payments – involve
groups under specific circumstances. the cooperation of (mostly) men in ways that
From this broader perspective, organized can enhance their inclusive fitness at the
crime groups with different characteristics expense of others. Because organized crime
or traits compete both among themselves groups (like any cooperative group) are vul-
and against ‘legitimate’ states for control and nerable to exploitation, their longevity
power over valuable resources. Historical depends on exploiting mechanisms that facil-
analyses suggest that organized crime groups itate cooperation among group members.
are most likely to emerge when there is a val- Three main evolutionary routes have been
uable concentration of defendable resources discussed: kin selection, direct and indirect
and when legitimate state processes for reciprocity, and cultural group selection.
enforcing the law are weak or non-existent. Many organized crime groups are based on a
The emergence of the Sicilian mafia in the nucleus of close family members and many
19th century, for example, occurred against more use cultural mechanisms to enhance
a backdrop of a weak rule of law in com- cooperation via the idea of ‘fictive kin’.
bination with a concentration of valuable Cooperation is also sustained through the
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND ORGANIZED CRIME 311

reciprocal exchange of resources and via strategies – Durrant, 2016) are likely to
mechanisms that encourage trustworthy reduce the lure of such groups. Finally, it is
behaviour, ultimately backed up by the threat worth considering how some highly desir-
of sanctions. Large-scale organized crime able commodities (e.g., drugs) and services
groups often have many of the features that (e.g., prostitution), currently monopolized
promote social cohesion in different group by organized crime groups because they are
contexts (e.g., religious groups), including illegal, may be better managed through legiti-
costly initiation ceremonies, symbolic mark- mate channels, thus eliminating many of the
ing of group members, and rigid codes of harms that arise through their (illegal) sale.
conduct. The capacity for organized crime It is unlikely that we will be able to eradicate
groups to exert power through the control of organized crime groups entirely because they
concentrated resources, the extraction of readily emerge in contexts where state power
‘taxes’, and the use of violence to enforce is weak or non-existent, or corruption is wide-
cooperation offers many similarities with the spread, but recognition of their evolutionary
functions of legitimate governments and may origins may encourage strategies that can
provide insights into the ultimate origin of reduce their largely negative effects on society.
the state in human history. Given the paucity
of research on organized crime from an evo-
lutionary perspective, there is substantial
scope for future research to explore many of REFERENCES
these avenues in further detail.
Finally, it is worth considering how the Abadinsky, H. (2016). Organized crime (11th
approach taken in this chapter suggests vari- edition). Boston, MA: Cengage Learning.
ous avenues for reducing the (largely) harm- Albanese, J. S. (2014). The Italian-American
ful impacts of organized crime on society. Mafia. In L. Paoli (Ed.), The Oxford handbook
Most obviously, if we can conceptualize of organized crime (pp. 142–158). Oxford,
organized crime groups as being in compe- UK: Oxford University Press.
tition with legitimate states, then we should Beeson, J. G. (2014). Psychology of human
employ strategies that support those charac- trafficking. In M. J. Palmiotto (Ed.), Combat-
teristics of states that allow them to promote ting human trafficking: A multidisciplinary
approach (pp. 47–60). London, UK: CRC
public goods – protection, safety, an effec-
Press.
tive rule of law, social services – as they are Boehm, C. (2012). Moral origins: The evolution
likely to reduce the impact and influence of of virtue, altruism, and shame. Philadelphia,
many organized crime groups that may offer PA: Basic Books.
similar ‘services’. Relatedly, approaches that Bourgeois, C. A., & Fisher, M. L. (2018). More
de-couple organized crime groups from legit- ‘bros,’ more woes? The prevalence of male
imate institutions through crackdowns on coalitions in crimes of robbery. Evolutionary
corruption are likely to be effective, as such Behavioral Sciences, 12, 126–131. doi:
groups can no longer exploit state actors and 10.1037/ebs000116
institutions to pursue their own objectives Bourgeois, P. (1995). In search of respect:
and goals. If membership in organized crimi- Selling crack in El Bario. Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University Press.
nal groups is seen by many men as a viable
Boyd, R., Gintis, H., Bowles, S., & Richerson, P. J.
route to obtain social status, as argued above, (2003). The evolution of altruistic punish-
then strategies to improve opportunities ment. Proceedings of the National Academy
for status attainment through legitimate of Sciences, 100, 3531–3535.
means (e.g., better education, reductions in Buonanno, P., Durante, R., Prarolo, G., & Vanin,
inequality, and anything that is likely to pro- P. (2015). Poor institutions, rich mines:
mote the development of slow life history Resource curse in the origins of the Sicilian
312 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

mafia. The Economic Journal, F175–F201. Dudai, R. (2018). Underground penality: The
doi: 10.1111/ecoj.12236 IRA’s punishment of informers. Punishment
Carneiro, R. L. (1970). A theory of the origin of & Society, 20, 375–395. doi: 10.1177/
the state: Traditional theories of state origins 1462474517701302
are considered and rejected in favor of a new Duntley, J. D., & Shackelford, T. K. (2008).
ecological hypothesis. Science, 169, 733–738. Darwinian foundations of crime and law.
Chudek, M., Zhao, W., & Henrich, J. (2013). Aggression and Violent Behaviour, 13,
Culture-gene coevolution, large-scale 373–382. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/97801953
cooperation, and the shaping of human 25188.001.0001
social psychology. In K. Sterelny, R. Joyce, Durrant, R. (2016). Putting risk factors in their
B. Calcott, & B. Fraser (Eds.), Cooperation place: An evolutionary-developmental
and its evolution (pp. 425–457). Cambridge, approach to understanding risk. Psychology,
MA: MIT Press. Crime & Law, 22, 17–32.
Clark, B. C., Thorne, C. B., Hendricks, P. S., Durrant, R. (2018). An introduction to
Sharp, C., Clark, S. K., & Cropsey, K. L. criminology psychology (2nd edition).
(2015). Individuals in the criminal justice London, UK: Routledge.
system show differences in cooperative behav- Durrant, R. (2019). Evolutionary approaches to
iour: Implications for cooperative games. understanding crime: Explaining the gender
Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health, 25, gap in offending. Psychology, Crime & Law,
169–180. doi: 10.1002/cbm.1920 25, 589–608. doi: 10.1080/1068316X.2018.
Clutton-Brock, T. (2009). Cooperation between 1558224
non-kin in animal societies. Nature, 462 Durrant, R., Adamson, S., Todd, F., & Sellman, D.
November, 51–57. doi: 10.1038/nature08366 (2009). Drug use and addiction: An
Confer, J. C., Easton, J. A., Fleischman, D. S., evolutionary perspective. Australian and New
Goetz, C. D., Lewis, D. M. G., Perilloux, C., & Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 43, 1049–1056.
Buss, D. M. (2010). Evolutionary psychology: doi: 10.3109/00048670903270449
Controversies, questions, prospects, and Durrant, R., & Poppelwell, Z. (2017). Religion,
limitations. American Psychologist, 65, 110– crime and punishment: An evolutionary
126. doi: 10.1037/a0018413 perspective. Cham, Switzerland: Springer.
Craparo, G., David, V., Costanzo, G., & Gori, A. Durrant, R., & Ward, T. (2015). Evolutionary
(2018). Cosa Nostra and the Camorra: criminology: Towards a comprehensive
Assessment of personality, alexithymic traits, explanation for crime. Amsterdam,
and attachment styles. International Journal of Netherlands: Academic Press.
Law and Psychiatry, 58, 17–26. doi: 10.1016/ Fehr, E., & Rockenbach, B. (2003). Detrimental
j.ijlp.2018.02.010 effects of sanctions on human altruism.
Cronk, L., & Leech, B. L. (2013). Meeting at Nature, 422, 137–140.
Grand Central: Understanding the social and Feinstein, A., & Holden, P. (2014). Arms
evolutionary roots of cooperation. Princeton, trafficking. In L. Paoli (Ed.), The Oxford
NJ: Princeton University Press. handbook of organized crime (pp. 444–459).
Currie, T. E., Turchin, P., & Gavrilets, S. (2019). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
History of agriculture and intensity of warfare Gintis, H., van Schaik, C., & Boehm, C. (2019).
shaped the evolution of large-scale human Zoon politikon: The evolutionary origins of
societies in Afro-Eurasia. SocArXiv Papers. human socio-political systems. Behavioural
doi: 10.31235/osf.io/9kmrw Processes, 161, 17–30.
Daly, M., & Wilson, M. (1988). Homicide. Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution
Hawthorne, NY: Aldine. of social behavior: I. Journal of Theoretical
Dawkins, R. (1976). The selfish gene. Oxford, Biology, 7, 1–16. doi: 10.1016/0022-5193
UK: Oxford University Press. (64)90038-4
Dimico, A., Isopi, A., & Olsson, O. (2017). Origins Henrich, J. (2016). The secret of our success: How
of the Sicilian Mafia: The market for lemons. culture is driving human evolution, domesticat-
The Journal of Economic History, 77, 1083– ing our species, and making us smarter. Prince-
1115. doi: 10.1017/S002205071700078X ton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND ORGANIZED CRIME 313

Hill, P. (2014). The Japanese Yakuza. In L. Paoli Kurzban, R., Burton-Chellew, M. N., & West,
(Ed.), The Oxford handbook of organized S. A. (2015). The evolution of altruism in
crime (pp. 234–253). Oxford, UK: Oxford humans. Annual Review of Psychology,
University Press. 66. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-
Hirschfeld, K. (2015). Gangster states: 015355
Organized crime, kleptocracy and political Lantz, B. (2019). Co-offending group composi-
collapse. New York, NY: Palgrave Macmillan. tion and violence: The impact of sex, age,
Jackson, J. C., Choi, V. K., & Gelfand, M. J. and group size on co-offending violence.
(2019). Revenge: A multi-level review and syn- Crime & Delinquency. Advance online publi-
thesis. Annual Review of Psychology, 70, cation. doi: 10.1177/0011128719834564
319–345. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010418 Lenzi, M., Sharkey, J., Vieno, A., Mayworm, A.,
Kassab, H. S., & Rosen, J. D. (2019). Illicit Dougherty, D., & Nylund-Gibson, K. (2015).
markets, organized crime, and global security Adolescent gang involvement: The role of
(pp. 63–85). Cham, Switzerland: Palgrave individual, family, peer, and school factors in
Macmillan. a multilevel perspective. Aggressive Behavior,
Kavish, N., Fowler-Finn, K., & Boutwell, B. B. 41, 386–397.
(2017). Criminology’s modern synthesis: Levi, M. (1998). Perspectives on ‘organised
Remaking the science of crime with Darwinian crime’: An overview. The Howard Journal,
insight. In T. K. Shackelford & V. Zeigler-Hill 37, 335–345.
(Eds.), The evolution of psychopathology Levi, M. (2014). Money laundering. In L. Paoli
(pp. 171–183). Cham, Switzerland: Springer (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of organized
International Publishing. crime (pp. 419–443). Oxford, UK: Oxford
Kenrick, D. T., Griskevicius, V., Neuberg, S. L., & University Press.
Schaller, M. (2010). Renovating the pyramid of Maier, K. H., & Ricciardelli, R. (2019). The
needs: Contemporary extensions built upon prisoner’s dilemma: How male prisoners
ancient foundations. Perspectives in Psycho- experience and respond to penal threat
logical Science, 5, 292–314. doi: 10.1177/ when incarcerated. Punishment & Society, 2,
1745691610369469 231–250. doi: 10.1177/1462474518757091
Khadjavi, M., & Lange, A. (2013). Prisoners and McCullough, M. E., Kurzban, R., & Tabak, B. A.
their dilemma. Journal of Economic (2013). Cognitive systems for revenge and
Behaviour and Organization, 92, 163–175. forgiveness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences,
doi: 10.1016/j.jebo.2013.05.015 36, 1–58.
Kirby, S., Francis, B., Humphreys, L., & Soothill, McDonald, M. M., Navarrete, C. D., & van
K. (2016). Using the UK general offender Vugt, M. (2012). Evolution and the
database as a means to measure and psychology of intergroup conflict: The male
analyse organized crime. Policing: An Inter- warrior hypothesis. Proceedings of the
national Journal of Police Strategies and Royal Society of London, B, 367,
Management, 39, 78–94. doi: 10.1108/ 670–679.
PJJPSM-03-2015-0024 Meier, S., Pierce, L., Vaccaro, A., & La Cara, B.
Kleemans, E. (2014). Theoretical perspectives on (2016). Trust and in-group favouritism in a
organized crime. In L. Paoli (Ed.), The Oxford culture of crime. Journal of Economic
handbook of organized crime (pp. 32–52). Behavior & Organization, 132, 78–92. doi:
Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. 10.1016/j.jebo.2016.09.005
Kleemans, E. R., & Smit, M. (2014). Human Nese, A., O’Higgins, N., Sbriglia, P., & Scudiero,
smuggling, human trafficking, and M. (2018). Cooperation, punishment and
exploitation in the sex industry. In L. Paoli organized crime: A lab-in-the-field experi-
(Ed.), The Oxford handbook of organized ment in southern Italy. European Economic
crime (pp. 381–401). Oxford, UK: Oxford Review, 107, 86–98. doi: 10.1016/j.
University Press. euroecorev.2018.05.004
Koivu, K. L. (2016). In the shadow of the state: Nesse, R. M. (1994). An evolutionary perspective
Mafias and illicit markets. Comparative on substance abuse. Ethology and
Political Studies, 49, 155–183. Sociobiology, 15, 339–348.
314 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Norenzayan, A. (2013). Big gods: How religion Brain Science, 39, e30. doi: 10.1017/
transformed cooperation and conflict. S0140525X1400106X
Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Roach, J., & Pease, K. (2013). Evolution and
Norenzayan, A., Shariff, A. F., Willard A. K., crime. London, UK: Routledge.
Slingerland, W. E., Gervais, R. A., McNamara, Roulette, C. J., Kazanji, M., Breurec, S., &
R. A., & Henrich, J. (2016). The cultural Hagen, E. H. (2018). High prevalence of
evolution of prosocial religions. Behavioral cannabis use among Aka foragers of the
and Brain Sciences, 39, e1. Congo basin and its possible relationship to
Paoli, L. (2003). Mafia brotherhoods: Organized helminthiasis. American Journal of Human
crime Italian style. New York, NY: Oxford Biology, 28, 5–15.
University Press. Saroglou, V. (2011). Believing, bonding,
Paoli, L. (2016). Towards a theory of organized behaving, and belonging: The big four religious
crime: Some preliminary reflections. In G. A. dimensions and cultural variation. Journal of
Antonopoulos (Ed.), Illegal entrepreneurship, Cross-Cultural Psychology, 42, 1320–1340.
organised crime, and social control (pp. 3– doi: 10.1177/0022022111412267
17). Cham, Switzerland: Springer. Scheidel, W. (2017). The great leveller: Violence
Paoli, L., & Vander Beken, T. (2014). Organized and the history of inequality from the stone
crime: A contested concept. In L. Paoli (Ed.), age to the twenty-first century. Princeton,
The Oxford handbook of organized crime NJ: Princeton University Press.
(pp. 13–31). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Scott, J. C. (2017). Against the grain: A deep
Press. history of the earliest states. New Haven, CT:
Pizzini-Gambetta, V. (2014). Organized crime: Yale University Press.
The gender constraints of illegal markets. Spapens, T. (2014). Illegal gambling. In L. Paoli
In R. Gartner & B. McCarthy (Eds.), The (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of organized
Oxford handbook of gender, sex, and crime crime (pp. 402–418). Oxford, UK: Oxford
(pp. 448–467). Oxford, UK: Oxford University University Press.
Press. Thoumi, F. E. (2014). Organized crime in
Purzycki, B. G., Henrich, J., Apicella, C., Colombia: The actors running the illegal
Atkinson, Q. C., Baimel, A., Cohen, E., & drug industry. In L. Paoli (Ed.), The Oxford
Norenzayan, A. (2018). The evolution of handbook of organized crime (pp. 177–195).
religion and morality; A synthesis of Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
ethnographic and experimental evidence Tilly, C. (1985/1992). War making and state
from eight societies. Religion, Brain & making as organized crime. In C. Besteman
Behaviour, 8, 101–132. doi: 10.1080/ (Ed.), Violence: A reader (pp. 35–60). New
2153599X.2016.1267027. York, NY: New York University Press.
Ragatz, L. L., Fremouw, W., & Baker, E. (2012). Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2010). Groups in
The psychological profile of white collar mind: The coalitional roots of war and
offenders: Demographics, criminal thinking, morality. In H. Høgh-Olesen (Ed.), Human
psychopathic traits, and psychopathology. morality and sociality: Evolutionary and
Criminal Justice & Behavior, 39, 978–997. comparative perspectives, (pp. 191–234).
Reuter, P. (2014). Drug markets and organized Cham, Switzerland: Palgrave Macmillan.
crime. In L. Paoli (Ed.), The Oxford handbook Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal
of organized crime (pp. 359–380). Oxford, altruism. Quarterly Review of Biology, 46,
UK: Oxford University Press. 35–57.
Reuter, P. (2016). On the multiple sources of Turchin, P. (2016). Ultra society: How 10,000 years
violence in drug markets. Criminology and of war made humans the greatest co-operators
Public Policy, 15, 1–7. on earth. Chaplin, CT: Beresta Books.
Richerson, P., Baldini, R., Bell, A. V., Demps, K., Turchin, P., Whitehouse, H., Korotayev, A.,
Frost, K., Hillis, V., & & Ross, C. (2016). Francois, P., Hoyer, D., Peregrine, P., & Currie,
Cultural group selection plays an essential T. E. (2018). Evolutionary pathways to
role in explaining human cooperation: A statehood: Old theories and new data.
sketch of the evidence. Behavioural and SocArXiv Papers. doi: 10.31235/osf.io/h7tr6
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND ORGANIZED CRIME 315

United Nations Office of Drugs and Crime Proceedings of the National Academy of
(2004). United Nations Convention Against Sciences, 113, 10824–10829. doi: 10.1073/
Transnational Organized Crime and the pnas.1606800113
Protocols Thereto. Retrieved from: www. van Vugt, M., De Cremer, D., & Janssen, D. P.
unodc.org/documents/treaties/UNTOC/ (2007). Gender differences in cooperation
Publications/TOC%20Convention/ and competition: The male warrior
TOCebook-e.pdf (Accessed 15 April 2019) hypothesis. Psychological Science, 18, 19–23.
van Dijk, M. (2007). Mafia markers: Assessing doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01842.x
organized crime and its impact upon van Vugt, M., & Tybur, J. M. (2016). The
societies. Trends in Organized Crime, 10, evolutionary foundations of hierarchy:
39–56. doi: 10.1007/s12117-007-9013-x Status, dominance, prestige, leadership. In
van Dijk, M., Kleemans, E., & Eichelsheim, V. D. M. Buss (Ed.), Handbook of evolutionary
(2018). Children of organized crime offenders: psychology (2nd edition) (pp. 788–809).
Like father, like child? An explorative and Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
qualitative study into mechanisms of Varese, F. (2014). Protection and extortion. In L.
intergenerational (dis)continuity in organized Paoli (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of
crime families. European Journal of Crime and organized crime (pp. 343–358). Oxford, UK:
Policy Research. Advance online publication. Oxford University Press.
doi: 10.1007/s10610-018-9381-6 Walsh, A., & Jorgensen, C. (2018). Evolutionary
van San, M. (2011). The appeal of ‘dangerous’ theory and criminology. In R. Hopcroft (Ed.),
men. On the role of women in organized Oxford handbook of evolution, biology and
crime. Trends in Organized Crime, 14, 281– society (pp. 517–542). Oxford, UK: Oxford
297. doi: 10.10007/s12117-011-9128-y University Press.
von Lampe, K. (2016a). Organized crime: Weber, M. (1978). Economy and society: An
Analyzing illegal activities, criminal structures, outline of interpretive sociology. Berkeley,
and extra-legal governance. Los Angeles, CA: University of California Press.
CA: Sage. West, S. A., El Mouden, C., & Gardner, A. (2011).
von Lampe, K (2016b). The ties that bind: A Sixteen common misconceptions about the
taxonomy of associational criminal structures. evolution of cooperation in humans. Evolution
In G. A. Antonopoulos (Ed.), Illegal and Human Behaviour, 32, 231–262. doi:
entrepreneurship, organised crime, and social 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.08.001
control (pp. 19–35). Cham, Switzerland: Whitehouse, H., McQuinn, B., Buhrmester, M.,
Springer. & Swann, W. (2014). Brothers in arms:
von Rueden, C., Gurven, M., & Kaplan, H. (2011). Libyan revolutionaries bond like family.
Why do men seek status? Fitness payoffs to Proceedings of the National Academy of
dominance and prestige. Proceedings of the Science, 111, 17783–17785. doi: 10.1073/
Royal Society of London, B, 278, 2223–2232. pnas.1416284111
doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.2145 Wrangham, R. W., & Glowacki, L. (2012).
von Rueden, C., & Jaeggi, A. V. (2016). Men’s Intergroup aggression in chimpanzees and
status and reproductive success in 33 war in nomadic hunter-gatherers: Evaluating
nonindustrial societies: Effects of subsistence, the chimpanzee model. Human Nature, 23,
marriage system, and reproductive strategy. 5–29. doi: 10.1007
16
Evolutionary Psychology
and Warfare
Anthony C. Lopez

INTRODUCTION and violence must have been reproductively


beneficial, on average.
The scientific study of the evolution of Second, given an understanding of the the-
human coalitional aggression has exploded oretical requirements, we are better situated
over the last several decades. In four parts, I to examine the historic and prehistoric record
explore and integrate several of the core for evidence of the existence of these con-
frameworks that have emerged to explain the ditions. This involves examination of many
human practice of inter-group violence. First, lines of evidence such as studies of non-
we have a better understanding of the condi- human primates, the archaeological record of
tions required for the evolution of adapta- coalitional aggression, and modern hunter-
tions for coalitional aggression. These gatherer evidence, as well as game-theoretic
include: the presence within a species of a evidence and lab evidence of the design of
unique socio-ecology that facilitates the psychological mechanisms for coalitional
emergence of a group-based population aggression. The latter is particularly impor-
structure; the existence of evolved cognitive tant given that the logic of natural selection
abilities that make possible the navigation of indicates that the extant structure of adapta-
challenges associated with group living that tions themselves provides, in part, a window
are necessary for coalitional aggression (e.g., into ancestral landscapes.
some forms of collective action); and the Third, I explore and integrate current
presence of low-cost/high-benefit reproduc- evidence of psychological adaptations for
tive opportunities that are uniquely well- ­coalitional aggression. There are two forms
seized via coalitional aggression. In short, a of inter-group engagement that have received
species must live in groups, it must be able to the most attention: raids and battles. I also
solve complex collective action problems, examine five areas of inquiry that suggest
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND WARFARE 317

special design for coalitional aggression. reproductive terms – to favor coalitional


These are: the collective action problem of violence as a unique strategy relative to alter-
coordinated violence; parochial altruism; native fitness-enhancing strategies (Enquist
attacker–defender asymmetries; leader–­ and Leimar, 1990; Lopez, 2016a).
follower dynamics; and sex differences in the The first two conditions being met (group-
costs and benefits of violence. structured population; coalitional psychology
Fourth, I speculate on the historical emer- of coordination and cooperation) mean that a
gence of modern human warfare. I do not use species likely possesses some of the psycho-
‘coalitional aggression’ and ‘warfare’ inter- logical tools that can be used for coalitional
changeably. Instead, evolved psychological aggression, but without the third condition,
adaptations for small-scale coalitional aggres- coalitional aggression is unlikely to occur
sion are what make the emergence of large- via these tools alone. Similarly, the mere
scale human warfare possible. Thus, in addition presence of low-cost/high-benefit opportu-
to the biological and evolutionary arguments nities for coalitional violence will not ipso
necessary to establish the existence of adapta- facto lead a species to coalitional aggression
tions for coalitional aggression, I conclude with if there is no group structure in the popula-
a speculative discussion of the historical and tion, or if language and cognitive abilities to
cultural shifts that may help to explain the com- coordinate are limited or absent. For exam-
plex modern phenomenon of human warfare. ple, this simple difference may be one among
many reasons that chimpanzees engage in
an intense and lethal form of coalitional vio-
lence that is nevertheless limited in duration
UNIVERSAL CONSTRAINTS ON THE and complexity relative to humans.
EVOLVABILITY OF COALITIONAL Although our ability to peer into the ances-
VIOLENCE tral past is imperfect, the ancestral past is far
from unknowable. Despite disagreement on
There are at least three necessary conditions the origins and prevalence of warfare, there
that favor the emergence of lethal coalitional is near universal agreement that the nec-
aggression in its broadest sense. First, the essary conditions for coalitional violence
species’ ecology must be such that the organ- existed among ancestral humans throughout
ism is adapted for group-based interaction the Holocene, Pleistocene, and perhaps ear-
(Chapman, 1986; Chapman et  al., 1995; lier to a common ancestor with chimpanzees.
Chapman and Chapman, 2000). Second, the I briefly discuss the evidence for each of the
species must possess neurocomputational three necessary conditions.
machinery that allows it to solve at least a First, although there is disagreement on
subset of a potentially infinite range of the size and structure of early human forager
n-person collective action problems pertinent bands, there is broad agreement that humans
to coalitional aggression (Tooby and associated in groups, that these groups ranged
Cosmides, 1988; Tooby et  al., 2006). This in size from 25 to several hundred individuals
can range from relatively simple coordina- (depending on distinctions between bands,
tion problems, to more complex cooperation tribes, societies, ethnolinguistic units, etc.),
problems involving conspiracy, coercion, and and that these groups included a substantial
alliance. For example, the existence of sym- number of kin as well as non-kin (Hill et al.,
bolic language is crucial for enabling com- 2011; Bird et  al., 2019). Group structure is
plex and large-scale coalitional action unequivocally a component of our species’
(Wrangham, 2019). Third, opportunities for long evolutionary history.
coalitional violence must exist that are Second, there is broad evidence that
sufficiently low-cost and/or beneficial – in humans are instinctively good at solving
318 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

coordination and cooperation problems in both These three universal constraint conditions
dyadic and n-person contexts (Tooby et  al., on the evolution of coalitional violence have
2006). Additionally, language and symbolic been met in humans. However, the presence
reasoning in humans are such that we are able of these three conditions still does not tell us
to build cultural institutions that interact with much about the specific form that coalitional
motivated reasoning to either enhance or limit violence will take in any species, and neither
group- and individual-level dynamics (Boyer does it allow us to make many inferences
and Petersen, 2011). It should come as no sur- about downstream or related considerations,
prise, therefore, that political institutions often such as the possibility of cultural evolution
focus on the nature and expression of author- or the existence of sex differences in fighting.
ity (Smith et al., 2007) and the distribution of Rather, it gives us a universal cross-species
resources (Petersen, 2012) – two recurrent and framework for conceptualizing the necessary
important challenges faced by all groups. In phenotypic ‘starting point’ of coalitional vio-
short, we have evolved in groups, and we are lence. In order to provide a richer explana-
well-adapted for navigating the many recur- tion of the form and function of coalitional
rent and reproductively significant challenges violence observed in a given species, we
associated with this coalitional landscape. must not only ask whether the three condi-
Third, opportunities for low-cost/high- tions mentioned above existed ancestrally;
benefit coalitional violence must exist. There we must also uncover species-specific details
are at least three foundational components regarding demographic and socio-ecological
that determine the cost profile of coalitional variables that influence the form and function
violence in almost any setting. First is the of coalitional violence such as group size and
proximity of out-groups. The more geograph- composition, fission–fusion dynamics, mat-
ically distal groups are, the more quickly the ing structure, parental-investment dynamics,
costs of coalitional violence accumulate in and the nature and distribution of opportuni-
terms of effort and coordination. Second, tar- ties for violence between groups. It is to this
gets must reliably exist that are vulnerable to evidence that I now turn.
low-cost attack. Third, their injury or death
must entail, either directly or indirectly, some
fitness benefit to the attacker(s). In chimpan-
zees, for example, group structure and feed- EVIDENCE OF ANCESTRAL HUMAN
ing habits result in occasions of lone travelers COALITIONAL VIOLENCE
who can be ambushed and killed, and the
reproductive benefits of such an attack, when Given the above, the logical next steps are to
delivered with overwhelming force of num- trace the practice of coalitional violence in
bers, are substantial (e.g., see Wrangham, humans, i.e. when did it emerge, what did it
1999; Wilson et  al., 2014). Similarly, in look like, and how has it changed over time? I
humans, relatively frail bodies (i.e. in com- address the first two questions in this section,
parison to chimpanzees) combined with his- and the final question in the next section.
torical innovation in lethal weaponry and
conspiratorial ability have turned humans
into ‘quintessential first-strike creatures’ Emergence of Coalitional Violence
(Gat, 2006). Furthermore, it is not uncom-
mon for inter-group raids to entail significant Scholarship on the evolutionary origins of
reproductive benefits as well (Glowacki and warfare is bifurcated in two traditions that
Wrangham, 2015). In short, low-cost/high- Allen refers to as the long chronology and
benefit opportunities for human coalitional short chronology of warfare (Allen and
violence have long existed. Jones, 2014). The short chronology views
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND WARFARE 319

human warfare as having emerged by or Raids – sometimes called stealth raids –


shortly after the agricultural revolution at the ­display particular characteristics: occurring
start of the Holocene, and in response to new by stealth and/or at night; exploiting cover/
social and ecological pressures relating to concealment; targeting one or a few lone
sedentarism and the accumulation of wealth unsuspecting individuals by surprise/ambush;
(Otterbein, 1997; Kelly, 2000; Fry, 2007; Fry quick retreat; exploiting numerical asym-
and Söderberg, 2013). Prior to this period, metries (e.g., at least 3:1 force imbalance);
humans likely engaged in sporadic violent overwhelmingly initiated by males; over-
encounters, but most occurred within kin whelmingly targeting out-group males. Raids
groups rather than between them and rarely are designed to maximize effect relative to
involved anything more complex than the the expected risks, principally via numerical
targeted killing of single individuals. In con- imbalance and the element of surprise.
trast, the long chronology accepts the chim- Humans are indeed ‘first-strike creatures’;
panzee model of low-cost/high-benefit however, in the context of lethal weaponry
opportunistic coalitional killing as perhaps and symbolic language that facilitates coor-
the most basic form of warfare, upon which dinated revenge, human raiding is relatively
humans slowly and over time built greater riskier than chimpanzee raiding and may be
complexity. In other words, we have been ‘scaffolded’ to some extent by cultural insti-
engaging in coalitional violence at least since tutions that incentivize some forms of inter-
the split with chimpanzees (Wrangham, 1999; group or otherwise ‘heroic’ violence and
Gat, 2006; McDonald et  al., 2012; Lopez, sacrifice (Wrangham and Glowacki, 2012;
2016b; Glowacki et al., 2017). Both short and Glowacki and Wrangham, 2013).
long chronologies recognize that develop- Prior to the Holocene, direct evidence that
ments in the early Holocene brought signifi- humans engaged in anything other than coali-
cant changes to human inter-group violence, tional raiding is patchy at best. Despite occa-
but whereas the short chronology sees this as sional findings of mass graves from as early
the beginning of ‘warfare’ per se, the long as the end of the Pleistocene (Lahr et  al.,
chronology sees it as a period of significant 2016), there is neither direct nor indirect evi-
transition in the scale and complexity of war- dence that, for example, humans assembled
fare (Keeley, 1996; Lopez, 2019a). in more than a handful of individuals at a
This dispute is not so much regarding time for the purpose of relatively symmetric
what humans ‘were actually doing’ in the melee coalitional aggression. In other words,
Pleistocene; rather, the more contentious ‘battles’ – in contrast to raids – are relatively
issues hinge on whether various activities recent and rare in both the ethnographic and
should be labeled ‘warfare’. Therefore, the archaeological record, and with no analogue
question ‘when did warfare begin?’ is some- in chimpanzee behavior.
what indeterminate to the extent that it rests on
arbitrary definitional assumptions, and should
be engaged with methodological caution.
AN EVOLVED PSYCHOLOGY OF
COALITIONAL VIOLENCE?
Forms of Ancestral Coalitional
The universal conditions for the evolution of
Violence
coalitional violence are satisfied in humans,
Both chronologies of war recognize that raid- and we also know that ancestral humans
ing has been and remains the oldest and com- engaged in at least some forms of inter-group
monest form of human inter-group violence violence (e.g., raids and possibly battles).
(Keeley, 1996; Otterbein, 2004; Gat, 2006). Combined with incomplete but rich available
320 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

information regarding the ancestral motivated individuals and concludes quickly,


coalitional landscapes in which humans
­ minimizing what would be an otherwise chal-
lived, it is possible to identify several over- lenging problem of n-person tracking that is
lapping and interconnected classes of adap- difficult for even modern militaries to master
tive problems that existed, and these problems and sustain (Biddle, 2006).
seem to underlie all instances of coalitional At a basic level, the initiation of any act
violence to a variable degree. These are: the of coalitional violence requires a solution
collective action problem of coordinated vio- to the near-simultaneous problems of indi-
lence; parochial altruism; attacker–defender vidual commitment as well as inter-personal
asymmetries; leader–follower dynamics; and efforts to manipulate the behavior and com-
sex differences in the costs and benefits of mitment of within-group others (Tooby and
violence. Cosmides, 1988; Lopez, 2017, 2019b). These
are problems of participation and enforce-
ment: should I participate? (How) should I
The Collective Action Problem of manipulate the behavior of others toward (or
away from) participation? Again, although
Coordinated Violence
modern societies solve these problems with
Despite significant disagreement regarding coercive institutions, these problems were
the origins and frequency of warfare in our solved by our ancestors via the mechanisms
species’ history, most agree that at the heart of individual formidability and interest. This
of the puzzle of warfare lies a within-group helps to explain why the commonest form of
collective action problem (Kitchen and human coalitional violence is that of small,
Beehner, 2007; Crofoot and Gilby, 2012; unstable, fraternal coalitions that sporadi-
Pietraszewski, 2016; Glowacki et  al., 2017; cally assemble to take advantage of offensive
Lopez, 2019b; Wiessner, 2019). Large, com- opportunities.
plex, sedentary societies are often able to
solve these problems through the use of
coercive institutions, while relatively smaller Parochial Altruism
nomadic societies tend to solve these prob-
lems via the implicit or explicit distribution The conceptual core of parochial altruism is
of material rewards or the leveraging of per- simple, yet its simplicity betrays what has
sonal reputation and status. become a complex and contentious area of
Any agonistic contest entails risks to the study. In its simplest form, parochial altruism
participants such as injury or retaliation, and refers to a class of motivations and behavior
species that engage in such contests must in which one pays a cost to benefit at least
possess evolved systems that regulate an some members of one’s own group at the
individual’s participation in such contests, direct or indirect expense of at least some
such as whether to engage, escalate, sub- members of an out-group. For example, lab
mit, or run away (Parker, 1974; Sell, 2011; and field evidence over many decades sug-
Chapin et al., 2019). The problem is magni- gest that the mere presence of inter-group
fied among coalitions, where assessments of competition (violent or otherwise) is often
the likelihood of success are compounded by sufficient to trigger many forms of within-
assessments of the feasibility and strength of group cooperation, sacrifice, and altruism
within-group coordination and cooperation. (Puurtinen and Mappes, 2009; Gneezy and
It is probably no surprise again, that the most Fessler, 2011; Bauer et  al., 2016; Chang
common form of human coalitional violence et al., 2016; but see also Jordan et al., 2017).
is the raid; it is a form of low-cost violence In the extreme, this is taken as evidence that
that can be initiated with a small group of inter-group competition in our species’
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND WARFARE 321

evolutionary history is directly responsible travel through time and space. This initial
for our remarkable capacity for large-scale obligate cost necessarily establishes an asym-
cooperation, even among strangers. At the metrical structure to the cost–benefit profile
very least, it suggests that the two processes of attacking versus defending, as well as
(in-group favoritism and out-group deroga- alternative strategies (e.g., to run/disperse).
tion) may have co-evolved to some degree For example, although much emphasis is
(Choi and Bowles, 2007; Bowles, 2009). rightly given to the fact that attackers can
However, it has been demonstrated that exploit surprise and relative numbers if
the desire to harm an out-group is often moti- appropriate targets are chosen, attackers also
vated by individual-level incentives (e.g., per- endure significant costs of coordination,
sonal status, reputation, privatized material motivation, and target retaliation, in addition
resources), and it has also been demonstrated to the obligate costs mentioned above. In
that in-group favoritism does not necessar- contrast, defenders can exploit resident
ily also trigger out-group hate (Patton, 2005; advantages such as superior knowledge of
Gavrilets and Fortunato, 2014; Rusch, 2014; local terrain and may often find it easier to
Weisel and Böhm, 2015; Romano et  al., solve coordination problems. Mirville et al.,
2017; Doğan et  al., 2018). Therefore, the for example, find that ‘large groups may be
evidently tight linkage between inter-group unsuccessful in inter-group conflict if they
competition and within-group cooperation enter a smaller group’s home range, as the
that lies at the center of parochial altruism is smaller group may respond with more
conditional at best and likely engages a range aggression to defend the area’ (2018: 178). It
of individual-level and group-level incentives is no surprise, therefore, that De Dreu and
depending on the immediate context. For Gross (2018) find that, on average, across
example, individuals may be driven more by many contexts, defenders have a greater suc-
group-level benefits in domains of defense, cess rate than attackers, which must be at
while driven more by individual-level ben- least partly due to this initial obligate
efits in domains of offense (Rusch, 2013; asymmetry.
Lopez, 2017). An adaptationist perspective Buckner and Glowacki (2019), however,
on coalitional violence, therefore, provides usefully point out that attackers have the
us with a lens through which to examine advantage of ‘setting the stage of conflict’,
the interplay between strategic context (e.g., which stacks the odds of success in attackers’
defense/offense) and individual differences favor. It is clear that when attackers can assem-
pertaining to capability (i.e. formidability, ble for brief incursions to catch defenders off-
or resource-holding potential) or interest. balance and unaware, the result is a low-cost/
This leads to the next two major domains of high-benefit effort that can be strongly favored
research: attacker/defender asymmetries and by selection. As mentioned above, Gat (2006)
leadership. extends this analysis by arguing that, given the
relatively weak physique of humans combined
with the use of weaponry in coalitional vio-
lence, humans evolved to be ‘quintessential
Attacker–Defender Asymmetries
first-strike creatures’. To the extent this is true,
Coalitional violence can be defined as the then in the language of international relations
attempt by a group of individuals to physi- theory, ancestral humans may have evolved in
cally damage at least one individual of an ‘offense-dominant’ environment, in which
another group. Unless the individuals of the costs of defending were greater than the
opposed groups are sitting immediately costs of attacking (Jervis, 1978; Biddle, 2001).
beside each other, any attack upon an out- Despite the fact that this ‘offense–defense’
group must minimally endure the costs of balance has shifted numerous times since
322 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

the Pleistocene, Gat argues that the gamble 2020). Thus, although the pattern of lethal
of inter-group violence was probably stacked violence in hunter-gatherers is characterized
in favor of well-organized attackers – as by relatively leaderless raiding parties organ-
Buckner and Glowacki (2019) agree – which ized in the absence of overt coercive manipu-
is perhaps best characterized by the well- lation, this is better described as the absence
known adage that the best defense is a good of formal leadership, in which behavior is
offense. instead informally motivated by reputation
Attacker–defender asymmetries are prob- and reciprocity. In short, the ancestral char-
ably more obvious and relevant in raiding acterization of ‘leaderless’ societies under-
contexts or contexts of small-scale conflict states the importance of relative influence
than in larger battle contexts. Furthermore, and status as constraints and facilitators of
in raiding contexts, defenders face a public- collective action (Boehm, 1999, 2012).
good problem, in which the benefits of suc- Leaders can help resolve coordination
cess are general to the group, while attackers problems (e.g., Where to meet? When to
face a club-good problem, in which the ben- leave?) as well as resolve and enforce more
efits of success are more privatized among complicated problems pertaining to the dis-
the attackers themselves. These attacker– tribution of costs and benefits of aggression.
defender asymmetries help to clarify the set By definition, highly formidable individuals
of unique problems that must be resolved have both a greater capacity to punish and
in each domain in order for either attack or to reward, as well as a greater incentive for
defense to be successful (Lopez, 2017). There attack in many cases. Therefore, highly for-
are clearly group-level macro variables that midable individuals are also more likely to
impinge upon outcomes, especially relative resort to deception to recalibrate otherwise
numbers. However, the successful prosecu- unmotivated minds. For example, when a
tion of attack and defense invariably depends formidable individual identifies a window
on group members being able to effectively of opportunity for attack, yet willing others
manage and navigate a constellation of inter- are immediately unavailable, the initiator can
individual dynamics (Tooby and Cosmides, resort to the direct manipulation of costs and
1988; Tooby et  al., 2006; Pietraszewski, benefits, or resort to an indirect manipulation
2016; Wiessner, 2019). Individuals must of the strategic context. Direct manipulation
solve a range of domain-specific collective of cost and benefit is clear: leaders are often
action problems specific to warfare, the solu- those who, by definition, are most able to
tions to which are conditional upon, inter alia, distribute such costs and benefits. However,
whether individuals are initiating or respond- initiators may also manipulate others’ beliefs
ing to an attack. about the nature of aggression, recasting
opportunistic aggression as defensive or
preemptive necessity (Lopez, 2019b). It has
been well-established that individuals are
Leader–Follower Dynamics
often more willing to fight for defense than
Leaders – whether formal or informal – are for offense (Böhm et al., 2016); if this asym-
not intrinsically necessary for solving collec- metry extended to ancestral environments,
tive action problems; however, their presence the result is a motivated bias among strongly
has been demonstrated to facilitate success in incentivized initiators (i.e. leaders; highly
inter-group contexts. Although ancestral formidable individuals) to falsely cast oppor-
human societies are believed to be relatively tunism as necessity.
egalitarian, these groups are also marked by Coalitional violence is not just a story of
asymmetries of influence and status that manipulation by the powerful; in fact, lead-
shape motivation and behavior (von Rueden, ers can provide a valuable public good in
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND WARFARE 323

the prosecution of inter-group competition find that female support for terrorism is not
(van Vugt and Kurzban, 2007; Hooper et al., only on the rise in several Arab countries,
2010; Glowacki and von Rueden, 2015). but that in many instances it equals male
Thus, research has found that individuals support for terrorism in these contexts. In
seem to prefer dominant or masculine lead- other words, violence, as the adaptive out-
ers in times of inter-group violence, precisely put of evolved psychological mechanisms,
because there is an implicit understanding is irrevocably conditional, and the structure
that these individuals, in these contexts, are of that conditionality depends in part on sex.
able to provide a unique service. This has led While male participation in coalitional vio-
to a growing field of research demonstrat- lence is likely highly personalized, physical,
ing that facial and vocal cues are implicitly and motived to seek opportunistic gains at
used by individuals to assess leadership in low cost, female participation is likely indi-
specific contexts (Anderson and Klofstad, rect, greater in defensive contexts, and when
2012; Klofstad et  al., 2012; Spisak, 2012; immediate resources are at stake (resources,
Spisak et  al., 2012; Laustsen and Petersen, territory, children) rather than when surro-
2017). In short, despite a relatively egalitar- gate resources are at stake, such as status and
ian group structure, individual variance in honor, although these remain somewhat open
both ­formidability and interest allowed our questions (Glowacki and Wrangham, 2013;
ancestors to facilitate coalitional violence Scalise Sugiyama, 2014; Lopez, 2017; Lynch
when necessary or beneficial. et al., 2019).
Greater male physical aggressiveness in
humans is directly predicted by sexual selec-
Sex Differences in the Costs and tion and parental investment theory, in which
sex differences in physical aggression follow
Benefits of Coalitional Violence
asymmetries in parental investment (Trivers,
The overall pattern of human violence at 1972; Daly and Wilson, 1983). Furthermore,
both the individual and group levels is that of these theoretical foundations have been well
male physical violence (van Vugt, 2009; elaborated by research on several established
McDonald et  al., 2012). Cross-culturally, mechanisms of sexual selection, such as con-
males are significantly more likely to be both test competition, in which same-sex com-
the perpetrators and targets of physical vio- petitors are excluded from mating through
lence (Daly and Wilson, 1988). Even in force, and mate choice, in which the posses-
modern contexts, for example, in which it sion of traits (e.g., intelligence, formidability,
has been observed that greater numbers of wealth) enhances one’s attractiveness toward
women are engaged in acts of suicide terror- members of the opposite sex (Puts, 2016;
ism, the overall numbers are skewed toward Hill et al., 2017).
overwhelmingly male involvement
(Goldstein, 2003).
Participation in physical violence should
be distinguished from support for violence. BEYOND THE MILITARY HORIZON:
Evidence of greater male aggressiveness is THE EMERGENCE OF WARFARE
strongest on indicators of physical acts of
violence, and weaker on indicators of indi- Humans possess the three universal criteria
rect or informal support, particularly in for the evolution of coalitional violence, and
defensive contexts, in which all benefit from a cursory review of the human pattern of
victory due to its public-good nature (Ginges ancestral violence, alongside a sketch of the
and Atran, 2011; Lopez, 2017). Thus, it is adaptive problems this has generated (e.g.,
little surprise that Fair and Shepherd (2006) collective action problems, attacker–defender
324 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

asymmetries) suggests that humans likely after the emergence of agriculture, while the
possess specialized psychological design for short-chronology perspective argues that this
navigating the challenges of coalitional vio- transition is in fact an origin. Aside from the
lence. What this means is that we possess the relatively arbitrary issue of what difference
psychological tools for reasoning about and there is between an origin and a transition, a
engaging in small-scale coalitional violence. more productive question is: what ecological
However, what we observe today arguably and social shifts were occurring at this time,
bears little resemblance to the behaviors our and how did this affect the nature of inter-
ancestors engaged in. The real question often group conflict?
reduces to: are the differences between The first thing to note about the emergence
ancestral and modern ‘warfare’ differences of agriculture and its effects on inter-group
in degree or kind? violence is that it does not occur everywhere
These questions probably cannot be at the same time, and therefore neither does
answered in a general sense. In some arenas, ‘warfare’. Otterbein, for example, while sit-
notably lethal weaponry, modern warfare is a ting somewhere between the long- and short-
faint shadow of its ancestral forms. However, chronology perspectives, argues that an older
in terms of certain principles of engagement, form of human warfare predates agriculture,
such as cover/concealment, the element of and emerges as a by-product of hunting.
surprise, force-imbalance ratios, rally ‘round As big game became scarce, so too did the
the flag effects, attacker–defender asym- practice of warfare, which then disappeared
metries, and many others, modern warfare until social innovations occurring after the
often still follows a fundamentally ancestral transition to agriculture made the practice of
logic, even if it is now manifested at much warfare profitable again. For Otterbein, war-
greater scales (Keeley, 1996; Lopez, 2016b, fare could not have occurred simultaneously
2019a). Guerrilla warfare, insurgency, and with the emergence of agriculture, since: ‘For
hybrid warfare follow kinetic principles of agriculture and permanent settlements to
violence that would likely be intuitive to any arise, there must be no warfare’ (Otterbein,
hunter-gatherer (Boot, 2013). 2004: 13). Innovations in agriculture require
This suggests that the real question peace, and somewhat ironically, the fruits
regarding the emergence of human warfare of agriculture make war again possible and
from smaller-scale versions is probably not profitable. In direct contrast, however, Ferrill
evolutionary so much as historical. In other argues instead that it was warfare that drove
words, once the psychological tools of the need for agriculture. Innovations in
coalitional violence were in place, significant weaponry that occurred around 10,000 BC,
changes in demographics and ecology such as bows and slings, increased the need
would have been sufficient to trigger both for defenses, such as fortified walls, and the
qualitative and quantitative innovations in resultant decline in mobility meant there was
the scale and method of coalitional violence, pressure to find new ways to live off the land
such that at some ambiguous and relatively (Ferrill, 1997).
arbitrary point in history, what we once This obvious chicken-or-egg problem
recognized as coalitional violence looks remains unresolved, but what is clear is that
more and more like modern warfare. How where agriculture emerged, it tended to have
did this happen? clear impacts upon the political economy of
The answer to this question forces at least human groups, which included the possibility
a brief return to an earlier question, that of of, and in some cases a perceived imperative
the origins of warfare. Recall that the long- for, what might be labeled ‘modern war-
chronology perspective argues that warfare fare’. Relatively sedentary groups practicing
is evolutionarily old and merely ‘transitions’ agriculture were larger, accumulated more
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND WARFARE 325

wealth, were more effective at command, cooperation and the steady intensification
control, and logistics, and were more efficient of warfare is brought to a head in the cur-
innovators. Gabriel, similar to Otterbein, rent era, in which great powers are restrained
argues that although this transition is under- in their conflicts with each other, and new
way after the transition to agriculture, inter- forms of violence have largely taken the
group violence does not begin to look like place of inter-state violence. In this sense, we
‘modern warfare’ until about 4,000–2,000 are witnessing a reemergence of small-scale
BC, by which time population centers, social violence – a renaissance of the raid. This fea-
and economic organization, lethal weaponry, ture is now characteristic of many forms of
and institutional command coalesce to pro- modern political violence. When great pow-
duce a form of warfare that is often associ- ers do engage in violence, it often takes the
ated with nation states – those that exhibit form of so-called ‘limited wars’, in which
steep vertical command structures, are char- quick incursions are meant to target key
acterized by sizeable armies that exploit col- individuals, groups, or facilities in order to
umn and line formations, and are capable of degrade or deter an adversary, often a non-
greater levels of destruction (Gabriel, 2002). state actor (Osgood, 1957; Stoker, 2016).
In other words, it is around this time that the When nation states are the targets of vio-
raid, as the predominant or even exclusive lence, the source may be a terrorist network
form of inter-group violence, now shares the or a nation state engaging in ‘hybrid war-
stage with (or in many cases is upstaged by) fare’ in which states bring to bear a motley
conventional battle between formal political range of tools, such as propaganda, cyber
organizations. instruments, and targeted assassinations,
As Levy and Thompson describe, this in order to effect outcomes short of war
transition begins in Mesopotamia and Egypt, (Hoffman, 2007). Lastly, the most common
followed later by transitions in Greece, Rome, form of political violence is now civil war
and China, during which: ‘Weapons became and insurgency (Lopez and Johnson, 2017),
much more lethal, military organizations which, again, often bears the ancestral hall-
became larger and more complex, and marks of many raiding dynamics (Boot,
states became more powerful’ (Levy and 2013). In short, the raid, having fallen from
Thompson, 2011: 122)1. In addition to these prominence over two thousand years ago, is
first two transitions, Levy and Thompson now returning to center stage in our species’
identify a third transition occurring within long evolution of warfare.
the last five centuries. Two trends that stand
out during this period include a decreasing
frequency of great power warfare alongside
a pernicious increase in the lethality of UNIVERSALITY, UNIQUENESS, AND
warfare (Gat, 2006; Levy and Thompson, CHANGE: WAR AS A WINDOW INTO
2011; Pinker, 2011). The particularities HUMAN NATURE
of this transition and the innovations and
security imperatives that in part drove it Perhaps it is the author’s conceit, but there is
are outside the scope of this inquiry; it is hardly any domain outside of warfare that
sufficient to note that this long history of the singularly unites so much about what makes
ever-unfolding nature of warfare is pithily us human – from our ugliest desires to our
summed up with Tilly’s famous assertion noblest sacrifices. The study of warfare is
that ‘war made the state, and the state made necessarily a tour through many if not all of
war’ (Tilly, 1975: 42). the big questions of human nature and the
This ironic, even if tragic, connec- nature of human psychology. Many of these
tion between expanding zones of political questions are not investigated here, and some
326 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

that are remain nevertheless unresolved. REFERENCES


However, one lesson that does emerge from
this analysis is that humans are social by Allen, Mark W., and Terry L. Jones, Eds. 2014.
design, and at least part of this design facili- Violence and Warfare among Hunter-­
tates aggression between groups. Gatherers. Walnut Creek, California: Routledge.
Evolution is a process of change, and Anderson, Rindy C., and Casey A. Klofstad.
natural selection is a force that explains one 2012. ‘Preference for Leaders with Masculine
kind of change: directional change in gene Voices Holds in the Case of Feminine Leadership
Roles’. PLOS ONE 7 (12): e51216. https://doi.
frequencies that can build complex adapta-
org/10.1371/journal.pone.0051216.
tions over time. The human mind is proposed
Bauer, Michal, Christopher Blattman, Julie
to consist, in part, of complex adaptations Chytilová, Joseph Henrich, Edward Miguel,
designed to facilitate the navigation of the and Tamar Mitts. 2016. ‘Can War Foster
natural world (which includes the social Cooperation?’ Journal of Economic
world) in a way that would have maximized Perspectives 30 (3): 249–74. https://doi.
reproductive success in ancestral environ- org/10.1257/jep.30.3.249.
ments. In other words, these complex adap- Biddle, Stephen. 2001. ‘Rebuilding the
tations are relatively slow to change, which Foundations of Offense-Defense Theory’.
means that many of the evolved systems that Journal of Politics 63 (3): 741–4. https://doi.
emerged to solve ancestral problems remain org/10.1111/0022-3816.00086.
———. 2006. Military Power: Explaining Victory
active today, ‘trying’ to make sense of a mod-
and Defeat in Modern Battle. Princeton, New
ern world that is a shadow of its ancestral self.
Jersey: Princeton University Press.
However, as Ridley (2003: 7) notes, ‘differ- Bird, Douglas W., Rebecca Bliege Bird, Brian F.
ence is the shadow of similarity’. Thus, some Codding, and David W. Zeanah. 2019.
of the most promising avenues for current ‘Variability in the Organization and Size of
research on the evolutionary psychology of Hunter-Gatherer Groups: Foragers Do Not
war deal with puzzles such as reconciling the Live in Small-Scale Societies’. Journal of
universal presence of the psychological tools Human Evolution 131 (June): 96–108. https://
of violence with variation in their expression doi.org/10.1016/j.jhevol.2019.03.005.
across space and time, explaining why the Boehm, Christopher. 1999. Hierarchy in the
preponderance of coalitional violence is still Forest: The Evolution of Egalitarian Behavior.
Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard
carried out by men, explaining why humans
University Press.
can sometimes be led to levels of violent
———. 2012. ‘Ancestral Hierarchy and
self-sacrifice that appear to contradict the Conflict’. Science 336 (6083): 844–7. https://
possibility of any fitness benefits, and cor- doi.org/10.1126/science.1219961.
recting past blind spots in research that has Böhm, Robert, Hannes Rusch, and Özgür Gürerk.
focused on Western subjects and data. All of 2016. ‘What Makes People Go to War?
these puzzles are tractable and indeed benefit Defensive Intentions Motivate Retaliatory and
from and require an evolutionary lens as we Preemptive Intergroup Aggression’. Evolution
untangle the sordid knot of human coalitional and Human Behavior 37 (1): 29–34. https://doi.
violence and warfare. org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2015.06.005.
Boot, Max. 2013. ‘The Evolution of Irregular
War’. Foreign Affairs, March 1, 2013. www.
foreignaffairs.com/articles/2013-02-05/
Note evolution-irregular-war
1  This is an admittedly brief summary of a master- Bowles, Samuel. 2009. ‘Did Warfare Among
ful and complex summary provided by Levy and Ancestral Hunter-Gatherers Affect the
Thompson. Interested readers are encouraged to Evolution of Human Social Behaviors?’
engage the reading directly, while for my pur- Science 324 (5932): 1293–8. https://doi.
poses here, a bird’s-eye view is sufficient. org/10.1126/science.1168112.
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND WARFARE 327

Boyer, Pascal, and Michael Bang Petersen. Neurobiological, Psychological, and Cultural
2011. ‘The Naturalness of (Many) Social Mechanisms for Attack and Defense within
Institutions: Evolved Cognition as Their and between Groups’. The Behavioral and
Foundation’. Journal of Institutional Brain Sciences 42: e116. https://doi.org/
Economics FirstView: 1–25. 10.1017/S0140525X18002170.
Buckner, William, and Luke Glowacki. 2019. Doğan, Gönül, Luke Glowacki, and Hannes
‘Reasons to Strike First’. Behavioral and Brain Rusch. 2018. ‘Spoils Division Rules Shape
Sciences 42, 17–18. https://doi.org/10.1017/ Aggression between Natural Groups’. Nature
s0140525x19000840. Human Behaviour 2 (5): 322–6. https://doi.
Chang, Linda W., Amy R. Krosch, and Mina org/10.1038/s41562-018-0338-z.
Cikara. 2016. ‘Effects of Intergroup Threat Enquist, Magnus, and Olof Leimar. 1990. ‘The
on Mind, Brain, and Behavior’. Current Evolution of Fatal Fighting’. Animal Behaviour
Opinion in Psychology, Intergroup relations, 39 (1): 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-
11 (October): 69–73. https://doi.org/10.1016/ 3472(05)80721-3.
j.copsyc.2016.06.004. Fair, C. Christine, and Bryan Shepherd. 2006.
Chapin, Kenneth James, Paulo Enrique Cardoso ‘Who Supports Terrorism? Evidence from
Peixoto, and Mark Briffa. 2019. ‘Further Fourteen Muslim Countries’. Studies in
Mismeasures of Animal Contests: A New Conflict & Terrorism 29 (1): 51–74. https://
Framework for Assessment Strategies’. doi.org/10.1080/10576100500351318.
Behavioral Ecology 30 (5): 1177–85. https:// Ferrill, Arther. 1997. The Origins Of War: From
doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arz081. The Stone Age To Alexander The Great,
Chapman, Colin, A. 1986. ‘Ecological Revised Edition. 1st edition. Boulder,
Constraints on Group-Size for 3 Species of Colorado: Westview Press.
Neotropical Primates’. American Journal of Fry, Douglas P. 2007. Beyond War: The Human
Primatology 10 (4): 394. Potential for Peace. 1st edition. New York:
Chapman, Colin A., and Lauren J. Chapman. NY: Oxford University Press.
2000. ‘Constraints on Group Size in Red Fry, Douglas P., and Patrik Söderberg. 2013.
Colobus and Red-Tailed Guenons: Examining ‘Lethal Aggression in Mobile Forager Bands
the Generality of the Ecological Constraints and Implications for the Origins of War’.
Model’. International Journal of Primatology Science 341 (6143): 270–3. https://doi.
21 (4): 565–85. org/10.1126/science.1235675.
Chapman, C. A., R. W. Wrangham, and L. J. Gabriel, Richard A. 2002. The Great Armies of
Chapman. 1995. ‘Ecological Constraints on Antiquity: Westport, Connecticut: Praeger.
Group-Size – an Analysis of Spider Monkey Gat, Azar. 2006. War in Human Civlization.
and Chimpanzee Subgroups’. Behavioral Oxford: Oxford University Press. www.loc.
Ecology and Sociobiology 36 (1): 59–70. gov/catdir/toc/ecip0614/2006017223.html
Choi, Jung-Kyoo, and Samuel Bowles. 2007. Gavrilets, Sergey, and Laura Fortunato. 2014.
‘The Coevolution of Parochial Altruism and ‘A Solution to the Collective Action Problem
War’. Science 318 (5850): 636–40. https:// in Between-Group Conflict with Within-
doi.org/10.1126/science.1144237. Group Inequality’. Nature Communications 5
Crofoot, Margaret Chatham, and Ian C. Gilby. (March): 3526. https://doi.org/10.1038/
2012. ‘Cheating Monkeys Undermine Group ncomms4526.
Strength in Enemy Territory’. Proceedings of the Ginges, Jeremy, and Scott Atran. 2011. ‘War as
National Academy of Sciences 109 (2): 501–5. a Moral Imperative (Not Just Practical Politics
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1115937109. by Other Means)’. Proceedings of the Royal
Daly, Martin, and Margo Wilson. 1983. Sex, Society B: Biological Sciences 278 (1720):
Evolution, and Behavior. Boston, 2930–8. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.
Massachusetts: Willard Grant Press. 2010.2384.
———. 1988. Homicide. New York: A. de Glowacki, Luke, and Christopher von Rueden.
Gruyter. 2015. ‘Leadership Solves Collective Action
De Dreu, Carsten K. W., and Jörg Gross. 2018. Problems in Small-Scale Societies’.
‘Revisiting the Form and Function of Conflict: Philosophical Transactions of the Royal
328 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Society B: Biological Sciences 370 (1683). Jordan, Matthew R., Jillian J. Jordan, and David G.
https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0010. Rand. 2017. ‘No Unique Effect of Intergroup
Glowacki, Luke, Michael L. Wilson, and Richard Competition on Cooperation: Non-Competitive
W. Wrangham. 2017. ‘The Evolutionary Thresholds Are as Effective as Competitions
Anthropology of War’. Journal of Economic between Groups for Increasing Human Coop-
Behavior and Organization, January. https:// erative Behavior’. Evolution and Human Behav-
doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2017.09.014. ior 38 (1): 102–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/
Glowacki, Luke, and Richard W. Wrangham. j.evolhumbehav.2016.07.005.
2013. ‘The Role of Rewards in Motivating Keeley, Lawrence H. 1996. War before Civiliza-
Participation in Simple Warfare’. Human tion: The Myth of the Peaceful Savage. New
Nature 24 (4): 444–60. https://doi.org/ York: Oxford University Press. www.loc.gov/
10.1007/s12110-013-9178-8. catdir/enhancements/fy0638/94008998-d.html
Glowacki, Luke, and Richard Wrangham. 2015. Kelly, Raymond C. 2000. Warless Societies and
‘Warfare and Reproductive Success in a Tribal the Origin of War. Ann Arbor, Michigan:
Population’. Proceedings of the National University of Michigan Press.
Academy of Sciences 112 (2): 348–53. Kitchen, Dawn M., and Jacinta C. Beehner. 2007.
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1412287112. ‘Factors Affecting Individual Participation in
Gneezy, Ayelet, and Daniel M. T. Fessler. 2011. Group-Level Aggression among Non-Human
‘Conflict, Sticks and Carrots: War Increases Primates’. Behaviour 144 (12): 1551–81.
Prosocial Punishments and Rewards’. Klofstad, Casey A., Rindy C. Anderson, and
Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Susan Peters. 2012. ‘Sounds like a Winner:
Sciences, 279, 219–223. June. https://doi. Voice Pitch Influences Perception of Leadership
org/10.1098/rspb.2011.0805. Capacity in Both Men and Women’. Proceed-
Goldstein, Joshua S. 2003. War and Gender: ings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological
How Gender Shapes the War System and Vice Sciences 279 (1738): 2698–2704. https://doi.
Versa. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. org/10.1098/rspb.2012.0311.
Hill, Alex, Drew H. Bailey, and David A. Puts. Lahr, Marta. Mirazón., Rivera, Frances., Power,
2017. ‘Gorillas in Our Midst? Human Sexual R. K., Mounier, Aurelien., Copsey, B.,
Dimorphism and Contest Competition in Crivellaro, F., Edung, J.E., Maillo Fernandez,
Men’. In Michel Tibayrenc and Francisco J. J.M.,… Foley RA. 2016. ‘Inter-Group Violence
Ayala (Eds) On Human Nature: Biology, among Early Holocene Hunter-Gatherers of
Psychology, Ethics, Politics, and Religion, West Turkana, Kenya’. Nature 529 (7586):
235–49. Academic Press. 394–8. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature16477.
Hill, Kim R., Robert S. Walker, Miran Božičević, Laustsen, Lasse, and Michael Bang Petersen.
James Eder, Thomas Headland, Barry 2017. ‘Perceived Conflict and Leader
Hewlett, A. Magdalena Hurtado, Frank Dominance: Individual and Contextual Factors
Marlowe, Polly Wiessner, and Brian Wood. Behind Preferences for Dominant Leaders’.
2011. ‘Co-Residence Patterns in Hunter- Political Psychology 38 (6): 1083–1101.
Gatherer Societies Show Unique Human https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12403.
Social Structure’. Science 331 (6022): 1286–9. Levy, Jack S., and William R. Thompson. 2011.
https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1199071. The Arc of War: Origins, Escalation, and
Hoffman, Frank. 2007. Conflict in the 21st Transformation. Chicago, Illinois: University
Century: The Rise of Hybrid Wars. Arlington, Of Chicago Press.
Virginia: Potomac Institute for Policy Studies. Lopez, Anthony C. 2016a. ‘Conditions Required
Hooper, Paul L., Hillard S. Kaplan, and James L. for Evolution of Warfare Adaptations’. In
Boone. 2010. ‘A Theory of Leadership in Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological
Human Cooperative Groups’. Journal of Science, edited by Viviana Weekes-
Theoretical Biology 265 (4): 633–46. https:// Shackelford, Todd K. Shackelford, and
doi.org/10.1016/j.jtbi.2010.05.034. Viviana A. Weekes-Shackelford, 1–10.
Jervis, Robert. 1978. ‘Cooperation under the Cham: Springer International Publishing.
Security Dilemma’. World Politics 30 (2): https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-16999-
167–214. https://doi.org/10.2307/2009958. 6_914-1.
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND WARFARE 329

———. 2016b. ‘The Evolution of War: Theory Otterbein, Keith F. 1997. ‘The Origins of War’.
and Controversy’. International Theory 8 (1): Critical Review 11 (2): 251–77.
97–139. https://doi.org/10.1017/S17529719 ———. 2004. How War Began. 1st edition.
15000184. Texas, US: TAMU Press.
———. 2017. ‘The Evolutionary Psychology of Parker, Geoff. 1974. ‘Assessment Strategy and
War: Offense and Defense in the Adapted the Evolution of Fighting Behaviour’. Journal
Mind’. Evolutionary Psychology 15 (4): of Theoretical Biology 47 (1): 223–43.
1474704917742720. https://doi.org/10.1177/ Patton, John Q. 2005. ‘Meat Sharing for
1474704917742720. Coalitional Support’. Evolution and Human
———. 2019a. ‘The Evolution of War’. In Carol Behavior 26 (2): 137–57. https://doi.org/
Ann Ireland, Jane Louise Ireland, Michael 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2004.08.008.
Lewis, and Anthony C. Lopez (Eds) Petersen, Michael Bang. 2012. ‘Social Welfare as
International Handbook of Collective Violence, Small-Scale Help: Evolutionary Psychology and
edited by Carol Ann Ireland, Jane Louise the Deservingness Heuristic’. American Journal
Ireland, Michael Lewis, and Anthony C. Lopez. of Political Science 56 (1): 1–16. https://doi.
3–16. New York, NY: Routledge Press. org/10.1111/j.1540-5907.2011.00545.x.
———. 2019b. ‘Making “My” Problem “Our” Pietraszewski, David. 2016. ‘How the Mind Sees
Problem: Warfare as Collective Action, and the Coalitional and Group Conflict: The
Role of Leader Manipulation’. The Leadership Evolutionary Invariances of n-Person Conflict
Quarterly, 31(2) May. https://doi.org/10.1016/ Dynamics’. Evolution and Human Behavior 37
j.leaqua.2019.05.001. (6).470–80. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Lopez, Anthony C., and Dominic D. P. Johnson. evolhumbehav.2016.04.006.
2017. ‘The Determinants of War in Pinker, Steven. 2011. The Better Angels of Our
International Relations’. Journal of Economic Nature: Why Violence Has Declined.
Behavior & Organization, October. https://doi. Johannesburg, SA: Viking Adult.
org/10.1016/j.jebo.2017.09.010. Puts, David. 2016. ‘Human Sexual Selection’.
Lynch, Robert, Virpi Lummaa, and John Loehr. Current Opinion in Psychology 7 (February):
2019. ‘Self Sacrifice and Kin Psychology in 28–32. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.
War: Threats to Family Predict Decisions to 2015.07.011.
Volunteer for a Women’s Paramilitary Organi- Puurtinen, Mikael, and Tapio Mappes. 2009.
zation’. Evolution and Human Behavior. 40 (6) ‘Between-Group Competition and Human
543–50. June. https://doi.org/10.1016/ Cooperation’. Proceedings of the Royal Society
j.evolhumbehav.2019.06.001. B: Biological Sciences 276 (1655): 355–60.
McDonald, Melissa M., Carlos David Navarrete, https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2008.1060.
and Mark Van Vugt. 2012. ‘Evolution and the Ridley, Matt. 2003. The Agile Gene: How
Psychology of Intergroup Conflict: The Male Nature Turns on Nurture. Reprint edition.
Warrior Hypothesis’. Philosophical Transactions New York: Harper Perennial.
of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 367 Romano, Angelo, Daniel Balliet, Toshio
(1589): 670–9. https://doi.org/10.1098/ Yamagishi, and James H. Liu. 2017. ‘Parochial
rstb.2011.0301. Trust and Cooperation across 17 Societies’.
Mirville, Melanie O., Amanda R. Ridley, Jean Pierre Proceedings of the National Academy of
Mucyo Samedi. Samedi, Veronica Vecellio, Felix Sciences 114 (48): 12702–7. https://doi.
Ndagijimana, Tara S. Stoinski, and Cyril C. Gru- org/10.1073/pnas.1712921114.
eter. 2018. ‘Low Familiarity and Similar “Group Rueden, Chris von. 2020. ‘Making and
Strength” between Opponents Increase the Unmaking Egalitarianism in Small-Scale
Intensity of Intergroup Interactions in Mountain Human Societies’. Current Opinion in
Gorillas (Gorilla Beringei Beringei)’. Behavioral Psychology, Power, Status and Hierarchy, 33
Ecology and Sociobiology 72 (11): 178. https:// (June): 167–71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
doi.org/10.1007/s00265-018-2592-5. copsyc.2019.07.037.
Osgood, Robert E. 1957. Limited War: The Rusch, Hannes. 2013. ‘Asymmetries in Altruistic
Challenge to American Strategy. 5th edition. Behavior during Violent Intergroup Conflict’.
Chicago, Illinois: University of Chicago Press. Evolutionary Psychology: An International
330 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Journal of Evolutionary Approaches to Organizational Behavior’. Managerial and


Psychology and Behavior 11 (5): 973–93. Decision Economics 27 (2–3): 103–29.
———. 2014. ‘The Evolutionary Interplay of Trivers, Robert. 1972. ‘Parental Investment and
Intergroup Conflict and Altruism in Humans: Sexual Selection’. In Bernard Campbell (Eds)
A Review of Parochial Altruism Theory and Sexual Selection and the Descent of Man.
Prospects for Its Extension’. Proceedings. Chicago, IL: Aldine Publishing.
Biological Sciences 281 (1794): 20141539. van Vugt, Mark. 2009. ‘Sex Differences in
https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2014.1539. Intergroup Competition, Aggression, and
Scalise Sugiyama, Michelle. 2014. ‘Fitness Costs Warfare: The Male Warrior Hypothesis’.
of Warfare for Women’. Human Nature 25 Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences
(4): 476–95. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110- 1167 (June): 124–34. https://doi.org/10.1111/
014-9216-1. j.1749-6632.2009.04539.x.
Sell, Aaron N. 2011. ‘The Recalibrational Theory van Vugt, Mark, and Rob Kurzban. 2007. ‘Cogni-
and Violent Anger’. Aggression and Violent tive and Social Adaptations for Leadership and
Behavior 16 (5): 381–9. https://doi.org/ Followership: Evolutionary Game Theory and
10.1016/j.avb.2011.04.013. Group Dynamics’.” In Joseph P. Forgas, Martie
Smith, Kevin B., Christopher W. Larimer, G. Haselton, and William von Hippel (Eds)
Levente Littvay, and John R. Hibbing. 2007. Evolution and the Social Mind: Evolutionary
‘Evolutionary Theory and Political Psychology and Social Cognition, 229–43.
Leadership: Why Certain People Do Not New York: Psychology Press.
Trust Decision Makers’. The Journal of Weisel, Ori, and Robert Böhm. 2015. ‘“Ingroup
Politics 69 (2): 285–99. https://doi. Love” and “Outgroup Hate” in Intergroup
org/10.1111/j.1468-2508.2007.00532.x. Conflict between Natural Groups’. Journal of
Spisak, Brian R. 2012. ‘The General Age of Experimental Social Psychology 60
Leadership: Older-Looking Presidential (September): 110–20. https://doi.org/
Candidates Win Elections during War’. PLoS 10.1016/j.jesp.2015.04.008.
ONE 7 (5): e36945. https://doi.org/10.1371/ Wiessner, Polly. 2019. ‘Collective Action for
journal.pone.0036945. War and for Peace: A Case Study among the
Spisak, Brian R., Astrid C. Homan, Allen Grabo, Enga of Papua New Guinea’. Current
and Mark Van Vugt. 2012. ‘Facing the Anthropology 60 (2): 224–44. https://doi.
Situation: Testing a Biosocial Contingency org/10.1086/702414.
Model of Leadership in Intergroup Relations Wilson, Michael L., Christophe Boesch, Barbara
Using Masculine and Feminine Faces’. The Fruth, Takeshi Furuichi, Ian C. Gilby, Chie
Leadership Quarterly 23 (2): 273–80. Hashimoto, Catherine L. Hobaiter, et al. 2014.
Stoker, Donald. 2016. ‘Everything You Think ‘Lethal Aggression in Pan Is Better Explained
You Know About Limited War Is Wrong’. by Adaptive Strategies than Human Impacts’.
War on the Rocks, December 22, 2016. Nature 513 (7518): 414–17. https://doi.
h t t p s : / / w a ro n t h e ro c k s . c o m / 2 0 1 6 / 1 2 / org/10.1038/nature13727.
everything-you-think-you-know-about- Wrangham, Richard. 1999. ‘Evolution of
limited-war-is-wrong/. Coalitionary Killing’. Yearbook of Physical
Tilly, Charles. 1975. The Formation of National Anthropology 42: 1–30.
States in Western Europe. Princeton: Princeton ———. 2019. The Goodness Paradox: The
University Press. Strange Relationship Between Virtue and
Tooby, John, and Leda Cosmides. 1988. ‘The Violence in Human Evolution. 1st edition.
Evolution of War and Its Cognitive New York: Pantheon.
Foundations’. Institute for Evolutionary Wrangham, Richard W., and Luke Glowacki.
Studies Technical Report 88–1. https://www. 2012. ‘Intergroup Aggression in Chimpanzees
cep.ucsb.edu/papers/EvolutionofWar.pdf and War in Nomadic Hunter-Gatherers:
Tooby, John, Leda Cosmides, and Michael E. Evaluating the Chimpanzee Model’. Human
Price. 2006. ‘Cognitive Adaptations for Nature. 23 (1): 5–29 March. https://doi.org/
N-Person Exchange: The Evolutionary Roots of 10.1007/s12110-012-9132-1.
PART 3

Applications to Technology,
Communications, and the Future
This page intentionally left blank
17
Evolutionary Psychology and
Artificial Intelligence: The Impact
of Artificial Intelligence on Human
Behaviour
Holly Wilson, Paul Rauwolf, and Joanna J. Bryson

1 INTRODUCTION the term, at least in the scope of this chapter.


Intelligence is the capacity to do the right
Artificial intelligence (AI) impacts our thing at the right time; thus artificial intel-
behaviour. Intelligence, whilst not in itself ligence refers to non-living artefacts that
defining humanity, is one of our key charac- demonstrate such capacities (Bryson, 2019).
teristics, inextricably linked to everything By this definition, AI has been prevalent for
from our implicit survival strategies to our decades, albeit less advanced than at present,
explicit self-concepts. Intelligence is also and not so apparent to the public eye. Present
integral to the social institutions on which awareness of AI has been magnified by two
contemporary existence depends. AI is a set different recent outcomes: first the sudden
of technologies that extend human intellec- prevalence of anthropomorphic capacities
tual capacities such as perception, action, such as ­conversational-speech recognition and
categorisation and pattern recognition. Some generation, or automobile driving; and second
systems, termed autonomous, may do all of the use of AI technology as part of a global
these things at once without human interven- assault on democracies, and through them key
tion, though only after human or human- institutions to maintaining peace under the
institutional inception. present global order, such as the EU and the
Determining the impact of AI is impera- North Atlantic Treaty Organization (NATO).1
tive for two reasons: for empowering us to We focus here on how behaviour is shaped
adapt to and optimise our existence with AI; by contemporary intelligent technology; first
and for developing AI and AI policies which by our cultural understanding of AI, then
maximise benefit and minimise harm to our by the technological reality of AI. Many of
society. Due to the myriad definitions of AI, our drives and behaviours, such as tribalism,
we begin by establishing what we mean by sex, and resource procurement, are sculpted
334 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

by thousands of years of evolution in dispa- the barriers to AI increasing accuracy in our


rate environments (Buss and Schmitt, 1993; perception by focusing on fake news. We con-
Kramer and Ellison, 2010; McDonald et al., sider the impact of information accuracy and
2012). Therefore, in this chapter we explore the battles of individuals against false beliefs.
from an evolutionary perspective how AI –
ubiquitous in the modern world – impacts
both individual and collective human behav-
iour. We put emphasis on the predictions 2 IMPACTS OF AI THUS FAR
from several published models that explain
how information transmission facilitates The narratives within a culture can have as
intelligence between intelligent agents. We much impact on behaviour as at least some
consider how these theories predict changes objective realities (Hammack, 2008). For this
to individual and collective behaviour as the reason, we begin by examining the current
information transmission is magnified or its narrative surrounding AI. According to Social
quality improved. We then compare, at least Representation Theory (SRT), when we
qualitatively, these predicted alterations to encounter a new or unknown phenomenon,
present societal trends. We finish with recom- we construct a representation of it based on
mendations for guiding AI development and collective narratives and interpersonal com-
interaction to maximise adaptation, progres- munication (Moscovici, 1981, 2001). It seems
sion and overall benefit to the individual and clear that public gaps in understanding AI are
society. often filled by fear-mongering entertainment
More specifically, in Section 2 we initially shows like Black Mirror, magazine articles on
tease apart the current actual impact of AI the feats of Alpha Go, and propaganda from
on society from the impact that our cultural businesses (Elish and Boyd, 2018). Indeed,
narratives surrounding AI have. We consider media exposure to science fiction has been
the evolutionary mechanisms that maintain found to predict fear of AI above and beyond
a stable society such as heterogeneity, flex- demographic variables (Liang and Lee, 2017).
ibility and cooperation. Given that AI can Problematically, such representations
constitute a prosthetic intelligence, we dis- assume AI with capacities far beyond the cur-
cuss the consequences of how it enhances rent feasibility – and in many cases, beyond
our connectivity, coordination, equality, dis- the computationally tractable – instilling awe
tribution of control and our ability to make and fear (Bryson and Kime, 2011). An inves-
predictions. We also give examples of how tigation of narratives surrounding the impact
transparency of thoughts and behaviours may of AI revealed that the most common visions
influence call-out culture, as well as behav- elicited anxiety (Cave et al., 2019). One such
ioural manipulation with consideration of vision was that by becoming over-reliant on
group dynamics and tribalism. In Section 3, AI and machines, we will replace the need
we discuss how AI may exacerbate the soci- for humans in jobs, relationships and social-
etal problems created by inequality. We place ising. Emotional arousal increases the effi-
focus on the vulnerability of human trust in cacy of information spread (Berger, 2011);
Section 4. We consider the contexts in which this mechanism is posited to have evolved
blind trust in information is either adaptive to transmit fitness-relevant information; i.e.
or maladaptive in an age where the cost of information relevant to survival which can
information is decreasing. We then bring the help organisms avoid dangers (Nairne et al.,
focus back onto trust in AI, and how we can 2009) or direct resources within a population
calibrate trust so as to avoid over-trust and (Teste et al., 2009). Yet the adaptive benefits
mistrust adaptively, using transparency as a of this in contemporary cultures are question-
mechanism. Finally, in Section 5, we explore able, now that the mechanism is known and
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND ARTIFICIAL INTELLIGENCE 335

manipulable. These narratives may distract of our global system impacting many others.
from or obscure the real problems and util- AI can be considered as our prosthetic nervous
ity of AI, resulting in sub-optimal allocation system, a tool we have developed to selectively
of energy and resources (Bryson and Kime, mediate the strength of edges between each
2011; Elish and Boyd, 2018). In the next few node. As an example, we can deploy hardy
sections, we discuss the mixture of benefits robots to explore subterranean and underwater
and potential problems we face with AI. environments inaccessible to humans, acting
and perceiving on our behalf (Siles and Walker,
2009), growing new edges of our agency.
2.1 Flexibility, Cooperation, Machine learning (ML), the ability to learn
Coordination, Perception: to perceive and categorise patterns based on
input data, also constitutes a prosthetic percep-
Humanity’s Survival Mechanisms
tion. With adequate computational resources,
AI does present a new landscape for ML for example allows us to search across
humanity – yet, despite popular rhetoric, this larger ranges of data than a human might oth-
does not necessarily pose an existential erwise be able to internalise, and to consider
threat (Gent, 2015; Müller and Bostrom, more candidate patterns. Enhanced perception,
2016). Throughout our evolutionary history, assuming accuracy, means a species has more
humans have succeeded in adapting to knowledge with which to react better to events
changing environments (Gilligan, 2007; in its environment. This constitutes an increase
Richerson et  al., 2005). Machines, in con- in human collective intelligence (Eagle and
trast, are typically fragile and short lived. As Pentland, 2003).
such, dangerous machines or technology are
unlikely to be allowed to persist in their
damaging behaviour long enough to destroy 2.2 AI Increases Connectivity
humanity as a whole, although technologi- Which Facilitates Coordination
cally mediated impacts such as climate but Also Transparency
change or hate crimes are already costing
lives. Cooperation and flexibility by means An enhanced capacity for coordination also
of heterogeneity (diversity) are two mecha- results from the increased connectivity that
nisms that enable adaptation, survival and AI and Information and Communication
progress in unstable, changing environments Technologies (ICT) more generally facilitate.
(Brown et  al., 2011; Lahr, 2016; Smaldino These technologies afford communication
et al., 2013). Here we discuss the impact of and coordination on a scale that human soci-
AI on human behaviour within the context of eties have never encountered before. This can
these two mechanisms. be expected to have – and be having – myriad
There are two core hypotheses as to the impacts on collective and individual behav-
role our nervous systems evolved to fill: the iour, not all of which we have yet recognised
sensory-motor view, to link senses to actions; (Bryson, 2015). Through increasing the
and the action-shaping view, to coordinate the number of individuals we can connect with,
body’s micro acts into macro acts (Godfrey- and decreasing temporal and spatial con-
Smith, 2017). These hypotheses are by no straints on doing so, AI creates a capacity for
means exclusive of each other. Likewise, our highly agile cooperation. Cooperation and
development of AI, amongst other things, group-level investment as a whole is known
has greatly enhanced our abilities to sense, to increase with capacity to communicate,
act and coordinate. Our society has become because this capacity allows for the increased
increasingly complex; we are connected probability of discovering mutually benefi-
world-wide, with perturbations in one part cial equilibria (Roughgarden et  al., 2006).
336 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

This cooperation may occur at historically behaviour with increasing accuracy (Zuboff,
large scales, but also at small scales, and with 2015). This seems qualitatively different to
higher frequency of change in aggregation our already adept prediction capacities: to
and direction. Social media platforms, for navigate our social world, we use mental
example, have facilitated mass organisation models of people to infer and predict the
of not only protests but also disaster recovery beliefs, actions and intentions of others
which may not otherwise have been feasible (Bradford et al., 2015). This ability, alongside
(Gerbaudo, 2018; Starbird and Palen, 2011; language (Smith, 2010), has been critical for
Vieweg et al., 2010). Additionally, increased facilitating our species’ large-scale social
access to knowledge enables us to predict cooperation. However, AI has enhanced our
threats or more quickly become aware of ability to predict the individual and collective
disasters (Chavan and Khot, 2013). beyond past capabilities. This can aid us to
Connectivity also has the effect of increas- better allocate our time and resources. For
ing behaviour transparency. Social media example, by anticipating that the number of
pages reveal the ‘likes’, ‘dislikes’ and actions people about to use a road is beyond capacity,
of a population. Problematically within this a navigation app may direct a proportion of
context, humans are often driven to take people along a different route, altering their
on characteristics of a group, and strive to behaviour advantageously for the collective.
behave according to group norms (Terry and At an individual level, the ‘optimal’ level of
Hogg, 1996), which may have a homogenis- sleep (Hao et al., 2013), exercise (Spring et al.,
ing or polarising effect, particularly during 2013), socialising and nutrition (Franco et al.,
periods when competition and identity poli- 2016) can be predicted then recommended.
tics are steep (McCarty et al., 2016). Before Unfortunately, businesses looking to maxim-
we were so connected via the digital world, ise profits will tailor their product – or just
the group norms we had access to were of far monetising efforts related to their product – to
smaller scale. This facilitated diversity – a the individual based on predictions. This can
global heterogeneity of group norms. Now, be with an incentive to reshape behaviour to
our access to the large scale, combined with make it even more easily predictable, or oth-
behaviour transparency, is widely believed erwise less expensive for the company, for
to homogenise behaviours and preferences example when insurance companies demand
(Morris, 2002). This is true even without digital access to evidence of healthy living
taking into account potential conformity (Raber et al., 2019). This, whilst not explic-
induced by physical, political, or economic itly detrimental to cooperation, may increase
threats for unacceptable behaviour, which homogeneity and therefore reduce group
we discuss further below. However, the data advantages and societal robustness (Fisher,
created by our digital activity is also made 1930; Shi et  al., 2019). It also speaks to an
transparent for use by the political and busi- additional issue with behaviour transparency:
ness, as well as the social, realms. This can capacity for behavioural control.
have positive impacts as well, if it is used to, Both the aforementioned examples of
for example, provide better public services or collective and individual behaviour tweak-
ensure greater customer satisfaction. ing and recommendations indicate a shift in
autonomy; it seems AI may be increasing
collective agency but decreasing opportunity
2.3 AI Facilitates Behaviour and drive for individual decision-making. In
fact, a survey of 970 respondents revealed
Prediction and Manipulation
that a core concern surrounding AI tech-
Ordinary collectives such as companies use nologies is the loss of human agency and
algorithms on big data sets to predict our input into decisions (Anderson et al., 2018).
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND ARTIFICIAL INTELLIGENCE 337

At one level, behavioural control may or may information has obvious applications for
not shift to autonomous artificial agents, but those interested in the outcomes of elections,
either way at a higher level it shifts to the which have apparently been deployed with
individuals or organisations who can moni- some success. Such techniques were report-
tor data and deploy any such agents. This edly originally designed and deployed to
enables execution of potentially regressive break up terrorist networks in conflict regions
social policing, albeit some well-intentioned. in the Middle East. The technique consists
Take for example, the rise of helicopter par- of identifying like-minded individuals with
enting (Lee et  al., 2014), or AI-powered minority opinions that are available locally
predictive policing systems (Meijer and or otherwise convenient to those doing the
Wessels, 2019). We do not claim such behav- aggregating, then introducing these individu-
iour manipulation is unique to AI. There als to each other and encouraging their politi-
are varying views on whether manipulation cal participation (Piette, 2018). AI-powered
techniques (not all of which necessarily use search can produce the ‘coincidence’ of good
AI) are ethical, when used for example to numbers of like-minded individuals in one
promote health (Behavioural Insights Team, place, convincing them all that their position
2010) or reduce debt (Behavioural Insights is secretly in the majority – a secret being
Team, 2012). We see the sense in policies kept by the political status quo which must
(such as that of the Institute of Electrical and therefore be attacked. Relatively simple AI
Electronics Engineers (IEEE), 2019) recom- allows the identification and coordination
mending that behaviour manipulation may of such target individuals; ICT allows the
be ethical in contexts where all the following application of such power from a distance
hold: it can be beneficial for the individual and across borders. Without laws and tech-
and/or society, transparency is provided as to nological enforcement for transparency, such
the nature of the manipulation, and the sub- manipulation may be done invisibly.
ject or a responsible adult representative of
the subject has consented. This (arguably, cf.
Simkulet, 2019) leaves the individual some 2.4 AI as Prosthetic Memory
control over the decision, at least at a higher
level. For example, clinicians, especially In the past, humans strove to both remember
under cognitive load, can demonstrate bias and to be remembered, yet the time and mon-
towards ethnic-minority patients, resulting etary cost of data retention were barriers to
in sub-optimal interaction, diagnosis and doing so. Now, however, these costs have
treatment (Stone and Moskowitz, 2011). reduced to the point where we no longer need
Evidence suggests that two tasks – perspec- to be picky about the quality of what we select
tive taking or categorising oneself to be in to retain. This has its advantages and disad-
a shared group with the ethnic minority – vantages. Biological brains forget (Kraemer
can reduce bias. In this scenario, consented and Golding, 1997). In humans, such a lack of
behavioural manipulation could involve an pruning memories, inability to forget, or
app-based intervention, where the clinician information overload can result in deficits in
chooses to engage with a bias-reduction task executive functioning, and an inability to
prior to seeing the patient. escape past autobiographical memories
This same public or semi-private – and (Parker et  al., 2006). Using AI-driven tech-
sometimes implicit – communication of pref- niques, even with surplus data, we can still
erences can be used deliberately to determine successfully store, reorganise, classify and
the personality types of individuals, and also retrieve the relevant information we require
their voting inclinations (Gelman et al., 2016; from large data stores. However, this pros-
Kosinski et al., 2013; Wu et al., 2015). Such thetic memory of digital expression
338 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

of opinions and beliefs – whether political, be seen. Uncompromised cybersecurity of


religious or otherwise – combined with not only data storage but transmission would
increased connectivity has resulted in, also be necessary for any digital records to
amongst other things, a resurgence of call-out have even a hope of remaining private.
culture and public shaming (Hess and Waller, The claim that homogenisation is a side
2014; Tucker, 2018; Webb et al., 2016). effect of AI may seem demonstrably false
The urge to normalise groups by exiling given the increase in identity politics and
or denouncing the credibility of individuals political polarisation. Coincidence is not
who diverge is a facet of tribalism prevalent necessarily causal, and even if there is a
throughout our history (Bechtel, 1991; Burns, causal link, it may be difficult to untangle.
2003). Novel though with the AI landscape is Polarisation is known to be correlated with
the relative immortality of digital memory. The wealth inequality, and to have been so since
social costs (and benefits) to acting or thinking before the advent of ICT and AI. Whether AI
in diverging ways have increased; it is hard to is presently contributing to inequality will
be forgotten. Thus again, we are homogenised be considered in the next section. But with
by our evolutionary urge to remain in the respect to homogenisation, it is worth say-
safety of the tribe. Mayer-Schönberger (2007) ing that both processes may well occur at the
advocates finding an equilibrium: by giving same time – rather than a plethora of perspec-
data an expiry date after which it will auto- tives, we may find strong forces towards con-
matically be deleted, we can maximise the formity with one of a small number of tribes.
benefits of a precise prosthetic memory, whilst Again, this is the opposite of what was antici-
preserving the right to be forgotten. However, pated with access to the Internet and cheap
such arbitrary truncation would also be an end self-publication, and there is also evidence of
to history, unless history were still retained in societal fragmentation (Pentland, 2015).
non-digital format. Even if exceptions were
made for those considered public individuals,
as is now the case for certain privacy laws, this
could have an unintended effect of reducing 3 AI AND INEQUALITY
social mobility, as those in situations of promi-
nence, privileged with being knowable, would Whether or not we become more homoge-
be more likely to garner further attention and nous in our beliefs and opinions, inequality
opportunity. in access to resources and quality of life is
Historical data also has applications rang- increasing. We discuss here what is known
ing from the social sciences to monitoring about how inequality is driven, and consider
the impacts of government policies. In an the impact of AI and the consequences for
era where behaviour modification might be the collective.
practised by subterfuge, accurate historical First, it should be observed that globally
data may be the only way to detect mali- inequality has been falling, an effect driven
cious actors working subtly over time. The primarily by the very poorest. The World
rights to freedom of opinion and thought Bank reports more than a third of humanity
are enshrined in the Declaration on Human moving out of extreme poverty since 1980
Rights (United Nations General Assembly, (Roser and Ortiz-Ospina, 2017), a shift that
1948), but without an option of perceivable has been facilitated by ICT including AI, as
expression such rights may be of limited populations have had more access to useful
value. There is substantial research being information such as weather predictions, fair
conducted in anonymisation, including for prices and how to obtain government sup-
data extraction and analysis – whether this port. Further, efforts to communicate politi-
proves mathematically tractable remains to cal and economic situations, and means to
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND ARTIFICIAL INTELLIGENCE 339

coordinate protest, are leading at least some the majority of individuals (Atkinson, 2015).
governments in rich areas such as the mem- This can be the effect not only of lack of
ber states of the Organisation for Economic social mobility and understanding, but even
Co-operation and Development (OECD) to of simple spatial segregation (Cassiers and
adopt policies that have been demonstrated Kesteloot, 2012). As discussed earlier, digital
to reduce inequality within populations by social media provides a ready platform for dis-
increasing wealth distribution. information, including caricaturised and exag-
Nevertheless, an individual’s access to pub- gerated distortions of others. An elite may also
lic goods such as schools, physical security, falsely assume that it can consolidate power
utilities and health depends to a large degree by extracting wealth from its own neighbours
on their geographic identity. Access to ICT and nearest contenders. However, inequality
and AI is no exception (Robinson et al., 2015; breeds instability as the whims of small coali-
Sujarwoto and Tampubolon, 2016), although tions or even individual actors are unpredict-
it is also influenced by other factors such as able (Scheidel, 2017). When greater collective
age. As we globally become more dependent action is required, solutions become more pre-
on such tools, the populations without access dictable and stable, ironically better ensuring
are exposed to higher risks of inequality. By the maintenance of rank order at the higher
moving towards equality of access to these end of society. Power over a collective is not
technologies, individuals may have improved only an animal thrill, it is also a mechanism
job opportunities and information in regard of security because it ensures more individuals
to the socioeconomic, political and cultural are invested in a mutually beneficial outcome
context in which they live. (Terkel, 1974). Trust is a factor in individuals
The increase in inequality may result in collectively investing in mutually beneficial
reduced social cohesion as it seems to be cor- outcomes, yet not only does inequality breed
related to reductions in social mobility and distrust (Barone and Mocetti, 2016) but, even
increases in political polarisation (McCarty when trust is held, it is vulnerable to exploita-
et al., 2016). ICT and AI may also reduce the tion and damage to the individual.
sort of localised social cohesion that is criti- Again, there is no clear evidence to date, but
cal to many forms of well-being and political rather active investigation, as to whether and in
engagement, by diverting social attention to what ways AI may be affecting inequality. It
others with shared interests in topics that are seems evident that any technology that reduces
not geographically centred. In this, it continues the cost of distance will also facilitate inequality
and expands on trends of mass media known through no particular malfeasance but simply
since the beginning of the information age by allowing excellent businesses to dominate
with, for example, the advent of national news- larger territories, and therefore market shares.
papers (Perlman and Sprick Schuster, 2016). In the case of some technologies now such as
For a substantial fraction of our society, the finance, media, pharmaceutical, aerospace, and
time we spend engaging with others as real, of course digital, this is approaching the limit
physical equals is replaced with ever-more- case of single corporations dominating markets
engaging digital entertainment. Research globally. This same phenomenon may explain
shows that whilst some US teens felt digital the similar surge in developed-world inequality
technologies connected them with others, oth- witnessed in the late 19th and early 20th centu-
ers felt it resulted in a lack of in-person contact ries (Atkinson, 2015; Bryson, 2019).
in their lives (Anderson and Jiang, 2018). The As mentioned in Section 2, another con-
interaction that would once have taken place in cern is that AI may alter employment. Whilst
person is now conducted through technology. the common concern is that ‘robots will take
There is a danger that inequality pro- all the jobs’, this seems highly unlikely as ‘all
duces an elite who no longer identify with the jobs’ is not defined. There is an infinite
340 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

number of ways we can better each other’s investor is given some monetary windfall and
lives. People tend to employ each other when must decide whether to keep the windfall, or
the economy is good, though this may be give some fraction to a trustee. The fraction
seen as tautological. But the advent of intel- offered by the investor is then multiplied by
ligent technology has been associated with some factor (typically three) and the trustee can
an increase in demand for both highly skilled then offer a fraction of the multiplied invest-
and very low-skilled and low-waged work, ment back to the investor. Human investors
with a declining demand and therefore wages tend to blindly trust their partner by offering
for the intermediate workers (Acemoglu and non-zero investments, even though it is in the
Autor, 2011). Acemoglu and Autor (2011) sug- trustee’s best interest to return nothing to the
gest that technology plays two roles in wages: investor. Whilst trust is moderated by several
in allowing all to be more productive, this factors, including framing effects (Burnham
somewhat increases the value of being highly et  al., 2000), geography (Johnson and Mislin,
skilled, but flattens any advantages of moder- 2011), gender (Buchan et al., 2008), risk prefer-
ate skill as people become more exchangeable. ences (Fehr, 2009) and whether participants are
Worryingly, recent decades seem to be domi- selected from a student population (Johnson
nated by the latter effect. and Mislin, 2011), individuals consistently tend
to blindly trust, investing some of their windfall
(Johnson and Mislin, 2011).
4 THE VULNERABILITY OF Significant research has sought to under-
HUMAN TRUST stand how blindly trusting others might be
ecologically rational. This proclivity to trust
Subjective evaluations of trustworthiness are has been explained as adaptive in relatively
deeply tied to how humans navigate the world. small populations where individuals have
The likelihood of a monetary transaction is reputation cues of their partners (Boero et al.,
largely dependent on the trust a buyer has in a 2009; Masuda and Nakamura, 2012) or if
seller (Kim et  al., 2008; Ponte et  al., 2015). the chance of knowing a partner’s strategy
Trust in politicians affects voter turnout exceeds some threshold (Manapat and Rand,
(Grönlund and Setälä, 2007) and electoral 2012; Manapat et al., 2013; McNamara et al.,
results (Hetherington, 1999). When evaluating 2009; Rauwolf and Bryson, 2018). The com-
a person’s face on multiple dimensions, subjec- mon theme is that blindly trusting another
tive trustworthiness is one of the most predic- can be adaptive if someone, somewhere, has a
tive measures for overall evaluation (Oosterhof chance of having information about a player,
and Todorov, 2008). Further, once trustworthi- including indirectly (e.g. if a population is
ness is perceived, it is relatively robust, persis- known to share trust-related characteristics
tently affecting behaviour (Delgado et  al., by some sort of contagion effect or enforce-
2005). If AI alters our capacity or predisposi- ment). Interestingly, on the other hand, indi-
tion to trust other humans, it will clearly have a viduals will not trust others when they know
deep impact on society. the others are likely to return (reciprocate) an
unfair amount in the TG, even if that amount
would still make trusting them beneficial.
4.1 Baseline Proclivities to
Blindly Trust
4.2 When Blind Trust Is Valuable
Using simple economic games, it has been
shown that individuals will blindly trust a stran- Rauwolf and Bryson (2018) demonstrate a
ger even when it leaves them vulnerable (Berg generally advantageous but unstable evolu-
et al., 1995). In a one-shot Trust Game (TG), an tionary dynamic that typically establishes
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND ARTIFICIAL INTELLIGENCE 341

trust. This demonstration is in the context of the other options of a prospective buyer. If an
simulations with computational agents play- individual is willing to go elsewhere, even by
ing one-shot TGs with each other. Agents blindly trusting a stranger, then the market is
were given several potential partners for play- forced to adjust and the buyer’s life is improved.
ing each game, and chose which agent to play,
if any. As in the natural world, the investing
agent knew the reputation for historic pay-off 4.3 Information Cost Reduces
of some partners, but did not know the pay- Benefits of Trust
off of others – information was partially
occluded. Each actor learned three things Importantly, whilst the adaptive models
socially from the strongest players: their of trust require that some information is
levels of trust in unknown players, the available, trust fails if information is fully
demanded level of reciprocation for known transparent (Manapat et  al., 2013; Rauwolf
players, and their own reciprocation rate. and Bryson, 2018). By definition, the act of
Rauwolf and Bryson (2018) demonstrate that trusting another requires some uncertainty in
this simple dynamic is sufficient to generate the outcome (Yamagishi, 2011). If informa-
trust between members of a population. When tion is fully transparent, then there is no need
the known pay-offs of partners are sufficiently to trust another; rather, each individual can
low, it can be in an agent’s interest to blindly make an informed decision. The consequence
select a partner whose history is unknown, is that there is no selective pressure to evolve
provided that the population has evolved high or learn the strategies and beliefs associated
enough levels of trust, which tends to coevolve with the riskier behaviour, but these are what
with high levels of reciprocation, and of bind a local community together.
course assuming sufficient extra benefit from We are currently living in an age where the
mutual development of the public good. In cost of information is dramatically decreas-
these cases, blind trust can be more valuable ing. As a result, the adaptive benefits of trust
to the agent than walking away with their are becoming increasingly obsolete. This
monetary windfall, trusting no one. On the may be for the best – we may have a more
other hand, trust will not evolve if the recipro- predictable environment with even higher
cation rate of many players is known, because rewards for ‘good’ behaviour. However, we
it becomes better to stick with the best-known should also be concerned about this being
available return rather than to take a chance another force for homogeneity, and further
with the unknown. loss of individual capacity to deal creatively
The insight from this work is that a will- with localised crises. The institution of trust
ingness to blindly trust others increases com- may be just a consequence of our inability to
petition between others, lowering prices by perfectly control our peers, but it may also
increasing the reciprocation rate. It is well- serve an adaptive advantage by reducing our
known that creating competition between responsibility to do so. Society does not need
sellers lowers prices. But, by being willing to come up with plans to handle every con-
to trust those whose information is unknown, tingency, because a desperate individual can
the pressure of competition is increased. Not always take advantage of the availability of
only do sellers need to compete with other trust without first seeking social approval of
sellers whose information is known, they now their plans.
need to compete with those whose informa- There are indications that in the near
tion is unknown. This tends to lower the mar- term moving from trust to full information
ket price even further. This is related to work is problematic in other ways. Because we
on outside options (André and Baumard, will not just ‘shut off’ our historic psycho-
2011). The value of a sale is contingent upon logical choice making, replacing trust with
342 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

information may mean that extant prejudices artefacts than they are by unscrupulous
become more rigidly a part of our behaviour. ­individuals. For example, one might assume
Not only is trust deeply tied to how individuals that a robot is unlikely to remember every-
make decisions, subjective trust is often biased thing you say because a person or pet would
and founded on unhelpful signals. People not, but the robot may in fact not only recall
find attractive individuals more trustworthy but transmit its full memory – a full record of
(Wilson and Eckel, 2006). Individuals will all interactions or even nearby events – storing
invest more if a profile picture has a smiling these offsite in a corporate cloud. Although
face (Scharlemann et al., 2001) or is visually in theory the same digital and architected fea-
perceived as more trustworthy (Bente et  al., tures of AI that make it more powerful as a
2012). The trust individuals place in profile manipulator should also make it easier to
information is often incorrect (Toma, 2010). govern, presently (2020) manipulation is out-
More generally, individuals perform close to stripping governance.
chance when predicting deception (Bond and Acceptance of AI can be increased in many
DePaulo, 2006). Ert et  al. (2016) show that ways, but given the vulnerability of the human
perceived trustworthiness of an Airbnb option trust system, care is needed to ensure trust is
correlates more with the profile photo than the extended with consent, and is not exploited
quantified reputation score of that option. This (IEEE, 2019). Whilst tapping into the vulner-
was confirmed by Fagerstrøm et  al. (2017), abilities of how humanity perceives trustwor-
who found that facial expressions in a renter’s thiness may be efficacious, it can also result
photo predicted likelihood to rent more than in unwarranted trust. People are already found
customer ratings. This demonstrates that the to perceive AI as more objective than human
transparency of information is not necessarily ­decision-makers, and in some cases to over-rely
sufficient to improve behaviour. Individuals on AI. For example, in a legal setting, people
must make decisions using that information demonstrated a preference to follow a machine
before it offers an advantage. advisor’s decision despite a human advisor
having judgement of higher accuracy (Logg
et al., 2019). In our own research (Wilson and
Theodorou, 2019), we have found that in vir-
4.4 Calibrating Trust in AI
tual reality (VR), AI actors presented to the
There is considerable discussion these days participants as human agents were perceived
about trust in AI and trustworthiness for AI. to be significantly less deterministic than those
Our own work and that of many in the UK’s presented explicitly as robots, despite both
ethics community more generally has taken a being controlled by the same AI system.
different tack, emphasising that trust is an It seems that many of us attribute properties
anthropocentric trait not truly useful for arte- to AI that do not exist, at least where that AI
facts, where transparency and accountability reminds us of humans (Sparrow, 2019). Some
are more desirable (Boden et  al., 2011; evidence suggests that anthropomorphising
Bryson and Theodorou, 2019). Improving robots increases interaction with them (Waytz
transparency in AI reduces the need to trust et al., 2014) via increased trust resistance (de
AI. Yet it is possible that transparency does Visser et al., 2016) and mind attribution. There
not affect the consumption of AI when the are uncertainties as to the impact of anthropo-
human consumer projects human-like iden- morphism when robots are more prevalent and
tity to intelligent technology (anthropomor- ‘normalised’ in our society. As we grow famil-
phises). By doing so, the consumer exposes iar with robots in our day-to-day lives, our
themselves to exploitation as their established mental models of robots may become more
biases concerning the likelihood of trustwor- accurate (Bryson and Kime, 2011). We may
thiness are even easier to exploit via designed perceive with more clarity the distinctions
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND ARTIFICIAL INTELLIGENCE 343

between artificially embodied cognition and AI tendrils enhance our ability to sense and
humans, or commercial products may be perceive. Yet, problematically, the collective
mandated to provide transparency. In these intelligence that could be garnered from this
cases, the impact of anthropomorphism may additional perception is hindered by two key
be reduced. Alternatively, viewing robots as barriers: the fact that data can be inaccurate
human-like may become normalised, and and misleading, and our own inability to
social robotics – believed to be human-like, handle and interpret data. In this section we
despite their inhuman, designed capacities – explore the origins, trajectory and impact of
an embedded aspect of our lives. inaccurate information in human communica-
We suggest that a safer and more long-term tion networks. We note our current inability to
stable approach is to work to increase AI trans- correctly deploy, infer and apply meaning
parency whilst simultaneously helping indi- from AI. We highlight current avenues for
viduals learn to make choices using empirical reducing these barriers in order to fully exploit
information. As the information age reduces our augmented collective intelligence.
the need for trust, individuals need to be trained
to operate in this information age, rather than
reinforcing poor decision-making tendencies 5.1 ‘Fake News’
based on fallible and manipulable perceptions
of trust. An example of increased AI transpar- Humans have long expressed a desire to
ency would be to have a QR code attached to record and share information: the first ency-
each robot that when scanned gives informa- clopaedia was written in AD 77 (Gudger,
tion on the robot’s maker, purpose and capabili- 1924); libraries have been dated back 2,000
ties. We have also been developing means for years earlier. The arrival of the telegraph and
allowing users to see the current goals and strat- Morse Code in 1835 enabled instantaneous
egies of a robot or other real-time interactive AI transmission of knowledge across great dis-
system (Rotsidis et al., 2019; Theodorou et al., tances (Burns, 1988); now databases are ubiq-
2017), and are presently experimenting to see uitous. Historical records indicate sharing
whether this reduces the moral-hazard aspects knowledge spurred many innovations (Bessen
of anthropomorphism. Whilst some may see and Nuvolari, 2016), and on a day-to-day
viewing AI as human-like to be an example of a basis, enables individuals to make informed
freedom of opinion or even association, we feel decisions and actions. Unfortunately, not
strongly that such opinions need to be informed all shared information is accurate.
where information is available, in order to Disinformation and misinformation, which
avoid unknowing exploitation. Willing exploi- often fall under the misnomer ‘fake news’, are
tation, like the manipulation of emotions that of rising public concern. Disinformation
occurs during a motion picture or other work of implies intentional creation and sharing of
fiction, is of course a perfectly acceptable part manipulated or false information, whereas
of life and entertainment. We only seek to avoid misinformation refers to inadvertent sharing
economic and political manipulation imposed (Lazer et al., 2018).
on unknowing others. Fake news is not new: since humans could
speak, misinformation has spread via word
of mouth. The spread increased and quick-
ened with the arrival of newspapers and
5 BARRIERS TO ACCURATE pamphlets, then with mass media such as
PERCEPTION AND DEVELOPMENTS television, finally exploding with the Internet
and especially social media (Burkhardt,
Our earlier metaphor for AI as an extension 2017). What is new is that, compared to past
of our nervous system posited that new technological mediums, social media largely
344 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

lacks filtering, editorial judgement and fact- as time, cognition, and sometimes emotional
checking (Allcott and Gentzkow, 2017). investment. Actions occurring as conse-
Further, the communication network is infil- quence of conversations result in further
trated by artificially intelligent bots, able to deployment of resources. We know that, as a
pass as or augment human users, which can social species, we respond to such evidence of
be used to quickly deploy, share and spread investment by others in our society (Zahavi,
information across networks (Machado and 1977). We posit that individual and collec-
Konopacki, 2018). Such bots can be used to tive resources may be consequently directed
sway public opinion. In fact, disinformation away from more accurate topics which are of
affects stock prices (Carvalho et  al., 2011), perhaps more importance to our survival and
political opinions (Howard et  al., 2018), flourishing.
and voting patterns (Allcott and Gentzkow,
2017) at least transiently. Evidence shows
that accurate stories take longer to spread but 5.2 The Impact of Information
have more purchase once spread (Vosoughi Accuracy
et  al., 2018). In previous work (Mitchell
et al., 2016), we have demonstrated that even In further work with simulations and formal
error-prone ‘gossip’ can be a better strategy analysis, we offered insight into the environ-
than direct experiential learning for acquir- ments where humans may be vulnerable to
ing true and useful information. The speed of utilising incorrect information (Rauwolf,
information transmission such as is provided 2016). Information requires both time and
by social media can in some circumstances energy to gather. If information gathering
outweigh the costs of incorrect information, comes at a non-trivial cost, then we would
particularly if disinformation can be identi- expect individuals to truncate their informa-
fied and quickly combated (Panagiotopoulos tion search after a period of time (Simon,
et al., 2014). 1956). However, given continual improve-
Nevertheless, in at least some contexts, our ments in technology, the cost of information
species seems often to communicate inac- is falling; as a result we might expect indi-
curate rather than accurate information. An viduals to be better informed. Importantly
analysis of a data set of rumour cascades on though, even if information is easily obtained,
Twitter revealed fake news was 70% more if the processing of that information is costly,
likely to be retweeted than the truth (Vosoughi limiting information can be advantageous
et al., 2018). There are suggestions that fake (Rauwolf and Jones, 2019).
news is more likely to be novel, and novelty Rauwolf et al. (2015) show that when the
captures human attention. Perhaps worry- benefits of group dynamics conflict with the
ingly, the average American spends 23.6 hours accuracy of beliefs, false beliefs can become
online weekly (Cole et  al., 2017), and 62% the least-costly option. Across a variety
get their news online (Gottfried and Shearer, of contexts, individuals tend to prioritise
2016). Whilst there is evidence that our trust of relationships with those who share simi-
such news has decreased, exposure alone may lar values – a trait called value-homophily
have negative impacts. Exposure can prime (McPherson et  al., 2001). We have demon-
thinking and conversational topics. When any strated that it is in precisely these contexts
news enters the conversational sphere, trust in where individuals can be expected to use
the information increases (Hajli et al., 2014). incorrect information (Rauwolf et al., 2015).
We humans seem to have a disposition to When the social value or benefit provided by
trust information communicated by word of the group outweighs the private cost of pos-
mouth (Atika et  al., 2018; Huete-Alcocer, sessing incorrect information, it is advanta-
2017). Conversations require resources such geous for the individual to maintain (or at
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND ARTIFICIAL INTELLIGENCE 345

least act on) their false beliefs. Given that the Fact-checking can be as simple as conducting
inaccuracy of political and religious beliefs a Web search on a topic and its source.
provides virtually no personal cost (Caplan,
2001), but group agreement may provide
social value through security, we would
expect humanity would struggle to remove 6 CONCLUSION
false beliefs, particularly in times of resource
scarcity and conflict (Stewart et al., 2020). In this chapter we have discussed the impact of
As the benefits conferred by AI and tech- AI on contemporary societies. We took a per-
nology at large continue to improve and spective of understanding how the changing
secure individual basic needs, individuals social and economic landscape induced by
will likely pay a reduced private cost for pos- AI interacts with the human information-
sessing incorrect information across a broad- processing biases which evolved in very
ening array of contexts. Regardless of the different environments. We consider a better
accuracy of an individual’s beliefs, the basic understanding of these impacts imperative for
needs of most individuals are improving. As our society going forward as we optimise our
such, if individuals pay small personal costs existence with AI, and for ensuring the AI and
for false beliefs, but garner large social ben- the regulations we design to govern its use
efits for group homogeneity, then we would both maximise benefit and minimise harm. We
expect an expanding and resilient battle discussed the impact on both collective and
against false beliefs. individual human behaviour. Here we summa-
Nevertheless, what costs an individual lit- rise the key foci of this chapter. First, the accu-
tle in isolation may cost a society a great deal racy of narratives surrounding AI could
due to aggregate responses, particularly in a critically impact optimal engagement with AI.
democracy (Chote et al., 2016; Lewis, 2017). Next, we compared AI to a prosthetic nervous
Whether or not we should strive for increased system, which increases our perception and
rate of information transmission in every case agency. AI also enhances our capacity to
(see the discussions of trust and freedom of remember, coordinate, connect and communi-
opinion above), we should almost certainly cate; this has many positive but also some
prefer accurate communication, though here, negative outcomes. We considered the impact
too, inaccuracy can sometimes lead to use- on freedom and diversity of opinion, political
ful innovation. Disinformation is a global and and economic impact, the mechanisms of
long-running issue, and there are global ini- information spread, and vulnerability of human
tiatives to combat it. For example, Facebook trust and social coherence. The increased dis-
flags potential news stories to be reviewed coverability and predictability facilitated by
by third-party fact checkers; and through the AI requires serious consideration; there are
messaging system WeChat in China, users can myriad beneficial and harmful current applica-
report fake news, which is then checked and tions of AI, and no doubt more of both to
flagged. Crucial new initiatives introduce criti- come. There are also of course many move-
cal literacy into the education curriculum – ments to ensure AI is beneficial to our society
training children to recognise and ques- rather than harmful, though we did not take
tion information sources, particularly online time to touch on those much here, we view
(Vasu et  al., 2018). Critical-thinking and such consideration and efforts as essential.
fact-checking skills, as well as basic under- Coordinating and enforcing such pro-social
standing of algorithm mechanics and their efforts has traditionally been called govern-
limitations, could enable the next genera- ance, and we hope this chapter may contribute
tion to be better prepared to avoid such sce- to making sensible governance easier to both
narios faced by us today (Guess et al., 2019). justify and employ constructively.
346 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Note Behavioural Insights Team (2010). Applying


behavioural insight to health. Technical
1  As of this writing, the impact of these assaults is Report 403936/1210, Cabinet Office, UK
still a matter of urgent research and debate, but
Government, London.
the fact of significant, long-term, and on-going
Behavioural Insights Team (2012). Applying
expenditure in the attempts has been established
in both courts and academic writings (e.g. Mach- behavioural insights to reduce fraud, error
ado and Konopacki, 2018; Woods, 2018; Landon- and debt. Technical Report 408779/0212,
Murray et al., 2019). Cabinet Office, UK Government, London.
Bente, G., Baptist, O., and Leuschner, H.
(2012). To buy or not to buy: Influence of
seller photos and reputation on buyer trust
and purchase behavior. International Journal
REFERENCES of Human-Computer Studies, 70(1):1–13.
Berg, J., Dickhaut, J., and McCabe, K. (1995).
Acemoglu, D. and Autor, D. (2011). Skills, tasks Trust, reciprocity, and social history. Games
and technologies: Implications for employ- and Economic Behavior, 10(1):122–142.
ment and earnings. In Ashenfelter, O. and Berger, J. (2011). Arousal increases social trans-
Card, D. (Eds.), volume 4 of Handbook of mission of information. Psychological Science,
Labor Economics, chapter 12, pages 1043– 22(7):891–893.
1171. Amsterdam: Elsevier. Bessen, J. and Nuvolari, A. (2016). Knowledge
Allcott, H. and Gentzkow, M. (2017). Social sharing among inventors: Some historical
media and fake news in the 2016 election. perspectives. In Harhoff, D. and Lakhani, K.
Journal of Economic Perspectives, 31(2): (Eds.), Revolutionizing Innovation: Users,
211–236. Communities, and Open Innovation, page
Anderson, J., Rainie, L., and Luchsinger, A. 135. MIT Press, Cambridge MA.
(2018). Artificial intelligence and the future Boden, M., Bryson, J., Caldwell, D., Dautenhahn,
of humans. Pew Research Center. K., Edwards, L., Kember, S., Newman, P., Parry,
Anderson, M. and Jiang, J. (2018). Teens, social V., Pegman, G., Rodden, T., Sorell, T., Wallis,
media & technology 2018. Washington, DC: M., Whitby, B., and Winfield, A. (2011).
Pew Internet & American Life Project. Retrieved Principles of robotics. The United Kingdom’s
June 3, 2018, from https://www.pewresearch. Engineering and Physical Sciences Research
org/internet/2018/05/31/teens-social-media- Council (EPSRC). Retrieved from https://epsrc.
technology-2018/ ukri.org/research/ourportfolio/themes/
André, J.-B. and Baumard, N. (2011). Social engineering/activities/principlesofrobotics/
opportunities and the evolution of fairness. Boero, R., Bravo, G., Castellani, M., and Squaz-
Journal of Theoretical Biology, 289:128–135. zoni, F. (2009). Reputational cues in repeated
Atika, A., Kusumawati, A., and Iqbal, M. trust games. The Journal of Socio-Economics,
(2018). The effect of electronic word of 38(6):871–877.
mouth, message source credibility, informa- Bond, C. F. J. and DePaulo, B. M. (2006). Accu-
tion quality on brand image and purchase racy of deception judgments. Personality and
intention. EKUITAS (Jurnal Ekonomi dan Social Psychology Review, 10(3):214–234.
Keuangan), 20(1):94–108. Bradford, E. E., Jentzsch, I., and Gomez, J.-C.
Atkinson, A. B. (2015). Inequality: What can be (2015). From self to social cognition: Theory
done? Cambridge, MA: Harvard University of mind mechanisms and their relation to
Press. executive functioning. Cognition, 138:21–34.
Barone, G. and Mocetti, S. (2016). Inequality Brown, G. R., Dickins, T. E., Sear, R., and
and trust: New evidence from panel data. Laland, K. N. (2011). Evolutionary accounts
Economic Inquiry, 54(2):794–809. of human behavioural diversity. Philosophical
Bechtel, L. M. (1991). Shame as a sanction of Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological
social control in biblical Israel: Judicial, political, Sciences, 366:313–324.
and social shaming. Journal for the Study of Bryson, J. J. (2015). Artificial intelligence and
the Old Testament, 16(49):47–76. pro-social behaviour. In Misselhorn, C.,
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND ARTIFICIAL INTELLIGENCE 347

editor, Collective Agency and Cooperation in European cities. Urban Studies, 49(9):
Natural and Artificial Systems: Explanation, 1909–1924.
Implementation and Simulation, volume 122 Cave, S., Coughlan, K., and Dihal, K. (2019).
of Philosophical Studies, pages 281–306. ‘Scary robots’: Examining public responses
Berlin: Springer. to AI. In Proceedings of the AIES. Retrieved
Bryson, J. J. (2019). The past decade and future May 24, 2020, from https://doi.org/10.1145/
of AI’s impact on society. In Towards a New 3306618.3314232
Enlightenment? In Cities, D (Eds.), A Trans- Chavan, S. and Khot, T. S. (2013). Efficient and
cendent Decade, pages 150–185. Madrid: reliable routing algorithm to enhance con-
Turner–BBVA. nectivity in disaster scenario: Abc algorithm.
Bryson, J. J. and Kime, P. P. (2011). Just an arti- International Journal of Science and
fact: Why machines are perceived as moral Research, 4(5):891–896.
agents. In Proceedings of the 22nd Interna- Chote, R., Nickell, S., and Parker, G. (2016).
tional Joint Conference on Artificial Intelli- Economic and fiscal outlook. Technical Report
gence, pages 1641–1646, Barcelona. Morgan Cm 9346, Office for Budget Responsibility,
Kaufmann. London, UK. Retrieved on 22 August, 2020
Bryson, J. J. and Theodorou, A. (2019). How from www.gov.uk/government/publications.
society can maintain human-centric artificial Cole, J., Suman, M., Schramm, P., and Zhou, L.
intelligence. In Toivonen-Noro, M. and Saari, (2017). The 2017 digital future report:
E. (Eds.), Human-Centered Digitalization and Surveying the digital future. Los Angeles, CA:
Services, chapter 16, pages 305–323. Singa- USC Annenberg School Center for the Digital
pore: Springer. Future.
Buchan, N. R., Croson, R. T., and Solnick, S. de Visser, E. J., Monfort, S. S., McKendrick, R.,
(2008). Trust and gender: An examination of Smith, M. A., McKnight, P. E., Krueger, F.,
behavior and beliefs in the investment game. and Parasuraman, R. (2016). Almost human:
Journal of Economic Behavior & Organiza- Anthropomorphism increases trust resilience
tion, 68(3):466–476. in cognitive agents. Journal of Experimental
Burkhardt, J. M. (2017). History of fake news. Psychology: Applied, 22(3):331.
Library Technology Reports, 53(8):5–9. Delgado, M. R., Frank, R. H., and Phelps, E. A.
Burnham, T., McCabe, K., and Smith, V. L. (2005). Perceptions of moral character mod-
(2000). Friend-or-foe intentionality priming in ulate the neural systems of reward during
an extensive form trust game. Journal of Eco- the trust game. Nature Neuroscience,
nomic Behavior & Organization, 43(1):57–73. 8(11):1611–1618.
Burns, R. (1988). The electric telegraph and the Eagle, N. and Pentland, A. S. (2003). Social net-
development of picture telegraphy. In Papers work computing. In Dey, A. K., Schmidt, A.,
Presented at the Sixteenth IEE Week-End and McCarthy, J. F. (Eds.), UbiComp 2003:
Meeting on the History of Electrical Engineer- Ubiquitous Computing, pages 289–296,
ing, pages 80–86. IET. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg.
Burns, W. E. (2003). Witch hunts in Europe and Elish, M. C. and Boyd, D. (2018). Situating
America: An encyclopedia. Westport, CT: methods in the magic of big data and AI.
Greenwood Publishing Group. Communication Monographs, 85(1):57–80.
Buss, D. M. and Schmitt, D. P. (1993). Sexual Ert, E., Fleischer, A., and Magen, N. (2016).
strategies theory: An evolutionary perspec- Trust and reputation in the sharing economy:
tive on human mating. Psychological Review, The role of personal photos in Airbnb. Tourism
100(2):204. Management, 55:62–73.
Caplan, B. (2001). Rational ignorance versus Fagerstrøm, A., Pawar, S., Sigurdsson, V.,
rational irrationality. Kyklos, 54(1):3–26. Foxall, G. R., and de Soriano, M. Y. (2017).
Carvalho, C., Klagge, N., and Moench, E. (2011). That personal profile image might jeopardize
The persistent effects of a false news shock. your rental opportunity! On the relative
Journal of Empirical Finance, 18(4):597–615. impact of the seller’s facial expressions upon
Cassiers, T. and Kesteloot, C. (2012). Socio- buying behavior on AirbnbTM. Computers in
spatial inequalities and social cohesion in Human Behavior, 72:123–131.
348 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Fehr, E. (2009). On the economics and biology Hao, T., Xing, G., and Zhou, G. (2013). isleep:
of trust. Journal of the European Economic Unobtrusive sleep quality monitoring using
Association, 7(2–3):235–266. smartphones. In Proceedings of the 11th
Fisher, R. A. (1930). The genetical theory of natu- ACM Conference on Embedded Networked
ral selection. Clarendon Press, Oxford (2nd Sensor Systems, page 4. ACM.
edn., 1958, New York, Dover. Variorum edition, Hess, K. and Waller, L. (2014). The digital pil-
Bennett JH (Ed.), 1999, Oxford University Press). lory: Media shaming of ‘ordinary’ people for
Franco, R. Z., Fallaize, R., Lovegrove, J. A., and minor crimes. Continuum, 28(1):101–111.
Hwang, F. (2016). Popular nutrition-related Hetherington, M. J. (1999). The effect of political
mobile apps: A feature assessment. JMIR trust on the presidential vote, 1968–96. Ameri-
mHealth and uHealth, 4(3):e85. can Political Science Review, 93(2):311–326.
Gelman, A., Goel, S., Rivers, D., and Roths- Howard, P. N., Kollanyi, B., Bradshaw, S., and
child, D. (2016). The mythical swing voter. Neudert, L.-M. (2018). Social media, news
Quarterly Journal of Political Science, and political information during the US elec-
11(1):103–130. tion: Was polarizing content concentrated in
Gent, E. (2015). Ai: Fears of ‘playing god’ swing states? arXiv preprint arXiv:1802.03573.
[control & automation artificial intelligence]. Huete-Alcocer, N. (2017). A literature review of
Engineering & Technology, 10(2):76–79. word of mouth and electronic word of
Gerbaudo, P. (2018). Tweets and the streets: mouth: Implications for consumer behavior.
Social media and contemporary activism. Frontiers in Psychology, 8:1256.
London: Pluto Press. Institute of Electrical and Electronics Engineers
Gilligan, I. (2007). Neanderthal extinction and (IEEE) (2019). Ethically aligned design: A
modern human behaviour: The role of cli- vision for prioritizing human well-being with
mate change and clothing. World Archaeol- autonomous and intelligent systems. Technical
ogy, 39(4):499–514. report, The IEEE Global Initiative on Ethics of
Godfrey-Smith, P. (2017). Other minds: The Autonomous and Intelligent Systems, first
octopus, the sea, and the deep origins of edition.
consciousness. New York, NY: Farrar, Straus Johnson, N. D. and Mislin, A. A. (2011). Trust
and Giroux. games: A meta-analysis. Journal of Economic
Gottfried, J. and Shearer, E. (2016). News Use Psychology, 32(5):865–889.
Across Social Medial Platforms 2016. Wash- Kim, D. J., Ferrin, D. L., and Rao, H. R. (2008).
ington, DC: Pew Research Center. Retrieved A trust-based consumer decision-making
24 May, 2020 from http://www.journalism. model in electronic commerce: The role of
org/2016/05/26/news-use-across-social-me trust, perceived risk, and their antecedents.
dia-platforms-2016/. Decision Support Systems, 44(2):544–564.
Grönlund, K. and Setälä, M. (2007). Political Kosinski, M., Stillwell, D., and Graepel, T.
trust, satisfaction and voter turnout. Com- (2013). Private traits and attributes are
parative European Politics, 5(4):400–422. predictable from digital records of human
Gudger, E. W. (1924). Pliny’s historia naturalis. behavior. Proceedings of the National
The most popular natural history ever pub- Academy of Sciences, 110(15), 5802–5805.
lished. Isis, 6(3):269–281. Kraemer, P. J. and Golding, J. M. (1997).
Guess, A., Nagler, J., and Tucker, J. (2019). Less Adaptive forgetting in animals. Psychonomic
than you think: Prevalence and predictors of Bulletin & Review, 4(4):480–491.
fake news dissemination on Facebook. Sci- Kramer, K. L. and Ellison, P. T. (2010). Pooled
ence Advances, 5(1), eaau4586. energy budgets: Resituating human
Hajli, N., Lin, X., Featherman, M., and Wang, Y. energy-allocation trade-offs. Evolutionary
(2014). Social word of mouth: How trust Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews,
develops in the market. International Journal 19(4):136–147.
of Market Research, 56(5):673–689. Lahr, M. M. (2016). The shaping of human
Hammack, P. L. (2008). Narrative and the cul- diversity: Filters, boundaries and transitions.
tural psychology of identity. Personality and Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Soci-
Social Psychology Review, 12(3):222–247. ety B, 371(1698):20150241.
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND ARTIFICIAL INTELLIGENCE 349

Landon-Murray, M., Mujkic, E., and Nussbaum, B. McDonald, M. M., Navarrete, C. D., and Van
(2019). Disinformation in contemporary US Vugt, M. (2012). Evolution and the psychol-
foreign policy: Impacts and ethics in an era of ogy of intergroup conflict: The male warrior
fake news, social media, and artificial intelli- hypothesis. Philosophical Transactions of the
gence. Public Integrity, 21(5):512–522. Royal Society B, 367(1589):670–679.
Lazer, D. M., Baum, M. A., Benkler, Y., Berinsky, McNamara, J. M., Stephens, P. A., Dall, S. R.,
A. J., Greenhill, K. M., Menczer, F., and and Houston, A. I. (2009). Evolution of trust
Schudson, M. (2018). The science of fake and trustworthiness: Social awareness
news. Science, 359(6380):1094–1096. favours personality differences. Proceedings
Lee, E., Bristow, J., Faircloth, C., and Macvar- of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences,
ish, J. (2014). Parenting culture studies. 276(1657):605–613.
London: Palgrave Macmillan. McPherson, M., Smith-Lovin, L., and Cook, J. M.
Lewis, M. (2017). Why the scariest nuclear threat (2001). Birds of a feather: Homophily in social
may be coming from inside the White House. networks. Annual Review of Sociology,
Vanity Fair. Retrieved 24 May, 2020, from 27(1):415–444.
https://www.vanityfair.com/news/2017/07/ Meijer, A. and Wessels, M. (2019). Predictive
department-of-energy-risks-michael-lewis. policing: Review of benefits and drawbacks.
Liang, Y. and Lee, S. A. (2017). Fear of autono- International Journal of Public Administration,
mous robots and artificial intelligence: Evi- 42(12), 1031–1039.
dence from national representative data Mitchell, D., Bryson, J. J., Rauwolf, P., and
with probability sampling. International Jour- Ingram, G. P. (2016). On the reliability of
nal of Social Robotics, 9(3):379–384. unreliable information: Gossip as cultural.
Logg, J. M., Minson, J. A., and Moore, D. A. Interaction Studies, 17(1), 1–18.
(2019). Algorithm appreciation: People Morris, N. (2002). The myth of unadulterated
prefer algorithmic to human judgment. culture meets the threat of imported
Organizational Behavior and Human Deci- media. Media, Culture & Society, 24(2):
sion Processes, 151:90–103. 278–289.
Machado, C. and Konopacki, M. (2018). Compu- Moscovici, S. (1981). On social representations.
tational power: Automated use of WhatsApp In Forgas, J. (Ed.), Social cognition: Perspec-
in the Brazilian elections. Medium. Retrieved tives on everyday understanding, 8(12):181–
24 May, 2020 from https://feed.itsrio.org/ 209, London: Academic Press.
computational-power-automated-use-of- Moscovici, S. (2001). Why a theory of social
whatsapp-in-the-elections-59f62b857033. representation? The representations of the
Manapat, M. L., Nowak, M. A., and Rand, D. Social. Bridging theoretical traditions.
G. (2013). Information, irrationality, and the 8–37.
evolution of trust. Journal of Economic Müller, V. C. and Bostrom, N. (2016). Future
Behavior & Organization, 90:S57–S75. progress in artificial intelligence: A survey of
Manapat, M. L. and Rand, D. R. (2012). expert opinion. In Müller, V. C. (Ed.), Funda-
Delayed and inconsistent information and mental Issues of Artificial Intelligence, pages
the evolution of trust. Dynamic Games and 555–572. Berlin: Springer.
Applications, 2:401–410. Nairne, J. S., Pandeirada, J. N., Gregory, K. J.,
Masuda, N. and Nakamura, M. (2012). Coevolu- and Van Arsdall, J. E. (2009). Adaptive
tion of trustful buyers and cooperative sellers memory: Fitness relevance and the hunter-
in the trust game. PLoS One, 7(9):1–11. gatherer mind. Psychological Science,
Mayer-Schönberger, V. (2007). Useful void: The 20(6):740–746.
art of forgetting in the age of ubiquitous Oosterhof, N. N. and Todorov, A. (2008). The
computing, (Working Paper No. RWP07-022). functional basis of face evaluation. Proceed-
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University. ings of the National Academy of Sciences,
McCarty, N. M., Poole, K. T., and Rosenthal, H. 105(32):11087–11092.
(2016). Polarized America: The dance of ide- Panagiotopoulos, P., Bigdeli, A. Z., and Sams, S.
ology and unequal riches. Cambridge, MA: (2014). Citizen–government collaboration on
MIT Press, second edition. social media: The case of Twitter in the 2011
350 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

riots in England. Government Information Information, Communication & Society,


Quarterly, 31(3):349–357. 18(5):569–582.
Parker, E. S., Cahill, L., and McGaugh, J. L. Roser, M. and Ortiz-Ospina, E. (2017). Global
(2006). A case of unusual autobiographical extreme poverty. Technical report, Our World
remembering. Neurocase, 12(1):35–49. in Data. Retrieved 24 December, 2019 from
Pentland, A. (2015). Social physics: How social https://ourworldindata.org/extreme-poverty.
networks can make us smarter. New York, Rotsidis, A., Theodorou, A., Bryson, J. J., and
NY: Penguin. Wortham, R. H. (2019). Improving robot
Perlman, E. R. and Sprick Schuster, S. (2016). transparency: An investigation with mobile
Delivering the vote: The political effect of augmented reality. In 28th IEEE Interna-
free mail delivery in early twentieth century tional Symposium on Robot and Human
America. The Journal of Economic History, Interactive Communication (RO-MAN), New
76(3):769–802. Delhi. IEEE.
Piette, A. (2018). Muriel Spark and fake news. Roughgarden, J., Oishi, M., and Akçay, E.
Textual Practice, 32(9):1577–1591. (2006). Reproductive social behavior: Coop-
Ponte, E. B., Carvajal-Trujillo, E., and Escobar- erative games to replace sexual selection.
Rodríguez, T. (2015). Influence of trust and Science, 311(5763):965–969.
perceived value on the intention to purchase Scharlemann, J. P., Eckel, C. C., Kacelnik, A., and
travel online: Integrating the effects of assur- Wilson, R. K. (2001). The value of a smile:
ance on trust antecedents. Tourism Manage- Game theory with a human face. Journal of
ment, 47:286–302. Economic Psychology, 22(5):617–640.
Raber, I., McCarthy, C. P., and Yeh, R. W. Scheidel, W. (2017). The great leveler: Violence
(2019). Health insurance and mobile health and the history of inequality from the Stone
devices: Opportunities and concerns. Journal Age to the twenty-first century. Princeton,
of the American Medical Association (JAMA), NJ: Princeton University Press.
321(18):1767–1768. Shi, F., Teplitskiy, M., Duede, E., and Evans, J.
Rauwolf, P. (2016). Understanding the ubiquity A. (2019). The wisdom of polarized crowds.
of self-deception: The evolutionary utility of Nature Human Behaviour, 3:329–336.
incorrect information. PhD thesis, University Siles, I. and Walker, I. D. (2009). Design, con-
of Bath. struction, and testing of a new class of
Rauwolf, P. and Bryson, J. J. (2018). Expecta- mobile robots for cave exploration. In
tions of fairness and trust co-evolve in envi- Mechatronics, 2009. ICM 2009. IEEE Inter-
ronments of partial information. Dynamic national Conference on, pages 1–6. IEEE.
Games and Applications, 8(4):891–917. Simkulet, W. (2019). Informed consent and
Rauwolf, P. and Jones, A. (2019). Exploring the nudging. Bioethics, 33(1):169–184.
utility of internal whistleblowing in healthcare Simon, H. A. (1956). Rational choice and the
via agent-based models. BMJ Open, 9(1). structure of the environment. Psychological
Rauwolf, P., Mitchell, D., and Bryson, J. J. (2015). Review, 63(2):129–138.
Value homophily benefits cooperation but Smaldino, P. E., Newson, L., Schank, J. C., and
motivates employing incorrect social infor- Richerson, P. J. (2013). Simulating the evolu-
mation. Journal of Theoretical Biology, tion of the human family: Cooperative
367:246–261. breeding increases in harsh environments.
Richerson, P. J., Bettinger, R. L., and Boyd, R. PLoS One, 8(11):e80753.
(2005). Evolution on a restless planet: Were Smith, E. A. (2010). Communication and col-
environmental variability and environmental lective action: Language and the evolution of
change major drivers of human evolution. human cooperation. Evolution and Human
In Woketits, F. M. and Ayala, F. J. (Eds.), Behavior, 31(4):231–245.
Handbook of Evolution, 2:223–242. Sparrow, R. (2019). Robotics has a race prob-
Robinson, L., Cotten, S. R., Ono, H., Quan- lem. Science, Technology, & Human Values,
Haase, A., Mesch, G., Chen, W., Schulz, J., 45(3), 538–560.
Hale, T. M., and Stern, M. J. (2015). Digital Spring, B., Gotsis, M., Paiva, A., and Spruijt-
inequalities and why they matter. Metz, D. (2013). Healthy apps: Mobile devices
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND ARTIFICIAL INTELLIGENCE 351

for continuous monitoring and intervention. Technological University. Retrieved 24 May,


IEEE Pulse, 4(6):34. 2020, from https://www.rsis.edu.sg/wp-­
Starbird, K. and Palen, L. (2011). ‘Voluntweet- content/uploads/2018/01/PR180313_Fake-
ers’: Self-organizing by digital volunteers in News_WEB.pdf
times of crisis. In Proceedings of the SIGCHI Vieweg, S., Hughes, A. L., Starbird, K., and
Conference on Human Factors in Computing Palen, L. (2010). Microblogging during two
Systems, CHI ‘11, pages 1071–1080, New natural hazards events: What Twitter may con-
York, NY. ACM. tribute to situational awareness. In Proceed-
Stewart, A. J., McCarty, N., and Bryson, J. J. ings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human
(2020). Polarization under rising inequality Factors in Computing Systems, CHI ‘10, pages
and economic decline. arXiv preprint arXiv: 1079–1088, New York, NY. ACM.
1807.11477. Vosoughi, S., Roy, D., and Aral, S. (2018).
Stone, J. and Moskowitz, G. B. (2011). Non- The spread of true and false news online.
conscious bias in medical decision making: Science, 359(6380):1146–1151.
What can be done to reduce it? Medical Waytz, A., Heafner, J., and Epley, N. (2014). The
Education, 45(8):768–776. mind in the machine: Anthropomorphism
Sujarwoto, S. and Tampubolon, G. (2016). Spa- increases trust in an autonomous vehicle.
tial inequality and the internet divide in Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,
Indonesia 2010–2012. Telecommunications 52:113–117.
Policy, 40(7):602–616. Webb, H., Jirotka, M., Stahl, B. C., Housley, W.,
Terkel, S. (1974). Working. New York: The New Edwards, A., Williams, M., Procter, R., Rana,
Press. O., and Burnap, P. (2016). Digital wildfires:
Terry, D. J. and Hogg, M. A. (1996). Group Hyper-connectivity, havoc and a global
norms and the attitude-behavior relationship: ethos to govern social media. ACM SIGCAS
A role for group identification. Personality and Computers and Society, 45(3):193–201.
Social Psychology Bulletin, 22(8):776–793. Wilson, H. and Theodorou, A. (2019). Slam the
Teste, F. P., Simard, S. W., Durall, D. M., Guy, R. D., brakes: Perceptions of moral decisions in
Jones, M. D., and Schoonmaker, A. L. (2009). driving dilemmas. In International Workshop
Access to mycorrhizal networks and roots in Artificial Intelligence Safety (AISafety),
of trees: Importance for seedling survival IJCAI, Macau.
and resource transfer. Ecology, 90(10): Wilson, R. K. and Eckel, C. C. (2006). Judging
2808–2822. a book by its cover: Beauty and expectations
Theodorou, A., Wortham, R. H., and Bryson, J. J. in the trust game. Political Research Quarterly,
(2017). Designing and implementing transpar- 59(2):189–202.
ency for real time inspection of autonomous Woods, L. (2018). ICO reacts to use of data ana-
robots. Connection Science, 29(3):230–241. lytics in micro-targetting for political purposes
Toma, C. L. (2010). Perceptions of trustworthi- reports: United Kingdom. European Data Pro-
ness online: The role of visual and textual tection Law Review (EDPL), 4:381–383.
information. In Proceedings of the 2010 Wu, Y., Kosinski, M., and Stillwell, D. (2015).
ACM Conference on Computer Supported Computer-based personality judgments are
Cooperative Work, pages 13–22. ACM. more accurate than those made by humans.
Tucker, B. (2018). ‘That’s problematic’: Tracing Proceedings of the National Academy of
the birth of call-out culture. Critical Reflec- Sciences, 112(4):1036–1040.
tions: A Student Journal on Contemporary Yamagishi, T. (2011). Trust: The evolutionary
Sociological Issues, 6. game of mind and society. Berlin: Springer,
United Nations General Assembly (1948). Uni- Berlin.
versal declaration of human rights. Technical Zahavi, A. (1977). The testing of a bond.
report, New York. Animal Behaviour, 25:246–247.
Vasu, N., Ang, B., Teo, T.-A., Jayakumar, S., Zuboff, S. (2015). Big other: Surveillance capi-
Raizal, M., and Ahuja, J. (2018). Fake news: talism and the prospects of an information
National security in the post-truth era. RSIS. civilization. Journal of Information Technology,
In Policy Report. Singapore: Nanyang 30(1):75–89.
18
Evolutionary Psychology
and Robotics
Robert Finkelstein

Evolutionary psychology can impact intelligent e.g., memory, perception, and language, just
robots in two different ways: as they do to physiological traits (Buss, 2005).
Darwinian evolution is a well-tested scientific
1 The expression of evolutionary psychology in the theory that explains how organic complexity
human brain and emergent mind allows it to create can arise from simplicity. Evolutionary psy-
intelligent robots, with some of their development chology is expected to provide a basis for
focused on duplicating the neurophysiology of
explaining the origin of the human brain, the
the human brain and the cognition of the human
mind. As with many complex technologies in the most complex entity in the known universe
past, it is uncertain whether human psychology (Aunger, 2002), its manifestation in the human
will be sufficient to manage the consequences of mind, and the mind’s psychological phenom-
its own invention. ena. The process for the evolution of the
2 The future expression of evolutionary psychology, autonomous intelligent robot, however, will
albeit with a different theoretical framework, will be Lamarckian evolution and not Darwinian
affect the autonomous intelligent robot in the evolution.
physical structure of its brain and the cognition of In biology, the discredited Lamarckian
its mind. theory of evolution claims that when the
environment changes the phenotype of an
organism changes, such that the organism
ROBOT EVOLUTION: LAMARCK is better adapted to the environmental factor
VS DARWIN that caused the change (Hull, 2000), and the
genotype can then pass the improved adapta-
Evolutionary psychology holds that Darwinian tion to the organism’s progeny (albeit, hav-
processes of natural selection apply to the ing died in 1829, Jean-Baptiste Lamarck
brain, mind, and resulting psychological traits, knew nothing of genes or the genotype).
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND ROBOTICS 353

Recent research has shown that for at least a networks, can produce robot behavior that is
few organisms, such as certain roundworms, as unpredictable and inexplicable as human
pseudo-Lamarckian evolution seems to take behavior. As robot cognition increases over
place by passing to its progeny, independent time, it is likely that robot minds will increas-
of its genome, an individual’s acquired immu- ingly exhibit psychological phenomena.
nity to a virus (Rechavi, 2009). Technology is The scientist and science-fiction writer
sometimes said to advance with Lamarckian Isaac Asimov anticipated the need, in the 21st
evolution, rather than with Darwinian evo- century, for what he called robopsychologists
lution, because of the high rate of techno- to analyze and cure the mental illness that
logical change compared with the relative he expected to emerge in some intelligent
slowness of Darwinian evolution, and the machines, and which would engender behav-
ability of changes in one generation of a ior that is dangerous to humans (Asimov,
technology to be passed on to the next gen- 1950). With the evolution of autonomous
eration. However, technological change was intelligent robots following Lamarckian,
not always this rapid for Homo sapiens and rather than Darwinian, principles, the study
its ancestors; a flint-cutting tool might have of robopsychology will be needed sooner
taken thousands of years before manifesting rather than later.
some improvement. In any event, the prin-
ciples of Lamarckian evolution with respect
to technological evolution have been a meta-
phor, not the actual evolutionary process that A COMPUTATIONAL THEORY OF
was claimed to take place in an organism. In MIND
the future, however, it might indeed become
the actual process for the evolution of robots. In 1943 Warren McCulloch and Walter Pitts
If a future robot experiences an incident that concluded that neurons perform computa-
causes it to change its behavior, physiologi- tional processes (McCulloch and Pitts, 1943).
cal structure, or cognitive ability to better Subsequently, the basis for a computational
survive or improve its functionality, it could theory (actually, a hypothesis) of mind was
then pass the improvement to its progeny. formed from the confluence of several disci-
The robot would create its descendants via plines: neuroscience, cognitive psychology,
autopoiesis (i.e., self-replication), albeit with AI, robotics, information science, and semiot-
improvements. Furthermore, such a robot ics. It envisions that the mind can be modeled
would be able to communicate globally with as a set of processes which have computa-
its contemporaries and could convince them tional equivalents (Albus and Meystel, 2001;
to implement the improvements immediately. Meystel and Albus, 2002). If such a theory
The advent of autonomous vehicles and were established on a foundation of evidence,
other robots, made possible by advances it could be tested experimentally with soft-
in neural networks with deep learning ware and hardware. Machines, such as robots,
(Goodfellow et  al., 2016) and other artifi- could be built which model the brain and test
cial intelligence (AI) tools and techniques, whether a human-like mind emerges from the
processing chips, communications networks, machine brain. This test would be a more
knowledgebases, and sensors, will become comprehensive version of the Turing test for
a foundational technology leading to mani- machine intelligence.
fold kinds of autonomous intelligent robots The computational theory of the human
over the coming decades. Over the ensuing mind intersects with evolutionary psychology
decades the cognitive ability of robots will in holding that the human brain is a system of
evolve to replicate or surpass human capa- computational subsystems designed by natu-
bilities. Some AI processes, such as neural ral selection over eons in our Homo sapiens
354 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

ancestors and their ancestors (e.g., Homo To further explain the meaning of some of
habilis, Homo erectus, Homo heidelbergen- the computational equivalents in the table,
sis, etc.) for foraging, hunting, reproducing, consider that:
and outmaneuvering entities such as animals,
plants, objects in the environment, and other “meaning is the set of semantic relationships that
exist between the internal knowledge database
hominids. It postulates that our cognition
and the external world. Meaning establishes what
and emotions are produced by evolved algo- is intended or meant by behavioral actions, and
rithms computed by our subconscious mind. defines what entities, events, and situations in the
Machines can be very good at developing knowledge database refer to in the world.”
and computing algorithms, often better than “Understanding occurs when the system’s internal
representation of external reality is adequate for
Homo sapiens.
generating intelligent behavior.” “Planning is a
A consequence of a computational theory thinking process whereby a system imagines the
of mind is that the mind consists of a set of future and selects the best course of action to
processes, each of which has a computational achieve a goal state.” “Awareness is a condition
equivalent. These processes include emotion, wherein a system has knowledge of the structure,
dynamics, and meaning of the environment in
imagination, thought, reason, feeling, percep-
which it exists.” “Consciousness is a state or condi-
tion, knowledge, cognition, meaning, under- tion in which an intelligent system is aware of itself,
standing, belief, planning, wisdom, attention, its surroundings, its situation, its intentions, and its
awareness, consciousness, intelligence, a feelings.” (Albus and Mystel, pp xii–xiii, 2001)
sense of good and evil, a sense of justice and
duty, and an appreciation of beauty. Each of Despite centuries of philosophical argument,
these processes can be represented in robots, it is now generally agreed that the mind is
as shown in Table 18.1 (from Albus, 2001): what the brain does, so there is no need to

Table 18.1  Processes of mind and their computational equivalents


Processes of the human mind Computational equivalents

Emotion Value judgment; evaluation of good and bad


Imagination Modeling, and simulation; visualization
Thought Analysis of what is imagined
Reason Logic applied to thought
Feeling Experience of sensory input or emotional state
Perception Transforming sensory data into knowledge
Knowledge Information organized so it is useful
Cognition Analysis, evaluation, and use of knowledge
Meaning Relationships between forms of knowledge
Understanding Correspondence between model and reality
Belief Level of confidence assigned to knowledge
Planning Examination of possible future actions
Wisdom Decisions likely to achieve long-term goals
Attention Focusing perception on what is important
Awareness Operational state of perceptual processes
Consciousness Operational state of cognitive processes
Intelligence Ability to achieve purpose or goals despite uncertainty
Sense of good and evil Value judgment output
Sense of justice and duty Value judgment output
Appreciation of beauty Value judgment output

Source: Albus (2001).


EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND ROBOTICS 355

invoke mysterious spiritual phenomena and procedures, tactics and strategies, facial
outside the realm of science. Likewise, the recognition, natural language (in multi-
­
robot mind consists of the computational ple languages) with the ability to banter or
processes of the robot brain. And its structure employ formal procedural language, and
and process will be subject to technological have general situational awareness with, for
evolution. example, the ability to discern subtle impor-
Thus the brain, whether human or robot, tant cues from the environment, then the
is primarily a control system, generating and robot will have to know much more than how
controlling behavior. It likely developed in to drive, just as its human equivalent would.
ancient primitive organisms to control mobil- Likewise, the intelligence of household
ity and path planning to avoid threats and robots may vary, depending on the needs and
take advantage of opportunities for survival wants of their owners.
and reproduction.
Any control system is designed, by nature
or humans, to achieve or maintain a goal (a
desired result or state of the system or world). ROBOT INTELLIGENCE, LEARNING,
Organisms with many offspring can sur- ADAPTATION, WISDOM, AND SELF-
vive with simple brains; higher intelligence AWARENESS
allows those with fewer offspring to survive
with higher probability. Robots need not have Autonomous robots will have varying
greater intelligence or more complex cogni- degrees of intelligence and ability to learn
tion than humans. They should only have and adapt. Robots with human-level intelli-
the ‘IQ’ needed to perform their functions, gence or greater will also need wisdom if
perhaps insect-level intelligence. Excessive humanity is to avoid the oft-predicted Robot
intelligence would be inefficient economi- Apocalypse. Robots will also need their
cally and operationally, perhaps even inter- behavior to be guided by an ethics and
fering with the ability of the machine to do morality, but wisdom provides the frame-
its job well. work for ethical and moral behavior.
However, many jobs will require robots to In a pragmatic definition, an intelligent
have human-level intelligence or better, some system is a system with the ability to act
with high levels of expertise that is narrowly appropriately (or make an appropriate choice
focused, such as for performing colonosco- or decision) in an uncertain environment.
pies, but others will need high levels of cog- An appropriate action (or choice) is that
nition that is broad in scope, such as being a which maximizes the probability of success-
fifth-grade school teacher where knowledge fully achieving the system’s mission, func-
and skill for interacting with human students tion, purpose, or goals (Meystel and Albus,
is needed along with knowledge for effective 2002). Machine intelligence need not be at
teaching. Some robots will need greater intel- the human level. For example, appropriate
ligence in order to interact with humans in autonomous vehicle intelligence is the abil-
a desirable way, despite their simpler basic ity of an autonomous vehicle to perform at
jobs. For example, an autonomous car need least as skilled as a good human driver would
only know how to drive in complex envi- under a variety of conditions. It does not
ronments in order to perform its basic func- need to know how to play chess or make an
tion as a chauffeur. But if human passengers omelet, although it could be given additional
would like to converse with the car in natu- cognition to enhance its abilities to perform
ral language on manifold topics, or if the functions other than driving. The appropriate
car is to be a partner of a law-enforcement robot intelligence for an application depends
officer or a soldier and must know policies on the user’s requirements and the technical,
356 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

operational, and economical feasibility of robots if they are to survive among them.
achieving the desired level of intelligence. The love of (or search for) wisdom is the
Many robots may only require insect-level original meaning of the word philosophy.
intelligence to perform their functions, but But the notion of wisdom is a bit elusive.
even these robots will need behavioral con- According to Martin Henry Fischer (1879–
straints to avoid running amok. Bees, ants, 1962), knowledge is a process of aggregating
and termites, for example, work together with facts while wisdom is their simplification.
their hive or colony cohort in a purposive way Our preferred definition of wisdom is: the
and generally avoid killing each other. ability to see the forest as well as the trees.
There are many definitions of learning. A decision by a human or robot may seem to
Our preferred synthesized definition of learn- be the right one when considered in the con-
ing is the acquisition of knowledge, skill, text of the details of the problem or the short
ability, or understanding by study, instruc- term, i.e., the trees, or in the context of the
tion, or experience, as evidenced by achiev- big picture or the long term, i.e., the forest,
ing growing success (improved behavior), but not both. For example, some immediate
with respect to suitable metrics, in a fixed or near-term solutions for problems may later
environment. Learning takes place when the become problems themselves, while some
system’s behavior increases the efficiency long-term solutions for problems may cause
with which data, information, and knowl- new near-term problems.
edge is processed so that desirable states are Self-awareness is the ability to simulate
reached, errors avoided, or a portion of the oneself, as in a mental world model. A robot
system’s environment is controlled. Robots can be considered to be self-aware if it is able
with varying degrees of intelligence will to predict what it will perceive in the future
be capable of learning as may be required based on its current state and behavior, and
by their applications, even robots with rela- use those predictions to plan future behavior
tively low intelligence. Even Caenorhabditis (Albus and Meystel, 2001).
elegans, a worm one millimeter in length
with just 302 neurons, is capable of learning
(Ardiel and Rankin, 2010).
In our view of learning is different from RULE-BASED SYSTEMS, NEURAL
adaptation. Our definition of adaptation is a NETWORKS, AND GENETIC
change in behavior (or structure) in response ALGORITHMS
to a changed environment. An adapted system
is able to maintain critical or essential vari- There are a number of AI tools which can be
ables within physical (or physiological) limits used to evolve the robot mind to higher cog-
(e.g., homeostasis). Adaptation occurs where nition and greater sentience, including sym-
the changed behavior (or structure) increases bolic processes, connectionist processes, and
the probability that the system can achieve genetic algorithms. It is likely that a synthe-
its function or purpose (e.g., maintain homeo- sis of these AI tools, perhaps along with
stasis) by adjusting to the new or changed envi- some that have not been invented yet, might
ronment. Thus, in our view, learning occurs in be required.
a fixed environment while adaptation occurs Symbolic processes, consisting of rule-based
in a changed environment. (or expert) systems, have been used success-
To understand wisdom requires wisdom fully to replicate specialized human intelli-
(Sternberg, 1995). While there are many gence or expertise within narrow domains of
projects to develop robot learning and intel- knowledge (Hayes-Roth et  al., 1983). Expert
ligence, there are none yet to develop robot systems, featuring strings of if-then format-
wisdom, an ability humans may need in their ted rules (Durkin, 1994), have achieved some
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND ROBOTICS 357

degree of success in commercial and military neural networks, expert systems, genetic
applications. They are suitable for such algorithms, and Bayesian inference. But the
autonomous robot tasks as high-level plan- major approach has been using ANN, which
ning, navigation, tactics, strategies, and por- have been developed for many decades but
tions of sensory processing, such as high-level until recently have only had limited success
vision. Conventional symbolic (knowledge- in achieving machine learning. In recent years
based) processes are limited by the ability there has been significant progress because
of a human expert to foresee situations and of improvements with neural network soft-
environments that a robot will encounter, ware and hardware, which are also referred
and to program, with sufficient if-then rules, to as deep learning and deep neural networks
appropriate responses to threats and opportu- (DNN). Early ANN had one layer of eight
nities that the robot may encounter. neurons; new DNN can contain more than
However, rule-based systems can be 150 layers of neurons, interconnected with
designed to learn, to modify themselves, about a billion connections. One DNN has
which would greatly broaden their domain. 60 million tunable parameters and 650,000
A robot interacting with the environment neurons (Krizhevsky et al., 2012; Lipson and
and encountering a situation that contradicts Kurman, 2016). They need fast and powerful
an existing rule, or does not have a rule to computers and advanced training algorithms
address the current situation, could modify an to function. As an example of the progress
existing rule to accommodate the new real- of deep learning, consider that an untrained
ity or create a new rule. Rule-based systems human can categorize images (i.e., identify
could be used to address a current weakness the content of images) with a 5% error rate,
of AI – its general inability to deal with con- which was far better than machine vision
text, such as the relationship of entities with until recently (Lipson and Kurman, 2016).
time, space, and causality (Brézillon, 1999).
• 2010: The winning ANN in a machine vision com-
Deep learning, for example, is not generally
petition categorized 100,000 test images with
suitable for teaching a robot context, such as 28% error rate.
how the existence and interaction of entities • 2011: The winning ANN categorized test images
relate to each other and the world in terms with 25% error rate, a slight improvement.
of spatial and temporal phenomena, or the • 2012: Deep learning emerged and the winning
chains of causal events (Edell, 2018). For ANN (a convolutional neural network) catego-
example, a rule-based system could state that rized test images with 15% error rate, a major
if a non-moveable object, like the Brooklyn improvement.
Bridge, is in one place, like New York City, • 2013: The winning DNN categorized test images
it cannot also be in another place, like next with 11.2% error rate.
to the Golden Gate Bridge in San Francisco. • 2014: The winning DNN categorized test images
with 6.66% error rate.
Or it can be instructed in, or directly expe-
• 2015: The winning DNN categorized test images
rience if it is in a robot, causality; e.g., if a with 3.57% error rate, beating human capability
person trips on a crack in the sidewalk, that (Lipson and Kurman, 2016).
may cause him to fall; and if he falls, he may
be injured. While DNN seems to be an effective and
Connectionist processes, consisting of arti- efficient approach to many intelligent
ficial neural networks (ANN) (Goodfellow machine applications, nevertheless there are
et  al., 2016), are suited for deep learning many problems. For example, DNN cannot
and autonomic responses in robots, such learn by analogy, as humans and animals can
as avoiding an imminent crash into a tree. do, that objects possess common attributes.
Machines can be designed to learn (or adapt) Despite their many shapes and sizes, humans
in various ways, including such AI tools as can quickly generalize the notion of a chair
358 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

or lamp. So far, DNN has failed to learn the based on DNA). As in nature, simple mecha-
concept of sameness and could not learn by nisms can generate complex structures and
analogy the underlying concepts linking simi- adaptive behaviors.
lar objects in a training set of images of those Genetic programming is an extension
objects (Lipson and Kurman, 2016). Also, of conventional GAs (Koza, 1992). It is a
DNN is not transparent and the reasons for its method for automatically creating a work-
outcomes are often inexplicable. It does not ing computer program from a high-level
learn like humans. People learn by abstracting statement of the problem, i.e., the program-
the principles underlying a few examples, mer need not specify the program code. It
while DNN needs massive databases to learn accomplishes this by genetically breeding a
(Lipson and Kurman, 2016). So far DNN, population of computer programs using the
while accomplished in machine learning, has principles of Darwinian natural selection and
not provided machines with human-like intel- biologically inspired operations. Genetic pro-
ligence or cognition. It seems that machines, gramming iteratively transforms a population
such as robots, must interact with the world to of computer programs into a new generation
discover the intuitive physics upon which cog- of programs by applying analogs of naturally
nitive inference depends. occurring genetic operations. As with GAs,
The application of genetic algorithms the genetic operations include crossover
(GAs) for robot evolution provides a hybrid (sexual recombination), mutation, reproduc-
case where Darwinian evolution can be used tion, gene duplication, and gene deletion.
to impel the rapid evolution of robot tech- The conventional robot selects a pre-­
nology, including the evolution of the robot programmed strategy (on the basis of infor-
mind. It is a Darwinian basis for Lamarckian mation stored in its databases, memory, and
technological evolution. world model, and acquired from the inter-
GAs (Goldberg, 1989; Davis, 1991) are action of its sensors with the internal and
mathematical algorithms that transform a external environments), to meet a defined
set (or population) of individual mathemati- goal or achieve a functional purpose. The
cal entities (typically fixed-length character conventional approach requires complex and
strings patterned after chromosome strings), costly preparation for the development of
each with an associated fitness value, into algorithms for each specific environment in
a new population (i.e., the next generation) which the robot is to operate. GAs require
using operations patterned after the Darwinian goals, feedback mechanisms, and sensory
principle of reproduction and survival of the access to the environment to learn to oper-
fittest and after naturally occurring genetic ate effectively and efficiently. They require
operations, such as sexual recombination. minimal engineering or programming. The
GAs are run in high-speed digital simula- designer need not be prescient about every
tions containing entities and their environ- possible contingency to write a long series
ment. Millions of simulations, representing of if-then rules (as for expert systems). The
the equivalent of millions of years of passing designer does not have to expend the time
time in the real world and millions of gen- and effort to train the system, as is the case
erations of living and dying, can be run in for most neural networks and deep learn-
seconds or minutes of processing time. The ing. GAs enable the simulated robot to adapt
GAs typically cause, over simulated genera- continuously, to cope with uncertain or
tions, the progressive modification of some changing environments, and to allow the
structure or structures, which may then be system (e.g., robot) to perform successfully
reflected in the behavior of the system of while becoming strategically prepared for
which the structures are a part (as animal future circumstances. This not only reduces
behavior is a reflection of genetic structures the cost and exertion of programming, but
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND ROBOTICS 359

enables the robot to adapt to a broader range sequential structures, a large number of
of environments (Schultz et al., 1993). patterns, or schemata, are implicitly tested.
­
For example, a simulated autonomous For a robot control system, for example, each
robot aircraft can learn, over millions of schemata may be a control strategy associated
attempts, to land safely on the simulated with multiple sensor inputs.
rolling, pitching, yawing deck of an aircraft Each structure in a relatively small set is
carrier – or how to land on various irregular tested against the environment, as genetically
surfaces. A simulated legged robot can learn designed organisms in nature are tested against
how to best control its legs to negotiate an the environment. Three primary operators
uneven terrain, and adapt to walking success- are used to combine the elements of two
fully over various kinds of difficult terrain. selected structures to form a new structure
The algorithms and physical designs (e.g., (Goldberg, 1989):
for legs) that are engineered in the simulated
• crossover: each structure is represented as a vector.
evolutionary generations can then be built A point along this vector is selected, and the new
into the real-world robot. The psychology of structure takes all elements before (and at) this
the real-world robot can be advanced by mil- position from one parent structure, and all of the
lions of generations of simulated evolution of elements after this point from the other. Crossover
simulated robots (Schultz et al., 1993). is the most used primary operator;
If a robot had sufficient computational • inversion: crossover produces a bias toward
power, GAs could be run within the brain of propagating closely linked schemata (i.e., sche-
the real-world robot. For example, if a robot mata with short physical distances between
were to encounter an unexpected problem or elements). Inversion compensates for this bias by
environment, it could pause in its movement causing successful schemata to become increas-
ingly closely linked physically; and
and run its own GAs to arrive at a solution
• mutation: over successive trials potentially impor-
that optimizes or satisfies an objective func- tant values in particular positions may disappear
tion, allowing the robot to solve the problem from the population of structures. A low mutation
or survive a dangerous situation. rate periodically reintroduces lost values.
The basic elements of the GA consist of
representations, patterns (or structures), and Information about large amounts of schemata
schemata (Goldberg, 1989), as follows: is processed at each stage of the simulation,
with subsequent trials making use of this
• representation: a blank vector of elements (vari- information, which is genetically embedded in
ables) which can take on numerical values;
the structures. The structures reproduce prob-
• pattern (or structure): numerical values are incor-
abilistically and the probabilities are assigned
porated into the representation; and
• schemata: sets of structures which are, in effect, by the algorithm on the basis of observed
strategies for optimizing fitness criteria. performance against fitness criteria. Over suc-
cessive trials, the expected number of occur-
GAs prescribe the maintenance of a small set rences of schemata mirrors their performance,
of structures and provide guidelines for the e.g., the ability of a robot to walk, swim, or
construction of new structures (Goldberg, play tennis against humans. Schemata that
1989). The structures generally combine in have demonstrated to be high-performance by
pairs (the equivalent of sexual reproduction) at surviving trials must reproduce themselves
a rate based on their observed performance. while new structures are also being generated
Each time the two structures reproduce to pro- and tested. There is an algorithm for assigning
duce a new structure, the new structure main- trials to structures which test schemata.
tains the optimal properties expected to result This algorithm must maximize current fit-
from the initial union, while simultaneously ness while also exploring new schemata.
correcting for biases in the algorithm. By testing Thousands of generations of simulated entities,
360 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

such as robots, are born, live, reproduce, and 100 deaths. If social media existed at the time
die. The equivalent of millions of years of natu- as it does now, generating public outrage over
ral evolution takes place in brief computer- the accidental automotive deaths, perhaps we
processing time (Goldberg, 1989). would still be riding in horse-drawn carriages.
The compressed Darwinian evolution of The goal for autonomous (robotic) vehicles is
a robot, including that of its mind and psy- zero deaths (National Safety Council, 2019).
chology, can be effected in the context of the The goal may not be achieved, but automotive
Lamarckian evolution of computer simulation casualties could become as rare as deaths from
technology. sharks (about six per year).
Psycho-social factors (as opposed to, for
example, engineering or technical factors) are
critical in the development and acceptance of
TECHNOLOGY AND PSYCHO-SOCIAL most – or all – technologies. For example, to
FACTORS compensate for a shortage of suitable wood in
the 19th century, some pencils were designed
Evolutionary psychology provides an account to be sheathed with paper instead of wood.
of the psycho-social gestalt and psycho-social Functionally, it worked well. But psychologi-
factors concerning technology, not just the cally people preferred to sharpen their pen-
efficacy of the technology. It will have a sig- cils by whittling them with a knife or a pencil
nificant influence on the public’s acceptance sharpener, rather than peeling the paper. The
of autonomous robots. As a consequence of paper pencil eventually became defunct, except
our psychology, one death is a tragedy while a for special-purpose applications (Teich, 2009).
million deaths is a statistic, as the Soviet dicta- New technologies are now critically evalu-
tor Joseph Stalin is alleged to have said. For ated in the context of aesthetics, such as
example, a few isolated accidental deaths environmental and design considerations.
caused by autonomous cars or other robots, Developers of technology, such as scientists
which are widely publicized by the media, and engineers, are influenced, consciously or
might elicit an exaggerated emotional response unconsciously, by the organizational and soci-
by the public, slowing or stifling the develop- etal culture in which they reside, which is a
ment of the technology. The National Highway manifestation of evolutionary psychology. The
Traffic Safety Administration (NHTSA) institutionalization of science and engineering
reports nearly 37,000 deaths in the United means that once a decision (even a bad one)
States from automotive accidents during 2018, is made concerning the development and com-
despite major improvement in vehicle safety mercialization of a technology, the momentum
driver-assist systems. The fatality rate per of the organizational bureaucracy and the pro-
vehicle mile traveled (VMT) has decreased cesses of systems engineering drive the prod-
greatly over the decades with improving auto- uct to its conclusion (Teich, 2009). Adverse
motive technology and infrastructure, but at unintended consequences can ensue.
the same time the number of vehicle miles
traveled increased greatly (e.g., with more
miles traveled per vehicle and more vehicles
on the road), contributing to the number of TECHNOLOGY AND UNINTENDED
deaths. But the public has become inured to CONSEQUENCES
these deaths (Evans, 2008). More to the point,
according to NHTSA there were 26 deaths in Ancient legend tells us that Prometheus
the United States from automotive accidents defied Zeus and brought technological
in 1899 when there were about 8,000 horse- knowledge, in the form of fire and the skill of
less carriages. By 1903 there were more than metalwork, to the human race. Zeus then
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND ROBOTICS 361

punished him for this by having him bound territory. Genetics, nanotechnology, and robot-
to a rock while a huge eagle ate his liver ics all offer the possibility of self-replicating
every day, only to have it grow back to be technology – autopoiesis. It is necessary to
eaten again the next day (Cartwright, 2013). examine the potential adverse consequences
New technology when first introduced can of new technology, for much of technology
have serious and dangerous flaws. For exam- has adverse consequences. Often the benefits
ple, early methods of vaccination for small- of the new technology exceed its adverse
pox could cause smallpox – and death (Teich, consequences, or its disadvantages can be
2009). Manufacturers of early high-pressure mitigated by improving – evolving – the tech-
steam engines often used inferior materials, nology. Cultural and technological systems
and poor design and construction contributed do not develop independently; the two evolve
to major malfunctions (Teich, 2009). Even together in complex feedback loops, wherein
well-built engines needed improved technol- each drives, restrains, and accelerates change
ogy to avoid catastrophic failures. Aside in the other. Darwinian evolutionary psychol-
from the engines, early railroads needed better ogy underlies human culture and its values,
technology for bridges and tracks to avoid while Lamarckian evolutionary psychology
death and destruction (Teich, 2009). Many will underlie robot culture and values.
technological advances had unexpected and
unwanted side effects. Associated with a tech-
nological advance having major benefits
might be unintended consequences causing AUTONOMOUS INTELLIGENT
death and destruction. Our ancestors gener- ROBOTS: A DISRUPTIVE AND
ally did not abandon the flawed technology TRANSFORMATIVE TECHNOLOGY
but persisted in trying to improve it, either
mitigating the adverse consequences or The first generation of fully autonomous
accepting them as a tradeoff for the benefits. intelligent robots will be vehicles: autono-
There is a tension between new technol- mous cars, trucks, and buses. This will be a
ogy and the preservation of individual and disruptive and transformative technology,
societal values. The new technology may whether for civilian or military applications.
threaten the values of some, while efforts to The technology will be disruptive because it
mitigate the adverse consequences of the new will cause old industries and companies to
technology may threaten the values of others. vanish while new ones emerge. With creative
Some people might then conclude that tech- destruction, jobs lost in one industry will be
nology is inherently hostile to human values. replaced by new jobs in another. The technol-
Technology has a direct impact on values ogy will be transformative because it will
because it generally increases the range of change society. The robots will alter military
choice and opportunity, and one’s values tactics, strategy, and doctrine; the national
cause one to make certain choices and pursue economy and power; political policy; cul-
particular opportunities. New technology can tural values and societal structures; and how
lead to a change in individual or cultural val- and where people live, work, and play.
ues by either bringing some previously unat- As an example of a disruptive and trans-
tainable goal within the realm of choice or formative technology similar to that of
by making some values easier to implement autonomous vehicles, consider the advent of
(Teich, 2009). the horseless carriage at the turn of the 19th
Ultimately, intelligent robots might drive century. The older technology at the time,
humans to extinction maliciously or unin- the horse and buggy, was a kind of intelli-
tentionally by competing more successfully gent vehicle in that the horse was sufficiently
for limited resources of matter, energy, and smart to avoid objects in the road, and could
362 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

even be autonomous, e.g., if the driver was organism design, by 1920 there were just
drunk or fell asleep, the horse could find its 100 US ­manufacturers with their automotive
way home. Horses pulling milk-delivery wag- designs converging to new industry stand-
ons learned their routes and did not require ards. By 1929 only 44 automotive manufac-
guidance from a human driver. The horse and turers remained in the United States, which
buggy (or cart) was an ancient, well-known decreased to 11 by 1976. In the early 1900s,
technology, with support from known goods only a few thousand cars were sold annually.
providers (e.g., buggy manufacturers, tack- For example, Ford sold 1,700 cars in 1904, its
equipment manufacturers, and horse breed- first year of business. After introducing factory
ers), service providers (e.g., blacksmiths, mass production in 1913 and thereby drasti-
livery stables, veterinarians), and infrastruc- cally reducing the cost of cars, Ford sold over
ture providers (e.g., horse troughs and barns). a million cars in 1920 (Wikipedia, 2019a).
The technology was acceptably reliable and The horseless carriage was a disruptive
able to traverse bad roads. Problems with the technology (The Economist, 2015). Across
technology included: daily storage mainte- many sectors of the economy it drove the
nance (barn, food, water, grooming, etc.) for creation of new technologies, industries,
the horse, whether it was used or not; a grow- and infrastructure, such as the oil industry,
ing manure problem in urban areas; and the asphalt and concrete roads, bridges, tires, fac-
cost to buy and maintain it. tory mass production, etc. Many, but not all,
The earliest horseless carriages were an horse- and buggy-related goods and services
unknown technology: unreliable, slow, and firms were driven out of business. The horse
very expensive, with no infrastructure (e.g., and buggy market shriveled but did not disap-
no suitable roads, available parts, fuel sta- pear entirely. To this day they remain common
tions, mechanics, rules and regulations, or in Amish areas of Pennsylvania and other
an understanding of the technology by the states. Most new entrants in the automotive
public). Some advantages over the existing industry also disappeared as the technology
technology included: no daily maintenance if advanced and designs, production, products,
not used; faster; more easily and compactly infrastructure, and marketplace standard-
stored; more compact fuel; no manure; and ized and stabilized. Punctuated equilibrium
status for early adopters. But the cost of the (Gould, 1983), as it does in natural evolution,
new technology was still too expensive for caused occasional disruptions to the industry,
most people. Early cars and manufactur- with paradigm shifts due to new technology,
ers proliferated, with varied technology and government regulation, or customer expecta-
designs (e.g., should it be a steering lever or tions. The current technology paradigm shift
a wheel; an internal or external combustion is driven by the technologies for energy, elec-
engine or electric motor?). As with natural tronics, and ergonomics, including electric
evolution during the Cambrian period when propulsion to reduce pollution, driver-assist
nature was experimenting with manifold systems to increase safety, and autonomous
new designs, new body plans, for complex vehicles.
animals, automotive designs evolved rapidly The horseless carriage was a transforma-
during the first two decades of the automo- tive technology (The Economist, 2015). It
tive age. Around the turn of the century there changed cultural values and societal struc-
were more than 2,000 automotive manu- ture. It caused the fashion industry to design
facturers across the United States, of which clothing to accommodate the car. It caused
many were startups in home garages, creat- the rise of suburbs, increased travel among
ing a proliferation of automotive designs the populace, and increased the homogeni-
(Wikipedia, 2019b). But as in the Cambrian zation of society. It gave rise to industry in
period with the evolutionary winnowing of place of agriculture and city living in place
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND ROBOTICS 363

of rural living. It abetted the influence of ROBOT EMOTION


massive new corporations. It changed family
dynamics with teenage and women drivers; There are development projects to give robots
the rise of teenage influence and their dating emotion (Hall, 2017), or the appearance to
customs, with the car as bedroom-on-wheels; humans of emotion. The robots are designed
and the loss of power by the husband and to interpret certain internal states as the equiv-
father. It increased family wealth, freedom, alent of human emotions, such as happy, sad,
and mobility, and contributed to the rise of angry, etc., and to express those emotions with
the middle class. It caused many casualties facial expressions and body language. The
due to crashes. In the long term it caused idea is for robots to be better able to interact
geopolitical and environmental changes, and work with people. Human emotion is
with the rise of oil cartels, religious and tribal based on algorithmic bio-chemical processes
conflicts in oil-rich lands, wars over oil, and which evolved to solve specific adaptive
environmental degradation leading to climate problems, e.g., the fear of being eaten and the
change and the potential destruction of much
pleasure of eating food and having sex
of the Earth’s species and living space for
(LeDoux, 2012). Emotions are also an impor-
humans.
tant part of generating and controlling human
Autonomous intelligent vehicles have a
behavior, as employed regularly by priests and
limited but sufficient domain of knowledge
politicians (Zaki, 2013). Rationality and logic
and ability, but they will serve as a kind of
are not necessarily what motivates people to
Ur-technology for all kinds of autonomous
achieve goals. Human emotion is an important
robots. Their success will serve as the basis
mechanism for making certain kinds of deci-
for developing and technologically evolving
sions, such as when time is critical or data are
autonomous robots into many robot ‘species’
able to perform many jobs across all eco- unavailable and objective analysis is not feasi-
nomic sectors. Biomimetic robots will prolif- ble. Computers can perform detailed analyses
erate, including humanoid and multi-legged much faster than people and until recently
chimera robots, like centaur robots; zoomor- emotion was largely ignored in the develop-
phic robots with forms like snakes, lizards, ment of intelligent machines. Robots, how-
fish, octopuses, primates, birds, insects, etc. ever, will be moving about and interacting
Disruption will spread across all sectors of with the environment. As humans do, they will
the economy, and changes in how people live, encounter sudden life-and-death situations in
work, and play will continue for the rest of which decisions cannot be made rationally,
the century. The technological evolution of where emotion, or its robot equivalent, may be
robots and the AI for their minds will con- called upon to make the decision.
tinue unabated across the globe despite any Emotions can represent an aggregation of
efforts of one nation or another to slow or certain critical memories (LeDoux, 1996),
stop its progress. Technology that is feasible allowing for a quick, nearly autonomic, fight-
and useful is hardy and always emerges like or-flight response (e.g., run from the tiger!).
a weed in a crack in the sidewalk. Perhaps Emotions have a profound effect on cognition
human psychology will be changed in the (Damasio, 2003), determining what is good or
future by genetic engineering or some other bad, loved or hated, important or unimportant,
technology and will be better able to cope beautiful or ugly, remembered or forgotten.
with the results of the mind’s creations Humans and other animals seem to have simi-
(Smith, 2019). It seems that the best way lar, if not identical, emotions because of the
to avoid the proverbial robot apocalypse is similarity of the physiological neural system
for people to be at least as smart as their and the evolution of the portions of the brain
robots. involved with emotions (LeDoux, 2012).
364 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

It seems likely that emotional-state variables be just one of the many delusions we con-
such as fear, hate, and love can be generated stantly create as the mind frantically tries to
by computational mechanisms and used for fill the gaps in perception caused by incom-
generating behavior in artificially intelligent plete sensory information from imperfect
systems. Computer models have been used to senses, inattention, and environmental noise,
model certain emotional processes such that along with missing knowledge about the envi-
the machine seems to behave emotionally. ronment, uncertainty about the behavior of
Even if the underlying sources of emotion others, and the inability to know everything
were different in machines, if the manifesta- about the past or to predict the future (Libet,
tion were the same as for humans, the con- 1993; Ayan, 2018; Koch, 2018).
clusion from a Turing test (Moore, 2003) A robot’s physiology and brain would be
designed to test robot emotions would be that different from that of humans. The techno-
robots have emotions. If a machine behaves as logical evolution of robot psychology could
if it is afraid or happy, who is to say it is not? lead to a robot consciousness that is superior
to that of people, where the robot is fully self-
aware. At all times the robot could be aware
of everything taking place within its body
ROBOT CONSCIOUSNESS AND and the state of its physiology and mind.
FREE WILL It is possible that a form of consciousness
can emerge from a robot that has a suitable
There is no universal agreement among cogni- representation of the world. A robot that can
tive scientists as to the nature or meaning of build and maintain a world model that repre-
consciousness (Edelman and Tononi, 2000; sents objects, events, entities, situations, and
Graziano, 2013). An aggregation of various relationships should be able to include itself
dictionary definitions of consciousness is: ‘the as one of the entities. A self-aware system can
state of being aware of oneself and one’s rela- keep track of itself, of its external and internal
tionship to the world; perceiving, apprehend- states and dynamics. A self-aware system can
ing, or noticing with a degree of controlled be aware of how it should respond to things –
observation; capable of or marked by thought, how it ‘feels’ about things. Events or objects
will, design, or perception’. Our preferred can be labeled with emotional state variables,
definition of consciousness is the state of such as fear, hate, and love. Pain-state vari-
meta-perception – the perception of percep- ables can indicate to the robot that it is dam-
tion. Self-awareness is quite limited in humans aged. An emotional response may not always
in that we are unaware of most of our physi- be desirable in robots or humans because the
ological states. Most of our physiological and consequent behavior may be excessive or inap-
mental processes take place without our being propriate to the situation. In life-threatening
aware of them, such as what is going on in our situations it may be quite appropriate. A robot
gastro-intestinal system or pancreas. Most of could have a world model with objects that
the cells and the DNA in and on our body are have assigned emotional-state variables. If the
not even ours, belonging to bacteria, fungi, robot observes an object labeled with a high-
worms, mites, mitochondria, and other sym- fear-state variable, it might exhibit fear by
bionts and parasites, all of which we are increasing its engine speed, assuming a defen-
blissfully unaware unless there is a gross mal- sive posture, or increasing its visual attention
function. Experiments of seemingly conscious and sensory awareness. It might articulate its
behavior indicate that it may be caused sub- fear, as the intelligent computer HAL did in
consciously and then retroactively attributed the movie version of Arthur C. Clarke’s ‘2001:
by the mind to a conscious decision (Libet, A Space Odyssey’ when it was being disman-
1993; Ayan, 2018; Koch, 2018). This would tled: ‘Stop, Dave, I’m afraid’.
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND ROBOTICS 365

As previously defined, robot intelligence is such as a 10-level scale, from no autonomy


not the same as self-awareness or conscious- to full autonomy. There is actually a contin-
ness. And robot consciousness is not the uum of autonomy and the levels discretize
same as what is usually considered free will, the continuum:
although the meaning of free will is elusive,
and the perception of free will in humans 1 System offers no assistance – operator must do
everything
may be a delusion (Gazzaniga, 2011; Harris,
2 System offers a complete set of action alterna-
2012). Self-awareness can be designed into tives to operator
the robot, or it may become an emergent 3 System narrows the action alternatives to a few
property as robot minds become more com- 4 System suggests a selection
plex. Free will, or the perception of free will, 5 System executes a selection if operator approves
may also emerge from complex robot minds. 6 System allows operator a restricted time to veto
Large, complex neural networks make cor- before automatic execution
rect decisions even now that are unexplain- 7 System executes automatically, then necessarily
able by human observers. Advanced robots informs operator
may make appropriate decisions and act 8 System informs operator after execution only if
accordingly without anyone understand- operator asks
9 System informs operator after execution – if
ing how or why. When asked, the robot
system decides to
may answer ‘I decided it was the right thing 10 System decides everything and acts autono-
to do’. In a future Turing test for free will, mously, essentially ignoring the human
human observers may conclude that robots
have free will. When robots are perceived to In recent years, as the technology for autono-
be intelligent, conscious, and have free will, mous cars advanced rapidly, the automotive
on what justification can people treat them as industry and the Society for Automotive
machines or slaves? Engineering defined six levels of autonomy.
A few slightly different variants of the six
levels are used by various autonomous vehi-
cle developers.
AUTONOMOUS MILITARY ROBOTS
Level 0: no autonomy
Level 1: a few functions, such as automated cruise
The US military is still refining its definition
control, are automated
of an autonomous system. For example, Level 2: the driver, while always monitoring the car,
DOD Directive No. 3000.09 (21 November does not have to steer or brake or accelerate
2012) defines an autonomous weapon system under some driving conditions
as a weapon system that, once activated, can Level 3: the vehicle can drive mostly autonomously,
select and engage targets without further but the driver must always be alert to take over
intervention by a human operator. For many as needed (vehicle may check the driver’s alert-
years a US government working group called ness regularly)
ALFUS (Autonomy Levels for Unmanned Level 4: the vehicle can operate fully autonomously
Systems) met regularly to try to define at least in some environments, e.g., on an inter-
autonomous systems based on three key vari- state road, without the driver having to be alert
while in that environment (but a driver must
ables (i.e., three dimensions): mission com-
always be in the vehicle)
plexity, environmental difficulty, and human Level 5: the vehicle can operate fully autonomously
interface. They were not entirely successful. without any human or driver in the vehicle or con-
Likewise, during the 1990s various aero- trolling or monitoring the vehicle from a distance
space companies and government agencies
attempted to define autonomous systems, In 2012 the US Department of Defense issued
often with lists of functional descriptions, a directive outlining steps to ensure that
366 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

autonomous weapons function as anticipated consequently lack compassion and empathy


and to minimize failures that could lead to (Brazier, 2018). While inherently saving
unintended engagements, or to loss of con- the lives of the human soldiers they replace,
trol of the system. The Defense Science autonomous robots can be programmed to be
Board (DSB) in 2016 conferred to improve more humane and rational than humans and
the future adoption and use of autonomous save civilians who would be otherwise killed,
systems, declaring that they had significant maimed, or raped by emotional, rampaging
military value. They recommended accelerat- human combatants. On the other hand, robots
ing the military’s adoption of autonomous can be designed to kill everyone efficiently
capabilities. The DSB’s definition was that an without empathy or remorse.
autonomous system has the ability to inde- Those opposing combat robots say that
pendently compose and select among differ- autonomous machines will not be able to
ent courses of action to accomplish goals apply human judgment in the application of
based on its knowledge and understanding of lethal force, or will not be constrained by the
the world, self, and situation. laws of war. But even humans cannot, or do
Along with the military’s accelerated adop- not, always distinguish between combatants
tion of autonomous systems, there are grow- and non-combatants, or between friendly and
ing efforts to ban autonomous lethal robots for enemy forces. The laws of war, such as they
military (and law-enforcement) applications. are, are frequently ignored. A robot could
Organizations like Human Rights Watch recognize that a potential target is already
and the Harvard Law School’s International wounded or trying to surrender and respond
Human Rights Clinic are alarmed about the appropriately. For example, DOD Directive
ethics of lethal autonomous robots. Calling 3000.09 (2012) concerning autonomy in
them ‘killer robots’, opponents urge an inter- weapons systems requires that command-
national treaty that would absolutely prohibit ers who authorize autonomous weapons
the development, production, and use of fully systems must do so in accordance with the
autonomous weapons. Opponents say that laws of war, treaties, safety rules, and rules of
killer robots will never possess the human engagement, and that the autonomous weap-
judgment that limits civilian casualties dur- ons systems must allow that such constraints
ing war and such machines could not be held be imposed by their human commanders.
accountable for war crimes. ‘Never’ is a very Opponents say robotic warfare is a slip-
long time, and it seems that their developers pery slope leading to an uncertain, poten-
and users could be held liable for war crimes, tially apocalyptic future. Even if the US DOD
similar to the liability issues when autono- were conscientious in deploying autonomous
mous cars cause accidents or, in the future, robots, other nations or non-state actors may
be used to commit crimes or terrorist acts. not be the same. Autonomous weapon sys-
Civilians have always been, and still are, tems are not inherently unlawful or unethical.
major casualties of war, and robot soldiers In any event, the development and deploy-
can be designed to reduce battlefield death, ment of autonomous weapon systems are
destruction, and atrocities. It is not certain that inevitable, and any attempt at a global ban
autonomous military robots would be worse will likely be ineffective. Given the historical
than human combatants. It depends on how failure of international treaties to ban other
the robots are designed, programmed, and seemingly abhorrent means of killing, it is
trained. Some say machines can never have unlikely to succeed for lethal robots as well.
compassion, but, again, ‘never’ is a very long It would likely be easier to get an interna-
time. In any event, many humans, including tional moratorium on their deployment or an
combatants, are sociopaths or suffer from agreement on how to constrain their behavior
clinical narcissistic personality disorder and than to prohibit them entirely, just as nuclear
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND ROBOTICS 367

weapons have not been used again in combat by their complete destruction (i.e., execution)
and lethal gas has been used rarely. or re-­programming, depending on whether the
cause of the deviant behavior is known and
repairable. Confining a future criminal robot
in a prison would only be justified if the robot
ROBOT LAWS, ETHICS, AND were deemed to be a conscious entity, worthy
MORALITY of human-like treatment, and it did not want
to be re-­programmed to have its mind altered.
Natural selection has provided humans with a In order to impose behavioral constraints
set of rules and constraints for behaving on robots, science-fiction author (and scien-
socially, such as not killing or eating one’s tist) Isaac Asimov devised the Three Laws of
children, or behaving fairly with others within Robotics in 1942 in a short story, which was
one’s family or tribe. These behavioral rules later used in many more of his stories (Asimov,
are limited and not always effective, so 1950). The Three Laws, later increased to
humans have devised larger sets of behavioral Four Laws, were a set of behavioral rules that
rules, such as laws, ethics, and morality. These were ‘laws’ in the sense that they were pro-
terms have many different definitions, with grammed constraints on a robot’s behavior.
often an overlap between ethics and morality. The Laws were designed to prevent robots
These definitions distinguish the rules. from harming humans while performing their
Laws are behavioral rules which, when functions, and Asimov later added a fourth (or
violated, can be punished by society with the zeroth) Law:
loss of one’s life, freedom, or possessions.
Ethics are rules of behavior which, when 1 A robot may not injure a human being or, through
violated, can be punished with the loss of inaction, allow a human being to come to harm.
one’s profession (if it includes ethical rules), 2 A robot must obey the orders given to it by
human beings, except where such orders would
or ostracism by business associates, friends,
conflict with the First Law.
or social groups. Morality is behavior which 3 A robot must protect its own existence as long
mitigates pain and suffering, as in the parable as such protection does not conflict with the First
of the Good Samaritan, which was tested or Second Law.
in a well-known psychological experiment 4 A robot may not harm humanity, or, by inaction,
(Darley and Batson, 1973). However, moral- allow humanity to come to harm.
ity does not require a belief system such as
religion; just the objective realization that The Laws are often cited as templates for
humankind benefits when the members of constraining robot behavior, to serve as a
its species refrain from causing pain and suf- basis for robot ethics and morality. But the
fering and strive to mitigate such suffering point of the stories (e.g., I Robot) was that
when it is caused by the brutality of nature or the Laws often failed for various unforeseen
circumstance. reasons, leading to unintended consequences.
Likewise, autonomous robots, including The narrative puzzle was to determine the
driverless vehicles in the near term, will need cause of the Law’s failure. For example, why
to conform to legal, ethical, and moral behav- would robots, with their behavior seemingly
ior, as well as understanding the human sense constrained by the Four Laws, enslave
of these rules and behavior constraints (Lin people? Let’s say that the robots decide,
et al., 2012). Robots will need to become what among themselves, that the greatest threat of
are called Artificial Moral Agents (Wallach harm to humanity is from humanity itself.
and Allen, 2009). Robots which violate human Thus, in order to protect humanity from harm
or robot laws, such as laws against murdering they must enslave humans for their own
humans or other robots, may lose their lives good. Under strict control of the robots,
368 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

humanity, while enslaved, would be p­ revented throw a person off the overpass to achieve
from causing its own extinction, from coming the same result. The reason for this illogical
to physical harm. The harm to humanity result was philosophically debated exten-
losing its freedom is either not apparent to the sively, but a scientific experiment provided
robots or is considered of relatively minor an explanation. It seems there is a mental
importance relative to physical harm. distinction, embedded in different areas of
Legal, ethical, and moral dilemmas are the brain, between an intended harmful act
being deliberated and debated now for the involving physical force, like pushing a per-
advent of autonomous vehicles (Herrman son off a bridge, and an act in which harm is a
et al., 2018). Insurance companies are among side effect, like the result of closing a switch.
those considering who will be legally and Functional magnetic resonance imaging
financially responsible for accidents involv- (fMRI) indicates that, for most people tested,
ing autonomous vehicles. Who will be legally, a part of the brain involved with rational-
ethically, and morally responsible for the ity (the prefrontal cortex) would close the
decisions that will be made by any intelligent switch, while a part of the brain involved with
autonomous robot, including lethal military emotion (the amygdala) would prevent them
robots? A typical question is: if your driver- physically throwing a person off the bridge.
less car were to encounter a bus full of school- Depending on the scenario, one or the other
children coming toward you on the wrong side would dominate the decision and determine
of a road near the edge of a cliff, how should the consequent act (Greene, 2012).
it react? Should it make a decision to swerve, A robot, or autonomous car, could decide
preventing a collision with the bus and spar- the trolley problem or an equivalent encoun-
ing the children but putting your life at risk? tered in the real world on a purely objective,
Or should it collide with the bus if there’s a rational basis. The metrics for determining
greater chance of you surviving? the rationality of the decision may go beyond
Recently, there has been much contempla- a simple numerical head count, to include
tion of the famous trolley problem, a moral the value to society of the individuals. Are
and psychological dilemma with several vari- the deaths of five laborers worth more than the
ations, as it relates to humans and autono- death of one individual who is the 21st cen-
mous vehicles or other robots (Shenhav tury equivalent of Einstein, Newton, Lincoln,
and Greene, 2010; Bohannon, 2011). In one or da Vinci? What if the five workers are all
variant of the narrative, a runaway trolley is single but the person on the bridge is sup-
about to kill five workers on a track. You are a porting a family of six? What if the five
bystander near a switch that would cause the workers are all old but the one individual is
trolley to change tracks and avoid killing the young? With the information that it has or
workers. However, there is one worker on can quickly acquire, the robot can swiftly
the other track who would be killed. Would evaluate the alternative decisions against
you close the switch and kill one person to the metrics and make an objective decision.
avoid killing five people? What if you are However, the metrics used as the bases for
on an overpass watching the runaway trolley these kinds of decisions need the seeds of
below. You can save the five workers if you wisdom, whether human or machine, for
throw a fat woman, standing next to you on their fruition.
the overpass, onto the track to stop the trolley. Objective decisions, however, may not
Would you throw the woman onto the tracks? always be commercially viable. People may
You would kill the woman to save the five avoid buying or riding in autonomous vehi-
workers. cles, or employing autonomous robots, if
Most people surveyed would close the they felt that, in an emergency, their lives may
switch to save five people but not physically not be considered as important as others’.
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND ROBOTICS 369

The algorithm for determining who should A robot can have knowledge of the local
be saved and who should be allowed to die ­customs of how to set a dining-room table
might then be skewed a bit, if not completely, and the proper way to greet guests and offer
toward the occupants of the vehicle or the them refreshments; or how to sell menswear
owner of the robot, offering a higher prob- in a retail store without cheating customers. In
ability of surviving an adverse incident. Or general, a top-down approach for robot legal,
autonomous vehicle software may offer alter- ethical, and moral behavior would require
native algorithms to users, allowing them to algorithms to guide robot behavior, i.e., the
select a preference for a higher probability of robots would need to know how to apply
survival in an accident (i.e., a selfish choice) the rules in any given situation. Programmers
or survival based on objective criteria, which cannot foresee every contingency, so the
may provide the passengers with a lower robot must also learn from experience how
probability (i.e., an altruistic choice). Human and when to apply the rules of law, ethics,
psychology again becomes a factor in deter- and morality.
mining robot psychology. Unlike robots, humans cannot be born
Autonomous robots can know the laws with these rules implanted in their minds, at
they are required to obey from ‘birth’, as least currently. People often have difficulty
soon as they emerge from the assembly line. interpreting these rules and knowing how
The laws may be the same as, or different to behave within their constraints. Many of
from, those constraining human behavior. the rules are not instinctual. They must be
They will be codified rules with specified acquired through years of experience – the
penalties, but it is likely the penalties will bottom-up approach.
be different for robots. If needed, autono- The bottom-up approach for robots to mas-
mous robots can have immediate internal or ter their rules of behavior – legal, ethical, and
external access to, and knowledge of, all moral – requires time for them to acquire
federal, state, and local laws, as well as judi- experience interacting with family, peers,
cial cases and precedents. However, for a and strangers, as do humans. While the time
robot to have more than superficial knowl- required to learn from experience may be less
edge of lawful behavior, and especially ethi- than that for humans, it still may take sev-
cal and moral behavior, it will need more eral years. Robots will need a combination
than access to a vast knowledgebase – just of top-down and bottom-up approaches to
as humans do. fully learn the norms, standards, and cultural
People are born with evolved behavioral aspects of moral and ethical behavior as prac-
rules that can be attributed to a moral and ticed by humans, and the behavior required
ethical code of conduct, such as manifested of robots (Lin et al., 2012).
in their behavior with parents and siblings. In the bottom-up experiential approach
They are not born knowing any societal emphasis is placed on creating an environ-
laws. Robots can be ‘born’ with lots of pre- ment where the robot explores courses of
programmed rules, legal, moral, and ethical. action and is rewarded for behavior that is
This is a top-down approach to robot behav- legally, ethically, and morally praiseworthy.
ioral constraints, where rules are pre-encoded Thus the robot learns and adapts through its
in software, like Asimov’s Laws of Robotics. experiences. Unlike the top-down approach,
Over the millennia people have devised, in the bottom-up process the rules are discov-
for example, the Code of Hammurabi, the ered or constructed, not explicitly mandated
Magna Carta, the Ten Commandments, the by fiat. The given behavioral rules, however,
US Constitution, the Rules of Professional serve to provide a framework and a context
Conduct of the American Bar Association, that guides and simplifies the experiential
and the Laws of the State of Maryland. rule-learning process.
370 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY OF to control the development of self-replicating


ROBOTS: FUTURE robots could be intentionally or unintention-
ally violated, as has happened with geneti-
Over the coming decades, to the end of this cally modified crops. Before the end of this
century, robot intelligence will evolve techno- century, autonomous robots will become
logically, a Lamarckian process that, like ubiquitous.
Darwinian evolution, is likely to be punctuated Over the coming decades, scientific dis-
in its progress, with intervals of relatively fast coveries and technological invention from
and slow change. New discoveries and inven- manifold fields could affect the evolution of
tions will impact robot software and hardware. robot minds. For example, there are thou-
sands of human brain organoids now being
Robot intelligence is likely to increase with
grown in nutrient solutions for study, some of
new algorithms and processors, and robot psy-
them on the International Space Station (Qian
chology will evolve. Robot performance is
et  al., 2019; Spectrum, 2019). These were
likely to improve with new technology for
originally human skin cells that were trans-
legs, wheels, arms, hands, and sensors. Robot
formed into stem cells, which were trans-
capabilities in 2099 are unpredictable, except
muted into neurons. The neurons clumped
to safely speculate that they will be orders of
into spherical organoids which formed inter-
magnitude greater than they are now.
nal structures like tubes and emitted synchro-
Ubiquitous robots and other intelligent
nized neuronal firing patterns. These are not
machines can provide humankind with a uto-
(yet) human brains, but there is some concern
pia or dystopia. The technologies of genetics, that they may become sentient and feel pain,
nanotechnology, and robotics have at least one or otherwise suffer. It is easy to imagine that
thing in common – they all offer the possibility conscious brain organoids might some day be
of self-replication. Autopoiesis is one poten- integrated in the brain of a robot, creating a
tial promise or danger of intelligent machines, hybrid entity with a mind having both human
whether they are as intelligent as a human or an and machine structure and psychology. Initial
amoeba, or the size of a human or an amoeba experiments linking brain organoids to a
(such as nanobots which will repair damaged robot have already been conducted. A multi-
organs). The autonomous autopoietic system legged spider-like robot received neuron sig-
is self-sustaining through replication, just as nals from a brain organoid causing it to walk.
organisms have been self-sustaining on Earth The robot sent sensory information, such as
for nearly four billion years. The power of when it encountered a wall, back to the brain
replication is astounding: on Earth there is an organoid.
unbroken chain, sustained through intermit- Our minds and their evolved psychology,
tent global catastrophes, for almost a third of ‘designed’ over eons for survival in their nat-
the age of the universe; from the first living ural environments, are imperfectly designed
aggregation of molecules to us. to function properly in our current complex
NASA’s astrobiological definition of life societies. Technology helped us survive the
is that it is a ‘self-sustained chemical system nastiness of nature for many millennia. We
capable of undergoing Darwinian evolution’. now need help to survive the nastiness of our
This may be modified to accommodate robotic human-made environment. Technology will
lifeforms: life is a self-sustained system capa- propel the rapid evolution of robot minds
ble of adapting to a changing environment. and their consequent psychology. Despite the
Autopoietic robots smart enough to create risks, such robots may be the best hope for
new technology could hasten the evolution of humanity to alleviate ignorance, poverty, and
robot psychology. Robot autopoiesis can be suffering, and to allow humans to become
dangerous, but any guidelines, or even laws, more humane.
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND ROBOTICS 371

REFERENCES and Dispositional Variables in Helping Behav-


ior, Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
Albus, James S. (2001), Engineering of Mind, ogy, 27, 100–108.
Presentation based on Albus, James S. and Davis, Lawrence (1991), Editor, Handbook of
Alexander M. Meystel (2001), Engineering of Genetic Algorithms. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
Mind, New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. Van Nostrand Reinhold.
Albus, James S. and Alexander M. Meystel Durkin, John (1994), Expert Systems: Design
(2001), Engineering of Mind, John Wiley & and Development, Prentice-Hall, Inc.
Edell, Aaron (2018), Recognizing context is still
Sons, Inc.
hard in Machine Learning, Towards Data
Ardiel, Evan L. and Catharine H. Rankin (2010),
Science. Retrieved from https://towards
An Elegant Mind: Learning and Memory in
datascience.com/recognizing-context-is-still-
Caenorhabditis Elegans, Learning and
hard-in-machine-learning-heres-how-to-tackle-
Memory, Brain Research Centre and Depart-
it-ed398a725f8c (Accessed 10 July 2020)
ment of Psychology, University of British
Edelman, Gerald M. and Giulio Tononi (2000),
Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia V6T
A Universe of Consciousness. New York, NY:
2B5, Canada, 17:191–201 # 2010 Cold
Basic Books.
Spring Harbor Laboratory Press, ISSN 1549-
Evans, Leonard (2008), Death in Traffic: Why
5485/10; www.learnmem.org
Are the Ethical Issues Ignored? Studies in
Asimov, Isaac (1950), I Robot, New York, NY:
Ethics, Law, and Technology, 2(1).
Doubleday.
Fisher, Martin Henry (1879–1962), http://liber-
Aunger, Robert (2002), The Electric Meme: A
tytree.ca/quotes/Martin.Fischer.Quote.1A58
New Theory of How We Think, New York, (Accessed 19 June 2020).
NY: The Free Press. Gazzaniga, Michael S. (2011), Who’s in Charge:
Ayan, Steve (2018), There Is No Such Thing as Free Will and the Science of the Brain, New
Conscious Thought, Scientific American, York, NY: Harper Collins.
December 2018. Retrieved from https:// Goldberg, David E. (1989), Genetic Algorithms
www.scientificamerican.com/article/there- in Search, Optimization, and Machine
is-no-such-thing-as-conscious-thought/ Learning. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley
?print=true (Accessed 10 July 2020) Publishing Co., Inc.
Bohannon, John (2011), A Time to Kill, J. Goodfellow, Ian, Yoshua Bengio, and Aaron
Science. Retrieved from https://www. Couville (2016), Deep Learning, MIT Press,
sciencemag.org/news/2011/06/time-kill Cambridge, MA.
(Accessed 10 July 2020) Gould, Stephen Jay (1983), Hen’s Teeth and
Brazier, Yvette (2018), All about Narcissistic Horses Toes: Further Reflections in Natural
Personality Disorder, Medical News Today. History. New York, N: W.W. Norton & Com-
Retrieved from https://www.medicalnewsto- pany, Inc.
day.com/articles/9741 Graziano, Michael S. (2013), Consciousness
Brézillon, Patrick (1999), Context in Artificial and the Social Brain. New York, NY: Oxford
Intelligence I: A Survey of the Literature, Com- University Press.
puters and Artificial Intelligence, 18(4) 1–28. Greene, Joshua D. (2012), Solving the Trolley
Buss, David M. (2005), (Ed.), The Handbook of Problem, In Sytsma, Justin and Wesley Buck-
Evolutionary Psychology, New Jersey: John walter (2016), (Eds.), A Companion to Exper-
Wiley & Sons, Inc. imental Philosophy, pp 175–178. Hoboken,
Cartwright, Mark (2013), Prometheus, Ancient NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
History Encyclopedia. Buffalo, NY: Prometheus Hall, Louisa (2017), How We Feel About Robots
Books. That Feel, MIT Technology Review. Retrieved
Damasio, Antonio (2003), Looking for Spinoza: from https://www.technologyreview.com/
Joy, Sorrow, and the Feeling Brain, New York, 2017/10/24/148259/how-we-feel-about-
NY: Harcourt, Inc. robots-that-feel/ (Accessed 10 July 2020)
Darley, J. M. and C. D. Batson (1973), From Harris, Sam (2012), Free Will. New York, NY:
Jerusalem to Jericho: A study of Situational Free Press.
372 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Hayes-Roth, Frederick, Donald A. Waterman, Intelligence (Studies in Cognitive Systems).


and Douglas B. Lenat (1983), (Eds.), Building New York, NY: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Expert Systems. Reading, MA: Addison- National Safety Council (2019), Road to Zero
Wesley Publishing Co., Inc. Presents Plan to Eliminate Roadway Deaths.
Herrman, Andreas, Walter Brenner, and Rupert Itasca, IL: Retrieved from https://www.nsc.org/
Stadler (2018), Autonomous Driving: How road-safety/get-involved/road-to-zero.
the Driverless Revolution Will Change the (Accessed 16 April 2019)
World. Bingley, UK: Emerald Publishing. Qian, Xuyu, Hongjun Song, and Guo-li Ming
Hull, David L. (2000), Taking Memetics Seri- (2019), Brain Organoids: Advances, Applica-
ously: Memetics Will Be What We Make It. In tions and Challenges, Development. 2019
Aunger, Robert, (Ed.), (2000), Darwinizing Apr 16;146(8): dev166074. doi: 10.1242/
Culture: The Status of Memetics as a Sci- dev.166074j.
ence, pp. 43–68. New York, NY: Oxford Rechavi, Oded (2009), Cell contact-dependent
University Press. acquisition of cellular and viral non-
Koch, Christof (2018), What Is Consciousness? autonomously encoded small RNAs, Genes &
Scientific American, pp. 60–64. June 1, 2018. Development, 23(16) 1971–9. doi: 10.1101/
Koza, John R. (1992), Genetic Programming, gad.1789609.
MIT Press, Cambridge, MA. Schultz, Alan C., John J. Grefenstette, and
Krizhevsky, Alex, Ilya Sutskever, and Geoffrey E. Kenneth A. De Jong (1993), Test and
Hinton (2012), ImageNet Classification with Evaluation by Genetic Algorithms, IEEE
Deep Convolutional Neural Networks, Expert, 8(5) 9–14. October 1993.
Proceedings of the 25th International Shenhav, Amitai and Joshua D. Greene (2010),
Conference on Neural Information Processing Moral Judgments Recruit Domain-General
Systems – Volume 1, pp. 1097–1105. Valuation Mechanisms to Integrate Repre-
LeDoux, Joseph (1996), The Emotional Brain. sentations of Probability and Magnitude,
New York, NY: Simon and Schuster. j. Neuron. 67, 667–677.
LeDoux, Joseph E. (2012), Evolution of Human Smith, Wesley (2019), Eugenics-Engineered
Emotion: A View through Fear, Progress in Babies’ Brains Changed by CRISPR, National
Brain Research, 195, 431–442. Review, February 21, 2019. Retrieved from
Libet, B. (1993), The Neural Time Factor in https://www.nationalreview.com/corner/
Conscious and Unconscious Events, Ciba genetic-engineering-crispr-changed-babies-
Foundation Symposium, 174, 123–137; brains/#:~:text=The%20brains%20of%20
discussion 137–146. two%20babies,probably%20impacted%20
Lin, Patrick, Keith Abney, and George A. Bekey by%20the%20procedure (Accessed 10 July
(2012), Robot Ethics: The Ethical and Social 2020)
Implications of Robotics, MIT Press. Cam- Spectrum (2019), Brain organoids show realistic
bridge, MA. neuronal firing rhythms, Spectrum Research
Lipson, Hod and Melba Kurman (2016), Driver- News. Retrieved from https://www.spectrum-
less: Intelligent Cars on the Road Ahead, MIT news.org/news/brain-organoids-show-realistic-
Press, Cambridge, MA. neuronal-firing-rhythms/#:~:text=Brain%20
McCulloch, Warren S. and Walter Pits (1943), A organoids%20made%20from%20
Logical Calculus of the Ideas Immanent in typical,at%20least%20four%20
Nervous Activity. In Anderson, James A. and months1.&text=A%20few%20features%20
Edward Rosenfeld, (Eds.), (1988), Neurocom- of%20the,preterm%20infants%2C%20
puting: Foundations of Research, pp. 18–31. the%20researchers%20say (Accessed 10 July
MIT Press, Cambridge, MA. 2020)
Meystel, Alexander M. and James S. Albus Sternberg, Robert (1995), Editor, Wisdom: Its
(2002), Intelligent Systems: Architecture, Nature, Origins, and Developments, New
Design, and Control. New York, NY: John York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Wiley & Sons, Inc. Teich, Albert H. (2009), Editor, Technology and
Moore, James H. (2003), Editor, The Turing the Future, Boston, MA: Wadsworth Cengage
Test: The Elusive Standard of Artificial Learning.
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND ROBOTICS 373

The Economist (2015), If Autonomous Vehicles Wikipedia (2019b), Timeline of Motor Vehicle
Rule the World: From Horseless to Driverless, Brands. Retrieved from https://en.wikipedia.
July 1, 2015. Retrieved from https://worldif. org/wiki/Timeline_of_motor_vehicle_brands
economist.com/article/12123/horseless- (Accessed 10 July 2020)
driverless (Accessed 10 July 2020) Zaki, Jamil (2013), Using Empathy to Use People:
Wallach, Wendell and Collin Allen (2009), Moral Emotional Intelligence and Manipulation,
Machines: Teaching Robots Right from Scientific American. Retrieved from https://
Wrong. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. blogs.scientificamerican.com/moral-universe/
Wikipedia (2019a), Ford Model T. Retrieved using-empathy-to-use-people-emotional-
from https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Ford_ intelligence-and-manipulation/ (Accessed
Model_T (Accessed 10 July 2020) 10 July 2020)
19
Evolutionary Psychology and
Dangerous Driving Behaviour
Deanna Singhal and David Wiesenthal

INTRODUCTION AGGRESSIVE AND RISKY DRIVING

This chapter discusses the contributions of Each driver at some point in their driving
evolutionary psychological and social learn- experience has likely witnessed, or even
ing theories to explain aggressive and risky engaged in, an act of aggressive or risky driv-
driving. Dangerous driving behaviour may ing on the road, such as tailgating, weaving
be influenced by a number of factors: ‘the in and out of traffic, running red lights, and
young male syndrome’ and observational driving at excessive speeds. Driving behav-
learning may contribute to the choice to drive iour is considered aggressive if ‘it is deliber-
or respond aggressively on the roads. ate, likely to increase the risk of collision,
Exposure to media, such as motion pictures and is motivated by impatience, annoyance,
and racing video games, can promote the hostility and/or an attempt to save time’
modelling of dangerous driving behaviours. (Tasca, 2000: 2). It has been suggested that
How situational factors interact with per- such behaviours can be frustration-driven
sonal and internal factors to promote risky and are more likely in an enabling environ-
driving is discussed within the context of the ment (Shinar, 1998). Some aggressive driv-
General Aggression Model (Anderson and ing involves hostile aggression, where the
Bushman, 2002). A summary of what is behaviour is directed at an object of frustra-
known about this topic is provided, including tion (e.g., cursing at another driver), whereas
a review of research studies using different other acts involve instrumental aggression,
methodologies (e.g., laboratory, archival), where the frustrated driver attempts to move
and considerations for public policy are ahead at the expense of other drivers’ rights
discussed. (e.g., weaving in and out of traffic)
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND DANGEROUS DRIVING BEHAVIOUR 375

(Roseborough, 2014; Shinar, 1998). In the bombarded young males with video games
context of evolutionary psychological theory, and motion pictures featuring chess-playing
frustration may not necessarily be the only scenarios that resulted in rewarding experi-
reason why these driving behaviours occur, ences as a result of competitiveness, it seems
as they may involve voluntarily initiated dis- reasonable to conclude that competition
plays of dominance. Males are at a greater between young males would take place on
risk of motor vehicle collisions than females chess boards rather than streets and high-
because they are more likely to be involved ways. Chess is a symbolic or cognitive exam-
in speeding, tailgating, and responding ple of warfare, with males comprising the vast
aggressively to perceived misdeeds of other majority of Grandmasters, a title awarded to
drivers (Wilson and Daly, 1985). world-class chess players by the International
The ‘young male syndrome’, coined by Chess Federation (FIDE). The most recent
Wilson and Daly (1985), refers to the pat- FIDE list of Grandmasters includes 1,680
tern of disproportionate involvement of males individuals from a variety of countries, 1,643
in risky or aggressive behaviours, such as of which are male (98%) (‘List of chess
gambling, homicide, and motor vehicle col- grandmasters’, n.d.). Evolutionary tenden-
lisions. In the context of evolution, aggres- cies are not the only influences on behaviour
sion can have an adaptive value and, in and the environmental or social exposure to
certain situations, males can perceive a ben- depictions of competition, in various media
efit. Competitive risk-taking among males is forms, can serve as representative examples
believed to have evolved as a result of repro- of which behaviours males should engage in
ductive competition, where males compete and how. The presentation that will follow
with other males for the valued resource of later in this chapter will review the effects of
females (Wilson and Daly, 1985). When envi- cinematic portrayals of risky driving.
ronmental resources are scarce, or unequally Roadways are a social environment that
distributed, and when the proportion of young affords displays of competitive behaviour.
males in the population is large, both risk-­ When driving, individuals can engage in
taking and aggression increase (Mesquida and risk-taking competitiveness (Wiesenthal
Wiener, 1996; Wiesenthal and Singhal, 2012). and Singhal, 2012), particularly among
Females may perceive these behaviours as young males, which can contribute to risky
indicators of a mate who will protect and or aggressive driving behaviours (Vingilis
provide for her and any offspring. These are et al., 2013). An overrepresentation of young
desired characteristics associated with paren- males in annual motor vehicle collisions was
tal investment (Buss, 1988). Males engage in reported in British Columbia in 2018 (Peng,
social displays of risky behaviour in an effort 2018). In particular, there was a doubling of
to secure the resources necessary to attract deaths involving motorcycles, where 27 of the
mates. Intra- and intergroup competition, 30 victims were male and a third of all victims
which involves prestige, rank, and reputation, were between the ages of 19 and 29 years.
may enhance the likelihood of attracting pro- Motorcycles may represent a riskier vehicle
spective mates by elevating the male’s posi- to drive and, by design, promote risky-driving
tion on a dominance hierarchy (Wilson and behaviours, such as speeding. One British
Daly, 1985). The ability to provide social and Columbian police agency reported a ‘major
psychological resources, in addition to mate- increase’ in excessive speeding province-wide
rial resources, is considered important for that was not specific to motorcycles, with
mate selection in females (Buss, 1988). 25% more drivers being stopped for this risky
Competition in young males is not behaviour (Peng, 2018). Though no known
restricted to driving scenarios. If society reasons for these increases were reported,
376 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

previous research has demonstrated that see and hear on television, in the movies, on
increases in male driving-related fatalities the Internet, or in newspapers and magazines
even occurred during a decrease in the male (Koordeman et  al., 2011; López-Guimerà
population (Wiesenthal and Singhal, 2012). et al., 2010). Media recognizes this influence
Though evolutionary psychological theory and, in some cases, limits what viewers can
can make an important contribution to the see, in an attempt to prevent imitation of a
understanding of risky driving behaviour, behaviour. For example, the Toronto Transit
societal influences, such as the presence of Commission (TTC) has an agreement with
others and individual difference variables local media to not treat subway suicides as
(e.g., personality), interact with male com- newsworthy, in an attempt to prevent imita-
petitiveness and can influence the decision to tion. Soon after the adoption of this standard
engage in risk-taking behaviours (Wiesenthal in 1971, the TTC reported a marked decrease
and Singhal, 2012; Wilson and Daly, 1985). in the number of suicides (Toronto Transit
An important framework to consider in this Commission, 2010).
context is social learning theory, which sug- With respect to road safety, modelling of
gests that transmission of behaviour can aggressive or risky driving can occur as a
occur through observational learning, where result of observational learning, with media
an individual acquires new patterns of behav- being an influential source. Media depictions
iour by observation (Bandura, 1971). Media of dangerous or aggressive driving and glo-
influences on the behaviour of children and rifications of such risk-taking have become
adults have long been a research concern in increasingly popular (Fischer et al., 2012). A
developmental, social, and clinical psychol- pivotal influence was the 1968 release of the
ogy. The advertising industry is based on motion picture Bullitt. This film featured an
the fact that consumers can be influenced exciting car chase, starring Steve McQueen,
by media messages. While most advertising which involved risky and aggressive driv-
is concerned with altering or influencing ing through the streets of San Francisco
relatively trivial consumer behaviours, the (Wiesenthal et  al., 2016). A more current
material presented in this chapter indi- driving movie franchise is Fast and Furious,
cates that media can trigger dangerous in which entire movies depict acts of ille-
and life-threatening actions in predisposed gal street racing and heists. The franchise
individuals. has grown in popularity since its first movie
The novelty, relevance, and consequences release, The Fast and the Furious, in June of
of the behaviour play a role in the imitative 2001. This movie grossed approximately two
effects of observation (Bandura, 1971). The million dollars at the worldwide box office,
term modelling encompasses the broader compared to a more recent franchise instal-
psychological effects that can occur with ment, Furious 7, which grossed over one
observational learning, beyond mere imi- and a half billion dollars worldwide, with an
tation or mimicry (Bandura, 1971). These additional 70 million dollars in DVD sales
could include cognitive changes (e.g., in (Nash Information Services, n.d.-c).
attitudes) that accompany the learning of Depictions of speeding, racing, dan-
a behaviour, even if the behaviour was not gerous passing, and aggressive behaviour
performed following observation (Modeling, towards other drivers are often shown in the
n.d.). Marketing relies upon individuals mim- absence of negative consequences (Beullens
icking the use of products shown in adver- et  al., 2011b; Greenberg and Atkin, 1983).
tising, and uses media to communicate this According to media effects theory, view-
information. How much alcohol individuals ers’ perceptions of what is normal can be
choose to drink, or what they perceive as an influenced by the more frequent portrayal of
ideal body image, is influenced by what they certain behaviours (Beullens et  al., 2011b).
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND DANGEROUS DRIVING BEHAVIOUR 377

The risky driving behaviours often depicted IMITATION AND MODELLING


in media are novel, such that viewers do not
commonly see them on the roads (Atkin,
Social Learning Theory
1989). The more normative the events appear,
the greater the possibility of viewer disin- Social learning theory explains the transmis-
hibition (Atkin, 1989). Combined with the sion of behaviours. Aggression, like other
lack of depiction of negative consequences, complex social behaviours, may be imitated
learning of aggressive or risky driving may as a result of observational learning or direct
be enhanced (Beullens et al., 2011b). Adding exposure (Bandura, 1971). Bandura et  al.’s
to the possibility of imitation is the portrayal (1963a) classic experiment investigated the
of a hero as the risky driver, which can be imitation of aggression in children following
attractive to viewers (Beullens et al., 2011b) the viewing of an adult model interacting with
and creates a scenario in which the behaviour a three-foot Bobo doll. Young children who
seems justified, further fostering disinhibi- viewed a video of the model being aggressive
tion (Atkin, 1989). Media which is ‘arousing, towards the doll (e.g., hitting the doll while
nonfictional, justified, positively reinforced, sitting on it, kicking and throwing it, pummel-
and performed by individuals with whom ling it with a mallet), displayed more aggres-
viewers identify and feel similar’ is the most sive acts when allowed to interact with the
likely to influence behaviour (Vitaglione, Bobo doll in a situation of frustration, com-
2012: 489). pared to the control group, who did not view
There is evidence that motion-picture the aggressive model (Bandura et al., 1963a).
production companies are aware of the pos- Though this study has been criticized for con-
sibility of modelling risky and unsafe driv- sidering the acts displayed by the children to
ing. Prior to the release of The Fast and the be aggressive, given that a Bobo doll is essen-
Furious in 2001, Universal Studios posted a tially designed to be pummelled (Freedman,
disclaimer on the film’s promotional website 2007; Milgram and Shotland, 1973), it did
stating, ‘All of the racing stunts in “The Fast demonstrate that children imitated specific
and the Furious” were performed in a staged observed behaviours, both physical (e.g., sit-
environment by professionals with years of ting on Bobo and hitting him with a mallet)
training and experience. Please do not try and verbal (e.g., shouting ‘Sock him in the
any of these yourself. Be smart. Drive safe. nose’) (Bandura et al., 1963a). Other research
Stay legal’ (Goldberg, 2001; Orwall, 2001). with children has suggested that early child-
In the days leading to the film’s release, they hood exposure to aggressive or harsh punish-
also ran two public-service announcements, ment from parents can provide a situation of
recorded by the two lead actors in the film, observational learning and result in aggressive
Vin Diesel and Paul Walker, emphasizing behaviour from children towards others (Weiss
safe driving. Given the awareness of pos- et al., 1992).
sible imitation, it has been said that media Though the social learning theory of
is a major part of the problem when imita- aggression stems from research with chil-
tive behaviour is undesirable or antisocial dren, it has implications for adults. Early
(Coleman, 2004). Though observational exposure to aggression and violence may
learning can be useful and efficient (i.e., contribute to children developing different
when errors in learning are costly or dan- cognitions associated with aggression. For
gerous or when teaching structured rules example, they may be more likely to attrib-
and skills is time-consuming and difficult) ute hostile intentions to others’ ambiguous
(Bandura, 1971), modelling behaviour that is behaviours (i.e., hostile attributional bias;
risky or aggressive can have serious negative Steinberg and Dodge, 1983). Therefore, they
consequences. may become more likely to select aggressive
378 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

solutions to situations in the future (Steinberg believed to be important for mate selection
and Dodge, 1983). In addition, the factors (Buss, 1988). The ownership of a fast car and
proposed to influence imitation of aggression the demonstration of a daring male driver
in a situation of observational learning, spe- may be perceived as more attractive and
cifically positive reinforcement, novelty, and dominant than his passenger counterparts.
relevance to a given social situation, are not The driver may also be viewed as possess-
specific to children (Atkin, 1989; Beullens ing similar characteristics to those of famous
et al., 2011b; Vitaglione, 2012). For example, actors portrayed as heroes in risky driving
the internal thrill or the accolades of friends movies, such as wealth and prestige.
associated with trying an aggressive or risky In addition to the influences of intrasexual
driving behaviour seen in a movie are exam- competition, peer pressure can influence
ples of positive reinforcement, which can behaviour, and the susceptibility to this social
influence this adult behaviour. influence is believed to be greatest during
early adolescence (Wilson and Daly, 1985).
Full development of resistance to peer pres-
sure does not necessarily occur between late
Social Interaction Theory
adolescence and early adulthood. During this
According to social interaction theory, period of time, young individuals may face
aggressive behaviour is social influence other risky behaviour choices, such as drug
behaviour, whereby coercive acts are used to use and teenage sexual encounters. If young
obtain things of value, bring about retributive males are already more likely to engage in
justice, or promote social and self-identities risky behaviours (Wilson and Daly, 1985),
(Tedeschi and Felson, 1994). Two social they may be more susceptible to the influ-
influences proposed to mediate aggression ence of their peers who encourage participa-
are socialization practices within the family tion (Steinberg and Monahan, 2007).
and peer influences (Wiesenthal and Singhal,
2012). For example, negative parenting,
including poor family organization and func- Influence of Behavioural
tioning, as well as marital conflict, contrib- Consequences
utes to aggression among adolescents (Talwar,
1998). Children may adopt peer values and Research on operant conditioning was rooted
beliefs as substitutes for parental values and in animal behaviour (Skinner, 1938), but
beliefs. When the peer influence is deviant consequences associated with an action were
and the individual is susceptible because of proposed to not only predict but also control
negative family influences, aggression an individual’s behaviour (Skinner, 1953). In
increases (Talwar, 1998). the case of childrearing, for example, rein-
With respect to driving, the presence of forcement and punishment are used to teach
others increases aggressive and risky behav- a child appropriate behaviour. An unwanted
iours. Teenage drivers engage in more risky behaviour results in a negative consequence,
driving (i.e., speeding and reduced headway) such as a ‘time-out’, while a desired response
with male passengers, compared to no pas- results in a positive consequence or reward,
sengers or female passengers, with the male such as a treat. Research on vicarious learn-
driver/male passenger combination produc- ing suggests that observing another individ-
ing approximately double the rate of risky ual experience consequences for a behaviour
or aggressive driving (Simons-Morton et al., can also influence one’s own behaviour. The
2005). According to evolutionary psycholog- influence of observing punishment or reward
ical theory, intrasexual competition can result on the imitation of aggression in children
in a greater display of desired resources was demonstrated in a follow-up study by
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND DANGEROUS DRIVING BEHAVIOUR 379

Bandura et al. (1963b). Children who viewed by Johann Wolfgang von Goethe, the young
the adult model being punished for aggres- Werther shoots himself after realizing that he
sive behaviour towards the Bobo doll were could never be with the woman he loved. In
less likely to imitate the behaviour than chil- following years, many young men killed
dren who viewed the aggressive model being themselves in the same manner, sitting at a
rewarded. In adults, vicarious learning can desk, dressed like Werther, with Goethe’s
result in attitude change, as demonstrated in novel in front of them (Coleman, 2004). To
one study where male university students investigate this effect, Phillips recorded the
reported being more accepting of interper- number of monthly suicides in the United
sonal violence against women following the States, following the publication of front-
viewing of movies depicting positive conse- page suicide stories in the New York Times,
quences associated with sexual violence during the period 1947–1967. Using the pre-
(Malamuth and Check, 1981). vious and subsequent year as a comparison,
Though evolutionary psychological theory he found a significant tendency for suicides
suggests that males, more than females, have to increase, and a dose-response relationship
a tendency to engage in aggressive behaviour between the number of suicides and the
in order to acquire desired resources (Wilson amount of front-page story coverage
and Daly, 1985), cognitive behaviourism (Phillips, 1974). The influence of the news-
suggests that individuals have free will to paper coverage was location-specific, such
contemplate and incorporate environmen- that stories published in the New York Times,
tal information, such as behavioural con- but not in Great Britain’s most popular news-
sequences of punishment and reward, into paper, The London Daily Mirror, resulted in
decision-making processes related to behav- a greater increase in suicides in the United
iour choice (Bandura, 2001b). The absence of States, compared to Great Britain. However,
negative consequences, as observed in many when the suicide story was covered in both
movies depicting risky and aggressive driv- newspapers, British suicides increased sig-
ing, may act as negative reinforcement. When nificantly (Phillips, 1974).
the undesirable consequences of risky driv- The Werther effect has also been demon-
ing, such as fines, collision, injury or death, strated for suicide stories covered on network
are removed, the driving behaviour will be television news programs. For example,
reinforced and more likely to occur in the in California, between 1968 and 1985, the
future. When such outcomes are commonly number of suicides significantly increased
depicted in the movies, vicarious learning 0–7 days after a publicized television story
also contributes to the probability of young (Phillips and Carstensen, 1988). The effect of
males engaging in this driving behaviour in this coverage can also be influenced by the
the real world. involvement of a celebrity. In Korea, between
2005 and 2008, television news coverage
of a celebrity suicide increased the number
of emergency department visits associated
The Copycat Effect
with suicide attempts or self-injury in the
The copycat effect refers to imitation or three weeks following the reported suicide
adoption of behaviours or practices of (Jeong et  al., 2012). The method of suicide
another, and its effects, usually negative and used by celebrities can also be imitated, as
unfavourable, are believed to be triggered by was demonstrated following the 2009 rail-
media (Coleman, 2004). Phillips (1974) way suicide of Robert Enke, a celebrated
coined the term Werther effect, describing the German football goalkeeper. German rail-
copycat effect associated with suicide. In the way suicides increased both in the short term
1774 novel The Sorrows of Young Werther, (i.e., two weeks following the event) and in
380 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

the long term (i.e., two years after the event) decision to remove a scene from their movie
(Hegerl et  al., 2013). A significant associa- The Program (Pristin and Fox, 1993). The
tion between frequency of Google searches scene depicted an inebriated college football
for ‘Enke’ and frequency of railway suicides quarterback lying in the middle of the high-
in Germany was documented (Koburger way, with cars passing by, barely ­missing him.
et  al., 2015), suggesting a media influence As the scene continued, other football play-
on this copycat behaviour. However, it was ers joined him and no one was hurt. Disney’s
not possible to determine if those who com- decision to remove the scene came after two
mitted suicide had experienced the media reported attempts of imitation in New Jersey
exposure. and Pennsylvania (Kotzen, 2013). One teen-
The copycat effect can also involve ager was killed and two others were seriously
criminal behaviour. Copycat crime refers to injured (Pristin and Fox, 1993).
imitative behaviour where an offence, origi- Television and social media are sources
nally portrayed in the media, is subsequently that can also contribute to the modelling of
performed in reality. It can be motivated by driving behaviour. Bird Box, a Netflix movie
real or fictional media depictions, and the released in 2018 (IMDb, 2018), contains
offender incorporates aspects of the origi- scenes of individuals manoeuvring through
nal crime (Helfgott, 2015). The 1994 film their environment blindfolded to avoid see-
Natural Born Killers has been linked to over ing a presence that urges them to commit sui-
a dozen copycat crimes (Helfgott, 2015). The cide when observed. This includes scenes of
film depicts a young, attractive couple on a driving in a vehicle with all windows covered
roadtrip/serial-mass-murder spree across the in tape. The ‘Bird Box Challenge’ became a
Southwestern United States, during which social media craze that encouraged people to
they kill over 50 people. Some couples, who try everyday tasks blindfolded (Deb, 2019).
committed similar murders, reported being Police in Utah had to issue a warning to its
obsessed with the movie and copied behav- citizens not to drive blindfolded. Though this
iours depicted in the film during their crimes. would seem obvious to most, the warning
In one copycat case, the young woman lured was issued after a female teenage driver, with
her victim to his death by promising sex, rep- a hat pulled over her eyes, drove into oncom-
licating a scene in the movie. Many examples ing traffic and hit another car (‘US driver’,
of copycat crime have been described, each 2019).
with its own unique media source, such as In California, following the release of
the news coverage of the Columbine school The Fast and the Furious, the Los Angeles
shooting. It has been suggested that the link Police Department increased patrols to stop
between viewing media violence and violent street racing (Goldberg, 2001). Supporting
crime is difficult to establish because of the their action was the knowledge that copycat
other factors that contribute to the behaviour behaviour had occurred in their area follow-
(e.g., individual, environmental, and situ- ing the release of Gone in 60 Seconds, which
ational elements; Helfgott, 2015). Though glamorized auto theft. On the day of this
it may be difficult to establish cause-and- film’s release, and the day after, the num-
effect links, later material in this chapter ber of stolen vehicles in the area more than
summarizes research that establishes rela- doubled, compared to the previous two years
tionships between viewing aggressive and (Goldberg, 2001). Increased enforcement
risky driving media and engagement in these was also reported for the release of the fourth
behaviours. instalment of the Fast and Furious franchise
With respect to movie content, there is in 2006, The Fast and The Furious: Tokyo
anecdotal evidence supporting the copycat Drift (Rowland, 2006). California Highway
effect. In 1993, Disney made a controversial Patrol reported being extra vigilant during
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND DANGEROUS DRIVING BEHAVIOUR 381

this time in stopping any unsafe or illegal GENERAL AGGRESSION MODEL


driving, such as street racing or speed con-
tests (Rowland, 2006). Increased enforce- The GAM (Anderson and Bushman, 2002) is
ment was also reported in Toronto for the a multifaceted approach to explaining aggres-
release of this film, with cruisers positioned sive behaviour, incorporating aspects from
outside theatres near highways (Grewal and social and evolutionary theories, as well as
Brennan, 2006). The Ontario Provincial components related to personality, cognition,
Police reported witnessing copycat behaviour and arousal. It is an episodic model that
and resulting crashes following the release of focuses on the individual in a particular situ-
the previous Fast and Furious film, Turbo- ation and consists of three overall compo-
Charged Prelude. They expressed the opin- nents: inputs involving person and situation
ion that young people get caught up in the factors, routes through which arousal, affect,
excitement and adrenaline of these films and and cognition play a role, and outcomes
attempt to imitate their heroes (Grewal and resulting from appraisal and decision pro-
Brennan, 2006).
cesses (Anderson and Bushman, 2002).
Mass media is a source of social or situ-
Person factors include things such as age,
ational influence on aggressive or risk-taking
sex differences in aggressive tendencies,
behaviour (Bandura et al., 1963a; Bushman,
and personality traits. Physical and verbal
1995; Eron et al., 1972; Paik and Comstock,
aggression, hostility, and anger can be inter-
1994; Vitaglione, 2012). In the context of
correlated and suggest overall trait aggres-
driving, when a driver speeds and weaves in
siveness, indicating that people who are angry
and out of traffic, following the viewing of
are more likely to aggress (Buss and Perry,
a movie that depicted such behaviour, they
1992). Sex differences have been reported
are not necessarily being violent or want-
ing to harm other drivers, but their risky and using the Aggression Questionnaire, such that
aggressive behaviour increases the likeli- males have a higher total score, are much more
hood of collision (Transport Canada, 2011), physically and verbally aggressive, and are
putting others at risk. Depictions of patterns somewhat more hostile than females (Buss
of aggressive behaviours can teach behav- and Perry, 1992).
iour styles, alter restraints on the behaviour, Sensation seeking refers to the need and
desensitize an individual to aggression, and willingness to engage in novel events that are
shape the viewer’s perceptions, which are highly arousing, even if they involve physi-
important bases for individual behaviour cal and social risk (Zuckerman, 1979). It is
(Grey et al., 1989). related to risky behaviours, such as danger-
Clearly, there are multiple factors that can ous driving (e.g., drunk driving and speeding)
contribute to driving aggressively or engag- (Wiesenthal et al., 2016), drug use, and minor
ing in risky driving behaviours. Evolutionary criminality (Arnett, 1994). It has been sug-
psychological theory suggests the importance gested that sensation seekers, having greater
of personal factors, such as age and sex dif- acceptance or reduced perception of risk, are
ferences, and social learning theory empha- more likely to engage in behaviours that are
sizes the need to consider situational factors both risky and aggressive (Wiesenthal et al.,
in the environment. In the context of aggres- 2016). Sensation seeking can serve as a bio-
sion, Anderson and Bushman (2002) devel- logical predisposition that interacts with the
oped the General Aggression Model (GAM), conditions of the social environment, such
which addresses how these, and other factors, that less restrictive environments allow for its
can interact to produce aggressive behaviour. greater expression (Arnett, 1994).
This model can serve as a basis for better In the context of evolutionary psychologi-
understanding aggressive or risky driving. cal theory, both trait aggression and sensation
382 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

seeking may benefit males in the context of in the broader cognitive neoassociation
intrasexual competition for mates. Displays theory of aggression. Berkowitz (2012) states
of aggression and willingness to take risks that the decision to act aggressively can
may present as qualities of an individual who begin with an aversive event, such as frus-
will protect and provide for a female and tration or provocation. This leads to nega-
their offspring. Mate-selection criteria for tive affect, which automatically stimulates
females include characteristics that demon- both fight and flight physiological pathways.
strate potential for parental investment, such A dominant fight reaction leads to irritation
as protection, social status, and the ability to or anger, whereas a dominant flight reaction
provide food and shelter (Buss, 1988). Males leads to fear. In addition to the activation of
who display the ability to provide social, psy- the physiological components of these path-
chological, and material resources may be ways, associated memories, thoughts, and
considered more desirable by females and motor responses will also be activated. Once
will compete strongly to demonstrate these the initial, more involuntary response tenden-
characteristics. Displays of these character- cies arise, higher-order cognition can become
istics do not require direct combat nor con- involved in the processing of appraisals and
tact with competitors, but males may engage attributions, as well as the consideration of
in representative behaviours at the expense rules and consequences associated with cer-
of others who are competing for females tain behaviour. These can modify or even
(Buss, 1988). extinguish the initial reaction, serving as self-
Situational factors, such as aggressive cues regulation or control processes. However, it
(e.g., weapons), provocation, frustration, is possible that an individual can become
and social opportunity for aggression (e.g., angry enough to respond aggressively, with-
church versus nightclub), interact with the out the intervention of higher-order cognition
individual’s internal state. These can influ- (Berkowitz, 2012).
ence affect, arousal, and cognition, which The physiological excitation produced
are interconnected, and can influence each from a given arousal-producing exposure, as
other in a reciprocal fashion (Anderson and mentioned in the cognitive neoassociation
Bushman, 2002). The existence of memory theory, does not necessarily end abruptly with
scripts can increase the likelihood of model- the termination of the exposure (Zillmann,
ling aggressive behaviour. A script is a type 1971). The arousal may linger for some time
of schema that is learned through experience and be carried over to a subsequent event or
and exposure, and comes to define a situa- experience, even one unrelated to the prior
tion. Highly associated concepts are linked exposure. This residual arousal can influence
together in memory and these associations the cognitive appraisal of the subsequent
often involve causal links, goals, and action emotional state, with greater residual arousal
plans (Abelson, 1981). For example, the use creating a more intense subsequent emotion.
of a gun can be associated with concepts such This suggests that a potentially stronger nega-
as anger, pain, retaliation, and the action of tive affect could be elicited by a provoking
shooting (Anderson and Bushman, 2002). event, producing an ‘over-intense’ response
When components are strongly linked, they to stimuli in a future scenario. The residual
become a unitary concept in semantic mem- arousal could ‘energize’ or facilitate associ-
ory and provide a guide for future behaviour. ated aggressive behaviour (Zillmann, 1971).
The greater the exposure to the behaviour, the The idea of interaction and reciproc-
greater the priming of these scripts, increas- ity between person, situation, and internal-
ing the likelihood of their use. state factors was also suggested by Bandura
The use of memories in the decision to (2001b) in his Triadic Reciprocal Causation
engage in aggressive behaviour is incorporated model of psychosocial functioning. This
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND DANGEROUS DRIVING BEHAVIOUR 383

model includes personal, behavioural, and media depicting this behaviour is a situational
environmental determinants, where personal factor that can influence the internal state. It
factors involve cognitive, affective, and bio- can increase arousal and influence affect
logical events. These determinants can influ- and cognition, partly through the priming
ence each other in a bidirectional fashion. For of existing memory scripts associated with
example, an event in the environment, such aggressive or risky behaviour or the forma-
as viewing an aggressive driving movie, can tion of new scripts through observational
influence behavioural patterns of the viewer, learning. In this context, media can play an
with past behavioural patterns and person influential role in one’s choice to engage in
factors, such as mood and personality, medi- aggression.
ating the choice of response. Figure 19.1 shows how the GAM factors
The output level of the GAM involves can interact and influence the modelling of
appraisal and decision processes, which can aggressive or risky driving behaviour.
lead to either impulsive or thoughtful action Elements of the ‘young male syndrome’
(Anderson and Bushman, 2002). For the contribute to person factors. Drivers who are
response to be a more thoughtful action, one young, male, and have higher levels of sen-
requires sufficient time and cognitive capac- sation seeking and trait aggression may be
ity to reappraise the situation. Alternative more susceptible to the situational factors of
responses are considered until one is chosen, media exposure to aggressive or risky driv-
but this does not imply that aggression has ing content and driving scenarios that involve
been averted. In fact, it is suggested that the frustration, provocation, and competition.
reappraisal process could activate memories Sensation seeking has been shown to be a
leading to an increase in anger, which could significant predictor of aggressive and risky
provide justification for retaliation and inter- driving, including the physical and verbal
fere with other higher-order cognitive pro- expression of driver anger and using the vehi-
cessing. Additionally, the individual could cle to express anger. It was also predictive of
become more aware of potential damage to lapses in concentration and minor losses of
their social image and the need to act aggres- vehicular control (Dahlen et  al., 2005). In
sively in order to maintain it (Anderson and the context of intrasexual competition, males
Bushman, 2002). may engage in displays of risky and aggres-
Related to the output level of the GAM, sive driving in an attempt to display charac-
Bandura (2001b) proposed that ‘human teristics that females desire in a mate, such
agency’ (i.e., using action to intentionally as social status and the ability to protect and
make things happen) involves both self- provide for a family. Males may emulate the
reactiveness and self-reflectiveness. The first revered actors portraying the onscreen heroes
involves individuals monitoring their own in an attempt to model such adaptive traits.
behaviour and being aware of the conditions Arousal produced from viewing a film
under which it is produced (i.e., cognitive and involving aggressive or risky driving may cre-
environmental). The second is an individual’s ate stronger aggressive emotional responses
higher metacognitive ability to reflect on and when the driver is later confronted with an
evaluate their actions, considering the effects, anger-provoking situation on the road. This
not only for themselves, but also for others. is particularly true if memory scripts for this
In a social context, when one encounters a behaviour already exist. The more one views
situation that is provoking, exciting, aggres- media depicting acts of aggressive or risky
sive, or risky, the GAM suggests there are a driving, the more likely memory scripts will
number of factors that will influence an indi- form, associating negative emotion and aggres-
vidual’s choice to respond with an aggressive sive driving responses with provoking or frus-
or risky behaviour. The recent viewing of trating driving situations. This contributes
384 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Figure 19.1  GAM factors included in the investigation of modelling of aggressive or risky driving

to the formation of driver anger and driver brakes, tires screeching, and quick acceleration
vengeance, where drivers more readily choose (Greenberg and Atkin, 1983). These acts
aggressive responses in certain driving situa- were estimated to be shown seven times per
tions (e.g., being cut off by another driver). hour, and drivers were predominantly young
males, rarely wearing seat belts, who engaged
in irregular and dangerous driving acts that
rarely resulted in negative consequences
TYPES OF MEDIA DEPICTING (e.g., death or injury, physical damage, or
AGGRESSIVE OR RISKY DRIVING legal penalties; Greenberg and Atkin, 1983).
A form of television programming specific
Various forms of aggressive or risky driving to driving is automobile commercials. Between
media can influence behaviour, including the years of 1998 and 2002, it was estimated
television, video games, movies, and the that 45% of North American automobile and
Internet. Research has specifically assessed truck commercials (n = 349) displayed unsafe
the magnitude of this content in the media. driving (Shin et  al., 2005). Aggressive driv-
During the years 1975 to 1980, it was esti- ing behaviour accounted for more than 80%
mated that within 784 driving scenes ana- of that content, with high speeds constitut-
lysed from primetime broadcasts of popular ing the majority (Shin et al., 2005). Between
television programmes, there were 1,301 acts the years of 2006 and 2007, it was found that
of irregular driving, with the most common approximately 20% of 200 Canadian automo-
acts being speeding, quick braking, squealing bile advertisements (i.e., television, magazine,
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND DANGEROUS DRIVING BEHAVIOUR 385

and newspaper) included some form of unsafe being damaged. In none of the scenes were
driving, such as speeding or hard stops (Watson legal penalties shown. Risky drivers tended
et al., 2010). Considering television commer- to be young males and lead characters, who
cials only, because of their ability to display were the heroes in the movie (Beullens et al.,
motion, it was estimated that 20% contained 2011b). These individuals can be perceived
depictions of speed. In each television adver- by young male viewers as successful, strong,
tisement in which speeding was displayed, a and dominant characters who serve as mod-
disclaimer was also presented, demonstrat- els, guiding them in how they should look and
ing the vehicle manufacturer’s awareness of behave in order to achieve similar status.
the unsafe or questionable driving displayed The popularity of social media, specifically
(Watson et al., 2010). YouTube, in the context of media depictions of
One type of televised event suggested aggressive or risky driving has been addressed
to clearly depict aggressive or risky driv- (Vingilis et  al., 2017). On this website, indi-
ing is competitive automobile racing within viduals can create a free account, post videos,
NASCAR (National Association for Stock Car and view others’ videos. Using the search
Racing), which has an estimated 75 million term ‘street racing’, over 33 million videos
fans viewing these events each year, and tens were found (as of April 30, 2015) and, of
of thousands attending in person (Vitaglione, the 10 most popular (i.e., averaging approxi-
2012). Dangerous and risky driving is broad- mately 787,677 views), nine were on public
cast live and is encouraged and reinforced roads, not racetracks. Searching terms of other
by both cheering fans and large monetary risky driving behaviours, such as ‘burnouts’
rewards. For example, the total purse for the or ‘drifting’, found similar results. The avail-
2015 Daytona 500 (the last year purse data ability of this information on the Internet is
was publicly available) was approximately evident, but who is watching these videos, and
$18 million. The winner received $1,581,453 how they influence driving behaviour, is not
and the last-place driver (i.e., the loser) still known, given the lack of research on this spe-
took home $262,390 (Mensching, 2015). The cific type of media (Vingilis et al., 2017).
celebratory nature surrounding awarding the A body of research, using various method-
winner with large sums of money creates an ologies, has demonstrated that risk-glorifying
air of acceptance of the behaviour and a ‘hero’ media, some including content depicting acts
for the fans. This combination has the capac- of aggressive and risky driving, has the capacity
ity to increase the possibility of modelling the to increase risk-promoting cognitions, arousal,
aggressive or risky driving behaviour (Atkin, affect, willingness to take risks in observed
1989; Beullens et al., 2011b). driving scenarios, and risky driving behaviour.
Popular action movies can contain scenes of These effects have been shown to last 24 hours,
aggressive or risky driving. Of 26 action mov- and even as long as several days, with some of
ies shown in Belgium between 2005 and 2006 the research demonstrating intervening vari-
(e.g., War of the Worlds, Casino Royale, and ables of sex differences and personality traits
The Fast and the Furious: Tokyo Drift), there of sensation seeking and aggressiveness.
were 287 driving scenes, with 129 depict-
ing risky driving (Beullens et  al., 2011b).
Frequently occurring risky driving behaviours
were speeding, tires screeching, brakes squeal- RESEARCH ON THE INFLUENCE
ing, and quick braking or sudden decreases OF MEDIA ON MODELLING
in speed. Risks associated with such driving AGGRESSIVE OR RISKY DRIVING
behaviours were rarely shown. Approximately
one-third were followed by a crash and showed Various research methods have been used to
the endangering vehicle, or its surroundings, investigate the influence of media on
386 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

modelling aggressive or risky driving. These higher levels of self-reported arousal and
include qualitative techniques (e.g., meta- more risk-related cognitions, compared to
analyses, surveys, correlation, and time series) a control group who played more neutral
and quantitative techniques (e.g., experimen- games, such as Tak, Crash Bandicoot or FIFA
tal studies). In a meta-analysis involving 88 2005 (Fischer et  al., 2007). No sex differ-
empirical studies investigating the effects of ences were demonstrated, which suggests this
different types of risk-glorifying media on media content influenced males and females
cognitions, affect, and behaviours, small to in the same manner. These games were also
moderate effects were found (Fischer et  al., shown to influence behaviour more specific
2011). These effects were documented across to driving, as measured with the Vienna Risk-
all types of research designs (e.g., experi- Taking Test. Males demonstrated greater
mental, longitudinal, correlational) and for risk-taking behaviour than females, as indi-
both males and females, though the effects cated by longer reaction times to abort risky
were larger for males and younger partici- driving manoeuvres depicted in video clips
pants (i.e., less than 24 years old). The largest (Fischer et al., 2007). The influence of video
effects were found when the stimuli in the game exposure on this task was also demon-
risk-glorifying media matched the context of strated to last a period of 24 hours (Fischer
the response measured (e.g., risky driving et al., 2009).
behaviour was most strongly influenced by Racing or ‘drive ‘em up’ video games are
risky driving media) and when the media accessible and can be played by young indi-
exposure was active versus passive (i.e., viduals who have never experienced driving
engaging in video game play versus viewing in the real world. This could influence their
media; Fischer et al., 2011). perception of risk on the roads and contribute
The Entertainment Software Association to the formation and use of memory scripts
(ESA) estimates that more than 164 mil- associated with risky driving behaviour. In
lion Americans play video games, with an one study, unlicensed adolescents indicated
estimated 60% of gamers being male (ESA, how often they played racing video games
2019a). One study surveyed over 4,000 US (e.g., Gran Turismo, Burnout) and then, two
high school students, aged 14 to 18 years, and years later, as licensed drivers, indicated their
found the estimated proportion of male gam- risk-taking attitudes and self-reported driv-
ers to be as high as 75% (Desai et al., 2010). ing behaviour for speeding, fun riding, and
The popularity of this industry is reflected drinking and driving (Beullens et al., 2011a).
in its 2019 generated revenue, estimated to Even after controlling for measured person-
be over $43 billion in the United States and ality traits of sensation seeking and aggres-
$135 billion globally (ESA, 2019a). Grand sion, video game playing was a significant
Theft Auto V was the third-best-selling video predictor of positive attitudes and actual self-
game of 2017 (ESA, 2019b), suggesting that reported driving behaviour related to speed-
a large proportion of young males are engag- ing and fun riding. Video game playing was a
ing in gaming content that promotes aggres- significant predictor of self-reported speeding
sive and risky driving behaviours. only in males, suggesting there may be moder-
Active engagement in aggressive and risky ating effects of sex on specific aggressive or
driving video games has been found to influ- risky driving behaviours. Video game play-
ence arousal levels and risk-promoting cog- ing was not associated with positive attitudes
nitions. Driving games, such as Need for towards drinking and driving nor with more
Speed, Burnout, and Midnight Racer require self-reported drinking and driving, suggesting
‘massive’ violations of traffic rules to win the modelling was specific to the content of the
(Fischer et al., 2007). A 20-minute session of media to which the participant was exposed
playing these games was shown to produce (Beullens et al., 2011a).
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND DANGEROUS DRIVING BEHAVIOUR 387

The influence of more passive types of Test) and controlled the exposure to risk-
media (i.e., movies and television) on the glorifying media, the media content was not
modelling of aggressive or risky driving has specific to aggressive or risky driving behav-
also been studied. Early exposure to reck- iours. Additionally, the simulated driving was
less driving (e.g., fast, careless) depicted in the context of a racing video game, which
in movies was found to influence adoles- could have influenced participants’ behav-
cents’ self-reported driving behaviour four iour through expectations of performance
years later (Kostermans et  al., 2014). Self- and a lower perceived risk for driving aggres-
report of unsafe driving behaviours included sively. Lastly, this research did not consider
exceeding the speed limit, weaving in and other mediating factors of arousal, sensation
out of traffic, and driving without a seatbelt seeking, trait aggression, or driving history.
fastened. Higher levels of sensation seek- Using archival data in the context of a
ing, measured using the Arnett Inventory of descriptive research approach, Vitaglione
Sensation Seeking (Arnett, 1994), were also (2012) analysed aggressive driving accident
found to be a significant predictor of reckless reports (i.e., needless dangerous behaviours
driving. Males with higher sensation seeking or increased risk of harm to other drivers), in
were more likely to report driving without relation to the timing of televised NASCAR
the use of a seatbelt, compared to females or events. West Virginia was selected as the test
those with lower levels of sensation seeking state because it was ranked first in the United
(Kostermans et al., 2014). States for the number of NASCAR fans per
Self-reporting of driving behaviour, par- capita and it does not have its own NASCAR
ticularly behaviours considered aggressive track (i.e., the predominant source of view-
or risky, may not produce accurate results. ing of these events is television). A period of
Participants may alter their responses to one week surrounding each of the 156 broad-
appear more socially desirable, which could casted NASCAR events, between 2003 and
produce under-reporting of aggressive or 2006, was used to investigate the cumulative
risky behaviour (e.g., more acceptable to modelling effects of mass media viewing of
society as a whole), as well as over-­reporting aggressive driving. This included the day
(e.g., an attempt to match a personal ideal before the event, the day of the event, and
of a ‘cool’ driver within a social circle of was limited to the five days immediately fol-
friends). Simulated driving has been used lowing the event, since NASCAR races could
to investigate the effects of watching risk-­ occur within one week of each other. Using
glorifying media on subsequent driving a time-series regression analysis, the rate
behaviour. In one study, participants watched of accidents and injuries due to aggressive
either a risk-promoting movie scene (e.g., a driving was regressed onto NASCAR dates
segment from a James Bond movie), or a neu- (Vitaglione, 2012).
tral scene from a local talk show, and then The number of aggressive driving acci-
drove on a simulator within the context of the dents significantly decreased on the day of the
racing video game Need for Speed (Fischer NASCAR events, possibly because fans were
et al., 2008). Controlling for experience with watching the televised event and not engag-
racing games, significant differences were ing in much driving that day (Vitaglione,
found, such that the risk-promoting condi- 2012). Both accidents and injuries due to
tion produced higher maximum speeds and aggressive driving significantly increased
more collisions for both males and females, on the fifth day following the event, but not
and less time to complete the entire course immediately following the race. During this
for males (Fischer et al., 2008). Though this same time period, alcohol-related accidents
experimental research used a measure closer and injuries did not change in the same way,
to realistic driving (i.e., Vienna Risk-Taking suggesting the specific media content was
388 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

associated with the m ­ odelling of s­pecific increases in this specific risky driving behav-
risky and aggressive driving behaviours iour occurred following the theatrical release
(Vitaglione, 2012). The delay in the effects of two Fast and Furious franchise movies,
of the media content may have been due to Fast and Furious 6 and Furious 7. Not all
resulting arousal from viewing a NASCAR aggressive or risky driving results in an acci-
race becoming associated with dangerous dent or collision, and, therefore, using the
or aggressive driving behaviours over time measure of speeding may produce a more
(Vitaglione, 2012). In the context of the accurate estimate of the amount of aggressive
GAM, the more an individual watches these or risky driving on the roads following such
races, the stronger the links become between media exposure. Both Fast and Furious mov-
arousal and associated aggressive memories, ies contain content that focused on aggres-
which include thoughts, feelings, and behav- sive or risky driving (i.e., car chases, racing,
iours. When an individual experiences similar excessive speeding) and had a large view-
arousal in a driving scenario, as the result of ership, based on their domestic box office
provocation, the associated aggressive driving performance (approximately $239 and $353
memory script may be activated or primed and million, respectively) (Nash Information
become consciously accessible. It may require Services, n.d.-a).
a number of days for these to be sufficiently Pre-movie-release speeding behaviour
activated in order to facilitate dangerous and was compared to post-movie-release speed-
risky driving (Vitaglione, 2012). However, an ing behaviour, with an emphasis on assessing
alternative explanation is that more immedi- effects immediately following movie release.
ate risky and aggressive driving behaviour A four-week post-movie-release period was
was occurring, but drivers had escaped unde- selected and compared to a four-week pre-
tected until the passage of time, resulting in movie-release period. These time periods
an accident or injury days later. Therefore, the were selected based on the steep trend in
measure used in this research was not sensi- the domestic box office profits for the first
tive enough to detect more immediate model- 30 days post movie release (Singhal, 2018).
ling effects (e.g., speeding) of this aggressive Figure 19.2 shows the daily cumulative
or risky driving media content. domestic box office for the eight Fast and
The United States National Highway Furious movies (Nash Information Services,
Traffic Safety Administration suggested that n.d.-a). The dotted line estimates the point at
excessive speeding be included in the defi- which the movies had played in theatres for
nition of aggressive driving, following con- approximately 30 days.
sensus among focus-group participants who Speeding infractions surrounding the
considered it aggressive, as well as aggressive release dates of Fast and Furious 6 and
drivers’ confessions of speeding more fre- Furious 7 (May 24, 2013 and April 3, 2015,
quently (Tasca, 2000). Though the intent of respectively) were provided by the Office
speeding behaviour may not always be known, of Traffic Safety in Edmonton, Alberta. All
the act of speeding has been used in research infractions included in the study were a
as a measure of risky driving (Iversen and result of automated enforcement using sta-
Rundmo, 2002; Jonah et  al., 2001; Simons- tionary cameras (i.e., photo radar). These
Morton et  al., 2005) and it is considered a cameras were permanently mounted, oper-
frequent antecedent behaviour to accident or ating 24 hours a day, seven days a week,
collision (Transport Canada, 2011). which ensured stable enforcement schedules
Building on the descriptive research over the selected time periods. The close
approach of Vitaglione (2012), Singhal (2018) proximity of cameras to theatres (i.e., five
used archival speeding infraction data from kilometres) allowed for the assessment of
Edmonton, Alberta, to investigate whether more immediate modelling effects of movie
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND DANGEROUS DRIVING BEHAVIOUR 389

Figure 19.2  Domestic box-office history for the Fast and Furious movies

content, though it was understood that closer precipitation (mm) and traffic volume were
distances were confounded with time (i.e., included in the analyses (Singhal, 2018), due
speeding detected closer to a theatre could to their influence on speeding (World Health
also reflect speeding which occurred a short Organization, 2004).
time after movie viewing). Infractions were Results revealed increases in aggressive
issued to the registered owner of the vehicle, or risky driving behaviour (i.e., speeding)
rather than the driver, since only the vehi- when Furious 7 was released and playing
cle was identified as being involved in the in theatres. Significant increases in speed-
offence. Unfortunately, personal identifiers, ing infractions were found specifically for
revealing driver and vehicle characteristics, the first weekend (i.e., opening weekend)
were not available in this study, limiting the and the first week, post movie release. A sig-
investigation of how these factors influence nificant increase in mean speed differential
speeding behaviour (Singhal, 2018). was also found for the first weekend, which
An interrupted time-series approach was means that not only did the number of speed-
used to assess whether movie release was ing infractions increase, but also the speed
related to increases in speeding. The speed- at which drivers were caught speeding was
ing infraction data were linked in time, such greater. These effects were significant even
that data points were recorded consecutively after controlling for precipitation, traffic
on a daily basis. The interrupted approach volume, Saturdays, Sundays, and autocor-
allowed for the specification of the movie relation in the data. This timeline of change
release date as a possible intervention in the was different from that reported for aggres-
time series, investigating whether changes in sive driving accidents following exposure to
this driving behaviour occurred. Coding of televised NASCAR races (Vitaglione, 2012),
specific days and intervention time periods which, as mentioned previously, was likely
(e.g., first weekend post movie release) was due to the use of a more sensitive measure
used to assess patterns of behaviour change of modelling aggressive or risky driving
post-intervention. Other variables of daily (Singhal, 2018).
390 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Though Furious 7 was released during an aggression, driver anger, driver vengeance,
Easter holiday, comparison analyses, using driving history, and movie and video game
time periods surrounding the previous two history were also included in the analyses
Easter holidays, failed to demonstrate the (Singhal, 2018).
same significant changes in speeding infrac- Significant differences in aggressive or
tions and mean speed differential, during the risky driving behaviour as a function of video
post-period. Overall, these results suggest condition were not found, though differences
that early modelling effects of aggressive were in the expected direction (e.g., higher
driving (i.e., speeding) can occur following mean speed, shorter time to completion, and
the release of movies containing aggressive greater passing frequency). Some issues sug-
or risky driving content (Singhal, 2018). gested to account for the lack of statistical sig-
Similar modelling effects were not found nificance were the use of videos with low plot
for Fast and Furious 6, but the difference content, a driving simulator test course with
in domestic and worldwide box office gross too few opportunities for speeding, and the
(Nash Information Services, n.d.-c n.d.-a) viewing of movie content in an experimen-
suggests Furious 7 had a greater viewership tal context and not a typical social environ-
(Singhal, 2018). Also, Furious 7 received ment, such as a theatre with friends (Singhal,
greater news coverage due to the accidental 2018). No sex differences were found for the
death of one of the lead actors (Paul Walker) behavioural measures of aggressive driving
prior to the completion of filming his role in and it was suggested that the lower number of
Furious 7 (Ying, 2015). Walker was killed in male than female participants may have con-
a high-speed collision unrelated to the movie tributed (i.e., a ratio of 1:2) (Singhal, 2018).
production. Curiosity surrounded how the Personality factors of higher trait aggres-
director would handle the actor’s death in sion and sensation seeking were related to
the movie and the ongoing storyline of the more aggressive and risky driving. In addi-
franchise. Even though the more recent fran- tion, those with higher driving vengeance and
chise instalment Fate of the Furious broke a history of violations, particularly speeding,
the global box office record for an opening engaged in more aggressive and risky driv-
weekend (April 14, 2017) (Nash Information ing, particularly during a provoking racing
Services, n.d.-b), it did not surpass the domes- scenario in which two sports cars chased one
tic box office gross for the opening weekend another. Given that the majority of violations
of Furious 7 (approximately $98 million reported were for speeding, this suggested
compared to $147 million; Nash Information that drivers with a history of speeding were
Services, n.d.-d). more influenced by environmental cues pro-
To further demonstrate the capacity of moting this behaviour (Singhal, 2018). These
aggressive or risky driving content to influ- findings support the interactivity of internal-
ence the modelling of this driving behav- state and situation factors associated with
iour, Singhal (2018) used an experimental aggressive driving, specifically the contribu-
research approach that incorporated multiple tion of personality factors and aggressive-
intervening factors together in a single study. promoting driving cognition (i.e., driving
University students were exposed to one of vengeance) on the choice to drive aggres-
three video clips, which differed in the level sively or with greater risk. Individuals with
of aggressive and risky driving content or these characteristics may be more suscepti-
arousal, and then drove through a set course ble to aggressive and risky driving content in
on a simulator. Measures of speed, accelera- movies.
tion, and overtaking or passing actions were Higher levels of sensation seeking and
used to assess risky driving, and individual trait aggression were both related to higher
factors of age, sex, sensation seeking, trait levels of driving anger, driving vengeance,
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND DANGEROUS DRIVING BEHAVIOUR 391

and number of violations, suggesting these the second study to contribute to aggressive
personality factors played a role in aggressive or risky driving (e.g., higher trait aggression,
driving cognition and behaviour. Sensation sensation seeking, and driving vengeance).
seeking was also positively correlated with Demographics were not known for the speed
the number of aggressive driving movies violators, but, according to the evolutionary
viewed in the past two years and the greater theory of modelling aggression (Mesquida
viewing of these movies was associated with and Wiener, 1996; Vingilis et  al., 2013;
higher levels of driving vengeance, which was Wiesenthal and Singhal, 2012; Wilson and
related to more violations (Singhal, 2018). Daly, 1985), the majority of these individuals
Some future research questions that arise were likely young males, who experienced a
from the Singhal (2018) studies include the higher level of anonymity on the roads, com-
longevity or duration of the cinematic influ- pared to driving in a laboratory setting.
ences and whether generalization occurs, Findings associated with higher levels of
such that risky behaviours manifest in other sensation seeking suggest that these personal-
realms. For example, will drivers who speed ity types may seek out and view more aggres-
after viewing Fast and Furious movies now sive and risky driving movies. This, in turn,
swim further from shore or be predisposed to contributes to the formation of aggressive and
gambling or engaging in risky sex when the risky driving scripts, which can produce cer-
situation presents itself? tain susceptibilities for the activation of these
scripts at a later time, with further viewing of
aggressive and risky driving movie content.
The eventual engagement in the modelling
THEORIES OF MODELLING OF of this behaviour may be more likely when
AGGRESSION REVISITED the driver is exposed to a provoking driving
scenario (e.g., racing or vengeance related).
The results of the two studies by Singhal These relationships support the interactivity
(2018) offer support to theories of modelling of the person, situation, and internal factors
aggression and contribute to a better under- of the modified GAM, with respect to mod-
standing of how aggressive and risky driving elling aggressive and risky driving (Figure
occurs. Both studies support the social learn- 19.1), and also support Bandura’s (2001b)
ing theory of modelling aggression (Bandura, Triadic Reciprocal Causation model of psy-
1971). Evidence of greater speeding sur- chosocial functioning.
rounding the release of Furious 7 suggested
that the modelling of aggressive or risky
driving behaviour was associated with the
release of this movie, which portrayed this CONCLUSION
behaviour being performed by a relatable
‘hero’ and, predominantly, in the absence of Given the profits associated with movies
negative consequences. Trends in the second containing aggressive or risky driving con-
study provided support for this theory of tent, a halt in their production is not likely to
modelling aggression, though differences occur. The large domestic box office gross
between the video conditions were not sig- associated with the Fast and Furious fran-
nificant. It is important to consider that these chise demonstrates that a large fan base
studies used different cohorts (i.e., speed exists for this genre of movie. The next
violators versus undergraduate psychology instalment, Fast and Furious 9, has an antici-
research pool participants). The speed viola- pated release date of April 2, 2021 (IMDb,
tors in the first study would have been more 2019). There are also indications of popular-
likely to possess the characteristics found in ity for other forms of media depicting acts of
392 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

aggressive and risky driving. As mentioned franchise (Orwall, 2001) and production
previously, the top 10 YouTube videos con- companies could incorporate these messages
taining this content, particularly street racing, into the credits of the movie, following its
average over 700,000 views and ‘likes’ completion. It may intrigue the movie-goer
(Vingilis et  al., 2017). These videos, which to view additional footage of the actors and
can be freely accessed and repeatedly viewed, the message may be more meaningful com-
are unlike movies in that the majority depict ing from these individuals, who are revered
real-world extreme driving events that were in their role. This may also delay the movie-
not performed by professionals in a staged goers from returning to their vehicles imme-
environment. Research has not begun to diately following the end of the movie,
understand how this content influences a potentially allowing for some decrease in
person’s driving behaviour (Vingilis et  al., their immediate level of arousal.
2017). Almost 20 years ago, Bandura (2001a: Lastly, we should not remove account-
271) recognized the ‘accelerated growth of ability from the viewer. Bandura’s early
video delivery technologies’ and how they work (Bandura, 1971; Bandura et al., 1963a)
facilitate exposure to a wide range of behav- demonstrated the basic principles of observa-
iours and ‘models’. Social media has tional learning and modelling, and his later
expanded that substantially since then. reflections emphasized that human behaviour
A complete halt in production of movies is not simply the result of stimulus-response
such as Furious 7 or other forms of media relationships. We, as humans, have ‘func-
depicting acts of aggressive or risky driving is tional consciousness’, part of which involves
not a realistic solution to reducing the model- regulating and evaluating the actions we
ling of this behaviour. However, the prolifera- choose to take (Bandura, 2001b). The two
tion of this content makes it important to raise core features of Bandura’s (2001b) proposed
awareness about the association between this ‘human agency’ are self-reactiveness and self-­
material and one’s driving behaviour, particu- reflectiveness, mentioned previously. It is these
larly the dangers associated with modelling. two elements that are emphasized to viewers
Singhal’s (2018) finding of portrayals of this of aggressive and risky driving media content.
behaviour in the movies having a significant Viewers make choices to expose themselves
impact on real-world speeding behaviour to this content and, therefore, have a respon-
supports increasing enforcement when these sibility to be aware of how this content can
movies are released and playing in theatres. influence their behaviour, and to evaluate their
This is particularly true for opening week- actions accordingly (e.g., considering driv-
ends and the first week post movie release. ing laws and the safety of others). Essentially,
Speeding is a risk factor in road traffic inju- Bandura (2001b) is highlighting the appraisal
ries and fatalities, because of the increased and decision-making process of the modi-
risk of collision and severity of the resulting fied GAM, as shown in Figure 19.1. Having
consequences. It is estimated that 30–50% viewers engage in self-reactiveness and self-­
of mortality on the roads is associated with reflectiveness could decrease the probability
speeding (World Health Organization, 2004). of an impulsive action and increase the prob-
Alternate strategies could be more proac- ability of a thoughtful one. This would, ulti-
tive, targeting the movie-goer and raising mately, hamper the modelling of aggressive
awareness of the potential for modelling the and risky driving following exposure to such
unsafe driving behaviour. This could include content in the media.
messages from the actors themselves, urging Drivers must be cognisant of the fact that
people to be responsible and smart on the they share the road with other drivers, vehi-
roads and to drive safely. Universal Studios cle occupants (e.g., children), cyclists, and
did this in the past for the Fast and Furious pedestrians. With this in mind, all drivers
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND DANGEROUS DRIVING BEHAVIOUR 393

should consider characteristics that consti- Bandura, A. (1971). Psychological modeling:


tute a ‘good’ driver. Qualities which lessen Conflicting theories. New Jersey: Aldine
the occurrence of factors that contribute to Transaction.
aggressive driving should be encouraged. For Bandura, A. (2001a). Social cognitive theory of
example, forethought, in the context of driv- mass communication. Media Psychology, 3,
265–299.
ing, would involve allowing sufficient time
Bandura, A. (2001b). Social cognitive theory:
to arrive at a final destination, which could
An agentic perspective. Annual Review of
prevent the development of frustration when Psychology, 52, 1–26.
confronted with delays in traffic or other Bandura, A., Ross, D., & Ross, S. A. (1963a).
hampering driver actions. Forbearance (i.e., Imitation of film-mediated aggressive models.
self-restraint and tolerance) and forgiveness Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology,
of other drivers’ minor mistakes (e.g., driving 66, 3–11.
too slowly in the fast lane) could lessen the Bandura, A., Ross, D., & Ross, S. A. (1963b).
likelihood of choosing an aggressive driving Vicarious reinforcement and imitative learn-
response. Factors such as these, in addition ing. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychol-
to being aware of the possibility of aggres- ogy, 67, 601–607.
sive driving motion-picture content influenc- Berkowitz, L. (2012). A cognitive-neoassociation
theory of aggression. In P. Van Lange, A.
ing driving behaviour, are encouraged in all
Kruglanski, & E. Higgins (Eds.), Theories of
drivers. Young males may need to be par-
Social Psychology (Vol. 2., pp. 99–117),
ticularly aware of heightened sensitivity to London: Sage.
provocation or competitiveness on the roads Beullens, K., Roe, K., & Van den Bulck, J.
and appropriately inhibit aggressive or risky (2011a). Excellent gamer, excellent driver? The
responses. Just as many drinking and driving impact of adolescents’ video game playing on
campaigns target the driver in emphasizing driving behavior: A two-wave panel study.
the dangers and potentially tragic conse- Accident Analysis and Prevention, 43, 58–65.
quences of impaired driving (Green, 2013; Beullens, K., Roe, K., & Van den Bulck, J. (2011b).
MADD, 2017), research has highlighted the The portrayal of risk-taking in traffic: A content
need for drivers to keep their safety, and the analysis of popular action movies. Journal of
safety of the public, at the forefront when Communications Research, 2, 21–27.
Bushman, B. J. (1995). Moderating role of trait
considering engaging in an aggressive or
aggressiveness in the effects of violent media
risky driving behaviour. on aggression. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 69, 950–960.
Buss, D. M. (1988). The evolution of intrasexual
competition: Tactics of mate attraction.
REFERENCES Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
54, 616–628.
Abelson, R. P. (1981). Psychological status of Buss, A. H. & Perry, M. (1992). The aggression
the script concept. American Psychologist, questionnaire. Journal of Personality and
36, 715–729. Social Psychology, 63, 452–459.
Anderson, C. A. & Bushman, B. J. (2002). Human Coleman, L. (2004). The copycat effect: How
aggression. Annual Review of Psychology, the media and popular culture trigger the
53, 27–51. mayhem in tomorrow’s headlines. New York:
Arnett, J. (1994). Sensation seeking: A Paraview Pocket Books.
new conceptualization and a new scale. Dahlen, E. R., Martin, R. C., Ragan, K., &
Personality and Individual Differences, 16, Kuhlman, M. M. (2005). Driving anger, sen-
289–296. sation seeking, impulsiveness, and boredom
Atkin, C. K. (1989). Television, socialization and proneness in the prediction of unsafe
risky driving by teenagers. Alcohol, Drugs, driving. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 37,
and Driving, 5, 1–11. 341–348.
394 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Deb, S. (2019, January 2). Netflix to ‘Bird Box’ racing games increase risk-taking cognitions,
fans: Please open your eyes. The New York affect, and behaviors? Journal of Experimen-
Times. Retrieved January 14, 2019, from tal Psychology: Applied, 13, 22–31.
www.nytimes.com/2019/01/02/arts/net Freedman, J. L. (2007). Television violence and
flix-bird-box-challenge-war ning.html? aggression: Setting the record straight. The
module=inline Media Institute Policy Views, 1, 1–10.
Desai, R. A., Krishnan-Sarin, S., Cavallo, D. & Goldberg, O. (2001). ‘Fast’ fallout film may
Potenza, M. N. (2010). Video-gaming among inspire copycat racers. The Free Library.
high school students: Health correlates, Retrieved June 8, 2020, from web.archive.
gender differences, and problematic gaming. org/web/20151023223740/www.thefreeli-
Pediatrics, 126(6), e1414-e1424. Retrieved brary.com/’FAST’+FALLOUT+FILM+MAY+INS
from https://pediatrics.aappublications.org/ PIRE+COPYCAT+RACERS.-a083598815
content/126/6/e1414 Green, M. (2013, July 24). Drunk driving pre-
Eron, L. D., Huesmann, L. R., Lefkowitz, M. M., vention and the effectiveness of media cam-
& Walder, L. O. (1972). Does television vio- paigns. Retrieved June 26, 2020, from www.
lence cause aggression? American Psycholo- absoluteadvocacy.org/drunk-driving-prevention-
gist, 27(4), 253–263. media-campaigns/
Entertainment Software Association (ESA) Greenberg, B. S. & Atkin, C. K. (1983). The por-
(2019a). ESA leadership desk: Telling our trayal of driving on television, 1975–1980.
industry’s story in a new way. Retrieved June Journal of Communication, 33, 44–55.
7, 2019, from www.theesa.com/perspectives/ Grewal, S. & Brennan, R. (2006, June 23).
esa-leadership-desk-telling-our-industrys- Racing film has police on edge. The Toronto
story-in-a-new-way/ Star, p. B4.
Entertainment Software Association (ESA) Grey, E. M., Triggs, T. J., & Haworth, N. L. (1989).
(2019b). 2018 Essential facts about the com- Driver aggression: The role of personality,
puter and video game industry. Retrieved social characteristics, risk and motivation
June 7, 2019, from www.theesa.com/ (Report No. CR 81). Federal Office of Road
esa-research/2018-essential-facts-about-the- Safety, Australia.
computer-and-video-game-industry/ Hegerl, U., Koburger, N., Rummel-Kluge, C.,
Fischer, P., Greitemeyer, T., Kastenmüller, A., Gravert, C., Walden, M., & Mergl, R. (2013).
Vogrincic, C., & Sauer, A. (2011). The effects One followed by many? Long-term effects of
of risk glorifying media exposure on risk- a celebrity suicide on the number of suicidal
positive cognitions, emotions, and behav- acts on the German railway net. Journal of
iours: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Affective Disorders, 146, 39–44.
Bulletin, 137, 367–390. Helfgott, J. B. (2015). Criminal behaviour and
Fischer, P., Greitemeyer, T., Morton, T., Kasten- the copycat effect: Literature review and
müller, A., Postmes, T., Frey, D., Kubitzki, J., theoretical framework for empirical investi-
& Odenwälder, J. (2009). The racing-game gation. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 22,
effect: Why do video racing games increase 46–64.
risk-taking inclinations? Personality and IMDb (2018, December 21). Bird Box (2018).
Social Psychology Bulletin, 35, 1395–1409. Retrieved June 28, 2019, from www.imdb.
Fischer, P., Guter, S., & Frey, D. (2008). The com/title/tt2737304/
effects of risk-promoting media on inclina- IMDb (2019, November 11). Fast & Furious 9
tions toward risk taking. Basic and Applied (2020). Retrieved June 7, 2020. from www.
Social Psychology, 30, 230–240. imdb.com/title/tt5433138/
Fischer, P., Krueger, J. I., Greitemeyer, T., Asal, K., Iversen, H. & Rundmo, T. (2002). Personality,
Aydin, N., & Vingilis, E. (2012). Psychological risky driving and accident involvement
effects of risk glorification in the media: among Norwegian drivers. Personality and
Towards an integrative view. European Review Individual Differences, 33, 1251–1263.
of Social Psychology, 23, 224–257. Jeong, J., Shin, S. D., Kim, H., Hong, Y. C.,
Fischer, P., Kubitzki, J., Guter, S., & Frey, D. Hwang, S. S., & Lee, E. J. (2012). The effects
(2007). Virtual driving and risk taking: Do of celebrity suicide on copycat suicide
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND DANGEROUS DRIVING BEHAVIOUR 395

attempt: A multi-center observational study. share of $18 million payday. Retrieved from
Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, www.sbnation.com/nascar/2015/2/22/
47, 957–965. 8085643/2015-daytona-500-purse-prize-
Jonah, B. A., Thiessen, R., & Au-Yeung, E. money
(2001). Sensation seeking, risky driving and Mesquida, C. G. & Wiener, N. I. (1996). Human
behavioral adaptation. Accident Analysis & collective aggression: A behavioral ecology
Prevention, 33, 679–684. perspective. Ethology and Sociobiology, 17,
Koburger, N., Mergl, R., Rummel-Kluge, C., 247–262.
Ibelshäuser, A., Meise, U., Postuvan, V., Milgram, S. & Shotland, R. L. (1973). Television
Roskar, S., Székely, A., Ditta Tóth, M., van and antisocial behavior: Field experiments.
der Feltz-Cornelis, C., & Hegerl, U. (2015). New York: Academic Press.
Celebrity suicide on the railway network: Modeling (n.d.). In Encyclopedia of Mental
Can one case trigger international effects? Disorders online. Retrieved on June 28, 2015
Journal of Affective Disorders, 185, 38–46. from www.minddisorders.com/Kau-Nu/
Koordeman, R., Anschutz, D. J., & Engels, R. C. Modeling.html
M. E. (2011). Exposure to alcohol commer- Nash Information Services (n.d.-a). The Num-
cials in movie theaters affects actual alcohol bers: Box office history for Fast and Furious
consumption in young adult high weekly movies. Retrieved March 1, 2017, from
drinkers: An experimental study. American www.thenumbers.com/movies/franchise/
Journal on Addictions, 20, 285–291. Nash Information Services (n.d.-b). The Num-
Kostermans, E., Stoolmiller, M., de Leeuw, R. bers: The Fate of the Furious (2017). Retrieved
N., Engels, R. C., & Sargent, J. D. (2014). April 18, 2017, from www.the-numbers.com/
Exposure to movie reckless driving in early movie/Fate-of-the-Furious-The#tab=summary
adolescence predicts reckless, but not inat- Nash Information Services (n.d.-c). The Num-
tentive driving. Public Library of Science bers: Furious 7 (2015). Retrieved February
(PLoS ONE), 9, e113927. 28, 2017, from www.the-numbers.com/
Kotzen, S. (2013, September 25). ‘The Pro- movie/Furious-7#tab=summary
gram’ turns 20, along with its notorious road Nash Information Services (n.d.-d). The Num-
scene (video). The Hollywood Reporter. bers: Movie comparison: The Fate of the
Retrieved on June 15, 2015 from www.hol- Furious (2017) vs. Furious 7 (2105). Retrieved
lywoodreporter.com/news/program-turns- April 20, 2017, from www.the-numbers.
20-along-notorious-636568 com/movies/custom-comparisons/Fate-of-
List of Chess Grandmasters (n.d.). Retrieved Sep- the-Furious-The/Furious-7
tember 15, 2019, from https://en.m.wikipedia. Orwall, B. (2001, June 21). Fearing copycats,
org/wiki/List_of_chess_grandmasters Universal warns against mimicking new
López-Guimerà, G., Levine, M. P., Sánchez- action film. The Wall Street Journal. Retrieved
Carracedo, D., & Fauquet, J. (2010). Influ- from www.wsj.com/articles/SB99307515845
ence of mass media on body image and 592312?mod=searchresults&page=1&pos=1
eating disordered attitudes and behaviors in Paik, H. & Comstock, G. (1994). The effects of
females: A review of effects and processes. television violence on antisocial behavior: A
Media Psychology, 13, 387–416. meta-analysis. Communication Research,
MADD (2017, June 9). And then, in a heartbeat, 21(4), 516–546.
everything changed! Retrieved on September Peng, J. (2018, September 24). Doubling of
15, 2019 from http://madd.ca/pages/and- motorcycle deaths in B.C. alarms safety offi-
then-in-a-heartbeat-everything-changed/ cials. The Star. Retrieved October 10, 2018,
Malamuth, N. M. & Check, J. P. V. (1981). The from www.thestar.com/vancouver/2018/09/
effects of mass media exposure on accept- 24/doubling-of-motorcycle-deaths-alarms-
ance of violence against women: A field safety-officials.html
experiment. Journal of Research in Personal- Phillips, D. P. (1974). The influence of sugges-
ity, 15, 436–446. tion on suicide: Substantive and theoretical
Mensching, K. (2015, February 22). Daytona implications of the Werther effect. American
500 purse 2015: Joey Logano take winner’s Sociological Review, 39, 340–354.
396 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Phillips, D. P. & Carstensen, L. L. (1988). The Tasca, L. (2000). A review of the literature on
effect of suicide stories on various demo- aggressive driving research. Ontario Advi-
graphic groups, 1968–1985. Suicide and sory Group on Safe Driving Secretariat, Road
Life-Threatening Behavior, 18, 100–114. User Safety Branch, Ontario Ministry of
Pristin, T. & Fox, D. (1993, October 23). Pop Transportation.
culture. Violence. Copycats. Blame?: Holly- Tedeschi, J. T. & Felson, R. B. (1994). Violence,
wood debates the responsibility issue after aggression, and coercive actions. Washington,
Disney’s edit of ‘The Program’. Los Angeles DC: American Psychological Association.
Times. Retrieved February 5, 2016, from Toronto Transit Commission (2010, February 17).
http://articles.latimes.com/1993-10-23/enter- Subway suicide prevention. Retrieved on May
tainment/ca-48913_1_responsibility-issue 28, 2015 from www.ttc.ca/About_the_TTC/
Roseborough, J. E. W. (2014). Retaliatory Commission_reports_and_information/Com-
aggressive driving: A justice perspective mission_meetings/2010/Feb_17_2010/Supple-
(Unpublished doctoral dissertation). York mentary_Reports/Subway_Suicide_Preve.pdf
University, Toronto. Transport Canada (2011). Road safety in
Rowland, B. (2006, June 23). CHP concerned Canada. Retrieved from www.tc.gc.ca/
after release of racing movie. Free Lance media/ documents/roadsafety/tp15145e.pdf
News. Retrieved from www.sanbenito.com/ US driver in ‘Bird Box blindfold’ crashes in Utah.
chp-concerned-after-release-of-racing-movie/ (2019, January 11). BBC News. Retrieved
Shin, P. C., Hallett, D., Chipman, M. L., Tator, January 14, 2019, from www.bbc.com/
C., & Granton, J. T. (2005). Unsafe driving in news/world-us-canada-46846981
North American automobile commercials. Vingilis, E., Seeley, J., Wiesenthal, D., Mann, R.,
Journal of Public Health, 27, 318–325. Vingilis-Jaremko, L., Vanlaar, W., & Leal, N.
Shinar, D. (1998). Aggressive driving: The con- (2013). Street racing and stunt driving in
tribution of the drivers and the situation. Ontario, Canada: Results of a web-based
Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psy- survey of car and racing enthusiasts. Trans-
chology and Behaviour, 1, 137–160. portation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology
Simons-Morton, B., Lerner, N., & Singer, J. and Behaviour, 21, 30–42.
(2005). The observed effects of teenage pas- Vingilis, E., Yildirim-Yenier, Z., Vingilis-Jaremko,
sengers on the risky driving behavior of teen- L., Wickens, C., Seeley, J., Fleiter, J., &
age drivers. Accident Analysis & Prevention, Grushka, D. H. (2017). Literature review on
37, 973–982. risky driving videos on YouTube: Unknown
Singhal, D. (2018). Modelling aggressive or risky effects and areas for concern? Traffic Injury
driving: The effect of cinematic portrayals of Prevention, 18, 606–615.
risky driving (Doctoral dissertation, York Uni- Vitaglione, G. D. (2012). Driving under the
versity, Toronto). Retrieved from YorkSpace. influence (of mass media): A four-year exam-
(http://hdl.handle.net/10315/34497) ination of NASCAR and West Virginia
Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organ- aggressive-driving accidents and injuries.
isms: An experimental analysis. New York: Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 42,
Appleton-Century-Crofts. 488–505.
Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human Watson, L., Lavack, A. M., Rudin-Brown, C.,
behavior. Toronto: Simon and Schuster. Burns, P., & Mintz, J. H. (2010). Message
Steinberg, M. S. & Dodge, K. A. (1983). Attri- content in Canadian automotive advertising:
butional bias in aggressive adolescent boys A role for regulation? Canadian Public Policy,
and girls. Journal of Social and Clinical Psy- 36(Supplement 1), S49–S67.
chology, 1, 312–321. Weiss, B., Dodge, K. A., Bates, J. E., & Pettit, G. S.
Steinberg, L. & Monahan, K. C. (2007). Age (1992). Some consequences of early harsh
differences in resistance to peer influence. discipline: Child aggression and a maladap-
Developmental Psychology, 43, 1531–1543. tive social information processing style. Child
Talwar, P. (1998). The family and peer group Development, 63, 1321–1335.
influences in aggression. Indian Journal of Wiesenthal, D. L., Lustman, M., & Roseborough,
Psychiatry, 40, 346. J. (2016). Aggressive driving. In A. Smiley
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND DANGEROUS DRIVING BEHAVIOUR 397

(Ed.), Human Factors in Traffic Safety (3rd ed., from www.who.int/violence_injury_preven-


pp. 169–193), Tucson, AZ: Lawyers & Judges tion/publications/road_traffic/world_report/
Publishing Company. speed_en.pdf
Wiesenthal, D. L. & Singhal, D. (2012). Ying, L. (2015, February 6). Hype’s must watch:
Evolutionary psychology, demography and Furious 7. Retrieved on May 15, 2015 from
driver safety research: A theoretical synthesis. https://hype.my/2015/40192/hypes-
In C. Roberts (Ed.), Applied Evolutionary must-watch-furious-7/
Psychology (pp. 399–413), Oxford, UK: Zillmann, D. (1971). Excitation transfer in
Oxford University Press. communication-mediated aggressive behavior.
Wilson, M. & Daly, M. (1985). Competitiveness, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 7,
risk taking, and violence: The young male syn- 419–434.
drome. Ethology and Sociobiology, 6, 59–73. Zuckerman, M. (1979). Sensation seeking:
World Health Organization (2004). Road Beyond the optimum level of arousal. Hillsdale,
safety – Speed. Retrieved on June 15, 2015 NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
20
Evolutionary Psychology and
Mass Media
Gayle S. Stever

When considering the topic of evolutionary To tackle these questions, one must first define
psychology and mass media, the questions mass media, also referred to as mass commu-
come very quickly. For example: nication. Mass communication is the result ‘of
forces set in motion when groups of manlike
• As media has become more a part of human [sic] animals first huddled together against the
life, have humans adapted to it in an evolutionary cold and danger of primitive times’ (Schramm,
sense, i.e. has natural selection had a chance to play 1960: 3). News media is relevant because of
a part in the way humans adapt to mass media?
the biological imperative for survival and the
• If survival and reproduction are the two evolutionary
function of news to give us information about
imperatives, which has been the bigger influence in
the way media has developed in the last century? our environment that will affect the likelihood
• If men and women have different reproductive of that survival (Grabe, 2011; Shoemaker,
agendas, how has that affected the different 1996). Are those the primary origins and func-
ways that men and women use media? tions of mass media, or are there others? This
• Are mass media and entertainment adaptations, chapter attempts to address these issues and
or are they by-products of adaptations? questions.
• Are there ways that mass media improves human
chances for survival?
• Are there ways that mass media affects our
opportunities to engage in sexual behavior and ORIGINAL USES FOR MEDIA
thus have more chances to reproduce?
• Or are there ways that mass media inhibits
our sexual behavior and keeps us from having What was the first form of media and for
chances to reproduce? what was it used? Schramm (1988) described
• What are the individual differences in how media the discovery of early cave paintings created
is processed as affected by both social construc- in prehistoric times and speculated as to their
tivism vs biological essentialism? purpose. The most likely use of these early
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND MASS MEDIA 399

paintings was for one generation of people to symbolic environment within an imaginary
educate the next as to what the customs and and representational world. These mediated
practices of that culture had been. It is possi- worlds are possible as technology becomes
ble that these early pictures represented magi- more sophisticated and it becomes possible
cal beliefs and secrets of that group (Schramm, to create more lifelike images. Education and
1988), shared with the youth of each succes- also enhanced experience are made possible
sive generation. Learning about the ways and through these mediated formats.
skills needed for survival was an essential part For example, The Henry Ford Museum in
of being socialized and insured that the group Dearborn, Michigan featured a film in 2018
would survive. In a basic fundamental way, about the true story of explorer Henry Bates’
the first media was an integral part of the sur- 11-year journey through the Amazon rainfor-
vival of individuals. est. Using IMAX technology, the viewer is
It has been suggested that the progress and transported to the Brazilian rainforest where
evolution of technology in human history a trip down the Amazon is experienced, one
proceeded along almost Lamarckian lines, if that few people would actually be able to
the inventions of one generation are able to be physically undertake. This expanded world-
communicated to a subsequent generation via view is possible because increasingly realis-
some media representation. If adaptive capac- tic media forms make it so.
ity is improved by tools that are shared from
one generation to the next via media represen-
tations, it can be speculated that brain capacity
is shaped and accelerated via this ‘inherited’ CONSIDERING MEDIA LIFE
shared knowledge. This is an example of the
way that interaction between culture and biol- It has been argued that we live ‘in’ rather
ogy can accelerate the development of knowl- than ‘with’ media in the 21st century (Deuze,
edge that affects the survival of individuals, 2011). If this is so, then it becomes important
thus increasing the chance that their genes to consider whether humans have adapted to
are passed on. Those whose genetic potential media, or have simply assimilated it into the
best leads to this kind of communication are non-mediated aspects of their lives. If you,
the ones who will survive to reproduce. Thus, the reader, find yourself doubting that this
media plays a critical role in Darwinian evolu- could be so, that may be because as media
tion (Toth and Schick, 1993). becomes a greater part of our lives, it also
From those earliest points moving for- becomes more invisible to us on a conscious
ward, media continued to provide a way level. As we become habituated to the pres-
that knowledge, history, and social struc- ence of media in our lives, we become less
tures were conveyed from one generation to aware of its presence and influence on a day-
the next. Education has been a fundamen- to-day or even minute-to-minute basis.
tal function of media and it is an important Think of it as like electricity. There was a
way to increase one’s odds of surviving to time in human life when electricity was not
reproduce. Media provides the cornerstone a pervasive part of every aspect of life. At
of societal norms, traditions, and skills for that time, it was easy to sort out what parts
preserving knowledge for future success and of life involved electricity and what parts did
continuation of a social group. not. As it became more available, fewer and
As media developed more sophisticated fewer things were done apart from electric-
forms of delivery, it provided better rep- ity. Cooking, cleaning, reading, even outdoor
resentations of day-to-day reality, starting camping became infused with electricity to
with drawings on cave walls and progress- the point where while once we considered
ing to the current forms of media that include whether a situation involved electricity, now
virtual reality, a complete recreation of a it is taken for granted that all parts of our life
400 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

involve use of electricity. In addition, and than looking for the effects of mass media on
more important for this analogy, we do not evolution, it is better to consider how human
think or talk about it much at this point in beings as they have already evolved are able
human history. It is a non-discursive aspect to adapt to a mediated environment. Because
of our shared lives together. our culture is dominated by various forms of
Mass media is becoming like that. We media, it could be argued that those who are
spend so much time in mediated worlds that best able to survive and reproduce in such an
we no longer consider the mediated parts of environment will pass their genes on to off-
our lives as being separate from the rest of spring. This has always been the case with
our lives. Consider what happens to you dur- social environments as they have shaped
ing a power failure. This recently happened natural selection in the past. Another way to
to me, and while I was seeking alternate say this is that to the extent that our environ-
forms of power for basic needs (lighting, ment becomes more media saturated, it
cooking, warmth), I was also seeking forms becomes more essential to adapt to this type
of media that did not require my home’s elec- of environment or not be successful in the
tricity (books, newspapers, battery-powered tasks of survival and reproduction. Changing
radios, my cell phone that didn’t depend on the overall structure of human inherited traits
my local electricity for a signal). When the is a very slow process (Miller, 2011). A fun-
availability of the media I’m used to having damental principle of evolution is that cur-
was cut off, I was sent scrambling to replace rent members of a species are born with the
those mediated sources of information and set of traits that better prepared their ances-
interaction with alternative forms to meet tors for survival and reproduction. This pre-
those needs that have developed as a result sumes that environments stay the same. If
of having mass media readily available, each there are radical changes in an environment,
and every day. the species must adapt or it will become
As media becomes more pervasive, it extinct. Humans have adapted to pay atten-
becomes more important to consider how tion to any stimuli that have adaptive signifi-
humans are developing to adapt to this major cance. The suggestion is that, rather than
change in our ways of living. Or are they? developing new structures in the brain to
This is an important question. facilitate media processing, media has been
created by the brain that has already devel-
oped. An ‘old’ brain isn’t necessarily an
outmoded brain (Grabe, 2011). It remains to
SURVIVAL AND REPRODUCTION be seen if the addition of mass media is
a large enough change to affect humans
Natural selection is the cornerstone of evolu- enough to change their survival trajectory
tion, and what drives natural selection are (Geher, 2014).
traits that facilitate either survival or repro-
duction, as traits that favor these things are
more likely to be passed on to the next gen- Nature or Nurture?
eration (Miller, 2011; West-Eberhard, 1979).
It makes sense that any aspect of media Any time one considers aspects of human
experience that improves chances either for growth and development, it is necessary to
survival or for reproduction will be liable, weigh in on the nature/nurture debate and
over time, to shape the evolution of our spe- attempt to identify which influences have a
cies. However, mass media has not been in biological basis and which influences have a
play long enough for this to have been a basis in socialization. In communication
factor in human evolution thus far. So rather studies, the tradition has been to look at
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND MASS MEDIA 401

nurture over nature and, until recently, this to find factors that explain a greater percent-
was a driving force in most media research age of the variability in how humans process
(Sherry, 2004). media. Studies to that time had achieved lim-
More recently, there has been a shift ited success in making such predictions. In
towards consideration of the effects of biol- explaining why communication had been
ogy on communication (Sherry, 2015; Weber, dominated by the nurture aspects of develop-
2015). For example, the field of strategic ment, he pointed out that the various theo-
communications is the study of how com- retical traditions that had informed research in
munication satisfies a specific long-term mass media were ideas like Bandura’s Social
goal. It has explored the specific ways that Learning Theory (Bandura, 2001) or Mead’s
understanding improves as one comprehends Symbolic Interaction (Mead, 1934), theories
the purposes, in an adaptive sense, that were that were more driven by social constructivism
fulfilled by the development of mental strate- than by considerations of inherited traits. By
gies. Studying motivations such as status or 2015, Sherry was writing about a newer model
affiliation sets the stage for a larger under- of communication research, one based on
standing of how human motivations adapt ‘complexity theory’ that had integrated con-
to favor both survival and reproduction siderations like evolution into the prevailing
(Seiffert-Brockmann, 2018). research paradigms. Indeed, a shift in think-
Humans have not yet evolved as the result ing by not only Sherry but also others in the
of mass media (Grabe, 2011). Evolution is field (Falk et  al., 2015; Weber, 2015) meant
a long-term process, and such adaptations that mass media and communication scholars
have not yet had time to emerge. Therefore, recognized that evolutionary psychology was
the current conversation is about how the becoming an important part of understanding
human brain as it exists today responds to media effects.
mass media, and how, moving forward, In justifying a model that blends theoretical
media might influence future evolution. The aspects of both nature and nurture, Malamuth
response to media has been influenced both (1996: 11) said this: ‘The human mind was
by the forces of social constructivism and designed by natural selection operating over
biological and inherited aspects of human many generations. To understand the influ-
development (Bandura, 2001; Sherry, 2015). ences of current environments, it is essential
If the argument can be made that heritable to consider the psychological mechanisms
traits influence the way we process media that are part of that design and are the result
(Toth and Schick, 1993), then an argu- of an interactive blend of nature and nurture’.
ment could be made that humans might be Mass media is part of the current environ-
expected to adapt and evolve in the future to ment that we are seeking to understand, and
an increasingly mediated world. to gain full understanding, both nature and
Sherry (2004) was one of the early commu- nurture must be considered.
nication scholars to engage this issue of social It is simplistic to talk about a model of
constructivism vs biological essentialism. He nature vs nurture that posits that these are
observed that, up until that time, communi- separate influences. In fact, it is the interac-
cation research had been more influenced by tion between genotypes and environments
learning theory and environmental determin- that explains the ways that humans have
ism than it had been by recognition of biologi- evolved to be a good fit for their various envi-
cal aspects of development. He recognized ronments, differentiated as they are (Scarr
that more adaptive traits are likely to be the and McCartney, 1983). ‘The dichotomy of
ones passed on to a new generation. He dis- nature and nurture has always been a bad one,
cussed the need for communication scholars to not only for the oft-cited reasons that both are
look more closely at biology and neuroscience required for development, but because a false
402 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

parallel arises between the two. We propose once (s)he has a choice, that includes a lot of
that development is indeed the result of nature music and will build her or his own environ-
and nurture but that genes drive experience’ ment to support that ability.
(1983: 425). A good example to illustrate how The important point in all of these exam-
this might work can be taken from my own ples is that nature and nurture as influences
family of origin. Both of my parents were on a developing child cannot be separated
career music educators and as a result I grew or evaluated in isolation one from the other.
up in an environment that was heavily influ- Biology shapes environment and environ-
enced by music. At various points, specula- ment shapes developing traits. It is this
tion was expressed whether the fact that my choosing of environments that can have a
siblings and I were musical was the result of direct effect on the eventual choice to repro-
nature/genes, or nurture/environment. Scarr duce or not reproduce.
and McCartney’s theory would say that my Said in a more technical way, phenotype
parents and their genotypes were an influ- (the visible trait) arises from a combination
ence on the environment they created in our of genotype interacting with environment.
household that resulted in a life heavily satu- To the extent that the genotype of the par-
rated by musical influences. Thus, it was not ents has a direct effect on the environment
nature plus nurture as if they were two sepa- of the child, that parent’s genotype is shap-
rate things, but rather nature and the resulting ing outcomes. The adoptive parent’s geno-
environment that was created by my par- type is not passed to the adopted child, but it
ents who had strong genotypes/phenotypes does shape the environment within which the
for musicality. Scarr and McCartney call child grows, which in turn has an effect on
this a ‘passive’ type of environmental effect the child’s survival and subsequent ability to
by parents who are genetically similar to reproduce. The child him or herself responds
offspring. to genetic tendencies that cause the choice of
But what if a child is adopted and grows certain environments over others. Thus natu-
up in an environment that is created by non- ral selection is a process fueled as much by
biological parents? According to Scarr and environment as by biology.
McCartney (1983) there would still be an This principle is important because
‘evocative’ influence whereby the child’s although the effect of the parents on the
own genotype affects the ways that she or survival of the child is more obvious, it
he responds to the parents, regardless of illustrates why it is important to consider the
their biological relationship. In this way the effect mass media has on the environment of a
child’s genetic makeup creates a certain person. This can affect the likelihood that the
type of environment that is consistent with the person will both survive and reproduce. The
genotype of the child. So if, for example, chapter will return to this point at the end,
the adopted child is naturally introverted when a person’s likelihood of developing a
but the adoptive parents are very extroverted, relationship with a media figure and how that
the child’s responses might be initially can directly affect that person’s likelihood of
introverted but become progressively more reproducing is discussed.
extroverted as (s)he adapts to this pattern of The conclusion to the question ‘Is human
interaction. development or growth the result of nature
The third type of influence Scarr and or nurture?’ has to be that both must be con-
McCartney (1983) call active niche-picking sidered to get an accurate picture of human
or niche-building. In this situation, the child behavior, particularly as shaped by mass
adopted to parents who are not musical but media. This is true in all aspects of develop-
who has a genetic proclivity to be musical ment including the development of sex and
will eventually seek out an environment, gender roles.
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND MASS MEDIA 403

Sex and Gender survival of all members of the group. Grabe


(2011) wrote about the surveillance function
In psychology, much of the biology vs sociali- of news, and how our monitoring of news
zation research has dealt with sex and gender, media is a way to remain apprised of poten-
an area where differences in specific biologi- tially survival-related information. Citing
cal aspects vs social aspects of development Shoemaker, she makes the argument that
seem easier to identify (Buss, 2016). Thus, it humans are hard-wired to pay attention to
is no surprise that much of the research tying negative news because of the way journalists
together evolutionary psychology with mass are able to warn the public about potential
media looks at the topic through the lens of dangers. She suggests that the gender differ-
gender or sex (Ellis, 1992; Grabe, 2011; ences similar to those reported in Kamhawi
Kamhawi and Grabe, 2006; Malamuth, 1996; and Grabe (2006) are a function of histori-
Miller, 2011; Salmon, 2012; Singh and Singh, cally early gender roles wherein women pro-
2011; Tifferet and Vilnai-Yavetz, 2014). tected children by avoiding danger while men
What do women find attractive in partners protected children by monitoring danger and
in contrast with what men prefer? The list for being prepared to defend against it.
women includes qualities like strength, youth, An international randomized study of
ability to provide and protect, and status, and Facebook pages and the way users present
one conclusion is that although women gener- themselves found that whereas men were
ally prefer these qualities in a potential mate, more likely to use images that conveyed
such preference does not usually occur at a status and ruggedness, women were more
conscious level. While this does not specifi- likely to choose images that related to family
cally address issues related to media, the appli- values and/or emotional expression. These
cation to how women might process mediated findings used theory-driven hypotheses from
figures compared to how men process them evolutionary psychology to explore aspects
can be inferred (Ellis, 1992). of evolutionary theory as it relates to behav-
More specific to media is Kamhawi and ior on social networking sites (Tifferet and
Grabe’s (2006) discussion of gender dif- Vilnai-Yavetz, 2014). Another social media
ferences in the reception of negative news. study, this time about Instagram, looked at
Citing research showing that women have a the variables that contributed to men’s desire
stronger avoidance of negative stimuli than do to date women whose varying photo types
men (e.g. Zald, 2003), they showed that when were presented. Neutral photos (defined as
news is given a more positive spin, women those not showing people) were more appeal-
are more likely to watch and remember. ing to men who felt lonely and had higher
Earlier studies had shown that women were need for belonging and popularity, whereas
less likely than men to watch and remember group selfies were more appealing to men
news stories about things like war and disas- who had lower intrasexual competition for
ters. This study suggested that women have mates, need to belong, and need to be popu-
both a socially learned and biological pre- lar. This study used a variation on Maslow’s
disposition to avoid negatively framed news hierarchy of needs that was modified based
(Kamhawi and Grabe, 2006). on principles of evolutionary psychology. It
Shoemaker (1996) described the function considered affiliation, status, and mate acqui-
of surveillance in early nomadic communi- sition as higher-order needs. One of the find-
ties. All members of the society had to survey ings is that factors that correlate with desire
the environment for danger, and some individ- for long-term relationships are different from
uals might have had this as their specialized short-term relationships. Overall, different
function in the group. Perceiving potential photo types were related to differing profiles
threats to the group greatly increased the of intrasexual competition and the various
404 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

social needs reported. Perceived narcissism erotic literature with romance literature,
was another key variable, with those post- romance novels contain pornography written
ing selfies and groupies perceived as more to appeal to women, while explicitly erotic
narcissistic than those posting neutral photos images and literature appeal more to men.
(Jin et al., 2019). Both of these social media These each represent billion-dollar indus-
studies suggest that sexual selection adapta- tries and the differences between them can be
tions have a complex effect on the ways that traced to the fact that sexual selection pres-
users consume social media images. sures faced by males and females during the
Some research has suggested that adoles- course of evolutionary history have not been
cent girls have a pre-occupation with male identical (Salmon, 2012). An ethnography
celebrities. Girls during this stage of devel- of American romance readers revealed that
opment are more likely to form romantic women are very much aware of what they
attachments to celebrities than are boys perceived to be a male proclivity for imper-
during these years (Stever, 1994). Erikson sonal sex compared to the stories women
(1959) suggested that while the formation of in this study preferred, stories that involved
identity in adolescence is ideally before the interpersonal connection (Radway, 1984).
development of intimacy which he associ- A second study of female romance read-
ated with young adulthood, often adolescent ers found that the basic themes of romance
girls put intimacy before identity and then novels were finding, attracting, and keeping
assume the identity of their romantic partner a desirable mate, certainly themes consist-
(e.g. she becomes ‘the doctor’s wife’ or ‘the ent with evolutionary psychology’s views
minister’s wife’). Emphasis on their histori- of the female mating agenda (Buss, 2016;
cally evolved role as nurturers over the male Hazen, 1983). Other studies have found sex
role of provider and protector could explain differences that involve variations in sexual
why this happens. Engle and Kasser (2005) fantasies that include visual images, context,
specifically queried a population of jun- personalization, partner variety and response,
ior high school-aged girls and linked such and other variables. It is argued that if there
interest to categories of secure and insecure are real differences between the sexes, then
attachment, an ethological theory driven by those differences should be even stronger in
principles of evolutionary psychology. More fantasies than in actual behaviors, as fanta-
will be said about attachment theory later but, sies don’t have the same practical constraints
for now, the observation was that girls who as do behaviors. In general, women have
had positive attachments to same-age boys more contextualized and personalized fan-
that they knew in real life were more likely to tasies and their fantasies are more likely to
idolize male celebrities. Overall, adolescent involve emotional rather than purely physical
girls are more romantically fixated than are responses. They are more likely to include
boys at this age, and differing evolved sex personalized and specific partners (Ellis and
roles is a possible explanation for this. Symons, 1990).
Malamuth (1996) looked at gender dif- These are all examples of how theory in
ferences in the consumption of sexually evolutionary psychology is driving research
explicit media. He considered research that on mass media and gender differences.
attempts to explain differing preferences of Malamuth (1996) pointed out that, in gen-
men and women in their consumption of eral, sex differences occur in those domains
this kind of material. Vast bodies of work for which men faced different problems
that advocate for both biological and social from the ones women faced. Adaptation for
explanations exist, not surprising due to the each sex then proceeded on a different path
controversial nature of this particular subject to solve these different problems. Because
(see also Salmon, 2018). When contrasting the consequences of reproductive behavior
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND MASS MEDIA 405

for each gender are not the same, men and connection one feels with a personality, usually
women developed different adaptations to mediated, whom one knows in some detail
their specific reproductive challenges. One of but without reciprocation. Attachments and
the largest differences is the parental involve- other social relationships meet a fundamen-
ment required for successful reproduction. tal need that is essential for optimal health
Women must make a much larger minimum and psychological functioning (Baumeister
investment than do men. This difference is and Leary, 1995). Stever (2017a) found that
the source of many of the differences in mat- parasocial attachment to mediated person-
ing strategies between the sexes. alities is the natural by-product of persis-
tent media consumption of the same face,
voice, and personality of an attractive per-
sona. Attachment theory would predict that
OTHER RESEARCH LINKING the face and voice are primary mechanisms
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY of attachment. A human infant will gaze
WITH MASS MEDIA longer at a human face than any other object
presented to it (Umilta et  al., 1996). He or
A content analysis of tabloid media using an she will look longer at a correct human face
a priori list of evolutionary-related topics than an incorrect one (Easterbrook et  al.,
showed that sampled tabloid stories were 1999), and will prefer the face and voice of
most often related to survival fitness-relevant the mother to other faces and voices (Blehar
topics, those related to reproductive success et al., 1977; DeCasper and Fifer, 1980; Muir
(De Backer and Fisher, 2012). Long-term et  al., 1994). Adults prefer attractive and
relationships, wealth status, health status, and symmetrical faces, and one theory is that
parental care were four of the most frequently such attractiveness is an indicator of good
coded topics in the sample of tabloid media health and a strong genetic background (Fink
that was analyzed. The interest in celebrity- and Penton-Voak, 2002). Reflecting back to
related gossip was linked to the prevalence of the argument that humans respond to media
parasocial relationships, discussed below. as if it were real (Grabe, 2011; Reeves and
Research addressing themes in popular Nass, 1996), humans respond to attractive
music linked to evolutionary themes pro- potential mates whether they are 10 feet away
duced similar findings, with country-western or on a television screen. The familiar other
songs frequently containing phrases relating is a potential mate. In some cases this is not
to sexuality and reproduction with an average adaptive in that sometimes the person who
of 10 such references per song (Hobbs and is fooled into thinking that attractive other is
Gallup Jr., 2011). Salmon (2018) observed attainable persists in her or his attraction and
that these themes are found in music in gen- refuses to give it up. This is discussed in more
eral, although they are found more often in the detail below.
most popular songs (e.g. those making Top Media use compensates for diminished
10 lists). Hip-hop and rap songs tend to focus social interaction in situations wherein people
on male interest in short-term mating, with experience isolation (Jonason et  al., 2008).
much bragging about the number of partners Because humans have evolved to be social
they’ve had. In this same genre, female art- beings, isolation creates psychological dis-
ists sing about wanting men with status and comfort. Various kinds of ‘social snacking’
resources. The country-western songs had were investigated, operationalized for this
more themes around parenting as well as the study as ‘talking to oneself’ and ‘watching
start-up or break-up of relationships. TV’. Whereas both genders used watching
Parasocial engagement (Tukachinsky TV to mitigate social isolation, women indi-
and Stever, 2019) is defined as the social cated greater amounts of self-talk compared
406 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

to men. Additionally, the researchers point one to feel connections with familiar others.
out that parasocial interaction is another way Extending this to feelings that viewers develop
people cope with loneliness (Cohen, 2001; for media personae, just as thoughts of the
Giles, 2002). real-life romantic partner can make one
Bandura (2001), in his work on role mod- feel safe and secure even when that partner
els and vicarious social learning, maintained is not physically present, mediated perso-
that media role models can be as powerful as nae are able to elicit feelings of safety and
proximal role models in shaping the behav- security in the viewers who have those para-
ior of those who admire and emulate them. social attachments (Stever, 2013, 2017a).
Already discussed has been the role that Psychological comfort and a sense of safety
learning adaptive behaviors from previous potentially could play a role in the ability of
generations is a fundamental aspect of how humans to adapt to social environments but
media might affect adaptation. Taking this more research is needed to solidify this link.
together with parasocial theory, it is clear that Parasocial attachment as a part of classical
both identification and attachment are fac- attachment theory is a new concept that needs
tors influencing the role that media plays in further exploration. A recent study exploring
the relationships people form with mediated the ways that viewers view media celebri-
others, relationships that can have adaptive ties as parental attachment figures, looking
consequences. to such a celebrity to meet a specific attach-
ment need, is a good example of this kind of
research (David et al., 2019).
Evolutionary Theory and
Attachment
Resulting Theories
Attachment theory, at its foundation, assumes
that attachment behavior has evolved to be an Out of work in media studies have come a
adaptive behavioral pattern designed or number of theories including social presence,
selected to assure survival of the infant. A media richness, and media naturalness (Kock,
repertoire of attachment behaviors and a cor- 2012). Presence is defined as ‘a psychological
responding set of caregiving behaviors on the state in which the virtuality of experience is
part of the adult caregiver complement each unnoticed’ (Lee, 2004: 494). Thus, interac-
other and work together towards the success- tion is carried on with the mediated nature of
ful development of the infant. Although that behavior being in the background and not
sexual selection and successful mating are noticed on a conscious level. If you ask the
important, all of that is for nothing if the participant if this behavior is mediated, they
infant does not survive (Keller, 2013). are able to state that it is, but otherwise they
Hazan and Shaver (1994) and others have behave as if the interaction is as real as face-
posited that the attachment system that favors to-face interaction. Lee concludes that ‘pres-
the survival of the infant is also a part of the ence research will be equally benefitted by
adult romantic relationship. Partners look seriously considering the implication of evo-
to one another for mutual support and care­ lutionary psychology for the explanation of
giving in a paradigm that defines attach- how people use and interact with media and
ment as the seeking of feelings of security simulation technologies’ (Lee, 2004: 501).
and safety. Consistent with the idea that a Social presence and media richness theo-
key aspect of evolutionary psychology is ries (Kock, 2004, 2012) each posit that face-
the heritable quality of emotional responses, to-face communication was most affected
attachment theory says that inborn behaviors by evolution. Because humans have evolved
elicit feelings and emotions that predispose to be good at face-to-face communication,
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND MASS MEDIA 407

media that best approximates face-to-face can talk to and hear one another. Specialized
communication is the most engaging. Daft areas of the brain have evolved that allow
and Lengel (1986) argued that richness was facial recognition, the recognition of emo-
dependent, in part, on the medium’s ability tional cues, and the processing of speech,
to convey nonverbal information as a part of and thus it makes sense that communicating
the communication. Written communication in this way will allow easy processing by all
has no nonverbal cues whereas traditional parties involved.
face-to-face communications have rich cues. When evaluating computer-mediated com-
Between those two extremes are a continuum munication, media richness theory can favor
of various forms of mediated communication a mode that features synchronicity and feed-
that might have more or fewer nonverbal cues back immediacy whereas media naturalness
available as a characteristic of that form. For theory favors the form that requires less cog-
example, communicating via FaceTime on a nitive effort, most often deemed to be face-
cell phone includes many nonverbal cues but to-face. In many examples, these two theories
not as many as being in the presence of the seem to come to similar conclusions, but
person, but quite a few more than just writ- the important consideration is the different
ing an e-mail. An earlier idea, social presence, emphases in each (Kock, 2004, 2012).
conveyed a similar distinction between face-
to-face vs written communications (Short
et al., 1976). Other aspects of social presence The Pleistocene Brain
or media richness include the availability of
immediate feedback from the communica- It is central to the arguments of this chapter
tion partner, as well as the ability to convey that humans expend a considerable amount of
aspects of one’s personality (Kock, 2004). time, effort, and resources in the production
As further research on these ideas was con- and consumption of mass media. Additionally,
ducted, there was evidence both for and the ability of such mediated forms to elicit
against the media richness theory, suggesting deep emotion from the consumer is something
the need for an alternative explanation, and that social scientists seek to explain using
resulting in the development of media natu- established epistemologies (Hennighausen
ralness theory (Kock, 2012). and Schwab, 2015). What is the origin of
Media naturalness theory contends that evolved psychological mechanisms and what
reduction of cognitive effort is the more purpose do they serve? These processes
important factor in explaining the greater evolved in response to adaptive problems
effectiveness of media forms that best that occurred in the Pleistocene (or Ice Age,
approximate face-to-face communication. ending 11,700 years ago) human past
Because humans evolved to communicate in (Barkow et  al., 1992). They evolved very
person and in a manner that is most often syn- slowly over many millennia in contrast to the
chronous, to the extent that these aspects are rapid social change that one can observe
employed, communication takes less cogni- beginning from that time to the present.
tive effort. Kock (2012) identifies five aspects Predominantly, this is an era also referred to
of communication that are involved in media as hunter/gatherer society. Experts appear to
naturalness. The first is the individuals’ abil- agree that the Pleistocene brain or ‘old brain’
ity to see and hear each other; the second is still the one that guides human behavior
is that communication be synchronous; the (Grabe, 2011). Have evolved psychological
third is the inclusion of facial expressions mechanisms that evolved during the
as a part of the message. Body language is Pleistocene to solve adaptive problems
the fourth element. Speaking directly to one served to determine how humans use media?
another is the fifth element, such that partners This is another important question.
408 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

GRABE VS REEVES AND NASS: A information into one’s day-to-day experience.


DEBATE ABOUT THE ‘OLD’ BRAIN: Is the processing of media as ‘real’ always
ADAPTED OR OUTMODED? adaptive? This question is addressed later in
this chapter.
Related to all of this is Reeves and Nass’ An important counterpoint to the research
on The Media Equation is work done by
(1996) theory, referred to as The Media
Ramos and colleagues (Ramos et  al., 2013).
Equation. Simply stated, their work suggests
Specifically, they found that for Hispanic
that humans interact socially with media in the
viewers who were the participants in their
same ways that they interact in face-to-face
study, more empathy for objects of violence in
social situations: ‘… our research shows that
a film was reported when they knew the vio-
media are perceived as real people and places,
lence was real compared to when it was part
and that human responses to media are deter-
of fiction. Previous exposure to violence did
mined by the rules that apply to social rela-
not affect the degree of empathy shown with
tionships and navigating the world’ (1996:
respect to these two types of violence, media
10). When we see people or events depicted
depicting real victims vs media depicting fic-
through media, reactions are governed by the
tional victims. A victim empathy scale was
rules of social interaction. For example, par-
used to measure the dependent variable in this
ticipants in research treated a computer as an study, a self-report measure. Actual physiolog-
active social partner, showing that computer ical responses were not recorded in this case.
courtesy and good manners. During experi-
ments, people used polite behavior when
interacting with a machine when that machine
asked for evaluations. We will tell the machine Entertainment as Central to
we are using that it is helpful, accurate, and Mass Media
friendly, but when asked to evaluate that same When considering the role of entertainment
computer from a different computer, responses as a result of evolution, two approaches are
were far less polite, suggesting that partici- recognized. The first is that entertainment is
pants were responding to the computer in a a by-product of evolution. The second is that
way similar to any other social partner. entertainment is itself an evolutionary adap-
Additionally, interpersonal distance as depicted tation to conditions in the environment
in pictures elicits similar responses to those (Hennighausen and Schwab, 2015). Other
figures as does interaction with real people. academics have also written about a connec-
When people in pictures seem close, our reac- tion between evolutionary psychology and
tions are more intense, and we pay more atten- entertainment (Vorderer et  al., 2006) and
tion and remember those people better (Reeves have considered whether humans develop an
and Nass, 1996). interest in being entertained as an adaptation,
By responding to media in the same way or as a by-product of adaptations. One view
we react to face-to-face others, does this that it is a by-product of adaptation posits
reflect that our Pleistocene brain has failed to that as humans became more adept at sur-
adapt to media and is reacting in a way that vival and no longer needed to expend all of
is outmoded in an evolutionary sense? Grabe their time and energy on it, this created lei-
(2011), in interpreting The Media Equation, sure time, and entertainment originated from
suggests that rather than look at this phenom- the desire to fill such leisure time with pleas-
enon as an outmoded ‘old’ brain that has not urable activity. The Media Equation approach
kept up with change, we rather consider that (Reeves and Nass, 1996) also assumes that
processing media as if it were the same as ‘real human responses to media are a by-product
life’ is an adaptive way to integrate mediated of other adaptations. We respond to media as
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND MASS MEDIA 409

if it were real because we have evolved social in life. In addition, at first look, entertain-
responses to real people, and media resem- ment and mediated experience do not appear
bles real people enough that we respond to it to have any real function in human life.
in a similar fashion. However, upon closer analysis, two key
However, if entertainment is an adaptation functions of human existence, sensory percep-
in itself, then humans might have developed tion and communication, seem to be related
skills in being interesting and engaging in in a positive way to mediated experience. In
order to attract potential mates. In this case, an evolutionary sense, problem solving has an
entertainment is part of the primary task of adaptive function and consuming media nar-
reproduction and sexual selection. Miller ratives, whether in books, films, and televi-
(2011) developed the idea that the human sion, or even on the Internet, offers a form of
mind, with its creativity and complexity, serves cognitive rehearsal for real-life events.
to attract mates in and of itself and as such is Nielsen (2012) identified the key element
an evolutionary adaptation. Darwin (1871) in this process as ‘imitation’. It is the pre-
called this a ‘hidden courtship function’. Said dominant way that culture is transmitted from
more simply, having a creative and interesting one individual to another and from parent to
mind is ‘sexy’ to the prospective mate (see offspring. However, a key aspect of human
Hennighausen and Schwab, 2015 for a more development was that successive generations
extensive discussion of these approaches). improved the tools they were given by par-
Memory, anticipation, decision-making, and ents. Such innovation explains, for example,
pleasure are all key aspects of sexual choice the rapid progression of modes of air travel
(Miller, 2011). These are all factors that easily once a basic plane had been developed. Each
can play into media choices as well and sexual generation didn’t have to start at the begin-
attraction is an important factor when choos- ning of the ‘can humans fly’ question but
ing one’s favorite celebrity (Stever, 1991). It rather could build on the accomplishments of
is possible that both factors, entertainment and their immediate predecessors. A prolonged
media as a by-product of adaptation, and as an stage of growth and development for humans
actual adaptation, might be at work, and that gives an opportunity for advanced cogni-
it does not have to be one or the other (Ohler tive skills to develop. Thus ‘childhood’ is a
and Nieding, 2006). Entertainment can be a unique feature of humans, and affords the
by-product of adaptations that led to increased time necessary to learn and develop. Play is
leisure time, but also could be a mechanism identified as a primary mechanism in imita-
for enhanced sexual selection. tion and learning during childhood.
Thus, a central part of development for
children is play (Erikson, 1974) whereby
children often rehearse tasks that they will
Emotion, Play, and Fantasy
be called upon to perform as adults. Media
This discussion of human evolution and how entertainment is an extension of the concept
media fits into the picture begins with the of ‘play’ and a higher-level form of cognitive
observation that a huge aspect of media effects rehearsal for tasks and situations that might
is the way media stimulates human emotions. come into play throughout life in various
It has been argued that our emotional responses relationships, so that we can practice inter-
to media are as intense as those same reactions personal, emotional, or social situations with-
to real events, as already noted (also Schwender out incurring any unnecessary risk to real
and Schwab, 2010). Some theorists have sug- relationships. This can be argued to have a
gested that the role of media in human life is possible real and positive influence on repro-
largely dysfunctional in that media distracts ductive outcomes for those who learn about
us from dealing with real day-to-day things social situations in this way.
410 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

It is recognized that play is a fundamental humans fall in love. She has concluded that
aspect of human behavior, and that play is the brain evolved to execute three brain func-
often the rehearsal of behaviors that are adap- tions related to love. These are lust, romantic
tive for either survival or reproduction. In love, and attachment. Attachment has already
play we ‘practice’ doing things that will later been discussed here and sexual attraction has
ensure that we can be successful in life. What been discussed as well. Romantic love has
is play? It is the creation of a fantasy envi- been reported across time and across cultures
ronment within which such practice can take in terms that are close to universal. Various
place. So, a little girl in her pretend kitchen terms are used to refer to the feelings associ-
‘cooks’ dinner for her family and serves it to ated with romantic love including infatua-
them on small facsimiles of dishes. From such tion, obsession, and passion. Fisher’s studies
play, she later is able to learn real cooking showed that the experiences and feelings
skills. A little boy pretends to care for his doll, associated with this state are similar across
and such play is practice for future parenting. cultures, ages, and genders.
Theorists have suggested that mass media Fisher recounts, ‘Lovers dote on the posi-
is experienced as ‘play’ by its recipients. tive qualities of their sweethearts, flagrantly
Stephenson (1988: 49) recounts that the daily disregarding reality’ (Fisher, 2004: 8). What
mix of media consumed by viewers ‘is repeti- she refers to here is the tendency to idealize
tious, like a child’s game played over and over the beloved and ignore negative qualities or
with variations on a familiar theme’. It is not turn those negative qualities into endearing
a big leap to see the consumption of fiction traits. It is not until the relationship comes
in mass media as a springboard for fantasy, up against reality that the rose-tinted glasses
which is related to play. A viewer watches a come off and the partner is seen as she or he
favorite television show and when the show really is. Over the course of time, idealization
is over, fantasizes about being in social situ- is tempered. This is potentially the founda-
ations with a favorite protagonist, scripting tion for why when viewers form ‘crushes’
conversations and practicing how to behave on media figures, those feelings persist and
in those social situations. This social behav- the object is idealized. Because there is never
ior is practice for future real encounters in the any dose of reality to bring those ideals into
same way that the child cooking in a pretend a more realistic perspective, the viewing of
kitchen is learning behaviors that will apply the media object as near to perfection is a
to real preparing of meals. The fantasy inter- common experience. Stever (1991) found
action with a favorite media persona is a type that when asked how big a fan someone was
of parasocial relationship. of their favorite celebrity, the best predictor
Thus, play involves fantasy and when one for that Likert scale was romantic attrac-
consumes media, one is transported into a tion. Clearly, these feelings of infatuation are
parasocial fantasy world where we interact heavily tied to why viewers become roman-
with the inhabitants as if they were real. We tically attracted over time to their favorite
assess possible fantasy mates in these worlds media personalities.
in the same way we might assess real poten-
tial mates in a face-to-face environment.

IMPLICATIONS FOR THE IMPACT OF


The Media Personae as an MEDIA ON INDIVIDUALS
Idealized Romantic Figure
The argument is that humans have not
Fisher (2004) has done extensive research on evolved beyond the hunter/gatherer society
the nature of romantic love and how and why with respect to social behavior. In order to
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND MASS MEDIA 411

understand the importance of the premise of in-person candidates for mating. Now women
The Media Equation, that people respond to can evaluate distant potential partners and, if
media as if it were real, and to understand the the potential mate is found worthy, she could
impact that mass media is having on social attempt to pursue him.
life, it is important to consider the ways in The problem comes when it occurs to this
which courtships have traditionally happened woman that she does not know how to meet
throughout recorded human history. It has the potential mate, and in some cases, she
been argued that the human brain has acted realizes that she is one of thousands who are
on the basis of one model of human interac- attracted to this same person. She is still in
tion, and that was the model established by the phase of romantic attraction where she
the Pleistocene hunter/gatherer society, a has idealized this possible mate, and so her
society that was conducted face-to-face. pursuit of him becomes a desire to meet him
In the Pleistocene era, if a woman saw a face-to-face in order to realize the fantasy in
man who was a good potential mate, she had real time and in real life. It is not until she
only to behave in ways that would be attrac- reaches this point in the ‘courtship’ that
tive to him, and she had every expectation of the problems with mediated reality become
being able to bear his offspring. The ways apparent.
potential mates were evaluated has already In my interviews with women who have
been discussed, but status, age, and resources been through this process with an attractive
were all factors in this evaluation. Because a distant media figure, there is one of three out-
woman’s reproductive investment was in one comes: 1) She has a reality check, gives up
man at a time, it was essential to her repro- her pursuit of this idealized mate, and seeks
ductive success that she attract the best candi- instead to find someone in proximity; 2) She
date with whom to mate. Throughout history refuses to give up her pursuit of the ideal-
and up until very recently, the only candi- ized other and proceeds to pursue him by
dates for potential mates were men that the any means available. This choice often ends
woman met in person and knew face-to-face. in despair as the pursuit is not successful; or
If she could get close enough to evaluate him 3) If she is vulnerable to mental illness, she
for things like strength, health, and attractive- enters a delusional state (referred to as ero-
ness, she would be able to attract him and tomanic schizophrenia), where she evaluates
bear his children. It was not even essential the fantasy relationship as ‘real’ and behaves
that she be his only partner. as if it is. In most cases of erotomania, the
Fast forward to the 20th century, where mentally ill person lives in isolation in her
film, television, and eventually the Internet fantasy world, firm in her belief that she has
made the images and information available been separated from her beloved and that
about potential candidates for mates that the if they could be reunited, all would be well
woman had not met in person, indeed might (Kennedy et  al., 2002). In a few cases, the
likely never meet in person. The ‘script’ for erotomanic becomes a stalker and pursues
mating was still written in the behavioral the person she believes is her partner via any
social system of the woman’s brain. In the means available. Both history and the news
earliest stages of attraction, initiation, and have reported various stories about this type
experimentation (Tukachinsky and Stever, of woman. David Letterman’s stalker (Bruni,
2018), it is easy for a woman to fantasize 1998) went so far as to enter his home, drive
about a potential mate, trying out various his car, and tell others that they were mar-
mating scenarios in her imagination, in order ried. (Eventually she took her own life, a
to evaluate the worthiness of a potential mate. frequent outcome for persons in this situa-
Throughout history, this was something tion.) Other celebrities have had to deal with
that women had been doing with respect to similar intrusions into their private lives by
412 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

people who believed that the celebrity was with a famous female celebrity. To succeed,
his or her romantic partner. Some research a woman only has to be one of potentially
has suggested that erotomania actually ‘may many possible partners. For a man to suc-
be viewed as a pathological variant of a spe- ceed, he has to become the ONLY partner
cific sexual strategy that evolved under selec- for that female celebrity, at least for a time.
tion pressures of the human environment Thus, traditionally the throngs of adoring
of evolutionary adaptedness’ (Brüne and fans who want to partner with the famous
Schröder, 2003: 83). This suggests that this celebrity have been more likely to be women.
kind of obsession with a media figure pos- However, men are vulnerable to this situation
sibly reflects sexual selection pressures gone as well.
wrong.
One of the main arguments of The Media
Equation and discussions of this theory has An Illustration from The Big Bang
been that if people perceive what they see Theory
in media as real, and react to it as if it were
real, what harm is done? Grabe (2011: 366) The idea that a fan would replace the oppor-
asks, ‘what existing or anticipated environ- tunity for a real relationship in the face-to-
mental need might compel human adapta- face world with a fantasy attachment to a
tion for the ability to separate media from media character is illustrated well in an epi-
physical reality….Why should our brains sode of the television sitcom The Big Bang
discriminate between media and physical Theory. Howard Wolowitz has been intro-
worlds if they are converging’? However, duced to Bernadette and Bernadette wants an
in the example above, a viewer’s belief intimate relationship with him. Howard hesi-
that the attractive potential mate she ‘meets’ tates to commit to the real relationship. Why?
through media is obtainable and that it As he explains to his friends (in season 3,
could or should be real has the potential to episode 9), ‘She wants a commitment and
wreak havoc on that viewer’s life for many I’m not sure she’s my type. Bernadette is
years. really nice. I just thought that when I was
In my years of participation in fan com- ready to settle down in a relationship, it
munities, I have met and/or seen numerous would be with someone different. More like
fans who were in deep despair because they Megan Fox from Transformers or Katee
realized they were in love with someone that Sackhoff from Battlestar Galactica’. When
they could never have or even meet. Although Penny replies, ‘You’re going to throw away a
this represents a small percentage of fandom, great girl like Bernadette because you’re
it is a real situation that I have encountered holding out for some ridiculous fantasy?’,
numerous times in the 30-plus years I have Howard’s answer is, ‘Just because you set-
been involved in participant observation in tled doesn’t mean I have to. I want what’s
media fandoms. inside too but why is it wrong to want those
So far, I have discussed this scenario with insides wrapped up in the delicious caramel
respect to women pursuing men and, indeed, that is Halle Berry?’ Sheldon’s observation at
most of the real-world cases I have observed this point is, ‘Biologically speaking, Howard
have been women in this situation (the fan- is perfectly justified in seeking the optimum
doms I have studied often were dominated mate for the propagation of his genetic line’.
by women). Looking at what we know about Later in the episode, fantasy Katee Sackhoff
gender differences in mate selection, it is not asks Howard, ‘Why are you playing make
hard to understand why a woman looking to believe with me when you could be out with
partner with a famous male celebrity would a real woman tonight?’ When Howard says,
be more common than a man seeking to mate ‘She’s not you’, Sackoff replies, ‘I’m not
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND MASS MEDIA 413

me…the point is you’ve got a real girl in CONCLUSION


your life and you’re ignoring her to spend
your time with a mental image…’. To summarize the main points from this chapter,
In 30 years of fan research, I have met first, mass media has not been a factor in
many dozens of fans who are holding out human evolution long enough to have shaped
for the ideal represented by their favorite inherited characteristics in the species.
celebrity and who would not consider some- Research supports the idea that the hunter/
one lesser than that ideal. In many cases, it gatherer Pleistocene-era brain is still the brain
meant that the fan did not date at all. From that regulates human social life (Barkow et al.,
a natural-selection perspective, people who 1992). Next, evolutionary psychologists have
idealize and stay single/celibate as a result spent a good deal of time investigating the
do not have their genes passed on to progeny. effects of sex and gender on media selection
This is particularly true for women who are and processing. Different reproductive agen-
less likely to engage in frivolous reproductive das and roles in the social group affect the
activity if they are holding out for a perfect
ways that men and women process and react
mate. Men can both hold out and also have
to media messages (Buss, 2016). The work of
casual sex because of their lesser investment
Sherry (2004, 2015) has suggested that
in potential offspring. The Howard Wolowitz
research into mass media has integrated both
character is a good example of this as well.
biological and sociocultural influences on
However, whether the celebrity ideal was a
mass media selection and consumption into
real person or a fictional character, the fan who
various theoretical models. These include
is unwilling to settle for less than the ideal is
social presence, media richness, and media
far less likely to reproduce than the one who is
naturalness theories (Kock, 2012). The Media
more realistic.
However, if media ‘fools’ the brain into Equation (Reeves and Nass, 1996) suggested
thinking that a real relationship is being that humans respond to media as if it were
experienced (real in the sense of repro- real, showing little difference in the way media
ductive potential), this is exactly the kind messages are processed compared to in-­person
of thing that Restak (1991) has described. messages. Face-to-face communication
There is a part of the brain that processes appears to require less cognitive effort and
what is seen as ‘real’, even if the perception thus mediated forms of communication that
is mediated. most closely resemble face-to-face communi-
Cultural anthropology teaches us about cation require less cognitive effort and are
status hierarchies within cultures and the desire more efficient and effective. Discussed were
of individuals to mate with higher-status the ways that entertainment, fantasy, and play
individuals than are currently available in
­ are part of evolutionary theoretical models,
one’s pool of potential mates (Wright, 1994). playing the function of rehearsal of future life
It makes sense that the attractive celebrities tasks, both social and occupational.
found in television, films, and other avenues The final major point is that the inability
of popular culture would be particularly to process mediated social situations differ-
attractive to the viewer who sees an individual ently from face-to-face social situations may
who is likely to be more attractive, wealthier, occasionally have maladaptive or undesirable
and of higher status than potential mates who outcomes, particularly if mating and reproduc-
are in closer proximity. The virtual proxim- tion are the desired outcomes. However, social
ity of such individuals tricks the viewer into situations available through media have the
thinking that high-status celebrity mates are potential to mitigate loneliness and support the
attainable. As Restak (1991) pointed out, The individual who is relatively isolated. The litera-
Brain Has a Mind of Its Own. ture on parasocial relationships and parasocial
414 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

attachments (Stever, 2017b), including the Bruni, F. (November 22, 1998). ‘Behind the
literature on celebrity worship (McCutcheon Jokes, a Life Of Pain and Delusion; For Letter-
et  al., 2002), suggests that there are both man Stalker, Mental Illness Was Family Curse
negative and positive potential outcomes for and Scarring Legacy’. The New York Times.
meeting one’s social needs through mediated Retrieved May 6, 2013. https://www.
nytimes.com/1998/11/22/nyregion/behind-
parasocial relationships.
jokes-life-pain-delusion-for-letterman-
Research investigating the effects of mass stalker-mental-illness-was-family.html
media from an evolutionary psychology per- Buss, D. M. (2016). The evolution of desire.
spective is relatively new and a great deal New York: Springer International Publishing.
more work is needed to investigate, in par- Cohen, J. (2001). Defining identification: A
ticular, the long-term effects of increasing theoretical look at the identification of audi-
levels of media consumption. This includes ences with media characters. Mass Commu-
the growing rate at which people are meeting nication & Society, 4(3), 245–264.
social needs through social media, as well as Daft, R. L., & Lengel, R. H. (1986). Organizational
texting, tweets, and various other messaging information requirements, media richness and
formats. The increasing presence of media structural design. Management Science,
32(5), 554–571.
devices in day-to-day lives, devices like cell
Darwin, C. (1871). The descent of man.
phones, iPads, laptops, personal computers, London: Gibson Square.
and other devices that are used on a daily basis David, K., Myers, M. E., Perry, S. D., Gouse, V., &
in more and more situations, also needs to be Stein, C. B. (2019). Examination of insecure
explored to see how the human brain as it has attachment and the potential for parasocial
evolved is suited to and fits with persistent use parental attachment (PPA) to a favorite celeb-
of these devices. Comparisons between heavy rity through attachment theory. North Ameri-
media users and those who refrain from using can Journal of Psychology, 21(2), 387–406.
media would be instructive in comparing brain De Backer, C. J., & Fisher, M. L. (2012). Tabloids
development that results from use or nonuse. as windows into our interpersonal relation-
ships: A content analysis of mass media
gossip from an evolutionary perspective. Jour-
nal of Social, Evolutionary, and Cultural Psy-
chology, 6(3), 404–424.
REFERENCES DeCasper, A., & Fifer, W. (1980). Of human
bonding: Newborns prefer their mothers’
Bandura, A. (2001). Social cognitive theory of voices. Science, 208, 1174–1176.
mass communication. Media Psychology, 3, Deuze, M. (2011). Media life. Media, Culture &
265–299. Society, 33(1), 137–148.
Barkow, J. H., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (Eds.). Easterbrook, M.A., Kisilvesky, B.S., Muir, D.W., &
(1992). The adapted mind: Evolutionary psy- LaPlante, D.P. (1999). Newborns discriminate
chology and the generation of culture. New schematic faces from scrambled faces,
York: Oxford University Press. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology,
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need 55(3), 231–241.
to belong: Desire for interpersonal attach- Ellis, B. J., & Symons, D. (1990). Sex differences
ments as a fundamental human motivation. in sexual fantasy: An evolutionary psycho-
Psychological Bulletin, 117(3), 497. logical approach. Journal of Sex Research,
Blehar, M., Lieberman, A., & Ainsworth, M. 27(4), 527–555.
(1977). Early face-to-face interaction and its Ellis, B. J. (1992). The evolution of sexual
relation to later infant-mother attachment. attraction: Evaluative mechanisms in women.
Child Development, 48(1), 182–194. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby, (Eds.),
Brüne, M., & Schröder, S. G. (2003). Erotomania The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology
variants in dementia. Journal of Geriatric and the generation of culture, (267–288).
Psychiatry and Neurology, 16(4), 232–234. New York: Oxford University Press.
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND MASS MEDIA 415

Engle, Y., & Kasser, T. (2005). Why do adolescent Keller, H. (2013). Attachment and culture. Journal
girls idolize male celebrities? Journal of Ado- of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 44(2), 175–194.
lescent Research, 20(2), 263–283. Kennedy, N., McDonough, M., Kelly, B., &
Erikson, E. (1959). Identity and the life-cycle. Berrios, G. E. (2002). Erotomania revisited:
New York: W.W. Norton & Co. Clinical course and treatment. Comprehensive
Erikson, E. (1974). Play and actuality. In R. J. Psychiatry, 43(1), 1–6.
Lifton & E. Olson (Eds.), Explorations in psy- Kock, N. (2004). The psychobiological model:
chohistory. The Wellfleet Papers (109–135). Towards a new theory of computer-mediated
New York: Simon and Schuster. communication based on Darwinian evolution.
Falk, E. B., Cascio, C. N., & Coronel, J. C. (2015). Organization Science, 15(3), 327–348.
Neural prediction of communication-relevant Kock, N. (2012). Media naturalness theory:
outcomes. Communication Methods and Human evolution and behaviour towards elec-
Measures, 9(1–2), 30–54. tronic communication technologies. In S. C.
Fink, B., & Penton-Voak, I. (2002). Evolutionary Roberts (Ed.), Applied Evolutionary Psychology,
psychology of facial attractiveness. Current (381–398). New York: Oxford University Press.
Directions in Psychological Science, 11(5), Lee, K. M. (2004). Why presence occurs: Evolu-
154–158. tionary psychology, media equation, and
Fisher, H. (2004). Why we love: The nature and presence. Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual
chemistry of romantic love. New York: Environments, 13(4), 494–505.
Macmillan. Malamuth, N. M. (1996). Sexually explicit media,
Geher, G. (2014). Evolutionary psychology 101. gender differences, and evolutionary theory.
New York: Springer. Journal of Communication, 46(3), 8–31.
Giles, D. C. (2002). Parasocial interaction: A McCutcheon, L. E., Lange, R., & Houran, J.
review of the literature and a model for future (2002). Conceptualization and measurement
research. Media Psychology, 4(3), 279–305. of celebrity worship. British Journal of Psy-
Grabe, M. E. (2011). News as reality-inducing, chology, 93(1), 67–87.
survival-relevant, and gender-specific stimuli. Mead, G. (1934). Mind, self, and society.
In S. C. Roberts (Ed.), Applied Evolutionary Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press.
Psychology, (361–377). New York: Oxford Miller, G. (2011). The mating mind: How sexual
University Press. choice shaped the evolution of human
Hazen, H. (1983). Endless rapture: Rape, nature. New York: Anchor Books.
romance and the female imagination. New Muir, D. W., Humphrey, E. E., & Humphrey, G. K.
York: Scribner’s. (1994). Pattern and space perception in young
Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. (1994). Attachment as an infants. Spatial Vision, 8(1), 141–165.
organizational framework for research on close Nielsen, M. (2012). Imitation, pretend play, and
relationships. Psychological Inquiry, 5, 1–22. childhood: Essential elements in the evolution
Hobbs, D. R., & Gallup Jr., G. G. (2011). Songs as of human culture? Journal of Comparative
a medium for embedded reproductive mes- Psychology, 126(2), 170–181.
sages. Evolutionary Psychology, 9(3), 390– Ohler, P., & Nieding, G. (2006). An evolutionary
416. doi:10.1177/147470491100900309. perspective on entertainment. In J. Bryan &
Jin, S. V., Ryu, E., & Muqaddam, A. (2019). P. Vorderer (Eds.), Psychology of Entertain-
Romance 2.0 on Instagram! ‘What type of ment, (423–434). New York: Routledge.
girlfriend would you date?’ Evolutionary Psy- Radway, J. (1984). Reading the romance: Women,
chology, 17(1), 1474704919826845. patriarchy, and popular literature. Chapel Hill,
Jonason, P., Webster, G., & Lindsey, A. (2008). NC: University of North Carolina Press.
Solutions to the problem of diminished social Ramos, R. A., Ferguson, C. J., Frailing, K., &
interaction. Evolutionary Psychology, 6(4), Romero-Ramirez, M. (2013). Comfortably
637–661. numb or just yet another movie? Media vio-
Kamhawi, R., & Grabe, M. E. (2006). Gender lence exposure does not reduce viewer empa-
differences in negative news reception: An thy for victims of real violence among primarily
evolutionary psychology explanation. Media Hispanic viewers. Psychology of Popular Media
Report to Women, 34(4), 15. Culture, 2(1), 2–10.
416 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Reeves, B., & Nass, C. I. (1996). The media equa- Stephenson, W. (1988). The play theory of
tion: How people treat computers, television, mass communication. New Brunswick, NJ:
and new media like real people and places. Transaction Publishers.
London: Cambridge University Press. Stever, G. (1991). The celebrity appeal question-
Restak, R. (1991). The brain has a mind of its naire. Psychological Reports, 68, 859–866.
own: Insights from a practicing neurologist. Stever, G. (1994). Parasocial attachments:
New York: Crown. Motivational antecedents. Doctoral Disserta-
Salmon, C. (2012). The pop culture of sex: An tion, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ.
evolutionary window on the worlds of por- Stever, G. (2013). Mediated vs. parasocial rela-
nography and romance. Review of General tionships: An attachment perspective. Jour-
Psychology, 16(2), 152–160. nal of Media Psychology, 17(3), 1–31.
Salmon, C. (2018). Evolutionary perspectives on Stever, G. S. (2017a). Evolutionary theory and
popular culture: State of the art. Evolutionary reactions to mass media: Understanding
Studies in Imaginative Culture, 2(2), 47–66. parasocial attachment. Psychology of Popu-
Scarr, S., & McCartney, K. (1983). How people lar Media Culture, 6(2), 95.
make their own environments: A theory of Stever, G. (2017b). Parasocial theory: Concepts
genotype environment effects. Child Devel- and measures. In P. Rossler (Ed.), Interna-
opment, 54(2), 424–435. tional Encyclopedia of Media Effects. (1–12).
Schramm, W. (Ed.). (1960). Mass communica- doi: 10.1002/9781118783764.wbieme0069.
tion. (2nd ed.). Urbana, IL: University of Illi- Tifferet, S., & Vilnai-Yavetz, I. (2014). Gender
nois Press. differences in Facebook self-presentation: An
Schramm, W. (1988). The story of human com- international randomized study. Computers
munication: Cave painting to microchip. in Human Behavior, 35, 388–399.
New York: Harper and Row. Toth, N., & Schick, K. (1993). Early stone indus-
Schwender, C., & Schwab, F. (2010). The tries and inferences regarding language and
descent of emotions in media: Darwinian cognition. In K. R. Gibson, & T. Ingold (Eds.),
perspectives. In K. Doveling, C. von Scheve & Tools, Language and Cognition in Human
E. A. Konijn (Eds.), The Routledge handbook Evolution, (346–362). Cambridge, UK: Cam-
of emotions and mass media, (29–50). New bridge University Press.
York: Routledge. Tukachinsky, R., & Stever, G. (2019). Theorizing
Seiffert-Brockmann, J. (2018). Evolutionary development of parasocial experiences.
psychology: A framework for strategic com- Communication Theory 29(3) 297–318.
munication research. International Journal of https://doi.org/10.1093/ct/qty032
Strategic Communication, 12(4), 417–432. Umilta, C., Simion, F., & Valenza, E. (1996).
Sherry, J. L. (2004). Media effects theory and Newborn’s preference for faces. European
the nature/nurture debate: A historical over- Psychologist, 1(3), 200–205.
view and directions for future research. Vorderer, P., Steen, F., & Chan, E. (2006). Moti-
Media Psychology, 6(1), 83–109. vation. In J. Bryan & P. Vorderer (Eds.), Psy-
Sherry, J. L. (2015). The complexity paradigm chology of entertainment, (3–17). New York:
for studying human communication: A Routledge.
summary and integration of two fields. Weber, R. (2015). Biology and brains – methodo-
Review of Communication Research, 3, logical innovations in communication science:
22–54. Introduction to the special issue. Communica-
Shoemaker, P. J. (1996). Hardwired for news: tion Methods and Measures, 9, 1–4.
Using biological and cultural evolution to West-Eberhard, M. J. (1979). Sexual selection,
explain the surveillance function. Journal of social competition, and evolution. Proceed-
Communication, 46, 32–47. ings of the American Philosophical Society,
Short, J., Williams, E., & Christie, B. (1976). The 123(4), 222–234.
social psychology of telecommunications. Wright, R. (1994). The moral animal: Why we
New York: Wiley. are the way we are. New York: Vintage Books.
Singh, D., & Singh, D. (2011). Shape and signifi- Zald, D. (2003). The human amygdala and the
cance of feminine beauty: An evolutionary emotional evaluation of sensory stimuli.
perspective. Sex Roles, 64(9–10), 723–731. Brain Research Review, 41, 88–123.
21
Evolutionary Psychology and
Communication
Ned Kock

INTRODUCTION Kock, 2004, 2009; Kock and DeLuca, 2007;


Wainfan and Davis, 2004). Whenever audio
Electronic collaboration (e-collaboration) is is available (e.g., teleconferencing, telephone
collaboration using electronic technologies conference calls, face-to-face meetings), tasks
among different individuals to accomplish a seem to be performed more easily and with
common task. This is a broad definition that fewer misunderstandings. Moreover, adding
encompasses not only computer-mediated video to an already present audio channel typ-
collaborative work, but also collaborative ically adds little to the e-­collaboration medi-
work that is supported by other types of tech- um’s ability to support group tasks (Burke
nologies that do not fit most people’s defini- and Aytes, 2001). While this is not a univer-
tion of a ‘computer’. One example of such sal phenomenon (see, e.g., Daly-Jones et al.,
technologies is the telephone, which is 1998; Baker, 2002; Kock et  al., 2015), its
not, strictly speaking, a computer. Another frequent appearance in the empirical research
example of technology that may enable literature merits a more robust theoretical
e-­collaboration is the teleconferencing suite, analysis.
whose main components are cameras, televi- An evolutionary explanation of the reason
sions, and telecommunications devices. oral speech is needed for effective communi-
One phenomenon that has often puzzled cation is proposed here, as a new theoretical
computer science and information-systems contribution to the e-collaboration literature.
researchers over the years, particularly It is argued that the high importance of oral
researchers interested in e-collaboration issues, speech is restricted to knowledge-intensive
is the high importance of having an audio tasks. The reason for that, which is advanced
channel for communication in the context of in more detail in the subsequent sections, is
e-collaborative tasks (Graetz et  al., 1998; that oral speech evolved among our hominid
418 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

ancestors as a costly trait to enable efficient b­ iologists), generally measured as the num-
and effective knowledge communication. ber of surviving offspring or grand-offspring
As a costly trait, oral speech is analogous to of an individual (Gillespie, 2004; Hartl and
the large train displayed by male peacocks Clark, 2007). The positive impact on fitness
to attract mates (often incorrectly called the results from the competing effects of a costly
peacock’s tail). trait on: (a) survival performance, a negative
Generally speaking, oral speech can be seen effect; and (b) a task performance attribute, a
as: (a) a survival handicap that only evolved positive effect. The net effect of these com-
because of its strong indirect effect on repro- peting effects on fitness is positive, leading to
ductive success, an effect that counteracts its an increase in the frequency of the genotype
negative effect on survival; and (b) particu- associated with the costly trait in the species.
larly important in the context of the task for One example of task performance attribute
which it evolved, namely communication of that could lead to such a positive net effect on
knowledge. Nevertheless, it is acknowledged fitness is the number of lifetime copulations an
here that even in knowledge-intensive tasks, individual participates in, a performance attrib-
the negative influence of suppression of oral ute associated with the task of mating. A clas-
speech may be countered by what is called sic example of a costly trait that evolved due to
here ‘compensatory adaptation’, whereby having increased the number of lifetime copu-
individuals adapt their communicative behav- lations individuals possessing the trait partici-
ior to overcome the limitations posed by the pated in, which in turn offset the survival cost of
suppression of oral speech. As it will be seen that trait, is the peacock’s train (Maynard Smith
later, the word ‘adaptation’ in the term ‘com- and Harper, 2003; Zahavi and Zahavi, 1997).
pensatory adaptation’ does not refer to evolu- The peacock’s train is frequently referred to,
tionary adaptation, but to adaptation that an incorrectly, as the peacock’s tail (Petrie et al.,
individual undergoes within his or her lifetime 1991; Zahavi and Zahavi, 1997). Both males
(often a fraction of one’s lifetime). and females have tails, but only males have the
tail appendage known as a train.
Costly traits may also exist that have com-
peting effects on survival, and that are unre-
COSTLY AND COSTLESS TRAITS: lated to mating, through intermediate effects
SURVIVAL COSTS, REPRODUCTIVE on other variables that themselves directly
SUCCESS, AND THE HANDICAP affect survival. For example, propensity
PRINCIPLE toward aggressive behavior among our ances-
tors might have increased their chances of
Costly traits are phenotypic traits that evolved being the target of violent behavior by other
in spite of having a negative impact on sur- individuals, which contributed to a decrease
vival performance (Gillespie, 2004; Maynard in survival, but might also have increased their
Smith, 1998; Rice, 2004). Survival perfor- access to nutritious food obtained through
mance is the performance of an individual in hunting (for which aggressiveness is impor-
the general task of survival, which can be tant), which in turn contributed to an increase
measured by the age of the individual at the in survival. In this sense, propensity toward
time of death. The older an individual is, the aggressive behavior might have evolved as a
more successful it is at surviving in spite of costly trait, where the positive indirect effect
survival threats (e.g., disease, predators, and on survival, mediated by increased access to
accidental falls). nutritious food, was stronger than the nega-
Costly traits evolve because they have a tive indirect effect on survival from attracting
positive impact on reproductive success (nor- violent behavior (Boaz and Almquist, 2001;
mally referred to as ‘fitness’ by evolutionary Dobzhansky et al., 1977).
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND COMMUNICATION 419

Costless traits are defined here as pheno- COMMON CHARACTERISTICS


typic traits that have no negative impact on OF COSTLY TRAITS: RARITY
survival performance. Most costless traits are AND STRONG EFFECTS
actually associated with enhanced survival
performance, and may be observable indica- The discussion presented here expands on and
tors of unobservable underlying traits that refines the handicap principle to cover any
enhance survival performance (Hamilton and costly trait, in connection with the perfor-
Zuk, 1982; Kokko et al., 2002). The ability of mance of any organism in any task that influ-
males of the fruit fly species Drosophila sub- ences fitness, not only signaling tasks. Two
obscura to engage in a rapid courtship dance key conclusions are reached, which are that
with females is an example of trait that fits costly traits should: be rare in nature, and be
this definition (Maynard Smith and Harper, costly not to use. The three sections that
2003). Males increase their success at the follow this section provide a mathematical
task of mating by demonstrating to females elucidation and basis for these conclusions.
that they possess the ability to dance vigor- While these conclusions are consistent with
ously in response to lead movements by the the handicap principle, they allow for predic-
females. This trait can be seen as a costless tions and explanations that go beyond signal-
trait, because it has no negative impact on ing tasks. As such, they provide the basis for
the survival success of males. In other words, the analysis of the evolution of oral speech
the dance itself has no negative effect on the and its importance in the task of knowledge
survival of males. Presumably the ability to communication.
dance is positively correlated with survival
performance, since it is an indicator of health.
The most widely cited theoretical frame- Costly Traits Should Be Rare in
work in connection with the evolution of costly Nature
traits was proposed by Zahavi (1975), centered
on what is known as the handicap principle The survival handicaps imposed by costly
(Walker, 2008; Zahavi and Zahavi, 1997). traits create obstacles for their evolution,
This framework is intuitively appealing, which eventually making those traits significantly
has led in part to its wide use in research by rarer in nature than costless traits. These
evolutionary biologists, in general (Hausken obstacles can be seen as ‘thresholds’ for evo-
and Hirshleifer, 2008), and more specifi- lution of the traits, where the thresholds are
cally by evolutionary psychology researchers proportional to the survival cost of the traits
(Griskevicius et al., 2007; Walker, 2008). (Gillespie, 2004; Hartl and Clark, 2007;
The handicap principle focuses on costly Maynard Smith and Harper, 2003).
traits used for signaling, and is founded on New traits (e.g., high intelligence, long
the notion that those traits are honest indica- legs, and slow fat metabolism) usually appear
tors of the signalers’ fitness. For example, the in populations of organisms as a result of ran-
large train displayed by peacocks is a survival dom genetic mutations; a general rule that
handicap, making them more vulnerable to applies to all organisms, including our homi-
predation (Maynard Smith and Harper, 2003; nid ancestors (Boaz and Almquist, 2001; Hartl
Zahavi and Zahavi, 1997). Consequently, and Clark, 2007; Mayr, 1976). Therefore, the
males with large trains and who are still alive effects of new traits on fitness are also ran-
at the age of reproductive maturity also must dom, whether those traits are costly or cost-
possess other traits that make them particu- less. Evolution is not an engineering process;
larly good at survival, such as vitality and it is a wasteful process of continuous tinker-
speed. The tails are a reliable indicator of fit- ing, where the vast majority of new traits
ness, exactly because they are costly. are in fact detrimental to fitness (Hartl and
420 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Clark, 2007; Wilson, 2000). Traits that have the eyes, various feather patterns occurring
a positive net effect on fitness are far and in different parts of the male’s body, and the
few between (McElreath and Boyd, 2007; level of bilateral (i.e., left–right) symmetry of
Wilson, 2000). these ornamentations (Darwin, 1871; Zahavi
Given that costly traits must overcome and Zahavi, 1997).
obstacles, or thresholds, to evolve, fewer
costly traits than costless traits are likely to
evolve. That is, the probability of evolution THE THRESHOLD FOR EVOLUTION
of costly traits in any species is generally OF COSTLY TRAITS
lower than that of costless traits. Moreover,
the higher the cost of the trait, the lower its One of the most important contributions to
probability of evolution. Thus, costly traits mathematical evolutionary thinking was
should be rarer in nature than costless traits; made by Price (1970). He showed that for
the costlier, the rarer. any trait to evolve through selection, in any
population or subpopulation of individuals of
the same species, the trait must satisfy
Costly Traits Should Be Costly Equation (1), whose main element is a covar-
Not to Use iance term. The fitness of an individual that
possesses the trait (e.g., number of surviving
Costly traits must have had a strong effect on offspring) is measured through W, and Z is a
the performance of the task for which they measure of the manifestation of the trait in
evolved in order to make up for the survival the individual (e.g., Z = 1 if the trait is pre-
costs imposed by those traits. Today, this sent, and Z = 0 if it is absent). The trait in
would translate into a higher correlation question can be any morphological, physio-
between costly traits’ measures and perfor- logical or behavioral trait; examples could be
mance attributes for the task, than between opposing thumbs, aggressiveness, or a large
costless traits’ measures and the same task train (tail appendage) with many eyespots.
performance attributes. That is, not using a
costly trait would be costlier, so to speak, Cov(W, Z) > 0 (1)
than not using a costless trait in the context
of the task for which the traits evolved. Equation (1) can be re-written as Equation
The above conclusions seem to be true (2) in terms of the standardized measures of
when we look at the classical example of W and Z, referred to as w and z. This allows for
costly trait, the peacock train. Petrie et  al. its use in the context of path analysis (Kock,
(1991) found that the costly ornamental train 2011; Kock and Moqbel, 2016; Wright, 1934,
of the male, and especially the number of 1960), which in turn greatly simplifies (as it
eyespots on the train, are far more attractive will be shown below) theoretical reasoning
traits for the peahens than other apparently based on comparative analyses of evolution
costless traits. Costless ornamental traits are of traits through selection.
more numerous in the peacock species than Cov(w · SW + W, z · SZ + Z)
costly ones, of which the only known one is
the train, and their relative importance in the = Cov(w · SW, z · SZ)
context of the mating task is dwarfed by the = SW · SZ Cov(w, z) > 0
importance of the train. Examples of costless ⇒ Cov(w · z) > 0 (2)
ornamental traits likely evolved for mating
in the male of peafowl are the crest atop the Figure 21.1 shows a path model where
male’s head, the brightly colored feathers on a costly trait measured by y is represented.
the male’s chest, various color patterns around All the measures are standardized, which is
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND COMMUNICATION 421

For a costless trait x, a trait with no negative


a effect on survival, Equation (3) is reduced to
pwa pay
Equation (4) because psy equals zero. What
w y this equation tells us is that a costless trait x
pas
will always evolve as long as it has a positive
pws psy causal relationship with a task performance
s attribute a, assuming that a has a positive
causal relationship with fitness (w).
Figure 21.1  Path model showing a costly pax > 0 (4)
trait and its relationship with fitness
In the task of mating, for example, any
costless trait x that increases mating success
why they are indicated with lowercase let-
(measured by a) would evolve through selec-
ters. The measure y has a positive causal rela-
tion, with trait frequency growth subject to the
tionship with a task performance attribute a.
constraints posed by chance events unrelated
For example, a could be number of lifetime
to the trait. That is, the trait would evolve to
copulations of an individual, a performance
the point of becoming widespread in a popu-
attribute associated with the mating task, in
lation only if it is not eliminated by chance
the case of a trait used for mate choice.
from the population at its early stages of evo-
The measure y has a negative causal rela-
lution; e.g., the only individual that initially
tionship with s, a measure of survival per-
possesses the trait is killed by a lightning
formance. For example, s could be age of an
strike before reaching reproductive matu-
individual at the time of death. Since any indi-
rity (Gillespie, 2004; Wen-Hsiung, 2000). A
vidual must be alive to perform any task, s also
costly trait y (e.g., the peacock’s train), on the
has a positive causal relationship with a. Both
other hand, would have to meet a more strin-
a and s have positive causal relationships with
gent requirement for evolution. It would only
fitness (w). The magnitudes of these relation-
evolve through selection if the trait’s positive
ships are given by the path coefficients pay, psy
effect on a surpassed the threshold given by
etc. All path coefficients are positive except
the right side of Equation (3).
for psy, which is negative since it refers to the
survival cost of trait y. (For simplicity, a trait
measured by y is also called trait y.)
In path analysis the covariance between PROBABILITY OF EVOLUTION OF
any pair of variables is given by the sum of
COSTLY TRAITS
the products of the path coefficients in all
paths connecting the two variables (Wright,
Let us assume that the appearance of a new
1934, 1960). Thus, combining Equation (2)
costly trait y in a population will lead to a
with Figure 21.1 leads to Equation (3), which
variation in pay and –psy that will be given by
must be satisfied for any costly trait y to
random numbers going from 0 to D. Let us
evolve through selection.
also represent T as in Equation (5):
pwa ∙ pay + pws ∙ psy + pwa ∙ pas ∙ psy > 0
 p 
 
⇒pwa ∙ pay > –psy ∙ (pws + pwa ∙ pas) T =  ws + pas  (5)
 p 
 wa 
⇒pay > –psy∙ (pws
pwa + pas)

 p  The value of T is assumed here to be


 
⇒ pay > − psy ⋅  ws + pas  (3) largely population specific, in a relatively
 p 
 wa  stable environment, and thus should remain
422 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

relatively constant as new costly or costless the performance of a task is more important
traits appear in a population and either evolve for fitness than survival, even for the all-
or disappear in response to selection pres- important task of mating. Let us consider,
sures. This can be illustrated for the task of for example, spider species where the males
mating, where a can be the number of life- are routinely cannibalized by their large and
time copulations a male of a species engages aggressive female mates during or after copu-
in. In this case, the effect of a on fitness (w), lation (see, e.g., Wilder and Rypstra, 2008).
the effect of survival performance (s) on w, In these species, the male spiders must still
and the effect of s on a are relatively constant successfully survive up to the moment of
for the males of the species. copulation. Therefore, when looked at as a
In a relatively stable environment this can subpopulation of the species to which they
be shown to hold for any task whose perfor- belong, those male spiders will likely have a
mance is measured by a. This conclusion ratio pws / pwa that is greater than 1 (and thus
also follows from the assumption that those a T greater than 1) for the task of mating,
effects can be represented through stable regardless of the fact that they contribute lit-
regression coefficients, an assumption that tle more than their sperm to the survival of
is routinely used in mathematical popula- their offspring (and thus to their own fitness).
tion genetics models (Gillespie, 2004; Kock, Equation (6) can be depicted in a graph, as
2011; Kock and Moqbel, 2016; Rice, 2004). shown in Figure 21.2. The graph shows the
On the other hand, the effects that new variation of the probability of evolution of
traits x or y have on a and/or s can vary a new costly trait y in a population (vertical
widely, since those traits appear in the pop- axis) based on values of T ranging from 1 to
ulation as a result of stochastic processes. 10 (horizontal axis).
Those effects will in turn ultimately dictate As can be inferred from Figure 21.2, costly
whether those traits will evolve or disappear traits will always have a lower probability of
in the species. For species that live today in evolution than costless traits, because the
environments similar to those in which most value of T for the latter traits is always zero.
of their traits evolved, the value of T can be This suggests that costly traits should be rarer
easily estimated empirically since the path in nature than costless ones, regardless of the
coefficients are standardized partial regres- task for which they were evolved, e.g., mat-
sion coefficients. ing, communication, fighting.
Given the above assumptions, the prob- Moreover, costly traits should be particu-
ability of evolution of a new costly trait y in larly rare in species where the value of T is
a population will be given by Equation (6), high. This would be the case in species where
assuming that T is equal to or greater than the number of offspring born to females was
one. This equation reflects the intersection small; and in species where the offspring
spaces of variation of D and DT, and can eas- relied heavily on their parents for survival
ily be verified through simple Monte Carlo in their early years of life, when most deaths
simulations. occur. (In these species, the effect of survival
on fitness would have been much higher
D 1
( )
P Evo ( y ) = ⋅
DT 2
than the effect of mating on fitness.) These
are characteristics of the human species,

1 and likely of the hominid ancestors in the
(
⇒ P Evo ( y ) = )
2⋅ T
(6) human lineage (Boaz and Almquist, 2001;
Cartwright, 2000). Thus, T values should
T is assumed to be equal to or greater than have been high for our hominid ancestors,
one because it is difficult to conceive of a spe- making the evolution of costly traits diffi-
cies population or subpopulation for which cult. This line of reasoning would suggest
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND COMMUNICATION 423

0.5
0.45
0.4
0.35
0.3
0.25
0.2
0.15
0.1
0.05
0
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10

Figure 21.2  Probability of evolution of a new costly trait

the existence of moderating effects in our for costless traits that evolve will then be given
model, which are not included because their by D divided by 2. The expected value of pay,
discussion would be beyond the scope of this on the other hand, will be given by (DT–D)/2,
chapter. This has no effect on the generality assuming that T is equal to or greater than 1.
of the discussion presented here or the related Therefore, the expected ratios between pay
conclusions. and pax will be given by Equation (7).
Of course, in order to evolve, costly and

) ( D/2 )
costless traits also have to satisfy the con- D⋅ T − D /2
dition that their covariance with fitness is

(
E pay / pax =
greater than zero (i.e., that they have a posi-
tive net impact on fitness), which rarely is ⇒ E( p / p ) = T −1
ay ax
(7)
the case for new genetic mutations. Most
new genetic mutations have either a nega- The graph in Figure 21.3 shows the vari­
tive or neutral effect on fitness; in the latter ation of the expected ratio between pay
case they may evolve by chance, through a and pax (vertical axis) based on values of T
process known as genetic drift (Gillespie, ranging from 1 to 10 (horizontal axis). The
2004; Hartl and Clark, 2007; Maynard Smith, ratio grows proportionally with T, and is a
1998). Costly traits, unlike costless ones, measure of how strong the expected effect
have another condition to satisfy: they must of a costly trait y on the task performance
overcome the survival costs that they impose. attribute a is, compared with the expected
effect of a costless trait x. For simplicity, it
is assumed here that both types of traits are
either independent from each other, or spread
DIFFERENT EFFECTS OF COSTLY to fix­ation in a species at different points in
AND COSTLESS TRAITS time. For dependent traits or traits that evolve
at the same time, the mathematical analysis
Let us also assume that the appearance of becomes more complex, but the results are
costly and costless traits in a population will qualitatively the same.
lead to random values of pax and –psy in the An expected ratio between pay and pax of
range from 0 to D. The expected value of pax 1.3, for example, means that the standardized
424 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

9
8
7
6
5

4
3
2
1
0
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10

Figure 21.3  Variation of the expected ratio between pay and pax

effect of any costly trait on a given task suffering from various aerodigestive tract
performance attribute a is on average 30% diseases such as gastroesophageal reflux
stronger than the effect of any costless trait (Laitman and Reidenberg, 1997), among
on the same task performance attribute. A other survival-related problems. Oral speech
ratio of 8 means that the costly trait is on aver- must have been particularly important for
age 800% stronger (e.g., pay=.4 and pax=.05). effective communication in our evolutionary
Standardized effects are expressed in terms of past, and effective communication must have
standard deviations of the variables to which been important for fitness enhancement
they refer. For example, a pay=.4 means that a (Pinker, 2003), otherwise its survival cost
1 standard deviation variation in y causes a .4 would have prevented complex speech from
standard deviation variation in a. evolving.
The situation with gastroesophageal reflux
is particularly telling with respect to the cost
of the evolution of oral speech in humans.
THE EVOLUTION OF ORAL SPEECH The aerodigestive tract adaptations that
IN HUMANS: A COSTLY TRAIT enable complex oral speech make modern
ASSOCIATED WITH CHOKING AND humans differ from their Australopithecine
ILLNESSES ancestors in ways that are analogous to the
differences between the aerodigestive tracts
Modern oral speech was enabled by the evo- of modern humans and modern non-human
lution of a larynx located low in the neck primates. In modern humans the incidence of
(Lieberman, 1998). The evolution of oral gastroesophageal reflux symptoms (at various
speech is one of the most important land- degrees of severity) has been reported to be as
marks in the evolution of the human species, high as 40%, and yet Glover et al. (2008) noted
having happened relatively recently in our that no modern non-human primate species
evolutionary history (Figure 21.4). However, was reported to display naturally occurring
the new larynx design also likely increased symptoms. The occurrence of gastroesopha-
significantly our ancestors’ chances of death geal reflux symptoms is associated with signif-
by choking during ingestion of food and liq- icantly greater rates of mortality among infants
uids, a leading cause of death among modern (Vandenplas et al., 1991). This is also the case
humans (Hemsley et  al., 2019), and of among adults, due to various conditions,
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND COMMUNICATION 425

Emergence of
Emergence of Homo sapiens
Australopithecines

Today
Millions of
years
3.5 2.0 1.5 1.0 0.1

Co-located and synchronous


communication through facial
expressions, body language, Complex
and simple sounds oral
speech

Figure 21.4  The evolution of oral speech in humans

including hemorrhagic reflux esophagitis since the evolution of a costly trait is a low-
(Rantanen and Salo, 1999). probability event, and low-probability events
Oral speech seems to exhibit the two com- frequently take more time to happen than
mon characteristics of costly traits. Oral high-probability events. In fact, the evolu-
speech is a rare costly trait among human tion of oral speech coincides with the evo-
traits involved in the transfer of communi- lution of our species, Homo sapiens, likely
cative stimuli. By all accounts, it is the only from another species within the genus Homo,
such trait that obviously imposed a survival namely Homo erectus (Boaz and Almquist,
cost as it evolved among our ancestors. In 2001; Lieberman, 1998). Many human evo-
addition to increasing our ancestors’ chances lution researchers believe that it was the evo-
of death by choking, and of developing aero­ lution of oral speech, with the complexity of
digestive tract diseases, it also hampered our human interactions that it enabled, that made
ancestors’ ability to breathe while drinking us truly human (Cartwright, 2000; Dunbar,
water. Water sources are likely to have been 1993; Lieberman, 1998).
a preferred site for predators to ambush prey Finally, empirical research on the effects
(Boaz and Almquist, 2001), as they are today, of electronic communication media that sup-
and the oxygen depletion caused by having press the ability to use oral speech suggests
to hold their breath while drinking created that it is costly not to use oral speech in com-
yet another survival cost for our ancestors. municative interactions. This is reflected in
Other communication-related traits, such as as much as a 10-fold reduction in commu-
the ability to use body language and facial nication fluency, coupled with a significant
expressions, do not seem to have imposed a increase in communication ambiguity and
similar survival handicap on our ancestors. perceived cognitive effort (Graetz et al., 1998;
Oral speech also appeared late in the evo- Kock, 2005; Kock and DeLuca, 2007; Kock
lutionary history of hominids, in the last et  al., 2007; Simon, 2006). Communication
100,000 years of that 3.5-million-year history, fluency is defined here as the number of ideas
or approximately the last 3% (Cartwright, effectively conveyed per unit of time, and
2000; Laitman, 1984; Lieberman, 1998). has been somewhat imprecisely measured
This is consistent with it being a costly trait, as the number of words conveyed per unit of
426 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Table 21.1  Oral speech and the two common characteristics of costly traits
Common characteristic of costly traits Evidence in connection with oral speech

Costly traits should be rare in nature Oral speech is the only communication-related trait that clearly imposed a survival
cost on our ancestors; conversely, various related costless traits seem to exist –
e.g., the ability to use body language and facial expressions for communication.
Costly traits should be costly not to Suppressing oral speech in electronic communication media is costly, leading
use to as much as a 10-fold reduction in communication fluency, coupled with
a significant increase in communication ambiguity and perceived cognitive effort.

time (Kock, 2005; Kock and DeLuca, 2007). It as discussed so far, needs further theoretical
seems that, when oral speech is removed from elaboration and refinement. Simple observa-
a communication medium, communication tion of modern human communication prac-
becomes rather cumbersome (Crowston et al., tices suggests that oral speech is not equally
2007; Graetz et al., 1998; Kock, 2005). Table important for all types of communication
21.1 summarizes the discussion so far regard- interactions. For example, if one person is
ing the relationship between oral speech and trying to communicate his or her home or
the two common characteristics of costly traits. work address to another, to be used on a
If the use of oral speech is enabled by an letter, then probably an e-mail will be just as
audio channel, adding a video channel typi- effective as a phone call. Also, web-based
cally has little impact on the effectiveness social communication tools that enable
or ease with which communication takes human interaction through short text mes-
place (Burke and Aytes, 2001; Daly-Jones sages and provide no audio channel, such as
et al., 1998; Simon, 2006). In this sense, oral in the early versions of Twitter, would prob-
speech could be seen to the communication ably not be as successful as they are if the
task as analogous to what the peacock’s train theoretical framework put forth here applied
is to the mating task (Petrie et al., 1991); both to all types of communicative interactions.
are costly traits that have an effect that dwarfs This takes us back to a review of why oral
the effects of other costless traits evolved in speech evolved in the first place. More spe-
connection with the same task. There are cifically, how did oral speech affect fitness
exceptions to this general rule regarding the among our ancestors? As discussed earlier,
importance of a video channel (see, e.g., only if oral speech had a net positive impact
Daly-Jones et  al., 1998), such as modern on fitness, by enhancing the performance of
tasks in which shared and real-time visuali- a fitness-relevant task, would it have over-
zation of an object or situation is important come the survival handicap associated with
for the task completion. Examples would be our customized vocal tract. The answer is
a surgical intervention involving two or more that oral speech enabled the exchange of
geographically distributed doctors, and a knowledge among our ancestors, which indi-
real-time collaborative design of a car engine. rectly increased their reproductive success by
allowing them to occupy what Pinker (2003)
refers to as the ‘cognitive niche’.
A common characteristic of the simple
ORAL SPEECH AND KNOWLEDGE exchanges illustrated above (communication
COMMUNICATION: THE FOCUSED of a home or work address and interaction
IMPACT ON FITNESS OF THIS through short text messages) is that these types
COSTLY TRAIT of exchanges involve little or no knowledge
transfer. Therefore, if we assume that oral
The notion that oral speech is particularly speech was evolved by our ancestors primarily
important in modern human communication, to enable the communication of knowledge,
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND COMMUNICATION 427

its effect should not be particularly strong in in higher quantities in certain areas (Hung,
communication interactions with little or no 2004; Kock et  al., 2008; Manipady et  al.,
knowledge content. There are other factors 2006). Without the ability to vicariously
that may induce modern humans to commu- obtain knowledge linking contexts with sur-
nicate electronically through text only, and vival threats, our ancestors would have to
with no audio – e.g., via e-mail without audio experience the survival threats, or observe
file attachments. Among those factors is the someone experiencing them at a close dis-
ease with which e-mail can be sent to many tance, in order to build that knowledge. Oral
individuals at the same time. Video and audio speech enabled vicarious knowledge acquisi-
blogs can be used for the same purpose, incor- tion regarding survival threats, and thus sig-
porating oral speech, but their use is still not as nificantly increased our ancestors’ chances of
widespread and embedded in communication survival, easily overcoming the extra survival
practices as is the use of e-mail. costs associated with our vocal tract.
Knowledge about ‘something’ is defined Costly traits evolved by our human ances-
here in a way that is analogous to how it is tors must have had a strong effect on the per-
defined by many cognitive psychologists: as formance of the task for which they evolved,
a set of mental schemas that allows one to in order to make up for the survival costs
predict the future, or find out more about a imposed by those traits. In the case of oral
present situation, based on information about speech, a strong candidate for the task in
the present or the past (Gardner, 1985; Kock, question is the knowledge communication
1999; Lee and Holyoak, 2008; Waldmann task, where oral speech evolved in part to
et  al., 1995). As noted by artificial intelli- increase the performance with which knowl-
gence researchers, with knowledge, one can edge about survival threats was communi-
build mental rules that can be expressed in cated among our ancestors. Oral speech may
the form of ‘if … then …’ statements (Luger also have influenced fitness in other ways,
and Stubblefield, 2008; Russel and Norvig, although avoidance of survival threats must
2002), or reworded as statements that con- have been an important element in the selec-
tain linguistic elements that express causal- tion of this costly trait. For example, vicari-
ity such as ‘the reason for … is …’, ‘this ous knowledge about s­urvival-enhancing
is … because …’, and ‘the cause for … is elements, such as seasonal availability of
…’ (Kock, 1999; Waldmann et  al., 1995). food, was likely also enabled by oral speech
For example, the statement ‘the temperature (Cartwright, 2000; Dunbar, 1999). So proba-
in room 118, where 100 people are attend- bly was the ability to build social relationships
ing a lecture, is now 78 degrees Fahrenheit’ and court potential mates (Dunbar, 1993;
contains only information, whereas the fol- Miller, 2000, 2002). This type of knowledge
lowing statement contains knowledge: ‘if the communication likely required reciprocal
temperature in room 118 reaches 80 degrees altruism to have evolved before, which math-
Fahrenheit, most of the 100 people attending ematical formalizations and empirical evi-
a lecture there will feel uncomfortable’. dence strongly suggest was the case in the
Our ancestors faced survival threats on human species (Fletcher and Zwick, 2007;
a regular basis – exposure to pathogens, Henrich, 2004; McElreath and Boyd, 2007;
attacks by predators or territorial animals, Trivers, 2002).
encounters with venomous insects or snakes, Knowledge communication performance
and ingestion of toxins, among others. These refers to both the effectiveness and effi-
events often occurred in specific contexts. ciency with which knowledge is communi-
For example, territorial animals would attack cated (Kock, 1999; Russel and Norvig, 2002;
when their habitat was invaded by our ances- Waldmann et al., 1995). Effective knowledge
tors, and venomous insects and snakes occur communication between two individuals
428 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

occurs when the knowledge possessed by effort and frustration were about the same for
one individual is comprehensively and unam- the ­face-to-face and telephone-­conferencing
biguously conveyed to the other individual. media, and significantly higher for the
Efficient knowledge communication occurs electronic-chat medium. In summary, the
­
when the knowledge possessed by one indi- medium that did not enable oral speech was
vidual is quickly and effortlessly conveyed to the least conducive to effortless and unam-
the other individual. biguous knowledge communication. This is
consistent with the view that oral speech is
a costly trait that is ‘costly not to use’ in the
context of knowledge communication.
THE EFFECTS OF ORAL SPEECH Particularly noteworthy is the finding by
IN E-COLLABORATIVE TASKS: Kock and DeLuca (2007), in a study of indi-
COMMUNICATION FLUENCY viduals in two different countries, that the use
AND AMBIGUITY of an electronic communication medium that
suppressed the ability to convey speech (a
It follows from the theoretical discussion version of e-mail) dramatically reduced com-
presented in the previous section that remov- munication fluency. In this study, communi-
ing the ability to convey speech from an cation fluency was measured as the number of
electronic communication medium is likely words conveyed per unit of time, a surrogate
to impair communication performance much measure. The reduction in fluency observed
more strongly than removing the medium’s by Kock and DeLuca was estimated to have
ability to convey other communicative stim- been more than 10-fold; that is, e-mail users’
uli – e.g., facial expressions, body language, fluency was less than 1/10 of their expected
olfactory cues, and tactile stimuli. However, fluency communicating over the phone or
this effect is moderated by the extent to face-to-face. This is too drastic a reduction to
which knowledge is being communicated. be explained by the known fact that typing is
This conclusion is consistent with the results mechanically more cumbersome than speak-
of various studies that compare the impact of ing, which would normally lead to a twofold
various media on communication perfor- reduction in fluency (Kock, 2004; McQueen
mance (Graetz et  al., 1998; Kock, 2004; et al., 1999). Again, it appears that our brain
Kock and DeLuca, 2007). was ‘designed’ by evolution to rely heavily on
Graetz et  al. (1998) compared the perfor- oral speech for effective and efficient knowl-
mance in four-person groups across three com- edge communication, because oral speech
munication media conditions: face-to-face, was costly to evolve. As a result, it is costly
telephone conferencing, and electronic chat. not to use oral speech in modern human com-
The experimental task required exchange of munication whenever a significant amount of
knowledge to be successfully accomplished, knowledge must be exchanged.
and the participants were given a limited
amount of time (approximately 30 minutes)
to review the information provided to them
by the researchers and to discuss it with the COMPENSATORY ADAPTATION:
other group members. Group outcome quality COUNTERACTING THE EFFECTS OF
was about the same through the face-to-face ORAL SPEECH SUPPRESSION BY
and telephone-conferencing media; slightly E-COLLABORATION TECHNOLOGIES
higher in the latter, a statistically insignificant
difference. Group outcome quality was sig- A possible conclusion based on the arguments
nificantly lower through the electronic chat presented thus far is that a decrease in commu-
medium. Measures of perceived cognitive nication fluency and an increase in ambiguity,
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND COMMUNICATION 429

caused by the suppression of oral speech in adapt their communicative behavior in order
an electronic medium, may lead to a decrease to compensate for the obstacles posed by the
in the quality of the outcomes accomplished e-collaboration medium’s suppression of oral
by a group using the medium for most of its speech (Short et al., 1976; Ulijn et al., 2001).
communication. Indeed, this seems to fre- Compensatory adaptation can be under-
quently be the case in short-duration tasks stood as a moderating effect. That is, the
(Graetz et al., 1998; Kahai and Cooper, 2003; effects of oral speech suppression on com-
Warkentin et al., 1997), but not necessarily in munication fluency and ambiguity are mod-
long-duration tasks (Burke and Chidambaram, erated by compensatory adaptation, whose
1999; Carlson, 1995; DeLuca, 2003; Kock, moderating effect is in turn positively cor-
2005; Kock and DeLuca, 2007). The reason related with e-collaborative task duration.
is that, in long-duration tasks, it is common In short-duration tasks, the negative effects
to observe a phenomenon known as compen- of oral speech suppression on communica-
satory adaptation (Kock, 2002). This phe- tion fluency and ambiguity are likely to be
nomenon may counteract the problems particularly acute, as there is no time for
associated with the suppression of oral compensatory adaptation to take place. In
speech (Kock, 2005; Kock et al., 2007). long-duration tasks, the e-collaborators may
An example of a short-duration task would adapt their behavior to compensate for the
be to write a one-page contract to sell a car, cognitive obstacles caused by the suppres-
where the contract would be written in one sion of oral speech. This phenomenon has
hour. A long-duration equivalent would be been referred to as compensatory adaptation
to write a 20-page contract to sell a fleet of to e-collaboration media of low naturalness
cars, where the longer contract would be (Kock, 2004).
developed over a period of two weeks. If an
electronic medium is to be used for commu-
nication to accomplish these tasks, where
oral speech is suppressed in the communica- DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSION
tion medium, we would expect a more drastic
drop in quality for the one-page contract than The arguments presented in the previous
for the longer one with 20 pages. Here, ‘qual- sections can be summarized into three main
ity’ could be measured based on a number of predictions. The first refers to the effects of
factors, including the degree of understand- oral speech suppression on communication
ability of the contract by the parties involved fluency and ambiguity in the context of
and the degree to which the contract clauses e-collaboration. The second refers to the
would be legally enforceable. moderating effect that the amount of
Compensatory adaptation seems to be knowledge communicated is likely to have
one of the reasons why groups performing on these effects. The third prediction refers to
knowledge-intensive tasks over a relatively the moderating effect that compensatory
long period of time (e.g., days, weeks, or adaptation is likely to have on the effects of
months), using an e-collaboration medium oral speech suppression on communication
that suppresses oral speech, often have the fluency and ambiguity. Compensatory
same or even better performance than groups adaptation itself is correlated with task
where oral interaction is not suppressed duration, and may take place even when a
(Kock, 2005). As long as there is motivation large amount of knowledge is being
among group members to expend additional communicated. These predictions are outlined
compensatory effort, which may be strongly below, and followed by recommendations
influenced by social factors (Bandura, 1986; for the use of e-collaboration tools in
Fulk, 1993), group members are likely to organizations.
430 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Communication Fluency unnatural (e.g., one that suppresses oral


and Ambiguity speech), seems to moderate the negative effects
of oral speech suppression on communication
A key prediction based on the discussion put fluency and ambiguity. Compensatory adapta-
forth here is that removing the ability to tion to media that suppress oral speech typi-
convey speech from an e-collaboration cally happens over time (e.g., days, weeks or
medium used by modern humans is likely to months), as individuals modify their commu-
decrease communication fluency and increase nicative behavior to make up for the shortcom-
communication ambiguity much more strongly ings of the medium. This may be one of the
than removing the medium’s ability to convey reasons why compensatory adaptation is not
other communicative stimuli such as facial normally observed in short-duration tasks requir-
expressions and body language. The reason is ing intense knowledge exchange. For example,
that the ability to use speech for communica- groups performing knowledge-intensive tasks
tion evolved at a much higher survival cost through text-based e-collaboration technolo-
among our human ancestors than the ability to gies, and where the tasks last from a few min-
use any other communicative stimulus. utes to a few hours, generally tend to produce
task outcomes of inferior quality. These groups
would be better off either: (a) performing the
The Moderating Effect of task face-to-face or using an e-collaboration
Knowledge Communication technology that provides an audio channel; or
(b) performing the task using a text-based
The negative effects of oral speech suppression e-collaboration technology, but over a long
on communication fluency and ambiguity are time period (e.g., a few days) so that compen-
moderated by the amount of knowledge com- satory adaptation can take place.
munication taking place in an e-collaborative The increasingly distributed nature of
task. Due to the context in which oral speech organizational processes (e.g., sets of activi-
evolved among our ancestors, oral speech is not ties that are repeated over and over again) and
equally important for all types of communica- projects (e.g., sets of activities that are carried
tion interactions among modern humans; it is out once or a few times) requires tasks to be
particularly important in knowledge-intensive accomplished by groups of individuals who
communication. Communicating one’s home are not only geographically distributed, but
address to another person, for example, can be also distributed across multiple time zones.
easily and effectively accomplished through Given this, it is impractical to try to ensure
e-mail. Conversely, if one engineer wants to that all activities in a process or project are
communicate knowledge about how to design a performed face-to-face, or even through
new car engine to a production manager, then e-­collaboration involving synchronous oral
the suppression of oral speech may be speech interactions. Sometimes ubiquitous
problematic. text-based asynchronous communication such
as e-mail must be used for part of the process
or project, due to cost constraints. It is also
The Moderating Effect of possible that asynchronous oral speech inter-
Compensatory Adaptation actions will be used (e.g., voice messaging or
e-mail with attached audio messages) for part
Another moderating effect, similar to but of a of the process or project, due to group mem-
different kind than knowledge communication, bers having to work from different time zones.
is compensatory adaptation. Compensatory A more practical piece of advice to manag-
adaptation, or the degree to which individuals ers, which follows from the theoretical discus-
adapt to a communication medium that is sion, is the following: (a) break organizational
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND COMMUNICATION 431

processes and projects into component collabo- Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of
rative activities; (b) rank those activities in terms thought and action. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
of the perceived amount of knowledge exchange Prentice Hall.
involved; (c) make sure that highly knowledge- Boaz, N.T., & Almquist, A.J. (2001). Biological
intensive activities are performed through media anthropology: A synthetic approach to
human evolution. Upper Saddle River, NJ:
that incorporate synchronous oral speech (e.g.,
Prentice Hall.
face-to-face or teleconferencing interaction),
Burke, K., & Aytes, K. (2001). Do media really
which may mean that certain group members affect perceptions and procedural structuring
will have to make special accommodations to among partially-distributed groups? Journal
participate in group discussions (e.g., attend a on Systems and Information Technology, 5(1),
meeting at 3 a.m., local time); (d) make sure that 10–23.
moderately knowledge-intensive activities are Burke, K., & Chidambaram, L. (1999). How
performed through media that incorporate some much bandwidth is enough? A longitudinal
form of oral speech, even if asynchronous (e.g., examination of media characteristics and
voice messaging or e-mail with attached audio group outcomes. MIS Quarterly, 23(4),
messages); and (e) encourage the use of text- 557–580.
based e-collaboration media for activities that Carlson, J.R. (1995). Channel expansion theory:
A dynamic view of media and information
involve little or no knowledge exchange among
richness perception. Doctoral dissertation.
participants, as this is likely to be the cheapest
Tallahassee, FL: Florida State University.
and most widely available organizational com- Cartwright, J. (2000). Evolution and human
munication medium. behavior: Darwinian perspectives on human
nature. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
Crowston, K., Howison, J., Masango, C., &
Eseryel, U.Y. (2007). The role of face-to-face
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS meetings in technology-supported self-
organizing distributed teams. IEEE Transac-
This book chapter is based on a previous ver- tions on Professional Communication, 50(3),
sion published as an article in the journal 185–203.
Electronic Markets, and also contains revised Daly-Jones, O., Monk, A., & Watts, L. (1998).
materials and ideas, originally developed by Some advantages of video conferencing over
the author, from articles published in the fol- high-quality audio conferencing: Fluency and
lowing journals: International Journal of awareness of attentional focus. International
e-Collaboration; Cognition, Technology & Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 49(1),
21–58.
Work; International Journal of Technology
Darwin, C.R. (1871). The descent of man, and
and Human Interaction; Journal of Systems
selection in relation to sex. London, England:
and Information Technology; and Journal of John Murray.
Evolutionary Psychology. This is done with DeLuca, D.C. (2003). Business process improve-
full permission by the copyright holders, and ment using asynchronous e-collaboration:
with the goal of timely and wide dissemina- Testing the compensatory adaptation model.
tion of scholarly work. Doctoral dissertation. Philadelphia, PA:
Temple University.
Dobzhansky, T., Ayala, F.J., Stebbins, G.L., &
Valentine, J.W. (1977). Evolution. San Fran-
REFERENCES cisco, CA: W.H. Freeman and Company.
Dunbar, R.I.M. (1993). Coevolution of neocortical
Baker, G. (2002). The effects of synchronous col- size, group size and language in humans.
laborative technologies on decision making: A Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16(4), 681–735.
study of virtual teams. Information Resources Dunbar, R.I.M. (1999). Culture, honesty and
Management Journal, 15(4), 79–94. the freerider problem. In: R.I.M. Dunbar,
432 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

C. Knight, & C. Power (Eds.), The evolution Hung, D.-Z. (2004). Taiwan’s venomous
of culture (pp. 194–213). New Brunswick, snakebite: Epidemiological, evolution and
NJ: Rutgers University Press. geographic differences. Transactions of the
Fletcher, J.A., & Zwick, M. (2007). The evolution Royal Society of Tropical Medicine and
of altruism: Game theory in multilevel selection Hygiene, 98(2), 96–101.
and inclusive fitness. Journal of Theoretical Kahai, S.S., & Cooper, R.B. (2003). Exploring
Biology, 245(1), 26–36. the core concepts of media richness theory:
Fulk, J. (1993). Social construction of commu- The impact of cue multiplicity and feedback
nication technology. Academy of Manage- immediacy on decision quality. Journal of
ment Journal, 36(5), 921–938. Management Information Systems, 20(1),
Gardner, H. (1985). The mind’s new science. 263–281.
New York, NY: Basic Books. Kock, N. (1999). Process improvement and
Gillespie, J.H. (2004). Population genetics. Balti- organizational learning: The role of collabo-
more, MD: The Johns Hopkins University Press. ration technologies. Hershey, PA: Idea Group
Glover, E.J., Leland, M.M., Dick Jr, E.J., & Hub- Publishing.
bard, G.B. (2008). Gastroesophageal reflux Kock, N. (2002). Compensatory adaptation:
disease in baboons (Papio sp.): A new animal Understanding how obstacles can lead to
model. Journal of Medical Primatology, success. Haverford, PA: Infinity Publishing.
37(1), 18–25. Kock, N. (2004). The psychobiological model:
Graetz, K.A., Boyle, E.S., Kimble, C.E., Thomp- Towards a new theory of computer-mediated
son, P., & Garloch, J.L. (1998). Information communication based on Darwinian evolu-
sharing in face-to-face, teleconferencing, tion. Organization Science, 15(3), 327–348.
and electronic chat groups. Small Group Kock, N. (2005). Compensatory adaptation to
Research, 29(6), 714–743. media obstacles: An experimental study of
Griskevicius, V., Tybur, J.M., Sundie, J.M., Cial- process redesign dyads. Information Resources
dini, R.B., Miller, G.F., & Kenrick, D.T. (2007). Management Journal, 18(2), 41–67.
Blatant benevolence and conspicuous con- Kock, N. (2009). Information systems theorizing
sumption: When romantic motives elicit based on evolutionary psychology: An inter-
costly displays. Journal of Personality and disciplinary review and theory integration
Social Psychology, 93(1), 85–102. framework. MIS Quarterly, 33(2), 395–418.
Hamilton, W.D., & Zuk, M. (1982). Heritable Kock, N. (2011). A mathematical analysis of the
true fitness and bright birds: A role for para- evolution of human mate choice traits: Impli-
sites? Science, 218(4570), 384–387. cations for evolutionary psychologists. Journal
Hartl, D.L., & Clark, A.G. (2007). Principles of of Evolutionary Psychology, 9(3), 219–247.
population genetics. Sunderland, MA: Sin- Kock, N., & DeLuca, D. (2007). Improving busi-
auer Associates. ness processes electronically: An action
Hausken, K., & Hirshleifer, J. (2008). Truthful research study in New Zealand and the U.S.
signalling, the heritability paradox, and the Journal of Global Information Technology
Malthusian equi-marginal principle. Theo- Management, 10(3), 6–27.
retical Population Biology, 73(1), 11–23. Kock, N., & Moqbel, M. (2016). A six-stage frame-
Hemsley, B., Steel, J., Sheppard, J.J., Maland- work for evolutionary IS research using path
raki, G.A., Bryant, L., & Balandin, S. (2019). models: Conceptual development and a social
Dying for a meal: An integrative review of networking illustration. Journal of Systems
characteristics of choking incidents and rec- and Information Technology, 18(1), 64–88.
ommendations to prevent fatal and nonfatal Kock, N., Carmona, J., & Moqbel, M. (2015).
choking across populations. American Jour- Media naturalness and compensatory adap-
nal of Speech-Language Pathology, 28(3), tation: Counterintuitive effects on correct
1283–1297. rejections of deceitful contract clauses. IEEE
Henrich, J. (2004). Cultural group selection, Transactions on Professional Communication,
coevolutionary processes and large-scale 58(4), 381–395.
cooperation. Journal of Economic Behavior & Kock, N., Chatelain-Jardón, R., & Carmona, J.
Organization, 53(1), 3–35. (2008). An experimental study of simulated
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND COMMUNICATION 433

web-based threats and their impact on Miller, G.F. (2000). The mating mind: How
knowledge communication effectiveness. sexual choice shaped the evolution of human
IEEE Transactions on Professional Communi- nature. New York, NY: Doubleday.
cation, 51(2), 183–197. Miller, G.F. (2002). How did language evolve?
Kock, N., Verville, J., & Garza, V. (2007). Media In: H. Swain (Ed.), Big questions in science
naturalness and online learning: Findings sup- (pp. 79–90). London, England: Jonathan Cape.
porting both the significant- and no-­significant- Petrie, M., Halliday, T., & Sanders, C. (1991).
difference perspectives. Decision Sciences Peahens prefer peacocks with elaborate
Journal of Innovative Education, 5(2), trains. Animal Behaviour, 41(2), 323–331.
333–356. Pinker, S. (2003). Language as an adaptation to
Kokko, H., Brooks, R., McNamara, J.M., & Hou- the cognitive niche. In M. Christiansen & S.
ston, A.I. (2002). The sexual selection con- Kirby (Eds.), Language evolution: States of
tinuum. Proceedings of the Royal Society of the Art (pp. 16–37). New York, NY: Oxford
London: Biological Sciences, 269(1498), University Press.
1331–1340. Price, G.R. (1970). Selection and covariance.
Laitman, J.T. (1984). The anatomy of human Nature, 227(1), 520–521.
speech. Natural History, 20(7), 20–27. Rantanen, T.K., & Salo, J.A. (1999). Gastroeso­
Laitman, J.T., & Reidenberg, J.S. (1997). The phageal reflux disease as a cause of death:
human aerodigestive tract and gastroesopha- Analysis of fatal cases under conservative
geal reflux: An evolutionary perspective. The treatment. Scandinavian Journal of Gastro-
American Journal of Medicine, 103(5), 2S–8S. enterology, 34(3), 229–233.
Lee, H.S., & Holyoak, K.J. (2008). The role of Rice, S.H. (2004). Evolutionary theory: Mathe-
causal models in analogical inference. Jour- matical and conceptual foundations. Sun-
nal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, derland, MA: Sinauer Associates.
Memory, and Cognition, 34(5), 1111–1122. Russel, S., & Norvig, P. (2002). Artificial intelli-
Lieberman, P. (1998). Eve spoke: Human lan- gence: A modern approach. Upper Saddle
guage and human evolution. New York, NY: River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
W.W. Norton & Company. Short, J.A., Williams, E., & Christie, B. (1976).
Luger, G.F., & Stubblefield, W.A. (2008). AI algo- The social psychology of telecommunica-
rithms, data structures, and idioms in Prolog, tions. London, England: John Wiley & Sons.
Lisp, and Java for artificial intelligence: Struc- Simon, A.F. (2006). Computer-mediated com-
tures and strategies for complex problem munication: Task performance and satisfac-
solving. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. tion. Journal of Social Psychology, 146(3),
Manipady, S., Menezes, R.G., & Bastia, B.K. 349–379.
(2006). Death by attack from a wild boar. Trivers, R. (2002). Natural selection and social
Journal of Clinical Forensic Medicine, 13(2), theory. Oxford, England: Oxford University
89–91. Press.
Maynard Smith, J. (1998). Evolutionary genet- Ulijn, J.M., Lincke, A., & Karakaya, Y. (2001).
ics. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Non-face-to-face international business
Maynard Smith, J., & Harper, D. (2003). Animal communication: How is national culture
signals. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. reflected in this medium? IEEE Transactions
Mayr, E. (1976). Evolution and the diversity of on Professional Communication, 44(2),
life. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. 126–138.
McElreath, R., & Boyd, R. (2007). Mathematical Vandenplas, Y., Goyvaerts, H., Helven, R., &
models of social evolution: A guide for the Sacre, L. (1991). Gastroesophageal reflux, as
perplexed. Chicago, IL: The University of measured by 24-hour pH monitoring, in 509
Chicago Press. healthy infants screened for risk of sudden
McQueen, R.J., Payner, K., & Kock, N. (1999). infant death syndrome. Pediatrics, 88(4),
Contribution by participants in face-to-face 834–840.
business meetings: Implications for collabo- Wainfan, L., & Davis, P.K. (2004). Challenges in
rative technology. Journal of Systems and virtual collaboration: Videoconferencing,
Information Technology, 3(1), 15–33. audioconferencing and computer-mediated
434 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

communications. Santa Monica, CA: RAND state-dependent sexual cannibalism in a


Corporation. wolf spider. Animal Behaviour, 76(2),
Waldmann, M.R., Holyoak, K.J., & Fratianne, A. 447–454.
(1995). Causal models and the acquisition of Wilson, E.O. (2000). Sociobiology: The new
category structure. Journal of Experimental synthesis. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univer-
Psychology: General, 124(2), 181–206. sity Press.
Walker, T. (2008). Could sexual selection have Wright, S. (1934). The method of path coeffi-
made us psychological altruists? Studies in cients. The Annals of Mathematical Statis-
History and Philosophy of Science Part C: tics, 5(3), 161–215.
Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological Wright, S. (1960). Path coefficients and path
and Biomedical Sciences, 39(1), 153–162. regressions: Alternative or complementary
Warkentin, M.E., Sayeed, L., & Hightower, R. concepts? Biometrics, 16(2), 189–202.
(1997). Virtual teams versus face-to-face Zahavi, A. (1975). Mate selection – A selection
teams: An exploratory study of a web-based for a handicap. Journal of Theoretical Biol-
conferencing system. Decision Sciences, ogy, 53(1), 205–214.
28(4), 975–996. Zahavi, A., & Zahavi, A. (1997). The Handicap
Wilder, S.M., & Rypstra, A.L. (2008). Sexual size Principle: A missing piece of Darwin’s puzzle.
dimorphism mediates the occurrence of Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
22
Evolutionary Psychology and
Climate Change: Understanding
the Impact of Time Perspective
on Carbon Emissions across
75 Countries
Mark A. Caudell

INTRODUCTION 2010; Jorgenson and Clark, 2013; Murtaugh


and Schlax, 2009; O’Neill et  al., 2010; Rosa
For the last 40 years, Darwinian approaches to and Dietz, 2012; Shi, 2003; Stephenson et al.,
human behavior have been successfully applied 2010). Indeed, population growth in the next
to understanding a diverse range of behaviors half-century, even when holding current per
operating at both individual and group levels capita emissions constant, could raise global
(Buss and Shackelford, 1997; Smith, 2000; emissions by half (Swim et al., 2010).
Tooby, 2018; Tooby and Cosmides, 1988). Caudell and Quinlan (2016) sought to
Despite these successes, the leveraging of these understand anthropogenic climate change
approaches to contextualize and address global through an approach that combined elements
issues has been limited (Caudell and Quinlan, of life history theory (LHT) and evolution-
2016; but see Sörqvist and Langeborg, 2019). ary psychology. This approach was used to
This is unfortunate given that many of the understand the ‘Population Paradox’ under-
behaviors driving global issues, including lying anthropogenic climate change and to
resource consumption dynamics, fertility pat- propose potential resolutions. The paradox
terns, and cooperation levels, can be partially is founded on data showing that per capita
explained as a consequence of human evolu- carbon emissions (i.e., energy consumption)
tionary history. A global issue amenable to are positively associated with both fertil-
explanation by an evolutionary perspective is ity rate increases and decreases (Shi, 2003;
anthropogenic climate change. Climate change Stephenson et al., 2010). This pattern contra-
studies have increasingly emphasized the role dicts LHT given that per capita energy con-
of demographic patterns, including fertility sumption is generally negatively associated
rates, age structure, and household size, in with fertility in mammals (Stearns, 1992).
shaping the emission of fossil fuels (Cohen, To explicitly frame this paradox within a
436 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

life history perspective, Caudell and Quinlan across 75 countries. This analysis provides a
drew upon the slow-to-fast life history con- more robust assessment of the proposed rela-
tinuum (Ellis et al., 2009). Specifically, they tionships among time orientation, fertility
argued that in high-income countries (HICs), and investment patterns, and global carbon
selection for slower life history strategies emissions. The analysis supports that role
results in high parental investment that tar- of time orientation in impacting life history
gets ‘dynastic fitness’ (Kaplan, 1996). These strategies and carbon emissions. More gener-
investment patterns result in large per capita ally, the analysis supports the application of
energy investments in skills, knowledge, and Darwinian perspectives on human behavior
other abilities that are necessary to compete to understand and address the demographic
in industrial labor markets (Burger et  al., and consumption patterns fueling anthropo-
2011). Given that industry and supporting genic climate change.
infrastructure (e.g., electricity, transportation)
are responsible for over 60% of carbon emis-
sions (Boden et al., 2017), these investment
patterns are likely to promote increased emis- BACKGROUND
sions. In addition, these investment patterns,
particularly in the context of HICs, are also Life History, Risk and
resource-intensive, thereby reducing Earth’s Reproduction
carrying capacity. Through limiting carrying
capacities and promoting carbon emissions In LHT, reproductive behavior is guided by
driving climate change, these slower life his- the costs and benefits of investing energy into
tory strategies also increase levels of envi- the ‘competing’ life functions of growth,
ronmental risk globally. Higher risk levels maintenance, repair, and reproduction (Roff,
in non-industrial areas (e.g., many low- and 2002; Stearns, 1992). Given that resources
middle-income countries (LMICs)) promote invested into one life function cannot be
selection for faster life history strategies that devoted to another, a set of trade-offs arise
increase fertility rates and ultimately create (Stearns, 1989). Simply, time and calories
larger per capita carbon legacies (i.e., the invested into attracting a mate are time and
expected future emissions produced by a par- calories that cannot be invested in caring for
ent and their surviving offspring; Murtaugh offspring. A fundamental trade-off in every
and Schlax, 2009). Viewed globally, these organism’s life history exists between current
two life history strategies give rise to invest- and future reproduction. That is, should energy
ment patterns in HICs that are decreasing be devoted towards producing offspring (i.e.,
Earth’s carrying capacity and fertility pat- reproductive effort) as soon as physiologically
terns in LMICs that increase the rate at which viable? Or should investment in reproductive
this capacity is reached. To resolve the posi- effort be delayed in favor of somatic invest-
tive association between carbon emissions ment, thereby increasing the quality of the
and slow and fast life histories, Caudell and organism to provide parental care?
Quinlan (2016) propose that the evolved psy- The trade-off between current and future
chological construct of time orientation can reproduction underlies the continuum between
motivate decreased fertility rates and be co- slow and fast life history strategies (Stearns,
opted to promote pro-environmental behav- 1992). Slower life history strategies delay
ior through investment in education, thus reproduction thereby reducing the quantity
resulting in reductions in carbon emissions. of potential offspring, but allow the organ-
In this chapter, I expand upon the origi- ism to allocate more resources to growth,
nal study by Caudell and Quinlan (2016) to maintenance, and repair (i.e., somatic effort).
include data from around 100,000 women Extended investment in somatic effort should
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND CLIMATE CHANGE 437

lengthen life expectancy and, through enabling In contrast, mortality from extrinsic risk
increased parental investment, result in higher- should occur independent of investment in
quality offspring. Higher-quality offspring are reproduction (mating, parental investment)
important in organisms with slower life his- as it results from largely unpredictable
tories given competition for resources. This sources (e.g., predation). As unpredictable,
competition emerges as slower life histories an organism is unable to change the survival
are found in stable and predictable environ- probabilities associated with extrinsic risks,
ments that allow species to trend towards the both for itself and its offspring, through
carrying capacity of a particular niche, thereby increased investment. A high extrinsic risk
intensifying competition for resources. Given environment favors faster strategies to ‘beat
this selection, species with slower life histo- the odds’ that a potential parent dies before
ries are often larger, have fewer offspring but reproduction or leaves no offspring, given
extended parental investment periods, and high juvenile mortality rates (Quinlan, 2007;
longer life expectancies (Stearns, 1989). Roff, 2002; Stearns, 1992). Selection for all
Fast life history strategies are more life history strategies, both across and within
likely to emerge in unstable and unpredict- species, depends largely on levels of extrinsic
able environments that, in turn, are typically risk in the local environment (Charlesworth,
less crowded. This unpredictability makes 1994; Quinlan, 2007; Stearns, 1989; Williams,
it unlikely that adaptations will evolve to 1957). Support for a relationship between
enhance the competitive abilities of organ- extrinsic risk and life history strategies has
isms, given that changing environments may been found across a diverse range of human
render these abilities moot in the future. These societies (Bulled and Sosis, 2010; Caudell and
unpredictable environments also decrease the Quinlan, 2012; Gant et al., 2009; Low et al.,
probability of surviving to adulthood. The 2008; Nettle, 2010; Walker et al., 2006; Wilson
consequences of these environments, in the and Daly, 1997), although other economic and
logic of life history, mean there is little fitness cultural considerations clearly impact fertil-
advantage in investing heavily in oneself or ity decisions as well (Caudell, 2015), while
one’s offspring, as own and offspring survi- the rapid pace of extrinsic risk changes (e.g.,
vorship is low. Instead, the fitness advantage infant mortality interventions in LMICs (Liu
lies in earlier and more frequent investment et  al., 2016)) could mask the relationship
in mating effort, which increases the quan- between extrinsic risk and fertility.
tity of offspring but decreases the quality of
offspring. This investment pattern, combined
with unpredictable environments, curtails Extrinsic Risk and Evolutionary
life expectancy of both parents and offspring Psychology
(Low, 1978; Promislow and Harvey, 1990;
Roff, 2002; Stearns, 1992; Trivers, 1972). The phenotypic effects of extrinsic risk
As stated above, the continuum of slow- should be apparent in a suite of evolved psy-
to-fast life histories is founded upon envi- chological, and within humans, cultural,
ronmental unpredictability. Within LHT, this traits that motivate behavior towards invest-
unpredictability is framed as environment ment in slow or fast life history strategies
risk, which is further divided into intrin- (Hill et  al., 1997; Kruger et  al., 2008;
sic risk and extrinsic risk. Intrinsic risk is Quinlan, 2010). For example, mate-choice
mortality risk that can be impacted through decisions are impacted by predation risk
investing in oneself or one’s offspring. When within the local environment (Jennions and
intrinsic risk dominates, selection for slower Petrie, 1997). Time orientation is another
strategies is strong as increased somatic aspect of psychology, particularly human
investment can produce a fitness advantage. psychology, that may function as a
438 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

mechanism that responds to risk during an slower or faster strategies be considered ben-
organism’s development (Hill et  al., 1997; eficial in the reduction of carbon emissions?
Kruger et  al., 2008). Time orientation, as a Here, we are confronted with the ‘Population
psychological dimension, is the degree to Paradox’, that carbon emissions are posi-
which the past, present, or future guides atti- tively associated with fertility rate increases
tudes and behaviors (Zimbardo and Boyd, and decreases (Shi, 2003; Stephenson et al.,
1999). Present-oriented individuals tend to 2010). In a life history framework, then, both
be influenced by the sensory qualities of the slow and fast life history strategies are asso-
current environment, seek immediate gratifi- ciated with increased emissions. However,
cation, and are more likely to exhibit risky the route through which this promotion
attitudes and behaviors (e.g., drugs, unpro- occurs differs and can be framed, as with life
tected sex) (Apostolidis et  al., 2006; Boyd history relationships, as a trade-off between
and Zimbardo, 2005; Caldwell et  al., 2006; current and future emissions.
Henson et  al., 2006; Kruger et  al., 2008; Slower life history strategies result in
Rothspan and Read, 1996; Schechter and larger and longer energy investments per
Francis, 2010). Future orientation allows one individual, both for parents and offspring, and
to ‘transcend’ stimuli in the current environ- so higher per capita carbon emissions in the
ment and delay gratification, particularly in present. For example, competition for mates
the service of future goals (e.g., education) in many cultures results in ‘conspicuous con-
(Horstmanshof and Zimitat, 2007; Peetsma, sumption’ by males (i.e., large investments in
2000; Zimbardo and Boyd, 1999). extrasomatic resources such as houses, cars,
Couched within a theoretical framework livestock, etc.), which is positively related
combining LHT and evolutionary psychology, to carbon emissions. In addition, the longer
present orientations may be adaptive in envi- lifespans and later ages at first marriage
ronments with high extrinsic risk given that characterizing slower strategies also result in
future investments, such as education, may smaller average household sizes, including
not increase fitness due to decreased survivor- more single adult households, which further
ship to adulthood. Future orientations may be increases per capita emissions (Jorgenson
adaptive in low extrinsic risk environments and Clark, 2013; O’Neill et al., 2012).
where future investments, such as education, Faster strategies result in lower per capita
may increase competitive advantage, thereby emissions in the present due to higher extrin-
increasing fitness. In the context of life his- sic risk, which decreases competition pres-
tory strategies, present orientations should sures. Reductions in competition pressures,
be associated with traits of a faster strategy, along with high extrinsic risks, mean that the
including limited somatic/offspring invest- benefits of somatic and extrasomatic invest-
ment, higher total fertility rates, and decreased ments in oneself and one’s offspring may not
lifespans, whereas future orientation should be be realized. Importantly, however, the larger
associated with slower strategies and traits that quantity of potential offspring increases an
include later onset of reproduction, fewer off- individual’s carbon legacy, again the expected
spring, and longer lifespans. future emissions produced by a parent and
their surviving offspring. Carbon legacies
have a much greater potential to impact emis-
LHT, Evolutionary Psychology, sions when compared to emission changes
associated with changes in current investment
and Climate Change
patterns. In the United States, for example,
But what can these various evolved strategies every child contributes 9,441 metric tons to
tell us about the factors promoting climate their mother’s carbon legacy while a mother
change in the present? Indeed, should either who recycled across her lifetime would save
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND CLIMATE CHANGE 439

about 17 tons (Murtaugh and Schlax, 2009). argument is correct, a positive correlation
Whether selection for slow-to-fast life history should exist between CO2 emissions and
strategies results in greater emissions, both in future orientation.
the present and future, is thus a function of
per capita emissions, population size, and the
rates at which these two factors rise or fall LHT and Evolutionary Psychology:
(Crowley, 2000; Dietz and Rosa, 1997). Resolving the Population Paradox
On the spectrum of human life history
variation, populations that comparatively The observation that population increases,
trend towards exhibiting slower strategies and decreases, elevate carbon emissions
have substantially larger carbon legacies complicates the development of solutions to
than those countries exhibiting faster strate- anthropogenic climate change. Indeed,
gies. This result is largely due to the fact that efforts to decrease extrinsic risk levels in
slower strategies are more common in HICs – LMICs would, theoretically, reduce the
those that emit the most carbon – whereas demographic contributions towards emis-
faster strategies are more common in LMICs, sions by promoting investment in slower
with lower overall carbon emissions (Boden strategies. Reductions in demographically
et al., 2017). Disparities in carbon emissions linked emissions, however, would likely be
between HICs and LMICs are so consider- offset by subsequent increases in per capita
able that they negate the potentially greater investments in potential parents and off-
carbon legacies associated with high-fertility spring. Critically, these consequences ensure
strategies. A woman in Bangladesh, for that fertility reductions by themselves will
example, would need to have 74 children not be sufficient in decreasing carbon emis-
before her carbon legacy equaled that of an sions (see discussion in Cohen, 2010).
American woman with one child (Murtaugh In light of the observations above, reduc-
and Schlax, 2009). tions in emissions will come through chan-
As argued above, carbon emission dis- neling somatic and parental investments
parities between HICs and LMICs can be towards pro-environmental investment. To this
viewed as byproducts of low extrinsic risk end, I argue that the life history component of
environments, which increase competition future orientation can be co-opted to produce
and allow for the benefits of larger invest- pro-environmental investment patterns. This
ments to be accrued in the future (Kaplan, co-opting can occur when somatic investment
1996). Conceptualized historically, these in potential parents and parental investment
low-risk environments permitted the emer- in offspring is directed towards investment in
gence and development of industrializa- education, particularly higher education. As
tion, which increased the need for sustained discussed further below, this argument rests on
parental investment through emphasizing evidence indicating that education promotes
education and other extrasomatic resources. future orientation and that future orientations,
Concurrently, these increases in industri- in some contexts, promote pro-environmental
alization also decreased environment risk, behaviors.
through advances in healthcare, sanitation, Education levels are positively corre-
and technology (Bar and Leukhina, 2010; lated with future orientation, or related con-
Boserup et al., 1983; Boucekkine et al., 2007; structs (e.g., delay discounting, patience,
Caldwell, 1976; Coale, 1984; Galor and risk-taking), across cultures, development
Weil, 2000). As the fitness benefits of more contexts, and subsistence types, including
future-oriented slower strategies increased, populations in the United States (Alan and
including future orientation, so too did the Ertac, 2014; Jaroni et al., 2004; Oreopoulos
per capita emissions of individuals. If this and Salvanes, 2011; Van der Pol, 2011),
440 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Denmark (Harrison et  al., 2002), Turkey levels (Franson and Garling, 1999; Johnson
(Alan and Ertac, 2014), Mexico (Perez-Arce, et  al., 2004; Olli et  al., 2001; Wall, 1995;
2011), Vietnam (Anderson et al., 2004), rural but see Wesely Schultz, 2001). Considering
populations in India (Bauer and Chytilová, the research above, the prediction can be
2013), and Uganda (Bauer and Chytilová, extended that an individual’s and country’s
2010), and in small-scale societies in Bolivia investment in higher education will mediate
(Kirby et  al., 2002). Longitudinal research the effect of future orientation on per capita
(Alan and Ertac, 2014; Perez-Arce, 2011) emissions through impacting both demo-
and studies using exogenous sources of vari- graphic and consumption patterns.
ation in education to infer causation (e.g., dif-
ferential access to education across villages,
changes in compulsory-schooling laws) have Predictions
shown that education can promote increases
in future time orientation, delay discounting, The analysis that follows assesses the following
and lower the propensity towards risk-taking predictions:
(Bauer and Chytilová, 2010; Lochner and
Moretti, 2004). Further, education promotes P1: Present orientations should be associated with
future time orientation across a range of risk traits of faster life history strategies whereas
future orientations should be associated with
contexts, as demonstrated in studies finding
slower strategies.
that education has the largest impact on time P2: A positive correlation should exist between a
orientation in disadvantaged youth, a finding country’s CO2 emissions and future orientation.
consistent with recent results suggesting that P3: Investment in higher education will mediate the
education has the largest impact on fertil- effect of future orientation on per capita emis-
ity within high-risk environments (Caudell, sions through impacting both demographic and
2015). consumption patterns.
Although education can produce a more
future-oriented psychology, future orientation
also is associated with pro-environmental atti- Methods
tudes and behaviors, both across individuals
and cultures (Arnocky et al., 2012; Carmi and Predications were tested through multilevel
Arnon, 2014; Joireman et al., 2004; Lindsay analysis of data provided by 95,752 women
and Strathman, 1997; Milfont and Gouveia, from 75 countries (Model 1) and linear
2006; Toepoel, 2010), although others have regression analysis of national-level data
not found a link (Ebreo and Vining, 2001). from these same 75 countries (Model 2).
Future orientation is an essential aspect of Model specifications and data sources are
sustainable and pro-environmental behav- discussed below. Stata 16 was used for analy-
ior as it emphasizes changing one’s behav- sis (StataCorp, 2019).
ior in the present to take responsibility for
future outcomes (Carmi and Arnon, 2014). Model 1: Measures and data
Important for the current argument, how- sources
ever, is that the association between future Total fertility rates were used as proxies to
orientation and pro-environmental behavior reflect the slow-to-fast LHT continuum.
is largely confined to highly educated indi- Rates were collected for women 55 years and
viduals, particularly those with a college older, who have largely completed fertility.
education. This proposed link is consist- Extrinsic risk levels were represented by
ent with results showing that environmental taking the average life expectancy at birth
concern and behavior are related to educa- (LEB) in the 10 years after a person was
tion level, but only at higher post-secondary born. Education level (EDU) was recoded
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND CLIMATE CHANGE 441

Table 22.1  Descriptive statistics for Model 1


Variable Mean Std dev. Min Max

Total fertility rate (TFR) 3.75 2.85 0.00 20.00


Education level (EDU) 1.25 1.00 0.00 3.00
Future orientation (FO) 45.54 24.88 0.00 100.00
Life expectancy at birth (LEB) 64.67 10.98 27.08 82.12

N = 95,752. Std dev. = standard deviation. Education level is coded as 0 = None, 1 = Primary, 2 = Secondary, 3 = Post-secondary.

into none, primary, secondary, and post-­ Data for Model 1 were collected from the
secondary. A country’s time orientation was World Values Survey (www.worldvalues-
quantified by Hoftstede’s (2001) Long-Term survey.org/; WVS, n.d.), European Values
Short-term Orientation measure. Long-Term Survey (www.europeanvaluesstudy.eu/; EVS,
Orientation represents the ‘fostering of virtues n.d.), and surveys available at the Integrated
oriented towards future rewards’, whereas Public Use Microdata Series-International
‘Short Term Orientation stands for the foster- from the Minnesota Population Center,
ing of virtues related to the past and present’ including the Demographic Health Surveys
(Hofstede, 2001: 359). Given the scale (www.ipums.org/; IPUMS-DHS, n.d.). Data
anchors, 0 (most present) to 100 (most future), were collected from years closest to the date
it will be referred to as future orientation. The the future orientation was measured in the
measure has been a source of some debate country. Not every country had data from the
(see Ashkanasy et al., 2004; Fang, 2003) but year the future orientation measure was col-
scores have been validated through factor lected but all data used in the analysis were
analysis of time orientation items in the World, plus or minus five years from this date. As
African, and European Values Surveys stated above, education level (EDU) was
(Minkov, 2007; Minkov and Hofstede, 2011). coded into four categories: ‘none’, ‘primary’,
In addition, studies have found that score vari- ‘secondary’, and ‘post-secondary’. Some
ations are associated with behaviors impacted surveys recorded an individual’s education in
by time orientation, including sales of life years, and these were recoded using the fol-
insurance, across cultures (Park and Lemaire, lowing criteria: 0–2 years = ‘none’, 3–5 years
2011). Future orientation scores were calcu- = ‘primary’, 6–12 years = ‘secondary’, 12+
lated for 75 countries to reflect a geographi- years = ‘post-secondary’. Life expectancy at
cally and economically diverse sample (e.g., birth (LEB) is defined as the number of years
different continents, high- versus low-income of life remaining at birth. Average LEB in
countries). See Table 22.1 for descriptive sta- the 10 years after an individual’s birth was
tistics and Table 22.2 for Pearson correlation calculated from data collected at the World
coefficients between Model 1 variables. Bank (http://data.worldbank.org/) (World

Table 22.2  Variable correlations for Model 1


Variable Total fertility Education Future orientation Life expectancy

Total fertility -
Education −0.48** -
Future orientation −0.36** 0.34* -
Life expectancy −0.39** 0.53** 0.42** -
**
p<.01. *p<.05
N = 95,752. Education level is coded as 0 = None, 1 = Primary, 2 = Secondary, 3 = Post-secondary.
442 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Bank, 2009). For Hofstede’s time-orientation partitioning variance in total fertility rate
measure, 72 of the 75 countries had specific (TFR) into variance existing at the within-
future orientation scores whereas three coun- and between-country levels. The ICC was .38
tries were assigned regional scores. These indicating that 38% of the variance in indi-
countries were Ethiopia and Kenya, which vidual TFRs is associated with countries (i.e.,
were assigned to the East Africa region, and between-group), whereas 62% is associated
Sierra Leone, which was assigned to the West with individuals (i.e., within-group). The
Africa region. Table 22.3 presents the coun- ICC can further be interpreted as the extent
tries examined in the analysis, the associated to which individuals in the same country are
future orientation scores, databases from more alike compared with individuals of a
which the fertility and education data were different country (Peugh, 2010). In this case,
taken, and associated years of collection. the TFRs within the same country are 38%
more similar relative to TFRs within a differ-
Model 1: Specification ent country. Given these ICCs, there is strong
Across the 75 countries in Model 1, the evidence that total TFRs are not independent
number of women ranged from 800 to 1,575 within countries and so a multilevel approach
per country, with an average cluster size of is justified.
≈910 women. A multilevel modeling Model specification proceeded by adding
approach was appropriate because fertility level-one predictors (i.e., EDU, LEB) as fixed
behavior within the same country is more effects. Fixed effects ensure the regression
likely to be correlated than fertility behavior coefficient of TFR on the level-one predictors
between countries due to shared sociocul- could not vary across countries. Next, random
tural, biological, and physical environments. effects were specified to allow the regression
As such, within-country observations on fer- coefficient of TFR on level-one predictors
tility lack independence. To compensate for to vary across countries. The specification
this non-independence, multilevel models of random effects is necessary as the effect
partition the variance in an outcome measure of education and LEB on TFR is hypoth-
into variance accounted for by within- and esized to vary across countries, in part due
between-group levels. This partitioning also to interactions within a culture’s future ori-
helps to prevent study conclusions marred by entation. In these models, education and LEB
the ecological fallacy, which occurs when were group-mean centered (national educa-
inferences about behaviors of individuals are tion level and average LEB 10 years after
based solely upon aggregate measures of the birth), the suggested centering technique to
group to which those individuals belong ensure correct interpretation of the level-one
(Hox, 2010). Using aggregate data measures effects in multilevel models (Peugh, 2010).
(e.g., national LEB) to infer information Following the inclusion of level-one predic-
about behaviors at the individual level can be tors, the level-two predictor of future orienta-
a serious problem in the cross-cultural study tion (FO) was regressed on the random slope
of life history as the majority of studies as predicted by the regression of TFR on
(Bulled and Sosis, 2010; Caudell and education and LEB. Importantly, regression
Quinlan, 2012; Low et  al., 2008) rely on of FO on the random slope allowed examina-
these aggregate measures. tion of whether FO had a significant effect on
To justify the use of a multilevel mod- the relationship between education and TFR
eling approach, a random intercept model and LEB and TFR. Finally, cross-level inter-
(sometimes referred to as ‘baseline’, ‘null’, action effects were specified between FO and
or ‘unconditional’ model) was initially EDU and LEB. As a level-two effect, FO was
specified to calculate the intraclass correla- grand-mean centered using the global mean
tion (ICC). The ICC is calculated through of FO.
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND CLIMATE CHANGE 443

Table 22.3  Data sources, years, and future orientation scores


Country Year(s) Database FO Country Year(s) Database FO

Albania 2008 IPUMS 61 Kyrgyzstan 1999 IPUMS 66


Algeria 2002 WVS 26 Latvia 1996 WVS 69
Argentina 2001 IPUMS 20 Lithuania 1997 WVS 82
Armenia 2005 IPUMS 61 Luxembourg 1999 EVS 64
Australia 2005 WVS 21 Malaysia 2006 WVS 41
Austria 2001 EVS 60 Mali 1998 IPUMS 20
Azerbaijan 2006 IPUMS 61 Malta 1999 EVS 47
Bangladesh 2007 IPUMS 47 Mexico 1996 WVS 24
Belarus 1999 WVS 81 Morocco 2003 IPUMS 14
Belgium 1999 EVS 82 Netherlands 2006 WVS 67
Bosnia and Herzegovina 1998 WVS 70 New Zealand 2004 WVS 33
Brazil 2000 WVS 44 Nigeria 2008 IPUMS 13
Bulgaria 1999 WVS 69 Pakistan 2007 IPUMS 50
Burkina Faso 2003 IPUMS 27 Peru 2000 IPUMS 25
Canada 2000 WVS 36 Philippines 2003 IPUMS 27
Chile 2002 WVS 31 Poland 2005 WVS 38
China 1990 IPUMS 87 Portugal 1999 EVS 28
Colombia 2005 IPUMS 13 Puerto Rico 1990 IPUMS 0
Croatia 1999 EVS 58 Republic of Korea 1990 WVS 100
Czech Republic 1999 WVS 70 Republic of Moldova 2005 WVS 71
Denmark 1999 EVS 35 Romania 2002 WVS 52
Dominican Republic 2007 IPUMS 13 Russian Federation 1999 WVS 81
Egypt 2005 IPUMS 7 Rwanda 2005 IPUMS 18
El Salvador 1999 WVS 20
Saudi Arabia 2003 WVS 36
Estonia 1996 WVS 82
Serbia 2006 WVS 52
Ethiopia 1997 WVS 32
Sierra Leone 2008 IPUMS 9
Finland 1996 WVS 38
Slovakia 1999 EVS 77
France 2006 WVS 63
Slovenia 2002 IPUMS 49
Georgia 2008 WVS 38
South Africa 1998 IPUMS 34
Germany 2006 WVS 83
Spain 2007 WVS 48
Ghana 2003 WVS 4
Sweden 1999 EVS 53
Greece 2001 EVS 45
Switzerland 2000 EVS 74
Hungary 2001 IPUMS 58
Taiwan 2006 WVS 93
Iceland 1999 EVS 28
Turkey 2003 IPUMS 46
India 2006 IPUMS 51
Uganda 2000 IPUMS 24
Indonesia 2007 IPUMS 62
Ukraine 2007 IPUMS 86
Iran (Islamic Republic of) 2006 WVS 14
United Kingdom 1998 WVS 51
Iraq 1997 WVS 25
United Republic of Tanzania 2004 IPUMS 34
Ireland 1999 EVS 24
United States of America 1999 WVS 26
Israel 1995 WVS 38
Venezuela 2001 IPUMS 16
Italy 2005 WVS 61
Vietnam 2002 IPUMS 57
Japan 2005 WVS 88
Jordan 2002 WVS 16 Zambia 2007 IPUMS 30
Kenya 2009 IPUMS 32 Zimbabwe 2005 IPUMS 15

EVS = European Values Survey. IPUMS = Integrated Public Use Microdata Series-International from the Minnesota Population
Center. WVS = World Values Survey. FO = future orientation score.
444 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Several steps were taken to ensure adequacy studies (Shi, 2003), total population is used
of model fit and interpretation. Year of data to represent P, GDP per capita is used to rep-
collection was entered as a control variable, resent A, and percentage of GDP from manu-
given the general decline in fertility rates facturing and percentage from services is
across time. Year did not have a significant used to represent T. Total populations are
impact on TFR, after controlling for EDU, midyear estimates. GDP per capita is gross
LEB, and FO. Models also included the domestic product in current US dollars
control variable Gross National Income per divided by midyear population. The percent-
capita (GNI per capita) (taken from World age of GDP from manufacturing is value
Bank data; http://data.worldbank.org/), given added, meaning it is ‘the net output of a
large correlations between education and sector after adding up all outputs and sub-
income that could produce a spurious effect tracting intermediate inputs. It is calculated
of education on TFR. As a country-level without making deductions for depreciation
effect, GNI per capita did not have a signifi- of fabricated assets or depletion and degrada-
cant effect on the relationship between LEB tion of natural resources’ (World Bank, n.d.).
and TFR or EDU and TFR. It is possible that Educational attainment represents the per-
entering an individual’s income may have centage enrollment in post-secondary educa-
impacted the results, although the inclusion tion across both males and females.
of wealth was not permitted given that it was Percentage enrollment in post-secondary
gathered differently across the surveys. To education (tertiary education) represents
determine whether a model provided a sig- total enrollment regardless of age. Hofstede’s
nificantly better fit (i.e., p < .05) compared to (2001) long-term short-term measure was
the previous model (e.g., fixed-effect model used to represent a country’s future orienta-
versus random-effect model), likelihood- tion. All variables were log transformed prior
ratio tests were conducted (Rabe-Hesketh to analysis, with the result that coefficients
and Skrondal, 2012). All models provided a are interpreted as percentage increases in
significantly better fit compared to the previ- carbon emissions per capita given a 1%
ously specified model. increase in the independent variable (e.g.,
population size) (Dietz and Rosa, 1997). Due
Model 2: Measures and data to differences in collection years for the time
sources orientation measure, the year in which the
Data included in country-level models were population, affluence, technology, and impact
secured to test the I=P.A.T equation. The variables were collected was matched to the
I=P.A.T equation treats environmental impact year the future orientation measure was col-
(I), here carbon emissions per capita, as due lected, if available. Model 2 variables were
to three factors: population (P), affluence (A), taken from the World Bank (http://data.
and technology (T) (Dietz and Rosa, 1997; worldbank.org/). Table 22.4 provides descrip-
Ehrlich and Holdren, 1971). Environmental tive statistics for model variables and Table
impact is an outcome variable representing 22.5 the correlations between these
carbon emissions. Carbon emissions per variables.
capita are expressed as metric tons and are
emissions from the burning of fossil fuels Model 2: Specification
and the manufacture of cement and include Ordinal least squares (OLS) regression was
CO2 produced during consumption of solid, used to assess how indicators of population,
liquid, and gas fuel, and gas flaring (i.e., affluence, and technology interacted with
controlled burning of natural gas during future orientation to impact carbon emission
exploration, production, and processing) per capita (Dietz and Rosa, 1997; Ehrlich
(World Bank, 2016). Consistent with other and Holdren, 1971). Regression diagnostics
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND CLIMATE CHANGE 445

Table 22.4  Descriptive statistics for Model 2


Variable Mean Std dev. Min Max

CO2 emissions per capita 5.240 4.613 0.065 20.208


Future orientation 46.434 23.680 7.000 100.000
Post-sec. education 33.531 21.451 0.676 82.439
GDP per capita 12880.650 16526.710 136.630 73270.630
Total population 66300000 188000000 281205 1260000000
% GDP from manufacturing 17.619 6.863 3.549 40.021
% GDP from service sector 56.560 12.996 13.250 80.982

Source: First published in Caudell and Quinlan (2016).


N = 75. Std dev. = standard deviation.

were assessed for the final models. Influential fitted (predicted) plots (Cohen et  al., 2003).
data points were examined through calculat- There was evidence of minor heteroscedas-
ing Cook’s D and rerunning models with ticity in the model so a Huber–White sand-
observations associated with values exceed- wich estimator was used to provide robust
ing 4/N (i.e., 4/75=0.05). Excluding observa- estimates (Huber, 1967).
tions above this threshold did not impact
model interpretation. Multicollinearity was
assessed by calculating variance inflation
factors (VIFs). VIFs for variables of interest RESULTS
were less than 2, well below the recom-
mended cut-off of 5 (Neter et  al., 1996). Model 1: Time Orientation
Normality of residuals was tested with the Interacts with Fundamental Life
Shapiro–Wilk Test, kernel density, and stand- History Relationships
ardized normal probability plots. Results
from all tests indicated only slight deviations Total fertility rate was 3.71 births when LEB
from normality. The homoscedasticity and EDU were at their within-country mean
assumption (i.e., homogeneity of variance of and future orientation was at its between-
residuals) was tested using residuals versus country mean (see Table 22.6). Controlling

Table 22.5  Variable correlations for Model 2


Variable CO2 FO EDU GDP Pop GDP_Man GDP _Serv

CO2 -
FO 0.24* -
EDU 0.63** 0.33* -
GDP 0.74** 0.18 0.54** -
Population −0.08 0.16 −0.20 −0.12 -
GDP_Man 0.17 0.36* 0.36** 0.08 0.27* -
GDP_Serv 0.56** 0.19 0.58** 0.64** −0.16 0.14 -
**
p<.01. *p<.05
Source: First published in Caudell and Quinlan (2016).
CO2 = CO2 emissions per capita. FO = Future orientation. EDU = Percentage enrolled in post-secondary education.
GDP = GDP per capita. Pop = Total population. GDP_Man = Percentage GDP from manufacturing sector. GDP_Serv = Percentage
GDP from service sector. N = 75.
446 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Table 22.6  Relationship between total for the other predictors, a one-year increase
fertility rates, time orientation, mortality, in LEB was associated with a 0.09 birth
and parental investment decrease, a one-unit increase in EDU (e.g.,
Total fertility none to primary) was associated with a 0.64
Fixed effects birth decrease, and a one-unit increase in
Education (γ10)a −0.635** future orientation was associated with a 0.06
Life expectancy (γ20)b −0.082** birth decrease. Cross-level interactions
Future orientation (γ01)c −0.059** between FO and EDU and FO and LEB were
EDU x future orientation (γ11) 0.003** significant and are graphed in Figures 22.1
LEB x future orientation (γ21) 0.010** and Figures 22.2 to facilitate interpretation
Intercept (γ00) 3.708** and are discussed further below. The negative
Random effects covariance between the random slope of TFR
Residual (σ2) 4.325** on EDU and intercept variances (τ03 =
Intercept (τ00) 1.351** −0.329) indicates that higher TFRs are asso-
Slope (τ10) LEB 0.012** ciated with a weaker relationship between
Slope (τ20) EDU 0.114**
EDU and TFR. No other covariances were
Covariance (τ01) LEB:EDU 0.011
significant. Significant variance remained
Covariance (τ02) LEB:RI −0.013
around the random slopes of LEB (τ10 =
Covariance (τ03) EDU:RI −.329**
0.012) and EDU (τ10 = 0.114), random inter-
Model summary
cept (τoo = 1.35) and around the grand mean
N (Groups) 95752 (75)
of TFR (σ2 = 4.325) after controlling for
Deviance statistics −95273
EDU, LEB, and FO.
Due to the large sample size (≈100,000), there
Note: a Parameter estimates reflect full information is a high likelihood that some results are signifi-
maximum likelihood robust estimation. b Variable was
cant but do not account for considerable vari-
group-mean centered. c Variable was grand-mean
centered. ** p<.01. *p<.05 ables in TFR. To determine variance accounted
for in the model, several equations for the coef-
ficient of determination were used. Calculation
Total fertility rate

Time orientation
Education level
No education

Figure 22.1  Effect of time orientation on total fertility as a function of education


EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND CLIMATE CHANGE 447

Figure 22.2  Effect of time orientation on total fertility as a function of mortality level

of variance accounted for by the final model is Equation 3: Residual variance accounted
complicated as variance exists both within and for between countries by all covariates at
between countries. For Equations 1–3 below, between levels
a = residual variance associated with uncondi-
tional/null model; b = residual variance associ-
R
2
=
( τ 00 ) a − (τ 00 ) b
2 2
= 0.598
(τ 002 ) a
ated with final model; and c = residual variance
from random intercept random slope model
without level-two predictors. Within and across
countries, the covariates accounted for around Model 2: Time Orientation
30% of the variance in TFR, around 14% of the Is Associated with Carbon
variance within countries, and 60% of the vari- Emission Differences
ance between countries.
Equation 1: Residual variance accounted The relationship between future orientation
for by all covariates at both levels and EDU on per capita carbon emissions
after controlling for the impacts of popula-

R
2
=
( σ 2 + τ 002 ) a − (σ 2 + τ 002 ) b tion, affluence, and technology are reported
in Table 22.7. For every 1% increase in future
(σ 2 + τ 002 ) a orientation there was a 0.37% increase in
carbon emissions per capita. A 1% increase
= 0.272 in EDU was associated with a 0.52% increase
in carbon emissions per capita. A significant
Equation 2: Residual variance accounted interaction effect was documented between
for within countries by all covariates EDU and FO on carbon emissions. This

R
2
=
( σ ) a − (σ ) b
2 2
= 0.137
interaction is graphed in Figure 22.3 to facili-
tate interpretation and discussed further
(σ 2 ) a below. GDP per capita was significantly
related to per capita emissions, with a 1%
448 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Table 22.7  Effect of time orientation and manufacturing sector were not significantly
education on carbon emissions related to carbon emissions within the 75 coun-
Emissions per capita tries examined.
Future orientation 0.373*
Education 0.527**
FO x education −0.337* DISCUSSION
GDP per capita 0.562**
FO x GDP −0.038 This chapter provides a robust analysis of the
Total population 0.058 Population Paradox proposed by Caudell and
% Manufacturing of GDP −0.059 Quinlan (2016). This paradox centers around
% Service of GDP −0.947** the fact that both increases and decreases in
Constant 3.624* fertility rates promote higher carbon emis-
R2 0.89 sions. Although seemingly paradoxical, these
N 75 trends can be understood within a framework
**
p<.01. *p<.05 combining theories of human behavior
Source: First published in Caudell and Quinlan (2016). inspired by evolutionary theory, particularly
All variables were log-transformed. LHT, and evolutionary psychology. From a
life history framework, the paradox can be
understood as an outcome of selection for
increase associated with a 0.56% increase in slow versus fast life history strategies, which
emissions. A 1% increase in GDP from the hold consequences for the demographic and
service sector was associated with a 0.95% consumption dynamics driving global carbon
decrease in carbon emissions per capita. emissions. Components of slower strategies,
Population and percentage of GDP from the including low mortality and high investment
CO2 emission per capita

Time orientation
Post-secondary enrollment (%)

Figure 22.3  Effect of time orientation on carbon emissions per capita as a function of
investment in education
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND CLIMATE CHANGE 449

(education level) are negatively correlated to have about two offspring across their
with total fertility rates across 100,000 ­reproductive lifespans (Figure 22.1). In addi-
women and 75 countries but also with higher tion, a crossover interaction existed between
carbon emissions per capita in the 75 coun- total fertility and mortality rates as time ori-
tries. Likewise, components of faster strate- entations changed (Figure 22.2). Women in
gies, including higher mortality and lower present-oriented countries exposed to high
investment, are associated with increased levels of environmental risk were predicted to
fertility rates and so increased carbon emis- have around seven children whereas women
sions in the future. In light of these results, exposed to low risk levels were predicted to
the paradox can be understood as an outcome have around five children. However, there
of how evolved human-mating strategies was a greater rate of decline in predicted TFR
respond to environmental risk levels. at high mortality levels as time orientation
The models presented here provide a robust became more future oriented. This greater
examination of how evolved psychological rate of decline resulted in a crossover inter-
constructs, in this case time orientation, may action such that high-mortality environments
respond to local levels of environmental risk. predicted lower fertility than low-mortality
Indeed, a country’s future orientation was environments. This result, seemingly incon-
associated with total fertility rates after con- sistent with LHT, demonstrates the impor-
trolling for mortality and investment levels, tance of including psychological constructs
suggesting the construct operates indepen- such as time orientation in determining
dently from other life history traits. In addi- behavioral responses to risk given they are
tion, while the effect of future orientation was patterned by cultural (Gao, 2016) and eco-
seemingly small, about a 0.6 birth decrease, nomic (Guthrie et al., 2009) factors that may
these effects could have substantial conse- operate independently from the risk envi-
quences for fertility and parental investment ronment. More generally, these results sug-
decisions. Consider two equally educated gest that the promotion of slower life history
women who grew up exposed to similar strategies, through lowering of environmen-
levels of environmental risk. One of these tal risk (e.g., improving health and sanitation
individuals resides in Chile, with a future ori- systems), could reduce the future magnitude
entation score of 31, and the other in India, of anthropogenic climate change by limiting
with an orientation score of 51. Compared to population growth in LMICs.
the Indian, the Chilean would be predicted While limiting population growth repre-
to have one more child over her reproduc- sents an important step in addressing climate
tive lifespan. Summed across a country, these change in LMICs, consumption patterns in
increases in the total fertility rate would HICs show how the potential demographic
have considerable consequences for climate benefits of slower life history strategies
change, for example, through increasing car- can be offset by corresponding increases in
bon legacies per woman. per capita investments. In support, Model
The analysis further highlights how 2 showed that future orientation was posi-
evolved cognitive constructs can interact with tively associated with carbon emissions in
environmental risk and parental investment 75 countries after controlling for the effects
to transform fertility patterns, sometimes in of population, affluence (consumption), and
unpredicted directions. For example, non- technology (Table 22.7). Importantly, the
educated women in present-oriented coun- model also provided support for the argu-
tries were predicted to have eight offspring ment that higher education may allow future
across their reproductive lifespans, whereas time orientation to be somewhat decoupled
in future-oriented countries, women with from its life history context and co-opted to
the same level of education were predicted support pro-environmental behavior. Indeed,
450 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

as enrollment in post-secondary education those that may promote pro-­ environmental


increased across the 75 countries, the effect investment patterns (for similar arguments,
of future orientation on per capita CO2 emis- see Milfont and Gouveia, 2006; Swim et al.,
sions decreased and was decoupled from car- 2010).
bon emissions in countries with the highest
enrollment levels (Figure 22.3). These results
suggest that education can help offset the
potential CO2 emissions from the increased STUDY LIMITATIONS AND FUTURE
parental investment levels promoted by future STUDIES
orientation. However, given the limited num-
ber of countries with time orientation data, Several aspects of the data used in this study
more cross-cultural data on time orientation may impact the applicability of results,
is needed to determine the global prevalence although the models do suggest how results
of this relationship. can be expanded through future studies.
Determining whether future orientations can First, demographic data come from women
be leveraged to promote pro-environmental only and so the relationships between time
behavior in HICs will be important for address- orientation, environmental risk, and life his-
ing climate change globally. First, HICs will tory variables may not be appropriately gen-
continue to have the largest effect on Earth’s eralizable to men. However, studies of time
carrying capacity due to resource consumption orientation suggest that the construct may
patterns within these countries. Second, emis- have greater impacts on the behavior of men,
sions from HICs will continue to comprise the especially ‘life history relevant’ behaviors
vast majority of global carbon output and so it such as risk taking (Zimbardo and Boyd,
is these countries that are, and will be, respon- 1999). Second, the Hofstede time orientation
sible for increases in global risk levels (Cohen, measures were collected once, meaning that
2010; Stephenson et al., 2010). From an LHT the models assumed that the time orientation
framework, these increases in risk would result measure did not vary. For example, if the
in selection for faster life history strategies in time orientation measure was collected in
areas where individuals are unable to change 2002, then the model assumed the measure
the morbidity/mortality associated with these was the same across a person’s development.
risks through increased investment. Many This assumption was also implicit in our
of these individuals will live in LMICs. As proxies of the risk environment (i.e., LEB),
such, in LMICs many of the risks predicted to where we assumed fertility responses were
result from climate change, including extreme patterned by early experiences with risk. In
weather events and civil unrest (Cohen, 2010; fact, data on childhood mortality patterns
Stephenson et  al., 2010), should be consid- (Liu et al., 2016) and the impact of the HIV/
ered extrinsic. These increasing levels of AIDS epidemic on fertility (Kalemli-Ozcan
extrinsic risk, combined with the tendency for and Turan, 2011) reveal that fertility prefer-
LMICs to exhibit faster strategies and present ences can respond quickly to the present risk
orientations, point to a potentially catastrophic environment. In order to consider these reac-
possibility: rapid population growth within tions more fine-grained longitudinal data are
those countries where growth has the largest needed. More broadly, research is needed to
impact on carbon emissions. To prevent this understand the pace of fertility preference
positive feedback loop between population and time orientation changes across an indi-
growth in LMICs and unsustainable invest- vidual’s life history and at a broader cultural
ment in HICs, future research must examine level. In terms of time orientation, model
the cultural, ecological, and biological fac- results suggest that time orientation is likely
tors impacting time orientations, particularly the product of numerous factors across
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND CLIMATE CHANGE 451

cultures given that orientation was associated increases and decreases – can be explained
with fertility patterns even after controlling through the consequences of selection for
for mortality and investment patterns. Third, slow-to-fast life history strategies in LHT.
the intraclass correlation in the current study While accounting for the paradox, an
revealed that 38% of the variance in individ- ­evolutionary-informed perspective also pro-
ual total fertility rates is associated with vides insight into overcoming the paradox
country-level differences. These levels of non- and limiting carbon emissions. A psychologi-
independence emphasize the many factors, cal consequence of life history selection, the
including cultural, economic, and historical, construct of time orientation, may be co-
that account for differences in fertility pat- opted to motivate pro-environmental behavior
terns within and between countries. To this in the present through investment in higher
end, a multilevel modeling approach is rec- education. Through its impacts on both fertil-
ommended for representing the interaction ity rates and investment patterns, time orien-
between an individual’s life history strategies tation represents an important intervention
and the higher-level cultural, political, and point to reduce both current and future pat-
geographic contexts in which these strategies terns of carbon emissions and the severity of
unfold. Finally, while changes in global anthropogenic climate change. Future studies
carbon emissions can be represented in a life should consider how other evolved psycho-
history framework as increases in risk, these logical constructs can be co-opted to address
emissions are driven by broader societal pressing global issues.
(e.g., switch from coal to natural gas) and
historical (advent of the combustible engine)
changes that pattern the nature of the risk
environment (e.g., intrinsic versus extrinsic REFERENCES
risk). Understanding how these shifts impact
the risk environment will provide insight into Alan, S., & Ertac, S. (2014). Good things come
future fertility and parental investment pat- to those who (are taught how to) wait:
terns and so to their potential consequences Results from a randomized educational inter-
on climate change. vention on time preference. Available at SSRN
2566405. Retrieved from http://papers.ssrn.
com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=2566405
(Accessed 23 March 2015)
Anderson, C. L., Dietz, M., Gordon, A., &
CONCLUSION Klawitter, M. (2004). Discount rates in
Vietnam. Economic Development & Cultural
Approaches that contextualize human behav- Change, 52(4), 873–887.
ior within an evolutionary perspective, includ- Apostolidis, T., Fieulaine, N., & Soulé, F. (2006).
ing evolutionary psychology and LHT, will be Future time perspective as predictor of can-
important tools in understanding the demo- nabis use: Exploring the role of substance
graphic and consumption patterns driving perception among French adolescents. Addic-
climate change. Identifying the factors under- tive Behaviors, 31(12), 2339–2343. https://
lying these patterns will assist in designing doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2006.03.008
Arnocky, S., Dupuis, D., & Stroink, M. L.
interventions to limit the magnitude of cli-
(2012). Environmental concern and fertility
mate change. The current study, building off intentions among Canadian university stu-
Caudell and Quinlan (2016), provides an dents. Population and Environment, 34(2),
example of the practicality of this approach 279–292.
through resolving the Population Paradox. Ashkanasy, N., Gupta, V., Mayfield, M. S., &
The paradox – that carbon emissions are both Trevor-Roberts, E. (2004). Future orientation.
positively associated with fertility rate In R. J. House, P. J. Hanges, J. Mansour,
452 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

P. W. Dorfman, & V. Gupta (Eds.), Culture, Caudell, M., & Quinlan, R. (2016). Life-history
leadership, and organization. The Globe Study theory and climate change: Resolving popu-
of 62 societies (pp. 282–342). Thousand Oaks, lation and parental investment paradoxes.
CA: Sage. Royal Society Open Science, 3(11). https://
Bar, M., & Leukhina, O. (2010). Demographic doi.org/10.1098/rsos.160470
transition and industrial revolution: A macro- Caldwell, R. M., Wiebe, R. P., & Cleveland, H. H.
economic investigation. Review of Economic (2006). The influence of future certainty and
Dynamics, 13(2), 424–451. https://doi.org/ contextual factors on delinquent behavior and
10.1016/j.red.2009.03.002 school adjustment among African American
Bauer, M., & Chytilová, J. (2010). The impact of adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence,
education on subjective discount rate in 35(4), 587–598. https://doi.org/10.1007/
Ugandan villages. Economic Development s10964-006-9031-z
and Cultural Change, 58(4), 643–669. Carmi, N., & Arnon, S. (2014). The role of future
Bauer, M., & Chytilová, J. (2013). Women, chil- orientation in environmental behavior: Analyz-
dren and patience: Experimental evidence ing the relationship on the individual and cul-
from Indian villages. Review of Development tural levels. Society & Natural Resources,
Economics, 17(4), 662–675. 27(12), 1304–1320. https://doi.org/10.1080/
Boden, T., Andres, R., & Marland, G. (2017). 08941920.2014.928393
Global, Regional, and National Fossil-Fuel CO2 Caudell, M. (2015). The interplay of mortality,
Emissions (1751—2014) (V. 2017) [Data set]. economics, and female empowerment in
https://doi.org/10.3334/cdiac/00001_v2017 fertility transformations: A fixed effects anal-
Boserup, E., Makhoul, N., Munn, R., Srinivasan, ysis across 40 years and 167 countries.
T., Robinson, J., & Rocha, C. (1983). Popula- Cross-Cultural Research, 49(4), 358–373.
tion and technological change: A study of https://doi.org/10.1177/1069397115591152
long-term trends. International Journal of Caudell, M., & Quinlan, R. (2016). Life-history
Health Services, 13(1), 15–31. theory and climate change: Resolving popu-
Boucekkine, R., Croix, D., & Peeters, D. (2007). lation and parental investment paradoxes.
Early literacy achievements, population density, Royal Society Open Science, 3(11). 8 https://
and the transition to modern growth. Journal doi.org/10.1098/rsos.160470
of the European Economic Association, 5(1), Caudell, M., & Quinlan, R. J. (2012). Resource
183–226. availability, mortality and fertility: A path ana-
Boyd, J. N., & Zimbardo, P. G. (2005). Time lytic approach to global life history variation.
perspective, health, and risk taking. In A. Human Biology, 84(2), 1.
Strathman & J. Joireman (Eds.), Understand- Charlesworth, B. (1994). Evolution in age-­
ing behavior in the context of time: Theory, structured populations. Cambridge: Cam-
research, and application. (pp. 85–107). bridge University Press.
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Coale, A. J. (1984). The demographic transition.
Bulled, N. L., & Sosis, R. (2010). Examining the The Pakistan Development Review, 23(4),
relationship between life expectancy, repro- 531–552.
duction, and educational attainment. Human Cohen, J. E. (2010). Population and climate
Nature, 21(3), 269–289. change. Proceedings of the American Philo-
Burger, O., DeLong, J. P., & Hamilton, M. J. sophical Society, 154(2), 158–182.
(2011). Industrial energy use and the human Cohen, P., Cohen, J., West, S., & Aiken, L.
life history. Scientific Reports, 56. https://doi. (2003). Applied multiple regression/
org/10.1038/srep00056 correlation analysis for the behavioral
Buss, D. M., & Shackelford, T. K. (1997). Human sciences. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
aggression in evolutionary psychological Associates.
perspective. Clinical Psychology Review, Crowley, T. J. (2000). Causes of climate change
17(6), 605–619. over the past 1000 years. Science, 289(5477),
Caldwell, J. C. (1976). Toward a restatement of 270–277.
demographic transition theory. Population Dietz, T., & Rosa, E. A. (1997). Effects of
and Development Review, 2(3), 321–366. population and affluence on CO2 emissions.
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND CLIMATE CHANGE 453

Proceedings of the National Academy of in Denmark: A field experiment. American


Sciences, 94(1), 175–179. Economic Review, 92(5), 1606–1617.
Ebreo, A., & Vining, J. (2001). How similar are Henson, J. M., Carey, M. P., Carey, K. B., &
recycling and waste reduction? Future orien- Maisto, S. A. (2006). Associations among
tation and reasons for reducing waste as health behaviors and time perspective in
predictors of self-reported behavior. Environ- young adults: Model testing with boot-
ment and Behavior, 33(3), 424–448. https:// strapping replication. Journal of Behavioral
doi.org/10.1177/00139160121973061 Medicine, 29(2), 127–137.
Ehrlich, P. R., & Holdren, J. P. (1971). Impact of Hill, E. M., Ross, L. T., & Low, B. S. (1997). The
population growth. Science, 171(3977), role of future unpredictability in human risk-
1212–1217. taking. Human Nature, 8(4), 287–325.
Ellis, B. J., Figueredo, A. J., Brumbach, B. H., & Hofstede, G. (2001). Culture’s consequences:
Schlomer, G. L. (2009). Fundamental dimen- Comparing values, behaviors, institutions,
sions of environmental risk. Human Nature, and organizations across nations (2nd ed.).
20(2), 204–268. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
EVS. (n.d.). European Values Study Longitudinal Horstmanshof, L., & Zimitat, C. (2007). Future
Data File 1981–2008 (EVS 1981–2008) (No. time orientation predicts academic engage-
ZA4804 Data file Version 3.0.0). Retrieved from ment among first-year university students.
GESIS Data Archive website: doi:10.4232/ British Journal of Educational Psychology,
1.12253 (Accessed 13 January 2015) 77(3), 703–718. https://doi.org/10.1348/
Fang, T. (2003). A critique of Hofstede’s fifth 000709906X160778
national culture dimension. International Hox, J. J. (2010). Multilevel analysis: Techniques
Journal of Cross Cultural Management, 3(3), and applications. New York: Routledge.
347–368. https://doi.org/10.1177/1470595 Huber, Peter J. The behavior of maximum likeli-
803003003006 hood estimates under nonstandard conditions.
Franson, N., & Garling, T. (1999). Environmental Proceedings of the Fifth Berkeley Symposium
concern: Conceptual definitions, measure- on Mathematical Statistics and Probability,
ment methods, and research findings. Journal Volume 1: Statistics, 221–233, University of
of Environmental Psychology, 19(4), 369–382. California Press, Berkeley, Calif., 1967. https://
https://doi.org/10.1006/jevp.1999.0141 projecteuclid.org/euclid.bsmsp/1200512988
Galor, O., & Weil, D. N. (2000). Population, IPUMS-DHS. (n.d.). Demographic and Health Sur-
technology, and growth: From Malthusian veys 1990–2009 [Data extract from DHS Recode
stagnation to the demographic transition and files]. Retrieved from Minnesota Population
beyond. American Economic Review, 90(4), Center and ICF International website: http://
806–828. idhsdata.org (Accessed 13 December 2014)
Gant, L., Heath, K. M., Ejikeme, G. G., Snell, Jaroni, J. L., Wright, S. M., Lerman, C., &
C., & Briar-Lawson, K. (2009). Early mother- Epstein, L. H. (2004). Relationship between
hood, high mortality, and HIV/AIDS rates in education and delay discounting in smokers.
Sub-Saharan Africa. Social Work in Public Addictive Behaviors, 29(6), 1171–1175.
Health, 24(1/2), 39–46. https://doi.org/ Jennions, M. D., & Petrie, M. (1997). Variation
10.1080/19371910802569435 in mate choice and mating preferences: A
Gao, X. (2016). Cultural differences between review of causes and consequences. Biologi-
East Asian and North American in temporal cal Reviews, 72(2), 283–327.
orientation. Review of General Psychology, Johnson, C. Y., Bowker, J. M., & Cordell, H. K.
20(1), 118–127. (2004). Ethnic variation in environmental
Guthrie, L. C., Butler, S. C., & Ward, M. M. belief and behavior: An examination of the
(2009). Time perspective and socioeconomic new ecological paradigm in a social psycho-
status: A link to socioeconomic disparities in logical context. Environment and Behavior,
health? Social Science & Medicine, 68(12), 36(2), 157–186. https://doi.org/10.1177/
2145–2151. 0013916503251478
Harrison, G. W., Lau, M. I., & Williams, M. B. Joireman, J. A., Van Lange, P. A., & Van Vugt,
(2002). Estimating individual discount rates M. (2004). Who cares about the
454 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

environmental impact of cars? Those with an Low, B. S., Hazel, A., Parker, N., & Welch, K. B.
eye toward the future. Environment and (2008). Influences on women’s reproductive
Behavior, 36(2), 187–206. https://doi.org/ lives: Unexpected ecological underpinnings.
10.1177/0013916503251476 Cross-Cultural Research, 42(3), 201–219.
Jorgenson, A. K., & Clark, B. (2013). The relation- https://doi.org/10.1177/1069397108317669
ship between national-level carbon dioxide Milfont, T. L., & Gouveia, V. V. (2006). Time
emissions and population size: An assessment perspective and values: An exploratory study
of regional and temporal variation, 1960– of their relations to environmental attitudes.
2005. PloS One, 8(2), e57107. https://doi.org/ Journal of Environmental Psychology,
10.1371/journal.pone.0057107 26(1), 72–82. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Kalemli-Ozcan, S., & Turan, B. (2011). HIV and jenvp.2006.03.001
fertility revisited. Journal of Development Minkov, M. (2007). What makes us different
Economics, 96(1), 61–65. and similar: A new interpretation of the
Kaplan, H. (1996). A theory of fertility and World Values Survey and other cross-cultural
parental investment in traditional and modern data. Sofia, Bulgaria: Klasika i Stil.
human societies. American Journal of Physical Minkov, M., & Hofstede, G. (2011). Hofstede’s
Anthropology, 101(s 23), 91–135. Fifth Dimension: New evidence from the
Kirby, K. N., Godoy, R., Reyes-García, V., Byron, World Values Survey. Journal of Cross-­
E., Apaza, L., Leonard, W., & Wilkie, D. Cultural Psychology, 43(1), 3–14. https://doi.
(2002). Correlates of delay-discount rates: org/10.1177/0022022110388567
Evidence from Tsimane’ Amerindians of the Murtaugh, P. A., & Schlax, M. G. (2009).
Bolivian rain forest. Journal of Economic Reproduction and the carbon legacies of
Psychology, 23(3), 291–316. https://doi.org/ individuals. Global Environmental Change,
10.1016/S0167-4870(02)00078-8 19(1), 14–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Kruger, D. J., Reischl, T., & Zimmerman, M. A. gloenvcha.2008.10.007
(2008). Time perspective as a mechanism for Neter, J., Kutner, M. H., Nachtsheim, C. J., &
functional developmental adaptation. Jour- Wasserman, W. (1996). Applied linear
nal of Social, Evolutionary, and Cultural Psy- statistical models (Vol. 4). Chicago, IL: Irwin
chology, 2(1), 1–22. https://doi.org/10.1037/ Chicago.
h0099336 Nettle, D. (2010). Dying young and living fast:
Lindsay, J. J., & Strathman, A. (1997). Predictors Variation in life history across English neigh-
of recycling behavior: An application of a borhoods. Behavioral Ecology, 21(2), 387.
modified health belief Model1. Journal of Olli, E., Grendstad, G., & Wollebaek, D. (2001).
Applied Social Psychology, 27(20), 1799–1823. Correlates of environmental behaviors:
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.1997. Bringing back social context. Environment
tb01626.x and Behavior, 33(2), 181–208. https://doi.
Liu, L., Oza, S., Hogan, D., Chu, Y., Perin, J., org/10.1177/0013916501332002
Zhu, J., & Black, R. E. (2016). Global, regional, O’Neill, B. C., Dalton, M., Fuchs, R., Jiang, L.,
and national causes of under-5 mortality in Pachauri, S., & Zigova, K. (2010). Global
2000–15: An updated systematic analysis demographic trends and future carbon emis-
with implications for the Sustainable sions. Proceedings of the National Academy
Development Goals. The Lancet, 388(10063), of Sciences, 107(41), 17521–17526. https://
3027–3035. doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1004581107
Lochner, L., & Moretti, E. (2004). The effect of O’Neill, B. C., Liddle, B., Jiang, L., Smith, K. R.,
education on crime: Evidence from prison Pachauri, S., Dalton, M., & Fuchs, R. (2012).
inmates, arrests, and self-reports. American Demographic change and carbon dioxide
Economic Review, 94(1), 155–189. emissions. The Lancet, 380(9837), 157–164.
Low, B. S. (1978). Environmental uncertainty https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1004581107
and the parental strategies of marsupials and Oreopoulos, P., & Salvanes, K. G. (2011). Price-
placentals. American Naturalist, 112(983), less: The nonpecuniary benefits of schooling.
197–213. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 25(1),
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND CLIMATE CHANGE 455

159–184. https://doi.org/10.1257/jep. Smith, E. A. (2000). Three styles in the


25.1.159 evolutionary analysis of human behavior. In
Park, S., & Lemaire, J. (2011). Culture matters: L. Cronk, N. Chagnon, & W. Irons (Eds.),
Long-term orientation and the demand for Adaptation and human behavior: An
life insurance. Asia-Pacific Journal of Risk anthropological perspective (pp. 27–46).
and Insurance, 5(2), 1–21. New York, NY: Aldine De Gruyter.
Peetsma, T. T. D. (2000). Future time perspec- Sörqvist, P., & Langeborg, L. (2019). Why
tive as a predictor of school investment. people harm the environment although they
Scandinavian Journal of Educational try to treat it well: An evolutionary-cognitive
Research, 44(2), 177–192. https://doi. perspective on climate compensation.
org/10.1080/713696667 Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 348.
Perez-Arce, F. (2011). The Effect of Education StataCorp. (2019). Stata Statistical Software:
on Time Preferences. Working Paper, RAND Release 16. StataCorp LLC.
Labor and Population. Santa Monica, CA. Stearns, S. C. (1989). Trade-offs in life-history
Peugh, J. L. (2010). A practical guide to multi- evolution. Functional Ecology, 3(3), 259–
level modeling. Journal of School Psychol- 268. https://doi.org/10.2307/2389364
ogy, 48(1), 85–112. Stearns, S. C. (1992). The evolution of life
Promislow, D. E. L., & Harvey, P. H. (1990). histories. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Living fast and dying young: A comparative Stephenson, J., Newman, K., & Mayhew, S.
analysis of life-history variation among mam- (2010). Population dynamics and climate
mals. Journal of Zoology, 220(3), 417–437. change: What are the links? Journal of Public
Quinlan, R. J. (2007). Human parental effort and Health, 32(2), 150–156. https://doi.org/
environmental risk. Proceedings. Biological Sci- 10.1093/pubmed/fdq038
ences / The Royal Society, 274(1606), 121–125. Swim, J., Clayton, S., Doherty, T., Gifford, R.,
https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2006.3690 Howard, G., Reser, J., & Weber, E. (2010).
Quinlan, R. J. (2010). Extrinsic mortality effects Psychology and global climate change:
on reproductive strategies in a Caribbean Addressing a multifaceted problem and set
community. Human Nature, 21(2), 124–139. of challenges. Washington DC: American
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-010-9085-1 Psychological Association.
Rabe-Hesketh, S., & Skrondal, A. (2012). Multi- Toepoel, V. (2010). Is consideration of future
level and longitudinal modeling using Stata consequences a changeable construct? Per-
(3rd ed.). College Station, TX: Stata Corp. sonality and Individual Differences, 48(8),
Roff, D. A. (2002). Life history evolution. Sun- 951–956. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
derland, MA: Sinauer. paid.2010.02.029
Rosa, E. A., & Dietz, T. (2012). Human drivers Tooby, J., & Pines, D. (1985). The emergence of
of national greenhouse-gas emissions. evolutionary psychology. In Emerging syn-
Nature Climate Change, 2(8), 581–586. theses in science (pp. 67–76). Santa Fe, NM:
Rothspan, S., & Read, S. J. (1996). Present Santa Fe Institute.
versus future time perspective and HIV risk Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1988). The evolution
among heterosexual college students. Health of war and its cognitive foundations. Insti-
Psychology, 15(2), 131. tute for Evolutionary Studies Technical
Schechter, D. E., & Francis, C. M. (2010). A life Report, 88(1), 1–15.
history approach to understanding youth time Trivers, R. (1972). Parental investment and
preference. Human Nature, 21(2), 140–164. sexual selection. In D. T. Campbell (Ed.),
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-010-9084-2 Sexual selection and the descent of Man (pp.
Shi, A. (2003). The impact of population pres- 136–179). Chicago, IL: Aldine de Gruyter.
sure on global carbon dioxide emissions, Van der Pol, M. (2011). Health, education and
1975–1996: Evidence from pooled cross- time preference. Health Economics, 20(8),
country data. Ecological Economics, 44(1), 917–929. https://doi.org/10.1002/hec.1655
29–42. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0921- Walker, R., Gurven, M., Hill, K., Migliano, A.,
8009(02)00223-9 Chagnon, N., Souza, R. D. E., & Yamauchi,
456 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

T.  (2006). Growth rates and life histories in World Bank. (n.d.). DataBank: Metadata Glos-
twenty-two small-scale societies. American sary. Retrieved August 20, 2019, from https://
Journal of Human Biology, 311, 295–311. databank.worldbank.org/metadataglossary/
https://doi.org/10.1002/ajhb.20510 africa-development-indicators/series/NV.AGR.
Wall, G. (1995). Barriers to individual environ- TOTL.CD
mental action: The influence of attitudes and World Bank. (2009). World Development Indi-
social experiences Canadian Review of Soci- cators. Retrieved from World Bank Develop-
ology/Revue Canadienne de Sociologie, ment Indicators website: www.worldbank.
32(4), 465–489. https://doi.org/10.1111/j. org/ (Accessed 13 December 2014)
1755-618X.1995.tb00182.x World Bank. (2016). World Development Indi-
Wesely Schultz, P. (2001). The structure of envi- cators. Retrieved December 13, 2014, from
ronmental concern: Concern for self, other World Bank Development Indicators website:
people, and the biosphere. Journal of Envi- http://www.worldbank.org/
ronmental Psychology, 21(4), 327–339. WVS. (n.d.). World Values Survey 1981–2014
https://doi.org/10.1006/jevp.2001.0227 official aggregate v.v.20150418 (JDSystems
Williams, G. C. (1957). Pleiotropy, natural No. Aggregate File Producer). Retrieved from
selection, and the evolution of senescence. World Values Survey Association website:
Evolution, 11, 398–411. www.worldvaluessurvey.org (Accessed 13
Wilson, M., & Daly, M. (1997). Life expectancy, December 2014)
economic inequality, homicide, and repro- Zimbardo, P. G., & Boyd, J. N. (1999). Putting
ductive timing in Chicago neighbourhoods. time in perspective: A valid, reliable individual-
British Medical Journal, 314(7089), differences metric. Journal of Personality and
1271–1274. Social Psychology, 77(6), 1271–1288.
23
Evolutionary Psychology and
Thanatology: Responses to Death
James R. Anderson

INTRODUCTION (including mutilation), affiliation (including


tending, transporting, and caretaking), sexual
Like humans, at some point(s) during their behavior, partial or full ingestion, abandon-
lives group-living nonhuman animals (hereaf- ment, and various ways of actively dispos-
ter: ‘animals’) will probably encounter death in ing of the corpse (see Gonçalves and Biro,
members of their own and other species. The 2018; Gonçalves and Carvalho, 2019). Some
dying and dead individuals may be complete reactions probably have a long history in
strangers, acquaintances, or others with whom human evolution, whereas others are more
strong emotional bonds are shared (e.g., kin, recent and culturally restricted (Pettitt, 2011).
close friends). Evolutionary thanatology seeks Emotional states that accompany behaviors in
to understand the origins, diversity, mecha- the presence of the dead range from arousal
nisms, and functions of humans’ and other and alarm, for example, in response to per-
species’ behavioral and emotional reactions ceiving no signs of life from a corpse and
toward the dying and the dead (Anderson sensing potential danger, to feelings of frus-
et  al., 2018). Although most research in this tration, anger, sadness, and in humans and
field focuses on intra-species phenomena, other primates at least, grief. Of course, many
responses toward sick, injured, or the corpses animals kill others, and therefore ­predator–
of other species are also of interest, as they prey relationships and perceptions of and
may offer future resources (usually food), responses to predation events fall within the
signal danger, or simply evoke curiosity. scope of evolutionary thanatology. In this
Behavioral reactions to the moribund and chapter, I use the literature on death and
dead vary widely within and across spe- dying in selected nonhuman taxa to explore
cies; a non-exhaustive list includes indiffer- how thanatological behaviors might be bet-
ence, avoidance, manipulation, aggression ter understood, or at least addressed, from
458 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

the perspective of ‘Tinbergen’s four ques- mature concept of death (typically achieved
tions’. The questions will be presented more by 10–11 years of age), mapping their death-
fully in the following section, but in the way related cognitions at various ages, and
of a brief introduction, immediate causa- exploring how environmental and experien-
tion refers to external and internal cues and tial influences help to shape these processes.
motivational states that give rise to a behav- For these aims psychologists typically use
ior. Development is addressed by looking at questionnaires, interviews, or other less
sources of individual differences in behavior, direct techniques such as drawings to explore
including genetic predispositions and learn- children’s private views and emotions (see
ing experiences. The question of evolution e.g., Speece and Brent, 1984; Slaughter and
evokes comparisons among species, mapping Griffiths, 2007; Bonoti et al., 2013). Despite
behaviors that are shared, ancestral traits, and some culture-related variations in attainment
others that have evolved more recently and of the above-mentioned components, the
in particular lineages. Finally, functions can overall developmental trajectory toward a
be addressed by looking at how the behav- mature understanding of death, especially
ior might contribute to fitness. For illustra- biological aspects, appears largely universal
tion, I focus on two striking thanatological (e.g., Brent et  al., 1996; Panagiotaki et  al.,
phenomena, namely necrophoric behavior in 2015).
social insects, and postmortem transport and In addition to psychologists and other
care of dead infants by adult female monkeys researchers, practitioners who deal with
and apes. This selective review introduces the death-related events and consequences in
broad field of evolutionary thanatology and humans include medical and nursing pro-
presents examples of researchable questions fessionals, counsellors, epidemiologists,
and approaches that it generates. pathologists, economists, sociologists, and
anthropologists. One particular group of
scientists – funerary archeologists – are par-
ticularly interested in the remains of those
THANATOLOGY: HUMANS AND who died long ago, hundreds, thousands, or
OTHER SPECIES even millions of years ago. Studying these
ancient remains can unearth valuable infor-
Most human adults know that life eventually mation about phenomena such as diet, age,
culminates in death, and that this applies to likely cause of death, non-fatal trauma and
all living creatures: death befalls everyone, diseases, and damage inflicted after death
without exception. This understanding (which might include defleshing and dis-
reflects two components of the typical adult’s membering). Furthermore, analyses of the
concept of death (at least in Western, indus- immediate surrounding environment (e.g.,
trialized societies), namely inevitability and other human and nonhuman remains, arte-
universality. Two other components of the facts, signs of deliberate burial or caching),
mature Western human’s concept of death are and aspects of the corpse (e.g., orientation,
non-functionality (dead individuals no longer posture, remnants of clothing or accessories)
perceive, think, feel, or act) and causality can be used to make inferences about the
(some knowledge of biological causes of evolution of human funerary activities within
death, e.g., damage to vital organs, loss of particular social and psychological contexts
blood, lack of oxygen) (see Gire, 2014 for a (Pettitt, 2011, 2018). By giving greater con-
discussion of differences in conceptualiza- sideration to various species’ immediate and
tions of death across cultures). Developmental long-term responses to death, and to relevant
psychologists with an interest in thanatology physical and social anthropological data,
have studied how children gradually form a thanatologists (most of whom probably do
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND THANATOLOGY 459

not explicitly identify themselves as such) phylogenetically related and looking to see
can construct a clearer picture of responses whether a behavior is common across related
to this universal and momentous event in the species (and hence more likely to be an
context of evolution. ancient trait) or whether it has more recently
One distinguishing feature of thanato- appeared within one (or more) specific taxo-
logical studies of nonhuman animals is that nomic group(s). Although there has been
because they cannot use verbal or projective debate about applying and updating
techniques for collecting data, researchers Tinbergen’s four questions 50 years on, there
must focus on publicly observable behavioral is general agreement about the enduring value
responses to moribund or dead companions of the Tinbergian approach (Barrett et  al.,
or others. Observational and experimen- 2013; Bateson and Laland, 2013; Taborsky,
tal studies both contribute. The latter may 2014; Kapheim, 2019). This approach is taken
involve exposing animals to death-related below to discuss two particular examples of
cues, for example visual or chemical signs thanatological behavior, namely necrophore-
of injury or death. Some researchers also sis in social insects, and postmortem infant
measure changes in physiological status in care and transport in nonhuman primates.
relation to an animal’s experiences of death.
Examples of both kinds of approaches will be
seen in later sections.
SOCIAL INSECTS: CORPSE
MANAGEMENT

TINBERGEN’S FOUR QUESTIONS The responses of social insects to dead con-


specifics are relatively well studied, and
Tinbergen (1963) proposed that a compre- illustrate nicely the value of Tinbergen’s
hensive understanding of any behavior was questions for thanatological investigations.
more likely to emerge if researchers consid- Furthermore, it can be argued that that
ered four questions: What is causing this responses to sick, injured, dead, or dying
behavior to happen now? What is the devel- insects are easier and less ethically challeng-
opmental history of this behavior within the ing to study than in longer-lived, cognitively
individual? What is the function of this and emotionally more complex vertebrate
behavior in terms of survival value? How did species, such as fish, birds, and mammals.
this behavior evolve? The first two questions For species that congregate permanently
are often considered as addressing proximate (or semi-permanently) at a fixed site, deaths
mechanisms, for example current environ- of community members pose problems for
mental and/or internal events eliciting the the living. The enclosed home environments
behavior (e.g., what triggers it, what moti- of social insects contain often high population
vates it), and the extent to which the behavior densities and food reserves, thereby provid-
can be considered as pre-programmed (or ing favorable conditions for the proliferation
‘innate’), or influenced by learning – the of potentially harmful organisms. Decaying
ontogeny issue. The latter two questions are corpses within the nest or hive may give rise
considered to address ultimate reasons, to disease; therefore corpse management
namely how is this behavior adaptive, or strategies are important. Social insects such
what is its past survival value (and ‘current as bees, wasps, ants, and termites deal with
utility’, see Bateson and Laland, 2013), and this problem through a suite of behaviors that
how and when did it evolve? The evolution- have evolved to ensure the rapid and effective
ary issue can be addressed, for example, by disposal of the dead (for reviews, see López-
comparing species that are more or less Riquelme and Fanjul-Moles, 2013; Sun et al.,
460 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

2018): 1) Necrophoresis is the general term any objects daubed with the substance – even
for transport and disposal of corpses either live, healthy nestmates – were transported by
outside the nest or at refuse sites within the healthy ants to refuse piles. Oleic acid is one
nest. 2) Cannibalism is the consumption of of several chemical cues that alone or in
dead conspecifics. Sometimes referred to as combination can elicit so-called undertaking
‘conspecific necrophagy’ to acknowledge behavior – detection, removal, and disposal
that no active killing is involved (Brown and of dead colony members – in social insects
Norris, 2004), it is common in termites, but (López-Riquelme and Fanjul-Moles, 2013).
also occurs in some ants. 3) Burial behavior Another fatty acid product of corpses – lin-
consists of covering corpses with soil or other oleic acid – also induces necrophoric reac-
matter; again, common in termites, it has also tions in ants; in cockroaches this compound
been reported in some ants. 4) Necrophobia had an even stronger repellent effect than
is the simple avoidance of corpses or areas oleic acid (Rollo et  al., 1994). The latter
associated with corpses, and is a widespread authors proposed the term ‘necromones’ for
response to corpses encountered both within chemicals that signaled death and triggered
and outside the nest. In some species there specific responses. Sometimes removal of
is flexibility: one behavioral response can be freshly dead nestmates occurs before the
replaced by another, depending on the nature buildup of necromones is sufficient to induce
of the corpse (e.g., age, whether conspecific necrophoresis. It has been shown that two
or allospecific: Renucci et  al., 2011; Neoh chemical compounds in live ants might pre-
et al., 2012; Sun et al., 2013). vent them from being responded to as corpses
by nestmates, by masking necromones; how-
ever, these compounds fade rapidly in freshly
dead workers, triggering the latter’s removal
Proximal Causes
even before necromones take effect (Choe
What elicits corpse management behaviors in et al., 2009). Several studies have shown that
social insects? Experiments using stationary different necromones from fresh and older
models of insects or other objects, the lack of corpses may induce different behaviors; in
identifiable sounds or thermal cues from termites, for example, ‘early’ death cues trig-
corpses, and the darkness within nests or gered cannibalism whereas ‘late’ cues gave
hives mean that visual, auditory, and tactile rise to burial responses (Sun et al., 2017; see
cues can usually be ruled out. Instead, there also Neoh et  al., 2012). Early experiments
is general agreement on a predominant role also demonstrated that chemical cues trig-
for chemical cues in triggering both necro- gered necrophoresis in honey bees (Visscher,
phobic and necrophoric reactions. In fact, 1983).
corpse disposal by social insects nicely illus-
trates the prototypical ethological sequence
in which a sign stimulus (in this instance, one Development
or more chemical signals) triggers an innate
releasing mechanism (a neural network in the An interesting aspect of corpse management
brain), which in turn produces a fixed action in social insects is that workers within the
pattern (here, the behavioral sequence result- colony show varying degrees of specializa-
ing in avoidance or disposal of the corpse). In tion in ‘undertaking’ activities. In one experi-
a pioneering study, Wilson et  al. (1958) mental study using three laboratory colonies
showed that a specific substance produced by of ants containing 48, 105, and 146 individu-
decaying corpses – oleic acid – caused ally marked workers, respectively, more than
healthy ants to dispose of the corpses. Oleic 75% of workers in each colony encountered
acid proved so effective in this regard that corpses but less than 30% showed
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND THANATOLOGY 461

undertaking activities, defined as picking up in the emergence or efficiency of the behav-


and carrying a corpse for more than 3 cm. ior; indeed, efficiency did not improve with
Some workers removed dead ants more fre- greater experience of removing corpses
quently than expected by chance (Julian and (Trumbo and Robinson, 1997) (but see Arathi
Cahan, 1999). In the smallest colony usually et al., 2000, for an alternative perspective on
only one undertaker engaged with the corpse the role of experience).
on each trial, whereas in the larger colonies López-Riquelme and Fanjul-Moles (2013)
several did so. Among the latter, those that present an overview of what little is known
handled a corpse most frequently (‘special- about the genetics of undertaking specializa-
ists’) were more likely than non-specialists to tion in social insects. They point out the likely
complete removal of the corpse and, in the relationship between increasing genetic
largest colony, in the shortest time. determination and reduced behavioral plas-
The authors suggested age-related or genetic ticity (e.g., in honey bees), and between high
differences in undertaking activities, but morphological specialization and reduced
ruled out an effect of individual learning on sensitivity to death cues (e.g., in soldier ants);
efficiency of performance, at least in the they also discuss neurobiological specializa-
24–27 trials per colony, conducted over a tions in undertakers.
two-week period.
An early study of individually marked
honey bees concluded that 1–2% of the colony Function
population were specialized undertakers, and
that they retained this specialized role over As indicated above, dead individuals in the
a relatively long period of time (Visscher, nest are a potential source of disease, so dis-
1983). Bumblebee workers that removed lar- posing of the dead should be of benefit in
val corpses but not adult corpses were also increasing survival of the living. An experi-
described as behaviorally more specialized ment with red ants demonstrates this advan-
than non-undertaker workers (Munday and tage of corpse disposal (Diez et  al., 2014).
Brown, 2018); the former were also sig- Survival rates were compared between colo-
nificantly larger than those who specialized nies in which ants could freely remove
in removing adult corpses. Later work on corpses and colonies in which the size of the
honey bees identified more undertakers – at nest entrance was made smaller to impede
least 20% of workers – and revealed several normal corpse disposal activities. Freshly
behavioral differences between undertakers killed nestmates were placed in each nest and
(and guards) compared to other workers (food left over a seven-week period. Results
storers and wax workers). Genotypic differ- showed that from the eighth day after corpse
ences in response thresholds to different kinds placement, survival rates of adult workers in
of environmental stimuli appear likely to con- colonies with restricted removal possibilities
tribute to the extent of undertaking activities. were significantly reduced compared to col-
Selective breeding can result in different levels onies with normal-size nest entrances.
of nest hygienic behaviors including corpse Furthermore, ants in control nests were seen
removal (e.g., Rothenbuhler, 1964; Oldroyd to engage in typical necrophoric behaviors,
and Thompson, 2007); however, the spe- whereas those in restricted-entrance nests
cialization of undertakers does not appear as cut up corpses and ejected the pieces, or
extreme as in some insect subcastes (Robinson moved them to areas in the nest so as to
and Page, 1988; Trumbo et al., 1997). In view maximize the distance between the corpses
of the relatively short period of time in which and the larvae. Over time, larvae survival
individuals specialize in undertaking – u­ sually rates also declined in the restricted-entrance
several days – learning may play little part nests.
462 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Evolution from corpses, but also that whereas some


species avoid the source or act to get rid of
Avoiding dead conspecifics or places where it, other species are positively attracted to it;
cues to conspecific death are detected (necro- necrophagous insects are another example of
phobia), and actively disposing of corpses the latter (e.g., Frederickx et al., 2012).
(necrophoresis), is clearly advantageous. The
latter, described in social bees, wasps, and
ants (hymenopterans), as well as termites
(isopterans), has been proposed as an exam- NONHUMAN PRIMATES: CARE AND
ple of convergent evolution (Sun and Zhou, TRANSPORT OF DEAD INFANTS
2013; Sun et al., 2018).
Yao et al. (2009) proposed an ancient ori- The mother–infant relationship is perhaps the
gin of sensitivity to chemical signals result- most studied of all social relationships in
ing in avoidance of corpses and death-related nonhuman primates. The newborn infant is
sites in both insects and crustaceans, dating dependent on its mother for survival: she
back possibly to ancient unicellular organ- provides nourishment and protection against
isms. Although some progress has been made multiple dangers including accidents, adverse
with regard to molecular bases in social weather, predatory attacks, and conspecific
insects, further studies on a wider range of aggression (see Altmann, 1981; Nicolson,
species are necessary to better understand 1987, 1991; Hayashi and Matsuzawa, 2017).
the evolution from simple ancestral death- In many species the strong emotional bond
recognition responses to more complex, that develops between mother and offspring
sequentially organized corpse management persists for as long as both are alive, particu-
strategies (Sun et al., 2018). larly in philopatric species where one or both
Digressing briefly from social insects to remain near to their birthplace for most or all
crustaceans, hermit crabs nicely illustrate of their lives (van Noordwijk, 2012).
diverging evolved responses to death cues The mother–infant relationship also figures
in marine vs terrestrial environments. In this prominently in the primate thanatology liter-
case, responses concern not avoidance or ature. Many authors have described mothers
disposal, but attraction. All ancestral hermit continuing to carry and care for their infant
crabs were marine animals, and marine her- even though it is dead. The phenomenon is
mit crabs today live in unremodeled shells widespread in primates, although it varies in
that become available upon the death of other incidence and duration, with influencing fac-
marine hermit crabs or gastropods. By con- tors including species, ecology, age of infant,
trast, terrestrial hermit crabs have become cause of death of infant, individual maternal
specialized to live only in shells that have traits, and climatic conditions (e.g., Sugiyama
been physically remodeled by conspecifics. et  al., 2009; Watson and Matsuzawa, 2018;
Valdes and Laidre (2019) recently demon- Das et  al., 2019). Acronyms used for mam-
strated differences in response to chemical malian mothers’ response to their dead
cues from dead conspecifics vs heterospe- offspring include DIC (‘deceased-infant car-
cifics in marine and terrestrial hermit crabs. rying’, in Das et al., 2019 for primates), and
Whereas the latter rapidly gathered around PAB (‘postmortem attentive behavior’, in
conspecific death sites in particular – pre- Bearzi et al., 2018 for cetaceans); the former
sumably because of the possible presence emphasizes carrying and transport, the latter
of a suitable new host shell – their marine implies caregiving more generally, not just
counterparts showed no such differential toward offspring but also other conspecifics.
response. This work shows not only the pow- Because primate mothers usually both carry
erful behavioral effects of chemical cues and take care of their infant’s corpse (e.g., by
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND THANATOLOGY 463

grooming and preventing insect larvae from Another likely causal factor in PICT is that
settling in it), I refer to this constellation of the dead infant still presents morphological
behaviors as ‘postmortem-infant care and features that make it attractive to the mother
transport’ (PICT). (and possibly others). Visual features (‘baby
schema’) including infantile coloration, coat
texture, and bodily shape and facial propor-
tions (e.g., Alley, 1980; Sato et al., 2012) are
Proximate Causes
attractive to mothers and others. For some
Whereas in social insects chemical cues from individuals including the mother, the total
the dead are the primary triggers of necro- lack of animacy in the infant corpse might
phobic or necrophoric responses in the living, also increase its interest value, at least for a
the picture is more complicated in primates. limited time. The dead infant also continues
The immediate causes of PICT in primate to provide tactile stimulation – known to be
mothers are probably multiple and include crucial in mother–infant bonding (Harlow,
both internal events and external sensory 1974) – but only if the mother touches or
cues. First, the physiological condition of the holds it. Unlike in social insects, there has
mother – reflecting hormonal changes in late been no suggestion that chemical cues play
pregnancy and the periparturitional stage – any role in primate PICT. But nor do such
causes her to be strongly attracted to and cues appear to be aversive. Although human
highly motivated to transport, care for, and observers have commented on the repulsive
protect her baby (Nicolson, 1991; Kristal, smell from infant primate corpses after a few
2009). In fact, experimental studies of days, this seems to have little or no effect on
mother–infant separation and adoption have the mother’s behavior, or indeed that of other
shown that new primate mothers will accept, members of the group (Biro et al., 2010; De
hold, and nurture any infant of appropriate Marco et al., 2018, but see Sugiyama et al.,
age, in some cases even if it is another spe- 2009). In fact, there are no experimental stud-
cies (Harlow, 1974; Smith, 1986; Owren and ies on sensitivity to putrescine or other puta-
Dieter, 1989). Recently, the possibility that tive necromones in nonhuman primates.
oxytocin plays an important role in PICT was With time elapsed since death the corpse
raised by Bercovitch (2020), although he progressively decays, and in terms of visual
emphasized that this hormone promotes and tactile features resemblance to a normal,
social bonding in general, rather than being a live infant diminishes. PICT sometimes con-
specific trigger of continued maternal behav- tinues even though the corpse has mummi-
ior toward dead infants. fied and portions of the body detach (e.g.,
Despite being hormonally primed to Sugiyama et al., 2009; Biro et al., 2010; De
behave maternally, especially if she has Marco et al., 2018). This deterioration, con-
raised other infants, the mother of a dead temporaneous with the mother’s changing
infant shows awareness that this one neither hormonal condition through lack of suck-
responds to anything she does nor shows ling, eventually leads to abandonment of the
any spontaneous behavior at all. A corpse corpse. Abandonment may be gradual – the
has no grasping reflex, so to carry and trans- mother lays the corpse on the ground for
port it the mother must make compensatory increasingly longer periods – or more abrupt:
behavioral adjustments such as holding the sometimes the mother is spotted without
corpse with one hand and walking triped- her dead infant although she was carrying
ally instead of quadrupedally, or even hold- it the previous evening. There are increas-
ing it in her mouth. While feeding arboreally ing reports of prosimian, monkey, and great
she may have to keep hold of the corpse or ape mothers ingesting parts of their infant’s
secure it in her groin pocket or under an arm. corpse. This ‘maternal cannibalism’ has been
464 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

interpreted as possible abnormal behavior accumulating behavioral and physiological


due to stressful environmental conditions data, a strong argument can be made that
(Tartabini, 1991; Dellatore et  al., 2009) or mothers showing PICT are experiencing
as a normal albeit rare event (Fedurek et al., grief in at least some ways comparable to the
2019; Trapanese et  al., 2020), reflecting a human experience following the death of a
qualitative change in the mother’s perception loved one (King, 2013; Anderson, 2017; Das
of the corpse from ‘her infant’ to a poten- et al., 2019).
tially edible object (Tokuyama et  al., 2017;
De Marco et al., 2018).
When considering causes of death-related Development
reactions in highly social and intelligent mam-
mals such as nonhuman primates, emotional PICT can be seen as an extension of normal
factors should not be neglected (Anderson, maternal behavior, albeit with compensatory
2016). There is compelling behavioral and adjustments for the lifelessness of the infant
physiological evidence that recent bereave- similar to but perhaps more obvious than
ment is stressful for primates (Engh et  al., those shown by mothers of physically handi-
2006), and the strong emotional bond that capped, injured, or sick infants (for exam-
the mother experiences with her infant is ples, see Nakamichi, 1986; Anderson et  al.,
one likely cause of PICT. Disruption of the 1995; Matsumoto et al., 2016). To this extent
mother–infant bond has profound emotional PICT is a product of the same genetically
consequences in both partners. If prolonged, determined and experiential factors that
in infants it can result in depression, as indi- underlie normal maternal behavior and per-
cated by behavioral and physiological param- formance. Genetic factors include sex differ-
eters, and symptom alleviation following ences in neuro-hormonal mechanisms that
antidepressant pharmacological intervention result in females being especially strongly
(for review see Worlein, 2014). Loss-induced attracted to and interested in infants (Nicolson
depression can contribute toward an early 1991), sex differences in play and other
death, especially in free-ranging conditions. social activities before puberty (Lonsdorf,
Responses to orphaned infants by individu- 2017) and, in females, pre- and postpartum
als other than the mother range from indif- hormonal changes that are critical for mater-
ference to limited caretaking and attempted nal behavior (Saltzman and Maestripieri,
adoption, but long-term survival prospects 2011). Other potentially relevant factors
of dependent orphans are poor (Rhine et al., include maternal temperament and style, and
1980; Goodall, 1983; Thierry and Anderson, stress (Altmann, 1980; Fairbanks, 1996;
1986; Fashing et  al., 2011). Infants might Sullivan et  al., 2011; Maestripieri, 2018);
either stay nearby or continue clinging to however, there are insufficient data to allow
their dead mother, in which case survival is any conclusions about individual differences
likely to be brief. Mothers of dead infants, in these traits being related to aspects of PICT.
by contrast, may continue to express the Experiences can affect all the genetically
emotional bond with the infant for longer, predisposed traits mentioned above. Greater
through PICT. Some authors have described experience with infants comes with parity,
behaviors in PICT mothers as possible indi- which is one maternal trait that has been
cators of intense emotion, such as the dazed examined in relation to PICT. At least two
expression of bereaved chimpanzee mothers competing predictions can be made: (1) pri-
(van Lawick-Goodall, 1968), reduced feed- miparous mothers should show more PICT
ing, increased passivity, and social isolation because they are primed to look after their
and withdrawal in various species (Das et al., first infant but are inexperienced with dead
2019; Takeshita et  al., 2020). Based on the vs live infants, vs (2) multiparous mothers
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND THANATOLOGY 465

should show more PICT because with aging have diminished. Indeed, monkeys and apes
each infant represents a greater biological t­
ypically abandon dead individuals either
investment; also, multiparous mothers might precisely at or near where they died, after
have prior experience of unconscious, unani- periods of time ranging from a few minutes
mated infants recovering (see next section on to several hours (e.g., Yang et  al., 2016;
functions). In a study of 157 cases in free- Teleki, 1973), although sometimes individu-
ranging, provisioned Japanese macaques als may revisit the place of death the follow-
over a 24-year period, Sugiyama et al. (2009) ing day (Gonçalves and Carvalho, 2019). As
used maternal age as a proxy for parity, but PICT is clearly non-abandonment, the ques-
reported no significant differences between tion of function remains. One hypothesis is
younger and older mothers in the incidence or that it is simply a non-adaptive error, a by-
duration of PICT. They also noted that some product of the combination of mother’s hor-
mothers were repeat carriers of dead infants; monal condition and sensitivity to stimulus
one carried three dead infants. By contrast, features of the dead infant. Another possibil-
Das et  al. (2019) found that older mothers ity is that the mother is unaware that her
carried dead infants for longer, supporting infant is dead (Gonçalves and Carvalho,
the hypothesis of greater investment in later- 2019). However, possible benefits have also
born offspring. Whether the strength of the been proposed. One plausible hypothesis is
attachment between mother and infant influ- that PICT expresses a ‘wait and see’ strategy:
ences PICT remains to be clarified (Watson producing an offspring is so costly that it
and Matsuzawa, 2018). Observations of two might be worthwhile for a mother to continue
mothers each carrying their dead infant at the caring for it; in some cases, it might recover
same time led to the suggestion of a role for (Nicolson, 1991; Hrdy, 2000; Watson and
observational learning (Biro et al., 2010). The Matsuzawa, 2018). This is precisely what
fact that some form of PICT occurs in at least happened in a case recently described by
19 primate species (Das et al., 2019) proba- Masi (2020). An infant female western
bly indicates a stronger biological than social gorilla fell from a tree and hit the ground
basis, but a related and more easily testable after dropping at least 15 m. The infant
hypothesis is that PICT is more likely if there appeared totally lifeless to the observers, but
are other females in the group carrying and her mother carried her, laid her on the ground
caring for live infants (the ‘social facilitation’ and gently touched her. Over the following
hypothesis: Watson and Matsuzawa, 2018). hour the infant slowly regained conscious-
ness and movement, and went on to make a
full recovery.
Some authors have proposed that PICT
Function
functions as a grief-buffering mechanism that
As reviewed above, in social insects corpses helps the mother come to terms with the emo-
within the nest are disposed of, and corpses tional distress arising from her infant’s death
encountered outside the nest may be avoided. (Nicolson, 1991; King, 2013; Gonçalves and
Active disposal requires time and energy, but Carvalho, 2019). In the first study to collect
the benefits in terms of intra-colony hygiene data on both behavioral and hormonal changes
and health mean that corpse management in a female monkey (Japanese macaque), who
behaviors have been positively selected kept her dead infant for 20 days, Takeshita
during evolution. In free-ranging nonhuman et al. (2020) recorded social withdrawal and
primates, a dead group member can be signs of stress during this period, but a rapid
avoided simply by moving to another part of return of glucocorticoids to normal levels.
the home range and staying away until any The authors speculated that PICT helped
risks associated with the decaying corpse reduce stress levels related to the death of the
466 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

infant, although they pointed out the need for s­urvival value of these responses appears
larger samples and more longitudinal data. clear, preparing the animal to deal with the
potential presence of a predator or other
danger. There is little evidence of any strong
Evolution effect of visual cues in the perception of death
in fish; the issue requires more research.
PICT in one form or another has been observed Furthermore, questions about the relative
in the three major primate groups, namely pro- roles of innateness vs experience in respond-
simians, New World monkeys, and Old World ing to death cues figure rarely in studies of
monkeys and apes. This broad distribution sug- aquatic species. For animals raised from birth
gests that a basic form of PICT existed in or hatched in controlled environments in
ancestral primate species, from the evolution- which exposure to death is never experienced,
ary emergence of strong mother–infant bonds. any responses to corpses are likely to be
Extant prosimian mothers carry and transport genetically wired, although how other indi-
their dead infants less often than anthropoid viduals react may also exert an influence.
primate mothers, likely due to physical con- How birds respond to death is also impor-
straints (Gonçalves and Carvalho, 2019). tant for evolutionary thanatological research.
Although most accounts of PICT have been Although some specialist scavengers may
described in macaques, baboons, and chimpan- use odor cues to detect carcasses, others
zees, these species may be over-represented in appear to rely on visual cues (for vultures, see
the literature because (1) they are the most Grigg et  al., 2017). Research on avian reac-
studied both in captivity and the wild, and tions to dead conspecifics has emphasized
(2) they often spend much time on the ground, visual rather than chemical cues. For exam-
where it is often easier to observe them. Das ple, when Western scrub-jays saw the dried
et  al. (2019) recently reported examples of skin and feathers of a dead conspecific near a
PICT in more arboreal primate species, but feeder, they emitted more loud vocalizations
more data are required on this issue. (‘cacophonous reactions’) and took fewer nuts
than when similar-colored, non-social objects
were presented instead. Playbacks of these
calls attracted other jays to the site. A stuffed
OTHER SPECIES, AND SOME owl elicited similar cacophonous aggrega-
UNANSWERED QUESTIONS tions and mobbing responses (Iglesias et al.,
2012; see Carlson et  al., 2017, for blue tits’
This attempt to address thanatological behav- reactions to a model sparrow hawk holding a
iors from a Tinbergian perspective has drawn dead blue tit). Iglesias et al. (2014) reported
on specific sets of responses in two widely that dead scrub-jay-size birds of other species
separated taxonomic groups, namely social also elicited calling and aggregations and sup-
insects and nonhuman primates. Of course, a pressed foraging near the carcass, compared
more complete evolutionary perspective on to dead but smaller birds.
responses to death requires information from Experiments with American crows have
other kinds of animals. For example, the shown broadly similar responses as scrub-
effects of necromones in several species of jays toward dead conspecifics. Also, crows
fish have been described; responses include reacted negatively to the sight of a hawk,
avoidance of areas associated with dead con- but more intensely to a hawk (or a human)
specifics, and freezing (e.g., Bryer et  al., with a dead crow. They were less respon-
2001; Hussain et  al., 2013; Oliviera et  al., sive to the carcass of a pigeon, and pigeons
2014); the latter study also found evidence of also took little notice of dead conspecifics
increased cortisol, indicating stress. The (Swift and Marzluff, 2015, 2018). In the first
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND THANATOLOGY 467

study of brain activity in response to death compounds that accumulate in vertebrate


cues in birds, positron emission tomography corpses – but not an anesthetized rat that was
on crows revealed particular activation of not treated with a compound; (3) buried inan-
the hippocampal region of the brain as they imate objects treated with the compounds but
watched a human holding a dead crow; other not untreated control objects; (4) buried a
risk-related stimuli activated other forebrain compound-treated object or corpse only when
regions (Cross et  al., 2013). Brain imag- their sense of smell was intact; (5) more read-
ing is likely to be increasingly valuable for ily buried an object treated with putrescine or
revealing neural circuits involved in perceiv- cadaverine than other substances: water, urine,
ing cues related to death in many species as ether, ammonia, or ethanol. The same authors
well as in humans. As with fish, no studies also referred to a report of food-deprived rats
on birds have investigated the ontogeny of eating older conspecific corpses rather than
responses to carcasses, but some responses fresher ones (Carr et  al., 1979), suggesting
have been linked to plausible functions: loud an alternative corpse-induced response in
vocalizations (alerts others to danger), con- the presence of a competing and overriding
gregating near the corpse (for identification motivational state. Prounis and Shields (2013)
of the corpse, detecting possible new forag- conducted field experiments on a community
ing and/or mating opportunities), and sup- of small mammals in a forest, and found that
pressed feeding (avoids the risk of poisoning fewer seeds were eaten from food trays that
or attack by scavenging predators). also contained a mouse corpse than trays that
Studies of non-primate mammals are also contained a colored ball instead of a corpse.
important. There are descriptions of responses In a laboratory study the same authors found
to dying and dead conspecifics – sometimes longer latencies to pass a conspecific corpse
but not always concerning mothers toward than a toy mouse placed in one arm of a
their offspring – in free-ranging giraffes Y-maze, indicating stronger avoidance of the
(Bercovitch, 2012; Strauss and Muller, former. No attempt was made to separate
2012), dingoes (Appleby et al., 2013), horses olfactory and visual cues in this research.
(Mendonça et  al., 2020), African and Asian
elephants (Douglas-Hamilton et  al., 2006;
Hawley et  al., 2017–2018; Goldenberg and
Wittemyer, 2019; Sharma et  al., 2020), and CONCLUSIONS: HUMAN
various cetacean and other aquatic and semi- THANATOLOGY, AND FUTURE
aquatic species (Bearzi et al., 2018; Reggente DIRECTIONS
et  al., 2018; Inman and Leggett, 2019),
among others (King, 2013), but through no Homicide investigations, demographic con-
fault of the observers there is usually a lack of sequences of natural disasters, wars, and
control over relevant variables, or behavioral epidemics, psychological, medical, and eco-
details are incomplete. In a rare experimental nomic consequences of bereavement, end-of-
study, without citing any research on chemi- life care, suicide, abortion, development of
cal releasers of necrophoresis in insects, Pinel death-related concepts, death education, and
et al. (1981) extended this line of research to cultural variations in funerary practices are
a mammalian species. In several experiments just a few of the issues that fall within the
they showed that individual rats: (1) used scope of modern human thanatology (Bradley
bedding material to cover the corpse of a et al., 2013; Meagher and Balk, 2013). Here
conspecific killed at least 40 hours earlier and I briefly discuss human responses to the dead
recently placed in the subject’s chamber; (2) with specific reference to the two topics used
similarly buried an anesthetized rat sprinkled to introduce the Tinbergian approach, namely
with either putrescine or cadaverine – two corpse disposal and responses to dead infants.
468 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Corpse Disposal increasingly recognized ‘places of the dead’


(cemeteries) and collective representations of
Like social insects, human societies with a death and the dead became more elaborate.
relatively fixed home base must dispose of Across the wide cultural variations in corpse-
corpses. Depending on cultural/religious, eco- disposal methods in human societies, the bio-
logical, and economic factors, among others, logical function of disposal remains clear:
the most common methods used today are reduction of the risks of disease.
cremation and ground burial (in single, shared, Concerning the immediate factors that move
or mass graves). Rarer methods include burial humans to start engaging in corpse disposal,
at sea, and above-ground burial (immure- visual and physical cues (e.g., obvious non-
ment), leaving the body to the elements and survivable injuries, absence of responsiveness
scavengers (Korpiola and Lahtinen, 2015; and agency, coldness, rigor mortis, absence of
Shimane, 2018). Past methods included mum- pulse, breath) are often sufficient to lead to a
mification followed by storing, and cannibal- declaration of death, but often medical tech-
ism. Based on paleoarcheological evidence, nology (e.g., vital signs monitors) is used to
Pettitt (2011) proposed that since the emer- confirm death, after which mortuary activi-
gence of the earliest hominins, five successive ties commence. If the corpse is discovered
and partially nested phases of ‘mortuary activ- only after some delay, then odor cues might
ities’ (behavior toward conspecific corpses) give an added sense of urgency to disposal. A
can be recognized. The core phase includes wide range of volatile organic compounds are
socially mediated interest in and manipulation associated with putrefaction of human bodies,
of the corpse, along with context-specific which in various combinations give rise to the
behavioral displays, and possibly some degree ‘odor of death’ (Verheggen et al., 2017). One
of corpse mutilation and/or cannibalism, such compound is putrescine. Revulsion is a
before abandonment at or near the place of widespread human reaction to the putrefy-
death. With the emergence of australopithe- ing odor of a corpse – human or nonhuman –
cines and early Homo, the archaic (or devel- and often leads to one or more actions such
oping) phase sees the continuation of most of as covering one’s nose and mouth, leaving the
the above features, but also the appearance of scene, masking the odor with another, and/or
‘funerary caching’, i.e., the use of natural fea- disposing of the corpse. Perhaps surprisingly,
tures of the environment to deposit or conceal although work has been done on the actions
a corpse, such as a cleft in a rock formation, or of putrescine and cadaverine at the molecu-
a space below a large boulder. Later, Homo lar level in humans (Izquierdo et  al., 2018),
neanderthalis and Homo sapiens further ritu- there are few scientific studies of humans’
alized behaviors surrounding corpses and behavioral reactions to necromones. In a
started to deliberately bury them instead of rare experimental study, Wisman and Shrira
simply abandoning or caching them. Also, in (2015) focused on the threat-signal property
this modernizing phase multiple corpses might of putrescine and hypothesized that it would
be buried in the same place, and material affect people’s behavior even if they were
objects left in the graves. In the modern phase not consciously aware of its presence. They
Homo sapiens further elaborated upon the showed that putrescine caused (1) increased
already existing ‘social theatre’ around the vigilance: reaction times were faster in partici-
corpse, and probably left signs to mark or pants who sniffed a jar containing putrescine
commemorate burial places. In the advanced before completing a computerized target-
phase, as humans spread across the world, detection task than in participants who sniffed
greater regional and cultural variability in an equally ‘repugnant’ but non-death-related
funerary activities and corpse-disposal meth- odor (ammonia); (2) ‘escape’: participants
ods appeared. Additionally, communities asked to smell putrescine and then respond to
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND THANATOLOGY 469

three simple questions walked away from the et  al., 2014). However, some studies have
test area faster than those similarly exposed to reported contradictory findings and substan-
ammonia. Participants exposed to putrescine tial individual differences in how mothers
also completed unfinished words by produc- respond to contact with their dead baby; one
ing more escape- and threat-related words influencing factor may be the manner in
than ammonia-exposed participants. Although which the baby is presented to the mother
not directly related to death, these experimen- (see Erlandsson et al., 2012; Redshaw et al.,
tally induced responses are consistent with 2016). There is currently no consensus
putrescine’s ability to trigger defensive reac- regarding the psychological outcomes of
tions in humans. holding a stillborn baby.
Regarding ontogeny of corpse disposal With regard to proximate influences, both
in humans, it seems clear that children’s visual and tactile input from the dead baby
views about death and how corpses should seem likely to be important in triggering
be treated are strongly culturally influ- positive feelings in the mother, especially
enced (Longbottom and Slaughter, 2018; if the baby is still warm, shortly after death
Panagiotaki et  al., 2018). However, little (Rådestad et al., 2009). However, the visual
information is available on the development appearance – real or imagined – of the baby
of children’s ideas specifically about ways to can influence the parents’ decision to view
dispose of corpses, and what factors might the baby (Sun et  al., 2014) and their expe-
influence these ideas. Similarly, although rience of contact (Ryninks et  al., 2014).
studies have looked at how they respond to Ontogenetic influences will clearly matter
visual depictions of death, such as scenarios in such complex situations, but we are still
that show signs of injury or fatal attacks (e.g., far from identifying specific developmental
Barrett and Behne, 2005), studies of young processes underlying how individual human
children’s reactions to olfactory cues that mothers respond to dead babies. From an
adults associate with death are lacking. evolutionary perspective, however, it is easy
to see parallels between the emotional and
behavioral consequences of loss of an infant
in human parents and nonhuman primate
Mothers and Dead Infants
mothers (and possibly fathers, in some spe-
Of course, human mothers grieve when their cies). The ‘vigilance in grief’ phenomenon
offspring dies. Although they show nothing involving heightened sensitivity to any infor-
as obvious or as widespread as nonhuman mation associated with the deceased has
primate PICT, it is common for parents to been suggested to psychologically prepare
keep reminders of their deceased offspring, the bereaved individual for a possible viable
such as toys or clothes (Foster et al., 2011); reunion with the former (White and Fessler,
this is a normal behavior that might only 2018). This recalls the ‘wait and see’ hypoth-
become problematic in some cases of pro- esis of PICT in nonhuman primate and ceta-
longed, pathological grief (Gorer, 1965). In cean mothers, discussed above. Furthermore,
cases of stillbirth or peri-natal death, in some in many cultures it is common for bereaved
societies mothers may desire to spend time in individuals to physically contact the corpse
physical contact with their baby, before not only of newborns but also older kin –
funerary arrangements are begun. From a sometimes over prolonged periods – despite
functional perspective, this period of contact the potential risk of disease. Based on data
could be seen as helping mothers to cope from 57 cultures, however, the risk of con-
with the emotional trauma of losing their tamination from mortuary rituals involving
infant (Rådestad et al., 2009), possibly buff- physical contact appeared lower than some
ering against maternal depression (Ryninks normative forms of contact with the living
470 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

(Murray et al., 2017), and White et al. (2017) or commemoration? Do the reactions of
concluded that ritual exposure to corpses ­primates to the corpse vary according to the
facilitated progress through and recovery social status of the individual when it was
from grieving. alive? (see Porter et  al., 2019, for possible
examples in wild mountain gorillas). Why do
primate mothers persist in PICT when their
infant’s corpse smells foul? Is putrescine less
Future Directions
repulsive for nonhuman primates than for
Comparative evolutionary thanatology needs humans?
more data on a wide range of species, and Much more research is required before we
from observational and, where ethically can give confident answers to these and other
acceptable, experimental approaches. Given relevant questions about reactions to the dead
the close evolutionary relationships between in primates and other species. Tinbergen’s
humans and the great apes in brain develop- questions provide an excellent framework for
ment and behavior, Pettitt and Anderson studying how and why responses to the dead
(2020) recently proposed some common goals may converge or diverge not just across but
and terminology as a way of fostering better also within species. Of course, despite often
communication and collaboration between being considered independently, the four
thanatology-oriented paleontologists and pri- questions are complementary, and answers
matologists. They also listed some questions to one can lead to insights relevant to oth-
that, if addressed by primatologists, would ers. Given the nature of the general topic, the
help in clarifying stages of ancestral human typical lack of control over circumstances
mortuary activities. Evolutionary thanatology and the lack of preparedness to record events
can be seen as a more general framework for (in primates), all four questions will continue
unifying the study of death and dying across a to be valuable. By addressing them, we can
wider range of species (Anderson et al., 2018). hope to establish an evolutionarily informed
There is little doubt that some behaviors thanatology that, as set out by Lewis et  al.
shown toward corpses in ancestral humans (2017), will (a) generate testable hypotheses,
(see Pettitt, 2011) are seen to various extents (b) test empirical predictions, and (c) allow
in modern nonhuman primates, including plausible interpretations.
gathering around and manipulating corpses,
mutilation, and cannibalism. Simple aban-
donment of the corpse is the norm in pri-
mates – albeit delayed in some cases of ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
PICT – but rare cases of partially covering
corpses with vegetation have been reported in I thank the editors for their help with the
chimpanzees (Boesch, 2012). This behavior manuscript, and an anonymous reviewer for
(dropping vegetation on the corpse) has only valuable comments and suggestions. While
been reported at one field site, and it is not writing the chapter I was supported by JSPS
clear precisely what motivates it. Re-visiting KAKENHI Grant No. 18K18693.
the site where a group member has recently
died has been reported in several species
(Gonçalves and Carvalho, 2019), but do sur-
vivors return to that location more frequently REFERENCES
or for longer than expected based on their
normal ranging patterns? Does their behav- Alley TR (1980) Infantile colouration as an
ior there change noticeably, for example in elicitor of caretaking behavior in Old World
any ways that might indicate recognition primates. Primates 21:416–429
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND THANATOLOGY 471

Altmann J (1980) Baboon mothers and Biro D, Humle T, Koops K, Sousa C, Hayashi M,
Infants. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Matsuzawa T (2010) Chimpanzee mothers at
Press Bossou, Guinea carry the mummified remains
Anderson JR (2016) Comparative thanatology. of their dead infants. Current Biology
Current Biology 26:R553–R556 20:R351–R352
Anderson JR (2017) Comparative evolutionary Boesch C (2012) Wild cultures: a comparison
thanatology of grief, with special reference between chimpanzee and human cultures.
to nonhuman primates. Japanese Review of Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Cultural Anthropology 18:173–189 Bonoti F, Leondari A, Mastora A (2013) Explor-
Anderson JR, Andre E, Wolf P (1995) Successful ing children’s understanding of death:
mother- and group-rearing of a newborn through drawings and the Death Concept
capuchin monkey (Cebus capucinus) follow- Questionnaire. Death Studies 37:47–60
ing emergency major surgery. Animal Wel- Bradley B, Feldman F, Johansson J (2013) The
fare 4:171–182 Oxford handbook of philosophy of death.
Anderson JR, Biro D, Pettitt P (2018) Evolutionary New York: Oxford University Press
thanatology. Philosophical Transactions of the Brent SB, Speece MW, Lin C, Dong Q, Yang C
Royal Society B 373:20170262 http://dx.doi. (1996) The development of the concept of
org/10.1098/rstb.2017.0262 death among Chinese and U.S. children 7–13
Appleby R, Smith B, Jones D (2013) Observa- years of age: from binary to ‘fuzzy’ concepts?
tions of a free-ranging adult female dingo Omega 33:67–83
(Canis dingo) and littermates’ responses to Brown MW, Norris ME (2004) Survivorship
the death of a pup. Behavioural Processes advantage of conspecific necrophagy in
96:42–46 overwintering boxelder bugs (Heteroptera:
Arathi HS, Burns I, Spivak M (2000) Ethology of Rhopalidae). Annals of the Entomological
hygienic behavior in the honey bee Apis mel- Society of America 97:500–503
lifera L. (Hymenoptera: Apidae): behavioural Bryer PJ, Mira RS, Chivers DP (2001) Chemo­
repertoire of hygienic bees. Ethology sensory assessment of predation risk by slimy
106:365–379 sculpins (Cottus cognatus): responses to
Barrett HC, Behne T (2005) Children’s under- alarm, disturbance, and predator cues. J
standing of death as the cessation of agency: Journal of Chemical Ecology 27:533–546
a test using sleep versus death. Cognition Carlson NV, Pargeter HM, Templeton CN (2017)
96:93–108 Sparrowhawk movement, calling, and pres-
Barrett L, Blumstein DT, Clutton-Brock TH, ence of dead conspecifics differentially
Kappeler PM (2013) Taking note of impact blue tit (Cyanistes caeruleus) vocal
Tinbergen, or: the promise of a biology of and behavioral mobbing responses. Behavio-
behavior. Philosophical Transactions of the ral Ecology and Sociobiology 71:133 DOI
Royal Society B 36820120352 http://dx.doi. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-017-2361-x
org/10.1098/rstb.2012.0352 Carr WJ, Landauer MR, Wiese RE, Morasco E,
Bateson P, Laland KN (2013) Tinbergen’s four Thor DH (1979) A natural food aversion in
questions: an appreciation and an update. Norway rats. J Journal of Comparative and
Trends in Ecology & Evolution 28:712–718 Physiological Psychology 89:574–584
Bearzi G, Kerem D, Furey NB, Pitman RL, Ren- Choe DH, Millar JG, Rust MK (2009) Chemical
dell L, Reeves RR (2018) Whale and dolphin signals associated with life inhibit necropho-
behavioural responses to dead conspecifics. resis in Argentine ants. Proceedings of the
Zoology 128:1–15 National Academy of Sciences USA
Bercovitch FB (2012) Giraffe cow reaction to 106:8251–8255
the death of her newborn calf. African Jour- Cross DJ, Marzluff JM, Palmquist I, Minoshima S,
nal of Ecology 51:376–379 Shimizu T, Miyaoka R (2013) Distinct neural
Bercovitch FB (2019) A comparative perspective circuits underlie assessment of a diversity of
on the evolution of mammalian reactions to natural dangers by American crows. Proceed-
dead conspecifics. Primates 61:21–28 https:// ings of the Royal Society B 280:20131046
doi.org/10.1007/s10329-019-00722-3 doi:10.1098/rspb.2013.1046
472 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Das S, Erinjery JJ, Desai N, Mohan K, Kumara Foster TL, Giler MJ, Davies B, Dietrich MS, Bar-
HN, Singh M (2019) Deceased infant carry- rera M, Fairclough DL, Vannatt K, Gerhardt
ing in nonhuman anthropoids: insights from CA (2011) Comparison of continuing bonds
systematic analysis and case studies of reported by parents and siblings after a
bonnet macaques (Macaca radiata) and lion- child’s death from cancer. Death Studies
tailed macaques (Macaca silenus). Journal of 35:420–440
Comparative Psychology 133:156–170 Frederickx C, Dekeirsschieter J, Verheggen FJ,
Dellatore DF, Waitt CD, Foitova I (2009) Two Haubruge E (2012) Responses of Lucilia seri-
cases of mother-infant cannibalism in orang­ cata Meigen (Diptera: Calliphoridae) to cadav-
utans. Primates 50:277–281 eric volatile organic compounds. Journal of
De Marco A, Cozzolino R, Thierry B (2018) Forensic Science 57:386–390 doi: 10.1111/
Prolonged transport and cannibalism of j.1556-4029.2011.s02010.x
mummified infant remains by a Tonkean Gire J (2014) How death imitates life: cultural
macaque mother. Primates 59:55–59 influences on conceptions of death and
Diez L, Lejeune P, Detrain C (2014) Keep the dying. Online Readings in Psychology and
nest clean: survival strategies of corpse Culture 6(2) https://doi.org/10.9707/2307-
removal in ants. Biology Letters 10:20140306 0919.1120
http://dex.doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2014.0306 Goldenberg SZ, Wittemyer G (2019) Elephant
Douglas-Hamilton I, Bhalla S, Wittemeyer G, behavior toward the dead: A review and
Vollrath F (1986) Behavioural reactions of insights from field observations. Primates 61,
elephants towards a dying and deceased 119–128.
matriarch. Applied Animal Behaviour Science Gonçalves A, Biro D (2018) Comparative thana-
100:87–102 tology, and integrative approach: exploring
Engh AL, Beehner JC, Bergman TJ, Whitten PL, sensory/cognitive aspects of death recogni-
Hoffmeier RR, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL tion in vertebrates and invertebrates. Philo-
(2006) Behavioural and hormonal responses sophical Transactions of the Royal Society B
to predation in female chacma baboons 373:20170263 http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/
(Papio hamadryas ursinus). Proceedings of rstb.2017.0263
the Royal Society B, 273:707–712 Gonçalves A, Carvalho S (2019) Death among
Erlandsson K, Warland J, Cacciatore J, Rådestad primates: a critical review of non-human
I (2012) Seeing and holding a stillborn baby: primate interactions towards their dead
mothers’ feelings in relation to how their and dying. Biological Reviews
babies were presented to them after birth – 94:1502–1529
Findings from an online questionnaire. Mid- Goodall J (1983) Population dynamics during a
wifery 29:246–250 15-year period in one community of free-
Fairbanks LA (1996) Individual differences in living chimpanzees in the Gombe National
maternal style: causes and consequences for Park, Tanzania. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie
mothers and offspring. Advances in the 61:1–60
Study of Behavior 25:579–611 Gorer G (1965) Death, grief and mourning in
Fashing PJ, Nguyen N, Barry TS, Goodale CB, contemporary Britain. Garden City, NY:
Burke RJ, Jones SCZ, Kerby JT, Lee LM, Nurmi Doubleday
NO, Venkataraman VV (2011) Death among Grigg NP, Krilow JM, Gutierrez-Ibanez C, Wylie
geladas (Theropithecus gelada): a broader DR, Graves GR, Iwaniuk AN (2017) Anatomi-
perspective on mummified infants and primate cal evidence for scent guided foraging in the
thanatology. American Journal of Primatology turkey vulture. Scientific Reports 7:17408
73:405–409 doi:10.1038/s41598-017-17794-0
Fedurek P, Tkaczynski P, Asiimwe C, Hobaiter C, Harlow HF (1974) Learning to love. New York:
Samuni L, Lowe AE, Dijrian AG, Zuberbühler Jason Aronson
K, Wittig RM, Crockford C (2019) Maternal Hawley CR, Beirne C, Meier A, Poulsen JR
cannibalism in two populations of wild chim- (2017–2018) Conspecific investigation of a
panzees. Primates 61:181–187 https://doi. deceased forest elephant (Loxodonta cyclo-
org/10.1007/s10329-019-00765-6 tis). Pachyderm 59:97–100
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND THANATOLOGY 473

Hayashi M, Matsuzawa T (2017) Mother-infant psychology: a how-to guide. American


interactions in captive and wild chimpan- Psychologist 72:353–373
zees. Infant Behavior and Development Longbottom S, Slaughter V (2018) Sources of
48:20–29 children’s knowledge about death and dying.
Hrdy SB (2000) Mother nature. London: Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Soci-
Random House ety B 373:20170267 http://dx.doi.
Hussain A, Saraiva LR, Ferrero DM, Ahuja G, org/10.1098/rstb.2017.0267
Krishna VS, Liberles SD, Korsching SI (2013) Lonsdorf EV (2017) Sex differences in nonhu-
High-affinity olfactory receptor for the man primate behavioral development. Jour-
death-associated odor cadaverine. Proceed- nal of Neuroscience Research 95:213–221
ings of the National Academy of Sciences López-Riquelme GO, Fanjul-Moles ML (2013) The
USA 110:19579–19584 funeral ways of social insects: social strategies
Iglesias TL, McElreath R, Patricelli GL (2012) for corpse disposal. Trends in Entomology
Western scrub-jay funerals: cacophonous 9:71–129
aggregations in response to dead conspecif- Maestripieri D (2018) Maternal influences in
ics. Animal Behaviour 84:1103–1111 primate social development. Behavioral Ecol-
Iglesias TL, Stetkevich RC, Patricelli GL (2014) ogy and Sociobiology 72:130 https://doi.
Dead heterospecifics as cues of risk in the org/10.1007/s00265-018-2547-x
environment: Does size affect response? Masi S (2020) Reaction to allospecific death and
Behaviour 151:1–22 to an unanimated gorilla infant in wild west-
Inman V, Leggett EA (2019) Observations on ern gorillas: insights into death recognition
the response of a pod of hippos to a dead and prolonged maternal carrying. Primates
juvenile hippo (Hippopotamus amphibious, Lin- 61:83–92 https://doi.org/10.1007/s10329-
naeus 1758) African Journal of Ecology 019-00745-w
doi:10.1111/aje.12644 Matsumoto T, Ito N, Inoue S, Nakamura M
Izquierdo C, Gómez-Tamayo JC, Nebel JC, (2016) An observation of a severely disabled
Pardo L, Gonzalez A (2018) Identifying infant chimpanzee in the wild and her inter-
human diamine sensors for death related actions with her mother. Primates 57:3–7
putrescine and cadaverine molecules. PLos Meagher DK, Balk DE (2013) Handbook of
Computational Biology 14(1):e1005945 thanatology, 2nd edition. New York:
https://doi.prg/10.1371/journal. Routledge
pcbi.1005945 Mendonça RS, Ringhofer M, Pinto P, Inoue S,
Julian GE, Cahan S (1999) Undertaking spe- Hirata S (2020) Feral horses’ (Equus ferus
cialization in the desert leaf-cutter ant Acro- caballus) behavior toward dying and dead
myrmex versicolor. Animal Behaviour conspecifics. Primates 61:49–54 https://doi.
58:437–442 org/10.1007/s10329-019-00728-x
Kapheim KM (2019) Synthesis of Tinbergen’s Munday Z, Brown MJF (2018) Bring out your
four questions and the future of socio­ dead: quantifying corpse removal in Bombus
genomics. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiol- terrestris, an annual eusocial insect. Animal
ogy 73:186 Behaviour 138:51–57
King BJ (2013) How animals grieve. Chicago: Murray DR, Fessler DMT, Kerry N, White C,
University of Chicago Press Marin M (2017) The kiss of death: three tests
Korpiola M, Lahtinen A (2015) Cultures of of the relationship between disease threat
death and dying in medieval and early and ritualized physical contact within tradi-
modern Europe. Helsinki: Helsinki Collegium tional cultures. Evolution and Human Behav-
for Advanced Studies ior 38:63–70
Kristal MB (2009) The biopsychology of mater- Nakamichi M (1986) Behavior of infant
nal behavior in nonhuman mammals. ILAR Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata) with
Journal 50:51–63 congenital limb malformations during their
Lewis DMG, Al-Shawaf L, Conroy-Beam D, first three months. Developmental
Asao K, Buss DM (2017) Evolutionary Psychobiology 19:335–341
474 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Neoh K-B, Yeap B-K, Tsunoda K, Yoshimura T, Porter A, Eckardt W, Vecellio V, Guschanski K,
Lee C-Y (2012) Do termites avoid carcasses? Niehoff PP, Ngobobo-As-Ibungu U, Pekeyake
Behavioral responses depend on the nature RN, Stoinski T, Caillaud D (2019) Behavioral
of the carcasses. PLoS ONE 7(4):e36375 responses around conspecific corpses in adult
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0036375 eastern gorillas (Gorilla beringei spp.) PeerJ
Nicolson NA (1987) Infants, mothers, and other 7:e6655 http://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.6655
females. In: Smuts BB, Cheney DL, Seyfarth Prounis GS, Shields WM (2013) Necrophobic
RM, Wrangham RW, Struhsaker TT (Eds.), behavior in small mammals. Behavioural Pro-
Primate Societies. Chicago: University of Chi- cesses 94:41–44
cago Press, pp. 330–342 Rådestad I, Säflund K, Wredling R, Oneläv E,
Nicolson NA (1991) Maternal behavior in human Steineck G (2009) Holding a stillborn baby:
and nonhuman primates. In: Loy JD, Peters mothers’ feelings of tenderness and grief.
CB (Eds.), Understanding Behavior. New York: British Journal of Midwifery 17:178–180
Oxford University Press, pp. 17–50 Redshaw M, Hennegan JM, Henderson J (2016)
Oldroyd BP, Thompson GJ (2007) Behavioural Impact of holding the baby following still-
genetics of the honey bee Apis mellifera. birth on maternal health and well-being:
Advances in Insect Physiology 33:1–49 findings from a national survey. BMJ Open
Oliviera TA, Koakoski G, Costa da Mota A, 6:e010996 doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2015-
Piato AL, Barreto RE, Volpato GL, Barcellos 010996
LJG (2014) Death-associated odors induce Reggente MALV, Papale E, McGinty N, Eddy L,
stress in zebrafish. Hormones and Behavior de Lucia GA, Bertulli CG (2018) Social rela-
65:340–344 tionships and death-related behavior in
Owren MJ, Dieter JA (1989) Infant cross-­ aquatic mammals: a systematic review. Phil-
fostering between Japanese (Macaca fuscata) osophical Transactions of the Royal Society B
and rhesus macaques (M. mulatta). Ameri- 373:20170260 http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/
can Journal of Primatology 18:245–250 rstb.2017.0260
Panagiotaki G, Hopkins M, Nobes G, Ward E, Renucci M, Tirard A, Provost E (2011) Complex
Griffiths D (2018) Children’s and adults’ undertaking behavior in Temnothorax
understanding of death: cognitive, parental, lichtensteini ant colonies: from corpse-burying
and experiential influences. Journal of Exper- behavior to necrophoric behavior. Insectes
imental Child Psychology 166:96–115 Sociaux 58:9–16
Panagiotaki G, Nobes G, Ashraf A, Aubby H Rhine RJ, Norton GW, Roertgen WJ, Klein HD
(2015) British and Pakistani children’s under- (1980) The brief survival of free-ranging
standing of death: cultural and developmental baboon infants (Papio cynocephalus) after
influences. British Journal of Developmental separation from their mothers. International
Psychology 33:31–44 Journal of Primatology 1:401–409
Pettitt P (2011) The Palaeolithic origins of Robinson GE, Page RE (1988) Genetic determi-
human burial. London: Routledge nation of guarding and undertaking in
Pettitt P (2018) Hominin evolutionary thanatol- honey-bee colonies. Nature 333:356–358
ogy from the mortuary to funerary realm. Rollo CD, Czyzewska E, Borden JH (1994) Fatty
The palaeoanthropological bridge between acid necromones for cockroaches. Naturwis-
chemistry and culture. “Philosophical Trans- senschaften 81:409–410
actions of the Royal Society B 373 https:// Rothenbuhler WC (1964) Behaviour genetics of
doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2018.0212 nest cleaning in honey bees. I. Responses of
Pettitt P, Anderson JR (2020) Primate thanatol- four inbred lines to disease-killed brood.
ogy an hominoid mortuary archeology. Pri- Animal Behaviour 12:578–583
mates 61:9–19 https://doi.org/10.1007/ Ryninks K, Roberts-Collins C, McKenzie-
s10329-019-00769-2 McHarg K, Horsch A (2014) Mothers’ experi-
Pinel JPJ, Gorzalka BB, Ladak F (1981) ence of their contact with their stillborn infant:
Cadaverine and putrescine initiate the burial an interpretative phenomenological analysis.
of dead conspecifics by rats. Physiology & BMC Pregnancy & Childbirth 14:203 www.
Behavior 27:819–824 biomedcentral.com/1471–2393/14/203
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND THANATOLOGY 475

Saltzman W, Maestripieri D (2011) The neu- stillbirth. International Journal of Nursing


roendocrinology of primate maternal behav- Studies 51:1153–1159
ior. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology Sun Q, Haynes KF, Zhou X (2013) Differential
and Biological Psychiatry 35:1192–1204 undertaking response of a lower termite to
Sato A, Koda H, Lemasson A, Nagumo S, Masa- congeneric and conspecific corpses. Scien-
taka N (2012) Visual recognition of age class tific Reports 3:1650 doi: 10.1038/srep01650
and preference for infantile features: implica- Sun Q, Haynes KF, Zhou X (2017) Dynamic
tions for species-specific vs universal cognitive changes in death cues modulate risks and
traits in primates. PLoS ONE 7(5):38387 rewards of corpse management in a social
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0038387 insect. Functional Ecology 31:697–706
Sharma N, Pokharel SS, Kohshima S, Sukumar Sun Q, Haynes KF, Zhou X (2018) Managing
R (2020) Behavioural responses of free- the risks and rewards of death in eusocial
ranging Asian elephants (Elephas maximus) insects. Philosophical Transactions of the
towards dying and dead conspecifics. Pri- Royal Society B 373:20170258
mates 61:129–138 https://doi.org/10.1007/ Sun Q, Zhou X (2013) Corpse management in
s10329-1089-00739-8 social insects. International Journal of Bio-
Shimane K (2018) Social bonds with the dead: logical Sciences. 9:313–321
how funerals transformed in the twentieth Swift K, Marzluff JM (2018) Occurrence and
and twenty-first centuries. Philosophical variability of tactile interactions between
Transactions of the Royal Society B wild American crows and dead conspecifics.
373:20170274 doi:10.1098/rstb.2017.0274 Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Soci-
Slaughter V, Griffiths M (2007) Death under- ety B 373:20170259 http://dx.doi.
standing and fear of death in young children. org/10.1098/rstb.2017.0259
Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry Swift KN, Marzluff JM (2015) Wild American
12:525–535 crows gather around their dead to learn
Smith S (1986) Infant cross-fostering in rhesus about danger. Animal Behaviour
monkeys (Macaca mulatta): a procedure for 109:187–197
the long-term management of captive popu- Taborsky M (2014) Tribute to Tinbergen: the
lations. American Journal of Primatology four problems of biology. A critical appraisal.
11:229–237 Ethology 120:224–227
Speece MW, Brent SB (1984) Children’s under- Takeshita RSC, Huffman MA, Kinoshit K, Berco-
standing of death: a review of three compo- vitch FB (2020) Changes in social behavior
nents of a death concept. Child Development and fecal glucocorticoids in a Japanese
55:1671–1686 macaque (Macaca fuscata) carrying her dead
Strauss MKL, Muller Z (2012) Giraffe mothers infant. Primates 61:35–45 https://doi.
in East Africa linger for days near the remains org/10.1007/s10329-019-00753-w
of their dead calves. African Journal of Ecol- Tartabini A (1991) Mother-infant cannibalism in
ogy 51:506–509 thick-tailed bushbabies (Galago crassicauda-
Sugiyama Y, Kurita H, Matsui T, Kimoto S, Shi- tus umbrosus). Primates 32:379–383
momura T (2009) Carrying of dead infants Teleki G (1973) Group response to the acciden-
by Japanese macaque (Macaca fuscata) tal death of a chimpanzee in Gombe National
mothers. Anthropological Science Park, Tanzania. Folia Primatologica 20:81–94
117:113–119 Thierry B, Anderson JR (1986) Adoption in
Sullivan, EC, Mondoza SP, Capitanio JP (2011) anthropoid primates. International Journal of
Similarity in temperament between mother Primatology 7:191–216
and offspring rhesus monkeys: sex differ- Tinbergen N (1963) On aims and methods in
ences and role of monoamine oxidase-A and Ethology. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie
serotonin transporter promoter polymor- 20:410–433
phism genotypes. Developmental Psychobi- Tokuyama N, Moore DL, Graham KE, Lokasola
ology 53:549–563 A, Furuichi T (2017) Cases of maternal can-
Sun JC, Rei W, Sheu SJ (2014) Seeing or not nibalism in wild bonobos (Pan paniscus) from
seeing: Taiwan’s parents’ experiences during two different field sites, Wamba and
476 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Kokolopori, Democratic Republic of the Visscher PK (1983) The honey bee way of
Congo. Primates 58:7–12 death: necrophoric behavior in Apis mellifera
Trapanese C, Bey M, Tonachella G Meunier H, colonies. Animal Behaviour 31:1070–1076
Masi S (2020) Prolonged care and cannibalism Watson CFI, Matsuzawa T (2018) Behaviour of
of infant corpse by relatives in semi-free-­ nonhuman primate mothers toward their
ranging capuchin monkeys. Primates 61:41–47 dead infants: uncovering mechanisms. Philo-
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10329-019-00747-8 sophical Transactions of the Royal Society B
Trumbo ST, Huang ZY, Robinson GE (1997) Divi- 373:20170261 http://dx.doi.org/
sion of labor between undertaker specialists 10.1098/rstb.2017.0261
and other middle-aged workers in honey bee White C, Fessler DMT (2018) An evolutionary
colonies. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiol- account of vigilance in grief. Evolution,
ogy 41:151–163 Medicine, and Public Health 2018 (1):34–42
Trumbo ST, Robinson GE (1997) Learning and doi:10.1093/emph/eox018
task interference by corpse-removal specialists White C, Marin M, Fessler DMT (2017) Not just
in honey bee colonies. Ethology 103:966–975 dead meat: an evolutionary account of
Valdes L, Laidre ME (2019) Scent of death: corpse treatment in mortuary rituals. Journal
Evolution from sea to land of an extreme of Cognition and Culture 17:146–168
collective attraction to conspecific death. Wilson, EO, Durlach, NI, Roth, LM (1958)
Ecology and Evolution 9:2171–2179 Chemical releaser of necrophoric behavior in
van Lawick-Goodall J (1968) The behavior of ants. Psyche 65:108–114
free-living chimpanzees in the Gombe Stream Wisman A, Shrira I (2015) The smell of death:
Reserve. Animal Behaviour Monographs evidence that putrescine elicits threat man-
1:161–311 agement mechanisms. Frontiers in Psychol-
van Noordwijk MA (2012) From maternal ogy 6:1274 doi: 10.33389/fpsyg.2015.0127
investment to lifetime maternal care. In: Worlein JM (2014) Nonhuman primate models
Mitani JC, Call J, Kappeler PM, Palombit RA, of depression: effects of early experience
Silk JB (Eds.), The Evolution of Primate Socie- and stress, ILAR Journal 55:259–273
ties. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, Yang B, Anderson JR, Li BG (2016) Tending a
pp. 321–342 dying adult in a wild multi-level primate soci-
Verheggen F, Perrault KA, Megido RC, Duboid ety. Current Biology 26:R403–R404
LM, Francis F, Haubruge E, Forbes SL, Focant Yao M, Rosenfeld J, Attridge S, Sidhu S,
JF, Stefanuto PH (2017) The odor of death: Askenov V, Rollo CD (2009) The ancient
an overview of current knowledge on char- chemistry of avoiding risks of predation and
acterization and applications. Bioscience disease. Evolutionary Biology 36:267–281
67:600–613
24
Evolutionary Psychology and
Reproduction
S t e p h e n W h y t e a n d B e n n o To r g l e r

SEXUAL REPRODUCTION AND reproduction have long been of interest to


MATING biologists, with Maynard Smith (1971) iden-
tifying as the most important the ability to
The evolutionary innovation of sexual repro- adapt to a novel environment that favors a
duction allows organisms to evolve rapidly new gene combination:
and thrive through genome reshuffling (Ball, If two different populations have over a long
2019), results in increased variation, and was period adapted to two different environments,
proposed over 100 years ago by August they will differ genetically at a number of loci; one
Weismann. ‘Such differences’, Weismann may be ABC…L and the other abc…l. Suppose
now a new environment becomes available for
explained, ‘afford the material by means of colonization, in which the best adapted genotype
which natural selection is able to increase or is, say AbC…l. If founders from both original
weaken each character according to the needs populations invade the new environment, then
of the species’ (as cited by Otto, 2009: S2). evolutionary adaptation will be faster by several
orders of magnitude if the founders can hybridize
For mammals, reproduction means that a
sexually. (Smith, 1971: 334)
new individual organism emerges that,
although physically and biologically attached Adaptation to this new environment in turn
to the female, is a mix of genetic variations increases the rate of evolutionary adaptation
from two parent organisms. Thus, unlike (Becks and Agrawal, 2012),2 so that even
other biological classes, mammals reproduce though the diverse array of sexually produced
sexually rather than self-cloning through the genotypes results in a lower average fitness of
asexual methods of binary fusion,1 fragmen- offspring, these well-adapted genotypes con-
tation, budding, parthenogenesis, and gem- tribute disproportionately to future genera-
mulation. The advantages of sexual tions. In general, then, sex is a bet-hedging
478 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

strategy that can result in offspring with a sexual bargain: ‘No human soap opera could
higher geometric mean fitness in a context of outstrip in violence, hypocrisy, and manipu-
continual environmental change (Lively and lation the daily drama of relations between
Morran, 2014). the sexes across the entire animal kingdom’
When such change is overly rapid, how- (Seabright, 2012: 8). Extreme manifesta-
ever, path-dependent genetic associations tions of this drama include the penis fencing
that build upon past selection can become enacted by hermaphroditic marine flatworms
detrimental to survival (Otto, 2009), so that a (Pseudobiceros hancockanus):
population of asexual organisms may pick up
mutations that slow their growth process and [T]wo of them coil around each other and engage
in an aggressive head-to-head wrestle with weap-
drag down the entire population (Zimmer,
ons drawn… Whichever worm wins does so by
2009). On the other hand, when selection var- piercing the head of the other with its spiked
ies over time and space, sexual reproduction organ, and coerces the loser into adopting the
is an evolutionary advantage (Otto, 2009), as female role in this relationship, and becoming the
supported by experimental evidence of co- sperm receptable and egg bearer. It’s easier to
produce sperm than it is eggs, and it’s harder to
evolutionary host-parasite interactions lead-
bear the younglings, so the individual who man-
ing to long-term persistence of sex (Lively aged to claim the male mantle remains childfree,
and Morran, 2014). Hence, consistent with and ready to crack on for another with a new
the Red Queen’s observation in Through the individual. (Rutherford, 2018: 100)
Looking-Glass, ‘(Carol, 1917)… it takes all
the running you can do, to keep in the same One topical focus in the fields of evolution-
place’ and species continually evolve sim- ary psychology and evolutionary ecology, as
ply to maintain their position with respect to well as in more specialized subfields like
other competing, evolving species. reproductive ecology, is the existence in evo-
Yet despite its advantages, sexual repro- lution of tradeoffs. These tradeoffs occur
duction is challenging and costly, particu- between survival and reproduction, growth
larly when males contribute nothing more and reproduction, quantity versus quality of
than genes. For example, in any given gen- offspring, investment in current versus future
eration, a sexual population produces half the offspring, and the division of energy between
number of offspring generated by an asexual investing in offspring and obtaining a mate
population of the same size, a potential one- (Sear, 2015: S44). As a result, evolutionary
half reduction in birth rate that makes the streams of research have tended to focus on
sexual population more likely to become the variations in female reproductive func-
extinct (Hadany and Comeron, 2008; Lively tions subject to environmental changes with
and Morran, 2014). This population is thus an emphasis on the distinct fitness constraints
obliged to produce twice as many offspring between males and females. One powerful
to avoid transmission disadvantage (Otto, conceptual framework for understanding
2009), which Maynard Smith (1978) labelled which conditions occur in male versus female
the ‘two-fold cost of males’. Completing the sexual selection is Trivers’ (1972) parental
genome through the matching of one gamete investment theory, which attributes females’
(egg) to another (sperm) at the right time and different reproductive strategies to their
place also requires a dating exercise to find higher metabolic demands (gestation, child-
and attract a potential partner. This mating birth, and lactation), lower potential lifetime
itself is energy consuming and subject to such reproductive success, and greater initial
risks as sexually transmitted diseases or mat- investment per gamete. These reproductive
ing predation (Otto, 2009). Hence, in an inter- factors require a greater investment of
play between cooperation and competition, resources in offspring, leading females of
both sexes must apply strategies to strike a many species to be choosier than males about
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND REPRODUCTION 479

their choice of mate (Bribiescas, 2001; Buss, important is not simply that men are show-offs but
2012; Barrett et al., 2014). Given that wom- that they flash their feathers in especially ostenta-
tious ways when they are in a mating frame of
en’s choices tend to reflect what has histori-
mind. Is it just a coincidence that human male
cally helped to increase the reproduction and showiness responds to mating motives in the way
survival of their offspring (Buss, 2012), it is that males in other species respond to the mating
not surprising that evolution has favored such season? Perhaps, but it seems unlikely. Remember,
selectivity. women prefer to mate with men who stand out
from the crowd. Thus the payoff is the same for
Females are thus more selective than
male humans as it is for peacocks and bighorn
males regarding characteristics that relate sheep, and it is the payoff that drives natural selec-
to human adaptive challenges, such as the tion at all levels – an increase in attractiveness to
power, wealth, intelligence, social status, the more discriminating sex, who will be disin-
dominance, ambition, and popularity that clined to mate with a male who does not demon-
strate his worth. (Kenrick, 2011: 144)
facilitate resource acquisition (Kenrick et al.,
1990; Buss, 2012). Because superior health
depends on better food and more abundant Based on observations from 37 cultures, human
territory but humans have spent most of their mating preferences differ greatly between the
evolutionary history as hunter-gatherers, sexes, with females valuing good financial
women’s evolved preferences include the prospects and ambition or industriousness more
modern equivalents of the qualities once highly than do males, and males displaying a
needed for successful hunting and gather- greater preference for physical attractiveness
ing of accruable, defensible, and controllable and younger mates (Buss, 1989, Buss et  al.,
resources (Buss, 2012). This human behavior 1990). Although women also value physical
thus again reflects past selection pressures attractiveness in a potential mate, they do so
(Tooby and Cosmides, 2005). more as a signal of genetic benefit (Gangestad
Male success in mating, in contrast, is and Buss, 1993; Gangestad et al., 1994) such as
dependent on intersexual selection and superior immunocompetence (Puts, 2010) or
intrasexual competition; that is, the need to pathogenic resistance (Gangestad and Buss,
charm females while threatening and con- 1993: 89).
quering other males (Symons, 1979; Puts, In our contemporary world, the drastic
2010). Indeed, some scholars have argued changes wrought by new technologies on
that sexual attraction and selective social human procreation and the mating game
attachments are among the most powerful have made human reproduction an ever more
driving forces of human behavior, allowing important area of research for those inter-
us to understand the richness of human art, ested in cultural evolution. In vitro fertiliza-
language, and creativity (Miller, 2000; Young tion (IVF), for example, has separated sex
and Wang, 2004). Miller (2000), for exam- from procreation; the contraceptive pill has
ple, emphasized that ‘the human mind’s most made procreation optional; and the Internet
impressive abilities are like the peacock’s has radically changed the way humans inter-
tail: they are courtship tools, evolved to act, form new relationships, and seek out
attract and entertain sexual partners’ (2000: partners, affording a novel setting in which to
4). Accordingly, because mate assessment is test the functioning of evolved psychological
relativistic, men attempt to stand out from the mechanisms. Historically, for example, dating
crowd through bold advertising cues (Buston (mate-choice) decisions occurred in person –
and Emlen, 2003) such as and sequentially – at a bar or restaurant or
in the workplace, but now the Internet has
spending money in more conspicuous ways, …
acting like heroes, … standing firm against group
untied the Gordian knot by allowing quasi-
opinion in ways that could make them look good, infinite choice from the comfort and privacy
and … strutting their artistic creativity. What is of home. The digital world has thus loosened
480 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

the time constraints of finding and match- interests in the mating game can lead to an
ing suitable partners and moved us far away arms race between deception and detection
from the traditional approach of contracting (Boyer, 2018). As Williams (1966: 184)
marriages at the community level (Dunbar, pointed out decades ago, ‘selection will fos-
2012). It has also reduced the mental transac- ter a skilled salesmanship among the males
tion costs of identifying the average, as well and an equally well-developed sales resist-
as the complications of finding a mate who ance and discrimination among females’.
meets different mating criteria, whose impor- Exploration of the new environments
tance have changed over time. For instance, brought about by technological change has
in a study of mate preferences across a already helped to generate empirical insights
57-year span, Buss et al. (2001) showed that on mating strategies, with a major goal of the
the importance attached to good looks has new empirics being to isolate confounding
increased, possibly because the shift from factors. For example, over the past several
mid-20th-century reliance on radio to more decades, an extensive literature has produced
visual technologies like television, movies, evidence of what is probably the most wide-
or the Internet has bombarded us with images spread mating pattern of vertebrates (Burley,
of physically attractive individuals (Buss 1983: 191); namely, positive assortative mat-
et  al., 2001). Nonetheless, because mate ing or homogamy (see, e.g., Hollingshead,
choice works based on relative differences, 1950; Warren, 1966; Vandenberg, 1972; Price
those evaluating potential mates need to pay and Vandenberg, 1979; Buss, 1985; Thiessen
special attention to trait variations from the and Gregg, 1980; Hinsz, 1989; Mare, 1991;
baseline, such as women not being attracted Thiessen, 1994; Thiessen et al., 1997; Little
to deep voices but rather to voices that are et al., 2006; Penton-Voak et al., 1999). Most
deeper than the average (Boyer, 2018). particularly, it has shown that homogamy fos-
Sex differences in mate preferences and ters altruistic social behavior, bonding, social
reproductive strategies reflect variations on cohesion and interaction, a common basis of
the adaptive problems faced in mate choice norms and values, and better communica-
by ancestral men and women (Buss, 1995). tion and conversations (Thiessen and Gregg,
Not only can competition to find a mate 1980; Buss, 1985; Kalmijn, 1994; Thiessen
lead to conflict, it may incentivize cheating et  al., 1997). Nonetheless, it remains meth-
and strategic tactical deception. For exam- odologically challenging to isolate mate
ple, to secure enough time to convince a preferences from the context of constraints or
female to mate, mourning cuttlefish mimic opportunities (Schwartz, 2013). For instance,
a female display to a male rival on one side positive assortative mating may be driven by
of their mantle but male courtship patterns who one has the opportunity to meet, with
to a potential female mating partner on the a greater likelihood of being surrounded
other (Brown et al., 2012). This female mim- by individuals with similar characteristics.
icry gives an advantage to undersized males Thus, sorting by education or income may
who have a comparative disadvantage in a be a function of a large amount of time spent
potential fight with a more dominant rival. among others with similar achievement lev-
In fact, because males can quickly switch els in the work environment, school, or col-
from dishonest to honest signals, dual gen- lege (Hitsch et al., 2010a).
der signaling is a frequent means of avoiding Online dating studies have the advantage of
combat during courtship, one that only mani- reducing such constraints and testing whether
fests when a rival male is nearby. In general, positive assortment is possible in these set-
mate-choice strategies reflect the fact that tings. As Hitsch et  al. (2010b: 162) empha-
nature operates in a market, meaning that the sized, ‘in online dating, assortative mating
dishonest signals that result from divergent arises in the absence of search frictions, due
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND REPRODUCTION 481

primarily to preferences and specific market other words, might decreased pressure to avoid
mechanisms by which matches are formed’. bad selection in anticipation of future parental
Hence, analyzing dating choices has the advan- investment affect how females discriminate in
tage of avoiding norm and preference adjust- their choices?
ments made by couples over time because of In general, new technologies can be strong
shared experiences, choices, lifestyles, or diets instruments of courtship, whose ultimate func-
(Little et  al., 2006). Nevertheless, an online tion from a biological standpoint is to increase
dating environment requires an agreed match the reproductive success of the individuals
between two parties whose individual goals involved (Grammer, 1989). Intrasexual com-
must converge, meaning that a potential part- petition encourages the signaling of mate
ner with unrealistically high aspirations may quality, through whose cost credibility is
not be able to attract mates with a higher fit- achieved. Nonetheless, as regards economist
ness level. The advent of IVF and assisted Paul Oyer’s (2014: 212) conceptualization of
reproductive technology (ART) in the late 20th the signal as a ‘best alternative to the fact that
century provides a new conduit for human talk is cheap’, it is harder to use signaling in
reproduction and mate choice. In contrast to standard online dating where everything can
online dating, the clinical sperm donor market be identified as cheap talk before the first date
allows participants access to sperm that would (see also Oyer, 2014).
be out of reach in a dating environment. Since On the other hand, both online dating and
the creation of the Internet in the early 2000s, the sperm donor market facilitate examination
online communities and organizations have of female preferences under a nonsequential
developed in which reproductive tissue can be choice mechanism, which insights should
donated directly between participants, free of increase our understanding of the decision-
clinical regulation. The Internet sperm market, making process in contexts characterized by
particularly, not being limited in preferred trait increased information and choice (Whyte
availability like private sperm banks, offers a and Torgler, 2015). In fact, one comparative
much larger choice set with greater potential study has already provided evidence that the
for obtaining the desired characteristics. norms that guide behavior offline hold true in
At the same time, it remains unclear to what many online environments (McLaughlin and
extent the mate-selection process is driven by Vitak, 2011), probably because both environ-
the female’s desire for her offspring to have ments require major life-event decisions that
traits similar to the male, and to what degree force individuals to identify their true prefer-
she selects skills that help to increase the like- ences prior to making a selection. Whereas
lihood of success in the raising and survival dating choices may be a frequent occurrence,
of her offspring (Whyte and Torgler, 2015, choosing a sperm donor tends to be a rare life
2016a). Examining sperm donation can throw experience (Waynforth and Dunbar, 1995;
new light on this dichotomy because it allows Pawłowski and Dunbar, 1999).
isolation of the male traits that genetically
impact his offspring from other factors like
parenting assistance (Scheib, 1997; Whyte
and Torgler, 2015, 2016b). One pertinent MATE-SELECTION INSTITUTIONS
question is whether factors such as resources, AND TECHNOLOGIES
power, and prestige (Flinn, 1986; Townsend,
1989; Mulder, 1990; Pérusse, 1993; Li et al., Traditional Matchmaker
2002) – all of which help to increase offspring
survival by providing the female with needed Only as late as the mid 18th to mid 20th cen-
assistance – are less relevant in this context tury did love-based arrangements replace
because genetic advantage matters more. In arranged or patriarchal marriages, whose
482 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

pervasiveness in hunter-gatherer cultures The details are then cross-checked with the person’s
(Walker et al., 2011; Apostolou, 2014; Fletcher friends, teachers, parents, business associates, and
local rabbi. It is not enough to know that a boy
et al., 2015) implies deep roots in human his-
went to a good rabbinical college. Mr Smith will
tory (Walker et al., 2011). In fact, historically, also want to enquire how he behaved there. Did he
marriage was often a means of building per- miss class? How did he spend his time? Was he
sonal networks to enhance access to hunting, ambitious? It is expected that each prospective
natural resources, or water holes, for example. partner will double-check the accuracy of this
information. This is usually done through similar
In other words, such unions were a way to
channels to the ones used by the Shadchan himself.
extend cooperative relations: ‘When people Mr Smith explains that before the couple meet he
married into new groups, it turned strangers wants to be at least 60% sure that the match will
into relatives and enemies into allies’ (Coontz, be successful. Once the candidate is screened, his/
2006: 59). For assistance overcoming the chal- her name will be filed along with the other names
in the file until a suitable partner is found. When a
lenges in forming such alliances, humans have
suitable partner is found a monitored courtship is
traditionally turned to a variety of agents or initiated. (Rockman, 1994: 282)
institutions for help in finding a mate.
Traditional matchmakers were often spirit- The couple are first seen individually by Mr
ual or religious leaders or respected elders in Smith who informs them of their date’s attrib-
the community (Finkel et al., 2012), such as utes and instructs them regarding ‘dating’
the shadchan of the Jewish community, who behavior. For example, he will remind the man
has a long tradition in Jewish life. A role his- that he is expected to pay for expenses on the
torically entrusted only to the most learned date and not do anything that will make him
and respectable men, the shadchan provided appear to lack confidence (e.g., bite his nails).
a unifying and stabilizing influence among The girl is encouraged to be ladylike. They are
the scattered Jewish diaspora during its then sent on their first date. The couple are
oppression and persecution in Europe in the expected to report back to the Smiths and to
13th and 14th centuries (Rockman, 1994). declare their intention to continue the relation-
The matchmaking services were necessary ship or terminate it. Mr Smith has a policy of
because the Jewish orthodox community, asking the boy’s opinion first so that if there is
despite its strong emphasis on finding a mate any rejection to be ‘inflicted’, he can let the
and creating a long-lasting family, mandates girl down gently. He believes that men are
almost complete segregation between the emotionally stronger than women and can take
sexes from puberty until marriage (Rockman, rejection in their stride.
1994). Rockman offers a detailed and color-
ful account of the procedural aspects of these There is no specific limit to the number of dates
allowed. However, friendship alone is not consid-
services, including the searching, matching,
ered to be a justified reason for meeting. It is up to
and interacting rules that are also essential in the shadchan to make sure that the relationship is
modern matching technologies: advancing in a particular direction. That direction is
towards the wedding canopy. Hence prolonged
a man or woman or their parents will approach Mr courtship is discouraged. There are some branches
Smith [the shadchan] with a request for him to find of orthodox Judaism where the couple are allowed
them a mate. He and his wife then conduct an to meet only once for one hour and then decide
interview with the client in question, lasting for whether they wish to marry.
about half an hour. During this time the couple
need to assess qualities such as sincerity, educa- If the couple decide to marry they are expected to
tion, family background, and the quality of the ‘look after’ the shadchan. There is no fixed fee but
person’s relationships with others. They will also be the belief is that a significant sum will bring the
looking at physical attributes (a man will always be couple good fortune in their relationship. The
expected to be taller than his prospective mate), shadchan may give the money to charity as does
sense of humor, financial prospects, and degree of Mr Smith, or use it in other ways. The shadchan is,
religious feeling. of course, invited to the wedding. Mr Smith
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND REPRODUCTION 483

explained that the business of matchmaking is adaptive desire for more options to ­eliminate
growing more difficult as people progress and the risk of incest and increase variety (Lenton
adapt to the modern world. Demands for the ‘per-
et  al., 2010), our limited cognition (Simon,
fect’ partner are increasing. Few are prepared to
compromise as they may have done in past genera- 1956) is used to handling only a few sequen-
tions. Despite this Mrs Smith told me she believed tial mate choices (Miller and Todd, 1998;
there is a match for everyone, it’s just a matter of Lenton et  al., 2010). Thus, the mechanism
bringing them together. She advises against match- that produced a successful solution in the
ing couples who both have psychiatric histories in
evolutionary past may induce (overload)
the family. She does see, however, that a match
between a strong healthy person and a troubled problems in today’s overabundant dating
person may be successful. All the information con- environment, especially given the individual
cerning illness is not always presented before the tendency to underestimate how daunting
couple meet. The matchmaker may decide that it increased choice can become (Iyengar and
will affect the success of the date and will only
Lepper, 2000; Lenton et al., 2010). It would
reveal the information at a more advanced stage of
the proceedings. (Rockman, 1994: 282–283) thus be useful for future research to include a
comparative assessment of whether narrow-
The shadchan therefore serves an important ing the market (Schwartz, 2004) increases the
function in putting potential mates in contact matching success rate.
and ensuring their unilateral awareness in the In Western countries such as the United
hope of mutual attraction and interest. It is States, mate selection before World War II
this unilateral awareness, together with con- was dominated by active family engagement
tact and mutuality, that is at the core of any in identifying a pool of potential mates within
matching agency or technology because fac- the neighborhood, the church congregation,
tors related to the fear of rejection or conflict- or among primary and secondary schoolmates
ing desires may lead individuals to search for (Rosenfeld and Thomas, 2012). Then, after
a partner like themselves (Folkes, 1982). Just World War II and up until the early 2010’s
as the solid reputation of the shadchan en­abled friends overtook family as the key resource
contact without risks in traditional communi- for finding both male and female partners
ties, the anonymity and distance inherent in (Rosenfeld and Thomas, 2012, Rosenfeld
modern technologies assure similar protection et al., 2019). Over the second half of the 20th
(Schnarch, 1997). Just like the shadchan, century, the workplace grew in importance
matching models (such those formulated in as a social milieu in which to meet a roman-
1962 by economists Gale and Shapley) take tic partner but has faded as an option in the
care of the search process at the core of mate Internet era (Rosenfeld and Thomas, 2012). In
selection (Rosenfeld and Thomas, 2012) and fact, growing sensitivity about sexual harass-
provide substantial information about the ment has reversed the view of it as an ideal
potential mate’s relative value. In this way, environment in which to form intimate rela-
new technologies help the shadchan open the tionships (Barraket and Henry-Waring, 2008).
‘bidding’ between potential partners
(Pawłowski and Koziel, 2002: 140).
New web technologies have also dramati-
Singles Ads and Video Dating
cally reduced search costs and allowed indi-
viduals to find mates outside of their existing In recent years, not only have delayed mar-
social network, although geographic prox- riage, changed marital expectations, shifting
imity is still important for the eventual goal sexual norms, birth control, and the search
of face-to-face interaction (Rosenfeld and for emotionally and intellectually gratifying
Thomas, 2012). On the other hand, a large partnerships created a singles boom (Ahuvia
pool of potential mates can make the selec- and Adelman, 1992), but growing career
tion task overwhelming because despite an pressures, increasing time poverty, and higher
484 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

mobility have reduced opportunities for research into personal ads, which has the
social activity and the sustenance of intimate great advantage of going beyond the labora-
relationships. As a result, new institutions tory setting to study the behavior of a wider
and technologies have emerged to help indi- sample of anonymous individuals under
viduals deal with the challenges of mate less artificial circumstances (Minervini and
choice and selection. In the late 1970s, these McAndrew, 2006). Lynn and Bolig (1985),
took the form of personal ads in the singles in their detailed discussion of the positive
columns of newspapers, becoming an estab- aspects of analyzing personal ads, empha-
lished feature that was heavily discussed in size this ability to explore real behavior in a
magazines and talk shows (Ahuvia and naturalistic setting with a more representa-
Adelman, 1992). However, because of a per- tive sample and longer-term consequences,
sistent stigma, throughout the 1980s (just as which ensures greater generalizability than
in the 1960s), only a small fraction of indi- laboratory-based behavioral observations. In
viduals (around 1% in the United States) met fact, before the inception of online dating,
via ‘the personals’ (Finkel et al., 2012). increasing the external validity of the results
An alternative means of meeting a mate, was a core goal of scholars working with
one offered by a small number of third-party laboratory data.
intermediaries, was video dating, in which Because placing or answering personal ads
a client first read a written description of can be seen as a form of courtship (Goode,
potential dates (most including an image) 1996), related studies have explored the rela-
and then decided which of the 2–10 minute tive importance of various factors using both
videos to watch. If the client made a choice, bogus (e.g., Sitton and Blanchard, 1995;
the selected person was provided with the cli- Goode, 1996) and actual advertisements
ent’s video (Ahuvia and Adelman, 1992), and (e.g., Lynn and Shurgot, 1984). After analyz-
if both agreed to meet, phone numbers were ing the ad content, these studies linked it with
given. Video dating provided an opportu- the number of responses received (Lynn and
nity to prescreen dates, furnish more natural Bolig, 1985) as a viable measure of success
information, and present a more purposeful (Pawłowski and Dunbar, 2001). Some studies
image by investing a great deal of effort in also compared the content with information
building a profile (Woll and Young, 1989). from the ad placers or explored the number
Videos also allowed exploration of nonver- of ads as a dependent variable with which to
bal communication in courtship settings, measure interpersonal relations (Ahuvia and
including the body language that is such an Adelman, 1992). Research into video dating
important source of mate-choice information has focused on similar questions and found
(Renninger et  al., 2004). A distinguishing comparable insights (Ahuvia and Adelman,
feature of the singles ads and videotapes was 1992). One research aspect that remains
the awarding of more personal control over underdeveloped, however, pertains to envi-
both the pace and nature of the interaction – ronmental shifts like legislative change, eco-
as well as the information provided (Woll nomic conditions, media events, and cultural
and Young, 1989) – an ability also facilitated holidays (Lynn and Bolig, 1985: 382), whose
by online interaction via new technologies relevance for the technological institutions
(Barraket and Henry-Waring, 2008). discussed in this chapter makes them a prom-
Research into the efficacy of these ising subject for further analysis. In particu-
resources has traditionally been conducted lar, if the event studied were exogeneous,
in a laboratory setting with a subject pool it could be treated as a natural experiment,
of college and university students asked to which would allow the use of methodologies
respond to hypothetical mating situations. able to address causality issues and identify
One exception is evolutionary psychological confounding factors.
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND REPRODUCTION 485

One notable finding from extant studies room. Organizers often provide participants
is that women are concerned with finding an with homogenous questions to ask potential
older partner with established resources (e.g., suitors, as well as score cards with which to
education and income or wealth), while men rate potential partners across a range of char-
prefer young, physically attractive women acteristics. Nonetheless, although popular
(see, e.g., Greenlees and McGrew, 1994; and still widely practiced (with commercial
Goode, 1996; Pawłowski and Koziel, 2002; speed-dating companies now running the
Minervini and McAndrew, 2006). On the events), speed dating’s participatory con-
other hand, in a study in Sweden, although straints (in-person attendance, limited
females were substantially more focused on number of potential mates, possible financial
resources than males, both cared about physi- cost, and potential anxiety about in-person
cal attractiveness (Gustavsson et  al., 2008), contact) have prevented it reaching the same
a particularly interesting finding given that level of popularity as online dating.
Scandinavian countries have high levels of The research into speed dating was part
economic equality between the sexes (Buss, of the shift that moved studies of attractive-
2012). Even when women greatly outnum- ness or relationships beyond the laboratory
ber men, as in a pool of dating-service par- into ‘live interactional settings’ (Korobov,
ticipants in Tel Aviv (646 males to 1,000 2011: 186). Methodologically, the speed-
females), rather than being less choosy or dating design is particularly valuable because
lowering their expectations, women are more it allows separation of the actor effect from
selective than males in most areas (Bokek- the partner effect (i.e., from ‘how do I behave
Cohen et  al., 2008). Such settings are espe- toward others?’ to ‘which behavior do I evoke
cially useful in allowing the differentiation in others?’; Asendorpf et al., 2011: 16). It can
of various competing theories – in this case, thus be seen as a microcosm of sequential
rational choice versus evolutionary psychol- mate choice in daily life but in a faster and
ogy, a comparative study type still underrep- more formalized manner (Todd et al., 2007).
resented in the literature. One consistent finding from this research
Research has also examined mail-order stream is that fundamental sex differences
brides, who represent an effort to vastly in mate selection continue to manifest even
broaden the opportunities afforded by tradi- in such modern forms of dating (Roberts
tional matchmaking by expanding the pool et  al., 2010; Buss, 2012). For example, one
of eligible mates – and thus the range of of the earliest speed-dating studies – a com-
choices – across national borders. The prior- pilation of observational data and additional
ity among one set of potential brides from survey information – provides strong sup-
Colombia, the Philippines, and Russia was to port for women being more selective than
find older males with access to resources and men (Kurzban and Weeden, 2005). This
a commitment to share them (Minervini and finding is confirmed experimentally using
McAndrew, 2006), which supports evolution- a sample of nearly 400 Colombia graduate
ary explanations of female mate preferences. students, among whom the females placed
greater importance on intelligence, while the
males were more likely to respond to physi-
cal attractiveness (Fisman et al., 2006). Men
Speed Dating
who grew up in affluent neighborhoods also
Speed dating, which first came to prominence had a comparative advantage.
in Los Angeles in the 1990s (Finkel et  al., In general, speed-dating research has
2007), involves a group of individuals meet- shown that female selectivity is more malle-
ing at one location and spending recursive able or context-dependent than that of males;
intervals of time with potential partners in the for instance, across 22 speed-dating events
486 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

with 546 participants, less attractive females and Henry-Waring, 2008). Services such as
and those with more irregular BMI were ScientificMatch and GenePartner even prom-
overall less selective at events characterized ise matchings based on genetic information
by higher intrasexual competition (Overbeek (Frazzetto, 2010). Moreover, recent decades
et al., 2013). Nonetheless, stated preferences have witnessed the emergence of myriad
do not necessarily match actual choices in variations, including free and paid services,
either the offline (Todd et al., 2007) or online dating applications linked to social media
(Whyte and Torgler, 2017a) setting. Rather, accounts and specific groups, and special
women tend to make more discriminating focus platforms ranging from participant
choices (Todd et al., 2006), which is again in sexuality and sexual behaviors to race, cul-
line with evolutionary models of human mat- ture, interests, occupation, and religious and
ing based on parental investment theory. political views.
As a result, over the past 20 years, behav-
ioral science has begun to explore human
Online Dating mating behavior in this unique new cyber
market across the gamut of scientific dis-
According to a nationally representative ciplines, including social and evolutionary
2017 survey in the United States, online psychology, economics, sociology, demogra-
dating has become the most popular way for phy, personality psychology, communication
heterosexual couples to meet (Rosenfeld research, and cyber-psychology. Scholars
et  al., 2019), although websites facilitating working on this topic emphasize the power of
online dating did not rise to prominence until online dating data given their basis in actual
after the 1990s proliferation of the informa- observed preferences, which, although also
tion superhighway. Date-matching software, characteristic of speed-dating experiments,
however, dates back to a 1959 Stanford class is contrary to the stated preferences used in
project when two electrical engineering mate-preference studies (e.g., Todd et  al.,
undergraduates wrote a matching program 2007; Finkel and Eastwick, 2008; Hitsch
that generated difference scores for each pos- et  al., 2010b). These latter, by asking part-
sible male–female pair based on their survey ners to infer backward what traits originally
responses, with lowest scores representing attracted them to each other, are made prob-
the best match and so on up the scale. In lematic by memory fallibility (Todd et  al.,
general, these students observed that both 2007), cognitive dissonance, or even self-
men and women were keen to find mates deception. On the other hand, online daters
with similar habits (Gillmor, 2007). may admittedly struggle to obtain sufficient
Although individuals register on these information on the experiential attributes
electronic platforms by building a personal (e.g., sense of humor) that are relevant when
profile – somewhat like that compiled by determining whether or not one person likes
the traditional matchmaker – the platforms another (Frost et al., 2008).
are less restrictive than either speed dat- One advantageous development for this
ing or everyday personal contacts, and are research is the increased use of dating apps,
not institutionally prestructured or selective which provide researchers with a valuable
(Skopek et al., 2011). Whereas some online opportunity to work either with a representa-
dating sites allow searches based on specifi- tive sample of the population or specific sub-
cally chosen characteristics (e.g., RSVP) or groups. As a result, researchers have been
site-provided personality-matching systems able to explore a plethora of online mate-
(e.g., eHarmony), others are based on social choice psychology and behaviors using very
networks that encourage users to bring in large populations, such as the over 22,000
new members (e.g., Friendster; Barraket online daters in Boston and San Diego studied
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND REPRODUCTION 487

by Hitsch et al. (2010a, 2010b). Using 2003 (Whyte and Torgler, 2017b). Likewise, a
data for a three-and-a-half-month period, Chinese study using over half a million online
these researchers not only documented a dater profiles found that women attached
female preference for income over physical greater importance than men to a potential
attributes, but also, as in other studies, a strong partner’s socioeconomic status (Su and Hu,
preference in both sexes for homogamy across 2019). Such female preferences tend to be
most choice characteristics (e.g., education, more refined during the women’s years of
ethnicity, marital status, religion, height, maximum fertility, after which their demand
smoking habits, and political views). specificity decreases with age. Male speci-
The research on such electronic mate- ficity, on the other hand, remains stable until
choice settings therefore seems to mirror the their 40s, when it becomes greater than that
findings of noncyber mate-choice research, of post-reproductive females, meaning that
which has identified key sex differences in it is at its highest during their peak career-
stated preferences, choices, and behaviors. earnings potential (Whyte et al., 2018). Also
For example, Skopek et  al.’s (2011) study consistent with evolutionary theory, research
of over 10,000 German online daters (with a focused specifically on the dating behavior of
biased sex ratio of 65.42% males) showed an older adults found that whereas men’s choices
average of 12.75 contacts for men compared are more consistent with evolutionary predic-
to 8.02 for women. These dater contact- tions, it is women who define the choices,
initiation data also indicated that the males implying a greater sensitivity to demographic
contacted females who were on average realities (Alterovitz and Mendelsohn, 2009).
4.76 years younger than themselves, whereas The pervasive modern use of the Internet
the females contacted males who were on also now assures that cyber populations are
average 2.74 years older. Although these age increasingly representative of the offline pop-
preferences shift with age for both sexes, they ulation (Valkenburg and Peter, 2007), which
do so differently for men and women, with reduces selection bias in online samples. That
the latter’s becoming generally more hetero- said, high-quality mates are scarce in any mar-
geneous with age (Skopek et al., 2011: 267). ket, and competition both between and within
Fiore et  al. (2010) similarly demonstrated the sexes is fierce. Hence, the anonymity
that among 11,600 active online daters, of the Internet may strongly encourage decep-
even though men outnumbered women sig- tive strategies to secure the highest-value mate
nificantly (57.9% male; 42.1% female), possible, just as in real life females enhance
almost four out of every five (77.1%) con- their physical beauty with make-up and males
tact initiations were by men toward women, wear shoe lifts or a hairpiece. In fact, the lower
with women receiving a median number of the personal attractiveness, the higher the
two contacts but men only 0.5. Moreover, probability of individuals enhancing their pro-
whereas men expressed a preference in their file photographs to enhance height, weight,
profiles for an average 9.1 characteristics in a or age characteristics while not extending
mate, women specified 11.9. such deception to non-aesthetic profile char-
As regards specific characteristics, accord- acteristics like income and occupation (Toma
ing to one Australian study of over 40,000 and Hancock, 2010). Yet sex differences are
online daters (Whyte et  al., 2018), women also observable in the characteristics likely
across all age groups (18–80 years) were to be embellished during profile building,
more likely to state a preference for a particu- with males more prone to exaggerate height,
lar education level in a potential mate, which but women more likely to understate weight
preferences were more likely to be homog- (Toma et al., 2008). Humans can even be mis-
enous (i.e., positively assorted) or at least led by their own self-deception in that online
have a lower minimum than those of males daters’ desired or preferred characteristics
488 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

in an ‘ideal partner’ do not always mirror An ideal conceptual framework for such
those of the individuals they actually contact investigation would be the ‘integration of
(Whyte and Torgler, 2017a). evolutionary psychology and cultural psy-
Needless to say, an individual’s personal- chology’, which Buss et al. (2001: 502–503)
ity also plays a role in how attractive they consider the ‘hallmark of modern evolution-
are – and how well they project themselves – ary science’.
to possible mates. For example, whereas Because evolution and changes in norms
Valkenburg and Peter (2007) showed that and cultural systems provide a rich avenue
those with low (vs high) dating anxiety are for understanding interpersonal interactions
more active in the online marketplace, Whyte and mating choices, future research within
et al. (2018) identified a positive linear rela- this theoretical context could provide valu-
tion between levels of extraversion and sexual able additional insights into the dynamics
frequency for both sexes. In this latter study, of communication, self-expression, types of
greater variance in male personality traits courting strategies, and characteristics (e.g.,
translated to men – but not women – having level of emotionality or aggressiveness). For
more sex and more children. On the other instance, institutional and environmental
hand, a German longitudinal study found that changes – particularly when exogenous –
those with less extrovert personalities were might serve as natural experiments, enabling
more likely to meet online, perhaps because the use of methodologies that can iden-
the Internet provides a more passive way to tify confounding factors and discriminate
initiate communication with potential mates. between different theories and alternative
Just as homogamy or positive assortment hypotheses. Likewise, informal institutional
is overwhelmingly the observed mate-choice factors such as trust and social exchange
behavior across cultures in real life (Buss, could provide valuable new insights into
1985), so the ecology of Internet mating human behavior in the mating or matching
appears no different. Not only did Whyte and markets. It is thus well worth exploring in
Torgler’s (2017b) analysis of around 220,000 more detail how evolutionary psychology
contact initiations reveal that more educated or biology could contribute to the design
online daters were more likely to positively and management of institutional changes
assort and less likely to contact those with that benefit individuals and societies by, for
lower educational levels, it also demonstrated example, increasing their levels of happiness
that, overall, males and females exhibit simi- (Piazza and Bering, 2009; Wilson, 2018).
lar preferences as they age. Admittedly,
however, most such research has been con-
ducted in OECD countries, raising a need
for detailed future studies of mate-choice REPRODUCTIVE DONATION
behavior in other regions to assess the robust-
ness of current results for the online environ- Reproductive Medicine and
ment. More cross-cultural evidence is also Donation
required to better understand human decision
rules when forming preference and choices, Although reproductive technologies offer an
including whether other societies employ the exceptional way to reproduce, become a
same ‘likes-attract’ rule common in Western parent, and construct a family, they also chal-
cultures (Buston and Emlen, 2003), particu- lenge the historical foundations of how
larly in evolutionary relevant traits (Thomas humans reproduce and cooperate to raise
et  al., 2019), which may help explain why offspring (Hargreaves, 2006), which for mil-
homogamous marriages are more common lennia has required the physical act of sex.
and successful than nonhomogamous ones. Late-20th-century advances in ART and IVF,
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND REPRODUCTION 489

however, have changed (and in part removed) in the related choices and behaviors. ART has
the reproductive need for physical copula- also provided evolutionary science a unique
tion. Although the first case of human artifi- opportunity to explore female reproductive
cial insemination dates back to around 1790 behaviors in a way that previously was not
when John Hunter successfully inseminated3 possible. Historically, internal fertilization
a patient with her husband’s sperm, it was in and gestation were mandatory for women to
the last decades of the 19th century that reproduce; medical science has now released
norms tying sex to procreation relaxed. The women from such constraint. As previous
first donor insemination was performed evolutionary axioms such as Trivers’ 1972
(quite unethically) in 1884 by Philadelphia parental investment theory relied on women
physician William Pancoast, who, having experiencing the greater opportunity cost (the
found the woman’s husband sterile from an physiological burden) of reproduction, it is
earlier bout of gonorrhea, procured semen unclear exactly what this fundamental change
from the medical student deemed handsomest means for sex-differentiated behavior based
in the class, anesthetized her, and inserted the on ‘female-choice’ constraints that may no
sperm via syringe, resulting in a successful longer be relevant. If modern reproduction via
pregnancy. Although Pancoast failed to donation only requires a resource-endowed
inform the couple at that time of the sperm participant and a willing donor or surrogate,
donor’s involvement, when another physician many previous mate-choice theories related
brought the case to light in 1909, Pancoast to sexual selection may need to be reexam-
confessed the truth and was relieved when the ined. In addition, one could also explore in
husband responded positively to the news, more detail the psychology and behavior of
albeit deciding that his wife should never kin not raised by their biological parents. Just
know the truth (Guttmacher, 1969: 567).4 as twin studies provide a unique opportunity
Clinics offering ART and IVF, as well to explore phenotypic variance on individual
as commercial markets for both sperm and development and behavior, so too do children
oocyte donation, have traditionally incen- born of the same donor parent, but raised by
tivized increased donation via one of the different (non)biological parents in differ-
two most effective motivators (Ernst et  al., ent environments. The way in which these
2007), either an altruistic (no personal gain) changes will impact and influence intergen-
or a financially remunerative model (Daniels, erational and population-level change is an
1989). The advent of the Internet, however, exciting and challenging research question.
together with the limited clinical supply of
both sperm and oocytes, has encouraged
the emergence and growing popularity of Sperm Donors and Recipients
online or informal markets for reproductive
tissue donation, which provide increased fit- Historically, males had great difficulty find-
ness opportunities for both men and women. ing and securing female mates who sought
As a result, the last decade has witnessed little or no paternal investment; most particu-
the launching of online connection web- larly because nomadic and agriculture-based
sites (analogous to dating websites) that societies were resource poor and raising off-
facilitate donor–recipient exchange glob- spring was (as ever) extremely costly. The
ally. Although explaining these services development of mutual investment in self-­
empirically as new behavioral adaptations regulated pair bonding (e.g., marriage) as a
may not be feasible given that they are only social norm thus appears eminently logical. In
a generation or two old, research can assess the current century, however, particularly in
whether previous mating market dynamics developed economies, resources are not scarce,
and underlying mechanisms are still present while historical-education, labor-market, and
490 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

legislative barriers to gender equity in resource donors are not only less altruistic than
acquisition are greatly reduced. Females are female (oocyte) donors but also more afflu-
thus no longer totally dependent on paternal ent (Schover et  al., 1992). From an evolu-
investment from males, while males no longer tionary psychological perspective, however,
need to remain in long-term monogamous sperm donation with no parental investment,
relationships to ensure increased fitness. whether spurred on by financial remunera-
From an evolutionary perspective, ceteris tion or the increased warm glow utility of
paribus on environmental condition (Quinlan, giving freely, is arguably the most selfish
2007), the optimal male reproductive strat- and strategic way a male can increase his
egy is to mate and reproduce with as many fitness. Indeed, research has confirmed that
females as possible – thereby realizing the males acknowledge the drive to reproduce or
greatest fitness gains – which requires no procreate as a key motivating factor in their
more investment than the time to copulate. donation decision (Woestenburg et al., 2016).
Females, in contrast, still bear the higher Yet the bulk of the qualitative social science
opportunity costs of gestation, nursing, child- literature in this area lumps unremunerated
rearing, and other disproportionately demand- donation under the blanket term of altruism
ing parental investment, even though many in even when males acknowledge no need to
developed economies no longer rely on male contribute toward parental investment and the
paternal investment. Hence, for the first time fitness gain of prolific offspring (Bergen and
in human history, males can and are encour- Delacroix, 2018).
aged to provide their reproductive tissue to as A more advantageous research focus is
many different women as possible purely for that on females in search of a sperm donor,
the purpose of reproduction and free of any which, by enabling the differentiation of
parental investment constraint. This commer- trait preferences from partnership factors,
cial donation market, with its two very differ- creates a controlled setting that isolates a
ent motivators (i.e., altruistic versus financial) male’s genetic impact on his offspring from
and its culturally varying legislation on donor other characteristics. This separation in turn
anonymity, thus presents a unique opportunity permits the isolation of aspects that reduce
to explore both men’s and women’s prefer- anticipated problems in child rearing via par-
ences and reproductive decisions free of his- enting assistance and improved environmen-
torical resource constraints. tal conditions for the offspring (Whyte and
One aspect examined to date by much of Torgler, 2015). According to experimental
the (nonevolutionary) scientific research on research, when assessing sperm donor char-
sperm donation is the motivation for altru- acteristics, women tend to rely on the same
istic nonpaid donation given that donors psychology used to choose a long-term mate
have no opportunity to designate their sperm (Scheib, 1994), with single (vs partnered)
for particular groups or populations or to women seeming to place higher value on
exclude specific categories or characteristics male proxies for resources and good charac-
(Pennings, 1995). In fact, according to one ter (Rodino et al., 2011). Such findings may
survey of Danish sperm donors active across also reflect females’ evolved adaptive prefer-
the last three decades, although altruism was ence for males who can compensate their dis-
the most frequently reported motivator, large proportionate reproductive investment with
numbers of respondents admitted that they other resources (Trivers, 1972), as reflected
would not donate if economic compensation by a preference for higher donor levels of
were removed (Bay et  al., 2014). Clinical education (Whyte et  al., 2016). Moreover,
survey data have confirmed this male ten- in line with previous mate-choice research,
dency to be motivated by financial compen- even when available choices are limited rela-
sation, while also showing that male (sperm) tive to traditional sequential dating markets,
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND REPRODUCTION 491

women still exhibit homogamous preferences potential recipients. Although at the time of
(Whyte and Torgler, 2015). writing research in this area is scarce (the set-
Donor insemination essentially operated ting being little more than a decade old), it
globally without any legal restriction until the has established that such factors as logistics,
Swedish parliament enacted the 1985 Law on health concerns, and access to information
Artificial Insemination, which granted chil- on genetic heritage matter greatly in sperm
dren born as a result of donor insemination donation environments outside fertility clinics
access to identifying information on their (Lavoie et  al., 2018: 184). These informal
donor father (Lalos et al., 2003). In 2005, the donors, although they tend to move between
UK government introduced similar legisla- clinical and informal settings, are primar-
tion (Daniels et al., 2005), after which other ily motivated to donate via the Internet by
countries followed suit, generating fierce a desire to know potential recipients and be
debate across all markets about donor ano- updated on the development of any result-
nymity and which functional model could ing offspring (Woestenburg et al., 2016). For
best regulate clinical donation. these men, who coincidentally donate to more
women and realize more offspring (Whyte
et al., 2018), motivation often relates to con-
tact and bonding with the recipient (Bossema
Online Sperm Donor Market
et al., 2014), although 94% of those with arti-
Because legislation and reform tend to lag ficially inseminated realized offspring, report
behind actual behavior, mushrooming social that sexual gratification played no role as a
media technologies have encouraged both motivating factor (Freeman et al., 2016).
donors and recipients of reproductive tissue As regards successful donor characteris-
to participate in a large but informal global tics, in addition to their age and income being
online market that is more interactive and positively correlated with number of female
less rigid than official channels (Whyte and recipients, shy, more intellectual, and/or more
Torgler, 2015). Such informal markets have organized informal donors realize more off-
emerged because of the excessive demand spring than the more outgoing (Whyte and
and inadequate supply that characterize the Torgler, 2016b), with conscientiousness also a
official sperm donor market (Yee, 2009; Van comparative advantage (Whyte et  al., 2018).
den Broeck et al., 2013; Whyte and Torgler, Given that informal donation requires a level
2015). Before the advent of this new setting, of organization, communication, and even
researchers struggled to align preferences logistical coordination (with the recipient) not
(e.g., for assortative mating) with actual con- required in clinical donation, it is not surpris-
straints in the matching process, including ing that more efficient or organized donors,
geographic limits and the social propinquity and those with more prosocial personality
and competition that substantially restricted traits, are more successful. In fact, these infor-
possibility sets and preferences. In the online mal donor behavioral traits appear more highly
sperm donor market, such limitations are valued by females than physical appearance or
reduced, and the geographic proximity even proxies for resources (Whyte and Torgler,
restriction of the online dating market mat- 2016b; Whyte et al., 2017).
ters less. Nonetheless, given the previous research
At the same time, the enactment of iden- focus on mate selection, more empirical evi-
tification laws has reduced the number of dence is needed on reproductive tissue dona-
clinical donors in traditionally anonymous tion if its selection and choice processes are to
markets (Ernst et al., 2007), prompting a sig- be fully understood. As emphasized by Buss
nificant and growing male population to turn (2012: 44–45), ‘the evaluation of evolutionary
to informal donation via coordination with formulations rests with the cumulative weight
492 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

of the evidence, and not necessarily with any providing select people and groups a com-
single prediction. Evolutionary hypotheses, parative advantage in resources, time, power,
when formulated precisely, are highly test- status, and reproduction. These advances
able and eminently capable of being falsified have dramatically changed the human spe-
when the evidence fails to support predic- cies’ ability to survive, thrive, and reproduce.
tions derived from them’. One particularly Two of the more recent and arguably most
promising avenue for discovery is the con- impactful technological leaps – stemming
nection websites that serve as a novel conduit from medical science and the information
for reproductive tissue donors and recipients, superhighway, respectively – are the devel-
free of medical and legislative regulatory opment and commercialization of assisted
oversight. It remains unclear, for example, reproductive technology and donor insemi-
exactly how participant characteristics and nation and the broadening of their availabil-
behaviors on these websites differ from those ity across global networks. Because of these
in the regulated donor insemination settings, technologies, for the first time in human his-
and more broadly from those in earlier elec- tory, females and males can pass on their
tronic mate-choice markets like online dat- genes and realize offspring to unknown indi-
ing. Compared to sperm donation research, viduals on the other side of the planet.
evidence on egg donors and recipients with Nonetheless, because sexual selection has for
and without co-parenting intention is very millennia favored those most able to identify,
limited. Connection sites also allow explora- access, and secure suitable partners, the cur-
tion of how participants initiate and receive rent impact of these technological advances
contact from other interested members, and remains unclear.
how likely they are to respond. These sites In this chapter, therefore, we have explored
also permit assessment of courtship inten- this impact on behavior and reproductive
sity and the presence of key sex differences outcomes by examining such topics as the
in profile formation, contact behavior, and online dating market and the implications of
choice processes. A comparison of sperm the Internet for nonsequential mate-choice
and egg donors would also facilitate evalua- decision making or sperm donation. In
tion of whether the former, as bearers of very doing so, we have shown ways to observe
different cost and investment levels in this human mate-choice behavior in electronic
reproductive market, are choosier and more and modern medical settings through the
selective than the latter. The reality that eggs lens of evolutionary psychology. The overall
are large, expensive, and scarce mandates conclusion drawn from this exploration is
that individuals be especially selective about that core evolutionary constructs like sexual
their source; while the small size, abundance, selection and parental investment are still
and low cost of sperm (Seabright, 2012) present and relevant in these 21st-century
afford males more mobility. In other words, reproductive behaviors. Women have evolved
males can court multiple females and more powerful preferences for long-term mate
aggressively maximize the number of poten- characteristics that help solve the adaptive
tial matings and search areas. problems of survival and reproduction.
Hence, even though newer ways of selection
seemingly achieve fundamentally different
choices from more traditional means of
CONCLUSION courtship or mate choice, evolutionarily,
men’s and women’s choices are relatively
Throughout human evolution, technological stable, showing no new responses to novel
change has been a key driver of success, technological conditions.
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND REPRODUCTION 493

Notes Bay, B., Larsen, P. B., Kesmodel, U. S., & Inger-


slev, H. J. (2014). Danish sperm donors across
1  Interestingly, a certain class of rotifers (a fresh- three decades: Motivations and attitudes.
water multicellular eukaryote) reproduce via sex
Fertility and Sterility, 101(1), 252–257.
when faced with environmental changes but
reproduce through cloning in environmentally
Becks, L., & Agrawal, A. F. (2012). The evolution
stable conditions (Greely, 2016). of sex is favoured during adaptation to new
2  This observation refers to experimental popula- environments. PLoS Biology, 10(5), e1001317.
tions of a facultatively sexual species of the hap- Bergen, N., & Delacroix, C. (2018). Bypassing
lodiploid monogonont rotifer, which changes to the sperm bank: Documenting the experi-
sexual reproduction in response to a chemical ences of online informal sperm donors. Criti-
stimulus. cal Public Health, 29(5) 1–12.
3  The writings of the Talmud discuss the possibil- Bernstein, G. (2002). The socio-legal accept-
ity of pregnancy unrelated to sexual intercourse; ance of new technologies: A close look at
namely, through accidental insemination in bath-
artificial insemination. Washington Law
water (Bernstein, 2002).
4  For a history of artificial insemination with its
Review, 77, 1035–1120.
hurdles and milestones, see Ombelet and Van Bokek-Cohen, Y., Peres, Y., & Kanazawa, S.
Robays (2015) and Foote (2002). (2008). Rational choice and evolutionary
psychology as explanations for mate selectiv-
ity. Journal of Social, Evolutionary, and Cul-
tural Psychology, 2(2), 42–55.
Bossema, E. R., Janssens, P. M., Treucker, R. G.,
REFERENCES Landwehr, F., van Duinen, K., Nap, A. W., &
Geenen, R. (2014). An inventory of reasons
Ahuvia, A. C., & Adelman, M. B. (1992). for sperm donation in formal versus informal
Formal intermediaries in the marriage settings. Human Fertility, 17(1), 21–27.
market: A typology and review. Journal of Boyer, P. (2018). Minds make societies: How
Marriage and the Family, 54(2), 452–463. cognition explains the world humans create.
Alterovitz, S. S.-R., & Mendelsohn, G. A. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
(2009). Partner preferences across the life Bribiescas, R. G. (2001). Reproductive ecology
span: Online dating by older adults. Psychol- and life history of the human male. Ameri-
ogy and Aging, 24(2), 513–517. can Journal of Physical Anthropology,
Apostolou, M. (2014). Sexual selection under 116(S33), 148–176.
parental choice: A revision to the model. Brown, C., Garwood, M. P., & Williamson, J. E.
Theory in Biosciences, 133(2), 111–115. (2012). It pays to cheat: Tactical deception in
Asendorpf, J. B., Penke, L., & Back, M. D. a cephalopod social signalling system.
(2011). From dating to mating and relating: Biology Letters, 8(5), 729–732.
Predictors of initial and long-term outcomes of Burley, N. (1983). The meaning of assortative
speed-dating in a community sample. Euro- mating. Ethology and Sociobiology, 4(4),
pean Journal of Personality, 25(1), 16–30. 191–203.
Ball, P. (2019). How to grow a human: Adven- Buss, D. M. (1985). Human mate selection:
tures in how we are made and who we are. Opposites are sometimes said to attract, but
Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. in fact we are likely to marry someone who
Barraket, J., & Henry-Waring, M. S. (2008). is similar to us in almost every variable.
Getting it on (line): Sociological perspectives American Scientist, 73(1), 47–51.
on e-dating. Journal of Sociology, 44(2), Buss, D. M. (1989). Sex differences in human
149–165. mate preferences: Evolutionary hypotheses
Barrett, L. T., Evans, J. P., & Gasparini, C. tested in 37 cultures. Behavioral and Brain
(2014). The effects of perceived mating Sciences, 12(1), 1–14.
opportunities on patterns of reproductive Buss, D. M. (1995). Psychological sex differ-
investment by male guppies. PLoS One, 9(4), ences: Origins through sexual selection.
e93780. American Psychologist, 50, 164–168.
494 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Buss, D. (2012). Evolutionary psychology: The 2010 43rd Hawaii International Conference
new science of the mind. Pearson: University on System Sciences (pp. 1–10). IEEE.
of Texas at Austin. Fisman, R., Iyengar, S. S., Kamenica, E., &
Buss, D.M., Abbott, M., Angleitner, A., Ashe- Simonson, I. (2006). Gender differences in
rian, A., Biaggio, A., Blanco-Villasenor, A., & mate selection: Evidence from a speed dating
Ekehammar, B. (1990). International prefer- experiment. Quarterly Journal of Economics,
ences in selecting mates: A study of 37 cul- 121(2), 673–697.
tures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, Fletcher, G. J., Simpson, J. A., Campbell, L., &
21(1), 5–47. Overall, N. C. (2015). Pair-bonding, romantic
Buss, D. M., Shackelford, T. K., Kirkpatrick, L. love, and evolution: The curious case of
A., & Larsen, R. J. (2001). A half century of Homo sapiens. Perspectives on Psychological
mate preferences: The cultural evolution of Science, 10(1), 20–36.
values. Journal of Marriage and Family, Flinn, M. V. (1986). Correlates of reproductive
63(2), 491–503. success in a Caribbean village. Human Ecol-
Buston, P. M., & Emlen, S. T. (2003). Cognitive ogy, 14(2), 225–243.
processes underlying human mate choice: Folkes, V. S. (1982). Forming relationships and
The relationship between self-perception the matching hypothesis. Personality and
and mate preference in Western society. Social Psychology Bulletin, 8(4), 631–636.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sci- Foote, R. H. (2010). The history of artificial
ences, 100(15), 8805–8810. insemination: Selected notes and notables.
Carroll, L. (1917). Through the looking glass: Journal of Animal Science, 80, 1–10.
And what Alice found there. Rand, McNally. Frazzetto, G. (2010). The science of online
Coontz, S. (2006). Marriage, a history: How love dating. EMBO Reports, 11(1), 25–27.
conquered marriage. New York: Penguin. Freeman, T., Jadva, V., Tranfield, E., & Golom-
Daniels, K. R. (1989). Semen donors: Their bok, S. (2016). Online sperm donation: A
motivations and attitudes to their offspring. survey of the demographic characteristics,
Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychol- motivations, preferences and experiences of
ogy, 7(2), 121–127. sperm donors on a connection website.
Daniels, K., Blyth, E., Crawshaw, M., & Curson, Human Reproduction, 31(9), 2082–2089.
R. (2005). Previous semen donors and their Frost, J. H., Chance, Z., Norton, M. I., & Ariely,
views regarding the sharing of information D. (2008). People are experience goods:
with offspring. Human Reproduction, 20(6), Improving online dating with virtual dates.
1670–1675. Journal of Interactive Marketing, 22(1), 51–61.
Dunbar, R. (2012). The science of love and Gale, D., & Shapley, L. S. (1962). College
betrayal. London: Faber & Faber. admissions and the stability of marriage. The
Ernst, E., Jakob Ingerslev, H., Schou, O., & American Mathematical Monthly, 69(1),
Stoltenberg, M. (2007). Attitudes among 9–15.
sperm donors in 1992 and 2002: A Danish Gangestad, S. W., & Buss, D. M. (1993). Patho-
questionnaire survey. Acta Obstetricia et gen prevalence and human mate prefer-
Gynecologica Scandinavica, 86(3), 327–333. ences. Ethology and Sociobiology, 14(2),
Finkel, E. J., & Eastwick, P. W. (2008). Speed- 89–96.
dating. Current Directions in Psychological Gangestad, S. W., Thornhill, R., & Yeo, R. A.
Science, 17(3), 193–197. (1994). Facial attractiveness, developmental
Finkel, E. J., Eastwick, P. W., & Matthews, J. stability, and fluctuating asymmetry.
(2007). Speed-dating as an invaluable tool Ethology and Sociobiology, 15(2), 73–85.
for studying romantic attraction: A method- Gillmor, C. S. (2007). Stanford, the IBM 650,
ological primer. Personal Relationships, and the first trials of computer date match-
14(1), 149–166. ing. IEEE Annals of the History of Comput-
Fiore, A. T., Taylor, L. S., Zhong, X., Mendel- ing, 29(1), 74–80.
sohn, G. A., & Cheshire, C. (2010, January). Goode, E. (1996). Gender and courtship
Who’s right and who writes: People, profiles, entitlement: Responses to personal ads. Sex
contacts, and replies in online dating. In Roles, 34(3–4), 141–169.
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND REPRODUCTION 495

Grammer, K. (1989). Human courtship behav- of human courtship: Qualifying the parental
iour: Biological basis and cognitive processing. investment model. Journal of Personality,
Sociobiology of Sexual and Reproductive 58(1), 97–116.
Strategies, 147, 169. Korobov, N. (2011). Mate-preference talk in
Greely, H. T. (2016). The end of sex and the speed-dating conversations. Research on Lan-
future of human reproduction. Cambridge, guage and Social Interaction, 44(2), 186–209.
MA: Harvard University Press. Kurzban, R., & Weeden, J. (2005). HurryDate:
Greenlees, I. A., & McGrew, W. C. (1994). Sex Mate preferences in action. Evolution and
and age differences in preferences and tac- Human Behavior, 26(3), 227–244.
tics of mate attraction: Analysis of published Lalos, A., Daniels, K., Gottlieb, C., & Lalos, O.
advertisements. Ethology and Sociobiology, (2003). Recruitment and motivation of
15(2), 59–72. semen providers in Sweden. Human Repro-
Gustavsson, L., Johnsson, J. I., & Uller, T. duction, 18(1), 212–216.
(2008). Mixed support for sexual selection Lavoie, K., Côté, I., & de Montigny, F. (2018).
theories of mate preferences in the Swedish Assisted reproduction in the digital age: Sto-
population. Evolutionary Psychology, 6(4), ries of Canadian sperm donors offering their
575–585. gametes online via introduction websites. The
Guttmacher, A. F. (1969). Artificial insemina- Journal of Men’s Studies, 26(2), 184–202.
tion. DePaul Law Review, 118(2), 566–583. Lenton, A. P., Fasolo, B., & Todd, P. M. (2010).
Hadany, L., & Comeron, J. M. (2008). Why are Who is in your shopping cart? Expected and
sex and recombination so common? Annals of experienced effects of choice abundance in
the New York Academy of Sciences, 1133(1), the online dating context. In N. Kock (Ed.),
26–43. Evolutionary Psychology and Information
Hargreaves, K. (2006). Constructing families Systems Research (pp. 149–167). Boston,
and kinship through donor insemination. MA: Springer.
Sociology of Health & Illness, 28(3), 261–283. Li, N. P., Bailey, J. M., Kenrick, D. T., &
Hinsz, V. B. (1989). Facial resemblance in Linsenmeier, J. A. (2002). The necessities and
engaged and married couples. Journal of luxuries of mate preferences: Testing the
Social and Personal Relationships, 6(2), tradeoffs. Journal of Personality and Social
223–229. Psychology, 82(6), 947.
Hitsch, G. J., Hortaçsu, A., & Ariely, D. (2010a). Lively, C. M., & Morran, L. T. (2014). The
Matching and sorting in online dating. ecology of sexual reproduction. Journal of
American Economic Review, 100(1), Evolutionary Biology, 27(7), 1292–1303.
130–163. Little, A. C., Burt, D. M., & Perrett, D. I. (2006).
Hitsch, G. J., Hortaçsu, A., & Ariely, D. (2010b). Assortative mating for perceived facial
What makes you click? – Mate preferences personality traits. Personality and Individual
in online dating. Quantitative Marketing and Differences, 40(5), 973–984.
Economics, 8(4), 393–427. Lynn, M., & Bolig, R. (1985). Personal advertise-
Hollingshead, A. B. (1950). Cultural factors in ments: Sources of data about relationships.
the selection of marriage mates. American Journal of Social and Personal Relationships,
Sociological Review, 15(5), 619–627. 2(3), 377–383.
Kalmijn, M. (1994). Assortative mating by Lynn, M., & Shurgot, B. A. (1984). Responses
cultural and economic occupational status. to lonely hearts advertisements: Effects of
American Journal of Sociology, 100(2), reported physical attractiveness, physique,
422–452. and coloration. Personality and Social Psy-
Kenrick, D. T. (2011). Sex, murder, and the chology Bulletin, 10(3), 349–357.
meaning of life: A psychologist investigates Mare, R. D. (1991). Five decades of educational
how evolution, cognition, and complexity assortative mating. American Sociological
are revolutionizing our view of human Review, 56(1) 15–32.
nature. New York: Basic Books. McLaughlin, C., Vitak, J., & Crouse, J. (2011).
Kenrick, D. T., Sadalla, E. K., Groth, G., & Trost, Online identity construction and expectation
M. R. (1990). Evolution, traits, and the stages of future interaction. In 2011 44th Hawaii
496 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

International Conference on System Sciences relationship at the proximate and ultimate


(pp. 1–10). IEEE. levels. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16(2),
Miller, G. (2000). The mating mind: How sexual 267–283.
choice shaped the evolution of human Piazza, J., & Bering, J. M. (2009). Evolutionary
nature. New York: Anchor Books. cyber-psychology: Applying an evolutionary
Miller, G. F., & Todd, P. M. (1998). Mate choice framework to Internet behavior. Computers
turns cognitive. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, in Human Behavior, 25(6), 1258–1269.
2(5), 190–198. Price, R. A., & Vandenberg, S. G. (1979).
Minervini, B. P., & McAndrew, F. T. (2006). The Matching for physical attractiveness in mar-
mating strategies and mate preferences of ried couples. Personality and Social Psychol-
mail order brides. Cross-Cultural Research, ogy Bulletin, 5(3), 398–400.
40(2), 111–129. Puts, D. A. (2010). Beauty and the beast:
Mulder, M. B. (1990). Kipsigis women’s prefer- Mechanisms of sexual selection in humans.
ences for wealthy men: Evidence for female Evolution and Human Behavior, 31(3),
choice in mammals? Behavioral Ecology and 157–175.
Sociobiology, 27(4), 255–264. Quinlan, R. J. (2007). Human parental effort
Ombelet, W., & Van Robays, J. (2015). Artificial and environmental risk. Proceedings of the
insemination history: Hurdles and mile- Royal Society B: Biological Sciences,
stones. Facts, Views & Vision in ObGyn, 7(2), 274(1606), 121–125.
137–143. Renninger, L. A., Wade, T. J., & Grammer, K.
Otto, S. P. (2009). The evolutionary enigma of (2004). Getting that female glance: Patterns
sex. American Naturalist, 174(S1), S1–S14. and consequences of male nonverbal behav-
Overbeek, G., Nelemans, S. A., Karremans, J., & ior in courtship contexts. Evolution and
Engels, R. C. (2013). The malleability of mate Human Behavior, 25(6), 416–431.
selection in speed-dating events. Archives of Roberts, S. C., Miner, E. J., & Shackelford, T. K.
Sexual Behavior, 42(7), 1163–1171. (2010). The future of an applied evolutionary
Oyer, P. (2014). Everything I ever needed to psychology for human partnerships. Review
know about economics I learned from online of General Psychology, 14(4), 318–329.
dating. Brighton, MA: Harvard Business Rockman, H. (1994). Matchmaker make me a
Review Press. match: The art and conventions of Jewish
Pawłowski, B., & Dunbar, R. I. (1999). Impact of arranged marriages. Sexual and Marital
market value on human mate choice deci- Therapy, 9(3), 277–284.
sions. Proceedings of the Royal Society of Rodino, I. S., Burton, P. J., & Sanders, K. A.
London. Series B: Biological Sciences, (2011). Mating by proxy: A novel perspective
266(1416), 281–285. to donor conception. Fertility and Sterility,
Pawlowski, B., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2001). 96(4), 998–1001.
Human mate choice strategies. Economics in Rosenfeld, M. J., & Thomas, R. J. (2012).
nature. Social dilemmas, mate choice and Searching for a mate: The rise of the Internet
biological markets, 187–202. as a social intermediary. American Sociologi-
Pawłowski, B., & Koziel, S. (2002). The impact cal Review, 77(4), 523–547.
of traits offered in personal advertisements Rosenfeld, M. J., Thomas, R. J., & Hausen, S.
on response rates. Evolution and Human (2019). Disintermediating your friends: How
Behavior, 23(2), 139–149. online dating in the United States displaces
Pennings, G. (1995). Should donors have the other ways of meeting. Proceedings of the
right to decide who receives their gametes? National Academy of Sciences, 116(36),
Human Reproduction, 10(10), 2736–2740. 17753–17758.
Penton-Voak, I. S., Perrett, D. I., & Peirce, J. W. Rutherford, A. (2018). The book of humans:
(1999). Computer graphic studies of the role The story of how we became us. Hachette
of facial similarity in judgements of attractive- UK.
ness. Current Psychology, 18(1), 104–117. Scheib, J. E. (1994). Sperm donor selection and
Pérusse, D. (1993). Cultural and reproductive the psychology of female mate choice. Ethol-
success in industrial societies: Testing the ogy and Sociobiology, 15(3), 113–129.
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND REPRODUCTION 497

Scheib, J. E. (1997). Female choice in the con- mating. Personality and Individual Differ-
text of donor insemination. In P. A. Gowaty ences, 22(2), 157–164.
(Ed.), Feminism and Evolutionary Biology: Thomas, A. G., Jonason, P. K., Blackburn, J.,
Boundaries, Intersections and Frontiers Kennair, L. E. O., Lowe, R., Malouff, J., & Li,
(pp. 489–504). New York: Chapman & Hall. N. P. (2019). Mate preference priorities in the
Schnarch, D. (1997). Sex, intimacy, and the East and West: A cross-cultural test of the
Internet. Journal of Sex Education and Ther- mate preference priority model. Journal of
apy, 22(1), 15–20. Personality, 88(3), 606–620.
Schover, L. R., Rothmann, S. A., & Collins, R. L. Todd, P. M., Penke, L., Fasolo, B., & Lenton, A. P.
(1992). The personality and motivation of (2007). Different cognitive processes underlie
semen donors: A comparison with oocyte human mate choices and mate preferences.
donors. Human Reproduction, 7(4), 575–579. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Schwartz, C. R. (2013). Trends and variation in Sciences, 104(38), 15011–15016.
assortative mating: Causes and consequences. Toma, C. L., & Hancock, J. T. (2010). Looks and
Annual Review of Sociology, 39, 451–470. lies: The role of physical attractiveness in
Seabright, P. (2012). The war of the sexes: How online dating self-presentation and deception.
conflict and cooperation have shaped men Communication Research, 37(3), 335–351.
and women from prehistory to the present. Toma, C. L., Hancock, J. T., & Ellison, N. B.
Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. (2008). Separating fact from fiction: An
Sear, R. (2015). Evolutionary contributions to examination of deceptive self-presentation in
the study of human fertility. Population online dating profiles. Personality and Social
Studies, 69(sup1), S39–S55. Psychology Bulletin, 34(8), 1023–1036.
Simon, H. A. (1956). Rational choice and the Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2005). In David M.
structure of the environment. Psychological Buss (Ed.), Evolutionary psychology: Concep-
Review, 63(2), 129–138. tual foundations. Evolutionary psychology
Sitton, S., & Blanchard, S. (1995). Men’s prefer- handbook. New York: Wiley. 1–55.
ences in romantic partners: Obesity vs addic- Townsend, J. M. (1989). Mate selection criteria:
tion. Psychological Reports, 77(3_suppl), A pilot study. Ethology and Sociobiology,
1185–1186. 10(4), 241–253.
Skopek, J., Schmitz, A., & Blossfeld, H. P. (2011). Trivers, R., (1972). Parental investment and
The gendered dynamics of age preferences – sexual selection. In Bernard Campbell (Ed.),
empirical evidence from online dating. ZfF – Sexual Selection & the Descent of Man (pp.
Zeitschrift für Familienforschung/Journal of 136–179). New York: Aldine de Gruyter,
Family Research, 23(3), 267–290. New York.
Smith, J. M. (1971). What use is sex? Journal of Valkenburg, P. M., & Peter, J. (2007). Who visits
Theoretical Biology, 30(2), 319–335. online dating sites? Exploring some charac-
Smith, J. M., & Maynard-Smith, J. (1978). The teristics of online daters. CyberPsychology &
evolution of sex (Vol. 4). Cambridge: Behavior, 10(6), 849–852.
Cambridge University Press. Van den Broeck, U., Vandermeeren, M., Vander-
Su, X., & Hu, H. (2019). Gender-specific prefer- schueren, D., Enzlin, P., Demyttenaere, K., &
ence in online dating. EPJ Data Science, 8(1), D’Hooghe, T. (2012). A systematic review of
12. sperm donors: Demographic characteristics,
Symons, D. (1979). The evolution of human attitudes, motives and experiences of the
sexuality. New York: Oxford University Press. process of sperm donation. Human Repro-
Thiessen, D. (1994). Environmental tracking by duction Update, 19(1), 37–51.
females. Human Nature, 5(2), 167–202. Vandenberg, S. G. (1972). Assortative mating,
Thiessen, D., & Gregg, B. (1980). Human assor- or who marries whom? Behavior Genetics,
tative mating and genetic equilibrium: An 2(2–3), 127–157.
evolutionary perspective. Ethology and Walker, R. S., Hill, K. R., Flinn, M. V., & Ells-
Socio­biology, 1(2), 111–140. worth, R. M. (2011). Evolutionary history of
Thiessen, D., Young, R. K., & Delgado, M. hunter-gatherer marriage practices. PloS
(1997). Social pressures for assortative One, 6(4), e19066.
498 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Warren, B. L. (1966). A multiple variable advantage? Sex, offspring & the big 5.
approach to the assortative mating phenom- Personality and Individual Differences, 139,
enon. Biodemography and Social Biology, 158–169.
13(4), 285–290. Whyte, S., Chan, H. F., & Torgler, B. (2018). Do
Waynforth, D., & Dunbar, R. I. (1995). Condi- men and women know what they want? Sex
tional mate choice strategies in humans: differences in online daters’ educational
Evidence from ‘Lonely Hearts’ advertise- preferences. Psychological Science, 29(8),
ments. Behaviour, 132(9), 755–779. 1370–1375.
Whyte, S., Savage, D. A., & Torgler, B. (2017). Whyte, S., Torgler, B., & Harrison, K. L. (2016).
Online sperm donors: the impact of family, What women want in their sperm donor: A
friends, personality and risk perception on study of more than 1000 women’s sperm
behaviour. Reproductive Biomedicine Online, donor selections. Economics & Human Biol-
35(6), 723–732. ogy, 23, 1–9.
Whyte, S., & Torgler, B. (2015). Selection criteria Williams, G. C. (1966). Adaptation and natural
in the search for a sperm donor: Behavioural selection: A critique of some current evolu-
traits versus physical appearance. Journal of tionary thought. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
Bioeconomics, 17(2), 151–171. University Press.
Whyte, S., & Torgler, B. (2016a). Assortative Wilson, D. S. (2018). This view of life: Complet-
mating in the online market for sperm ing the Darwinian revolution. New York: Pen-
donation. Journal of Bioeconomics, 18(3), guin Random House.
169–194. Woestenburg, N. O., Winter, H. B., & Janssens,
Whyte, S., & Torgler, B. (2016b). Determinants P. M. (2016). What motivates men to offer
of online sperm donor success: How women sperm donation via the internet? Psychology,
choose. Applied Economics Letters, 23(8), Health & Medicine, 21(4), 424–430.
592–596. Woll, S. B., & Young, P. (1989). Looking for Mr.
Whyte, S., & Torgler, B. (2017a). Preference or Ms. Right: Self-presentation in videodat-
versus choice in online dating. Cyberpsychol- ing. Journal of Marriage and the Family,
ogy, Behavior, and Social Networking, 20(3), 51(2), 483–488.
150–156. Yee, S. (2009). ‘Gift without a price tag’: altru-
Whyte, S., & Torgler, B. (2017b). Things change ism in anonymous semen donation. Human
with age: Educational assortment in online Reproduction, 24(1), 3–13.
dating. Personality and Individual Differ- Young, L. J., & Wang, Z. (2004). The neurobiol-
ences, 109, 5–11. ogy of pair bonding. Nature Neuroscience,
Whyte, S., Brooks, R. C., Chan, H. F., & Torgler, 7(10), 1048.
B. (2019). Do certain personality traits Zimmer, C. (2009). On the origin of sexual repro-
provide a mating market competitive duction. Science, 324(5932), 1254–1256.
25
Evolutionary Psychology
and Cyberwarfare
Robert Finkelstein

CYBERWARFARE AND MEMES from which culture emerges, then changes,


along with the values underlying the culture.
Conflicts involving terrorism and asymmetric This results in a changed culture, the changes
adversaries, who may be state or non-state being beneficial or harmful to people com-
actors, are fundamentally information wars of prising the culture. The prospective disci-
the mind featuring psychosocial manipulation, pline of cultural engineering (or accelerated
often as precursors to physical violence which cultural evolution) may be used intentionally
they instigate. Cyberwarfare, or cyber warfare, to modify what is considered a dysfunctional
encompasses a loose set of adversarial pro- culture or subculture (Finkelstein, 2012).
cesses involving computers, software, and net- It was hypothesized (Dawkins, 1976) that,
works (RAND Corporation, 2019), of which a on an evolutionary timescale, memes may
subset involves influencing the mental state of affect the development of the human mind,
individuals and populations through social and that memes needed for survival by our
media, such as influencing elections (Council ancient ancestors (e.g., Homo habilis, Homo
on Foreign Relations, 2019) or sowing conflict erectus, and Homo heidelbergensis) led to the
among subcultures in a nation (US Senate, emergence of enlarged brains and more cog-
2019). The long war against terrorism is funda- nitively advanced minds for Homo sapiens.
mentally a war of ideas, and at the core in this Evolutionary psychology contends that
information war are memes (Finkelstein, Darwinian evolutionary processes of natu-
2011). A common metaphor for memes is ral selection apply to the brain, mind, and
mind viruses (Asher, 2005; Brodie, 1996). resulting psychological traits, e.g., memory,
Memes can alter the psychology of individ- perception, and language, just as they do to
uals, their values, and their consequent behav- physiological traits (Buss, 2005). Darwinian
ior. The aggregated behavior of individuals, evolution is a well-tested scientific theory
500 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

that explains how complexity can arise from by memes, like the evolution of technology,
simplicity, and so evolutionary psychology is (metaphorically) Lamarckian rather than
is expected to provide a basis for explain- Darwinian (Blackmore, 1999). Human psy-
ing the origin of the human brain, perhaps chology, shaped over eons by Darwinian
the most complex entity in the known uni- evolution, reacts in the near term to memes
verse (Aunger, 2002). Some traits, physical via Lamarckian evolution, in the metaphori-
or mental, which were previously optimal for cal sense that meme-caused changes in the
human survival, may no longer be as benefi- behavior of an individual can cause changes in
cial in the context of modern technology and the behavior of others. (Lamarckian evolution
cultural values. Racism and xenophobia, for occurs literally when the environment changes
example, may have served to benefit survival the phenotype of an organism such that the
in small bands of hunter-gatherers for hun- organism is better adapted to the environmental
dreds of millennia, but are now harmful in factor that caused the change (Hull, 2000), and
our complex, technologically advanced soci- where the genotype can then pass the improved
eties. The psychological states that give rise adaptation to the organism’s progeny.)
to these phenomena are no longer rational,
and the behavior they cause cannot be toler-
ated in a democratic society. Nevertheless,
racism and xenophobia, among other obso- WHAT IS A MEME?
lete mental programming, remain endemic in
society as a remnant of once-useful evolved The literature addressing memes reveals a
psychology, like an unnecessary inflamed plethora of definitions, with most being varia-
appendix, with memes used to inflame emo- tions of the neologism coined by Dawkins
tions that are otherwise under control or dor- (1976), although there are indications of earlier
mant within susceptible recipients. roots. Dawkins defined the meme as a self-
Over an evolutionary timescale the meme reproducing and propagating information
is postulated to affect human psychological structure analogous to a gene in biology. He
traits arising from natural selection, i.e., the focused on the meme as a replicator, analogous
evolved psychology arising from changes in to the gene, able to affect human evolution
the brain and mind that drive individual and through the evolutionary algorithm of varia-
cultural behavior. But the reverse can also be tion, replication, and differential fitness, and
conjectured: that the consequences of evolved then eventual physical alteration of the brain
psychology affect the memes to which we are due to the beneficial impact on human survival
now most susceptible, the memes propagating and reproduction of certain memes, such as
through social media every second, flooding detailed instructions requiring extensive mental
the Twitterverse and impacting a multitude information processing and storage about how
of minds. Memes having profound impact to make a flint cutting tool, how to hunt a
can alter the values of susceptible individuals wooly mammoth, how to find water in the
and cause them to engage in behavior detri- desert, or how to make fire. Thus, he claimed,
mental to them and to society. Contemplating the meme had evolutionary effects on human
Darwinian evolutionary timescales for the physiology, psychology, and culture. He pro-
effects of memes on the brain and mind may posed that a meme’s information structure can
suggest interesting phenomena for research; have many forms, including words, symbols,
but contemplating memes in the current images, icons, and artifacts (Dawkins, 1976).
timeframe, in the context of evolved psy- In the near term, memes can change the
chology, is more compelling given their imme- values and behavior of individuals and the
diate consequences. The near-term evolution culture they comprise. Memes can cause
of human values, behavior, and culture caused individuals and their cultures to evolve in the
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND CYBERWARFARE 501

sense of changing, albeit not in the sense of • A contagious information pattern that replicates
Darwinian (and genetically based) evolution by parasitically infecting human minds and alter-
of the human brain, mind, and psychology. ing their behavior, causing them to propagate
The latter phenomenon, the original empha- the pattern. Individual slogans, catch-phrases,
sis for Dawkins, has yet to be scientifically melodies, icons, inventions, and fashions are
typical memes. An idea or information pattern is
established, while the former phenomenon
not a meme until it causes someone to replicate
can be regularly observed. it, to repeat it to someone else. All transmitted
Most of the definitions offered by meme knowledge is memetic
researchers agree that a meme is a unit • The smallest idea that can copy itself while
(whatever that means) of cultural transmis- remaining self-contained and intact – essentially
sion (or a unit of information – whatever sets of instructions that can be followed to pro-
that means), where culture may be defined duce behavior
as the total pattern of behavior (and its prod-
ucts) of a population of agents, embodied in Some have contended that a crisp definition of
thought, behavior, and artifacts, and depend- ‘unit of information’ or ‘unit of cultural trans-
ent upon the capacity for learning and trans- mission’ is not critical to a theory of memes,
mitting knowledge to succeeding generations e.g., Darwin’s Theory of Evolution was scien-
(Finkelstein, 2006). tifically useful before anyone knew about
A few of the many definitions extracted genes and DNA. But a pragmatic definition
from the literature include: and associated metrics would provide a better
foundation for research and would facilitate
• A self-reproducing and propagating information the ability to generate testable predictions and
structure analogous to a gene in biology falsifiable hypotheses, and control effects,
• A unit of cultural transmission (or a unit of imita- which DNA and genetic engineering now pro-
tion) that is a replicator that propagates in the vides to Darwin’s theory. Darwin’s theory was
meme pool leaping from brain to brain via (in a scientifically useful before anyone knew about
broad sense) imitation; examples: ‘tunes, ideas, genes and DNA because Darwin and other
catch-phrases, clothes fashions, ways of making scientists had physical objects (e.g., fossils or
pots or of building arches’ (Dawkins, 1976: 192) living physiological structures) to measure.
• Ideas that embody programs for their own Metrics existed with which to compare and
retransmission or propagation
contrast entities. A coherent theory could
• Actively contagious ideas or thoughts
be developed in which phenomena could be
• Shared elements of a culture learned through imi-
tation from others – with culture being defined quantified and predicted. This is not the case
rather broadly to include ideas, behaviors, and for memes, and there is not as yet an analo-
physical objects gous Theory of Memetics.
• An element of a culture transmitted by non- The author (Finkelstein, 2011) distin-
genetic means, especially imitation guishes between memes that can be described
• Information patterns infecting human minds by their external phenomena (e-memes) and
• A (cognitive) information structure able to rep- internal phenomena (i-memes). E-memes are
licate using human hosts and to influence their manifested by their effects on human behav-
behavior to promote replication ior and culture. I-memes are manifested
• Cultural information units that are the smallest
by their effects on an individual’s neuronal
elements that self-replicate with reliability and
behavior and brain. E-memes and i-memes
fecundity
• A unit of cultural information as it is represented are not two types of memes, but rather two
in the brain effects or manifestations of memes.
• A pattern of information, one that happens to The tools and techniques for studying the
have evolved a form which induces people to two manifestations of memes differ. E-memes
repeat that pattern can be studied via their simulated or actual
502 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

propagation over social networks (modern recipients; a meme, as a subset of informa-


social media are especially efficient and effec- tion, could have different consequences for
tive for propagating memes). Tools and tech- different recipients. For example, if a recipi-
niques include: Internet and phone text and ent is already familiar with the content of a
voice message propagation; rumor and gossip message, it does not reduce uncertainty at all,
propagation; buzz and viral marketing. There i.e., it is not information for that recipient.
are also neuroeconomics behavioral experi- To distinguish memes from other kinds
ments involving game theory, risk, attention of information, an elaboration of the defini-
and awareness, learning, valuation, motiva- tion can invoke measurable thresholds, to be
tion, emotion, behavior, trust and attachment, determined, for propagation and persistence.
and addictive behavior. I-memes can be stud- Impact might take place in the individual, in
ied using tools and techniques such as func- terms of neurophysiological changes (e.g., as
tional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) or detected by a brain scan) or manifestations
other kinds of neuroimaging; genetic profil- of behavior; or it might take place in soci-
ing; p­sycho-pharmacological manipulations; ety (groups of individuals) as manifested by
psycho-physiology (EMG, ERP, and EEG); behavior. Metrics can be determined (each
behavioral measures; psychological testing; with submetrics) to facilitate the measure-
blood chemistry; hormone analysis; neuro- ment and comparison of memes. Additional
chemical reactions; and single neuron record- metrics may be defined based on the results
ing. The study of the two manifestations of of experiments on brain processes with tools
memes must be integrated with a synthesis and such as fMRI. Other tools for meme research
multi-disciplinary study of the phenomenon. include the modeling and simulation of
While Dawkins focused on the meme as social networks and information propagation
a replicator, analogous to the gene, able to through social networks, as well as the tools
affect human evolution through the evolu- and techniques of neuroeconomics, which is
tionary algorithm of variation, replication, the study of how the brain interacts with the
and differential fitness, for near-term practi- external environment to make choices (deci-
cal applications the relevant characteristics of sions), and which combines neuroscience,
the meme are that it consists of information economics, and psychology.
which persists, propagates, and influences Metrics are needed to establish a scientific
human behavior. A metrics-based, pragmatic and engineering basis for memetics. Research
definition of the meme can be constructed to is needed to determine the metrics. Based on
distinguish a meme from other sorts of infor- the Information – Propagation – Impact –
mation (such as common daily utterances Persistence (IPIP) definition for a meme,
like ‘it’s time for lunch’). some makeshift metric examples include: the
A meme is information which propagates, number, type, and dispersion of recipients;
persists, and has impact. Information, in the duration of transmission and memory;
Claude Shannon’s definition (and associated the degree of entropy; and the individual and
equations), is that which reduces uncertainty societal consequences of the meme.
(Shannon, 1948). A message carries informa- The relative importance of meme size
tion inasmuch as it conveys something not (e.g., small, medium, large) remains to be
already known. It is the difference between determined, but shorter (smaller) memes are
two states of uncertainty before and after a easier to remember and propagate over social
message has been received. Psychology, in media than longer ones, and are likely to
the form of subjective perception, is involved have greater immediate impact. Entropy as a
in evaluating the states of uncertainty. The metric of information may be better for deter-
same message can have different influence mining the size of message than the number
or impacts, depending on the states of the of words (or bits) because it can be applied to
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND CYBERWARFARE 503

any language as well as symbols, signs, and mind and psychology of the observer), and
images – semiotics in general. Larger memes this can be a disturbing concept to some.
may be memeplexes, e.g., the typical human The entropy of a meme, whether long or
brain can only hold about 7 ‘chunks’ of infor- short, can be estimated and calculated from the
mation in short-term memory (McLeod, 2009). individual letters or symbols, i.e., based on
Claude Shannon’s mathematical theory the total number in the originating set, or from
of communication (Shannon, 1948) meas- the individual words, based on the vocabulary
ures the amount of information in any mes- in the originating set. For example, the Oxford
sage using a function identical to that of English Dictionary contains 616,500 words.
Boltzmann’s formulation of entropy (Sharp The average vocabulary of an American English
and Matschinsky, 2015). The use of this speaker is estimated to be between 10,000 and
entropy is noncontroversial as long as it is 20,000 words, or optimistically between 50,000
used to measure the freedom of choice in and 70,000 words (de Leon Huld, 2020). The
messages and not semantic content (i.e., a average American would actively use only a
specific message is selected from a larger fraction of that vocabulary. Typically, there
set). For example, the information (entropy) are about 800 words per page with 12-point
needed to select one word out of a vocabulary Times Roman font, although publications vary
of 200 words is 7.6 bits (S = log2 200 = 7.6 considerably in the number of words, based
bits). That is, the word is within the first 100 on page size, format, and number of pages.
words or the second 100 words. It takes one Shannon (1948) calculated the entropy per let-
bit to determine that, or one guess as to which ter of an eight-letter chunk of English as 2.3 bits
of the two sets it was in – if the word is in the per letter, using a 26-character set (although a
first 100 words, it takes another bit (guess) to 76-symbol set, including 26 letters, might be
determine which of the two sets of 50 words more appropriate). Assuming a vocabulary of
it is in; and so on. It takes between 7 and 8 bits 30,000 words the entropy of a single word is
(guesses) to find the one word in 200 words. about 15 bits (S = log2 30,000 = 15 bits). This
Likewise, a 50-word meme requires 380 bits kind of metric for information might be used in
(for a 200-word vocabulary). Nothing is indi- engineering memes for maximum efficacy.
cated about the meaning or significance of Metrics for propagation may be based, for
the message that results from selecting and example, on the number, type, and dispersion
stringing together the 50 words. of recipients of memes. For example, neighbor-
A problem arises when entropy is used as hood gossip may propagate among 10 people
a measure of informational order and disor- and persist for 10 hours, while a fundamental-
der. For example, a manager has a desk with ist insurgency may propagate among 100,000
mounds of paper covering it – is this a high people and persist for 100,000 hours (about 10
or low entropy case? The manager’s associ- years). The persistence of memes, for example,
ate cannot find a paper he is searching for may be measured in terms of the duration of
until he has made a large number of guesses, their transmission and re-transmission and the
separating the piles of papers – a very high duration in the memory of the recipients. The
entropy case. But the manager knows exactly impact of memes may be measured in terms of
where every paper is located – the mounds their consequences on individuals and society.
of paper are quite organized in her mind; it
would take her no guesses to find the crucial
paper. It is then a very low entropy case. Both
cases concern the same set of items, but two MEME TRANSMISSION
different observers. Entropy then becomes
a subjective measure of information (or a The transmission of the meme resembles
measure of the information contained in the Claude Shannon’s iconic schematic of a
504 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

general communications system: an informa- The meme influenced the behavior of at least
tion source sends a message via a transmitter one recipient of the meme, a young man who
of some sort, which issues a signal of some then armed himself and invaded the pizza
kind, which is usually adversely affected by parlor to free the supposedly enslaved chil-
noise. The signal is received by a receiver of dren. He was sentenced to four years in
some kind, which issues the message, perhaps prison. While many Internet memes, like cat
altered by noise, to the destination. This com- videos, may be amusing, other memes may
munications template was modified by motivate recipients to commit terrorist acts
Gunders and Brown (Truszkowski et al., 2020) or perpetrate mass murders.
into a model of a meme life-cycle, known as
the Component Meme Concept (CMC):

• A memetic ‘engineer’ intentionally or uninten- TYPES OF MEMES


tionally creates a meme
• A hook attracts people to the meme Most people today associate memes with the
• Bait is the desired result promised by the meme Internet and social media, but over the eons
to attract people to contemplate it and propa-
they have been much more than that. As
gate it to others
information that propagates, has impact, and
• The vector is the medium used to transport the
original meme and its subsequent propagation persists (IPIP), memes may consist of many
(there may be multiple vectors) different forms, including words, ideas, sym-
• The host refers to the carrier that sent the original bols, icons, logos, tunes, poems, catch-
meme and subsequent propagation (there may phrases, fashion, technological processes
be multiple hosts) (e.g., making arrowheads or gumbo), fables,
• The memotype refers to the content of the meme religion, graffiti, images, novels, movies,
• The sociotype is the social and cultural environ- metaphors, and narratives. A meme that sat-
ment of the memotype isfies the IPIP criteria may consist of:

Consider the following example known in the • a simple exclamation, like the military ‘hooah!’ or
press as ‘Pizzagate’, where some of its facts the religious ‘Allah!’
have been changed for convenience in our • a catchphrase, slogan, or exhortation, such as
example. It was claimed (falsely) that a child ‘remember the Maine!’, ‘Death to America!’,
sex ring, managed by the wife of a former ‘Jesus loves you!’, ‘When it absolutely, positively
president, is being run out of the basement of has to get there overnight!’, ‘You’ll wonder where
the yellow went when you brush your teeth with
a Washington, DC pizza parlor. To those able
Pepsodent!’
to think critically, this extraordinary claim • a sound-bite, prayer, incitement, or joke (‘Why
would seem to require extraordinary evidence does the chicken cross the road?’)
to be believed, or even considered for more • neighborhood gossip or a text message; a news-
than a few seconds. Sadly, for many the meme paper article or an urban legend
life-cycle is resistant to critical thinking. • a flyer, poster, advertisement, notice, propaganda
(‘Make America Great Again!’)
• Memetic engineer: <John Smith> • political speech (e.g., the Gettysburg Address),
• Hook: <Child Sex Ring> sermon (e.g., Martin Luther King Jr’s ‘I Have a
• Bait: <It Will Make You Shocked And Angry> Dream’ speech), polemic, article, book, or blog
• Vector: <Twitterverse; Then Internet, Phone Text, • a recipe or an entire cookbook; a political mani-
TV, Radio, And Newspapers> festo or ideology; a religious canon, such as the
• Host: <Twitter; Then Facebook, Etc.> Bible, Koran, Vedas, or the Pali; a dictionary; an
• Memotype: <Verbal Description> Army field manual
• Sociotype: <Conspiracy Fanatics; Ill-Educated • instructions for building a cathedral or a nuclear
Young Males> submarine
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND CYBERWARFARE 505

• instructions for growing wheat or hunting wooly Neurocognitive warfare targets an adversary’s
mammoths; a burial ritual; instructions for making cognitive and psychological functions, and the
a flint-cutting tool, a spear, a wheel, or fire physiological and behavioral consequences of
those functions. The means for neurocogni-
Some memes are literally etched in stone or tive warfare could include drugs, microbes,
clay tablets, others are written on parchment toxins, and neuroweapons (Giordano, 2017)
or paper, or painted on canvas (e.g., the Mona such as microwaves and infrasound.
Lisa or Edvard Munch’s ‘The Scream’), or In the more than 40 years since the meme
sculpted from stone (e.g., Michelangelo’s was identified as an entity of interest, there
‘David’). Whether the memes that survive for has not been substantial research to deter-
generations alter our physical brain as mine whether memetics can be placed on a
Dawkins (1976) suggested, they can influ- scientific basis so that its phenomena and
ence our values, behavior, or culture in the effects can be quantified, predicted, and con-
context of the psychology that has evolved. trolled. Most of the research of memetics,
Shorter-duration memes, such as those that such as it is, has been conducted in scattered
persist a few days, weeks or months, can also efforts, often as personal projects on the part
affect our behavior and even our values and of dedicated academics, many outside the
culture, in the context of the evolved psy- United States. There have been few signifi-
chology, such as by altering political or con- cant, coherent efforts to explore whether or
sumer preferences. Even memes with brief how a scientific framework, and a consequent
persistence can be propagated widely via engineering discipline, can be established for
social media and have significant immediate memetics. The attempt to establish a scien-
impact on their recipients. tific basis for memetics is important. For
example, within a suitable memetics frame-
work could be the means to prevent irrational
conflict and promote rational solutions to
MEMETICS AND NEUROCOGNITIVE endemic national and international problems,
WARFARE and to develop effective countermeasures
to adversarial manipulation of social media
Foreign or domestic terrorism is the act or to influence population behavior, such as
threat of violence intended to influence an attempts to sway voting in elections or to
audience. The memes that emerge from a instigate hatred and violence between groups.
violent terrorist act (e.g., words and images Of course, without safeguards memetics is a
about the act), or memes that merely threaten double-edged sword.
violent acts, can adversely alter the psycho- Useful technology can be successfully
logical state of a nation. Some terrorist developed without formal engineering and
memes may persist for days or weeks, while scientific disciplines through trial and error,
others persist for years. It is critical for as has occurred over eons, such as mak-
national defense to be able to counter adver- ing weapons or preparing food. People
sarial memes. Words matter. Over the centu- have been buying and selling (or trading)
ries, political and ideological propaganda goods and services for thousands of years.
have proven to be potent. Lies become the Over the millennia, survival in the market-
truth and the truth becomes lies (Orwell, place sharpened the methods – the tools and
1946; Poole, 2006). techniques – of marketing, i.e., what goods
Military memetics is the application of and services to produce and how to distribute
memes for national security (i.e., a compo- and sell them. Success at marketing depends
nent of cyberwarfare), and it may be con- on knowing the needs and wants of the cus-
sidered a subset of neurocognitive warfare. tomer and convincing the customer that you
506 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

can satisfy those needs and wants. At the end The US military could undertake meme and
of the marketing process, selling success- counter-meme operations involving national
fully depends on the ability to convince the security because it has long engaged in psy-
customer to part with something of value chological operations (PSYOP) to influence
(e.g., money, item, or service) in exchange or reinforce foreign attitudes and behavior
for what is being sold. The development of that are favorable to the military’s (and
marketing methods accelerated during the nation’s) objectives. PSYOP also employs
20th century in the age of mass communica- selected information to affect foreign audi-
tion, including the refinement of advertising ences’ emotions, motives, and objective rea-
campaigns in the media age of radio, televi- soning. The ultimate goal is to alter the
sion, magazines, newspapers, and now social behavior of foreign governments, organiza-
media, to convince the prospective buyer that tions, groups, and individuals so that they
the advertised product is just what he or she will be accommodating to US military objec-
needs or wants. The tools and techniques tives. PSYOP also exploits the psychological
advanced over the years largely by trial and vulnerabilities of US adversaries to induce
error, honed by lessons learned with the fear and confusion in order to destroy their
countless buying and selling of myriad goods morale and disrupt their operations (Joint
and services. Likewise through trial and error Publication, 2003).
the marketing of ideas advanced with hard- The US military also has longstanding
earned experience learned from public rela- tools and techniques for Military Deception
tions, political propaganda, and ideological (MILDEC) (Joint Publication, 2012). This
influencing campaigns. consists of actions intended to deliberately
There have been a number of occasional mislead an adversary’s military decision-
projects concerning, or related to, memet- makers concerning friendly military capabili-
ics sponsored by the Defense Advanced ties, intentions, and operations. The goal is to
Research Projects Agency (DARPA), an cause the adversary to act (or not act) in ways
agency of the Department of Defense focused favorable to friendly forces. The US military
on the research and development of emerging is expressly forbidden to use MILDEC on the
technologies. These projects included: US public, Congress, or news media.
The US military is also charged with engag-
• 2006: the Epidemiology of Ideas, intended to pre-
ing in counterpropaganda. This is intended to
dict changing culture and examine the creation,
identify and counter an adversary’s propa-
propagation, and impact of ideas in the context
of the processes by which diseases propagate ganda by exposing the adversary’s efforts to
(Lynch, 1996) influence friendly populations and their mili-
• 2006–2009: Military Memetics, to determine tary forces. To mitigate enemy propaganda,
whether memetics can be established as a sci- the US military is supposed to provide truth-
ence with the ability to explain and predict phe- ful information, as well as to use technology
nomena; to define memes and memetic metrics; and military operations to disrupt, degrade,
and to use tools and techniques to examine and disable the tools and techniques of an
Information Propagation, Impact, and Persistence adversary’s propaganda (Joint Publication,
• 2011–2012: Social Media in Strategic 2006).
Communications, to develop automated tools and
The US military engages in Public Affairs
techniques for detecting, identifying, evaluating,
(PA) operations to increase their combat effec-
and countering adversarial memes
• 2011–2012: Narrative Networks, to perform quan- tiveness by keeping soldiers informed (FM-
titative analysis of narratives, investigate their 46-1, 1997). PA also monitors public opinion
effects on human psychology and neurobiology, in environments of interest in order to detect
and determine their influence on individuals and political, social, and cultural shifts, along with
culture adversary propaganda and disinformation.
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND CYBERWARFARE 507

A goal is to provide ­commanders with situ- then the Constitution; George Washington
ational awareness of the international public chopping down the cherry tree; Betsy Ross
information environment. Commanders are sewing the first flag; the freezing revolution-
given the means to use information to take ary soldiers at Valley Forge, etc.). Likewise,
offensive and preemptive defensive actions the many narratives of US history meant to
against adversaries. The US military is pro- instill patriotism, such as the expansion to the
hibited from using PA to mislead the public, west; the pony express and wagon trains; the
national leaders, or the media (FM-46-1, 1997). epic building of the continental railroad;
the stain of slavery and the Civil War; the US
Cavalry and Indian wars; arrival of the New
York immigrant multitudes; the many wars
Memes and Narratives
of the 20th century; the transformational US
Narratives may be considered a subset of inventions of the 19th and 20th centuries –
memes. A narrative is a story whether long or all of the narratives that have served to unify
short; past, present, or future; with or without a diverse country may need to be revitalized
much detail; and told for any purpose. in a time of increasing cultural fragmentation
Typically, a story has a beginning, middle, and and the intentional spread of memes meant to
ending. Cultural narratives are important in divide the nation.
forming and maintaining cultures (or nations, Narratives are fundamental to the human
or other groups), in providing or maintaining mind and perception (Leon, 2017). Sensory
a sense of identity, cohesiveness, and purpose data and consequent perception are inter-
to individuals within a culture: what is our mittent, with only the individual’s illusion
past, why are we here, what should we do, and and delusion of continuity and coherence.
what is our future? National narratives instill Plagued with uncertainty and flawed informa-
patriotism and can unify a diverse populace. tion, the subconscious mind creates an illusion
National strategic narratives describe the core of a coherent narrative, constructing relation-
interests of a nation and provide a foundation ships, motives, and intentions based on limited
for statecraft and national security. For exam- or incorrect information. The mind fills in
ple, the US post-World War II strategic narra- details and revises its narratives as needed,
tive provided the basis for the global struggle with or without evidence, even retroactively.
against communism. There is as yet no suit­ The mind autonomically, unconsciously, and
able post-communism narrative for the global continuously constructs narratives, weaving
struggle against asymmetric adversaries and the weft of subjective reality among the warp
non-state actors. of physical reality. Thus individuals create
More critically, there is not yet a suit- personal stories about who they are and what
able narrative theory or narrative engineer- the world is like; narratives by which they live
ing to counter adverse individual and social their lives. These individual narratives can
behavior originating from narratives, and to aggregate and evolve into cultural narratives
alter dysfunctional cultures. There is not yet influencing the behavior of the family, clan,
an integrated system of tools and techniques tribe, or nation. Narratives can strongly shape
for automating the ability to detect, identify, or modify a culture.
and evaluate narratives and for creating and The structure, function, and ontology of
propagating counternarratives. The narrative narratives are shaped by their building blocks,
threads, true and false, about the founding including metaphors, symbols, symbolic
of the nation and the founding fathers may actions, semiotics, myths, frames, scenar-
be fraying in the age of social media (the ios, etc. For example, metaphors shape and
debates in the heat of Independence Hall to express cultural thought and style as embed-
adopt the Declaration of Independence and ded in language, artifacts, and behavior.
508 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

They shape ideology and religion and can 2002). The most effective of such messages
spark insurgencies. The major sources of met- have ‘stickiness’, i.e., they stick in memory
aphor vary from culture to culture, whether and have impact. Simple changes in the
from the human body, animals, dwellings, presentation or structuring of information
landscapes, sport, war, etc. Many cultures can make a big difference in their impact, as
have common metaphors, i.e., identical map- advertisers have known for decades. Ancient
ping of metaphor sources onto target domains; advertising slogans still stick in the memory
but seemingly identical metaphors might have of those exposed to them as children decades
different implications in different cultures. For ago, such as: ‘Winston Tastes Good Like a
example, ‘Joan lives in a sick neighborhood’ Cigarette Should’ (1954); ‘It Takes a Licking
can mean a bad neighborhood or a good (awe- and Keeps on Ticking’ (1956); and ‘Good to
some) neighborhood, depending on the culture the Last Drop’ (1926). Slogans are among the
of the speaker. most effective of memes.
The rules of the Tipping Point apply to
memes. According to Gladwell, the three
Memetics and the Tipping Point rules of the Tipping Point are:

Gladwell described how ideas, products, 1 The law of the few: few people can cause large
messages, and behavior spread like viruses, effects (e.g., changes in mass behavior)
seemingly in a moment when everything 2 The stickiness factor: with stickiness the message
stays in memory
changes all at once (Gladwell, 2002). This is
3 The power of context: impact on behavior
the effect of memes with the most impact on depends on context
their recipients. In Gladwell’s telling, a small
number of people in a small number of situ- Starting an information epidemic, and lever-
ations start behaving differently, and that aging the law of the few, requires concentrat-
behavior spreads and changes the behavior of ing resources on a few key areas. The success
others in similar situations. It is contagious of a social epidemic requires the involvement
behavior, which could be intentionally initi- of three types of people: connectors, mavens,
ated by, for example, viral marketing. There and salesmen.
have been such trends, for example, in fash-
ion, crime or anti-crime, music, fads, reli- • ‘Connectors’: These people are gregarious and
gion, and ideology. Small changes can have know many people. They are active in many
big effects. A few percentage-point changes different social and professional niches and
in initial conditions can lead to non-linear subcultures.
outcomes, just as a single match with limited • ‘Mavens’: These people are experts who are
socially motivated to share information.
energy can start a forest fire. Changes can
• ‘Salesmen’: These people are persuaders who
occur quickly rather than gradually. This is are articulate. They can obtain the trust of others
the equivalent of disease epidemics or the and convince them of the truth and value of their
effects of punctuated equilibrium in evolu- messages.
tion (Gould, 2007).
People expect linearity and gradualness, Thus mavens generate the message, the con-
not change with exponential or geometric nectors spread it, and the salesmen persuade
progression (University College London, the recipients, who might be otherwise uncon-
2017). But social change can be as sud- vinced, to accept and trust the message.
den as the phase change of water to ice. A In order to achieve stickiness in a meme, the
more extreme version of the heuristic 80/20 message should be made practical and seem-
rule (Koch, 1999) is that 5% of people cause ingly personal for the recipient. The message
95% of the new ideas that spread (Gladwell, should be as simple as possible and irresistible
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND CYBERWARFARE 509

to the intended audience. The context is impor- dropping extraneous details and exaggerating
tant because the behavior the meme is intended other details, so that the message acquires a
to evoke is a function of the social context of deeper meaning and is better understood by
the meme and its recipients. Context tends to be the intended recipients. For example, the com-
underrated in its influence on behavior, while plex issue of immigration can be boiled down
character is overrated (Choi, 2003). To explain to: ‘murderers and rapists are pouring over the
or predict the behavior of others, people tend US southern border!’ People can be induced
to focus on individual character traits rather to transform their usual behavior or beliefs
than contextual circumstances. But it is groups with the right impetus. Social change can be
in a social context that have a critical role in volatile and often inexplicable. Insufficient
spreading social epidemics – and memes. To knowledge of evolutionary psychology means
bring about a fundamental change in people’s that human behavior, and the motivation lead-
beliefs and behaviors, a change that would per- ing to the behavior, is often a mystery. The
sist and serve as an example to others, a com- values and behavior of seemingly stolid and
munity must be created around the beliefs. For stable people may be transformed with the
groups to serve as incubators of messages, such slightest impetus, such as a false rumor-meme
as memes, they should be kept below about that is propagated to the right recipients at
150 members. This is the maximum size for the right time. False rumor-memes can cause
informal, person-to-person contact and cohe- mobs of villagers to suddenly gather to attack
sive group loyalty, and it is about the size of and kill their long-time neighbors, with whom
an army infantry company, an effective corpo- they have lived peacefully and who are inno-
rate department, or a tribe of hunter-gatherers. cent of the acts of which they are accused,
Larger groups tend to become spontaneously such as kidnapping village children, killing
divided and alienated (Dunbar, 2005). sacred cows, or blasphemy.
Rumor may be the most contagious of
all social messages. According to Gladwell
(2002), rumors that have a kernel of truth, Meme Research and Engineering
such as an urban legend, can be distorted
three ways in order to increase their recep- A research and development program in mili-
tivity by the recipients and effectiveness in tary memetics could develop a new approach
evoking the desired behavior. In terms of to countering would-be terrorists. Such a pro-
memetics, the meme engineer can: gram could prevent or mitigate irrational con-
flict and promote rational solutions to national
1 Level the narration or story (i.e., leave out details and international problems. It could strengthen
essential for a recipient to understand the true the US military in peacekeeping missions,
meaning of the rumor-meme or an incident that psychological operations, recruitment, and
it describes); this simplification, or even falsifica- training, as well making new discoveries con-
tion, makes the rumor-meme more relevant and cerning the human brain, cognition, and social
understandable for the intended recipients.
networks.
2 Sharpen the story (i.e., make the remaining details
A design for a Meme Warfare Center
of an incident described in the rumor-meme more
specific to the expected interests of the recipient); (MWC) was proposed by an officer in the US
this encompasses the hook for the rumor-meme. Marine Corps (Prosser, 2006). The conceptual
3 Assimilate the story (i.e., change the story so it MWC would have two complementary subdi-
makes sense to the recipients of the rumor-meme visions: the External Meme Center (EMC) and
who then subsequently spread the rumor-meme). the Internal Meme Center (IMC). The IMC
would provide the commander with advice
The connectors, mavens, and salesmen can concerning memes needed to be engineered
thus make an idea or rumor contagious by and propagated to influence the culture of
510 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

the friendly combat force in the context of military and civilian analysts familiar with
circumstances such as the composition and the enemy and knowledge of tactical and
posture of friendly forces, command climate, strategic maneuver warfare. They would
commander’s guidance, command philoso- also have expertise in social and behavio-
phy, force morale, warfare philosophy, and ral science, systems analysis and operations
rules of engagement. These memes would be research (especially game theory), and cogni-
managed, analyzed, and transmitted through tive science.
common distribution mediums within the The two components of meme commu-
joint force, such as annual training venues, nications, the MCC and SCC, are separate
intranet media, email, text, and other unclassi- cells with divergent purposes. The purpose of
fied media. To create and manage the memes, the MCC is to transform the memes emerg-
the IMC would employ senior officer and ing from the MEC and MAC into the most
enlisted personnel, human-resource manag- effective formats suitable for transmission
ers, clinical psychologists, equal-opportunity by various media suitable for the intended
specialists, civilian personnel and assessment recipients (e.g., whether TV, radio, newspa-
specialists. The goal of the IMC is to create per, billboard, email, or Twitter). The MCC
and manage internal organizational memes would leverage available media outlets suit-
similar to how corporations employ memes able for an intended audience in order for the
to effect organizational cultural change. meme to have maximum impact and persis-
The EMC would advise the commander tence. The SCC would have personnel who
about enemy combat forces, noncombat- are SME familiar with local, regional, and
ant indigenous personnel, and the strategic global media of all types. They would have
audience. The EMC organization would the technical expertise to compete success-
consist of a Meme Engineering Cell (MEC), fully with the adversary’s use of media to
a Meme Analysis Cell (MAC), a Meme transmit their memes.
Communications Cell (MCC), and a Strategic The concept for a Meme Control Center is
Communications Cell (SCC). a variation on the MWC, and it could be a
The MEC would create memes, identify civil or military-based organization. It would
and target suitable recipients, and ‘inoculate’ be managed by a Meme Control Officer. It
them with the memes. The MEC person- would consist of a Meme Processing Center
nel would consist of various subject-matter and a Counter-Meme Processing Center.
experts (SME) from the US and other coun- A Memetics for Intelligence and National
tries as needed. They might include cul- Defense (MIND) Research and Development
tural anthropologists, economists, linguists, Center would provide the tools and tech-
targeting specialists, technical personnel niques employed by the Meme and Counter-
(e.g., in communications networks), and Meme Processing Centers, where the former
assessment experts. The MEC generates the gathers adversarial memes from around the
memes needed to gain a psychological and world and the latter propagates counter-
ideological advantage in an asymmetric, non- memes (which are also memes) around the
linear battlefield. The MEC offers the Joint world. Both processing centers exchange
Force Commander situational context and information, (e.g., feedback) to improve
insight into the enemies and noncombatants the efficiency with which effective counter-
comprising the audience for the engineered memes can be developed and propagated.
memes. The MIND R&D Center would consist
The MAC monitors and records the meme of four directorates focused on disciplines
feedback loop. The MEC would then ana- able to develop new tools and techniques
lyze and assess the feedback provided by the to improve meme effectiveness and effi-
MAC. The MAC personnel would consist of ciency for their engineering, propagation,
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND CYBERWARFARE 511

impact, and persistence. They would study • Observation and analysis of social networks
the effects of memes and counter-memes • Social media in strategic communication
on the human mind and behavior to deter- • Narrative networks and strategic narratives
mine how to best engineer and propagate • Genetic profiling
them. These directorates would consist of the • Cultural anthropology and cultural awareness
Neurological, Psychological, Sociological,
and Cybernetical Artificial Intelligence The Cybernetical/AI Directorate would per-
Directorates. The neurological field (e.g., form meme-related R&D concerning:
brain scans) would be focused primarily
on i-memes, while the psychological (e.g., • Memetic ontology
psychological testing, game theory) is con- • Natural language processing
• Text, document, data, and concept mining
cerned with both i-memes and e-memes.
• Statistical semantics
The sociological (e.g., modeling and simula- • Network theory
tion of social networks) and cybernetical/AI • Information theory and entropy
(e.g., natural language processing) fields are • Control theory and intelligent control systems
focused on e-memes. • Genetic, memetic, and evolutionary algorithms
The Neurological Directorate would per- • Neural networks and deep learning
form meme-related R&D concerning: • Expert systems
• Context-based reasoning
• Neuroimaging: fMRI and Positron Emission
Tomography (PET)
Information from a variety of sources, such
• Pharmacological manipulation
as databases, documents, social and commu-
• Electro-encephalography (EEG)
• Event-Related Potential (ERP) nications networks, and human intelligence
• Electromyography (EMG) (HUMINT) would be obtained and input at
• Single neuron recording the Meme Processing Center. Vast quantities
• Hormone and blood analyses of information would be processed autono-
mously with machine intelligence employing
The Psychological Directorate would perform a variety of tools and techniques, such as natu-
meme-related R&D concerning: ral language, information mining, network
analysis, and semantics, where value-driven
• Psychological testing
logic, in conjunction with a World Model and
• Behavioral testing
Knowledge Database, would extract and eval-
• Modeling Theory of Mind
• Decision-making uate detected memes. The World Model
• Game Theory includes entities, events, images, networks,
• Uncertainty and risk and psychological, cultural, and behavioral
• Learning/adaptation phenomena. The Value Judgment module con-
• Utility and valuation tains algorithms to determine, for the detected
• Motivation memes, priority, utility, importance, propaga-
• Emotion tion, impact, and persistence.
• Trust and attachment Detected memes that are considered to be
• Addictive behavior important and to require countermeasures
• Belief systems
are passed to the Counter-Meme Processing
Center. Creating counter-memes will be more
The Sociological Directorate would perform
labor-intensive than detecting memes in the
meme-related R&D concerning:
environment, requiring a multi-disciplinary
• Modeling and simulation of social networks Meme Engineering staff consisting of cul-
• Rumor and Gossip Theory and Rumor and Gossip tural anthropologists, linguists, psycholo-
Propagation Models gists, semanticists, social network software
512 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

designers and programmers, operations memes are countered and neutralized, and
research analysts, economists, joint intelli- that friendly memes are received as intended.
gence and PSYOPS analysts, and joint ser- An engineered meme or counter-meme may
vice personnel. be designed for optimum transmission and
With supervised automation, the selected reception. Processes already successful for
extant memes are assessed with metrics in the conventional advertising, ideological propa-
context of media and public opinion. Using ganda, neuromarketing, neuroeconomics,
various tools and techniques from the four and rumor mongering can be adapted for
Directorates, counter-memes are created and meme warfare, which would be enhanced by
passed to various Memetics Communications new technological tools and techniques.
Agencies (e.g., Intelligence, Joint PSYOPS, Since Dawkins’ (1976) presentation there
Joint Forces Information Operations, and have emerged a coterie of proponents, skep-
Public/Civil Affairs), which then propagate tics, and opponents of the concept of the
the counter-memes to the selected audiences meme. There has been no substantial effort
via viral marketing and multi-media (e.g., to test and develop the concept, or refute it.
social networks, gossip and rumor, TV, radio, Memetics needs a general theory, a theoreti-
newspapers, billboards, etc.) as needed. The cal foundation for the development of a sci-
counter-memes become memes whose effi- entific discipline. The concept needs a solid
cacy (efficiency and effectiveness) in terms pragmatic definition that is widely accept-
of the metrics of propagation, impact, and able. There must be an ability to make test-
persistence can then be evaluated by the able predictions and falsifiable hypotheses,
Meme Processing Center and feedback of as with any scientific discipline (Finkelstein,
the results provided to the Counter-Meme 2008). The discrete meme must be defined,
Processing Center. identified, and distinguished in the near
continuum of information, just as the dis-
crete gene can be identified (more or less)
in a string of DNA nucleotides. A quantita-
CONCLUSION tive basis for memes must be established,
perhaps by employing such tools and tech-
Cyberwarfare involves consequences of niques as evolutionary psychology; physi-
evolved psychology that are not yet well ological phenomena; information theory and
understood, such as the receptivity of indi- entropy; genetic, memetic, and evolutionary
viduals to memes that can alter their values algorithms; modeling and simulation; and
and behavior, and thus the values and behav- neuroeconomics.
ior of the culture or subcultures they com- A coherent memetics program of sufficient
prise. In turn, memes over time may have an duration can establish a solid scientific and
effect on evolved psychology. engineering basis for the information war.
Memes can be defined in a way that is Research in evolutionary psychology may
useful for establishing meme metrics, a identify why some human minds are more
scientific basis for memetics, and a meme receptive and reactive to some messages
engineering process. Memes may be defined over others, and how to best structure the
around the concepts of information, propa- content and format of the messages (memes)
gation, impact, and persistence. Meme and designed to modify the behavior of selected
counter-meme engineering, an essential populations. And most importantly, a wide-
component of cyberwarfare, may be instanti- spread effort is needed to establish a canon of
ated in a Meme Control Center. To succeed ethics to guide and constrain the development
in cyberwarfare, it is critical that adversarial and application of cyberwarfare memetics.
EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY AND CYBERWARFARE 513

REFERENCES FM-46-1 (1997), Public Affairs Operations, HQ


Department of the Army, Washington, DC.
Asher, Ely (2005), Disinfect Your Mind, Galiel. https://www.globalsecurity.org/military/
Net Publishing. library/policy/army/fm/46-1/46_1.pdf
Aunger, Robert (2002), The Electric Meme: A (Accessed 30 August 2020)
New Theory of How We Think. New York, Giordano, James (2017), Weaponizing the Brain:
NY: The Free Press. Neuroscience Advancements Spark Debate,
Blackmore, Susan (1999), The Meme Machine, National Defense.
Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Gladwell, Malcolm (2002), The Tipping Point:
Brodie, Richard (1996), Virus of the Mind: The How Little Things Can Make A Big Difference,
New Science of the Meme. Seattle, WA: Back Bay Books.
Integral Press. Gould, Stephen Jay (2007), Punctuated Equilib-
Buss, David M. (2005), Editor, The Handbook rium, Belknap Press: An Imprint of Harvard
of Evolutionary Psychology. Hoboken, NJ: University Press.
John Wiley & Sons. Hull, David L. (2000), Taking Memetics Seri-
Choi, Jin Nam (2003), How Does Context Influ- ously: Memetics Will Be What We Make It, in
ence Individual Behavior? PsycArticles: Jour- Aunger, Robert, Editor, ‘Darwinizing Culture:
nal Article, https://psycnet.apa.org/ The Status of Memetics as a Science’, (pp.
buy/2003-09567-004 (Accessed 30 August 43–67) Oxford University Press.
2020) Joint Publication (2003), 3-53 Joint Doctrine for
Council on Foreign Relations (2019), Cyber Threats Psychological Operations, The Joint Staff,
and Mid-Term Elections, www.cfr.org/blog/ Washington DC.
year-review-cyber-threats-and-mid-term-us- Joint Publication (2006), 3-13 Information
elections (Accessed 30 August 2020) Operations, The Joint Staff, Washington DC.
Dawkins, Richard (1976), The Selfish Gene, Joint Publication (2012), 3-13.4 Military Decep-
Oxford University Press. tion, The Joint Staff, Washington DC.
de Leon Huld, Nickee (2020), How Many Words Koch, Richard (1999), The 80/20 Principle,
Does the Average Person Know? https://word- Double Day Publishing Group.
counter.io/blog/how-many-words-does-the- Leon, Carlos (2017), Narrative as a Fundamen-
average-person-know/ (Accessed 10 July 2020) tal Information Unit in the Mind, A Standard
Dunbar, Robin (2005), The Human Story, Gard- Model of Mind: AAAI Technical Report
ners Books. FS-17-05.
Finkelstein, Robert (2006), A Memetics Com- Lynch, Aaron (1996), Thought Contagion, Basic
pendium, Defense Advanced Research Pro- Books.
jects Agency, Arlington, VA. McLeod, Saul (2009), Short Term Memory,
Finkelstein, Robert (2008), Defining Memes, Simply Psychology, www.simplypsychology.
Presented at The Second Symposium on org/short-term-memory.html (Accessed 10
Memetics Memory, Social Networks, and July 2020)
Language: Probing the Meme Hypothesis II, Orwell, George (1946), Politics and the English
Victoria College, University of Toronto. Language, Herbert W. Simpson Publ.
Finkelstein, Robert (2011), Tutorial: Military Poole, Steven (2006), Unspeak: How Words
Memetics, Presented at the Social Media for Become Weapons, How Weapons Become A
Defense Summit, Alexandria, Virginia. Message, And How That Message Becomes
Finkelstein, Robert (2012), Neuro-Cognitive A Reality, Grove Press.
Warfare: Accelerated Cultural Evolution Prosser, Michael B. (2006), Memetics: A Growth
(ACE) To Prevent Terrorism and Mass Atroci- Industry in U.S. Military Operations, Thesis,
ties, Presented at The Center for Strategic School of Advanced Warfighting, Marine Corps
Counterterrorism Communications, Office of University, US Marine Corps, Quantico, VA.
the Under Secretary for Public Diplomacy RAND Corporation (2019), Cyber-Warfare,
and Public Affairs, US Department of State, www.rand.org/topics/cyber-warfare.html
Washington, DC. (Accessed 10 July 2020)
514 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Shannon, Claude (1948), A Mathematical Edward (2020), Robot Memetics: A Space


Theory of Communication, Reprinted with Exploration Perspective, Springer.
corrections from The Bell System Technical University College London (2017), Humans are
Journal, 27: 379–423, 623–656, July, hard-wired to follow the path of least resist-
October. ance, Science Daily. Retrieved from https://
Sharp, Kim and Franz, Matschinsky (2015), www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2017/02/
Translation of Ludwig Boltzmann’s Paper 170221101016.htm (Accessed 15 June
‘On the Relationship between the Second 2020)
Fundamental Theorem of the Mechanical US Senate (2019), Report of the Select Commit-
Theory of Heat and Probability Calculations tee on Intelligence, United States Senate, on
Regarding the Conditions for Thermal Russian Active Measures Campaigns and Inter-
Equilibrium’ (1909), Entropy, 17 (4), ference in the 2016 U.S. Election, Volume 2:
1971–2009. Russia’s Use of Social Media with Additional
Truszkowski, Walt., Rouff, Christopher., Views, 116th Congress, Washington, DC.
Akhavannik, Mohammad and Tumstel,
Index
Abadinsky, H. 299, 300, 302, 305, 307 Alexander, R. D. 57, 178
Abbas, M. J. 36 ALFUS 365
ABC model 4, 5, 17n1 Allcott, H. 344
Abed, R. 24–50 Allen, C. 367
Abelson, R. P. 382 Allen, F. A. 224
abuse Allen, M. W. 318
of animals 148–50 Allen, N. B. 34
health effects 128, 133 Almquist, A. J. 418, 419, 422, 425
substance 13–14, 16, 37–8 Alper, M. 278
see also drugs Alpert, G. P. 212
accuracy of information 344–5 Alschuler, A. W. 223, 224, 225
Acemoglu, D. 179, 340 altruism 173–4, 303–4
Ache tribe 272 empathy 272
adaptation 477–8 health 133
coalitional violence 321, 326 intrasexual competition 282
suicide 57–9, 61, 65 parochial 320–1
trust 340 punishment 227
see also natural selection reciprocal 173, 174, 427
adaptive lags 114–15 sperm donors 490
Adelman, M. B. 483–4 suicide 57–8, 59
adolescent-limited offenders (AL) 193 vaccination 136
Adults in the Making program 286 Alzheimer’s disease (AD) 41
adversarial allegiance 233 ambush training 210, 215
age American Psychiatric Association 25, 32,
Alzheimer’s disease 41 35, 39
criminal behavior 192–6 Amiot, C. E. 147
mental disorders 31 amygdala 116
suicides 56–7 analytical rumination hypothesis 34
age-crime curve 192, 193, 194, 195, 196, analytical therapy 6
198, 284 anchoring effects 232
age-graded theory 192–6, 280 Anderson, C. A. 374, 381–3
agency 392 Anderson, J. 336, 457–76
aggression 300–1 Anderson, M. 339
costly traits 418 Andreasen, N. 25
dangerous driving 374–97 Andrews, D. A. 279–80, 287, 288
displays 208 Andrews, P. W. 34
policing 203–20 animals
retaliation 258 abuse 148–50
Aggression Questionnaire 381 ethics 144–68
agriculture experimentation 155–6
organized crime 309–10 suicide 53
and warfare 324–5 Anna, K. 296
Aharoni, E. 221–42 anorexia nervosa (AN) 38–40
Ahmad, N. 272 anthropomorphism
Ahuvia, A. C. 483–4 artificial intelligence 342–3
Akil, H. 24 ethics 147–8, 153
Albanese, J. S. 298 anthrozoology 147
Albert, W. D. 286 antisocial behavior 190–5, 201,
Albuquerque, U. P. 107–22 277, 280
Albus, J. S. 353, 354, 355, 356 conscience 231
alcohol addiction 37–8 intrasexual competition 282–4
Alderson-Day, B. 98 Antonopoulos, G. A. 305, 307
516 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

anxiety Bandura, A. 376, 377, 379, 381, 382–3, 391–2, 401


artificial intelligence 334 Barash, D. P. 207–8
disorders 12, 16 Bargh, J. A. 114
mindfulness 94 Barkow, J. H. 265, 266, 407, 413
Apaolaza-Ibáñez, V. 109, 110, 111, 114 Barnes, J. C. 195
Apostolou, M. 40 Barnett, J. 160
Appel, M. 146 Barone, G. 339
Appleton, J. 114 Barraclough, B. 73
archetypes 6 Barraket, J. 483, 484, 486
Arluke, A. 148, 149 Barrett, H. C. 109, 111, 112, 115
Arnett Inventory of Sensation Seeking 387 Barton, S. 107, 108–9, 114, 117
Arnett, J. 387 Bashir, N. Y. 161
Arnhart, L. 248 Bastian, B. 147
Arnon, S. 440 Bates, H. 399
Arrow, K. 175 Bateson, P. 459
artificial intelligence 333–51, 427 Batson, C. D. 272
Artificial Moral Agents 367 Baumard, N. 161
artificial neural networks (ANN) 357 Baumeister, R. F. 62, 63, 67
Asendorpf, J. B. 485 Bay, B. 490
Asimov, I. 353, 367, 369 Beach, S. R. H. 286
assisted reproductive technology (ART) 481, 488–9 Beaulieu, D. A. 211
associational criminal structures 299 Beaver, K. M. 195
Atkin, C. K. 376, 377, 378, 384, 385 Beccaria, C. 223, 271
Atkinson, A. B. 339 behavior manipulation 337
Atkinson, J. A. 110 behavioral ecology 128, 297
attachment theory 34, 41, 404, 405, 406 behavioral plasticity 129, 137, 461
attacker–defender asymmetries 321–2, 323–4 Belsky, J. 30, 281–2, 285, 286, 290
attention Belzung, C. 75
dangerous animals 116 Bendor, J. 270
mindfulness 96–7 Bentham, J. 223
attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) 31 Bercovitch, F. B. 463, 467
attractiveness 284, 342, 405, 479–80, 485, 487 Berg, J. 340
Aunger, R. 352, 500 Bergen, N. 490
Aureli, F. 244 Berger, J. 334
autistic-spectrum disorder (ASD) 31, 32, 36 Bering, J. M. 52–3, 56, 58, 59, 65
autonomic nervous system (ANS) 266 Berkowitz, L. 382
autonomous military robots 365–7, 368 Beullens, K. 376–7, 378, 385, 386
autonomous vehicles 353, 355, 360, 361–3, 365, 367, bias
368–9 implicit 209, 215
autopoiesis 361, 370 punishment 232–3, 237–8
Autor, D. 340 race 209, 337
availability heuristic 232 Biddle, S. 320, 321
Awad, E. 146 Bieling, P. J. 102
Axelrod, R. 174 The Big Bang Theory 412–13
Axial Age 94 Binder, A. 209, 210
biological sensitivity to context theory 278, 281–2, 291
Badcock, C. 36 biophilia 113–14, 115, 116–17, 148
Badcock, P. B. 34 bipolar disorder (BPD) 42
Baden, J. 262–3 Birch, L. L. 160
Baechler, J. 64 Bird Box 380
Bagehot, W. 174–5 Bird, D. W. 317
Bailey, D. H. 262 birds, death response 466–7
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. 277, 281, 285, 286–7, Biro, D. 457, 463, 465
290 Biswas-Diener, R. 98
Balinski, M. L. 180 Bittman, M. 157
Bancroft, J. 61 Bittner, E. 203, 206, 214, 216
Bandes, S. 268–9 Black Lives Matter 205
INDEX 517

Blackmore, S. 500 Bribiescas, R. G. 56


Blair, C. 99 Brody, G. H. 286
Blanchard, D. C. 71 Broesch, J. 111, 115, 116
Blome, M. W. 108 Brooke, P. 152
Bloom, P. 148 Brosnan, S. F. 12
Blumenstein, L. 211 Brosschot, J. F. 97–8
Boaz, N. T. 418, 419, 422, 425 Brower, D. 79n4
Bockman, J. 151, 156 Brown, C. 480
Boden, T. 436, 439 Brown, D. 53, 225
body language 407 Brown, G. R. 115, 247
Boehm, C. 271, 300 Brown, G. W. 75
Boesch, C. 470 Brown, M. J. F. 461
Bohm, R. 260 Brown, R. M. 58, 62, 253
Bokek-Cohen, Y. 485 Brown, S. L. 253
Bolen, J. 267 Brune, M. 24, 27, 28, 29, 30, 33, 34, 35, 41
Bolhuis,J. J. 110 Brunero, J. 262
Bolig, R. 484 Bruni, F. 411
Bollard, L. 144, 156 Bryant, C. 160
Boltzmann, L. 503 Bryson, J. 333–51
Bonta, J. 279–80, 287, 288 Buckels, E. 67
Boot, M. 324, 325 Buckholz, J. 259, 270
Booth, D. A. 156 Buckner, W. 321–2
borderline personality disorder 11 Buddhism 94–7, 98, 102
Borone, E. J. 25 Budge, S. L. 16
Bossema, E. R. 491 Bugental, D. B. 211
Bostrom, N. 146 Bukowski, W. M. 211
Botanov, Y. 8 bulimia nervosa (BN) 38–40
Bourgeois, C. A. 301 Bullitt 376
Bourgeois, P. 303 Buonanno, P. 260
Bowlby, J. 6, 34, 41, 110, 209 Burger, O. 436
Bowles, S. 321 Burger, W. 260
Boyce, W. T. 281 Burgess, J. W. 175
Boyd, D. 334, 335 Burkhardt, J. M. 343
Boyd, R. 304 Burns, J. 35
Boyer, P. 177, 318, 480 Bushman, B. J. 374, 381–3
brachiation 108–9 Bushway, S. D. 249
brachycephalic dogs 154 Buss, A. H. 381
Bradford, E. E. 336 Buss, D. M. 15, 66, 77, 180, 199–201, 204, 235, 250,
Bradshaw, J. W. 147 282, 283, 291, 375, 378, 382, 403, 404, 413,
brain 368 479–80, 485, 488, 491–2
emotions 266–7 Buston, P. M. 479, 488
group size 262
mass media 407, 408, 411, 413 cadaverine 468
memes 499–500 Cahan, S. 461
neocortex size 262 Camilleri, J. A. 283
organoids 370 Campbell, A. 289
rationality & emotion 267 Campbell, T. S. 100
romantic love 410 cannibalism 422, 460, 463–4, 468, 470
size 262 Cannon, W. B. 175
theory of mind 353–5 Canton, R. 265, 269
trolley problem 368 Caplan, B. 345
see also cognitive abilities carbon emissions 435–56
Braithwaite, J. 272–3 carbon legacies 436, 438–9, 449
Braithwaite, V. 145, 146 caregiver distress 253
Brand, P. W. 63, 65 Carmi, N. 440
Brazier, Y. 366 Carmody, J. 94–106
Brézillon, P. 357 Carneiro, R. L. 309
518 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

carnism 147, 149 Chu, C. 58


Carr, W. J. 467 Churchill, W. 159
cars civil war 325
autonomous 353, 355, 360, 361–3, 365, 367, civil-rights movements 150
368–9 Cladder-Micus, M. 102
commercials 384–5 Clark, A. G. 418, 419–20, 423
dangerous driving 374–97 Clark, B. C. 307
racing 385, 387–8, 389 Clark, M. 159
speeding 388–92 classical conditioning 209
Carstensen, L. L. 379 clean meat 159–60
Carter, G. 75 Clear, T. 251
Carter, S. 209 Cleveland, H. H. 286
Cartwright, J. 422, 425, 427 cliff edge fitness functions model 35–6
Carvalho, S. 457, 465, 466, 470 climate change 435–56
case conceptualization 4–5 clinical heterogeneity 30
Cassiers, T. 339 cloning 155, 173
Caudell, M. A. 435–56 co-morbidity 32
causal heterogeneity 30 coalitional aggression, warfare 316–30
causality 26–9, 44 coalitions, political institutions 180
Cavanagh, J. T. O. 73 Cochran, J. C. 278, 280
Cave, S. 334 cognitive abilities
Caviola, L. 146, 147, 155 emotion effect 363
celebrities warfare 317
mass media 404, 411–13, 414 see also brain
romantic love 410 Cognitive Behavioral Therapy (CBT) 4, 12, 13, 290
tabloid media 405 cognitive competence 64–5
Werther effect 379 Cognitive Evolutionary Therapy for Depression
Cerel, J. 51 (CETD) 8–10, 16
Chagnon, F. 73 Cognitive Evolutionary Therapy for Personality Disor-
Chagnon, N. A. 271 ders (CET-PD) 11
Chamberlain, J. M. 12 cognitive niche 426
Chan, D. 99 cognitive psychology 131, 353, 427
Chapman, C. A. 317 cognitive social learning 280
Chapman, L. J. 317 Cognitive Therapy (CT) 10
Chapple, A. 55 Cohen, J. 406, 435, 439, 450
Chavan, S. 336 Cohen, P. 445
cheating 136–7, 173, 178, 207, 264, 304 Cohen, R. 153
mate choice 480 Coleman, J. L. 222
punishment 227–9, 230–1 Coleman, L. 377, 379
reciprocity 258 collective action problem 320, 323
tit-for-tat 272 collective efficacy 196
Cheng, J. T. 211 collectivization 176–7
chess playing 375 Colley, K. 115
Chiao, J. Y. 180 Colomer, J. M. 179
Chicago school of criminology 196 commitment in relationships 14–15
children/adolescents common mental disorder (CMD), suicide 73, 75–6
animal abuse 149–50 communication
celebrity attachments 404 fluency 425–6, 429–30
death response 469 mass media 398–416
delinquent behavior 273, 285–6 oral speech 417–34
morality 261 strategic 401
parent incarceration 251 suicide as 60–2
punishment 378–9 community supervision 278, 285, 287, 289
Chiurliza, B. 79 Community-orienteered policing (COP) 215
Choe, D. H. 460 compensatory adaptation 418, 428–31
Choi, J.-K. 321 complexity theory 401
Cholbi, M. 59 Component Meme Concept (CMC) 504
INDEX 519

computational theory of mind 353–5 incarceration 243–57


Condorcet, Marquis de 172, 178 organized 296–315
Confer, J. C. 300 policing 203–20
Connolly, K. 227, 234 recidivism 277–81, 287, 289–91
conscience 230–1, 266 sentencing 221–42
consciousness, robots 364–5, 367 crime-reduction effect 249
consequentialism, animal ethics 145 Crisis Intervention Team (CIT) 214
control behavior 270 Cronk, L. 303–4
Cook, M. 116 Cross, D. J. 467
Coolidge effect 15 Crow, J. F. 31
Cooney, N. 151, 157, 159 Crow, T. J. 35
Coontz, S. 482 Crowley, T. J. 439
cooperation 222, 261–2 cuckoldry 283, 290
artificial intelligence 335–6 Cullen, F. T. 277, 278, 287, 288, 290
e-collaboration 417–18, 428–31 Culotta, E. 79
health 133 cultural engineering 499
ingroups/outgroups 210, 215 cultural evolution 304, 499
organized crime 297, 303–8, 310–11 Currie, E. 260
political institutions 171–4, 176, 178 Cushman, F. 229, 230, 236
retaliation 226 cuteness, animal ethics 152–4, 155, 162
vaccination 136 cyberwarfare 499–514
voter suppression 180
warfare 317–18 Daft, R. L. 407
coordination, artificial intelligence 335–6 Dahl, R. 175
coping mechanisms 12 Dahlen, E. R. 383
copycat effect 379–81 Dahlgreen, W. 160
Corning, P. A. 176 Daly, M. 194, 195, 196–9, 227, 234, 235, 284, 288–90,
Cornwell, R. E. 79 323, 375, 376, 378–9
cortical dysconnectivity hypothesis 35 Damasio, A. R. 97, 363
Cosmides, L. 56, 59, 65, 66, 70, 71, 78, 110, 114, 116, danger, impulsive behavior 130
244, 245, 259, 272, 303, 317 dangerous animals, response to 116
Coss, R. G. 108 Daniels, K. R. 489, 491
cost–benefit 12 Dart, T. 214
analysis 173, 208 Darwin, C. 52, 53, 172, 189, 221, 266, 352–3, 370,
outcomes 4, 7 409, 420
punishment 233–4 Das, S. 462, 464, 465, 466
trust 341–2 David, K. 406
warfare 317, 318–19, 321, 323 Davidson, C. 181
costless traits 419–20, 422–4, 426 Davies, N. 208
costly traits 418–28 Davies, P. C. W. 177
Coulthard, T. J. 109 Davis, L. 358
counseling 3–23 Davis, M. 223
counter-exploitation strategies 243, 244–5, 253 Davis, S. 226
counteractive niche construction 247–8 Dawkins, R. 56, 58, 177, 207, 304, 499, 500–2,
counterpropaganda 506 505, 512
couples therapy 14–16, 17 De Backer, C. J. 405
Cowhig, M. 277, 278 De Dreu, C. K. W. 321
Cox, G. W. 180 de Leon Huld, N. 503
Craig, M. 267 De Marco, A. 463, 464
Craig, T. 115 de Villiers, B. 30
Crank, J. P. 212 de Visser, E. J. 342
Craparo, G. 303 de Waal, F. 244, 264–5, 270
Creanza, N. 179 de-escalation 213–14, 216
Crespi, B. J. 31, 32, 36 death, responses to 457–76
crime 188–202 death penalty 259, 261, 268–9, 271
copycat 380 DeCatanzaro, D. 56, 58, 60, 62, 65
corrections and rehabilitation 277–95 decentering 102
520 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

deep learning 357 Dudley, R. 38


deep neural networks (DNN) 357–8 Dunbar, R. I. 262, 480, 481, 484
deep time 189 Dunbar’s Number 262
Defense Advanced Research Projects Agency Duntley, J. D. 54, 55
(DARPA) 506 Durant, A. 271
DeGregori, J. 44 Durant, W. 271
Deikman, A. J. 101 Durisko, Z. 30
Del Giudice, M. 25, 26, 27–9, 31, 32, 33, 36, 41–2 Durkheim, E. 53, 59, 63, 271
Delacroix, C. 490 Durose, M. R. 278
delay discounting 131 Durrant, R. 243, 246–8, 296–315
DeLoache, J. S. 116 Dworkin, R. 222
Delton, A. W. 227–8 dysfunctional thinking 4, 9, 10, 12
DeLuca, D. C. 428, 429 dysmorphic disorder 12–13
democracy 177–81
Demographic Transition Theory 117 e-collaboration 417–18, 428–31
demographics, climate change 435 Eagle, N. 335
Denzler, M. 229, 234 Easton, D. 175
depression 6–10, 16, 24, 32–4 Eastwick, P. W. 486
antisuicide 76 eating disorders 13, 16, 31, 38–40
HTTLPR gene 30 Eaves, L. J. 30
meta-awareness 102 Eccleston, C. 70
parental age 31 Eckel, C. C. 342
Desai, R. A. 386 Edgar, B. 66
deterrence 223–9, 231, 233–7, 249, 260, 263 education, carbon emissions 436, 438–42, 444–6,
Deuze, M. 399 448–51
Devlin, P. 264 Efklides, A. 67
diametrical model of psychosis 36 Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. 204
Diamond, J. 148, 153 Eisenberger, N. I. 64, 70
dictatorship 176, 178 electoral systems 177–81
Dietz, T. 435, 439, 444 Elish, M. C. 334, 335
Diez, L. 461 Ellis, A. 4, 5, 179
differential susceptibility 30, 278, 281–2, 285–7, 290–1 Ellis, B. J. 281
diffuse tensor imaging (DTI) 267 Elwood, R. W. 145, 146
direct reciprocity 226, 228, 304, 305–6, 308 Emlen, S. T. 479, 488
discriminate sociality 244 emotion
disease, memory 113 death response 457–9, 462, 464–5, 469
disengagement 34, 195 decision-making 266–7
moral 147 decoding accuracy 250–1
tactical 213 mass media 409
disgust robots 363–4
animal ethics 152, 155–6, 157, 160, 162 emotional exhaustion 252
distastefulness in insects 173 emotional intelligence (EI) 250–1
disinformation 343–4 emotional pathways 37
distastefulness in insects 173 empathy 265–6, 271–2
Dobzhansky, T. 189 animal ethics 147–8, 149, 152, 155–6, 157
Dodge, K. A. 250 mass media 408
Dolan, K. 180 employment, artificial intelligence 334, 339–40
dopamine 253, 259, 262 Engel, C. 70
Dos Santos, M. 270 Engh, A. L. 464
DRD4 gene 286 Engle, Y. 404
driving behavior, dangerous 374–97 Enke, R. 379–80
drugs entertainment
addiction 37–8 animal ethics 148–9
organized crime 296, 298, 299, 301, 302–3, mass media 408–9, 413
305–7, 311 Entertainment Software Association (ESA) 386
DSM 10, 25–6, 31–2, 34–5, 38–9, 41 entrepreneurial criminal structures 299
dual taxonomic theory 192–4, 195 entropy 502–3
INDEX 521

Environment of Evolutionary Adaptedness (EEA) fender defenses 66–7, 71


110–11, 209 Ferguson, C. J. 40
environmental sciences 107–22 Fernyhough, C. 98
Epperly, B. 180 Ferreira, W. S. Jr 107–22
Eren, O. 233 Ferrill, A. 324
Erikson, E. 404, 409 fertility 40, 179, 487
Ernst, E. 491 age 56, 132
erotomania 411–12 bipolar disorder 42
escape, suicide as 62, 63–5 carbon emissions 435–6, 437–42, 444–51
Escobar, P. 299, 305 health 123
essentialism 401 schizophrenia 35, 36
ethics in vitro fertilization 479, 481, 488–92
animal 144–68 wealth 117, 173
autonomous military robots 366, 368 see also reproduction
behavior manipulation 337 Fessler, D. M. T. 156, 157, 469
robots 367–9 feuds 271
suicide prevention 76 Feygin, D. L. 7, 12
see also morality ‘fictive kin’ 305, 308
eugenics 173, 176 Fiddes, N. 156
European Convention on Human Rights 243 Fiddick, L. 226
Evolutionarily Stable Strategy (ESS) 173–4, 178, 207 ‘fight or flight’ 209–10, 363, 382
evolutionary by-product models 35 films
evolutionary democracy hypothesis 179 copycat effect 380–1
Evolutionary Fitness Scale (EFS) 9, 10 dangerous driving 376–7, 384, 385, 387, 388–92
exercise 132 empathy 408
explore/ exploit dilemma 176, 177 The Fast and Furious 376, 377, 380–1, 385,
External Meme Center (EMC) 509–10 388–92
external memes (e-memes) 501–2, 511 Fink, B. 284
externalizing behaviors 286 Finkel, E. J. 482, 484, 485, 486
extrinsic risk 437–8, 439, 440, 450–1 Finkelstein, R. 352–73, 499–514
eye movement desensitization and reprocessing First, M. B. 16, 25, 75
(EMDR) 7 Fischbacher, U. 227
Fischer, P. 376, 386, 387
Facebook 345, 403 Fisher, H. 410
Fachner, G. 209 Fisher, M. L. 301, 405
facial emotion decoding accuracy 250–1 Fisher, R. A. 173, 336
factory farming 150, 151, 157, 159 Fiske, S. T. 251
Fagan, J. 215, 216 Fisman, R. 485
Fair, C. C. 323 fitness
fake news 343–5 artificial intelligence 334
Falger, E. L. F. 58 costly traits 418
Falger, V. S. E. 58 crime 244, 250–1, 254
Fanèovièová, J. 116 drug use 38
Fanjul-Moles, M. L. 460, 461 life history theory 436–9
fantasies 404, 409–10, 411–13 new traits 419–20
Farb, N. A. S. 100 primary goods 248
Farberow, N. L. 60 prosocial behavior 253
Farrow, T. 266 public health 123
The Fast and Furious 376, 377, 380–1, 385, 388–92 punishment 226, 227, 228, 231–2
fast–slow life history 28–9, 31, 32, 33, 36–7, 38, 40–2 reproduction 477–8
Fast Track Randomized Controlled Trial 286 suicide 52, 54–6, 61–3
fear, threat perception failure 209–10 warfare 318
Fehr, E. 227, 308 see also inclusive fitness
Feld, E. 171–87 Fitzgerald, A. J. 150
Feldman, M. W. 179 fleeing famine hypothesis 39
Felson, R. B. 211 Fleischman, D. S. 144–68
feminism 180, 181 Flynn, C. P. 149
522 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Foley, R. 107, 110 gender 191–2


Folkes, V. S. 483 Aggression Questionnaire 381
food animal ethics 152, 154–5
degrees of suffering 151–2, 156–8 attractiveness 284
taboos 156 clean meat 160
vegetarians 151, 155, 157, 158–61 couples therapy 14–15
forgiveness 272–3 crime 190–2, 199–200
Foster, T. L. 469 dangerous driving 374, 375–6, 378–9, 383,
Fox, M. 41 386–7, 390
Fox, R. L. 179 depression 33–4
Fradella, H. F. 203 drug use 38
Francione, G. L. 145, 155 eating disorders 39
Francis, R. C. 205 electoral systems 180
Frank, H. 153 fandom 412–13
Frank, M. G. 153 incarceration 251
Frank, R. H 228, 230 intrasexual competition 282–3, 288–90
Frank, S. A. 178, 179, 180 mass media 403–5
Franklin, J. C. 58, 75 organized crime 301
Frazzetto, G. 486 police culture 210–11
free will, robots 364–5 schizophrenia 35
Freeman, T. 491 sensation seeking 382
freerider problem 176 two-fold cost of males 478
Freud, S. 4, 5–6, 52, 79, 222 warfare 323
Frey, R. G. 145 young male syndrome 194, 195, 199, 374, 375, 383
Fridlund, A. J. 228, 229, 233 see also mate selection; parental investment
Fried, E. I. 33 General Aggression Model (GAM) 374, 381–4, 388,
Friesem, D. E. 109 391, 392
Frischmann, B. M. 176 genetic algorithms (GAs) 358–9
Frost, P. 110 genetic drift 423
FSD model 32 genetic noise 57
Fujisawa, K. K. 245 genetic plasticity 282, 285–6
Fuller, L. 222 genetic programming 358–60
funerary archeology 458 genetics 44
Funk, C. 155 mental disorders 29–31
Furman vs. Georgia 261 see also heritability
Füstös, J. 99 genomic imprinting 31
future discounting 38, 132, 197 Gentzkow, M. 344
Fyfe, J. J. 210, 213 Gest, T. 203
Ghirlanda, S. 153, 154
Gabriel, R. A. 325 Gil-White, F. J. 211
Gächter, S. 227 Gilbert, D. T. 97
Gaes, G. 259–60 Gilbert, P. 12, 13
Gale, J. 4 Giles, D. C. 406
Gallup, G. G. 58 Gillespie, J. H. 418, 419, 421, 422, 423
game theory 173–4, 175, 206–9, 227, 264, 303 Gillmor, C. S. 486
forgiveness 272 Gintis, H. 227
hawk-dove game 206–7, 216 Giordano, J. 505
see also Prisoner’s Dilemma (PD) Giosan, C. 3–23
Gandhi, S. G. 55 Gladwell, M. 508–9
Garcia, J. 147 Glantz, K. 14, 15–16
Gardner, J. 222 Glover, E. J. 424
gastroesophageal reflux 424–5 Glowacki, L. 300, 318, 319, 321–2
Gat, A. 318, 319, 321–2, 325 Gluckman, P. D. 26, 28
Gavrilets, S. 261 goal-directed behavior 130
Geary, D. 66, 96, 210, 211, 262 goals
Gebusi tribe 205 depression 7
Geher, G. 400 psychotherapy 4
INDEX 523

Godfrey-Smith, P. 335 Hammack, P. L. 334


Godin, J. 226 handicap principle 419
Goethe, J. W. von 379 Hanson, M. A. 28
Goldberg, D. 75, 76, 358, 359–60 Hardin, G. 118, 176, 177, 207
Goldberg, O. 377, 380 Harding, D. J. 249, 251
Goldstein, J. S. 323 harm-avoidance model 44
Gollwitzer, M. 229, 234 Harman, G. H. 77
Gonçalves, A. 457, 465, 466, 470 harmful dysfunction (HD) 25
Gone in 60 Seconds 380 Harper, D. G. C. 418, 419
Goode, E. 484, 485 Harris, E. C. 73
Goodenough, O. 269 Harris, M. 59
Goodfellow, I. 353, 357 Harris, N. 245
Goodwin, S. A. 251 Harris, T. O. 75, 76
Goodyer, I.73 75, 76 Hart, H. L. A. 222
Gorer, G. 469 Hartl, D. L. 418, 419, 423
Gottfredson, M. 261 Hartmann, P. 109, 110, 111, 114
Gould, S. J. 78, 362 Harvey, A. G. 75
governance crimes 298, 299 Hasen, R. 181
Grabe, M. E. 398, 400, 401, 403, 405, 407, 408, 412 Hatch, V. 268
Grabo, A. 180 hawk-dove game 206–7, 216
Gradual Exposure Therapy 13 Hawthorne, N. 258
Graetz, K. A. 417, 425–6, 428–9 Hayes, S. C. 100
Grammer, K. 481 Hazan, C. 406
Grand Theft Auto V 386 Heerwagen, J. H. 114
Grandjean, D. 250 Hegel, G. 223
Green, C. 151 Heintzelman, S. J. 67
Greenberg, B. S. 376, 384 Helfgott, J. B. 380
Greene, J. D. 145, 368 ‘helper’s high’ 253
Griebel, G. 71 Hendin, H. 76
Grigg, N. P. 466 Hennighausen, 407, 408, 409
Grofman, B. 180 Henrich, J. 178, 211, 270
Gross, J. 321 Henry, A. D. 117
group selection 36, 59, 304 Henry Ford Museum 399
group size Henry-Waring, M. S. 483, 484, 486
brain size relationship 262 herd immunity 136, 137
Hutterites 263 heritability 29–31, 204
groups eating disorders 39
tribalism 338 obesity 124–5
warfare 316–30 schizophrenia 35
see also ingroups/outgroups suicide 52
guilt 230–1, 245 Herrman, A. 368
Guisinger, S. 39 Herzog, H. 146, 147, 149, 152, 153, 155, 156, 157
Gullone, E. 150 Herzog, K. 154
Gunn, J. F. III 51–93 heterogeneity 30, 32
Gunnell, J. G. 175 hierarchy of needs 403
Gustavsson, L. 485 hijack hypothesis 37
Guttmacher, A. F. 489 Hill, A. 323
Hill, K. R. 317
Haaga, D. A. F. 12 Hill, P. 299, 306, 307
Hagen, E. H. 34, 60 Hill, R. A. 262
Haidt, J. 152, 153, 154, 160, 265, 267 Hirschfeld, K. 297, 310
Haig, D. 31 Hirschi, T. 261
Haldane, J. B. S. 173, 227 Hirsh-Pasek, K. 153
Hall, L. 363 Hitsch, G. J. 480, 487
Hamilton, A. 172 Hoaken, P. N. 250
Hamilton, W. D. 57, 58, 173, 174, 227, 250, 304 Hobbes, T. 174, 205, 222
Hamilton’s Rule 227 Hodal, K. 159
524 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Hodson, G. 161 inequality, artificial intelligence 338–40


Hoffman, F. 325 inequity aversion 270
Hoffman, M. B. 221–42 infidelity 14–15, 17
Hofstede, G. 441–2, 444, 450 Information–Propagation–Impact–Persistence (IPIP)
Hogg, M. A. 336 502, 504
Holmes, O. W. 222 ingroups/outgroups 210, 215, 300–1
Homberg, J. R. 30 group selection 304
Horowitz, A. V. 32 outgroup intolerance hypothesis 36
Horrobin, D. F. 35 punishment 233
HTTLPR gene 30 warfare 318, 321
Hu, S. 115 Insel, T. 26
Huber, F. 68 Instagram 403
Hublin, J. J. 108 Institute of Electrical and Electronics Engineers
Huffziger, S. 100 (IEEE) 337
Hull, D. L. 352, 500 Institutional Analysis and Development (IAD)
human–animal relations (anthrozoology ) 147 framework 177
human evolution 107–9, 209, 262, 435 institutions
communication 407 organized crime 308–10
death 457, 468 political 171–87
environments 107–18 instrumental goods 248
mass media 411 Integrated Motivational–Volitional Model of Suicidal
memes 499 Behavior (IMV) 63, 78
oral speech 424–5 intelligence of robots 355–6
theory of mind 353–4 inter-individual exploitation 243, 244
warfare 318–19 Internal Meme Center (IMC) 509–10
human rights 135, 243, 338, 366 internal memes (i-memes) 501–2, 511
human trafficking 298, 301 Interpersonal–Psychological Theory of Suicide (IPTS)
Hume, D. 171–2, 235 62–4, 78
Hume’s Gap see naturalistic fallacy intragenomic conflict 31
Humphrey, J. 213, 216 intrasexual competition 278, 282–4, 287–91, 378
Humphrey, N. 59, 64–6, 67, 79n5 Izquierdo, C. 468
Hunter, J. 489
Hurford, P. 158 Jablensky, A. 75
Hutterites 262–3 Jackson, J. C. 304
Huxley, J. 36 Jackson, S. 252
Huxley, T. H. 172 Jacobson, J. D. 8
Jacoby, S. 271
ICD-10 26, 31–2, 38–9, 41 Jain monks 149
identifiable victim effect 232 James, L. 203–20
Iglesias, T. L. 466 James, S. 212
Imel, Z. E. 16, 75 James, W. 100
imitation 376–81, 385 jealousy 15, 17
copycat effect 379–81 Jefferson, T. 172, 205
mass media 409 Jeong, J. 379
implicit bias 209, 215 Jervis, R. 321
impulsive behavior 130, 132 Jiang, J. 339
in vitro fertilization (IVF) 479, 481, 488–92 Jin, S. V. 404
incapacitation 223, 224–5, 227, 228, 233, 234, 236, Johnson, E. I. 251
244, 249, 260 Johnson, R. R. 210
incarceration see prison Joiner, T. E. 62, 69, 79
inceptive niche construction 247 Jonason, P. 405
inclusive fitness 7, 9, 33, 173, 245, 297 Jones, A. 344
organized crime 297, 310 Jones, J. M. 203
prisons 250, 251 Jones, O. D. 237
suicide 57–60, 62, 63 Jones, T. L. 318
indirect reciprocity 228, 304, 305–6, 308 Jonson, C. L. 278
industrial revolution 114, 126, 206 Jorgensen, C. 258–76
INDEX 525

Joy, M. 147 Knauft, B. M. 205


Julian, G. E. 461 knowledge communication 426–8, 430
Jung, C. G. 6 Koburger, N. 380
jurisprudence 221–42 Kock, N. 116, 406–7, 413, 417–34
Koivu, K. L. 309, 310
Kabat-Zinn, J. 96, 99, 100 Konopacki, M. 344
Kaeble, D. 277, 278 Kostermans, E. 387
Kahane, G. 145 Kotrschal, K. 147
Kahneman, D. 232 Koza, J. R. 358
Kamhawi, R. 403 Koziel, S. 483
Kaminski, J. 153 Krasnow, M. M. 227–8
Kanazawa, S. 195, 204 Krebs, J. 208
Kant, I. 223, 263–4, 269 Kriegman, D. 5
Kaplan, H. 436, 439 Kristiansen, J. E. 42
Kappeler, P. M. 53 Kropotkin, P. 172–3
Kappeler, V. E. 211 Krupnik, V. 7–8
Karwoski, L. 8 Kuhl, J. 67
Kasperbauer, T. J. 148 Kuhn, T. 43
Kassab, H. S. 301, 306, 307 Kurman, M. 357–8
Kasser, T. 404 Kurzban, R. 161, 244, 304
Katchadourian, H. 230
Kavan, M. G. 25 Lahr, M. 319
Kearney, J. 156 Laidre, M. E. 462
Keeley, L. H. 319, 324 Laland, K. N. 115, 177, 247, 459
keeper defenses 66–7, 69–75, 77–8 Lam, D. 4
Kendell, R. 75 Lamarckianism 352–3, 370, 399, 500
Kendler, K. S. 24, 30 Lambert, E. G. 252
Kennedy, N. 411 landscape, preferences 107, 108–11, 113–15, 117
Kennedy, P. 55 Lane, J. C. 180
Kenrick, D. T. 302 Lange, A. 307
Kessler, R. C. 75 language, coalitional aggression 317, 318
Kesteloot, C. 339 Lankford, A. 56, 57, 58, 59
Khadjavi, M. 307 Lantz, B. 301
Khot, T. S. 336 Lao, S. 99
Kiehl, K. A. 231, 233 lateral prefrontal cortex (LPFC) 266
Killingsworth, M. A. 97 Latimer, J. 273
Kime, P. P. 334, 335, 342 Latner, M. 171–87
kin Laub, J. H. 192, 280
fictive 305, 308 Lauder, G. V. 77
investment 7 Lavoie, K. 491
organized crime 308 law
suicide effects 55–6, 61 behavioral rules 367
see also kin selection early legal codes 261
kin selection 173, 174, 304 jurisprudence 221–42
animal ethics 152–3 policing 203–20
punishment 227 robots 367, 369
suicide 57–8, 59 Lawless, J. L. 179
King, J. 260 Lawrence, C. K. F. 159
King, L. A. 67 Layard, R. 67
King, R. 203, 208 Lazar, S. W. 100
Kinney, D. K. 35, 58 Lazer, D. M. 343
Kirby, S. 301 leader–follower dynamics 322–3
Kirillova, G. P. 13 Leary, M. R. 62, 63, 244
Kleemans, E. 300 LeDoux, J. 363
Klein, R. 66, 259, 264 Lee, K. M. 406
Klinger, D. A. 216 Lee, S. A. 334
Klonsky, E. D. 73 Leech, B. L. 303–4
526 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

legal positivism 222 McCullough, M. 236, 304


legal realism 222–3 McCutcheon, L. E. 414
Leibbrandt, A. 270 Macdonald, C. J.-H. 68
Leigh, E. G. 178 MacDonald, G. 63
Leiter, B. 222, 223 McDonald, M. M. 300–1
Lengel, R. H. 407 McGaffin, B. J. 14
Lenton, A. P. 483 McGann, A. 178, 179
Leon, C. 507 McGirr, A. 79n3
Lesch, K. P. 30 McGrath, J. J. 35
Lester, D. 56, 58, 62, 63, 78, 79 McGuire, M. 4, 7, 9
Letterman, D. 411 Machado, C. 344
Levi, M. 297–8 Machan, T. R. 145
Levinson, D. B. 100 Machine learning (ML) 335, 358
Levy, J. S. 325 MacInnis, C. C. 161
Lew-Levy, S. 148 McKay, H. D. 196
Lewis, D. M. G. 470 McKibbin, W. F. 273
Lewontin, R. C. 78 Mackie, J. 263
Liang, Y. 334 McLaren, M. E. 159
Lieberman, M. D. 64, 70 McLaughlin, C. 481
Lieberman, P. 424, 425 McLeod, S. 503
life expectancy 117 McNally, R. J. 66
climate change 437, 440–1 McNiel, D. E. 250
crime 196–8 Macovei, M. 243
life history theory (LHT) 28–9, 32, 36–7, 38, 40, 41, McPherson, M. 344
42, 130, 131–2, 435, 436–51 McQueen, R. J. 428
life-course persistent offenders (LCP) 193 Madison, J. 171–2
limbic system 145, 266–7 Madrigal, A. C. 159
override 265, 266 Mafia 298, 300, 301, 302, 305–10
Lin, P. 369 Maier, K. H. 306, 307
Lindemann, B. 156 mail-order brides 485
Linehan, M. M. 60 major depressive disorder (MDD) 25, 32, 34
linoleic acid 460 Malamuth, N. M. 180, 401, 404
lipid metabolism hypothesis 35 male warrior hypothesis 300–1, 304, 310
Lipsey, M. W. 279, 288 Malo, Pablo 51–93
Lipson, H. 357–8 Malthus, T. 172
Little, A. C. 481 March, J. G. 176
LoBue, V. 116 market-based crimes 298
Locke, J. 205 Marks, I. F. M. 12
Loerinc, A. G. 12 Marois, R. 259, 270
Logan, C. 259–60 Martín-López, B. 116–17
Lopez, A. C. 316–30 Martlew, M. 227
López-García, L. M. 108 Maruna, S. 247, 248
Lopez-Perez, R. 270 Marx, K. 222
López-Riquelme, G. O. 460, 461 Masci, D. 155
Lorenz, K. 204 Masi, S. 465
loss of psychomotor energy (LoPE) 77, 78 Maslach, C. 252
Luks, A. 253 Maslow, A. 403
Lusk, J. 157–8 Mason, J. 157
Lutz, J. 100 mass media 398–416
Lynch, T. 180 Massey, D. 265, 267
Lynn, M. 484 Mastrofski, S. D. 215
mate guarding 15, 194
McAndrew, F. T. 484, 485 mate poaching 200
McBride, K. 264 mate selection 478–92
McCartney, K. 401–2 mass media 403, 411–12
McCarty, N. M. 336, 339 online dating 480–1
McCoy, D. R. 172 risky behavior 375, 382
McCulloch, W. 353 Matland, R. E. 180
INDEX 527

Matschinsky, F. 503 misinformation 343–4


Matsuzawa, T. 462, 465 mismatch 3, 6, 11, 27–9, 36, 125, 128, 137,
Mauer, M. 243, 251 232, 236
Mayer-Schönberger, V. 338 Alzheimer’s disease 41
Maynard Smith, J. 57, 173–4, 206–7, 208, 418, 419, depression 33
423, 477, 478 EFS 9
Mead, G. 401 organized crime 300
Mealey, L. 39, 41 SCH 40
Mears, D. P. 278, 280 SSDs 36
measles, antivaccination movement 136 substance abuse 13, 37, 38
meat consumption 147, 148, 151, 152, 156–61 suicide 58
media effects theory 376–7 Mitchell, D. 344
media equation 408, 411–12, 413 Mocan, N. 233
media naturalness 406–7, 413 Mocetti, S. 339
media richness 406–7, 413 modelling, risky behavior 374, 376–7, 380, 382–93
meditation 94–106 Moehl, M.-B. 14, 15–16
Mehling, W. E. 99 Moffitt, T. E. 192–3, 202n3
Meme Analysis Cell (MAC) 510 Mohorcich, J. 160
Meme Communications Cell (MCC) 510 Moir, R. D. 41
Meme Engineering Cell (MEC) 510 Monin, B. 161
Meme Warfare Center (MWC) 509–10 monogamy 178
memeplexes 503 Mood, A. 152
memes 499–512 Moore, J. H. 364
Memetics for Intelligence and National Defense Moore, M. 108, 264, 267
(MIND) 510–11 Moqbel, M. 420, 422
memory Moraitou, D. 67
dangerous animals 116 moral anthropocentrism 146
of diseases 113 moral disengagement 147
prosthetic 337–8 moral foundations theory 136
scripts 382, 383 morality 172
survival scenario 112 children 261
mens rea 229 death penalty 268–9
mental disorders 3–4 retribution 263–5
concept 25 robots 367
psychiatry 24–50 see also ethics
mental health Moran, T. 272
food consumption 128–9 Morikawa, T. 58, 59
immediate vs long-term rewards 131, 133 Morris, N. 336
meta-awareness 100, 102 Moscovici, S. 334
meta-cognition 102 Moskowitz, G. B. 337
Meystel, A. M. 353, 355, 356 ‘motherese’ 153
Military Deception (MILDEC) 506 Motto, J. 9–10
military memetics 505–12 Moura, J. M. B. 107–22
military robots 365–7, 368 Mugisha, J. 55
Millan, C. 204 Munday, Z. 461
Miller, G. 66, 161, 400, 409, 479, 483 Murray, H. A. 63
Miller, J. G. 175 Murtaugh, P. A. 435, 436, 439
Miller, M. B. 67, 79 music 402, 405
mindfulness 7, 94–106 mutation load 30–1, 35
Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction (MBSR) 96,
99, 100 Naghavi, M. 51
Mineka, S. 116 Nahmias, E. 229, 231, 233
Minervini, B. P. 484, 485 Nairne, J. S. 111, 334
minimal parental investment (MPI) 191–2, 200 Nakajima, S. 153
Minkov, M. 441 narcissism 404
Minson, J. A. 161 narratives 507–8
Mirville, M. O. 321 NASCAR 385, 387–8, 389
Mishara, B. L. 73 Nass, C. I. 405, 408, 413
528 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

National Incident-Based Reporting System observational learning 374, 376–8, 383, 392
(NIBRS) 289 ‘observing self’ 101
National Institute for Health and Care Excellence Obsessive Compulsive Disorder (OCD) 12, 31, 44
(NICE) 35 Odling-Smee, F. J. 246, 247
Natural Born Killers 380 offsetting behavior 132–3
natural law 222 Öhman, A. 12
natural selection 30, 172, 173, 189, 204, 477 oleic acid 460
behavioral rules 367 Oliphant, B. 268
emotions 265 omega-3 fatty acids 35
genetic programming 358, 360 online dating 480–1, 483, 486–8, 492
mass media 400, 401 operant conditioning 378
memes 500 optimal strategies 130–2
punishment 231–2, 236, 259 oral speech 417–34
robotics 352–3 Orbell, J. 58, 59
suicide 52, 53 Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Develop-
threat cues 209 ment (OECD) 339, 488
warfare 326 Orians, G. H. 111, 114
naturalistic fallacy 201, 235 Orsolini, L. 38
naturalistic mind 111–13 Ortiz-Ospina, E. 338
nature/nurture debate 400–2 Ortner, C. N. M. 101
Navarrete, C. D. 156 Orwall, B. 377, 392
necromones 460, 463, 466, 468 Orwell, G. 505
necrophoric behavior 458–62, 463 Osgood, R. E. 325
Nedelec, J. L. 188–202 ostracism 260
Need for Speed 387 Ostrom, E. 177
negative news 403 Otterbein, K. F. 319, 324
neocortex size 262 Otto, S. P. 477, 478
neoteny, animal ethics 152, 153–4 outgroups see ingroups/outgroups
Nese, A. 308 Owen, M. J. 35
Nesse, R. M. 7, 12, 24, 25, 26, 27, 33, 34, 35–6, 37, oxytocin 463
40–1, 42, 44, 63, 76, 209, 236, 302 Oyer, P. 481
Nettersheim, J. 40
Neuberg, S. L. 204, 209, 211 Pachirat, T. 150
neural networks 353, 357–8 pain, animal ethics 145–6, 151, 157
neurocognitive warfare 505–7 pain escape, suicide 62, 63–5
neurotoxin regulation hypothesis 37 ‘pain-and-brain’ framework 52, 64–5, 66–7, 69–70, 73,
Newsome, J. 277–95 75–6, 78
niche construction theory 246–7, 254 palmar grasp reflex 108
niche-building 402 Panagiotopoulos, P. 344
niches 177 Pancoast, W. 489
Nicholas, J. S. C. 153 panic attacks 101
Nielsen, M. 409 Paoli, L. 296, 297, 298, 300, 301, 306
Nisa 148 Paoline, E. A. 210, 211, 213
nociception 145 Papanicolaou, 305, 307
Nock, M. K. 73 Paquette, D. 211
noise theories, suicide 56–7 parasitism, political institutions 172
non-communicable diseases (NCDs), public health parasocial engagement 405–6
124, 134 parental investment 282–3, 288, 405, 478–9
non-social theories 33–4 life history theory 436–7, 439, 449–51
Norenzayan, A. 304 minimal 191–2, 200
normative law 222, 223 postmortem transport 458, 462–6
Norwood, F. B. 157–8 reproductive technology 489–90
Nowak, M. 174, 228, 264, 265, 266 risk-taking behavior 375
NR3C1 gene 286 Parker, G. A 78
Nutt, D. J. 70, 71 Parlapiano, A. 154
parochial altruism 320–1
Obama, B. 212 Paternoster, R. 249
obesity, public health 123–9, 134–5 pathogens, disgust sensitivity 156
INDEX 529

Patterson-Kane, E. G. 149, 150 Pobbe, R. 71


Paul, A. S. 159 policing, aggression/restraint balance 203–20
Paul, E. S. 147 Polimeni, J. 36
Paulhus, D. L. 67 political institutions 171–87
Pawlowski, B. 481, 483, 484 Poppelwell, Z. 304
Peck, J. H. 203 population growth 435
peer influences 378 Population Paradox 435, 438, 439–40, 448, 451
Peña-Guzmán, D. M. 53 Posner, M. I. 96
Peng, J. 375 Post, M. 159
Penn, D. J. 117–18 Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) 13
Penner, L. A. 253 postmortem transport 458, 462–6
Penney, S. 259 postpartum depression 11–12
Pennings, G. 490 poverty 338
Pentland, A. S. 335 Power, R. A. 33, 35, 36, 42
People for the Ethical Treatment of Animals (PETA) Pratt, T. C. 278
151, 157 predatory crimes 298–9
Perry, M. 381 prefrontal cortex 266–7, 368
Perry, S. A. 51, 59, 64, 66 Prehn, K. 269
personality Price, G. R. 173–4, 420
five dimensions 41 Price, J. 34, 36
traits 488 primary human goods 248
personality disorders (PD) 10–11, 16, 41–2 prison 243–57, 258, 260
Pessoa, L. 99, 266 revolving-door phenomenon 249
Peter, J. 487, 488 staff burnout 251–2, 253
Petersen, M. B. 177, 229, 230, 233, 243, 244–6, 253, Prisoner’s Dilemma (PD) 174, 176, 264, 303, 307–8
259, 318 privatization 176–7
Petrie, M. 418, 420, 426 probation 249, 251, 285
Petrinovich, L. 146, 153, 155 problem solving 409
Pettitt, P. 457, 458, 468, 470 The Program 380
pharmacophagy hypothesis 37–8 Prokop, P. 116
Phillips, C. J. 160 proportional representation 178, 179, 180
Phillips, D. P. 379 prosocial behavior 253, 282–3, 291
Phillips, T. 282 prostitution 298, 302, 303, 311
phobias 116, 209 protection payments 297, 299, 302, 310
anxiety disorders 12 Prounis, G. S. 467
distal causes 4 Prunetti, E. 11
Piette, A. 337 psychache 63–4, 69
Pincus, H. A. 16, 75 psychiatry 24–50
Pinel, J. P. J. 467 psycho-social factors, technology 360
Pinho, J. R. 116 psychoanalysis 5–6
Pinizzotto, A. J. 214 psychological mechanisms, obesity 127–9
Pinker, S. 67, 149, 150, 157, 235, 325, 424, 426 psychological operations (PSYOP) 506, 512
Piper, H. 149, 150 psychopaths 231, 267
Pitts, W. 353 psychopharmacology 42–3, 76
‘Pizzagate’ 504 psychotherapy 3–23
Pizzini-Gambetta, V. 301 public affairs 506–7
placebo effect 40–1 public health 123–43
plants punctuated equilibrium 362, 370
co-evolution 37–8 punishment 258–76, 304
landscape preferences 117 children 378–9
toxicity 116 corrections and rehabilitation 277–95
play crime 221–42
with animals 148–9 excessive 271
mass media 409–10, 413 incarceration 243–57
status seeking 211 see also retribution
pleiotropy 60, 61 Purzycki, B. G. 308
Plomin, R. 35 Pusatory, M. 214
Pluess, M. 281, 282 putrescine 468–9
530 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Puts, D. 323, 479 reintegrative shaming 272–3


Puurtinen, M. 210 Renninger, L. A. 484
reproduction 9
quasigovernment criminal structures 299 costly traits 418, 421
Quinlan, R. 435–6, 437, 442, 448, 451, 490 drug use 38
Quinsey, V. L. 283 IVF 479, 481, 488–9
mass media 400, 409
Raber, I. 336 oral speech 418
race technology 477–98
bias 337 see also fertility
implicit bias 209 Reproductive Suppression Hypothesis 39
policing 203, 206, 209, 212 Research Diagnostic Criteria (RDoC) 26
racism 175, 500 resource-holding potential (RHP) 207, 208, 216
stereotypes 181 Restak, R. 413
Rachlinski, J. J. 234 restitutive justice 271
Rådestad, I. 469 retaliation 226, 304
Radway, J. 404 retribution 223, 224–5, 229–30, 234–7, 259–65,
Radzinowicz, L. 260 267–9, 271
Ragatz, L. L. 303 Reynolds, A. 180
Rahr, S. 214 Riaz, A. 116
raids 300, 309–10, 318, 319, 320, 322, 325 Ricciardelli, R. 306, 307
Raine, A. 267 Rice, S. H. 418, 422
Ramos, R. A. 408 Rice, S. K. 214
Ramsden, E. 53 Richardson, G. 38
Rand, D. G. 272 Richter, D. 108
Randler, C. 116 Ridley, M. 326
rank theories 34 Riffkin, R. 150–1, 155
Rantala, M. J. 33, 34, 43 Riordan, D. 58
Rapaport, A. 174, 175 risk factors
Raphael, S. 251, 260, 288 extrinsic 437–8, 439, 440, 450–1
Rational Emotive Behavior Therapy (REBT) 9 organized crime 303
rationality 266–7, 363 risk-need-responsivity (RNR) model 277, 278–81,
Rauch, A. 151 286–7, 288, 291
Rauwolf, P. 333–51 risk-taking behavior 124, 131–2, 374–97
Rawls, J. 178 rituals 304
recalibration theory of counter-exploitation 244–5, 253 ‘Robbers Cave’ experiments 210, 215
Rechavi, O. 353 Robertson, C. T. 238
recidivism 277–81, 287, 289–91 robotics 352–73
reciprocal altruism 173, 174, 427 three laws of 367, 369
reciprocity 172–3, 261–2, 264–5 Rockenbach, B. 308
cheating 258 Rockman, H. 482–3
direct 226, 228, 304, 305–6, 308 Rodino, I. S. 490
indirect 228, 304, 305–6, 308 Roehrborn, C. 57
strong 174, 227, 270 Rogers, J. R. 79
trust 340–1 role models, media 406
reconciliation 272–3 Rollo, C. D. 460
Red Queen hypothesis 201, 478 romance novels 404
Reese, J. 150, 151, 159, 161 romantic love 410
Reeves, B. 405, 408, 413 Rosa, E. A. 435, 439, 444
reflective decisions 130, 132 Rose, H. 235
Regan, T. 145 Rose, S. 235
regularity principle 113 Rosebury, B. 261
rehabilitation 223, 224–5, 229, 231, 234–7, 243–4, Rosen, J. D. 301, 306, 307
245–9, 252–4, 260, 278 Rosenfeld, M. J. 483, 486
RNR model 277, 278–81, 286–7, 291 Roser, M. 338
Reiman, J. 205–6 Roshchina, V. V. 42
reinforcement 378–9 Rossi, P. H. 245
INDEX 531

Rothbart, M. K. 96 self-interest 221–2, 259


Rotsidis, A. 343 self-preservation instinct 62
Rottenberg, J. 33 selfishness 172
Roughgarden, 335 Sell, A. 244, 245, 253
Roulette, C. J. 302 senescence 56, 60
Rousseau, J.-J. 205 sensation seeking 381–2, 383, 385–7, 390–1
Roy, M. M. 153 sentencing 221–42
Rozin, P. 115, 160 Sentience Institute 150
Rueppell, O. 57 sentientism 145–6, 156
Rule, W. 180 serotonin 30
rumination hypothesis 34 Serpell, J. 147, 150, 152, 154, 155
Rusch, H. 321 Sesardic, N. 78
Russell, S. 123–43 sex ratios 178
Rusu, A. S. 243–57 Sexual Competition Hypothesis (SCH) 39–40
Ryder, R. D. 145 sexual fantasies 14
Ryninks, K. 469 sexual selection 36, 38, 39, 409, 412
sexually explicit media 404
Saad, L. 155 Shackelford, T. K. 15, 53, 59, 62, 64, 65, 204, 250,
Sadalla, E. 231 273, 283
St John-Smith, P. 24–50 shadchan 482–3
Salmon, C. 404, 405 shame 230–1, 272–3
Sampaio, M. B. 117 Shannon, C. 502, 503–4
Sampson, R. J. 192, 196, 280 Shapiro, F. 7
Sandøe, P. 154 Sharp, J. S. 151
Sandry, J. 112 Sharp, K. 503
Santos, M. L. S. 156 Shaver, P. 406
Sapolsky, R. M. 96 Shaw, C. R. 196
Saroglou, V. 304 Shem, S. 15
‘sati’ 95 Shepherd, B. 323
Savage, J. 204 Sherif, M. 210
savanna hypothesis 107, 108–11, 114 Sherman, G. D. 153, 154
Scarr, S. 401–2 Sherry, J. L. 401, 413
Scharf, P. 209, 210 Shi, A. 435, 438, 444
Scheib, J. E. 481, 490 Shi, F. 336
Scheidel, W. 309, 339 Shields, W. M. 467
Scheiter, S. 118 Shinar, D. 374, 375
Schick, K. 399, 401 Shiner, R. A. 223
schizophrenia spectrum disorders (SSD) 24, 31, 34–7 Shneidman, E. S. 60, 63, 69, 79
schizotypy 36–7 Shoemaker, P. J. 398, 403
Schlax, M. G. 435, 436, 439 Shonin, E. 96
Schlebusch, C. M. 108 Short, J. 407
Schnarch, D. 483 Shorter, J. 57
Schover, L. R. 490 Shostak, M. 148
Schramm, W. 398–9 Shrira, I 468–9
Schulkin, J. 63 Shugart, M. 179
Schultz, A. C. 359 Shultz, S. 262
Schwab, F. 407, 408, 409 Sigmund, K. 228, 266
Schwender, C. 409 Siles, I. 335
Scott, J. C. 309 Silva, R. H. da 107–22
Scottish Enlightenment 171 Silva, T. C. 107–22
Seabright, P. 478, 492 Silver, I. A. 277–95
secondary goods 248 Simons, R. L. 285
Segal, Z. V. 96, 99 Simons-Morton, B. 378, 388
Seiffert-Brockmann, J. 401 Singer, B. 14
Selby, E. A. 63 Singer, P. 145, 147, 156, 157
self-aware systems 364–5 Singhal, D. 374–97
self-defense 226 singles ads 484
532 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Skinner, B. F. 378 Sparrow, R. 342


Skopek, J. 486, 487 speciesism 146–7, 149, 155
Skyrms, B. 176, 178 speed dating 485–6
slaughterhouses 150 Sperber, D. 161
slavery, supply chain 159 sperm donation 481, 489–92
Smith, A. 259 Staats, A. W. 79
Smith, E. A. 336 Stanley, I. 55
Smith, J. M. 78 ‘state of nature’ 205
Smith, W. 363 Steadman, H. J. 211
smoke detector principle 40–1, 44 Stearns, S. C. 28, 435, 436–7
Sneddon, L. U. 146 Stein, D. J. 26
social anxiety (SA) 12 Steinfeld, H. 157
social bonds 280 Stengel, E. 64
crime 192, 194, 195 Stephenson, J. 435, 438, 450
organized crime 301, 302, 306 Stephenson, W. 410
social brain theory 35 Stevens, A. 6, 36
social choice 175, 178 Stever, G. 398–416
social cohesion 339 Stewart, C. A. 248
social competition 34 Stewart, P. 261
social constructivism 401 Still, M. C. 195
social constructs 222 Stohler, H. R. 42
social contagion 180 Stoker, D. 325
social contract 205–6, 223, 261, 264 Stoll, M. 251
Social Contract Theory (SCT) 205 Stone, J. 337
social Darwinism 43, 175 strategic communications 401
social emotions 265 Strategic Communications Cell (SCC) 510
social exchange theory 227–8 Strauss, M. 155
social information processing model 250 stress
social insects 62 biological sensitivity 281
necrophoric behavior 458–62, 463 death response 465–6
suicide 57–8 mindfulness 94
social interaction theory 378 response system 130
social learning theory 376, 377–8, 401 Stringer, C. 107
social media 336 Strong African American Families (SAAF) 286
Bird Box Challenge 380 Stroup, R. 262–3
cyberwarfare 499 Studlar, D. T. 180
Facebook 345, 403 Stylianou, S. 245
fake news 343–4, 345 substance dependence/abuse 13–14, 16, 37
gender differences 403–4 alcohol addiction 37–8
Twitter 344, 426, 500 see also drugs
social mimicry 193 Sugiyama, Y. 462, 463, 465
social pain 52, 64, 69, 71, 72, 76 suicide 9–10, 376
social presence 406–7, 413 antisuicide defenses 65–79
social rank, crime 194 celebrities 379–80
Social Representation Theory (SRT) 334 ‘gene’ 60
social risk theory 34 suicidology 51–93
social snacking 405–6 Werther effect 379
social-cognitive theory of power 251 Sullivan, C. J. 285
socioeconomic status Sullivan, R. J. 37
crime 196 Sun, J. C. 469
obesity 126, 135 Sun, Q. 459, 460, 462
policing 206 Surrey, J. 15
reproduction 131–2 surveillance 278, 403
Socrates 221, 222 survival handicap 418
Solomon, A. 6 Suwa, G. 266
Soper, C. A. 51–93 Swistak, P. 270
Soudriette, R. 179 symbolic interactionism 401
INDEX 533

Syme, K. L. 60–1 Townsend, J. B. 107, 108–9, 114, 117


Symons, D. 59, 479 tragedy of the commons 118, 207
transference 5
Taagepera, R. 181 transparency
tabloid media 405 artificial intelligence 336–7, 342–3
taboos, food 156 trust 341
tabula rasa 6, 15 Treating Depression Downhill (TDD) 7–8
tactical disengagement 213 Treiman, R. 153
Takeshita, R. S. C. 464, 465–6 Triad brotherhood 305
Talwar, P. 378 Triadic Reciprocal Causation model 382–3, 391
Tanaka, M. 58 trial and error learning 115
Tang, Y.-Y. 99 tribalism 338
Tasca, L. 374, 388 Trivers, R. 173, 191, 226, 228, 229, 304, 323, 478, 490
task analysis 66, 76 Troisi, A. 4, 7, 9, 25, 34, 44
Teasdale, J. D. 102 trolley problem 368
technology Trower, P. 12
cyberwarfare 499–514 Trull, T. J. 28
psycho-social factors 360 Trust Game (TG) 340–1
reproduction 477–98 trustworthiness, artificial intelligence 340–3, 344
robotics 352–73 Truszkowski, W. 504
Teich, A. H. 360, 361 Tsuang, M. 30
Teilhard de Chardin, P. 189 Tukachinsky, R. 405, 411
temporal discounting 131 Turchin, P. 179, 309, 310
Terkel, S. 339 Turing test 364, 365
Terrill, W. 206, 211 Turner, W. G. 153
terrorism Tversky, A. 232
cyberwarfare 499 Twitter 344, 426, 500
memes 504, 505, 509 Tybur, J. M. 156, 302
Terry, D. J. 336 Tyler, T. R. 215, 216
Teste, F. P. 334
thanatology 457–76 Ule, A. 270
Theodorou, A. 342, 343 Ulrich, R. S. 114
therapeutic cheerleading 9 United Nations 296, 338
Therapeutic Lifestyle Change for Depression urbanisation 125
(TLC-D) 8 Urquiza-Haas, E. G. 147
Thomas, R. J. 483 Usall, J. 57
Thompson, W. R. 325 utilitarianism 145, 223, 225, 230, 267
Thomson, J. A. 34
Thrasher, F. M. 211 vaccinations 135–8
Thrasymachus 221, 222 Valdes, L. 462
threat perception failure (TPF) 209–10 Valkenburg, P. M. 487, 488
Tifferet, S. 403 value-homophily 344
Tilly, C. 308–9, 325 Van Camp, T. 273
Tinbergen, N. 26–7, 44, 458, 459 Van der Beken, T. 296, 297
tit-for-tat 174, 264, 270, 272–3 van der Hout, E. 178
Todak, N. 210, 211, 216 van Dijk, M. 302, 305, 310
Todd, P. M. 115, 483, 485, 486 van IJzendoorn, M. H. 277, 281, 285,
Tomasik, B. 145, 146, 157, 158 286–7, 290
Tomasula, D. 157 Van Maanen, J. 212, 216
Tomlin, D. 96 Van Orden, K. A. 62, 78
Tooby, J. 56, 59, 66, 70, 71, 78, 110, 114, 116, 244, van San, M. 302–3
245, 259, 272, 303, 317 van Vugt, M. 180, 300, 302
Topolski, R. 146, 155 Vandenplas, Y. 424
Torgler, B. 477–98 vantage sensitivity 30
Toronto Transit Commission (TTC) 376 variability, suicide 52
totemism 149 Varki, A. 79n4
Toth, N. 399, 401 Värnik, P. 57
534 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY

Veale, D. 13 Watson, J. 6
vegetarians 151, 155, 157, 158–61 Watson, L. 385
Verheggen, F. 468 Wattenberg, M. P. 179
Verweij, M. 266 Waynforth, D. 481
victim–offender characteristics 196, 198–201 Waytz, A. 342
video dating 484 Weatherford, J. 284
video games 386, 387 Weber, M. 309
Video-Feedback Intervention to Promote Positive Weedon, S. L. 58
Parenting and Sensitive Discipline 286 Weismann, A. 477
Vienna Risk-Taking Test 386 Weiss, B. 377
‘vigilance in grief’ 469 welfare tradeoff ratio (WTR) 244–5
vigilance-based cycle 97 Wells, A. 102
Vilnai-Yavetz, I. 403 Wells, J. 258–76
Vingilis, E. 375, 385, 391, 392 Wemmers, J. A 273
Vining, D. R. 40 Weng, H. Y. 272
violence Wertheimer, A. 52, 61
animal abuse link 149–50 Werther effect 379
in prisons 250 West, S. A. 253
towards females 283–4 West-Eberhard, M. J. 400
victim empathy 408 Westen, D. 16
virtual reality (VR), trust 342 Westermarck effect 78
virtue signaling, animal welfare 152, 160–2 White, C. 469, 470
Visscher, P. K. 460, 461 White, M. D. 203
Vitaglione, G. D. 387–8, 389 Whyte, S. 477–98
Vitak, J. 481 Wiblin, R. 144, 156
Volk, A. A. 110 Widiger, T. A. 28
Von Gunten, A. 41 Wiebe, R. 264, 267, 273
von Lampe, K. 298–9, 302, 304–7 Wiesenthal, D. 374–97
von Rueden, C. 322–3 Wiley, J. 60
Vorderer, P. 408 Wilkinson, G. S. 258
Vory v zakone 305 Wilks, M. 160
Vosoughi, S. 344 Williams, G. 27, 54, 56, 58, 60, 61, 65, 75, 76, 77,
‘vote/buy gap’ 159 236, 480
Williams, J. M. G. 63
wages, artificial intelligence 340 Williams, P. 4
Wakefield, J. C. 25, 26, 32 Willner, P. 75
Waldfogel, J. 251 Wilson, D. 53, 60
Waldmann, M. R. 71 Wilson, E. O. 114, 115, 148, 460
Walker, I. D. 335 Wilson, H. 333–51
Walker, J. K. 155 Wilson, J. D. 57
Walker, P. 390 Wilson, J. Q. 260
Walker, R. S. 482 Wilson, M. 194, 195, 196–9, 227, 234, 235, 284,
Walker, S. 177, 205, 211 288–90, 323, 375, 376, 378–9
Wall, P. D. 63, 70 Wilson, R. K. 342
Wallace, J. 231 Winstead, B. A. 15
Wallach, W. 367 Wisman, A. 468–9
Waller, B. M. 153 Witwicki, K. 159
Walsh, A. 258–76 Woestenburg, N. O. 490, 491
Walters, J. 214 Woll, S. B. 484
Wampold, B. E. 16, 75 Wong, C. 238
War of Attrition model 207, 208 Woodruff, P. 266
Ward, T. 243, 246–8 Woollacott, M. 99
warfare 316–30 World Bank 338
cyber 499–514 World Health Organization (WHO) 26, 39, 51
Wasserman, D. 76 Wrangham, R. 156, 300, 317, 318, 319
Watkins, E. 98 Wright, R. 56, 59
Watson, C. F. I. 462, 465 Wright, S. 56, 420, 421
INDEX 535

Yabuta, S. 207 Zaki, J. 363


Yakuza 299, 300, 305, 306, 307 Zald, D. 403
Yamagishi, T. 341 Zalsman, G. 76
Yancey, P. 63, 65 Zanette, L. Y. 13
Yang, L. 111 Zaraska, M. 151, 156, 160
Yanomamo people 271 Zhang, W. 117
Yao, M. 462 Zhou, X. 462
Yorzinski, J. L. 116 Zillmann, D. 382
Young, H. P. 180 Zimmer, C. 478
young male syndrome 194, 195, 199, 374, 375, 383 Zinger, I. 279
Young, P. 484 zoonotic diseases 160
Young, S. G. 111 Zuboff, S. 336
YouTube 385, 392 Zuckerman, M. 381

Zahavi, A. 344, 418, 419, 420


Zajonc, R. B. 107

You might also like