You are on page 1of 13

Top Lang Disorders

Vol. 34, No. 3, pp. 197–209


Copyright c 2014 Wolters Kluwer Health | Lippincott Williams & Wilkins

A Comprehensive Definition
of Morphological Awareness
Implications for Assessment
Kenn Apel

Purpose: Although there is an increasing body of evidence for the important role morphological
awareness plays in written language development, there is little consensus on a fully specified
definition of this linguistic awareness ability. Without agreement on a definition, several problems
arise, at least one of which impacts researchers’ and practitioners’ ability to fully and consistently
assess students’ morphological awareness abilities. Method: In the first portion of this article, I
discuss insufficiencies with current definitions of morphological awareness and propose a more
comprehensive definition. Following this, I relate the implications of this new definition for
assessment: the inability of current norm-referenced tests and experimenter-designed tasks to meet
the definition and considerations from the research to keep in mind about stimuli on morphological
awareness tasks. Conclusion: In the final segment of the article, I provide implications and
suggestions for practitioners who wish to assess morphological awareness in a comprehensive
manner, using the new definition as a guide to measure students’ morphological awareness abilities.
Key words: assessment, morphological awareness, reading, spelling, written language

F OR decades, researchers and practi-


tioners have focused predominantly on
phonemic awareness when assessing the po-
able for use in research studies and in clinical
and school settings (e.g., Wagner & Torgesen,
1987; Wagner, Torgesen, & Rashotte, 1999;
tential impact of linguistic awareness on stu- Yopp, 1992).
dents’ reading and spelling abilities (e.g., Ehri More recently, morphological awareness
et al., 2001; Griffith & Olson, 1992). A uni- has begun to receive attention as another
form and mutually agreed upon definition of linguistic awareness skill that impacts
phonemic awareness is that it is a conscious written language abilities. Multiple inves-
awareness of the sounds of language and the tigations have demonstrated the power-
ability to talk about and manipulate those ful influence it has on word-level read-
sounds. This definition is accepted by re- ing, reading comprehension, and spelling
searchers and practitioners alike, and several development (e.g., Apel, Wilson-Fowler,
norm-referenced phonemic awareness mea- Brimo, & Perrin, 2012; McCutchen, Green,
sures that align with the definition are avail- & Abbott, 2008; Nagy, Berninger, Abbott,
Vaughan, & Vermeulen, 2003; Roman, Kirby,
Parrila, Wade-Woolley, & Deacon, 2009;
Walker & Hauerwas, 2006; Wolter, Wood, &
Author Affiliation: Department of Communication D’zatko, 2009). Furthermore, recent reviews
Sciences and Disorders, University of South
Carolina, Columbia. of morphological awareness interventions
have demonstrated that instruction in mor-
The author has indicated that he has no financial and
no nonfinancial relationships to disclose. phological awareness can lead to improve-
ment in students’ written language abilities
Corresponding Author: Kenn Apel, Department of
Communication Sciences and Disorders, University of (Bowers, Kirby, & Deacon, 2010; Goodwin
South Carolina, 124 Sumter St, Suite 300, Columbia, SC & Ahn, 2013). Collectively, these findings
29201 (kennapel@sc.edu). suggest that morphological awareness is an
DOI: 10.1097/TLD.0000000000000019 important linguistic awareness ability that

197

Copyright © 2014 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
198 TOPICS IN LANGUAGE DISORDERS/JULY–SEPTEMBER 2014

deserves as much attention for the role it plays level of detail (see Table 1 for examples).
in reading and spelling development as does Although many definitions involve some varia-
phonemic awareness (e.g., Berninger, Abbott, tion of the notion of the conscious awareness
Nagy, & Carlisle, 2010). of, or an ability to manipulate, morphemes,
Given the importance of morphological they differ in whether they specify modality
awareness for written language development, (i.e., some specify spoken only), vary in
it would seem that a consensus definition emphasis on individuals’ ability to reflect on
of morphological awareness must exist; how- morphemes, and, as a whole, do not address
ever, this is not the case. Without agreement all aspects of morphological awareness.
on a definition, several problems arise, some What is missing from all current definitions
of which impact the assessment of morpho- is the full level of specificity that might guide
logical awareness, as well as the scientific re- researchers and practitioners in evaluating
porting of researchers’ findings of this skill. In students’ morphological awareness abilities
the first portion of this article, I discuss insuf- comprehensively. Indeed, without an inclu-
ficiencies with current definitions of morpho- sive definition of a skill such as morphological
logical awareness and propose a more com- awareness, one cannot comprehensively as-
prehensive definition. Then, in the second sess that skill. Furthermore, because of
portion of this article, I relate the implications the lack of specificity in these definitions,
of this new definition for assessment. Specif- incomplete and, in some cases, incorrect data
ically, in the second section, I discuss the in- have been used to draw conclusions about
ability of current norm-referenced tests and students’ morphological awareness abilities.
experimenter-developed tasks to assess the For example, most researchers who have
construct represented by the proposed defi- assessed students’ morphological awareness
nition. I also offer considerations on the basis abilities have administered tasks in the spoken
of the research to keep in mind about stim- mode. When researchers have used tasks that
uli on morphological awareness tasks. I con- involved written language, they frequently
clude the article with implications and sug- have read the items to the students and asked
gestions for practitioners who wish to assess the students to respond orally. This is appar-
morphological awareness in a comprehensive ently based on the rationale that any written
manner. responses would be affected by the students’
(potential lack of) orthographic knowledge,
DEFINING MORPHOLOGICAL confounding the assessment of students’ mor-
AWARENESS phological awareness. This is an interesting
perspective, given that, as mentioned previ-
Morphemes are the smallest units of ously, part of morphological awareness nec-
meaning in our language. Thus, the term essarily involves an understanding of written
“morphological awareness” implies an morphemes, what they look like orthograph-
awareness of these small units of meaning. ically, how they are spelled, and how they
Given that morphemes occur in spoken and attach to base words and roots. When this
written language, morphological awareness aspect of morphological awareness is not as-
necessarily involves an awareness of both sessed, then a complete picture of students’
spoken and written morphemes, including morphological awareness abilities is lacking.
an understanding of what written affixes There also has been some confusion
(i.e., prefixes and suffixes) look like or- between the concepts of morphological
thographically and the rules that govern awareness and morphological production,
how affixes attach to base words or roots. sometimes referred to as morphological
Unfortunately, current definitions used by knowledge. Morphological awareness is a
researchers and authors come up short in conscious reflection on morphemes. Morpho-
defining morphological awareness at this logical production involves the unconscious

Copyright © 2014 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
A Comprehensive Definition of Morphological Awareness 199

Table 1. Sample definitions of morphological awareness

Definition of
Researchers Morphological Awareness

Berninger et al. (2010) “ . . . judgments about semantic or semantic-syntactic


relationships that depend upon the form of the word
or its parts” (p. 142).
Carlisle (2000) “ . . . the ability to parse words and analyze constituent
morphemes for the purpose of constructing meaning”
(p. 170).
Deacon, Kirby, and Casselman-Bell “ . . . the awareness of and ability to manipulate the
(2009) minimal units of meaning . . . ” (p. 301).
Deacon, Benere, and Pasquarella “ . . . the awareness of and the ability to manipulate
(2013) morphemes in the oral language . . . ” (p. 1113).
Guo, Roehrig, and Williams (2011) “ . . . explicit knowledge of the way in which words are
built up by combining smaller meaningful units, such
as prefixes, roots, and suffixes . . . ” (p. 160).
Kirby et al. (2012) “ . . . conscious awareness of the morphemic structure of
words and (their) ability to reflect on and manipulate
that structure . . . ” (p. 389).
Kuo and Anderson (2006) “The ability to reflect upon and manipulate morphemes
and employ word formation rules in one’s
language . . . ” (p. 161).
Tong, Deacon, Kirby, Cain, and “ . . . conscious awareness of the morphemic structure of
Parrila (2011) words and (individuals’) ability to reflect on and
manipulate that structure . . . ” (p. 523).

use of morphemes, typically during spon- language impairments and not on morpholog-
taneous spoken language. When individuals ical awareness (i.e., with the goal to increase
converse, they produce morphemes but typ- the children’s ability to consciously consider
ically do not think actively about the mor- or think about morphemes). An inconsistent
phemes they are producing. Mature writers definition, then, led to introduction of data
also may write without thinking actively about into both systematic syntheses that was in-
the specific morphemes they are writing, par- consistent with the purposes of the reviews.
ticularly when engaged in less formal writing Including them thus had the potential to al-
situations, such as notes, e-mails, and the like. ter the outcomes of the reviews and related
In the past, however, researchers have not conclusions. An agreed upon comprehensive
always kept these two notions distinct. For definition of morphological awareness may re-
example, two investigations by Tyler, Lewis, duce such problems in the future.
Haskill, and Tolbert (2002, 2003) were incor- Assessment of morphological awareness
porated into separate recent syntheses of mor- also hinges on a complete and unified defi-
phological awareness interventions—a sys- nition. The definitions used by others have
tematic review (Bowers et al., 2010) and a been incomplete. I recommend that a more
meta-analysis (Goodwin & Ahn, 2013); how- comprehensive definition of morphological
ever, the two studies conducted by Tyler awareness is needed that acknowledges mor-
et al. (2002, 2003) were focused on interven- phology in both spoken and written modal-
tions to increase the production of spoken ities. Such an all-encompassing definition
morphemes in preschoolers with speech and would allow researchers and practitioners the

Copyright © 2014 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
200 TOPICS IN LANGUAGE DISORDERS/JULY–SEPTEMBER 2014

specificity to address all aspects of morpho- Norm-referenced morphological


logical awareness when they assess students’ awareness measures
morphological awareness. I propose that such
a definition should take the following form: No norm-referenced measures dedicated
solely to assessing morphological awareness
Morphological awareness includes: (a) awareness have been published. One norm-referenced
of spoken and written forms of morphemes; (b) test, the Process Assessment of the Learner—
the meaning of affixes and the alterations in mean-
Second Edition (PALS-2; Berninger, 2007),
ing and grammatical class they bring to base
contains a few subtests that are identified as
words/roots (e.g., -ed causes a verb to refer to the
past as in walked; -er can change a verb to a noun, morphological awareness tasks (e.g., students
as in teach to teacher); (c) the manner in which view printed words and identify those con-
written affixes connect to base words/roots, in- taining affixes, students determine whether
cluding changes to those base words/roots (e.g., two presented words share the same root).
some suffixes require a consonant to be doubled The PALS-2 thus far has not been used widely
or dropped when attached to a base word/root in the field of speech–language pathology and
in written form, such as in hop to hopping and may not be familiar to language specialists.
hope to hoped); and (d) the relation between base Additionally, there are a few norm-referenced
words/roots and their inflected or derived forms measures, or subtests of norm-referenced
(e.g., knowing that a variety of words are related
measures, that assess some aspect of mor-
because they share the same base word/root, such
phological awareness (see Table 2 for exam-
as act, action, react, and activity).
ples), at least indirectly. These tasks, however,
Applying this definition to current mea- are not identified as measures of morpholog-
sures can reveal whether they provide ade- ical awareness and never have been used in
quate coverage of the key components of this any research study as a measure of morpho-
definition, either solely or in combination, to logical awareness. Furthermore, they tend to
constitute a comprehensive assessment of stu- assess awareness of inflectional morphology
dents’ morphological awareness. In the fol- more than derivational morphology, which is
lowing section, I provide a review of several a limitation because inflectional morpholog-
current norm-referenced assessments that po- ical awareness is less predictive of reading
tentially assess morphological awareness, in in students beyond the early primary grades
which I examine their consistency with the (e.g., Kuo & Anderson, 2006). In addition, the
proposed definition. I follow this with a sim- range of affixes is highly constrained in num-
ilar review of experimenter-developed tasks ber and types.
that have been used in the research literature The norm-referenced measures reviewed in
to examine students’ morphological aware- Table 2, therefore, assess only a limited por-
ness skills. tion of the construct encompassed by the
proposed definition. Among other limitations,
they all are measures of students’ awareness
CURRENT TASKS USED TO ASSESS of spoken morphology only. They do not as-
MORPHOLOGICAL AWARENESS sess students’ awareness of written morphol-
ogy, including students’ awareness of what
Morphological awareness has received less written morphemes look like orthographi-
attention than phonemic awareness, both in cally, the manner in which written affixes
the research literature and in everyday as- connect to base words, and the changes that
sessment and intervention practice. Thus, it can occur to base words because of those
is not surprising that there are fewer exam- affix additions. In addition, they assess stu-
ples of norm-referenced measures that assess dents’ awareness of the relation between base
students’ morphological awareness than there words and their derived forms only minimally.
are experimenter-developed tasks. Thus, even in combination, they would fail to

Copyright © 2014 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
A Comprehensive Definition of Morphological Awareness 201

Table 2. Examples of norm-referenced measures that assess aspects of morphological


awareness

Aspect of Proposed
Morphological
Awareness
Measure Mode Description Definition Assessed

Test for Examining Spoken Assesses awareness of five Awareness of the


Expressive Morphology inflections (present relation of base words
(Shipley, Stone, & Sue, progressive—ing, plurals, to their inflected and
1983) possessives, third person derived forms
singular, past tense) and two
derivational forms (the
comparative —er and the
superlative—est).
Students complete a sentence
with an affixed word (e.g.,
here is one boat, here are
two _____ [boats]).
Test of Language Spoken Assesses awareness of 13 Awareness of the
Development—Primary: inflections (seven plurals, relation of base words
4 (Hammill & Newcomer, four possessives, and two to their inflected and
2008a) past tense) and five derived forms
Subtest: Morphological derivational forms (three
Completion agentive and two
comparative).
Students complete a
sentence with an
affixed word (e.g.,
“Carla has a dress,
Denise has a dress. They
have two ____[dresses]).
Test of Language Spoken Some of the target items tap Awareness of the
Development— into inflectional and meaning of affixes and
Intermediate: 4 (Hammill derivational knowledge, the alterations in
& Newcomer, 2008b) whereas others rely more on meaning they bring to
syntactic awareness. base words
Subtest: Morphological Students read a sentence and
Comprehension must judge whether the
sentence sounds
grammatically correct (e.g.,
“Those boys is happy”).
Illinois Test of Spoken Assesses awareness of 13 Awareness of the
Psycholinguistic Abilities: inflections (six plurals and relation of base words
3 (Hammill, Mather, & seven past tense) and 13 to their inflected and
Roberts, 2001) derivational forms (four derived forms
superlatives, three
comparative, and six others).
(Continues)

Copyright © 2014 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
202 TOPICS IN LANGUAGE DISORDERS/JULY–SEPTEMBER 2014

Table 2. Examples of norm-referenced measures that assess aspects of morphological


awareness (Continued)

Aspect of Proposed
Morphological
Awareness
Measure Mode Description Definition Assessed

Subtest: Morphological Students asked to fill in a final


Closure missing part (e.g., “big,
bigger, ____[biggest]).
Clinical Evaluation of Spoken Assesses awareness of 12 Awareness of the
Language Fundamentals: inflections (two plurals, two relation of base words
4 (Semel, Wiig, & Secord, third person singular, three to their inflected and
2006) possessives, four present derived forms
progressive—ing, and one
past tense) and four
derivational forms (one
superlative, two
comparative, and one other).
Subtest: Word Structure Students complete a sentence
with an affixed word (e.g.,
“This man sings. He is called
a _______[singer]).

cover all of the components of the proposed The tasks that researchers have adminis-
definition. Partially in response to these lim- tered have taken various forms, including
itations, researchers have constructed tasks production tasks, judgment tasks, blending/
that fill some of the gaps in assessing the key segmenting tasks, and analogy tasks, each
components of the proposed, comprehensive with several subvarieties. In addition, in one
definition. investigation, my colleagues and I included a
measure of children’s awareness of written af-
Experimenter-designed morphological fixes (Apel, Diehm, & Apel, 2013). I provide a
awareness tasks brief overview of each of these categories of
Different researchers have used different tasks below.
experimenter-designed morphological aware-
ness tasks, and often just one or two tasks, Productive morphological awareness
to assess students’ morphological awareness tasks
skills. Because the focus in the literature has Some researchers have used productive
been on the more general definition of mor- morphological awareness tasks to assess
phological awareness, it is not surprising that students’ morphological awareness skills;
experimenter-designed tasks have not cov- these tasks have varied in their requirements.
ered the range of abilities identified in the The cloze procedure has been a typical
proposed comprehensive definition. I would means for assessing students’ productive
argue, then, that researchers should discuss morphological awareness abilities (e.g., Apel
their findings in a manner that acknowl- et al., 2013; Apel & Lawrence, 2011; Casalis
edges that students’ morphological awareness & Cole, 2009; McCutchen et al., 2008; Wolter
skills have been measured in an incomplete et al., 2009). For example, a common task
manner. involves providing a word, such as a base

Copyright © 2014 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
A Comprehensive Definition of Morphological Awareness 203

word (e.g., help), and then requiring the about the semantic and/or syntactic accuracy
student to use an inflected or derived form of of a word, often within a multiple choice
the word to complete a sentence (e.g., The paradigm (e.g., “When Ali Baba’s wife saw
Girl Scout was a great _____[helper]). The the gold coins, she was _______ speechi-
target response has varied by researchers. fied, specialized, speechmaker, speechless”;
Sometimes, the student changes a multimor- Berninger et al., 2010; Nagy et al., 2003; Nagy
phemic word into a base word (farmer. Bill et al., 2006; Nippold & Sun, 2008). These
works on a ____[farm]). Other times, the stu- types of judgment tasks have varied addition-
dent changes a base word into an inflected or ally in their presentation mode (some were
derived word (as in the aforementioned exam- spoken-only, some were spoken but also pro-
ple with helper). When the response involves vided in print) as well as stimulus type (i.e.,
a change into a derived word, the response inflectional and/or derivational items). Deriva-
may be a transparent form (tall–taller) or one tional items varied in whether they were trans-
that involves either a phonological (magic– parent or involved a phonological and/or or-
magician) and/or an orthographic shift thographic shift. Finally, as can be seen in the
(silly–silliness). Other cloze tasks have been aforementioned example of speechified, on
similar to norm-referenced tasks discussed some tasks, pseudowords were used with real
earlier (e.g., Here is one dress; Here are two affixes.
____[dresses]; e.g., Berninger et al., 2010).
Fluency tasks offer a different approach. Morphological awareness tasks
These require students to name as many involving blending or segmenting
inflected or derived forms of a base word as A few researchers have used blending
possible (e.g., act could yield action, acting, or segmenting tasks to assess students’
react, etc.). Some researcher-developed pro- morphological awareness skills (e.g., Casalis
duction tasks have included definition tasks et al., 2004; Lyster, 2002). For example,
(e.g., Apel et al., 2013; Casalis, Cole, & Sopo, Casalis et al. required their 8- to 12-year-old
2004; Jeon, 2011; Tsesmeli & Seymour, 2006). students to blend and segment base words
To elicit productions of definitions, for exam- and their affixes to either create or decom-
ple, Casalis et al. required their 8- 12-year-old pose, respectively, a multimorphemic word.
students to finish sentences with a derived An example was blending the French base
form that completed a definition (e.g., “a word nettoie and the suffix age to produce
person who paints is a . . . ..painter”). Finally, nettoyage. In another segmenting task, they
some experimenter-designed production required their students to pronounce the base
tasks require students to read (e.g., Carlisle, portions of multimorphemic words produced
2000) or spell multimorphemic words (e.g., by the experimenters (e.g., producing the
Apel et al., 2012; Kirk & Gillon, 2009). base jour from the French word journeée).

Morphological awareness judgment Morphological awareness tasks based


tasks on word analogy
Some investigators have constructed judg- Word analogy tasks also have been used
ment tasks to measure morphological aware- to assess students’ morphological aware-
ness. Judgment tasks often require students ness skills (e.g., Bryant, Nunes, & Bindman,
to make decisions about the semantic re- 1997; Deacon & Kirby, 2004; Kirby et al.,
lationship between two words (e.g., “Does 2012; Roman, Kirby, Parrila, Wade-Woolley, &
ban come from banana?”; Berninger et al., Deacon, 2009; Tsesmeli & Seymour, 2006).
2010; Kuo & Anderson, 2006; Mahony, These tasks, which commonly are presented
Singson, & Mann, 2000; Nagy, Berninger, via spoken language, follow the typical for-
& Abbott, 2006). Students simply respond mat for an analogy: A is to B as C is to D.
yes or no. Others have involved a judgment For morphological awareness tasks, students

Copyright © 2014 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
204 TOPICS IN LANGUAGE DISORDERS/JULY–SEPTEMBER 2014

are asked to complete the fourth component they may be measuring different aspects of
of the analogy after hearing either three morphological awareness that constitute a
words (e.g., mess: messy, fun: _____) or three multifactorial construct. Some indirect evi-
sentences (e.g., Peter plays at school: Pe- dence suggests that the latter is true; that is,
ter played at school; Peter works at home: that the different tasks used actually may be
_____________). The studies using word measuring different aspects of morphological
analogies have varied in whether the response awareness.
required an inflected or derived word (e.g., For example, two research teams examined
Kirby et al., 2012; Roman et al., 2009); all whether the morphological awareness tasks
investigations using sentence analogies have they used in their study related to one an-
required only inflected words (e.g., Bryant other and found only moderate correlations
et al., 1997). (r values ranged from .46 to .58; Deacon
et al., 2013; Ramı́rez, Chen, & Pasquarella,
A task involving identification 2013). In addition, in the study by Apel
of written affixes et al., we found that certain morphological
Finally, my colleagues and I conducted an awareness tasks predicted early elementary
investigation of children’s identification of grade students’ reading abilities better than
written affixes (Apel et al., 2013). In our other morphological awareness tasks, and
study, we provided first- and second-grade that some tasks discriminated between grades
children from low SES homes, with a paper better than others. These findings suggest
containing a list of pseudowords with real af- that not all morphological awareness tasks
fixes (e.g., rinning). The children were given measure the same aspects of morpholog-
3 min to circle all affixes (i.e., “add-ons”) they ical awareness, and that some tasks may
saw. The task allowed us to examine the stu- relate to students’ written language skills
dents’ recognition of written prefixes and suf- differently.
fixes. At the very least, no one type of task
(i.e., judgment task, production task, blend-
Morphological awareness assessment ing/segmentation task, analogy task, affix
challenges identification task) adequately assesses all of
Overall, quite a range of tasks have been the components of the proposed morphologi-
used by researchers to assess students’ mor- cal awareness definition. Before discussing im-
phological awareness abilities. As mentioned plications for researchers and practitioners, I
previously, with few exceptions (e.g., Apel first discuss other factors that impact students’
et al., 2013), most researchers have used performances on these types of measures.
only one, or perhaps two, of these tasks to When researchers have examined students’
assess students’ morphological awareness morphological awareness abilities, they have
skills. Even when some researchers have used found that several specific item features af-
multiple measures, albeit an incomplete set of fect task performance, including morpheme
tasks, to represent morphological awareness, type and transparency between base words
they then combined responses from those and their inflected or derived forms. For ex-
tasks into one composite measure (e.g., Clin, ample, in general, children in the primary el-
Wade-Woolley, & Heggie, 2009; Tong et al., ementary grades demonstrate greater aware-
2011). This can make it difficult to understand ness of inflectional forms than derivational
patterns of strengths and weaknesses. Thus, forms; it is around third grade that children
for both researchers and practitioners, it is typically demonstrate greater awareness of
unknown whether the variety of morphologi- derivational morphology (e.g., Kuo & Ander-
cal awareness tasks used across investigations son, 2006). Not surprisingly, then, inflectional
measure the same underlying construct and morphological awareness is mostly associated
can be used interchangeably or whether with literacy abilities in younger elementary

Copyright © 2014 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
A Comprehensive Definition of Morphological Awareness 205

school students rather than upper-grade el- CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS AND


ementary school students (e.g., Carlisle & SUGGESTIONS
Nomanbhoy, 1993). Task items that represent
transparent relations between base words and Additional research is required to inform
their derived forms are typically easier to com- assessment practices. Investigators will need
plete than items that represent a shift phono- to determine whether different morphologi-
logically and/or orthographically (e.g., Apel cal awareness tasks measure the same or dif-
& Thomas-Tate, 2009; Carlisle, 2000). How- ferent aspects of morphological awareness.
ever, both of these item feature issues, mor- By determining whether multiple measures
pheme type and transparency, may be con- are needed to capture the full range of com-
founded by morpheme and word frequency ponents contained in the recommended def-
issues. inition, researchers and practitioners will be
Word frequency issues can function on mul- better prepared to assess students’ morpho-
tiple levels. There are far fewer inflectional logical awareness abilities. In the meantime,
affixes than derivational affixes, and those the important role of morphological aware-
inflectional affixes are more frequent in occur- ness in the development of students’ written
rence and shorter in letter length, which may language abilities necessitates that practition-
lead to ease in performance on tasks. Non- ers consider how they can best assess their
transparent forms also may be confounded students’ morphological awareness abilities
by word frequency; many multimorphemic now.
words that involve shifts are lower in word Although practitioners await the needed re-
frequency counts than transparent forms. search, they have several options available to
Across previous investigations, researchers them. My first suggestion is to use the pro-
have not always accounted for these item- posed definition as a guide for assessment.
specific features on a complete, consistent Using this definition will guide practitioners
basis. in their use of different tasks that tap into
Another factor to consider is students’ the different components of the definition
prior vocabulary knowledge. Some re- (see Table 3 for suggestions). For example,
searchers have discussed whether morpho- to assess students’ awareness of spoken mor-
logical awareness is actually another mea- phemes, a segmenting or blending task can
sure of vocabulary, supported by findings be used on which students are asked to either
on the strong relation between vocabulary break up multimorphemic words into their
and morphological awareness (e.g., Kuo & component morphemes or, conversely, blend
Anderson, 2006). However, in studies that morphemes into multimorphemic words
have examined the associations of morpho- (e.g., see Casalis et al., 2004). An affix iden-
logical awareness and vocabulary simultane- tification task, such as the timed task that re-
ously, with reading as an outcome (e.g., Apel quired students to circle affixes within writ-
et al., 2012; Nagy et al., 2003), morphologi- ten pseudowords (i.e., Apel et al., 2013),
cal awareness has uniquely predicted reading can be used to assess awareness of written
beyond vocabulary knowledge. Thus, there morphemes.
appear to be distinct aspects of morpholog- Awareness of the meaning of affixes and
ical awareness that separate it from general the alterations in meaning they bring to base
vocabulary abilities. Finally, with additional words can be assessed by using spoken and
studies, such as the investigation conducted written production and judgment tasks (e.g.,
by Mitchell and Brady (2014), more informa- see Apel et al., 2013; Berninger et al., 2010).
tion about specific affixes and when they are On these tasks, students may be asked to com-
acquired will provide needed developmental plete cloze tasks with appropriate morpho-
information for assessment and hierarchy of logically related words or to make decisions
stimuli. about the semantic relation between two

Copyright © 2014 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
206 TOPICS IN LANGUAGE DISORDERS/JULY–SEPTEMBER 2014

Table 3. Examples of morphological awareness tasks aligned with proposed definition

Aspect of Proposed
Morphological Awareness Potential
Definition Assessed Mode Assessment Tasks Sample Item

Awareness of the morphemes Spoken Segmenting task “Tap out how many ‘parts’
in words and what they you hear in recyclable”
“sound like” (see Casalis et al., 2004).
Awareness of the morphemes Written Affix identification “Circle all the ‘add-ons” you
in words and what they task see in this list of words”
“look like” (e.g., rinning; see Apel
et al., 2013).
Awareness of the meaning of Spoken Definition task “A person who makes art is
affixes and the alterations in an . . . artist”; see Casalis
meaning they bring to base et al., 2004).
words
Awareness of the meaning of Written Suffix choice task “Matthew was not known
affixes and the alterations in for being overly [friendly,
grammatical class they bring friendship, friendliness,
to base words friends] to others” (see
Nagy et al., 2003).
Awareness of spelling of Written Spelling “Spell dresses. Spell
written affixes multimorphemic plainest . . . ..” (see Apel
words et al., 2013).
Awareness of how the addition Written Derivational spelling “Which ending goes with
of morphemes can affect task luck to make lucky: —y,
base word spellings ie, —ey” (see Sangster
and Deacon, 2011).
Awareness of the relation of Spoken Spoken relatives task “Farm. My uncle is a ____
base words to their inflected farmer. (See Apel et al.,
and derived forms 2013).
Awareness of the relation of Written Written relatives task “Shine. The boy’s bike was
base words to their inflected very ____shiny. (See Apel
and derived forms et al., 2013).

words. Students’ awareness of the spelling of logical awareness, which then will lead to spe-
written affixes and the consequences (rule cific targeted intervention goals.
affects base word changes) when attached As practitioners are using researcher-
to base words or roots can be assessed us- designed tasks, they should keep in mind the
ing multimorphemic spelling tasks (e.g., see potential factors that may influence students’
Apel et al., 2013). Finally, awareness of the performance on those tasks. As mentioned,
relationship of base words to their inflected derivational items likely will be more chal-
and derived forms also may be assessed using lenging than inflectional items. Furthermore,
spoken and written cloze tasks (e.g., teach. derivational items that involve a shift away
Mrs. Smith is my favorite ______[teacher]). from the base word, either a phonological
By using the whole range of tasks representing shift or an orthographic shift, may be more
the comprehensive definition proposed, prac- challenging than one in which the base word
titioners will have a clearer idea of students’ is transparent within the derived word. Prac-
possible strengths and weaknesses in morpho- titioners can be aware of these effects, noting

Copyright © 2014 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
A Comprehensive Definition of Morphological Awareness 207

their impact on students’ performance and us- something different. As long as the practi-
ing that information to inform intervention tioner follows the procedures outlined by the
(i.e., if a phonological or orthographic shift publisher, the scores obtained still may be
response was more challenging for a student, compared with the normative database and
then intervention would start with stimuli standard scores obtained. The results then
that involved only transparent forms). Fur- may be interpreted qualitatively with respect
thermore, as Mitchell and Brady (2014) point to what they indicate about a student’s mor-
out, students appear to be aware of prefixes phological awareness. As with the suggestions
earlier than suffixes. Should this developmen- for the experimenter-designed tasks, practi-
tal sequence be found in assessment, practi- tioners will need to determine whether the
tioners would use this information to guide items on the norm-referenced task are im-
intervention practices as well. pacted by morpheme type (i.e., inflected or
Finally, practitioners should be aware of derived forms) and transparency of the base
norm-referenced measures they are using that word or root (i.e., transparent or shift form).
may be assessing aspects of their students’ Morphological awareness is an important
morphological awareness abilities although linguistic awareness skill that impacts the de-
the tasks are not labeled as such. Although velopment of written language: reading and
an assessment test may state that it measures spelling. With a comprehensive definition, re-
one skill, practitioners should be conscious of searchers and practitioners will be prepared
what the task truly measures. Furthermore, if to assess students’ morphological awareness
the practitioner’s work setting requires stan- skills more completely. This may enable them
dard scores for students to receive direct ser- to form a more complete and inclusive picture
vices, these assessment tasks can be used of students’ morphological awareness abilities
to assess morphological awareness even if and, if needed, develop a prescriptive, optimal
the name assigned to the measure suggests plan for intervention.

REFERENCES

Apel, K., Diehm, E., & Apel, L. (2013). Using multiple Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 39, 141–163.
measures of morphological awareness to assess its re- doi:1007/s10936-009-9130-6
lation to reading. Topics in Language Disorders, 33, Bowers, P. N., Kirby, J. R., & Deacon, S. H. (2010).
42–56. doi:10.1097/TLD.Ob013e318280f57b The effects of morphological instruction on liter-
Apel, K., & Lawrence, J. (2011). Contributions of acy skills: A systematic review of the literature.
morphological awareness skills to word-level read- Review of Educational Research, 80, 144–179.
ing and spelling in first-grade children with and doi:10.3102/0034654309359353
without speech sound disorder. Journal of Speech, Bryant, P., Nunes, T., & Bindman, M. (1997). Backward
Language, and Hearing Research, 54, 1312– readers’ awareness of language: strengths and weak-
1327. nesses. European Journal of Psychology of Educa-
Apel, K., & Thomas-Tate, S. (2009). Morphological aware- tion, 12, 357–372. doi:10.1007/BF03172798
ness skills of fourth grade African American students. Carlisle, J. F. (2000). Awareness of the struc-
Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, ture and meaning of morphologically complex
40, 312–324. words: Impact on reading. Reading and Writ-
Apel, K., Wilson-Fowler, E. B., Brimo, D., & Perrin, N. A. ing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 12, 169–190.
(2012). Metalinguistic contributions to reading and doi:10.1023/A:1008131926604
spelling in second and third grade students. Reading Carlisle, J .F., & Nomanbhoy, D. M. (1993). Phonological
and Writing, 25, 1283–1305. and morphological awareness in first graders. Applied
Berninger, V. W. (2007). The process assessment of the Psycholinguistics, 14, 177–195.
learner—second edition. San Antonio, TX: Psycho- Casalis, S., & Cole, P. (2009). On the relationship
logical Corporation. between morphological and phonological aware-
Berninger, V. W., Abbott, R. D., Nagy, W., & Carlisle, ness: Effects of training in kindergarten and in
J. (2010). Growth in phonological orthographic, first-grade reading. First Language, 29, 113–142.
and morphological awareness in grades 1 to 6. doi:10.1177/0142723708097484

Copyright © 2014 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
208 TOPICS IN LANGUAGE DISORDERS/JULY–SEPTEMBER 2014

Casalis, S., Cole, P., & Sopo, D. (2004). Morphologi- vices in Schools, 40, 341–351. doi:10.1044/0161-
cal awareness in developmental dyslexia. Annals of 1461(2008/08-0009)
Dyslexia, 54, 114–138. Kuo, L. J., & Anderson, R. C. (2006). Morphologi-
Clin, E., Wade-Woolley, L., & Heggie, L. (2009). Prosodic cal awareness and learning to read: A cross lan-
sensitivity and morphological awareness in children’s guage perspective. Educational Psychologist, 41(3),
reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 161–180.
104(2), 197–213. Lyster, S. H. (2002). The effects of morphological
Deacon, S. H., Benere, J., & Pasquarella, A. (2013). Recip- versus phonological awareness training in kinder-
rocal relationship: Children’s morphological aware- garten on reading development. Reading and Writ-
ness and their reading accuracy across grades 2 to 3. ing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 15, 261–294.
Developmental Psychology, 49, 1113–1126. doi:10.1023/A:1015272516220
Deacon, S. H., & Kirby, J. R. (2004). Morphological aware- Mahony, D., Singson, M., & Mann, V. (2000). Reading abil-
ness: Just “more phonological”? The roles of mor- ity and sensitivity to morphological relations. Reading
phological and phonological awareness in reading de- and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 12, 191–
velopment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 223–238. 218. doi:10.1023/A:1008136012492
doi:10.1017.S0124716404001117 McCutchen, D., Green, L., & Abbott, R. D. (2008). Chil-
Deacon, S. H., Kirby, J. R., & Casselman-Bell, dren’s morphological knowledge: Links to literacy.
M. (2009). How robust is the contribution Reading Psychology, 29(4), 289–314.
of morphological awareness to general spelling Mitchell, A. M., & Brady, S. A. (2014). Assessing affix
outcomes? Reading Psychology, 30, 301–318. knowledge using both pseudoword and real-word
doi:10.1080/02702710802412057 measures. Topics in Language Disorders, 34(3),
Ehri, L. C., Nunes, S. R., Willows, D. M., Schuster, B. V., 210–227.
Yaghoub-Zadeh, Z., & Shanahan, T. (2001). Phonemic Nagy, W., Berninger, V., & Abbott, R. D. (2006). Contri-
awareness instruction helps children learn to read: butions of morphology beyond phonology to literacy
Evidence from the National Reading Panel’s meta- outcomes of upper elementary and middle-school stu-
analysis. Reading Research Quarterly, 36, 250–287. dents. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 134–
Goodwin, A. P., & Ahn, S. (2013). A meta-analysis of mor- 147. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.98.1.134
phological interventions in English: Effects on literacy Nagy, W., Berninger, V., Abbott, R., Vaughan, K., &
outcomes for school-age children. Scientific Studies Vermeulen, K. (2003). Relationship of morphology
of Reading, 17(4), 257–285. and other language skills to literacy skills in at-risk
Griffith, P. L., & Olson, M. W. (1992). Phonemic aware- second-grade readers and at-risk fourth grade writ-
ness helps beginning readers break the code. Reading ers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 730–742.
Teacher, 45, 516–523. doi:10.1037/0022-06663.95.4.730
Guo, Y., Roehrig, A. D., & Williams, R. S. (2011). The rela- Nippold, M. A., & Sun, L. (2008). Knowledge of mor-
tion of morphological awareness and syntactic aware- phologically complex words: A developmental study
ness to adults’ reading comprehension: Is vocabulary of older children and young adolescents. Language,
knowledge a mediating variable? Journal of Literacy Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 39, 365–
Research, 43(2), 159–183. 373. doi:10.1044/0161-1461(2008/034)
Hammill, D. D., Mather, N., & Roberts, R. (2001). Illi- Ramı́rez, G., Chen, X., & Pasquarella, A. (2013). Cross-
nois test of psycholinguistic abilities—Third Edition linguistic transfer of morphological awareness in
(ITPA-3). Austin, TX: Pro-ed. Spanish-speaking English language learners: The fa-
Hammill, D. D., & Newcomer, P. L. (2008a). Test of cilitating effect of cognate knowledge. Topics in Lan-
language development—primary, Fourth Edition. guage Disorders, 33(1), 73–92.
Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. Roman, A. A., Kirby, J. R., Parrila, R., Wade-Woolley, L., &
Hammill, D. D., & Newcomer, P. L. (2008b). Test of lan- Deacon, S. H. (2009). Towards a comprehensive view
guage development—intermediate, Fourth Edition. of the skills involved in word reading in Grades 4,
Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. 6, and 8. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
Jeon, E. H. (2011). Contribution of morphological 102, 96–113. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2008.01.004
awareness to second-language reading comprehen- Sangster, L., & Deacon, S. H. (2011). Development
sion. The Modern Language Journal, 95, 217–235. in children’s sensitivity to the role of deriva-
doi:10.1111/j.1540 4781.2011.01179x tions in spelling. Canadian Journal of Experimen-
Kirby, J. R., Deacon, S. H., Bowers, P. N., Izenberg, L., tal Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie
Wade-Woolley, L., & Parrila, R. (2012). Children’s expérimentale, 65(2), 133.
morphological awareness and reading ability. Read- Semel, E., Wiig, E. H., & Secord, W. A. (2006). Clini-
ing and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 25, cal evaluation of language fundamentals (4th ed).
389–410. doi:10.1007/s11145-010-9276-5 London: Harcourt.
Kirk, C., & Gillon, G. T. (2009). Integrated morpho- Shipley, K. G., Stone, T. A., & Sue, M. B. (1983). Test for
logical awareness intervention as a tool for improv- Examining Expressive Morphology (TEEM). Tucson,
ing literacy. Language, Speech, and Hearing Ser- AZ: Communication Skill Builders.

Copyright © 2014 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
A Comprehensive Definition of Morphological Awareness 209

Tong, X., Deacon, S. H., Kirby, J. R., Cain, K., & Parrila, Wagner, R. K., & Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature of
R. (2011). Morphological awareness: A key to un- phonological processing and its causal role in the
derstanding poor reading comprehension in English. acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin,
Journal of Educational Psychology, 103, 523–534. 101, 192–212.
doi:10.1037/a0023495 Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A.
Tsesmeli, S. N., & Seymour, P. H. K. (2006). Derivational (1999). Comprehensive Test of Phonological Process-
morphology and spelling in dyslexia. Reading and ing. Austin, TX: PRO-ED.
Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 19, 587–625. Walker, J., & Hauerwas, L. B. (2006). Development
doi:10.1007/s11145-006-9011-4 of phonological, morphological, and orthographic
Tyler, A. A., Lewis, K. E., Haskill, A., & Tolbert, L. C. knowledge in young spellers: The case of inflected
(2002). Efficacy and cross-domain effects of a mor- verbs. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary
phosyntax and a phonology intervention. Language, Journal, 19, 819–843.
Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 33, Wolter, J. A., Wood, A., & D’zatko, K. W. (2009). The
52–66. influence of morphological awareness on the liter-
Tyler, A. A., Lewis, K. E., Haskill, A., & Tolbert, L. acy development of first-grade children. Language,
C. (2003). Outcomes of different speech and lan- Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 40, 286–
guage goal attack strategies. Journal of Speech, 298. doi:10.1044/0161-1461(2009/08-0001)
Language, and Hearing Research, 46, 1077– Yopp, H. K. (1992). Developing phonemic awareness in
1094. young children. The Reading Teacher, 45, 696–703.

Copyright © 2014 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

You might also like