You are on page 1of 12

An Integrative Model of Organizational Trust: Past, Present, and Future

Author(s): F. David Schoorman, Roger C. Mayer and James H. Davis


Source: The Academy of Management Review , Apr., 2007, Vol. 32, No. 2 (Apr., 2007),
pp. 344-354
Published by: Academy of Management

Stable URL: https://www.jstor.org/stable/20159304

REFERENCES
Linked references are available on JSTOR for this article:
https://www.jstor.org/stable/20159304?seq=1&cid=pdf-
reference#references_tab_contents
You may need to log in to JSTOR to access the linked references.

JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide
range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and
facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at
https://about.jstor.org/terms

Academy of Management is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to
The Academy of Management Review

This content downloaded from


128.239.99.140 on Sat, 03 Dec 2022 18:09:08 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
Copyright of the Academy of Management, all rights reserved. Contents may not
? Academy o? Management Review be copied, emailed, posted to a listserv, or otherwise transmitted without the
2007, Vol. 32, No. 2, 344-354. copyright holder's express written permission. Users may print, download, or email
articles for individual use only.

EDITOR'S FORUM

AN INTEGRATIVE MODEL OF
ORGANIZATIONAL TRUST: PAST, PRESENT,
AND FUTURE
F. DAVID SCHOORMAN
Purdue University

ROGER C. MAYER
The University of Akron

JAMES H. DAVIS
University of Notre Dame

A considerable amount of research has examined trust since our 1995 publication. We
revisit some of the critical issues that we addressed and provide clarifications and
extensions of the topics of levels of analysis, time, control systems, reciprocity, and
measurement. We also recognize recent research in new areas of trust, such as affect,
emotion, violation and repair, distrust, international and cross-cultural issues, and
context-specific models, and we identify promising avenues for future research.

As we wrote our 1995 paper on trust (Mayer, search that our paper has been cited over 1,100
Davis, & Schoorman, 1995), we were struck by times (according to Google Scholar). In addition
the relative scarcity of research in the main to management and general business, it has
stream management literature focusing directly been cited in such diverse areas as marketing,
on trust. This led us to several bodies of litera accounting, finance, economics, information
ture, including management, psychology, phi systems, industrial engineering, political sci
losophy, and economics. We found that scholars ence, communication, ethics, law, psychology,
from diverse disciplines were presenting many sociology, health care, and agribusiness. We
insightful views and perspectives on trust but would like to use this opportunity to revisit some
that many of them seemed to talk past one an of the issues raised by our 1995 paper and re
other. Our goal was to integrate these perspec view how the field has dealt with them. We will
tives into a single model. also discuss the new concerns and opportunities
This work came to fruition at about the same for future research on trust.
time as several other works on trust. Papers on
trust by Hosmer (1995) and McAllister (1995) were
also published in Academy of Management CLARIFICATIONS AND EXTENSIONS OF THE
journals that year, followed the next year by a MODEL OF TRUST
book edited by Kramer and Tyler (1996). The con
fluence of these works, fueled by practical con
Trust As an Aspect of a Relationship
cerns raised by now infamous government and One of the difficult conceptual decisions that
corporate scandals over the next decade, pro we faced as we developed our definition of trust
duced a groundswell of interest in understand was to break with the widely accepted ap
ing this basic and ubiquitous construct. proach, to that point, that trust was dispositional
Since we were drawing perspectives from and "trait-like" and to argue that trust was an
multiple disciplines as inputs to the model, we aspect of relationships. That meant it varied
wanted to provide a model that was generally within person and across relationships. With
applicable and would be used across multiple some exceptions (e.g., Driscoll, 1978; C. L. Scott,
disciplines. We were gratified to find in a recent 1980), the dominant conceptual and operational
344

This content downloaded from


128.239.99.140 on Sat, 03 Dec 2022 18:09:08 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
2007 Schoorman, Mayer, and Davis 345

definition of trust in the literature was Rotter's research points out that trust should be exam
(1967). We then went the next step and included ined at both the macro and micro levels within
ability as an antecedent of trust that allowed a an organization (McEvily, Perrone, & Zaheer,
party's trust to vary within a given trustee but 2003; Zaheer, McEvily, & Perrone, 1998). While
across domains. The dispositional aspects of the need to understand trust has been noted in
trust considered by Rotter are contained in the areas of study both within and between organi
construct of propensity to trust in our model. The zations, methodological difficulties can arise in
literature that has followed our model has not the absence of a clear multilevel conceptual
questioned this decision and has accepted the model (Mossholder & Bedeian, 1983; Rousseau,
view that trust is based in relationships. 1985).
Just as perceptions about an individual's abil
ity, benevolence, and integrity will have an im
Application Across Levels of Analysis
pact on how much trust the individual can gar
The importance of multilevel and cross-level ner, these perceptions also affect the extent to
perspectives is gaining increasing attention in which an organization will be trusted. We de
organizational research. This has led to a call fined each of these trustworthiness dimensions
for examining trust across levels of organization so that it could be applied to interpersonal, in
al analysis (e.g., Rousseau, Sitkin, Burt, & Cam tergroup, or interorganizational levels of analy
erer, 1998). There is a need to understand trust sis.
both within and between organizations because At higher levels of analysis, such as between
methodological difficulties can arise in the ab organizations, viewing the trustee in terms of
sence of a clear multilevel conceptual model ability and integrity seems to be well accepted.
(Currall & Inkpen, 2002; Mossholder & Bedeian, At macro levels of analysis, however, benevo
1983; Rousseau, 1985). lence has received little attention. We defined
We have heard from a number of scholars that benevolence as the extent to which a party is
the 1995 framework is fairly robust across levels believed to want to do good for the trusting
of analysis. A bit of history on the development party, aside from an egocentric profit motive.
of our theory may shed light on this issue. Early Does the other company hold the focal compa
drafts of our paper developed our trust model ny's best interests as highly important? While
across multiple levels of analysis. One of our we may be able to identify situations, such as
initial goals was to develop a theory that would sole proprietorships, where the owners have
be applicable across levels of analysis. We were strong bonds that display significant benevo
careful to develop constructs that would cross lence toward one another, the more traditional
levels of analysis, and we developed examples mode is probably one wherein each company is
of how cross-level applications of the model motivated primarily by its own financial inter
would work. Perhaps it was fortunate that early ests. If this is indeed the norm, benevolence is
reviewers of our paper made the accurate obser not likely to be the most important factor in the
vation that the paper was very cumbersome development of interorganizational trust. How
(and long) because it developed the multilevel ever, acts of benevolence (e.g., allowing bench
model. They recommended that we restrict our marking) from a potential partner in a joint ven
paper to a single level. The fact that our initial ture would help to build trust.
goal was to develop a multilevel theory is prob We contend that all three factors of ability,
ably why the model works as well as it does benevolence, and integrity can contribute to
across levels, but we do agree with those who trust in a group or organization. Consider, for
argue that one of the weaknesses in much of the instance, a supplier-buyer relationship. The
current trust research is that it is limited to re buyer may believe that a supplier is able to
lationships at a single level of analysis, consid provide a quality product in a timely fashion.
ering either dyadic trust relationships within However, this only assures that the supplier
organizations or trust between organizations. couid perform. This does not mean that it will
Several authors have recognized differences perform, and, therefore, the supplier will not
in trust for single referents at different hierar necessarily be trusted. The perception that the
chical levels within an organization (e.g., Cook supplier has integrity suggests that it will fulfill
& Wall, 1980; Driscoll, 1978; D. Scott, 1980). Recent agreements as promised. Yet even if there is an

This content downloaded from


128.239.99.140 on Sat, 03 Dec 2022 18:09:08 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
346 Academy of Management Review April

agreement, if the supplier's ability to deliver is time as the relationship between the parties de
questionable, it will not be trusted. If the sup velops" (1995: 722).
plier is perceived as benevolent, it will have a Despite these assertions, in many empirical
strong desire to serve this particular buyer's studies researchers have raised questions about
needs. If the supplier's integrity is suspect be the high observed correlation between benevo
cause, for instance, its track record with other lence and integrity and have questioned the in
firms is inconsistent with its stated policies, dependence of these variables. In a discussion
trust will again be lacking. As the perception of of several empirical studies, Schoorman (2002)
each of these factors increases, we would expect observed that the findings as a whole were com
an increase in willingness to take a risk in the pletely consistent with the model. Those studies
relationship. conducted in laboratory settings were more
The trust of either the dominant coalition or likely to show a high correlation between be
the management team is critical to understand nevolence and integrity because the relation
ing organizational trust, since it is this level of ships had not had time to develop any real data
trust that will govern the strategic actions of the about benevolence. In field samples where the
organization (Cyert & March, 1963; Simon, 1957). parties had longer relationships, benevolence
As with individuals, we propose that some or and integrity were more likely to be separable
ganizations develop greater propensities to factors. We continue to find this pattern to be
trust than do others. For organizations, these consistent in our research. We think it would be
propensities develop from geographic, industry, interesting for future research to establish more
and economic histories. A series of previous in specifically the process and time frames in
teractions with other organizations that resulted which each of the variables contributes to trust.
in, for example, lawsuits or monetary losses
would lower an organization's propensity to
Trust Risk, and Control Systems
trust. Conversely, a series of such experiences
as mutual benchmarking with various organiza In our model we argued that trust would lead
tions that significantly improved the quality to risk taking in a relationship (see Proposition
processes for an organization would increase its 5). Perceived risk moderates the relationship be
trust propensity. tween trust and risk taking in our model. Trust is
In summary, groups and organizations can the "willingness to take risk," and the level of
both garner trust from other parties and trust trust is an indication of the amount of risk that
other parties. Our model was designed to under one is willing to take. Clearly, control systems
stand the major factors that explain trust from are an alternate mechanism for dealing with
not only the individual level but from the group risk in relationships. Recently, several scholars
and organizational perspectives as well. have speculated about the relationship between
trust and control systems in dealing with risk
The Time Dimension (McEvily et al., 2003; Sitkin & George, 2005).
Our views on this issue are developed further
One of the issues explicit in our theory was in Davis, Schoorman, and Donaldson (1997), in
that "time" would play an important role in the which we argue that one of the major distinc
meaningfulness of the variables in the model. tions between agency theory and stewardship
We noted that propensity, as a dispositional theory is the use of trust versus control systems
quality, would be an important factor at the very to manage risk. However, we do not see these
beginning of the relationship. We also noted mechanisms as being mutually exclusive. On
that judgments of ability and integrity would the contrary, when the risk in a situation is
form relatively quickly in the course of the rela greater than the trust (and, thus, the willingness
tionship and that benevolence judgments would to take risk), a control system can bridge the
take more time. Proposition 3 states that "the difference by lowering the perceived risk to a
effect of integrity on trust will be most salient level that can be managed by trust. For exam
early in the relationship prior to the develop ple, in an organization that has a culture of
ment of meaningful benevolence data" (1995: "open book management" and transparency in
722); Proposition 4 states that "the effect of per numbers (a control system), the levels of per
ceived benevolence on trust will increase over ceived risk may be lower. There is a greater

This content downloaded from


128.239.99.140 on Sat, 03 Dec 2022 18:09:08 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
2007 Schoorman, Mayer, and Davis 347

opportunity to empower employees by trusting has been a different story. We defined trust as a
them to manage larger budgets and for employ willingness to be vulnerable to another party.
ees to trust the supervisor that performance As such, suitable measurement of the construct
based compensation is fair. necessitates that questions be asked that assess
However, there is an important caveat that the extent to which a trustor is willing to volun
must be noted. If there is a very strong system of tarily take risks at the hands of the trustee.
controls in an organization, it will inhibit the We developed a short, four-item measure,
development of trust. Not only will there be few with each of the items tapping into how willing
situations where there is any remaining per the trustor was to be vulnerable to the trustee.
ceived risk but trustworthy actions will be at We (Schoorman, Mayer, & Davis, 1996a) found
tributed to the existence of the control system that veterinary doctors took bigger risks with
rather than to the trustee (cf. Strickland, 1958). those employees they trusted more. The impact
Thus, a trustee's actions that should be inter of trust went beyond that explained by the abil
preted as driven by benevolence or by integrity ity, benevolence, and integrity of the trustee.
may be viewed simply as responses to the con Despite the measure's brevity, we found its in
trol systems. The use of control systems is how ternal consistency strong (Cronbach's alpha =
agency theory proposes dealing with risk man .82).
agement, and this does not foster the develop We (Davis, Schoorman, Mayer, & Tan, 2000)
ment of trust. used the same measure in a restaurant setting
to measure the trust that employees had in their
general manager and found an alpha of .62. This
The Reciprocity of Trust
measure of trust in the leader significantly pre
One of the limitations of our model that we dicted subsequent sales, profits, and employee
noted in the conclusion of our 1995 paper was turnover in the restaurants (Davis, Mayer, &
that our conceptualization was unidirectional. Schoorman, 1995; Davis et al., 2000). Based on
We did not explore the reciprocity in trusting these results, we concluded that if trust in the
relationships. This is a particularly salient issue general manager could be developed and sus
in the area of leader-subordinate relationships, tained, it would be a significant competitive ad
since the dominant view among leadership the vantage to the firm, and the framework, includ
orists is that leader-member exchange (LMX) is ing ability, benevolence, and integrity, merited
mutual and reciprocal (Graen & Uhl-Bien, 1995; further consideration as an approach to build
Liden, Wayne, & Still well, 1993). In an extension ing trust in management.
of our model, Brower, Schoorman, and Tan (2000) Using this same measure, a quasi-experiment
argued that, unlike LMX, trust is not necessarily (Mayer & Davis, 1999) showed that trust in top
mutual and is not reciprocal. One of the impli management was significantly improved by
cations of this argument is that, in a relation identifying and replacing an invalid appraisal
ship, A can trust B, but B may not trust A. This is system. While the alpha in this study was lower
completely consistent with the approach to trust (i.e., .59 and .60 in two waves of data), the test
and trust formation that we presented in our retest reliability was quite strong, at .75 over
model but is inconsistent with the views in the five months and .66 over nine months. Further
leadership literature. Empirical studies that ex more, the quasi-experimental results were sig
amine this reciprocal linkage of how one party's nificant, even though the sizes of the groups
trust affects the other party's trust in return, being compared were modest (i.e., twenty-two
rather than assuming them to be equivalent, are and fifty-seven). In additional analyses we
rare (Serva, Fuller, & Mayer, 2005). We feel that found an average interitem correlation of r = .32.
this presents a fruitful area for future research. For each item we calculated a correlation be
tween it and a composite of the other three
items. These correlations ranged from .23 to .49,
Measurement of Trust As "Willingness to Be
Vulnerable" with an average of .38.
These results fall well within Kline's (1986)
While the theory of trust described in the 1995 description of a measure of a complex construct
paper has been very influential in the develop that has maximum validity. Kline noted that
ment of trust research, the measurement of trust such a measure would only have low internal

This content downloaded from


128.239.99.140 on Sat, 03 Dec 2022 18:09:08 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
348 Academy of Management Review April

consistency reliability. Internal consistency es than desirable alpha levels of our original mea
timates suffer because of the brevity of this mea sure. While we think that a combination of its
sure. As Cortina says, "[Alpha] must be inter conceptual clarity, test-retest reliability, and re
preted with the number of items in mind" (1993: lationship with other variables in the nomolog
102). If the measure were truly unreliable, we ical net across a number of studies is just as
would not have been able to attain the signifi important a consideration as the alpha level of a
cant results we have repeatedly found with its four-item measure, we recognize that develop
use (Nunnally, 1978). The results of these studies ment of the more recent longer measures with
all support the idea that while our four-item higher alpha levels was warranted. The mea
measure of willingness to be vulnerable has at sures we have developed more recently appear
times had lower than desired levels of Cron to have overcome concerns with Cronbach al
bach's alpha, it is a robust, stable, and valid pha levels. We believe that as these measures
measure of the construct as we defined it. become more widely available, more research
More recent efforts to develop the measure ers will choose to measure trust as defined in
have yielded higher alpha levels. Mayer and our 1995 paper.
Gavin (2005) expanded the measure to ten items, On a related note, work on trust would be
yielding alphas of .82 and .76 for the plant man facilitated by further development of measures
ager and the top management team, respec of propensity. Many researchers have found Rot
tively. Further analysis revealed two factors, ter's (1967) twenty-five-item measure too long to
however. One factor consisted of the original include as a variable in studies with many other
measure with an additional item, whereas the variables and are concerned about its multidi
second factor consisted of five items that gener mensionality. Our adaptation (Mayer & Davis,
ally cited willingness to engage in specific be 1999; Schoorman et al., 1996a), while much
haviors that would put a trustor at risk, such as shorter and unidimensional, has not consis
communicating sensitive information to the tently produced high Cronbach's alphas (e.g., .55
trustee. The five-item general willingness to be and .66 in Mayer & Davis, 1999). We are not
vulnerable scale yielded an alpha of .81 for the aware of any brief, unidimensional published
focal plant manager and .72 for the top manage measure of propensity that produces consis
ment team. Both of these were improvements of tently high alpha levels. Development of such a
.06 and .07, respectively, over the four-item mea measure might enable finding more relation
sure. While results using the five-item scale ships between propensity and other variables of
were reported in that study, the ten-item scale interest, particularly early in the development of
yielded nearly identical results. a relationship.
In what may be the most promising measure
to date, Schoorman and Ballinger (2006) ex
panded the original measure to seven items, NEW DIMENSIONS IN THE RESEARCH ON
taking care to maintain the conceptual defini TRUST
tion but not create redundant items. It has pro
Affect, Emotion, and the Impact on Trust
duced an alpha level of .84 in a sample of vet
erinary hospital employees. This measure The basis of our model was to understand how
appears in the Appendix. parties process information about others,
Gillespie (2003) developed and validated a thereby deciding how much risk to take with
measure of trust based on the willingness to be those others. Perceptions of others and percep
vulnerable definition. This ten-item Behavioral tions of risk inherent in the behaviors being
Trust Inventory has good psychometric proper considered must be processed in order to come
ties and shows promise for future research to decisions about taking risks. For instance, as
based on this conceptual definition. one evaluates a trustee's ability in the domain
The upshot of this discussion is that measur of interest or considers relevant inputs about a
ing trust as we defined it involves asking ques trustee's integrity, one is thinking. As such, our
tions that measure a trustor's willingness to be model represents a cognitive approach to trust.
vulnerable. We are aware from numerous per More recent work has pointed to the fact that
sonal communications from other researchers trust also involves emotion. Williams (2001) has
that many of them are concerned with the lower pointed out that affective responses influence

This content downloaded from


128.239.99.140 on Sat, 03 Dec 2022 18:09:08 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
2007 Schoorman, Mayer, and Davis 349

how people evaluate their level of trust in an ative responses. Dating back to the 1950s (e.g.,
other party. Similarly, Jones and George (1998) Kruglanski, 1970; Strickland, 1958), some work
have argued that emotions and moods provide has been done that begins to lay the ground
people with information on how they are expe work for how attributions are made when trust is
riencing trust. Dunn and Schweitzer (2005) found broken. We suggest that which trustworthiness
that emotional states?even unrelated to the factor is damaged and how it was damaged
trustee or the situation?have an effect on trust. influence not only how repairable the damage is
Weber, Malhotra, and Murnighan (2005) have likely to be but how effective various repair
shown that emotional attachments can cause a strategies are likely to be. Repair attempts
trustor to take a sudden risk not warranted by might be looked at either as mediating pro
the available evidence. cesses within the feedback arrow in our model
Proponents of the strictly cognitive approach or as influencing the trustworthiness factors di
to decision making about trust would argue that rectly (e.g., receiving information about trust
while emotions may create a temporary "irra worthiness factors outside the process of having
tionality" about the data on ability, benevo taken a risk that went awry). Further theoretical
lence, and integrity, after a period of time the work in this area to understand the conditions
perception would return to a rational perspec under which various repair strategies are effec
tive. Nonetheless, it appears to be clear that tive would help in developing prescriptive road
emotions do influence the perception of the an maps for repairing broken trust.
tecedents of trust and, therefore, the trust in re Forgiveness is another evolving area that
lationships. It is also likely that this emotion holds promise for understanding trust repair af
does dissipate over time after a violation of ter a violation. An insightful paper by Aquino,
trust. What is not clear is whether it ever com Grover, Goldman, and Folger (2003) provides an
pletely dissipates and returns to a nonemotional important separation between resolving nega
evaluation. Alternatively, while emotions are tive emotions (i.e., forgiveness itself) and behav
being experienced, they may lead the trustor to iors that follow that restore the relationship.
update prior perceptions of the trustworthiness More work in this area is needed to determine
dimensions and trust such that even after the such basic issues as the conditions under which
emotions dissipate, the effect on the cognitive forgiveness enhances trust repair after a viola
evaluations remains. We think the role of emo tion, what conditions increase the likelihood
tions is a very interesting area of research and that an offended party will forgive the violator,
will add a new dimension to the model. and the role that forgiveness plays in the trust
repair process.
Violation and Trust Repair
Some exemplary work has been done to un
The Concept of Distrust
derstand trust violation and repair (e.g., Lewicki
& Bunker, 1996; Morrison & Robinson, 1997; Rob There has been considerable discussion in the
inson, 1996), but this topic warrants more re literature about the concept of distrust, as well
search. As part of their contributions, these au as the relationship between trust and distrust.
thors highlight the idea that violation of trust is Lewicki, McAllister, and Bies (1998) framed the
likely to be an emotional event for the trustor. At debate in the organizational literature, arguing
the time of this writing, a special issue of the that trust and distrust are separate dimensions
Academy of Management Review (edited by and not the opposite ends of a single continuum.
Dirks, Lewicki, and Zaheer) focused on trust re They noted that two-factor models of satisfac
pair is in preparation. This is a very appropriate tion and dissatisfaction had been proposed be
topic and promises to add valuable insight into fore (Herzberg, Mausner, & Snyderman, 1967),
the process by which trust development can and that the contemporary evidence from stud
move forward after trust has been damaged. ies of positive and negative affectivity supports
We believe that in order to repair trust, it is this view. Their main reason for suggesting such
critical to first understand how it was damaged an approach to the trust-distrust distinction was
in the first place, since different means of dam because "relationships are multifaceted or mul
aging trust are likely to require different repar tiplex" (1998: 442), and we need a model that

This content downloaded from


128.239.99.140 on Sat, 03 Dec 2022 18:09:08 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
350 Academy of Management Review April

allows for both trust and distrust to exist in the that you don't need different constructs to ac
same relationship. count for this.
In our model we chose to take the opposite McKnight and Chervany (2001) produced an
(and more traditional) view that trust and dis excellent summary of the literature on defini
trust are the opposite ends of the same contin tions of trust and distrust and the models that
uum. This is consistent with dictionary defini describe each of the constructs. They reviewed
tions?for example, Webster's defines distrust the literature on trust and distrust and devel
as "the lack or absence of trust" and Random oped separate conceptual models (antecedent
House as "to have no trust in." In sociology, and contextual variables) for each construct.
Ross, Mirowski, and Pribesh define "mistrust" as The resulting models are identical for both trust
the "absence of faith in other people" (2001: 568). and distrust, which suggests to us that perhaps
Luhmann argues that distrust is a "functional we do not need both models. In fact, these au
equivalent of trust" (1979: 71). Our definition of thors conclude that "most trust theorists agree
trust?willingness to take risk (i.e., be vulnera that trust and distrust are separate constructs
ble) in a relationship?means that at the lowest that are opposites of each other" (2001: 42). We
level of trust, one would take no risks at all. We would simply add that if they are opposites of
felt that the complete lack of trust and distrust each other, there is little added value to treating
are the same thing. them as separate constructs.
In our model of trust, however, we argued that A review of empirical work on the conceptual
ization of distrust in the literature produced sur
ability is an important antecedent of trust, along
with benevolence and integrity. This was a de prising results. Some who argue that trust and
distrust are different dimensions only study one
viation from an emerging view that trust was
of the constructs at a time, making it difficult to
more affective. This point is further clarified in
develop data on the differences. It is particularly
Schoorman, Mayer, and Davis (1996b). The im
interesting that some researchers who study the
portant implication of the addition of ability to
the antecedents of trust is that it creates a concept of distrust (e.g. McAllister, Pang, Tan, &
Ruan, 2006) have used our measure of trust as a
framework of trust that is domain specific. This
willingness to be vulnerable (Mayer & Davis,
is noted in our original article (1995: 717) and 1999; Schoorman et al., 1996b) and reverse
follows from the work of Zand (1972). Trust being
scored it to represent their measure of distrust.
domain specific allows for the multifaceted and In sum, we can find no credible evidence that a
multiplex relationships about which Lewicki et concept of distrust that is conceptually different
al. (1998) raised concerns. For example, it may be from trust is theoretically or empirically viable.
appropriate to trust a colleague to do a good job
collaborating on a research project but to not
trust him/her to do a good job teaching your International and Cross-Cultural Implications
class in your absence. The difference in the level for Trust
of trust within the same relationship is a func Over the same time frame in which interest in
tion of the different abilities across different do
trust accelerated, there was a significant in
mains. The skills required to present and inter crease in interest in studying cross-national and
act effectively in class differ from those cross-cultural differences. It is therefore not sur
necessary to do research. prising that much of the explosion of interest in
Lewicki et al. (1998) produced a chart with a trust research has come from around the globe.
high trust and high distrust condition in which Over 20 percent of the 1,100 studies listed in
one would presumably "trust, but verify." We Google Scholar that cite our paper were written
feel this is not a reasonable argument within in a language other than English. The World
domain. If you trust a partner, you do not need to Economic Forum is made up of the world's lead
verify. Doing so would be the clearest indication ing politicians and business leaders. It meets
that you do nof trust. We do agree that you might annually in Davos, Switzerland to discuss a
trust your colleague to produce a literature re wide range of international issues. It has been
view but may need to verify his/her ability to monitoring public trust levels since 2003 through
deliver in the classroom by reviewing his/her a biannual global public opinion poll conducted
lecture notes and presentation. Our model says by GlobeScan Incorporated. The latest findings

This content downloaded from


128.239.99.140 on Sat, 03 Dec 2022 18:09:08 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
2007 Schoorman, Mayer, and Davis 351

from the poll show that trust in a range of insti laborative, being-oriented, "feminine" cultures
tutions has dropped significantly since January tend to put more emphasis on the benevolence
2004 to levels not seen since the months follow variable. While these are broad generalizations
ing the September 11 terrorist attacks in 2001 of relationships between culture and trust, they
(www.weforum.org). GlobeScan reports that the illustrate the potential value of future research
overall trust level for global companies is the to develop these links more carefully.
lowest since the tracking began. While we are
not sure what definition of trust was used in this
poll, these results present obvious reasons for
concern. Context-Specific Models of Trust
The recent GLOBE project by House and colour intention in developing the model
It was
leagues (House, Hanges, Javidan, Dorfman,
to be as &parsimonious as possible and to de
Gupta, 2004; House, Javidan, Hanges, velop
& Dorfa model that would be generalizable to
man, 2002) measured the cultural orientation of
the broadest number of contexts. In order to
sixty-two societies around the world based
achieve this, we neglected many specific con
largely on the cultural dimensions identified by that would be relevant to a more
text variables
Hofstede (1980). These scholars used restricted
nine ditrust domain. We think it would be
mensions of culture, as well as twenty-one lead to specify contextual variables for
appropriate
ership dimensions. All of this research has led to that are unique to studying trust
the model
the inevitable question of how trust is different
within a particular context. For example, much
across cultures (Den Hartog, 2004; Wasti, Tan,
of the research on trust in organizations has
Brower, & Onder, in press). focused on the relationship between supervisors
We believe that one of the ways inandwhich subordinates. In this context, the hierarchi
culture affects trust is through the propensity
cal power difference and the asymmetry of in
variable. We have proposed that the anteced
formation that exist between the two individu
ents of propensity include personality, experi
als incul
ences, and culture. There is evidence in the the trusting relationship have some
important implications for how trust might de
ture literature that initial trust of strangers
velop. Ifof
varies across cultures. One of the dimensions the supervisor has more access to infor
mation about the subordinate and can initiate
culture that is most relevant to this issue is the
opportunities to gather information about abil
task versus relationship orientation of a culture.
ity, benevolence, and integrity, and if these op
Task-oriented cultures seem to have a higher
portunities are not available to the subordinate,
initial trust of strangers and therefore a higher
propensity, while relationship-oriented we would expect that the supervisor's trust in
cultures
the subordinate
need time to develop a relationship prior to would develop more quickly
than vice versa.
working on the task. The cultural variable of
uncertainty avoidance is well-establishedAdditionally,
as a since risk taking in the relation
shipor
predictor of predispositions to take risk is caused
be by an interaction between trust
and risk,
risk averse (Sully de Luque & Javidan, 2004). If one's perceptions of risk in the action
being contemplated
trust is the willingness to take risk in a relation (which our model separates
from
ship, how does uncertainty avoidance as a dis the trustee in question) will affect risk
taking
positional quality affect the development of actions. Ceteris paribus, the party who
trust? We think there is considerable workhas more
thatpower in the relationship will likely
needs to be done in fine-tuning what we perceive?by
know virtue of that power?less risk
about the influence of culture on the propensity and, thus, will engage in more risk-taking ac
to trust. tions. This would give the appearance that this
Culture can also affect the perception of abil party's trust is higher still. In the context of trust
ity, benevolence, and integrity and the impor between peers, there is likely to be a different
tance given to each of these variables in the set of variables that predict the development
model. More action-oriented, competitive, per and use of trust. We expect that studies in par
formance-oriented cultures?what Hofstede has ticular contexts will develop additional vari
called "masculine" cultures?tend to place a ables that help better explain the antecedents
higher value on the ability variable. More col and consequences of trust.

This content downloaded from


128.239.99.140 on Sat, 03 Dec 2022 18:09:08 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
352 Academy of Management Review April

SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS relative importance of ability, benevolence, and


integrity across cultures. Finally, we note that
In this paper we have taken the opportunity to
while we deliberately focused on a model that
reflect on our thinking as we developed the 1995
was maximally generalizable and parsimoni
piece, "An Integrative Model of Organizational
ous, it is appropriate to now examine context
Trust," and the knowledge context in which it
specific variables that might add to the model of
occurred. This paper has given us an opportu trust. One such area that our research has taken
nity to revisit some of the decisions that we
us into is the specific relationship between su
made and to evaluate them in light of the re
pervisor and subordinate in the workplace. Is
search that followed. A part of our charge in sues of power and information asymmetry make
writing this paper was to indicate how our own
this relationship and the trust it produces some
research in this area has progressed and how what unique.
we view the growing body of literature on the While a great deal of research has occurred in
topic. the area of trust over the past decade, the new
The levels-of-analysis issue continues to be research only suggests that there is a lot more to
an interesting topic of discussion, and more ex be done and many very promising avenues to
plicit extensions to group and organizational pursue. We hope that our comments in this pa
levels are warranted. We note that time issues
per can be a catalyst for some of this research.
addressed in our paper have not received the
attention that we expected. Research attempts
to study trust in very short laboratory simula
tions have yielded mixed but not unexpected
APPENDIX
results. By including a consideration of time, Trust Items from Schoorman and Ballinger
studies of trust should lead to more predictable (2006)
results. The interplay of trust, risk, and control
systems continues to be a much debated topic. _My supervisor keeps my interests in mind when
making decisions.
We have attempted to clarify our thinking on
_I would be willing to let my supervisor have complete
these dynamics. We see trust and control sys control over my future in this company.
tems as alternate and sometimes compatible _If my supervisor asked why a problem occurred, I
means for managing risk. We have extended our would speak freely even if I were partly to blame.
thinking about the reciprocity of trust to explic _I feel comfortable being creative because my
supervisor understands that sometimes creative
itly recognize the notion that, unlike relational solutions do not work.
leadership constructs (e.g., LMX), trust is not mu _It is important for me to have a good way to keep an
tual and not necessarily reciprocal. The mea eye on my supervisor.
sures of trust that we had developed based on _Increasing my vulnerability to criticism by my
our definition of trust have produced mixed re supervisor would be a mistake.
_If I had my way, I wouldn't let my supervisor have
sults in terms of internal consistency reliability any influence over decisions that are important to me.
estimates. Here we have explored some of the
developments and advances in the measure
ment area and have noted the need for contin
Response Scale
ued improvement.
We also have reviewed some of the interest 12 3 4 5
ing new directions in the research on trust.
Prominent among these is the inclusion of the strongly so
role of affect and emotion, trust violations, and
disagree disagree agree agree agree
nor
repair. We believe these constructs will add new disagree
dimensions to the model of trust and provide for
valuable research in the future. Another area
seeing rapid growth in interest is the role that REFERENCES
international and cross-cultural dimensions
Aquino, K., Grover, S. L., Goldman, B., & Folger, R. 2003. When
play in the model of trust. We see the greatest push doesn't come to shove: Interpersonal forgiveness
opportunities in the development of the concept in workplace relationships. Journal of Management In
of propensity across cultures, as well as for the quiry, 12: 209-216.

This content downloaded from


128.239.99.140 on Sat, 03 Dec 2022 18:09:08 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
2007 Schoorman, Mayer, and Davis 353

Brower, H. H., Schoorman, F. D., & Tan, H. H. 2000. A model of Jones, G. R., & George, J. M. 1998. The experience and evolu
relational leadership: The integration of trust and lead tion of trust: Implications for cooperation and teamwork.
er-member exchange. Leadership Quarterly, 11: 227-250. Academy of Management Review, 23: 531-546.
Cook, J., & Wall, T. 1980. New work attitude measures of trust, Kline, P. 1986. A handbook of test construction: Introduction
organizational commitment and personal need nonful to psychometric design. London: Methuen.
fillment. Journal of Occupational Psychology, 53: 39-52.
Kramer, R. M., & Tyler, T. R. (Eds.). 1996. Trust in organiza
Cortina, J. 1993. What is coefficient alpha? Journal of Applied tions: Frontiers of theory and research. Thousand Oaks,
Psychology, 78: 98-104. CA: Sage.
Currall, S. C, & Inkpen, A. C. 2002. A multilevel approach to Kruglanski, A. W. 1970. Attributing trustworthiness in super
trust in joint ventures. Journal of International Business visor-worker relations. Journal of Experimental Psychol
Studies, 33: 479-495. ogy, 6: 214-232.
Cyert, R. M., & March, I. G. 1963. A behavioral theory of the Lewicki, R. J., & Bunker, B. B. 1996. Developing and maintain
firm. Englewood Cliffs, NI: Prentice-Hall. ing trust in work relationships. In R. M. Kramer & T. R.
Tyler (Eds.), Trust in organizations: Frontiers of theory
Davis, J. H., Mayer, R. C, & Schoorman, F. D. 1995. The trusted
and research: 114-139. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
general manager and firm performance: A strategic ad
vantage. Paper presented at the Annual International Lewicki, R. J., McAllister, D. J., & Bies, R. J. 1998. Trust and
Strategic Management Society Conference, Mexico City. distrust: New relationships and realities. Academy of
Davis, J. H., Schoorman, F. D., & Donaldson, L. 1997. Toward Management Review, 23: 438-458.
a stewardship theory of management. Academy of Man Liden, R. C, Wayne, S. J., & Stillwell, D. 1993. A longitudinal
agement Review, 22: 20-47. study on the early development of leader-member ex
changes. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78: 662-674.
Davis, J. H., Schoorman, F. D., Mayer, R. C, & Tan, H. H. 2000.
The trusted general manager and business unit perfor Luhmann, N. 1979. Trust and power. New York: Wiley.
mance: Empirical evidence of a competitive advantage.
Mayer, R. C, & Davis, J. H. 1999. The effect of the performance
Strategic Management Journal, 21: 563-576.
appraisal system on trust for management: A field qua
Den Hartog, D. N. 2004. Assertiveness. In R. J. House, P. J. si-experiment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84: 123?
Hanges, M. Javidan, P. Dorfman, & V. Gupta (Eds.), Cul 136.
ture, leadership and organizations: The GLOBE study of
Mayer, R. C, Davis, J. H., & Schoorman, F. D. 1995. An inte
62 societies: 395-431. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
grative model of organizational trust. Academy of Man
Driscoll, J. W. 1978. Trust and participation in organizational agement Review, 20: 709-734.
decision making as predictors of satisfaction. Academy Mayer, R. C, & Gavin, M. B. 2005. Trust in management and
of Management Journal, 21: 44-56.
performance: Who minds the shop while the employees
Dunn, J. R., & Schweitzer, M. E. 2005. Feeling and believing: watch the boss? Academy of Management Journal 48:
The influence of emotion on trust. Journal of Applied 874-888.
Psychology, 88: 736-748.
McAllister, D. J. 1995. Affect- and cognition-based trust as
Gillespie, N. 2003. Measuring trust in work relationships: The foundations for interpersonal cooperation in organiza
Behavioral Trust Inventory. Paper presented at the an tions. Academy of Management Journal, 38: 24-59.
nual meeting of the Academy of Management, Seattle.
McAllister, D. J., Pang, K., Tan, H. H., & R?an, Y. 2006. Social
Graen, G. B., & Uhl-Bien, M. 1995. Relationship-based ap dynamics of paranoia and distrust in teams. Paper pre
proach to leadership: Development of leader-member sented at the annual meeting of the Academy of Man
exchange (LMX) theory of leadership over 25 years: Ap agement, Atlanta.
plying a multi-level multi-domain perspective. Leader
McEvily, B., Perrone, V., & Zaheer, A. 2003. Trust as an orga
ship Quarterly, 6: 219-247.
nizing principle. Organization Science, 14: 91-103.
Herzberg, F., Mausner, B., & Snyderman, B. B. 1967. The mo
McKnight, D. H., & Chervany, N. L. 2001. Trust and distrust
tivation to work (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley.
definitions: One bite at a time. In R. Falcone, M. Singh, &
Hofstede, G. 1980. Culture's consequences. Beverly Hills, CA: Y.-H. Tan (Eds.), Trust in cyber-societies: 27-54. Berlin &
Sage. Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag.
Hosmer, L. T. 1995. Trust: The connecting link between orga Morrison, E. W., & Robinson, S. L. 1997. When employees feel
nizational theory and philosophical ethics. Academy of betrayed: A model of how psychological contract viola
Management Review, 20: 379-403. tion develops. Academy of Management Review, 22:
226-256.
House, R., Javidan, M., Hanges, P., & Dorfman, P. 2002. Un
derstanding cultures and implicit leadership theories Mossholder, K. W., & Bedeian, A. G. 1983. Cross-level infer
across the globe: An introduction to project GLOBE. Jour ence and organizational research: Perspectives on inter
nal of World Business, 37: 3-10. pretation and application. Academy of Management Re
view, 8: 547-558.
House, R. J., Hanges, P. J., Javidan, M., Dorfman, P., & Gupta,
V. 2004. Culture, leadership and organizations: The Nunnally, J. C. 1978. Psychometric theory. New York:
GLOBE study of 62 societies. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. McGraw-Hill.

This content downloaded from


128.239.99.140 on Sat, 03 Dec 2022 18:09:08 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
354 Academy of Management Review April

Robinson, S. L. 1996. Trust and breach of the psychological Serva, M. A., Fuller, M. A., & Mayer, R. C. 2005. The reciprocal
contract. Administrative Science Quarterly, 41: 574-599. nature of trust: A longitudinal study of interacting
Ross, C. E., Mirowsky, J., & Pribesh, S. 2001. Powerlessness teams. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 26: 625-648.
and the amplification of threat: Neighborhood disadvan Simon, H. 1957. Administrative behavior. New York: Macmil
tage, disorder and mistrust. American Sociological Re lan.
view, 66: 568-591.
Sitkin, S. B., & George, E. 2005. Managerial trust-building
Rotter, J. B. 1967. A new scale for the measurement of inter through the use of legitimating formal and informal
personal trust. Journal of Personality, 35: 651-665. control mechanisms. International Sociology, 20: 307
Rousseau, D. M. 1985. Issues of level in organizational re 338.
search: Multi-level and cross-level perspectives. He Strickland, L. H. 1958. Surveillance and trust. Journal of Per
search in Organizational Behavior, 7: 1-37. sonality, 24: 200-215.
Rousseau, D. M., Sitkin, S. B., Burt, R. S., & Camerer, C. 1998.
Sully de Luque, M.# & Javidan, M. 2004. Uncertainty avoid
Not so different after all: A cross-discipline view of trust. ance. In R. J. House, P. J. Hanges, M. Javidan, P. Dorfman,
Academy of Management Review, 23: 393-404. & V. Gupta (Eds.), Culture, leadership and organizations:
Schoorman, F. D. 2002. Discussant at symposium titled Inte The GLOBE study of 62 societies: 603-644. Thousand
grating trust perspectives: Foundations for a revised in Oaks, CA: Sage.
tegrative model of organizational trust. Annual meeting
Wasti, S. A., Tan, H. H.f Brower, H. H., & Onder, C. In press.
of the Academy of Management, Denver.
Cross-cultural measurement of supervisor trustworthi
Schoorman, F. D., & Ballinger, G. A. 2006. Leadership, trust ness: An assessment of measurement invariance across
and client service in veterinary hospitals. Working pa three cultures. Leadership Quarterly.
per, Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN.
Weber, J. M., Malhotra, D., & Murnighan, J. K. 2005. Normal
Schoorman, F. D., Mayer, R. C, & Davis, J. H. 1996a. Empow acts of irrational trust: Motivated attributions and the
erment in veterinary clinics: The role of trust in delega trust development process. Research in Organizational
tion. Presented in a symposium on trust at the 11th Behavior, 26: 75-101.
Annual Conference, Society for Industrial and Organi
Weiner, B. 1986. An attributional model of motivation and
zational Psychology (SIOP), San Diego.
emotion. New York: Springer-Verlag.
Schoorman, F. D., Mayer, R. C, & Davis, J. H. 1996b. Organi
zational trust: Philosophical perspectives and concep Williams, M. 2001. In whom we trust: Group membership as
tual definitions. Academy of Management Review, 21: an affective context for trust development. Academy of
337-340. Management Review, 26: 377-396.
Scott, C. L., III. 1980. Interpersonal trust: A comparison of Zaheer, A., McEvily, B., & Perrone, V. 1998. Does trust matter?
attitudinal and situational factors. Human Relations, 33: Exploring the effects of interorganizational and inter
805-812. personal trust on performance. Organization Science, 9:
141-159.
Scott, D. 1980. The causal relationship between trust and the
assessed value of management by objectives. Journal of Zand, D. E. 1972. Trust and managerial problem solving.
Management, 6: 157-175. Administrative Science Quarterly, 17: 229-239.

F. David Schoorman (schoor@purdue.edu) is a professor of management in the Kran


nert Graduate School of Management at Purdue University. He earned his Ph.D. from
Carnegie Mellon University. He has published research in the areas of trust, leader
ship, decision making, and organizational effectiveness.

Roger C. Mayer (Rmayer@uakron.edu) is professor of management at The University


of Akron. He earned his Ph.D. from Purdue University. His research includes employee
motivation, decision making, organizational effectiveness, and social capital.

James H. Davis (Davis.31@nd.edu) is an associate professor, the Siegfried Director of


Entrepreneurship, and the John F. O'Shaughnessy Professor of Family Enterprise at
the University of Notre Dame. He earned his Ph.D. from the University of Iowa. His
research interests include trust, corporate governance, family business, stewardship
theory, and social capital.

This content downloaded from


128.239.99.140 on Sat, 03 Dec 2022 18:09:08 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms

You might also like