You are on page 1of 15

Review

Linking ADHD to the Neural


Circuitry of Attention
Adrienne Mueller,1,* David S. Hong,2 Steven Shepard,1 and
Tirin Moore1,3
Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a complex condition with a
Trends
heterogeneous presentation. Current diagnosis is primarily based on subjec-
Deficits in many different cognitive
tive experience and observer reports of behavioral symptoms – an approach domains are associated with ADHD.
that has significant limitations. Many studies show that individuals with ADHD Cognitive batteries that assess the
performance of individuals within these
exhibit poorer performance on cognitive tasks than neurotypical controls, and different cognitive domains show that
at least seven main functional domains appear to be implicated in ADHD. We the disorder is very heterogeneous.
discuss the underlying neural mechanisms of cognitive functions associated
ADHD medication that improves per-
with ADHD, with emphasis on the neural basis of selective attention, demon- formance in one cognitive measure
strating the feasibility of basic research approaches for further understanding does not necessarily improve it in
others.
cognitive behavioral processes as they relate to human psychopathology. The
study of circuit-level mechanisms underlying executive functions in nonhuman Studies in human and nonhuman pri-
primates holds promise for advancing our understanding, and ultimately the mates have revealed much about the
underlying mechanisms of endogen-
treatment, of ADHD. ous, exogenous, and spatial- and fea-
ture-based selective attention.
Bridging the Gap
ADHD is a complex condition affecting up to 10% of children worldwide [1] and comprises a The regions of the brain and the neu-
romodulators that influence selective
heterogeneous set of behavioral dysfunctions. While prior work demonstrates validity of the attention and other cognitive domains
ADHD construct in general, ongoing debate regarding subtype validity demonstrates the need implicated in ADHD are non-
for a more coherent model. An etiological basis for ADHD remains elusive, as have been efforts overlapping.
to subtype ADHD using biological indicators rather than solely relying on clinical assessments.

Decades of ADHD studies in humans have been conducted in parallel with research in animal
models on the neural mechanisms and neural circuitry underlying attention and other cognitive
functions. However, success in aligning these two bodies of evidence has been limited. In this
review we discuss the apparent gap between the clinical definition of ADHD and our current
understanding of the neural circuits of cognition, with particular focus on selective attention as
an example paradigm. We begin by describing the current challenges faced by clinicians
diagnosing ADHD and the heterogeneity of cognitive dysfunction in the disorder. Next, we
summarize current knowledge of the neural basis of selective attention in human and nonhu-
man primates. We then discuss how the neural mechanisms of selective attention might relate
to mechanisms of other cognitive functions associated with ADHD. Finally, we discuss possible 1
Department of Neurobiology,
ways to move forward in mapping symptom phenotypes onto specific cognitive domains, and Stanford University, Stanford, CA
cognitive domains onto neural representations. 94305, USA
2
Department of Psychiatry, Stanford
University, Stanford, CA 94305, USA
Diagnosis of ADHD 3
Howard Hughes Medical Institute,
In current clinical practice, ADHD is diagnosed through observation and self-report of behavior. Stanford University, Stanford, CA
These are typically conducted through clinical interviews with the individual and family, and 94305, USA

often use rating scales of ADHD symptoms. Consensus criteria for ADHD, as defined by the
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5; http://www.psychiatry.org/ *Correspondence:
psychiatrists/practice/dsm), require a persistent pattern of inattention and/or hyperactivity and alm04@stanford.edu (A. Mueller).

474 Trends in Cognitive Sciences, June 2017, Vol. 21, No. 6 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2017.03.009
© 2017 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
impulsivity over a period greater than 6 months. A standard clinical encounter might unfold as Glossary
follows: parents present to a pediatrician’s office with concerns that their child has ADHD. The Conners’ continuous
pediatrician reviews the family’s concerns, noting whether the child is struggling in the performance task: a computer-
classroom, at home, or in peer relationships. The clinician obtains a detailed developmental based assessment of attention
performance. It is not routinely used
history, reviews recent behavior, and interviews the child, carefully noting evidence for persis- for diagnosis, but it is the most
tent patterns of disorganization, inattention, hyperactivity, and/or impulsivity. The clinician also common test used in research on
provides assessment forms for the family and teachers to fill out for collateral information. clinical populations.
Catecholamines: catecholamines
Review of the clinical assessment and standardized rating scales assists the clinician in
are a class of neuromodulators that
determining whether the current and past behavior of the child demonstrates six or more include dopamine and
features of inattention and/or six or more features of hyperactivity/impulsivity (Figure 1, left) to norepinephrine. Norepinephrine is a
establish a diagnosis of ADHD. synonym for noradrenaline.
Norepinephrine binds to
noradrenergic receptors, and is
Thus, current diagnosis of ADHD is primarily based on subjective experience and observer released from noradrenergic neurons.
reports of behavioral symptoms. This approach has significant limitations, particularly the Disorder of delay aversion: the
difficulty of correlating these observed behaviors with underlying neurobiological processes. proposal that ADHD results from an
altered motivational state and reward
Attempts have been made to develop more quantitative assessments, such as Conners’
processing that penalizes delays [14].
continuous performance task (CPT, see Glossary) [2]. However, these tools have only poor- Disorder of inhibition: the proposal
to-fair predictive power (e.g., [3]). The CPT and similar instruments lack the specificity required that ADHD results from a failure in
to capture the broad heterogeneity in cognitive phenotypes that converge on the ADHD the suppression of actions that
would interfere with goal-driven
diagnosis (e.g., [4]); as such, neuropsychological tools are not routinely used in clinical practice. behavior.
Furthermore, the existing nosology of ADHD is restricted to three presentations – inattentive, Nosology: the branch of medical
hyperactive/impulsive, and combined – which also do not capture the full heterogeneity of the science dealing with the classification
disorder (e.g., [5,6]). These subtypes have been shown to be limited in their ability to predict of diseases. The nosology of ADHD
therefore deals with the clinical
treatment response to currently available interventions in ADHD [7], further reflecting the classification of ADHD.
inadequacies of the existing diagnostic framework. It is increasingly accepted that the current Prefrontal cortex (PFC): the front
conceptualization of ADHD reflects a constellation of related, but distinct, functional deficits part of the frontal lobe of the brain;
comprises many subregions
(e.g., [8]). Therefore, a more granular classification of the disorder based on known neurobio-
including the frontal eye field, the
logical circuits would result in greater diagnostic accuracy and, most importantly, in more supplementary eye field, dorsolateral
targeted treatments. PFC, and anterior cingulate cortex.
Saliency map: a neural
representation of the environment
Genetic approaches also hold promise in providing insights into the heterogeneity of ADHD.
that incorporates different types of
Recent reviews [9,10] have identified numerous candidate genes in the dopaminergic, adren- information (e.g., color, contrast) into
ergic, serotonergic, and cholinergic pathways, demonstrating potentially dissociable pathways a global measure of conspicuity.
of risk. Intriguingly, they also suggest an association between ADHD and genes involved in Visuospatial neglect: a neurological
condition which presents as a deficit
general synaptic function (e.g., SNAP-25 and VAMP-2, members of the SNARE complex
in attention to one region of space,
involved in endocytosis of synaptic vesicles). Future studies along these lines may ultimately without an apparent deficit in
lead to the identification of risk genes, again allowing targeted treatment for a subpopulation of sensation for that region.
ADHD individuals. A better suite of tools to identify and classify ADHD is clearly needed, and this
requires a better understanding of the neural mechanisms underlying this complex disorder.
Drawing on advances in human and nonhuman primate research on the neural mechanisms of
attention may provide important insights for updating the current ADHD clinical paradigm.

Cognitive Dysfunction in ADHD


Many studies show that individuals with ADHD exhibit poorer performance on cognitive tasks
compared to neurotypical controls (reviewed in [11]). Meta-analyses indicate that populations
of individuals with ADHD exhibit relatively consistent deficits in specific cognitive functions [12].
However, this is countered by substantial heterogeneity among the identified domains, leading
to variable characterizations of ADHD as a predominant disorder of inhibition (e.g., [13]),
delay aversion [14], or temporal processing [15], among others (e.g., [16]). The lack of a
framework linking current DSM criteria to underlying neurobiological constructs reflects the
complexity of the disorder. A better understanding of which specific cognitive domains are
impaired in an individual increases the likelihood of identifying which specific neural circuits are

Trends in Cognitive Sciences, June 2017, Vol. 21, No. 6 475


ADHD Diagnos eria DSM-5 RDoC
construct implicated in ADHD

Ina phenotype
ails to give close a kes careless
mistak .
ouble holding a
ten when spoken to directly.
A e on Sustained
ollow thr ails to finish
schoolwork, chor Complex
ouble organizing t .
voids, dislikes, or is reluctant to tasks that require
a e on
mental effo
Response
or t precision [22]
acted.
or
rol/
flexibility [23]
Hyper ty/impulsivity phenotype
aps hands or feet, or squirms in seat.
ves seat i remaining seated is Working
expected. memory [24]
e it is not Working
appropiate. Execu ve memory Temporal
y or take part in leisur .
driven by a motor.’ n info
alks excessively. processing [15]
fore a que on has been
completed. Response
rol/
errupts or intrudes on others. re ]

n
Example task Example experimental results
implicated in ADHD
eceptors in monkey PFC causes
Dich listening e changes in visual cortex [47]. A maps exist
n [104] in human occipital, parietal and fronta .

Sustained Noradrenergic neurons are vated by a a


Co nuous
performance [2] vigilance task in monkeys [51].
ventra te tained a
Spiking variability in monkey premotor cortex predicits r
Response refrontal cortex increases
Re [105]
precision re variability [54].

Wisconsin card va key PFC impair xibility in a


[107] dose-dependent fashion [56]. More and less abstra
flexibility switches are represented in different parts of human PFC [57].
Correlates of working memory in monkey PFC are dopamine
Working Delayed saccade [108] related with PFC
memory ormance in humans [109].
Temporal Dura on ons in monkey LIP corr
info [110] measur ral
processing info rocessing [111].
Neurons in monk
Response Stop task [52] movements [65]. Dopamine r
n striatum is correlated with r .

Figure 1. Relationship of Functional Domains Implicated in ADHD to Clinical Diagnostic Criteria and Basic Research
Findings. The central column lists functional domains implicated in ADHD, referenced with a representative study. Each
functional domain has an associated example task and example findings from human and nonhuman primate literature
that yield insight into the underlying neural circuitry. To the left of the central column we show how these functional domains
would map onto RDoC constructs, DSM-5 cognitive domains; as well as the ADHD diagnostic criteria which would fall
under both complex attention and executive function. Abbreviations: DSM-5, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders; LIP, lateral intraparietal cortex; PFC, prefrontal cortex; PP, posterior parietal cortex; RDoC, research domain
criteria; SEF, supplementary eye field. (See [2,13,15,19–24,43,45,47,51,52,54,56,57,63,65,104–112].)

476 Trends in Cognitive Sciences, June 2017, Vol. 21, No. 6


Box 1. ADHD Cognitive Batteries
Several studies have attempted to find more sensitive measures than the continuous performance test and which also
have better predictive power. Attempts have been made to use cognitive batteries to ascertain what variables are the
best predictors of ADHD (e.g., [12]). One study [16] performed a cognitive test battery on adults with ADHD and was
ultimately able ‘to confirm that adult ADHD is neuropsychologically heterogeneous’. Another study [5] attempted to
identify neuropsychological subtypes in individuals with and without ADHD and found several, related, groups that could
be derived from typically-developing groups. Another study [17] used a cognitive test battery to predict which children
would and would not respond to methylphenidate, and they identified several subgroups that might be good
candidates. Notably, there are also cognitive function-independent attempts to subcategorize ADHD populations.
For example, one study [90] proposes the use of temperament dimensions which could be tested for using physiological
measurements.

compromised, which has substantial clinical implications. With this in mind, we surveyed the
literature of cognitive behaviors implicated in the diagnostic criteria for ADHD, placing particular
emphasis on studies that tested the performance of individuals with ADHD on batteries of
cognitive behaviors (Box 1: ADHD Cognitive Batteries). We identified seven functional domains
implicated in ADHD (Figure 1) that were described in the majority of several recent cognitive
batteries [5,12,16,17], with an eye toward connecting cognitive domains to functions studied in
animal models. Specific psychophysical tasks have been designed to measure the ability of an
individual in each of the identified cognitive domains. Experiments in humans and monkeys,
using variations on these tasks, have been informative in suggesting potential underlying
circuits (Figure 1, bottom). We define these functional domains as follows.

Selective Attention. The preferential processing of one stimulus in the presence of other stimuli
(distractors).
Sustained Attention. The ability to continuously perform a task over a prolonged period (e.g.,
minutes) without significant loss in performance (note that we distinguish this from ‘vigilance’,
which can imply sustained attention that is specific to threats or dangers [18]).
Response Precision. Temporal and/or spatial precision in behavioral responses to stimuli or
relevant events. Reaction-time variability is a measure of temporal response precision.
Cognitive Flexibility. The ability to switch between tasks without significant loss of performance.
Working Memory. The ability to preserve a representation of information over time. Most
neurophysiological or neuroimaging studies of working memory probe retention of information
for periods on the order of seconds (e.g., [19]).
Temporal Information Processing. The ability to accurately recognize or reproduce time
intervals.
Response Inhibition. The suppression of actions that are inappropriate for a given task.

Deficits in each of these domains have been described for individuals with ADHD (e.g., selective
attention [20], sustained attention [21], response precision [22], cognitive flexibility [23], working
memory [24], temporal information processing [15], and response inhibition [13]). While a
review of the extensive literature on all of these cognitive domains is beyond the scope of this
paper, we highlight recent research in one of these areas, selective attention, to better illustrate
the potential alignment between human and animal research.

Neural Mechanisms of Selective Attention


Selective attention is one cognitive domain implicated in ADHD (Box 2: Selective Attention Deficits
in ADHD) for which there is significant insight into the underlying neural mechanisms. The term
‘attention’ is often used as shorthand for ‘selective attention’, which is the selective processing of
specific stimuli over others (e.g., [25]). This is distinct from ‘sustained attention’, which refers more
specifically to the maintenance of attention over a longer period of time, and may depend on the
level of arousal [18]. Selective attention can be directed to a specific modality (e.g., auditory, visual)

Trends in Cognitive Sciences, June 2017, Vol. 21, No. 6 477


Box 2. Selective Attention Deficits in ADHD
Numerous studies have compared selective attention in children diagnosed with ADHD to non-ADHD subjects (e.g.,
[91]). Interestingly, these studies have produced conflicting results, either yielding no clear impairments (e.g., [92,93]), or
relatively robust ones (e.g., [94–101]). However, it is thought that these contradictory results are in large part attributable
to the immense diversity in the sensitivity and validity of behavioral assessments used in these studies [91]. Another
potential source of the discrepancy of results may be variability in performance because of age [102]. Nevertheless,
there appears to be ample evidence of an impairment in the filtering of distractors by children with ADHD compared to
those without (e.g., [95,97,101]).

at the expense of others, to a specific region in space (spatial attention), or to a specific feature
(feature-based attention). Attention also operates endogenously and/or exogenously. Figure 2
depicts example tasks that are specific to each of these categories. Endogenously driven
attention, sometimes called top-down, involves selection based on current goals (e.g., search
for lost keys). Exogenously driven attention, sometimes called bottom-up, causes a stimulus to be
selected owing to its greater physical salience (e.g., bright things and moving things). For a child
with ADHD, an endogenous selective attention impairment might manifest as difficulty in attending
to the specific voice of a teacher against the background noise of other children chatting.
Alternatively, that child might be more sensitive to salient, but irrelevant, sensory information,
such as car horns honking outside the classroom, owing to heightened exogenous attention.

The use of specific tasks (Figure 2) that isolate different dimensions of selective attention
(spatial vs feature-based; endogenous vs exogenous) has facilitated progress in identifying
the distinct neural mechanisms and circuitry underlying those dimensions, as well as the
mechanisms they likely have in common (Figure 3). For example, in the visual modality,
evidence from neurophysiological studies in nonhuman primates indicates that endogenous
spatial attention appears to be controlled by a trio of structures including the frontal eye field
(FEF) in prefrontal cortex (PFC) (e.g., [26]), the lateral intraparietal area (LIP) in parietal cortex
[27], and the superior colliculus (SC) (e.g., [28]). Evidence to date suggests that one or more of
these structures drives the selection of attended stimuli within the posterior visual cortex ([29]
for review). However, much less is known about the mechanisms driving (endogenous)

Feature-based

Judge orienta Find red horizontal bar

Endogenous
(top-down)

Exogenous

Figure 2. Taxonomy of Selective Attention. Varieties of selective attention in the visual modality. Selective processing of
visual stimuli can occur either endogenously, in which particular goals, rules, or motivations determine which of multiple
stimuli is selectively processed, or exogenously, in which salient, external events determine selection (top and bottom
rows). Selection can also occur across spatial and feature domains; stimuli can be selected based on their location or on
their component visual features (e.g., color or shape). The examples shown depict visual displays that require a subject to
attend to a particular location (top left) or to a particular object (top right) during endogenous attention, or in which attention
is exogenously drawn to a location (flashed white circle, bottom left) or to a unique object (red bar among green, bottom
right).

478 Trends in Cognitive Sciences, June 2017, Vol. 21, No. 6


LIP

Prefrontal
Higher visual cortex
cortex

TPJ
Superior
colliculus
IFJ

Key: IT
Exogenous

Endogenous
(top-down)
Feature-based

Figure 3. Selective Attention Networks in the Primate Brain. This combines results from human and nonhuman primates.
Blue arrows indicate the flow of endogenous spatial information. Signals from the superior colliculus reach higher visual
cortex by way of the thalamus (broken line). Red arrows denote exogenous signals. Green arrows denote feature-based
signals. Abbreviations: IFJ, inferior frontal junction; IT, inferotemporal cortex; LIP, lateral intraparietal cortex (in humans this
is more generally referred to as middle intraparietal sulcus); TPJ, temporoparietal junction.

feature-based selective attention. Although there is a rich literature describing the modulation
of neural activity during feature-based attention (e.g., [30]), only recently has evidence
emerged of a causal role of distinct subregions of PFC in its control [31].

Compared to endogenous attention, considerably less is known about how attention is drawn
to the physical, non-task-driven salience of particular stimuli. Although it is clear that salient
stimuli preferentially activate neurons in the visual system (e.g., V1 [32], V4 [33]), the basis of
these effects remains unclear. It has been proposed that exogenous and endogenous
attention mechanisms are combined in area LIP [34], the SC [35], and the FEF (e.g., [31])
to generate a saliency map [34]. This and other evidence (e.g., [36]) suggest that exogenous
and endogenous attention are independent systems acting on the same substrate (i.e., visual
cortex).

Parallel studies in humans also point to separable endogenous and exogenous attention
systems [37]: specifically, the dorsal and ventral attention networks. fMRI studies show that
the human homologs of areas FEF and LIP (intraparietal sulcus in humans) are activated during
endogenous spatial and feature-based attention tasks [37,38], and interference with these
areas using transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) impairs performance on said tasks (e.g.,
[39]). fMRI studies have also identified unique regions that are activated during exogenous
attention tasks [40], specifically within the temporal–parietal junction (TPJ). TMS of the TPJ
resulted in visuospatial neglect [41]. Monkey homologs of the human ventral (exogenous)
attention network have not yet been established [42].

As in nonhuman primate experiments, there is also evidence that exogenous and endoge-
nous signals are combined at some stages in the human brain. For example, one study [43]
showed that TMS of putative dorsal network parietal cortex caused changes in fMRI-
recorded activations of a ventral network site. Further, a recent study of lesions in humans
[44] provides evidence that the middle frontal gyrus is an area where endogenous and

Trends in Cognitive Sciences, June 2017, Vol. 21, No. 6 479


exogenous signals converge, specifically indicating it in switching attention from exogenous
to endogenous control. Studies involving human subjects thus suggest that separate endog-
enous and exogenous attention networks exist, but that their signals are likely combined
either in parietal cortex or the PFC, perhaps as a priority map (reviewed in e.g., [45]).

Current evidence from animal studies is consistent with cholinergic signaling contributing
preferentially to exogenous attention [46], whereas dopamine may preferentially contribute
to endogenous attention [47]. Regarding the latter, changes in dopaminergic signaling have
long been implicated in ADHD ([48], but cf [49]), and many of the medications currently
prescribed act on dopamine release and re-uptake. A large body of research has explored
the effect of stimulants, which act on catecholamine pathways, on ADHD symptoms
(reviewed in e.g., [50]).

Selective attention circuits are complex, but the evidence described above demonstrates our
ability to understand them using animal models and human imaging and stimulation techni-
ques. This progress in identifying the components of exogenous and endogenous attention
and the underlying circuit mechanisms, using both human and animal models, demonstrates
the feasibility of such an approach for further understanding cognitive behavioral processes as
they relate to human psychopathology. This has the potential to fill important gaps in current
knowledge of these circuits in ADHD.

Neural Mechanisms of Other Cognitive Domains Associated with ADHD


Figure 4 summarizes the extensive literature describing the relationship between ADHD-related
functional domains and the neural and neuromodulatory circuits that steer them. In addition to
selective attention, described above, we provide a brief overview of current understanding of
these other cognitive domains.

Sustained Attention
Sustained attention may be at least partially mediated by the arousal system, and gated by
norepinephrine neurons in the locus coeruleus (reviewed in [18]). One study [51] found that
locus coeruleus neurons are selectively activated in a sustained attention task in monkeys.
Another study [52] also showed that lesioning noradrenergic output from the locus coeruleus
caused deficits in rats performing a sustained attention task. The cholinergic system has also
been implicated in sustained attention, but the results are less conclusive ([18] for review).

Response Precision
An increase in response-time variability is a hallmark of ADHD (e.g., [22]), and many sources
could contribute to it. An increase in variability is also apparent in the spatial domain: individuals
with ADHD exhibit more-variable movements (e.g., [53]). Imaging studies in humans most
consistently link response precision with abnormalities in PFC volumes (e.g., [54]) and acti-
vations (e.g., [55]).

Cognitive Flexibility
Goal-related information used in cognitive flexibility tasks is primarily represented in the PFC in
monkeys (e.g., [56]) and humans [57]. Cognitive flexibility may also be tied to the neuro-
modulator acetylcholine. Cholinergic neurons may mediate the inhibition of a previously learned
strategy encoded by PFC neurons (reviewed in [58]). Cholinergic signaling in the monkey
amygdala may also play a role in this switch [59]. Alternatively, one study [60] proposes that,
again, dopamine can steer cognitive flexibility, although in this case acting selectively through
D2 dopamine receptors. It has also been shown that depleting serotonin from one region of
marmoset PFC did not affect attentional set-shifting [61].

480 Trends in Cognitive Sciences, June 2017, Vol. 21, No. 6


Basal Parietal Cerebellum
ve ganglia cortex
n

Sustained Sensory
n cortex

Response
precision Prefrontal cortex
Amygdala
ACC SEF
flexibility

Working Serotonin
memory FEF
dlPFC
Temporal
info
processing

Response
n
Dopamine

Acetylcholine
Norepinephrine

Figure 4. Summary of the Interaction of Neuromodulators and Brain Regions Important for Specific Functional Domains.
The diagram shows neuromodulatory input to brain structures implicated in one or more functional domains as well as
some of the major connections. The neuromodulators serotonin, dopamine, norepinephrine, and acetylcholine are
primarily released by specific subcortical nuclei: serotonergic neurons are located in the dorsal raphe nuclei, dopamine
neurons that project to the PFC in the ventral tegmental area, norepinephrine-releasing neurons in the locus coeruleus, and
cholinergic neurons in the nucleus basalis. Abbreviations: ACC, anterior cingulate cortex; dlPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal
cortex; FEF, frontal eye field; PRC, prefrontal cortex; SEF, supplementary eye field.

Working Memory
Numerous studies, using a wide range of techniques, have demonstrated the importance of the
PFC in working memory (reviewed in [62]). As is the case with selective attention, dopamine D1
receptors also appear to modulate visuospatial working memory-related activity in the monkey
PFC (e.g., [19]), suggesting that both functions may be mediated by similar neuromodulatory
mechanisms despite non-overlapping networks controlling those functions.

Temporal Information Processing


Whether specific neuromodulator systems are involved in temporal information processing
remains to be determined, but regions such as the PFC, the cerebellum, and the basal ganglia
all appear to be involved (reviewed in [15]). Interestingly, a recent study by [63] showed that
neurons in monkey area LIP exhibit activity that is correlated with temporal information.

Response Inhibition
Signaling in the PFC (particularly the inferior frontal cortex and the ventrolateral PFC) and the basal
ganglia is associated with response inhibition (reviewed in [64]). In monkeys, oculomotor response
inhibition may be regulated by a specific PFC area, the supplementary eye field, that can inhibit the
initiation of eye movements [65]. Several studies in rodents have implicated serotonin in response

Trends in Cognitive Sciences, June 2017, Vol. 21, No. 6 481


inhibition (reviewed in [66]). Serotonergic neurons are active while rats wait for delayed rewards,
and blocking serotonin neuron activity results in premature responses [67].

This review has focused on high-level cognitive phenotypes of ADHD and their underlying
circuits, but there are an increasing number of studies that suggest ADHD could be a disorder
of motivation and the reward system (reviewed in e.g., [68]). One study [69] proposes that
different subpopulations with ADHD exist with distinguishable symptoms, cognitive and physi-
ological profiles, which are caused by deficits in either the modulation of cortical control centers
or reward circuits. Recent studies in nonhuman primates (i) identified the orbitofrontal cortex as
a region that processes reward but not working memory-related information [70], (ii) demon-
strated reward signals integrate with action signals in at least one region of the PFC (dorsal
anterior cingulate cortex) [71], and (iii) showed that administration of methylphenidate affected
the temporal discounting of rewards [72].

Current DSM-5 criteria do not differentiate between cognitive domain phenotypes, nor is
that its purpose as a diagnostic instrument. However, this inadvertently results in imprecise
amalgamation of underlying mechanisms. For example, ‘Often fails to give close attention to
details or makes careless mistakes in schoolwork, at work, or with other activities', could
represent a working memory, or selective or sustained attention deficit. Our current lim-
itations in identifying affected cognitive domains has significant real-world consequences,
including increased exposure to medications without a biological basis for improving an
affected domain (and associated behavioral symptoms), and increased duration of
experiencing symptoms before treatment with an effective therapy. This problem has been
somewhat elided in current practice, given that stimulants, the current cornerstone of ADHD
therapy, act broadly and non-specifically on dopamine and norepinephrine reuptake
throughout the brain [73], effectively reducing symptoms for a large proportion of individuals
with ADHD. However, future development of drugs with more-specific therapeutic targets
could reduce side effects and curtail unrecognized negative consequences on neurodevel-
opment caused by the long-term non-specific increase in synaptic catecholamines. Data on
long-term adverse effects for alternative ADHD medications (such as atomoxetine, clonidine,
and buproprion) are equally lacking, additionally highlighting the need for resolving the
heterogeneity associated with ADHD. With adequate tools, we anticipate separation of
ADHD cases into more meaningful groups, and this may facilitate the development of more-
selective treatments.

Bridging the Gap


Utility of Nonhuman Primate Models
A fuller understanding of the neural circuits and neuromodulatory systems that underlie the
cognitive diversity of individuals with ADHD will be key to improving their diagnosis and
treatment. Indeed, recent research has shown that these systems are highly complex, and
the ADHD phenotype cannot adequately be characterized as a dopaminergic deficit as
previously thought. Instead, much more sophisticated and direct interrogation of these neuro-
modulatory circuits is needed. Indeed, it should be pointed out that ADHD is not unique in this
regard. Many psychiatric disorders present with similar heterogeneity that arises from a
complexity of neurological dysfunction that is not clearly directly related to current treatments
(e.g., schizophrenia [74]).

While mechanistic hypotheses have been proposed for the behavioral features of ADHD (e.g.,
[75]), empirical research validating these frameworks remains elusive. Experimental disruption
of distinct neuropsychological circuits to determine the associated behavioral deficit may
provide significant insights into ADHD taxonomy, particularly into the ways that primary

482 Trends in Cognitive Sciences, June 2017, Vol. 21, No. 6


deficits converge (or diverge) on downstream behavioral deficits. Unfortunately, our ability to
tackle these questions in humans is limited, particularly in pediatric populations where causal
techniques such as TMS are not routinely used. At present, nonhuman primates represent
excellent models for better understanding the neural basis of ADHD. With nonhuman
primates we can, for example, interrogate how catecholaminergic circuits in the PFC con-
tribute to ADHD-linked behaviors (e.g., [19,47]) and explore the specific effect of ADHD
medication on said circuits (e.g., [76]). The perceptual system of the monkey closely matches
our own (reviewed in [77]), and the same cognitive tasks employed in human psychophysical
studies can be used in neurophysiological studies with nonhuman primates (e.g., [78]). Finally,
because we have greater access to the macaque brain than we do the human, we can study
the neural mechanisms at play in greater detail. Although the majority of the nonhuman
primate studies covered in this review were performed in macaque, the marmoset also
represents an excellent model organism for interrogating visuocognitive neural circuits [79].
As with the macaque, its visual system closely corresponds to humans and it is capable of
performing complex cognitive tasks (e.g., [61]). The marmoset also breeds quickly and can
more readily be genetically manipulated [80].

Utility of a Common Framework


We urge clinical and basic researchers to use the same tasks in their subjects, allowing us to
leverage what we learn in one species for understanding the other. It is critical to know how
drugs that are currently being prescribed to treat ADHD affect the machinery that contrib-
utes to different cognitive behaviors. Unfortunately, experiments probing how ADHD drugs
affect animals performing these behaviors have not been exhaustive (Figure 5). While the
primary effects of first-line medications such as stimulants and atomoxetine are known, the
specific mechanisms by which their upregulation of catecholamines results in cognitive
effects remain unclear. This crucial knowledge gap also exists for other commonly pre-
scribed ADHD medications, such as a2 agonists and bupropion, and can be elucidated with
greater precision in nonhuman animal models. In addition, neurobiological mechanisms for
other emerging treatment options, such as behavior therapy and cognitive training, have yet
to be rigorously investigated. Behavior therapy, a program of behavioral correction usually
guided by a clinician, has shown some promise in addressing ADHD symptoms [81], but
there are very few longitudinal data on the effectiveness of this approach. Preliminary
evidence in macaques shows that behavioral therapy using neurofeedback can modify
cortical circuits and improve behavior [82]. Several studies show that cognitive training
(regular practice of specific cognitive tasks) improves ADHD symptoms, but very few use
quantitative measures of cognitive performance, and their results are inconclusive (reviewed
in e.g., [83]). Training individuals with ADHD on one specific cognitive domain (e.g., working
memory) has been found to result in improved performance on the trained and similar tasks
[84–86]. However, while some studies [84,85] show subsequent improvement in ADHD
symptoms, others do not (e.g., [86], reviewed in e.g., [87]). Two recent studies [88,89]
showed that training in one cognitive domain does not transfer to other domains, therefore
one possibility for the lack of general ADHD symptom improvement is that individuals with
ADHD have deficits in more than one domain, or, again, that subgroups of individuals with
ADHD who specifically exhibit working memory deficits should be targeted for this type of
intervention. Finally, the ADHD literature to date is largely limited by the exclusively symp-
tom-based characterization of the disorder. Reduced reliance on the clinical diagnosis, and
more widespread use of well-established cognitive domains to characterize ADHD deficits in
future research, will allow greater capitalization on neurobiological advances and cross-
species research.

The results of the studies that have been performed so far lend support to the use of nonhuman
primates as models for understanding this disorder. They appear to react to the drugs in similar

Trends in Cognitive Sciences, June 2017, Vol. 21, No. 6 483


Alt n
n
type and class
Amphetamine Methylphenidate Atomox Guanfacine Bupropion

Promotes release and


Inhibits reuptake of Inhibits reuptake of
Mechanisms of inhibits reuptake of
dopamine and
pres vates dopamine and
dopamine
norepinephrine
norepinephrine α2 receptors norepinephrine
n and norepinephrine transporter

ve
n
† † †
[113] [114 ] [115 ] [116 ] [117]

Sustained
n
‡ ‡ † † †
[118] [119 ] [120, 118] [121 , 122 ] [120] [123 ] [124, 125] [126] [127] [128 ]

Response
precision † † † ‡
[129] [130 ] [131] [76] [132 ] [131] [133 ] [134, 135 ]

flexibility † † † † †
[136] [137] [138] [139] [137] [140 , 141 ] [142] [142] [140 ] [143] [144, 145 ]

Working
memory
* * * * † † * † † ‡ †
[136] [146 ] [147] [148 , 149 ] [76, 108 ] [140 , 121 ] [108 ] [150 , 140 ] [151, 152] [126] [117, 144, 135 ] [12 ]

Temporal
info
processing **
[153 , 154] [146]

[155 ]

[156 ]

Response
n ‡
* †
[157, 118, 158] [159, 160] [120,118] [76 ] [121 ] [120] [161, 162,150 ] [163, 164] [134] [159]

Key:
Dose-dependent
†ADHD ‡ADHD *
Rat Monkey Human and neurotypical ** ,
not repro n

Improved Impaired No effect n

Figure 5. Summary of Effects of Drugs on Different Functional Domains Implicated in ADHD Across Several Different Species: Rodent, Monkey and Human. Drugs to
treat ADHD come in different classes: typical examples of each of the types, as well as a superficial description of their method of action, are provided. Green indicates
an improvement in behavior, red a worsening in behavior, and black no change in behavior. Split colors indicate cases where more than one effect was observed. In
several cases different outcomes were observed to be caused by drug dosage – for example a small dose might cause an improvement in working memory, and large
dose a deficit. These studies are denoted by an asterisk (*). Note that clonidine and guanfacine are both a2 receptor agonists but can have very different effects [103].
Human studies were either performed in neurotypical populations, ADHD populationsy, or mixed populationsz. (See [12,76,108,113–164].)

ways to humans and, in general, there is very good correspondence in the behavioral response
to drug manipulations across different species. However, there are still many gaps in the
literature that it will be crucial to fill. First we must identify, where not yet known, whether these
drugs affect specific behaviors. Then we can begin to explore the mechanisms through which
medications affect cognition so we can deploy them more strategically.

484 Trends in Cognitive Sciences, June 2017, Vol. 21, No. 6


Concluding Remarks Outstanding Questions
Many challenges currently face research and clinical paradigms for quantifying and specifying To what extent are different popula-
the symptoms and forms of ADHD (see Outstanding Questions). Use of more rigorous cognitive tions of ADHD pathologies separable
using psychophysical tasks? Are there
tests will help to characterize ADHD deficits, and the study of circuit-level mechanisms psychophysical signatures of subtypes
underlying executive functions in nonhuman primates holds promise for advancing our under- within this broadly defined disorder?
standing, and ultimately the treatment, of ADHD.
Changes in which neural circuits con-
References tribute to the deficits in response pre-
1. Polanczyk, G. et al. (2007) The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: 21. Egeland, J. et al. (2009) Differentiating between ADHD sub- cision that are a hallmark of ADHD?
a systematic review and metaregression analysis. Am. J. Psy- types on CCPT measures of sustained attention and vigilance.
chiatry 164, 942–948 Scand. J. Psychol. 50, 347–354
How do non-standard medications
2. Conners, C.K. et al. (2000) Conners’ Continuous Performance 22. Kofler, M.J. et al. (2013) Reaction time variability in ADHD: a
affect temporal information processing
Test II (CPT II V.5), Multi-Health Systems meta-analytic review of 319 studies. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 33,
795–811 in individuals with ADHD and neuro-
3. Maoz, H. et al. (2015) Association between continuous perfor-
mance and response inhibition tests in adults with ADHD. J. 23. Borella, E. et al. (2013) Beyond interference control impairment
typical controls?
Atten. Disord. Published online April 28, 2015. http://dx.doi.org/ in ADHD: evidence from increased intraindividual variability in the
10.1177/1087054715584056 color-Stroop test. Child Neuropsychol. 19, 495–515 How do ADHD medications affect
4. Egeland, J. and Kovalik-Gran, I. (2010) Measuring several 24. Kennedy, R.J. et al. (2016) Comparison of two measures of monkeys performing different cogni-
aspects of attention in one test: the factor structure of Conners’s working memory impairments in 220 adolescents and adults
tive tasks?
continuous performance test. J. Atten. Disord. 13, 339–346 with ADHD. J. Atten. Disord. Published online August 6, 2016.
5. Fair, D.A. et al. (2012) Distinct neuropsychological subgroups in http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1087054716661232
typically developing youth inform heterogeneity in children with 25. Kastner, S. (2004) A neural basis for human visual attention. In Relatedly, how do ADHD medications
ADHD. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 109, 6769–6774 The Visual Neurosciences (Chalupa, L.M. and Werner, J.S., change the activity of neurons in
6. Willcutt, E.G. et al. (2012) Validity of DSM-IV attention deficit/ eds), pp. 1514–1523, MIT press regions of the monkey brain that are
hyperactivity disorder symptom dimensions and subtypes. J. 26. Gregoriou, G.G. (2014) Lesions of prefrontal cortex reduce implicated in ADHD in humans?
Abnorm. Psychol. 121, 991–1010 attentional modulation of neuronal responses and synchrony
7. van der Donk, M.L. et al. (2016) Predictors and moderators of in V4. Nat. Neurosci. 17, 1003–1011
treatment outcome in cognitive training for children with ADHD. 27. Wardak, C. et al. (2002) Saccadic target selection deficits after
Is it possible to determine the appro-
J. Atten. Disord. Published online March 7, 2016. http://dx.doi. lateral intraparietal area inactivation in monkeys. J. Neurosci. 22, priate dosage for a medication, partic-
org/10.1177/1087054716632876 9877–9884 ularly those with known inverted-U
8. Roberts, B.A. et al. (2017) Are there executive dysfunction 28. Zénon, A. and Krauzlis, R.J. (2012) Attention deficits without effects on behavior, using read-out
subtypes within ADHD? J. Atten. Disord. 21, 284–293 cortical neuronal deficits. Nature 489, 434–437 from simple psychophysical tests?
9. Gizer, I.R. et al. (2009) Candidate gene studies of ADHD: a 29. Squire, R.F. et al. (2013) Prefrontal contributions to visual selec-
meta-analytic review. Hum. Genet. 126, 51–90 tive attention. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 36, 451–466
Many studies examining the effects of
10. Li, Z. et al. (2014) Molecular genetic studies of ADHD and its 30. Baldauf, D. and Desimone, R. (2014) Neural mechanisms of
a2-adrenergic receptor agonists in
candidate genes: a review. Psychiatry Res. 219, 10–24 object-based attention. Science 344, 424–427
monkeys have used guanfacine,
11. Frazier, T.W. et al. (2004) Meta-analysis of intellectual and 31. Bichot, N.P. et al. (2015) A source for feature-based attention in
neuropsychological test performance in attention-deficit/hyper- the prefrontal cortex. Neuron 88, 832–844
whereas clonidine is more typically
activity disorder. Neuropsychology 18, 543–555 prescribed for humans. Do the two
32. Wang, F. et al. (2015) Modulation of neuronal responses by
12. Willcutt, E.G. et al. (2005) Validity of the executive function theory exogenous attention in macaque primary visual cortex. drugs have similar effects on behavior
of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic J. Neurosci. 35, 13419–13429 in the same individuals? If not, which is
review. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 1336–1346 33. Burrows, B.E. and Moore, T. (2009) Influence and limitations of superior at ameliorating ADHD
13. Barkley, R.A. (1997) Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder and popout in the selection of salient visual stimuli by area V4 symptoms?
the Nature of Self-Control, Guilford Press neurons. J. Neurosci. 29, 15169–15177
14. Sonuga-Barke, E.J. (2003) The dual pathway model of AD/HD: 34. Arcizet, F. et al. (2011) A pure salience response in posterior
How effective is behavioral therapy at
an elaboration of neuro-developmental characteristics. parietal cortex. Cereb. Cortex 21, 2498–2506
Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 27, 593–604 alleviating different cognitive deficits in
35. Trappenberg, T.P. (2001) A model of saccade initiation based on
15. Toplak, M.E. et al. (2006) Temporal information processing in the competitive integration of exogenous and endogenous sig-
ADHD, and what are the neural mech-
ADHD: findings to date and new methods. J. Neurosci. Methods nals in the superior colliculus. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 13, 256–271 anisms underlying it?
151, 15–29 36. Busse, L. et al. (2008) Temporal dynamics of neuronal modula-
16. Mostert, J.C. et al. (2015) Cognitive heterogeneity in adult atten- tion during exogenous and endogenous shifts of visual attention Many of the cognitive domains impli-
tion deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a systematic analysis of in macaque area MT. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 105,
cated in ADHD are steered, at least in
neuropsychological measurements. Eur. Neuropsychopharma- 16380–16385
col. 25, 2062–2074
part, by the PFC. How are PFC circuits
37. Vossel, S. et al. (2014) Dorsal and ventral attention systems:
activated by different tasks, and how
17. Elliott, G.R. et al. (2014) Cognitive testing to identify children with distinct neural circuits but collaborative roles. Neuroscientist 20,
ADHD who do and do not respond to methylphenidate. J. Atten. 150–159 do ADHD medications affect neural
Disord. Published online August 14, 2014. http://dx.doi.org/ 38. Corbetta, M. et al. (2002) Neural systems for visual orienting and signaling in this area during said tasks?
10.1177/1087054714543924 their relationships to spatial working memory.
18. Oken, B.S. et al. (2006) Vigilance, alertness, or sustained atten- J. Cogn. Neurosci. 14, 508–523
tion: physiological basis and measurement. Clin. Neurophysiol. 39. Blankenburg, F. et al. (2010) Studying the role of human parietal
117, 1885–1901 cortex in visuospatial attention with concurrent TMS-fMRI.
19. Vijayraghavan, S. (2007) Inverted-U dopamine D1 receptor Cereb. Cortex 20, 2702–2711
actions on prefrontal neurons engaged in working memory. 40. Kincade, J.M. et al. (2005) An event-related functional magnetic
Nat. Neurosci. 10, 376–384 resonance imaging study of voluntary and stimulus-driven ori-
20. Mason, D.J. et al. (2005) Insights into the control of attentional enting of attention. J. Neurosci. 25, 4593–4604
set in ADHD using the attentional blink paradigm. J. Child. 41. Meister, I.G. et al. (2006) Hemiextinction induced by transcranial
Psychol. Psychiatry 46, 1345–1353 magnetic stimulation over the right temporo-parietal junction.
Neuroscience 142, 119–123

Trends in Cognitive Sciences, June 2017, Vol. 21, No. 6 485


42. Patel, G.H. et al. (2015) Functional evolution of new and 67. Miyazaki, K.W. et al. (2012) Activation of dorsal raphe serotonin
expanded attention networks in humans. neurons is necessary for waiting for delayed rewards.
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 112, 9454–9459 J. Neurosci. 32, 10451–10457
43. Leitão, J. et al. (2015) Concurrent TMS-fMRI reveals interactions 68. Luman, M. et al. (2005) The impact of reinforcement contingen-
between dorsal and ventral attentional systems. J. Neurosci. 35, cies on AD/HD: a review and theoretical appraisal. Clin. Psychol.
11445–11457 Rev. 25, 183–213
44. Japee, S. et al. (2015) A role of right middle frontal gyrus in 69. Sonuga-Barke, E.J. (2002) Psychological heterogeneity in
reorienting of attention: a case study. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 9, ADHD – a dual pathway model of behaviour and cognition.
23 Behav. Brain Res. 130, 29–36
45. Sprague, T.C. and Serences, J.T. (2013) Attention modulates 70. Kennerley, S.W. and Wallis, J.D. (2009) Encoding of reward and
spatial priority maps in the human occipital, parietal and frontal space during a working memory task in the orbitofrontal cortex
cortices. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 1879–1887 and anterior cingulate sulcus. J. Neurophysiol. 102, 3352–3364
46. Knudsen, E.I. (2011) Control from below: the role of a midbrain 71. Hayden, B.Y. and Platt, M.L. (2010) Neurons in anterior cingu-
network in spatial attention. Eur. J. Neurosci. 33, 1961–1972 late cortex multiplex information about reward and action.
47. Noudoost, B. and Moore, T. (2011) Control of visual cortical J. Neurosci. 30, 3339–3346
signals by prefrontal dopamine. Nature 474, 372–375 72. Rajala, A.Z. et al. (2015) Decision-making: effects of methylphe-
48. Swanson, J.M. et al. (2007) Etiologic subtypes of attention- nidate on temporal discounting in non-human primates.
deficit/hyperactivity disorder: brain imaging, molecular genetic J. Neurophysiol. 114, 70–79
and environmental factors and the dopamine hypothesis. 73. Wood, S. et al. (2014) Psychostimulants and cognition: a con-
Neuropsychol. Rev. 17, 39–59 tinuum of behavioral and cognitive activation. Pharmacol. Rev.
49. Gonon, F. (2009) The dopaminergic hypothesis of attention- 66, 193–221
deficit/hyperactivity disorder needs re-examining. 74. Miyamoto, S. et al. (2005) Treatments for schizophrenia: a
Trends Neurosci. 32, 2–8 critical review of pharmacology and mechanisms of action of
50. Arnsten, A.F. (2006) Fundamentals of attention-deficit/hyperac- antipsychotic drugs. Mol. Psychiatry 10, 79–104
tivity disorder: circuits and pathways. J. Clin. Psychiatry. 67 75. Killeen, P.R. et al. (2013) A behavioral neuroenergetics theory of
(Suppl. 8), 7–12 ADHD. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 625–657
51. Aston-Jones, G. et al. (1994) Locus coeruleus neurons in mon- 76. Rajala, A.Z. et al. (2012) Dissociative effects of methylphenidate
key are selectively activated by attended cues in a vigilance task. in nonhuman primates: trade-offs between cognitive and behav-
J. Neurosci. 14, 4467–4480 ioral performance. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 24, 1371–1381
52. Carli, M. et al. (1983) Effects of lesions to ascending noradren- 77. Orban, G.A. et al. (2004) Comparative mapping of higher visual
ergic neurones on performance of a 5-choice serial reaction task areas in monkeys and humans. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 315–324
in rats; implications for theories of dorsal noradrenergic bundle 78. Nakahara, K. et al. (2002) Functional MRI of macaque monkeys
function based on selective attention and arousal. Behav. Brain performing a cognitive set-shifting task. Science 295,
Res. 9, 361–380 1532–1536
53. Papadopoulos, N. et al. (2015) Is there a link between motor 79. Mitchell, J.F. et al. (2014) Active vision in marmosets: a model
performance variability and social-communicative impairment in system for visual neuroscience. J. Neurosci. 34, 1183–1194
children with ADHD-CT: a kinematic study using an upper limb
80. Sasaki, E. et al. (2009) Generation of transgenic non-human
fitts’ aiming task. J. Atten. Disord. 19, 72–77
primates with germline transmission. Nature 459, 523–527
54. Stuss, D.T. et al. (2003) Staying on the job: the frontal lobes
81. Safren, S.A. et al. (2005) Cognitive-behavioral therapy for ADHD
control individual performance variability. Brain 126, 2363–2380
in medication-treated adults with continued symptoms. Behav.
55. Simmonds, D.J. et al. (2007) Functional brain correlates of Res. Ther. 43, 831–842
response time variability in children. Neuropsychologia 45,
82. Schafer, R.J. and Moore, T. (2011) Selective attention from
2147–2157
voluntary control of neurons in prefrontal cortex. Science 332,
56. Kamigaki, T. et al. (2012) Neurodynamics of cognitive set shifting 1568–1571
in monkey frontal cortex and its causal impact on behavioral
83. Rapport, M.D. et al. (2013) Do programs designed to train
flexibility. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 24, 2171–2185
working memory, other executive functions, and attention ben-
57. Kim, C. et al. (2011) Common and distinct mechanisms of efit children with ADHD? A meta-analytic review of cognitive,
cognitive flexibility in prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 31, academic, and behavioral outcomes. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 33,
4771–4779 1237–1252
58. Prado, V.F. et al. (2016) Cholinergic circuits in cognitive flexibility. 84. Klingberg, T. et al. (2002) Training of working memory in children
Neuroscience with ADHD. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 24, 781–791
59. Saez, A. et al. (2015) Abstract context representations in primate 85. Gropper, R.J. et al. (2014) Working memory training in college
amygdala and prefrontal cortex. Neuron 87, 869–881 students with ADHD or LD. J. Atten. Disord. 18, 331–345
60. Puig, M.V. et al. (2015) Editorial: neuromodulation of executive 86. Cortese, S. et al. (2015) Cognitive training for attention-deficit/
circuits. Front. Neural Circuits 9, 58 hyperactivity disorder: meta-analysis of clinical and neuropsy-
61. Clarke, H.F. et al. (2005) Prefrontal serotonin depletion affects chological outcomes from randomized controlled trials.
reversal learning but not attentional set shifting. J. Neurosci. 25, J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 54, 164–174
532–538 87. Ansari, S. (2015) The therapeutic potential of working memory
62. Lara, A.H. and Wallis, J.D. (2015) The role of prefrontal cortex in training for treating mental disorders. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9,
working memory: a mini review. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 9, 173 481
63. Jazayeri, M. and Shadlen, M.N. (2015) A neural mechanism for 88. Liu, Z.X. et al. (2017) Effects of working memory training on
sensing and reproducing a time interval. Curr. Biol. 25, neural correlates of go/nogo response control in adults with
2599–2609 ADHD: a randomized controlled trial. Neuropsychologia 95,
64. Aron, A.R. et al. (2014) Inhibition and the right inferior frontal 54–72
cortex: one decade on. Trends Cogn. Sci. 18, 177–185 89. Dovis, S. et al. (2015) Improving executive functioning in children
65. Stuphorn, V. and Schall, J.D. (2006) Executive control of coun- with ADHD: training multiple executive functions within the con-
termanding saccades by the supplementary eye field. Nat. text of a computer game. A randomized double-blind placebo
Neurosci. 9, 925–931 controlled trial. PLoS One 10, e0121651

66. Miyazaki, K. et al. (2012) The role of serotonin in the regulation of 90. Karalunas, S.L. et al. (2014) Subtyping attention-deficit/hyper-
patience and impulsivity. Mol. Neurobiol. 45, 213–224 activity disorder using temperament dimensions: toward biolog-
ically based nosologic criteria. JAMA Psychiatry 71, 1015–1024

486 Trends in Cognitive Sciences, June 2017, Vol. 21, No. 6


91. Handbook of Disruptive Behavior Disorders. Quay, H.C. and 115. Balthazor, M.J. et al. (1991) The specificity of the effects of
Hogan, A.E., eds), 1999.Springer stimulant medication on classroom learning-related measures
92. Hooks, K. et al. (1994) Sustained and selective attention in boys of cognitive processing for attention deficit disorder children.
with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 19, 35–52
J. Clin. Child Psychol. 23, 69–77 116. Nagashima, M. et al. (2014) Neuropharmacological effect of
93. Dalebout, S.D. et al. (1991) Selective auditory attention and atomoxetine on attention network in children with attention
children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder effects of deficit hyperactivity disorder during oddball paradigms as
repeated measurement with and without methylphenidate. assessed using functional near-infrared spectroscopy.
Lang. Speech Hear. Serv. Sch. 22, 219–227 Neurophotonics 1, 025007

94. Ceci, S.J. and Tishman, J. (1984) Hyperactivity and incidental 117. Tiplady, B. et al. (2005) Selective effects of clonidine and tema-
memory: evidence for attentional diffusion. Child Dev. 55, zepam on attention and memory. J. Psychopharmacol. 19,
2192–2203 259–265

95. Prior, M. et al. (1985) Auditory attentional abilities in hyperactive 118. Andrzejewski, M.E. (2014) The effects of clinically relevant doses
children. J. Child. Psychol. Psychiatry 26, 289–304 of amphetamine and methylphenidate on signal detection and
DRL in rats. Neuropharmacology 79, 634–641
96. Satterfield, J.H. (1994) Preferential neural processing of
attended stimuli in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and 119. Sostek, A.J. et al. (1980) Effects of amphetamine on vigilance
normal boys. Psychophysiology 31, 1–10 performance in normal and hyperactive children. J. Abnorm.
Child Psychol. 8, 491–500
97. Carter, C.S. et al. (1995) Abnormal processing of irrelevant
information in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatry 120. Navarra, R. et al. (2008) Effects of atomoxetine and methylphe-
Res. 56, 59–70 nidate on attention and impulsivity in the 5-choice serial reaction
time test. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 32,
98. Pearson, D.A. et al. (1996) Comparison of sustained and selec-
34–41
tive attention in children who have mental retardation with and
without attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am. J. Ment. 121. Tamminga, H.G. et al. (2016) Effects of methylphenidate on
Retard. 100, 592–607 executive functioning in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder
across the lifespan: a meta-regression analysis. Psychol. Med.
99. Jonkman, L.M. et al. (1997) Event-related potentials and perfor-
46, 1791–1807
mance of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: children and
normal controls in auditory and visual selective attention tasks. 122. Lufi, D. et al. (2015) The effect of methylphenidate on sustained
Biol. Psychiatry 41, 595–611 attention among adolescents with attention-deficit hyperactivity
disorder. Neurocase 21, 802–808
100. Friedman-Hill, S.R. (2010) What does distractibility in ADHD
reveal about mechanisms for top-down attentional control? 123. Ni, H.C. et al. (2016) Atomoxetine could improve intra-individual
Cognition 115, 93–103 variability in drug-naïve adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity
disorder comparably with methylphenidate: a head-to-head
101. Mason, D.J. et al. (2003) Exploring selective attention in ADHD:
randomized clinical trial. J. Psychopharmacol. 30, 459–467
visual search through space and time.
J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 44, 1158–1176 124. Bushnell, P.J. et al. (1997) Detection of visual signals by rats:
effects of chlordiazepoxide and cholinergic and adrenergic
102. Brodeur, D.A. and Pond, M. (2001) The development of selec-
drugs on sustained attention. Psychopharmacology 134,
tive attention in children with attention deficit hyperactivity dis-
230–241
order. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 29, 229–239
125. Sagvolden, T. (2006) The alpha-2a adrenoceptor agonist guan-
103. Jäkälä, P. et al. (1999) Guanfacine, but not clonidine, improves
facine improves sustained attention and reduces overactivity
planning and working memory performance in humans. Neuro-
and impulsiveness in an animal model of attention-deficit/hyper-
psychopharmacology 20, 460–470
activity disorder (ADHD). Behav. Brain Funct. 2, 41
104. Cherry, E.C. (1953) Some experiments on the recognition of
126. Decamp, E. et al. (2011) Effects of the alpha-2 adrenoceptor
speech, with one and with two ears. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 25,
agonist guanfacine on attention and working memory in aged
975–979
non-human primates. Eur. J. Neurosci. 34, 1018–1022
105. Teichner, W.H. (1954) Recent studies of simple reaction time.
127. Coull, J.T. et al. (1995) Clonidine and diazepam have differential
Psychol. Bull. 51, 128–149
effects on tests of attention and learning. Psychopharmacology
106. Marcos, E. et al. (2013) Neural variability in premotor cortex is 120, 322–332
modulated by trial history and predicts behavioral performance.
128. Conners, C.K. et al. (1996) Bupropion hydrochloride in attention
Neuron 78, 249–255
deficit disorder with hyperactivity. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc.
107. Berg, E.A. (1948) A simple objective technique for measuring Psychiatry 35, 1314–1321
flexibility in thinking. J. Gen. Psychol. 39, 15–22
129. Loos, M. et al. (2010) Inhibitory control and response latency
108. Gamo, N.J. et al. (2010) Methylphenidate and atomoxetine differences between C57BL/6J and DBA/2J mice in a go/no-go
enhance prefrontal function through a2-adrenergic and dopa- and 5-choice serial reaction time task and strain-specific
mine D1 receptors. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 49, responsivity to amphetamine. Behav. Brain Res. 214, 216–224
1011–1023
130. Aggarwal, A. and Lillystone, D. (2000) A follow-up pilot study of
109. D’Ardenne, K. et al. (2012) Role of prefrontal cortex and the objective measures in children with attention deficit hyperactivity
midbrain dopamine system in working memory updating. disorder. J. Paediatr. Child Health 36, 134–138
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 109, 19900–19909
131. Jentsch, J.D. et al. (2009) Effects of atomoxetine and methyl-
110. Divenyi, P.L. and Danner, W.F. (1977) Discrimination of time phenidate on performance of a lateralized reaction time task in
intervals marked by brief acoustic pulses of various intensities rats. Psychopharmacology 202, 497–504
and spectra. Percept. Psychophys. 21, 125–142
132. Coghill, D.R. et al. (2014) Effects of methylphenidate on cogni-
111. Vicario, C.M. et al. (2013) Temporal accuracy and variability in tive functions in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/
the left and right posterior parietal cortex. Neuroscience 245, hyperactivity disorder: evidence from a systematic review and a
121–128 meta-analysis. Biol. Psychiatry 76, 603–615
112. Robertson, C.L. et al. (2015) Striatal D1- and D2-type dopamine 133. Bédard, A.C. et al. (2015) Differential impact of methylphenidate
receptors are linked to motor response inhibition in human and atomoxetine on sustained attention in youth with attention-
subjects. J. Neurosci. 35, 5990–5997 deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 56,
113. Grilly, D.M. et al. (1989) Effects of cocaine and d-amphetamine 40–48
on sustained and selective attention in rats. 134. Logemann, H.N. et al. (2013) The effect of noradrenergic atten-
Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 33, 733–739 uation by clonidine on inhibition in the stop signal task.
114. López, J. et al. (2004) Effect of psychostimulants on distinct Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 110, 104–111
attentional parameters in attentional deficit/hyperactivity disor- 135. Bilder, R.M. et al. (2016) Cognitive effects of stimulant, guanfa-
der. Biol. Res. 37, 461–468 cine, and combined treatment in child and adolescent attention-

Trends in Cognitive Sciences, June 2017, Vol. 21, No. 6 487


deficit/hyperactivity disorder. 150. Chamberlain, S.R. (2007) Atomoxetine improved response inhi-
J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 55, 667–673 bition in adults with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
136. Featherstone, R.E. (2008) A sensitizing regimen of amphetamine Biol. Psychiatry 62, 977–984
that disrupts attentional set-shifting does not disrupt working or 151. Marrs, W. et al. (2005) Alpha-2 adrenoceptor activation inhibits
long-term memory. Behav. Brain Res. 189, 170–179 phencyclidine-induced deficits of spatial working memory in
137. Soto, P.L. et al. (2012) Long-term exposure to oral methylphe- rats. Neuropsychopharmacology 30, 1500–1510
nidate or dl-amphetamine mixture in peri-adolescent rhesus 152. Mair, R.D. et al. (2005) Effects of clonidine in the locus coeruleus
monkeys: effects on physiology, behavior, and dopamine sys- on prefrontal-and hippocampal-dependent measures of atten-
tem development. Neuropsychopharmacology 37, 2566–2579 tion and memory in the rat. Psychopharmacology 181, 280–288
138. Advokat, C. (2010) What are the cognitive effects of stimulant 153. Hampson, C.L. et al. (2010) Comparison of the effects of 2,5-
medications? Emphasis on adults with attention-deficit/hyper- dimethoxy-4-iodoamphetamine and d-amphetamine on the
activity disorder (ADHD). Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 34, ability of rats to discriminate the durations and intensities of light
1256–1266 stimuli. Behav. Pharmacol. 21, 11–20
139. Chu, R. et al. (2016) Differentiation of rodent behavioral pheno- 154. Sanchez-Castillo, H. et al. (2015) Subjective and real time:
types and methylphenidate action in sustained and flexible coding under different drug states. Int. J. Comp. Psychol. 28,
attention tasks. Brain Res. 1641, 306–319 26255
140. Yang, L. et al. (2012) Comparative study of OROS-MPH and 155. Wilson, T.W. et al. (2013) Estimating the passage of minutes:
atomoxetine on executive function improvement in ADHD: a deviant oscillatory frontal activity in medicated and unmedicated
randomized controlled trial. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. ADHD. Neuropsychology 27, 654–665
15, 15–26 156. Luman, M. et al. (2015) The unique and combined effects of
141. Dyme, I.Z. et al. (1982) Perseveration induced by methylpheni- reinforcement and methylphenidate on temporal information
date in children: preliminary findings. Prog. Neuropsychophar- processing in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Clin.
macol. Biol. Psychiatry 6, 269–273 Psychopharmacol. 35, 414–421
142. Seu, E. et al. (2009) Inhibition of the norepinephrine transporter 157. van Gaalen, M.M. (2009) Amphetamine decreases behavioral
improves behavioral flexibility in rats and monkeys. inhibition by stimulation of dopamine D2, but not D3, receptors.
Psychopharmacology 202, 505–519 Behav. Pharmacol. 20, 484–491
143. Lapiz, M.D.S. and Morilak, D.A. (2006) Noradrenergic modula- 158. Sagvolden, T. and Xu, T. (2008) L-Amphetamine improves poor
tion of cognitive function in rat medial prefrontal cortex as sustained attention while d-amphetamine reduces overactivity
measured by attentional set shifting capability. Neuroscience and impulsiveness as well as improves sustained attention in an
137, 1039–1049 animal model of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).
144. Choi, Y. et al. (2006) The effect of a-2 adrenergic agonists on Behav. Brain Funct. 4, 3
memory and cognitive flexibility. Cogn. Behav. Neurol. 19, 159. Acheson, A. and de Wit, H. (2008) Bupropion improves attention
204–207 but does not affect impulsive behavior in healthy young adults.
145. Taylor, F.B. and Russo, J. (2001) Comparing guanfacine and Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 16, 113
dextroamphetamine for the treatment of adult attention-deficit/ 160. Allman, A.A. et al. (2010) Effect of d-amphetamine on inhibition
hyperactivity disorder. J. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 21, 223–228 and motor planning as a function of baseline performance.
146. Schulze, G.E. and Paule, M.G. (1990) Acute effects of d- Psychopharmacology 211, 423–433
amphetamine in a monkey operant behavioral test battery. 161. Graf, H. et al. (2011) Neural correlates of error monitoring mod-
Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 35, 759–765 ulated by atomoxetine in healthy volunteers. Biol. Psychiatry 69,
147. Mattay, V.S. et al. (2000) Effects of dextroamphetamine on 890–897
cognitive performance and cortical activation. Neuroimage 162. Nandam, L.S. et al. (2011) Methylphenidate but not atomoxetine
12, 268–275 or citalopram modulates inhibitory control and response time
148. Berridge, C.W. et al. (2006) Methylphenidate preferentially variability. Biol. Psychiatry 69, 902–904
increases catecholamine neurotransmission within the prefron- 163. Kawaura, K. (2014) Stimulation of postsynapse adrenergic a2A
tal cortex at low doses that enhance cognitive function. receptor improves attention/cognition performance in an animal
Biol. Psychiatry 60, 1111–1120 model of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Behav. Brain
149. Arnsten, A.F. and Dudley, A.G. (2005) Methylphenidate Res. 270, 349–356
improves prefrontal cortical cognitive function through alpha2 164. Bari, A. et al. (2011) Prefrontal and monoaminergic contributions
adrenoceptor and dopamine D1 receptor actions: relevance to to stop-signal task performance in rats. J. Neurosci. 31,
therapeutic effects in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. 9254–9263
Behav. Brain Funct. 1, 2

488 Trends in Cognitive Sciences, June 2017, Vol. 21, No. 6

You might also like