You are on page 1of 6

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/40679132

Mother and Father Connectedness and Involvement During Early Adolescence

Article  in  Journal of Family Psychology · December 2009


DOI: 10.1037/a0016438 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS
148 2,640

2 authors:

Randal D. Day Laura M. Padilla-Walker


Brigham Young University - Provo Main Campus Brigham Young University - Provo Main Campus
75 PUBLICATIONS   3,472 CITATIONS    130 PUBLICATIONS   6,094 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Validation of a Multidimensional Measure of Prosocial Behavior View project

Measuring the Three Ethics of Autonomy, Community, and Divinity View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Laura M. Padilla-Walker on 17 March 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Journal of Family Psychology © 2009 American Psychological Association
2009, Vol. 23, No. 6, 900 –904 0893-3200/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0016438

BRIEF REPORTS

Mother and Father Connectedness and Involvement During


Early Adolescence
Randal D. Day and Laura M. Padilla-Walker
Brigham Young University

The purpose of the current study was to explore how mother’s and father’s connectedness and
involvement individually and collectively influence the lives of their children. Specifically,
we asked how fathers’ and mothers’ parent– child connectedness and behavioral involvement
influenced both problem behaviors (externalizing and internalizing behaviors) and positive
outcomes (prosocial behaviors and hope) during early adolescence. Data for this study were
taken from the Flourishing Families Project, from which 349 mothers and fathers were
selected, along with their early adolescent child (mean age ⫽ 11.23 years, SD ⫽ .96).
Hierarchical regression analyses revealed (even after controlling for child age, gender, and
self-regulation) that mothers’ and fathers’ contributions differed, primarily as a function of
child outcome. Namely, father (but not mother) connectedness and involvement were nega-
tively related to adolescents’ internalizing and externalizing behaviors, whereas mother (but
not father) connectedness and involvement were positively related to adolescents’ prosocial
behaviors and hope. We also found that when one parent’s involvement was low (for
whatever reason), the other parent’s involvement made a significant and important contribu-
tion to the child’s well-being, particularly in the area of internalizing behaviors.

Keywords: parenting, fathers, mothers, connectedness, behavioral involvement in early


adolescence

One of the more compelling research stories to emerge strong parent-adolescent bond tend to feel closer to other
within the study of parenting is that parenting behaviors ex- people, utilize others as a resource, and are perceived as
hibited by mothers and fathers independently influence child more approachable (Allen & Land, 1999; Lee, Draper, &
development (Stolz, Barber, & Olsen, 2005). Understanding Lee, 2001). Parental involvement is also key, with research-
the differences in mothering and fathering is especially impor- ers suggesting that parental engagement or involvement is
tant as we seek to understand the challenges of early adoles- central to both fathering and mothering (Lamb, Pleck, &
cence (Collins & Laursen, 2004). The current study shows that Levine, 1985; Pleck & Masciadrelli, 2004). Taken together,
parents matter in the lives of their children, and explores how we would expect adolescents who report higher levels of
fathers’ and mothers’ connectedness and behavioral involve- parental connection and involvement to score higher on
ment differentially influence children during early adolescence. positive and lower on negative behaviors.

Parental Connectedness and Involvement


Differential Influence of Mothers and Fathers
Research on parent– child connectedness has found that
adolescents with higher levels of social connection and a Researchers claim that men and women approach the
parenting of adolescents differently (Steinberg & Silk,
2002), but few studies interview both fathers and mothers in
Randal D. Day and Laura M. Padilla-Walker, School of Life, the same family, with even fewer including children’s
Brigham Young University, Provo, Utah. views. The few available studies report that mothers and
We thank the Family Studies Center at Brigham Young Uni- fathers play both different and combined roles in parenting,
versity (BYU), the School of Family Life, and the College of and that this is often dependent upon the gender of the
Family Home and Social Science at BYU for their generous adolescent. For example, one study found that mothering
support for this project. We also recognize the generous support of
the many private donors who provided support for this project. We
was related to their sons’ antisocial behaviors and depres-
also thank those families who were willing to spend valuable hours sion, while fathering was more important for their daugh-
with our team in interviews. ters’ depression (Stolz et al., 2005). Other studies have
Correspondence should be directed to Randal D. Day, School found that mothering was more important for both boys’
of Life, Brigham Young University, Provo UT 84602. E-mail: and girls’ prosocial behaviors (Hastings, McShane, Parker,
day@byu.edu & Ladha, 2007), whereas fathering has been found to ex-

900
BRIEF REPORTS 901

plain unique variance in behavioral and externalizing prob- ness (sample item, “I feel close to my child”). Parental
lems (Williams & Kelly, 2005). In sum, researchers are involvement was assessed using 8 items from the Inventory
moving away from the general idea of “parenting,” and of Father Involvement (Hawkins et al., 2002). Mothers (␣ ⫽
toward the more refined constructs of mothering and father- .64) and fathers (␣ ⫽ .77) were both assessed, with higher
ing, which are impacted both by the gender of the child, and scores representing higher involvement (sample item, “How
the outcome being examined. often do you help your child with homework?”).
Child self-regulation. Adolescents’ self-regulation was
Research Questions self-reported using a 5-item measure of emotional self-
regulation (Novak & Clayton, 2001). Higher scores repre-
We hypothesized that fathers’ and mothers’ connected- sented greater self-regulation (␣ ⫽ .76; sample item, “I have
ness and behavioral involvement would be significantly a hard time controlling my temper”).
related to their child’s positive and negative outcomes, and Child negative behaviors. Adolescents’ externalizing
we explored whether parental connectedness and involve- and internalizing behaviors were assessed using the Child
ment would differ in their associations with outcome vari- Behavior Checklist (CBCL; Achenbach, 1991). Adolescents
ables as a function of the gender of the parent and the child. responded to 13 items assessing internalizing behaviors
Second, we examined whether one parent’s connectedness (␣ ⫽ .87; e.g., “I feel lonely”) and 9 items assessing
or involvement might act as a buffer against low levels of externalizing behaviors (␣ ⫽ .62; e.g., “I use alcohol or
parenting from the other parent, or whether high levels of drugs”).
parenting from both parents resulted in the most positive Child positive behaviors. Adolescents’ prosocial be-
outcomes. Finally, given research suggesting the impor- haviors were assessed using the Values in Action Inventory
tance the adolescent’s temperament on both positive and of Strengths (Peterson & Seligman, 2004), with higher
negative outcomes (Eisenberg et al., 2005), the current scores representing higher levels of prosocial behaviors
study examined the role of parenting after controlling for (␣ ⫽ .84; sample item, “I help others, even if it is difficult
adolescent temperament (i.e., adolescents’ emotional self- for me”). Adolescents’ self-reported hope was assessed
regulation). using 9 items from the Children’s Hope Scale (Snyder,
2005). Higher scores represented higher levels of hope (␣ ⫽
.79; sample item, “I have hope for my future”).
Method
Participants and Procedure Results
Study participants come from the Flourishing Families Descriptive Statistics and Correlations
Project (FFP). The FFP is an ongoing, longitudinal study
involving families with a child between the ages of 10 and Because outcomes varied as a function of child age and
14 years. Participant families for the FFP were randomly gender, these variables were added as control variables, and
and purposively selected from a large northwestern city and gender interactions were explored. It should be noted that
were interviewed in 2007 (for more information about our connectedness was moderately and positively related to
sample, please contact the first author). Families were in- involvement for both mothers and fathers (mothers’ r ⫽ .38,
terviewed in their homes, with each interview consisting of fathers’ r ⫽ .49), so regressions were conducted separately
a 1-hr video (not reported here) and a 1.5-hr self- for each aspect of parenting. Furthermore, mothering and
administered questionnaire completed by the child, mother, fathering were not highly correlated with one another (con-
and father. Our overall response rate of eligible families was nectedness r ⫽ .20, involvement r ⫽ .21), which minimized
68%. our concern about colinearity.
The final sample consisted of the 349 intact families (151
single-parent families were not used here), which included Hierarchical Regression Analysis of Parenting
the adolescent child (mean age ⫽ 11.23 years, SD ⫽ .96, Predicting Child Outcomes
range ⫽ 11–14), mother (mean age ⫽ 43.44, SD ⫽ 5.54),
and father (mean age ⫽ 45.32, SD ⫽ 6.23). Ninety-five In each regression analysis, the main effects of child age,
percent of mothers and 90% of fathers reported being bio- gender (0 ⫽ boys, 1 ⫽ girls), and self-regulation were
logical parents. Seventy-nine percent of the participants entered in the first step, mother and father connectedness
were European American, and 70% of families reported an (Table 1) or involvement were entered in the second step,
income of more than $50,000 per year. and the three, 2-way interactions were entered in the third
step. None of the 3-way interactions were statistically sig-
Measures nificant and are thus not presented. The scores on the
predictors were centered prior to analyses to reduce colin-
Parent– child connectedness and involvement. Parent– earity formed by the interaction, and interactions were ex-
child connectedness was assessed using 9 items from the amined by calculating simple regression slopes (Aiken &
Social Connectedness Scale (Lee et al., 2001). Mothers West, 1991).
(␣ ⫽ .85) and fathers (␣ ⫽ .88) were both assessed, with Externalizing behaviors. For connectedness, at Step 1
higher scores representing higher parent– child connected- adolescents’ age, gender, and self-regulation accounted for
902 BRIEF REPORTS

a statistically significant proportion of variance, F(3, 332) ⫽

Involvement
17.84, p ⬍ .001, with age positively, and gender and self-

.19ⴱⴱⴱ

.06ⴱⴱⴱ
.04ⴱⴱ

.15ⴱⴱ

.06ⴱⴱ
2.51ⴱⴱ
.02
.09

.00

.02
.02
⫺.02
regulation negatively related to externalizing behaviors (see
Table 1). No other steps resulted in a significant increase in
variance. For involvement, at Step 2 (Step 1 is the same as
Hope

above), mother and father involvement produced a .02 in-


crease in the proportion of variance accounted for, F⌬(2,
Connectedness

.19ⴱⴱⴱ 330) ⫽ 3.33, p ⬍ .05, with father involvement negatively


.04ⴱⴱ

.05ⴱⴱ

.05ⴱ
2.05ⴱ
related to externalizing behaviors (see Table 2). However,
.02
.09

.07
⫺.08

.09
⫺.02
⫺.03
using general linear modeling (GLM) custom hypothesis
testing, it was determined that the coefficient for fathers was
not significantly larger than the coefficient for mothers
(difference ⫽ .16, SE ⫽ .23, p ⫽ .48). Thus, it can be
Involvement

concluded that fathers’ involvement was significantly re-


.29ⴱⴱⴱ
.27ⴱⴱⴱ
.16ⴱⴱⴱ

.18ⴱⴱⴱ

.19ⴱⴱⴱ
10.21ⴱⴱⴱ
.09⫹

lated to children’s externalizing behaviors and mothers’


.12ⴱ

⫺.17ⴱ
⫺.08

.07

.00
Prosocial behaviors

involvement was not, but we cannot conclude that fathering


mattered more than mothering in this regard.
Internalizing behaviors. For connectedness, at Step 1
Hierarchical Regression Analysis of Parent–Child Connectedness and Involvement Predicting Child Outcomes

adolescents’ age, gender, and self-regulation accounted for


Connectedness

a statistically significant proportion of variance, F(3, 332) ⫽


50.80, p ⬍ .001, with self-regulation negatively related to
.29ⴱⴱⴱ
.27ⴱⴱⴱ
.16ⴱⴱⴱ

.17ⴱⴱⴱ

.18ⴱⴱⴱ
.18ⴱⴱⴱ
8.19ⴱⴱⴱ
.12ⴱ
⫺.08

.01

⫺.08
⫺.07

internalizing behaviors. At Step 2, mother and father con-


Child outcomes

nectedness produced a .02 increase in the proportion of


variance accounted for, F⌬(2, 332) ⫽ 4.29, p ⬍ .01, with
Note. All regression values represent standardized beta weights. For child gender, 0 ⫽ boys, 1 ⫽ girls.

father connectedness negatively related to internalizing be-


haviors. Using GLM custom hypothesis testing, it was de-
Involvement

termined that the coefficient for fathers was significantly


⫺.56ⴱⴱⴱ
.31ⴱⴱⴱ

.33ⴱⴱⴱ

.36ⴱⴱⴱ
20.14ⴱⴱⴱ
⫺.14ⴱⴱ

.14ⴱⴱ

larger than the coefficient for mothers (difference ⫽ 1.38,


Internalizing behaviors

.03
.05

.03

.10
⫺.05

SE ⫽ .57, p ⫽ .02). This suggests that fathering mattered


more than mothering in this regard.
For involvement, at Step 2 mother and father involve-
ment produced a .02 increase in the proportion of variance
Connectedness

accounted for, F⌬(2, 330) ⫽ 4.72, p ⬍ .01, with father


⫺.56ⴱⴱⴱ
.31ⴱⴱⴱ

.33ⴱⴱⴱ

.35ⴱⴱⴱ
19.42ⴱⴱⴱ
⫺.14ⴱⴱ

⫺.12⫹
.09⫹

involvement negatively related to internalizing behaviors.


.03
.05

.05

⫺.07

Using GLM custom hypothesis testing, it was determined


that the coefficient for fathers was significantly larger than
the coefficient for mothers (difference ⫽ 1.56, SE ⫽ .73,
p ⫽ .03). This suggests that fathering mattered more than
mothering in this regard. At Step 3, the interactions pro-
Involvement

.21ⴱⴱⴱ

⫺.31ⴱⴱⴱ
.14ⴱⴱⴱ

.16ⴱⴱⴱ

.16ⴱⴱⴱ
7.16ⴱⴱⴱ

duced a .02 increase in the proportion of variance accounted


⫺.16ⴱⴱ

⫺.12ⴱ
Externalizing behaviors

for, F⌬(3, 327) ⫽ 9.49, p ⬍ .01, with the interaction


⫺.04

.06
⫺.02
⫺.04

between mother and father involvement positively related to


p ⬍ .06. ⴱ p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01. ⴱⴱⴱ p ⬍ .001.

internalizing behaviors. After examining simple slopes, this


interaction suggests that when father involvement was high,
there was little difference in internalizing behaviors as a
Connectedness

function of mother involvement. However, when father


.21ⴱⴱⴱ
⫺.31ⴱⴱⴱ
.14ⴱⴱⴱ

.14ⴱⴱⴱ

.16ⴱⴱⴱ
6.95ⴱⴱⴱ
⫺.16ⴱⴱ

involvement was low, high mother involvement resulted in


⫺.04
⫺.09

.05
⫺.01
.04

lower internalizing behaviors. In other words, either parent


was acting as a buffer against low involvement from the
other parent.
Prosocial behaviors. For connectedness, at Step 1 ad-
Child self-regulation

olescents’ age, gender, and self-regulation accounted for a


A ⫻ Child gender
B ⫻ Child gender

statistically significant proportion of variance, F(3, 331) ⫽


20.11, p ⬍ .001, with child gender and self-regulation
Mothering (A)
Fathering (B)
Child gender

positively related to prosocial behaviors. At Step 2, mother


Child age

and father connectedness produced a .01 increase in the


Final R2
A⫻B

proportion of variance accounted for, F⌬(2, 331) ⫽ 2.71,


Table 1

F test
Step 1

Step 2

Step 3

p ⫽ .06, with mother connectedness positively related to


R2

R2

prosocial behaviors. However, using GLM custom hypoth-



BRIEF REPORTS 903

esis testing, it was determined that the coefficient for moth- compensated for low levels of involvement from the other
ers was not significantly larger than the coefficient for parent when they perceived their early adolescent child to
fathers (difference ⫽ .12, SE ⫽ .08, p ⫽ .13). Thus, we be displaying internalizing problems at such an early age.
cannot conclude that mothering mattered more than father- Future research should continue to examine interactions
ing in this regard. Step 3 did not result in a significant between mothering and fathering as adolescents get older,
increase in the proportion of variance accounted for, despite because the lack of significant findings on other outcome
significant parameter estimates. variables may have been because of the age of our sample
For involvement, at Step 2 mother and father involve- and the relatively low level of problem behaviors during
ment produced a .03 increase in the proportion of variance early adolescence.
accounted for, F⌬(2, 329) ⫽ 5.45, p ⬍ .01, with mother The last finding that was of note was that adolescent
involvement positively related to prosocial behaviors. How- temperament was the strongest and most consistent predic-
ever, using GLM custom hypothesis testing, it was deter- tor of adolescent outcomes. Although this relation may be
mined that the coefficient for mothers was not significantly overinflated in the current study because adolescents self-
larger than the coefficient for fathers (difference ⫽ .06, reported both their temperament and the behavioral out-
SE ⫽ .10, p ⫽ .54). Thus, we cannot conclude that moth- comes, findings are consistent with research suggesting that
ering mattered more than fathering in this regard. Step 3 did adolescents’ ability to self regulate is negatively related to
not result in a significant increase in the proportion of negative outcomes and positively related to positive out-
variance accounted for, despite significant parameter esti- comes (Eisenberg et al., 2005). It will be important for
mates. future research to examine not only how parenting matters
Hope. For connectedness, at Step 1 adolescents’ age, after controlling for temperament, but also how different
gender, and self-regulation accounted for a statistically sig- aspects of parenting are differentially important for children
nificant proportion of variance, F(3, 331) ⫽ 4.83, p ⬍ .01, with different temperaments.
with self-regulation positively related to hope. For involve- Although there is a large body of research that suggests
ment, at Step 2 mother and father involvement produced a that mothers and fathers matter in the lives of their children,
.02 increase in the proportion of variance accounted for, there are few studies that examine the contribution of one
F⌬(2, 329) ⫽ 3.80, p ⬍ .05, with mother involvement parent after controlling for the contribution of the other in
positively related to hope. Using GLM custom hypothesis
an attempt to determine unique contribution (Parke, 2002).
testing, it was determined that the coefficient for mothers
The current study is consistent with the small body of
was significantly larger than the coefficient for fathers (dif-
research suggesting that even after considering the redun-
ference ⫽ .16, SE ⫽ .08, p ⫽ .05). This suggests that
dancy in parenting across mothers and fathers, both parents
mothering mattered more than fathering in this regard.
matter in unique ways (Hastings et al., 2007; Williams &
Kelly, 2005), and it adds to this literature by suggesting that
Discussion fathering is particularly salient in protecting against nega-
This study confirmed that both mothers and fathers mat- tive behaviors, whereas mothering is more consistently re-
tered, however, mothering was more consistently related to lated to positive behaviors.
adolescents’ positive behaviors, and fathering was more Our first limitation is that this was a community-based
consistently related to adolescents’ problem behaviors. sample, and larger generalizations cannot be extrapolated.
Mothers’ parenting may be more centered on relationship- The current sample focuses only on intact families and did
building skills whereas fathers’ parenting is more focused not examine parenting of nonresidential fathers, or low-
on norm compliance (Lamb, 2004). Our findings did not income, ethnically diverse families. However, one of the
support the idea that involvement was a “fathering” variable strengths of this sample is that it is somewhat “population
and connectedness was a “mothering” variable. Rather, they normal” in that we did not oversample children or families
suggested that both aspects of parenting were important for in trouble.
both mothers and fathers, but their relations to outcomes Second, we also did not report these findings from the
were consistently different. A key in our findings was that strength of a longitudinal design. To more accurately assess
parental connectedness and involvement in the lives of their the power of mothers’ and fathers’ contribution to the
children matters, with fathering being more important for well-being of children, it is necessary to view those inter-
internalizing behaviors and mothering being more important actions and contributions over time. For example, children
for hope. Although this is not a new idea in the literature, who exhibit higher levels of positive behaviors and lower
most research projects do not directly assess the power of levels of negative behaviors may possibly elicit more con-
different aspects of mothering and fathering with reports nectedness and involvement from parents.
from multiple respondents and with multiple outcomes. Third, this study did not specify what it is that fathers and
We also found that when one parent’s involvement was mothers do differently that may produce different outcomes
low (for whatever reason), the other parent’s involvement in children’s well-being as reported here. Indeed, our find-
made a significant and important contribution to the child’s ings do not suggest that mothers do not matter for negative
well-being, particularly in the area of internalizing behav- behaviors and that fathers do not matter for positive behav-
iors (Stolz et al., 2005). Given our inability to determine iors, only that when controlling for the other parents’ par-
direction of effects, it is possible that one or the other parent enting, mothers parenting was more consistently related to
904 BRIEF REPORTS

positive outcomes and fathering was more consistently re- to make nice: Parental socialization of young sons’ and daugh-
lated to negative outcomes. ters’ prosocial behaviors with peers. Journal of Genetic Psychol-
Fourth, note that there are only modest results connecting ogy, 168, 177–200.
parenting and child outcomes. This may be a reflection of a Hawkins, A. J., Bradford, K. P., Palkovitz, R., Day, R. D., Chris-
tiansen, S. L., & Call, V. C. (2002). The inventory of father
key measurement issue facing family/parenting researchers. involvement: A pilot study of a new measure of father involve-
To date, there is no single instrument that adequately assess ment. Journal of Men’s Studies, 10, 183–196.
mother and father involvement. We have retrofitted a scale Lamb, M. E. (Ed.). (2004). The role of the father in child devel-
here that was initially used to assess father involvement, opment (4th ed.). New York: Wiley.
while other measures were clearly written with mothers in Lamb, M. E., Pleck, J. H., & Levine, J. A. (1985). The role of
mind. This challenge needs to be addressed in future re- father in child development: The effects of increased paternal
search. involvement. In B. B. Hahey & A. E. Kazdin (Eds.), Advances in
In conclusion, although there is a large body of research clinical child psychology (Vol. 8, pp. 229 –266). New York:
that suggests that mothers and fathers matter in the lives of Plenum.
their children, there are few studies that examine the cont- Lee, R. M., Draper, M., & Lee, S. (2001). Social connectedness,
dysfunctional interpersonal behaviors, and psychological dis-
ribution of one parent after controlling for the contribution tress: Testing a mediator model. Journal of Counseling Psychol-
of the other in an attempt to determine unique contribution ogy, 48, 310 –318.
(Parke, 2002). Thus, the current study is consistent with the Novak, S. P., & Clayton, R. R. (2001). The influence of school
small body of research suggesting that even after consider- environment and self-regulation on transitions between stages of
ing the redundancy in parenting across mothers and fathers, cigarette smoking: A multilevel analysis. Health Psychology, 20,
that both parents matter in unique ways, and suggests that 196 –207.
fathering is particularly salient in protecting against nega- Parke, R. D. (2002). Fathers and families. In M. H. Bornstein
tive behaviors, while mothering is more consistently related (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 3: Being and Becoming a
to positive behaviors. This study is an important first step in Parent (2nd ed., pp. 27–73). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
continuing to examine the combined and relative impor- Associates.
Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2004). Character strengths
tance of both mothers and fathers in the lives of their and virtues: A handbook and classification. Washington, DC:
adolescent children. Oxford University Press.
Pleck, J. H., & Masciadrelli, B. P. (2004). Paternal involvement by
References U.S. residential fathers. In M. E. Lamb (Ed.), The role of the
father in child development (pp. 222–271). New York: Wiley.
Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Manual for the youth self-report and Snyder, C. R. (2005). Measuring hope in children. In K. Moore &
1991 profile. Burlington: University of Vermont, Department of D. Lippman (Eds.), What do children need to flourish: Concep-
Psychiatry. tualizing and measuring indicators of positive development (pp.
Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing 61–73). New York: Springer.
and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Steinberg, L., & Silk, J. S. (2002). Parenting adolescents. In M. H.
Allen, J. P., & Land, D. (1999). Attachment in adolescence. In J. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 1: Children and
Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, parenting (2nd ed., pp. 103–133). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erl-
research, and clinical applications (pp. 319 –335). New York: baum Associates.
Guilford Press. Stolz, H. E., Barber, B. K., & Olsen, J. A. (2005). Toward
Collins, W. A., & Laursen, B. (2004). Parent-adolescent relation- disentangling fathering and mothers: An assessment of relative
ships and influences. In R. Lerner & L. Steinberg (Eds.), Hand- importance. Journal of Marriage and Family, 67, 1076 –1092.
book of adolescent psychology (2nd ed., pp. 331–362). New Williams, S. K., & Kelly, F. D. (2005). Relationships among
York: Wiley. involvement, attachment, and behavioral problems in adoles-
Eisenberg, N., Sadovsky, A., Spinrad, T. L., Fabes, R. A., Losoya, cence: Examining father’s influence. Journal of Early Adoles-
S. H., & Valiente, C. (2005). The relations between problem cence, 25, 168 –196.
behavior status to children’s negative emotionality, effortful con-
trol, and impulsivity: Concurrent relations and prediction of Received September 11, 2008
change. Developmental Psychology, 41, 193–211. Revision received March 17, 2009
Hastings, P., McShane, K., Parker, R., & Ladha, F. (2007). Ready Accepted March 30, 2009 䡲

View publication stats

You might also like