You are on page 1of 14

The Attitudes and Practices 257

that Shape Children’s


Drawing Experience at
Home and at School
Esther Burkitt, Richard Jolley and Sarah Rose

Abstract

Concept: Few empirical studies have investi- Results: The findings are discussed in five
gated the influence of teachers, parents and chil- sections. First, the positive perceptions of chil-
dren on children’s drawing experience. The dren’s drawing behaviour and attitudes. Second,
current study aims to examine the attitudes and the perceived importance and principal aims of
practices of these three key players that shape the National Curriculum for Art and Design Educa-
children’s drawing experience. tion. Third, the numerous sources of encourage-
ment and support for drawing development.
Method: A survey methodology was used, as Fourth, the differing perceptions of what consti-
typically found in previous research in this area. tutes a good and bad drawing. Fifth, issues
Participants were 270 5–14 year old children, 44 surrounding an age-related decline in children’s
of their teachers of the National Curriculum for drawing activity.
Art and Design, and 146 of their parents. The
teachers and children participated in individual Conclusions: The findings are related to theories
interviews and the parents completed a postal of drawing education, and implications for chil-
survey. Responses to most interview questions dren’s drawings and drawing pedagogy are
were transcribed and content analysis used to discussed.
identify salient themes. The other questions
involved responses on five-point scales, these
were analysed by reporting percentages.

JADE 29.3 (2010)


© 2010 The Authors. JADE © 2010 NSEAD/Blackwell Publishing Ltd
258 Introduction be unclear about what art teaching strategies to
Esther Burkitt, It is generally recognised that drawing is not only use, and instead use their own art values in their
Richard Jolley and an activity enjoyed by most children but also a very teaching. For instance, Anning (2002) observed
Sarah Rose
beneficial activity for them to participate in. In line that primary school teachers encouraged even
with a selection of Government initiatives in very young children to produce more life-like
England, for example, the Excellence and Enjoy- representations, which soon discourages these
ment initiative (Clark 2003) and the formation of children in their school-based drawing activities.
Creative Partnerships, drawing (and more gener- Elsewhere, she comments further that the teach-
ally the arts) has the potential to make children’s ers were more confident managing the children
education across the curriculum more enjoyable and giving positive feedback, rather than in
and innovative. These Government schemes have promoting and developing specific drawing skills
been augmented by privately-funded art enter- (Anning & Ring 2004). Despite these observa-
prises, such as the the Campaign for Drawing, tions, little is known more generally about the
which during the current decade has successfully potentially broad range of attitudes and practices
raised the profile of drawing by promoting it as a of teachers that are likely to influence and shape
tool for thought, creativity, and social and cultural children’s experience of drawing and the draw-
engagement. More specifically, the campaign has ings they produce (for a recent review, see Rose
argued that drawing can foster development in et al. 2006).
observation, communication, creativity and the Rose et al. (2006) note a similar paucity in the
expression of the child’s inner world of fascina- literature of studies investigating the role that
tions, fantasies and fears (Power Drawing 2002). In parents and guardians have in facilitating or
observational drawing, for example, drawing discouraging children’s drawing behaviour. In
develops the child’s analysis of the subject matter seven detailed case studies of parents sitting
(people, things and places) from the world that with their child while discussing their drawing,
they choose to draw, whereas creative drawing Anning & Ring (2004) noticed that the children
enables children to develop their imaginative and had more freedom in their drawing activities and
expressive thinking. more opportunity to experiment than they had
As with other subjects taught in English observed from children’s school-based drawing
schools, classroom learning of art and design is activities. The positive parental support for chil-
informed by the National Curriculum. The class- dren’s drawing observed by Anning & Ring was
room teachers have the principal role in develop- confirmed by Kantner & Hoffman’s (1992) ques-
ing and delivering the content of the art lessons tionnaire study on 88 American parents. As Rose
that children receive, and in particular, shaping et al. (2006) note, however, both these studies
their children’s drawing behaviour and experi- focused on children under seven years of age, so
ence. While it is usual for secondary schools to little is known about whether the positive draw-
have a dedicated and trained art teacher who the ing environment in the home extends to children
children go to for their art lessons, this is relatively beyond this age.
uncommon in primary schools where children While considering the influences of teachers
are typically taught art by their ‘generalist’ class and parents on children’s drawing experience,
teacher (as was indeed the case in all but 2 of the one must not forget the attitudes and practices of
22 primary schools involved in this study). It has the children themselves, and also how children
been noted that many primary school teachers may shape other children’s drawing experience.
report feeling vulnerable teaching art (Clement Brent and Marjorie Wilson have long since alerted
1994). Indeed, primary school teachers have us to the role children have on other children’s
often received very little training in how to facili- drawing development, and in a wider context the
tate children’s drawing experience, or have little influence of cultural and media images on chil-
artistic experience themselves (Cleave & Sharp dren’s drawings (Wilson & Wilson 1977). Again,
1986; Crace 2003). Consequently, teachers may Rose et al. (2006) note that few studies have ques-

JADE 29.3 (2010)


© 2010 The Authors. JADE © 2010 NSEAD/Blackwell Publishing Ltd
tioned children about their attitudes towards their Methodology 259
drawing behaviour. For instance, we know little Esther Burkitt,
about what help children perceive they are given Participants Richard Jolley and
Sarah Rose
for their drawing, or indeed what help they would Participants were 270 children, 44 of their teach-
like to receive. Rose et al. comment that the ers and 146 of their parents. Thirty children partic-
sparse literature has reported somewhat mixed ipated from each of nine year groups making up
findings regarding the often held assumption that the three key stages to which the National Curric-
time spent in drawing activity, and children’s ulum for Art and Design is taught in England. Key
enjoyment thereof, declines with age. If such a Stage 1 (KS1) children are aged 5–7, Key Stage 2
decline does occur, it is clearly important to estab- (KS2) children are between ages 7 and 11 and Key
lish its developmental point, and the suggestions Stage 3 (KS3) includes children aged between 11
children, their teachers and parents provide to and 14. These age groups were selected so a
encourage the continuing practice of drawing complete overview of the years receiving statu-
throughout childhood (see also Jolley 2010). tory art education in England could be achieved.
Mindful of the undoubted importance of the All the parents of all participating children agreed
roles teachers, parents and children have in chil- to complete the survey and all were provided
dren’s drawing experience, and the lack of stud- with a copy of the questionnaire, 146 were
ies investigating their influences, we have returned representing a return rate of 54 per cent.
conducted a large-scale survey and interview The participating teachers were those principally
study in which views from children, their teachers responsible for delivering the National Curricu-
and their parents (or legal guardians) were lum for Art and Design to the participating chil-
collected. The aim of the study was to investigate dren. These teachers ranged from those in their
a wide range of factors associated with children’s first year of teaching since qualifying as teachers
drawing experiences at home and at school, to those who had been teaching for 40 years
focusing on those with the most direct contact (mean 12 years). Some 35 of the participating
with children’s drawings, that of teachers, parents teachers taught at primary schools (31 females
and the children themselves. This survey study and 4 males), and 9 taught at secondary schools
produced a huge data set tapping into a wide (all female). Most of the children and teachers
range of issues, but this article will concentrate were of white ethnic origin and all attended
upon, and consider more fully, the findings relat- schools in an English city. The children were
ing to five educational issues which influence selected randomly by their teachers from classes
children’s experience of drawing both at home at 29 schools, and no attempt was made to test
and at school. The objective of this article is to those either gifted or especially motivated in the
describe and discuss findings relating to these visual arts.
five issues:
Procedure
1. Perceptions of drawing behaviour and Surveys are the most commonly used methodol-
attitudes. ogy for collecting information about attitudes and
2. Principal aims and importance of the National practices (Oppenheim 2003), and have been
Curriculum for Art and Design Education. used in much of the previous work researching
3. The numerous sources of encouragement attitudes and practices surrounding children’s
and support for drawing development. drawing activity (e.g. Barry & Townsend 1995;
4. Perceptions of what constitutes a good and Kanter & Hoffman 1992; Watts 2005; for a review
bad drawing. see Rose et al. 2006). Consequently, the method-
5. An age-related decline in children’s drawing ology for the current study was derived from the
activity and associated issues. previous research in the area. Three surveys were
generated and piloted, one each for teachers,
parents and children. The format of the surveys

JADE 29.3 (2010)


© 2010 The Authors. JADE © 2010 NSEAD/Blackwell Publishing Ltd
260 included both rankings on five-point Likert scales ended question on each survey independently of
Esther Burkitt, and open-ended questions, a combination found responses to the other two surveys. In cases
Richard Jolley and in the majority of previous research in this area. where the same or a similar question was asked
Sarah Rose
The open-ended questions allowed participants in more than one survey, there are instances
greater freedom of expression and ensured that therefore where the titles of themes generated
no bias was present due to limited response between groups of respondent differ slightly. We
ranges. The responses on the five-point scales considered it important that the themes were
allowed participants to either indicate their level generated to reflect principally the responses
of agreement to a statement or their perceived given by the responding group of participants
frequency of an event. Likert-type scales were (teachers, parents or children), rather than be
used to enable comparisons to be easily made influenced unduly by the responses from one or
between the three Key Stages and respondent both of the other groups of participants. Coding
groups. Both styles of question complemented was carried out by one rater who coded all of the
each other to gain an insight into the attitudes surveys by scoring which themes each partici-
and practices directly related to children’s draw- pant mentioned in response to each open-ended
ing experiences. Each survey covered the follow- question. To ensure that this was done reliably a
ing topics: frequency of drawing behaviour, chil- second rater scored 20 per cent of the responses
dren’s perceived self-efficacy, children’s to each question and inter-rater reliability was
enjoyment of and attitude towards drawing, chil- calculated to be above 90 per cent for all themes,
dren’s motivation to draw, art values, principal for all questions, on all surveys. Responses to the
aims of the National Curriculum for Art and Likert scales were analysed by percentage per
Design, support children received for drawing, point on scale, and sometimes broken down
external influences on children’s drawing, further by Key Stage.
perceived benefits of drawing and issues
surrounding any possible decline in drawing. Results and discussion
In the pilot study children were interviewed From our wider data set we extracted the five
individually, whereas teachers and parents self- broad issues outlined in our objectives that have
completed the survey. The pilot work highlighted the most educational relevance to the attitudes
very few instances of question ambiguity or and practices reported from our respondents as
participant misunderstanding. In the few ques- they relate to drawing behaviour.
tions where this had occurred questions were
reworded. Additionally it was decided that the Time spent, self-efficacy and enjoyment
teachers as well as the children would be inter- We asked KS2 and KS3 children to estimate how
viewed in the main study to insure that the fullest many hours they spend drawing both at school
and most complete responses were received. and at home. We considered that KS1 children
Accordingly, all the children and their teachers would find it too difficult to provide reliable esti-
were interviewed individually with the questions mates, so we asked teachers and parents for time
from their respective surveys at their schools. estimates for drawing at school and at home
The children’s parents filled in the parents’ survey respectively for these younger children. In contrast
in their own time. to the speculative claims made in the literature
All the interviews were transcribed and citing a general decline in drawing behaviour
content analysis was used to generate themes of between early and middle childhood (e.g. Cox
response for all the open-ended questions. This 1989, 1992; Gardner 1980; Golomb 2002; Luquet
is a well-recognised technique suitable for 1927/2001; Matthews 2003; Thomas & Silk 1990;
systematically and objectively analysing the Winner 1982), our data, based on time estimates
message contained within speech or text (Krip- made by our three groups of respondents most
pendorf 1980; Neuendorf 2002). In the present closely in touch with children’s drawing behaviour,
study the themes were generated for each open- do not reflect this view. Taking the children’s esti-

JADE 29.3 (2010)


© 2010 The Authors. JADE © 2010 NSEAD/Blackwell Publishing Ltd
mates at KS2 and KS3 and the teachers’ estimates In Figure 2 we see that the majority of children liked 261
of children at KS1, there is little evidence of a drawing a lot, with very little evidence for a devel- Esther Burkitt,
drawing decline at school. Indeed, children opmental decline. Furthermore, we found addi- Richard Jolley and
Sarah Rose
reported that at KS3 they spend more time at tional evidence from both children and teachers
school drawing than KS2 children. The picture at that most children of all ages that we surveyed
home shows a decrease in reported time spent enjoyed drawing. First, when the children were
drawing at home between KS1 and the older chil- asked how their enjoyment of drawing has
dren. Nevertheless, a similar amount of time changed over time, 59 per cent of KS3 children
drawing at home was estimated between the KS2 reported that they enjoyed drawing more than
and KS3 children. they used to, with 14 per cent replying it was about
the same. Second, we asked teachers how many
of their children enjoy doing drawing activities
(‘almost all’, ‘more than half’, ‘less than half’, ‘very
few’). Of the teachers who taught KS1 and KS2
children, 83 per cent (KS1) and 70 per cent (KS2)
stated that almost of all their children enjoyed
drawing, with the remaining teachers all reporting
that over half of their children did. Although the
distribution of responses among the KS3 teachers
were less favourable, no teacher stated that either
‘less than half’ or ‘very few’ of their children
enjoyed drawing. The Goodlad Study (Goodlad
1984) found that most children, from elementary
These data from the three participant groups do through to senior high, rated the arts as the school
present a surprisingly positive and coherent picture subject that they most liked. These feelings of
of time spent drawing, and indicates the assump- positivity towards drawing are echoed in our own
tion in the literature that drawing declines by early findings, although we do not know how children in
adolescence may need to be reassessed. this study would have compared art to the other
A common reason suggested in the literature school subjects. Similar to the findings of the
for the supposed developmental decline in draw- current study, percentages in the Goodlad study
ing activity is that children begin to feel less posi- indicated only a small age-related decline in prefer-
tive about drawing. Our survey assessed this in ence, art being ranked as the most liked school
two ways by asking children to rate on five-point subject by 93 per cent of elementary pupils and 84
scales how much they liked drawing and how per cent of senior high pupils.
good they thought they were at drawing. The In terms of perceived drawing self-efficacy,
data for their responses by Key Stage are shown our survey shows that all age groups presented a
in Figures 2 and 3 respectively. positive attitude towards their own drawing abil-
ity. As indicated in Figure 3, the KS1 children were
particularly positive. Research has shown that
younger children have a general tendency to
overestimate their ability across many domains Figure 1
Estimates of time
of expertise, and that a cognitive immaturity and
children spend
an engagement in ‘wishful thinking’ has been drawing at home and
suggested to account for these overestimations in school per week
(Schuster et al. 1998; Stipek 1984; Stipek & Figure 2
MacIver 1989), whereas older children have an KS1, KS2 and KS3
children’s ratings of
increasing competence in perceiving their own their enjoyment for
abilities correctly (e.g. Harter 1982; Newman drawing

JADE 29.3 (2010)


© 2010 The Authors. JADE © 2010 NSEAD/Blackwell Publishing Ltd
262 1984; Spinath & Spinath 2005). In respect of draw- the principal aims of the National Curriculum for
Esther Burkitt, ing, for example, young children choose human Art and Design for the age group(s) they teach.
Richard Jolley and figure drawings depicting higher levels of repre- The most common answer was skill develop-
Sarah Rose
sentational realism over more age-appropriate ment (48 per cent), with gaining experience with
drawings when asked to indicate which figure materials (36 per cent), developing creativity (36
looks most like how they draw a person (Jolley per cent) and art appreciation (34 per cent) also
2010). Research on children’s self-presentation frequently cited. The frequency with which teach-
strategies showing that children learn to present ers mentioned art appreciation is notable, since
themselves as less boastful with age may also be this may represent a relatively recent change in
relevant here (Watling & Banerjee 2005). the content of children’s drawing education, as
until recently art education curricula have focused
almost exclusively on production (Gardner 1990).
Drawing curricula such as Discipline Based Arts
Education, have advocated the importance of
teaching children about art history, criticism and
aesthetics alongside production skills (J. Paul
Getty Trust, 1992), and this integrated approach is
also echoed in the National Curriculum for Art and
Design (Department for Education and Employ-
ment 1999). Parents were asked a similar ques-
tion to the teachers, namely, what they thought
school art education was for. Expression and
In summary, our survey data on time spent draw- communication was easily the most popular
ing, enjoyment and self-efficacy of drawing, show answer (37 per cent), with the related theme of
little evidence for an age-related decline in chil- creativity and imagination (21 per cent) being the
dren’s drawing behaviour and experience. Overall, second most common response. Also, around
most children presented a very positive picture, one-fifth of parents cited that art education devel-
seemingly spending a significant amount of time oped children’s thinking and learning. In contrast
drawing, enjoying their drawing and holding posi- to teachers, skill development was commented
tive perceptions of their drawing ability. These upon much less by parents (17 per cent) with
positive outcomes, reported by children who have gaining experience with materials barely featur-
received the National Curriculum for Art and ing at all in the parental responses. Skill develop-
Design throughout all their school years, are reas- ment and experience with art materials is more
suring. Children are engaging in drawing and intrinsic to the skills-based and assessment
enjoying it, despite this experience occurring in the driven environment teachers work in, whereas
context of criticisms of the Curriculum and fears of parents are free from such concerns, which in
art being marginalised by education policy makers. turn may allow them to express their personal
Nevertheless, the time children choose to draw at views on the role of art in facilitating expression
home does decrease somewhat for older children, and creativity. Interestingly, when we asked
by which point drawing might be becoming a teachers what benefits children get from making
casualty of the increasing demands upon older drawings, a question that more directly addresses
children’s time and their fast-paced lifestyle (see their own opinions rather than curriculum consid-
later discussion in ‘drawing decline’ section). erations, their most common response was also
expression and communication (see below).
Perceptions of the principal aims and Accordingly, both teachers and parents may hold
importance of the National Curriculum similar views to the purpose of art education,
for Art and Design valuing expression and communication. In terms
We asked teachers what they considered to be of the National Curriculum we note that expres-

JADE 29.3 (2010)


© 2010 The Authors. JADE © 2010 NSEAD/Blackwell Publishing Ltd
sion appears to be given equal weight to observa- that they ‘set clear expectations through demon- 263
tion, although is underrepresented in the associ- stration and instruction’, echoing the children’s Esther Burkitt,
ated Schemes of Work published by the own theme of ‘directions’. Around 18 per cent of Richard Jolley and
Sarah Rose
Qualifications and Curriculum Authority (QCA) teachers provided the generic ‘techniques and
that support the curriculum by providing teachers skills’, but where reference was made to particu-
with suggested art-based activities. lar skills, 30 per cent of teachers reported that
We also asked teachers and parents to respond they encouraged observational skills, and 9 per
to a 10-point scale indicating to what extent they cent to encouraging realism. These practices are
regarded the importance of children’s art educa- reflective of teachers’ views about the primary
tion within the context of children’s whole educa- aim of the National Curriculum for Art and Design
tion (10 being ‘extremely important’). Teachers being to develop graphic skills. Teachers’ reports
thought it to be very important (mean 8.28), with generally indicated that children’s classroom
similar ratings given by both primary and second- drawing experiences are frequently aligned with
ary teachers, while parents reported that it was the historical educational pedagogies of Ruskin
fairly important (mean 6.84). The slightly higher (1857), who advocated a problem solving
ratings among the teachers may be due the teach- approach to representational drawing. Very few
ers’ professional understanding of child develop- teachers alluded to fostering creativity and
ment and the importance that practice-based expression in children’s drawings as has been
learning and activity for children, in addition to encouraged by educationalists such as Lowen-
their professional position in supporting the role feld (1954) and Richardson (1948). Nevertheless,
of art in the child’s education. as reported in the previous section, expression
was recognised by teachers as one of the princi-
Sources of encouragement and support for pal aims of the National Curriculum for Art and
drawing development Design. Maybe this disparity between attitudes
A number of questions were addressed to chil- and practices is reflective of a lack of drawing
dren, their teachers and parents about the educa- confidence in teaching expressive drawing
tional support for children drawing at school and among some primary school teachers, and possi-
in the home. We sought to ascertain the nature bly an underrepresentation of expression in the
and type of help children received, what addi- Schemes of Work published by the QCA.
tional help children requested, and the difficulties Although the evidence from children’s and
teachers and parents encounter in helping their teachers’ responses that children at times are
children to draw. The most common theme in directed what to do in their art lessons, many of
children’s responses to the help they receive the primary school teachers did acknowledge that
from teachers was ‘graphical and spatial demon- children have more freedom in drawings made in
strations’ (37 per cent), referring to guidance from non-art lessons. Nevertheless, children’s drawing
a drawing made on the board or on paper, or freedom was most evident in the home, where
where the teacher drew an outline or shape with there appears to be greater opportunity and
their hand. Many children also commented that support for free and imaginative drawing. Further-
they received ‘verbal suggestions’ (24 per cent) in more, our data showed little evidence from either
the form of advice and tips, in contrast to ‘direc- the children’s perceptions of the help they receive
tions’ (20 per cent) where the children referred to from parents, or in the parents reporting of the
the teacher giving more specific and precise help they give children, of parents giving firm direc-
instructions (which may include demonstrations) tions in their child’s drawing activities. In other
on what to draw and how. Only 17 per cent of respects, however, the educational support given
children reported receiving help with techniques by parents reflected that given at school. For Figure 3
and skills. instance, the most frequently cited assistance chil- Self-ratings of
drawing self-efficacy
When we asked teachers what help they gave dren reported receiving from parents was of for KS1, KS2 and
children with their drawing, 41 per cent reported ‘graphical/spatial demonstrations’ (28 per cent) KS3 children

JADE 29.3 (2010)


© 2010 The Authors. JADE © 2010 NSEAD/Blackwell Publishing Ltd
264 and ‘verbal suggestions’ (23 per cent). Verbal longer in shared drawing time with them enjoyed
Esther Burkitt, support was confirmed by the parents in the two drawing more and had a higher perception of
Richard Jolley and most commonly cited themes from their survey, their drawing ability. This suggests that any possi-
Sarah Rose
‘verbal suggestions’ (38 per cent) and ‘encourage- bilities in the school environment for teachers,
ment’ (36 per cent). Interestingly, graphical demon- other adults or even older children, to share in an
stration was not a prominent theme in the parental individual child’s drawing experience should be
responses (18 per cent). The difference in weight- promoted and developed.
ing of graphic and verbal support in the home When we asked children about the help they
between the children and parents may be due received from other children in their drawing, the
simply to a possible distinction in the way the two most common response was they did not get
kinds of support are each conceptualised between any help (31 per cent)! Nevertheless, others
children and their parents, but it could also reflect responded with ‘verbal suggestions’ (23 per
that children find graphical demonstrations the cent), and ‘graphical and spatial demonstrations’
most useful type of help and therefore are more (17 per cent), again reflecting common themes of
likely to report this (see below). help received from teachers and parents. Some
In our examination of the children’s and of the children’s responses indicated that some
parents’ responses we noticed that some of the of the support children gained through other chil-
parental help appeared to occur during a shared dren occurs through collaborative drawing activi-
drawing experience, where the parent sat with ties. We also asked children whether anyone else
the child. This is something which Matthews helped them with their drawing, apart from their
(2003) identifies as being of particular importance teachers, parents and other children. Siblings or
to young children. Matthews has collected cousins (24 per cent), grandparents (21 per cent),
evidence which demonstrates that drawing uncles, aunts and family friends (9 per cent) were
episodes are composed of rapidly alternating mentioned. Only five children said that an artist
bursts of action, and that like speech, are related helped them with their drawing. Graphical/spatial
to the breathing patterns of young children. demonstrations were the most frequently cited
Initially it is necessary for an adult to interact with help from these varied sources.
the child to facilitate this conversation, the child In addition to support directly and personally
will then develop and be able to maintain the given by people, there are other external and
‘conversation’ on their own. Further evidence for more generic influences on children drawing,
the importance of adult involvement in the draw- particularly those that are media driven. We asked
ing experiences of young children comes from teachers and parents what other influences, apart
Kindler (1995) who observed, in a day-care centre from themselves, there were on children’s draw-
for the under threes, that although art materials ings. Television was the most cited response,
were plentiful children rarely experimented with including art programmes for children as well as
them unless an adult was present and became characters from cartoons. Similarly, the computer
involved in what they were doing. To reflect this (including play stations) and the internet were
importance of adult involvement we asked in the reported by teachers and parents as providing
parents’ survey how often the parent sat and/or drawing inspiration for children. Books, maga-
talked with their child as their child drew. Shared zines and popular culture images were also cited.
drawing experiences were reported to be more Experts in child art are divided about the effect of
frequent than we had expected, with as many as such images on children’s drawings. Arnheim
49 per cent of parents of KS1 children stated they (1974) believed them to stifle the natural creativity
did so most days. Although shared drawing time of children, while others pointed out that children
decreased for parents of older children, 72 per can learn graphic principles such as foreshorten-
cent of parents of KS3 children spent time with ing from these images and then apply them in
their children drawing at least once a week. flexible and inventive ways in their own work
Furthermore, children whose parents spent (Wilson & Wilson 1987). However, not all the influ-

JADE 29.3 (2010)


© 2010 The Authors. JADE © 2010 NSEAD/Blackwell Publishing Ltd
ences in our survey findings were pictorial. skill from the teacher (Barnes 2002). Furthermore, 265
Parents, in particular, commented on the chil- this art curriculum tends to deal in generalist Esther Burkitt,
dren’s personal experiences (e.g. holidays, school statements whereas some teachers may need Richard Jolley and
Sarah Rose
visits) and general observations of their surround- more specific direction. Overall, this seems to
ings, and even internal psychological states (e.g. suggest that further professional training should
the child’s mood, their interests). be made available for those teaching in art and
The children were asked what extra help they design, especially as such little time is devoted to
would like for their drawing, either now or in the it in the training of primary school teachers.
past. Some 27 per cent reported that they did not
need any further help. These children may have Perceptions of what constitutes a good
interpreted an affirmative answer as indicating an and bad drawing
admission of being weak at drawing, or in need of When the children were asked about what they
special instruction. Alternatively, it may be an thought made a good drawing, 30 per cent of chil-
encouraging sign that many children are quite dren referred to the use of colours (e.g. many of
content with their drawings and the level of them, bright), 24 per cent to the subject matter
support they are given. Interestingly, the most (most children mentioned ‘pleasant’ topics/
common additional help cited was the child scenes reflecting their own preferences), 21 per
requesting more pictures to copy (14 per cent), cent to detail (lots of it), and 20 per cent to care/
which provides some tension with one opinion effort. These children’s opinions are consistent
within art education that copying negates both with the interview data collected by Parsons
the problem solving and expressive purposes of (1987) who found that younger school-aged chil-
art (e.g. Arnheim 1989). dren tended to conceive a good painting as one
In terms of the difficulties teachers and parents that was beautiful and realistic. For teachers,
reported when helping children to draw, the most however, care and effort was most important in a
frequently cited inhibitor in both the teachers’ and child’s drawing (45 per cent), followed by detail
parents’ surveys was a limitation with their own and realistic representations (both 20 per cent).
drawing skill (teachers: 30 per cent; parents: 19 References to evidence of formal properties (18
per cent). However, 57 per cent of the parents per cent) and in particular to colour and shading
said that they did not have any difficulties when (14 per cent) also featured, as did comments on
supporting their children’s drawing development, the creativity/individuality of the drawing (18 per
whereas only 18 per cent of teachers said so. This cent). Parents, on the other hand, thought that
different pattern of responses between teachers the main reasons why a drawing can be appraised
and parents is surprising considering that teach- as good depended on its level of creativity,
ers more than parents will have received training expressive communication and imagination (31
to teach drawing. However, the limited time per cent), the child’s enjoyment (27 per cent) and
devoted to art in primary teacher training the level of care and concentration that had been
programmes is likely to be insufficient for some applied to the drawing (27 per cent). Colour (14
teachers to enable them to fulfil their role as per cent) and detail (12 per cent) were also
sensitive facilitators to children’s drawing experi- mentioned. When children were asked about
ences. Furthermore, the higher expectations on what they thought adults believed made a good
teachers to provide professional educational drawing, the most commonly cited theme, in line
support may highlight to some teachers, particu- with the teachers’ reports, was that a good draw-
larly at the non-specialist primary level, a ing was one in which care and attention were
perceived weakness of their own artistic abilities. evident (27 per cent). However, almost a quarter
Teachers may have become increasingly aware of the children (24 per cent) referred to subject
of their own limitations since 2001 when the QCA matter, which on the face of the teachers’ and
published schemes of work for the study of art: parents’ responses appears to be a misconcep-
the activities suggested assume a certain level of tion possibly based on children’s own values.

JADE 29.3 (2010)


© 2010 The Authors. JADE © 2010 NSEAD/Blackwell Publishing Ltd
266 In response to what makes a bad drawing, many continues for some adolescents outside school.
Esther Burkitt, children (18 per cent), teachers (20 per cent) and Those teachers and parents who agreed that
Richard Jolley and parents (30 per cent) agreed that there was no there was a developmental decline in children’s
Sarah Rose
such thing. Where reasons for a poor drawing drawing activity were asked to state whether they
were given, a lack of care and effort featured considered that this decline mattered. Although
prominently among the explanations (children: 75 per cent of teachers said that it did matter, only
22 per cent; teachers: 61 per cent; parents: 27 40 per cent of parents thought that it did. Just as
per cent). Some children also cited the related with the perceived importance of drawing, this
theme of a lack of neatness (17 per cent), while difference in the views of teachers and parents
some parents mentioned a child’s poor attitude might be explained by teacher’s professional role
(19 per cent). Of all three groups of participants, in supporting drawing in the child’s education.
children were most preoccupied with issues Furthermore, the teachers’ awareness of the
relating to scribbling (14 per cent), realism (13 per learning and problem-solving potential of draw-
cent), detail (9 per cent) and dull/unpleasant ing, as well as its potential to facilitate learning in
subject matter (9 per cent) as contributory factors other domains and in meeting the needs of chil-
in a poor drawing, echoing Parsons’ (1987) find- dren with different learning styles, may have
ing of children’s negativity towards unrealistic caused more teachers to say that it does matter.
and unpleasant subject matter in paintings. Chil- The most commonly cited reasons given by these
dren’s views on what adults considered a bad teachers and parents were that drawing develops
drawing was consistent with their responses to creativity, imagination and is a means of self-
previous answers, namely lack of care/attention expression and communication. Some teachers
(26 per cent), unpleasant subject matter (14 per and parents also commented that the decline
cent) and all drawings are good (13 per cent). results in losing a skill and having less confidence
Although the National Curriculum for Art and in drawing, and that children then will lose one
Design covers evaluation, a lot of the comments means by which they can gain a sense of personal
made by teachers suggest that many are reluc- achievement. When we asked children whether
tant to evaluate and when they do it is generally they thought it was a good or bad thing that chil-
not based on the graphic skills shown. dren tend to draw less as they get older, 43 per
cent said it was bad, mainly due to the loss of
Perceptions about influences on drawing skills. Of those children, teachers and
drawing decline parents who believed that a decline in drawing did
We asked teachers and parents on a five-point not matter, it was frequently explained by all three
scale how much they agreed with the statement groups as being down to the personal choice of
that the amount of time children choose to spend the child, and also that it was natural for children
drawing declines with age (apart from the draw- to draw less as they got older.
ing they are required to do in school). Most teach- All parties were asked about what they thought
ers (86 per cent) and parents (57 per cent) either might help to arrest a decline in drawing behav-
agreed or strongly agreed, with both groups esti- iour or encourage children to draw more as they
mating that the decline occurs around 11 years of got older. The children said that they would like to
age (although there was a considerable range in be given more time and opportunity for art (26 per
estimates). This finding is consistent with cent), both at school and at home, and this was
comments in the broader literature on children’s echoed by teachers (34 per cent) and parents (23
drawings (e.g. Cox 1989; Gardner 1980; Golomb per cent). Specific suggestions included more
2002; Jolley 2010; Luquet 2001; Matthews 2003; opportunities to draw in after-school clubs, in
Thomas & Silk 1990). The stronger belief held by community workshops and projects, with more
teachers in an age-related decline in drawing may art homework and less homework from other
be because they would be expected to be less subjects. Teachers (32 per cent) and parents (21
aware of the child-initiated drawing activity that per cent) also specifically referred to raising the

JADE 29.3 (2010)


© 2010 The Authors. JADE © 2010 NSEAD/Blackwell Publishing Ltd
profile of drawing in school. As well as the general many in the wider adult population, have long 267
comment that there should be a stronger empha- since stopped engaging in art for their own inter- Esther Burkitt,
sis on art in school (including funding), these ests. For their continuing professional develop- Richard Jolley and
Sarah Rose
teachers and parents also commented that art ment, government and privately run workshops
activities should be integrated more into other need to be made available to help them feel
subjects, particular at secondary school (11–16 empowered to facilitate children’s drawing/art
years of age), have day trips to art galleries and development.
have visiting artists. Some teachers (27 per cent)
also felt that children could receive more support 3. Opportunities for creative and expressive
for their drawing at home (more encouragement drawing may be under represented and under
and interest from parents, restricted use of facilitated in schools. It might be that some teach-
computers and TV, more art materials), which ers feel particularly unclear how to facilitate chil-
some parents agreed with (12 per cent). Some dren’s development of creative and expressive
teachers and parents made more general drawing. More examples, for example in the
comments that children should be encouraged schemes of work published by the QCA, of tasks
and confidence built in their drawing, a theme involving expression might be of benefit to teach-
that was particularly strong in the children’s ers. In addition, the training teachers receive in art
responses. A further theme consistently raised education, whether as part of their pre- or post-
by all three groups was that children should be qualification training, should give weight to
inspired to draw more. Children themselves expressive, imaginative and creative drawing
exemplified the tone in these comments by equal to that given to representational activities.
remarking that their school art lessons should
include more varied drawing activities that are 4. As teachers commented that care and atten-
exciting and fun to participate in. tion of the child were the most important consid-
erations for what makes a good drawing, a view
Implications acknowledged by children in what they thought
teachers valued the most, it raises the question
1. Our survey painted a very positive picture of whether this should be the primary feedback chil-
children’s enjoyment and self-efficacy in their dren receive at school for their drawing. While
drawing experience, and the time they continue children no doubt benefit from encouraging a
to spend on drawing, throughout their statutory conscientious approach to drawing and art
art education (ages 5–14). It gives some reassur- making, some children do ask for more specific
ance if not indeed relief to those of us concerned help with their graphic skills. Where there is a
with the perceived undermining of arts education reluctance among teachers to comment on these
in schools that children continue to feel positively skills, it may reflect a misunderstanding that such
about drawing. Ironically, it may be because of feedback would represent interference to the
the increased focus on numeracy and literacy development of the children’s own natural crea-
during their education that children’s enjoyment tivity. In contrast, appropriately channelled
remains buoyant in subjects and activities that constructive comments by art-informed teachers
allow them more freedom and creativity. can empower children to build upon and develop
the techniques they have already acquired.
2. Some teachers may need more training to
develop effectiveness and confidence in facilitat- 5. Giving children pictures to copy may have a
ing children’s learning experiences in art and role in some children’s drawing development, as
design. Relatively little time is allocated to the indicated by a minority of children asking for this
teaching of art in teacher training programmes. type of help. Although considered a somewhat
Such allocation will be insufficient for some unpopular teaching aid in Western education (for
generalist primary school teachers who, like instance, see Lowenfeld 1954), the practice is

JADE 29.3 (2010)


© 2010 The Authors. JADE © 2010 NSEAD/Blackwell Publishing Ltd
268 found in Chinese and Steiner Education, while value of one-to-one time across all subject areas
Esther Burkitt, the copying of the manga style is common and classroom assistants often make one-to-one
Richard Jolley and among Japanese children. We agree with Brent learning experiences possible in literacy and
Sarah Rose
Wilson’s (2000) position that showing graphic numeracy classes, but this is a regrettably an infre-
models does not produce stereotyped drawing quent occurrence in art classes, and indeed many
but enhances children’s drawing skills. Teachers other subject lessons too.
should feel free to show a wide variety of graphic
models to children, particularly those that inspire Acknowledgement
children in their drawing activities. The authors are grateful to The Leverhulme
Trust for funding this research (grant number
6. To keep children engaged in art as they get F/00 799/B).
older, more time and opportunity for art was References
recommended by children, teachers and parents.
Anning, A. (2002) A conversation around young
In line with current government incentives to
children’s drawing: the impact of the beliefs of
dedicate additional extra curriculum sessions for
significant others at home and at school,
promoting artistic appreciation, the current study
International Journal of Art & Design Education,
suggests that children would appreciate further
Vol. 21, pp. 197–208
educational opportunities in school and at home
for the encouragement of their artistic skills devel- Anning, A, & Ring, K. (2004) Making Sense of
opment. Children wished that school art activities Children’s Drawings. Maidenhead: Open
were more fun and inspiring, calling for more art University Press
clubs and communal art projects to get involved
Arnheim, R. (1974) Art and Visual Perception:
in. As well documented in the literature (e.g. see
A Psychology of the Creative Eye: The new
Pariser et al. 2008) the critical period appears to
version. Berkeley, CA: University of California
be period before and during adolescence, and
Press
we feel that presenting social art activities to this
age group may have a key role in preventing many Arnheim, R. (1989) Thoughts on Art Education.
children from giving up on art and drawing. Los Angeles, CA: Getty Centre for Education in
the Arts
7. Parents should be encouraged to spend time
Barnes, R. (2002) Teaching Art to Young Children
with children as they draw, as this was associated
4–9, 2nd edn. London: RoutledgeFalmer
with children’s positive attitude to their drawing.
But some parents may need to be made more Barry, C. & Townsend, M. A. R. (1995) Teachers’
aware of the benefits of drawing to the child’s judgements about the teaching of art in primary
wider education. schools, New Zealand Journal of Educational
Studies, Vol. 30, No. 1, pp. 51–63
8. The association we found between parents’
Clark, C. (2003) Excellence and Enjoyment:
shared drawing activities with their children and
A Strategy for Primary Schools. Nottingham:
the children’s positive attitude to drawing indicates
DCSF Publications Centre
that this should be applied to school settings,
wherever possible. For instance, more one-to-one Cleave, S. & Sharp, S. (1986) The Arts, A
time spent with individual children by teachers or Preparation to Teach – Initial Training for Primary
other adults is likely to further bolster children’s Teachers. London: National Foundation for
confidence in their drawing. Even older children Educational Research
spending time in younger children’s classes to this
effect may be similarly beneficial, as long as such Clement, R. (1994) The readiness of primary
mentoring experience was mutually beneficial for schools to teach the National Curriculum in
the older child. Teachers are well aware of the Art and Design, Journal of Art and Design
Education, Vol. 13, No. 1, pp. 9–19

JADE 29.3 (2010)


© 2010 The Authors. JADE © 2010 NSEAD/Blackwell Publishing Ltd
Cox, M. V. (1989) Children’s drawings, in D. J. Lowenfeld, V. (1954) Your Child and His Art. 269
Hargreaves [Ed.] Children and the Arts. Milton New York: Macmillan Esther Burkitt,
Keynes: Open University Press, pp. 43–58 Richard Jolley and
Luquet, G. H. (2001) Children’s Drawings [1927], Sarah Rose
Cox, M. V. (1992) Children’s Drawings. trans. A. Costall. London and New York: Free
Harmondsworth: Penguin Association Books

Crace, J. (2003) Smart squiggles, Guardian Matthews, J. (2003) Drawing and Painting:
Education, 15 July, pp. 4–5 Children and Visual Representation. London:
Sage
Department for Education and Employment
(1999) Art and Design: the National Curriculum Neuendorf, K. A. (2002) The Content Analysis
for England. London: Stationery Office Guidebook. London: Sage

Gardner, H. (1980) Artful Scribbles: The Newman, R. S. (1984) Children’s achievement


Significance of Children’s Drawings. London: and self-evaluations in mathematics: A
Jill Norman longitudinal study, Journal of Educational
Psychology, Vol. 76, No. 5, pp. 857–73
Gardner, H. (1990) Art Education and Human
Development. Los Angeles, CA: J. Paul Getty Oppenheim, A. N. (2003) Questionnaire Design,
Trust Interviewing and Attitude Measurement.
London: Continuum
Golomb, C. (2002) Child Art in Context: A Cultural
and Comparative Perspective. Washington, Pariser, D.A., Kindler, A.M. & van den Berg, A.
DC: American Psychological Association (2008) Drawing and aesthetic judgments across
cultures: diverse pathways to graphic
Goodlad, J. I. (1984) A Place Called School:
development, in C. Milbrath & H. M. Trautner
Prospects for the Future. New York: McGraw-Hill
[Eds] Children’s Understanding and Production
Harter, S. (1982) The perceived competence of Pictures, Drawings and Art: Theoretical and
scale for children, Child Development, Vol. 53, Empirical Approaches. Cambridge, MA:
pp. 87–97 Hogrefe & Huber, pp. 293–317

Jolley, R. P. (2010) Children and Pictures: Parsons, M. J. (1987) How We Understand Art:
Drawing and Understanding. Oxford: Wiley- A Cognitive Developmental Account of
Blackwell Aesthetic Experience. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press
J. Paul Getty Trust (1992) The DBAE Handbook:
An Overview of Discipline Based Art Education. Power Drawing (2002) Start Drawing! Hove:
Santa Monica, CA: J. Paul Getty Trust Laceys Printers

Kantner, L. & Hoffman, S. (1992) Parents’ Richardson, M. (1948) Art and the Child.
perception of their children’s early art experience, London: University of London Press
Visual Art Research, Vol. 18, pp. 50–6
Rose, S. E., Jolley, R. P. & Burkitt, E. (2006) A
Kindler, A.M. (1995) Significance of adult input in review of children’s, teachers’ and parents’
early childhood artistic development, in C. M. influences on children’s drawing experience,
Thompson [Ed.] The Visual Arts and Early International Journal of Art & Design Education,
Childhood Learning. Reston, VA: National Art Vol. 25, No. 3, pp. 341–9
Education Association, pp. 10–14
Ruskin, J. (1857) The Elements of Drawing.
Krippendorf, K. (1980) Content Analysis: An London: Smith, Elder
Introduction to its Methodology. London: Sage

JADE 29.3 (2010)


© 2010 The Authors. JADE © 2010 NSEAD/Blackwell Publishing Ltd
270 Schuster, B., Ruble, D., & Weinert, F. (1998) Wilson, B. & Wilson, M. (1987) Pictorial
Esther Burkitt, Cultural influences and the positivity bias in composition and narrative structure: themes
Richard Jolley and children: the role of the covariation principle, and the creation of meaning in the drawings of
Sarah Rose
Child Development, Vol. 69, No. 6, pp. 1577–96 Egyptian and Japanese children, Visual Arts
Research, Vol. 12, No. 2, pp. 10–21
Spinath, B. & Spinath, F. (2005) Longitudinal
analyses of the link between learning motivation Winner, E. (1982) Invented Worlds: The
and competence beliefs among elementary Psychology of the Arts. Cambridge, MA:
school children, Learning and Instruction, Harvard University Press
Vol. 15, pp. 87–102

Stipek, D. J. (1984) Young children’s


performance expectations: logical analysis or
wishful thinking? in J. G. Nicholls [Ed.] Advances
in Motivation and Achievement. Vol. 3: The
Development of Achievement Motivation.
Greenwich, CT: JAI Press, pp. 33–56

Stipek, D. & MacIver, D. (1989) Developmental


change in children’s assessment of intellectual
competence, Child Development, Vol. 60, No. 3,
pp. 521–38

Thomas, G. V. & Silk, A. M. J. (1990) An


Introduction to the Psychology of Children’s
Drawings. Hemel Hempstead: Harvester
Wheatsheaf

Watling, D. & Banerjee, R. (2007) Children’s


understanding of modesty in front of peer and
adult audiences, Infant and Child Development,
Vol. 16, pp. 227–36

Watts, R. (2005) Making art in primary school,


International Journal of Art & Design Education,
Vol. 24, No. 3, pp. 243–53

Wilson, B. (2000) Empire of signs revisited:


children’s manga and the changing face of
Japan, in L. Lindstrom [Ed.] The Cultural Context:
Comparative Studies of Art Education and
Children’s Drawings. Stockholm: Stockholm
Institute of Education Press, pp. 160–78

Wilson, B. & Wilson, M. (1977) An iconoclastic


view of the imagery sources in the drawings of
young people, Art Education, Vol. 30, No. 1,
pp. 4–12

JADE 29.3 (2010)


© 2010 The Authors. JADE © 2010 NSEAD/Blackwell Publishing Ltd

You might also like