You are on page 1of 15

Sex Roles (2010) 62:568–582

DOI 10.1007/s11199-009-9665-x

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

How Sexy are Sexist Men? Women’s Perception of Male


Response Profiles in the Ambivalent Sexism Inventory
Gerd Bohner & Katrin Ahlborn & Regine Steiner

Published online: 25 June 2009


# Springer Science + Business Media, LLC 2009

Abstract In Studies 1 to 3, German female students sexism as comprising two sets of beliefs with opposite
(total N=326) rated the likability and typicality of male evaluative implications, which have nonetheless been
targets: a nonsexist, a benevolent sexist, a hostile sexist, shown to often coexist in a perceiver’s mind. Whereas
and (in Studies 2 and 3) an ambivalent sexist. When hostile sexism (HS) consists of negative beliefs and
targets were presented as response profiles in the Ambiv- resentments against women (e.g., “Women are too easily
alent Sexism Inventory (Glick and Fiske 1996) (Studies 2 offended”), benevolent sexism (BS) consists of beliefs that
and 3), the benevolent sexist was rated to be most likable are subjectively positive in affective tone and tend to
but least typical, whereas the ambivalent sexist was rated prescribe prosocial or intimacy-seeking behaviors (e.g.,
to be highly typical. Thus, women were aware of a link “In a disaster, women ought to be rescued before men”).
between benevolent and hostile sexism and approved of To measure the two components of sexism, Glick and
men’s benevolent sexism, especially when it was not Fiske (1996) developed a self-report instrument, the
paired with hostile sexism. Likability ratings were mod- Ambivalent Sexism Inventory (ASI). Numerous studies
erated by participants’ own benevolent sexism and using the ASI showed that HS and BS are positively
feminist attitude. correlated both at the individual level (often around r=.40
or higher; e.g., Eckes 2001; Glick and Fiske 1996; Glick
Keywords Ambivalent sexism . Attraction . et al. 2000) and at the level of national averages for both
Feminist attitudes . Gender relations . Person perception men and women (r=.89; Glick et al. 2000). These findings
support the notion that HS and BS form complementary
parts of a sexist ideology.
Introduction Indeed, despite their positive appearance, benevolent
sexist beliefs may contribute to the subordination of women
Relationships between men and women do not corre- in a subtle way. Research demonstrates that benevolent-
spond well with classic notions of prejudice (e.g., Allport sexist beliefs and acts may have particularly debilitating
1954), as cultural images of women have never been effects on women, e.g., by hampering women’s job
uniformly negative. Starting from this insight, ambivalent performance (Dardenne et al. 2007), or predicting the
sexism theory (Glick and Fiske 1996) conceptualizes extent to which students blamed the female victim of an
acquaintance rape (Abrams et al. 2003). Detrimental effects
of BS are suggested as well by a cross-cultural comparison:
G. Bohner : K. Ahlborn : R. Steiner
Across 19 countries, national levels of both HS and BS
Universität Bielefeld,
Bielefeld, Germany were inversely related to indices of gender equality (Glick
et al. 2000). Several lines of research thus indicate that BS
G. Bohner (*) often produces damaging consequences, sometimes even
Abteilung für Psychologie, Universität Bielefeld,
more than HS (Dardenne et al. 2007).
Postfach 100131,
33501 Bielefeld, Germany As Glick and Fiske (2001) argued, women may be
e-mail: gerd.bohner@uni-bielefeld.de tempted to accept inequality in order to obtain the benefits
Sex Roles (2010) 62:568–582 569

that BS supposedly entails: “Benevolent sexism is disarm- The Current Research


ing. ... It promises that men’s power will be used to
women’s advantage, if only they can secure a high-status Our research follows up on the study by Kilianski and
male protector” (p. 111). From this it follows that women Rudman (1998). We will first point out two methodological
may tend to like, and be attracted to, benevolent-sexist men. ambiguities; then we will present new predictions and an
However, the exact cognitive processes underlying this extended, more balanced approach to the assessment of
attraction are not yet well understood. One possibility is women’s perceptions of male sexism.
that women are attracted to benevolent-sexist men without A first methodological problem that makes it difficult to
being aware that those men’s beliefs often go together with interpret their findings is that Kilianski and Rudman (1998)
hostile-sexist attitudes. Another possibility is that women did not present a fourth, ambivalent-sexist profile. If such a
consciously accept a certain degree of hostility in men as profile had been presented, participants could have been
long as it is counterbalanced by benevolence. And a final asked to rate how typical or realistic each of the four
possibility is that women like particulary those benevolent- profiles was. If women were indeed unaware of a
sexist men who do not also endorse hostile sexism. To coexistence of hostile and benevolent sexist attitudes, they
study these possibilities, we assessed women’s perceptions should have rated the purely hostile and purely benevolent
of male targets differing in BS and HS, using a new profiles each as more typical than the ambivalent profile.
methodology based on the ASI. Before describing our own Instead, Kilianski and Rudman only assessed participants’
research, we will discuss a previous study by Kilianski and ratings of the possibility that two distinct profiles, one
Rudman (1998) which addressed the same topic. described as hostile-sexist, the other as benevolent-sexist,
Kilianski and Rudman (1998) hypothesized that women “apply to the same person.” The two profiles, however, had
may fail to realize that benevolent and hostile sexist beliefs been introduced explicitly as applying to different individ-
typically go together. They further argued that women, uals, as designated by their initials. For a participant to
because of this failure to recognize BS and HS’ coexis- respond that the two might actually be the same person
tence, may indirectly support HS in society by “wanting it would thus have meant to contradict previous experimental
both ways”, i.e. opposing HS but approving of BS. To instructions, which had told her that she was judging two
examine this proposal, they presented female students with different men. Furthermore, just before making that
three male profiles, describing a hostile sexist, a benevolent “covariation estimate”, participants had indicated their
sexist, and a nonsexist. For example, the hostile sexist was approval of each profile. Especially those participants
described as someone who “believes that women underval- whose approval ratings for the benevolent and hostile
ue men and fail to appreciate everything that men do for profile greatly differed (i.e., the “equivocal egalitarians”)
them”, whereas the benevolent sexist was depicted as may have been motivated to answer consistently with their
someone who “believes that women possess a naturally divergent approval ratings — they would thus be inclined
superior aesthetic sensibility which makes them better to indicate that the two profiles are probably not describing
judges in matters of culture and taste” (pp. 349–350). the same person. Both experimental demand characteristics
When female students were asked to rate their impression and a motivation to answer consistently may thus have
of each profile on a scale from “strongly unfavorable” (0) to contributed to the findings that the “covariation estimate”
“strongly favorable” (6), they generally approved most was rather low overall and was correlated negatively with
strongly of the nonsexist (M=4.36), but still expressed the equivocal egalitarianism measure.
moderate liking for the benevolent sexist (M= 3.41), Another methodological problem is that Kilianski and
whereas they clearly disliked the hostile sexist (M=1.03). Rudman’s (1998) hostile and benevolent profiles each
When the students were later asked to judge the possibility contained information on only one of the two aspects of
that the hostile and benevolent profiles applied to the same ambivalent sexism, thus allowing for different deductions
person, they found it quite unlikely that this might be the about other characteristics of the target person: he could
case (M=2.56 on a scale from 0, “very unlikely”, to 6, endorse the other aspect of ambivalent sexism, or he could
“very likely”). In addition, the more participants had reject the other aspect of sexism. This may have implica-
preferred the benevolent sexist over the hostile sexist—a tions for ratings of the benevolent-sexist profile’s likability.
variable Kilianski and Rudman called “equivocal egalitar- If women indeed see BS and HS as covarying, then
ianism”—the lower they rated the likelihood that the two participants’ moderate likability ratings of the benevolent-
profiles may pertain to the same person (r=−.30). From sexist profile could be an underestimate of how they would
these findings, Kilianski and Rudman concluded that rate a highly benevolent-sexist man if they were assured
women are not fully aware of the link between BS and that he is not also a highly hostile-sexist man.
HS, and that by responding positively to BS women may In our own Studies 2 and 3, we addressed these
unwittingly support HS as well. problems by correspondent changes to the methodology
570 Sex Roles (2010) 62:568–582

employed. First, we added a fourth male profile: that of an lowest likability ratings for the hostile sexist, with both the
ambivalent sexist. Secondly, we provided information on nonsexist and the ambivalent sexist falling in between. Thus,
the level of both BS and HS for each of the four profiles. when assured that a man’s BS is not accompanied by HS,
We thus used a 2 (profile’s HS: low, high) x 2 (profile’s BS: women will rate his profile as highly attractive, even as
low, high) within-subjects design, yielding profiles of a compared to a nonsexist man. Kilianski and Rudman’s
nonsexist (low HS, low BS), a benevolent sexist (low HS, (1998) findings, by contrast, would suggest an interaction
high BS), a hostile sexist (high HS, low BS), and an effect of the profiles’ hostility and benevolence, whereby the
ambivalent sexist (high HS, high BS). To keep the four nonsexist profile, which is low in both hostility and
profiles as parallel as possible, we did not use texts benevolence, would be rated as the most likable.
paraphrasing contents of the ASI, but instead presented With respect to the typicality ratings, we based our
men’s alleged answers to the ASI itself. Female participants predictions on the fact that hostile and benevolent sexism
thus saw ASI items along with the answers that each of four are typically positively related (Glick et al. 2000). In daily
particular male respondents had ostensibly given. They life, women should therefore interact more frequently with
were then asked to judge the four targets on scales ambivalent sexists than with either purely benevolent
assessing each target’s likability and typicality. With this sexists or purely hostile sexists. The ambivalent sexist
extended methodology, participants’ awareness of whether profile should thus appear highly realistic to women.
benevolent and hostile sexist attitudes typically coexist in
one person should be reflected in their relative typicality Hypothesis 3 Women rate the ambivalent sexist profile as
ratings: If participants believe that the two aspects of more typical than both the benevolent and the hostile sexist
sexism are usually distinct, as hypothesized by Kilianski profile.
and Rudman (1998), then the ambivalent sexist should
receive lower typicality ratings than both the benevolent Again, an opposite prediction would follow from
sexist and the hostile sexist. If, however, participants Kilianski and Rudman (1998). If women were unaware of
believe that BS and HS usually go together, then the the connection between hostile and benevolent sexism, then
ambivalent sexist should receive higher typicality ratings the profiles that are high in just one component of sexism
than both the benevolent sexist and the hostile sexist. (i.e., the benevolent and the hostile sexist) should be rated
Finally, likability ratings for the benevolent sexist might be as more typical than the ambivalent sexist profile.
relatively higher than those found by Kilianski and Rudman We further assumed that participants’ likability ratings
because of the explicit pairing of high BS with low HS. would be influenced by their own level of BS and HS (see
We set out to explore several hypotheses. In line with Kilianski and Rudman 1998). We predicted a special case
Kilianski and Rudman (1998), we assumed that women of an attitude similarity effect: People generally like others
would generally disapprove of the openly negative prejudice who share their attitudes (e.g., Byrne 1971; Newcomb
against women that is expressed in men’s hostile sexism: 1961). Furthermore, recent research dealing with women’s
reactions to BS has shown that women high (vs. low) in BS
Hypothesis 1 Women rate male profiles high in HS (i.e., the were more likely to accept restrictions imposed on them by
hostile sexist and the ambivalent sexist) as less likable than a man that were justified with benevolent sexist motives
male profiles low in HS (i.e., the nonsexist and the (Moya et al. 2007). In Studies 2 and 3 we therefore
benevolent sexist). assessed participants’ own BS and HS and performed
multiple regression analyses to predict likability differences
Furthermore, based on previous research on the percep- among the profiles from these variables. We made the
tion of benevolent-sexist attitudes (Barreto and Ellemers following predictions.
2005; Swim et al. 2005) we predicted that women would
generally approve of men’s more benevolent prejudices Hypothesis 4 The lower women’s HS, the greater is their
toward women: preference in likability ratings for low-HS male profiles
over high-HS male profiles.
Hypothesis 2 Women rate male profiles high in BS (i.e., the
benevolent sexist and the ambivalent sexist) as more likable Hypothesis 5 The higher women’s BS, the greater is their
than male profiles low in BS (i.e., the nonsexist and the preference in likability ratings for high-BS male profiles
hostile sexist). over low-BS male profiles.

Taken together, the independent main effects of HS and We further hypothesized that participants with high-
BS implied by Hypotheses 1 and 2, respectively, should yield feminist attitudes, who should be more sensitive to issues of
the highest likability ratings for the benevolent sexist and the gender equality (Morgan 1996), would recognize the
Sex Roles (2010) 62:568–582 571

harmful effects of both hostile sexim and benevolent States’ score is .769 (rank 18). On the gender development
sexism exhibited by men. Feminist women should therefore index, a gender-related version of the human development
show an even greater preference for low-hostile sexist men, index reflecting economic wealth, life expectancy, and
but less of a preference for high-benevolent sexist men. In educational level, the two countries’ scores are almost
Study 2, we assessed women’s feminist attitudes and used identical at .937 (Germany: rank 21 of 157; USA: rank 19)
them as an additional predictor in regression analyses: (United Nations 2008).
In Glick et al.’s (2000) cross-cultural comparison of
Hypothesis 6(a) The higher women’s feminist attitudes, the ASI scores in 19 countries, the German and US samples
greater is their preference in likability ratings for low-HS also showed highly similar results. Their factor structures
male profiles over high-HS male profiles. were parallel, and the mean scores for HS and BS were
similarly low, occupying neighboring ranks (15/16 and 13/
Hypothesis 6(b) The higher women’s feminist attitudes, the 14, respectively). Positive correlations between HS and
lesser is their preference in likability ratings for high-BS BS at the individual level were also present in both
male profiles over low-BS male profiles. countries, although they were somewhat lower in Ger-
many (men: r=.25; women: r=.31) than in the USA
In each study we also employed measures of equivocal (r=.44 for both men and women). If the magnitude of
egalitarianism, defined as the difference in likability ratings these correlations affects women’s perception of the
between the benevolent and hostile sexist profiles (see likelihood that both BS and HS may be present in the
Kilianski and Rudman 1998). Specifically, we tested same person, then the typicality of ambivalent sexism
whether equivocal egalitarianism would predict typicality should be rated as being lower by German respondents
ratings of the ambivalent sexist profile. A negative than by US respondents. This would make it less likely
correlation (Hypothesis 7) would conceptually replicate that our Hypothesis 3 would be supported.
Kilianski and Rudman’s findings. Overall, then, cultural differences in women’s perception
Finally, in Study 3 we assessed broader aspects of of male sexism seemed unlikely. To provide more direct
likability, including items assessing each targets’ short- evidence for this prediction, we conducted an initial study
term mate value and sexual attractiveness. This was done in which we closely replicated the relevant parts of
to explore whether men high in benevolent sexism might Kilianski and Rudman’s (1998) procedures with a German
be seen not only as nice and likable, but also as “sexy” sample. If their results were due to aspects of the procedure,
and of high mate value. The latter would be in line with as discussed above, then we should find similar results in
traditional views of romance (e.g., the “knight in shining this direct replication study.
armor”) and with research showing that benevolent sexist
attitudes are related to notions of paternalistic chivalry
(Viki et al. 2003). For the mate value and sexual Study 1
attractiveness items we entertained the same hypotheses
as for likability in general. Method

Consideration of Cultural Differences between Germany Participants and Procedure


and the USA
One hundred and twelve female undergraduates at the
Before testing our hypotheses with the proposed new University of Bielefeld (Germany) completed a questionnaire
methodology, we examine if any differences in results this in a large group session during regular class hours. Partic-
methodology might produce could be due to cultural ipants’ age ranged from 19 to 41 years (M=22.63, SD=4.31).
differences between Germany and the USA. Based on Where applicable, participants received partial course credit.
relevant national indicators and previous sexism research,
one should not expect large cultural differences. Germany Materials
and the USA are highly industrialized, Western societies,
and both are doing relatively well in terms of the United The questionnaire contained (1) the 22-item ASI (Glick and
Nations’ two main measures of gender equality. On the Fiske 1996; German version by Eckes and Six-Materna
gender empowerment measure, which reflects women’s 1999) with its subscales HS (Cronbach's alpha=.87) and BS
(relative to men’s) political and economic participation and (alpha = .90); (2) German translations of the three male
power over resources, Germany’s most recent score is profiles used by Kilianski and Rudman (1998, pp. 349–
.852 (rank 8 of 108 nations surveyed), and the United 350), each followed by a single-item favorability rating
572 Sex Roles (2010) 62:568–582

(0=strongly unfavorable to 6=strongly favorable); and (3) a moderate negative correlation with the covariation estimate
single-item covariation estimate, where participants indi- (r=−.32, p=.001).
cated the possibility that the hostile and benevolent profiles
applied to the same person (0=very unlikely to 6=very Conclusions
likely).
In sum, the relevant patterns of means and correlations in
Results and Discussion Study 1 were highly similar to those obtained by Kilianski
and Rudman (1998) in the USA several years earlier.
Profile Ratings Ratings of the nonsexist were most favorable, those of the
hostile sexist were least favorable, and those of the be-
The pattern of means for the favorability ratings closely nevolent sexist fell in between. With respect to the co-
replicated Kilianski and Rudman (1998). The nonsexist was variation estimate, German undergraduates were even less
evaluated most favorably (M=4.42; SD=1.16), followed by likely than their US counterparts to rate the hostile and
the benevolent sexist (M=2.94; SD=1.42) and the hostile benevolent profiles as describing the same person. Repli-
sexist (M=1.44; SD=1.13). A repeated-measures ANOVA cating Kilianski and Rudman, lower covariation estimates
(MSE=1.58) showed a significant effect of profile, F(2, 220)= went along with higher levels of equivocal egalitarianism.
156.08, and paired-samples t-tests revealed that all pairwise Applying the same methodology in Germany thus
comparisons were significant, all t>8.00, all p<.001. yielded almost identical results. Given this high degree of
correspondence between our data and those of Kilianski
Covariation Estimate and Rudman (1998), it would be quite implausible to
explain high typicality ratings of an ambivalent sexist
Participants’ ratings of the possibility that the hostile and profile (compared to purely benevolent or hostile profiles),
benevolent profiles applied to the same person were low and high likability ratings of a benevolent sexist profile
(M=1.96; SD=1.57) and significantly different from the (compared to all other profiles), which we predicted for our
scale midpoint of 3, one-sample t(111)=7.00, p<.001. The two subsequent studies, by cultural differences.
mean obtained in our German sample was even signifi-
cantly lower than that obtained by Kilianski and Rudman
(M=2.56; SD=1.99), t(210)=2.41, p<.02. Thus, partici- Study 2
pants generally indicated that the hostile and benevolent
profiles were probably not describing the same person. In our second study, we set out to test Hypotheses 1 to 7 as
described in the Introduction. We applied the new method-
Correlation Analyses ology of presenting four different ASI response profiles and
assessing each profile’s likability and typicality. To keep the
HS and BS scores were calculated by averaging across the information load manageable for participants, only ten ASI
respective subscale items. The mean for HS was 1.94 (SD= items (5 HS, 5 BS) were used to construct each target profile.
.86), that of BS was 2.36 (SD=.99). An analysis of partial The remaining 12 items (6 HS, 6 BS) were used to assess
correlations showed that participants’ HS scores (controlling participants’ own HS and BS, respectively. By avoiding any
for BS) predicted favorability ratings of the hostile profile, overlap between the profiles and items that the participants
r(108)=.38, p<.001, but not of the benevolent profile, answered, we tried to minimize potential effects of superfi-
r(108)=.15, p>.11, whereas participants’ BS scores (control- cial similarity, which might result from participants’ rating
ling for HS) predicted favorability ratings of the benevolent as more likable those targets that gave similar answers as
profile, r=.35, p<.001, but not of the hostile profile, r(108)= they had given themselves on certain items.
−.16, p>.09. These partial correlations are similar in mag-
nitude to those reported by Kilianski and Rudman (1998). Method
A continuous measure of equivocal egalitarianism was
calculated by subtracting the favorability rating of the Participants
hostile profile from that of the benevolent profile. This
measure was positive overall (M=1.52; SD=1.71). Equiv- One hundred and eight female undergraduates at the
ocal egalitarianism was unrelated to participants’ HS University of Bielefeld (Germany) volunteered to partici-
(r=.07, p=.48) but positively related to participants’ BS pate. Participants’ age ranged from 19 to 41 years (M=
(r=.35, p<.001); most importantly, replicating Kilianski 23.25, SD=4.17). Each participant received a free cinema
and Rudman’s results, equivocal egalitarianism showed a ticket or partial course credit.
Sex Roles (2010) 62:568–582 573

Procedure the top of each profile, a response scale with six options
was shown: “−3=do not agree at all, −2=do not agree,
Participants were seated at separate tables in a laboratory. −1=tend not to agree, +1=tend to agree, +2=agree, +
They first completed a questionnaire containing a 12-item 3=agree completely”. A handwritten number appeared in a
short form of the ASI and a 10-item scale measuring space to the left of each item; each profile was prepared in
feminist attitudes. Then they received a sheet with four a different handwriting to suggest that sheets had been
10-item ASI profiles that had ostensibly been completed by photocopied from actual men’s questionnaire responses.
four different men, and were asked to answer several Profile A showed agreement (all positive numbers) with
questions pertaining to each of these men’s likability and both HS and BS items; profile H showed agreement with
typicality. Later, they indicated their age and subject of HS items but disagreement (all negative numbers) with BS
study. Then they were thoroughly debriefed. items; profile B showed agreement with BS items but
disagreement with HS items; and profile N showed
Materials disagreement with both HS and BS items. The profiles’
HS and BS means in versions A and B differed slightly
ASI 12-item short form To assess participants’ HS and BS, (.2 scale points at most) because the alleged answers
we generated two 12-item versions of the German ASI were based on previous responses of a male student
(Eckes and Six-Materna 1999). Each version contained six sample such that high and low benevolence (or hostility)
items assessing HS and 6 items assessing BS. Version A of were represented by numbers that were one SD above or
the participant questionnaire contained items 2, 3, 6, 7, 8, below the empirical item mean, respectively, rounded to
10, 11, 13, 15, 18, 19, and 20; version B contained items 1, the nearest integer. Participants were instructed to take a
4, 5, 8, 9, 11, 12, 14, 16, 17, 21, and 22 (for item wording, few minutes to form an impression of each target by
see Appendix and Glick and Fiske 1996; note that items imagining how this person would behave in everyday
eight and 11 appeared in both versions). Each item was to situations.
be answered on a response scale from 1=do not agree at all
to 6=completely agree. Internal consistencies were some- Dependent Variables
what lower than the values reported for the full scale.
In version A, Cronbach’s alpha was .70 and .55 for HS Participants then answered several items pertaining to each
and BS, respectively; in version B, the respective values target. These were printed on a separate page for each
were .74 and .65. target, with targets appearing in the same order as on the
profile sheet. Participants were allowed to look at all four
Feminist attitudes To assess participants’ feminist attitudes, targets while answering.
we used a German adaptation of an 11-item short form of
the Liberal Feminist Attitude and Ideology Scale (LFAIS; Manipulation checks Two items were used to check
see Morgan 1996, p. 380). Some US-specific item contents whether the target profiles’ benevolence and hostility,
were adapted to the German context, and item eight was respectively, were perceived as intended (blanks were
omitted, which yielded a ten-item scale. An item example always filled with the letter denoting the profile): “How
was “A woman should have the same job opportunities as a benevolent is person __’s attitude toward women?” (1=not
man” (1=do not agree at all to 6=completely agree). The at all benevolent to 6=very benevolent); and “How hostile
scale’s internal consistency was satisfactory (Cronbach’s is person __’s attitude toward women?” (1=not at all hostile
alpha=.71). to 6=very hostile).

ASI profiles ASI profiles of four male target persons were Likability Two items were used to assess the target’s
constructed using those ten ASI items (5 HS and 5 BS) likability: “How likable do you rate person __?” (1=very
that participants had not answered themselves, yielding dislikable to 6=highly likable); and “Would you consider
two versions (A and B; see Appendix) that were randomly having someone like person __ as a romantic partner?”
assigned. The four profiles were presented together on a (1=certainly not to 6=certainly yes). The two items correlat-
large (29.7 cm×42.0 cm) fold-out sheet, two at the top and ed highly across profiles (r=.56 to .83) and were averaged to
two at the bottom. The ambivalent sexist, hostile sexist, form a likability index.
nonsexist, and benevolent sexist profiles were labeled with
different letters, here designated, for ease of presentation, Typicality Three items were used to assess the target’s
as “A”, “H”, “N”, and “B”, respectively. To control for typicality: “What do you think, how often do men answer in
order effects, four versions were used according to a Latin a similar way as person __?” (1=very rarely to 6=very
square design (AHNB, HNBA, NBAH, and BAHN). At often); “How typical, do you think, are the answers given
574 Sex Roles (2010) 62:568–582

by person __?” (1=very atypical to 6=very typical); and “In Table 1 Ratings of likability and typicality for four male target
profiles (Study 2).
my opinion, person ___ is ...” (1=definitely fictitious to
6=definitely real). The last item was presented on a separate Measure a

page, after participants had been informed that some or all


of the profiles might be fictitious. The three items showed Likability Typicality
moderate to high internal consistency across profiles
Target Profile
(alpha=.57 to .77) and were averaged to form a typicality
Nonsexist 3.30b (1.18) 3.13a (.84)
index.
Benevolent Sexist 4.62c (.95) 3.06a (.83)
Hostile Sexist 2.23a (.89) 3.81b (.69)
To ascertain that the likability and typicality items
Ambivalent Sexist 3.53b (1.05) 4.07c (.88)
measured separate constructs, we performed a factor
analysis for each profile, entering the two likability and N=108. Standard deviations appear in parentheses.
the three typicality items. We used maximum-likelihood a
Scales from 1 to 6, with higher values representing higher likability
extraction with promax rotation to allow for correlated and typicality, respectively. Likability was measured with two items,
factors (see Fabrigar et al. 1999). Results showed that the typicality with 3 items. Within each column, means not sharing a
subscript differ significantly (p<.02) according to paired-samples t-tests.
typicality items and the likability items consistently loaded
on separate factors, which were not highly intercorrelated
(r ranging from −.04 to +.33). 110.83, p<.001, supporting Hypothesis 1. The ANOVA also
showed a strong main effect of the profiles’ benevolence, F
Results and Discussion (1, 107)=123.22, p<.001, supporting Hypothesis 2. There
was no interaction effect, F<1. The female students thus
Manipulation Checks rated as more favorable the profiles low (vs. high) in hostility
and the profiles high (vs. low) in benevolence. All pairwise
Participants’ ratings of the profiles’ benevolence and hostility, comparisons among means, except for the comparison
respectively, were analyzed with 2 (profile’s HS: high, low) x between the nonsexist and the ambivalent sexist profile
2 (profile’s BS: high, low) repeated-measures ANOVAs. (p>.19), were highly significant (p<.001).
These revealed that, as intended, participants rated the high-
hostile profiles as higher on hostility (M=3.63) than the low- Typicality In line with Hypothesis 3, female students rated
hostile profiles (M=2.20), F(1, 106)=246.12, p<.001, and the ambivalent sexist profile as highly typical (M=4.07;
rated the high-benevolent profiles as higher on benevolence SD=.88), followed by the hostile sexist profile (M=3.81;
(M=4.56) than the low-benevolent profiles (M=3.13), F(1, SD=.69) and the nonsexist profile (M=3.13; SD=.84).
107)=188.09, p<.001. In addition, the high-hostile profiles The benevolent sexist profile was rated to be the least
were rated as lower in benevolence than the low-hostile typical of actual men (M=3.06; SD=.83). In the ANOVA,
profiles, F(1, 107)=238.10, p<.001, and the high-benevolent this pattern was reflected in a main effect of the profiles’
profiles were rated as lower in hostility than the low- hostility, F(1,107)=82.94, p<.001, and an interaction effect
benevolent profiles, F(1, 106)=124.30, p<.001. No interac- of the profiles’ hostility and benevolence, F(1,107)=4.66,
tion effects emerged, both p>.22. p<.04, but no main effect of the profiles’ benevolence,
F(1,107)=1.66, p>.20. Participants thus perceived profiles
Likability and Typicality Differences Among Profiles high in HS to be more typical of actual men than profiles low
in HS. Furthermore, they rated the profile combining high
The likability and typicality indices were each analyzed hostility and high benevolence (the ambivalent sexist) as
with repeated-measures ANOVAs, followed by pairwise particularly typical for actual men. All pairwise comparisons,
comparisons among the four profiles via paired-samples t- except for the comparison between the nonsexist and the
tests. Means and standard deviations for both indices are benevolent sexist profile (p>.54), were significant (p<.02).
shown in Table 1.
Effects of Participants’ Sexism and Feminist Attitudes
Likability As predicted in Hypotheses 1 and 2, female
students rated the benevolent sexist profile as the most likable To test Hypotheses 4 to 6, which predicted moderating
(M=4.62; SD=.95) and the hostile sexist profile as the least effects of participants' own BS and HS, as well as their
likable (M=2.23; SD=.89); ratings for the nonsexist (M= feminist attitudes, on their relative preferences for low-HS
3.30; SD=1.18) and ambivalent sexist profile (M=3.53; SD= and high-BS profiles, respectively, we conducted two
1.05) fell in between (see Table 1). The ANOVA showed a separate multiple regression analyses. The dependent
strong main effect of the profiles’ hostility, F(1, 107)= variable in the first analysis was participants’ relative
Sex Roles (2010) 62:568–582 575

preference for low-HS profiles over high-HS profiles; the Equivocal Egalitarianism and Perceived Typicality
dependent variable in the second analysis was participants’ of the Ambivalent Sexist Profile
relative preference for high-BS profiles over low-BS
profiles. Because of the within-subjects variation of the We computed a continuous measure of equivocal egalitar-
profiles’ BS and HS, we could define each of these ianism by subtracting the likability index for the hostile
preferences as a difference score in likability ratings. Note sexist from the likability index for the benevolent sexist
that these scores represent the main effects of profiles’ HS (see Kilianski and Rudman 1998). Higher scores on this
and BS, respectively, that were found in the ANOVAs, such measure represent relatively greater amounts of equivoca-
that the regression analyses test moderating effects of tion. If, as proposed by Kilianski and Rudman and stated in
participants’ attitudes on these ANOVA findings. Partic- Hypothesis 7, women with equivocal egalitarian attitudes
ipants’ HS (M=3.12; SD=.72), BS (M=3.56; SD=.79), and fail to perceive a link between BS and HS, then scores on
LFAIS scores (M=4.53; SD=.51) were entered as concur- the equivocal egalitarianism measure should be negatively
rent predictor variables to control for their covariation and related to ratings of the ambivalent sexist profile’s
test their unique contributions. The correlation of HS and typicality. A correlation analysis showed, however, that
BS was r(105)=.37, p<.001. Feminist attitudes correlated this was not the case: Equivocal egalitarianism was
negatively with both HS, r(105)=−.36, p<.001, and BS, uncorrelated with the typicality index of the ambivalent
r(105)=−.21, p=.03. sexist profile, r(106)=.10, p=.30. Thus, Hypothesis 7,
As shown in the top part of Table 2, the effect of which was based on findings by Kilianski and Rudman
participants’ HS on the relative preference for low-hostile that had replicated in Study 1 using their original
(vs. high-hostile) profiles was negative as predicted, but not methodology, was not supported when women had the
significant (β=−.10, p=.33); Hypothesis 4 thus received opportunity to rate an ambivalent sexist target directly.
little support. In line with Hypothesis 6(a), however,
women’s feminist attitudes did predict their preference for Conclusions
low-hostile profiles (β=.27, p=.008). As shown in the
bottom part of Table 2, only participants’ BS influenced their Female students rated the purely benevolent sexist as the most
relative preference for high-benevolent (vs. low-benevolent) likable of the ASI profiles and the purely hostile sexist as the
profiles (β=.35, p=.001), supporting Hypothesis 5. Contrary least likable. The ambivalent sexist and the nonsexist were
to Hypothesis 6(b), however, there was no evidence for a judged to be more likable than the hostile sexist but less
negative effect of participants’ feminist attitudes on their likable than the benevolent sexist; they did not differ from
relative preference for high-BS profiles (β=−.01, p=.90). each other in likability. Female participants’ likability ratings
thus reflect independent effects of the profiles’ levels of HS
Table 2 Results of multiple regression analyses predicting relative and BS; as predicted in Hypotheses 1 and 2, male targets’ high
preferences in likability for low- (vs. high-) HS profiles and high- (vs. BS and low HS each led to greater liking. Female students’
low-) BS profiles from participants’ hostile sexism, benevolent
sexism, and feminist attitudes (Study 2). judgments of the profiles’ typicality were affected by the
profiles’ level of HS, with high-HS profiles (H and A) being
Dependent Variable Predictora Betab rsp R2 judged as more typical than low-HS profiles (N and B).
Importantly, the typicality ratings also showed an interaction
Likability
effect of the profiles’ HS and BS, such that the ambivalent
Relative Preference for Low-HS HS −.10 −.09
sexist profile was judged to be most typical of men in general,
vs. High-HS Profiles BS .08 .07
whereas the purely benevolent profile was judged to be least
LFAIS .27** .25** .10*
typical. Hypothesis 3, which had predicted that the ambivalent
sexist would be rated as more typical than both the benevolent
Relative Preference for High-BS HS −.01 −.00
and the hostile sexist, was clearly supported. Furthermore,
vs. Low-BS Profiles BS .35** .32**
contrary to Hypothesis 7, typicality ratings of the ambivalent
LFAIS −.01 −.01 .12**
sexist profile were unrelated to participants’ equivocal
N=108. egalitarianism.
a
HS=Hostile Sexism; BS=Benevolent Sexism; LFAIS=Liberal These results suggest conclusions that differ from those
Feminist Attitude and Ideology Scale. drawn by Kilianski and Rudman (1998). These authors had
b
Beta=Standardized partial regression coefficient; rsp=semipartial inferred that women may be unaware of the link between
correlation coefficient; HS and BS. We argued, by contrast, that an unbiased test of
R2 =squared multiple correlation coefficient for regression model. women’s awareness of such a link would require a direct
** p<.01; * p < .05. comparison between nonsexist, benevolent, hostile, and
576 Sex Roles (2010) 62:568–582

ambivalent profiles of men. In Study 2, where women had Although these initial results based on judgments of four
an opportunity to compare and rate all four profiles, they ASI profiles are promising, we note a potential methodo-
seemed to be well aware that the ambivalent sexist profile logical limitation. In order to make agreement and
represents a typical male. Also, our female participants disagreement with items unambiguously visible in the
seemed to be aware of the fact that purely benevolent sexist target profiles, the nonsexist profile featured a pattern of
men are quite atypical in contemporary society. clear disagreement with all items. Participants might
Nonetheless, our participants judged the benevolent inadvertently have used this consistent disagreement as a
sexist profile to be the most likable of all profiles, and the heuristic cue in judging the nonsexist target, which may
ambivalent sexist profile to be no less likable than the have reduced that target's perceived likability ("someone
nonsexist profile. These findings suggest that women, in who disagrees with everything cannot be very likable").
spite of their being aware of the connection between BS
and HS, still fail to resist and criticize BS exhibited by men.
On the contrary, when presented with a man who— Study 3
atypically—exhibits high BS accompanied by low HS,
they find this man more attractive than a nonsexist who To provide further tests of Hypotheses 1 to 7, as described
exhibits neither high BS nor high HS. This is a remarkable in the Introduction, we designed a conceptual replication of
finding which we will follow up on in Study 3, using a Study 2. The methodology was slightly changed in two
wider range of attraction measures. ways. First, in order to counteract any heuristic-based
Contrary to previous findings (Kilianski and Rudman rejection of the nonsexist profile, all profiles now featured
1998; the present Study 1) and to Hypothesis 4, participants’ neutral filler items with which the target had ostensibly
own level of HS did not moderate their relative preference for agreed. Secondly, the range of items measuring likability
low-HS profiles. Consistent with previous research and with was extended; specifically, more items representing a man’s
Hypothesis 5, however, participants’ level of BS did mate value and sexual attractiveness were included. These
moderate their relative preference for high-BS profiles. additions were exploratory, as we did not formulate
Women who were themselves high (vs. low) in BS thus different hypotheses for different kinds of likability items.
preferred benevolent and ambivalent sexist male targets even Instead, we aimed at exploring whether increased liking for
more over nonsexist and hostile targets. Apparently, women’s benevolent sexist men would generalize to perceptions of
own endorsement of benevolent sexist attitudes poses an the target as a potential short-term mate or dating partner.
obstacle against recognizing men’s benevolent sexist attitudes This would complement work showing positive correlations
as harmful. That women’s BS had a stronger moderating between benevolent sexism and notions of traditional
influence on their evaluation of the respective attributes in courtship behavior (Viki et al., 2003).
men than did women’s HS may be due to the greater overall
effect of profiles’ BS (vs. HS) on likability ratings. Method
The moderating effects of participants’ feminist attitudes
on relative preferences for low-HS and high-BS profiles, Participants
respectively, only partly supported our predictions. In line
with Hypothesis 6(a), high-feminist women showed an One hundred and six female students at the University of
even greater relative preference for low-hostile men than Bielefeld (Germany) volunteered to participate in a series of
did low-feminist women, but contrary to Hypothesis 6(b), shorter studies that were ostensibly conducted together for
high-feminist women were just as likely to prefer high- reasons of economy. Participants’ age ranged from 19 to 47
benevolent men over low-benevolent men as were low- years (M=22.87, SD=3.79). Each participant received a
feminist women. Holding feminist attitudes thus seems to chocolate bar and was entered in a lottery for a 50-Euro gift
intensify women’s rejection of hostile sexism, although it voucher.
may contribute little to recognizing that men’s benevolent
sexist attitudes can be just as harmful. However, the short Procedure
LFAIS (Morgan 1996), which we used to measure
feminism, mainly assesses attitudes that oppose blatant Participants were seated at separate tables in a laboratory.
sexist prejudice and open discrimination; a desideratum for They first completed a 12-item short version of the ASI.
the future development of feminism scales would thus be to Then they received a fold-out sheet with the four ASI
include items that also capture women’s awareness of more profiles, and a questionnaire with items pertaining to each
subtle aspects of sexism, as expressed in men’s benevolent profiles’ likability, sexual attractiveness, and typicality. On
sexist attitudes and behavior. a final sheet, participants indicated their age and subject of
Sex Roles (2010) 62:568–582 577

study, and were allowed to comment on the study. After across the four profiles. They were thus combined to form
completing all materials, participants were debriefed. an index of typicality (Cronbach’s alpha across profiles=.66
to .77).
Materials The likability and sexual attraction items loaded on
either two factors (for profiles A and B) or a single factor
ASI 12-item short form The short forms used to assess (for profiles N and H). For exploratory reasons, we forced a
participants’ HS and BS were identical to those used in solution with two factors for all profiles and found that one
Study 1. Again, two versions were used; in version A, factor was consistently defined by the items “likable” and
Cronbach’s alpha was .79 and .72 for HS and BS, respective- “long-term partner”; as in Study 2, these items were thus
ly; in version B, the respective values were .67 and .66. averaged to form an index of likability (intercorrelation of
items across profiles=.64 to .78). The other four items
ASI profiles Each of the four male ASI profiles contained (“good-looking”, “one-night stand”, “passionate”, and
the same items as in Study 1 plus five filler items of neutral “attractive”) showed substantial loadings on a separate
content (e.g., “The Internet offers the opportunity for factor; they were thus averaged to form an index that we
women and men to communicate with people all over the called short-term sexual attraction (STSA; Cronbach’s
world”), which were interspersed with the ASI items. The alpha across profiles = .64 to .75). It should be noted,
fillers always appeared in positions 2, 4, 5, 8, and 11, and however, that the intercorrelations of the likability and
the answers marked always indicated agreement (twice +1, STSA factors were high across profiles (r=.48 to .66).
once +2, and twice +3). Again, four presentation orders of
the profiles were used according to a Latin square design.
Results and Discussion
Dependent Variables
Manipulation Checks
Manipulation checks The same items as in Study 2 were
used to check whether the target profiles’ benevolence and Participants’ ratings of the profiles’ benevolence and
hostility, respectively, were perceived as intended. hostility, respectively, were analyzed with 2 (profile’s HS:
high, low) x 2 (profile’s BS: high, low) repeated-measures
Likability, sexual attraction, and typicality Six items ANOVAs. As intended, participants rated the high-hostile
pertained to the target’s likability and sexual attraction: profiles as higher on hostility (M=3.57) than the low-
“How likable do you rate person __?” (1=very dislikable to hostile profiles (M=2.32), F(1, 104)=153.51, p<.001, and
6=highly likable); “Would it be conceivable for you to have rated the high-benevolent profiles as higher on benevolence
someone like person __ as a long-term romantic partner?” (M=4.53) than the low-benevolent profiles (M=3.08), F(1,
(1=definitely not conceivable to 6=well conceivable); “How 105)=271.51, p<.001. In addition, the high-hostile profiles
good-looking do you think is person __?” (1=not good- were rated as lower in benevolence than the low-hostile
looking at all to 6=very good-looking); “Would it be con- profiles, F(1, 105) = 114.62, p < .001, and the high-
ceivable for you to have a one-night stand with person __?” benevolent profiles were rated as lower in hostility than
(1=definitely not conceivable to 6=well conceivable); “Do the low-benevolent profiles, F(1, 104)=105.86, p<.001. No
you think person __ is a passionate man?” (1=not at all interaction effects emerged, both p>.27.
passionate to 6=very passionate); “How attractive do you
rate person __?” (1=not at all attractive to 6=very Likability, STSA, and Typicality Differences Among Profiles
attractive).
The three items pertaining to the target’s typicality were Participants’ likability, STSA, and typicality ratings were
the same as in Study 2. A minor change was made to the analyzed with repeated-measures ANOVAs and with pair-
response scale of the third item; it now read “In my wise comparisons among the four profiles. Means and
opinion, person ___ is ...” (1=definitely not real to standard deviations for the three dependent variables are
6=definitely real). shown in Table 3.

Forming indices based on factor analysis For each profile, Likability Recall that Hypotheses 1 and 2 predicted higher
we performed maximum-likelihood factor analyses with likability ratings for low-HS and high-BS profiles, respec-
promax rotation on the likability, sexual attraction, and tively. In line with these predictions, and replicating the
typicality items, allowing for correlated factors (see findings of Study 2, female students rated the benevolent
Fabrigar et al., 1999). These analyses showed that the three sexist profile as the most likable (M=4.57; SD=.92) and the
typicality items consistently loaded on a separate factor hostile sexist profile as the least likable (M=2.33; SD=.85).
578 Sex Roles (2010) 62:568–582

Table 3 Ratings of likability, short-term sexual attraction, and typicality for four male target profiles (Study 3).
a
Measure

Likability Short-Term Typicality


Sexual Attraction

Target Profile
Nonsexist 3.35b (1.06) 3.11b (.94) 3.65b (.68)
Benevolent Sexist 4.57d (.92) 3.52c (.85) 3.13a (.91)
Hostile Sexist 2.33a (.85) 2.50a (.78) 3.83b (.77)
Ambivalent Sexist 3.67c (1.07) 3.21b (.88) 3.68b (.80)

N=106. Standard deviations appear in parentheses.


a
Scales from 1 to 6, with higher values representing higher likability, short-term sexual attraction, and typicality, respectively. Likability was
measured with two items, short-term sexual attraction with four items, typicality with three items. Within each column, means not sharing a
subscript differ significantly (p<.05) according to paired-samples t-tests.

Ratings for the nonsexist (M=3.35; SD=1.06) and ambiva- least typical (M=3.13; SD=.91). The nonsexist profile
lent sexist profile (M=3.67; SD=1.07) again fell in between (M=3.65; SD=.68), the hostile sexist profile (M=3.83;
(see Table 3). The ANOVA yielded a strong main effect of SD=.77), and the ambivalent sexist profile (M=3.68; SD=
the profiles’ hostility, F(1,105)=102.99, p<.001, supporting .80) were all rated higher in typicality than the benevolent
Hypothesis 1. It also yielded a strong main effect of the sexist profile, but did not differ significantly from one
profiles’ benevolence, F(1,105)=150.91, p<.001, supporting another (see Table 3). Hypothesis 3 was thus only partially
Hypothesis 2. The interaction effect was not significant, supported, as the female students rated the ambivalent
F<1. The students thus again rated as more likable the profile as more typical than the benevolent profile, but not
profiles low (vs. high) in hostility and high (vs. low) in as more typical than the hostile profile. The ANOVA
benevolence. All pairwise comparisons among means were revealed main effects of both the profiles’ hostility,
significant (all p<.04). F(1,105)=17.48, p<.001, and the profiles’ benevolence, F
(1,105)=14.21, p<.001, as well as an interaction effect of
Short-Term Sexual Attraction Although, not surprisingly, the two, F(1,105)=7.14, p=.009. As in Study 2, then,
its means were generally lower, the STSA index showed a female students found profiles high in HS to be more
pattern very similar to that of likability. Female students typical for actual men than profiles low in HS. Whereas
rated the benevolent sexist profile as most sexually they rated the two high-hostile profiles (H and A) as
attractive (M=3.52; SD=.85) and the hostile sexist profile comparably high in typicality, they rated the profile
as least sexually attractive (M=2.50; SD=.78). Ratings for combining low hostility and high benevolence (B) to be
the nonsexist (M=3.11; SD=.94) and ambivalent sexist the least typical.
profile (M=3.21; SD=.88) fell in between (see Table 3).
Main effects of both the profiles’ hostility, F(1,105)=29.88, Effects of Participants’ Sexism
p<.001, and the profiles’ benevolence, F(1,105)=45.00,
p<.001, supported Hypotheses 1 and 2: The high-BS (vs. The means of participants’ HS and BS scores were 3.19
low-BS) profiles and the low-HS (vs. high-HS) profiles (SD=.81) and 3.78 (SD = .88), respectively, and their
were rated as higher in sexual attractiveness (see Table 3). intercorrelation was r(104)=.37, p<.001. In multiple regres-
An interaction of profiles’ BS and profiles’ HS also sion analyses, we tested if women’s own sexist attitudes
emerged, reflecting that the hostile sexist profile (low BS influenced their relative preferences for low-HS (vs. high-
/ high HS) was rated as particularly unattractive as a short- HS) and for high-BS (vs. low-BS) in terms of both likability
term mate, F(1, 105)=4.42, p=.04. Pairwise comparisons and STSA, as was predicted in Hypotheses 4 and 5.
among means were significant (all p<.01) except for the Analyses were analogous to those in Study 2, but only
comparison between the nonsexist and the ambivalent participants’ BS and HS were used as concurrent predictors.
sexist (p>.41). As shown in the top part of Table 4, the effect of
participants’ HS on their relative preference in likability for
Typicality Recall that Hypothesis 3 predicted higher typi- low-hostile (vs. high-hostile) profiles was negative as
cality ratings for the ambivalent-sexist profile than for both predicted, but not significant (β=−.10, p=.34). However,
the benevolent-sexist and the hostile-sexist profiles. As in there was a near-significant negative effect of participants’
Study 2, the benevolent sexist profile was judged to be the BS (β=−.20, p=.051), and the overall regression model
Sex Roles (2010) 62:568–582 579

Table 4 Results of multiple regression analyses predicting relative subtracting the score for the hostile sexist from the
preferences in likability and short-term sexual attraction for low- (vs.
respective score for the benevolent sexist. Recall that
high-) HS profiles and high- (vs. low-) BS profiles from participants’
hostile sexism and benevolent sexism (Study 3). Hypothesis 7 predicted a negative correlation of equivocal
egalitarianism and typicality ratings for the ambivalent
Dependent Variable Predictora Betab rsp R2 sexist profile. Correlation analyses showed that the two
Likability
equivocal egalitarianism measures were highly intercorre-
lated, r(104)=.76, p<.001, but neither measure was related
Relative Preference HS −.10 −.09
for Low-HS to the typicality index for the ambivalent sexist profile,
vs. High-HS Profiles BS −.20+ −.19+ .07* r(104)=.13 and .06, respectively, both p>.19. Hypothesis 7
thus again received no support.
Relative Preference HS .08 .07
for High-BS Conclusions
vs. Low-BS Profiles BS .35** .32** .15***
The inclusion of new items assessing aspects of physical
Short-Term Sexual Attraction attraction and sexual interest allowed us to differentiate two
Relative Preference HS .01 .01 liking indices in Study 3. The first index (items “likable”
for Low-HS
vs. High-HS Profiles BS −.20+ −.19+ .04 and “long-term romantic partner”) was identical to the
likability index used in Study 2 and combined general liking
Relative Preference HS .06 .05
with long-term romantic interest. The second index (items
for High-BS “good-looking”, “attractive”, “one-night stand”, and “pas-
vs. Low-BS Profiles BS .07 .06 .01 sionate”) was interpreted as reflecting the profiles’ short-
term sexual attraction for participants. A caveat is in place
N=106.
a
regarding the conceptual distinctiveness of the two indices,
HS=Hostile Sexism; BS=Benevolent Sexism.
b
because a one-dimensional interpretation of the six liking
Beta=Standardized partial regression coefficient; rsp=semipartial
items would have been feasible. We still found it useful, for
correlation coefficient;
exploratory purposes, to distinguish between a more general,
R2 =squared multiple correlation coefficient for regression model.
+ long-term liking measure and short-term sexual attraction.
*** p<.001; ** p < .01; * p < .05; p<.06.
For the likability index, the pattern observed in Study 3
showed remarkable consistency with the results of Study 2
was significant, R2 =.07, F(2, 103)=3.63, p=.03. Hypoth- (see Tables 1 and 3). Supporting Hypotheses 1 and 2, male
esis 4 thus received qualified support: Women’s own sexist targets’ low HS and high BS again each led to higher
attitudes did influence their relative preference for low- ratings of likability. Female students thus again rated the
hostile over high-hostile profiles, but this effect was mainly purely benevolent sexist as the most likable, and the purely
due to participants’ BS, not their HS. Furthermore, hostile sexist as the least likable. Ratings for the nonsexist
participants’ BS showed the predicted positive effect on and ambivalent sexist again fell in between, but in Study 3
the relative preference in likability for high-benevolent the ambivalent sexist was rated as significantly more likable
(vs. low-benevolent) profiles (β=.35, p=.001), supporting than the nonsexist.
Hypothesis 5. The effect of participants’ HS was in the The sexual attraction ratings, although generally lower than
same direction, but not significant (β=.08, p=.42). the likability ratings, showed parallel influences of the profiles’
The bottom part of Table 4 shows that there was a BS and HS, lending further support to Hypotheses 1 and 2.
marginal effect of participants’ BS on their relative The way in which women see high-BS (and low-HS) men
preferences in sexual attraction for low-hostile (vs. high- as “sexy” thus seems to be in terms of general likability and
hostile) profiles (β=−.20, p=.055); this paralleled the effect suitability as a long-term partner, as well as in terms of short-
on relative preferences in the likability ratings. No other term sexual interest. Remarkably, again women perceived the
moderating effects of participants’ sexist attitudes were benevolent sexist as more likable and as more sexually
found for the sexual attraction ratings, all p>.51. attractive than the nonsexist. Viewed from an evolutionary
perspective (e.g., Buss and Schmitt 1993), one may conclude
Equivocal Egalitarianism and Perceptions that displaying benevolent sexist behavior may have adaptive
of the Ambivalent Sexist Profile advantages for men in terms of both long-term and short-term
mating strategies (see also Li and Kenrick 2006).
We computed two continuous measures of equivocal Regression analyses showed that women’s own sexist
egalitarianism, one based on the general likability index attitudes moderated the effects of the profiles’ BS and HS
(as in Study 2) and one based on the STSA index, each time on likability and sexual attraction. Consistent with Study 2,
580 Sex Roles (2010) 62:568–582

it was participants’ BS that amplified a preference for males benevolent, or ambivalent-sexist way (for a methodology
holding benevolent sexist attitudes. Somewhat surprisingly, that might be adapted for such use, see Siebler et al. 2008).
there was a trend for high-BS women to be less bothered by If, under such circumstances, women still favor benevolent
men displaying a high level of hostile sexism. and ambivalent sexists over nonsexists, a clearer case could
Compared to Study 2, we note the relatively higher be made for their strengthening the status quo and supporting
typicality ratings for the nonsexist profile. It is possible the subordination of women (cf. Jost and Kay 2005;
that this profile in particular was rendered more realistic by Kilianski and Rudman 1998). Another realistic context in
the inclusion of filler items featuring positive response which it might be fruitful to pursue these ideas further is the
values. We had suspected that the rather low evaluation of study of human mating preferences and behavior (Buss and
the nonsexist in Study 2 may have been caused by heuristic Schmitt, 1993). If a mere set of ASI responses can elicit large
processing of the "negative" responses shown by this differences in the perceived sexual attractiveness of the
profile. However, although the inclusion of filler items did person who allegedly gave these responses, how much more
increase the nonsexist profile's perceived typicality, it did powerful must be men’s vivid displays of paternalistic
not change its relative evaluation compared to the other “chivalrous” behavior in daily interaction?
profiles. The benevolent sexist profile was still seen as We found moderating effects of women's own BS (Studies
clearly more likable, and now also as more sexually 2 and 3) on their approval of the target profiles' benevolence
attractive, than the nonsexist profile. Furthermore, the (and even on their tolerance for the target profiles’ hostile
ambivalent sexist profile, though less clearly than in Study sexism). Study 2 also showed that feminist attitudes may
2, was again rated as high in typicality. increase the preference for low hostility in men. We had also
predicted that high feminism would reduce women’s
preference for benevolent sexist males. However, this
General Discussion prediction was not supported by our data. These findings
may offer suggestions for potential interventions. The aim of
Taken together, our research conveys both good news and educating women toward approving less of male BS might
bad news. The good news is that women do not seem to be reached by questioning their own benevolent sexist
lack awareness of the empirical link between BS and HS, beliefs, whereas fostering more feminist attitudes may in
contrary to what previous research using a different itself not be sufficient. More optimistic findings in this
methodology may have suggested (cf. Kilianski and regard, however, were reported by Branscombe and Deaux
Rudman 1998). In two studies, female students rated (1991): Women whose feminist attitudes had been made
ambivalent sexist male ASI profiles as highly typical of accessible by asking them to complete a feminism scale
men in general, and rated purely benevolent sexist ASI reported lower intentions than women in a control condition
profiles as the most atypical. The bad news is that in spite to act in a way that invites male benevolent sexist behavior
of this awareness, participants still approved of profiles (e.g., waiting for a man to open a door for them; p. 413).
expressing high benevolence at least as much as they
approved of nonsexist profiles; and they judged a purely Future Directions
benevolent sexist profile even to be significantly more
likable and sexually attractive than a nonsexist profile. It would further be useful to replicate our studies in other
Thus, when women are assured that a man’s benevolent cultures. In the Introduction we addressed issues of culture
sexism is not accompanied by hostile sexism, on average and tentatively concluded that the USA and Germany were
they find him highly attractive, even more attractive than a quite similar in terms of gender-related attitudes and societal
completely nonsexist man. development. This insight, and the present Study 1,
Is this pattern problematic? Does liking benevolent sexists strengthened our conviction that results produced with our
but knowing they are rare birds strengthen the status quo of new methodology would not be culture-specific. Yet neither
male dominance? When women say they like benevolent Germany nor the USA are representative for the world at
sexists, do they approve of those men who blamed victims of large, and it would be exciting to see our new method of
sexual violence in research by Abrams et al. (2003)? Do they assessing approval for others’ sexism applied to different
support attitudes in men that lead to the kinds of remarks that research questions in different cultural contexts.
hamper women's performance (Dardenne et al. 2007) or A potential limitation of our methodological approach is
contribute to sexual harassment (Fiske and Glick 1995)? that asking participants to rate four target profiles in a fully
This appears plausible, but further research is needed to within-subjects design may exaggerate the perception of
corroborate such conclusions. For example, one might differences among the profiles. It would thus be worthwile
conceptually replicate the present studies in a more realistic to conduct conceptual replications in which independent
context in which men behave in a nonsexist, or purely groups of participants rate one target profile each.
Sex Roles (2010) 62:568–582 581

Finally, while the focus of the present investigation was used in version B of profiles; QA=item was used in version
on further developing the methodology to assess percep- A of participant questionnaire; QB=item was used in
tions of sexism in others, future research should pay closer version B of participant questionnaire.
attention to conceptual questions. For example, might the
fact that benevolent sexist men are viewed as rare 1. Egal wie erfolgreich ein Mann auch sein mag, ohne
contribute to their greater perceived likability? Although eine Frau, die ihn liebt, fehlt ihm etwas sehr Wichtiges.
in our present data, the ratings of typicality and likability (PA, QB)
were generally unrelated, several lines of research suggest 2. Viele Frauen versuchen unter dem Deckmantel der
that perceived scarcity of an object or attribute may increase Gleichberechtigung, besondere Vergünstigungen zu
its perceived value (e.g., Cialdini 1993) or enhance the erlangen, z. B. eine Bevorzugung bei der Besetzung
extremity of evaluations (Ditto and Jemmott 1989). der Arbeitsstelle. (PB, QA)
3. Bei einer Katastrophe sollten Frauen vor Männern
Conclusion gerettet werden. (PB, QA)
4. Die meisten Frauen interpretieren harmlose Äußerungen
Using ASI profiles as stimulus materials offers a useful and oder Handlungen als frauenfeindlich. (PA, QB)
well-controlled approach to studying women's perceptions 5. Frauen sind zu schnell beleidigt. (PA, QB)
of men's sexist attitudes (or, generally speaking, for 6. Man kann in seinem Leben erst richtig glücklich sein,
studying any group’s perception of any target’s sexist wenn man einen Partner hat, den man liebt. [Modified
attitudes). Using this approach to study female students' in Studies 1 and 3 to enhance clarity of meaning: Ein
perceptions of male targets' likability, sexual attraction, and Mann kann in seinem Leben erst richtig glücklich sein,
typicality yielded interesting new insights. Not only did wenn er eine Partnerin hat, die er liebt.] (PA, QB)
female students perceive the coexistence of benevolent and 7. Was Feministinnen wirklich wollen, ist, dass Frauen
hostile sexist beliefs in a single male individual as highly mehr Macht bekommen als Männer. (PB, QA)
typical, they also approved more of males exhibiting high 8. Viele Frauen besitzen eine Art von Ehrlichkeit, die nur
rather than low benevolent sexism. Most remarkably, when wenige Männer besitzen. (QA, QB)
a male target’s high benevolent sexism was explicitly 9. Frauen sollten von Männern umsorgt und beschützt
accompanied by low hostile sexism, women tended to like werden. (PA, QB)
this purely benevolent-sexist man even more than a 10. Die meisten Frauen sehen gar nicht, was Männer alles
completely nonsexist man. für sie tun. (PB, QA)
11. Frauen versuchen, Macht zu erlangen, indem sie
Acknowledgements Preliminary reports of this research were Männer immer mehr beherrschen. (QA, QB)
given as invited presentations at Pontificia Universidad Católica de 12. Jeder Mann sollte eine Frau haben, die er wirklich
Chile, Santiago, Chile (September 2004), the University of Halle-
liebt. (PA, QB)
Wittenberg, Germany (June 2006), the University of Basel, Switzer-
land (April 2007), the University of Magdeburg, Germany (May 13. Männer sind ohne Frauen unvollkommen. (PB, QA)
2007), the Universities of Cape Town and Pretoria, South Africa 14. Frauen übertreiben Probleme, die sie am Arbeitsplatz
(August 2007), and the University of Graz, Austria (March 2008). haben. (PA, QB)
We thank Caroline Erdmann for her help with the data collection and
15. Hat eine Frau erst mal einen Mann ”rumgekriegt“,
analyses in Study 1, and the "Astoria" cinema, Bielefeld, for
providing free tickets as incentives for participants in Study 2. dann versucht sie, ihn an die kurze Leine zu legen.
Thanks also to Friederike Eyssel and Nina Vanselow, who provided (PB, QA)
helpful comments on a previous draft. 16. Wenn Frauen in einem fairen Wettkampf gegenüber
Männern den Kürzeren ziehen, behaupten sie gerne,
sie seien diskriminiert worden. (PA, QB)
Appendix 17. Eine Frau sollte von ihrem Mann auf Händen getragen
werden. (PA, QB)
German Items Used to Assess Participants’ Benevolent 18. Viele Frauen haben Spaß daran, mit Männern zu
Sexism and Hostile Sexism and to Construct Male Sexism “spielen“, indem sie sich erst verführerisch geben, und
Profiles dann aber die Annäherungsversuche der Männer
zurückweisen. (PB, QA)
Numbers correspond to item numbers in Glick and Fiske 19. Verglichen mit Männern haben Frauen das bessere
(1996). No item numbers were presented to participants. moralische Empfinden. (PB, QA)
Abbreviations in parentheses pertain to Studies 2 and 3: 20. Ein Mann sollte bereit sein, sein eigenes Wohl zu
PA=item was used in version A of profiles; PB=item was opfern, um für seine Frau zu sorgen. (PB, QA)
582 Sex Roles (2010) 62:568–582

21. Feministinnen stellen an Männer vollkommen überzo- Fiske, S. T., & Glick, P. (1995). Ambivalence and stereotypes cause
sexual harassment: A theory with implications for organizational
gene Forderungen. (PA, QB)
change. Journal of Social Issues, 51(1), 97–115.
22. Verglichen mit Männern haben Frauen einen feineren Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. (1996). The ambivalent sexism inventory:
Sinn für Kultur und einen besseren Geschmack. (PA, Differentiating hostile and benevolent sexism. Journal of
QB) Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 491–512.
Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. (2001). An ambivalent alliance: Hostile and
benevolent sexism as complementary justifications for gender
inequality. American Psychologist, 56, 109–118.
Glick, P., Fiske, S. T., Mladinic, A., Saiz, J. L., Abrams, D., Masser, B.,
References
et al. (2000). Beyond prejudice as simple antipathy: Hostile and
benevolent sexism across cultures. Journal of Personality and
Abrams, D., Viki, G. T. N., Masser, B., & Bohner, G. (2003). Social Psychology, 79, 763–775.
Perceptions of stranger and acquaintance rape: The role of Jost, J. T., & Kay, A. C. (2005). Exposure to benevolent sexism and
benevolent and hostile sexism in victim blame and rape proclivity. complementary gender stereotypes: Consequences for specific
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 111–125. and diffuse forms of system justification. Journal of Personality
Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Cambridge: Addison- and Social Psychology, 88, 498–509.
Wesley. Kilianski, S. E., & Rudman, L. A. (1998). Wanting it both ways:
Barreto, M., & Ellemers, N. (2005). The burden of benevolent sexism: Do women approve of benevolent sexism? Sex Roles, 39, 333–
How it contributes to the maintenance of gender inequalities. 352.
European Journal of Social Psychology, 35, 633–642. Li, N. P., & Kenrick, D. T. (2006). Sex similarities and differ-
Branscombe, N. R., & Deaux, K. (1991). Feminist attitude accessi- ences in preferences for short-term mates: What, whether, and
bility and behavioral intentions. Psychology of Women Quarterly, why. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 468–
15, 411–418. 489.
Buss, D. M., & Schmitt, D. P. (1993). Sexual strategies theory: An Morgan, B. L. (1996). Putting the feminism into feminism scales:
evolutionary pespective on human mating. Psychological Review, Introduction of a liberal feminist attitude and ideology scale
100, 204–232. (LFAIS). Sex Roles, 34, 359–390.
Byrne, D. (1971). The attraction paradigm. New York: Academic Press. Moya, M., Glick, P., Expósito, F., de Lemus, S., & Hart, J. (2007). It’s
Cialdini, R. B. (1993). Influence: science and practice (3rd ed.). New for your own good: Benevolent sexism and women’s reactions to
York: Harpercollins College Publishers. protectively justified restrictions. Personality and Social Psy-
Dardenne, B., Dumont, M., & Bollier, T. (2007). Insidious dangers of chology Bulletin, 33, 1421–1434.
benevolent sexism: Consequences for women’s performance. Newcomb, T. M. (1961). The acquaintance process. New York: Holt,
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 764–779. Rinehart, & Winston.
Ditto, P. H., & Jemmott, J. B. I. I. I. (1989). From rarity to evaluative Siebler, F., Sabelus, S., & Bohner, G. (2008). A refined computer
extremity: Effects of prevalence information on evaluations of harassment paradigm: Validation, and test of hypotheses about
positive and negative characteristics. Journal of Personality and target characteristics. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 32, 22–
Social Psychology, 57, 16–26. 35.
Eckes, T. (2001). Ambivalenter Sexismus und die Polarisierung von Swim, J. K., Mallett, R., Russo-Devosa, Y., & Stangor, C. (2005).
Geschlechterstereotypen [Ambivalent sexism and the polarization Judgments of sexism: A comparison of the subtlety of sexism
of gender stereotypes]. Zeitschrift für Sozialpsychologie, 32, measurs and sources of variability in judgments of sexism.
235–247. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 29, 406–411.
Eckes, T., & Six-Materna, I. (1999). Hostilität und Benevolenz: Eine United Nations (2008). Human development report 2007/2008.
Skala zur Erfassung des ambivalenten Sexismus [Hostility and Retrieved January 5, 2009, from the United Nations Develop-
benevolence: A scale for the assessment of ambivalent sexism]. ment Programme Web site: http://hdr.undp.org/en/media/
Zeitschrift für Sozialpsychologie, 30, 211–228. HDR_20072008_EN_Complete.pdf.
Fabrigar, L. R., Wegener, D. T., MacCallum, R. C., & Strahan, E. J. Viki, G. T., Abrams, D., & Hutchison, P. (2003). The "true" romantic:
(1999). Evaluating the use of exploratory factor analysis in Benevolent sexism and paternalistic chivalry. Sex Roles, 49, 533–
psychological research. Psychological Methods, 4, 272–299. 537.

You might also like