You are on page 1of 50

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/336652618

LEARNING VOCABULARY THROUGH READING, LISTENING, AND VIEWING

Article  in  Studies in Second Language Acquisition · July 2020


DOI: 10.1017/S0272263119000494

CITATIONS READS
47 3,942

2 authors, including:

Yanxue Feng
The University of Western Ontario
1 PUBLICATION   47 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Yanxue Feng on 15 May 2021.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Studies in Second Language Acquisition

Learning vocabulary through reading, listening, and


viewing: Which mode of input is most effective?
Fo

Journal: Studies in Second Language Acquisition


rP
Manuscript ID SLA-RA-2019-0048.R2

Manuscript Type: Research Article

Incidental vocabulary learning, Prior vocabulary knowledge, Mode of


ee

Keywords:
input, Frequency of occurrence
rR
ev
iew

Cambridge University Press


Page 1 of 48 Studies in Second Language Acquisition

VOCABULARY LEARNING THROUGH DIFFERENT MODES 1

Learning vocabulary through reading, listening, and viewing: Which mode of input is most

effective?

Abstract

This study used a pretest-posttest-delayed posttest design at one-week intervals to determine

the extent to which written, audio, and audiovisual L2 input contributed to incidental vocabulary

learning. Seventy-six university students learning EFL in China were randomly assigned to four

groups. Each group was presented with the input from the same television documentary in
Fo

different modes: reading the printed transcript, listening to the documentary, viewing the
rP

documentary, and a non-treatment control condition. Checklist and multiple-choice tests were

designed to measure knowledge of target words. The results showed that L2 incidental
ee

vocabulary learning occurred through reading, listening, and viewing, and that the gain was
rR

retained in all modes of input one week after encountering the input. However, no significant
ev

differences were found between the three modes on the posttests indicating that each mode of

input yielded similar amounts of vocabulary gain and retention. A significant relationship was
iew

found between prior vocabulary knowledge and vocabulary learning, but not between frequency

of occurrence and vocabulary learning. The study provides further support for the use of L2

television programs for language learning.

Cambridge University Press


Studies in Second Language Acquisition Page 2 of 48

VOCABULARY LEARNING THROUGH DIFFERENT MODES 2

Introduction

Among the different types of input that L2 learners may encounter, viewing television and

movies, listening to songs, and playing video games appear to be the preferred sources of L2

input outside of the classroom for learners (Lindgren & Muñoz, 2013; Peters, 2018). For

example, Peters (2018) found that the most commonly encountered types of L2 input that L2

learners in Belgium were exposed to were television and movies, songs, and computer games.

She reported that 40% of the learners who were surveyed watched L2 television and movies

several times a week in their free time, whereas in contrast, only 1% of these learners read L2
Fo

books to the same extent. Moreover, in a survey of young L2 learners from seven European
rP

countries, Lindgren and Muñoz (2013) found that the amount of time participants were exposed

to L2 input through listening to songs and viewing television and movies was more than three
ee

times that of reading.


rR

Because exposure to L2 television, movies and songs might be greater than written input
ev

outside of the classroom, we might expect there to be a great deal of research on language

learning through these sources of input. However, surprisingly there are almost no intervention
iew

studies of L2 learning through viewing L2 television and movies and listening to L2 songs.

There are a small number of studies investigating these sources of L2 input which suggest that

viewing L2 television and listening to L2 songs also contribute to L2 vocabulary learning (See

Pavia, Webb, & Faez, 2019, for a study of listening to L2 songs; Peters & Webb, 2018; Rodgers

& Webb, 2019, for studies of viewing L2 television). There are several reasons why there is little

research investigating alternative sources of input apart from written text: historically they have

not been as commonly encountered in the classroom, they can be difficult to manipulate for

Cambridge University Press


Page 3 of 48 Studies in Second Language Acquisition

VOCABULARY LEARNING THROUGH DIFFERENT MODES 3

research, and they are typically perceived as forms of entertainment rather than learning. Another

reason why studies investigating alternative sources of L2 input are lacking might be that reading

is seen as the most effective form of input for L2 vocabulary learning (Krashen, 1989).

The value of reading for L2 vocabulary acquisition has been well established (e.g., Brown,

Waring, & Donkaewbua, 2008; Horst et al., 1998; Pellicer- Sánchez & Schmitt, 2010; Waring &

Takaki, 2003). The efficacy of other modes of input for L2 vocabulary learning is less clear. In

comparison to learning words through reading, there are relatively few studies investigating L2

vocabulary learning through listening (van Zeeland & Schmitt, 2013; Vidal, 2003, 2011) and
Fo

viewing (Neuman & Koskinen, 1992; Peters & Webb, 2018). The extent to which these different
rP

modes help to develop vocabulary knowledge in comparison to reading remains to be

determined. Research has indicated that reading leads to greater incidental learning of single-
ee

word items than listening (Brown et al., 2008; Vidal, 2011), but that there is no difference in the
rR

amount of learning of multiword items between these two modes (Webb & Chang, 2019). Only
ev

one study has compared vocabulary learning through viewing (with and without captions) and

reading-while-listening. Interestingly, Neuman and Koskinen (1992) found that viewing with
iew

captions led to greater word learning than reading-while-listening.

There are two main cognitive schemes that suggest that vocabulary learning through viewing

may be effective. Multimedia Learning Theory is based on the idea that there are separate

channels in the working memory to process words and pictures (Mayer, 2009). Dual Coding

Theory indicates that human cognition consists of two coding systems, a verbal system that

processes objects encoded in verbal modality, and an imagery system that processes objects

encoded in nonverbal modality (Sadoski & Paivio, 2001; Sadoski, 2005). Both theories suggest

Cambridge University Press


Studies in Second Language Acquisition Page 4 of 48

VOCABULARY LEARNING THROUGH DIFFERENT MODES 4

that presenting information in verbal and pictorial forms together can improve learning,

suggesting that learning through viewing might be superior to reading and listening. However,

there are no empirical studies comparing the three modes of input in the ways they are

encountered most frequently, that is, reading-only, listening-only, and viewing without captions.

Without any empirical studies comparing the extent to which the different modes of L2 input

(reading, listening, and viewing) contribute to language learning, it is impossible to know their

relative values for L2 learning. The primary aim of this study is to partially fill this gap in the

research literature by investigating the extent which audiovisual, aural and written L2 input
Fo

contribute to vocabulary learning. A full-length TV documentary presented in the three modes


rP

was used as the research materials. L2 television was chosen as the source of L2 input, because

in recent years, researchers have begun to advocate extensive viewing of L2 television both
ee

inside and outside of the language learning classroom (Peters & Webb, 2018; Rodgers & Webb,
rR

2019; Webb, 2015; Webb & Nation, 2017). However, if L2 television is to be used regularly as
ev

the material for language learning in the classroom, then it is important to determine the extent to

which it contributes to learning in relation to other types of input such as spoken and written text.
iew

A secondary aim of the study was to examine how frequency of word occurrence and prior

vocabulary knowledge influence vocabulary learning in these different modes of input.

Literature Review

Incidental Vocabulary Learning through Reading

Many researchers have argued that reading is the most important source of L1 vocabulary

acquisition (e.g., Jenkins, Stein, & Wysocki, 1984; Nagy, Anderson, & Herman, 1987; Nagy et

al., 1985). Although the vocabulary gains shown in L1 reading studies have been rather small,

Cambridge University Press


Page 5 of 48 Studies in Second Language Acquisition

VOCABULARY LEARNING THROUGH DIFFERENT MODES 5

L1 learners experience a rapid and substantial vocabulary growth during their school years

because they receive a large amount of written input (Elley, 1989; Nagy et al., 1987). Research

has also revealed the value of reading for L2 incidental vocabulary learning (e.g, Horst et al.,

1998; Hulstijn, 1992; Pellicer-Sánchez & Schmitt, 2010; Waring & Takaki, 2003). Incidental

learning occurs when unknown words are encountered repeatedly in meaning-focused input.

During reading, learners may gain knowledge of written form, part of speech, collocation, and

gain some knowledge of the word meaning from the context. Knowledge of encountered words

may accumulate as they meet the words in various contexts. Therefore, to increase the potential
Fo

for incidental vocabulary learning through reading, a large amount of written input is needed.
rP

One solution to increase the amount of written input is extensive reading.

Extensive reading is a language learning approach that involves learners reading a large
ee

quantity of materials for pleasure. Research has shown that extensive reading is an effective
rR

approach for L2 learners to increase their vocabulary knowledge (Horst, 2005; Pigada &
ev

Schmitt, 2006; Webb & Chang, 2015b). An obvious advantage of extensive reading is the

repeated occurrence of words across texts. Empirical studies have confirmed that repeated
iew

encounters with unfamiliar words are needed for L2 incidental vocabulary learning to occur

through reading (Hu, 2013; Pigada & Schmitt, 2006; Waring & Takaki, 2003). Nation and Wang

(1999) examined the vocabulary load of 42 graded readers, which are commonly used as

extensive reading materials for L2 learners, and found that the books at each level had almost

40% of the vocabulary at that level occurring ten times or more. This indicated the potential

value of graded reading material to contribute to vocabulary learning.

Research on L2 incidental vocabulary learning through reading other types of texts has also

Cambridge University Press


Studies in Second Language Acquisition Page 6 of 48

VOCABULARY LEARNING THROUGH DIFFERENT MODES 6

indicated that more encounters with unfamiliar words is likely to result in greater vocabulary

gains (Chen & Truscott, 2010; Rott, 1999; Webb, 2007). Various frequency of occurrence

thresholds have been suggested for incidental learning to occur. For example, Webb (2007) used

sets of short sentences as reading materials to control the frequency of occurrence of target

words. His results showed positive correlations between vocabulary knowledge and the

frequency of occurrence of target words. He suggested a minimum of 10 encounters to ensure a

relatively large increase in vocabulary knowledge to occur through reading. Chen and Truscott

(2010) used self-composed short stories as reading materials. They found that words that
Fo

occurred seven times had significantly higher learning rates than the words that only occurred
rP

once. On the other hand, in a study examining incidental word learning through reading a graded

reader, Waring and Takaki (2003) concluded that it may take more than 20 encounters with
ee

words for vocabulary knowledge to be retained. Taken together, the research suggests that there
rR

is not a threshold of frequency of word occurrence that can ensure vocabulary learning because
ev

some words are learned after few encounters while others are not learned after many encounters

(Saragi, Nation, & Meister, 1978; Webb, 2020). However, the more often that words are
iew

encountered during reading, the more likely they are to be learned.

Incidental Vocabulary Learning through Listening

Werner and Kaplan (1950) reported that listening is children’s sole source of learning L1

words before they acquire the ability to read. Research has also indicated that listening to aural

input can contribute to L2 incidental vocabulary learning (Brown et al., 2008; van Zeeland &

Schmitt, 2013; Vidal, 2003, 2011). Similar to reading, repeated encounters are needed for

learning to take place. For example, through listening learners may gain knowledge of the

Cambridge University Press


Page 7 of 48 Studies in Second Language Acquisition

VOCABULARY LEARNING THROUGH DIFFERENT MODES 7

spoken forms of unknown words, their grammatical functions, collocations, and derive their

meanings from the contexts in which they occur (Webb & Nation, 2017). The potential for

learning words through listening is also a function of the amount of spoken input encountered;

the greater the amount of input, the more likely that words will be encountered enough times for

learning to occur. However, the comparison between L2 vocabulary learning gains through

reading and listening has revealed an unfavorable position for listening. Brown et al. (2008)

compared the vocabulary gains occurring through reading, listening, and reading-while-listening

to graded readers. They found that both written and combined written and aural input modes
Fo

contributed to significantly larger gains in vocabulary knowledge than aural input. Vidal (2011)
rP

compared incidental vocabulary learning through reading and listening to academic texts. The

results indicated that incidental vocabulary learning occurred through both input modes, but the
ee

reading mode resulted in significantly higher scores than the listening mode in the study. Taken
rR

together, these studies indicate that the mode of aural input may be inferior to the mode of
ev

written input for incidental vocabulary learning. One reason why written input might contribute

to larger gains in vocabulary knowledge than aural input is that listening requires faster
iew

processing and it may therefore be difficult for learners to attend to unknown words in aural

input (Goh, 2000; Renandya & Farrell, 2011; van Zeeland & Schmitt, 2013). Vidal (2011) also

argued that written input has the advantage of enabling learners to dwell and re-read while

listening requires real-time processing.

Research on L2 incidental vocabulary learning also indicates that a higher number of

encounters with words is required for learning to take place through listening compared to

reading. Brown et al. (2008) found that words that were met 15 to 20 times in aural input only

Cambridge University Press


Studies in Second Language Acquisition Page 8 of 48

VOCABULARY LEARNING THROUGH DIFFERENT MODES 8

had a 3% chance to be learned, while words that were met 10 to 13 times had a 20% chance to be

learned through reading, and a 21% chance to be learned through reading-while-listening. Brown

et al. reported that words should be encountered more than 20 times in aural input for incidental

learning to occur. They also suggested that 50 or even 100 encounters may not always be enough

for incidental vocabulary learning to take place through listening. Van Zeeland and Schmitt

(2013) found support for this claim. Their results indicated that even when target words were met

15 times, there was relatively little learning that occurred through listening to aural input. This

suggests that L2 learners may need to receive a considerable amount of aural input for listening
Fo

to be a useful source of incidental vocabulary learning.


rP

Incidental Vocabulary Learning through TV Viewing

L1 research has revealed that children can incidentally acquire vocabulary knowledge
ee

through viewing (Oetting, Rice, & Swank, 1995; Rice & Woodsmall, 1988) and that it may have
rR

a positive effect on children’s vocabulary growth (Rice, Huston, Truglio, & Wright, 1990). A
ev

series of L2 studies have revealed that viewing may also contribute to L2 incidental vocabulary

learning (d’Ydewalle, 2002; d’Ydewalle & Pavakanun, 1997; d’Ydewalle & Poel, 1999;
iew

Neuman & Koskinen, 1992; Rodgers & Webb, 2019).

In fact, viewing television may offer the same potential for L2 vocabulary acquisition as

reading. TV shows are popular among L2 learners and are easily accessible. Surveys of EFL

learners’ out-of-class exposure to English showed that L2 learners watch L2 television a lot more

than they read books (Lindgren & Muñoz, 2013; Peters, 2018). Moreover, research indicated that

television programs provide repeated encounters with both high and low frequency words and

could thus potentially fuel L2 vocabulary growth with regular viewing (Rodgers & Webb, 2011;

Cambridge University Press


Page 9 of 48 Studies in Second Language Acquisition

VOCABULARY LEARNING THROUGH DIFFERENT MODES 9

Webb, 2010, 2015; Webb & Rodgers, 2009). Research on incidental vocabulary learning through

viewing television programs provides some support for this. Peters and Webb (2018) found that

L2 learners could learn 14% of the unknown words after watching one full-length TV

documentary. Rodgers & Webb (2019) investigated incidental vocabulary learning through

watching 10 episodes of a TV series and found that participants could learn 25% percent of the

unknown target words. Together, the research suggests that viewing large amounts of L2

television for pleasure (i.e. extensive viewing) could have a similar potential to extensive reading

to contribute to L2 vocabulary growth (Webb, 2015). However, as there have yet to be any direct
Fo

comparisons of learning between the two modes of input, it remains to be determined whether L2
rP

viewing could be as useful for word learning as L2 reading.

Neuman and Koskinen (1992) conducted the only study that compared incidental vocabulary
ee

learning through audiovisual and written input. Nine short segments of a TV program about
rR

science for children was viewed as part of a science class for bilingual children. In a between
ev

participants design, a control group read science textbooks, and three experimental groups read

and listened to the audio input from the videos, watched the videos without captions, and
iew

watched the videos with captions. Participants were tested on form recognition, form-meaning

connection, and meaning recognition of selected target words after the treatments. The results

revealed significantly higher scores for the captioned viewing group than those in the reading-

while-listening group on all three types of word knowledge test. Neuman and Koskinen’ study

provided empirical evidence that viewing might be a useful source of incidental vocabulary

learning. However, the extent to which the most common modes of encountering L2 input

(reading, listening, and viewing) have yet to be compared.

Cambridge University Press


Studies in Second Language Acquisition Page 10 of 48

VOCABULARY LEARNING THROUGH DIFFERENT MODES 10

For viewing to be a valuable mode of L2 incidental vocabulary learning input, words need to

be encountered multiple times and this likely requires viewing large quantities of television.

While research has suggested that in reading and listening input, the more that words were

encountered, the more likely they could be learned (Brown et al., 2008; Horst et al., 1998; van

Zeeland & Schmitt, 2013; Waring & Takaki, 2003; Webb, 2007), there are few studies

investigating the relationship between frequency of word occurrence and vocabulary learning

through audiovisual input. Peters, Heynen, and Puimège (2016) and Peters and Webb (2018)

both reported positive correlations between frequency of occurrence and language learning
Fo

through viewing. This suggests that a similar frequency effect may apply to L2 incidental
rP

vocabulary acquisition through viewing. The extent to which frequency of occurrence affects

vocabulary learning in different modes of L2 input has yet to be investigated. A secondary aim
ee

of this study is to help clarify this issue.


rR

There are two reasons why it is useful to investigate the extent to which frequency of
ev

occurrence affects vocabulary learning in different modes of input. First, examining the

relationship between frequency of occurrence and vocabulary learning allows us to better


iew

understand the extent to which different materials may contribute to vocabulary learning. This in

turn may help materials creators to design resources that more effectively promote vocabulary

learning. Second, this line of research should help to reveal the different amounts of input that

are necessary to promote vocabulary learning in different modes. Corpus driven studies have

looked at the extent to which vocabulary learning may occur through encountering different

amounts of input. However, the frequency of occurrence that may indicate that learning may

occur in these studies tends to vary from study to study (e.g., Cobb, 2007; Nation, 2015; Webb,

Cambridge University Press


Page 11 of 48 Studies in Second Language Acquisition

VOCABULARY LEARNING THROUGH DIFFERENT MODES 11

2010).

Prior Vocabulary Knowledge and Incidental Vocabulary Learning

Prior vocabulary knowledge has been found to have a positive impact on L2 incidental

vocabulary learning through reading (Horst et al., 1998; Tekmen & Dalog̈lu, 2006; Zahar, Cobb,

& Spada, 2001) and reading while listening (Webb & Chang, 2015a). The reason why learners

with larger vocabulary sizes may learn more words than those with smaller vocabulary sizes

could be because greater vocabulary knowledge is likely to yield greater comprehension (Hu &

Nation, 2000; Schmitt, Jiang, & Grabe, 2011; Webb & Paribakht, 2015), making the context
Fo

more accessible and help learners to successfully infer the meanings of unknown words (Liu &
rP

Nation, 1985).
ee

Surprisingly there is no research that has investigated the relationship between vocabulary

knowledge and vocabulary learning through listening. However, three studies have examined the
rR

relationship between prior vocabulary knowledge and vocabulary learning through viewing.
ev

Rodgers (2013) investigated vocabulary learning through viewing 10 episodes of a TV program


iew

with and without captions. He found significant correlations (r = .307 and r = .270 for the two

vocabulary knowledge tests used in the study) between prior vocabulary knowledge and

vocabulary learning for those who viewed TV with captions but not for those who viewed the

program without captions. Montero Perez, Peters, Clarebout, and Desmet (2014) also found

significant positive relationships (b =.12, b = .16, b = .01, and b = .02 for the four vocabulary

knowledge tests used in the study) between vocabulary size and L2 incidental vocabulary gain

when the scores of participants who viewed video clips with captions and participants who

Cambridge University Press


Studies in Second Language Acquisition Page 12 of 48

VOCABULARY LEARNING THROUGH DIFFERENT MODES 12

viewed video clips without captions were examined together. Peters and Webb (2018) found a

significant positive correlation (b = .028) between vocabulary size and vocabulary learning for

learners who viewed a full-length documentary TV program without captions.

Taken together, the research indicates that the higher learners’ vocabulary level, the more

likely they could learn new words through reading and viewing. However, the extent to which

prior vocabulary knowledge is related to the amount of vocabulary learning in different modes of

L2 input is yet to be examined. The current study aims to fill this gap in the research literature.
Fo
There are two reasons why it is useful to investigate this question. First, answering this question

sheds light on how individual differences in vocabulary size may affect vocabulary learning in
rP

the most common forms of L2 input (reading, listening, viewing). This in turn may help teachers
ee

to better understand the degree to which their students may learn words through input, as well as

which students are likely to need greater support for their learning. Second, answering this
rR

question can help guide L2 learners to select appropriate learning materials based on their
ev

vocabulary level. This may further increase learners’ motivation and self-efficacy for vocabulary
iew

learning.

The Present Study

The aim of this study was to compare vocabulary learning through reading, listening, and

viewing. A L2 television program was used as the source of the L2 input. Experimental groups

either viewed the television program, listened to the audio of the program, or read a transcript of

the language encountered in aural form in the program. Thus, a direct comparison of learning

vocabulary in different modes of input was possible by prioritizing control of input; participants

Cambridge University Press


Page 13 of 48 Studies in Second Language Acquisition

VOCABULARY LEARNING THROUGH DIFFERENT MODES 13

encountered the same input in each mode, with the only difference between the conditions being

the mode of input. The research should provide some indication of the extent of learning in the

different modes of input. This is necessary because if television is to be used in the classroom

regularly to promote L2 learning as some researchers have recommended (e.g., Peters & Webb,

2018; Rodgers & Webb, 2011, 2019; Webb, 2011, 2015; Webb & Nation, 2017; Webb &

Rodgers, 2009), research examining its efficacy in relation to reading and listening is clearly

needed.

Research Questions
Fo

This study aims to investigate the effect that input mode has on L2 incidental vocabulary
rP

learning. In particular, it aims to explore how written and audio input contribute to incidental

vocabulary learning in comparison to audiovisual input using a full-length TV documentary. The


ee

following research questions were examined:


rR

1. To what extent does incidental vocabulary learning occur through encountering


ev

written, aural, and audiovisual input? To what extent is vocabulary gain retained one

week after learning?


iew

2. How does vocabulary gain compare across written, aural, and audiovisual input?

How does retention compare among different modes of input?

3. In each input mode, what is the relationship between vocabulary learning and the

following two factors: vocabulary knowledge and frequency of word occurrence?

Method

Participants

The research was a quasi-experimental study in an EFL context with 76 participants ranging

Cambridge University Press


Studies in Second Language Acquisition Page 14 of 48

VOCABULARY LEARNING THROUGH DIFFERENT MODES 14

in age from 19 to 21. The participants were students majoring in English Translation at a

university in China. They were in four classes that were randomly assigned by the university,

with 21 second-year students being in one class and 55 third-year students divided into the other

three. Twenty-one participants were assigned to a reading group, fifteen participants were

assigned a listening group, twenty-one participants were in a viewing group, and nineteen were

assigned to a control group. Data collection took place during their class time and their pre-

assigned classes were used as the experimental and control groups.

The updated Vocabulary Levels Test (VLT; Webb et al., 2017) was administered in a paper
Fo

and pencil format to the participants to measure learners’ prior vocabulary knowledge. The test
rP

results indicated that all of the participants had mastered the most frequent 1000 words, 70

(92.1%) of them had mastered the most frequent 2000 words or more, and 46 (60.5%) of them
ee

had mastered the most frequent 3000 words. The total VLT scores of the were used as the
rR

indicator of their vocabulary knowledge in this study. Levene’s test revealed that homogeneity of
ev

variances was met. One-way ANOVA was used to show the relationship between the treatment

group and VLT scores. Results showed that there was no significant difference between the two
iew

variables, F (3, 564.43) =2.717, p = .051. This result indicated that participants’ vocabulary

levels were equivalent among the four groups. Table 1 shows the average scores of each group

and the participants as a whole on the VLT.

Table 1

The Average VLT Scores of Each Group and the Participants as a Whole

Reading Listening Viewing Control All participants


N 21 15 21 19 76

Cambridge University Press


Page 15 of 48 Studies in Second Language Acquisition

VOCABULARY LEARNING THROUGH DIFFERENT MODES 15

M 111.76 123.40 121.19 122.32 119.30


SD 15.22 12.42 9.79 18.63 14.41

Materials

The documentary, Why the Towers Fell (Kennedy & Klein, 2002), originally broadcast by

the Public Broadcasting Service (PBS) was used as the research materials. The program is about

the reasons for the collapse of the World Trade Center in the 9/11 attacks. The video was 54

minutes and 14 seconds long and the script contained 6240 running words. The script was

obtained from the PBS website (available at


Fo

http://www.pbs.org/wgbh/nova/transcripts/2907_wtc.html). The script and the vocabulary in the


rP

video were examined to ensure that there were no differences between the spoken and written

language. The script was analyzed using AntWordProfiler (Anthony, 2014) and the BNC/COCA
ee

lists (Nation, 2012) to determine its lexical profile (the proportion of words that are found in
rR

different word frequency lists). Results of the analysis showed that the most frequent 2000 word
ev

families plus proper nouns, interjections, and transparent compounds (e.g., airplane, firemen)

provided 81.65%, 90.30%, and 95.80% coverage of the most frequent 1000, 2000, and 3000
iew

word families, respectively.

Researchers have proposed different lexical coverage targets for comprehension, ranging

from 90% to 98%, depending on the type of discourse (Hu & Nation, 2000; Laufer, 1989;

Nation, 2006; Webb & Rodgers, 2009; Van Zeeland & Schmitt, 2013) or the benchmark on

which the comprehension level was based (Hu & Nation, 2000; Stæhr, 2008). Schmitt, Jiang, &

Grabe (2011) found a linear relationship between lexical coverage and reading comprehension

with reading comprehension tending to increase as coverage increased from 90% to 100%. Van

Cambridge University Press


Studies in Second Language Acquisition Page 16 of 48

VOCABULARY LEARNING THROUGH DIFFERENT MODES 16

Zeeland and Schmitt (2013) found that coverage levels of 90% and 95% tended to provide

adequate listening comprehension to L2 learners. In a study examining how lexical coverage

affected comprehension of television programs, Rodgers (2013) found a lot of variation in

comprehension among EFL students with different levels of coverage, and that many learners

with less than 94% were able to adequately understand episodes of television programs. In his

Input Hypothesis, Krashen (1985) suggested that in order for vocabulary learning to happen, the

input needs to be one level higher than the learner’s current level yet still be comprehensible. It is

important to note that because the measure of vocabulary knowledge used in this study (VLT)
Fo

uses a written format, it might have overestimated the participants’ knowledge of spoken
rP

vocabulary. This in turn may have provided further learning chances for the listening and

viewing groups. Based on these findings, we believed that the participants’ lexical coverage of
ee

the transcript was at a level that was appropriate for the participants in each group.
rR

Target Words
ev

Forty-three words encountered in the transcript of the TV documentary were selected as

target items. The frequency of the target words in the script ranged from 3 to 33 occurrences.
iew

Most of the target items (39/43) were words that were less frequent than the most frequent 2000

word level in Nation’s (2012) BNC/COCA lists. The target words, their word frequency levels,

and their number of occurrences in the transcript is shown in Table 2.

Table 2

Target Words and Their Frequency of Occurrence in Why the Towers Fell

Target Word (Word Family) Word List (1,000) Number of Occurrences

Cambridge University Press


Page 17 of 48 Studies in Second Language Acquisition

VOCABULARY LEARNING THROUGH DIFFERENT MODES 17

Strike 1 6
Steel 2 31
Flame 2 4
Trap 2 4
Collapse 3 33
Column 3 22
Core 3 21
Structure 3 16
Fuel 3 9
Fo

Aircraft 3 7
rP

Crew 3 7
Elevate 3 6
ee

Emergency 3 6
Jet 3 6
rR

Occupy 3 5
ev

Reveal 3 5
Concrete 3 4
iew

Component 3 3
Crucial 3 3
Destruction 3 3
Essential 3 3
Fragment 3 3
Initial 3 3
Severe 3 3
Terror 3 3

Cambridge University Press


Studies in Second Language Acquisition Page 18 of 48

VOCABULARY LEARNING THROUGH DIFFERENT MODES 18

Trigger 3 3
Evacuate 4 10
Ladder 4 10
Bolt 4 5
Vertical 4 5
Intact 4 3
Exterior 5 7
Ignite 5 3
Lateral 5 3
Fo

Lurch 6 3
rP

Truss 9 17
Stairwell 9 4
ee

Stalwart 9 3
Squeegee 16 4
rR

Skyscraper 33 6
ev

Sheetrock 33 3
Drywall Not in The List 10
iew

Fireproofing Not in The List 7

Instruments

The VLT. The VLT (Webb, Sasao, & Ballance, 2017) measures receptive knowledge of

form-meaning connection of 5 word frequency levels from Nation’s (2012) BNC/COCA word

lists: 1000, 2000, 3000, 4000, and 5000. Each level of the test contains 30 questions. Schmitt,

Schmitt, and Clapham’s (2001) suggestion of 26/30 correct answers or higher was used as the

Cambridge University Press


Page 19 of 48 Studies in Second Language Acquisition

VOCABULARY LEARNING THROUGH DIFFERENT MODES 19

cutting point for mastery of a level.

Checklist test. This paper and pencil test required participants to respond yes or no to

indicate whether they knew the provided words. Adapted from the yes/no EFL vocabulary test

designed by Meara (1992), this test consisted of 60 test items, the 43 target words, 10 words that

were expected to be known, and seven nonwords (See Appendix 1 in supplementary material).

Including 10 words that were likely to be known should have helped to encourage the

participants to complete the test. Analysis of these words was excluded from the results. The

seven nonwords were selected from the ARC Nonword Database (Rastle, Harrington, &
Fo

Coltheart, 2002). All the nonwords in the database are based on the phonotactic and orthographic
rP

constraints of Australian English and Standard Southern British English monosyllables, therefore

they looked and sounded like real English words. Nonwords were included to reduce the
ee

limitation caused by test takers overestimating their vocabulary knowledge or not taking the test
rR

seriously by ticking the words as they pleased. A “yes” response to a nonword was marked as a
ev

“false alarm”; a “yes” to a target word was marked as a “hit”. The proportion of words truly

known, p(k), was calculated using the formula from Anderson and Freebody (1982) and Shu,
iew

Anderson, & Zhang (1995):

𝑝(ℎ𝑖𝑡) ― 𝑝(𝑓𝑎𝑙𝑠𝑒 𝑎𝑙𝑎𝑟𝑚)


𝑝(𝑘) =
1 ― 𝑝(𝑓𝑎𝑙𝑠𝑒 𝑎𝑙𝑎𝑟𝑚)

A maximum score of 100% could be achieved on the checklist test.

The items were randomized across a pretest, immediate posttest, and delayed posttest. The

participants heard the spoken forms of the test items and saw their written forms to ensure that

Cambridge University Press


Studies in Second Language Acquisition Page 20 of 48

VOCABULARY LEARNING THROUGH DIFFERENT MODES 20

the testing mode was not biased against any of the treatment modes. This should have helped to

maintain the reliability of the data (Brown, et al., 2008). Giving all groups both aural and written

forms of test items provided internal consistency of the tests and may have helped to increase the

sensitivity of the test to reveal potential learning. The audio version of the test was recorded by a

North American English native speaker prior to the study. Each word was read twice with a five-

second pause between items.

Multiple-choice test. This test was a prompted recognition four-choice test with the key and

three distractors in the participants’ L1 (Mandarin, see example in Table 3). An “I don’t know
Fo

this word” option was presented as a fifth option to reduce the effect of guessing. The test items
rP

were randomly ordered across the pretest, immediate, and delayed posttests. The same 10 words

that were likely to be known and 7 nonwords from the checklist test were also included to reduce
ee

the potential that participants tried to intentionally learn items during the test intervals. The
rR

responses for these 17 words were not included in the results.


ev

Table 3

Example of the multiple-choice test


iew

Target word Steel

Options A. 塑料 B. 静止 C. 偷窃 D. 钢铁 E. I don’t know


this word
English A. plastic B. Being C. Take D. A hard, E. I don’t know
Translation inactive and without strong metal this word
quite permission material
Note. English translation was not provided in the test.

Procedure

All participants were told that the purpose of this study was to investigate the effect that

Cambridge University Press


Page 21 of 48 Studies in Second Language Acquisition

VOCABULARY LEARNING THROUGH DIFFERENT MODES 21

mode of input could have on comprehension. This was to encourage the participants to focus on

the content of the materials rather than paying deliberate attention to any unknown words during

the treatment.

In the first week of the study, all participants completed the VLT followed by a pretest

consisting of a checklist test and then a multiple-choice test. Participants were given as much

time as needed to complete the tests. After seven days, the participants completed the treatment

in their assigned groups in separate classrooms. Each classroom was equipped with a multimedia

system that included a computer, a projector, and speakers. Participants in the Reading Group
Fo

were each given individual copies of the transcript to read. The Listening Group listened to the
rP

documentary without the support of the video or transcript. The Viewing Group watched the

documentary without captions or subtitles. The reading group was given as much time as needed
ee

to finish the treatment. A posttest consisting of the same checklist and multiple-choice tests was
rR

completed by the participants immediately after the treatment. The control group took the
ev

posttest but did not complete a treatment. Participants were given sufficient time to complete all

treatments and tests. After another seven days, all participants took the same checklist and
iew

multiple-choice test again as a delayed posttest. The participants were given sufficient time for

everyone to finish the tests. This was followed by a ten-minute debriefing session, which was to

clarify the real purpose of this study.

Results

Preliminary Analyses

Descriptive statistics of the checklist and multiple-choice tests at three test time points are

presented in Tables 4 and 5. Cronbach’s alpha showed acceptable reliability for the tests, α =

Cambridge University Press


Studies in Second Language Acquisition Page 22 of 48

VOCABULARY LEARNING THROUGH DIFFERENT MODES 22

0.762. From pretest to immediate posttest, all experimental groups showed increased mean

scores on the checklist test and the multiple-choice test, while the control group showed a

decreased mean score on the checklist test and increased mean score on the multiple-choice test.

From pretest to delayed posttest, all four groups showed increased mean scores on both the

checklist test and the multiple-choice test. Inferential analyses were then carried out to examine

if the observed differences were statistically significant. The assumption of normal distribution

was met for all variables using visual inspection of histograms and Q-Q plots. Confidence

interval of 95% was used in all the analyses.


Fo

Table 4
rP

Descriptive Statistics of Checklist Test Scores


ee

Pretest Posttest Delayed posttest


Group N
M SD M SD M SD
rR

Reading 21 0.745 0.106 0.749 0.127 0.781 0.111


Listening 15 0.678 0.087 0.772 0.080 0.816 0.098
ev

Viewing 21 0.592 0.150 0.743 0.096 0.743 0.147


iew

Control 19 0.684 0.155 0.672 0.167 0.755 0.152


Total 76 0.674 0.140 0.732 0.126 0.771 0.131

Note. Maximum score is 1 (100%).

Cambridge University Press


Page 23 of 48 Studies in Second Language Acquisition

VOCABULARY LEARNING THROUGH DIFFERENT MODES 23

Table 5

Descriptive Statistics of Multiple-choice Test Scores

Pretest Posttest Delayed posttest


Group N
M SD M SD M SD
Reading 21 25.48 6.32 30.10 7.26 29.86 6.16
Listening 15 28.50 3.43 32.25 4.11 31.63 4.24
Viewing 21 26.05 4.94 30.29 5.16 28.76 6.17
Control 19 26.74 6.01 27.32 7.70 28.37 8.00
Total 76 26.49 5.37 29.84 6.42 29.61 6.36
Fo

Note. Maximum score is 43.


rP
ee

RQ 1: To what extent is vocabulary knowledge learned and retained through each mode?

To answer the first research question, Repeated Measures ANOVA was conducted to
rR

determine whether there was a statistically significant difference in the test scores among the
ev

three test time points associated with treatment. Time (pre, post, and delayed-post) was used as
iew

the within-group factor and Treatment was the between-group variable in SPSS. A Simple

Effects test with Bonferroni correction was followed as the post hoc analysis.

For the checklist test data, the assumption of sphericity was violated, as assessed by

Mauchly's test of sphericity, χ2(2) = 0.876, p = .009. Therefore, a Greenhouse-Geisser correction

was applied (ε = 0.889). The results of Repeated Measures ANOVA revealed a significant Time

x Treatment interaction effect on the checklist test scores, F (5.336, 128.068) = 4.888, p < .001,

ηp2 = .169. For the multiple-choice test data, the assumption of sphericity was also violated, as

Cambridge University Press


Studies in Second Language Acquisition Page 24 of 48

VOCABULARY LEARNING THROUGH DIFFERENT MODES 24

assessed by Mauchly's test of sphericity, χ2(2) = 0.889, p = .016. Therefore, a Greenhouse-

Geisser correction was applied (ε = 0.900). The results of Repeated Measures ANOVA revealed

a significant Time x Treatment interaction effect on the multiple-choice test scores, F (5.402,

129.654) = 3.455, p < .001, ηp2 = .126)

The results of the Simple Effects test with Bonferroni correction revealed where the

differences between Time were in each treatment group. The analysis indicated that for checklist

test the immediate posttest scores were significantly higher than the pretest scores for both the

Listening Group (p < .01) and the Viewing Group (p < .001), but no significant differences were
Fo

detected between the pretest and immediate posttest scores for the Reading Group (p = .889) and
rP

the Control Group (p = .677). Both the Listening and Viewing groups also scored significantly

higher on the delayed posttest than the pretest (p < .001), and no significant differences were
ee

detected between the delayed posttest scores and the immediate posttest scores in either of the
rR

two groups, indicating that the vocabulary gain was retained a week after the treatment. The
ev

delayed posttest scores of the Control group were significantly higher than their scores on the

immediate posttest (p < .05), indicating that some learning took place after the immediate
iew

posttest.

In terms of multiple-choice test data, the results of the Simple Effects analysis with

Bonferroni correction showed that for Reading, Listening, and Viewing groups, the immediate

posttest scores were significantly higher than their pretest scores (p < .001). There were no

significant differences between the pretest score and the immediate posttest scores for the

Control group (p = .463). Simple effects analysis revealed significantly higher scores on the

delayed posttest than the pretest for the Reading Group (p < .001), Listening Group (p < .001),

Cambridge University Press


Page 25 of 48 Studies in Second Language Acquisition

VOCABULARY LEARNING THROUGH DIFFERENT MODES 25

and Viewing group (p < .005), indicating that the vocabulary knowledge gained by these three

groups was retained a week after the treatment. No significant difference was detected for the

Control group (p = .065) from the pretest to the delayed posttest.

RQ2: How does vocabulary acquisition and retention compare across different modes?

To answer the second research question, one-way ANCOVA was conducted separately in

the prediction of Posttest scores and Delayed Posttest scores with Treatment as the between-

group variable. Pretest scores and VLT were entered in the modal as covariates, though VLT had

to be excluded because it has a significant correlation with Pretest scores (Pearson’s r = .32 for
Fo

Checklist test and Pearson’s r = .648 for Multiple-choice test).


rP

Before conducting ANCOVA, the assumption of homogeneity of regression slopes was

assessed by the interaction term between Treatment and Pretest scores when predicting each
ee

dependent variable. The results showed p-values > .05 for Checklist Posttest, Checklist Delayed
rR

Posttest, Multiple-choice Posttest, and Multiple-choice Delayed Posttest, indicating the


ev

assumption of homogeneity of regression slopes was met for each dependent variable.

The results of ANCOVA showed that there was a significant difference between the four
iew

groups on the posttest score after controlling for the pretest score both for the checklist test, F (1,

71) = 4.562, p < .01, ηp2 = .162, and the multiple-choice test, F (3,71) = 5.604, p < .005, ηp2 =

.191. The pretest score, as the covariate, was significantly related to the posttest score for the

checklist test, F (1,71) = 26.323, p < .001, and for the multiple-choice test, F (1, 71) = 173.594, p

< .001. There was no significant difference between the four groups on the delayed posttest score

on the checklist test (p = .280) and the multiple-choice test (p = .131), indicating that the

retention rate of the gained vocabulary knowledge after a week was not significantly different

Cambridge University Press


Studies in Second Language Acquisition Page 26 of 48

VOCABULARY LEARNING THROUGH DIFFERENT MODES 26

between the groups.

A Bonferroni post hoc test of the immediate posttest revealed that the Listening Group and

the Viewing Group both had significantly higher scores on the checklist posttest than the Control

Group, p < .05 and p < .01, respectively. There were no significant differences between the

immediate posttest scores of the Reading Group and the Control Group on the checklist test, p =

1.000. For the multiple-choice test, a Bonferroni post hoc test revealed that the Reading,

Listening, and Viewing groups all scored significantly higher than the Control Group on the

immediate posttest, p < .005, p < .05, and p < .05, respectively. No significant differences were
Fo

detected between the three experimental groups, indicating that the learning effect on incidental
rP

vocabulary acquisition through written, audio, and audiovisual input was not significantly

different. The complete results of the Bonferroni post hoc test on the immediate posttest scores
ee

are displayed in Table 6.


rR

Table 6
ev

The p Values in Pairwise Comparisons using the Bonferroni Post Hoc Test

Group Control Viewing Listening


iew

CL MC CL MC CL MC
Reading 1.000 .003** .326 1.000 1.000 1.000
Listening .039* .044* 1.000 1.000
Viewing .007** .008*
Note. *p < .05. **p < .01.CL = Checklist test. MC = Multiple-choice test.

RQ 3.1 The Relationship between Prior Vocabulary Knowledge and Vocabulary Learning

Pearson’s r correlation test was carried out to examine the relationship between the VLT

Cambridge University Press


Page 27 of 48 Studies in Second Language Acquisition

VOCABULARY LEARNING THROUGH DIFFERENT MODES 27

scores in the three experimental groups and their Posttest scores. When all three groups were

tested as a whole, the results showed that the VLT scores had significant positive correlations

with the Immediate Posttest and Delayed Posttest scores on both test formats. Significant

positive correlations were also found for the Reading and Viewing groups, but not the Listening

group. The results suggested that the more vocabulary the participants in the reading and viewing

groups knew, the greater their vocabulary learning gains. These findings are presented in Table

7.

Table 7
Fo

Pearson’s Correlation r on VLT scores and the Test Scores


rP

Group Checklist test Multiple-choice test


ee

Immediate Immediate
Delayed posttest Delayed posttest
posttest posttest
rR

Reading (n = 21) .601 (p < .005) .511 (p < .05) .455 (p < .05) .420 (p = .058)
Listening (n = 15) .305 (p = .268) -.079 (p = .780) .291 (p = .293) .126 (p = .654)
ev

Viewing (n = 21) .391 (p = .080) .482 (p < .05) .706 (p < .001) .567 (p < .005)
iew

All experimental
.462 (p <.001) .322 (p <.01) .480 (p < .001) .383 (p <.005)
groups (N = 57)

RQ 3.2 The Relationship between Frequency of Occurrence and Vocabulary Learning

In order to analyze the relationship between incidental vocabulary learning and frequency of

occurrence, the proportion of participants who learned each word (learning rate) was used as the

indicator of vocabulary learning. The average frequency of occurrence was 7.44 with SD =

Cambridge University Press


Studies in Second Language Acquisition Page 28 of 48

VOCABULARY LEARNING THROUGH DIFFERENT MODES 28

7.196. The assumption of normal distribution was met for all variables by visual inspection of

histograms and Q-Q plots. Pearson’s r was used to investigate the correlation between frequency

of occurrence and the learning rate of each word. Results did not find any statistically significant

correlations between the two variables in any group on either of the tests. Descriptive Statistics

and the results of the Pearson’s r correlation test are displayed in Table 8 and Table 9.

Table 8

Descriptive Statistics for the Proportion of Participants Who Learned the Word

All Experimental
Fo
Reading Listening Viewing
Groups
M SD n M SD n M SD n M SD N
rP

Checklist Test 0.635 0.326 39 0.500 0.412 42 0.625 0.317 40 0.600 0.268 43
ee

Multiple-
0.431 0.290 42 0.540 0.371 40 0.424 0.325 42 0.492 0.220 43
choice Test
Note. The numbers of target words varied because there were words that were known by all the
rR

participants in the pretest in each group. These words were marked as missing when analysis was
ev

carried out in SPSS.


iew

Table 9

Pearson’s Correlation r on frequency of occurrence and the learning rate of each word

Checklist test Multiple-choice test


Viewin All Viewin All
Reading Listening Reading Listening
g groups g groups
Frequency r .071 .057 .124 .194 .235 -.050 -.038 .247
of p .668 .721 .445 .213 .135 .758 .809 .111
occurrence n 39 42 40 43 42 40 42 43
Note. The numbers of target words varied because there were words that were known by all the

participants in the pretest in each group. These words were marked as missing when analysis was

Cambridge University Press


Page 29 of 48 Studies in Second Language Acquisition

VOCABULARY LEARNING THROUGH DIFFERENT MODES 29

carried out in SPSS.

Discussion

This study fills research gaps in two ways. It is the first study to compare incidental

vocabulary learning through written, audio, and audiovisual input. Several studies have

compared written and audio input but did not include audiovisual input (e.g., Brown et al., 2008;

Vidal, 2011; Webb & Chang, 2012). Only one study has compared audiovisual input and

reading-while-listening but did not include written-only and audio-only input (Neuman &

Koskinen, 1992). This study is also the first to use a full-length TV documentary to compare the
Fo

incidental learning outcome through different modes of input. Earlier studies either used short
rP

video segments as viewing material or listening input (Neuman & Koskinen, 1992; Vidal, 2011),

or simplified texts such as graded readers as reading input (Brown et al., 2008). Therefore, the
ee

present study should provide greater evidence to illustrate the extent to which each mode of input
rR

could contribute to incidental vocabulary learning.


ev

Incidental Vocabulary Learning in each Mode of Input

In answer to the first research question, the results show that significant vocabulary learning
iew

can occur through encountering written, audio, and audiovisual input. The results were consistent

with the findings of earlier research where vocabulary knowledge was gained incidentally

through reading written input (e.g, Nagy, et al., 1985; Waring & Takaki, 2003), listening to audio

input (e.g., Elley, 1989; van Zeeland & Schmitt, 2013; Vidal, 2003) and through viewing

audiovisual input (e.g., Montero Perez, et al., 2014; Neuman & Koskinen, 1992; Peters & Webb,

2018). In terms of retention, the learned vocabulary knowledge was retained one week after the

treatment in all three learning conditions. This is consistent with Brown et al.’s (2008) findings

Cambridge University Press


Studies in Second Language Acquisition Page 30 of 48

VOCABULARY LEARNING THROUGH DIFFERENT MODES 30

for retention of vocabulary knowledge in reading-only and listening-only modes on a one-week

delayed posttest.

Apart from supporting the beneficial effects of the more widely researched modes of input

(i.e., reading and listening) for incidental vocabulary learning, the results also provided more

evidence in support of learning vocabulary through viewing L2 television, a mode of input that

learners appear to be highly motivated to learn with (Gieve & Clark, 2005; Rodgers, 2013). This

is particularly useful because many earlier studies have investigated incidental vocabulary

learning through viewing specialized materials such as academic speeches (e.g., Vidal, 2003,
Fo

2011), and short segments of videos (e.g., Montero Perez et al., 2014; Sydorenko, 2010), which
rP

might be considered to be less common forms of viewing. The finding in this study showing that

viewing L2 television contributes to incidental vocabulary learning is consistent with Peters and
ee

Webb’s (2018) showing that EFL learners in Belgium incidentally learned words through
rR

watching a one-hour TV documentary.


ev

The Comparison of Vocabulary Learning across Different Modes of Input

In answer to the second research question, the results indicated that reading, listening, and
iew

viewing were equally effective for incidental vocabulary learning. This suggests that second

language TV programs could contribute to similar amounts of vocabulary learning no matter

what kind of input the learners choose to learn from and indicates that viewing L2 television

should be considered a useful alternative or supplement to reading. Reading graded readers has

been widely advocated as the key resource for L2 vocabulary learning (e.g., Nation, 2001;

Pigada & Schmitt, 2006; Webb & Macalister, 2013). Because research suggests that L2 learners

tend to watch L2 television more than read L2 text as a recreational activity, the results provide

Cambridge University Press


Page 31 of 48 Studies in Second Language Acquisition

VOCABULARY LEARNING THROUGH DIFFERENT MODES 31

powerful evidence in support of the use of L2 television programs for lexical development.

It might be considered surprising that audiovisual input did not lead to superior vocabulary

gain to the other input modes because Multimedia Learning Theory (Mayer, 2009) and Dual-

coding theory (Sadoski & Paivio, 2001) suggest that multimodal input should result in greater

vocabulary learning than unimodal input. One explanation for this contrast is that the participants

may not have had as much experience learning in the viewing mode in comparison to learning

through reading and listening, because L2 television is not commonly used for learning in EFL

classrooms in China while the latter two modes are used for learning on a daily basis.
Fo

The finding that listening and reading led to similar vocabulary learning gains contrasts
rP

earlier studies that compared vocabulary learning through reading and listening. Brown et al.

(2008) found that EFL learners learned more words incidentally through reading graded readers
ee

than listening to graded readers. Vidal (2011) found that students in an ESP program made
rR

greater vocabulary gain through reading academic texts compared to listening to academic
ev

lectures. The difference in findings between the present study and the earlier two studies

suggests that more research comparing these two modes of input is warranted.
iew

The Relationship between Vocabulary Learning and Frequency of Occurrence

In answer to the third research question, the results indicated that frequency of occurrence

was not significantly related to incidental vocabulary learning in any of the three modes. This

contrasts earlier findings indicating that a word is more likely to be learned if encountered more

times in written texts (Brown et al., 2008; Waring & Takaki, 2003), in spoken texts (Brown et

al., 2008; Vidal, 2011), and in viewing materials (Peters & Webb, 2018; Rodgers & Webb,

2019).

Cambridge University Press


Studies in Second Language Acquisition Page 32 of 48

VOCABULARY LEARNING THROUGH DIFFERENT MODES 32

There are several reasons for the inconsistency in findings. First, the material was originally

designed to present information with both audio and visual support, making it less ecologically

appropriate to serve as reading material alone. The lack of visual support in reading mode could

lead to the material becoming less informative and interesting. Overlap between the occurrence

of images in the video and the use of the word in the audio (or written input in the case of

captions) would likely increase the potential that the meanings of unknown words are learned

(Rodgers, 2018). The lack of visual support may have also caused the participants to become less

engaged and pay less attention to the unknown words, thus reducing the impact of frequency of
Fo

occurrence on learning. As for listening, the results are in line with van Zeeland and Schmitt
rP

(2013)’s finding that the relationship between frequency of occurrence and listening was not

clear. Perhaps other factors such as salience and relevance were more important than the
ee

frequency of word occurrence in the non-reading modes, given how densely the information is
rR

provided in a documentary. Ellis (2006) suggests that words may need more salient cues in the
ev

contexts around them to arouse the learning attention. For example, Vidal (2011) argued that

when encountering cognates, words that are similar to L1 words, it is easier to notice them in
iew

audio input than in written input. As much as repeated encounters are needed, salience may also

be necessary for successful guessing from context to happen.

The Relationship between Vocabulary Learning and Prior Vocabulary Knowledge

The analysis revealed that there were significant relationships between learners’ prior

vocabulary knowledge and incidental vocabulary learning for the reading and viewing groups, as

well as the three groups combined. This is supported by earlier studies that have found positive

correlations between prior vocabulary knowledge and vocabulary gain through reading (Horst et

Cambridge University Press


Page 33 of 48 Studies in Second Language Acquisition

VOCABULARY LEARNING THROUGH DIFFERENT MODES 33

al., 1998; Tekmen & Dalog̈lu, 2006; Zahar, Cobb, & Spada, 2001) and viewing (Peters & Webb,

2018). The result for viewing contrasts Rodgers & Webb (2019) study which found no

significant correlation between the two factors for EFL learners who viewed 10 episodes of a TV

drama. The variation in findings suggests that there may be other influential factors affecting

incidental vocabulary learning through viewing and perhaps impacted the results more strongly

than prior vocabulary knowledge in the earlier study. Research suggests that learner variables

such as L2 proficiency, age (primary, secondary, postsecondary) and treatment conditions such

as text type, intended text audience, and lexical coverage may moderate incidental vocabulary
Fo

learning (Webb, Uchihara, & Yanagisawa, 2019). For example, Vidal (2003) suggested that
rP

students with higher levels of English proficiency tend to acquire more vocabulary knowledge

through listening. In terms of the current study, participants with higher vocabulary levels may
ee

not necessarily have higher English proficiency levels because the latter is a superordinate
rR

concept of which vocabulary knowledge is just one aspect. It would be useful to investigate
ev

which different aspects of language skills other than prior vocabulary knowledge affect

incidental vocabulary learning in a follow-up study.


iew

The lack of a significant correlation between prior vocabulary knowledge and word learning

for the listening group might be explained in two ways. First, it may be that because the VLT

measures knowledge of written form-meaning connections, these results do not accurately

represent prior vocabulary knowledge of the spoken form-meaning connections of words.

Research has revealed that knowledge of spoken form-meaning connections and written form-

meaning connections varies (Milton & Hopkins, 2006). It may be that variation between prior

knowledge of spoken and written form-meaning connections make VLT scores too imprecise to

Cambridge University Press


Studies in Second Language Acquisition Page 34 of 48

VOCABULARY LEARNING THROUGH DIFFERENT MODES 34

reveal a relationship between prior knowledge and learning for listening. It would be useful to

compare the relationships between knowledge of spoken and written form-meaning connections

and learning words through listening in a follow-up study.

A second explanation for the lack of significant correlation is that perhaps other factors such

as familiarity with L2 spoken input, speech rate, and the amount of connected speech have a

greater impact on vocabulary learning through listening than prior vocabulary knowledge.

Investigating the extent to which different factors are correlated with vocabulary learning

through listening would be a useful follow up to the present study.


Fo

Pedagogical Implications
rP

Pedagogically, the findings suggest that there is value in learning in all three of the input

modes examined in this study. In the classroom, it would be useful for teachers to familiarize
ee

their students with a range of modes of input and help to support their reading, listening, and
rR

viewing comprehension. This may have the dual effect of revealing to students which mode of
ev

input best suits their learning preference, and encourage students to learn with their choice of

input outside of the classroom. Relatively few words are likely to be learned incidentally within
iew

the classroom, because large amounts of input are necessary for many words to be learned

through encountering meaning-focused input. However, if students encounter large amounts of

input on their own outside of the classroom, there is the potential for meaning-focused input to

fuel L2 lexical development (Webb & Nation, 2017). Moreover, in contrast to a great deal of

literature claiming that reading is the primary contributor to lexical development (e.g., Krashen

1985; Nagy, Herman, & Anderson, 1985), the findings suggest that vocabulary growth may not

depend on extensive reading. The present study provides further empirical evidence to support

Cambridge University Press


Page 35 of 48 Studies in Second Language Acquisition

VOCABULARY LEARNING THROUGH DIFFERENT MODES 35

the positive impact that extensive listening and extensive viewing may have on vocabulary

development.

Future Directions

One limitation of this study is that while the audiovisual material had ecologically validity

(L2 television is commonly viewed by learners), the reading and listening materials were less

common forms of spoken and written input. The documentary was designed to come with audio

input and imagery support. This could thus have a positive effect on learning in the audiovisual

mode, yet the script alone and the audio alone may lack contextual information that may
Fo

encourage attention or help understanding of unknown words. However, the fact that vocabulary
rP

learning still occurred in the reading and listening modes suggests that perhaps any negative

effect of the materials may have been minimal. Ecological validity is likely to be an issue in any
ee

study that compares different modes of input that include viewing, because learners are unlikely
rR

to encounter the same language for the same content in audiovisual and written modes. In her
ev

comparison of vocabulary learning from reading and listening, Vidal (2011) tried to deal with

this by controlling content, but not language. However, in this case, it cannot be clear whether
iew

words are learned because of the mode or the linguistic variables that moderate learning such as

the presence of more contextual clues in one mode. More research comparing learning through

different modes of input is clearly necessary in order to have a more precise understanding of

how mode influences learning.

It would also be useful for future research to compare incidental vocabulary learning through

viewing a full-length TV program with and without captions as there are a large number of

English TV programs with captions available now. It is very common for L2 learners to access

Cambridge University Press


Studies in Second Language Acquisition Page 36 of 48

VOCABULARY LEARNING THROUGH DIFFERENT MODES 36

these viewing materials through various online platforms, and it could be easy to utilize these

materials as extensive viewing input for vocabulary development. Rodgers (2013) is the only

study to examine vocabulary learning through viewing complete TV programs with and without

captions. However, that study involved viewing 10 episodes of one program over 13 weeks and

so the degree to which learning occurs through viewing an individual program remains to be

determined. Investigating whether captions increase vocabulary learning through viewing full

length TV programs would expand on earlier studies of viewing short segments of video with

captions (e.g., Montero Perez et al., 2014; Sydorenko, 2010). Moreover, it would also be useful
Fo

to look at how learner-related variables play a role in viewing with and without captions to better
rP

understand to what extent captions may contribute to learning across L2 learners.

Another limitation of this study is that the only aspect of vocabulary knowledge measured
ee

was form-meaning connection. Research indicates that different modes of L2 input may also
rR

contribute to incidental gains in other aspects of vocabulary knowledge. For instance, Webb
ev

(2007) found that reading contributed to incidental learning of five different aspects of

knowledge: orthography, grammatical functions, association, collocation, and form-meaning


iew

connection. Hsu (2014) also found that audiovisual input contributed to improvement in the use

of target words in a writing task, whereas, Webb et al. (2013) and Pellicer-Sanchez (2017) found

that participants gained knowledge of collocation incidentally through reading with audio

support, and reading, respectively. The results of these studies suggest that the vocabulary gains

from different modes of input may also be revealed through tests measuring different aspects of

vocabulary knowledge. It would be useful to measure multiple aspects of vocabulary knowledge

in future research comparing modes of input.

Cambridge University Press


Page 37 of 48 Studies in Second Language Acquisition

VOCABULARY LEARNING THROUGH DIFFERENT MODES 37

A final limitation is that only 76 participants learning EFL in China took part in this study. It

would be useful to carry out follow up studies that compared vocabulary learning in different

modes of input with participants in different contexts to determine the extent to which the results

of this study can be generalized across different learner populations.

Conclusion

Overall, this study showed that viewing a full-length TV documentary, reading its transcript,

and listening to its audio could all contribute to significant incidental vocabulary learning and

that there were no statistically significant differences in the amount of vocabulary knowledge
Fo

gained through the three modes. The results suggest that there is value in learning with all three
rP

modes of input within the classroom with perhaps an aim towards enabling students to learn

extensively with their preferred mode of input. While there has long been support and programs
ee

designed to promote extensive reading, the findings suggest that perhaps there should also be
rR

programs developed to encourage extensive listening and extensive viewing.


ev
iew

Cambridge University Press


Studies in Second Language Acquisition Page 38 of 48

VOCABULARY LEARNING THROUGH DIFFERENT MODES 38

References

Anthony, L. (2014). AntWordProfiler (Version 1.4.1) [Computer Software]. Tokyo, Japan:

Waseda University. Retrieved from http://www.laurenceanthony.net/

Brown, R., Waring, R., & Donkaewbua, S. (2008). Incidental vocabulary acquisition from

reading, reading-while-listening, and listening to stories. Reading in a Foreign Language,

20(2), 136–163.

Chen, C., & Truscott, J. (2010). The effects of repetition and L1 lexicalization on incidental
Fo
vocabulary acquisition. Applied Linguistics, 31(5), 693–713.

http://doi.org/10.1093/applin/amq031
rP

Cobb, T. (2007). Computing the vocabulary demands of L2 reading. Language Learning &
ee

Technology, 11(3), 38–63.


rR

d’Ydewalle, G. (2002). Foreign-language acquisition by watching subtitled television programs.

Journal of Foreign Language Education and Research, 4, 59–77.


ev

d’Ydewalle, G., & Pavakanun, U. (1997). Could Enjoying a Movie Lead to Language
iew

Acquisition?. In P.Winterhoff-Spurk & T. Van der Voort (Eds.), New Horizons in Media

Psychology (pp. 145–155). Opladen, Germany: Westdeutscher-Verlag GmbH.

d’Ydewalle, G., & Poel, M. Van De. (1999). Incidental foreign-language acquisition by children

watching subtitled television programs. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 28(3), 227–

244.

Cambridge University Press


Page 39 of 48 Studies in Second Language Acquisition

VOCABULARY LEARNING THROUGH DIFFERENT MODES 39

Elley, W. B. (1989). Vocabulary acquisition from listening to stories. Reading Research

Quarterly, 24(2), 174–187. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/stable/747863

Ellis, N. C. (2006). Selective attention and transfer phenomena in L2 acquisition: Contingency,

cue competition, salience, interference, overshadowing, blocking, and perceptual

learning. Applied Linguistics, 27(2), 164–194. http://doi.org/10.1093/applin/aml015

Gieve, S., & Clark, R. (2005). “The Chinese approach to learning”: Cultural trait or situated

response? The case of a self-directed learning programme. System, 33, 261–276.


Fo

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2004.09.015
rP

Goh, C. C. M. (2000). A cognitive perspective on language learners’ listening comprehension

problems. System, 28, 55–75.


ee

Horst, M. (2005). Learning l2 vocabulary through extensive reading: A measurement study. The
rR

Canadian Modern Language Review, 61(3), 355–382.


ev

Horst, M., Cobb, T., & Meara, P. (1998). Beyond a clockwork orange: acquiring second
iew

language vocabulary through reading. Reading in a Foreign Language, 11(2), 207–223.

Hsu, W. (2014). The effects of audiovisual support on EFL learners’ productive vocabulary.

ReCALL : The Journal of EUROCALL, 26(1), 62–79.

http://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0958344013000220

Hu, M. & Nation, I.S.P. (2000). Unknown vocabulary density and reading comprehension.

Reading in a Foreign Language, 13(1), 403–430.

Cambridge University Press


Studies in Second Language Acquisition Page 40 of 48

VOCABULARY LEARNING THROUGH DIFFERENT MODES 40

Hu, H. M. (2013). The effects of word frequency and contextual types on vocabulary acquisition

from extensive reading: a case study. Journal of Language Teaching and Research, 4(3),

487–495. http://doi.org/10.4304/jltr.4.3.487-495

Hulstijn, J. H. (1992). Retention of inferred and given word meanings: Experiments in incidental

vocabulary learning. In P. J. L. Arnaud & H. Béjoint (Eds.), Vocabulary and Applied

Linguistics (pp. 113–125). Basingstoke: Macmillan. http://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-349-

12396-4_11
Fo

Jenkins, J. R., Stein, M. L., & Wysocki, K. (1984). Learning vocabulary through reading.

American Educational Research Journal, 21(4), 767–787.


rP

Kennedy, G. (Producer), & Klein, L. (Director). (2002, April 30). Why the towers fell [Television
ee

broadcast]. Public Broadcasting Service.


rR

Krashen, S. (1985). The Input Hypothesis. London and New York: Longman.
ev

Krashen, S. (1989). We acquire vocabulary and spelling by reading: Additional evidence for the
iew

input hypothesis. The Modern Language Journal, 73(4), 440-464.

Lindgren, E., & Muñoz, C. (2013). The influence of exposure, parents, and linguistic distance on

young European learners' foreign language comprehension. International Journal of

Multilingualism, 10(1), 105-129.

Liu, N., & Nation, I. S. P. (1985). Factors affecting guessing vocabulary in context. RELC

Journal, 16(1), 33–42.

Mayer, R. E. (2009). Multimedia Learning (2nd ed.). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Cambridge University Press


Page 41 of 48 Studies in Second Language Acquisition

VOCABULARY LEARNING THROUGH DIFFERENT MODES 41

Meara, P. (1992). EFL Vocabulary Tests (2nd ed.). Swansea: Centre for Applied Language

Studies, University College Swansea.

Milton, J., & Hopkins, N. (2006). Comparing phonological and orthographic vocabulary size: Do

vocabulary tests underestimate the knowledge of some learners? Canadian Modern

Language Review, 63(1), 127-147.

Montero Perez, M., Peters, E., Clarebout, G., & Desmet, P. (2014). Effects of captioning on

video comprehension and incidental vocabulary learning. Language Learning &


Fo

Technology, 18(181), 118–141.


rP

Nagy, W. E., Anderson, R. C., & Herman, P. A. (1987). Learning word meanings from context

during normal reading. American Educational Research Journal, 24(2), 237–270.


ee

http://doi.org/10.3102/00028312024002237
rR

Nagy, W. E., Herman, P. A., & Anderson, R. C. (1985). Learning words from context. Reading
ev

Research Quarterly, 20(2), 233–253. http://doi.org/10.2307/747758


iew

Nation, I. S. P. (2006). How large a vocabulary is needed for reading and listening? The

Canadian Modern Language Review, 63(1), 59–81. http://doi.org/10.3138/cmlr.63.1.59

Nation, I. S. P. (2012). The BNC/COCA word family lists. Retrieved from

https://www.victoria.ac.nz/lals/about/staff/paul-nation#vocab-programs

Nation, I. S. P. (2013). Learning vocabulary in another language (2nd eds.). New York, NY:

Cambridge University press.

Cambridge University Press


Studies in Second Language Acquisition Page 42 of 48

VOCABULARY LEARNING THROUGH DIFFERENT MODES 42

Nation, I. S. P. (2015). Principles guiding vocabulary learning through extensive reading.

Reading in a Foreign Language, 27(1), 136–145.

Nation, P., & Wang, M. K. (1999). Graded readers and vocabulary. Reading in a Foreign

Language, 12(2), 355–380.

Neuman, S. B., & Koskinen, P. (1992). Captioned television as “comprehensible input”: Effects

of incidental word learning from context for language minority students. Reading Research

Quarterly, 27(1), 95–106. http://doi.org/10.2307.747835


Fo

Oetting, J., Rice, M. L., & Swank, L. K. (1995). Quick Incidental Learning (QUIL) of words by
rP

school-age children with and without SLI. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38,

434–445. http://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3802.434
ee

Pavia, N., Webb, S., & Faez, F. (2019). Incidental vocabulary learning through listening to
rR

songs. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 1-24. Advance online publication.


ev

doi:10.1017/S0272263119000020
iew

Pellicer-Sánchez, A. (2017). Learning L2 collocations incidentally from reading. Language

Teaching Research, 21(3), 381-402.

Pellicer-Sánchez, A., & Schmitt, N. (2010). Incidental vocabulary acquisition from an authentic

novel: Do Things Fall Apart?. Reading in a Foreign Language, 22(1), 31–55.

Peters, E. (2018). The effect of out-of-class exposure to English language media on learners’

vocabulary knowledge. ITL-International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 169(1), 142-168.

Cambridge University Press


Page 43 of 48 Studies in Second Language Acquisition

VOCABULARY LEARNING THROUGH DIFFERENT MODES 43

Peters, E., Heynen, E., & Puimège, E. (2016). Learning vocabulary through audiovisual input:

The differential effect of L1 subtitles and captions. System, 63, 134–148.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2016.10.002

Peters, E., & Webb, S. (2018). Incidental Vocabulary Acquisition through Viewing L2

Television and Factors that Affect Learning. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 1-27.

Pigada, M., & Schmitt, N. (2006). Vocabulary acquisition from extensive reading: A case study.

Reading in a Foreign Language, 18(1), 1–28.


Fo

Rastle, K., Harrington, J., & Coltheart, M. (2002). 358,534 nonwords: the ARC Nonword
rP

Database. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55A, 1339–1362.

http://doi.org/10.1080/02724980244000099
ee

Renandya, W. A., & Farrell, T. S. C. (2011). “Teacher, the tape is too fast!” Extensive listening
rR

in ELT. ELT Journal, 65(1), 52–59. http://doi.org/10.1093/elt/ccq015


ev

Rice, M. L., Huston, A. C., Truglio, R., & Wright, J. (1990). Words from “Sesame Street”:
iew

Learning vocabulary while viewing. Developmental Psychology, 26(3), 421–428.

Rice, M. L., & Woodsmall, L. (1988). Lessons from television: Children’s word learning when

viewing. Child Development, 59(2), 420–429.

Rodgers, M. P. (2013). English language learning through viewing television: An investigation

of comprehension, incidental vocabulary acquisition, lexical coverage, attitudes, and

captions (Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from

http://researcharchive.vuw.ac.nz/xmlui/bitstream/handle/10063/2870/thesis.pdf?sequence=2

Cambridge University Press


Studies in Second Language Acquisition Page 44 of 48

VOCABULARY LEARNING THROUGH DIFFERENT MODES 44

Rodgers, M. P. H., & Webb, S. (2011). Narrow Viewing: The vocabulary in related television

programs. TESOL Quarterly, 45(4), 689–717.

Rodgers, M. P. H., & Webb, S. (2019). Incidental vocabulary learning through watching

television. ITL - International Journal of Applied Linguistics.

Rott, S. (1999). The effect of exposure frequency on intermediate language learners’ incidental

vocabulary acquisition and retention through reading. Studies in Second Language

Acquisition, 21(4), 589–619.


Fo

Sadoski, M. (2005). A dual coding view of vocabulary learning. Reading & Writing Quarterly,

21(3), 221–238. http://doi.org/10.1080/10573560590949359


rP

Sadoski, M., & Paivio, A. (2001). Imagery and text. A Dual Coding Theory of Reading and
ee

Writing. Mahwah, New Jersey, London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.


rR

Saragi, T., Nation, I. S. P., & Meister, G. F. (1978). Vocabulary learning and
ev

reading. System, 6(2), 72-78.


iew

Schmitt, N., Jiang, X., & Grabe, W. (2011). The percentage of words known in a text and

reading comprehension. The Modern Language Journal, 95(1), 26–43.

http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4781.2011.01146.x

Schmitt, N., Schmitt, D., & Clapham, C. (2001). Developing and exploring the behaviour of two

versions of the vocabulary levels test. Language Testing, 18(1), 55–88.

http://doi.org/10.1177/026553220101800103

Shu, H., Anderson, R. C., & Zhang, H. (1995). Incidental learning of word meanings while

Cambridge University Press


Page 45 of 48 Studies in Second Language Acquisition

VOCABULARY LEARNING THROUGH DIFFERENT MODES 45

reading: A Chinese and American cross-cultural study. Reading Research Quarterly, 76-95.

Stæhr, L. S. (2008). Vocabulary size and the skills of listening, reading and writing. Language

Learning Journal, 36(2), 139–152. http://doi.org/10.1080/09571730802389975

Sydorenko, T. (2010). Modality of input and vocabulary acquisition. Language Learning &

Technology, 14(2), 50–73.

van Zeeland, H., & Schmitt, N. (2013). Incidental vocabulary acquisition through L2 listening: A

dimensions approach. System, 41(3), 609–624. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2013.07.012


Fo

Vidal, K. (2003). Academic listening: a source of vocabulary acquisition?. Applied Linguistics,


rP

24(1), 56–89.
ee

Vidal, K. (2011). A comparison of the effects of reading and listening on incidental vocabulary

acquisition. Language Learning, 61(1), 219–258. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-


rR

9922.2010.00593.x
ev

Waring, R., & Takaki, M. (2003). At what rate do learners learn and retain new vocabulary from
iew

reading a graded reader?. Reading in a Foreign Language, 15(2), 130–163.

http://doi.org/10.1177/003368828501600214

Webb, S. (2007). The effects of repetition on vocabulary knowledge. Applied Linguistics, 28(1),

46–65. http://doi.org/10.1093/applin/aml048

Webb, S. (2010). A corpus driven study of the potential for vocabulary learning through

watching movies. International Journal of Corpus Linguistics, 15(4), 497–519.

http://doi.org/10.1075/ijcl.15.4.03web

Cambridge University Press


Studies in Second Language Acquisition Page 46 of 48

VOCABULARY LEARNING THROUGH DIFFERENT MODES 46

Webb, S. (2011). Selecting television programs for language learning: investigating television

programs from the same genre. International Journal of English Studies, 11(1), 117–135.

Webb, S. (2020). Incidental vocabulary learning. In S. Webb (Ed.) The Routledge Handbook of

Vocabulary Studies (pp. 225-239). New York: Routledge.

Webb, S., & Chang, A. C.-S. (2015a). How does prior word knowledge affect vocabulary

learning progress in an extensive reading program?. Studies in Second Language

Acquisition, 37(4), 651–675. http://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263114000606


Fo

Webb, S., & Chang, A. C.-S. (2015b). Second language vocabulary learning through extensive
rP

reading with audio support: How do frequency and distribution of occurrence affect
ee

learning?. Language Teaching Research, 19(6), 667–686.

http://doi.org/10.1177/1362168814559800
rR

Webb, S., & Chang, A. C-S. (2019). How does mode of input affect the incidental learning of
ev

multiword combinations. Manuscript submitted for publication.


iew

Webb, S., & Nation, P. (2017). How vocabulary is learned. Oxford University Press.

Webb, S., & Paribakht, T. S. (2015). What is the relationship between the lexical profile of test

items and performance on a standardized English proficiency test?. English for Specific

Purposes, 38, 34-43.

Webb, S., & Rodgers, M. P. H. (2009). The lexical coverage of movies. Applied Linguistics,

30(3), 407–427. http://doi.org/10.1093/applin/amp010

Cambridge University Press


Page 47 of 48 Studies in Second Language Acquisition

VOCABULARY LEARNING THROUGH DIFFERENT MODES 47

Webb, S., Sasao, Y., & Ballance, O. (2017). The updated Vocabulary Levels Test: Developing

and validating two new forms of the VLT. ITL - International Journal of Applied

Linguistics, 168(1), 34-70.

Webb, S., Uchihara, T., & Yanagisawa, A. (2019). How effective is incidental vocabulary

learning? A meta-analysis. Manuscript in preparation.

Werner, H., & Kaplan, E. (1950). Development of word meaning through verbal context: An

experimental study. The Journal of Psychology, 29, 251–257.


Fo

Zahar, R., Cobb, T., & Spada, N. (2001). Acquiring vocabulary through reading: effects of
rP

frequency and contextual richness. The Canadian Modern Language Review, 57(4), 541–

572.
ee
rR
ev
iew

Cambridge University Press


Studies in Second Language Acquisition Page 48 of 48

Checklist Test 1

Checklist Test

Read and listen to the words carefully.

For each word: if you know what it means, write Y (for Yes) in the box; if you don't know
what it means, or if you are not sure, write N (for No) in the box.

1. ☐component 2. ☐stalwart 3. ☐levy


4. ☐collapse 5. ☐reveal 6. ☐skyscraper

7. ☐rhipps 8. ☐emergency 9. ☐terror

10. ☐return 11. ☐trigger 12. ☐concrete


Fo

13. ☐fireproofing 14. ☐strike 15. ☐truss


rP

16. ☐staff 17. ☐trap 18. ☐souz

19. ☐ignite 20. ☐bolt 21. ☐jet


ee

22. ☐manks 23. ☐steel 24. ☐structure


rR

25. ☐fuel 26. ☐lateral 27. ☐lurch

28. ☐state 29. ☐computer 30. ☐stairwell


ev

31. ☐core 32. ☐destruction 33. ☐essential


iew

34. ☐sheetrock 35. ☐phleeze 36. ☐drywall

37. ☐koax 38. ☐aircraft 39. ☐occupy

40. ☐skous 41. ☐gene 42. ☐ladder

43. ☐choice 44. ☐fragment 45. ☐squeegee

46. ☐crucial 47. ☐vertical 48. ☐note

49. ☐evacuate 50. ☐flame 51. ☐quaks

52. ☐crew 53. ☐severe 54. ☐elevate

55. ☐initial 56. ☐intact 57. ☐exterior

58. ☐column 59. ☐mutual 60. ☐photograph

Session 1
Cambridge University Press

View publication stats

You might also like