You are on page 1of 15

SCOTT M.

STANLEY University of Denver

GALENA K. RHOADES University of Denver*

SARAH W. WHITTON University of Cincinnati**

Commitment: Functions, Formation,


and the Securing of Romantic Attachment

In this theoretical article, we review central Markman, 1992). We review central concepts in
concepts in the psychological literature on rela- the literature on commitment as a foundation for
tionship commitment to provide a foundation discussing two topics. The first topic discussed
to discuss two themes related to long-term is the role commitment plays in stabilizing
romantic relationships and marriages. First, romantic attachment. The second is how what
we describe and discuss the role that commit- are now becoming common trajectories of
ment plays in stabilizing romantic attachment. couple development could undermine long-
Second, we use empirical research on cohabi- term commitment, such as in marriage. The
tation to highlight how what are now common first topic underscores the importance of
trajectories of couple development can under- commitment in romantic relationships, and the
mine the formation of commitment. The first second emphasizes dynamics in the formation
topic underscores an increasingly important of commitment. In both cases, we draw
role for commitment in an age of companionate broadly from diverse literatures that are seldom
marriage. The second topic draws attention to integrated, with our goal being to advance theory
dynamics that can affect the strength of romantic about romantic relationship development and
commitments, especially in marriage. stability.

This article focuses on commitment in romantic THE PSYCHOLOGICAL CONSTRUCT


relationships, which is generally defined as the OF COMMITMENT
intention to maintain a relationship over time
(M. P. Johnson, 1973; Rusbult, 1980; Stanley & Modern theories of commitment are rooted in
interdependence theory (e.g., Kelley & Thibaut,
1978; Thibaut & Kelley, 1959) and social
University of Denver, Psychology, 2155 S. Race St, exchange theories (e.g., Cook & Emerson, 1978;
Denver, CO 80208 (sstanley@du.edu). Homans, 1958). Interdependence theory posits
*University of Denver, Psychology, 2155 S. Race St, that the tendency for relationships to develop
Denver, CO 80208 (grhoades@du.edu). and persist depends not only on the personal
**University of Cincinnati, Psychology, 4150 Edwards
characteristics of the two individuals but also
Building One, P.O. Box 210376, Cincinnati, OH on the interdependence that develops between
45221-0376 (whittosh@ucmail.uc.edu). the two partners. Partners’ level of satisfaction
Key Words: attachment, cohabitation, commitment, interde- with the relationship and their perceptions of
pendence theory, marriage, sacrifice. the quality of alternatives to the relationship
Journal of Family Theory & Review 2 (December 2010): 243–257 243
DOI:10.1111/j.1756-2589.2010.00060.x
244 Journal of Family Theory & Review

determines the level of interdependence in a intention to be together, to have a future, and to


relationship (Thibaut & Kelley, 1959). share an identity as a couple, the state of being
Rusbult (1980) developed an extension of committed can be thought of most simply as
interdependence theory referred to as the invest- having a sense of ‘‘us with a future.’’
ment model, linking interdependence to the
development of commitment. This perspective
has generated a large body of literature (see Le COMMITMENT DYNAMICS: DEDICATION
AND CONSTRAINT
& Agnew, 2003). The investment model sug-
gests that dependence on a relationship develops The commitment theorists Rusbult, Johnson,
on the basis not only of the level of satisfac- Levinger, Stanley, and their respective col-
tion and the quality of alternatives but also of leagues have put forth several specific models of
the investment that an individual has put into commitment, each highlighting different com-
the relationship. Investments refer to resources ponents of commitment or commitment dynam-
that are attached to the relationship that would ics. Although different theorists prefer different
be lost or lose value if the relationship were terms for specific components, one model of
to end; investments may take any number of commitment easily translates to another. Also, a
forms, including emotional investments such construct viewed as a component of commitment
as self-disclosure (Stanley & Markman, 1992) in one analysis may be viewed as a correlate or
and structural investments such as money and outcome of commitment in another, depending
possessions (M. P. Johnson, 1973). Most rele- on the research question (Stanley & Markman,
vant to the present discussion, the investment 1992). Although this overlap may lead to confu-
model proposes that increasing interdependence sion between the construct itself and the factors
leads to relationship commitment, which Rus- that affect it, we believe that the differences
bult (1980) defined as a desire to persist in between particular commitment theories are not
the relationship and maintain emotional attach- critical, as there is notable consistency across
ment. Numerous studies support the investment theories and empirical findings.
model, demonstrating that relationship com- From this point forward, we focus on the
mitment grows as satisfaction and investments model of commitment presented by Stanley
increase while perceived quality of alternatives and Markman (1992), which highlights two key
decreases (e.g., Rusbult & Buunk, 1993). dynamics involved in commitment: dedication
The interdependence framework also ad- and constraint. The model’s simplicity facilitates
dresses how two individuals come to develop an the discussion ahead. The model was strongly
identity as a couple. Kelley and Thibaut (1978) influenced by the works of the psychologists
theorized that those in developing and contin- already mentioned (Thibaut, Kelley, Levinger,
uing relationships begin to take into account and Rusbult) and by the sociologist Michael
long-term goals for the relationship as an entity Johnson, whose model of commitment described
additional to the needs and desires of the two three aspects of commitment (see M. P. Johnson,
individuals. They called the process of moving Caughlin, & Huston, 1999): personal, moral, and
from acting based on self-interest to acting based structural. Personal commitment is the desire to
on preferences for joint outcomes the trans- be with the partner in the future. Moral commit-
formation of motivation. Similarly, Levinger ment comprises values and beliefs that promote
(1979) noted that ‘‘as interpersonal involve- persistence. Structural commitment addresses
ment deepens, one’s partner’s satisfactions and the ways in which elements such as the quality
dissatisfactions become more and more identi- of alternatives, the amount of investment in the
fied with one’s own’’ (p. 175). In economic and relationship, and the difficulty of steps needed to
exchange theory terms, this transformation pro- end a relationship affect the likelihood of remain-
duces an exchange market (between partners) ing in a relationship regardless of its quality.
that is noncompetitive and in which the goal is Stanley and Markman (1992) focused broadly
to maximize joint outcomes (Cook & Emerson, on the psychological aspects of the ‘‘want
1978). Although one partner should not lose to’’ and the ‘‘have to’’ in commitment: ded-
his or her identity in the other, couples develop ication and constraint, respectively. Johnson’s
a degree of ‘‘we-ness’’ (Agnew, Van Lange, personal commitment is best represented by
Rusbult, & Langston, 1998) or couple identity dedication in this model and his structural and
(Stanley & Markman, 1992). Combining the moral commitment by constraint. Stanley and
Commitment 245

Markman’s (1992) two-component model also to reinforce the development, maintenance, or


is consistent with Levinger’s (1965) discussion redevelopment of dedication. Further, because
of cohesion theory in which he focused on past dedication generally produces constraints,
attraction forces and barrier forces. Stanley and they have some potential to remind the indi-
Markman (1992) described a model reflecting vidual of that dedication. Although there are
this basic push and pull of commitment as most limits to the strength of such a mechanism,
people experience it in romantic relationships, there is evidence that preferences can cohere to
especially marriage: choices already made (e.g., Simon, Krawczyk,
& Holyoak, 2004). Therefore, constraints should
Personal dedication refers to the desire of an have some ability to produce or enhance or rein-
individual to maintain or improve the quality of vigorate dedication. Of course, constraints are
his or her relationship for the joint benefit of destructive when they serve to keep people in
the participants. It is evidenced by a desire (and damaging relationships.
associated behaviors) not only to continue in the In all models of commitment, alternatives
relationship, but also to improve it, to sacrifice play an important role. Commitment can be con-
for it, to invest in it, to link personal goals to
ceptualized as making a choice to give up other
it, and to seek the partner’s welfare, not simply
one’s own. In contrast, constraint commitment choices (Stanley, 2005). In choosing one option
refers to forces that constrain individuals to over others, commitment can be considered an
maintain relationships regardless of their personal act of choosing to be increasingly constrained
dedication to them. Constraints may arise from because of the desire to persist, exclusively, on
either external or internal pressures, and they the chosen path. Although the quality and avail-
favor relationship stability by making termination ability of alternatives is a factor in the overall
of a relationship more economically, socially, level of constraint, the monitoring of alterna-
personally, or psychologically costly. (p. 595) tives is more closely associated with dedication.
Those who are more dedicated to their partners
Constraint commitment can be subdivided report less monitoring of alternatives (Stanley
into variables that represent structural commit- & Markman, 1992; Stanley, Markman, & Whit-
ment (e.g., economic investment, shared pos- ton, 2002), and they actively derogate attractive
sessions), quality of alternatives, social pressure alternatives (D. J. Johnson & Rusbult, 1989).
to remain together, and variables reflective of From this point forward, when we use the term
moral commitment (e.g., thinking divorce is commitment without further specification, the
wrong, believing that you must finish what you reference is predominantly to dedication. When
start) (M. P. Johnson et al., 1999; Stanley & we mean to denote constraint commitment, we
Markman, 1992). Constraints explain why low- will always use the word constraint.
quality relationships continue; when satisfaction
is low but constraints are high, it may be too
costly to leave the relationship. However, it THE INFLUENCE OF COMMITMENT ON
is important to note that individuals generally RELATIONSHIP BEHAVIORS AND OUTCOMES
do not consider constraints negative until or A central feature of commitment is the intention
unless satisfaction declines to the point that to be together in the future—to have a long-
the desire to leave exists. Happy couples tend term time horizon for a relationship. Being
to perceive constraints such as shared prop- committed to a relationship for the long-term has
erty, friends, and children as sources of joy and a powerful influence on individuals’ relationship
evidence of investment. It is only when dissat- behaviors, promoting actions that serve the best
isfaction sets in that constraints may become interest of the couple rather than the short-term
more salient and generate feelings of being interest of the self. An important benefit of
stuck in a relationship. Although it would be having a long-term view in relationships like
hard to prove, it seems likely that few relation- marriage is that the relationship is evaluated
ships are continually satisfying enough to persist on the basis of an extended period of time
without at least a few constraints that help put rather than only on the basis of what happens
the brakes on impulsive, destabilizing behaviors in the here and now (Stanley, 2005; Stanley,
during temporary periods of unhappiness (Stan- Lobitz et al., 1999). Because few relationships
ley, Lobitz, & Markman, 1999). At times of are continuously satisfying, a here-and-now
transitory dissatisfaction, constraints may serve focus would put great pressure on the current
246 Journal of Family Theory & Review

exchange of positives and negatives as the basis Of course, not all relationship sacrifices are
for evaluating the relationship. When confident created equal. Sacrifices that the giver perceives
that a relationship will persist into the future, an as harmful to the self are linked with elevated
individual is more likely to behave in ways that depressive symptomatology and poorer relation-
do not always benefit the self immediately but ship quality (Whitton et al., 2007). Similarly,
enhance the long-term quality of the relationship. sacrifices that are motivated by efforts to avoid
A large body of research supports links conflict or guilt are associated with lower emo-
between higher commitment and pro-relation- tional well-being and poorer relationship quality
ship responses to dissatisfaction (Rusbult, (Impett, Gable, & Peplau, 2005). Partly because
Zembrodt, & Gunn, 1982), the devaluation of of evidence that higher commitment is associ-
attractive alternatives (D. J. Johnson & Rus- ated with perceiving sacrifices to be less harmful
bult, 1989), and constructive responses to neg- to self-interest (Whitton et al., 2007), we believe
ative partner behaviors (Rusbult & Verette, that the presence of commitment is a crucial
1991)—supporting the expected linkages not factor in determining not only whether an indi-
only with better relationship quality but also to vidual will work to strengthen a relationship by
increased relationship stability (Impett, Beals, sacrificing but also how he or she perceives those
& Peplau, 2001). Although there are numerous sacrifices (e.g., as helpful rather than harmful to
pro-relationship behaviors that tend to occur in self-interest).
the presence of strong commitment, we focus on Taken as a whole, such findings about the
one, relationship sacrifice, which demonstrates nature of sacrifice in romantic relationships
important commitment dynamics particularly provide further evidence of the transformation
well. of motivation that is part of Thibaut and Kelley’s
The tendency of individuals to sacrifice, or (1978) formulation of relationship development.
forgo immediate self-interest for the good of In fact, sacrifice may be potent because it
the partner or relationship, is strongly dependent provides information about the presence or
on the presence of commitment. Not only does absence of that transformation, thus functioning
commitment predict the number of sacrifices as a strong behavioral signal of commitment and
performed for partners (Van Lange et al., 1997); security between partners (Wieselquist et al.,
it also is associated with both the degree to 1999). The potency of sacrifice may be rooted in
which individuals feel satisfied with sacrificing the salience of negatives, which are believed to
for their partner’s benefit (Stanley & Markman, be particularly impactful in marriage (Markman
1992) and their willingness to sacrifice (Van & Floyd, 1980). We suspect that sacrifice may
Lange et al., 1997; Wieselquist, Rusbult, Foster, function as a salient but positive signal that
& Agnew, 1999). Whitton, Stanley, and counters the salience of negative behavior in
Markman (2007) showed that commitment to relationships; the salience is rooted in the way
the relationship’s future is strongly related to that acts not based in self-interest are more
whether day-to-day relationship sacrifices are likely to stand out from the day-to-day stream of
perceived as harmful to the self—especially exchanged behaviors to which partners become
for men. Commitment to the future may habituated. For this and likely numerous other
transform the understanding of daily exchanges reasons, sacrifice is therefore an important part
between partners that could otherwise be seen as of the maintenance of high-quality, long-term
winner–loser, zero-sum scenarios because there romantic relationships.
is no trust in future exchanges that can provide With this background of theory and research
reciprocal or mutual benefits. Wieselquist et al. on commitment, we turn to our two major
(1999) demonstrated that sacrifice serves to theoretical goals in this article. First, we
increase trust between partners, which fosters consider how commitment functions to secure
growth in commitment and reciprocation of romantic attachment by shifting relationships
more sacrifice (cf. Molm, 2006). Stanley, from uncertainty to stability. Second, we argue
Whitton, Low, Clements, and Markman (2006) that the process through which a commitment
showed that satisfaction with sacrifice in early forms matters for how well it can secure
marriage is associated with global relationship attachment. Further, we argue that certain
quality in the long term; in fact, satisfaction with patterns of relationship development that have
sacrifice was a better predictor of future marital become increasingly common in recent decades
adjustment than early marital adjustment. may undermine the formation of commitment.
Commitment 247

SECURING ROMANTIC ATTACHMENT: in adult romantic relationships. Longitudinal


A CENTRAL FUNCTION OF COMMITMENT research has demonstrated that people who expe-
We begin this section by establishing that there rienced a secure attachment to their primary,
are two ways to conceptualize attachment, differ- childhood caregiver tend to display similarly
entiating romantic attachment from attachment secure attachment styles toward romantic part-
security. We go on to discuss the role we believe ners in early adulthood (e.g., Roisman, Collins,
that commitment plays in securing attachments Sroufe, & Egeland, 2005), although the effect
between romantic partners during relationship sizes are generally quite small, as cumula-
development. Finally, in this section, we briefly tive experiences with peers and environmen-
discuss some of the ways that the three con- tal contexts can modify adult working models
structs of commitment, romantic attachment, and and attachment security (see Davila & Cobb,
attachment security intersect in the development 2004).
and functioning of romantic unions. In the literature on attachment security,
In most of the literature in psychology, the Zeifman and Hazan (2008) argued that adult,
concept of attachment refers to a theory of emo- romantic relationships can develop into full-
tional security based primarily on experiences blown attachments. They defined full-blown
infants have with caregivers (Bowlby, 1969). At attachment relationships as those that exhibit
the core, the constellation of ideas under the the same defining characteristics Bowlby (1969)
heading of attachment theory suggests humans outlined for parent-infant attachment relation-
are ‘‘born equipped to form attachment rela- ships: (a) proximity maintenance, (b) separation
tionships’’ (see Selcuk, Zayas, & Hazan, 2010 distress, (c) safe haven, and (d) secure base.
[in this volume]). Further, childhood experi- Zeifman and Hazan (2008) suggested that it
ences and continuing relationship experiences takes two years for a full-blown attachment
into adulthood create relational tendencies in relationship to develop between romantic part-
individuals, guided by internal working mod- ners. However, short of full-blown attachments,
els, that become expressed in different enduring attachment styles affect an individual’s behavior
attachment styles (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007). in relationships that do not yet, or may never,
Working models are based on the accumulated qualify as full-blown attachments. Indeed, it is
beliefs about the likelihood of others being avail- well established that internal working models
able and responsive to one’s needs for support and attachment styles play a central role in both
and security in the face of distress. Although peer relationships and romantic relationships as
a variety of terms are used, there are three adolescents move toward adulthood (Collins &
attachment styles commonly discussed in the lit- Sroufe, 1999; Furman & Collins, 2008). Fur-
erature on attachment security: secure, anxious, ther, Mikulincer and Shaver (2007) suggested
and avoidant. Securely attached individuals view that even flirtation activates dynamics related to
other people as reliably available and responsive enduring attachment dynamics around security.
to meet their needs. In contrast, insecurity about There is another concept of attachment that
the whether important others will be there when both social scientists and laypeople use that does
needed characterizes both of the latter groups; not invoke the conceptual schema of attachment
the anxiously attached person copes by trying too theory. Attachment in this sense can describe
hard to secure love, whereas the avoidant person a strong romantic and emotional connection
copes by resisting intimacy and dependence. between two adults without reference to their
In an article that became a watershed in histories of felt security with parents, peers, and
the field of adult attachment theory, Hazan others. If someone remarks, ‘‘Mary is strongly
and Shaver (1987) showed how attachment attached to Bill,’’ it speaks not to Mary’s attach-
dynamics beginning in childhood have impor- ment style in relation to her childhood history
tant implications for adult relationships. The but to the depth of her romantic connection with
specific application of notions regarding attach- Bill. Attachment in this common use of the word
ment styles to adolescent and adult romantic describes a deep, romantic emotional bond; we
relationships has generated a vast literature (see use the term romantic attachment for this con-
Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007; Zeifman & Hazan, struct. Although we could use a different word
2008). In general, this body of work focuses from attachment to avoid confusion, other words
on how internal working models and attach- seem inadequate, as deep emotional attach-
ment styles that developed in childhood manifest ments in romantic relationships seem poorly
248 Journal of Family Theory & Review

and inaccurately captured by terms such as con- This link among commitment, romantic
nection and attraction. We also note that the way attachment, and anxiety was suggested earlier
we view this concept of romantic attachment is by Stanley, Lobitz et al. (1999): ‘‘We believe
different from the concept of pair bond that is that commitment develops in the first place in
commonly used in the attachment theory litera- response to anxiety about losing the partner that
ture. As Zeifman and Hazan (2008) stated, pair one has become so attached to during the dating
bonds ‘‘involve the integration of three social- process. Commitment reassures two attached
behavioral systems: sexual mating, caregiving partners that each will be there for the other into
(parenting), and attachment’’ (p. 442). the future’’ (p. 388).
Other researchers use the word attachment in This type of loss anxiety will arise when
ways more consistent with what we are calling the strength of the romantic attachment exceeds
romantic attachment than attachment theory the level of commitment between two partners.
conceptualizations of attachment security. For We assert that this anxiety is a common char-
example, Fisher (2005) suggested that there acteristic in romantic relationship development,
are three related but different brain systems for particularly in the earlier stages of what turn into
courtship, mating, and parenting: lust, romantic long-term, committed relationships. (This anxi-
love, and male/female attachment. Clearly, in the ety can return for people who retain a romantic
latter term, she is referring to something more attachment to a partner who is straying or oth-
akin to romantic attachment than to attachment erwise threatening the commitment in the rela-
security or enduring styles from childhood. tionship, because the level of commitment drops
With terms defined, we now focus on the back below the level of romantic attachment for
role of commitment in securing romantic attach- at least one of the partners.) This anxiety does not
ment. As romantic relationships develop, they refer to insecurity in attachment based in child-
are not necessarily stable or certain. That is, in hood history and prior relationships. In fact, we
early stages of dating, individuals are typically believe it universally arises as romantic relation-
not certain that the relationship will persist into ships progress, even among those with secure
the future or that their new partner will be reli- attachment styles. However, as we discuss,
ably available to them when needed. Nor should someone with a background of insecure attach-
individuals be certain of such things, because ment would experience this normal anxiety
before becoming committed in long-term rela- about loss of a romantic attachment most acutely.
tionships, people are searching for a good fit There is empirical evidence supporting this
with a partner (Glenn, 2002). Before finding a function of commitment in the early stages of
good fit, commitment would interfere with this relationship development. Eastwick and Finkel
search. As relationships that have the potential to (2008) posited that anxiety about loss of a
become significant and long term continue over romantic partner is typical in fledgling roman-
time, romantic attachment grows. In tandem, we tic unions and represents a normative, statelike
believe that a particular type of anxiety com- phenomenon in the individual that is specific
monly develops in which the individual begins to a romantic relationship. They distinguish
to fear the loss of the partner as a love object. The between this phenomenon and attachment styles
potential pain of the loss becomes greater as sat- that have traitlike characteristics, parallel to our
isfaction and relational benefits grow. Accord- distinction between romantic attachment and
ingly, anxiety grows as well. We believe that a attachment security. Eastwick and Finkel (2008)
fundamental role of commitment is to secure the demonstrated that statelike, relationship-specific
romantic attachment between partners, thereby anxiety not only exists in fledgling romantic rela-
reducing this type of anxiety. Although there tionships but also leads to the types of behaviors
are many other functions of commitment—such discussed in the attachment theory literature as
as those that promote family stability for chil- responses to anxiety and distress, such as prox-
dren—we propose that one of the key functions imity seeking to increase the sense of security
of commitment throughout history has been to about the relationship.
secure romantic attachment. We believe that this Eastwick and Finkel (2008) found that the
function has become increasingly important in statelike anxiety in a fledgling relationship is
this age of companionate marriage, in which reduced once partners clarify that a relationship
love is desired as the primary foundation for exists. Although they did not link clarity about
marriage (Coontz, 2006). the existence of a relationship to the construct
Commitment 249

of commitment, what they described reflects the sense of stability and emotional security from
emergence of initial levels of commitment as evidence of the other partner’s growing ded-
described in various theories of interdependence ication. It is Bill’s expression of dedication
and commitment. The emerging awareness that a that reassures Mary about the security of the
relationship exists is consistent with the transfor- romantic attachment, and vice versa. Although
mation of two individuals toward a shared couple Mary could be reassured in some degree by her
identity (Levinger, 1965; Stanley & Markman, own level of commitment, or even project her
1992). Eastwick and Finkel (2008) showed how own commitment onto Bill, the unambiguous
the emergence of this early form of couple iden- evidence of Bill’s commitment—demonstrated
tity reduces anxiety about loss, well before there by his behavioral expressions of dedication—
is either a fully developed commitment or a full- should do the most to build security in Mary.
blown attachment relationship. This emergence In turn, she is likely to feel and behave in ways
of commitment stabilizes the relationship and that express her growing commitment, thereby
enhances the sense of emotional security in the increasing Bill’s felt security. In contrast, a lack
individuals. of evidence of a reciprocal commitment from
If commitment develops partly to secure the partner may serve to help the more commit-
romantic attachments, which aspects of the ted partner to recognize the danger of a power
broader construct of commitment serve this imbalance. Consistent with the principle of least
purpose? Going back to the simple distinction interest (Waller, 1938), the one who is least com-
between dedication and constraint, dedication mitted has the most power, with imbalances lead-
should be most directly related to providing a ing to various problems in relationship dynamics
sense of security. Constraint can foster a sense (e.g., Sprecher, Schmeeckle, & Felmlee, 2006).
of permanence, which can contribute to over- Some reasonable balance in actual commitment
all security, but dedication will be reflected levels, as well as mutual clarity of signaling
in behaviors that are more readily viewed as commitment, is important so that both partners
under personal control and, thereby, informa- are reassured that they are not being taken advan-
tive about commitment in ways that fosters trust tage of in the developing relationship. (Related
and security between partners. Such behaviors to this and theories touching on commitment, one
could include all of those we considered in the of the most important uses of exchange theory
prior section: derogating alternatives, making in the field of sociology has been the explica-
the relationship a priority, acting consistently tion of such power dynamics and implications
with having a couple identity, and demonstrating for imbalances based in differential quality of
a desire to be with the partner in the future. Sac- alternatives [e.g., Cook, Cheshire, & Gerbasi,
rificial behavior may play a particularly potent 2006].)
role in reducing anxiety about loss in romantic Is there a conceptual difference between
attachments by how it signals commitment to romantic attachment and commitment? This is
the future (Stanley, Whitton, Low et al., 2006; a particularly important question when commit-
Wieselquist et al., 1999). In short, all behaviors ment is framed as dedication. More specifically,
consistent with the construct of dedication have is it really the dedication of one partner that
the potential benefit of reducing anxiety about reassures the other, or more simply, is it mere
the loss of the partner because the behavioral evi- evidence of a mutual romantic attachment? Such
dence of dedication is diagnostic of intention and questions can easily become mired in tautology
desire. Consistent with this view, Buss (2003) without careful definition of the constructs. An
highlighted the importance of acts of love in con- important difference between commitment and
veying information about commitment (p. 43). romantic attachment lies in the fact that inten-
He suggested that it was historically most impor- tion is central in understanding commitment,
tant for females to discern the commitment levels whereas romantic attachment implies only depth
of males because of the comparative advantages of emotional connection. Broadly, commitment
to a woman of securing a male’s devotion of (dedication and constraint) highlight an intention
resources given the personal costs of pregnancy to persist in the relationship. Commitment in the
and childbirth. form of dedication refines the focus, thus reflect-
When a relationship is developing and ing that the intended persistence is also what
progressing, the growth in commitment is is desired. In contrast, a romantic, emotional
bidirectional; each partner draws an increasing attachment may not mean than an individual
250 Journal of Family Theory & Review

intends to pursue a future with the partner or 2002). This should have consequences because
is ready to signal that such an intention is of the potency of such emblems for securing
growing. We would predict, were it possible romantic attachment; they move beyond what
to test, that the linkage between romantic attach- one person does to what both partners are willing
ment and dedication has weakened over the past to signal to the world. One partner can miscode
few decades as romantic and sexual connections the commitment level of one partner, but it would
fueled emotional attachments without necessar- be much harder for either partner to miscode a
ily leading to the development and clarification public, cultural emblem such as engagement.
of commitment. In other words, we believe that Therefore, where cultural emblems of commit-
romantic attachment is currently a necessary but ment diminish (e.g., ‘‘going steady’’ has mostly
not sufficient condition for the development of a gone away), we predict there should be an
mutual intention to have a future. increase in the situations in which one partner
It is important to keep in mind that the roman- misinterprets the commitment level of the other.
tic attachment of one partner does not mean that Marriage is a culturally imbued, societally
the other partner has, or will develop, the inten- sanctioned emblem with high signal value with
tion to have a future. We can sharpen the contrast regard to commitment. The sociologists Nock,
further by expressing a hypothesis about all three Sanchez, and Wright (2008) wrote: ‘‘Marriage,
constructs considered in this section: People with and a willingness to marry, signal commit-
anxious attachment styles should feel especially ment and exclusivity, acceptance of normative
anxious as romantic attachment deepens unless guidelines for good interpersonal behavior, and
and until mutual intentions for the relationship credibility as a dependable, mature citizen to the
(i.e., commitment) become clear between part- partner, employers, and the government. The
ners. In other words, we posit that a growing, marriage commitment contains both interper-
mutual commitment helps alleviate anxiety over sonal and community messages’’ (p. 79). Nock
loss for most people and that this normative et al. (2008) emphasized how marriage func-
process becomes all the more crucial for those tioned, historically, to support couples’ unions
who have a prior disposition to be anxious about because of the meaning embedded in the reli-
security in important relationships. gious, ritualistic, and cultural aspects of the insti-
The distinction between romantic attachment tution. They drew attention to the signal value of
and commitment becomes stronger still when we marriage based on the writings of the economist
shift to interpersonal behavior that is linked to Rowthorn (2002) who explained that, although
culturally determined emblems of commitment. marriage remains a signal of commitment, the
For expressions of commitment to create secu- signal value has diminished because of high rates
rity about romantic attachment, they must have of divorce. In fact, societal views of marriage
two characteristics: mutuality and clarity at the as a lifelong commitment have decreased to
couple level. Commitment cannot secure roman- where some people are choosing alternate types
tic attachment unless it both exists in each partner of marriage, such as covenant marriage offered
and is signaled between partners. As noted ear- in Louisiana. Covenant marriage differs from
lier, a strong commitment level of one partner but traditional marriage in that both partners have to
not the other may merely highlight the unpleas- agree to higher entry and exit costs. Nock et al.
ant circumstance of asymmetrical commitment (2008) suggested some couples choose covenant
and power. Our discussion thus far has focused marriage precisely because it offers a stronger
on signals between partners based on behavioral signal of commitment than standard marriage.
representations of dedication. Another level of Following from the ideas presented here and
dyadic signaling is based in cultural emblems the existing body of research on how attachment
of commitment, such as reflected in the cultural styles affect adult relationships in general (see
practice of engagement to be married. Engage- Feeney, Noller, & Roberts, 2000; Zeifman &
ment is a culturally understood relationship form Hazan, 2008), we have several predictions about
that is emblematic of a high degree of both mutu- how enduring attachment styles may influence
ality and clarity between two partners regarding the development and securing of adult romantic
commitment to the future. relationships. Individuals who have an anx-
The widespread use of cultural emblems of ious attachment style will have a particularly
commitment may be diminishing in industri- strong need for security in romantic attach-
alized nations (a theme discussed in Stanley, ments and will therefore benefit most from clear
Commitment 251

and well-formed commitment. However, in their to see changed). Drawing on evidence that anx-
insecurity, those with anxious attachment styles ious attachment hinders constructive reactions
may also experience angst about pressing for to negative relationship events, they evaluated
greater commitment, or commitment clarity, out the hypothesis that higher levels of commitment
of fear that they will push a prospective long- would buffer negative behaviors exhibited by
term partner away. Hence, some people with people who were anxiously attached. Using a
anxious attachment styles may desire and need measure of commitment consistent with ded-
security based in strong expressions of com- ication, Tran and Simpson found that higher
mitment even while accepting a high degree of levels of partners’ commitment were associated
ambiguity about the status of romantic relation- with more constructive responses by anxiously
ships (see discussion in Stanley, Rhoades, & Fin- attached individuals. This is the first published
cham, in press). In such cases, ambiguity is moti- study we are aware of that directly shows how
vated, being preferable to loss. Consistent with commitment and attachment styles interact in
our prior point about the diminishment in the use marital behavior in ways entirely consistent with
of cultural emblems of commitment, we believe the ideas we present here.
that ambiguity in romantic relationships is on the In this section, we have argued that com-
rise. mitment plays a fundamental role in securing
In contrast to anxiously attached individu- romantic attachments. Next, we discuss how pat-
als, those who have avoidant attachment styles terns of relationship formation that are becoming
will resist increasing the level of commitment increasingly common in Western societies may
because of their desire to limit closeness and affect the process of commitment formation in
obligation. Their individual needs for avoid- ways that may interfere with (or undermine) the
ance will inhibit felt anxiety about romantic development of security.
attachment and the development of commitment
on the dyadic level. When these two differ-
ent, insecure attachment styles are combined in COMMITMENT FORMATION: INSIGHTS FROM
one relationship, it is easy to see how the dyadic RESEARCH ON COHABITATION
commitment processes that may provide security
for one of the partners could increase anxiety for Our goal in this section is to use findings from
the other. Clear and mutually expressed commit- research on premarital cohabitation to show
ment should lower anxiety about loss in romantic how common patterns of relationship devel-
attachment for those with anxious attachment opment before marriage may undermine the
styles. In contrast, the matter should be quite development of commitment, thereby undermin-
complicated for those with avoidant attachment ing secure romantic attachments and marriages.
styles because they may simultaneously benefit We use research on cohabitation to develop our
from security and stability in romantic attach- points. Cohabitation is a heterogeneous relation-
ment but have difficulties with the dependence ship state (Thornton, Axinn, & Xie, 2007). It
that such a need betrays. can be a step toward the possibility of mar-
Insecure attachment styles can lead to mar- riage, a move of convenience, a form of dating,
riages that are stable but based on insecurity an arrangement of economic convenience, or an
more than satisfaction (Davila & Bradbury, alternative to marriage. The theoretical points we
2001). Overall, attachment insecurities both con- make here can apply to many forms of cohab-
tribute to relationship problems (Simpson, 1990) itation, as well as a wide range of relationship
and complicate the development of commit- transitions, but we focus on cohabitation that
ment (see Birnie, McClure, Lydon, & Holmberg, leads to marriage with particular attention to the
2009). Can commitment buffer against problems implications of relationship transition dynam-
related to insecure attachment styles? Emerging ics for the formation of commitment. What
evidence suggests it can. Tran and Simpson follows highlights our belief that the study of
(2009) conducted a study in which they used relationship transitions may yield considerable
observational methods to examine the emotional and additional insights about romantic relation-
and behavioral reactions of married partners to ships beyond what can be obtained by studying
threatening interpersonal situations (discussions differences in relationship statuses (in support
in which one partner discusses a characteristic of this point, see also Casper & Bianchi, 2001;
of the other partner that he or she would like Thornton et al., 2007).
252 Journal of Family Theory & Review

Sequencing Dedication and Constraint We call this phenomenon inertia, which is the
Most couples who marry in the United States property in physics representing the amount of
cohabit before marriage (Bumpass & Lu, 2000). energy it would take to move an object from its
Premarital cohabitation has become normative present trajectory or position to another. We sug-
(Smock, 2000), yet it has been associated with gest that living together, especially when sharing
poorer communication, lower relationship satis- a single address, makes it relatively more diffi-
faction, higher levels of domestic violence, and cult than dating without cohabiting for a couple
divorce (e.g., Cohan & Kleinbaum, 2002; Kamp to veer from a path toward a future together,
Dush, Cohan, & Amato, 2003; Kline et al., even into marriage (see Stanley, Rhoades et al.,
2004; Stanley, Whitton, & Markman, 2004; for 2006). Glenn (2002) referred to a similar risk to
a review, see Stanley, Rhoades, & Markman, mate selection, called premature entanglement,
2006). Much research has examined whether which interferes with the search for a good fit
selection factors can explain the increased risk between partners.
for marital difficulties among those who cohab- Inertia implies that there is a subset of those
ited first (see Smock, 2000). Commonly con- who cohabited before marriage who would not
sidered selection factors are variables such as have married had they not been living together.
religious beliefs, attitudes about marriage and The idea is not that cohabitation increases risk
divorce, and other sociodemographic variables but that cohabitation before clear and mutual
that are associated with both the likelihood of commitment to the future makes higher risk
cohabiting before marriage and difficulties in relationships more likely to continue. In terms
marriage. Plenty of evidence exists that selection of commitment as we have discussed it, this
variables explain some or all of the premarital risk model suggests that many couples increase
cohabitation risk (e.g., Brown, Sanchez, Nock, their constraints before clarifying dedication.
& Wright, 2006). Nevertheless, numerous stud- The inertia perspective also suggests that those
ies have controlled for selection variables (often couples who are fully committed to a future
many, simultaneously), finding that it does not together before cohabitation—by being engaged
completely explain the risk associated with or having mutual plans to marry—have less-
cohabiting before marriage (e.g., Kamp Dush than-average risk of marrying, or remaining
et al., 2003; Stafford, Kline, & Rankin, 2004) with, someone partly because of constraints.
or cohabiting before engagement (Kline et al., Therefore, the risks of premarital cohabitation
2004; Rhoades, Stanley, & Markman, 2009b; should be concentrated among those who cohab-
Stanley, Rhoades, Amato, Markman, & C. A. ited before having mutual plans for marriage.
Johnson, 2010). Our research team (the authors Numerous studies support this prediction; the
along with Howard Markman) has focused on risks associated with premarital cohabitation are
examining commitment in attempting to fur- clearest for those who began to cohabit before
ther understand premarital cohabitation. This being engaged or having mutual, clear intentions
work has led us to a series of ideas about to marry (Kline et al., 2004; Goodwin, Mosher,
how common patterns of relationship develop- & Chandra, 2010; Rhoades, Stanley, & Mark-
ment may weaken commitment intentions and man, 2009b; Stanley, Rhoades, Amato, et al.,
follow-through. 2010).
Stanley, Whitton, and Markman (2004) This emphasis on the transition into cohab-
assessed dedication commitment in a random itation raises questions about how cohabitation
national (U.S.) sample to compare married typically begins. In their qualitative research,
respondents who did or did not cohabit premar- Manning and Smock (2005) found that more
itally. They found that married men who lived than half of cohabiting couples reported drifting
with their wives before marriage reported signif- or sliding into cohabitation rather than having
icantly less dedication to their wives than those any discussion about the transition (cf. Macklin,
who did not cohabit before marriage. This find- 1972). Hence, many couples slide into cohab-
ing led to speculation that the well-replicated iting rather than actively talking and deciding
risks associated with premarital cohabitation about what they are doing. Deliberating about
may, in part, be due to a subset of couples any major relationship step could lower the risk
in which the men were always less committed to of the transitions being event driven—such as
their partners but nevertheless propelled by the moving in together mostly because one’s lease
greater constraints of cohabitation into marriage. was up—compared to being more relationship
Commitment 253

driven (cf. Surra & Hughes, 1997). That cohab- such a process of well-formed commitment and
itation often begins without deliberation is con- how cohabitation often comes about is that, in
sistent with the notion that cohabitation is, in the former, the constraints are chosen rather than
general, ambiguous with regard to commitment merely experienced as a result of one’s behavior.
(Lindsay, 2000); it is an incomplete institution
that does not contain the same signal value of
other relationship conditions such as marriage, The Formation of Intention: Sliding Versus
engagement, or having mutual plans to marry Deciding
(Nock, 1995). A large percentage of couples who The points we have been making are not unique
marry begin cohabiting before reducing ambi- to cohabitation. People slide into having sex.
guity by developing clarity around commitment People slide into having children. People slide
intentions (e.g., before either being engaged into dangerous relationships. In contrast to slid-
or having mutual plans to marry) (Rhoades, ing, there are strong conceptual reasons to
Stanley, & Markman, 2009b). Further, the pro- suggest that clear decisions generally build the
cess often happens rapidly (Sassler, 2004). most resilient intentions. Sliding transitions may
Hence, the lack of clarity of intentions around undermine the formation of dedication in rela-
the time couples transition into cohabitation can tionships, even in those that continue. In these
add to future risk for some couples because con- various cases of sliding, a person can experience
straints increased before the full development of a reduction in future options—such as health
dedication (Stanley, Rhoades et al., 2006). complications or single parenthood—without
In addition, we see no reason why this risk having formed a clear intention or commit-
model would not apply to a diverse range of ment related to the transition responsible for
couples. For example, some racial minorities, as diminishing the future options.
well as people with lower levels of education or As already noted, the process of making a
income, are more likely to cohabit instead of, or commitment entails making a decision to choose
before, marriage in the United States (Smock & one alternative over others. The fields of social
Manning, 2004). They are also at greater risk for psychology and behavioral economics provide
relationship dissolution (Lichter & Qian, 2008). a vast amount of evidence that the strength of
However, the fact that these demographic groups the decisions at the base of the intentions affects
are at greater average levels of risk does not mean follow-through. We highlight the literature on
that these sociodemographic variables moderate cognitive dissonance as one example in support
the mechanisms of risk we propose here. We pre- of this point (Festinger, 1957). In the cognitive
dict that greater clarity between partners about dissonance framework, the more a person con-
commitment and the meaning of a transition siders or wrestles with a choice between two
into cohabitation—before the transition occur- options, the greater the strength of dissonance
ring—has protective benefits for most couples. mechanisms that support follow-through consis-
This is why we expect it to remain generally true tent with the choice (Brehm, 2007). For example,
that couples who cohabit after having mutual E. Harmon-Jones and C. Harmon-Jones (2002)
plans to marry, or after marriage, are at some- drew attention to how dissonance produces
what lower risk in marriage than other couples. action tendencies in support of commitments one
Linking this discussion with the previous has made. Here is a simple example: Suppose
one, we believe that romantic attachments drive one is considering buying a car and focuses on
much of the entry into cohabitation (e.g., the two options: a Ford Taurus and a Honda Accord.
most frequent answer chosen for why people Suppose that there is much to like about both
began to cohabit is to spend more time with models and that the decision is difficult. Suppose
their partner) (Rhoades, Stanley, & Markman, the decision is for the Taurus. According to dis-
2009a). If transitions into cohabitation did not sonance theory, awareness that the other option
increase constraints, it is possible that none of still exists, even after a choice of the Taurus has
this would matter. Constraints, by definition, been made, creates internal dissonance because
limit options, which makes the sequencing thinking about the alternative is not consistent
of dedication and constraint important. Well- with the commitment already made. What disso-
formed commitments also limit options in that nance does is help a person create more mental
they involve choosing to constrain oneself to distance between the attractiveness of the two
a specific path. The crucial difference between alternatives, providing a strong, internal bias in
254 Journal of Family Theory & Review

favor of focusing on positive information about dynamics at the base of their romantic relation-
the Taurus and negative information about the ships. We further would argue (but likely cannot
Accord (see E. Harmon-Jones & C. Harmon- directly test) that ever greater numbers of people
Jones, 2002). The implication here is that sliding are reaching adulthood with insecurities about
through transitions is a process that cannot pro- attachment (both kinds) because of the roman-
vide as much support for sustained commitment tic relationship patterns of their parents: Ever
as the case in which one intentionally makes greater numbers of children are experiencing
a decision to become committed as part of the family instability (Bumpass & Lu, 2000) and
transition process. exposure to multiple romantic partners in their
There are additional reasons to be concerned parents’ lives (Cherlin, 2009; McLanahan, in
about commitments that form after constraints press). As such, the need for well-formed, clear
have already developed. Again, we use cohabi- commitment that can secure romantic attach-
tation as an example. From an exchange theory ments may be increasing even as this becomes
perspective, it has been argued that risky or harder to achieve. It is important that we, as a
chaotic relationship structures allow for greater field, continue to advance understandings of how
ability to interpret the relationship motivations relationship trajectories and transitions affect the
of others (see Molm, 2006). Therefore, the exis- formation of commitment and to what conse-
tence of higher levels of constraints should quence for couples, families, and children.
stabilize relationships at the expense of clar-
ity in interpreting motives. Thus, if Bill knows
that Mary has poor options for leaving, Bill may NOTE
attribute Mary’s continuance in the relationship A grant from the National Institute of Child Health and
less to internal motivations than to her poor Human Development (NICHD) to the first and second
options. As a result, Mary has less ability to sig- authors (5R01HD047564) partly supported preparation of
this article. Some elements of this article were developed
nal dedication to Bill under conditions of higher for a conference paper supported by the Explaining Family
constraint. This dyadic effect has an intraper- Change project under a contract between the NICHD and
sonal corollary. When exit costs increase before Duke University, N01-HD-3-3354, with subcontracts to the
clear decisions about commitment are made, it University of Maryland and the University of California–Los
Angeles. Support for the Explaining Family Change project
may be harder for the individual to sustain a also came from the Maryland Population Research Center,
sense of intrinsic commitment, or dedication, in the California Center for Population Research, and the
the relationship. The stronger the external rea- University of Southern California. The contents are solely the
sons for staying, the harder it is to infer internal responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent
reasons. Hence, some people may be more vul- the official views of the National Institutes of Health or
NICHD.
nerable later, at challenging times in the history
of their relationships, to constraint-laden attribu-
tions such as, ‘‘I never really committed to you REFERENCES
anyway; I just got swept up in this.’’ Although
Agnew, C. R., Van Lange, P. A., Rusbult, C. E., &
constraints can induce increases in dedication,
Langston, C. A. (1998). Cognitive interdepen-
as suggested earlier, it does not seem possible dence: Commitment and the mental representation
that commitments can be generally as strong of close relationships. Journal of Personality and
where constraints developed before dedication. Social Psychology, 74, 939 – 954.
We believe that there has been a decided shift Birnie, C., McClure, M. J., Lydon, J. E., & Holm-
in recent decades in the direction of individuals berg, D. (2009). Attachment avoidance and com-
entering constrained pathways in romantic rela- mitment aversion: A script for relationship failure.
tionships before the development of dedication. Personal Relationships, 16, 79 – 97.
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss. Vol. 1.
Attachment. New York: Basic Books.
THE INSECURE FUTURE AHEAD Brehm, J. W. (2007). A brief history of dissonance
theory. Social and Personality Psychology Com-
We believe that important relationship transi- pass, 1, 381 – 391.
tions increasingly come about because of sliding Brown, S., Sanchez, L., Nock, S., & Wright, J.
processes versus deciding process. As such, (2006). Links between premarital cohabitation and
we expect that current normative patterns of subsequent marital quality, stability, and divorce:
couple development will result in increasing A comparison of covenant versus standard mar-
numbers of couples with weakened commitment riages. Social Science Research, 35, 454 – 470.
Commitment 255

Bumpass, L. L., & Lu, H.-H. (2000). Trends in Furman, W. & Collins, W. A. (2008). Adoles-
cohabitation and implications for children’s family cent romantic relationships and experiences. In
contexts in the United States. Population Studies, K. H. Rubin, W. Bukowski, & B. Laursen (Eds.),
54, 29 – 41. Peer interactions, relationships, and groups
Buss, D. M. (2003). The evolution of desire. New (pp. 341 – 360). New York: Guilford Press.
York: Basic Books. Glenn, N. D. (2002). A plea for greater concern about
Casper, L. M., & Bianchi, S. M. (2001). Continuity the quality of marital matching. In A. J. Hawkins,
and change in the American family. Thousand L. D. Wardle, & D. O. Coolidge (Eds.), Revitaliz-
Oaks, CA: Sage. ing the institution of marriage for the twenty-first
Cherlin, A. J. (2009). The marriage-go-round: The century: An agenda for strengthening marriage
state of marriage and the family in America today. (pp. 45 – 58). Westport, CT: Praeger.
New York: Knopf. Goodwin, P. Y., Mosher, W. D., & Chandra, A.
Cohan, C. L., & Kleinbaum, S. (2002). Toward a (2010). Marriage and cohabitation in the United
greater understanding of the cohabitation effect: States: A statistical portrait based on Cycle
Premarital cohabitation and marital communi- 6 (2002) of the National Survey of Family
cation. Journal of Marriage and Family, 64, Growth. Vital Health Stat 23(28). Washington,
180 – 192. DC: National Center for Health Statistics.
Collins, W. A., & Sroufe, L. A. (1999). Capacity for Harmon-Jones, E., & Harmon-Jones, C. (2002).
intimate relationships: A developmental construc- Testing the Action-Based Model of cognitive
tion. In W. Furman, C. Feiring, & B. B. Brown dissonance: The effect of action orientation on
(Eds.), Contemporary perspectives on adolescent postdecisional attitudes. Personality of Social Psy-
romantic relationships (pp. 123 – 147). New York: chology Bulletin, 28, 711 – 723.
Cambridge University Press. Hazan, C. & Shaver, P. (1987). Romantic love con-
Cook, K., Cheshire, C., & Gerbasi, A. (2006). Power, ceptualized as an attachment process. Journal of
dependence, and social exchange. In P. J. Burke Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 511 – 524.
(Ed.), Contemporary social psychological theories Homans, G. C. (1958). Social behavior as exchange.
(pp. 194 – 216). Stanford, CA: Stanford University American Journal of Sociology, 63, 597 – 606.
Press. Impett, E. A., Beals, K. P., & Peplau, L. A. (2001).
Cook, K. S., & Emerson, R. M. (1978). Power, equity Testing the investment model of relationship
and commitment in exchange networks. American commitment and stability in a longitudinal study
Sociological Review, 43, 721 – 739. of married couples. Current Psychology, 20,
Coontz, S. (2006). Marriage, a history: From 312 – 326.
obedience to intimacy; or, How love conquered Impett, E. A., Gable, K. P., & Peplau, L. A. (2005).
marriage. New York: Viking. Giving up and giving in: The costs and benefits of
Davila, J., & Bradbury, T. N. (2001). Attachment daily sacrifice in intimate relationships. Journal of
insecurity and the distinction between unhappy Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 327 – 344.
spouses who do and do not divorce. Journal of Johnson, D. J., & Rusbult, C. E. (1989). Resisting
Family Psychology, 15, 371 – 393. temptation: Devaluation of alternative partners
Davila, J., & Cobb, R. (2004). Predictors of change as a means of maintaining commitment in close
in attachment security during adulthood. In relationships. Journal of Personality and Social
W. S. Rholes & J. A. Simpson (Eds.), Adult Psychology, 57, 967 – 980.
attachment: Theory, research, and clinical impli- Johnson, M. P. (1973). Commitment: A conceptual
cations (pp. 133 – 156). New York: Guilford Press. structure and empirical application. Sociological
Eastwick, P. W., & Finkel, E. J. (2008). The attach- Quarterly, 14, 395 – 406.
ment system in fledgling relationships: An acti- Johnson, M. P., Caughlin, J. P., & Huston, T. L.
vating role for attachment anxiety. Journal of (1999). The tripartite nature of marital commit-
Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 628 – 647. ment: Personal, moral, and structural reasons to
Feeney, J., Noller, P., & Roberts, N. (2000). Attach- stay married. Journal of Marriage and the Family,
ment and close relationships. In C. Hendrick 61, 160 – 177.
& S. S. Hendrick (Eds.), Close relationships: A Kamp Dush, C. M., Cohan, C. L., & Amato, P. R.
sourcebook (pp. 185 – 201). Thousand Oaks, CA: (2003). The relationship between cohabitation
Sage. and marital quality and stability: Change across
Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. cohorts? Journal of Marriage and Family, 65,
Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. 539 – 549.
Fisher, H. (2005). Broken hearts: The nature and risks Kelley, H. H., & Thibaut, J. W. (1978). Interpersonal
of romantic rejection. In A. Booth & A. C. Crouter relations: A theory of interdependence. New York:
(Eds.), Romance and sex in adolescence and Wiley.
emerging adulthood: Risks and opportunities Kline, G. H., Stanley, S. M., Markman, H. J., Olmos-
(pp. 3 – 28). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Gallo, P. A., St. Peters, M., Whitton, S. W., &
256 Journal of Family Theory & Review

Prado, L. (2004). Timing is everything: Pre- Roisman, G., Collins, A., Sroufe, A., & Egeland, B.
engagement cohabitation and increased risk (2005). Predictors of young adults’ representa-
for poor marital outcomes. Journal of Family tions of and behavior in their current romantic
Psychology, 18, 311 – 318. relationship: Prospective tests of the prototype
Le, B., & Agnew, C. R. (2003). Commitment and hypothesis, Attachment and Human Development,
its theorized determinants: A meta-analysis of 7, 105 – 121.
the investment model. Personal Relationships, 10, Rowthorn, R. (2002). Marriage as a signal. In A. W.
37 – 57. Dnes and R. Rowthorn (Eds.), The law and eco-
Levinger, G. (1965). Marital cohesiveness and disso- nomics of marriage and divorce (pp. 132 – 156).
lution: An integrative review. Journal of Marriage New York: Cambridge University Press.
and the Family, 27, 19 – 28. Rusbult, C. E. (1980). Commitment and satisfaction
Levinger, G. (1979). A social exchange view on the in romantic associations: A test of the investment
dissolution of pair relationships. In R. L. Burgess model. Journal of Experimental Social Psychol-
& T. L. Huston (Eds.), Social exchange in deve- ogy, 16, 172 – 186.
loping relationships. New York: Academic Press. Rusbult, C. E., & Buunk, B. P. (1993) Commitment
Lichter, D., & Qian, Z. (2008). Serial cohabitation processes in close relationships: An interdepen-
and the marital life course. Journal of Marriage dence analysis. Journal of Social and Personal
and Family, 70, 861 – 878. Relationships, 10, 175 – 204.
Lindsay, J. M. (2000). An ambiguous commitment: Rusbult, C. E., & Verette, J. (1991). An interdepen-
Moving into a cohabiting relationship. Journal of dence analysis of accommodation processes in
Family Studies, 6, 120 – 134. close relationships. Representative Research in
Macklin, E. (1972). Heterosexual cohabitation among Social Psychology, 19(1), 3 – 33.
unmarried college students. Family Coordinator, Rusbult, C. E., Zembrodt, I. M., & Gunn, L. K.
21, 463 – 472. (1982). Exit, voice, loyalty, and neglect: Responses
Manning, W. D., & Smock, P. J. (2005). Measuring to dissatisfaction in romantic involvement. Jour-
and modeling cohabitation: New perspectives from nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43,
qualitative data. Journal of Marriage and Family, 1230 – 1242.
67, 989 – 1002. Sassler, S. (2004). The process of entering into cohab-
Markman, H. J., & Floyd, F. (1980). Possibilities for iting unions. Journal of Marriage and Family, 66,
the prevention of marital discord: A behavioral 491 – 505.
perspective. American Journal of Family Therapy, Selcuk, E., Zayas, V., & Hazan, C. (2010). Beyond
8, 29 – 48. satisfaction: The role of attachment in marital func-
McLanahan, S. (in press). Children in fragile families. tioning. Journal of Family Theory and Review, 2,
In M. Carlson & P. England (Eds.), Changing fam- 258 – 279.
ilies in an unequal society. Stanford, CA: Stanford Simon, D., Krawczyk, D. C., & Holyoak, K. J.
University Press. (2004). Construction of preferences by constraint
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2007). Attachment satisfaction. Psychological Science, 15, 331 – 336.
in adulthood: Structure, dynamics, and change. Simpson, J. A. (1990). Influence of attachment styles
New York: Guilford Press. on romantic relationships. Journal of Personality
Molm, L. D. (2006). The social exchange framework. and Social Psychology, 59, 971 – 980.
In P. J. Burke (Ed.), Contemporary social psy- Smock, P. J. (2000). Cohabitation in the United
chological theories (pp. 24 – 45). Stanford, CA: States: An appraisal of research themes, findings,
Stanford University Press. and implications. Annual Review of Sociology, 26,
Nock, S. L. (1995). A comparison of marriages and 1 – 20.
cohabiting relationships. Journal of Family Issues, Smock, P. J., & Manning, W. D. (2004). Living
16, 53 – 76. together unmarried in the United States: Demo-
Nock, S. L., Sanchez, L. A., & Wright, J. D. (2008). graphic perspectives and implications for family
Covenant marriage and the movement to reclaim policy. Law and Policy, 26, 87 – 117.
tradition. Piscataway, NJ: Rutgers University Sprecher, S., Schmeeckle, M., & Felmlee, D. (2006).
Press. The principle of least interest: Inequality in emo-
Rhoades, G. K., Stanley, S. M., & Markman, H. J. tional involvement in romantic relationships. Jour-
(2009b). The pre-engagement cohabitation effect: nal of Family Issues, 27, 1255 – 1280.
A replication and extension of previous findings. Stafford, L., Kline, S. L., & Rankin, C. (2004). Mar-
Journal of Family Psychology, 23, 107 – 111. ried individuals, cohabiters, and cohabiters who
Rhoades, G. K., Stanley, S. M., & Markman, H. J. marry: A longitudinal study of relational and indi-
(2009a). Couples’ reasons for cohabitation: vidual well-being. Journal of Social and Personal
Associations with individual well-being and Relationships, 21, 231 – 248.
relationship quality. Journal of Family Issues, 30, Stanley, S. M. (2002, July). What is it with men and
233 – 258. commitment, anyway? Keynote address to the 6th
Commitment 257

annual Smart Marriages Conference, Washington, and premarital or nonmarital cohabitation. Journal
DC. Retrieved May 28, 2010, from http://www. of Family Issues, 25, 496 – 519.
prepinc.com/main/docs/commitment.pdf Surra, C. A., & Hughes, D. K. (1997). Commitment
Stanley, S. M. (2005). The power of commitment. San processes in accounts of the development of pre-
Francisco: Jossey-Bass. marital relationships. Journal of Marriage and the
Stanley, S. M., Lobitz, W. C., & Dickson, F. (1999). Family, 59, 5 – 21.
Using what we know: Commitment and cogni- Thibaut, J. W., & Kelley, H. H. (1959). The social
tions in marital therapy. In W. Jones & J. Adams psychology of groups. New York: Wiley.
(Eds.), Handbook of interpersonal commitment Thornton, A., Axinn, W. G., & Xie, Y. (2007). Mar-
and relationship stability (pp. 379 – 392). New riage and cohabitation. Chicago: University of
York: Plenum Press. Chicago Press.
Stanley, S. M., & Markman, H. J. (1992). Assessing Tran, S., & Simpson, J. A. (2009). Prorelationship
commitment in personal relationships. Journal of maintenance behaviors: The joint roles of attach-
Marriage and the Family, 54, 595 – 608. ment and commitment. Journal of Personality and
Stanley, S. M., Rhoades, G. K., Amato, P. R., Mark- Social Psychology, 97, 685 – 698.
man, H. J., & Johnson, C. A. (2010). The timing Van Lange, P. A. M., Rusbult, C. E., Drigotas, S. M.,
of cohabitation and engagement: Impact on first Arriaga, X. B., Witcher, B. S. & Cox, C. L. (1997).
and second marriages. Journal of Marriage and Willingness to sacrifice in close relationships.
Family 72, 906 – 918. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72,
Stanley, S. M., Rhoades, G. K., & Fincham, F. D. 1373 – 1395.
(in press). Understanding romantic relationships Waller, W. (1938). The family: A dynamic interpre-
among emerging adults: The significant roles of tation. New York: Gordon.
cohabitation and ambiguity. In F. D. Fincham & Whitton, S.W., Stanley, S. M., & Markman, H. J.
M. Cui (Eds.), Romantic relationships in emerg- (2007). If I help my partner, will it hurt me? Percep-
ing adulthood. New York: Cambridge University tions of sacrifice in romantic relationships. Journal
Press. of Social and Clinical Psychology, 26, 64 – 92.
Stanley, S. M., Rhoades, G. K., & Markman, H. J. Wieselquist, J., Rusbult, C. E., Foster, C. A.,
(2006). Sliding vs. deciding: Inertia and the pre- & Agnew, C. R. (1999). Commitment,
marital cohabitation effect. Family Relations, 55, pro-relationship behavior, and trust in close rela-
499 – 509. tionships. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
Stanley, S. M., Whitton, S. W., Low, S. M., chology, 77, 942 – 966.
Clements, M. L., & Markman, H. J. (2006). Zeifman, D., & Hazan, Z. (2008). Pair bonds
Sacrifice as a predictor of marital outcomes. Family as attachments. Reevaluating the evidence. In
Process, 45, 289 – 303. J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of
Stanley, S. M., Whitton, S. W., & Markman, H. J. attachment: Theory, research, and clinical appli-
(2004). Maybe I do: Interpersonal commitment cations (436 – 455). New York: Guilford Press.

You might also like