You are on page 1of 27

Journal of Personal Selling & Sales Management

ISSN: 0885-3134 (Print) 1557-7813 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rpss20

An investigation of salespeople’s nonverbal


behaviors and their effect on charismatic
appearance and favorable consumer responses

Sandra Pauser, Udo Wagner & Claus Ebster

To cite this article: Sandra Pauser, Udo Wagner & Claus Ebster (2018) An investigation of
salespeople’s nonverbal behaviors and their effect on charismatic appearance and favorable
consumer responses, Journal of Personal Selling & Sales Management, 38:3, 344-369, DOI:
10.1080/08853134.2018.1480383

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/08853134.2018.1480383

© The Authors. Published with license by


Taylor and Francis Group, LLC.

Published online: 31 Oct 2018.

Submit your article to this journal

View Crossmark data

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=rpss20
Journal of Personal Selling & Sales Management, 2018
Vol. 38, No. 3, 344–369, https://doi.org/10.1080/08853134.2018.1480383

An investigation of salespeople’s nonverbal behaviors and their effect on charismatic appearance


and favorable consumer responses
Sandra Pauser, Udo Wagner and Claus Ebster
Faculty of Business, Economics and Statistics, University of Vienna, Oskar-Morgenstern-Platz 1, Vienna, 1090, Austria
(Received 16 February 2017; accepted 20 May 2018)

The concept of charisma has gained considerable interest among social scientists in multiple disciplines. Nevertheless,
research on charisma in the marketing field is scarce, and little is known about which specific nonverbal behaviors pre-
dict perceived charisma and lead to positive consumer responses. Therefore, the aim of this article is to identify nonver-
bal behaviors that lead to a salesperson’s perceived charisma in a personal selling context by means of high-precision,
time-locked coding. This research explores aspects of body language that differentiate salespeople from each other and
investigates whether such differences are antecedents of perceived charisma. The findings indicate that certain arm
actions, arm postures, and action functions have a significant effect on charismatic appearance and can in turn produce
favorable attitudes toward the salesperson.
Keywords: charisma; nonverbal behaviors; symmetry; asymmetry; attitude toward the salesperson; culture

Uncovering characteristics and factors that predict a content of a conversation) and nonverbally (communica-
salesperson’s performance is of significant interest to tion other than words), with delivery of a message being
managers and researchers (Chakrabarty, Widing, and the “key determinant of perceived charisma” (Awamleh
Brown 2014). In the past 25 years, the concept of cha- and Gardner 1999, 350). Thus, charismatic nonverbal
risma has gained considerable interest among social sci- communication is conceptualized as “the ability to modu-
entists, and its positive effects on performance outcomes late nonverbal behavior to enhance client engagement”
have been reported in multiple research areas. Likewise, (Heide 2013, 305). Similarly, sales literature has stressed
Bass (1997) noted a strong association between a sales- the importance of nonverbal messages as a prominent
person’s charisma and effective selling. In short, tool for salespeople (Stewart, Hecker, and Graham 1987).
researchers and practitioners agree that charisma is an Evidence shows that nonverbal messages account for the
important asset for salespeople. Despite this agreement, majority of overall communication and can boost favor-
studies on the role of charisma in marketing and personal able customer responses (Leigh and Summers 2002;
selling literature are scarce. A recent call for papers in Wood 2006). Both streams of literature agree on the gen-
the Journal of Personal Selling & Sales Management eral importance of nonverbal communication for a multi-
(JPSSM 2017) stressed the importance of incorporating tude of response factors and the limited repertoire of
theories and concepts from outside the field into market- thoroughly analyzed communication behaviors. At the
ing. In line with this call, we argue that charisma is a same time, they are vague on providing guidance to sales
significant skill for members of the sales force. Thus, the representatives on specific behaviors to employ during
charisma of salespeople, specifically the way charisma sales conversations and thus recommend further research
manifests during a sales talk, its perception by potential in this area (e.g., DePaulo 2014). To fill this gap, we
buyers, and its effects on customers’ responses (e.g., atti- extend previous research by objectively measuring and
tude toward sellers), defines the content of this article. examining specific nonverbal behaviors as antecedents of
Recent conceptualizations refer to charisma as the perceived charisma in a sales context and demonstrate
collection of communication behaviors that enhance a that the salesperson’s perceived charisma influences a
person’s appeal in an inspirational manner (Bolkan and buyer’s attitude toward the salesperson, which is an
Goodboy 2014). Charisma manifests both verbally (actual important driver of salesperson effectiveness.

Corresponding author. Email: sandra.pauser@univie.ac.at


ß The Authors. Published with license by Taylor and Francis Group, LLC. This is an Open Access article distributed under the
terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.
0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly
cited, and is not altered, transformed, or built upon in any way.

# 2018 Pi Sigma Epsilon National Educational Foundation


Journal of Personal Selling & Sales Management 345

As noted, recommendations on specific nonverbal sections, we discuss our conceptual considerations,


communication behaviors in the literature are rare; how- including a literature review. Two research hypotheses
ever, there are some notable exceptions. Mehrabian for the empirical investigations follow. The subsequent
(1969), a pioneer in nonverbal communication and sections present our methodology. Finally, the theoretical
impression formation, revealed in his experimental stud- and methodological contributions, implications for mar-
ies that a communicator’s symmetrical arm movements keters, limitations, and future research opportunities con-
positively affect the recipient’s attitude toward the com- clude the article.
municator. Moreover, recent research confirms the posi-
tive relationship between symmetrical arm gestures and
charisma (Cui et al. 2017). We build on this evidence Conceptual model and hypotheses
and focus specifically on nonverbal behaviors such as The impact of the salesperson’s nonverbal communica-
arm actions and postures and analyze whether they are tion messages on the customer’s perceptions and sales-
executed symmetrically or asymmetrically. related responses denotes the focus of this research.
To our knowledge, no studies have used a detailed Conceptually, we draw on Williams, Spiro, and Fines’s
and objective method to measure nonverbal behaviors. In (1990) salesperson–customer interaction framework,
previous research, nonverbal behaviors were coded by depicting communication at the core of a sales dyad.
binary measures or on a scale that neglected the duration Rooted in social psychology, this model is based on ear-
of certain movements over the course of time (i.e., lier work by Mehrabian (1969) and Mehrabian and
Antonakis, Fenley, and Liechti 2011; Clark and Williams (1969) on interpersonal relations holding the
Greatbatch 2011). We apply an objective method for assumption that nonverbal communication composes the
encoding nonverbal communication behaviors that differs majority of the overall message and a conversation part-
from previous research in that it uses the body action ner elicits perceptions and attitudes based on these cues.
and posture (BAP) coding system (Dael, Mortillaro, and Conceptualized as impression formation, these inferences
Scherer 2012) supplemented by the Anvil video annota- are referred to as social impressions that are formed dur-
tion software (Kipp 2000) that allows for high-precision, ing an initial encounter (Burgoon 1985; Riggio and
time-locked coding. To address these research gaps, this Friedman 1986). The model captures the observer’s for-
article includes two empirical studies. In Study 1, we mation of inferences about social impressions based on
videotaped 44 salespeople from two cultures while they the sender’s nonverbal cues (Patterson 1991). Prior
pitched their products or services and then coded their research has identified nonverbal cues as a key determin-
nonverbal behaviors using the BAP coding scheme. In ant in the formation of initial impressions across disci-
Study 2, participants evaluated the videotaped sales pre- plines – that is, in social psychology (Riggio and
sentations by rating the salesperson’s charisma and indi- Friedman 1986) and in charisma research – in the man-
cating their attitudes toward him or her. agement and leadership literature (Gardner and Avolio
In summary, this article makes four significant con- 1998). Leigh and Summers (2002) were the first to study
tributions to the sales literature. First, it explores aspects buyers’ impressions of salespeople’s nonverbal cues in
of body language that differentiate salespeople from buyer–seller interactions. In theoretical conformity with
each other by objectively recording their nonverbal com- earlier work on impression formation, customers form
munication behaviors by means of high-precision, time- perceptions and attitudes toward the salesperson based
locked coding. Second, it investigates which specific on the latter’s nonverbal communication behaviors.
nonverbal behaviors are antecedents of perceived cha- Applying this reasoning about the scope of impression
risma in a sales context, an area in which little empirical formation to the present case, charisma is the construct
evidence exists. Third, it demonstrates that the seller’s to be analyzed, nonverbal communication messages
perceived charisma influences the buyer’s attitude (i.e., symmetric vs. asymmetric arm movements) are the
toward the seller, and thus customer response is medi- seller’s cues sent, and customer impressions are the
ated by perceived charisma. This is important for estab- observer’s inferences drawn.
lishing charisma as a relevant variable in the marketing Figure 1 illustrates our conceptual model depicting a
field. Fourth, to investigate salesperson charisma in sales dyad. In line with prior work in social psychology
greater depth and because prior studies are limited (Mehrabian 1969), charisma research (Awamleh and
mainly to one culture, we conduct the studies in different Gardner 1999), and the personal selling literature (Leigh
cultural contexts. While charisma is a universal phenom- and Summers 2002), perceptions and attitudes toward
enon, the results of this cultural comparison indicate that the salesperson form the social impressions investigated
the nonverbal behaviors that predict charisma in a sales herein. The tenets of impression formation suggest that
conversation are culture specific, a finding of particular certain nonverbal cues should trigger charismatic appear-
importance to international marketers. In the next ance and, in turn, produce favorable attitudes toward the
346 S. Pauser et al.

Figure 1. Conceptual model.

salesperson. Thus, the impact of the salesperson’s com- charisma as the “articulation of specific behaviors”
munication messages on the customer’s perceptions and (Bolkan and Goodboy 2014, 137) and highlight the import-
attitudes denotes the focus of this research, as charisma ance of nonverbal communication. Conceptualizations in
is transmitted through these communication messages. In social psychology refer to charisma as the conveyance of
line with Figure 1, the subsequent literature review positive dispositions during personal interactions through
guides the reader through the conceptual model starting affability and the ability to guide others. Thus, “displays of
on the salesperson’s side with charisma being discussed charismatic behavior are necessary for charisma to be
as a learnable quality. The manifestation of a sales- accurately and consistently perceived by observers”
person’s charisma through nonverbal communication (Tskhay et al. 2018, 147). Applying the construct of cha-
messages follows, serving as a basis for discussing the risma to a personal selling context, we define charisma as
effects of nonverbal messages on customer impressions. the articulation of communication messages that enhance a
The mediating role of perceived charisma on the rela- salesperson’s appeal.
tionship between nonverbal messages and attitude Collectively, the literature suggests that charisma is
toward the salesperson concludes this review, before
relevant to the study of personal selling. For example,
introducing the moderated role of culture.
Bass (1997) identifies a strong association between a
salesperson’s charisma and effective selling, as strong cor-
Charisma in personal selling relations exist between a salesperson’s charismatic behav-
ior and actual sales (Garcia 1995). In line with this view,
The term charisma stems from the Greek word charis-
mata, which means “the gift of grace” (Weber 1947), we propose charisma as a potential driver of sales success.
implying that charisma is innate. In modern conceptualiza-
tions, charisma is considered a learnable quality Nonverbal messages
(Antonakis, Fenley, and Liechti 2011; Towler 2003) that
yields favorable performance outcomes across disciplines. Manifestation of charisma
Hwang, Khatri, and Srinivas (2005, 962–963) argue that Charisma manifests both verbally and nonverbally
charismatic individuals have the “ability to convey mes- (Heide 2013); however, research consistently highlights
sages in a most convincing manner to charm their the importance of delivery versus communication con-
followers.” Other recent conceptualizations refer to tent (for a tabular review see Appendix 1). For example,
Journal of Personal Selling & Sales Management 347

scholars experimentally tested the effect of the content arm gestures are associated with favorable outcomes
(i.e., visionary vs. nonvisionary) and delivery (i.e., strong (for a tabular review see Appendix 2), including cus-
vs. weak) of a message on perceptions of charisma in a tomer satisfaction with the service provided (Beck,
leadership context (Holladay and Coombs 1994; Daughtridge, and Sloane 2002), and most important,
Awamleh and Gardner 1999). Their results suggest that they enhance perceptions of charisma (Cui et al. 2017)
both content and delivery are important but that delivery and yield favorable attitudes toward the communicator
has a stronger effect on the perception of charisma, and (Mehrabian and Williams 1969). This is because sym-
this holds true when communication is enhanced through metrical stimuli are more easily processed (Little and
eye contact, gestures, vocal variety, and facial expres- Jones 2003; Wilson and Wilkinson 2002), and they pro-
sions (Holladay and Coombs 1994). Thus, charisma duce significantly higher brain activity than asymmet-
tends to better reflect nonverbal than verbal rical control stimuli (Sasaki et al. 2005; Tyler et al.
communication. 2005). In accordance with these findings, Anasingaraju
Across disciplines, research has focused on the iden- et al. (2016, 2) propose the term charisma gesture and
tification of nonverbal charismatic communication define it as“synchronous-parallel-outward hand gesture.”
behaviors that yield performance outcomes. The third Still other studies on gesticulation suggest that people
column of Appendix 1 applies the categorization of use symmetric arm actions and two-handed gestures
nonverbal charismatic communication proposed by more frequently than one-sided gesticulations (e.g.,
Antonakis, Fenley, and Liechti (2011) on nonverbal Efron 1972).
behaviors analyzed by the respective authors and distin-
guishes among voice characteristics, body movements,
and facial expressions. However, the repertoire of the Effects of nonverbal communication messages on
analyzed nonverbal communication behaviors is rather customers’ impressions
limited and still leaves open questions, especially with Perceived charisma
regard to sales. Clark and Greatbatch (2011) also note Extant literature, as presented in Appendix 3, shows that
the limited repertoire of bodily behaviors; they indicate nonverbal communication style elicits perceptions of
that a research gap remains with respect to specific bod- charisma. These perceptions can be improved by
ily behaviors (e.g., arm actions, arm postures) that employing symmetrical gestures to deliver the message
express a person’s charisma. (Cui et al. 2017). Similarly, sales-related research (for a
Although prior research has demonstrated the import- tabular review see Appendix 4) investigates the effect-
ance of nonverbal communication, none of the studies iveness of nonverbal behavior and shows that nonverbal
(see Appendix 1, column six) use a detailed and object- cues such as gaze and speech characteristics influence
ive method to encode nonverbal behaviors. So far, customers’ perceptions of the salesperson (Leigh and
researchers have mostly employed experimental studies Summers 2002). Gabbott and Hogg (2000) examined
and used actors who manipulated their nonverbal behav- service evaluations and demonstrated the effects of non-
iors as stimuli to observe their effects on attributions of verbal communication on customers’ perceptions. Their
charisma and other outcomes. We further extend this experimental design provides evidence of a significant
research by examining specific nonverbal behaviors as difference in customers’ reactions to a salesperson’s non-
antecedents of perceived charisma in a sales context and verbal behavior. Nevertheless, DePaulo (2014) called for
by applying an objective method for encoding nonverbal further studies to investigate different levels of nonverbal
communication behaviors. behavior and to link them with customers’ impressions.
The study of perceptions is well established in sales
research (Hall, Ahearne, and Sujan 2015; Mullins et al.
The role of symmetry in charismatic communication 2014). On the one hand, research has found that it is
Whereas most scholars report on the general relationship feasible to make accurate intuitive judgments of a cus-
between charismatic communication and perceived cha- tomer’s needs based on the customer’s nonverbal behav-
risma or response variables, recent research is more con- iors (Alavi, Wieseke, and Guba 2016). On the other
crete, recommending the use of symmetrical arm hand, customers’ formed perceptions of the sales force
gestures to increase charisma. Research on symmetry can affect their responses (Babin, Babin, and Boles
versus asymmetry is widely spread among disciplines 1999). We follow this latter view, focusing on customer
such as social psychology, biological science, evolution, perceptions of salespeople.
and human behavior. For example, Palmer and The literature provides strong evidence of the posi-
Hemenway (1978) show that symmetry is an important tive relationship between nonverbal behavior and percep-
determinant in interactions that yields various positive tions in charisma and sales research. Thus, we anticipate
perceptions. Research further reveals that symmetrical that customers form perceptions of a salesperson’s
348 S. Pauser et al.

charisma depending on his or her nonverbal communica- established faster than more elaborate customer response
tion behaviors. Given the close link between charisma variables (i.e., attitude toward the salesperson). In
and nonverbal communication and the prominent role of accordance with person perception research, very brief
symmetry within the context of nonverbal behaviors and observations of nonverbal behavior have been shown to
charisma (Cui et al. 2017), we further anticipate that impact consumers’ perceptions of salespeople, which in
salespeople who employ more symmetrical gestures will turn translate into more elaborate customer responses
be perceived as more charismatic. (Ambady, Krabbenhoft, and Hogan 2006). Moreover,
De Vries, Bakker-Pieper, and Oostenveld (2010) provide
convincing evidence for the mediating role of charisma
Attitude toward the salesperson in the relation between communication style and out-
The sales literature suggests that nonverbal messages can come variables (i.e., performance, satisfaction, commit-
influence not only the customers' perceptions of the ment). Thus, we expect customers’ perceptions of a
salesperson but also favorable customer responses salesperson’s charisma to mediate the relationship
(Wood 2006; Leigh and Summers 2002). The relation- between nonverbal communication messages and cus-
ship between nonverbal communication and social tomer response.
impressions (i.e., attitudes) is well established in psych- H1: Salespeople’s perceived charisma mediates the
ology (Mehrabian 1969) and charisma research (Gardner positive (negative) relationship between salespeople’s
and Avolio 1998). Rooted in social psychology, most of symmetrical (asymmetrical) nonverbal behaviors and
the research on the role of nonverbal communication in customer’s attitude toward them.
impression formation dates back to Mehrabian and his
colleagues revealing that a communicator’s symmetrical
arm movements positively affect the recipient’s attitude Symmetric and asymmetric nonverbal communication
toward the communicator (Mehrabian and Williams behaviors across cultures
1969). Similarily, Babin, Babin and Boles (1999) point Cultural traditions play an important role in nonverbal
to the prominent role of a salesperson’s appearance and communication. For example, Khatri, Ng, and Lee
behavior and its effect on customers’ attitudes, which in (2001) note that charismatic communication is likely to
turn might impact their purchase decisions. Against this have culture-specific facets, even though the definition
background, and in line with Mehrabian (1969) and of charisma holds true across cultures. Similarly,
Mehrabian and Williams (1969), we posit a positive rela- research provides direct empirical evidence that hand
tionship between a salesperson’s symmetrical nonverbal and arm movements vary across cultures and that these
messages and customers’ attitudes toward them. movements account for the majority of human gestures
during interactions. Efron (1972) was the first to uncover
that symmetry and asymmetry in the employment of ges-
The mediating role of perceived charisma
turing matter across cultures. In line with this finding,
Research has extensively examined the relationship several studies compare gestural actions across cultures
between nonverbal communication and performance out- and find that there are cultural differences in the fre-
comes (i.e., Holladay and Coombs 1994), though propo- quency and type of gesture used. One widely adopted
sitions as to whether this influence is direct (charismatic cultural classification is based on high- and low-gesture
nonverbal behaviors ! response) or indirect (charis- cultures with respect to the “complexity of gesturing”
matic nonverbal behaviors ! perceived charisma ! (Kendon 1981). For example, English and German are
response) are mixed. Nevertheless, research is in con- low-gesture languages, while Hebrew is considered a
formity that the delivery of a message is a key determin- high-gesture language (Pika, Nicoladis, and Marentette
ant of perceived charisma (Awamleh and Gardner 1999), 2006). The proximity hypothesis suggests that cultural
and individuals who are perceived as more charismatic and linguistic proximity between different ethnicities
are further able to induce favorable outcomes (Holladay also extends to nonverbal communication; that is, people
and Coombs 1994). Recent support of the indirect effect with greater cultural and linguistic proximity will inter-
stems from Sy, Choi, and Johnson (2013), who reveal act more easily nonverbally than individuals originating
that perceptions of charisma mediate the effect of non- from less similar cultures (Rosenthal et al. 1979). Thus,
verbal behaviors on response variables. Given their in line with the classification of languages, Germanic
appeal, individuals who are perceived as more charis- languages possess a greater linguistic proximity to
matic are better able to exhibit favorable impressions English than non-Indo-European languages (e.g.,
during interactions. Our empirical study analyzes exten- Hebrew). To date, research has been predominantly con-
sive rather than impulsive purchase decisions, which is ducted in English-speaking countries (see Appendix 2,
in line with the view that perceptions of charisma are last column), and therefore low-gesture cultures are well
Journal of Personal Selling & Sales Management 349

represented in the literature. Studies on gesticulation link these behaviors to customers’ impressions in two
conducted in low-gesture cultures suggest that symmetry distinct cultures.
in the arrangement of arms is more prevalent (e.g., The empirical investigation conducts a comparison
McNeill and Levy 1982; Shuter 1979) and symmetrical between a low- and a high-gesture culture. We selected
(versus asymmetrical) arm gesturing yields favorable Austria as representative of a low-gesture culture, while
outcomes, including attitudes toward the communicator Israel served as representative of a high-gesture culture
(Mehrabian 1969). In line with these results, charisma because of extensive relevant evidence (see Appendix 2).
research conducted in low-gesture cultures identifies par- Common to prior studies in sales is the use of laboratory
allel outward-focused (symmetrical) gestures as particu- settings (see Appendix 1), thus challenging the external
larly charismatic (Cui et al. 2017). The dominance of validity of the results. Therefore, to improve external
investigations of low-gesture cultures might bias these validity, we use sales staff (entrepreneurs/managers of
results, since the mode of gesticulation in certain cul- start-up companies) instead of a confederate/actor and
tures can differ significantly from the symmetrical ges- analyze their natural nonverbal communication during
turing style (Efron 1972; Shuter 1979). For example, sales presentations. Furthermore, we improve measure-
asymmetrical nonverbal behaviors are especially preva- ment by employing a coding system that includes
lent in high-gesture cultures, such as Israel. In his study, recording time stamps per variable over the observation
Efron (1972) reports that in Israel, people frequently ges- period and considers a greater variety of body move-
ture with one arm only; when they use both arms, they ments than existing measures applied in sales research.
tend to move them in an asymmetrical fashion (Efron As customer attitudes are likely to vary across prod-
1972; Kendon 2004; Shuter 1979). In line with the prox- ucts or services of the different start-up companies (Arias-
imity hypotheses, asymmetrical gestures are more Bolzmann, Chakraborty, and Mowen 2000), we include a
“inherited” among this culture (Rosenthal et al. 1979, covariate measuring the customer’s attitude toward the
74); thus, we expect asymmetry to be perceived more product/service, as suggested by Yuhmiin and Thorson
favorably in high-gesture cultures. (2004), to account for this potential impact (see Figure 1).
As gesturing in a high-gesture culture differs signifi- This is also in line with the suggestion of Babin, Babin,
cantly from symmetric gesturing patterns, we postulate and Boles (1999), who provide evidence of the import-
that customers will perceive asymmetrical nonverbal ance of attitude toward the product in a sales context. In
behaviors as more charismatic in high-gesture cultures addition, considering this effect will allow a more reliable
and that they will have a more positive response to sales- assessment of the postulated hypotheses because of a
people who display asymmetrical nonverbal behaviors. more completely specified model.
H2a: Culture moderates the relationship between
nonverbal behaviors and perceived charisma. Specifically,
in high-gesture cultures, customers will perceive
Stimuli preparation
salespeople as more charismatic when they display more The same stimuli material – short, videotaped sales pre-
asymmetrical nonverbal behaviors. sentations (elevator pitches) from different companies in
the two cultures – was common to both studies. Study 1
H2b: Culture moderates the relationship between contributed to the setup of the model and explored
nonverbal behaviors and attitude toward the salespeople. aspects of nonverbal communication that differentiate
Specifically, in high-gesture cultures, customers will salespeople from each other by analyzing the presenters’
have a more positive attitude toward the salespeople
body language. Study 2 investigated H1 and H2 empiric-
when salespeople display more asymmetrical nonverbal
behaviors.
ally and asked potential customers to evaluate these
stimuli. One author videotaped elevator pitches from 44
people from the two cultures (low- and high-gesture cul-
tures) who were either company founders or start-up
Method
salespeople. These 44 people pitched actual products or
Design of empirical investigations and complementing services in a one-minute presentation; the upper panel of
the conceptual model Appendix 5 presents their demographics (e.g., 34%
Taken together, findings from the literature suggest that women, mean age 30 across both cultures). To provide a
communication style affects perceived charisma. In add- standardized setting for focusing on nonverbal body lan-
ition, studies on specific nonverbal behaviors and cues guage rather than other factors, the videos were recorded
that predict charisma are scarce. The current study aims in a professional setting, using the same camera position
to investigate different levels of nonverbal aspects of set to a medium-full shot and filmed against a white
charisma, with special emphasis on symmetric versus background. The salespeople’s bodies were visible from
asymmetric nonverbal communication behaviors, and to their heads to their knees. Each salesperson dressed in a
350 S. Pauser et al.

Figure 2. Examples of elevator pitch stimuli.

dark suit and a white shirt. No accessories, additives, or gesture culture) and 42 (for high-gesture culture) behav-
aids were allowed during the sales pitch. Figure 2 exhib- ioral variables (assigned to 12 categories) remained,
its examples of the stimuli. which formed the database for subsequent analysis.
Table 1 provides an overview of these variables and
their corresponding category. The manual by Dael,
Study 1: Impact of charisma on transmitting Mortillaro, and Scherer (2012) offers detailed descrip-
nonverbal messages tions of the coded variables. The recording granularity
Coding procedure was 25 observations per second (which resulted in
After recording the sales presentations, we measured the approximately 1,500 observations per video) for all vari-
nonverbal behaviors of the company founders/salespeo- ables. For tractability, we put aside dynamic aspects but
ple with the BAP coding system (Dael, Mortillaro, and aggregated data over time. Thus, analysis was based on
Scherer 2012). Two trained observers independently percentages obtained by summing the behavioral dummy
coded the videos using the Anvil video annotation soft- variables over all observations and tying them to the
ware (Kipp 2000) and eliminated coding divergences by total length of the sales presentation. For example, this
communicative matching. The BAP classifies 141 behav- approach shows that on average, salespersons from a
ioral variables into 12 categories: (i) head orientation, low-gesture country (assigned to cluster 1, category (i),
(ii) head action, (iii) head posture, (iv) trunk orientation, in Table 1) faced the audience during 83% of their pres-
(v) trunk action, (vi) trunk posture, (vii) arm action, entation time. The final data set included one observa-
(viii) arm posture, (ix) whole body posture, (x) gaze, (xi) tion per elevator pitch for all 43 variables, thereby
action functions, and (xii) other (Dael, Mortillaro, and describing the body language of all 44 salespeople.
Scherer 2012). The coding procedure required approxi-
mately 90 hours per coder for all 44 one-minute videos. Results
The text files exported from Anvil showed the time Categorization of the nonverbal behaviors of salespeople
frame (25 frames per second) in rows and the behavioral constituted the core interest of this study. Therefore,
(dummy) variables in columns (1 ¼ the presence of a cluster analysis represents the canonical method for pro-
body action or posture, 0 ¼ its absence in a given time viding insights into the structure of the data in both cul-
frame). After we removed several behavioral variables tures. Beginning with hierarchical clustering, we used
because of nonoccurrence, use of the sequential approach Ward’s fusion criterion and the squared Euclidean dis-
suggested by Dael, Mortillaro, and Scherer (2012) tance as a measure of dissimilarity to determine the
resulted in several pooled variables (e.g., we combined appropriate number of clusters: two for both cultures. A
“lateral head turn toward a left position” and “lateral k-means clustering fine-tuned the results. Table 1 dis-
head turn toward a right position” into a behavioral cat- plays means (per cluster and culture) for the behavioral
egory labeled “lateral head turn”). In all, 38 (for low- variables; moreover, it highlights (by exhibiting statistically
Table 1. Nonverbal communication variables analyzed and their means per cluster.

Low-gesture culture High-gesture culture


Cluster 1 Cluster 2 Cluster 1 Cluster 2
Nonverbal messages classified as
Category Behavioral variable symmetric asymmetric p-value symmetric asymmetric p-value

(i) Head orientation Head orientation facing .83(a) .69 .87 .75
(ii) Head action Head action .01 .04 .19 .20
(iii) Head posture Lateral head turn (left/right) .19 .28 .11 .20
Lateral head tilt (left/right) .09 .05 .18 .29
Vertical head tilt (upward/downward) .02 .03 .01 .01
Neck extension toward a forward .00 .01 .01 .00
head position
Neck retraction toward a backward .00 .00 .00 .05
head position
(iv) Trunk orientation Trunk orientation facing .99 1.00 .97 .97
(v) Trunk action Trunk action .00 .00 .07 .04
(vi) Trunk posture Trunk lean toward a forward position .05 .05 .04 .02
Trunk lean toward a backward position .04 .01 .00 .00
Lateral trunk lean (left/right) .28 .14 .03 .03
Lateral trunk rotation (left/right) .11 .26 .08 .05
(vii) Arm action Left arm action .32 .55 .30 .45
Right arm action C2 .31 .56 .33 .73 
Symmetrical arm action C1 .18 .06  .17 .23
Asymmetrical arm action C2 .17 .68  .18 .58 
(viii) Arm posture Arms at side (left arm/right arm) C2 .21 .29 .16 .38 
Left arm held in front C2 .00 .02 .03 .14 
Right arm held in front(b) .02 .04
Both arms held in front .00 .03 .02 .03
Left hand in pocket .00 .25 .02 .00
Right hand in pocket .01 .00
One arm holds other in front(c) C1 .42 .03  .47 .05 
Journal of Personal Selling & Sales Management

Left shoulder up .00 .14


Left shoulder down .00 .00
Symmetrical arms posture C1 .60 .08  .63 .15 
Asymmetrical arms posture C2 .04 .38 .11 .47 
(ix) Whole body posture Whole body moves or leans toward a .04 .00 .01 .01
forward position
Whole body moves or leans toward a .01 .00
backward position
(x) Gaze Gaze toward .92 .96 .88 .89
Gaze upward .02 .00 .01 .00
Gaze downward .01 .02 .03 .02
Gaze averted sideways .04 .01 .05 .08
Eyes closed .02 .01 .02 .01
(Continued on next page)
351
Table 1. Continued.
352
Low-gesture culture High-gesture culture
Cluster 1 Cluster 2 Cluster 1 Cluster 2
Nonverbal messages classified as
Category Behavioral variable symmetric asymmetric p-value symmetric asymmetric p-value

(xi) Action functions Emblem(d) C2 .03 .02 .03 .09 


Illustrator(e) C2 .30 .56  .28 .51 
Beat(f) C2 .00 .13 .01 .14 
Deictic(g) .02 .00 .03 .04
Manipulator(h) C1 .44 .06  .49 .07 
(xii) Other Knee bend C1 .34 .02  .03 .03
Leg movement C1 .10 .04  .07 .17
Touch .01 .00
Notes:
(a)
On average, members of this cluster faced the audience during 83% of their presentation time.
(b)
Blank entries correspond to missing data for this behavioral variable.
(c)
One arm holds other in front: “one arm rests on the other or is held by the other in front of the body” (Dael, Mortillaro, and Scherer 2012, 107).
(d)
Emblem: “a symbolic and conventionalized body action with a culturally defined fixed form-meaning relationship” (Dael, Mortillaro, and Scherer 2012, 107).
(e)
Illustrator: “conversational action that supports accompanying speech” (Dael, Mortillaro, and Scherer 2012, 107).
(f)
Beat: “repetitive action that accentuates points in time, illustrating structural or rhythmic aspects of co-occurring speech” (Dael, Mortillaro, and Scherer 2012, 107).
(g)
Deictic: “referential action indicating a real or abstract object, person, event or location in space” (Dael, Mortillaro, and Scherer 2012, 107).
(h)
Manipulator: “an action in which one part of the body manipulates another body part” (Dael, Mortillaro, and Scherer 2012, 108).
Means between clusters are significantly different at a 1% level, based on a Mann–Whitney test, N ¼ 22.
Means between clusters are significantly different at a 5% level, based on a Mann–Whitney test, N ¼ 22.
S. Pauser et al.

Significant at a 10% level, based on a Mann–Whitney test, N ¼ 22.


Variables representative of cluster 1 (symmetric) are marked with C1. Variables representative of cluster 2 (asymmetric) are marked with C2.
Journal of Personal Selling & Sales Management 353

significant differences) variables that contribute substan- are marked with C2. Moreover, the means are larger for
tially to differentiating between clusters. Only a few varia- the first cluster for symmetrical aspects, such as one arm
bles differentiated between clusters. Moreover, these holds other in front or symmetrical arms posture, but
variables primarily belong to the categories arm action, larger for the second cluster for more asymmetrical
arm posture, and action functions. Postures are general aspects, such as asymmetrical arm action or illustrator.
alignments to a particular resting position (e.g., arms in Salespeople belonging to cluster 1 spent an extensive
front), whereas actions represent local excursions from this amount of time using symmetrical arm postures (means
configuration (e.g., pointing arm gesture). Actions are fur- 60% and 63% for the two cultures, respectively, of the
ther described on a functional level as, for example, illus- total length of the video), one arm holds other in front
trators and manipulators. Manipulators are defined here as (means 42% and 47%, respectively) (as demonstrated by
hand-to-hand movements and closely correspond to a pos- person on the far left in Figure 2), and other arm functions
ture in which salespeople hold one arm in front of such as manipulators (means 44% and 49%, respectively).
the other. Members of cluster 2 used mainly asymmetric arm
There is also a high degree of consistency in the dif- actions (means 68% and 58%, respectively) and asymmet-
ferentiating variables for the two cultures (i.e., the same ric arm postures (means 38% and 47%, respectively).
behavioral variables in both cultures dominate cluster Among the action functions, salespeople applied illustra-
assignment). When comparing across cultures, we tors most frequently (means 56% and 51%, respectively).
observe the same patterns between means per cluster for Right arm action was more common for this cluster than
the behavioral variables (i.e., higher mean values in clus- for the other cluster and particularly strong for the high-
ter 1 than in cluster 2 for both cultures, or vice versa) gesture culture (mean 73%). These results substantiate the
and high correlations between the means of the same decision to name the two clusters as symmetrical and
clusters for the two cultures (i.e., .95 and .94 are the asymmetrical with respect to nonverbal messages.
between-sample correlations for clusters 1 and 2, As Appendix 5 shows, the two clusters did not differ
respectively). Therefore, interpretation of the clusters largely in age, but for the sample representing the low-
occurs simultaneously for both cultures by investigating gesture culture, there was some difference in terms of
the respective means for the behavioral variables. gender: women (57%) dominated the symmetric cluster,
First, 12 variables differed substantially between while men (88%) dominated the asymmetric cluster. The
clusters for at least one culture: (1) right arm action, (2) percentage of women was also higher in the symmetric
asymmetrical arm action, (3) arms at side (left arm/right cluster (40% vs. 25%) for the sample representing the
arm), (4) left arm held in front, (5) one arm holds other high-gesture culture.
in front, (6) symmetrical arms posture, (7) asymmetrical After the recording the elevator pitches, salespeople
arms posture, (8) emblem, (9) illustrator, (10) beat, (11) were asked to fill out a short questionnaire on their demo-
manipulator, and (12) knee bend. Table 1 presents their graphic and psychographic properties. One question (single
means and highlights variables with significantly differ- item, 7-point rating scale) asked for a self-perception of
ent means by reporting p levels: a Mann–Whitney test their charisma. We use this information as a secondary
would be significant for a type I error of 1% or 5% in and confirmatory means to describe the two clusters,
these cases. (We provide this information for descriptive however aim to point out that our conceptual model
purposes only, as caution should be used when employ- addresses the customer’s perception of the salesperson’s
ing the same data for clustering and testing; we use a charisma, which is in accordance with recent research
nonparametric statistic to account for the rather small pointing to the importance of customers’ perceptions
cluster sizes.) Because of the considerable between- over self-reported behaviors (Lussier and Hall 2018).
cluster differences, these variables contributed substan- Salespeople assigned to the symmetric cluster and repre-
tially to forming the two clusters. Second, two additional senting the low-gesture culture evaluated their charisma
variables had somewhat different means per cluster higher (5.57) than salespeople assigned to the asymmet-
(marked with asterisks in Table 1; a Mann–Whitney test ric cluster (4.63). The reverse holds for the high-gesture
would be significant for a type I error of 10%): symmet- culture (4.14 vs. 5.00). On the one hand, we find a statis-
rical arm action and leg movement. These variables also tically significant interaction effect (p ¼ .03 according to
assisted in building the two clusters. Finally, only minor an ANOVA F-test, df ¼ 40) between cluster membership
(but not significant) differences existed between the two and culture. On the other hand, more charismatic sales-
clusters with respect to the other eight categories. persons favor symmetric body movements in the low-
Thus, the two clusters primarily differ in terms of gesture culture and asymmetric body movements in the
various aspects of arm movements. Variables representa- high-gesture culture. These results are in accordance with
tive of cluster 1 (symmetric) in Table 1 are marked with the theoretical reasoning outlined earlier and therewith
C1 and variables representative of cluster 2 (asymmetric) confirm the setup of the model – that is, employment of
354 S. Pauser et al.

arm movements for nonverbal communication behaviors, agree), which produced reliable measurement (Cronbach’s
manifestation of charisma by nonverbal messages and, in a ¼ .94). Both attitude scales, also rated on a 7-point scale,
particular, by symmetric versus asymmetric movements. showed reliable measurement: the scale measuring attitude
toward the advertiser comprised three items (Cronbach’s
a ¼ .90), and the scale measuring attitude toward the prod-
Study 2: Impact of nonverbal communication messages uct/service consisted of four items (Cronbach’s a ¼ .95).
on perceived charisma and attitude toward the Appendix 6 shows the items employed and further psycho-
salespeople metric properties of the scales, which are also satisfac-
Descriptives and measurement tory in terms of significant factor loadings, construct,
The online study was a between-subjects design and and discriminant validity.
exposed respondents to the 44 elevator pitches employed To diminish the likelihood of common method vari-
in Study 1. We applied a quota-sampling procedure ance, we followed the procedure as recommended by
(based on age and gender) for the sample representing Podsakoff et al. (2003). First, in terms of the study
the low-gesture culture; respondents from an online design, we protected respondent anonymity, reduced
panel constitute the sample representing the high-gesture evaluation apprehension, made use of different response
culture. Random panelists were approached until the formats, and counterbalanced the question order ran-
respective quotas (age, gender) were completed. The domly. In addition, the questionnaire was carefully pre-
lower panel of Appendix 5 presents the demographics of tested. Second, in terms of statistical remedies, we
these samples. Respondents evaluated the perceived cha- applied the marker variable approach (Lindell and
risma of the presenters, indicating their attitudes toward Whitney 2001). Familiarity with the start-up company
the presenters as a measure of customer response. A pro- served as a marker variable since this variable is theoret-
cedure randomly assigned respondents to one of the 22 ically unrelated to the central variables of interest. The
elevator pitches per culture. They watched the one- mean of our marker variable’s correlation with the other
minute video presentation then filled out a questionnaire response variables was .06. Besides this approach,
containing well-established scales for charisma (Khatri, Harman’s single-factor test, as suggested by Podsakoff
Ng, and Lee 2001), attitude toward the salesperson et al. (2003), points to the argument that common
(MacKenzie and Lutz 1989), and attitude toward the method variance does not inflate the hypothesized rela-
product/service (Holbrook and Batra 1987) and, finally, tionships. Appendix 7 reports the correlations, means,
provided demographic information. and standard deviations of perceived charisma, self-
The charisma scale consisted of nine items rated on a perceived charisma of the salesperson, and attitude
7-point Likert scale (1 ¼ strongly disagree, 7 ¼ strongly toward the salesperson.

Table 2. Statistical analysis and investigation of the hypothesized relationships.

Response variable: Response variable:


Perceived charisma of salesperson Attitude toward the salesperson
Unstandardized Confidence Unstandardized Confidence
Predictor variables regression coefficient interval p value regression coefficient interval p value

Nonverbal behavior(a) .23 [.08, .54] .14 .06 [.09, .21] .44
Perceived charisma .58 [ .43, .74] <.01
Culture(b) .58 [ .29, .86] <.01 .44 [.60, .27] <.01
Nonverbal behavior  Culture .49 [.95, .03] .04 .22 [.46, .01] .06
Attitude toward the product/service .66 [ .43, .88] <.01 .28 [ .13, .43] <.01
Constant .60 [.37, 1.58] .22 1.45 [ .98, 1.93] <.01
R2 ¼ .68 F ¼ 21.04 <.01 R2 ¼ .89 F ¼ 58.67 <.01

Indirect Confidence Direct Confidence Total Confidence


Culture effect interval effect interval p value effect interval p value

Low-gesture culture .15 [.35, .01] .16 [.33, .00] .04 .32 [ .11, .56] .01
High-gesture culture .14 [.05, .32] .06 [.09, .21] .44 .20 [.01, .39] .08
Notes:
(a)
0 ¼ symmetric, 1 ¼ asymmetric.
(b)
0 ¼ low-gesture culture, 1 ¼ high-gesture culture.
*Bias-corrected confidence intervals are based on 5,000 bootstrap samples and a type I error of 5%.
Journal of Personal Selling & Sales Management 355

Figure 3. Evaluations of perceived charisma (left panel) and attitude toward the salesperson (right panel) per cluster and culture.

Results more symmetric body language (e.g., arm action and


The right-hand side of Figure 1 shows the conceptual arm posture) as more charismatic than salespeople favor-
model underlying Study 2. It represents a moderated ing asymmetric nonverbal messages (4.29 vs. 3.75); by
mediation model, with nonverbal communication mes- contrast, symmetric movements resulted in inferior per-
sages (i.e., symmetric vs. asymmetric movements) as the ceptions of a salesperson’s charisma for the sample rep-
independent variable, attitude toward the salesperson as resenting the high-gesture culture (3.37 vs. 3.71). The
the dependent variable, perceived charisma as the medi- effect of symmetrical gestures on perceived charisma
ator, and culture (low-gesture vs. high-gesture) as the depends on culture. Perceived charisma is generally lower
moderator. Attitude toward the product/service serves as for the high-gesture culture than the low-gesture culture
a covariate. The results of the cluster analysis of Study 1 (3.49 vs. 4.09); this result is in line with the literature, as
provided the binary independent variable. Mean evalua- Shamir (1994) reports relatively low values for perceived
tions for perceived charisma, attitude toward the sales- charisma in his study conducted in this culture. The
person, and attitude toward the product/service (all regression coefficient of the covariate attitude toward
averaged over the different between-subjects conditions product/service has a plausible sign; we note that despite
of Study 2) provided the mediator, the dependent vari- this highly significant influence, the other variables add
able, and the covariate. Hayes’s (2013) PROCESS pro- substantially to the explanatory power (in terms of stand-
cedure, model 8, analyzed the data. Presentations of the ardized regression coefficients) of the model. These find-
results (cf. Table 2) follow the recommendations pro- ings provide support for H2a from a statistical standpoint.
vided by Hayes (2013, 369).
Impact of nonverbal behavior and culture on attitude
toward the salesperson (H2b)
Impact of nonverbal behavior and culture on perceived The upper right panel of Table 2 presents the results of
charisma (H2a) the regression analysis. The overall model is statistically
The upper left panel of Table 2 presents the results of significant and explains a high percentage (89%) of the
the regression analysis on perceived charisma of the criterion variable’s variance. Perceived charisma (i.e.,
salesperson. The overall model is statistically significant .58), culture (i.e., .44), interaction nonverbal behav-
and explains a high percentage (68%) of the criterion ior  culture interaction (i.e., .22; borderline), and the
variable’s variance. Parameter estimates for culture covariate (i.e., .28) are all statistically significant. The
(i.e., .58), interaction of nonverbal behavior  culture panel on the right-hand side of Figure 3 depicts the inter-
(i.e., .49), and the covariate (i.e., .66) are all statistic- action effect; that is, body movements affect attitude
ally significant. The left panel of Figure 3 depicts the toward the salesperson differently, depending on culture.
interaction effect; that is, body movements (symmetric Respondents in the low-gesture culture had more posi-
vs. asymmetric) affect perceived charisma differently, tive attitudes toward presenters who employed symmet-
depending on culture. Respondents in the low-gesture ric actions and postures than toward presenters who used
culture perceived salespeople who communicated via asymmetric movements (4.82 vs. 4.23); by contrast,
356 S. Pauser et al.

those in a high-gesture culture had more negative atti- the relationship among nonverbal behaviors, perceived
tudes toward such salespeople (4.58 vs. 4.88). Attitude charisma, and customer response. While research on
toward the salesperson was lower, on average, in the management and leadership practices has contributed a
low- than in the high-gesture culture (4.60 vs. 4.69). The great deal of insight, understanding of the specific non-
sign of the regression coefficient of perceived charisma verbal behaviors that predict charisma in a personal sell-
is plausible and the estimate significant, which is a pre- ing situation is still limited. This article extends the
requisite for support of H1. Finally, the estimated impact research stream on charismatic communication and per-
of the covariate (attitude toward product/service) is sonal selling by identifying specific nonverbal behaviors
plausible and significant; its explanatory contribution to as antecedents of perceived charisma. Consistent with
the model is less pronounced than for perceived cha- previous findings in personal selling and services mar-
risma. These findings provide support for H2b from a keting (Gabbott and Hogg 2000; Leigh and Summers
statistical standpoint. 2002), we show that nonverbal communication matters
in a personal selling context. Nonverbal messages
Mediation analysis for both cultures (H1) enhance a salesperson’s charismatic appeal, which in
turn leads to favorable attitudes toward the salesperson.
In the following discussion, symmetrical body move-
We elaborate theoretical research on impression forma-
ments represent the reference category due to the coding
tion and strategic social influence (impression manage-
of the binary variable nonverbal behavior; that is, we
ment) (Jones et al. 2001; Perrett et al. 1999). Existing
interpret effects of asymmetric body movements. The
research in strategic social influence studies how nonver-
lower left panel of Table 2 shows the indirect effect of
bal cues are purposely employed to elicit desirable per-
nonverbal behavior on attitude toward the salesperson
ceptions and attributions (Jones and Pittman 1982). We
(via perceived charisma), conditional on culture. Based
contribute to this literature by investigating specific bod-
on bootstrapping (5,000 samples), this indirect effect
(i.e., .15) is statistically significant for the low-gesture ily behaviors that enhance a person’s charisma and pro-
culture. The negative sign implies that asymmetric body vide guidance on how salespeople can strategically make
movements are detrimental for generating positive atti- use of these nonverbal behaviors (e.g., symmetry/asym-
tudes toward the salesperson. Moreover, these indirect metry of arm actions, arm postures). Moreover, we extend
effects are statistically different from each other (low- prior literature by providing evidence that charisma is cul-
versus high-gesture culture; .15 versus .14); that is, the ture specific in the sense that symmetrical or asymmet-
corresponding bootstrap confidence interval for a type I rical gesturing can evoke contrary outcomes. Our findings
error of 5% is [.57, .06]. The lower middle panel of may also benefit other disciplines that focus on communi-
Table 2 presents the direct effects of nonverbal behavior cation and interaction, such as social psychology, as we
on attitude toward the salesperson, conditional on culture were able to broaden the study of charisma to other disci-
(i.e., .16 and .06). It replicates the results shown previ- plines closely related to sales.
ously (upper right panel; i.e., .06 and .22), though the
p values indicate that this direct effect (conditional on
Methodological contribution
culture when considering the impact of perceived cha-
risma) is only significant for the sample representing the From a methodological standpoint, elevator pitches are rarely
low-gesture culture. Complementing the analysis of H1, used as stimuli in a marketing context. In addition, the use of
the lower right panel of Table 2 displays the total effects the BAP coding system represents an innovative element of
(the sum of direct and indirect effects; i.e., .32 this research study. Both methods require additional effort
and .20) and corresponding p values per culture: nonver- with respect to expertise and time. However, elevator pitches
bal behavior (weakly) significantly affects attitude contribute to the external validity of this research, especially
toward the salesperson when we control for culture. This given that some of those sales presentations were later used
finding provides support (i.e., partial mediation) for H1 for commercial purposes. The BAP coding system adds to
for the sample representing the low-gesture culture. the objectivity of measuring bodily behaviors because it
greatly reduces coder subjectivity. Furthermore, this study is
the first to investigate specific nonverbal behaviors that lead
Discussion to a salesperson’s charisma and customer responses. This
Theoretical contribution unique approach allows us to make use of high-precision,
Although research has linked charisma to various busi- time-locked coding “capable of providing systematic and reli-
ness outcomes and performance measures in politics, able descriptions of body movements” (Dael, Mortillaro, and
management, and leadership (Howell and Frost 1989; Scherer 2012, 98). To date, scholars have focused mainly on
Kirkpatrick and Locke 1996), it has not yet investigated binary measures by coding for the presence or absence of a
Journal of Personal Selling & Sales Management 357

behavior, without taking into account the duration of certain in high-gesture countries, exercise caution when applying
movements over time. the findings of previous studies on nonverbal charismatic
behaviors (Antonakis, Fenley, and Liechti 2011; Holladay
and Coombs 1994; Clark and Greatbatch 2011).
Managerial implications Finally, while sales professionals should be critical
US firms spend on average $1,252 annually on an and selective when incorporating nonverbal charismatic
employee’s training and learning expenditures (Association behaviors into their repertoire of sales routines, the
for Talent Development 2016). Researchers and manag- results of this research suggest that perceived charisma is
ers clearly understand that effective selling requires the linked to a customer’s attitude toward the salesperson.
salesperson’s ability to manage favorable impressions; Because attitude toward the salesperson influences sales
thus, their ability to manage customers’ perceptions is effectiveness and perceived charisma is an antecedent of
of importance (Lussier and Hall 2018). Our research the buyer’s attitude toward the salesperson, in personal
identifies specific learnable displays of charismatic selling, charisma is an important aspect that ultimately
behaviors as a means of enhancing customer impres- contributes to effective sales.
sions. To encourage a positive customer response, man-
agers need to consider the salesperson’s charisma and
should encourage and train their sales force to use sym- Limitations and further research
metric or asymmetric communication tactics, depending Despite the theoretical and managerial contributions of
on the cultural setting. While charisma research in other this research, several limitations exist. The sample of
social science disciplines has identified enhanced body salespeople is quite small. Further studies could investi-
movements as a driver of charisma, the results of our gate the nonverbal cues of both the salesperson and the
study extend previous findings by identifying specific customer, while including interaction aspects such as
nonverbal cues (out of a broad range of bodily behav- mirroring. DePaulo (2014) notes that data collection
iors) to use during sales presentations. Importantly, our from both parties is valuable for future work, as personal
findings shed light on how managers can enhance their selling is an interpersonal communication process. Field
charismatic appearance, which in turn leads to positive studies could also provide insights into a more realistic
attitudes toward the salesperson. In low-gesture cultures sales situation. Nevertheless, Bateson and Hui (1992,
such as the United States or Central Europe, particular 278) show experimentally that the use of videos in sales
emphasis should be put on symmetrical arm postures, research offers an ecologically valid method to simulate
actions, and functions; asymmetrical movements should service exchanges, as videos “evoke the same psycho-
be emphasized in high-gesture cultures such as Israel. logical and behavioral phenomena as the actual service
Consequently, sales professionals should be encour- setting.” However, a field study might provide additional
aged to use such nonverbal behaviors. Considering that benefits, as further dependent variables could be investi-
nonverbal communication messages are often encoded or gated. Replicating this study in other cultural contexts
decoded unconsciously (Stewart, Hecker and Graham might lead to new insights. While the measurement of
1987), our research provides managers with guidance on nonverbal behavior involved a laborious categorization
ways to enhance the awareness of nonverbal communi- scheme with attempts to inhibit subjective ratings as
cation messages by strategically making use of symmet- much as possible, an even more objective coding mech-
rical and asymmetrical nonverbal behaviors to enhance anism for recording the nonverbal cues of a sales dyad
favorable customer impressions. An effective way to could be applied in further studies by making use of
achieve this might be to practice these behaviors in sales wearable electronic devices. This may allow for the
training sessions (i.e., role plays, video feedback), espe- inclusion of additional variables of interest, such as
cially considering that charisma can be learned and voice characteristics, which could enrich the research
trained in a sales context (Pauser and Wagner 2018). objective. Finally, studies could also investigate dynamic
As hypothesized, for international marketers, the most aspects of nonverbal behaviors.
important finding is that the specific nonverbal behaviors
that predict perceived salesperson charisma have been
shown to be culture specific. Although arm gestures influ- Conclusion
enced charisma perceptions in both cultures, depending The sales literature focuses on determining which char-
on the cultural context either the symmetrical or asym- acteristics enhance sales performance and effectiveness.
metrical movements enhanced the salesperson’s charisma, With the goal of incorporating theories and concepts
a finding that is in concordance with extant literature on from outside the field into marketing, this article focuses
nonverbal behavior. In light of these findings, we likewise on charisma, a novel construct in sales research, which
suggest that sales professionals, specifically those located seems to play a significant role in personal selling
358 S. Pauser et al.

situations. Social scientists in multiple business fields SIGGRAPH ASIA 2016 Symposium on Education, Macau,
have reported that charisma positively affects organiza- China, December 5–8. doi:10.1145/2993352.2993355
Antonakis, John, Marika Fenley, and Sue Liechti. 2011. “Can
tional performance. Nevertheless, research on charisma
Charisma Be Taught? Tests of Two Interventions.”
in a marketing context is scarce. This article investigated Academy of Management Learning & Education 10
nonverbal behaviors that lead to perceived salesperson (3):374–96. doi:10.5465/amle.2010.0012
charisma and influence consumer responses in two cul- Arias-Bolzmann, Leopoldo, Goutam Chakraborty, and John C.
tures. The results showed that specific symmetrical arm Mowen. 2000. “Effects of Absurdity in Advertising: The
Moderating Role of Product Category Attitude and the
postures, actions, and functions significantly influence a
Mediating Role of Cognitive Responses.” Journal of
salesperson’s perceived charisma in low-gesture cultures, Advertising 29 (1):35–49. doi:10.1080/00913367.2000.
whereas the opposite holds true for high-gesture cultures. 10673602
Specifically, salespeople who used more symmetric arm Association for Talent Development. 2016. State of the
movements in a low-gesture culture received better eval- Industry Report. Alexandria, VA: Association for Talent
Development.
uations than those who used asymmetrical postures, Awamleh, Raed, and William L. Gardner. 1999. “Perceptions
actions, and functions. Thus, symmetrical arm actions, of Leader Charisma and Effectiveness: The Effects of
including manipulators, combined with symmetrical pos- Vision Content, Delivery, and Organizational Performance.”
tures may be an effective method for increasing a sales- Leadership Quarterly 10 (3):345–73. doi:10.1016/S1048-
person’s perceived charisma and eliciting favorable 9843(99)00022-3
Babin, Laurie A., Barry J. Babin, and James S. Boles. 1999.
consumer responses in this culture. However, these find-
“The Effects of Consumer Perceptions of the Salesperson,
ings are culture specific and hold credence only in low- Product and Dealer on Purchase Intentions.” Journal of
gesture cultures, as previous research is limited to the Retailing and Consumer Services 6 (2):91–7. doi:10.1016/
United States and Central Europe. By contrast, asymmet- S0969-6989(98)00004-6
ric movements are favored and salespeople employing Bass, Bernard M. 1997. “Personal Selling and Transactional/
Transformational Leadership.” Journal of Personal Selling
these movements are perceived as more charismatic in
and Sales Management 17 (3):19–28. doi:10.1080/
high-gesture cultures; salespeople employing asymmetric 08853134.1997.10754097.
movements receive higher ratings of charisma, and cus- Bateson, John E. G., and Michael K. Hui. 1992. “The
tomers’ attitudes increase. The two studies represent an Ecological Validity of Photographic Slides and Videotapes
important first step in conceptualizing charismatic sales in Simulating the Service Setting.” Journal of Consumer
Research 19 (2):271–81. doi:10.1086/209301
behaviors and thus provide valuable insights for fur- Beck, Rainer S., Rebecca Daughtridge, and Philip D. Sloane.
ther research. 2002. “Physician-Patient Communication in the Primary
Care Office: A Systematic Review.” The Journal of the
American Board of Family Practice 15 (1):25–38.
Declaration of interest Bolkan, San, and Alan K. Goodboy. 2014. “Communicating
No potential conflict of interest was reported by Charisma in Instructional Settings: Indicators and Effects
the authors. of Charismatic Teaching.” College Teaching 62
(4):136–42. doi:10.1080/87567555.2014.956039
Burgoon, Judee K. 1985. “Nonverbal Signals.” In Handbook of
Interpersonal Communication, edited by Mark L. Knapp
Funding and Gerald R. Miller, 344–90. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
The research was funded in part by a generous grant by Chakrabarty, Subhra, Robert E. Widing, and Gene Brown.
the dean of the faculty of business, economics and statis- 2014. “Selling Behaviours and Sales Performance: The
tics at the University of Vienna. Moderating and Mediating Effects of Interpersonal
Mentalizing.” Journal of Personal Selling and Sales
Management 34 (2):112–22. doi:10.1080/08853134.2014.
References 890899.
Alavi, Sascha, Jan Wieseke, and Jan H. Guba. 2016. “Saving Clark, Timothy, and David Greatbatch. 2011. “Audience
on Discounts Through Accurate Sensing – Salespeople’s Perceptions of Charismatic and Non-Charismatic Oratory:
Estimations of Customer Price Importance and Their The Case of Management Gurus.” Leadership Quarterly
Effects on Negotiation Success.” Journal of Retailing 92 22 (1):22–32. doi:10.1016/j.leaqua.2010.12.004
(1):40–55. doi:10.1016/j.jretai.2015.08.002 Cortinovis, Tim. 2016. “Ja, Nat€urlich ist Charisma Erlernbar.”
Ambady, Nalini, Mary A. Krabbenhoft, and Daniel Hogan. Accessed March 29, 2018. http://www.huffingtonpost.de/
2006. “The 30-Sec Sale: Using Thin-Slice Judgments to tim-cortinovis/ja-naturlich-ist-charisma-erlernbar_b_9615408.
Evaluate Sales Effectiveness.” Journal of Consumer html.
Psychology 16 (1):4–13. doi:10.1207/s15327663jcp1601_2 Cui, Jian, Voicu Popescu, Nicoletta Adamo-Villani, Susan
Anasingaraju, Saikiran, Meng-Lin Wu, Nicoletta Adamo- Wagner Cook, Katherine A. Duggan, and Howard S.
Villani, Voicu Popescu, Susan Wagner Cook, Mitchell Friedman. 2017. “Animation Stimuli System for Research
Nathan, and Martha Alibali. 2016. “Digital Learning on Instructor Gestures in Education.” IEEE Computer
Activities Delivered by Eloquent Instructor Avatars: Graphics and Applications 37 (4):72–83. doi:10.1109/
Scaling with Problem Instance.” Paper presented at the mcg.2017.3271471.
Journal of Personal Selling & Sales Management 359

Dael, Nele, Marcello Mortillaro, and Klaus Scherer. 2012. Howell, Jane M., and Peter J. Frost. 1989. “A Laboratory Study
“The Body Action and Posture Coding System (BAP): of Charismatic Leadership.” Organizational Behavior and
Development and Reliability.” Journal of Nonverbal Human Decision Processes 43 (2):243–69. doi:10.1016/
Behavior 36 (2):97–121. doi:10.1007/s10919-012-0130-0 0749-5978(89)90052-6
DePaulo, Peter J. 2014. “Applications of Nonverbal Behavior Hwang, Alvin, Naresh Khatri, and E. S. Srinivas. 2005.
Research in Marketing and Management.” In Applications “Organizational Charisma and Vision Across Three
of Nonverbal Behavioral Theories and Research, edited by Countries.” Management Decision 43 (7/8):960–74.
Robert S. Feldman, 63–87. New York, NY: Psychology doi:10.1108/00251740510609965
Press. Jones, B. C., A. C. Little, I. S. Penton-Voak, B. P. Tiddeman, D. M.
De Vries, Reinout E., Angelique Bakker-Pieper, and Wyneke Burt, and D. I. Perrett. 2001. “Facial Symmetry and Judgements
Oostenveld. 2010. “Leadership ¼ Communication? The of Apparent Health.” Evolution and Human Behavior 22
Relations of Leaders’ Communication Styles with (6):417–29. doi:10.1016/S1090-5138(01)00083-6.
Leadership Styles, Knowledge Sharing and Leadership Jones, Edvard E., and Thane S. Pittman. 1982. “Toward a
Outcomes.” Journal of Business and Psychology 25 General Theory of Strategic Self-Presentation.” In
(3):367–80. doi:10.1007/s10869-009-9140-2 Psychological Perspectives on the Self, edited by Jerry
Efron, David. 1972. Gesture, Race and Culture. The Hague, Suls, 231–62. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
The Netherlands: Mouton & Co. JPSSM. 2017. “Call for Papers: Applying Learning from Other
Fink, Bernhard, Nick Neave, John T. Manning, and Karl Domains to Sales.” Journal of Personal Selling and Sales
Grammer. 2006. “Facial Symmetry and Judgements of Management 37 (2):185. doi:10.1080/08853134.2017.1326263.
Attractiveness, Health and Personality.” Personality and Kendon, Adam. 1981. “Geography of Gesture.” Semiotica
Individual Differences 41 (3):491–9. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2006. 37:129–63.
01.017 Kendon, Adam. 2004. “Gesture, Culture and the Communication
Frese, Michael, Susanne Beimel, and Sandra Schoenborn. Economy.” In Gesture: Visible Action as Utterance, 326–54.
2003. “Action Training for Charismatic Leadership: Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Two Evaluations of Studies of a Commercial Training Khatri, Naresh, H. Alvin Ng, and Tracy Lee. 2001. “The
Module on Inspirational Communication of a Vision.” Distinction Between Charisma and Vision: An Empirical
Personnel Psychology 56 (3):671–98. doi:10.1111/j.1744- Study.” Asia Pacific Journal of Management 18
6570.2003.tb00754.x (3):373–93. doi:10.1023/A:1010653929261
Gabbott, Mark, and Gillian Hogg. 2000. “An Empirical Kipp, Michael. 2000. “Anvil: The Video Annotation Research
Investigation of the Impact of Non-Verbal Communication Tool.” Accessed March 29, 2018. http://www.anvil-
on Service Evaluation.” European Journal of Marketing 34 software.org.
(3/4):384–98. doi:10.1108/03090560010311911 Kirkpatrick, Shelley A., and Edwin A. Locke. 1996. “Direct
Garcia, Jose L. 1995. “Transformational Leadership Processes
and Indirect Effects of Three Core Charismatic Leadership
and Salesperson Performance Effectiveness: A Theoretical
Components on Performance and Attitudes.” Journal of
Model and Partial Empirical Examination.” Doctoral
Applied Psychology 81 (1):36–51. doi:10.1037/0021-
Dissertation, Fielding Institute.
9010.81.1.36
Gardner, William L., and Bruce J. Avolio. 1998. “The
Leigh, Thomas W., and John O. Summers. 2002. “An Initial
Charismatic Relationship: A Dramaturgical Perspective.”
Evaluation of Industrial Buyers Impressions of
Academy of Management Review 23 (1):32–58.
doi:10.5465/AMR.1998.192958 Salespersons' Nonverbal Cues.” Journal of Personal
Hall, Zachary R., Michael Ahearne, and Harish Sujan. 2015. Selling and Sales Management 22 (1):41–53. doi:10.1080/
“The Importance of Starting Right: The Influence of 08853134.2002.10754292.
Accurate Intuition on Performance in Salesperson–Customer Limbu, Yam B., C. Jayachandran, Barry J. Babin, and
Interactions.” Journal of Marketing 79 (3):91–109. Robin T. Peterson. 2016. “Empathy, Nonverbal Immediacy,
doi:10.1509/jm.13.0505 and Salesperson Performance: The Mediating Role of
Harrigan, Jinni A., Thomas E. Oxman, and Robert Rosenthal. Adaptive Selling Behavior.” Journal of Business &
1985. “Rapport Expressed Through Nonverbal Behavior.” Industrial Marketing 31 (5):654–67. doi:10.1108/JBIM-03-
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 9 (2):95–110. doi:10.1007/ 2015-0048
bf00987141 Lindell, Michael K., and David J. Whitney. 2001. “Accounting
Hayes, Andrew F. 2013. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, for Common Method Variance in Cross-Sectional Research
and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Designs.” Journal of Applied Psychology 86 (1):114–21.
Approach. New York, NY: The Guilford Press. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.86.1.114
Heide, Frederick J. 2013. “‘Easy to Sense But Hard to Define’: Little, Anthony C., and Benedict C. Jones. 2003. “Evidence
Charismatic Nonverbal Communication and the Against Perceptual Bias Views for Symmetry Preferences
Psychotherapist.” Journal of Psychotherapy Integration 23 in Human Faces.” Proceedings: Biological Sciences 270
(3):305–19. doi:10.1037/a0032481 (1526):1759–63. doi:10.1098/rspb.2003.2445
Holbrook, Morris B., and Rajeev Batra. 1987. “Assessing the Lussier, Bruno, and Zachary R. Hall. 2018. “Cooperation in
Role of Emotions as Mediators of Consumer Responses to B2B Relationships: Factors That Influence Customers'
Advertising.” Journal of Consumer Research 14 Perceptions of Salesperson Cooperation.” Industrial
(3):404–20. doi:10.2307/2489501 Marketing Management 69:209–20. doi:10.1016/
Holladay, Sherry J., and Timothy W. Coombs. 1994. j.indmarman.2017.09.019
“Speaking of Visions and Visions Being Spoken: An MacKenzie, Scott B., and Richard Lutz. 1989. “An Empirical
Exploration of the Effects of Content and Delivery on Examination of the Structural Antecedents of Attitude
Perceptions of Leader Charisma.” Management Communication Toward the Ad in an Advertising Pretesting Context.”
Quarterly 8 (2):165–89. doi:10.1177/0893318994008002002 Journal of Marketing 53 (2):48–65. doi:10.2307/1251413
360 S. Pauser et al.

McNeill, David, and Elena Levy. 1982. “Conceptual Sasaki, Yuka, Wim Vanduffel, Tamara Knutsen, Christopher
Representations in Language Activity and Gesture.” In Tyler, and Roger Tootell. 2005. “Symmetry Activates
Speech, Place, and Action, edited by Robert Jarvella and Extrastriate Visual Cortex in Human and Nonhuman
Wolfgang Klein, 271–95. New York, NY: John Wiley & Primates.” Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sons Ltd. Science of the United States of America 102 (8):3159–63.
Mehrabian, Albert. 1969. “Significance of Posture and Position doi:10.1073/pnas.0500319102
in the Communication of Attitude and Status Schmidt-Tanger, Martina. 2009. Charisma-Coaching: Von der
Relationships.” Psychological Bulletin 71 (5):359–72. Ausstrahlungskraft zur Anziehungskraft; Pr€asenz f€ur
doi:10.1037/h0027349 Wesentliches. Paderborn, Germany: Junfermann Verlag
Mehrabian, Albert, and Martin Williams. 1969. “Nonverbal GmbH.
Concomitants of Perceived and Intended Persuasiveness.” Schneider, Marlies. 2016. “5 Tipps f€ur mehr Charisma.”
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 13 Accessed March 29, 2018. http://www.news.at/a/mehr-
(1):37–58. doi:10.1037/h0027993 charisma-tipps.
Mullins, Ryan R., Michael Ahearne, Son K. Lam, Zachary R. Shamir, Boas. 1994. “Ideological Position, Leaders' Charisma,
Hall, and Jeffrey P. Boichuk. 2014. “Know Your and Voting Preferences: Personal vs. Partisan Elections.”
Customer: How Salesperson Perceptions of Customer Political Behavior 16 (2):265–87. doi:10.1007/BF01498880
Relationship Quality Form and Influence Account Shuter, Robert. 1979. “A Study of Nonverbal Communication
Profitability.” Journal of Marketing 78 (6):38–58. Among Jews and Protestants.” Journal of Social Psychology
doi:10.1509/jm.13.0300 109 (1):31–41. doi:10.1080/00224545.1979.9933636
Palmer, Steven E., and Kathleen Hemenway. 1978. Stewart, David W. Sid Hecker, and John L. Graham. 1987.
“Orientation and Symmetry: Effects of Multiple, “It’s More Than What You Say: Assessing the Influence of
Rotational, and Near Symmetries.” Journal of Experimental Nonverbal Communication in Marketing.” Psychology and
Psychology Human Perception and Performance 4 Marketing 4 (4):303–22.
(4):691–702. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.4.4.691 Sy, Thomas, Jin Nam Choi, and Stefanie K. Johnson. 2013.
Patterson, Miles L. 1991. “A Functional Approach to “Reciprocal Interactions Between Group Perceptions of
Nonverbal Exchange.” In Fundamentals of Nonverbal Leader Charisma and Group Mood Through Mood
Behavior, edited by Robert Stephen Feldman and Bernard Contagion.” The Leadership Quarterly 24 (4):463–76.
Rime, 458–95. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University
doi:10.1016/j.leaqua.2013.02.002
Press.
Talley, Linda, and Samuel Temple. 2015. “How Leaders
Pauser, Sandra, and Udo Wagner. 2018. “The Dose Makes the
Influence Followers Through the Use of Nonverbal
Poison: Investigating the Optimum Level of a
Communication.” Leadership & Organization Development
Salesperson’s Charisma.” Marketing ZFP 40 (1):35–47.
Journal 36 (1):69–80. doi:10.1108/LODJ-07-2013-0107
doi:10.15358/0344-1369-2018-1-35
Towler, Annette J. 2003. “Effects of Charismatic Influence Training
Perrett, David I., Michael Burt, Ian S. Penton-Voak, Kieran J.
Lee, Duncan A. Rowland, and Rachel Edwards. 1999. on Attitudes, Behavior, and Performance.” Personnel Psychology
“Symmetry and Human Facial Attractiveness.” Evolution 56 (2):363–81. doi:10.1111/j.1744-6570.2003.tb00154.x
and Human Behavior 20 (5):295–307. doi:10.1016/S1090- Tskhay, Konstantin O., Rebecca Zhu, Christopher Zou, and
5138(99)00014-8 Nicholas O. Rule. 2018. “Charisma in Everyday Life:
Peterson, Robin T. 2005. “An Examination of the Relative Conceptualization and Validation of the General Charisma
Effectiveness of Training in Nonverbal Communication: Inventory.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
Personal Selling Implications.” Journal of Marketing 114 (1):131–52. doi:10.1037/pspp0000159
Education 27 (2):143–50. doi:10.1177/0273475305276627 Tyler, Christopher W., Heidi A. Baseler, Leonid L.
Pika, Simone, Elena Nicoladis, and Paula F. Marentette. 2006. Kontsevich, Lora T. Likova, Alex R. Wade, and Brian A.
“A Cross-Cultural Study on the Use of Gestures: Evidence Wandell. 2005. “Predominantly Extra-Retinotopic Cortical
for Cross-Linguistic Transfer?” Bilingualism: Language and Response to Pattern Symmetry.” Neuroimage 24
Cognition 9 (03):319–27. doi:10.1017/S1366728906002665 (2):306–14. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.09.018
Podsakoff, Philip M., Scott B. MacKenzie, Jeong-Yeon Lee, Weber, Max. 1947. The Theory of Social and Economic
and Nathan P. Podsakoff. 2003. “Common Method Biases Organization. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
in Behavioral Research: A Critical Review of the Williams, Kaylene, C. Rosann L. Spiro,, and Leslie M. Fine.
Literature and Recommended Remedies.” Journal of 1990. “The Customer-Salesperson Dyad: An Interaction/
Applied Psychology 88 (5):879–903. doi:10.1037/0021- Communication Model and Review.” Journal of Personal
9010.88.5.879 Selling and Sales Management 10 (3):29–43.
Riggio, Ronald E., and Howard S. Friedman. 1986. Wilson, Hugh R., and Frances Wilkinson. 2002. “Symmetry
“Impression Formation: The Role of Expressive Behavior.” Perception: A Novel Approach for Biological Shapes.” Vision
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 50 Research 42 (5):589–97. doi:10.1016/S0042-6989(01)00299-1
(2):421–7. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.50.2.421 Wood, John Andy. 2006. “NLP Revisited: Nonverbal
Rosenthal, Robert, Dane Archer, Judith A. Hall, M. Robin Communications and Signals of Trustworthiness.” Journal
DiMatteo, and Peter L. Rogers. 1979. “Measuring of Personal Selling and Sales Management 26
Sensitivity to Nonverbal Communication: The PONS (2):197–204. doi:10.2753/PSS0885-3134260206
Test.” In Nonverbal Behavior, edited by Aaron Wolfgang, Yuhmiin, Chang, and Esther Thorson. 2004. “Television and
67–98. New York, NY: Academic Press. doi:10.1016/ Web Advertising Synergies.” Journal of Advertising 33
B978-0-12-761350-5.50012-4. (2):75–84. doi:10.1080/00913367.2004.10639161
Appendix 1. Findings from the literature about charismatic nonverbal communications.

Nonverbal charismatic Key finding: delivery Measurement/


Authors Year Classification/aggregation communication versus content coding procedure Design/sample

Howell 1989 Voice characteristics: Captivating and engaging No comparison of delivery No coding procedure applied Actors as experimental
and Frost voice tone versus content confederates for
Alter between pacing operationalizing charisma
and sitting
Body movements: Forward lean
Relaxed posture
Facial expressions: Direct eye contact
Animated facial expression
Holladay 1994 Voice characteristics: Vocal variety “Delivery contributes more Items in respondent Trained speaker
and Coombs Body movements: Relevant and strongly to perceptions of leader questionnaire, e.g., the as experimental
effective gestures charisma than does supervisor had an expressive confederate for
Facial expressions: Facial expressiveness content” (180). face; the supervisor rarely operationalizing charisma
Good eye contact changed his
facial expression.
Kirkpatrick 1996 Voice characteristics: Captivating voice tone “Surprisingly, charismatic No coding procedure applied Actors as experimental
and Locke Body movements: Shaking hands communication style had few confederates for
Powerful, confident, and direct or indirect effects on operationalizing charisma
dynamic interaction style performance and few effects on
Facial expressions: Direct eye contact attitudes” (46).
Animated facial
expressions
Awamleh 1999 Voice characteristics: Exhibit vocal fluency “A strong as opposed to weak No coding procedure applied Actor as experimental
and Gardner Body movements: Engage in dynamic hand style of message delivery will confederate for
and body gestures elicit higher levels of perceived operationalizing charisma
Facial expressions: Use facial expressions leader charisma and
Maintain eye contact effectiveness” (352).
Frese, Beimel, 2003 Voice characteristics: Variation of speed No comparison of delivery Coding form containing a 5- Midlevel managers
and Schoenborn Variation of loudness versus content point response format for received charisma training
Body movements: Gestures assessing how often each
Journal of Personal Selling & Sales Management

Orientation principle was present


toward audience
Towler 2003 Voice characteristics: Animated voice tone No comparison of delivery Coding of nonverbal Business students received
Body movements: Body gestures versus content behaviors on a 7-point scale: charisma training
Facial expressions: Facial expressions e.g., voice tone was animated
Eye contact (vs. boring)

Antonakis, 2011 Voice characteristics: Animated voice tone No comparison of delivery Binary measure for presence Mature working adults
Fenley, Body movements: Body gestures versus content or absence of received charisma training
and Liechti Facial expressions Facial expressions nonverbal behaviors
(Continued on next page)
361
Continued.
362
Nonverbal charismatic Key finding: delivery Measurement/
Authors Year Classification/aggregation communication versus content coding procedure Design/sample

Clark 2011 Voice characteristics: Voice intonation “The speakers rated as Presence or absence of Trained speakers
and Greatbatch Variation/speed charismatic differ significantly behavior was coded
Body movements: Gestures from their non-charismatic
Movement counterparts only in terms of
Appearance delivery [ … ]. The present
Facial expressions: Eye contact study confirms the findings of
prior work that has emphasized
the importance of strong
delivery over content” (29).

Bolkan 2014 Voice characteristics: Vocal variety “Delivery style [ … ] seems to Coding of nonverbal Teacher
and Goodboy Body movements: Gesturing be at the heart of charismatic behaviors on a 4-point scale:
Facial expressions: Eye contact teaching behaviors” (141). e.g., uses a monotone/dull
Smiling Charisma is therefore defined as voice when talking to
the “articulation of specific the class
behaviors” that communicators
engage in. (137)
S. Pauser et al.
Appendix 2. Findings about symmetric and asymmetric body (and in particular arm) movements.

Key findings: symmetry versus asymmetry and Country of


Field of study Author Year response variables Investigation

General Palmer 1978 “Vertical symmetry is perceptually most salient” (691). US


findings and Hemenway
about Perrett et al. 1999 “This experiment indicated that increasing symmetry in face UK
symmetry shape increased attractiveness” (300).
Jones et al. 2001 “The relationship between facial symmetry and attractiveness is UK
mediated by judgements of apparent health” (427).
Wilson 2002 “Symmetry is easily perceived” (589). CA
and Wilkinson
Little and Jones 2003 “Symmetry preferences are a consequence of greater ease of UK
processing symmetrical images in the visual system” (1759).
Sasaki et al. 2005 “Symmetric visual stimuli produce increased fMRI US
activity” (3159).
Tyler et al. 2005 “This initial evaluation of cortical processing of symmetry US
establishes that symmetry/ non-symmetric alternation is a
sufficient stimulus for significant fMRI activation” (313).
Fink et al. 2006 “Faces high in symmetry received significantly higher ratings of AUT
attractiveness, health, and certain personality attributes” (491).
Findings about Mehrabian 1969 “Symmetry in the arrangement of arms and legs […] seem US
symmetrical promising variables for exploring attitudes and status
arm gesturing relationships between communicators” (369).
Mehrabian 1969 More symmetrical postures convey positive attitudes toward the US
and Williams communicator (38).
Harrigan, Oxman, 1985 “High rapport doctors tended to hold their arms in open US
and Rosenthal symmetrical postures” (102). “Doctors who were rated less
favorably were more likely to have their arms in an
asymmetrical position” (106).
Beck, Daughtridge, 2002 “Those behaviors associated with favorable outcomes included US
and Sloane less mutual gaze, head nodding of the provider, forward lean,
more direct body orientation, uncrossed legs and arms, and
arm symmetry” (34).
Journal of Personal Selling & Sales Management

Talley and Temple 2015 “Specific hand gestures may be more effective than others” US/Canada
(76). Examples of symmetrical hand gestures include
community hands, humility hands and steepling hands (71).
Cui et al. 2017 “Parallel outward focused gestures are particularly US
charismatic” (81).
(Continued on next page)
363
Appendix 2. Continued. 364
Key findings: symmetry versus asymmetry and Country of
Field of study Author Year response variables Investigation

Finding about Efron 1972 In the Jewish culture gesturing is employed mainly Italians versus
asymmetrical asymmetrical e.g., gesturing starts with one arm and is Jews and
arm gesturing continued with the other (131). Jewish gestures are displayed “assimilated”
more frontal than peripheral and are rather angular (131). versus
Head movements are an important part of gesturing in this “traditional”
culture (131).
Shuter 1979 “It has been reported that Jews […] have substantially different Religious
gesture patterns” (32). “Jews also gestured significantly more group:
with one hand than two hands, in addition, they displayed Protestant
significantly more frontal than peripheral gestures” (37). Americans
versus
Jews
Kendon 2004 “In Jewish gesturing frequently only one arm would be used at Italians versus
a time, or if both were used, there tended to be an asymmetry Jews and
in their employment or, at times, gesturing begun with one “assimilated”
hand would be continued with another” (333). versus
“traditional”
S. Pauser et al.
Appendix 3. Findings from the literature about general consequences of charismatic communication

Author Year Key findings: effect of charismatic communication Perceived charisma Response variable

Holladay and Coombs 1994 There is “a strong positive relationship between perceptions þ þ
of leader charisma and perceptions of leader effectiveness” (Leader effectiveness)
(171). “It seems reasonable to expect that some delivery
[ … ] factors are more critical to perceptions of charisma
than are others” (182-183).
Kirkpatrick and Locke 1996 “Charismatic communication style affected only the þ “No other significant
perception of charisma” (36). attitude effects of
communication style
were found.” (43)
Gardner and Avolio 1998 “Charismatic presentations [ … ] elicit high levels of þ þ
emotional arousal [ … ] and positive affect for the (Leader effectiveness)
leader” (51).
Awamleh and Gardner 1999 “The results indicate that delivery is a major determinant of þ þ
perceived leader charisma and leader effectiveness” (360). (Leader effectiveness)
Schmidt-Tanger 2009 A charismatic person makes use of symmetrical gestures. Not empirically investigated
Clark and Greatbatch 2011 “Findings signify the importance of delivery as a key þ Not investigated
determinant of the difference between charismatic and
non-charismatic speakers” (29).
Bolkan and Goodboy 2014 “If teachers want to be perceived as charismatic in the þ þ
classroom, they should consider employing behaviors that (Intrinsic motivation and
are associated with [ … ] delivering their content well” perceptions of learning)
(141). “Instructors’ charismatic behaviors are associated
with students’ perceptions of their learning both directly
and indirectly through their association with intrinsic
motivation” (140).
Cortinovis 2016 Symmetrical gesturing increases a person’s charisma. Not empirically investigated
Schneider 2016 Symmetrical gesturing is an important determinant of a Not empirically investigated
Journal of Personal Selling & Sales Management

person’s charisma.
Anasingaraju et al. 2016 Charismatic personalities make use of “synchronous-parallel- Not empirically investigated
outward hand gesture[s]” (2).
Cui et al. 2017 “Parallel outward focused gestures are particularly þ Not investigated
charismatic” (81).
Note: þ ¼ Significant positive relationship of charismatic communication on perceived charisma, other response variables, respectively.
365
Appendix 4. Selected findings from the literature about consequences of nonverbal behaviors in a sales context
366
Customer's
perception of Customer's
Author Year Key findings: effect of nonverbal cues in a sales context salesperson response

Gabbott and Hogg 2000 “The results indicate significant differences in respondents' reactions to the scenario according to þ þ
the non-verbal behavior of the service provider” (384). “Without the correct body language or (Perceptions (Service
paralinguistic cues customers are either dissatisfied or fail to develop the empathy with the of service) evaluation)
provider which leads to repurchase behavior” (394).
Leigh and Summers 2002 “Nonverbal cues are found to influence selected dimensions of buyers' perceptions of the þ þ
salesperson and their evaluations” (41). Nonverbal cues impacting perception of salesperson: (Salesperson (Sales
eye gaze (þ), formal posture (n.s.), gesturing (n.s.), speech hesitations (n.s.),professional attire impressions, presentation
(þ); Nonverbal cues impacting customer response: eye gaze (þ), formal posture (n.s.), perceptions) evaluation)
gesturing (n.s.), speech hesitations (-), professional attire (n.s.)
Peterson 2005 “Training in body language can improve sales effectiveness. The study evidenced support for the Not empirically þ
value of nonverbal communication training in producing a proxy for willingness to investigated (Willingness
purchase” (148). to purchase)
Ambady, Krabbenhoft, 2006 “Previous research has found that nonverbal cues affect both perceptions of salespeople and
and Hogan sales performance” (7). “A successful sale depends on a customer's perception of the Not empirically investigated
salesperson” (4). “Perceptions of salespeople translate into real-world results, such as sales
performance and customer satisfaction” (4).
Limbu, Jayachandran, 2016 “Several studies have emphasized the significance of nonverbal communication” (657). Not empirically þ
Babin and Peterson “Salesperson nonverbal immediacy skills are essential for cultivating relationships with investigated (Relationship
customers” (662). performance)
Note: þ ¼ Significant positive relationship of nonverbal cues on customer’s perception of the salesperson, other response variables, respectively;  ¼ significant negative relationship of nonverbal
S. Pauser et al.

cues on customer’s perception of the salesperson, other response variables, respectively; n.s. ¼ lack of statistical significance of nonverbal cues on customer’s perception of the salesperson, other
response variables, respectively.
Journal of Personal Selling & Sales Management 367

Appendix 5. Sample descriptives

Study 1 Low-gesture culture High-gesture culture

Number of elevator pitches from company $ # R $ # R


founders or salespeople of start-ups 9 13 22 6 16 22
Mean age and age range of presenter 35, 22–65 25, 18–40
Cluster analysis assigns these persons to 2
clusters based on their nonverbal behavior
Cluster 1, symmetric body movements $ # R $ # R
are dominating 8 6 14 4 10 14
Mean age and age range of presenter 35, 24–53 26, 21–40
Cluster 2, asymmetric body movements $ # R $ # R
are dominating 1 7 8 2 6 8
Mean age and age range of presenter 35, 22–65 23, 18–30

Study 2 Low-gesture culture High-gesture culture


Sample size $ # R $ # R
496 440 936 369 354 723
Mean age and age range of sample 43, 14–95 43, 13–80
Employment: in education 19% 6%
employees/officers 40% 12%
workers 10% 53%
self-employed 13% 11%
retired 14% 12%
unemployed 4% 6%
Average number of respondents evaluating one 43 33
elevator pitch (between-subjects design
exposing respondents to one of the 22
elevator pitches)
368 S. Pauser et al.

Appendix 6: Scales employed in Study 2 and their psychometric properties

Perceived charisma (Khatri, Ng, and Lee 2001)


Please indicate to which extent you agree/disagree with the following statements Factor loadings
regarding the person shown in the video: EFA CFA

Has good social skills .84 .81 


Often understands the mood of his/her counterparts .81 .76 
Is quite interesting/lively .86 .86 
Is sensitive to the needs and feelings of others .74 .69 
Is able to relate to others .80 .75 
Commands our attention through his/her actions or words .84 .83 
Has the gift of the gab .79 .77 
Makes me perform something purely by his or her words .82 .81 
Is charismatic .85 .84 
Response format: 7-point Likert scale (1 ¼ strongly disagree, 7 ¼ strongly agree) VE1 ¼ .67 CR ¼ .94
a ¼ .94 AVE ¼ .63
AGFI ¼ .75
MAXCorr2 ¼ .56
Attitude toward the salesperson (MacKenzie and Lutz 1989)
Please rate the person shown in the video using the following scales: Factor loadings
EFA CFA(a)
Good/bad .90
Pleasant/unpleasant .91
Favorable/unfavorable .91
Response format: 7-point bipolar scale VE1 ¼ .83
a ¼ .90
Attitude toward the product/service (Holbrook and Batra 1987)
Please rate the product/service using the following scales: Factor loadings
EFA CFA
Good/bad .92 .88 
Like/dislike .96 .95 
Favorable/unfavorable .94 .93 
Positive/negative .93 .90 
Response format: 7-point bipolar scale VE1 ¼ .88 CR ¼ .95
a ¼ .95 AVE ¼ .84
AGFI ¼ .99
MAXCorr2 ¼ .79
Note: EFA ¼ exploratory factor analysis; VE1 ¼ variance explained by one factor solution; a ¼ Cronbach alpha reliability; CFA ¼ confirmatory factor
analysis; CR ¼ construct reliability; AVE ¼ average variance explained; AGFI ¼ adjusted goodness of fit; MAXCorr2 ¼ maximum of squared inter-
item correlations (used to check discriminant validity according to the Fornell–Larcker criterion, i.e., MAXCorr2 should be smaller than AVE).
a
Analysis not possible because of insufficient number of degrees of freedom.
Journal of Personal Selling & Sales Management 369

Appendix 7. Descriptive statistics: correlations, means, and standard deviations

Correlations
Mean SD 1 2

1. Perceived charisma 3.80 .58


2. Self-perceived charisma of salesperson 4.84 1.33 .43
3. Attitude toward the salesperson 4.65 .46 .73 .21
Note: SD ¼ standard deviation. All correlations are significant at the .01 level (two-tailed), N ¼ 44.

You might also like