You are on page 1of 366

Clinical Handbooks in Neuropsychology

William B. Barr
Chris Morrison Editors

Handbook on the
Neuropsychology
of Epilepsy
Clinical Handbooks in Neuropsychology

Series Editor
William B. Barr

More information about this series at http://www.springer.com/series/8438


William B. Barr • Chris Morrison
Editors

Handbook on the
Neuropsychology
of Epilepsy
Editors
William B. Barr Chris Morrison
Dept. Neurosurgery Dept. Neurosurgery
New York University School New York University School
of Medicine of Medicine
New York, NY, USA New York, NY, USA

ISBN 978-0-387-92825-8 ISBN 978-0-387-92826-5 (eBook)


DOI 10.1007/978-0-387-92826-5
Springer New York Heidelberg Dordrecht London

Library of Congress Control Number: 2014951844

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015


This work is subject to copyright. All rights are reserved by the Publisher, whether the whole or
part of the material is concerned, specifically the rights of translation, reprinting, reuse of
illustrations, recitation, broadcasting, reproduction on microfilms or in any other physical way,
and transmission or information storage and retrieval, electronic adaptation, computer software,
or by similar or dissimilar methodology now known or hereafter developed. Exempted from this
legal reservation are brief excerpts in connection with reviews or scholarly analysis or material
supplied specifically for the purpose of being entered and executed on a computer system, for
exclusive use by the purchaser of the work. Duplication of this publication or parts thereof is
permitted only under the provisions of the Copyright Law of the Publisher’s location, in its
current version, and permission for use must always be obtained from Springer. Permissions for
use may be obtained through RightsLink at the Copyright Clearance Center. Violations are liable
to prosecution under the respective Copyright Law.
The use of general descriptive names, registered names, trademarks, service marks, etc. in this
publication does not imply, even in the absence of a specific statement, that such names are
exempt from the relevant protective laws and regulations and therefore free for general use.
While the advice and information in this book are believed to be true and accurate at the date of
publication, neither the authors nor the editors nor the publisher can accept any legal responsibility
for any errors or omissions that may be made. The publisher makes no warranty, express or
implied, with respect to the material contained herein.

Printed on acid-free paper

Springer is part of Springer Science+Business Media (www.springer.com)


For KBM—in recognition of all the sacrifices he has made
for me.
To CNB—my wife and my love.
Preface

Epilepsy is a prevalent condition affecting approximately 1 % of the general


population with approximately 30 % of those individuals suffering from
refractory seizures. While medical management of seizures and other
symptoms (e.g., mood and behaviors) is most common, a significant number
of these individuals can also benefit from various neurosurgical interventions.
The cognitive, behavioral, and emotional effects of this condition and
associated etiological comorbidities can be as debilitating as those caused by
ongoing seizures. As a result, clinical neuropsychologists have become key
members of medical teams that evaluate and treat patients with epilepsy. In
fact, for the past 50 years, neuropsychologists have played an integral role in
epilepsy surgical management teams with the goal of helping to reduce the
risk for postoperative deficits in language and memory, as well as in
developing treatment plans for cognitive, behavioral, and mood issues that
may be present before and/or after medical/surgical interventions.
As neuropsychological services are needed to meet established standards
of care in a specialized epilepsy treatment setting, neurology and neurosurgery
teams are increasingly turning to their local neuropsychology colleagues to
engage their services in the context of developing new epilepsy care centers
and surgical programs. Over the years we have been contacted by many
neuropsychology and medical colleagues who have found themselves
transitioning into these new settings and being asked to join the assessment
and treatment decision-making process (including type and extent of surgery)
of individuals with epilepsy. The conceptualization and development of this
book was in part generated from such requests.
While doctoral-level practitioners can be highly skilled in clinical
neuropsychology, the issues, procedures, and special considerations within
the epilepsy population are unique in some respects and even a well-trained
neuropsychologist will need some supplementary education and training to
function optimally in this setting and to provide the best care possible to this
population. Dedicated training to obtain fluency in the issues and skills
needed to serve this patient group is often obtained within the context of an
epilepsy center-based postdoctoral fellowship and/or under the supervision of
an experienced neuropsychologist within an established comprehensive
epilepsy center. That said, such training is not always available and/or
practical. For example, the established neuropsychologist who has been

vii
viii Preface

asked to expand their role within a new epilepsy program at their center can-
not leave their paid position for prolonged training at another clinic. The cur-
rent volume is written with these issues in mind and with the goals of outlining
the range of services provided by neuropsychologists in specialized epilepsy
centers and serving as an accessible, introductory “how-to” guide.
Given this “skills-based” goal, the format of the book is a radical departure
from traditional literature reviews and aggregate data style papers and
chapters that are available on the neuropsychology of epilepsy. In the chapters
that follow detailed descriptions, reviews of the recent literature, and clinical
vignettes, as well as concrete recommendations on how to conduct specialized
procedures, are provided. The information contained in this book will also be
useful to more veteran practitioners who want to know the “latest information”
on neuropsychological practice involving patients with epilepsy. These
advanced readers will be able to obtain a quick review of the research
literature on these procedures in addition to “inside tips” from practitioners
with years of experience in conducting these tests.
Neurologists and other medical professionals involved in the care of
patients with epilepsy will also find this book to be helpful. Many medical
practitioners consider the work of clinical neuropsychologists in their settings
to be somewhat “mysterious.” This book attempts to change this perception
by making the details of neuropsychological practice more accessible. While
medical professionals might be familiar with some of the research findings
relevant to neuropsychological procedures in epilepsy patients, they are not
likely to have been exposed to more “inside knowledge” on how
neuropsychologists actually perform these procedures. The information
provided in this book will help inform neurologists in charge of running
epilepsy centers on the tasks required of neuropsychologists before making
offers of employment. Added knowledge on these procedures will also
enhance other professionals’ ability to understand and better appreciate the
information provided in neuropsychological reports.
The organization and content of this volume emphasizes working with
adults aged 20–55 (those most often considered for surgery) as well as the pre-
surgical special procedures that can involve evaluation of cognition, mood, and
behavior. In Section I, Neuropsychological Assessment Across the Lifespan,
the chapters provide an overview of neuropsychological assessment in patients
with epilepsy with separate attention to how and why evaluations are performed
in children and in older adults. Seizures often occur for different reasons in
children as compared to older individuals and the influence of medical and
surgical treatments, methods of assessment, reasons for referral, and types of
recommendations needed are often unique at these ends of the age spectrum.
Section II focuses on evaluation of the epilepsy surgical candidate wherein
there is an obvious role for the clinical neuropsychologist. However, many of
the procedures covered in this section also apply to patients undergoing neu-
rosurgery for focal lesions, such as brain tumors or vascular malformations,
where there may be perturbation of cerebral cortex en route to the lesion and/
or cortex may be affected by surgical removal of the lesion itself. The section
opens with a review of the purposes of neuropsychological testing in this
context, including efforts to identify seizure localization/lateralization, estab-
Preface ix

lish a presurgical baseline, predict surgical outcome, and provide outcome


markers that can be useful in clinical research. There is review of common
methods for assessing various domains of functioning with elaboration on the
typical neuropsychological test findings in the most common populations of
epilepsy surgery patients. Importantly, there is also a review of many of the
potential confounds to neuropsychological test data as well as a case vignette
to highlight the process and integration of cognitive data for rendering a neu-
ropsychological opinion of relevance to the surgical team.
The subsequent chapters within Section II review various specialized pro-
cedures associated with presurgical evaluations. Functional techniques such
as fMRI and MEG, are becoming more widely used clinically to identify
language lateralization. Research to understand how these methods can be
used to characterize the functional integrity of specific brain regions, such as
hippocampal support of memory functioning, is burgeoning with some prom-
ising data emerging. In Chaps. 8 and 9 the reader is presented with the basics
for understanding fMRI and MEG as well as the methods for cognitive test-
ing that can be integrated into these protocols.
Though functional imaging techniques are becoming more widely
available, there continue to be many centers that use the Wada procedure in at
least a subset of surgical candidates to identify language dominance and
memory functionality in each of the hemispheres. This “deactivation”
procedure remains the gold standard for obtaining select types of information.
Chapter 5 provides an overview of the Wada procedure and the various
methods for performing the test, calculating outcome variables, and clinically
interpreting the scores. The Medical College of Georgia protocol is used to
take the reader through an example of how the procedure might be conducted.
When we have been contacted by neuropsychologists and neurologists at
other hospitals, the Wada procedure and electrocortical mapping of language
eloquent cortex are the two procedures that we have been asked to provide
education and training on the most frequently. The challenge here is that
these procedures are the least amenable to learning via textbook study and are
the least standardized in terms of assessment of cognition and behavior. In an
attempt to operationalize mapping of language eloquent cortex as much as
possible, Chap. 6 provides an overview of the procedure, details for
understanding how to conduct the procedure, and common methods used for
assessing various aspects of language functioning. The overall structure of
this chapter and the case example within are probably the most “manualized”
in organization of all the chapters in this volume.
While there is an obvious role of structural imaging in the presurgical
work-up, this procedure less commonly has a direct role for neuropsychology.
That said, highlights within Chap. 7 include how variables obtained from
structural imaging are being used in combination with cognitive and
behavioral data to better understand functional neuroanatomy. Given the
complexity of the brain, this area of research will continue long into the
future and the critical role of the neuropsychologist in developing, applying,
modifying, repeating, and interpreting cognitive and behavioral measures
will remain important in facilitating high-quality and well-controlled research
in this area.
x Preface

Section III takes the reader through a few of the special topics and
populations that the clinician will need to be familiar with when working with
an epilepsy population. The chapters address the very high frequency of
psychiatric comorbidity in epilepsy patients as well as psychiatric conditions/
behaviors that may be confused with seizures. Clinical neuropsychologists
need to be well versed in assessing mood issues, personality disorders,
conversion disorders, and behavioral and pharmacological treatments for
psychiatric features in a person with an epilepsy syndrome and/or with
conversion, psychogenic nonepileptic seizures. Clinical neuropsychologists,
with their understanding of functional neuroanatomy as it relates to cognition
AND mood (i.e., that mood disorders in these individuals can be reactive and/
or the result of the underlying epilepsy), as well as their training in
psychopathology and psychological interventions are uniquely positioned to
assist in evaluation and treatment planning.
The epilepsy population is extremely diverse in terms of cognitive ability
and level of adaptive functioning. Because about a third of epilepsy patients
can be fairly well controlled with one medicine, a clinical neuropsychologist
may encounter very high functioning individuals with advanced education,
challenging vocations, and well-controlled seizures. These individuals can
present with cognitive complaints, perceived cognitive changes, and/or other
emotional/adjustment factors related or unrelated to their neurological condi-
tion. In contrast, other patients may be cognitively devastated by their epilepsy
syndrome and the underlying brain abnormality(ies) that produce their sei-
zures (e.g., tuberous sclerosis, migrational abnormalities, complex epilepsy
syndromes). When first confronted with extremely low functioning individu-
als, the neuropsychologist might not immediately appreciate how a neuropsy-
chological evaluation could be of value to the treatment team. Chapters 13 and
14 address issues related to assessing individuals at these extremes of func-
tioning to highlight the special considerations in these evaluations and the
modifications in testing approach that the neuropsychologist might make.
Finally, performing neuropsychological assessments in multicultural,
multilingual, and/or non-English speaking individuals is a challenge ubiqui-
tous to the field neuropsychology and not unique to the epilepsy population.
However, because individuals may travel from different regions, even differ-
ent countries, to obtain care at a specialized clinic, practitioners in a tertiary
care epilepsy center may encounter a higher proportion individuals whose
language and/or cultural background is disparate from that of the neuropsy-
chological test items and test normative samples we have available. Thus con-
siderable caution and limitations to interpretation of data are warranted. In
the final chapter of this volume, special considerations in assessing ethnically
and linguistically diverse populations are discussed. While the content of this
chapter is heavily weighted to assessment of Spanish speakers (or bilingual
Spanish/English speakers), the general principles regarding the assessment
process and application of available assessment tools to a person of a differ-
ent linguistic and cultural background remain relevant.

New York, NY, USA William B. Barr


Chris Morrison
Contents

1 Neuropsychological Assessment of Patients with Epilepsy ...... 1


William B. Barr
2 Evaluation of Children and Adolescents with Epilepsy ........... 37
William S. MacAllister and Elisabeth M.S. Sherman
3 Geriatric Patients with Epilepsy ................................................. 63
Jessica Chapin and Richard Naugle
4 Neuropsychological Evaluation of the Epilepsy
Surgical Candidate....................................................................... 87
Daniel L. Drane
5 The Wada Test: Current Perspectives and Applications .......... 123
David W. Loring and Kimford J. Meador
6 Electrocortical Mapping of Language........................................ 139
Chris Morrison and Chad E. Carlson
7 Structural and Quantitative MRI in Epilepsy........................... 155
Karen Blackmon and Thomas Thesen
8 Functional MRI in the Presurgical Epilepsy Evaluation ......... 169
Sara J. Swanson, Jeffrey R. Binder, Manoj Raghavan,
and Matthew Euler
9 MEG in the Presurgical Epilepsy Evaluation............................ 195
Matthew Euler, Sylvain Baillet, and Sara J. Swanson
10 Evaluation of Psychiatric and Personality Disorders ............... 213
Jana E. Jones
11 Psychotherapeutic and Cognitive Remediation
Approaches ................................................................................... 235
Luba Nakhutina
12 Evaluation and Management of Psychogenic
Nonepileptic Attacks .................................................................... 257
John T. Langfitt and William Watson
13 Developmentally Delayed Children and Adults ........................ 275
Erica M. Brandling-Bennett

xi
xii Contents

14 The Neuropsychological Evaluation of High-Functioning


Patients with Epilepsy.................................................................. 301
Sarah G. Schaffer
15 The Neuropsychological Assessment of Culturally
and Linguistically Diverse Epilepsy Patients............................. 317
H. Allison Bender

Index ...................................................................................................... 345


List of Contributors

Sylvain Baillet, Ph.D. McConnell Brain Imaging Centre Montreal


Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada
William B. Barr, Ph.D., A.B.P.P. Dept. Neurosurgery, New York University
School of Medicine, New York, NY, USA
H. Allison Bender, Ph.D. Department of Neurology, Mount Sinai School of
Medicine, New York, NY, USA
Jeffrey R. Binder, M.D. Department of Neurology, Medical College of
Wisconsin, Milwaukee, WI, USA
Karen Blackmon, Ph.D. Department of Neurology, New York University
School of Medicine, New York, NY, USA
Erica M. Brandling-Bennett Valley Medical Center, Valley Neuroscience
Institute, Renton, WA, USA
Chad E. Carlson, M.D. Department of Neurology, Medical College of
Wisconsin, Milwaukee, WI, USA
Jessica Chapin Aurora Health Care, Grafton, WI, USA
Daniel L. Drane, Ph.D., A.B.P.P. (C.N.) Emory University School of
Medicine, Atlanta, GA, USA
University of Washington School of Medicine, Seatle, WA, USA
Matthew Euler, Ph.D. Department of Psychology, University of Utah, Salt
Lake City, UT, USA
Jana E. Jones, Ph.D. Matthews Neuropsychology Lab, Department of
Neurology, School of Medicine and Public Health, University of Wisconsin,
Madison, WI, USA
John T. Langfitt, Ph.D., A.B.P.P. Strong Epilepsy Center, University of
Rochester, Rochester, NY, USA
Deptartment of Psychiatry and Neurology, School of Medicine and Dentistry,
University of Rochester Medical Center, University of Rochester, Rochester,
NY, USA

xiii
xiv List of Contributors

David W. Loring, Ph.D., A.B.P.P.-C.N. Department of Neurology, Emory


University, Atlanta, GA, USA
William S. MacAllister, Ph.D., A.B.P.P.-C.N. NYU Comprehensive
Epilepsy Center, NYU School of Medicine, New York, NY, USA
Kimford J. Meador, M.D. Department of Neurology & Neurological
Sciences, Stanford Comprehensive Epilepsy Center, Stanford University
School of Medicine, Stanford, CA, USA
Chris Morrison, Ph.D., A.B.P.P. Dept. Neurosurgery, New York University
School of Medicine, New York, NY, USA
Luba Nakhutina, Ph.D., A.B.P.P.-C.N. SUNY Downstate Medical Center,
Brooklyn, NY, USA
Richard Naugle, Ph.D., A.B.P.P. Neurological Institute, Cleveland Clinic,
Cleveland, OH, USA
Manoj Raghavan, M.D., Ph.D. Department of Neurology, Medical College
of Wisconsin, Milwaukee, WI, USA
Sarah G. Schaffer, Ph.D., A.B.P.P.-C.N. Comprehensive Epilepsy Care
Center, Cushing Neuroscience Institute, Hofstra North Shore-LIJ School of
Medicine, Great Neck, NY, USA
Elisabeth M.S. Sherman, Ph.D., RPsych. Copeman Healthcare Centre,
Adjunct Associate Professor, Faculty of Medicine, University of Calgary, Canada
Copeman Healthcare Centre, Calgary, AB, Canada
Sara J. Swanson, Ph.D., A.B.P.P. Department of Neurology, Medical
College of Wisconsin, Milwaukee, WI, USA
Thomas Thesen, D.Phil. Department of Neurology, New York University
School of Medicine, New York, NY, USA
William Watson, Ph.D. Department of Psychiatry and Neurology, School
of Medicine and Dentistry, University of Rochester Medical Center,
University of Rochester, Rochester, NY, USA
Neuropsychological Assessment
of Patients with Epilepsy 1
William B. Barr

functional strengths and weaknesses, and tracking


Introduction the effects of various treatments.
The goal of this chapter is to introduce the
Neuropsychological assessment provides essen- reader to many of the general issues relevant to
tial and unique information for diagnosing and neuropsychology as it applies to assessment of
managing patients with epilepsy. While the goal patients with epilepsy. It will begin with a brief
of most medical and neurological approaches is review of epilepsy and seizures and will continue
naturally to cease or reduce seizures, the symp- with a description of neuropsychological assess-
toms most commonly associated with epilepsy, ment of general adult patients (ages, 20–55) who
neuropsychology focuses more on understanding are evaluated in a medical or neurological setting.
and measuring disorders of cognition and behav- Chapters focusing on neuropsychological assess-
ior associated with epilepsy and using this infor- ment of patients from other age groups and those
mation to aid in diagnosis and treatment evaluated in a surgical and other settings will fol-
planning. low in this volume.
Neuropsychology provides, in many ways, the
most comprehensive approach to evaluating the
patient with epilepsy by allowing the clinician to Background
integrate medical, developmental, and social fac-
tors in coming to an understanding of how these Seizures and Epilepsy
factors influence cognitive and behavioral func-
tioning. With its occurrence in an estimated 1 % Epilepsy is a condition defined by both a combi-
of the population, epilepsy is a relatively com- nation of recurrent seizures and alterations in
mon disorder that is seen on a frequent basis by behavior (Engel & Pedley, 2008). It is important
most neuropsychologists in practice settings. to establish a basic understanding of epilepsy and
Clinical neuropsychologists also play important the seizures characterizing the disorder. Epilepsy
roles in epilepsy specialty centers by providing is a heterogeneous condition characterized by
information that is useful for diagnosis, outlining multiple seizure types. It is quite prevalent, with
statistics indicating that its occurrence is as com-
mon as breast cancer and other medical condi-
W.B. Barr, Ph.D., A.B.P.P. (*) tions receiving much more publicity. Among
Dept. Neurosurgery, New York University School
neurological conditions, it is more prevalent
of Medicine, 223 East 34th Street, New York,
NY, 10016, USA multiple sclerosis, muscular dystrophy, cerebral
e-mail: william.barr@nyumc.org palsy, and Parkinson’s disease combined. Public

W.B. Barr and C. Morrison (eds.), Handbook on the Neuropsychology of Epilepsy, 1


Clinical Handbooks in Neuropsychology, DOI 10.1007/978-0-387-92826-5_1,
© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015
2 W.B. Barr

understanding of epilepsy remains poor despite Our understanding of epilepsy and seizures is
its prevalence. Much of this is the result of con- forever changing, due to ongoing developments in
tinuing stigma associated with epilepsy, dating science and technology. The International League
back to centuries old misconceptions about asso- Against Epilepsy (ILAE) is a multidisciplinary
ciations between seizures and supernatural phe- group of health care professionals that has been
nomena, including demonic possession. Medical responsible for providing some standardization to
professionals and patient advocacy groups have the terms that are used across the world for charac-
worked hard together to remove much of the terizing seizures and various epilepsy syndromes.
stigma associated with epilepsy. In the year 2010, the ILAE provided an important
The diagnosis of epilepsy is based on clinical update and revision to the concepts and terminol-
criteria, including the presence of recurring sei- ogy used for classification of seizures and epilep-
zures, with the neurophysiological basis of the sei- sies (Berg et al., 2010). A listing of the revised
zures confirmed through neurodiagnostic testing. classification of seizures is provided in Table 1.1.
Electroencephalography (EEG) is generally con- In the 2010 ILAE classification, one finds the
sidered the “gold standard” for diagnosing epi- well-known distinction between generalized sei-
lepsy. Neurologists trained in EEG are able to zures and focal seizures, differing in the charac-
identify the presence of epilepsy through analysis teristics of their underlying seizure networks,
of characteristic waveforms associated with sei- which are known to be bilaterally distributed in
zures. Information from the EEG can also be useful the case of generalized seizures and more local-
in localizing the onset of the seizures. Additional ized in focal seizures. Generalized seizures can
information is often provided through structural include tonic–clonic seizures, which is the most
brain imaging using computed tomographic (CT) obvious and dramatic symptom of epilepsy, in
or magnetic resonance (MR) technology, with the addition to more subtle types of generalized
latter known as providing the most detailed infor- seizures such as absence attacks and myoclonic
mation regarding neuroanatomic abnormalities seizures, which are seen mostly in childhood
associated with seizures. Functional brain imaging epilepsy syndromes.
with single photon emission computed tomogra- Terms such as “simple” and “complex” partial
phy (SPECT), positron emission tomography seizures have been dropped from the new classi-
(PET), or functional MRI (fMRI) can also provide
useful information regarding reductions in local-
Table 1.1 Classification of seizures—ILAE revised
ized blood flow and/or metabolism associated with (2010)
the location of the seizure focus. Interested readers
Generalized seizures
can go to Chaps. 7 and 8 for more information
Tonic–clonic (in any combination)
about those modalities.
Absence
It is important to remember, however, that epi-
Typical
lepsy is defined as a condition by its recurring sei- Atypical
zures. There are a number of situations, such as in Absence with special features
acute drug intoxication or CNS infection, where Myoclonic absence
one might develop a single seizure, but not have Eyelid myoclonia
epilepsy. Seizures in patients with epilepsy may Myoclonic
manifest in various forms. The terms used to Myoclonic
describe different types of seizures can be confus- Myoclonic atonic
ing at times, given the fact that many patients and Myoclonic tonic
even some health care professionals persist in using Clonic
outdated terms such as petit mal or grand mal to Tonic
describe variants of seizure activity. Use of these Atonic
older terms obscures many of the important etio- Focal seizures
logic, symptomatic, and neuroanatomic character- Unknown
Epileptic spasms
istics of seizures and should be avoided altogether.
1 Neuropsychological Assessment of Patients with Epilepsy 3

fication as a result of common misuse or mis- might experience changes in consciousness


understanding of those terms. The current accompanied by behaviors that seem out of their
classification makes much less specification control. These behavioral “automatisms” have
regarding focal seizures, placing more emphasis formed the basis of much debate on the role of
on describing these seizures based on features consciousness in behavior, both from a medical
that are most useful for a given purpose. The and legal perspective. Table 1.2 includes a sum-
symptomatic characteristics of focal seizures are mary of terms that are now used to describe the
varied and reflect functional characteristics asso- range of impairment associated with focal sei-
ciated with the brain region from which they zures. Neuropsychologists are urged to become
originate. For this reason, neuropsychologists familiar with the terminology from this table as a
and other professionals interested in the study of starting point for providing a full clinical descrip-
behavior have shown a marked interest in focal tion of the phenomena reported by their patients.
epilepsy and its tendency to provide a “window”
into underlying brain mechanisms.
Focal seizures often begin with an aura, which Epilepsy Syndromes
commonly reflects the anatomic origin of the
initial site of abnormal brain activity. Seizures In the past, clinicians evaluating and treating
originating in temporal or limbic zones will often patients with epilepsy focused primarily on seizure
be preceded by onset of a visceral feeling or types and EEG abnormalities when classifying vari-
behavioral manifestations involving cognition or ous forms of epilepsy. With technological develop-
emotion. For example, seizures originating from ments in brain imaging and molecular genetics,
the hippocampus are commonly characterized by clinicians now focus more on characterizing epi-
mnestic changes, such as the well-known déjà vu lepsy syndromes, which now extend beyond the
phenomenon whereas those involving the amyg- disorder’s electroclinical characteristics. A listing of
dala might have more of an affective component, the syndrome recognized by the ILAE is provided
such as the subjective feeling of fear. Auras ema- in Table 1.3. Neuropsychologists working with epi-
nating from more posterior brain regions might lepsy patients are encouraged to be well acquainted
involve somatosensory or visual changes associ- with these syndromes.
ated with parietal or occipital regions respec-
tively. Details regarding these auras provide
valuable information about the localization of the
Table 1.2 Descriptors of focal seizures according to
seizure onset in addition to helpful clues about
degree of impairment during seizure—ILAE Revised
the nature of the functional impairment that (2010)
might exist when the individual is not having
Without impairment of consciousness or awareness with
seizures. observable motor or autonomic components. This
The evolution of focal seizures can be varied in roughly corresponds to the concept of “simple partial
nature, with the variability reflecting the track of seizure.” “Focal motor” and “autonomic” are terms that
abnormal electrical activity through a complex may adequately convey this concept depending on the
seizure manifestations.
neural network involving subcortical and cortical
Involving subjective sensory or psychic phenomena only.
brain regions. Some seizures will not evolve This corresponds to the concept of an aura, a term
beyond the level of the aura, which makes them endorsed in the 2001 Glossary.
brief and simple in their characteristics. Other With impairment of consciousness or awareness. This
focal seizures will spread into a generalized roughly corresponds to the concept of complex partial
seizure. “Dyscognitive” is a term that has been proposed
tonic–clonic convulsion. In between these for this concept.
extremes, seizures will involve varying changes in Evolving to a bilateral, convulsive seizure (involving
consciousness, which are often difficult to charac- tonic, clonic, or tonic and clonic components). This
terize. In some cases, individuals are totally aware expression replaces the term “secondarily generalized
of their surroundings. In other cases, individuals seizure.”
4 W.B. Barr

Table 1.3 Electroclinical syndromes and other Table 1.3 (continued)


epilepsies—ILAE Revised (2010)
Electroclinical syndromes arranged by age at onset
Electroclinical syndromes arranged by age at onset Hemiconvulsion–hemiplegia–epilepsy
Neonatal period Epilepsies that do not fit into any of these diagnostic
Benign familial neonatal epilepsy (BFNE) categories can be distinguished first on the basis of the
Early myoclonic encephalopathy (EME) presence or absence of a known structural or metabolic
Ohtahara syndrome condition (presumed cause) and then on the basis of the
primary mode of seizure onset (generalized vs. focal)
Infancy
Epilepsies attributed to and organized by structural-
Epilepsy of infancy with migrating focal seizures
metabolic causes
West syndrome
Malformations of cortical development
Myoclonic epilepsy in infancy (MEI) (hemimegalencephaly, heterotopias, etc.)
Benign infantile epilepsy Neurocutaneous syndromes (tuberous sclerosis
Benign familial infantile epilepsy complex, Sturge–Weber, etc.)
Dravet syndrome Tumor
Myoclonic encephalopathy in nonprogressive disorders Infection
Childhood Trauma
Febrile seizures plus (FS+) (can start in infancy) Angioma
Panayiotopoulos syndrome Perinatal insults
Epilepsy with myoclonic atonic (previously astatic) Stroke
seizures Etc.
Benign epilepsy with centrotemporal spikes (BECTS) Epilepsies of unknown cause
Autosomal-dominant nocturnal frontal lobe epilepsy Conditions with epileptic seizures that are traditionally
(ADNFLE) not diagnosed as a form of epilepsy per se
Late onset childhood occipital epilepsy (Gastaut type) Benign neonatal seizures (BNS)
Epilepsy with myoclonic absences Febrile seizures (FS)
Lennox–Gastaut syndrome
Epileptic encephalopathy with continuous spike-and-
wave during sleep (CSWS)
Landau–Kleffner syndrome (LKS) A full discussion of the pediatric epilepsy syn-
Childhood absence epilepsy (CAE) dromes is beyond the scope of the current discus-
Adolescence—Adult sion. Excellent reviews of this material are
Juvenile absence epilepsy (JAE) provided in Chaps. 2 and 12. However, from the
Juvenile myoclonic epilepsy (JME) perspective of a neuropsychologist working with
Epilepsy with generalized tonic–clonic seizures alone adults with epilepsy, it is important to remember
Progressive myoclonus epilepsies (PME) that many forms of the disorder are developmen-
Autosomal dominant epilepsy with auditory features
tal in nature, meaning that patients seen as adults
(ADEAF)
Other familial temporal lobe epilepsies
might have experienced one of these develop-
Less specific age relationship mental syndromes during childhood. This is par-
Familial focal epilepsy with variable foci (childhood to ticularly true for adults with focal epilepsy
adult) beginning in childhood secondary to perinatal
Reflex epilepsies stroke, brain tumor, traumatic brain injury, or any
Distinctive constellations one of a number of causes of localized brain dis-
Mesial temporal lobe epilepsy with hippocampal turbance. It is important to recognize that
sclerosis (MTLE with HS) cognitive deficits in these early onset cases will
Rasmussen syndrome
appear different from those in cases where sei-
Gelastic seizures with hypothalamic hamartoma
zures develop in adulthood. In other cases, epi-
(continued) lepsy syndromes resulting from genetic or
1 Neuropsychological Assessment of Patients with Epilepsy 5

metabolic causes will be exhibited in adults as lobe epilepsy (P. D. Williamson et al., 1993), is
causes of intellectual disability. It is clear that the often seen in individuals with a history of febrile
scope of the neuropsychological evaluation will convulsions, auras of a visceral nature, and sei-
be affected by specific characteristics and needs zures beginning in the first decade of life.
of the patient. Hippocampal sclerosis, a condition characterized
Most pediatric epilepsy syndromes character- by gliosis and atrophy of the hippocampus, is the
ized by the presence of absence or myoclonic sei- most common pathological feature. Patients with
zures do not extend into the adult years. There temporal lobe seizures originating from a more
are, however, some important exceptions. lateral neocortical onset often have auras involv-
Juvenile myoclonic epilepsy (JME) is a form of ing illusory auditory or visual phenomena in
epilepsy developing in adolescence or early addition to more complex behavioral automa-
adulthood characterized by bilateral repetitive tisms (Pacia et al., 1996). The cause of epilepsy
myoclonic jerks occurring predominately in the in these cases is varied with etiologies ranging
upper extremities. These are, in many cases, from brain tumors, cortical dysplasia, to head
accompanied by generalized tonic–clonic sei- trauma.
zures and possibly absence attacks. While the From a neuropsychological standpoint,
condition is considered a form of generalized patients with mesial temporal lobe epilepsy are
epilepsy, neuropsychological studies find that known to have intact intelligence with particular
affected patients exhibit a pattern of neuropsy- deficits in word finding and memory retention
chological test performance characterized by (B. P. Hermann, Seidenberg, Schoenfeld, &
intellectual functioning within the average range Davies, 1997). Memory deficits in these patients
and deficits in executive functions including are believed to result from a primary disruption
planning, concept formation, and verbal fluency of consolidation processes secondary to the hip-
(Piazzini, Turner, Vignoli, Canger, & Canevini, pocampal pathology. These patients are thought
2008). More generalized patterns of neuropsy- to differ on a neuropsychological basis from
chological dysfunction have been identified in patients with lateral temporal lobe seizures, who
patients with idiopathic generalized epilepsy are thought to exhibit more difficulty with mem-
(Shehata & Bateh Ael, 2009). There is now inter- ory encoding in addition to a more pronounced
est in determining whether there are, in fact, impairment in naming skills (Hamberger, Seidel,
structural brain abnormalities in patients with Goodman, Perrine, & McKhann, 2003).
generalized epilepsies that might provide an Frontal lobe epilepsy is the second most fre-
explanation as to why some forms, such as JME, quently observed form of focal epilepsy. Seizures
show more localized patterns of neuropsycho- in patients with frontal lobe seizures include a
logical dysfunction. combination of unusual movements and/or vocal-
Neuropsychologists working with epilepsy izations often accompanied by abnormal motor
patients should become very familiar with focal activity (Patrikelis, Angelakis, & Gatzonis,
epilepsy syndromes as these are seen in more 2009). Like temporal lobe epilepsy, one encoun-
than half (60 %) of patients with epilepsy and are ters seizures manifested specifically by changes
those most often referred for assessment of cog- in awareness or consciousness, previously
nitive functioning. Temporal lobe epilepsy is the referred to as complex partial seizures.
most well known and frequent of the focal epi- Differentiating patients with frontal lobe epilepsy
lepsy syndromes, occurring in approximately from those with temporal lobe epilepsy based on
70–90 %. Most of these patients demonstrate an neuropsychological testing has proven more dif-
onset of seizures emanating from the medial tem- ficult than what was suggested by the early litera-
poral lobe region, involving structures such as the ture (Barr & Goldberg, 2003; B. P. Hermann,
hippocampus and amygdala. This form of the Wyler, & Richey, 1988; Milner, 1964). However,
disorder, often referred to as mesial temporal there is now published evidence indicating that
6 W.B. Barr

patients with frontal lobe epilepsy exhibit more irritability, they also have a simultaneous effect on
impairment in motor programming and deficits neuronal excitability, which has the potential to
on tests of response inhibition (Helmstaedter, reduce cognitive efficiency. Many of the most
Gleibner, Zentner, & Elger, 1998). commonly used AEDs, their indications, and their
Seizure localization in posterior cortical effects on cognition are listed in Table 1.4. There
regions is much less common than those originat- is an interesting history to some of the older drugs,
ing from the temporal and frontal lobes. Given where their effectiveness for reducing seizures
the infrequency of the posterior cortical epilep- was identified “accidentally” in laboratory studies
sies they are often lumped into a general classifi- with animal models. While these drugs are known
cation of “extratemporal seizures.” Seizures to be effective in treating multiple types of sei-
originating from the occipital lobe are character- zures in humans, they are also known to have sig-
ized by visual auras, eye blinking, alterations in nificant effects on cognition, which was not a
consciousness, and motor phenomena (Jobst focus of study when the use of these drugs was
et al., 2010). Those with an onset in the parietal first initiated. As a result, many of the newer
lobe are often accompanied by somatosensory AEDs were developed with the goal of modulat-
phenomena but little else that is specific to a dis- ing alternative neuronal mechanisms with the ulti-
ruption of that part of the brain (P. D. Williamson mate goal of improving seizure reduction with
et al., 1992). Deficits in attention and processing minimal effects on cognition.
speed are seen in some patients with occipital Many patients require treatment with more
lobe seizures, but neuropsychological evidence than one AED to achieve adequate seizure con-
of disruption of higher or visuoperceptual or lan- trol. Clinicians prescribing these medications
guage functions associated with focal seizures must consider the balance between the need to
originating from the posterior cortex is mixed minimize seizure recurrence and the desire to
(Guerrini et al., 1997). reduce side effects as polypharmacy is the num-
It is important to remember that focal epilepsy ber one treatment factor known to affect cogni-
can be the result of any one of a number of differ- tive functioning (Loring, Marino, & Meador,
ent pathological processes. Brain tumor is the 2007). Surprisingly little is known about the pro-
second most frequent cause of seizures. Stroke is file of cognitive side effects associated with spe-
another common cause of epilepsy with seizures cific AEDs. Most of this is the result of the
known to develop at a higher rate following
embolic events and from cerebrovascular events
Table 1.4 Cognitive side effects of antiepileptic drugs
involving the middle cerebral artery. While neu- (AEDs)
ropsychological profiles in patients with seizures
Epilepsy type Cognitive effects
resulting from one of these conditions are clearly
Older AEDs
influenced by lesion location, one must also con-
Phenobarbital Generalized, focal Significant
sider the influence of seizure variables, medica-
Phenytoin Focal, generalized Moderate
tion effects, and other medical and emotional Carbamazepine Focal Moderate
characteristics of the patient (Morrison & Sodium valproate Generalized, focal Moderate
Nakhutina, 2007). Newer AEDs
Felbatol Focal Mild
Lamotrigine Generalized, focal Mild
Treatment of Epilepsy Vigabatrin Focal, generalized Mild
Gabapentin Focal Mild
Nearly every patient with an established diagnosis Topiramate Focal, generalized Moderate
of epilepsy will undergo some form of medication Tiagabine Focal Mild
management with antiepileptic drugs (AEDs). Oxcarbazepine Focal, generalized Mild
While these drugs are known to be effective in Zonisamide Focal Mild
reducing seizures through effects on neuronal Levetiracetam Focal, generalized Mild
1 Neuropsychological Assessment of Patients with Epilepsy 7

multitude of methodological challenges one emphasize that, based on results of experimental


faces when attempting to conduct controlled studies, the AED effects on cognition are known to
studies on AEDs in patient samples (Kwan & be minimal. The clinician should also inform them
Brodie, 2001). Some of this has been the result of that the initial cause of the epilepsy and the ongo-
a failure of neuropsychology, as a field, to agree ing effects of seizures are likely to be exerting an
upon a set of measures to evaluate cognitive effect on their cognition and that their subjective
functions in randomized controlled drug trials, sense of attention or memory disturbance is not
much in the manner that the Alzheimer’s Disease likely to be caused solely by the drugs they are tak-
Assessment Scale – cognitive subscale (ADAS- ing. They also need to be reminded that while the
Cog) is used in Alzheimer’s disease studies drugs are known to have some side effects, these
(Mohs et al., 1997). are likely to be minimal in comparison to the
What is known from existing research is that decline in cognitive functioning that would poten-
the newer drugs have less severe side effects than tially result if one were to discontinue medication
the older drugs. It is generally known that pheno- management altogether, which would lead to
barbital is the drug with the most potent cognitive uncontrolled seizures in many patients taking
side effects. Based on the older literature, it has AEDs and the possibility of a steady decline in
generally been assumed that other drugs, such as cognitive functioning.
phenytoin and carbamazepine, have more moder- While most patients with epilepsy achieve ade-
ate level effects on attention, processing speed, quate seizure control with AED treatment, there
and memory retrieval. At one point, investigators remain a substantial number who become “drug-
were arguing whether these drugs’ influence on resistant,” which is defined as a failure following
processing speed was the primary result of atten- an appropriate trial of two (or more) appropriately
tional or motoric factors (Dodrill, 1975, 1992; used medications. It is now estimated that 30–40 %
Trimble & Thompson, 1983). of patients will eventually become refractory to
Findings on newer AEDs have shown more treatment, defined by medical terms (Kwan,
generalized effects on cognitive functioning. Schachter, & Brodie, 2011). One must also con-
Many note that some drugs, such as lamotrigine, sider a number of other factors when determining
have only minor CNS side effects in addition to whether an individual has reached a treatment
some “alerting” properties associated with bene- refractory state. One of these involves whether the
ficial effects on mood (Kwan & Brodie, 2001; patient has “socially disabling” seizures, which are
Loring et al., 2007). Topiramate, an extremely seizures that reach a point where they have a
effective antiepileptic agent, is the one among the marked detrimental effect on one’s lifestyle. It is
newer drugs that stands out as having differential important to recognize that some patients, possi-
effects than the other newer drugs. Patients tak- bly in the context of intellectual disability, can tol-
ing this drug have been described as having more erate the occurrence of daily seizures, if they are
effects on cognition than other drugs with addi- residing in a safe and supportive environment.
tional effects reported on executive functions and Other patients, such as those working in profes-
language skills including naming and verbal flu- sional roles or with family responsibilities, might
ency. It has been demonstrated that these effects not be able to tolerate one seizure per year without
can be reduced with careful titration and minimal sustaining a substantial disruption on their lives. It
dosing, when possible (Loring, Williamson, is important to realize that cognitive functioning,
Meador, Wiegand, & Hulihan, 2011). as assessed through neuropsychological testing,
In practice, neuropsychologists need to recog- can provide significant information in helping to
nize that patients with epilepsy will require vari- determine whether patients are achieving optimal
able levels and types of medications to control their treatment of their seizures.
seizures. Some patients tend to report more cogni- There are a number of alternatives to drug
tive side effects than others. In counseling patients treatment for epilepsy, although most are used in
receiving medication management, one should combination with drugs in the majority of cases.
8 W.B. Barr

Surgery is the most well known of these associate neuropsychologists with assessment
treatments, with results demonstrating that sig- services, the role they play in specialized epi-
nificant relief from seizures can be obtained in lepsy centers often extends far beyond testing. As
approximately 70 % of patients undergoing sur- found in many clinical settings, neuropsycholo-
gical resection of the epileptic focus (Wiebe, gists are valued for their ability to integrate infor-
Blume, Girvin, Eliasziw, & Effectiveness and mation obtained through various sources. As a
Efficiency of Surgery for Temporal Lobe Epilepsy result, they are often the only clinicians present
Study Group, 2001). However, it is recognized on the team who are in a position to provide a
that there is risk to decline in verbal memory and comprehensive and balanced description of the
naming following these procedures, with greater patient based on their medical, cognitive,
levels of impairment observed in those undergo- emotional, and psychosocial characteristics.
ing left side surgery (Sherman et al., 2011). More Neuropsychologists are also able to provide valu-
details about the neuropsychological aspects of able input to the treatment team by taking into
epilepsy surgery can be found in Chap. 4. Vagal account unique characteristics of the patient’s
nerve stimulation is another one of the other social and cultural background when making
available nondrug treatments involving surgical clinical recommendations.
placement of a programmable pulse generator in There are numerous reasons to conduct a neu-
the patient’s chest. While the efficacy of this ropsychological assessment on a patient with epi-
treatment is considered somewhat modest, there lepsy. Some of these recommended by the NAEC
have been some suggestions that it has less of an are listed in Table 1.5. While most attention is
effect on cognitive functioning with possible typically based on assessment for epilepsy sur-
improvement seen in some patients. Information gery (see Chap. 4), there are many other questions
about cognitive functions associated with some that can be addressed by performing neuropsy-
of the newer epilepsy treatments such as electri- chological testing on a patient with epilepsy.
cal or pharmacological stimulation is currently Neuropsychological assessment is often
limited. requested in situations when there is a need to
know a patient’s particular strengths and weak-
nesses. This knowledge will be put to use in dif-
Neuropsychological Assessment ferent ways depending on the patient’s age and
the context of their daily activities. For example,
Clinical neuropsychologists are considered a student in college might need this information
essential members of the interdisciplinary teams to receive academic accommodations while
found at most epilepsy centers. In fact, guidelines another patient might benefit from receiving
established by the National Association of
Epilepsy Centers consider doctoral level neuro- Table 1.5 Guidelines for neuropsychological services:
psychologist among the staff members recom- National Association of Epilepsy Centers (NAEC, 2010)
mended for staffing at all Level 3 and 4 epilepsy 1. Comprehensive neuropsychological test batteries for:
specialty centers (NAEC Guidelines, 2010). The (a) Evaluation of cerebral dysfunction for vocational
guidelines encourage neuropsychologists work- and rehabilitative services.
ing in these settings to have postdoctoral training (b) Localization of cerebral dysfunction in evaluation
in neuropsychology with specific experience in for epilepsy surgery.
using neuropsychological tests for evaluation of (c) Basic assessment of characterological and
psychopathological issues.
epilepsy surgery along with a background in
2. An established referral arrangement for
interpreting results of intracarotid amobarbital comprehensive management of psychogenic
procedures (Wada Tests). nonepileptic events.
Neuropsychologists play a number of impor- 3. Clinical psychological services for assessment and
tant roles in an epilepsy center by virtue of the basic treatment of emotional disorders associated with
unique training they have received. While many chronic epilepsy.
1 Neuropsychological Assessment of Patients with Epilepsy 9

information from testing to guide vocational that will either be confirmed or disconfirmed
planning. Other patients complaining of cogni- through the results of formal testing. In a patient
tive difficulties will appear for testing with the with epilepsy, the clinical interview provides an
goal of verifying whether their subjective sense occasion for the neuropsychologist to pose ques-
of memory or concentration impairment is “real” tions directly to the patient regarding the charac-
and finding if there is a way that something can teristics of their seizures in addition to how the
be done to reduce it. seizures affect their cognitive and behavioral
Evaluations by neuropsychologists are also functioning.
commonly obtained when the treatment team Before considering the elements of a clinical
desires input on cognition to help in making a dif- interview with an epilepsy patient, it is useful to
ferential diagnosis. This information might come present a model for understanding how epilepsy
in handy when there is a need to determine affects behavior and cognition. In a comprehen-
whether a geriatric patient’s reported memory dis- sive review published in 1984, Bruce Hermann
turbance is related more to the influence of ongo- and Steven Whitman provided a model for under-
ing seizures or to the effects of an underlying standing epilepsy’s effects on behavior and per-
neurodegenerative process such as Alzheimer’s sonality, which can be adapted easily to explain
disease. In other cases, testing can be useful in cognitive factors (B. P. Hermann & Whitman,
monitoring effects of nonsurgical treatments such 1984). The model conceptualizes behavior in
as the side effects of medications. patients with epilepsy as influenced by various
While neuropsychological assessment of a effects, beginning with brain-related factors, such
patient with epilepsy generally focuses on evalu- as the initial cause of the epilepsy or the effects of
ation of cognitive functions it is important to rec- seizures. Secondly, one must consider treatment
ognize that clinicians working in epilepsy centers effects, particularly the influence of AEDs and
also play a role in addressing mental health surgery. Lastly, one needs to acknowledge that
issues. As listed in Table 1.5, neuropsychologists various “non-brain” variables will affect behav-
often play a significant role in evaluating and ior, including social variables and the effects of
treating patients with psychological nonepileptic mood. Figure 1.1 provides a depiction of the rela-
seizures (PNES) and other types of psychiatric tive contributions of these various factors.
disorders. Details regarding these types of evalu- Characterizing the effects of these three variables
ations are provided in Chaps. 9 and 11. on cognition can be very helpful to clinicians
when attempting to integrate information rele-
vant to the assessment and when providing feed-
Clinical Interview back to patients regarding the effects of the
epilepsy on their attention and memory
Many consider the neuropsychologist’s clinical functioning.
interview to be the most important component of When beginning the interview, the
the assessment process. In many cases, it is the neuropsychologist should start with a description
opportunity for the clinician to obtain a direct of the nature and purposes of the testing. The
description of symptoms and important historical examinee must have the ability to provide
elements while observing the patient’s behavior informed consent to proceed with the evaluation,
in an open-ended condition. The Russian neuro- consistent with the ethical principles of the
psychologist, A.R. Luria, emphasized the “pre- American Psychological Association (APA,
liminary conversation” as the point where the 2002). During this point of the evaluation pro-
clinician develops initial theories about the cess, the examiner should be in a position to
patient and his or her condition that will guide the address any questions the patient might have
choice of assessment procedures (Luria, 1966). about the testing or their condition and life in
The interview often serves as the time when the general. It is also important to inform the patient
clinician arrives at a set of relevant hypotheses that a report will be prepared and will be sent to
10 W.B. Barr

Fig. 1.1 Factors contribut-


ing to cognitive and
behavioral impairment in
epilepsy (from Kwan &
Brodie, 2001. Reproduced
with permission)

the referring party. One might also ask whether assessment (Thompson & Corcoran, 1992).
the report should be sent to any other parties and However, the clinician should always remember
what arrangements will be made to provide direct that patients and referral sources are not psychol-
feedback to the patient regarding the final results. ogists and, as a result of a lack of knowledge, may
The information-gathering portion of the inter- refer to cognitive difficulties more generally as
view should begin with the patient’s subjective memory difficulties in cases when attention or
description of their symptoms and how the symp- executive functions are the actual domains that
toms affect their daily lives. Based on research are affected. One will encounter many cases
findings, we know that approximately 70 % of where a patient with epilepsy will complain of
patients with epilepsy experience a subjective dis- memory difficulties and it is discovered that their
turbance of memory, making this the most com- major problem is more consistent with impair-
mon cause for a referral for neuropsychological ment in executive functions such as organization
1 Neuropsychological Assessment of Patients with Epilepsy 11

or planning. A guided description of the patient’s have on neuropsychological test performance.


symptoms and the context in which they arise will From a neuropsychological perspective it is impor-
provide the neuropsychologist with valuable tant to estimate the age of onset of the epilepsy, as
insights into the presenting problem. it is known that an onset of seizures early in life
After coming to an understanding of the will have a more generalized and detrimental
patient’s cognitive complaints it is important to effect on test performance (Dikmen, Matthews, &
turn to their seizure history. To begin with, it is Harley, 1975; Elger, Helmstaedter, & Kurthen,
important to have a good idea of when the sei- 2004). In terms of seizure type, it is clear that gen-
zures began. While this might be assumed to be eralized tonic–clonic seizures provide a greater
when the patient first came to medical attention, impact on neuropsychological functioning than
that is not likely to be the exact point when the other types of focal seizures (Dodrill, 1986). It is
seizures first appeared. Many patients will also important to determine whether the patient
describe unusual experiences occurring during has ever experienced status epilepticus, a condi-
their lifetime that, in retrospect, were likely to tion characterized by prolonged or clustered sei-
have been the effects of focal seizures, long zures, which is known to provide a significant
before they received medical treatment. It also long-term disruption to memory and other func-
helps to differentiate when in life the patient tions in some patients (Dodrill & Wilensky, 1990).
developed habitual seizures, as opposed to other Patients often have a difficult time quantifying
types of seizures that had appeared at some point the frequency and severity of their seizures.
during their lifetime. In this context, it is impor- While many are asked to keep a diary on their
tant, for example, to determine whether a patient seizure activity, these are often found to be inac-
ever experienced a febrile seizure during infancy curate as a result of the fact that some patients are
or early childhood (<3 years), which is known to not aware that a seizure has occurred and, with-
be a strong risk factor for mesial temporal epi- out the help of a reliable collateral informant,
lepsy in a patient presenting later in life with a might not be able to specify how many seizures
history of focal seizures. they are having with any acceptable degree of
At some point, the interview will turn to the reliability. While it is clear that having frequent
patient’s own description of their seizures. It is seizures is not good for one’s cognitive function-
often useful to obtain these descriptions in spite ing, it is not totally clear how “seizure burden,” a
of the fact that the medical record might contain concept designed to capture a combination of sei-
detailed information on their seizure history. As zure type and frequency, affects performance on
in other contexts, it is often good to go back to the neuropsychological testing. Further research on
original source for a full description. Coming to this topic is clearly needed.
an understanding of the patient’s subjective A good clinical interview will also address the
account of the seizure, as opposed to what others etiology of the epilepsy, as this is likely to have a
have told them, will help the interviewer deter- major impact on neuropsychological functioning,
mine to what degree consciousness is affected independent of the effects of seizures. This should
during the seizure and whether there are any spe- start with a detailed neurodevelopmental history,
cific subjective phenomena that might provide beginning with details on the patient’s birth and
clues to localization. Negative information, such early maturation. This will include information
as the patient claiming to be totally amnestic for regarding the age at which the patient began walk-
the seizures, can also be extremely helpful, as it ing and talking. One will also want to learn details
has been shown that those who are amnestic for of the patient’s school history and whether there
seizure activity are more likely to experience was any evidence of learning disability (LD) or
bitemporal abnormalities on EEG (Palmini, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)
Gloor, & Jones-Gotman, 1992). identified by parents, school, or medical person-
A number of studies over the past 40 years have nel. Information regarding the patient’s favorite
examined the effects that various seizure variables subjects in school, or those subjects they disliked
12 W.B. Barr

immensely, has the potential of providing valuable the patient is taking any other medications for
input to cognitive deficits that might have origi- other physical illnesses or ailments with a poten-
nated in childhood. It is also important to learn tial for similar side effects. The interviewer
about the patient’s social development, in terms of should be aware if there is a family history of epi-
their ability to establish friendships and peer rela- lepsy or any other types of family based neuro-
tionships. One might also ask if they remember logical or neuropsychiatric disorders. As with
being bullied or excluded from activities as a result any evaluation, the clinician should determine
of their seizures. whether there is any history of alcohol or drug
In patients with a history of focal seizures, it is abuse.
important to obtain medical information on the Patients with epilepsy are known to have a
causative factor, as this will also have a likely higher rate of mental illness than the general pop-
impact on neuropsychological functioning. In ulation. This topic is covered in detail in Chap. 9.
those patients with a history of brain tumor, one It is estimated that approximately 30 % of patients
should understand when symptoms of the tumor with epilepsy will encounter depression at some
first appeared and whether that coincided with point in their lives (Kanner, 2013). A substantial
the onset of epilepsy. One must also know the number of patients with epilepsy also experience
pathological type and grade of the tumor in addi- anxiety disorders. Clinicians should ask whether
tion to whether any surgery has been performed. the patient has ever been under any form of psy-
In cases of traumatic brain injury (TBI), it is nec- chological or psychiatric treatment. The neuro-
essary to gain information on the nature of the psychologist will need to conduct a detailed
initial injury, including whether there was a loss assessment of mood disorder symptoms during
of consciousness, post-traumatic amnesia, or the clinical interview and should be in a position
coma. One must come to an understanding of the to refer for psychiatric treatment, if indicated.
overall severity of the injury, as it is clear that While seen more rarely, one should also be on the
epilepsy will occur much more frequently fol- lookout for the presence of psychotic symptoms,
lowing a severe level of TBI as opposed to mild either emerging in the context of seizure clusters
TBI (Lowenstein, 2009). Neuropsychologists or occurring totally independent of seizure
will also need to obtain details regarding illness activity.
and onset of seizures in patients with epilepsies It is helpful to learn details about the patient’s
resulting from infectious and other medical social background. One might ask questions on
causes. where they were born and raised. At this point,
One must always remember that a patient with one can get information about the family back-
epilepsy is first and foremost a person living their ground, including the number of siblings. It is
life who will face the same medical, psycho- often helpful to know details about family mem-
logical, and social factors that affect all other bers and their educational and occupational
individuals undergoing a neuropsychological backgrounds. As a result of the condition and its
evaluation. The clinician must be in a position to effects on cognition and education, some patients
determine whether there are any ongoing medical with epilepsy may be prone to feeling as if they
factors, such as hypertension, hypercholesterol- have achieved less than others in their family,
emia, diabetes, or cardiac abnormalities that which can have an effect on them from a psycho-
might affect the patient’s neuropsychological social perspective.
functioning. One must know whether the patient It is very important for the neuropsychologist
has undergone any type of surgical procedure, to have a full understanding of the patient’s cul-
whether or not it is related to treatment for epi- tural background before proceeding with the
lepsy. It is important to have information on the evaluation. One must know first whether English
full regimen of medications the patient is taking. is the patient’s native language. The vast majority
While the cognitive side effects of AEDs are of neuropsychological tests used in North
reviewed above, one must also determine whether America are developed and normed in English.
1 Neuropsychological Assessment of Patients with Epilepsy 13

When planning the evaluation, it is important to rather broad and encompasses test selection,
understand whether or not the patient can com- interpretation of the data, and integrating the
plete the testing with the use of standard instru- findings with clinical information obtained from
ments, or whether testing in their native language other sources. One must not confuse the assess-
will be required. More detailed discussion of this ment process with that of test administration,
issue is provided in Chap. 14. Secondly, it is which is clearly an important component of the
known that patients’ and families’ understanding overall assessment process, but requires less
and perception of illness and seizures varies extensive training with heavy reliance on proce-
widely among cultures. Neuropsychologists, by dural guidelines provided in test manuals.
virtue of their specialized training, are in a posi- It is recognized that professional preparation
tion to recognize many of these factors and inte- for practice in neuropsychological assessment
grate them into their evaluation and interpretation requires extensive coursework and clinical train-
of the test findings. ing in neuropsychology and knowledge about
Turning back to the patient, it is helpful to learn epilepsy syndromes and effects of seizures and
details about their educational history. One will treatments on cognitive functions. In terms of
also want to obtain a detailed occupational history education and training, this typically requires a
with emphasis on how seizures and cognitive doctoral degree in professional psychology (e.g.,
effects were expressed during the course of the Ph.D. or Psy.D.), clinical training in generic clin-
individual’s employment and how employers, ical psychology, and 2 years of postdoctoral
supervisors, and coworkers reacted to these issues. training in clinical neuropsychology.
It is often helpful to break down the patient’s Practice in clinical neuropsychology requires
workday and activities to determine if any work- a professional license in psychology, consistent
place accommodations are warranted if supported with local state or provincial laws. The scope of
by the neuropsychological test findings. knowledge and skills required for effective prac-
Finally, one will want to know details about tice in clinical neuropsychology, with its focus on
the patient’s home and social life. Asking about the study of the science of psychology, statistical
who is living with the patient in their current methods, and psychometrics, is distinctly differ-
home environment is a good way to address rela- ent from the education and training obtained in
tionship issues without any ties to traditional val- receipt of a typical medical degree or in other
ues. It is important to understand the quality of health care fields, which do not include a compa-
relationships and how these individuals react and rable level of education in any of these topics. It
support the patient in terms of their epilepsy. At is becoming more and more common for clinical
this point, one might also obtain information neuropsychologists to obtain board certification
about the patient’s interests and their daily activi- (e.g., ABCN) to formally document their training
ties. One might also query on how seizures and/ and proficiency. Commensurate with the medical
or cognitive issues affect their ability to perform model and the expectations made of our physi-
and enjoy these activities. cian colleagues, obtaining board certification is
highly recommended for those planning to work
in comprehensive epilepsy centers.
Neuropsychological Testing Results from professional surveys indicate that
over 50 % of neuropsychologists utilize ancillary
Background on Testing staff, such as graduate school trainees or techni-
While the specific roles that clinical neuropsy- cians, to administer neuropsychological tests
chologists will play in each epilepsy center might under their direct supervision (Sweet, Meyer,
vary, their responsibilities will almost always Nelson, & Moberg, 2011). Results of a survey
include neuropsychological assessment as a pri- conducted on neuropsychologists practicing specifi-
mary function. It is important to remember that cally in epilepsy centers indicate that 63 % employ
the process of neuropsychological assessment is technicians to assist with test administration
14 W.B. Barr

(Djordjevic & Jones-Gotman, 2011). The process can be identified in adults with epilepsy with
is analogous to that of a radiologist who interprets duration of illness appearing to be the most reli-
images produced from the work of a technician able predictor of its occurrence (Seidenberg,
rather than obtaining the images directly. The pro- Pulsipher, & Hermann, 2007). More detailed
fessional component lies in the interpretation and information about this issue can be found in
communication of the results, rather than from Chap. 3. Scores from neuropsychological testing
collecting the data. This practice, when used by naturally provide the optimal data for addressing
neuropsychologists, is recognized legislatively in this issue. Empirical documentation of cognitive
nearly every state and is reimbursed by third-party decline can be provided by demonstrating a sta-
payers across the country including the Centers tistically rare discrepancy between current test
for Medicare and Medicaid Services (CMS) with performance and measures used to estimate
specific Current Procedural Terminology (CPT) expected “premorbid” levels of functioning or
codes (e.g., CPT code 96119). Interested readers through demonstrating on repeat testing that the
can obtain further details about the use of techni- interval change in test scores exceeds what is
cians in neuropsychological testing through pub- expected by “chance” or as a result of “practice
lished statements provided by professional effects.” Clinicians using neuropsychological
organizations including Division 40 of the APA tests to address these questions must therefore be
(APA Division 40, 1991), the National Academy adept at using psychometric principles for inter-
of Neuropsychology [NAN, (Puente, Adams, preting base rates of discrepancy between test
Barr, Bush, & N. P. P. Committee, 2006)], and the scores and formal statistical methods for assess-
American Academy of Clinical Neuropsychology ing changes in test scores over time.
(AACN, 1999). Based on contents of the clinical descriptive
Neuropsychological tests provide an empiri- literature, we are well aware that patients with
cally based method for evaluating and measuring epilepsy can exhibit “normal” levels of intellec-
cognition and behavior. Successful use of these tual functioning while experiencing specific defi-
tests requires adherence to standardized admin- cits in cognitive areas such as attention, executive
istration guidelines and the proper use of psy- functions, and/or memory. However, identifying
chometric and normative principles when specific weaknesses in cognitive functioning in a
interpreting the results. The guidelines for neu- valid and reliable manner through neuropsycho-
ropsychological services provided in Table 1.5 logical testing can be a challenge to even the
above include general reasons for administering most seasoned clinical neuropsychologist. When
test batteries. However, from a purely empirical using a comprehensive battery of tests, one must
basis, the information provided by standardized be very careful to avoid making a Type I statistical
testing is most useful in advising the treatment error resulting from erroneously identifying a
team regarding three basic questions: (1) has deficit based on what might be statistically
there been a general decline in the patient’s level “chance” findings. The field of neuropsychology
of intellectual functioning, (2) are there weak- has been paying more attention to this issue in
nesses or impairments in specific aspects of cog- recent years with an increasing number of pub-
nitive functioning, and (3) has there been a lished papers demonstrating the frequency of
relevant and identifiable change in performance “abnormal” test scores found in healthy control
or functioning on testing performed between samples who have completed neuropsychologi-
Time 1 and Time 2? cal testing (Binder, Iverson, & Brooks, 2009;
Clinicians treating patients with epilepsy are Schretlen, Testa, Winicki, Pearlson, & Gordon,
very interested in knowing whether a given 2008). Neuropsychologists working in epilepsy
patient is experiencing any form of cognitive centers must be aware of these base rates of abnor-
decline that can be attributed to the epilepsy or mal test findings and exercise care when reviewing
seizures. Results from longitudinal studies indi- a sheet of scores from a neuropsychological test
cate that a progressive pattern of cognitive decline battery by making sure that a sufficient number of
1 Neuropsychological Assessment of Patients with Epilepsy 15

scores are in the abnormal range and that the hybrid approach to assessment is now used by the
pattern of abnormal scores fit some reasonable majority of practicing neuropsychologists, char-
conception of an identifiable cognitive deficit. acterized by what is termed as a flexible battery
One of the most powerful uses of neuropsy- of tests, chosen for use with specific patient
chological testing is to identify the presence of groups. This approach is based on the use of a
cognitive changes over time. Making inferences core battery of tests with various patient groups
from changes in test scores obtained over two or supplemented by adding or subtracting certain
more evaluations provides information that is tests from the battery depending on characteris-
useful for measuring treatment effects from tics and needs of the patient. This is the approach
medication and/or surgery and, as discussed recommended for use when evaluating patients
above, can be helpful in identifying changes with epilepsy.
associated with a pattern of cognitive decline. A neuropsychological test battery for epi-
Neuropsychologists working in the field of epi- lepsy, based significantly on use of measures
lepsy have been at the forefront of developing from the HRNB, was developed by Carl Dodrill
empirical methods for assessing interval change. in the 1980s (Dodrill, 1978). However, based on
Valuable information from a pioneering set of results from experimental studies on cognition in
studies performed in the 1990s has been used to epilepsy, most clinicians evaluating patients with
determine “normal” patterns of change over time epilepsy using a flexible battery of tests for assess-
in samples of epilepsy patients using a variety of ment of adults are encouraged to include measures
different tests (Chelune, Naugle, Luders, & of general intelligence, attention, executive func-
Awad, 1991; B. P. Hermann et al., 1996). Data tions, language, and memory. There is currently
from these studies, combined with empirical no universally accepted battery of tests. Common
methods for assessing change, such as the reli- with traditions in neuropsychology, individuals
able change index (RCI) and standardized from various centers tend to use tests that are
regression-based (SRB) methods, have provided most familiar to them based on a number of
the field with a statistically rigorous methodol- scientific and nonscientific factors including the
ogy for assessing change associated with drug preference of their clinical supervisors. This has
treatment, surgery, and other factors (Barr, 2002). led to differences in test usage based on the
Neuropsychologists are encouraged to use this regional location of the epilepsy center. There are
information when assessing changes in their efforts in the field of neuropsychology to phase
patients. out the tendency to choose tests based on comfort
level in favor of selecting a set of measures that
Neuropsychological Test Battery has been validated for clinical decision-making
There is no universally accepted approach to con- through empirical research (Chelune, 2010).
ducting a neuropsychological assessment in epi- Clinicians aiming to develop a clinical test bat-
lepsy patients or in those with any other clinical tery are often informed by learning which tests
condition. Over the years, methodological are used by their colleagues. Several surveys of
approaches to neuropsychological testing have fluc- neuropsychological test usage among clinicians
tuated between an emphasis on purely empirical or have been published (Rabin, Barr, & Burton,
quantitative methods, such as that which is charac- 2005), including some conducted on neuropsy-
terized by use of a fixed-standardized approach chologists working in specialized epilepsy cen-
such as the Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological ters. Djordjevic and Jones-Gotman (2011)
Test Battery (HRNB) (Reitan & Wolfson, 1985) conducted a recent survey from 75 respondents
and an emphasis on purely flexible approach working in epilepsy centers spanning across 17
using clinical or qualitative methods, such as countries, with the majority (65 %) of them from
what might be found using the Boston Process North America. The results of the survey are listed
Approach (Kaplan, 1988) to assessment. in Table 1.6. The findings demonstrate that, on
However, according to recent surveys, a more average, the duration of neuropsychological
16 W.B. Barr

Table 1.6 Summary of survey of neuropsychological The lack of a standardized battery of neuro-
test usage among neuropsychologists working in special-
psychological tests has posed a problem for epi-
ized epilepsy centers (Djordjevic & Jones-Gotman, 2011)
lepsy investigators attempting to conduct
Domain Many use large-scale multicenter investigations of cogni-
Most use (>50 %) (20–50 %)
tion or medication side effects. Recent recom-
Intelligence WAIS/WASI
Attention WAIS Digit Span Letter-Number
mendations for a standardized set of tests have
Sequencing been made by a subcommittee of the National
WAIS Digit WMS Spatial Span Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke
Symbol (NINDS) of the Common Data Elements (CDE)
Trail making Test Project in Epilepsy (Loring, Lowenstein, et al.,
Executive WCST Stroop 2011). The aim of this group was to produce a
Letter Fluency battery of tests that would enable some form of
(COWAT)
standardized data collection to facilitate direct
Category Fluency
comparisons across studies and to enable data
Motor Grooved Pegboard Finger Tapping
pooling in NINDS-funded studies. The commit-
Motor Strength
Language Boston Naming Token Test
tee chose tests with the goal of capturing relevant
Test areas of cognitive functioning across a variety of
WAIS Vocabulary WRAT epilepsy syndromes. Tests were more likely to be
Visuospatial WAIS Block Judgment of Line included if there was evidence of widespread use
Design Orientation in epilepsy studies and if the test is in the public
WAIS Picture Facial Recognition domain. A listing of the recommended CDE bat-
Completion Test
tery is provided in Table 1.7.
Rey Complex
Figure Copy There is surprisingly little information regard-
Memory WMS RAVLT ing the use of cognitive test batteries in patients
Rey Complex CVLT with epilepsy. While measures such
Figure Recall as the Woodcock-Johnson battery (WJR-III)
Mood BDI (Woodcock, McGrew, & Mather, 2001) have
BAI been used in children with epilepsy, there is little
Personality MMPI information on the use of this measure in adults,
PAI in spite of there being ample norms for this
BAI Beck Anxiety Inventory, BDI Beck Depression group. Investigators from the United Kingdom
Inventory, COWAT Controlled Oral Word Association Test, have demonstrated some success using the Adult
CVLT California Verbal Learning Test, MMPI Minnesota
Multiphasic Personality Inventory, PAI Personality Memory and Information Processing Battery
Assessment Inventory, RAVLT Rey Auditory Verbal (AMIPB) (Baxendale, Thompson, & Duncan,
Learning Test, WCST Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, WAIS 2008; Baxendale, Thompson, Harkness, &
Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, WASI Wechsler Duncan, 2006), a set of tests including speeded
Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence, WMS Wechsler Memory
Scale, WRAT Wide Range Achievement Test cancellation tests and measures of verbal and
nonverbal memory. There is little information on
the value of using the Repeatable Battery for
evaluations performed in epilepsy centers is 5.6 h. Assessment of Neuropsychological Status
The number of tests administered varies widely, (RBANS) (Randolph, Tierney, Mohr, & Chase,
with a range from 12 to 56 tests considered part of 1998) or the Neuropsychological Assessment
a “standard” battery. Neuropsychologists across Battery (NAB) (Stern & White, 2003) in patients
the globe are in general agreement about the with epilepsy in spite of growing use of these
domains of functioning assessed in patients with batteries in North America.
epilepsy and the type of core battery that is used, In spite of the entry of computer tests into the
although there is certainly evidence that many market, only a few studies have reported the use
centers continue to use their own specific choice of these batteries with epilepsy patients (Witt,
of tests for many applications. Alpherts, & Helmstaedter, 2013). Hoppe and
1 Neuropsychological Assessment of Patients with Epilepsy 17

Table 1.7 Epilepsy common data elements: recom- and provide means for assessing individual
mended neuropsychological instruments—adult (16+
strengths and weaknesses through comprehen-
years old) (Loring, Lowenstein et al., 2011)
sive statistical methods.
Domain Test One of the most important issues encountered
General IQ Estimation American National Adult
in the context of a neuropsychological evaluation
Reading Test (AmNART)
Formal IQ Testinga Wechsler Adult Intelligence
is whether or not the patient has experienced any
Scale (WAIS-IV) limitation in general intellectual functioning sec-
Wechsler Abbreviated Scale ondary to epilepsy or any other causes.
of Intelligence (WASI) Standardized tests of intelligence are generally
Executive Function Trail making A and B used for this purpose with the Wechsler Adult
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Intelligence Scale (WAIS), now in its fourth edi-
(WCST-64)
tion (WAIS-IV) (Wechsler, 2008), as the most
Motor Speed Grooved Pegboard
commonly used of these tests. Recent editions of
Language Boston Naming Test (BNT)
the WAIS have moved from a previous focus on
Controlled Oral Word
Association Test (COWAT) intelligence quotient (IQ) scores to analysis of
Animal Naming four cognitive composite scores, which is more
Verbal Memory Rey Auditory Verbal Learning consistent with results of factor analytic research
Test (AVLT) on intelligence. This allows clinicians to examine
Either WAIS-IV or WASI is recommended
a and measure factors that are more relevant to the
effects of various clinical disorders. There is a
rather large literature on the use of the various
colleagues demonstrated “fair” to “good” Wechsler scales in patients with epilepsy, provid-
diagnostic utility of computerized measures in ing a rationale for standard use with that
comparison to established neuropsychological population.
tests. Based on initial investigations examining While the WAIS also provides valuable infor-
the reliability and validity of computerized tests, mation for specific diagnostic purposes, such as
there is a long way to go before there will be any the identification of intellectual disability (ID),
evidence that such measures have any diagnostic there is evidence that its use in epilepsy centers
advantage over traditional neuropsychological has declined over the years (Djordjevic & Jones-
tests (Hoppe, Fliessbach, Schlegel, Elger, & Gotman, 2011). Some clinicians might prefer the
Helmstaedter, 2009). use of other tests, such as the Stanford Binet
Intelligence Scales, now in their fifth revision
Intellectual Functioning (SB5) (Roid, 2003), for evaluation of those
As mentioned above, measuring and tracking patients with IQ scores falling below 70. When
cognitive decline is one of the most important addressing the issue of ID, the clinician needs to
functions a neuropsychologist plays in a compre- be sure to take into account all of the issues rele-
hensive epilepsy center. Assessment of intellec- vant to that diagnosis, as spelled out by definition
tual functioning is therefore an essential provided by the American Association of
component of the neuropsychological test bat- Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities
tery. Administering a measure of intelligence (AAIDD) (A. T. C. Committee & Schalock,
provides one of the most sensitive means of 2007). In adults with possible ID it is often help-
tracking cognitive functions primarily as a result ful to supplement the IQ testing with assessment
of the fact that these tests provide composite of adaptive functioning through either interview
scores that are the most reliable measures found (e.g., Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scale-II)
within the test battery. Furthermore, these batter- (Sparrow, Cicchetti, & Balla, 2006) or question-
ies provide assessment of a number of different naire (Adaptive Behavior Assessment Scale—
functional domains that can be compared statisti- ABAS-II) (Harrison & Oakland, 2003)
cally, based on the fact that they are co-normed techniques.
18 W.B. Barr

There are a number of other measures of intel- also a number of performance-based indices
ligence that are available, although much less is where indices of vocabulary or oral reading are
known about their validity and clinical utility in used (e.g., WTAR, TOPF) (Pearson Assessment,
evaluating cognitive functions in patients with 2009) or the North American version of the
epilepsy. Some criticize the Wechsler scales as National Adult Reading Test (NART) (Blair &
taking too long to administer. Some measures Spreen, 1989), either alone or in combination
designed to provide IQ estimates in a relatively with demographic indices, to arrive at estimates
short period of time, including the Shipley- of IQ scores. To date, while these methods have
Hartford Scale (Shipley, 1940), have been avail- been demonstrated to be of use in identifying the
able for many years but have not been widely degree of cognitive decline observed in clinical
used in patients with epilepsy. Dodrill (1981) samples of dementia or traumatically brain
demonstrated that the Wonderlic Test of injured patients, no studies have validated their
Intelligence (Wonderlic, 2004) can be used effec- use in patients with cognitive impairment sec-
tively for rapid assessment of intelligence in epi- ondary to epilepsy.
lepsy patients. A number of shorter forms of the
WAIS, including the abbreviation methods rec- Attention and Processing Speed
ommended by Kaufman (Kaufman & Most discussions on the neuropsychology of
Lichtenberger, 2005), have been used in some attention begin with the topic of attention span,
studies as well as the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale which is typically assessed through the Digit
of Intelligence (WASI-II), which is the short Span subtest from one of the Wechsler scales
form developed by publishers of the WAIS (Wechsler, 2008). Scores from this subtest are
(Wechsler, 2011). While norms for adults are generally combined with the score from the
available for other tests, such as the Woodcock Arithmetic subtest to comprise with Working
Johnson Tests of Cognitive Abilities (WJ3) Memory Index (WMI). While scores on the WMI
(Woodcock et al., 2001) and the Reynolds are reduced in some patients, performances on
Intelligence Screening Tests (RIST) (Kamphaus digit span tasks, both forward and backward, are
& Reynolds, 2003), these measures do not appear generally preserved in patients with epilepsy,
to be used commonly in adults with epilepsy. In similar to what is seen in many other clinical
some cases, where native language might be an populations (Wilde et al., 2001).
issue tests such as the Raven Standard Progressive There are no indications that Digit Span per-
Matrices (RSPM) (Raven, Raven, & Court, 1998) formance is affected appreciably by either drug
or Test of Nonverbal Intelligence (TONI-4) effects or the ongoing influence of uncontrolled
(Brown, Sherbenou, & Johnson, 2010) can be seizures. The claims from clinical folklore, that
used for assessment of intelligence through non- forward and backward span tasks assessed differ-
verbal means. ent neuropsychological processes, have not been
Up until 20 years ago, neuropsychologists supported through empirical study of patients
would address the issue of cognitive decline with epilepsy (Bowden, Petrauskas, Bardenhagen,
through informal “historical” means comparing Meade, & Simpson, 2013). While interest in the
IQ scores to estimations of intelligence vaguely spatial span analog of the digit span subtest was
defined by the individual’s demographic, educa- generated through early studies by Milner and
tional, and occupational backgrounds Corsi (Milner, 1968), there has been little evi-
(Vanderploeg & Schinka, 1995). More recently, a dence that addition of a spatial span task adds any
number of actuarial methods have been devel- novel information about cognitive functioning or
oped for this purpose. Some of these involve use cerebral localization in routine assessment of epi-
of statistical regression techniques, where vari- lepsy patients (Piekema et al., 2007).
ous demographic indices can be inserted into an Patients with epilepsy commonly demonstrate
equation to arrive at predicted WAIS IQ scores decrements in processing speed, defined primar-
(Barona, Reynolds, & Chastain, 1984). There are ily through performance on paper-pencil tests.
1 Neuropsychological Assessment of Patients with Epilepsy 19

Reduced scores are commonly observed on the is an important component to the clinical test
Digit Symbol and Symbol Search subtests from battery for evaluation of epilepsy patients,
the Wechsler scales, both of which comprise the although in practice one sees that only a limited
Processing Speed Index (PSI). Low scores on number of tests are used on a regular basis. The
these tests can be attributed to a combination of Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) (Heaton,
different factors, including medication effects Chelune, Talley, Kay, & Curtiss, 1993) and mea-
and more generalized effects of seizures and sures of verbal fluency are the measures most
brain dysfunction. Clinicians should keep in commonly used in epilepsy centers (Djordjevic
mind that signs of reduced processing speed can & Jones-Gotman, 2011). The WCST is com-
also be observed on other neuropsychological monly utilized as a measure of problem solving
tests, including many tests of executive function, and set shifting while the verbal fluency mea-
such as Part A of the Trail Making Test (Reitan & sures are used for evaluation of initiation, partic-
Wolfson, 1985), the initial trials of the Stroop ularly in reference to left hemisphere functions.
Color Naming Test (Golden, 1978), and on mea- While the results of pioneering studies by Milner
sures of verbal and visual fluency. (1964) demonstrated that low levels of perfor-
On an interesting historical note, neuropsy- mance on these tests could be used to identify
chologists should be aware that the paradigm for patients with frontal lobe seizures, more recent
the Continuous Performance Test (CPT) com- studies have demonstrated that performance dec-
monly associated with evaluations of patients rements are also common in patients with epilep-
with ADHD was initially developed for assess- tic foci localized in other regions (Barr &
ment of attentional lapses in patients with epi- Goldberg, 2003; B. P. Hermann et al., 1988).
leptic seizures (Rosvold, Mirsky, Sarason, There are a number of other tests that are use-
Bransome, & Beck, 1956). While one can argue ful for evaluating executive functions in patients
that use of the CPT might provide additional data with epilepsy. Impairments in mental rigidity and
to diagnose ADHD in an adult with epilepsy, the higher-order sequencing are commonly identified
evidence is that these measures are used on a rou- through poor performance on Part B of the Trail
tine basis in only 13 % of epilepsy centers Making Test. The Stroop Color Naming Test
(Djordjevic & Jones-Gotman, 2011) and that epi- (Golden, 1978) can be useful for identifying dif-
lepsy patients, as a whole, demonstrate minimal ficulties with impulsivity or response inhibition.
signs of impairment on these measures (Fleck, While there had been suggestions that verbal flu-
Shear, & Strakowski, 2002). Studies using exper- ency tests, such as the COWAT, could be com-
imental computerized tasks have demonstrated bined with measures of figural fluency [e.g., Ruff
decrements in reaction time in epilepsy patients, Figural Fluency Test (Ruff, 1988)] to help lateral-
particularly in studies of medication side effects ize frontal lobe dysfunction, the empirical evi-
(Witt et al., 2013). However, neuropsychologists dence to support this practice appears mixed
have apparently been reluctant to include com- (Suchy, Sands, & Chelune, 2003).
puterized assessment of reaction time, processing Many clinicians have now turned to the Delis
speed, or vigilance into their routine test batter- Kaplan Executive Function System (DKEFS)
ies, as only 5 % indicate usage of these tests on a (Delis, Kaplan, & Kramer, 2001) versions of
routine basis (Djordjevic & Jones-Gotman, many executive function measures as a result of
2011). its inclusion of co-normed instrument across a
wide age spectrum. While the research literature
Executive Functions on use of the DKEFS in assessment of epilepsy
The term executive functions is commonly used patients is limited, there are suggestions that
to describe a wide range of psychological con- many of these measures can be used successfully
structs, including initiation, sequencing, organi- in patients with frontal and temporal lobe sei-
zation, and planning. Most neuropsychologists zures (McDonald, Delis, Norman, Tecoma, &
will agree that evaluation of executive functions Iragui, 2005; McDonald, Delis, Norman, Wetter,
20 W.B. Barr

et al., 2005). While the use of executive function- more severe forms of epilepsy. Performance on
ing questionnaires can be useful for identifying the Grooved Pegboard Test has also been dem-
daily functioning in some patients, there remains onstrated to be a measure that is sensitive to
little published research supporting the use of detecting psychomotor slowing associated with
measures, such as the Behavioural Assessment of antiepileptic drug treatment (Meador, Loring,
the Dysexecutive Syndrome (BADS) (Wilson, Huh, Gallagher, & King, 1990). Additional
Alderman, Burgess, Emslie, & Evans, 1996) or measures of motor functioning, such as the
Frontal Systems Behavior Scale (FrSBE) (Grace Finger Tapping Test and Hand Dynamometer,
& Malloy, 2001), for evaluating patients with can also be added to the neuropsychological
epilepsy. test battery to determine the presence of consis-
tent lateralized performance decrements,
Motor and Sensory Functions although these tests have been shown to be less
It is important to include a comprehensive assess- sensitive to detecting subtle motor deficits in
ment of handedness when assessing motor func- this population (Klove & Matthews, 1966).
tions in patients with epilepsy. An association Neuropsychologists have paid much less
between hand preference patterns and underlying attention to a formal assessment of somatosen-
patterns of brain organization has been demon- sory functions in patients with epilepsy with only
strated in numerous studies (Sveller et al., 2006). 16 % reporting that they evaluate these functions
The Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield, on a routine basis (Djordjevic & Jones-Gotman,
1971) is a ten-item instrument that provides an 2011). A formal evaluation of a wide range of
effective means of measuring hand preference somatosensory tasks, including manual extinc-
patterns on a continuum by computing a lateral- tion, graphesthesia, finger gnosis, and stereogno-
ity quotient range from +100 (totally right sis, can be accomplished through the use of the
handed) to −100 (totally left handed). Location Reitan-Klove Sensory Perceptual Examination
on this continuum has been demonstrated to cor- (Reitan & Wolfson, 1985). Evaluation of these
relate with degree of language lateralization functions can be helpful when the examiner
(Isaacs, Barr, Nelson, & Devinsky, 2006). wants to go beyond assessment of motor func-
Knowing whether a patient exhibits a pattern of tions for assessment of lateralized symptoms or
left or mixed-handedness can be extremely help- when there is a suspected focus in parietal regions
ful when interpreting unusual combinations of adjacent to the post-central gyrus.
verbal and nonverbal performance in patients
with epilepsy, as deficits in perceptual tasks sec- Language
ondary to the “crowding effects” are known to Neuropsychologists commonly begin their
exist in patients with right hemisphere language assessment of language functions in patients with
representation (Loring et al., 1999). epilepsy by examining the profile of performance
The Grooved Pegboard Test is included in on the Wechsler scales. Weaknesses in language
many neuropsychological test batteries as the skills can be identified through relative decre-
sole measure of motor functions, owing to its ments on the Verbal Comprehension Index (VCI)
demonstrated sensitivity to detecting general relative to other scales, which correspond in
effects of brain dysfunction. This test is cur- some cases to more widespread deficits in aca-
rently used by more than 60 % of neuropsy- demic skills such as reading and spelling. Further
chologists practicing in specialized epilepsy screening of those academic skills can be accom-
centers (Djordjevic & Jones-Gotman, 2011). plished quickly with a screening measure such as
Based on published research studies, patients the Wide Range Achievement Test (WRAT)
with epilepsy exhibit relative decrements on (Wilkinson & Robertson, 2006) or in a more
this test in comparison to healthy controls comprehensive fashion with the Wechsler
(Kupke & Lewis, 1985). Greater levels of Individual Achievement Test (WIAT-III) (NCS
impairment are generally seen in patients with Pearson, 2009).
1 Neuropsychological Assessment of Patients with Epilepsy 21

For analysis of more specific language (PRI) from the WAIS can be helpful in identifying
functions, neuropsychologists will often turn weaknesses in perceptual functions that are poten-
their attention to scores from the subtests com- tially the result of a disruption of right hemisphere
prising the VCI score, most notably Vocabulary. functions. This can be followed up with further
In terms of adding specific language tests to the analysis of performance on any one of a number of
battery, most will include the Boston Naming other more specific tests.
Test (Kaplan, Goodglass, & Weintraub, 1983), Based on survey results (Djordjevic & Jones-
which has demonstrated to be a sensitive indica- Gotman, 2011), more than 20 % of neuropsychol-
tor of language dysfunction in patients with left ogists in epilepsy centers commonly use the tests
temporal lobe seizures (B. P. Hermann et al., of Judgment of Line Orientation (JLO) and Facial
1999; Loring et al., 2008). There has also been Recognition Test (FRT) developed by Benton and
recent interest in assessment of naming in rela- colleagues for more detailed evaluation of right
tion to auditory descriptors, which is reported to hemisphere functions in patients with epilepsy
be a more sensitive indicator than visual naming (Benton, Hamsher, Varney, & Spreen, 1983). Both
as a result of its reliance on a more widespread tests, used in combination, can be helpful in iden-
network of representation in left hemisphere lan- tifying lesions affecting either the dorsal (where)
guage zones (Hamberger et al., 2003). or ventral (what) perceptual streams described in
Additional information about language flu- the posterior cortex in the 1980s (Ungerleider &
ency and semantic retrieval can be identified Mishkin, 1982). While studies have shown that
through analysis of performances on verbal flu- patients with temporal lobe epilepsy do not typi-
ency tests, such as the COWAT and the Animal cally exhibit impairments on the FRT prior to sur-
Naming Test. To complete the most thorough gery, they do exhibit declines on that test following
evaluation of language, one would add tests of surgery, consistent with the effects of a disruption
sentence repetition and comprehension. The of the ventral stream (B. P. Hermann, Seidenberg,
Token Test is commonly used for the latter. Wyler, & Haltiner, 1993). Low scores on the JLO,
Administration of complete language batteries theoretically affecting the dorsal stream, have
such as the Multilingual Aphasia Examination been demonstrated in patients with epilepsy local-
(MAE) (Benton & Hamsher, 1978) or the Boston ized in more posterior brain regions (Guerrini
Diagnostic Aphasia Examination (BDAE) et al., 1997).
(Goodglass & Kaplan, 1983) is generally reserved While measures of visual construction are a
for those patients showing frank signs of aphasia standard component of most neuropsychological
during the interview or on simple mental status test batteries, interpretation of performance dec-
testing. rements can be problematic, owing to the multi-
factorial nature of these tasks. Determining the
Visual Perceptual and Spatial Skills core deficit on these tasks will often require anal-
Neuropsychologists exhibit a natural interest in ysis of scores from measures of executive func-
evaluating a broad category of visual, spatial, and tions and motor skills to determine the degree to
constructive skills that are typically given one of a which the decrement is associated with efferent
number of hybrid names, such as visuospatial or factors as opposed to purely perceptual factors.
visuoconstructive functions. In spite of years of Survey results indicate that the majority of neuro-
study, our understanding of these skills and their psychologists working in epilepsy centers include
relationship to underlying brain function in epi- the WAIS Block Design subtest (79 %) and copy
lepsy and most other neurological conditions is of a complex figure (68 %) in their test batteries
not well developed. Most neuropsychologists (Djordjevic & Jones-Gotman, 2011). It is impor-
working in epilepsy centers begin their assessment tant to remember that much about the mecha-
of these skills with use of the Wechsler subtests nisms underlying dysfunction on these tests can
(Wechsler, 2008). Identification of differentially be identified through a careful analysis of the
low scores on the Perceptual Reasoning Index process by which the patient proceeds with his or
22 W.B. Barr

her construction of the figure or design (Kaplan, provide the clinician more detailed information
1988; Swenson, Bettcher, Barr, Campbell-Marsh, about the underlying processes involved in learn-
& Libon, 2013). Patients with disturbances of the ing and recall, results of empirical studies indi-
left hemisphere will commonly make errors with cate that the RAVLT is actually more sensitive to
perceptual details while those with right hemi- the effects of left hemisphere dysfunction (Loring
sphere disturbance are prone to making more et al., 2008). The remaining centers appear to use
globally based errors, affecting the larger contour a combination of other tasks, including selective
of the figure (Barr, 2003). reminding measures, or lists of words meeting
specific language and/or normative needs for the
Learning and Memory population served at that center.
Memory is a primary area of concern for most Neuropsychologists continue to find the
patients with epilepsy, with over 70 % reporting Paired Associates and Logical Memory subtests
subjective concerns about their memory func- from the WMS helpful for evaluation of memory
tions at any given time (Thompson & Corcoran, skills in patients with epilepsy, with evidence that
1992). Examination of memory skills thus these measures remain in use in nearly 50 % of
becomes one of the major purposes of the neuro- the surveyed centers (Djordjevic & Jones-
psychological evaluation and a thorough assess- Gotman, 2011). Many argue that inclusion of
ment of these skills requires observation of a paired associate learning tasks provides a useful
patient’s performance on a range of different adjunct to the information obtained from list-
types of memory tasks. To accomplish this, most learning tests with suggestions that these tests
neuropsychological batteries will include tests of might even be more sensitive to the effects of hip-
rote learning, using lists of individual words or pocampal dysfunction than other tests (Rausch &
word pairs to study learning skills and retention. Crandall, 1982). The Logical Memory subtest, in
Many will also use story recall tasks for evalua- turn, is alleged to provide a more ecologically
tion of larger units of learning and retention. valid measure than the rote memory tasks. There
Inclusion of tests of nonverbal memory, using is also evidence that a decline in performance on
learning and recall of figures and designs, is still delayed memory versus immediate memory trials
common, although questions about the validity of provides a valid and sensitive measure reflecting
that practice have been raised over the years due consolidation impairment, particularly in patients
to a lack of empirical data supporting the use of with left temporal lobe epilepsy (Delaney, Rosen,
these tests. Mattson, & Novelly, 1980).
Most neuropsychologists will agree that ver- Inclusion of tests of “nonverbal” memory
bal list-learning tasks provide extremely valuable remains common in most neuropsychological test
information on the degree and type of memory batteries, based on Milner’s (1967) early descrip-
impairment observed in patients with epilepsy. tion of dual “material-based” memory systems
Useful information about the nature of encoding involving the left and right hemispheres. While
can be determined through analysis of the initial empirical support for the relationship between
learning trials while details about retention and verbal memory impairment and dysfunction in
retrieval can be discerned through analysis of the left temporal lobe has been strong, there is
delayed recall and recognition trials. There is, little support for the existence of specific deficits
however, a lack of consensus on which test to in nonverbal memory in patients with right tem-
use. Survey results (Djordjevic & Jones-Gotman, poral lobe impairment (Barr et al., 1997;
2011) indicate that the majority of clinicians Kneebone, Lee, Wade, & Loring, 2007). Based on
working in epilepsy centers use either the Rey these findings, the recommendation from an NIH
Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT—40 %) panel was to omit nonverbal memory tests from
(Schmidt, 1996) or the California Verbal Learning inclusion of the Common Data Elements test bat-
Test (CVLT—31 %) (Delis, Kramer, Kaplan, & tery for evaluation of patients with epilepsy (see
Ober, 2000). While the CVLT is alleged to Table 1.7) (Loring, Lowenstein, et al., 2011).
1 Neuropsychological Assessment of Patients with Epilepsy 23

Survey results indicate that, in spite of the In a continuing search for a valid test of
empirical findings, neuropsychologists continue nonverbal memory, there have been some posi-
to use measures of figural reproduction in their tive findings from use of facial memory tests,
test batteries, nonetheless, with most including although the results from these studies, too, have
the Rey Complex Figure Test (RCFT—65 %) and been mixed. Findings from studies using the
a lesser amount using the Visual Reproduction Facial Recognition subtest of the Recognition
subtest (21 %) from the WMS (Djordjevic & Memory Test (Warrington, 1984) have been posi-
Jones-Gotman, 2011). The results of a large-scale tive in studies performed in the United Kingdom
study on 757 patients demonstrated that neither (Baxendale, 1997), but these findings have not
of these tests is useful for lateralizing the hemi- been replicated in North America (B. P. Hermann,
spheric location of memory dysfunction in tem- Connell, Barr, & Wyler, 1995; Kneebone,
poral lobe epilepsy patients before surgery (Barr Chelune, & Luders, 1997). Results from studies
et al., 1997). However, there are suggestions from using the Facial Recognition subtest from the
other studies indicating that useful information WMS have also been mixed (Wilde et al., 2001),
can be obtained from the RCFT when more qual- while there have been more positive results using
itatively based scoring systems are used (Barr, other tests (Barr, 1997). Further work in develop-
2003). The challenge becomes whether there is ing valid tests for assessment of nonverbal mem-
value added by going through the rigorous pro- ory is clearly needed.
cess of using one of the many qualitative systems
developed for scoring these figures. Many have Performance Validity Testing
now turned to a basic subjective phenomenal There has been an increasing trend in the field of
analysis of the figure, determining whether it neuropsychology to include tests of validity,
shows qualitative signs, such as fragmentation or effort, and response bias when administering a
spatial errors, associated with right hemisphere clinical test battery. A distinction is now made
dysfunction. between performance validity tests (PVTs),
Some neuropsychologists have reported suc- which are measures designed to evaluate the
cess using figural learning tests, with procedures validity of cognitive test performance, and symp-
that are analogous to the verbal list-learning para- tom validity tests (SVTs), which are scales used
digms, using groups of geometric designs pre- to evaluate the validity and response biases in
sented over multiple trials instead of words relation to symptom reporting (Larrabee, 2012).
(Helmstaedter, Pohl, Hufnagel, & Elger, 1991; In terms of PVTs, neuropsychologists distinguish
Jones-Gotman et al., 1997). Patients with right between the use of free-standing measures such
temporal lobe seizures have been observed to as the Test of Memory Malingering (TOMM)
perform poorly across the initial learning trials (Tombaugh, 1996) or Victoria Symptom Validity
but do not typically show impairment on the Test (VSVT) (Slick et al., 1997) and embedded
delayed recall trial (Jones-Gotman et al., 1997). measures obtained from tests that are included
These findings suggest that individuals with right routinely in the test battery, such as the Reliable
temporal lobe dysfunction might exhibit a quali- Digit Span (RDS) (Greiffenstein, Baker, & Gola,
tatively different type of impairment in learning 1994) or recognition trials from the RAVLT and
and memory than what is seen in patients with CVLT (Lu, Rogers, & Boone, 2007).
left temporal lobe dysfunction. The challenge has While inclusion of PVTs and SVTs is now
been that most of the published data on figural considered routine in forensic applications where
list-learning tests are based on measures that are rates of malingering are estimated in the range of
not widely available to other clinicians and inves- 40 %, the question is whether these tests need to
tigators. Attempts to replicate these findings with be used routinely in clinical settings, where the
commercially available tests, such as the Brief estimated rate of invalid performance is less than
Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised (BVMT-R), 10 % (Mittenberg, Patton, Canyock, & Condit,
have been unsuccessful (Barr, Morrison, Zaroff, 2002). Based on survey results, PVTs are used by
& Devinsky, 2004). less than 7 % of neuropsychologists working in
24 W.B. Barr

epilepsy centers in spite of the fact that there are the presence of malingering, but may identify
studies demonstrating that rates of invalid test other clinical factors, such as the effects of sei-
performance exceeding 20 % on single tests zures, that would signal the presence of poten-
(Djordjevic & Jones-Gotman, 2011). However, tially invalid test findings.
the rates of failure on multiple tests are shown to
be much lower, with failures on two or more tests Mood and Personality
seen in less than 5 % (Cragar, Berry, Fakhoury, It is well known that increased rates of psychiat-
Cibula, & Schmitt, 2006). ric disturbance are observed in patients with epi-
The highest rates of test failure in epilepsy lepsy as compared to the general population.
samples are seen in studies using the VSVT Screening for disturbances in mood and person-
where failure rates exceeding 20 % have been ality is thus a key component to neuropsycho-
reported (Cragar et al., 2006; Loring, Lee, & logical evaluations performed in this population.
Meador, 2005). It has been suggested that this While most well-trained neuropsychologists
test might yield a high false positive rate in this learn to identify the presence of many psychiatric
population due to its dependence on IQ and conditions through interview techniques and
working memory skills, which are known to be observational methods, it is often useful to
reduced in epilepsy samples relative to the gen- include self-report as part of the neuropsycho-
eral population (Keary et al., 2013). Similarly, logical test battery to obtain more objective data
false positives were also identified in another to support those conclusions.
study using the Word Memory Test (Drane et al., Nearly a third of patients with epilepsy will
2006; Green, 2003), suggesting that PVTs in this experience a depressive episode in their lifetime,
population are not limited to identification of making this the most common psychiatric comor-
potential malingering, but can be useful for iden- bidity encountered in patients with epilepsy
tifying invalid test performance secondary to low (Kanner, 2013). In the majority of these cases, the
intelligence, amnesia, or increased seizures patients will also exhibit clinically relevant fea-
(Drane et al., 2006). Failure rates on the Test of tures of anxiety. Survey results indicate that
Memory Malingering (TOMM) are reported to approximately 50 % of neuropsychologists work-
be less than 3 % in patients with epilepsy (Cragar ing in epilepsy centers use a self-report symptom
et al., 2006). inventory to screen for underlying depressive and
Results from other studies have suggested that anxiety disorders (Djordjevic & Jones-Gotman,
increased rates of PVT failure are seen in patients 2011). The majority of these centers report use of
with diagnoses of PNES, with failure rates in the the Beck Depression Inventory (Beck, Steer, &
range of 30–50 % (Drane et al., 2006). Again the Brown, 1996) and Beck Anxiety Inventory (Beck,
increased rates observed in these samples might 1993). There are indications that depression and
not necessarily be the result of outright malinger- anxiety might be manifested in qualitatively dif-
ing, but rather a result of somatization and other ferent manner in patients with epilepsy, leading
psychological processes related to the expression some investigators to develop specialized mea-
of seizures in that group (D. J. Williamson, sures for assessment of mood disorder in this
Holsman, Chaytor, Miller, & Drane, 2012). population (Kanner et al., 2012). The Neurological
Taken together, the suggestion is to include Disorders Depression Inventory in Epilepsy
routine administration of at least one free- (NDDI-E) is one such measure, consisting of six
standing PVT (e.g., TOMM or WMT) in the neu- items designed to provide a more sensitive assess-
ropsychological test battery in addition to ment of depressive symptoms in epilepsy by
utilizing data from one or more of the embedded avoiding overlap with symptoms that could poten-
measures (e.g., RDS or CVLT-II). However, it is tially be the result of medication side effects
important for clinicians to remember that failures (Kessler et al., 1996).
on these tests seen in epilepsy patients evaluated More comprehensive evaluations of mood and
in a clinical setting will not necessarily indicate personality are performed by clinicians at nearly
1 Neuropsychological Assessment of Patients with Epilepsy 25

one-third of epilepsy centers, with most includ- Psychosocial Seizure Inventory (WPSI), which
ing either the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality was a 132-item questionnaire designed to iden-
Inventory (MMPI) (Butcher, Dahlstrom, Graham, tify various difficulties in emotional, interper-
Tellegen, & Kaemmer, 1990) or the Personality sonal, and vocational adjustment, in addition to
Assessment Inventory (PAI) (Morey, 1991) as other factors including family background, finan-
part of their routine test battery. One of the major cial status, adjustment to seizures, and medical
advantages to using these more comprehensive management. The WPSI never gained wide
measures is that they naturally include a number acceptance or usage in spite of a number of
of validity scales that enable them to serve as papers demonstrating its empirical utility. The
SVTs by helping identify patients that might be Bear-Fedio Personality Inventory (Bear & Fedio,
overreporting or underreporting symptoms. 1977) was another inventory developed to mea-
These measures also go beyond assessment of sure the temporal lobe personality that had been
depression and anxiety, providing a means for the described by Geschwind and others (Waxman &
neuropsychologist to potentially identify other Geschwind, 1975). However, that scale never
psychiatric conditions, particularly those involv- gained wide acceptance due to its lack of empiri-
ing the presence of underlying thought disorder. cal validity and inability to distinguish personal-
Many clinicians are moving towards the ity factors in patients with epilepsy from those
use of the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality observed in other clinical samples (Devinsky &
Inventory-2, Restructured Format (MMPI-2-RF), Najjar, 1999).
which is a shortened and newly validated version During the 1990s attention turned from assess-
of the older instrument (Ben-Porath & Tellegan, ment from the development of measures to assess
2008). This version of the test includes a number quality of life (QOL) in patients with epilepsy.
of the newer validity scales, such as the Symptom The ultimate goal was to provide a means of eval-
Validity Scale (FBS-r) and Response Bias Scale uating treatment effects by moving beyond
(RBS) that have been demonstrated to be useful assessment of frequency into more meaningful
in identifying invalid test profiles in epilepsy aspects of an individual’s life and experience.
patients as well as in other clinical samples The result was the development of a number of
(Nelson, Hoelzle, Sweet, Arbisi, & Demakis, Quality of Life in Epilepsy Instruments (QOLIE)
2010; Wygant et al., 2010). This test also pro- (Devinsky et al., 1995). These instruments
vides a means for identifying underlying psy- include a number of epilepsy-specific items such
chotic or behavioral disorders through analysis of as seizure severity, fear of having a seizure, the
newly developed superordinate scales, including associated loss of control over one’s life, cogni-
the Thought Dysfunction (THD) and Behavioral/ tive and behavioral dysfunction, social limita-
Externalizing Dysfunction (BXD) scales. It is tions and stigma, sexual functioning, driving
important to remember, however, that certain restrictions, and medication side effects. The
interpretive strategies need to be employed QOLIE-89 is the longest and most commonly
when interpreting this and all other versions of used of these instrument. The QOLIE-31 and
the MMPI as some of the symptoms associated QOLIE-10 are both shorter instruments derived
with seizures might serve to inflate scales asso- from the larger form that are useful for screening
ciated with somatic (RC1) or bizarre (RC8) QOL in conjunction with other instruments.
symptoms. The test has also been shown to be Survey results indicate that these instruments are
very helpful in identifying patients with PNES used routinely in approximately 30 % of epilepsy
(Locke et al., 2010). centers (Djordjevic & Jones-Gotman, 2011).
There have been prior attempts to develop
“epilepsy-specific” measures to screen for psy- Interpreting and Reporting Test Results
chopathology in this population. In the 1980s, The major task following completion of the neu-
Dodrill and colleagues (Dodrill, Batzel, Queisser, ropsychological test battery is to score the test
& Temkin, 1980) developed the Washington protocols, interpret them, and communicate the
26 W.B. Barr

results to the patient, referral source, and other chapter. They should also be aware of the set of
health care professionals. Scoring the test proto- empirically derived “cognitive phenotypes” iden-
cols is a rather straightforward process, with tified by Hermann and Seidenberg (B. Hermann,
detailed criteria typically specified in test manu- Seidenberg, Lee, Chan, & Rutecki, 2007) through
als. Most manuals also contain normative infor- cluster analytic techniques. These investigators
mation that will suffice for interpretation of the found statistical support for three groups using
test data in most cases. However, there are some cluster analytic techniques, based on distinct neu-
instances when the neuropsychologist might ropsychological test, clinical, and structural
want to obtain more detailed normative informa- imaging data. The first was a group with minimal
tion relative to a demographically matched sam- cognitive impairment. The second included a
ple. In those cases, he or she is encouraged to group with specific impairment in the domain of
utilize the Comprehensive Norms for an memory and the third involved a group with com-
Expanded Halstead-Reitan Battery developed by bined impairment in memory, executive func-
Heaton and colleagues (2004) and supplemental tions, and processing speed. Knowledge of these
material for WAIS-IV interpretation (Brooks, empirically validated profiles can be extremely
Holdnack, & Iverson, 2011). Use of those norms useful in guiding the interpretive process when
will also facilitate profile interpretation by plac- integrating test data from an individual patient.
ing test scores on a common metric with co- The clinician’s knowledge and training in brain-
normed instruments. Neuropsychologists are behavior relationships will influence their ability
known to vary across patients and tests in the cri- to identify relevant profiles not meeting any of
teria they set to identify “impairment,” with defi- these typical patterns, which in some cases may
nitions of abnormally low scores often ranging reflect the effects of dysfunction in extra-temporal
from −1 SD to −2 SDs below the normative brain regions.
mean. There is a movement in the field of neuro- Results of the neuropsychological examina-
psychology to make clinicians aware of the pos- tion are typically summarized in a written report.
sibility of identifying impairment by “chance” The scope and length of the report will vary
when numerous test scores are analyzed. The widely across settings. There is a movement, in
reader is encouraged to consult papers providing general, for neuropsychologists to reduce the
tables and interpretive guidelines that will enable length of their reports, which might vary from a
them to reduce the probability of making a Type one-page length to over 30 pages across centers
I error regarding identification of cognitive (Donders, 2001a, 2001b). There is no set stan-
impairment when analyzing test profiles (Binder dard or limit with regard to page length. The ulti-
et al., 2009; Schretlen et al., 2008). mate length of a report will depend on the degree
Moving on to profile interpretation, this chap- to which the clinician summarizes the medical
ter has provided some background on the tests and neurological history. In many epilepsy set-
and methods used to evaluate a range of different tings, only minor details about seizures and their
cognitive domains. As mentioned, impairments onset are required, as this information is com-
in attention, executive functions, and memory monly summarized in great detail in neurological
will be observed most often in patients with epi- notes and other sources. In most cases, the neuro-
lepsy, owing to the combined effects of neural psychologist might want to focus on providing
systems involved in propagation of seizures, more detail on psychosocial factors, including
medication, and other factors. Individuals work- the patient’s education, occupation, and home
ing in epilepsy settings should be prepared to life, which are often not specified in great detail
identify neuropsychological profiles in patients in other medical records. In some settings, the
with idiopathic generalized (Shehata & Bateh neuropsychological report might also be the only
Ael, 2009), mesial-temporal (B. P. Hermann place where details about the patient’s psychiat-
et al., 1997), and lateral temporal (Pacia et al., ric and emotional state are provided. Details
1996) lobe syndromes, as outlined earlier in the regarding other aspects of the patient’s medical
1 Neuropsychological Assessment of Patients with Epilepsy 27

history will be outlined to the degree that is how these results relate to the presence of
necessary for interpretation of the test findings. seizures, medication, and underlying brain func-
For example, reports for patients with a history of tions and/or to other relevant medical or
traumatic brain injury will require some detail psychiatric factors. In some settings, a written
regarding the nature and functional outcome of description of these factors will suffice. In other
that injury. In contrast, reporting of other medical settings, a formal diagnosis will be required. In
factors, such as those placing the patient at risks terms of the DSM-V (APA, 2013), clinicians
for the metabolic syndrome (e.g., hypertension, evaluating patients will be commonly using the
diabetes, and hypercholesterolemia), only need diagnosis Mild Neurocognitive Dysfunction due
be provided in list form. to Another Medical Condition (331.83; Epilepsy,
There are also wide variations in the degree to 345.90) to characterize the diagnosis reached
which neuropsychologists report the neuropsy- through administration of the neuropsychologi-
chological test findings. Some provide a simple cal test battery.
paragraph-length summary of the entire protocol, The neuropsychological test report will gener-
while others provide detailed summaries of vari- ally conclude with recommendations, which will
ous cognitive domains assessed through the test again vary widely by setting. In some settings,
battery, with specific information about perfor- recommendations from the neuropsychologist
mance on each test (Donders, 2001a). Again, the regarding further medical work-up, including
degree to which the clinician provides this detail EEG and MRI, can be helpful, although these
will vary across setting. However, it is known that same recommendations will often not be appro-
many physicians prefer to hear the neuropsychol- priate in a specialized epilepsy center, where it
ogist’s bottom-line account of the test findings can be assumed that the referring epileptologist
and often ignore many of the details provided in has addressed these issues in a competent man-
the test results section. There are variations in the ner. One needs to keep in mind what recommen-
degree to which neuropsychologists report spe- dations can be drawn from the neuropsychological
cific test scores. Some reports will include evaluation that will be unique to the care of the
selected test scores embedded in the body of the patient in comparison to those recommendations
report, while others will include summary tables originating from more medically oriented evalua-
of scores from specific tests (e.g., Wechsler tions. In some settings, the neuropsychologist
scales) or the entire test battery. Another approach might be the primary clinician who is identifying
is to omit test scores from the report entirely, the need for further psychiatric and/or psycho-
while making them available in summary sheet logical care through initiation of psychopharma-
form in the event that another neuropsychologist cological and/or psychotherapeutic interventions.
needs them for interpretation of change on a sub- The neuropsychologist is most often the primary
sequent report. clinician utilized for identification and docu-
All neuropsychological test reports should mentation of a patient’s need for accommoda-
conclude with a concise summary section, con- tions in school or the workplace secondary to the
taining a brief review of the findings, an outline effects of cognitive dysfunction. Results from
of the final interpretation, and the final conclu- the testing will also be used to determine the
sions. This is the most important section of the patient’s need for any form of cognitive remedia-
report, which reflects the ultimate work product, tion. Further information regarding psychologi-
where the clinician has provided a final integra- cal treatments for patients with epilepsy can be
tion of results from the interview, record review, found in Chap. 10. Finally, the neuropsychologi-
and test findings. The summary section should cal recommendations should address if or when
include specific information to directly address the patient will need to return for follow-up test-
the referral question. This will commonly include ing to monitor or assess any changes over time
details about the presence or absence of cognitive secondary to a progression of disease or to the
decline, observed strengths and weaknesses, and effects of treatment.
28 W.B. Barr

Preparation of the written report will often be


followed by providing more specific feedback Case Example
directly to the patient. Many neuropsychologists
prefer to provide this feedback in a personalized This chapter will conclude with a case example
manner through a formally scheduled session highlighting many of the issues and procedures
with the patient and his or her family. While this described in this chapter. The patient is a 29-year-
is often done in person, there are certain circum- old right-handed woman with a history of com-
stances such as distance that will dictate perform- plex partial seizures dating back to childhood.
ing this session by telephone. In an effort to She was referred for a neuropsychological evalu-
become more “consumer friendly,” neuropsy- ation as part of a comprehensive outpatient evalu-
chologists have worked very hard on developing ation for assessment of reported difficulties with
and improving the feedback process and the memory and word finding. The examination was
patient’s experience of the entire testing process performed in the outpatient department of a large
(Postal & Armstrong, 2013). There has been a comprehensive epilepsy center located in an aca-
growing emphasis on using the feedback session demic medical center.
for therapeutic purposes, using some of the tech- According to the history that was obtained,
niques developed in a style of motivational inter- the patient’s first witnessed seizure occurred in
viewing (Gorske & Smith, 2009). The feedback adolescence when her father saw her experience
appointment should begin with an opportunity to an event while she was watching television.
address the patient’s perceptions of the testing However, the patient reported having similar pre-
process and their thoughts about their test perfor- vious events since early childhood (i.e., 6 or 7
mance. This can be followed by the clinician’s years of age), but assumed that they were a nor-
systematic review of the test findings and conclu- mal occurrence that everyone experienced. With
sions. A review of recommendations will often regard to current semiology, the patient typically
be provided in the context of further exploration has an aura (i.e., feeling of deja vu) followed by a
of the patient’s responses to the test results. brief period of altered consciousness during
The written report provides the most common which she is generally unable to understand or
means to communicate the assessment results to respond to others. Despite this, she can still often
the referral source. However, some clinicians will execute purposeful tasks (e.g., stop at a red light
prefer to receive an additional brief communica- when walking, continue running on a treadmill).
tion through telephone or some other HIPAA Seizures can also include oral automatisms and
compliant form of transmission where there is an repetitive right hand fist clenching, with events
opportunity for other questions to be addressed. generally lasting a few minutes.
It is important for the neuropsychologist to know The patient reported that lack of sleep, stress,
these preferences when cultivating their referral alcohol use, and missing medication dosages all
sources. In epilepsy specialty centers, the staff provoke events. The patient has experienced two
neuropsychologists are often asked to present the lifetime secondarily generalized seizures related
assessment results at a multidisciplinary treat- to these precipitating factors. Despite good medi-
ment meeting. In these settings, the clinician will cation compliance more recently, she has been
often need to keep in mind the unique contribu- experiencing several seizures occurring in a clus-
tions offered by the neuropsychological evalua- ter (i.e., over the course of a day or 2) approxi-
tion, and how this is often the only means of mately every 4–6 weeks. Her typical events are
communicating the factors that are unique to the characterized as complex partial seizures. She
patient as a person and how these will influence was taking lamotrigine at the time of the neuro-
the developing treatment plan. The challenge is psychological evaluation.
to reduce what is often very rich material into a Video-EEG performed within months of the
few brief sentences that will be absorbed effec- neuropsychological evaluation captured four sei-
tively by those working in other professions. zures, including “one complex partial seizure
1 Neuropsychological Assessment of Patients with Epilepsy 29

with secondary generalization arising from the a Polish-American background, and English is
left posterior quadrant, one complex partial sei- her first and only language. She currently lives in
zure arising from the left frontotemporal region,” a large northeast metropolitan region with her
and “two complex partial seizures arising from husband. The patient reported doing well in
the left hemisphere [which could not be local- school, experiencing no learning difficulties. She
ized].” An early MRI of the brain reportedly indi- obtained a Bachelor of Arts degree from a small
cated left mesial temporal sclerosis. However, northeastern university. She currently works as a
additional MRI studies performed subsequently product representative for a large pharmaceutical
were interpreted as normal. industry firm.
The patient reported some mild and chronic The neuropsychological test results are listed
memory difficulties. She described herself as a in Table 1.8. The test findings indicated that her
“forgetful” person in general, noting that her general level of intellectual functioning was in
family and friends would characterize her as the high average range. Higher-order verbal rea-
such, if asked. Specifically, she noted trouble soning abilities, visuospatial skills, attention, and
recalling smaller details of past events. In addi- most aspects of executive function were all areas
tion, she felt that her memory difficulties had of strength. Word finding deficits were clearly
worsened somewhat over time. The patient documented, in addition to relative weaknesses
reported word finding difficulties as well, noting in verbal memory, more specifically related to
that it takes her a few moments to recall peoples’ memory retention or retrieval. No mood-related
names and words more generally. difficulties were endorsed on self-report invento-
This patient had been informed by her neu- ries. However, some internalizing behaviors were
rologist that she might be a surgical candidate at evident on the MMPI-2-RF, with evidence of a
some time in the future, but she expressed a mild focus on somatic symptoms.
desire to delay any decisions for surgery while The test findings demonstrated that the patient
making a decision to start a family. is a bright woman with an average level of intel-
With regard to her birth and early develop- ligence and some specific weaknesses in verbal
mental history, no delivery complications or memory and word finding. The resulting test
delays in achievement of developmental mile- profile was rather consistent with the effects of
stones were reported. The patient experienced left temporal lobe epilepsy, which was compati-
febrile convulsions at 8 months related to an ill- ble with the reported findings from EEG and
ness (roseola), which included a generalized imaging studies. There was no evidence of any
tonic–clonic event lasting 30–45 min followed by underlying mood disorder, but there was evi-
a brief right hemiparesis. She reported that, at the dence of a mild focus on somatic symptoms,
time of the assessment, she was in good physical which could warrant a referral for psychothera-
health, with no significant medical problems. She peutic intervention. The patient did exhibit some
denied any prior history of concussion, head mild cognitive deficit and was provided with an
injury, surgical intervention, or substance abuse. opportunity to request appropriate accommoda-
The patient did not report a history of psycho- tions in the workplace, which she declined, indi-
logical difficulties and has never sought psycho- cating that she felt that she was holding her own
therapeutic treatment. There is no family history on the job. She was offered a referral to one of
of memory disorder or psychiatric problems. the university’s ongoing memory enhancement
Both parents are alive and in good health. The groups. She was informed that records from the
patient’s father is left-handed. She grew up in a neuropsychological evaluation would be kept on
large northeastern city where she lived with her file in the event that they are needed for future
parents and three sisters. She is Caucasian, from reference.
30 W.B. Barr

Table 1.8 Neuropsychological test scores


Demographics: 29 year old Caucasian female with 16 years of formal education. Complex partial seizures diagnosed
in adolescence. Experiences clusters of seizures every 4
TOPF, Estimated FSIQ =
WAIS-IV Standard Score (SS) Demographic T-Score
– Verbal Comprehension Index (VCI) 115 55
– Perceptual Reasoning Index (PRI) 113 55
– Working Memory Index (WMI) 111 53
– Processing Speed Index (PSI) 102 46
– General Abilities Index (GAI) 115 56
– Full Scale Intelligence Quotient (FSIQ) 113 53
WAIS-IV Digit Span, T-Score = 56 Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT)
– Forward, raw = 10, ss = 9 – Total Learning (5-Trials), raw = 51, T = 51
– Backward, raw = 11, ss = 13 – Delayed Recall, raw = 7/15, T = 34
– Sequenced, raw = 13, ss = 16 – Recognition (Hits/FP), Hits = 15, FP = 0
Trail making Test WMS-IV Logical Memory
– Trails A, raw = 16, T = 63 – LMI, raw = 22, T = 41
– Trails B, raw = 49, T = 46 – LMII, raw = 21, T = 43
Stroop Color Naming Test Rey Complex Figure
– Word, raw = 96, T = 44 – Copy Trial = 34.5, WNL
– Color, raw = 82, T = 53 – 30-min recall = 21, T = 43
– Color-Word, raw = 50, T = 55 WMS-IV Visual Reproduction
Ruff Figural Fluency Test (RFFT) – VRI, raw = 37, T = 43
– Unique Designs = 95, T = 51 – VRII, raw = 27, T = 44
– Perseverations = 5, T = 47 Test of Memory Malingering (TOMM)
Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT) – Trial 1, raw score = 47/50 correct
– Raw Score = 55, T = 55 – Trial 2, raw score = 50/50 correct
WCST-64
– Categories, raw = 4
– Errors, raw = 9, T = 58
– Perseverative Errors, raw = 7, T = 47
Boston Naming Test Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II)
– Raw Score = 51, T = 36 – raw score = 3
Animal Naming Test Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI)
Raw Score = 22, T = 41 – raw score = 5
MMPI-2-RF
– Validity Scales—L-r, F-r, and K-r, <60
• FBS, T = 57
• RBS, T = 59
– Clinical Profile
• EID, T = 74
• RC1, T = 72

diagnostic methods. Clinical neuropsychologists


Conclusions are now considered key members of the special-
ized treatment team. Those entering positions in
Epilepsy is a neurological disorder that is unique comprehensive epilepsy centers will need to have
as a result of its relatively high rate of cognitive background knowledge on the heterogeneity of
and behavioral disorders. It has been established seizures, various epilepsy syndromes, and the
over the years that neuropsychological testing is effects of treatment. Neuropsychological assess-
valuable to the care of patients with epilepsy, pro- ment strategies will focus on evaluation of atten-
viding information that is unique to what is tion, executive functions, and memory, which are
obtained using EEG, brain imaging, and other the skills most often affected in patients with
1 Neuropsychological Assessment of Patients with Epilepsy 31

epilepsy. The assessment will also extend to a dates. Journal of the International Neuropsychological
Society, 3, 435–443.
formal evaluation of mood and behavior with
Barr, W. B., & Goldberg, E. (2003). Pitfalls in the method
standardized methods. of double dissociation: Delineating the cognitive func-
The neuropsychologist’s primary aim is to tions of the hippocampus. Cortex, 39, 153–157.
establish how the deficits identified in cognition Barr, W. B., Morrison, C., Zaroff, C., & Devinsky, O.
(2004). Use of the Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-
and behavior relate to other clinical information
Revised (BVMT-R) in neuropsychological evaluation
relevant to the patient’s seizures and their under- of epilepsy surgery candidates. Epilepsy & Behavior,
lying cause. The goal of this opening chapter was 5(2), 175–179. doi:10.1016/j.yebeh.2003.12.010.
to provide the reader with a general overview of Baxendale, S. (1997). The role of the hippocampus in rec-
ognition memory. Neuropsychologia, 35, 591–598.
epilepsy and the important role of neuropsycho-
Baxendale, S., Thompson, P., Harkness, W., & Duncan,
logical assessment as it relates to this population. J. (2006). Predicting memory decline following epilepsy
The reader is now invited to obtain more specific surgery: A multivariate approach. Epilepsia, 47(11),
clinical information regarding the neuropsychol- 1887–1894. doi:10.1111/j.1528-1167.2006.00810.x.
Baxendale, S., Thompson, P. J., & Duncan, J. S. (2008).
ogy of epilepsy in the chapters following.
Improvements in memory function following anterior
temporal lobe resection for epilepsy. Neurology,
71(17), 1319–1325. doi:10.1212/01.
wnl.0000319699.04265.fd.
References Bear, D. M., & Fedio, P. (1977). Quantitative analysis of
interictal behavior in temporal lobe epilepsy. Archives
A. T. C. Committee, & Schalock, R. L. (2007). The of Neurology, 34, 454–467.
renaming of mental retardation: Understanding the Beck, A. T. (1993). Beck anxiety inventory (BAI). San
change to the term intellectual disability. Intellectual Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
and Developmental Disabilities, 45, 116–124. Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual
AACN. (1999). American Academy of Clinical for the Beck depression inventory—Second edition
Neuropsychology policy on the use of non-doctoral- (BDI-II). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
level personnel in conducting clinical neuropsycho- Ben-Porath, Y. S., & Tellegan, A. (2008). MMPI-2-RF:
logical evaluations. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, Manual for administration scoring and interpretation.
13, 385. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.
APA. (2002). Ethical principles of psychologists and code Benton, A. L., & Hamsher, K. S. (1978). Multilingual
of conduct. American Psychologist, 57, 1060–1073. aphasia examination manual. Iowa City: University of
APA. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental Iowa.
disorders—Fifth edition (DSM-V). Washington, DC: Benton, A. L., Hamsher, K. S., Varney, N. R., & Spreen,
American Psychiatric Association. O. (1983). Contributions to neuropsychological
APA Division 40. (1991). Recommendations for educa- assessment: A clinical manual. New York, NY: Oxford
tion and training of nondoctoral personnel in clinical University Press.
neuropsychology. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 5, Berg, A. T., Berkovic, S. F., Brodie, M. J., Buchhalter, J.,
20–23. Cross, J. H., van Emde Boas, W., … Scheffer, I. E.
Barona, A., Reynolds, C. R., & Chastain, R. (1984). A (2010). Revised terminology and concepts for organi-
demographically based index of premorbid intelli- zation of seizures and epilepsies: Report of the ILAE
gence for the WAIS-R. Journal of Consulting and Commission on Classification and Terminology,
Clinical Psychology, 52, 885–887. 2005–2009. Epilepsia, 51(4), 676–685.
Barr, W. B. (1997). Examining the right temporal lobe’s doi:10.1111/j.1528-1167.2010.02522.x.
role in nonverbal memory. Brain and Cognition, 35(1), Binder, L. M., Iverson, G. L., & Brooks, B. L. (2009). To
26–41. err is human: “Abnormal” neuropsychological scores
Barr, W. B. (2002). Neuropsychological testing for assess- and variability are common in healthy adults. Archives
ment of treatment effects: Methodologic issues. CNS of Clinical Neuropsychology, 24(1), 31–46.
Spectrums, 7(4), 300–302, 304–306. doi:10.1093/arclin/acn001.
Barr, W. B. (2003). Assessment of temporal lobe epilepsy Blair, J. R., & Spreen, O. (1989). Predicting premorbid
using the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test. In J. A. IQ: A revision of the National Adult Reading Test. The
Knight & E. F. Kaplan (Eds.), The handbook of Rey- Clinical Neuropsychologist, 3, 129–136.
Osterrieth Complex Figure usage: Clinical and Bowden, S. C., Petrauskas, V. M., Bardenhagen, F. J.,
research applications. Odessa, FL: Psychological Meade, C. E., & Simpson, L. C. (2013). Exploring the
Assessment Resources. dimensionality of digit span. Assessment, 20(2), 188–
Barr, W. B., Chelune, G. J., Hermann, B. P., Loring, 198. doi:10.1177/1073191112457016.
D. W., Perrine, K., Strauss, E., … Westerveld, M. Brooks, B. L., Holdnack, J. A., & Iverson, G. L. (2011).
(1997). The use of figural reproduction tests as mea- Advanced clinical interpretation of the WAIS-IV and
sures of nonverbal memory in epilepsy surgery candi- WMS-IV: Prevalence of low scores varies by level of
32 W.B. Barr

intelligence and years of education. Assessment, 18(2), emotional, and social function in patients with epi-
156–167. doi:10.1177/1073191110385316. lepsy. Epilepsia, 27(4), 399–411.
Brown, L., Sherbenou, R. J., & Johnson, S. K. (2010). Test Dodrill, C. B. (1992). Problems in the assessment of cog-
of nonverbal intelligence, Fourth edition (TONI-4). nitive effects of antiepileptic drugs. Epilepsia,
Austin, TX: PRO-ED. 33(Suppl. 6), S29–S32.
Butcher, J. N., Dahlstrom, W. G., Graham, J. R., Tellegen, Dodrill, C. B., Batzel, L. W., Queisser, H. R., & Temkin,
A. M., & Kaemmer, B. (1990). MMPI-2: Minnesota N. (1980). An objective method for the assessment of
multiphasic personality inventory—2. Manual for psychological and social problems among epileptics.
administration and scoring. Minneapolis, MN: Epilepsia, 21, 123–135.
University of Minnesota Press. Dodrill, C. B., & Wilensky, A. J. (1990). Intellectual
Chelune, G. J. (2010). Evidence-based research and prac- impairment as an outcome of status epilepticus.
tice in clinical neuropsychology. The Clinical Neurology, 40(5 Suppl. 2), 23–27.
Neuropsychologist, 24(3), 454–467. Donders, J. (2001a). A survey of report writing by neuro-
doi:10.1080/13854040802360574. psychologists, I: General characteristics and content.
Chelune, G. J., Naugle, R. I., Luders, H., & Awad, I. A. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 15(2), 137–149.
(1991). Prediction of cognitive change as a function of doi:10.1076/clin.15.2.137.1893.
preoperative ability status among temporal lobectomy Donders, J. (2001b). A survey of report writing by neuro-
patients seen at 6-month follow-up. Neurology, 41(3), psychologists, II: Test data, report format, and docu-
399–404. ment length. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 15(2),
Cragar, D. E., Berry, D. T., Fakhoury, T. A., Cibula, J. E., 150–161. doi:10.1076/clin.15.2.150.1902.
& Schmitt, F. A. (2006). Performance of patients with Drane, D. L., Williamson, D. J., Stroup, E. S., Holmes,
epilepsy or psychogenic non-epileptic seizures on four M. D., Jung, M., Koerner, E., … Miller, J. W. (2006).
measures of effort. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, Cognitive impairment is not equal in patients with epi-
20(3), 552–566. doi:10.1080/13854040590947380. leptic and psychogenic nonepileptic seizures.
Delaney, R. C., Rosen, A. J., Mattson, R. H., & Novelly, Epilepsia, 47(11), 1879–1886.
R. A. (1980). Memory function in focal epilepsy: A doi:10.1111/j.1528-1167.2006.00611.x.
comparison of non-surgical, unilateral temporal lobe Elger, C. E., Helmstaedter, C., & Kurthen, M. (2004).
and frontal lobe samples. Cortex, 16, 103–117. Chronic epilepsy and cognition. The Lancet. Neurology,
Delis, D. C., Kaplan, E., & Kramer, J. (2001). Delis- 3(11), 663–672. doi:10.1016/s1474-4422(04)00906-8.
Kaplan executive function system: Examiner’s man- Engel, J., & Pedley, T. A. (2008). Introduction: What is
ual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. epilepsy? In J. Engel (Ed.), Epilepsy: A comprehensive
Delis, D. C., Kramer, J. H., Kaplan, E., & Ober, B. A. textbook. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott, Williams, &
(2000). California verbal learning test (2nd ed.). San Wilkins.
Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. Fleck, D. E., Shear, P. K., & Strakowski, S. M. (2002). A
Devinsky, O., & Najjar, S. (1999). Evidence against the reevaluation of sustained attention performance in
existence of a temporal lobe epilepsy personality syn- temporal lobe epilepsy. Archives of Clinical
drome. Neurology, 53(5 Suppl. 2), S13–S25. Neuropsychology, 17, 399–405.
Devinsky, O., Vickrey, B. G., Cramer, J., Perrine, K., Golden, C. (1978). Stroop color and word test. Wood
Hermann, B., Meador, K., & Hays, R. D. (1995). Dale, IL: Stoelting Company.
Development of the quality of life in epilepsy inven- Goodglass, H., & Kaplan, E. (1983). The assessment of
tory. Epilepsia, 36(11), 1089–1104. aphasia and related disorders. Philadelphia, PA: Lee
Dikmen, S., Matthews, C. G., & Harley, J. P. (1975). The & Febiger.
effect of early versus late onset of major motor epi- Gorske, T. T., & Smith, S. R. (2009). Collaborative thera-
lepsy upon cognitive-intellectual performance. peutic neuropsychological assessment. New York,
Epilepsia, 16, 73–81. NY: Springer Science + Business Media, LLC.
Djordjevic, J., & Jones-Gotman, M. (2011). Inquiry on Grace, J., & Malloy, P. (2001). Frontal systems behavior
assessments across epilepsy centers in different coun- scale: Professional manual. Lutz, FL: Psychological
tries. In C. Helmstaedter, B. Hermann, M. Lassonde, Assessment Resources.
P. Kahane, & A. Arzimanoglou (Eds.), Green, P. (2003). Green’s word memory test for windows:
Neuropsychology in the care of people with epilepsy. User’s manual. Edmonton, AL: Green’s Publishing.
Montrouge, FR: John Libbey Eurotext. Greiffenstein, M. F., Baker, W. J., & Gola, T. (1994).
Dodrill, C. B. (1975). Diphenylhydantoin serum levels, Validation of malingered amnesia measures with a
toxicity, and neuropsychological performance in large clinical sample. Psychological Assessment, 6,
patients with epilepsy. Epilepsia, 16(4), 593–600. 218–224.
Dodrill, C. B. (1978). A neuropsychological battery for Guerrini, R., Dubeau, F., Dulac, O., Barkovich, A. J.,
epilepsy. Epilepsia, 19, 611–623. Kuzniecky, R., Fett, C., … Andermann, F. (1997).
Dodrill, C. B. (1981). Rapid evaluation of intelligence in Bilateral parasagittal parietooccipital polymigrogyria
adults with epilepsy. Epilepsia, 21, 359–367. and epilepsy. Annals of Neurology, 41, 65–73.
Dodrill, C. B. (1986). Correlates of generalized tonic- Hamberger, M. J., Seidel, W. T., Goodman, R. R., Perrine,
clonic seizures with intellectual, neuropsychological, K., & McKhann, G. M. (2003). Temporal lobe
1 Neuropsychological Assessment of Patients with Epilepsy 33

stimulation reveals anatomic distinction between audi- Hoppe, C., Fliessbach, K., Schlegel, U., Elger, C. E., &
tory naming processes. Neurology, 60(9), 1478–1483. Helmstaedter, C. (2009). NeuroCog FX: Computerized
Harrison, P. L., & Oakland, T. (2003). Adaptive behavior screening of cognitive functions in patients with epi-
assessment system—Second edition (ABAS-II). San lepsy. Epilepsy & Behavior, 16, 298–310.
Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. Isaacs, K. L., Barr, W. B., Nelson, P. K., & Devinsky, O.
Heaton, R. K., Chelune, G. J., Talley, J. L., Kay, G. G., & (2006). Degree of handedness and cerebral domi-
Curtiss, G. (1993). Wisconsin card sorting test man- nance. Neurology, 66(12), 1855–1858. doi:10.1212/01.
ual: Revised and expanded. Odessa, FL: Psychological wnl.0000219623.28769.74.
Assessment Resources, Inc. Jobst, B. C., Williamson, P. D., Thadani, V. M., Gilbert,
Heaton, R. K., Miller, S. W., Taylor, M. J., & Grant, I. K. L., Holmes, G. L., Morse, R. P., … Roberts, D. W.
(2004). Revised comprehensive norms for and (2010). Intractable occipital lobe epilepsy: clinical char-
expanded Halstead-Reitan battery: Demographically acteristics and surgical treatment. Epilepsia, 51(11),
adjusted neuropsychological norms for African 2334–2337. doi:10.1111/j.1528-1167.2010.02673.x.
American and Caucasian adults professional manual. Jones-Gotman, M., Zatorre, R. J., Olivier, A., Andermann,
Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. F., Cendes, F., Staunton, H., … Wieser, H. G. (1997).
Helmstaedter, C., Gleibner, U., Zentner, J., & Elger, C. E. Learning and retention of words and designs following
(1998). Neuropsychological consequences of epilepsy excision from medial or lateral temporal lobe struc-
surgery in frontal lobe epilepsy. Neuropsychologia, tures. Neuropsychologia, 35, 963–973.
36(4), 333–341. Kamphaus, R. W., & Reynolds, C. R. (2003). Reynolds
Helmstaedter, C., Pohl, C., Hufnagel, A., & Elger, C. E. intellectual screening test (RIST). Lutz, FL:
(1991). Visual learning deficits in nonresected patients Psychological Assessment Resources.
with right temporal lobe epilepsy. Cortex, 27, Kanner, A. M. (2013). The treatment of depressive disor-
547–555. ders in epilepsy: What all neurologists should know.
Hermann, B. P., Connell, B., Barr, W. B., & Wyler, A. R. Epilepsia, 54(Suppl. 1), 3–12. doi:10.1111/epi.12100.
(1995). The utility of the Warrington Recognition Kanner, A. M., Schachter, S. C., Barry, J. J., Hersdorffer,
Memory Test for temporal lobe epilepsy: Pre- and D. C., Mula, M., Trimble, M., … Gilliam, F. (2012).
postoperative results. Journal of Epilepsy, 8, Depression and epilepsy, pain and psychogenic non-
139–145. epileptic seizures: Clinical and therapeutic perspec-
Hermann, B. P., Perrine, K., Chelune, G. J., Barr, W., tives. Epilepsy & Behavior, 24(2), 169–181.
Loring, D. W., Strauss, E., .… Westerveld, M. (1999). doi:10.1016/j.yebeh.2012.01.008.
Visual confrontation naming following left anterior Kaplan, E. (1988). A process approach to neuropsycho-
temporal lobectomy: A comparison of surgical logical assessment. In T. Boll & B. K. Bryant (Eds.),
approaches. Neuropsychology, 13(1), 3–9. Clinical neuropsychology and brain function:
Hermann, B., Seidenberg, M., Lee, E. J., Chan, F., & Research, measurement, and practice (pp. 125–167).
Rutecki, P. (2007). Cognitive phenotypes in temporal Washington, DC: American Psychological
lobe epilepsy. Journal of the International Association.
Neuropsychological Society, 13(1), 12–20. Kaplan, E., Goodglass, H., & Weintraub, S. (1983). The
doi:10.1017/S135561770707004X. Boston naming test. Philadelphia, PA: Lea and Febiger.
Hermann, B. P., Seidenberg, M., Schoenfeld, J., Peterson, Kaufman, A. S., & Lichtenberger, E. O. (2005). Assessing
J., Leveroni, C., & Wyler, A. R. (1996). Empirical adolescent & adult intelligence (3rd ed.). Hoboken,
techniques for determining the reliability, magnitude, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
and pattern of neuropsychological change after epi- Keary, T. A., Frazier, T. W., Belzile, C. J., Chapin, J. S.,
lepsy surgery. Epilepsia, 37(10), 942–950. Naugle, R. I., Najm, I. M., & Busch, R. M. (2013).
Hermann, B. P., Seidenberg, M., Schoenfeld, J., & Davies, Working memory and intelligence are associated with
K. (1997). Neuropsychological characteristics of the Victoria symptom validity test hard item performance
syndrome of mesial temporal lobe epilepsy. Archives in patients with intractable epilepsy. Journal of the
of Neurology, 54, 369–376. International Neuropsychological Society, 19(3), 314–
Hermann, B. P., Seidenberg, M., Wyler, A., & Haltiner, A. 323. doi:10.1017/S1355617712001397.
(1993). Dissociation of object recognition and spatial Kessler, R., Nelson, C., McGonagle, K., Liu, J., Swartz,
localization abilities following temporal lobe lesions M., & Blazer, D. (1996). Comorbidity of DSM-III-R
in humans. Neuropsychology, 7, 343–350. major depressive disorder in the general population:
Hermann, B. P., & Whitman, S. (1984). Behavioral and Results from the U.S. National Comorbidity Survey.
personality correlates of epilepsy: A review, method- The British Journal of Psychiatry, 30, 17–30.
ological critique, and conceptual model. Psychological Klove, H., & Matthews, C. G. (1966). Psychometric and
Bulletin, 95, 451–497. adaptive abilities in epilepsy with differential etiology.
Hermann, B. P., Wyler, A. R., & Richey, E. T. (1988). Epilepsia, 7, 330–338.
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance in patients Kneebone, A. C., Chelune, G. J., & Luders, H. O. (1997).
with complex partial seizures of temporal-lobe origin. Individual patient prediction of seizure lateralization
Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, in temporal lobe epilepsy: A comparison between neu-
10(4), 467–476. doi:10.1080/01688638808408253. ropsychological memory measures and the Intracarotid
34 W.B. Barr

Amobarbital Procedure. Journal of the International Lowenstein, D. H. (2009). Epilepsy after head injury: An
Neuropsychological Society, 3(2), 159–168. overview. Epilepsia, 50(Suppl. 2), 4–9. doi:10.1111/
Kneebone, A. C., Lee, G. P., Wade, L. T., & Loring, D. W. j.1528-1167.2008.02004.x.
(2007). Rey Complex Figure: Figural and spatial Lu, P. H., Rogers, S. A., & Boone, K. B. (2007). Use of
memory before and after temporal lobectomy for standard memory tests to detect suspect effort. In
intractable epilepsy. Journal of the International K. B. Boone (Ed.), Assessment of Feigned cognitive
Neuropsychological Society, 13(4), 664–671. impairment: A neuropsychological perspective.
doi:10.1017/S1355617707070828. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
Kupke, T., & Lewis, R. (1985). WAIS and neuropsycho- Luria, A. R. (1966). Higher cortical functions in man.
logical tests: Common and unique variance within an New York, NY: Basic Books.
epileptic population. Journal of Clinical and McDonald, C. R., Delis, D. C., Norman, M. A., Tecoma,
Experimental Neuropsychology, 7(4), 353–366. E. S., & Iragui, V. J. (2005). Discriminating patients
doi:10.1080/01688638508401269. with frontal-lobe epilepsy and temporal-lobe epilepsy:
Kwan, P., & Brodie, M. J. (2001). Neuropsychological Utility of a multilevel design fluency test.
effects of epilepsy and antiepileptic drugs. The Lancet, Neuropsychology, 19(6), 806–813.
357(9251), 216–222. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.19.6.806.
Kwan, P., Schachter, S. C., & Brodie, M. J. (2011). Drug- McDonald, C. R., Delis, D. C., Norman, M. A., Wetter,
resistant epilepsy. The New England Journal of S. R., Tecoma, E. S., & Iragui, V. J. (2005). Response
Medicine, 365, 919–926. inhibition and set shifting in patients with frontal lobe
Larrabee, G. J. (2012). Performance validity and symp- epilepsy or temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy &
tom validity in neuropsychological assessment. Behavior, 7(3), 438–446. doi:10.1016/j.
Journal of the International Neuropsychological yebeh.2005.05.005.
Society, 18, 1–7. Meador, K. J., Loring, D. W., Huh, K., Gallagher, B. B., &
Locke, D. E., Kirlin, K. A., Thomas, M. L., Osborne, D., King, D. W. (1990). Comparative cognitive effects of
Hurst, D. F., Drazkowski, J. F., … Noe, K. H. (2010). anticonvulsants. Neurology, 40, 391–394.
The Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory-2- Milner, B. (Ed.). (1964). Some effects of frontal lobectomy
Restructured Form in the epilepsy monitoring unit. in man. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
Epilepsy & Behavior, 17(2), 252–258. doi:10.1016/j. Milner, B. (Ed.). (1967). Brain mechanisms suggested by
yebeh.2009.12.004. studies of temporal lobes. New York, NY: Grune &
Loring, D. W., Lee, G. P., & Meador, K. J. (2005). Victoria Stratton.
symptom validity test performance in non-litigating Milner, B. (1968). Disorders of memory after brain lesions
epilepsy surgery candidates. Journal of Clinical and in man. Neuropsychologia, 6, 175–179.
Experimental Neuropsychology, 27(5), 610–617. Mittenberg, W., Patton, C., Canyock, E. M., & Condit, D.
doi:10.1080/13803390490918471. (2002). Base rates of malingering and symptom exag-
Loring, D. W., Lowenstein, D. H., Barbaro, N. M., geration. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Fureman, B. E., Odenkirchen, J., Jacobs, M. P., … Neuropsychology, 24, 1094–1102.
Stout, A. (2011). Common data elements in epilepsy Mohs, R. C., Knopman, D., Petersen, R. C., Ferris, S. H.,
research: development and implementation of the Ernesto, C., Grundman, M., … The Alzheimer’s
NINDS epilepsy CDE project. Epilepsia, 52(6), Disease Cooperative Study. (1997). Development of
1186–1191. doi:10.1111/j.1528-1167.2011.03018.x. cognitive instruments for use in clinical trials of anti-
Loring, D. W., Marino, S., & Meador, K. J. (2007). dementia drugs: Additions to the Alzheimer’s Disease
Neuropsychological and behavioral effects of antiepi- Assessment Scale that broaden its scope. Alzheimer
lepsy drugs. Neuropsychology Review, 17(4), 413– Disease and Associated Disorders, 11(S2), S13–S21.
425. doi:10.1007/s11065-007-9043-9. Morey, L. C. (1991). Personality assessment inventory
Loring, D. W., Strauss, E., Hermann, B. P., Barr, W. B., professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological
Perrine, K., Trenerry, M. R., … Bowden, S. C. (2008). Assessment Resources.
Differential neuropsychological test sensitivity to left Morrison, C. E., & Nakhutina, L. (2007).
temporal lobe epilepsy. Journal of the International Neuropsychological features of lesion-related epi-
Neuropsychological Society, 14, 394–400. lepsy in adults: An overview. Neuropsychology
Loring, D. W., Strauss, E., Hermann, B. P., Perrine, K., Review, 17(4), 385–403. doi:10.1007/s11065-
Trenerry, M. R., Barr, W. B., … Meador, K. J. (1999). 007-9044-8.
Effects of anomalous language representation on neu- National Association of Epilepsy Centers (NAEC) (2010)
ropsychological performance in temporal lobe epi- Guidelines for Essential Services, Personnel, and Facilities
lepsy. Neurology, 53(2), 260–260. doi:10.1212/ in Specialized Epilepsy Centers. Minneapolis, MN.
wnl.53.2.260. NCS Pearson. (2009). Wechsler individual achievement
Loring, D. W., Williamson, D. J., Meador, K. J., Wiegand, test—Third edition (WIAT-III). San Antonio, TX: NCS
F., & Hulihan, J. (2011). Topiramate dose effects on Pearson.
cognition: A randomized double-blind study. Nelson, N. W., Hoelzle, J. B., Sweet, J. J., Arbisi,
Neurology, 76(2), 131–137. doi:10.1212/WNL. P. A., & Demakis, G. J. (2010). Updated meta-anal-
0b013e318206ca02. ysis of the MMPI-2 symptom validity scale (FBS):
1 Neuropsychological Assessment of Patients with Epilepsy 35

Verified utility in forensic practice. The Clinical Roid, G. H. (2003). Stanford-Binet intelligence scales,
Neuropsychologist, 24(4), 701–724. doi:10.1080/ Fifth edition (SB5). Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing.
13854040903482863. Rosvold, H. E., Mirsky, A. F., Sarason, I., Bransome,
Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of E. D., & Beck, L. H. (1956). A continuous perfor-
handedness: The Edinborough inventory. mance test of brain damage. Journal of Consulting
Neuropsychologia, 9, 97–113. Psychology, 20, 343–350.
Pacia, S. V., Devinsky, O., Perrine, K., Ravdin, L., Ruff, R. M. (1988). Ruff figural fluency test administra-
Luciano, D., Vazquez, B., & Doyle, W. (1996). tion manual. San Francisco, CA: Neuropsychological
Clinical features of neocortical temporal lobe epi- Resources.
lepsy. Annals of Neurology, 40, 724–730. Schmidt, M. (1996). Rey auditory and verbal learning
Palmini, A. I., Gloor, P., & Jones-Gotman, M. (1992). test: A handbook. Los Angeles, CA: Western
Pure amnestic seizures in temporal lobe epilepsy. Psychological Services.
Brain, 115, 749–769. Schretlen, D. J., Testa, S. M., Winicki, J. M., Pearlson,
Patrikelis, P., Angelakis, E., & Gatzonis, S. (2009). G. D., & Gordon, B. (2008). Frequency and bases of
Neurocognitive and behavioral functioning in frontal abnormal performance by healthy adults on neuropsy-
lobe epilepsy: A review. Epilepsy & Behavior, 14(1), chological testing. Journal of the International
19–26. doi:10.1016/j.yebeh.2008.09.013. Neuropsychological Society, 14, 436–445.
Pearson Assessment. (2009). Advanced clinical solutions Seidenberg, M., Pulsipher, D. T., & Hermann, B. (2007).
for the WAIS-IV/WMS-IV. San Antonio, TX: Pearson. Cognitive progression in epilepsy. Neuropsychology
Piazzini, A., Turner, K., Vignoli, A., Canger, R., & Review, 17(4), 445–454. doi:10.1007/
Canevini, M. P. (2008). Frontal cognitive dysfunction s11065-007-9042-x.
in juvenile myoclonic epilepsy. Epilepsia, 49(4), 657– Shehata, G. A., & Bateh Ael, A. (2009). Cognitive function,
662. doi:10.1111/j.1528-1167.2007.01482.x. mood, behavioral aspects, and personality traits of adult
Piekema, C., Fernandez, G., Postma, A., Hendriks, M. P., males with idiopathic epilepsy. Epilepsy & Behavior,
Wester, A. J., & Kessels, R. P. (2007). Spatial and non- 14(1), 121–124. doi:10.1016/j.yebeh.2008.08.014.
spatial contextual working memory in patients with Sherman, E. M., Wiebe, S., Fay-McClymont, T. B.,
diencephalic or hippocampal dysfunction. Brain Tellez-Zenteno, J., Metcalfe, A., Hernandez-
Research, 1172, 103–109. doi:10.1016/j. Ronquillo, L., … Jette, N. (2011). Neuropsychological
brainres.2007.07.066. outcomes after epilepsy surgery: systematic review
Postal, K., & Armstrong, K. (Eds.). (2013). Feedback that and pooled estimates. Epilepsia, 52(5), 857–869.
sticks: The art of communicating neuropsychological doi:10.1111/j.1528-1167.2011.03022.x.
assessment results. New York, NY: Oxford University Shipley, W. (1940). A self-administering scale for mea-
Press. suring intellectual impairment and deterioration.
Puente, A. E., Adams, R. L., Barr, W. B., Bush, S., & N. P. Journal of Psychology, 9, 371–377.
P. Committee. (2006). The use, education, training and Slick, D. J., Tan, J. E., Strauss, E., Mateer, C. A.,
supervision of neuropsychological test technicians Harnadek, M., & Sherman, E. (1997). Manual for the
(psychometrists) in clinical practice: Official state- Victoria symptom validity test. Odessa, FL:
ment of the National Academy of Neuropsychology. Psychological Assessment Resources.
Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 21, 837–839. Sparrow, S. S., Cicchetti, D. V., & Balla, D. A. (2006).
Rabin, L. A., Barr, W. B., & Burton, L. A. (2005). Vineland adaptive behavior scales—Second edition
Assessment practices of clinical neuropsychologists in (Vineland-II). San Antonio, TX: Pearson.
the United States and Canada: A survey of INS, NAN, Stern, R. A., & White, T. (2003). NAB administration,
and APA Division 40 members. Archives of Clinical scoring, and interpretation manual. Lutz, FL:
Neuropsychology, 20(1), 33–65. doi:10.1016/j. Psychological Assessment Resources.
acn.2004.02.005. Suchy, Y., Sands, K., & Chelune, G. J. (2003). Verbal and
Randolph, C., Tierney, M. C., Mohr, E., & Chase, T. N. nonverbal fluency performance before and after sei-
(1998). The repeatable battery for the assessment of zure surgery. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
neuropsychological status (RBANS): Preliminary Neuropsychology, 25(2), 190–200. doi:10.1076/
clinical validity. Journal of Clinical and Experimental jcen.25.2.190.13640.
Neuropsychology, 20, 310–319. Sveller, C., Briellmann, R. S., Saling, M. M., Lillywhite,
Rausch, R., & Crandall, P. H. (1982). Psychological status L., Abbott, D. F., Masterson, R. A. J., & Jackson, G. D.
related to surgical control of temporal lobe seizures. (2006). Relationship between language laterlization
Epilepsia, 23, 191–202. and handedness in left-hemisphere partial epilepsy.
Raven, J., Raven, J. C., & Court, J. H. (1998). Manual for Neurology, 67, 1813–1817.
Raven’s progressive matrices and vocabulary scales, Sweet, J. J., Meyer, D. G., Nelson, N. W., & Moberg, P. J.
Section 1. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment. (2011). The TCN/AACN 2010 “salary survey”:
Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (1985). The Halstead- Professional practices, beliefs, and incomes of U.S.
Reitan neuropsychological test battery. Theory and neuropsychologists. The Clinical Neuropsychologist,
clinical interpretation. New York, NY: Hemisphere. 25(1), 12–61. doi:10.1080/13854046.2010.544165.
36 W.B. Barr

Swenson, R., Bettcher, B. M., Barr, W. B., Campbell- epilepsy: Group differences and individual classifica-
Marsh, M., & Libon, D. J. (2013). Visuoconstructive tion. Journal of the International Neuropsychological
test performance and process approach application: Society, 7(7), 881–891.
Block design, object assembly, and the Rey-Osterrieth Wilkinson, G. S., & Robertson, G. J. (2006). Wide range
complex figure. In L. Ashendorf, R. Swenson, & D. J. achievement test—Fourth edition (WRAT-4). Lutz, FL:
Libon (Eds.), The Boston process approach to neuro- Psychological Assessment Resources.
psychological assessment: A practitioner’s guide. Williamson, P. D., Boon, P. A., Thadani, V. M., Darcey,
New York, NY: Oxford University Press. T. M., Spencer, D. D., Spencer, S. S., … Mattson,
Thompson, P. J., & Corcoran, R. (1992). Everyday mem- R. H. (1992). Parietal lobe epilepsy: Diagnostic con-
ory failures in people with epilepsy. Epilepsia, siderations and results of surgery. Annals of Neurology,
33(Suppl. 6), S18–S20. 31, 193–201.
Tombaugh, T. N. (1996). TOMM: Test of memory malin- Williamson, P. D., French, J., Thadani, V. M., Kim, J. H.,
gering. North Tonawanda, NY: Multi-Health Systems. Novelly, R. A., Spencer, S. S., … Mattson, R. H. (1993).
Trimble, M. R., & Thompson, P. J. (1983). Anticonvulsant Characteristics of medial temporal lobe epilepsy:
drugs, cognitive function, and behavior. Epilepsia, II. Interictal and ictal scalp electroencephalography, neu-
24(S1), S55–S63. ropsychological testing, neuroimaging, surgical results,
Ungerleider, L. G., & Mishkin, M. (1982). Two cortical and pathology. Annals of Neurology, 34, 781–787.
visual systems. In D. J. Ingle, M. A. Goodale, & R. J. Williamson, D. J., Holsman, M., Chaytor, N., Miller,
W. Mansfield (Eds.), Analysis of visual behavior. J. W., & Drane, D. L. (2012). Abuse, not financial
Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. incentive, predicts non-credible cognitive perfor-
Vanderploeg, R. D., & Schinka, J. A. (1995). Predicting mance in patients with psychogenic non-epileptic sei-
WAIS-R IQ premorbid ability: Combining subtest per- zures. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 26(4),
formance and demographic variable predictors. 588–598. doi:10.1080/13854046.2012.670266.
Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 10, 225–239. Wilson, B. A., Alderman, N., Burgess, P. W., Emslie, H.,
Warrington, E. K. (1984). Recognition memory test. & Evans, J. J. (1996). Behavioural assessment of the
Berkshire: NFER-Nelson. dysexecutive syndrome (BADS). London: Thames
Waxman, S. G., & Geschwind, N. (1975). The interictal Valley Test Company.
behavior syndrome of temporal lobe epilepsy. Archives Witt, J. A., Alpherts, W., & Helmstaedter, C. (2013).
of General Psychiatry, 32, 1580–1586. Computerized neuropsychological testing in epilepsy:
Wechsler, D. (2008). Wechsler adult intelligence scale— Overview of available tools. Seizure, 22(6), 416–423.
Fourth edition (WAIS-IV). San Antonio, TX: doi:10.1016/j.seizure.2013.04.004.
Pearson. Wonderlic, I. (2004). Wonderlic personnel test & scholas-
Wechsler, D. (2011). Wechsler abbreviated scale of intel- tic level exam users’s manual. Libertyville, IL:
ligence—Second edition (WASI-II). San Antonio, TX: Wonderlic.
Pearson. Woodcock, R. W., McGrew, K. S., & Mather, N. (2001).
Wiebe, S., Blume, W. T., Girvin, J. P., Eliasziw, M., & Woodcock-Johnson III (WJ3). Itasca, IL: Riverside
Effectiveness and Efficiency of Surgery for Temporal Publishing.
Lobe Epilepsy Study Group. (2001). A randomized, Wygant, D. B., Sellbom, M., Gervais, R. O., Ben-Porath,
controlled trial of surgery for temporal lobe epilepsy. Y. S., Stafford, K. P., Freeman, D. B., & Heilbronner,
The New England journal of medicine, 345, 311–318. R. L. (2010). Further validation of the MMPI-2 and
Wilde, N., Strauss, E., Chelune, G. J., Loring, D. W., MMPI-2-RF Response Bias Scale: Findings from dis-
Martin, R. C., Hermann, B. P., … Hunter, M. (2001). ability and criminal forensic settings. Psychological
WMS-III performance in patients with temporal lobe Assessment, 22(4), 745–756. doi: 10.1037/a0020042.
Evaluation of Children
and Adolescents with Epilepsy 2
William S. MacAllister and Elisabeth M.S. Sherman

placement, allocation of educational services in a


Introduction and Comments formal individualized educational plan), toward
on General Intellectual Function the goal of helping each child maximize aca-
demic and occupational potential.
Children with epilepsy have a range of cognitive In most neuropsychological assessments,
ability, from profound global impairment to evaluation of overall intellectual functioning
superior skills. Nevertheless, cognitive and forms the foundation of the test battery. The most
behavioral difficulties have a higher base rate in frequently employed measures of intelligence are
children with epilepsy than in healthy children, the Wechsler scales (i.e., Wechsler Primary and
and in some children, cognitive difficulties may Preschool Scale of Intelligence—Third Edition
predate first seizure (Fastenau et al., 2009). For [WPPSI-III]; Wechsler, 2002, Wechsler
this reason, neuropsychological assessment is Intelligence Scale for Children—Fourth Edition
helpful in identifying strengths and weaknesses [WISC-IV]; Wechsler, 2003, or the Wechsler
in children with epilepsy, particularly early on in Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence [WASI];
the condition (Fastenau et al., 2009). Appropriate Wechsler, 1999), and these scales have certainly
assessment and characterization of cognitive and seen the most use in the study of cognition in
behavioral strengths and weaknesses may prove pediatric epilepsy. Other commonly utilized
beneficial toward planning and tracking medical intelligence measures include the Stanford-
treatment (e.g., pharmacological, surgical, Binet—Fifth Edition (Roid, 2003), the Reynolds
rehabilitative) and for educational planning (e.g., Intellectual Assessment System (Reynolds &
Kamphaus, 2003), the Woodcock-Johnson
W.S. MacAllister, Ph.D., A.B.P.P.-C.N. (*)
Cognitive-III (Woodcock, McGrew, & Mather,
NYU Comprehensive Epilepsy Center, NYU School 2001), and the Kaufman Assessment Battery for
of Medicine, 223 East 34th Street, New York, NY Children-2 (Kaufman & Kaufman, 2002).
10016, USA Though assessment of overall intellectual
e-mail: william.macallister@nyumc.org
function is a crucial “first pass” toward
E.M.S. Sherman, Ph.D., RPsych understanding the child, it has long been
Copeman Healthcare Centre, Adjunct Associate
Professor, Faculty of Medicine, University of
recognized that school performance in children
Calgary, Suite 400, 628 12th Ave SW, Canada, with epilepsy is often lower than what would be
Copeman Healthcare Centre,
predicted based on global measures of intelligence
Calgary, AB, Canada, T2R 0H6 (i.e., IQ scores; Farwell, Dodrill, & Batzel, 1985).
e-mail: esherman@copemanhealthcare.com In fact, for many children with epilepsy, IQ is

W.B. Barr and C. Morrison (eds.), Handbook on the Neuropsychology of Epilepsy, 37


Clinical Handbooks in Neuropsychology, DOI 10.1007/978-0-387-92826-5_2,
© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015
38 W.S. MacAllister and E.M.S. Sherman

somewhat lower than that seen in the general partial-complex seizures), as well as multiple
population but still within the average range antiepileptic drugs (Bourgeois et al., 1983;
(Bourgeois, Prensky, Palkes, Talent, & Busch, Bulteau et al., 2000; Reynolds, 1983) have all
1983). This being said, intelligence in children been associated with lower intellectual
with epilepsy has varied across studies. Whereas functioning, but it should be noted that these
studies by Camfield suggest that children with cannot be disentangled from epilepsy severity.
epilepsy have IQs similar to the normal population For example, a child with frequent seizures is
(Camfield et al., 1984), others have more more likely to be placed on two or more
consistently shown that these individuals have antiepileptic medications than a child with
IQs that are significantly lower than age-matched infrequent seizures. Some research has shown
peers (O’Leary, Burns, & Borden, 2006; Singhi, that generalized tonic-clonic seizures and
Bansal, Singhi, & Pershad, 1992; Smith, Elliott, complex partial seizures are more likely to result
& Lach, 2002). The differences across studies in prominent cognitive dysfunction (Hoie et al.,
reflect factors such as the populations studied 2005), but subtler interictal, preictal, and postictal
(e.g., community-based samples versus tertiary effects may also affect cognition.
care patients). The age of the child at the onset of seizures is
Several neurological and epilepsy-specific also an important factor to consider, as this has
factors may be predictive of lowered IQ and been linked to level of intellectual function across
cognitive problems more generally in children several studies. For example, in a large community
with epilepsy. Children with intractable sei- sample, an age of onset prior to 5 years was the
zures (i.e., drug-resistant epilepsy) often show strongest predictor of lower intelligence (Berg
lower IQ compared to children with milder et al., 2008). Similar findings were reported by
forms of epilepsy, and this lowered IQ may others (e.g., Farwell et al., 1985; O’Leary et al.,
reflect a combination of regression of skills, 1983; Schoenfeld et al., 1999).
cognitive plateauing, or delayed acquisition of There are also setting-specific factors that
skills. Specific antiepileptic drugs (AEDs) may should be considered with respect to IQ expecta-
also affect IQ in children, and medication tox- tions in children with epilepsy. Those practicing
icity may produce marked declines in in community settings may see children with less
IQ. Overall, the newer-generation medications severe epilepsy whose seizures can be managed
typically have more favorable side effect pro- quite adequately in nonspecialized clinics and
files than earlier epilepsy drugs. For example, general outpatient centers; these children will
phenobarbital still sees frequent use in very present with a lower base rate of cognitive dis-
young children/infants presenting to emergency ability, with most functioning in the average
rooms, despite significant cognitive and behav- range. In contrast, clinicians working in hospitals
ioral side effects, including reducing IQ with specialized epilepsy programs will see a
(Farwell et al., 1990). At the same time, newer broader range of cognitive ability, including a
AEDs are not free of cognitive side effects; number of children with cognitive impairments,
medications such as topiramate and zonisamide, as the children with more complex management
for example, can produce significant cognitive issues often present to these specialized centers.
deficit in some individuals. Most AEDs, Moreover, neuropsychologists working in these
whether older or newer generation, have dose- settings often evaluate children being consid-
dependent side effects which increase with ered for surgical interventions. Within this sub-
polytherapy and primarily consist of sedation population, the frequency of children with
and inhibitory effects, along with behavioral intellectual disability is considerably higher than
effects such as irritability (Lee, 2010). in community settings, with many showing intel-
A high seizure frequency (Bourgeois et al., lectual function in the range of mild intellectual
1983; Farwell et al., 1985), multiple seizure types disability or lower. This issue is further discussed
(e.g., generalized tonic-clonic seizures, absences, below.
2 Evaluation of Children and Adolescents with Epilepsy 39

Though it is commonly held that the side of such, prior to evaluating children and adolescents
seizure onset may predict a pattern of performance with epilepsy, it is imperative for clinicians to
on IQ measures (e.g., left-sided onset is related to have a working understanding of the cognitive
reduced verbal IQ versus right-sided onset profile seen in specific syndromes such that the
predicting lowered performance or nonverbal evaluation can be planned appropriately, targeting
IQ), this may not be true in most children with areas of known or suspected deficits. A
epilepsy. In young children with epilepsy, the comprehensive review of specific cognitive
onset of seizures may coincide with the period of profiles associated with childhood epilepsy
critical language development. It is well syndromes is outside the scope of this chapter
established that early cerebral damage may (see MacAllister & Schaffer, 2007), but some
impact the cortical representation of language; common syndromes seen by neuropsychologists
whereas a normally developing right-handed assessing children with epilepsy will be briefly
child will likely show left-hemisphere language introduced as illustrative examples.
dominance, children with left-hemisphere Benign rolandic epilepsy (BRE) is the most
epilepsy may show functional reorganization, common epilepsy syndrome of childhood,
particularly when seizures are secondary to a representing about 15 % of all childhood seizure
catastrophic or extensive lesion experienced disorders (Sidenvall, Forsgren, Blomquist, &
early in life (e.g., left middle cerebral artery Heijbel, 1993), with an onset typically occurring
stroke). Accordingly, global measures of verbal between age 3 and 13 years. Boys are more often
and performance/perceptual intelligence may not affected. Characteristic EEG findings in BRE
show lateralized patterns because of neural involve centrotemporal spikes followed by slow
plasticity/functional reorganization seen in young waves that tend to be activated by sleep. Seizure
children with neurologic conditions. In a group semiology commonly involves brief, simple
of pediatric epilepsy patients for whom language partial, and hemifacial motor seizures that may
dominance was established via Wada procedure, secondarily generalize. Treatment with
only about a third of those with typical language antiepileptic medications is not always warranted
organization, and under a quarter of those with in BRE, as seizures tend to be infrequent,
atypical language organization, showed typically occur at night, and usually spontaneously
noteworthy VIQ/PIQ discrepancies. Of the total remit in adolescence (Bourgeois, 2000).
sample, side of focus was correctly predicted for Despite its designation as a “benign”
only 24 %, with 8 % being incorrectly lateralized. syndrome, neuropsychological impairments
The authors appropriately concluded that a VIQ/ often accompany BRE. For example, some
PIQ discrepancy alone is not effective in studies indicated deficits in language (vocabulary,
lateralizing the hemisphere of seizure onset prosody, phonological awareness, speech, verbal
(Blackburn et al., 2007). fluency), memory, motor skills, attention, and
executive functioning (Croona, Kihlgren,
Lundberg, Eeg-Olofsson, & Eeg-Olofsson, 1999;
Epilepsy Syndromes Gunduz, Demirbilek, & Korkmaz, 1999;
Northcott et al., 2005). Interestingly, EEG
It is important to appreciate that epilepsy is not a features in BRE may be only minimally
unitary condition, but represents a diverse associated with neuropsychological function,
neurological condition associated with many with no relation seen between cognition and
underlying causes. Accordingly, there is no single spike burden or laterality (Northcott et al., 2005).
neuropsychological profile associated with The International League Against Epilepsy
“epilepsy” in children, and heeding the specific specifies three common generalized idiopathic
syndrome is important, as cognitive deficits more epilepsies of childhood including childhood
closely reflect characteristics of the syndrome, absence seizures, juvenile absence seizures, and
rather than seizure or EEG characteristics. As juvenile myoclonic epilepsy. In childhood
40 W.S. MacAllister and E.M.S. Sherman

absence epilepsy (CAE), onset is typically As with adults with epilepsy, children
between age 3 and the teenage years, with girls frequently present with focal epilepsy syndromes.
being more frequently affected (Wirrell, 2003). Temporal lobe epilepsy is among the most
In juvenile absence epilepsy (JAE), seizures tend frequently occurring epilepsy syndromes in both
to develop in puberty. These syndromes are adults and children. In children, temporal lobe
challenging to differentiate, and studies of epilepsy typically has an onset in the school age
absence epilepsies often consider them together. years and, with a younger onset, cognitive
As such, information specific to JAE is limited, outcomes are often poorer and such children are
though this syndrome is often associated with at a high risk for learning disabilities (Jambaque
generalized tonic-clonic seizures upon awakening et al., 1993; Williams et al., 1996). Given the
(Loiseau, Duche, & Pedespan, 1995). Further, onset in the temporal lobes, it is not surprising
myoclonic seizures are present in 15 % of cases, that the most prominent neuropsychological
which also makes this syndrome difficult to deficits are in memory, which may be seen even
distinguish from juvenile myoclonic epilepsy in the presence of average IQ. However,
(JME; Reutens & Berkovic, 1995). impairments are often broader, and as in other
Overall, neuropsychological findings in CAE epilepsies, factors such as age of seizure onset
suggest that visual spatial and visual memory and seizure frequency contribute to greater
functions are more affected than verbal functions. neuropsychological impairment (Hermann &
One study of children with CAE showed that Seidenberg, 2002; Hermann et al., 2002).
these individuals had lower full-scale IQs than Children with TLE may also present with
matched controls (Pavone et al., 2001), though executive function deficits (Guimaraes et al.,
81 % had scores in the average range. Patients 2007; Rzezak et al., 2007, 2009) and attention
showed poorer visual spatial skills, nonverbal impairments (Dunn & Kronenberger, 2005;
memory, and delayed recall in comparison to Schubert, 2005). Further, language deficits, such
controls, with verbal skills and verbal memory as poor naming and reduced vocabulary, have
being less affected (Pavone et al., 2001). These been seen in children with left-sided temporal
findings were generally commensurate with later lobe epilepsy (Jambaque et al., 1993).
work on generalized idiopathic epilepsies Frontal lobe epilepsies are also seen in chil-
(Jambaque, Dellatolas, Dulac, Ponsot, & dren and are often caused by cortical dysplasia,
Signoret, 1993; Nolan et al., 2003). tumors, vascular lesions, trauma, or genetic
JME, the most common primary generalized factors. Unfortunately, frontal lobe epilepsies
epilepsy syndrome in adolescence, usually has an are often refractory to conventional treatments
onset between age 12 and 18 and requires lifelong and are associated with frequent seizures that
AED treatment. This syndrome is associated with tend to propagate and generalize (Bancaud &
myoclonic jerks of the neck, shoulders, and arms Talairach, 1992; Lawson et al., 2002). Pediatric
(Janz, 1985), and many also have generalized frontal lobe epilepsy may be associated with
tonic-clonic seizures and absences (Asconape & executive functioning problems such as diffi-
Penry, 1984). JME patients show difficulty on culties with planning, impulse control, tempo-
tests of executive functioning, including mental ral orientation, sequencing, categorization,
flexibility and concept formation (Devinsky mental flexibility, and verbal reasoning
et al., 1997; Holmes, Quiring, & Tucker, 2010; (Auclair, Jambaque, Dulac, LaBerge, &
Pulsipher et al., 2009). A study of 50 JME Sieroff, 2005; Culhane-Shelburne, Chapieski,
patients showed more extensive deficits; patients Hiscock, & Glaze, 2002; de Guise et al., 1999;
had poorer attention, inhibition, working Hernandez et al., 2002; Lendt et al., 2002;
memory, processing speed, and mental flexibility, Parisi et al., 2010; Patrikelis, Angelakis, &
in addition to difficulties with verbal and visual Gatzonis, 2009; Perez, Davidoff, Despland, &
memory, naming, and verbal fluency (Pascalicchio Deonna, 1993; Riva, Saletti, Nichelli, &
et al., 2007). Bulgheroni, 2002).
2 Evaluation of Children and Adolescents with Epilepsy 41

Occipital lobe epilepsies account for between idiopathic West syndrome may have a better
6 and 8 % of focal epilepsies (Manford, Hart, outcome (Guzzetta, 2006; Koo, Hwang, &
Sander, & Shorvon, 1992) and are more common Logan, 1993; Matsumoto et al., 1981). Lennox-
in children than in adults (Sveinbjornsdottir & Gastaut is a rare syndrome with an estimated
Duncan, 1993). Neuropsychological findings in incidence 1–2 per 100,000 children (Cowan,
pediatric occipital lobe epilepsy are inconsistent. 2002) with an age of onset between 2 and 8 years
One study recently demonstrated that children of age (Wirrell et al., 2005), though many have a
with occipital lobe epilepsy performed more prior history of infantile spasms. The syndrome
poorly than controls on measures of intellectual usually results from focal, multifocal, or diffuse
functioning, with PIQ being particularly affected brain dysfunction arising from diverse etiologies
(Gulgonen, Demirbilek, Korkmaz, Dervent, & (e.g., malformation, perinatal stroke), but idio-
Townes, 2000). Another study found VIQ to be pathic cases are seen that usually have an older
low relative to controls, with no difference found age at onset (Nabbout & Dulac, 2003). In
between patients and controls on PIQ (Germano Lennox-Gastaut syndrome, both tonic and aki-
et al., 2005). In both of these studies, however, netic seizures are seen, and EEG shows slow gen-
the children with epilepsy showed poor eralized spike-and-wave discharges. The outcome
performances across many other for these children is usually poor, with over 90 %
neuropsychological domains, including attention, showing mental retardation (Cowan, 2002).
memory, visuospatial skills, language skills, and Moreover, intellectual functioning may further
motor skills. deteriorate as these children age (Oguni, Hayashi,
The epileptic encephalopathies refer to a & Osawa, 1996). Earlier onset (i.e., below age 3)
group of severe epilepsy syndromes that often and a history of infantile spasms are associated
include deterioration of sensory and motor with poorer outcomes. In some, vagus nerve
skills. Each tends to include frequent seizures stimulation can improve seizure frequency and
and/or prominent interictal activity (Nabbout & may also result in some degree of cognitive
Dulac, 2003). Moreover, in all there is a regres- improvement. However, it is not clear if such
sion of cognitive development or a failure to gains are maintained over time (Majoie et al.,
attain developmental milestones, and as such 2001; Majoie, Berfelo, Aldenkamp, Renier, &
the long-term outcomes are quite guarded. A Kessels, 2005).
complete discussion of each of the epileptic Though fewer than 1 % of childhood epilep-
encephalopathies is outside the scope of this sies are characterized by continuous spike and
chapter, but the most well known are introduced waves during slow-wave sleep (Kramer et al.,
briefly below (i.e., West syndrome, Lennox- 1998), these syndromes have received a fair
Gastaut, Landau-Kleffner syndrome). amount of attention due to the dramatic effects on
West syndrome, with its onset in infancy, is neuropsychological functioning. In this group,
characterized by infantile spasms, psychomotor partial and generalized seizures are seen at the
deterioration, and hypsarrhythmia on EEG. The onset, and EEG shows generalized spike-wave
syndrome may be caused by neurological insult discharges that may occupy more than 85 % of
(e.g., infection or hypoxia-ischemia), cortical slow-wave sleep (Camfield & Camfield, 2002).
malformations, neurocutaneous syndrome (e.g., The age of onset is usually between 5 and 7 years
tuberous sclerosis complex, Sturge-Weber syn- (McVicar & Shinnar, 2004), and though most had
drome), genetic disorders, or an inborn error of normal development prior to onset, preexisting
metabolism (Wirrell, Farrell, & Whiting, 2005), neurological abnormalities are reported in about
though a third of cases are idiopathic (Nabbout & a third. Overall intellectual functioning, language
Dulac, 2003). Early signs of cognitive deficit skills, spatial orientation, motor skills, and
involve reduced social contact (Guzzetta, 2006). behavior are all adversely affected (Tassinari
About 80 % of individuals with West syndrome et al., 2000). The most well-studied disorder
present with intellectual disability, but those with associated with continuous spike and waves dur-
42 W.S. MacAllister and E.M.S. Sherman

ing slow-wave sleep is Landau-Kleffner syndrome and Adolescent Symptom Inventories indicated
(i.e., acquired epileptic aphasia), which includes that 11.4 % had possible ADHD-combined sub-
severe language regression at onset, involving type, 44 % had possible ADHD-inattentive sub-
profound deficits in comprehension (often type, and 2.3 % had ADHD-hyperactive subtype.
referred to as an auditory agnosia), though it usu- The inattentive subtype of ADHD was more
ally progresses to involve expressive language common in children with epilepsy; in children
deficits as well (Landau & Kleffner, 1957; with developmental ADHD, the combined sub-
Rotenberg & Pearl, 2003). Clinical seizures are type (with features of both inattention and hyper-
present in only about 80 % of children with activity) is far more common. Further, girls with
Landau-Kleffner syndrome and are not necessary epilepsy may be more likely to show attention
to make the diagnosis. problems. Epilepsy-related variables (e.g., sei-
zure type, location of onset) were not significant
predictors of ADHD symptoms (Dunn, Austin,
Evaluation of Specific Cognitive Harezlak, & Ambrosius, 2003).
Domains Hermann et al. (2007) evaluated the rate, sub-
type, and clinical correlates of ADHD in children
Attention and Executive Functions with idiopathic epilepsy recruited from neurology
clinics in the Midwestern United States. Results
As with most neurologic populations, attention were similar to prior findings, indicating that
and executive function deficits are quite common ADHD is more common in children with epilepsy
in childhood epilepsy. As such, a comprehensive than controls (31.5 % versus 6.4 %). Again, the
neuropsychological evaluation must include inattentive subtype was more common (52.1 %)
assessment of these skills, especially considering than hyperactive (13.1 %), combined subtypes
the fact that attentional and executive function (13.1 %), or ADHD—not otherwise specified
deficits often lead to considerable academic (17.4 %). Notably, the symptoms of ADHD pre-
underachievement. In epilepsy, attentional dated onset of epilepsy in 82 % of cases, and those
impairment is secondary to several factors, with ADHD and epilepsy showed poorer perfor-
including the underlying brain pathology, which mance across most neuropsychological tasks,
causes both the cognitive deficits and seizures, as with motor/psychomotor speed and executive
well as the seizures themselves (causing preictal, functioning (e.g., response inhibition, mental
ictal, and postictal symptomology). Moreover, flexibility, working memory, etc.) often being
interictal EEG phenomena often results in affected. Epilepsy variables, such as specific syn-
disrupted attention as many antiepileptic drome, medications, age of onset, and duration of
medication side effects. It should also be noted epilepsy, did not predict ADHD diagnosis. Given
that certain seizure types (e.g., absence seizures) the high likelihood of ADHD in children with epi-
have, as their primary manifestation, a behavioral lepsy, a competent neuropsychological evaluation
arrest which may be behaviorally indistinguish- should heed the guidelines set forth by the
able from inattention. It should not be surprising American Academy of Pediatrics to appropriately
that, given the above, attention deficit/hyperac- diagnose ADHD. These guidelines suggest that
tivity disorder (ADHD) is quite common in chil- the clinician must assess the formal DSM-IV cri-
dren with epilepsy. teria through evidence obtained from both parents
For example, in a study of 175 children and and teachers, including information regarding the
adolescents with various seizure types, 42 % of age of onset, duration of symptoms, and degree of
adolescents and 58 % of children were in the “at- functional impairment (American Academy of
risk” range for attention problems on the Child Pediatrics, 2000).
Behavior Checklist (CBCL), and 25 % adoles- “Executive functions,” mediated by a network
cents and 37 % of children fell in the “clinical” of neuroanatomical circuits involving the
range. Categorical classifications on the Child prefrontal cortex and its connections (Miller &
2 Evaluation of Children and Adolescents with Epilepsy 43

Cummings, 2007; Stuss & Knight, 2002), refer to behavioral problems, and those from community
higher-order cognitive functions such as planning, settings, the lowest. On the other hand, only
inhibition, set shifting, self-monitoring, recently in the pediatric epilepsy literature have
organization, working memory, and initiating executive functions been systematically assessed.
and sustaining motor and mental activity. Consequently, some earlier studies on neuropsy-
Assessment of executive dysfunction in children chological functioning in children with epilepsy
with epilepsy is important both because of the may not have reported executive deficits simply
high rate of such problems in clinical samples because these were not assessed.
and the fact that executive deficits predict poorer There are a number of well-known paradigms
quality of life in children. In particular, the that have been used in children with epilepsy and
constellation of executive dysfunction, low in other clinical groups to assess executive
adaptive level, high medication load, and a functions. The most well known is the Wisconsin
history of drug-resistant epilepsy contributes Card Sorting Test (WCST; Kongs, Thompson,
significantly to the risk of poor quality of life in Iverson, & Heaton, 2000), a test that measures
children with epilepsy (Sherman, Slick, & Eyrl, conceptual reasoning and ability to shift mental
2006). Executive dysfunction is associated with set. In one pediatric study involving tertiary-
earlier age of epilepsy onset and higher seizure center temporal lobe patients, the WCST had the
frequency (Hoie, Mykletun, Waaler, Skeidsvoll, highest sensitivity among executive functioning
& Sommerfelt, 2006), as well as with school tests (77 %; including 50 % for Trail Making B
performance problems (Hoie et al., 2006). and 26–40 % for fluency tasks; Rzezak et al.,
Further, long-term functional difficulties 2009). The highest executive dysfunction
associated with executive dysfunction include detection was when the WCST was used in
behavioral disturbance, social difficulties, and combination with at least one other executive
reduced educational and occupational attainment functioning test (94 %).
(Baron, 2004; Lezak, 2004). Although continuous performance tests
Executive problems are seen in a number of (CPTs), such as the Test of Variables of Attention
childhood epilepsies, including frontal lobe (Greenberg, 1988), Conners’ Continuous
epilepsy, temporal lobe epilepsy (see above), Performance Test II (Conners, 2004), and the
idiopathic absence epilepsy (Vuilleumier, Assal, Integrated Visual and Auditory Continuous
Blanke, & Jallon, 2000), childhood absence epi- Performance Test (Sandford & Turner, 1995), are
lepsy (Caplan, Siddarth, Stahl, Lanphier, Vona, typically considered sustained attention
Gurbani, et al., 2008), and benign rolandic epi- paradigms, all also involve an inhibition
lepsy (Nicolai, Aldenkamp, Arends, Weber, & component that requires the child to resist
Vles, 2006). Children with mild epilepsy (i.e., impulsive responding over time. Because these
recent onset, well controlled with medication, tasks also measure reaction time, continuous
normal IQ) tend to have poorer executive func- performance tasks allow for the measurement of
tioning than healthy children (Parrish et al., multiple neuropsychological domains. These
2007). At the more severe end of the epilepsy tasks are also generally well validated in terms of
spectrum, 40–50 % of children seen in tertiary sensitivity and validity in clinical groups (Riccio
settings present with clinically significant prob- & Reynolds, 2001; Riccio, Reynolds, & Lowe,
lems with executive functioning (Rzezak et al., 2001). In our experience, CPTs are especially
2007; Slick, Lautzenhiser, Sherman, & Eyrl, sensitive to subtler forms of inattention,
2006). Accurately synthesizing the literature on impulsivity, and executive dysfunction, likely
executive dysfunction in epilepsy requires because they are not administered through direct
remaining aware of ascertainment biases, which one-to-one interaction. Instead, the child works
influence the rate of cognitive deficits in epilepsy; independently on a computer and must maintain
as indicated above, children from tertiary centers a goal-directed, focused stance in order to
often have the highest rates of cognitive and perform well. CPTs are also typically longer than
44 W.S. MacAllister and E.M.S. Sherman

many executive functioning tasks, which may (Korkman, Kirk, & Kemp, 2007; for review, see
mitigate the novelty effect that sometimes masks Brooks, Sherman, & Strauss, 2009) and have also
executive difficulties. been used to assess executive functioning in chil-
Fluency tasks are also a classic method for dren with epilepsy (Bender, Marks, Brown,
detecting initiation, effortful search, and Zaroff et al. 2007; Parrish et al., 2007).
organization problems evident in the verbal and It is well established that performance-based
visual modality. Word fluency tasks require the tests of attention and executive functions are not
child to spontaneously provide lists of specific always sensitive to dysfunction by virtue of being
words (starting with a certain letter) or of specific administered in a structured, quiet, one-on-one
exemplars (from a given category such as testing environment that reduces the need for
animals), within a given timeframe. The self-initiated organization and problem-solving
analogous nonverbal tasks are design fluency (Strauss et al., 2006). Therefore, specialized
tests, which require generation of novel designs questionnaires completed by family members
under time constraints. Both the word fluency and teachers to assess attention and executive
and design fluency have been relatively well functioning in daily life are useful. To assess
studied in adults with epilepsy (e.g., Martin et al., attention (with consideration of formal diagnostic
2000; Suchy, Sands, & Chelune, 2003). Fluency criteria for ADHD), indices such as the SNAP-IV
tasks are part of the standard battery in most (Swanson, Schuck, Mann, et al., 2001), Conners’
epilepsy centers assessing children, though its Scales (Conners, 2001), or other similar DSM-
sensitivity to laterality in children remains to be IV-oriented scales are useful, though more
empirically determined. In one study, general rating scales that assess attention in
approximately 26–40 % of children with temporal addition to other psychological factors are also
lobe epilepsy from a tertiary care center had frequently employed (e.g., CBCL; Achenbach,
impaired scores on categorical fluency tasks 1991); Behavior Assessment System for
(Rzezak et al., 2009). Children-2 (BASC-2; Reynolds & Kamphaus,
The Rey Complex Figure Test (Rey, 1941), 2004) and these latter scales have utility in
although primarily considered a visual memory pediatric epilepsy (Bender, Auciello, Morrison,
test, includes a copy trial that can provide MacAllister, & Zaroff, 2008). Considering
information on organization skills in the visual executive functions per se, the Behavior Rating
modality, in addition to visual spatial ability, Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF; Gioia,
discussed below. Usually, the comparison to Isquith, Guy, & Kenworthy, 2000) is increasingly
performance on a simpler visual design copying used in the neuropsychological assessment of
task, such as the Beery-Buktenica visuomotor children with epilepsy. The BRIEF shows
integration test (VMI; Beery & Beery, 2006), can sensitivity to executive deficits in children with
inform as to whether deficits are primarily severe epilepsy (Slick et al., 2006), as well as in
organizational or reflect a more fundamental children with recent-onset epilepsy with good
problem with visuospatial or visuomotor skills. seizure control (Parrish et al., 2007). A version
Other tasks sensitive to age-dependent expression also exists for preschoolers (BRIEF-P; Gioia,
of executive deficits include the Stroop, Trail Espy, & Isquith, 2003), which is particularly
Making Test, Tower of London, Contingency welcome because reliable performance-based
Naming Test, and Twenty Questions Test (Jacobs, measures of executive functioning designed for
Harvey, & Anderson, 2007; Rzezak et al., 2007, this age group for clinical use are lacking.
2009; Strauss, Sherman, & Spreen, 2006). Many Ideally, the detection of attention and executive
of these basic paradigms have been used as deficits is the first step toward treatment. To date,
inspiration for executive functioning tests in there have been no studies aimed at improving
comprehensive batteries such as the Delis-Kaplan executive deficits per se in children with epilepsy,
Executive Function System (DKEFS; Delis, but there are a few studies demonstrating success-
Kaplan, & Kramer, 2001) and NEPSY-II ful pharmacological treatment of ADHD in chil-
2 Evaluation of Children and Adolescents with Epilepsy 45

dren with epilepsy, some finding improvements in literature. Classically, verbal memory deficits
quality of life after treatment (e.g., Yoo et al., have been associated with left-hemisphere
2009). There are also guidelines for treating phy- dysfunction, whereas memory for visually
sicians on how to balance AED treatment with presented information is suggestive of right-
pharmacological treatment of ADHD symptoms hemisphere pathology. However, the material
(Parisi et al., 2010). When assessing children with specificity of memory deficits in childhood
epilepsy, the neuropsychologist should include epilepsy has been variable across studies.
recommendations on treatments aimed explicitly Generally speaking, much of the empirical
at improving executive dysfunction such as refer- evidence suggests that children with memory
ral for consideration of stimulant medication and deficits may show more general cognitive
behavioral interventions. In addition, many parent impairment, though those with left-sided
and teacher resources aimed at children with pri- temporal lobe epilepsy may perform particularly
mary ADHD (e.g., Barkley, 2000) are useful for poorly. This should not seem all that surprising
children with epilepsy. given that an early memory deficit will result in
difficulties in the acquisition of knowledge,
which will lead to lower overall abilities.
Learning and Memory The hypothesis that memory in epilepsy
exhibits material specificity (i.e., that left-sided
Memory deficits are common in children with seizures are associated with verbal memory
epilepsy. Further, it has been shown that memory problems and right-sided seizures with visual
is more disrupted in children with complex partial memory problems) has a long history in the
seizures than other seizure types because these epilepsy literature. For example, in a study by
seizures tend to arise from temporal and Fedio and Mirsky (1969), children with left
frontotemporal zones, areas associated with temporal lobe epilepsy showed poor retention of
memory and learning. However, generalized verbal information, but both immediate and
epilepsy syndromes such as CAE and BRE may delayed nonverbal memory was intact.
also be associated with memory problems, Conversely, children with right temporal lobe
including poorer memory for nonverbal epilepsy showed the opposite pattern; these
information in comparison to verbal information children had impaired recall for nonverbal
(Pavone et al., 2001; Pinton et al., 2006). Notably, memory items (Fedio & Mirsky, 1969). Similar
a 2001 study did not show differences in memory results were reported by Jambaque et al. (1993)
performance across seizure types (Williams though the left temporal lobe epilepsy group
et al., 2001). Given the high base rate of memory studied here was lower functioning than the right
problems in epilepsy and the importance of temporal lobe group overall. However, numerous
memory in the acquisition of knowledge, a thor- other studies have failed to show such differences
ough assessment of memory skills is crucial. in children (e.g., Camfield et al., 1984; Engle &
Hermann, Seidenberg, & Bell (2002) demon- Smith, 2010; Helmstaedter & Elger, 2009), with
strated progressive cognitive decline (including researchers essentially finding no difference
memory and intellectual functioning) in children between visual and verbal memory between left
with temporal lobe epilepsy, with the decline and right temporal lobe epilepsy groups. Of note,
being associated with epilepsy duration. The a recent study of children with temporal lobe
authors posited that temporal lobe epilepsy affects epilepsy demonstrated that children with mesial
a negative impact on brain structure, setting into temporal involvement had lower memory skills
motion a cascade of cognitive deficits related to than those with lateral involvement. In this group,
reduced cognitive reserve and continuing seizures facial recognition tasks differentiated left and
(Hermann, Seidenberg, & Bell, 2002). right temporal lobe epilepsies, with the latter
The issue of material specificity of memory showing poorer performance, but other memory
deficits is an ongoing debate in the empirical tasks were not helpful in determining side of
46 W.S. MacAllister and E.M.S. Sherman

epilepsy onset (Gonzalez, Anderson, Wood, hood epilepsy (Giordani et al., 2006). The
Mitchell, & Harvey, 2007). Moreover, a recent NEPSY-II Edition also includes verbal and
study indicated that though lateralization of visual memory tests, including a task of mem-
seizure focus was not associated with verbal ory for faces, which again may be of particular
versus visual memory deficits, the laterality importance when evaluating memory deficits in
influenced the correlation between attention- and the presence of a right-hemisphere onset. Facial
material-specific memory (Engle & Smith, memory tasks are also seen in batteries such as
2010). the Children’s Memory Scale (Cohen, 1997),
Variable findings across studies may reflect the TOMAL-2 (Reynolds & Kamphaus, 2004),
psychometric issues to some extent but also and the KABC-2 (Kaufman & Kaufman, 2002),
may be due to the fact that children may show a larger cognitive battery that extends down to
less hemispheric specialization than adults. the preschool age.
Further, early damage or disruption of matura-
tional processes of the left hemisphere may be
associated with functional reorganization, with Language
the right hemisphere subsuming some aspects
of linguistic processing, which likely explains One of the most dramatic examples of language
why some studies report lower functioning deficits in the context of pediatric epilepsy is the
overall in children with left temporal lobe epi- Landau-Kleffner Syndrome, which has, as its
lepsy (Jambaque et al., 1993). It has also been primarily manifestation, an auditory agnosia
suggested that the rapid spread of seizures from followed by receptive (and later expressive)
one temporal lobe results in dysfunction in both language regression. However, as indicated
hippocampi, regardless of the side of seizure above, subtler language deficits can be seen in
origin, thus muddying the material specificity other epilepsy syndromes. For example, naming
of results. There are numerous instruments and vocabulary deficits are often seen in children
available to assess memory in children, with with left-sided temporal lobe epilepsy (Jambaque
several older instruments undergoing revisions et al., 1993), and earlier onset is often associated
that have significantly improved their psycho- with developmental language problems.
metric properties. The CVLT-C has been fre- However, in other children, language problems
quently utilized in the study of neurologic may arise or remit depending on epilepsy-related
illness in children and has shown sensitivity to factors. For example, a sample of young children
memory impairment in children with epilepsy with left frontal simple partial seizures was
as well (e.g., Williams et al., 2001). As in adult followed longitudinally. Results suggested
studies, the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure has dissociations between language comprehension
been utilized in the study of visual memory and expressive language. Specifically,
deficits in childhood epilepsy (Schouten, comprehension gradually improved, reaching
Hendriksen, & Aldenkamp, 2009). Many pedi- average performance by age 7. In contrast,
atric neuropsychologists prefer using co- expressive skills remained poorer (Cohen & Le
normed batteries of memory tests so that visual Normand, 1998). Volkl-Kernstock, Bauch-Prater,
and verbal memory can be directly compared. Ponocny-Seliger, and Feucht (2009) described a
The past decade has seen the revisions of sev- sample of children with benign rolandic epilepsy
eral such batteries, including the Wide Range that showed impairments in receptive language,
Assessment of Memory and Learning, Second expressive speech, and expressive vocabulary. An
Edition (WRAML-2; Sheslow & Adams, 2003), important finding in this study was that after
and the Test of Memory and Learning, Second remission of the seizure disorder, language defi-
Edition (TOMAL-2; Reynolds & Voress, 2007). cits were no longer seen in this group.
Prior research has shown the sensitivity of the Caplan, Siddarth, et al. (2009) commented on
WRAML (original version) to deficits in child- the paucity of research conducted on children
2 Evaluation of Children and Adolescents with Epilepsy 47

with epilepsy and normal intellectual function. under semi-structured conditions and may serve
Interestingly, their study, which included 183 as the initial foundational screening of language
children with either generalized or partial on which a more comprehensive evaluation may
seizures, a quarter of the younger children, a third be built. Measures of confrontation naming may
of the intermediate-aged children, and over half prove important, such as the Boston Naming Test
of the adolescent group, showed language deficits (Kaplan, Goodglass, & Weintraub, 1983) and
in comparison to their age-matched peers. Their Expressive One Word Vocabulary Test, Third
findings were interpreted to suggest an “age- Edition (Brownell, 2000), as well as measures of
related rise” in the vulnerability to language verbal initiation and fluency reviewed above.
deficits. In a 2008 study of 69 children with Examples of letter fluency and semantic fluency
absence seizures, nearly half showed language are available in several batteries with
deficits despite, on average, normal IQ (Caplan developmentally appropriate normative data,
et al., 2008). such as the DKEFS (Delis et al., 2001) and the
It is also important to recognize the fact that NEPSY-II (Korkman et al., 2007).
language skills, such as phonological awareness, If a more comprehensive evaluation of lan-
are the underpinnings of competent reading guage is deemed necessary based on parent and/
ability, and such skills have been shown to be or teacher concerns or clinician impressions
deficient in many children with epilepsy (Vanasse, during screening, several comprehensive lan-
Beland, Carmant, & Lassonde, 2005). Language- guage batteries are commercially available. The
based learning disabilities are therefore more Test of Language Development and Test of
prevalent in children with epilepsy than in the Adolescent Language (Newcomer & Hammil,
general population and more prominent in some 1988) have shown sensitivity to language defi-
kinds of epilepsy versus others. For example, cits in pediatric epilepsy (Caplan, Siddarth,
reading deficits are more prevalent in children et al., 2009) offering assessment of vocabulary,
with temporal lobe epilepsy compared to children syntax, and phonology. School systems often
with idiopathic generalized epilepsy and benign have familiarity with the Clinical Evaluation of
rolandic epilepsy (Chaix et al., 2006). Language Fundamentals (CELF)—4th Edition
The assessment of language is therefore an (Semel, Wiig, & Secord, 2003), which provides
important consideration in the comprehensive a comprehensive evaluation of receptive and
evaluation of neurocognitive functioning in expressive language abilities. Earlier versions of
pediatric epilepsy. The astute clinician will note the CELF have been employed in prior studies;
apparent receptive and expressive language for example, in a sample of children with stroke,
deficits in casual conversation, and the ability to those with comorbid epilepsy showed greater
understand verbal instructions during other language deficits on the CELF-Revised
neuropsychological tasks may be initial evidence (Ballantyne, Spilkin, & Trauner, 2007). The
of receptive language deficits, if present. CELF-IV also provides indices of phonological
Moreover, clinicians should pay close attention awareness, which is a foundational skill for
to the quality of verbal utterances during casual competent reading. Other measures of phono-
discourse, paying special attention to the rate, logical awareness include subtests from the
rhythm/prosody of speech, articulation, and the NEPSY-II (Korkman et al., 2007) or the
quality of grammatical and syntactical Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing
constructions. Certain subtests often routinely (Wagner, Torgesen, & Rashotte, 1999). The lat-
administered during assessment of intellectual ter may be particularly important when
functioning provide excellent opportunities to language-based learning disabilities are sus-
observe the latter; for example, the Vocabulary, pected, which again are more prevalent in indi-
Similarities, and Comprehension subtests of the viduals with epilepsy than in the general
WISC-IV allow observation of verbal expression population.
48 W.S. MacAllister and E.M.S. Sherman

Visuospatial and Visuomotor Skills ment of visuomotor skills (see Loring, Marino, &
Meador, 2007).
In surveying the extant literature on cognition in Numerous instruments are available for the
pediatric epilepsy, it becomes readily apparent assessment of visuospatial and visuomotor skills
that visual spatial skills have received fairly in children. As indicated, studies have shown that
sparse attention in comparison to other cognitive the PIQ (and/or perceptual reasoning) subtests of
domains. This may relate to the fact that deficits the Wechsler scales have shown sensitivity to
in domains such as attention or memory are more such deficits (Gulgonen et al., 2000). The Beery
likely to manifest as problems in the child’s day- VMI (Beery & Beery, 2006), a graphomotor task,
to-day life. Nevertheless, research clearly is perhaps one of the most frequently employed
suggests that children with epilepsy may show tasks by pediatric neuropsychologists to assess
visuospatial and motor deficits. these skills. The NEPSY battery includes an
Occipital-parietal areas are crucial in the analogous task (i.e., Design Copy) that has shown
processing of visuospatial information. This said, sensitivity to visuospatial dysfunction in child-
as indicated above, neuropsychological findings hood epilepsy (Zach, et al., 2007). Similarly,
in pediatric occipital lobe epilepsy have been other NEPSY subtests assess visual spatial skills
inconsistent with some studies suggesting PIQ that do not involve a motor component (e.g.,
deficits (Gulgonen et al., 2000) and others Arrows) and have been successfully used in this
showing poorer VIQ (Germano et al., 2005). population (Bender, Marks, Brown, Zach, et al.,
Deficits in visuospatial skills have also been 2007). It should also be noted that other more
demonstrated in other seizure types as well. For “classic” neuropsychological tasks that were ini-
example, in a sample of children with benign tially designed for use in adults, such as the Rey-
rolandic epilepsy, deficits were seen in spatial Osterrieth Complex Figure (Rey, 1941) and
orientation and spatial memory, and deficits were Judgment of Line Orientation test (Benton,
not associated with side of epilepsy focus (Volkl- Varney, & Hamsher, 1978), have pediatric norms
Kernstock, Willinger, & Feucht, 2006). In a study available and may be used in children. To assess
employing the NEPSY in children with various fine manual dexterity, many pediatric neuropsy-
epilepsy types, some showed deficits on chologists employ the Purdue Pegboard, which
visuospatial processing tasks. Specifically, 15 % has shown sensitivity to motor skills deficits in
of children showed deficits on the Design Copy childhood epilepsy. The grooved pegboard is
subtest, a task of graphomotor construction, and another option for such assessment (Mathews &
16 % showed impairment on the “Arrows” Klove, 1964). It is recommended that the assess-
subtest, a motor-free spatial perception task. Of ment of visual spatial skills in children with epi-
the subtests within the NEPSY battery, the task lepsy employs both motor tasks and pure
most sensitive to impairment was the Visuomotor perceptual tasks that will not be adversely
Precision subtest, a complex task of visual affected by poor motor control.
perception, speed, attention, and motor control;
73 % were impaired on this task (Bender, Marks,
Brown, Zach, Zaroff et al. 2007). In a study of Mood and Quality of Life
children with localization-related epilepsy, a
temporal onset was associated with poorer copy Unfortunately, mood disorders are fairly com-
of the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure (Schouten mon in pediatric epilepsies, and for this reason, a
et al., 2009). Numerous studies have demon- comprehensive neuropsychological evaluation
strated deficits in fine motor control in children should involve assessment of the child’s mental
with epilepsy (e.g., Giordani et al., 2006), and it health. A review of depression and anxiety disor-
should be noted that certain antiepileptic medica- ders in pediatric epilepsy was recently published
tions may produce a tremor and motor slowing as (Ekinci, Titus, Rodopman, Berkem, & Trevathan,
a side effect, which may complicate the assess- 2009), and readers are referred there for a more
2 Evaluation of Children and Adolescents with Epilepsy 49

comprehensive discussion, but major findings complicated the medical management is, the
will be discussed here. In studies of pediatric restrictions placed on child and family as a result
epilepsy, the prevalence rates of psychological of the illness, and the innate coping skills of
challenges vary quite widely due to methodolog- family members and the resources afforded to
ical issues, including how mood disorders were them (see Camfield, Breau, & Camfield, 2001).
assessed (e.g., self-report forms, parent-report As many as 50 % of mothers of children with
forms versus clinical interview). In a population- epilepsy are at risk for psychiatric difficulties
based survey of mental health problems in chil- such as clinical depression (Ferro & Speechley,
dren with epilepsy completed by Davies, 2009; Wood, Sherman, Hamiwka, Blackman, &
Heyman, and Goodman (2003), over 16 % of Wirrell, 2008). The total impact that epilepsy has
children had noteworthy mental health prob- on a family has been shown to relate to factors
lems. Another study employed a more compre- such as cognitive problems, neurologic
hensive interview of psychiatric problems (e.g., complications, as well as variables such as age of
the Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and onset, frequency of seizures, medications, and
Schizophrenia [KSADS]) and found that 12 % the frequency of visits to the doctor. Other studies
of children with complex partial seizures had have examined the impact of epilepsy on social
anxiety or depression, and 13 % of those with competence and peer relations. One study, for
childhood absence epilepsy had anxiety and/or example, showed that lower IQ and externalizing
depression. Importantly, suicidal ideation was behaviors are related to poorer social competence
also reported in many of these children (Ott (Caplan, Sagun, et al., 2005).
et al., 2001). Quality of life is increasingly important in the
With respect to factors that predict mental assessment of the impact of illness in people with
health problems, gender effects have been seen in medical conditions. Health-related quality of life
some studies. For example, one study showed (HRQOL) refers to a rating of subjective well-
that girls with epilepsy were more often depressed being as it relates to the impact of a medical
than boys (Dunn, Austin, & Huster, 1999). Some condition on physical functioning, psychological
have shown that seizure frequency may be related functioning, social functioning, and behavioral
to mood (Oguz, Kurul, & Dirik, 2002), and others functioning. Factors such as low IQ, low adaptive
have shown that children with epilepsy and functioning, psychosocial difficulties, high
associated intellectual disability may show more seizure frequency, intractability, polydrug
severe mood disorders (Buelow et al., 2003; therapy, long-standing epilepsy duration, parental
Davies et al., 2003). Psychiatric problems may maladjustment, low family income, and older age
also be related to antiepileptic medication use. tend to be associated with lower HRQOL in
One study, for example, reported a high rate of children with epilepsy (Buelow et al., 2003;
depression in children treated with phenobarbital Devinsky et al., 1999; Miller, Palermo, & Grewe,
in comparison to those treated with carbamazepine 2003; Sabaz, Cairns, Lawson, Bleasel, & Bye,
(Brent, Crumrine, Varma, Allan, & Allman, 2001; Sabaz et al., 2003; Sherman et al., 2002;
1987); depression subsided when phenobarbital Sillanpaa, Haataja, & Shinnar, 2004).
was discontinued (Brent, Crumrine, Varma, Several epilepsy-specific HRQOL instruments
Brown, & Allan, 1990). An extremely noteworthy have been validated for use in children and
finding is that of children having significant adolescents. Some examples include the Quality
mental health problems; only a third may receive of Life in Childhood Epilepsy Questionnaire
proper treatment for such difficulties (Caplan, (QOLCE, Sabaz et al., 2000), the Quality of Life
Sagun, et al., 2005). in Epilepsy Inventory for Adolescents
The impact of having a child with epilepsy (QOLIE-48; Cramer et al., 1999), the Impact of
also places considerable strain on a family. The Childhood Neurologic Disability Scale (ICND;
degree of impact is related to several key factors, Camfield, Breau, & Camfield, 2003), and the
including the severity of the epilepsy, how HRQOL in children with epilepsy (Ronen,
50 W.S. MacAllister and E.M.S. Sherman

Streiner, Rosenbaum, & Canadian Pediatric disorders for the purposes of rendering formal
Epilepsy Network, 2003). Generic instruments DSM-IV diagnoses, may be appropriate; this
that can be used across medical conditions are instrument is often considered to be the “gold
also useful, such as the PedsQL (Varni, Seid, standard” for the assessment of DSM-IV
& Rode, 1999). Not all instruments meet psy- disorders and has been used successfully in many
chometric, reliability, and validity standards research studies of psychiatric disturbance in
for all purposes, however; for a more detailed children with epilepsy (e.g., Caplan, Siddarth,
review of HRQOL instruments for use in epi- et al., 2005). However, as this instrument is fairly
lepsy, see Cowan and Baker (2004) and Eiser labor intensive if the full interview is completed,
and Morse (2001). routine use in a busy clinical practice is not
As in any population, the assessment of psy- always practical or necessary, but it should be
chological factors in pediatric epilepsy begins noted that prior work suggests that full KSADS
with a thorough clinical interview that assesses interview will identify more children with
mood and affect, appetite, sleep disturbance, as psychological problems than will screening
well as family and peer relationships. In the instruments such as the CBCL (Ott et al., 2001).
context of a full neuropsychological evaluation,
which in itself is extremely time intensive,
screening for psychological difficulties may The Role of the Pediatric
suffice, and several broadband scales are available Neuropsychologist
for these purposes. The most frequently employed in the Presurgical Evaluation
measures are the Child Behavior Checklist
(Achenbach, 1991) and the Behavior Assessment Within the context of a pediatric epilepsy
System for Children, 2nd Edition (Reynolds & presurgical evaluation, the neuropsychological
Kamphaus, 2004). Recent research in children assessment must meet several needs, including
with epilepsy has demonstrated that these two addressing questions regarding cognitive risks
instruments are strongly intercorrelated (Bender and benefits of surgery, determining the
et al., 2008). It is also important to note that these concordance of the cognitive profile with imaging
forms have parallel forms for self-report, parent and other investigations which may point to the
report, and teacher report. These are important in possibility of abnormal function outside areas
order to assess the child’s function across multiple identified by EEG and imaging, identifying any
settings, as each person viewing the child sees additional investigations needed to inform
him or her in a unique setting that may inform the surgical decision-making (e.g., language
overall clinical picture. This may also be of mapping, Wada exam), and addressing
particular importance as the children and psychosocial and school-based needs. Further,
adolescents themselves may show a tendency to the neuropsychologist helps families understand
minimize problems. Self-report indices have also the nature of their child’s cognitive strengths and
proven helpful in clinical assessment. For weaknesses, which can alleviate frustration and
example, short self-report inventories such as the help families develop realistic expectations with
Manifest Anxiety Scales (Reynolds & Richmond, regard to behavior, schooling, and family
1978) and the Child Depression Inventory adjustment.
(Kovacs, 1985) have been used in several studies At its core, the function of the presurgical
of children with epilepsy (Baki et al., 2004; neuropsychological evaluation is to determine
Caplan, Siddarth, et al., 2005). the neuropsychological risks and benefits of
In some cases, a more structured assessment undergoing a specific surgical procedure.
of psychological functioning may be warranted. Practically speaking, there are no clear guide-
Instruments such as the KSADS (Kaufman, lines on how this is accomplished in children.
Birmaher, Brent, et al., 1997), a formalized semi- However, there are several key concepts inherent
structured clinical interview for psychological to the presurgical evaluation of patients with
2 Evaluation of Children and Adolescents with Epilepsy 51

epilepsy. First, a risk/benefit analysis rests on The presurgical test battery should have
assessing two main components: the adequacy several features: (1) it should be comprehensive
of the to-be-resected tissue and the functional enough for the purposes of surgical decision-
reserve capacities of the non-resected tissue making, (2) it should show utility in detecting
(Chelune, 1995). Specifically, the neuropsycho- common cognitive and behavioral problems
logical capabilities of the tissue that will be specific to children with severe epilepsy, and (3)
resected informs clinicians of the likelihood of it should include measures that show sensitivity
declines after surgery (i.e., higher functional and responsiveness to treatment effects, be they
adequacy means the surgery may remove func- surgical or pharmacological. It is important to
tional tissue, which may lead to deficits after sur- recognize that the neuropsychological profile of
gery), whereas cognitive recovery depends on children with epilepsy often reflects the additive
the capacity of the brain circuits that will remain effects of the underlying neurological condition
postsurgically. In general, children with high from which seizures arise (e.g., perinatal stroke,
cognitive reserve (e.g., high IQ), discrete focal cortical dysplasia), on which are superimposed
lesions (e.g., unilateral mesial temporal sclero- the cognitive effects of antiepileptic drugs,
sis), and no factors that may hinder plasticity seizure activity, interictal discharges, and pre-
(e.g., a high likelihood of seizure freedom after and postictal cognitive alterations. For these
surgery) will have a higher potential for reorga- reasons, the neuropsychological profile of
nization and compensation for deficits than those children with epilepsy, particularly those seen as
with low reserve, extensive or multifocal lesions, part of a presurgical evaluation, may best be
and factors that hinder plasticity (e.g., a likeli- considered a current “snapshot,” rather than as a
hood of continuing seizures after surgery). The stable and enduring estimate of cognition;
clinician should consider factors that affect neu- surgical candidates are by definition assessed
ropsychological outcome, such as age at epi- when all other treatments have failed, often while
lepsy onset, age at surgery, type of underlying on several antiepileptic drugs at maximum
abnormality, side of resection, and likelihood of dosages, and when seizures are at their worst in
ongoing seizures after surgery, all of which are frequency or intensity. Accordingly, cognitive
prognostic factors for cognitive recovery, com- disruption caused by seizures and medication
pensation, and reorganization of function (Elger, side effects may be at their peak when children
Helmstaedter, & Kurthen, 2004; Helmstaedter & are assessed presurgically.
Kockelmann, 2006; Hermann & Loring, 2008). Finally, at the time of the evaluation, the exact
At the same time, it is important to emphasize seizure focus and the exact surgical approach that
that children may show a potential for recovery will be offered to the patient may be unknown.
of functions both ipsilateral and contralateral to For example, an outpatient EEG might indicate
the surgical site that is greater than that of adult left temporal discharges, which on further
patients (Gleissner, Sassen, Schramm, Elger, & investigations (e.g., inpatient video EEG,
Helmstaedter, 2005). Other pragmatic aspects of intracranial monitoring, etc.) actually represent
evaluating patients with epilepsy such as ictal secondary spread from a distant focus.
and interictal effects on cognition during testing Consequently, the neuropsychological evaluation
and antiepileptic medication load at the time of should be comprehensive and flexible enough to
testing (Helmstaedter, Kurthen, Lux, Reuber, & allow the neuropsychologist to address issues
Elger, 2003) should be documented, as these pertinent to different kinds of resections, should
may affect the validity of the findings. Ideally, the surgical plan change after the evaluation is
the surgical work-up will be followed by a post- completed.
surgical assessment, typically conducted 1 year As in other evaluations, determination of the
after surgery, to help identify cognitive recovery, child’s IQ typically comprises the first step in the
rule out further decline, and assist with ongoing presurgical evaluation. Although the choice of IQ
treatment planning. measure is up to the clinician, the vast majority of
52 W.S. MacAllister and E.M.S. Sherman

research on intellectual function in pediatric Other important domains to assess are atten-
epilepsy has employed Wechsler tests (see tion and executive functions. Although deficits in
above). In our experience, the high base rate of these functions may have a high base rate in sur-
children with cognitive disabilities and the need gical candidates including children with epileptic
to document these for school and community foci outside the frontal lobe (Guimaraes et al.,
programming justify the use of full IQ batteries 2007; Rzezak et al., 2009), improvements in
rather than short forms, though these decisions these functions may follow surgery (Kim, Lee,
must be balanced against practical considerations, Yoo, Kang, & Lee, 2007; Martin et al., 2000).
including the fact that some evaluations are con- Visuospatial testing is also critical, and assess-
ducted during inpatient EEG monitoring in less- ment of children undergoing posterior resections
than-optimal testing conditions with children who should include screening for attentional and
may not be able to participate in lengthy testing visuo-constructional problems, including neglect
sessions. Adequately measuring IQ is particularly (Gleissner, Kuczaty, Clusmann, Elger, &
important in surgical candidates because declines Helmstaedter, 2008; Luerding, Boesebeck, &
in cognition may indicate that seizures are wors- Ebner, 2004). Assessing motor skills is also
ening or the presence of medically critical situa- important to determine the presence of lateral-
tions such as nonconvulsive status epilepticus or ized difficulties but also for measuring the impact
medication toxicity, all of which require immedi- of AEDs.
ate review by the treating epileptologist. Other A surgical battery should further provide a
children may exhibit lags in development result- comprehensive coverage of psychosocial
ing from frequent seizures or from an underlying function that allows for screening for the most
neurodevelopmental condition, which can be common psychiatric comorbidities in pediatric
detected by tracking IQ over time. In our experi- epilepsy, namely, ADHD, anxiety, and depression
ence, approximately 1/3 to ½ of children evalu- (Pellcock, 2004). A surgical battery should
ated for epilepsy surgery have a cognitive include an adaptive functioning scale to help
disability (i.e., IQ below 70 with associated adap- determine the presence of cognitive disability,
tive function deficits). another highly prevalent condition in children
The most crucial domains assessed during with intractable seizures, and because adaptive
presurgical evaluations are language and memory skills may change over time (Berg et al., 2004).
due to the large impact deficits these domains Ideally, the battery should include an assessment
may cause. In adults, verbal memory declines are of health-related quality of life using scales that
commonly reported side effects of temporal lobe have been validated in pediatric epilepsy, to help
surgery, with base rates of between 30 and 50 % determine the functional impact of surgery on
in left-sided surgeries (Engman, Andersson- daily functioning on the child and family.
Roswall, Svensson, & Malmgren, 2004; Stroup Neuropsychologists working in epilepsy
et al., 2003). Visual memory deficits may not be surgery centers should have some expertise in
as consistently reported with right temporal evaluating very low-functioning children who
lobectomy but do occur with similar incidence in may be “untestable” using standardized face-to-
some series (Dulay et al., 2009). face measures. At times, evaluations of this sort
A comprehensive surgical battery should will consist of a parent interview, behavioral
also include measures of expressive and recep- observations and non-standardized testing, and a
tive language. In adults, declines on naming small battery of standardized questionnaires/
tasks after left temporal lobe surgery range structured interviews for assessing adaptive
between 20 and 50 % (Davies, Risse, & Gates, functioning, such as the Adaptive Behavior
2005; Schwarz, Pauli, & Stefan, 2005), consis- Assessment System-II (Harrison & Oakland,
tent with the dominant temporal lobe’s role in 2003) or Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales
naming and receptive language in most right- (Sparrow, Balla, & Cicchetti, 1984). Although
handed individuals. the value of assessing “untestable” children may
2 Evaluation of Children and Adolescents with Epilepsy 53

have been questioned in the past, parents and & Elger, 2009). Unfortunately, with continuing
treating physicians frequently appreciate the chronic seizures in childhood, the outlook for
insights these evaluations bring to understanding psychological adjustment and quality of life is
the child’s condition, as well as the extra services poor, and this includes educational, social,
that can be advocated for by documenting the marital, and occupational functioning (Elger
degree of cognitive and language delay in the et al., 2004; Helmstaedter et al., 2003; Sherman,
neuropsychological evaluation. Standardized 2009; Sillanpaa et al., 2004). The family must
questionnaires of adaptive function and quality of then determine, with the assistance of the
life also serve as a benchmark for measuring treatment team, whether surgery and its attendant
improvements after surgery in children whose risk of cognitive changes is an acceptable risk in
severe cognitive and behavioral challenges light of the possibility of better seizure control.
preclude standardized testing. Determining language dominance is an
One of the most important aspects of the neu- important component of the presurgical evalua-
ropsychological assessment is providing infor- tion because surgical approaches will differ
mation to the family that will help guide them in based on whether surgery occurs in the domi-
decision-making regarding their child’s epilepsy nant or non-dominant hemisphere and whether
surgery. Oddly, there is very little information in the surgery may involve eloquent cortex. In
the literature on how this should be accom- children, the probability of atypical speech
plished. In our experience in providing feedback increases with seizure onset before age 6, left-
to families, there are three broad areas to cover: sided seizure focus, extra-temporal seizures,
(1) the child’s neuropsychological functioning and left-handedness (Saltzman-Benaiah, Scott,
overall, with a discussion of specific strengths & Smith, 2003). In addition, over half of chil-
and weaknesses; (2) the factors presumed to dren with left-hemisphere lesions and seizure
underlie any cognitive problems, including the onset before age 6 show atypical language lat-
location of the lesion based on imaging and EEG eralization that is unrelated to the type of
findings and the combined impact of seizures pathology underlying the epilepsy, be it devel-
and AEDs on cognition; (3) surgical risks and opmental (e.g., cortical dysplasia, tuberous
benefits from a cognitive and psychosocial sclerosis), acquired (e.g., mesial temporal scle-
standpoint; and finally, (4) implications of test rosis, encephalitis), or tumor related (Kadis
results for school programming and home. In et al., 2009).
most cases, the main risks that need to be dis- Techniques for determining language domi-
cussed with families are the possibility of nance range in terms of invasiveness, sensitivity,
declines in language and/or declines in memory, and spatial resolution. Such techniques include
particularly declines in verbal memory since this handedness, dichotic listening, fMRI, Wada test-
is the deficit that may have the greatest impact ing, and language mapping, discussed here in
on academics. order of increasing invasiveness. Readers are
Importantly, the costs of not proceeding to referred to other sources for further information
surgery should also be discussed with the family. on these and other techniques for mapping cogni-
Risks of this kind can rarely be conclusively tive functions, such as MEG (Papanicolaou et al.,
determined on an individual basis (unless the 2004) and near infrared spectroscopy (Gallagher
child has a clear risk factor for cognitive decline, et al., 2007).
such as repeated and uncontrollable bouts of Determining handedness provides an initial
status epilepticus), but the family should be “ball-park” estimate of language dominance.
informed of factors relevant to the child’s future Because the vast majority of right-handers are
cognitive prognosis, including the possibility of left-hemisphere dominant, left-handedness
cognitive plateauing or decline with continued (without a family history of sinistrality) may be a
uncontrolled seizures, an established risk in red flag for atypical language dominance but will
chronic epilepsy (Elger et al., 2004; Helmstaedter in no way provide a definitive estimate of which
54 W.S. MacAllister and E.M.S. Sherman

hemisphere subserves language. For example, testing. Research on language mapping in chil-
although the base rate of atypical language dren is more limited than in adults but indicates
dominance is much higher in left-handed people that language mapping may have low sensitivity
with epilepsy, 50 % of these left-handers to identification of language-critical sites in chil-
nevertheless have left-hemisphere language dren younger than 8–10 years (Ojemann, Berger,
dominance, and less than 10 % will have full Lettich, & Ojemann, 2003; Schevon et al., 2007),
right-hemisphere language dominance (Lee, although some report success with children as
2010). Handedness can be inferred with interview young as 2 years of age (Duchowny et al., 1996;
and unstructured observation or determined via a Ojemann et al., 2003). Children also show greater
standardized lateral dominance examination variability in language sites compared to adults
involving manipulation of real objects. (Ojemann et al., 2003). Reduced efficacy of map-
Dichotic listening tasks have a long history in ping in younger children has been attributed to
the determination of language dominance (Strauss immature myelination, which requires higher
et al., 2006). However, they too are not definitive, stimulation thresholds (Schevon et al., 2007).
as lesion affects peripheral hearing impairments, Intraoperative mapping can be accomplished in
and speech-processing problems may complicate awake children during a craniotomy, but this is
interpretation of results (Fernandes, Smith, typically reserved for higher-functioning, older
Logan, Crawley, & McAndrews, 2006; Gramstad, children, and some centers conduct only extra-
Engelsen, & Hugdahl, 2003). operative mapping in children. Extra-operative
fMRI paradigms to assess language have language mapping can be conducted in even very
become a standard in many centers. Semantic young or behaviorally challenged patients,
decision tasks, verbal fluency, sentence genera- although the resolution of the subdural grid may
tion, and listening to sentences have been used not be as high as mapping that uses stimulation
successfully with children in fMRI paradigms points chosen by the surgeon during an awake
(Hertz-Pannier et al., 2000). The disadvantages craniotomy (Ojemann et al., 2003).
of fMRI are that widespread circuits may be acti- Extra-operative language mapping is typically
vated which may make it difficult to identify cru- carried out by the neuropsychologist and
cial language zones against a more generalized neurologist at bedside using a variety of stimuli
language network for surgical planning, which is and paradigms to assess language functions.
where language mapping may be of particular Although many paradigms are used across
utility. For this reason, fMRI language para- centers, the procedure typically includes testing
digms are used principally to determine which of language comprehension (e.g., following
hemisphere subserves language but are less use- simple commands, sentence completion tasks),
ful for determining with precision the exact confrontation naming, and continuous speech.
demarcation lines of language zones in the surgi- Before mapping, it is crucial that baseline testing
cal field. fMRI requires children who are suffi- be undertaken to identify a series of items that
ciently testable and old enough to cooperate with can be performed error free; results from the
the fMRI testing procedure, which may include neuropsychological assessment are often very
relatively lengthy sessions of immobility. useful in gearing these items to the child’s
Because many children being considered for epi- capacities. Because many children feel lethargic
lepsy surgery have intellectual disability or or uncomfortable during the intracranial
behavioral disorders, such techniques are not monitoring period, language mapping is often
feasible for all children. carried out in several short sessions using
Cortical stimulation of language areas (lan- generous praise and reinforcements. For more
guage mapping or electrocortical stimulation in-depth discussions of language mapping, see
mapping) permits a precise localization of lan- Lee (2010) or Chap. 5.
guage-dependent cortical zones and provides a Wada testing (i.e., the intracarotid amobarbital
more fine-grained localization than fMRI or Wada procedure) is a deactivation procedure during
2 Evaluation of Children and Adolescents with Epilepsy 55

which one of the cerebral hemispheres is Appropriate assessment and characterization of


anaesthetized in order to test the language and cognitive and behavioral strengths and
memory capacity of the contralateral hemisphere. weaknesses as part of the neuropsychological
As such, it provides a “reversible surgery” assessment of children with epilepsy is beneficial
simulation and can be used for predicting patients for planning and tracking medical treatment
who may be at risk of amnesia postsurgery. (pharmacological, surgical, and rehabilitative)
Although an in-depth discussion of the history, and for educational planning, toward the goal of
rationale, and limitations of the Wada test is helping each child maximize academic and
beyond the scope of this chapter (see Chap. 4), occupational potential.
the routine use of this procedure has been
increasingly questioned, and some centers no
longer administer the Wada to all surgical
candidates (Baxendale, Thompson, & Duncan,
References
2008a, 2008b; Kubu, Girvin, McLachlan, Pavol, Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Manual for the child behavior
& Harnadek, 2000). Wada testing may have checklist. Burlington: Department of Psychiatry,
reduced sensitivity in children compared to adults University of Vermont.
(Jansen et al., 2002), particularly under age 10, American Academy of Pediatrics. (2000). Clinical prac-
tice guideline: Diagnosis and evaluation of the child
and may cause obtundation in some (Schevon with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. American
et al., 2007). However, Wada testing has been Academy of Pediatrics. Pediatrics, 105(5),
used successfully in children as young as 2 and in 1158–1170.
very cognitively impaired children to identify Asconape, J., & Penry, J. K. (1984). Some clinical and
EEG aspects of benign juvenile myoclonic epilepsy.
language dominance, though memory testing is Epilepsia, 25(1), 108–114.
more difficult to assess in younger children Auclair, L., Jambaque, I., Dulac, O., LaBerge, D., &
(Jansen et al., 2002). Sieroff, E. (2005). Deficit of preparatory attention in
children with frontal lobe epilepsy. Neuropsychologia,
43(12), 1701–1712.
Baki, O., Erdogan, A., Kantarci, O., Akisik, G., Kayaalp,
Conclusions L., & Yalcinkaya, C. (2004). Anxiety and depression
in children with epilepsy and their mothers. Epilepsy
The neuropsychological assessment of children & Behavior, 5(6), 958–964.
Ballantyne, A., Spilkin, A., & Trauner, D. (2007).
and adolescents with epilepsy is a complex Language outcome after perinatal stroke: Does side
undertaking. The competent clinician will have matter? Child Neuropsychology, 13(6), 494–509.
knowledge of the cognitive deficits associated Bancaud, J., & Talairach, J. (1992). Clinical semiology of
with specific epilepsy syndromes to provide an frontal lobe seizures. Advances in Neurology, 57, 3–58.
Barkley, R. A. (2000). Taking charge of ADHD: The com-
adequate assessment of cognitive skills that plete authoritative guide for parents. New York, NY:
mirrors known or suspected areas of difficulty in Guilford.
a given child. Moreover, a skilled clinician must Baron, I. (2004). Neuropsychological evaluation of the
be aware of various factors associated with child. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Baxendale, S., Thompson, P. J., & Duncan, J. S. (2008a).
cognitive deficits in children with epilepsy, as The role of the Wada test in the surgical treatment of
well as of tasks sensitive to neuropsychological temporal lobe epilepsy: An international survey.
strengths and weaknesses in order to provide Epilepsia, 49(4), 715–720. discussion 720–725.
accurate and helpful treatment recommendations. Baxendale, S. A., Thompson, P. J., & Duncan, J. S.
(2008b). Evidence-based practice: A reevaluation of
Furthermore, those working in specialized the intracarotid amobarbital procedure (Wada test).
epilepsy centers must demonstrate expertise in Archives of Neurology, 65(6), 841–845.
specialized procedures such as cortical mapping Beery, K. E., & Beery, N. A. (2006). The Beery Buktenica
and tools for determining language dominance developmental test of visual-motor integration—Fifth
edition: Administration, scoring, and teaching man-
and have skills in determining the cognitive risks ual. Minneapolis, MN: NCS Pearson.
and benefits of surgery for individual children as Bender, H. A., Auciello, D., Morrison, C., MacAllister,
part of a comprehensive presurgical evaluation. W. S., & Zaroff, C. M. (2008). Comparing the
56 W.S. MacAllister and E.M.S. Sherman

convergent validity and clinical utility of the BASC- Camfield, C., Breau, L., & Camfield, P. (2001). Impact of
PRS and CBCL in children with epilepsy. Epilepsy & pediatric epilepsy on the family: A new scale for
Behavior, 13, 237–242. clinical and research use. Epilepsia, 42(1), 104–112.
Bender, H. A., Marks, B. C., Brown, E. R., Zach, L., & Camfield, C., Breau, L., & Camfield, P. (2003). Assessing
Zaroff, C. M. (2007). Neuropsychologic performance the impact of pediatric epilepsy and concomitant
of children with epilepsy on the NEPSY. Pediatric behavioral, cognitive, and physical/neurologic
Neurology, 36(5), 312–317. disability: Impact of childhood neurologic disability
Bender, H. A., Marks, B. C., Brown, E., & Zaroff, C. M. scale. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology,
(2007). The clinical utility of the NEPSY in assessing 45(3), 152–159.
young children with epilepsy. Pediatric Neurology, 36, Camfield, P., & Camfield, C. (2002). Epileptic syndromes
312–317. in childhood: Clinical features, outcomes, and
Benton, A. L., Varney, N. R., & Hamsher, K. D. (1978). treatment. Epilepsia, 43(Suppl 3), 27–32.
Visuospatial judgment: A clinical test. Archives of Camfield, P. R., Gates, R., Ronen, G., Camfield, C.,
Neurology, 35, 364–367. Ferguson, A., & MacDonald, G. W. (1984).
Berg, A. T., Langfitt, J. T., Testa, F. M., Levy, S. R., Comparison of cognitive ability, personality profile,
DiMario, F., Westerveld, M., et al. (2008). Global and school success in epileptic children with pure right
cognitive function in children with epilepsy: A versus left temporal lobe EEG foci. Annals of
community-based study. Epilepsia, 49(4), 608–614. Neurology, 15(2), 122–126.
Berg, A. T., Smith, S. N., Frobish, D., Beckerman, B., Caplan, R., Levitt, J., Siddarth, P., Wu, K. N., Gurbani, S.,
Levy, S. R., Testa, F. M., et al. (2004). Longitudinal Sankar, R., et al. (2009). Frontal and temporal volumes
assessment of adaptive behavior in infants and young in childhood absence epilepsy. Epilepsia, 50(11),
children with newly diagnosed epilepsy: Influences of 2466–2472.
etiology, syndrome, and seizure control. Pediatrics, Caplan, R., Sagun, J., Siddarth, P., Gurbani, S., Koh, S.,
114(3), 645–650. Gowrinathan, R., et al. (2005). Social competence in
Blackburn, L. B., Lee, G. P., Westerveld, M., Hempel, A., pediatric epilepsy: Insights into underlying
Park, Y. D., & Loring, D. W. (2007). The verbal IQ/ mechanisms. Epilepsy & Behavior, 6(2), 218–228.
Performance IQ discrepancy as a sign of seizure focus Caplan, R., Siddarth, P., Gurbani, S., Hanson, R., Sankar,
laterality in pediatric patients with epilepsy. Epilepsy R., & Shields, W. D. (2005). Depression and anxiety
& Behavior, 10(1), 84–88. disorders in pediatric epilepsy. Epilepsia, 46(5),
Bourgeois, B. F. (2000). Drug treatment of benign focal 720–730.
epilepsies of childhood. Epilepsia, 41(8), 1057–1058. Caplan, R., Siddarth, P., Stahl, L., Lanphier, E., Vona, P.,
Bourgeois, B. F., Prensky, A. L., Palkes, H. S., Talent, Gurbani, S., et al. (2008). Childhood absence epilepsy:
B. K., & Busch, S. G. (1983). Intelligence in epilepsy: Behavioral, cognitive, and linguistic comorbidities.
A prospective study in children. Annals of Neurology, Epilepsia, 49(11), 1838–1846.
14(4), 438–444. Caplan, R., Siddarth, P., Vona, P., Stahl, L., Bailey, C.,
Brent, D. A., Crumrine, P. K., Varma, R. R., Allan, M., & Gurbani, S., et al. (2009). Language in pediatric
Allman, C. (1987). Phenobarbital treatment and major epilepsy. Epilepsia, 50(11), 2397–2407.
depressive disorder in children with epilepsy. Chaix, Y., Laguitton, V., Lauwers-Cances, V., Daquin, G.,
Pediatrics, 80(6), 909–917. Cances, C., Demonet, J. F., et al. (2006). Reading
Brent, D. A., Crumrine, P. K., Varma, R., Brown, R. V., & abilities and cognitive functions of children with
Allan, M. J. (1990). Phenobarbital treatment and epilepsy: Influence of epileptic syndrome. Brain &
major depressive disorder in children with epilepsy: A Development, 28(2), 122–130.
naturalistic follow-up. Pediatrics, 85(6), 1086–1091. Chelune, G. J. (1995). Hippocampal adequacy versus
Brooks, B. L., Sherman, E. M., & Strauss, E. (2009). Test functional reserve: Predicting memory functions
review: NEPSY-II: A developmental following temporal lobectomy. Archives of Clinical
neuropsychological assessment, second edition. Child Neuropsychology, 10(5), 413–432.
Neuropsychology, 16, 80–101. Cohen, M. (1997). The children’s memory scale. San
Brownell, R. (2000). Expressive one-word picture vocab- Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
ulary test—Third edition manual. Novato, CA: Cohen, H., & Le Normand, M. (1998). Language develop-
Academic Therapy Publications. ment in children with simple-partial left-hemisphere
Buelow, J. M., Austin, J. K., Perkins, S. M., Shen, J., epilepsy. Brain and Language, 64(3), 409–422.
Dunn, D. W., & Fastenau, P. S. (2003). Behavior and Conners, C. K. (2001). Conners’ rating scales: Revised.
mental health problems in children with epilepsy and North Tonawanda, NY: Multi-Health Systems.
low IQ. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, Conners, C. K. (2004). Conners’ CPT II continuous per-
45(10), 683–692. formance test II. North Tonawanda, NY: Multi-Health
Bulteau, C., Jambaque, I., Viguier, D., Kieffer, V., Systems.
Dellatolas, G., & Dulac, O. (2000). Epileptic syn- Cowan, L. D. (2002). The epidemiology of the epilepsies
dromes, cognitive assessment and school placement: in children. Mental Retardation and Developmental
A study of 251 children. Developmental Medicine and Disabilities Research Reviews, 8(3), 171–181.
Child Neurology, 42(5), 319–327.
2 Evaluation of Children and Adolescents with Epilepsy 57

Cowan, J., & Baker, G. A. (2004). A review of subjective Journal of the American Academy of Child and
impact measures for use with children and adolescents Adolescent Psychiatry, 38(9), 1132–1138.
with epilepsy. Quality of Life Research, 13(8), Dunn, D., & Kronenberger, W. (2005). Childhood epi-
1435–1443. lepsy, attention problems, and AD/HD: Review and
Cramer, J. A., Westbrook, L. E., Devinsky, O., Perrine, K., practical considerations. Seminars in Pediatric
Glassman, M. B., & Camfield, C. (1999). Development Neurology, 12, 222–2228.
of the quality of life in epilepsy inventory for Eiser, C., & Morse, R. (2001). A review of measures of
adolescents: The QOLIE-AD-48. Epilepsia, 40(8), quality of life for children with chronic illness.
1114–1121. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 84(3), 205–211.
Croona, C., Kihlgren, M., Lundberg, S., Eeg-Olofsson, Ekinci, O., Titus, J. B., Rodopman, A. A., Berkem, M., &
O., & Eeg-Olofsson, K. E. (1999). Neuropsychological Trevathan, E. (2009). Depression and anxiety in
findings in children with benign childhood epilepsy children and adolescents with epilepsy: Prevalence,
with centrotemporal spikes. Developmental Medicine risk factors, and treatment. Epilepsy & Behavior,
and Child Neurology, 41(12), 813–818. 14(1), 8–18.
Culhane-Shelburne, K., Chapieski, L., Hiscock, M., & Elger, C. E., Helmstaedter, C., & Kurthen, M. (2004).
Glaze, D. (2002). Executive functions in children with Chronic epilepsy and cognition. Lancet Neurology,
frontal and temporal lobe epilepsy. Journal of the 3(11), 663–672.
International Neuropsychological Society, 8(5), Engle, J. A., & Smith, M. L. (2010). Attention and mate-
623–632. rial-specific memory in children with lateralized epi-
Davies, S., Heyman, I., & Goodman, R. (2003). A popula- lepsy. Neuropsychologia, 48(1), 38–42.
tion survey of mental health problems in children with Engman, E., Andersson-Roswall, L., Svensson, E., &
epilepsy. Developmental Medicine and Child Malmgren, K. (2004). Non-parametric evaluation of
Neurology, 45(5), 292–295. memory changes at group and individual level
Davies, K. G., Risse, G. L., & Gates, J. R. (2005). Naming following temporal lobe resection for pharmaco-
ability after tailored left temporal resection with resistant partial epilepsy. Journal of Clinical and
extraoperative language mapping: Increased risk of Experimental Neuropsychology, 26(7), 943–954.
decline with later epilepsy onset age. Epilepsy & Farwell, J. R., Dodrill, C. B., & Batzel, L. W. (1985).
Behavior, 7(2), 273–278. Neuropsychological abilities of children with epilepsy.
de Guise, E., del Pesce, M., Foschi, N., Quattrini, A., Epilepsia, 26(5), 395–400.
Papo, I., & Lassonde, M. (1999). Callosal and cortical Farwell, J. R., Lee, Y. J., Hirtz, D. G., Sulzbacher, S. I.,
contribution to procedural learning. Brain, 122(Pt 6), Ellenberg, J. H., & Nelson, K. B. (1990). Phenobarbital
1049–1062. for febrile seizures—Effects on intelligence and on
Delis, D. C., Kaplan, E., & Kramer, J. H. (2001). Delis- seizure recurrence. The New England Journal of
Kaplan executive function system (DKEFS): Medicine, 322(6), 364–369.
Examiner’s manual. San Antonio, TX: The Fastenau, P. S., Johnson, C. S., Perkins, S. M., Byars,
Psychological Corporation. A. W., deGrauw, T. J., Austin, J. K., et al. (2009).
Devinsky, O., Gershengorn, J., Brown, E., Perrine, K., Neuropsychological status at seizure onset in children:
Vazquez, B., & Luciano, D. (1997). Frontal functions Risk factors for early cognitive deficits. Neurology,
in juvenile myoclonic epilepsy. Neuropsychiatry, 73(7), 526–534.
Neuropsychology, and Behavioral Neurology, 10(4), Fedio, P., & Mirsky, A. F. (1969). Selective intellectual
243–246. deficits in children with temporal lobe or
Devinsky, O., Westbrook, L., Cramer, J., Glassman, M., centrencephalic epilepsy. Neuropsychologia, 7,
Perrine, K., & Camfield, C. (1999). Risk factors for 287–300.
poor health-related quality of life in adolescents with Fernandes, M. A., Smith, M. L., Logan, W., Crawley, A.,
epilepsy. Epilepsia, 40(12), 1715–1720. & McAndrews, M. P. (2006). Comparing language
Duchowny, M., Jayakar, P., Harvey, A. S., Resnick, T., lateralization determined by dichotic listening and
Alvarez, L., Dean, P., et al. (1996). Language cortex fMRI activation in frontal and temporal lobes in
representation: Effects of developmental versus children with epilepsy. Brain and Language, 96(1),
acquired pathology. Annals of Neurology, 40(1), 106–114.
31–38. Ferro, M. A., & Speechley, K. N. (2009). Depressive
Dulay, M. F., Levin, H. S., York, M. K., Mizrahi, E. M., symptoms among mothers of children with epilepsy:
Verma, A., Goldsmith, I., et al. (2009). Predictors of A review of prevalence, associated factors, and impact
individual visual memory decline after unilateral on children. Epilepsia, 50(11), 2344–2354.
anterior temporal lobe resection. Neurology, 72(21), Gallagher, A., Theriault, M., Maclin, E., Low, K., Gratton,
1837–1842. G., Fabiani, M., et al. (2007). Near-infrared
Dunn, D. W., Austin, J. K., Harezlak, J., & Ambrosius, W. T. spectroscopy as an alternative to the Wada test for
(2003). ADHD and epilepsy in childhood. Developmental language mapping in children, adults and special pop-
Medicine and Child Neurology, 45(1), 50–54. ulations. Epileptic Disorders, 9(3), 241–255.
Dunn, D. W., Austin, J. K., & Huster, G. A. (1999). Germano, E., Gagliano, A., Magazu, A., Sferro, C.,
Symptoms of depression in adolescents with epilepsy. Calarese, T., Mannarino, E., et al. (2005). Benign
58 W.S. MacAllister and E.M.S. Sherman

childhood epilepsy with occipital paroxysms: longitudinal study in temporal lobe epilepsy. Annals of
Neuropsychological findings. Epilepsy Research, Neurology, 54(4), 425–432.
64(3), 137–150. Hermann, B., Jones, J., Dabbs, K., Allen, C. A., Sheth, R.,
Gioia, G. A., Espy, K. A., & Isquith, P. K. (2003). Behavior Fine, J., et al. (2007). The frequency, complications
rating inventory of executive function: Preschool ver- and aetiology of ADHD in new onset paediatric
sion. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. epilepsy. Brain, 130(Pt 12), 3135–3148.
Gioia, G. A., Isquith, P. K., Guy, S. C., & Kenworthy, L. Hermann, B. P., & Loring, D. W. (2008). Improving neu-
(2000). Behavior rating inventory of executive ropsychological outcomes of epilepsy surgery.
function: Professional manual. Lutz, FL: Epilepsy & Behavior, 13(1), 5–6.
Psychological Assessment Resources. Hermann, B., & Seidenberg, M. (2002). Neuropsychology
Giordani, B., Caveney, A. F., Laughrin, D., Huffman, and temporal lobe epilepsy. CNS Spectrums, 7(5),
J. L., Berent, S., Sharma, U., et al. (2006). Cognition 343–348.
and behavior in children with benign epilepsy with Hermann, B., Seidenberg, M., Bell, B., Rutecki, P., Sheth,
centrotemporal spikes (BECTS). Epilepsy Research, R., Ruggles, K., et al. (2002). The neurodevelopmental
70(1), 89–94. impact of childhood-onset temporal lobe epilepsy on
Gleissner, U., Kuczaty, S., Clusmann, H., Elger, C. E., & brain structure and function. Epilepsia, 43(9),
Helmstaedter, C. (2008). Neuropsychological results 1062–1071.
in pediatric patients with epilepsy surgery in the Hermann, B. P., Seidenberg, M., & Bell, B. (2002b). The
parietal cortex. Epilepsia, 49(4), 700–704. neurodevelopmental impact of childhood onset
Gleissner, U., Sassen, R., Schramm, J., Elger, C. E., & temporal lobe epilepsy on brain structure and function
Helmstaedter, C. (2005). Greater functional recovery and the risk of progressive cognitive effects. Progress
after temporal lobe epilepsy surgery in children. Brain, in Brain Research, 135, 429–438.
128(Pt 12), 2822–2829. Hernandez, M. T., Sauerwein, H. C., Jambaque, I., De
Gonzalez, L. M., Anderson, V. A., Wood, S. J., Mitchell, Guise, E., Lussier, F., Lortie, A., et al. (2002). Deficits
L. A., & Harvey, A. S. (2007). The localization and in executive functions and motor coordination in
lateralization of memory deficits in children with children with frontal lobe epilepsy. Neuropsychologia,
temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsia, 48(1), 124–132. 40(4), 384–400.
Gramstad, A., Engelsen, B. A., & Hugdahl, K. (2003). Hertz-Pannier, L., Lehericy, S., Cordoliani, Y., Le Bihan,
Left hemisphere dysfunction affects dichotic listening D., Marsault, C., & Brunelle, F. (2000). Brain
in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. The functional MRI: Physiological, technical, and
International Journal of Neuroscience, 113(9), methodological bases, and clinical applications. [IRM
1177–1196. fonctionnelle cerebrale: bases physiologiques,
Greenberg, L. M. (1988). T.O.V.A. continuous perfor- techniques et methodologiques, et applications
mance test. Los Alamitos, CA: Universal Attention cliniques]. Journal de Radiologie, 81(6 Suppl),
Disorders. 717–730.
Guimaraes, C. A., Li, L. M., Rzezak, P., Fuentes, D., Hoie, B., Mykletun, A., Sommerfelt, K., Bjornaes, H.,
Franzon, R. C., Augusta Montenegro, M., et al. (2007). Skeidsvoll, H., & Waaler, P. E. (2005). Seizure-related
Temporal lobe epilepsy in childhood: Comprehensive factors and non-verbal intelligence in children with
neuropsychological assessment. Journal of Child epilepsy. A population-based study from western
Neurology, 22(7), 836–840. Norway. Seizure, 14(4), 223–231.
Gulgonen, S., Demirbilek, V., Korkmaz, B., Dervent, A., Hoie, B., Mykletun, A., Waaler, P. E., Skeidsvoll, H., &
& Townes, B. D. (2000). Neuropsychological Sommerfelt, K. (2006). Executive functions and
functions in idiopathic occipital lobe epilepsy. seizure-related factors in children with epilepsy in
Epilepsia, 41(4), 405–411. western Norway. Developmental Medicine and Child
Gunduz, E., Demirbilek, V., & Korkmaz, B. (1999). Neurology, 48(6), 519–525.
Benign rolandic epilepsy: Neuropsychological Holmes, M. D., Quiring, J., & Tucker, D. M. (2010).
findings. Seizure, 8(4), 246–249. Evidence that juvenile myoclonic epilepsy is a
Guzzetta, F. (2006). Cognitive and behavioral outcome in disorder of frontotemporal corticothalamic networks.
West syndrome. Epilepsia, 47(Suppl 2), 49–52. NeuroImage, 49(1), 80–93.
Harrison, P. L., & Oakland, T. (2003). Adaptive behavior Jacobs, R., Harvey, A. S., & Anderson, V. (2007).
assessment system manual (2nd ed.). San Antonio, Executive function following focal frontal lobe
TX: Harcourt Assessment. lesions: Impact of timing of lesion on outcome.
Helmstaedter, C., & Elger, C. E. (2009). Chronic temporal Cortex, 43(6), 792–805.
lobe epilepsy: A neurodevelopmental or progressively Jambaque, I., Dellatolas, G., Dulac, O., Ponsot, G., &
dementing disease? Brain, 132(Pt 10), 2822–2830. Signoret, J. L. (1993). Verbal and visual memory
Helmstaedter, C., & Kockelmann, E. (2006). Cognitive impairment in children with epilepsy.
outcomes in patients with chronic temporal lobe Neuropsychologia, 31(12), 1321–1337.
epilepsy. Epilepsia, 47(Suppl 2), 96–98. Jansen, F. E., Jennekens-Schinkel, A., Van Huffelen,
Helmstaedter, C., Kurthen, M., Lux, S., Reuber, M., & A. C., Van Veelen, W. M., Van Rijen, C. P., Alpherts,
Elger, C. E. (2003). Chronic epilepsy and cognition: A W. C., et al. (2002). Diagnostic significance of Wada
procedure in very young children and children with
2 Evaluation of Children and Adolescents with Epilepsy 59

developmental delay. European Journal of Paediatric Lendt, M., Gleissner, U., Helmstaedter, C., Sassen, R.,
Neurology, 6(6), 315–320. Clusmann, H., & Elger, C. E. (2002).
Janz, D. (1985). Epilepsy with impulsive petit mal (juve- Neuropsychological outcome in children after frontal
nile myoclonic epilepsy). Acta Neurologica lobe epilepsy surgery. Epilepsy & Behavior, 3(1),
Scandinavica, 72(5), 449–459. 51–59.
Kadis, D. S., Kerr, E. N., Rutka, J. T., Snead, O. C., III, Lezak, M. (2004). Neuropsychological assessment (4th
Weiss, S. K., & Smith, M. L. (2009). Pathology type ed.). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
does not predict language lateralization in children Loiseau, P., Duche, B., & Pedespan, J. M. (1995). Absence
with medically intractable epilepsy. Epilepsia, 50(6), epilepsies. Epilepsia, 36(12), 1182–1186.
1498–1504. Loring, D. W., Marino, S., & Meador, K. J. (2007).
Kaplan, E. F., Goodglass, H., & Weintraub, S. (1983). The Neuropsychological and behavioral effects of
Boston naming test (2nd ed.). Philadelphia, PA: Lea antiepilepsy drugs. Neuropsychology Review, 17(4),
and Febiger. 413–425.
Rau, U., Flynn, C., Moreci, et al. (1997). Schedule for Luerding, R., Boesebeck, F., & Ebner, A. (2004).
affective disorders and schizophrenia for school-age Cognitive changes after epilepsy surgery in the
children-present and lifetime version (K-SADS-PL): posterior cortex. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery,
Initial reliability and validity data. Journal of the and Psychiatry, 75(4), 583–587.
American Academy of Child and Adolescent MacAllister, W. S., & Schaffer, S. G. (2007).
Psychiatry, 36(7), 980–988. Neuropsychological deficits in childhood epilepsy
Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. L. (2002). Kaufman syndromes. Neuropsychology Review, 17(4),
assessment battery for children, second edition. Circle 427–444.
Pines, MN: American Guidance Service. Majoie, H. J., Berfelo, M. W., Aldenkamp, A. P., Evers,
Kim, C. H., Lee, S. A., Yoo, H. J., Kang, J. K., & Lee, J. K. S. M., Kessels, A. G., & Renier, W. O. (2001). Vagus
(2007). Executive performance on the Wisconsin card nerve stimulation in children with therapy-resistant
sorting test in mesial temporal lobe epilepsy. European epilepsy diagnosed as Lennox-Gastaut syndrome:
Neurology, 57(1), 39–46. Clinical results, neuropsychological effects, and cost-
Kongs, S. K., Thompson, L. L., Iverson, G. L., & Heaton, effectiveness. Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology,
R. K. (2000). Wisconsin card sorting test—64 card 18(5), 419–428.
version. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Majoie, H. J., Berfelo, M. W., Aldenkamp, A. P., Renier,
Resources. W. O., & Kessels, A. G. (2005). Vagus nerve
Koo, B., Hwang, P. A., & Logan, W. J. (1993). Infantile stimulation in patients with catastrophic childhood
spasms: Outcome and prognostic factors of epilepsy, a 2-year follow-up study. Seizure, 14(1),
cryptogenic and symptomatic groups. Neurology, 10–18.
43(11), 2322–2327. Manford, M., Hart, Y. M., Sander, J. W. A. S., & Shorvon,
Korkman, M., Kirk, U., & Kemp, S. N. (2007). NEPSY-II: S. D. (1992). The national general practice study of
A developmental neuropsychological assessment. San epilepsy: The syndromic classification of the
Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. international league against epilepsy applied to
Kovacs, M. (1985). The children’s depression, inventory epilepsy in a general population. Archives of
(CDI). Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 21(4), Neurology, 49(8), 801–808.
995–998. Martin, R. C., Sawrie, S. M., Edwards, R., Roth, D. L.,
Kramer, U., Nevo, Y., Neufeld, M. Y., Fatal, A., Leitner, Y., Faught, E., Kuzniecky, R. I., et al. (2000). Investigation
& Harel, S. (1998). Epidemiology of epilepsy in child- of executive function change following anterior
hood: A cohort of 440 consecutive patients. Pediatric temporal lobectomy: Selective normalization of verbal
Neurology, 18(1), 46–50. fluency. Neuropsychology, 14(4), 501–508.
Kubu, C. S., Girvin, J. P., McLachlan, R. S., Pavol, M., & Mathews, C. G., & Klove, K. (1964). Instruction manual
Harnadek, M. C. (2000). Does the intracarotid for the adult neuropsychology test battery. Madison,
amobarbital procedure predict global amnesia after WI: University of Wisconsin Medical School.
temporal lobectomy? Epilepsia, 41(10), 1321–1329. Matsumoto, A., Watanabe, K., Negoro, T., Sugiura, M.,
Landau, W. M., & Kleffner, F. R. (1957). Syndrome of Iwase, K., Hara, K., et al. (1981). Infantile spasms:
acquired aphasia with convulsive disorder in children. Etiological factors, clinical aspects, and long term
Neurology, 7(8), 523–530. prognosis in 200 cases. European Journal of
Lawson, J. A., Cook, M. J., Vogrin, S., Litewka, L., Pediatrics, 135(3), 239–244.
Strong, D., Bleasel, A. F., et al. (2002). Clinical, EEG, McVicar, K. A., & Shinnar, S. (2004). Landau-kleffner
and quantitative MRI differences in pediatric frontal syndrome, electrical status epilepticus in slow wave
and temporal lobe epilepsy. Neurology, 58(5), sleep, and language regression in children. Mental
723–729. Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research
Lee, G. (2010). Neuropsychology of epilepsy and epilepsy Reviews, 10(2), 144–149.
surgery. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
60 W.S. MacAllister and E.M.S. Sherman

Miller, B. L., & Cummings, J. L. (2007). The human fron- controlled study. The Journal of Headache and Pain,
tal lobes: Functions and disorders. New York, NY: 11(1), 45–51.
Guilford Press. Parrish, J., Geary, E., Jones, J., Seth, R., Hermann, B., &
Miller, V., Palermo, T. M., & Grewe, S. D. (2003). Quality Seidenberg, M. (2007). Executive functioning in
of life in pediatric epilepsy: Demographic and disease- childhood epilepsy: Parent-report and cognitive
related predictors and comparison with healthy assessment. Developmental Medicine and Child
controls. Epilepsy & Behavior, 4(1), 36–42. Neurology, 49(6), 412–416.
Nabbout, R., & Dulac, O. (2003). Epileptic encephalopa- Pascalicchio, T. F., de Araujo Filho, G. M., da Silva Noffs,
thies: A brief overview. Journal of Clinical M. H., Lin, K., Caboclo, L. O., Vidal-Dourado, M.,
Neurophysiology, 20(6), 393–397. et al. (2007). Neuropsychological profile of patients
Newcomer, P. L., & Hammil, D. (1988). Test of language with juvenile myoclonic epilepsy: A controlled study
development-2. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. of 50 patients. Epilepsy & Behavior, 10(2), 263–267.
Nicolai, J., Aldenkamp, A. P., Arends, J., Weber, J. W., & Patrikelis, P., Angelakis, E., & Gatzonis, S. (2009).
Vles, J. S. (2006). Cognitive and behavioral effects of Neurocognitive and behavioral functioning in frontal
nocturnal epileptiform discharges in children with lobe epilepsy: A review. Epilepsy & Behavior, 14(1),
benign childhood epilepsy with centrotemporal spikes. 19–26.
Epilepsy & Behavior, 8(1), 56–70. Pavone, P., Bianchini, R., Trifiletti, R. R., Incorpora, G.,
Nolan, M. A., Redoblado, M. A., Lah, S., Sabaz, M., Pavone, A., & Parano, E. (2001). Neuropsychological
Lawson, J. A., Cunningham, A. M., et al. (2003). assessment in children with absence epilepsy.
Intelligence in childhood epilepsy syndromes. Neurology, 56(8), 1047–1051.
Epilepsy Research, 53(1–2), 139–150. Pellcock, J. M. (2004). Understanding co-morbidities
Northcott, E., Connolly, A. M., Berroya, A., Sabaz, M., affecting children with epilepsy. Neurology, 62(5
McIntyre, J., Christie, J., et al. (2005). The neuro- Suppl 2), S17–S23.
psychological and language profile of children with Perez, E. R., Davidoff, V., Despland, P. A., & Deonna, T.
benign rolandic epilepsy. Epilepsia, 46(6), (1993). Mental and behavioral deterioration in
924–930. children with epilepsy and CSWS: Acquired epileptic
Oguni, H., Hayashi, K., & Osawa, M. (1996). Long-term frontal syndrome. Developmental Medicine and Child
prognosis of lennox-gastaut syndrome. Epilepsia, Neurology, 35, 661–674.
37(Suppl 3), 44–47. Pinton, F., Ducot, B., Motte, J., Arbues, A. S., Barondiot,
Oguz, A., Kurul, S., & Dirik, E. (2002). Relationship of C., Barthez, M. A., et al. (2006). Cognitive functions
epilepsy-related factors to anxiety and depression in children with benign childhood epilepsy with
scores in epileptic children. Journal of Child centrotemporal spikes (BECTS). Epileptic Disorders,
Neurology, 17(1), 37–40. 8(1), 11–23.
Ojemann, S. G., Berger, M. S., Lettich, E., & Ojemann, Pulsipher, D. T., Seidenberg, M., Guidotti, L., Tuchscherer,
G. A. (2003). Localization of language function in V. N., Morton, J., Sheth, R. D., et al. (2009).
children: Results of electrical stimulation mapping. Thalamofrontal circuitry and executive dysfunction in
Journal of Neurosurgery, 98(3), 465–470. recent-onset juvenile myoclonic epilepsy. Epilepsia,
O’Leary, S. D., Burns, T. G., & Borden, K. A. (2006). 50(5), 1210–1219.
Performance of children with epilepsy and normal Reutens, D. C., & Berkovic, S. F. (1995). Idiopathic gen-
age-matched controls on the WISC-III. Child eralized epilepsy of adolescence: Are the syndromes
Neuropsychology, 12(3), 173–180. clinically distinct? Neurology, 45(8), 1469–1476.
O’Leary, D. S., Lovell, M. R., Sackellares, J. C., Berent, Rey, A. (1941). L’examen psychologique dans les sac
S., Giordani, B., Seidenberg, M., et al. (1983). Effects d’encephalopathie traumatique [psychological
of age of onset of partial and generalized seizures on examination of traumatic encephalopathy]. Archives
neuropsychological performance in children. Journal de Psychologie, 29, 286–340.
of Nervous and Mental Disease, 10, 624–629. Reynolds, E. H. (1983). Mental effects of antiepileptic
Ott, D., Caplan, R., Guthrie, D., Siddarth, P., Komo, S., medication: A review. Epilepsia, 24(Suppl 2),
Shields, W. D., et al. (2001). Measures of psychopa- S85–S95.
thology in children with complex partial seizures and Reynolds, C. R., & Kamphaus, R. W. (2003). Reynolds
primary generalized epilepsy with absence. Journal of intellectual assessment scales. Lutz, FL: PAR.
the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Reynolds, C. R., & Kamphaus, R. W. (2004). Behavior
Psychiatry, 40(8), 907–914. assessment system for children (2nd ed.). Circle Pine,
Papanicolaou, A. C., Simos, P. G., Castillo, E. M., Breier, MN: American Guidance Service.
J. I., Sarkari, S., Pataraia, E., et al. (2004). Reynolds, C., & Richmond, B. (1978). What I think and
Magnetocephalography: A noninvasive alternative to feel: A revised measure of children’s manifest anxiety.
the Wada procedure. Journal of Neurosurgery, 100(5), Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 6, 271–280.
867–876. Reynolds, C. R., & Voress, J. K. (2007). Test of memory
Parisi, P., Verrotti, A., Paolino, M. C., Urbano, A., and learning (2nd ed.). Austin, TX: ProEd.
Bernabucci, M., Castaldo, R., et al. (2010). Headache Riccio, C. A., & Reynolds, C. R. (2001). Continuous per-
and cognitive profile in children: A cross-sectional formance tests are sensitive to ADHD in adults but
2 Evaluation of Children and Adolescents with Epilepsy 61

lack specificity. A review and critique for differential and behavioral status of children with complex partial
diagnosis. Annals of the New York Academy of seizures. Developmental Medicine and Child
Sciences, 931, 113–139. Neurology, 41(11), 724–731.
Riccio, C. A., Reynolds, C. R., & Lowe, P. A. (2001). Schouten, D., Hendriksen, J. G. M., & Aldenkamp, A.
Clinical application of continuous performance tests: (2009). Performance of children with epilepsy on the
Measuring attention and impulsive responding in Rey-Osterrieth complex figure test: Is there an effect
children and adults. New York, NY: John Wiley & of localization or lateralization? Epilepsy Research,
Sons. 83(2–3), 184–189.
Riva, D., Saletti, V., Nichelli, F., & Bulgheroni, S. (2002). Schubert, R. (2005). Attention deficit disorder and epi-
Neuropsychologic effects of frontal lobe epilepsy in lepsy. Pediatric Neurology, 32(1), 1–10.
children. Journal of Child Neurology, 17(9), Schwarz, M., Pauli, E., & Stefan, H. (2005). Model based
661–667. prognosis of postoperative object naming in left
Roid, G. H. (2003). Stanford-Binet intelligence scales— temporal lobe epilepsy. Seizure, 14(8), 562–568.
Fifth edition. Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing. Semel, E., Wiig, E. H., & Secord, W. A. (2003). Clinical
Ronen, G. M., Streiner, D. L., Rosenbaum, P., & Canadian evaluation of language fundamentals (4th ed.). San
Pediatric Epilepsy Network. (2003). Health-related Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
quality of life in children with epilepsy: Development Sherman, E. M. (2009). Maximizing quality of life in
and validation of self-report and parent proxy people living with epilepsy. The Canadian Journal of
measures. Epilepsia, 44(4), 598–612. Neurological Sciences, 36(Suppl 2), S17–S24.
Rotenberg, J., & Pearl, P. L. (2003). Landau-Kleffner syn- Sherman, E. M., Slick, D. J., Connolly, M. B., Steinbok,
drome. Archives of Neurology, 60(7), 1019–1021. P., Camfield, C., Eyrl, K. L., et al. (2002). Validity of
Rzezak, P., Fuentes, D., Guimaraes, C. A., Thome-Souza, three measures of health-related quality of life in
S., Kuczynski, E., Guerreiro, M., et al. (2009). children with intractable epilepsy. Epilepsia, 43(10),
Executive dysfunction in children and adolescents 1230–1238.
with temporal lobe epilepsy: Is the Wisconsin card Sherman, E. M., Slick, D. J., & Eyrl, K. L. (2006).
sorting test enough? Epilepsy & Behavior, 15(3), Executive dysfunction is a significant predictor of
376–381. poor quality of life in children with epilepsy. Epilepsia,
Rzezak, P., Fuentes, D., Guimaraes, C. A., Thome-Souza, 47(11), 1936–1942.
S., Kuczynski, E., Li, L. M., et al. (2007). Frontal lobe Sheslow, D., & Adams, W. (2003). Wide range assessment
dysfunction in children with temporal lobe epilepsy. of memory and learning, second edition administration
Pediatric Neurology, 37(3), 176–185. and technical manual. Wilmington, DE: Wide Range.
Sabaz, M., Cairns, D., Bleasel, A., Lawson, J., Grinton, Sidenvall, R., Forsgren, L., Blomquist, H. K., & Heijbel,
B., Scheffer, I., et al. (2003). The health-related quality J. (1993). A community-based prospective incidence
of life of childhood epilepsy syndromes. Journal of study of epileptic seizures in children. Acta
Paediatrics and Child Health, 39(9), 690–696. Paediatrica, 82(1), 60–65.
Sabaz, M., Cairns, D. R., Lawson, J. A., Bleasel, A. F., & Sillanpaa, M., Haataja, L., & Shinnar, S. (2004). Perceived
Bye, A. M. (2001). The health-related quality of life of impact of childhood-onset epilepsy on quality of life
children with refractory epilepsy: A comparison of as an adult. Epilepsia, 45(8), 971–977.
those with and without intellectual disability. Singhi, P. D., Bansal, U., Singhi, S., & Pershad, D. (1992).
Epilepsia, 42(5), 621–628. Determinants of IQ profile in children with idiopathic
Sabaz, M., Cairns, D. R., Lawson, J. A., Nheu, N., Bleasel, generalized epilepsy. Epilepsia, 33(6), 1106–1114.
A. F., & Bye, A. M. (2000). Validation of a new quality Slick, D. J., Lautzenhiser, A., Sherman, E. M., & Eyrl, K.
of life measure for children with epilepsy. Epilepsia, (2006). Frequency of scale elevations and factor struc-
41(6), 765–774. ture of the behavior rating inventory of executive func-
Saltzman-Benaiah, J., Scott, K., & Smith, M. L. (2003). tion (BRIEF) in children and adolescents with
Factors associated with atypical speech representation intractable epilepsy. Child Neuropsychology, 12(3),
in children with intractable epilepsy. Neuropsychologia, 181–189.
41(14), 1967–1974. Smith, M. L., Elliott, I. M., & Lach, L. (2002). Cognitive
Sandford, J. A., & Turner, A. (1995). Manual for the inte- skills in children with intractable epilepsy: Comparison
grated visual and auditory continuous performance of surgical and nonsurgical candidates. Epilepsia,
test. Richmond, VA: Brain Train. 43(6), 631–637.
Schevon, C. A., Carlson, C., Zaroff, C. M., Weiner, H. J., Sparrow, S., Balla, D., & Cicchetti, D. (1984). Vineland
Doyle, W. K., Miles, D., et al. (2007). Pediatric adaptive behavioral scales. Circle Pines, MN:
language mapping: Sensitivity of neurostimulation American Guidance Service.
and Wada testing in epilepsy surgery. Epilepsia, 48(3), Strauss, S., Sherman, E. M. S., & Spreen, O. (2006). A
539–545. compendium of neuropsychological tests:
Schoenfeld, J., Seidenberg, M., Woodard, A., Hecox, K., Administration, norms, and commentary (3rd ed.).
Inglese, C., Mack, K., et al. (1999). Neuropsychological New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
62 W.S. MacAllister and E.M.S. Sherman

Stroup, E., Langfitt, J., Berg, M., McDermott, M., Pilcher, loss of consciousness in absences. Epilepsia, 41(6),
W., & Como, P. (2003). Predicting verbal memory 687–693.
decline following anterior temporal lobectomy (ATL). Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1999).
Neurology, 60(8), 1266–1273. The comprehensive test of phonological processing:
Stuss, D. T., & Knight, R. T. (2002). Principles of frontal Examiner’s manual. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
lobe function. New York, NY: Oxford University Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler abbreviated scale of intel-
Press. ligence. New York, NY: Psychological Corporation.
Suchy, Y., Sands, K., & Chelune, G. J. (2003). Verbal and Wechsler, D. (2002). Wechsler primary and preschool
nonverbal fluency performance before and after sei- scale of intelligence—Third edition. San Antonio, TX:
zure surgery. Journal of Clinical and Experimental The Psychological Corporation.
Neuropsychology, 25(2), 190–200. Wechsler, D. (2003). Wechsler intelligence scale for chil-
Sveinbjornsdottir, S., & Duncan, J. S. (1993). Parietal and dren—Fourth edition. New York, NY: The
occipital lobe epilepsy: A review. Epilepsia, 34(3), Psychological Corporation.
493–521. Williams, J., Phillips, T., Griebel, M. L., Sharp, G. B.,
Swanson, J., Schuck, S., Mann, M., Carlson, C., Hartman, Lange, B., Edgar, T., et al. (2001). Patterns of memory
K., Sergeant, J., & McCleary, R. (2006). Categorical performance in children with controlled epilepsy on
and dimensional definitions and evaluations of symp- the CVLT-C. Child Neuropsychology, 7(1), 15–20.
toms of ADHD: The SNAP and SWAN Rating Scales. Williams, J., Sharp, G., Bates, S., Griebel, M., Lange, B.,
University of California, Irvine. Spence, G. T., et al. (1996). Academic achievement
Tassinari, C. A., Rubboli, G., Volpi, L., Meletti, S., d’Orsi, and behavioral ratings in children with absence and
G., Franca, M., et al. (2000). Encephalopathy with complex partial epilepsy. Education and Treatment of
electrical status epilepticus during slow sleep or ESES Children, 19(2), 143–152.
syndrome including the acquired aphasia. Clinical Wirrell, E. C. (2003). Natural history of absence epilepsy
Neurophysiology, 111(Suppl 2), S94–S102. in children. The Canadian Journal of Neurological
Vanasse, C. M., Beland, R., Carmant, L., & Lassonde, M. Sciences, 30(3), 184–188.
(2005). Impact of childhood epilepsy on reading and Wirrell, E., Farrell, K., & Whiting, S. (2005). The epilep-
phonological processing abilities. Epilepsy & tic encephalopathies of infancy and childhood. The
Behavior, 7(2), 288–296. Canadian Journal of Neurological Sciences, 32(4),
Varni, J. W., Seid, M., & Rode, C. A. (1999). The PedsQL: 409–418.
Measurement model for the pediatric quality of life Wood, L. J., Sherman, E. M., Hamiwka, L. D., Blackman,
inventory. Medical Care, 37(2), 126–139. M. A., & Wirrell, E. C. (2008). Maternal depression:
Volkl-Kernstock, S., Bauch-Prater, S., Ponocny-Seliger, The cost of caring for a child with intractable epilepsy.
E., & Feucht, M. (2009). Speech and school perfor- Pediatric Neurology, 39(6), 418–422.
mance in children with benign partial epilepsy with Woodcock, R. W., McGrew, K. S., & Mather, N. (2001).
centro-temporal spikes (BCECTS). Seizure, 18(5), Woodcock-Johnson III tests of cognitive abilities.
320–326. Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing.
Volkl-Kernstock, S., Willinger, U., & Feucht, M. (2006). Yoo, H. K., Park, S., Wang, H. R., Lee, J. S., Kim, K.,
Spacial perception and spatial memory in children Paik, K. W., et al. (2009). Effect of methylphenidate
with benign childhood epilepsy with centro-temporal on the quality of life in children with epilepsy and
spikes (BCECTS). Epilepsy Research, 72(1), 39–48. attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: And open-
Vuilleumier, P., Assal, F., Blanke, O., & Jallon, P. (2000). label study using an osmotic-controlled release oral
Distinct behavioral and EEG topographic correlates of delivery system. Epileptic Disorders, 11(4), 301–308.
Geriatric Patients with Epilepsy
3
Jessica Chapin and Richard Naugle

symptoms between, and co-occurrence of, these


Introduction two disorders.
This chapter provides the clinician seeing
Elderly patients with epilepsy share many char- elderly patients information about the presenta-
acteristics with their younger cohort, but there tion of epilepsy in this population, including
are important differences between these groups characteristics of epilepsy, cognitive functioning,
that are relevant for the neuropsychologist work- psychosocial considerations, and treatment alter-
ing with older adults. Elderly patients with epi- natives. This is followed by practical consider-
lepsy have unique risk factors for comorbidities ations when completing a neuropsychological
and complications that put them at increased risk evaluation with an elderly epilepsy patient,
of neurocognitive sequelae. The medical, cogni- including interview, test selection, result inter-
tive, and psychosocial characteristics of this pretation, and recommendations. Finally, a case
group often result in different referral questions is presented that illustrates these unique consid-
and approaches for the development of clinical erations in an elderly patient with epilepsy.
hypotheses, selection of tests, interpretation of In this chapter, “elderly” will be used to indi-
results, and provision of recommendations. cate individuals aged 60 years and older. We
Because the prevalence and incidence of epi- acknowledge that this is a fairly young point at
lepsy are greatest in those over age 60, neuro- which to start applying this descriptor, but this age
psychologists who conduct evaluations with range is not completely arbitrary, since the inci-
adults with epilepsy are likely to be asked to dence of unprovoked epilepsy sharply increases
assess these patients. It is also important for after age 60 (Hauser, Annegers, et al., 1993), and
neuropsychologists conducting dementia evalu- age-related physiological changes that complicate
ations to understand the interface between pharmacokinetics and pharmacodynamics of anti-
dementia and epilepsy because of the overlap in epileptic drugs (AEDs) are often evident by age
65. Incidence of relevant medical comorbidities
such as stroke increases after about age 55 years
J. Chapin (*) (Thorvaldsen, Asplund, et al., 1995). Studies on
Aurora Health Care, 215 W. Washington Street,
older adults in the literature generally define the
Grafton, WI, 53024, USA
e-mail: jessica.chapin@aurora.org elderly as over 55 or 60. Because of the above
findings, and to maintain consistency with existing
R. Naugle, Ph.D., A.B.P.P.
Neurological Institute, Cleveland Clinic, 9500 Euclid literature, this chapter defines the elderly as those
Avenue/P57, Cleveland, OH, 44195, USA aged 60 and older unless stated otherwise.

W.B. Barr and C. Morrison (eds.), Handbook on the Neuropsychology of Epilepsy, 63


Clinical Handbooks in Neuropsychology, DOI 10.1007/978-0-387-92826-5_3,
© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015
64 J. Chapin and R. Naugle

sector of the population in the industrialized


Background world, the number of elderly people with epi-
lepsy will continue to rise.
Prevalence and Incidence of Epilepsy
in Elderly Patients
Unique Characteristics of Epilepsy
Epilepsy has an increased prevalence and inci- in Elderly Patients
dence in elderly patients, as suggested by studies
from several countries (e.g., de la Court, Breteler, Types of Seizures
et al., 1996; Hauser, Annegers, et al., 1991; While the incidence of both focal and general-
Wallace, Shorvon, et al., 1998). Data from the ized unprovoked epilepsy increases in older
Rochester Epidemiology Project collected from adults, the most dramatic increase is in focal epi-
1935 to 1984 revealed a relatively high incidence lepsy (Hauser, Annegers, et al., 1993). In those
of “unprovoked” epilepsy (i.e., recurrent seizures over age 65, focal seizures with alteration of con-
with no identified immediate precipitant) during sciousness are the most frequent seizure type
the first year of life (82/100,000) that declined (48 %), followed by generalized (29 %) and focal
throughout childhood and remained generally seizures without alteration of consciousness
low until it started rising again at age 55–60, (13 %) (Hauser, 1992). In elderly patients with
peaking in those aged 75 years and older chronic rather than new-onset epilepsy, seizures
(139/100,000; Hauser, Annegers, et al., 1993; may become briefer and less elaborate over time,
Hauser, Annegers, et al., 1996). See Fig. 3.1 for a and generalized tonic-clonic seizures may
graphical representation of these results. become less frequent or even disappear (Tinuper,
Likewise, prevalence of active epilepsy steadily Provini, et al., 1996). Whereas focal seizures
increased over adulthood and reportedly affected most often arise from the temporal lobe in the
approximately 1 % of the population over 75 general population, these seizures in elderly
years of age (de la Court, Breteler, et al., 1996; patients often originate from extratemporal or
Hauser, Annegers, et al., 1996). Because older frontal regions frequently affected by stroke
individuals make up the most rapidly growing (Ramsay, Rowan, et al., 2004).

Fig. 3.1 Age-specific incidence


of generalized-onset (solid circles)
and partial-onset (plus signs)
unprovoked epilepsies based on
data collected in the Rochester
Epidemiology Project from 1935
to 1984 (Hauser et al., 1993) Used
with permission from Epilepsia
3 Geriatric Patients with Epilepsy 65

Etiology of Seizures (as described in Ramsay, Rowan, et al. (2004)).


Cerebrovascular disease is the most common In patients with TIAs or strokes, the diagnosis of
non-idiopathic etiology of new-onset epilepsy in seizures was almost always delayed. It took an
those aged 65 and older, accounting for 28 % of average of 1.7 years before correct diagnosis was
cases (Hauser, Annegers, et al., 1993). made in this sample. This diagnostic difficulty is
Approximately 20 % of cases were attributed to based, in part, on the limited knowledge of sei-
degenerative diseases, and less than 5 % were zure semiology in the elderly, the reduced fre-
related to CNS tumors, trauma, or infection. quency of interictal discharges, the variety of
Etiology remains unknown in 25–50 % (Hauser, EEG patterns seen, and the increased incidence
Annegers, et al., 1993; Ramsay, Rowan, et al., of other conditions that may mimic seizures in
2004). See Fig. 3.2 for a comparison of etiologies this population (Van Cott, 2002). Further, it may
of newly diagnosed epilepsy across the life span. be difficult to understand the presence or charac-
teristics of spells in elderly individuals because
Diagnostic Complications patients are often more socially isolated (i.e., are
Diagnosing epilepsy in elderly patients can be more likely to live alone, be unemployed, have
complicated, and there is increased risk of both fewer social activities) and may have more mem-
over- and underdiagnosis in this group. It is ory problems than their younger cohort.
important for neuropsychologists conducting Because seizures tend to originate from extra-
evaluations with older adults to keep this in mind, temporal foci more often among elderly patients,
since presenting symptoms may reflect undiag- they are less likely to exhibit the typical clinical
nosed seizures. Older adults who presented with manifestations characteristic of temporal lobe
tonic-clonic seizures were correctly diagnosed seizures (Ramsay & Pryor, 2000). Seizure symp-
66.7 % of the time, although only 25.4 % of those toms are often nonspecific and may include
with focal seizures without alteration of con- altered mentation, staring, unresponsiveness,
sciousness received an initial correct diagnosis blackouts, and auras of dizziness (Ramsay &

Fig. 3.2 Proportion of cases of


newly diagnosed epilepsy
assigned to specific etiologic
categories within age groups,
including idiopathic/cryptogenic
category. Area: idiopathic (gray
cross-hatched), congenital
(dashed), trauma (dotted), trauma
(widely dotted), infection
(hatched), cerebrovascular
(closely dotted), tumor (black),
degenerative (light cross-hatch)
66 J. Chapin and R. Naugle

Pryor, 2000). Postictal confusion may persist for Table 3.1 Main differential diagnosis of seizures in elderly
patients
several days in older patients, compared to min-
utes in younger patients (Cloyd, Hauser, et al., Neurological
2006; Sheth, Drazkowski, et al., 2006). – Transient ischemic attack
These nonspecific symptoms make seizures – Transient global amnesia
more difficult to diagnose based on description – Migraine
– Restless leg syndrome
alone. Several types of disorders or events may
– Dyskinesia
mimic seizures in elderly patients. Focal seizures
Cardiovascular
with alteration of consciousness may be mistaken
– Vasovagal syncope
for TIAs or other cardiovascular disease, syn-
– Orthostatic hypotension
cope, dementia, arrhythmias, or fluctuations in – Cardiac arrhythmias
blood pressure or blood sugar levels – Structural heart disease
(Sheorajpanday & De Deyn, 2007). The most – Carotid sinus syndrome
common initial diagnoses in patients in the VACS Endocrine/metabolic
428 study whose diagnoses were later confirmed – Hypoglycemia
as epilepsy included altered mental status – Hypocalcemia
(41.8 %), confusion (37.5 %), blackout spells – Hypomagnesemia
(29.3 %), and syncope (16.8 %) (Brodie & Kwan, Sleep disorders
2005; Ramsay, Rowan, et al., 2004). See Table 3.1 – Obstructive sleep apnea
for the primary differential diagnosis of seizures – Narcolepsy
in elderly patients. – Rapid eye movement sleep disorders
Status epilepticus (SE) often presents with no – Hypnic jerks
convulsive activity in elderly patients and may Psychological
– Nonepileptic psychogenic seizures
appear simply as confusion or minimal motor
movements. In a prospective study, nonconvul- Adapted from (Brodie & Kwan, 2005)
sive SE was diagnosed in 16 % of elderly patients
presenting with confusion of unknown origin
(Baxendale, 1998). Thus, SE may go undiag- indicating a seizure tendency (Van Cott, 2002).
nosed for several days in ambulatory elderly Therefore, video-EEG monitoring may be valu-
patients with ictal confusion (Sheth, Drazkowski, able in establishing the diagnosis (Brodie &
et al., 2006). This is concerning since even after it Kwan, 2005; McBride, Shih, et al., 2002; Van
is treated, SE can result in persistent cognitive Cott, 2002).
dysfunction. Further, SE is more common and
has a higher morbidity rate in elderly patients
(DeLorenzo, Hauser, et al., 1996). Cognition in Elderly Patients
Finally, further complicating an epilepsy with Epilepsy
diagnosis, EEG is less sensitive and specific for
epilepsy in elderly patients (Brodie & Kwan, Effect of Age on Cognition in Epilepsy
2005). Absence of interictal epileptiform activity Epilepsy is associated with cognitive impair-
on routine EEG does not rule out the diagnosis, ment, the cause of which is often multifactorial
since its presence decreases with age and only and may include underlying seizure etiology,
occurs in 35 % of patients with preexisting epi- ictal and interictal neuronal discharges, AED side
lepsy (mean age = 65) and 26 % of elderly effects, and psychosocial confounds (Kwan &
patients with new-onset seizures (mean age = 70) Brodie, 2001). This is no different in elderly
(Drury & Beydoun, 1998). Conversely, benign patients with epilepsy (Caramelli & Castro,
EEG changes are associated with normal aging 2005) and several factors may increase the risk of
and have the potential to be misinterpreted as cognitive dysfunction in this group.
3 Geriatric Patients with Epilepsy 67

First, there is a subset of elderly patients with 2.89 times the risk of experiencing a first ever
epilepsy who have had epilepsy for many years. stroke compared to an elderly control group
There is evidence for greater cognitive morbid- (Cleary, Shorvon, et al., 2004). Epilepsy has
ity when epilepsy has been long-standing also been associated with increased inflamma-
(Helmstaedter, Kurthen, et al., 2003; Hermann, tory markers, both through the effects of sei-
Seidenberg, et al., 2006). zures themselves (Vezzani & Granata, 2005) as
Second, even “healthy” aging is associated well as AED effects (Verrotti, Basciani, et al.,
with decreased processing speed and fluid intel- 2001). Finally, Hermann (2008) describes life-
ligence, likely resulting from neurophysiological style factors which are both associated with
changes including loss of synapses, neurons, poor cognitive aging and epilepsy, including
neurotransmitters, and neuronal networks (Fillit, decreased social networks and physical activity
Butler, et al., 2002). Furthermore, normal aging (Bjorholt, Nakken, et al., 1990; Nakken, 1999).
often results in mild levels of cerebral atrophy, Although the above factors raise concern that
ventricular enlargement, hippocampal atrophy, elderly patients with epilepsy may be at greater
and deposition of beta-amyloid peptide and neu- risk of cognitive dysfunction than younger
rofibrillary tangles (Smith & Rush, 2006). The patients with epilepsy or older individuals with-
presence of epilepsy potentially exacerbates out epilepsy, there has been little research pub-
these changes. lished in this area. Older adults with chronic
Third, both aging and epilepsy have been epilepsy performed more poorly across most
associated with increased incidence of dementia cognitive measures compared to healthy older
and other disorders and lifestyle factors that can controls, both in a sample of medically intracta-
cause cognitive dysfunction (Hermann, ble patients (Martin, Griffith, et al., 2005) and a
Seidenberg, et al., 2008b). Compared to popula- sample in which 63 % were successfully con-
tion-based controls, individuals with epilepsy trolled with medications (Piazzini, Canevini,
had an increased relative risk of being diagnosed et al., 2006). Griffith and colleagues (2006) found
with Alzheimer’s disease at least 1 year after that memory deficits in older adults with chronic
epilepsy diagnosis, with relative risk values epilepsy were similar to deficits in patients with
ranging from 1.2 to 4.0 (Breteler, van Duijn, amnestic mild cognitive impairment, and the
et al., 1991). Hermann (2008b) makes a compel- patients with epilepsy had greater difficulty on
ling case that chronic epilepsy has been associ- measures of executive functioning. Further sug-
ated with several risk factors for poorer cognitive gestive of memory impairment in this group,
aging. As a group, those with chronic epilepsy elderly patients with chronic epilepsy did not
have greater vascular risk factors, including demonstrate practice effects on a measure of ver-
more ischemic heart disease, hypertension, bal memory at 2–3-year follow-up compared to
heart failure, diabetes, and cerebrovascular dis- healthy elderly controls tested over the same
ease (Gaitatzis, Carroll, et al., 2004; Tellez- interval (Griffith, Martin, et al., 2007), suggest-
Zenteno, Matijevic, et al., 2005). This may be ing that elderly epilepsy may be associated with
partially attributable to side effects (e.g., meta- failure to learn compared to healthy elderly
bolic disorders, increased homocysteine) asso- subjects.
ciated with select AEDs such as valproic acid Although these studies support cognitive mor-
and enzyme-inducing medications (Hamed & bidity associated with epilepsy in elderly patients,
Nabeshima, 2005; Isojarvi, Rattya, et al., 1998; no study has directly investigated if this cognitive
Luef, Waldmann, et al., 2004; Ono, Sakamoto, dysfunction is any greater than that experienced
et al., 1997; Pylvanen, Knip, et al., 2003; in younger adults with epilepsy, and evidence for
Schwaninger, Ringleb, et al., 2000; Sheth, accelerated cognitive decline associated with
2004). Elderly patients with epilepsy who had epilepsy in older adults is mixed. A recent cross-
no preexisting cerebrovascular disease were at sectional study by Helmstaedter and Elger (2009)
68 J. Chapin and R. Naugle

suggested against accelerated deterioration of the above comorbidities are more likely to start
episodic memory with older age in patients with showing effects.
TLE. Examination of verbal learning and mem-
ory in 1,156 patients with TLE and 1,000 con- Epilepsy and Dementia
trols over the life span revealed that the slow There is evidence for a bidirectional relationship
linear decline in verbal learning exhibited by nor- between epilepsy and dementia. It is well estab-
mal controls after age 25 was mirrored in patients lished that those with dementia are at increased
with TLE (although at a much lower level). Thus, risk of developing epilepsy, and there is also
although the TLE group had a lower level of per- some evidence that those with epilepsy may be at
formance, the rate of decline was the same increased risk of developing dementia. Patients
between the groups. There was no relative with dementia have a five- to tenfold increase in
decrease in memory in the TLE group compared risk of seizures (Hesdorffer, Hauser, et al., 1996).
to controls with advancing age. However, Most of this research has been conducted in
although patients ranged in age from 6 to 70 Alzheimer’s disease (AD), although there are a
years, most were 50 years and younger, with only few studies reporting the presence of seizures in
17 patients over 60 years of age. dementia with Lewy bodies (Weiner, Hynan,
Conversely, there are other data supporting the et al., 2003) and Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease
possibility of age-accelerated cognitive decline (Marchiori, Yasuda, et al., 1996). It is estimated
in select patients with epilepsy. Another cross- that between 10 and 22 % of patients with AD
sectional study by this same group suggested an will experience at least one seizure (Mendez &
age-accelerated decline in the retention of audi- Lim, 2003). The incidence of seizures increases
tory information in preoperative patients who had with the severity of the dementia (Hesdorffer,
temporal lobe epilepsy not confined to mesial Hauser, et al., 1996; McAreavey, Ballinger, et al.,
temporal sclerosis (e.g., normal imaging or other 1992; Mendez & Lim, 2003), with seizure onset
lesions such as tumor) (Helmstaedter, Reuber, often 7 years after dementia diagnoses (Mendez,
et al., 2002). Further, in a longitudinal study, Catanzaro, et al., 1994). However, seizures can
Hermann et al. (2006) found that older age was begin at any time during the course of the illness
one of the several predictors of decline in simple (Hauser, Morris, et al., 1986), even as early as 3
and complex psychomotor speed, but not other months after diagnosis (Hesdorffer, Hauser,
cognitive domains including memory, relative to et al., 1996). The mechanism causing seizures in
expected values over a 4-year period in patients Alzheimer’s disease remains unknown, but sus-
with chronic temporal lobe epilepsy. However, pected factors include the accumulation of
patients were relatively young in both of the amyloid-β plaques, neurofibrillary tangles, selec-
above studies, with the average age in the low 30s. tive loss of inhibitory neurons, comorbid vascular
In summary, there are several unique risk fac- lesions, and excessive neuronal cell loss in hip-
tors for cognitive morbidity in older adults with pocampal and parietal cortices (Forstl, Burns,
epilepsy. The few published studies examining et al., 1992; Mendez, Catanzaro, et al., 1994;
the relationships among epilepsy, aging, and cog- Mendez & Lim, 2003). The onset of seizures has
nition suggest that older adults with epilepsy been associated with a faster progression of cog-
have more cognitive dysfunction than older nitive and functional impairment in patients with
adults without epilepsy and younger adults with AD (Volicer, Smith, et al., 1995).
epilepsy. It is unclear if this is due to additive There is also evidence that those with epilepsy
or interactive effects of epilepsy and aging. may be at increased risk of developing a progres-
However, most participants in these studies have sive dementia. Breteler et al. (1991) reviewed
been under 60 years old. More research is needed four studies that examined the relative risk of a
to determine the relationship between epilepsy subsequent diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease in
and cognition as patients reach older age when those with an epilepsy diagnosis. Diagnoses were
3 Geriatric Patients with Epilepsy 69

based on interview with an informant in three patients. However, no patient showed any evi-
studies and medical record review in the other. dence of dementia on cognitive testing, and no
Compared to population-based controls, individ- other AD-related pathology was identified.
uals with epilepsy had an increased relative risk Postoperative follow-up (mean 3.7 years, range
of being diagnosed with AD at least 1 year after 2–7) of the ten patients with senile plaques
epilepsy diagnosis (relative risk values ranging revealed no clinical suggestion of dementia, sug-
from 1.2 to 4.0). The reason for the increased risk gesting that the senile plaques were not associ-
of subsequent AD diagnosis in those with epi- ated with dementia or cognitive deterioration for
lepsy is unknown, although data from this study at least several years (Mackenzie, McLachlan,
suggested that cumulative effects of long- et al., 1996). Overall, these findings suggest that
standing seizures did not appear to be a factor in TLE is associated with increased formation of
the development of dementia. The greatest risk senile plaques, but these plaques do not have an
for a diagnosis of AD was in patients who had apparent effect on cognition, including the devel-
epilepsy less than 10 years (relative risk = 2.4) opment of dementia.
versus 10 years or more (relative risk = 1.4).
Alternatively, seizures may represent early path-
ological dementia-related changes in these Psychosocial Considerations
patients, or the presence of both seizures and in Elderly Patients with Epilepsy
dementia may reflect shared risk factors. In a
follow-up study, Breteler et al. (1995) found that Epilepsy can have profound effects on mood,
patients diagnosed with epilepsy had a relative anxiety, and quality of life (QOL). Elderly
risk of 1.5 for being diagnosed with dementia patients may be particularly vulnerable to these
over the next 8 years compared to other hospital symptoms because they often live alone and may
patients. In an investigation of all patients diag- have additional physical and cognitive vulnera-
nosed with probable AD by a neurologist over a bilities that put them at increased risk for loss of
6-year period, 6.8 % had a history of epilepsy independence.
and/or were taking AEDs at the time of diagnosis The concern for worse QOL in the elderly com-
(Lozsadi & Larner, 2006). In half of these cases, pared to younger patients with epilepsy has not
seizure onset occurred at about the same time as been born out in research findings. Comparisons
the onset of cognitive decline, with no identified between QOL measures in the elderly and younger
acute cause of the seizures identified. Data from adults suggest similar QOL in these groups (Baker,
the Canadian Study on Health and Aging found Jacoby, et al., 2001; Laccheo, Ablah, et al., 2008)
that a diagnosis of epilepsy in those aged 65 years or that older patients may even cope better with
and older had a relative risk of 1.56 for being epilepsy than middle-aged peers (Pugh, Copeland,
diagnosed with dementia over the next 5 years et al., 2005). A potential confounding factor is that
compared to community-dwelling controls with- QOL measures are generally developed and
out epilepsy, although this did not meet statistical normed for those under 65 years of age. When
significance (Carter, Weaver, et al., 2007). Martin and colleagues (2005) gave a group of
Another way to determine whether epilepsy community-dwelling elderly adults a blank paper
increases the risk of dementia is to compare brain to list their concerns regarding living with epi-
tissue from epilepsy patients with tissue from lepsy, results were similar in content to concerns
patients and unaffected, age-matched controls. voiced by younger epilepsy patients and included
Mackenzie and Miller (1994) compared the num- driving/transportation (64 %), medication side
ber and location of senile plaques in temporal effects (64 %), personal safety (39 %), AED costs
lobe tissue from epilepsy patients and normal (29 %), employment (26 %), social embarrass-
controls. The age-related incidence of senile ment (21 %), and memory loss (21 %). Thus,
plaques was significantly higher in epilepsy the elderly with epilepsy may not have worse
70 J. Chapin and R. Naugle

perceived QOL than younger patients but do report There is also little known about the most
lower QOL than the general population (Laccheo, effective treatment for depression and other men-
Ablah, et al., 2008) or older adults without epi- tal health disorders in the elderly with epilepsy.
lepsy (McLaughlin et al. 2008). Cognitive behavioral and interpersonal are two
Depression and anxiety rates are higher in therapy modalities that have received the most
patients with epilepsy in general, and this has empirical support for treatment of depression
been shown to extend to the elderly with epilepsy (Barlow, 2001), although these studies have not
(Haut, Katz, et al., 2009). Further, many elderly specifically examined the elderly with epilepsy.
people have been found to suffer from “subsyn- Referral to a psychiatrist with specialized knowl-
dromal” depression, in which they report signifi- edge in treating emotional distress in those with
cant depressive symptomatology but fail to meet epilepsy is often very helpful. The patient’s AED
formal diagnostic criteria for major depression regimen should also be examined, since select
(Strober & Arnett, 2009). Mental health disorders AEDs have been associated with adverse mood
can be more difficult to diagnose in elderly side effects, most notably those such as pheno-
patients with a neurological disorder compared to barbital, primidone, vigabatrin, topiramate, and
younger, otherwise healthy, individuals due to levetiracetam (Frey, 2007). Finally, stressing
several factors including atypical presentation, medication compliance is especially important in
difficulty distinguishing between symptoms of this group, since depressed elderly medical
the mental health disease and epilepsy, and lack of patients were found to be less compliant with
assessment tools developed specifically for this medications than nondepressed elderly medical
population (Strober & Arnett, 2009). Nevertheless, patients (Carney, Freedland, et al., 1995).
it is often the responsibility of the neuropsycholo-
gist to determine whether or not a patient has a
mental health disorder that requires treatment. It Treatment of Epilepsy in Elderly
is important to understand unique characteristics Patients
of the presentation and treatment of mental health
problems in the elderly with epilepsy. It has been AEDs
suggested that depression in elderly individuals Epilepsy is more frequently controlled with
may present with more weight loss and fewer AEDs in patients aged 65 years and over com-
feelings of worthlessness and guilt than younger pared to younger patients (Mohanraj & Brodie,
people (Frey, 2007), and depressive symptoms in 2006). After starting AEDs for newly diagnosed
patients with epilepsy may present as intermittent epilepsy, approximately 80 % of patients aged 65
irritability, lack of energy, anxiety, and somatic years and older remained seizure-free at 1-year
symptoms such as pain (i.e., interictal dysphoric follow-up (Brodie & Kwan, 2005).
disorder). Many of the commonly used self-report Unfortunately, elderly patients are also gener-
inventories for depression do not have established ally more susceptible to the adverse effects of
cutoff scores for identifying depression in the drugs than younger patients. Overall, AED side
elderly with epilepsy, although a recent review of effects are more pronounced in elderly patients,
depression assessment in the elderly with neuro- there are more adverse drug interactions, and risk
logical disorders summarized the recommended of toxicity is greater (Sheorajpanday & De Deyn,
cutoff scores for common measures when used 2007). It is important for the neuropsychologist
with neurological populations (Strober & Arnett, to be aware of situations in which AEDs may be
2009). The atypical presentation of depression in contributing to cognitive deficits. Adverse side
the elderly with epilepsy heightens the impor- effects are more likely to occur with the specific
tance of a thorough clinical interview of the AEDs described below, fast dose escalation rates,
patient’s symptoms and lack of strict adherence to high doses, and polypharmacy (Sheorajpanday
formal diagnostic criteria. et al. 2007).
3 Geriatric Patients with Epilepsy 71

Despite these concerns, elderly individuals et al. (2010) found that neither LTG nor CBZ
are underrepresented in AED clinical trials, and caused significant changes in health-related qual-
much of the information about AEDs used in ity of life. Other medications have not received as
elderly patients is derived from studies with extensive study in the elderly with epilepsy,
younger adults (Beghi, Savica, et al., 2009). although oxcarbazepine appeared promising
The few data available suggest that greater cog- regarding tolerability in patients 65 years and
nitive side effects of AEDs in elderly patients older (Kutluay, McCague, et al., 2003).
may be associated with polypharmacy (Griffith,
Martin, et al., 2006; Piazzini, Canevini, et al., Surgery
2006) and old generation AEDs (Massimiliano, Epilepsy remains intractable to medications in
Rodolfo, et al., 2009). In addition, although approximately 20 % of elderly patients (Mohanraj
cognitive side effects of topiramate have not & Brodie, 2006). Surgery is a successful treat-
been specifically examined in the elderly with ment option for many patients with medically
epilepsy, there is reason to suspect that it car- intractable epilepsy, and a recent study demon-
ries cognitive risk based on the side effects seen strated similar rates of seizure outcome (i.e.,
in studies not restricted to the elderly (Sommer Engel classes I and II) after temporal lobectomy
& Fenn, 2010). with pathologically confirmed hippocampal scle-
Research has supported the effectiveness and rosis in those 50 and older (i.e., 95.2 %) com-
tolerability of lamotrigine (LTG) and gabapentin pared with those under age 50 (i.e., 90.3 %) at
(GBP) in elderly patients. Randomized con- follow-up approximately 10 years later. However,
trolled trials that have specifically recruited there has been some hesitancy to provide surgical
elderly patients found that LTG and/or GBP treatment to elderly patients (Acosta, Vale, et al.,
resulted in less early termination compared to 2008; Grivas, Schramm, et al., 2006; Sirven,
carbamazepine (CBZ) with similar efficacy Malamut, et al., 2000), in part due to concerns
(Brodie, Overstall, et al., 1999; Rowan, Ramsay, that surgery may exacerbate age-related cogni-
et al., 2005; Saetre, Perucca, et al., 2007). An tive decline (Grivas, Schramm, et al., 2006;
exception to this is one study that found no sig- Sirven, Malamut, et al., 2000).
nificant difference in effectiveness and tolerabil- The extent to which concern is warranted for
ity of LTG and CBZ in elderly patients, although poor cognitive outcome from surgery in elderly
there were trends for greater tolerability of LTG patients is unknown. Cross-sectional studies
and greater seizure-free rates of CBZ (Saetre, raise the possibility that temporal lobe surgery
Perucca, et al., 2007). Several additional studies may accelerate memory decline with increasing
also found a good tolerability profile for, and age (Helmstaedter, Reuber, et al., 2002; Rausch,
effectiveness of, LTG in patients aged 60 or 65 Kraemer, et al., 2003). Helmstaedter et al. (2002)
and older (Arif, Buchsbaum, et al., 2010; found age-accelerated decline in postoperative
Ferrendelli, French, et al., 2003; Giorgi, Gomez, verbal learning scores in patients after anterior
et al., 2001; Mauri Llerda, Tejero, et al., 2005). temporal lobectomy compared to controls. This
Treatment guidelines from the International effect was not present in preoperative scores, in
League Against Epilepsy state that, in focal epi- which age regression was similar to controls,
lepsies in elderly patients, LTG and GBP should suggesting that standard anterior temporal lobec-
be considered for initial monotherapy due to the tomy has worse memory outcome for older
available efficacy and effectiveness data (Glauser, individuals. However, there are sederal careats.
Ben-Menachem, et al., 2006). Not all evidence is First, age-related memory decline was specific to
convergent on the superiority of LTG and GBP in patients who underwent standard anterior tem-
elderly patients, however. A recent survey of poral lobectomy for TLE that was not confined
patients aged 65–90 found no differences in to mesial temporal sclerosis (e.g., normal imag-
adverse effects between LTG, CBZ, and sodium ing or other lesions such as tumor), as age-
valproate (Brodie & Stephen, 2007), and Saetre accelerated memory decline was not found in the
72 J. Chapin and R. Naugle

selective amygdalohippocampectomy group higher risk of cognitive deficits 3 months after


with MTS. Second, older patients may have had surgery compared to young adults or middle-
a later age of epilepsy onset and longer duration aged patients, as well as elderly controls. While
of epilepsy, both of which were correlated with prevalence of cognitive dysfunction in the
poorer memory, so results may, at least in part, younger groups returned to the levels of age-
reflect these factors. Finally, the average age of matched controls at 3 months after surgery (i.e.,
subjects in that study was 30 years, with no young adults 5.7 % and middle aged 5.6 %), the
patient aged 60 or older. Similarly, Rausch et al. prevalence of cognitive dysfunction in the
(2003) found that patients showed increasing elderly group was significantly higher (12.7 %).
deviation from age-corrected normative data on Additional predictors of cognitive dysfunction
episodic auditory memory tests both 1 year and at 3-month follow-up included cognitive dys-
~12 years after left temporal lobectomy. function at hospital discharge, increased age,
However, patients were rather young in this history of cerebral vascular accident with no
study as well, with an average age of 40 years at residual impairment, and years of education.
the long-term follow-up. These results are similar to those of another
Two studies have been published to date that similar large-scale study (Moller, Cluitmans,
directly compare cognitive outcome in older et al., 1998). Thus, older age appears to be a risk
versus younger patients after surgery for epi- factor for cognitive dysfunction after major sur-
lepsy. Both found that the older adults did not gery in general.
demonstrate greater memory declines than In summary, the elderly may be at increased
younger adults. Sirven et al. (2000) found no risk of postoperative cognitive decline after any
difference in memory change between 17 older major surgery. Regarding cognitive risk associ-
(approximate age range 50–66) and 180 ated specifically with surgery for epilepsy, pre-
younger (approximate age range 18–49 years) liminary cross-sectional data suggest that
patients before and after temporal lobectomy. anterior temporal lobectomy may accelerate
Similarly, Grivas et al. (2006) did not find sig- age-related memory decline in patients with
nificant differences in memory changes between pathology other than MTS, although this
34 older adults (approximate age range 50–71) increased cognitive risk for elderly patients has
and 359 younger adults (age <50 years) before not been born out in direct comparisons of mem-
and after temporal lobectomy. However, there ory decline in older versus younger adults.
was a trend for greater decline in postoperative However, the few available studies in this area
auditory memory performance in the eight have been limited by a small sample size of older
patients over 60 years of age than in those under patients, with few if any of the samples com-
60 years of age. posed of those over age 60 years.
Further, elderly patients appear to be at
greater risk of cognitive decline after major sur-
gery in general compared to younger adults, Methods
even when the surgery does not involve the heart
or brain. A well-designed study by Monk and Role of the Neuropsychologist
colleagues (2008) found that although about Assessing Elderly Patients
31–40 % of non-demented adult patients experi- with Epilepsy
ence cognitive dysfunction (i.e., deficits in epi-
sodic memory, executive functioning, and There are several different reasons why a neuro-
processing speed compared to control group) at psychologist may be asked to conduct an evalua-
hospital discharge with no difference between tion of an elderly patient with epilepsy. Referral
those who are young adult, middle aged, or questions can include those typical of a younger
elderly, the elderly patients were at significantly adult with epilepsy, such as characterizing the
3 Geriatric Patients with Epilepsy 73

impact of seizures, AEDs, or surgery on cogni- broken up into multiple sessions. It may be
tion; obtaining a baseline of cognitive function- helpful to structure testing so that measures
ing by which to measure any changes in the within a specific cognitive domain are spaced
future (e.g., after surgery, AED changes, or ongo- throughout the evaluation to avoid an entire
ing seizures); and providing insight into the level domain being affected by fatigue towards the end
of cognitive functioning in order to help estimate of a testing session, as well as maximizing the
functional abilities. Additionally, in the elderly chance of getting a variety of cognitive domains
with epilepsy, it may be important to determine assessed even if the patient has to discontinue
whether or not the patient appears to be experi- before the end of the evaluation. Scheduling test-
encing a comorbid progressive neurodegenera- ing for the morning is often preferred, since evi-
tive process. Any neuropsychological evaluation dence suggests that older adults perform better
for an elderly patient with epilepsy should take on cognitive tasks in the morning than in the
into account the unique etiology, cognitive con- afternoon (Hasher, Chung, et al., 2002).
siderations, psychosocial functioning, and treat-
ment implications of this group that are outlined
above. Interview
Neuropsychologists are often asked to evalu-
ate the cognitive functioning and comment on the In addition to obtaining standard background
potential etiology of deficits in elderly patients information in a neuropsychological interview,
with episodes of confusion, memory complaints, when assessing the elderly with epilepsy, it is
and changes in behavior in the absence of epi- even more important to gain a complete and
lepsy. It is important to be mindful of the possi- detailed understanding of the history of onset and
bility that seizures may be occurring in these progression of cognitive, behavioral, and mood
patients presenting with rather nonspecific symp- symptoms and how these overlay any changes in
toms. Since there is a great deal of overlap seizures, AEDs, other medications and medical
between symptoms of dementia and epilepsy, problems, and other relevant life events (e.g.,
careful history taking and evaluation of any focal retirement, death of a spouse). The information
area(s) of neurocognitive deficit are key features obtained in the interview is often the most valu-
of the examination in order to determine whether able data for establishing a list of possible etiolo-
seizures are a potential etiology that deserve fur- gies for subjective or objective cognitive
ther neurological investigation. impairment. Common etiologies of cognitive
dysfunction to consider include:
• Acute and chronic effects of seizures
Appointment Scheduling • AED or other medication side effects
• Progressive dementia
Elderly patients with epilepsy may require slight • Cerebrovascular disease
modifications of the assessment procedure. • Head trauma
Obtaining a collateral report is particularly • Sleep problems
important when conducting a neuropsychologi- • Emotional distress
cal evaluation of elderly patients with epilepsy • Other medical comorbidities, including any
due to possible difficulty describing seizure- known cause of epilepsy
related details and unawareness of cognitive Cerebrovascular etiology or comorbid disease
symptoms, particularly in the case of suspected should be considered in all elderly patients with
progressive dementias. Elderly patients may epilepsy, since cerebrovascular disease accounts
become fatigued more quickly during testing, for about one third of newly diagnosed cases of
and a shorter battery is often required. epilepsy in elderly patients. Furthermore, elderly
Alternatively, in some settings, testing could be patients with epilepsy are at a higher risk of
74 J. Chapin and R. Naugle

experiencing a first ever stroke than the elderly tion, such as dementia and cardiovascular dis-
without epilepsy (Cleary, Shorvon, et al., 2004). ease, even if not directly related to the referral
Similarly, evaluations should consider the pres- question to help track possible changes in the
ence of possible progressive dementias, since future (e.g., inclusion of a dementia screening
about 12 % of newly diagnosed cases of epilepsy measure). Finally, as in any neuropsychological
in elderly patients are attributed to degenerative evaluation, test selection should take into account
disorders, and some evidence suggests that epi- the specific referral question. In the epilepsy pop-
lepsy increases the risk of developing AD ulation, measures to help localize and lateralize
(Breteler, van Duijn, et al., 1991). Other condi- dysfunction should be included. Considerations
tions affecting cognition in older adults not listed for interpreting data in the context of the com-
above also need to be considered as comorbid mon referral questions listed above are addressed
conditions, although they are not necessarily in the below section “Reporting the Findings.”
associated with epilepsy specifically. These
include, but of course are not limited to, Parkinson Domains Assessed
disease, normal-pressure hydrocephalus, hypo- Based on the issues listed above, recommended
thyroidism, vitamin B12 deficiency, thiamine tests are listed in Table 3.2. This is not meant to
deficiency, and sleep breathing disorders. Finally, be an all-inclusive list of useful tests, nor is this
it is also particularly important to assess activities meant to suggest that all tests need to be admin-
of daily living, since this is helpful in differential istered. Rather, this list is provided as a core of
diagnosis and forming recommendations. select measures useful in the elderly epilepsy
population and could be modified based on the
specific referral question, the clinician’s pre-
Test Selection liminary impression, and any restrictions posed
by a patient such as fatigue or sensory/motor
General Considerations problems.
Neuropsychological evaluation of any elderly Some clinicians may prefer to use the Wechsler
patient requires special considerations. There are Fourth Editions rather than the older versions of
limited normative data on many common these measures. At the time of this writing,
neuropsychological measures for people of research regarding the applicability of the newer
advanced age, although this has improved over measures in this population was not available.
the past decade. Test selection should take into
careful account the robustness of the norms for
older adults. The MOANS and MOAANS studies Reporting the Findings
in particular provide good normative data for
individuals between the ages of 56 and 95 (e.g., Characterize Results
Ivnik, Malec, et al., 1996; Lucas, Ivnik, et al.,
2005). Increased frequency of motor and sensory As in a typical neuropsychological report, a good
deficits in this population requires understanding first step is to describe the cognitive profile of
any limitations of the patient and adapting tests strengths and weaknesses. Emotional, personality,
appropriately. For example, grooved pegboard and functional data should also be reviewed, includ-
performance may be affected by peripheral ing the results of depression and anxiety rating
injury, and results would not reflect brain dys- scales, personality questionnaires, quality of life rat-
function per se. Unique mood, quality of life, and ings, and activities of daily living scales. Report
functional issues in elderly patients suggest the whether or not cognitive or behavioral/emotional
use of specific questionnaires developed for this impairment exists, and if so, the nature of the impair-
population. Consideration should be given to ment (e.g., affected cognitive domain(s) and sever-
measures targeting cognitive symptoms specific ity). If relevant, report the extent to which results are
to common medical comorbidities in this popula- lateralizing or localizing.
3 Geriatric Patients with Epilepsy 75

Table 3.2 Recommended tests for neuropsychological Address Referral Questions


assessment of elderly patients with epilepsy
Attention and working memory Providing Differential Diagnosis
– Digit Span (WAIS-III/WMS-III) for Cognitive Symptoms
– Spatial Span (WMS-III) The referral questions often include, either
– Arithmetic (WAIS-III)a explicitly or implicitly, discussion of the etiology
Processing speed
of objective and/or subjective cognitive impair-
– Oral Symbol Digit Modalities Test
ment. This requires integration of information
– Trails Ab
obtained from a variety of sources including
– Digit Symbol Coding (WAIS-III)a
interview with the patient and a collateral source,
Motor
– Finger tapping
medical history including brain imaging, behav-
– Grooved pegboard ioral observations, and test scores. Potential eti-
Language ologies discussed above should be considered.
– Reading subtest from the Wide Range Achievement Differential diagnosis will often depend on the
Test—Fourth Edition reported onset and progression of cognitive prob-
– Boston Naming Testb lems or lack thereof, corroboration from imaging
– Verbal fluency: Categoriesb and CFLb data, and cognitive profile.
– Token Testb Evaluating for a comorbid progressive neurode-
Visuospatial/constructional skills generative process may be difficult. No study to
– Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure. If figure may be date has attempted to establish measures differenti-
too difficult, the Greek cross may be substituted
ating diagnoses of epilepsy and progressive demen-
– Clock Drawing Test
tia. Griffith et al. (2006) found that older patients
– Judgment of Line Orientationb
– Picture Completion (WAIS-III)a
with epilepsy demonstrated reduced executive
Executive functioning functioning and similar memory performance com-
– Trails Bb pared to patients with amnestic mild cognitive
– Wisconsin Card Sorting Test impairment. Factors helpful in diagnosing a pro-
– Similarities (WAIS-III)a gressive dementia versus cognitive impairment due
– Stroopb to epilepsy include the onset and progression of
Memory cognitive symptoms and the patient’s neurocogni-
– Logical Memory (WMS-III) tive profile of strengths and weaknesses, and how
– Rey’s Auditory-Verbal Learning Testb these overlap with their onset, progression, and type
– Brief Visual Memory Test of epilepsy or epilepsy treatments compared to the
Dementia screening characteristic profile of progressive dementias.
– Dementia Rating Scale—II (includes orientation However, cognitive dysfunction in epilepsy is not
items)b
always focal, even in the case of a focal epilepsy
Mood/QOL/ADL
(Hermann & Seidenberg, 2002). If the reported
– Quality of Life in Epilepsy Inventories (i.e.,
QOLIE-89 Devinsky, Vickrey, et al., 1995; QOLIE- onset and progression of cognitive difficulties or the
31, Cramer, Perrine, et al., 1998). These are freely pattern of subjective or objective cognitive-
available online at http://www.rand.org/health/ behavioral symptoms are not sufficient to distinguish
surveys_tools/qolie/
the etiology, serial assessment may be required to
– Questionnaires to determine ability to manage the
basic and instrumental activities of daily living
differentiate the cause of the symptoms.
– Measure of psychological variables including as
depression, such as the Neurological Disorders
Depression Inventory for Epilepsy (Gilliam, Barry,
Characterizing the Impact of Epilepsy
et al., 2006); anxiety; and motivation on Cognition
a
WAIS-III subtests to be used to calculate Full-Scale IQ Epilepsy, particularly when chronic, has been
(Donnell et al., 2007) associated with cognitive dysfunction. Several
b
MOANS norms available variables have been associated with increased
76 J. Chapin and R. Naugle

cognitive impairment, primarily in temporal lobe memory decline, including dominant mesial
epilepsy. While not all studies have found similar temporal resection, normal imaging, and later
results, the most commonly reported variables of age of seizure onset, as well as risk factors asso-
note include younger age of onset, longer dura- ciated with major surgeries in general that have
tion of epilepsy, smaller hippocampal and total emphasized the importance of cerebral reserve
brain volume, poor seizure control, involvement (i.e., education and previous cerebrovascular
of the dominant lobe, history of generalized injury; Monk, Weldon, et al., 2008). There is
tonic-clonic seizures, and lower baseline IQ indirect evidence from Helmstaedter and col-
(Glosser, Cole, et al., 1997; Hermann, Seidenberg, leagues (2002) that selective amygdalohippo-
et al., 2002; Hermann, Seidenberg, et al., 2006; campectomy may carry less age-associated
Oyegbile, Dow, et al., 2004; Strauss, Loring, cognitive risk than standard anterior 2/3 tempo-
et al., 1995; Thompson & Duncan, 2005). Again, ral lobectomy for temporal lobe epilepsy, but
the extent of corroborating evidence between sei- this requires further study.
zure localization data (e.g., EEG, MRI) and the
onset, static, or progressive course and pattern of
neurocognitive strengths and weaknesses can Recommendations
provide evidence for or against seizures being a
primary etiology of cognitive dysfunction. Recommendations will obviously be made on an
individualized basis, but there are several that
Determining AED Cognitive Side deserve special consideration among elderly
Effects patients with epilepsy.
As noted above, AEDs carry risk for cognitive
side effects. Determining the contribution of vari- Modification of Activities
ous medications to any given patient’s cognitive Reduction in activities and functional assistance
dysfunction requires a careful review of the tim- may be warranted based on the interview, medi-
ing of medication changes (the introduction and cal history, and examination results. An occupa-
discontinuation of medications, changes in dos- tional therapy assessment of specific functional
ages, and speed with which changes are made) capacities may be helpful to determine if modifi-
and the onset or exacerbation of cognitive com- cations in the patient’s activities are required.
plaints. Overlooking the possibility that medica- Basic safety concerns may include confusion
tions are responsible for some cognitive regarding medication management, inability to
inefficiencies potentially precludes the effective consistently remember to take medications, for-
treatment of those symptoms by modifying the getting to turn off the stove or iron, and wander-
medication regimen. ing/getting lost. Financial issues should be
considered, such as the ability to manage the
Assessing Risk of Cognitive Decline household finances and balance a checkbook.
After Surgical Resection Return to work issues are less common in the
As reviewed above, there have been limited data elderly, but occasionally arise. Several potential
addressing the potential for increased risk of issues must be balanced, including the personal
postoperative cognitive decline due to advanced and financial benefits that often accompany con-
age. The available data suggest a cautious tinued work with the risk of overexertion that can
approach is prudent, but there are no direct data result in physical and emotional fatigue and fur-
to suggest that advanced age alone is a strong ther medical/emotional problems, as well as
contraindication for surgery from a cognitive financial risks of losing legitimate disability ben-
standpoint. Rather, in the absence of strong evi- efits if the patient fails to perform well and is let
dence in either direction, the unique risk factors go. Gradual reintroduction to the position is often
should be examined for each individual patient, prudent, such as resuming work a few hours per
including the well-established predictors of day and increasing the time every few weeks
3 Geriatric Patients with Epilepsy 77

until they find their optimal schedule. If the sei- bly MRI), and EEG. Because interictal epilepti-
zures are controlled and the patient wants to form discharges are less common in older adults
resume driving, it is important to make sure the (Drury & Beydoun, 1998), if there is strong sus-
patient is cognitively able to drive in a consis- picion of seizures, video-EEG may be war-
tently safe fashion and navigate well. To this end, ranted. Finally, because of the broad differential
results of the neuropsychological evaluation may diagnoses and common comorbidities, neurolo-
suggest that a formal driving evaluation is war- gists may order additional tests to rule out other
ranted. Finally, medical decision-making abili- associated problems (e.g., cerebrovascular, car-
ties should be considered, as these have been diac) (Van Cott, 2002).
found to be impaired in elderly patients with
chronic focal epilepsy, even in those living inde- Improve Quality of Life
pendently in the community (Bambara, Griffith, Epilepsy is associated with greater impairment in
et al., 2007). quality of life, increased frequency of depression
and anxiety, and decreased social resources
Review of Medications (Hermann, Seidenberg, et al., 2008a). Many
A recommendation may be warranted to consider elderly with newly diagnosed epilepsy and their
changing AEDs and/or other medications with families may have little knowledge of what epi-
suspected adverse cognitive or behavioral side lepsy is, and education about basic facts about
effects. epilepsy and its cognitive and mood implications
can be empowering. Reassurance that percep-
Additional Assistance in Differentiating tions and treatment of those with epilepsy have
Potential Etiologies changed over the years may be important. There
Understanding the extent and type of any cogni- are several excellent online resources for educa-
tive progression through serial neuropsychologi- tion and online forums specific to seniors with
cal evaluations may be required to delineate epilepsy:
etiology of cognitive dysfunction. Other medical – Epilepsy Foundation has a section devoted to
referrals may also help to determine etiology, seniors with epilepsy: senior-specific educa-
such as a neurological evaluation to determine if tion about epilepsy, online discussion forum,
brain imaging, additional lab work, or other tests and information on practical issues of living
may be appropriate. with epilepsy: http://www.epilepsyfounda-
In some cases, a patient may be referred tion.org/living/seniors/
without a diagnosis of epilepsy but present with – This site provides educational information
seizure-like symptoms such as unexplained about seniors with epilepsy: http://www.epi-
brief episodes of confusion or disorientation. lepsy.com/info/seniors
This is particularly relevant for elderly patients, – Epilepsy Ontario has a special section for
where seizures can be difficult to diagnose but seniors with epilepsy regarding education and
are often treatable with AEDs. As discussed practical life issues. The section on support
above, only 25 % of elderly individuals with services, however, is targeted mainly to those
new-onset focal seizures without alteration of in Ontario: http://www.epilepsyontario.org/
consciousness were correctly diagnosed with client/EO/EOWeb.nsf/web/Seniors+Living+
epilepsy at symptom onset, and it took an aver- with+Epilepsy+(kit)
age of 18 months to diagnose (Ramsay, Rowan, – Many states and cities have local epilepsy
et al., 2004). Thus, if seizure activity is sus- groups that can be helpful for locating ser-
pected, referral to an epileptologist or expert vices for the individual patient.
neurologist for seizure evaluation is recom- Referral to a psychotherapist, counselor, or
mended. In addition to a comprehensive neuro- psychiatrist may be indicated to address mood
logical workup, they will often order routine and/or behavioral concerns. Consultation with
serum laboratory tests, neuroimaging (prefera- a social worker is often helpful to provide
78 J. Chapin and R. Naugle

information regarding community resources quently repeated herself, her thinking speed had
such as support groups, job placement ser- significantly slowed, and she had difficulty mak-
vices, and other potential services. Finally, ing decisions. The patient’s daughter said that
referral for occupational or speech therapy can these problems had progressed over the past 2–3
assist in learning skills to compensate for iden- years and were accompanied by functional
tified cognitive problems. impairment; she had lived alone since her hus-
band died 20 years ago, and her daughter had
started to help her manage her medications,
Case Presentation finances, and appointments. The patient’s two
daughters checked on her daily. She continued to
Background groom herself and cook independently without
obvious difficulty. She was reportedly diagnosed
Our patient is a 66-year-old, right-handed, with Alzheimer’s disease 2 years earlier by a doc-
African-American, widowed woman with 12 tor at an outside hospital, but no additional infor-
years of formal education. She was referred by mation about this was available. Her daughter
her epileptologist as part of a comprehensive denied any obvious relationship between previ-
evaluation for epilepsy surgery. Her seizures ous changes in AEDs and cognitive or functional
started at age 41 following the birth of her son changes.
and were characterized by an aura of a nonspe- She worked as a housekeeper for most of her
cific “funny feeling” followed by chewing move- career before retiring several years ago. The
ments and hand automatisms. She was amnestic patient denied a history of depression or other
for these events. Seizures lasted up to a few min- significant emotional distress, with the exception
utes and were followed by postictal confusion. of an incident 16 years ago in which she seemed
She typically experienced one to three seizures confused and left town without the family’s
per day, most days of the week. Seizure fre- knowledge. She was found wading in a river and
quency had waxed and waned with no obvious admitted to a psychiatric hospital. She denied
change over the past several years, and she had alcohol, tobacco, and recreational drug use.
never been seizure-free for more than 1 month Medical history is otherwise unremarkable.
since epilepsy onset. However, because she lived There is no family history of neurological disor-
alone, seizure frequency was difficult to deter- der. Her only medications included Carbatrol and
mine with certainty. She had been taking gabapentin.
Carbatrol (800 mg, four times daily) and gaba-
pentin (400 mg, twice daily) and denied experi-
encing medication side effects. Video-EEG Neuropsychological Evaluation
monitoring of AEDs suggested bitemporal sei-
zures, and brain MRI showed left MTS and mild Evaluation Results
nonspecific white matter lesions. PET showed Behavioral observations were most notable for
hypometabolism in the left anterior temporal the patient’s difficulty providing a detailed and
lobe and mesial temporal structures, as well as well-articulated history. She was unable to recall
mild hypometabolism in the right anterior tempo- two previous car accidents reported by her
ral regions. daughter or the incident of wandering 16 years
When asked about cognitive functioning, the earlier, even when prompted by the daughter. She
patient described difficulty recalling conversa- reported that her husband died 4 years ago rather
tions and remembering to take her medications. than 20 and said she had six, rather than nine,
She perceived her thinking speed as slowed, and grandchildren. Word-finding difficulties were
she reported the onset of word-finding difficul- noted in casual conversation, and she appeared to
ties. Her daughter corroborated these changes but process information slowly. She did not initiate
at greater severity; she said that her mother fre- conversation with the examiner and, at times, was
3 Geriatric Patients with Epilepsy 79

unresponsive to questioning. Eye contact was was better than expected in the context of other
poor and she often sat with her eyes closed. language problems observed. She did not report
Affect was very flat. She was oriented to the day any significant current symptoms of depression.
of week and time of day, but was off on the date Although the possibility that her confusion and
by 1 day. She was oriented to the city and state, difficulty comprehending task instructions under-
but not hospital. She often appeared confused mined her performance cannot be ruled out, her
during the testing portion of the evaluation and test performances were consistent with signifi-
frequently required repetition of task instruc- cant cerebral dysfunction.
tions. Her performance on the Token Test was There are several possible etiologies of the
well below expectation; therefore, it is unclear patient’s deficits in language, memory, working
how well she understood task instructions. memory, and executive functioning. It is likely
Although she performed within expectation on a that some of her current impairments are due to
formal effort measure, her poor comprehension many years of frequent seizures. Her cognitive
and apparent confusion suggested that the current profile was somewhat more complicated than the
test results may have underestimated her actual auditory memory and language deficits often
level of functioning. associated with dominant temporal lobe epilepsy.
Tests were selected based on several factors, Visual memory was impaired in addition to audi-
including (1) the presence of adequate normative tory memory, which could be attributed to non-
data for a 66-year-old African-American woman, dominant temporal lobe involvement suggested
(2) the nature of her cognitive complaints and by bitemporal seizures on video-EEG monitoring
suspected epileptogenic focus, (3) the support in and PET mild hypometabolism on the right side.
the literature regarding prediction of postopera- The deficits in working memory and executive
tive decline, and (4) the need to maintain core functioning can also be observed in patients with
elements of an existing clinical/research battery. temporal lobe epilepsy (Hermann & Seidenberg,
Results are presented in Table 3.3. 2002). However, concern was raised for a pro-
gressive neurodegenerative disorder based on her
Summary, Interpretations, daughter’s report of slowly progressive cognitive
and Recommendations and functional declines over the past 2–3 years.
If taken at face value, this patient’s assessment Importantly, these changes did not appear to cor-
was notable in multiple respects. In the context of respond to changes in AEDs or obvious changes
baseline abilities estimated to have been in the in her seizure frequency or semiology. Finally,
low average to average range, she demonstrated the mild nonspecific white matter lesions noted
marked language and memory difficulties. Her on MRI may also be contributing to her cognitive
confrontation naming was particularly poor, deficits, including the more recent progressive
attaining only 12/60 words. Memory deficits declines. It was recommended that other etiolo-
were characterized by impairment in both audi- gies of cognitive decline be ruled out, such as
tory and visual memory. Poor performance was endocrine/metabolic abnormalities and vitamin
also observed on measures of working memory deficiency.
and executive functioning. Conversely, she per- Results also raise concern for cognitive
formed within the expected range on tests of pro- decline if she were to undergo surgery,
cessing and motor speed, simple and complex presumably a left temporal lobectomy. Aside
visuomotor sequencing, and visuospatial intel- from her age and potential progressive dementia,
lectual abilities. Some of the typical “hold” indi- patients with bilateral poor memory and bitem-
ces such as vocabulary and information may have poral seizures and imaging abnormalities are at
been affected by word-retrieval problems, as increased risk of memory problems after surgery
scores on these measures were lower than (Baxendale, 1998). As discussed above, there is
expected based on her education and WRAT-III mixed evidence regarding age as a risk factor, but
reading subtest. Her phonemic verbal fluency there is some research suggesting that it may be
80 J. Chapin and R. Naugle

Table 3.3 Test results for case presentation


Measure Raw Standard scorea
WAIS-III Full-Scale IQ 75
Verbal Comprehension Index 74
Perceptual Organization Index 82
Working Memory Index 65
Processing Speed Index 91
Vocabulary 80
Similarities 80
Arithmetic 65
Digit Span 80
Information 70
Letter Number Sequencing 70
Picture Completion 85
Digit Symbol 95
Block Design 90
Matrix Reasoning 80
Symbol Search 90
WMS-III Auditory Immediate 65
Visual Immediate 78
Auditory Delayed 64
Visual Delayed 65
Auditory Recognition 75
Logical Memory I 14 65
Faces I 29 85
Verbal Paired Associates I 0 70
Family Pictures I 21 80
Word List total I 25 95
Word List learning slope 4 95
Spatial Span 10 80
Logical Memory II 2 65
Faces II 26 75
Verbal Paired Associates II 0 75
Family Pictures II 13 70
Word List long delay 0 80
Word List recognition 14 65
Trails Ab 61 (1 error) 95
Trails Bb 219 (3 errors) 90
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Total correct 69
Total errors 59 84
Perseverative errors 26 93
Categories 0 <1 percentile
Set failures 3 6–10 percentile
Ruff Total unique designs (corrected) 32 67
Error ratio 0.2857 82
Finger tapping Dominant 29.6 84
Nondominant 31.8 93
Grooved pegboard Dominant 113 90
(continued)
3 Geriatric Patients with Epilepsy 81

Table 3.3 (continued)


Measure Raw Standard scorea
Nondominant 114 93
Boston Naming Testb 12 (1/7 semantic 60
cues; 5/14 phonemic
cues)
COWATb 30 100
Animal fluency 4 60
Token Testb 27 70
WRAT-3 Reading 38 95
Beck Depression Inventory-II 6 “minimal”
a
Higher standard scores indicate better performance
b
Based on MOAANS. All other norms are based on the manuals with the exception of Heaton Norms (2008) used for
grooved pegboard and finger tapping

associated with further memory decline after mated and to determine if seizures, while on
temporal lobectomy. Finally, risk to patients with medication, arise only from the left side.
a potential progressive dementia is also unknown, Results of repeat video-EEG monitoring sug-
but there is evidence to suggest that those with gested much more frequent seizures while on
less cognitive reserve going into surgery may not AEDs than previously thought (i.e., 3–4 per
fare as well as those with more cognitive reserve hour). All arose from the left temporal region.
(Moller, Cluitmans, et al., 1998; Monk, Weldon, Based on discussions between the epilepsy team,
et al., 2008). Taken together, these results suggest the patient, and her family, it was decided to
that the patient was at increased risk for further modify her AEDs to optimize seizure control. If
memory declines following a left, presumably her seizures continued to occur frequently despite
dominant temporal lobectomy. further medication trials, surgical options would
The patient’s cognitive difficulties raised be considered. One and a half years after these
concern about her ability to live independently. AED changes, the patient has been doing better,
In order to ensure her safety, it was recom- with an estimated one to two seizures per week.
mended that she not use the stove or oven inde- Social work has followed the patient, who con-
pendently and that she receive assistance in tinues to live independently with daily calls and
managing her medications. Finally, it was rec- visits from family.
ommended that the family begins the discussion
of power of attorney for health care and finan-
cial decision-making. Significance for Neuropsychological
Evaluations of the Elderly
with Epilepsy
Medical Follow-Up
It may be difficult to differentiate progressive
Neurologists disagreed about cognitive and func- dementia and epilepsy etiologies, and much will
tional surgical risks in the context of an underly- depend on the onset and progression of cognitive
ing dementia. They recommended social symptoms in relation to seizures, as well as a
interventions to optimize medication compliance neurocognitive pattern consistent with that
as well as efforts to obtain an accurate descrip- expected for dementia versus where the seizures
tion of seizure frequency. Repeat video-EEG are thought to arise from. There is no consensus
monitoring was recommended while on her typi- and little data on which to base a decision regard-
cal AEDs, so that seizure burden could be esti- ing risk of cognitive and functional decline after
82 J. Chapin and R. Naugle

epilepsy surgery in elderly patients with possible Lamotrigine Elderly Study Group. Epilepsy Research,
37(1), 81–87.
progressive dementia. It is often difficult to deter-
Brodie, M. J., & Stephen, L. J. (2007). Outcomes in
mine seizure frequency in patients who live elderly patients with newly diagnosed and treated epi-
alone, and video-EEG monitoring can be helpful lepsy. International Review of Neurobiology, 81,
in this regard. Finally, it is important to address 253–263.
Caramelli, P., & Castro, L. H. (2005). Dementia associ-
common psychosocial issues in the elderly with
ated with epilepsy. International Psychogeriatrics,
epilepsy, such as ability to living alone, safety 17(Suppl 1), S195–S206.
concerns including driving, and medical decision- Carney, R. M., Freedland, K. E., Eisen, S. A., Rich, M. W.,
making abilities. & Jaffe, A. S. (1995). Major depression and medica-
tion adherence in elderly patients with coronary artery
disease. Health Psychology, 14(1), 88–90.
Carter, M. D., Weaver, D. F., Joudrey, H. R., Carter, A. O.,
References & Rockwood, K. (2007). Epilepsy and antiepileptic
drug use in elderly people as risk factors for dementia.
Acosta, I., Vale, F., Tatum, W. O., & Benbadis, S. R. (2008). Journal of Neurological Sciences, 252(2), 169–172.
Epilepsy surgery after age 60. Epilepsy and Behavior, Cleary, P., Shorvon, S., & Tallis, R. (2004). Late-onset sei-
12, 324–325. zures as a predictor of subsequent stroke. Lancet,
Arif, H., Buchsbaum, R., Pierro, J., Whalen, M., Sims, J., 363(9416), 1184–1186.
Resor, S. R., Jr., et al. (2010). Comparative effective- Cloyd, J., Hauser, W. A., Towne, A., Ramsay, R., Mattson,
ness of 10 antiepileptic drugs in older adults with epi- R., Gilliam, F., et al. (2006). Epidemiological and
lepsy. Archives of Neurology, 67(4), 408–415. medical aspects of epilepsy in the elderly. Epilepsy
Baker, G. A., Jacoby, A., Buck, D., Brooks, J., Potts, P., & Research, 68(Suppl 1), S39–S48.
Chadwick, D. W. (2001). The quality of life of older Cramer, J. A., Perrine, K., Devinsky, O., Bryant-
people with epilepsy: Findings from a UK community Comstock, L., Meador, K., & Hermann, B. (1998).
study. Seizure, 10(2), 92–99. Development and cross-cultural translations of a
Bambara, J. K., Griffith, H. R., Martin, R. C., Faught, E., 31-item quality of life in epilepsy inventory. Epilepsia,
Wadley, V. G., & Marson, D. C. (2007). Medical deci- 39(1), 81–88.
sion-making abilities in older adults with chronic par- de la Court, A., Breteler, M. M. B., Meinardi, H., Hauser,
tial epilepsy. Epilepsy and Behavior, 10(1), 63–68. W. A., & Hofman, A. (1996). Prevalence of epilepsy in
Barlow, D. H. (2001). Clinical handbook of psychological the elderly: The Rotterdam study. Epilepsia, 37(2),
disorders. New York, NY: The Guilford Press. 141–147.
Baxendale, S. (1998). Amnesia in temporal lobectomy DeLorenzo, R. J., Hauser, W. A., Towne, A. R., Boggs, J.
patients: Historical perspective and review. Seizure, G., Pellock, J. M., Penberthy, L., et al. (1996). A pro-
7(1), 15–24. spective, population-based epidemiologic study of sta-
Beghi, M., Savica, R., Beghi, E., Nobili, A., & Garattini, tus epilepticus in Richmond, Virginia. Neurology,
L. (2009). Utilization and costs of antiepileptic drugs 46(4), 1029–1035.
in the elderly: Still an unsolved issue. Drugs and Devinsky, O., Vickrey, B. G., Cramer, J., Perrine, K.,
Aging, 26(2), 157–168. Hermann, B., Meador, K., et al. (1995). Development
Bjorholt, P. G., Nakken, K. O., Rohme, K., & Hansen, H. of the quality of life in epilepsy inventory. Epilepsia,
(1990). Leisure time habits and physical fitness in 36(11), 1089–1104.
adults with epilepsy. Epilepsia, 31(1), 83–87. Donnell, A. J., Pliskin, N., Holdnack, J., Axelrod, B., &
Breteler, M. M., de Groot, R. R., van Romunde, L. K., & Randolph, C. (2007). Rapidly-administered short
Hofman, A. (1995). Risk of dementia in patients with forms of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-3rd
Parkinson’s disease, epilepsy, and severe head trauma: edition. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 22(8),
A register-based follow-up study. American Journal of 917–924.
Epidemiology, 142, 1300–1305. Drury, I., & Beydoun, A. (1998). Interictal epileptiform
Breteler, M. M., van Duijn, C. M., Chandra, V., Fratiglioni, activity in elderly patients with epilepsy. Electroen-
L., Graves, A. B., Heyman, A., et al. (1991). Medical cephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 106(4),
history and the risk of Alzheimer’s disease: A collab- 369–373.
orative re-analysis of case-control studies. EURODEM Ferrendelli, J. A., French, J., Leppik, I., Morrell, M. J.,
Risk Factors Research Group. International Journal of Herbeuval, A., Han, J., et al. (2003). Use of levetirace-
Epidemiology, 20(Suppl 2), S36–S42. tam in a population of patients aged 65 years and
Brodie, M. J., & Kwan, P. (2005). Epilepsy in elderly older: A subset analysis of the KEEPER trial. Epilepsy
people. BMJ, 331(7528), 1317–1322. and Behavior, 4(6), 702–709.
Brodie, M. J., Overstall, P. W., & Giorgi, L. (1999). Fillit, H. M., Butler, R. N., O’Connell, A. W., Albert, M.
Multicentre, double-blind, randomised comparison S., Birren, J. E., Cotman, C. W., et al. (2002).
between lamotrigine and carbamazepine in elderly Achieving and maintaining cognitive vitality with
patients with newly diagnosed epilepsy. The UK aging. Mayo Clinic Proceedings, 77(7), 681–696.
3 Geriatric Patients with Epilepsy 83

Forstl, H., Burns, A., Levy, R., Cairns, N., Luthert, P., & Hauser, W. A., Annegers, J. F., & Kurland, L. T. (1991).
Lantos, P. (1992). Neurologic signs in Alzheimer’s Prevalence of epilepsy in Rochester, Minnesota:
disease. Results of a prospective clinical and neuro- 1940–1980. Epilepsia, 32(4), 429–445.
pathologic study. Archives of Neurology, 49(10), Hauser, W. A., Annegers, J. F., & Kurland, L. T. (1993).
1038–1042. Incidence of epilepsy and unprovoked seizures in
Frey, M. (2007). Psychiatric complications of epilepsy in Rochester, Minnesota: 1935–1984. Epilepsia, 34,
the geriatric patient: Diagnostic and treatment consid- 453–468.
erations. In A. B. Ettinger & A. M. Kanner (Eds.), Hauser, W. A., Annegers, J. F., & Rocca, W. A. (1996).
Psychiatric issues in epilepsy: A practical guide (2nd Descriptive epidemiology of epilepsy: Contributions
ed.). Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. of population-based studies from Rochester,
Gaitatzis, A., Carroll, K., Majeed, A., & W Sander, J. Minnesota. Mayo Clinic Proceedings, 71(6),
(2004). The epidemiology of the comorbidity of epi- 576–586.
lepsy in the general population. Epilepsia, 45(12), Hauser, W. A., Morris, M. L., Heston, L. L., & Anderson,
1613–1622. V. E. (1986). Seizures and myoclonus in patients with
Gilliam, F. G., Barry, J. J., Hermann, B. P., Meador, K. J., Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology, 36(9), 1226–1230.
Vahle, V., & Kanner, A. M. (2006). Rapid detection of Haut, S. R., Katz, M., Masur, J., & Lipton, R. B. (2009).
major depression in epilepsy: A multicentre study. Seizures in the elderly: Impact on mental status, mood,
Lancet Neurology, 5(5), 399–405. and sleep. Epilepsy and Behavior, 14(3), 540–544.
Giorgi, L., Gomez, G., O’Neill, F., Hammer, A. E., & Heaton, R. K., Miller, S. W., Taylor, M. J., & Grant, I.
Risner, M. (2001). The tolerability of lamotrigine in (2008). Revised comprehensive norms for an expanded
elderly patients with epilepsy. Drugs and Aging, 18(8), Halstead-Reitan battery: Demographically adjusted
621–630. neuropsychological norms for African American and
Glauser, T., Ben-Menachem, E., Bourgeois, B., Cnaan, A., Caucasian adults. Lutz, FL: PAR.
Chadwick, D., Guerreiro, C., et al. (2006). ILAE treat- Helmstaedter, C., & Elger, C. E. (2009). Chronic temporal
ment guidelines: Evidence-based analysis of antiepi- lobe epilepsy: A neurodevelopmental or progressively
leptic drug efficacy and effectiveness as initial dementing disease? Brain, 132(Pt 10), 2822–2830.
monotherapy for epileptic seizures and syndromes. Helmstaedter, C., Kurthen, M., Lux, S., Reuber, M., &
Epilepsia, 47(7), 1094–1120. Elger, C. E. (2003). Chronic epilepsy and cognition: A
Glosser, G., Cole, L. C., French, J. A., Saykin, A. J., & longitudinal study in temporal lobe epilepsy. Annals of
Sperling, M. R. (1997). Predictors of intellectual per- Neurology, 54(4), 425–432.
formance in adults with intractable temporal lobe epi- Helmstaedter, C., Reuber, M., & Elger, C. C. (2002a).
lepsy. Journal of International Neuropsychological Interaction of cognitive aging and memory deficits
Society, 3(3), 252–259. related to epilepsy surgery. Annals of Neurology,
Griffith, H. R., Martin, R. C., Bambara, J. K., Faught, E., 52(1), 89–94.
Vogtle, L. K., & Marson, D. C. (2007). Cognitive Helmstaedter, C., Reuber, M., & Elger, C. C. E. (2002b).
functioning over 3 years in community dwelling older Interaction of cognitive aging and memory deficits
adults with chronic partial epilepsy. Epilepsy Research, related to epilepsy surgery. Annals of Neurology, 52,
74(2–3), 91–96. 89–94.
Griffith, H. R., Martin, R. C., Bambara, J. K., Marson, D. Hermann, B., & Seidenberg, M. (2002). Neuropsychology
C., & Faught, E. (2006). Older adults with epilepsy and temporal lobe epilepsy. CNS Spectrums, 7(5),
demonstrate cognitive impairments compared with 343–348.
patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Hermann, B. P., Seidenberg, M., Dow, C., Jones, J.,
Epilepsy and Behavior, 8(1), 161–168. Rutecki, P., Bhattacharya, A., et al. (2006a). Cognitive
Grivas, A., Schramm, J., Kral, T., von Lehe, M., prognosis in chronic temporal lobe epilepsy. Annals of
Helmstaedter, C., Elger, C. E., et al. (2006). Surgical Neurology, 60(1), 80–87.
treatment for refractory temporal lobe epilepsy in the Hermann, B. P., Seidenberg, M., Dow, C., Jones, J.,
elderly: Seizure outcome and neuropsychological Rutecki, P., Bhattacharya, A., et al. (2006b). Cognitive
sequels compared with a younger cohort. Epilepsia, prognosis in chronic temporal lobe epilepsy. Annals of
47, 1364–1372. Neurology, 60, 80–87.
Hamed, S. A., & Nabeshima, T. (2005). The high athero- Hermann, B., Seidenberg, M., Sager, M., Carlsson, C.,
sclerotic risk among epileptics: The atheroprotective Gidal, B., Sheth, R., et al. (2008). Growing old with
role of multivitamins. Journal of Pharmacological epilepsy: The neglected issue of cognitive and brain
Sciences, 98(4), 340–353. health in aging and elder persons with chronic epi-
Hasher, L., Chung, C., May, C. P., & Foong, N. (2002). lepsy. Epilepsia, 49, 731–740.
Age, time of testing, and proactive interference. Hermann, B. P., Seidenberg, M., & Bell, B. (2002). The
Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56(3), neurodevelopmental impact of childhood onset tem-
200–207. poral lobe epilepsy on brain structure and function and
Hauser, W. A. (1992). Seizure disorders: The changes the risk of progressive cognitive effects. Progress in
with age. Epilepsia, 33(Suppl 4), S6–S14. Brain Research, 135, 429–438.
84 J. Chapin and R. Naugle

Hermann, B., Seidenberg, M., & Jones, J. (2008). The Massimiliano, B., Rodolfo, S., Beghi, E., Nobili, A., &
neurobehavioural comorbidities of epilepsy: Can a Garattini, L. (2009). Utilization and costs of antiepi-
natural history be developed? Lancet Neurology, 7(2), leptic drugs in the elderly. Drugs and Aging, 26(2),
151–160. 157–168.
Hesdorffer, D. C., Hauser, W. A., Annegers, J. F., Kokmen, Mauri Llerda, J. A., Tejero, C., Mercade, J. M., Padro, L.
E., & Rocca, W. A. (1996). Dementia and adult-onset L., & Salas Puig, J. (2005). Lamotrigine and epilepsy
unprovoked seizures. Neurology, 46(3), 727–730. in the elderly: Observational study of low-dose mono-
Isojarvi, J. I., Rattya, J., Myllyla, V. V., Knip, M., therapy. International Journal of Clinical Practice,
Koivunen, R., Pakarinen, A. J., et al. (1998). Valproate, 59(6), 651–654.
lamotrigine, and insulin-mediated risks in women McAreavey, M. J., Ballinger, B. R., & Fenton, G. W.
with epilepsy. Annals of Neurology, 43(4), 446–451. (1992). Epileptic seizures in elderly patients with
Ivnik, R. J., Malec, J. F., Smith, G. E., Tangalos, E. G., & dementia. Epilepsia, 33(4), 657–660.
Petersen, R. C. (1996). Neuropsychological tests’ McBride, A. E., Shih, T. T., & Hirsch, L. J. (2002). Video-
norms above age 55: COWAT, BNT, MAE Token, EEG monitoring in the elderly: A review of 94 patients.
WRAT-R Reading, AMNART, STROOP, TMT, and Epilepsia, 43(2), 165–169.
JLO. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 10, 262–278. McLaughlin, D. P., Pachana, N. A., & McFarland, K.
Kutluay, E., McCague, K., D’Souza, J., & Beydoun, A. (2008). Stigma, seizure frequency and quality of life:
(2003). Safety and tolerability of oxcarbazepine in The impact of epilepsy in late adulthood. Seizure,
elderly patients with epilepsy. Epilepsy and Behavior, 17(3), 281–287.
4(2), 175–180. Mendez, M. F., Catanzaro, P., Doss, R. C., ARguello, R.,
Kwan, P., & Brodie, M. J. (2001). Neuropsychological & Frey, W. H., II. (1994). Seizures in Alzheimer’s dis-
effects of epilepsy and antiepileptic drugs. Lancet, ease: Clinicopathologic study. Journal of Geriatric
357(9251), 216–222. Psychiatry and Neurology, 7(4), 230–233.
Laccheo, I., Ablah, E., Heinrichs, R., Sadler, T., Baade, L., Mendez, M., & Lim, G. (2003). Seizures in elderly
& Liow, K. (2008). Assessment of quality of life patients with dementia: Epidemiology and manage-
among the elderly with epilepsy. Epilepsy and ment. Drugs and Aging, 20(11), 791–803.
Behavior, 12(2), 257–261. Mohanraj, R., & Brodie, M. J. (2006). Diagnosing refrac-
Lozsadi, D. A., & Larner, A. J. (2006). Prevalence and tory epilepsy: Response to sequential treatment sched-
causes of seizures at the time of diagnosis of probable ules. European Journal of Neurology, 13(3),
Alzheimer’s disease. Dementia and Geriatric 277–282.
Cognitive Disorders, 22(2), 121–124. Moller, J. T., Cluitmans, P., Rasmussen, L. S., Houx, P.,
Lucas, J. A., Ivnik, R. J., Willis, F. B., Ferman, T. J., Rasmussen, H., Canet, J., et al. (1998). Long-term
Smith, G. E., Parfitt, F. C., et al. (2005). Mayo’s Older postoperative cognitive dysfunction in the elderly
African Americans Normative Studies: Normative ISPOCD1 study. ISPOCD investigators. International
data for commonly used clinical neuropsychological Study of Post-Operative Cognitive Dysfunction.
measures. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 19(2), Lancet, 351(9106), 857–861.
162–183. Monk, T. G., Weldon, B. C., Garvan, C. W., Dede, D. E.,
Luef, G. J., Waldmann, M., Sturm, W., Naser, A., Trinka, van der Aa, M. T., Heilman, K. M., et al. (2008).
E., Unterberger, I., et al. (2004). Valproate therapy and Predictors of cognitive dysfunction after major non-
nonalcoholic fatty liver disease. Annals of Neurology, cardiac surgery. Anesthesiology, 108(1), 18–30.
55(5), 729–732. Nakken, K. O. (1999). Physical exercise in outpatients
Mackenzie, I. R., McLachlan, R. S., Kubu, C. S., & Miller, with epilepsy. Epilepsia, 40(5), 643–651.
L. A. (1996). Prospective neuropsychological assess- Ono, H., Sakamoto, A., Eguchi, T., Fujita, N., Nomura, S.,
ment of nondemented patients with biopsy proven Ueda, H., et al. (1997). Plasma total homocysteine
senile plaques. Neurology, 46(2), 425–429. concentrations in epileptic patients taking anticonvul-
Mackenzie, I. R., & Miller, L. A. (1994). Senile plaques in sants. Metabolism, 46(8), 959–962.
temporal lobe epilepsy. Acta Neuropathologica Oyegbile, T. O., Dow, C., Jones, J., Bell, B., Rutecki, P.,
(Berlin), 87, 504–510. Sheth, R., et al. (2004). The nature and course of neu-
Marchiori, P. E., Yasuda, N., Azevedo, H. C., Orfao, M., ropsychological morbidity in chronic temporal lobe
Callegaro, D., Yamamoto, F. I., et al. (1996). epilepsy. Neurology, 62(10), 1736–1742.
Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease. A survey of 14 patients. Piazzini, A., Canevini, M. P., Turner, K., Chifari, R., &
Arquivos de Neuro-Psiquiatria, 54(4), 577–583. Canger, R. (2006). Elderly people and epilepsy:
Martin, R. C., Griffith, H. R., Faught, E., Gilliam, F., Cognitive function. Epilepsia, 47(Suppl. 5), 82–84.
Mackey, M., & Vogtle, L. (2005). Cognitive function- Pugh, M. J., Copeland, L. A., Zeber, J. E., Cramer, J. A.,
ing in community dwelling older adults with chronic Amuan, M. E., Cavazos, J. E., et al. (2005). The impact
partial epilepsy. Epilepsia, 46, 298–303. of epilepsy on health status among younger and older
Martin, R., Vogtle, L., Gilliam, F., & Faught, E. (2005). adults. Epilepsia, 46(11), 1820–1827.
What are the concerns of older adults living with epi- Pylvanen, V., Knip, M., Pakarinen, A. J., Turkka, J.,
lepsy? Epilepsy and Behavior, 7(2), 297–300. Kotila, M., Rattya, J., et al. (2003). Fasting serum
3 Geriatric Patients with Epilepsy 85

insulin and lipid levels in men with epilepsy. Sommer, B. R., & Fenn, H. H. (2010). Review of topira-
Neurology, 60(4), 571–574. mate for the treatment of epilepsy in elderly patients.
Ramsay, R. E., & Pryor, F. (2000). Epilepsy in the elderly. Clinical Interventions in Aging, 5, 89–99.
Neurology, 55(5 Suppl 1), S9–S14. Strauss, E., Loring, D., Chelune, G., Hunter, M., Hermann,
Ramsay, R. E., Rowan, A. J., & Pryor, F. M. (2004). B., Perrine, K., et al. (1995). Predicting cognitive
Special considerations in treating the elderly patient impairment in epilepsy: Findings from the Bozeman
with epilepsy. Neurology, 62(Suppl 2), S24–S29. epilepsy consortium. Journal of Clinical and
Rausch, R., Kraemer, S., Pietras, C. J., Le, M., Vickrey, B. Experimental Neuropsychology, 17(6), 909–917.
G., & Passaro, E. A. (2003). Early and late cognitive Strober, L. B., & Arnett, P. A. (2009). Assessment of
changes following temporal lobe surgery for epilepsy. depression in three medically ill, elderly populations:
Neurology, 60(6), 951–959. Alzheimer’s disease, Parkinson’s disease, and stroke.
Rowan, A. J., Ramsay, R. E., Collins, J. F., Pryor, F., The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 23(2), 205–230.
Boardman, K. D., Uthman, B. M., et al. (2005). New Tellez-Zenteno, J. F., Matijevic, S., & Wiebe, S. (2005).
onset geriatric epilepsy: A randomized study of gaba- Somatic comorbidity of epilepsy in the general popu-
pentin, lamotrigine, and carbamazepine. Neurology, lation in Canada. Epilepsia, 46(12), 1955–1962.
64(11), 1868–1873. Thompson, P. J., & Duncan, J. S. (2005). Cognitive
Saetre, E., Perucca, E., Isojärvi, J., & Gjerstad, L. decline in severe intractable epilepsy. Epilepsia,
(2007). An international multicenter randomized 46(11), 1780–1787.
double-blind controlled trial of lamotrigine and sus- Thorvaldsen, P., Asplund, K., Kuulasmaa, K., Rajakangas,
tained-release carbamazepine in the treatment of A. M., & Schroll, M. (1995). Stroke incidence, case
newly diagnosed epilepsy in the elderly. Epilepsia, fatality, and mortality in the WHO MONICA project.
48(7), 1292–1302. World Health Organization Monitoring Trends and
Schwaninger, M., Ringleb, P., Annecke, A., Winter, R., Determinants in Cardiovascular Disease. Stroke,
Kohl, B., Werle, E., et al. (2000). Elevated plasma con- 26(3), 361–367.
centrations of lipoprotein(a) in medicated epileptic Tinuper, P., Provini, F., Marini, C., Cerullo, A., Plazzi, G.,
patients. Journal of Neurology, 247(9), 687–690. Avoni, P., et al. (1996). Partial epilepsy of long duration:
Seaetre, E., Abdelnoor, M., Perucca, E., Taubøll, E., Changing semiology with age. Epilepsia, 37(2), 162–164.
Isojärvi, J., & Gjerstad, L. (2010). Antiepileptic drugs Van Cott, A. C. (2002). Epilepsy and EEG in the elderly.
and quality of life in the elderly: Results from a ran- Epilepsia, 43(Suppl 3), 94–102.
domized double-blind trial of carbamazepine and Verrotti, A., Basciani, F., Trotta, D., Greco, R., Morgese,
lamotrigine in patients with onset of epilepsy in old G., & Chiarelli, F. (2001). Effect of anticonvulsant
age. Epilepsy and Behavior, 17(3), 395–401. drugs on interleukins-1, -2 and -6 and monocyte che-
Sheorajpanday, R. V., & De Deyn, P. P. (2007). Epileptic moattractant protein-1. Clinical and Experimental
fits and epilepsy in the elderly: General reflections, Medicine, 1(3), 133–136.
specific issues and therapeutic implications. Clinical Vezzani, A., & Granata, T. (2005). Brain inflammation in
Neurology and Neurosurgery, 109(9), 727–743. epilepsy: Experimental and clinical evidence.
Sheth, R. D. (2004). Metabolic concerns associated with Epilepsia, 46(11), 1724–1743.
antiepileptic medications. Neurology, 63(10 Suppl 4), Volicer, L., Smith, S., & Volicer, B. J. (1995). Effect of
S24–S29. seizures on progression of dementia of the Alzheimer
Sheth, R. D., Drazkowski, J. F., Sirven, J. I., Gidal, B. E., type. Dementia, 6(5), 258–263.
& Hermann, B. P. (2006). Protracted ictal confusion in Wallace, H., Shorvon, S., & Tallis, R. (1998). Age-specific
elderly patients. Archives of Neurology, 63(4), incidence and prevalence rates of treated epilepsy in
529–532. an unselected population of 2 052 922 and age-specific
Sirven, J. I., Malamut, B. L., O’Connor, M. J., & Sperling, fertility rates of women with epilepsy. Lancet, 352,
M. R. (2000). Temporal lobectomy outcome in older 1970–1973.
versus younger adults. Neurology, 54, 2166–2169. Weiner, M. F., Hynan, L. S., Parikh, B., Zaki, N., White, C.
Smith, G., & Rush, B. (2006). Normal aging and mild L., III, Bigio, E. H., et al. (2003). Can Alzheimer’s dis-
cognitive impairment. In D. K. Attix & K. A. Welsh- ease and dementias with Lewy bodies be distinguished
Bohmer (Eds.), Geriatric neuropsychology. New clinically? Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and
York, NY: Guilford. Neurology, 16(4), 245–250.
Neuropsychological Evaluation
of the Epilepsy Surgical Candidate 4
Daniel L. Drane

Neurosurgical intervention is the primary treatment also ideally equipped to explain the potential risks
option for patients with partial epilepsies that are and benefits of surgery to patients, to identify and
refractory to treatment with antiepileptic drugs address comorbid psychiatric issues, and to direct
(AEDs). The probability that a patient with epilepsy the course of cognitive rehabilitation when neces-
will be successfully treated with an AED greatly sary following surgery. The goals of the current
diminishes after two drug trial failures (Kwan & chapter include elucidating the purpose of neuropsy-
Brodie, 2000), and such patients are typically consid- chology in epilepsy surgery, exploring potential dif-
ered for surgical intervention (Kwan et al., 2010). ficulties involved in obtaining a valid assessment of
Neuropsychological assessment is an integral com- the epilepsy patient (e.g., dealing with the possible
ponent of the surgical management of patients with effects of acute seizure activity or the confounding
epilepsy, representing a useful clinical method for effect of AEDs), and providing concrete recommen-
identifying optimal surgical candidates and maximiz- dations regarding the selection of tests to achieve
ing outcome parameters while serving as a primary both clinical and research goals. A summary of the
research tool. The practice of neuropsychology in the findings that have been amassed over the years
setting of the epilepsy monitoring unit has greatly regarding the presurgical confirmation of the sei-
enriched our understanding of neurocognitive pro- zure focus and postsurgical identification of neuro-
cesses and their underlying neural substrates, as this cognitive deficits resulting from surgery is provided.
environment provides a unique opportunity to study In this context, research regarding the usefulness of
brain functions in a highly controlled fashion before neuropsychological results to predict surgical out-
and after surgical resection. Likewise, the clinical come is covered as well, and a clinical vignette is
impact of neuropsychology has been profound, as it included to highlight several of the central topics.
represents a means to confirm seizure onset, to pre-
dict the possible effect of surgical intervention, and to
track changes over time. Neuropsychologists are Role of Neuropsychology
in the Evaluation of the Epilepsy
Surgical Patient
D.L. Drane, Ph.D., A.B.P.P. (C.N.) (*)
Departments of Neurology and Pediatrics and Emory, Neuropsychological assessment can make multi-
The Emory University School of Medicine,
ple contributions to the assessment of the epi-
Woodruff Memorial Research Building, 101 Woodruff
Circle, Suite 6111, Atlanta, GA, 30322, USA lepsy surgical patient including:
1. Confirming the lateralization and localiza-
Department of Neurology, University of Washington
School of Medicine, Seattle, WA, USA tion of seizure focus for surgical planning.
e-mail: ddrane@emory.edu While the gold standard of determining

W.B. Barr and C. Morrison (eds.), Handbook on the Neuropsychology of Epilepsy, 87


Clinical Handbooks in Neuropsychology, DOI 10.1007/978-0-387-92826-5_4,
© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015
88 D.L. Drane

seizure focus remains the electrophysiolog-


ical data obtained through video-EEG mon- Review of Research Outlining
itoring, neuropsychological testing provides Commonly Observed Pre-
a useful adjunct for confirming seizure and Postsurgical Deficits in Epilepsy
focus. Studies have demonstrated that surgi- Surgical Patients
cal outcome is better in terms of neurocog-
nitive functioning and seizure freedom It is essential that the clinical neuropsychologist
when independent sources of data (e.g., has a working knowledge of the commonly
neuroimaging, electrophysiology, neuro- observed presurgical deficits in epilepsy patients
psychological testing) point to the same in order to conduct an optimal evaluation. In this
probable seizure focus (Holmes, Miles, section, commonly observed presurgical deficits
Dodrill, Ojemann, & Wilensky, 2003; are described for each of the major epilepsy sur-
Lineweaver et al., 2006). gical populations, including temporal lobe epi-
2. Establishing the presurgical neurocognitive lepsy, frontal lobe epilepsy, and the posterior
and emotional/psychological baseline for the cortical epilepsies. Knowledge of the most com-
potential surgical candidate. This allows us to mon postsurgical deficits is also essential in order
recognize presurgical emotional/psychologi- to select tests that will be useful for monitoring
cal factors that may require treatment to change over time. A summary of findings for
improve postsurgical success. Establishing a each syndrome appears in a table at the end of
baseline also makes it possible to recognize each section.
change following surgery, allowing for appro-
priate neurological rehabilitation interven-
tions, education/vocational adjustments, or Findings in Temporal Lobe
assistance from social services. Epilepsy (TLE)
3. Predicting surgical outcome. Establishing a
presurgical neuropsychological baseline Approximately 50 % of patients with epilepsy
allows us to estimate the risk of potential are believed to experience partial seizures
decline in neurocognitive, functional, emo- (Williamson, 1987). Among patients diagnosed
tional, or psychosocial status following sur- with complex partial seizures, 70–90 % of
gery. Baseline neuropsychological status is patients have seizures arising from the temporal
one of several predictors of neurocognitive lobe (TL). Patients with mesial temporal sclero-
outcome and the likelihood of achieving sei- sis (MTS), which is neuronal loss and gliotic
zure freedom. Some have attempted to create scarring of the hippocampal formation/mesial TL
formulas for predicting outcome that are structures, are typically medication refractory.
based in part on neurocognitive performance. Individuals with TLE are often ideal candidates
4. Providing outcome markers that can be useful for surgical treatment to achieve seizure control
in the context of research and program (Wiebe, Blume, Girvin, & Eliasziw, 2001).
development/evaluation. The overall goal of For many years, the prototypical pattern of
epilepsy surgery is to alleviate seizure occur- deficit in presurgical TLE patients has been
rence while minimizing any secondary neuro- described as a material-specific pattern of mem-
cognitive, emotional/psychosocial, or physical ory dysfunction (Milner, 1958). More specifi-
sequelae of this procedure. Neuropsychological cally, auditory/verbal memory deficits are often
assessment plays a pivotal role in quantifying observed in patients with dominant TL seizure
baseline performance, predicting and evaluat- onset, while visual memory deficits have been
ing change, and elucidating underlying brain- more associated with nondominant TL seizure
behavior relationships. onset (Blakemore & Falconer, 1967; Jones-
4 Neuropsychological Evaluation of the Epilepsy Surgical Candidate 89

Gotman, 1986; Loring, Lee, Martin, & Meador, demonstrated significant impairment on recall
1988; McDonald, Bauer, Grande, Gilmore, & following a delay, while right TLE patients
Roper, 2001; Milner, 1968a; Pillon et al., 1999). demonstrated impaired learning of abstract
Neuroimaging studies have bolstered this find- designs but recalled what they were able to learn.
ing, demonstrating that auditory/verbal and non- This pattern represents a possible way that a task
verbal stimuli can preferentially activate the left paradigm might interact with material-specific
or right MTL, respectively (Golby et al., 2001; findings.
Powell et al., 2005). In contrast, other behav- Hermann and colleagues provide evidence
ioral studies have not found this material-spe- that both TLs may eventually be affected in a
cific pattern (Barr et al., 1997; Pigott & Milner, number of patients by seizures of unilateral TL
1993), and some neuroimaging studies have onset. In one study, they used a cluster analysis to
suggested an interaction between the mesial demonstrate that common presurgical neuropsy-
TLs modulated by specific task demands chological profiles may exist for patients with
(Kennepohl, Sziklas, Garver, Wagner, & Jones- TLE (Hermann, Seidenberg, Lee, Chan, &
Gotman, 2007). Visual memory deficits associ- Rutecki, 2007). Approximately half of their sam-
ated with nondominant hemisphere dysfunction ple did not differ from healthy controls with
have been particularly difficult to establish. regard to IQ, perception, primary attention, or
Overall, a material-specific pattern of memory immediate memory yet showed mild deficits (no
dysfunction can be observed in presurgical TLE more than 1 SD below the mean of the control
patients, which can be useful for confirming sei- sample) on tasks tapping delayed memory, con-
zure focus in the individual patient. However, a frontational naming ability, executive control
variety of confounding factors can obscure this processes (e.g., generative fluency), and cogni-
pattern and can lead to divergent findings at tive/psychomotor processing speed. These defi-
both the individual and group level. Emerging cits sound reminiscent of the prototypical findings
research also suggests the material-specific pat- reported over the years, with the exception that
tern of memory dysfunction model may be these patients showed disruption of both verbal
affected by other task parameters and disease- and visual memory functioning. A second group,
related variables. For example, material-specific which comprised another 24 % of the TLE sam-
findings may be easier to detect when examin- ple, was described as primarily experiencing
ing both learning and recall patterns rather than memory impairment, with immediate and
only examining one-trial learning (Jones- delayed memory scores that were more than 2
Gotman, Zatorre, Olivier, et al., 1997). Further, SDs below the mean of the control sample. They
material-specific findings may be altered by side also displayed mild deficits in all remaining cog-
of seizure onset (Vannest, Szaflarski, Privitera, nitive domains. Finally, the third cluster, includ-
Schefft, & Holland, 2008; Weber, Fliessbach, ing another 29 % of participants, was described
Lange, Kugler, & Elger, 2007), and both TLs as a memory-, executive-, and speed-impaired
may eventually be impacted by a chronic dura- group and performed more than 2 SDs below
tion of unilateral TLE (Cheung, Chan, Chan, controls on all administered measures. The third
Lam, & Lam, 2006). cluster exhibited the greatest impairment, took
Jones-Gotman and colleagues at the Montreal the most AEDs, and had the longest duration of
Neurological Institute have suggested that domi- epilepsy and greatest MRI volumetric abnormali-
nant and nondominant presurgical TLE patients ties. This group also exhibited the worst cogni-
may differ in terms of their ability to learn and tive course of the three samples. Limitations in
retain information (Jones-Gotman et al., 1997; this study included a small sample size and an
Majdan, Sziklas, & Jones-Gotman, 1996). Their exclusive focus on patients with childhood or
data indicated that left TLE patients had minimal adolescent onset of epilepsy. Work of this nature
difficulty with initial learning for words but is promising and needs to be replicated across
90 D.L. Drane

epilepsy centers and with other tests, in order to TLE patients, particularly those with dominant
rule out the impact of latent variables as described seizure onset, are also more likely to experience
below and to determine how such patterns may deficits in naming ability (e.g., confrontational
differ from patients with adult seizure onset. naming, naming to description) than healthy con-
There is also growing evidence that different trols or patients with extratemporal seizure onset
types of learning tasks are mediated by different (Mayeux, Brandt, Rosen, & Benson, 1980).
subregions of the TL. In general, associational Several studies have found that left TLE groups
learning (e.g., word pairs) has been thought to be perform worse than right TLE groups on visual
related to the hippocampus, but it appears that confrontational naming tasks (Hermann & Wyler,
other aspects of the mesial TL region may actu- 1988; Hermann, Wyler, & Somes, 1991; Langfit
ally prove to be more critical (e.g., perirhinal or & Rausch, 1996), although the right TLE groups
entorhinal cortices) (Weintrob, Saling, Berkovic, often perform worse than healthy controls (Langfit
& Reutens, 2007; Weniger, Boucsein, & Irle, & Rausch, 1996). A few studies have found both
2004). In either case, associational learning tends left and right TLE patients to be equally impaired
to be one of the more sensitive tasks to mesial TL preoperatively (Hermann, Wyler, Steenman, &
dysfunction (Saling, 2009; Squire, 1993). Recall Richey, 1988). Patients may be impaired on these
of easier, semantically related word pairs is often tasks for reasons other than seizure focus. For
mildly impaired in patients with dominant TL example, those with lower IQ show restricted
dysfunction, while recall of more difficult, unre- naming scores which are more likely due to their
lated word pairs is often severely compromised. general level of impairment and subsequent limi-
Contextual learning, such as reflected by the tations on learning. One recent study provided a
commonly used paragraph and story recall tasks, regression equation that takes into account such
has proven less sensitive to mesial temporal lobe moderator variables in an effort to use a naming
pathology (Rausch & Babb, 1993; Saling et al., task (i.e., Boston Naming Test) to aid in preopera-
1993). These tasks, along with other measures tive seizure localization (Busch, Frazier, Haggerty,
that lend themselves to being organized & Kubu, 2005). This study provides promising
semantically (e.g., category-related word list results but requires further confirmation and pos-
learning), may be more dependent on lateral TL sible refinement. Hamberger and colleagues have
structures. For example, Helmstaedter and col- introduced a naming-to-description task which
leagues (Helmstaedter, Elger, Hufnagel, Zentner, appears promising for predicting preoperative lat-
& Schramm, 1996; Helmstaedter, Grunwald, eralization as well (i.e., Columbia Auditory
Lehnertz, Gleissner, & Elger, 1997) demonstrate Naming Test) (Hamberger & Seidel, 2003;
that selective amygdalohippocampectomies con- Hamberger & Tamny, 1999). Finally, patients
tribute to declines in associational learning (par- undergoing dominant TL resection often show
ticularly for unrelated information), while significant declines on naming tasks (Davies
standard TL resections (which includes lateral et al., 1998; Hermann, Davies, Foley, & Bell,
TL as well) also affect list learning and semanti- 1999), while those undergoing nondominant TL
cally related word pairs. They suggest that lateral resection do not (Saykin, Stafiniak, Robinson,
TL cortex is related to data acquisition and work- et al., 1995). This pattern again highlights the
ing memory, while the medial TL is involved in seemingly critical involvement of the dominant
consolidation processes. Saling and colleagues TL in naming ability.
have suggested that the medial TL structures Evidence exists that the naming deficits of
(particularly perirhinal cortex) play a critical role TLE patients may be more extensive than previ-
in establishing a relationship between items that ously recognized. A few studies demonstrate that
are yet to be linked in either personal episodic or left (dominant) TLE patients are impaired at
semantic memory or which may conflict with naming famous faces (Drane, Ojemann, Aylward,
preexisting knowledge (Saling, 2009). et al., 2008; Glosser, Salvucci, & Chiaravalloti,
4 Neuropsychological Evaluation of the Epilepsy Surgical Candidate 91

2003; Seidenberg, Griffith, Sabsevitz, et al., patients with FL onset can often be distinguished
2002), and at least two of these studies suggest from those with TL onset using a semantic flu-
that these deficits may be worse following ATL ency paradigm that contrasts cued and uncued
resection. Drane et al. (Drane et al., 2008; Drane, performance (Drane et al., 2006). This builds
Ojemann, Tranel, Ojemann, & Miller, 2004) upon the idea that semantic fluency requires
recently extended these findings to include other both an executive component mediated by FL
visually complex item categories (e.g., famous regions (i.e., organization/retrieval problems
landmarks, animals) even when performance is making it difficult to search one’s own semantic
completely normal on standard naming measures memory stores and/or deficits involving initia-
(e.g., BNT). They have argued that commonly tion of action or self-monitoring) (Sylvester &
used naming measures lack sensitivity to the Shimamura, 2002) and a semantic memory
specific item categories that appear the most component that is thought to be mediated by the
affected by anterior TLE surgery, as such mea- TLs (Martin & Fedio, 1983).
sures employ a restricted range of object type While studies examining postoperative
(i.e., mostly man-made objects). Once again, semantic fluency appear to be limited in nature,
these findings are bolstered by functional imag- there is evidence that performance in this domain
ing paradigms highlighting a critical role for the (e.g., generating types of animals) declines fol-
anterior TLs in naming certain object categories lowing dominant TL resection (Loring, Meador,
(Damasio, Grabowski, Tranel, Hichwa, & & Lee, 1994) and perhaps following nondomi-
Damasio, 1996; Griffith, Richardson, Pyzalski, nant TL resection as well (Martin et al., 1990),
et al., 2006). although the latter study had only a 1-week post-
It is also relatively common for presurgical operative follow-up period. One additional study
TLE patients to exhibit deficits on verbal fluency supports the idea that both left and right TL
tests (i.e., generating items from categories, let- resections may both affect semantic fluency per-
ter fluency) (Troster et al., 1995). However, defi- formance but found that the relative impact of
cits in semantic fluency are often present surgery may vary greatly between hemispheres
preoperatively in patients with either dominant depending upon the category of object (Jokeit,
or nondominant TL seizure onset (Bartha, Benke, Heger, Ebner, & Markowitsch, 1998).
Bauer, & Trinka, 2005; Joanette & Goulet, 1986; Performance on letter fluency tasks can be
Martin, Loring, Meador, & Lee, 1990) and can impaired preoperatively due to both FL and TL
be observed in patients with either dominant or impairment (Helmstaedter, Kemper, & Elger,
nondominant frontal lobe (FL) seizure onset as 1996; Martin, Sawrie, Edwards, et al., 2000),
well (Drane et al. 2006). As these findings could although performance on these measures typically
be affected by the use of AEDs, many of which does not decline following TL surgery. Deficits
affect generative fluency and speech production observed on this task in TL patients may occur due
rates (Fritz et al., 2005), it should be noted that to the disruption of widespread neural networks
other types of lesions in these regions have also secondary to epileptiform activity, and some evi-
been shown to impact semantic fluency in dence exists that improvement on these measures
patients without epilepsy (Joanette & Goulet, can be seen if the TL patient becomes completely
1986; Stuss et al., 1998). The complex nature of seizure-free following surgery (Martin et al.,
semantic fluency tasks likely requires the involve- 2000). Therefore, as letter fluency performance
ment of several neural systems for successful appears not to decline following TL surgery, this
completion. Therefore, the presence of baseline provides further evidence that the anterior TL does
semantic fluency deficits may not be of lateraliz- not play a major role in performance of this task.
ing or localizing value when viewed in isolation Despite the deficits mentioned in naming abil-
but may be helpful in this regard if the compo- ity and generative fluency, presurgical TLE
nent parts of this task are examined. For exam- patients do not demonstrate a classic aphasia
ple, one recent study suggests that preoperative (unless it is due to a separate neurological injury
92 D.L. Drane

or disease, such as a prior stroke). Most TLE worse on tasks that are three-dimensional, a few
patients will not exhibit problems with compre- studies suggest that left TL patients are more
hension, speech fluency, or repetition of words or impaired on object-location tasks that are two-
phrases, nor will they present with positive signs dimensional (Kessels, Hendriks, Schouten, Van
of aphasia in spontaneous speech (e.g., parapha- Asselen, & Postma, 2004).
sic errors). Similarly, standard anterior TL resec- Recent studies also indicate that nondominant
tion does not typically lead to declines in these TLE patients often exhibit object recognition
areas (Saykin et al., 1995). deficits. For example, at least four studies dem-
With the exception of interest in visual mem- onstrate that postoperative right ATL patients
ory functioning, less work has been focused on exhibit recognition deficits for famous faces
the nondominant TL in the context of epilepsy. when compared to controls (Drane et al., 2008;
Most have assumed that the nondominant TL is Glosser et al., 2003; Seidenberg et al., 2002).
less critical than the dominant one, and there is There is also evidence that recognition deficits
likely some truth to this idea. Declines in audi- for animals and famous landmarks may occur in
tory/verbal memory and naming ability, findings patients following nondominant resection as well
predominantly associated with the dominant TL and that even a sense of “familiarity” may be
epilepsy and resective surgery, can be particu- impaired in these individuals (Drane et al., 2004,
larly devastating, while many patients seem to be 2008). Patients with these deficits have greater
less hampered by visual memory declines. difficulty recognizing familiar individuals, which
Nevertheless, there is evidence that we may not may compromise social functioning.
fully appreciate the functions of the nondominant One perhaps unexpected neurocognitive find-
TL, and the field’s tendency to think of visual ing in presurgical TLE patients is evidence that
memory as a unitary construct may be misleading such patients often exhibit deficits in executive
(e.g., creating a visual memory index with several functioning tasks thought to be mediated by FL
disparate tasks that may eventually prove to rely regions (see above paragraph on letter fluency
on different neural substrates). Evidence outside deficits in TLE). This pattern of dysfunction has
of epilepsy indicates that visual memory may been attributed to widespread disruption of neu-
involve several dissociable components (Bird & ral networks by recurrent seizure activity. The
Burgess, 2008). For example, human “place” cells “nociferous cortex hypothesis” of Wilder Penfield
in the hippocampus are believed to respond to is sometimes invoked to explain this phenome-
one’s position, functioning to activate (recall) non (Penfield & Jasper, 1954). Evidence includes
broader spatial maps in order to allow for success- impaired performances observed on neurocogni-
ful localization of position in relation to the envi- tive measures such as complex problem-solving
ronment and objects within it (Byrne, Becker, & tasks (e.g., Wisconsin Card Sorting Test) and
Burgess, 2007). In addition, functional neuroim- generative fluency measures (Hermann &
aging studies demonstrate that route learning/way Seidenberg, 1995; Kim, Lee, Yoo, Kang, & Lee,
finding, which is rarely assessed clinically, 2007; Martin et al., 2000). Similar findings have
appears to activate both mesial TLs (Treyer, Buck, also recently been reported in children (Rzezak,
& Schnider, 2005), and several experimental stud- Fuentes, Guimaraes, et al., 2007). Preliminary
ies (some examining TLE patients) suggest that work has shown that patients with successful sei-
performance on these tasks can be compromised zure control following TL resection may show
by right anterior TL damage (Spiers, Burgess, significant improvement on these tasks (Hermann,
Maguire, et al., 2001). Finally, object-location Wyler, & Richey, 1988; Martin et al., 2000), sug-
tasks also tend to be impaired following right gesting that these distributed networks are func-
mesial TL dysfunction (Crane & Milner, 2005; tioning better in the absence of electrophysiological
Pigott & Milner, 1993), although performance here disruption. Similarly, functional neuroimaging
can be affected by varying task parameters. For studies have demonstrated that TLE patients
example, while right TL patients tend to perform often show hypometabolism of the FLs that cor-
4 Neuropsychological Evaluation of the Epilepsy Surgical Candidate 93

relates with aspects of executive function perfor- While the conclusions drawn in this section
mance (Jokeit et al., 1997; Takaya, Hanakawa, should be viewed as more tentative in nature due to
Hashikawa, et al., 2006). One additional fMRI the limited number of completed studies, some
study provides evidence that patterns of FL cere- clear trends appear to be emerging. For example,
bral activation can show “normalization” follow- there is growing evidence that presurgical patients
ing successful TL resection (Maccotta, Buckner, with FLE often exhibit problems with response
Gilliam, & Ojemann, 2007). inhibition (McDonald et al., 2005; Upton &
Depression is the most common psychiatric Thompson, 1996), although this problem has not
comorbidity in epilepsy (Fuller-Thomson & been universally observed (Helmstaedter, Kemper,
Brennenstuhl, 2009), and preoperative depres- et al., 1996). One study that compared a sizeable
sion has been associated with worse seizure number of FLE and TLE patients found that FLE
outcome following TL resective surgery patients performed significantly worse than the
(Anhoury, Brown, Krishnamoorthy, & Trimble, TLE group on a measure of response inhibition
2000). Other psychiatric comorbidities of TLE, (Stroop Color-Word Interference Test) (Upton &
most of which have been less well studied than Thompson, 1996). In contrast, Helmstaedter,
depression, include anxiety disorders, sub- Kemper, et al. (1996) found that their FLE sample
stance abuse, psychosis, and personality disor- performed worse than TLE patients on all Stroop
ders (Table 4.1). conditions, which suggests their primary limitation
may have involved reading or processing speed.
McDonald et al. (2005) added a matched control
Findings in Frontal Lobe Epilepsy (FLE) sample to the previous paradigm of comparing
FLE and TLE patients, demonstrating that the FLE
Research examining the neurocognitive func- group was impaired on the Stroop task from the
tioning of presurgical frontal lobe epilepsy Delis-Kaplan Executive Functioning System
(FLE) patients has been less commonly com- (DKEFS) when compared to the control subjects
pleted. In part, the lack of research in this area is while the TLE group was not. The left FLE group
likely due to greater difficulty obtaining ade- was more impaired than the right FLE group or
quate sample sizes for such studies, as FLE either TLE group on this portion of the task. There
patients are believed to reflect only 10–20 % of is at least one study suggesting that response inhibi-
all surgical referrals (Jokeit & Schacher, 2004). tion performance may decline with unilateral FLE
It is usually necessary to collaborate across epi- resections (Helmstaedter, Kemper, et al., 1996).
lepsy centers or to spend many years obtaining Performance on motor tasks is often decreased
large enough sample sizes to achieve adequate in FLE patients as compared to controls or other
statistical power to answer basic questions. samples of epilepsy patients, and there is some
Research is also hampered in this area by limita- evidence that decline occurs on these tasks with
tions in our understanding of FL functions and some FLE resections. For example, Helmstaedter,
the adequate development of tests and methods Kemper, et al. (1996) demonstrated that a small
to assess them, as well as the same latent vari- sample of FLE patients (n = 23) performed worse
ables that plague epilepsy research in general. In than a set of TLE patients on measures of psycho-
addition, there is a great deal of heterogeneity in motor speed and motor coordination. Upton and
terms of pathology and seizure localization Thompson (1996) compared the performance of
within FL regions, which contributes to differing presurgical FLE patients to that of TLE patients
neurocognitive profiles. Complex partial sei- on a variety of motor tasks, finding that the FLE
zures in FLE commonly arise from the frontal group performed worse than the TLE group on
poles and from the orbital, medial, and dorsolat- tasks of motor sequencing and bimanual hand
eral FL regions (Williamson, Spencer, Spencer, movements (left FLE worse than right FLE).
Novelly, & Mattson, 1985). Studies in children with FLE seizure onset have
94 D.L. Drane

Table 4.1 Core pre- and postsurgical deficits in temporal lobe epilepsy patients
Type of presurgical deficit Laterality findings Change with surgery
Language functions
Naming (auditory/visual) Both left and right TLE—frequently exhibit Left TLE—decline
deficits (left > right) Right TLE—no decline
Semantic fluency Both left and right TLE—frequently exhibit Left TLE—decline
deficits (may vary by type of category) Right TLE—data is sparse/appears
decline possible
Letter fluency Both left and right TLE—often exhibit deficits Left and right TLE—no decline/may
improve with seizure freedom
Action (verb) fluency Possibly same pattern as letter fluency/data is Possibly same/data is sparse
sparse
Other language functions Left TLE—usually normal unless patient has Left TLE—no decline unless
dysphasia secondary to an additional encroaching on classic language areas
neurological cause (e.g., stroke) Right TLE—no decline
Right TLE—intact core language functions
Memory and learning
Material-specific memory Left TLE—often exhibits auditory/verbal Left TLE—auditory/verbal memory
deficits may be observed memory deficits often declines
Right TLE—sometimes (but less consistently) Right TLE—visual memory often
exhibits visual memory deficits declines (depends more on specific
tasks than previously recognized)
Task-specific Left TLE—impairment on difficult Declines in functioning can occur that
dissociations associational and rote learning paradigms are consistent with the specified
in performance more associated with mesial TL dysfunction. preoperative patterns
Impairment on semantically related tasks more More research is needed to pin down
associated with lateral TL dysfunction (e.g., specific structure-function
story recall, easy word pairs) relationships
Right TLE—often exhibits deficits involving
object-location recall (particularly when
assessed in three dimensions) and route
learning
General memory Both left and right TLE—sometimes exhibit Decline still seems to reflect the task
dysfunction is observed general or global memory dysfunction at and material-specific patterns that
in some TLE patients baseline occur with surgery
Learning and retention Left TLE—often exhibits good initial retention No data has been published examining
patterns may differ by of words, but poor retention over time the differential learning patterns
side of seizure onset Right TLE—often exhibits problems with postoperatively
initial learning of visual material but retain
most of what is encoded/learned over time
Visuo-perceptual/visual-spatial processing and object recognition
Visuo-perceptual deficits Right TLE—occasionally has visuo-perceptual Both left TLE and right TLE—may
tend to be infrequent deficits experience a visual-field cut following
surgery
Left TLE—no decline on perceptual
tasks
Right TLE—mild decline on some
tasks on occasion
Visual-spatial deficits Right TLE patients—may rarely have Left TLE—no decline
tend to be infrequent visual-spatial deficits (e.g., problems judging Right TLE—very rare decline on
spatial features) some tasks
Category-related object Left TLE—occasionally sees mild recognition Left TLE—possible decline on
recognition deficits deficits for man-made objects with posterior recognition of man-made objects with
TL involvement encroachment on left posterior regions
Right TLE—frequently exhibit recognition Right TLE—frequent decline on
deficits for famous persons/faces, landmarks, recognition of famous persons/faces,
and animals with anterior TL dysfunction landmarks, and animals
(continued)
4 Neuropsychological Evaluation of the Epilepsy Surgical Candidate 95

Table 4.1 (continued)


Type of presurgical deficit Laterality findings Change with surgery
General intellectual functioning
IQ scores Typically normal in TLE May see “material-specific” declines
Left TLE will sometimes show greater in line with presurgical status
problems with verbal tasks, and right TLE will May see improvements in processing
sometimes show greater problems with and attention regardless of hemisphere
perceptual tasks of seizure focus if patient experiences
a reduced seizure burden and a
reduction in AEDs
Executive control processes
Complex problem Both left and right TLE—often exhibit deficits Both left and right TLE—frequently
solving, response in one of more of these areas that is assumed show improved performance in these
inhibition, generative to result from the disruption of temporofrontal domains if experiencing seizure
fluency, complex networks secondary to epileptiform activity freedom
attention
For the purposes of this table, we are assuming normal language lateralization to the left cerebral hemisphere (i.e.,
left = dominant)
TLE temporal lobe epilepsy, TL temporal lobe, AEDs antiepileptic drugs

reported greater problems with motor coordina- suggesting lateralization to the nondominant hemi-
tion (Hernandez, Sauerwein, Jambaque, et al., sphere (Jones-Gotman & Milner, 1977) and others
2002) as compared to normal controls or children not (McDonald, Delis, Norman, Tecoma, & Iragui,
with other seizure onset. 2005). One study reported worse performance for
Performance on complex problem-solving tasks patients with left FLE (McDonald, Delis, Norman,
also appears to be impaired in some FLE patients, Tecoma, et al., 2005) as compared to right FLE
and this finding has been observed both pre- and while another demonstrated that FLE patients per-
postoperatively (Upton & Thompson, 1996). formed worse than TLE patients regardless of sei-
Milner published a classic case study involving zure onset laterality (Suchy, Sands, & Chelune,
patient “K.M.,” who demonstrated severe deficits 2003). A fourth study found that patients with FLE
on the WCST following bilateral resection of the produced a similar number of designs as did
anterior FLs for the control of seizures despite patients with TLE but made more design errors
maintaining normal IQ. As noted in the section on (Helmstaedter, Kemper, et al., 1996). Discrepancies
TLE, however, complex problem-solving deficits across studies may in part reflect differences in the
can also be seen in patients with TLE, likely due to design fluency tasks themselves, as these measures
the spread of seizure activity to FL regions. differ in regard to the structure they provide, the
Verbal fluency, as noted in the TLE section, is presence or absence of concomitant task demands
often impaired in presurgical FLE regardless of (e.g., shifting attention), and the aspect of perfor-
laterality of seizure onset and includes both mance emphasized (e.g., design generation, self-
semantic and letter fluency. There is also some monitoring, shifting).
data indicating that action (verb) fluency is also Some evidence suggests that FLE patients
decreased in FLE patients. Preliminary evidence may perform worse than TLE patients on mea-
exists that performance on these tasks can some- sures of attention, working memory, and psycho-
times yield localizing data when explored in motor speed. For example, Helmstaedter,
ways that examine component skills required for Kemper, et al. (1996) demonstrated that a small
successful completion (Drane et al., 2006). sample of FLE patients performed worse than a
Design fluency has also been shown to be set of TLE patients on measures of attention and
decreased in FLE patients relative to other epilepsy psychomotor speed. In the FLE group, differ-
patients or healthy controls, with some studies ences were not related to side of seizure focus or
96 D.L. Drane

presence of a structural lesion. Upton and and colleagues (2001) more recently demon-
Thompson (1996) found that a small group of strated this pattern in postoperative FLE patients
FLE patients made more errors on a complex and provided further evidence that encoding/
visual scanning and tracking measure than did a retrieval deficits may underlie this pattern. In
comparable TLE group. their study, postsurgical TLE patients did not dis-
There are many additional studies with FLE play release from proactive interference, and
patients demonstrating deficits on a variety of there was no difference between the TLE and
tasks presumed to be sensitive to FL dysfunction, FLE groups in terms of consolidation of stimuli
but most of these are isolated findings that have (i.e., they showed similar rates of retention over
yet to be replicated. Areas of reported dysfunc- trials). Milner has also shown that FLE patients
tion have included deficient cost estimation have difficulty structuring and segregating events
(Upton & Thompson, 1996), an elevated rate of in memory (Milner, 1968b), and her FLE patient
questions required to identify objects on the samples have also exhibited problems with orga-
Twenty Questions Test from the DKEFS (Upton nization of materials to be learned and have had
& Thompson, 1999), problems with determining trouble recalling the temporal order of informa-
temporal order (McAndrews & Milner, 1991), tion (Milner, Petrides, & Smith, 1985). These
and aspects of social cognition (e.g., humor findings applied to a wide range of stimuli and
appreciation, recognition of facial emotion, per- may be material specific in nature (Milner, Corsi,
ception of eye gaze expression) (Farrant, Morris, & Leonard, 1991).
Russell, et al., 2005). Kemper, Helmstaedter, & There have been few systematic studies of
Elger (1993) reported that FLE patients per- psychiatric functioning in patients with FLE,
formed much worse than TLE patients on a plan- although a number of case reports describe wide-
ning task, suggesting that this difference correctly ranging interictal behavioral abnormalities. For
predicted the seizure focus of 80 % of all cases. example, Boone et al. (1988) described a young
Memory performance has not been studied adolescent girl with FL seizures who experienced
extensively in patients with FL seizure onset, and reversible behavioral changes including sexual
available results are somewhat mixed. Most stud- disinhibition, loss of concern for personal
ies have either compared performance between hygiene, physical and verbal aggression, and
FLE and TLE patients with one another or with pressured and tangential speech accompanying
healthy controls. While some of these studies interictal anterior FL discharges. Patients with
have failed to demonstrate differences between anterior cingulate seizure foci have also been
FLE and controls on memory measures (Delaney, reported to develop interictal psychosis, aggres-
Rosen, Mattson, & Novelly, 1980), others have sion, sociopathic behavior, sexual deviancy, irri-
reported worse functioning for FLE, at least on tability, obsessive-compulsive disorder, and poor
some types of tasks. FLE patients tend to perform impulse control (Devinsky, Morrell, & Vogt,
worse on more complex learning paradigms (e.g., 1995). Additionally, Helmstaedter (2001) has
list-learning tasks), with their limitations attrib- also reported that FLE patients have an elevated
uted to problems with encoding and/or retrieval. rate of behavioral problems compared to other
There have also been some interesting studies epilepsy patients and controls but noted that these
suggesting that certain aspects of learning and tended to be mild as compared to the findings
memory are perhaps more impaired in FLE than obtained in other neurological patients with
in other epilepsy groups. For example, Pigott and structural FL lesions. Based on the limited stud-
Milner (1993) demonstrated that preoperative ies available, psychiatric conditions such as
FLE patients exhibit problems with release from depression and anxiety appear to be more com-
proactive interference (i.e., earlier memories mon in TLE (Gilliam et al., 2004).
interfere with of learning new information). In summary, it appears that patients with FLE
Milner attributed this deficit to problems with present with a variety of deficits involving motor
encoding and retrieval mechanisms. McDonald functioning, executive control processes, attention,
4 Neuropsychological Evaluation of the Epilepsy Surgical Candidate 97

speed of processing, and aspects of memory per- prospective studies of neurocognitive functioning
formance, as well as some possible behavioral in these patients that include both pre- and post-
abnormalities. These functions have been mini- operative analysis. Studies appearing in the liter-
mally explored in FLE patients with few studies ature tend to involve retrospective analysis of
using a presurgical/postsurgical decline and most clinical data. For example, one recent study
seeming to be underpowered. There have been vir- examined retrospective pre- and postsurgical
tually no attempts to explore functions by FL sub- clinical data collected on 28 PCE patients
region in any epilepsy study, yet this methodology between 1991 and 2000 (Luerding, Boesebeck,
has proven useful in other areas. Similarly, numer- & Ebner, 2004). These investigators indicated
ous cognitive functions attributed to the FLs have that mild declines occurred in performance IQ
yet to be explored in FLE patients. Some areas of from the WAIS-R regardless of resected hemi-
dysfunction observed in patients with FLE have sphere and also reported declines in some mea-
also been observed in patients with TLE, presum- sures of visual-spatial processing. Postsurgical
ably due to seizure spread across large intercon- gains were made on some tasks thought to be
nected neural networks. There also appear to be mediated by the FLs, and there was no decline in
some distinct patterns between patients with FLE WAIS-R verbal IQ. Of note, however, not only
and TLE that may yet be useful for confirming the was the sample size very small, but this resulted
region of seizure onset (Table 4.2). in a pool of subjects with potentially very differ-
ent lesions (e.g., left temporo-occipital verssu
right inferior parietal). Also, only a limited num-
Findings in Posterior Cortical ber of subtests from the WAIS-R were available
Epilepsy (PCE) for examination. Overall, while this type of study
of neurocognitive function of patients with PCE
Seizures arising from the parietal lobe, the occip- is sorely needed, such studies cannot definitively
ital lobe, the occipital border of the temporal answer these questions due to a lack of sufficient
lobe, or a combination of these regions are some- power and inadequate coverage of potential
times referred to as posterior cortical epilepsies, domains to be examined. Other retrospective
as it is difficult to find clear anatomic or patho- studies of PCE, particularly those involving pari-
physiological differences in these regions etal lobe dysfunction, have reported changes in
(Dalmagro, Bianchin, Velasco, et al., 2005). The visual functioning, visual-spatial processing, and
occurrence of posterior cortical epilepsies tends visuo-perceptual abilities (Siegel & Williamson,
to be much rarer, and such conditions have been 2000). Sensory changes are sometimes observed
less well studied (Binder, Lehe, Kral, et al., when surgical resection extends into the postcen-
2008). Therefore, for the purposes of this chapter, tral gyrus, and one study has reported distur-
we have decided to consider all of the work bances of body image in a few patients with right
related to neurocognitive profiles related to pari- inferior parietal corticectomies (Salanova,
etal or occipital lobe seizure onset together. Andermann, Rasmussen, Olivier, & Quesney,
Dalmagro and colleagues (2005) reported that 1995). One very small, retrospective study exam-
just over 5 % of their total referrals for long-term ining the neurocognitive status of children with
video-EEG monitoring experienced PCE, and of occipital lobe (OL) seizure onset suggested that
these, approximately half were actually surgical such patients experience an elevated rate of scho-
candidates. Overall, this group makes up well lastic difficulty, psychiatric disorders (i.e., pri-
under 10 % of the total surgical referrals seen by marily depression), and cognitive dysfunction
a standard epilepsy surgical program, making involving problems with face processing and
this type of seizure onset even less common making spatial judgments (Chilosi, Brovedani,
than FLE. Moscatelli, Bonanni, & Guerrini, 2006).
Cognitive studies of PCE patients are lacking A recent study completed in Germany with a
in general, and there have been no systematic small series of OL epilepsy surgical patients
98 D.L. Drane

Table 4.2 Core pre- and postsurgical deficits in frontal lobe epilepsy patients
Type of presurgical deficit Laterality findings Change with surgery
General intellectual functioning Typically normal in FLE Typically no significant change
Language Typically normal apart from verbal Typically no significant change
fluency deficits (unless neurologic/ with the exception of verbal
functional disruption of classic speech fluency performance (see
regions, e.g., Broca’s area) below)
Motor functioning Left and right FLE—often exhibit motor Both left and right FLE—may
deficits contralateral to side of seizure show a decline in the motor
focus (e.g., gross motor speed, fine motor performance of their
speed, and dexterity) contralateral limbs (particularly
when surgery encroaches on
precentral gyrus region)
Response inhibition Both left and right FLE—often exhibit Both left and right FLE—may
deficits decline depending upon location
of FL resection
Complex problem solving Both left and right FLE—often exhibit Both left and right FLE—may
deficits decline depending upon location
of FL resection
Verbal fluency Both left and right FLE—often exhibit Both left and right FLE—may
baseline deficits on all types of verbal decline on all verbal fluency
fluency tasks tasks, although semantic fluency
may improve in some cases
Design fluency Both left and right FLE—often exhibit Both left and right FLE—may
deficits decline
Memory functioning Both left and right FLE—often exhibit Surgery may improve or worsen
poor learning/encoding and decreased baseline problems based on
free recall with good recognition memory location of surgery and
Both left and right FLE—often exhibit postsurgical seizure freedom
problems with release from proactive
interference
Attention Both left and right FLE—often exhibit Data is lacking. Any change is
deficits in primary and complex attention likely dependent upon seizure
status and AED regimen at
follow-up assessment
Social cognition Some patients, regardless of laterality, Data is lacking. Theoretically,
have shown problems with recognizing it appears that some functions
humor and faux pas errors, recognition of could decline depending upon
facial emotion, and perception of eye surgical variables, while seizure
gaze expression freedom and decreased AEDs
may contribute to mild gains
Visuo-perceptual, visual- Typically normal on most visuo- Data is lacking. However, no
spatial, and constructional perceptual and visual-spatial tasks reports of significant declines in
praxis Often exhibit decreased performance on these areas exist in the research
constructional tasks due to poor literature
organization and planning
For the purposes of this table, we are assuming normal language lateralization to the left cerebral hemisphere (i.e.,
left = dominant)
FLE frontal lobe epilepsy, AEDs antiepileptic drugs

prospectively examined visual-field integrity, (Binder et al., 2008). This study and others have
demonstrating that a significant proportion of shown that preoperative patients with seizure
these patients experienced visual-field defects onset involving the mesial OL are more likely to
postoperatively (i.e., 42 % of OL patients experi- exhibit baseline visual-field defects (e.g., reports
enced new or increased visual-field defects) suggest approximately 40–50 %) than those with
4 Neuropsychological Evaluation of the Epilepsy Surgical Candidate 99

Table 4.3 Core pre- and postsurgical deficits in posterior cortical epilepsy patients
Type of presurgical deficit Laterality findings Change with surgery
General intellectual Typically normal in PCE at baseline Limited research suggests
functioning possibly mild declines in PIQ for
PCE patients regardless of side
of surgery/laterality and mild
improvements in VIQ
Language Depends on seizure focus: Typically no significant change
Left PLE—may exhibit classic language deficits with the exception of possible
(e.g., naming, repetition, comprehension) language declines with some left
Right PLE and both left and right OLE—unlikely parietal lesions
to exhibit language deficits
Visuo-perception, acuity, Both left and right OLE—often exhibit problems Left and right OLE—often exhibit
and visual fields with visuo-perception (including face processing/ new or increased visual-field cuts
recognition) Left and right PCE—in general,
Left and right OLE—often exhibit baseline may exhibit mild or greater
visual-field cuts (much more common for medial visuo-perceptual decrements
OL seizure onset) depending on aspects of surgery
Left and right OLE—might expect baseline (i.e., location and extent)
deficits in color processing and object
localization, as well as positive visual phenomena
(yet epilepsy specific research is absent)
Visual-spatial processing Both left and right OLE and right PLE—often Both left and right PCE—declines
exhibit problems with visual-spatial judgments in some aspects of visual-spatial
processing (limited research)
Memory Presumed to be normal Presumed to remain at baseline
apart from possible gains related
to improvements in seizure status
and reduced AED regimen
Sensory functioning Both left and right PLE—may exhibit baseline Sensory functioning is often
problems with sensory discrimination worse in patients if surgery
encroaches upon postcentral gyrus
Disturbance of body image has
been reported in patients with
inferior parietal resections
Motor Presumed to be normal Presumed to be normal
For the purposes of this table, we are assuming normal language lateralization to the left cerebral hemisphere (i.e.,
left = dominant)
PCE posterior cortical epilepsy, PLE parietal lobe epilepsy, OLE occipital lobe epilepsy, AEDs antiepileptic drugs

lateral OL onset (e.g., ranging from 0 to 18 %) studies in other neurological disorders and func-
(Binder et al., 2008; Blume, Wiebe, & Tapsell, tional imaging paradigms, that dysfunction in the
2005). While focusing on perceptual rather than OL could cause problems with face recognition,
cognitive testing per se, this type of pre-/postop- object localization, color processing, or object
erative design is exactly what is needed in this recognition (Kiper, Zesiger, Maeder, Deonna, &
area. At present, we lack definitive profiles for Innocenti, 2002) and that lesions in the parietal
preoperative functioning in the posterior cortical region could cause deficits involving visual-
epilepsies and have no prospective postsurgical spatial processing, object recognition, sensory
outcome studies available for this patient group. discrimination, arithmetic skills, and aspects of
One would assume, based on available lesion language functioning (Table 4.3).
100 D.L. Drane

AEDs is therefore critical to interpreting neuro-


Potential Confounds cognitive results in this patient population, and
in the Neuropsychological there may be instances where it is worthwhile to
Assessment of the Epilepsy Surgical take a patient off of their usual AEDs prior to
Patient evaluation.

In assessing epilepsy surgical patients, a variety


of factors can obscure an individual’s true neuro- Problems with Instrument Design
cognitive profile, including disease- and
treatment-related variables, as well as limitations Problems with features of test design and selec-
associated with current assessment paradigms tion can also muddle the interpretation of neuro-
and our knowledge of brain-behavior relation- cognitive data. For example, test selection
ships. The clinical neuropsychologist must be potentially becomes a barrier to discovering
aware of these issues in order to take them into accurate brain-behavior relations when we
consideration when interpreting assessment employ measures that require the interaction of
results. Specific factors with the potential to cre- multiple cognitive skills controlled by different
ate “noise” in our assessments include the effect brain regions without a clear awareness of these
of AEDs on brain functioning, problems with the relationships. For example, the Family Pictures
specific tests being employed, comorbid medical subtest of the 3rd edition of the Wechsler Memory
and psychiatric conditions, acute ictal/interictal Scale (Wechsler, 1997) contributes to the Visual
epileptiform activity, and the acute and long-term Memory Index from this battery, yet there is
impact of seizure activity on brain regions distant strong evidence that it loads on a verbal factor
from the seizure focus. There is no absolute (Dulay et al., 2002). This is likely due to the
approach to dealing with these issues, and this necessity to accurately name the pictured indi-
leads to a number of decisions regarding when viduals in order to get credit for any aspect of
and where to conduct the neuropsychological recall on this task. In turn, however, we often see
assessment and the selection of appropriate tests. a decline on Family Pictures, as well as the Visual
Memory Index to which it contributes, in patients
who undergo a dominant (typically left)
Effects of AEDs hemisphere resection (Chapin, Busch, Naugle, &
Najm, 2009). If someone were to explore the pos-
Many AEDs can have an appreciable impact upon sibility of material-specific memory deficits in
brain functioning, as they function to dampen the TLE using the Verbal and Visual Memory Indices
neuronal irritability that constitutes a seizure, yet of the WMS-3, they could easily draw wrong
they also more broadly dampen neuronal excit- conclusions if they were unaware of this pattern
ability in general (Drane & Meador, 2002). This of findings.
can lead to the emergence of cognitive deficits
that are unrelated to the epileptic focus. For exam-
ple, a TLE patient treated with topiramate may Nociferous Cortex Hypothesis
present with limitations in primary attention, ver-
bal fluency, and processing speed that have noth- The nociferous cortex or “neural noise” hypoth-
ing to do with their underlying seizure focus esis suggests that seizure activity can disrupt
(Kockelmann, Elger, & Helmstaedter, 2003; more expansive neural networks that extend
Ojemann et al., 2001). Presurgical evaluation in beyond the irritative zone of the seizure (Penfield
such a patient may fail to confirm the lateraliza- & Jasper, 1954). As covered earlier, executive
tion or localization of the seizure focus and will control processes thought to be primarily medi-
provide a significant underestimation of the ated by FL regions (e.g., letter fluency, complex
patient’s abilities. Knowledge of the effects of problem solving) can be disrupted in patients
4 Neuropsychological Evaluation of the Epilepsy Surgical Candidate 101

with TL seizure onset (Hermann & Seidenberg, profile pattern in the case of focal seizure onset, it
1995). These apparent deficits frequently resolve can also obscure this pattern when there is second-
with the successful control of the TL seizure ary generalization or non-focal patterns of interic-
activity (Martin et al., 2000), and these altera- tal discharges (Aarts, Binnie, Smit, & Wilkins,
tions in the functioning of the FL cortex can be 1984; Aldenkamp & Arends, 2004; Binnie, 2003;
captured with functional neuroimaging (Jokeit Kasteleijn-Nolst Trenite & Vermeiren, 2005).
et al., 1997; Maccotta et al., 2007). It is also
thought that FL seizures will disrupt limbic and
TL regions, although less research is available to Practice Effects
confirm such patterns. Overall, the potential for
distal effects of epileptiform activity can obvi- Neuropsychological assessment in epilepsy
ously obscure the central seizure focus. requires repeated testing over time, which neces-
sitates consideration of possible practice effects.
Some studies have been completed that examine
Effect of Comorbid Conditions test-retest changes in either epilepsy patients or
in healthy controls and that provide reliable
Medical and psychiatric conditions that are often change indices to allow one to determine if a
comorbid with epilepsy can also introduce greater given change is related to the treatment interven-
noise into a patient’s neurocognitive profile. For tion as opposed to a simple practice effect
example, patients with epilepsy experience a (Martin, Sawrie, Gilliam, et al., 2002). This can
higher rate of depression and a slightly elevated be particularly important when one recognizes
rate of psychosis as compared to the general pub- that a lack of an expected practice effect may
lic (Blumer, Montouris, & Hermann, 1995; reflect a limitation in a postsurgical patient.
Manchanda, 2002). Epilepsy patients struggling
with mood issues or perhaps even actively psy-
chotic may be less able to actively engage in test- Atypical Language Lateralization
ing. Similarly, epilepsy sometimes reflects a
secondary condition resulting from a more pri- Given that epilepsy is often associated with
mary medical condition (e.g., brain tumor, stroke, comorbid neurological disorders/injury, it is not
HSV encephalitis) or injury (e.g., traumatic brain surprising that one observes an elevated rate of
injury). The primary disease or injury contributes atypical language lateralization in this population
uniquely to the patient’s pattern of dysfunction (i.e., right or bilateral language). Many of these
and can mask any potential lateralizing/localiz- cases appear to represent language reorganiza-
ing neurocognitive findings. For example, tion that has occurred in individuals experiencing
patients with focal TL seizure onset resulting early-life injuries, although a few studies suggest
from posttraumatic epilepsy may exhibit signifi- there is likely a subset of patients with rare but
cant executive function impairment that is related naturally occurring atypical language lateraliza-
to potentially widespread cerebral dysfunction tion (Drane, Ojemann, Ojemann, et al., 2009;
resulting from the head trauma. Knecht, Jansen, Frank, et al., 2003). The possibil-
ity of atypical language lateralization must be
borne in mind when analyzing neurocognitive
Effect of Ictal and Interictal data and making outcome predictions.
Discharges In summary, it is recommended that the clini-
cal neuropsychologist makes every effort to be
There is growing awareness that acute ictal or aware of potential latent variables and to control
interictal epileptiform discharges can alter neuro- for their presence when possible. In this manner,
cognitive profiles. While the impact of such epi- one may be able to better localize or lateralize a
leptiform activity can sometimes accentuate a seizure event in a patient that otherwise showed
102 D.L. Drane

no focal findings. One may also gain better changes in performance in relation to ictal and
insight into the patient’s genuine performance interictal discharges. Inpatient testing also gives the
baseline in the absence of seizure activity. examiner a greater span of time to conduct tests.
Occasionally, it may also be possible to use these The downside of inpatient assessment involves
variables to one’s advantage such as using ictal or potentially dealing with acute changes in medica-
postictal assessment techniques to enhance focal tions, as the patient’s standard AED regimen may
findings. be altered or discontinued in order to provoke sei-
zures. Similarly, some EMUs will also employ
sleep deprivation as a means to induce seizures
Decisions About more rapidly. Depending upon the practice of the
Neuropsychological Assessment inpatient unit, these issues can often be more
in Epilepsy Surgical Patients effectively managed by agreeing upon a standard
start time for neurocognitive testing. In our prac-
Inpatient Versus Outpatient tice, we initiate the baseline testing at the first full
Assessment day of monitoring, prior to the patient being sleep
deprived and often prior to any changes in AED
There are various pros and cons to either testing regimen. This also provides us with electrophysi-
presurgical epilepsy patients on the inpatient unit ological data from the day of admit to insure that
versus in the outpatient clinic, and both practices acute seizure activity has not immediately pre-
continued to be employed with regularity by epi- ceded our assessment.
lepsy surgical programs throughout the world. Ideally, the epilepsy neuropsychology service
Conducting evaluations in the epilepsy moni- will have dedicated rooms on the inpatient moni-
toring unit (EMU) allows one to be aware of the toring unit that preserve the uninterrupted, pri-
presence of ictal and interictal epileptiform dis- vate environment while allowing the patient to
charges, as the patient is undergoing continuous continue with their video-EEG monitoring. If
video-EEG monitoring. This is probably its largest such rooms are not available, bedside testing can
advantage, as increasing data demonstrates that also work adequately, if proper steps are taken to
subclinical and interictal epileptiform activity can educate staff about not interrupting. While deal-
have a transient disruptive impact upon neurocog- ing with additional staff creates additional work
nitive functioning (Aldenkamp & Arends, 2004; for the examiner, having them available to assist
Kasteleijn-Nolst Trenite & Vermeiren, 2005). It is with the patient if a seizure occurs is quite advan-
likely that transient changes in performance some- tageous, particularly with patients that experi-
times lead to an underestimation of the patient’s ence seizures on a frequent basis.
baseline functioning (see current clinical vignette), The advantages and disadvantages of conduct-
which can obscure change over time (e.g., leading ing presurgical evaluations in the outpatient setting
one to miss declines in performance subsequent to are essentially the opposite of those just cited for
surgical intervention or other treatment) and con- inpatient assessment. The major disadvantage is
tribute to false predictions regarding outcome. For that one has no objective knowledge of the imme-
example, if someone appears to have severely diate electrophysiological functioning of the patient
impaired verbal memory due to transient epilepti- that they are assessing. Epileptiform activity may
form activity, we might erroneously predict that be occurring during the examination, with direct
their risk of decline is small due to their poor base- impact upon the assessment, and neither the exam-
line. This sort of error potentially gives the patient, iner nor the patient will be aware of its occurrence.
the neurosurgeon, and the rest of the treatment Similarly, the patient may have experienced a sei-
team misinformation for making their decisions zure within the last 24 h yet lack any recollection of
regarding the risk associated with surgery. On the this occurrence. In contrast, the patient is less likely
other hand, having concurrent electrophysiologi- to be sleep deprived, and they are most likely con-
cal data allows the neuropsychologist to explore tinuing with their standard treatment regimen.
4 Neuropsychological Evaluation of the Epilepsy Surgical Candidate 103

Our group, as well as others, has suggested that the Choice of Neuropsychological Tests
eventual standard for outpatient neuropsychologi-
cal assessment of epilepsy should include obtain- In general, when putting together a battery to
ing simultaneous EEG recordings (Patrikelis, assess epilepsy surgical patients, one wants to
Angelakis, & Gatzonis, 2009). cover all of the standard neurocognitive domains.
Financial considerations may also play a role However, the relative emphasis to place on a
in deciding upon assessment venue, as some pay- given domain and the choice of specific tests
ors may incentivize one type of assessment over should be guided by the overarching purposes of
another. Nevertheless, we have often been suc- the neuropsychological evaluation in the epilepsy
cessful in getting policies adjusted by providing surgical setting. More specifically, tests should
data to explain our preferences in assessment be chosen that are potentially useful for:
location depending upon the factors involved. (a) Confirming the epileptic focus: Knowledge
This is an area where standardized policies could of deficits commonly observed in various
conceivably be established by interested practice epilepsy syndromes preoperatively will aid
organizations. in the selection of tests that are useful for lat-
eralizing/localizing purposes.
(b) Demonstrating postoperative change in func-
Reliable Change Indices Versus tion: One should include measures that
Alternate Test Forms examine functions known to commonly
decline with a particular type of surgery (e.g.,
The serial assessment of epilepsy patients under- verbal memory decline following dominant
going surgery necessitates correction for practice TL resection), allowing one to assess out-
effects. Since the early 1990s, some neuropsy- come. Of note, postsurgical change can also
chologists have started using advanced methods be positive, as in the case of someone show-
of measuring change, including the Reliable ing improvement in processing speed and
Change Indices (RCIs) and standard regression- attention secondary to becoming seizure-free
based (SRB) change score norms (Hermann and discontinuing their AED regimen.
et al., 1996; Sawrie, Chelune, Naugles, & Luders, (c) Insuring valid interpretation: Measures cho-
1996). Both are methods that attempt to statisti- sen in this area assess the impact of latent
cally control for test-retest effects and measure- variables commonly occurring in the epi-
ment error, allowing the clinician to better lepsy surgical setting. For example, by using
evaluate whether change in performance is actu- performance validity measures, one deter-
ally related to a specific treatment intervention. mines whether the evaluation has been inval-
RCI and SRB scores based on the performance of idated by factors such as poor motivation or
unoperated patients with epilepsy and on healthy task engagement on the part of the patient or
control subjects are available for a number of the possible effect of other disease-related
neurocognitive measures (Dikmen, Heaton, variables (e.g., subclinical or interictal epi-
Grant, & Temkin, 1999; Heaton et al., 2001; leptiform discharges).
Temkin, Heaton, Grant, & Dikmen, 1999). (d) Allowing research to be performed: Measures
Examining the performance of the same type of should be used allowing one to research basic
patient allows for the greatest control of other questions about brain-behavior functions and
disease-related variables. surgical outcome for specific cognitive skills,
Another approach is to use alternative forms psychosocial and vocational functioning, emo-
of commonly used tests when available. The tional/psychiatric processing, seizure freedom,
major problem involved in using alternative test and quality of life.
forms relates to the difficulty we have insuring Table 4.4 lists specific neurocognitive func-
that each alternative version of a test is actually tions proven important to assess for several com-
equivalent to the original. mon types of epilepsy and specific tests that have
104 D.L. Drane

Table 4.4 Core neurocognitive functions to be assessed in epilepsy surgical patients and suggested tests
Neurocognitive Within domain areas to emphasize Possible tests to consider
domains during assessment
Language − Naming (e.g., visual, auditory/ − Boston Naming Test, Columbia Auditory Naming
naming to description, category Test, Category- Related Naming Tests
related) − Category fluency tasks (e.g., animals, supermarket
− Verbal fluency (semantic, letter, items), DKEFS Verbal Fluency, Controlled Oral
and action) Word Association Test, action fluency
− Screen reading and other core − Recognition Reading Subtest of the Wide Range
language tasks Achievement Test, American Version of the
National Adult Reading Test (AMNART), Wechsler
Test of Adult Reading, Token Test, sentence
repetition
Attention − Primary attention (auditory and − Digit Span Forward (WAIS), Picture
visual) Completion (WAIS)
− Complex attention (auditory and − Digit Span Backwards and Letter-Number
visual) Sequencing (WAIS), Trail Making Tests, spatial
− Sustained attention (auditory and span
visual) − Continuous Performance Test (not used as
commonly by most epilepsy centers)
Visual processing − Visuo-perception − Visual Object and Space Perception (VOSP)
− Visual-spatial Battery, Facial Recognition Test
− Object recognition – Judgment of Line Orientation
− Famous Faces Test, Category-Related Object
Recognition Tests
Constructional − Graphomotor copying tasks − Copying simple shapes (e.g., Greek cross, Necker
praxis − Assembly tasks cube), Rey Complex Figure Test (Copy)
− Block Design (WAIS)
Memory and − Auditory/verbal Learning, memory − Rey Auditory/Verbal Learning Test, California
learning retention, and recognition Verbal Learning Test, Verbal Selective
− List-learning Tasks Reminding Test
− Contextual memory − Logical Memory Subtest (Wechsler Scales), Reitan
− Associative learning Story Memory
− Visual learning, memory retention, − Verbal Paired Associates (VPA) Subtest (Wechsler
and recognition Scales; WMS-III VPA appears less helpful than
− Simple geometric designs other versions, as it eliminated the easier word
− Face recall pairs)
− Complex visual designs − Visual Reproduction (Wechsler Memory Scales:
− Remote recall older versions appear to be more useful for
lateralization than the 3rd edition)
− Face Recall/Hospital Facial Recognition Task
− Rey Complex Figure Test, MCG Complex Figures
− Information Subtest (WAIS)
Executive control − Complex problem solving − Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, Brixton Spatial
processes − Response inhibition Anticipation Task, Iowa Gambling Task
− Complex attention/mental − Color-Word Interference (Stroop) Test, Hayling
flexibility Test, Go/No-Go Tasks
− Abstract reasoning − Trail Making Test, Mental Control (WMS)
− Generative fluency tasks (verbal − Similarities Subtest/Matrix Reasoning Subtest
and visual) (WAIS)
− Metacognition − DKEFS Verbal Fluency, DKEFS Design Fluency,
5-Point Design Fluency
− Cognitive Estimation Tasks
General – Verbal and performance IQ – Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (various editions)
intellectual – Wechsler ASI
functioning
(continued)
4 Neuropsychological Evaluation of the Epilepsy Surgical Candidate 105

Table 4.4 (continued)


Neurocognitive Within domain areas to emphasize Possible tests to consider
domains during assessment
Academic – Reading recognition – Wide Range Achievement Test (WRAT)—reading
achievement – Reading comprehension recognition
– Mathematical skills – Gray Oral Reading Test (GORT)
– Spelling ability – WRAT—arithmetic
– WRAT—spelling
Performance – Determine task engagement. This – Word Memory Test
validity testing can be disrupted due to issues – Medical Symptom Validity Test
including poor motivation as well – Victoria Symptom Validity Test
as the impact of acute seizures and – “Embedded” Measures of Task Engagementa
epileptiform activity
WAIS Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, WMS-III Wechsler Memory Scale (3rd edition), MCG Medical College of
Georgia
a
Embedded measures of task engagement refer to attempts to use improbable performances on standard clinical tests in
order to recognize possible test invalidity

Table 4.5 Sensory, motor, mood and personality, and quality-of-life variables to be assessed in epilepsy surgical
patients and suggested tests
Neurocognitive Within domain areas to emphasize
domains during assessment Possible tests to consider
Sensory – Visual, auditory, and tactile acuities – Snellen eye chart
– Extinction to double simultaneous stimulation
– Tactile Form Recognition
– Reitan-Klove Sensory Examination
Motor – Handedness – Edinburgh Handedness Scale
– Gross motor speed – Finger Tapping Test
– Fine motor speed and dexterity – Grooved Pegboard Test
– Grip strength – Hand dynamometer
– Psychomotor speed – WAIS subtests (e.g., symbol search, digit symbol)
Mood and – Mood and emotional status – Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory
personality – Psychopathology (2nd edition or restructured form)
– Personality features – Personality Assessment Inventory (PAI)
– Somatizational/conversion profile – Brief Self-Report Inventories (e.g., Beck
Depression and Anxiety Scales)
– Mini Psychiatric Inventory (MINI)
Quality of life – Adjustment to seizures and treatment Quality of Life in Epilepsy (QOLIE)
(e.g., AEDs, surgical intervention) Washington Psychosocial Seizure Inventory (WPSI)
– Satisfaction with social support and
vocational and interpersonal functioning
WAIS Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, AEDs antiepileptic drugs

been used to assess these functions in the context used and integrated in the neuropsychological
of epilepsy. Table 4.5 presents similar data for assessment.
motor and sensory functioning, mood, personal-
ity, and quality-of-life assessment. Obviously,
tests selected for use need to be psychometrically Language
sound (e.g., valid, reliable) and preferably have
RCI or SRB scores or alternative forms for repeat It is essential to assess aspects of naming and ver-
assessment. In addition, the following section bal fluency given the presurgical baseline deficits
provides a brief overview of how the findings in in these areas and due to the postsurgical decline
each of these neurocognitive domains may be in these skills observed following dominant TL
106 D.L. Drane

resection (see the presurgical/postsurgical defi- worthwhile but does not appear to be routinely
cits section above and corresponding tables for implemented in most clinical evaluations of
specifics). Studies suggest sampling a wider epilepsy.
array of categories with these tasks and indicate
that altering basic task demands can lead to dif-
ferent yet equally important findings (e.g., use of Visual Processing and Object
auditory naming-to-description tasks versus stan- Recognition
dard visual confrontational naming) (Drane et al.,
2008; Hamberger & Seidel, 2003). An assess- A thorough assessment should include a screen
ment of reading ability is useful to establish a of visual acuity and gross confrontational evalua-
patient’s ability to perform tasks requiring read- tion of visual fields to insure that the patient can
ing, to estimate premorbid function, and to moni- adequately process visual information (some-
tor postsurgical changes in this function. times this information is available from the neu-
Screening other language functions (e.g., audi- rology exam). Additionally, we routinely assess
tory comprehension, repetition) is also recom- both visuo-perception and visual-spatial process-
mended with epilepsy surgical patients. However, ing. We use the former term to denote the ability
a complete language assessment is typically not to perceive visual images (including object rec-
required, unless the proposed surgical resection ognition) and the latter to refer to the processing
is thought to encroach upon classic language of spatial relationships between objects and
regions (e.g., Wernicke’s and Broca’s areas), potentially the viewer. Theoretically, the visual
which most do not, or when the epilepsy surgical processing stream is divided into an inferior com-
candidate is experiencing baseline aphasia (e.g., ponent which mediates visuo-perception/object
usually the aphasia and seizures are resulting recognition processing (ventral stream “what”
from a common neurological cause in these pathway) and a superior stream that is involved in
cases, such as stroke or tumor). visual-spatial processing (dorsal stream “where”
pathway) (Ungerleider & Mishkin, 1982). The
ventral stream runs from the occipital lobe to the
Attention temporal lobe, while the dorsal stream runs from
the occipital lobe to the parietal lobe, and both
An assessment of primary attention processing can be disrupted in a variety of ways by seizures
(i.e., with minimal demands placed upon mental and surgical resection. For example, occipital
manipulation of information) is recommended lobe disturbances can cause a primary loss of
for a variety of reasons, including the assessment vision or disturbance in color processing, parietal
of the effect of AEDs and other medications lobe disturbances are often associated with defi-
(which can disrupt this domain) and insuring a cits in spatial and constructional processing, and
patient has the ability to focus on more complex inferior TL dysfunction can be associated with
tasks (which can be disturbed in patients not fully object recognition deficits. The Visual Object and
recovered from a seizure). More complex aspects Space Perception (VOSP) Battery (Warrington &
of attention should also be assessed that require James, 1991) has four subtests that load on a per-
mental flexibility and alternation/switching ceptual factor and four that load on a spatial fac-
between tasks and that place greater demands on tor, making it a solid, efficient method of
working memory capacity. Assessing both audi- evaluating many of these functions (Rapport,
tory and visual aspects of attention is useful, as Millis, & Bonello, 1998). Despite a lack of clini-
discrepancies between domains can sometimes cal tests, an assessment of object recognition is
be of lateralizing value (Duncan, Mirsky, recommended for epilepsy surgical patients given
Lovelace, & Theodore, 2009). Examining sus- recent evidence of category-related object recogni-
tained attention and response inhibition using a tion deficits in patients with right anterior TL dys-
continuous performance paradigm may also be function (e.g., famous faces, landmarks, animals)
4 Neuropsychological Evaluation of the Epilepsy Surgical Candidate 107

(Drane et al., 2008, 2009; Glosser et al., 2003; different forms of learning can uncover residual
Seidenberg et al., 2002). FL dysfunction can also functions in a patient upon which one can capital-
potentially contribute to problems with frontal ize in future memory training and target in efforts
eye fields and visual tracking (Thurtell, to improve surgical outcome.
Mohamed, Luders, & Leigh, 2009), as well as the In general, associational learning (e.g., word
organization/planning that is required for many pairs) has proven to be one of the more sensitive
constructional tasks. tasks to mesial TL pathology (particularly unre-
lated word pairs) (Akanuma, Alarcon, Lum,
et al., 2003; Hermann et al., 1994; Squire, 1993).
Memory and Learning Nevertheless, semantically related word pairs
may be impacted by lateral TL resection and
Assessment of memory and learning is a core should be included as well. Having both easy
part of the evaluation of epilepsy surgical patients, (semantically related) and hard items also pro-
as the majority of them have seizures arising vides a better performance floor, which some
from TL or FL regions. Patterns of memory dys- believe improves our ability to lateralize dys-
function, as outlined above, can be very helpful function across the cerebral hemispheres. In this
for confirming seizure localization to one of these regard, the original versions of the Verbal Paired
regions and for lateralizing side of seizure onset. Associates task from the Wechsler Memory Scale
In addition, these functions are potentially at risk have fared better in terms of seizure lateralization
in such patients and therefore need to be carefully than did the version from the 3rd edition of this
examined. test, which dropped the easy items. Animal and
A minimal assessment of memory in epilepsy experimental literature suggests that other forms
should include a variety of test formats (e.g., free of associational learning may be beneficial for
recall versus recognition), measures with differ- the assessment of epilepsy as well (e.g., binding
ent learning demands (e.g., associational learn- sensory/perceptual data with verbal labels).
ing, rote recall, contextual and gist learning), and List-learning tasks have also been shown to
stimuli tapping different sensory modalities (e.g., be sensitive to left mesial TL dysfunction
auditory/verbal, visual). Including a recognition (Grammaldo, Giampa, Quarato, et al., 2006;
task format can help to distinguish encoding from Loring, Strauss, Hermann, et al., 2008). As with
retrieval deficits, which can contribute to local- associational tasks, using lists of words that are
ization of brain dysfunction (e.g., FL versus TL more difficult to link semantically places greater
impairment). Using both auditory/verbal and demands on mesial TL structures. This is borne
visual stimuli, one can explore possible material- out in studies examining the usefulness of various
specific patterns, which can aid in lateralizing the list-learning paradigms, with those allowing for
involved cerebral hemisphere. Using tasks that easier categorization (e.g., California Auditory/
place different demands on forms of learning is Verbal Learning Test) appearing less able to later-
recommended due to mounting evidence that alize dysfunction (Loring et al., 2008).
performance dissociations frequently occur Contextual learning, such as reflected by the
between learning approaches, which presumably commonly used paragraph and story recall tasks
reflects the underlying involvement of different that are available, has proven less sensitive to
brain regions (Saling, 2009). Ultimately, such mesial temporal lobe pathology (Rausch & Babb,
data will likely contribute to more specific identi- 1993; Saling et al., 1993). However, these mea-
fication of dysfunctional regions within a hemi- sures are affected by lateral TL pathology, and
sphere. While the specific structure-function some patients will experience decline on these
relationships in this regard remain murky (i.e., tasks with a standard TL resection. Inclusion of
which mesial TL structures are associated with contextual memory tasks also helps establish
which forms of learning?), research is starting to whether the patient has any functional memory
provide preliminary models. In addition, testing capacity remaining. Someone performing well
108 D.L. Drane

on these tasks seems to retain the capacity to et al., 2000). Given that FL and TL seizure onset
work at many jobs despite declines in other represent the bulk of partial epilepsies, the
aspects of more complex learning, as they appear assessment of executive functions frequently
to retain the gist of what transpires despite having plays a role in confirming seizure focus, estimat-
a difficult time recalling specific details. Such ing risk of decline with FL surgery and helping
persons can still decline with surgery (particu- to make predictions about possible areas of post-
larly resections including lateral cortex), even if surgical improvement in TL patients. A thorough
associational and rote learning are already evaluation of executive control functions should
severely impaired. Performance on contextual assess the three major divisions of the FLs (e.g.,
learning tasks has also been associated with drug dorsolateral, orbitofrontal, and mesial frontal).
effects (Salinsky et al., 2005), which can be use- Assessment of executive functions often requires
ful for teasing out the effect of AEDs. development and use of new tasks, as this is one
Finally, there are a number of learning para- cognitive area where novelty is particularly
digms that have yet to be commonly employed important (e.g., problem-solving tasks often
in the clinical epilepsy realm that should be become easier once a successful solution has
explored. These include route learning/way- been derived on a given occasion).
finding tasks, visual memory tasks that place
greater demands on allocentric memory (i.e.,
memory that is not based on one’s own posi- General Intellectual Functioning
tion in relation to the stimulus of interest),
semantic learning, autobiographical memory An assessment of general intellectual function-
and learning to criteria paradigms, and com- ing, which provides a broad overview of one’s
plex associational tasks that combine different overall level of ability, is useful for estimating
styles of learning. performance in specific neurocognitive domains
and making determinations regarding functional
capacity (e.g., ability to live independently, man-
Executive Control Processes age finances and medications, capacity to suc-
ceed in a vocational or academic environment).
Executive control processes refer to complex General cognitive ability also enters into deter-
cognitive activities typically attributed to the FL mining the patient’s capacity to make decisions
regions of the brain (e.g., abstract reasoning, regarding surgery. Performance patterns on IQ
self-monitoring, problem solving, response inhi- testing have not proven to be reliable indicators
bition, mental flexibility, complex attention). of lateralized seizure onset. However, they are
These skills coordinate the activity of more gen- sometimes helpful for this purpose, particularly
eral cognitive and perceptual functions mediated when purer indices of verbal and perceptual abil-
by other brain regions (e.g., effectively coordi- ity are compared after controlling for problems
nating motor and perceptual skills while altering with attention, processing speed, and motor dys-
performance in response to changing task function. Finally, general intellectual functioning
demands). As mentioned, dysfunction associated remains normal in the majority of patients with
with the FLs is often observed in both FL and TL epilepsy, although lower scores will be observed
epilepsy patients, with the latter patients pre- in a large number of patients, particularly those
sumably experiencing disruption of broader experiencing an early onset of refractory epi-
interconnections between brain regions second- lepsy, a long disease duration, comorbid neuro-
ary to epileptiform activity (Hermann & logical pathology, and a history of multiple
Seidenberg, 1995; Penfield & Jasper, 1954). FL episodes of generalized tonic-clonic seizures and
dysfunction observed in TL patients will often status epilepticus (Glosser, Cole, French, Saykin,
resolve with successful seizure control (Martin & Sperling, 1997).
4 Neuropsychological Evaluation of the Epilepsy Surgical Candidate 109

Academic Achievement Motor Functioning

An assessment of academic ability can be benefi- Evaluation of grip strength and manual dexterity
cial for making decisions regarding school func- of both upper extremities is recommended. The
tioning, which is particularly important for Finger Tapping Test provides a measure of gross
children and adolescents and adult patients motor speed while a task like the grooved peg-
engaged in educational pursuits. A brief screen- board evaluates fine motor dexterity and speed.
ing of academic skills typically assesses word These functions are sometimes diminished in
recognition, reading comprehension, spelling, patients with FL dysfunction (Helmstaedter,
and arithmetic. Presurgical performances in these Kemper, et al., 1996) and may decrease in
areas are occasionally helpful in localizing extra- patients undergoing resections that encroach
temporal seizures. Postsurgical changes in these upon the precentral gyrus of the FL (motor strip)
functions are not very common in FL and TL (Helmstaedter et al., 1998). A careful assessment
resections, which again represent the vast major- of handedness is also needed, as this can inform
ity of surgical patients. predictions regarding language lateralization.
Research suggests that “footedness” may add
additional predictive value as well (Drane, 2006).
Sensory Functioning A quick way to assess both in lieu of a standard-
ized rating scale can be to ask about which hand
As noted previously, a gross screening of percep- the patient uses to write and which foot they
tual functioning is essential to establish that would prefer to use to kick a ball through a goal-
patients can adequately perceive test stimuli. post in American football.
This should include checking visual acuity (e.g.,
Snellen eye chart), testing visual fields to con-
frontation, and examining basic tactile and audi- Mood, Personality, and Psychiatric
tory sensory functioning. Testing these functions Inventories
can be incorporated into an evaluation of hemi-
inattention to visual, auditory, and tactile stimu- Measures are needed to examine current mood
lation (e.g., exploring unilateral as well as and emotional issues, to examine lifetime preva-
bilateral simultaneous stimulation of the sensory lence of psychiatric disorders, and to rule out
modality being tested). Evaluation of apraxia, psychogenic nonepileptic spells (PNES).
finger agnosia, astereognosis, right/left orienta- Measures of mood are useful for monitoring lev-
tion, and achromatopsia and a more in-depth els of distress, tracking change over time, and
examination of tactile form recognition can pro- picking up on critical issues in need of interven-
vide lateralizing/localizing data in some patients. tion (e.g., active suicidal ideation). Epilepsy
Some research has been completed examining patients exhibit higher rates of psychiatric distur-
olfactory functioning in TLE patients as well, bance than does the general population, with
suggesting that such patients are often deficient commonly occurring comorbid conditions
at odor naming, discrimination, and recall (usu- including depression, anxiety disorders, and sub-
ally right TLE worse than left TLE and other epi- stance abuse (Manchanda, 2002). In the surgical
lepsies) (Carroll, Richardson, & Thompson, context, it is important to recognize and effec-
1993; Eskenazi, Cain, Novelly, & Mattson, 1983) tively deal with any psychiatric issues prior to
and that they may experience further decline with undergoing surgery and to recognize the develop-
surgery. A thorough sensory examination is cer- ment of any postsurgical problems in this regard.
tainly warranted in cases where posterior cortical Of note, however, measures of mood do not typi-
epilepsy is suspected and can be useful in all epi- cally allow for making psychiatric diagnoses and
lepsy surgical patients even if they have recently do not provide any information with regard to
undergone a neurological exam. lifetime prevalence rates. Kanner and colleagues
110 D.L. Drane

have demonstrated that knowledge of psychiatric neuropsychological data and make it possible to
syndrome and personal and familial psychiatric predict outcome related to surgery. Such infor-
history can be of benefit in predicting adverse mation includes age of seizure onset, duration of
responses to AEDs (Kanner, 2009; Kanner, Wuu, epilepsy, occurrence of febrile seizures, number
Faught, et al., 2003) and in determining the opti- of AEDs being taken, and developmental history.
mal treatment regimen for patients with such One also needs to know about general medical
comorbid conditions (Kanner & Barry, 2003). and psychiatric history, a history of head trauma,
Therefore, structured psychiatric inventories are and a detailed history of the patient’s seizures
helpful for both clinical and research purposes if (e.g., types of spells, frequency of each seizure
time permits for their use. type, occurrence of secondary generalization,
PNES occurs in a subset of epilepsy patients, episodes of status epilepticus). Knowledge of
although base rates of comorbid occurrence of AED regimen is also required to take their impact
this condition tend to be overestimated in most on cognitive into account. There will often be
studies. Research using the most rigorous diag- available neuroimaging data (e.g., MRI, SPECT,
nostic criteria suggests that approximately PET) and EEG findings available for review, as
5–10 % of patients with epilepsy will also experi- well as information regarding language laterality
ence PNES events (Martin, Burneo, Prasad, et al., (e.g., fMRI data, Wada results). Video-EEG
2003). There are also a handful of studies sug- results will frequently provide information
gesting that some patients undergoing surgery regarding seizure focus and can be informative
will develop these events and that these events regarding interictal discharges (discharges occur-
were not apparent prior to their surgery. We have ring between seizures). Results of the MRI of the
found that there may be risk factors for develop- brain will often identify structural abnormalities
ing PNES following surgery. if present. In particular, the presence of mesial
temporal sclerosis has proven useful in predicting
neuropsychological outcome.
Quality of Life Throughout the entire interview and assess-
ment, one should take note of signs of seizure
Measures have been developed to examine occurrence, including brief pauses in perfor-
patient satisfaction with various aspects of life mance or alterations in response style. When
functioning, and these measures have frequently subtle changes appear to be observed, it can be
been adopted as end points in outcome studies. useful to ask the patient if they have experienced
Quality-of-life measures attempt to evaluate the a loss of time or if they recall what they were
noxious impact of seizures and their treatment thinking about, and orientation can be rechecked
(e.g., side effects of AEDs) on self-ratings of as well.
cognitive functioning, mood, and satisfaction
with social support, vocational/academic perfor-
mance, and other aspects of daily functioning. Common Test Batteries
One criticism with quality-of-life measures has
been that they sometimes share too much overlap Given the difficulty involved in amassing ade-
with measures of mood and psychopathology. quate data to answer many research questions,
there has been a recent push by the National
Institutes of Health to create “common data ele-
Clinical Interview and Medical ments” for many neurological diseases including
Record Review epilepsy. A committee was tasked with creating a
core set of neurocognitive tests for use in epi-
There are key pieces of supporting information lepsy during 2009 and these suggestions were
that need to be gathered through clinical inter- published during 2011 (Loring et al., 2011). As
view and record review that set the context for the with other common data element projects, the
4 Neuropsychological Evaluation of the Epilepsy Surgical Candidate 111

resulting battery is intended to serve as a minimum Table 4.6 Factors predictive of outcome in temporal
lobe epilepsy patients undergoing surgery
collection of tests, taking no more than 1 h to
administer. This provides epilepsy researchers with Factors associated with a Factors associated with a
a common core of data available for future stud- favorable neurocognitive poor neurocognitive
outcome outcome
ies pooling information across centers. It also
Presence of mesial Normal brain imaging
provides epilepsy researchers who do not tradi- temporal sclerosis on MRI
tionally include cognitive elements with some Younger age of patient at Older age of patient at time
very basic guidelines for collecting such data time of surgery of surgery
when appropriate. Early age of seizure onset Adult onset of seizures
Since the 1980s, a loose collaboration of neu- (<15 years at onset)
ropsychology labs from several major epilepsy Surgery performed on Surgery performed on
nondominant cerebral language dominant cerebral
centers in the USA has existed as the Bozeman hemisphere hemisphere
consortium. This group has used this model of Wada memory Wada memory performance
sharing data to publish a number of papers that performance intact impaired for cerebral
the member sites could not have completed on for cerebral hemisphere hemisphere contralateral
contralateral to seizure to seizure focus
their own (Hermann, Perrine, Chelune, et al.,
focus
1999; Lee, Westerveld, Blackburn, Park, & Impaired neurocognitive Intact neurocognitive ability
Loring, 2005; Wilde et al., 2003). Their data- ability (more to lose)
sharing initiative should serve as a model for
future collaborations, which can be particularly
useful for studying events with low-frequency mesial TL dysfunction resulting from disease
base rates, such as the occurrence of FL and pos- or resection (Scoville & Milner, 1957).
terior cortical epilepsies. Such collaborations Research with the Wada procedure contributed
also lead to new projects as interactions contrib- additional support to this model, as it has
ute to cross-fertilization of ideas. become clear that patient’s performing poorly
on memory tasks during testing of the contra-
lateral (i.e., remaining) TL structures are at
Predicting Outcome in Epilepsy increased risk for significant postsurgical mem-
Surgery ory decline (Chelune, 1995). While the struc-
tural integrity of the contralateral hippocampal
Neuropsychological assessment can be useful in structures appear to be important for avoiding
the prediction of neurocognitive outcome, post- global amnesia, having a functionally adequate
surgical emotional and psychiatric status, and hippocampus has not prevented material-spe-
even seizure freedom. There are some general cific memory loss with unilateral TL resection
rules of thumb that have developed related to out- (such changes are common).
come prediction (see Table 4.6), and there are The functional adequacy model has support
also a limited number of available prediction for- from growing evidence that the adequacy of the
mulas. However, most of the available formulas ipsilateral hippocampus (i.e., structure on side to
relate to specific tests only and often have been be resected) better predicts material-specific
produced exclusively for TLE patients. postsurgical memory declines. This model pre-
dicts that TLE patients with intact memory func-
tion are likely to experience more pronounced
Functional Reserve and Functional decline in memory performance than those for
Adequacy Hypotheses in TLE Surgery whom memory is already impaired. This is a
common finding from years of memory research
The functional reserve hypothesis was based in TLE surgery, and the functional adequacy
primarily on studies documenting severe model also accounts for the observation that
amnestic disorders of patients with bilateral patients with high presurgical memory function-
112 D.L. Drane

ing are at greater risk for significant decline in ables upon neurocognitive performance (e.g.,
this area than those with low average or impaired subclinical epileptiform activity), and prediction
presurgical memory performance. of outcome.

Pertinent Information to Relay Background


to the Referring Physician or
Epilepsy Monitoring Unit Team Mr. Jones is a 42-year-old, right-handed, mar-
ried, Caucasian male who was referred for neuro-
In preparing a written report of the neuropsycho- psychological assessment while undergoing
logical assessment of an epilepsy patient, it is video-EEG monitoring as part of a presurgical
typically important to comment on whether the evaluation for possible epilepsy surgery. The
findings suggest lateralized or localized dysfunc- patient has a reported history of measles enceph-
tion and to relate these findings to the other avail- alitis at the age of 5 and later developed complex
able test results (e.g., MRI, video-EEG results). partial seizures at the age of 14. The patient’s sei-
One should also convey a thorough baseline of zures are characterized by unresponsiveness and
performance in terms of both neurocognitive lip smacking, but there is no evidence of motor
ability and emotional status and attempt to assess involvement, loss of bowel or bladder control, or
the risks and benefits of potential treatment tongue biting. The patient believes that he experi-
options (e.g., surgery, changes in AED regimen). ences clusters of spells every 1–3 months.
One should recognize when it is possible that Medical records suggest that he has experienced
brain reorganization appears likely (e.g., naming rare secondary generalization of seizures, all in
and verbal memory deficits are observed in a the context of medical noncompliance or sub-
patient with right TL seizure onset and normal therapeutic AED levels. Mr. Jones has no history
visual memory) or when it is possible that ongo- of birth injury, developmental delay, febrile sei-
ing seizures are likely disrupting distal brain zures, or head trauma. He denied any personal or
regions (e.g., executive dysfunction in TL patients familial history of psychiatric disturbance.
with no other findings of FL abnormality) and Current medications included sodium valproate
work these hypotheses into the final summary of (1,250 mg TID), gabapentin (600 mg TID), and
results. Recommendations should be provided mephobarbital (200 mg BID). The patient com-
for interventions that may be required prior to pleted high school and junior college and has
surgery, such as completion of a Wada or fMRI been employed as a technician for the telephone
study or a referral for psychotherapy in someone company for the past 15 years. He and his wife
with untreated psychiatric issues or a high risk of have been married for more than 20 years and
developing PNES events. Recommendations for have two children together.
follow-up testing and referral for cognitive reha-
bilitation should be made as appropriate as well.
Results of Other Relevant Medical
Procedures
Clinical Vignette
MRI of the brain revealed the presence of left
The following brief case study is intended to mesial temporal sclerosis and mild, diffuse vol-
highlight several of the principles from the cur- ume loss. Video-EEG results were unavailable at
rent chapter, including a description of the pro- the time of our evaluation. However, we would
cess of examining the neurocognitive results for later learn that the patient was experiencing fre-
lateralizing or localizing patterns for use in con- quent left anterior TL spikes throughout the mon-
firming seizure focus, integrating other medical itoring, as well as several subclinical events
findings, considering the impact of latent vari- during our clinical interview and testing.
4 Neuropsychological Evaluation of the Epilepsy Surgical Candidate 113

Neuropsychological Findings there is nothing to lose secondary to surgery.


Likewise, an underestimation of baseline perfor-
Mr. Jones’ neuropsychological test scores appear mance could also lead to erroneous conclusions
in Table 4.7, including his initial presurgical eval- in outcome studies or research using neurocogni-
uation completed during inpatient video-EEG tive scores as an end point (e.g., we might falsely
monitoring and a brief follow-up that was com- determine that there was no decline or even an
pleted when he came back to undergo the Wada improvement following surgery, when instead the
procedure. As will become clear, his initial patient may have actually gotten worse).
testing appeared to be impacted by subclinical As can be seen from examining his initial
epileptiform activity, which actually contributed assessment results in Table 4.7, Mr. Jones exhib-
to our ability to confirm the seizure focus, yet also ited a pattern of performance that suggested left
transiently disrupted his performance resulting in frontotemporal lobe dysfunction, including
an underestimation of his actual baseline abili- severely impaired performances on most tasks of
ties. We first suspected that his performance was auditory/verbal learning and memory and visual
affected by some latent variable when he pro- confrontational naming ability despite average to
duced a genuine impairment profile on a perfor- high-average visual memory performance. Mr.
mance validity test, as well as severely impaired Jones also exhibited mild deficits involving
memory scores on multiple measures of complex aspects of executive functioning, including prob-
auditory/verbal learning and memory. This pat- lems with generative fluency, response inhibition,
tern suggested that Mr. Jones was either severely and mental flexibility. Of note, however, as per-
amnestic/demented (which was not consistent formance on semantic fluency (impaired) was
with his presentation, which included a good much worse than letter fluency (low average),
recent work history) or that some unknown factor this pattern could again suggest primarily TL
was creating noise in his profile. When we met as dysfunction. It is also important to recognize that
a group to discuss the surgical cases for the week, despite the acute epileptiform activity, the patient
it was discovered that the patient had experienced still exhibited many scores that were average or
subclinical epileptiform discharges involving the better on tasks involving visual memory, general
left TL almost continuously throughout our eval- intellectual functioning, remaining executive
uation. Neither the patient nor his examiners had skills, and most aspects of language processing.
suspected that his performance was altered. Overall, this pattern was suggestive of dominant
However, by completing additional testing when (presumably left) TL dysfunction, with possible
the patient returned approximately 6 weeks later disruption of FL regions as well. The latter find-
to undergo the Wada procedure, the patient exhib- ing could be conceptualized as perhaps reflecting
ited average performances in these domains, pre- dysfunction of the broader temporofrontal lobe
sumably when not experiencing such activity. networks secondary to ongoing seizures, a find-
AED regimen was unchanged at follow-up ing that we have noted is often observed in TLE
assessment. This confirmed our suspicions that patients (see earlier coverage of the nociferous
the patient’s inpatient performance was likely cortex hypothesis).
disrupted secondary to epileptiform activity and An examination of Mr. Jones’ follow-up test-
also afforded us the opportunity to better docu- ing from approximately 6 weeks later when he
ment his baseline ability. As the test sessions returned for the Wada procedure demonstrated
were very close together, we decided to use alter- that the initial findings during video-EEG moni-
nate forms of measures or similar tests thought to toring were suboptimal for him. We felt confident
tap the same neurocognitive domains of interest. that these results reflected the transient impact of
Of note, an underestimation of a patient’s base- epileptiform activity that was not present on the
line performance can affect the neurosurgeon’s day of the Wada. While the same performance
approach to their case, as they could assume that pattern was present, the patient’s baseline ability
an impaired memory performance means that was clearly far better than our initial assessment
114 D.L. Drane

Table 4.7 Select results of neurocognitive testing during video-EEG monitoring and 1 month later at time of Wada
procedure
Tests administered Results during video-EEG stay Results on day of Wada
General IQ WAIS-III FSIQ = 99, VIQ = 104, WASI FSIQ = 108, VIQ = 110, PIQ = 102
PIQ = 91
Performance validity testing Failed 3/3 effort measures from the Passed all performance validity tests,
Word Memory Test (oral version) including the Medical Symptom Validity
but produced a genuine Test and the Word Memory Test
impairment profile (oral version)
Boston Naming Test Raw = 39/60, impaired Raw = 48/60, mildly impaired
Semantic fluency (animals) Raw = 9, <1st percentile Raw = 14, 4th percentile
Letter fluency (COWA) Raw = 18, 2nd percentile Raw = 24, 4th percentile
Screen of auditory Normal Normal
comprehension and repetition
Complex list learning Rey Auditory/Verbal Learning Test Verbal Selective Reminding Test (Form 1)
Trial 1 = 4/15, 6th percentile LTS = 108, 44th percentile
Trial 5 = 6/15, 1st percentile CLTR = 74, 23rd percentile
Trial 5 total = 27/75, 1st percentile Delayed = 11/12, 51st percentile
Immediate = 0/15, <1st percentile Recognition = 12/12 normal
Delayed = 2/15, 1st percentile
Recognition = 3/15, <1st percentile
WMS-III Logical Memory Raw = 41, 50th percentile N/A
Immediate recall
Delayed recall Raw = 17, 25th percentile
Verbal Paired Associates WMS-III WMS-I
Immediate Raw = 2, 1st percentile Raw = 16, 21st percentile
Delayed Raw = 0, 1st percentile Raw = 9, 12th percentile
Face recall WMS-III faces N/A
Immediate Raw = 34, 25th percentile
Delayed Raw = 40, 75th percentile
Recall of designs WMS-III Visual Reproduction N/A
Immediate Raw = 76, 16th percentile
Delayed Raw = 77, 75th percentile
Recognition Raw = 44, 50th percentile
Trail Making Test Part A = 29 s, 23rd percentile Part A = 26 s, 37th percentile
Part B = 129 s, 1st percentile Part B = 68 s, 30th percentile
Finger Tapping Test Raw = 49, 21st percentile Raw = 54, 50th percentile
Dominant hand
Nondominant hand Raw = 46, 30th percentile Raw = 48, 45th percentile
Grip strength Raw = 45, 18th percentile Raw = 45, 18th percentile
Dominant hand
Nondominant hand Raw = 43, 18th percentile Raw = 43, 18th percentile
Category test 13 errors, normal N/A
IQ intellectual quotient, WAIS-III Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (3rd edition), FSIQ full-scale IQ, VIQ verbal IQ,
PIQ performance IQ, WASI Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence, COWA Controlled Oral Word Association Test,
LTS long-term storage, CLTR continuous long-term retrieval, WMS-III Wechsler Memory Scale (3rd edition), WMS-I
Wechsler Memory Scale

had suggested. This latter performance was also ability during the second evaluation, yet these
more in keeping with the patient’s reported daily results were much better than the severely
functioning. As can be seen from examining impaired performances initially observed during
Table 4.7, the patient continued to exhibit mild monitoring. It is interesting to note the dissocia-
deficits in auditory/verbal learning and naming tion between the contextual memory test and the
4 Neuropsychological Evaluation of the Epilepsy Surgical Candidate 115

list-learning and associative-learning tasks, as the surgery and was counseled regarding the poten-
former did not appear to be affected by the sub- tial risks of decline that he might face. These
clinical epileptiform activity. This finding high- risks have to be balanced against the impact of
lights the dissociation that occurs between tests ongoing seizures, which obviously contribute to
of memory and is supportive of the position that transient disruptions of functioning and for some
the latter tasks are more dependent on mesial TL individuals likely contribute to more permanent
structures. The patient also exhibited mild gains changes in brain functioning over time. The
on some of the attentional measures and overall patient will ultimately be scheduled for a 1-year
IQ, although it is difficult to disentangle practice postsurgical follow-up with our service, at which
effects and differences between measures for time we will repeat most of the initial battery of
these smaller changes. tests.
Mr. Jones did not exhibit any significant emo-
tional distress during clinical interview or on for-
mal measures of mood, although he did report Direction of Future Practice
some mild anxiety and concern over the effect of and Research
ongoing seizures and what he viewed as transient
disruptions in his ability to function at work and Clinical neuropsychology in the epilepsy surgical
at home. He did not have any risk factors for setting already holds a solid position as a means
developing PNES events and did not exhibit of confirming the epileptic seizure focus, estab-
somatizational tendencies on formal personality lishing a baseline level of functioning for the
assessment. patient, and providing natural end points for out-
Mr. Jones’ Wada results demonstrated left come studies and other research. The ongoing
hemispheric language lateralization and demon- effort to improve our ability to localize and later-
strated that his right cerebral hemisphere was alize functions should continue in the future, with
independently capable of encoding novel infor- an emphasis on developing improved techniques
mation. Therefore, he was scheduled for surgical for assessing the extratemporal epilepsies. This
resection. Based on the constellation of findings will include further establishing the validity and
from neurocognitive performance and the other usefulness of current measures for testing FL
available data, we predicted that he would be at subregions and posterior cortical epilepsies, as
risk for further auditory/verbal memory decline well as developing new tests and paradigms. The
with surgery. Primarily, this is because he had assessment of FL function in particular will likely
grossly intact baseline naming and verbal mem- benefit from efforts to examine broader test pat-
ory functioning (i.e., something to lose), and the terns (e.g., teasing out the relative contribution of
resection was to be performed on his dominant non-frontal lobe functions to executive control
cerebral hemisphere. Seizure onset was during processes). Establishing collaborative efforts
his teen years, although there was really no indi- across centers (such as the NIH Common Data
cation of any brain reorganization based on avail- Elements Initiative) will also be critical for
able preoperative data. For example, he was obtaining adequate sample sizes to explore many
exhibiting baseline deficits in naming and verbal pertinent questions with these less common epi-
memory (i.e., the areas that we were examining lepsy syndromes. Another area of needed growth
for evidence of a possible reorganization), and in the neuropsychology of epilepsy involves
these deficits were enhanced by experiencing developing and implementing better methods of
epileptiform activity involving the left TL (i.e., assessing functional capacity and monitoring a
suggesting that they are still mediated by this wider array of outcome variables (e.g., vocational
region). The one factor arguing against a decline success, avoidance of disability, marital statis-
with surgery, and which we mentioned could tics). Although not emphasized in the current
possibly mediate the effects of surgery, was the chapter due to coverage elsewhere in this book,
presence of MTS. The patient decided to have the need for neuropsychologists to be actively
116 D.L. Drane

involved in broader assessment paradigms such cognitive functions. Journal of Neurology,


Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 30, 364–367.
as cortical stimulation mapping, Wada evalua-
Blume, W. T., Wiebe, S., & Tapsell, L. M. (2005).
tions, and development of fMRI paradigms for Occipital epilepsy: Lateral versus mesial. Brain, 128,
language lateralization and other purposes is 1209–1225.
greatly encouraged. The future also holds a place Blumer, D., Montouris, G., & Hermann, B. (1995).
Psychiatric morbidity in seizure patients on a neurodiag-
for combining neuropsychological data with
nostic monitoring unit. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry
many of the emerging technologies, such as and Clinical Neurosciences, 7, 445–456.
structural volumetric analysis, diffusion imaging, Boone, K. B., Miller, B. L., Rosenberg, L., Durazo, A.,
magnetoencephalography, dense array EEG, and McIntyre, H., & Weil, M. (1988). Neuropsychological
and behavioral abnormalities in an adolescent with
virtual reality paradigms for the purposes of bet-
frontal lobe seizures. Neurology, 38, 583–586.
ter understanding brain functioning and develop- Busch, R. M., Frazier, T. W., Haggerty, K. A., & Kubu,
ing new means for assessing presurgical function C. S. (2005). Utility of the Boston Naming Test in pre-
and preventing postsurgical decline. dicting ultimate side of surgery in patients with medi-
cally intractable temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsia, 46,
1773–1779.
Byrne, P., Becker, S., & Burgess, N. (2007). Remembering
References the past and imagining the future: A neural model of
spatial memory and imagery. Psychological Review,
Aarts, J. H., Binnie, C. D., Smit, A. M., & Wilkins, A. J. 114, 340–375.
(1984). Selective cognitive impairment during focal Carroll, B., Richardson, J. T., & Thompson, P. (1993).
and generalized epileptiform EEG activity. Brain, 107, Olfactory information processing and temporal lobe
239–308. epilepsy. Brain and Cognition, 22, 230–243.
Akanuma, N., Alarcon, G., Lum, F., Kissani, N., Chapin, J. S., Busch, R. M., Naugle, R. I., & Najm, I. M.
Koutroumanidis, M., Adachi, N. et al. (2003). (2009). The Family Pictures subtest of the WMS-III:
Lateralizing value of neuropsychological protocols for Relationship to verbal and visual memory following
presurgical assessment of temporal lobe epilepsy. temporal lobectomy for intractable epilepsy. Journal of
Epilepsia, 44, 408–418. Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 31(4),
Aldenkamp, A. P., & Arends, J. (2004). The relative influ- 498–504.
ence of epileptic EEG discharges, short nonconvulsive Chelune, G. J. (1995). Hippocampal adequacy versus
seizures, and type of epilepsy on cognitive function. functional reserve: Predicting memory functions fol-
Epilepsia, 45, 54–63. lowing temporal lobectomy. Archives of Clinical
Anhoury, S., Brown, R. J., Krishnamoorthy, E. S., & Neuropsychology, 10, 413–432.
Trimble, M. R. (2000). Psychiatric outcome after tem- Cheung, M. C., Chan, A. S., Chan, Y. L., Lam, J. M., &
poral lobectomy: A predictive study. Epilepsia, 41, Lam, W. (2006). Effects of illness duration on memory
1608–1615. processing of patients with temporal lobe epilepsy.
Barr, W. B., Chelune, G. J., Hermann, B. P., Loring, D. W., Epilepsia, 47, 1320–1328.
Perrine, K., Strauss, E., et al. (1997). The use of figural Chilosi, A. M., Brovedani, P., Moscatelli, M., Bonanni, P.,
reproduction tests as measures of nonverbal memory in & Guerrini, R. (2006). Neuropsychological findings in
epilepsy surgery candidates. Journal of the idiopathic occipital lobe epilepsies. Epilepsia, 47,
International Neuropsychological Society, 3, 435–443. 76–78.
Bartha, L., Benke, T., Bauer, G., & Trinka, E. (2005). Crane, J., & Milner, B. (2005). What went where?
Interictal language functions in temporal lobe epi- Impaired object location learning in patients with right
lepsy. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and hippocampal lesions. Hippocampus, 15, 216–231.
Psychiatry, 76, 808–814. Dalmagro, C. L., Bianchin, M. M., Velasco, T. R.,
Binder, D. K., Lehe, M. V., Kral, T., Bien, C. G., Urbach, Alexandre, V., Jr., Walz, R., Terra-Bustamante, V. C.,
H., Schramm, J., et al. (2008). Surgical treatment of et al. (2005). Clinical features of patients with poste-
occipital lobe epilepsy. Journal of Neurosurgery, 109, rior cortical epilepsies and predictors of surgical out-
57–69. come. Epilepsia, 46, 1442–1449.
Binnie, C. D. (2003). Cognitive impairment during epi- Damasio, H., Grabowski, T. J., Tranel, D., Hichwa, R. D.,
leptiform discharges: Is it ever justifiable to treat the & Damasio, A. (1996). A neural basis for lexical
EEG? The Lancet. Neurology, 2, 725–730. retrieval. Nature, 380, 499–505.
Bird, C. M., & Burgess, N. (2008). The hippocampus and Davies, K. G., Bell, B. D., Bush, A., Hermann, B. P.,
memory: Insights from spatial processing. Nature Dohan, F. C. J., & Jaap, A. S. (1998). Naming decline
Reviews. Neuroscience, 9, 182–194. after left anterior temporal lobectomy correlates with
Blakemore, C. B., & Falconer, M. A. (1967). Long-term pathological status of resected hippocampus.
effects of anterior temporal lobectomy on certain Epilepsia, 39, 407–419.
4 Neuropsychological Evaluation of the Epilepsy Surgical Candidate 117

Delaney, R. C., Rosen, A. J., Mattson, R. H., & Novelly, tive side effects of tiagabine and topiramate in patients
R. A. (1980). Memory function in focal epilepsy: A with epilepsy. Epilepsy & Behavior, 6, 373–381.
comparison of non-surgical, unilateral temporal lobe Fuller-Thomson, E., & Brennenstuhl, S. (2009). The asso-
and frontal lobe samples. Cortex, 16, 103–117. ciation between depression and epilepsy in a nation-
Devinsky, O., Morrell, M. J., & Vogt, B. A. (1995). ally representative sample. Epilepsia, 50, 1051–1058.
Contribution of anterior cingulate cortex to behaviour. Gilliam, F. G., Santos, J., Vahle, V., Carter, J., Brown, K.,
Brain, 118, 279–306. & Hecimovic, H. (2004). Depression in epilepsy:
Dikmen, S. S., Heaton, R. K., Grant, I., & Temkin, N. R. Ignoring clinical expression of neural network dys-
(1999). Test–retest reliability and practice effects of function? Epilepsia, 45(Suppl. 2), 28–33.
Expanded Halstead–Reitan Neuropsychological Test Glosser, G., Cole, L. C., French, J. A., Saykin, A. J., &
Battery. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Sperling, M. R. (1997). Predictors of intellectual per-
Society, 5, 346–356. formance in adults with intractable temporal lobe epi-
Drane, D. L. (2006). The intracarotid amobarbital proce- lepsy. Journal of the International Neuropsychological
dure (Wada test) should nearly always be performed in Society, 3, 252–259.
temporal lobectomy candidates. In J. W. Miller & Glosser, G., Salvucci, A. E., & Chiaravalloti, N. D. (2003).
D. L. Silbergeld (Eds.), Epilepsy surgery: Principles Naming and recognizing famous faces in temporal
and controversy. New York, NY: Taylor & Francis. lobe epilepsy. Neurology, 61, 81–86.
Drane, D. L., Lee, G. P., Cech, H., Huthwaite, J. S., Golby, A. J., Poldrack, R. A., Brewer, J. B., Spencer, D.,
Ojemann, G. A., Ojemann, J. G., et al. (2006). Desmond, J. E., Aron, A. P., et al. (2001). Material-
Structured cueing on a semantic fluency task differen- specific lateralization in the mesial temporal lobe and
tiates patients with temporal versus frontal lobe sei- prefrontal cortex during memory encoding. Brain,
zure onset. Epilepsy and Behavior, 9, 339–344. 124, 1841–1854.
Drane, D. L., & Meador, K. J. (Eds.). (2002). Cognitive Grammaldo, L. G., Giampa, T., Quarato, P. P., Picardi, A.,
toxicity of antiepileptic drugs. Boston, MA: Mascia, A., Sparano, A., et al. (2006). Lateralizing value
Butterworth-Heinemann. of memory tests in drug-resistant temporal lobe epilepsy.
Drane, D., Ojemann, G., Aylward, E., et al. Ojemann, J. G., European Journal of Neurology, 13, 371–376.
Johnson, L. C., Silbergeld, D. L., et al. (2008). Category- Griffith, H. R., Richardson, E., Pyzalski, R. W., Bell, B.,
specific naming and recognition deficits in temporal lobe Dow, C., Hermann, B. P., et al. (2006). Memory for
epilepsy. Neuropsychologia, 46, 1242–1255. famous faces and the temporal pole: Functional imag-
Drane, D. L., Ojemann, G. A., Ojemann, J. G., Aylward, ing findings in temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy and
E., Silbergeld, D. L., Miller, J. W., et al. (2009). Behavior, 9, 173–180.
Category-specific recognition and naming deficits fol- Hamberger, M. J., & Seidel, W. T. (2003). Auditory and
lowing resection of a right anterior temporal lobe visual naming tests: Normative and patient data for
tumor in a patient with atypical language lateraliza- accuracy, response time, and tip-of-the-tongue.
tion. Cortex, 450, 630–640. Journal of the International Neuropsychological
Drane, D. L., Ojemann, G. A., Tranel, D., Ojemann, J. G., Society, 9, 479–489.
& Miller, J. W. (2004). Category-specific naming and Hamberger, M. J., & Tamny, T. R. (1999). Auditory nam-
recognition deficits in patients with temporal lobe epi- ing and temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy Research, 35,
lepsy. Journal of the International Neuropsychological 229–243.
Society, 10, 202. Heaton, R. K., Temkin, N., Dikmen, S., Avitable, N.,
Dulay, M. F., Schefft, B. K., Testa, S. M., Fargo, J. D., Taylor, M. J., Marcotte, T. D., et al. (2001). Detecting
Privitera, M., & Yeh, H. S. (2002). What does the change: A comparison of three neuropsychological
Family Picture subtest of the Wechsler Memory Scale- methods, using normal and clinical samples. Archives
III Measure? Insight gained from patients evaluated of Clinical Neuropsychology, 16, 75–91.
for epilepsy surgery. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, Helmstaedter, C. (2001). Behavioral aspects of frontal
16, 452–462. lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy and Behavior, 2, 384–395.
Duncan, C. C., Mirsky, A. F., Lovelace, C. T., & Theodore, Helmstaedter, C., Elger, C. E., Hufnagel, A., Zentner,
W. H. (2009). Assessment of the attention impairment J., & Schramm, J. (1996). Different effects of ante-
in absence epilepsy: Comparison of visual and audi- rior temporal lobectomy, selective amygdalohippo-
tory P300. International Journal of Psychophysiology, campectomy, and temporal cortical lesionectomy,
73, 118–122. and temporal cortical lesionectomy on verbal learn-
Eskenazi, B., Cain, W. S., Novelly, R. A., & Mattson, R. ing, memory, and recognition. Journal of Epilepsy,
(1983). Odor perception in temporal lobe epilepsy 9, 35–45.
patients with and without temporal lobectomy. Helmstaedter, C., Gleissner, U., Zentner, J., & Elger, C. E.
Neuropsychologia, 24, 553–562. (1998). Neuropsychological consequences of epilepsy
Farrant, A., Morris, R. G., Russell, T., Elwes, R., surgery in frontal lobe epilepsy. Neuropsychologia,
Akanuma, N., Alarcon, G., et al. (2005). Social cogni- 36, 681–689.
tion in frontal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy and Behavior, 7, Helmstaedter, C., Grunwald, T., Lehnertz, K., Gleissner,
506–516. U., & Elger, C. E. (1997). Differential involvement of
Fritz, N., Glogau, S., Hoffmann, J., Rademacher, M., Elger, left temporolateral and temporomesial structures in
C. E., & Helmstaedter, C. (2005). Efficacy and cogni- verbal declarative learning and memory: Evidence
118 D.L. Drane

from temporal lobe epilepsy. Brain and Cognition, 35, Jokeit, H., Heger, R., Ebner, A., & Markowitsch, H. J.
110–131. (1998). Hemispheric asymmetries in category-specific
Helmstaedter, C., Kemper, B., & Elger, C. E. (1996). word retrieval. NeuroReport, 9, 2371–2373.
Neuropsychological aspects of frontal lobe epilepsy. Jokeit, H., & Schacher, M. (2004). Neuropsychological
Neuropsychologia, 34, 399–406. aspects of type of epilepsy and etiological factors in
Hermann, B., Davies, K., Foley, K., & Bell, B. (1999). adults. Epilepsy & Behavior, 5, 14–20.
Visual confrontation naming outcome after standard Jokeit, H., Seitz, R. J., Markowitsch, H. J., Neumann, N.,
left anterior temporal lobectomy with sparing versus Witte, O. W., & Ebner, A. (1997). Prefrontal asym-
resection of the superior temporal gyrus: A randomized metric interictal glucose hypometabolism and cogni-
prospective clinical trial. Epilepsia, 40, 1070–1076. tive impairment in patients with temporal lobe
Hermann, B. P., Perrine, K., Chelune, G. J., et al. (1999). epilepsy. Brain, 120, 2283–2294.
Visual confrontation naming following left anterior Jones-Gotman, M. (1986). Right hippocampal excision
temporal lobectomy: A comparison of surgical impairs learning and recall of a list of abstract designs.
approaches. Neuropsychology, 13, 3–9. Neuropsychologia, 24, 659–670.
Hermann, B., & Seidenberg, M. (1995). Executive system Jones-Gotman, M., & Milner, B. (1977). Design fluency:
dysfunction in temporal lobe epilepsy: Effects of The invention of nonsense drawings after focal corti-
nociferous cortex versus hippocampal pathology. cal lesions. Neuropsychologia, 15, 653–674.
Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, Jones-Gotman, M., Zatorre, R. J., Olivier, A., Andermann,
17, 809–819. F., Cendes, F., & Staunton, H. (1997). Learning and
Hermann, B., Seidenberg, M., Lee, E. J., Chan, F., & retention of words and designs following excision
Rutecki, P. (2007). Cognitive phenotypes in temporal from medial or lateral temporal-lobe structures.
lobe epilepsy. Journal of the International Neuropsychologia, 35, 963–973.
Neuropsychological Society, 13, 12–20. Kanner, A. M. (2009). Can antiepileptic drugs unmask a
Hermann, B. P., Seidenberg, M., Schoenfeld, J., Peterson, susceptibility to psychiatric disorders? Nature Clinical
J., Leveroni, C., & Wyler, A. R. (1996). Empirical Practice. Neurology, 5, 132–133.
techniques for determining the reliability, magnitude, Kanner, A. M., & Barry, J. J. (2003). The impact of mood
and pattern of neuropsychological change after epi- disorders in neurological diseases: Should neurolo-
lepsy surgery. Epilepsia, 37, 942–950. gists be concerned? Epilepsy & Behavior, 4, 3–13.
Hermann, B. P., & Wyler, A. R. (1988). Effects of anterior Kanner, A. M., Wuu, J., Faught, E., Tatum, W. O., 4th, Fix,
temporal lobectomy on language function: A con- A., French, J. A., et al. (2003). A past psychiatric his-
trolled study. Annals of Neurology, 23, 585–588. tory may be a risk factor for topiramate-related psychi-
Hermann, B. P., Wyler, A. R., Somes, G., Dohan, F. C., atric and cognitive adverse events. Epilepsy and
Berry, A. D., & Clement, L. (1994). Declarative mem- Behavior, 4, 548–552.
ory following anterior temporal lobectomy in humans. Kasteleijn-Nolst Trenite, D. G., & Vermeiren, R. (2005).
Behavioral Neuroscience, 108, 3–10. The impact of subclinical epileptiform discharges on
Hermann, B. P., Wyler, A. R., Steenman, H., & Richey, E. T. complex tasks and cognition: Relevance for aircrew and
(1988). The interrelationship between language function air traffic controllers. Epilepsy and Behavior, 6, 31–34.
and verbal learning/memory performance in patients Kemper, B., Helmstaedter, C., & Elger, C. E. (1993).
with complex partial seizures. Cortex, 24, 245–253. Neuropsychological assessment in patients with fron-
Hermann, B. P., Wyler, A. R., & Richey, E. T. (1988). tal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsia, 34, 170.
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance in patients Kennepohl, S., Sziklas, V., Garver, K. E., Wagner, D. D.,
with complex partial seizures of temporal-lobe origin. & Jones-Gotman, M. (2007). Memory and the medial
Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, temporal lobe: Hemispheric specialization reconsid-
10, 467–476. ered. NeuroImage, 36, 969–978.
Hermann, B. P., Wyler, A. R., & Somes, G. (1991). Kessels, R. P. C., Hendriks, M. P. H., Schouten, J., Van
Language function following anterior temporal lobec- Asselen, M., & Postma, A. (2004). Spatial memory
tomy: Frequency and correlates. Neurosurgery, 35, deficits in patients after unilateral selective amygdalo-
52–57. hippocampectomy. Journal of the International
Hernandez, M. T., Sauerwein, H. C., Jambaque, I., De Neuropsychological Society, 10, 907–912.
Guise, E., Lussier, F., Lortie, A., et al. (2002). Deficits Kim, C. H., Lee, S. A., Yoo, H. J., Kang, J. K., & Lee, J. K.
in executive function and motor coordination in chil- (2007). Executive performance on the Wisconsin Card
dren with frontal lobe epilepsy. Neuropsychologia, 40, Sorting Test in mesial temporal lobe epilepsy.
384–400. European Neurology, 57, 39–46.
Holmes, M. D., Miles, A. N., Dodrill, C. B., Ojemann, Kiper, D. C., Zesiger, P., Maeder, P., Deonna, T., &
G. A., & Wilensky, A. J. (2003). Identifying potential Innocenti, G. M. (2002). Vision after early-onset
surgical candidates in patients with evidence of bitem- lesions of the occipital cortex: I. Neuropsychological
poral epilepsy. Epilepsia, 44, 1075–1079. and psychophysiological studies. Neural Plasticity, 9,
Joanette, Y., & Goulet, P. (1986). Criterion-specific reduc- 1–25.
tion of verbal fluency in right brain-damaged right- Knecht, S., Jansen, A., Frank, A., van Randenborgh, J.,
handers. Neuropsychologia, 24, 875–879. Sommer, J., Kanowski, M., et al. (2003). How atypical
4 Neuropsychological Evaluation of the Epilepsy Surgical Candidate 119

is atypical language dominance? NeuroImage, 18, of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 18,
917–927. 416–430.
Kockelmann, E., Elger, C. E., & Helmstaedter, C. (2003). Martin, R., Burneo, J. G., Prasad, A., Powell, T., Faught,
Significant improvement in frontal lobe associated E., Knowlton, R., et al. (2003). Frequency of epilepsy
neuropsychological functions after withdrawal of topi- in patients with psychogenic nonepileptic seizures
ramate in epilepsy patients. Epilepsy Research, 54, monitored by video-EEG. Neurology, 61, 1791–1792.
171–178. Martin, A., & Fedio, P. (1983). Word production and com-
Kwan, P., Arzimanoglou, A., Berg, A. T., Brodie, M. J., prehension in Alzheimer’s disease: The breakdown of
Hauser, A. W., Mathern, G., et al. (2010). Definition semantic knowledge. Brain and Language, 19,
of drug resistant epilepsy: Consensus proposal by the 124–141.
ad hoc Task Force of the ILAE Commission on Martin, R. C., Loring, D. W., Meador, K. J., & Lee, G. P.
Therapeutic Strategies. Epilepsia, 51(6), 1069–1077. (1990). The effects of lateralized temporal lobe dys-
Kwan, P., & Brodie, M. J. (2000). Early identification of function on formal and semantic word fluency.
refractory epilepsy. New England Journal of Medicine, Neuropsychologia, 28, 823–829.
342, 314–319. Martin, R. C., Sawrie, S. M., Edwards, R., Roth, D. L.,
Langfit, J. T., & Rausch, R. (1996). Word-finding deficits Faught, E., Kuzniecky, R. I., et al. (2000). Investigation
persist after left anterotemporal lobectomy. Archives of executive function change following anterior tem-
of Neurology, 53, 72–76. poral lobectomy: Selective normalization of verbal
Lee, G. P., Westerveld, M., Blackburn, L. B., Park, Y. D., fluency. Neuropsychology, 14, 501–508.
& Loring, D. W. (2005). Prediction of verbal memory Martin, R., Sawrie, S., Gilliam, F., Mackey, M., Faught,
decline after epilepsy surgery in children: Effectiveness E., Knowlton, R., et al. (2002). Determining reliable
of Wada memory asymmetries. Epilepsia, 46, 97–103. cognitive change after epilepsy surgery: Development
Lineweaver, T. T., Morris, H. H., Naugle, R. I., Najm, I. M., of reliable change indices and standardized regres-
Diehl, B., & Bingaman, W. (2006). Evaluating the con- sion-based change norms for the WMS-III and WAIS-
tributions of state-of-the-art assessment techniques to III. Epilepsia, 43, 1551–1558.
predicting memory outcome after unilateral anterior Mayeux, R., Brandt, J., Rosen, J., & Benson, D. F. (1980).
temporal lobectomy. Epilepsia, 47, 1895–1903. Interictal memory and language impairment in tempo-
Loring, D. W., Lee, G. P., Martin, R. C., & Meador, K. J. ral lobe epilepsy. Neurology, 30, 120–125.
(1988). Material-specific learning in patients with par- McAndrews, M. P., & Milner, B. (1991). The frontal cor-
tial complex seizures of temporal lobe origin: tex and memory for temporal order. Neuropsychologia,
Convergent validation of memory constructs. Journal 29, 849–859.
of Epilepsy, 1, 53–59. McDonald, C. R., Bauer, R. M., Grande, L., Gilmore, R.,
Loring, D. W., Lowenstein, D. H., Barbaro, N. M., & Roper, S. (2001). The role of the frontal lobes in
Fureman, B. E., Odenkirchen, J., Jacob, M. P. et al. memory: Evidence from unilateral frontal resections
(2011). Common data elements in epilepsy research: for relief of intractable epilepsy. Archives of Clinical
Development and implementation of the NINDS epi- Neuropsychology, 16, 571–585.
lepsy CDE project. Epilepsia, 52, 1186–1191. McDonald, C. R., Delis, D. C., Norman, M. A., Tecoma,
Loring, D. W., Meador, K. J., & Lee, G. P. (1994). Effects E. S., & Iragui, V. J. (2005). Discriminating patients
of temporal lobectomy on generative fluency and other with frontal-lobe epilepsy and temporal-lobe epilepsy:
language functions. Archives of Clinical Utility of a multilevel design fluency test.
Neuropsychology, 9, 229–238. Neuropsychology, 19, 806–813.
Loring, D. W., Strauss, E., Hermann, B. P., Barr, W. B., McDonald, C. R., Delis, D. C., Norman, M. A., Wetter,
Perrine, K., Trenerry, M. R., et al. (2008). Differential S. R., Tecoma, E. S., & Iragui, V. J. (2005). Response
neuropsychological test sensitivity to left temporal inhibition and set shifting in patients with frontal lobe
lobe epilepsy. Journal of the International epilepsy or temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy and
Neuropsychological Society, 14, 394–400. Behavior, 7, 438–446.
Luerding, R., Boesebeck, F., & Ebner, A. (2004). Milner, B. (1958). Psychological deficits produced by
Cognitive changes after epilepsy surgery in the poste- temporal lobe excision. In H. C. Solomon, S. Cobb, &
rior cortex. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and W. Penfield (Eds.), The brain and human behavior:
Psychiatry, 75, 583–587. Proceedings of the Association for Research in
Maccotta, L., Buckner, R. L., Gilliam, F. G., & Ojemann, Nervous and Mental Disease. Baltimore, MD: The
J. G. (2007). Changing frontal contributions to mem- Williams and Wilkins Company.
ory before and after medial temporal lobectomy. Milner, B. (1968a). Visual recognition and recall after
Cerebral Cortex, 17, 443–456. right temporal-lobe excision in man. Neuropsychologia,
Manchanda, R. (2002). Psychiatric disorders in epilepsy: 6, 191–209.
Clinical aspects. Epilepsy and Behavior, 3, 39–45. Milner, B. (1968b). Effects of different brain lesions on
Majdan, A., Sziklas, V., & Jones-Gotman, M. (1996). card sorting. Archives of Neurology, 9, 90–100.
Performance of healthy subjects and patients with Milner, B., Corsi, P., & Leonard, G. (1991). Frontal-lobe
resection from the anterior temporal lobe on matched contribution to recency judgments. Neuropsychologia,
tests of verbal and visuoperceptual learning. Journal 29, 601–618.
120 D.L. Drane

Milner, B., Petrides, M., & Smith, M. L. (1985). Frontal ful neuropsychological change following epilepsy sur-
lobes and the temporal organization of memory. gery. Journal of the International Neuropsychological
Human Neurobiology, 4, 137–142. Society, 2, 556–564.
Ojemann, L. M., Ojemann, G. A., Dodrill, C. B., Crawford, Saykin, A. J., Stafiniak, P., Robinson, L. J., Flannery, K.
C. A., Holmes, M. D., & Dudley, D. L. (2001). A., Gur, R. C., O’Connor, M. J., et al. (1995).
Language disturbances as side effects of topiramate Language before and after temporal lobectomy:
and zonisamide therapy. Epilepsy and Behavior, 2, Specificity of acute changes and relation to early risk
579–584. factors. Epilepsia, 33, 1071–1077.
Patrikelis, P., Angelakis, E., & Gatzonis, S. (2009). Scoville, W. B., & Milner, B. (1957). Loss of recent mem-
Neurocognitive and behavioral functioning in frontal ory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. The Journal of
lobe epilepsy: A review. Epilepsy and Behavior, 14, Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 12,
19–26. 103–113.
Penfield, W., & Jasper, H. H. (1954). Epilepsy and the Seidenberg, M., Griffith, R., Sabsevitz, D., Moran, M.,
functional anatomy of the human brain. Boston, MA: Haltiner, A., Bell, B., et al. (2002). Recognition and iden-
Little, Brown. tification of famous faces in patients with unilateral tem-
Pigott, S., & Milner, B. (1993). Memory for different poral lobe epilepsy. Neuropsychologia, 40, 446–456.
aspects of complex visual scenes after unilateral tem- Siegel, A. M., & Williamson, P. D. (2000). Parietal lobe
poral- or frontal-lobe resection. Neuropsychologia, epilepsy. Advances in Neurology, 84, 189–199.
31, 1–15. Spiers, H. J., Burgess, N., Maguire, E. A., Baxendale, S.
Pillon, B., Bazin, B., Deweer, B., Ehrlé, N., Baulac, M., & A., Hartley, T., Thompson, P. J., et al. (2001).
Dubois, B. (1999). Specificity of memory deficits after Unilateral temporal lobectomy patients show lateral-
right or left temporal lobectomy. Cortex, 35, ized topographical and episodic memory deficits in a
561–571. virtual town. Brain, 124, 2476–2489.
Powell, H. W., Koepp, M. J., Symms, M. R., Boulby, P. A., Squire, L. R. (1993). The organization of declarative
Salek-Haddadi, A., Thompson, P. J., et al. (2005). and nondeclarative memory. In T. Ono, L. R. Squire,
Material-specific lateralization of memory encoding M. E. Raichle, M. Fukuda, & D. I. Perret (Eds.),
in the medial temporal lobe: Blocked versus event- Brain mechanisms of perception and memory: From
related design. NeuroImage, 27, 231–239. neuron to behaviour. New York, N:Y: Oxford
Rapport, L. J., Millis, S. R., & Bonello, P. J. (1998). University Press.
Validation of the Warrington theory of visual process- Stuss, D. T., Alexander, M. P., Hamer, L., Palumbo, C.,
ing and the Visual Object and Space Perception Dempster, R., Binns, M., et al. (1998). The effects of
Battery. Journal of Clinical and Experimental focal anterior and posterior brain lesions on verbal flu-
Neuropsychology, 20, 211–220. ency. Journal of the International Neuropsychological
Rausch, R., & Babb, T. L. (1993). Hippocampal neuron Society, 4, 265–278.
loss and memory scores before and after temporal lobe Suchy, Y., Sands, K., & Chelune, G. J. (2003). Verbal and
surgery for epilepsy. Archives of Neurology, 50, nonverbal fluency performance before and after sei-
812–817. zure surgery. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Rzezak, P., Fuentes, D., Guimaraes, C. A., Thome-Souza, Neuropsychology, 25, 190–200.
S., Kuczynski, E., Li, L. M., et al. (2007). Frontal lobe Sylvester, C.-Y. C., & Shimamura, A. P. (2002). Evidence
dysfunction in children with temporal lobe epilepsy. for intact semantic representations in patients with
Pediatric Neurology, 37, 176–185. frontal lobe lesions. Neuropsychology, 16, 197–207.
Salanova, V., Andermann, F., Rasmussen, T., Olivier, A., Takaya, S., Hanakawa, T., Hashikawa, K., Ikeda, A.,
& Quesney, L. F. (1995). Parietal lobe epilepsy: Sawamoto, N., Nagamine, T., et al. (2006). Prefrontal
Clinical manifestations and outcome in 82 patients hypofunction in patients with mesial temporal lobe
treated surgical between 1929 and 1988. Brain, 118, epilepsy. Neurology, 67, 1674–1676.
607–627. Temkin, N. R., Heaton, R. K., Grant, I., & Dikmen, S. S.
Saling, M. M. (2009). Verbal memory in temporal lobe (1999). Detecting significant change in neuropsycho-
epilepsy: Beyond material specificity. Brain, 132, logical test performance: A comparison of four mod-
570–582. els. Journal of the International Neuropsychological
Saling, M. M., Berkovic, S. F., O’Shea, M. F., Kalnins, R., Society, 5, 357–369.
Darby, D., & Bladin, P. (1993). Lateralization of ver- Thurtell, M. J., Mohamed, A., Luders, H. O., & Leigh, R. J.
bal memory and unilateral hippocampal sclerosis: (2009). Evidence for three-dimensional cortical control
Evidence of task-specific effects. Journal of Clinical of gaze from epileptic patients. Journal of Neurology,
and Experimental Neuropsychology, 15, 608–618. Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 80, 683–685.
Salinsky, M. C., Storzbach, D., Spencer, D. C., Oken, Treyer, V., Buck, A., & Schnider, A. (2005). Processing
B. S., Landry, T., & Dodrill, C. B. (2005). Effects of content or location: Distinct brain activation in a mem-
topiramate and gabapentin on cognitive abilities in ory task. Hippocampus, 15, 684–689.
healthy volunteers. Neurology, 64, 792–798. Troster, A. I., Warmflash, V., Osorio, I., Paolo, A. M.,
Sawrie, S. M., Chelune, G. J., Naugles, R. I., & Luders, Alexander, L. J., & Barr, W. B. (1995). The roles of
H. O. (1996). Empirical methods for assessing meaning- semantic networks and search efficiency in verbal
4 Neuropsychological Evaluation of the Epilepsy Surgical Candidate 121

fluency performance in intractable temporal lobe epi- Weintrob, D. L., Saling, M. M., Berkovic, S. F., &
lepsy. Epilepsy Research, 21, 19–26. Reutens, D. C. (2007). Impaired verbal associative
Ungerleider, L. G., & Mishkin, M. (1982). Two cortical learning after resection of left perirhinal cortex. Brain,
visual systems. In D. J. Ingle, M. A. Goodale, & R. J. 130, 1423–1431.
Mansfield (Eds.), Analysis of visual behavior Weniger, G., Boucsein, K., & Irle, E. (2004). Impaired
(pp. 549–586). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. associative memory in temporal lobe epilepsy subjects
Upton, D., & Thompson, P. J. (1996). General neuropsy- after lesions of hippocampus, parahippocampal gyrus,
chological characteristics of frontal lobe epilepsy. and amygdala. Hippocampus, 14, 785–796.
Epilepsy Research, 23, 169–177. Wiebe, S., Blume, W. T., Girvin, J. P., & Eliasziw, M.
Upton, D., & Thompson, P. J. (1999). Twenty questions (2001). Effectiveness and efficiency of surgery for
task and frontal lobe dysfunction. Archives of Clinical temporal lobe epilepsy group. A randomized,
Neuropsychology, 14, 203–216. controlled trial of surgery for temporal-lobe epi-
Vannest, J., Szaflarski, J. P., Privitera, M. D., Schefft, lepsy. New England Journal of Medicine, 345,
B. K., & Holland, S. K. (2008). Medial temporal fMRI 311–318.
activation reflects memory lateralization and memory Wilde, N. J., Strauss, E., Chelune, G. J., Hermann, B. P.,
performance in patients with epilepsy. Epilepsy and Hunter, M., Loring, D. W., et al. (2003). Confirmatory
Behavior, 12, 410–418. factor analysis of the WMS-III in patients with tempo-
Warrington, E. K., & James, M. (1991). Visual Object and ral lobe epilepsy. Psychological Assessment, 15,
Space Perception Battery. Edmunds, UK: Thames 56–63.
Valley Company. Williamson, P. D. (1987). Intensive monitoring of com-
Weber, B., Fliessbach, K., Lange, N., Kugler, F., & Elger, C. E. plex partial seizures: Diagnosis and subclassification.
(2007). Material-specific memory processing is related to Advances in Neurology, 46, 69–84.
language dominance. NeuroImage, 37, 611–617. Williamson, P., Spencer, D. D., Spencer, S. S., Novelly,
Wechsler, D. (1997). Wechsler memory scale—3rd edition R. A., & Mattson, R. H. (1985). Complex partial sei-
(3rd ed.). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological zures of frontal lobe origin. Annals of Neurology, 18,
Corporation. 497–504.
The Wada Test: Current
Perspectives and Applications 5
David W. Loring and Kimford J. Meador

The Wada test has been an integral part of the pre- considered in the development and application of
operative evaluation for epilepsy surgery since the other techniques to assess focal cerebral function/
1950s. Originally designed to establish cerebral dysfunction.
language laterality, it was subsequently modified
by including a memory component to estimate
postoperative risk to recent memory function fol- Historical Background
lowing unilateral temporal lobe resection. This
chapter will briefly review the use of Wada test- The Wada test was conceived by Juhn Wada in
ing, discuss the various approaches used to tran- post World War II Japan as a technique to limit
siently anesthetize brain regions prior to cognitive bilateral seizure effects of ECT in acute schizo-
assessment, and conclude with the specific meth- phrenic psychosis and manic-depressive illness
ods developed at the Medical College of Georgia (Wada, 1997). Wada reasoned that by “anestheti-
(MCG). The Wada test is not only employed to zation through a carotid route to prevent seizure
predict postoperative risks of language and mem- bilateralization,” the cognitive side effects associ-
ory deficits but is also a marker of temporal lobe ated with bilateral ECT would be minimized.
dysfunction and as such should be sensitive to Before Wada was able to implement this approach
seizure-onset laterality in patients with temporal with patients undergoing bilateral ECT, he was
lobe epilepsy. Wada test results should always be faced with a patient with repeated episodes of
interpreted within the context of other clinical and partial status epilepticus and occasional general-
diagnostic test findings. The lessons learned from ization following a penetrating missile injury.
pitfalls in Wada testing should be carefully Chloral hydrate, the standard treatment of status
epilepticus at that time, failed to break the partial
status. Consequently, Wada chose to administer
D.W. Loring, Ph.D., A.B.P.P.-C.N. (*) the amobarbital, which he had procured for ECT
Department of Neurology, Emory University, use, in an attempt to arrest the status, and unilat-
101 Woodruff Circle, Suite 6000, Atlanta,
GA 30322, USA eral carotid amobarbital injection to this patient
e-mail: dloring@emory.edu was successful in stopping the status.
K.J. Meador, M.D. The Wada procedure as a technique to estab-
Department of Neurology & Neurological Sciences, lish cerebral language representation in epilepsy
Stanford Comprehensive Epilepsy Center, surgery candidates was introduced to the
Stanford University School of Medicine, Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) in 1956
300 Pasteur Drive (Room A343), Stanford,
CA 94305-5235, USA when Dr. Wada was at the MNI on fellowship.
e-mail: kmeador@stanford.edu Since epilepsy was known to potentially alter

W.B. Barr and C. Morrison (eds.), Handbook on the Neuropsychology of Epilepsy, 123
Clinical Handbooks in Neuropsychology, DOI 10.1007/978-0-387-92826-5_5,
© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015
124 D.W. Loring and K.J. Meador

cerebral language dominance, the approach of scoring criteria, as well as the anesthetic agent
introducing transient hemispheric anesthesia used. Because of the variability in testing proto-
became routine when any possibility of atypical cols, it has been difficult to establish a representa-
language existed, and Wada testing became the tive evidence base for Wada testing overall, with
gold standard for determining language laterality. some centers reporting robust findings, while other
The memory component of the Wada was centers have failed to find clinically relevant
introduced several years later after several cases results. Further, a major methodological limitation
of severe memory decline were observed follow- has been the confound that Wada results are often
ing unilateral temporal lobectomy (Penfield & used to establish surgical candidacy. Consequently,
Milner, 1958). The memory declines in these the predictor and outcome variables are not com-
patients were thought to be the results of signifi- pletely independent. Finally, some centers perform
cant bilateral disease that went unrecognized dur- Wada testing only on selected patients, precluding
ing the presurgical evaluation. In these patients a consecutive series of patients from being
with significant temporal lobe disease contralat- reported, thus introducing methodological bias
eral to surgery, there was insufficient residual into the results.
temporal lobe function following surgery to sus-
tain new memory formation. At this time in the
late 1950s, the state of the art in diagnostic imag- Language
ing was the pneumoencephalogram. In addition,
the standard clinical EEG consisted of only eight In most patients, the identification of cerebral
channels, and video/EEG monitoring was years language dominance is straightforward, since
away. Thus, the number of tools to identify sig- most individuals have left cerebral language
nificant temporal lobe impairment contralateral dominance. In these cases, there is disruption of
to the proposed surgery was limited. By intro- multiple aspects of language following left hemi-
ducing items to be remembered during the period sphere injection, such as speech arrest, loss of
of hemispheric anesthesia, Milner reasoned that comprehension, naming deficits, and during
the effects of surgical resection on memory recovery, paraphasic errors. In contrast, there is
could be modeled and risk for significant post- no change in language abilities following right
surgical decline estimated (Milner, Branch, & hemisphere injection. However, as is the case
Rasmussen, 1962). with memory testing, the reliability and validity
The initial and still the most common anes- of Wada language testing, particularly in the case
thetic agent used in Wada testing is amobarbital, of atypical language representation, depends in
although shortages and limitations of availability part on which language tasks are used and the cri-
worldwide have led to other medications being teria that are employed for inferring language
employed. Although initially introduced using a representation based upon patient response.
direct carotid stick, the medication is routinely As the number of epilepsy surgery centers
delivered via a femoral catheter through the aor- began to expand in the mid-1980s, concern
tic arch and into the carotid artery. Most centers emerged over the large discrepancy in reported
administer drug by hand rather than machine prevalence of bilateral language representation.
injection, and the anterior two-thirds of each Specifically, some epilepsy centers observed
hemisphere is transiently anesthetized while the bilateral language as often as 60 % of patients
patient is presented with multiple language and being studied, whereas other centers reported
other cognitive tasks. bilateral language rarely or in some cases never
The Wada test is more accurately characterized (Snyder, Novelly, & Harris, 1990).
as a procedure rather than a test, since no standard- The observed discrepancy in rates of bilateral
ized protocol exists across epilepsy centers. There language exceeded what could easily be attributed
are multiple procedural variations including to patient population differences given the relative
medication dose, language and memory stimuli, homogeneity of most epilepsy surgical samples
5 The Wada Test: Current Perspectives and Applications 125

with similar syndrome classification. Instead, this performed during right-sided surgery have
disparity reflected center differences in the clas- exhibited the expected relationships (Jabbour,
sification criteria used to infer bilateral language Hempel, Gates, Zhang, & Risse, 2005; Loring
since uniform standards do not exist to guide et al., 1990a).
interpretation. High rates of bilateral language The second approach to validate the use of
were reported in centers using speech cessation to Wada language results is to examine the degree to
infer language representation. Subsequent investi- which Wada language results predict postopera-
gations of Wada language criteria compared to tive change in naming ability. Although the
fMRI activation patterns, however, revealed that robust relationship between naming decline and
speech cessation was unrelated to language acti- left/language resection vs. right/nonlanguage
vation using fMRI (Benbadis et al., 1998). Speech resection has been adequately described (Glosser
arrest following nondominant hemisphere injec- & Donofrio, 2001; Ruff et al., 2007), surpris-
tion may result from acute drug effects associated ingly, there appears to be only a single report in
with a bolus of amobarbital and, thus, lead to the literate directly addressing the issue of Wada
inflated estimates of bilaterality. laterality scores and language outcomes. In a
We consider the presence of positive parapha- series of 24 patients undergoing left anterior tem-
sic errors to be a much stronger sign of linguistic poral lobectomy, the Wada language asymmetry
impairment related to language representation score reflecting the interhemispheric difference
than negative responses, such as speech arrest or language performance following left and right
failure to respond to task requests. However, the hemisphere injections was significantly related to
absence of response to multiple linguistic tasks the magnitude of postoperative decline on the
(e.g., comprehension, naming) helps provide Boston naming test (Sabsevitz et al., 2003).
greater confidence of linguistic impairment com- However, in these patients, an even stronger pre-
pared to an isolated failure on a single task such dictor of postoperative naming decline was lan-
as expressive speech cessation. Paraphasic errors guage asymmetry scores using fMRI.
are less likely to be observed following short- The final approach to validating the Wada lan-
acting agents such as methohexital given the guage component has used noninvasive
rapid return to baseline levels of function. approaches such as fMRI and MEG. Although
the majority of these studies have relied on Wada
results as the gold standard of language and as
Wada Language Validity the independent variable, the strong relationship
between Wada language results and these nonin-
There are three primary approaches to validate vasive measures provides support that language
Wada language testing. The first is not truly sta- testing during hemispheric anesthesia reliably
tistical in nature and involves the degree to which establishes cerebral language representation
language representation inferred by Wada test- (Arora et al., 2009; Binder et al., 1996; Doss,
ing is confirmed in patients undergoing electrical Zhang, Risse, & Dickens, 2009; Gaillard et al.,
stimulation language mapping. There are multi- 2002; Papanicolaou et al., 2004; Suarez et al.,
ple reports indicating a strong correlation in 2009; Woermann et al., 2003). Despite overall
patients with left hemisphere language findings agreement across studies in the 90 % range, the
(Ojemann, 1980; Wyllie et al., 1990), but this reasons for the discrepancies between approaches
relationship is more convincing for patients with continue to be explored and debated. In general,
atypical language representation in the right the greatest disagreements occur in patients con-
hemisphere given the very high base rates of left sidered to have bilateral language representation
hemisphere language. Although infrequent, by one approach and unilateral language by
patients with right hemisphere or bilateral lan- another, with complete disagreements of results
guage in whom stimulation mapping can be being rare (Gaillard et al., 2004).
126 D.W. Loring and K.J. Meador

Wada memory correlations with seizure-onset


Memory laterality. Although the goal of Wada memory test-
ing is to forecast postoperative memory decline,
Wada memory testing is used primarily to evalu- demonstrating the sensitivity of Wada memory
ate risk for postoperative memory decline, results to lateralized temporal lobe impairments
although Wada memory interhemispheric differ- provides a surrogate to the goal of postoperative
ence score may also be used to verify seizure- seizure outcome prediction. Thus, in the absence
onset laterality. When the hemisphere ipsilateral of a strong relationship between Wada memory
to a medial temporal lobe seizure focus is anes- asymmetry findings and seizure-onset laterality, it
thetized, the functional reserve capacity of the should not be surprising to conclude that the Wada
contralateral temporal lobe to sustain memory test is a poor predictor of memory outcome. The
function in isolation is assessed (Chelune, 1995). potential confound of aphasia on verbal memory
This score provides an estimate of the ability of stimuli is well recognized (Kirsch et al., 2005), but
the contralateral temporal lobe to sustain recent many Wada protocols include words as memory
memory function when the temporal lobe with items. Thus, generalizations of specific Wada
the seizure focus is resected. Functional reserve memory findings to other Wada memory protocols
assessment was the original goal of Wada mem- must necessarily be made cautiously (Meador &
ory testing in order to avoid significant postoper- Loring, 2005).
ative amnesia following unilateral temporal lobe Even within our own center, we have reported
resection (Milner et al., 1962). different “utility” of the Wada test based upon
In the early 1990s, improved MRI imaging different methodologies employed at the time.
techniques (Jack et al., 1992) created greater For example, we observed that successful post-
interest examining the degree of sclerosis in the operative memory outcomes could be observed
temporal lobe to be resected. Since there are following Wada memory failure (Loring, Lee,
varying degrees of residual function in the dis- et al., 1990); our Wada protocol at that time
eased/ipsilateral temporal lobe, the functional employed relatively large total doses of amobar-
adequacy of the temporal lobe associated with bital with some patient demonstrating greater
seizure onset is assessed by the injection contra- sedation which was induced by multiple incre-
lateral to seizure onset. Thus, ipsilateral and con- mental amobarbital injections. However, after we
tralateral injections contribute differently to demonstrated an adverse dose effect on Wada
memory function in the preoperative patient with memory results, we established a protocol with
temporal lobe epilepsy and are used to predict real objects and lower dosing, and we observed a
different aspects of postoperative memory case of postoperative amnesia in which Wada
outcomes. memory results appeared to predict that outcome
Because Wada protocols are not standardized, (Loring, Hermann, et al., 1994).
it is difficult to estimate to what degree method Establishing the validity of Wada memory
variance contributes to some of the reported vari- testing presents multiple challenges. Prior to the
ability in memory outcome prediction. For exam- mid-1980s, when there were few techniques
ple, protocol differences in memory stimuli will other than the Wada test to identify patients at
lead to different results about the utility of Wada risk for postoperative memory decline, a major
testing as part of a comprehensive epilepsy sur- problem with establishing validity was the con-
gery evaluation (Testa, Ward, Crone, & Brandt, founding of the independent and dependent vari-
2008). We have shown that factors such as stimu- ables associated with using Wada memory results
lus type (pictures vs. real objects) (Loring et al., to establish surgical candidacy. Thus, patients
1997), timing of stimulus presentation (Loring, thought to be at risk for significant postoperative
Meador, et al., 1994), mixed stimuli (Lee, Park, memory decline were excluded from surgery, and
Westerveld, Hempel, & Loring, 2002), and amo- the ability of Wada memory testing to predict
barbital dose (Loring, Meador, & Lee, 1992) affect postoperative amnesia could not be established.
5 The Wada Test: Current Perspectives and Applications 127

However, as other methods to predict postopera- side and the disruptive effects of the pharmaco-
tive memory change such as MRI or PET started logic inactivation on the other side.
to emerge as additional approaches to forecast Interhemispheric Wada memory asymmetry
cognitive outcomes, patients with questionable (WMA) scores have often been considered lateral-
Wada memory results could more often proceed izing based solely on the direction of WMA (Cohen-
to surgery than when Wada was only a technique Gadol, Westerveld, Alvarez-Carilles, & Spencer,
to predict cognitive change. Thus, the existence 2004; Lee, Westerveld, Blackburn, Park, & Loring,
of some “false-positive” error rate could be 2005; Sabsevitz, Swanson, Morris, Mueller, &
established. Seidenberg, 2001), although other reports have
Wada validity has relied on a number of indi- required a larger discrepancy to be considered later-
rect approaches, the most common being seizure- alizing (Perrine et al., 1995; Sperling et al., 1994) or
onset laterality as a surrogate measure of have determined WMAs based on the pattern of
hippocampal function. On the group level, Wada memory failure between both hemispheres
patients with unilateral temporal lobe epilepsy (Alpherts, Vermeulen, & van Veelen, 2000). Using a
should perform relatively high levels when the single cut point for Wada asymmetry classification
hemisphere ipsilateral to seizure onset is injected discards potentially relevant information from inter-
since the relatively diseased and nonfunctioning val measurement scaling by using cut points to
temporal lobe is being anesthetized and thus create a dichotomous outcome. Multiple-level like-
should create little additional memory impair- lihood ratios (LRs) avoid artificial classification
ment. In contrast, when the hemisphere contralat- dichotomies by examining classification rates
eral to seizure onset is injected, there should be across a range of scores (Grimes & Schulz, 2005;
poor memory due to bilateral temporal lobe dys- Hosmer & Lemeshaw, 2000; Strauss, Richardson,
function due to effects of the seizure focus on one Glasziou, & Haynes, 2005).

Seizue Focus
20
left
right

15
Frequency

10

0
8.5 7.5 6.0 5.0 4.0 3.0 2.0 1.0 .0 -1.0 -2.0 -3.0 -4.0 -5.0 -6.0 -7.0 -8.0

Fig. 5.1 Histogram of WMA in MCG patient series of TL patients by seizure-onset laterality. (From Loring et al., 2009)
128 D.W. Loring and K.J. Meador

Table 5.1 Multiple-level likelihood ratios (LRs) for other extreme, no left TL patients obtained a
different magnitudes of WMAs using the MCG Wada
difference score of −8 or less, although 12 right
protocol
TLE patients (7.9 %) obtained a difference score
Left TL Right TL of −8. Thus, the left TL LR is infinitely large for
(N = 172) (N = 152)
WMAs greater than +4 and infinitely small for
Freq Freq
WMAs (column %) (column %) LRs WMAs = −8. In contrast to traditional sensitivity
>4.0 40 (23.3) 0 (0.0) Infinitively large and specificity classification using logistic regres-
2.5 to 4.0 34 (19.8) 4 (2.6) 7.6 sion, or dichotomous LRs, the multiple-level LRs
0.5 to 2.0 32 (18.6) 7 (4.6) 4.0 indicate greater diagnostic sensitivity for larger
−1.5 to 38 (22.1) 24 (15.7) 1.4 WMA magnitudes.
−0.0 In addition to memory outcome studies,
−3.5 to 16 (9.3) 30 (19.7) 0.47 numerous reports indicate a relationship between
−2.0
Wada memory scores and hippocampal volume
−5.5 to 8 (4.7) 36 (23.7) 0.20
−4.0
or cell counts (Baxendale et al., 1997; Cohen-
−7.5 to 4 (2.3) 39 (25.7) 0.089 Gadol et al., 2004; Davies, Hermann, & Foley,
−6.0 1996; Loring et al., 1993; Sass et al., 1991). Both
−8.0 0 (0.0) 12 (7.9) Zero hippocampal volumes and Wada memory asym-
From Loring et al., 2009 metries are related to postoperative verbal
WMA Wada memory asymmetries, TL temporal lobe memory decline (Cohen-Gadol et al., 2004; Lee
et al., 2005; Loring et al., 1995; Perrine et al.,
1995; Sabsevitz et al., 2001; Sperling et al., 1994;
The use of different cut points is illustrated in Stroup et al., 2003).
a recent report from our MCG patient series Although intact baseline verbal memory func-
(Loring, Bowden, Lee, & Meador, 2009). As tion and Wada memory performance following
illustrated in Fig. 5.1, the magnitude of WMA in injection contralateral to the seizure focus both
this group of temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE) make independent contributions to verbal mem-
patients who went to surgery varies as a function ory outcome prediction beyond laterality of
of seizure-onset laterality, although there is also resection and presence of lesions other than
considerable overlap. For patients with left TLE, medial temporal sclerosis (MTS), baseline
the average memory score following left hemi- delayed verbal memory predicted post-op out-
sphere injection was 4.2/8 (SD = 2.5) and follow- come at a higher level of statistical significance
ing right injection = 2.7/8 (SD = 2.8), with a compared to Wada memory (Stroup et al., 2003).
WMA = 1.6 (SD = 3.4). For patients with right However, this report employed real objects for
TLE, the average memory score following left only 75 % of the memory stimuli, and individual
hemisphere injection was 1.7/8 (SD = 2.2) and Wada scores for each group following left and
following right injection = 5.6/8 (SD = 2.2), with right hemisphere injections are not presented.
an average WMA = −3.9 (SD = 2.8). Although the In a separate report, Wada memory scores
left and right WMA are in the correct direction, it were superior to baseline neuropsychological test
is clear from examining the language of the findings in predicting verbal memory change fol-
WMA for each group that that disruptive effects lowing left temporal lobectomy, although the
of language impairment following left hemi- magnitude of this improvement was sufficiently
sphere injection appear to affect the asymmetry small that the authors did not feel that significant
score by approximately 2 points. “added benefit” was obtained to justify subject-
Classifying patients using multiple-level LRs, ing all patients to Wada testing (Baxendale,
we observed that 40 left TLE patients (23.3 %) Thompson, Harkness, & Duncan, 2007).
obtained asymmetry scores greater than +4, However, we note that the Wada protocol
whereas no right TLE patients obtained asymme- employed contained four verbal (naming to
try scores in this range (see Table 5.1). At the description) and four visual memory stimuli (real
5 The Wada Test: Current Perspectives and Applications 129

objects); further, four of the eight stimuli were Comparing performances across procedures
presented again during active drug effect with indicates nonequivalence of protocols and differ-
four foils, and postdrug recognition memory test- ent sensitivity to lateralized temporal lobe dys-
ing was performed with the other four targets and function. The failure of Wada memory testing to
four additional foils. Thus, their memory scores satisfactorily predict postoperative outcome may
combined performance during active drug and simply reflect protocol-specific limitations rather
following return to baseline, employed a target to than suggesting that all Wada protocols fall short
foil ratio of 1:1, and used both verbal and nonver- in this regard. In general, those centers that
bal stimuli. Across this patient series, the left strongly advocate against the utility of the Wada
minus right injection WMA for left temporal test have protocols that may be less robust (e.g.,
lobectomy (TL) patients was −0.15 (SD = 1.2), Kirsch et al., 2005). If a Wada protocol has not
whereas the WMA for right TL patients was −1.8 been empirically validated by any of the
(SD = 1.5), indicating little sensitivity to seizure- approaches described above (e.g., sensitivity to
onset laterality. The average score for left TL seizure-onset laterality across left and right TLE
patients following left hemisphere injection was patients), then the likelihood of clinical outcome
7.1/8 and following right hemisphere injection prediction may indeed be poor since the protocol
was 7.2/8 or declines from a nondrug state of itself may be inadequate to demonstrate unilat-
only 10–11 %. This compares to declines in left eral temporal lobe dysfunction.
TL patients of 48 % (left hemisphere injection) In a comparison of fMRI and Wada memory
and 66 % (right hemisphere injection) compared with other predictors of verbal memory outcome
to nondrug expectations in the MCG patient following ATL, baseline memory and age of sei-
series (Loring et al., 2009). zure onset together accounted for roughly 50 %
The same concern exists for a different report of the variance in memory outcome, and fMRI
stating that Wada memory added nothing to post- explained an additional 10 % of this variance
operative outcome prediction over baseline neu- (Binder et al., 2008). Neither Wada memory
ropsychological testing and MRI in patients asymmetry nor Wada language asymmetry
undergoing left anterior temporal lobectomy added additional predictive power beyond these
(ATL) (Elshorst et al., 2009). Their primary Wada noninvasive measures. The Wada protocol
memory protocol consists of 18 items (4 objects, employed at the Medical College of Wisconsin
5 written words, 1 number, 1 color, 1 command, (MCW) is based upon the MCG protocol and
3 pictures, 1 phase, and 2 definitions). Unlike the employs real objects. However, even here there
Baxendale results above, the authors report a are potentially important procedural differences,
clear effect of medication with an average recog- with the presumed side of seizure onset at MCW
nition score of 62 % following the left (ipsilat- always injected first, whereas the order of injec-
eral) injection but only 39 % following right tion for the MCG protocol randomized across
(contralateral) hemisphere injection in this series patients. Thus, there is the potential for dimin-
of left ATL patients. However, almost 25 % of ished WMA scores in the Binder series if there is
their sample had atypical cerebral language rep- any residual drug effect from the initial (ipsilat-
resentation (right or bilateral), and it is unclear eral) injection when performing the second (con-
what the effects of language impairment may tralateral) study.
have had in this sample given the prominent lin- WMA scores that are inconsistent with the side
guistic content of their memory stimuli. A better of seizure onset appear to indicate a risk for pre-
test of predicting outcome may have been to dicting postoperative verbal memory change
study patients with typical left hemisphere lan- (Baxendale et al., 2007; Sabsevitz et al., 2001).
guage dominance, as well as reporting bilateral This relationship would be expected since WMA
Wada memory scores in those undergoing scores are a combination of functional reserve and
right ATL. functional adequacy. In addition, Wada memory
130 D.W. Loring and K.J. Meador

testing suggests that verbal memory Wada results amobarbital administration, methohexital is typi-
(right hemisphere injection) are moderately cor- cally given in divided doses of 3 mg followed
related with baseline neuropsychological verbal shortly by 2 mg, although the reasons for this
memory results (Vingerhoets, Miatton, Vonck, rather than a single mg injection are not clear and
Seurinck, & Boon, 2006). One limitation of the whether this approach is superior in any way to a
Wada test, at least currently in the US healthcare single 5 mg injection has not been established.
system, is that the number of patients seen in fol- Single doses of 6–8 mg have been used without
low-up is low. In fact, follow-up rates are increas- any apparent difficulty (Marla Hamberger, per-
ingly low due to insurance coverage limitations in sonal communication, December 5, 2009). In
which follow-up assessments are not covered, general, language and memory results using
except in the instances of significant postopera- methohexital appear to be equivalent to those
tive change. Current levels of evidence required obtained using amobarbital. However, there
by the journal Neurology require a loss to follow- appears to be increased seizure risk since metho-
up rate of 20 % or less in order to infer a Class I hexital is associated with a decreased seizure
level (Gross & Johnston, 2009). threshold (Loddenkemper, Moddel, Schuele,
Wyllie, & Morris, 2007).
Propofol is a nonbarbiturate anesthesia with a
Alternative Medications very short duration of action and has been suc-
cessfully used as a substitute for amobarbital.
The availability of amobarbital became sporadic Dosing of 20 mg of propofol produces results
beginning in the late 1990s. In addition, amobar- comparable to 120 mg of amobarbital as reflected
bital is no longer manufactured by Lilly but is by the time to verbal and nonverbal responses
produced by Raxbury, which does not distribute (Mikati et al., 2009). However, due to its short
the drug outside the USA. This had two direct duration of action, a number of patients require
effects on Wada testing. First, alternative anes- incremental injections to maintain the desired
thetic agents including methohexital, propofol, level of motor weakness. Approximately one-
and etomidate were explored as agents to create third of patients had an adverse event following
hemispheric anesthesia for traditional Wada lan- propofol injection, with 12 % of all patients hav-
guage and memory testing. The second effect, ing increased muscle tone with twitching, rhyth-
however, was to focus attention on issues regard- mic movements, or tonic posturing. Patients
ing whether Wada testing should be the standard older than 55 or receiving a total injection dose
of care in the evaluation of all epilepsy surgery greater than 20 mg were more likely to have sig-
candidates. nificant adverse events, which in turn carries a
Although formal surveying has not been done, risk of incompletion or inaccuracy of the Wada
the second most common anesthetic after amo- test.
barbital is likely methohexital (Brevital). Another nonbarbiturate anesthetic alternative
Methohexital was introduced for Wada testing at to amobarbital is etomidate, which has rapid
the University of Florida due to its relatively onset and short duration of action. Several
short duration of action that would facilitate both approaches to etomidate administration have
hemispheres being studied on the same day been successfully employed. At the MNI, a bolus
because left and right amobarbital Wada injec- of 2 mg etomidate that is administered followed
tions were performed on different days at many by infusion insures a uniform period of anesthe-
surgical centers (Gilmore, Heilman, Schmidt, sia (Jones-Gotman et al., 2005). The bolus is fol-
Fennell, & Quisling, 1992). The largest study lowed by an infusion (0.003–0.004 mg/kg/min)
using methohexital is from the University of at a rate of 6 mL/h. The use of a constant infusion
Michigan (Buchtel, Passaro, Selwa, Deveikis, & allows direct control of the period of hemispheric
Gomez-Hassan, 2002), and this drug has also anesthesia, insuring that all testing and memory
been more recently adopted by the Cleveland item presentation is presented during maximum
Clinic (Lineweaver et al., 2006). In contrast to anesthesia rather than during recovery from anes-
5 The Wada Test: Current Perspectives and Applications 131

thesia in which the medication effects are not administered (to produce a flaccid contralateral
fully known and may not be equivalent between hemiplegia), and individual patient differences in
hemispheres. drug responsivity. However, behavioral recovery
tends to be rapid, rarely lasting more than 10 min.
In our experience, comprehension of complex
Comment two-stage commands involving inverted syntax is
typically the last language-related ability to
The difficulty in obtaining amobarbital has had recover, and repetition is the next most sensitive
the benefit of making centers ask the question of language measure to mild residual medication
whether Wada testing can be or should be per- effects. Typically, the last motor deficits to
formed in all patients and to evaluate to what resolve include pronator drift, decreased motor
degree Wada testing should continue to be per- speed, or asterixis contralateral to the side of
formed in preoperative epilepsy surgery evalua- injection. Return of language and motor func-
tion. Advances in functional and structural tions to baseline levels is necessary prior to
imaging have provided predictors of memory out- assessing memory.
come, thereby decreasing the sole reliance on Patients undergoing Wada testing have a pre-
Wada memory results in this context. However, test baseline evaluation that serves two purposes.
there continues to be confusion about the role of The first is to familiarize the patient with the spe-
the Wada test since so many procedural variations cific techniques that will be administered during
exist. Further, difficulty also exists given the vari- the test. The second is to establish a baseline
ability in neuropsychological memory outcome level of function, which is particularly useful for
measures and what the definitions of “significant language tasks since they may be affected by
change” are employed. Ultimately, these pieces of schooling, clinical history, as well as having idio-
information are necessary to inform epilepsy pro- syncratic regional dialects. With few exceptions,
grams which patient should be considered for patients will obtain perfect scores of 8/8 for
Wada testing since the cost/benefit ratio will vary memory testing using our recognition approach.
based upon clinical semiology, patient character- The order of injection between the presumed
istics, and Wada protocol employed. ipsilateral and contralateral sides is alternated
across patients in order to ensure that there is not
a systematic confound associated with injection
Miscellaneous order. For example, to the extent that there are
residual amobarbital effects lasting beyond the
The potency of anesthetic effects of amobarbital typical testing window, always beginning the
is reduced by carbonic anhydrase inhibitor medi- ipsilateral injection may tend to decrease WMA
cations such as topiramate or zonisamide magnitude. Both hemispheres are typically tested
(Bookheimer, Schrader, Rausch, Sankar, & on the same day, with a minimum of 30 min sepa-
Engel, 2005; Burns et al., 2009; Kipervasser rating the two hemispheric studies. Patients are
et al., 2004). Thus, amobarbital dose adjustments forewarned that they may experience arm weak-
may be required, or testing patients off their car- ness or difficulty talking, although memory for
bonic anhydrase medications may be preferred. either impairment is often poor. Patients are also
told that the medication makes some people feel
a little bit drunk or sleepy. This is included to
Medical College of Georgia (MCG) reduce anxiety and to create an expectancy to
Wada Protocol: Clinical Core help the patient interpret subjective drug-induced
effects. After beginning to tell patients about
The total duration of amobarbital anesthetic potential subjective drug effects, we have
effect depends both on the initial drug dose, decreased the number of “confused” or mildly
whether additional incremental injections are agitated responses to nearly zero.
132 D.W. Loring and K.J. Meador

General Language

Patients begin counting repeatedly from 1 to 20 Language assessment contains five common lan-
with their hands held up, their palms turned guage domains (viz., counting disruption, com-
upward, and fingers spread. An injection of prehension, naming, repetition, and reading).
100 mg amobarbital is administered by hand over Although we have developed a formalized
a 4–5 s interval via a percutaneous transfemoral approach to calculate a language laterality ratio
catheter. Following demonstration of hemiplegia [(left score minus right score)/(left score plus
and evaluation of eye-gaze deviation, the patient right score), (L − R)/(L + R)], this laterality ratio
is requested to execute a simple midline com- is used for research purposes and is not routinely
mand (e.g., “stick out your tongue”). We do not reported clinically (Loring et al., 1990b).
require a complete flaccid hemiplegia in order to
proceed with cognitive testing and do not base
our timing of stimulus presentation according to Expressive Language/Counting
EEG-based information. If there is only minimal
change in muscle tone, an incremental injection At the outset of the Wada test, patients begin
of 25 mg may be used. However, we try to avoid counting from 1 to 20 repeatedly. The expressive
more than one additional dose increment since it language score (0–4) is based upon disruption of
has been our experience that multiple injections counting (4 = normal, slowed, or brief pause
increase the likelihood of patient obtundation <~20 s; 3 = counting perseveration with normal
compared to an identical total dose delivered sequencing; 2 = sequencing errors; 1 = single
with a single bolus. Complete lack of motor defi- number or word perseveration; 0 = arrest >~20 s).
cit suggests misplacement of the catheter. Based upon the findings that a brief language
Following evaluation of simple comprehen- pause following drug administration does not
sion for midline commands and eye gaze, eight correspond to laterality scores obtained from
common objects are presented for 4–8 s each in fMRI (Benbadis et al., 1998), we require at least
the visual field midline and ipsilateral to the a 20 s speech arrest to insure that counting inter-
injection, and the object names are repeated twice ruption is not due to acute generalized medica-
to the patient. On average, this occurs beginning tion effects. If speech arrest occurs, patients are
approximately 30–45 s following injection. immediately asked to begin counting again start-
Examples of Wada memory items include a com- ing with “1, 2, 3, …” since the overlearned por-
bination of ordinary household items (e.g., fork, tion of the initial counting sequence will be less
mousetrap), small toys (e.g., doll, plastic shark), likely disrupted from generalized medication
and plastic food (e.g., hotdog, pizza). At times, effects.
due to patient confusion, inattention, or nonre-
sponsiveness, the patient’s eyes are held open.
Language, discussed below, is assessed in detail Comprehension
following presentation of memory items. We also
impose a minimum of 10 min between drug Simple comprehension is assessed after the
administration and memory assessment. assessment of eye-gaze deviation, by requesting
Recognition memory of material presented the patient to execute a simple midline command
during the procedure is tested after amobarbital (e.g., “stick out your tongue”). The ability to exe-
effects have worn off as demonstrated by return cute simple midline commands is one of the first
baseline language performance on all tasks comprehension tasks that patients with aphasia
described below, return of 5/5 strength, and are able to execute and is included at this point in
absence of pronator drift, tactile extinction, the assessment to help evaluate an early possible
asterixis, and bradykinesia. dissociation between receptive and expressive
5 The Wada Test: Current Perspectives and Applications 133

language. Following object memory stimulus etition is graded on a 0–3 rating scale. If unable
presentation, comprehension is systematically to provide any response, the patient is asked to
assessed with a modified token test. The token repeat “Mary had a little lamb.”
test consists of four geometric shapes of different
colors, which are presented vertically to the sub-
ject’s ipsilateral visual field. They are presented Reading
vertically to minimize the effects of visual field
or visual attentional deficits associated with uni- Patients are asked to read either “The car backed
lateral injection. over the curb” or “The rabbit hopped down the
Comprehension is rated based upon the level lane.” Performance is qualitatively rated on a 0–3
of syntactic complexity in the command that is point scale.
correctly executed: 1. “point to the blue circle
after the red square”; 2. “point to the red circle
and then point to the blue square”; and 3. “point General Language Considerations
to the red square.” A score of 3 is awarded for
completion of a complex two-stage command When language impairments are present, lan-
with inverted syntax, a score of 2 reflects suc- guage stimuli are presented throughout the recov-
cessful simple two-stage command, 1 is scored ery phase to monitor drug effects, and the time of
for one-stage command, and 0 if the subject can- complete language recovery is noted. The same
not perform any commands. or alternative stimuli as those employed during
A screen and metric approach to assessment is the initial assessment are used with the exception
used, although significant discretion is afforded to of repetition. Repetition is a very sensitive mea-
the examiner. For a typical language-dominant sure of mild language impairment, and additional
patient, in which complete speech arrest is obtained, repetition items such as “Methodist-Episcopal”
subjects are presented with the most simple one- and sentences from the Boston Diagnostic
step command in order to test the current status of Aphasia Examination are used to monitor recov-
receptive language systems. If the patient is able to ery (e.g., “The spy fled to Greece”). Positive par-
successfully perform the task, then the syntax com- aphasic responses are considered the single
plexity is incrementally made more difficult. In strongest evidence of language representation in
contrast, with a nondominant hemisphere injection, the hemisphere being studied.
the patient is typically presented initially with the
most difficult of the available tasks.
Memory

Confrontation Naming A minimum of 10 min following amobarbital


injection is required prior to memory assessment.
Two line drawings of common objects (i.e., Although free recall of object memory stimuli is
watch and jacket) are presented and the subject is obtained, clinical interpretation of Wada memory
asked to name the objects and parts of the performance is based solely on object
objects (e.g., watchband, collar). Performance is recognition.
qualitatively scored on a 0–3 point scale. Both
items are presented for both left and right hemi-
sphere injections. Ipsilateral Performance

Each of the eight objects is presented randomly


Repetition interspersed with 16 foils, and forced choice rec-
ognition is obtained. One-half the number of
Following object naming, the patient repeats false-positive responses is subtracted from the
phrases (e.g., “No if’s, and’s, or but’s”) and rep- number of objects correctly recognized to correct
134 D.W. Loring and K.J. Meador

for possible response bias and guessing. Thus, correct. Wada memory asymmetries (WMA) of
the expected score in the absence of true recog- at least two are interpreted as evidence of lateral-
nition is 0. ized impairment, although greater asymmetries
are interpreted with more confidence and provide
more diagnostically useful information (Loring
Laterality Scores et al., 2009). In addition, allowances may be
made for the presence of aphasia, which tend to
Since Wada memory scores are used to assist in dampen the overall group performance following
seizure-onset lateralization by demonstrating lat- left hemisphere injection by approximately one
eralized dysfunction, the order of injection is ran- point. When asymmetries in the “wrong” direc-
domized across subjects and memory results are tion are observed, they are cause for particular
interpreted in a blind fashion. To assess lateral- concern, and the procedure may be repeated
ized asymmetries, interhemispheric Wada mem- bilaterally using a 75 mg dose beginning on the
ory difference scores (i.e., [left injection] − [right side ipsilateral to the presumed seizure onset.
injection]) derived from corrected memory per-
formances are computed; positive scores suggest
left temporal lobe dysfunction and negative Conclusions
scores suggest right temporal lobe impairment.
The eight target objects are interspersed with The preoperative evaluation for epilepsy surgery
16 distractor items. Each item is presented to the involves not only localization of the epileptic
patient, and they are asked to indicate whether or focus but also localization of cerebral dysfunc-
not they saw each item previously. Occasionally a tion. Wada test results are not considered abso-
patient will indicate that they saw an item but that lute and are always considered in the context of
it was not in the current stimulus set. They are other clinical factors, such as consistency of sei-
then asked if they had seen it anytime during the zure onset; presence of a structural lesion, such
Wada procedure, and they are given credit even if as tumor or hippocampal atrophy on MRI; and
they believe that they had seen the item during a other functional measures (e.g., fMRI, PET). We
previous stimulus set. To correct for guessing, we suggest that as part of studies describing Wada
subtract one-half the number of false-positive rec- clinical utility or lack thereof, the sensitivity of
ognitions from the total number of targets cor- the specific Wada protocol to seizure-onset later-
rectly recognized. Thus, any random response set ality as a surrogate marker of temporal lobe dys-
in the absence of any true memory signal will lead function should also be reported. In the future,
to an expected value of zero. However, it is use of the Wada test is likely to decline as less
unusual that there are more than one to two false- invasive procedures to localize cerebral dysfunc-
positive errors, and we have always obtained sim- tion are employed. The lessons learned from
ilar group findings when we analyzed our data Wada testing should be considered to avoid
with raw or corrected memory scores. repeating past mistakes.

General Memory Considerations References


Fixed pass/fail criteria are not employed for Alpherts, W. C. J., Vermeulen, J., & van Veelen, C. W. M.
memory performance following injection ipsilat- (2000). The Wada test: Prediction of focus lateraliza-
eral to the seizure onset. However, we generally tion by asymmetric and symmetric recall. Epilepsy
Research, 39(3), 239–249.
require a score of at least 2/8 following the injec-
Arora, J., Pugh, K., Westerveld, M., Spencer, S., Spencer,
tion ipsilateral to the seizure focus in order to not D. D., & Todd Constable, R. (2009). Language lateral-
repeat the Wada memory assessment and are ization in epilepsy patients: fMRI validated with the
more comfortable with scores of at least 3/8 Wada procedure. Epilepsia, 50(10), 2225–2241.
5 The Wada Test: Current Perspectives and Applications 135

Baxendale, S., Thompson, P., Harkness, W., & Duncan, Postoperative memory prediction in left temporal lobe
J. (2007). The role of the intracarotid amobarbital pro- epilepsy: The Wada test is of no added value to preop-
cedure in predicting verbal memory decline after tem- erative neuropsychological assessment and MRI.
poral lobe resection. Epilepsia, 48(3), 546–552. Epilepsy & Behavior, 16(2), 335–340.
Baxendale, S. A., Van Paesschen, W., Thompson, P. J., Gaillard, W. D., Balsamo, L., Xu, B., Grandin, C. B.,
Duncan, J. S., Shorvon, S. D., & Connelly, A. (1997). Braniecki, S. H., Papero, P. H., et al. (2002). Language
The relation between quantitative MRI measures of dominance in partial epilepsy patients identified with
hippocampal structure and the intracarotid amobarbi- an fMRI reading task. Neurology, 59(2), 256–265.
tal test. Epilepsia, 38(9), 998–1007. Gaillard, W. D., Balsamo, L., Xu, B., McKinney, C.,
Benbadis, S. R., Binder, J. R., Swanson, S. J., Fischer, M., Papero, P. H., Weinstein, S., et al. (2004). fMRI lan-
Hammeke, T. A., Morris, G. L., et al. (1998). Is speech guage task panel improves determination of language
arrest during Wada testing a valid method for deter- dominance. Neurology, 63(8), 1403–1408.
mining hemispheric representation of language? Brain Gilmore, R. L., Heilman, K. M., Schmidt, R. P., Fennell,
and Language, 65(3), 441–446. E. M., & Quisling, R. (1992). Anosognosia during
Binder, J. R., Sabsevitz, D. S., Swanson, S. J., Hammeke, Wada testing. Neurology, 42(4), 925–927.
T. A., Raghavan, M., & Mueller, W. M. (2008). Use of Glosser, G., & Donofrio, N. (2001). Differences between
preoperative functional MRI to predict verbal memory nouns and verbs after anterior temporal lobectomy.
decline after temporal lobe epilepsy surgery. Epilepsia, Neuropsychology, 15, 39–47.
49(8), 1377–1394. Grimes, D. A., & Schulz, K. F. (2005). Refining clinical
Binder, J. R., Swanson, S. J., Hammeke, T. A., Morris, diagnosis with likelihood ratios. The Lancet,
G. L., Mueller, W. M., Fischer, M., et al. (1996). 365(9469), 1500–1505.
Determination of language dominance using func- Gross, R. A., & Johnston, K. C. (2009). Levels of evi-
tional MRI: A comparison with the Wada test. dence: Taking neurology(R) to the next level.
Neurology, 46(4), 978–984. Neurology, 72(1), 8–10.
Bookheimer, S., Schrader, L. M., Rausch, R., Sankar, R., Hosmer, D. W., & Lemeshaw, S. (2000). Applied logistic
& Engel, J. (2005). Reduced anesthetization during regression (2nd ed.). New York, NY: John Wiley &
the intracarotid amobarbital (Wada) test in patients Sons.
taking carbonic anhydrase-inhibiting medications. Jabbour, R. A., Hempel, A., Gates, J. R., Zhang, W., &
Epilepsia, 46(2), 236–243. Risse, G. L. (2005). Right hemisphere language map-
Buchtel, H. A., Passaro, E. A., Selwa, L. M., Deveikis, J., ping in patients with bilateral language. Epilepsy &
& Gomez-Hassan, D. (2002). Sodium methohexital Behavior, 6(4), 587–592.
(Brevital) as an anesthetic in the Wada test. Epilepsia, Jack, C. R., Jr., Sharbrough, F. W., Cascino, G. D.,
43(9), 1056–1061. Hirschorn, K. A., O’Brien, P. C., & Marsh, W. R.
Burns, T. G., Lee, G. P., McCormick, M. L., Pettoni, (1992). Magnetic resonance image-based hippocam-
A. N., Flamini, J. R., & Cohen, M. (2009). Carbonic pal volumetry: Correlation with outcome after tempo-
anhydrase-inhibiting medications and the intracarotid ral lobectomy. Annals of Neurology, 31(2), 138–146.
amobarbital procedure in children. Epilepsy & Jones-Gotman, M., Sziklas, V., Djordjevic, J., Dubeau, F.,
Behavior, 15(2), 240–244. Gotman, J., Angle, M., et al. (2005). Etomidate speech
Chelune, G. J. (1995). Hippocampal adequacy versus and memory test (eSAM): A new drug and improved
functional reserve: Predicting memory functions fol- intracarotid procedure. Neurology, 65(11),
lowing temporal lobectomy. Archives of Clinical 1723–1729.
Neuropsychology, 10, 413–432. Kipervasser, S., Andelman, F., Kramer, U., Nagar, S.,
Cohen-Gadol, A. A., Westerveld, M., Alvarez-Carilles, J., Fried, I., & Neufeld, M. Y. (2004). Effects of topira-
& Spencer, D. D. (2004). Intracarotid Amytal memory mate on memory performance on the intracarotid
test and hippocampal magnetic resonance imaging amobarbital (Wada) test. Epilepsy & Behavior, 5(2),
volumetry: Validity of the Wada test as an indicator of 197–203.
hippocampal integrity among candidates for epilepsy Kirsch, H. E., Walker, J. A., Winstanley, F. S., Hendrickson,
surgery. Journal of Neurosurgery, 101(6), 926–931. R., Wong, S. T., Barbaro, N. M., et al. (2005).
Davies, K. G., Hermann, B. P., & Foley, K. T. (1996). Limitations of Wada memory asymmetry as a predic-
Relation between intracarotid amobarbital memory tor of outcomes after temporal lobectomy. Neurology,
asymmetry scores and hippocampal sclerosis in 65(5), 676–680.
patients undergoing anterior temporal lobe resections. Lee, G. P., Park, Y. D., Westerveld, M., Hempel, A., &
Epilepsia, 37(6), 522–525. Loring, D. W. (2002). Effect of Wada methodology in
Doss, R. C., Zhang, W., Risse, G. L., & Dickens, D. L. predicting lateralized memory impairment in pediatric
(2009). Lateralizing language with magnetic source epilepsy surgery candidates. Epilepsy & Behavior,
imaging: Validation based on the Wada test. Epilepsia, 3(5), 439–447.
50(10), 2242–2248. Lee, G. P., Westerveld, M., Blackburn, L. B., Park, Y. D.,
Elshorst, N., Pohlmann-Eden, B., Horstmann, S., Schulz, & Loring, D. W. (2005). Prediction of verbal memory
R., Woermann, F., & McAndrews, M. P. (2009). decline after epilepsy surgery in children: Effectiveness
136 D.W. Loring and K.J. Meador

of Wada memory asymmetries. Epilepsia, 46(1), Mikati, M. A., Naasan, G., Tarabay, H., El Yamen, S.,
97–103. Baydoun, A., & Comair, Y. G. (2009). Intracarotid pro-
Lineweaver, T. T., Morris, H. H., Naugle, R. I., Najm, pofol testing: A comparative study with amobarbital.
I. M., Diehl, B., & Bingaman, W. (2006). Evaluating Epilepsy & Behavior, 14(3), 503–507.
the contributions of state-of-the-art assessment tech- Milner, B., Branch, C., & Rasmussen, T. (1962). Study of
niques to predicting memory outcome after unilateral short-term memory after intracarotid injection of
anterior temporal lobectomy. Epilepsia, 47(11), sodium Amytal. Transactions of the American
1895–1903. Neurological Association, 87, 224–226.
Loddenkemper, T., Moddel, G., Schuele, S. U., Wyllie, E., Ojemann, G. A. (1980). Brain mechanisms for language:
& Morris, H. H., III. (2007). Seizures during intraca- Observations during neurosurgery. In J. S. Lockard &
rotid methohexital and amobarbital testing. Epilepsy A. A. Ward (Eds.), Epilepsy: A window to brain mech-
& Behavior, 10(1), 49–54. anisms. New York, NY: Raven.
Loring, D. W., Bowden, S. C., Lee, G. P., & Meador, K. J. Papanicolaou, A. C., Simos, P. G., Castillo, E. M., Breier,
(2009). Diagnostic utility of Wada Memory Asymmetries: J. I., Sarkari, S., Pataraia, E., et al. (2004).
Sensitivity, specificity, and likelihood ratio characteriza- Magnetocephalography: A noninvasive alternative to
tion. Neuropsychology, 23(6), 687–693. the Wada procedure. Journal of Neurosurgery, 100(5),
Loring, D. W., Hermann, B. P., Meador, K. J., Lee, G. P., 867–876.
Gallagher, B. B., King, D. W., et al. (1994). Amnesia Penfield, W., & Milner, B. (1958). Memory deficit pro-
after unilateral temporal lobectomy: A case report. duced by bilateral lesions in the hippocampal zone.
Epilepsia, 35(4), 757–763. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 79, 475–497.
Loring, D. W., Hermann, B. P., Perrine, K., Plenger, P. M., Perrine, K., Westerveld, M., Sass, K. J., Devinsky, O.,
Lee, G. P., & Meador, K. J. (1997). Effect of Wada Dogali, M., Spencer, D. D., et al. (1995). Wada mem-
memory stimulus type in discriminating lateralized ory disparities predict seizure laterality and postopera-
temporal lobe impairment. Epilepsia, 38(2), 219–224. tive seizure control. Epilepsia, 36(9), 851–856.
Loring, D. W., Lee, G. P., Meador, K. J., Flanigin, H. F., Ruff, I. M., Swanson, S. J., Hammeke, T. A., Sabsevitz,
Smith, J. R., Figueroa, R. E., et al. (1990). The intraca- D., Mueller, W. M., & Morris, G. L. (2007). Predictors
rotid amobarbital procedure as a predictor of memory of naming decline after dominant temporal lobectomy:
failure following unilateral temporal lobectomy. Age at onset of epilepsy and age of word acquisition.
Neurology, 40(4), 605–610. Epilepsy & Behavior, 10(2), 272–277.
Loring, D. W., Meador, K. J., Lee, G. P., Flanigin, H. F., Sabsevitz, D. S., Swanson, S. J., Hammeke, T. A., Spanaki,
King, D. W., & Smith, J. R. (1990a). Crossed aphasia M. V., Possing, E. T., Morris, G. L., III, et al. (2003).
in a patient with complex partial seizures: Evidence Use of preoperative functional neuroimaging to pre-
from intracarotid amobarbital testing, functional corti- dict language deficits from epilepsy surgery.
cal mapping, and neuropsychological assessment. Neurology, 60(11), 1788–1792.
Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, Sabsevitz, D. S., Swanson, S. J., Morris, G. L., Mueller,
12(2), 340–354. W. M., & Seidenberg, M. (2001). Memory outcome
Loring, D. W., Meador, K. J., Lee, G. P., King, D. W., after left anterior temporal lobectomy in patients with
Gallagher, B. B., Murro, A. M., et al. (1994). Stimulus expected and reversed Wada memory asymmetry
timing effects on Wada memory testing. Archives of scores. Epilepsia, 42(11), 1408–1415.
Neurology, 51(8), 806–810. Sass, K. J., Lencz, T., Westerveld, M., Novelly, R. A.,
Loring, D. W., Meador, K. J., Lee, G. P., King, D. W., Spencer, D. D., & Kim, J. H. (1991). The neural sub-
Nichols, M. E., Park, Y. D., et al. (1995). Wada mem- strate of memory impairment demonstrated by the
ory asymmetries predict verbal memory decline after intracarotid amobarbital procedure. Archives of
anterior temporal lobectomy. Neurology, 45(7), Neurology, 48(1), 48–52.
1329–1333. Snyder, P. J., Novelly, R. A., & Harris, L. J. (1990). Mixed
Loring, D. W., Meador, K. J., Lee, G. P., Murro, A. M., speech dominance in the Intracarotid Sodium Amytal
Smith, J. R., Flanigin, H. F., et al. (1990b). Cerebral Procedure: Validity and criteria issues. Journal of
language lateralization: Evidence from intracarotid Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 12(5),
amobarbital testing. Neuropsychologia, 28(8), 629–643.
831–838. Sperling, M. R., Saykin, A. J., Glosser, G., Moran, M.,
Loring, D. W., Meador, K. J., & Lee, G. P. (1992). French, J. A., Brooks, M., et al. (1994). Predictors of
Amobarbital dose effects on Wada memory testing. outcome after anterior temporal lobectomy: The intrac-
Journal of Epilepsy, 5, 171–174. arotid amobarbital test. Neurology, 44(12), 2325–2330.
Loring, D. W., Murro, A. M., Meador, K. J., Lee, G. P., Strauss, S. E., Richardson, W. S., Glasziou, P., & Haynes,
Gratton, C. A., Nichols, M. E., et al. (1993). Wada R. B. (2005). Evidence-based medicine: How to prac-
memory testing and hippocampal volume measure- tice and teach EBAM (3rd ed.). Edinburgh: Elsevier
ments in the evaluation for temporal lobectomy. Churchill-Livingstone.
Neurology, 43(9), 1789–1793. Stroup, E., Langfitt, J., Berg, M., McDermott, M., Pilcher,
Meador, K. J., & Loring, D. W. (2005). The Wada test for W., & Como, P. (2003). Predicting verbal memory
language and memory lateralization. Neurology, decline following anterior temporal lobectomy (ATL).
65(5), 659. Neurology, 60(8), 1266–1273.
5 The Wada Test: Current Perspectives and Applications 137

Suarez, R. O., Whalen, S., Nelson, A. P., Tie, Y., The limits of material specificity. Epilepsy & Behavior,
Meadows, M. E., Radmanesh, A., et al. (2009). 8(2), 422–428.
Threshold-independent functional MRI determina- Wada, J. A. (1997). Youthful season revisited. Brain and
tion of language dominance: A validation study Cognition, 33, 7–13.
against clinical gold standards. Epilepsy & Behavior, Woermann, F. G., Jokeit, H., Luerding, R., Freitag, H.,
16(2), 288–297. Schulz, R., Guertler, S., et al. (2003). Language later-
Testa, S. M., Ward, J., Crone, N. E., & Brandt, J. (2008). alization by Wada test and fMRI in 100 patients with
Stimulus type affects Wada memory performance. epilepsy. Neurology, 61(5), 699–701.
Epilepsy & Behavior, 13(3), 458–462. Wyllie, E., Luders, H., Murphy, D., Morris, H., III, Dinner,
Vingerhoets, G., Miatton, M., Vonck, K., Seurinck, R., & D., Lesser, R., et al. (1990). Intracarotid amobarbital
Boon, P. (2006). Memory performance during the (Wada) test for language dominance: Correlation with
intracarotid amobarbital procedure and neuropsycho- results of cortical stimulation. Epilepsia, 31(2),
logical assessment in medial temporal lobe epilepsy: 156–161.
Electrocortical Mapping
of Language 6
Chris Morrison and Chad E. Carlson

Cortical mapping, whether intraoperative or at malformations of cortical development (Burchiel,


the patient’s bedside, involves direct stimulation Clark, & Ojemann, 1989; Devinsky et al., 2000;
of the brain in order to determine the functionality Ojemann, Ojemann, Lettich, & Berger, 1989).
of the underlying cortex with regard to specific Identification of neuroanatomical landmarks and
sensory, motor, or cognitive processes. Cortical regions of eloquent cortex via mapping
mapping procedures provide neurosurgeons with procedures can impact the extent of surgical
functional data which can be correlated with the resection. For example, if regions of eloquent
anatomical landmarks underlying the implanted cortex are identified near to or overlapping with
electrodes. This procedure is particularly useful the planned resection zone, then a more restricted
when working with patients at risk for atypical resection may be made in an effort to reduce
cortical representation of function. While the postoperative language deficits. Conversely,
anatomy of primary cortical regions may seem demonstrating the absence of eloquent language
relatively fixed, primary sensory and motor areas cortex near the planned surgery may allow for a
may be atypically distributed in a brain that has more aggressive resection.
malformations and/or disease from an early age
(Farrell, Burbank, Lettich, & Ojemann, 2007).
Relative to primary sensory and motor regions, History of Cortical Mapping
language areas can be even less definitively
localized and may be atypically distributed in the Mapping procedures have been used in standard
brain of an individual with epileptogenic foci or clinical practice for over 70 years. Early reports
other form of pathology such as focal lesions or of this technique describe intraoperative mapping
of motor and somatosensory regions (Foerster,
1936). The work of Penfield and Ojemann, among
C. Morrison, Ph.D., A.B.P.P. (*)
others (see, e.g., Ojemann, 1979; Penfield &
Dept. Neurosurgery, New York University Roberts, 1959), greatly expanded knowledge and
School of Medicine, 223 East 34th Street, use of cortical mapping and demonstrated how
New York, NY 10016, USA the procedure could be used to identify not just
e-mail: chris.morrison@nyumc.org
motor and sensory cortex but more cognitively
C.E. Carlson, M.D. based (e.g., language functions) cortical regions.
Department of Neurology, Medical College of
Wisconsin, 9200 West Wisconsin Avenue,
Language mapping as a technique has been used
Milwaukee, WI 53226, USA in patients with epilepsy and those with focal
e-mail: ccarlson@mcw.edu lesions in a language zone and no epilepsy.

W.B. Barr and C. Morrison (eds.), Handbook on the Neuropsychology of Epilepsy, 139
Clinical Handbooks in Neuropsychology, DOI 10.1007/978-0-387-92826-5_6,
© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015
140 C. Morrison and C.E. Carlson

In the early years, mapping procedures were con- [fMRI] or magnetoencephalography, MEG) pro-
ducted intraoperatively under local anesthesia cedures may increase or decrease the likelihood
and could last several hours (Luders et al., 1988), of mapping. Specifically, if hemispheric language
which could be quite arduous for patients. In the dominance is found to be contralateral to the
late 1970s and early 1980s, chronic implantation planned resection, mapping can be avoided; if it
of intracranial electrodes became increasingly is ipsilateral, mapping may be necessary. The
prevalent. Intracranial EEG monitoring over a localization information obtained from the proce-
sustained period of time (i.e., days to weeks) to dures listed above may help to make this
capture seizures allowed for improved lateraliza- decision.
tion and localization of the ictal onset zone(s) as Absent these data, mapping is generally
more detailed recordings from larger contiguous indicated in a left-hemispheric resection of
brain areas could be obtained (Lueders et al., frontal or temporal regions in a right-handed
1982). Chronically implanted electrodes also person. Clinicians may also consider language
facilitated extra-operative mapping of cortical mapping in a right-hemispheric procedure
functions that could be performed at the patient’s when the patient is left-handed and/or there is a
bedside (Lesser, Hahn, Lueders, Rothner, & family history of left-handedness as the poten-
Erenberg, 1981). Moving the mapping procedure tial for partial or complete language representa-
from the operating room to the bedside provided tion in the right hemisphere is much higher than
significant advantages for both the clinician and in right-handed individuals (Rassmussen &
the patient. Intraoperative mapping can increase Milner, 1977). A left-hemispheric developmen-
the overall duration of the procedure, thereby tal abnormality or lesion (i.e., a lesion that was
increasing the duration of immobility and expo- present prior to or around the development of
sure to general anesthesia. In addition, most language) may also result in mixed or right
patients tolerate mapping better in the less intimi- dominant cortical organization of language
dating setting of the bedside, compared with an (Rassmussen & Milner, 1977). A preoperative
awake craniotomy; improved tolerability often IAP in these circumstances is recommended as
translates to improved cooperation and thereby it may minimize or eliminate the need for a
improved quality of the data obtained. Although mapping procedure. Finally, mapping of select
it is not always possible, bedside mapping can language functions is recommended when a
eliminate the need for an awake craniotomy in mixed language dominance pattern is seen dur-
many patients. ing the IAP. In this setting, focused mapping of
a subset of language abilities may be necessary.
For example, if during an IAP receptive lan-
Indications for Language Mapping guage is localized contralaterally and expres-
sive language (or, e.g., just naming) is localized
Ultimately, the need for cortical mapping will be ipsilateral to the surgical side, then just those
assessed by the surgical team and is based on the latter abilities may need to be mapped.
location of the planned resection and the poten-
tial for overlap of that region with eloquent cor-
tex. If the surgeon feels that the resection can be Bedside Mapping
performed with no significant risk to eloquent
cortex, mapping will not be necessary. As is true As with the IAP, language mapping with direct
with many aspects of cortical mapping, a fair cortical stimulation has never been standardized
amount of variability will be seen between cen- in the way that neuropsychological tests are. As a
ters and between surgeons. That is, verification of result, there is substantial variability in the
hemispheric language dominance with invasive stimulation techniques (and equipment), testing
(i.e., intracarotid amobarbital procedure [IAP] or stimuli, and the clinicians involved with the
Wada test) or noninvasive (e.g., functional MRI mapping procedure across surgical centers. Given
6 Electrocortical Mapping of Language 141

this variability, a definitive set of “rules for wrap. Subsequently, the EEG technician will
mapping” cannot be delineated, particularly connect the intracranial electrodes to the EEG
given the fact that the variability is in large part amplifier system. The cables that connect the
due to limited, if any, published data or protocols. electrodes to the amplifier vary based upon the
As a result, the discussion below will provide the vendor, and the exact process will depend on the
reader with examples of methodology and a case equipment. Similarly, different video-EEG
example for demonstration of how the procedure system vendors will have variations in the process
is performed at our and other regional centers. by which the computer software recognizes the
We will try to identify points of variation that electrodes and how many electrodes can be
may be seen and what potential advantages or simultaneously recorded; most systems can
disadvantages these may convey to the examiner record at least 128 channels simultaneously.
and the patient. Once this process is completed, the patient will
be monitored to capture interictal and ictal data
as needed.
Before the Mapping Procedure As is the case with any evaluation of
neurocognitive functions, there are numerous
Cortical mapping at the patient’s bedside is factors which can affect the mapping procedure.
predicated on the presence of intracranial Among the most important is the impact of the
electrodes which can be isolated and stimulated patient’s antiepileptic drug (AED) regimen or
directly with a cortical stimulator. These lack thereof. If a patient was just re-initiated on
electrodes are placed via a craniotomy and/or full-dosage AEDs (e.g., after being off AEDs for
burr hole performed by the neurosurgeon. Several several days) an hour before mapping,
companies currently manufacture electrodes, and significantly poorer performance on cognitive
each company has a variety of electrode arrays. tasks may be seen due to medication side effects
These arrays can range from two contact strips up such as sedation or nausea. As a result, an effort
to 64 contact grids and beyond. In general, the should be made to limit AED administration
electrodes are small discs of either stainless steel (particularly of sedating AEDs) immediately
or platinum. The surgeon places these electrodes prior to mapping; if AEDs need to be loaded
subdurally, directly on the surface of the pia. intravenously or orally prior to mapping, it is best
Depth electrodes are thin electrodes that are to allow for at least an hour or more for the patient
placed, as the name implies, directly into the to return to baseline following the loading doses.
cortex and underlying white matter. These are Although the approach to AED adjustments
often utilized to sample mesial structures (e.g., can vary from physician to physician, in general,
cingulate cortex or hippocampus) which are not video-EEG monitoring focuses on capturing the
accessible with subdurally placed strips or grids. beginning of the seizure on EEG (i.e., capturing
In some cases, technical issues such as dural and characterizing the ictal onset zone) to
adhesions will limit or prohibit subdural determine the region(s) to be resected. In order to
placement of the electrode arrays. In these record seizures, AED dosages may be decreased
situations, surgeons will either forego placement or discontinued. Once an adequate number of
of electrodes in that region, utilize depth seizures have been captured to determine the ictal
electrodes, or place the electrodes epidurally. onset zone and the resection plan, AEDs are
Stimulation of electrodes placed epidurally will restarted. At our center, mapping is typically
produce significant pain and as such should be done after adequate ictal data are available and
avoided. Once all electrodes are implanted, the AEDs are restarted; some centers choose to
wires will be tunneled and the surgeon will close perform functional mapping prior to the initial
the craniotomy site. The head is wrapped securely AED reduction or in multiple sessions throughout
and the connections from the intracranially the recording period. The advantage to mapping
placed electrodes are visible outside the head after the ictal data are collected is that the surgical
142 C. Morrison and C.E. Carlson

resection is largely planned, and thus mapping Baseline Language Testing: Once the patient
can be targeted to the area(s) in question; if is on the AED regimen deemed appropriate for
mapping is done prior to capturing ictal data, a mapping by the epilepsy team, baseline testing
broader mapping effort is often required to should be performed. Baseline language testing
prepare for different potential ictal onset zones. serves as a “dress rehearsal” for the patient and
The disadvantage to mapping after all the ictal for the team independent of the potential
data are collected is that it often leads to very confounding influence of ADs, seizures, and
limited windows of time in which to perform the cortical stimulation. It allows patients to become
mapping. This limited time window is of greatest familiar with the procedure and helps them to be
importance in patients who have technically more relaxed and comfortable with the process.
difficult mapping procedures (i.e., frequent Given the frequency with which these individuals
afterdischarges or seizures) in which obtaining have had prior experience with neuropsychological
adequate data prior to the resection becomes testing (which often includes 5–6 h of challenging
difficult or impossible. testing), they may come to the procedure with
Afterdischarges (ADs) are, as their name concerns and anxieties about their ability to meet
implies, epileptiform discharges that occur in the challenges of the tasks in this situation. A
response to direct stimulation of the cortex; these second and equally important reason for
can range from a single discharge that spontane- performing the pre-mapping baseline testing
ously resolves to a train of discharges which relates to the need to establish the patient’s post-
evolve into a clinical seizure. As with AEDs, implant level of performance as this may not be
ADs and/or stimulation-induced seizures can the same as that obtained during the presurgical
impact the baseline cognitive performance of the neuropsychological evaluation. Finally, thorough
patient. The need to stimulate regions of the cor- baseline testing using all of the planned stimuli
tex with increased baseline excitability (i.e., the also allows the team to gauge the patient’s current
area of ictal onset or tissue overlying lesions) performance level throughout the entire
necessitates both appropriate prophylaxis with procedure; many patients will develop significant
AEDs prior to stimulation and vigilance on the fatigue as the mapping progresses, and baseline
part of the team to monitor for epileptiform activ- performance may worsen significantly.
ity. At our center, mapping is done with an EEG Ultimately, the more clearly delineated baseline
system at the bedside so that ADs can be identi- performance is, the easier it will to detect a devia-
fied and, when necessary, disrupted. For elec- tion from this level of functioning to identify
trode sites with extremely frequent ADs, testing “hits” for language disruption.
may not be possible as the effects of stimulation Patient Education: We have found that
versus the effects of the ADs may be difficult or thoroughly educating the patient in what to
impossible to separate. At our center, if either expect and how they contribute to the mapping
clinical seizures or very frequent ADs are seen, process (beyond responding to stimulus cues and
additional AEDs are given to facilitate reliable instructions) is very effective in reducing anxiety
testing. If clinical seizures are triggered at a site, and adverse reactions that can lead to reduced
typically an effort is made to avoid further stimu- cooperation and degradations of responses. For
lation at that location; in general, testing is per- example, the triggering of a seizure can be
formed at sites with ADs that do not disrupt distressing to patients and is a risk of the
performance of the task. At our center, with fre- procedure that must be discussed prior to
quent ADs, “baseline” task performance is reas- stimulation to minimize any distress this may
sessed while ADs are active. If ADs disrupt cause. Stimulation-induced motor responses, if
performance of the task, the response to stimula- unexpected, can be also distressing to patients.
tion at that electrode pair cannot be fully evalu- Similarly, the possibility of ipsilateral facial
ated if ADs persist as ADs may involve additional involvement or dural spread (resulting in physical
cortical regions beyond the stimulated pair. discomfort) should be addressed prior to
6 Electrocortical Mapping of Language 143

stimulation. Once the procedure has been very low levels (e.g., 1–5 mA) and gradually
thoroughly explained, the patient is adequately increased over brief 2–3 s stimulation durations
protected from potential seizures with AEDs, and until the target amperage is reached (at least
a baseline level of performance has been 12 mA, preferably 15 mA) (Lesser et al., 1987).
established, it is time to begin the actual cortical While stimulation at lower levels may elicit a
stimulation. motor response or sensory experience, stimula-
tion below 11 mA in our experience may not be
sufficient to reliably disrupt language functions.
Stimulating the Brain To efficiently utilize the “titration” phase of stim-
ulation, which allows for assessment of AD
Although some variations will be present for thresholds and minimizes the likelihood of sig-
the exact stimulation parameters, cortical stim- nificant pain, we ask the patient to count out loud
ulation is predicated on delivering short trains so that speech or motor disruptions at low levels
of high-frequency electrical stimulation to focal of stimulation can be identified. Once the safety
regions of the cortex. At our center, we stimu- and tolerability of stimulation at the target cur-
late with 50 Hz trains. High-frequency stimula- rent is established, stimulation during the lan-
tion temporarily disrupts functioning in local guage tasks is typically executed for 3 s (multiple
populations of neurons, thereby creating a short durations of stimulation during expressive
“functional” lesion while the stimulation is prose) to 7 s (during comprehension items).
applied. When stimulation is removed, neuro- Stimulation is performed with an electrode
nal functioning returns to normal. It is this pair; adjacent electrodes are selected and isolated
property that is utilized to identify eloquent for stimulation either directly (e.g., by removing
cortex. For example, when a threshold level of them from the recording system and placing them
stimulation is applied to a region involved in in the stimulator) or electronically (e.g., via a
language output while the patient is producing selection interface and hardware systems for iso-
continuous speech (e.g., reciting a phrase or lating electrodes provided by some EEG manu-
poem), speech arrest may be observed. In this facturers). For “screening,” testing is typically
manner “critical areas” (i.e., areas that, when done at electrodes 1–2, and then at 3–4, and then
disrupted, will disrupt function) for language 5–6, and so forth. Testing electrodes two at a time
functioning can be identified. can expedite the process of identifying language
Pain and discomfort are a concern for patients silent brain regions. If further spatial accuracy is
prior to beginning the mapping procedure. necessary, an electrode pair that is associated
Because the brain has no nociceptive receptors, with language disruption can be “split” to deter-
stimulation of the cortex is not painful. When the mine if one or both of the electrodes overlay elo-
grid is in close proximity to the seventh cranial quent cortex. In the example above testing could
nerve and there is sufficient current spread, the then be done at 1–2, and then 2–3, and then 3–4.
patient may experience ipsilateral sensation/pain In addition, testing of an electrode of interest
or face motor contraction. These phenomena will with an electrode that is vertically related (e.g., in
generally be noticeable at low current levels. a typical 8 × 8 contact grid, 1–9) can allow even
Unlike the cortex, the dura mater and vascular greater spatial information. Additional spatial
structures do have nociceptors, and stimulation information may also be possible with the use of
that reaches these structures can produce smaller electrode arrays (e.g., with smaller elec-
discomfort or overt pain depending on the trode diameters and ½ centimeter or smaller
location, structure(s) involved, and intensity of interelectrode distances). The team must be
stimulation. Dural spread is most frequent near aware of the technical limitations of their cortical
the edges of a grid or on smaller strips (e.g., stimulator system and the testing it has under-
single row of four electrodes). For these reasons, gone; many systems have not had safety testing
the stimulation amperage should be started at for these newer small electrode arrays.
144 C. Morrison and C.E. Carlson

Fig. 6.1 Schematic representation of the electrode tive placement anatomically. Colored electrodes represent
implantation. Depth electrodes are designated on the the ictal onset zone (red) or additional regions of cortical
schematic with a color-coded star to illustrate their rela- hyperexcitability (green)

When considering a strategy for moving are now superior to the sylvian fissure) is the
through the grid of electrodes, if there is a most expeditious approach and minimizes fatigue
neocortical (as opposed to hippocampal) ictal for the patient.
onset zone, it is generally prudent to start the When performing the mapping procedure, it is
mapping process at electrodes that are distal from helpful to work from some type of graphic repre-
those determined to overly ictal foci and then sentation. Some integrated electrode switching
proceed through the grid in a systematic fashion systems will have a schematic representation
(see case example below). The approach for built directly into the switching software. Prior to
moving through the grid will also be guided by utilizing this integrated system, we utilized a
known functional neuroanatomy. For example, schematic representation of the implanted elec-
when the grid is placed over frontotemporal trodes across the hemisphere with color designa-
regions in an individual with temporal lobe tions of the ictal and interictal findings during
seizure onset, language mapping the posterior monitoring (see Fig. 6.1). This schematic reflects
superior portion of the grid, which may overly the surgeon’s approximation of where the grid is
post-central gyrus, may not be necessary. In this located; it is not intended to represent an exact
setting, starting inferiorly and working up until scale representation of the electrode locations.
motor and/or somatosensory responses occur For more precise anatomical representations,
with stimulation (indicating that the electrodes post-implant neuroimaging (e.g., CT or MRI
6 Electrocortical Mapping of Language 145

scans), with or without intraoperative photos, can It is important to avoid exclusively using yes/
be reviewed and even reconstructed to provide no questions, to limit the potentially misleading
very detailed and accurate images of the elec- effects of patient guessing. It is not uncommon
trode localizations in relation to cerebral struc- for patients to try to be overly compliant with this
tures. Even when working from precise figural “test.” They are, with good reason, highly
representations of the electrodes on the brain, a invested in the process and often try their best to
thorough mapping procedure, which identifies “answer correctly” to, from their perspective,
not only language but also at least one motor/sen- facilitate the process. As a result, they may try to
sory response, is necessary to produce a more guess at answers to “get it right and be a good
exact interpretation of the electrode localizations patient.” Even if yes/no response formats are
in relation to specific landmarks of eloquent cor- avoided, a patient may try to “guess” the
tex (e.g., primary motor or somatosensory areas). appropriate response from the pool of eight to ten
Identifying the location of these landmarks is not possibilities that they have become very familiar
necessarily as straightforward as might be with over the course of the procedure. This can
expected as patients with long-standing cerebral be avoided by counseling the patient in advance
abnormalities may have developed atypical orga- that they should not guess as this will only
nization of functions. This makes identification confound the results.
of key anatomical landmarks particularly impor- Timing of item presentation and response
tant when efforts are being made to reduce post- must be precisely coordinated. Specifically, items
operative functional deficits. and responses must be provided during the
stimulation epoch. For example, the clinician
should not begin providing an auditory
Language Testing comprehension item prior to stimulation onset,
and the response must be given before stimulation
General Principles is discontinued for the site to be considered
“cleared” for that language function. If the patient
There are many types of language tasks utilized responds after discontinuation of the stimulation
in mapping procedures. Before discussing spe- or not at all, then, provided other causes for
cific tasks, some general principles of the lan- response failure are ruled out (e.g., ADs or motor
guage mapping procedure are presented. In responses), the site is considered a “hit” for that
terms of item selection, regardless of the modal- function. Thus, the clinician who is controlling
ity being tested, the stimulus items need to be the stimulation and the clinician who is
chosen such that the patient can respond quickly performing the behavioral testing need to be
and without any hesitation. For each patient, working together to ensure a reliable and accurate
several items may need to be screened, and dif- mapping result.
ficult stimuli for that patient should be elimi- The necessary inter-trial interval will vary
nated. Therefore, for every modality assessed from patient to patient. While Devinsky, Perrine,
during the procedure, clinicians will need a Llinas, Luciano, and Dogali (1993) recommend
somewhat large pool of items as each patient 20 s between trials, some patients will be able to
may not find them all equally easy. Typically a proceed at a faster pace without any decrease in
set of eight to ten items for each modality is their language quality or response reliability.
utilized in pseudorandom rotation during the However, patients with particularly irritable
procedure. Practice effects are not an issue cortex resulting in frequent ADs (and perhaps
when eloquent language cortex is temporarily seizures), those who are particularly fatiguable,
rendered nonfunctional via electrical stimula- individuals who are lower functioning cognitively,
tion; therefore, it is not problematic to use the or those who are evidencing a great many “hits”
same set of stimuli repeatedly throughout the during the mapping process may require longer
procedure. inter-trial intervals to avoid overtaxing their
146 C. Morrison and C.E. Carlson

ability to respond. This balance between patient’s error rate remains constant throughout
expeditious completion of testing and avoiding the procedure (which we have found is not always
difficulties from repeated, frequent stimulation the case) and necessitates numerous trials to
requires ongoing assessment of both the clinical establish the percentage. Another method
performance and electrographic responses. involves clearing a site after establishing that a
The criteria for a pass (an area of cortex is high proportion of stimulation trials are responded
“cleared,” i.e., it does not subserve that language to accurately at a given site (Hamberger,
function) or fail (i.e., a “hit”) vary across surgical Goodman, Perrine, & Tamny, 2001); however,
centers. We utilize and recommend a procedure again this necessitates numerous trials for each
wherein any missed items clearly reflect function at each electrode pair tested. This
pathognomonic performance (i.e., a “hit”) as all approach will prolong the procedure and fatigue
items utilized were answered correctly with the patient.
100 % accuracy at baseline testing. If the patient There may be several points during the map-
reliably misses a second item of the same type ping where an item is “failed.” At these times, it
(e.g., two pictures are not named during separate is important to ascertain that the item is truly
stimulation trials), then that site is considered to failed and not spoiled by other factors. In addi-
be eloquent for that function. It is recommended tion, important clinical information can be
that at least two trials of each task at an electrode gleaned through secondary assessment following
pair are answered correctly to “clear” a cortical the “failed” stimulation trial. If, for example, on a
site. This serves to clarify the reliability of the repetition, naming, or comprehension item, a
response. If stimulation is applied adjacent to a response is not provided during stimulation, after
language zone, then testing may result in discontinuing stimulation ask the patient to repeat
intermittent correct and incorrect responses. Such the item or name the object that was presented. It
a phenomenon might be classified as a partial hit is often enlightening if the patient can then
and indicate that the underlying brain region may comply with repeating the phrase/question or
subserve language functioning but that a naming the stimulus item (i.e., naming the
postoperative language deficit might not occur if pictured object that may still be in visual working
it were removed. memory) that was presented during stimulation,
If the patient appeared to lose focus or the as this helps to validate the language “hit.”
results were unclear from the initial stimulus When mapping in the frontal lobe near primary
trials, additional items can be administered. Prior or secondary motor areas, it must be determined
to continuing with stimulation testing, baseline that an incorrect or distorted response is not due
testing (i.e., testing without stimulation) should to motor phenomena rather than language
be performed again. Thereafter, presentation disruption. Speech may sound dysarthric or the
trials should proceed with intermittent stimulation patient may report that they “felt something” in
during some of the item trials to look for their mouth or throat area during stimulation.
disruption of a correct response pattern. We will This can reflect motor contractions as a result of
often utilize this intermittent stimulation stimulation of the mouth/face area of the motor
paradigm in a blinded fashion wherein the strip. Have the patient produce sustained
neuropsychologist is not informed at the time of nonlinguistic speech sounds that utilize different
presentation as to whether stimulation is being levels of the speech apparatus (e.g., “kakakakaka”
performed. This allows for a more objective or “lalalalala”) while delivering intermittent
assessment of performance and its relationship stimulation for durations of 1–2 s each. If
(or lack thereof) to stimulation. production of these sounds is disrupted with
An alternate method for establishing pass/fail stimulation, then a motor, rather than a language,
criteria uses a specified percentage above the area has been identified. When stimulating
baseline error rate as a criterion (Ojemann, electrodes that are directly overlying the motor
1983b). However, this method assumes that the strip or are somewhat more anterior and superior,
6 Electrocortical Mapping of Language 147

more obvious motor phenomena are evident (e.g., ability to reliably respond correctly. In this
contralateral facial contraction, forced tongue situation, try taking a 10–15 min break.
deviation or retraction). If it is suspected that Depending on the health, endurance, and
tongue motor involvement is disrupting the tolerance of the patient, as well as how irritable
language response, have the patient extend their (hyperexcitable) the cortex within the region of
tongue and look for forced tongue retraction/ stimulation is, the mapping procedure may need
deviation during stimulation. Occasionally there to be discontinued and resumed on another day to
are very subtle motor effects (due to current achieve maximum reliability and quality of
spread) which can be “talked through.” That is, responses. To the greatest extent possible, include
the patient may be able to tolerate the motor this potential need for additional hours (or days)
phenomenon and try to produce the language in the pre-resection surgery planning.
response despite the stimulation effects on oral
motor functioning. It is important to try this when
possible as there can be interdigitation between Language Testing: Specific Tasks
motor and language areas (Lesser, Gordon, &
Uematsu, 1994). Attempting language testing at Expressive Speech
these “soft” motor sites may help to exonerate
language cortex in a planned resection zone. Stimulation during continuous speech has been
With auditory comprehension, it is important shown to be a sensitive method for identifying
to be mindful of how a patient’s interpretation of language eloquent cortex (Lesser et al., 1987).
their experience can shape their responses. It is However, selection of the stimulus material is
not uncommon for patients to report that they did important. The content of the speech monologue
not “hear” the stimulus item that was presented must not contain prosody (such as having the
(e.g., during an auditory naming or sentence patient sing a song) as this may engage language
completion item). In this circumstance, first centers in the contralateral hemisphere. Counting
determine if in fact there is a primary perceptual should also not be used for this purpose as
problem. Often the bandaging around the head knowledge of numbers can be so overlearned that
can cover the ear and reduce auditory acuity. the patient may be able to produce accurate
However, when there is a comprehension deficit responses even when stimulation of that site
due to stimulation, it may be reported by the would ordinarily disrupt other more complex
patient as a “hearing” problem as this may be types of verbal output (Bookheimer, Zeffiro,
how the patient interprets their inability to Blaxton, Gaillard, & Theodore, 2000). Instead, a
understand the command or sentence. monologue that is syntactically more complex
There can be reasons for item failure that are but is well known to the patient and can be
independent of the language and motor effects of repeatedly produced accurately should be used.
stimulation. Afterdischarges can disrupt cognitive For individuals who are born and educated in the
functioning (Lesser et al., 1987). Therefore, United States we often use the Pledge of
when language disruption is noted during Allegiance for this purpose. Other types of
stimulation, it is critical that the EEG be monologues that can be used include prayers,
monitored to determine whether the impaired poems, or lines from a play that are very well
response occurred in the context of ADs or known to the patient. These types of passages are
whether the EEG immediately post stimulation selected because they are rote for the patient but
was “clean.” If there were ADs, then the trial not resilient to disruption caused by stimulation,
must be repeated until it is determined that the such as can be the case with reciting numbers. If
patient can/cannot complete the task in the no well-known fixed monologue can be identified
absence of epileptiform activity. Finally, the for a particular patient, then, as a last resort, the
cumulative effects of ADs, small seizures, and/or patient can be instructed to speak on a topic of
fatigue can progressively decrease the patient’s their choice. This latter choice is somewhat risky
148 C. Morrison and C.E. Carlson

as it is more difficult for the examiners to identify “what do we tell time with?” At our center, we
errors and paraphasias since the words and use the stimuli developed by the Columbia group
sentences are not provided in a predetermined for this purpose (Hamberger & Tamny, 1999).
and fixed manner. As an alternative, patients can In terms of verbal generative fluency, tradi-
be asked to recite the months of the year or days tional verbal fluency tasks (i.e., continuous pro-
of the week in a continuous and repeating manner. ductions for words belonging to a specific
We tend to use this type of task with lower category, such as those that start with a specific
functioning individuals. letter) are not widely used in mapping procedures.
Visual confrontation naming has been used as They can be problematic for the same reason that,
one of the “gold standards” for identifying for example, continuous extemporaneous speech
language eloquent cortex during mapping since can be. Specifically, if the patient naturally
the time the procedure was developed. This type hesitates in their speech or in their thought
of task has been one of the most sensitive in process during completion of the task, it could be
identifying language eloquent cortex (Ojemann, confused as a “hit,” rather a natural pause.
1983a). Contrary to the clinical neuropsychology Ojemann and colleagues have used a somewhat
setting where tasks with graduating difficulty are related single-word generation task (Ojemann,
often used (e.g., the Boston Naming Test), in the Ojemann, & Lettich, 2002). Borrowing from the
mapping setting, pictures of items that are easily, functional imaging literature, they employed a
rapidly, and consistently named by the individual verb generation task. In this task, patients are
should be used. As indicated, the clinician should shown a picture of a concrete noun and asked to
have a large selection of picture stimuli available generate a verb appropriate to the picture
and then determine at baseline a patient-specific stimulus. Consistent with the robust findings
set of approximately ten items that the patient can from functional imaging studies, Ojemann et al.
respond to easily at the time of mapping. We have (2002) were able show both overlapping and
found the stimuli published by Snodgrass and disparate verb generation sites relative to those
Vanderwart (1980) to be extremely useful for this showing disruption of object naming in the
purpose. frontal and temporoparietal cortices.
Although visual naming has been the most Select types of aphasia can be differentiated
common format for assessing word retrieval in based on whether repetition functions are intact
the context of brain mapping, Hamberger and or not. Repetition tasks assess the integrity of
others (Hamberger et al., 2001; Lesser et al., connections between phonological input and
1987) have demonstrated that auditory naming output. Tasks that can be included in a mapping
(also known as descriptive or responsive naming) procedure may involve presenting a word or short
tasks can yield important and unique information. phrase and having the patient repeat it during a
Specifically, it has been established that auditory stimulation trial. It is important to keep in mind
and visual confrontation naming can be that the phrase/sentence length should not be too
co-localized; however, in many instances these long as this will prolong the stimulation duration
functions can also be distributed across distinct unnecessarily, perhaps leading to ADs or seizures.
sites in the brain. It could be argued that auditory
naming (i.e., retrieving a specific word while you
are formulating what you want to say) can be Comprehension
considered more ecologically valid than visual
naming, or at least as critical as naming an object Assessment of comprehension is a critical part of
in one’s visual field. Thus, it is an important the language mapping process, particularly when
language function to assess during the mapping stimulating in the posterior regions of the
procedure. Examples of auditory naming items temporal lobe or the temporal-parietal juncture.
include “what is the thing that unlocks a door” or There are a range of methods for assessing
6 Electrocortical Mapping of Language 149

auditory comprehension, such as token tasks, Writing


asking the patient to follow simple commands
(e.g., point to your nose, point to the door), or Although writing can be assessed during a
having the patient provide the final word in a mapping procedure (Lesser et al., 1984), this type
sentence stem. An example of the latter type of of task can be difficult due to the time constraints
task includes “The bicycle had a flat ____.” It of completing an item during a stimulation trial.
should be noted, however, that the sentence stem Single words may be produced to command or
task also has a naming component and therefore with presentation of a stimulus item (e.g.,
is likely assessing multiple language functions picture). However, this type of task requires
simultaneously. Nevertheless, we have often seen multiple stages of language processing which are
dissociations between auditory and visual naming not separable using this method. Some success
performances and the ability to respond with identifying independent cortex eloquent for
accurately to sentence stems. Once again, the writing when using a writing-to-dictation
commands and sentence stems should not be paradigm has been seen (Lubrano, Roux, &
overly long as the item and response must be Demonet, 2004).
provided during a single stimulation trial.

Mapping in Special Populations


Reading
The procedures described thus far apply to the
Having the patient read single phonologically routine monolingual adult patient. Naturally the
regular words or passages has been found to be a clinician will encounter situations where the
sensitive task during language mapping for standard procedures require modification. When
detecting areas specific to this function (Devinsky working with bilingual or multilingual
et al., 1993; Lesser, Lueders, Dinner, Hahn, & individuals, additional decisions must be made
Cohen, 1984). This procedure can be executed by regarding which language(s) to map. In those
having the patient attempt to read single words individuals who function in two or more
on a card during stimulation. Another method for languages in their daily life, it is important to
executing this task is to have the patient read map all the languages that the patient relies on for
from a book or magazine of their choosing while either work or interacting with family and friends.
the clinician periodically introduces stimulation. Mapping each language relevant to the patient’s
A method for assessing reading comprehension adaptive functioning is critical as the dominant
involves having the patient read sentences and and secondary languages can be co-localized in
complete the missing last word (Devinsky et al., some brain regions and differentially distributed
1993). However, it is important to note that this in others (Lucas, Mckhann, & Ojemann, 2004).
function actually requires two separate stages of In situations where the patient is multilingual but
written language processing, the ability to read they do not rely on their secondary language(s)
and the ability to comprehend, which are for work or home interactions, we have not
separable language processes. If the patient mapped the additional language. Finally, we do
passes these latter types of items, then that area of not typically map a secondary language in which
underlying cortex is cleared for both reading the patient is not fluent.
ability and reading comprehension. However, if Pediatric patients can prove to be a special
the patient “fails” these written sentence challenge and, as might be expected, working
completion items, then a separate reading task with children requires some adjustment to the
(without a comprehension component) can be procedure. For example, the stimulation
administered to determine if the skills are parameters might need to be adjusted. Such
dissociated at that site. modifications can include lengthening stimulation
150 C. Morrison and C.E. Carlson

durations (Lesser et al., 1994) to give a longer testing found average general intellectual func-
time for responses. Depending on the age of the tioning (WAIS-III GAI = 93) with comparable
child, cooperation can be an issue. To assess verbal and performance abilities. Verbal abstract
comprehension, for example, several of the reasoning, self-monitoring, and rapid behavioral
child’s toys can be lined up in an array, and flexibility were subtle areas of weakness. The
commands such as pick up/touch/point to a patient’s approach to the testing process reflected
certain toy can be given during stimulation. The difficulty with staying focused on tasks that she
child may also be asked a series of question to perceived as overwhelming. At other times, she
which he/she is very familiar with the answer tended to sacrifice speed for accuracy, which
(e.g., “What color is Barney?”). With regard to adversely affected some scores. New learning
expressive speech, identifying and/or encouraging deficits were observed when she was presented
a child to repeatedly generate prose could prove with large amounts of material at once. Long-
difficult or impossible in some cases. At these term retention and retrieval of newly learned
times, having the child say a nursery rhyme or information were often more preserved when
perhaps just generating speech via telling a story information was presented in a piecemeal fash-
may be the only options. For example, one could ion, over multiple learning trials, and/or in dual
ask the child to tell the story of Red Riding Hood modalities. This pattern of scores, with somewhat
and periodically deliver stimulation during the worse performance on verbal memory tasks, was
child’s discourse to observe if there is speech interpreted as suggesting relative left frontotem-
disruption. When attempting a mapping poral dysfunction.
procedure in a young patient, it should be kept in The patient underwent implantation of a left
mind that language mapping results in children frontotemporal grid and depth electrodes. On the
under the ages of 10–12 have been found to be schematic representation shown in Fig. 6.1, the
less reliable; the exact etiology for this finding ictal onset zone for her typical clinical seizure is
remains unclear (Schevon et al., 2007). shown in red, and regions of cortical hyperexcit-
ability beyond the ictal onset zone (i.e., addi-
tional regions demonstrating epileptiform sharp
Case Example waves and/or spikes) are shown in green. Seizures
were recorded from the left anterior temporal
There are many options for tasks and procedures lobe arising on the distal and, to a lesser extent,
(outlined above) when mapping eloquent lan- proximal contacts of the depth electrodes.
guage cortex. The following is a description of a In preparation for the mapping procedure, a
typical mapping procedure as executed at our post-implant baseline was established. The
center. The patient is a 54-year-old right-handed patient is a monolingual English speaker who
high-school educated woman with a 22-year his- was easily able to recite The Pledge of Allegiance
tory of treatment-resistant focal epilepsy. Seizure with only two practice trials. A set of ten pictures,
semiology and video/EEG monitoring indicated eight auditory naming stimuli, and eight sentence
a left temporal seizure focus for some of her completion items that she could answer easily
recorded events; some semiologically similar were identified from the Hamberger and Seidel
events were surface negative on scalp video/ (2003) stimuli. As is typical of our protocol,
EEG. An MRI of the brain showed mild increased mapping began with the anterior inferior
T2 signal in the left temporal lobe cortex, amyg- electrode pair, and we proceeded posteriorly
dale, and hippocampal formation. The patient across the rows. Figure 6.2 shows a schematic
underwent an IAP which revealed left hemi- representation of the functional mapping results.
sphere dominance for language. Memory func- Multiple sites with disruption of language
tioning was similar and relatively preserved function were identified including regions with
bilaterally (left = 9/12; right = 8/12) by the stan- impairment of spontaneous speech, visual
dards used at our center. Neuropsychological naming, auditory naming, and comprehension.
6 Electrocortical Mapping of Language 151

Fig. 6.2 Schematic representation of cortical mapping results. Each response elicited is designated by the color. Sites
for which multiple findings were obtained are designated with bars representing each individual finding

As is typical of our testing protocol, a motor epileptologists and neuropsychologists improves


response (tongue) was identified; this serves as both the accuracy of the test results (with multiple
confirmation of electrode locations superior to observers for correlating behavior and EEG find-
the Sylvian fissure. In this patient, resection was ings) and efficiency (with the epileptologist pri-
planned for the anterior and mesial and left tem- marily responsible for setting up and executing
poral lobe; as a result, no mapping beyond the individual stimulation trials and correlating EEG
identified motor responses was required. activity and the neuropsychologist responsible for
While some clinicians may choose to conduct assessing baseline performance and presenting
the mapping procedure alone (i.e., a single person test stimuli). In addition, the overlapping, but
operates the stimulator, performs all the behavioral unique skill sets and knowledge domains of the
testing, observes verbal and nonverbal responses two specializations have improved assessment of
from the patient, monitors the EEG, records all the responses and problem solving in cases with
necessary information, and changes the elec- unique responses or potential technical concerns;
trodes), we have found that a multidisciplinary continued testing is performed until a consensus is
approach with a team that includes neurologists/ reached for each electrode pair.
152 C. Morrison and C.E. Carlson

now a third person to coordinate with in terms of


Intraoperative Mapping presenting items and noting responses during
stimulation. The neurosurgeon will be holding
When placement of a subdural grid to record sei- the probe directly on the brain while the neurolo-
zure activity is not necessary, there is no opportu- gist engages and disengages stimulation. The
nity for a bedside mapping procedure. On these third member of the team (often a neuropsychol-
occasions, such as when a patient is undergoing ogist) will present stimuli (auditory or visual)
brain tumor resection, the clinician will be per- and observe responses while coordinating with
forming the mapping procedure intraoperatively the neurologist to ensure that stimuli and
with the surgical team. Intraoperative language responses are given during stimulation.
mapping may also be required if bedside map- The intraoperative environment can present
ping identified language cortex within the barriers to the patient’s ability to cooperate. Not
planned resection zone. In this circumstance, the infrequently patients complain that their throat
surgeon may want to more carefully map the lan- hurts or their mouth/lips are too dry to talk. In
guage regions identified at bedside. This process these circumstances it is helpful to moisten their
serves to verify the bedside language findings. It lips with a wet gauze or to let them suck on a wet
is possible, due to such factors as shifting of the gauze. Nausea, pain, and lethargy can all limit a
grid during surgery, that the identified language patient’s ability to focus on the language testing.
cortex does not exactly correlate with the lan- The anesthesiologist can often provide medication
guage map schematic that was developed from to alleviate these symptoms. Due to the sterile
the bedside procedure. Thus, in an effort to draping around the craniotomy site, part of the
reduce the potential for a postoperative language patient’s visual field may be obstructed, thus
deficit, intraoperative mapping may result in reducing their ability to perceive visually
modifications to the planned surgery. presented stimuli. Sometimes it is possible to
When performing an intraoperative proce- work with the surgical team to make adjustments
dure, the mapping team is generally called into that will reduce this problem; often the
the procedure after the craniotomy is performed neuropsychologist needs to reposition to make
and the patient is waking from general anesthe- him/herself and the stimuli visible to the patient.
sia. This may be a slow process for some patients. Some patients become upset, anxious, or fearful
Once the patient is sufficiently awake, reestab- during the awake portion of the surgical
lishing a baseline will be necessary. For all but procedure. Understandably, this is quite a unique
the highest functioning and stalwart patients, a experience that some individuals find rather
simplification of the language stimuli is likely to disconcerting. These symptoms can interfere
be necessary. For example, often patients are too with or completely derail the mapping procedure
fatigued or are in too much discomfort to focus due to the patient’s inability to remain calm
on The Pledge of Allegiance, therefore, repeated enough to comply with the task demands. We
recitation of the months of the year and days of have found that counseling the patient
the week is often used. While we attempt to presurgically and even presurgical baseline
include both visual and auditory confrontation testing itself helps reduce their anxieties and
naming, there are times when we may omit audi- prepare them for the procedure to come. Finally,
tory naming due to the need to expedite the pro- as with the bedside procedure, lethargy can
cedure in the intraoperative setting. The selection decrease the quality of the patient’s responses;
of visual over auditory naming is made based on however, in the intraoperative setting the
the former identified as the “gold standard”; how- threshold for fatigue is generally much sooner
ever, there has been some evolution in thinking than when testing at the bedside. In this
on this point (Hamberger, 2007). The procedure circumstance brief breaks might be necessary,
in the operating room is somewhat more compli- and the team will want to be mindful not to
cated than that of the bedside mapping as there is “overtest.” If the patient’s ability to respond
6 Electrocortical Mapping of Language 153

degrades, the difficulty of the task might need to


References
be lowered. For example, if the patient was
reciting the Pledge of Allegiance, switching to Bookheimer, S. Y., Zeffiro, T. A., Blaxton, T. A., Gaillard,
months of the year might be helpful. W. D., & Theodore, W. H. (2000). Activation of lan-
Occasionally surgeons may guide the extent of guage cortex with automatic speech tasks. Neurology,
the resection based on ongoing feedback about 55, 1157.
Burchiel, K., Clark, H., & Ojemann, G. A. (1989). Use of
the patient’s language functioning. Thus it might stimulation mapping and corticography in the excision
be necessary to engage the patient in an ongoing of arteriovenous malformations in sensorimotor and
dialogue during the resection to assess quality of language-related neocortex. Neurosurgery, 24(3),
speech output and comprehension. At these times, 323–327.
Devinsky, O., Perrine, K., Hirsch, J., McMullen, W.,
we typically engage the patient in “small talk,” Pacia, S., & Doyle, W. (2000). Relation of cortical lan-
asking them questions about themselves, as auto- guage distribution and cognitive function in surgical
biographical information and/or areas of personal epilepsy patients. Epilepsia, 41, 400–404.
interest are generally easy for people to have a Devinsky, O., Perrine, K., Llinas, R., Luciano, D., &
Dogali, M. (1993). Anterior temporal language areas
discourse on. During this time the neuropsycholo- in patients with early onset of temporal lobe epilepsy.
gist is listening to the quality of the speech output Annals of Neurology, 34, 727–732.
(e.g., if paraphasias or increased word-finding dif- Farrell, D., Burbank, N., Lettich, E., & Ojemann, G. A.
ficulty occurs) and the patient’s ability to respond (2007). Individual variation in human motor-sensory
(rolandic) cortex. Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology,
appropriately to questions or commands (which 24(3), 286–293.
may herald a developing comprehension deficit) Foerster, O. (1936). The motor cortex in man in the light
and notifying the surgeon of any changes from the of Hughling Jackson’s doctrines. Brain, 59, 135–159.
intraoperative baseline. Frequent breaks are key Hamberger, M. J. (2007). Cortical language mapping in
epilepsy: A critical review. Neuropsychology Review,
in this process as the patient will generally become 17, 477–489.
more fatigued and less able to focus and respond Hamberger, M. J., Goodman, R. R., Perrine, K., & Tamny,
as the procedure goes on. T. (2001). Anatomic dissociation of auditory and
visual naming in the lateral temporal cortex.
Neurology, 56, 56–61.
Hamberger, M. J., & Seidel, W. T. (2003). Auditory and
visual naming tests: Normative and patient data for
Summary accuracy, response time, and tip-of-the-tongue.
Journal of the International Neuropsychological
In the overview above we have provided a brief Society, 9, 479–489.
Hamberger, M., & Tamny, T. (1999). Auditory naming
history of electrocortical language mapping in and temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy Research, 35,
the context of a planned surgical resection, as 229–243.
well as a summary of many of the specific lan- Lesser, R., Gordon, B., & Uematsu, S. (1994). Electrical
guage tasks used for this process. In addition, we stimulation and language. Journal of Clinical
Neurophysiology, 11, 191–204.
highlight a number of potential pit falls, compli- Lesser, R. P., Hahn, J., Lueders, H., Rothner, A., &
cations, and logistical challenges that can occur Erenberg, G. (1981). The use of chronic subdural elec-
during this procedure. Finally, using a case exam- trodes for cortical mapping of speech. Epilepsia, 22,
ple, we illustrate how we perform a language 240.
Lesser, R., Luders, H. O., Klem, G., Dinner, D. S., Morris,
mapping procedure at our center. While there is H. H., Hahn, J. F., et al. (1987). Extraoperative cortical
certainly variance in how this procedure is per- functional localization in patients with epilepsy.
formed across surgical centers, we provided spe- Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology, 4, 27–53.
cific pros and cons for many of the standard Lesser, R. P., Lueders, H., Dinner, D., Hahn, J., & Cohen,
D. (1984). The location of speech and writing func-
techniques that are widely used. What we have tions in the frontal language area. Brain, 107,
found most useful in performing these proce- 275–291.
dures is an integrated multidisciplinary approach Lubrano, V., Roux, F., & Demonet, J. (2004). Writing-
that takes advantage of the skill set and special- specific sites in frontal areas: A cortical stimulation
study. Journal of Neurosurgery, 101, 787–798.
ized knowledge that neurologists, neurosurgeons, Lucas, T., II, Mckhann, G., II, & Ojemann, G. A. (2004).
and neuropsychologists bring to the table. Functional separation of languages in the bilingual
154 C. Morrison and C.E. Carlson

brain: A comparison of electrical stimulation language Ojemann, G. A., Ojemann, J., Lettich, E., & Berger, M.
mapping in 25 bilingual and 117 monolingual control (1989). Cortical language localization in the left, dom-
patients. Journal of Neurosurgery, 101, 449–457. inant hemisphere. Journal of Neurosurgery, 71,
Luders, H., Lesser, R. P., Dinner, D. S., Morris, H., Wyllie, 316–326.
E., & Godoy, J. (1988). Localization of cortical func- Ojemann, J., Ojemann, G. A., & Lettich, E. (2002).
tion: New information from extraoperative monitoring Cortical stimulation mapping of language cortex by
of patients with epilepsy. Epilepsia, 29, S56–S65. using a verb generation task: Effects of learning and
Lueders, H., Hahn, J., Lesser, R. P., Dinner, D. S., Rothner, comparison to mapping based on object naming.
D., & Erenberg, G. (1982). Localization of epilepto- Journal of Neurosurgery, 97, 33–38.
genic spike foci: Comparative study of closely spaced Penfield, W., & Roberts, L. (1959). Speech and brain-
scalp electrodes, nasopharyngeal, sphenoidal, subdu- mechanisms. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University
ral, and depth electrodes. In H. Akimoti, Press.
H. Kazamatsuri, M. Seino, & A. Ward (Eds.), Rassmussen, T., & Milner, B. (1977). The role of early
Advances in epileptology. XIII Epilepsy International left-brain injury in determining lateralization of cere-
Symposium (pp. 185–189). New York, NY: Raven. bral speech functions. Annals of the New York
Ojemann, G. A. (1979). Individual variability in cortical Academy of Sciences, 299, 355–369.
localization of language. Journal of Neurosurgery, 50, Schevon, C., Carlson, C., Zaroff, C., Weiner, H., Doyle,
164–169. W., Miles, D., et al. (2007). Pediatric language map-
Ojemann, G. A. (1983a). Brain organization for language ping: Sensitivity of neurostimulation and Wada testing
from the perspective of electrical stimulation map- in epilepsy surgery. Epilepsia, 48, 539–545.
ping. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 6, 190–206. Snodgrass, J. G., & Vanderwart, M. (1980). A standard-
Ojemann, G. A. (1983b). Electrical stimulation and the ized set of 260 pictures: Norms for name agreement,
neurobiology of language. The Behavioral and Brain image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity.
Sciences, 2, 221–230. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 6, 174–215.
Structural and Quantitative MRI
in Epilepsy 7
Karen Blackmon and Thomas Thesen

more complete resection of abnormal tissue, and


Introduction improved surgical outcome (Sisodiya et al., 1997).
The most reliable predictor of seizure freedom after
Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) is a critical surgery for cortical malformations is the extent to
component of the basic clinical assessment of which the lesion is resected with incomplete resec-
epilepsy. Detection of abnormalities on MRI is tion decreasing the chance of seizure freedom from
essential to establishing an initial diagnosis 77 to 20 % (Lerner et al., 2009). Therefore, imag-
(idiopathic, cryptogenic, or symptomatic) and ing methods that increase sensitivity and accuracy
determining options for treatment. MRI studies in delineating the presence and extent of lesions are
are especially critical in focal epilepsy, as a valuable addition to preoperative evaluations. An
resective surgery of an epileptogenic lesion has MRI study is also important in determining the
the potential to render many patients seizure-free extent of healthy tissue, which can inform predic-
(Engel, 1996; Wiebe, Blume, Girvin, & Eliasziw, tions of postsurgical functional status.
2001). Improvements in high-resolution MRI and The following chapter will review MRI meth-
quantitative post-processing methods now permit ods and applications that are commonly used in
detection of structural abnormalities in patients comprehensive epilepsy centers, with a particular
that are not otherwise visually apparent. This is emphasis on the role of structural MRI in presur-
especially important as an MRI-identified lesion gical evaluations. Although there is evidence to
is associated both with poorer response to suggest that there may be structural abnormali-
medications (Cardoso et al., 2006; Spooner, ties associated with idiopathic generalized epi-
Berkovic, Mitchell, Wrennall, & Harvey, 2006) lepsy (Betting et al., 2006; Woermann, Sisodiya,
and with a greater likelihood of seizure freedom Free, & Duncan, 1998), research in this area is
following resective surgery (Berkovic et al., limited with generally nonspecific findings and
1995; Jeha et al., 2007). no current clinical application. Therefore, the
Accurate and sensitive presurgical identification focus of this chapter will be limited to structural
of abnormal tissue allows for more targeted place- MRI methods used to detect abnormalities
ment of intracranial EEG electrodes, potentially in localization-related epilepsy. Additionally, a
special mention will be made of the integration
K. Blackmon, Ph.D. (*) • T. Thesen, D.Phil. of quantitative MRI with neuropsychological
Department of Neurology, New York University assessment methods in focal epilepsy syndromes,
School of Medicine, 223 East 34th Street,
although this is a very new and exciting research
New York, NY 10016, USA
e-mail: karen.blackmon@nyumc.org; area that has yet to be fully realized in most clini-
thomas.thesen@med.nyu.edu cal settings.

W.B. Barr and C. Morrison (eds.), Handbook on the Neuropsychology of Epilepsy, 155
Clinical Handbooks in Neuropsychology, DOI 10.1007/978-0-387-92826-5_7,
© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015
156 K. Blackmon and T. Thesen

Table 7.1 Basic clinical MRI sequences for visualizing The most important factor in optimizing an
brain structure and pathology
MRI protocol for epilepsy is clear visualization
Images Description Advantages of mesial temporal structures. This requires
T1-weighted CSF is black Good visualization of brain thinner slices, with voxel volumes of 1 mm4
images Gray matter morphology
being the current norm at 3T, which improves
is dark Used to initially define
White matter brain anatomy spatial resolution but also increases scan time and
is bright noise. Images must be obtained with appropriate
T2-weighted CSF is white Pathological areas stand alignment around the “hippocampal axis” rather
images Gray matter out as brighter (increased than the “machine axis” so that the whole of the
is bright signal)
hippocampus can be visualized in a single slice
White matter
is dark with minimal partial volume effects. Coronal
Fluid- CSF is black Pathological areas stand images should transect the hippocampus at right
attenuated Gray matter out as brighter (increased angles, and axial images should follow the long
inversion is bright signal) axis of the hippocampus. Good spatial resolution
recovery White matter Focal cortical lesions are
images is dark easier to identify; however,
and signal-to-noise ratio are essential components
(FLAIR) there is increased of a dedicated epilepsy MRI protocol. The most
artifactual signal in mesial commonly used sequence is magnetization-
temporal regions prepared 180° radio-frequency pulses and rapid
This table describes MRI sequences that are typically gradient echo (MPRAGE). One-to-two mm
acquired as part of the standard epilepsy diagnostic workup coronal MPRAGE slices (which provide T1
contrast) are typically used to evaluate the
The Epilepsy Protocol temporal lobe, whereas axial slices are obtained
if extratemporal epilepsy is suspected. When
In a typical comprehensive epilepsy evaluation, focal cortical dysplasias are suspected, a
structural MRI images are presented as T1-weighted sequence optimized for gray-white
T1-weighted images, T2-weighted images, and T2 matter contrast is important, as abnormal imaging
fluid-attenuated inversion recovery (FLAIR), with features include increased cortical thickness and
advantages associated with each (see Table 7.1). It blurring of the gray-white matter boundary. For a
is essential that a dedicated center-specific “epi- detailed list of recommended high-contrast T1
lepsy” MRI protocol be developed through a col- sequences for various scanner types, see http://
laborative process involving both radiologists and www.loni.ucla.edu/ADNI/Research/Cores/.
epileptologists. Careful consideration must be
given to disease-specific factors (Jackson &
Kuzniecky, 2008). The importance of an epilepsy- What Can Be Identified on MRI
dedicated protocol was demonstrated in a pro- with the Visual Eye in Epilepsy?
spective study of 123 consecutive patients
undergoing epilepsy surgery evaluations (Von Detection of hippocampal sclerosis (HS) is an
Oertzen et al., 2002). Sensitivity of “nonexpert” important goal of a basic MRI screening study, as
review of standard MRI reports for focal lesions this pathology is the most prevalent of the focal
was 39 %; of “expert” review of standard MRI, abnormalities in partial epilepsy. Features of HS
50 %; and of epilepsy-dedicated MRI, 91 %. include altered signal in the hippocampus, hip-
Dedicated MRI showed focal lesions in 85 % of pocampal asymmetries, and loss of internal hip-
patients with “non-lesional” standard MRI. In par- pocampal architecture (Jackson & Kuzniecky,
ticular, hippocampal sclerosis was missed in 86 % 2008). Using T2 optimized sequences, high-
of cases. Neuropathological diagnoses were pre- intensity signal associated with sclerosis can be
dicted correctly (with histopathological confirma- detected in the hippocampal gray matter.
tion) in 89 % of dedicated MRI reports compared Abnormal signal can best be seen on FLAIR
to only 22 % of “nonexpert” standard MRI reports. images; however, artifactual errors are common
7 Structural and Quantitative MRI in Epilepsy 157

in the mesial temporal regions. Therefore, expert tissue or cortical dysplasia, more intensive MRI
interpretation and confirmation with T2 studies should be considered, such as 3D volu-
sequences are recommended. Hippocampal atro- metric acquisition sequences, T2 relaxometry,
phy is often subtle and only visually apparent magnetic resonance spectroscopy, and quantita-
with well-aligned images free of partial volume tive methods.
effects. Furthermore, unilateral atrophy must be
differentiated from normal variations in right and
left hippocampal asymmetries (Mingrino et al., Quantitative MRI
n.d.; Walhovd et al., 2009). Finally, morphomet-
ric variations in internal hippocampal architec- Advantages over Visual Analysis
ture can be best seen on heavily T1-weighted
sequences, such as an inversion recovery Automated MRI brain image analysis tech-
sequence. Although experts who are trained in niques, such as voxel-based morphometry
mesial temporal anatomy can make such differ- (VBM) (Ashburner & Friston, 2000), offer
entiations, quantitative methods are becoming advantages over subjective techniques (e.g.,
more influential in detecting hippocampal shape visual inspection) and have been employed to
abnormalities and atrophy. detect subtle anatomical anomalies in individual
Malformations of cortical development neurological patients compared to healthy con-
(MCDs), particularly focal cortical dysplasia trols (Bernasconi et al., 2004; Mehta, Grabowski,
(FCD), are increasingly recognized as the most Trivedi, & Damasio, 2003; Wilke, Kassubek,
common etiology in pediatric epilepsy and the Ziyeh, Schulze-Bonhage, & Huppertz, 2003).
second most common etiology in adults with Morphometric analysis of structural MRI has
medically intractable seizures (Lerner et al., become a widely used and established approach
2009). Advances in MR imaging have allowed to study normal brain development and neuro-
for improved visual detection and diagnosis of logical diseases. T1-weighted images are primar-
FCD and other cortical malformations in patients ily used to study gray matter properties, whereas
with epilepsy (Barkovich, Kuzniecky, & Dobyns, diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) is employed in
2001). Common features of FCD include the investigation of white matter features.
abnormal gyral and sulcal pattern, thickening of Whereas most conventional clinical MRI
the cortical gray matter, blurring of the gray- review is performed by visual inspection of images
white matter junction, abnormal neural and glia- slice by slice, quantitative MRI involves 3D image
derived cell types, and high signal on T2 or acquisition and systematic quantification of brain
FLAIR sequences (Barkovich et al., 2001; morphology, typically across the entire brain.
Palmini et al., 2004; Widdess-Walsh, Diehl, & Brain structures may be traced manually for volu-
Najm, 2006). In a sample of 102 individuals with metric estimation; however, as this is very labor-
MCDs (84 % of whom had epilepsy), abnormally intensive, it is not feasible in a clinical setting and
small hippocampi were found in 30 %, along therefore largely constrained to research studies.
with abnormal hippocampal rotation and shape The availability of increased computing power
and abnormally large hippocampi in individuals and advancements in signal processing in medical
with lissencephaly and subcortical laminar het- imaging has enabled automatic quantification of
erotopia (Montenegro et al., 2006). brain structures with minimal user intervention
Initial MRI screening is also important in the and, crucially, within reasonable time frames.
detection of tumors, vascular abnormalities, and Importantly, quantitative MRI has the potential to
traumatic and developmental lesions that are detect subtle abnormalities in an individual patient
apparent to the visual eye. In the case of more by comparing metrics derived from a single scan
subtle lesions such as small areas of sclerotic to large normal control data sets.
158 K. Blackmon and T. Thesen

Different Methodologies warping procedures have been applied. This may


and Software for Quantitative reduce the sensitivity to detect group differences
MRI Analyses in cortical regions (Ashburner & Friston, 1999).
Likewise, VBM’s smoothing step neglects the
Voxel-Based Morphometry increased variability in anatomical relationships
Voxel-based morphometry is the most widely across a folded surface (Singh et al., 2006).
used method for automatic quantification of brain
structure and whole-brain structure-symptom
analyses (Wright et al., 1995). A detailed review Applications of VBM in Epilepsy
of VBM methods is provided by Ashburner and
Friston (Ashburner & Friston, 2000). VBM first VBM investigations have identified widespread
aligns brains into a common stereotactic space by cortical and subcortical abnormalities associated
morphing each individual brain to a standard with temporal lobe epilepsy that are not apparent
template. The most often used template is an upon visual inspection. In addition to hippocam-
average of 152 brains provided by the Montreal pal atrophy (Bernasconi et al., 2003; Salmenperä,
Neurological Institute (i.e., MNI152). Averaging Kälviäinen, Partanen, & Pitkänen, 2001), struc-
brains to a standard template allows matching of tural abnormalities are found in the entorhinal
anatomical structures across individuals and cortex (Bonilha et al., 2007), fornix (Kuzniecky
comparisons of findings, for example, from et al., 1999), parahippocampal gyrus (Bernasconi
reported x–y–z coordinates of areas of significant et al., 2003), amygdala (Bernasconi et al., 2003;
differences found in other studies (Ashburner & Salmenperä et al., 2001), basal ganglia (Dreifuss
Friston, 1999). The next step segments tissue into et al., 2001), thalamus (DeCarli et al., 1998), lat-
white and gray matter based on image intensities eral temporal lobe (Jutila et al., 2001; J. W. Lee
from T1-weighted images. This requires an MRI et al., 1998; Moran, Lemieux, Kitchen, Fish, &
image with high gray-white matter contrast. The Shorvon, 2001), and cerebellum (Sandok,
gray matter partitions obtained from the segmen- O’Brien, Jack, & So, 2000). As summarized in a
tation are compared statistically on a voxel-by- comprehensive review of VBM group studies
voxel basis. The result is a statistical parametric comparing patients with TLE to healthy controls
map across the whole brain that identifies and (Keller & Roberts, 2008), hippocampal atrophy
localizes areas of significant differences in gray ipsilateral to the seizure focus and bilateral tha-
matter concentration/density between groups or lamic atrophy are common findings; however,
between a group and a single patient. several neocortical abnormalities can also be
The main advantage of VBM is that it repre- observed. Such findings are consistent with the
sents a comprehensive assessment of anatomical diverse cognitive deficits demonstrated in TLE
differences throughout the brain and therefore outside of memory impairment (Hermann, Lin,
does not require any a priori clinically defined Jones, & Seidenberg, 2009). However, wide-
anatomical region nor is it biased to any well- spread neocortical atrophy associated with sei-
defined structures. Some VBM tools, such as zure burden must be differentiated from the
SPM, have a very useful batch scripting tool that, presence of dual pathology, as the presence of
once set up by an expert, allows quick and easy extratemporal structural abnormalities is associ-
analysis and visualization of VBM results by ated with a reduction in the likelihood of seizure
nonexperts. The main disadvantage of VBM is freedom following a mesial temporal resection
that there is potential for misalignment of cortical (Sisodiya et al., 1997).
structures during registration procedures. Cortical VBM methods have also been applied to the
variability in folding patterns complicates characterization of focal cortical abnormalities in
registration from native space to standard space, individuals with visually apparent malformations
increasing the probability of misclassifying gray of cortical development. In one study, VBM
and white matter tissue, even after nonlinear identified gray matter abnormalities in 79 % of
7 Structural and Quantitative MRI in Epilepsy 159

visually identified FCDs, as well as abnormalities as well as measures several volume and surface-
extending beyond the known lesion (Colliot based morphological MRI features.
et al., 2006). In another investigation, 9 out of 11
patients showed significant gray matter
concentration excess that was correspondent to Applications of Surface-Based
the visually detected FCD (Leonardo Bonilha Methods in Epilepsy
et al., 2006). VBM methods have also reliably
identified regions of increased T2 signal that Consistent with VBM studies, surface-based
correspond to visually identified MCDs in 18 out methods have identified widespread neocortical
of 20 patients (Rugg-Gunn, Boulby, Symms, abnormalities associated with mesial temporal
Barker, & Duncan, 2005). Importantly, all of lobe epilepsy (MTLE), particularly cortical thin-
these studies identified additional gray matter ning of the frontal and lateral temporal regions
abnormalities that were not apparent to the visual (Lin et al., 2007; McDonald et al., 2008b).
eye. However, the epileptogenic potential of such Greater asymmetries in cortical thickness have
putatively occult lesions was not demonstrated, been observed in the medial temporal cortex of
and the detection of so-called “MRI-negative” MTLE patients relative to controls, with more
lesions remains an active area of research. abnormalities in the regions ipsilateral to the sei-
zure focus (Bernhardt et al., 2008; Lin et al.,
2007; McDonald et al., 2008b). Ipsilateral abnor-
Surface-Based Morphometry malities are also apparent in frontal regions
(Bernhardt et al., 2008). However, both cortical
More recent developments in brain morphometry thinning and reduction in cortical complexity are
include surface-based methods for registration found in bilateral extratemporal regions, even in
and group comparison of cortical structures. The the case of unilateral seizure foci (Lin et al.,
fundamental difference from volume-based 2007). Interestingly, differential patterns of neo-
approaches, such as VBM, is that computations cortical thinning are observed in TLE with MTS
are not performed on voxels within a 3D volume (TLE-MTS) and TLE without MTS (TLE-no),
but involve the more elaborate geometric con- with more thinning in the inferior medial and
struction of inner and outer cortical boundaries or posterior temporal regions in TLE-MTS and
surfaces (Fischl & Dale, 2000; Miller, Massie, more thinning in lateral temporal and opercular
Ratnanather, Botteron, & Csernansky, 2000). The regions in TLE-no (Mueller et al., 2009). This
point of measurement in volume-based suggests that surface features can help to distin-
approaches is the voxel; when dealing with sur- guish whether differential epileptogenic net-
faces, it is the vertex. This allows for more pre- works might be involved in TLE with and without
cise matching of cortical structures to a standard MTS. An exciting area for further research would
spherical template by directly aligning corre- be to determine whether there is a relationship
sponding sulcal and gyral patterns (Fischl, between these differential patterns of cortical
Sereno, & Dale, 1999). Surfaces also offer advan- thinning and different patterns of performance on
tages in visualization as they can be inflated to neuropsychological tests (e.g., greater memory
show abnormalities hidden in sulci. A common deficits associated with MTS and greater word-
measure obtained from surface-based methods is finding difficulties associated with MTS-no).
cortical thickness, derived from measuring the Studies on the relationship between surface-
distance between corresponding points on the based measures of neocortical abnormalities and
white and pial surfaces across the whole brain cognitive dysfunction in TLE are only beginning
(see Fig. 7.1). For example, FreeSurfer (http:// to emerge. A relationship between cortical thin-
surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu) is a popular com- ning and general cognitive phenotypical patterns
prehensive toolbox that performs automatic sub- has been recently demonstrated. Increasing
cortical segmentation and surface reconstruction, abnormalities in temporal and extratemporal
160 K. Blackmon and T. Thesen

Fig. 7.1 T1-weighted 3D anatomical MRI scan with imaging sequence optimized for gray-white matter contrast.
Automated tracing of white matter (green) and pial (blue) surfaces

cortical thickness were found to distinguish cal thickening, blurred gray-white matter bound-
between different cognitive phenotypes (minimally ary, and hyperintense T1 signal (Besson,
impaired; memory impaired; memory, executive Bernasconi, Colliot, Evans, & Bernasconi, 2008).
function, and speed impaired) in a stepwise fash- These characteristic features, particularly in
ion, with the most cognitively impaired group dem- combination, can be used to detect subtle abnor-
onstrating the most significant cortical thinning malities that escape visual detection. Figure 7.2
(Dabbs, Jones, Seidenberg, & Hermann, 2009). shows an example of a 38-year-old female patient
Also consistent with VBM studies, surface- who had suffered from epilepsy all her life.
based methods can be used to detect cortical Several past MRIs showed no sign of a focal
abnormalities associated with MCDs. Surface- lesion until she was referred to our epilepsy cen-
based methods have identified that areas of focal ter, and a small T2-FLAIR abnormality was dis-
cortical dysplasia can be preferentially located at covered in her right mesial frontal lobe.
the bottom of abnormally deep sulci (Besson, Quantitative comparison revealed significant
Andermann, Dubeau, & Bernasconi, 2008), a thickening and blurring in the same region, imag-
finding which may be used to direct the search ing features consistent with focal cortical dyspla-
for developmental abnormalities, particularly sia. The epileptogenic character of the region was
when MRI features are only mildly abnormal or subsequently confirmed by intracranial electro-
absent. Uniquely, surface-based methods can physiology, as well as pathology and outcome
characterize subtle surface features, such as corti- after resection.
7 Structural and Quantitative MRI in Epilepsy 161

Fig. 7.2 Multimodal results for Patient 4. (a) Sagittal T1 matter junction. EEG traces from intracranial electrodes
with pial and white matter surface tracings used for calcu- near the dysplastic area showing focal interictal and ictal
lating cortical thickness and G-W contrast. Green circle discharges (orange), compared to slightly more distant
marks area of visually identified focal cortical dysplasia. electrode locations (white), showing a co-localization of
(b) Patient’s reconstructed white matter surface with area detected lesion and pathological electrophysiology. (e)
of significant cortical thickening marked in red/yellow Mean gray-white matter contrast for the single patient and
(z > 3.5, cluster corrected) within the expert-delineated all control subjects. More positive values denote increased
lesion margin (green tracing). (c) Mean cortical thickness gray-white boundary blurring. (f) Resected area after sur-
of the true positive cluster for patient (red cross, top left) gery (in white). Cross hair marks area of maximum thick-
and all normal control subjects in blue. (d) Same as b, but ness increase within the detected lesion. This patient is
for gray-white matter contrast. The significant cluster seizure-free 1 year after surgery
shows an area of significant blurring of the gray-white

shape and image intensity characteristics.


Subcortical Segmentation Widely used software packages that perform
automatic segmentation and volume calcula-
Surface-based coordinate systems are appropri- tions for various subcortical structures (such as
ate for registration of cortical structures; how- hippocampus, amygdala, thalamus, caudate,
ever, different methods are necessary for etc.) include Harvard’s FreeSurfer and Oxford’s
alignment and intersubject averaging of white FIRST. Both programs require a good-quality,
matter and subcortical gray matter structures. high-resolution T1-weigthed MPRAGE scan as
Automated segmentation tools for obtaining input and function with only minimal manual
subcortical volumetric information involve intervention.
162 K. Blackmon and T. Thesen

Fig. 7.3 Subcortical segmentation of a patient with bilat- volume = −5.61, and right HC volume = −7.74. SDs below
eral HC atrophy. MRI volumetry revealed: left HC vol- age-matched control volumes
ume = 1,731 mm3, right HC = 1,790 mm3, left HC

Applications of Automatic
Segmentation in Epilepsy Diffusion Tensor Imaging

Automatic subcortical segmentation methods DTI allows the measurement of fiber tracts in the
have had as high as a 74 % accuracy rate in the brain by calculating water diffusion. Water mol-
lateralization of epilepsy through hippocampal ecules diffuse more easily along structures and
volumetric comparisons, compared to 78 % with thus showing larger anisotropy on diffusion scans
manual segmentation methods (Akhondi-Asl, where white matter tracts are present. The most
Jafari-Khouzani, Elisevich, & Soltanian-Zadeh, widely used index in DTI, fractional anisotropy
2010). Reductions in hippocampal volumes ipsi- (FA), is expressed in values between 0 and 1,
lateral to seizure focus in MTLE have been iden- with higher anisotropy occurring in white matter
tified, along with bilateral reductions in thalamic and the corpus callosum and lower values in cere-
and cerebellar gray matter volumes (McDonald brospinal fluid and gray matter. FA is often used
et al., 2008a). Some companies, like San Diego- in studies as a measure of white matter integrity.
based CorTechs, Inc., offer FDA-approved user- Tractography is more limited in the vicinity of
friendly processing and visualization of mesiotemporal and neocortical regions, where
quantitative MRI morphometry with automatic fibers are of smaller diameter, have more branch-
report generation, which are reimbursable by ing, and are more likely to cross, increasing the
insurance companies. An example where quanti- difficulty of determining directionality. Methods
tative analysis offered increased accuracy com- are being developed to correct for this by
pared to visual inspection alone is shown by a improved partial volume corrections and calcula-
patient evaluated at our center for presurgical tion of secondary fiber directions.
evaluation and who had undergone a full epilepsy
MRI protocol. The clinical MRI report indicated
right hippocampal atrophy. However, when a Applications of DTI in Epilepsy
subcortical segmentation was performed and the
patient’s hippocampal volumes were compared A general decrease in white matter volume in
to 40 normal controls, it was evident that both chronic TLE has been well documented; however,
hippocampi were severely atrophied, with more DTI allows for the investigation of specific fiber
atrophy seen in the right hemisphere (Fig. 7.3). tract integrity. Bilateral symmetrical reduction in
7 Structural and Quantitative MRI in Epilepsy 163

fractional anisotropy (FA) is observed in the for- a valuable tool in the assessment of structural
nix and cingulum of patients with TLE (Concha, abnormalities associated with epilepsy; however,
Beaulieu, & Gross, 2005), as well as frontotem- the systematic implementation of such methods
poral white matter tracts (arcuate fasciculus and in the basic clinical assessment of epilepsy has
uncinate fasciculus) (Lin, Riley, Juranek, & yet to be realized in many comprehensive cen-
Cramer, 2008; Matsumoto et al., 2008; Rodrigo ters. Reasons for this lag between research find-
et al., 2007), temporal-occipital connections ings and clinical implementation include the
(inferior longitudinal fasciculus), frontal-occipi- need for labor-intensive, expert-dependent man-
tal connections (inferior frontal-occipital fascicu- ual tracings and operator-dependent interpreta-
lus) (Ahmadi et al., 2009), and the corpus tion of results, both of which are impractical in
callosum (Arfanakis et al., 2002; Concha, most clinical settings (Brewer, 2009). Recent
Beaulieu, Collins, & Gross, 2009). Patients with quantitative tools have solved these problems
left TLE have greater, more diffuse changes in with full automation of registration, segmenta-
FA values across multiple fiber tracts, whereas tion, and comparison procedures, which provide
patients with right TLE showed changes that are output that is relatively quick to generate and
primarily ipsilateral to the side of seizure onset clinically interpretable.
(Ahmadi et al., 2009). Quantitative MRI methods also have the
Consistent with cortical abnormalities found promising potential of being able to incorporate
in TLE, whole-brain voxelwise analysis of DTI neuropsychological data directly into an analysis.
data has revealed extensive bilateral white matter Appreciation of structural and behavioral
diffusion abnormalities in the frontal and correlates can be done simultaneously and
temporal lobes, with more involvement in the empirically. Such information could prove to be
hemisphere ipsilateral to seizure onset (Shon invaluable in the lateralization and localization of
et al., 2010; Thivard et al., 2005). In TLE patients focal epilepsy, as well as in characterizing the
with left- and right-sided resective surgery, wide- extent of structural and functional damage
spread tract degradation was observed in the associated with seizure activity.
uncinate fasciculus, the fronto-occipital The general trend toward standardization of
fasciculus, the superior longitudinal fasciculus, neuroimaging data in making group or individual
the corpus callosum, and the corticospinal tract comparisons is intuitive for most neuropsycholo-
(Schoene-Bake et al., 2009). Furthermore, tract gists. Difficulties in accurate registration of brain
degradation has been shown to be associated with structures to a standard template are not unlike the
memory and language impairments in TLE dilemma neuropsychologists face in choosing
patients (McDonald et al., 2008), suggesting that normative groups that are demographically simi-
DTI can provide important information for pre- lar to their individual patient. Similarly, quantita-
dicting cognitive status in individuals with epi- tive neuroimaging methods might benefit from
lepsy (Table 7.2). implementation of standard neuropsychological
practices such as limiting the normative group to a
more constrained age range or education bracket.
Conclusions/Future Possibilities Identification of abnormalities must be sensitive
and specific, a goal that is shared by neuroimagers
Visual inspection of MRI images obtained and neuropsychologists. Whether this goal can be
through a dedicated epilepsy protocol has become more precisely realized through direct combina-
a standard component of the comprehensive epi- tion of neuroimaging and neuropsychological
lepsy workup. Quantitative MRI methods provide methods will be exciting to see in the near future.
164 K. Blackmon and T. Thesen

Table 7.2 Software packages for structural MRI analysis


Software
Method package Web site
VBM
SPM http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/ Free
FSLVBM http://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslvbm Free
Jena VBM http://dbm.neuro.uni-jena.de/vbm/ Free
Segmentation
NVM http://neuromorphometrics.org:8080/nvm/ Free
FSL FIRST http://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/first/index.html Free
BrainVISA http://brainvisa.info/ Free
BioImage Suite http://bioimagesuite.org/ Free
Caret http://brainvis.wustl.edu/wiki/index.php/Caret:About Free
ITK-SNAP http://www.itksnap.org/pmwiki/pmwiki.php Free
FreeSurfer http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/ Free
LONI Brain http://loni.ucla.edu/Software/BrainParser
Parser
NeuroQuant http://www.cortechs.net/products/neuroquant.php Commercial
Surface based
FreeSurfer http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/ Free
Caret http://brainvis.wustl.edu/wiki/index.php/Caret:About Free
BrainVISA http://brainvisa.info/ Free
LONI http://loni.ucla.edu/Software/BrainSuite
BrainSuite
DTI
DSI Studio http://dsi-studio.labsolver.org/ Free
FSL FTD http://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fdt/index.html Free
BrainMagix http://www.imagilys.com/brainmagix-neuroimaging-fmri-software/ Commercial
BioImage Suite http://bioimagesuite.org/ Free
Camino http://web4.cs.ucl.ac.uk/research/medic/camino/pmwiki/pmwiki.php
Manual tracing LONI http://loni.ucla.edu/Software/MultiTracer
MultiTracer
Visualization
3D Slicer www.slicer.org Free
BrainVoyager http://www.brainvoyager.com/ Commercial
MRIcro http://www.cabiatl.com/mricro/mricro/index.html Free
FSLView http://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslview/index.html Free
Mango http://ric.uthscsa.edu/mango/
DICOMWorks http://dicom.online.fr/ Free
This is by no means intended as an exhaustive list of all available neuroimaging tools but does list the most popular
software toolboxes used in clinical and basic research. Further information on software packages for neuroimaging is
available at the NIH Neuroimaging Informatics Tools and Resources Clearinghouse (NITRC) http://www.nitrc.org/

Akhondi-Asl, A., Jafari-Khouzani, K., Elisevich, K., &


Soltanian-Zadeh, H. (2010). Hippocampal volumetry
References for lateralization of temporal lobe epilepsy: Automated
versus manual methods. NeuroImage. doi:10.1016/j.
Ahmadi, M. E., Hagler, D. J., McDonald, C. R., Tecoma, neuroimage.2010.03.066.
E. S., Iragui, V. J., Dale, A. M., et al. (2009). Side matters: Arfanakis, K., Hermann, B. P., Rogers, B. P., Carew, J. D.,
Diffusion tensor imaging tractography in left and right Seidenberg, M., & Meyerand, M. E. (2002). Diffusion
temporal lobe epilepsy. AJNR. American Journal of tensor MRI in temporal lobe epilepsy. Magnetic
Neuroradiology, 30(9), 1740–1747. doi:10.3174/ajnr. Resonance Imaging, 20(7), 511–519.
A1650.
7 Structural and Quantitative MRI in Epilepsy 165

Ashburner, J., & Friston, K. J. (1999). Nonlinear spatial Behavioural Neurology, 21(1), 21–28. doi:10.3233/
normalization using basis functions. Human Brain BEN-2009-0226.
Mapping, 7(4), 254–266. Cardoso, T. A. M., Coan, A. C., Kobayashi, E., Guerreiro,
Ashburner, J., & Friston, K. J. (2000). Voxel-based mor- C. A. M., Li, L. M., & Cendes, F. (2006). Hippocampal
phometry—The methods. NeuroImage, 11(6 Pt 1), abnormalities and seizure recurrence after antiepilep-
805–821. doi:10.1006/nimg.2000.0582. tic drug withdrawal. Neurology, 67(1), 134–136.
Barkovich, A. J., Kuzniecky, R. I., & Dobyns, W. B. doi:10.1212/01.wnl.0000223350.08394.06.
(2001). Radiologic classification of malformations of Colliot, O., Mansi, T., Bernasconi, N., Naessens, V.,
cortical development. Current Opinion in Neurology, Klironomos, D., & Bernasconi, A. (2006).
14(2), 145–149. Segmentation of focal cortical dysplasia lesions on
Berkovic, S. F., McIntosh, A. M., Kalnins, R. M., Jackson, MRI using level set evolution. NeuroImage, 32(4),
G. D., Fabinyi, G. C., Brazenor, G. A., et al. (1995). 1621–1630. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.04.225.
Preoperative MRI predicts outcome of temporal lobec- Concha, L., Beaulieu, C., Collins, D. L., & Gross, D. W.
tomy: An actuarial analysis. Neurology, 45(7), (2009). White-matter diffusion abnormalities in tem-
1358–1363. poral-lobe epilepsy with and without mesial tempo-
Bernasconi, N., Bernasconi, A., Caramanos, Z., Antel, ral sclerosis. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery,
S. B., Andermann, F., & Arnold, D. L. (2003). Mesial and Psychiatry, 80(3), 312–319. doi:10.1136/
temporal damage in temporal lobe epilepsy: A volu- jnnp.2007.139287.
metric MRI study of the hippocampus, amygdala and Concha, L., Beaulieu, C., & Gross, D. W. (2005). Bilateral
parahippocampal region. Brain, 126(Pt 2), 462–469. limbic diffusion abnormalities in unilateral temporal
Bernasconi, N., Duchesne, S., Janke, A., Lerch, J., Collins, lobe epilepsy. Annals of Neurology, 57(2), 188–196.
D. L., & Bernasconi, A. (2004). Whole-brain voxel- doi:10.1002/ana.20334.
based statistical analysis of gray matter and white mat- Dabbs, K., Jones, J., Seidenberg, M., & Hermann, B.
ter in temporal lobe epilepsy. NeuroImage, 23(2), (2009). Neuroanatomical correlates of cognitive pheno-
717–723. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.06.015. types in temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy & Behavior,
Bernhardt, B. C., Worsley, K. J., Besson, P., Concha, L., 15(4), 445–451. doi:10.1016/j.yebeh.2009.05.012.
Lerch, J. P., Evans, A. C., et al. (2008). Mapping lim- DeCarli, C., Hatta, J., Fazilat, S., Fazilat, S., Gaillard,
bic network organization in temporal lobe epilepsy W. D., & Theodore, W. H. (1998). Extratemporal atro-
using morphometric correlations: Insights on the rela- phy in patients with complex partial seizures of left
tion between mesiotemporal connectivity and cortical temporal origin. Annals of Neurology, 43(1), 41–45.
atrophy. NeuroImage, 42(2), 515–524. doi:10.1016/j. doi:10.1002/ana.410430110.
neuroimage.2008.04.261. Dreifuss, S., Vingerhoets, F. J., Lazeyras, F., Andino,
Besson, P., Andermann, F., Dubeau, F., & Bernasconi, A. S. G., Spinelli, L., Delavelle, J., et al. (2001).
(2008). Small focal cortical dysplasia lesions are Volumetric measurements of subcortical nuclei in
located at the bottom of a deep sulcus. Brain, 131(Pt patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. Neurology,
12), 3246–3255. doi:10.1093/brain/awn224. 57(9), 1636–1641.
Besson, P., Bernasconi, N., Colliot, O., Evans, A., & Engel, J. (1996). Surgery for seizures. The New England
Bernasconi, A. (2008). Surface-based texture and mor- Journal of Medicine, 334(10), 647–652.
phological analysis detects subtle cortical dysplasia. Fischl, B., & Dale, A. M. (2000). Measuring the thickness
Medical Image Computing and Computer-Assisted of the human cerebral cortex from magnetic resonance
Intervention, 11(Pt 1), 645–652. images. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Betting, L. E., Mory, S. B., Lopes-Cendes, I., Li, L. M., Sciences of the United States of America, 97(20),
Guerreiro, M. M., Guerreiro, C., et al. (2006). MRI 11050–11055. doi:10.1073/pnas.200033797.
reveals structural abnormalities in patients with idio- Fischl, B., Sereno, M. I., & Dale, A. M. (1999). Cortical
pathic generalized epilepsy. Neurology, 67(5), 848– surface-based analysis. II: Inflation, flattening, and a
852. doi:10.1212/01.wnl.0000233886.55203.bd. surface-based coordinate system. NeuroImage, 9(2),
Bonilha, L., Montenegro, M. A., Rorden, C., Castellano, G., 195–207. doi:10.1006/nimg.1998.0396.
Guerreiro, M. M., Cendes, F., et al. (2006). Voxel-based Hermann, B. P., Lin, J. J., Jones, J. E., & Seidenberg, M.
morphometry reveals excess gray matter concentration (2009). The emerging architecture of neuropsycho-
in patients with focal cortical dysplasia. Epilepsia, logical impairment in epilepsy. Neurologic Clinics,
47(5), 908–915. doi:10.1111/j.1528-1167.2006.00548.x. 27(4), 881–907. doi:10.1016/j.ncl.2009.08.001.
Bonilha, L., Rorden, C., Halford, J. J., Eckert, M., Jackson, G., & Kuzniecky, R. I. (2008). Structural neuro-
Appenzeller, S., Cendes, F., et al. (2007). imaging. In J. Engel, T. A. Pedley, J. Aicardi, et al.
Asymmetrical extra-hippocampal grey matter loss (Eds.), Epilepsy: A comprehensive textbook (2nd ed.,
related to hippocampal atrophy in patients with pp. 917–944). Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams
medial temporal lobe epilepsy. Journal of Neurology, & Wilkins.
Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 78(3), 286–294. Jeha, L. E., Najm, I., Bingaman, W., Dinner, D., Widdess-
doi:10.1136/jnnp.2006.103994. Walsh, P., & Lüders, H. (2007). Surgical outcome and
Brewer, J. B. (2009). Fully-automated volumetric MRI prognostic factors of frontal lobe epilepsy surgery.
with normative ranges: Translation to clinical practice. Brain, 130(Pt 2), 574–584. doi:10.1093/brain/awl364.
166 K. Blackmon and T. Thesen

Jutila, L., Ylinen, A., Partanen, K., Alafuzoff, I., Mervaala, focal lesion detection in individuals. NeuroImage,
E., Partanen, J., et al. (2001). MR volumetry of the ento- 20(3), 1438–1454.
rhinal, perirhinal, and temporopolar cortices in drug- Miller, M. I., Massie, A. B., Ratnanather, J. T., Botteron,
refractory temporal lobe epilepsy. AJNR. American K. N., & Csernansky, J. G. (2000). Bayesian construc-
Journal of Neuroradiology, 22(8), 1490–1501. tion of geometrically based cortical thickness metrics.
Keller, S. S., & Roberts, N. (2008). Voxel-based mor- NeuroImage, 12(6), 676–687. doi:10.1006/nimg.
phometry of temporal lobe epilepsy: An introduction 2000.0666.
and review of the literature. Epilepsia, 49(5), 741–757. Mingrino, J., Blackmon, K., Halgren, E., Devinsky, O.,
doi:10.1111/j.1528-1167.2007.01485.x. Carlson, C., Dubois, J., et al. (n.d.). Morphometric
Kuzniecky, R., Bilir, E., Gilliam, F., Faught, E., Martin, variations in cortical gray matter, white matter, and
R., & Hugg, J. (1999). Quantitative MRI in temporal subcortical volumes across the early to middle adult
lobe epilepsy: Evidence for fornix atrophy. Neurology, lifespan. Submitted to Epilepsy Research and
53(3), 496–501. Treatment.
Lee, J. W., Andermann, F., Dubeau, F., Bernasconi, A., Montenegro, M. A., Kinay, D., Cendes, F., Bernasconi,
MacDonald, D., Evans, A., et al. (1998). Morphometric A., Bernasconi, N., Coan, A. C., et al. (2006). Patterns
analysis of the temporal lobe in temporal lobe epilepsy. of hippocampal abnormalities in malformations of
Epilepsia, 39(7), 727–736. cortical development. Journal of Neurology,
Lerner, J. T., Salamon, N., Hauptman, J. S., Velasco, T. R., Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 77(3), 367–371.
Hemb, M., Wu, J. Y., et al. (2009). Assessment and doi:10.1136/jnnp.2005.070417.
surgical outcomes for mild type I and severe type II Moran, N. F., Lemieux, L., Kitchen, N. D., Fish, D. R., &
cortical dysplasia: A critical review and the UCLA Shorvon, S. D. (2001). Extrahippocampal temporal
experience. Epilepsia, 50(6), 1310–1335. lobe atrophy in temporal lobe epilepsy and mesial
doi:10.1111/j.1528-1167.2008.01998.x. temporal sclerosis. Brain, 124(Pt 1), 167–175.
Lin, J. J., Riley, J. D., Juranek, J., & Cramer, S. C. (2008). Mueller, S. G., Laxer, K. D., Barakos, J., Cheong, I.,
Vulnerability of the frontal-temporal connections in Garcia, P., & Weiner, M. W. (2009). Widespread neo-
temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy Research, 82(2–3), cortical abnormalities in temporal lobe epilepsy with
162–170. doi:10.1016/j.eplepsyres.2008.07.020. and without mesial sclerosis. NeuroImage, 46(2), 353–
Lin, J. J., Salamon, N., Lee, A. D., Dutton, R. A., Geaga, 359. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.02.020.
J. A., Hayashi, K. M., et al. (2007). Reduced neocorti- Palmini, A., Najm, I., Avanzini, G., Babb, T., Guerrini, R.,
cal thickness and complexity mapped in mesial tem- Foldvary-Schaefer, N., et al. (2004). Terminology and
poral lobe epilepsy with hippocampal sclerosis. classification of the cortical dysplasias. Neurology,
Cerebral Cortex, 17(9), 2007–2018. doi:10.1093/cer- 62(6 Suppl 3), S2–S8.
cor/bhl109. Rodrigo, S., Oppenheim, C., Chassoux, F., Golestani, N.,
Matsumoto, R., Okada, T., Mikuni, N., Mitsueda-Ono, T., Cointepas, Y., Poupon, C., et al. (2007). Uncinate fas-
Taki, J., Sawamoto, N., et al. (2008). Hemispheric ciculus fiber tracking in mesial temporal lobe epilepsy.
asymmetry of the arcuate fasciculus: A preliminary Initial findings. European Radiology, 17(7), 1663–
diffusion tensor tractography study in patients with 1668. doi:10.1007/s00330-006-0558-x.
unilateral language dominance defined by Wada test. Rugg-Gunn, F. J., Boulby, P. A., Symms, M. R., Barker,
Journal of Neurology, 255(11), 1703–1711. G. J., & Duncan, J. S. (2005). Whole-brain T2 map-
doi:10.1007/s00415-008-0005-9. ping demonstrates occult abnormalities in focal epi-
McDonald, C. R., Ahmadi, M. E., Hagler, D. J., Tecoma, lepsy. Neurology, 64(2), 318–325. doi:10.1212/01.
E. S., Iragui, V. J., Gharapetian, L., et al. (2008). WNL.0000149642.93493.F4.
Diffusion tensor imaging correlates of memory and Salmenperä, T., Kälviäinen, R., Partanen, K., & Pitkänen,
language impairments in temporal lobe epilepsy. A. (2001). Hippocampal and amygdaloid damage in
Neurology, 71(23), 1869–1876. doi:10.1212/01. partial epilepsy: A cross-sectional MRI study of 241
wnl.0000327824.05348.3b. patients. Epilepsy Research, 46(1), 69–82.
McDonald, C. R., Hagler, D. J., Ahmadi, M. E., Tecoma, Sandok, E. K., O’Brien, T. J., Jack, C. R., & So, E. L.
E., Iragui, V., Dale, A. M., et al. (2008a). Subcortical (2000). Significance of cerebellar atrophy in intracta-
and cerebellar atrophy in mesial temporal lobe epi- ble temporal lobe epilepsy: A quantitative MRI study.
lepsy revealed by automatic segmentation. Epilepsy Epilepsia, 41(10), 1315–1320.
Research, 79(2–3), 130–138. doi:10.1016/j. Schoene-Bake, J., Faber, J., Trautner, P., Kaaden, S.,
eplepsyres.2008.01.006. Tittgemeyer, M., Elger, C. E., et al. (2009). Widespread
McDonald, C. R., Hagler, D. J., Jr., Ahmadi, M. E., affections of large fiber tracts in postoperative tempo-
Tecoma, E., Iragui, V., Gharapetian, L., et al. (2008b). ral lobe epilepsy. NeuroImage, 46(3), 569–576.
Regional neocortical thinning in mesial temporal doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.03.013.
lobe epilepsy. Epilepsia, 49(5), 794–803. Shon, Y., Kim, Y., Koo, B., Lee, J., Kim, H. J., Kim, W. J.,
doi:10.1111/j.1528-1167.2008.01539.x. et al. (2010). Group-specific regional white matter
Mehta, S., Grabowski, T. J., Trivedi, Y., & Damasio, H. abnormality revealed in diffusion tensor imaging of
(2003). Evaluation of voxel-based morphometry for medial temporal lobe epilepsy without hippocampal
7 Structural and Quantitative MRI in Epilepsy 167

sclerosis. Epilepsia, 51(4), 529–535. doi:10.1111/j. Walhovd, K. B., Westlye, L. T., Amlien, I., Espeseth, T.,
1528-1167.2009.02327.x. Reinvang, I., Raz, N., et al. (2009). Consistent neuro-
Singh, V., Chertkow, H., Lerch, J. P., Evans, A. C., Dorr, anatomical age-related volume differences across
A. E., & Kabani, N. J. (2006). Spatial patterns of corti- multiple samples. Neurobiology of Aging.
cal thinning in mild cognitive impairment and doi:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.05.013.
Alzheimer’s disease. Brain, 129(Pt 11), 2885–2893. Widdess-Walsh, P., Diehl, B., & Najm, I. (2006).
doi:10.1093/brain/awl256. Neuroimaging of focal cortical dysplasia. Journal of
Sisodiya, S. M., Moran, N., Free, S. L., Kitchen, N. D., Neuroimaging, 16(3), 185–196. doi:10.1111/j.1552-
Stevens, J. M., Harkness, W. F., et al. (1997). 6569.2006.00025.x.
Correlation of widespread preoperative magnetic reso- Wiebe, S., Blume, W. T., Girvin, J. P., & Eliasziw, M.
nance imaging changes with unsuccessful surgery for (2001). A randomized, controlled trial of surgery for
hippocampal sclerosis. Annals of Neurology, 41(4), temporal-lobe epilepsy. The New England Journal of
490–496. doi:10.1002/ana.410410412. Medicine, 345(5), 311–318.
Spooner, C. G., Berkovic, S. F., Mitchell, L. A., Wrennall, Wilke, M., Kassubek, J., Ziyeh, S., Schulze-Bonhage, A.,
J. A., & Harvey, A. S. (2006). New-onset temporal & Huppertz, H. J. (2003). Automated detection of gray
lobe epilepsy in children: Lesion on MRI predicts poor matter malformations using optimized voxel-based
seizure outcome. Neurology, 67(12), 2147–2153. morphometry: A systematic approach. NeuroImage,
doi:10.1212/01.wnl.0000248189.93630.4f. 20(1), 330–343.
Thivard, L., Lehéricy, S., Krainik, A., Adam, C., Dormont, Woermann, F., Sisodiya, S., Free, S., & Duncan, J. (1998).
D., Chiras, J., et al. (2005). Diffusion tensor imaging Quantitative MRI in patients with idiopathic general-
in medial temporal lobe epilepsy with hippocampal ized epilepsy. Evidence of widespread cerebral struc-
sclerosis. NeuroImage, 28(3), 682–690. doi:10.1016/j. tural changes. Brain, 121(9), 1661–1667. doi:10.1093/
neuroimage.2005.06.045. brain/121.9.1661.
Von Oertzen, J., Urbach, H., Jungbluth, S., Kurthen, M., Wright, I. C., McGuire, P. K., Poline, J. B., Travere, J. M.,
Reuber, M., Fernández, G., et al. (2002). Standard Murray, R. M., Frith, C. D., et al. (1995). A voxel-based
magnetic resonance imaging is inadequate for patients method for the statistical analysis of gray and white
with refractory focal epilepsy. Journal of Neurology, matter density applied to schizophrenia. NeuroImage,
Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 73(6), 643–647. 2(4), 244–252. doi:10.1006/nimg.1995.1032.
Functional MRI in the Presurgical
Epilepsy Evaluation 8
Sara J. Swanson, Jeffrey R. Binder, Manoj Raghavan,
and Matthew Euler

prediction allowing epileptologists, neurosur-


Introduction geons, and patients to make informed decisions
prior to undertaking resective surgery.
In the past two decades, functional neuroimaging While neuropsychologists historically used
techniques have revolutionized our understanding cognitive test data to aid in lateralization and
of normal brain functioning (Binder, Desai, et al., localization of dysfunction relative to a focal sei-
2009; D'Esposito, 2000) and contributed to the zure disorder (Jones-Gotman, 1991), sophisti-
evaluation and treatment of patients with neuro- cated anatomical and electrographic methods are
logical disorders (Bookheimer, 2007; Detre, typically relied upon for surgical decision mak-
2006; Freilich & Gaillard, 2010). Understanding ing. For example, EEG and MRI, when com-
how and when to use functional localization bined, are of high lateralization value (93 %) with
methods such as fMRI in combination with or as neuropsychological testing adding little addi-
an alternative to more traditional mapping meth- tional variance (2 %) (Akanuma, Alarcon, et al.,
ods such as the intracarotid amobarbital (Wada) 2003; Moser, Bauer, et al., 2000; Swanson,
test is critical for the clinical treatment of epilepsy 2006). However, preoperative neuropsychologi-
surgery candidates. FMRI detects task-correlated cal test data provide essential information for
or event-related changes in blood flow associated making predictions about risks for language and
with underlying neuronal activity and is now con- memory morbidity after anterior temporal lobe
sidered a noninvasive alternative to Wada testing (ATL) resection. The best outcome predictions
for language (Binder, 2011). Provides informa- are made by combining preoperative cognitive
tion not only about lateralization of higher cogni- data, fMRI language laterality indexes (LIs), side
tive functions but also localization and outcome of seizure focus, hippocampal status, and age at
onset of seizures (Baxendale & Thompson, 2010;
Binder, Gross, et al., 2010; Binder, Sabsevitz,
S.J. Swanson, Ph.D., A.B.P.P. (*) • J.R. Binder, M.D.
et al., 2008; Bonelli, Powell, et al., 2010; Powell,
M. Raghavan, M.D., Ph.D.
Department of Neurology, Medical College Richardson, et al., 2008; Richardson, Strange,
of Wisconsin, 9200 W. Wisconsin Avenue, et al., 2006; Sabsevitz, Swanson, et al., 2003).
Milwaukee, WI 53226, USA Therefore, neuropsychologists will benefit from
e-mail: SSwanson@mcw.edu; jbinder@mcw.edu;
understanding how to combine functional imag-
mraghavan@mcw.edu
ing, history, and cognitive variables when work-
M. Euler, Ph.D.
ing with epilepsy surgery candidates.
Department of Psychology, University of Utah,
380 South 1530 East, Salt Lake City, UT 84112, USA The present chapter aims to provide clinicians
e-mail: matt.euler@psych.utah.edu with a practical understanding of how to conduct

W.B. Barr and C. Morrison (eds.), Handbook on the Neuropsychology of Epilepsy, 169
Clinical Handbooks in Neuropsychology, DOI 10.1007/978-0-387-92826-5_8,
© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015
170 S.J. Swanson et al.

fMRI with epilepsy surgery candidates. Technical carry hemoglobin molecules. A hemoglobin mol-
and clinical information necessary for conduct- ecule in the deoxygenated state is paramagnetic,
ing fMRI are discussed including how to select i.e., it functions as a microscopic magnet and
the necessary equipment, data analysis software alters the homogeneity (uniformity of strength)
packages, and cognitive task combinations as of the scanner’s magnetic field, while oxygenated
well as how to detect valid task-correlated activ- hemoglobin is magnetically inert and has no
ity, compute language lateralization indexes effect on the underlying magnetic field.
(LIs), and use regression formulas for predicting Microscopic variations in the magnetic field
language and memory outcomes following ATL strength within a voxel (resulting from deoxyhe-
surgery. moglobin molecules) cause the protons within
the voxel to spin (or precess) at slightly different
frequencies, because the spin frequency is
fMRI directly related to field strength. Protons within a
voxel spinning at different frequencies will over
Biophysical Basis of fMRI time become out of phase with each other (“intra-
voxel spin dephasing”). Because the signal mea-
Most fMRI scanning is based on detection of sured by an MRI scanner is stronger when more
local changes in oxygen concentration resulting protons are spinning in phase with each other, the
from changes in neural activity, known as blood MRI signal from a given voxel will be higher
oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) imaging when the hemoglobin molecules in the voxel are
(Ogawa, Lee, et al., 1990). Though the phenom- oxygenated (highly uniform field) and lower
ena underlying the BOLD signal are quite com- when the hemoglobin is deoxygenated (less uni-
plex, the main processes can be briefly form field). This dependency of the MRI signal
summarized. Localized increases in neural activ- on homogeneity of the magnetic field within a
ity produce a chemical signal that leads to voxel is referred to as the T2* (“T2 star”) effect.
localized increases in blood flow to the activated Although BOLD is the most popular form of
region, a process called neurovascular coupling. fMRI, other techniques are also used. The most
The increase in blood flow is relatively slow and common alternative is to measure local changes
delayed compared to the change in neural activ- in blood flow more directly, an approach known
ity, beginning within a second or so but reaching generally as perfusion imaging. Perfusion imag-
a peak only 4–5 s after the neural event. This ing can be based on tracking an injected bolus of
delayed change in blood flow is called the hemo- gadolinium (Belliveau, Kennedy, et al., 1991),
dynamic response function and is important to but a more common approach is to “label” pro-
keep in mind for understanding data analysis and tons with a magnetic pulse as they enter the cra-
certain timing issues in data acquisition and task nium and track their movement into higher image
design. The localized increase in blood flow slices, a technique known as arterial spin labeling
floods the activated region with highly oxygen- (Alsop & Detre, 1998). One advantage of perfu-
ated blood in the form of oxygenated hemoglobin sion imaging is that it provides a more direct
molecules, resulting in a net increase in the ratio measure of blood flow than BOLD imaging,
of oxygenated to deoxygenated hemoglobin. which can be useful in certain situations. As dis-
“BOLD signal” refers to the fact that the cussed below, BOLD imaging is susceptible to
brightness, or signal level, of a voxel (volume image degradation resulting from nonuniformity
element) in an fMRI image increases as a func- of the magnetic field, which is not the case with
tion of the oxyhemoglobin/deoxyhemoglobin perfusion imaging. On the other hand, BOLD
ratio in that voxel. To understand why this is, methods generally provide a higher signal-to-
consider that a voxel in a brain image contains noise ratio (SNR) and better temporal resolution
numerous small blood vessels, each of which than perfusion techniques.
8 Functional MRI in the Presurgical Epilepsy Evaluation 171

Image Acquisition Hardware Repetition time, or TR, is the time required for
one cycle of the pulse sequence, and with EPI
fMRI depends on rapid image acquisition in sequences this is usually equivalent to the time
order to track hemodynamic responses occurring required to collect one image volume. Typical
throughout the brain. BOLD fMRI uses special- values are 2 or 3 s. The term TR is also often used
ized pulse sequences designed to be sensitive to loosely to refer to the image volume collected in
intra-voxel spin dephasing, known as T2*- each cycle of the pulse sequence. Echo time, TE,
weighted sequences. Echoplanar imaging (EPI) is the time between the excitation pulse and reg-
is a pulse sequence capable of very fast acquisi- istration of the signal by the RF receive coils.
tion of T2*-weighted images and is the most Typical values are 40–50 ms when scanning at
common sequence used for BOLD fMRI. Related 1.5 T and 25–35 ms at 3 T. Voxel dimensions are
T2* methods such as spiral imaging are also used determined by the slice thickness, the rectangular
(Noll, Stenger, et al., 1999). All of these methods dimensions of each slice (called the field of view,
require very rapid alterations of the magnetic or FOV), and the in-plane matrix, which is sim-
field to create a spatial grid for pulse excitation ply the number of voxels within the x, y spatial
and signal localization (i.e., spatial encoding), grid of the slice. The in-plane voxel dimensions
necessitating high-performance gradient coils. are obtained simply by dividing the FOV by the
MRI scanners continue to improve in terms of the matrix dimensions. For example, a FOV of
speed (slew rate) and amplitude of these gradient 208 mm × 240 mm and matrix of 64 × 64 pro-
devices, which are the main factors determining duces voxels with in-plane size 3.2 mm × 3.75 mm.
the number of voxels that can be encoded in a A FOV of 192 mm × 192 mm, matrix of 96 × 96,
given amount of time and thus the spatial and and slice thickness of 2.5 mm would yield per-
temporal resolution that can be achieved. fectly cubic (or isotropic) voxels measuring
The signal measured in MRI is based on 2.5 mm in each direction.
energy given off by protons after excitation with A significant problem with T2* imaging, par-
a brief (radiofrequency range, or RF) magnetic ticularly EPI, is extreme loss of signal (“signal
pulse. This signal is detected by coils called RF dropout”) near air-tissue and air-bone interfaces
receive coils, which are positioned as close to the in the head, such as the frontal and temporal
head as possible to maximize sensitivity, typi- sinuses (Ojemann, Akbudak, et al., 1997). These
cally in a cylindrical, cage-like structure sur- interfaces produce macroscopic gradients in the
rounding the head. Most current scanners employ magnetic field that often extend several centime-
multichannel RF coil arrays with 8–32 receive ters into the brain tissue, causing severe intra-
coils providing input to separate channels on the voxel spin dephasing in the affected areas. This
scanner. This arrangement improves SNR by dephasing, of course, is the same phenomenon
summation of signals across channels and makes that underlies the BOLD signal but operates over
possible a newer technique called parallel imag- a larger spatial scale. As a result, the same T2*-
ing. Parallel imaging takes advantage of the spa- weighted images that are optimally sensitive to
tial arrangement of receivers in a multichannel microscopic gradients from deoxyhemoglobin
array coil to reduce the number of gradient molecules always contain areas of severe signal
changes needed for spatial encoding. This trans- loss from macroscopic gradients. The main brain
lates into faster image acquisition, though with regions affected are the ventromedial frontal
some reduction of SNR. lobes, anteromedial temporal lobes (often includ-
ing the amygdala), and the middle portion of the
ventrolateral temporal lobes (inferior temporal
Pulse Sequence Parameters and fusiform gyri). Figure 8.1 shows consistent
regions of signal loss.
An understanding of some imaging parameters can Imaging parameters can be adjusted to reduce
be helpful in choosing an optimal fMRI protocol. the severity of signal dropout (Merboldt,
172 S.J. Swanson et al.

Fig. 8.1 Probabilistic map of areas showing severe signal SFNR values below 20. The overlap of these low SFNR
loss in a typical fMRI study using echoplanar imaging. regions across subjects after normalization to stereotaxic
The study was done at 3 T using TE = 25 ms and voxel space is shown. Green indicates overlap in at least 25 % of
size = 2.5-mm isotropic. Signal-to-fluctuation noise ratio the sample, red overlap in at least 50 %, and yellow over-
(SFNR) maps were calculated for each of 34 healthy con- lap in at least 75 %
trol subjects and thresholded to identify voxels with

Finterbusch, et al., 2000; Morawetz, Holz, et al., factors, and these effects tend to increase with
2008). The shorter the TE, the less time is avail- field strength and matrix size. The result of this
able for spins to become out of phase within a warping is a spatial mismatch between some
voxel, thus the smaller is the effect of an intra- regions of the EPI image and the high-resolution
voxel gradient. Unfortunately, shortening the TE structural images that are usually obtained as an
will also reduce the size of the BOLD effect, for anatomical reference. A common method for
the same reason. Use of smaller voxels can be reducing geometric distortion is to acquire spe-
helpful, since differences in field strength across cial images during the scan that are used later to
a gradient spanning the voxel will be smaller, construct maps of the underlying magnetic field
producing less dephasing. Decreasing the voxel (Jezzard & Balaban, 1995). The phase informa-
size also improves spatial resolution, but this tion in these maps can then be used to “unwarp”
strategy is limited by the fact that smaller voxels the EPI data off-line, using, for example, the
contain fewer protons, thus signal levels will be FUGUE program available in FSL (www.fmrib.
smaller in regions not affected by macroscopic ox.ac.uk/fsl/fugue/).
gradients. Reducing the voxel size also means Artifacts due to head motion should be
that more voxels are needed to cover the entire reduced as much as possible during image acqui-
brain, which requires more time (i.e., a longer sition using three techniques. First, the patient
TR), resulting in lower temporal resolution. should be made as comfortable as possible with
Fortunately, these limitations are becoming less cushions for support and blankets if needed for
important with the advent of parallel imaging and warmth. Second, freedom of head movement
continued improvements in gradient speed. with the head array coil should be reduced with
comfortable pads at the temples and perhaps tape
across the forehead. In our experience, highly
Geometric Distortion and Motion restrictive devices such as bite bars and rigid
Artifacts molded head pillows can induce anxiety and dis-
comfort and are usually counterproductive,
EPI images typically show varying degrees of though some centers use these routinely. Finally,
geometric distortion or “warping” of the image the patient should be encouraged before each
due to underlying field inhomogeneity and other scan to relax the body, especially the shoulders
8 Functional MRI in the Presurgical Epilepsy Evaluation 173

and neck, and should receive verbal feedback on speakers, which are available in a small in-ear
the degree of movement that occurred in the pre- format (http://www.stax.co.jp/index-E.html).
vious scan, if this information is available from These devices do not yet provide any passive
an online analysis tool. attenuation and so must be used with an over-the-
Noise due to head motion can be reduced dur- ear muff.
ing post-processing using 3-dimensional image Manual responses can be recorded with button
registration. A variety of tools are available for or key-press boxes, joysticks, and other devices.
this purpose, such as the “3dvolreg” (rigid-body Several technologies are available for operation
alignment) or “3dAllineate” (nonlinear warping) within a strong magnetic field, and all work well.
routines in AFNI (www.afni.nimh.nih.gov/pub/ Vocal responses can also be recorded during
dist/doc/program_help/3dvolreg.html). In addi- scanning. The best device for this purpose is an
tion to registering the successive images in an adaptive noise-canceling microphone designed
fMRI time series to each other, these programs specifically for MRI use (http://www.optoacous-
produce a text file containing the estimated move- tics.com/medical/fomri-iii). In our experience
ment that occurred at each time point, relative to this device is easy to use and provides good qual-
the base image, in terms of three translation and ity recordings even during continuous EPI image
three rotation parameters. These movement vec- acquisition.
tors can be used later as noise covariates when A number of software products are available
modeling the observed time series, an approach for controlling stimulus presentation and logging
that in our experience improves the sensitivity input from response devices (Table 8.1). These
and specificity of the activation maps (Johnstone, vary in cost, ease of use, reliability, capabilities,
Ores Walsh, et al., 2006; Morgan, Dawant, et al., and availability of technical support, and all have
2007). relative strengths and drawbacks.

Stimulus and Task Apparatus General Principles of Task Design


and Timing
A variety of MRI-compatible stimulus presenta-
tion and response recording devices are available A great variety of tasks and timing schemes have
for fMRI. Visual stimuli are typically presented been used successfully in fMRI studies. Some
with a video projector and a screen placed at the aspects of task selection for language and verbal
foot of the patient or via goggles containing min- memory paradigms are discussed in a later sec-
iature LCD displays. A variety of headphones are tion. In designing paradigms for functional map-
available for audio. The least expensive of these ping in individuals, it is critical to bear in mind
operate by air conduction of sound through a that fMRI measurements contain significant
length of plastic tubing, which is inserted into the amounts of “noise” and that signal averaging is
subject’s ear canal at one end and attached at the necessary to produce stable maps (Desmond &
other end to a small speaker at some distance Glover, 2002; Huettel & McCarthy, 2001). Many
from the magnet. These devices generally include clinicians overemphasize the need to keep scan-
either an ear plug or an external muff for passive ning sessions short because of concerns over
attenuation of the scanner noise. The main limita- patient fatigue or (more likely) scanning costs.
tion of this approach is the severe loss of high- Radiology practitioners are often accustomed to
frequency sounds during air conduction through running structural MRI protocols that last no
the tube, which can only be partly corrected by more 30 min, whereas a high-quality fMRI study
boosting these frequencies at the source and generally requires much more time, particularly
which results in a somewhat muffled sound with if several activation protocols are attempted in
speech stimuli. Much higher-quality audio is the same session. In our experience, almost all
available using MRI-compatible electrostatic adolescent and adult neurology patients easily
174 S.J. Swanson et al.

Table 8.1 Software for fMRI stimulus presentation and response timing
Name and vendor/source Free OS Ease Satisfaction
E-Prime (Psychology Software Tools) No Win Good High
DMDX (http://www.u.arizona.edu/~kforster/dmdx/dmdx.htm) Yes Win Fair High
Presentation (http://www.neuro-bs.com) No Win Low Fair
Psyscope X (http://psy.ck.sissa.it) Yes Mac Good High
Psyscope Classic (http://psyscope.psy.cmu.edu) Yes Mac Classic Good Low
ERTS (www.erts.edu) No MS-DOS Good Low
SuperLab (www.superlab.com) No Mac, Win Good Low
Matlab (www.mathworks.com) No All Low Fair

tolerate an hour in the scanner, and most have no “Event related” or “single trial” refers to a
problem with a 90-min session. As a general rule design in which trials are not blocked by condi-
of thumb, we advise a minimum of 5 min of total tion. This approach is more appropriate when a
scanning (summed across blocks or trial events) single task set is maintained throughout a scan,
for each condition of an fMRI contrast. For a and the focus is on bottom-up effects of particu-
simple design contrasting two conditions, this lar stimulus characteristics. Some well-known
means a minimum of 10 min of scanning, whereas examples include passive listening to speech vs.
a complex, factorial, or parametric design might reversed speech, naming animal vs. tool object
require much more. We emphasize this aspect of pictures, lexical decision on words vs. pseudo-
protocol design because it is undeniable that words, recognition testing on novel vs. previ-
increasing the sample size (i.e., number of data ously encoded words, grammaticality judgments
points) significantly improves power, sensitivity, on active vs. passive sentences, etc. Event-related
and reliability in fMRI studies, and underpow- designs minimize top-down attention, arousal,
ered results are a waste of effort and resources and “cognitive strategy” differences between
that can only lead to substandard clinical care. conditions because the random intermixing of
The term “block design” refers to the group- stimuli from different conditions makes it impos-
ing of trials in a particular condition into blocks. sible for the subject to anticipate the type of stim-
The blocks can all be of equal length or vary ran- ulus coming next. Differences in attention and
domly in length. In general, blocks should not be arousal can be important confounds in block
longer than about 30 s, as the noise in fMRI is design studies and also lead to differences
dominated by low frequencies, which can reduce between conditions in the degree of “mind wan-
SNR if the frequency of the block alternation dering” that occurs. It is frequently said that
becomes too low (Aguirre & D'Esposito, 1999). event-related designs are less sensitive than block
The main use of block designs is for contrasts designs because blocking elicits a larger change
between “task sets,” for example, a semantic in the BOLD signal. One should keep in mind,
decision task and a phonological decision task. In however, that some of this larger change in
such cases, the stimuli used in the contrasting BOLD signal could be due to nonspecific atten-
conditions are often matched on a number of tion and arousal differences associated with
variables, and the focus is on the top-down effects blocking rather than to the effects of interest.
of altering attention to different aspects of the Event-related designs also allow trials to be
stimuli or retrieving different kinds of informa- sorted or modeled according to performance
tion associated with the stimuli. In such experi- measures such as accuracy or reaction time.
ments the stimuli are blocked to reduce the These parameters can be entered into the analysis
amount of task switching needed and to allow the model to identify the neural correlates of,
subject to settle fully into each task state. for example, a correct vs. incorrect response, a
8 Functional MRI in the Presurgical Epilepsy Evaluation 175

remembered vs. forgotten word, or a long vs. during scanning, such as in difficult auditory per-
short RT. A final advantage of event-related ceptual tasks or when high-quality vocal record-
designs is in experiments that use an overt vocal ings are needed. Clustered acquisition refers to
response, such as naming or reading tasks. Vocal grouping the acquisition of the 2-D slices com-
responses produce significant movement artifact prising each volume as closely in time as possible
in the BOLD signal time course. However, (typically 2 s or less) and separating these vol-
because the hemodynamic response to a single ume acquisitions by a silent period. The silent
trial occurs later than the artifact from a spoken period can be relatively brief, though the 4–5-s
response, the trial-by-trial modeling of the hemo- delay to the peak of the hemodynamic response
dynamic response in event-related designs can should be kept in mind to optimize image acqui-
distinguish these two sources of signal change. sition at the peak of the response rather than the
The analysis of event-related fMRI data trough. A longer silent period can be used to min-
assumes a linear summation of hemodynamic imize contamination of the fMRI data by neural
responses from adjacent trials. This assumption responses to the scanner noise, which is espe-
appears to be reasonable as long as trials are not cially useful for mapping early auditory cortex
too closely spaced (Buckner & Braver, 1999). (Humphries, Liebenthal, et al., 2010).
The spacing of trials depends to some degree on
the hypothesized timing of neural events. For a
low-level perceptual stimulus such as a flash of Some Principles of Data Analysis
light or brief tone, the neural events of interest
might be over within a few hundred ms, and trials The general approach to analysis of fMRI data is
can be separated by as little as 1 s or so. With to model the effects, over time, of one or more
more complex stimuli and tasks, such as naming experimental variables (or conditions) on each
a picture, the neural events might last well over voxel’s BOLD signal, typically with a multivari-
1 s, and trials should be separated by a longer able general linear regression model. A beta
interval. As a general rule of thumb, space the coefficient and statistical parameter are returned
trials by at least twice the average RT on the task. for each variable in the model at each voxel loca-
Varying or “jittering” the interstimulus interval tion, resulting in a 3-D array of values known as
on a pseudorandom schedule is also helpful for a statistical parametric map. The beta coefficient
statistical separation of adjacent hemodynamic is a response amplitude index that reflects the
responses during the analysis. Several freely amount of change in the BOLD signal, relative to
available tools exist for designing statistically baseline, that can be attributed to the correspond-
optimal stimulus and ISI sequences for event- ing experimental condition. Planned contrasts
related fMRI, such as Optseq (http://surfer.nmr. can be conducted comparing the size of the
mgh.harvard.edu/optseq/) and the RSFgen tool in response between conditions. A uniform thresh-
AFNI. old is typically applied across all voxels to depict
A final type of design involves “sparse” or the areas of “significant” activation.
“clustered” acquisition of images (Hall, Haggard, Thresholding of activation maps is a complex
et al., 1999). Image volumes in fMRI are col- and contentious issue, particularly for clinical
lected initially as a sequence of adjacent applications. The purpose of thresholding is to
2-dimensional slices, which are later combined to highlight regions of strong or reliable activation.
construct image volumes for each TR. By default, A traditional alpha threshold of p < 0.05 is often
most fMRI sequences distribute the collection of adopted, though because statistical tests are typi-
the 2-D slices evenly throughout each TR. The cally performed on thousands of voxels, a correc-
rapid gradient switching necessary for spatial tion for multiple comparisons is necessary to
encoding in fMRI creates a loud acoustic noise avoid an unacceptable level of false-positive
burst during each 2-D acquisition. In some set- results. Bonferroni correction and similar meth-
tings it is desirable to create periods of silence ods are overly conservative, and a standard
176 S.J. Swanson et al.

Bonferroni correction for, say, 10,000 voxels (a be less variable from session to session than
typical number for the whole brain) would significance values and is far less dependent on
require a p threshold of 0.000005. A common sample size (Voyvodic, 2006). At the moment,
alternative method is to combine a more lenient much more research is needed to define optimal
voxel p threshold, say p < 0.001, with a cluster methods for thresholding in presurgical mapping
size threshold (Forman, Cohen, et al., 1995). applications.
That is, after thresholding at p < 0.001, clusters of For most clinical applications, the focus is on
surviving voxels are eliminated if they do not activations in individual subjects. It is common
exceed a minimum size. The rationale for this in research studies, however, to construct “aver-
approach is that false-positive voxels are unlikely age” group maps by combining beta coefficients
by chance to form large contiguous clusters. (or other similar measures of response ampli-
Methods are available in most software packages tude) across subjects after spatial normalization.
for determining the probability of a cluster sur- Spatial normalization refers to fitting each sub-
viving any given combination of voxel-wise p ject’s image data to a common spatial template,
threshold and cluster size threshold, using either which can be done using several methods. The
random field theory (e.g., SPM) or randomization simplest approach is a multiparameter linear
testing (AFNI). fitting of the volume to a template brain. Other
There are potential problems with standard volumetric methods use nonlinear deformation
thresholding approaches in clinical contexts. algorithms, which can dramatically improve
Thresholds are inevitably arbitrary, and it is not at the precision of matching to a template (Klein,
all clear what threshold is optimal for valid sepa- Andersson, et al., 2009). Other approaches proj-
ration of “clinically meaningful” from “clinically ect the volumetric data onto a 2-dimensional
unimportant” activation. For presurgical map- surface model of the subject’s cortex and use
ping, we advocate use of relatively lenient thresh- nonlinear warping methods to align the subject’s
olds to avoid false-negative results, with clear surface to a template surface (Fischl, Sereno,
color coding of the values to indicate relative lev- et al., 1999). None of these methods can per-
els of significance. Another problem arises from fectly align brain structures from different sub-
the nature of statistical measurements. Standard jects, however, nor are the relative locations of
statistical values such as z or t scores depend both functional areas expected to be exactly the same
on the amplitude of a signal and on measurement across subjects. Therefore it is necessary in the
noise. Measurement noise arises from many creation of group maps to spatially smooth indi-
sources, including head movement, respiratory vidual activation maps to maximize the spatial
and cardiac aliasing, and hardware fluctuations, summation of activation. Smoothing is typically
and the level of noise can vary considerably performed using a 3-D Gaussian kernel. The
between subjects and even across scans in the degree of smoothing is typically in the range
same subject. This variation results in large varia- of 3–8 mm full width at half maximum, with
tions in the overall distribution of observed statis- smaller values appropriate when higher spatial
tical values and thus the proportion of voxels that resolution is desired.
survive thresholding. In our experience, it is not Several free, well-supported software pack-
uncommon to see over a tenfold difference ages are available for fMRI data analysis. SPM
between subjects in the number of voxels that sur- (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/) is the most
vive standard thresholding. As discussed above, widely used and is popular because of its ease of
measurement noise is suppressed by repeated use and broad range of capabilities, including a
averaging, but there are practical limits to this. For full range of preprocessing and statistical tools,
presurgical mapping, it may be that the amplitude several methods for volumetric spatial normal-
of the response (e.g., beta coefficient or percent ization, advanced connectivity modeling tools,
signal change) is more relevant than its statistical and extensions for EEG and MEG data. SPM
significance. Response amplitude also appears to requires MATLAB software to be installed for
8 Functional MRI in the Presurgical Epilepsy Evaluation 177

Fig. 8.2 Algorithm for


determining when to
conduct fMRI or Wada
testing

full functionality (http://www.mathworks.com/ method prior to surgery. Figure 8.2 provides an


products/matlab/index.html). AFNI (http://afni. algorithm for making this decision. This algo-
nimh.nih.gov/afni) is also widely used and pow- rithm is based on the empirical data supporting
erful, with a full range of preprocessing, para- three contentions: (1) fMRI is suitable for map-
metric and nonparametric statistical tools, and a ping language (Binder, 2011), (2) fMRI predicts
range of normalization options. AFNI empha- both language and memory outcome after ATL
sizes flexibility with its open architecture, multi- more accurately than Wada (Binder, Sabsevitz,
ple line-command subprograms, and support for et al., 2008; Sabsevitz et al., 2003), and (3) there
plug-ins. AFNI also provides support for surface- is no empirical data showing that fMRI can pre-
based analysis through its SUMA module. AFNI dict or prevent amnesia. In situations where there
requires no additional software. FSL (http:// is a risk for developing an amnestic syndrome
www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/) is a suite of modules (e.g., evidence of temporal lobe pathology contra-
that cover a range of standard fMRI analysis lateral to the side of seizure focus), a Wada alone
steps and also includes advanced tools for seg- or in addition to an fMRI is recommended.
mentation of structural MRI volumes, image Using this algorithm, if a patient has an
unwarping and normalization, and analysis of implanted metal device or cognitive dysfunction
diffusion tensor imaging data. SPM, AFNI, and that precludes fMRI task compliance, the patient
FSL all run on multiple operating system plat- should be referred for Wada testing. If the patient
forms, and all use the common NIFTI image for- has evidence of pathology contralateral to a
mat, allowing flexible combinations of modules left-sided seizure focus such as hippocampal
and subprograms from different packages to be sclerosis, contralateral epileptiform activity, tem-
used on the same data. poral lesion, or atrophy (thereby increasing risk
for amnesia after left ATL), a Wada should be
conducted. If there is evidence of pathology con-
Patient Selection tralateral to a right-sided seizure focus, an fMRI
can be conducted. However, if the fMRI shows
The first step in conducting clinical functional right language dominance in a patient with a right
neuroimaging is determining who should have seizure focus and contralateral (left) pathology, a
fMRI and Wada or more than one mapping Wada should be conducted as well.
178 S.J. Swanson et al.

Participation in cognitive fMRI studies typi- safety committee. Patients with vagal nerve stim-
cally requires a higher level of cognitive func- ulators can undergo anatomical imaging if the
tioning and sustained attention than does device is turned off, but VNS has not been
participation in Wada testing. Individuals with approved for fMRI pulse sequences at either 1.5
intellectual functioning below full-scale IQ of 70, or 3 T.
slow processing speed, or severe inattention
likely will have difficulty performing most tasks
developed for fMRI. Task instructions can be Clinical Use of fMRI by
modified, and rate of administration of stimuli Neuropsychologists: CPT Codes
can be slowed to be suitable for children down to
the age of 10 and adults with cognitive impair- The fundamental difference between functional
ment. For example, with a semantic decision and anatomical imaging is the application of cog-
task, patients are asked to press a button for ani- nitive tasks during the imaging procedure. The
mal names that are both found in the United proper execution of fMRI requires a multidisci-
States and used by humans (Binder, Swanson, plinary team including those knowledgeable in
et al., 1996). The semantic decision task instruc- neuropsychology, radiology, biophysics, statis-
tions can be simplified to just one decision, “ani- tics, and neuroanatomy. The cognitive tasks con-
mals that are used by humans.” For children the ducted during fMRI require expertise in the
instructions may be modified to “animals that development of psychological tests by behavioral
have four legs” or “animals with fur.” Individuals neurologists or neuropsychologists to optimally
with verbal intellectual abilities below full-scale probe cognitive systems of interest, and for the
IQ of 70 typically will not have the semantic development of control or contrast tasks that
knowledge or attentional capacity to perform eliminate unwanted or nonessential activation.
above chance on semantic decision tasks. Those Neuropsychologists with training in functional
with severely impaired processing speed or work- neuroanatomy and psychometric principles are
ing memory cannot keep pace with the rate of ideally suited for developing tasks and conduct-
presentation of stimuli or lose track of the num- ing fMRI since the ability to produce valid maps
ber of targets presented. When performance falls of higher cognitive functions requires an under-
below chance, the validity of the activation pat- standing of the function of interest as well as the
terns should be questioned. In such instances, nonessential cognitive processing subcompo-
these patients may be better served by Wada test- nents that may be activated by the task.
ing if a suitable task is not available. Testing the Neuropsychologists conducting fMRI bill medi-
patient on the task outside of the scanner prior to cal insurance (since in the case of epilepsy sur-
scheduling an fMRI will allow the examiner to gery candidates the diagnosis of epilepsy is
avoid the expense of wasted scanner time. medical) using CPT code 96020. This code cov-
Subject factors other than level of cognitive ers testing of language, memory, cognition,
functioning that affect the quality of the imaging motor skills, and other neurological functions in
data include head motion, age, body habitus association with fMRI. The code descriptor from
(morbid obesity, head size, cranial vault abnor- CPT 2009 for 96020 specifies “neurofunctional
malities, neck length), motivational state, adop- testing selection and administration during non-
tion of idiosyncratic task strategies, and task invasive imaging functional brain mapping, with
performance (Hammeke, Bellgowan, et al., 2000; test administered entirely by a physician or psy-
Weber, Wellmer, et al., 2006). For patients with chologist, with review of test results and report.”
implanted metal devices, it is necessary to have An official position statement was published
the vendor and part/serial number of the medical by the Division of Clinical Neuropsychology
device from the physician. Information on device (Division 40 of the American Psychological
safety for 1.5- and 3-T scanners can be obtained Association) on the role of neuropsychologists in
from www.MRIsafety.com or from the local MRI the clinical use of fMRI (2004). In addition, the
8 Functional MRI in the Presurgical Epilepsy Evaluation 179

recommended training for neuropsychologists Tasks used during fMRI can be classified as
engaged in functional neuroimaging has been passive or active, overt or covert (Binder,
described (Bobholz, Rao, et al., 2007). A physi- Swanson, et al., 2008). The ideal task is active
cian, often a radiologist or behavioral neurolo- and overt (allowing behavioral monitoring so that
gist, bills 70555, and the neuropsychologist bills performance variables can be examined) and has
96020 for (1) administering tests of higher cog- a perceptual control or contrast task that is
nitive functioning such as memory, attention, lan- matched for difficulty and performance accuracy,
guage, and executive functioning, (2) instructing thereby eliminating all sensory and other pro-
and practicing with the patient prior to the scan- cesses unrelated to the cognitive function of
ning, (3) adapting the tests as necessary based on interest. Word generation tasks (e.g., generating a
the patient’s behavioral performance, (4) moni- verb or series of verbs in response to a noun or
toring behavioral response performance during generating words in response to phonemic or cat-
the scanning, (5) collaborating with physicians egorical semantic cues) are commonly used for
on test findings, and (6) generating a clinical language mapping. While word generation is
report. considered an active task, it is typically con-
ducted covertly leading to uncertainty about task
compliance and often is contrasted with rest.
fMRI Tasks Another drawback of silent word generation
tasks is that activation has been observed pre-
The validity of fMRI maps critically depends on dominantly in frontal areas (Benson, FitzGerald,
the activation protocol since poorly designed et al., 1999), and concordance between fMRI and
probe or contrast tasks can lead to both false- Wada has been found in frontal but not temporal
positive and false-negative mapping errors. A regions of interest (Lehericy, Cohen, et al., 2000),
variety of stimuli have been used to induce lan- making it unlikely that this data could ever be
guage processing. These include auditory non- used for tailoring temporal resections.
speech (e.g., pure tones), auditory phonemes While there are no universally agreed upon
(speech sounds, pseudowords), visual nonletter tasks for language and memory mapping, some
(false font), visual nonpronounceable letter common methodological pitfalls can be avoided.
strings (FKJVB), visual pseudowords with ortho- These pitfalls are (1) using rest, visual fixation, or
graphic and phonological features of real words a non-perceptually matched contrast task during
(e.g., SNADE), visual and auditory words and language and memory activation paradigms, (2)
sentences, and visual objects. Tasks may require failing to incorporate behavior monitoring, and
phonetic decision (detecting rhymes or sylla- (3) attempting to image memory without consid-
bles), orthographic decision (letter identification, ering that episodic memory encoding occurs
case matching), and semantic decision (requiring continuously.
decisions based on stimulus meaning). Sentence When attempting to image higher cognitive
reading and story listening may require no deci- functions such as language and memory, it is not
sion but automatically elicit complex semantic possible to find a task that activates the function
processing. The various language tasks place dif- of interest and no extraneous or more elementary
ferent demands with respect to semantic search functions in the process. The contrast between
and lexical retrieval (e.g., naming, word genera- the component functions subsumed by the tasks
tion). Different combinations of task contrasts should be theoretically selected to isolate the
will result in different patterns of activation based cognitive function of interest. For the purpose of
on the subtraction analysis. See Binder (2009) for concisely describing “how to” conduct fMRI in
a complete review of the regions in which robust epilepsy surgery patients, the present chapter will
activations are typically observed in association focus on a well-documented semantic decision
with various task contrasts for language mapping task that has been shown to be reliable (Binder,
(Binder, 2009). Hammeke, et al., 2001), concordant with the
180 S.J. Swanson et al.

Table 8.2 Functional components of two language acti- 2006; McKiernan, Kaufman, et al., 2003). In fact,
vation tasks
recent studies capitalize on resting state coher-
Semantic monitoring task ence, the activation and connectivity between
Tone Semantic neurons that occur at rest as a measure of the
Functional components discrimination decision
default mode network with both fMRI and MEG
Semantic processing +
(de Pasquale, Della Penna, et al., 2010; Fox &
Phonetic processing +
Raichle, 2007; Liu, Fukunaga, et al., 2010; Smith,
Attention, working memory + +
Fox, et al., 2009). These widely distributed rest-
Auditory processing + +
ing state networks closely resemble those evoked
Motor response + +
by cognitive tasks. To further emphasize this
point, a recent study compared maps obtained
using the semantic decision-tone decision (per-
Wada test (Binder, Swanson, et al., 1996), accu- ceptual control) task contrast to activation maps
rate in representing language lateralization across obtained with a semantic decision/rest contrast
a continuum from left to right dominance (Binder, Swanson, et al., 2008). Figure 8.3 shows
(Springer, Binder, et al., 1999), and predictive of that there were fewer areas of activation seen
language (Sabsevitz, et al., 2003) and verbal when language is contrasted with rest than when
memory outcome after left ATL resection language is contrasted with a perceptual control
(Binder, Sabsevitz, et al., 2008). task, presumably because language that occurs
Table 8.2 shows the theoretical underpinning automatically during rest has been subtracted out.
for the probe and contrast conditions used in the This reduces the sensitivity of the language
semantic decision task (Binder, Frost, et al., protocol such that critical language areas can be
1997; Démonet, Chollet, et al., 1992). In this missed.
paradigm, the probe task involves pressing a but- Figure 8.4 shows the overlap (in green) of
ton in response to animal names that are both areas activated in the language/perceptual control
“found in the United States” and “used by and language/rest contrasts and the areas removed
humans.” This is contrasted in a block design in a typical ATL resection for epilepsy (red).
with a tone decision task. Subjects hear a brief There is very little language activation within the
series of tones of varying pitch and are instructed typical resection volume when rest is used as a
to press a button in response to tone trains con- contrast task. Activation observed in the left
taining two high tones. The semantic and tone angular gyrus, posterior cingulate gyrus, left
decision tasks were matched for stimulus inten- medial frontal lobe, and left medial temporal lobe
sity and duration, trial duration, and frequency of during semantic decision contrasted with tone
targets. The contrast task is nonlinguistic and decision disappeared when semantic decision
controls for auditory, motor, executive, and work- was contrasted with rest. If maps derived using
ing memory functions that are not specific to lan- rest as a control task are used for surgical plan-
guage. E-Prime scripts (Psychology Software ning, a poor cognitive outcome could result.
Tools, www.pstnet.com) for teaching and run- An alternative language task is an auditory
ning the tasks in the scanner are available from responsive naming task that requires a spoken
the authors on request. response. Clustered acquisition techniques (scan-
Rest or visual fixation does not provide an ning immediately after each spoken response)
optimal baseline condition for language or mem- can be used to allow patients to speak in the scan-
ory studies (Stark & Squire, 2001) and is known ner so that imaging of language production sys-
to be an active state characterized by uncon- tems can be conducted. Such tasks should be
strained conceptual processing or spontaneous contrasted with a control task that also includes
neural activity during which self-initiated lin- an oral motor response if the aim is to image
guistic and semantic processes occur (Binder, expressive language rather than the oral motor
Frost, et al., 1999; McKiernan, D'Angelo, et al., apparatus. Normative data has been published for
8 Functional MRI in the Presurgical Epilepsy Evaluation 181

Fig. 8.3 Activation patterns from 30 health participants include angular gyrus, posterior cingulate gyrus, medial
obtained for contrasts between (a) tone decision versus and ventral frontal lobe, and ventral temporal lobe. In (b)
rest, (b) semantic decision versus rest, and (c) semantic semantic decision is contrasted with “rest.” Note that the
decision versus tone decision. The maps show group acti- default network regions in (a) show relatively little activa-
vations thresholded at whole-brain corrected p < 0.05. The tion, indicating equivalent BOLD signals during the
left side of the brain is on the reader’s left. In (a) tone mon- semantic decision task and the resting state. In (c) semantic
itoring was contrasted with “rest” and shows regions in decision is contrasted with tone monitoring. Strong left-
blue that are more active during the resting state (“default lateralized activation is observed throughout the default
network”) than during this nonlinguistic task. These network, inferior frontal lobe, and temporal lobe

Fig. 8.4 Amount of activation within (green) and outside (blue) the typical resection volume (red) when semantic deci-
sion is contrasted with rest (top panel) and a perceptual control task (bottom panel)
182 S.J. Swanson et al.

this auditory responsive naming or definition semantic decision task (Weber et al., 2006), there
naming task (Hammeke, Kortenkamp, et al., was no significant difference in the LIs for the
2005). In this task, subjects are provided with an group with the best performance compared to
auditory cue such as “jewelry for the finger” or patients with the worst performance. It was con-
“first president of the United States” which elicits cluded that the fMRI language LIs were indepen-
a verbal response of a common or proper, living, dent of task performance though behavioral
or nonliving noun. The contrast task involves the monitoring is necessary to ensure task compli-
presentation of brief sequences of noise filtered ance and that the patients are performing above
for high band frequencies matching the same chance levels.
spectrum of human speech. A rising pitch was
added to some stimuli (the targets). Patients are
asked to say aloud the number of target stimuli. Episodic Memory
While this task requires a spoken response, the
definition naming task activates cerebral regions Attempts to conduct fMRI of memory are fraught
associated with both language production and with methodological issues since episodic mem-
“receptive” language semantic system because it ory is always “on” making selection of contrast
requires comprehension of the semantic cue. states difficult. Investigators have compared
The LI from the definition naming task signifi- encoding of stimuli to recognition of stimuli
cantly correlates with the Wada language score without considering that during the recognition
and the LI from semantic decision (Larson, trial, the foils are being encoded along with the
Hammeke, et al., 2004). One advantage of the second exposure to the previously viewed targets.
definition naming task is that it produces greater In this case, the contrast task has subtracted out a
activation of the anterior temporal lobes, propor- good deal of the function of interest. An event-
tionally greater on the left side. The finding of related memory task that compares stimuli that
greater anterior temporal activation associated are later recalled or recognized outside the scan-
with naming to an auditory cue is consistent with ner to targets that were not later recognized is one
previous extra-operative grid mapping studies method for avoiding this problem.
showing a greater number of anterior temporal Another method for avoiding the confound of
“hits” with auditory compared to visual naming being unable to “turn off” episodic memory is to
stimuli (Hamberger, McClelland, et al., 2007). contrast stimuli that are highly encodable (e.g.,
Finally, some investigators have advocated complex scenes) with stimuli that are poorly
use of multiple language tasks or a task panel to recalled (e.g., randomly retiled images of the
improve concordance with the Wada (Gaillard, scenes) (Binder, Bellgowan, et al., 2005). However,
Balsamo, et al., 2004). When using a combina- since verbal memory decline occurs with much
tion of language tasks, one can examine areas of greater frequency after left ATL (Baxendale &
activation overlap across tasks which reduces the Thompson, 2005; Baxendale, Thompson, et al.,
likelihood of false-positive activated voxels. 2006; Bell, Lin, et al., 2011; Chelune, 1995; Dulay,
Levin, et al., 2009; Leunen, Caroff, et al., 2009;
Martin, Sawrie, et al., 1998; Naugle, Chelune,
Behavioral Monitoring et al., 1993; Sabsevitz, Swanson, et al., 2001) than
nonverbal memory decline occurs after right ATL
Behavioral monitoring during mapping of higher (Dulay et al., 2009; Leunen et al., 2009), the opti-
cognitive functions helps ensure task compliance mal memory task for presurgical epilepsy patients
and allows one to compare accuracy (difficulty) is one that results in maximal activation in the left
between the probe and contrast task. However, a hippocampus or left temporal lobe. Nonverbal
high rate of response accuracy does not seem to tasks like Roland’s Hometown Walking Task
be critical for obtaining valid fMRI data. In an (Roland, Eriksson, et al., 1987) and scene encod-
fMRI language study of 195 epilepsy patients ing tasks (Avila, Barros-Loscertales, et al., 2006;
with vastly different performance levels on a Bellgowan, Binder, et al., 1998; Szaflarski,
8 Functional MRI in the Presurgical Epilepsy Evaluation 183

Holland, et al., 2004; Vannest, Szaflarski, et al., lobe activation seem to be most useful for
2008) are more likely to produce bilateral hippo- predicting verbal memory decline after left
campal activation, correlate with Wada memory ATL. Nonverbal memory rarely declines and
scores (Jokeit, Okujava, et al., 2001; Rabin, tasks that produce bilateral activation are better
Narayan, et al., 2004), and are associated with side suited for determining side of seizure focus. Both
of seizure focus (Bellgowan et al., 1998; Jokeit structural and functional imaging studies support
et al., 2001). However, tasks that produce bihip- the notion that obtaining information about func-
pocampal activation are less useful for predicting tional adequacy of the hippocampus ipsilateral to
verbal memory decline than semantic tasks with the resection is most predictive of the type of
verbally encodable information. In fact, preopera- memory morbidity associated with left ATL.
tive language lateralization was correlated with
postoperative verbal memory decline (Binder,
Swanson, et al., 2010; Labudda, Mertens, et al., Clinical Applications of fMRI
2010), while hippocampal activation during a in Epilepsy Surgery Candidates
scene encoding task which tends to produce bilat-
eral hippocampal activation was not related to ver- Introduction
bal memory outcome (Binder, Swanson, et al.,
2010). However, activation asymmetries during There are a variety of clinical applications for fMRI
scene encoding related to side of seizure focus and in epilepsy surgery candidates (Bookheimer, 1996;
Wada memory asymmetry scores (Binder, Detre, 2004; Powell & Duncan, 2005). These
Swanson, et al., 2010). include localization of motor and sensory areas
Thus, risk for verbal memory decline is related (Bookheimer, 1996), lateralization and localization
to lateralization of semantic material-specific of language (for reviews see Bookheimer, 2007;
networks, and it appears that hippocampal ade- Swanson, Sabsevitz, et al., 2007), determination of
quacy (degree to which there is functional tissue hemispheric representation of memory functions
ipsilateral to the seizure focus) is more important (Binder, Swanson, et al., 2010; Detre, Maccotta,
than functional reserve (capacity of the hippo- et al., 1998; Golby, Poldrack, et al., 2002; Koylu,
campus contralateral to the seizure focus to sub- Trinka, et al., 2006; Powell, Koepp, et al., 2004;
serve memory). This thinking is in line with Powell, Richardson, et al., 2007; Szaflarski et al.,
studies and reviews showing that the degree of 2004; Vannest et al., 2008), predicting side of sei-
ipsilateral hippocampal sclerosis is a strong pre- zure focus (Bellgowan et al., 1998; Jokeit et al.,
dictor of verbal memory outcome after left ATL 2001), and predicting outcome after temporal
(Chelune, 1995; Hermann, Wyler, et al., 1992, lobectomy with regard to seizures (Killgore,
1994a, 1994b; Seidenberg, Hermann, et al., 1996; Glosser, et al., 1999), language (Sabsevitz et al.,
Trenerry, Jack, et al., 1993) and a better predictor 2003), and memory (Binder, Sabsevitz, et al., 2008;
than the functional reserve of the contralateral Labudda et al., 2010; Powell et al., 2008; Rabin
hippocampus. Also consistent with the functional et al., 2004; Richardson, Strange, et al., 2004). The
adequacy model, verbal memory decline was present chapter focuses on the primary clinical role
predicted by fMRI activation in the dominant but for fMRI in the presurgical work-up which is lan-
not the nondominant hippocampus (Powell et al., guage and memory mapping for the purpose of
2008). In this study, pictures, faces, and words predicting cognitive outcome.
were presented visually during scanning. Images
later recognized were examined in an event-
related analysis. Using this paradigm, greater Steps for Conducting Clinical fMRI
ipsilateral activation in response to words was
associated with greater verbal memory decline Since preoperative language and memory abili-
after dominant ATL. ties along with age at seizure onset are predictors
In conclusion, verbal semantic or verbal of postoperative cognitive decline (Baxendale
encoding tasks that maximize dominant temporal et al., 2006; Binder, Sabsevitz, et al., 2008;
184 S.J. Swanson et al.

Chelune, Naugle, et al., 1991; Hermann et al., scoring as well as cost. The SRT and Rey AVLT
1994a, 1994b; Sabsevitz et al., 2003), data from are available without cost. It is expected that an
neuropsychological testing is necessary to outcome prediction formula for the Rey AVLT
incorporate into fMRI regression formulas for will be developed in the near future. For the pres-
cognitive outcome prediction. Preoperative neu- ent, the SRT can be administered prior to surgery
ropsychological testing is recommended for all if precise verbal memory outcome predictions
epilepsy surgery candidates (Jones-Gotman, are desired.
Smith, et al., 2010) given the risk for cognitive All of the primary aims for presurgical lan-
morbidity in object naming and verbal memory guage and memory mapping including predicting
that occurs with left ATL. The National Institute cognitive morbidity can be accomplished with
of Neurological Disorders and Stroke Epilepsy the semantic decision task. Thus, for the purpose
Standards and Common Data Elements recom- of a description of “how to” conduct fMRI, we
mendations for neuropsychological test selection will describe the procedure using semantic deci-
can be viewed online at: http://www.commonda- sion for fMRI and using the Boston Naming Test
taelements.ninds.nih.gov/Doc/EPI/Assessments and Selective Reminding Test for pre- and post-
AndExaminations/Recommended Neuro-psychology operative testing.
Instruments Adult.doc. Declines in object nam- The algorithm outlined in Fig. 8.2 is applied to
ing occur in 25–60 % of patients who undergo determine if the patient is suitable for undergoing
left ATL (Bell, Davies, et al., 2000; Davies, Bell, fMRI. Patients are requested to arrive at the scan-
et al., 1998; Hermann, Perrine, et al., 1999; ner 1 h prior to their imaging session to complete
Langfitt & Rausch, 1996; Sabsevitz et al., 2003; the Edinburgh Handedness Questionnaire
Stafiniak, Saykin, et al., 1990). Similarly signifi- (Oldfield, 1971) and safety screening question-
cant verbal memory decline has been reported in naires and undergo pre-scan training on the cog-
30–60 % of patients who undergo left ATL nitive tasks. The patient is fitted with headphones,
(Baxendale et al., 2006; Binder, Sabsevitz, et al., padding is placed in the head coil to reduce head
2008; Lineweaver, Morris, et al., 2006; Naugle movement, the button box is positioned for use
et al., 1993; Sabsevitz et al., 2001; Stroup, with the nondominant hand, and the patient is
Langfitt, et al., 2003). provided with a squeeze ball which activates an
The battery of tests administered to epilepsy alarm system to use in case of emergency.
surgery candidates should include an auditory or Imaging can be conducted on a 1.5- or 3-T scan-
visual object naming test such as the Boston ner. High-resolution, T1-weighted anatomical
Naming Test (Kaplan, Goodglass, et al., 2001) reference images of the entire brain are acquired
available at Psychological Assessment Resources using a spoiled-gradient-echo sequence. Whole-
(PAR) and measures of verbal memory. While brain functional imaging using a T2*-weighted
story memory tasks are recommended for their gradient-echo and echoplanar sequence has the
face validity since much new learning is contex- following parameters at 1.5 T: TE = 40 ms,
tual, verbal list learning tasks are most sensitive TR = 3,000 ms, field of view = 240 mm, pixel
to decline after left ATL. A list learning task such matrix = 4 × 64, 19 sagittal slices, and voxel
as the Rey AVLT (Schmidt, 1996), Buschke size = 3.75 × 3.75 × 7 mm. Imaging parameters for
Selective Reminding Test (SRT) (Buschke, 1973; 3 T are TE = 25 ms, TR = 3000 ms, field of
Buschke & Fuld, 1974), or California Verbal view = 224, pixel matrix = 64 × 64, 34 axial slices,
Learning Test (CVLT) (Delis, Kramer, et al., and voxel size = 3.5 mm3.
2000) is recommended. The currently published The sound system is checked to ensure that
regression equation (Binder, Sabsevitz, et al., the patient can hear the stimuli over the scanner
2008) for predicting memory outcome using noise following the acquisition of the anatomical
fMRI is based on the SRT, but the Rey AVLT imaging. Real-time data is displayed showing
(Schmidt, 1996) is recommended in the Common head movement in multiple vectors within the
Data Elements for its ease of administration and scanner which can be used to provide feedback to
8 Functional MRI in the Presurgical Epilepsy Evaluation 185

the patient. Prior to beginning the semantic task, Table 8.3 Regression formulas for predicting naming
and verbal memory outcome with fMRI
the patient is briefly re-instructed on how to con-
duct the task and to remain motionless with eyes Predicted = Constant − β (preop score) − β
closed. Two runs are acquired using a block score (age at seizure onset) − β (fMRI LI)
BNT change = 13. 64 − 0.3334 (preop BNT) − 10.528
design with alternation between probe (semantic
(fMRI temporal LI)
decision) and contrast (tone decision) tasks. Both LTS change = 22.92 − 0.621 (preop LTS) − 0.304
tasks are active and both require a behavioral (age at onset) − 12.57 (fMRI lateral LI)
response. CLTR change = 17.67 − 0.704 (preop CLTR) − 0.280
Images are processed and statistical analyses (age at onset) − 12.1 (fMRI lateral LI)
are conducted using AFNI (http://afni.nimh.nih. Delayed = 3.76 − 0.688 (pre-delayed recall) − 0.093
gov/afni) for individual subjects by first aligning recall (age at onset) − 2.14 (fMRI lateral LI)
the images to reduce movement artifact. Multiple BNT Boston Naming Test, LTS long-term storage, CLTR
consistent long-term retrieval
regression is used to detect task-related MR sig-
nal changes. This method compares the time
series of MRI signal values in each image voxel variance in outcome above that predicted by the
with an idealized hemodynamic response to the temporal lobe ROI and preoperative BNT score.
task alternation. The idealized response is mod- Using a >2 standard deviation decline from the
eled by convolving a gamma function with a time largest decline in the R-ATL group to define poor
series of impulses representing each task trial. outcome, the fMRI temporal lobe LI showed
The regression model includes six movement 100 % sensitivity and 73 % specificity for a posi-
vectors to reduce the effects of head movement tive predictive value of 81 % for predicting poor
on the estimation of the task response. outcome with the LI threshold set at 0.25
Language lateralization is measured for five (Sabsevitz et al., 2003).
regions of interest: frontal, temporal, lateral Three memory scores derived from the SRT
angular, and whole hemisphere (minus the cere- can be predicted using the fMRI lateral ROI
bellum). Significantly activated voxels (uncor- LI. These include (1) long-term storage (LTS)
rected p < 0.001) are counted for each ROI on the which is any word that is recalled on two con-
left and right. Laterality indexes (LIs) represent secutive trials, (2) consistent long-term retrieval
the relative activation for each ROI based on the (CLTR) which is an index of the ability to consis-
differences between number of activated voxels tently retrieve the word (words recalled on every
in the left and right hemisphere where the LI for subsequent trial), and (3) delayed recall which is
each ROI = (L − R)/(L + R) (Binder, Frost, et al., the number of words freely recalled after a
1996; Binder, Sabsevitz, et al., 2008; Sabsevitz 30-min delay. The prediction formulas are pre-
et al., 2003; Springer et al., 1999). LIs range from sented in Table 8.3. Using these regression equa-
+1 (completely left hemisphere dominant) to −1 tions, preoperative score and age at onset of
(completely right hemisphere dominant). seizures accounted for 37, 49, and 54 % of the
variance in outcome on LTS, CLTR, and delayed
recall, respectively. The fMRI lateral LI added
Predicting Cognitive Outcome significantly to outcome prediction raising the
with Laterality Indexes amount of variance predicted to 48 % for LTS,
59 % for CLTR, and 61 % for delayed recall. The
Using the temporal ROI laterality index, the addition of Wada language LI or Wada memory
formula shown in Table 8.3 is used to determine asymmetry scores did not predict significant
predicted BNT decline from pre- to post-left additional variance beyond fMRI (Binder,
ATL. BNT outcome is predicted by preoperative Sabsevitz, et al., 2008).
raw score on BNT and the fMRI LI for the tem- In conclusion, greater lateralization toward
poral lobe. Age at seizure onset, while typically a the left hemisphere and higher preoperative
significant predictor of decline, did not predict scores predict more significant decline in object
186 S.J. Swanson et al.

naming and verbal memory after left ATL. In EEG/fMRI


addition, earlier age at seizure onset predicts less
memory decline. Using regression equations, In addition to using fMRI for mapping higher
predicted outcome scores can be obtained which cognitive functions, BOLD signal changes
are useful for counseling patients about potential associated with interictal epileptiform spikes can
cognitive morbidity. Measures of hippocampal be used for localizing epileptic foci (Al-Asmi,
sclerosis or volume eventually will need to be Benar, et al., 2003; Archer, Briellmann, et al., 2003;
incorporated into regression equations since Baudewig, Bittermann, et al., 2001; Benar,
studies show that structural status of the hippo- Aghakhani, et al., 2003; Jager, Werhahn, et al.,
campus is a strong predictor of verbal memory 2002; Krakow, Woermann, et al., 1999; Lazeyras,
outcome (Baxendale, van Paesschen, et al., 1998; Blanke, et al., 2000; Patel, Blum, et al., 1999;
Chelune, 1995; Hermann et al., 1992, 1994a, Symms, Allen, et al., 1999; Vulliemoz, Lemieux,
1994b; Seidenberg et al., 1996; Trenerry et al., et al., 2010; Warach, Ives, et al., 1996).
1993; Wendel, Trenerry, et al., 2001), Continuously recording EEG during scanning
allows detection of interictal epileptiform dis-
charges which can be used to trigger echoplanar
Tailoring Resections image acquisitions at a fixed time interval after
the spike. This allows for the imaging of hemo-
It is not clear whether activation maps from lan- dynamic correlates of EEG events. Multislice
guage protocols can be used to precisely guide data is typically acquired by having an epilep-
the resection boundaries since it has not been tologist manually trigger the scanner within
demonstrated that resection of activated voxels 3–5 s of spike detection. The data acquired
is correlated with language decline. There are within several seconds of triggering is within the
no published empirical studies that indicate that window for the hemodynamic response follow-
the fMRI maps can be used to tailor surgical ing spikes (Benar, Gross, et al., 2002).
resections for epilepsy patients. To obtain more Functional MRI activation associated with
robust anterior temporal activation within the interictal spikes correlates with other methods for
surgical zone, a semantically complex story lis- localizing spike generators including intracranial
tening task can be contrasted with a semanti- EEG recordings (Al-Asmi et al., 2003; Bagshaw,
cally shallow serial addition task (Binder, Gross, Aghakhani, et al., 2004; Detre, Sirven, et al.,
et al., 2010). The story task requires attentive 1995) and EEG dipole modeling (Lemieux,
listening to Aesop’s fables vignettes with con- Krakow, et al., 2001; Seeck, Michel, et al., 2001).
trolled word rate, speaking style, and prosodic One study examined seizure outcome after surgi-
features. Following the presentation of the cal resection in ten patients who underwent EEG-
vignette, the participant is given a forced two- triggered fMRI (Thornton, Laufs, et al., 2010).
choice alternative about the main theme of the Seven of the ten patients were seizure free after
story, with a required button press (left button surgery, and, of these, six of seven had the areas
for the first choice, right button for the second of maximal BOLD signal change that was “con-
choice). This task combination produces bilat- cordant” with the resection. In the three who
eral activation in lateral and medial temporal were not seizure free, EEG-correlated BOLD
lobes due to the semantic and conceptual pro- signal lay outside the resection.
cessing demands of the task. This task provides Technical issues and methodological limita-
more information about the activation in the sur- tions associated with conducting EEG spike-triggered
gical region of interest which can be used in fMRI include differences between the duration of
combination with the lateralization data from the hemodynamic response when compared to the
the semantic decision task. However, outcome time course for neuronal events (limited temporal
data is needed before these maps can be used to resolution), signal dropout in the temporal lobes
tailor resections. (Bagshaw et al., 2004), dependence on identification
8 Functional MRI in the Presurgical Epilepsy Evaluation 187

of spikes using scalp EEG, requirement that by studying the brain at rest (Fox & Raichle, 2007).
patients have frequent well-defined interictal While beyond the scope of this chapter, studies of
spikes, and uncertainty about whether the spikes connectivity in diseased brains or patients with epi-
are an indication of the “irritative zone” versus the lepsy hold great promise for determining the func-
“epileptogenic zone.” tional viability of the tissue around epileptogenic
foci (Bettus, Bartolomei, et al., 2010).

Resting State or Neuronal


Connectivity Studies fMRI Sample Report

Historically fMRI has been used to image brain See Fig. 8.5 for a sample report with tables
activation in response to a stimulus or correlated (behavioral data, LIs, predicted change scores) of
with a cognitive task. There has been an explosion an fMRI clinical report on a patient being consid-
of studies examining the “default network” or con- ered for left ATL. See Fig. 8.6 for the activation
nectivity between populations of neurons deter- maps associated LIs and predicted decline scores
mined by spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity described in the sample report.

Fig. 8.5 fMRI sample report


188 S.J. Swanson et al.

Fig. 8.5 (continued)


8 Functional MRI in the Presurgical Epilepsy Evaluation 189

Fig. 8.5 (continued)


190 S.J. Swanson et al.

Fig. 8.6 Language maps for the case described in the signal-to-fluctuation noise ratio (SFNR) map thresholded
sample report. The data are displayed in axial, coronal, to show in red areas with acceptable SFNR (>20). This
and sagittal sections using a DICOM image viewer that map allows clinicians to better judge the quality of the
allows the user to step through each volume in 1-mm data, especially in typical regions of signal dropout
increments using a slider. The image at lower right is a

Conclusions fMRI data and outcome predictions can be used


to inform professionals as they select surgical
Functional MRI for language mapping has candidates and to counsel patients about potential
become a vital part of the work-up for epilepsy cognitive risks prior to surgical intervention. In
surgery candidates and can be used as a replace- the future, it is anticipated that refinement in
ment for more invasive mapping methods such as functional imaging with fMRI, MEG (as dis-
Wada testing. Language lateralization indexes cussed in the following chapter), and resting state
can be examined for various brain regions of functional connectivity studies will produce
interest. These LIs, when combined with knowl- maps for individual patients that can precisely
edge of the side of seizure focus, age at onset of guide surgical interventions.
the seizures or initial precipitating event, and
most importantly, preoperative neuropsychologi-
cal test scores, contribute significantly to predict- References
ing cognitive morbidity in patients undergoing
Aguirre, G. K., & D'Esposito, M. (1999). Experimental
left anterior temporal lobectomy. Moreover, design for brain fMRI. In C. T. W. Moonen & P. A.
while fMRI memory protocols have not been Bandettini (Eds.), Functional MRI (pp. 369–380).
refined, data shows that fMRI language lateral- Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
ization scores predict cognitive morbidity not Akanuma, N., Alarcon, G., et al. (2003). Lateralising
value of neuropsychological protocols for presurgical
only for language (object naming) but also for assessment of temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsia,
memory (verbal list learning) better than 44(3), 408–418 [Erratum appears in Epilepsia, 2003,
functional MRI data obtained from hippocampi. 44(7), 990].
8 Functional MRI in the Presurgical Epilepsy Evaluation 191

Al-Asmi, A., Benar, C. G., et al. (2003). fMRI activation Benson, R. R., FitzGerald, D. B., et al. (1999). Language
in continuous and spike-triggered EEG-fMRI studies dominance determined by whole brain functional
of epileptic spikes. Epilepsia, 44(10), 1328–1339. MRI in patients with brain lesions. Neurology, 52(4),
Alsop, D. C., & Detre, J. A. (1998). Multisection cerebral 798–809.
blood flow MR imaging with continuous arterial spin Bettus, G., Bartolomei, F., et al. (2010). Role of resting
labeling. Radiology, 208, 410–416. state functional connectivity MRI in presurgical inves-
American Psychological Association. (2004). Official tigation of mesial temporal lobe epilepsy. Journal of
position of the division of clinical neuropsychology Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 81(10),
(APA division 40) on the role of neuropsychologists in 1147–1154.
clinical use of fMRI: approved by the Division 40 Binder, J. R. (Ed.). (2009). fMRI of language systems.
Executive Committee July 28, 2004. Clinical fMRI techniques and protocols, neuromethods.
Neuropsychologist, 18(3), 349–351. New York, NY: Humana.
Archer, J. S., Briellmann, R. S., et al. (2003). Spike- Binder, J. R. (2011). Functional MRI is a valid noninva-
triggered fMRI in reading epilepsy: Involvement of sive alternative to Wada testing. Epilepsy and Behavior,
left frontal cortex working memory area. Neurology, 20(2), 214–222.
60(3), 415–421. Binder, J. R., Bellgowan, P. S. F., et al. (2005). A compari-
Avila, C., Barros-Loscertales, A., et al. (2006). Memory son of two FMRI protocols for eliciting hippocampal
lateralization with 2 functional MR imaging tasks in activation. Epilepsia, 46(7), 1061–1070.
patients with lesions in the temporal lobe. AJNR. American Binder, J. R., Desai, R. H., et al. (2009). Where is the
Journal of Neuroradiology, 27(3), 498–503. semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis
Bagshaw, A. P., Aghakhani, Y., et al. (2004). EEG-fMRI of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. Cerebral
of focal epileptic spikes: analysis with multiple hae- Cortex, 19(12), 2767–2796.
modynamic functions and comparison with gadolin- Binder, J. R., Frost, J. A., et al. (1996). Function of the left
ium-enhanced MR angiograms. Human Brain planum temporale in auditory and linguistic process-
Mapping, 22(3), 179–192. ing. Brain, 119(Pt 4), 1239–1247.
Baudewig, J., Bittermann, H. J., et al. (2001). Simultaneous Binder, J. R., Frost, J. A., et al. (1997). Human brain lan-
EEG and functional MRI of epileptic activity: A case guage areas identified by functional MRI. Journal of
report. Clinical Neurophysiology, 112(7), 1196–1200. Neuroscience, 17(1), 353–362.
Baxendale, S., & Thompson, P. (2005). Defining mean- Binder, J. R., Frost, J. A., et al. (1999). Conceptual pro-
ingful postoperative change in epilepsy surgery cessing during the conscious resting state. A func-
patients: Measuring the unmeasurable? Epilepsy and tional MRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,
Behavior, 6(2), 207–211. 11(1), 80–95.
Baxendale, S., & Thompson, P. (2010). Beyond localiza- Binder, J. R., Gross, W. L., et al. (2010). Mapping anterior
tion: The role of traditional neuropsychological tests temporal lobe language areas with fMRI: A multicenter
in an age of imaging. Epilepsia, 51(11), 2225–2230. normative study. NeuroImage, 54(2), 1465–1475.
Baxendale, S., Thompson, P., et al. (2006). Predicting Binder, J. R., Hammeke, T. A., et al. (2001). Reliability
memory decline following epilepsy surgery: A multi- and validity of language dominance assessment with
variate approach. Epilepsia, 47(11), 1887–1894. functional MRI. Neurology, 56(Suppl A), 158.
Baxendale, S. A., van Paesschen, W., et al. (1998). The Binder, J. R., Sabsevitz, D. S., et al. (2008). Use of preop-
relationship between quantitative MRI and neuropsy- erative functional MRI to predict verbal memory
chological functioning in temporal lobe epilepsy. decline after temporal lobe epilepsy surgery. Epilepsia,
Epilepsia, 39(2), 158–166. 49(8), 1377–1394.
Bell, B. D., Davies, K. G., et al. (2000). Confrontation Binder, J. R., Swanson, S. J., et al. (1996). Determination of
naming after anterior temporal lobectomy is related language dominance using functional MRI: A compari-
to age of acquisition of the object names. son with the Wada test. Neurology, 46(4), 978–984.
Neuropsychologia, 38(1), 83–92. Binder, J. R., Swanson, S. J., et al. (2008). A comparison
Bell, B., Lin, J. J., et al. (2011). The neurobiology of cog- of five fMRI protocols for mapping speech compre-
nitive disorders in temporal lobe epilepsy. Nature hension systems. Epilepsia, 49(12), 1980–1997.
Reviews Neuroscience, 7(3), 154–164. Binder, J. R., Swanson, S. J., et al. (2010). A comparison
Bellgowan, P. S., Binder, J. R., et al. (1998). Side of seizure of two fMRI methods for predicting verbal memory
focus predicts left medial temporal lobe activation dur- decline after left temporal lobectomy: Language later-
ing verbal encoding. Neurology, 51(2), 479–484. alization versus hippocampal activation asymmetry.
Belliveau, J. W., Kennedy, D. N., et al. (1991). Functional Epilepsia, 51(4), 618–626.
mapping of the human visual cortex by magnetic reso- Bobholz, J. A., Rao, S. M., et al. (2007). Clinical use of
nance imaging. Science, 254, 716–719. functional magnetic resonance imaging: reflections on
Benar, C., Aghakhani, Y., et al. (2003). Quality of EEG in the new CPT codes. Neuropsychology Review, 17(2),
simultaneous EEG-fMRI for epilepsy. Clinical 189–191.
Neurophysiology, 114(3), 569–580. Bonelli, S. B., Powell, R. H. W., et al. (2010). Imaging
Benar, C. G., Gross, D. W., et al. (2002). The BOLD memory in temporal lobe epilepsy: Predicting the
response to interictal epileptiform discharges. effects of temporal lobe resection. Brain, 133(Pt 4),
NeuroImage, 17(3), 1182–1192. 1186–1199.
192 S.J. Swanson et al.

Bookheimer, S. Y. (1996). Functional MRI applications in Forman, S. D., Cohen, J. D., et al. (1995). Improved
clinical epilepsy. NeuroImage, 4(3 Pt 3), S139–S146. assessment of significant activation in functional mag-
Bookheimer, S. (2007). Pre-surgical language mapping netic resonance imaging (fMRI): Use of a cluster-size
with functional magnetic resonance imaging. threshold. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine, 33(5),
Neuropsychology Review, 17(2), 145–155. 636–647.
Buckner, R. L., & Braver, T. S. (1999). Event-related func- Fox, M. D., & Raichle, M. E. (2007). Spontaneous fluc-
tional MRI. In C. T. W. Moonen & P. A. Bandettini tuations in brain activity observed with functional
(Eds.), Functional MRI (pp. 441–452). Berlin: Springer. magnetic resonance imaging. Nature Reviews
Buschke, H. (1973). Selective reminding for analysis of Neuroscience, 8(9), 700–711.
memory and learning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Freilich, E. R., & Gaillard, W. D. (2010). Utility of func-
Verbal Behavior, 12, 543–550. tional MRI in pediatric neurology. Current Neurology
Buschke, H., & Fuld, P. A. (1974). Evaluating storage, and Neuroscience Reports, 10(1), 40–46.
retention, and retrieval in disordered memory and Gaillard, W. D., Balsamo, L., et al. (2004). fMRI language
learning. Neurology, 24(11), 1019–1025. task panel improves determination of language domi-
Chelune, G. J. (1995). Hippocampal adequacy versus nance. Neurology, 63(8), 1403–1408.
functional reserve: Predicting memory functions fol- Golby, A. J., Poldrack, R. A., et al. (2002). Memory later-
lowing temporal lobectomy. Archives of Clinical alization in medial temporal lobe epilepsy assessed by
Neuropsychology, 10(5), 413–432. functional MRI. Epilepsia, 43(8), 855–863.
Chelune, G. J., Naugle, R. I., et al. (1991). Prediction of Hall, D. A., Haggard, M. P., et al. (1999). Sparse temporal
cognitive change as a function of preoperative ability sampling in auditory fMRI. Human Brain Mapping, 7,
status among temporal lobectomy patients seen at 213–223.
6-month follow-up. Neurology, 41(3), 399–404. Hamberger, M. J., McClelland, S., 3rd, et al. (2007).
Davies, K. G., Bell, B. D., et al. (1998). Prediction of ver- Distribution of auditory and visual naming sites in
bal memory loss in individuals after anterior temporal nonlesional temporal lobe epilepsy patients and
lobectomy. Epilepsia, 39(8), 820–828. patients with space-occupying temporal lobe lesions.
de Pasquale, F., Della Penna, S., et al. (2010). Temporal Epilepsia, 48(3), 531–538.
dynamics of spontaneous MEG activity in brain net- Hammeke, T. A., Bellgowan, P. S., et al. (2000). fMRI:
works. Proceedings of the National Academy of Methodology–cognitive function mapping. Advances
Sciences of the United States of America, 107(13), in Neurology, 83, 221–233.
6040–6045. Hammeke, T. A., Kortenkamp, S. J., et al. (2005).
Delis, D. C., Kramer, J. H., et al. (2000). California Verbal Normative data on 372 stimuli for descriptive naming.
Learning Test – Second edition. Adult version. San Epilepsy Research, 66(1–3), 45–57.
Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. Hermann, B. P., Perrine, K., et al. (1999). Visual confron-
Démonet, J.-F., Chollet, F., et al. (1992). The anatomy of tation naming following left anterior temporal lobec-
phonological and semantic processing in normal sub- tomy: A comparison of surgical approaches.
jects. Brain, 115, 1753–1768. Neuropsychology, 13(1), 3–9.
Desmond, J. E., & Glover, G. H. (2002). Estimating sam- Hermann, B. P., Wyler, A. R., et al. (1992). Pathological
ple size in functional MRI (fMRI) neuroimaging stud- status of the mesial temporal lobe predicts memory
ies: Statistical power analyses. Journal of Neuroscience outcome from left anterior temporal lobectomy.
Methods, 118, 115–128. Neurosurgery, 31(4), 652–656. discussion 656–657.
D'Esposito, M. (2000). Functional neuroimaging of cog- Hermann, B. P., Wyler, A. R., et al. (1994a). Dysnomia
nition. Seminars in Neurology, 20(4), 487–498. after left anterior temporal lobectomy without func-
Detre, J. A. (2004). fMRI: Applications in epilepsy. tional mapping: Frequency and correlates.
Epilepsia, 45(Suppl 4), 26–31. Neurosurgery, 35(1), 52–56. discussion 56–57.
Detre, J. A. (2006). Clinical applicability of functional Hermann, B. P., Wyler, A. R., et al. (1994b). Declarative
MRI. Journal of Magnetic Resonance Imaging, 23(6), memory following anterior temporal lobectomy in
808–815. humans. Behavioral Neuroscience, 108(1), 3–10.
Detre, J. A., Maccotta, L., et al. (1998). Functional MRI Huettel, S. A., & McCarthy, G. (2001). The effects of
lateralization of memory in temporal lobe epilepsy. single-trial averaging upon the spatial extent of fMRI
Neurology, 50(4), 926–932. activation. Neuroreport, 12, 2411–2416.
Detre, J. A., Sirven, J. I., et al. (1995). Localization of Humphries, C., Liebenthal, E., et al. (2010). Tonotopic
subclinical ictal activity by functional magnetic reso- organization in human auditory cortex. NeuroImage,
nance imaging: Correlation with invasive monitoring. 50(3), 1202–1211.
Annals of Neurology, 38(4), 618–624. Jager, L., Werhahn, K. J., et al. (2002). Focal epileptiform
Dulay, M. F., Levin, H. S., et al. (2009). Changes in indi- activity in the brain: Detection with spike-related
vidual and group spatial and verbal learning character- functional MR imaging–preliminary results.
istics after anterior temporal lobectomy. Epilepsia, Radiology, 223(3), 860–869.
50(6), 1385–1395. Jezzard, P., & Balaban, R. S. (1995). Correction for geo-
Fischl, B., Sereno, M. I., et al. (1999). High-resolution metric distortion in echo planar images from B0-field
intersubject averaging and a coordinate system for the variations. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine, 34,
cortical surface. Human Brain Mapping, 8, 272–284. 65–73.
8 Functional MRI in the Presurgical Epilepsy Evaluation 193

Johnstone, T., Ores Walsh, K. S., et al. (2006). Motion Liu, Z., Fukunaga, M., et al. (2010). Large-scale sponta-
correction and the use of motion covariates in multi- neous fluctuations and correlations in brain electrical
ple-subject fMRI analysis. Human Brain Mapping, activity observed with magnetoencephalography.
27(10), 779–788. NeuroImage, 51(1), 102–111.
Jokeit, H., Okujava, M., et al. (2001). Memory fMRI Martin, R. C., Sawrie, S. M., et al. (1998). Individual
lateralizes temporal lobe epilepsy. Neurology, 57(10), memory change after anterior temporal lobectomy: A
1786–1793. base rate analysis using regression-based outcome
Jones-Gotman, M. (1991). Localization of lesions by neuro- methodology. Epilepsia, 39(10), 1075–1082.
psychological testing. Epilepsia, 32(Suppl 5), S41–S52. McKiernan, K. A., D'Angelo, B. R., et al. (2006).
Jones-Gotman, M., Smith, M. L., et al. (2010). The contri- Interrupting the "stream of consciousness": An fMRI
bution of neuropsychology to diagnostic assessment in investigation. NeuroImage, 29(4), 1185–1191.
epilepsy. Epilepsy and Behavior, 18(1–2), 3–12. McKiernan, K. A., Kaufman, J. N., et al. (2003). A para-
Kaplan, E., Goodglass, H., et al. (2001). The Boston metric manipulation of factors affecting task-induced
Naming Test (2nd ed.). Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott deactivation in functional neuroimaging. Journal of
Williams & Wilkins. Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(3), 394–408.
Killgore, W. D., Glosser, G., et al. (1999). Functional MRI Merboldt, K. D., Finterbusch, J., et al. (2000). Reducing
and the Wada test provide complementary information inhomogeneity artifacts in functional MRI of human
for predicting post-operative seizure control. Seizure, brain activation: Thin sections vs. gradient compensa-
8(8), 450–455. tion. Journal of Magnetic Resonance, 145, 184–191.
Klein, A., Andersson, J., et al. (2009). Evaluation of 14 Morawetz, C., Holz, P., et al. (2008). Improved functional
nonlinear deformation algorithms applied to human mapping of the human amygdala using a standard
brain MRI registration. NeuroImage, 46(3), 786–802. functional magnetic resonance imaging sequence with
Koylu, B., Trinka, E., et al. (2006). Neural correlates of simple modifications. Magnetic Resonance Imaging,
verbal semantic memory in patients with temporal 26, 45–53.
lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy Research, 72(2–3), 178–191. Morgan, V. L., Dawant, B. M., et al. (2007). Comparison
Krakow, K., Woermann, F. G., et al. (1999). EEG- of fMRI statistical software packages and strategies
triggered functional MRI of interictal epileptiform for analysis of images containing random and stimu-
activity in patients with partial seizures. Brain, 122(Pt lus-correlated motion. Computerized Medical Imaging
9), 1679–1688. and Graphics, 31(6), 436–446.
Labudda, K., Mertens, M., et al. (2010). Presurgical lan- Moser, D. J., Bauer, R. M., et al. (2000).
guage fMRI activation correlates with postsurgical Electroencephalographic, volumetric, and neuropsy-
verbal memory decline in left-sided temporal lobe epi- chological indicators of seizure focus lateralization in
lepsy. Epilepsy Research, 92(2–3), 258–261. temporal lobe epilepsy. Archives of Neurology, 57(5),
Langfitt, J. T., & Rausch, R. (1996). Word-finding deficits 707–712.
persist after left anterotemporal lobectomy. Archives Naugle, R. I., Chelune, G. J., et al. (1993). Detection of
of Neurology, 53(1), 72–76. changes in material-specific memory following tem-
Larson, E. R., Hammeke, T. A., et al. (2004). Comparison poral lobectomy using the Wechsler Memory Scale-
of receptive and expressive language fMRI activation Revised. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 8(5),
and Wada language lateralization. Epilepsia, 45, 175. 381–395.
Lazeyras, F., Blanke, O., et al. (2000). EEG-triggered Noll, D. C., Stenger, V. A., et al. (1999). Spiral scanning in
functional MRI in patients with pharmacoresistant fMRI. In C. T. W. Moonen & P. A. Bandettini (Eds.),
epilepsy. Journal of Magnetic Resonance Imaging, Functional MRI (pp. 149–160). Berlin: Springer.
12(1), 177–185. Ogawa, S., Lee, T. M., et al. (1990). Brain magnetic reso-
Lehericy, S., Cohen, L., et al. (2000). Functional MR eval- nance imaging with contrast dependent on blood oxy-
uation of temporal and frontal language dominance genation. Proceedings of the National Academy of
compared with the Wada test. Neurology, 54(8), Sciences of the United States of America, 87,
1625–1633. 9868–9872.
Lemieux, L., Krakow, K., et al. (2001). Comparison of Ojemann, J. G., Akbudak, E., et al. (1997). Anatomic
spike-triggered functional MRI BOLD activation and localization and quantitative analysis of gradient refo-
EEG dipole model localization. NeuroImage, 14(5), cused echo-planar fMRI susceptibility artifacts.
1097–1104. NeuroImage, 6, 156–167.
Leunen, D., Caroff, X., et al. (2009). Verbal and spatial Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of
learning after temporal lobe excisions in children: An handedness: The Edinburgh inventory.
adaptation of the Grober and Buschke procedure. Neuropsychologia, 9(1), 97–113.
Epilepsy and Behavior, 16(3), 534–538. Patel, M. R., Blum, A., et al. (1999). Echo-planar func-
Lineweaver, T. T., Morris, H. H., et al. (2006). Evaluating tional MR imaging of epilepsy with concurrent EEG
the contributions of state-of-the-art assessment tech- monitoring. AJNR. American Journal of
niques to predicting memory outcome after unilateral Neuroradiology, 20(10), 1916–1919.
anterior temporal lobectomy. Epilepsia, 47(11), Powell, H. W. R., & Duncan, J. S. (2005). Functional
1895–1903. magnetic resonance imaging for assessment of
194 S.J. Swanson et al.

language and memory in clinical practice. Current Prediction by age at 1st risk for seizures. Neurology,
Opinion in Neurology, 18(2), 161–166. 40(10), 1509–1512.
Powell, H. W. R., Koepp, M. J., et al. (2004). The applica- Stark, C. E., & Squire, L. R. (2001). When zero is not
tion of functional MRI of memory in temporal lobe zero: The problem of ambiguous baseline conditions
epilepsy: A clinical review. Epilepsia, 45(7), 855–863. in fMRI. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Powell, H. W. R., Richardson, M. P., et al. (2007). Sciences of the United States of America, 98(22),
Reorganization of verbal and nonverbal memory in 12760–12766.
temporal lobe epilepsy due to unilateral hippocampal Stroup, E., Langfitt, J., et al. (2003). Predicting verbal
sclerosis. Epilepsia, 48(8), 1512–1525. memory decline following anterior temporal lobec-
Powell, H. W. R., Richardson, M. P., et al. (2008). tomy (ATL). Neurology, 60(8), 1266–1273.
Preoperative fMRI predicts memory decline following Swanson, S. J. (2006). Neuropsychological testing is of
anterior temporal lobe resection. Journal of Neurology, limited value for predicting the epileptogenic zone. In
Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 79(6), 686–693. J. M. D. Silbergeld (Ed.), Controversies in epilepsy
Rabin, M. L., Narayan, V. M., et al. (2004). Functional surgery (pp. 215–220). New York, NY: Marcel Dekker
MRI predicts post-surgical memory following tempo- Inc.
ral lobectomy. Brain, 127(Pt 10), 2286–2298. Swanson, S. J., Sabsevitz, D. S., et al. (2007). Functional
Richardson, M. P., Strange, B. A., et al. (2004). Pre- magnetic resonance imaging of language in epilepsy.
operative verbal memory fMRI predicts post-operative Neuropsychology Review, 17(4), 491–504.
memory decline after left temporal lobe resection. Symms, M. R., Allen, P. J., et al. (1999). Reproducible
Brain, 127(Pt 11), 2419–2426. localization of interictal epileptiform discharges using
Richardson, M. P., Strange, B. A., et al. (2006). Memory EEG-triggered fMRI. Physics in Medicine and
fMRI in left hippocampal sclerosis: Optimizing the Biology, 44(7), N161–N168.
approach to predicting postsurgical memory. Szaflarski, J. P., Holland, S. K., et al. (2004). High-
Neurology, 66(5), 699–705. resolution functional MRI at 3T in healthy and epi-
Roland, P. E., Eriksson, L., et al. (1987). Does mental lepsy subjects: Hippocampal activation with picture
navigation along memorized routes activate the hip- encoding task. Epilepsy and Behavior, 5(2), 244–252.
pocampus, precuneus, and insula. Journal of Thornton, R., Laufs, H., et al. (2010). EEG correlated
Neuroscience, 7, 2373–2389. functional MRI and postoperative outcome in focal
Sabsevitz, D. S., Swanson, S. J., et al. (2001). Memory epilepsy. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and
outcome after left anterior temporal lobectomy in Psychiatry, 81(8), 922–927.
patients with expected and reversed Wada memory Trenerry, M. R., Jack, C. R., Jr., et al. (1993). MRI
asymmetry scores. Epilepsia, 42(11), 1408–1415. hippocampal volumes and memory function before
Sabsevitz, D. S., Swanson, S. J., et al. (2003). Use of pre- and after temporal lobectomy. Neurology, 43(9),
operative functional neuroimaging to predict language 1800–1805.
deficits from epilepsy surgery. Neurology, 60(11), Vannest, J., Szaflarski, J. P., et al. (2008). Medial temporal
1788–1792. fMRI activation reflects memory lateralization and
Schmidt, M. (1996). Rey Auditory and Verbal Learning memory performance in patients with epilepsy.
Test: A handbook. Los Angeles, CA: Western Epilepsy and Behavior, 12(3), 410–418.
Psychological Services. Voyvodic, J. T. (2006). Activation mapping as a percent-
Seeck, M., Michel, C. M., et al. (2001). EEG mapping and age of local excitation: fMRI stability within scans,
functional MRI in presurgical epilepsy evaluation. between scans and across field strengths. Magnetic
Revue Neurologique (Paris), 157(8–9 Pt 1), 747–751. Resonance Imaging, 24, 1249–1261.
Seidenberg, M., Hermann, B. P., et al. (1996). Vulliemoz, S., Lemieux, L., et al. (2010). The combina-
Hippocampal sclerosis and verbal encoding ability tion of EEG source imaging and EEG-correlated func-
following anterior temporal lobectomy. tional MRI to map epileptic networks. Epilepsia,
Neuropsychologia, 34(7), 699–708. 51(4), 491–505.
Smith, S. M., Fox, P. T., et al. (2009). Correspondence of Warach, S., Ives, J. R., et al. (1996). EEG-triggered echo-
the brain's functional architecture during activation planar functional MRI in epilepsy. Neurology, 47(1),
and rest. Proceedings of the National Academy of 89–93.
Sciences of the United States of America, 106(31), Weber, B., Wellmer, J., et al. (2006). Presurgical lan-
13040–13045. guage fMRI in patients with drug-resistant epilepsy:
Springer, J. A., Binder, J. R., et al. (1999). Language dom- Effects of task performance. Epilepsia, 47(5),
inance in neurologically normal and epilepsy subjects: 880–886.
A functional MRI study.[see comment]. Brain, 122 Wendel, J. D., Trenerry, M. R., et al. (2001). The relation-
(Pt 11), 2033–2046. ship between quantitative T2 relaxometry and memory
Stafiniak, P., Saykin, A. J., et al. (1990). Acute naming in nonlesional temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsia,
deficits following dominant temporal lobectomy: 42(7), 863–868.
MEG in the Presurgical Epilepsy
Evaluation 9
Matthew Euler, Sylvain Baillet,
and Sara J. Swanson

Magnetoencephalography involves noninva- the emerging literature on its role in localization


sively measuring the magnetic fields generated of language and memory functions in presurgical
by brain activity. These fields result from the epilepsy evaluations.
rapid electrochemical activity of neural cell
assemblies and can be measured essentially in
real time. MEG thus provides a direct measure of Physiological Basis of MEG signals
neural activity with excellent temporal resolu-
tion. When combined with detailed spatial infor- As understood from basic cellular neurophysiol-
mation obtained from structural (and even ogy, the depolarization or hyperpolarization of a
functional) MRI, MEG enables the creation of neural membrane due to synaptic activity results
spatiotemporal cortical activation maps with mil- in the flow of charged ions into or out of the cell.
lisecond temporal resolution and sub-centimeter These ion flows and resulting electrical potential
spatial resolution (Kaufmann & Lu, 2003). Thus, changes constitute excitatory or inhibitory post-
it is a powerful tool to help understand various synaptic potentials (E/IPSPs), respectively. Due
aspects of both healthy cognition and neuropa- to the longitudinal structure of pyramidal cells
thology in the context of presurgical evaluation (possessing basal dendrites near the soma and
for medically intractable epilepsy. Below, we apical dendrites at the distal end), changes in the
review the critical conceptual and practical transmembrane potential at either end of the cell
details of MEG data collection and analysis, and result in the creation of an ionic current source-
sink dynamic across its length and of a comple-
mentary magnetic field around the resulting
M. Euler, Ph.D. (*) intracellular current. When a population of simi-
Department of Psychology, University of Utah,
larly oriented cells is synchronously activated
380 South 1530 East, Salt Lake City, UT 84112, USA
e-mail: matt.euler@psych.utah.edu due to E/IPSPs, the resulting net electric current
generates a very small magnetic field, which
S. Baillet, Ph.D.
McConnell Brain Imaging Centre, Montreal nonetheless is of sufficient strength to be detected
Neurological Institute, McGill University, extracranially.
WB 315, Montreal, QC, Canada H3A 2B4 Thus, the magnetic fields measured in MEG
http://sylvain.baillet@mcgill.ca
principally result from ionic current flows created
S.J. Swanson, Ph.D. by synchronous E/IPSPs within populations of
Department of Neurology, Medical College of
pyramidal cells (Baillet, Mosher, & Leahy, 2001;
Wisconsin, 9200 W. Wisconsin Avenue,
Milwaukee, WI 53226, USA Kaufmann & Lu, 2003). Due to several favor-
e-mail: SSwanson@mcw.edu able aspects of their anatomical organization,

W.B. Barr and C. Morrison (eds.), Handbook on the Neuropsychology of Epilepsy, 195
Clinical Handbooks in Neuropsychology, DOI 10.1007/978-0-387-92826-5_9,
© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015
196 M. Euler et al.

pyramidal cell assemblies within neocortical lay- are subject to volume conduction effects (distor-
ers II/III and V in particular generate magnetic tion from signal mixing and cerebrospinal fluid,
fields of sufficient strength (approximately meningeal, skull, and scalp resistance), cortical
10fT = 10e−15 T) to be detectable outside of the magnetic fields pass through the skull and other
head (Baillet, 2010). These cell aggregates are tissues virtually unimpeded (Okada, Lahteenmäki,
generally understood to form the basic generators & Xu, 1999). This “transparence” of the human
of most MEG signals. Theoretically, fields of this body to magnetic fields permits localization of the
magnitude may correspond to populations of generators of the observed magnetic fields within a
approximately 50,000 synchronously active reasonable spatial resolution (about 5 mm, depend-
pyramidal cells, within cortical areas of about ing on source depth) (Leahy, Mosher, Spencer,
1 mm2 or larger, and current flows arising perpen- Huang, & Lewine, 1998), via the additional step of
dicularly to the cortical surface (Lopes da Silva, co-registering MEG source imaging with the sub-
2010; Murakami & Okada, 2006). ject’s structural MRI volume (Kaufmann & Lu,
In addition to understanding the mechanism 2003). Taken together, the excellent temporal reso-
by which neural ionic currents result in detect- lution of the raw data, the sensitivity to paroxystic
able extracranial magnetic fields, there are two interictal events, and the spatial resolution of mag-
additional considerations pertinent to under- netic source analysis make MEG a desirable addi-
standing the basis of MEG signals. MEG source tion to functional mapping studies in the context of
imaging was originally thought to be relatively presurgical epilepsy evaluations.
less sensitive to fields emanating from gyral
crests and deeper sources, than to those gener-
ated by sources located within sulcal walls (Vrba MEG Data Collection
& Robinson, 2001). Fortunately, advances in the
modeling of the physics of electromagnetic fields Equipment
in head tissues permit state-of-the-art whole-head
MEG systems to detect the majority of cortical Most MEG laboratories utilize a common set of
signals (Hillebrand & Barnes, 2002). Moreover, components to optimize detection of the magnetic
detection of various subcortical and cerebellar fields of interest. The most important of these is
sources is increasingly being reported under suit- obviously the MEG instrument itself, consisting of
able experimental conditions (e.g., Attal, et al., a large dewar molded into a helmet-shaped surface
2009; Martin, et al., 2006; Riggs, et al., 2009). at its base, containing approximately 300 magnetic
It should also be noted that the contribution of sensors. Different MEG systems involve various
action potentials to MEG signals are assumed combinations of sensors arrays, and the most basic
to be negligible under most circumstances, due sensor is a magnetometer, consisting of a pickup
to their rapid time course, and lower likelihood coil paired with a current detector. Single magne-
of synchronization across cells than E/IPSPs. tometers (which measure absolute magnetic fields)
Of note however, this may not hold to account and paired magnetometers or “gradiometers”
for the generation of some very high-frequency (which measure differences in field strength across
(150 Hz and higher) neural signals with MEG or short distances, and are thus less sensitive to far-
in the context of epileptiform spike discharges field noise sources) are in turn connected to super-
(Lopes da Silva, 2010). conducting current detectors (i.e., superconducting
Like EEG and other electrophysiological quantum interference devices, or SQUIDs) which
recording techniques, MEG provides millisecond ultimately enable detection of the miniscule field
temporal resolution of neural activity, enabling strengths generated by neural activity. This super-
observations of rapidly changing network dynam- conducting sensing technology is cooled at
ics and transient oscillatory phenomena. However, −269 °C with liquid helium and is necessary to
unlike electroencephalographic potentials which keep instrumental noise levels at less than a few
derive primarily from extracellular currents and femtotesla per square root hertz.
9 MEG in the Presurgical Epilepsy Evaluation 197

Working with ultrasensitive sensors requires technicians. Operation of a working MEG lab
solving some associated challenges, as they are requires regular delivery and replenishment of
very good at picking up the nuisances and elec- liquid helium which is necessary for operation of
tromagnetic perturbations generated by external the sensors, as well as one or more technicians
noise sources of environmental noise. A magneti- who can maintain consistency in subject prepara-
cally shielded room (MSR) made of layers of tion and data acquisition. The technology
metal alloys (and possibly complemented by involved in MEG sensing, the weekly helium
active shielding solutions) is employed to attenu- refills, and the materials building the MSR makes
ate external magnetic fields and in turn makes MEG a costly piece of equipment. Exciting
MEG recordings possible. The attenuation of recent developments, however, contribute to con-
electromagnetic perturbations through the MSR stant progress in cost-effectiveness, practicality,
walls is considerable and facilitates MEG record- and the future of MEG sensing science.
ings, even in noisy environments like hospitals
(even near MRI suites) and in the vicinity of road
traffic. Stimulus presentation in the MSR, espe- Subject Preparation
cially when it requires external devices, needs to
be considered carefully to avoid introducing A unique and challenging aspect of MEG is its
supplementary electromagnetic perturbations. sensitivity to multiple possible artifacts.
Fortunately, MEG centers can benefit from most Specifically, MEG data may be contaminated by
of the equipment available to fMRI studies, as it physiological sources of magnetic fields such as
is specified along the same constraints regarding muscles, eye blinks and movements, heartbeats,
magnetic compatibility. Therefore, audio and facial movements, and speech and from any mag-
video presentations can be performed using elec- netic material moving in the vicinity of the sen-
trostatic transducers and mirror systems that sors. To permit subsequent detection and
beam video projection. Electrical stimulation for attenuation of ocular and cardiac artifacts, sub-
somatosensory mapping generates artifacts of ject preparation typically involves application of
short durations that do not overlap with the earli- electrooculogram and electrocardiogram leads.
est brain responses (>20 ms latency) or in some Careful experimental design and data collection
instances (e.g., pediatric populations) may be is required to either avoid or account for contami-
advantageously replaced by air puff delivery. nation due to speech-related artifacts. Large arti-
MEG laboratories are also equipped with safety facts due to metallic and magnetic parts (coins,
features such as ventilation systems permitting air- credit cards, some dental retainers, body pierc-
flow, maintenance and emergency exhausts for ing, bra supports, etc.) or particles (makeup, hair
evaporating liquid helium, and audio and video spray, tattoos) can be readily and visually
systems enabling constant contact between the detected as they cause major low-frequency
subject and experimenter throughout the study. The deflections in sensor traces. They are usually
MEG gantry may be positioned to perform with the emphasized with respiration and/or eye blinks or
subject seated or supine to maximize their comfort jaw movements. However, some causes of arti-
or accommodate clinical constraints. Finally EEG, facts may not be easily circumvented. In particu-
other ancillary electrophysiological recordings lar, recent prior participation in an MRI study
(such as electrooculogram, myogram, and cardio- may result in strong, long-term magnetization of
gram), various behavioral responses, and even eye dental retainers, for example, which generally
tracking can be recorded simultaneously with brings the MEG session to a premature close.
MEG, allowing detection of complementary physi- This may be avoided by scheduling a patient’s
ological and behavioral measures, as well as pos- MRI after their MEG session rather than before.
sible sources of physiological artifacts. Onsite demagnetization may be attempted using
The MEG system is in turn connected to data “degaussing” techniques—usually using a con-
acquisition software that is run by one or more ventional magnetic tape eraser, which attenuates
198 M. Euler et al.

and scrambles magnetization—with limited of three fiducial points should be localized using
chances of success, though. As a rule, it is best to a 3D digitizer device (most commonly near the
ask subjects to refrain from wearing clothing nasion and left and right periauricular points),
with metallic components, piercings, makeup, or and their application should be well-standardized
other potentially magnetic materials. across technicians in a given laboratory. To
For patient populations, artifacts generated by reduce ambiguity in the detection of these points
sources such as ventricular shunts and aneurysm in the MR volume data, they can be marked using
clips may be partially compensated for by filter- vitamin E pills or any other solid marker that is
ing out implicated frequencies in the MEG sig- readily visible in T1-weighted MR images, if an
nals or, if necessary, excluding the most affected MRI is scheduled immediately after the MEG
channels from further analysis. As vagal nerve session. Digitization of EEG electrode locations
stimulators create large artifacts (even when is also mandatory for accurate, subsequent source
turned off), they have generally precluded collec- analysis (EEG recording is possible concurrently
tion of meaningful data (Makela, 2010). However, with MEG, without generating increased noise
the recent development of more sophisticated levels). Overall, about 15 min are required for
signal processing techniques has begun to subject preparation for an MEG-only session,
improve correction for more minor, regular arti- which can extend up to about 45 min if
facts such as heartbeat and eye blinks (Delorme simultaneous high-density EEG is required.
& Makeig, 2004) and has even enabled removal
of VNS or DBS artifacts from MEG data in case
reports (Carrette et al., 2011; Kringelbach, et al., Data Acquisition
2007; Ramirez, et al., 2009; Tanaka, Thiele,
Madsen, Bourgeois, & Stufflebeam, 2009). To The time dimension accessible to MEG offers
permit interpretation of the scalp topography considerable variety in the design of experimen-
within raw MEG data, or accurate source local- tal paradigms, enabling collection of various
ization, there is also a need to maintain a record types of event-related brain activity, in addition
of the location of the subject’s head with respect to measurement of “resting” states, passive stim-
the sensor array. This is accomplished by also ulation, and oscillatory activity (Salmelin &
applying head-positioning coils (HPI) to the sub- Baillet, 2009). In the context of presurgical epi-
ject’s head to detect its position with respect to lepsy evaluations, it is often necessary to obtain
the sensor array while recording. This is critical “resting-state” data to permit the epileptologist to
because although head motion is not encouraged, review the individual sensor traces for epilepti-
it is very likely to occur within and between runs, form activity or to apply automated spike-
especially with young children and some patients. detection algorithms. In contrast, localization of
The HPIs drive a current at some adjustable sensory/motor or cognitive functions usually
higher frequency (about 300 Hz) that is readily involves collection of event-related data, such as
detected by the MEG sensors at the beginning of during sustained performance of a task or in the
each run, permitting their localization within sec- experimental contexts reviewed below.
onds with millimeter accuracy. Some MEG sys- MEG paradigms commonly utilize automated
tems feature the possibility for continuous stimulus presentation to ensure the necessary
head-position monitoring during the recording as temporal precision between experimental events
well as offline head movement compensation and data collection. As with other computer-
(Wehner, Hämäläinen, Mody, & Ahlfors, 2008). assisted psychological research, the typical case
If advanced source analysis is required, addi- involves exposure to conditions presented via a
tional 3D digitization of anatomical fiducial succession of individual trials according to blocks
points is necessary to ensure that subsequent or in a randomized or pseudo-randomized order.
registration to the subject’s MRI anatomical The number of trials in each condition must be
volume is successful and accurate. A minimum high enough to allow data analysis in single
9 MEG in the Presurgical Epilepsy Evaluation 199

subjects, accounting for loss of up to 20 % due to Quality control of the data is recommended to
artifacts. Practically, this translates into collec- be performed before artifact rejection through the
tion of 120 trials for investigators adopting con- visual examination of the trials themselves, the
servative approaches in cognitive paradigms, to nature and degree of artifactual contamination,
50 or fewer for robust sensory responses to single and review of spatial topography of the single tri-
stimuli (Salmelin & Parkkonen, 2010). The dura- als and averages. This step increases the investiga-
tions of interstimulus intervals are typically much tor’s familiarity with the responses elicited by the
shorter than in fMRI paradigms and range from a task and ensures that the subsequent analysis pro-
few tens of milliseconds to a few seconds. In ceeds with valid and meaningful data. Two addi-
order to maximize subject comfort and participa- tional steps to further clean the data and facilitate
tion (and minimize blink and movement-related detection of the signals of interest are subtracting
artifacts), it is generally advised to divide the the averaged baseline activity from the epochs
MEG session into a series of runs, each lasting (baseline correction) and applying filters to frame
less than 10 min, and to minimize the total the detection of signals within a preselected fre-
recording time when possible. In addition to quency range. While it is perfectly valid in many
recording the subject’s brain activity itself, it is contexts to restrict the analysis to the averaged
often useful to obtain an empty-room recording evoked responses obtained at the sensor level, the
for purposes of evaluating the environmental most valuable contribution from MEG in presur-
noise levels before subsequent source analysis. gical epilepsy evaluations would typically involve
magnetic source analysis. This in turn requires a
basic understanding of MEG source estimation
MEG Data Analysis (the inverse modeling problem).

Quality Control
The MEG Inverse Problem
The raw MEG data is stored as a series of traces,
quantifying the continuous time-varying amplitude Fundamentally, magnetic source estimation and
of the magnetic flux recorded by each sensor. For analysis is a modeling problem, which involves
experimental paradigms and functional tests, the the evaluation of the so-called “forward” and
traces are then divided into single trials or “epochs,” “inverse” models. In general terms, solving the
time locked to an event, which is either the stimulus forward modeling problem means mathematically
presentation or the subject’s response. Artifact modeling unknown observations from a set of
rejection generally proceeds at the level of continu- known input parameters. In MEG, this translates
ous data or single trials and is accomplished to predicting the electromagnetic fields generated
through various means. These include: visually by any arbitrary neural source distribution, in
inspecting and manually rejecting contaminated terms of location, orientation of the current flow,
trials, using automated amplitude threshold or and its amplitude. MEG forward modeling
other feature-based rejection, or signal classifica- considers that some parameters are known and
tion techniques such as independent component fixed: the geometry of the head, conductivity of
analysis that detects the spatiotemporal “signature” tissues, sensor locations, etc. Generally speaking,
of a given artifact and removes it from the data forward modeling problems are often considered
(Delorme & Makeig, 2004). Most commonly, the to be “well-posed,” meaning that since the input
epochs are then averaged across trials, to yield a and other parameters of data generation are
single time-locked average “evoked” response at assumed to be known, the problem consisting in
each given sensor. More recently, computational predicting the experimental measures has a
and conceptual advances have enabled investiga- unique solution.
tion of non-phase-locked oscillatory responses, Inverse modeling constitutes the reciprocal
which are briefly discussed below. situation where we aim to model the sources
200 M. Euler et al.

(i.e., neural currents) of the MEG observations. ics allow for head geometry to be simplified as a
This is accomplished by mathematically combin- sphere (see: Feynman, Leighton, & Sands, 1964;
ing the sensor data with the MEG forward model, Hämäläinen, Ilmoniemi, Knuutila, & Lounasmaa,
thereby enabling estimation of the source param- 1993), and hence, the simplest and consequently
eters (location, orientation of current flow, mag- by far most popular model of head geometry in
nitude time series). Given the approximations of MEG is a single-layer sphere. In MEG, only the
forward modeling, the noise in the data, and the location of the center of the spherical head geom-
underlying physics of MEG, the inverse model- etry matters. The respective conductivity and
ing problem admits multiple possible solutions radius of the spherical layers have no influence on
that may equivalently predict the observations. the measured MEG fields. This is not the case in
This situation is not unique to MEG, and in imag- EEG, where the location, radii, and respective
ing science, quantitative approaches to dealing conductivity of each spherical shell (typically:
with such so-called “ill-posed” inverse problems brain, cerebrospinal fluid, skull, and scalp) influ-
involve reducing the number of unknown param- ence the distribution and amplitude of surface
eters and/or adding supplementary constraints to electrical potentials. Thus, for applications involv-
the estimation of sources, to make it a well-posed ing simultaneous EEG acquisition (and their
problem (Baillet, 2010). Practically in MEG, this source analysis), more complex head modeling
means constraining the sources to the cortex, approaches such as boundary and finite element
specifying the number of sources in advance, or head modeling are strongly suggested (Wolters,
using fMRI-based spatial weighting (e.g., et al., 2006). These provide more realistic head
Henson, Flandin, Friston, & Mattout, 2010). models via geometric tessellations of the different
head tissue envelopes extracted from the subject’s
individual MRI volume data, though they remain
Models of Neural Generators significantly more intensive computationally.
and Head Tissues While the topics of forward and inverse mod-
eling are complex and would require an in-depth
MEG forward modeling consists of two basic review per se (see Hansen, Kringelbach, &
models that work together in a complementary Salmelin, 2010), this is beyond the scope of this
manner: a physical model of neural sources and a chapter. To a clinical investigator embarking on
model that predicts how these sources generate an MEG evaluation program, it is necessary to be
electromagnetic fields outside the head. aware that MEG source analysis is fundamen-
Regarding the former, the canonical source tally a modeling problem, involving various trad-
model of the net primary intracellular currents eoffs and approximations, which can be
within a neural assembly can be reduced to an approached in a number of ways. The specific
equivalent current dipole (ECD) model (although approach taken should be dictated by the goals
higher-order models based on multipolar expan- and constraints of the study. Today, a reasonable
sions of the current distribution are available) degree of technical maturity has been reached by
(Jerbi, Mosher, Baillet, & Leahy, 2002). That is, electromagnetic brain imaging using MEG, and
a focal source generating a “dipolar” magnetic the approaches to source estimation reduce to
field in the scalp topography (see Fig. 9.1 below). only a handful of well-identified classes.
In turn, predicting the electromagnetic fields
produced by these source models requires another
modeling step. This head modeling process is MEG Source Modeling: Localization
necessary to address the influence of the head vs. Imaging Approaches
geometry and electromagnetic properties of head
tissues on the magnetic fields measured outside The localization approach to MEG source
the head. Briefly, several well-justified and sim- estimation considers that brain activity at any
plifying assumptions following from MEG phys- time instant is generated by a relatively small
9 MEG in the Presurgical Epilepsy Evaluation 201

Fig. 9.1 Basic electrophysiological principles of MEG mary currents are shown using dashed lines arranged in
and EEG. (a) Large neural cells—just like this pyramidal circles about the dipole source. (b) At a larger spatial
neuron from cortex layer V—drive ionic electrical cur- scale, the mass effect of currents due to neural cells sus-
rents. These latter are essentially impressed by the differ- taining similar mixtures of postsynaptic potentials add up
ence in electrical potentials between the basal and apical locally and behave also as a current dipole (shown in red).
dendrites or the cell body, which is due to a mixture of This primary generator induces secondary currents
excitatory and inhibitory postsynaptic potentials, which (shown in yellow) that travel through the head tissues.
are slow (>10 ms) relatively to action potentials firing and They eventually reach the scalp surface, where they can
therefore add up efficiently at the scale of synchronized be detected with pairs of electrodes by EEG. Magnetic
neural ensembles. These primary currents can be modeled fields (in green) travel more freely within tissues and are
using an equivalent current dipole, here represented by a less distorted than current flows. They can be captured
large black arrow. The electrical circuit of currents is with arrays of magnetometers by MEG. The distribution
closed within the entire head volume by secondary, vol- of blue and red colors on the scalp illustrates the contin-
ume currents shown with the dark plain lines. Additionally, uum of magnetic and electric fields and potentials distrib-
magnetic fields are generated by the primary and second- uted at the surface of the head
ary currents. The magnetic field lines induced by the pri-
202 M. Euler et al.

number (a handful, at most) of brain regions. source models do not yield small sets of local
Each source is therefore represented by an ele- elementary models but rather the distribution of
mentary model, such as an ECD, that captures “all” neural currents. As the estimated sources
local distributions of neural currents. Ultimately, are spatially fixed in this approach, they are
each elementary source is back projected or con- homologous to the pixels of a digital image.
strained to the subject’s brain volume or an MRI Hence, the resulting estimations are a set of
anatomical template, for further interpretation. images or maps, representing an estimation of the
Localization models are essentially compact, in intensity of neural currents, distributed within the
terms of number of generators involved and their entire brain volume or constrained at the cortical
surface extension (from point-like to small corti- surface. Contrary to the localization model
cal surface patches). though, there is no intrinsic sense of distinct,
The early MEG literature is abundant in stud- active source regions per se. Explicit identifica-
ies reporting on single-dipole source models. The tion of activity issued from discrete brain regions
somatotopic, tonotopic, and retinotopic responses usually necessitates complementary analysis,
are examples where the single-dipole model con- such as empirical or inference-driven amplitude
tributed to the better temporal characterization thresholding, to discard elementary sources of
of primary sensory area responses. However, nonsignificant contribution according to the sta-
later components of evoked fields usually neces- tistical appraisal. In that respect, MEG source
sitate that more elementary sources be adjusted, images are very similar in essence to the activa-
and this is detrimental to the numerical stability tion maps obtained in fMRI, with the supplemen-
and robustness of the inverse model. The num- tary benefit of time resolution (Fig. 9.2).
ber of elementary sources in the model is often Just like in the context of source localization
qualitatively assessed by expert users, which where, for example, the number of sources is
may undermine the reproducibility of the analy- a restrictive prior as a remedy to ill-posedness,
ses. Hence, special care should be brought to the imaging models also must be complemented
evaluation of the stability and robustness of the by a priori information. Several academic soft-
estimated source models. With all that in mind, ware packages have addressed this issue by
source localization techniques have proven to be implementing variations of the well-described
effective, even in complex experimental para- minimum norm model (Lin, et al., 2006; Lin,
digms (Helenius, Parviainen, Paetau, & Salmelin, Belliveau, Dale, & Hämäläinen, 2006). For pres-
2009). Indeed, as we outline below, dipole-based ent purposes, it is important only that readers be
approaches currently constitute the most widely aware of the equivalent need for prior information
applied and best-replicated paradigms for pre- in both the localization and imaging approaches.
surgical language mapping with MEG. The alter- These priors are necessary to determine a work-
native imaging approaches to source modeling able solution to a problem with an infinite num-
were originally inspired by the plethoric research ber of solutions, in theory.
in digital image restoration and reconstruction in A final set of approaches involve signal clas-
other fields of engineering. sification and spatial filtering (e.g., “beam form-
In these approaches, dense grids of current ing”) to translate the source localization problem
dipoles are predefined over the entire brain vol- into a signal detection issue. In essence these are
ume or limited to the cortical gray matter surface. scanning techniques that systematically sift
These dipoles are fixed in location and, generally, through the brain space to evaluate how a prede-
orientation, and their amplitudes are estimated at termined elementary source model would fit the
once. Thus, the nonlinear parameters (e.g., the data at every voxel of the brain volume. These
elementary source locations) now become fixed techniques are efficient alternative approaches in
priors as provided by anatomical information, that respect and have gained considerable
and the remaining free parameters are the ampli- momentum in the MEG community in the recent
tudes of the elementary source currents distrib- years. They have proven especially fruitful in
uted on the brain’s geometry. The resulting image several applications (for both source estimation
9 MEG in the Presurgical Epilepsy Evaluation 203

Fig. 9.2 Inverse modeling: localization (a) vs. imaging the contralateral central sulcus as revealed by the three-
(b) approaches. Source modeling through localization dimensional rendering obtained after the source location
consists in decomposing the MEG/EEG generators in a has been registered to the individual anatomy. In the imag-
handful of elementary source contributions, the simplest ing approach, the source model is spatially distributed on
source model in this situation being the ECD. This is illus- a large number of ECDs. Here, a surface model of MEG-
trated here with experimental data from testing the EEG generators was constrained to the individual brain
somatotopic organization of primary cortical representa- surface extracted from T1-weighted MR images.
tions of hand fingers. The parameters of the single ECD Elemental source amplitudes are interpolated onto the
have been adjusted on the 20–40 ms time window follow- cortex, which yields an image-like distribution of the
ing stimulus onset. The ECD was found to localize along amplitudes of cortical currents

and artifact suppression) and the interested reader functioning MEG laboratory requires the effort of
is referred to a review article by Hillebrand, a multidisciplinary team. The various require-
Singh, Holliday, Furlong, and Barnes (2005) for ments would ideally be met by a group of indi-
an introduction to the approach. viduals with collective expertise in MEG physics,
Each of these three approaches, MEG source data acquisition, software engineering, task design
localization, imaging, and scanning, makes use and development, cognitive evaluation, and neuro-
of phenomenally complex methodology and anatomy, epileptology, neuro-oncology, etc.
modeling techniques to noninvasively localize Developing a standardized data acquisition and
the source of magnetic neural activity within the analysis protocol simply requires explicitly plan-
brain. As with any experimental observation, ning through the steps outlined above (e.g., subject
they are subject to error and benefit from explicit preparation, data acquisition, quality control,
assessments of their sensitivity to measure noise source modeling, etc). It is important to remember
and modeling approximations (see, e.g., Darvas, that the tasks, stimuli, and analysis approach in
et al., 2005; Mosher, Spencer, Leahy, & Lewis, MEG protocols should be designed in a coordi-
1993). Ultimately, an informed consideration of nated and complementary fashion and specifically
their various assumptions, strengths, and weak- with sensitivity to how the temporal aspects of the
nesses, as described above, can guide their opti- stimuli, and expected behavioral and neural
mal application to any given clinical evaluation. responses will condition the access to a subset of
methods for data analysis. For example, it might
be discussed whether or not to collect behavioral
Practical Considerations data simultaneously with MEG or whether the
data should be analyzed as stimulus-locked vs.
Despite the many complex aspects of magnetic response-locked epochs. Event-related responses
source imaging, the preceding exposition can eas- in cognitive tasks most often contain both early
ily be reduced to a handful of essential points to components (50–200 ms post-stimulus) reflecting
consider before beginning a program of MEG pre- the primary sensory processing and integration of
surgical evaluation. As with fMRI, an efficiently the stimulus and later components (>200 ms
204 M. Euler et al.

post-stimulus) that are typically thought to reflect most important of these is always having appro-
activity in association cortices. It has therefore priately trained personnel ready to rapidly enter
been argued that the mapping of higher cognitive and assist the patient within the enclosed MSR,
functions with MEG may require restricting analy- in case of an acute seizure or other medical
ses to the later components of the brain responses emergency.
(Simos, Papanicolaou, Castillo, & Buchanan,
2009). Decisions such as duration of stimulus pre-
sentation and length and variability of interstimu- Clinical Use of MEG by
lus intervals (ISIs) need to be guided by the Neuropsychologists
specifics of the task modality and of the underly-
ing scientific hypothesis to be tested (e.g., visual For the same reasons outlined in the fMRI chapter
stimuli tend to elicit saccades, auditory stimuli of this volume, neuropsychologists can clearly
unfold over time, etc.) and the expected nature of play an appropriate and valuable role in the
the neural responses of interest (Salmelin & design, administration, and interpretation of
Parkkonen, 2010). There are several academic or MEG studies of higher cognitive functions. In
commercial software packages for MEG data 2009, the American Academy of Neurology
analysis, and the major MEG vendors provide published a policy regarding MEG, including
their own software for many aspects of data pro- indications to “localize and preserve eloquent
cessing. Additionally, the reference papers for sev- cortex during resective surgery,” in the context of
eral excellent and freely available software surgeries for tumors and arteriovenous malfor-
packages can be found at: http://www.hindawi. mations, in addition to MEG indications for epi-
com/journals/cin/2011/si.ebm/. lepsy surgical planning (see Wu et al., 2010).
As discussed above, MEG recording is greatly MEG CPT codes have been issued for these clini-
facilitated if caution is taken prior to scanning cal conditions http://acmegs.org/id77.html under
to remove easily eliminated sources of magnetic “MEG recording and analysis” for localization of
artifacts (e.g., pieces of metal in garments). To language, as well as sensory and motor functions,
this end, it is a good investment of time to briefly with the former representing an excellent venue
conduct a noise run with the subject sitting or for integrating neuropsychological expertise. As
laying under the MEG dewar to help identify any of this writing, we are not aware of any official
unsuspected artifacts, before subject preparation position statements regarding the role of neuro-
is completed by applying HPI coils and, option- psychologists in the clinical use of MEG. Given
ally, EEG leads. This step has the additional ben- the historical importance that localization of cog-
efit of familiarizing the patient with the MSR nitive functions played within the discipline of
environment before the clinical scan per se. For clinical neuropsychology, as well as ongoing dis-
the most part, the prior recommendations regard- cussions about expanding roles for neuropsy-
ing behavioral monitoring and testing outside of chologists (Bilder, 2011; Miller, Elbert, Sutton,
the instrument in the context of fMRI also apply & Heller, 2007), this may be a timely topic for
to MEG. Also like fMRI, it has been reported that discussion within neuropsychological profes-
language-related activity is impacted by factors sional organizations and training programs.
such as subject alertness, task compliance, and
IQ (Merrifield, Simos, Papanicolaou, Philpott,
& Sutherling, 2007; Simos et al., 2009), though Tasks
there are ongoing efforts to develop MEG lan-
guage protocols that are better suited for use There are a number of tasks that have been
with pediatric and cognitively lower-functioning utilized to map language-specific cortical regions
individuals. Unlike the highly magnetized MRI with MEG. Fewer studies have examined
environment, patient safety concerns within the episodic memory with MEG in ways that are
MSR primarily relate to clinical concerns. The immediately relevant to presurgical epilepsy
9 MEG in the Presurgical Epilepsy Evaluation 205

evaluations. By far, the most widely studied and tion to variants of the CRM task outlined above,
best-replicated task for evaluation of the lateral- they cite numerous other paradigms designed to
ity of language functions with MEG is the con- either determine hemispheric language domi-
tinuous recognition memory (CRM) paradigm nance or localize systems related to specific
developed by Papanicolaou and colleagues (Frye, functions (Pirmoradi et al., 2010). These include:
Rezaie, & Papanicolaou, 2009). A common ver- passive listening and counting paradigms (Kim
sion of the task involves presenting the subject & Chung, 2008; Martin et al., 1993; Szymanski
with a list of target words (abstract English et al., 2001; Szymanski, Rowley, & Roberts,
nouns) immediately prior to the scan, which they 1999); grammatical or semantic categorization,
are asked to study and accurately repeat. The matching, or judgment (Härle, Dobe, Cohen, &
acquisition scan involves presenting the subject Rockstroh, 2002; Kamada et al., 2006, 2007;
with frequent spoken targets that are randomly McDonald et al., 2009; Simos, Breier, Zouridakis,
interspersed with novel distractor items. Subjects & Papanicolaou, 1998); and note/tone vs. vowel
are instructed to immediately issue a manual comparisons (Gootjes, Raij, Salmelin, & Hari,
response (a finger lift) upon presentation of a tar- 1999; Kirveskari, Salmelin, & Hari, 2006).
get and otherwise make no response. Thus, the Other reports have more explicitly examined
task essentially represents an auditory verbal rec- neural systems involved in language output, via
ognition memory paradigm. both isolated and combined studies of covert
The analysis stream most often utilized for reading and picture naming (Breier et al., 2001;
this task involves identifying the locations of suc- Hirata et al., 2010; Kober et al., 2001), delayed
cessive dipolar sources that account for the mag- and overt naming (Fisher et al., 2008; Levelt,
netic flux distribution in the “late” (i.e., >200 ms) Praamstra, Meyer, Helenius, & Salmelin, 1998;
portion of the post-stimulus period. The acquisi- Salmelin, Hari, Lounasmaa, & Sams, 1994) and
tion run is often divided into two subsets where reading (Salmelin, Schnitzler, Schmitz, &
the spatial and temporal overlap of the obtained Freund, 2000), and delayed, covert, and overt
dipoles is assessed, and those which are reliable word or verb generation tasks (Bowyer et al.,
between the two subsets are retained. The result- 2005; Fisher et al., 2008; Pang, Wang, Malone,
ing dipole maps depict clusters of reliable source Kadis, & Donner, 2011; Ressel et al., 2006;
locations. In a manner similar to fMRI, a lateral- Yamamoto et al., 2006). Despite over a decade of
ity index can be calculated by taking the differ- research, and many reliable findings, the MEG
ence between the number of right- and language and memory mapping literature is still
left-hemisphere sources divided by their sum, at somewhat of an earlier stage of development
thereby providing a hemispheric dominance than that of fMRI, owing largely to the smaller
index (LI) that varies between 1 and −1 (see MEG community and the more recent maturation
Simos et al., 2009 for additional details). This of well-delineated source modeling approaches
task has been shown to localize activity within and the availability of software tools.
posterior language areas (i.e., in the vicinity of
the superior and middle temporal gyri) in a num-
ber of empirical reports (e.g., Breier et al., 2001; Activation and Contrast Tasks
Merrifield et al., 2007; Papanicolaou et al., 1999)
including large samples of epilepsy patients The source localization approach has been the
(Papanicolaou et al., 2004), in both auditory and dominant paradigm in MEG cognitive mapping
visual stimulus modalities (Papanicolaou et al., for a number of years, with applications of
2006) and in samples of non-English speakers. imaging approaches emerging more recently.
As recently reviewed by Pirmoradi, Béland, For studies utilizing ECDs the number of suc-
Nguyen, Bacon, and Lassonde (2010), many other cessive dipoles within a region of interest (or
tasks have also been investigated for functionally their difference between the hemispheres) has
mapping language functions with MEG. In addi- generally been utilized as a measure of the
206 M. Euler et al.

“extent” of neural activation. The report of perceptual attributes (McDonald et al., 2009).
Bowyer et al. (2005) is one notable exception in Finally, like fMRI, the extent of “activated” cor-
that they utilized an imaging approach and the tex detected by an MEG source model strongly
relative duration of activity within left- and depends on the nature of the task, modeling
right-hemisphere regions for calculating LIs, approach, and the statistical thresholds applied
thereby highlighting the diverse metrics avail- (Liljeström et al., 2009).
able for studying language lateralization with
MEG. Given that focal source models necessar-
ily spatially constrain what is presumably dis- Behavioral Monitoring
tributed activation (at least in the case of higher
cognitive functions), source imaging and spatial Due to the large magnetic artifacts created by the
filtering approaches naturally provide superior facial muscles involved in speaking, overt
indices of the spatial distribution of language- language tasks present a challenge for monitoring
related neural activity. spoken responses in MEG. Nonetheless, MEG
Contrast tasks have been utilized less fre- has been successfully utilized for localizing both
quently in MEG language research than in fMRI, receptive and expressive aspects of language
though this trend is changing, especially with the functions, including with paradigms that permit
wider application of source imaging models (e.g., behavioral monitoring. A number of studies
Kamada et al., 1998; Kober et al., 2001; report localization of language-related activity
McDonald et al., 2009). Whereas changes in the without recording behavioral data (Hirata et al.,
BOLD signal are measured on the order of sec- 2010; Yamamoto et al., 2006) though most
onds, in many instances MEG paradigms are paradigms have utilized covert reading or naming
designed to detect only the inherently transient in conjunction with manual responses to ensure
changes, which are thought to be more phase accurate task performance. Some groups have
locked than time locked to stimulus presentation. utilized vigilance trials which are unrelated to the
This has been suggested as a reason for lack of task, such as a randomly interspersed stimulus
correspondence with fMRI results (Liljeström, prompting a button press, to help ensure attention
Hultén, Parkkonen, & Salmelin, 2009; Pang to the task. Overt and delayed responses have
et al., 2011). Devising effective MEG contrasts is also been successfully collected in MEG studies
thus complicated by the additional dimension of (Salmelin et al., 1994), including for challenging
time, since the timing of component neural pro- contexts such as chronic stuttering (Salmelin
cesses might meaningfully differ depending on et al., 2000).
the features of probe and contrast tasks (Salmelin Although behavioral monitoring is clearly
& Parkkonen, 2010). In such cases, a simple sub- desirable to ensure task compliance and/or equate
traction of contrast and probe task maps could for difficulty between conditions, functional
lead to erroneous conclusions if the contrast does language mapping has been less successful in
not optimally equate for processes peripheral to individuals who are unable to comply with task
the study question (e.g., visual processing, atten- demands. For this subset of patients, it would be
tion) and in a way that does not alter their timing. advantageous to have paradigms capable of
As a solution to this issue, Levelt et al. (1998) establishing hemispheric language dominance
argued that if the component processes of a given using tasks requiring minimal behavioral
behavior can be separated into their temporal compliance. At least two reports have approached
stages (i.e., object recognition, lexical access, this by examining lateralization of the 100 ms
etc., in the case of picture naming), one can auditory response (N100m) to speech sounds in a
manipulate any given stage and only alter the simple behavioral condition (Gootjes et al., 1999;
time course of the subsequent stages. The absence Kirveskari et al., 2006). In these studies healthy
of activity in the subtraction maps at early laten- subjects listened to pairs of tones or spoken
cies provides some assurance that probe and con- vowel sounds and were asked to respond with a
trast tasks were well matched in terms of their finger lift when they perceived that the two
9 MEG in the Presurgical Epilepsy Evaluation 207

stimuli were identical. This task is arguably the majority of control subjects but not LMTS
minimally cognitively demanding. Nontarget patients (86 vs. 33 %). Conversely, right medial
sounds were analyzed and in both reports results temporal dipole clusters were observed in all
demonstrated stronger N100m responses to LMTS patients (100 %), relative to only 2 of 7
speech sounds but not pure tones in the left control subjects (28 %), with the difference being
compared to the right hemisphere, in right- statistically significant. The direction of medial
handed subjects. The latter report also included temporal LIs also significantly differed between
weakly right-handed and left-handed subjects, groups, with patients demonstrating rightward
and LIs showed trends for less left-lateralized mesial temporal laterality, relative to left lateral-
N100m responses to speech sounds in the those ization in controls. Finally, patients and controls
groups; an effect that significantly differed also significantly differed with regard to the num-
between strong right-handers and left-handers ber of runs on which each respective group
(Kirveskari et al., 2006). showed right medial temporal activity (67 vs.
Separately, Kim and Chung (2008) employed 7 %).
an auditory oddball task involving two spoken Speaking to the validity of their findings, the
monosyllabic words, with each being the standard authors noted that medial temporal activity always
and deviant stimulus, respectively. Participants followed what was presumed to be language-
were epilepsy patients who were merely asked to related activity in posterior temporal areas and
ignore the auditory stimuli and watch a movie that both left and right medial temporal sources
during the recording, making no response to the showed similar time courses. The results of this
spoken words. LIs generated from the oscillatory study are consistent with other research demon-
mismatch negativity response to deviant stimuli strating abnormal lateralization of memory func-
demonstrated 71 % concordance with Wada tions in LMTS (Richardson, Strange, Duncan, &
results for a posterior superior temporal gyrus Dolan, 2006) and provide evidence that the CRM
ROI and between 76 % and 94 % concordance elicits both perisylvian and medial temporal lobe
for a posterior inferior frontal gyrus ROI. activity.
Another report explicitly examined MEG data
obtained from successful verbal memory
Episodic Memory encoding to identify a paradigm for determining
hemispheric memory lateralization with
There have been relatively few MEG studies that MEG. Maestú et al. (2009) examined lateraliza-
have specifically examined memory functions in tion of medial temporal activity, along with other
epilepsy. One study utilized a variant of the con- regions, in a group of nine epilepsy patients with
tinuous recognition memory paradigm to examine left hippocampal sclerosis and nine age-matched
medial temporal lobe activity in a group of healthy controls (Maestú et al., 2009). The authors com-
subjects and patients with left mesial temporal puted event-related averages from encoding trials
sclerosis (LMTS) (Ver Hoef, Sawrie, Killen, & presenting words later recalled following a delay
Knowlton, 2008). The authors reasoned that since and utilized a source localization approach to
the performance of the CRM task involves both model the data. Among other findings, results
language and memory processes, the paradigm demonstrated a group-by-hemisphere interaction
might elicit lateralized medial temporal lobe for the medial temporal region of interest, where
activity in addition to the robust activation it elic- controls exhibited greater numbers of left medial
its in posterior temporal areas. A variant of the temporal dipoles throughout the analysis window
auditory CRM protocol was investigated in a final relative to patients. In addition, medial temporal
sample of six patients with LMTS and seven LIs between controls and patients with hippo-
healthy controls, and a source localization campal sclerosis significantly differed in a 400–
approach involving clusters of dipoles was uti- 600 ms latency window. Within-group analyses
lized. Across two runs of the CRM task, left demonstrated significant rightward asymmetry in
mesial temporal dipole clusters were observed in the patient group in an overlapping time window,
208 M. Euler et al.

whereas controls showed a nonsignificant sessions) was assessed for the auditory CRM par-
tendency toward leftward asymmetry. While just adigm. Findings demonstrated overall inter-rater
a handful of reports have explicitly investigated reliability = 0.88 for LIs derived from two differ-
medial temporal activation in the context of epi- ent approaches to epoch selection prior to averag-
lepsy using MEG (Hanlon et al., 2003), these and ing, whereas intersession reliability ranged from
similar studies are increasingly providing the 0.60 to 0.69 across raters (Lee, Sawrie, Simos,
“proof of principal” supporting a role for MEG in Killen, & Knowlton, 2006).
mapping medial temporal memory systems. In addition to the numerous reports investigat-
ing variants of the CRM paradigm, other studies
with substantial sample sizes have employed
MEG Language Mapping tasks such as visual word categorization (100 %
in Presurgical Epilepsy Patients concordance in 87 patients using combined MEG
and fMRI data (Kamada et al., 2007), and silent
The continuous recognition memory paradigm reading (85 % using a frontal LI in 60 patients
studied by Papanicolaou and colleagues has been (Hirata et al., 2010), and also demonstrated good
the most widely investigated language task used concordance between MEG hemispheric domi-
in functional mapping in epilepsy. The largest of nance and Wada. For a detailed review of MEG
these studies assessed hemispheric dominance language studies and concordance with Wada,
using the CRM paradigm in a group of 100 see Pirmoradi et al. (2010).
consecutive patients between the ages of 8 and 56 Overall, the recognition memory paradigm
with medically intractable epilepsy (Papanicolaou employed by Papanicolaou et al. (1999) has been
et al., 2004). The task and analysis were the most rigorously validated of available MEG
performed according to the usual parameters language paradigms. Whereas Wada testing has
utilized by this group, with 85 patients included been considered the “gold standard” against
in the final sample. MEG LIs were compared to which to compare MEG laterality assessments,
Wada hemispheric dominance ratings and the CRM in many ways could be considered the
demonstrated 87 % concordance, with most gold standard for MEG assessment of language
discordant cases (7/11) being those where MEG laterality. It has a number of features to recom-
showed significant bilateral activity while Wada mend it, including: the relative simplicity of the
showed left-hemisphere language dominance. task, acquisition, and typical analysis protocols; a
There were no cases where MEG and Wada solid record of replicability within and across
indicated language dominance solely in opposite instruments and laboratories (Doss, Zhang, Risse,
hemispheres from one another. Compared to & Dickens, 2009; Lee et al., 2006) and in differ-
Wada-based ratings of language presence or ent stimulus modalities (Papanicolaou et al.,
absence in the operative hemisphere, the CRM 2004); and its high degree of concordance with
procedure demonstrated specificity = 0.83, and Wada results in a large clinical sample of epi-
sensitivity, and negative and positive predictive lepsy patients.
values all exceeding 0.91. While implementing any given mapping proce-
Other studies have examined the effect of vari- dure involves accepting various tradeoffs, there are
ations to the CRM analysis procedures in smaller some areas in which other tasks could offer advan-
groups of epilepsy patients and controls. In one tages over the CRM paradigm. Depending on the
report, results showed that an average of four clinical needs of the specific context, these might
different data reduction techniques demonstrated include: stronger differentiation of language from
the best sensitivity, specificity, and concordance memory processes, greater elicitation of sources
with Wada (Merrifield et al., 2007). However, the outside of posterior temporal areas, more precise
strength of those results was diluted when data manipulation of task difficulty or stimulus features
from several subjects with IQs below the average (Salmelin, 2007), and implementation of contrast
range were included. In another report, inter-rater tasks. Also, while not an inherent feature of the
and intersession reliability (separate same-day task, it is clear that source imaging and spatial fil-
9 MEG in the Presurgical Epilepsy Evaluation 209

tering approaches possess an inherent advantage three of twelve patients, all of whom had shown
over dipole-based source models for spatiotempo- bilateral language representation on the basis of
ral mapping of language-related activity and other preoperative Wada testing (two of whom were
distributed cognitive functions. Finally, of particu- rated as having bilateral language dominance on
lar importance to the presurgical epilepsy context, the basis of preoperative MEG). In addition,
it has been reported that certain MEG language postoperative MEG suggested an inferior dis-
tasks elicit strong bilateral activation of posterior placement of language-related activity in the left-
superior temporal sources, thereby biasing source hemisphere region of interest in a subset (3 of 7)
localization approaches toward neglecting weaker, of patients who showed left-hemisphere language
but lateralized activity in nearby anterior or ventral dominance based on the Wada, as well as in one
temporal regions (McDonald et al., 2009). Given patient with bilateral language representation
the importance of anterior temporal lobe activation preoperatively (both MEG and Wada). The small
in particular to predicting surgical outcomes from sample size precluded formal statistical compari-
functional imaging maps, this is likely to be an sons of neuropsychological test performance
especially important direction for future paradigm across time points; however, the authors noted
development in MEG language mapping. that in general those who showed left-hemisphere
language dominance on MEG tended to perform
better on verbal and performance IQ measures
Assessing Postsurgical relative to those showing atypical lateralization.
Change with MEG The authors suggested that these findings
implicate reorganization of posterior temporal
Given the relative novelty of MEG language map- language areas following anterior temporal resec-
ping research, little work has been done investi- tions and noted that the obtained displacements
gating its utility in predicting cognitive outcomes exceeded their previous estimates of intraindi-
following surgery, and no large-scale studies vidual test-retest reliability. While additional
have been conducted. However, in one study, research is likely needed to better understand the
Pataraia et al. (2005) examined pre- to postopera- effects of surgical removal of language-related
tive changes in MEG language maps following areas on MEG source localizations, studies such
left anterior temporal lobectomy in a sample of as this provide a foundation for future applica-
12 patients with intractable epilepsy. Participants tions of MEG postoperative assessment.
completed the CRM task prior to surgery and
at a mean of 40 months postoperatively. Ten of
the twelve patients were right-handed, and Wada Future Directions: Investigating
testing indicated left-hemisphere language domi- Neuronal Connectivity,
nance in seven of them, with bilateral language a Multimodal Approach
representation in the remaining five. Preoperative and Contribution from MEG
dominance ratings obtained with the MEG CRM
task suggested left-lateralized dominance in Computational and conceptual advances in the
eight patients, with bilateral representation in last decade or so have greatly expanded MEG/
four. MEG and Wada assessments were discor- EEG research beyond simply analyzing time-
dant (suggesting left and bilateral representation, locked, event-related averages (Makeig, Debener,
respectively) in a single patient. Reliably obtained Onton, & Delorme, 2004). Clinical and academic
perisylvian sources were retained as putatively investigators are increasingly recognizing the
comprising Wernicke’s area, and postoperative value that various time-frequency measures (e.g.,
change in the location of language-related activ- event-related desynchronization, induced activ-
ity was defined as a 15 mm or greater displace- ity, intertrial/regional phase locking, cross-fre-
ment in the center of the reliable dipole cluster. quency coupling) can add to studies of cognition
Postoperative MEG assessment suggested (for introductions see Canolty & Knight, 2010;
rightward shift of language representation in Singer, 1999; Varela, Lachaux, Rodriguez &
210 M. Euler et al.

Martinerie, 2001). These measures have broad- of dipole localization accuracy in MEG using the
bootstrap. NeuroImage, 25, 355–368.
ened MEG analysis beyond conventional studies
Delorme, A., & Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: an open
of time-locked averages and now enable observa- source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynam-
tions of ongoing neural oscillations, single-trial ics including independent component analysis.
analysis, and interregional connectivity. The var- Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 134(1), 9–21.
Doss, R. C., Zhang, W., Risse, G. L., & Dickens, D. L.
ious source modeling approaches outlined above
(2009). Lateralizing language with magnetic source
have been integrated with these indices to vary- imaging: validation based on the Wada test. Epilepsia,
ing degrees, and several reports listed here have 50(10), 2242–2248.
successfully analyzed neural oscillatory activity Feynman, R. P., Leighton, R. B., & Sands, M. L. (1964).
The Feynman lectures on physics (Vol. 2). Readnig,
for functional mapping (e.g., Hirata et al., 2010;
MA: Addison-Wesley.
Kim & Chung, 2008; Pang et al., 2011). Moving Fisher, A. E., Furlong, P. L., Seri, S., Adjamian, P., Witton,
forward, integrating magnetic source analysis C., Baldeweg, T., et al. (2008). Interhemispheric dif-
with emerging oscillatory measures will enable ferences of spectral power in expressive language: A
MEG study with clinical applications. International
MEG to increase its contribution to the collabo-
Journal of Psychophysiology, 68(2), 111–122.
ration with fMRI and clinical neuropsychology Frye, R. E., Rezaie, R., & Papanicolaou, A. C. (2009).
in the successful evaluation, counseling, and Functional neuroimaging of language using magne-
surgical management of medically intractable toencephalography. Physics of Life Reviews, 6(1), 1–10.
Gootjes, L., Raij, T., Salmelin, R., & Hari, R. (1999).
epilepsy.
Left-hemisphere dominance for processing of vowels:
a whole-scalp neuromagnetic study. Neuroreport,
10(14), 2987–2991.
References Hämäläinen, M., Hari, R., Ilmoniemi, R., Knuutila, J., &
Lounasmaa, O. V. (1993). Magnetoencephalography
Attal, Y., Bhattacharjee, M., Yelnik, J., Cottereau, B., – Theory, instrumentation, and applications to nonin-
Lefévre, J., Okada, Y., et al. (2009). Modelling and vasive studies of the working human brain. Reviews of
detecting deep brain activity with MEG and EEG. Modern Physics, 65, 413–497.
IRBM, 30(3), 133–138. Hanlon, F. M., Weisend, M. P., Huang, M., Lee, R. R.,
Baillet, S. B. (2010). The dowser in the fields: Searching for Moses, S. N., Paulson, K. M., et al. (2003). A non-
MEG sources. In P. C. Hansen, M. L. Kringelbach, & R. invasive method for observing hippocampal function.
Salmelin (Eds.), MEG: An introduction to methods (pp. NeuroReport, 14(15), 1957–1960.
83–124). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Hansen, P. C., Kringelbach, M. L., & Salmelin, R. (Eds.).
Baillet, S., Mosher, J. C., & Leahy, R. M. (2001). (2010). MEG: An introduction to methods. Oxford:
Electromagnetic brain mapping. IEEE Signal New York.
Processing Magazine, 18, 16. Härle, M., Dobe, C., Cohen, R., & Rockstroh, B. (2002).
Bilder, R. M. (2011). Neuropsychology 3.0: Evidence- Brain activity during syntactic and semantic process-
based science and practice. Journal of the International ing—A magnetoencephalographic study. Brain
Neuropsychological Society, 17, 7–13. Topography, 15, 3–11.
Bowyer, S. M., Moran, J. E., Weiland, B. J., Mason, K. M., Helenius, P., Parviainen, T., Paetau, R., & Salmelin, R.
Greenwald, M. L., Smith, B. J., et al. (2005). Language (2009). Neural processing of spoken words in specific
laterality determined by MEG mapping with language impairment and dyslexia. Brain, 132(7),
MR-FOCUSS. Epilepsy and Behavior, 6(2), 235–241. 1918–1927.
Breier, J. I., Simos, P. G., Wheless, J. W., Constantinou, J. Henson, R. N., Flandin, G., Friston, K. J., & Mattout, J.
E., Baumgartner, J. E., Venkataraman, V., et al. (2001). (2010). A parametric empirical Bayesian framework
Language dominance in children as determined by for fMRI-constrained MEG/EEG source reconstruc-
magnetic source imaging and the intracarotid amobar- tion. Human Brain Mapping, 31(10), 1512–1531.
bital procedure: a comparison. Journal of Child Hillebrand, A., & Barnes, G. R. (2002). A quantitative
Neurology, 16(2), 124–130. assessment of the sensitivity of whole-head MEG to
Canolty, R. T., & Knight, R. T. (2010). The functional role activity in the adult human cortex. NeuroImage, 16(3
of cross-frequency coupling. Trends in Cognitive Pt 1), 638–650.
Sciences, 14(11), 506–515. Hillebrand, A., Singh, K. D., Holliday, I. E., Furlong, P.
Carrette, E., De Tiege, X., De Beeck, M. O., De Herdt, V., L., & Barnes, G. R. (2005). A new approach to
Meurs, A., Legros, B., et al. (2011). Magnetoen- neuroimaging with magnetoencephalography.
cephalography in epilepsy patients carrying a vagus Human Brain Mapping, 25(2), 199–211.
nerve stimulator. Epilepsy Research, 93(1), 44–52. Hirata, M., Goto, T., Barnes, G., Umekawa, Y.,
Darvas, F., Rautiainen, M., Pantazis, D., Baillet, S., Yanagisawa, T., Kato, A., et al. (2010). Language
Mosher, J. C., Garnero, L., et al. (2005). Investigations dominance and mapping based on neuromagnetic
9 MEG in the Presurgical Epilepsy Evaluation 211

oscillatory changes: Comparison with invasive proce- Lin, F.-H., Belliveau, J. W., Dale, A. M., & Hämäläinen, M. S.
dures. Journal of Neurosurgery, 112(3), 528–538. (2006). Distributed current estimates using cortical orien-
Jerbi, K., Mosher, J. C., Baillet, S. B., & Leahy, R. M. (2002). tation constraints. Human Brain Mapping, 27(1), 1–13.
On MEG forward modelling using multipolar expan- Lin, F.-H., Witzel, T., Ahlfors, S. P., Stufflebeam, S. M.,
sions. Physics in Medicine and Biology, 7(4), 532–555. Belliveau, J. W., & Hämäläinen, M. S. (2006).
Kamada, K., Kober, H., Saguer, M., Möller, M., Assessing and improving the spatial accuracy in MEG
Kaltenhäuser, M., & Vieth, J. (1998). Responses to source localization by depth-weighted minimum-
silent Kanji reading of the native Japanese and German norm estimates. NeuroImage, 31(1), 160–171.
in task subtraction magnetoencephalography. Lopes da Silva, F. H. (2010). Electrophysiological basis of
Cognitive Brain Research, 7(1), 89–98. MEG signals. In M. L. Kringelbach, P. C. Hansen, &
Kamada, K., Sawamura, Y., Takeuchi, F., Kuriki, S., R. Salmelin (Eds.), MEG: An introduction to methods
Kawai, K., Morita, A., et al. (2007). Expressive and (pp. 1–23). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
receptive language areas determined by a non-invasive Maestú, F., Campo, P., García-Morales, I., del Barrio, A.,
reliable method using functional magnetic resonance Paul, N., del Pozo, F., et al. (2009). Biomagnetic pro-
imaging and magnetoencephalography. Neurosurgery, files of verbal memory success in patients with mesial
60(2), 296–305. discussion 305–306. temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy and Behavior, 16(3),
Kamada, K., Takeuchi, F., Kuriki, S., Todo, T., Morita, A., 527–533.
& Sawamura, Y. (2006). Dissociated expressive and Makeig, S., Debener, S., Onton, J., & Delorme, A. (2004).
receptive language functions on magnetoencephalog- Mining event-related brain dynamics. Trends in
raphy, functional magnetic resonance imaging, and Cognitive Science, 8(5), 204–210.
amobarbital studies. Case report and review of the lit- Makela, A. M. (2010). The use of MEG in clinical settings.
erature. Journal of Neurosurgery, 104(4), 598–607. In M. L. Kringelbach, P. C. Hansen, & R. Salmelin
Kaufmann, L., & Lu, Z. (2003). Basis of neuromagnetism (Eds.), MEG: An introduction to methods (pp. 373–
and magnetic source imaging. In Z. Lu & L. Kaufmann 402). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
(Eds.), Magnetic source imaging of the human brain. Martin, N. A., Beatty, J., Johnson, R. A., Collaer, M. L.,
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Viñuela, F., Becker, D. P., et al. (1993). Magneto-
Kim, J. S., & Chung, C. K. (2008). Language lateraliza- encephalographic localization of a language process-
tion using MEG beta frequency desynchronization ing cortical area adjacent to a cerebral arteriovenous
during auditory oddball stimulation with one-syllable malformation. Journal of Neurosurgery, 79(4), 584–588.
words. NeuroImage, 42(4), 1499–1507. Martin, T., Houck, J. M., Bish, J. P., Kičić, D., Woodruff,
Kirveskari, E., Salmelin, R., & Hari, R. (2006). C. C., Moses, S. N., et al. (2006). MEG reveals differ-
Neuromagnetic responses to vowels vs. tones reveal ent contributions of somatomotor cortex and cerebel-
hemispheric lateralization. Clinical Neurophysiology, lum to simple reaction time after temporally structured
117(3), 643–648. cues. Human Brain Mapping, 27(7), 552–561.
Kober, H., Moller, M., Nimsky, C., Vieth, J., Fahlbusch, McDonald, C. R., Thesen, T., Hagler, D. J., Jr., Carlson,
R., & Ganslandt, O. (2001). New approach to localize C., Devinksy, O., Kuzniecky, R., et al. (2009).
speech relevant brain areas and hemispheric domi- Distributed source modeling of language with magne-
nance using spatially filtered magnetoencephalogra- toencephalography: Application to patients with
phy. Human Brain Mapping, 14(4), 236–250. intractable epilepsy. Epilepsia, 50(10), 2256–2266.
Kringelbach, M. L., Jenkinson, N., Green, A. L., Owen, S. L. Merrifield, W. S., Simos, P. G., Papanicolaou, A. C.,
F., Hansen, P. C., Cornelissen, P. L., et al. (2007). Deep Philpott, L. M., & Sutherling, W. W. (2007).
brain stimulation for chronic pain investigated with mag- Hemispheric language dominance in magnetoenceph-
netoencephalography. NeuroReport, 18(3), 223–228. alography: Sensitivity, specificity, and data reduction
210.1097/WNR.1090b1013e328010dc328013d. techniques. Epilepsy and Behavior, 10(1), 120–128.
Leahy, R. M., Mosher, J. C., Spencer, M. E., Huang, M. Miller, G. A., Elbert, T., Sutton, B. P., & Heller, W. (2007).
X., & Lewine, J. D. (1998). A study of dipole localiza- Innovative clinical assessment technologies:
tion accuracy for MEG and EEG using a human skull Challenges and opportunities in neuroimaging.
phantom. Electroencephalography and Clinical Psychological Assessment, 19(1), 58–73.
Neurophysiology, 107(2), 159–173. Mosher, J. C., Spencer, M. E., Leahy, R. M., & Lewis, P.
Lee, D., Sawrie, S. M., Simos, P. G., Killen, J., & S. (1993). Error bounds for EEG and MEG dipole
Knowlton, R. C. (2006). Reliability of language map- source localization. Electroencephalography and
ping with magnetic source imaging in epilepsy surgery Clinical Neurophysiology, 86, 303–321.
candidates. Epilepsy and Behavior, 8(4), 742–749. Murakami, S., & Okada, Y. (2006). Contributions of prin-
Levelt, W. J., Praamstra, P., Meyer, A. S., Helenius, P., & cipal neocortical neurons to magnetoencephalography
Salmelin, R. (1998). An MEG study of picture naming. and electroencephalography signals. The Journal of
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10(5), 553–567. Physiology, 575(Pt 3), 925–936.
Liljeström, M., Hultén, A., Parkkonen, L., & Salmelin, R. Okada, Y. C., Lahteenmäki, A., & Xu, C. (1999).
(2009). Comparing MEG and fMRI views to naming Experimental analysis of distortion of magnetoen-
actions and objects. Human Brain Mapping, 30(6), cephalography signals by the skull. Clinical
1845–1856. Neurophysiology, 110(2), 230–238.
212 M. Euler et al.

Pang, E. W., Wang, F., Malone, M., Kadis, D. S., & Salmelin, R., Schnitzler, A., Schmitz, F., & Freund, H. J.
Donner, E. J. (2011). Localization of Broca’s area (2000). Single word reading in developmental stutter-
using verb generation tasks in the MEG: Validation ers and fluent speakers. Brain, 123(6), 1184–1202.
against fMRI. Neuroscience Letters, 490(3), 215–219. Simos, P. G., Breier, J. I., Zouridakis, G., & Papanicolaou,
Papanicolaou, A. C., Pazo-Alvarez, P., Castillo, E. M., A. C. (1998). Assessment of functional cerebral later-
Billingsley-Marshall, R. L., Breier, J. I., Swank, P. R., ality for language using magnetoencephalography.
et al. (2006). Functional neuroimaging with MEG: Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology, 15(4), 364–372.
Normative language profiles. NeuroImage, 33(1), Simos, P. G., Papanicolaou, A. C., Castillo, E. M., &
326–342. Buchanan, D. S. (2009). Evoked magnetic fields. In A.
Papanicolaou, A. C., Simos, P. G., Breier, J. I., Zouridakis, C. Papanicolaou (Ed.), Clinical magnetoencephalog-
G., Willmore, L. J., Wheless, J. W., et al. (1999). raphy and magnetic source imaging. Cambridge:
Magnetoencephalographic mapping of the language- Cambridge University Press.
specific cortex. Journal of Neurosurgery, 90(1), 85–93. Singer, W. (1999). Neuronal synchrony: A versatile code
Papanicolaou, A. C., Simos, P. G., Castillo, E. M., Breier, for the definition of relations? Neuron, 24(1), 49–65.
J. I., Sarkari, S., Pataraia, E., et al. (2004). Szymanski, M. D., Perry, D. W., Gage, N. M., Rowley, H.
Magnetocephalography: A noninvasive alternative to A., Walker, J., Berger, M. S., et al. (2001). Magnetic
the Wada procedure. Journal of Neurosurgery, 100(5), source imaging of late evoked field responses to vowels:
867–876. Toward an assessment of hemispheric dominance for
Pataraia, E., Billingsley-Marshall, R. L., Castillo, E. M., language. Journal of Neurosurgery, 94(3), 445–453.
Breier, J. I., Simos, P. G., Sarkari, S., et al. (2005). Szymanski, M. D., Rowley, H. A., & Roberts, T. P. (1999).
Organization of receptive language-specific cortex A hemispherically asymmetrical MEG response to
before and after left temporal lobectomy. Neurology, vowels. Neuroreport, 10(12), 2481–2486.
64(3), 481–487. Tanaka, N., Thiele, E. A., Madsen, J. R., Bourgeois, B. F.,
Pirmoradi, M., Béland, R., Nguyen, D. K., Bacon, B. A., & Stufflebeam, S. M. (2009). Magnetoencephalographic
& Lassonde, M. (2010). Language tasks used for the analysis in patients with vagus nerve stimulator.
presurgical assessment of epileptic patients with Pediatric Neurology, 41(5), 383–387.
MEG. Epileptic Disorders, 12(2), 97–108. Varela, F., Lachaux, J. P., Rodriguez, E., & Martinerie, J.
Ramirez, R. R., Kopell, B. H., Butson, C. R., Gaggl, W., (2001). The brainweb: Phase synchronization and
Friedland, D. R., & Baillet, S. (2009). Neuromagnetic large-scale integration. Nature Reviews Neuroscience,
source imaging of abnormal spontaneous activity in tin- 2(4), 229–239.
nitus patient modulated by electrical cortical stimulation. Ver Hoef, L. W., Sawrie, S., Killen, J., & Knowlton, R. C.
Minneapolis, MN: Annual International Conference of (2008). Left mesial temporal sclerosis and verbal
the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society. memory: A magnetoencephalography study. Journal
Ressel, V., Wilke, M., Lidzba, K., Preissl, H., Krägeloh-Mann, of Clinical Neurophysiology, 25(1), 1–6. doi:10.1097/
I., & Lutzenberger, W. (2006). Language lateralization in WNP.1090b1013e318163a318166c318160.
magnetoencephalography: Two tasks to investigate hemi- Vrba, J., & Robinson, S. E. (2001). Signal processing in
spheric dominance. NeuroReport, 17(11), 1209–1213. magnetoencephalography. Methods, 25(2), 249–271.
1210.1097/1201.wnr.0000230506.0000232726.be. Wehner, D. T., Hämäläinen, M. S., Mody, M., & Ahlfors,
Richardson, M. P., Strange, B. A., Duncan, J. S., & Dolan, S. P. (2008). Head movements of children in MEG:
R. J. (2006). Memory fMRI in left hippocampal scle- Quantification, effects on source estimation, and com-
rosis. Neurology, 66(5), 699–705. pensation. NeuroImage, 40(2), 541–550.
Riggs, L., Moses, S. N., Bardouille, T., Herdman, A. T., Wolters, C. H., Anwander, A., Tricoche, X., Weinstein, D.,
Ross, B., & Ryan, J. D. (2009). A complementary ana- Koch, M. A., & MacLeod, R. S. (2006). Influence of
lytic approach to examining medial temporal lobe tissue conductivity anisotropy on EEG/MEG field and
sources using magnetoencephalography. NeuroImage, return current computation in a realistic head model: A
45(2), 627–642. simulation and visualization study using high-resolution
Salmelin, R. (2007). Clinical neurophysiology of lan- finite element modeling. NeuroImage, 30, 813–826.
guage: The MEG approach. Clinical Neurophysiology, Wu, J. Y., Salamon, N., Kirsch, H. E., Mantle, M. M.,
118(2), 237–254. Nagarajan, S. S., Kurelowech, L., et al. (2010).
Salmelin, R., & Baillet, S. (2009). Electromagnetic brain Noninvasive testing, early surgery, and seizure free-
imaging. Human Brain Mapping, 30(6), 1753–1757. dom in tuberous sclerosis complex. Neurology, 74(5),
Salmelin, R., Hari, R., Lounasmaa, O. V., & Sams, M. 392–398.
(1994). Dynamics of brain activation during picture Yamamoto, M., Ukai, S., Shinosaki, K., Ishii, R.,
naming. Nature, 368(6470), 463–465. Kawaguchi, S., Ogawa, A., et al. (2006). Spatially fil-
Salmelin, R., & Parkkonen, L. (2010). Experimental tered magnetoencephalographic analysis of cortical
design. In M. L. Kringelbach, P. C. Hansen, & R. oscillatory changes in basic brain rhythms during the
Salmelin (Eds.), MEG: An introduction to methods Japanese ‘Shiritori’ Word Generation Task.
(pp. 75–82). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Neuropsychobiology, 53(4), 215–222.
Evaluation of Psychiatric
and Personality Disorders 10
Jana E. Jones

identified psychological disturbances as occurring


Psychiatric Comorbidity in Epilepsy as part of the seizure itself (ictal) and between sei-
zures (interictal) resulting in cognitive and behav-
Historical Perspective ioral dysfunction. Gowers (1881) described the
difference between epileptic convulsions and
Throughout history, epilepsy has been viewed as nonepileptic convulsions. In Europe with the
being caused by evil possession, a curse, and development of institutions and hospitals for the
even as a divine intervention by God. Hippocrates, insane, individuals with epilepsy were sent to
in The Sacred Disease written in 400 BC, first these institutions and were treated by psychiatry
suggested that epilepsy was a disease with a natu- until the twentieth century (Reynolds & Trimble,
ral cause, not a divine cause. During the Greco- 2009). In the 1940s and 1950s after the EEG
Roman era, the term “lunatic” was used to began to be used clinically, the temporal lobe
describe individuals with epilepsy separate from focus in epilepsy was identified in 1949, and with
“maniacs” who were possessed by evil (Hill, the increased understanding of the limbic system,
1981). In the Middle Ages, epilepsy was viewed it was often anticipated that people with epilepsy
as mystical, magical, or caused by possession of would also have psychological problems. By the
evil spirits, and this view remained as the most 1960s the World Health Organization (WHO)
prominent cause of epilepsy until the seventeenth classified epilepsy as a neurological disorder, not
and eighteenth centuries (Temkin, 1971). a psychiatric disorder due to the identification of
In the mid- to late nineteenth century with the preictal, ictal, postictal, and interictal psychiatric
emergence of neurology from psychiatry symptoms (Reynolds & Trimble, 2009).
(Reynolds & Trimble, 2009), Hughlings Jackson Around that time, a study in England reported
(1931) described epilepsy as “occasional, sudden that 29 % of individuals with epilepsy had psy-
excessive, rapid and local discharges of gray mat- chological issues (Pond & Bidwell, 1960). In
ter”. In France, Briquet (1859) and Morel (1860) Iceland, Gudmundsson (1966) found that 50 % of
the sample had psychological or personality
changes with more psychological problems asso-
ciated with brain lesions. Similarly Graham and
J.E. Jones, Ph.D. (*) Rutter (1968) reported a higher rate of psychiat-
Matthews Neuropsychology Lab, Department of ric disorders among individuals with brain lesions
Neurology, School of Medicine and Public Health,
and/or mental retardation. Currently, it is under-
University of Wisconsin, 1685 Highland Avenue,
Room 7229, Madison, WI 53705, USA stood that there are a complex number of factors
e-mail: jejones@neurology.wisc.edu including biological, medication, and social

W.B. Barr and C. Morrison (eds.), Handbook on the Neuropsychology of Epilepsy, 213
Clinical Handbooks in Neuropsychology, DOI 10.1007/978-0-387-92826-5_10,
© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015
214 J.E. Jones

factors that influence the expression of psychopa- endorsed symptoms of depression, while Mensah,
thology in epilepsy (LaFrance, Kanner, & Beavis, Thapar, and Kerr (2006) used the Hospital
Hermann, 2008; Swinkels, Kuyk, van Dyck, & Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS) and
Spinhoven, 2005). Today, it is commonly under- reported that 11.2 % endorsed symptoms of depres-
stood that people with epilepsy are at a higher sion. Based on the Clinical Interview Schedule
risk for psychiatric disorders compared to the (CIS), Edeh and Toone (1987) reported that 22 %
general population (LaFrance et al., 2008), and of their sample met criteria for depression. Tellez-
this was exemplified by Fisher et al. (2005) pro- Zenteno et al. (2007) utilized the Composite
posing that the definition of epilepsy includes the International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI) and
coexistence of psychiatric disorders. reported that 17.4 % of their sample had depres-
sion. Adding to the complexity of psychiatric
comorbidity in epilepsy, Hesdorffer, Hauser,
Axis I Psychiatric Disorders Annegers, and Cascino (2000) and Hesdorffer,
in Epilepsy Hauser, Olafsson, Ludvigsson, and Kjartansson
(2006) reported that individuals with a history of
The prevalence of psychiatric disorders ranges depressive disorders or suicidal behavior were at
from 19 % to 80 % (Swinkels et al., 2005). increased risk to develop epilepsy overtime, indi-
Swinkels and collaborators (2005) attribute the cating that there may be a bidirectional relationship
variability in rates to heterogeneity of samples between psychiatric disorders and epilepsy.
and lack of control groups. Additionally, very When compared to depression, anxiety disor-
few of these studies are population based, and as ders are less common in the general population
a result, many studies are based on tertiary care with lifetime prevalence rates ranging from 1.9 to
center samples possibly biasing the sample with 5.1 % (Wittchen et al., 2002; Wittchen, Zhao,
more severe forms of epilepsy and higher rates of Kessler, & Eaton, 1994). However, anxiety disor-
psychiatric disorders (Lacey, Salzberg, Roberts, ders are thought to be the second most common
Trauer, & D'Souza, 2009). Finally, different psychiatric disorder in epilepsy with prevalence
methodologies have been utilized to measure the rates ranging from 11 to 40 %. In a multicenter
presence or absence of psychiatric comorbidity study, 174 consecutive adults were interviewed
in epilepsy including medical record reviews, using the Mini International Neuropsychiatric
self-report measures, psychiatric interviews, and Interview (MINI), and 30 % of the sample met
self-identifying as having a particular psychiatric criteria for an anxiety disorder (Jones et al.,
disorder. Davies, Heyman, and Goodman (2003) 2003). Based on the CIDI, Swinkels, Kuyk, de
and Tellez-Zenteno, Patten, Jette, Williams, and Graaf, van Dyck, and Spinhoven (2001) reported
Wiebe (2007) utilized community-based samples that in a sample of 209 individuals with epilepsy,
and similar interview-based methodologies and 24.9 % had anxiety disorders. Kobau, Gilliam,
reported lower rates of psychiatric comorbidity and Thurman (2006) reported that 39 % of the
(37 and 23.5 %, respectively). population-based sample had an anxiety disorder
The most prevalent psychiatric diagnoses in epi- based on self-report.
lepsy include mood disorders, anxiety disorders, Psychotic disorders are also more frequent in
and psychotic disorders. Depression is the most people with epilepsy compared to the general
common psychiatric disorder in epilepsy with life- population (~3 %) (Perala et al., 2007) with some
time prevalence rates reported from 20 to 60 %, studies reporting rates as high as 10 % (LaFrance
which is consistently higher than the general popu- et al., 2008). As part of a chart review in a
lation with lifetime prevalence rates of 16–20 % population-based health survey in Sweden,
(Kessler et al., 2005). In community-based sam- Forsgren (1992) reported that schizophrenia and
ples, Ettinger, Reed, and Cramer (2004) used the psychosis were prevalent in 0.8 and 0.7 %,
Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression respectively. Utilizing International Classification
Scale (CES-D) and found that 36 % of the sample of Diseases 9 (ICD-9) criteria, Stefansson,
10 Evaluation of Psychiatric and Personality Disorders 215

Olafsson, and Hauser (1998) found that 1.2 % of criteria for a personality disorder. Similarly
the sample had schizophrenia and 6.2 % had psy- Manchanda et al. (1996) assessed 300 epilepsy
chosis. Using similar criteria, Gaitatzis, Trimble, surgery candidates using DSM-IIIR criteria and
and Sander (2004) in the United Kingdom found that 18.3 % had personality disorders with
reported the prevalence of schizophrenia as 0.7 % the primary disorders in Cluster C. In a much
and prevalence of psychosis as 9 %. smaller sample using the SCID, Arnold and
In summary, in spite of the fact that different Privitera (1996) reported that among 27 people
methodologies and samples have been used, there with epilepsy, 18 % were identified with person-
is significant evidence to support the overall con- ality disorders, of which avoidant personality dis-
clusion that there are higher rates of Axis I disor- order was the most common type. Based on the
ders among individuals with epilepsy when SCID-II, Lopez-Rodriguez et al. (1999) found
compared to the general population. that 21 % of 52 individuals evaluated for surgery
had personality disorders particularly those in
Cluster C. In a sample of individuals with epi-
Axis II Personality Disorders lepsy (n = 35) and individuals with nonepileptic
in Epilepsy seizures (n = 10), Krishnamoorthy, Brown, and
Trimble (2001) found that among individuals
In epilepsy, little research has been conducted on with epilepsy, 17 % had personality disorders
the prevalence and types of Axis II disorders or from Cluster A and 17 % from Cluster C.
personality disorders common in people with Similar to the general population, Axis II dis-
epilepsy. There are a number of studies using the orders are less prevalent compared to Axis I dis-
Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory orders. The above studies indicated that
(MMPI) to identify personality dysfunction in approximately 18 % of the samples met criteria
epilepsy, but the use of this instrument in epi- for a personality disorder. Additionally, Cluster C
lepsy has been criticized (Provinciali, Franciolini, personality disorders appear to be a more com-
del Pesce, & Signorino, 1989), and for purposes mon personality disorder in epilepsy. In the
of this chapter, the MMPI-based literature in epi- worldwide population, prevalence estimates indi-
lepsy will not be reviewed. Additionally, very cate that 6.1 % have any personality disorder and
few studies have been conducted utilizing the of those 3.6 % are Cluster A, 1.5 % are Cluster B,
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental and 2.7 % are Cluster C (Huang et al., 2009).
Disorders (DSM) or ICD diagnostic criteria to Cluster A personality disorders include odd or
identify and classify personality disorders in epi- eccentric characteristics and correspond to para-
lepsy. In a small sample of individuals with epi- noid, schizoid, and schizotypal personality disor-
lepsy (n = 21) and controls with other neurological ders. Cluster B personality disorders are described
conditions (n = 24), Schwartz and Cummings as dramatic, emotional, and erratic and include
(1988) reported that 38 % of the individuals with antisocial, borderline, narcissistic, and histrionic
epilepsy met DSM-III criteria for a personality personality disorders. Cluster C personality dis-
disorder and only 4 % of controls. Fiordelli, orders are those disorders that are considered
Beghi, Bogliun, and Crespi (1993) evaluated 100 anxious or fearful and include avoidant personal-
individuals with epilepsy seen in an outpatient ity disorder, dependent personality disorder, and
clinic compared to 100 controls using the Clinical obsessive-compulsive personality disorder.
Interview Schedule (CIS) and DSM-IIIR diag- In a voxel-based morphometry (VBM) study,
nostic criteria. Only 4 % of individuals with epi- using the SCID-II, de Araujo Filho et al. (2009)
lepsy and no controls meet criteria for personality examined the relationship between personality
disorders. Victoroff, Benson, Grafton, Engel, and disorders, specifically Cluster B, juvenile myo-
Mazziotta (1994) utilized the Structured Clinical clonic epilepsy (JME), and structural brain
Interview for DSM (SCID) to assess 60 surgery abnormalities. Significant reductions in the thala-
candidates and identified 18.33 % as meeting mus and increases in the mesioprefrontal and
216 J.E. Jones

frontobasal region volumes were reported in indi- be present from hours to 1–3 days before a sei-
viduals with JME and personality disorders. zure (Blanchet & Frommer, 1986). Kanner, Soto,
Since the orbitofrontal cortex is believed to be and Gross-Kanner (2004) systematically studied
involved in mood reactivity, impulsivity, and postictal symptoms of depression in individuals
social behavior and is considered to be dysfunc- with refractory partial seizures and found that
tional in individuals with Cluster B personality depressive symptoms were present 50 % of the
disorders, the authors hypothesized that there is time and lasted for a median duration of 24 h and
some indication there may be neuronal dysfunc- occurred in 43 % of the sample. The most fre-
tion impacting the development of seizures and quently reported depressive symptoms in the ictal
personality disorders. phase include anhedonia, guilt, and suicidal ide-
There have been a few personality disorder ation. Mood changes are typically brief, stereo-
studies that have compared individuals with epi- typical, and followed by a change in consciousness
lepsy to those individuals with nonepileptic psy- as the ictus moves from a simple to complex par-
chogenic events (NES). In a study by Harden tial seizure (LaFrance et al., 2008). Additionally,
et al. (2009) using the SCID-II, individuals with an interictal form of depression, interictal dys-
NES were compared to individuals with epilepsy phoric disorder, has been described in the litera-
in order to identify the complex personality prob- ture and is reported to be present in about 30 % of
lems associated with NES and epilepsy. individuals with epilepsy and mood disorders.
Individuals with NES were significantly more The symptoms are often described as chronic
likely to have Cluster A and B diagnoses com- dysthymic symptoms that are variable and can be
pared to individuals with epilepsy who were mixed with periods of euphoria, irritability, anxi-
more likely to present with Cluster C diagnoses. ety, paranoid feelings, and somatic symptoms
Additionally, Kuyk, Swinkels, and Spinhoven (Blumer & Altshuler, 1998).
(2003), using the CIDI, compared individuals Symptoms of anxiety and fear are the most
with NES to individuals with NES and epilepsy. commonly reported ictal psychiatric symptoms.
Among individuals with NES, somatoform disor- It can be the sole or predominant clinical symp-
ders were more common, and those with com- tom of a partial seizure or aura. It is usually
bined NES and epilepsy were more likely to believed to originate in the temporal lobe. Ictal
present with personality disorders with signifi- panic can also be present. Ictal panic is short in
cantly higher rates of Cluster C disorders. duration lasting only 30 s; it is stereotypical in
Similar to Axis I disorders, Axis II disorders presentation, is out of context with the situation,
appear to be common in individuals with epi- and is often associated with confusion, altered
lepsy. However, the research in this area is some- consciousness, and automatisms. In 25 % of indi-
what limited and additional population-based viduals with ictal panic, interictal panic attacks
studies in epilepsy need to be conducted. are also reported (Vazquez & Devinsky, 2003).
Ictal psychotic symptoms typically occur in
the context of nonconvulsive status and are often
Diagnostic Issues in Epilepsy accompanied by automatisms and unresponsive-
(Preictal, Ictal, Postictal, Interictal, ness. An EEG will assist in confirming that the
AEDs, Surgery) psychotic symptoms are ictal phenomenon.
Postictal psychotic disorders in epilepsy typically
When evaluating psychopathology in epilepsy, it have a short duration from hours to a few days
is important to determine if there is a relationship and occur in 7 % of individuals with partial epi-
between the psychiatric symptoms and the sei- lepsy (Kanner, 2004). The first symptom is often
zure itself. The most common symptoms associ- insomnia, and this is followed by symptoms that
ated with seizures are depression, anxiety, and are often affective and delusional. An increase in
psychosis. Typically, preictal depressive symp- secondarily generalized seizures, seizure dura-
toms are reported as a dysphoric mood and may tion of more than 10 years, and bilateral focus are
10 Evaluation of Psychiatric and Personality Disorders 217

risk factors for postictal psychotic episodes asso- Impact of Psychiatric Disorders
ciated with epilepsy (LaFrance et al., 2008). in Epilepsy on QOL, Psychosocial
Following a temporal lobectomy, new cases of Factors, and Healthcare Utilization
psychotic disorders have been reported in 3.8–
35.7 % of the surgical cases (Trimble, 1992), and Psychiatric disorders can negatively impact over-
some have hypothesized that this is related to the all psychosocial functioning and quality of life
concept of “forced normalization” (Akanuma among individuals with epilepsy. Several studies
et al., 2005), which is believed to occur following have demonstrated the negative effect of depres-
the normalization of the EEG and subsequent sion on quality of life. Using the BDI, Gilliam
appearance of psychotic symptoms. (2002) reported that the more symptoms of
Antiepileptic drugs (AEDs) can cause psychi- depression independently correlated with lower
atric symptoms (McConnell & Duncan, 1998). health-related quality of life when seizure fre-
The GABAergic properties of some AEDs, like quency did not. Cramer, Blum, Reed, and Fanning
phenobarbital, primidone, benzodiazepines, (2003) found that depression, as measured by the
tiagabine, and vigabatrin, can cause depressive CES-D, negatively impacted quality of life
symptoms. Additionally some of the newer AEDs regardless of seizure type. Additionally, it was
also cause depressive symptoms including felb- reported that even mild levels of depression had a
amate, topiramate, levetiracetam, and zonisamide negative impact on health-related quality of life.
(Kanner, 2003). Finally, depressive symptoms Also using the BDI, Boylan et al. (2004) found
are associated with the discontinuation of some that in treatment resistant epilepsy, depression
AEDs, and these medications include carbam- was a significant predictor of poorer quality of
azepine, valproic acid, and lamotrigine (Ketter life compared to seizure variables which had lit-
et al., 1994). tle impact on quality of life. Finally, Johnson,
Mood and anxiety disorders have been associ- Jones, Seidenberg, and Hermann (2004) reported
ated with the surgical treatment of epilepsy and that depression and anxiety disorders were inde-
primarily with anterior temporal lobectomy. pendently more powerful predictors of lower
Symptoms often present during the first 3–12 quality of life than seizure variables like fre-
months following surgery. Depressive episodes quency and severity.
are more likely to occur in individuals with a his- When examining psychosocial outcomes, a
tory of mood disorders. However, it is possible number of studies have identified a link between
for there to be a complete remission of a mood symptoms of depression and psychosocial status.
disorder associated with a remission of seizures Among individuals with epilepsy, Reisinger and
from surgery. Devinsky et al. (2005) reported that DiIorio (2009) reported that unemployment,
depression and anxiety improved after surgery in social support, and stigma were related to higher
a group of patients with improved seizure con- rates of depressive symptoms, as measured by the
trol. Altshuler, Rausch, Delrahim, Kay, and CES-D. Lee, Lee, and No (2009) found that
Crandall (1999) reported that among individuals social support, unemployment, anxiety, and self-
with temporal lobe epilepsy, 77 % had a history efficacy were predictors of increased depressive
of depression prior to surgery, and 50 % had a symptoms as measured by the BDI. Grabowska-
remission in depressive symptoms following sur- Grzyb, Jedrzejczak, Naganska, and Fiszer (2006)
gery. Only 10 % reported de novo depressive dis- also examined the relationship between depres-
orders after surgery. sion and psychosocial factors in individuals with
It is important to have a clear understanding of epilepsy. They found that unemployment and low
the psychiatric symptoms that are present prior educational attainment were significant predic-
to, during, and after the seizure occurs. The rela- tors of depressive symptoms as measured by the
tionship between the psychiatric symptoms and BDI and Ham-D. The authors discussed the high
the seizure needs to be identified so that an appro- prevalence rates of unemployment and depres-
priate diagnosis can be made. sion in people with epilepsy and indicated that it
218 J.E. Jones

was difficult to understand if this significant rela- Administration (FDA) that indicated that AEDs
tionship with unemployment was secondary to increased suicidal risk, there is a heightened
depression or separate from depression. urgency and awareness to assess and treat individu-
Depression is known to have a negative impact als with epilepsy and comorbid psychiatric condi-
on all facets of life including family, work, tions in order to reduce the risk of suicidal ideation
school, and friends, making it difficult to deter- and suicide in this population (US Department
mine which came first. of Health and Human Services, May 21, 2008).
The impact of psychiatric comorbidity in epi- As clinicians working with and providing ser-
lepsy on healthcare utilization has been exam- vices to people with epilepsy, it important for us
ined in a few studies. Cramer, Blum, Fanning, to understand the implications and impact of psy-
and Reed (2004) reported that depression in epi- chiatric comorbidity in epilepsy. It is increasingly
lepsy contributed to increased healthcare utiliza- part of our responsibility to assess, identify, and
tion. Based on the CES-D, depression, not seizure provide recommendations for treatment of psychi-
type, increased medical and psychiatric visits but atric disorders in this population.
did not increase emergency room visits or total
hospital days. In a community-based sample of
652 individuals with epilepsy, Lacey et al. (2009) Measures for Identifying Psychiatric
examined levels of psychological distress in a and Personality Disorders
sample as measured by the Kessler 10 (K10) to in Epilepsy
see if there was a relationship between psycho-
logical distress and healthcare utilization. High There is a vast array of diagnostic tools available
levels of distress were reported in 24 % of the to assess and identify psychiatric disorders.
sample with epilepsy, and this was elevated com- These tools range from self-report measures to
pared to the general population (13 %). structured interviews. A select subset has been
Individuals with epilepsy and high to very high used in epilepsy both clinically and in research.
levels of distress had significantly more visits to In the following summary, a number of instru-
the general practitioner, hospital outpatient clinic, ments used to assess psychiatric disorders are
and emergency department compared to those described; reliability and validity of each mea-
with low to moderate distress. Additionally, indi- sure is presented; utility and information regard-
viduals treated only by a specialist or in combina- ing how to find each instrument is included.
tion with a general practitioner had higher levels
of distress compared to those who were treated
solely by a general practitioner. Semi-structured Interviews
for Psychiatric Disorders

Conclusion Diagnostic Interview Schedule-IV


(Robins, Marcus, Reich, Cunningham,
In summary, psychiatric comorbidity in epilepsy & Gallagher, 1996) and Composite
has adverse consequences on many life outcomes, International Diagnostic Interview
making it a priority to identify and provide appro- (CIDI) (Robins et al., 1988)
priate treatment to individuals with epilepsy who Composite International Diagnostic Interview
are doubly impacted by seizures and psychiatric (CIDI) (Robins et al., 1988). The CIDI is a highly
disorders. Psychiatric comorbidity has been recog- structured interview designed for lay interviewers
nized as a priority and is listed as a Research to assess current and lifetime psychiatric disor-
Benchmark in the National Institute of Neurological ders based on the diagnostic criteria. The
Disorders and Stroke (NINDS) (Kelley, Jacobs, & Diagnostic Interview Schedules (DIS, DIS-IV)
Lowenstein, 2009). Additionally, with the recent were the precursors for the CIDI which was devel-
alert in 2008 issued by the US Food and Drug oped to be used across cultures in epidemiological
10 Evaluation of Psychiatric and Personality Disorders 219

studies and provides ICD-10 and DSM-IV crite- from http://www.hcp.med.harvard.edu/wmhcidi/


ria. CIDI Version 3.0 (Kessler & Üstün, 2004) index.php. Training is required in order to obtain
includes 22 diagnoses, clinical significance crite- a copy of the CIDI 3.0 Computer-Assisted
ria, and a clinical severity measure. A screening Personal Interview (CAPI) and to use the Paper
section is administered first. Both of these instru- and Pencil (PAPI) and its scoring algorithm.
ments are administered by reading verbatim ques- Training occurs at the University of Michigan
tions with a predetermined set of responses once or twice a year. The website provides costs,
arranged by diagnostic sections. Clinical explora- contact information, and computer system and
tion of responses is limited to probe questions, software requirements.
and interviewers are discouraged from rephrasing Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview
misunderstood questions. (MINI) (Sheehan et al., 1998) is a structured
diagnostic interview that was developed in order
Reliability: DIS-IV: No reliability studies have to provide a brief diagnostic evaluation of the
been conducted with English-speaking inter- most common mental disorders identified by epi-
viewers. There are no reliability studies of the demiological studies. The MINI was designed to
computerized version of the DSI (C-DIS). CIDI: be shorter and less intensive than traditional diag-
Field trials indicated good test-retest reliability nostic interviews used in academic research, and
with agreement rates over 85 %. Joint reliability it was to be more thorough than the diagnostic
for all diagnoses was 97 %. interviews used in primary care settings. It can be
used in multicenter clinical trials and epidemio-
Validity: DIS-IV: Eaton, Neufeld, Chen, and Cai logical studies. The MINI utilizes DSM-IV and
(2000) reported poor agreement between the ICD-10 criteria for diagnoses and as a result can
DIS-IV and Schedules for Clinical Assessment in be used internationally. There are two versions:
Clinical Neuropsychiatry (SCAN) in the diagno- the MINI and the MINI Plus. The MINI contains
sis of major depression (κ = 0.20) and a sensitivity 19 modules evaluating 17 Axis I disorders (major
of only 29 %. CIDI: The CIDI 3.0 was reported depressive disorder, dysthymic disorder, mania,
to have high agreement with SCID diagnoses in panic disorder, agoraphobia, social phobia, spe-
field trials in France, Italy, Spain, and the United cific phobia, obsessive-compulsive disorder, gen-
States. eralized anxiety disorder, alcohol dependence
and abuse, drug dependence and abuse, psychotic
Utility: These instruments may be difficult to use disorder, anorexia nervosa, bulimia, and PTSD).
in a clinical setting due to the complexity of the The MINI Plus contains 8 more disorders (hypo-
interviews and time required to complete the chondriasis, body dysmorphic disorder, pain
interviews (90 min to 2 h). These interviews help disorder, conduct disorder, attention-deficit/
to identify significant diagnoses and help deter- hyperactivity disorders, adjustment disorder, pre-
mine the progression and duration of clinically menstrual dysphoric disorder, and mixed anxiety-
significant symptoms and disorders. Both of depressive disorder) and provides rule outs,
these instruments provide diagnostic reliability. subtyping, and chronology (Sheehan et al., 1997).

How to find it: DIS-IV: This instrument is no lon- Reliability: A high level of inter-rater reliability
ger available in paper and pencil. It is only avail- has been reported with κ values all above 0.75
able in a computerized format (C-DIS). A license and 70 % of the κ values were above 0.90
for the C-DIS can be obtained from http://epi. (Sheehan et al., 1997). The κ values ranged from
wustl.edu/dis/dishome.htm. A license is required 0.79 for current mania to 1.00 for major depres-
for each project and the license is valid for the sive disorder, obsessive-compulsive disorder,
duration of the project. CIDI: There is a com- current alcohol dependence, anorexia, and buli-
puter-based interview as well as a paper and pen- mia. Test-retest reliability was also good with
cil version. CIDI 3.0 information can be obtained 61 % of the κ values above 0.75.
220 J.E. Jones

Validity: Sheehan et al. (1998) reported that I/P with Psychotic Screen for psychiatric patients
agreement between the MINI and SCID-P was who do not need a full psychotic disorder assess-
good with 73 % of the κ values 0.60 or greater. ment, and the SCID-CV Clinical Version (First,
Only one disorder had a κ value less than 0.50 Gibbon, Spitzer, Williams, & Benjamin, 1997)
(current drug dependence κ = 0.43). Additionally, which covers only common diagnoses seen in
agreement between the MINI and CIDI was good clinical practice with simplified Mood Disorders
with 64 % of the κ values above 0.60. There were and Substance Use Disorder modules.
two disorders with κ values under 0.50 (simple
phobia (κ = 0.43) and generalized anxiety disor- Reliability: The test-retest reliability of the SCID-I
der (κ = 0.36)). for DSM-IV was excellent to good (κ = 0.44–0.78)
with one exception (κ = 0.35 for dysthymia). Inter-
Utility: The mean time for administration of the rater reliability was excellent for alcohol abuse/
MINI is 18 min with a median of 15 min. The MINI dependence, other substance abuse/dependence,
takes more time to administer than the PRIME- PTSD, major depressive disorder, eating disorder,
MD but is significantly shorter than the SCID or and dysthymia and fair to good for generalized
CIDI. The MINI has been used in clinical trials, anxiety, obsessive-compulsive disorder, panic dis-
epidemiological investigations, as well as outpa- order, and social phobia.
tient and inpatient settings. The MINI has been
translated into 40 different languages. It requires Utility: The SCID-I is a user-friendly clinician-
2 h of training for psychiatrists and psychologists administered interview but can require a signifi-
and 3 h for general practitioners. cant amount of time to complete. It may take an
hour or less to administer the entire SCID-I to
How to find it: Free copies of the MINI are avail- individuals with little or no psychopathology.
able from the MINI website at www.medical- More complicated psychiatric histories will
outcomes.com. There is an electronic version of likely extend the interview significantly. The
the MINI which can be purchased. SCID-I requires the interviewer to be trained and
Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV have clinical knowledge of psychopathology and
Axis I Disorders (SCID-I) (First, Spitzer, Gibbon, psychiatric diagnosis.
& Williams, 2002) is a clinician-administered
semi-structured interview to be used with nonpa- How to find it: The Research Version of the
tients in the community or psychiatric patients. It SCID-I, user’s guide, and training videos can be
was developed to cover a wide range of psychiat- obtained from www.scid4.org. The SCID-CV is
ric diagnoses based on the DSM-IV. The SCID-I published by American Psychiatric Publishing:
starts with an overview section that addresses voice, 800-368-5777, or Web, www.appi.org.
demographic information, work history, chief
complaint, present and past history of psychiatric
illness, treatment history, and current function- Questionnaires for Psychiatric
ing. There are nine diagnostic modules, including Disorders
Mood Episodes, Psychotic Symptoms, Psychotic
Disorders Differential, Mood Disorders General Health Questionnaire (GHQ) (Goldberg,
Differential, Substance Use, Anxiety, 1972; Goldberg & Williams, 1991) is a screening
Somatoform Disorders, Eating Disorders, and instrument used to assess psychiatric distress
Adjustment Disorders. Modules can be omitted related to general medical illness. It does not pro-
to focus on areas of diagnostic interest. Two ver- vide a psychiatric diagnosis but rather a screen-
sions of the measure are available: SCID-I ing instrument to determine if further evaluation
Research Version (First et al., 2002) which comes is required. The GHQ has a threshold score indi-
in three editions, SCID-I/P for psychiatric cating 95 % probability that criteria will be met
patients, SCID-I/NP for nonpatients, and SCID- for a psychiatric disorder. The GHQ was designed
10 Evaluation of Psychiatric and Personality Disorders 221

to evaluate psychological stress and ill health. It can cheer you up) during the past 30 days.
assesses the individual’s ability to carry out daily Responses are based on a 5-category Likert scale:
functions and identifies the development of new all of the time, most of the time, some of the time,
psychiatric symptoms not lifelong personality a little of the time, and none of the time. These
characteristics. The GHQ is a paper and pencil, scales were developed based on modern item
self-administered questionnaire. There are four response theory methods to improve the preci-
officially recognized versions: 60 items, 30 items, sion of the scales. This results in a dimensional
28 items, and 12 items. The GHQ-30 is consid- measure of nonspecific distress in the range
ered a better measure of psychological distress found in clinical samples in order to maximize
because it excludes items present in physical ill- the ability to discriminate cases of serious mental
ness. It takes 3–15 min to complete depending illness from noncases. The K10 and K6 scales
upon which version is utilized. were developed to be used in the US National
Health Interview Survey (NHIS). The K10 was
Reliability: Good internal consistency was dem- used in the National Comorbidity Survey
onstrated on the 60-, 30-, and 12-item versions Replication as well as in all the national surveys
with Cronbach α coefficients from 0.82 to 0.93 in the World Health Mental Health (WMH)
and split-half coefficients from 0.78 to 0.95. The Initiative. The K10 has been translated into 15
test-retest reliability appears to be the best for the different languages.
28-item version (r = 0.90).
Reliability: Internal consistency is high for both
Validity: The manual reports adequate content measures. The Cronbach α is reported to be 0.93
validity with each item having discrimination for the K10 and 0.89 for the K6.
between those who are psychologically dis-
tressed and those who are not. The GHQ scores Validity: The sensitivity of both measures ranges
have been shown to correlate with psychiatric from 90th to the 99th percentile. A small valida-
diagnoses based on structured interviews tion study indicated that the six-item scale is as
(r = 0.65–0.70). All versions demonstrated sensitive as the ten-item scale for distinguishing
acceptable sensitivity and specificity with the serious mental illness from noncases. The K10
60-item version demonstrating the best specific- and K6 have been shown to outperform the GHQ-
ity (89 %). 12 (Furukawa, Kessler, Slade, & Andrews, 2003).
The K6 has been shown to be more consistent in
Utility: The GHQ has been used in a variety of samples with physical disabilities.
clinical settings. It has been translated into 36 dif-
ferent languages. It is useful in screening for gen- Utility: It can be easily completed in 2–3 min.
eral emotional distress but should not be used for The K10 and K6 have been demonstrated to iden-
a diagnosing a specific disorder. tify cases of anxiety and depression based on
DSM-IV criteria. Additionally, the K10 and K6
How to find it: The four different versions of the can be used to measure secondary outcomes of
GHQ and manual are available from nferNelson: nonspecific impairments as indicated by the
toll free, +44 845 602 1037; email, information@ Global Assessment of Functioning (GAF).
nfer-nelson.co.uk; or Web, www.nfer-nelson.co.uk.
Kessler 10 (K10) and Kessler 6 (K6) (Kessler How to find it: It is one of the projects in the
et al., 2002, 2003) are 10-item and 6-item self- National Comorbidity Survey Harvard Medical
report measures, respectively, that are used to School. Web, http://www.hcp.med.harvard.edu/
produce a global measure of nonspecific psycho- ncs/k6_scales.php.
logical distress. They are based on questions of Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory
nervousness, agitation, psychological fatigue, (MMPI-2) (Butcher, Dahlstrom, Graham, Tellegen,
and depression (e.g., feeling so sad that nothing & Kaemmer, 1989; Butcher, Graham, Ben-Porath,
222 J.E. Jones

Tellegen, & Dahlstrom, 2001) is a self-report screen for 13 of the most common DSM-IV Axis I
inventory developed to assess and diagnose psy- disorders in outpatient mental health settings.
chopathology in adults. The current edition has These include eating disorders, mood disorders,
been restandardized using a contemporary norma- anxiety disorders, substance use disorders, and
tive sample. The 13 clinical and validity scales are somatoform disorders. It is comprehensive yet can
only minimally changed compared to the original be completed in approximately 15 min before a
version. The MMPI-2 contains 567 true or false clinic visit. It has a quick and easy scoring format
items presented in a random order with the first 370 that can be completed by administrative staff. In
items used for the clinical and validity scales. order to be used in the most valuable manner, the
The basic syndrome scales are as follows: clinician should review the results prior to the inter-
Hypochondriasis (Hs), Depression (D), Hysteria view with the patient so that the clinician can fol-
(Hy), Psychopathic Deviate (Pd), Schizophrenia low up to confirm any diagnoses identified by the
(Sc), Hypomania (Ma), Masculinity-Femininity instrument.
(MF), Social Introversion (Si), Lie (L), Infrequency
(F), and Correction (K). A sixth grade reading level Reliability: The PDSQ has a moderate to high
is recommended to complete the MMPI-2, and it internal consistency (mean Cronbach α = 0.82
typically takes 1–1.5 h to complete. and all subscale α > 0.70 with one exception of
bulimia [0.69]). Good test-retest reliability was
Reliability: Internal consistency estimates reported with a mean = 0.84 and all subscale r
(Cronbach α coefficients) range from 0.34 to 0.87 values over 0.70.
for the basic scales.
Validity: Discriminant and convergent validity
Validity: Over 10,000 references support the were good. High correlations were reported with
validity of the standard scales of the MMPI and other measures of the same construct (mean
MMPI-2 in an array of applications that include correlation = 0.72).
psychiatric, general medical, forensics, and voca-
tional assessments. Convergent validity is thought Utility: This is brief screener that allows for the
to be stronger than discriminant validity. identification of principal and secondary
diagnoses.
Utility: The MMPI has been used most often in
psychiatric inpatient and outpatient settings as a How to find it: Western Psychological Services:
screening instrument, but it has also been used voice, 800-648-8857; email, customerservice@
quite extensively in medical, neurological, foren- wpspublish.com; or Web, www.wpspublish.com.
sic, and vocational settings. The MMPI-2 is quite Symptom Checklist-90-Revised (SCL-90-R)
lengthy and requires a significant amount of time to (Derogatis, 1994; Derogatis, Lipman, & Covi,
complete. Additionally, the MMPI is quite com- 1973) is a 90-item self-administered question-
plex and may limit its use in busy clinical settings. naire, and it takes 12–20 min to complete. A sixth
grade reading level is recommended. Respondents
How to find it: The manuals, questionnaires, are instructed to report how much discomfort
computer versions, scoring, and interpretive ser- each item caused them during the past week. The
vices are available from Pearson Assessments: opening stem for each item reads, “How much
voice, 800-627-7271; email, pearsonassess- were you distressed by…” Respondents mark one
ments@pearson.com; or Web, www.pearsonas- numbered circle for each item on a Likert-type
sessments.com. scale: 0 = not at all, 1 = a little bit, 2 = moderately,
Psychiatric Diagnostic Screening Questionnaire 3 = quite a bit, and 4 = extremely. Raw scores and
(PDSQ) (Zimmerman & Mattia, 1999) is a self- T-scores are provided for each of the nine
administered 126-item questionnaire developed to dimensions: Somatization, Obsessive-Compulsive,
10 Evaluation of Psychiatric and Personality Disorders 223

Interpersonal Sensitivity, Depression, Anxiety, Questionnaires for Specific


Hostility, Phobic Anxiety, Paranoid Ideation, and Psychiatric Disorders
Psychoticism. Global indexes are calculated:
Global Severity Index (mean of all items), Mood Disorders
Positive Symptom Total (number of items with Beck Depression Inventory (BDI, BDI-II) (Beck,
nonzero scores), and Positive Symptom Distress Steer, & Brown, 1996; Beck, Ward, Mendelson,
Index (symptom severity). Additionally, there Mock, & Erbaugh, 1961) was designed to assess
is a shorter 53-item version, Brief Symptom behavioral symptoms of depression in adoles-
Inventory (BSI), which is derived from the cents and adults (Beck et al., 1961) and to moni-
SCL-90-R (Derogatis, 1993; Derogatis & tor changes in symptoms over time. It contained
Melisaratos, 1983). a series of statements with a value assigned to
each statement. Twenty-one behavioral symp-
Reliability: Coefficients of internal consistency toms were assessed. The BDI-II (Beck et al.,
range from 0.77 to 0.90 in a sample of psychiatric 1996) was modified to reflect DSM-IV criteria
outpatients and symptomatic volunteers. and simplify wording. Four items were dropped
(body image change, work difficulty, weight loss,
Validity: Studies have supported better conver- somatic preoccupation) and four items were
gent validity than divergent validity. The nine pri- added (agitation, worthlessness, loss of energy,
mary symptom dimensions of the SCL-90-R concentration difficulties), and the time frame
correlated significantly in a convergent fashion was changed from “right now” to in “the last 2
with similar score constructs on the weeks.” It takes 5–10 min to complete. The most
MMPI. Correlations between the depression sub- recent guidelines suggest the following interpre-
scale on the SCL-90-R and the Beck Depression tation of the scores: 0–13 minimal, 13–19 mild,
Inventory (BDI) ranged from 0.73 to 0.80. 20–28 moderate, and 29–63 severe. Scores can
also be calculated for somatic-affective factor
Utility: The SCL-90-R has been used in diverse and a cognitive factor. It is available in English
populations as a screening instrument for global and Spanish versions.
psychological distress and a multidimensional
measure of symptom profiles. The questionnaire Reliability: The BDI-II reports high internal con-
may be used as a onetime assessment or repeat- sistency (Cronbach’s α of 0.93 in college students
edly to document changes over time or to quan- and 0.92 in outpatients).
tify pretreatment and posttreatment outcomes.
Normative data is available for the following Validity: Beck et al. (1996) reported a correlation
groups: adult nonpatients, adult psychiatric out- of 0.71 with the Ham-D in outpatients. The
patients, and adult psychiatric inpatients. BDI-II had a sensitivity of 94 % and a specificity
Adolescents as young as 13 can complete this of 92 % in diagnosing major depression when a
questionnaire, and there are separate norms for cutoff score of 18 was used (Arnau, Meagher,
this age group. Norris, & Bramson, 2001).

How to find it: This instrument is copyrighted by Utility: The BDI-II is recommended to be used in
Leonard R. Derogatis, Ph.D., with all rights a previously diagnosed population to monitor
reserved. The manuals, questionnaires, computer depressive symptoms over time. The BDI-II may
versions, scoring, and interpretive services are also be used to screen for depressive symptoms in
available from Pearson Assessments: voice, 800- undiagnosed individuals, but it is recommended
627-7271; email, pearsonassessments@pearson. that a diagnostic or clinical interview occur to
com; or Web, www.pearsonassessments.com. diagnose the individual. It is a very easy to use,
224 J.E. Jones

understand, and score. In medical populations it How to find it: The CES-D is part of the public
is recommended to use the lower scores as the domain and was published in its entirety by
cutoff scores (13–19 mild). Radloff (1977).
Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (HAM-
How to find it: This scale is copyrighted and can D) (Hamilton, 1960) was designed to assess the
be obtained from Harcourt Assessment: voice, severity of depressive symptoms in individuals
800-211-8378, or Web, http://harcourtassess- with primary depression. This measure was
ment.com. designed to quantify symptom severity prior to
Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression treatment, monitor the effects of treatment, and
Scale (CES-D) (Radloff, 1977) was developed to identify the recurrence of symptoms. It is the
assess symptoms of depression in community most frequently used observer-rated depressive
populations. The items were selected to capture symptom rating scale. Since validity studies have
major components of depression based on the lit- been conducted on populations with major
erature and factor analytic studies and include the depressive disorders, applying it to other patient
following: depressed mood, worthlessness, hope- populations could be problematic. The Ham-D
lessness, loss of appetite, poor concentration, and has 21 items, but it is recommended that only the
sleep disturbance. The scale is a comprised of 20 first 17 items be scored because the final four
items selected primarily from the Zung SDS, symptoms occur infrequently. It takes about
BDI, and Minnesota Multiphasic Personality 15–20 min to administer. The Ham-D uses a list
Inventory Depression Scale (MMPI-D). Scores of items on a scale of 0–4 or 0–2 with 4 indicat-
range from 0–60 and scores 16 or higher identify ing greater severity. The scoring thresholds are as
individuals with depressive disorders. It takes follows: >23 very severe, 19–22 severe, 14–18
5 min to administer. It has been translated into a moderate, 8–13 mild, and <7 normal. The Ham-D
number of languages including Chinese is to be administered by physicians, psycholo-
(Mandarin and Cantonese), French, Greek, gists, and social workers with experience with
Japanese, and Spanish. psychiatric patients. It was originally designed to
be scored with two interviewers rating the same
Reliability: Internal consistency was high among subject, but this is rarely used.
different populations with Cronbach α ranging
from 0.85 in community samples to 0.90 in psy- Reliability: Reliability of the Ham-D is generally
chiatric samples. Split-half reliabilities ranged acceptable. Internal consistency ranges from 0.76
from 0.77 to 0.92. to 0.92. Joint reliability ranges from 0.65 to 0.90
for the total score.
Validity: Concurrent validity of the CES-D and
the SCL-90-R depression subscale revealed high Validity: The Ham-D has correlations with global
correlation coefficients (r = 0.73–0.89) in several measures of depression ranging from 0.65 to
different groups of outpatients. Correlations with 0.90. Validity is not high in older patients with
the Ham-D were somewhat variable ranging general medical conditions.
from 0.49 for patients with acute depression to
0.85 for patients with schizophrenia. Utility: The total score of the Ham-D is used to
gauge the degree of symptom severity among
Utility: The CES-D was developed to measure individuals who are depressed. It is useful to
depression in community-based samples. Studies monitor change in symptoms after the introduc-
do not support its use to assess depression in tion of an intervention. It should be administered
community populations but rather this measure by a trained interviewer. The Ham-D was devel-
should be used as a screening instrument with oped before the DSM-III and thus does not
further evaluation needed if the scores are include a number of symptoms that are required
elevated. for the diagnosis of major depression. It gives
10 Evaluation of Psychiatric and Personality Disorders 225

more weight to somatic symptoms than to cogni- worded. The time frame used is the present. The
tive symptoms. Zung SDS takes 5 min but can take additional
time to complete depending on the population
How to find it: The most commonly used form of being assessed. A total severity score is reported,
the Ham-D is the ECDEU form, and it is a modi- and the scores are interpreted in the following
fied version of the original Ham-D (Guy, 1976). ranges: <50 within the normal range, 50–59 mild
Neurological Disorders Depression Inventory depression, 60–69 moderate depression, and ≥70
for Epilepsy (NDDI-E) (Gilliam et al., 2006) is a severe depression.
validated 6-item questionnaire for depression in
individuals with epilepsy. It can be used to rapidly Reliability: Split-half reliability in psychiatric
and reliably detect depression in a busy clinical set- populations was reported to have a correlation of
ting. Additionally, the NDDI-E has demonstrated r = 0.73. In a community-based study, Cronbach
an ability to differentiate symptoms of depression α was good (0.79).
from those of medication toxicity and cognitive
effects of epilepsy. A Likert scale is used to rate Validity: The Zung SDS scores discriminate
symptom frequency and ratings ranging from between depressed and nondepressed samples
always or often, sometimes, rarely to never. A cut- (Zung, 1965). A significant correlation (r = 0.65)
off score of greater than 15 demonstrated optimal was reported with the MMPI-D. It is less sensi-
sensitivity, specificity, and predictive power. There tive to changes in symptoms over time.
are no items related to sadness or irritability, and
this may be related to the fact that a number of Utility: The Zung SDS is often used to rate the
studies have indicated that people with epilepsy severity of depressive symptoms in people with
often report hopelessness, anhedonia, and frustra- diagnosed depressive illness. The best results are
tion, not sadness or a depressed mood. reported in samples with mild to moderate symp-
toms of depression. It has been used in primary care
Reliability: Internal consistency was good and the community, but it is not a very good mea-
(Cronbach α = 0.85). Test-retest reliability was sure to be used as a screening tool for depression.
also good (0.78).
How to find it: The Zung SDS and instructions
Validity: Construct validity was high for the BDI are presented in full in the original article
(r = 0.78) and the CES-D (r = 0.77). (Zung, 1965).

Utility: The NDDI-E was found to have a unique Anxiety Disorders


role in assessing depression in epilepsy. Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI) (Beck, Epstein,
Brown, & Steer, 1988) is designed to assess anxi-
How to find it: The NDDI-E is in the public ety and focuses on somatic symptoms. It was
domain. See Gilliam et al. (2006) for question- developed to discriminate between anxiety and
naire and scoring instructions. depression. It is a 21-item self-report measure of
Zung Self-Rating Depression Scale (Zung symptoms present within the past week, and
SDS) (Zung, 1965) is a self-report measure of items include typical symptoms of anxiety,
depression severity. Items were included to assess including nervousness, inability to relax, dizzi-
affective, cognitive, behavioral, and physiologi- ness, light-headedness, and heart pounding or
cal symptoms of depression. The items were racing. It takes about 5 min to complete. The total
selected based on factor analytic studies and score ranges from 0 to 63 and scores 0–9 are nor-
diagnostic criteria. It contains 20 items, ten items mal, 10–18 mild to moderate, 19–29 moderate to
negatively worded and ten items positively severe, and 30–63 severe anxiety.
226 J.E. Jones

Reliability: The BAI has high internal consis- BAI and the anxiety subscale of the SCL-90-R
tency with Cronbach α ranging from 0.90 to (r = 0.72 and r = 0.74, respectively). The GAD-7
0.94 in psychiatric inpatients, outpatients, under- was strongly associated with functional impair-
graduates, and adults in the community. ments with increasing scores, demonstrating
good construct validity.
Validity: The BAI correlates highly with the STAI
(r = 0.47–0.58) and the anxiety subscale of the Utility: The GAD-7 allows for identification of
SCL-90-R (r = 0.81). However, correlations with cases of GAD. It also allows for the assessment
instruments that measure depressive symptoms of symptom severity, and increasing scores are
are also high: BDI (r = 0.62) and depression sub- associated with functional impairment and dis-
scale of the SCL-90-R (r = 0.62). The BAI is able ability days. The GAD-7 can also be used to
to discriminate between individuals with anxiety monitor change over time, but there is limited
disorders and individuals with depression and evidence of its ability to detect change over time.
nonclinical control subjects.
How to find it: The GAD-7 is in the public
Utility: It is an easily administered and scored domain. See Spitzer et al. (2006) for question-
instrument. It is typically used to monitor changes naire and scoring instructions.
with treatment. It has also been used as a screen- Penn State Worry Questionnaire (PSWQ)
ing measure for symptoms of anxiety in the gen- (Meyer, Miller, Metzger, & Borkovec, 1990) was
eral medical setting. developed to assess trait symptoms of pathologi-
cal worry. It was designed to evaluate the propen-
How to find it: This scale is copyrighted and can sity of an individual to worry, the excessiveness
be obtained from Harcourt Assessment: voice, of the worry, and the tendency for the worry to be
800-211-8378, or Web, http://harcourtassess- generalized and not focused on a small number of
ment.com. situations. This scale assesses the three DSM-IV
Generalized Anxiety Disorder 7 (GAD-7) criteria for generalized anxiety disorder, includ-
(Spitzer, Kroenke, Williams, & Lowe, 2006) is a ing the duration of the worry at least 6 months,
brief seven-item self-report measure used to iden- excessiveness of the worry, and generalized
tify one of the most common anxiety disorders— symptoms. It contains 16 items and measures the
generalized anxiety disorder (GAD). This is one frequency and intensity of worry symptoms. It
of only a few anxiety measures that is specifi- can be administered in 5 min. It utilizes a Likert
cally related to the DSM-IV criteria. Responses scale from 1 = not at all typical to 5 = very typical.
are based on a 4-category Likert scale: not at Individuals with generalized anxiety typically
all, several days, more than half the days, and score ≥60, and individuals with anxiety disorders
nearly every day. A cutoff score of greater than score about 10 points lower, and their score is
10 identifies cases of GAD. A score of 5 indi- higher than individuals who are not anxious.
cates mild, 10 moderate, and 15 severe levels of Notably, individuals in a major depressive epi-
anxiety. Additionally, a statement assessing the sode score as high as those with generalized
level of impairment at work, home, and people anxiety.
is included.
Reliability: A high degree of internal consistency
Reliability: The GAD-7 internal consistency was has been demonstrated in several populations
excellent with a Cronbach α = 0.92. Test-retest with Cronbach α ranging from 0.86 to 0.93 for
reliability was also good with an intraclass cor- individuals diagnosed with anxiety and 0.91 to
relation of 0.83. 0.95 for community samples.

Validity: Convergent validity was good as dem- Validity: In clinical samples, the PSWQ was
onstrated by the significant correlations with the weakly correlated with Hamilton Anxiety Rating
10 Evaluation of Psychiatric and Personality Disorders 227

Scale, the trait scale of the STAI and the Zung Utility: The STAI can assist in the clinical diag-
SDS; the correlation coefficients ranged from nosis of anxiety, and it can help differentiate anx-
0.02 to 0.18. The PSWQ has demonstrated to iety from depression. It has been used in
measure worry symptoms distinctly different psychological and health research. It has been
from obsessive-compulsive symptoms measured used in a wide array of settings, including medi-
by the Padua Inventory (PI). cal, surgical, and psychiatric.
How to find it: Mind Garden Inc. 855 Oak
Utility: The PSWQ is a brief measure of worry in Grove Ave., Suite 215, Menlo Park, CA 94025.
generalized anxiety. It is used as a measure of Voice, (650) 322-6300, or Web, http://www.
severity of pathological worry, and it can be used mindgarden.com.
to monitor change over time. It can be used as a
screen for pathological worry, but it should not be
used to diagnose generalized anxiety disorder Semi-structured Interviews
because worry can accompany other disorders for Personality Disorders
(OCD, depression). It also does not assess fre-
quency, intensity, content, or degree of control of Diagnostic Interview for the DSM-IV Personality
the worry or anxiety, which are required for a Disorders (DIPD-IV) (Zanarini, Frankenburg,
diagnosis. Chauncey, & Gunderson, 1987; Zanarini,
Frankenburg, & Sickel, 1996) categorically
How to find it: The PSWQ is in the public domain assesses personality disorders based on the
and may be obtained by contacting Thomas DSM-IV, and it is organized based on the criteria
Borkovec, Ph.D., Department of Psychology, for each personality disorder. The interview also
Pennsylvania State University. Voice, 814-863- contains 108 sets of yes/no and open-ended ques-
1725, or email, tdb@psu.edu. tions, and each criterion is indicated by 0 = absent
State Trait Inventory (STAI) (Spielberger, or clinically insignificant, 1 = present but uncer-
Gorsuch, & Lushene, 1970) Form Y is used to tain clinical significance, 2 = present and clini-
measure anxiety in adults. Form X was the first cally significant, or NA if not applicable. Traits
version of the measure. There is a child form or behaviors must be characteristic of the person
available. The STAI differentiates between the throughout the person’s adulthood.
temporary “state anxiety” and more long-
standing “trait anxiety.” The STAI contains forty Reliability and validity: The DIPD-IV appears to
questions and is written on a sixth grade reading have good psychometric data. It has been used in
level. It utilizes a 4-point Likert scale and pro- a multisite study on the validity of personality
vides three scores: state anxiety, trait anxiety, and disorder diagnosis. Inter-rater reliability kappa
overall anxiety. It has been translated into 48 lan- coefficients ranged from κ = 0.58 to 1.00 (Zanarini
guages and is one of the most commonly used et al., 2000). Few studies have been conducted to
instruments to measure anxiety. examine the reliability and validity of this
instrument.
Reliability: According to the test-retest correla-
tions provided by Spielberger et al. (1970), the Utility: The DIPD-IV requires 90 min to com-
correlations are as follows: r = 0.54 (state) and plete. This instrument requires the interviewer to
0.86 (trait). be trained and have clinical knowledge of psycho-
pathology, psychiatric diagnosis, and personality
Validity: In terms of concurrent validity, the disorders and can be administered by someone
STAI-state and STAI-trait were found to be posi- with a bachelor’s degree with at least one year of
tively correlated with the Taylor Manifest Anxiety clinical training. The manual has limited informa-
Scale (r = 0.80) and the Multiple Affect Checklist tion on administration and scoring. A Spanish ver-
(0.52). sion of this instrument is available.
228 J.E. Jones

How to find it: The DIPD-IV, training videos, and Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV
workshops are available by contacting Mary Axis II Disorders (SCID-II) (First et al., 1997) is
C. Zanarini, Ed.D., Director, Laboratory for the a clinician-administered semi-structured inter-
Study of Adult Development, McLean Hospital, view to assess DSM-IV personality disorders.
(voice 617-855-2660; email zanarini@mclean. The SCID-II starts with a screening question-
harvard.edu). naire with 119 yes/no questions to identify the
International Personality Disorder personality disorders that need to be examined
Examination (IPDE) (Loranger, Sartorius, further in the SCID-II interview. Each criterion is
Andreoli, & Berger, 1994) is a semi-structured indicated by 1 = absent/false, 2 = subthreshold,
interview that categorically and/or dimensionally 3 = threshold/true, or ? = inadequate information.
assesses personality disorders based on DSM-IV Personality disorders are assessed categorically
and/or ICD-10. The IPDE was developed based and/or dimensionally.
on the World Health Organization and National
Institutes of Health field trials on personality dis- Reliability: The reliability coefficients reported
orders. It contains two versions: a DSM-IV-based are good. However, there is some concern that the
version that contains 99 sets of questions and an queries used are insufficient compared to other
ICD-10 module that contains 67 sets of ques- instruments that assess personality disorders.
tions. The questions are organized in the follow-
ing domains: work, self, interpersonal Utility: The SCID-II is a user-friendly clinician-
relationships, affect, reality testing, and impulse administered interview and requires 90 min to
control. A true/false screening questionnaire is complete. The SCID-II requires the interviewer
available for both versions to help reduce inter- to be trained and have clinical knowledge of psy-
view time by omitting items that are less likely to chopathology, psychiatric diagnosis, and person-
be present. The IPDE is a well-established Axis II ality disorders.
semi-structured interview.
How to find it: The SCID-II manual is published
Reliability: The IPDE has demonstrated good separately from the SCID-I. The manual, inter-
inter-rater reliability and temporal stability. view, screen, and scoring sheets can be obtained
from American Psychiatric Publishing: voice,
Utility: The average administration time is 800-368-5777; email, appi@psych.org; or Web,
90 min. The self-administered screening ques- www.appi.org. A computer-administered version
tionnaire is written on a 4th grade reading level is available through Multi-Health Systems: voice,
and takes less than 15 min to complete, reducing 800-456-3003, or Web, www.mhs.com.
the overall interview time. It is recommended
that the IPDE be administered by experienced
clinicians. Training workshops are available. The Questionnaires for Personality
thematic format of the IPDE may be easier for Disorders
patients because there are fewer redundancies
since it is organized by common issues and Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory-2
themes rather than diagnostic criteria. Personality Disorder Scales (MMPI-2 PD)
(Somwaru & Ben-Porath, 1995) were developed
How to find it: The DSM-IV module, ICD-10 to assess symptoms of personality disorders that
module, screen, manual, scoring sheets, and could be extracted from the MMPI-2. These
optional computer scoring program can be scales would allow for a dimensional assessment
obtained from Psychological Assessment of the 10 DSM-IV personality disorders using the
Resources: voice, 800-331-8378, or Web, www. standard MMPI-2. Morey, Waugh, and Blashfield
parinc.com. (1985) created 11 scales to assess each of the
10 Evaluation of Psychiatric and Personality Disorders 229

personality disorders in the DSM-III, and when questionnaire, and it also has a parallel form that
the MMPI-2 was published, several items in can be completed by a peer, spouse, or expert
these scales were deleted. Somwaru and Ben- rater. There is also a short form that contains 60
Porath (1995) developed a new set of subscales items and provides scores for the five domains
based on the DSM-IV; over 60 % of the items in only. Profiles are based on gender and age. There
these new subscales were not included in the are two age groups based on the normative sam-
Morey et al. (1985) scales. ples: college age (17–20 years) and adults
(21 years and older).
Reliability: The internal consistency is reported
to be above 0.85, which is an improvement over Reliability: Internal consistency coefficients
the Morey et al. (1985) scales which were (Cronbach’s α) for both forms ranged from 0.86
reported to be 0.74 (Hicklin & Widiger, 2000). to 0.95. Test-retest reliability ranged from 0.63 to
0.83 for the five domains on both forms.
Validity: Hicklin and Widiger (2000) reported
that the Somwaru and Ben-Porath scales are as Validity: High correlations between the domain
valid as the Morey et al. (1985) scales with stron- scores and facet scores (0.89–0.95) were reported
ger convergent validity reported for Antisocial, by the authors.
Borderline, and Schizoid dimensions.
Additionally, Rossi, Van den Brande, Tobac, Utility: It takes 35–40 min to complete. A Spanish
Sloore, and Hauben (2003) found that the version is available. The NEO-PI-R is a measure
MMPI-2 PD scales had comparable convergent of the five factors of normal personality traits. It
validity with similar scales on the Millon Clinical can be used to assess personality attributes and
Multiaxial Inventory-III (MCMI-III). their impact on overall functioning. The manual
suggests that the NEO-PI-R can be used in select-
Utility: The MMPI-2 PD scales provide dimen- ing treatments, vocational counseling, and indus-
sional assessment of 10 DSM-IV personality dis- trial and organizational consulting.
orders including the following: paranoid,
schizoid, schizotypal, antisocial, borderline, his- How to find it: The NEO-PI-R is copyrighted and
trionic, narcissistic, avoidant, dependent, and can be purchased by contacting Psychological
obsessive-compulsive. Since the MMPI-2 PD is Assessment Resources: voice, 800-331-8378, or
derived from the standard MMPI-2, a significant Web, www.parinc.com.
amount of data can be obtained by using this one Personality Assessment Inventory (PAI)
instrument. (Morey, 1991) was developed to assess clinical
syndromes, personality features, and treatment
How to find it: In order to obtain the MMPI-2 complications. The PAI contains 344 items pro-
items and scoring instructions for the MMPI-2 ducing 22 scales, of which 10 are divided into 31
PD, contact: Yossef S. Ben-Porath, Ph.D., Kent subscales. There are 4 validity scales, 11 clinical,
State University, voice, 330-672-2684, or email, 2 interpersonal, and 5 treatment related. The 11
ybenpora@kent.edu. clinical scales contain 2 scales that assess person-
NEO Personality Inventory-Revised (NEO- ality features including the following: borderline
PI-R) (Costa & MCCrae, 1992) was designed to features (affective instability, identity problems,
assess the five major domains of personality from negative relationships with others, and impulsive
the five-factor model of personality which self-harm) and antisocial features (antisocial
includes: Neuroticism, Extraversion, Openness, behaviors, egocentricity, and poor empathy).
Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness. Within
each domain there are six facets of personality Reliability: Internal consistency coefficients in
and each facet is measured by eight items on a various samples were all reported to be above
5-point scale. This is a 240-item self-administered 0.80 for full scales and 0.70 for subscales.
230 J.E. Jones

Median test-retest reliability coefficients range Blanchet, P., & Frommer, G. P. (1986). Mood change pre-
ceding epileptic seizures. Journal of Nervous and
from 0.77 to 0.80.
Mental Disease, 174(8), 471–476.
Blumer, D., & Altshuler, L. (1998). Affective disorders. In
Validity: Morey (1991) reported that the J. Engel Jr. & T. A. Pedley (Eds.), Epilepsy: A compre-
Dominance scale correlated (r = 0.71) highly with hensive textbook (pp. 2083–2099). Philadelphia, PA:
Lippincott-Raven.
the Assertiveness Facet of the NEO-PI. Differences
Boylan, L. S., Flint, L. A., Labovitz, D. L., Jackson, S. C.,
between gender, race, and age were reported to Starner, K., & Devinsky, O. (2004). Depression but not
be small. seizure frequency predicts quality of life in treatment-
resistant epilepsy. Neurology, 62(2), 258–261.
Briquet, P. (1859). Traité Clinique et therapeutique de
Utility: It takes 40–50 min to complete the PAI. It
l’hysteric. Paris: Plou, Nourit et Co.
was developed for adults 18 years and older with Butcher, J. N., Dahlstrom, W. G., Graham, J. R., Tellegen,
a 4th grade reading level. There is a Spanish ver- A., & Kaemmer, B. (1989). Minnesota multiphasic
sion. The PAI has a limited number of personality personality inventory-2 (MMPI-2): Manual for admin-
stration and scoring. Minneaoplis, MN: University of
scales, but the reliability, validity, and clinical
Minnesota Press.
utility of the scales appear to be good. It also Butcher, J. N., Graham, J. R., Ben-Porath, Y. S., Tellegen,
assesses many Axis I disorders including depres- A., & Dahlstrom, W. G. (2001). Minnesota multipha-
sion, anxiety, and substance abuse. sic personality inventory – 2 (MMPI-2): Manual for
administration and scoring (Revisedth ed.).
Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.
How to find it: The PAI is copyrighted and Costa, R. M., & MCCrae, R. R. (1992). NEO-PI-R profes-
can be purchased by contacting Psychological sional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment
Assessment Resources: voice, 800-331-8378, or Resources.
Cramer, J. A., Blum, D., Fanning, K., & Reed, M. (2004).
Web, www.parinc.com.
The impact of comorbid depression on health resource
utilization in a community sample of people with epi-
lepsy. Epilepsy and Behavior, 5(3), 337–342.
References Cramer, J. A., Blum, D., Reed, M., & Fanning, K. (2003).
The influence of comorbid depression on quality of
life for people with epilepsy. Epilepsy and Behavior,
Akanuma, N., Kanemoto, K., Adachi, N., Kawasaki, J.,
4(5), 515–521.
Ito, M., & Onuma, T. (2005). Prolonged postictal psy-
Davies, S., Heyman, I., & Goodman, R. (2003). A popula-
chosis with forced normalization (Landolt) in tempo-
tion survey of mental health problems in children with
ral lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy and Behavior, 6(3),
epilepsy. Developmental Medicine and Child
456–459.
Neurology, 45(5), 292–295.
Altshuler, L., Rausch, R., Delrahim, S., Kay, J., &
de Araujo Filho, G. M., Jackowski, A. P., Lin, K.,
Crandall, P. (1999). Temporal lobe epilepsy, temporal
Guaranha, M. S., Guilhoto, L. M., da Silva, H. H.,
lobectomy, and major depression. The Journal of
et al. (2009). Personality traits related to juvenile myo-
Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 11(4),
clonic epilepsy: MRI reveals prefrontal abnormalities
436–443.
through a voxel-based morphometry study. Epilepsy
Arnau, R. C., Meagher, M. W., Norris, M. P., & Bramson,
and Behavior, 15(2), 202–207.
R. (2001). Psychometric evaluation of the Beck
Derogatis, L. R. (1993). Brief symptom inventory (BSI):
Depression Inventory-II with primary care medical
Adminstration, scoring and procedures manual (3rd
patients. Health Psychology, 20(2), 112–119.
ed.). Minneapolis, MN: National Computer Systems.
Arnold, L. M., & Privitera, M. D. (1996). Psychopathology
Derogatis, L. R. (1994). The SCL 90-R: Administration,
and trauma in epileptic and psychogenic seizure
scoring, and procedures manual (3rd ed.).
patients. Psychosomatics: Journal of Consultation
Minneapolis, MN: National Computer Systems.
Liaison Psychiatry, 37(5), 438–443.
Derogatis, L. R., Lipman, R. S., & Covi, L. (1973). SCL-
Beck, A. T., Epstein, N., Brown, G., & Steer, R. A. (1988).
90: An outpatient psychiatric rating scale–preliminary
An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety:
report. Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 9(1), 13–28.
Psychometric properties. Journal of Consulting and
Derogatis, L. R., & Melisaratos, N. (1983). The Brief
Clinical Psychology, 56(6), 893–897.
Symptom Inventory: An introductory report.
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Beck
Psychological Medicine, 13(3), 595–605.
Depression Inventory-II manual. San Antonio, TX:
Devinsky, O., Barr, W. B., Vickrey, B. G., Berg, A. T.,
Psychological Corporation.
Bazil, C. W., Pacia, S. V., et al. (2005). Changes in
Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., &
depression and anxiety after resective surgery for epi-
Erbaugh, J. (1961). An inventory for measuring depres-
lepsy. Neurology, 65(11), 1744–1749.
sion. Archives of General Psychiatry, 4, 561–571.
10 Evaluation of Psychiatric and Personality Disorders 231

Eaton, W. W., Neufeld, K., Chen, L.-S., & Cai, G. (2000). Grabowska-Grzyb, A., Jedrzejczak, J., Naganska, E., &
A comparison of self-report and clinical diagnostic Fiszer, U. (2006). Risk factors for depression in
interviews for depression: Diagnostic Interview patients with epilepsy. Epilepsy and Behavior, 8(2),
Schedule and Schedules for Clinical Assessment in 411–417.
Neuropsychiatry in the Baltimore Epidemiologic Graham, P., & Rutter, M. (1968). Organic brain dysfunc-
Catchment Area follow-up. Archives of General tion and child psychiatric disorder. BMJ [British
Psychiatry, 57(3), 217–222. Medical Journal], 3(5620), 695–700. doi:10.1136/
Edeh, J., & Toone, B. (1987). Relationship between inter- bmj.3.5620.695.
ictal psychopathology and the type of epilepsy. Results Gudmundsson, G. (1966). Epilepsy in Iceland. A clinical
of a survey in general practice. British Journal of and epidemiological investigation. Acta Neurologica
Psychiatry, 151, 95–101. Scandinavica, 43(Suppl 25), 21–124.
Ettinger, A., Reed, M., & Cramer, J. (2004). Depression Guy W. (1976). ECDEU Assessment Manual for
and comorbidity in community-based patients with Psychopharmacology, Revised Edition (DHEW Publ
epilepsy or asthma. Neurology, 63(6), 1008–1014. No ADM 76–388). Washington, DC, US Dept of
Fiordelli, E., Beghi, E., Bogliun, G., & Crespi, V. (1993). Health, Education and Welfare.
Epilepsy and psychiatric disturbance: A cross- Hamilton, M. (1960). A rating scale for depression.
sectional study. British Journal of Psychiatry, 163, Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry,
446–450. 23, 56–62.
First, M. B., Gibbon, M., Spitzer, R. L., Williams, J. B. Harden, C. L., Jovine, L., Burgut, F. T., Carey, B. T.,
W., & Benjamin, L. S. (1997). Structured clinical Nikolov, B. G., & Ferrando, S. J. (2009). A compari-
interview for DSM-IV axis II personality disorders son of personality disorder characteristics of patients
(SCID-II). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric with nonepileptic psychogenic pseudoseizures with
Press. those of patients with epilepsy. Epilepsy and Behavior,
First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. 14(3), 481–483.
W. (2002). Structured clinical interview for DSM- Hesdorffer, D. C., Hauser, W. A., Annegers, J. F., &
IV-TR axis I disorders, research version (SCID-I). Cascino, G. (2000). Major depression is a risk factor
New York, NY: Biometrics Research, New York State for seizures in older adults. Annals of Neurology,
Psychiatric Institute. 47(2), 246–249.
Fisher, R. S., van Emde Boas, W., Blume, W., Elger, C., Hesdorffer, D. C., Hauser, W. A., Olafsson, E., Ludvigsson,
Genton, P., Lee, P., et al. (2005). Epileptic seizures and P., & Kjartansson, O. (2006). Depression and suicide
epilepsy: Definitions proposed by the International attempt as risk factors for incident unprovoked sei-
League Against Epilepsy (ILAE) and the International zures. Annals of Neurology, 59(1), 35–41.
Bureau for Epilepsy (IBE). Epilepsia, 46(4), Hicklin, J., & Widiger, T. A. (2000). Convergent validity
470–472. of alternative MMPI-2 personality disorder scales.
Forsgren, L. (1992). Prevalence of epilepsy in adults in Journal of Personality Assessment, 75(3), 502–518.
northern Sweden. Epilepsia, 33(3), 450–458. Hill, D. (1981). Historical review. In E. Reynolds &
Furukawa, T. A., Kessler, R. C., Slade, T., & Andrews, G. M. Trimble (Eds.), Epilepsy and psychiatry (pp. 1–11).
(2003). The performance of the K6 and K10 screening Edinburgh: Churchill Livingstone.
scales for psychological distress in the Australian Huang, Y., Kotov, R., de Girolamo, G., Preti, A.,
National Survey of Mental Health and Well-Being. Angermeyer, M., Benjet, C., et al. (2009). DSM-IV
Psychological Medicine, 33(2), 357–362. personality disorders in the WHO World Mental
Gaitatzis, A., Trimble, M. R., & Sander, J. W. (2004). The Health Surveys. British Journal of Psychiatry, 195(1),
psychiatric comorbidity of epilepsy. Acta Neurologica 46–53.
Scandinavica, 110(4), 207–220. Jackson, J. (1931). Selected writings. In J. Taylor (Ed.),
Gilliam, F. (2002). Optimizing health outcomes in active On epilepsy and epileptiform convulsions (Vol. 1).
epilepsy. Neurology, 58(8 Suppl 5), S9–S20. London: Hodder & Stoughton.
Gilliam, F. G., Barry, J. J., Hermann, B. P., Meador, K. J., Johnson, E. K., Jones, J. E., Seidenberg, M., & Hermann,
Vahle, V., & Kanner, A. M. (2006). Rapid detection of B. P. (2004). The relative impact of anxiety, depres-
major depression in epilepsy: A multicentre study. sion, and clinical seizure features on health-related
Lancet Neurology, 5(5), 399–405. quality of life in epilepsy. Epilepsia, 45(5), 544–550.
Goldberg, D. P. (1972). The detection of psychiatric ill- Jones, J. E., Hermann, B. P., Barry, J. J., Gilliam, F. G.,
ness by questionnaire. Oxford: Oxford University Kanner, A. M., & Meador, K. J. (2003). Rates and risk
Press. factors for suicide, suicidal ideation, and suicide
Goldberg, D. P., & Williams, P. (1991). A user's guide to attempts in chronic epilepsy. Epilepsy and Behavior,
the general health questionnaire. Berkshire: 4(Suppl 3), S31–S38.
Nfer-Nelson. Kanner, A. M. (2003). Depression in epilepsy: Prevalence,
Gowers, W. R. (1881). Epilepsy and other chronic convul- clinical semiology, pathogenic mechanisms, and treat-
sive diseases: Their causes, symptoms, and treatment. ment. Biological Psychiatry, 54(3), 388–398.
London: J & A Churchill.
232 J.E. Jones

Kanner, A. M. (2004). Recognition of the various expres- Lopez-Rodriguez, F., Altshuler, L., Kay, J., Delarhim, S.,
sions of anxiety, psychosis, and aggression in epilepsy. Mendez, M., & Engel, J., Jr. (1999). Personality disor-
Epilepsia, 45(Suppl 2), 22–27. ders among medically refractory epileptic patients.
Kanner, A. M., Soto, A., & Gross-Kanner, H. (2004). The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical
Prevalence and clinical characteristics of postictal Neurosciences, 11(4), 464–469.
psychiatric symptoms in partial epilepsy. Neurology, Loranger, A. W., Sartorius, N., Andreoli, A., & Berger, P.
62(5), 708–713. (1994). The International Personality Disorder
Kelley, M. S., Jacobs, M. P., & Lowenstein, D. H. (2009). Examination: The World Health Organization/
The NINDS epilepsy research benchmarks. Epilepsia, Alcohol, Drug Abuse, and Mental Health
50(3), 579–582. Administration international pilot study of personality
Kessler, R. C., Andrews, G., Colpe, L. J., Hiripi, E., disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 51(3),
Mroczek, D. K., Normand, S. L., et al. (2002). Short 215–224.
screening scales to monitor population prevalences Manchanda, R., Schaefer, B., McLachlan, R. S., Blume,
and trends in non-specific psychological distress. W. T., Wiebe, S., Girvin, J. P., et al. (1996). Psychiatric
Psychological Medicine, 32(6), 959–976. disorders in candidates for surgery for epilepsy.
Kessler, R. C., Barker, P. R., Colpe, L. J., Epstein, J. F., Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry,
Gfroerer, J. C., Hiripi, E., et al. (2003). Screening for 61(1), 82–89.
serious mental illness in the general population. McConnell, H. W., & Duncan, P. W. (1998). Treatment of
Archives of General Psychiatry, 60(2), 184–189. psychiatric comorbidity in epilepsy. In H. W.
Kessler, R. C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., McConnell & P. J. Snyder (Eds.), Psychiatric comor-
Merikangas, K. R., & Walters, E. E. (2005). Lifetime bidity in epilepsy: Basic mechanisms, diagnosis, and
prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV treatment (pp. 245–361). Washington, DC: American
disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Psychiatric Association.
Replication. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62(6), Mensah, S. A., Beavis, J. M., Thapar, A. K., & Kerr, M.
593–602. (2006). The presence and clinical implications of
Kessler, R. C., & Üstün, T. B. (2004). The World Mental depression in a community population of adults
Health (WMH) Survey Initiative Version of the World with epilepsy. Epilepsy and Behavior, 8(1),
Health Organization (WHO) Composite International 213–219.
Diagnostic Interview (CIDI). International Journal of Meyer, T. J., Miller, M. L., Metzger, R. L., & Borkovec,
Methods in Psychiatric Research, 13(2), 93–121. T. D. (1990). Development and validation of the Penn
Ketter, T. A., Malow, B. A., Flamini, R., White, S. R., State Worry Questionnaire. Behaviour Research and
Post, R. M., & Theodore, W. H. (1994). Anticonvulsant Therapy, 28(6), 487–495.
withdrawal-emergent psychopathology. Neurology, Morel, B. (1860). Traité des malades metales. Paris:
44(1), 55–61. Masson et Cie.
Kobau, R., Gilliam, F., & Thurman, D. J. (2006). Morey, L. C. (1991). Personality assessment inventory:
Prevalence of self-reported epilepsy or seizure disor- Professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological
der and its associations with self-reported depression Assessment Resources.
and anxiety: Results from the 2004 HealthStyles Morey, L. C., Waugh, M. H., & Blashfield, R. K. (1985).
Survey. Epilepsia, 47(11), 1915–1921. MMPI scales for DSM-III personality disorders: Their
Krishnamoorthy, E. S., Brown, R. J., & Trimble, M. R. derivation and correlates. Journal of Personality
(2001). Personality and Psychopathology in Assessment, 49(3), 245–251.
Nonepileptic Attack Disorder and Epilepsy: A Perala, J., Suvisaari, J., Saarni, S. I., Kuoppasalmi, K.,
Prospective Study. Epilepsy and Behavior, 2(5), Isometsa, E., Pirkola, S., et al. (2007). Lifetime preva-
418–422. lence of psychotic and bipolar I disorders in a general
Kuyk, J., Swinkels, W. A. M., & Spinhoven, P. (2003). population. Archives of General Psychiatry, 64(1),
Psychopathologies in patients with nonepileptic sei- 19–28.
zures with and without comorbid epilepsy: How dif- Pond, D. A., & Bidwell, B. H. (1960). A survey of epi-
ferent are they? Epilepsy and Behavior, 4(1), 13–18. lepsy in fourteen general practices. II. Social and psy-
Lacey, C. J., Salzberg, M. R., Roberts, H., Trauer, T., & chological aspects. Epilepsia, 1, 285–299.
D'Souza, W. J. (2009). Psychiatric comorbidity and Provinciali, L., Franciolini, B., del Pesce, M., & Signorino,
impact on health service utilization in a community M. (1989). Influence of neurological factors on the
sample of patients with epilepsy. Epilepsia, 50(8), personality profile of patients with temporal lobe epi-
1991–1994. lepsy. Journal of Epilepsy, 2(4), 239–244.
LaFrance, W. C., Jr., Kanner, A. M., & Hermann, B. Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D Scale: A self-report
(2008). Psychiatric comorbidities in epilepsy. depression scale for research in the general popula-
International Review of Neurobiology, 83, 347–383. tion. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1(3),
Lee, S. A., Lee, S. M., & No, Y. J. (2009). Factors contrib- 385–401.
uting to depression in patients with epilepsy. Epilepsia, Reisinger, E. L., & DiIorio, C. (2009). Individual, seizure-
20, 20. related, and psychosocial predictors of depressive
10 Evaluation of Psychiatric and Personality Disorders 233

symptoms among people with epilepsy over six Swinkels, W. A. M., Kuyk, J., van Dyck, R., & Spinhoven,
months. Epilepsy and Behavior, 15(2), 196–201. P. (2005). Psychiatric comorbidity in epilepsy.
Reynolds, E. H., & Trimble, M. R. (2009). Epilepsy, psy- Epilepsy and Behavior, 7(1), 37–50.
chiatry, and neurology. Epilepsia, 50(Suppl 3), 50–55. Tellez-Zenteno, J. F., Patten, S. B., Jette, N., Williams, J.,
Robins, L. N., Marcus, W., Reich, W. C., Cunningham, R., & Wiebe, S. (2007). Psychiatric comorbidity in epi-
& Gallagher, T. (1996). NIMH diagnostic interview lepsy: A population-based analysis. Epilepsia, 48(12),
schedule, version IV (DIS-IV). St. Louis, MO: 2336–2344.
Department of Psychiatry, Washington School of Temkin, O. (1971). The falling sickness: A history of epi-
Medicine. lepsy from the Greeks to the beginnings of modern
Robins, L. N., Wing, J., Wittchen, H. U., Helzer, J. E., neurology (2nd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins
Babor, T. F., Burke, J., et al. (1988). The composite Press.
international diagnostic interview. An epidemiologic Trimble, M. R. (1992). Behaviour changes following tem-
Instrument suitable for use in conjunction with differ- poral lobectomy, with special reference to psychosis.
ent diagnostic systems and in different cultures. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry,
Archives of General Psychiatry, 45(12), 1069–1077. 55(2), 89–91.
Rossi, G., Van den Brande, I., Tobac, A., Sloore, H., & US Department of Health and Human Services, F. a.
Hauben, C. (2003). Convergent validity of the MCMI- D. A., Center for Drug Evaluation and Research,
III personality disorder scales and the MMPI-2 scales. Office of Translation Science, Office of Biostatistics.
Journal of Personality Disorders, 17(4), 330–340. (May 21, 2008). Statistical review and evaluation
Schwartz, J., & Cummings, J. L. (1988). Psychopathology :Antiepileptic drugs and suicidality.
and epilepsy: An outpatient consultation-liaison expe- Vazquez, B., & Devinsky, O. (2003). Epilepsy and anxi-
rience. Psychosomatics: Journal of Consultation ety. Epilepsy and Behavior, 4(Suppl 4), S20–S25.
Liaison Psychiatry, 29(3), 295–300. Victoroff, J. I., Benson, F., Grafton, S. T., Engel, J., Jr., &
Sheehan, D. V., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K. H., Amorim, P., Mazziotta, J. C. (1994). Depression in complex partial
Janavs, J., Weiller, E., et al. (1998). The Mini- seizures. Electroencephalography and cerebral meta-
international neuropsychiatric interview (MINI): The bolic correlates. Archives of Neurology, 51(2),
development and validation of a structured diagnostic 155–163.
psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. Wittchen, H. U., Kessler, R. C., Beesdo, K., Krause, P.,
Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 59(Suppl 20), 22–33. Hofler, M., & Hoyer, J. (2002). Generalized anxiety
quiz 34–57. and depression in primary care: Prevalence, recogni-
Sheehan, D. V., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K. H., Janavs, J., tion, and management. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry,
Weiller, E., Keskiner, A., et al. (1997). The validity of 63(Suppl 8), 24–34.
the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview Wittchen, H. U., Zhao, S., Kessler, R. C., & Eaton, W. W.
(MINI) according to the SCID-P and its reliability. (1994). DSM-III-R generalized anxiety disorder in the
European Psychiatry: The Journal of the Association National Comorbidity Survey. Archives of General
of European Psychiatrists, 12(5), 232–241. Psychiatry, 51(5), 355–364.
Somwaru, D. P., & Ben-Porath, Y. S. (1995) Development Zanarini, M. C., Frankenburg, F. R., Chauncey, D. L., &
and reliability of MMPI-2 based personality disorder Gunderson, J. G. (1987). The diagnostic Interview for
scales. Paper presented at the 30th Annual Workshop Personality Disorders: Interrater and test-retest reli-
and Symposium on Recent Developments in the Use of ability. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 28(6), 467–480.
the MMPI-2 and MMPI-A, St. Petersburg Beach, FL. Zanarini, M. C., Frankenburg, F. R., & Sickel, A. E.
Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., & Lushene, R. E. (1996). Diagnostic interview for DSM-IV personality
(1970). Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory. disorders (DIPD-IV). Belmont, MA: Laboratory for
Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. the Study of Adult Develpment, McLean Hospital, and
Spitzer, R. L., Kroenke, K., Williams, J. B., & Lowe, B. Department of Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School.
(2006). A brief measure for assessing generalized anx- Zanarini, M. C., Skodol, A. E., Bender, D., Dolan, R.,
iety disorder: The GAD-7. Archives of Internal Sanislow, C., Schaefer, E., et al. (2000). The
Medicine, 166(10), 1092–1097. Collaborative Longitudinal Personality Disorders
Stefansson, S. B., Olafsson, E., & Hauser, W. A. (1998). Study: Reliability of axis I and II diagnoses. Journal of
Psychiatric morbidity in epilepsy: A case controlled Personality Disorders, 14(4), 291–299.
study of adults receiving disability benefits. Journal of Zimmerman, M., & Mattia, J. I. (1999). The reliability
Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 64(2), and validity of a screening questionnaire for 13
238–241. DSM-IV Axis I disorders (the Psychiatric Diagnostic
Swinkels, W. A. M., Kuyk, J., de Graaf, E. H., van Dyck, Screening Questionnaire) in psychiatric outpatients.
R., & Spinhoven, P. (2001). Prevalence of psychopa- Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 60(10), 677–683.
thology in Dutch epilepsy inpatients: A comparative Zung, W. W. (1965). A self-rating depression scale.
study. Epilepsy and Behavior, 2(5), 441–447. Archives of General Psychiatry, 12(1), 63–70.
Psychotherapeutic and Cognitive
Remediation Approaches 11
Luba Nakhutina

As the medical management of epilepsy (QOL) in this population. Epilepsy patients con-
advanced, it became evident that a considerable sistently report lower education, higher rates of
number of patients with epilepsy (PWE), unemployment, lower income, and being single
including those with well-controlled seizures, compared with people without epilepsy (Hermann
have psychological and cognitive problems and et al., 2008). Unemployment or reduced employ-
are in need of further support. The lifetime ment options and earning potential of PWE are
prevalence of depressive disorders, anxiety much more frequent than in the general popula-
disorders, and suicidal thoughts is all significantly tion, and according to some studies, as much as
higher in PWE than in the general public double that of the general population (Smeets,
(Hermann, Seidenberg, & Jones, 2008). There van Lierop, Vanhoutvin, Aldenkamp, & Nijhuis,
are numerous psychosocial consequences 2007). Patients’ abilities to manage their epilepsy
associated with epilepsy, including stigmatization treatment and adhere to medication regimens are
and social isolation (Morrell, 2002), which also impacted by emotional and cognitive prob-
contribute to distress and poor coping with the lems. In fact, treatment nonadherence is PWE is
disease. Cognitive impairment, particularly high, with estimates ranging from 20 to 60 %.
memory dysfunction, is experienced by many Nonadherence naturally results in increased sei-
epilepsy patients, and these deficits negatively zures and increased health-care utilization costs
impact their functional and social independence. and is the most significant risk for sudden unex-
While ictal and postictal cognitive and behavioral plained death in epilepsy or SUDEP (Faught,
disturbances have been long recognized and Duh, Weiner, Guerin, & Cunnington, 2008, 2009,
targeted in treatment, interictal disturbances in Faught, Weiner, et al., 2009). Finally, studies
cognition and mood remain largely unaddressed, have shown that epilepsy patients report lower
although in some individuals these problems may QOL compared to persons with other chronic
be more disabling than the seizures themselves. illnesses and disabilities (e.g., Hermann et al.,
Emotional distress and persistent cognitive 1996). Symptoms of depression and seizure
disturbance associated with epilepsy are major worry were shown to be the most important
causes of disability and poor quality of life factors affecting QOL in patients with intractable
epilepsy (Loring, Meador, & Lee, 2004), and
cognitive impairment is one of the most frequently
L. Nakhutina, Ph.D., A.B.P.P.-C.N. (*) identified factors in reducing the overall QOL
SUNY Downstate Medical Center, (Bishop & Allen, 2003; Leidy et al., 1999).
450 Clarkson Ave. Box 1275, Brooklyn,
NY 11203, USA As awareness of these problems has increased,
e-mail: luba.nakhutina@downstate.edu it has become evident that management of

W.B. Barr and C. Morrison (eds.), Handbook on the Neuropsychology of Epilepsy, 235
Clinical Handbooks in Neuropsychology, DOI 10.1007/978-0-387-92826-5_11,
© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015
236 L. Nakhutina

epilepsy with AEDs and neurosurgery is sion and anxiety disorders is significantly
insufficient, and comprehensive care that includes higher in PWE than in the general public or in
behavioral interventions is necessary. Yet, while patients with several other chronic conditions,
the literature on the medical management of such as asthma (Ettinger, Reed, Cramer, &
epilepsy is extensive, there remains a paucity of Epilepsy Impact Project Group, 2004; Kanner,
psychotherapeutic and cognitive remediation 2007). Depression is the most common emo-
approaches that have been validated empirically tional concern in epilepsy, with the rates of
for use in the comprehensive management of depression reported to be as high as 55 %
PWE. Available research in this area is among patients treated in specialized medical
complicated by methodological pitfalls, such as centers (Dodrill, 2008). The level of depression
small sample sizes, sample selection bias, is not usually well accounted for by seizure
absence of adequate control group and variables, suggesting that psychological, social,
randomization procedures, and controversies as and medication variables play a significant part
to measures of outcome. Despite these limitations, in this relationship (Bishop & Allen, 2007). It
there is evidence to suggest that behavioral has been shown that depression but not seizure
interventions can reduce emotional distress and frequency predicts QOL in treatment resistant
cognitive morbidity, increase treatment epilepsy, and this has led many investigators to
compliance, and possibly reduce seizure conclude that treatment of depression may be
frequency and severity. By targeting the inadequately prioritized in the epilepsy man-
psychological and cognitive symptoms of PWE, agement in these patients (e.g., Boylan et al.,
effective behavioral interventions also have the 2004; Thapar, Kerr, & Gordon, 2009).
potential to increase patients’ independence, Anxiety disorders are also commonly comor-
reduce epilepsy-related disability and the bid with epilepsy, with reported prevalence rates
associated costs, as well as enhance patients’ ranging from 19 % to over 50 % across studies
overall QOL. and the different settings where epilepsy patients
The purpose of this chapter is to (1) briefly were assessed (Beyenburg, Mitchell, Schmidt,
discuss prevalent emotional and cognitive Elger, & Reuber, 2005). The unpredictable tim-
problems associated with epilepsy, (2) summarize ing and intrusive nature of seizures demand con-
evidence-based strategies and guidelines offered stant vigilance and often result in chronic and
for interventions in other patient groups (e.g., disabling anxiety, which can have a profound
TBI, stroke), (3) review the limited empirical influence on the QOL of epilepsy patients.
information available from behavioral Seizure frequency has been linked to anxiety in
intervention studies in epilepsy, and (4) provide some but not all studies. In older adults, includ-
guidance to neuropsychologists who are looking ing those with infrequent seizures (i.e., less than
to design and implement psychotherapeutic and one per year), the fear induced by the possibility
cognitive remediation treatments for PWE. of a seizure may be sufficient to significantly
reduce QOL (McLaughlin, Pachana, &
Mcfarland, 2008).
Psychotherapeutic Interventions The level of distress in PWE is also increased
by cognitive problems, and distress in turn
Emotional Distress and Psychosocial exacerbates cognitive dysfunction. At the same
Factors in Patients with Epilepsy time, distressed patients are more prone to
overreactions to memory failures, some of which
In management of epilepsy, it is important to may be normal forgetfulness. It has been shown
consider the high rates of emotional distress that perceived cognitive functioning was strongly
and psychosocial problems, which negatively correlated with mood and weakly correlated with
impact patients’ ability to cope with disease and memory performance scores (Elixhauser, Kline
their QOL. The lifetime prevalence of depres- Leidy, Meador, Means, & Willian, 1999).
11 Psychotherapeutic and Cognitive Remediation Approaches 237

Living with epilepsy also implies psychosocial consequences of AEDs misuse, with resultant
challenges. Psychosocial functioning in PWE has increase in seizure frequency, misguided
to do with the level of effectiveness with which medication changes or emergency room visits, as
individuals are able to function in their social well as heightened perceptions of stigma, social
environment, including situations pertaining to withdrawal, and depression.
education, employment, interpersonal To address emotional distress, psychosocial
relationships, as well as relationships with their problems, and lack of disease-related information
physicians, adjustment to seizures, and medical among PWE, a number of educational and
management of epilepsy. For instance, psychotherapeutic interventions have been
psychosocial factors that have been implicated in developed. These interventions have been
the high unemployment rate among PWE include provided by various types of professionals,
social isolation, lack of information, social skills including specialist epilepsy nurses, social
deficits, and lack of family support (Bishop & workers, health psychologists, and only
Allen, 2007). Some of the psychosocial problems infrequently neuropsychologists. Infrequent
may be related to the perceived social stigma participation of neuropsychologists is surprising
associated with epilepsy. In fact, epilepsy in given the need for understanding of cognitive
many cultures has often been regarded as one of deficits in PWE when attempting to effect
the most stigmatizing conditions. The negative behavior change in these patients and to teach
attitudes that PWE encounter typically result them new information. In the next section,
from lack of information and understanding of empirical evidence from psychoeducational and
epilepsy; however, feeling stigmatized does not psychotherapeutic programs for PWE will be
always correlate with the presence of prejudice reviewed. It should be noted that differentiation
but relies heavily on individuals’ perceptions. among these interventions is difficult to make, as
Stigma can be one of the most distressing there is usually much overlap, whereby
consequences of having seizures, and it can lead psychotherapy commonly includes patient
to social isolation and have a negative impact on education and psychoeducational programs often
QOL (Prus, Nakhutina, & Grant, 2009). instruct patients in coping skills.
Perceptions of lower self-efficacy to manage
epilepsy and poorer adherence to treatment were
found in PWE who report higher levels of stigma Psychoeducational Programs
(DiIorio, Osborne Shafer, Letz, et al., 2003), and
lower levels of stigma were associated with The focus of psychoeducation has been to impart
finding and maintaining employment (Smeets knowledge about epilepsy and its treatment,
et al., 2007). Smith et al. (2009) found complex improve coping skills and QOL, and increase
interactions between demographic, clinical, and compliance with treatment and seizure control.
psychosocial factors at play with the perception The educational programs have varied in their
of stigma and suggested that perceptions of self- approach, and only a few have been evaluated
efficacy in managing epilepsy are a point of using randomized, controlled methodology (May
intervention to improve stigma. & Pfafflin, 2005). The results of studies conducted
Compounding the emotional and psychosocial thus far are promising, and the need for
problems is the fact that epilepsy patients often educational interventions has been highlighted
have inadequate knowledge and misconceptions (Bradley & Lindsay, 2008).
about their illness and its treatment. They may
also possess poor understanding of the cognitive Programs Targeting Epilepsy-Related
and emotional problems they may be experiencing Knowledge and Attitudes
and the way these problems may be impacting Reviews by the Cochrane group (Bradley &
their daily lives. Lack of information and Lindsay, 2008; Shaw et al., 2007) identified only
erroneous beliefs can result in dangerous two randomized controlled studies, evaluating
238 L. Nakhutina

programs that have targeted epilepsy-related Programs Aimed at Increasing


knowledge and attitudes (Helgeson, Mittan, Tan, Medication Compliance
& Chayasirisobhon, 1990; May & Pfafflin, 2002). Despite the recognized importance of treatment
Seizures and Epilepsy Education (SEE; Helgeson adherence in determining health and disability
et al., 1990) is a 2-day psychoeducational inter- outcomes in PWE, research on interventions
vention for adults with epilepsy aimed at improv- aimed specifically at improving adherence in
ing understanding of epilepsy and viewed these patients has been lacking (Bradley &
essential to effective coping with the disease and Lindsay, 2008). The one existing randomized
related disability. The program covers the medi- controlled trial (RCT) showed that a brief inter-
cal aspects of epilepsy, placing an emphasis on vention focusing on patient education about the
improving medication adherence, as well as on importance of compliance and training in
the social and emotional aspects of the disease. behavioral strategies (e.g., medication con-
Helgeson et al. (1990) found that, in comparison tainer, prescription refill reminder cards sent by
to the wait-list control group (n = 18), the treat- mail) can be effective in improving medication
ment group (n = 20) demonstrated significant adherence and self-reported seizure control
improvements in disease-related knowledge and (Petersen, McLean, & Millingen, 1984). These
reduction in epilepsy-related fears (e.g., fear that results are encouraging; however, the research
a single seizure will lead to brain damage or in this area unfortunately has not progressed
death). They also demonstrated improvement in much beyond this study, and a handful of addi-
compliance with medication regimens, as mea- tional interventions conducted since had many
sured by drug serum levels. However, there were methodological limitations. Recently, DiIorio
no significant changes in anxiety, depression, and colleagues have published descriptions of
QOL, self-efficacy, or seizure frequency. two interventions, one telephone based and one
Modular Service Package Epilepsy (MOSES; web based, that are under investigation in RCTs
Reid, Specht, Thornbecke, Goecke, & Wohlfarth, (DiIorio et al., 2009, DiIorio, Reisinger, Yeager,
2001) is designed for group education and aims & McCarty, 2009). These nurse-delivered inter-
to engage patients in an active role in managing ventions focus on medication adherence strate-
their disability and become experts in dealing gies, information about epilepsy and seizures,
with epilepsy. The program consists of multiple and stress management. Based on constructs
modules with goals of improving individual from social cognitive theory and motivational
participants’ understanding of epilepsy, its interviewing, these interventions focus on iden-
treatment, and its psychosocial consequences tifying and addressing barriers to adherence
and, thus, increasing their self-confidence, through a collaborative, confidence-enhancing
teaching them to cope with the disease, and approach.
enabling them to become more autonomous. May
and Pfafflin (2002) investigated the efficacy of
MOSES, given as a 2-day course, in a randomized Psychotherapy Approaches
controlled study. The findings indicated that the
program was particularly effective in providing Psychotherapeutic interventions include
information and self-management strategies. cognitive behavioral therapy, relaxation therapy,
Patients in the treatment group (n = 113) reported and EEG biofeedback, as well as patient
reduction in seizure frequency and better education. These interventions have been used
tolerability of AED treatment. The program did alone or in combination in treatment of epilepsy
not have a significant impact on QOL, epilepsy- patients, with the aim of reducing depression and
related fears, or emotional disturbances, such as anxiety symptoms, decreasing seizure frequency,
depression. and improving QOL.
11 Psychotherapeutic and Cognitive Remediation Approaches 239

Cognitive Behavioral Therapy ship of stress and seizures, identification of


A major focus of interventions that have applied seizure-provoking situations, cognitive restruc-
cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) to treatment turing, and training in relaxation and stress man-
of PWE has been on cognitive restructuring to agement techniques. The patients in the treatment
improve patients’ coping and stress management group (eight adults) were reported to show sig-
skills in order to reduce depression and anxiety nificant gains in self-efficacy or the ability to
and enhance their overall QOL. Using CBT manage seizures and stress, as was assessed by a
techniques, patients are taught to confront modified Epilepsy Self-Efficacy Scale (ESES;
cognitive distortions, irrational beliefs, and DiIorio & Yeager, 2003) and in QOL (QOLIE-
negative thoughts related to epilepsy and self- 31; Cramer et al., 1998) when evaluated at
perception. Another major application of CBT 3-month follow-up. These changes were not
with epilepsy patients has been to teach self- observed in the wait-list control group (nine
management techniques for improvement of adults), and no significant reduction in seizure
seizure control. These strategies have included frequency was noted following treatment.
observation of seizure triggers and initiation of Goldstein, McAlpine, Deale, Toone, and
countermeasures, such as breathing techniques or Mellers (2003) reported positive results of CBT
coping statements. The methods employed in (12 sessions) led by a nurse specialist and
CBT interventions for PWE have varied widely designed to address epilepsy-related problems
and have included treatment protocols of varying and the associated psychopathology. Treatment
length, individual or group format, with different focused on decreasing psychiatric symptoms by
types of professionals administering treatment. assisting patients in adjustment and introducing
While only a limited number of RCTs have been countermeasures, such as coping statements,
conducted, with mixed findings, possibly shifting attention, and deep breathing, to reduce
attributable to varying methodologies, they seizures. Upon completion of treatment, patients
suggest that CBT can be a valuable approach in rated their epilepsy-related problems as having
treatment of epilepsy patients. less impact on their daily lives than previously.
In earlier studies, Davis, Armstrong, Donovan, At 1-month follow-up, patients also reported
& Temkin (1984) reported positive results from decreased use of escape-avoidance coping strate-
an RCT evaluating a 6-week treatment with 13 gies, as well as improved self-rated work and
adults with epilepsy, designed to increase social adjustment. These results were observed
participants’ skills in managing depression. despite the absence of a significant decrease in
Cognitive behavioral techniques included seizure frequency.
maximizing pleasant events and physical Cognitive behavioral techniques have also
exercise, increasing assertiveness, identifying been used in attempts to prevent depression in
faulty belief systems, and increasing positive those determined to be at risk for the development
cognitions and self-reinforcing statements. The of depression. Martinovic, Simonovi, and Djoki
treatment group was reported to evidence marked (2006) randomized 30 adolescent patients with
reduction in depression, as assessed by self- equivalent levels of depressive symptoms prior to
report measures. There was also a significant treatment to receive either CBT or counseling as
decrease in self-reported anxiety/stress and anger usual. Depressive symptoms improved in patients
and increased involvement in social activities. who received CBT, and patient QOL ratings
Subsequently, Au et al. (2003) evaluated correlated with mood improvement. In contrast, a
effectiveness of cognitive behavioral intervention few of the patients in the control group developed
(eight 2-h sessions) focused particularly on cog- depression at follow-up.
nitive restructuring and seizure control. The Studies evaluating interventions that
sessions were led by two clinical psychologists specifically targeted seizure frequency, severity,
and included information about the relation- or duration have yielded mixed results. In a
240 L. Nakhutina

study by Tan and Bruni (1986), patients were related emotional and psychosocial problems.
randomized to CBT, supportive counseling, or Continued research evaluating this approach in
wait-list control. The treatment did not result in epilepsy is needed.
significant reduction in seizure frequency.
Moreover, improvements in psychosocial Progressive Relaxation Training
adjustment were observed in both therapy groups, It is widely accepted that stress is a common
and thus, no specific advantage of CBT was precipitant of seizures in PWE, and it has been
demonstrated. In contrast, Gillham (1990) inferred that behavioral approaches used to
reported an improvement in seizure control and minimize stress would therefore improve seizure
psychological symptoms after individual CBT control. A number of studies have tested this
that involved monitoring of symptoms and hypothesis employing different relaxation
initiation of countermeasures, such as breathing training protocols and have reported positive
techniques. Spector, Tranah, Cull, and Goldstein results, although the mechanisms by which
(1999) reported positive efficacy results of a relaxation training appears to reduce seizure
group intervention (8 weekly sessions) designed frequency are unclear. Dahl, Melin, and Lund
to improve seizure control in seven adult patients (1987) found a significant advantage of relaxation
with intractable seizures. Treatment included therapy in decreasing seizure frequency in 18
psychoeducation and cognitive behavioral adults treated over 6 weeks (1 h weekly), as
techniques. Seizure frequency was significantly compared to supportive therapy attention control
reduced in intervention participants, and the or no treatment. The treatment effects were
treatment effects persisted at 2-month follow-up. maintained at 10- and 30-week follow-up.
However, no significant improvement in Rousseau, Hermann, and Whitman (1985)
psychosocial functioning in these patients was reported that implementing progressive relaxation
observed. Finally, Lundren, Dahl, Melin, and training over 3 weeks in eight adults with
Kies (2006) reported that a short-term acceptance uncontrolled epilepsy resulted in 30 % reduction
and commitment therapy, combined with seizure in median seizure frequency. Similarly, in a study
behavior management techniques, had significant examining the efficacy of progressive muscle
effects on seizure frequency, duration, and QOL, relaxation (six sessions conducted over 4–8
as compared to supportive therapy. Treatment weeks) in reducing seizure activity, Puskarich
was distributed in two individual and two group et al., 1992 found that the treatment resulted in
sessions during a 4-week period. 29 % reduction in mean seizure frequency,
In summary, the evidence from a number of whereas there was only 3 % decrease for the
studies suggests that CBT may be effective in control (i.e., quiet sitting) group. The effects of
helping PWE manage depression and anxiety relaxation therapy on psychosocial functioning
symptoms, improve their ability to control sei- have also been investigated (Snyder, 1983);
zures, as well as improve their overall QOL. however, no significant difference in scores on
However, the validity of reported results is lim- the Washington Psychosocial Seizure Inventory
ited by methodological problems, including a (WPSI; Dodrill, Batzel, Queisser, & Temkin,
small number of participants, lack appropriate 1980) were found between treatment and control
control groups or randomization procedures, and groups after relaxation therapy, and no
insufficient information with regard to seizure information regarding the seizure frequency was
type and concomitant antidepressant therapy provided.
(Ramaratnam, Baker, & Goldstein, 2008). While These studies indicate possible beneficial
it is difficult to draw definitive conclusions about effects of relaxation training on seizure frequency;
the efficacy of CBT in treatment of PWE in view however, due to methodological deficiencies,
of these methodological limitations, the evidence including considerable difference in the seizure
gathered thus far suggests that CBT may prove to frequencies at baseline among patients in the con-
be an effective method in addressing epilepsy- trol and treatment groups, definitive conclusions
11 Psychotherapeutic and Cognitive Remediation Approaches 241

cannot be made, and further studies are needed in social and emotional cognition. Memory
(Ramaratnam et al., 2008). impairment, attention and concentration deficits,
and mental slowing are the most frequent cog-
Use of Biofeedback in Treatment nitive problems associated with epilepsy
of Epilepsy Patients (Aldenkamp, 2006; Jokeit & Ebner, 1999) and are
The biofeedback technique involves monitoring commonly reported subjective complaints of
of physiological responses, such as heart rate, PWE. Cognitive impairment in PWE appears to
galvanic skin response (GSR), or brain wave result from multiple factors, including the under-
pattern (EEG), and providing feedback to the lying etiology, neuronal dysfunction or loss, sei-
individual to help him or her actively control the zure-related variables, localization of the
physiological response. The effects of epileptogenic zone, adverse effects of antiepilep-
biofeedback on seizure frequency have been tic drugs (AEDs), and comorbid psychiatric con-
explored in numerous studies, and there is a ditions, such as depression and anxiety
significant body of literature on EEG biofeedback (Aldenkamp, Baker, & Meador, 2004; Hermann
(see review by Sterman, 1990). Ramaratnam & Whitman, 1984). These factors interrelate,
et al. (2008) critically reviewed this literature and resulting in heterogeneous nature of cognitive
identified only one trial where randomized profiles that range widely in severity of deficits.
controlled procedures were employed (Lantz & Early seizure onset, long duration of the disease,
Sherman, 1988). In this study, the authors and poor seizure control are associated with poor
investigated the effects of EEG biofeedback cognitive outcome (Elger, Helmstaedter, &
(6-week treatment) on 24 adults with uncontrolled Kurthen, 2004). While epilepsy surgery is an
epilepsy who were randomized to contingent important treatment option allowing many
EEG biofeedback, noncontingent feedback, and patients to achieve seizure freedom, the surgical
no intervention control. Significant seizure intervention carries the risk of causing additional
frequency reduction (61 %) following contingent cognitive impairments.
EEG biofeedback at 6 weeks follow-up was Patients often list memory problems as their
reported (Lantz & Sherman, 1988). greatest concern (Hendriks, 2001) with memory
Nagai, Goldstein, Fenwick, and Trimble deficits, in varying degrees of severity, being
(2004) studied the effects of GSR biofeedback the most common finding in objective neuropsy-
training or sham feedback on seizure frequency chological evaluations. Seizures often originate
in 18 adult patients with treatment resistant in mesiotemporal structures that are essential for
epilepsy in a single blind RCT. These investigators memory processes (Squire, Stark, & Clark,
reported 50 % greater reduction in seizure 2004), and NMDA receptors, critical in learn-
frequency in the treatment group relative to ing and memory, appear to play a prominent
patients in the control group. role in the neuropathology of epilepsy (Engel,
Pedley, Aicardi, & Dichter, 2007). Learning
and memory difficulties are the most frequent
Cognitive Remediation Approaches cognitive changes in patients with temporal
lobe epilepsy (TLE), irrespective of the pres-
Cognitive Deficits in Patients ence of overt damage in temporal lobe struc-
with Epilepsy tures (Giovagnoli & Avanzini, 1999). Frontal
lobe epilepsy (FLE) may affect an array of cog-
Epilepsy patients can present with cognitive defi- nitive abilities, including memory, attention,
cits in various domains, including intellectual and executive functions (Helmstaedter, Kemper,
abilities, attention and concentration, memory, & Elger, 1996). The overall prevalence of mem-
executive functions (e.g., organization, mental ory problems in patients with refractory epi-
flexibility, problem-solving), language, visuospa- lepsy has been estimated as high as 20–50 %
tial skills, processing and motor speed, as well as (Halgren et al., 1991).
242 L. Nakhutina

Deficits in cognitive functions are considered Cognitive Rehabilitation: Definition


more debilitating than the actual seizures by some
patients. Cognitive functioning may be a signifi- Cognitive rehabilitation has been defined as a
cant factor in determining educational attainment “systematic, functionally oriented service of
and employment status, and even subtle deficits therapeutic activities that is based on assessment
may manifest themselves in demanding profes- and understanding of the patient’s brain-
sional or academic settings. Cognitive deficits are behavioral deficits” (Cicerone et al., 2000).
also likely to negatively impact patients’ ability to Wilson has defined cognitive rehabilitation as
adhere to their treatment regimens, especially “any intervention strategy or technique which
when the regimens are complex and, thus, reliant intends to enable clients or patients, and their
on many cognitive abilities. In a study focused on families, to live with, manage, by-pass, reduce or
medication adherence, study, 56 % of epilepsy come to terms with cognitive deficits precipitated
patients reported forgetfulness as a reason for poor by injury to the brain” (Wilson, 1997). A
medication adherence, and the overall nonadher- comprehensive neuropsychological evaluation
ence rate was 68 % (Nakhutina, Gonzalez, Prus, & plays an important role in guiding cognitive
Grant, 2009). Finally, cognitive dysfunction is also remediation based on detailed information about
one of the most frequently identified factors in patients’ level of intellectual functioning,
reducing QOL (Bishop & Allen, 2003), even in cognitive profile (e.g., areas of cognitive deficits
patients with well-controlled epilepsy. In particu- and preserved abilities), and severity of cognitive
lar, individuals with impaired memory were found impairment. Cognitive remediation is also guided
to have significantly worse QOL than the ones by patients’ functional goals or what is expected
with unimpaired memory (Leidy et al., 1999). of them (e.g., return to work or school,
Evidence-based cognitive remediation has the participation at home).
potential to improve cognitive status, functional
abilities, and QOL in PWE. The interest in the
possibility of rehabilitation of cognitive problems Rehabilitation of Memory
has greatly expanded in the last decades, and
there is now a considerable amount of literature Memory rehabilitation is a component of
on cognitive rehabilitation (e.g., Sohlberg & cognitive rehabilitation. Two broad approaches
Mateer, 2001; Wilson, 1987); however, the focus to memory rehabilitation have been employed:
of this literature remains primarily on traumatic (1) restitution training and (2) strategy training.
brain injury (TBI) and stroke, whereas Using the restitution training approach, memory
applications to epilepsy patients have been very rehabilitation programs rely heavily on repetitive
limited (Shulman & Barr, 2002). The attempts to drills and practice of cognitive skills in attempts
investigate the efficacy of cognitive rehabilitation to restore the underlying impaired function.
in PWE with adequate study of outcomes have Tasks utilized for this purpose are often
been very few, and RCTs are grossly lacking. In computerized, and software packages have been
the following section, (1) definition of cognitive made commercially available. This approach has
remediation will be provided, (2) select not been found to lead to general improvements
remediation strategies with supportive evidence in memory functioning (Prigatano et al., 1984).
for use with brain injury and stroke patients will The strategy training approach to rehabilitation
be summarized, and (3) limited empirical has received more research support, especially
evidence from studies of cognitive interventions when targeting memory problems (Cicerone
in PWE will be reviewed. The focus will be on et al., 2000, 2005). This approach focuses on
rehabilitation of memory and attention problems, teaching patients compensatory strategies to
the most prevalent complaints in PWE and key circumvent difficulties that arise as a result of
domains targeted by existing remediation inter- their memory impairment (Sohlberg & Mateer,
ventions in epilepsy patients. 1989). Patients are taught the use of internal
11 Psychotherapeutic and Cognitive Remediation Approaches 243

memory aids, such as mnemonics, visual imagery, brain functioning. Twenty-one participants, in
and rehearsal, to help them remember and recall groups of five, attended 16-day treatment over a
information and/or external memory aids, such period of 8 weeks. Memory training with partici-
as notebooks, voice organizers (Van den Broek, pants focused on increasing understanding of
Downes, Johnson, Dayus, & Hilton, 2000), memory functioning and on teaching strategies
alarms, pagers (Wilson, Emslie, & Evans, 2001), (e.g., diary) for improving memory, while target-
mobile phones (Wade & Troy, 2001), and other ing their individual cognitive problems, as identi-
assistive devices and environmental manipula- fied by the neuropsychological evaluation. A few
tions, to help reduce memory failures and execu- meetings were also conducted with a parallel
tive functioning problems. The generalizability group of participants’ significant others. The
of internal memory aid use to natural settings has results included improvements in the overall
also been questioned. These techniques have QOL, as assessed by QOLIE-89 (Devinsky et al.,
been found ineffective for many patients with 1995), administered prior to enrollment, immedi-
significantly compromised intellectual function- ately after completion of the program and at 6
ing and may be useful only for acquisition of spe- months posttreatment. Qualitative assessments of
cific and small bodies of information (Sohlberg treatment outcome revealed improved under-
& Mateer, 1989). The external memory aids can standing and control of memory problems (as
be very effective, particularly when dealing with reported by 10 of 21 participants), increased
severe memory impairment; however, these aids knowledge about epilepsy, and improved self-
are often given with minimal instruction or for- confidence. Although patients derived benefit
mal training (Sohlberg & Mateer, 1989). from the holistic approach, their feedback also
Cicerone and colleagues (2000) provided indicated that the time devoted to memory train-
evidence-based recommendations for cognitive ing was insufficient for participants whose mem-
rehabilitation with TBI and stroke patients. Based ory problems were more severe.
on the review of findings from well-designed Barr, Morrison, Isaacs, and Devinsky (2004)
prospective RCTs, along with supportive developed and evaluated a group-based
evidence from less rigorous trials, Cicerone et al. intervention focused specifically on treatment of
(2005) suggested that the evidence for memory memory difficulties in PWE. Twenty-three
strategy training (including the use of internalized patients were enrolled in treatment, consisting of
strategies and external memory compensations) six 75-min-long sessions led by two
for individuals with mild memory problems was neuropsychologists. Sixteen patients had
“compelling enough” to recommend it as a previously received surgical intervention. Each
practice standard. Teaching external memory session included educational presentations about
aids with direct application to functional activities epilepsy and memory, followed by instruction in
was offered as practice guidelines for patients the use of memory aids (e.g., calendar, electronic
with moderate to severe memory impairment, organizers, pagers) and mnemonic techniques
based on evidence of probable effectiveness (e.g., (e.g., imagery and association). Participants
as seen in nonrandomized cohort studies or case- shared personal experiences and strategies they
control studies). found helpful for managing memory problems.
They were provided with informational hand-
Remediation of Mild to Moderate outs and given homework assignments utilizing
Memory Deficits concepts introduced in the group. The treatment
Johanson, Chaplin, and Wedlund (2001) con- outcome was assessed by self-report inventories
ducted memory training for PWE as part of a (i.e., QOLIE-10, Cramer, Perrine, Devinsky, &
holistic neurorehabilitation program that inte- Meador, 1996; modified Memory Complaints
grated psychotherapy and aimed at addressing Inventory or MCI; Green & Allen, 1997),
poor self-image and education about epilepsy and given at baseline and at 1–2-month follow-up.
244 L. Nakhutina

While no significant changes in global ratings of intervention. In a study investigating the


QOL were observed, patient responses suggested effectiveness of visual imagery as a mnemonic
improvements in subjective ratings of verbal technique in aiding the recall of verbal informa-
memory and word finding. Patients also reported tion in patients after unilateral temporal lobec-
an increase in medication side effects which, as tomy (TL) for treatment of refractory epilepsy,
the authors suggested, may have been due to Jones (1974) showed that on a paired-associate
increased awareness of these issues through edu- learning task, patients with left TL could partially
cational presentations. compensate for their verbal memory deficits by
Ponds and Hendriks (2006) described a using visual imagery, and thus improve their
treatment program also focused on memory recall. In contrast, patients with amnestic syn-
rehabilitation, reporting positive results. dromes (resulting from bilateral damage to the
Participants were epilepsy patients with mesial temporal structures) derived no benefit
objectively defined memory deficits, as from this strategy, and patients with right TL per-
determined by the diagnostic neuropsychological formed similarly to normal controls.
assessment. Everyday memory problems were The effects of cognitive remediation on
assessed with questionnaires completed by memory outcome after temporal lobe epilepsy
patients and their significant others, who were (TLE) surgery were also evaluated by
also asked to monitor patients’ memory problems Helmstaedter et al. (2008). Patients began
on a daily basis for 2 weeks. Information gleaned treatment 3–15 days after surgery, and the average
from these assessments resulted in a list of ten duration of treatment was 29.3 days. The
concrete memory problems, formulated as intervention aimed to strengthen compensation
individual treatment goals. In six to eight strategies for coping with cognitive impairment
sessions, scheduled every 2 weeks, patients and involved repeated practice of computer-
learned compensatory strategies for their based exercises for attention, memory, and
personally formulated treatment goals. Rather executive functioning, with difficulty level
than prescribing strategies, participants were adjusted to each patient. The software employed
encouraged to choose strategies for the memory was intended for general use with individuals
problems they were experiencing, apply them to with compromised brain functioning. The
everyday life situations, and discuss in group intervention was conceptualized as holistic and
their experience using these strategies. included psychoeducation into the effects of
Participants were also provided with information brain functioning, cognitive deficits, impact of
about epilepsy and memory functioning. Three personality and emotional reactions, as well as
months after finishing treatment, participants individual counseling and occupational and
returned for “brush-up” session and a physical therapy. Baseline and outcome
neuropsychological reevaluation to measure assessments included attention measures (letter
treatment effects. The authors reported positive cancellation) and tests of verbal and figural
results of this treatment approach, as assessed memory.
with subjective rating scales completed by 21 Helmstaedter et al. (2008) reported that left
patients and their relatives. The participants TL patients profited less from the rehabilitation
reported being more acquainted with strategies to than those post-right TL patients. Figural memory
support memory problems and better ability to was not affected by the rehabilitation, and
cope with memory problems in their daily lives. attention improved independent of treatment.
In addition, significant improvements were The authors concluded that rehabilitation can
observed on verbal memory tasks (Hendriks, counteract the verbal memory decline after
2001; Ponds & Hendriks, 2006). temporal lobe resection, particularly in right TL
The question has been raised by some patients with verbal memory problems. However,
investigators whether postsurgical memory these results should be interpreted with caution
decline can be counteracted with a cognitive due to methodological limitations, given that
11 Psychotherapeutic and Cognitive Remediation Approaches 245

there was no randomization, with patients from goals were to improve attention and memory for
another center serving as a control group. In auditory and written information. Exercises
addition, strategies similar to those covered in included visual search for targets of increasing
cognitive rehabilitation were also taught in complexity, listening to and reading news articles,
occupational therapy, making it difficult to tease and providing a summary of this information.
out the independent effects of the cognitive Subsequent treatment goals targeted more
intervention. Finally, it is unclear whether the complex functions, including verbal
benefits of the intervention generalized, as the conceptualization and flexibility. The treatment
effects on everyday functioning and on daily approach cognitive flexibility emphasized func-
living were not investigated. tional goals (e.g., to attend classes toward a cer-
Other reports in the area of cognitive tificate in child care) and provided guided practice
remediation in epilepsy patients are case studies in use of strategies in daily life, employing task
(e.g., Gupta & Naorem, 2003; Langenbahn, analysis, structure, graduated cuing, and shaping
Peery, Rodriguez, Payton, & Dams-O’Connor, (Langenbahn et al., 2008).
2008). Gupta and Naorem (2003) described
cognitive retraining with an epilepsy patient who Cognitive Interventions for Patients
was taught compensatory strategies (e.g., with Severe Memory Disturbance
chunking, imagery) for memory and given Sohlberg and Mateer (1989) provided a training
exercises for attention (e.g., cancellations). The protocol for teaching individuals with severe
patient was also instructed in deep breathing memory impairments to independently utilize a
relaxation techniques. There was a parallel home- compensatory memory notebook. This portable
based intervention program under the supervision notebook is personalized to the functional needs of
of a trained family member. Assessment included the patient. It serves as a means to organize and
neuropsychological measures of memory store information (e.g., calendar, names and phone
(Rivermead Behavioral Memory Test; Wilson, numbers of important contacts) and can be adapted
Cockburn, & Baddeley, 1985) and attention to patients at various levels of cognitive function-
(e.g., digit cancellation task), a self-report ques- ing. The training of patients in functional memory
tionnaire, and observer ratings of patient’s book system incorporates principles of learning
emotional status, administered pre- and post- theory and consists of three steps. First, during the
training. The training was reported to be effective acquisition stage, the patient becomes familiar
in improving attentional and memory capacities, with the names and functions of each notebook
as well as the emotional status of the patient. section. Then, the application stage involves train-
However, the issue of practice effects was not ing in applying the use of the notebook to the
addressed, and it is unclear whether the training appropriate situations. Finally, during the adapta-
effects generalized to everyday functioning. tion stage, the individual demonstrates the ability
Langenbahn et al. (2008) reported positive to adapt and modify skill when faced with novel
results of rehabilitation of a 39-year-old, situations. Each level of instructions incorporates
predominantly Spanish-speaking, Hispanic an efficiency building learning parameter, consist-
woman with seizure disorder and left TL. The ing of accuracy and consistency measures that
patient was treated in individual cognitive result in skill maintenance and use. Using this pro-
remediation, psychotherapy, and psychosocial tocol, Sohlberg and Mateer (1989) found that an
group sessions, once weekly, over a period of 17 intensive, systematic approach to training in mem-
months. Treatment was conducted in Spanish by ory book system utilization may improve may
staff with the knowledge about acculturation, improve functional capacity in individuals with
patient family structure and values, spiritual severe memory deficits and that many patients
beliefs, and cultural beliefs about treatment and went on to independent living.
recovery. Utilizing techniques used with acquired Aldenkamp and Vermeulen (1991) described
brain injury patients, initial cognitive remediation memory training efforts with amnestic epilepsy
246 L. Nakhutina

patients. The training of compensatory memory of six weekly, individual, 1-h sessions. Patients in
aids was the primary objective. Patients were also the retraining method had to rehearse responses
taught specific information, relevant to their jobs on tasks, as task complexity increased automati-
(e.g., as a phone operator in a sheltered work- cally with improved performance. During treat-
shop). The group treatment approach was chosen ment using the compensation method, patients
as most patients found this motivating and a were made aware of their attention and memory
source of support. The training, conducted in six failures in daily life and then taught compensatory
sessions, was co-led by a neuropsychologist and a strategies to target these failures. All patients were
vocational trainer, both with expertise in epilepsy. evaluated with neuropsychological measures and
Patients underwent a neuropsychological assess- self-report inventories at pre-training, post-train-
ment prior to participation in treatment and at 2 ing, and a 6-month follow-up. The results suggested
months after completion of the program. Based improvements on objective neuropsychological out-
on their observations, the authors concluded that come measures, self-reported cognitive outcomes,
external memory aids can be used to bypass some and QOL at 6-month follow-up both, in the retrain-
of the problems in severely amnestic epilepsy ing and the compensation method groups relative
patients. They also concluded that it is possible to the wait-list control group. However, the com-
to teach epilepsy patients with memory impair- pensation method was more effective in improving
ment specific bits of information, skills, or behav- self-reported neuropsychological outcomes and
iors that are important to their daily needs. QOL, especially for patients with less education
(Engelberts et al. 2002a).

Rehabilitation of Attention Deficits


Evaluation of Outcome in Behavioral
Rehabilitation programs to address attention Interventions for PWE
impairments have been developed for patients
with head injury (Fasotti, Kovacs, Eling, & Measurement of outcome in psychotherapy and
Brouwer, 2000; Sohlberg, McLaughlin, Pavese, cognitive remediation is a major challenge.
Heidrich, & Posner, 2000), schizophrenia (Hayes Methodological issues in outcome measurement
& McGrath, 2000), and multiple sclerosis have been discussed previously by many authors
(Plohmann, Kappos, & Brunnschweiler, 1994); (e.g., Neuropsychological Rehabilitation Special
however, very few studies have specifically Issue, Fleminger & Powell, 1999) and, thus, will
targeted attention problems in epilepsy patients not be addressed here. In the epilepsy literature,
(e.g., Engelberts et al., 2002a). Based on their intervention outcomes have been evaluated by
review of evidence-based cognitive remediation objective tests, subjective measures, and observer
of attention deficits in TBI and stroke patients, ratings. In addition, functional measures, such as
Cicerone et al. (2000, 2005) recommended com- QOL ratings (QOLIE, Devinsky et al., 1995),
pensation strategy training as a practice standard aimed at understanding perceptions of PWE in a
during postacute period of rehabilitation. broad sense have become the “standard of care”
Engelberts et al. (2002a) evaluated the effec- in recent years (Gilliam, 2002).
tiveness of cognitive rehabilitation for attention In cognitive remediation interventions, the
deficits in adults with focal seizures, specifically biggest treatment effects are found when
comparing the retraining and the compensation subjective patient report measures are used, and
methods in a randomized controlled study. Fifty evaluations of results with objective measures
patients with no psychiatric comorbidity, IQ over have generally been disappointing (Hendriks,
80, and receiving carbamazepine monotherapy 2001). It has also been noted that responses on
were randomly assigned to the retraining method, subjective measures may correlate poorly with
the compensation method, or the wait-list control objective findings. For example, only moderate
group. Both methods of rehabilitation consisted correlations (i.e., 0.30–0.40) were found between
11 Psychotherapeutic and Cognitive Remediation Approaches 247

self-reported memory problems and the results of Studies using appropriate, well-validated
assessments using neuropsychological tests outcome assessment instruments will give insight
(Brown, Dodrill, Clark, & Zych, 1991). When into the benefits of psychological and cognitive
objective neuropsychological measures were interventions for PWE and into the generalizabil-
used and positive results were reported, the limi- ity of intervention training to everyday life. In
tations due to practice effects, overlap between addition, long-term follow-up with these assess-
the outcome measures and the intervention, and ments would shed light on the durability of treat-
the ecological validity of most measures often ment effects over time.
were not addressed.
In psychotherapeutic interventions and those
providing psychoeducation, outcomes have been Recommendations for Educational
measured primarily by self-report ratings. and Psychotherapeutic Interventions
Positive effects of interventions were reported for PWE
when using patient ratings of disease-related
knowledge (e.g., Helgeson et al., 1990), The general recommendations for educational
perceptions of self-efficacy in managing epilepsy programs and psychotherapy provided below are
(DiIorio, Escoffery, et al., 2009), work and social based on the findings of intervention studies in
adjustment (Goldstein et al., 2003), as well as PWE. Due to the very limited number of studies
symptoms of depression (e.g., Martinovic et al., with randomized controlled methodologies thus
2006), anxiety (e.g., Davis et al., 1984), and far, definitive guidelines for practice cannot be
epilepsy-related fears (Helgeson et al., 1990). made. Given the promising results of many of the
Positive outcomes were also observed in studies interventions discussed above and the dire need
where self-reported seizure frequency and for such interventions for PWE, treatment
seizure control were the dependent measures programs that are adequately evaluated and can
(May & Pfafflin, 2002; Petersen et al., 1984). provide evidence of improved outcomes in PWE
Unfortunately, objective confirmation of are necessary.
improvement was generally lacking, with the The content of psychoeducation interventions
exception of studies investigating patient for PWE should aim to address the following
adherence to medication regimens, where drug goals:
serum levels were obtained (Helgeson et al., (a) Improve knowledge about epilepsy (e.g.,
1990; Petersen et al., 1984). etiology, types of seizures)
It is evident that in order to demonstrate (b) Improve understanding of treatment options
benefits of behavioral interventions for PWE, and possible adverse effects of treatment
selecting appropriate outcome measures is (c) Improve understanding of the importance of
critical. In cognitive remediation, standardized treatment adherence
neuropsychological measures may not necessarily (d) Improve seizure management (e.g., first
measure what is expected to change, or these aid practices, understanding precipitating
tests may not be sensitive to changes that do take factors)
place as a result of an intervention. Wilson (1997) (e) Improve awareness of safety issues (e.g., as
has pointed out that most interventions target pertains to driving)
impairments, identified by scores on tests, rather (f) Instruct in careful recording of seizures
than disabilities or manifestation of problems in and adverse events (e.g., to better guide
everyday life, yet rehabilitation is concerned with medication changes)
the treatment of disabilities. Focusing on health (g) Teach basics about brain functioning
and functional outcomes has also been and commonly experienced cognitive
recommended by others in the field (e.g., problems
Fleminger & Powell, 1999). Future studies will (h) Increase awareness of commonly experi-
need to evaluate the efficacy of these approaches. enced emotional disturbances
248 L. Nakhutina

(i) Discuss common psychosocial problems, cognitive remediation where those problems can
including perceptions of stigma be addressed more specifically. In general, it is
(j) Discuss other medical conditions (e.g., important for group leaders to be aware of
cerebrovascular disease) that are frequently patients’ cognitive problems and ensure that
comorbid with epilepsy and can impact education materials and handouts are appropriate
cognition to their level of cognitive and intellectual
(k) Improve coping skills functioning.
(l) Provide support With regard to psychotherapy, the task is to
(m) Educate family members about epilepsy and adapt an approach shown effective in RCTs in
its consequences order to meet the needs of PWE in an evidence-
(n) Provide professional referrals for patients based treatment. Extensive research has been
with severe emotional and cognitive conducted with other chronic disease patients,
problems such as HIV and diabetes, specifically evaluating
(o) Provide information about local resources behavioral interventions that promote effective
for PWE coping and examining the various mechanisms of
In addition, educational programs that coping. In contrast, there has not been similar
encompass group discussion provide patients progress in research of this area in epilepsy. In
with an opportunity to share their experiences epilepsy there has not been similar progress in
and receive feedback from others. Furthermore, research of this area. Findings from studies with
interventions that span over a longer period of other chronic disease patients can inform inter-
time allow a bond among group members to vention efforts with epilepsy patients; however,
develop, and a social network can be formed, more studies are needed to determine the applica-
which has been described by participants in pre- bility of these techniques. In the epilepsy litera-
vious studies as one of the most valuable compo- ture, among specific coping strategies, cognitive
nents of interventions (Johanson et al., 2001). restructuring and problem-solving were identi-
During sessions, group members should also be fied as strategies that appear to be associated with
given opportunities to ask questions, so that the lower levels of anxiety, depression, and other
necessary clarifications can be provided and mis- indicators of psychosocial well-being in PWE
conceptions about epilepsy and its treatment can (Bishop & Allen, 2007).
be targeted. Patients at different points in the Psychotherapy approach will likely depend on
course of having epilepsy can benefit from edu- the baseline psychological functioning, the level
cational programs; however, it is likely that more of education and intellectual functioning, as well
benefit could be derived by patients who receive as seizure variables. Among the various
education early on after receiving epilepsy diag- approaches, cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT)
nosis. Moreover, educational programs after the appears to have a significant potential for
first seizure could be valuable as a preventive alleviating epilepsy patients’ emotional distress
measure against developing epilepsy. and improving their psychosocial functioning,
Given the findings of limited efficacy of self-efficacy, and possibly seizure control.
educational programs in reducing emotional Cognitive therapy (Beck, 1976) has been studied
distress, as neither SEE (Helgeson et al., 1990) in more RCTs than any other psychosocial
nor MOSES (May & Pfafflin, 2002) showed a treatment and has been shown to provide effective
significant impact on QOL or mood symptoms, treatment for numerous clinical problems and
appropriate referrals and recommendations for disorders, including those in the context of
psychotherapy (group or individual) will need to chronic illnesses. Psychotherapy can be based on
be made, and problems of patients with severe traditional CBT approaches to the treatment of
psychopathology may require immediate depression and anxiety, combined with
attention. Similarly, patients evidencing notable intervention techniques most applicable to
cognitive problems will require referrals for patients with chronic illness in general, and with
11 Psychotherapeutic and Cognitive Remediation Approaches 249

the knowledge of issues relevant specifically to The problem-solving component of therapy


epilepsy patients. Therapy can be conducted in can be informed by interventions shown to be
individual sessions or in groups, as there is no effective in patients with other chronic illnesses,
definitive evidence in the epilepsy literature that specifically by work of Nezu and colleagues
either one format is more beneficial to patients (Nezu, Nezu, Friedman, Faddis, & Houts, 1998,
than the other. Nezu, Nezu, Felgoise, McClure, & Houts, 2003)
Cognitive behavioral therapy, while struc- conducted with cancer patients. Using the tech-
tured, allows for flexibility, whereby treatment niques they describe, aimed at lessening emo-
can be adapted to the specific needs of tional distress and improving QOL, epilepsy
PWE. Therapists working with epilepsy patients patients can be taught to define the problem, gen-
need to make sure that patients are able to erate alternatives, implement the solution, and
adequately attend, engage in, and benefit from review the outcome. Problem-solving techniques
the intervention provided, given that patients may also include teaching patients to take an over-
present with cognitive deficits, including whelming task and break it into manageable
problems with attention, executive functions, steps. A problem list can be collaboratively
memory, and mental slowing. For example, it arrived at with the epilepsy patient. Targets for
may be necessary to adjust the pace of the problem-solving with epilepsy patients can
intervention, include concrete examples, and include obtaining information about treatment
incorporate strategies, such as memory notebook options, planning and organizing to ensure AED
to ensure carryover. Memory aids and compliance, addressing treatment side effects,
informational handouts will also help patients and confronting social isolation.
when epilepsy-related psychoeducation is
provided as part of therapy.
In formulating hypotheses in treatment about Recommendations for Cognitive
factors causing and maintaining patients’ Remediation for PWE
problems, specific attention needs to be paid to
epilepsy-related issues. Cognitive restructuring The following section is intended to provide
strategies can be targeted at patients’ negative guidance to clinicians when designing and
automatic thoughts and beliefs about epilepsy, its implementing cognitive rehabilitation interven-
treatments, and themselves, as individuals who tions for PWE. It would be premature to propose
have a seizure disorder. Integrating progressive any definitive guidelines for this type of treat-
relaxation techniques is often beneficial ment, given that the cognitive remediation litera-
(Puskarich et al., 1992), particularly in patients ture as applied to epilepsy is in its infancy, with
for whom stress is a frequent precipitant of very few randomized controlled studies con-
seizures or those with other identifiable seizure ducted. The design of cognitive remediation
triggers. Other special considerations in therapy interventions for PWE can be informed from
with epilepsy patients include attention to safety multiple sources, including neuropsychology,
when introducing the idea of maximizing learning theory, interventions with demonstrated
pleasurable activities. Specific discussions of the efficacy conducted with other patient groups, and
effects of mood symptoms on ability to manage by the findings from existing interventions with
epilepsy and regular monitoring of their epilepsy patients.
functioning in this area are particularly important. As a first step, a thorough neuropsychological
Of critical importance is the evaluation of suicide assessment can guide cognitive remediation by
risks throughout treatment, given that epilepsy is providing the necessary information about the
associated with higher prevalence of suicidal nature and the severity of cognitive impairment
ideation (Tellez-Zenteno, Patten, Jette, Williams, and about the overall level of intellectual func-
& Wiebe, 2007). tioning. Detailed understanding of individual’s
250 L. Nakhutina

cognitive strengths and weaknesses and an appre- standard based on findings of studies in TBI and
ciation of his or her functional goals (e.g., return stroke (Cicerone et al., 2000, 2005).
to work or studies, participation at home) will Compensatory strategies for attention deficits can
help to identify relevant areas for rehabilitation. include modifications to the environment and the
Treatment goals can be identified collaboratively use of external aids to help track and organize
with a patient and, whenever possible, their fam- information (Sohlberg & Mateer, 2001).
ily member or significant other. These goals must Interventions targeting memory problems in
be concrete and adjusted to the specific needs of PWE can focus on memory strategy training, as
patients. It is also important to assess patient’s this approach has received the most research
level of awareness of deficits and their willing- support (Cicerone et al., 2000, 2005). In cognitive
ness to engage in the intervention, as lack of remediation for participants with mild memory
awareness is a significant barrier to remediation problems, strategies can include external and
(Prigatano, 1999). Improving patients’ under- internal compensatory aids. External memory
standing of their cognitive strengths and weak- strategies can include: (1) means to organize and
nesses and the effects of cognitive deficits in store information (e.g., calendar, diary, mobile
their daily lives can make them more likely to phone), (2) methods to remind patients to perform
engage in the intervention and learn compensa- a particular activity at a specified time in the
tory behaviors. future (e.g., pagers, alarms), and (3) environmental
While memory problems are the dominant modifications to decrease the impact of memory
complaint in the clinical practice of epilepsy care deficit on daily life.
(Hendriks, 2001) and are the key domain targeted Internal aids can include verbal strategies
by most existing interventions for PWE, attention (e.g., forming acronyms) and visual imagery
problems often underlie memory difficulties, as (e.g., constructing visual images for information
well as problems with other higher order that is to be remembered; loci method). Many of
functions. Attentional disturbances can obscure these techniques have been around for centuries,
intact cognitive abilities and constitute a major and detailed discussions of the various strategies
obstacle to rehabilitation (Ben-Yishai, Piasetsky, have been offered by many authors (e.g.,
& Rattock, 1987). Thus, in patients with primary Sohlberg & Mateer, 2001; Wilson, 1987).
deficits in attention-concentration, it is important Selection of cognitive remediation strategies for
to ameliorate these problems before proceeding patients can be guided by the findings of their
with attempts to remediate other functions, such neuropsychological evaluation. In PWE, the lat-
as memory (Ben-Yishai et al., 1987). Specific eralization and localization of the epileptogenic
techniques can include Attention Process focus are important factors to consider when
Training (APT; Sohlberg & Mateer, 1987), offering strategies. Patients with left temporal
designed to remediate and improve attentional lobe lesions have been found to be able to par-
systems, including sustained, alternating, tially compensate for their verbal memory defi-
selective, and divided attention. While this cits using visual imagery (Jones, 1974). In
technique has not been evaluated specifically in contrast, patients with right temporal lobe lesions
PWE, it has been shown to be effective in other have been observed to resort to verbal labeling in
patient populations, such as those with acquired attempts to partially compensate for their mem-
brain injury (Sohlberg et al., 2000) and cancer ory deficits for certain kinds of nonverbal
(Butler & Copeland, 2002). In epilepsy patients, material, such as unfamiliar faces (Kimura,
while both retraining and compensation methods 1963; Milner, 1968).
for attention deficits were shown to be effective, In terms of localization of seizure focus and
the use of compensatory techniques was more cognitive dysfunction, patients with deficits in
effective with patients who have low levels of executive functioning, which may be prominent
education (Engelberts et al., 2002a), and this with seizures originating in the frontal lobe, but
approach has been recommended as a practice also present in TLE and other types of epilepsy,
11 Psychotherapeutic and Cognitive Remediation Approaches 251

may not be able to initiate and apply internal aid or behaviors that are important to their daily
strategies. Depending on the extent of their needs. Sohlberg and Mateer (1989) describe
deficits, they may also require additional techniques for teaching domain-specific
assistance with organization and execution of knowledge, which include mnemonic strategies,
external procedures. Many individuals, including expanded rehearsal, and use of priming. Tasks
those with minimal executive dysfunction, considered domain specific can include
benefit from organizational strategies, including procedures for operating a computer and learning
category formation, chunking, simplifying, and one’s medication schedule, orientation
reducing the amount of information to be information, and names of people. In their
remembered. Strategies to improve problem- intervention for amnestic epilepsy patients,
solving, which are sometimes incorporated in Aldenkamp and Vermeulen (1991) employed
psychotherapeutic interventions, as discussed external memory aids and verbal mnemonic
above, can also be used to target deficits in strategies to teach an amnestic patient key phone
executive functioning. For example, Rath, Simon, numbers, enabling her to perform a job as a
Langenbahn, Sherr, and Diller (2003) described telephone operator in a sheltered workshop. Her
group treatment for problem-solving deficits for job was also organized with a fixed, step-by-step
patients with TBI and reported positive results of scheme. While the techniques employed in their
this intervention as compared to conventional intervention to confront memory problems in
group neuropsychological rehabilitation. Patients severely amnestic patients were not specific for
demonstrated improvements in problem-solving, epilepsy, given their positive results, these
as assessed by various measures, including tests techniques were deemed “sufficiently operational
of executive functions, self-appraisal ratings, and to be applicable in a clinical group such as
objective observer ratings. Future studies can epilepsy” (Aldenkamp & Vermeulen, 1991).
evaluate the efficacy of interventions for problem- In addressing cognitive deficits in PWE, the
solving in PWE. possible contribution of emotional disorder needs
Epilepsy patients with severe memory to be considered. Patients’ cognitive deficits may
impairment require external memory aids with be exacerbated by their emotional distress, and
direct application to functional activities and they may be unable to benefit from cognitive
needs. Training in the use of a compensatory remediation unless their mood symptoms are
memory notebook can be conducted according addressed. Depending on the severity of
to the procedure detailed by Sohlberg and emotional distress, decisions about an appropriate
Mateer (1989). Possible memory notebook sec- course of action and referrals will need to be
tions can include pertinent autobiographical made. Effective interventions targeting cognitive
data, calendar, to-do lists, medications list and problems require a clinician to attend to patients’
schedule, names, and journal of daily events. emotional states and provide the necessary
Thus, the notebook can include sections to support, education, and strategies for managing
record past events (i.e., retrospective memory distress.
support) and intended actions or future events The educational component, as part of
(i.e., prospective memory support). Neuropsy- cognitive remediation interventions, can be
chological evaluation results can be used to devoted to providing PWE with information
determine the level of complexity of the note- about the effects of epilepsy, its treatment,
book system. Alternatively, electronic memory commonly experienced cognitive and mood
aids may be more useful for some individuals. problems, and the effect of mood on cognition. In
Regardless of the method used, family members discussing memory, realistic expectations. About
need to understand the system and assist in possibilities for improvement in memory func-
implementing it (Sohlberg & Mateer, 1989). tioning will need to be set. Patients should also be
Patients with severe memory impairment can made aware that there is no single technique that
also be taught specific bits of information, skills, can produce positive effects on memory and that
252 L. Nakhutina

the best method is likely to differ for each indi- important when dealing with a heterogeneous
vidual. Information provided in psychoeducation patient population. Given the individual
for PWE should be accompanied by handouts to differences among cognitive profiles and
aid learning. In fact, participants often report that functional needs in epilepsy patients, there is a
being given informational handouts is one of the clear need for flexible approach in treatment.
most valuable components of an intervention This is easier to accomplish in individualized
(e.g., Barr et al., 2004). treatment rather than in groups; however,
When designing a cognitive remediation flexibility of approach is an essential consideration
intervention for PWE, decisions will need to be regardless of the format chosen.
made regarding the timing, length, and format of
an intervention. With regard to timing, it is
unclear when during the course of epilepsy Conclusion
cognitive remediation can be most effective for
patients. Questions have been raised whether Comprehensive care of epilepsy patients requires
interventions prior to surgery would be beneficial, that emotional, cognitive, and psychosocial
and at which point postsurgery interventions can problems be addressed, given their high
be used to counteract possible postsurgical prevalence in this patient population. Despite the
memory decline (Helmstaedter et al., 2008). The increased awareness of these problems, few
epilepsy literature does not yet provide definitive psychotherapeutic and cognitive remediation
answers to these questions, and well-designed treatments for PWE have been developed, and
trials of pre- versus postoperative rehabilitation these are infrequently utilized and evaluated
would be informative. With regard to the length using proper methodology. The findings of stud-
of an intervention, Aldenkamp and Vermeulen ies reviewed in this chapter suggest that behav-
(1991) found short training periods beneficial ioral interventions can reduce emotional distress
and observed a plateau in improvement after four and cognitive morbidity, increase treatment com-
or five sessions. Other interventions have been pliance, and improve psychosocial functioning
more intensive and/or spanning a number of and QOL, as well as seizure control in epilepsy
weeks (e.g., Helmstaedter et al., 2008; patients. The results are encouraging not only in
Langenbahn et al., 2008), with positive results studies of patients with refractory seizures, but
reported. This approach allows more opportunity also when psychological interventions were
for skill practice. administered to patients with well-controlled epi-
The choice of treatment format (i.e., group or lepsy (Engelberts et al., 2002a, 2002b). Thus,
individual) will likely be guided by the criteria there is a clear need for behavioral intervention
used in selecting participants. In general, the development in epilepsy and for robust and well-
group format in prior studies has been found by designed trials evaluating the various approaches.
participants to be more motivating than individual Until epilepsy-specific intervention strategies
training sessions, and participants tend to form a are available, treatment techniques supported by
supportive network (Aldenkamp, Hendriks, & research with other patient groups can be utilized.
Vermeulen, 2000). The role of social support However, epilepsy-specific factors will have to
cannot be underestimated, and in PWE it has be taken into account, and the applicability of
been found to relate to QOL, irrespective of these techniques examined. In contrast to TBI or
seizure control (Amir, Roziner, Knoll, et al., stroke where after an acute phase, patients will
1999). Interventions conducted in the form of generally improve, epilepsy is chronic, and con-
group training also have the advantages of being cerns about cognitive, psychological, and social
cost-effective. On the other hand, cognitive functioning deteriorating over time have been
remediation conducted with a patient one-on-one voiced. Given that not all patients with epilepsy
allows for more individualized work toward will necessarily show cognitive deterioration
treatment goals, which may be particularly (Hermann et al., 2008), the findings from inter-
11 Psychotherapeutic and Cognitive Remediation Approaches 253

ventions literature in TBI and stroke are likely disease severity and quality of life in patients with epi-
lepsy. Epilepsia, 40, 216–224.
applicable. In addition, findings from behavioral
Au, A., Chan, F., Li, K., Leung, P., Li, P., & Chan,
treatment studies conducted with other chronic J. (2003). Cognitive-behavioral group treatment
disease patients can also inform the treatment of program for adults with epilepsy in Hong Kong.
PWE. Epilepsy & Behavior, 4, 441–446.
Barr, W. B., Morrison, C., Isaacs, K., & Devinsky, O.
Finally, it should be noted that while the
(2004). Group treatment of memory disorders in
importance of considering cultural relevance patients with epilepsy. Epilepsia, 45(S7), 171.
when providing an intervention has been Beck, A. T. (1976). Cognitive therapy and the emotional
recognized (Gupta & Naorem, 2003), there are disorders. New York, NY: International Universities
Press.
very few reports of such interventions. More
Ben-Yishai, Y., Piasetsky, E. B., & Rattock, J. (1987). A
efforts to gain better understanding of cultural systematic method for ameliorating disorders of basic
beliefs about epilepsy and its treatment are attention. In M. J. Meyer, A. L. Benton, & L. Diller
necessary, as these beliefs influence the treatment (Eds.), Neuropsychological Rehabilitation. Edinburgh:
Churchill Livingstone.
process and outcome. In addition, better
Beyenburg, S., Mitchell, A. J., Schmidt, D., Elger, C. E., &
understanding of factors impacting QOL in Reuber, M. (2005). Anxiety in patients with epilepsy:
epilepsy patients from different cultural, ethnic, Systematic review and suggestions for clinical manage-
and socioeconomic backgrounds is needed in ment. Epilepsy & Behavior, 7, 161–171.
Bishop, M., & Allen, C. A. (2003). The impact of epilepsy
order to provide effective treatment to diverse
on quality of life: a qualitative analysis. Epilepsy and
patient groups. Behavior, 4, 226–233.
In summary, successful interventions have the Bishop, M., & Allen, C. A. (2007). Coping with epilepsy:
potential to greatly impact the quality of care for Research and interventions. In E. Martz, H. Livneh, &
B. A. Wright (Eds.), Coping with chronic illness and
PWE and could lead to improved outcomes and
disability: Theoretical, empirical, and clinical aspects
lowered disability and health-care costs. (pp. 241–266). New York, NY: Springer.
Future randomized controlled studies with Boylan, L. S., Flint, L. A., Labovitz, D. L., Jackson, S. C.,
adequate sample sizes and employing well- Starner, K., & Devinsky, O. (2004). Depression but not
seizure frequency predicts quality of life in treatment-
validated outcome assessment instruments will
resistant epilepsy. Neurology, 62, 258–261.
give insight into the benefits of behavioral Bradley, P. M., & Lindsay, B. (2008). Care delivery and self-
interventions in epilepsy and will be in the management strategies for adults with epilepsy. Cochrane
position to evaluate the empirical evidence for Database of Systematic Reviews, Issue 1, Art. No.:
CD006244. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD006244.pub2.
the recommendations made in this chapter.
Brown, F. H., Dodrill, C. B., Clark, T., & Zych, K. (1991).
An investigation of the relationship between self-
report of memory functioning and memory test
performance. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 47,
References 772–777.
Butler, R. W., & Copeland, D. R. (2002). Attentional pro-
Aldenkamp, A. P. (2006). Cognitive impairment in epi- cesses and their remediation in children treated for
lepsy: state of affairs and clinical relevance. Seizure, cancer: a literature review and the development of a
15, 219–220. therapeutic approach. Journal of International
Aldenkamp, A. P., Baker, G. A., & Meador, K. (2004). Neuropsychological Society, 8, 115–124.
The neuropsychology of epilepsy: What are the factors Cicerone, K. D., Dahlberg, C., Kalmar, K., Langenbahn,
involved? Epilepsy and Behavior, 5, S1–S2. D. M., Malec, J. F., Bergquist, T. F., et al. (2000).
Aldenkamp, A. P., Hendriks, M., & Vermeulen, J. (2000). Evidence-based cognitive rehabilitation: recommenda-
Cognitive deficits in epilepsy: is there a treatment? In tions for clinical practice. Archives of Physical Medicine
D. Schmidt & S. C. Schachter (Eds.), Epilepsy: and Rehabilitation, 81, 1596–1615.
Problem solving in clinical practice. London: Martin Cicerone, K. D., Dahlberg, C., Malec, J. F., Langenbahn,
Dunitz, Ltd. D. M., Felicetti, T., Kneipp, S., et al. (2005). Evidence-
Aldenkamp, A. P., & Vermeulen, J. (1991). based cognitive rehabilitation: updated review of the
Neuropsychological rehabilitation of memory function literature from 1998 through 2002. Archives of Physical
in epilepsy. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 3, Medicine and Rehabilitation, 86, 1681–1692.
199–214. Cramer, J. A., Perrine, K., Devinsky, O., Bryant-
Amir, M., Roziner, I., Knoll, A., et al. (1999). Self efficacy Comstock, L., Meador, K., & Hermann, B. P. (1998).
and social support as mediators in the relation between Development and cross-cultural translation of a
254 L. Nakhutina

31-item quality of life questionnaire (QOLIE-31). Engelberts, N. H. J., Klein, M., Kasteleijn-Nolst Trenite,
Epilepsia, 39, 81–88. D. G. A., Heimans, J. J., & van der Ploeg, H. M.
Cramer, J. A., Perrine, K., Devinsky, O., & Meador, K. (2002b). The effectiveness of psychological interven-
(1996). A brief questionnaire to screen for quality of tions for patients with relatively well-controlled epi-
life in epilepsy The QOLIE-10. Epilepsia, 37(6), lepsy. Epilepsy and Behavior, 3, 420–426.
577–582. Ettinger, A., Reed, M., Cramer, J., & Epilepsy Impact
Dahl, J., Melin, L., & Lund, L. (1987). Effects of a contin- Project Group. (2004). Depression and comorbidity in
gent relaxation treatment program on adults with community-based patients with epilepsy or asthma.
refractory epileptic seizures. Epilepsia, 28(2), Neurology, 63, 1008–1014.
125–132. Fasotti, L., Kovacs, F., Eling, P. A., & Brouwer, W. H.
Davis, G. R., Armstrong, H. E., Jr., Donovan, D. M., & (2000). Time pressure management as a compensatory
Temkin, N. R. (1984). Cognitive-behavioral treatment strategy training after closed head injury.
of depressed affect among epileptics: Preliminary find- Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 10, 47–65.
ings. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 40(4), 930–935. Faught, E., Duh, M. S., Weiner, J. R., Guerin, A., &
Devinsky, O., Vickrey, B. G., Cramer, J., Perrine, K., Cunnington, M. C. (2008). Nonadherence to antiepilep-
Hermann, B., Meador, K., et al. (1995). Development tic drugs and increased mortality: Findings from the
of the quality of life in epilepsy inventory. Epilepsia, RANSOM study. Neurology, 71, 1572–1578.
36, 1089–1104. Faught, E., Weiner, J. R., et al. (2009). Impact of nonad-
DiIorio, C., Escoffery, C., Yeager, K. A., McCarty, F., herence to antiepileptic drugs on health care utiliza-
Henry, T. R., Koganti, A., et al. (2009). WebEase: tion and costs: Findings from the RANSOM study.
Development of a Web-based epilepsy self- Epilepsia, 50, 501–509.
management intervention. Preventing Chronic Fleminger, S., & Powell, J. (1999). Editorial.
Disease, 6(1), 1–7. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 9, 225–230.
DiIorio, C., Osborne Shafer, P., Letz, R., et al. (2003). The Gillham, R. (1990). Refractory epilepsy: An evaluation of
association of stigma with self-management and psychological methods in out-patient management.
perceptions of health care among adults with epilepsy. Epilepsia, 31, 427–432.
Epilepsy & Behavior, 4, 259–267. Gilliam, F. G. (2002). Optimizing health outcome in
DiIorio, C., Reisinger, E. L., Yeager, K. A., & McCarty, F. active epilepsy. Neurology, 58(Suppl. 5), S9–S19.
(2009). A telephone-based self-management program Giovagnoli, A. R., & Avanzini, G. (1999). Learning
for people with epilepsy. Epilepsy & Behavior, 14, and memory impairment in patients with temporal
232–236. lobe epilepsy: Relation to the presence, type, and
DiIorio, C., & Yeager, K. (2003). The epilepsy self- location of brain lesion. Epilepsia, 40 (7),
efficacy scale. In O. L. Strickland & C. DiIorio (Eds.), 904–911.
Measurement of nursing outcomes: Self care and Goldstein, L. H., McAlpine, M., Deale, A., Toone,
coping (2nd ed., Vol. 3, pp. 40–51). New York, NY: B. K., & Mellers, J. D. C. (2003). Cognitive behav-
Springer Publishing Company. ioral therapy with adults with intractable epilepsy
Dodrill, C. (2008). Emotional and psychosocial factors in and psychiatric co-morbidity: Preliminary observa-
epilepsy. In J. E. Morgan & J. H. Ricker (Eds.), tions on changes in psychological state and seizure
Textbook of clinical neuropsychology. New York, NY: frequency. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41(4),
Taylor and Francis. 447–460.
Dodrill, C. B., Batzel, L. W., Queisser, H. R., & Temkin, Green, P., & Allen, L. (1997). Memory complaints inven-
N. (1980). An objective method for the assessment of tory. Durham, NC: CogniSyst.
psychological and social problems among epileptics. Gupta, A., & Naorem, T. (2003). Case study. Cognitive
Epilepsia, 21, 123–135. retraining in epilepsy. Brain Injury, 17(2), 161–174.
Elger, C. E., Helmstaedter, C., & Kurthen, M. (2004). Halgren, E., Stapleton, J., Domalski, T., Swartz, B. E.,
Chronic epilepsy and cognition. Lancet Neurology, Delgado-Excueta, A. V., & Walsh, G. O. (1991).
3(11), 663–672. Memory dysfunction in epilepsy: patient as a
Elixhauser, A., Kline Leidy, N., Meador, K., Means, S., & derangement of normal physiology. In D. Smith,
Willian, M. K. (1999). The relationship between D. Treiman, & M. Trumble (Eds.), Advances in
memory performance, perceived cognitive function, neurology; Neurobehavioral problems (Vol. 55).
and mood in patients with epilepsy. Epilepsy Research, New York, NY: Raven Press.
37, 13–24. Hayes, R. L., & McGrath, J. J. (2000). Cognitive rehabili-
Engel, J., Pedley, T. A., Aicardi, J., & Dichter, M. A. tation for people with schizophrenia and related condi-
(2007). Epilepsy: A comprehensive textbook. tions. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews,
Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Issue 3, Art. No.: CD000968.
Engelberts, N. H. J., Klein, M., Ader, H., Heimans, J. J., Helgeson, D. C., Mittan, R., Tan, S. Y., & Chayasirisobhon,
Kasteleijn-Nolst Trenite, D. G. A., & van der Ploeg, S. (1990). Sepulveda epilepsy education: The efficacy
H. M. (2002a). The effectiveness of cognitive rehabili- of a psychoeducational treatment program in treating
tation for attention deficits in focal seizures: A ran- medical and psychosocial aspects of epilepsy.
domized controlled study. Epilepsia, 43, 587–595. Epilepsy, 31, 75–82.
11 Psychotherapeutic and Cognitive Remediation Approaches 255

Helmstaedter, C., Kemper, B., & Elger, C. E. (1996). validation of the quality of life in epilepsy inventory –
Neuropsychological aspects of frontal lobe epilepsy. 89 (QOLIE-89). Epilepsia, 40(1), 97–106.
Neuropsychologia, 34, 399–406. Loring, D. W., Meador, K. J., & Lee, G. P. (2004).
Helmstaedter, C., Loer, B., Wohlfahrt, R., Hammen, A., Determinants of quality of life in epilepsy. Epilepsy
Saar, J., Steinhoff, B. J., et al. (2008). The effects of and Behavior, 5, 976–980.
cognitive rehabilitation on memory outcome after Lundren, T., Dahl, J., Melin, L., & Kies, B. (2006). Evaluation
temporal lobe epilepsy surgery. Epilepsy & Behavior, of acceptance and commitment therapy for drug refrac-
12, 402–409. tory epilepsy: A randomized controlled trial in South
Hendriks, M. P. H. (2001). Neuropsychological com- Africa – A pilot study. Epilepsia, 47, 2173–2179.
pensatory strategies for memory deficits in patients Martinovic, Z., Simonovi, P., & Djoki, R. (2006).
with epilepsy. In M. Pfafflin, R. T. Fraser, Preventing depression in adolescents with epilepsy.
R. Thorbecke, U. Specht, & P. Wolf (Eds.), Epilepsy & Behavior, 9, 619–624.
Comprehensive (pp. 87–94). Herts: John Libbey May, T. W., & Pfafflin, M. (2002). The efficacy of an edu-
& Company Ltd. cational treatment program for patients with epilepsy
Hermann, B. P., Seidenberg, M., & Jones, J. (2008). The (MOSES): Results of a controlled, randomized study.
neurobehavioral comorbidities of epilepsy: Can a nat- Epilepsia, 43(5), 539–549.
ural history be developed. Lancet of Neurology, 7, May, T. W., & Pfafflin, M. (2005). Psychoeducational pro-
151–160. grams for patients with epilepsy. Disease Management
Hermann, B. P., Vickrey, B., Hays, R. D., Cramer, J., and Health Outcomes, 13(3), 185–199.
Devinsky, O., Meador, K., et al. (1996). A comparison McLaughlin, D. P., Pachana, N. A., & Mcfarland, K.
of health-related quality of life in patients with (2008). Stigma, seizure frequency, and QOL: The
epilepsy, diabetes and multiple sclerosis. Epilepsy impact of epilepsy in late adulthood. Seizure, 17(3),
Research, 113–118. 281–287.
Hermann, B. P., & Whitman, S. (1984). Behavioral and Milner, B. (1968). Visual recognition and recall after right
personality correlates of epilepsy: a review, temporal-lobe excision in man. Neuropsychologia, 6,
methodological critique, and conceptual model. 191–209.
Psychological Bulletin, 95(3), 451–497. Morrell, M. J. (2002). Stigma and epilepsy. Epilepsy and
Johanson, M., Chaplin, J. E., & Wedlund, J. (2001). A Behavior, 3(6(S2)), 21–25.
holistic neurorehabilitation programme for people Nagai, Y., Goldstein, L. H., Fenwick, P. B. C., & Trimble,
with epilepsy: Theoretical approach, evaluation and M. R. (2004). Clinical efficacy of galvanic skin
long-term follow up. In M. Pfafflin, R. T. Fraser, response biofeedback training in reducing seizures in
R. Thorbecke, U. Specht, & P. Wolf (Eds.), adult epilepsy: A preliminary randomized controlled
Comprehensive care for people with epilepsy study. Epilepsy & Behavior, 3, 216–223.
(pp. 203–211). Herts: John Libbey & Company Ltd. Nakhutina, L., Gonzalez, J., Prus, N., & Grant, A. C.
Jokeit, H., & Ebner, A. (1999). Long term effects of (2009). Adherence to antiepileptic drugs and beliefs
refractory temporal lobe epilepsy on cognitive about medication among patients with epilepsy.
abilities: a cross sectional study. Journal of Epilepsia, 50, 115.
Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 67, 44–50. Nezu, A. M., Nezu, C. M., Felgoise, S. H., McClure,
Jones, M. K. (1974). Imagery as a mnemonic aid after left K. S., & Houts, P. S. (2003). Project genesis: Assessing
temporal lobectomy: contrast between material- the efficacy of problem-solving therapy for distressed
specific and generalized memory disorders. adult cancer patients. Journal of Consulting and
Neuropsychologia, 12, 21–30. Clinical Psychology, 71, 1036–1048.
Kanner, A. M. (2007). Epilepsy and mood disorders. Nezu, A. M., Nezu, C. M., Friedman, S. H., Faddis, S., &
Epilepsia, 48(Suppl. 9), 20–22. Houts, P. S. (1998). Helping cancer patients cope: A
Kimura, D. (1963). Right temporal lobe damage. Archives problem-solving approach. Washington, DC:
of Neurology, 8, 264–271. American Psychological Association.
Langenbahn, D. M., Peery, S., Rodriguez, I. A., Payton, Petersen, G. M., McLean, S., & Millingen, K. S. (1984).
G., & Dams-O’Connor, K. (2008). Neuropsychological A randomized trial of strategies to improve patient
rehabilitation of a hispanic woman and seizure compliance with anticonvulsant therapy. Epilepsia,
disorder. New York, NY: National Academy of 25, 412–417.
Neuropsychology. Presented at the Annual Conference Plohmann, A., Kappos, L., & Brunnschweiler, H.
of the National Academy of Neuropsychology. (1994). Evaluation of a computer-based attention
Lantz, D. L., & Sherman, M. B. (1988). Neuropsychological retraining program for patients with multiple sclero-
assessment of subjects with uncontrolled epilepsy: sis. Swiss Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry,
effects of EEG biofeedback training. Epilepsia, 29(2), 145, 35–36.
163–171. Ponds, R., & Hendriks, M. (2006). Cognitive rehabilita-
Leidy, N. K., Elixhauser, A., Rentz, A. M., Beach, R., tion of memory problems in patients with epilepsy.
Pellock, J., Schachter, S., et al. (1999). Telephone Seizure, 15, 267–273.
256 L. Nakhutina

Prigatano, G. P. (1999). Principles of neuropsychological Sohlberg, M., & Mateer, C. A. (1989). Training use of
rehabilitation. New York, NY: Oxford University compensatory memory books: A three stage behavioral
Press. approach. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Prigatano, G., Fordyce, D., Zeiner, H., Roueche, J., Neuropsychology, 11(6), 871–891.
Pepping, M., & Wood, B. (1984). Neuropsychological Sohlberg, M., & Mateer, C. A. (2001). Cognitive rehabili-
rehabilitation after closed head injury in young adults. tation. An integrative neuropsychological approach.
Journal of Neurology, Neurology, Neurosurgery and New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
Neuropsychiatry, 47, 505–513. Sohlberg, M., McLaughlin, K. A., Pavese, A., Heidrich,
Prus, N., Nakhutina, L., & Grant, A. C. (2009). Perceived A., & Posner, M. I. (2000). Evaluation of attention
epilepsy stigma and quality of life in epilepsy. process training and brain injury education in persons
Epilepsia, 50(Suppl 11), p219. with acquired brain injury. Journal of Clinical and
Puskarich, C. A., Whitman, S., Dell, J., Hughes, J. R., Experimental Neuropsychology, 22, 656–676.
Rosen, A. J., & Hermann, B. P. (1992). Controlled Spector, S., Tranah, A., Cull, C., & Goldstein, L. H.
examination of effects of progressive muscle (1999). Reduction in seizure frequency following a
relaxation training on seizure reduction. Epilepsia, short-term group intervention for adult with epilepsy.
33(4), 675–680. Seizure, 8(5), 297–303.
Ramaratnam, S., Baker, G. A., & Goldstein, L. H. (2008). Squire, L. R., Stark, C. E. L., & Clark, R. E. (2004). The
Psychological treatments for epilepsy. Cochrane medial temporal lobe. Annual Review of Neuroscience,
Database of Systematic Reviews, Issue 3, Art. No.: 27, 279–306.
CD002029. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD002029.pub3. Sterman, M. B. (1990). Basic concepts and clinical find-
Rath, J. F., Simon, D., Langenbahn, D. M., Sherr, R. S., & ings in the treatment of seizure disorders with EEG
Diller, L. (2003). Group treatment of problem-solving operant conditioning. Clinical Electroencephalography,
deficits in outpatients with traumatic brain injury: A 32(1), 45–55.
randomized outcome study. Neuropsychological Tan, S. Y., & Bruni, J. (1986). Cognitive behavioral ther-
Rehabilitation, 13(4), 461–488. apy with adult patients with epilepsy: A controlled
Reid, S., Specht, U., Thornbecke, R., Goecke, K., & outcome study. Epilepsia, 27(3), 225–233.
Wohlfarth, R. (2001). MOSES: An educational Tellez-Zenteno, J. F., Patten, S. B., Jette, N., Williams, J.,
program for patients with epilepsy and their relatives. & Wiebe, S. (2007). Psychiatric comorbidity in epi-
Epilepsia, 42(Suppl 3), 76–80. lepsy: A population-based analysis. Epilepsia, 48(12),
Rousseau, A., Hermann, B., & Whitman, S. (1985). 2336–2344.
Effects of progressive relaxation on epilepsy: Analysis Thapar, A., Kerr, M., & Gordon, H. (2009). Stress, anxi-
of a series of cases. Psychological Reports, 57, ety, depression, and epilepsy: Investigating the rela-
1203–1212. tionship between psychological factors and seizures.
Shaw, E. J., Stokes, T., Camosso-Stefinovic, J., Baker, R., Epilepsy & Behavior, 14, 134–140.
Baker, G. A., & Jacoby, A. (2007). A self-management Van den Broek, M. D., Downes, J., Johnson, Z., Dayus,
education for adults with epilepsy. Cochrane Database B., & Hilton, N. (2000). Evaluation of an electronic
of Systematic Reviews, Issue 2, Art. No.: CD004723. memory aid in the neuropsychological rehabilitation
doi:10.1002/14651858.CD004723.pub2. of prospective memory deficits. Brain Injury, 14(5),
Shulman, M. B., & Barr, W. B. (2002). Treatment of mem- 455–462.
ory disorders in epilepsy. Epilepsy & Behavior, 3, Wade, T. K., & Troy, J. C. (2001). Mobile phones as a new
S30–S34. memory aid: A preliminary investigation using case
Smeets, V. M., van Lierop, B. A., Vanhoutvin, J. P., studies. Brain Injury, 15(4), 305–320.
Aldenkamp, A. P., & Nijhuis, F. J. (2007). Epilepsy Wilson, B. (1987). Rehabilitation of memory. New York,
and employment: Literature review. Epilepsy & NY: Guilford Press.
Behavior, 10, 354–362. Wilson, B. (1997). Cognitive rehabilitation: How it is and
Smith, G., Ferguson, P. L., Saunders Lee, L., Wagner, how it might be. Journal of the International
J. L., Wannamaker, B. B., & Selassie, A. W. (2009). Neuropsychological Society, 3, 487–496.
Psychosocial factors associated with stigma in Wilson, B. A., Cockburn, J., & Baddeley, A. (1985). The
adults with epilepsy. Epilepsy & Behavior, 16, Rivermead Behavioral Memory Test. Thames Valley
484–490. Test Co. Gaylord, MI: National Rehabilitation
Snyder, M. (1983). Effect of relaxation on psychological Services.
functioning in persons with epilepsy. Journal of Wilson, B. A., Emslie, H. C., & Evans, J. J. (2001).
Neuroscience Nursing, 15(4), 250–254. Reducing everyday memory and planning problems
Sohlberg, M., & Mateer, C. A. (1987). Effectiveness of an by means of a paging system: a randomized control
attention training program. Journal of Clinical and crossover study. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery,
Experimental Neuropsychology, 9, 117–130. and Psychiatry, 70, 477–482.
Evaluation and Management
of Psychogenic Nonepileptic 12
Attacks

John T. Langfitt and William Watson

(e.g., brief episodes of staring or inattention,


Introduction startle or other reflexes in infants, nocturnal
myoclonus). PNEAs are further characterized by
Psychogenic nonepileptic attacks (PNEAs) are aspects of the patient’s history and behavior that
paroxysmal behaviors that look like epileptic sei- suggest a psychological cause.1 Some object to
zures but an EEG recorded during the event does the term “psychogenic,” because it implies a
not show epileptiform activity. Unlike organi- known, causal link between unobservable mental
cally based, nonepileptic events that resemble events and behaviors. Nevertheless, the term is
seizures (e.g., syncope, transient ischemic heuristically useful as long as one is clear that it
attacks), there are no physical findings that reflects evolving theories about mind-body inter-
explain the symptoms. PNEAs also are distin- actions that are supported by a large body of data.
guished from normal-range behaviors that may Clinical psychologists (including clinical neu-
be mistaken for seizures by educated observers ropsychologists) often are asked to consult on
PNEA patients seen by neurology or epilepsy
services. It is not enough to tell patients what
they do not have (i.e., ruling out epilepsy and
Dr. Langfitt, a clinical neuropsychologist, and Dr. Watson,
other causes). Patients also need to know what
a family psychologist, have evaluated and treated patients
with PNEA admitted to the Strong Epilepsy Center for 30 they do have, so they can understand it and begin
years combined. to manage it more effectively. Most neurologists
J.T. Langfitt, Ph.D., A.B.P.P. (*) do not have the time or expertise to provide psy-
Departments of Neurology and Psychiatry, University chiatric diagnosis, education, or management.
of Rochester Medical Center, 601 Elmwood Avenue, Guidelines for establishing specialized epilepsy
Box 673, Rochester, NY 14642, USA centers recommend an integrated approach to com-
Deptartment of Psychiatry and Neurology, prehensive management of PNEA that involves
School of Medicine and Dentistry,
University of Rochester Medical Center,
University of Rochester, 1
A number of terms have been used to describe these
601 Elmwood Ave, Box PSYCH,
events, including “pseudoseizures,” “psychogenic nonepi-
Rochester, NY 14642, USA
leptic seizures,” “nonepileptic attacks,” or “stress
e-mail: John_Langfitt@urmc.rochester.edu
seizures” (Benbadis, 2010; LaFrance, 2010). We prefer
W. Watson, Ph.D. the term “psychogenic nonepileptic attacks” (PNEA),
Departments of Psychiatry and Neurology, University because it clearly communicates the assumption that the
of Rochester Medical Center, 601 Elmwood Ave, events are generated from the mind and unequivocally
Box PSYCH, Rochester, NY 14642, USA distinguishes them from “seizures” (which most people
e-mail: William_Watson@URMC.Rochester.edu associate with epilepsy).

W.B. Barr and C. Morrison (eds.), Handbook on the Neuropsychology of Epilepsy, 257
Clinical Handbooks in Neuropsychology, DOI 10.1007/978-0-387-92826-5_12,
© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015
258 J.T. Langfitt and W. Watson

doctoral-level psychologists with appropriate Certain behaviors are more common in PNEA
training (Gumnit RJ, Walczak TS, National and less common in epileptic seizures (Reuber &
Association of Epilepsy Centers, 2001). Elger, 2003).These include undulating, rhythmic,
Psychologists can play a critical role in these set- asynchronous, purposeful, or goal-directed
tings by (1) formulating an underlying psychiat- movements, pelvic thrusting, side-to-side head
ric diagnosis, (2) ensuring that the psychological movements, crying, closed eyes and resistance to
etiology of the symptoms is clearly communi- opening, verbal responses, lack of cyanosis dur-
cated in terms that are easily understood, (3) pro- ing a prolonged “tonic” phase, and rapid reorien-
moting acceptance of the diagnosis and the tation postictally. These behaviors should raise
treatment plan by the patient and family, and (4) suspicions of PNEA, but they sometimes can be
providing treatment. misleading. For example, directed aggression
By virtue of their training, psychologists are during the ictus is almost always considered a
well positioned to evaluate the intra- and inter- sign of nonepileptic activity. However, in a
personal characteristics that predispose a patient recently reported case, a young man repeatedly
to develop PNEA, precipitate their onset, and made focused verbal threats to physicians that
perpetuate their occurrence. These characteris- were accompanied by profanity and “shooting”
tics also may become barriers to their under- them with his index finger. The events were asso-
standing and acceptance of the diagnosis that ciated with bifrontal epileptiform activity and
require psychotherapeutic communication skills hyperperfusion in the right lateral and orbitofron-
to overcome, skills that are central to psycholo- tal cortex on SPECT imaging (Shih, LeslieMazwi,
gists’ training. Psychologists can speak authori- Falcao, et al., 2009). One of our patients was
tatively to health-care providers, the patient, and partly responsive during his seizures and had a
the family about how psychological factors con- remote history of drug abuse. He was repeatedly
tribute to the experience of physical symptoms barred from the local emergency room and even-
in ways that many physicians cannot. tually assaulted and incarcerated by police
In this chapter, we review the diagnostic because staff assumed he was faking seizures to
approach to PNEA and their epidemiology, risk obtain drugs. He was later shown by video-EEG
factors, etiologic theories, treatments, and prog- monitoring to have frontal lobe seizures. Major
nosis. We then describe a multidisciplinary, col- injuries during PNEA are rare but not unheard of.
laborative approach for evaluating patients and One of our PNEA patients had broken an arm
presenting the diagnosis, illustrated by case during an attack. It later came out that she had
vignettes. intentionally and repeatedly broken her hand
with a hammer or bat on several occasions as a
child, in response to severe family dysfunction
Relevant Clinical Literature and abuse.
Because of the significant risk for misdiagno-
Approach to Diagnosis sis based on clinical signs, the “gold-standard”
diagnostic method for PNEA is to show absence
The physical signs of PNEA are as variable as of epileptiform discharges during the patient’s
epileptic seizures themselves. They can range typical event(s) which usually requires inpatient,
from mild tremulousness to full convulsions. video-EEG scalp monitoring. In rare cases,
Patients may remain aware, may be partially PNEAs diagnosed based on scalp monitoring
responsive, or may be unaware, unresponsive, later have been shown to be epileptic with inva-
and amnestic. Like epileptic seizures, PNEAs are sive monitoring, but all these cases had simple
difficult to diagnose just based on the history. partial or complex partial events and interictal
Physicians rarely see them and typically have to EEG abnormalities or other evidence of neuro-
rely on descriptions from untrained (and often logic involvement (Wyler, Hermann, Blumer,
distressed) witnesses. et al., 1993). Inpatient evaluation also allows a
12 Evaluation and Management of Psychogenic Nonepileptic Attacks 259

thorough medical and psychological evaluation (e.g., panic disorder, PTSD, dissociative disor-
to rule out nonepileptic, organic causes of ders). There are few studies where psychiatric
seizures and to evaluate risk factors for diagnosis has been prospectively established by
PNEA. Unfortunately, definitive diagnosis is standard psychiatric interviews. The majority of
often delayed, by an average of 7 years in one diagnoses were conversion disorder (36–63 %) or
study (Reuber, Fernandez, Bauer, et al., 2002). other somatoform disorder (16–23 %), with other
During this time, patients are exposed to side diagnoses including anxiety disorders (7–15 %)
effects of antiepilepsy drugs (AEDs), social and dissociative disorders (4–7 %) (Galimberti,
stigma, and activity restrictions, often resulting Ratti, Murelli, et al., 2003; Marchetti, Kurcgant,
in an intractable, disabled lifestyle. In our experi- Neto, et al., 2008).
ence, the longer the patient has carried an epi- PNEAs as an expression of malingering or
lepsy diagnosis, the harder it is for them to factitious disorder are rare, in our experience.2
understand and accept that the events are psycho- These diagnoses should be suspected in forensic
genic. This is particularly important in light of and other settings where there is an external
data indicating that successful recovery from incentive (injury compensation, avoidance of
PNEA is directly correlated with the patient’s major role responsibility, or culpability) or other
understanding and acceptance of the diagnosis evidence that suggests deliberate attempts to
(Carton, Thompson, & Duncan, 2003). deceive (e.g., significant inconsistencies in the
history and examination). Rarely, children have
admitted to manufacturing attacks to draw atten-
Psychiatric Diagnosis tion to ongoing abuse or neglect. Certain results
of psychometric assessment may be useful in
PNEAs are symptoms of one of a number of psy- supporting these diagnoses (see “Utility of
chiatric diagnoses. Most commonly, PNEAs are Psychometric Assessment,” below).
manifestations of an underlying somatoform dis-
order. The most recent version of psychiatric
nomenclature (DSM-V) refers to these as somatic Epidemiology
symptom disorders (American Psychiatric
Association, 2013). These patients have physical Medically unexplained symptoms are the most
symptoms that suggest a general medical condi- common symptoms in primary care settings.
tion but cannot be explained on the basis of the Prevalence ranges from 8 to 25 %, depending of
physical findings. Patients show undue worry and the severity and number of symptoms (Escobar,
concerns about their symptoms, even in the face Waitzkin, Silver, et al., 1998; Kroenke, Spitzer,
of reassurance. In severe cases, symptoms deGruy, et al., 1997). Prevalence of conversion in
become the focus of the patient’s life and may neurology clinics has been estimated at 20 %
dominate their relationships with others. (Mace & Trimble, 1996). The exact incidence
PNEAs that occur as the lone unexplained and prevalence of PNEA is not known. There are
symptom reflect a conversion disorder. When no community-based studies, no doubt, because
PNEAs are one of a number of medically unex- of the expense of obtaining video-EEG monitor-
plained symptoms, these patients should be diag- ing as a gold-standard diagnosis in a community-
nosed with somatic symptom disorder, with based sample. PNEA patients represent
different specifiers, depending on the role of pain ~10–20 % of patients referred to epilepsy centers
in the presentation, duration, and severity. and a somewhat higher percentage of monitored
Severity is based on the degree of associated
behavioral disturbance and the number and sever- 2
Of course, they may be more common than we believe.
ity of symptoms. PNEAs that appear as paroxys-
Only when the patient admits that the symptoms are vol-
mal changes in mood, sensation, or cognition untarily produced can these diagnoses be made with abso-
may be symptoms of other psychiatric diagnoses lute certainty.
260 J.T. Langfitt and W. Watson

patients. This has led to an estimated prevalence (Binzer, Stone, & Sharpe, 2004). Their family
of 2–33/100,000 (Benbadis & Hauser, 2000). members have higher levels of concerns about
PNEA usually onsets between 20 and 30 years of their own health. They perceive their families as
age, but there have been cases as young as 4 and more hostile and critical than family members of
as old as 70. Ten to 50 % of patients also have patients with epilepsy (Wood, McDaniel,
current or past epileptic seizures, making diagno- Burchfiel, et al., 1998).
sis and treatment even more difficult. Risk factors for the broader range of somato-
form disorders are also of interest, since many
PNEA patients carry these diagnoses. In a large,
Risk Factors German population-based cohort, somatoform
symptoms were associated with female gender,
The characteristics that distinguish patients with older age, lower education, lower income, and
PNEA from patients with epilepsy and healthy rural residence (Hiller, Rief, & Brahler, 2006). In
controls have been reviewed extensively (Bodde, a prospectively followed, population-based
Brooks, Baker, et al., 2009, Reuber & Elger, cohort of adolescents, the new onset of a somato-
2003). Since antiquity, PNEAs have been seen form diagnosis was predicted by female gender,
more often in women, such that they were often lower social class, substance use, anxiety or
felt to be a uniquely female problem. Modern affective disorder, and the experience of trau-
studies suggest that 75 % of patients are women matic sexual or physical threat events (Lieb,
(Lesser, 1996). PNEA patients are more likely Zimmermann, Friis, et al., 2002). Severe illness
than epilepsy patients to have other psychiatric in a parent predicted later somatoform symptoms
diagnoses, particularly personality disorders with in a population-based birth cohort (Hotopf,
borderline features (Bowman & Markand, 1996; 2002). Patients with somatoform disorder and
Reuber, Pukrop, Bauer, et al., 2004). High rates of histrionic personality disorder also are more
clinically significant depressive symptoms likely to have a first-degree relative with antiso-
(52 %), suicidal ideation (40 %), and prior suicide cial personality disorder (Lilienfeld, Van, Larntz,
attempts (20 %) have also been reported (Ettinger, et al., 1986). Somatoform patients also are more
Devinsky, Weisbrot, et al., 1999). PNEA patients susceptible to hypnotic suggestion (Roelofs,
have high levels of psychopathology, as measured Hoogduin, Keijsers, et al., 2002).
by the MMPI. The variety of profile types is felt to An important limitation of the literature on
represent the diversity of psychopathology under- PNEA risk factors is that most studies compared
lying the symptoms (Cragar, Berry, Schmitt, patients with PNEA to patients with epileptic sei-
et al., 2005; Dodrill, Wilkus, & Batzel, 1993). zures. Comparisons to psychiatric samples with-
PNEA patients are more likely to have difficulty out seizure-like events are rare. Therefore, some
describing their own emotional states or recogniz- of these risk factors may reflect risk for psycho-
ing them in others (alexithymia) (Bewley, Murphy, pathology in general and not specifically for
Mallows, et al., 2005). PNEA. In one of the few studies to include other
PNEA patients often have a past history of psychiatric patients, a history of childhood sexual
trauma and recent social stress, particularly abuse was more frequent in PNEA patients than
involving family. They report a higher frequency in inpatients with other psychiatric diagnoses
of past traumatic experiences in general (Betts & Boden, 1992). In another study, PNEA
(Rosenberg, Rosenberg, Williamson, et al., patients and patients with other somatoform dis-
2000). Reported childhood sexual or physical orders had similar rates of comorbid psychiatric
abuse is more common in PNEA (33 %) than in disorders and levels of anxiety, depression, and
epilepsy patients (9 %) (Alper, Devinsky, Perrine, anger. These rates were higher than in healthy
et al., 1993). PNEA patients experienced more controls (Mokleby, Blomhoff, Malt, et al., 2002).
adverse or traumatic events in the 12 months However, PNEA patients were more likely than
prior to diagnosis than patients with epilepsy other somatoform patients and controls to report
12 Evaluation and Management of Psychogenic Nonepileptic Attacks 261

a history of minor head injuries, have more subtle view. Both hold that sensory and motor changes
EEG abnormalities, and express higher levels of are caused by subjective, unobservable mental
hostility. processes whose neurologic basis is not speci-
fied. The psychoanalytic concepts of repression
and symbolic representation of past events in the
Etiology unconscious are not falsifiable. Modern explana-
tions of the phenomenon of PNEA rely on objec-
Pre-modern Theories: Many premodern writers tive observations, are potentially falsifiable, and
recognized that PNEAs were more common in invoke mechanisms by which sensation and
women and were associated with emotionality behavior might plausibly emerge from the central
and psychosocial stressors, but their theories to nervous system.
explain these observations were naturally con-
strained by the knowledge of the time (Temkin, Psychological Theories: Attachment theory has
1971). The Hippocratic writers saw hysterical sei- been invoked to explain observed associations
zures as a form of epilepsy. The Roman physician between over-reporting of physical symptoms and
Celsus wrote that “a disease originating from the early traumatic experiences (Lamberty, 2008).
uterus sometimes made women so weak it pros- Attachment theory holds that early experiences
trated them as in epilepsy.” Renaissance physi- with caretakers have an enduring influence on
cians explained all hysterical phenomena as the how people perceive themselves and how they
effect of vapors rising from the uterus to the brain. form relationships as adults (Bowlby, 1969).
Recognizing the interplay of constitutional and Validated measures allow researchers to classify
environmental factors, Todd, a nineteenth-century various attachment “styles” (e.g., “secure,” “anx-
physician, described a “highly excitable” hysteric ious/attached,” “anxious/avoidant,” “dismissing,”
who had been “subjected to moral, and perhaps “preoccupied,” and “fearful”) that reflect interest
physical influences also, well-calculated to keep in, and comfort with, engagement in intimate and
up that state.” Briquet theorized that hystero-epi- autonomous relationships (Ainsworth, 1967;
lepsy embodied painful emotions that prompted Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991). Preoccupied
reactions of the emotional centers of the brain. and fearfully attached persons report more physi-
Women were more prone, due to an innate emo- cal symptoms than securely attached persons
tionality that allowed them to fulfill their “noble (Ciechanowski, Walker, Katon, et al., 2002).
mission in life.” Breuer’s description of the hys- Preoccupied persons also used more health care
terical pregnancy of Anna O., “a young woman of than fearful ones, suggesting that attachment style
exceptional cultivation and talents,” played a key also influences strategies for managing symp-
role in initially convincing Freud that repression toms. In a community-based sample, persons with
of unpleasant memories and emotions (especially a history of childhood trauma were more likely to
sexual ones) led to “conversion” into physical report physical symptoms and to have an insecure
symptoms that symbolized the original trauma attachment style (Waldinger, Schulz, Barsky,
(Gay, 1988). Janet’s concept of dissociation was et al., 2006). Patients with insecure or preoccu-
one of the first purely cognitive theories. It held pied attachment styles may over-report physical
that psychosomatic symptoms reflect a narrowing symptoms because they are hypervigilant to a
or fragmenting of attention in response to extreme range of perceived threats. Over-reporting also
emotional stress. This caused symptoms ranging may elicit concerns and support that strengthen
from over-attending to subjective sensations to their attachment in key relationships.
unresponsiveness and amnesia (Janet, 1907). In a similar way, somatoform symptoms have
Despite continued heuristic value in lay and been described as a “bodily idiom of distress,” a
clinical circles, older conceptualizations of con- way to acceptably communicate emotions that
version and dissociation are limited as explana- might otherwise disrupt important relationships.
tory theories, from a strictly scientific point of In one case series, in-depth interviews revealed
262 J.T. Langfitt and W. Watson

that 13/14 conversion patients had been experi- support from the parents in a face-saving way that
encing intolerable family situations that they felt also provides relief from overwhelming demands.
unable to address openly (Griffith, Polles, & Adults also may use a compulsively compliant
Griffith, 1998). The authors described these as strategy to manage “unspeakable dilemmas”
“unspeakable dilemmas” where “family, social, where they feel they have to comply with intoler-
religious or political circumstances… have able demands and suppress their own needs out of
imposed forced choices… and consequent suffer- a fear of threatening important attachments (e.g.,
ing must remain hidden from important persons the abused spouse, the overworked single parent).
involved in the situation.” Many of the patients
were the least expressive family members during Neurobiological Theories: Attachment and social
the family interviews. They later confided that communication theories point to testable hypoth-
they presently were feeling threatened by physi- eses about how a person’s history of relationships
cal or sexual assault or were having difficulty might predispose them to develop PNEA. However,
coping with a psychiatrically disabled family they do not address a central question: how does
member. Social communication theories may the central nervous system produce such dramatic
also explain some of the many outbreaks of changes in sensation or behavior without apparent
“mass hysteria” that have occurred across cul- awareness or voluntary control? Watching some of
tures and over time (Evans & Bartholomew, these events, it is difficult to imagine that they are
2009). For example, episodes of mass hysteria in not done on purpose, yet patients’ protestations
Malaysia were virtually unheard of prior to the typically seem credible. There also are so many
imposition of strict Islamic codes of female other less painful symptoms that would have a
behavior in 1960. Subsequent episodes often similar effect. Updating the concept of dissocia-
involved female students or sweatshop workers tion, Brown (2004) has proposed that medically
and were accompanied by outspoken criticism of unexplained symptoms reflect disruption of nor-
authorities. Interpreting them as signs of a physi- mal attention systems. These systems shape the
cal problem provided a culturally acceptable contents of consciousness and control behavior via
explanation for an otherwise intolerable breach spreading activation of hierarchically organized,
of the social order (Bartholomew, 1997). neural networks. According to one such model,
Attachment and social communication theories primary attention systems (PAS) govern sensory
have been combined to explain conversion symp- and motor representations instantiated in neural
toms presenting in “good” children in the absence networks that are activated rapidly and efficiently
of the usual risk factors (e.g., abuse, overt stress- (e.g., sensory perception, skilled motor move-
ors, family dysfunction) (Kozlowska, 2001). ments, implicit memory) (Norman & Shallice,
These children often do very well in school, are 1986). Behaviors activated by PAS are typically
admired by peers, and generally lack recognizable experienced as occurring without conscious effort
psychopathology. Their families tend to be “super- (e.g., walking, some aspects of driving), while per-
normal” and expect a high degree of academic and ceptions and thoughts are experienced as intuitive,
athletic achievement, social propriety, and family self-evident, or automatic (e.g., simple visual dis-
cohesion. Providers tend to find these parents crimination, “realizing” that you forgot to put out the
mildly intimidating. According to Kozlowska, trash). Secondary attention systems (SAS) activate
children become “compulsively compliant” in sensory, cognitive, and motor representations that
order to bolster insecure attachments in the face of subserve higher-level, goal-directed behaviors. These
mounting expectations. They anticipate parents’ are experienced as effortful and associated with feel-
needs and expectations. They focus on performing ings of conscious volition and self-awareness.
tasks instead of attending to relationships. In this Within this framework, medically unexplained
setting, conversion symptoms are seen as passive symptoms reflect inappropriate PAS activation of
resistance that secures attachment by avoiding chronically overactive “rogue representations.”
conflict. Symptoms elicit functional and emotional These have been acquired during prior experiences
12 Evaluation and Management of Psychogenic Nonepileptic Attacks 263

(e.g., trauma) or strengthened by chronic over- imaging studies, patients with conversion paraly-
attention to bodily sensations (e.g., in somatically sis show decreased activity in primary motor
focused persons). In the sensory realm, inappro- areas and increased activity in functionally
priate activation of “rogue” representations ampli- related, inhibitory areas that also are involved in
fies normal sensory inputs (e.g., psychogenic pain attention networks (Halligan, Athwal, Oakley,
or discomfort) or suppresses them (e.g., psycho- et al., 2000; Marshall, Halligan, Fink, et al.,
genic anesthesia or amnesia). These experiences 1997). In studies of conversion anesthesia,
feel “real” to the person but are inconsistent with peripheral stimulation that was not perceived
sensory inputs (i.e., medically unexplained). resulted in deactivation of contralateral, primary
Activation of “rogue” motor representations by sensory cortex and corresponding activation of
PAS produces behaviors that are inappropriate to limbic structures (Mailis-Gagnon, Giannoylis,
the goals subserved by representations activated Downar, et al., 2003; Vuilleumier, 2005). Brown’s
by SAS. Behaviors are experienced as “real” but attentional theory and these neuroimaging find-
involuntary and outside awareness (e.g., PNEA). ings are far from definitive, but they do provide a
Brown’s theory extends more modern theories neurologically plausible framework for testing
of dissociative behavior (such as occurs during hypotheses about the neural correlates of abnor-
hypnosis) that posit an “amnesic barrier” between mal perceptual processing in these patients.
executive systems and lower-level perceptual and
motor systems (Hilgard, 1977). Evidence that an Multi-factorial Models: Recently, two groups
amnesic barrier can be affected by suggestion in have proposed similar multifactorial models to
PNEA comes from a study of patients with ictal explain the wide range of risk factors for PNEA
amnesia. Under hypnotic suggestion designed to (Bodde et al., 2009; Reuber, 2009). Each model
facilitate recall, 85 % of PNEA patients, but no suggests that PNEAs arise from a complex
epilepsy patients, were able to recall what had interplay of predisposing, precipitating, and per-
occurred during a prior event (Kuyk, Spinhoven, petuating factors that varies from patient to
& van Dyck, 1999). The idea that PNEA patients patient. Predisposing factors include constitu-
have aberrant, unconscious attention mechanisms tional factors (e.g., genetics, temperament, learn-
is supported by a recent study where subjects ing disabilities, subtle structural brain
were asked to view and rapidly name colors abnormalities) and early experiences (e.g., abuse,
under interference conditions, similar to the neglect, other sources of insecure attachment).
Stroop task (Bakvis, Roelofs, Kuyk, et al., 2009). Later exposure to stressful experiences (e.g.,
The colors were preceded by a picture of a face actual or recalled trauma, actual or perceived
that was flashed for 14 ms and backward masked threats, unspeakable dilemmas, or physical ill-
in order to prevent conscious processing. PNEA ness) precipitates the onset of events. Events are
patients were slower than healthy controls to perpetuated by distress over social stigma, isola-
name colors when the colors were preceded by tion, dependency and diagnostic uncertainty, and
angry faces, but not by happy ones. This suggests by factors that reinforce the “sick role” (e.g.,
that PNEA patients have a preconscious atten- increased family concern or support, avoidance
tional bias to threatening stimuli that places of conflicts or burdensome responsibilities, dis-
demands on information processing and affects ability benefits). The main advantage of these
subsequent behavior. Interestingly, PNEA models is that they account for (1) the heteroge-
patients with a greater attentional bias were more neity in the history and presentation of PNEA
likely to report a history of sexual trauma and to patients, (2) no single risk factor being either nec-
have higher basal cortisol levels, indicating HPA essary or sufficient to cause PNEA, and (3)
axis dysfunction (Bakvis, Spinhoven, & Roelofs, PNEA risk factors being risk factors for many
2009). Whether these findings are specific to other psychiatric conditions. They provide a
PNEA or are seen with other psychiatric disor- clinically useful framework for thinking about
ders remains to be determined. In functional how the many facets of an individual patient’s
264 J.T. Langfitt and W. Watson

constitution and experience may interact to pro- other hand, definitive diagnosis of PNEA has
duce PNEA. They remain somewhat limited from been associated with an 85 % reduction in health-
a scientific standpoint. Predisposing, precipitat- care costs in the following 6 months, including
ing, and perpetuating factors are quite common virtual elimination of emergency room visits
and how some factors (and not others) interact to (Martin, Gilliam, Kilgore, et al., 1998). Outcome
produce PNEA is not highly specified. Therefore, in children appears to be more favorable, with
almost any set of factors can be retrospectively 80 % in remission at three years in one study
invoked to explain PNEA in a given patient. They (Wyllie, Friedman, Luders, et al., 1991).
also do not propose mechanisms that explain how Others have reviewed a wide range of patient
PNEAs are experienced as “real” by the patient characteristics that has been associated with prog-
yet involuntary and outside awareness. nosis (Bodde et al., 2009, Reuber et al., 2003).
Favorable outcomes are associated with female
Summary: It is apparent at this point that no single gender, younger age, higher intelligence and
theory or model sufficiently explains how biologi- socioeconomic status, living independently, mild
cal vulnerabilities, environmental stressors, and or no other psychiatric history, an identifiable psy-
psychological and physiological processes give chological precipitant to the onset of the illness,
rise to PNEA. On the other hand, it is clear that shorter duration of illness, milder or less dramatic
many PNEA patients experience early environ- attacks, no ongoing use of AEDs, and lower scores
ments that make it very difficult for them to man- on personality measures reflecting inhibitedness,
age stress in their relationships later in life. They emotional dysregulation, and compulsiveness.
may also have aberrant cognitive and physiologi- There have been few studies of the effect of psy-
cal functioning that plausibly play a role in creat- chiatric diagnosis on outcome. It is our experience
ing changes in sensation and behavior that they that patients with conversion disorder do better
experience as abnormal, unconscious, and invol- that those with other somatoform disorders.
untary. The neurologic basis for this remains Conversion patients often are more recently diag-
unclear. It is perhaps unreasonable to expect that nosed and have fewer unexplained symptoms. For
any one theory could explain the wide variety of patients with somatic symptom disorder, the
PNEA behaviors and their causes. As we shall attacks often are the latest in a series of medically
see, the clinical challenge is to engage the patient unexplained symptoms going back many years.
in examining their own history and present cir-
cumstances so that they can develop a personal-
ized, plausible theory that leads to cognitive and Treatment
behavioral strategies that make their events stop.
A number of treatment modalities for somatoform
disorders in general have been studied with appro-
Prognosis priate research designs (Kroenke, 2007), but there
have been very few well-controlled studies of
In some cases, simply making the diagnosis leads treatment specifically for PNEA. A 2006 National
to an abrupt cessation of events (Farias, Thieman, Institutes of Health symposium concluded that
& Alsaadi, 2003), but there are few well-controlled the field had “not progressed much beyond anec-
studies of long-term outcome. Remission rates in dotal reports of treatments… and no blinded, ran-
case series vary widely (Bodde et al., 2009; domized, controlled trials of treatment for (PNEA)
Reuber, Pukrop, Bauer, et al., 2003). Follow-up have been completed” (LaFrance, Alper, Babcock,
studies vary greatly in sample sizes, duration, and et al., 2006). A Cochrane systematic review of
completeness of follow-up and treatments. Most non-pharmacologic treatment identified 23 treat-
studies report long-term remission rates <50 %, ment studies, only three of which were blinded,
and relapse is common if patients are followed randomized, controlled trials (RCTs) (Brooks,
long enough. Studies of other outcomes suggest Goodfellow, Bodde, et al., 2007). Hypnosis in
that psychosocial disability often persists. On the general conversion patients did not provide any
12 Evaluation and Management of Psychogenic Nonepileptic Attacks 265

symptomatic benefit over and above an intensive closely with the inpatient treatment team on a
inpatient behavioral program but was superior to daily basis. At the recent NIH symposium, par-
being on a treatment waiting list when used in an ticipants could not agree on whether a psycholo-
outpatient setting (Moene, Spinhoven, Hoogduin, gist or psychiatrist needed to be present when the
et al., 2002, 2003). Outcomes for PNEA patients diagnosis was delivered.
were not reported separately. After twice-daily The process described below presumes a high
sessions of paradoxical intention (imagining degree of psychologist integration with the inpa-
anxiety-provoking experiences) during a 3-week tient treatment team and engagement with family
inpatient stay versus outpatient treatment with members. Our level 4, comprehensive epilepsy
diazepam, 93 % of the paradoxical intention center supports 1.8 full-time equivalent mental
group and 60 % of the diazepam group were in health providers. A companion (typically a fam-
remission in the final two weeks of the study ily member) is usually present during most of the
(Ataoglu, Ozcetin, Icmeli, et al., 2003). monitoring session to identify typical events. The
More recently, a double-blind, placebo- psychologist often interviews the patient along
controlled RCT of sertraline showed no significant with the family member, almost always helps
effect of treatment on the primary outcome measure deliver the diagnosis, and may later provide out-
(relative change in attack rates), but treated patients patient treatment. Such integration and access to
experienced a 45 % reduction in attack frequency, family may not be typical, but we believe that
compared to an 8 % increase in the placebo group many features of this process can be replicated in
(LaFrance, Keitner, Papandonatos, et al., 2010). less integrated settings with adequate support of
There were no group differences in secondary out- mental health services, careful triage, and timely
comes (depression, anxiety, impulsivity, somatic communication among team members.
symptoms, quality of life, and psychosocial func-
tioning). In an RCT of supportive treatment (ST)
versus supportive treatment plus cognitive behav- Diagnostic Interview
ioral therapy (CBT) for 12 weeks, median monthly
attack frequency in the CBT group had declined at The goals of the diagnostic interview are to docu-
6-month follow-up from 12.0 to 1.5, compared to ment risk factors that would positively support
8.0 to 5.0 in the ST-only group (p < 0.0001) the PNEA diagnosis and to identify any underly-
(Goldstein, Chalder, Chigwedere, et al., 2010). ing psychiatric diagnosis in order to make appro-
There were no significant effects on secondary out- priate treatment recommendations. It is assumed
comes. In a pilot RCT, attack frequency in patients that the reader is well practiced in diagnostic
treated with CBT (with or without sertraline) interviewing and how to formulate a psychiatric
declined by 50–60 % over 4 months (LaFrance Jr, diagnosis. We focus here on parts of the interview
Baird, Barry, et al., 2014). Some functional improve- that are particularly important in identifying risk
ments were apparent. Neither the sertraline-only factors and facilitating the patient’s understand-
group nor the treatment as usual group showed sig- ing and acceptance of the PNEA diagnosis.
nificant benefit. By the time patients are referred for definitive
diagnosis, many have been dismissed (often by a
harried first responder or emergency room physi-
Description of the Procedure cian) as having “pseudoseizures” that are “faked”
or “all in their head.” Encountering a psycholo-
The role of the psychologist in evaluating and gist often makes them even more defensive,
managing the PNEA patient varies from center to uncooperative, and angry. The first task is to
center, depending on how closely the psycholo- model an open-minded, non-blaming attitude
gist is integrated with the medical team. Some that lowers defensiveness and sets the stage for
consult primarily on an outpatient basis after the exploring links between the events and risk fac-
PNEA diagnosis has been made. Others work tors that emerge in the interview. In our experience,
266 J.T. Langfitt and W. Watson

this is easier to accomplish when the interview ily dysfunction, somatizing tendencies, or per-
precedes delivery of the PNEA diagnosis.3 At our sonality disturbance often emerge in the course
center, patients are flagged for psychological of asking about past and current medical prob-
evaluation early in the admission if PNEA is sus- lems and support systems, including family.
pected, based on a preadmission screening of his- Abuse history may emerge naturally from a dis-
tory and symptoms. Patients who are wary about cussion of family relationships. If not, we ask
meeting with a psychologist are told that “sei- about it matter-of-factly as one of a number of
zures” (or whatever term they have been using) routine stress-related, “exposure” questions (e.g.,
are known to cause significant stress and can also family history of substance abuse, psychiatric ill-
be provoked by stressors. The role of the psy- ness, significant traumatic experiences, etc.) that
chologist is to understand how stress may be normally might influence how they have learned
playing a role in their events so we can help them to manage stress.
to stop or at least manage them better. Statements When asked directly, patients will often deny
such as “no one thinks you are crazy,” accompa- any awareness of stressors around the onset of the
nied by self-deprecatory humor about “shrinks,” events. However, persistent questioning with
can be effective in anticipating and defusing careful attention to chronology will very often
defensiveness. If they raise the question of a reveal a temporal relationship to other important
PNEA diagnosis, we ask what ideas they have life events. Patients may not initially connect
about that question and what they have heard these events to the onset of the illness. They may
from others. We also acknowledge that PNEA be resistant to the idea that events could be stress
is one of the diagnoses that has to be consid- related in the first place. Life events that would
ered, but we do not yet know for sure. It remains strike others as quite stressful have come to seem
to be determined by gold-standard, objective “normal” to them. They may have difficulty rec-
analysis of ictal, video-EEG data. When PNEA ognizing and articulating feelings associated with
seems very likely or they seem particularly these events (alexithymia).
defensive, we may educate them about the diag- Including family members in the interview is
nosis in the abstract, using a normalizing and extremely useful when assessing risk factors. It
non-blaming approach (see “Delivering the also can promote understanding and acceptance
Diagnosis,” below). This general approach low- of a PNEA diagnosis. Family often provides crit-
ers initial defensiveness in six ways: it normal- ical details about the patient’s history and about
izes and destigmatizes the mental health the circumstances surrounding the onset of the
encounter; it models open-mindedness about attacks and triggers that the patient may omit or
the diagnostic process; it reinforces reliance on may not be aware of. Seeing patients interact
objective data; it provides a more positive view with family provides important insights into the
of what remains a potential (and thus less patient’s personality, attachment style, how the
threatening) diagnosis; and it gives them time family responds to illness behavior (e.g., hyper-
to mull over information raised in the interview vigilant, overly solicitous, blaming/rejecting),
while waiting to have an event. and other predisposing or perpetuating factors.
Some PNEA risk factors (e.g., learning dis- This is also a chance to develop an alliance with
ability, psychiatric diagnoses, substance abuse family that sets the stage for accepting the diag-
history) can be elicited by routine history taking. nosis when it is given. Families typically feel
Others require a more indirect approach because anxious and helpless in supporting the patient
of their sensitive nature. Information about fam- cope with terrifying symptoms, conflicting or
confusing diagnoses, and ineffective treatments.
Attentive listening to their observations, fears,
3
It is also more appropriate if one accepts that making the
and concerns can lower the anxiety and defen-
PNEA diagnosis requires positive evidence of psycho-
genic risk factors, not just absence of ictal epileptiform siveness of the whole family system and foster
activity. trust in the medical team.
12 Evaluation and Management of Psychogenic Nonepileptic Attacks 267

A systematic review of risk factors allows the mendations is based on a published protocol
psychologist to formulate a theory about which (Shen, Bowman, & Markand, 1990) that has been
predisposing, precipitating, and perpetuating fac- modified to enhance understanding and accep-
tors may be operating in a particular case, which tance (Watson & Langfitt, 2010).
has implications for treatment. However, patients After the monitoring and diagnostic interview
and families are hardly passive participants. Most have been completed, the neurologist and psy-
have entertained (perhaps reluctantly) the hypoth- chologist jointly present the PNEA diagnosis to
esis that their events are stress related. Taking the patient (and a family member, if possible),
them through a systematic, stress-related “review typically on the day of discharge. It may be dif-
of systems” in an open-minded, nonjudgmental ficult logistically to get all these people together
manner prior to making the diagnosis allows at the same time, but it has important advantages.
them the time, space, and data to evaluate this It ensures an unambiguous, consistent, and data-
hypothesis more carefully on their own. Indeed, based message. It limits misunderstanding that
it is not uncommon following the interview for can occur when dealing with somatoform ill-
patients to spontaneously conclude that their nesses and keeps patients, family, and providers
events are psychogenic, even before an event has all “on the same page” (Harden, Burgut, &
been recorded. Kanner, 2003). Questions requiring medical and
psychological expertise can be answered together.
The neurologist validates and supports the psy-
Utility of Psychometric Testing chologist’s input and vice versa. Their mutual
presence literally embodies the holistic mind-
Unlike other areas of clinical neuropsychology, body framework that is necessary to understand
psychometric testing is less central to the diagno- and treat the symptoms.
sis and management of PNEA. Psychometric The neurologist first presents the medical data
indices of psychopathology have shown differ- and explains that biomedical causes can be defin-
ences between PNEA and epilepsy patients itively ruled out. To lower anxiety and defensive-
(Dodrill et al., 1993). However, specificity and ness, we present the diagnosis as the good news
sensitivity are insufficient for diagnostic pur- that it truly is (e.g., there is no evidence for brain
poses (Hermann, 1993). In any event, psycho- dysfunction, no need for AEDs or emergency
metric diagnosis of the events is largely redundant treatment, a low risk of injury). The attacks are a
when video-EEG data are available. Personality way that emotional stress is manifesting physi-
measures may be useful in informing manage- cally, but they are nevertheless “real” and not
ment and treatment. PNEA patients have shown being “faked.” They are a serious health problem
inconsistent effort on symptom validity testing that requires careful attention and treatment.
(Drane, Williamson, Stroup, et al., 2006). Below- Attacks are normalized by noting how often
chance scores indicate malingered cognitive PNEA diagnoses are made on the monitoring
complaints and should raise the strong suspicion unit. Patients often are reassured that they do not
that the patient’s PNEA may represent malinger- have a rare condition that no one has ever
ing as well. heard of.
At this point, patients typically ask “If it is not
epilepsy, what is it?” After providing a brief
Reporting the Findings overview of PNEA, the neurologist then “hands
off” the presentation to the psychologist, who
Making an accurate diagnosis is not useful if the elaborates further on common ways that emo-
patient and family do not understand and accept tions normally are unconsciously and involun-
it sufficiently to follow the appropriate treatment tarily embodied in physical responses (e.g.,
plan. The following multidisciplinary approach smiling when seeing a friend, blushing when
to presenting the diagnosis and treatment recom- embarrassed, nausea and palpitations before
268 J.T. Langfitt and W. Watson

speaking in public) (Watson, 2007). PNEAs are Duncan, 2003). Patients with persisting events
described as a more extreme version of this nor- were more likely to have been confused or angry
mal human tendency. Patients often express about the diagnosis.
amazement or skepticism that emotions could
produce such dramatic physical symptoms and
confusion about why this would happen to them. Case Vignettes
If they do, we share their amazement, acknowl-
edging our (and the field’s) limited understanding The following cases illustrate some of the differ-
of the precise cognitive and physiological mecha- ent ways that predisposing, precipitating, and
nisms that cause the attacks. Nevertheless, we perpetuating factors combine to produce
confidently fall back on the empirical data (results PNEA. They also include variations in the way
of the video-EEG monitoring, the risk factors the diagnosis was communicated that take these
elicited in the diagnostic interview) and our many individual differences into account. Many nones-
years of experience in making the diagnosis. If sential details have been changed to obscure
they express confusion about why this would patients’ identities.
happen to them, we then discuss generally, and in
lay terms, some of the modern etiologic theories Case 1: A 42-year-old woman with a long history
and mechanisms that have been proposed (as dis- of neck pain and a number of other medically
cussed above). We invite them to consider unexplained symptoms was referred for daily
whether any of these mechanisms might apply to “staring spells” lasting 6–8 min, during which
them. We are careful not to impose our own ideas she was aware but unable to respond. She had had
about specific predisposing, precipitating, and them “off and on for years,” but they had greatly
perpetuating factors that may be active in their increased in frequency in the past several months,
case. By openly admitting the limits of our prompting evaluation.
knowledge and by not imposing our own expla- She was the oldest of nine children of a
nation, we invite them to explore collaboratively severely mentally ill, largely absent mother and
how this process may play out in their own lives. an alcoholic father. The patient was effectively
By having them actively “connect the dots” the primary caregiver for her siblings at an early
themselves rather than having a (potentially age. A neighbor tried to rape her at age 8. She
inapt) explanation imposed by the “expert,” we became pregnant at age 17 and married her boy-
encourage them to come to an understanding that friend to escape her home. He soon became phys-
feels more like their own. This enhances under- ically abusive. Her staring spells began during
standing and acceptance of the diagnosis and this marriage as did a host of vague somatic com-
willingness to pursue psychological treatment. plaints that led to multiple surgeries. Spells were
Patients certainly vary in how they receive this happening 2–3 times per week but decreased in
information. While most express relief and under- frequency to a few times per year when her hus-
standing, others remain confused and resistant. band was deployed overseas. They recurred sev-
They may say that they understand and accept the eral years later when her teenaged daughter
diagnosis, but their body language implies other- revealed that she had been sexually abused as a
wise. We are not aware of specific empirical evi- child by the patient’s nephew. In the past year, her
dence that this approach is superior to others or daughter had become unexpectedly pregnant and
affects outcome. Anecdotally, we find that patients moved in with her boyfriend.
are more confused about the diagnosis at follow- The patient worked as a pharmacy technician.
up when the psychologist was not present at the She had recently been out on disability following
discharge discussion. In one follow-up study, only a complicated cholecystectomy for vague abdom-
one-third of PNEA patients interviewed demon- inal symptoms. Over the past several months, she
strated an understanding of the psychological had been taking night classes toward becoming a
basis of their attacks (Carton, Thompson, & pharmacist. Her current boyfriend, who was
12 Evaluation and Management of Psychogenic Nonepileptic Attacks 269

present during the interview, reminded her that emotional and material support. Dissociative epi-
she had also been caring for a number of ill rela- sodes were precipitated by being in an intolerably
tives over the past several months and sleeping insecure and threatening relationship. Frequency
and eating poorly and that her ex-husband was decreased when this threat was reduced. Attacks
due to be discharged from the military soon. increased when this threat reemerged symboli-
During the interview, the patient acknowl- cally with her daughter’s revelation of prior
edged her busy, difficult life but indicated that it sexual abuse and again in anticipation of her abu-
was “nothing I haven’t handled before.” Her sive ex-husband’s discharge from the service.
approach was to “just deal with it.” She was frus- Episodes were perpetuated by ever-increasing
trated at her daughter’s poor choices, depen- emotional and physical demands, stemming from
dence, and immaturity because the patient was her tendency to compulsively over-function to
doing everything for her to ensure that “she secure relationship needs, a tendency acquired in
doesn’t have to live the life I lived.” She also childhood as a survival strategy. She also was
reported having been diagnosed 15 years ago as unable to set appropriate boundaries around her
“bipolar,” based on a history of extreme mood daughter’s dependence, out of a fear of replicat-
lability and self-cutting. She was currently being ing her own mother’s failure as a parent. When
followed by a counselor and taking an SSRI. her over-functioning was framed as a more posi-
The patient’s attacks were diagnosed as dis- tive (albeit maladaptive) attribute, she was able to
sociative episodes. She was resistant to the diag- experience less shame, which reduced her defen-
nosis initially because she did not feel her life siveness around the diagnosis and the need to
was any more stressful than usual. She was more make changes. Although there were fewer
accepting when it was framed more positively as PNEAs at follow-up, prognosis remained guarded
a physical sign of stress that can emerge in very because of the long history of other somatoform
active, caring persons who are so focused on the symptoms and the entrenched behaviors and
needs and concerns of others that they neglect social relationships that perpetuated them.
their own. She resonated with the idea that there
often was no one to “take care of the caretaker.” Case 2: A 41-year-old warehouse manager with a
She would need to learn how to do a better job of strong family history of cardiac disease had a his-
this herself by pacing her activities and more tory of cardiac symptoms that had been in remis-
assertively setting limits on what she allowed sion without treatment for the past 5 years. He
others to expect from her. She returned to her suffered a severe and unexpected episode of chest
therapist. Three months later, episodes had pain that required hospitalization. Work-up was
decreased from daily to about once a week. She unrevealing. Shortly after discharge, he was
was being somewhat more assertive with her found by his wife, confused with intermittent
daughter, but her other somatic complaints and left-sided weakness. Work-up was again nega-
mood lability persisted. tive. He had three similar episodes in the follow-
ing week but nothing further. He reluctantly went
Comment: Predisposing factors in this case on short-term disability and spent much time at
appear to be borderline personality traits reflect- home, ruminating about his health. He did not see
ing an insecure or chaotic attachment style and a cardiologist until a month later, when a number
hypersensitivity to emotional and physical threats of diagnostic possibilities were raised and inva-
due to her early environment and first marriage. sive tests were recommended. The next day, he
This may have left her prone to view vague phys- had episodes of left upper extremity numbness
ical symptoms as threats of illness, accounting that migrated to the right side. Physicians at a
for her many surgeries and diagnoses. Early rela- local hospital gave conflicting diagnoses, so the
tionships provided poor models for asserting her family requested an urgent transfer. By this time,
needs within a trusting relationship, such that events involved all four extremities and loss of
physical symptoms became the safest and most awareness and were happening multiple times
reliable way to express distress and secure per day.
270 J.T. Langfitt and W. Watson

The diagnostic interview revealed no history onset of organic symptoms that initially sug-
of abuse or trauma and no significant family con- gested a very dire prognosis but ultimately turned
flicts. His wife seemed appropriately concerned out to be more benign. They became highly anx-
and supportive. He had a history of significant ious because they lacked adequate knowledge,
cocaine abuse in his teens and early 20s but had experience, or coping skills to manage health cri-
undergone rehabilitation. He had been abstinent ses. Anxiety was exacerbated by rumination
and functioning well at work for the past nine during enforced periods of inactivity, lack of
years. He had close, healthy relationships with access to normal emotional outlets, and lack of
his family and co-workers. He denied any psychi- confidence in providers. Referring to this as
atric symptoms or treatment. He related well dur- “Humpty-Dumpty syndrome,” Ford emphasized
ing the interview, and there was no suggestion of these patients’ fragile ability to cope with a health
personality disturbance. He acknowledged being crisis and how difficult it can be to “put the pieces
very anxious about the shaking and confusional back together,” once symptoms have set in.
episodes and about his general health since the Fortunately, our patient had no psychopathology,
episode of chest pain. a strong work ethic, and good family support,
When given the diagnosis of acute conversion, such that a simple, reassuring message that the
he was skeptical that the events were related to events did not reflect organic disease led them to
stress but was relieved to hear that his neurologic resolve, and he returned to his normal lifestyle.
work-up continued to be entirely negative. It was
suggested that his events could spontaneously Case 3: A 25-year-old woman with mild cerebral
remit with the relief that such knowledge palsy had been experiencing episodes in the prior
afforded, as we had seen happen on numerous 3 months that were felt to be non-electrical by her
occasions with other patients. At one-month fol- neurologist, based on their semiology and serial
low-up, he had had no further events, was back at negative, interictal EEGs. She was encouraged to
work, and had resumed all regular activities. He seek mental health care. Local counselors could
still wondered about the nature of the original find no evidence of psychological disturbance
presenting event, but he was satisfied that thor- and told her that something medical was being
ough cardiac and neurologic work-ups had been missed. She was brought to the emergency
negative and that he had had no further department after she presented to her neurolo-
symptoms. gist’s office with total amnesia for recent and
remote events in her life, including the names of
Comment: Many of the typical predisposing fac- family members, the type of work she did, where
tors (history of abuse, family conflicts, personal she had grown up, and any specific facts about
psychopathology) are absent in this case. Instead, her early life. History was therefore obtained
the history of early cocaine abuse suggests a pre- from her sister.
disposition to manage stress maladaptively. His She was the younger of two daughters. Her
personal and extensive family cardiac history older sister was an accomplished attorney in a
sensitized him to health-related threats. The tim- high-profile, local law firm. Her parents were
ing of the events strongly suggest that they were professionals who traveled extensively for work.
precipitated by the unexpected chest pain and Her father was described as somewhat emotion-
resurgent anxiety about his long-term prognosis. ally distant. Nevertheless, the sister described the
This was perpetuated when physicians raised the family as close, loving, and supportive. There
specter of additional cardiac diagnoses and was no personal or family history of abuse and
openly disagreed about them, undermining his psychiatric or behavioral issues. The patient had
trust in the system. worked extremely hard to overcome physical and
Ford (1977) described a case series of action- learning disabilities, but this left her little time to
oriented, previously healthy, medically and emo- develop friendships. After college graduation,
tionally unsophisticated men who developed she moved into the city to be near her sister and
conversion symptoms in the context of an acute found a job in retail but had difficulty making
12 Evaluation and Management of Psychogenic Nonepileptic Attacks 271

friends and struggled to keep jobs. Her convul- felt unable to rely on her family for support, out
sive events began shortly after moving. She had of fear of disappointing them. Her events resolved
not told her family about her struggles in her with (a) confidently worded reassurance of her
work or her symptoms. physical and mental health, (b) the suggestion
During the interview, she sat passively, that there were workable and less stressful alter-
answered few questions, and appeared fearful natives to her compulsively compliant style, and
and confused. When asked why she seemed so (c) giving her an immediate incentive to get
afraid, she whispered that she did not understand better, as she would have viewed becoming
why she was so sick and she was afraid that she dependent as a failure.
would never get better and would never be able to
resume living independently. She was gently but
firmly told that her body was physically healthy Summary
but that the events she was experiencing repre-
sented a severe stress response. The symptoms PNEAs are commonly encountered in epilepsy
were very likely to respond to her making changes practices. They are challenging for physicians to
in the way she managed stressors, with the appro- manage on their own. A large part of this challenge
priate help. She was reassured that she would still is communicating the diagnosis to the patient in a
make progress toward her goals but needed to way that takes into account their prior experience
take a more gradual and flexible approach. of stigma and misunderstanding, their preconcep-
However, she could not go back to her apartment tions about psychological explanations for physi-
in her current amnesic state and needed to move cal symptoms, and our limited and still-evolving
in with someone until her symptoms had understanding of the etiology of the condition.
improved. Within an hour, the patient’s amnesia Psychologists are well qualified to help meet this
had resolved. She became more alert and interac- challenge, by providing a mental health encounter
tive and announced that she felt ready to be that is respectful and informative and invites the
discharged. patient’s and family’s participation in the diagnos-
At follow-up three weeks later, she had had no tic process. We believe that the process that we
further events or memory problems. She acknowl- have described goes a long way toward this end.
edged that she had always internalized her feel- We hope that providing a detailed description of
ings. Because of her learning disability, she had this approach will lead to studies that assess its
always tried to show people that she could do effectiveness, relative to other approaches.
more than was expected of her. She acknowl-
edged that she needed to manage her stressors
better and was willing to engage in therapy. References

Comment: This patient shows many of the char- Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1967). Infancy in Uganda: Infant
care and the growth of love. Baltimore, MD: Johns
acteristics of the “good child” described by
Hopkins University Press.
Kozlowska: insecure attachment to parents (due Alper, K., Devinsky, O., Perrine, K., Vazquez, B., &
to learning problems, difficulty competing with Luciano, D. (1993). Nonepileptic seizures and child-
her accomplished older sister, and father’s lack of hood sexual and physical abuse. Neurology, 43(10),
1950–1953.
availability) that she compensated for by striving
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic
for academic achievement at the expense of and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.).
developing relationships. Her events appear to Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association.
have been precipitated by failure of this compul- Ataoglu, A., Ozcetin, A., Icmeli, C., & Ozbulut, O. (2003).
Paradoxical therapy in conversion reaction. Journal of
sively compliant strategy when she began to
Korean Medical Science, 18(4), 581–584.
struggle with work. Having had little time grow- Bakvis, P., Roelofs, K., Kuyk, J., Edelbroek, P.M., Sinkels,
ing up to learn how to develop relationships, she W.A., & Spinhoven, P. (2009). Trauma, stress, and
also struggled socially in a new environment. She preconscious threat processing in patients with psy-
272 J.T. Langfitt and W. Watson

chogenic nonepileptic seizures. Epilepsia, 50(5), Cragar, D. E., Berry, D. T., Schmitt, F. A., & Fakhoury,
1001–1011. T.A. (2005). Cluster analysis of normal personality
Bakvis, P., Spinhoven, P., & Roelofs, K. (2009). Basal traits in patients with psychogenic nonepileptic sei-
cortisol is positively correlated to threat vigilance in zures. Epilepsy & Behavior, 6(4), 593–600.
patients with psychogenic nonepileptic seizures. Dodrill, C. B., Wilkus, R. J., & Batzel, L. W. (1993). The
Epilepsy & Behavior, 16(3), 558–560. MMPI as a diagnostic tool in non-epileptic seizures. In
Bartholomew, R. E. (1997). Epidemic hysteria: A dia- A. J. Rowan & J. R. Gates (Eds.), Non-epileptic sei-
logue with François Sirois. Medical Principles and zures (1st ed.). Boston, MA: Butterworth-Heinemann.
Practice, 6, 38–44. Drane, D. L., Williamson, D. J., Stroup, E. S., Holmes,
Bartholomew, K., & Horowitz, L. M. (1991). Attachment M.D., Jung, M., Koerner, E., et al. (2006). Cognitive
styles among young adults: A test of a four-category impairment is not equal in patients with epileptic and
model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, psychogenic nonepileptic seizures. Epilepsia, 47(11),
61(2), 226–244. 1879–1886.
Benbadis, S. R. M. (2010). Psychogenic nonepileptic "sei- Escobar, J. I., Waitzkin, H., Silver, R. C., Gara, M., &
zures" or "attacks"?: It's not just semantics: Attacks. Holman, A. (1998). Abridged somatization: A study in
[Miscellaneous]. Neurology, 75, 84–86. primary care. Psychosomatic Medicine, 60(4),
Benbadis, S. R., & Hauser, W. A. (2000). An estimate of 466–472.
the prevalence of psychogenic non-epileptic seizures. Ettinger, A. B., Devinsky, O., Weisbrot, D. M.,
Seizure, 9(4), 280–281. Ramakrishna, R.K., & Goyal, A. (1999). A compre-
Betts, T., & Boden, S. (1992). Diagnosis, management hensive profile of clinical, psychiatric, and psychoso-
and prognosis of a group of 128 patients with non- cial characteristics of patients with psychogenic
epileptic attack disorder. Part II. Previous childhood nonepileptic seizures. Epilepsia, 40(9), 1292–1298.
sexual abuse in the aetiology of these disorders. Evans, H., & Bartholomew, R. E. (2009). Outbreak: The
Seizure, 1(1), 27–32. encyclopedia of extraordinary social behavior. San
Bewley, J., Murphy, P. N., Mallows, J., & Baker, G.A. Antonio, TX: Anomalist Books.
(2005). Does alexithymia differentiate between Farias, S. T., Thieman, C., & Alsaadi, T. M. (2003).
patients with nonepileptic seizures, patients with epi- Psychogenic nonepileptic seizures: Acute change in
lepsy, and nonpatient controls? Epilepsy & Behavior, event frequency after presentation of the diagnosis.
7(3), 430–437. Epilepsy & Behavior, 4(4), 424–429.
Binzer, M., Stone, J., & Sharpe, M. (2004). Recent onset Ford, C. V. (1977). A type of disability neurosis: The
pseudoseizures – Clues to aetiology. Seizure, 13(3), humpty dumpty syndrome. International Journal of
146–155. Psychiatry in Medicine, 8, 285–294.
Bodde, N. M., Brooks, J. L., Baker, G. A., Boon, P.A., Galimberti, C. A., Ratti, M. T., Murelli, R., Marchioni, E.,
Hendriksen, J.G., Mulder, O.G., et al. (2009). Manni, R., & Tartara, A. (2003). Patients with psycho-
Psychogenic non-epileptic seizures–definition, etiol- genic nonepileptic seizures, alone or epilepsy-associ-
ogy, treatment and prognostic issues: A critical review. ated, share a psychological profile distinct from that of
[Review] [95 refs]. Seizure, 18(8), 543–553. epilepsy patients. Journal of Neurology, 250(3),
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss, vol 1: Attachment. 338–346.
New York, NY: Basic Books. Gay, P. (1988). Freud: A life for our time. New York, NY:
Bowman, E. S., & Markand, O. N. (1996). Psychodynamics WW Norton’s.
and psychiatric diagnoses of pseudoseizure subjects. Goldstein, L. H., Chalder, T., Chigwedere, C., Kondoker,
American Journal of Psychiatry, 153(1), 57–63. M.R., Moriarty, J., Toone, B.K., et al. (2010).
Brooks, J. L., Goodfellow, L., Bodde, N. M., Aldenkamp, Cognitive-behavioral therapy for psychogenic nonepi-
A., & Baker, G.A. (2007). Nondrug treatments for leptic seizures: A pilot RCT. Neurology, 74(24),
psychogenic nonepileptic seizures: What's the evi- 1986–1994.
dence. [Review] [80 refs]. Epilepsy & Behavior, 11(3), Griffith, J. L., Polles, A., & Griffith, M. E. (1998).
367–377. Pseudoseizures, families, and unspeakable dilemmas.
Brown, R. J. (2004). Psychological mechanisms of medi- Psychosomatics, 39(2), 144–153.
cally unexplained symptoms: An integrative concep- Gumnit RJ, Walczak TS, National Association of
tual model. [Review] [168 refs]. Psychological Epilepsy Centers. (2001). Guidelines for essential
Bulletin, 130(5), 793–812. services, personnel, and facilities in specialized epi-
Carton, S., Thompson, P. J., & Duncan, J. S. (2003). Non- lepsy centers in the United States. Epilepsia, 42(6),
epileptic seizures: Patients' understanding and reac- 804–814.
tion to the diagnosis and impact on outcome. Seizure, Halligan, P. W., Athwal, B. S., Oakley, D. A., &
12(5), 287–294. Frackowiak, R.S. (2000). Imaging hypnotic paralysis:
Ciechanowski, P. S., Walker, E. A., Katon, W. J., & Russo, Implications for conversion hysteria. Lancet,
J.E. (2002). Attachment theory: A model for health 355(9208), 986–987.
care utilization and somatization. Psychosomatic Harden, C. L., Burgut, F. T., & Kanner, A. M. (2003).
Medicine, 64(4), 660–667. The diagnostic significance of video-EEG monitor-
12 Evaluation and Management of Psychogenic Nonepileptic Attacks 273

ing findings on pseudoseizure patients differs Lesser, R. P. (1996). Psychogenic seizures. [Review] [84
between neurologists and psychiatrists. Epilepsia, refs]. Neurology, 46(6), 1499–1507.
44(3), 453–456. Lieb, R., Zimmermann, P., Friis, R. H., Hofler, M., Tholen,
Hermann, B. P. (1993). Neuropsychological assessment in S., & Wittchen, H.U. (2002). The natural course of
the diagnosis of non-epileptic seizures. In A. J. Rowan DSM-IV somatoform disorders and syndromes among
& J. R. Gates (Eds.), Non-epileptic seizures (1st ed.). adolescents and young adults: A prospective-longitu-
Boston, MA: Butterworth-Heinemann. dinal community study. European Psychiatry, 17(6),
Hilgard, E. R. (1977). Divided consciousness: Multiple 321–331.
controls in human thought and action. New York, NY: Lilienfeld, S. O., Van, V. C., Larntz, K., & Akiskal, H.S.
Wiley. (1986). The relationship of histrionic personality dis-
Hiller, W., Rief, W., & Brahler, E. (2006). Somatization in order to antisocial personality and somatization disor-
the population: From mild bodily misperceptions to ders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 143(6),
disabling symptoms. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric 718–722.
Epidemiology, 41(9), 704–712. Mace, C. J., & Trimble, M. R. (1996). Ten-year prognosis
Hotopf, M. (2002). Childhood experience of illness as a of conversion disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry,
risk factor for medically unexplained symptoms. 169(3), 282–288.
[Review] [51 refs]. Scandinavian Journal of Mailis-Gagnon, A., Giannoylis, I., Downar, J., Kwan,
Psychology, 43(2), 139–146. C.L., Mikulis, D.J., Crawley, A.P., et al. (2003).
Janet, P. (1907). The major symptoms of hysteria. Altered central somatosensory processing in chronic
New York, NY: Macmillan. pain patients with "hysterical" anesthesia. Neurology,
Kozlowska, K. (2001). Good children presenting with 60(9), 1501–1507.
conversion disorder. Clinical Child Psychology and Marchetti, R. L., Kurcgant, D., Neto, J. G., von Bismark,
Psychiatry, 6, 575–591. M.A., Marchetti, L.B., & Fiore, L.A. (2008).
Kroenke, K. (2007). Efficacy of treatment for somatoform Psychiatric diagnoses of patients with psychogenic
disorders: A review of randomized controlled trials. non-epileptic seizures. Seizure, 17(3), 247–253.
[Review] [64 refs]. Psychosomatic Medicine, 69(9), Marshall, J. C., Halligan, P. W., Fink, G. R., Wade, D.T.,
881–888. & Frackowiak, R.S. (1997). The functional anatomy
Kroenke, K., Spitzer, R. L., deGruy, F. V., III, Hahn, S.R., of a hysterical paralysis. Cognition, 64(1), B1–B8.
Linzer, M., Williams, J.B., et al. (1997). Martin, R. C., Gilliam, F. G., Kilgore, M., Faught, E., &
Multisomatoform disorder. An alternative to undiffer- Kuzniecky, R. (1998). Improved health care resource
entiated somatoform disorder for the somatizing utilization following video-EEG-confirmed diagnosis
patient in primary care. Archives of General Psychiatry, of nonepileptic psychogenic seizures. Seizure, 7(5),
54(4), 352–358. 385–390.
Kuyk, J., Spinhoven, P., & van Dyck, R. (1999). Hypnotic Moene, F. C., Spinhoven, P., Hoogduin, K. A., & van
recall: A positive criterion in the differential diagnosis Dyck, R. (2002). A randomised controlled clinical
between epileptic and pseudoepileptic seizures. trial on the additional effect of hypnosis in a compre-
Epilepsia, 40(4), 485–491. hensive treatment programme for in-patients with con-
LaFrance, W. C. J. (2010). Psychogenic nonepileptic "sei- version disorder of the motor type. Psychotherapy and
zures" or "attacks"?: It's not just semantics: Seizures. Psychosomatics, 71(2), 66–76.
[Miscellaneous]. Neurology, 75, 87–88. Moene, F. C., Spinhoven, P., Hoogduin, K. A., van Dyck,
LaFrance, W. C. Jr., Baird, G. L., Barry, J. J., Blum, A.S., R. (2003). A randomized controlled clinical trial of a
Webb, F., Keitner, G.I., et al. (2014) Multicenter pilot hypnosis-based treatment for patients with conversion
treatment trial for psychogenic nonepileptic seizures: disorder, motor type. International Journal of Clinical
A randomized clinical trial. JAMA Psychiatry, and Experimental Hypnosis., 51(1), 29–50.
doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.817, Published Mokleby, K., Blomhoff, S., Malt, U. F., Dahlstrom, A.,
online July 2, 2014. Tauboll, E., & Gjerstad, L. (2002). Psychiatric comor-
LaFrance, W. C., Jr., Alper, K., Babcock, D., Barry, J.J., bidity and hostility in patients with psychogenic non-
Benbadis, S., Caplan, R., et al. (2006). Nonepileptic epileptic seizures compared with somatoform disorders
seizures treatment workshop summary. Epilepsy & and healthy controls. Epilepsia, 43, 193–198.
Behavior, 8(3), 451–461. Norman, D. A., & Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to action:
LaFrance, W. C., Jr., Keitner, G. I., Papandonatos, G. D., Willed and automatic control of behavior. In R. J.
Blum, A.S., Machan, J.T., Ryan, C.E., et al. (2010). Davidson, G. E. Schwartz, & D. Shapiro (Eds.),
Pilot pharmacologic randomized controlled trial for Consciousness and self-regulation, vol 4. Advances in
psychogenic nonepileptic seizures. Neurology, 75(13), research and theory. New York, NY: Plenum.
1166–1173. Reuber, M. (2009). The etiology of psychogenic non-
Lamberty, G. J. (2008). Understanding somatization in epileptic seizures: Toward a biopsychosocial model.
the practice of clinical neuropsychology. Oxford: [Review] [115 refs]. Neurologic Clinics, 27(4),
Oxford University Press. 909–924.
274 J.T. Langfitt and W. Watson

Reuber, M., & Elger, C. E. (2003). Psychogenic nonepi- epilepsy: A video-EEG and ictal SPECT case study.
leptic seizures: Review and update. [Review] [185 Neurology, 73, 1804–1806.
refs]. Epilepsy & Behavior, 4(3), 205–216. Temkin, O. (1971). The falling sickness (2nd ed.).
Reuber, M., Fernandez, G., Bauer, J., Helmstaedter, C., & Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins Press.
Elger, C.E. (2002). Diagnostic delay in psychogenic Vuilleumier, P. (2005). Hysterical conversion and brain
nonepileptic seizures. Neurology, 58(3), 493–495. function. [Review] [123 refs]. Progress in Brain
Reuber, M., Pukrop, R., Bauer, J., Helmstaedter, C., Research, 150, 309–329.
Tessendorf, N., & Elger, C.E. (2003). Outcome in psy- Waldinger, R. J., Schulz, M. S., Barsky, A. J., & Ahern,
chogenic nonepileptic seizures: 1 to 10-year follow-up D.K. (2006). Mapping the road from childhood trauma
in 164 patients. Annals of Neurology, 53, 305–311. to adult somatization: The role of attachment.
Reuber, M., Pukrop, R., Bauer, J., Derfuss, R., & Elger, Psychosomatic Medicine, 68(1), 129–135.
C.E. (2004). Multidimensional assessment of person- Watson, W. (2007). Bridging the mind–body split: Towards
ality in patients with psychogenic non-epileptic sei- an integrative framework for thinking about somato-
zures. [See comment]. Journal of Neurology, form symptoms. The Family Psychologist, 23, 30–33.
Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 75(5), 743–748. Watson, W., & Langfitt, J. T. (2010). Psychogenic non-
Roelofs, K., Hoogduin, K. A., Keijsers, G. P., Naring, epileptic attacks. Family Therapy Magazine, 9, 30–38.
G.W., Moene, F.C., & Sandijck, P. (2002). Hypnotic Wood, B. L., McDaniel, S., Burchfiel, K., & Erba, G.
susceptibility in patients with conversion disorder. (1998). Factors distinguishing families of patients
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111(2), 390–395. with psychogenic seizures from families of patients
Rosenberg, H. J., Rosenberg, S. D., Williamson, P. D., & with epilepsy. Epilepsia, 39(4), 432–437.
Wolford, G.L., II (2000). A comparative study of Wyler, A. R., Hermann, B. P., Blumer, D., & Richey, E.T.
trauma and posttraumatic stress disorder prevalence in (1993). Pseudo-pseudoepileptic seizures. In A. J.
epilepsy patients and psychogenic nonepileptic sei- Rowan & J. R. Gates (Eds.), Non-epileptic seizures
zure patients. Epilepsia, 41(4), 447–452. (1st ed.). Boston, MA: Butterworth-Heinemann.
Shen, W., Bowman, E. S., & Markand, O. N. (1990). Wyllie, E., Friedman, D., Luders, H., Morris, H., Rothner,
Presenting the diagnosis of pseudoseizure. [See com- D., & Turnbull, J.(1991). Outcome of psychogenic sei-
ment]. Neurology, 40(5), 756–759. zures in children and adolescents compared with adults.
Shih, J. J., LeslieMazwi, T., Falcao, G., & Van Gerpen, J. Neurology, 41(5), 742–744.
(2009). Directed aggressive behaviors in frontal lobe
Developmentally Delayed Children
and Adults 13
Erica M. Brandling-Bennett

including structural brain malformations,


Introduction genetic or metabolic abnormalities, birth anoxia,
and cerebrovascular insults, some of which
Epilepsy is a disorder that can arise from a overlap with the neuropathological underpin-
variety of etiologies, such as structural brain nings of seizures. Epidemiological studies have
malformations, genetic or metabolic abnormali- shown that epilepsy occurs much more often in
ties, birth anoxia, cerebrovascular insults, cen- people with intellectual disability than in the
tral nervous system infections, brain tumors, or general population, with estimates ranging from
moderate to severe brain injury. It is a disorder 10 % to over 60 % of people with intellectual
that most often begins in childhood and adoles- disability having some form of epilepsy
cence but can have an onset at any point during (Forsgren, Edvinsson, Blomquist, Heijbel, &
adulthood, including older adulthood. Given the Sidenvall, 1990; Lhatoo & Sander, 2001). In
heterogeneity of epilepsy, it is not surprising those patients with intellectual impairment and
that patients with epilepsy can have vastly dif- epilepsy concurrently, seizures usually present
ferent levels of intellectual and neurocognitive before the age of 5 years, with the severity of
functioning. Specific factors such as the under- seizures and intellectual impairment being
lying pathology of seizures, age of onset, seizure highly correlated (Kaufman, 2007). It is well
type, seizure frequency, as well as antiepileptic beyond the scope of this chapter to try to charac-
medication regimen can have an impact on intel- terize the intellectual functioning associated
lectual and neurocognitive functioning. with all the different seizure types and epilepsy
Additionally, intellectual disabilities are caused syndromes. However, there are several epilepsy
by a variety of neuropathological processes, syndromes that commonly have comorbid intel-
lectual impairment and developmental delay,
and there are several common developmental
syndromes that are characterized by both intel-
lectual impairment and seizures. This chapter
will briefly outline a few syndromes that most
commonly present with developmental delays
E.M. Brandling-Bennett (*) and seizures. More importantly, it will attempt
University of Washington Medicine - Valley Medical to provide a discussion of how neuropsycholog-
Center, Neuroscience Institute, ical assessment can assist in the assessment and
3915 Talbot Road South, Suite 104, Renton,
WA 98055, USA treatment of developmentally delayed children
e-mail: ebrandlingbennett@gmail.com and adults with epilepsy.

W.B. Barr and C. Morrison (eds.), Handbook on the Neuropsychology of Epilepsy, 275
Clinical Handbooks in Neuropsychology, DOI 10.1007/978-0-387-92826-5_13,
© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015
276 E.M. Brandling-Bennett

in domains other than memory (Lee, Yip, &


Neurocognitive Functioning Jones-Gotman, 2002). Furthermore, it has been
in Epilepsy Syndromes demonstrated that patients with chronic, uncon-
and Developmental Syndromes trolled temporal lobe epilepsy often experience a
with Comorbid Epilepsy significant decline in their overall IQ after about
30 years of uncontrolled seizure activity, although
There are a number of different classifications of those findings are also mediated by a number of
epilepsy and specific epilepsy syndromes with factors, including the frequency and severity of
which patients can be diagnosed, as designated the patient’s seizures and premorbid cognitive
by the International League Against Epilepsy and intellectual functioning (Jokeit & Ebner,
(ILAE), many of which are diagnosed in child- 2002). It has also been demonstrated that some
hood. There are also a number of different devel- proportion of patients with temporal lobe epi-
opmental syndromes in which children commonly lepsy have premorbid intellectual impairment, as
present with intellectual impairment, develop- lesions, areas of epileptogenesis, and seizures all
mental delays, or severe learning disorders as interfere with the maturation and development of
well as a history of seizures or a chronic epi- the brain which can lead to a global impact on
lepsy diagnosis. While the following list of cognitive development and a significant decrease
classifications and syndromes is not exhaus- in overall intellectual functioning such that even
tive, it highlights several of the more common “typical” mesial temporal epilepsy with hippo-
types of epilepsy syndromes and developmental campal sclerosis can be associated with impaired
syndromes that have comorbid developmental intellectual functioning (Sauerwein et al., 2005).
delay or intellectual impairment with concurrent
seizures.
Primary Generalized Epilepsy

Temporal Lobe Epilepsy There are a number of primary generalized epi-


lepsies, also known as idiopathic generalized epi-
It has been estimated that simple and complex lepsies, most of which are presumed to have
focal seizures make up as many as 70 % of all the multiple genetic underpinnings that are only just
diagnosed epilepsies, and half of those originate beginning to be elucidated. They include syn-
in temporal lobe structures. Thus, approximately dromes such as childhood absence epilepsy, juve-
30 % of all epilepsies are thought to be temporal nile absence epilepsy, juvenile myoclonic
lobe epilepsy, with two-thirds of temporal lobe epilepsy, epilepsy with generalized tonic-clonic
epilepsies having an epileptogenic focus in seizures upon awakening, and generalized epi-
mesial temporal areas and the other third having lepsies associated with febrile events (Shouse &
an epileptogenic focus in lateral temporal lobe Quigg, 2008). For the most part, these epilepsies
regions (Crawford, 2000; Hauser, Annegers, & respond well to antiepileptic medications, partic-
Kurland, 1991; Panayiotopoulos, 2002; Wiebe, ularly valproic acid, lamotrigine, topiramate,
2000). For the most part, temporal lobe epilepsy levetiracetam, and ethosuximide. Additionally,
is not commonly associated with developmental many children with one of these primary general-
delay or intellectual impairment (Sauerwein, ized epilepsies are seizure-free within 14 years of
Gallagher, & Lassonde, 2005). However, cogni- their initial diagnosis and do not need to take
tive functioning in patients with temporal lobe medication chronically, with the exception of
epilepsy varies from individual to individual, children with juvenile myoclonic epilepsy who
with the occurrence and severity of status epilep- often need to take antiepileptic medication
ticus or a series of generalized tonic-clonic sei- throughout their lives (Shahar, Barak, Andraus,
zures presenting the greatest risk factors for & Kramer, 2004; Williams & Sharp, 2000).
amnestic syndromes and neurocognitive decline Regarding the cognitive correlates of primarily
13 Developmentally Delayed Children and Adults 277

generalized epilepsy, studies have shown that spasms will be intellectually disabled (Trevathan,
children demonstrate average IQs with only mild Murphy, & Yeargin-Allsopp, 1999). Additionally,
cognitive weaknesses in the areas of attention, that study also demonstrated that approximately
concentration, and impulsiveness, as well as a 12 % of 10-year-old children with severe intel-
higher rate of diagnosed learning disorders lectual disability have a history of infantile
(Bhise, Burack, & Mandelbaum, 2010; Shahar spasms. Interestingly, when the etiology of
et al., 2004). In particular, juvenile myoclonic infantile spasms is cryptogenic, approximately
epilepsy is associated with a good intellectual 50 % of the children have a normal cognitive
prognosis, particularly if it is well controlled with outcome, particularly if the seizures are well
medication (Williams & Sharp, 2000). controlled with medications or surgery (Murphy
& Dehkharghani, 1994).

West Syndrome (Infantile Spasms)


Tuberous Sclerosis
West syndrome, also known as infantile spasms,
begins between 4 and 6 months of life and gener- Tuberous sclerosis is characterized by smooth
ally involves clusters of myoclonic seizures. and firm nodules on the face that usually appear
There are a variety of etiologies of infantile in adolescence. In a minority of children affected
spasms, including CNS infection, CNS develop- by tuberous sclerosis, they also present with sei-
mental abnormalities, head trauma, intrapartum zures and intellectual disability due to cerebral
asphyxia, metabolic disorders, and neurodegen- tubers that correlate with the skin lesions.
erative disease (Williams & Sharp, 2000), thus Additionally, children with CNS involvement of
many of the syndromes described in this chapter tuberous sclerosis often present with autistic-like
have West syndrome or infantile spasms as a pre- symptoms (Hunt & Dennis, 1987; Kaufman,
senting or core feature. The overall incidence of 2007). The severity of neuropsychiatric and intel-
infantile spasms has been estimated at 2–7 per lectual impairment is correlated with the number
10,000 live births and often has comorbid devel- and location of cortical tubers (Caplan, Gillberg,
opmental delay (Hauser et al., 1991; Lee & Ong, Dunn, & Spence, 2008). While approximately
2001; Lúthvígsson, Olafsson, Sigurthardóttir, & 50 % of children with tuberous sclerosis need to
Hauser, 1994), making it one of the most com- be institutionalized due to intractable seizures
mon epilepsy syndromes of childhood or presen- and deteriorating intellectual functioning, the
tation of childhood seizures that has comorbid other 50 % of children with tuberous sclerosis
intellectual impairment. Interestingly, the myo- have only minimal cognitive impairment, and
clonic seizures of infancy usually wane by the their epilepsy is easily controlled with antiepilep-
age of 4 years old. As many as 60 % of children tic medication. However, children with cerebral
with a history of infantile spasms then develop tubers often have several tubers. When children
other types of seizures and go on to have chronic have several cortical tubers, that is often corre-
epilepsy that may or may not be treatable lated with more severe seizures that are often
with antiepileptic medication (Murphy & intractable to antiepileptic medication, and their
Dehkharghani, 1994). In terms of long-term intellectual disability is often more severe. While
prognosis, up to 20 % of infants with infantile not all cortical tubers are epileptogenic, the like-
spasms will die in infancy, usually as a result of lihood of intellectual impairment occurring in
the underlying disease or complications from patients with tuberous sclerosis has been associ-
their medical condition (Jeavons, Bower, & ated with a history of seizures, particularly infan-
Dimitrakoudi, 1973). Of those children who sur- tile spasms (Joinson et al., 2003; Kaufman,
vive infantile spasms, one epidemiological study 2007). Importantly, one study demonstrated that
demonstrated that by the age of 10 years old, treatment of the infantile spasms with vigabatrin
83 % of children with a history of infantile resulted in a very high rate of seizure control and
278 E.M. Brandling-Bennett

a commensurate improvement in behavioral and sometime between 2 and 11 years of age following
intellectual functioning, suggesting that seizures a period of normal development. It is a relatively
significantly contribute to the presentation of rare epilepsy syndrome but one in which neuro-
intellectual impairment and autistic-like behavior cognitive functioning is a hallmark feature of the
in children with tuberous sclerosis and not sim- syndrome. Importantly, the child should have
ply the presence of cortical tubers (Jambaqué, achieved age-appropriate developmental mile-
Chiron, Dumas, Mumford, & Dulac, 2000). stones, including age-appropriate speech, at the
time of the initial onset of verbal auditory agno-
sia (Fenichel, 2001; Panayiotopoulos, 2002). The
Lennox-Gastaut Syndrome severity of verbal deficits is highly variable
between children diagnosed with the syndrome,
Lennox-Gastaut syndrome is characterized by as it can simply involve verbal auditory agnosia
the following triad of symptoms: (1) multiple in which children have difficulty distinguishing
types of intractable seizures that are mainly tonic, speech from nonspeech sounds, or children can
atonic, and atypical absence seizures, (2) cogni- become totally unresponsive in oral communica-
tive and behavioral abnormalities, and (3) diffuse tion and eventually demonstrate a progressive
slow spike and waves and paroxysms of fast deterioration of expressive speech and vocabu-
activity on EEG (Panayiotopoulos, 2002). The lary. Interestingly, the course over which verbal
syndrome usually first presents between 1 and 7 deficits are acquired is often fluctuating with
years of age, with a peak incidence occurring remission and exacerbation causing a waxing and
between 3 and 5 years of age (Rantala & waning pattern in their behavioral and neurocog-
Putkonen, 1999; Trevathan, Murphy, & Yeargin- nitive presentation (Paquier, Van Dongen, &
Allsopp, 1997). Approximately 50 % of children Loonen, 1992). Approximately 75 % of children
with West syndrome progress to Lennox-Gastaut with Landau-Kleffner syndrome have comorbid
syndrome, and conversely up to 30 % of children seizures, which may have an onset before, dur-
with Lennox-Gastaut transition to having West ing, or after the development of verbal deficits.
syndrome (Panayiotopoulos, 2002, Rantala & Most commonly, children have generalized tonic-
Putkonen, 1999). The seizures are often difficult clonic seizures and focal motor seizures (Murphy
to control and status epilepticus is common. In & Dehkharghani, 1994). The seizures are usually
about 50 % of children who are eventually diag- easily treatable with antiepileptic medication,
nosed with Lennox-Gastaut syndrome, cognitive with almost all children becoming seizure-free
and behavioral abnormalities are evident prior to by 15 years of age. Unfortunately, seizure remit-
the initial diagnosis of the epileptic syndrome, tance is not associated with an improvement in
but eventually intellectual disability is diagnosed aphasia or verbal deficits, although some children
in up to 96 % of children with the syndrome. do spontaneously recover all language function.
Additionally, the course of intellectual impairment To date, studies have not been able to determine
and developmental delay is usually progressive, and what contributes to long-term recovery of lan-
many children with the syndrome eventually require guage in some children, although an earlier age
significant assistance in most aspects of daily func- of onset is associated with a poorer neurocogni-
tioning if they reach adulthood (Gastaut, 1982). tive outcome (Gordon, 1990). Children diag-
nosed with Landau-Kleffner syndrome also
demonstrate a variety of behavioral problems,
Landau-Kleffner Syndrome including hyperactivity, inattention, and impul-
sivity. Importantly, nonverbal abilities are gener-
Landau-Kleffner syndrome, also known as ally preserved and Landau-Kleffner syndrome is
acquired epileptic aphasia, is characterized by a not usually associated with global cognitive
sudden or gradual onset of verbal deficits, par- decline or intellectual impairment (Panayio-
ticularly verbal auditory agnosia, which occurs topoulos, 2002; Williams & Sharp, 2000).
13 Developmentally Delayed Children and Adults 279

Electrical Status Epilepticus delay (Kaufman, 2007). Additionally, approximately


During Sleep 13–57 % of patients with Down syndrome
develop seizures or an abnormal EEG at some
Electrical status epilepticus during sleep, also point in their lives (Romano et al., 1990; Tangye,
known as epilepsy with continuous spike and 1979). There is commonly a bimodal distribution
waves during slow-wave sleep, is characterized of seizure onset, with the highest rate of onset
by: (1) continuous spikes and waves during more occurring in childhood and another spike in onset
than 85 % of non-REM sleep on EEG, (2) occurring in middle age (Prasher, 1995; Tatsuno,
seizures, and (3) neurocognitive decline Hayashi, Iwamoto, Suzuki, & Kuroki, 1984).
(Panayiotopoulos, 2002). Much like Landau- Additionally, patients with Down syndrome can
Kleffner syndrome, it is a relatively rare epilepsy present with a number of different seizure types,
syndrome in which a decline in intellectual and including infantile spasms (Pollack, Golden,
cognitive functioning is a hallmark feature. Schmidt, Davis, & Leeds, 1978), generalized
Usually children have a period of typical devel- tonic-clonic seizures, complex-partial seizures
opment followed by the onset of continuous spike (Romano et al., 1990), and Lennox-Gastaut syn-
and slow waves during sleep sometime between drome (Tatsuno et al., 1984).
the ages of 4 and 14 years of age, which is accom-
panied by neurocognitive regression, although
some children demonstrate premorbid Angelman Syndrome
developmental delays prior to the onset of elec-
troencephalographic changes (Williams & Sharp, Angelman syndrome is a genetic disorder in
2000). Unlike Landau-Kleffner syndrome, which which all patients present with developmental
is thought to be a highly related syndrome delay that becomes apparent between 6 and 12
(Feekery, Parry-Fielder, & Hopkins, 1993), chil- months of age. Behavioral and cognitive features
dren with electrical status epilepticus during include severely impaired expressive language,
sleep demonstrate more global neurocognitive an excessively happy demeanor, very poor atten-
decline (Jayakar & Seshia, 1991). Similar to tion span, and autistic-like symptoms (Kaufman,
Landau-Kleffner syndrome, the seizures always 2007). Additionally, seizures, abnormal electro-
remit, usually at some point during adolescence. encephalography, and microcephaly are
Studies have suggested that there are some recov- observed in over 80 % of patients with the syn-
ery of neurocognitive function and improvement drome. In fact, it has been estimated that about
in behavioral problems that occur with the remis- 6 % of the children who present with severe
sion of the continuous spike and slow waves dur- intellectual disability and epilepsy have
ing sleep. However, less than 25 % of children Angelman syndrome (Guerrini, Cararozzo,
with this syndrome achieve an average level of Rinaldi, & Bonnani, 2003). Patients with
intellectual and cognitive functioning, instead Angelman syndrome almost always develop sei-
presenting with some level of cognitive and zures before 3 years of age. The most frequent
adaptive disability throughout their lives seizure types are myoclonic, atonic, generalized
(Panayiotopoulos, 2002, Paquier et al., 1992). tonic-clonic, atypical absence seizures (Galván-
Manso, Campistol, Conill, & Sanmartí, 2005),
as well as periods of insidious myoclonic status
Down Syndrome epilepticus (Viani et al., 1995). Fortunately,
there are a number of medications that have been
Down syndrome or trisomy 21 is characterized shown to effectively treat the seizures in these
by a range of intellectual and developmental dis- patients during early childhood, including val-
ability, along with a number of physical and med- proic acid, benzodiazepines, and ethosuximide,
ical problems. It is the most common genetic often in combination (Guerrini et al., 2003), with
cause of intellectual disability and developmental additional control of seizures also being
280 E.M. Brandling-Bennett

achieved after children are 9 years of age or which time development slows and then deterio-
older with some combination of those antiepi- rates with a loss of language skills, a regression
leptic medications (Galván-Manso et al., 2005). of motor abilities, and a decrease in cognitive
capacity. Eventually, girls with Rett syndrome
are considered developmentally delayed with
Fragile X Syndrome severe to profound intellectual disability and
autistic-like symptoms (Huppke, Held, Laccone,
Fragile X syndrome is a sex-linked chromosomal & Hanefeld, 2003; Kaufman, 2007). It has been
disorder that is characterized by intellectual estimated that epilepsy occurs in 80–95 % of
impairment and distinctive non-neurological girls with Rett syndrome (Steffenburg, Hagberg,
physical features. It is widely considered to be & Hagberg, 2001), but some studies have also
the most common cause of inherited intellectual argued that the incidence of seizure comorbidity
disability, as it is responsible for about 10 % of in Rett syndrome is overestimated due to the fre-
all cases of intellectual disability (Kaufman, quent occurrence of autonomic dysfunction and
2007). Approximately 80 % of males with fragile motor symptoms that are common in the syn-
X syndrome are moderately or severely intellec- drome, such as twitching, jerking, head turning,
tually disabled, while 10–15 % of males with falling forward, and trembling, all of which can
fragile X syndrome are mildly intellectually be easily mistaken for seizures (Glaze, Schultz,
disabled or have IQs in the borderline range with & Frost, 1998). When seizures are identified in
significant language impairment and autistic-like girls with Rett syndrome, they are commonly
symptoms. Additionally, most males with this complex-partial seizures with onset occurring
syndrome have symptoms of attention deficit around 4 years of age. Most often, the occurrence
hyperactivity disorder, including distractibility of seizures is highly correlated with smaller head
and impulsivity (Fryns, Jacobs, Kleczkowska, & circumference. Additionally, girls with Rett syn-
van den Berghe, 1984). In contrast, females car- drome can also present with generalized tonic-
rying the fragile X gene are often asymptomatic. clonic, absence, myoclonic jerk, atonic, and tonic
Approximately 30 % of females with the gene seizures. The severity and incidence of seizures
present with IQs lower than 85, and those of aver- decrease with increasing age, and many women
age intellectual functioning are at increased risk with the syndrome are seizure-free (Steffenburg
for learning disabilities (de Vries et al., 1995; et al., 2001).
Kaufman, 2007). Seizures occur in approximately
10–20 % of patients, both male and female, with
fragile X syndrome, with a slightly higher preva- Dravet Syndrome (Severe Myoclonic
lence of seizures in males with fragile X syndrome. Epilepsy in Infancy)
The seizures that occur in fragile X syndrome most
resemble those of benign focal epilepsy of child- Dravet syndrome, also known as severe myo-
hood, including a common pattern of centrotemporal clonic epilepsy in infancy, is a rare form of pro-
spikes, which are usually easily controlled with gressive epileptic encephalopathy that is thought
antiepileptic medication and often remit before 20 to have genetic underpinnings. It is estimated to
years of age (Berry-Kravis, 2002; Sabaratnam, affect approximately 6 % of infants diagnosed
Vroegop, & Gangadharan, 2001). with epilepsy. It is characterized by initial febrile
and afebrile, generalized and unilateral clonic or
tonic-clonic seizures that occur during the first
Rett Syndrome year of life in an otherwise typically developing
infant (Dravet & Bureau, 2008). Over time, chil-
Rett syndrome occurs only in females and is dren will then begin to have other types of sei-
characterized by normal birth and typical devel- zures, including myoclonic, atypical absences,
opment for the first 6–18 months of life, after and partial seizures, with periods of absence
13 Developmentally Delayed Children and Adults 281

status epilepticus frequently occurring between 2 tions and have revealed behavioral and cognitive
and 4 years of age. One hallmark feature of the deficits, altered neurochemistry, and reduced
syndrome is that the seizures are often refractory brain weight in the children after birth (Fisher &
to treatment with antiepileptic medication Vorhees, 1992; Gaily & Meador, 2007). In a
(Panayiotopoulos, 2002). As the seizures prog- groundbreaking study examining the effects of
ress between 2 and 4 years of age, psychomotor fetal exposure to four different antiepileptic med-
delays, behavioral disturbances, and cognitive ications, the Neurodevelopmental Effects of
impairment become apparent. Eventually, Antiepileptic Drugs (NEAD) study group dem-
patients with Dravet syndrome go on to have per- onstrated that exposure to valproic acid in utero
sistent refractory convulsive seizures. There has resulted in significantly lower IQs at 3 years of
been one neuropsychological study of teenagers age compared with in utero exposure to carbam-
with Dravet syndrome that demonstrated 50 % of azepine, lamotrigine, and phenytoin (Meador
patients with this syndrome were severely intel- et al., 2009). However, it is important to note that
lectually disabled, and all teenagers had signifi- children exposed to valproic acid in utero had an
cant neurocognitive deficits in motor, verbal, and average IQ of 92, with IQs ranging from 88 to 97.
visuospatial abilities. The teenagers were Thus, none of the children exposed to valproic
described as hyperactive and psychotic-like with acid met criteria for borderline intellectual func-
some autistic traits (Cassé-Perrot, Wolff, & tioning or intellectual impairment. While it is
Dravet, 2001; Wolff, Cassé-Perrot, & Dravet, important to consider in utero exposure to anti-
2001). Approximately 15 % of children with epileptic medication as a possible factor when
Dravet syndrome die as a direct or indirect result assessing patients born to mothers with epilepsy
of their seizures. For those patients who survive who took antiepileptic medication during their
into adulthood, their seizures always remain pregnancy, there has been little evidence to sug-
refractory to antiepileptic medication. gest that in utero exposure to antiepileptic medi-
Additionally, they are usually intellectual dis- cations is a single causative factor for
abled and present with limited language, poor developmental delay or intellectual impairment.
fine motor functions, generalized slowing,
autistic-like behaviors, and occasionally psychi-
atric features, all of which are severe enough that Neuropsychological Assessment
they usually cannot work or live independently of Patients with Developmental
(Dravet & Bureau, 2008). Delay or Intellectual Impairment
and Concurrent Epilepsy

Neurocognitive Effects Developmentally delayed or intellectually


of Antiepileptic Drugs impaired patients with epilepsy are referred for
During Pregnancy neuropsychological for a variety of reasons. Most
commonly, they are referred by their neurologists
Epilepsy can affect not only the intellectual for evaluation of their overall general intellectual
capacity of patients with the condition but also functioning and their level of general adaptive
the intellectual functioning of children born to functioning. In those cases, their degree of intel-
mothers with epilepsy. It has long been estab- lectual, neurocognitive, and developmental dis-
lished that exposure to some antiepileptic medi- ability needs to be determined so that appropriate
cations in utero, particularly phenobarbital and recommendations and referrals can be provided.
other antiepileptic medications in high doses, can A frequently asked additional referral question is
lead to fetal structural malformations (Holmes, whether or not the patient’s current antiepileptic
Wysznski, & Lieberman, 2004). More recent medication regimen is negatively impacting their
studies have also examined the effects of fetal degree of intellectual, neurocognitive, or devel-
exposure to lower doses of antiepileptic medica- opmental impairment or delay. It has been well
282 E.M. Brandling-Bennett

established that many antiepileptic medications (i) Leisure.


can have a negative impact on neurocognition, (j) Health issues
particularly memory and verbal functioning (k) Safety
(Aldenkamp, Baker, & Mulder, 2000; Martin 3. An onset of deficits before the age of 18 years.
et al., 1999; Loring & Meador, 2001; Meador, Additionally, the DSM-IV-TR specifies the
Loring, Hulihan, Kamin, & Karim, 2003; criteria for defining the severity of a patient’s
Motamedi & Meador, 2004), and it is important level of mental retardation or intellectual disabil-
to determine whether a patient with intellectual ity into the following levels: borderline intellec-
impairment is being further negatively impacted tual functioning (IQ 70–85), mild mental
by their antiepileptic medication regimen. retardation (IQ of 50–75), moderate mental retar-
Finally, developmentally delayed or intellectu- dation (IQ of 35–55), severe mental retardation
ally impaired patients with epilepsy are also (IQ of 20–40), and profound mental retardation
referred by neurosurgeons for neuropsychologi- (IQ of less than 20). The International
cal assessment to assist with determining epilep- Classification of Diseases, 10th revision (ICD-
togenic lateralization and location, as well as to 10, World Health Organization, 1993) denotes
predict the potential impact of surgery on cogni- similar definitions of the four levels of mental
tive, behavioral, and adaptive functioning. It is retardation and also specifies that intellectual
important for neuropsychologists to consider the retardation is determined by both a low measured
referral source and referral question when assess- IQ and deficits in cognitive, language, motor,
ing developmentally delayed or intellectually social, and other adaptive behavior skills.
impaired patients with epilepsy so that they Approximately 85 % of people who are diag-
design their battery appropriately, with special nosed with intellectual disability are mildly men-
consideration to the ability of the patient to par- tally retarded, which means they can usually
ticipate in a comprehensive evaluation versus a attain at least a sixth grade level of academic
shorter evaluation and the relative value of achievement and lead somewhat independent
administering some tests over others. lives (McLaren & Bryson, 1987). Adults with
mild mental retardation often work or volunteer
in their community and live in assisted living
Definition of Intellectual Disability facilities where they are minimally supervised.
Given that the severity of disability and a patient’s
The Diagnostic and Statistic Manual of Mental needs increase with classifications of moderate,
Disorders, Fourth Edition, Text Revision (DSM- severe, or profound mental retardation, it is often
IV-TR, 2000) provides the following criteria for helpful to accurately assess a patient’s level of
diagnosing mental retardation or intellectual intellectual disability in order to best inform
disability: patients’ caretakers of their expected quality of
1. A measured IQ of 70 or below on an adminis- life.
tered test of intellectual functioning.
2. Concurrent age-related deficits in adaptive
functioning in at least two of the following Neurocognitive Functioning
areas: in Patients with Developmental
(a) Communication. Delays or Intellectual Disability
(b) Self-care.
(c) Home living. Patients with documented intellectual disability
(d) Social skills. have, by definition, delays in several aspects of
(e) Use of community resources. cognition and adaptive functioning. However, the
(f) Self-direction. nature and degree of impairment in various areas
(g) Functional academic skills. of cognition is highly variable from patient to
(h) Work. patient. Studies have demonstrated that a diagnosis
13 Developmentally Delayed Children and Adults 283

of mild mental retardation does not always cor- Similarly, the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale,
relate with impairments in attention, memory, Fourth Edition (WAIS-IV; Wechsler, 2008) is
verbal comprehension, or visual perception. In appropriate for adults aged 16–91 years old and
fact, when patients with mild or moderate general provides subtests to assess verbal comprehen-
intellectual disability have borderline to average sion, perceptual reasoning, processing speed, and
functioning in attention, memory, or verbal com- working memory. If the administration of a full
prehension, that is highly predictive of employ- WPPSI-III, WISC-IV, or WAIS-IV is not possi-
ment success (Su, Lin, Wu, & Chen, 2008). Thus, ble given the severity of a patient’s intellectual
even though a patient may present with a mea- disability, the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of
sured IQ below 70, that does not necessarily Intelligence (WASI, Wechsler, 1999) is appropri-
mean that all other aspects of their neurocogni- ate for both children and adults aged 6–90 years
tive profile will also be in the impaired range. A old, takes only 30 min to administer, and pro-
careful assessment of all aspects of the neurocog- vides verbal (estimated VIQ), nonverbal (esti-
nitive profile of patients with intellectual disabil- mated PIQ), and overall (estimated FSIQ)
ity, particularly patients with mild or moderate measures of intellectual functioning. The
intellectual disability, is warranted as that pro- Wechsler scales have the advantage of being the
vides the most accurate evaluation of all aspects most widely used measures that have the greatest
of their functioning. Additionally, a thorough basis for empirical validity in the research litera-
neuropsychological assessment provides crucial ture. Additionally, the more recent versions of the
information about the possible neurocognitive Wechsler scales have been tailored to have shorter
effects of antiepileptic medication or the possible administration times, so they are more amenable
neurocognitive outcome of surgery. to testing patients with significant cognitive limi-
tations. However, it is not uncommon for patients
who are assessed at major epilepsy centers
Intelligence Tests around the United States and in other countries to
be given older versions of the Wechsler scales
Given that a patient’s measured IQ is the founda- (i.e., WPPSI-R, WISC-III, and WAIS-III), as
tion for a diagnosis of mental retardation or intel- those centers have often invested a great deal of
lectual disability, it is often essential to at least time and effort in collecting valuable databases
attempt to obtain a measured IQ score for patients that incorporate older versions of the Wechsler
who are thought to be developmentally delayed scales, and so they continue to gather consistent
or intellectually impaired. The most common data about the performance of patients with epi-
measures used to assess overall intellectual and lepsy on those older measures.
general cognitive functioning are the Wechsler When using the Wechsler scales to obtain a
scales, which allow for the assessment of chil- measure of IQ or to estimate general overall
dren from the age of two and a half years old to intellectual functioning, it is important to con-
adults aged 91 years old. The Wechsler Preschool sider the patient’s level of verbal and nonverbal
and Primary Scale of Intelligence, Third Edition abilities in the context of both their level of dis-
(WPPSI-III; Wechsler, 2002) is appropriate for ability and a possible lateralization of epilepto-
children age 2 years, 6 months to 7 years, 3 genic areas. While it is not unusual for patients
months and provides verbal and performance with intellectual disabilities to have bilateral or
indices for all children as well as a processing diffuse pathology, both in relation to their epi-
speed index for older children. The Wechsler lepsy and with regard to other factors contribut-
Intelligence Scale for Children, Fourth Edition ing to their cognitive limitations, it is also
(WISC-IV; Wechsler, 2003) is appropriate for possible that they may have a lateralized epilep-
children aged 6–17 years old and provides sub- togenic focus that will result in a significant dis-
tests to assess verbal comprehension, perceptual crepancy between their verbal and perceptual
reasoning, processing speed, and working memory. reasoning abilities and thus artificially lower
284 E.M. Brandling-Bennett

their overall measured IQ. Thus, IQ alone is not a aspects of neurocognitive functioning. If possible,
good predictor of postoperative cognition and again depending on the stamina of the patient
seizure outcome, necessitating the gathering of being assessed, it is preferable to administer
additional neurocognitive data (Gleissner, multiple measures within each neurocognitive
Clusmann, Sassen, Elger, & Helmstaedter, 2006). domain in order to assure the reliability the
data obtained on a given measure within a neu-
rocognitive domain. However, there are a num-
Nonverbal Tests of Intelligence ber of valuable screening tests that sample a
variety of neurocognitive domains in a short
Many patients with intellectual disabilities have period of time. Probably the most widely used
significant verbal limitations, even when the epi- neuropsychological screening measure is the
leptogenic focus is in the nondominant hemi- Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of
sphere. Frequently, their verbal limitations are so Neuropsychological Status (RBANS;
severe that administration of a Wechsler scale to Randolph, 1998), which can be administered to
measure IQ results in a significant underestimate adult patients aged 20–89 years old. The
of their abilities. It is important to consider a RBANS provides assessments of immediate
patient’s verbal abilities before administering the memory (immediate list learning and immedi-
comprehensive but time-consuming scales. Even ate story), visuoperceptual abilities (figure
if it is possible to administer a comprehensive copy and line orientation), language (naming
measure of intellectual functioning, such as the and semantic fluency), attention (digit span
aforementioned Wechsler scales, it is often useful forward and digit-symbol coding), and delayed
to administer a nonverbal test of intelligence that memory (delayed list memory with recogni-
can be used to assess the validity of the measured tion, delayed story memory, and delayed figure
IQ score. For children, the most widely used non- memory). More recently, the Neuropsy-
verbal measure of intelligence is the Leiter chological Assessment Battery (NAB; Stern &
International Performance Scale, Revised Edition White, 2003) has provided an additional neuro-
(Leiter-R; Roid & Miller, 1997), which can be psychological screening measure. The NAB
used for children aged 2–21 years, provides a can be administered to patients aged 18–97
brief measure of mental age, and covers a wide years old. The battery is made up of six mod-
range of abilities using a simple format. Another ules, each of which includes a number of dif-
commonly administered brief nonverbal test of ferent tests. The modules are (1) the Screening
intellectual function is the Test of Nonverbal Module, (2) the Attention Module, (3) the
Intelligence, Third Edition (TONI-3; Brown, Language Module, (4) the Memory Module,
Sherbenou, & Johnson, 1997), which can be used and (5) the Executive Functions Module. Within
for patients aged 6–90 years old and allows for a each module are a number of different tests that
very brief and completely nonverbal estimate of assess the designated domain. The Screening
overall intellectual functioning. In contrast to the Module incorporates brief versions of several
Leiter-R, the TONI-3 does not assess a variety of of the tests from each of the five other modules.
different abilities, but it is much quicker to Thus, administration of the NAB Screening
administer. Module provides a brief assessment of all
global, basic aspects of neurocognitive func-
tioning, in much the same way as the RBANS
Comprehensive Neurocognitive does. Furthermore, the NAB then provides the
Screening Tests option of administering more detailed measures
of a specific domain of neurocognitive func-
In addition to assessing overall intellectual tioning, if the Screening Module reveals par-
functioning in patients with intellectual dis- ticular weaknesses or strengths within that
abilities, it is also important to assess other domain.
13 Developmentally Delayed Children and Adults 285

Language estimate of a patient’s age- and grade-related


receptive language abilities. Additionally, it is
In assessing a patient’s language abilities, if a full now possible to make direct comparisons
Wechsler scale measure of intellectual function- between receptive and expressive vocabulary
ing is administered, the individual verbal subtests with the administration of the Expressive
from the scale may be able to provide some Vocabulary Test, Second Edition (EVT-2;
insight into strengths and weaknesses in the ver- Williams, 2007), which was normed on the
bal profile, particularly with regard to expressive same sample as the PPVT-4. The EVT-2 can
(i.e., the vocabulary and similarities subtests) also be administered to patients aged 2 years, 6
versus receptive (i.e., the comprehension subtest) months to 90 years old and only takes about
verbal functioning. Additionally, it may be pos- 20 min to administer. The test requires a patient
sible to administer some of the more common to provide an acceptable one-word response
measures of language functioning, such as the when presented with a picture and a question
Boston Naming Test (BNT; Kaplan, Goodglass, administered by the examiner. The PPVT-4 and
& Weintraub, 2001) for which there is good nor- EVT-2 have been validated on samples with
mative data for patients aged 5 to 90 years old ADHD, emotional/behavioral disturbance,
(Heaton, Miller, Taylor, & Grant, 2005; Kaplan giftedness, hearing impairment, language/
et al., 2001) and measures of phonemic and speech delay, language/speech disorder, learn-
semantic verbal fluency for which there is good ing disability, and intellectual impairment and
normative data for patients aged 8–90 years old thus have proven valid for assessment of both
(Heaton et al., 2005; Welsh, Pennington, & receptive and expressive language abilities in
Groisser, 1991). However, it is important to rec- patients with developmental delay or intellec-
ognize that the subtests of the Wechsler scales, tual impairment.
the BNT, and verbal fluency require relatively Finally, as mentioned before, the RBANS and
strong verbal skills, which can be a significant the NAB also incorporate assessment of language
limitation for patients with developmental delay functioning. Specifically, the RBANS has a sim-
or intellectual impairment. If patients perform ple naming measure, which has a low ceiling but
very poorly on those measures, they provide little can be sensitive in measuring naming deficits in
insight into relative strengths and weaknesses in lower functioning patients. Additionally, the
the verbal profile. That said, it can be valuable to RBANS has a semantic fluency measure that
do a qualitative comparison of a patient’s verbal requires patients to name as many fruits and veg-
output on phonemic versus semantic fluency, as etables as they can in a 1-min period, which also
that may provide insight into whether frontal or has a low ceiling but can be sensitive in measur-
temporal language areas are more affected or ing verbal fluency deficits in lower functioning
involved (Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, patients (Randolph, 1998). The NAB has a lan-
Alexander, & Stuss, 2002). guage module comprised of six subtests, which
For patients with significant limitations in can be administered individually, including an
their expressive language abilities, a valuable oral production subtest, an auditory comprehen-
measure of receptive language is the Peabody sion subtest, a naming subtest, a reading compre-
Picture Vocabulary Test, Fourth Edition hension subtest, a writing subtest, and a bill
(PPVT-4; Dunn & Dunn, 2007), which can be payment subtest (Stern & White, 2003). These
administered to patients aged 2 years, 6 months subtests, particularly the oral production and the
to 90 years old. This test requires patients to naming subtests, have the advantage of evaluat-
select one picture from an array of four pic- ing verbal fluency and confrontation naming,
tures that best represents a word that is said to respectively, with greater sensitivity for lower
them by the examiner. The test has the advan- functioning patients than the BNT and traditional
tage of taking only about 15 min to administer. verbal fluency tests (i.e., phonemic and semantic
From the PPVT-4, it is possible to obtain an fluency).
286 E.M. Brandling-Bennett

Visuospatial Functioning valuable information about visuospatial abilities


are the Benton Judgment of Line Orientation
In assessing a patient’s visuospatial abilities, if a (Benton JOLO; Benton, Sivan, Hamsher, Varney,
full Wechsler scale measure of intellectual func- & Spreen, 1994), which can be administered to
tioning is administered, the individual perfor- patients aged 18–74 years old, and the Hooper
mance subtests from the scale may be able to Visual Orientation Test (Hooper VOT; Hooper,
provide some insight into strengths and weak- 1983), which can be administered to patients
nesses in visuospatial abilities (i.e., the block aged 25–69 years old.
design, matrix reasoning, visual puzzles, and pic- Once again, the RBANS and the NAB also
ture completion subtests), particularly if both incorporate assessment of visuospatial function-
quantitative and qualitative aspects of a patient’s ing. The RBANS requires the copy of a complex
performance can be evaluated. However, given figure that is much simpler than the RCFT and a
that the subtests of the Wechsler scales are diffi- judgment of line orientation subtest that is simi-
cult, patients with intellectual impairment and lar to but simpler than the Benton JOLO
developmental disability often experience floor (Randolph, 1998). The NAB Spatial Module is
effects on those subtests, which make meaning- comprised of four subtests, which can be admin-
ful interpretation of patient’s performances istered individually, including a visual discrimi-
difficult. Similarly, the administration of the nation subtest, the design construction subtest,
Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test copy trial the figure drawing subtest, and the map reading
(RCFT; Meyers & Meyers, 1995) will likely also subtest. The visual discrimination subtest has the
be negatively confounded by a patient’s global advantage of being a motor-free test that requires
cognitive limitations. However, it is often still matching-to-target discrimination that is rela-
valuable to administer the copy trial of the RCFT, tively sensitive and has a low floor for greater
as a qualitative evaluation of a patient’s copying sensitivity in evaluating patients with intellectual
of the figure may provide some information disability or developmental delay (Stern &
about localization or lateralization of an epilep- White, 2003).
togenic focus (Schouten, Hendriksen, &
Aldenkamp, 2009).
There are several other simpler tests of visuo- Memory
spatial abilities that are worth administering,
most of which sample a greater range of abilities Assessment of memory functioning is particu-
and are not as susceptible to floor effects that can larly critical in patients with known or suspected
make their administration on patients with intel- temporal lobe epilepsy, in patients who are being
lectual impairment potentially meaningless. In considered for anterior temporal lobectomy, and
particular, the Beery-Buktenica Developmental in patients who self-report or whose families/
Test of Visual-Motor Integration, Fifth Edition caretakers report particular memory problems. If
(Beery VMI; Beery, Buktenica, & Beery, 2006) a full Wechsler intelligence scale is administered,
can be administered to patients aged 2–100 years it may also be possible to administer one of the
old. It is a simple test that involves subtests that co-normed Wechsler memory scales: the
include freehand copying of increasingly com- Children’s Memory Scale (CMS; Cohen, 1997)
plex figures, guided motor coordination copying for children aged 5–16 years old, the Wechsler
of tracings of the same figures, and a motor-free Memory Scale, Third Edition (WMS-III;
visuoperceptual matching of figures. The test Wechsler, 1997) for patients aged 16–89 years
provides both age- and grade-equivalent scores old and useful for comparison with the WAIS-III,
and has a relatively low floor, making it ideal for or the more recently released Wechsler Memory
administration to patients with developmental Scale, Fourth Edition (WMS-IV; Wechsler, 2009)
delay and intellectual impairment. Other tests for patients aged 16–90 years old and useful for
that are also simple to administer and can provide comparison with the WAIS-IV. In addition to the
13 Developmentally Delayed Children and Adults 287

fact that these comprehensive memory batteries period, leading to a shorter overall administration
can be meaningfully compared to scales of over- time and lower floor effects. Another list learning
all intellectual functioning, these inclusive batter- measure that is shorter and easier than the
ies are made up of multiple measures of verbal CVLT-II is the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test,
and visual memory functioning, which is particu- Revised (HVLT-R; Brandt & Benedict, 2001) for
larly valuable when attempting to lateralize hip- patients aged 16–92 years old. The HVLT-R
pocampal dysfunction. However, once again it is involves three learning trials of a 12-item word
important to recognize that the subtests of the list followed by a 20–25-min delay period. It is
Wechsler scales are difficult and are designed for easy to administer and score and is well toler-
assessment of memory abilities in higher func- ated even by significantly impaired individuals.
tioning patients, thus patients with developmen- Its use has been validated with brain-disordered
tal delay and intellectual impairment often populations (e.g., Alzheimer’s disease,
experience floor effects on those measures, which Huntington’s disease, amnestic disorders).
makes interpretation of impaired performances Additionally, six distinct forms of the HVLT-R
on the Wechsler memory scales difficult and are available, eliminating practice effects on
often meaningless. Another comprehensive repeated administrations.
memory battery that also provides comparisons With regard to visual learning and memory,
of verbal and visual memory, along with some there are significantly fewer sensitive measures
measures of general memory and attention/ in comparison with verbal learning and memory.
concentration, is the Wide Range Assessment of The most commonly cited measure of visual
Memory and Learning, Second Edition learning and memory is the RCFT (Meyers &
(WRAML2; Sheslow & Adams, 2003), which Meyers, 1995), but, as stated earlier, the copy
can be administered to patients aged 5–90 years trial of the RCFT is significantly confounded by
old. While the WRAML2 does not provide data intellectual functioning, making it an insensitive
for a direct comparison between overall intellec- measure for the quantitative assessment of visual
tual functioning and memory abilities, it does have memory in patients with developmental delay or
a greater range of sensitivity, making it a more intellectual impairment. However, there are
valuable for the assessment of memory function- qualitative aspects of memory for the RCFT that
ing in patients with cognitive limitations. can be valuable to observe, so administration of
One of the most commonly administered and delayed memory trials for qualitative if not
researched measures of verbal learning and quantitative evaluation can provide valuable
memory in populations of patients with epilepsy information, even in patients with intellectual
is the California Verbal Learning Test, Second impairment. A simpler measure of visual learn-
Edition (CVLT-II; Delis, Kramer, Kaplan, & ing and memory is the Brief Visuospatial Memory
Ober, 2000), which can be administered to Test, Revised (BVMT-R; Benedict, 1997) for
patients aged 16–89 years old, or the children’s assessment of patients aged 18–79 years old. The
version of that list learning task, the California test involves three learning trials of an array of
Verbal Learning Test, Children’s Edition six line drawings presented together on a page
(CVLT-C; Delis, Kramer, Kaplan, & Ober, 1986), which the patient has 10 s to observe before
which can be administered to children aged 5–17 attempting to reproduce the line drawings. There
years old. The standard version of the CVLT-II is also a 20-min delay trial followed by a recogni-
requires five learning trials of a 16-item word list, tion trial and an optional copy trial (which can be
followed by a 20-min delay period prior to used to roughly assess simple visuoperceptual
delayed memory testing. For the assessment of abilities). Much like the HVLT-R, six distinct
adult patients with developmental delay or intel- forms of the BVMT-R are available, eliminating
lectual impairment, the CVLT-II has a short-form practice effects on repeated administrations.
version that involves four learning trials of a Once again, the RBANS and the NAB also
9-item word list followed by a 15-min delay incorporate assessment of memory functioning.
288 E.M. Brandling-Bennett

The RBANS immediate memory component is much briefer administrations and a low floor for
made up of a list learning task and a brief story greater sensitivity in evaluating patients with
memory task. Similar to the CVLT-II Short Form intellectual disability or developmental delay
and the HVLT-II, the RBANS list learning task is (Stern & White, 2003).
made up of four learning trials of a 10-item word
list, making it more sensitive to verbal memory
deficits in patients with cognitive limitations. The Adaptive Functioning
RBANS story memory task is also relatively sim-
ple as it is a short story that is repeated twice. Perhaps the most important aspect of a neuropsy-
Additionally, the RBANS has a delayed memory chological assessment of patients with intellec-
component that is made up of both a free recall tual disabilities is an assessment of their adaptive
and recognition trial of the word list, a free recall functioning. As highlighted earlier, age-related
trial of the story, and a free recall trial of the com- deficits in adaptive functioning are critical aspects
plex figure that was copied as part of the visuo- of developmental delay and intellectual impair-
spatial component. Overall, both the immediate ment, both for diagnosis and for the purpose of
and delayed memory components of the RBANS empirically evaluating everyday functioning.
are relatively brief and useful in measuring This aspect of a neuropsychological evaluation
immediate and delayed learning and memory can often be the most meaningful for families and
deficits in patients with cognitive limitations caretakers. The most widely used measure of
(Randolph, 1998). The NAB has a Memory adaptive functioning is the Vineland Adaptive
Module that is comprised of four subtests, which Behavior Scales, Second Edition (Vineland-II;
can be administered individually, including a list Sparrow, Cicchetti, & Balla, 2005), which can be
learning task, a shape learning task, a story learn- used to assess adaptive functioning in patients
ing task, and a daily living memory task. Much from birth to 90 years old. The Vineland-II
like the RBANS, the NAB list learning task is includes the option of administering a survey
brief, involving three learning trials of a 12-item interview of parents or caregivers that can take up
word list, followed by an interference list trial, to one hour to administer, an expanded interview
then a short delay free recall trial, a long delay of parents or caregivers that can take an hour and
free recall trial, a long delay forced-choice trial, a half to administer, a parent/caregiver rating
and a recognition trial. The NAB story memory form that can take up to an hour for caregivers to
involves two learning trials of a five-sentence complete, or a teacher rating form that takes
story with measures of both immediate and approximately 20 min for teachers to complete.
delayed free recall of a five-sentence story. The The measure assesses five different domains,
NAB shape learning task involves three learning including communication (receptive, expressive,
trials and multiple-choice immediate recognition and written language skills), daily living skills
of nine visual stimuli, followed by delayed rec- (personal, domestic, and community-related
ognition and forced-choice delayed recall of the skills), socialization (interpersonal relationships,
items. Finally, the daily living task involves ver- play and leisure time, and coping skills), motor
bal learning as well as immediate and delayed skills (fine and gross motor skills), and an
memory of information encountered in daily liv- optional maladaptive behavior index (internaliz-
ing, such as medication instructions, names ing, externalizing, and other behaviors). Overall,
addresses, and phone numbers, making it a the Vineland-II provides important information
unique assessment of memory for aspects of a about everyday adaptive functioning that aids in
patients adaptive functioning. Overall, the NAB the diagnosis of developmental delay and intel-
Memory Module provides measures of both ver- lectual impairment. Additionally, the results of
bal and visual immediate and delayed memory, the measure can be used to determine a patient’s
much like the aforementioned memory batteries eligibility for special services in schools or treat-
such as the WMS-IV and the WRAML2, but with ment facilities, to plan for rehabilitation or
13 Developmentally Delayed Children and Adults 289

intervention programs, and to track a patient’s of a psychologist or neuropsychologist (Harrison


progress as it correlates with medical or surgical & Oakland, 2003).
treatment of their epilepsy. An important aspect If it is possible to directly evaluate a patient’s
of the Vineland-II is that the interviews, both the abilities, the most commonly used measure of
survey interview and the expanded interview, direct assessment of everyday abilities is the
must be administered by a trained and qualified Independent Living Scales measure (ILS; Loeb,
licensed psychologist, which may increase the 1996). This test is an individually administered
validity and reliability of the measure, particu- assessment of the degree to which adults are
larly over repeated administrations that are capable of caring for themselves and their prop-
designed to track a patient’s progress over a num- erty. Although the ILS is usually administered to
ber of years. patients aged 65 years old and older, there is nor-
Another commonly used measure for the mative data for patients with intellectual disabili-
assessment of adaptive functioning is the ties who are aged 17 years old and older. The ILS
Adaptive Behavior Assessment System, Second is composed of five scales: memory/orientation,
Edition (ABAS-II; Harrison & Oakland, 2003), managing money, managing home and transpor-
which can be used to assess adaptive functioning tation, health and safety, and social adjustment.
from birth to 89 years. The measure is a behavior The performance-based results from the 68 ILS
rating scale that is typically completed by par- items are more objective and reliable than third-
ents, caregivers, and/or teachers. If the behavior party observations or examinees’ self-reports
rating scale is given to multiple raters, it can pro- (Loeb, 1996). Normative data are provided for
vide different perspectives on what a given the different scales so the various areas of compe-
patient can or cannot do in their everyday lives tence can be identified and compared.
without the assistance of others. The measure Additionally, it is possible to administer individ-
assesses adaptive skills in ten different areas ual subtests from the ILS in order to assess spe-
within three different domains, including con- cific areas of everyday functioning.
ceptual (communication skills, functional aca-
demics, and self-direction), social (social skills
and leisure skills), and practical (self-care, Summary
home or school living, community use, work,
and health/safety) domains. Similar to the When a patient with developmental delays or
Vineland-II, the ABAS-II provides information intellectual impairment and concurrent epilepsy
about a patient’s everyday adaptive functioning, is referred for neuropsychological evaluation, it
aids in the diagnosis of intellectual impairment is particularly important to consider the referral
and developmental disabilities, and can assist question and the capabilities of the patient when
with treatment recommendations. The ABAS-II designing the neuropsychological test battery. As
has the advantage of being a behavior rating with all neuropsychological assessments, gather-
scale that does not need to be administered by a ing information about all domains of neurocogni-
licensed psychologist and is therefore less time- tive functioning is important, if it is possible.
consuming from an administration perspective. However, patients with developmental delay or
Additionally, given that the behavior rating intellectual impairment often cannot tolerate a
scale can be given to multiple people, it can pro- lengthy evaluation, and the traditionally adminis-
vide a more accurate profile of a patient’s level of tered neuropsychological tests often have high
adaptive functioning in a variety of environ- floor effects, making interpretation of impaired
ments. The data available on the validity of the performances meaningless. Thus, it is often valu-
ABAS-II suggest that it is just as valid a measure able to utilize screening measures (i.e., the
of adaptive functioning as the Vineland-II, RBANS or the NAB) or other tests with lower
despite being simply a behavior rating scale that floor effects in order to gather more accurate
is completed by patients without the direct input information about neurocognitive functioning.
290 E.M. Brandling-Bennett

Additionally, the inclusion of measures of adaptive eral ventricle and a small cerebellar hemisphere
functioning may be essential for the accurate diag- with the inferior portion of the cerebellum appear-
nosis of intellectual impairment, particularly if a ing asymmetric. An EEG conducted during a PET
patient is referred for an initial diagnosis of intel- scan demonstrated, “an abnormal EEG on account
lectual impairment or for qualification of special- of mild diffuse slowing of the background” which
ized services. Finally, as is highlighted in the suggested, “diffuse cerebral dysfunction associ-
following case reports, it is often necessary for neu- ated with a broad differential diagnosis.” PET
ropsychologists to use both quantitative and qualita- imaging demonstrated, “marked hypometabolism
tive observations of an intellectually impaired of the right cerebrum and right cortical gray mat-
patient’s performance in order to fully understand ter, predominantly affecting the right temporal
their neurocognitive strengths and weaknesses. lobe. In the right clinical setting, this may function
as a zone of epileptogenesis.” During inpatient
video EEG monitoring, six seizures were observed
Case Examples and recorded. Clinically, during a seizure, G.L.
raised her arms above her head, repositioned her-
Case 1 self in bed, and moved nonspecifically. She then
extended her left arm and leg, followed by con-
G.L. was a 26-year-old female who began experi- traction and shaking of her left hand and fingers.
encing complex-partial seizures that secondarily She moaned and vocalized nonspecifically, some-
generalized when she was 3 years old. G.L.’s devel- times followed commands, and often grabbed the
opmental history was notable for intellectual dis- rail of the bed or her oxygen mask. She had diffi-
ability and developmental delay. Her mother culty answering questions but recovered postic-
indicated that she began walking at 2 years old, tally in less than 15 s. Electrographically, her
spoke her first words at 8 years old (i.e., “Ma” and seizures were all stereotyped with development of
“Dad”), and began speaking in sentences at 13 years an ictal rhythm over the right temporal lobe and
old. G.L. started taking formal special education then gradual slowing that became progressively
classes in a mainstream school in sixth grade when more prominent over right frontal regions.
she was 11 years old. She received a certificate of Given the results of the brain MRI, the PET
completion when she graduated high school at 21 imaging, and the inpatient video EEG monitor-
years old. Her mother reported that upon leaving ing, G.L. was thought to have right temporal lobe
high school, G.L. was reading at a third grade level epilepsy and was being considered for a standard
and was performing mathematics at a second grade right anterior temporal lobectomy to neurosurgi-
level. Since graduating from high school, G.L. had cally treat her medically refractory epilepsy. G.L.
been part of a day program service associated with was referred for a neuropsychological evaluation
a local regional center. Through this program, she to assess her current level of neurocognitive func-
spent weekdays volunteering in the community and tioning and to determine the extent of the neuro-
participating in skills training classes. She lived psychological risks (e.g., loss of memory or
with her mother and sister. G.L. said she enjoyed language function) involved in a neurosurgical
spending her free time with her family, liked read- intervention. Regarding general cognition, G.L.
ing books, and enjoyed singing. was unable to articulate and reflect on her overall
Regarding a presurgical medical workup, an level of intellectual functioning. Her mother and
MRI of her brain revealed no evidence of an acute sister reported that she had demonstrated general
process but suggested “diffuse volume loss of improvement across all cognitive domains as she
white matter of the right cerebral hemisphere” as had gotten older. Although her family was not
well as “Wallerian degeneration of the right pyra- aware of any formal testing results, her mother
midal tract.” The MRI also indicated that “the right indicated that G.L. was diagnosed with intellec-
hippocampus is smaller and diffusely hyperin- tual impairment at an early age.
tense, suggesting mesiotemporal sclerosis.” The preoperative neuropsychological test
Finally, the brain MRI indicated an enlarged lat- results are given in Table 13.1. Given her
13 Developmentally Delayed Children and Adults 291

Table 13.1 Neuropsychological test results in Case 1, a Table 13.1 (continued)


26-year-old mildly intellectually disabled female with
Raw
right temporal lobe epilepsy who is being considered for a
Visuospatial skills score T score Description
standard right anterior temporal lobectomy
Beery VMI (age 15 <45 Impaired
General intellectual Raw Scaled Eq = 5.6)
functioning score score Description
Beery visual perception 18 <45 Impaired
Peabody Picture 111 SS = 43 Impaired (age Eq = 6.2)
Vocabulary Test-4 Beery motor coordination 16 <45 Impaired
WAIS-III (age Eq = 5.6)
Vocabulary 9 3 Impaired Raw Z
Similarities 5 2 Impaired Verbal memory score score Description
Comprehension 5 3 Impaired WMS-III
Digit span 14 8 Low average Logical memory I 16 ss = 3 Impaired
Block design 12 4 Borderline (5,3,8)
Matrix reasoning 6 5 Borderline Logical memory II 11 ss = 6 Low average
(4,7)
Attention and Raw Recognition (8,10) 18/30 Borderline
concentration score T score Description
CVLT-II 44 T = 38 Low average
WAIS-III digit span 14 ss = 8 Low average total = (6,7,8,12,11)
Forward 7 List A Trial 1 6 −1.0 Low average
Backward 3 Trial 2 7 −1.5 Borderline
Trails A (seconds) 53″ 22 Impaired Trial 3 8 −2.0 Impaired
Errors 0 Trial 4 12 −0.5 Average
Raw Trial 5 11 −1.5 Borderline
Language score T score Description List B 7 0.0 Average
Phonemic fluency: 6 12 Impaired List A short delay 10 −0.5 Average
F(2), A(2), S(2) free recall
Animal naming 9 15 Impaired List A short delay 7 −2.5 Impaired
Stroop color 140 z = −6.49 Impaired cued recall
Errors/ 1/2 List A long delay free 11 −0.5 Average
self-corrections recall
Stroop word 116 z = −10.60 Impaired List A long delay 6 −3.0 Impaired
cued recall
Errors/ 0/2
self-corrections Recognition hits 16 0.0 Average
Boston Naming 35,0,4 25 Impaired Recognition false 2 −0.5 Average
Test positives
Total recognition 3.4 0.0 Average
Language screening 13/24
discriminability
WRAT-3 reading 24 SS = 49 Impaired
(grade Eq = 2) Raw
WRAT-3 spelling 22 SS = 57 Impaired Nonverbal memory score T score Description
(grade Eq = 2) WMS-III
WRAT-3 arithmetic 22 SS = 50 Impaired Visual 57 ss = 3 Impaired
(grade Eq = 2) reproduction I
WJ-III word attack 6 SS = 62 Impaired Visual 0 ss = 1 Impaired
(grade Eq = 1.7) reproduction II
Raw Recognition 38/48 ss = 4 Borderline
Visuospatial skills score T score Description BVMT-R 8 <20 Impaired
WAIS-III block design 12 ss = 4 Borderline total = (2,2,4)
WMS-III visual 7/7 Average Trial 1 2 25 Impaired
reproduction discrim Trial 2 2 <20 Impaired
Rey-O complex figure 14.5 Impaired Trial 3 4 <20 Impaired
copy (continued)
(continued)
292 E.M. Brandling-Bennett

Table 13.1 (continued) examiner, and her comprehension of simple


Raw questions and basic task instructions was ade-
Nonverbal memory score T score Description quate. In contrast, however, she demonstrated
Learning 2 40 Low considerable difficulty when required to appreci-
average ate the broad or abstract concepts and context of
Delay 4 <20 Impaired conversational speech. Generally, she
Percent retention 100 Average demonstrated qualitatively weaker complex com-
Recognition 6, 1fp Low
prehension of the gestalt of speech, thus indicat-
average
ing weaknesses in right hemisphere contributions
Rey-O complex
figure to complex language information processing.
Immediate recall 3.5 <20 Impaired She demonstrated significant strengths on mea-
Delayed recall 2.0 <20 Impaired sures of simple auditory attention, indicating that
Recognition 18/24 28 Impaired she was able to attend to information at an almost
Whole figure no age-appropriate level. On measures of verbal
recognition memory, she demonstrated poor learning and
Raw memory for contextual verbal information, which
Executive functions score T score Description was likely negatively impacted by her inability to
WAIS-III similarities 5 ss = 2 Impaired appreciate semantic and verbal contextual and
WAIS-III 5 ss = 3 Impaired
gestalt cues, once again likely reflecting weak-
comprehension
WAIS-III matrix 6 ss = 5 Borderline
nesses in right hemisphere contributions to lan-
reasoning guage. In contrast, she demonstrated a significant
Phonemic fluency: 6 12 Impaired strength on a task of verbal list learning and
F(2), A(2), S(2) memory, which was felt to be indicative of ade-
Trails B (s) 300″ 2 Impaired quate left mesial temporal functioning. On mea-
Errors 1 sures of visual memory, she demonstrated
Stroop interference 218 z = −3.68 Impaired impaired learning and memory for all visual
Errors/ 0/14 information, even relatively simple visual infor-
self-corrections
mation, which was felt to be indicative of
impaired right mesial temporal functioning.
extremely low level of general intellectual func- Furthermore, her performances on a number of
tioning, there are a number of important aspects other measures of neuropsychological function-
to her performance that should be considered. ing implicated right hemisphere involvement in
Her generally extremely low level of intellectual her seizures. Specifically, she demonstrated dif-
functioning, significantly slowed speed of infor- ficulty with visual perception and visual-motor
mation processing, and impaired motor coordina- integration. Qualitatively, she was unable to
tion likely negatively impacted her performance appreciate the gestalt of both verbal and visual
on a number of measures on the evaluation. information, which also suggested right hemi-
Therefore, in addition to the quantitative results sphere involvement in her seizures.
of the current evaluation, it was important to con- While her impaired performances on some of
sider qualitative aspects of her performance in the neuropsychological tasks may have been neg-
order to more accurately characterize her atively impacted by her extremely low level of
strengths and weaknesses, in the context of her general intellectual functioning, her relative
generally extremely low level of intellectual and strength in visual abstract reasoning indicated
information processing abilities. relatively adequate nonverbal reasoning skills
Qualitatively, her verbal skills were slightly and supported the idea that she was able to pro-
stronger than indicated by her impaired perfor- cess visuospatial information at a level commen-
mance on formal measures of language function- surate with her overall level of intellectual
ing because she was able to converse with the functioning. Thus, her qualitative weaknesses on
13 Developmentally Delayed Children and Adults 293

the current evaluation likely represented true that she understood what the procedure involved,
neurological weaknesses in right hemisphere as her cooperation would be essential to the
functioning, rather than simply impairments sec- validity of the results. Furthermore, when G.L.
ondary to her extremely low level of general was asked about her understanding of the neuro-
intellectual functioning. Overall, the results of psychological evaluation and the possibility of
the neuropsychological evaluation suggested a undergoing resection surgery, she indicated
specific involvement of right mesial temporal limited understanding of the process. Thus, an
areas along with general involvement of the right additional recommendation of the evaluation was
hemisphere, with relatively spared right frontal that the surgical procedure and possible
lobe functioning. These findings were felt to indi- sequelae of the surgical intervention be
cate an area of epileptogenic focus in right mesial explained to her at a developmentally appropri-
temporal structures, consistent with findings ate level given her extremely low level of gen-
from a brain MRI, PET imaging, and inpatient eral cognitive functioning in order to ensure
video EEG monitoring. her cooperation and compliance with pre- and
Although G.L.’s performance was in the postsurgical management.
extremely low range across most neuropsycho-
logical domains, her strong performance on a
measure of verbal learning and memory sug- Case 2
gested adequate left mesial temporal functioning.
While her quantitative performances on measures B.R. was a 55-year-old mildly intellectually dis-
of language functioning were also extremely low, abled female with left anterior temporal lobe epi-
there was some qualitative and behavioral evi- lepsy who had been seizure-free for 2 years
dence for relatively good functioning of left following a standard left anterior temporal lobec-
hemisphere language areas which were not tomy and continued medication management on
reflected by neuropsychological measures that a relatively low level of lamotrigine. She was
were insensitive to performances in the extremely referred for a neuropsychological evaluation to
low range. Furthermore, she demonstrated a rela- assess her postoperative level of neurocognitive
tive strength (both qualitatively and quantita- and behavioral functioning. In particular, her
tively) in her visuospatial reasoning skills, which mother reported that B.R. had a tendency to talk
indicated that she can appreciate visuospatial about events that did not really happen, so she
information appropriately and has relatively good had concerns that B.R. was either lying or was
functioning of frontal lobe structures, bilaterally. experiencing medication-related hallucinations.
Thus, her extremely low performances on mea- Her mother also expressed concern that B.R.’s
sures of visual learning and memory, combined memory had worsened since the surgery.
with dramatically disrupted processing of visual B.R. had undergone two prior presurgical neu-
and verbal gestalt and contextual information, ropsychological evaluations. The results of the
were felt to provide evidence for right mesial first presurgical neuropsychological evaluation
temporal and generalized right hemisphere inef- indicated, “This woman demonstrates intellec-
ficiencies, with relatively spared right frontal tual abilities which fall within the range of mild
lobe functioning. It was recommended that if intellectual disability overall with visual-spatial
G.L. became a candidate for right anterior tem- abilities just lightly stronger than those in the ver-
poral resection, Wada testing be conducted to bal area. Her level of academic knowledge is just
determine the ability of her relatively stronger slightly below her level of general intelligence,
left mesial temporal lobe to support memory and academic achievement may never have been
postsurgically. However, the neuropsychological especially strong. The battery of neuropsycho-
team noted that it would be important that the logical tests which was administered resulted in
Wada procedure be thoroughly explained to G.L. the identification of moderate impairment in
at a developmentally appropriate level, to ensure brain functions with findings implicating both
294 E.M. Brandling-Bennett

cerebral hemispheres about equally. This impair- mildly intellectually disabled and, per medical
ment is expected to have a substantial impact records, had limited insight into her cognitive
upon functioning in daily life…In terms of pre- limitations. Her mother reported that B.R.’s
dictors for seizure relief following resection sur- memory was poor prior to her undergoing the left
gery for epilepsy, it was noted that only one of anteromedial temporal resection but had seemed
the four predictors of likely relief from seizures worse since the surgery. Additionally, she
arising from this battery of tests were within a reported that B.R. had a tendency to confabulate
favorable range. While this is not a positive and displayed some paranoid ideation. For exam-
result, it should be noted that a full prognostic ple, 1 day B.R. could not find her cat and assumed
statement of likely relief from seizures following someone had stolen it; in fact, the cat had simply
resection surgery must take into account other wandered away for a short time. Another time,
critical clinical and EEG data.” The results of the she found that money was gone from her hus-
second presurgical neuropsychological evalua- band’s wallet and assumed someone had stolen it,
tion indicated, “The patient is a 53-year-old but he had just put the money in his pocket. Of
right-handed female with moderate intellectual note, medical records prior to surgery indicated
disability. This is conferred on the current IQ that B.R. had a tendency to talk about memory
assessment. The majority of neuropsychological for events that did not really happen even back
measures completed are consistent with her over- then. Thus, it appeared that her tendency to con-
all level of cognitive function. Interestingly, an fabulate was not new since the surgery. Further
area of particular strength was seen for repeat tri- consistent with presurgical reports, B.R. had
als of verbal learning and memory…The patient always been easily confused and forgetful about
interacts at a higher level than would be antici- upcoming appointments and had often misplaced
pated based on her IQ…Oral and cognitive pre- items. However, her mother felt that those prob-
sentation is reflective of generalized cortical lems had worsened since surgery. Her mother
dysfunction with preservation of left temporal also reported that B.R. had begun to make literal
lobe capacity. These findings are consistent with paraphasic errors in conversation, such as saying
her history of bilateral seizures and significant that she wanted to go to the “feel good” store
illness during critical development.” It was con- instead of the Goodwill store. This was report-
cluded that improved seizure control would prob- edly new since she had undergone surgery. Her
ably positively improve her life in terms of her mother expressed concerns that these perceived
personal safety and functional ability, with some cognitive changes were being caused by her
risks to her verbal memory that would likely not long-term medication regimen of lamotrigine.
be apparent in her day-to-day functional The postoperative neuropsychological test
capacity. results are given in Table 13.2. Consistent with
Developmentally, B.R. and her husband lived previous presurgical evaluations, the postsurgical
with her parents. She was able to bathe and dress neuropsychological evaluation measured B.R.’s
herself and do a few chores around the house, overall level of intellectual functioning to be
such as taking care of her cat, doing some of the mildly intellectually disabled. On testing, B.R.
cleaning, and folding the laundry, all of which performed in the impaired range on most tests of
she could do successfully with minimal supervi- neuropsychological functioning, consistent with
sion or guidance. She did not do the shopping or her performances on two previous presurgical
cooking, nor did she manage any finances. She evaluations. She once again demonstrated a rela-
was not able to manage her medications indepen- tive strength in her verbal learning and memory
dently and relied on her mother to remind her to abilities, as her performances were largely border-
take her medication. line to low average across those tasks. Compared
Postsurgically, B.R. was unable to provide with the most recent previous presurgical neuro-
subjective information about her cognitive diffi- psychological evaluation, however, she demon-
culties. It had been well documented that she was strated a very mild decline in her verbal list
13 Developmentally Delayed Children and Adults 295

Table 13.2 Neuropsychological test results in Case 2, a Table 13.2 (continued)


postsurgical evaluation of a 55-year-old mildly intellectu-
General intellectual Raw Scaled
ally disabled female with left anterior temporal lobe epi-
functioning score score Description
lepsy who is seizure-free following a standard left anterior
temporal lobectomy Perceptual 71 60 0.05
reason—working
General intellectual Raw Scaled memory
functioning score score Description Perceptual 71 65 NS
WAIS-IV (standard reason—
norming) processing speed
Verbal Working 60 65 NS
comprehension memory—
subtests processing speed
Similarities 11 4 Impaired FSIQ—GAI 59 63 0.05
Vocabulary 10 3 Impaired
Academic abilities Raw Std Description
Information 3 3 Impaired score score
Comprehension 8 3 Impaired
WRAT-4
Perceptual reasoning Impaired
Word reading (grade 35 67
subtests
Eq = 3.7)
Block design 20 6 Low Impaired
Sentence 15 62
average comprehension
Matrix reasoning 5 4 Impaired (grade Eq = 2.8)
Visual puzzles 6 5 Borderline Reading composite 129 63 Impaired
Working memory Spelling (grade 26 68 Impaired
subtests Eq = 3.3)
Digit span 13 3 Impaired Math computation 20 61 Impaired
Arithmetic 6 3 Impaired (grade Eq = 1.9)
Processing speed Attention and Raw Scaled Description
subtests concentration score score
Symbol search 16 5 Borderline WAIS-IV
Coding 20 2 Impaired Digit span 13 3 Impaired
Index scores Forward span 4 Cum % = 100.00
Full Scale IQ 38 SS = 59 Impaired Backward span 3 Cum % = 97.00
Verbal 10 SS = 61 Impaired Sequencing span 3 Cum % = 98.00
Comprehension Arithmetic 6 3 Impaired
Index Symbol search 16 5 Borderline
Perceptual 15 SS = 71 Borderline Coding 20 2 Impaired
Reasoning Index
D-KEFS
Working Memory 6 SS = 60 Impaired Average
Trails: visual 29″ 8
Index
scanning
Processing Speed 7 SS = 65 Impaired
Errors 0
Index
Trails: number 44″ 9 Average
General Ability 25 SS = 63 Impaired
sequencing
Index
Errors 0
Index-level Score Score Significance
Trails: letter 64″ 5 Borderline
discrepancies 1 2
sequencing
Verbal comp— 61 71 0.05
perceptual Errors 0
reasoning (continued)
Verbal comp— 61 60 NS
working memory
Verbal comp— 61 65 NS
processing speed
(continued)
296 E.M. Brandling-Bennett

Table 13.2 (continued) Table 13.2 (continued)


Raw Scaled Raw
Speech and language score score Description Verbal memory score Z score Description
WAIS-IV Verbal paired 33/40 Cum % = 3–9
Vocabulary 10 3 Impaired associates
recognition
Similarities 11 4 Impaired
Auditory Memory 23 SS = 75 Borderline
D-KEFS
Index
Verbal fluency: 9 2 Impaired
CVLT-II 32 T = 31 Borderline
letter
Total = (4,5,7,8,8)
Verbal fluency: 21 3 Impaired
List A Trial 1 4 –1.5 Borderline
category
Trial 2 5 –2.0 Impaired
CW Interfere: 34″ 8 Average
color naming Trial 3 7 –1.5 Borderline
Errors/ 0/1 Trial 4 8 –2.0 Impaired
self-corrections Trial 5 8 –2.0 Impaired
CW interfere: 30″ 6 Low average List B 4 –1.0 Low average
word reading List A short delay 6 –1.5 Borderline
Errors/ 1/0 free recall
self-corrections List A short delay 5 –3.0 Impaired
Boston Naming 40,1,6 z = −5.63 Impaired cued recall
Test List A long delay 7 –1.5 Borderline
WRAT-4 free recall
Word reading 35 SS = 67 Impaired List A long delay 7 –2.0 Impaired
(grade Eq = 3.7) cued recall
Sentence 15 SS = 62 Impaired Recognition hits 14/16 –0.5 Average
comprehension Recognition false 1/32 (–0.5) Average
(grade Eq = 2.8) positives
Reading 129 SS = 63 Impaired Recognition 3.0 0.0 Average
composite discriminability
Scaled Forced choice 16/16 Cum % = 100
Visuospatial skills Raw score score Description recognition
WAIS-IV Raw
Block design 20 6 Low average Nonverbal memory score T score Description
Visual puzzles 6 5 Borderline WMS-IV
BVMT-R copy 8/12 Visual 21 ss = 4 Impaired
Rey-O complex 18.5 Borderline reproduction I
figure copy recall
Raw Visual 8 ss = 6 Low average
Verbal memory score Z score Description reproduction II
recall
WMS-IV
Visual 0/7 Cum % ≤ 2
Logical memory I 14 ss = 5 Borderline reproduction II
(10,4)
recognition
Logical memory II 12 ss = 6 Low average BVMT-R 4 <20 Impaired
(9,3)
total = (1,2,1)
Logical memory 20/30 Cum % = 3–9 Trial 1 1 27 Impaired
recognition
(10,10) Trial 2 2 <20 Impaired
Verbal paired 16 ss = 6 Low average Trial 3 1 <20 Impaired
associates I Delay 3 22 Impaired
(2,4,5,5) Percent retention 100 % Average
Verbal paired 5 ss = 6 Low average Recognition 4, 1fp Borderline
associates II (continued)
(continued)
13 Developmentally Delayed Children and Adults 297

Table 13.2 (continued) learning capacity and retention, which was not
Raw surprising given that left mesial temporal struc-
Nonverbal memory score T score Description tures that support verbal learning and memory
Rey-O complex were resected. Once again, her visual learning and
figure memory was not as good as her verbal learning
3′ immediate 6 23 Impaired
and memory. Interestingly, she also demonstrated
recall
30′ delayed recall 1 <20 Impaired
a mild decline in her visual learning and memory
Recognition 19/24 42 Low average abilities compared with presurgical performances.
Whole figure Yes Overall, the findings of the postsurgical neuropsy-
recognition chological evaluation were consistent with a
Raw Scaled woman who suffered generalized cortical dysfunc-
Executive functions score score Description tion and who, as a result, had mild intellectual dis-
WAIS-IV ability and significant developmental delays. As
Similarities 11 4 Impaired had been demonstrated on previous presurgical
Comprehension 8 3 Impaired neuropsychological evaluations, B.R. had a few
Matrix reasoning 5 4 Impaired areas of relative cognitive strength, including her
D-KEFS verbal learning and memory ability and some
Trails: visual 29″ 8 Average aspects of social and executive skills. Her verbal
scanning
learning and memory abilities continued to be a
Errors 0
relative strength for her, despite showing a mild
Trails: number 44″ 9 Average
sequencing decline from presurgical levels. It was concluded
Errors 0 that she was a woman who had clearly led a life
Trails: letter 64″ 5 Borderline that had been meaningful to her, and she continued
sequencing to have the capacity to participate in her life at a
Errors 0 level that gave her a sense of accomplishment and
Trails: letter- 131″ 7 Low average kept her feeling happy. Given that she was now
number seizure-free, she was leading a healthier life, and it
switching
was likely that she would continue to lead a happy
Errors 4
and productive life for the foreseeable future, with
Trails: motor speed 54″ 7 Low average
Verbal fluency: 9 2 Impaired
very minimal to no change in her cognitive abili-
letter ties since the surgery. Overall, it was felt that the
Verbal fluency: 21 3 Impaired neurosurgical resection to treat her refractory epi-
category lepsy was successful. Furthermore, there was no
Verbal fluency: 7 3 Impaired evidence to suggest that her antiepileptic medica-
switching tion regimen was negatively impacting her cogni-
Verbal fluency: 6 4 Impaired
tive functioning or causing a significant change in
switching
accuracy her psychiatric or cognitive functioning.
CW interfere: CW 82″ 6 Low average
inhibition
Errors/ 2/4
self-corrections References
CW interfere: CW 154″ 1 Impaired
inhib/switching Aldenkamp, A. P., Baker, G., & Mulder, O. G. (2000). A
Errors/ 13/4 multicenter randomized clinical study to evaluate the
self-corrections effect on cognitive function of topiramate compared with
valproate as add on therapy to carbamazepine in patients
Mood/personality Raw score Rating with partial-onset seizures. Epilepsia, 41, 1167–1178.
BDI-II 15 Mild American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic
BAI 3 Minimal and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.).
Washington, DC: Author. Text Revision.
298 E.M. Brandling-Bennett

Beery, K. E., Buktenica, N. A., & Beery, N. A. (2006). Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, D. M. (2007). PPVT-4: Peabody
BEERY VMI: The Beery-Buktenica developmental test Picture Vocabulary Test (4th ed.). San Antonio, TX:
of visual-motor integration (Administration, scoring, Pearson Assessments.
and teaching manual 5th ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Feekery, C. J., Parry-Fielder, B., & Hopkins, I. J. (1993).
Western Psychological Services. Landau-Kleffner syndrome: Six patients including
Benedict, R. H. B. (1997). BVMT-R: Brief Visuospatial discordant monozygotic twins. Pediatric Neurology, 9,
Memory Test – Revised: Professional manual. Lutz, 49–53.
FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc. Fenichel, G. M. (2001). Clinical pediatric neurology: A
Benton, A. L., Sivan, A. B., Hamsher, K. D., Varney, signs and symptoms approach (4th ed.). Philadelphia,
N. R., & Spreen, O. (1994). Contributions to neuro- PA: WB Saunders.
psychological assessment (A clinical manual 2nd ed.). Fisher, J. E., & Vorhees, C. (1992). Developmental toxicity
New York, NY: Oxford University Press, Inc. of antiepileptic drugs: Relationship to postnatal dys-
Berry-Kravis, E. (2002). Epilepsy in fragile X syndrome. function. Pharmacological Research, 26, 207–221.
Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, Forsgren, L., Edvinsson, S. O., Blomquist, H. K., Heijbel,
44(11), 724–728. J., & Sidenvall, R. (1990). Epilepsy in a population of
Bhise, V. V., Burack, G. D., & Mandelbaum, D. E. (2010). mentally retarded children and adults. Epilepsy
Baseline cognition, behavior, and motor skills in chil- Research, 6(3), 234–248.
dren with new-onset, idiopathic epilepsy. Fryns, J. P., Jacobs, J., Kleczkowska, A., & van den
Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 52, Berghe, H. (1984). The psychological profile of the
22–26. fragile X syndrome. Clinical Genetics, 25(2),
Brandt, J., & Benedict, R. H. B. (2001). The Hopkins 131–134.
Verbal Learning Test – Revised. Lutz, FL: Gaily, E., & Meador, K. J. (2007). Neurodevelopmental
Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc. effects. In J. Engel Jr. & T. A. Pedley (Eds.), Epilepsy:
Brown, L., Sherbenou, R. J., & Johnson, S. K. (1997). Test A comprehensive textbook (2nd ed., pp. 1225–1233).
of nonverbal intelligence (3rd ed.). Austin, TX: Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott, Williams, & Wilkins.
Pro-Ed. Galván-Manso, M., Campistol, J., Conill, J., & Sanmartí,
Caplan, R., Gillberg, C., Dunn, D. W., & Spence, S. J. F. X. (2005). Analysis of the characteristics of epi-
(2008). Psychiatric disorders in children. In J. Engel lepsy in 37 patients with the molecular diagnosis of
Jr. & T. A. Pedley (Eds.), Epilepsy: A comprehensive Angelman syndrome. Epileptic Disorders, 7(1),
textbook (2nd ed., pp. 1225–1233). Philadelphia, PA: 19–25.
Lippincott, Williams, & Wilkins. Gastaut, H. (1982). The Lennox-Gastaut syndrome:
Cassé-Perrot, C., Wolff, M., & Dravet, C. (2001). Comments on the syndrome’s terminology and noso-
Neuropsychological aspects of severe myoclonic epi- logical position among the secondary generalized epi-
lepsy in infancy. In I. Jambaqué, M. Lassonde, & lepsies of childhood. Electroencephalography and
O. Dulac (Eds.), The neuropsychology of childhood Clinical Neurophysiology, 35, S71–S84.
epilepsy (pp. 131–140). New York, NY: Plenum. Glaze, D. G., Schultz, R. J., & Frost, J. D. (1998). Rett
Cohen, M. (1997). Children’s memory scale manual. San syndrome: Characterization of seizures versus non-
Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. seizures. Electroencephalography and Clinical
Crawford, P. M. (2000). Epidemiology of intractable focal Neurophysiology, 106(1), 79–83.
epilepsy. In J. M. Oxbury, C. E. Polkey, & Gleissner, U., Clusmann, H., Sassen, R., Elger, C., &
M. Duchowny (Eds.), Intractable focal epilepsy Helmstaedter, C. (2006). Postsurgical outcome in
(pp. 25–40). London: WB Saunders. pediatric patients with epilepsy: A comparison of
de Vries, B. B., Robinson, H., Stolte-Dijkstra, I., Tjon patients with intellectual disabilities, subaverage intel-
Pian Gi, C. V., Dijkstra, P. F., van Doorn, J., et al. ligence, and average-range intelligence. Epilepsia,
(1995). General overgrowth in the fragile X syndrome: 47(2), 406–414.
Variability in the phenotypic expression of the FMR1 Gordon, N. (1990). Acquired aphasia in childhood: The
gene mutation. Journal of Medical Genetics, 32(10), Landau-Kleffner syndrome. Developmental Medicine
764–769. and Child Neurology, 32, 270–274.
Delis, D., Kramer, J., Kaplan, E., & Ober, B. (1986). The Guerrini, R., Cararozzo, R., Rinaldi, R., & Bonnani, P.
California Verbal Learning Test: Children’s version. (2003). Angelman syndrome: Etiology, clinical fea-
San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. tures, diagnosis, and management of symptoms.
Delis, D. C., Kramer, J. H., Kaplan, E., & Ober, B. A. Paediatric Drugs, 5(10), 647–661.
(2000). CVLT-II: California Verbal Learning Test, Harrison, P., & Oakland, T. (2003). ABAS-II: Adaptive
second edition, adult version, manual. San Antonio, behavior assessment system – Second edition. Los
TX: The Psychological Corporation. Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.
Dravet, C., & Bureau, C. (2008). Severe myoclonic epi- Hauser, W. A., Annegers, J. F., & Kurland, L. T. (1991).
lepsy in infancy (Dravet syndrome). In J. Engel Jr. & The Prevalence of epilepsy in Rochester, Minnesota:
T. A. Pedley (Eds.), Epilepsy: A comprehensive text- 1940–1980. Epilepsia, 32(4), 429–445.
book (2nd ed., pp. 2337–2342). Philadelphia, PA: Heaton, R. K., Miller, W., Taylor, M. J., & Grant, I.
Lippincott, Williams, & Wilkins. (2005). Revised comprehensive norms for an expanded
13 Developmentally Delayed Children and Adults 299

halstead-reitan battery: Demographically adjusted McLaren, J., & Bryson, S. E. (1987). Review of recent epi-
neuropsychological norms for African-American and demiological studies in mental retardation: Prevalence,
Caucasian Adults. Lutz, FL: Psychological associated disorders, and etiology. American Journal of
Assessment Resources. Mental Retardation, 92, 243–254.
Holmes, L. B., Wysznski, D. F., & Lieberman, E. (2004). The Meador, K.J., Loring, D.W., Hulihan, J.F., Kamin, M., &
AED (antiepileptic drug) pregnancy registry: A 6-year Karim, R. (2003). Differential cognitive and behav-
experience. Archives of Neurology, 61, 673–678. ioral effects of topiramate and valproate. Neurology,
Hooper, H. E. (1983). Hooper Visual Organization Test 60(9), 1483–1488.
(VOT) manual. Los Angeles, CA: Western Meador, K. J., Baker, G. A., Browning, N., Clayton Smith,
Psychological Services. J., Combs-Cantrell, D. T., Cohen, M., et al. (2009).
Hunt, A., & Dennis, J. (1987). Psychiatric disorder among Cognitive function at 3 years of age after fetal expo-
children with tuberous sclerosis. Developmental sure to antiepileptic drugs. The New England Journal
Medicine and Child Neurology, 29, 190–198. of Medicine, 360(16), 1597–1605.
Huppke, P., Held, M., Laccone, F., & Hanefeld, F. (2003). Meador, K. J., Loring, D. W., Vahle, V. J., Ray, P. G.,
The spectrum of phenotypes in females with Rett Werz, M. A., Fessler, A. J., et al. (2005). Differential
Syndrome. Brain Development, 25(5), 346–351. cognitive and behavioral side effects of topiramate and
Jambaqué, I., Chiron, C., Dumas, C., Mumford, J., & valproate. Neurology, 60, 1483–1488.
Dulac, O. (2000). Mental and behavioural outcome of Meyers, J. E., & Meyers, K. R. (1995). RCFT: Rey
infantile epilepsy treated by vigabatrin in tuberous scle- Complex Figure Test and Recognition Trial:
rosis patients. Epilepsy Research, 38(2–3), 151–160. Professional manual. Lutz, FL: Psychological
Jayakar, P. B., & Seshia, S. S. (1991). Electrical status epi- Assessment Resources, Inc.
lepticus during slow-wave sleep: A review. Journal of Motamedi, G. K., & Meador, K. J. (2004). Antiepileptic
Clinical Neurophysiology, 8, 299–311. drugs and memory. Epilepsy & Behavior, 5, 435–439.
Jeavons, P. M., Bower, B. D., & Dimitrakoudi, M. (1973). Murphy, J. V., & Dehkharghani, F. (1994). Diagnosis of
Long term prognosis of 150 cases of “West syndrome. childhood seizure disorders. Epilepsia, 35(Suppl 2),
Epilepsia, 14(2), 153–164. S7–S17.
Joinson, C., O’Callaghan, F. J., Osborne, J. P., Martyn, C., Panayiotopoulos, C. P. (2002). A clinical guide to epilep-
Harris, T., & Bolton, P. F. (2003). Learning disability tic syndromes and their treatment. Oxfordshire:
and epilepsy in an epidemiological sample of patients Bladon Medical Publishing.
with tuberous sclerosis complex. Psychological Paquier, P. F., Van Dongen, H. R., & Loonen, C. B. (1992).
Medicine, 33(2), 335–344. The Landau-Kleffner syndrome or acquired aphasia
Jokeit, H., & Ebner, A. (2002). Effects of chronic epilepsy with convulsive disorder. Archives of Neurology, 49,
on intellectual functions. Progress in Brain Research, 354–359.
135, 455–463. Pollack, M. A., Golden, G. S., Schmidt, R., Davis, J. A.,
Kaplan, E., Goodglass, H., & Weintraub, S. (2001). & Leeds, N. (1978). Infantile spasms in Down syn-
Boston Naming Test (2nd ed.). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. drome: A report of 5 cases and a review of the litera-
Kaufman, D. M. (2007). Clinical neurology for psychia- ture. Annals of Neurology, 3(5), 406–408.
trists (6th ed.). Philadelphia, PA: Saunders Elsevier. Prasher, V. P. (1995). Epilepsy and associated effects on
Lee, W. L., & Ong, H. T. (2001). Epidemiology of West adaptive behaviour in adults with Down syndrome.
syndrome in Singapore. Epilepsia, 23(7), 584–585. Seizure, 4, 53–56.
Lee, T. M., Yip, J. T., & Jones-Gotman, M. (2002). Randolph, C. (1998). RBANS: Repeatable battery for the
Memory deficits after resection from left or right ante- assessment of neuropsychological status: Manual. San
rior temporal lobe in humans: A meta-analytic review. Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
Epilepsia, 43(3), 283–291. Rantala, H., & Putkonen, T. (1999). Occurrence, outcome,
Lhatoo, S. D., & Sander, J. W. (2001). The epidemiology and prognostic factors of infantile spasms and Lennox-
of epilepsy and learning disability. Epilepsia, 42(Suppl Gastaut syndrome. Epilepsia, 40(3), 286–289.
1), 6–9. Roid, G. H., & Miller, L. J. (1997). Leiter International
Loeb, P. A. (1996). Independent Living Scales (ILS). San Performance Scale – Revised. Lutz, FL: Psychological
Antonio, TX: Pearson. Assessment Resources, Inc.
Loring, D. W., & Meador, K. J. (2001). Cognitive and Romano, C., Tiné, A., Fazio, G., Rizzo, R., Colognola,
behavioral effects of epilepsy treatment. Epilepsia, R. M., Sorge, G., et al. (1990). Seizures in patients
42(Suppl 8), 24–32. with trisomy 21. American Journal of Medical
Lúthvígsson, P., Olafsson, E., Sigurthardóttir, S., & Hauser, Genetics. Supplement, 7, 298–300.
W. A. (1994). Epidemiologic features of infantile Sabaratnam, M., Vroegop, P. G., & Gangadharan, S. K.
spasms in Iceland. Epilepsia, 35(45), 802–805. (2001). Epilepsy and EEG findings in 18 males with
Martin, R., Kuzniecky, R., Ho, S., Hetherington, H., Pan, fragile X syndrome. Seizure, 10(1), 60–63.
J., Sinclair, K., et al. (1999). Cognitive effects of topi- Sauerwein, H. C., Gallagher, A., & Lassonde, M. (2005).
ramate, gabapentin, and lamotrigine in healthy young Neuropsychological deficits in children with temporal
adults. Neurology, 52, 321–327. lobe epilepsy. In A. Arzimanoglou, A. Aldenkamp,
H. Cross, M. Lassonde, S. Moshé, & B. Schmitz
300 E.M. Brandling-Bennett

(Eds.), Cognitive dysfunction in children with tempo- verbal fluency: The effects of focal frontal- and
ral lobe epilepsy (pp. 1–12). Paris: John Libbey temporal-lobe lesions. Neuropsychologia, 40(5),
Eurotext. 562–566.
Schouten, D., Hendriksen, J. G., & Aldenkamp, A. P. Viani, F., Romeo, A., Viri, M., Mastrangelo, M., Lalatta,
(2009). Performance of children with epilepsy on the F., Selicorni, A., et al. (1995). Seizure and EEG pat-
Rey-Osterrieth complex figure test: Is there an effect terns in Angelman’s syndrome. Journal of Child
of localization or lateralization? Epilepsy Research, Neurology, 10(6), 467–471.
83(2–3), 184–189. Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of
Shahar, E., Barak, S., Andraus, J., & Kramer, U. (2004). Intelligence: Administration and scoring manual. San
Primary generalized epilepsy during infancy and early Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
childhood. Journal of Child Neurology, 19(3), 170–174. Wechsler, D. (1997). WMS-III: Wechsler Memory
Sheslow, D., & Adams, W. (2003). WRAML2: Wide range Scale – Third edition: Administration and scoring
assessment of memory and learning: Administration manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological
and scoring manual. Lutz, FL: Psychological Corporation.
Assessment Resources, Inc. Wechsler, D. (2002). Wechsler Preschool and Primary
Shouse, M. N., & Quigg, M. S. (2008). Chronobiology. In Scale of Intelligence – Third edition: Administration
J. Engel Jr. & T. A. Pedley (Eds.), Epilepsy: A compre- and scoring manual. San Antonio, TX: The
hensive textbook (2nd ed., pp. 1961–1974). Psychological Corporation.
Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott, Williams, & Wilkins. Wechsler, D. (2003). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for
Sparrow, S., Cicchetti, D., & Balla, D. (2005). Vineland Children – Fourth edition: Administration and scoring
Adaptive Behavior Scales – Second edition. San manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological
Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. Corporation.
Steffenburg, U., Hagberg, G., & Hagberg, B. (2001). Wechsler, D. (2008). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale –
Epilepsy in a representative series of Rett syndrome. Fourth edition: Administration and scoring manual.
Acta Paediatrica, 90(1), 34–39. San Antonio, TX: NCS Pearson, Inc.
Stern, R. A., & White, T. (2003). NAB: Neuropsychological Wechsler, D. (2009). Wechsler Memory Scale – Fourth
assessment battery. Lutz, FL: Psychological edition. San Antonio, TX: NCS Pearson, Inc.
Assessment Resources, Inc. Welsh, M. C., Pennington, B. F., & Groisser, D. B. (1991).
Su, C. Y., Lin, Y. H., Wu, Y. Y., & Chen, C. C. (2008). The A normative-developmental study of executive func-
role of cognition and adaptive behavior in employ- tion: A window on prefrontal function in children.
ment of people with mental retardation. Research in Developmental Neuropsychology, 7(22), 131–149.
Developmental Disabilities, 29(1), 83–95. Wiebe, S. (2000). Epidemiology of temporal lobe epi-
Tangye, S. R. (1979). The EEG and incidence of epilepsy lepsy. Canadian Journal of Neurological Sciences,
in Down’s syndrome. Journal of Mental Deficiency 27(Suppl 1), S6–S10.
Research, 23(1), 17–24. Williams, K. T. (2007). EVT-2: Expressive Vocabulary
Tatsuno, M., Hayashi, M., Iwamoto, H., Suzuki, Y., & Test, second edition. San Antonio, TX: Pearson
Kuroki, Y. (1984). Epilepsy in childhood Down syn- Assessments.
drome. Brain Development, 6(1), 37–44. Williams, J., & Sharp, G. B. (2000). Epilepsy. In K. O.
Trevathan, E., Murphy, C. C., & Yeargin-Allsopp, M. Yeates, M. D. Ris, & H. G. Taylor (Eds.), Pediatric
(1997). Prevalence and descriptive epidemiology of neuropsychology: Research, theory, and practice
Lennox-Gastaut syndrome among Atlanta children. (pp. 47–73). New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
Epilepsia, 38(12), 1283–1288. Wolff, M., Cassé-Perrot, C., & Dravet, C. (2001).
Trevathan, E., Murphy, C. C., & Yeargin-Allsopp, M. (1999). Neuropsychological disorders in children with severe
The descriptive epidemiology of infantile spasms among myoclonic epilepsy. Epilepsia, 42(Suppl 2), 61.
Atlanta children. Epilepsia, 40(6), 748–751. World Health Organization. (1993). International
Troyer, A. K., Moscovitch, M., Winocur, G., Alexander, Classification of Diseases: 10th revision. Ann Arbor, MI:
M. P., & Stuss, D. (2002). Clustering and switching on Commission on Professional and Hospital Activities.
The Neuropsychological
Evaluation of High-Functioning 14
Patients with Epilepsy

Sarah G. Schaffer

Epilepsy is a heterogeneous disorder with Along these lines, in many professions the
countless possible presentations. Knowing that competition for advancement can be fierce
someone has a diagnosis of epilepsy tells you (e.g., law, finance, academia). While any “edge”
very little about that person’s experience. There can bump a person up a rung on the ladder toward
are numerous factors that might moderate the success, the inverse is also true. The legality of
impact of this diagnosis on any one person’s life. denying someone a job or a promotion because of
Issues directly related to the occurrence of factors related to medical status is not in dispute.
seizures might include type/severity of seizures, But whether this is “legal” or not is irrelevant, as
frequency with which they occur, predictability this is reality. While the stigma associated with
of occurrence (e.g., only at night, certain time of epilepsy might result in discrimination based on
the month), presence or absence of warning signs faulty perceptions by employers and co-workers,
or “aura,” level of postictal debilitation, and fre- the direct consequences of uncontrolled seizures,
quency of injury (e.g., tongue biting, falls, etc.). such as lost days of work and mental clouding
However, there are also secondary factors to be associated with antiepileptic medications, might
considered that are unique to each individual. be just as counterproductive to employment
For example, not being able to drive might be goals. Furthermore, most people with refractory
more of a burden on a mother with young epilepsy are on high doses of antiepileptic
children living in a rural community than it medications and/or polytherapy (i.e., prescribed
would on a businesswoman living in a major multiple medications), which increases the likeli-
metropolitan area with a well-connected public hood of such adverse medication side effects.
transportation system. Conversely, while that In light of these issues, it becomes clear that
same businesswoman might not be impacted by the aim of neuropsychological evaluations of
her inability to drive, the cognitive impact of patients with epilepsy is multifactorial.
seizures and the cognitive side effects of medica- Presurgical evaluations do not simply boil down
tions might make it difficult for her to complete to lateralization of seizure focus and prediction
her work-related activities. of postsurgical cognitive decline. Rather, a
holistic approach that accounts for the full
S.G. Schaffer, Ph.D., A.B.P.P.-C.N. (*) spectrum of cognitive and psychosocial issues
Comprehensive Epilepsy Care Center, Cushing unique to each patient is required. As comprehen-
Neuroscience Institute, Hofstra North Shore-LIJ
sive neuropsychological evaluations involve
School of Medicine, 611 Northern Blvd, Suite 150,
Great Neck, NY 11021, USA examination of both cognitive and “psychological”
e-mail: sschaffer1@nshs.edu factors that impact upon a person’s overall level of

W.B. Barr and C. Morrison (eds.), Handbook on the Neuropsychology of Epilepsy, 301
Clinical Handbooks in Neuropsychology, DOI 10.1007/978-0-387-92826-5_14,
© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015
302 S.G. Schaffer

functioning, the neuropsychologist might be more with magnification of subjective cognitive


likely to consider the entire range of issues that impairment in this population. The goal of such a
are coming into play and as such might be in the review will be to delineate for the clinician the
best position to advocate on the patient’s behalf. potential pitfalls to be aware of when attempting
To this end, it is also helpful to know some of the to identify the salient issues to address when
issues that are common among specific subsets of rendering conclusions. The second part of this
patients. This chapter aims to delineate such chapter focuses on specific issues related to the
potential issues as they pertain to high-functioning comprehensive neuropsychological evaluation of
patients, that is, those who have demonstrated an high-functioning epilepsy patients.
ability to maintain active professional careers.

Cognitive Impact
Consequences of Epilepsy
Numerous studies have shown that cognitive
It is well established in the literature that seizures impairments in people with chronic epilepsy
can have an adverse impact on brain functions. extend beyond memory difficulties to include
The extent to which patients actually experience problems related to attention and concentration,
declines in cognition due to seizure activity slower information processing and psychomotor
depends on a number of factors, including speed, language deficits, and executive dysfunc-
seizure type (focal vs. generalized), seizure fre- tion when compared to performances of normal
quency, age of onset, and number of years with controls on objective neuropsychological tests.
seizures. There are several “secondary” factors The poorest cognition is associated with early
(i.e., those not directly related to seizures) that age at seizure onset and, similarly, longer dura-
can also impact cognition, including the presence tion of epilepsy, especially in the presence of a
of underlying psychopathology (which is seen at history of generalized tonic–clonic seizures,
higher rates in epilepsy patients than in the repeated episodes of status epilepticus, and
general population), the presence of a lesion, and increased exposure to antiepileptic drugs (AEDs).
medication side effects. There is considerable The age of seizure onset represents a particularly
evidence to suggest that these factors work in important variable related to degree of cognitive
concert with each other to produce the subjective impairment. Childhood-onset temporal lobe epi-
experience of cognitive decline that is so often lepsy has been associated with a generalized
reported in this patient population. adverse neurodevelopmental impact on brain
The most common cognitive complaint in structure and function (Hermann et al. 2002;
patients with epilepsy is that of memory difficul- Hermann, Seidenberg, & Bell, 2002). Late-onset
ties. Such complaints are of course validated by patients exhibit considerably fewer volumetric
decades of research showing the selective impact and cognitive abnormalities compared with
of seizures on memory functioning. Other com- healthy controls despite a history of chronic tem-
mon complaints include mental slowing and poral lobe epilepsy (16.2 years). Hermann and
problems with attention. While the impact of epi- colleagues (2007) performed a cluster analysis
lepsy on neuropsychological functioning is well using comprehensive neuropsychological data
established in the research literature, it should be from 96 patients with chronic temporal lobe
noted that a number of studies have found that epilepsy and 85 healthy controls, and three
subjective complaints do not always correlate distinct cognitive subgroups or phenotypes were
with objective impairments. The first part of this identified: (1) minimally impaired (47 %), (2)
chapter provides a brief review of the literature memory impaired (24 %), and (3) memory, exec-
related to cognitive and psychosocial functioning utive, and speed impaired (29 %). Common
in patients with epilepsy, as well as a brief intro- characteristics of patients in the most cognitively
duction of the various factors that are associated impaired group (Cluster 3) included older age at
14 The Neuropsychological Evaluation of High-Functioning Patients with Epilepsy 303

evaluation, longest duration of epilepsy, higher not only the risks and benefits of going forward
number of antiepileptic medications, and more with surgery but also the risks associated with
abnormal brain volumes (total, white matter and ongoing seizure activity should they choose not
CSF). They also showed the most adverse cogni- to move forward. In order to do this, one must be
tive course, especially when compared to Cluster familiar with the literature regarding the cogni-
1. Although not significant, there were meaningful tive prognosis associated with chronic epilepsy.
trends related to other clinical seizure variables in There are some in the field who propose a neu-
the Cluster 3 group, including greater number of rodegenerative model when characterizing the
lifetime GTCs, history of status epilepticus, progression of cognitive decline associated with
and presence of initial precipitating injuries. ongoing seizures. In a large cross-sectional study
Consistent with results from their earlier study specifically intended to examine this issue,
regarding the differential impact of earlier age of Helmstaedter and Elger (2009) compared the
onset on cognitive prognosis, Cluster 3 was char- memory profiles of patients with chronic intrac-
acterized by earlier neurodevelopmental insult. It table temporal lobe epilepsy at varying stages of
should be noted that although Cluster 1 exhibited life to those of healthy controls. They found an
the most intact cognition of the three groups, initial discrepancy in the acquisition of learning
their performance on measures of language, and memory skills during childhood and particu-
immediate and delayed memory, executive func- larly during adolescence that resulted in an ear-
tion, and psychomotor speed was nevertheless lier “learning peak” in the epilepsy patients
significantly worse than that of controls. (16–17 years vs. 23–24 years). Subsequent
The inverse relationship between age of onset declines in performance related to normal aging
and degree of cognitive decline can likely be ran in parallel (i.e., there was not a steeper decline
attributed to that fact that there is greater oppor- in the epilepsy group), but because of the initial
tunity for acquisition of skills in childhood prior distance in memory abilities between the groups,
to seizure onset in those patients who develop those with epilepsy reached very poor perfor-
seizures later. It is, thus, noteworthy that later age mance levels much earlier than controls. It was
of onset has also been associated with increased concluded that although there does not appear to
risk of postsurgical decline. This might reflect the be a progressive and/or accelerated decline
fact that although these patients may have avoided related to chronic seizures in patients with TLE,
the neurodevelopmental “hit” seen in patients the negative interaction of the initially estab-
with younger onset, thereby resulting in a higher lished damage with the normal aging process
level of functioning at the time of surgical inter- results in reduced reserve capacities at an older
vention, this increases the likelihood that the epi- age (Helmstaedter & Elger, 2009). These results
leptogenic zone involves functional tissue. support the notion that poor cognitive function-
Studies have indicated that cognitive decline is ing is related to neurodevelopmental processes in
determined more by the amount of functional tissue those patients with earlier age of onset.
resected than the ability for remaining tissue to Furthermore, they suggest that rather than bene-
support cognitive functions (Chelune, 1995). fiting from the increased plasticity that is com-
monly ascribed to the developing brain, the
Cognitive Prognosis Associated presence of recurrent seizures during develop-
with Chronic TLE ment appears to be associated with an adverse
When counseling patients who are in the process effect on both brain structure and function.
of deciding whether or not to undergo surgical In his review of the literature on cognitive
intervention of intractable seizures, there are progression in epilepsy, Seidenberg noted that
many issues that must be considered. However, duration of epilepsy appears to be the most reli-
with high-functioning patients, none is more able predictor of cognitive decline. However, he
relevant than the potential impact on cognition. further pointed out that duration of epilepsy
Therefore, it is necessary to be able to communicate likely signifies the presence of a more complex
304 S.G. Schaffer

and/or severe epilepsy syndrome. At the very presurgical cognitive deficits. Stated another
least, the duration of epilepsy increases the pos- way, those with normal neurocognitive profiles
sibility of exposure to factors that result in neuro- going into surgery have more to lose. Helmstaedter
nal damage (Seidenberg, Pulsipher, & Hermann, pointed out in his discussion of the neuropsycho-
2007). Hermann and colleagues looked at the logical aspects of epilepsy surgery (2004) that
cognitive trajectories of patients with chronic although patients with better baseline memory
TLE and found that most patients (70–75 %) show greater losses than patients with poor
appeared to have an unproblematic prospective baseline performance, those who are better
cognitive course. Skills that were more vulnera- preoperatively will nevertheless be better postop-
ble to decline (i.e., decline seen in >25 % of eratively. That being said, for some patients the
patients) included confrontation naming, delayed prospect of even mild memory decline might be
visual memory, delayed verbal memory, and unacceptable. Indeed, this issue might be of
bilateral motor speed (Hermann, Seidenberg, particular concern for the population under
Dow, & et al., 2006). In general, the patients with consideration in the current chapter, as a general
adverse cognitive outcomes were older, with assumption would be that their status as “profes-
longer duration of epilepsy, lower baseline full- sional/high-functioning” individuals almost by
scale IQ, and quantitative MRI abnormalities at definition implies a certain degree of intact cog-
baseline. Baseline volumetric abnormalities and nition (or rather precludes the presence of signifi-
lower IQ were the strongest predictors of abnor- cant and/or widespread impairments). However,
mal trajectories in IQ, language, perception, it is possible that the relative stability of cognitive
memory, executive skills, and motor coordina- functions seen in these patients can be attributed
tion over a 4-year period. to the development and/or use of compensatory
The findings that patients who exhibited strategies that are made possible because of
significant adverse cognitive outcomes were higher cognitive reserve seen in these patients.
characterized by significantly lower baseline
full-scale IQ are consistent with the general Cognitive Effects of Antiepileptic
notion of increased cognitive vulnerability Medications
among those with lower intellectual capacity. In a Problems with learning and cognition have fre-
study that examined long-term outcomes in indi- quently been cited among the primary disadvan-
viduals who had childhood-onset epilepsy now tages of having epilepsy (Hayden, Penna, &
aged 20 years and older, Wakamoto and colleagues Buchanan, 1992), and the majority of patients with
concluded that patients who had normal intelli- epilepsy attribute their impairments to the side
gence had more favorable prognosis (Wakamoto, effects of their antiepileptic medication (Carpay,
Nagao, Hayashi, & Morimoto, 2000). It stands to Aldenkamp, & van Donselaar, 2005). There are
reason that high-functioning patients have some general patterns to follow when considering
avoided the neurodevelopmental hit that appears the potential impact of medication side effects on
to characterize those patients with poorer base- cognition. Newer agents tend to produce fewer
line functioning, thereby allowing them to side effects than older drugs (e.g., carbamazepine
acquire the skills needed for a stronger cognitive and phenobarbital), with the exception of topira-
foundation. Interestingly, and perhaps more mate (Topamax), which is a newer medication that
germane to the goals of this chapter, this pattern has consistently been shown to have an adverse
is opposite of that observed following anterior impact on cognition. Patients on topiramate are
temporal lobectomy for the treatment of intrac- more likely to discontinue this medication due to
table seizures. That is, those with the least amount cognitive side effects. Regardless of the type of
of cognitive impairment going into surgery expe- medication used, there is a dose-related effect on
rience the greatest postsurgical declines (at least cognition (even at therapeutic blood serum levels),
with respect to verbal memory), whereas the with polytherapy resulting in more adverse effects
inverse is true of individuals with significant than monotherapy.
14 The Neuropsychological Evaluation of High-Functioning Patients with Epilepsy 305

Unfortunately, given that surgery is considered Given these findings, it is important to try to
for patients who are refractory to medications, tease out the relative contributions of each factor
these patients are more likely to be prescribed to the overall profile, using validated neuropsy-
high doses and/or multiple medications, and in chological and psychological measures, and to
fact for many the desire to reduce the number of counsel the patient when discrepancies between
medications is a primary motivation in seeking objective impairment and subjective experience
surgery. Uijl and colleagues found that among arise.
173 patients who were well controlled on
medication, 67 % nevertheless reported moder-
ate to severe subjective complaints. Cognitive Psychosocial Impact
complaints were reported most frequently, and
the prevalence and severity of complaints were The psychosocial impact of epilepsy can be
associated with AED polytherapy and higher profound. Results from multiple population-
scores on “psycho neuroticism” on the Dutch based surveillance studies representing nearly
version of the Symptom Check List (SCL-90) half of the United States indicated that compared
(Uijl et al., 2006). The relationship of self-reported with healthy controls, adults with a history of
complaints and high levels of neuroticism has active epilepsy consistently reported higher rates
been reported in other studies as well (Vermeulen, of unemployment, lower income (<$25 K), lower
Aldenkamp, & Alpherts, 1993). education, and being single compared with
people without epilepsy. Furthermore, those
Subjective Cognitive Complaints patients with the most active epilepsy (i.e.,
While the potential impact of chronic epilepsy on seizures within the past 3 months) were most
neuropsychological status is not in dispute, likely to report more mentally and physically
patients’ complaints of cognitive impairment (i.e., unhealthy days and more limitations on activ-
their subjective experience regarding the severity ity (Kobau et al., 2004, 2007, 2008; Kobau,
and functional impact of such impairments) are Gilliam, & Thurman, 2006; Tellez-Zenteno,
not always borne out via objective neuropsycho- Patten, Jette, Williams, & Wiebe, 2007).
logical evaluation. Numerous studies have shown In a large population-based study conducted
that self-reported cognitive complaints do not gen- in the United Kingdom, psychiatric disorders
erally correlate with actual impairments. In gen- were found to occur twice as often in patients
eral, research has supported an inverse relationship with epilepsy as in the general population
between subjective and objective impairment. (Gaitatzis, Carroll, Majeed, & W Sander, 2004).
That is, more severe cognitive complaints are Depression is the most frequently diagnosed psy-
associated with milder impairment on objective chiatric disorder among people with epilepsy.
testing, and vise versa. One study that examined The factors that have been associated with the
the hypothesis that more complaints might be increased risk of depression in this patient
seen in patients with higher cognitive demands in population include employment status, social
daily life found the inverse to be true (although support, stigma, self-management, financial
the effect was mild; Gleissner, Helmstaedter, strain, and activity restriction due to seizures
Quiske, & Elger, 1998). Several studies have indi- (Reisinger & DiIorio, 2009). In a survey of 503
cated a much stronger relationship between sub- patients with intractable epilepsy, almost half
jective complaints and mood than is seen between (46 %) of those surveyed agreed that epilepsy
either subjective and objective cognition or mood often caused them to suffer from depression
and objective cognitive performance (Hayden (Wheless, 2006). Patients with intractable tempo-
et al., 1992; Liik, Vahterb, Gross-Pajub, & ral lobe epilepsy with the additional syndrome of
Haldrea, 2009; Marino, Meador, Loring, Okun, mesial temporal sclerosis have been found to be
Fernandez, Fessler, et al., 2009; Piazzini, at an even higher risk of developing psychiatric
Canevini, Maggiori, & Canger, 2001). disorders, with lifetime prevalence rates up to
306 S.G. Schaffer

80 % (Gaitatzis, Trimble, & Sander, 2004; of seizure worry and employment status, self-
LaFrance, Kanner, & Hermann, 2008)! Not only efficacy and social support, and quality care and
is the lifetime prevalence of major depression in age at seizure onset (Smith et al., 2009).
epilepsy patients far greater than that estimated Regarding employment status, the highest level
for the general population (5–25 % according the of stigma was reported by individuals who were
DSM-IV-TR), but it is almost double the esti- disabled or unemployed and reported a higher
mated prevalence rate of 20–25 % that is cited in level of seizure worry, followed by those who
the DSM-IV-TR as occurring in other chronic or were disabled or unemployed but reported a
severe general medical conditions (e.g., diabetes, lower level of seizure worry; the lowest level of
myocardial infarction, carcinomas, stroke). These stigma was reported by those who were employed
statistics are all the more concerning given con- or “other” (not working but student, housewife,
sistent evidence implicating depression as one of retired, etc.) and reported lower level of seizure
the strongest predictors of reduced quality of life worry. It should be noted that people with epi-
in patients with refractory epilepsy (Perrine et al., lepsy do not universally feel stigmatized by their
1995 ). Despite the focus on depression in disorder. In an older surveillance study conducted
epilepsy over the past decade, studies indicate by Ryan, Kempner, and Emlen (1980), 81 % of
that it is woefully underdiagnosed and largely the 445 respondents felt that they had been treated
untreated (Boylan et al., 2004). fairly in society, and 70 % felt neither unreason-
ably limited nor treated differently because of
Quality of Life their epilepsy (Ryan et al., 1980).
While depression has been identified as an Researchers have also been concerned with
overarching factor related to poor outcome in identifying those factors that might be associ-
patients with epilepsy, quality-of-life research ated with better psychosocial adjustment in
has uncovered a variety of specific factors that patients with epilepsy. Resilience, for example,
are associated with patients’ overall sense of has been found to be associated with better
well-being. Perceived stigma has emerged as one QOL. Sociodemographic characteristics such as
of the primary issues leading to reduced quality gender, education, and income level have been
of life in epilepsy patients. Goffman conceptual- found to be highly predictive of resilience, with
ized stigma as “a loss of status and power result- poorer resilience manifested by women and
ing from the separation of those stigmatized from individuals who had lower levels of education
the general population based on a characteristic and lower income (Campbell-Sills, Forde, &
that has been culturally defined as different and Stein, 2009). There is a positive association
undesirable” (Goffman, 1963). In epilepsy between resilience and cognitive reserve (as indi-
patients, stigma is associated with low self- cated by higher educational level or occupational
esteem, self-efficacy, and sense of mastery, per- attainment and increased participation in mindful
ceived helplessness, increased rates of anxiety activities). The well-established relationship
and depression, increased somatic symptomol- between generalized cognitive impairment and
ogy, and reduced life satisfaction (Jacoby, Snape, duration of epilepsy has been shown to be attenu-
& Baker, 2009). In a study out of the Netherlands, ated (to nonsignificant levels) by having more
stigma accounted for twice the amount of vari- years of formal education (Oyegbile et al., 2004).
ance in QOL scores as did clinical variables such The authors suggest that years of education may,
as seizure frequency and antiepileptic drug side in fact, be a marker “for those who, at the outset
effects, and it was identified as the fourth most of the disorder, are on different trajectories
important factor in determining QOL after regarding educational attainment and lifespan
psychological distress, loneliness, and adjustment cognition.” In fact, the role of educational level as
(Suurmeijer, Reuvekamp, & Aldenkamp, 2001). a predictor of QOL in adults has been emphasized
Results of a recent study showed that reported in a number of studies (Loring, Meador, & Lee,
levels of stigma were associated with interactions 2004; Pulsipher, Seidenberg, Jones, & Hermann,
14 The Neuropsychological Evaluation of High-Functioning Patients with Epilepsy 307

2006). Socioeconomic status has also been lished. Some argue that the stigma associated
identified as an important predictor of QOL in with having a diagnosis of epilepsy is one of the
patients with epilepsy (Alanis-Guevara et al., most critical barriers to employment. Stigma is
2005), and Senol and colleagues found that while associated not only with discriminatory prac-
income and education predict overall QOL, only tices, but patients often internalize the negative
income was found to significantly predict mental attitudes of others and come to view their epi-
and physical health and cognitive function scores lepsy in a similar light. Because of this potential
in a multiple regression analysis (Senol, Soyuer, for discrimination, people with epilepsy are often
Arman, & Ozturk, 2007). Therefore, in high- hesitant to disclose their epilepsy status to
functioning adults, the potential for loss of employers. Problems at work can lead to a lack
income has far-reaching implications. of opportunities for career advancement, and
studies suggest that many people with epilepsy
Impact on Employment do not reach the employment potential corre-
In a review of the literature regarding employ- sponding to their qualifications and age.
ment and epilepsy, Smeets et al. (2007) noted that In a study that examined the employment
employment is seen by many as an important trajectories of individuals with single seizures or
ingredient of the quality of life of people with recently diagnosed epilepsy, significantly lower
epilepsy. Employment is considered to be a sig- employment rates were seen at study entry and
nificant predictor of well-being of people with 2- and 4-year follow-up than were seen for the
epilepsy and a very important factor in psycho- general population (Holland, Lane, Whitehead,
social adjustment. In a large population-based Marson, & Jacoby, 2009). Having fair/poor
study by Jacoby (1995), employed people with self-rated health and experiencing four or more
epilepsy experienced fewer psychosocial prob- seizures predicted nonemployment at all time
lems than unemployed people with epilepsy points. Although there was little social class
(Jacoby, 1995). There is little agreement in the mobility during follow-up, there was evidence of
literature on specific unemployment and under- some downward mobility between first seizure(s)
employment rates, which vary widely between and study entry.
communities and countries. It is generally Wheless and colleagues reported findings
accepted, however, that people with epilepsy are from a large-scale survey that focused solely on
more likely to be employed in unskilled and the experiences, attitudes, and quality of life of a
manual jobs. Some studies suggest a relationship refractory epilepsy population (Wheless, 2006).
between seizure type and employment status, The resulting data represented three groups of
with increased likelihood of being unemployed participants (n = 903): those with epilepsy who
for patients classified as having GTC seizures self-reported on their condition (Group 1,
(Jacoby, Baker, Steen, Potts, & Chadwick, 1996) n = 503), the caregivers of those with refractory
and decreased likelihood of attributing current epilepsy (Group 2, n = 200), and those with epi-
employment situation to epilepsy in patients who lepsy who had their condition reported on by a
were in remission with respect to seizure activity caregiver (Group 3, n = 200). The unpredictable
(Chaplin, Wester, & Tomson, 1998). Although nature of seizure occurrence was responsible for
seizure frequency is an important factor deter- much of the fear and uncertainty associated with
mining employment status, it may not be the epilepsy in both patients and caregivers. Epilepsy
most important factor to consider. Other factors was blamed for the loss of many of the everyday
that can play an important role in predicting activities most people take for granted, such as
employment status include medication side independence, a social life, education, work,
effects, stigma, and psychosocial variables such relationships, respect, and oftentimes a future.
as low self-esteem, passive coping style, and low It was noted that this survey sample was surpris-
self-efficacy. The impact of employment status ingly well educated compared to a prior survey
on self-esteem and self-image has been estab- conducted by Fisher and colleagues (2000), with
308 S.G. Schaffer

approximately 64 % of those with epilepsy from with epilepsy and the tendency of this population
Group 1 having graduated from high school and to use of passive coping strategies to manage
26 % having received a college undergraduate or their disabilities can generate a vicious cycle,
graduate degree. Despite the high level of educa- especially with respect to employment. High
tion in this survey, 58 % of those in Group 1 were self-efficacy and an active coping strategy often
not employed outside the home or were unable to result in successful psychosocial adaptation to
work. They obtained an unemployment rate of the disorder. Thus, self-efficacy beliefs and active
29 % when homemakers, students, or volunteer coping strategies might play a key role in helping
workers were excluded from the equation, which people with epilepsy to enhance their employ-
is higher than the 25 % unemployment rate ment opportunities.
reported by Fisher et al. (2000). Based on their literature review, Smeets et al.
Sillanpaa and colleagues recently reported emphasized the important role that coping strat-
results from a prospective longitudinal study egy, self-efficacy, and social support play in
aimed at determining the long-term employment accepting and managing the psychosocial conse-
and predictive factors in adults with childhood- quences or effects of having epilepsy. They note
onset epilepsy living in the community (Sillanpaa that specific attention should be focused on recent
& Schmidt, 2010). At the mean age of 23 years, European studies that reveal that people with epi-
85 (71 %) of the 119 patients living in the com- lepsy who develop more active coping strategies
munity were employed. At the mean age of 48 have fewer psychosocial problems. Research
years, only 45 (59 %) of 76 patients living in the findings further indicate that coping strategies
community were employed, compared to 63 play an important role in adaptation to epilepsy.
(78 %) of 81 controls (patients vs. controls,
p = 0.01). The variables that predicted employ-
ment in young adults included normal intelligence, Neuropsychological Evaluation
vocational education, and age at onset of epilepsy of High-Functioning Individuals
older than 6 years, whereas those that predicted with Careers
lasting employment into middle age included
normal intelligence, having offspring, uninter- Many of the issues to be considered when con-
rupted remission, and no history of status ducting neuropsychological evaluations with
epilepticus. high-functioning epilepsy patients overlap with
There are several factors that can moderate the those that come up in the general discussion of
impact of epilepsy on employment outcomes, neuropsychological evaluation of epilepsy
including personality (e.g., coping strategies and patients. Some of these issues will be addressed
self-efficacy), social support, and intellectual briefly, but for a more comprehensive discussion,
abilities. Perceptions regarding self-efficacy can the reader is referred to Chap. 2 (Neuro-
play a significant role in either mitigating or psychological Testing of Patients with Epilepsy)
exacerbating the impact of epilepsy on daily and Chap. 3 (Neuropsychological Evaluation of
functioning. How effective an individual with the Surgical Candidate).
epilepsy perceives him- or herself to be in coping
with the everyday problems of life is based on
self-generated core beliefs that are central to this Seizure-Related Issues
process (Tedman, Thornton, & Baker, 1995), yet
study findings indicated that the physical effects Obtaining detailed information regarding seizure-
of seizures, the unpredictable nature of epilepsy, related variables should be a top priority in every
and adverse social reactions can exhaust the epilepsy evaluation, particularly given their
coping resources of people with epilepsy. The association with both cognitive and psychosocial
combination of low self-efficacy seen in people status. However, some of these variables are
14 The Neuropsychological Evaluation of High-Functioning Patients with Epilepsy 309

more relevant to high-functioning patients than observed by others, is important insofar as it can
others. For example, age of seizure onset and not only provide key information regarding local-
duration of epilepsy are good prognostic indica- ization of onset and general severity of events but
tors with respect to both current and long-term also the potential impact that might be associated
cognitive status, with earlier onset and longer with having a seizure at work (e.g., do others
duration being associated with a greater likeli- notice, is there a lapse in awareness, can the
hood of current cognitive deficits and future patient resume normal activities?). Although not
decline. Hence, the expectation would be that necessarily a “seizure-related” variable per se,
high-functioning patients might, as a general information regarding medications, including
rule, present with later age of onset. Conversely, type(s), dosage, and perceived side effects, is
knowing that a patient’s seizures started later in necessary when determining potential causes of
life might lead to an initial hypothesis regarding any cognitive deficits observed on objective
potential cognitive status. That being said, later testing.
age of onset does not always mean better out- It is also important to understand the perceived
come. While this relationship is more likely in impact that these factors have on the patient’s
patients for whom a precipitating cause cannot overall quality of life. What does the patient
be identified (epilepsies of unknown cause), later consider to be the most impairing aspect of
age of onset might also indicate the presence of having epilepsy? How does having a diagnosis of
an underlying neurologic process (e.g., neo- epilepsy impact their day-to-day experience? In
plasm) or injury (e.g., stroke or TBI) that led to assessing the impact of epilepsy from the patient’s
the secondary development of seizures, which perspective, it is important to keep in mind
would obviously complicate the clinical picture research findings regarding the effect that patient
quite a bit. In such cases, the clinician must con- perceptions can have on overall well-being. For
sider not only the impact of current seizure activ- example, perceived stigma is associate with poor
ity on cognition but also the cognitive factors employment outcomes, and perceived cognitive
associated with the precipitating insult. Although impairment has been associated with reduced
the current discussion will be limited to those quality of life. Regarding the latter, one must also
patients who present with “typical” epilepsy syn- keep in mind that subjective complaints do not
dromes, it should be clear that there are a variety generally coincide with actual impairment, and in
of factors that might alter the approach taken by fact when reported complaints exceed actual defi-
the clinician and the importance of identifying cits, one might suspect the presence of an under-
such factors early on in an evaluation cannot be lying mood disorder that needs to be further
overstated. explored.
Seizure frequency is also important to con-
sider, particularly given its differential impact on
psychosocial status and overall quality of life Employment-Related Issues
relative to other seizure variables. Seizure fre-
quency is also related to seizure worry, which has Epilepsy is related to poor employment outcomes
been associated with reduced occupational func- compared to the general population, and employ-
tioning. Therefore, in addition to identifying ment status is considered to be an important
specific data regarding seizure frequency, it is factor in psychosocial adjustment; employed
important to assess the patient’s perception of people experience fewer psychosocial problems
their seizure frequency. Characterization of than unemployed people with epilepsy. Therefore,
seizure semiology, including both the patient’s evaluations of patients with professional careers
own experience of the seizures (e.g., presence of should be completed with an eye toward ensuring
aura/warning, awareness during the actual event, ongoing employment, and risks to employment
and postictal symptoms) and the behaviors status should be identified early and addressed
310 S.G. Schaffer

accordingly. The factors that might threaten the patient’s attitude about the diagnosis, and
employment status include impact of seizures on how does he or she think it has affected the ability
job attendance, frequency of seizures (at work or to advance in his/her current field? Higher level
otherwise), unpredictability of seizures, potential of reported stigma is seen in unemployed patients
for job-related injury, impact of medication on with high seizure worry, whereas the lowest
cognition, and perceived stigma associated with stigma is reported by employed patients with low
the diagnosis of epilepsy. seizure worry. Perceived stigma and seizure
When assessing the potential impact of epi- frequency might, therefore, be of particular
lepsy on job performance, the clinician must first importance when attempting to delineate the fac-
consider the full range of factors and associated tors that might lead to a decline in employment
consequences that might result from seizure activ- status. Of note, this might not be as much of a
ity. For example, do the seizures always occur at concern in patients who have achieved a high
the same time of day (e.g., at night, when the risk level of career success given the association
of others witnessing the events would be slim), or between occupational attainment, income level,
are the unpredictable? Is the patient worried about and resilience, the latter of which has been asso-
the possibility of having a seizure at work? If they ciated with better quality of life.
have occurred at work, what were the circum-
stances surrounding the event(s) and what was the
outcome (both real and perceived)? Are the sei- Cognitive Issues
zures associated with injury that might make it
difficult to attend or complete work? Similarly, is In the absence of psychosocial risk factors that
the postictal period characterized by cognitive might impact overall well-being in otherwise
changes that might impact work performance? A high-functioning patients, one might suspect the
more pressing and ethically challenging issue per- cognitive deficits (or at least the functional impact
tains to the potential for reduced work perfor- of such deficits) associated with epilepsy to be
mance to result in harm to others, be it physical, minimal. The status as a “professional/high-
legal, financial, or otherwise. This issue is compli- functioning” patient almost by definition implies
cated even further by the fact that many profes- a certain degree of intact cognition (or rather
sionals choose not to disclose their diagnosis to precludes the presence of significant and/or
employers, which should also be addressed. widespread impairments). When cognitive com-
The assumption when assessing patients with plaints do arise in such patients, it is important to
professional careers is that intellectual function- keep in mind that in general such complaints may
ing is within or above the normal range. Normal not to be associated with actual “brain dysfunc-
intelligence and higher educational attainment tion,” but rather raise the question of functional
are considered to be protective when it comes to changes associated with an underlying depres-
overall prognosis, which is believed to be associ- sion and/or anxiety (Liik et al., 2009, Marino
ated with resilience. Therefore, these patients et al., 2009). By the same token, it should also be
might be expected to have a better prognosis than kept in mind that higher-functioning individuals
patients with lower baseline IQ. That being said, tend to be more sensitive to even subtle changes
the stigma associated with epilepsy can have a in cognition, which might not be detectable using
negative impact on employment status, and in standard normative data (e.g., although an “aver-
fact some consider this to be one of the most criti- age” or even “high average” performance would
cal barriers to employment. Therefore, even in be considered intact, these may represent marked
the absence of cognitive barriers to success, it is declines in someone who was once functioning at
vital that the clinician ascertain the level of a much higher level). Nevertheless, cognitive
stigma that the patient perceives to be associated impairments are a known consequence of intrac-
with his/her diagnosis, along with a more general table seizures, and the importance of determining
assessment of psychosocial functioning. What is cognitive status and of preventing future cognitive
14 The Neuropsychological Evaluation of High-Functioning Patients with Epilepsy 311

decline cannot be overemphasized in this pop-


ulation. This is not as much of an issue in patients Interpretation
who are well controlled on medication, unless of Neuropsychological Results
such control is only achieved with the use of high
doses of medication, older antiepileptic agents The neuropsychological battery that is used in
(or topiramate, a newer agent that is known to general epilepsy evaluations would also be appro-
have cognitive side effects), and/or polytherapy. priate for use with high-functioning patients. The
If this is the case, such patients might be consid- interpretation of the ensuing results would also
ered candidates for epilepsy surgery. In general, be the same in regard to lateralization/localiza-
however, the potential risk for cognitive decline tion of seizure focus and the general factors asso-
is more of a concern in patients who are refrac- ciated with better or worse outcome (e.g., better
tory to medication. presurgical memory predicts greater postsurgical
When considering the impact of chronic decline). However, there are several additional
epilepsy on neuropsychological status, it is interpretive considerations in higher-functioning
important to keep in mind that although some patients. At the outset of the evaluation, the
patients do exhibit significant impairments clinician should inquire about any prior evalua-
(memory being the most common), most patients tions. The ability to compare current results to a
evidence minimal deficits that tend to be confined prior baseline can be invaluable in this popula-
to the areas of language (primarily naming), tion, primarily because it provides information
immediate and delayed memory, executive func- regarding stability and/or progression of cogni-
tion, and psychomotor speed (Hermann et al., tive symptoms, which might impact treatment
2006). Those patients who do exhibit more pro- planning. Furthermore, such comparisons can
nounced and/or widespread impairments tend to reveal focal change in neurocognitive function-
be characterized by earlier age of seizure onset ing (i.e., change confined to one specific area of
and lower baseline IQ. That being said, there is a functioning), which can provide valuable insight
risk of progressive decline in patients with into seizure focus, particularly when EEG data is
chronic intractable epilepsy, which is strongly inconclusive. Regardless of whether a baseline
predicted by duration of epilepsy. The factors that exists or not, it is important to obtain an estimate
are believed to protect against such declines of premorbid functioning using valid and reliable
include later onset of seizures (and thus shorter measures. This can also be used to ascertain the
duration), higher educational attainment, and likelihood that there has been a decline from a
normal baseline IQ, all of which suggest a higher prior level or relative to an expected level based
level of cognitive reserve and thus better capacity on demographic variables that are known to be
to absorb the impact of seizures in these patients. associated with intellectual functioning.
In fact, the well-established relationship between Including demographic variables in the equation
generalized cognitive impairment and duration of might be particularly useful in patients with onset
epilepsy has been shown to be attenuated (to non- of seizures in childhood, as this might provide
significant levels) by having more years of formal clues regarding whether or not there was a devel-
education. Given the presumed demographic opmental impact from the seizures.
variables associated with higher-functioning The usual interpretation of the VCI–PRI
patients, particularly those in professional split might also change with high-functioning
careers, it would stand to reason that such patients patients, specifically those with high educational
fall into this low-risk group. However, the poten- attainment. Education level is positively correlated
tial for progression of cognitive symptoms must with verbal abilities on the Wechsler intelligence
be addressed when counseling the patient about scales. Therefore, a VCI > PRI split on the WAIS-IV
treatment options. This will be discussed further would be expected in more highly educated
under the section on surgical considerations. patients and would not necessarily indicate right
312 S.G. Schaffer

hemisphere seizure onset (as might otherwise be Along these lines, there is increased likeli-
the case). In contrast, the opposite might be con- hood that high-functioning patients would be
cluded from a split in the reverse direction (i.e., a able to utilize a wider array of compensatory
PRI > VCI discrepancy might be even more strategies in the event of a cognitive “hit,” and
meaningful in patients with high educational this should be considered when determining the
attainment than it would otherwise be). The base overall risk to employment statues. In particular,
rates of VCI–PRI discrepancies that are provided high-functioning patients and those in profes-
in the WAIS-IV manual are broken down by abil- sional careers might already use a smartphone or
ity (IQ) level, and thus such splits should not similar handheld device (PDA), and they might
present a problem in interpretation in cases where thus be in a better position to use these devices to
FSIQ is consistent with premorbid estimates. their advantage as a way to compensate for lost
When this is not the case, however, it might be ability.
prudent to also examine the base rate using the
premorbid estimate as the “ability” level. Finally,
it is often the case that individuals enter into pro- Surgical Considerations
fessions that are better suited to their particular
strengths, and in epilepsy patients such decisions The general issues for patients considering surgi-
might be associated with underlying functional cal intervention have been discussed in detail in
capacity associated with region of seizure onset Chap. 3. Therefore, the current discussion will
in the brain. For example, someone with left TLE focus on surgical decision-making only insofar as
might find herself gravitating toward art classes it pertains to high-functioning patients and/or
in high school and computer graphics classes in those with professional careers. The decision to
college, and she might ultimately find herself in a proceed with surgery is made when patients are
career as a web designer. This proclivity might found to be refractory to pharmacologic interven-
even hold true in the event of later seizure onset, tion or, in some cases, when the side effects of the
which is more typical in higher-functioning latter are deemed to be intolerable. As a general
patients, as many studies have indicated that cog- rule, the earlier one enters into surgery, the better
nitive symptoms predate the diagnosis of epi- the prognosis not only for seizure outcome but
lepsy. In any event, if strengths on for cognitive outcome as well. In all cases, the
neuropsychological assessment coincide with risks and benefits of proceeding with surgery
occupation (and weaknesses are not as important must be weighed against the alternative. However,
for work-related activities), this might indicate a this issue tends to be more complicated in
better prognosis related to employment outcome patients who are functioning at a high level going
in the event of postsurgical declines. In general it into surgery. Most patients being considered for
is important to consider the cognitive demands surgery are unable to work, experience increased
associated with the patient’s line of work, as the levels of depression and other psychiatric dis-
impact of potential declines might be exacerbated orders, and report generally poor quality of life.
or mitigated depending on work demands. For In such cases, the risks associated with cognitive
example, if the results of an evaluation of a law- decline may be weighed less heavily in the con-
yer show strong verbal memory and good expres- text of the potential improvement in quality of
sive language, skills that are obviously necessary life that a reduced seizure burden might bring.
for a successful attorney (particularly a litigator), Indeed, patients who have experienced seizure
the risk to employment outcome associated with freedom from surgery but show postsurgical
a left temporal lobe resection might be unaccept- memory declines tend not to report declines in
able. That being said, the possibility of the patient QOL associated with the latter. In the professional
agreeing to a lateral promotion to a job that patient, however, the risk of cognitive decline
requires less litigation and more paperwork might might be much more of a factor in the decision-
reduce this risk to a more acceptable level. making process, particularly given the fact that
14 The Neuropsychological Evaluation of High-Functioning Patients with Epilepsy 313

the risk of such cognitive declines is increased in given that this is the road one might be headed
these patients. These patients are more likely to down anyway? If the decision is toward the
have a later age of seizure onset and normal IQ, former, an additional factor to consider is that
both of which are associated with greater cogni- older age at surgery is associated with poorer out-
tive declines, and it is now generally accepted come, presumably due to reduced neural plastic-
that the greater the cognitive abilities going into ity. Therefore, the deficit might be even bigger if
surgery, the more there is to lose. The Wada test the surgery is postponed. This is further compli-
can assist in determining risk for decline, as good cated by the fact that there are no guarantees of
memory in the hemisphere under consideration is being seizure-free. Finally, the surgical decision
associated with greater risk of decline (regardless must also be weighed in the context of the fact
of the ability of the contralateral hemisphere’s that continued seizures could lead to additional
capacity to support memory functions). That independent seizure foci (that may or may not be
being said, these patients also tend to have greater amenable to surgery or that may eliminate the
cognitive reserve and better compensatory skills, potential for postoperative seizure freedom) and
and although patients with better baseline mem- the small but tragic risk of SUDEP.
ory show greater losses than patients with poor
baseline performance, those who are better pre-
operatively will nevertheless be better postop- Postsurgical Outcome
eratively. It is possible that the relative stability
of cognitive functions seen in these patients can In addition to primary issues related to surgical
be attributed to the development and/or use of outcome, the neuropsychologist should also
compensatory strategies that are made possible discuss goals and expectations associated with
because of a stronger cognitive foundation, and surgery. Patients sometimes set unrealistic goals
such strategies might be particularly beneficial in that need to be reined in with the help of the
the event of postsurgical declines in cognitive surgical team, including the neuropsychologist.
abilities. That being said, for some patients the The potential for disappointment and reduced
prospect of even mild memory decline might be postsurgical adjustment needs to be circumvented
unacceptable. by bringing the patient’s goals in line with reality.
Knowing this risk, patients must determine For example, while seizure freedom is obviously
what the functional impact of a memory hit the goal, would the possibility that seizures might
would mean at work. If memory is found to be only be lessened in frequency and/or severity
adequate before surgery and work is not being alter the patient’s decision regarding surgery?
affected, is it better to live with seizures and con- Additionally, for many patients the motivation to
tinue to work at the current level, or is it more seek surgery stems from the intolerability of
important to be seizure-free and possibly take a AED side effects, and the possibility of not hav-
hit? Are job demands dependent on memory and ing to take medication supersedes the prospect of
language? Conversely, how might employment being seizure-free. It is important that patients
status improve in the absence of seizures and its realize that medications will be tapered off
associated factors? In the event that patients slowly, if at all, and that the likelihood of not
deem any memory decline to be unacceptable, having to take medications any more is by no
there needs to be an additional discussion regard- means guaranteed. Finally, some patients expe-
ing the cognitive prognosis associated with rience poor postsurgical adjustment due to a
uncontrolled seizures. Therefore, in the end, the paradoxical effect associated with the dramatic
decision for the patient boils down to: “When do positive changes that can accompany seizure
I want to suffer a memory decline?” Would it be freedom, a phenomenon that has been termed the
better to live with seizures and have another 10 or “burden of normality.” Conversely, some patients
so years to work, or is it worth it to risk the cogni- expect dramatic and immediate changes to ensue
tive decline for the prospect of seizure freedom, as a result of seizure freedom, and it is important
314 S.G. Schaffer

to evaluate such expectations and make them should make it clear that there are many factors
more realistic in order to prepare the patient for that must be considered in the evaluation of high-
potential changes and reduce the level of disap- functioning patients, any one of which has the
pointment that might accompany a lack of change potential to change the outcome.
in some areas. These issues might be tempered in
higher-functioning patients, as they tend to have
increased levels of self-efficacy and higher resil- References
ience going into surgery. Nevertheless, the range
of potential changes that might occur as a result Alanis-Guevara, I., Pena, E., Corona, T., Lopez-Ayaa, T.,
Lopez-Meza, E., & Lopez-Gomez, M. (2005). Sleep
of surgery should be discussed thoroughly with
disturbances, socioeconomic status, and seizure con-
the patient. trol as main predictors of quality of life in epilepsy.
After patients proceed to surgery, it is impor- Epilepsy & Behavior, 7(3), 481–485.
tant to measure postsurgical outcome. Patients Boylan, L. S., Flint, L. A., Labovitz, D. L., Jackson, S. C.,
Starner, K., & Devinsky, O. (2004). Depression but not
should be made aware of the fact that many of the
seizure frequency predicts quality of life in treatment-
declines experienced immediately after surgery resistant epilepsy. Neurology, 62(2), 258–261.
will resolve within the first year, while some will Campbell-Sills, L., Forde, D. R., & Stein, M. B. (2009).
likely remain. In the event that the patient reports Demographic and childhood environmental predictors
of resilience in a community sample. Journal of
significant declines in the months after surgery, a
Psychiatric Research, 43(12), 1007–1012.
brief neuropsychological follow-up exam can be Carpay, J. A., Aldenkamp, A. P., & van Donselaar, C. A.
conducted in order to assess the validity and (2005). Complaints associated with the use of antiepi-
nature of such complaints. Recall that most com- leptic drugs: Results from a community-based study.
Seizure, 14(3), 198–206.
plaints are not substantiated by objective testing
Chaplin, J. E., Wester, A., & Tomson, T. (1998). Factors
and in such cases complaints might better reflect associated with the employment problems of people
poor psychosocial adjustment such as an under- with established epilepsy. Seizure: The Journal of the
lying mood disorder. In the event that significant British Epilepsy Association, 7(4), 299–303.
Chelune, G. J. (1995). Hippocampal adequacy versus
declines are noted (using reliable change indi-
functional reserve: Predicting memory functions fol-
ces), a referral for cognitive rehabilitation might lowing temporal lobectomy. Archives of Clinical
be warranted. In the absence of significant post- Neuropsychology: The Official Journal of the National
surgical cognitive complaints, a more compre- Academy of Neuropsychologists, 10(5), 413–432.
Fisher, R. S., Vickrey, B. G., Gibson, P., Hermann, B.,
hensive follow-up exam can be conducted one
Penovich, P., Scherer, A., et al. (2000). The impact of
year after surgery in order to establish a postsur- epilepsy from the patient's perspective I. Descriptions
gical baseline and assess the accuracy of surgical and subjective perceptions. Epilepsy Research, 41(1),
predictions. While assessment of cognitive status 39–51.
Gaitatzis, A., Carroll, K., Majeed, A., & W Sander,
is important in surgical patients, ongoing follow-
J. (2004). The epidemiology of the comorbidity of epi-
up to assess psychosocial well-being might be lepsy in the general population. Epilepsia, 45(12),
even more important, and these can be done when 1613–1622.
the patient comes in for follow-up appointments Gaitatzis, A., Trimble, M. R., & Sander, J. W. (2004). The
psychiatric comorbidity of epilepsy. Acta Neurologica
with their epileptologist, who should be reminded
Scandinavica, 110(4), 207–220.
of the need for such appointments. Gleissner, U., Helmstaedter, C., Quiske, A., & Elger, C. E.
It is important to keep in mind when conduct- (1998). The performance-complaint relationship in
ing any neuropsychological evaluation that clini- patients with epilepsy: A matter of daily demands?
Epilepsy Research, 32(3), 401–409.
cal decision that are based on established research
Goffman, E. (1963). Stigma: Notes on the management
findings should be undertaken with caution. of the spoiled identity. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
When counseling patients, it is prudent to remind Prentice-Hall.
them periodically that predictions are based on Hayden, M., Penna, C., & Buchanan, N. (1992). Epilepsy:
Patient perceptions of their condition. Seizure: The
group data and that these general “rules” are by
Journal of the British Epilepsy Association, 1(3),
no means steadfast. If anything, this chapter 191–197.
14 The Neuropsychological Evaluation of High-Functioning Patients with Epilepsy 315

Helmstaedter, C., & Elger, C. E. (2009). Chronic temporal LaFrance, W. C., Jr., Kanner, A. M., & Hermann, B.
lobe epilepsy: A neurodevelopmental or progressively (2008). Psychiatric comorbidities in epilepsy.
dementing disease? Brain: A Journal of Neurology, International Review of Neurobiology, 83, 347–383.
132(Pt 10), 2822–2830. Liik, M., Vahterb, L., Gross-Pajub, K., & Haldrea, S.
Hermann, B., Seidenberg, M., Bell, B., Rutecki, P., Sheth, (2009). Subjective complaints compared to the results
R., Ruggles, K., et al. (2002a). The neurodevelopmen- of neuropsychological assessment in patients with
tal impact of childhood-onset temporal lobe epilepsy epilepsy: The influence of comorbid depression.
on brain structure and function. Epilepsia, 43(9), Epilepsy Research, 84, 194–200.
1062–1071. Loring, D. W., Meador, K. J., & Lee, G. P. (2004).
Hermann, B., Seidenberg, M., Lee, E. J., Chan, F., & Determinants of quality of life in epilepsy. Epilepsy &
Rutecki, P. (2007). Cognitive phenotypes in temporal Behavior, 5(6), 976–980.
lobe epilepsy. Journal of the International Marino, S. E., Meador, K. J., Loring, D. W., Okun, M. S.,
Neuropsychological Society: JINS, 13(1), 12–20. Fernandez, H. H., Fessler, A. J., et al. (2009).
doi:10.1017/S135561770707004X Subjective perception of cognition is related to mood
Hermann, B. P., Seidenberg, M., Dow, C., et al. (2006). and not performance. Epilepsy & Behavior, 14,
Cognitive prognosis in chronic temporal lobe epilepsy. 459–464.
Annals of Neurology, 60, 80–87. Oyegbile, T. O., Dow, C., Jones, J., Bell, B., Rutecki, P.,
Hermann, B. P., Seidenberg, M., & Bell, B. (2002b). The Sheth, R., et al. (2004). The nature and course of neu-
neurodevelopmental impact of childhood onset tem- ropsychological morbidity in chronic temporal lobe
poral lobe epilepsy on brain structure and function and epilepsy. Neurology, 62(10), 1736–1742.
the risk of progressive cognitive effects. Progress in Perrine, K., Hermann, B. P., Meador, K. J., Vickrey, B. G.,
Brain Research, 135, 429–438. Cramer, J. A., Hays, R. D., et al. (1995). The relation-
Holland, P., Lane, S., Whitehead, M., Marson, A. G., & ship of neuropsychological functioning to quality of
Jacoby, A. (2009). Labor market participation follow- life in epilepsy. Archives of Neurology, 52(10),
ing onset of seizures and early epilepsy: Findings from 997–1003.
a UK cohort. Epilepsia, 50(5), 1030–1039. Piazzini, A., Canevini, M. P., Maggiori, G., & Canger, R.
Jacoby, A. (1995). Impact of epilepsy on employment sta- (2001). The perception of memory failures in patients
tus: Findings from a UK study of people with well- with epilepsy. European Journal of Neurology, 8(6),
controlled epilepsy. Epilepsy Research, 21(2), 613–620.
125–132. Pulsipher, D. T., Seidenberg, M., Jones, J., & Hermann, B.
Jacoby, A., Baker, G. A., Steen, N., Potts, P., & Chadwick, (2006). Quality of life and comorbid medical and psy-
D. W. (1996). The clinical course of epilepsy and its chiatric conditions in temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy
psychosocial correlates: Findings from a U.K. com- & Behavior, 9(3), 510–514.
munity study. Epilepsia, 37(2), 148–161. Reisinger, E. L., & DiIorio, C. (2009). Individual, seizure-
Jacoby, A., Snape, D., & Baker, G. A. (2009). Determinants related, and psychosocial predictors of depressive
of quality of life in people with epilepsy. Neurologic symptoms among people with epilepsy over six
Clinics, 27(4), 843–863. months. Epilepsy & Behavior, 15(2), 196–201.
Kobau, R., DiIorio, C. A., Price, P. H., Thurman, D. J., Martin, Ryan, R., Kempner, K., & Emlen, A. C. (1980). The
L. M., Ridings, D. L., et al. (2004). Prevalence of epilepsy stigma of epilepsy as a self-concept. Epilepsia, 21(4),
and health status of adults with epilepsy in Georgia and 433–444.
Tennessee: Behavioral risk factor surveillance system, Seidenberg, M., Pulsipher, D. T., & Hermann, B. (2007).
2002. Epilepsy & Behavior, 5(3), 358–366. Cognitive progression in epilepsy. Neuropsychology
Kobau, R., Gilliam, F., & Thurman, D. J. (2006). Review, 17(4), 445–454.
Prevalence of self-reported epilepsy or seizure disor- Senol, V., Soyuer, F., Arman, F., & Ozturk, A. (2007).
der and its associations with self-reported depression Influence of fatigue, depression, and demographic,
and anxiety: Results from the 2004 HealthStyles sur- socioeconomic, and clinical variables on quality of life
vey. Epilepsia, 47(11), 1915–1921. of patients with epilepsy. Epilepsy & Behavior, 10(1),
Kobau, R., Zahran, H., Grant, D., Thurman, D. J., Price, 96–104.
P. H., & Zack, M. M. (2007). Prevalence of active epi- Sillanpaa, M., & Schmidt, D. (2010). Long-term employ-
lepsy and health-related quality of life among adults ment of adults with childhood-onset epilepsy: A pro-
with self-reported epilepsy in california: California spective population-based study. Epilepsia, 51,
health interview survey, 2003. Epilepsia, 48(10), 1053–1060.
1904–1913. Smith, G., Ferguson, P. L., Saunders, L. L., Wagner,
Kobau, R., Zahran, H., Thurman, D. J., Zack, M. M., J. L., Wannamaker, B. B., & Selassie, A. W. (2009).
Henry, T. R., Schachter, S. C., et al. (2008). Epilepsy Psychosocial factors associated with stigma in
surveillance among adults–19 states, behavioral risk adults with epilepsy. Epilepsy & Behavior, 16(3),
factor surveillance system, 2005. MMWR. Surveillance 484–490.
Summaries: Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report. Smeets, V. M., van Lierop, B. A., Vanhoutvin, J. P.,
Surveillance Summaries/CDC, 57(6), 1–20. Aldenkamp, A. P., & Nijhuis, F. J., (2007). Epilepsy
316 S.G. Schaffer

and employment: literature review. Epilepsy & (2006). A cross-sectional study of subjective com-
Behavior, 10, 354–362. plaints in patients with epilepsy who seem to be
Suurmeijer, T. P., Reuvekamp, M. F., & Aldenkamp, B. P. well-controlled with anti-epileptic drugs. Seizure,
(2001). Social functioning, psychological functioning, 15, 242–248.
and quality of life in epilepsy. Epilepsia, 42(9), Vermeulen, J., Aldenkamp, A. P., & Alpherts, W. C.
1160–1168. (1993). Memory complaints in epilepsy: Correlations
Tedman, S., Thornton, E., & Baker, G. (1995). with cognitive performance and neuroticism. Epilepsy
Development of a scale to measure core beliefs and Research, 15, 157–170.
perceived self efficacy in adults with epilepsy. Seizure: Wakamoto, H., Nagao, H., Hayashi, M., & Morimoto, T.
The Journal of the British Epilepsy Association, 4(3), (2000). Long-term medical, educational, and social
221–231. prognoses of childhood-onset epilepsy: A population-
Tellez-Zenteno, J. F., Patten, S. B., Jette, N., Williams, J., based study in a rural district of japan. Brain &
& Wiebe, S. (2007). Psychiatric comorbidity in Development, 22(4), 246–255.
epilepsy: A population-based analysis. Epilepsia, Wheless, J. W. (2006). Intractable epilepsy: A survey of
48(12), 2336–2344. patients and caregivers. Epilepsy & Behavior, 8(4),
Uijl, S. J., Uiterwaal, C. S. M. P., Aldenkamp, A. P., 756–764.
Carpay, J. A., Doelman, J. C., Keizer, K., et al.
The Neuropsychological
Assessment of Culturally 15
and Linguistically Diverse
Epilepsy Patients

H. Allison Bender

According to the last US census, approximately (National Conference on Public Health and
31.1 million immigrants currently reside in the Epilepsy, 2003). Epidemiologists attribute this
United States, representing well over 10 % of the disproportionately high percentage to an elevated
country’s total population (US Bureau of the prevalence of cysticercosis (DeGiorgio et al.,
Census 1999, 2000). This 57 % rise in immigra- 2005; Medina, Rosas, Rubio-Donnadieu, &
tion over the previous decade is without parallel Sotelo, 1990), birth trauma, traumatic brain
during any time in recent history. Hispanic injury, and stroke in Hispanic populations
immigrants comprise the most rapidly growing (Santiago-Grisoli, 2000). Although large-scale
segment of the country’s population, represent- investigations of seizure-related risk factors have
ing over half of all immigrants to the United also been limited in non-Hispanic immigrant
States (US Bureau of the Census 1999, 2000). groups, cysticercosis and other parasitic diseases
Recent trends also indicate that immigration have also been implicated as having a significant
from Asian countries is steadily increasing and is etiological role in the development of seizure dis-
currently the leading region of birth of persons orders diagnosed in African nations (Preux &
becoming legal permanent residents of the Druet-Cabanac, 2005).
United States (36.9 %; Department of Homeland The elevated incidence of epilepsy in ethni-
Security, 2009). cally and culturally diverse immigrant groups
Much of the available literature suggests that necessitates increased attention to factors which
immigrant populations are at increased risk for intrinsically and extrinsically limit the reliability,
congenital and acquired neurological disorders validity, and utility of neuropsychological assess-
(DeGiorgio et al., 2005; Gill, Lenz, & Amolat, ments in these populations. In order to fully
2003; White et al., 2005; Zahuranec et al., 2006). address these concerns, it is first necessary to
Epilepsy is no exception. For example, while sei- consider the role of culture-, language-, and
zures are relatively common across all ethnic immigration-specific variables throughout all
groups, Hispanic adults residing in the United aspects of the testing session, data interpretation,
States have a twofold higher incidence of this and case formulation. Although specific proce-
disorder than non-Hispanic US residents dures may vary due to the patient’s individual
needs (e.g., surgical candidacy, language abili-
ties, degree of bilingualism, mental and psychiat-
H.A. Bender, Ph.D. (*) ric status), the neuropsychological assessment of
Department of Neurology, Mount Sinai School epilepsy patients typically includes the follow-
of Medicine, 5 East 98th Street, Box 1139,
New York, NY 10029, USA ing: (1) clinical interview, (2) assessment of men-
e-mail: heidi.bender@mssm.edu tal status and/or (3) estimation of premorbid

W.B. Barr and C. Morrison (eds.), Handbook on the Neuropsychology of Epilepsy, 317
Clinical Handbooks in Neuropsychology, DOI 10.1007/978-0-387-92826-5_15,
© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015
318 H.A. Bender

abilities, (4) evaluation of intellectual function- cific group that can be approached, avoided, or
ing and specific neuropsychological abilities, (5) achieved by an individual (Caltabiano, 1984).
examination of behavioral and psychiatric func- In its simplest form, ethnic identity is a mul-
tioning, and (6) assessment of health-related tidimensional affiliative construct, whereby a
quality of life. Accordingly, the focus of this person views himself or herself (and others) as
chapter is to address each component of the neu- belonging to a specific ethnic or cultural group
ropsychological assessment, as enumerated (Phinney & Rotheram, 1987). As a result, immi-
above, by providing both a framework and practi- grants may delineate boundaries between “self”
cal recommendations specific to the evaluation of and “others” or “us” and “them” altering the
culturally and linguistically diverse populations dynamic between examiner-patient dyads, par-
with epilepsy and seizure disorders. While many ticularly in cases of differing ethnic identities.
of the specific examples will focus on the com- In general, the development and subsequent shift
paratively extensive body of literature describing in ethnic identification can occur as early as 4–8
Hispanic immigrants residing in the United years of age, necessitating careful consideration
States, results could generalize to other immi- in clinical settings. Clinicians should first attempt
grant, non-native English-speaking populations. to establish ethnic identity based on the patient’s
viewpoint. For example, does a recent Korean
immigrant self-identify as Korean, Korean-
The Clinical Interview American, Asian, Asian-American, American,
or another classification? Careful questioning
Prior to initiating interview and assessment, it is is essential to establishing the patient’s prefer-
first necessary to consider the patient’s unique ences, as well as any shifts in his or her ethnic
worldview and sociocultural experiences. While identification post-emigration, in order to facili-
this is an important step for all individuals under- tate a greater understanding of the individuals’
going a neuropsychological evaluation, regard- psychological needs.
less of race, ethnicity, or cultural background,
research in the fields of sociology, psychology,
and anthropology suggests that immigrant popu- Acculturation and Acculturative
lations are at particular risk for developing cogni- Stress
tive schemas which could negatively impact their
performance on testing. Specifically, shifts in Ethnic and cultural identity are strongly related
ethnic identity, acculturative stress, and stereo- to the theoretical construct of acculturation, the
type threat may limit the individual’s willingness, process in which an individual belonging to cul-
desire, and capacity to fully participate in the tural minority changes their behaviors, beliefs,
testing experience. Although one’s awareness of values, or customs after coming into continuous
the presence and effects of these constructs is contact with a “majority” or mainstream culture
highly variable, the clinical interview represents (Berry, 1980; Berry, Trimble, & Olmedo, 1986;
the first opportunity that the clinician has to Marin, 1992). The process of acculturation in
ascertain the patient’s perspective and immigrant populations is generally a fluid pro-
experiences. cess during which the person may identify exclu-
sively with their “home” (native country) culture,
“host” (new country) culture, or a combination of
Ethnic Identity both. Sociological studies suggest that this long-
term, dynamic process is shaped by each person’s
When individuals migrate from one nation or unique immigrant experience and occurs at dif-
culture to another, there is a high likelihood that fering rates, even within the same families.
cultural and ethnic identity will change. Ethnic It has been widely suggested that the construct
identity is the “felt belongingness” to a spe- of acculturation is an important culture-specific
15 The Neuropsychological Assessment of Culturally and Linguistically Diverse Epilepsy Patients 319

moderator of neuropsychological test perfor- ior that are central to the individual in both of
mance among non-native US populations (e.g., these domains. Another instrument, the Short
Arnold, Montgomery, Castaneda, & Longoria, Acculturation Scale for Hispanics (SASH; Marin,
1994; Boone, Victor, Wen, Razani, & Ponton, Sabogal, Marin, Otero-Sabogal, & Perez-Stable,
2007; Harris, Tulsky, & Schultheis, 2003; 1987), evaluates a broader range of acculturation-
Kennepohl, Douglas, Nabors, & Hanks, 2004). In related constructs, including sociocultural rela-
a recent study evaluating the relationship between tions. One advantage of the SASH is that its
acculturation and neuropsychological function- questions are presented in both English and
ing in a clinical neurological sample, Boone et al. Spanish, allowing for use with either bilingual or
(2007) reported that acculturation is a significant monolingual Spanish-speaking populations.
predictor of neuropsychological test performance The process of acculturation can be the source
and is not attenuated by the presence of psychiat- of significant psychological turmoil for many
ric or neurologic illness. Although these investi- immigrants, termed “acculturative stress.” Berry
gators did not find acculturation to be a significant (1980) proposes a model of acculturation which
performance moderator of nonverbal neuropsy- serves to explain the sources of stress based upon
chological test measures, others suggest that the individual’s degree of participation in home
acculturation accounts for diminished perfor- (e.g., cultural maintenance) and host (contact
mance on nonverbal measures of tactile percep- participation) culture. In this two-dimensional
tion, problem solving, motor speeds, and model, individuals who subscribe to neither their
executive functioning (e.g., Arnold et al., 1994; home nor host culture are described as undergo-
Coffey, Marmol, Schock, & Adams, 2005). ing “marginalization” and are therefore at con-
Given the potential impact of acculturation siderable risk for psychological and emotional
on neuropsychological test performance, it is distress. Similarly, those who maintain contact
necessary to gather information via real-world with their native culture and forgo participation
proxies of this construct. While much of this in majority culture are considered “separated” or
information can be gathered during the inter- “segregated” and are also at increased risk for
view process, several self-report rating scales economic stressors (e.g., difficulty finding suit-
or “acculturation instruments” can be used to able employment, adequate housing, or sufficient
quantitatively assess the extent that individuals income) and coping difficulties. Even individuals
have adopted and assimilated in various aspects who assimilate with the host culture often do so
of the dominant (“host”) culture, as well as how at the expense of the long-standing cultural iden-
much of the person’s nondominant (“home”) tity and beliefs. As a result, assimilation can be
cultural traits have been retained (see Marin & associated with guilt and unstable support sys-
Marin, 1992). Specifically, acculturation is most tems within one’s ethnic or cultural group.
frequently measured by examining several proxy Optimized long-term outcomes (e.g., economic,
variables, including: (1) language preferences, social, psychological, and physical) are most
(2) behavior, (3) knowledge, (4) cultural iden- often achieved with “integration,” whereas cul-
tity, and (5) values (Cortes, Rogler, & Malgady, tural attitudes and mores are accepted from both
1994; Marin & Gamba, 1996). One such example home and host cultures. Although the direct rela-
is the Bidimensional Acculturation Scale (BAS; tionship between acculturative stress and neuro-
Marin & Gamba, 1996), a self-report inventory psychological test performance has yet to be
that assesses acculturation among Hispanics. clearly established, clinicians are encouraged to
This scale provides an acculturation score for consider its role in cases where their patients
two major cultural dimensions (Hispanic and report feelings of isolation or in those who lack a
non-Hispanic domains) which measures three support system of culturally similar peers. The
language-related areas: (1) language use, (2) lin- Social, Attitudinal, Familial, and Environmental
guistic proficiency, and (3) electronic media. The Acculturation Stress Scale (SAFE; Mena, Padilla,
BAS evaluates bidirectional changes in behav- & Maldonado, 1987) is a useful tool for evaluat-
320 H.A. Bender

ing an individual’s feelings of stress and perceived Cultural Competence and Respect
discrimination within Hispanic populations. for Cross-Cultural Differences

Beyond the impact of factors stemming from the


Stereotype Threat patient’s worldview and immigrant experiences,
several variables within the testing room must
Stereotype threat is another powerful psychologi- also be considered prior to testing and interpreta-
cal process with the potential to limit the validity tion. In an effort to provide optimum care to non-
and reliability of neuropsychological assessment native US populations, neuropsychologists
with minority populations. Most often studied in should strive to broaden their cultural compe-
the context of racial and gender differences, ste- tence through peer supervision, independent
reotype threat is a situational feeling or awareness research, participation in continuing education,
of alleged intellectual inferiority, thereby creat- and/or journal clubs and by remaining abreast of
ing anxiety and fear of confirming the possibility current world events. Although a daunting and, at
(Aronson et al., 1999; Aronson, Steele, Salinas, times, overwhelming prospect, the first step in
& Lustina, 1998; Steele, 1997; Steele & Aronson, raising a clinician’s own level of awareness can
1995). Such increased anxiety may interfere with, be achieved by obtaining a basic understanding
and decreases performance on, tasks to which the about the culture and history of the country from
stereotype applies. In their seminal study, Steele which each patient originates. In doing so, themes
and colleagues (1997) assessed the performance salient to a patient’s degree of openness and will-
of well-educated African-American young adults ingness to provide an honest, complete history
on difficult verbal items from the Graduate may emerge. For example, immigrants from
Record Exam; half of the sample was told that the countries recently engaged in warfare may hesi-
task measured intellectual ability, and the others tate to fully participate in the evaluation process
were informed that it was a laboratory problem- due to concerns of retribution, stigmatism, and/or
solving task. African-American students per- deportation.
formed below their Caucasian peers with matched The aforementioned research and study
SAT scores when told that their scores were should, at a minimum, include a basic under-
being used to measure intellectual ability, while standing of culturally based communication
African-American students in the non-ability practices, interactional style, family structure,
condition performed equally to Caucasian stu- and taboos. Failure to consider the presence and
dents. In these cases, the salience of racial stereo- impact of such social mores can lead to uninten-
types to African-Americans depressed their score tional offenses which may interfere with fluid
on measures of cognitive functioning. Similar rapport. For example, in several cultures, being
findings have been reported in a sample of West addressed by one’s given name, rather than his/
Indian immigrants, who are typically perceived her surname, is indicative of disrespect. The use
as Black in the United States (Deaux et al., 2007). of titles (e.g., Mr., Mrs., Miss, Ms.), last name
Interestingly, while first-generation immigrants (apellido), hyphenated surnames (apellidos in
do not exhibit a decrease in performance on test- Hispanic culture, e.g., Señora Rodriguez-Rivera,
ing conditions which activate stereotype threat, indicating the patient’s mother’s last name,
US-born second-generation immigrants exhib- Rivera, and father’s last name, Rodriguez), and
ited stereotype threat effects and a concomitant patronymics (e.g., Svetlana Grigorievna
decrease in performance. Although it is diffi- Lermontov, denoting that Svetlana is the daugh-
cult to gauge a patient’s feelings of stereotype ter of Grigory) is unique to individual cultures
threat during the testing session, it is certainly a and should be carefully studied and considered
potential performance moderator that should be prior to assessment. Similarly, pronoun usage can
thoroughly guarded against, and its effects fully be highly culture specific and reflective of status,
considered. rank, formality, and/or familiarity of a relationship;
15 The Neuropsychological Assessment of Culturally and Linguistically Diverse Epilepsy Patients 321

the pronoun “you” in Spanish can differ to indi- cal resistance. To this end, a review of available
cate the informality of a relationship, “tú” [used medical records and/or a discussion with a family
with children, family, and friends by most member, community elder, or patient representa-
groups], or great respect, rank, and courtesy, tive may be a particularly important source of
“usted” [used with older adults and with profes- medically relevant information (e.g., seizure
sionals, such as teachers]. semiology and postictal sequelae) and seizure-
A patient’s communication style is also related risk factors (e.g., traumatic brain injury
largely governed by cultural practices and expec- and central nervous system infection). Similarly,
tations. For example, individuals from Latino non-verbal behaviors which a European-American
culture may often engage in plática or platicar clinician may view as his/her own expressions of
(loosely translated as “small talk” in Spanish) keen interest, warmth, and sincerity (e.g., direct
upon introduction with a health-care provider eye contact, body contact, hand shaking) can be
(Ríos & Fernández Torres, 2004). In such sce- construed as inappropriate by culturally dissimi-
narios, patients may initially discuss themes lar patients. For example, individuals from China
unrelated to their illness, such as the weather or may consider extended direct eye contact from a
traffic. Although many Western or European male examiner to be rude or sexual in nature
countries value communication efficiency, many towards females (Ivey & Ivey, 2003).
areas in Latin America or Mexico would con-
sider it rude or offensive to begin an interaction
without such preliminaries. According to Davila Consideration of the Patient’s
and colleagues (2011), plática was shown to be Family Structure
instrumental when establishing empathetic rela-
tionships and facilitating cooperation in a focus Family structure and hierarchy varies consider-
group designed to evaluate the health behaviors ably across cultures and can reflect societies
of Spanish-speaking Hispanics. Anecdotal evi- which are matriarchal, patriarchal, individualis-
dence further suggests that beginning clinical tic, or collectivistic. For example, Hispanic popu-
interviews with similar, non-health-related lations are largely collectivistic, valuing group
themes is useful for establishing rapport and a needs and goals above one’s own (Marin &
therapeutic alliance (M. Rodriguez-Rivera, per- Marin, 1991a, 1991b). Familismo, or the care and
sonal communication). concern for a multigenerational, extended family
Knowledge and awareness of cultural taboos network is frequently noted, whereas family
are often important and necessary considerations members often take a high degree of responsibil-
in the assessment of culturally and ethnically ity in the day-to-day care and long-term planning
diverse immigrant patients. Beyond the need to of individuals with epilepsy. Neuropsychologists
demonstrate respect of the patients’ belief sys- should be particularly mindful of the potential
tems, understanding cultural taboos is also instru- consequences of collectivistic culture or familismo
mental in gathering and contextualizing the during the clinical interview, as patients may rely
historical information provided by the patient heavily on family members for assistance and
(and his or her family) during the clinical inter- can often rely on them as behavioral and attitudi-
view. Specifically, direct discussion of death, nal referents (Marin & Marin, 1991a, 1991b).
dying, or accidents can be considered “bad luck” Accordingly, clinicians should take care as to not
in many Asian cultures (Wong, Strickland, misinterpret diminished independence as reduced
Fletcher-Janzen, Ardila, & Reynolds, 2000). In adaptive functioning or health-related decline.
such cases, an Asian patient’s reluctance to dis- Moreover, family members, including those out-
cuss the history of present illness or his/her vague side the “nuclear family,” should be involved in
responses to health-based questions should not be the assessment process (where possible and with
potentially misinterpreted as the result of memory the patient’s consent) and may be excellent col-
difficulties, emotional disturbance, or psychologi- lateral sources of information.
322 H.A. Bender

Selecting the Primary Language (3) language spoken at home and in social set-
of Assessment tings, (4) primary language of education, (5) pri-
mary language spoken at current job, (6)
In large part because of its changing cultural language-related media preferences (e.g., televi-
landscape, America is also experiencing a collec- sion, radio, newspaper), (7) frequency that pri-
tive shift away from the previously held assump- mary and/or secondary languages were spoken in
tion of English as the nation’s primary “mother recent months, (8) and patient’s subjective sense
tongue.” According to a recent US Census Bureau of impairment in primary vs. secondary language
report, the number of people aged five and older (e.g., word-finding difficulties are more promi-
who primarily speak a language other than nent in French (L1) than English (secondary lan-
English at home has doubled from 1980 to 2007 guage; L2). Only after obtaining such information
(US Bureau of the Census, 2007 American can neuropsychologists plan an appropriate
Community Survey). In that time frame, the per- assessment strategy, including performing the
centage of non-English speakers has reportedly evaluation in the patient’s native language, in
risen by 140 %; a dramatic increase compared to English only, or obtaining language-related mea-
an overall population growth of 34 %. Spanish- sures in both languages. Should the latter option
speakers represent the largest numeric increase be selected, it is important to evaluate the patient’s
(23.4 million more speakers in 2007, as com- comparative level of abilities across languages by
pared to 1980), whereas the Vietnamese-speaking assessing functioning in all aspects of this con-
population accounts for the largest percentage struct, including word reading, reading compre-
increase (an approximate 511 % rise in the same hension, verbal fluency, confrontation naming.
time frame).
While compelling, these statistics do not fully
capture the degree of English-language profi- The Neuropsychological Evaluation
ciency or bilingualism in non-native US popula-
tions. Notably, it is relatively rare to encounter While there is well-documented evidence for test
patients who are capable of functioning equally bias in neuropsychological testing with culturally
well in either language in each domain or activity diverse, immigrant populations (Ardila, Rosselli,
(ambilingualism; Halliday, McKintosh, & & Puente, 1994; Artiola i Fortuny & Mullaney,
Strevens, 1970) or who use both languages equiv- 1997; Gasquoine, 1999), further investigation is
alent to monoglot speakers in their respective necessary to determine the presence of bias both
languages (balanced bilinguals). Rather, it is at the level of test instrument (i.e., test construc-
much more common for non-native English- tion and data interpretation) (Anastasi, 1988) and
speaking, particularly newly arrived immigrants, at the level of the individual (Arnold et al., 1994).
to demonstrate either monolingualism for their Test bias, in this case, refers to any measure
primary language (L1) or functional bilingual- upon which non-native US-born individuals
ism. To this end, ascertaining an individual’s achieve lower scores than English-speaking
degree of bilingualism is a critical aspect of the Caucasians due to reasons other than neurologi-
clinical interview of non-native English-speaking cal differences (Anastasi, 1988). From the for-
populations undergoing assessment in an epi- mer perspective, the overwhelming majority of
lepsy setting. Such information has important neuropsychological tests used in the United
implications for the patient’s obtained neuropsy- States were designed by Americans, Canadians,
chological profile, and, by extension, the lateral- or Europeans and may have a Eurocentric world-
izing value of the data and post-operative outcome. view and derivative psychometric technology
Direct questioning of the patient is necessary to (Trimble, Lonner, & Boucher, 1983). Although
establish the following information: (1) age of language-specific testing instruments obviate
primary and secondary language acquisition, (2) some of these concerns, test measures available
setting in which secondary language was learned, in languages other than English are few in num-
15 The Neuropsychological Assessment of Culturally and Linguistically Diverse Epilepsy Patients 323

ber and have relatively untested psychometric Russian, pronounced “geem-nas-ti-che-skii-


properties in clinical populations (Wong et al., zall,” a much longer, consonant-dense word
2000). It is important to recognize that in any which is more difficult to pronounce than the
assessment context, it is unwise to examine the English version) and a similar idiomatic meaning
validity, reliability, and sensitivity of data in iso- in order to evaluate the underlying abilities in the
lation, that is, without considering the medical same manner the original does. Although several
and experiential history of the individual. alternative Spanish-language phrases have been
Interpretation and application of test data taken suggested, including “No hay pero que valga”
out of context may result in increased error (There is nothing that is not of worth; as cited in
variance. Valle, 1990), “Si no bajo, entonces me subo” (If I
do not go down then I will go up; as cited in
Valle, 1990), and “Él quiere irse a casa” (He
The Use of Translated Test Materials wants to go home; Teng et al., 1994), they lack
comparable frequency and pragmatic ease.
In cases where clinicians opt to use test measures To obviate similar problems, a French transla-
which were translated from English (source text) tion of the CERAD (Consortium to Establish a
into another language (target text), careful atten- Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease) battery substi-
tion must be paid to the specific translation prac- tuted “no ifs, ands, or buts” with the phrase, “pas
tices and procedures. The process of translatorship de si ni de mais” (literally translated as “neither
is a socially defined role, involving at least two yes, nor but” or, more loosely as “no ifs or buts”),
languages and two cultural traditions, each of as this phrase preserves the overall meaning and
which is being governed by its own set of culture- the grammatical categories of its constituents,
bound standards of behavior (e.g., “norms”) and it also serves to evaluate the same cognitive
(Toury, 1980, as cited in Bender, García, & Barr, and articulatory functions as Geschwind’s
2010). Inasmuch, translators can choose to sub- original phrase (e.g., articulatory agility, compre-
scribe to the norms of either the source or the tar- hension, and presence of paraphasias in repeti-
get culture, thereby indicating the overall tion; Demers et al., 1994).
sociocultural relevance of the translation for the Neuropsychological test translation is further
target audience. If the source culture is selected, complicated by transliterated words (e.g., “laun-
as in the case of most neuropsychological test dromat” or “launderette” are referred to as
measures and practices, cultural and linguistic “washatería” by Mexican populations residing in
incompatibilities may occur, thus favoring ade- Texas or “el londri” for Hispanic individuals
quacy of the source text. However, if the transla- residing in Los Angeles; Pontón & Ardila, 1999)
tor follows the target text, he/she will deviate or those that are region- or country-specific (e.g.,
from the source and will consequentially foster the word “bus” is most commonly referred to as
target-culture acceptability. “autobús” in Spain, “camión” in Mexico, and
When selecting translated test materials, clini- “guagua” in Cuba, Canarias, and Antillas). When
cians should attempt to evaluate the balance translating the Boston Naming Test (CERAD ver-
between acceptability and adequacy. For exam- sion) into French, test translators acknowledged
ple, concern has been raised regarding linguistic differences in intracultural salience of a picture of
equivalence of the phrase “no ifs, ands, or buts,” a canoe to French-speaking Canadians, as com-
across multiple languages. When translated from pared to the French population residing in France
English into another language, this Folstein Mini- (Demers et al., 1994). To this ENP, test makers
Mental Status Exam (Folstein, Folstein, & should consult multiple translators from a variety
McHugh, 1975) item must possess an equivalent of communities speaking derivatives of the same
number of function words, a comparable mixture official language (e.g., French text translated by
of vowels and consonants (e.g., the word “gym- professionals in French-speaking Canada, Haiti,
nasium” in English is “гимнастический зал” in France, Belgium, and the Republic of the Congo)
324 H.A. Bender

to resolve such sociocultural disparities (American Lin, Wang, & Liu, 2007). In addition to its broad
Psychiatric Association, 2000). Information appeal, another advantage of the CASI is that it
detailing translation procedures, including those can be used to estimate scores on other more
used to minimize region-specific dialectical dif- commonly administered clinical measures (i.e.,
ferences and colloquialisms, should be provided the Mini-Mental State Examination, Modified
in the test manual. Prior to using such test mea- Mini-Mental State Test, and Hasegawa Dementia
sures, neuropsychologists should critically evalu- Screening Scale).
ate these processes in relation to the needs and
abilities of their individual patients. Neuropsi
The NEUROPSI (Ostrosky-Solís, Ardila, &
Rosselli, 1997, 1999) is an extended mini-mental
Assessment of Mental Status evaluation which was developed for and normal-
and Estimated Premorbid Abilities ized in Spanish-speaking adults. Among one of
the only assessment measures created directly in
The following subsection will provide brief Spanish rather than translated from another
descriptions of measures of mental status and source language, the NEUROPSI includes mea-
general intellectual functioning appropriate for sures of orientation, attention, concentration,
culturally and linguistically diverse epilepsy executive functioning (e.g., categorization and
patients. Of note, many of the tests described abstraction), visuomotor skills, language skills
herein are appropriate for Hispanic populations, (e.g., naming, repletion, fluency, and comprehen-
as multiple instruments have been developed or sion), memory (e.g., immediate and delayed
translated for use with Spanish-speakers. recall and visual-nonverbal functioning), read-
ing, writing, and calculation. Task items on the
The Cognitive Abilities Screening NEUROPSI were designed to be completed by
Instrument individuals with a broad range of literacy, socio-
The Cognitive Abilities Screening Instrument economic status, and educational attainment. In
(CASI) is a brief measure of mental status devel- this vein, language-based items include high-,
oped for use with geriatric populations of varying medium-, and low-frequency Spanish-language
levels of education and literacy (Teng et al., words. Also of note, the authors of the NEUROPSI
1994). Scored out of a possible 100 points, this sought to maximize the cultural salience of con-
instrument includes items assessing orientation, cepts underlying task items. For example, unlike
attention, concentration, abstraction, repetition, other available mental status examinations or
visuospatial reproduction, naming, verbal flu- dementia-rating scales, the NEUROPSI does not
ency, handwriting abilities, judgment, remote include an item asking for the season of the year
memory, new learning, and delayed recall. The in order to assess the patient’s orientation; the
CASI has been translated into multiple lan- concept of a “season” may be less clearly defined
guages, including Japanese, Chinese, Vietnamese, in tropical areas or may refer to a wet season or a
and Spanish. Test authors have reportedly modi- dry season. The NEUROPSI was normalized on a
fied standard mental status items (e.g., the repeti- sample of 800 Mexican individuals aged 16–85
tion item, “no ifs, ands, or buts,” was substituted years (i.e., 16–30 years, 31–50 years, 51–65
for the more linguistically salient Spanish- years, and 66–85 years).
language item “Él quiere irse a casa,” which Developed by the same team of investigators,
means “he wants to go home;” Teng et al., 1994). the NEUROPSI Attention and Memory test
The clinical utility of the CASI has been well (Ostrosky-Solís et al., 2003, 2007) is a Spanish-
validated throughout dementia literature among language instrument which assesses attentional,
international studies of community-based sam- executive, and memory abilities. Specific items
ples (Graves et al., 1996; Liu et al., 1994; Tsai, tapping attention and executive abilities include
15 The Neuropsychological Assessment of Culturally and Linguistically Diverse Epilepsy Patients 325

assessment of alertness, orientation, supraspan, relations and to reason by analogy independent


vigilance, selective attention, mental flexibility, of language and education. Despite being widely
concept formation, and inhibitory control. identified as a “culture-fair” test measure, mul-
Immediate memory and delayed recall are also tiple studies have demonstrated differences on
assessed in visual and verbal modalities. RSPM performance based on race and ethnicity
(Lynn, Backhoff, & Contreras, 2005; Rushton &
Word Accentuation Test Jensen, 2005).
The initial Spanish version of the National Adult Like the RSPM, the Comprehensive Test of
Reading Test (NART; Nelson, 1982), the Word Nonverbal Intelligence (CTONI; Hammill,
Accentuation Test, can be used as an estimate of Pearson, & Wiederhold, 1997) is a measure of
premorbid intelligence (Del Ser, Gonzalez- nonverbal reasoning (i.e., analogical reasoning,
Montalvo, Martinez-Espinosa, Delgado-Villapalos, categorical classifications, and sequential reason-
& Bermejo, 1997). ing) which requires limited spoken language or
First validated in Spain, more recent versions motor skills. Appropriate for individuals aged 6
were adapted and normalized for Spanish- through 90, the CTONI consists of six subtests:
speaking individuals residing in Argentina pictorial analogies, pictorial categories, pictorial
(Burin, Jorge, Arizaga, & Paulsen, 2000) and the sequences, geometric analogies, geometric cate-
United States (Schrauf, Weintraub, & Navarro, gories, and geometric sequences. The resultant
2006). This measure consists of a list of 30 low- data yield three composite indices, Nonverbal
frequency words printed without their corre- Intelligence Quotient (NIQ), Pictorial Nonverbal
sponding accent (e.g., “moare” is written without Intelligence Quotient (PNIQ), and Geometric
the accented “é.” It means a type of silk material) Nonverbal Intelligence Quotient (GNIQ). The
and requires examinees to add the accent by ver- Test of Nonverbal Intelligence-3 (TONI-3;
bally placing the stress on the correct phoneme. Brown, Sherbenou, & Johnsen, 1997), a 50-item
To do so, individuals must possess both word- test which takes approximately 15 min to admin-
specific knowledge and awareness of the rules of ister, can be similarly used as an estimate of pre-
accentuation (e.g., which syllable requires a lexi- morbid intellectual abilities. Raw scores of are
cal stress, which necessitates an accent). Del Ser then converted to Deviation Quotients with a
and colleagues (1997) reported that the WAT has mean of 100 and an SD of 15; the TONI-3 man-
strong correlations with the Spanish (Spain) ver- ual indicates that non-native English speakers
sion of the WAIS (e.g., Vocabulary subtest (i.e., English as a second language), African-
r, 0.842 and Picture Completion r, 0.722). American individuals, and Hispanic persons
Regression equations are provided for estimating obtained average scores of 93, 95, and 96,
Batería Woodcock-Muñoz Revisada (BWM-R) respectively.
and Ravens Progressive Matrices scores from
WAT scores (Schrauf et al., 2006).
Evaluation of Intellectual Abilities
Measures of Nonverbal Intelligence
Ravens Standard Progressive Matrices (SPM; More comprehensive assessments of intellectual
Raven, Raven, & Court, 1993) is one of the most functioning are often warranted when perform-
widely administered measures of nonverbal rea- ing neuropsychological evaluations with epi-
soning and estimated premorbid intelligence in lepsy patients. Notably, such measures can be
the world. For each of the 60 test items, examin- used to more accurately assess premorbid intel-
ees are required to correctly select one of the ligence, identify areas of strengths and weak-
multiple-choice options in order to complete a nesses, track performance from a neurocognitive
pattern within an organized matrix. This mea- baseline, and as a prognostic indicator of post-
sure assesses one’s ability to form perceptual surgical outcomes.
326 H.A. Bender

Escala de Inteligencia de Wechsler properties, extended “floors” for lower function-


para Adultos ing patients, and a reduced administration time.
A version of the WAIS-III has also been devel-
The Escala de Inteligencia de Wechsler para oped for individuals from Spain (TEA Ediciones,
Adultos (EIWA; Wechsler, Green, & Martinez, 2001); this version of the EIWA-III was normed
1968) is an adaptation of the original version of the on 1,369 subjects representative of the adult pop-
Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS) which ulation of Spain in the late 1990s.
was normed on 616 Puerto Ricans, ranging in age
from 16 to 54 years. Although the development of
the original EIWA represented an important first Assessment of Neurocognitive
step in the assessment of Spanish-speaking per- Domains
sons, it has been widely criticized for having ques-
tionable psychometric properties and a restricted The goals of evaluations aimed solely at charac-
standardization sample (e.g., Lopez & Taussig, terizing seizure focus have considerable overlap
1991; Melendez, 1994). Of note, significant perfor- with those conducted in preoperative contexts, as
mance disparities emerged among bilingual indi- test data are interpreted to assess functional brain
viduals (Spanish/English) administered both the status in populations with epilepsy and, by exten-
WAIS and the EIWA (Melendez, 1994). Perhaps sion, lateralize a resectable region of cortex
most strikingly, cognitive functioning in non-Eng- responsible for seizure onset (Loring, 1997).
lish-speaking Hispanics was shown to be underes- Inasmuch, neuropsychologists administer
timated by the WAIS-R but overestimated using the domain-specific tests with relevance to hemi-
EIWA, whereas WAIS-R subtests indicated cogni- spheric lateralization and functional postopera-
tive impairment in neurologically healthy Spanish tive outcome (e.g., language, visuospatial
speakers, and the EIWA subtests suggested less processing new learning, and delayed recall)
cognitive impairment in participants with (Jones-Gotman, 1991). Briefly, it is believed that
Alzheimer’s disease (Lopez & Taussig, 1991). when the left hemisphere is dominant, left-sided
Attempts were made to develop a “short form” of seizure focus is identified by a relatively consis-
the EIWA, similar to the English-language tent material-specific deficit in verbal abilities
Wechsler Abbreviated Intelligence Scale (WASI), (e.g., naming, verbal fluency) and verbal declara-
by using derivations of 2–5 of the existing subtests; tive memory (Jones-Gotman, 1991). Right-sided
however, many of these combinations lacked the lesions (again, in patients with left hemisphere
ability to calculate verbal and performance IQs dominance) typically correlate with poor visuo-
(Demsky, Gass, Edwards, & Golden, 1998). spatial skills (e.g., visuospatial construction,
Moreover, none of the brief forms with strong reli- visual abstract reasoning) and nonverbal memory
abilities and validities (0.95–0.98) included Object deficits. At times, a clear pattern of material-
Assembly, Digit Span, and Digit Symbol Coding; specific deficits in language or memory-related
the latter two subtests are known to be potential abilities emerge during preoperative assessments,
areas of weakness in epilepsy patients. thereby suggesting the side of epileptogenesis
More recently, several revisions and updates (Buelow & McNelis, 2002; Loring & Meador,
of the EIWA have been published to address 2001; Milner, 1975).
many of the aforementioned concerns. The While there is a considerable, albeit inconsis-
Escala de Inteligencia de Wechsler para tent, body of literature evaluating the predictive
Adultos—Tercera edición (EIWA-III; Wechsler, power of material-specific patterns of neuropsy-
2004, 2008) was published with specific consid- chological deficit in lateralizing seizure focus in
erations and modifications appropriate for Puerto English speakers, comparably few investigations
Ricans. Developed in collaboration with the have studied the efficacy of neuropsychologi-
Ponce School of Medicine in Puerto Rico, the cally predicted lateralization in non-US-born,
EIWA-III possesses improved psychometric non-native English-speaking individuals with
15 The Neuropsychological Assessment of Culturally and Linguistically Diverse Epilepsy Patients 327

epilepsy. Yet, it is well known that a variety of Controlled Oral Word Association Test), (b) ver-
methodological and ecological factors place eth- bal learning (World Health Organization—UCLA
nic minorities at an inherent disadvantage on Auditory Verbal Learning Test final learning,
testing, when compared to monolingual Anglo- short-delay free recall following a distracter, and
Americans, resulting in considerable perfor- 20-min delayed recall trials), (c) attention-mental
mance disparities (Ardila et al., 1994; Rosselli, control (EIWA Digit Symbol and Block Design
Ardila, & Rosas, 1990). Clinically, consistently subtests, as well as Color Trails 1 and 2), (d)
low scores, which may or may not reflect the visuospatial (Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure
patient’s true level of cognitive functioning, can Test—Copy and Delayed Recall scores, as well as
result in an overestimation of cognitive impair- the Ravens Standard Progressive Matrices), and
ment in non-native English-speaking minorities. (e) psychomotor (Pin Test) (Osterrieth, 1944).
Therefore, the risk of misdiagnosis is great. The The construct validity of the NeSBHIS was
subsequent subheadings will introduce three lin- recently evaluated in a sample of Spanish-
guistically appropriate test batteries (e.g., speaking epilepsy patients with Grooved Pegboard
NeSBHIS, RBANS-SRE, and the CCNB) and substituted for the Pin Test. (Bender et al., 2009a,
several domain-specific measures with the 2009b). Specifically, data were analyzed using
potential to yield the breadth and quality of neu- confirmatory factor analysis with the a priori
ropsychological test data necessary for the diag- assumption that variables would load accord-
nostic decision-making and treatment planning ing to the theoretical expectations enumerated
of epilepsy patients. above (Pontón et al., 2000). Findings indicated
that overall model fit indices were in the desired
The Neuropsychological Screening range: Comparative Fit Index = 0.936, Tucker
Battery for Hispanics Lewis Index = 0.915, RMSEA = 0.090, and
The Neuropsychological Screening Battery for SRMR = 0.069. Furthermore, all NeSBHIS sub-
Hispanics (NeSBHIS; Pontón et al., 1996) was tests were found to load significantly (p < 0.001)
specifically developed to address the fundamental on their respective factors; the standardized load-
lack of resources available for use with Spanish- ings were high, ranging from 0.562 to 0.995,
speaking Hispanics. Based on a battery of tests with the exception of Block Design (−0.308).
first used by the World Health Organization (Maj These findings suggest that the NeSBHIS has
et al., 1993, 1994), the NeSBHIS assesses several robust construct validity in a sample of Spanish-
neurocognitive domains, including language, speaking epilepsy patients; all participants were
memory, mental control, psychomotor speed, evaluated at New York University Medical
visuospatial functioning, and nonverbal reason- Center’s Comprehensive Epilepsy Center, a ter-
ing. The principal advantage of the NeSBHIS is tiary care setting in the Northeastern United
that it is one of the limited few to provide norma- States. The diagnostic utility of the NeSBHIS
tive data stratified by age, gender, and education for use with Spanish-speaking epilepsy patients
using a moderately large (N = 300) standardiza- was also evaluated by Barr et al. (2009). Overall,
tion sample of community-referred, Spanish- over 40 % of participants exhibited impairment
speaking Hispanics. According to the findings of (as defined as scores >2 standard deviations
Pontón, Gonzalez, Hernandez, Herrera, and below the normative mean) on measures of nam-
Higareda (2000), the NeSBHIS has a stable factor ing and processing speed. Similar deficits were
structure, indicating that this battery adequately observed in 29 and 26 % of the sample on mea-
measures the putative neuropsychological sures of verbal and visual recall, respectively.
domains that it was designed to assess. The fol- The obtained profile of impairment suggests that
lowing five distinct factors emerged: (a) language the NeSBHIS is sensitive to identifying cognitive
(as measured by the Escala de Inteligencia impairments commonly seen in patients with epi-
Wechsler para Adultos [EIWA] Digit Span sub- lepsy. However, there may be some limitations to
test, Pontón-Satz Boston Naming Test, and the this battery’s sensitivity to identifying deficits in
328 H.A. Bender

patients with lateralized temporal lobe seizures, Repeatable Battery for the Assessment
whereas no significant differences in test per- of Neuropsychological Status
formance were observed between patients with The Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of
video-EEG evidence of left (N = 48) versus right Neuropsychological Status (RBANS; Randolph,
(N = 24) temporal lobe epilepsy. 1998a, 1998b) is a 35–50-min neuropsychologi-
Although the NeSBHIS represents a significant cal test battery often administered in inpatient
advancement in Spanish-language neuropsycho- settings due to its brief administration time.
logical assessment, stringent inclusionary criteria Furthermore, It is a useful tool in hospitals and
were applied to the sample of healthy volunteers clinics because it has parallel forms, allowing NP
(e.g., participants were free of neurological or psy- functioning to be tracked serially over time. IT
chiatric disorder, head trauma, and/or substance was designed to evaluate the domains of atten-
use), thereby potentially limiting its generaliz- tion, language, visuospatial/visuoconstructional
ability to clinically and psychiatrically referred abilities, learning, and delayed memory. The
patient populations. Furthermore, future studies twelve subtests of this battery generate five index
are necessary before utilizing this battery with a scores, as well as an overall total score.
geographically heterogeneous sample of Spanish The RBANS has been translated into Spanish
speakers. According to Pontón et al. (1996), par- by the test developers (Repeatable Battery for
ticipants included in the NeSBHIS reference the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status—
group originated from the following Spanish- Spanish Research Edition; RBANS—SRE;
speaking world regions: Mexico—62 %, Central Randolph, 1998b), and back translation has
America—15 %, and “Other”—23 %. Though the been independently verified by multiple native
composition of the NeSBHIS normative sample Spanish speakers with extensive neuropsychol-
was representative of the Hispanic population ogy training (Barr, personal communication).
(by place of origin) in 1992, these data are not Preliminary evaluation the Spanish-language
indicative of the current cultural characteristics of RBANS suggests that this battery is useful in
the Hispanic population across the United States identifying impairments that are frequently
today. For example, the percentage of Hispanic observed in populations with epilepsy, such as
individuals from Mexico and Puerto Rico differs reduced confrontation naming, new learning,
greatly between New York (Mexico, 9.1 % and delayed verbally and visually mediated recall.
Puerto Rico, 36.63 %) and California (Mexico, However, these observed findings may be an
77.11 % and Puerto Rico, 1.28 %). More broadly, artifact of test bias. In an investigation evalu-
the proportion of Mexican and Puerto Rican ating 42 Spanish speakers with epilepsy
immigrants residing in the Northeastern states administered the RBANS-SRE and 45 age-
(e.g., Connecticut, Maine, Massachusetts, New and education-matched English- speaking epi-
Hampshire, New Jersey, New York, Pennsylvania, lepsy patients administered the RBANS,
Rhode Island, and Vermont) varies considerably (Bender et al., 2009b) significant performance
from the other geographic regions of the con- differences were observed on the Language
tiguous United States. Accordingly, while the Index (p ≤ 0.05), Picture Naming (p ≤ 0.01),
NeSBHIS reference groups are, and continue to and Digit Span ( p ≤ 0.05); linear trends were
be, representative of Hispanic population resid- also noted on the line orientation and list recall
ing in much of the United States, the normative subtests (both p ≤ 0.10). Although diminished
data provided by Pontón et al. (1996) may not performances obtained using the RBANS-SRE
accurately reflect the “standard” performance of a may be attributed to the lexical dialectical issues
Northeastern, predominantly Puerto Rican sample discussed throughout this chapter, such factors
of Spanish speakers. Accordingly, caution should cannot explain the disparities on Digit Span or
be used when administering the NeSBHIS to line orientation subtests. Rather, it is important to
Hispanic epilepsy patients who immigrated from note that the RBANS-SRE lacks unique norms
countries other than Mexico. for Spanish speakers; the English-language
15 The Neuropsychological Assessment of Culturally and Linguistically Diverse Epilepsy Patients 329

standardization sample is used to normalize this Digit Span. As a whole, Hispanics included in
measure regardless of the language of adminis- the normative sample obtained lower scores on
tration. Until a Spanish-language normative the Digit Span and animal fluency subtests than
sample can be collected, the results of the all other groups. Moreover, scores on the Body
RBANS-SRE should be interpreted with Part Naming, WAIS-R Digit Span, and Trail
extreme caution. In the interim, raw scores Making Test did not accurately distinguish neu-
should be used in situations where neuropsy- rologically healthy Hispanic older adults from
chological testing is needed to track changes those with cognitive impairment. Also, as a
from baseline or to compare pre- and postopera- stand-alone measure, the CCNB is not appropri-
tive functioning. ate for the comprehensive assessments typically
administered in an epilepsy setting, as this bat-
Cross-Cultural Neuropsychological tery does not assess verbal learning, verbal
Test Battery delayed recall, and confrontation naming, each
The Cross-Cultural Neuropsychological Test of which is a potential area of impairment in
Battery (CCNB, as discussed in Dick, Teng, individuals with seizure disorders, regardless of
Kempler, Davis, & Taussig, 2002) was developed language and/or country of origin (e.g., Campos-
to assess a broad range of cognitive functioning Castelló & Campos-Soler, 2004; Elger,
in five racially, culturally, and/or linguistically Helmstaedter, & Kurthen, 2004; Jones-Gotman,
diverse populations (i.e., African-American, 1991; Lippé & Lassonde, 2004; Loring, 1997).
Caucasian, Hispanic, Vietnamese, and Chinese
individuals). Taking 90 min to administer, the
CCNB is comprised of 11 subtests, assessing Tests of Language Abilities
functioning in six domains, including: attention
(i.e., WAIS-R Digit Span), reasoning ability (i.e., Measures evaluating expressive language are
Modified Picture Completion), language (i.e., particularly susceptible to sociocultural bias.
Body Part Naming, CERAD category fluency for While confrontation naming tasks have been
animal naming, and Auditory Comprehension), widely recognized as being among the most sen-
visuospatial skills (i.e., Read & Set Time, sitive for identifying and quantifying language
CERAD Drawing, WAIS-R Block Design), psy- impairment, they are also some of the most dif-
chomotor speed (i.e., Trail Making Test, Part A), ficult to maintain equivalence between the elic-
and recent memory (Common Objects Memory ited verbal responses in the source language
Test, including 3 learning trials, 3–5-min recall, versus the target language. Thus, it is important
30-min recall, 5-min recognition, and 30-min for clinicians to maintain awareness of potential
recognition). Of note, the CASI (see the effects of these dialectal lexical differences, as
Assessment of Mental Status and Estimated different terms may be culturally salient to spe-
Premorbid Abilities subheading) is a component cific world regions. For instance, the terms
of the CCNB. “cometa,” “papalote,” “chiringa,” and “barri-
Although the CCNB represents an admirable lete” are all technically correct Spanish-language
effort to design a battery of “culture-fair” test responses to a line drawing of a kite presented on
measures, both education and ethnicity were a confrontation naming test (as presented on the
shown to significantly effect performance on Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of
several of the subtests. According to Dick and Neuropsychological Status—Spanish Research
colleagues (2002), education accounted for Edition; RBANS—SRE; Randolph, 1998b).
approximately 15 % of the variance among While extant Spanish-language confrontation
scores. While ethnicity was not found to account naming subtests (e.g., the Pontón-Satz BNT;
for as much variance, it was still shown to sig- Pontón et al., 1992) provide multiple responses
nificantly impact performance on animal flu- to each test item, it would be nearly impossible
ency, the CERAD drawing task, and the WAIS-R to provide every potentially correct response in
330 H.A. Bender

all regional dialects of Spanish (e.g., with most able to provide exemplars from a broader semantic
notable differences occurring between Peninsular category. Although region-specific standardiza-
Spanish and Spanish of the Americas). In such tion samples and base rates would obviate much
cases, phonemic cueing provided to patients of these concerns (i.e., reduced verbal fluency
after an obvious failure (e.g., when the response produced by Puerto Ricans given instructions
for “latch” is supplied by the patient as “diving “verduras” would not confer as great a disadvan-
board”) becomes nothing more than a culturally tage if the normative sample and base rates of
based “guessing game” when judging which ini- impairment reflected the additional difficulty of
tial phoneme to provide. It is highly unlikely that this subtest), most translated subtests do not pos-
even fully bilingual neuropsychologists have the sess local normative samples which are unique to
knowledge and language ability required to con- world regions or country of origin.
sider all existing correct synonymous options for
each test item administered. Although standard-
ized administration practices dictate that points Test de Vocabulario en Imágenes
can only be awarded for correct responses pro- Peabody
vided in the manual, clinicians should factor in
the role of dialectical differences when interpret- Test de Vocabulario en Imágenes Peabody:
ing the obtained test data. Adaptación Hispanoamericana (TVIP; Dunn,
The role of dialectal lexical differences should Padilla, Lugo, & Dunn, 1986) is a Spanish-
also be considered when evaluating the appropri- language version of the Peabody Picture
ateness and applicability of test instructions and Vocabulary Test, a widely administered measure
their meaning for each individual patient. For of receptive vocabulary. For each of the 125
example, subtest instructions provided for the items, participants are shown four line-drawn
RBANS—SRE (Form A) Semantic Fluency sub- pictures and are asked to select the picture which
test have the potential to affect test performance best describes the word provided by the exam-
and interpretation of derivative test data. The iner. According to test developers, all TVIP items
instructions for this test are as follows: “Ahora were selected through careful item analysis to
quiero que me diga todos los nombres de los dife- maximize their universality and cultural appro-
rentes tipos de frutas y verduras que pueda recor- priateness for Spanish-speaking populations. The
dar.” Although the comparable instructions TVIP possesses two major advantages over other
provided with the English version of the RBANS tests of language abilities, as this measure does
(Harcourt Assessment, 1998) require examinees not require spoken language from the participant,
to “…tell [me] all the names of all different kinds and it possesses normative samples that are spe-
of fruits and vegetables that you can think of,” the cific to Mexican and Puerto Rican examinees, as
Spanish-language text, provided above, is not well as to Hispanics who originate from, or reside
semantically equivalent. Rather, the Spanish in, other world regions (e.g., Compuestas) While
word “verduras” indicates to examinees that they this “compuestas” normative group is a thought-
should provide semantic exemplars from a spe- ful attempt at improving linguistic appropriate-
cific subset of comestible plant life, rather than ness, it is akin to developing one unified set of
the more general, all encompassing category of norms for individuals residing in the U.S., Belize,
“vegetables.” According to the Real Academia Nigeria, New Zealand, Singapore, and Ireland, as
Española’s Diccionario de la lengua Española— English is considered to be the primary language
Vigésima segunda edición (2003), “verduras” is spoken in each of these countries.
defined as “vegetables, especially leafy greens.”
The difference between instructions may result in Pontón-Satz BNT
reduced semantic fluency among Spanish speak- Although naming is one of the most fundamental
ers, stemming from the additional cognitive bur- functions of language, confrontation naming has
den as compared to English speakers who are long been recognized as one of the most sensitive
15 The Neuropsychological Assessment of Culturally and Linguistically Diverse Epilepsy Patients 331

tasks for identifying and quantifying language atively. These data revealed that the patient’s
impairment (Neils et al., 1995). In a well- performance on the English-language BNT (44
designed study, a moderate-sized sample of out of 60 pictures were correctly identified) was
young, well-educated bilingual adults were superior to her Spanish-language naming abili-
administered the Boston Naming Test (BNT; ties (32 out of 60 pictures were correctly identi-
Kaplan, Goodglass, & Weintraub, 1983) in both fied); an additional four responses were
English (L2) and their native language, Spanish considered to be phonemic paraphasias. Although
(L1) (Kohnert, Hernandez, & Bates, 1998). the patient’s diminished Spanish-language naming
Surprisingly, higher scores were obtained in the abilities were assumed to be the direct result of
English-language administration, rather than the the resection of the AVM (presumably leaving
Spanish-language administration. Khonert and substrates underlying English naming abilities
colleagues (Kohnert et al., 1998) posited that the intact), it is difficult, if not impossible, to draw
difference in word frequency across languages such conclusions without an adequate baseline in
was one possible explanation for their findings both languages.
(i.e., the word “funnel” is a higher-frequency Administered as part of the NeSBHIS battery,
word in English than the Spanish word the Pontón-Satz Boston Naming Test (P-S BNT;
“embudo”). Methodologically, it is important to Pontón et al., 1992) is the 30-item Spanish-
note that the aforementioned sample was com- language equivalent of the 60-item English-
prised of well-educated subjects who were edu- language BNT; the Spanish-language adaptation
cated almost entirely in English. The results may also differs in presentation order from the stan-
have been strikingly different had the authors dard BNT. Culturally loaded items for Hispanics
chosen to study a less-educated and less- (e.g., “pretzel” and “beaver”) and stimuli with
acculturated sample. These data underscore the ambiguous or multiple dialectical variations were
need to obtain detailed cultural-linguistic infor- also excluded to promote cultural and linguistic
mation (e.g., onset and trajectory of L1 develop- relevance.
ment and L2 acquisition, acquisition setting,
proficiency level of each language, and language Measures of Verbal Fluency
of education) and acculturation during the clini- Measures of verbal fluency are typically key
cal interview. components to the evaluation of epilepsy patients
As stated elsewhere, bilingual individuals and are frequently shown to be an area of weak-
with epilepsy undergoing neuropsychological ness in this poplication. That said, assessment of
assessment, particularly surgical candidates, verbal fluency is susceptible to test bias due to
often require language testing in both their pri- several linguistically, demographically, and cul-
mary and secondary languages. Gomez-Tortosa, turally mediated variables, including lexical or
Martin, Gaviria, Charbel, and Asuman (1995) dialectical differences, age, level of education,
report a case study of a 25-year-old bilingual bilinguality, and gender (Bender et al., 2009a,
woman undergoing testing prior to surgical resec- 2010; Bethlehem, de Picciotto, & Watt, 2003; da
tion of an arteriovenous malformation (AVM) in Silva, Petersson, Faisca, Ingvar, & Reis, 2004;
the left presylvian region. The patient immigrated Ostrosky-Solís, Gutierrez, Flores, & Ardila,
from a Spanish-speaking country at the age of 10, 2007). In a recent study, Ostrosky-Solís,
receiving the majority of her education in English, Gutierrez, et al. (2007) conducted an investiga-
her second language. As a graduate of a US high tion to determine the contribution of age, educa-
school, the patient was believed to be a “profi- tion, and Spanish-speaking country of origin
cient bilingual” preoperatively. An abbreviated, (e.g.., Mexico, Spain, Argentina) on semantic
30-item BNT was administered in Spanish prior verbal fluency. Although significant performance
to surgery; the English-language BNT was not differences reportedly emerged between coun-
administered. Both Spanish and English adminis- tries, much of the disparity was attenuated when
trations of the BNT were administered postoper- age and education were considered. Beyond the
332 H.A. Bender

variability in age and education, Ostrosky-Solís, istration of this subtest in other languages
Gómez, et al. (2007); Ostrosky-Solís, Gutierrez, necessitates single-digit, multisyllable words
et al. (2007) suggested that the observed differ- (e.g., the number “4” is three syllables in
ences may be the result of administration and Russian). Although language-specific normative
scoring differences. They recommended utilizing sampling would obviate much of these concerns,
a standardized set of instructions (e.g., “I am such data is unavailable for most culturally and
going to ask you to mention all the names of ani- linguistically diverse populations.
mals that come to mind, you have 1 min, and I am
going to tell you when to stop”). Furthermore,
items should receive full, one-point credit if the Tests of Visuospatial Functioning
following criteria are met: (1) the name provided
is one of an animal (e.g., actual, extinct, imagi- Assessment of visuospatial functioning is of par-
nary, or mythical animals); (2) the animal does ticular importance when assessing non-native
not belong to a supraordinate category (e.g., fish); English-speaking epilepsy patients. Specifically,
(3) the animal(s) is not a different breed of the a profile of lateralized deficit whereas visuospa-
same species (e.g., beagle, Chihuahua, fox terrier); tial skills are more impaired than verbally
or (4) animals do not belong to an interspecies mediated skills (e.g., ideally indicating right
variation (e.g., horse/mare). hemisphere pathology), in cultural and linguistic
Language-related bias on a verbal fluency minorities may be is presumably more reliable
measures may also arise secondary to cross- and valid than data indicating a left hemisphere
cultural variations in the phonological structure lateralization (i.e., language problems > visuo-
of words and the amount of effort necessary to spatial problems), due to the widespread test
produce a single word (i.e., the word length biases confounding performance on language-
effect). Speakers of languages with more multi- based tasks. However, multiple anthropological
syllabic words may be at a disadvantage, due to and psychological investigations have demon-
the longer amount of time it takes to produce strated that culture can affect the development of
each word (e.g., Escandell, 2002; Kempler, non-verbal skills, such as perceptual and con-
Teng, Dick, Taussig, & Davis, 1998). To illus- structional abilities (cf. Ardila, 2005; Ardila &
trate, the word “orange” in Russian is Moreno, 2001; Berry, 1979). Yet, in neuropsy-
Оранжевый, pronounced a-ran-zhi-viy, which chology, our understanding of the cognitive dis-
is a 4-syllable word. The Spanish the word for turbances that are pathognomonic for underlying
“orange” is naranja, a 3-syllable word, as com- neuropathology is largely based on a very limited
pared to the 2-syllable word in English. Based and heterogeneous subgroup of world cultures,
on this example, when asked to rapidly produce namely, Western, urbanized, middle-class, liter-
exemplars to the category of “fruit,” neurologi- ate, brain-damaged individuals (Ardila &
cally healthy Russian speakers would ostensibly Moreno, 2001). To this end, clinicians should
be able to provide fewer words than their contextualize their patient’s performance on
Spanish-speaking counterpart, who, in turn, visuospatial skills, just as they would on tests of
would be able to generate less words than their verbal materials—on a case-by-base basis by
English-speaking peer in a 1-min period of time, considering previous exposure and cultural
due solely to the longer length of each word and relevance.
the amount of time each word requires to pro- Among the first studies implicating cultural
duce. Moreover, the impact of the word length differences in visuospatial abilities is the land-
effect on Digit Span subtests should also be care- mark study by Gay and Cole (1967) in which
fully considered; although English requires the Kpelle farmers were compared to working-class
working memory abilities to hold single-digit, Americans on a cognitive estimations task; farm-
single-syllable number in mind (with the excep- ers were considerably more accurate in visually
tion of the two-syllable number “seven”), admin- estimating the amount of rice presented on bowls
15 The Neuropsychological Assessment of Culturally and Linguistically Diverse Epilepsy Patients 333

of different sizes containing varying amounts of positively correlated with performance on the
rice. A similar, much more recent evaluation of RSPM (Spearman r, 0.411, p = 0.006), BVMT-R
the Aruaco Indians, an indigenous population total recall (Spearman r, 0.344, p = 0.018),
residing in Colombia (Ardila & Moreno, 2001), RCFT Copy (Spearman r, .386, p = 0.007), and
further underscores performance disparities on delayed recall (Spearman r, 0.338, p = 0.022).
visual tasks, such as the Wechsler Intelligence However, degree of acculturation was inversely
Scale for Children-Revised (WISC-R) Block correlated with visuomotor dexterity bilaterally
Design subtest (Spanish-language administra- (Spearman r, −0.361, −0.318, p ≤ 0.05). These
tion), the Rey-Osterreith Complex Figure Test preliminary findings demonstrate that the impact
(ROCFT), Recognition of Overlapped Figures, of acculturation is not attenuated by neurologi-
and the Ideomotor Praxis Test. Specifically, 15 % cal illness and suggest that acculturation should
of the participants studied, those with no formal be considered when administering tests of visuo-
education and very low levels of Western accul- spatial and visuomotor abilities to an epilepsy
turation, were “virtually unable” to draw a cube sample.
or copy the ROCFT, as they had never used a
pencil before or had they engaged in any drawing
or copying activity. Although considerable diffi- Tests of Learning and Memory
culties were also noted on the WISC-R Block
Design subtest (i.e., each of these participants As stated above, subtests of the NeSBHIS,
received a score of zero), performance on the RBANS, CCNB, and NEUROPSI Attention
Overlapped Figures and Ideomotor Praxis tests and Memory test include multiple measures of
were nearly perfect. Ardila and Moreno (2001) new learning and delayed recall in both verbal
posit that the pencil-based tasks, and those reliant and non-verbal modalities (e.g., WHO-UCLA
on time, lacked meaningfulness to the less- Auditory Verbal Learning Test, ROCFT RBANS
acculturated Aruacos, whereas tasks evaluating List Learning, Story Memory, and Figure Recall,
the functions which underlie their activities of Common Objects Memory Test) (Osterrieth,
daily living (e.g., visual discrimination and motor 1944). While the NeSBHIS and RBANS have
coordination which are required to hunt) were demonstrated clinical utility and adequate psycho-
much more successful. metric properties for use with epilepsy patients,
Both the NeSBHIS and the RBANS include the CCNB, NEUROPSI Attention and Memory
measures of visuospatial construction and percep- test, and other Spanish-language measures of
tion (i.e., Ravens Standard Progressive Matrices learning and memory (i.e., the Story Memory Test
(RSPM), ROCFT, RBANS Figure Copy and and Spanish Verbal Learning Test) would require
RBANS Line Orientation); however, the extent similar empirical study and revalidation in a neu-
to which these tasks are affected by cultural fac- rological sample.
tors is largely unclear, particularly in clinical The Story Memory Test (SMT; Artiola i
populations. A recent study by Saez et al. (2008) Fortuny, Heaton, & Hermosillo, 1998) is a
examined the relationship between accultura- 29-item measure of verbal learning which gener-
tion and visuospatial/visuomotor performance ates learning efficiency (based on learning trials
in a Spanish-speaking epilepsy sample; partici- 1–5), delayed retention (based on a 1-h delayed
pants were administered the ROCFT, Ravens recall), and recognition indices (36 items pre-
Standard Progressive Matrices (RSPM), Grooved sented in a “yes/no” response paradigm presented
Pegboard (GP), and Brief Visual Memory Test- after the 1-h delayed recall assessment). The
Revised (BVMT-R). The English domain of SMT is modeled after English-language Story
the Bidimensional Acculturation Scale (BAS) Memory Test adapted by Heaton, Grant, and
was used to determine participants’ degree of Matthews (1991); however, the English-language
acculturation to English-speaking, U.S. culture. version has a memory evaluation after a 4-h
Findings revealed that level of acculturation was period. The Spanish Verbal Learning Test (SVLT;
334 H.A. Bender

Artiola i Fortuny et al., 1998) is a serially pre- Digit Span subtest would load on the Language
sented list learning task modeled after the format Factor, not on the Attention-Mental Control
of the English-language California Verbal Factor. While findings from an epilepsy sample
Learning Test (CVLT; Delis, Kramer, Kaplan, & (Bender et al., 2009a) were consistent with these
Ober, 1987). The SVLT includes two distinct a priori assumptions, the obtained factor structure
16-item lists (Lists A and B) which share seman- was unexpected, as the Digit Span subtest is typi-
tic categories. According to Artiola i Fortuny and cally a robust measure of sustained auditory atten-
coauthors (1998), words were selected by asking tion and executive functioning in English-speakers
45 Spanish speakers from ten different Spanish- with epilepsy (e.g., Bornstein, Drake, & Pakalnis,
speaking countries to generate words belonging 1988). The above findings obtained in a Spanish-
to several semantic categories; categories which speaking epilepsy sample suggest that the mea-
did not share regional commonalities were dis- sures subsumed by the NeSBHIS
carded. Six of the remaining categories were Attention—Mental Control Factor (i.e., EIWA
selected for inclusion on lists A and B if they Digit Span, EIWA Block Design, and Color Trails
shared words that possessed the same meaning in A and B) require multiple cognitive processes
the Spanish-speaking regions of the world that (e.g., cognitive flexibility, speed of processing,
were sampled. Both the SMT (Aprendizaje de response suppression), rather than a single unitary
Prosa) and SVLT (Aprendizaje de Palabras) are function. Accordingly, assessing specific qualita-
presented in the Appendix A of Artiola i Fortuny, tive and quantitative variables, including Color
Heaton & Hermansillo (1998). Trails 2 set loss errors, COWAT number of pro-
duced words, perseverances, and errors, as well as
maximum span on Digits Backward, may provide
Tests of Attention and Executive further explanations of the NeSBHIS component
Abilities structure and, by extension, executive abilities in
Spanish-speaking Hispanic populations. Similar
As described above, the NeSBHIS contains an attempts to define specific factors from frontally
Attention-Mental Control Factor, which includes mediated tasks have been successfully undertaken
the EIWA Digit Symbol and Block Design sub- throughout the extant literature (American
tests and Color Trails 1 and 2. Of note, the EIWA Psychiatric Association, 2000; Burgess & Wood,
Block Design subtest is not a strong indicator of 1990; Nagahama et al., 2003; Rodriguez-Aranda
Attention and Mental Control, as it failed to meet & Sundet, 2006).
the criteria for factor loading in Pontón and co- Clinicians should also consider the moderat-
authors’ (2000) original factor analytic study of ing role of measurable culture-specific variables
the NeSBHIS (it was included on this factor based on attention and executive functioning, as task
on historical data). Although EIWA Block Design performance in this domain has been shown to be
loaded onto this factor in a more recent confirma- strongly influenced by acculturation (Coffey
tory analysis evaluating the construct validity of et al., 2005). Results of the aforementioned study
the NeSBHIS in a Hispanic epilepsy population suggested that higher levels of acculturation were
(Bender et al., 2009a, 2009b), 91 % of subtest associated with significantly improved Wisconsin
variance was explained by other constructs. This Card Sorting Test performance. Future studies
finding is not entirely surprising, as Block Design are needed to assess the role of acculturation on
may not accurately measure the putative cognitive performance on other measures of attention and
domain that it was developed to assess in non- executive abilities in epilepsy samples, as well as
native English-speaking, non-Western cultures, the effects of other culture-specific factors (e.g.,
because it requires specific cognitive processes degree of bilinguality) on this neurocognitive
that may not be taught and cultivated across cul- domain. Although a series of studies by
tures (Ardila, 1995; Cohen, 1969). Similarly, the Bialystock (1987) suggest that bilinguals are
original theoretical model of the NeSBHIS superior to monolinguals on tasks requiring high
(Pontón et al., 2000) indicated that the EIWA levels of selective attention, these observations
15 The Neuropsychological Assessment of Culturally and Linguistically Diverse Epilepsy Patients 335

should be further evaluated in adult, non-English- ogy of affective disorders in this population is
speaking clinical samples. multifactorial in nature and includes neurobio-
logical and clinical substrates (e.g., seizure
foci), presence of psychosocial stressors (e.g.,
Motor Speed ability to maintain employment, financial con-
cerns, marital status), and diminished health-
Time, as a construct, is strongly culture driven and related quality of life (e.g., unpredictable nature
is a known moderator of neuropsychological test of seizures, medication-related side effects) (see
performance (Perez-Arce & Puente, 1996). For Barry et al., 2007, for review). Specific culture-
example, while the concept of a “fast perfor- related factors which may contribute to
mance” needed by a patient to perform well on increased affective distress in non-US-born,
measures of motor speed, is believed to be an immigrant populations with epilepsy may
important cultural value in the United States, it is include feelings of stigmatization, real or per-
not necessarily valued in many other cultural ceived community-based discrimination, mis-
groups (Ardila, 2005). Specifically, in Western, conceptions regarding disease symptoms, and
industrialized nations, the concept of time holds its etiology, and/or poor access to health care.
elevated importance, and therefore the culture
emphasizes punctuality and adherence to a
schedule (e.g., Rojas-Méndez, Davies, Omer, Personality Inventories
Chetthamrongchai, & Madran, 2002). In contrast,
the ethos in developing countries, as well as sev- Currently, there is no “gold standard” for the
eral others in Eastern Europe, Latin America, and mental health assessment of non-native English-
the Mediterranean, emphasizes the “quality” of speaking and/or immigrant populations. Many
the interaction, rather than the time spent engag- well-standardized objective measures of person-
ing in it. In this vein, individuals may take a more ality generate concerns regarding their appro-
relaxed, flexible attitude towards scheduling and priateness and applicability cross-culturally.
spending increased lengths of time with people Although translated into 150 languages for use
and on tasks that are interesting, enjoyable, or in 50 countries, many of the translations of the
mentally challenging (Perez-Arce & Puente, Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory
1996). Thus, a patient who is not a native of the (MMPI or MMPI-2; Hathaway & McKinley,
United States or another country which upholds a 1989) have been reported to be inaccurate and
“clock time” orientation may be at a distinct dis- lacking in linguistic equivalence (Butcher, 1985).
advantage upon speed-dependant tasks. For example, in an English-to-Spanish transla-
tion of the MMPI-2, the item “dirt frightens or
disgusts me” is translated as “the concept of dirti-
Assessment of Psychological ness/being dirty bothers me” (Butcher, 1985).
and Behavioral Functioning As a result of this difference in linguistic con-
notation, the statement was endorsed 58–68 %
Patients with epilepsy have a high prevalence of more frequently by Mexican adolescents and
comorbid psychiatric disorders. young adults than in the standardization sample.
Depression is among the most common, par- Butcher (1985) attributes this high rate of item
ticularly in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy, endorsement as being the result of the alteration
with estimates ranging from 13 to 18 % or in meaning; in Mexican cultures the concept of
higher (Patten et al., 2005; Tellez-Zenteno, dirtiness is commonly associated with disease,
Patten, Jetté, Williams, & Wiebe, 2007). Anxiety poverty, and infection.
disorders are also frequently noted affecting Multiple investigators have also demonstrated
approximately one third of epilepsy patients that elevations on MMPI/MMPI-2 scores can be
(Barry, Lembke, Gisbert, & Gilliam, 2007; attributed to a Hispanic immigrant’s traditional-
Kobau, Gilliam, & Thurman, 2006). The etiol- ity or retention of “home culture” (see Dana,
336 H.A. Bender

1995). The importance of acculturation as a sig- tinguish it from panic disorder and dissociative
nificant moderating variable again underscores disorder. Symptom clarification in patients with
the need to address this construct during both the ataque de nervios is also particularly challenging
clinical interview and via self-report ratings. due to its similar presentation to epilepsy.
Such data provides clinicians with the ability to
“correct” elevated MMPI/MMPI-2 profiles
which may otherwise falsely indicate the pres- Projective Testing
ence of psychopathology.
Although projective testing is not typically
included in standard assessments of patients with
Diagnostic Interviews seizure disorders they may be used as a supple-
mental measure of psychological status, social-
Given the paucity of well-standardized, valid, emotional functioning, and personality. Such
and reliable psychometric instruments, the use measures are also useful for individuals with lim-
of informal, unstructured, or semi-structured ited levels of formal education and literacy or
interviews may be a valuable tool in obtaining those who may not possess the level of language
clinically relevant psychological information. proficiency needed to complete written self-
Rather than relying on confusing, misleading, report inventories or audiotaped versions of per-
or culturally charged questionnaires and per- sonality inventories. To this end, the TEMAS
sonality inventories, contextual information, (“temas” corresponds to the Spanish word
such as acculturation level, can also be dis- “themes”) or Tell-Me-A-Story test is a culturally
cussed. Diagnostic interviews may also be ben- sensitive version of the Thematic Apperception
eficial because many of the traditional, Western Test (TAT) initially developed for urban Hispanic
behavior rating scales cannot fully capture the children and adolescents. Created by Costantino,
“culture-bound syndromes” discussed in the Malgady, and Rogler (1988), the TEMAS is par-
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental ticularly effective for use with Hispanics and
Disorders (DSM-V; 2013). For example, com- other children of non-Anglo backgrounds. This
monly administered symptom rating scales measure is comprised of brightly colored pictures
would likely misdiagnose an individual suffer- of Hispanic and African-American characters
ing from an “ataque de nervios,” a culture-bound engaged in different situations within an urban
idiom of distress consisting of a dramatic dis- setting and presents a conflict that provides the
play of emotion with an acute onset principally basis for a child-driven narrative. While viewing
reported in Latino and Caribbean populations. each picture, the child is asked to create a story
It is not very highly uncommon for individu- about the character and the scene he/she is
als of Latino heritage to suffer from an ataque engaged in. Responses are transcribed verbatim
de nervios upon receiving bad news, such as the and scored for its cognitive style, affective state,
death of a loved one. Although varying in pre- and personality functioning. Multiple cognitive
sentation, sufferers may fall to the ground and lie dimensions are evaluated by the TEMAS, each
there motionless or violently convulse as if hav- with varying degrees of reliability and validity,
ing a seizure (Guarnaccia, Lewis-Fernandez, & including 18 “cognitive” areas (e.g., fluency,
Marano, 2003). Although these episodes are typi- imagination, reaction time, and sequencing),
cally brief, a period of post ataque exhaustion, affective state (e.g., “ambivalent,” “angry,” “inap-
similar to postictal fatigue exhibited in epilepsy propriate,” “neutral,” or “sad”) and personality
patients, is not unusual. Although experienc- functions (e.g., aggression, anxiety/depression,
ing an ataque de nervios is strongly associated interpersonal relations, reality testing, and sexual
with meeting criteria for a range of anxiety and identity). Normative data are available for Puerto
depression-related diagnoses, (Guarnaccia et al., Rican and non-Puerto Rican respondents; the lat-
2003), there are typically subtle factors that dis- ter group is further partitioned by country of origin
15 The Neuropsychological Assessment of Culturally and Linguistically Diverse Epilepsy Patients 337

or region, including the Dominican Republic, manner, encouraging prayer, rituals, and offer-
Mexico, and South America. Although the struc- ings to rid the individual of demonic influence.
ture and psychometric properties of the TEMAS A national survey conducted for the Epilepsy
have been a target of criticism in recent years, Foundation of America (2005) reported that
this measure may yield important insights onto 30 % of Hispanic individuals with epilepsy
the patient’s worldview, affective state, and per- believe that seizures can be successfully treated
sonality traits which may be untapped by other by herbal, holistic remedies, a spiritual healer, or
measures. an exorcism (6 %).
Related investigations sought to develop a
reliable and valid rating scale, the Epilepsy
Health-related Quality of Life Beliefs and Attitudes Scale (EBAS), to assess the
beliefs and attitudes about epilepsy in a culturally
Limited studies have empirically assessed the diverse sample residing in North America (e.g.,
attitudes and beliefs of culturally and linguisti- Caucasians, East Asians, and South Asians)
cally diverse patients towards epilepsy or other (Gajjar, Geva, Humphries, Peterson-Badali, &
chronic neurological conditions. In 2005, Sirven Ostubo, 2000). The revised version of the scale,
and collaborators conducted a survey examining which was refined from pilot testing, consisted of
knowledge and perceptions about epilepsy in a 51 items pertaining to a vignette about a child
sample of 760 Spanish-speaking adults living in with complex partial seizures, 18 items examin-
the seven metropolitan areas of the U.S. with the ing the medical beliefs of the individuals’
highest concentrations of Hispanics. Several biological, psychological, and neurological con-
interesting themes emerged regarding the general stitution, 16 items describing non-medical beliefs
terminology and connotations of the disorder in influencing individuals to have seizures, and 13
this population. Although convulsiones or ataque items indicating positive and negative attitudes
were the most common terms to describe seizure- towards people with epilepsy. Overall, Caucasians,
related phenomena, the third most frequently as a group, were more likely to select neurologi-
provided term was “sobredosis,” which is trans- cal explanations as the root causes of epilepsy
lated as “overdose,” suggesting a connection with than South or East Asians, who endorsed more
drug or alcohol abuse. In a similar vein, seizures enviro-psycho-physical and metaphysical causes.
were more likely to be perceived by Hispanics to The differences in attitudes and beliefs about epi-
be the result of bad behavior (e.g., “sins,” “lack of lepsy and its potential impact on psychological
spiritual faith,” or “evil spirits”), severe medical and psychosocial functioning reflect the need to
condition (e.g., brain tumors, anoxia at birth, formally address these constructs during the clin-
head injury during infancy, neurocysticercosis), ical intake, assessment (via the EBAS or similar
or contagious illness (e.g., meningitis), as com- scale), or feedback session(s) with the patients
pared to non-Hispanic controls. Moreover, a per- and/or their family.
son with epilepsy was perceived as “dangerous to Given the broad range of attitudes and beliefs
others” in 31 % of Hispanic respondents, as com- surrounding an epilepsy diagnosis, it is not unex-
pared to 17 % on non-Hispanic interviewees. pected that the concept of health-related quality
Given the health attitudes surrounding epi- of life would also vary greatly across cultures.
lepsy, it is not surprising that many Hispanic indi- Although multiple instruments assessing quality
viduals with epilepsy consult with traditional of life (QOL) in epilepsy have been developed
faith healers, such as santeros, curanderos, or (e.g., the QOLIE-89 and Liverpool QOL Battery),
yerberos, in addition to or instead of, Westernized scales appropriate for multi-national cohorts are
health-care providers. In addition to being more difficult to develop due to translation issues and
accessible to historically underserved popula- differing conceptualizations of “health” across
tions, “spiritual practitioners” often address the cultures (Cramer et al., 1998). A 31-item mea-
cause of seizures in a more culturally acceptable sure of QOL, the Quality of Life in Epilepsy
338 H.A. Bender

Inventory (QOLIE-31), was derived from the to defer their decision-making or transfer their
QOLIE-89 and professionally translated into power to another family member, community
Chinese, Danish, Dutch, German, Canadian elder, or advocate. In such cases, clinicians need
French, French, Italian, Spanish, Swedish, and to balance their adherence to privacy practice
UK English. QOLIE-31 items were shown to regulations with their respect of the patient’s
cluster into two distinct factors: emotional/ wishes. Although eager to learn and share the
psychological effects (i.e., overall QOL, seizure results of testing with their family and/or com-
worry, emotional well-being, and energy/ munity members, patients may wish to disclose
fatigue) and medical/social effects (i.e., medica- certain aspects of test findings but not others
tion effects, work-driving-social limitations, and (e.g., having the clinician divulge the cognitive
cognitive functioning subscales) (Cramer et al., sequelae of a disorder but not the psychiatric and
1998). Although validation of the QOLIE-31 is behavioral symptoms). These requests should be
still needed for most languages and related cultural explicitly discussed with the patients prior to the
group, this instrument possesses the strong psy- start of the group feedback session.
chometric properties (e.g., reliability and validity) Neuropsychologists should also recognize that
needed to comprehensively address epilepsy- a patient’s view of the doctor-patient relationship
specific QOL across a range of cultural groups. can vary greatly based on their culture/ethnicity.
Specifically, individuals raised in cultures which
emphasize adherence to a hierarchy of power or
Feedback of Neuropsychological authority may be reluctant to challenge or contra-
Evaluation Findings dict the clinician and his or her recommendations.
To illustrate, Streltzer (2004) presented an anec-
The informational and therapeutic benefits of dote of a middle-aged Filipino-American patient
feedback sessions have been well described with dangerously high blood pressure who refused
throughout the extant literature (e.g., Finn & to tell the prescribing physician that he had
Tonsager, 1992; Quirk, Strosahl, Kreilkamp, stopped taking anti-hypertensives several weeks
& Erdberg, 1995). During such sessions clini- prior (due to intolerable side effects) in deference
cians have the opportunity to not only explain to the physician’s perceived “authority.” Similarly
the neuropsychological test findings and their deferential attitudes has been described in middle-
“real-world” implications through face-to-face or class Argentine culture (Madinaveitia, personal
telephone meetings. However, the goals of such communication). To facilitate open, honest com-
sessions often widen further in breadth and scope munication, neuropsychologists should explicitly
with culturally and linguistically diverse popu- encourage patients and their families to utilize a
lations. Rather, it may be necessary to provide collaborative team approach to treatment plan-
psychoeducation regarding the causes, mani- ning. Rather than dictating a treatment plan and
festations, and comorbidities of epilepsy to the potentially fostering an “us” (patient/family) ver-
patient and their family members. Providing this sus “them” (neuropsychologist and medical treat-
type of information is of particular importance ment team) mentality, it is important to actively
when treating newly arrived immigrant popula- engage the patients in the decision-making
tions and/or individuals who are predominantly process via an “us” (the neuropsychologist, medi-
monolingual speakers of a language other than cal treatment team, patient, and their family) versus
English, such patients are likely to have limited “disease” approach. Once these parameters are
access to other health-care providers. Like all established, neuropsychologists should be encour-
other aspects of neuropsychological assessment, aged to taking a directive, problem-focused
the presentation of assessment findings is largely approach to treatment. Recommendations should
governed by culture-specific factors, such as be made explicit and presented in a “cause and
interactional style, expectations, and health atti- effect” format, for example, “if your Mother
tudes. For example, it is not unusual for patients attends psychotherapy, we not only expect for her
15 The Neuropsychological Assessment of Culturally and Linguistically Diverse Epilepsy Patients 339

symptoms of depression to lessen, but also for ating effects of culture-specific experiences (e.g.,
an improvement in any mood-related memory years of education, level of acculturation, degree
problems.” of bilinguality) should also be thoroughly consid-
ered and objectively measured, prior to, during,
and after engaging in testing procedures.
Summary
Acknowledgement Preparation of this chapter was com-
Careful consideration of the psychometric and pleted with the assistance of Dr. Victoria Stein, Dr. Ann
De Sollar, Dr. Jessica Spat, Ms. Melinda Cornwell, and
culture-specific factors with the potential for
Ms. Maria.
causing test bias is particularly critical in the neu-
ropsychological evaluation of epilepsy patients.
Failure to do so may obscure the true pattern of
the patient’s neuropsychological deficit, resulting
References
in potentially incorrect test administration, scoring, American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and
data interpretation, case formulation, and treat- statistical manual of mental disorders – Text revision
ment recommendations. Rather, clinicians should (DSM-IV-TR). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric
critically evaluate the sources of information Association.
Anastasi, A. (1988). Psychological testing (6th ed.).
prior to drawing definitive conclusions about the New York, NY: Macmillan.
causal factors of the patient’s neuropsychological Ardila, A. (1995). Directions of research in cross-cultural
and behavioral performance and functioning neuropsychology. Journal of Clinical and Experimen-
(which, at times, can differ considerably). To this tal Neuropsychology, 17, 143–150.
Ardila, A. (2005). Cultural values underlying psychomet-
end, several key pieces of information should be ric cognitive testing. Neuropsychology Review, 15,
considered. First, the test measures should be 185–195.
professionally translated, back translated, and Ardila, A., & Moreno, S. (2001). Neuropsychological test
critically assessed for dialectal and/or cultural performance in Aruaco Indians: An exploratory study.
Journal of the International Neuropsychological
artifacts. Second, selected assessment measures Society, 7, 510–515.
should have strong psychometric properties when Ardila, A., Rosselli, M., & Puente, A. E. (1994).
evaluating a clinical sample (e.g., populations Neuropsychological evaluation of the Spanish speaker.
with known cerebral dysfunction). More specifi- New York, NY: Plenum.
Arnold, B. R., Montgomery, G. T., Castaneda, I., &
cally, it should be statistically determined Longoria, R. (1994). Acculturation and performance
whether or not test data correctly measures what of Hispanics on selected Halstead–Reitan neuropsy-
it was intended to measure (e.g., construct valid- chological tests. Assessment, 1, 239–248.
ity), reliably evaluates the desired constructs, and Aronson, J., Lustina, M. J., Good, C., Keough, K., Steele,
C. M., & Brown, J. (1999). When white men can't do
is stable over time (E.G. Test Retest Reliability). math: Necessary and sufficient factors in stereotype
Third, the battery selected should assess a broad threat. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35,
range of functions, and these abilities should be 29–46.
differentially compared across the patient’s pri- Aronson, J., Steele, C. M., Salinas, M. F., & Lustina, M. J.
(1998). The effects of stereotype threat on the stan-
mary and secondary languages. Fourth, the nor- dardized test performance of college students. In
mative sample selected to normalize patient data E. Aronson (Ed.), Readings about the social animal
should be as closely matched to the sample charac- (8th ed., pp. 415–430). New York, NY: Freeman.
teristics of the standardization sample as possible. Artiola i Fortuny, L., Heaton, R. K., & Hermosillo, D.
(1998). Neuropsychological comparisons of Spanish-
When such comparisons are unavailable (e.g., the speaking participants from the US-Mexico border
English-language standardization sample was region versus Spain. Journal of the International
used to “norm” test data produced by a Spanish Neuropsychological Society, 4, 363–379.
speaker), these discrepancies should be clearly Artiola i Fortuny, L., & Mullaney, H. A. (1997).
Neuropsychology with Spanish speakers: Language
noted throughout the patient’s report and care- use and proficiency issues for test development.
fully considered during all aspects of the case Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology,
conceptualization. Lastly, the potentially moder- 19, 615–622.
340 H.A. Bender

Barr, W. B., Bender, H. A., Morrison, C., Cruz-Laureano, Burgess, P. W., & Wood, R. L. (1990). Neuropsychology
D., Vazquez, B., & Kuzniecky, R. (2009). Diagnostic of behaviour disorders following brain injury. In R. L.
validity of a neuropsychological test battery for Wood (Ed.), Neurobehavioural sequelae of traumatic
Hispanic patients with epilepsy. Epilepsy and brain injury (pp. 110–132). London: Taylor &
Behavior, 16, 479–483. Francis.
Barry, J. J., Lembke, A., Gisbert, P. A., & Gilliam, F. Burin, D. I., Jorge, R. E., Arizaga, R. A., & Paulsen, J. S.
(2007). Affective disorders in epilepsy. In A. Ettinger (2000). Estimation of premorbid intelligence: The
& I. Kanner (Eds.), Psychiatric issues in epilepsy: A Word Accentuation Test – Buenos Aires version.
practical guide to diagnosis and treatment (pp. 203– Journal of Clinical and Experimental Psychology, 22,
247). Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams & 677–685.
Williams. Butcher, J. N. (1985). Current developments in MMPI
Bender, H. A., Cole, J. R., Aponte, M., Cruz-Laureano, use: An international perspective. In J. N. Butcher &
D., Myers, L., Vazquez, B., et al. (2009a). Construct C. D. Spielberger (Eds.), Advances in personality
validity of the NeSBHIS in a neurological sample. assessment (Vol. 4, pp. 83–94). Hillsdale, NJ:
Journal of the International Neuropsychological Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.
Society, 15, 217–224. Caltabiano, N. (1984). Perceived differences in ethnic
Bender, H. A., García, A. M., & Barr, W. B. (2010). An behavior: A pilot study of Italo-Australian Canberra
interdisciplinary approach to neuropsychological test residents. Psychological Reports, 55, 867–873.
construction: Perspectives from translation studies. Campos-Castelló, J., & Campos-Soler, S. (2004).
Journal of the International Neuropsychological Neuropsychology and epilepsy. Revista de Neurologia,
Society, 16, 227–232. 39, 166–177.
Bender, H. A., Rivera-Rodriguez, M., Aponte-Samalot, Coffey, D. M., Marmol, L., Schock, L., & Adams, W.
M., MacAllister, W. S., Murphy, K., Karantzoulis, S., (2005). The influence of acculturation on the
et al. (2009b). The clinical utility of the RBANS- Wisconsin card sorting test by Mexican Americans.
Spanish Language Edition in a neurological sample. Archives for Clinical Neuropsychology, 20, 795–803.
San Diego, CA: 7th Annual Conference of the Cohen, R. A. (1969). Conceptual styles, culture conflict,
American Academy of Clinical Neuropsychology. and nonverbal tests. American Anthropologist, 71,
Berry, J. W. (1979). Culture and cognition style. In 828–856.
A. Mrsella, R. G. Tharp, & T. J. Ciborowski (Eds.), Cortes, D. E., Rogler, L. H., & Malgady, R. G. (1994).
Perspectives in cross-cultural psychology (pp. 117– Biculturality among Puerto Rican adults in the United
135). New York, NY: Academic. States. American Journal of Community Psychology,
Berry, J. W. (1980). Acculturation as varieties of adapta- 22, 707–721.
tion. In A. Padilla (Ed.), Acculturation: Theory, mod- Costantino, G., Malgady, R. G., & Rogler, L. H. (1988).
els and some new findings (pp. 9–26). Boulder, CO: Manual for the TEMAS thematic apperception tests.
Westview Press. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.
Berry, J. W., Trimble, J. E., & Olmedo, E. L. (1986). Cramer, J. A., Perrine, K., Devinsky, O., Bryant-
Assessment of acculturation. In W. J. Lonner & J. W. Comstock, L., Meador, K., & Hermann, B. (1998).
Berry (Eds.), Field methods in cross-cultural research Development and cross-cultural translations of a
(pp. 291–325). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. 31-item quality of life in epilepsy inventory. Epilepsia,
Bethlehem, D., de Picciotto, J., & Watt, N. (2003). 39, 81–88.
Assessment of verbal fluency in bilingual Zulu- Cultural Competence in Clinical Psychiatry (Core
English speakers. South African Journal of Psychology, Competencies in Psychotherapy S) by Wen-Shing
33, 236–240. Tseng and Jon, M.D. Streltzer (Aug 2004).
Bialystock, E. (1987). Words as things: Development of da Silva, C. G., Petersson, K. M., Faisca, L., Ingvar, M., &
word concept by bilingual children. Studies in Second Reis, A. (2004). The effects of literacy and education
Language Learning, 9, 133–140. on the quantitative and qualitative aspects of semantic
Boone, K. B., Victor, T. L., Wen, J., Razani, J., & Ponton, verbal fluency. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
M. (2007). The association between neuropsychologi- Neuropsychology, 26, 266–277.
cal scores and ethnicity, language, and acculturation Dana, R. H. (1995). Culturally competent MMPI assess-
variables in a large patient population. Archives of ment of Hispanic populations. Hispanic Journal of
Clinical Neuropsychology, 22, 355–365. Behavioral Sciences, 17, 305–319.
Bornstein, R. A., Drake, M. E., Jr., & Pakalnis, A. (1988). Davila, Y. R., Reifsnider, E., & Pecina, I. (2011).
WAIS-R factor structure in epileptic patients. Familismo: influence on Hispanic health behaviors.
Epilepsia, 29, 14–18. Applied Nursing Research, 24, 67–72.
Brown, L., Sherbenou, R., & Johnsen, S. (1997). Test of Deaux, K., Bikmen, N., Gilkes, A., Ventuneac, A., Joseph,
nonverbal intelligence: A language-free measure of Y., Payne, R., et al. (2007). Becoming American:
cognitive ability. Austin, TX: Pro-ed. Stereotype threat effects in Black immigrant groups.
Buelow, J. M., & McNelis, A. (2002). Should every child Social Psychology Quarterly, 70, 384–404.
with epilepsy undergo a neuropsychological evalua- DeGiorgio, C., Pietsch-Escueta, S., Tsang, V., Corral-
tion? Epilepsy & Behavior, 3, 210–213. Leyva, G., Ng, L., Medina, M. T., et al. (2005). Sero-
15 The Neuropsychological Assessment of Culturally and Linguistically Diverse Epilepsy Patients 341

prevalence of Taenia solium cysticercosis and Taenia Gajjar, M., Geva, E., Humphries, T., Peterson-Badali, M.,
solium taeniasis in California, USA. Acta Neurologica & Ostubo, H. (2000). A new scale to assess culture-
Scandinavica, 111, 84–88. specific beliefs and attitudes about epilepsy. Epilepsy
Del Ser, T., Gonzalez-Montalvo, J. I., Martinez-Espinosa, and Behavior, 1, 235–255.
S., Delgado-Villapalos, C., & Bermejo, F. (1997). Gasquoine, P. G. (1999). Variables moderating cultural
Estimation of premorbid intelligence in Spanish peo- and ethnic differences in neuropsychological assess-
ple with the Word Accentuation Test and its applica- ment: The case of Hispanic Americans. Clinical
tion to the diagnosis of dementia. Brain and Cognition, Neuropsychology, 13, 376–383.
33, 343–356. Gay, J., & Cole, M. (1967). The new mathematics and an
Delis, D. C., Kramer, J. H., Kaplan, E., & Ober, B. A. old culture: A study of learning among the Kpelle of
(1987). California verbal learning test. San Antonio, Liberia. New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.
TX: The Psychological Corporation. Gill, J. R., Lenz, K. A., & Amolat, M. J. (2003). Gunshot
Demers, P., Robillard, A., LaFleche, G., Nash, F., fatalities in children and adolescents in New York
Heyman, A., & Fillenbaum, G. (1994). Translation of City. Journal of Forensic Science, 48, 832–835.
clinical and neuropsychological instruments into Gomez-Tortosa, E., Martin, E. M., Gaviria, M., Charbel,
French: The CERAD experience. Age and Ageing, 23, F., & Asuman, J. I. (1995). Selective deficit of one lan-
449–451. guage in a bilingual patient following surgery in the left
Demsky, Y., Gass, C., Edwards, W. T., & Golden, C. J. perisylvian area. Brain and Language, 48, 320–325.
(1998). Optional short forms of the Spanish WAIS Graves, A. B., Larson, E. B., Edland, S. D., Bowen, J. D.,
(EIWA). Assessment, 5, 361–364. McCormick, W. C., McCurry, S. M., et al. (1996).
Department of Homeland Security. (2009). 2009 Yearbook Prevalence of dementia and its subtypes in the
of immigration statistics. Washington, DC: Office of Japanese American population of King County,
Immigration Statistics. http://www.dhs.gov/xlibrary/ Washington State: The Kame Project. American
assets/statistics/yearbook/2009/ois_yb_2009.pdf. Journal of Epidemiology, 144, 760–771.
Dick, M. B., Teng, E. L., Kempler, D., Davis, D. S., & Guarnaccia, P. J., Lewis-Fernandez, R., & Marano, M. R.
Taussig, I. M. (2002). The cross-cultural neuropsycho- (2003). Toward a Puerto Rican popular nosology:
logical test battery. In F. R. Ferraro (Ed.), Minority and Nervios and ataque de nervios. Cultural Medical
cross-cultural aspects of neuropsychological assess- Psychiatry, 27, 339–366.
ment (pp. 17–41). Lisse: Swets and Zeitlinger. Halliday, M. A. K., McKintosh, A., & Strevens, P. (1970).
Dunn, L., Padilla, E., Lugo, D., & Dunn, L. (1986). Test The users and the uses of language. In J. A. Fishman
de vocabulario en imagenes peabody: Adaptacion (Ed.), Readings in the sociology of language (pp. 139–
Hispano-Americana (peabody picture vocabulary 169). The Hague: Mouton.
test-Latin american adaptation). Circle Pines, MN: Hammill, D. D., Pearson, N. A., & Wiederhold, J. L.
American Guidance Services. (1997). Comprehensive Test of Nonverbal Intelligence
Ediciones, T. E. A. (2001). WAIS-III, Escala de (CTONI). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Inteligencia Wechsler para Adultos – III. Madrid: TEA Harris, J. G., Tulsky, D. S., & Schultheis, M. T. (2003).
Ediciones. Assessment of non-native English speaker:
Elger, C. E., Helmstaedter, C., & Kurthen, M. (2004). Assimilating history and research to guide clinical
Epilepsy and cognition. Lancet Neurology, 3, 663–672. practice. In D. S. Tulsky & D. H. Saklofske (Eds.),
Epilepsy Foundation of America. (2003). Report of the Clinical interpretation of the WAIS-III and WMS-III
2003 National Conference on Public Health and (pp. 343–390). San Diego, CA: Academic.
Epilepsy. Landover, MD: Epilepsy Foundation of Hathaway, S. R., & McKinley, J. C. (1989). MMPI-2.
America. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.
Epilepsy Foundation of America. (2005). Survey: Beliefs Heaton, R. K., Grant, I., & Matthews, C. (1991).
about epilepsy among Hispanics a significant national Comprehensive norms for an expanded Halstead–
health issue. Landover, MD: Epilepsy Foundation of Reitan Battery: Demographic corrections, research
America. findings, and clinical applications. Odessa, FL:
Escandell, V. A. (2002). Cross-cultural neuropsychology Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.
in Saudi Arabia. The cross-cultural neuropsychologi- Ivey, A., & Ivey, M. (2003). Intentional interviewing and
cal test battery. In F. R. Ferraro (Ed.), Minority and counseling: Facilitating client development in a multi-
cross-cultural aspects of neuropsychological assess- cultural society. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.
ment (pp. 299–325). Lisse: Swets and Zeitlinger. Jones-Gotman, M. (1991). Localization of lesions by neu-
Finn, S. E., & Tonsager, M. E. (1992). Therapeutic effects ropsychological testing. Epilepsia, 82, S41–S52.
of providing MMPI-2 test feedback to college students Kaplan, E. F., Goodglass, H., & Weintraub, S. (1983). The
awaiting therapy. Psychological Assessment, 4, Boston naming test (2nd ed.). Philadelphia, PA: Lea &
278–287. Febinger.
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). Kempler, D., Teng, E. L., Dick, M., Taussig, I. M., &
Mini mental state: A practical guide for grading the Davis, S. (1998). The effects of age, education, and
cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of ethnicity on verbal fluency. Journal of the International
Psychiatric Research, 12, 189–198. Neuropsychological Society, 4, 531–538.
342 H.A. Bender

Kennepohl, K., Douglas, S., Nabors, N., & Hanks, R. Marin, G., Sabogal, F., Marin, B. V., Otero-Sabogal, R., &
(2004). African American acculturation and neuropsy- Perez-Stable, E. J. (1987). Development of a short
chological test performance following traumatic brain acculturation scale for Hispanics. Hispanic Journal of
injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Behavioral Sciences, 9, 183–205.
Society, 10, 566–577. Medina, M. T., Rosas, E., Rubio-Donnadieu, F., & Sotelo,
Kobau, R., Gilliam, F., & Thurman, D. J. (2006). J. (1990). Neurocysticercosis as the main cause of
Prevalence of self-reported epilepsy or seizure disor- late-onset epilepsy in Mexico. Archives of Internal
der and its associations with self-reported depression Medicine, 150, 325–327.
and anxiety: results from the 2004 HealthStyles Melendez, F. (1994). The Spanish version of the WAIS:
Survey. Epilepsia, 47, 1915–1921. Some ethical considerations. The Clinical
Kohnert, K. J., Hernandez, A. E., & Bates, E. (1998). Neuropsychologist, 8, 388–393.
Bilingual performance on the Boston Naming Test: Mena, F. J., Padilla, A. M., & Maldonado, M. (1987).
Preliminary norms in Spanish and English. Brain and Acculturative stress and specific coping strategies
Language, 65, 422–440. among immigrant and later generation college stu-
Lippé, S., & Lassonde, M. (2004). Neuropsychological dents. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 9,
profile of intractable partial epilepsies. Revue 207–225.
Neurologique (Paris), 160, S144–S153. Milner, B. (1975). Psychological aspects of focal epilepsy
Liu, H. C., Chou, P., Lin, K. N., Wang, S. J., Fuh, J. L., and its neurosurgical management. Advances in
Lin, H. C., et al. (1994). Assessing cognitive abilities Neurology, 8, 299–321.
and dementia in a predominantly illiterate population Nagahama, Y., Okina, T., Suzuki, N., Matsuzaki, S.,
of older individuals in Kinmen. Psychological Yamauchi, H., Nabatame, H., et al. (2003). Factor
Medicine, 24, 763–770. structure of a modified version of the Wisconsin card
Lopez, S. R., & Taussig, I. M. (1991). Cognitive–intellec- sorting test: An analysis of executive deficit in
tual functioning of Spanish-speaking impaired and non- Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment.
impaired elderly: Implications for culturally sensitive Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 16,
assessment. Psychological Assessment, 3, 448–454. 103–112.
Loring, D. W. (1997). Neuropsychological evaluation in Neils, J., Baris, J. M., Carter, C., Dell’aira, A. L., Nordloh,
epilepsy surgery. Epilepsia, 38, S18–S23. S. J., Weiler, E., et al. (1995). Effects of age, educa-
Loring, D. W., & Meador, K. J. (2001). Cognitive and tion, and living environment on Boston Naming Test
behavioral effects of epilepsy treatment. Epilepsia, 42, performance. Journal of Speech and Hearing
24–32. Research, 38, 1143–1149.
Lynn, R., Backhoff, E., & Contreras, L. A. (2005). Ethnic Nelson, H. E. (1982). National adult reading test: Test
and racial differences on the Standard Progressive manual. Windsor, Berks: NFER-Nelson. Office of
Matrices in Mexico. Journal of Biosocial Science, 37, Population Censuses and Surveys (OPCS) (1980).
107–113. Classification of occupations. London: HMSO.
Maj, M., D’Elia, L., Satz, P., Janssen, R., Zaudig, M., Osterrieth, P. A. (1944). Le test de copie d’une figure
Uchiyama, C., et al. (1993). Evaluation of two new complex. Contribution a l’etude de la perception et de
neuropsychological tests designed to minimize cul- la memoire. Archives de Psychologie, 30, 286–356.
tural bias in the assessment of HIV-1 seropositive per- Ostrosky-Solís, F., Ardila, A., & Rosselli, M. (1997).
sons: A WHO study. Archives of Clinical Manual, instructivo y protocolo de aplicación.
Neuropsychology, 8, 123–135. Neuropsi: Evaluación neuropsicológica breve en
Maj, M., Satz, P., Janssen, R., Zaudig, M., Starace, F., Español.. México: Bayer de México.
D’Elia, L., et al. (1994). WHO Neuropsychiatric Ostrosky-Solís, F., Ardila, A., & Rosselli, M. (1999).
AIDS study, cross-sectional phase II: Neuropsi: A brief neuropsychological test battery in
Neuropsychological and neurological findings. Spanish with norms by age and educational level.
Archives of General Psychiatry, 51, 51–61. Journal of the International Neuropsychological
Marin, G. (1992). Issues in the measurement of accul- Society, 5, 413–433.
turation among Hispanics. In K. Geisinger (Ed.), Ostrosky-Solís, F., Gómez, M. E., Ardila, A., Rosselli,
Psychological testing of Hispanic (pp. 235–251). M., Pineda, D., & Matute, E. (2003). NEUROPSI:
Washington, DC: American Psychological Atención y memoria. Manual, perfiles y material.
Association. México: American Bookstore & Teletón.
Marin, G., & Gamba, R. J. (1996). A new measurement of Ostrosky-Solís, F., Gómez, E., Matute, E., Rosselli, M.,
acculturation for Hispanics: The Bidimensional Ardila, A., & Pineda, D. (2007). NEUROPSI attention
Acculturation Scale for Hispanics (BAS). Hispanic and memory: A neuropsychological test battery in
Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 18, 297–316. Spanish with norms by age and educational level.
Marin, G., & Marin, B. V. (1991a). Research with Applied Neuropsychology, 14, 156–170.
Hispanic populations. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Ostrosky-Solís, F., Gutierrez, A. L., Flores, M. R., & Ardila,
Marin, G., & Marin, B. V. (1991b). Applied social science A. (2007). Same or different? Semantic verbal fluency
research methods series: vol 23. Research with across Spanish-speakers from different countries.
Hispanic populations. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 22, 367–377.
15 The Neuropsychological Assessment of Culturally and Linguistically Diverse Epilepsy Patients 343

Patten, S. B., Beck, C. A., Kassam, A., Williams, J. V., Rosselli, M., Ardila, A., & Rosas, P. (1990). Neuropsycho-
Barbui, C., & Metz, L. M. (2005). Long-term medi- logical assessment in illiterates. II. Language and
cal conditions and major depression: strength of praxic abilities. Brain and Cognition, 12, 281–296.
association for specific conditions in the general Rushton, J. P., & Jensen, A. R. (2005). Thirty years of
population. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 50, research on race differences in cognitive ability.
195–202. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 11, 235–294.
Perez-Arce, P., & Puente, A. (1996). Neuropsychological Saez, P., Bender, H. A., Barr, W. B., Morrison, C.,
assessment of ethnic minorities. In R. J. Sbordone & C. J. Vazquez, B., & Rivera-Mindt, M. (2008). The rela-
Long (Eds.), Ecological validity of neuropsychological tionship between acculturation and non-verbal neu-
tests (pp. 283–300). Delray Beach, FL: GR Press. ropsychological test performance among Hispanic
Phinney, J. S., & Rotheram, M. J. (1987). Children’s eth- epilepsy patients. Atlanta, GA: 37th Annual
nic socialization: pluralism and development. London: Meeting of the International Neuropsychological
Sage Publications. Society.
Pontón, M. O., & Ardila, A. (1999). The future of neuro- Santiago-Grisoli, J. S. (2000). Epilepsy of the border-
psychology with Hispanic populations in the United lands: seizure disorders in US Latinos. Epilepsy and
States. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 17, Behavior, 1, 150–152.
565–580. Schrauf, R. W., Weintraub, S., & Navarro, E. (2006). Is
Pontón, M. O., Gonzalez, J. J., Hernandez, I., Herrera, L., adaptation of the Word Accentuation Test of premor-
& Higareda, I. (2000). Factor analysis of the neuro- bid intelligence necessary for use among older,
psychological screening battery for Hispanics Spanish-speaking immigrants in the United States?
(NeSBHIS). Applied Neuropsychology, 7, 32–39. Journal of the International Neuropsychological
Pontón, M. O., Satz, P., Herrera, L., Ortiz, F., Urrutia, Society, 12, 391–399.
C. P., Young, R., et al. (1996). Normative data strati- Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air: How stereotypes
fied by age and education for the Neuropsychological shape the intellectual identities and performance of
Screening Battery for Hispanics (NeSBHIS): Initial women and African Americans. American
report. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Psychologist, 52, 613–629.
Society, 2, 96–104. Steele, C. M., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and
Pontón, M. O., Satz, P., Herrera, L., Young, R., Ortiz, F., the intellectual test performance of African-
D’Elia, L., et al. (1992). Modified Spanish version of Americans. Journal of Personality and Social
the Boston Naming Test. The Clinical Neuropsycho- Psychology, 69, 797–811.
logist, 3, 334. Tellez-Zenteno, J. F., Patten, S. B., Jetté, N., Williams, J.,
Preux, P., & Druet-Cabanac, M. (2005). Epidemiology & Wiebe, S. (2007). Psychiatric comorbidity in epi-
and aetiology of epilepsy in sub-Saharan Africa. The lepsy: A population-based analysis. Epilepsia, 48,
Lancet Neurology, 1, 21–31. 2336–2344.
Quirk, M. P., Strosahl, K., Kreilkamp, T., & Erdberg, P. Teng, E. L., Hasegawa, K., Homma, A., Imai, Y., Larson,
(1995). Personality feedback consultation to families E., Graves, A., et al. (1994). The cognitive abilities
in a managed mental health care practice. Professional screening instrument (CASI): A practical test for
Psychology: Research and Practice, 26, 27–32. cross-cultural epidemiological studies of dementia.
Randolph, C. (1998a). RBANS manual. San Antonio, TX: International Psychogeriatrics, 6, 45–58.
The Psychological Corporation. Toury, G. (1980). In search of a theory of translation.
Randolph, C. (1998b). RBANS manual (US Spanish Tel Aviv: The Porter Institute for Poetics and
research). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Semiotics.
Corporation. Trimble, J., Lonner, W., & Boucher, J. D. (1983). Stalking
Raven, J., Raven, J. C., & Court, J. H. (1993). Manual for the Wily Emic: Alternatives to cross-cultural measure-
the Raven’s progressive matrices and vocabulary ment. In S. H. Irvine & J. W. Berry (Eds.), Human
scales. Oxford: Oxford Psychologists Press. assessment and cultural factors. New York, NY:
Real Academia Española. (2003). Diccionario de la len- Plenum.
gua española, vigésima segunda edición. Madrid: Tsai, R. C., Lin, K. N., Wang, H. J., & Liu, H. C. (2007).
Espasa Calpe. Evaluating the uses of the total score and the domain
Ríos, J., & Fernández Torres, J. (2004). McGraw-Hill’s scores in the Cognitive Abilities Screening Instrument,
Spanish for healthcare providers. New York, NY: Chinese version (CASI C-2.0): Results of confirma-
McGraw-Hill. tory factor analysis. International Psychogeriatrics,
Rodriguez-Aranda, C., & Sundet, K. (2006). The frontal 19, 1051–1063.
hypothesis of cognitive aging: Factor structure and age U.S. Bureau of the Census. (1999). United States Census,
effects on four frontal tests among healthy individuals. race, Hispanic origin, and ancestry: Why, what
The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 167, 269–287. and how. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of
Rojas-Méndez, J. I., Davies, G., Omer, O., Commerce.
Chetthamrongchai, P., & Madran, C. (2002). A time U.S. Bureau of the Census. (2000). Statistical abstract of
attitude scale for cross cultural research. Journal of the United States: 2000 Washington, DC. Suitland,
Global Marketing, 15, 117–147. MD: U.S. Bureau of the Census.
344 H.A. Bender

Valle, R. (1990). Cultural and ethnic issues in Alzheimer’s stroke subtype incidence among whites, blacks, and
disease family research. In E. Light & B. Lebowitz Hispanics: The Northern Manhattan Study.
(Eds.), Alzheimer’s disease treatment and family stress Circulation, 111, 1327–1331.
(pp. 122–154). London: Taylor & Francis. Wong, T. M., Strickland, T. L., Fletcher-Janzen, E.,
Wechsler, D. (2004). Escala de Memoria de Wechsler Ardila, A., & Reynolds, C. R. (2000). Theoretical and
III. Manual Tecnico. Madrid: TEA Ediciones. practical issues in the neuropsychological assessment
Wechsler, D. (2008). Manual de administración y puntu- and treatment of culturally dissimilar patients. In
ación. Escala de Inteligencia Wechsler para Adultos – E. Fletcher-Janzen, T. L. Strickland, & C. R. Reynolds
III. San Antonio, TX: Pearson. (Eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural neuropsychology
Wechsler, D., Green, R. F., & Martinez, J. N. (1968). (pp. 3–18). New York, NY: Academia.
Manual para la Escala de Inteligencia Wechsler para Zahuranec, D. B., Brown, D. L., Lisabeth, L. D., Gonzales,
Adultos (EIWA). Cleveland, OH: Psychological N. R., Longwell, P. J., Eden, S. V., et al. (2006).
Corporation. Differences in intracerebral hemorrhage between
White, H., Boden-Albala, B., Wang, C., Elkind, M. S., Mexican Americans and non-Hispanic whites.
Rundek, T., Wright, C. B., et al. (2005). Ischemic Neurology, 66, 30–34.
Index

A Armstrong, H.E., 239


Acculturation process Arnold, L.M., 215
immigrant populations, 318 Artiola i Fortuny, L., 334
personality inventories, 336 Ashburner, J., 158
potential impact, 319 Associational learning, 90, 107
two-dimensional model, 319 Asuman, J.I., 331
Acculturative stress, 319 ATL. See Anterior temporal lobectomy (ATL)
Acquired epileptic aphasia, 278 Attachment theory, 261
AD. See Alzheimer’s disease (AD) Attention
Adaptive Behavior Assessment System, Second Edition deficits, rehabilitation, 246
(ABAS-II), 289 epilepsy surgical patients, 106
ADHD. See Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), 11, 42
(ADHD) Attention-Mental Control Factor, 334
Adolescents with epilepsy Atypical language lateralization, 101–102
epilepsy syndromes, 39–42 Au, A., 239
evaluation of cognitive domains Automatic subcortical segmentation methods, 162
attention functions, 42–45 Automatisms, behavioral, 3
executive functions, 42–45 Axis II personality disorders, 215–216
language, 46–47 Axis I psychiatric disorders, 214–215
learning and memory, 45–46
mood and quality of life, 48–50
visuomotor skills, 48 B
visuospatial skills, 48 BADS. See Behavioural Assessment of the Dysexecutive
intellectual function, 37–39 Syndrome (BADS)
neuropsychological assessment, 37 BAI. See Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI)
seizures, 38–40 Baillet, S., 195–210
Adult Memory and Information Processing Battery Baker, G.A., 50
(AMIPB), 16 Barr, W.B., 1–31, 243, 327
AEDs. See Antiepileptic drugs (AEDs) BAS. See Bidimensional Acculturation Scale (BAS)
Aldenkamp, A.P., 245, 251, 252 Baseline language testing, 142
Altshuler, L., 217 Bauch-Prater, U., 46
Alzheimer’s disease (AD), 9, 68, 69 BDI. See Beck Depression Inventory (BDI)
Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale–cognitive Beavis, J.M., 216
subscale (ADAS-Cog), 7 Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI), 225
Amobarbital anesthetic effect, 131 Beck Depression Inventory (BDI), 223
Angelman syndrome, 279–280 Bedside mapping, 140–141
Animal Naming Test, 21 Beery-Buktenica Developmental Test
Annegers, J.F., 214 of Visual-Motor Integration, Fifth Edition
Anterior temporal lobectomy (ATL), 129, 169 (Beery VMI), 286
Antiepileptic drugs (AEDs), 6–8, 38 Beghi, E., 215
cognitive side effects, 6, 7, 12, 76, 304–305 Behavioral automatisms, 3
epilepsy surgical candidate, 87, 100 Behavioral interventions, for PWE, 246–247
geriatric patients with epilepsy, 70–71 Behavioral monitoring
Anxiety disorders, 48, 225–227, 236, 335 functional MRI, 182
Ardila, A., 333 MEG and, 206–207

W.B. Barr and C. Morrison (eds.), Handbook on the Neuropsychology of Epilepsy, 345
Clinical Handbooks in Neuropsychology, DOI 10.1007/978-0-387-92826-5,
© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015
346 Index

Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function CCNB. See Cross-Cultural Neuropsychological Test
(BRIEF), 44 Battery (CCNB)
Behavioural Assessment of the Dysexecutive Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale
Syndrome (BADS), 20 (CES-D), 214, 224
Bell, B., 45 Cerebrovascular disease, 65
Bender, H.A., 317–339 Chapin, J., 63–82
Benign rolandic epilepsy (BRE), 39 Chaplin, J.E., 243
Benign syndrome, 39 Charbel, F., 331
Ben-Porath, Y.S., 229 Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL), 42
Benson, F., 215 Childhood absence epilepsy (CAE), 39–40
Benton, A.L., 21 Chung, C.K., 207
Berry, J.W., 319 Cicerone, K.D., 243
Bialystock, E., 334 Cognitive Abilities Screening Instrument (CASI), 324
Bidimensional Acculturation Scale (BAS), 319, 333 Cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT), 239–240
Billingsley-Marshall, R.L., 209 Cognitive dysfunction, 73
Binder, J.R., 169–190 Cognitive functions, deficits, 242
Biofeedback technique, 241 Cognitive rehabilitation, 242
Blackmon, K., 155–164 Cognitive remediation approaches, PWE, 249–252
Blashfield, R.K., 228 attention deficits rehabilitation, 246
Blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) cognitive deficits, 241–242
effect, 172 cognitive interventions, 245–246
signal, 170, 171, 174 cognitive rehabilitation, 242
Blum, D., 217, 218 memory deficits, 243–245
BNT. See Boston Naming Test (BNT) memory rehabilitation, 242–243
Bogliun, G., 215 Cole, M., 332
Boone, K.B., 96, 319 Comorbid disease, 73
Borkovec, T., 227 Complex partial seizures, 5
Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination (BDAE), 21 Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI),
Boston Naming Test (BNT), 21, 285, 331 218–219
Bowyer, S.M., 206 Comprehensive Test of Nonverbal Intelligence
Boylan, L.S., 217 (CTONI), 325
Brain imaging, 157, 170 Consistent long-term retrieval (CLTR), 185
Brandling-Bennett, E.M., 275–297 Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s
BRE. See Benign rolandic epilepsy (BRE) Disease (CERAD), 323
Breteler, M.M., 68, 69 Contextual learning, 90, 107, 108
Brief Visuospatial Memory Test, Revised Continuous performance tests (CPTs), 19, 43–44
(BVMT-R), 287 Continuous recognition memory (CRM) paradigm, 205
Briquet, P., 213 Continuous spike, epilepsy with, 279
Brown, R.J., 215, 262 Corsi, P., 18
Brown’s theory, 263 Cortical malformation, 155, 157
Bruni, J., 240 Cortical mapping, 139–141
Burden of normality, 313 Cowan, J., 50
Butcher, J.N., 335 CPTs. See Continuous performance test (CPTs)
Cramer, J.A., 214, 217, 218
Crandall, P., 217
C Crespi, V., 215
California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT), 22, 334 Cross-Cultural Neuropsychological Test Battery
California Verbal Learning Test, Children’s Edition (CCNB), 329
(CVLT-C), 287 Cull, C., 240
California Verbal Learning Test, Second Edition Culturally and linguistically diverse populations
(CVLT-II), 287 Attention-Mental Control Factor, 334
Camfield, P.R., 38 CASI, 324
Campo, P., 207 clinical interview
Caplan, R., 46 acculturation, 318–320
Carbamazepine (CBZ), 71 ethnic identity, 318
Carlson, C.E., 139–153 patient’s family structure, 321
Cascino, G., 214 primary language of assessment, 322
CBCL. See Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) stereotype threat, 320
CBZ. See Carbamazepine (CBZ) diagnostic interviews, 336
Index 347

EIWA, 326 Lennox-Gastaut syndrome, 278


executive abilities test, 334–335 NEAD during pregnancy, 281
feedback, 338–339 overview, 275
health-related quality of life, 337–338 primary generalized epilepsy, 276–277
intellectual abilities, 325 Rett syndrome, 280
language abilities test, 329–330 temporal lobe epilepsy, 276
learning/memory test, 333–334 tuberous sclerosis, 277–278
motor speed, 335 West syndrome, 277
neurocognitive domains assessment, 326 Devinsky, O., 145, 217, 243
CCNB, 329 Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders
NeSBHIS, 327–328 (DSM), 215
RBANS, 328–329 Diagnostic and Statistic Manual of Mental Disorders,
NEUROPSI, 324–325 Fourth Edition, Text Revision, 282
nonverbal intelligence measures, 325 Dichotic listening tasks, 54
overview, 317–318 Dick, M.B., 329
personality inventories, 335–336 Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI), 157, 162–163
projective testing, 336–337 DiIorio, C., 238
psychological/behavioral functioning, 335 Diller, L., 251
translated test materials, 323–324 Djoki, R., 239
TVIP, 330–332 Djordjevic, J., 15
visuospatial functioning test, 332–333 DKEFS. See Delis-Kaplan Executive Functioning
Word Accentuation Test, 325 System (DKEFS)
Culture-bound syndromes, 336 Dodrill, C., 15, 25
Cunningham, R., 218 Dodrill, C.B., 18
CVLT. See California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT) Dogali, M., 145
Donovan, D.M., 239
Down syndrome, 279
D Drane, D.L., 87–116
Dahl, J., 240 Dravet syndrome, 280–281
Dalmagro, C.L., 97 DTI. See Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI)
Davies, S., 49, 214
Davila, 321
Davis, G.R., 239 E
de Araujo Filho, G.M., 215 Echoplanar imaging (EPI), 171, 172
de Graaf, E.H., 214 Edeh, J., 214
Deale, A., 239 Edinburgh Handedness Inventory, 20
Delis-Kaplan Executive Functioning System (DKEFS), E/IPSPs. See Excitatory/inhibitory postsynaptic
19, 44, 93, 96 potentials (E/IPSPs)
Delrahim, S., 217 Eiser, C., 50
Del Ser, T., 325 EIWA. See Escala de Inteligencia de Wechsler para
Dementia, epilepsy and, 68–69 Adultos (EIWA)
Depression, 48, 93, 214, 216, 217, 235, 236, 239, 305, Electrical brain stimulation, 143–145
306, 335 Electrical status epilepticus, during sleep, 279
Developmental delay Electroclinical syndromes, 4
Angelman syndrome, 279–280 Electrocortical mapping of language
case examples, 290–297 bedside mapping, 140–141
Down syndrome, 279 brain stimulation, 143–145
Dravet syndrome, 280–281 case study, 150–151
fragile X syndrome, 280 comprehension, 148–149
intellectual disability and, 275, 281 cortical mapping, 139–140
adaptive functioning, 288–289 expressive speech, 147–148
comprehensive neurocognitive screening tests, 284 functional mapping, 141, 150, 151
definition, 282 intraoperative mapping, 152–153
inherited, 280 language mapping, 140
IQ score measurement, 283–284 language testing
language, 285 expressive speech, 147–148
memory, 286–288 principles, 145–147
neurocognitive functioning, 282–283 mapping
nonverbal tests, 284 procedure, 141–143
visuospatial functioning, 286 in special populations, 149–150
Landau-Kleffner syndrome, 278, 279 reading and writing, 149
348 Index

Elger, C.E., 67, 96, 303 and object recognition, 106–107


Emlen, A.C., 306 quality of life, 110
Emotional distress, 235 RCIs vs. alternate test forms, 103
Employment, psychosocial impact on, 307–308 sensory functioning, 109
Engelberts, N.H.J., 246 visual processing, 106–107
Engel, J. Jr., 215 neuropsychological evaluation of, 87
EPI. See Echoplanar imaging (EPI) neuropsychological findings, 113
Epilepsy neuropsychology role, 87–88
cognitive and behavioral impairment in, 10 neurosurgical intervention, 87
cognitive impact nociferous cortex hypothesis, 92, 100–101
antiepileptic medications, 304–305 outcome, 111
chronic TLE, 303–304 PCE
description, 302–303 cognitive studies, 97–98
subjective, 305 pre-and postsurgical deficits in, 99
with continuous spike, 279 potential confounds in, 100
defined, 1, 2 practice effects, 101
psychosocial impact pre and postsurgical deficits, 88
description, 305–306 problems with instrument design, 100
on employment, 307–308 TLE
quality of life, 306–307 associational learning, 90
Epilepsy Beliefs and Attitudes Scale (EBAS), 337 contextual learning, 90
Epilepsy monitoring unit (EMU), 102, 112 fluency tasks, 91
Epilepsy surgery, 123, 124, 126, 130, 131, 156, 183 functional neuroimaging studies, 92
Epilepsy surgical patients MTS, 88
AEDs, 87, 100 pre-and postsurgical deficits in, 94–95
atypical language lateralization, 101–102 visual memory deficits, 89
clinical vignette, 112–115 Epilepsy syndromes, 3
effect of comorbid conditions, 101 BRE, 39
EMU, 112 CAE, 39–40
FLE focal lobe, 5, 6
memory performance, 96 frontal lobe epilepsies, 40
motor tasks, 93 JAE, 40
neurocognitive functioning, 93 JME, 5, 40
pre-and postsurgical deficits in, 98 occipital lobe epilepsies, 41
problem-solving tasks, 95 pediatric, 4–5
psychiatric functioning, 96 temporal lobe, 5
verbal fluency, 95 TLE, 40
functional adequacy model, 111 Epileptic encephalopathies, 41
functional reserve hypothesis, 111 Equivalent current dipole (ECD) model, 200
future practice and research, 115–116 Escala de Inteligencia de Wechsler para Adultos
ictal and interictal discharges, 101 (EIWA), 326, 334
medical procedures, 112 Escala de Inteligencia de Wechsler para Adultos—
neurocognitive domains, 105 Tercera edición (EIWA-III), 326
neurocognitive functions to, 104–105 Ethnic identity, epilepsy, 318
neurocognitive testing, 114 Ettinger, A., 214
neuropsychological assessment, 87, 100, 101 Euler, M., 169–190, 195–210
academic achievement, 109 Evidence-based cognitive remediation, 242
attention, 106 Excitatory/inhibitory postsynaptic potentials
clinical interview, 110 (E/IPSPs), 195
common test batteries, 110–111 Executive abilities test, 334–335
executive control processes, 108 Executive dysfunction, in children, 43
general intellectual functioning, 108 Expressive Vocabulary Test, Second Edition
inpatient vs. outpatient assessment, 102–103 (EVT-2), 285
language, 105–106 Extra-operative language mapping, 54
learning, 107–108
medical record review, 110
memory, 107–108 F
mood, personality, and psychiatric inventories, Facial Recognition Test (FRT), 21, 23
109–110 Fanning, K., 217, 218
motor functioning, 109 FCD. See Focal cortical dysplasia (FCD)
neuropsychological tests, 103 Fedio, P., 45
Index 349

Fenwick, P.B.C, 241 tailoring resections, 186


Feucht, M., 46 tasks, 179–182
Finger Tapping Test, 20, 109 design and timing, 173–175
Fiordelli, E., 215 Functional neuroanatomy, 144
Fisher, R.S., 307 Functional reserve hypothesis, 111
Fiszer, U., 217
FLE. See Frontal lobe epilepsy (FLE)
fMRI. See Functional MRI (fMRI) G
Focal cortical dysplasia (FCD), 157 Gabapentin (GBP), 71
Focal epilepsy, 5, 150, 155, 163 GAD-7. See Generalized Anxiety Disorder 7 (GAD-7)
Focal seizures, 2, 66, 77, 246 Gaitatzis, A., 215
clinical review, 12 Gallagher, T., 218
descriptors of, 3 Gaviria, M., 331
effects of, 11 Gay, J., 332
evolution of, 3 General Health Questionnaire (GHQ), 220–221
Folstein Mini-Mental Status Exam, 323 Generalized Anxiety Disorder 7 (GAD-7), 226
Ford, C.V., 270 Geriatric patients with epilepsy, 63
Fractional anisotropy (FA), 162 case study
Fragile X syndrome, 280 background, 78
Friston, K.J., 158 interpretations and recommendations, 79, 81
Frontal lobe epilepsy (FLE), 5, 40, 241 medical follow-up, 81
memory performance, 96 neuropsychological evaluation, 78–82
motor tasks, 93 cognition impact, 75–76
neurocognitive functioning, 93 cognitive decline risk, 76
pre-and postsurgical deficits in, 98 and dementia, 68–69
problem-solving tasks, 95 depression and anxiety rates, 70
psychiatric functioning, 96 diagnosis
verbal fluency, 95 for cognitive symptoms, 75
Frontal lobe (FL) seizure, 91 complications, 65–66
Frontal Systems Behavior Scale (FrSBE), 20 effect of age on cognition in, 66–68
FRT. See Facial Recognition Test (FRT) elderly patients, 64
Functional adequacy model, 111 modification of activities, 76–77
Functional mapping neuropsychological assessment, 75
designing paradigms for, 173 neuropsychologist role, 72–73
language, 141, 150, 151 appointment scheduling, 73
MEG, 208 interview, 73–74
Functional MRI (fMRI), 169 potential etiologies, 77
behavioral monitoring, 182 prevalence and incidence, 64
biophysical basis of, 170 psychosocial considerations in, 69–70
clinical applications, 183 QOL, 69, 77–78
clinical use, 178–179 reporting the findings, 74
clustered acquisition, 175 review of medications, 77
conducting clinical steps, 183–185 risk factors for cognitive morbidity, 68
data analysis principles, 175–177 seizures
determination algorithm, 177 diagnosis, 66
EEG and, 186–187 etiology, 65
EPI, 171, 172 type, 64
epilepsy surgery, 183 selection of test, 74, 75
episodic memory, 182–183 treatment
geometric distortion, 172–173 AEDs, 70–71
image acquisition hardware, 171 surgery, 71–72
for language mapping, 190 Gillham, R., 240
and LI, 185–186 Gilliam, F., 214, 217
motion artifacts, 172–173 Goffman, E., 306
by neuropsychologists, CPT Codes, 178–179 Goldstein, L.H., 239–241
patient selection, 177–178 Gómez, E., 332
prediction cognitive outcome, 185–186 Gomez-Tortosa, E., 331
pulse sequence parameters, 171–172 Gonzalez, J.J., 327
resting state/neuronal connectivity studies, 187 Goodman, R., 49, 214
sample report, 187–190 Gowers, W.R., 213
stimulus and task apparatus, 173 Grabowska-Grzyb, A., 217
350 Index

Grafton, S.T., 215 memory, 286–288


Graham, P., 213 neurocognitive functioning, 282–283
Griffith, H.R., 67, 75 nonverbal tests, 284
Grivas, A., 72 visuospatial functioning, 286
Grooved Pegboard Test, 20 Intellectual function, 37–39
Gross-Kanner, H., 216 International Classification of Diseases, 10th revision
Gudmundsson, G., 213 (ICD-10), 282
Gutierrez, A.L., 331, 332 International League Against Epilepsy (ILAE),
2, 39, 276
Interstimulus intervals (ISIs), 204
H Intractable epilepsy, 305, 311
Hamberger, M.J., 148, 150 Ipsilateral abnormalities, 159
Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (HAM-D), 224 Isaacs, K., 243
Hand Dynamometer, 20
Harden, C.L., 216
Hauben, C., 229 J
Hauser, W.A., 214, 215 Jackson, H., 213
Health-related quality of life (HRQOL), 49 Jacoby, A., 307
Heaton, R.K., 26, 334 Jedrzejczak, J., 217
Helgeson, D.C., 238 Jette, N., 214
Helmstaedter, C., 67, 71, 76, 90, 93, 95–96, 244, 303, JME. See Juvenile myoclonic epilepsy (JME)
304 Johanson, M., 243
Hendriks, M., 244 Johnson, E.K., 217
Hermann, B., 9, 42, 67, 89, 240, 302, 304 Jones, J., 244
Hermann, B.P., 26, 45, 68, 217 Jones, J.E., 213–230
Hermosillo, D., 334 Jones-Gotman, M., 15, 89
Hernandez, I., 327 Juvenile absence epilepsy (JAE), 40
Herrera, L., 327 Juvenile myoclonic epilepsy (JME), 5, 40, 215
Hesdorffer, D.C., 214
Heyman, I., 49, 214
Hicklin, J., 229 K
Higareda, I., 327 Kanner, A.M., 216
Hillebrand, A., 203 Kaufman, A.S., 15
Hippocampal atrophy, 157 Kay, J., 217
Hippocampal sclerosis (HS), 5, 156 Kemper, B., 93, 96
Hippocampus, 156, 161 Kempner, K., 306
Hopkins Verbal Learning Test, Revised (HVLT-R), 287 Kerr, M., 216
Hoppe, C., 16 Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and
Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS), 214 Schizophrenia (KSADS), 49, 50
HS. See Hippocampal sclerosis (HS) Kies, B., 240
Human “place” cells, 92 Kim, J.S., 207
Humpty-Dumpty syndrome, 270 Kjartansson, O., 214
Hystero-epilepsy, 261 Kobau, R., 214
Kohnert, K.J., 331
Kozlowska, K., 262
I Krishnamoorthy, E.S., 215
Idiopathic generalized epilepsy. See Primary generalized KSADS. See Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders
epilepsy and Schizophrenia (KSADS)
ILAE. See International League Against Epilepsy (ILAE) Kuyk, J., 214, 216
Image acquisition hardware, 171
Independent Living Scales (ILS), 289
Infantile spasms, 277 L
Intellectual abilities, evaluation, 325 Lacey, C.J., 218
Intellectual disability, 275, 281 Lamotrigine (LTG), 71
adaptive functioning, 288–289 Landau-Kleffner syndrome, 42, 46, 278, 279
comprehensive neurocognitive screening tests, 284 Langenbahn, D.M., 245, 251
definition, 282 Langfitt, J.T., 257–271
inherited, 280 Language
IQ score measurement, 283–284 abilities test, 329–330
language, 285 deficits in children, 46–47
Index 351

dominance determination, 53 equipment, 196–197


electrocortical mapping of (see Electrocortical subject preparation, 197–198
mapping of language) and EEG, 201
epilepsy surgical patients, 105–106 episodic memory, 207–208
intellectual disability, 285 functional mapping, 208
neuropsychological test, 20–21 head tissues model, 200
primary, assessment, 322 inverse problem, 199–200
Language mapping, 54, 140 language mapping, 208–209
functional MRI for, 190 multimodal approach, 209–210
MEG and, 208–209 neural generators model, 200
Laterality index (LI), 170, 185–186 postsurgical changes, 209
Learning practical considerations, 203–204
deficits in children, 45–46 signals, physiological basis, 195–196
and memory test, 333–334 source modeling
Learning disability (LD), 11 inverse modeling, 203
Lee, S.A., 217 localization models, 202
Lee, S.M., 217 localization vs. imaging approaches, 200–203
Left mesial temporal sclerosis (LMTS), 207 single-dipole source models, 202
Leiter International Performance Scale, tasks, 204–205
Revised Edition, 284 activation and contrast, 205–206
Lennox-Gastaut syndrome, 41, 278 Malformations of cortical development (MCDs), 157
Levelt, W.J.M., 206 Manchanda, R., 215
LI. See Laterality index (LI) Marcus, W., 218
List-learning tasks, 107 Martin, E.M., 331
Llinas, R., 145 Martinovic, Z., 239
LMTS. See Left mesial temporal sclerosis (LMTS) Martin, R., 69
Logical memory subtest, 22 Mateer, C.A., 245, 251
Lopez-Rodriguez, F., 215 May, T.W., 238
Loring, D.W., 123–134 Mazziotta, J.C., 215
LTG. See Lamotrigine (LTG) McAlpine, M., 239
Luciano, D., 145 McDonald, C.R., 93, 96
Ludvigsson, P., 214 MCDs. See Malformations of cortical development
Lund, L., 240 (MCDs)
Lundren, T., 240 Meador, K.J., 123–134
Luria, A.R., 9 MEG. See Magnetoencephalography (MEG)
Melin, L., 240
Mellers, J.D.C., 239
M Memory
MacAllister, W.S., 37–55 deficits
Mackenzie, I.R., 69 in children, 45–46
Maestú, F., 207 PWE, 243–245
Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), 155 episodic
automatic segmentation, 162 functional MRI, 182–183
brain structure and pathology, 156 MEG, 207–208
DTI, 162–163 intellectual disability, 286–288
epilepsy protocol, 156 neuropsychological test, 22–23
quantitative, visual analysis, 157 test, 333–334
software packages for, 164 wada test, 126–130
subcortical segmentation, 161 Memory dysfunction, 89
surface-based morphometry, 159–161 Memory rehabilitation, 242–243
VBM, 158–159 Mensah, S.A., 216
visual eye in epilepsy, 156–157 Mental health disorders, 70
Magnetization-prepared 180° radio-frequency pulses and Mesial temporal lobe epilepsy (MTLE), 5, 159
rapid gradient echo (MPRAGE), 156 Mesial temporal sclerosis (MTS), 88, 128
Magnetoencephalography (MEG), 195 Miller, L.A., 69
behavioral monitoring, 206–207 Milner, B., 18, 19, 22, 96, 124
clinical use, 204 Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI), 219
data analysis, quality control, 199 Mini-Mental Status Exam, 323
data collection Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI),
data acquisition, 198–199 215, 221–222, 335
352 Index

Mirsky, A.F., 45 intellectual function, 37


MMPI. See Minnesota Multiphasic Personality neuropsychological test (See Neuropsychological
Inventory (MMPI) test)
Monk, T.G., 72 presurgical evaluation, 53
Mood disorders, 48, 223–225 seizures and epilepsy, 1–3
Morel, B., 213 Neuropsychological Assessment Battery (NAB), 16, 284
Moreno, S., 333 language abilities, 285
Morey, L.C., 228–230 memory functioning, 287–288
Morphometry NAB Spatial Module, 286
surface-based, 161 visuospatial functioning, 286
applications, 159–160 Neuropsychological dysfunction, 5
VBM, 158 Neuropsychological evaluations
applications, 158–159 cognitive impact, 310–311
Morrison, C., 243 antiepileptic medications, 304–305
Morrison, C.E., 139–153 chronic TLE, 303–304
Morse, R., 50 description, 302–303
Motor functions, 20 subjective, 305
Motor speed, 335 culturally and linguistically diverse populations (see
MPRAGE. See Magnetization-prepared 180° Culturally and linguistically diverse
radio-frequency pulses and rapid gradient echo populations)
(MPRAGE) employment-related issues, 309–310
MRI. See Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) interpretation
MTLE. See Mesial temporal lobe epilepsy (MTLE) description, 311–312
MTS. See Mesial temporal sclerosis (MTS) postsurgical outcome, 313–314
Multi-factorial models, 263–264 surgical considerations, 312–313
Multilingual Aphasia Examination (MAE), 21 overview, 301–302
psychosocial impact
description, 305–306
N on employment, 307–308
NAB. See Neuropsychological Assessment Battery quality of life, 306–307
(NAB) seizure-related issues, 308–309
NAEC. See National Association of Epilepsy Centers Neuropsychological Screening Battery for Hispanics
(NAEC) (NeSBHIS), 331
Nagai, Y., 241 advantage, 327
Naganska, E., 217 Attention-Mental Control Factor, 334
Nakhutina, L., 235–253 construct validity, 327
National Adult Reading Test (NART), 18, 325 diagnostic utility, 327
National Association of Epilepsy Centers (NAEC), 8 Hispanic epilepsy population, 334
Naugle, R., 63–82 learning/memory tests, 333
NEPSY battery, 48 significant, 328
NES. See Nonepileptic psychogenic events (NES) tests of visuospatial functioning, 333
NeSBHIS. See Neuropsychological Screening Battery Neuropsychological services, guidelines, 8
for Hispanics (NeSBHIS) Neuropsychological test, 5
Neural noise hypothesis, 100 attention and processing speed, 18–19
Neurodevelopmental Effects of Antiepileptic Drugs background on testing, 13–15
(NEADs), 281 battery, 15–17
Neurological Disorders Depression Inventory in Epilepsy case study, 28–30
(NDDI-E), 24 epilepsy data elements, 17
NEUROPSI, 324–325 epilepsy surgical patients, 103
attention and memory test, 324 executive functions, 19–20
Neuropsychological assessment, 1, 8–9 intellectual functioning, 17–18
case study, 28–30 interpreting and reporting test results, 25–28
clinical interview language, 20–21
elements, 9 learning and memory, 22–23
factors, 9–10 motor and sensory functions, 20
focal seizures, 12 PVTs, 23–24
TBI, 12 visual perceptual and spatial skills, 21–22
tonic–clonic seizures, 11 Nociferous cortex hypothesis, 92, 100–101
epilepsy surgical candidate, 87 Nonbarbiturate anesthesia, 130
geriatric patients with epilepsy, 75 Nonepileptic psychogenic events (NES), 216
Index 353

Nonverbal intelligence presurgical evaluation, pediatric


measures, 325 neuropsychologist role
tests, 284 comprehensive surgical battery, 52
No, Y.J., 217 language dominance determination, 54
language mapping, 54
neuropsychological assessment, 53
O presurgical test battery, 51–52
Object recognition, epilepsy surgical patients, 106–107 risk/benefit analysis, 50–51
Occipital lobe (OL) wada testing, 54–55
epilepsy, 41 seizures, 38–40
seizure, 97 Pegboard, P., 48
Ojemann, G.A., 139 Penfield, W., 92, 139
Ojemann, J., 148 Penn State Worry Questionnaire (PSWQ), 226
Olafsson, E., 214, 215 Performance validity tests (PVTs), 23–24
Orbitofrontal cortex, 216 Perrine, K., 145
Ostrosky-Solís, F., 331, 332 Personality disorders
axis II, 215–216
measures for identifying, 218
P questionnaires, 228–230
Papanicolaou, A.C., 208 semi-structured interviews for, 227–228
Paroxysmal behaviors, 257 Pfafflin, M., 238
PAS. See Primary attention systems (PAS) Pigott, S., 96
Pataraia, E., 209 Pirmoradi, M., 205
Patients with epilepsy (PWE), 235 PNEAs. See Psychogenic nonepileptic
cognitive remediation approaches, 249–252 attacks (PNEAs)
attention deficits rehabilitation, 246 PNES. See Psychogenic nonepileptic spells (PNES);
cognitive deficits, 241–242 Psychological nonepileptic seizures (PNES)
cognitive interventions, 245–246 Ponds, R., 244
cognitive rehabilitation, 242 Ponocny-Seliger, E., 46
memory deficits, 243–245 Pontón, M.O., 327, 328, 334
memory rehabilitation, 242–243 Pontón-Satz BNT, 330–331
educational interventions, 247–249 Posterior cortical epilepsy (PCE)
outcome in behavioral interventions for, 246–247 cognitive studies, 97–98
psychotherapeutic interventions, 247–249 pre-and postsurgical deficits in, 99
biofeedback in treatment of, 241 Pregnancy, NEAD, 281
CBT, 239–240 Presurgical test battery, 51, 52
emotional distress, 236–237 Primary attention systems (PAS), 262
knowledge and attitudes, 237–238 Primary generalized epilepsy, 276–277
medication compliance, 238 Privitera, M.D., 215
progressive relaxation training, 240–241 Processing Speed Index (PSI), 19
psychoeducational programs, 237 Progressive relaxation training, 240–241
psychosocial factors in, 236–237 Projective testing, 336–337
psychosocial functioning, 237 Propofol, 130
psychotherapy approaches, 238 PSI. See Processing Speed Index (PSI)
Patten, S.B., 214 PSWQ. See Penn State Worry Questionnaire (PSWQ)
PCE. See Posterior cortical epilepsy (PCE) Psychiatric comorbidity in epilepsy
Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, Fourth Edition axis II personality disorders, 215–216
(PPVT-4), 285 axis I psychiatric disorders, 214–215
Pediatric epilepsy diagnostic issues, 216–217
epilepsy syndromes, 4–5, 39–42 historical perspective, 213–214
evaluation of cognitive domains ictal psychotic symptoms, 216
attention functions, 42–45 impact of, 217–218
executive functions, 42–45 VBM study, 215
language, 46–47 Psychiatric disorders
learning and memory, 45–46 axis I, 214–215
mood and quality of life, 48–50 impact of, 217–218
visuomotor skills, 48 measures for identifying, 218
visuospatial skills, 48 quality of life, 217
intellectual function, 37–39 questionnaires for, 222
neuropsychological assessment, 37 anxiety disorders, 225–227
354 Index

Psychiatric disorders (cont.) Rath, J.F., 251


GHQ, 220–221 Rausch, R., 72, 217
mood disorders, 223–225 Ravens Standard Progressive Matrices (RSPM),
semi-structured interviews for, 218–220 18, 325, 333
CIDI, 218–219 RAVLT. See Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test
MINI, 219 (RAVLT)
Psychoeducational programs, 237 RBANS. See Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of
Psychogenic nonepileptic attacks (PNEAs), 257–258 Neuropsychological Status (RBANS)
case vignettes, 268–271 RCFT. See Rey Complex Figure Test (RCFT)
diagnosis RCI. See Reliable change index (RCI)
approach to, 258–259 RCT. See Randomized controlled trial (RCT)
diagnostic interview, 265–267 Recognition Memory Test, 23
procedure, 265 Reed, M., 214, 217, 218
epidemiology, 259–260 Refractory epilepsy, 301, 306, 307
etiology Reich, W.C., 218
multi-factorial models, 263–264 Reliable change index (RCI), 15, 103
neurobiological theories, 262–263 Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of
pre-modern theories, 261 Neuropsychological Status (RBANS), 16, 284
psychological theories, 261–262 language abilities, 285
prognosis, 264 learning/memory tests, 333
psychiatric diagnosis, 259 memory functioning, 287–288
psychometric testing, 267 neurocognitive domains assessment, 328–329
reporting the findings, 267–268 tests of visuospatial functioning, 333
risk factors, 260–261 visuospatial functioning, 286
treatment, 264–265 Response Bias Scale (RBS), 25
Psychogenic nonepileptic spells (PNES), 109, 110 Restitution training approach, 242
Psychological nonepileptic seizures (PNES), 9 Rett syndrome, 280
Psychosis, 214, 216 Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT), 22, 23
Psychotherapy, 238 Rey Complex Figure Test (RCFT), 23, 44
Psychotic disorders, 214 Reynolds Intelligence Screening Tests (RIST), 18
Pulse sequence parameters, 171–172 Rey-Osterreith Complex Figure Test (ROCFT)
Puskarich, C.A., 240 memory, 287
PVTs. See Performance validity tests (PVTs) visuospatial functioning, 286, 333
PWE. See Patients with epilepsy (PWE) RIST. See Reynolds Intelligence Screening Tests (RIST)
Pyramidal cells, 195 Robins, L.N., 218
ROCFT. See Rey-Osterreith Complex Figure Test (ROCFT)
Rossi, G., 229
Q Rousseau, A., 240
QOL. See Quality of life (QOL) RSPM. See Ravens Standard Progressive Matrices
QOLIE. See Quality of Life in Epilepsy Instruments (RSPM)
(QOLIE) Rutter, M., 213
QOLIE-31. See Quality of Life in Epilepsy Inventory Ryan, R., 306
(QOLIE-31)
Quality of life (QOL), 235
health-related, 337–338 S
psychiatric disorders, 217 Saetre, E., 71
psychosocial impact of epilepsy, 306–307 Saez, P., 333
sociodemographic characteristics, 306 Saling, M.M., 90
Quality of Life in Childhood Epilepsy Questionnaire Sander, J.W., 215
(QOLCE), 49 SAS. See Secondary attention systems (SAS)
Quality of Life in Epilepsy Instruments (QOLIE), 25 Schaffer, Sarah G., 301–314
Quality of Life in Epilepsy Inventory (QOLIE-31), 337–338 Screening Module, NAB, 284
Quantitative MRI SE. See Status epilepticus (SE)
advantages over visual analysis, 157 Secondary attention systems (SAS), 262
VBM, 158–159 Seidel, W.T., 150
Seidenberg, M., 26, 45, 217, 303
Seizures
R classification, 2
Raghavan, M., 169–190 complex partial, 5
Ramaratnam, S., 241 focal (see Focal seizures)
Randomized controlled trial (RCT), 238 tonic-clonic, 11, 38
Index 355

Senol, V., 307 T


Sensory functions, 20 Tan, S.Y., 240
Severe myoclonic epilepsy in infancy, 280–281 TAT. See Thematic Apperception Test (TAT)
Sheehan, D.V., 220 TBI. See Traumatic brain injury (TBI)
Sherman, E.M.S., 37–55 Tellez-Zenteno, J.F., 214
Sherr, R.S., 251 TEMAS, 336–337
Shipley-Hartford Scale, 18 Temkin, N.R., 239
Siddarth, P., 46 Temporal lobectomy (TL), 126, 244
Sillanpaa, M., 308 Temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE), 5, 40, 241
Simon, D., 251 associational learning, 90
Simonovi, P., 239 cognitive prognosis associated with, 303–304
Singh, K.D., 203 contextual learning, 90
Single-dipole source models, 202 developmental delay, 276
Sirven, J.I., 72 fluency tasks, 91
Sleep, electrical status epilepticus during, 279 functional neuroimaging studies, 92
Sloore, H., 229 MTS, 88
Slow-wave sleep, 41–42, 279 outcome, 111
Smeets, 307, 308 pre-and postsurgical deficits in, 94–95
Smith, G., 237 visual memory deficits, 89
SMT. See Story Memory Test (SMT) Test battery, 8, 14–17
Snodgrass, J.G., 148 presurgical, 51, 52
Social, Attitudinal, Familial, and Environmental (SAFE) Test de Vocabulario en Imágenes Peabody (TVIP),
Acculturation Stress Scale, 319–320 330–332
Sohlberg, M., 245, 251 Test of Nonverbal Intelligence, Third Edition (TONI-3),
Somwaru, D.P., 229 284, 325
Soto, A., 216 Thapar, A.K., 216
Spanish Verbal Learning Test (SVLT), 333–334 Thematic Apperception Test (TAT), 336
Spector, S., 240 Thesen, T., 155–164
Spielberger, C.D., 227 Thompson, P.J., 93, 96
Spinhoven, P., 214, 216 Thurman, D.J., 214
SQUIDs. See Superconducting quantum interference TLE. See Temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE)
devices (SQUIDs) Tobac, A., 229
Standardized regression-based (SRB) methods, 15 Token Test, 21
Standard Progressive Matrices (SPM), 325 TONI-3. See Test of Nonverbal Intelligence, Third
Stanford Binet Intelligence Scales, 17 Edition (TONI-3)
Status epilepticus (SE), 66 Tonic-clonic seizures, 11, 38
Steele, C.M., 320 Toone, B., 214
Stefansson, S.B., 215 Toone, B.K., 239
Stereotype threat, 320 Topiramate, 7
Stigma Tranah, A., 240
Goffman conceptualized, 306 Traumatic brain injury (TBI), 12, 242
impact on employment, 307 Trimble, M.R., 215, 241
Story Memory Test (SMT), 333–334 Trisomy 21, 279
Strategy training approach, 242 Tuberous sclerosis, 277–278
Streltzer, 338 TVIP. See Test de Vocabulario en Imágenes Peabody
Stress, acculturative, 319 (TVIP)
Structured Clinical Interview for DSM (SCID), 215
Subcortical segmentation, 161
Superconducting quantum interference devices U
(SQUIDs), 196 Uijl, S.J., 305
Surface-based coordinate systems, 161 Upton, D., 93, 96
Surface-based morphometry, 159–161
SVLT. See Spanish Verbal Learning Test (SVLT)
SVTs. See Symptom validity tests (SVTs) V
Swanson, S.J., 169–190 Van den Brande, I., 229
Swinkels, W.A.M., 214, 216 Vanderwart, M., 148
Symptom Validity Scale (FBS-r), 25 van Dyck, R., 214
Symptom validity tests (SVTs), 23 VBM. See Voxel-based morphometry (VBM)
356 Index

VCI. See Verbal Comprehension Index (VCI) reading, 133


Verbal auditory agnosia, 278 repetition, 133
Verbal Comprehension Index (VCI), 20 Wada test, 54–55, 313
Verbal fluency alternative medications, 130–131
assessment, 331 epilepsy surgery, 123, 124, 126, 130, 131
language-related bias on, 332 and fMRI, 177
measures, 331–332 historical background, 123–124
Vermeulen, J., 245, 251, 252 language, 124–125
Victoria Symptom Validity Test (VSVT), 23 validity, 125
Victoroff, J.I., 215 memory, 126–130
Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Second Edition WAIS. See Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS)
(Vineland-II), 288 Wakamoto, H., 304
Visual memory Washington Psychosocial Seizure Inventory (WPSI), 25
deficits, 89 Watson, W., 257–271
functioning, 92 Waugh, M.H., 228
Visual Memory Index, 100 WCST. See Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST)
Visual Object and Space Perception (VOSP) Battery, 106 Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS), 17, 18, 326
Visual process, epilepsy surgical patients, 106–107 Wechsler, D., 37
Visual stimuli, 173 Wechsler Individual Achievement Test (WIAT-III), 20
Visuomotor integration test (VMI), 44 Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, Fourth Edition
Visuomotor Precision subtest, 48 (WISC-IV), 283
Visuomotor skills, deficits, 48 Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence,
Visuospatial functioning Third Edition (WPPSI-III), 283
assessment, 332 Wechsler scales
cultural differences, 332 intelligence tests, 283
intellectual disability, 286 language and, 285
test, 332–333 memory, 287
Visuospatial skills, deficits, 48 nonverbal tests, 284
Volkl-Kernstock, S., 46 visuospatial functioning, 286
Volumetrics, 157 Wedlund, J., 243
Voxel-based morphometry (VBM), 158–159 West syndrome, 41, 277
Voxel dimensions, 171 Wheless, J.W., 307
VSVT. See Victoria Symptom Validity Test (VSVT) Whitman, S., 9, 240
Wide Range Achievement Test (WRAT), 20
Wide Range Assessment of Memory and Learning,
W Second Edition (WRAML2), 287
Wada, J., 123 Widiger, T.A., 229
Wada memory asymmetries (WMA), 127, 134 Wiebe, S., 214
Wada procedure, 114 Williams, J., 214
Wada protocol, 129 Wilson, B., 242, 247
Medical College of Georgia (MCG) Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST), 19, 43
clinical care, 131 WISC-R Block Design subtest, 333
comprehension, 132–133 WMA. See Wada memory asymmetries (WMA)
confrontation naming, 133 Word Accentuation Test, 325
expressive language/counting, 132 Working Memory Index (WMI), 18
general, 132 WRAT. See Wide Range Achievement Test (WRAT)
ipsilateral performance, 133–134
language assessment, 132
language considerations, 133 Z
laterality scores, 134 Zanarini, M.C., 228
memory, 133, 134 Zung Self-Rating Depression Scale (Zung SDS), 225

You might also like