You are on page 1of 10

14610248, 2010, 12, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1461-0248.2010.01536.

x by Univ Nacional de Mar del Plata UNMDP, Wiley Online Library on [29/11/2022]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Ecology Letters, (2010) 13: 1449–1458 doi: 10.1111/j.1461-0248.2010.01536.x

IDEA AND
PERSPECTIVE Parasitism, predation and the evolution of animal
personalities

Abstract
1 2
Raine Kortet, * Ann V. Hedrick Trade-offs between behavioural traits promoting high life-history productivity and
and Anssi Vainikka3 mortality may fuel the evolution of animal personalities. We propose that parasites,
1
Department of Biology, including pathogens, impose fitness costs comparable to those from predators, and
University of Eastern Finland, influence the adaptiveness of personality traits associated with productivity (PAPs).
P.O. Box 111, FI 80101 Joensuu,
Whether personality traits are adaptive or not may also depend on individual
Finland
2
immunological capacity. We illustrate this using a conceptual example in which the
Department of Neurobiology,
optimal level of PAPs depends on predation, parasitism and host compensation
Physiology & Behavior and
Animal Behavior Graduate
(resistance and tolerance) of parasitismÕs negative effects. We assert that inherent
Group, Division of Biological
differences in host immune function can produce positive feedback loops between
Sciences, One Shields Avenue, resource intake and compensation of parasitismÕs costs, thereby providing variation
University of California, Davis, underlying the evolution of stable personalities. Our approach acknowledges the
CA 95616, USA condition dependence of immune function and co-evolutionary dynamics between hosts
3
Department of Biology, and parasites.
University of Oulu, P.O. Box
3000, FI 90014 University of Keywords
Oulu, Finland Behavioural syndrome, feedback loop, growth-mortality trade-off, parasitism, pathogen,
*Correspondence: E-mail: personality.
raine.kortet@uef.fi

Ecology Letters (2010) 13: 1449–1458

identified as an important factor in the evolution of


INTRODUCTION
personality traits, such as activity, exploration and boldness
Observations on consistent, individually characteristic (e.g. Dingemanse et al. 2007). Predation incurs not only
behaviours across a wide variety of taxa have motivated direct mortality costs, but also the loss of resources via
enormous interest in animal personalities and their evolu- intimidation effects (Preisser et al. 2005; Stamps 2007).
tionary importance (reviewed by Dall et al. 2004; Sih et al. Consequently, the adaptive significance of risk taking
2004; Dingemanse & Réale 2005; Bell 2007; Réale et al. depends on the relative costs and benefits of being bold
2007; Biro & Stamps 2008; Stamps and Groothuis 2010). at different times and in different microenvironments: bold
Even though animal personalities can sometimes appear individuals obtain a fitness advantage (higher resource-
maladaptive in contrast to fully flexible context-dependent intake rates), if predation risk is low, whereas shy individuals
behaviour, their possible adaptive significance is becoming can have higher fitness, if predation risk is high (Sih et al.
evident (Sih & Bell 2008; Dingemanse et al. 2010; Luttbeg & 2004). As the fitness costs of risk taking may increase in
Sih 2010). In this article, we define (animal) ÔpersonalityÕ parallel with the fitness benefits accompanying increased
(also known as ÔtemperamentÕ or Ôcoping styleÕ) as behaviour resource-intake rates, individuals with different levels of
that varies among individuals, but is consistent across time boldness may, on average, achieve equal lifetime fitness and
and ⁄ or contexts within individuals (Stamps and Groothuis a continuum of individuals from shy to bold will remain in a
2010). For example, animals with a ÔshyÕ personality are population (Stamps 2007; Biro & Stamps 2008).
consistently cautious and avoid risk taking, whereas those Although predation is widely recognized as an important
with a ÔboldÕ personality are consistently incautious and agent in the evolution of personality, the potential effect of
prone to take risks (Sih et al. 2004). parasitism on the evolution of animal personalities has been
Predation is generally considered the major cause of largely ignored. Nonetheless, parasitism imposes fitness
mortality for ÔboldÕ, risk-taking individuals (e.g. Smith & costs comparable to those of predation (Raffel et al. 2008;
Blumstein 2008; Biro & Booth 2009), and thus has been Rohr et al. 2009; Schmid-Hempel 2009), as parasites

 2010 Blackwell Publishing Ltd/CNRS


14610248, 2010, 12, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1461-0248.2010.01536.x by Univ Nacional de Mar del Plata UNMDP, Wiley Online Library on [29/11/2022]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
1450 R. Kortet, A. V. Hedrick and A. Vainikka Idea and Perspective

(including microparasites and pathogens such as viruses and of personalities. However, long-term stability was achieved
bacteria) are significant sources of mortality and sublethal when these negative feedback mechanisms were combined
fitness costs. Very recently, Barber and Dingemanse (2010) with a positive feedback mechanism, state-dependent safety
outlined the potential importance of parasites in the (Luttbeg & Sih 2010). Thus, state-dependent safety, for
evolution of animal personality. Here, in accordance with example, condition-dependent avoidance of predation – or
Barber and Dingemanse (2010), we argue that because as we argue here, parasitism – could favour the development
parasites have major impacts on fitness (including behavio- of stable personalities by maintaining and reinforcing
ural components of fitness), and because certain personality individual differences that arise from small disparities in
traits can affect the probability of acquiring and resisting original assets (Luttbeg & Sih 2010). Importantly, models by
parasites (Lozano 1991; Wilson et al. 1993; Hart 1997), Stamps (2007; also see Biro & Stamps 2008) and Luttbeg
parasites should influence the evolution of personality. and Sih (2010) are not mutually exclusive and yield the same
We also discuss the implications of this for recent models prediction: high life-history productivity and bold behav-
for the evolution of personality, and explain why parasitism iours will be coupled. Here, we discuss how parasitism can
should help generate and maintain consistent interindividual change the predictions of productivity ⁄ mortality models
variation in behaviour. (Stamps 2007; Biro & Stamps 2008) and generate positive
feedback loops between host immune function and behav-
iour, analogous to those generated by condition-dependent
Growth-mortality trade-offs, and feedback loops between
avoidance of predation (Luttbeg & Sih 2010). We first
food intake and fitness: mechanisms for the evolution
separate the effects of parasite exposure on personality
of animal personalities
variation from parasite-induced selection on personality
Stamps (2007) proposed that animal personalities are traits, and then discuss how state-dependent safety, gener-
relatively consistent over time since risk taking and other ated by the hostÕs immune function, affects the costs of
behaviours promoting high food intake rates are driven by acquiring parasites through certain behavioural styles.
individually consistent intrinsic growth rates. An animalÕs
behavioural foraging potential, composed of traits, such as
Parasites and pathogens as selective agents
activity, exploration, boldness and aggressiveness, is associ-
ated with growth rate, given that growth is regulated by the In general, parasites and pathogens are just as important as
intake of resources (e.g. Petherick et al. 2002; Ward et al. predators in determining individualsÕ fitness (e.g. Raffel et al.
2004). As natural selection acts on intrinsic growth rates 2008; Schmid-Hempel 2009). Even though many parasites
(Arendt 1997), adaptive individual differences in behaviours do not directly kill their hosts, they often weaken or
(e.g. activity, boldness and aggressiveness) might be manipulate their hosts, so that the hosts become more
explained by interindividual variation in intrinsic growth susceptible to predation (e.g. Milinski 1990; Seppälä &
capacity and trade-offs between growth and mortality that Jokela 2008). Moreover, parasites can cause low fecundity,
differ among individuals, but allow them to achieve equal sterility or low reproductive rates due to poor success in
fitness (Stamps 2007; Biro & Stamps 2008; also see intraspecific or interspecific competition (e.g. Kavaliers et al.
Adrianssens & Johnsson 2008). However, this mechanism 2000; Barber et al. 2004; Newey & Thirgood 2004). They
for the evolution of personality relies upon pre-existing may also decrease the hostÕs mating success due to the
genetic differences among individuals in intrinsic growth opposite sexÕs avoidance of parasitized mates (Hamilton &
rates (or underlying traits) to drive the evolution of different Zuk 1982).
behavioural types (personalities). Direct effects of parasitism on host behaviour are diverse
Alternative mechanisms that emphasize links between and relatively well known. Exploitation of hosts by parasites
behaviour and relatively stable state variables have been can affect the hostsÕ feeding rates, sociability, migration and
proposed for the evolution of animal personalities. These success in sexual selection (Lozano 1991; Hart 1997; Jog &
mechanisms include asset protection, i.e. trade-offs between Watve 2005; Fleurance et al. 2007). In addition, parasites
current vs. future reproduction and avoidance of starvation manipulate hosts to increase their own transmission rates,
(Wolf et al. 2007; but see Massol & Crochet 2008), and often by increasing the hostsÕ risk of predation (Seppälä &
positive feedback loops in state-dependent behaviour Jokela 2008) or the hostsÕ aggressiveness (as in rabies;
(McElreath et al. 2007; Luttbeg & Sih 2010). Recently, Niezgoda et al. 2002). Therefore, parasites may, by directly
Luttbeg and Sih (2010) showed that negative feedback altering host behaviour, create apparent personalities or
mechanisms, such as asset protection and starvation behavioural syndromes during the hostÕs life time (e.g. Coats
avoidance eventually lead to convergence of behavioural et al. 2010; see review in Barber and Dingemanse 2010). Our
types over timescales comparable to an individualÕs lifetime, focus here, however, is on parasite-generated selection
and therefore cannot account for the long-term stability on genotypes. For example, parasite avoidance can be

 2010 Blackwell Publishing Ltd/CNRS


14610248, 2010, 12, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1461-0248.2010.01536.x by Univ Nacional de Mar del Plata UNMDP, Wiley Online Library on [29/11/2022]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Idea and Perspective Personalities and parasitism 1451

considered as a behavioural form of parasite resistance that of an individual and might also reflect an individualÕs
evolves in response to parasitism. Parasite avoidance is investment in immune defence in earlier phases of life.
comprised of behaviours that reduce exposure to parasites, Various models for the evolution of personality (e.g.
for example, moving away from particular areas, such as Luttbeg & Sih 2010) rely upon initial differences in assets
beddings or nests (Moore 2002; Ezenwa 2004), or removing among individuals, even if these are very small. However,
macroparasites by grooming or scratching. Population-level hypotheses relying on stochastic initial differences in assets
studies have demonstrated that populations under selection or state of individuals do not ultimately explain the adaptive
by certain parasites show increased parasite avoidance origin of variation in animal personalities. Here, we provide
behaviour (e.g. Cruz et al. 2008). Notably, parasites (espe- an ultimate explanation for adaptive variation in animal
cially microparasites) are more difficult to avoid through personality by arguing that inherently co-evolutionary and
immediate behavioural responses than predators, because frequency-dependent host parasite interactions maintain
the infectious stages of many parasites cannot be observed. interindividual variation in immune function and therefore
When parasite exposure is associated with personality set different adaptive values for different behavioural styles
traits, direct and indirect parasite-induced selection for under the risk of parasitism. We argue that innate
personality traits may occur. Potential examples of this differences may be reinforced through positive feedback
phenomenon occur in juvenile pumpkinseed sunfish, in loops in which high resource-intake rates lead to efficient
which parasite fauna differ among individuals along a shy– immune function. Therefore, the immune system is poten-
bold continuum (Wilson et al. 1993); Siberian chipmunks, in tially a generator of variation in initial assets, necessary for
which non-random distributions of parasites among hosts the development of different personalities (Ôbehavioural
result from personality-related differences in space use typesÕ, Luttbeg & Sih 2010), and of positive feedback loops
(Boyer et al. 2010); and in feral cats, in which higher levels of that promote and maintain this divergence. Moreover, as
lethal feline immunodeficiency virus occur in males with individuals with efficient immune function pay a smaller
more aggressive personalities (Natoli et al. 2005). Likewise, cost for parasite exposure than individuals that accumulate
in domestic cats, feline immunodeficiency virus is associated high parasite loads due to inefficient immune function, we
with aggressiveness, large body size and potentially, earlier predict, under the positive feedback framework, that
age at first reproduction (Pontier et al. 1998). immunologically competent individuals should generally be
more bold and active than less immunocompetent individ-
uals (also see Barber and Dingemanse 2010). We argue that
Role of host parasite avoidance and resistance
although individuals with inefficient immune function could
Links between behavioural traits and the cost of parasitism try to compensate for their innately poor immunocompe-
are necessary for parasitism to affect the evolution of animal tence by increasing boldness, the compensation is unlikely
personality. To evaluate the effects of past and current to be successful due to frequency-dependent factors or
behaviour on the cost of parasitism, it is important to stochastic events involved in feeding behaviour (see also
consider individual differences in both exposure to parasites discussion in Dall et al. 2004) and the high potential for
and immune function. These two factors have very different extra costs of acquiring more parasites through increased
natures. While exposure to parasites may be a function of boldness.
certain behaviours, the cost of that exposure likely varies We also argue that direct parasite-induced selection on
between individuals in accordance with their capacity to certain personality traits is very likely to occur (also see
resist parasites. In vertebrates, the effectiveness of the Barber and Dingemanse 2010). For example, active, social
immune system depends on the innate availability of or group-living individuals encounter parasitized conspecif-
particular major histocompatability (MHC) alleles and ics more often than do sedentary or solitary individuals,
previous exposure to parasites (Woelfing et al. 2009). which can considerably accelerate the transmission of
In contrast, invertebrate immunity relies more on intrinsic, parasites, particularly horizontally (directly) transmitting
non-specific defences, such as encapsulation and melaniza- parasites and pathogens (Coté & Poulin 1995; Hart 1997;
tion of intruders by the prophenoloxidase-cascade (e.g. Altizer et al. 2003). Increasing the frequency of social
Cerenius & Söderhäll 2004; Lee 2006). In addition, parasite contacts significantly increases parasite spread among
resistance and tolerance are energetically costly, and involve mammals (Altizer et al. 2003; Vicente et al. 2007). This
trade-offs with other life-history traits (e.g. Zuk & Stoehr increased transmission of parasites via social contact is
2002; Rantala et al. 2003; Tschirren & Richner 2006). especially important for infectious diseases, such as influ-
In turn, the higher is the resource availability, the more enza or sexually transmitted diseases (e.g. Smith & Dobson
organisms are expected to invest in immune function 1992; Mosure et al. 1996; Altizer et al. 2003). Indeed,
(Houston et al. 2007). Therefore, the cost of parasitism will Schaller & Murray (2008) recently proposed that infectious
depend on the intrinsic and condition-dependent qualities diseases can impose strong selection on human personality

 2010 Blackwell Publishing Ltd/CNRS


14610248, 2010, 12, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1461-0248.2010.01536.x by Univ Nacional de Mar del Plata UNMDP, Wiley Online Library on [29/11/2022]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
1452 R. Kortet, A. V. Hedrick and A. Vainikka Idea and Perspective

traits, such as sociosexuality, extraversion and openness to would be weaker than that predicted by a predation-only
experience. model.
Another key factor determining the risk of parasitism The examples above illustrate the fact that associations
(reviewed in Altizer et al. 2003) is dominance status, which is between parasite exposure and particular behaviours are
usually associated with aggressiveness as well as activity and probably multifaceted and should often be considered
boldness (e.g. Kortet & Hedrick 2007). Dominance status species- and context-specific. Ultimate mechanisms that
affects both parasite encounter rates and immune function. may influence the impact of parasites on the evolution of
Dominant individuals typically have better access to food personality include the host–parasite populationsÕ environ-
resources than subordinate individuals (e.g. Stahl et al. ment and their preceding co-evolutionary history. For
2001). Although this may increase the risk of acquiring example, host social organization and mating system should
indirectly transmitted parasites along with food, enhanced influence not only parasite diversity and prevalence, but also
resource intake often improves condition-dependent fitness advantages to parasites of different transmission
immune function. Thus, improved immune function may strategies (Altizer et al. 2003).
compensate all or some of the negative effects of food-
borne parasitism (Møller 1997). Dominant individuals often
Interindividual and intraindividual variation in behaviour
have stronger immune defence, for example, in field crickets
(Rantala & Kortet 2004) and drumming wolf spiders For personalities to be detected in a population, both low
(Ahtiainen et al. 2006). In social species, high dominance intraindividual and relatively high interindividual variation in
status can increase grooming behaviour received from other behaviour must be present (Bell 2007; Stamps and Groothuis
individuals, which may decrease the cost of ectoparasitism 2010). Inherently frequency-dependent co-evolutionary
(Foster et al. 2010). Dominance status can also affect dynamics between hosts and parasites generate and maintain
endocrine–immunity interactions by affecting the levels genetic variation among hosts (Anderson & May 1982;
of immunosuppressive androgens and stress hormones Salathé et al. 2007; Woelfing et al. 2009), as parasites, due to
(Hillgarth & Wingfield 1997). For example, subordination their shorter generation times, usually evolve faster than their
stress in low-ranking fish can increase the risk of parasitism hosts, and impose continuously changing selection pressures
via stress-induced immunosuppression favouring dominant on their hosts. This is probably not the case in predator–prey
individuals (Conte 2004). Similarly, in some mammals, co-evolution, as predators usually have longer or similar
subordination stress in low-ranking individuals causes generation times compared to prey. Co-evolutionary para-
immunosuppression (Sapolsky 2005). In contrast, sex site–host dynamics mean that parasites can contribute to the
hormones that promote aggression but induce immunosup- maintenance of genetic variation both in initial assets that
pression can indirectly increase the cost of parasitism for favour personality differences, and in direct parasite-induced
dominant individuals in a number of species (Hillgarth & selection on personality traits. Thus, parasites impose strong
Wingfield 1997; Easterbrook et al. 2007). Moreover, in some selection on host immune function and favour maintenance
animals including cats and wild rats, dominant individuals of variation in several facets of immune defence, including
have a higher, behaviourally mediated risk of parasitism MHC genes (Anderson & May 1982; Woelfing et al. 2009).
through injuries from aggressive encounters (Glass et al. We therefore argue that host parasite co-evolution, which is
1988; Pontier et al. 1998; Natoli et al. 2005; Easterbrook ubiquitous in nature, is responsible for intrinsic differences
et al. 2007). in host immune function and that these induce initial
In some cases, parasitism strengthens the behaviourally differences in individual state (see Luttbeg & Sih 2010).
mediated (via bold, active, aggressive behaviours) trade-off However, we acknowledge that interindividual variation in
between productivity and mortality compared to the trade- initial assets may also be generated by other mechanisms
off induced by predation alone. For example, aggressive, including, for example, differences in metabolic rate (Careau
dominant rats with a higher risk of parasitism from injury et al. 2009), differences in anti-predator ability, susceptibility
during fights (Glass et al. 1988) might also incur higher risks to stress or intrinsic growth rate (Stamps 2007), differences
of predation than shy, subordinate rats, because of their in parental investment or luck early in life (Luttbeg & Sih
higher activity levels. Here, selection against aggressiveness ⁄ 2010).
boldness ⁄ activity would be stronger than that predicted by a We argue here that intrinsic differences in host immune
predation-only model, possibly promoting the persistence of function affect the individual-level cost of parasite exposure.
shy behavioural types. In contrast, when behavioural traits We argue further that due to the lower cost of parasite
such as aggressiveness are associated with a lower risk of exposure for individuals with a high intrinsic capacity to
parasitism, as occurs in many species in which dominant resist parasites, condition-dependent improvement of
individuals have lower parasitism risks (e.g. Altizer et al. immune defence is unlikely to override but rather reinforces
2003), selection against aggressiveness ⁄ boldness ⁄ activity initial state differences. Individuals may decrease the cost

 2010 Blackwell Publishing Ltd/CNRS


14610248, 2010, 12, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1461-0248.2010.01536.x by Univ Nacional de Mar del Plata UNMDP, Wiley Online Library on [29/11/2022]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Idea and Perspective Personalities and parasitism 1453

of parasite exposure by investing more resources in parasite of parasitism whereas hostsÕ parasite resistance is not
resistance, but only in relation to their intrinsic resistance affected. In Fig. 1b, PAPs are independent of the risk of
capacity. Therefore, we postulate that the stronger is the parasitism but increasing PAPs improve parasite resistance
selection imposed by parasites, the more likely is low so that the cost of parasitism decreases with increasing level
intraindividual (see section below) and high interindividual of PAPs. We aim to explain: (1) the consistency of
variation in behavioural strategies, linked to the cost of individual behaviour, (2) average level of PAPs and (3)
parasite exposure. We also emphasize that interindividual population-level variation in PAPs as a function of the
variation in behaviours is not random, but adaptive, abovementioned factors. We assume that interindividual
particularly with respect to individual constraints set by differences are generated and maintained by the mecha-
the physiological capacity to resist parasites. nisms we have discussed above.
In our example, the risks and costs of predation and
parasitism are additive and independent of host body size.
A conceptual example adding parasitism to
By this definition, cost of one type imposes a moderate
productivity ⁄ mortality models
trade-off between productivity and mortality, and the
We argue that the adaptiveness of personality traits maximum cost imposes a strong trade-off. We also assume
associated with productivity (hereafter PAPs, i.e. personality that individuals do not use special allocation strategies
traits that promote fast growth ⁄ high fecundity), which in between anti-predator and anti-parasite strategies vs. growth
some species contribute to active, bold and aggressive and reproduction, but that once increasing resource-intake
behaviours (Stamps 2007; Biro & Stamps 2008), depend not rates, they increase investment in both life-history produc-
only on predation risk, but also on how they affect an tivity and safety provided by enhanced immune function
individualÕs parasite exposure (Fig. 1a) and resistance (Houston et al. 2007). We also assume that food intake rates
(Fig. 1b). In our conceptual example (Fig. 1), we assume are independent of population density.
two possibilities with varying magnitudes regarding parasit- To predict evolution, we make the following simplifying
ism risk. In Fig. 1a, increasing PAPs per se increase the risk assumptions about how trade-off strength relates to the

(a) (b)
Trade-off with parasitism, Strong trade-off, Strong trade-off,
Directional selection, the highest
medium consistency high consistency
Degree of compensation by PAPs

high consistency consistency


Cost of parastism

Additive trade-off, Additive trade-off,


medium consistency elevated consistency

Weak trade-off, Trade-off with predation Moderate trade-off, Strong trade-off,


low consistency medium consistency medium consistency high consistency

Cost of predation Cost of predation

Figure 1 Predicted level of personality traits associated with productivity (PAPs) as: (a) a function of the cost of predation and cost of
parasitism, and (b) as a function of the cost of predation and resource-intake-dependent immunity (compensation). The predictions apply
both in evolutionary time at a population level and in ecological time at an individual level given that individuals vary in their initial state with
respect to both parasite resistance and anti-predator ability. (a) We assume that the risks of predation and parasitism are additive and no
compensation of either type of cost occurs. (b) We assume that PAPs mediate compensation of the cost of parasitism. The cost of parasitism
is assumed to be high at the x-axis (when no compensation occurs). At an individual level, the degree of compensation also applies to
differences in the efficiency of immune function. Phenotypically plastic compensation of the cost of predation does not occur in either (a) or
(b). The darker the circle, the more active ⁄ bolder an individual ⁄ individuals should be, and the lighter the circle the less active an
individual ⁄ individuals should be. The greater the diameter of the circle, the stronger is the expected consistency in personality traits, i.e. the
more likely it is that animal personalities may develop ⁄ evolve given that interindividual variation occurs. In contrast to the left lower corner in
(a), in the upper left corner of (b) a high level of interindividual variation in current parasite resistance (from reinforcement of initial
differences through state-dependent positive feedback, see the main text) induces a high level of intraindividual behavioural consistency. Note
that the amount of interindividual variation in PAPs is not demonstrated in the figure, but likely depends on the strength of selection and
variation in individual assets (see the main text).

 2010 Blackwell Publishing Ltd/CNRS


14610248, 2010, 12, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1461-0248.2010.01536.x by Univ Nacional de Mar del Plata UNMDP, Wiley Online Library on [29/11/2022]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
1454 R. Kortet, A. V. Hedrick and A. Vainikka Idea and Perspective

optimal level of PAPs and the population-level variation in shy and sedentary behaviour). In such cases, parasites will
PAPs. If the fitness costs of increasing PAPs increase more decrease the cost of boldness ⁄ high activity and weaken the
rapidly than the benefits, i.e. the costs are accelerating, for trade-off further (not shown in the figure).
example because intake-dependent compensation is ineffi- Thus, when we abandon the potential for condition-
cient against a high risk of parasites, selection is likely dependent variation in parasite resistance, and the costs of
leading to a single evolutionary endpoint (c.f. Hoyle et al. parasitism and predation are additive, animal personalities
2008). In contrast, when PAPs increase survival, for most likely develop in environments with high risks of both
example, by decreasing the cost of parasitism, and the costs parasites and predators (Fig. 1a, upper right corner), as both
are strongly decreasing, evolution may proceed towards predators and parasites can affect the adaptiveness of
maximal average levels of activity ⁄ boldness (c.f. Hoyle et al. behaviours in relation to intrinsic variation in individual
2008). However, with more weakly accelerating, linear or state. This occurs because we assume that interindividual
decelerating costs, the evolutionary outcomes are more variation in initial assets is constant along the lower left to
difficult to predict but may, at least in non-sexual models, upper right diagonal (Fig. 1a), and consistency in individual
include a stable polymorphism of different behavioural behaviour increases as the cost increases of deviating from
types (de Mazancourt & Dieckmann 2004; Hoyle et al. the optimal trajectory set by initial assets. However, in
2008). In this respect, note that Fig. 1 should be interpreted nature the likelihood of detecting personalities in a
only as an explanatory example and not as a quantitative population depends also on the amount of interindividual
model. Our assumed scenarios differ in that Fig. 1a assumes variation in personality traits. This is set by variation in
no feedback – host resistance varies, but not in response to individual assets determining how tightly individuals follow
PAPs, whereas Fig. 1b assumes a positive feedback, where the same behavioural trajectory (see also Bell 2007).
PAPs bring in the energy that drives an increase in Simple predictions from Fig. 1a are that: (1) when the
host resistance. Host parasite interactions are inherently cost of parasitism and risk of predation increase, the
frequency-dependent (Anderson & May 1982), and negative expected average level of PAPs (e.g. activity ⁄ boldness)
frequency-dependent selection pressure arising from the decreases; and with lower costs of parasites and lower risk
host population itself or from parasites could complicate of predation, the expected level of PAPs increases. We also
our simple predictions. predict that (2) when the trade-off between PAPs and
fitness costs is strong, it is more likely that individual
Personality and parasite exposure behaviour will match the assets generated by immune
According to our conceptual example, the trade-off between function (or other physiological factors), and consequently,
PAPs and mortality in the productivity-mortality model will it is more likely that animal personalities will develop or
be strengthened when parasites are acquired through PAPs evolve within a population. In this scenario, personality
and there is no resource-dependent improvement of parasite variation is based wholly on initial rank order state
resistance (Fig. 1a, along y-axis). The stronger the trade-off differences, which may remain even when individual state
is, the more consistently an individual should follow the feeds back positively to individual state (see below).
behavioural trajectory set by its initial and current assets and
the more likely it is that animal personalities will develop Personality and the compensation of parasitism
(Fig. 1a). This is explained by the increasing cost of In Fig. 1b, we assume that high levels of PAPs increase the
deviating from an optimal behavioural trajectory that is set efficiency of immune function through enhanced food
by initial and current assets. For example, an individual with intake rates and thus generate safety and decrease the
inefficient immune function will do poorly by behaving in a strength of the trade-off between PAPs and fitness costs.
way that increases parasite exposure, if a behavioural way to We argue that the stronger is the selection imposed by
avoid parasites exists, and similarly, an individual with parasites, the more likely it is that low intraindividual and
efficient immune function loses potential benefits by high interindividual variation in behavioural strategies,
unnecessarily avoiding parasites. The same is true with linked to the cost of parasite exposure, will be maintained
predators if individuals differ in qualities that affect their (Fig. 1b). Consequently, animal personalities will most likely
likelihood of being killed by a predator. When the risks of develop in a situation where immune defence is efficient at
predation and parasitism are close to zero, we would expect decreasing the cost of parasite exposure and when predation
to see a maximum level of PAPs, but intraindividual risk adds to the cost of deviating from the individually
variation in PAPs may exceed that of interindividual optimal behavioural trajectory set by initial and current
variation, and personalities may not develop (Fig. 1a, lower assets (Fig. 1b, upper right corner).
left corner). The situation is even more pronounced if The safety generated by high levels of PAPs decreases the
parasites are not acquired through PAPs, but through fitness cost of PAPs and therefore favours higher levels of
behaviours that are coupled with low productivity rates (e.g. PAPs than would be expected by the non-compensatory

 2010 Blackwell Publishing Ltd/CNRS


14610248, 2010, 12, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1461-0248.2010.01536.x by Univ Nacional de Mar del Plata UNMDP, Wiley Online Library on [29/11/2022]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Idea and Perspective Personalities and parasitism 1455

predation cost model only. This leads to the prediction that resource acquisition is still likely to be generated (Andrew
individuals with efficient immune function should show Sih, personal communication). Second, comparative studies
higher levels of PAPs than individuals with less efficient could reveal whether species or populations living in
immune function (Fig. 1b). Similarly, if individuals can environments with high predation and parasitism risk more
decrease the cost of predation, for example, by growing often show personalities than species or populations living
quickly to a large (safe) size, individuals are expected to in low predation and parasitism areas (c.f. Dingemanse et al.
show higher levels of PAPs than in situations where PAPs 2007). We predict that high risks of predation or parasitism
only increase the cost of predation (not shown in the favour high levels of boldness, when compensation of
figures). If there is full compensation of increased parasite predation and parasitism induced fitness costs is efficient,
exposure by condition-dependent improvement of immune but low levels of boldness, when the animals have little or
defence (Fig. 1b, upper line), the productivity-mortality no means of compensating for the high costs of predation
trade-off in predation-only models (Stamps 2007; Biro & and parasitism. Third, comparative studies or studies using
Stamps 2008) will not change except for the fact that asset selected lines (e.g. selected for increased and decreased
differences generated by immune function may still con- immune function) could reveal whether abundant but non-
tribute to individual consistency of behaviour. virulent parasites, against which the host displays condition-
Predictions based on Fig. 1b are that: (1) when increasing dependent parasite resistance, select for higher boldness or
levels of PAPs promote compensation of the cost of stronger behavioural consistency under constant risk of
parasites more than they increase the cost of predation, the predation than virulent parasites that cannot be compen-
expected level of PAPs should increase (in individuals or sated for by foraging more.
over evolutionary time) and (2) when increasing levels of We predict that predation-associated costs will dominate
PAPs increase the cost of predation more than they provide the evolution of personalities in systems where the cost of
compensation for parasites, the expected level of PAPs parasitism is not clearly associated with host immune
should decrease (in individuals or over evolutionary time). function. Similar patterns will occur when host immune
Therefore, the maximum level of PAPs (e.g. maximally functions are inefficient at reducing the costs of parasitism.
active and bold behaviour) is expected when PAPs reduce However, as some anti-predator strategies, including
the cost of parasitism and there are no predators (as in behavioural traits, are also condition-dependent (e.g. Pett-
Fig. 1b, or when anti-predator strategies are positively erson & Brönmark 1999; Skajaa et al. 2004), we predict that
dependent on PAPs). fitness compensation through PAPs (e.g. Ôstate-dependent
safetyÕ; Luttbeg & Sih 2010) also occurs in predation-driven
systems, and suggest that the consequences of this
PREDICTIONS AND CONCLUSION
mechanism for the maintenance of variation in animal
Our analysis yields predictions that are readily testable. First, personalities should be evaluated further using the founda-
an empiricist could test whether individuals with efficient tion laid by Luttbeg and Sih (2010). Most importantly, we
immune function are bolder, more active or explore more argue that parasites maintain variation in personality traits by
than individuals with less efficient immune function and maintaining variation in immune function through host
whether this relationship is genetically determined. This parasite co-evolution. This process is inherently frequency-
prediction arises from the claim that intrinsically immuno- dependent and therefore unquestionably generates variation
competent individuals suffer a small cost of acquiring (Anderson & May 1982). How frequency-dependent factors
parasites along with food, whereas all individuals can affect interindividual and intraindividual variation in PAPs,
improve their immune function and parasite tolerance by depending on population-level feedback mechanisms such
acquiring more resources (e.g. Houston et al. 2007). The as density-dependent food availability and probability of
prediction should hold for a wide continuum of taxa and it aggressive encounters, is less clear. Whereas maximum
does not separate intrinsic immunity from condition- activity and low consistency is the apparent outcome in our
dependent immunity, as according to Luttbeg and SihÕs simplified scenarios with zero costs, frequency- and density-
(2010) positive-feedback model with state-dependent safety, dependent factors will probably come into play in real
the positive correlation between boldness and immune situations, and maximal aggressiveness, for example, would
efficiency is reinforced over time. If resource-intake- be an evolutionarily stable strategy in a predator and parasite
dependent improvement of immune function fully com- free environment only if an individual was short of food (see
pensates for increased parasite exposure, bolder individuals e.g. Houston & McNamara 1988). On the other hand, certain
should have lower parasite loads. If the compensation is not frequency-dependent behaviours are capable of maintaining
efficient enough, they should have higher parasite loads. rank order differences in individual state and do not affect
Note that if the cost of higher tolerance of parasites requires life-history trajectories induced by differences in initial state
higher activity, positive coupling between boldness and (Dall et al. 2004). In any case, the effect of factors such as

 2010 Blackwell Publishing Ltd/CNRS


14610248, 2010, 12, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1461-0248.2010.01536.x by Univ Nacional de Mar del Plata UNMDP, Wiley Online Library on [29/11/2022]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
1456 R. Kortet, A. V. Hedrick and A. Vainikka Idea and Perspective

density-dependent availability of food and developmental


REFERENCES
and physiological constraints in food intake rates and
growth rates should be taken into account to fully Adrianssens, B. & Johnsson, J.I. (2008). Personality and life-history
understand the effects of frequency dependence. Thus, a productivity: consistent or variable association? Trends Ecol.
Evol., 24, 179–180.
more detailed formal modelling approach is needed.
Ahtiainen, J.J., Alatalo, R., Kortet, R. & Rantala, M.J. (2006). Im-
To date there are very few empirical tests of the ideas mune function, dominance and mating success in drumming
presented here. However, Kortet et al. found a positive male wolf spiders Hygrolycosa rubrofasciata. Beh. Ecol. Sociobiol., 60,
correlation in field crickets between boldness and lytic 826–832.
activity, a measure of the cricketsÕ defence against bacterial Altizer, S., Nunn, C.L., Thrall, P.H., Gittleman, J.L., Antonovics, J.,
parasites (Kortet et al. 2007). Correlations such as this are Cunningham, A.A. et al. (2003). Social organization and parasite
difficult to interpret without knowing the relationships risk in mammals: integrating theory and empirical studies. Annu.
Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst., 34, 517–547.
between behaviour, parasitism and the condition depen-
Anderson, R.M. & May, R.M. (1982). Coevolution of hosts and
dence of studied measures of immune function. In this parasites. Parasitology, 85, 411–426.
particular example (Kortet et al. 2007), the correlation could Arendt, J.D. (1997). Adaptive intrinsic growth rates: an integration
suggest that bold behaviour facilitates a high resource- across taxa. Quart. Rev. Biol., 72, 149–177.
acquisition rate, which facilitates an aspect of strong Barber, I. & Dingemanse, N.J. (2010). Parasitism and the evolu-
immune function. Alternatively, innately resistant individu- tionary ecology of animal personality. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B, in
als may be better able to cope with the risk of acquiring press.
Barber, I., Hoare, D. & Krause, J. (2004). Effects of parasites on
parasites with food, and for this reason might behave more
fish behaviour: a review and evolutionary perspective. Rev. Fish
actively, aggressively or boldly than their less resistant Biol. Fish., 10, 131–165.
conspecifics (see also López et al. 2005). Indeed, it may be Bell, A.M. (2007). Future directions in behavioural syndromes
possible that both of these two causal mechanisms are at research. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci., 274, 755–761.
work. Tests of immunity–personality interactions are war- Biro, P.A. & Booth, D.J. (2009). Extreme boldness precedes
ranted and the results will help to further develop the ideas starvation mortality in six-lined trumpeter (Pelates sexlineatus).
presented here. For example, positive feedback loops Hydrobiologia, 635, 395–398.
Biro, P.A. & Stamps, J.A. (2008). Are animal personality traits linked
between PAPs and immune functions, particularly during
to life-history productivity? Trends Ecol. Evol., 23, 361–368.
ontogeny, should generate divergent personalities which Boyer, N., Réale, D., Marmet, J., Pisanu, B. & Chapuis, J. (2010).
may appear heritable in quantitative genetic analyses due to Personality, space use and tick load in an introduced population
the underlying inheritance of immune components. of Siberian chipmunks Tamias sibiricus. J. Anim. Ecol., 79, 538–
In conclusion, we assert that parasites and pathogens can 547.
strongly affect the costs and benefits of many personality Careau, V., Bininda-Emonds, O.R.P., Thomas, D.W., Réale, D. &
traits. We also propose that initial variation in individual Humphries, M.M. (2009). Exploration strategies map along fast–
slow metabolic and life-history continua in muroid rodents.
assets can be generated by host parasite co-evolution, and
Funct. Ecol., 23, 150–156.
that the consequently arising differences in immunity can Cerenius, L. & Söderhäll, K. (2004). The prophenoloxidase-
induce positive feedback loops between fitness and behav- activating system in invertebrates. Immunol. Rev., 198, 72–82.
iours promoting high food intake rates, favouring different, Coats, J, Poulin, R. & Nakagawa, S. (2010). The consequences of
individually consistent behaviours (personality traits) over parasitic infections for host behavioural correlations and
long periods of time. Our work adds to the premises that repeatability. Behaviour, 147, 367–382.
trade-offs between behaviourally mediated productivity and Conte, F.S. (2004). Stress and the welfare of cultured fish. Appl.
Anim. Behav. Sci., 86, 205–223.
mortality (Stamps 2007; Biro & Stamps 2008) and positive
Coté, I. & Poulin, R. (1995). Parasitism and group size in social
feedback loops between state-dependent safety and behav- animals: a meta-analysis. Behav. Ecol., 6, 159–165.
iour (Luttbeg & Sih 2010) can guide the evolution of animal Cruz, A., Prather, J.W., Wiley, J.W. & Weaver, P.F. (2008). Egg
personalities. rejection behavior in a population exposed to parasitism: Village
Weavers on Hispaniola. Behav. Ecol., 19, 398–403.
Dall, S.R.X., Houston, A.I. & McNamara, J.M. (2004). The
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS behavioural ecology of personality: consistent individual
differences from an adaptive perspective. Ecol. Lett., 7, 734–
This research has been supported by the Academy of Finland
739.
(project 127398), the National Science Foundation (IOS- Dingemanse, N.J. & Réale, D. (2005). Natural selection and animal
0716332) and by a grant from the Emil Aaltonen foundation. personality. Behaviour, 142, 1159–1184.
We thank Andy Sih, Judy Stamps and Kelly Smith for very Dingemanse, N.J., Wright, J., Kazem, A.J, Thomas, D.K., Hickling,
helpful comments, and Iain Barber and Niels Dingemanse, R. & Dawnay, N. (2007). Behavioural syndromes differ pre-
as well as Barney Luttbeg and Andy Sih, for providing us dictably between 12 populations of three-spined stickleback.
with pre-publication copies of their manuscripts. J. Anim. Ecol., 76, 1128–1138.

 2010 Blackwell Publishing Ltd/CNRS


14610248, 2010, 12, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1461-0248.2010.01536.x by Univ Nacional de Mar del Plata UNMDP, Wiley Online Library on [29/11/2022]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
Idea and Perspective Personalities and parasitism 1457

Dingemanse, N.J., Kazem, A.J.N., Réale, D. & Wright, J. (2010). de Mazancourt, C. & Dieckmann, U. (2004). Trade-off geometries
Behavioural reaction norms: animal personality meets individual and frequency-dependent selection. Am. Nat., 164, 765–778.
plasticity. Trends Ecol. Evol., 25, 81–89. McElreath, R., Luttbeg, B., Fogarty, S., Brodin, T. & Sih, A. (2007).
Easterbrook, J.D., Kaplan, J.B., Glass, J.E., Pletnikov, M.V. & Evolution of animal personalities. Nature, 450, E5–E6.
Klein, S.L. (2007). Elevated testosterone and reduced 5-HIAA Milinski, M. (1990). Parasites and host decision-making. In: Para-
concentrations are associated with wounding and hantavirus sitism and Host Behaviour (eds Barnard, C.J. & Behnke, J.M.).
infection in male Norway rats. Horm. Behav., 52, 474–481. Taylor and Francis, London, pp. 95–116.
Ezenwa, V.O. (2004). Selective defecation and selective foraging: Møller, A.P. (1997). Parasitism and the evolution of host life his-
antiparasite behaviour in wild ungulates? Ethology, 110, 851–862. tory. In: Host-Parasite Evolution: General Principles and Avian Models
Fleurance, G., Duncan, P., Fritz, H., Cabaret, J., Cortet, J. & (eds Clayton, D.H. & Moore, J.). Oxford University Press,
Gordon, I.J. (2007). Selection of feeding sites by horses at Oxford, pp. 105–127.
pasture: testing the anti-parasite theory. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci., Moore, J. (2002). Parasites and the Behavior of Animals. Oxford Uni-
108, 288–301. versity Press, Oxford
Foster, M.W., Gilby, I.C., Murray, C.M., Johnson, A., Wroblewski, Mosure, D.D., Berman, S., Kleinbaum, D. & Halloran, M.E.
E.E. & Pusey, A.E. (2010). Alpha male chimpanzee grooming (1996). Predictors of Chlamydia trachomatis infection among
patterns: implications for dominance style. Am. J. Primat., 71, female adolescents: a longitudinal analysis. Am. J. Epidemiol.,
136–144. 144, 997–1003.
Glass, G.E., Childs, J.E., Korch, G.W. & LeDuc, J.W. (1988). Natoli, E., Say, L., Cafazzo, S., Bonanni, R., Schmid, M. & Pontier,
Association of intraspecific wounding with hantaviral infection D. (2005). Bold attitude makes male urban feral domestic cats
in wild rats (Rattus norwegicus). Epidem. Inf., 101, 459–472. more vulnerable to Feline Immunodeficiency Virus. Neurosci.
Hamilton, W.D. & Zuk, M. (1982). Heritable true fitness and bright Biobehav. Rev., 29, 151–157.
birds: a role for parasites? Science, 218, 384–386. Newey, S. & Thirgood, S.J. (2004). Parasite-mediated reduction in
Hart, B.L. (1997). Behavioral defence. In: Host-Parasite Evolution: fecundity of mountain hares. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci., 271,
General Principles and Avian Models (eds Clayton, D.H. & Moore, J.). S413–S415.
Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 57–77. Niezgoda, M., Hanlon, C.A. & Rupprecht, C.E. (2002). Animal
Hillgarth, N. & Wingfield, J.C. (1997). Parasite-mediated sexual rabies. In: Rabies (eds Jackson, A.C. & Wunner, W.H.). Academic
selection: endocrine aspects. In: Host-Parasite Evolution: General Press, New York, pp. 163–218.
Principles and Avian Models (eds Clayton, D.H. & Moore, J.). Petherick, J.C., Holroyd, R.G., Doogan, V.J. & Venus, B.K. (2002).
Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 78–104. Productivity, carcass and meat quality of lot-fed Bos indicus cross
Houston, A.I. & McNamara, J.M. (1988). Fighting for food: a steers grouped according to temperament. Aust. J. Exp. Agric.,
dynamic version of the Hawk-Dove game. Evol. Ecol., 2, 51–64. 42, 389–398.
Houston, A.I., McNamara, J.M., Barta, Z. & Klasing, K.C. (2007). Petterson, L.B. & Brönmark, C. (1999). Energetic consequences of
The effect of energy reserves and food availability on optimal an inducible morphological defence in crucian carp. Oecologia,
immune defence. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci., 274, 2835–2842. 121, 18–18.
Hoyle, A., Bowers, R.G., White, A. & Boots, M. (2008). The Pontier, D., Fromont, E., Courchamp, F., Artois, M. & Yoccoz, N.G.
influence of trade-off shape on evolutionary behavior in classical (1998). Retroviruses and sexual size dimorphism in domestic cats
ecological scenarios. J. Theor. Biol., 250, 498–511. (Felis catus L.). Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci., 265, 167–173.
Jog, M. & Watve, M. (2005). Role of parasites and commensals in Preisser, E.L., Bolnick, D.I. & Benard, M.F. (2005). Scared to
shaping host behaviour. Curr. Sci., 89, 1184–1191. death? The effects of intimidation and consumption in predator-
Kavaliers, M., Coldwell, D.D. & Choleris, E. (2000). Parasites and prey interactions Ecology, 86, 501–509.
behaviour: an ethopharmacological perspective. Parasitol. Today, Raffel, T.R., Martin, L.B. & Rohr, J.R. (2008). Parasites as preda-
16, 464–468. tors: unifying natural enemy ecology. Trends Ecol. Evol., 23,
Kortet, R. & Hedrick, A. (2007). A behavioural syndrome in the field 610–618.
cricket Gryllus integer: intrasexual aggression is correlated with Rantala, M.J. & Kortet, R. (2004). Male dominance and immuno-
activity in a novel environment. Biol. J. Linn. Soc., 91, 475–482. competence in the field cricket (Gryllus bimaculatus). Behav. Ecol.,
Kortet, R., Rantala, M.J. & Hedrick, A. (2007). Boldness in anti- 15, 187–191.
predation behavior and immune defence in field crickets. Evol. Rantala, M.J., Kortet, R., Kotiaho, J.S., Vainikka, A. & Suhonen, J.
Ecol. Res., 9, 185–197. (2003). Condition dependence of pheromones and immune
Lee, K.A. (2006). Linking immune defenses and life history at the function in the grain beetle Tenebrio molitor. Funct. Ecol., 17,
level of the individual and the species. Integr. Comp. Biol., 46, 534–540.
1000–1015. Réale, D., Reader, S.M., Sol, D., McDougall, P.T. & Dingemanse,
López, P., Hawlena, D., Polo, V., Amo, L. & Martı́n, J. (2005). N.J. (2007). Integrating animal temperament within ecology and
Sources of individual shy-bold variations in antipredator evolution. Biol. Rev., 82, 291–318.
behaviour of male Iberian rock lizards. Anim. Behav., 69, 1–9. Rohr, J.R., Swan, A., Raffel, T.R. & Hudson, P.J. (2009). Parasites,
Lozano, G.A. (1991). Optimal foraging theory: a possible role for info-disruption, and the ecology of fear. Oecologia, 159, 447–454.
parasites. Oikos, 60, 391–395. Salathé, M., Kouyos, R.D., Regoes, R.R. & Bonhoeffer, S. (2007).
Luttbeg, B. & Sih, A. (2010). Risk, resources and state-dependent Rapid parasite adaptation drives selection for high recombina-
adaptive behavioral syndromes. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B, in press. tion rates. Evolution, 62, 295–300.
Massol, F. & Crochet, P. (2008). Do animal personalities emerge? Sapolsky, R.M. (2005). The influence of social hierarchy on primate
Nature, 451, E8–E9. health. Science, 308, 648.

 2010 Blackwell Publishing Ltd/CNRS


14610248, 2010, 12, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.1461-0248.2010.01536.x by Univ Nacional de Mar del Plata UNMDP, Wiley Online Library on [29/11/2022]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
1458 R. Kortet, A. V. Hedrick and A. Vainikka Idea and Perspective

Schaller, M. & Murray, D.R. (2008). Pathogens, personality, Tschirren, B. & Richner, H. (2006). Parasites shape the optimal
and culture: disease prevalence predicts worldwide variability in investment in immunity. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci., 273, 1773–
sociosexuality, extraversion, and openness to experience. 1777.
J. Pers. Soc. Psychol., 95, 212–221. Vicente, J., Höfle, U., Fernández-De-Mera, I.G. & Gortazar, C.
Schmid-Hempel, P. (2009). Parasites – the new frontier: celebrating (2007). The importance of parasite life history and host density
Darwin 200. Biol. Lett., 5, 625–627. in predicting the impact of infections in red deer. Oecologia, 152,
Seppälä, O. & Jokela, J. (2008). Host manipulation as a parasite 655–664.
transmission strategy when manipulation is exploited by non- Ward, A.J., Thomas, P., Hart, P.J.B. & Krause, J. (2004). Correlates
host predators. Biol. Lett., 4, 663–666. of boldness in three-spined sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus).
Sih, A. & Bell, A.M. (2008). Insights for behavioral ecology from Beh. Ecol. Sociobiol., 55, 561–568.
behavioral syndromes. In: Advances in the Study of Behavior (eds Wilson, D.S., Coleman, K., Clark, A.B. & Biederman, L. (1993).
Brockmann, H.J., Roper, T.J., Naguib, M., Wynne-Edwards, K.E., Shy-bold continuum in pumpkinseed sunfish (Lepomis gibbosus):
Barnard, C. & Mitani, J.C.). Academic Press, San Diego, 38, an ecological study of a psychological trait. J. Comp. Psych., 107,
pp. 227–281. 250–260.
Sih, A., Bell, A.M., Johnson, J.C. & Ziemba, R.E. (2004). Behavioral Woelfing, B., Traulsen, A., Milinski, M. & Boehm, T. (2009). Does
syndromes: an integrative overview. Quart. Rev. Biol., 79, 241–277. intra-individual major histocompatability complex diversity keep
Skajaa, K., Fernö, A. & Folkvord, A. (2004). Ontogenetic- and a golden mean? Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B, 364, 117–128.
condition-related effects of starvation on responsiveness in Wolf, M., van Doorn, G.S., Leimar, O. & Weissing, F.J. (2007).
herring larvae (Clupea harengus L.) during repeated attacks by a Life-history trade-offs favour the evolution of animal person-
model predator. J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol., 312, 253–269. alities. Nature, 447, 581–584.
Smith, B.R. & Blumstein, D.T. (2008). Fitness consequences of Zuk, M. & Stoehr, A.M. (2002). Immune defense and host life
personality: a meta-analysis. Behav. Ecol., 19, 448–455. history. Am. Nat., 160, S9–S22.
Smith, G. & Dobson, A.P. (1992). Sexually transmitted diseases in
animals. Parasitol. Today, 8, 159–166.
Stahl, J., Tolsma, P.H., Loonen, M.J.J.E. & Drent, R.H. (2001).
Subordinates explore but dominants profit: resource competition Editor, Andrew Sih
in high Arctic barnacle goose flocks. Anim. Behav., 61, 257–264. Manuscript received 24 June 2010
Stamps, J.A. (2007). Growth-mortality tradeoffs and Ôpersonality First decision made 5 August 2010
traitsÕ in animals. Ecol. Lett., 10, 355–363. Manuscript accepted 11 September 2010
Stamps, J.A. & Groothuis, T.G.G. (2010). The development of
animal personality: relevance, concepts and perspectives. Biol.
Rev., 85, 301–325.

 2010 Blackwell Publishing Ltd/CNRS

You might also like