You are on page 1of 19

Personality and Social Psychology Review Copyright © 2000 by

2000, Vol. 4, No. 4, 337–354 Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

The Distinctiveness Principle:


Identity, Meaning, and the Bounds of Cultural Relativity
Vivian L. Vignoles, Xenia Chryssochoou, and Glynis M. Breakwell
Social Psychology European Research Institute
University of Surrey, Guildford, England

Extending theories of distinctiveness motivation in identity (Breakwell, 1987;


Brewer, 1991; Snyder & Fromkin, 1980), we discuss the precise role of distinctive-
ness in identity processes and the cross-cultural generality of the distinctiveness
principle. We argue that (a) within Western cultures, distinctiveness is necessary for
the construction of meaning within identity, and (b) the distinctiveness principle is
not incompatible with non-Western cultural systems. We propose a distinction
among three sources of distinctiveness: position, difference, and separateness, with
different implications for identity and behavior. These sources coexist within cul-
tures, on both individual and group levels of self-representation, but they may be em-
phasized differently according to culture and context.

According to Snyder and Fromkin (1980), “The need selves as less similar to others than others are to them-
to see oneself as unique is a potent and continuous force selves (Codol, 1984a, 1987).
in our society” (p. 3). This view appears within psycho- These findings can be interpreted as evidence for
analytic and developmental theories of self and identity the distinctiveness principle, defined as a motive
(Erikson, 1959; Fromm, 1942; Maslach, 1974; Ziller, within identity pushing toward establishing and main-
1964) and is reflected in European and North American taining a sense of differentiation from others (after
literature (reviewed by Snyder & Fromkin, 1980). Breakwell, 1986a; Brewer, 1991). This motive has im-
Studies conducted in Europe and North America plications for cognition and behavior. On a cognitive
have shown that information is better memorized if level, people will emphasize the distinctiveness of as-
it distinguishes the self from others (Leyens, pects of their identities and will perceive as more cen-
Yzerbyt, & Rogier, 1997); groups are often rated as tral to identity those aspects providing a sense of
more heterogeneous if the rater is a group member1 distinctiveness. On a social level, people will act espe-
(Brewer, 1993b; Park & Rothbart, 1982); feelings of cially in ways that show their distinctiveness to others.
extreme similarity to others are associated with neg- Where distinctiveness needs are threatened or frus-
ative affect (Fromkin, 1972), positive evaluation of trated, people will engage in cognitive or behavioral
scarce experiences (Fromkin, 1970), and greater coping strategies to restore a sense of distinctiveness
identification with distinctive groups (Brewer & (Branscombe, Ellemers, Spears, & Doosje, 1999;
Pickett, 1999); and people generally describe them- Breakwell, 1988; Brewer, 1991).
We should emphasize that feelings of distinctive-
ness may be constructed in many ways. A central as-
This work was funded by a bursary from the School of Human
Sciences, University of Surrey, and forms part of Vivian L. sertion of this article is that distinctiveness may be
Vignoles’s doctoral dissertation. constructed on both individual and group levels of
We thank the editor and four anonymous reviewers for their con- identity, in terms of difference from others, separate-
structive comments on earlier drafts of this article. ness from others, or one’s position within social rela-
Requests for reprints should be sent to Vivian L. Vignoles or
tions. Thus, distinctiveness is taken to include, but is
Xenia Chryssochoou, Social Psychology European Research In-
stitute, School of Human Sciences, University of Surrey, Guild- not restricted to, those senses in which the term tradi-
ford, GU2 5XH, England. E-mail: v.vignoles@surrey.ac.uk or tionally has been understood within uniqueness theory
x.chryssochoou@surrey.ac.uk.
1
(Snyder & Fromkin, 1980), social identity theory
However, some studies have failed to find this effect or have (Tajfel & Turner, 1986), self-categorization theory
found the opposite, where self-categorization is at an intergroup level
(Turner, 1987), and optimal distinctiveness theory
(Haslam, Oakes, Turner, & McGarty, 1995), the ingroup is of a lower
status than the outgroup (Lorenzi-Cioldi, 1995), the ingroup is a mi- (Brewer, 1991).
nority (B. Simon & Brown, 1987), or under threatening conditions The principle is understood to push toward distinc-
(Thompson, Kohles, Otsuki, & Kent, 1997). tiveness on both individual and group levels of iden-

337
VIGNOLES, CHRYSSOCHOOU, & BREAKWELL

tity. Although the distinction between these levels is a (Breakwell, 1986a; Brewer, 1991; Brewer & Gardner,
useful theoretical tool in many circumstances (Tajfel, 1996; Codol, 1981; Festinger, 1954; Lemaine, 1974;
1981; Turner, 1987) and appears to have some psycho- Snyder & Fromkin, 1980; Tajfel & Turner, 1986), but
logical reality (Breakwell, 1983; Trafimow, Triandis, comparatively little effort has gone into theorizing ex-
& Goto, 1991), focusing on one level at the expense of plicitly why distinctiveness is important for identity.
the other may lead researchers to miss both the similar- Yet, given the reservations identified previously, theo-
ities and the interconnections between them rizing a distinctiveness principle without an account of
(Breakwell, 1987; Deaux, 1992; Lorenzi-Cioldi, 1995; why distinctiveness should be a good thing seems un-
Reid & Deaux, 1996; Turner, 1999). If group identities wise. In this section, we review the assumptions under-
are shaped partly by individual processes and motives lying existing theories of distinctiveness motivation,
and individual identities are shaped partly by group which have portrayed distinctiveness as a social value,
processes (Breakwell, 1986a; Brewer, 1991; Hogg & an aspect of self-enhancement, a fundamental human
Abrams, 1993; Marková, 1987; Reicher, 1995; B. Si- need, and a basic property of the construction of mean-
mon, 1997; Tajfel & Turner, 1986; Turner, 1987), then ing within identity.
there is no apparent reason to assume that different sets
of motivational principles will apply to individual and
group levels of identity. Distinctiveness as a Social Value
The distinctiveness principle has been understood
to guide the processes shaping identity in interaction Some theorists suggest that distinctiveness has a
with motives for continuity, efficacy, and self-esteem role in shaping identity because of its social value. This
(Breakwell, 1993; Vignoles, Chryssochoou, & argument is implicit in uniqueness theory (Snyder &
Breakwell, 2000) as well as belonging, inclusion Fromkin, 1980) and is made explicit in identity process
within groups, and similarity to others (Baumeister & theory (Breakwell, 1987).
Leary, 1995; Brewer, 1993a; Snyder & Fromkin, Uniqueness theory (Snyder & Fromkin, 1980) pro-
1980). In focusing here on distinctiveness, we intend in poses that different degrees of similarity to others are
no way to dismiss the vital significance of these other encoded at different levels of acceptability, moderate
motives for identity. As we discuss later, distinctive- similarity being the most acceptable and very high or
ness and similarity or inclusion in fact are connected very low similarity—very low or very high distinctive-
especially closely in the process of constructing a ness—being the least acceptable outcomes. In a series
meaningful sense of identity. of studies, Snyder and Fromkin induced feelings of
Breakwell (1987, 1993) identified three potential moderate to extreme similarity in participants, finding
reservations about the distinctiveness principle. First, convergent evidence for the positive value of moderate
the principle appears to push toward moderate rather distinctiveness across a range of affective and behav-
than extreme levels of distinctiveness. Second, distinc- ioral outcomes. However, uniqueness theory does not
tiveness often has been theorized as a source of self-es- explain directly why distinctiveness should be valued
teem rather than a basic motive, implicitly reducing the according to this curvilinear pattern. Snyder and
principle to a subordinate status within identity dynam- Fromkin suggested that the pattern is moderated by in-
ics. Third, the principle might be specific to Western or dividual differences in need for uniqueness, but their
individualistic cultures. Given these reservations, it research did not address the question of why people
seems difficult to support the assertion of a distinctive- need uniqueness.
ness principle without careful examination of two key In theorizing a relation between distinctiveness and
issues: (a) the nature of the role of distinctiveness within acceptability, uniqueness theory might be understood
identity processes and (b) whether—and, if so, in what to tie distinctiveness motivation to self-esteem mainte-
sense—the importance of distinctiveness is moderated nance, although this is not made explicit. One study ap-
by culture. In this article, we review each issue in turn pears to confirm this interpretation. Ganster,
and outline some connected theoretical developments McCuddy, and Fromkin (1977, cited in Snyder &
that have contributed to our own research into the dis- Fromkin, 1980) gave false feedback to participants
tinctiveness principle. about their similarity to others. They reported that
feedback of moderate similarity led to higher levels of
self-esteem than did higher or lower similarity. The ob-
The Role of Distinctiveness in Identity served relation between distinctiveness and self-es-
Processes teem might be understood in terms of the cultural value
of distinctiveness. Snyder and Fromkin described
Many Western theorists have asserted the impor- uniqueness as “probably an integral and necessary part
tance of distinguishing oneself from others on both in- of existence within our Western culture, which empha-
dividual and group levels of self-representation sizes the individual,” although they also noted that “in

338
THE DISTINCTIVENESS PRINCIPLE

many situations, people want not to be unique but to be within intergroup comparisons (Tajfel, 1981; Tajfel &
similar to others” (pp. 215–216). Turner, 1986; Turner, 1975). Although the theory is
Breakwell (1987, 1988, 1993) portrayed the distinc- not explicit on this point, one interpretation of why
tiveness principle explicitly as a cultural value, which positive group distinctiveness is important is that it
is reified within individuals. Breakwell described four serves to enhance self-esteem (Abrams & Hogg,
identity principles (distinctiveness, continuity, effi- 1988), and this interpretation has been adopted exten-
cacy, and self-esteem) that guide the intra-individual sively in the social identity literature (see Long &
processes shaping identity but constitute “a so- Spears, 1997; Rubin & Hewstone, 1998).
cially-established set of criteria against which identity Social comparison processes are understood to be
is measured” (1987, p. 107). Thus, insofar as the dis- most stable and most effective as a means of achieving
tinctiveness requirement is socially defined, what is self-esteem if the target is moderately similar to one-
accepted as an appropriate level or manifestation of self on the dimension of comparison (Festinger, 1954,
distinctiveness will be culture specific. Unlike Snyder Hypothesis 3; Wills, 1991). This might help explain
and Fromkin (1980), who focused exclusively on indi- the pressure observed toward moderate levels of dis-
vidual distinctiveness, Breakwell argued that both in- tinctiveness (Snyder & Fromkin, 1980), which provide
dividual and group distinctiveness may be valued a balance between difference and similarity.
within the identity of the individual. However, the evi- However, as in the social value account, distinctive-
dence she presented focused more on the role of dis- ness is important here only insofar as it is positively
tinctiveness in self-enhancement processes than on the functional for self-esteem. The two accounts involve
social value of distinctiveness in itself. Thus, it seems very different ways in which distinctiveness may con-
that the relation between the social value of distinctive- tribute to self-esteem: In the previous argument, the
ness and its force as a principle underlying identity value of distinctiveness itself ties distinctiveness to
processes remains to be demonstrated empirically. self-esteem; here, self-esteem is derived from positive
distinctiveness on other dimensions of value. Never-
theless, neither account presents a strong case for a
Positive Distinctiveness and separate need for distinctiveness independent of the
Self-Enhancement self-esteem principle.

The distinctiveness principle has also been related


to processes of self-enhancement, in which self-esteem Distinctiveness as a Fundamental
is achieved by establishing positive distinctiveness Human Need
(Breakwell, 1987; see also Abrams & Hogg, 1988;
Wills, 1991). Studies conducted in North America An alternative view is that distinctiveness is a fun-
have shown that people display more confidence about damental need in its own right. This is assumed within
the accuracy of social comparisons in which they are optimal distinctiveness theory (Brewer, 1991, 1993a),
positively distinguished from others (J. M. Schwartz & which deals especially with group distinctiveness. Op-
Smith, 1976), people overestimate the uniqueness of timal distinctiveness theory proposes two motivational
their positive attributes (Campbell, 1986; Taylor & principles, a need for “differentiation of the self from
Brown, 1988), and, especially in threatening situa- others” and a need for “inclusion of the self into larger
tions, people prefer to compare themselves with others social collectives,” which are understood to act in op-
from whom they are positively distinguished (Crocker, position to each other (1993a, p. 3). Optimal distinc-
Thompson, McGraw, & Ingerman, 1987; Gruder, tiveness occurs at a point of equilibrium between the
1977; Hakmiller, 1966), although similarity with posi- two needs, which normally will be a state of moderate
tively valued others also has been associated with distinctiveness. This explains the curvilinear relation
self-enhancement (Wheeler, 1966). The relation of between distinctiveness and acceptability described in
positive distinctiveness with self-esteem maintenance uniqueness theory. The distinctiveness principle ap-
is further supported by findings that, where positive parently functions with only moderate force not for in-
distinctiveness is not available, people seek or create trinsic reasons but because of its opposition with
alternative dimensions of comparison (Lemaine, 1974; another basic motive.
Lemaine, Kastersztein, & Personnaz, 1978) or try to The central predictions of optimal distinctiveness
avoid social comparison entirely (e.g., Breakwell, theory are similar to those of uniqueness theory but
1986b; Brickman & Bulman, 1977). have been applied especially to questions of group
The quest for positive group distinctiveness has an identification, finding support across a wide range of
important place within social identity theory, which as- experimental and applied contexts (Brewer &
serts that many features of intergroup behavior are di- Pickett, 1999; Brewer & Weber, 1994; Chiu &
rected toward establishing positive distinctiveness Hong, 1999; Henderson-King, Henderson-King,

339
VIGNOLES, CHRYSSOCHOOU, & BREAKWELL

Zhermer, Posokhova, & Chiker, 1997; Hornsey & tiveness is theorized as a fundamental and universal hu-
Hogg, 1999; Lee & Ottati, 1995; L. Simon et al., man need, necessary for self-definition and not just for
1997). self-enhancement, and the motive for distinctiveness is
According to Brewer and Pickett (1999), the needs separated empirically from the motive for self-esteem.
for differentiation and inclusion are “universal human
motives” rather than cultural values (p. 85). Brewer
(1991) described the negative implications of failing to Distinctiveness and Meaning
satisfy their requirements: “Being highly individuated
leaves one vulnerable to isolation and stigmatization Extending Brewer’s (1991) account of distinctive-
… . However, total deindividuation provides no basis ness as a fundamental human need, a theoretical argu-
for comparative appraisal or self-definition” (p. 478). ment can be advanced that distinctiveness has an
Nevertheless, some cultural, individual, and temporal essential role in the construction of a meaningful iden-
variation is expected in their relative strengths, which tity rather than being a specific property of particular
will affect the point of equilibrium. identities (after Codol, 1981).
Brewer (1991) explicitly separated these needs Identity may be defined as the subjective concept of
from self-esteem maintenance: “Optimal distinctive- oneself as a person (adapted from Reber, 1985) and is,
ness is independent of the evaluative implications of therefore, a form of representation. A notable feature
group membership, although, other things being equal, of any system of representation is that concepts are not
individuals will prefer positive group identities to neg- meaningful independently of each other but are de-
ative identities” (p. 478). Supporting this, Brewer, fined in relation to each other, involving a process of
Manzi, and Shaw (1993) eliminated effects of group differentiation. Saussure illustrated this: “If I am to ex-
status on ingroup favoritism for majority and minority plain to someone the meaning of stream I must tell him
minimal groups when they aroused their participants’ about the difference between a stream and a river, a
need for differentiation by priming them with deper- stream and a rivulet, etc.” (cited in Culler, 1976, p. 24).
sonalizing information. When the differentiation mo- The same principle applies to the particular case of the
tive was frustrated, participants showed more mental representation of oneself. An illustration is the
favoritism for minority than majority ingroups, regard- identity statement “I am a musician.” Clearly, this
less of group status. A related finding comes from re- statement implies that I have something in common
search by Mlicki and Ellemers (1996) into national with other people who are, or describe themselves as,
identities among Polish and Dutch students. Poles de- musicians. But equally it implies a distinction from
scribed their national identity in negative terms but those who are not, or do not describe themselves as,
identified strongly and stressed their negative national musicians. Without such a distinction, in what sense
distinctiveness. Mlicki and Ellemers interpreted this as the statement would be meaningful is not clear.2 More
a reaction to the threat to Polish distinctiveness posed generally, I cannot have a sense of who I am without a
by Poland’s historical status. Furthermore, we have sense of who I am not, which entails distinctiveness.
found in research into identity processes among Angli- In the same vein, Codol (1981) argued for the im-
can parish priests (Vignoles et al., 2000) that the level portance of individual distinctiveness as a basic prop-
of distinctiveness associated with items of self-de- erty of self-awareness:
scription is a significant predictor of their perceived
centrality within subjective identity structures after There is no self-image unless the individual can iden-
controlling for associations of the items with self-es- tify a certain object as defining himself. This identifi-
teem. cation assumes that the individual can conceive of
Although optimal distinctiveness theory is restricted himself as a particular object, different from all others.
In this way self-awareness inevitably expresses in the
to the discussion of distinctiveness on the level of group
first place the recognition by an individual, on a cogni-
memberships, Brewer and Gardner (1996) extended the
tive level, that he is different from all other objects, and
same logic to individual and interpersonal levels of
self-representation, suggesting that the opposing needs 2
Niedenthal and Beike (1997) offered a competing view by distin-
for differentiation and assimilation are played out at the
guishing between interrelated and isolated self-concepts. Interre-
individual level in terms of uniqueness and similarity lated concepts are defined in relation to other concepts at the same or
and at the interpersonal level in terms of separation and a superordinate level of abstraction; isolated concepts are defined in
intimacy. They argued additionally that frustration of terms of their subordinate features. Applying this to the current ex-
either need at one level might lead to a shift between lev- ample, being a musician might be defined in terms of the subordinate
feature of being able to play music, rather than by direct comparison
els of self-representation.
with other categories on the same or a higher level of abstraction.
In summary, optimal distinctiveness theory provides a However, it remains unclear in this case how the key subordinate fea-
stronger theoretical and empirical argument than the pre- ture can be defined except in terms of a distinction at this level be-
ceding accounts for the distinctiveness principle. Distinc- tween those who are or are not able to play music.

340
THE DISTINCTIVENESS PRINCIPLE

especially that he is different from those specific ob- preferences. Self-categorization was more pronounced
jects which are other persons. (Codol, 1981, p. 114) if the group was perceived to be numerically distinctive,
but this was only the case if the categorization was ap-
Consistent with this, developmental studies have sug- parently meaningful. Spears and Jetten (1998, cited in
gested that the distinction between self and others Branscombe et al., 1999) examined the effect on inter-
arises very early in life in association with other dimen- group discrimination of manipulating the
sions of identity (Damon & Hart, 1988; Lewis & meaningfulness of a minimal group categorization. Par-
Brooks-Gunn, 1979; Stern, 1985). Furthermore, the ab- ticipants displayed intergroup discrimination only in
sence of this distinction is experienced as a loss of self the low meaningfulness condition, implying that they
in some forms of psychosis (Apter, 1983). were seeking positive distinctiveness to give meaning to
According to Codol (1981), it is also important that the groups. Hogg and colleagues (Grieve & Hogg, 1999;
distinctiveness be recognized socially. Identity, in Hogg & Abrams, 1993; Mullin & Hogg, 1998, 1999)
common with other forms of representation also emphasized the importance of meaning for social
(Moscovici, 1984; Rommetveit, 1990), is not con- identity, suggesting that uncertainty reduction might be
structed solely within the individual but emerges a central motive underlying social identity processes.
through an interaction of processes of perception, cog- In summary, the argument developed here proposes
nition, and communication, involving the insertion of a central role for distinctiveness in establishing and pro-
the individual within a social environment (Lewis & jecting a meaningful sense of identity. This illuminates
Brooks-Gunn, 1979; Marková, 1987; Reicher, 1995). Brewer’s (1991) assertion that differentiation is neces-
Thus, projecting a sense of distinctiveness to others, sary for self-definition but does not contradict the pre-
and not solely to acquire a sense of distinctiveness for ceding accounts of the social value of distinctiveness
oneself, is important. and the role of distinctiveness in self-enhancement.
This argument also entails that similarity will be im-
portant in constructing a meaningful sense of identity. In
the earlier example, the concept of stream was defined Conclusions About the Role of
in terms of its distinctiveness from concepts of river and Distinctiveness in Identity Processes
rivulet, but this distinctiveness was only useful in as
much as the three concepts were also connected to each We have discussed assumptions that distinctiveness
other—attempting to define the concept of stream in is important for identity because of the social value of
comparison with alternative concepts of gorilla and distinctiveness itself, as a means of self-enhancement
spaceship would be less useful. Similarly, in defining through social comparison, as a fundamental human
one’s identity, we can expect that nondistinctiveness as need, and as a basic property of self-definition. None
well as distinctiveness will be important (Codol, of these assumptions is incompatible with the others.
1984b). Without any perceived connection between self However, research has focused on the whole on dem-
and others, no conceptual framework will be available onstrating the relation of distinctiveness to particular
within which to anchor one’s concept of self. identity states. These arguments have been invoked as
Note that distinctiveness will be important not only explanations of the findings but have received compar-
on an individual level of comparison according to this atively little direct empirical attention.
account; group distinctiveness also will be important in The hypothesis that distinctiveness is important for
two senses. First, multiple group identifications might identity because of its social value has not been ad-
be combined as a route to individual distinctiveness (B. dressed directly. An empirical demonstration would
Simon, 1997; Snyder & Fromkin, 1980). Second, iden- need to show that differences in the strength of distinc-
tification with a group will not be meaningful if the tiveness motivation were accounted for by existing or
group is not distinguished at all from others (Turner, experimentally induced differences in the value of dis-
1987). Even identities appearing to be extremely inclu- tinctiveness. To the best of our knowledge, no such re-
sive often have distinctive meanings—for example, a search has been conducted. On the other hand, the role of
person defining herself as a “citizen of the world” might positive distinctiveness in self-enhancement appears to
do so partly to distinguish herself from others who do be relatively well supported empirically within Western
not have this self-understanding. studies. However, the motivation for distinctiveness ap-
Research conducted with group identities is consis- parently is not entirely accounted for by self-esteem
tent with an account of distinctiveness as a source of maintenance, providing implicit support for the as-
meaning within identity. B. Simon, Hastedt, and sumption that distinctiveness is a basic property of
Aufderheide (1997) manipulated the apparent numeri- meaningful self-definition, although this assumption
cal distinctiveness and meaningfulness of participants’ has not been tested directly. The latter account has a the-
stated preferences for urban or rural life and measured oretical advantage in terms of parsimony. The role of
self-categorization in terms of groups based on these distinctiveness in identity processes is explained in

341
VIGNOLES, CHRYSSOCHOOU, & BREAKWELL

terms of more general principles of human representa- The Western concept of personhood is tied to politi-
tion. Furthermore, given the interaction of the distinc- cal, economic, and ethical aspects of individualism
tiveness principle with a motive for self-esteem, this (Durkheim, 1898/1969; Lukes, 1973; Marková et al.,
account subsumes the argument concerning positive 1998; Triandis, 1993, 1995; Triandis, Chan, Bhawuk,
distinctiveness as a means of self-enhancement. Iwao, & Sinha, 1995). Definitions of individualism
Important, too, is that all of the theoretical perspec- vary considerably, but most accounts involve a core
tives outlined heretofore and almost all of the empirical theme of “the conception of individuals as autonomous
studies mentioned are of Western origin. Although the from groups” (Triandis et al., 1995, p. 462). The emer-
social value account is by definition culture bound, ar- gence and development of individualism has been
guments for the role of distinctiveness in self-definition linked to specific historical circumstances, including
and self-enhancement are in principle more amenable to the early development of Christianity, development of
generalization because distinctiveness is viewed as a individual property rights, Renaissance, invention of
structural dimension of identity, rather than an aspect of the printing press, Reformation, and rise of liberal so-
identity content. We discuss the generalization of the cial philosophy (Dumont, 1986; Farr, 1991;
distinctiveness principle to non-Western cultures in de- Macfarlane, 1978; Marková et al., 1998), but individu-
tail in the following section. alism since has become a powerful and pervasive cul-
An implicit or explicit feature of all of the accounts is tural phenomenon. Thus, core values of individualism
that the distinctiveness principle is balanced with pres- survived into the 1990s within central Europe, despite
sures against distinctiveness, whether toward similarity 40 years of totalitarian attempts to eradicate them
(Snyder & Fromkin, 1980), inclusion in social groups (Marková, 1997; Marková et al., 1998).
(Brewer, 1991), or belonging (Baumeister & Leary, Individualism has had a substantial influence on
1995). These opposing forces have been understood to Western psychology, shaping both the content and
account for the limited size of observed effects of the structure of many theories (Farr, 1996; Sampson, 1977).
distinctiveness principle. We return in greater detail to For example, the “fundamental attribution error” (Ross,
the issue of how the distinctiveness principle interacts 1977) has been accepted as a universal feature of person
with other motivations in the final section of this article. perception, but in fact it is tied closely to assumptions of
individualism and does not appear to generalize across
cultures (I. Choi, Nisbett, & Norenzayan, 1999;
Ichheiser, 1949; Miller, 1984; Morris & Peng, 1994).
Culture and the Distinctiveness Hence, some have argued that social psychologists
Principle should be aware of cultural assumptions other than their
own as a means of reducing individualistic bias (Kim &
The second issue we review here is that of whether Berry, 1993; Sampson, 1978). Given the salience of dis-
the distinctiveness principle might be moderated by cul- tinctiveness within the Western concept of the person,
ture, and if so, in what sense? Breakwell (1987) ex- that the distinctiveness principle might be moderated by
pressed caution that the distinctiveness principle, and culture seems reasonable. In the following discussion,
especially the importance for identity of individual dis- we focus especially on the argument for relativity
tinctiveness, might be specific to Western or individual- Triandis (1995) advanced in his theory of individualism
istic cultures. This concern can be traced to and collectivism, which suggests that individual dis-
anthropologist Clifford Geertz’s (1975) famous caveat: tinctiveness will be less important and inclusion within
groups more important in collectivist cultures than in in-
The Western conception of the person as a bounded,
dividualist cultures.
unique, more or less integrated motivational and cogni-
tive universe, a dynamic center of awareness, emotion,
judgment, and action organized into a distinctive whole
and set contrastively both against other such wholes and Collectivism and Distinctiveness
against a social and natural background is, however in-
corrigible it may seem to us, a rather peculiar idea Triandis’s concept of collectivism is derived mainly
within the context of the world’s cultures. (p. 48) from psychometric studies comparing North American
and Southeast Asian populations (Triandis, 1989,
The Western concept of personhood is saturated with 1993, 1995; Triandis et al., 1995; Triandis, McCusker,
individual distinctiveness: The person is ‘bounded’ and & Hui, 1990). Collectivism is defined in opposition to
‘unique,’ contrasted with others and the social and individualism: “The central theme of individualism is
physical environment. But these features are not em- the conception of individuals as autonomous from
phasized in concepts of personhood observed within groups; the central theme of collectivism is the concep-
non-Western cultures (Geertz, 1975; Markus & tion of individuals as aspects of groups or collectives”
Kitayama, 1991; Shweder & Bourne, 1984). (Triandis et al., 1995, p. 462). These themes account

342
THE DISTINCTIVENESS PRINCIPLE

for a range of cross-cultural differences in the self-con- reversal of the outgroup homogeneity effect, which has
cept, values, behavior, and social structure (Triandis et been connected theoretically to the motivation for indi-
al., 1990). According to Triandis (1993), the self is vidual distinctiveness (Brewer, 1993b). However, in
construed as independent and different from others un- their study, participants rated ingroups and outgroups
der individualism, but interdependent and the same as together, a condition in which the effect has been ob-
others under collectivism. served elsewhere to be eradicated through the activation
Triandis (1995) integrated the concepts of individu- of an intergroup comparative context (Haslam, Oakes,
alism and collectivism with optimal distinctiveness Turner, & McGarty, 1995). Furthermore, judgments
theory. He suggested that cultural differences will af- were made in terms of agreement about values, which
fect the balance between the opposing needs for differ- might imply a judgment of group cohesiveness, rather
entiation and inclusion so that equilibrium will occur at than similarity in traits or attitudes as in most studies of
a lower degree of distinctiveness in collectivist than in perceived homogeneity (e.g., Judd, Ryan, & Park, 1991;
individualistic cultures.3 This implies that group iden- Park & Rothbart, 1982). Both of these studies might be
tities (understood to be less differentiating and more interpreted as showing variation in the cultural value of
including) will be more important, whereas individual individual distinctiveness (or its opposite, ingroup ho-
identities will be less important in collectivist than in mogeneity), which is not necessarily related to the im-
individualist cultures, resulting in a reduction in the portance of distinctiveness for identity. These results
importance of individual distinctiveness for self-defi- might lead to a prediction of cross-cultural variation in
nition (see also Smith & Bond, 1993). the distinctiveness principle, according to the social
value argument described earlier, but they have not
demonstrated this variation.
Evidence for Cross-Cultural Variation More secure is the evidence for variation in the use
of positive distinctiveness as a strategy of self-en-
Neither Triandis (1995) nor Smith and Bond (1993) hancement. Research conducted in Japan generally has
presented evidence directly addressing the previous as- failed to replicate the results of Western studies of
sertions. Indeed, researchers have paid little attention self-enhancement (Heine et al., 1999). Takata (1987)
to distinctiveness motivation within Asian literature on found that Japanese participants were more ready to
self and identity, although this fact in itself might be accept failures than successes in social comparison,
understood to speak against cross-cultural generality. while Markus and Kitayama (1991) reported a study in
(Heine, Lehman, Markus, & Kitayama, 1999, asserted which they replicated the false-uniqueness bias
a similar argument about self-esteem.) Nevertheless, (Campbell, 1986) found among U.S., but not among
some evidence exists for cross-cultural variation. Japanese, participants. These results at first appear
In a pancultural study of individual differences con- consistent with Triandis’s (1995) discussion of distinc-
ducted in Japan, Korea, and the United States, tiveness. However, Heine and Lehman (1997) reported
Yamaguchi, Kuhlman, and Sugimori (1995) found that parallel cross-cultural differences between Canadian
allocentrism, an individual differences variable concep- and Japanese participants in both self-serving and
tually equivalent to collectivism, was negatively corre- group-serving biases. Canadians rated both themselves
lated with “need for uniqueness.” Although the close and their groups as significantly more positively dis-
resemblance of some items measuring the two con- tinctive than did Japanese participants. Thus,
structs casts some doubt on the authors’ interpretation of cross-cultural differences in self-enhancement cannot
the reported correlations, the findings indicate the pres- be accounted for by a shift in emphasis from individual
ence of systematic differences in the endorsement of to group identities when moving from individualist to
distinctiveness-related attitude items that were partially collectivist cultures. These findings question the
accounted for by respondents’ cultural backgrounds. cross-cultural generality of the function of distinctive-
Meanwhile, Triandis et al. (1990) found that respon- ness as a means to self-esteem but do not address the
dents from China and Asians living in Hawaii perceived argument about distinctiveness and meaning, which
members of their ingroups to be more similar to each specifically states that distinctiveness is important for
other than members of a range of outgroups, an apparent identity independently of self-esteem.

3
Triandis’s (1995) account does not follow automatically from op- Indigenous Perspectives on
timal distinctiveness theory. If the differentiation and inclusion needs Collectivism
are “universal human motives” (Brewer & Pickett, 1999, p. 85), the
opposite version is also possible: Perhaps the need for inclusion is
less satisfied and hence more aroused in individualistic cultures, Triandis’s (1993, 1995) portrayal of collectivism
whereas the need for differentiation is less satisfied and hence more has not always been echoed within the indigenous psy-
aroused in collectivist cultures. chologies of countries he classified as collectivist. The-

343
VIGNOLES, CHRYSSOCHOOU, & BREAKWELL

orists from Southeast Asia and Asia Minor have whole, but this does not involve denying the value or the
described their own cultures in terms of an orientation existence of individual distinctiveness. On the contrary,
emphasizing relationships between distinct individu- distinctiveness may be derived explicitly from a per-
als, rather than the anonymous membership of undif- son’s location within a social or natural environment.
ferentiated collectives (Hamaguchi, 1985; Ho, 1993; Gao (1996) described a Confucian understanding of
Kâgitçibasi, 1994; Kim, 1994; Yuki & Brewer, 1999). selfhood: “Self … is defined by a person’s surrounding
Kim (1994) distinguished among three modes of relations, which often are derived from kinship net-
collectivism, involving different representations of the works and supported by cultural values such as filial
relation between individual and group. The traditional piety, loyalty, dignity, and integrity” (p. 83). This defi-
conceptualization of collectivism is focused on the un- nition clearly can be contrasted with the Western
differentiated mode, characterized by “firm and ex- “bounded” individual. According to Gao, the Chinese
plicit group boundaries, coupled with undifferentiated self “needs to be recognized, defined, and completed
self-group boundaries” (p. 33). The relational mode is by others” (p. 84). However, under Confucianism, pre-
characterized by “porous boundaries between in-group cisely through relationships with others do people de-
members that allow thoughts, ideas, and emotions to fine themselves as individuals: “Without others, the
flow freely,” and “focuses on the relationship shared very notion of individual identity loses meaning” (Ho,
by the in-group members” (p. 34). The coexistence 1995, p. 131). Analyses of Chinese and Japanese
mode is characterized by a separation between publicly words relating to self and identity support this under-
collectivist and privately individual and relational standing. The Chinese word for ‘person,’ ren, is pri-
selves. marily associated with “the place of the individual in a
Kim (1994) argued that the relational and coexis- web of interpersonal relationships” (Hsu, 1985, p. 33),
tence modes are generally representative of Southeast and shenfen, or ‘identity,’ is also associated with “a
Asian cultures (see also Chang & Lee, 2000; S. C. person’s social ‘place’” (Ho, 1993, p. 256). Mean-
Choi, Kim, & Choi, 1993; Hamaguchi, 1985; Ho, while, the Japanese term for ‘self,’ jibun, is a com-
1993; Lebra, 1992; Sinha & Tripathi, 1994), but these pound of bun, which means “portion, share, part, or
modes frequently are mistaken for the undifferentiated fraction” (Lebra, 1976, p. 67).
mode within the cross-cultural literature. The rela- The construction of distinctiveness in terms of so-
tional mode also appears consistent with Mexican cial position is not specific to Southeast Asian cultures.
(Lomnitz, 1976/1977), traditional African (Beattie, Macdonald (1997) described the significance of local-
1980; Ma & Schoeneman, 1997), and Pacific Island ness in a community in the Scottish Hebrides, which
(White & Kirkpatrick, 1985) cultures, and forms of the provides both belonging and distinctiveness in terms
coexistence mode can be recognized within Bedouin of “who is and who is not ‘part of the place’” (p. 131).
(Abu-Lughod, 1985) and Moroccan (Geertz, 1975) Geographical location underlies a relationally oriented
cultures. form of distinctiveness, which “locates individuals not
Interestingly, Yuki and Brewer’s (1999) research just in a particular geography, but in a set of kin and
suggests that the representation of groups as undiffer- neighbours” (p. 144).
entiated might be more characteristic of North Ameri- An extreme example of relational orientation is in
can than Japanese culture. Yuki and Brewer Geertz’s (1975) description of Balinese culture, where
contrasted this representation with a model of the the person is construed as the temporary occupant of a
group as a network of interdependent but distinct in- timeless social position. But far from indicating a ne-
dividuals, consistent with the relational mode, which gation of individual distinctiveness, it is precisely the
appears to be more characteristic of Japan, as well as importance of social position that differentiates one in-
the representation of smaller groups within the United dividual from another. Again, distinctiveness comes
States. This implies that Triandis’s (1995) predictions from one’s place within social relations rather than
about distinctiveness might be associated with the from boundedness or uniqueness.
misapplication of a North American concept of group
to Asian cultures.
Possible Negations of the
Distinctiveness Principle
Relational Forms of Distinctiveness
Although the previously reviewed evidence is con-
Many indigenous and anthropological portrayals of sistent with the importance of individual distinctiveness
relationally oriented cultures demonstrate the possibil- for identity within nonindividualistic cultures, we
ity of alternative forms of distinctiveness to Western should address possible exceptions to this rule. We dis-
concepts of boundedness and uniqueness. The person is cuss here Ho’s (1995) exploration of self and identity
seen as interdependent with others and part of a greater within the philosophical and religious systems of Tao-

344
THE DISTINCTIVENESS PRINCIPLE

ism, Hinduism, and Buddhism, as well as recent re- that relationally oriented participants would be more
search into the distinction between analytic and holistic sensitive than individualistic participants to distinc-
forms of thought (I. Choi, Nisbett, & Smith, 1997). tions between people within social relations.
According to Ho (1995), Taoism, Buddhism, and
Hinduism advocate forms of selflessness, which involve
negating the self–nonself boundary. Within Taoism, the Defining the Boundaries of Cultural
goal is harmony with nature without “making distinc- Relativity
tions” (p. 121). According to Hinduism, the illusion of
individuated selfhood must be overcome to realize the In summary, contrary to the concerns about cultural
true self, Atman (breath/spirit). Buddhism denies the specificity previously expressed by Breakwell (1987)
ontological reality of the self altogether. These three tra- and amplified in the work of Triandis (1993, 1995), the
ditions can be viewed implicitly as psychological theo- evidence reviewed here is entirely consistent with the
ries, all of which are opposed to the argument that importance of individual distinctiveness for a meaning-
distinctiveness is necessary to achieve meaning in iden- ful sense of identity within nonindividualistic cultures.
tity. However, the psychological realization of these The only possible exception we have noted is where
claims is less clear. Ho based his discussion on pure people actively seek to negate their distinctiveness
forms of each tradition but noted that real life is some- through attempts to achieve selflessness, but even this
what different. Scientific research into the psychologi- implies the prior existence of a distinctiveness motive.
cal implications of adopting these philosophies is in its Nevertheless, distinctiveness appears to take very
infancy. Moreover, all three traditions acknowledge that different forms according to individualistic or rela-
selflessness can only be achieved through the transcen- tional orientations. Under individualism, distinctive-
dence of preexisting concepts of self and individual dis- ness is constructed mainly in terms of difference and
tinctiveness. Even Buddhism’s denying the ontological separateness from others, corresponding to the empha-
reality of the self implicitly acknowledges its psycho- sis on uniqueness and boundedness within the Western
logical reality. Buddhist and Vedic meditation tech- concept of person (Geertz, 1975). Under relational ori-
niques, which involve transcending self–nonself entation, distinctiveness appears to have more to do
boundaries to eliminate self-consciousness (de Silva, with one’s position within social relationships, reflect-
1993; Orme-Johnson, Zimmerman, & Hawkins, 1997), ing the significance of social place within the Chinese
are entirely consistent with the understanding that dis- concept of ren (Ho, 1993). Hence, a possible approach
tinctiveness is essential for identity. would be to understand the distinctiveness principle as
The negation of self–other distinctiveness advo- a generative structure, a universal potential that is real-
cated by these traditions might be connected with the ized differently across cultures (Fiske et al., 1998).
observed inclination of members of Asian cultures to- Thus, it would be suggested that distinctiveness is a
ward holistic modes of thought—emphasizing rela- universal principle guiding identity processes, but the
tions between elements—as opposed to the Western sources of distinctiveness used to define identity will
dominance of analytic modes of thought—emphasiz- be inherently variable across cultures.
ing categorical distinctions between elements (Fiske, Even at this level, it is important to avoid
Kitayama, Markus, & Nisbett, 1998; Peng & Nisbett, overexaggerating or reifying cultural differences. A
1999). Research conducted by I. Choi et al. (1997) sug- common theme in accounts implicitly or explicitly
gests that Americans use categorical distinctions in in- dealing with cultural conceptions of self is the asser-
ductive reasoning more readily than do Koreans. tion that these constructs coexist and vary considerably
Taken on its own, this result appears to cast some within cultures: Variations have been observed be-
doubt on the generality of the importance of distinc- tween groups (Hinkle & Brown, 1990; Lorenzi-Cioldi,
tiveness for the construction of a meaningful identity. 1995) and between individuals (Gudykunst et al.,
If members of East Asian cultures generally are less 1996; Triandis et al., 1995) within cultures. Variations
predisposed to represent concepts in terms of distinc- also have been observed within individuals in out-
tiveness, then this might be expected to apply to their comes associated with cross-cultural differences, ac-
representations of themselves. However, when their cording to social context (Chatman & Barsade, 1995;
reasoning task concerned categories of people, I. Choi Holland & Kipnis, 1994), physical surroundings
et al. found the opposite effect: Koreans used categori- (Speller, Lyons, & Twigger-Ross, 1999), level of
cal distinctions more readily than Americans did. We self-representation (Gardner, Gabriel, & Lee, 1999),
should note that this condition is more relevant to iden- different identifications (Realo, Allik, & Vadi, 1997),
tity, which concerns the processing of information and the cueing of cultural frames (Hong, Chiu, &
about people rather than nonsocial categories. In fact, Kung, 1997; Hong, Morris, Chiu, & Benet-Martínez,
this effect was interpreted specifically in terms of the 1999). Furthermore, given the current trends toward
relational orientation of Korean culture: It was argued globalization of communication networks and interna-

345
VIGNOLES, CHRYSSOCHOOU, & BREAKWELL

tional migration (Hermans & Kempen, 1998), refer- man motivation exists to see oneself as distinctive,
ring to “cultures” as discrete entities in the traditional which derives from the importance of distinctiveness
manner of cross-cultural research is becoming increas- for meaningful self-definition.
ingly problematic, at least within industrialized na- In relating distinctiveness to self-definition within
tions. Theorizing culture in such a way that nationality this proposition, we are distancing ourselves explicitly
and ethnicity are not the only ways to operationalize from the position that distinctiveness is important only
the term will be important. as a source of self-esteem. This is not to deny that dis-
tinctiveness is important for self-esteem in many cir-
cumstances (Breakwell, 1987; Gruder, 1977; Wills,
Theoretical Development of the 1991). However, we suggest that the primary function
Distinctiveness Principle of the distinctiveness principle is at a more basic level
than considerations of self-enhancement. In addition,
In this article, we set out to examine two issues: (a) we do not suggest that distinctiveness is the only im-
the precise role of distinctiveness within identity pro- portant dimension of meaning within identity. On the
cesses, and (b) whether—and, if so, in what contrary, similarity is clearly important in this respect
sense—this role is modified by culture. We have an- (Codol, 1984b; Festinger, 1954), as is some form of
swered (a) that, at least within Western cultures, dis- subjective continuity (Apter, 1983; Chandler &
tinctiveness appears to have a fundamental role in the Lalonde, 1995; Codol, 1981).
construction of meaning within identity, and (b) that Moreover, we do not assert that the distinctiveness
the distinctiveness principle, thus theorized, is not in- principle is necessarily universal, although we cer-
compatible with non-Western cultural systems, pro- tainly expect it to be pervasive. In linking the principle
vided that distinctiveness is understood sufficiently to processes of self-definition, it is possible to specify
broadly to include relationally oriented as well as indi- that the principle will be in operation—and the rest of
vidualistic forms, although additional functions of dis- this argument will hold—wherever people use distinc-
tinctiveness as a means to self-esteem do not appear to tions between concepts to represent social entities (see
generalize. I. Choi et al., 1997). Although imagining such an oc-
Having dealt with these issues, we now turn to a for- currence is difficult, in the unlikely event that this con-
mal theoretical statement of the distinctiveness princi- dition were not met, assuming the presence of a
ple. We discuss the following propositions (Ps): distinctiveness motive would be wrong. However, pro-
vided that this basic condition is met, the construction
P1. A pervasive human motivation exists to see of a meaningful sense of identity necessarily must have
oneself as distinctive, which derives from the some grounding in distinctiveness.
importance of distinctiveness for meaningful Thus, it is predicted that members of both individual-
self-definition. istic and relationally oriented cultures will be motivated
P2. Distinctiveness may be constructed in multi- to distinguish themselves from others and their groups
ple ways, using dimensions of position, differ- from other groups both cognitively and behaviorally.
ence, and separateness on both individual and This motive is not equated with self-esteem mainte-
group levels. nance, and it will be aroused especially where distinc-
P3. As sources of distinctiveness, position, differ- tiveness is threatened or frustrated. These predictions
ence, and separateness coexist within cultures have been supported by research conducted in Europe
but vary in their availability, accessibility, and and North America (Branscombe et al., 1999;
value according to culture and context. Breakwell, 1986a; Brewer et al., 1993; Brewer &
P4. These sources of distinctiveness have differ- Pickett, 1999; Codol, 1984a, 1987; Hornsey & Hogg,
ent implications for the interaction of the dis- 1999; Jetten, Spears, & Manstead, 1999; Leyens et al.,
tinctiveness principle with other motives in 1997; Mlicki & Ellemers, 1996; L. Simon et al., 1997;
identity dynamics. Snyder & Fromkin, 1980; Vignoles et al., 2000). How-
ever, further research is needed to test these predictions
across a range of cultures and examine the relation be-
The Presence of a Motive for tween distinctiveness and meaning in self-definition,
Distinctiveness which is a central assumption of our account.

Our earlier discussion of the role of distinctiveness


in identity favored the interpretation that distinctive- Sources of Distinctiveness
ness is a basic necessity for the construction of mean-
ing within identity. Thus, P1 reflects the views of The preceding discussion of identity and distinctive-
Brewer (1991) and Codol (1981) that a pervasive hu- ness across cultures has highlighted the importance of

346
THE DISTINCTIVENESS PRINCIPLE

defining distinctiveness broadly, in terms of the multi- ter” does not entail that I necessarily feel different or
ple ways in which people are able to differentiate them- separate from Peter, but it distinguishes me in that it lo-
selves from each other within a given context. Hence, P2 cates me as an individual within the interpersonal net-
is that distinctiveness may be constructed in multiple work of my family. Intergroup distinctions also may be
ways, using dimensions of position, difference, and sep- constructed in terms of position: Many group identities
arateness on both individual and group levels. These entail particular relationships with other groups (e.g.,
sources of distinctiveness represent aspects of the rela- teachers and students, doctors and patients, supporters
tionship between self and others that may be empha- of opposing football teams). In operational terms, posi-
sized to achieve a sense of distinctiveness. tion is a complex construct, which may incorporate
Difference refers to distinctiveness in what are per- concepts of role (Hoelter, 1983; Stryker, 1987), social
ceived to be intrinsic qualities of the individual, such as status (Lorenzi-Cioldi, 1995), and positions within in-
abilities, opinions, traits, physical characteristics, and so tergroup relations, which have been understood else-
forth. Perceived difference also may serve as a basis for where as the outcome of identity processes but may
group memberships, either according to defined criteria also contribute to the psychological meaning of groups
for membership—for example, most occupations re- themselves (Tajfel & Turner, 1986).
quire some demonstration of knowledge or ability be- Position, difference, and separateness may not be
fore membership is granted—or through processes of functionally independent of each other, but they are in-
social categorization (Oakes, Haslam, & Turner, 1994; dependent concepts in the sense that they can be de-
Turner, 1987). Perceptions of difference have been fined without reference to each other. Furthermore, all
measured or manipulated, both in Western studies and three sources are necessary for a comprehensive con-
cross-cultural replications, to operationalize theoretical ceptual or operational definition of distinctiveness.
constructs of distinctiveness, uniqueness, similarity, ho- This is illustrated by research we have conducted into
mogeneity, metacontrast, and so on (e.g., Fromkin, the construction of distinctiveness among parish
1972; Hakmiller, 1966; Haslam et al., 1995; Heine & priests (Vignoles, 2000; Vignoles, Chryssochoou, &
Lehman, 1997; Jetten et al., 1999; Judd et al., 1991; Breakwell, 1999). Each participant generated a series
Leyens et al., 1997; Park & Rothbart, 1982; Takata, of self-description items in response to the question
1987; Wheeler, 1966). “Who am I?” (after Kuhn & McPartland, 1954), then
The construct of separateness, also rooted in West- rated each item for associations with distinctiveness
ern thinking, is closely related to the concept of the and with position, difference, and separateness in par-
bounded individual (Geertz, 1975). Separateness en- ish and clergy contexts. We examined contributions of
compasses feelings of boundedness, independence, or the three sources to distinctiveness using multilevel re-
distance from others. Feelings of separateness may be gression models predicting the variance among items
reinforced through the manipulation of physical in distinctiveness ratings for each context. In both con-
boundaries, such as designing housing for greater pri- texts, all three sources made significant, unique, and
vacy (Speller et al., 1999), or through the use of sym- substantial contributions to the distinctiveness ratings.
bolic boundaries, such as a religious leader wearing However, the importance of distinguishing among
robes to symbolize the conceptual boundary between these three constructs lies also in the fact that each has
sacred and profane (after Durkheim, 1912/1915). Sep- different implications for identity and behavior. The
arateness may apply to both interpersonal and inter- construction and projection of a meaningful sense of
group differentiation. An extreme example of group identity in terms of position, difference, or separate-
separateness would be those religious orders that func- ness will be associated with an emphasis on different
tion as closed communities, avoiding contact with the aspects of identity within self-definition and different
outside world and thus combining both physical and forms of behavior in social settings. A person focused
symbolic boundaries. Separateness has received con- on his or her position in a given situation will be espe-
siderably less attention than difference within research cially aware of relationships with others and may show
into identity. However, items referring to the subjec- his or her distinctive position by conforming to role
tive value of individual separateness can be found in prescriptions; a person focused on his or her difference
some psychometric measures of individualism–collec- from others will be especially aware of personal quali-
tivism (e.g., Triandis, 1995; Triandis et al., 1995). ties and may show his or her difference by avoiding
Position, on the other hand, is tied to a relational conformity; a person focused on separateness from
orientation. Where separateness implicitly involves others in a given situation may simply withdraw from
the negation of relationships, the concept of position the situation either psychologically or physically.
refers explicitly to one’s place within social relation- The definitions of position, difference, and sepa-
ships. A paradigm example of distinctiveness con- rateness outlined here have not been restricted to indi-
structed in terms of position is given by family vidual distinctiveness, but also pertain to group
relationships: The statement “I am the brother of Pe- distinctiveness in that groups may have particular rela-

347
VIGNOLES, CHRYSSOCHOOU, & BREAKWELL

tions of position, difference, or separateness between 1997) or indirectly by emphasizing individual sepa-
them. On this level, an additional dimension of distinc- rateness from others, by increasing identification with
tiveness is the size of the group, with smaller groups numerically distinctive groups (cf. Brewer & Pickett,
being more distinctive (Brewer, 1991; Brewer & 1999), or by engaging in intergroup discrimination (cf.
Pickett, 1999; B. Simon & Brown, 1987; B. Simon et Jetten et al., 1999). An interesting illustration of the in-
al., 1997). The distinctiveness principle may push to- terplay between constructions of distinctiveness is
ward group distinctiveness for several reasons. Where Speller et al.’s (1999) research into the enforced relo-
people categorize themselves in terms of a salient cation of a traditional English coal-mining community.
group membership, the meaningfulness of identity will Before relocation, a strong sense of belonging was evi-
be contingent on the meaningfulness of the group, dent in which group distinctiveness was derived from
which rests in part on group distinctiveness (Oakes et the uniqueness of the community (group difference)
al., 1994; Turner, 1987). However, self-categorization and individual distinctiveness was derived from rela-
on an individual level also involves information about tionships within the community (individual position).
group memberships as well as information that might However, the new environment undermined both the
potentially form the basis for group memberships (B. uniqueness of the community and the relationships in
Simon, 1997). Thus, even where the group level of which individual positions were grounded. Neverthe-
self-representation is not salient, group distinctiveness less, new signs of differentiation began to emerge in
may be an important dimension in the construction of a the new setting as many villagers increased their pri-
meaningful sense of identity. vacy (individual separateness) and personalized their
We should stress that a single aspect of identity may houses with colorful external decorations (individual
be distinctive in terms of more than one source and on difference).
more than one level of self-representation. For example, Distinguishing among position, difference, and sep-
I might derive distinctiveness from my identity as a mu- arateness as sources of individual and group distinc-
sician in terms of individual difference (e.g., other peo- tiveness provides a more comprehensive theoretical
ple do not have my skills) and in terms of my intergroup and operational definition of distinctiveness than pre-
position (e.g., I have a particular relationship with my viously was available and highlights the wide range of
audience as a performer). Moreover, people may derive cognitive and behavioral outcomes that might be af-
distinctiveness in different ways from the same identity. fected by the distinctiveness principle depending on
Millward’s (1995) study of how nurses represented be- how distinctiveness is constructed. Research now is
ing a nurse illustrates this: Some focused on profes- needed to investigate in greater depth the extent to
sional distinctiveness, stressing group-level difference, which different constructions of distinctiveness are
whereas others were more patient-centered, emphasiz- complementary in satisfying the distinctiveness princi-
ing their individual position within interpersonal rela- ple and the different consequences of these construc-
tionships. Hence, the distinction between sources of tions for identity and action.
distinctiveness is not intended to produce a typology of
identities (cf. Deaux, Reid, Mizrahi, & Ethier, 1995).
Although different identities may provide different op- Culture and Context
portunities for achieving distinctiveness, our aim is not
to categorize identities but to demonstrate that multiple The distinction among sources of distinctiveness is
ways exist in which a sense of distinctiveness may be related theoretically to cultural variations in individu-
constructed. A key issue in operationalizing position, alistic and relational orientations, but the distribution
difference, and separateness will be to establish the indi- of these orientations across cultures is far from clear
vidual’s construction of his or her distinctiveness, rather cut, with considerable variation within cultures as well
than treating these dimensions as intrinsic properties of as trends toward globalization blurring the boundaries
particular identities. between cultures. Hence, P3 is that, as sources of dis-
An important implication of distinguishing between tinctiveness, position, difference, and separateness co-
multiple ways of constructing a sense of distinctive- exist within cultures but vary in their availability,
ness is that a considerable range of strategies may be accessibility, and value according to culture and con-
used to maintain or enhance distinctiveness where the text.
principle has been threatened or frustrated. Strategies We already have acknowledged the availability of
may include shifting between levels of self-representa- position as a source of distinctiveness within Western
tion (Brewer & Gardner, 1996) or between sources of cultures. Manifestations include family relationships
distinctiveness. For example, the negative implications and many intergroup relationships. Similarly, we ex-
of feeling excessively similar to other individuals pect difference and separateness to be available as
might be attenuated directly by emphasizing dissimi- sources of distinctiveness in relationally oriented cul-
larity from others (Fromkin, 1972; L. Simon et al., tures. Studies into self-perception (Cousins, 1989),

348
THE DISTINCTIVENESS PRINCIPLE

causal attribution (I. Choi et al., 1999), folk psychol- oriented cultures. Self-construals and implicit theories
ogy (S. C. Choi, Kim, & Choi, 1993; Enriquez, 1993), of personhood also have been shown to vary within cul-
and indigenous theories of personality (Yang, 1997) tures according to context, both in natural settings (Hol-
show that members of Southeast Asian cultures make land & Kipnis, 1994) and using subtle experimental
dispositional inferences about themselves and others, manipulations (Gardner et al., 1999; Hong et al., 1999).
entailing the availability of difference as a source of Preliminary support for the importance of
distinctiveness, although inferences are often tied to self-construals comes from our research with parish
specific contexts. The availability of separateness as a priests. Despite participating in a culture normally
source of distinctiveness is harder to ascertain. Individ- classed as individualistic (United Kingdom), the priests
ual separateness is inconsistent with basic assumptions showed strongly interdependent self-construals. Of the
of relational orientation (Ho, 1993). On the other hand, various sources of distinctiveness we measured, they
some very salient symbolic boundaries might be un- emphasized especially distinctiveness in terms of their
derstood as instances of group separateness: Geisha position within the parish context in constructing their
might be understood as a separate group in Japan, identities (Vignoles et al., 1999).
while aspects of the caste system might be seen as a Third, cross-cultural differences in values (S. H.
source of separateness in India. However, empirical re- Schwartz, 1994) will affect the subjective value of po-
search is needed to determine whether these and other sition, difference, and separateness as sources of dis-
boundaries are perceived in terms of separateness. tinctiveness. For example, cultures emphasizing the
A significant result of theorizing distinctiveness to value of relationships will be less likely to value sepa-
include relational as well as individualistic sources is rateness as a source of distinctiveness, cultures empha-
that the arguments of Triandis (1995) and Smith and sizing the value of individual autonomy might be less
Bond (1993) may be reevaluated. Rather than hypothe- likely to value position, and cultures emphasizing so-
sizing a shift from individual to group identity when cial order might be less likely to value difference. Fol-
moving from Western to Eastern cultures, involving a lowing the account of distinctiveness as a social value,
reduction in the importance of individual distinctive- one reasonably might assume that the social value of
ness for self-definition, we suggest that individual dis- position, difference, and separateness will affect their
tinctiveness may be of comparable importance for use as sources of distinctiveness in the construction of
self-definition in relationally oriented cultures as in in- a meaningful and valued sense of identity. As with the
dividualistic cultures, but distinctiveness will be availability and accessibility of position, difference,
achieved in different ways according to both culture and separateness, their subjective value as sources of
and context. distinctiveness most likely also will vary substantially
Even if position, difference, and separateness are within cultures. S. H. Schwartz and Bilsky (1990)
present at some level in individualistic and found individual differences in values within many
relationally oriented cultures, their contextual avail- cultures. Furthermore, the endorsement of individual-
ability will vary in degree with cross-cultural differ- istic or relational values is sensitive to manipulations
ences in social organization. Cultural variations in of self-construals (Gardner et al., 1999) and cultural
family structures, communication styles, child-rear- frames (Hong et al., 1997).
ing patterns, work, and religion (see Fiske et al., For all of these reasons, we not only expect that dif-
1998; Triandis, 1995) all may be expected to affect ference and separateness will be more important for
the chronic availability of sources of distinctiveness. self-definition in individualistic cultures, whereas po-
Availability of the sources will also vary within cul- sition will be more important in relationally oriented
tures according to context. In some contexts (e.g., cultures, but we also predict that the relative emphasis
family meetings), position will be more available, on position, difference, or separateness in shaping
whereas in others (e.g., public examinations), differ- self-definition will vary systematically within cultures
ence and separateness will be emphasized. Also, according to the contextual availability, accessibility,
changes in the physical environment can lead to and value of each source. Thus, the use of distinguish-
chronic changes in the availability of different forms ing between sources of distinctiveness is not restricted
of distinctiveness (Speller et al., 1999). to the explanation of cross-cultural differences.
Second, cultural assumptions about personhood and
selfhood (Fiske et al., 1998; Markus & Kitayama, 1991)
will affect the cognitive accessibility of the three Interaction With Other Motives
sources when constructing a sense of distinctiveness.
Difference and separateness are more consistent with an An important issue for further development is the
independent self-construal, prevalent in individualistic relation between the distinctiveness principle and
cultures, whereas position is more consistent with an in- other motivations within identity dynamics
terdependent self-construal, prevalent in relationally (Breakwell, 1987; Vignoles et al., 2000). The value of

349
VIGNOLES, CHRYSSOCHOOU, & BREAKWELL

theorizing relations between multiple motivations has tween these principles might well be a function of con-
been exemplified by optimal distinctiveness theory textual constraints, rather than necessarily being intrin-
(Brewer, 1991). P4 is that these sources of distinctive- sic to the principles themselves. For example, we can
ness have different implications for the interaction of expect that the relation between distinctiveness and
the distinctiveness principle with other motives in self-esteem principles will depend on the availability
identity dynamics. of positive distinctiveness of each source, as well as the
The three sources appear to have very different im- positive or negative social evaluation of the sources in
plications for the relation between differentiation and themselves. This is illustrated by Breakwell’s (1986b)
inclusion needs, as theorized within optimal distinc- explanation for the apparent absence of distinctiveness
tiveness theory. Within Brewer’s (1991) model, dis- motivation among a group of unemployed young
tinctiveness is operationalized as group size. Thus, we women on a government training program. For these
see a clear opposition between the needs for differenti- women, she argued, distinctiveness seeking would in-
ation, satisfied by intergroup distinctiveness, and in- volve making negative social comparisons, which
clusion, satisfied by intragroup inclusiveness: The would create an opposition with the self-esteem princi-
larger the group, the more inclusion; the smaller the ple. To theorize the expected contributions of these
group, the more differentiation. Within our perspective various motivations (and their interrelationships with
on the individual level of self-representation, the oppo- each other) to the shaping of identity processes and so-
sition between needs for differentiation and inclusion cial behavior within a given context, the subjective
can be generalized to the individualistic sources of dif- meaning of identities, groups, and intergroup relations
ference and separateness. Difference is opposed with to those involved in the context must be examined in
similarity, while separateness is opposed with inti- detail, focusing on the availability of sources of dis-
macy or belonging (corresponding to the individual tinctiveness and the opportunities afforded for satisfy-
and interpersonal levels according to Brewer & ing these other motives.
Gardner, 1996). However, position may satisfy mo-
tives for differentiation and inclusion simultaneously:
Indeed, position actually depends on inclusion within Conclusions
social relationships. This is illustrated with another re-
sult from our study of parish priests (Vignoles, 2000; In summary, we argue herein that the distinctive-
Vignoles et al., 1999). Rating their responses to the ness principle has a fundamental role in establishing
“Who am I?” task, those items that the priests associ- meaning in identity, which does not appear to be spe-
ated with greater distinctiveness from their parishio- cific to individualistic cultures. An important product
ners in terms of position were also associated with of this argument has been the assertion that distinctive-
greater feelings of closeness to parishioners. Thus, ness can be achieved in terms of position, difference,
where distinctiveness was constructed in terms of posi- or separateness. These constructs coexist within cul-
tion, the opposition between differentiation and inclu- tures and individuals, but they will be emphasized dif-
sion needs was removed. ferently according to culture and context, and they
Returning to the group level of self-representation, have different implications for identity processes and
where group identities are represented as interpersonal behavior. In addition, the distinction between sources
relationships (cf. Millward, 1995), the situation is psy- of distinctiveness will be a useful theoretical tool for
chologically the same as for the individual level, and thinking about distinctiveness in different applied con-
the same predictions can be expected to hold. On the texts, especially where multiple motives and goals
other hand, where the group itself is salient, predic- need to be taken into account. We hope that future re-
tions may be considerably more complex, depending search conducted within and across a variety of cul-
on both the relationship between ingroup and outgroup tures and contexts will help illuminate the diverse
and the relationship between members within the ways in which distinctiveness can be achieved, the im-
group: Inclusion and differentiation needs potentially plications of these processes for other motivational
might be met both within the group and within the in- principles within identity, and the ways in which multi-
tergroup relationship. In this scenario, it will be impor- ple principles may interact with each other to guide
tant to pay close attention to group members’ identity processes and shape behavior.
subjective understandings of both within-group and
between-group sources of distinctiveness to predict
outcomes of the distinctiveness principle.
References
Such a situation will be complicated further by the
presence of other identity principles, such as motives Abrams, D., & Hogg, M. A. (1988). Comments on the motivational
for continuity, efficacy, and self-esteem (Breakwell, status of self-esteem in social identity and intergroup discrimi-
1993; Vignoles et al., 2000). The interrelationships be- nation. European Journal of Social Psychology, 18, 317–334.

350
THE DISTINCTIVENESS PRINCIPLE

Abu-Lughod, L. (1985). Honor and the sentiments of loss in a Bed- Chatman, J., & Barsade, S. G. (1995). Personality, organizational
ouin society. American Ethnologist, 12, 245–261. culture, and cooperation: Evidence from a business simulation.
Apter, M. J. (1983). Negativism and the sense of identity. In G. M. Administrative Science Quarterly, 40, 423–443.
Breakwell (Ed.), Threatened identities (pp. 75–90). New York: Chiu, C. Y., & Hong, Y. Y. (1999). Social identification in a political
Wiley. transition: The role of implicit beliefs. International Journal of
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire Intercultural Relations, 23, 297–318.
for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motiva- Choi, I., Nisbett, R. E., & Norenzayan, A. (1999). Causal attribution
tion. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497–529. across cultures: Variation and universality. Psychological Bul-
Beattie, J. (1980). Review article: Representations of the self in tradi- letin, 125, 47–63.
tional Africa. Africa, 50, 313–320. Choi, I., Nisbett, R. E., & Smith, E. E. (1997). Culture, category sa-
Branscombe, N. R., Ellemers, N., Spears, R., & Doosje, B. (1999). lience, and inductive reasoning. Cognition, 65, 15–32.
The context and content of social identity threat. In N. Ellemers, Choi, S. C., Kim, U., & Choi, S. H. (1993). Indigenous analysis of
R. Spears, & B. Doosje (Eds.), Social identity: Context, commit- collective representations: A Korean perspective. In U. Kim &
ment, content (pp. 35–58). Oxford, England: Blackwell. J. W. Berry (Eds.), Indigenous psychologies: Research and ex-
Breakwell, G. M. (1983). Formulations and searches. In G. M. perience in cultural context (pp. 193–210). Newbury Park, CA:
Breakwell (Ed.), Threatened identities (pp. 3–26). New York: Sage.
Wiley. Codol, J. P. (1981). Une approche cognitive du sentiment d’identité
Breakwell, G. M. (1986a). Coping with threatened identities. Lon- [A cognitive approach to the feeling of identity]. Social Science
don: Methuen. Information, 20, 111–136.
Breakwell, G. M. (1986b). Identities at work. In H. Beloff (Ed.), Codol, J. P. (1984a). La perception de la similitude interpersonnelle:
Getting into life (pp. 43–66). London: Methuen. Influence de l’appartenance catégorielle et du point de référence
de la comparaison [The perception of interpersonal similarity:
Breakwell, G. M. (1987). Identity. In H. Beloff & A. Coleman (Eds.),
Influence of category membership and of the comparison refer-
Psychology Survey 6 (pp. 94–114). Leicester, England: British
ence point]. L’Année Psychologique, 84, 43–56.
Psychological Society.
Codol, J. P. (1984b). Social differentiation and non-differentiation.
Breakwell, G. M. (1988). Strategies adopted when identity is threat-
In H. Tajfel (Ed.), The social dimension: European develop-
ened. Revue Internationale de Psychologie Sociale, 1, 189–203.
ments in social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 314–337). Cambridge,
Breakwell, G. M. (1993). Social representations and social identity. England: Cambridge University Press.
Papers on Social Representations, 2, 198–217.
Codol, J. P. (1987). Comparability and incomparability between one-
Brewer, M. B. (1991). The social self: On being the same and differ- self and others: Means of differentiation and comparison refer-
ent at the same time. Personality and Social Psychology Bulle- ence points. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 7,
tin, 17, 475–482. 1–19.
Brewer, M. B. (1993a). The role of distinctiveness in social identity Cousins, S. D. (1989). Culture and self-perception in Japan and the
and group behaviour. In M. A. Hogg & D. Abrams (Eds.), United States. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Group motivation: Social psychological perspectives (pp. 56, 124–131.
1–16). London: Harvester Wheatsheaf. Crocker, J., Thompson, L. L., McGraw, K. M., & Ingerman, C.
Brewer, M. B. (1993b). Social identity, distinctiveness, and in-group (1987). Downward comparison, prejudice, and evaluations of
homogeneity. Social Cognition, 11, 150–164. others: Effects of self-esteem and threat. Journal of Personality
Brewer, M. B., & Gardner, W. (1996). Who is this “we”? Levels of and Social Psychology, 52, 907–916.
collective identity and self representations. Journal of Person- Culler, J. (1976). Saussure. Glasgow, Scotland: Fontana.
ality and Social Psychology, 71, 83–93. Damon, W., & Hart, D. (1988). Self-understanding in childhood and
Brewer, M. B., Manzi, J. M., & Shaw, J. S. (1993). In-group identifi- adolescence. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University
cation as a function of depersonalization, distinctiveness, and Press.
status. Psychological Science, 4, 88–92. de Silva, P. (1993). Buddhist psychology: A therapeutic perspective.
Brewer, M. B., & Pickett, C. L. (1999). Distinctiveness motives as a In U. Kim & J. W. Berry (Eds.), Indigenous psychologies: Re-
source of the social self. In T. R. Tyler, R. M. Kramer, & O. P. search and experience in cultural context (pp. 221–239).
John (Eds.), The psychology of the social self (pp. 71–87). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Deaux, K. (1992). Personalizing identity and socializing self. In G.
Brewer, M. B., & Weber, J. G. (1994). Self-evaluation effects of in- M. Breakwell (Ed.), Social psychology of identity and the self
terpersonal versus intergroup social comparison. Journal of concept (pp. 9–33). London: Surrey University Press.
Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 268–275. Deaux, K., Reid, A., Mizrahi, K., & Ethier, K. A. (1995). Parameters
Brickman, P., & Bulman, R. J. (1977). Pleasure and pain in social of social identity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
comparison. In J. M. Suls & R. L. Miller (Eds.), Social compari- ogy, 68, 280–291.
son processes: Theoretical and empirical perspectives (pp. Dumont, L. (1986). Essays on individualism: Modern ideology in an-
149–186). New York: Wiley. thropological perspective. Chicago: University of Chicago
Campbell, J. D. (1986). Similarity and uniqueness: The effects of at- Press.
tribute type, relevance, and individual differences in self-esteem Durkheim, E. (1915). The elementary forms of the religious life (J. W.
and depression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Swain, Trans.). London: Allen & Unwin. (Original work pub-
50, 281–294. lished 1912)
Chandler, M. J., & Lalonde, C. E. (1995). The problem of self-conti- Durkheim, E. (1969). Individualism and the intellectuals (S. Lukes,
nuity in the context of rapid personal and cultural change. In A. Trans.). Political Studies, 17, 14–30. (Original work published
Oosterwegel & R. A. Wicklund (Eds.), The self in European and 1898)
North American culture: Development and processes (pp. Enriquez, V. G. (1993). Developing a Filipino psychology. In U. Kim
45–63). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer. & J. W. Berry (Eds.), Indigenous psychologies: Research and
Chang, W. C., & Lee, L. (2000). Multifaceted Chinese selfhood. experience in cultural context (pp. 152–169). Newbury Park,
Manuscript submitted for publication. CA: Sage.

351
VIGNOLES, CHRYSSOCHOOU, & BREAKWELL

Erikson, E. H. (1959). Identity and the life cycle. Psychological Is- Hermans, H. J. M., & Kempen, H. J. G. (1998). Moving cultures: The
sues, 1, 1–171. perilous problems of cultural dichotomies in a globalizing soci-
Farr, R. M. (1991). Individualism as a collective representation. In V. ety. American Psychologist, 53, 1111–1120.
Aebischer, J. P. Deconchy, & E. M. Lipiansky (Eds.), Hinkle, S., & Brown, R. (1990). Intergroup comparisons and social
Idéologies et représentations sociales (pp. 129–143). Paris: Del identity: Some links and lacunae. In D. Abrams & M. A. Hogg
Val. (Eds.), Social identity theory: Constructive and critical ad-
Farr, R. M. (1996). The roots of modern social psychology. Oxford, vances (pp. 48–70). London: Harvester Wheatsheaf.
England: Blackwell. Ho, D. Y. F. (1993). Relational orientation in Asian social psychol-
Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Hu- ogy. In U. Kim & J. W. Berry (Eds.), Indigenous psychologies:
man Relations, 7, 117–140. Research and experience in cultural context (pp. 240–259).
Fiske, A. P., Kitayama, S., Markus, H. R., & Nisbett, R. E. (1998). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
The cultural matrix of social psychology. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Ho, D. Y. F. (1995). Selfhood and identity in Confucianism, Taoism,
Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology Buddhism, and Hinduism: Contrasts with the West. Journal for
(4th ed., Vol. 2, pp. 915–981). Boston: McGraw-Hill. the Theory of Social Behaviour, 25, 115–139.
Fromkin, H. L. (1970). Effects of experimentally aroused feelings of Hoelter, J. (1983). The effects of role evaluation and commitment on
indistinctiveness upon valuation of scarce and novel experi- identity salience. Social Psychology Quarterly, 46, 140–147.
ences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 16, Hogg, M. A., & Abrams, D. (1993). Towards a single-process uncer-
521–529. tainty-reduction model of social motivation in groups. In M. A.
Fromkin, H. L. (1972). Feelings of interpersonal undistinctiveness: Hogg & D. Abrams (Eds.), Group motivation: Social psycho-
An unpleasant affective state. Journal of Experimental Re- logical perspectives (pp. 173–190). London: Harvester
search in Personality, 6, 178–182. Wheatsheaf.
Fromm, E. (1942). The fear of freedom. London: Routledge & Kegan Holland, D., & Kipnis, A. (1994). Metaphors for embarrassment and
Paul. stories of exposure: The not-so-egocentric self in American cul-
Gao, G. (1996). Self and OTHER: A Chinese perspective on interper- ture. Ethos, 22, 316–342.
sonal relationships. In W. B. Gudykunst, S. Ting-Toomey, & T. Hong, Y. Y., Chiu, C. Y., & Kung, T. M. (1997). Bringing culture out
Nishida (Eds.), Communication in personal relationships in front: Effects of cultural meaning system activation on social
across cultures (pp. 81–101). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. cognition. In K. Leung, Y. Kashima, U. Kim, & S. Yamaguchi
Gardner, W. L., Gabriel, S., & Lee, A. Y. (1999). “I” value freedom, (Eds.), Progress in Asian social psychology (Vol. 1, pp.
but “we” value relationships: Self-construal priming mirrors 135–146). Singapore: Wiley.
cultural differences in judgment. Psychological Science, 10, Hong, Y. Y., Morris, M. W., Chiu, C. Y., & Benet-Martínez, V.
321–326. (1999). Multicultural minds: A dynamic constructivist ap-
Geertz, C. (1975). On the nature of anthropological understanding. proach to culture and cognition. Manuscript submitted for pub-
American Scientist, 63, 47–53. lication.
Grieve, P. G., & Hogg, M. A. (1999). Subjective uncertainty and in- Hornsey, M. J., & Hogg, M. A. (1999). Subgroup differentiation as a
tergroup discrimination in the minimal group situation. Person- response to an overly-inclusive group: A test of optimal distinc-
ality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 926–940. tiveness theory. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29,
Gruder, C. L. (1977). Choice of comparison persons in evaluating 543–550.
oneself. In J. M. Suls & R. L. Miller (Eds.), Social comparison Hsu, F. L. K. (1985). The self in cross-cultural perspective. In A. J.
processes: Theoretical and empirical perspectives (pp. 21–41). Marsella, G. DeVos, & F. L. K. Hsu (Eds.), Culture and self:
New York: Wiley. Asian and Western perspectives (pp. 24–55). New York:
Gudykunst, W. B., Matsumoto, Y., Ting-Toomey, S., Nishida, T., Tavistock.
Kim, K., & Heyman, S. (1996). The influence of cultural indi- Ichheiser, G. (1949). Misunderstandings in human relations: A study
vidualism–collectivism, self construals, and individual values in false social perception. American Journal of Sociology, 55 (2,
on communication styles across cultures. Human Communica- Pt. 2).
tion Research, 22, 510–543. Jetten, J., Spears, R., & Manstead, A. S. R. (1999). Group distinctive-
Hakmiller, K. L. (1966). Threat as a determinant of downward com- ness and intergroup discrimination. In N. Ellemers, R. Spears, &
parison. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 2(Suppl. B. Doosje (Eds.), Social identity: Context, commitment, content
1), 32–39. (pp. 107–126). Oxford, England: Blackwell.
Hamaguchi, E. (1985). A contextual model of the Japanese: Toward a Judd, C. M., Ryan, C. S., & Park, B. (1991). Accuracy in the judg-
methodological innovation in Japan studies. Journal of Japa- ment of in-group and out-group variability. Journal of Person-
nese Studies, 11, 289–321. ality and Social Psychology, 61, 366–379.
Haslam, S. A., Oakes, P. J., Turner, J. C., & McGarty, C. (1995). So- Kâgitçibasi, Ç. (1994). A critical appraisal of individualism and col-
cial categorization and group homogeneity: Changes in the per- lectivism: Toward a new formulation. In U. Kim, H. C. Triandis,
ceived applicability of stereotype content as a function of com- Ç. Kâgitçibasi, S. C. Choi, & G. Yoon (Eds.), Individualism and
parative context and trait favourableness. British Journal of collectivism: Theory, method, and applications (pp. 52–65).
Social Psychology, 34, 139–160. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Heine, S. J., & Lehman, D. R. (1997). The cultural construction of Kim, U. (1994). Individualism and collectivism: Conceptual clarifi-
self-enhancement: An examination of group-serving biases. cation and elaboration. In U. Kim, H. C. Triandis, Ç.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 1268–1283. Kâgitçibasi, S. C. Choi, & G. Yoon (Eds.), Individualism and
Heine, S. J., Lehman, D. R., Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1999). Is collectivism: Theory, method, and applications (pp. 19–40).
there a universal need for positive self-regard? Psychological Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Review, 106, 766–794. Kim, U., & Berry, J. W. (Eds.). (1993). Indigenous psychologies: Re-
Henderson-King, E., Henderson-King, D., Zhermer, N., Posokhova, search and experience in cultural context. Newbury Park, CA:
S., & Chiker, V. (1997). In-group favoritism and perceived sim- Sage.
ilarity: A look at Russians’ perceptions in the post-Soviet era. Kuhn, M. H., & McPartland, T. S. (1954). An empirical investigation
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 1013–1021. of self-attitudes. American Sociological Review, 19, 68–76.

352
THE DISTINCTIVENESS PRINCIPLE

Lebra, T. S. (1976). Japanese patterns of behavior. Honolulu: Uni- Moscovici, S. (1984). The phenomenon of social representations. In
versity of Hawaii Press. R. M. Farr & S. Moscovici (Eds.), Social representations (pp.
Lebra, T. S. (1992). Self in Japanese culture. In N. R. Rosenberger 3–69). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
(Ed.), Japanese sense of self (pp. 105–120). Cambridge, Eng- Mullin, B. A., & Hogg, M. A. (1998). Dimensions of subjective un-
land: Cambridge University Press. certainty in social identification and minimal intergroup dis-
Lee, Y. T., & Ottati, V. (1995). Perceived in-group homogeneity as a crimination. British Journal of Social Psychology, 37, 345–365.
function of group membership salience and stereotype threat. Mullin, B. A., & Hogg, M. A. (1999). Motivations for group member-
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 610–619. ship: The role of subjective importance and uncertainty reduc-
Lemaine, G. (1974). Social differentiation and social originality. Eu- tion. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 21, 91–102.
ropean Journal of Social Psychology, 4, 17–52. Niedenthal, P. M., & Beike, D. R. (1997). Interrelated and isolated self-con-
Lemaine, G., Kastersztein, J., & Personnaz, B. (1978). Social differ- cepts. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 1, 106–128.
entiation. In H. Tajfel (Ed.), Differentiation between social Oakes, P. J., Haslam, S. A., & Turner, J. C. (1994). Stereotyping and
groups: Studies in the social psychology of intergroup relations social reality. Oxford, England: Blackwell.
(pp. 269–300). London: Academic. Orme-Johnson, D. W., Zimmerman, E., & Hawkins, M. (1997).
Lewis, M., & Brooks-Gunn, I. (1979). Social cognition and the ac- Maharishi’s Vedic psychology: The science of the cosmic psy-
quisition of self. New York: Plenum. che. In H. S. R. Kao & D. Sinha (Eds.), Asian perspectives on
Leyens, J. P., Yzerbyt, V. Y., & Rogier, A. (1997). Personality traits psychology (pp. 282–308). New Delhi, India: Sage.
that distinguish you and me are better memorized. European Park, B., & Rothbart, M. (1982). Perception of out-group homogene-
Journal of Social Psychology, 27, 511–522. ity and levels of social categorization: Memory for the subordi-
Lomnitz, L. A. (1977). Networks and marginality: Life in a Mexican nate attributes of in-group and out-group members. Journal of
shantytown (C. Lomnitz, Trans.). London: Academic. (Original Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 1051–1068.
work published 1976) Peng, K., & Nisbett, R. E. (1999). Culture, dialectics, and reasoning
Long, K., & Spears, R. (1997). The self-esteem hypothesis revisited: about contradiction. American Psychologist, 54, 741–754.
Differentiation and the disaffected. In R. Spears, P. J. Oakes, N. Realo, A., Allik, J., & Vadi, M. (1997). The hierarchical structure of
Ellemers, & S. A. Haslam (Eds.), The social psychology of ste- collectivism. Journal of Research in Personality, 31, 93–116.
reotyping and group life (pp. 296–317). Oxford, England: Reber, A. S. (1985). Dictionary of psychology. London: Penguin.
Blackwell. Reicher, S. (1995). Three dimensions of the social self. In A.
Lorenzi-Cioldi, F. (1995). The self in collection and aggregate Oosterwegel & R. A. Wicklund (Eds.), The self in European and
groups. In I. Lubek, R. van Hezewijk, G. Pheterson, & C. North American culture: Development and processes (pp.
Tolman (Eds.), Trends and issues in theoretical psychology (pp. 277–290). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer.
46–52). New York: Springer. Reid, A., & Deaux, K. (1996). Relationship between social and per-
Lukes, S. (1973). Individualism. Oxford, England: Blackwell. sonal identities: Segregation or integration? Journal of Person-
Ma, V., & Schoeneman, T. J. (1997). Individualism versus collectiv- ality and Social Psychology, 71, 1084–1091.
ism: A comparison of Kenyan and American self-concepts. Ba- Rommetveit, R. (1990). On axiomatic features of a dialogical approach
sic and Applied Social Psychology, 19, 261–273. to language and mind. In I. Marková & K. Foppa (Eds.), The dy-
Macdonald, S. (1997). Reimagining culture: Histories, identities and namics of dialogue (pp. 83–104). London: Harvester Wheatsheaf.
the Gaelic renaissance. Oxford, England: Berg. Ross, L. (1977). The intuitive psychologist and his shortcomings. In
Macfarlane, A. (1978). The origins of English individualism. Oxford, L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychol-
England: Blackwell. ogy (Vol. 10, pp. 173–220). New York: Academic.
Marková, I. (1987). Knowledge of the self through interaction. In K. Rubin, M., & Hewstone, M. (1998). Social identity theory’s self-es-
Yardley & T. Honess (Eds.), Self and identity: Psychosocial teem hypothesis: A review and some suggestions for clarifica-
perspectives (pp. 65–80). New York: Wiley. tion. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2, 40–62.
Marková, I. (1997). The individual and the community: A post-Com- Sampson, E. E. (1977). Psychology and the American ideal. Journal
munist perspective. Journal of Community and Applied Social of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 767–782.
Psychology, 7, 3–17. Sampson, E. E. (1978). Scientific paradigms and social values:
Marková, I., Moodie, E., Farr, R. M., Drozda-Senkowska, E., Erös, Wanted—A scientific revolution. Journal of Personality and
F., Plichtová, J., Gervais, M. C., Hoffmannová, J., & Mullerová, Social Psychology, 36, 1332–1343.
O. (1998). Social representations of the individual: A Schwartz, J. M., & Smith, W. P. (1976). Social comparison and the
post-Communist perspective. European Journal of Social Psy- inference of ability difference. Journal of Personality and So-
chology, 28, 797–829. cial Psychology, 34, 1268–1275.
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implica- Schwartz, S. H. (1994). Beyond individualism/collectivism: New
tions for cognition, emotion and motivation. Psychological Re- cultural dimensions of values. In U. Kim, H. C. Triandis, Ç.
view, 98, 224–253. Kâgitçibasi, S. C. Choi, & G. Yoon (Eds.), Individualism and
Maslach, C. (1974). Social and personal bases of individuation. Jour- collectivism: Theory, method, and applications (pp. 85–119).
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 29, 411–425. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Miller, J. G. (1984). Culture and the development of everyday social Schwartz, S. H., & Bilsky, W. (1990). Toward a theory of the universal
explanation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, content and structure of values: Extensions and cross-cultural repli-
961–978. cations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 878–891.
Millward, L. J. (1995). Contextualizing social identity in consider- Shweder, R. A., & Bourne, E. J. (1984). Does the concept of the per-
ations of what it means to be a nurse. European Journal of Social son vary cross-culturally? In R. A. Shweder & R. A. LeVine
Psychology, 25, 303–324. (Eds.), Culture theory: Essays on mind, self, and emotion (pp.
Mlicki, P. P., & Ellemers, N. (1996). Being different or being better? 158–199). New York: Cambridge University Press.
National stereotypes and identifications of Polish and Dutch Simon, B. (1997). Self and group in modern society: Ten theses on
students. European Journal of Social Psychology, 26, 97–114. the individual self and the collective self. In R. Spears, P. J.
Morris, M. W., & Peng, K. (1994). Culture and cause: American and Oakes, N. Ellemers, & S. A. Haslam (Eds.), The social psychol-
Chinese attributions for social and physical events. Journal of ogy of stereotyping and group life (pp. 318–335). Oxford, Eng-
Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 949–971. land: Blackwell.

353
VIGNOLES, CHRYSSOCHOOU, & BREAKWELL

Simon, B., & Brown, R. (1987). Perceived intragroup homogeneity Triandis, H. C. (1995). Individualism and collectivism. Boulder, CO:
in minority–majority contexts. Journal of Personality and So- Westview.
cial Psychology, 53, 703–711. Triandis, H. C., Chan, D. K. S., Bhawuk, D. P. S., Iwao, S., & Sinha, J.
Simon, B., Hastedt, C., & Aufderheide, B. (1997). When self-catego- B. P. (1995). Multimethod probes of allocentrism and
rization makes sense: The role of meaningful social categoriza- idiocentrism. International Journal of Psychology, 30,
tion in minority and majority members’ self-perception. Jour- 461–480.
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 310–320. Triandis, H. C., McCusker, C., & Hui, C. H. (1990). Multimethod
Simon, L., Greenberg, J., Arndt, J., Pyszczynski, T., Clement, R., & probes of individualism and collectivism. Journal of Personal-
Solomon, S. (1997). Perceived consensus, uniqueness, and ter- ity and Social Psychology, 59, 1006–1020.
ror management: Compensatory responses to threats to inclu- Turner, J. C. (1975). Social comparison and social identity: Some
sion and distinctiveness following mortality salience. Personal- prospects for intergroup behaviour. European Journal of Social
ity and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 1055–1065. Psychology, 5, 5–34.
Sinha, D., & Tripathi, R. C. (1994). Individualism in a collectivist Turner, J. C. (1987). A self-categorization theory. In J. C. Turner, M.
culture: A case of coexistence of opposites. In U. Kim, H. C. A. Hogg, P. J. Oakes, S. D. Reicher, & M. S. Wetherell (Eds.),
Triandis, Ç. Kâgitçibasi, S. C. Choi, & G. Yoon (Eds.), Individ- Rediscovering the social group; A self-categorization theory
ualism and collectivism: Theory, method, and applications (pp. (pp. 42–67). Oxford, England: Blackwell.
123–136). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Turner, J. C. (1999). Some current issues in research on social iden-
Smith, P. B., & Bond, M. H. (1993). Social psychology across cul- tity and self-categorization theories. In N. Ellemers, R. Spears,
tures: Analysis and perspectives. London: Harvester & B. Doosje (Eds.), Social identity: Context, commitment, con-
Wheatsheaf. tent (pp. 6–34). Oxford, England: Blackwell.
Snyder, C. R., & Fromkin, H. L. (1980). Uniqueness: The human pur- Vignoles, V. L. (2000). Identity, culture and the distinctiveness prin-
suit of difference. New York: Plenum. ciple. Manuscript in preparation.
Speller, G., Lyons, E., & Twigger-Ross, C. (1999, September). Vignoles, V. L., Chryssochoou, X., & Breakwell, G. M. (1999, July).
Semi-detached bliss? A study of cultural transition as a tradi- Distinctiveness, identity and individualism among Anglican
tional mining community is relocated. Paper presented at the parish priests. Poster session presented at the 12th General
Annual Conference of the British Psychological Society Social Meeting of the European Association of Experimental Social
Psychology Section, Lancaster University, England. Psychology, Oxford, England.
Stern, D. N. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant: A view Vignoles, V. L., Chryssochoou, X., & Breakwell, G. M. (2000). Eval-
from psychoanalysis and developmental psychology. New uating models of identity motivation: Self-esteem is not the
York: Basic Books. whole story. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Stryker, S. (1987). Identity theory: Developments and extensions. In Wheeler, L. (1966). Motivation as a determinant of upward compari-
K. Yardley & T. Honess (Eds.), Self and identity: Psychosocial son. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 2(Suppl. 1),
perspectives (pp. 89–103). New York: Wiley. 27–31.
Tajfel, H. (1981). Human groups and social categories: Studies in social White, G. M., & Kirkpatrick, J. (Eds.). (1985). Person, self, and expe-
psychology. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. rience: Exploring Pacific ethnopsychologies. Berkeley: Uni-
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1986). The social identity theory of inter- versity of California Press.
group behaviour. In S. Worchel & W. G. Austin (Eds.), Psychol- Wills, T. A. (1991). Similarity and self-esteem in downward compar-
ogy of intergroup relations (2nd ed., pp. 7–24). Chicago: Nel- ison. In J. Suls & T. A. Wills (Eds.), Social comparison: Con-
son-Hall. temporary theory and research (pp. 51–78). Hillsdale, NJ: Law-
Takata, T. (1987). Self-deprecative tendencies in self-evaluation rence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
through social comparison. Japanese Journal of Experimental Yamaguchi, S., Kuhlman, D. M., & Sugimori, S. (1995). Personality
Social Psychology, 27, 27–36. correlates of allocentric tendencies in individualist and collec-
Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-being: A so- tivist cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 26,
cial psychological perspective on mental health. Psychological 658–672.
Bulletin, 103, 193–210. Yang, K. S. (1997). Theories and research in Chinese personality: An
Thompson, S. C., Kohles, J. C., Otsuki, T. A., & Kent, D. R. (1997). indigenous approach. In H. S. R. Kao & D. Sinha (Eds.), Asian
Perceptions of attitudinal similarity in ethnic groups in the U.S.: perspectives on psychology (pp. 236–262). New Delhi, India:
Ingroup and outgroup homogeneity effects. European Journal Sage.
of Social Psychology, 27, 209–220. Yuki, M., & Brewer, M. B. (1999, August). Japanese collectivism
Trafimow, D., Triandis, H. C., & Goto, S. G. (1991). Some tests of the versus American collectivism: A comparison of group-loyalty
distinction between the private self and the collective self. Jour- processes across cultures. Paper presented at the 3rd Confer-
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 649–655. ence of the Asian Association of Social Psychology, Taipei,
Triandis, H. C. (1989). The self and social behavior in differing cul- Taiwan.
tural contexts. Psychological Review, 96, 506–520. Ziller, R. C. (1964). Individuation and socialization: A theory of as-
Triandis, H. C. (1993). Collectivism and individualism as cultural similation in large organizations. Human Relations, 17,
syndromes. Cross-Cultural Research, 27, 155–180. 341–360.

354

You might also like