You are on page 1of 9

The Sounds of Silence: Language, Cognition,

and Anxiety in Selective Mutism


KATHARINA MANASSIS, M.D., ROSEMARY TANNOCK, PH.D., E. JANE GARLAND, M.D.,
KLAUS MINDE, M.D., ALISON McINNES, PH.D., AND SANDRA CLARK, PH.D.

ABSTRACT
Objectives: To determine whether oral language, working memory, and social anxiety differentiate children with selective
mutism (SM), children with anxiety disorders (ANX), and normal controls (NCs) and explore predictors of mutism severity.
Method: Children ages 6 to 10 years with SM (n = 44) were compared with children with ANX (n = 28) and NCs (n = 19) of
similar age on standardized measures of language, nonverbal working memory, and social anxiety. Variables correlating
with mutism severity were entered in stepwise regressions to determine predictors of mute behavior in SM. Results:
Children with SM scored significantly lower on standardized language measures than children with ANX and NCs and
showed greater visual memory deficits and social anxiety relative to these two groups. Age and receptive grammar ability
predicted less severe mutism, whereas social anxiety predicted more severe mutism. These factors accounted for 38% of
the variance in mutism severity. Conclusions: Social anxiety and language deficits are evident in SM, may predict mutism
severity, and should be evaluated in clinical assessment. Replication is indicated, as are further studies of cognition and of
intervention in SM, using large, diverse samples. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry, 2007;46(9):1187Y1195. Key
Words: selective mutism, anxiety disorders, social anxiety, language impairment, working memory.

Selective mutism (SM) is an intriguing condition found in McInnes and Manassis, 2005), without reference to
at the interface between child psychopathology and an etiological understanding of SM. Improving this
disorders of speech and language. SM occurs when understanding could lead to empirically based, poten-
children fail to speak in certain social environments tially effective treatments.
(often at school) despite speaking in others (usually Two main theoretical accounts have been proposed
home) (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). for important etiological factors in children with SM
Although considered relatively uncommon (Bergman who are being schooled in their native language:
et al., 2002), this condition can be debilitating both anxiety, especially social anxiety, and language impair-
socially and academically and shows a high persistence ment or broader developmental delays (Kristensen,
over time (Steinhausen and Juzi, 1996). Treatment 2000). Recent findings also suggest a deficit in the
strategies have been developed pragmatically (reviewed auditory efferent system as an etiological factor (Bar-
Haim et al., 2004). It is postulated that this deficit
prevents children from desensitizing to their own
vocalizations and anxious children cope with this
Accepted March 23, 2007. problem through selective mutism.
Drs. Manassis and Tannock are with The Hospital for Sick Children and the
University of Toronto; Drs. Garland and Clark are with the University of Other theories are proposed for children who are
British Columbia, Vancouver; Dr. Minde is with McGill University, Montreal; recent immigrants and may be particularly vulnerable to
and Dr. McInnes is with the University of Windsor, Windsor, Ontario, Canada. SM (Bradley and Sloman, 1975; Elizur and Perednik,
This work was supported by a grant from the Ontario Mental Health
Foundation.
2003; Toppelberg et al., 2005), but these children have
Reprint requests to Dr. Katharina Manassis, Department of Psychiatry, The been the subject of little research. Elizur and Perednik
Hospital for Sick Children, 555 University Avenue, Toronto, Ontario, M5G (2003), however, reported that children of recent
1X8, Canada; e-mail: katharina.manassis@sickkids.ca.
immigrants with SM had higher levels of social anxiety
0890-8567/07/4609-1187Ó2007 by the American Academy of Child
and Adolescent Psychiatry. and lower rates of neurodevelopmental problems than
DOI: 10.1097/chi.0b013e318076b7ab children with SM from nonimmigrant families

J. AM. ACAD. CHILD ADOLESC. PSYCHIATRY, 46:9, SEPTEMBER 2007 1187

Copyright @ 2007 American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
MANASSIS ET AL.

(N = 19). Recently immigrated non-English speakers physician referred and from all socioeconomic classes, with some
overrepresentation of more affluent relative to less affluent families
were not included in the present study because our
(mean Hollingshead level II; minor professional) and of white
language measures would not be applicable to them. families relative to other racial groups. There were no group
Our group designed a unique protocol to system- differences in socioeconomic status or racial composition.
atically assess the language and learning abilities of The sample included 44 children with SM (32 girls, 12 boys), 28
children with Axis I anxiety disorders (ANX; 13 girls, 15 boys; 17
children with SM as well as their anxiety (Manassis generalized anxiety disorder, 7 social phobia, 4 separation anxiety
et al., 2003). Due to limited verbal expression in SM, disorder), 19 normal controls (NCs; 14 female, 5 male). There was a
assessing such factors can be challenging. In a previous trend toward a group gender difference on #2 ( p = .05; female
preponderance in mute and normal groups).
pilot study comparing a small group of children with
SM with socially phobic children of similar age on this
protocol, we found greater language impairment in SM, Inclusion Criteria for SM
impairment in SM relative to normative scores on a task
Children ages 6 to 10 with a primary diagnosis of SM on
requiring nonverbal working memory (NVWM), and structured parent and (where possible) child interviews (Anxiety
comparable levels of anxiety in the SM and socially Disorders Interview Schedule [ADIS; Silverman and Albano,
phobic groups (Manassis et al., 2003; McInnes et al., 1996]), where primary was defined as the diagnosis associated
with the greatest impairment. All of the sites had already been
2004). The present study sought to replicate and trained on this interview and been using it routinely for several
expand these findings by studying a larger group of years. All of the interviewers were experienced child and adolescent
children with SM and including both normal and psychiatrists or child and adolescent psychologists. The duration of
anxious comparison groups. To do so, we included the mutism had to be at least 1 month, but not the first month of
school. Children with comorbid conditions were included in the
academic centers in three metropolitan sites (Toronto, study as long as the conditions did not account for the mutism.
Vancouver, and Montreal). No children from the pilot Comparison children had to have a primary diagnosis of ANX on
study were included in the present report. ADIS and not have SM or (in NCs) did not meet criteria for any
Axis I disorder on this interview. NCs were recruited through
We further sought to determine whether oral advertisements in the same geographic areas as subjects, and a
language, working memory, and social anxiety could subsequent snowball technique (i.e., all of the control subjects were
predict mutism severity. If so, then group differences asked to name a friend who may be interested in participating).
could be related to clinical impairment in SM and
intervention in these areas may be helpful for affected
Exclusion Criteria
children. Our study is unique in that no previous
studies of SM have systematically examined these three Children were excluded from the study if they were not primarily
English speaking because we could administer measures only in
factors within the same sample. English, and testing in the child`s nonnative tongue may confound
Hypotheses included children with SM will have language results. We required a minimum of 2 years in an English
language deficits relative to nonmute anxious children school or daycare setting to consider them primarily English
speaking. Our Montreal site was located in a predominantly
and controls without a psychiatric disorder, children English-speaking area of the city. There were other languages
with SM will have NVWM deficits relative to nonmute spoken in some homes in addition to English, including Cantonese
anxious children and controls without a psychiatric (8.3%), Portuguese (4.7%), Spanish (4.2%), and Tagalog (2.3%).
disorder, children with SM and nonmute anxious The proportion of homes where other languages were spoken did
not differ significantly between groups.
children will have higher levels of social anxiety than Children with an abnormal audiometric screen at 500, 2,000,
controls without a psychiatric disorder, and language and 4,000 Hz (hearing impairment can contribute to mutism); with
ability, NVWM scores, and social anxiety level will mutism related to trauma, major depression, psychosis, or a
neurological condition; with nonverbal intelligence scores <80 on
predict children`s degree of mutism as reported by the WISC-III; or children who were unable to complete measures
parents on a standardized inventory. were excluded. Children already taking serotonin reuptake
inhibitors or other psychotropic medication were also excluded
because these medications may affect some of the cognitive measures
METHOD administered.
In practice, exclusions occurred only for children with SM: two
were already taking serotonergic medication and three were unable
Sample
to complete key measures (unresponsive to assessor, even with
After obtaining approval from research ethics boards at all of the nonverbal administration). Numerous non-English speakers with
participating sites, the sample was recruited through specialized SM were screened out at intake, so we do not have exact data on
anxiety disorders clinics in three urban areas. Children were these individuals.

1188 J. AM . ACAD. CHILD ADOLESC. PSYCH IATRY, 46:9, SE PTE MBER 2007

Copyright @ 2007 American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
SELECTIVE MUTISM AND LANGUAGE

Sample Size The latter measure, the Visual Patterns Test (Della Sala et al.,
1997), requires the subject to memorize a series of black and white
Because this is the first study comparing anxiety, language, and squares in grids of varying sizes. The subject then attempts to
cognition in these diagnostic groups, we were interested in large reproduce each pattern by coloring a blank grid. The test does not
effects that would merit further investigation and clinical attention. require the visual locations to be sequenced (as in the Corsi Blocks
To detect a large effect ( f = 0.40), a minimum of 21 participants Forward and Backward Test) and thus provides a purer test of visual
per group was needed to maintain a power of 0.80 and ! of .05 memory. Good test-retest reliability has been shown (0.73Y0.75).
(Cohen, 1988). More children in the clinical groups than in the Distinction from the Corsi Blocks Forward and Backward Test in
control group were seen, however, likely reflecting their families` both normal and clinical subjects has been demonstrated, suggesting
high motivation to access our centers. it is a measure of visual as opposed to spatial nonverbal short-term
memory.
Anxiety. The ADIS (Silverman and Albano, 1996) is a
Measures semistructured diagnostic interview focusing on childhood anxiety
After families provided informed consent, an assessment visit was disorders as described in DSM-IV, but screening for all of the
scheduled for study participation. In most cases, all of the measures major child psychiatric diagnoses. It includes clinician severity
and diagnostic interviews were completed in 1 day, but some ratings, reflecting the impact of the disorder on the child`s life.
children required a second visit to complete measures due to fatigue. It has shown high concurrent validity with established anxiety
Measure administration was counterbalanced to minimize order measures such as the Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Children
effects and selection of measures considered both psychometric (MASC; Rynn et al., 2006) and is used extensively in research
quality (well-validated, reliable measures preferred) and the need for on childhood ANX. The ADIS was attempted with children
the measure to be completed without requiring verbal responses. and parents, but given the nature of SM, most information was
We attempted to keep measure administrators blind to group parent derived.
assignment, but this was often not possible with SM, given the Reports from two informants were obtained for each ques-
obvious lack of oral communication. tionnaire measure because correspondence between informants is
Language. The Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-III (PPVT-III; usually only fair for internalizing psychopathology (reviewed in
Dunn and Dunn, 1997) is an extensively norm-based measure that Barbosa et al., 2002). The questions were read aloud to the child by
estimates children`s receptive vocabulary skills. The child is asked to the examiner if the child needed help with reading, and children
point to one of four pictures that matches a spoken word. were allowed to point to the response. Children in all age groups
The Lindamood Auditory Conceptualization Test (LACT; under 8 years generally required help with reading. Data from the
Lindamood and Lindamood, 1971) is used to assess children`s two informants were analyzed separately for both measures.
phonemic awareness ability, which is a strong predictor of later The Social Anxiety Scale for Children-R (SASC, parent and
reading achievement. Two related abilities (one task each) are tested: child; La Greca and Stone, 1993) was included because SM has been
the ability to discriminate one speech sound from another and the linked most closely to social anxiety. The SASC consists of 22 items
ability to perceive and compare the number and order of sounds using a 5-point Likert scale (from not at all to all of the time)
within spoken patterns. Children respond by selecting and assessing children`s anxiety in relation to peers and social situations.
manipulating colored blocks that represent sounds and sound The items are summed to yield three subscales: Fear of Negative
patterns. Converted scores are calculated for each task, but not Evaluation, Social Anxiety Specific, and Social Anxiety General.
scaled or standardized for age. The LACT has high test-retest Acceptable concurrent validity, internal consistencies (0.60Y0.90)
reliability (0.96) in the age group of interest and is highly predictive and test-retest reliability over 4 months (0.70) have been established
of standardized reading and spelling scores. (La Greca and Stone, 1993).
The Test of Reception of Grammar (TROG; Bishop, 2003) is a The Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Children (MASC, parent
well-validated test of grammar that requires a pointing response to and child; March, 1997) is a 39-item, 4-point Likert scale to assess a
select which of four pictures best represents a spoken sentence. For broad spectrum of anxiety symptoms in children. Four robust factors
example, the child is asked to choose which of four pictures best have been found by factor analysis: physical symptoms, social anxiety,
matches the sentence BThey are sitting on the table.^ The pictures harm avoidance, and separation anxiety. High internal reliability
include two children standing at a table, two children sitting on the (itemYtotal correlations of 0.4Y0.8) and test-retest reliability at 3
floor (lexical distractors), one child sitting on a table (grammatical weeks and 3 months have been demonstrated. Convergent validity
distractor), and two children sitting on a table (correct answer). The specific to other anxiety measures (versus measures of depression or
TROG has been standardized on 2000 British children ages 4 to 12 externalizing behavior) has been established. Parents completed the
years. It has demonstrated good discriminant validity in the age measure as they understood their child`s symptoms (consistent with
group of interest, concurrent validity with measures of similar March, 1997), but results were analyzed separately by informant
linguistic concepts, and test-retest reliability (0.74). because previous studies have found only marginally significant
NVWM. To allow a more detailed yet not overly taxing parentYchild concordance (March, 1997).
examination of NVWM, we administered the spatial span and spatial The Selective Mutism Questionnaire (SMQ; Bergman et al.,
working memory subtests from the WISC-III-PI (also known as Corsi 2001) was also included. This is a specific parent- and teacher-report
Blocks Forward and Backward Test). This measure provides scaled measure of mutism severity. Parents or teachers report the child`s
scores for both subtests, for ease of comparison to normative samples, speaking behaviors and difficulties associated with lack of speech in
and was the main NVWM measure examined in the analyses. three domains (at school, at home/with family, and in public) on a 4-
The NVWM measure from our preliminary study (finger point rating scale. They indicate the speaking behavior occurred
windows forward and backward [Manassis et al., 2003]) was never/not at all to always/extremely in a given domain (i.e., lower
repeated for replication and supplemented with a test of pure visual scores indicated less speech). The three factors have satisfactory
memory to distinguish this function from NVWM. internal consistency and significant correlations with corresponding

J. AM. ACAD. CHILD ADOLESC. PSYCHIATRY, 46:9, SEPTEMBER 2007 1189

Copyright @ 2007 American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
MANASSIS ET AL.

interference items, and the sum of all factors correlates significantly more poorly compared to children with ANX and NCs
with overall interference rating (Bergman et al., 2001). Cronbach ! ( p = .001 for PPVT post hoc comparisons; p < .05 for
values in our preliminary study were SMQ (total) = .87, SMQ
(school) = .85, SMQ (home) = .76, and SMQ (public) = .82. LACT and TROG post hoc comparisons). The
Maternal report was most consistently available in this sample, and percentage of children with SM with clinically sig-
thus it was used in all of the analyses. nificant scores (i.e., >1 SD below the mean) was also
Data were analyzed using the Statistical Package for the Social
Sciences (SPSS, version 12). Groups were compared using analyses greater than for the ANX and NC groups on all three
of covariance for continuous measures (see below) and # 2 for measures (PPVT: 18%, 10%, 12%, respectively; LACT
categorical measures. (total): 34%, 20%, 12%, respectively; TROG: 43%,
20%; 12%, respectively) on # 2 analyses.
RESULTS To test the second hypothesis concerning NVWM
Group Differences in Comorbidity, Severity, and Age
deficits in children with SM relative to children with
ANX and NCs, measures of nonverbal function were
Twenty-seven of the children with SM also met
compared across groups using analyses of variance
criteria for social phobia, and these children had more
(Table 1). Analysis of covariance was used (controlling
severe social anxiety by maternal report (t = 2.94, for age) for Backward Finger Windows, the only
p < .01), but not self-report, than those without social measure that is not age standardized. Post hoc Tukey
phobia. Additional comorbidities in this group included tests for measures where groups differed significantly
oppositional defiant disorder (three children) and specific revealed that children with SM had deficits relative to
phobia (one child). Seven children in the anxiety group both children with ANX and NCs on the visual
had more than one disorder (all were anxiety disorders). patterns test (p = .001 for both) and spatial span
The role of comorbidity was only examined in selectively forward (p = .025; p = .001, respectively). These are
mute children with and without social phobia, given the both measures of the ability to hold (temporarily
small number of other comorbidities. store) spatial information in memory; the former does
Mean clinician severity ratings on ADIS were higher not include a sequencing requirement, whereas the
for children with SM than children with ANX (t = 2.34, latter does. Children with SM also had deficits relative
p < .05) and consistent with a markedly disturbing/ to NCs but not relative to children with ANX for
disabling (severe) problem in children with SM versus a spatial span backward (p = .005), a measure of
definitely disturbing/disabling (moderate) problem in NVWM. They also had a higher percentage of
children with ANX. children with clinically significant scores (i.e., >1 SD
The mean age was 8.48 years (SD 1.51), with below the mean) for the visual patterns test and the
children with SM significantly younger than the other spatial span backward on # 2 analyses (20% and 34%,
groups (Table 1); mean performance IQ was 107.80 respectively for children with SM, with other groups
(SD 15.59) with a trend toward the performance IQ in <12%), but not the spatial span forward.
NCs being significantly higher than other groups (p = To test the third hypothesis concerning increased
.09; Table 1). To account for group differences, all of social anxiety in SM and children with ANX relative to
the analyses for measures that were not age standardized NCs, groups were compared on a general measure of
included age as a covariate. Sex differences were also anxiety (MASC) and on a specific measure of social
examined for all of the analyses. None were found, but anxiety (SASC) on both mother and child report
this may have been due to sample size constraints. (Table 1). Significant group differences were found. On
post hoc analyses, children with SM self-reported
Testing Main Hypotheses greater social anxiety than the other two groups (p =
To test the first hypothesis concerning language .003 for children with ANX; p = .001 for NCs), and
deficits in children with SM relative to children with their mothers perceived more social anxiety than
ANX and NCs, language measures were compared mothers of NCs (p = .001) but not more than mothers
across groups using analyses of variance (all measures of children with ANX. Self-reported anxiety on the
age standardized), and significant group differences MASC was greatest in children with ANX relative to
were probed with post hoc Tukey tests (Table 1). For all the other two groups (p = .020 for children with SM;
three language measures, children with SM performed p = .025 for NCs). Mothers also perceived more anxiety

1190 J. AM . ACAD. CHILD ADOLESC. PSYCH IATRY, 46:9, SE PTE MBER 2007

Copyright @ 2007 American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
SELECTIVE MUTISM AND LANGUAGE

TABLE 1
Means and Standard Deviations by Diagnostic Group
Measure Selective Mutism (n = 44) Anxiety Disorder (n = 28) Normal Controls (n = 19)
Age, y 7.87 (1.57) 9.09 (1.10) 9.07 (1.35)***
Performance IQ 106.77 (15.46) 105.23 (15.89) 115.00 (15.32)
Percentage nonnative English households 20.5% 21.4% 15.8%
SMQ (raw score)
School 1.45 (0.45) 3.08 (0.56) 3.56 (0.49)***
Family 3.03 (0.61) 3.69 (0.35) 3.94 (0.12)***
Other 1.80 (0.63) 2.86 (0.48) 3.57 (0.54)***
Total 6.28 (1.31) 9.63 (1.21) 11.08 (1.04)***
Language measures
PPVT-III (scaled score) 97.35 (14.76) 111.00 (17.86) 110.95 (13.64)***
LACT
1 (converted) 24.26 (5.64) 26.79 (2.30) 26.25 (1.95)
2 (converted) 41.10 (20.94) 52.97 (19.78) 54.75 (17.78)a
Total converted score 66.08 (24.80) 80.11 (20.94) 79.56 (17.73)*
TROG (standard score) 89.86 (19.14) 100.86 (14.20) 102.26 (15.92)**
Working memory
Sp. Span (scaled scores):
Forward 8.86 (3.04) 10.79 (3.57) 11.78 (2.76)**
Backward 9.34 (3.58) 9.93 (3.02) 11.89 (2.40)*
Finger Windows
Forward (scaled score) 10.00 (3.03) 9.61 (3.46) 11.58 (3.50)
Backward (raw score) 7.16 (4.10) 10.71 (4.12) 10.26 (3.81)a
Visual Patterns Test (scaled score) 67.89 (9.83) 77.18 (11.22) 81.74 (20.53)***
Anxiety measures
SASC (raw score)
Mother 56.06 (14.15) 52.58 (12.48) 36.44 (8.89)***
Child 48.80 (10.65) 40.71 (11.75) 39.79 (9.50)*
MASC (t score)
Mother 57.97 (18.08) 68.56 (11.67) 48.59 (15.60)***
Child 50.26 (11.39) 58.50 (9.97) 49.62 (11.70)*
Note: Direction of significant effects: lowest age, SMQ scores, language scores, and cognitive scores for SM versus other groups (except
spatial span backward where children with SM were comparable to children with ANX); highest MASC scores for ANX versus other groups;
higher SASC scores for SM and ANX (maternal report) groups than normal controls. SMQ = Selective Mutism Questionnaire; PPVT =
Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test; LACT = Lindamood Auditory Conceptualization Test; TROG = Test of Reception of Grammar; Sp. Span =
Spatial Span; SASC = Social Anxiety Scale for Children; MASC = Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Children.
a
Significant, but not when controlled for age.
* p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001.

in children with ANX than in the other two groups (p = that differentiated groups were examined (Table 2).
.009 for children with SM; p = .001 for NC). Significant negative correlations were found between
the SMQ and social anxiety by both mother and child
Exploration of Possible Predictors of SM report (i.e., higher social anxiety associated with less
Children with SM differed from both comparison speech), and significant positive correlations were
groups on the SMQ, showing greater evidence of found between the SMQ and measures of language
mutism in all environments (p = .001 for all post hoc and NVWM (i.e., better performance on these
comparisons; Table 1). This provides some evidence of measures associated with more speech). These measures
the construct validity of the measure. Because results were also highly intercorrelated and negatively corre-
were so consistent across environments, only the total lated with child report (but not mother report) of social
SMQ was used in subsequent analyses. Bivariate anxiety. Age was positively correlated with SMQ (i.e.,
correlations between the SMQ and all other measures increased speech with increased age).

J. AM. ACAD. CHILD ADOLESC. PSYCHIATRY, 46:9, SEPTEMBER 2007 1191

Copyright @ 2007 American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
MANASSIS ET AL.

TABLE 2
Bivariate Correlations Between SMQ and Variables Differentiating Groups in Children With SM (n = 44)
Variable Age SASC (M) SASC (C) Sp. Span(f ) VPT LACT TROG PPVT-3 SMQ
Age 1
SASC (M) j0.110 1
SASC (C) j0.305** 0.335** 1
Sp. Span(f) 0.003 j0.211 j0.417** 1
VPT 0.525** j0.235 j0.377** 0.379** 1
LACT 0.425** .099 j0.354** 0.316** 0.474** 1
TROG 0.081 j0.073 j0.247* 0.317** 0.216* 0.502** 1
PPVT-3 0.154 j0.193 j0.325** 0.250* 0.302* 0.421** 0.664** 1
SMQ 0.403** j0.463** j0.401** 0.260* 0.394** 0.331** 0.401** 0.397** 1
Note: SASC = Social Anxiety Scale for Children; M = maternal; C = child; Sp. Span(f) = Spatial Span (forward); VPT = Visual Patterns Test;
LACT = Lindamood Auditory Conceptualization Test; TROG = Test of Reception of Grammar; PPVT = Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test;
SMQ = Selective Mutism Questionnaire.
* p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001.

Because we did not have any a priori hypotheses as to specific hypothesis in this area. Analyses of covariance
which specific measures would be the best predictors of (controlling for age) for all cognitive and language
SMQ, a series of stepwise regression analyses was done measures revealed that children with SM with social
to explore these predictors for the SM group only phobia did better on the Backward Finger Windows
(Table 3). No more than five predictors were entered in test for NVWM (F3,38 = 4.19, p < .05) than children
a given analysis, given the statistical need to have at least with SM without social phobia, but this finding was no
eight SM subjects per predictor (total number of longer significant when applying the Bonferroni
children with SM = 44). Entering age, mother-reported correction for multiple comparisons.
social anxiety, and the TROG accounted for a larger We also reran all language and cognitive group
proportion of the variance than any other stepwise comparisons covarying for performance IQ. Significant
regression, with an adjusted R 2 of 0.38. differences remained on two linguistic measures (PPVT
and TROG: F4,84 = 9.04, p = .001 and F4,84 = 4.79, p =
.011, respectively) with children with SM performing
Secondary Analyses worse than the comparison groups, and one NVWM
We were interested in the role of comorbid social measure (spatial span forward F4,84 = 6.46, p = .002)
phobia in cognitive and linguistic aspects of SM, with SM performing worse than children with ANX
recognizing that sample size was not adequate to test a and NCs.

TABLE 3
Stepwise Regression Models for SMQ in Children With Selective Mutism (N = 44)
Model " t p F (Model) R 2 (Model) Adjusted R 2 SE
1. Constant 2.12 .037 16.41 0.19 0.18 2.03
Age 0.436 4.05 .000
2. Constant 3.99 .000 16.60 0.33 0.31 1.87
Age 0.396 3.97 .000
SASC (M) j0.370 j3.71 .000
3. Constant 1.70 .094 15.51 0.41 0.38 1.76
Age 0.362 3.82 .000
SASC (M) j0.361 j3.84 .000
TROG 0.288 3.05 .003
Note: SMQ = Selective Mutism Questionnaire; SASC (M) = Social Anxiety Scale for Children-Maternal; TROG = Test of Reception of
Grammar.

1192 J. AM . ACAD. CHILD ADOLESC. PSYCH IATRY, 46:9, SE PTE MBER 2007

Copyright @ 2007 American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
SELECTIVE MUTISM AND LANGUAGE

Finally, we re-ran all language and cognitive group may contribute to social anxiety, for example, by
comparisons covarying for socioeconomic status. Sig- impairing recognition of faces or recognition of other
nificant differences remained on two linguistic mea- visual social cues. The high intercorrelation found
sures (LACT total converted and PPVT; F4,84 = 4.01, between visual memory and child-reported social anxiety
p = .020 and F4,84 = 4.35, p = .018, respectively) and on measures is consistent with this hypothesis. Another
the visual patterns test (F4,84 = 4.67, p = .014), with possibility is that the motor demands of the tasks
children with SM performing worse than comparison (coloring a grid for the Visual Patterns test; manipulating
groups. blocks for the Spatial Span) are problematic in SM (and
possibly in ANX as well), consistent with the finding of
Kristensen and Oerbeck (2006) that motor function was
DISCUSSION
a strong predictor of visual memory in SM. They found
Although our findings are limited by small compar- reduced verbal memory span, but not reduced visual
ison groups and variability in age, the consistently memory span once results were controlled for motor
poorer performance of the SM group on linguistic function. The lack of group differences on the Finger
measures is striking. All three linguistic measures Windows test, which makes fewer motoric demands, is
differentiated groups with children with SM performing consistent with this hypothesis. There was also a trend
worst. This difference remained significant for two of toward worse performance on this task for children with
three measures even when covarying for performance IQ SM without (rather than with) comorbid social phobia
and socioeconomic level. This finding is consistent with that merits further investigation. Further studies of
data from our previous study and other studies reporting language, cognition, auditory processing, and the
subtle language impairments in SM (Kristensen, 2000; relationships among them are clearly needed to under-
Kristensen and Oerbeck, 2006; Manassis et al., 2003; stand etiological mechanisms of SM.
McInnes et al., 2004). Detailed nonverbal linguistic Our findings replicate the specific link proposed
assessment may be indicated in children with SM (see between social anxiety (versus other forms of anxiety) in
McInnes and Manassis, 2005 for further description). SM. Consistent with previous studies (Kristensen,
The nature and origin of language deficits in SM also 2000), selectively mute children showed greater social
merit further study. For example, auditory processing anxiety both by self- and parent report than normal
deficits have been recently linked to SM (Bar-Haim children or children with ANX. Overall anxiety as
et al., 2004). It is unclear whether such deficits reported on the MASC, however, was greatest for
represent the biological substrate for SM-related children with ANX. Unfortunately, the number of
language impairment or a separate problem. If, as children with social phobia in the sample was too small
Bar-Haim and colleagues suggest, auditory efferent to determine whether SM represents an extreme of
activity is impaired in SM such that these children fail social phobia as some have suggested (Anstendig,
to desensitize to their own vocalizations (and are 1999), possibly because diagnosis was of necessity
therefore overstimulated by them), this deficit would be based mainly on parent report. Our previous study
particularly relevant in processing extended speech (Manassis et al., 2003) suggested that children with SM
(versus single sounds or words). Consistent with this were comparable to nonmute socially phobic children
hypothesis, we found increased proportions of children in their level of social anxiety.
with SM in the clinical range as the requirement to In this study the SMQ appeared promising in
process spoken language (and presumably repeat it discriminating SM from other childhood anxieties.
internally while selecting and executing the nonverbal Therefore, it was used as a rough, quantitative measure
response) in each of the linguistic tasks increased (i.e., of the child`s degree of mutism, to examine potential
most for the TROG, least for the PPVT). predictors of SM severity. Age, social anxiety (mother
Group differences on cognitive measures were less report), and one language measure (TROG) accounted
consistent. Measures of visual memory appeared to for 38% of the variance in SMQ score among the
differentiate children with SM from other anxious children with SM. This finding supports the need to
children and normals but measures of NVWM did not assess both social anxiety and language when such
do so consistently. A specific deficit in visual memory children present clinically. It is possible that cognitive

J. AM. ACAD. CHILD ADOLESC. PSYCHIATRY, 46:9, SEPTEMBER 2007 1193

Copyright @ 2007 American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
MANASSIS ET AL.

factors contributed to severity as well, but their with English as a first language. Teasing apart
contribution was likely obscured by their correlation symptoms of social phobia affecting speech from
with social anxiety or age. symptoms of SM is also a measurement challenge in
The fact that age was predictive of greater speech this type of study.
supports previous findings of gradual improvement of Studies with more economically and racially diverse
SM over time but with infrequent complete remission samples are also indicated as well as children in rural
(Kolvin and Fundudis, 1981; Steinhausen and Juzi, settings (all of the participating sites were urban).
1996). Alternatively, families of older children with Assessing the degree of mutism by multiple informants
severe SM or younger children with mild SM may have and including more detailed linguistic testing would also
been less motivated to participate in the study (i.e., strengthen future studies.
sampling bias). Families of children with severe,
persistent SM may have given up on the possibility of Clinical Implications
successful intervention, whereas families of young, Children with SM are often highly impaired, perhaps
mildly affected children may have assumed that the even more so than those with ANX (as rated by
condition could still be outgrown. Consistent with this clinicians in our study). Speech and language assess-
idea, six of nine children with SM who did not follow ment appears important in the assessment of these
through on physician referral to the study were older children. Collaboration between child and adolescent
than the mean for SM study participants. It is also psychiatrists and speech-language pathologists is there-
possible that speech quantity in certain environments fore likely to serve them well (McInnes and Manassis,
increases with development in nonmute children as well 2005). There may be a therapeutic role for language
as in children with SM. Only prospective, longitudinal remediation in SM, although this intervention awaits
studies of the development of children with SM relative further study.
to their nonmute peers will determine the natural history Social anxiety must also be examined and addressed
of this disorder and its response to various interventions. in SM including reports from multiple informants.
There is already some evidence for anxiolytic medica-
Limitations
tions in SM (Black and Uhde, 1994; Dummit et al.,
The small sample size and age differences between 1996), particularly selective serotonin reuptake inhibi-
groups constitute limitations of this study. A larger tors, and psychological interventions targeting social
sample would not only strengthen our findings but also anxiety are being developed and evaluated (reviewed in
would allow relationships between various predictors of McInnes and Manassis, 2005). The SMQ appears to be
SM to be examined using more rigorous methods (e.g., a promising measure of SM severity linked to both social
path analysis). The use of stepwise regression is a anxiety and language. This measure may also serve as a
limitation of the present study, although consistent useful baseline for examining treatment outcomes.
with the exploratory nature of this analysis. The Finally, clinicians must keep abreast of new findings
underrepresentation of boys and small number of in relation to SM because a growing number of research
NCs constitute further sampling limitations. groups are examining potential etiological factors in
The availability of questionnaires only in English also and potential interventions for this highly debilitating
eliminated a large segment of the SM population from condition.
this study: those who are not attending school in their
native language (Bradley and Sloman, 1975). Alter-
natively, factors accounting for SM may differ in this Disclosure: Dr. Manassis obtains royalties from her book Keys to
Parenting Your Anxious Child, which was used in the clinical care of
population. Elizur and Perednik (2003), for example, some participating children and families. The other authors have no
found that SM in immigrant children was associated financial relationships to disclose.
with fewer developmental problems than SM in
nonimmigrant children, suggesting different pathways REFERENCES
to SM in these populations. Nonnative speakers
American Psychiatric Association (1994), Diagnostic and Statistical Manual
attending school for extended periods in English of Mental Disorders, 4th edition (DSM-IV). Washington, DC: American
(included in this study) may also differ from children Psychiatric Association

1194 J. AM . ACAD. CHILD ADOLESC. PSYCH IATRY, 46:9, SE PTE MBER 2007

Copyright @ 2007 American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
SELECTIVE MUTISM AND LANGUAGE

Anstendig KD (1999), Is selective mutism an anxiety disorder? Rethinking Kolvin I, Fundudis T (1981), Elective mute children: psychological,
DSM-IV classification. J Anxiety Disord 13:417Y434 development, and background factors. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 22:
Bar-Haim Y, Henkin Y, Ari-Even-Roth D, Tetin-Schneider S, Hildesheimer 219Y232
M, Muchnik C (2004), Reduced auditory efferent activity in childhood Kristensen H (2000), Selective mutism and comorbidity with developmental
selective mutism. Biol Psychiatry 55:1061Y1068 disorder/delay, anxiety disorder, and elimination disorder. J Am Acad
Barbosa J, Manassis K, Tannock R ( 2002), Measuring anxiety: Parent- Child Adolesc Psychiatry 39:249Y256
child reporting differences in clinical samples. Depress Anxiety 15: Kristensen H, Oerbeck B (2006), Is selective mutism associated with deficits
61Y65 in memory span or visual memory? An exploratory case-control study.
Bergman RL, Keller M, Wood J, Piacentini J, McCracken J (2001), Selective Depress Anxiety 23:71Y76
Mutism Questionnaire (SMQ): development and findings. Presented at La Greca AM, Stone WL (1993), Social Anxiety Scale for Children-Revised:
the Annual Meeting of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent factor structure and concurrent validity. J Clin Child Psychol 22:17Y27
Psychiatry, Honolulu, October 23Y28 Lindamood C, Lindamood P (1971), Lindamood Auditory Conceptualisation
Bergman RL, Piacentini J, McCracken JT (2002), Prevalence and Test Manual. R. Allen, TX: DLM Teaching Resources
description of selective mutism in a school-based sample. J Am Acad Manassis K, Fung D, Tannock R, Sloman L, Fiksenbaum L, McInnes A
Child Adolesc Psychiatry 41:938Y946 (2003), Characterizing selective mutism: is it more than social anxiety?
Bishop DVM (2003), Test for Reception of Grammar (Version 2): TROG-2 Depress Anxiety 18:153Y161
Manual. London: Psychological Corporation March J (1997), The Multidimensional Anxiety Scale for Children (MASC):
Black B, Uhde T (1994), Treatment of elective mutism with fluoxetine: a factor structure, reliability, and validity. J Am Acad Child Adolesc
double blind placebo-controlled study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 36:554Y565
Psychiatry 33:1000Y1006 McInnes A, Fung D, Manassis K, Fiksenbaum L, Tannock R (2004),
Bradley S, Sloman L (1975), Elective mutism in immigrant families. J Am Narrative skills in children with selective mutism: an exploratory study.
Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 14:510Y514 Am J Speech Lang Pathol 13:304Y315
Cohen J (1988), Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd ed. McInnes A, Manassis K (2005), When silence is not golden: an integrated
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum approach to selective mutism. Semin Speech Lang 26:201Y210
Della Sala S, Gray C, Baddeley A, Wilson L (1997), Visual Patterns Test: Rynn MA, Barber JP, Khalid-Khan S, et al. (2006), The psychometric
A Test of Short-Term Visual Recall. Suffolk, UK: Thames Valley Test properties of the MASC in a pediatric psychiatric sample. Anxiety Dis
Dummit ES III, Klein R, Tancer NK, Ashe B, Martin J (1996), Fluoxetine 20:139Y157
treatment of children with selective mutism: an open trial. J Am Acad Silverman WK, Albano AM (1996), Anxiety Disorders Interview Schedule for
Child Adolesc Psychiatry 35:615Y621 DSM-IV. San Antonio, TX: Graywind
Dunn LM, Dunn LM (1997), Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test III-Revised. Steinhausen HC, Juzi C (1996), Elective mutism: an analysis of 100 cases.
Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Services J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 35:606Y614
Elizur Y, Perednik R (2003), Prevalence and description of selective mutism Toppelberg CO, Tabors P, Coggins A, Lum K, Burger C (2005),
in immigrant and nonimmigrant families: a controlled study. J Am Acad Differential diagnosis of selective mutism in bilingual children. J Am
Child Adolesc Psychiatry 42:1451Y1459 Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 44:592Y595

Sleep Arrangements and Behavior of Bed-Sharing Families in the Home Setting Sally A. Baddock, PhD, Barbara C.
Galland, PhD, Barry J. Taylor, MBChB, FRACP, David P.G. Bolton, MRCP, PhD

Objectives: We aimed to provide a quantitative analysis of the sleep arrangements and behaviors of bed-sharing families to further
understand the risks and benefits as well as the effects of infant age and room temperature on bed-sharing behaviors. Methods:
Forty infants who regularly bed shared with Q1 parent Q5 hours per night were recruited. Overnight video of the family and
physiological monitoring of the infant was conducted in infants` homes. Infant sleep position, potential for exposure to expired
air, head covering and uncovering, breastfeeding, movements, family sleep arrangements, responses to the infant, and
interactions were logged. Results: All infants slept with their mother. Fathers were included in 18 studies and siblings in 4. Infants
usually slept beside the mother, separated from the father/siblings (if present), facing the mother, with head at mothers` breast
level, touching, or with mother cradling. Median overnight breastfeeding duration was 40.5 minutes. Mothers commonly faced
their infant, but infants were rarely in a position that potentially exposed them to maternal expired air. Fathers were seldom in
contact with the infant during sleep. Of the 102 head-covering episodes observed in 22 infants, 80% were because of changes in
adult sleep position. Sixty-eight percent of head uncovering was facilitated by the mother; half of these events were prompted by
the infant. A 1-C increase in room temperature decreased infant head covering by 0.2 hours. Conclusions: The mother-infant
relationship is of prime importance during bed sharing, whether the father is present or not. The focus around breastfeeding
often dictates the sleep position of the infant and mother, though room temperature may also influence this. In colder rooms
infants tend to spend more time with their face covered by bedding. Frequent maternal interactions rely on the ability of the
mother to arouse with little stimulation. Mothers, perhaps impaired by alcohol, smoking, or overtiredness, may not be able to
respond appropriately. Pediatrics 2007;119:e200Ye207.

J. AM. ACAD. CHILD ADOLESC. PSYCHIATRY, 46:9, SEPTEMBER 2007 1195

Copyright @ 2007 American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

You might also like