You are on page 1of 7

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/230089145

A Double-edged Sword: Event Centrality, PTSD and Posttraumatic Growth

Article  in  Applied Cognitive Psychology · September 2011


DOI: 10.1002/acp.1753

CITATIONS READS

131 651

2 authors, including:

Adriel Boals
University of North Texas
91 PUBLICATIONS   3,128 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Adriel Boals on 07 April 2020.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Applied Cognitive Psychology, Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 25: 817–822 (2011)
Published online 22 October 2010 in Wiley Online Library (wileyonlinelibrary.com) DOI: 10.1002/acp.1753

A Double-edged Sword: Event Centrality, PTSD and Posttraumatic Growth

ADRIEL BOALS* and DARNELL SCHUETTLER


Department of Psychology, University of North Texas, USA

Summary: Previous research has demonstrated negative mental health consequences (including PTSD symptoms) of construing a
potentially traumatic event as central to one’s identity. In the current paper, we replicated an association between event centrality
and PTSD symptoms. We also found event centrality similarly predicts posttraumatic growth (PTG) even after controlling for
PTSD symptoms, depression, DSM-IV A1 and A2 status of the event, coping styles and cognitive processing of the event. Because
predictive relationships between event centrality and PTSD symptoms, as well as event centrality and PTG were positive,
construing an event as central to one’s identity can indeed become a double-edged sword, allowing for both debilitation and
growth. Copyright # 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

INTRODUCTION the life story and (3) a core component of personal identity.
Not surprisingly, CES scores are highly correlated with
Recently, there has been growing interest on the impact of PTSD symptoms and depression (Berntsen & Rubin). These
events central to one’s identity. These events are repeatedly correlations have been replicated in samples using older
reflected upon and can alter our interpretations of past and adults (Berntsen & Thomsen, 2005), college students (Boals,
current experiences. These event memories often mark the 2010; Rubin, Boals, & Berntsen, 2008) and Iraq War veterans
beginning and end of lifetime periods (Conway & Pleydell- (Brown, Antonius, Kramer, Root, & Hirst, 2010). In addition,
Pearce, 2000) and have been referred to as ‘self-defining higher CES scores correlate with greater emotional intensity,
memories’ (Singer, 1995). Self-defining events give meaning greater visceral reactions, worse overall physical health
and continuity to one’s sense of self and life story (Boals, 2010) and higher levels of complicated grief in
(McAdams, Josselson, & Lieblich, 2006) and influence bereaved participants (Boelen, 2009). Importantly, CES
behaviour and goals (Sutin & Robins, 2008). Individuals scores remain a significant predictor of PTSD symptoms,
vary greatly in terms of events used to construct a sense of even when controlling for anxiety, depression and dis-
self (McLean & Fournier, 2008; Sutherland & Bryant, 2005). sociation (Berntsen & Rubin, 2007; Rubin et al.). Thus
In fact, an inability of the autobiographical memory system construing a trauma as central to one’s identity is associated
to allow important events to alter the self can be pathological with multiple poor psychological outcomes.
(Crane, Goddard, & Pring, 2010). A problem occurs when an Although negative effects of trauma exposure have been
individual experiences a stressful or traumatic event, and this well-documented, positive effects are also being explored.
negative event becomes a detrimental organizing principle Much of this research centers on benefit finding and
for the individual’s sense of self and view of the world. posttraumatic growth (PTG). PTG occurs when individuals
Construing a traumatic event as central to one’s identity formulate adaptive interpretations or worldviews as a result
keeps the memory highly available, leading to further of experiencing a traumatic event. PTG is measured by
rehearsal. The rehearsal in turn maintains and strengthens the perceived changes in self, relationships with others,
memory and the memory’s emotional impact over long philosophy of life, spirituality and new possibilities
periods of time. For example, McNally, Lasko, Macklin, and (Tedeschi & Calhoun, 1996). A meta-analysis found PTG
Pitman (1995) noted that Vietnam veterans who came to the is related to less depression and greater well-being, yet
laboratory wearing regalia (medals, fatigues, etc.) had positively related to intrusive thoughts about the event
disproportionately higher rates of PTSD and were more (Helgeson, Reynolds, & Tomich, 2006). The relationship
likely to recall memories from the war, in comparison to between PTG and intrusive thoughts may occur because PTG
Vietnam veterans not wearing regalia. only comes from events that result in a struggle significant
The Centrality of Events Scale (CES; Berntsen & Rubin, enough to force re-evaluation of worldviews. Thus, the term
2006) assesses the extent to which an individual construes a ‘adversarial growth’ has also been used in lieu of the term
stressful or traumatic event as a primary reference to identity. PTG.
The questionnaire includes items such as, ‘I feel that this The purpose of the current study is to explore the
event has become part of my identity’, ‘This event has relationship between the centrality of an event and PTG.
become a reference point for the way I understand myself Although one may assume negative and positive con-
and the world’ and ‘I feel that this event has become a central sequences of trauma exposure are two opposite ends of a
part of my life story’. The CES measures three functions of singular ‘consequences of trauma’ construct, empirical
memories: the extent to which a memory becomes (1) a evidence supports the possibility of independent negative
reference point for everyday inferences, (2) a turning point in and positive trauma consequences (Linley, Joseph, Cooper,
Harris, & Meyer, 2003). In other words, even when an
*Correspondence to: Adriel Boals, Department of Psychology, University of individual’s struggle with a traumatic event leads to PTG,
North Texas, Denton, TX 76203, USA. E-mail: adriel@unt.edu this does not necessarily mean a concurrent decrease in

Copyright # 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.


818 A. Boals and D. Schuettler

negative symptoms will also occur. Thus, one cannot assume Table 1. Means and standard deviations
the direction of the PTG and CES relationship would be the
Variable Mean (SD) Range
opposite direction of the PTSD and CES relationship.
Because PTG is believed to result from a challenge to and re- Centrality of Events Scale 17.9 (8.8) 7–35
examination of core beliefs (Tedeschi, Calhoun, & Cann, Posttraumatic Growth Inventory 53.2 (28.7) 0–105
PTSD Checklist 28.3 (12.9) 17–85
2007), we hypothesize PTG will be positively correlated with
A1 0.42 (0.5) 0–1
CES scores. Such results would suggest that construing a A2 0.47 (0.5) 0–1
traumatic event is like a double-edged sword. On one hand, Problem Coping 20.7 (4.7) 8–32
this construal is associated with higher levels of PTSD Emotion Coping 24.7 (5.4) 10–40
symptoms and depression. On the other hand, this construal Avoidance Coping 18.0 (4.9) 10–40
QIDS (depression) 15.5 (4.2) 0–27
is also associated with higher levels of PTG.
Positive Cognitive Resolution 11.7 (5.3) 3–21
To adequately examine the relationship between PTG and Downward Comparisons 16.9 (4.5) 3–21
event centrality, a number of methodological issues were Resolution 20.0 (6.3) 4–28
considered. First, in the current study, we used a college Denial 9.6 (5.5) 4–28
student sample to examine the relationship between CES and Regrets 9.3 (5.5) 3–21
PTG. This issue is not a concern since rates of trauma Note: A1 and A2 were coded 0 (no) and 1 (yes). Positive Cognitive
exposure and associated symptoms in college student Resolution, Downward Comparisons, Resolution, Denial and Regrets are
subscales of the CPOTS.
samples are comparable to that of the general population
(Bernat, Ronfeldt, Calhoun, & Arias, 1998; Frazier, Anders,
et al., 2009; Smyth, Hockemeyer, Heron, Wonderlich, &
Pennebaker, 2008). In addition, such a sample will yield a categories of potentially traumatic events, such as being a
wide variety of stressful/traumatic events and large levels of victim of physical or sexual abuse (all nine events are listed
variance in PTSD symptoms, CES scores and PTG scores. in Table 1). Participants respond either ‘yes’ or ‘no’ to
The second issue concerns types of stressful events whether they experienced each event during their lifetime.
nominated by a non-clinical sample. Since not all The TEQ also allows for unspecified traumatic events to be
participants will have experienced a DSM-IV-defined listed. If the respondent indicates ‘no’ to all listed traumatic
traumatic event, it is possible the relationship between events (i.e. the respondent had not experienced any of the
PTG and CES may vary as a function of whether the eliciting listed traumatic events), they are asked to ‘describe briefly
event meets the DSM criteria as a traumatic event. Thus, we the most traumatic thing to happen to you’. At the end of the
included measures of the DSM-IV-TR A1 and A2 criteria questionnaire, the respondent indicates which aforemen-
[American Psychiatric Association (APA), 2000] for all tioned event was ‘the most traumatic thing to have happened
events nominated by participants. The inclusion of these to you’. The TEQ evidences good test–retest reliability over
criteria will allow for comparisons between events that do a 2-week period for the number of traumatic events
meet the stated criteria and those that do not meet the criteria. experienced (r ¼ 0.91). Test–retests for specific events
The third issue concerns the measurement of PTG. Some ranged from r ¼ 0.72 to 1.00 (Vrana & Lauterbach, 1994).
evidence suggests current measurements of PTG may reflect
coping as opposed to actual growth (Frazier, Tennan, Gavian, A1 criterion
Park, Tomich, & Tashiro, 2009; McFarland & Alvaro, 2000). Participants responded to questions phrased in exact DSM-
Thus, we included a widely used coping measure to IV-TR wording; ‘Did you experience, witness or confront an
statistically partial out coping variance and increase the event that involved actual or threatened death or serious
chance of measuring actual growth. Lastly, because one injury, or threat to [your] physical integrity or that of others?’
could argue the CES does not measure event centrality but Participants indicated ‘yes’ or ‘no’ in reference to the most
rather simply reflects general cognitive bias, a measure of traumatic event they nominated on the TEQ.
cognitive processing of trauma was included.
A2 criterion
METHODS Participants again responded to questions phrased in exact
DSM-IV-TR wording, ‘Did your response include fear,
Participants helplessness or horror?’ Participants indicated ‘yes’ or ‘no’
in reference to the most traumatic event they nominated on
A sample of 929 (603 female) undergraduates from the
the TEQ.
University of North Texas volunteered for partial course
credit. The average age was m ¼ 20.1 (SD ¼ 3.6) with a range
Brief COPE (BCOPE)
of 18–60. The sample was 59% Caucasian, 16% African–
The BCOPE (Carver, 1997) measures different coping
American, 15% Hispanic, 5% Asian and 5% ‘other’.
strategies. It consists of 28 questions that measure three
primary coping styles: problem focused coping, emotion
focused coping and avoidant coping. Item examples include:
Materials
‘I’ve been thinking hard about what steps to take’ (problem
Traumatic Events Questionnaire (TEQ) focused coping); ‘I’ve been getting emotional support from
The TEQ (Vrana & Lauterbach, 1994) assesses lifetime others’ (emotion focused coping) and ‘I’ve been refusing to
exposure to traumatic events. The TEQ includes nine believe that it has happened’ (avoidant coping). Responses

Copyright # 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 25: 817–822 (2011)
Double-edged sword 819

range from 1 (I haven’t been doing this a lot) to 4 (I’ve been within the last 7 days. Symptoms include sleep patterns,
doing this a lot). Each of the scales have internal reliabilities sadness, appetite, weight levels (gain and loss), general
ranging from a ¼ .50 to .90. In the current study, the interest, view of self, suicidal ideation, energy level and
reliability for the problem focused coping subscale was feelings of restlessness. Items are rated on a four-point scale
a ¼ .76, the emotion focused coping subscale was a ¼ .76 ranging from non-depressive to extreme levels. High internal
and the avoidance coping subscale was a ¼ .76. consistencies (a ¼ 0.86) have been found for the QIDS-SR,
with this number increasing from a ¼ 0.73 at baseline to
Centrality of Events Scale (CES) a ¼ 0.92 at 12 week follow-up. This measure has shown to be
The short version of the CES (Berntsen & Rubin, 2006) is a highly correlated with other measures of depression such as
7-item questionnaire designed to measure the extent to which the Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology (Trivedi et al.,
an event is viewed as central to one’s identity. Participants 2004). In the current sample, reliability was a ¼ .77.
are asked to rate the extent to which they agree with the
statements from 1 (totally disagree) to 5 (totally agree).
Procedure
Example items include, ‘This event permanently changed
my life’ and ‘I feel that this event has become part of my As part of a mass testing session, participants completed an
identity’. The scale’s reported reliability range is a ¼ .88. In online battery of questionnaires that took approximately
the current sample, reliability was a ¼ .94. 1 hour to complete. Participants completed questionnaires
through an online survey in exchange for partial course
Cognitive Processing of Trauma Scale (CPOTS) credit. Although there were other questionnaires included
The CPOTS (Williams, Davis, & Millsap, 2002) is a 17-item throughout the survey, the questionnaires reported in this
measure of cognitive processing of traumatic experiences. study were completed by participants in the following order:
Each item is rated on a scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 BCOPE, TEQ, PCL-S, PTGI, CES, A1, A2, CPOTS, and
(strongly agree). The CPOTS is composed of five subscales: QIDS. The PCL-S, PTGI, CES, A1, A2, and CPOTS were all
positive cognitive restructuring, downward comparison, completed in reference to the most traumatic event each
resolution, denial and regrets. In the current sample, participant indentified on the TEQ.
reliabilities for each subscale were .85, .82, .84, .77 and
.82, respectively. RESULTS

Posttraumatic Growth Inventory (PTGI) Participants identified a wide variety of potentially traumatic
The PTGI (Tedeschi & Calhoun, 1996) is a 21-item events on the TEQ. The most frequently nominated event
questionnaire used to measure growth following a traumatic was ‘unexpected death of someone’ (18%), followed by
event. Items were scaled from 0 (I did not experience this ‘serious danger of losing your life’ (7%), ‘serious car
change as a result of my crisis) to 5 (I experienced this accident’ (7%), ‘child sexual or physical abuse’ (7%),
change to a very great degree as a result of my crisis) and the ‘natural disaster’ (6%), ‘experiences like these you can’t tell
sum score is reported. The reported coefficient reliability is about’ (4%), ‘rape, robbery or assault’ (4%), ‘other traumatic
.90 and test–retest reliability is .71. In the current sample event like these’ (3%), ‘adult physical abuse’ (2%), ‘witness
reliability was a ¼ .97. a serious injury or death’ (2%), ‘unwanted adult sexual
experience’ (3%). The remaining 37% of participants
PTSD Checklist (PCL-S) indicated they had not experienced any of the traumatic
The PCL-S (Blanchard, Jones-Alexander, Buckley, & events listed on the TEQ. These participants were asked to
Forneris, 1996) measures intrusive and avoidant thoughts. nominate the most traumatic event they had experienced.
The PCL-S is a 17-item measure of PTSD symptoms in Commonly nominated events included romantic relationship
reference to a specific event. Participants are asked to rate breakups, parents divorcing and academic failures.
items on a scale of 1 (not at all) to 5 (extremely) regarding the Since the current sample was not limited to a clinical
extent to which the event produced each of the official B, C sample, we examined a number of operationalizations of
and D PTSD symptoms during the past month. A score of traumatic event to determine whether the inclusion of non-
44 or higher is indicative of probable PTSD. Internal clinical participants affected the results. The first operatio-
consistency as are acceptable, a ¼ .58–.92, and convergent nalization we examined was whether the event selected by
correlations for the subscales between the PCL-S and CAPS the participant was one of the predetermined traumatic
(Clinician Administered PTSD Scale; Blake et al., 1995) events listed on the TEQ. Excluding participants who
were substantial, ranging from r ¼ .58 to .92 (Blanchard indicated they had not experienced any of the traumatic
et al.; Palmieri, Weathers, Difede, & King, 2007). Test–retest events on the TEQ did not affect the pattern of the results
reliability was r ¼ .96 (Blanchard et al.). In the current obtained, nor did any significant interactions involving this
sample, reliability was a ¼ .94. variable emerge. We next examined whether the event met
the DSM-IV A1 status affected the results. Excluding
The Quick Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology—Self participants whose event did not meet the A1 criterion did
Report (QIDS-SR) not affect the pattern of the results obtained, nor did any
The QIDS-SR (Rush et al., 2003) was used as a measure of significant interactions involving this variable emerge.
depression. The 16-item questionnaire asks the participant to Lastly, we examined whether participants’ score on the
rate how much each depression symptom has been applicable PCL-S affected the pattern of results. A score of 44 or higher

Copyright # 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 25: 817–822 (2011)
820 A. Boals and D. Schuettler

on the PCL-S is considered indicative of probable clinical presented in Table 2 are considered secondary to the primary
PTSD (Blanchard et al., 1996). Excluding participants with hypotheses.
PCL scores below this clinical cutoff score did not affect Because the CES was also related to a number of other
the pattern of the results obtained. The only significant variables included in Table 2, simultaneous multiple
interaction to emerge was an interaction between CES scores regressions were conducted to examine whether relation-
and PCL-S <44 on PTGI scores, F(1, 940) ¼ 5.45, p < .05. ships between the CES and the PCL-S and between the CES
The correlation between the CES and PTGI was stronger and the PTGI would remain significant when other variables
when participants’ PCL-S scores were below the clinical are included in the regression model. A multiple regression
cutoff, r(815) ¼ .57, p < .0001, in comparison to when scores model was conducted to predict PTGI scores. Predictor
were above the clinical cutoff, r(127) ¼ .25, p < .01. In variables involved all 13 variables (except the PTGI)
summary, excluding participants whose most traumatic event included in Tables 1 and 2. The overall model was
failed to meet a variety of conventional standards for being significant, F(13, 846) ¼ 59.12, p < .001, adjusted
considered a traumatic event or produce symptoms that R2 ¼ .47. The results of the regression model revealed the
exceed clinical levels of PTSD did not affect the pattern of CES continued to significantly predict PTG and even more
results reported in this study. This pattern of results is importantly, was the strongest predictor, t(1, 846) ¼ 14.77,
consistent with the idea that the objective nature of p < .001, b ¼ .46. Other significant predictor variables
potentially traumatic events is unrelated to mental health included the PCL-S, t(1, 846) ¼ 7.35, p < .001, b ¼ .25,
(Boals & Schuettler, 2009; Hathaway, Boals, & Banks, 2010; problem-focused coping, t(1, 846) ¼ 6.10, p < .001, b ¼ .19,
Rubin et al., 2008). Consequently, no participants were cognitive restructuring, t(1, 846) ¼ 5.09, p < .001, b ¼ .14,
excluded from the analyses based on the nature of their most downward comparisons, t(1, 846) ¼ 3.97, p < .001,
traumatic event. Means and standard deviations of variables b ¼ .11, depression (QIDS), t(1, 846) ¼ 3.33, p < .001,
are listed in Table 1. b ¼ .10, and resolution, t(1, 846) ¼ 2.07, p < .05, b ¼ .07.
In addition to exploring whether trauma-related variables The remaining variables (A1, A2, emotion-focused coping,
affected outcomes of the study, we examined possible avoidant coping, denial and regret) were non-significant.
gender interactions. Boals (2010) previously found that We next conducted a similar multiple regression model
females report higher CES scores than male. This gender predicting PCL-S scores. Predictor variables involved all 13
difference was replicated in the current data set. Females variables (except the PCL-S) included in Tables 1 and 2. The
reported significantly higher CES scores (m ¼ 18.8, overall model was significant, F(13, 846) ¼ 64.43, p < .001,
SD ¼ 8.9) than did males (m ¼ 16.5, SD ¼ 8.5), adjusted R2 ¼ .49. The results of the regression model
t(939) ¼ 3.77, p < .001, d ¼ .26. Despite this gender differ- replicated past results in that the CES continued to
ence, no gender significant interactions emerged from significantly predict PCL-S scores and even, the same as
analyses. Thus gender was excluded from all reported in the model predicting PTG, CES scores were the strongest
analyses. predictor, t(1, 846) ¼ 7.22, p < .001, b ¼ .24. Other signifi-
As can be seen in Table 2, we replicated previous findings cant predictor variables included the PTGI, t(1, 846) ¼ 7.35,
between the CES, the PCL-S and depressive symptoms. The p < .001, b ¼ .24, depression (QIDS), t(1, 846) ¼ 6.29,
results also supported the hypothesis that CES scores would p < .001, b ¼ .19, regret, t(1, 846) ¼ 5.57, p < .001,
be positively correlated with PTGI scores. In fact, the b ¼ .15, resolution, t(1, 846) ¼ 4.52, p < .05, b ¼ .14, A2,
correlation between the CES and the PTGI was the strongest t(1, 846) ¼ 3.79, p < .05, b ¼ .10, cognitive restructuring, t(1,
correlation obtained in the correlation matrix. Thus, the more 846) ¼ 3.75, p < .001, b ¼ .10, downward comparisons,
a participant construed a traumatic/stressful event as central t(1, 846) ¼ 3.03, p < .01, b ¼ .09, avoidance coping, t(1,
to their identity, the greater their likelihood of both PTSD 846) ¼ 2.68, p < .01, b ¼ .08, problem-focused coping, t(1,
symptoms and levels of PTG. The rest of the correlations 846) ¼ 2.10, p < .05, b ¼ .07 and denial, t(1, 846) ¼ 1.96,

Table 2. Correlation matrix


2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14
         
1. Centrality of Events .59 .55 .04 .18 .14 .16 .20 .29 .06 .10 .29 .06.23
2. Posttraumatic Growth — .44 .07 .13 .33 .26 .11 .07 .18 .08 .08 .11 .13
3. PTSD Checklist — — .10 .25 .12 .15 .34 .43 .12 .06 .34 .20 .36
4. A1 — — — .41 .02 .05 .01 .07 .02 .08 .06 .06 .07
5. A2 — — — — .01 .09 .09 .15 .09 .06 .06 .03 .09
6. Problem Coping — — — — — .58 .13 .06 .16 .05 .13 .01 .02
7. Emotion Coping — — — — — — .35 .07 .10 .11 .12 .02 .08
8. Avoidance Coping — — — — — — — .49 .10 .04 .21 .19 .23
9. QIDS (depression) — — — — — — — — .15 .01 -.28 .14 .22
10. Pos Cog Resolution — — — — — — — — — .28 .38 .02 .03
11. Downward Comparison — — — — — — — — — — .47 .10 .01
12. Resolution — — — — — — — — — — — .13 .21
13. Denial — — — — — — — — — — — — .35
14. Regrets — — — — — — — — — — — — —
Note: Positive Cognitive Resolution, Downward Comparisons, Resolution, Denial, and Regrets are subscales of the CPOTS.

p < .05; p < .01; p < .001.

Copyright # 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 25: 817–822 (2011)
Double-edged sword 821

p < .05, b ¼ .05. Only the variables A1 and emotion-focused and resolution, along with depression and PTSD symptoms.
coping were non-significant. Thus the construct of PTG appears to encompass a number of
complex mechanisms, including coping, cognitive proces-
DISCUSSION sing and emotional reactions. Whether these mechanisms are
the result of actual growth should continue to be explored in
Previous research has demonstrated negative consequences future empirical studies.
of construing a potentially traumatic event as central to Certain limitations should be acknowledged when con-
one’s identity (Berntsen & Rubin, 2006; Rubin et al., 2008). sidering this research. First, no formal PTSD diagnoses were
Specifically, event centrality predicted PTSD symptoms, made. However, the PCL-S is a well-established measure of
even after controlling for anxiety, depression and dis- PTSD symptoms in both general (Blanchard et al., 1996) and
sociation (Berntsen & Rubin, 2007). In the current paper, we clinical populations (Weathers, Litz, Herman, Huska, &
replicated the significant correlation between event centrality Keane, 1993). Second, because the data were cross-sectional,
and PTSD symptoms. This relationship remained significant no causal conclusions can be made about relationships
after controlling for depression, cognitive processing of between event centrality and outcomes. It is possible that
the trauma, coping styles, PTG, and A1 and A2 status of ensuing PTSD symptoms or perceptions of growth cause an
the event. We also found a significant correlation between event to be construed as more central to identity.
event centrality and PTG. As with predicting PTSD Longitudinal studies should be conducted to examine the
symptoms, the relationship between event centrality and time course of trauma aftermath. Lastly, we used self-report
PTG remained significant, even after controlling for measures when observation or peer/family report may have
depression, cognitive processing of the event, coping styles, yielded different results. However, our findings were
PTSD symptoms, and A1 and A2 status of the event. The consistent with previous research despite these limitations
direction of the correlations between event centrality and and further support the critical effect event centrality appears
PTSD symptoms/PTG were both positive. Therefore, to have on trauma aftermath.
construing a potentially traumatic experience as central to There are two primary conclusions to draw from this
one’s identity can indeed become a double-edged sword, research. First, event centrality does not only indicate
allowing for both debilitation and growth. negative trauma aftermath, but also the possibility of positive
When a multiple regression approach was used to predict consequences of trauma. As Tedeschi, Calhoun, and Cann
PTG scores with 13 trauma related variables, event centrality (2007) stated, ‘growth follows a challenge to and re-
emerged as the strongest predictor. Similar to other trauma examination of core beliefs, not every bad experience’ p.403.
research, event centrality has also been shown to be one of The CES certainly assesses a challenge and re-examination
the strongest predictors of PTSD symptoms even when a of the self based on a specific experience, indicating definite
large number of other trauma-related variables are used in a potential to influence levels of growth following trauma.
multiple regression model (Rubin et al., 2008). Additionally, Secondly, researchers have long hypothesized why some
the predictive contribution of event centrality to both PTSD individuals demonstrate resilience in the face of potentially
symptoms and PTG, even when controlling for five measures traumatic experiences when others in the same situation
of cognitive processing (subscales of the CPOTS), suggests become traumatized. Multiple predisposing factors such as
the CES does a good job of assessing a belief-system demographics, trauma history, type of trauma and mental
construct independent of cognitive biases. health status have been examined to clarify PTSD
Although not central to the hypotheses, the results allowed susceptibility versus resilience to such events, yet there is
us to revisit findings by Frazier, Tennen, et al. (2009) that the a general dissatisfaction among the psychology community
PTGI may measure a coping process, as opposed to actual with the current available explanations. Much controversy
growth. In addition, findings by McFarland and Alvaro exists regarding the type of events that should receive trauma
(2000) have demonstrated that perceived growth after a status and who can truly develop PTSD following certain
traumatic event may be the result of motivated self- types of events. Thus far, event centrality research would
enhancement, once again suggesting that the PTGI may suggest subjective rather than objective aspects of a
not measure actual growth. In our regression model potentially traumatic event are more critical to trauma
predicting PTSD symptoms, PTGI scores continued to be outcomes.
a significant predictor even when measures of coping styles,
along with several other trauma-related variables were
included in the model. If the PTGI measures nothing beyond REFERENCES
general coping, we would expect the PTGI to be non-
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical man-
significant when coping styles are included in the model.
ual of mental disorders (4th ed., text revision). Washington, DC: Amer-
Since this is not the pattern of results we observed, our results ican Psychological Association.
suggest the PTGI measures something beyond just general Bernat, J., Ronfeldt, H., Calhoun, K., & Arias, I. (1998). Prevalence of
coping. When the PTGI was predicted with a number of traumatic events and peritraumatic predictors of posttraumatic stress
trauma-related variables, problem-focused coping was a symptoms in a nonclinical sample of college students. Journal of Trau-
significant predictor, but emotion-focused and avoidant matic Stress, 11, 645–664.
Berntsen, D., & Rubin, D. (2006). The centrality of event scale: A measure
coping were not. A number of cognitive processing variables of integrating a trauma into one’s identity and its relation to post-
remained significant with coping styles in the model, traumatic stress disorder symptoms. Behavior Research and Therapy,
including cognitive restructuring, downward comparisons, 44, 219–231.

Copyright # 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 25: 817–822 (2011)
822 A. Boals and D. Schuettler

Berntsen, D., & Rubin, D. (2007). When a trauma becomes a key to identity: McLean, K. C., & Fournier, M. A. (2008). The content and processes of
Enhanced integration of trauma memories predicts posttraumatic stress autobiographical reasoning in narrative identity. Journal of Research in
disorder symptoms. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 21, 417–431. Personality, 42, 527–545.
Berntsen, D., & Thomsen, D. K. (2005). Personal memories for remote McNally, R., Lasko, N., Macklin, M., & Pitman, R. (1995). Autobiogra-
historical events: Accuracy and clarity of flashbulb memories related phical memory disturbance in combat-related posttraumatic stress
to World War II. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134, disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33, 619–630.
242–257. Palmieri, P. A., Weathers, F. W., Difede, J., & King, D. W. (2007).
Blake, D. D., Weathers, F., Nagy, L., Kaloupek, D. G., Gusman, F. D., Confirmatory analysis of the PTSD checklist and the Clinician-
Charney, D. S., et al. (1995). The development of a clinician-administered Administered PTSD scale is disaster workers exposed to the World
PTSD scale. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 8, 75–90. Trade Center ground zero. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116,
Blanchard, E., Jones-Alexander, J., Buckley, T., & Forneris, C. (1996). 329–341.
Psychometric properties of the PTSD Checklist (PCL). Behavior Rubin, D., Boals, A., & Berntsen, D. (2008). Memory in posttraumatic stress
Research and Therapy, 34, 669–673. disorder: Properties of voluntary and involuntary, traumatic and nontrau-
Boals, A. (2010). Autobiographical memories that have become central to matic autobiographical memories in people with and without posttrau-
identity: Gender differences in the Centrality of Events Scale for positive matic stress disorder symptoms. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
and negative events. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24, 107–121. General, 137, 591–614.
Boals, A., & Schuettler, D. (2009). PTSD symptoms in response to traumatic Rush, J., Trivedi, M., Ibrahim, H., Carmody, T., Arnow, B., Klein, D., et al.
and non-traumatic events: The role of respondent perception and A2 (2003). The 16-item Quick Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology
criterion. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 23, 1–5. (QIDS), clinician rating (QIDS-C), and self-report (QIDS-SR): A
Boelen, P. A. (2009). The centrality of a loss and its role in emotional psychometric evaluation in patients with chronic major depression.
problems among bereaved people. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47, Biological Psychiatry, 54, 573–583.
616–622. Singer, M. (1995). Illuminating language: A review of Pinker, S. (1994) The
Brown, A. D., Antonius, D., Kramer, M., Root, J., & Hirst, W. (2010). Language Instinct. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49,
Trauma centrality and PTSD in veterans returning from Iraq and Afgha- 577–581.
nistan. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 23, 496–499. Smyth, J. M., Hockemeyer, J. R., Heron, K. E., Wonderlich, S. A., &
Carver, C. (1997). You want to measure coping but your protocol’s too long: Pennebaker, J. W. (2008). Prevalence, type, disclosure, and severity of
Consider the brief cope. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 4, adverse life events in college students. Journal of American College
92–100. Health, 57, 69–76.
Conway, M., & Pleydell-Pearce, C. (2000). The construction of autobio- Sutherland, K., & Bryant, R. A. (2005). Self-defining memories in post-
graphical memories in the self-memory system. Psychological Review, traumatic stress disorder. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 44, 591–
107, 261–288. 598.
Crane, L., Goddard, L., & Pring, L. (2010). Brief report: Self-defining and Sutin, A., & Robins, R. (2008). Going forward by drawing from the past:
everyday autobiographical memories in adults with autism spectrum Personal strivings, personally meaningful memories, and personality
disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, 383–391. traits. Journal of Personality, 76, 1386–1397.
Frazier, P., Anders, S., Perera, S., Tomich, P., Tennen, H., Park, C., et al. Tedeschi, R., & Calhoun, L. (1996). The posttraumatic growth inventory:
(2009). Traumatic events among undergraduate students: Prevalence and Measure the positive legacy of trauma. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 9,
associated symptoms. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 56, 450–460. 455–471.
Frazier, P., Tennen, H., Gavian, M., Park, C., Tomich, C., & Tashiro, T. Tedeschi, R., Calhoun, L., & Cann, A. (2007). Evaluating resource gain:
(2009). Does elf-reported posttraumatic growth reflect genuine positive Understanding and misunderstanding posttraumatic growth. Applied
change? Psychological Science, 20, 912–919. Psychology: An International Review, 56, 396–406.
Hathaway, L. M., Boals, A., & Banks, J. B. (2010). PTSD symptoms and Trivedi, M. H., Rush, A. J., Ibrahim, H. M., Carmody, T. J., Biggs, M. M., &
dominant emotional response to a stressful event: An examination of Suppes, T., et al. (2004). The Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology,
DSM-IV criterion A2. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 23, 119–126. Clinician Rating (IDS-C) and Self-Report (IDS-SR), the Quick Inventory
Helgeson, V., Reynolds, K., & Tomich, P. (2006). A meta-analytic review of Depressive Symtomatology, Clinician Rating (QIDS-C) and Self-
of benefit finding and growth. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Report (QIDS-SR) in public sector patients with mood disorders: a
Psychology, 74, 797–816. psychometric evaluation. Psychological Medicine, 34, 73–82.
Linley, P., Joseph, S., Cooper, R., Harris, S., & Meyer, C. (2003). Positive Vrana, S., & Lauterbach, D. (1994). Prevalence of traumatic events and post-
and negative changes following vicarious exposure to the September 11 traumatic psychological symptoms in a nonclinical sample of college
terrorist attacks. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 16, 481–485. students. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 7, 289–302.
McAdams, D. P., Josselson, R., & Lieblich A. (Eds.), (2006). Identity and Weathers, F. W., Litz, B. T., Herman, D. S., Huska, J. A., & Keane, T. M.
story: Creating self in narrative. Washington, DC: American Psycho- (1993). The PTSD checklist (PCL). Unpublished scale available from the
logical Association. National Center for PTSD.
McFarland, C., & Alvaro, C. (2000). The impact of motivation on temporal Williams, R. M., Davis, M. C., & Millsap, R. E. (2002). Development of
comparisons: Coping with traumatic events by perceiving personal the cognitive processing of trauma scale. Clinical Psychology and
growth. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 327–343. Psychotherapy, 9, 349–360.

Copyright # 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 25: 817–822 (2011)

View publication stats

You might also like