You are on page 1of 8

Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services xxx (xxxx) xxx–xxx

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jretconser

The impact of different price promotions on customer retention


Jungkeun Kim1
Department of Marketing, Advertising, Retailing, and Sales, Auckland University of Technology, Private Bag 92006, Auckland 1142, New Zealand

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: This paper investigates the impact of different price promotion effects regarding two different types of service
Price promotion cost (i.e., a joining fee vs. a monthly fee) on retention intentions for new customers in the long-term service
Service retention industry. Based on the depreciation curve, mental accounting, and the escalation of commitment theory, we
Price framing predict that customers’ retention intentions will be higher when they start using a service with a free monthly fee
Depreciation
promotion rather than when they start it with a free joining fee promotion. This expectation was confirmed by
Long-term service
five studies; experiments, customer survey, and secondary data. The theoretical and managerial implications are
also discussed.

1. Introduction that price promotions did not increase either manufacturers’ or re-
tailers’ revenues. In sum, the previous literature has especially sug-
Some services or products require customers to use or subscribe to gested that price promotions are not effective tools for enhancing cus-
them in the relatively long term such as insurance services, sports tomer loyalty or a company's profits in the long run, especially in
membership, gyms, and satellite/cable TV. In these long-term contract repeated-purchase or non-long-term contract situations, such as grocery
industries, it is very important for marketers to keep or maintain their shopping.
customers in the long run. The previous research has suggested many Similarly, marketers in long-term contract industries face a trade-off
factors that influence customer retention. One factor is the impact of regarding sales or price promotions between (i) the short-term effect of
previous price or sales promotions on retention (e.g., Blattberg et al., attracting new customers to their initial service and (ii) the long-term
2009; Polo et al., 2011). Along with this stream of research, we examine negative effect of sales or price promotions, such as customer termi-
the impact of previous price promotion on customer retention in this nation. If marketers could reduce the negative effect of price promo-
paper. tions, they could enhance their overall positive retailing performance
Practically, sales or price promotions are the most frequently used based on price or sales promotions. Therefore the question, “How can
marketing tools for attracting new customers in these industries (e.g., marketers in long-term contract industries reduce the negative effect of price
Schweidel et al., 2008). The previous literature has suggested that sales promotions?” is the key research inquiry of this paper. We suggest and
or price promotions are some of the most effective tools for enhancing provide empirical evidence that different price-promotion framing may
short-term sales (e.g., Bemmaor and Mouchoux, 1991; Bucklin and reduce the negative effect, based on previous literature demonstrating
Lattin, 1992). However, another stream of research has suggested that differential price-framing effects (e.g., Gourville and Soman, 1998;
the long-term effect of price promotion on marketing outcomes is Soman and Gourville, 2001).
generally not positive, compared to the short-term effect (see Blattberg In particular, we investigate the retailing situation where the costs
and Nelson, 1990 for a summary of these findings). For instance, Mela are twofold. Specifically, many long-term services (e.g., golf member-
et al. (1997, 1998) suggested that frequent price promotions could in- ship, gym, satellite TV) require customers to pay two different types of
crease customers’ price sensitivity, ultimately resulting in reduced costs: (i) a fee that customers need to pay one time when they start
brand loyalty and/or brand equity. Similarly, Gedenk and Neslin (1999) using the service (i.e., a joining fee, installation fee, or initiation fee)
investigated the impact of in-store price promotions on purchase event and (ii) a fee that customers need to pay based on their usage for a fixed
feedback (i.e., influence of ‘the current purchase on future brand pre- period of time (i.e., monthly rate, annual fee, or monthly dues). For
ference’) and found that price promotions are negatively related to instance, a new customer at a fitness center needs to pay an initial
purchase event feedback, suggesting a negative long-term effect of price membership joining fee, as well as a monthly fee for the first month. At
promotions. In a different perspective, Srinivasan et al. (2004) found the same time, in order to attract new customers, many services

E-mail address: jkkim@aut.ac.nz.


1
The author would like to thank Professor Namwoon Kim for his wise comments.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jretconser.2017.10.007
Received 31 July 2016; Received in revised form 21 September 2017; Accepted 14 October 2017
0969-6989/ © 2017 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Please cite this article as: Kim, J., Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services (2017), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jretconser.2017.10.007
J. Kim Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services xxx (xxxx) xxx–xxx

frequently use price promotions, such as no joining fee or making the Simester, 2004; Ailawadi and Neslin, 1998; Gupta, 1988). Recently,
first month free (e.g., Schweidel et al., 2008). based on an examination of 20 different product categories in the US
In this paper, we focus on two different types of price promotion and UK, Bogomolova et al. (2015) found that price promotion was more
(i.e., price discounting for the joining fee vs. for the period rate) on common after the global financial crisis. Therefore, understanding price
consumers’ responses. More specifically, we use two economically si- promotion is very important for the retailing and service industries.
milar promotions, which permits us to test our argument, above and One of the research streams regarding price promotion involves
beyond the impact of the absolute amount of the discount. investigating the subtle differences of price promotion on consumer
Furthermore, we investigate the impact of previously different types of perceptions and responses regarding price promotions. For example,
price promotion on new customers’ retention intentions for subsequent some researchers have investigated the different types of framing on
periods, as well as existing customers’ responses. In particular, we ex- price discounts (i.e., amount-off vs. percentage-off format) in retail
pect that customers’ intentions to continue the service would be high settings (Bambauer-Sachse and Massera, 2015; Bitta et al., 1981). In the
when they start using the service with the free monthly fee promotion service domain, Soman and Gourville (2001) suggested that the simple
rather than with the free joining fee promotion. The differential de- price information of bundling could influence service usage intention in
preciation curve (e.g., Heath and Fennema, 1996) and escalation of repetitive consumption situations, such as ski packages. Similarly, other
commitment (e.g., Staw, 1981) explain our expectation. In addition, we researchers focus more on the effect of subtle differences in price pro-
also expect that the effect could have an opposite implication for ex- motion, such as the just-below price promotion effect (e.g., “up to 39%
isting customers. We elaborate the key hypothesis in the following off,” Banerjee et al., 2016), different price discount for trade-ins (Kim
section after reviewing the relevant literature. Then, we report five et al., 2011), different price discount for bundle offers (Janiszewski, and
experimental studies and one field survey and make our conclusions Cunha, 2004), simple price promotion cues, such as “sale” (Inman et al.,
regarding this research. 1990), payment frequency versus the amount discount in multiple
payment consumption (Jhang and Kim, 2009), or comparing the price
2. Theoretical arguments, primary data analysis, and prediction discount and bonus pack (Hardesty and Bearden, 2003). The impact of
price promotion is different for the types of customers. For example,
2.1. Customer retention Anderson and Simester (2004) found with field experiments that deeper
price discounting could increase sales from new customers, but de-
In this section, we review the factors influencing customer retention creased sales from established customers in durable products.
rates, especially in the relatively long-term context. One stream of re- Along the same lines of existing research, we investigate the impact
search regards the relationship between customer retention rates and of subtle price promotion effects by emphasizing different elements and
satisfaction (e.g., Bolton, 1998; Crosby and Stephens, 1987; Rust and holding the economic benefit equal; moreover, we focus on the dif-
Zahorik, 1993) or service quality (e.g., Bolton et al., 2006; Keaveney, ferent effects across new and existing customers in this paper.
1995; Rust et al., 1995). For example, Rust and Zahorik (1993) pro-
vided empirical evidence that customer satisfaction was one of the 2.3. Depreciation and mental accounting
greatest influences for retention rates in the retail banking market.
Bolton (1998) also found similar results from cellular customer, in that Imagine that two customers signed up to join a fitness center a few
customer satisfaction was strongly related to the duration of the re- months ago. The regular joining fee and monthly fee were the same.
lationship between customers and service providers, especially when Due to the price promotion, customer A was exempted from paying the
customers had a great deal of previous experience with the providers. In monthly fee and paid only the joining fee for the first month, whereas
addition, Bolton et al. (2006) showed that service quality was an im- customer B was exempted from paying the joining fee and paid only the
portant antecedent for renewal rates in the high-technology commu- monthly fee. Later they found that their interest in the fitness center
nication market. On the contrary, Keaveney (1995) found that was not high, and they considered stopping the service. Which one
switching behavior could be primarily explained by service failure. would be higher in terms of service retention intention, customer A or
Other factors for retention rates have been investigated in terms of B? In this paper, we answer this question.
various marketing mixes, such as loyalty programs (e.g., Bolton et al., Like the company's accounting system, people also update or de-
2000; Lewis, 2004; Yi and Jeon, 2003), advertising and sales promo- preciate the value of a product based on their history of purchase and
tions (e.g., Drèze and Bonfrer, 2008; Polo et al., 2011; Schweidel et al., usage. Heath and Fennema (1996, p. 95) define this mental deprecia-
2008; Uncles et al., 2013), and channel factors (e.g., Reinartz et al., tion as people's tendency to spread the fixed costs of their expenses over
2005), For example, Lewis (2004) provided theoretical and empirical time, mentally and implicitly. They also provide empirical evidence
evidence of the long-term effect of loyalty programs on customer re- that the depreciation curve is a linear function, in that people tend to
tention in grocery and drugstore data. reduce the value of a product linearly over time. Along similar lines,
One important factor from the marketing mix is the impact of price Gourville and Soman (1998) define payment depreciation as the “gra-
on customer retention (e.g., Blattberg et al., 2009; Lewis, 2004; dual adaptation to an upstream transaction cost” (p. 161) and provide
Keaveney, 1995; Polo et al., 2011). For instance, Blattberg et al. (2009) empirical evidence that students who paid their athletic facility as a
conceptually suggested the importance of price activities on retention. semiannual payment plan attended the facility less frequently as the
Empirically, Keaveney (1995) showed that high prices or unfair/de- months passed.
ceptive pricing could significantly reduce retention. Polo et al. (2011) Based on the concept of depreciation, we expect a similar depre-
further suggested that the price effect on customer retention was par- ciation for customers’ payments of long-term service. However, the
ticularly effective in the later stages of market liberalization. depreciation curve could be different for two types of fees: a joining fee
Compared to the current literature investigating the direct impact of and a monthly fee. This is because people might hold different mental
price on retention, this paper attempts to suggest a different perspective accounting perceptions (Thaler, 1985) toward joining fees and monthly
to increase customer retention by changing the different framing of fees. Mental accounting (Thaler, 1985; Thaler and Johnson, 1990)
price promotions, which is covered later. suggests that we maintain a different mental account for each type of
transaction. When a cost is incurred, we open an account related to the
2.2. Subtle price promotion effect cost, maintain/update the account with a cost and benefit analysis, and
close the account when the transaction ends or when the cost and
Price promotion is one of the important marketing/retailing tools to benefit are mostly matched.
attract new customers and maintain existing ones (e.g., Anderson and Specifically, we predict that people have relatively long period to

2
J. Kim Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services xxx (xxxx) xxx–xxx

100%

80%

Retenon rate
High Promoon Customers (n = 164,000)
60%
Low Promoon Customers (n = 112,000)

40%

20%

Fig. 1. Different depreciation curves for two types of fees. 0%

M+1
M+2
M+3
M+4
M+5
M+6
M+7
M+8
M+9
M+10
M+11
M+12
M+13
M+14
M+15
M+16
M+17
M+18
M+19
M+20
M+21
M+22
M+23
M+24
M+25
M+26
M+27
M+28
M+29
M+30
M
close the joining fee because such a fee is not related to the first period
Fig. 2. Study 1: retention rates for two types of customers.
of time, but is related to the benefit of using the service over a length of
time. For example, people could maintain a relatively higher remaining
value for their joining fee, even after using the service for a few months. were categorized two groups: high-promotion customers (n = 164,000)
In contrast, people have a relatively short period of time to stop the and low- promotion customers (n = 112,000), based on two different
monthly fee, in that the monthly cost is easily matched to the benefits channels of retailing. A typical promotion includes a discount for the
for each month. For example, people are expected to perceive a very price of the phone itself and a one-time subscription fee; therefore, this
low depreciated value for the previous monthly fee in the next month. price promotion practice could be categorized as a one-time joining/
In sum, we expect that the duration of the remaining value after the initiation fee discount rather than a monthly fee discount. The retention
depreciation could be longer for the joining fee (vs. the monthly fee), as rates were recorded from M+1 (February 2015) to M+30 (July 2017),
shown in Fig. 1. as shown in Fig. 2.
The results shows that the different levels of price promotion sig-
nificantly influenced the retention or survival rates. For the relatively
2.4. Main expectation
short-term context (i.e., within 12 months after the initial contract), the
average retention rate was 2.4% higher for the high- (vs. low-) pro-
Based on depreciation and mental accounting theory, we mainly
motion customers. For example, in the M+6 month (i.e., July 2015),
predict a differential price discounting effect of the joining fee and
customers in the high-promotion condition showed a relatively higher
monthly fee on service retention in the long run. Based on the different
retention rate (M = 97.2%) than those in the low-promotion condition
depreciation curve (i.e., Fig. 1), we expect different service retention
(M = 93.5%, difference = 3.7%; χ2 (1) = 2,213.3, p < .0001).
intentions for customers who previously had different price promotion
However, the retention rate crossed over in the +13 month. For the
benefits. For the scenario of this section, we expect that service reten-
subsequent years after the initial contract (i.e., M+13~M+24), the
tion should be higher for customer A, who paid only the joining fee
average retention rate was 6.5% higher for the low- (vs. high-) pro-
rather than customer B, who paid only the monthly fee in the first
motion customers. For the second year after the initial contract (i.e., M
month because of their differential depreciated values for their initial
+25~M+30), the difference between low- and high-promotion cus-
payments. Only customer A could have a relatively positive depreciated
tomers was more dramatic, in that the average retention rate was
value for the joining fee after a few months later from the time of
21.8% higher for the low- (vs. high-) promotion customers. For ex-
joining. We also predict that people's retention intentions will be in-
ample, in the M+30 month (i.e., July 2017), customers in the low-
fluenced by historic payments (i.e., escalation of commitment; Arkes
promotion condition showed a significantly higher retention rate (M =
and Blumer, 1985; Staw, 1981), given that people tend to make deci-
73.1%) than those in the low-promotion condition (M = 48.5%, dif-
sions in order to protect their previous efforts and/or monetary
ference = 24.6%; χ2 (1) = 16,586.9, p < .0001).
spending. Based on this expectation, we generate the formal Hypothesis
In sum, the secondary data analysis from actual customers’ behavior
1.
clearly shows a negative long-term effect of joining or initiation fee
Hypothesis 1:. The different type of price promotion will influence the discounts on χcustomer retention rates. Consequently, it is very im-
service retention intentions for customers. Specifically, service retention portant for retailers to reduce the negative effect of price promotions
intentions will be higher for those receiving a free monthly fee and paying a and to retain current customers in the long run. However, there is a
joining fee rather than for those receiving a free joining fee and paying a limitation of using secondary data to establish causality. In order to
monthly fee. enhance the internal validity of our findings of Study 1, we utilize more
controlled experiments in the following studies. We show that the
manipulation of subtle price promotion presentation could change the
3. Study 1: empirical evidence of the impact of price promotion on retention rate in the subsequent experimental studies, resulting in
retention rates strong evidence of a casual relationship.

As mentioned earlier, the previous literature has suggested that


4. Study 2: establishing the phenomenon
price promotions generate a negative long-term effect, especially in
repeated-purchase situations. We expected a similar negative effect of
The purpose of study 2 is to provide initial empirical evidence of the
price promotions involving a one-time joining or initiation fee on re-
impact of different price promotions on service retention intentions
tention rates for long-term contract industries. In order to provide
(i.e., H1). We mainly use a similar scenario of the previous section.
empirical evidence, the researcher investigated the impact of a joining
fee promotion on retention in the long term. The detailed retention rate
data were collected from one of three major mobile carrier companies 4.1. Method
in South Korea. The key customers (n = 276,000) started to use this
mobile service in January 2015 with brand-new top-10 premium Thirty-five undergraduates from a large university (48.6% female,
phones, such as the iPhone 6, Galaxy 5, Galaxy Note 5, etc. Customers average age = 21.9) participated in this study. They were randomly

3
J. Kim Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services xxx (xxxx) xxx–xxx

assigned to one of 2 conditions (type of price promotion: free joining fee price promotion: free joining fee vs. free monthly fee vs. no price pro-
vs. free monthly fee price promotion) in a between-subjects design. motion) in a between-subjects design.
Participants were first asked to imagine that they signed up for a The general procedure and task were similar to those of study 1,
fitness center 4 month ago. The joining fee of the fitness center was except for a few modifications. Participants were first asked to imagine
$100, and the bi-monthly fee was also $100. When they signed up for that they signed up for a fitness center 4 month ago. The joining fee of
the center, there was a special marketing promotion. Participants in the the fitness center was $45, and the monthly fee was $50. Participants in
free joining fee condition were informed that they were exempted from the no price promotion condition were asked to pay the regular prices.
paying the joining fee and needed to pay only a bi-monthly fee ($100). Participants in the free joining fee condition were informed that they
On the other hand, participants in the free monthly fee condition were needed to pay only a monthly fee ($50) from a special marketing
informed that they were exempted from paying the first bi-monthly fee promotion. Finally, participants in the free monthly fee condition were
and needed to pay only a joining fee ($100) for the first two months. informed that they needed to pay only a joining fee ($45) for the first
Then, all participants were further asked to imagine that they have month.
used the fitness center for 4 months and found that their motivation for Then, all participants were further asked to imagine similar in-
exercise was not as strong as previously expected. They only visited the structions in study 1, and they were asked to rate their retention in-
fitness center 4 or 5 times in the last month. After that, participants tentions along the same scale of study 1. In order to check the different
were asked to rate their retention intentions along a 9-point scale (1 = depreciation for the two prices, they were asked to rate their perceived
definitely I would stop using fitness center, 9 = definitely I would wastefulness for each payment (i.e., if I stop using the fitness center today,
continue using fitness center). Finally, they were thanked and de- my previous payment of the “joining fee [monthly fee]” would be) along a 9-
briefed. point scale (1 = not at all wasteful, 9 = very wasteful). Finally, they
were thanked and debriefed.
4.2. Results and discussion
5.2. Results
As expected, the different types of previous price promotion sig-
nificantly influenced service retention intentions (F (1, 33) = 4.25, p < 5.2.1. Perceived wastefulness
.05). Specifically, participants in the free monthly fee condition (M = We expect that perceived wastefulness would be higher when
4.63) showed higher retention intentions than those in the free joining people paying only for the joining fee (vs. monthly fee) decided to
fee condition (M = 3.05). Put differently, retention intentions were discontinue the service. The results confirmed our hypothesis.
higher for those who paid only the joining fee (vs. the bi-monthly fee). Specifically, for perceived wastefulness involving the previous joining
Therefore, this study provided initial evidence of the differential price fee, the main effect of the type of price promotion was significant (F (2,
promotion effect. 94) = 3.38, p < .05) as shown in Fig. 3. Post hoc tests indicated that
One weakness of this study is the different amounts for the joining perceived wastefulness for the joining fee was higher for the free
fee and the monthly fee. Since the joining fee is $100 and the monthly monthly fee condition (M = 5.81) than for the free joining fee condi-
fee is $50 per month (i.e., bi-monthly fee is $100), people might show tion (M = 4.45, p < .05). In addition, perceived wastefulness for the
higher service retention intentions when they paid a higher amount for joining fee in no price promotion condition (M = 5.94) was higher than
the joining fee. This is simply because of the sunk cost effect, indicating that for the free joining fee condition (M = 4.45, p < .05). In sum, the
that people tend to keep the previously high-paid option (e.g., Staw, wastefulness regarding the joining fee was higher for people those who
1981). In order to exclude this alternative explanation, we use the paid (vs. those who did not pay) the joining fee. On the other hand, the
monthly fee rather than the bi-monthly fee in study 3. In addition, we main effect of the type of price promotion was not significant for per-
use a scenario with a slightly higher price for the monthly fee (vs. ceived wastefulness involving the previous monthly fee, (F (2, 94) =
joining fee). 1.47, p > .10). Put differently, perceived wastefulness for the previous
monthly fee was not influenced by the earlier price promotion (M_no
5. Study 3: replicating and extending the phenomenon price promotion = 5.03 vs. M_free monthly fee = 5.35 vs. M_free joining fee =
6.10). In conclusion, the empirical data supported our key assumption
The purpose of study 3 is to provide additional evidence of the regarding the differential depreciation for the joining fee and the
impact of different price promotions on service retention intentions. In monthly fee with perceived wastefulness.
this study, we also include a no price promotion condition as the con-
trol condition. Arkes and Blumer (1985) suggested that trying to avoid 5.2.2. Retention intentions
appearing wasteful is related to the escalation of commitment. There- As expected and as shown in Fig. 3, the different types of previous
fore, we measured perceived wastefulness for each payment in order to price promotion significantly influenced service retention intentions (F
check our assumption regarding different depreciation curves from the (2, 94) = 4.82, p < .05). Post hoc tests confirmed that participants in
joining fee and the monthly fee. the free monthly fee condition (M = 4.84) showed higher retention
We expect people to have a higher level of being wasteful regarding intentions than those in the free joining fee condition (M = 3.35, p <
the previous payment of joining fees if they decide to discontinue using .01), thus replicating the previous result. In addition, participants in the
the fitness center. However, the same perception was not applied to the no price promotion condition (M = 4.37) also showed higher retention
previous payment of the monthly fee, because their depreciated value intentions than those in the free joining fee condition (M = 3.35, p <
for the monthly fee could be very small at the end of each month. This is .05). However, service retention intentions were similar between the
because the costs and benefits of the monthly fee are expected to be free monthly fee and no price promotion conditions (p > .10) (Fig. 3).
easily matched within the monthly period. Finally, we use a different
scenario, in which the amount of the joining fee is little less than that of 5.3. Follow-up study
the monthly fee in order to remedy the weakness of study 2.
The scenario-based method with extra instruction (i.e., asking about
5.1. Method retention intention a few months later) could have a limitation, espe-
cially in measuring retention intention. The purpose of follow-up is to
Ninety-seven undergraduate and postgraduate students from a large provide different evidence of the impact of different price promotions
university (66.7% female, average age = 22.6) participated in this on service retention decisions with a different dependent variable.
study. They were randomly assigned to one of 3 conditions (type of Specifically, we mainly use estimated months of fitness center use based

4
J. Kim Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services xxx (xxxx) xxx–xxx

Perceived wastefulnesss regarding the joining fee: (i) the standard joining fee; (ii) whether there
Retenon intenons was a price discount promotion on the joining fee at the time they
7 signed up for the gym; and (iii) the amount of the discounted joining fee
[if yes in (ii)]. The participants were asked to answer questions re-
5.94 5.81 garding the monthly fee: (i) the regular monthly fee; (ii) whether there
6
was a price discount promotion on the monthly fee at the time they
signed up for the gym; and (iii) the amount of the discounted monthly
5 4.84
4.45 fee [if yes in (ii)].
4.37
Third, participants were asked to rate their current usage (i.e., “How
4 often do you currently visit the gym/fitness center?”) along a 7-point
3.35 scale (1 = not at all, 7 = very often) and retention intention (i.e., “How
much do you want to keep/maintain this membership?) along two 7-
3
point scales (1 = I will stop the membership right now/I will cease the
membership immediately, 7 = I will keep the membership indefinitely/
2 I will maintain the membership forever). Finally, participants were
asked to provide their demographic information.
1
No price promoon Free monthly fee Free joining fee 6.2. Results
Fig. 3. Study 3: effects of different price discounts.
6.2.1. Customers grouping
on two different price promotion situations. We expect that people who First, based on the self-reported data, we categorized 4 different
get the free monthly free promotion (i.e. paying the joining fee) should groups: (i) Group #1: no joining fee & no monthly fee discounted group
show a higher service duration estimate than those who get a free (n = 45); (ii) Group #2: only monthly fee discounted group (n = 11);
joining fee (i.e., paying the same amount in the name of the monthly fee (iii) Group #3 only joining fee discounted group (n = 40); and (iv)
upon starting). Group #4: both joining fee & monthly fee discounted group (n = 31).
Eighty-three US adults recruited from Amazon MTurk (average age Their current membership fee across the 4 groups was not significantly
= 36.7, 32.5% female) participated. They were randomly assigned to different from each other (F (3, 122) = 1.30, p > .10).
one of 2 conditions (type of price promotion: free joining fee vs. free
monthly fee price promotion) in a between-subjects design. The general 6.2.2. Retention intention and other statistics
procedure was similar to that of study 2, except for a few modifications. Since the two measurements of retention intention were highly
Participants were first asked to imagine the similar scenario of study 2, correlated (Pearson correlation r = .77, p < .001), we used the mean of
in that the joining and monthly fee of the fitness center was $50 each. two variables in the main analysis. The detailed data pattern is illu-
Based on the different experimental conditions, they were asked to strated in Table 1. Furthermore, we mainly focus on the comparison
imagine that they only paid either the joining fee or the monthly fee between Group #2 (i.e., only monthly fee discounted group) and Group
due to a special price promotion. Then, they were asked to estimate the #3 (i.e., only joining fee discounted group). The type of previous price
minimum months they expected to use the fitness center. promotion significantly influenced current retention intention (F (1, 49)
The participants’’ estimated months to use the fitness center were = 4.02, p = .05). Specifically, gym membership retention intention
highly positively skewed (skewness = 5.78). Therefore, we used log was higher when members joined with the only monthly fee discounted
transformations. The result of an ANCOVA (i.e., age and exercise level promotion (i.e., Group #2; M = 6.00, SD = 1.16) than when they
as covariates & gender as a control variable) indicated that the type of joined with the only joining fee discounted promotion (i.e., Group #3;
price promotion significantly influenced the estimated months (F (1, M = 4.91, SD = 1.69).
78) = 4.13, p < .05). Specifically, participants in the free monthly fee Similar results were found for the duration of current membership
condition (M = 10.38 months) showed a higher estimation of months (F (1, 49) = 4.14, p < .05), in that the members of Group #2 (i.e., only
to use the fitness center than those in the free joining fee condition (M monthly fee discounted group; M = 35.18 months, SD = 49.18)
= 6.09 months). showed a longer duration of having a membership for the gym than
those in Group #3 (i.e., only joining fee discounted group; M = 16.28
6. Study 4: evidence from actual gym customers
Table 1
All previous experiments showed a causal relationship between the Study 4: effects of different price discounted promotions on outcomes.

type of price promotion and retention intention. In this study, we ex- Group #1 Group #2 Group #3 Group #4
tend the external validity of this paper by investigating the retention No joining Only Only joining Both joining fee
rate by different price promotions. fee & no monthly fee fee & monthly fee
monthly fee discounted discounted discounted
discounted group group group
6.1. Method group*

One hundred twenty-seven US adults (average age = 35.8, 56.9% Retention 5.06 6.00 4.91 5.29
female) from an online panel (Amazon MTurk) participated in this intention (1.14) (1.16) (1.69) (1.22)
Duration of 20.78 35.18 16.28 12.81
study. The study was intended only for those who were currently using current months months months months
a gym or fitness center at the time of the survey (i.e., May 2017). membership (20.88) (49.18) (17.75) (1.17)
First, after confirming whether they had current gym/fitness center Current usage 5.18 5.91 4.68 5.35
membership, participants were asked to provide the duration of their (1.21) (1.04) (1.66) (1.31)
Current monthly $37.82 $47.54 $29.87 $29.74
current membership and the name/brand of the gym/fitness center.
membership (30.13) (53.02) (27.66) (28.60)
Second, we explained the cost structure (e.g., “Usually, there are a fee
one-time joining fee and a fixed monthly fee for gym/fitness center
membership”). Then, we asked participants to respond to questions *The values in the parentheses are the standard deviations.

5
J. Kim Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services xxx (xxxx) xxx–xxx

months, SD = 17.75). In addition, the previous promotion also sig- 7.1. Method
nificantly influenced current usage (F (1, 49) = 5.47, p < .05), in that
the members of Group #2 (M = 5.91, SD = 1.04) showed a longer Fifty-two undergraduates from a large university (50.0% female,
duration of having a membership for the gym than those in Group #3 average age = 22.6) participated in this study. They were randomly
(M = 4.68, SD = 1.68). assigned to one of 2 conditions (type of price promotion: free joining fee
In order to investigate the mediating role of the duration of current vs. free monthly fee price promotion) in a between-subjects design.
membership and current usage on retention intention, we conducted a The general procedure and task were similar to those of study 1,
mediation analysis for Groups #2 and 3, based on Preacher and Hayes except for a few modifications. Participants were first asked to imagine
(2008); Hayes et al. (2017); SPSS macro modules #4 with 5000 boot- that they signed up for a fitness center 4 month ago and paid both a
strapping samples). The regression model (i.e., retention = f (current $100 joining fee and a $100 bi-monthly fee for the fitness center. In
usage, duration of current membership, type of price promotion)) was addition, there was a recent special marketing promotion for new
significant (R2 = .10, F (1, 49) = 5.47, p < .05). In detail, the current members. Participants in the free joining fee [free monthly fee] con-
usage was significant (b = .37, t = 2.61, p < .05), but the duration of dition were informed that new members would be exempted from
current membership was not (b = .01, t = .47, p > .10). More im- paying the joining fee [first bi-monthly fee] and needed to pay only a
portantly, the type of price promotion was not significant (b = − .56, t bi-monthly fee [a joining fee] ($100) for the first two months.
= − .96, p > .10), suggesting significant mediation. Furthermore, the After that, participants were asked to rate their overall satisfaction
results indicated an insignificant direct effect (95% bootstrap CI: − toward the membership of the fitness center along four 9-point scales (1
1.717, .607). Furthermore, the results showed a significant indirect = dissatisfied/ unhappy/ disappointed/ displeased, 9 = satisfied/
effect of current usage (95% bootstrap CI: − 1.158, − .046), but an happy/ delighted/ pleased, modified from Wirtz and Lee, 2003, Cron-
insignificant indirect effect of the duration of current membership (95% bach α = .93). They were also were asked to rate the perceived fairness
bootstrap CI: − .465, .147). The Sobel test was also marginally sig- (i.e., “fair” and “justified”) regarding the price they paid along two 9-
nificant for current usage (z = − 1.68, p < .10), but was not sig- point scales (1 = strongly disagree, 9 = strongly agree, based on
nificant for the duration of current membership (z = −. 41, p = .68). Greenberg, 1993, Cronbach α = .86) and retention intentions for using
Thus, we found a significant mediating role of current usage, suggesting the fitness center (1 = stop using, 9 = continue using).
that people who joined a gym with a discounted monthly fee [joining
fee] visited the gym more [less] frequently and, subsequently, showed 7.2. Results
higher [lower] retention intention in the future. However, the higher
retention intention was not mainly driven simply by a longer mem- 7.2.1. Overall satisfaction
bership duration. First, overall satisfaction scores across the two experimental con-
In sum, we found a significant effect of the previous price promotion ditions were lower than the neutral point (3.38 vs. ‘5’, t (51) = − 7.65,
on current usage and retention intention from actual gym members. p < .001), suggesting an overall negative effect of price promotion for
Similar to the results of the previous experiment, we found that cus- new customers. We expect higher satisfaction from participants in the
tomers who paid the joining fee (i.e., Group #2) attended the gym more free joining fee (vs. free monthly fee) condition. As expected in
frequently and had higher retention intention, as well. Hypothesis 2, the main effect of the different types of previous price
promotions was significant (F (1, 50) = 4.47, p < .05). Specifically,
participants in the free joining fee condition (M = 3.83) showed higher
7. Study 5: impact of price promotion on existing customers overall satisfaction than those in the free monthly fee condition (M =
2.96).
The purpose of study 5 is different from those of the previous study.
In the previous studies, we investigate the impact of different price 7.2.2. Perceived fairness
promotions in the perspective of new customers who received those We expect higher perceived fairness from participants in the free
benefits. In this study, we investigate the effect from the perspective of joining fee (vs. free monthly fee) condition. As expected, the different
existing customers who did not receive the price benefits, and conse- types of previous price promotion for new members significantly in-
quently, experienced a loss from not enjoying the price discount ben- fluenced perceived fairness for existing customers who paid the whole
efits. For existing customers, they will compare their payment structure price (F ( 1, 50) = 4.84, p < .05). Specifically, participants in the free
with that of new customers. When the current price promotion waives monthly fee condition (M = 2.96) showed a lower level of perceived
the joining fee, existing customers might compare this benefit (i.e., $0 fairness than those in the free joining fee condition (M = 4.02). A
for the joining fee for new customers) with their depreciated joining fee mediation analysis was conducted for testing the mediating role of
(i.e., depreciated joining fee: [$0 < $JF_depreciated < $JF_original] for perceived fairness on overall satisfaction. The results indicated a sig-
themselves), resulting in relatively low feelings of unfairness for the nificant indirect effect of perceived similarity (95% bootstrap CI: −
new price promotion. On the other hand, when the current price pro- 1.430, − .087), but an insignificant direct effect (95% bootstrap CI: −
motion waives the monthly fee, people might compare this benefit (i.e., .594, .367). The Sobel test was also significant (z = − 2.15, p < .05).
$0 for the monthly fee for new customers) with their current monthly Thus, we found a significant mediating role of perceived fairness.
fee (i.e., almost the full monthly fee for themselves), resulting in a high
level of feelings of unfairness (e.g., Bolton et al., 2003; Haws and 7.2.3. Retention intentions
Bearden, 2006) for the new price promotion. In sum, we expect that the Overall, retention intentions across the two experimental conditions
free monthly fee [vs. the free joining fee] price promotion will generate were similar to the neutral point (5. 46 vs. “5,” t (51) = 1.48, p > .01).
negative responses for existing customers who did not receive the price However, the different types of previous price promotions did not sig-
promotion benefits. nificantly influence service retention intentions (F (1, 50) = .45, p >
.10). These insignificant results could be attributed to the different
Hypothesis 2:. The different type of price promotion for new customers will
scenario of this study, in that the lack of motivation was not included in
influence service retention intentions for existing customers. Specifically,
this study.
existing customers’ evaluation of the price promotion for new customers will
be more negative for the price promotion of receiving a free monthly fee and
8. General discussion
paying a joining fee rather than for the price promotion of those receiving a
free joining fee and paying a monthly fee.
Price promotion is one of the common marketing/retailing tools for

6
J. Kim Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services xxx (xxxx) xxx–xxx

long-term service providers to attract new customers. In this paper, we the previous literature. For example, some literature regarding cogni-
investigate the impact of subtle difference of price promotions (e.g., a tive dissonance theory illustrates that a simple self- choice (vs. com-
free joining fee vs. a monthly fee for economically the same value) on puter-choice) action could influence the spread of preference (i.e., dif-
customers’ retention intentions for the subsequent period. We also in- ference in the evaluation of the chosen and rejection options) over
vestigate the impact of different price promotions on responses from multiple years (e.g., Sharot et al., 2012). For example, Sharot et al.
existing customers who did not receive the same benefit. Based on (2012) showed that simple choice action influences attitudes toward
mental accounting theory and the different depreciation curve for the the chosen and rejected options, even 3 years later. Based on this ob-
joining and monthly fees, we mainly find the differential price dis- servation, we believe that the experience of the previous promotion in
counting effect of the joining fee and monthly fee on service retention the initial stage could influence retention decisions substantially later.
intentions in the long run. The results of studies 2 and 3 suggest that Sixth, the results of this study could be moderated by promotion-
new customers who started to use the service by paying only the joining proneness, which can be defined as the “tendency to use promotional
fee [vs. paying only the monthly fee] showed higher service retention information as a reference to make purchase decisions”, Martínez and
intentions later on. In addition, the secondary data of study 1 and the Montaner (2006, p. 158). Since people who are high (vs. low) in pro-
survey from actual customers in study 4 add external validity to our motion-proneness are likely to be influenced significantly by the pro-
findings. In addition, study 5 illustrated the notion that different price motion in the attitude and choice task (Crespo-Almendros and Del
promotions influence existing customers, based on the comparison Barrio-García, 2016), we expect that the impact of the two different
process and on feelings of fairness. Moreover, these different price price promotions on future retention intention will be more for high (vs.
promotions revealed a moderating effect of customer type (i.e., existing low) promotion-prone customers. Future studies need to investigate the
vs new customers) on the price promotion effect. moderating role of promotion-proneness.
The current research has noteworthy theoretical implications. First, Finally, the study has implications for the literature of service re-
this paper could contribute to the depreciation effect on service reten- tention intentions. For example, our results are the same as those of
tion decisions. In contrast to payment depreciation (Gourville and Schweidel et al. (2008), in that both research studies suggest the im-
Soman, 1998), which focuses on the time gap between the purchase and portance of previous promotion effects on retention decisions. How-
consumption of a product, we focus on the impact of the previous price ever, our research provides more detailed predictions (i.e., different
promotion effect regarding future retention intentions in repeated price promotions) regarding service retention intentions.
consumption situations. In addition, we suggest that the depreciation The results of this research suggest important practical and man-
curve could depend on the types of price promotions. agerial implications. First, the results suggest a straightforward take-
Second, this study is related to preference reversal for economically home message for retailers and marketers. When retailers and service
equivalent options (e.g., Bambauer-Sachse and Massera, 2015; Kim, providers have the option of choosing the target of a price promotion
2017; Soman and Gourville, 2001; Thaler, 1985; Tversky and for two different prices, it is better to provide price discounting for a fee
Kahneman, 1981). For example, Tversky and Kahneman (1981) sug- related to a fixed period rather than a fee related to a one-time payment
gested that people's likelihood of purchasing a ticket for a play would for starting up the service, especially when focusing on new customers.
be higher when they lose $10 in cash rather than a pre-purchased ticket Second, Vigna and Malmendier (2006) analyzed long-term con-
of the same value. Soman and Gourville (2001) provide empirical evi- tracted gym members. They found that most of the members did not
dence that ski intensions for the last days were higher when a four-day frequently visit their contracted gym. For example, the average member
ticket was presented individually rather versus in a bundled format. In paid over $70 for the monthly fee, but visited the contacted gym only
this paper, we suggest subtle price framing (i.e., a different price pro- 4.3 times per month, indicating that members could save money if they
motion effect) as a source for preference reversal for the long-term pay the daily fee (e.g., $10) rather than the monthly contracted fee.
service industry. This low visiting rate was the main reason for canceling a long-term
Third, this study could importantly contribute to the literatures in contract. The results of our survey results from study 4 suggest that the
price fairness and the sunk cost effect. The implications of this study are type of previous price promotion significantly influences the current
similar to Bolton et al. (2003), emphasizing the importance of the usage of gym members. Specifically, members who paid the joining fee
communicating strategy for marketing activities in perceived fairness. (vs. monthly fee) actually visit the gym more frequently. This frequent
However, this research suggests that customers’ perceived (un)fairness visiting rate may increase customer satisfaction, resulting in long-term
could be influenced by the selection of the price promotion, which has high retention. Marketers of long-term contracts who need to manage
never been investigated. In addition, this study could extend our un- usage can utilize the results of this study.
derstanding of the sunk cost effect (Staw, 1981), given that the impact Third, based on the results of study 5, above recommendation could
of previous spending on subsequent decisions could depend on different be the opposite effect for existing customers who did not receive a si-
types of price promotion framing. milar benefit. Therefore, marketers should choose the best options
Fourth, the main finding of this paper is based on the assumption based on the importance of new vs. existing customers and service re-
that (i) consumers remember accurately what they paid upon joining tention rates. In addition, this different price promotion could address
the service and that (ii) this memory regarding previous payment in- the problem of low customer retention from the variety-seeking ten-
fluences future retention judgment. The first part of the assumption dency (e.g., Berné et al., 2001) or satiation (also see Redden, 2008).
could be supported by literature regarding memory-based price judg- Finally, the findings of study 1 have several implications. Compared
ment (e.g., Binkley and Bejnarowicz, 2003; Ofir et al., 2008), showing to the previous literature showing the effect of price promotion on re-
that consumers have a detailed and accurate memory regarding a tention, the results indicate that a heavy (vs. low) price promotion
previous purchase. The second part of the assumption could also be could have a positive effect in the relatively short-term context, but
supported by literature showing the effect of the previous payment could also have an unintended negative effect, especially in the rela-
amount (e.g., Arkes and Blumer, 1985; Thaler, 1985) or method (e.g., tively long-term context. Based on this observation, retailers may
Soman and Gourville, 2001) on future judgment. Therefore, we believe maximize their profit, based on the scope of their target market and
that the previous payment could have significantly influenced future market situations.
judgment. A few limitations in our research suggest the need for future re-
Fifth, related to the previous discussion, the actual data from studies search. One limitation is that we did not pay a great deal of attention to
1 and 4 suggest that the previous decision itself could have a significant the types of service categories. In addition, we did not cover the si-
impact on the subsequent period, such as over 2 years. This long-term tuation of partial discounts for the two types of pricing (e.g., 50%
effect of a relatively simple decision action is not surprising, based on discounting for both fees rather than 100% discounting for a single fee)

7
J. Kim Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services xxx (xxxx) xxx–xxx

or different types of price promotion (e.g., dollar discounting vs. per- timing on customer equity. J. Interact. Mark. 22 (1), 36–50.
Gedenk, K., Neslin, S.A., 1999. The role of retail promotion in determining future brand
centage discounting) effects on retention. Second, even though we ar- loyalty: its effect on purchase event feedback. J. Retail. 75 (4), 433–459.
gued that a different depreciation curve is the key underlying me- Gourville, J.T., Soman, D., 1998. Payment depreciation: the behavioral effects of tem-
chanism of our finding, other alternative explanations could influence porally separating payments from consumption. J. Consum. Res. 25 (2), 160–174.
Greenberg, J., 1993. Stealing in the name of justice: informational and interpersonal
the results. Future study needs to empirically test our key underlying moderators of theft reactions to underpayment inequity. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis.
mechanism more rigorously. Third, there are a few weaknesses in Study Process. 54 (1), 81–103.
4, in that poor memory could influence the results; moreover, we did Gupta, S., 1988. Impact of sales promotions on when, what, and how much to buy. J.
Mark. Res. 25 (4), 342–355.
not measure gym users’ perceived health benefits. Future study needs to Hardesty, D.M., Bearden, W.O., 2003. Consumer evaluations of different promotion types
consider these factors. Fourth, even though we used multiple methods and price presentations: the moderating role of promotional benefit level. J. Retail.
(i.e., secondary data, survey, and experiment) and sampling (i.e., stu- 79 (1), 17–25.
Haws, K.L., Bearden, W.O., 2006. Dynamic pricing and consumer fairness perceptions. J.
dents and online panels) in this study, future research needs to in-
Consum. Res. 33 (3), 304–311.
vestigate this issue by manipulating different promotions from actual Hayes, A.F., Montoya, A.K., Rockwood, N.J., 2017. The analysis of mechanisms and their
businesses in order to extend the external validity of our findings. contingencies: process versus structural equation modeling. Australas. Mark. J. 25
Finally, the main experiment based on scenario-based method could (1), 76–81.
Heath, C., Fennema, M.G., 1996. Mental depreciation and marginal decision making.
have the limitation of external validity. However, the scenario-based Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 68 (2), 95–108.
experimental method is frequently used for retailing and service re- Inman, J.J., McAlister, L., Hoyer, W.D., 1990. Promotion signal: proxy for a price cut? J.
search (e.g., Baumann and Hamin, 2014; Soman and Gourville, 2001). Consum. Res. 17 (1), 74–81.
Janiszewski, C., Cunha, M., 2004. The influence of price discount framing on the eva-
Future research might investigate this research from these perspectives, luation of a product bundle. J. Consum. Res. 30 (4), 534–546.
such as conducting a field experiment with different price promotion Jhang, J., Kim, J., 2009. Payment frequency discount vs. payment amount discount: the
manipulations. framing effect on preference reversal. In: In: McGill, A.L., Shavitt, S. (Eds.), NA-
Advances in Consumer Research 36. pp. 1021–1023.
In sum, this paper suggests one clear take-home message that dis- Keaveney, S.M., 1995. Customer switching behavior in service industries: an exploratory
counting the monthly fee (vs. the joining fee) is better for long-term study. J. Mark. 59 (2), 71–82.
retention for new customers. Kim, J., 2017. The influence of graphical versus numerical information representation
modes on the compromise effect. Mark. Lett. 28 (3), 397–409.
Kim, J., Rao, R.S., Kim, K., Rao, A.R., 2011. More or less: a model and empirical evidence
References on preferences for under-and overpayment in trade-in transactions. J. Mark. Res. 48
(1), 157–171.
Lewis, M., 2004. The influence of loyalty programs and short-term promotions on cus-
Ailawadi, K.L., Neslin, S.A., 1998. The effect of promotion on consumption: buying more
tomer retention. J. Mark. Res. 41 (3), 281–292.
and consuming it faster. J. Mark. Res. 35 (3), 390–398.
Martínez, E., Montaner, T., 2006. The effect of consumer's psychographic variables upon
Anderson, E.T., Simester, D.I., 2004. Long-run effects of promotion depth on new versus
deal-proneness. J. Retail. Consum. Serv. 13 (3), 157–168.
established customers: three field studies. Mark. Sci. 23 (1), 4–20.
Mela, C.F., Gupta, S., Lehmann, D.R., 1997. The long-term impact of promotion and
Arkes, H.R., Blumer, C., 1985. The psychology of sunk cost. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis.
advertising on consumer brand choice. J. Mark. Res. 34 (2), 248–261.
Process. 35 (1), 124–140.
Mela, C.F., Jedidi, K., Bowman, D., 1998. The long-term impact of promotions on con-
Bambauer-Sachse, S., Massera, L., 2015. Interaction effects of different price claims and
sumer stockpiling behavior. J. Mark. Res. 35 (2), 250–262.
contextual factors on consumers' reference price adaptation after exposure to a price
Ofir, C., Raghubir, P., Brosh, G., Monroe, K.B., Heiman, A., 2008. Memory-based store
promotion. J. Retail. Consum. Serv. 27, 63–73.
price judgments: the role of knowledge and shopping experience. J. Retail. 84 (4),
Banerjee, P.J., Tripathi, S., Sahay, A., 2016. When less is better than more: just-below
414–423.
discount in tensile price promotions. J. Retail. Consum. Serv. 31, 93–102.
Polo, Y., Sese, F.J., Verhoef, P.C., 2011. The effect of pricing and advertising on customer
Baumann, C., Hamin, H., 2014. Premium generic brand (PGB) choice vis-à-vis generic and
retention in a liberalizing market. J. Interact. Mark. 25 (4), 201–214.
national brands: a scenario comparison for self-use, family consumption and gift
Preacher, K.J., Hayes, A.F., 2008. Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and
giving in a food versus non-food and cross-cultural context. J. Retail. Consum. Serv.
comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behav. Res. Methods 40 (3),
21 (4), 492–501.
879–891.
Bemmaor, A.C., Mouchoux, D., 1991. Measuring the short-term effect of in-store pro-
Redden, J.P., 2008. Reducing satiation: the role of categorization level. J. Consum. Res.
motion and retail advertising on brand sales: a factorial experiment. J. Mark. Res. 28
34 (5), 624–634.
(2), 202–214.
Reinartz, W., Thomas, J.S., Kumar, V., 2005. Balancing acquisition and retention re-
Berné, C., Múgica, J.M., Yagüe, M.J., 2001. The effect of variety-seeking on customer
sources to maximize customer profitability. J. Mark. 69 (1), 63–79.
retention in services. J. Retail. Consum. Serv. 8 (6), 335–345.
Rust, R.T., Zahorik, A.J., 1993. Customer satisfaction, customer retention, and market
Binkley, J.K., Bejnarowicz, J., 2003. Consumer price awareness in food shopping: the case
share. J. Retail. 69 (2), 193–215.
of quantity surcharges. J. Retail. 79 (1), 27–35.
Rust, R.T., Zahorik, A.J., Keiningham, T.L., 1995. Return on quality (ROQ): making
Bitta, A.J., Monroe, K.B., McGinnis, J.M., 1981. Consumer perceptions of comparative
service quality financially accountable. J. Mark. 59 (2), 58–70.
price advertisements. J. Mark. Res. 18 (4), 416–427.
Schweidel, D.A., Fader, P.S., Bradlow, E.T., 2008. Understanding service retention within
Blattberg, R.C., Nelson, S.A., 1990. Sales Promotion. Prentice-Hall, New Jersey.
and across cohorts using limited information. J. Mark. 72 (1), 82–94.
Blattberg, R.C., Malthouse, E.C., Neslin, S.A., 2009. Customer lifetime value: empirical
Sharot, T., Fleming, S.M., Yu, X., Koster, R., Dolan, R.J., 2012. Is choice-induced pre-
generalizations and some conceptual questions. J. Interact. Mark. 23 (2), 157–168.
ference change long lasting? Psychol. Sci. 23 (10), 1123–1129.
Bogomolova, S., Dunn, S., Trinh, G., Taylor, J., Volpe, R.J., 2015. Price promotion
Soman, D., Gourville, J.T., 2001. Transaction decoupling: how price bundling affects the
landscape in the US and UK: depicting retail practice to inform future research
decision to consume. J. Mark. Res. 38 (1), 30–44.
agenda. J. Retail. Consum. Serv. 25, 1–11.
Srinivasan, S., Pauwels, K., Hanssens, D.M., Dekimpe, M.G., 2004. Do promotions benefit
Bolton, L.E., Warlop, L., Alba, J.W., 2003. Consumer perceptions of price (un)fairness. J.
manufacturers, retailers, or both? Manag. Sci. 50 (5), 617–629.
Consum. Res. 29 (4), 474–491.
Staw, B.M., 1981. The escalation of commitment to a course of action. Acad. Manag. Rev.
Bolton, R.N., 1998. A dynamic model of the duration of the customer's relationship with a
6 (4), 577–587.
continuous service provider: the role of satisfaction. Mark. Sci. 17 (1), 45–65.
Thaler, R., 1985. Mental accounting and consumer choice. Mark. Sci. 4 (3), 199–214.
Bolton, R.N., Kannan, P.K., Bramlett, M.D., 2000. Implications of loyalty program
Thaler, R.H., Johnson, E.J., 1990. Gambling with the house money and trying to break
membership and service experiences for customer retention and value. J. Acad. Mark.
even: the effects of prior outcomes on risky choice. Manag. Sci. 36 (6), 643–660.
Sci. 28 (1), 95–108.
Tversky, A., Kahneman, D., 1981. The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice.
Bolton, R.N., Lemon, K.N., Bramlett, M.D., 2006. The effect of service experiences over
Science 211 (4481), 453–458.
time on a supplier's retention of business customers. Manag. Sci. 52 (12), 1811–1823.
Uncles, M.D., East, R., Lomax, W., 2013. Good customers: the value of customers by mode
Bucklin, R.E., Lattin, J.M., 1992. A model of product category competition among grocery
of acquisition. Australas. Mark. J. 21 (2), 119–125.
retailers. J. Retail. 68 (3), 271–294.
Vigna, S.D., Malmendier, U., 2006. Paying not to go to the gym. Am. Econ. Rev. 96 (3),
Crespo-Almendros, E., Del Barrio-García, S., 2016. Do online discounts and free gifts
694–719.
damage brand image of service? The moderating role of promotion-proneness. Serv.
Wirtz, J., Lee, M.C., 2003. An examination of the quality and context-specific applicability
Bus. 10 (1), 31–58.
of commonly used customer satisfaction measures. J. Serv. Res. 5 (4), 345–355.
Crosby, L.A., Stephens, N., 1987. Effects of relationship marketing on satisfaction, re-
Yi, Y., Jeon, H., 2003. Effects of loyalty programs on value perception, program loyalty,
tention, and prices in the life insurance industry. J. Mark. Res. 24 (4), 404–411.
and brand loyalty. J. Acad. Mark. Sci. 31 (3), 229–240.
Drèze, X., Bonfrer, A., 2008. An empirical investigation of the impact of communication

You might also like