You are on page 1of 6

Received: 7 October 2019 Revised: 15 February 2020 Accepted: 17 February 2020

DOI: 10.1002/eat.23254

BRIEF REPORT

Avoidance coping during mealtimes predicts higher eating


disorder symptoms

Irina A. Vanzhula MS1 Margarita Sala MA2


| Caroline Christian BS1 |
|
Rowan A. Hunt BA1 | Ani C. Keshishian BA1 | Valerie Z. Wong3 |
Sarah Ernst BS1 | Samantha P. Spoor BS1 | Cheri A. Levinson PhD1

1
Department of Psychological and Brain
Sciences, University of Louisville, Louisville, Abstract
Kentucky Objective: Eating disorders (EDs) are characterized by significant anxiety during meal-
2
Southern Methodist University, Dallas, Texas
time that contributes to food avoidance and weight loss. Individuals with EDs commonly
3
Department of Psychology, Yale University,
New Haven, Connecticut use avoidance coping (e.g., distraction) to tolerate meals and comply with meal plans.
Although this strategy may be effective short term, a large body of anxiety literature sug-
Correspondence
Irina A. Vanzhula, Department of Psychological gests that avoidance can lead to worsening of psychological symptoms long term.
and Brain Sciences, University of Louisville, Method: The current study (N = 66 individuals diagnosed with ED) used ecological
Louisville, KY.
Email: irina.vanzhula@louisville.edu momentary assessment (EMA) to examine the short-term and long-term associations
of avoidance coping on ED symptoms.
Action Editor: Ruth Weissman
Results: Distraction during meals predicted a reduction in anxiety in the short term,
and both distraction and avoidance of emotions predicted increases in excessive
exercise in the short term. Distraction and avoidance of emotions predicted increases
in bulimic symptoms 1 month after completion of EMA.
Discussion: These results are consistent with prior literature on avoidance and sug-
gest that avoidance coping during meals may contribute to the increase of ED behav-
iors in the long term. Coping strategies that encourage approach and tolerance of
difficult thoughts and emotions (e.g., acceptance-based strategies) rather that avoid-
ance coping may promote longer-term symptom reduction.

KEYWORDS

anxiety, avoidance, eating disorders

1 | I N T RO DU CT I O N Avoidance coping (i.e., the use of avoidance of thoughts,


avoidance or denial of emotions; Christiano & Mizes, 1997) during
Individuals with eating disorders (EDs) experience heightened anxiety mealtimes is common in individuals with EDs (Cowdrey, Stewart, Rob-
and fear during mealtimes, which can foster avoidance of eating and erts, & Park, 2013). Avoidance coping strategies employed during
reduced food intake (Levinson & Byrne, 2015; Steinglass et al., 2010). mealtimes include avoiding distressing foods, eating alone, food rituals
Low caloric intake during meals contributes to weight loss and poor (e.g., eating foods in a specific order; tearing food), and distraction
treatment outcomes (Gianini et al., 2015; Treasure, Cardi, & Kan, 2012). (i.e., redirecting attention to less distressing stimuli; Ulian et al., 2013;
Factors contributing to mealtime anxiety (e.g., worry about weight gain Long, Meyer, Leung, & Wallis, 2011). Avoidance coping has been
and perfectionistic cognitions) predict a future increase in ED behaviors widely studied in the anxiety literature (Berman, Wheaton, Mcgrath, &
(Levinson et al., 2018). Coping with mealtime anxiety presents an Abramowitz, 2010), showing that avoidance reduces anxiety in the
important area for research and a possible intervention target. short term, but maintains anxiety in the long term, creating a

Int J Eat Disord. 2020;1–6. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/eat © 2020 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. 1


2 VANZHULA ET AL.

reinforcing cycle of anxiety-avoidance (Beesdo-Baum et al., 2012; attributes typically associated EDs. It has demonstrated good internal
Wheaton, Gershkovich, Gallagher, Foa, & Simpson, 2018). consistency, as well as good convergent and discriminant validity
Individuals with EDs may avoid eating a meal out of fear of (Garner & Garfinkel, 1982). Three subscales of the EDI-2 were
weight gain, which prevents learning that eating does not lead to included: Drive for Thinness, Bulimic Symptoms, and Body Dissatis-
immediate or substantial weight gain. This experience “teaches” the faction. In the current sample, internal consistency for each subscale
individuals that it was avoidance of eating that prevented weight gain was excellent (α = .99 for all subscales).
and fosters further avoidance to prevent a feared catastrophe. Con-
tinued efforts to avoid such anxiety may increase ED behaviors in the
short term, such as restricting, excessive exercising, and purging, 2.4 | EMA measures
which help individuals escape anxiety and distress (Kleifield, Wag-
ner, & Halmi, 1996). However, after the temporary relief from anxiety, Daily Life Daily Habits Questionnaire (Levinson et al., 2018) assesses
anxiety usually returns at higher levels, which may then drive the con- cognitions, behaviors, and emotions around mealtimes (Levinson
tinual reliance on food avoidance and ED behaviors, thereby rein- et al., 2018). Intraclass correlation coefficient was computed for each
forcing ED symptoms over the long term (Levinson et al., 2018). item (see Table 1); values above 0.75 indicate excelled reliability
Although research shows that avoidance coping (specifically emotion- (Koo & Li, 2016). Avoidance coping was measured using two items
focused coping and distraction) is positively associated with maladap- (“I tried to avoid my emotions during the meal” and “I tried to distract
tive eating behaviors (MacNeil, Esposito-Smythers, Mehlenbeck, & myself during the meal or snack”). Anxiety was measured with one
Weismoore, 2012; Spoor, Bekker, Van Strien, & van Heck, 2007), no item (“How anxious are you currently feeling about your last meal or
study to date has examined avoidance coping in individuals with EDs snack?”). Additionally, participants were asked how much they had
during mealtimes and its longitudinal effects on ED symptoms. used the following behaviors in the time since their last meal or snack:
The current study used ecological momentary assessment (EMA) to (a) restriction, (b) excessive exercise, (c) body-checking, (d) weighing,
examine the possible role of avoidance coping during meals on the main- (e) binge eating, and (f) compensatory behaviors. Items were mea-
tenance of ED cognitions and behaviors. We aimed to test both the sured on a 6-point Likert-type scale (1 = not at all, 6 = extremely).
short-term impact of avoidance coping on anxiety and ED behaviors, as
well as the longer-term impact of avoidance coping on ED symptoms.
We hypothesized that emotional avoidance and distraction during meal- 2.5 | Data analysis
time would predict (a) a reduction of anxiety in the short term, but predict
an increase of ED behaviors (e.g., purging and excessive exercise) short Data were analyzed with SPSS using multilevel modeling. We used
term and (b) higher overall ED symptoms in the long term. within-subject cross-lag panel analyses (Hamaker, Kuiper, & Grasman,
2015). In these analyses, we disaggregated time varying predictors

2 | METHODS TABLE 1 Descriptive statistics

Baseline 1-month follow-up


2.1 | Procedure ED symptoms M (SD) M (SD)
Drive for thinness 4.60 (.90) 4.67 (.89)
This study is a secondary data analysis as described in Levinson et al.
Bulimic symptoms 2.18 (.96) 2.12 (.91)
(2018). Please see Levinson et al., 2018 for recruitment and data col-
Body dissatisfaction 4.71 (.94) 4.84 (.78)
lection procedures. All procedures were approved by the Washington
EMA items ICC M (SD)
University Institutional Review Board.
Emotional avoidance (“I tried to .34 2.83 (1.43)
avoid my emotions during the meal”)

2.2 | Participants Distraction (“I tried to distract myself .55 2.89 (1.43)
during the meal or snack”)
Anxiety (“How anxious are you currently .56 2.62 (1.63)
Participants were 66 individuals diagnosed with an ED. Diagnoses
feeling about your last meal or snack?”)
were assessed prior to beginning the EMA protocol using the ED
Restriction .23 2.46 (1.71)
Diagnostic Scale (Stice, Telch, & Rizvi, 2000). Please see Supplemental
Binge eating .03 1.28 (1.00)
Table 1 for demographic and diagnostic information.
Compensatory behaviors −.21 1.31 (.97)
Excessive exercise −.38 1.63 (1.30)

2.3 | Self-report measures Weighing .41 1.59 (1.45)


Body checking .08 3.02 (1.87)
Eating Disorder Inventory-2 (EDI-2; Garner, Olmstead, & Polivy, 1983). Abbreviations: ED, eating disorder; EMA, ecological momentary
The EDI-2 is a self-report questionnaire measuring psychological assessment; ICC, intraclass correlation coefficient.
VANZHULA ET AL. 3

(TVPs) into the participants' average across all assessments (TVPmean; In the results outlined below, we only report TVPdev effects, as
the between-person component) and the participants' deviation from these effects represent quasi-casual effects of the predictor on the
their own mean (TVP TVPdev the within-person component; outcome (i.e., allowing us to understand how changes in the predictor
TVPdev = TVPraw − TVPmean) (Hamaker et al., 2015). variable influence changes in the outcome variable; Hamaker et al.,
We tested lagged relationships, such that we tested whether the 2015; Curran & Bauer, 2011). However, we included both TVPmean
time varying predictors (at time t) predicted subsequent changes in and TVPdev effects in our analyses, as including both is necessary to
the dependent variables (at time t + 1) controlling for the dependent assess TVPdev effects (Hamaker et al., 2015).
variable at time t. The intercept was included as a random effect, and
we used an AR1 covariance structure. In addition, we tested whether
mean levels of distraction and emotion avoidance predicted ED symp- 3 | RE SU LT S
toms in the long term (at time 2), while controlling for baseline ED
symptoms (at time 1). Our model can be described as: 3.1 | Descriptive statistics

Outcomeðt + 1Þi = b0i + b1i*TVPmeani + b1i*TVPdevit + b2i*outcometi + εit : See Table 1 for means and SDs for all variables. EMA compliance aver-
aged 74% (range = 14–100%).
Outcome(t + 1)i is the outcome (e.g., ED behaviors and meal anxi-
ety) for individual i time point at t + 1. TVPmeani is the participants'
mean across all 28 assessments for the predictor variable 3.2 | Distraction
(e.g., distraction and emotion avoidance). TVPdevit is the participant's
deviation from the mean at each assessment for the predictor vari- Higher distraction significantly predicted lower subsequent meal anxi-
able. Outcometi is the outcome variable for individual i at time point t. ety and higher subsequent excessive exercise but did not other ED

T A B L E 2 Emotional avoidance and


Distraction
distraction predicting meal anxiety and
eating disorder behaviors Over 4 hr (EMA) b SE p d
Anxiety −.08 .03 .027 −.20
Restriction .00 .03 .914 .01
Binge eating −.02 .02 .316 −.01
Compensatory behaviors −.01 .02 .603 −.03
Excessive exercise .04 .02 .042 .14
Weighing .00 .02 .960 .00
Body-checking −.04 .03 .184 −.10
1-month follow-up β SE p Part r F Df p R2
Drive for thinness .09 .39 .300 .08 55.80 2,48 <.001 .69
Bulimic symptoms .13 .26 .031 .13 125.19 2,50 <.001 .83
Body dissatisfaction −.02 .38 .786 −.02 78.38 2,50 <.001 .75

Emotional avoidance

Over 4 hr (EMA) b SE p d
Anxiety −.06 .03 .075 −.17
Restriction −.01 .03 .753 −.02
Binge eating −.02 .02 .398 −.07
Compensatory behaviors −.01 .02 .621 −.03
Excessive exercise .04 .02 .039 .14
Weighing .00 .02 .924 .00
Body-checking −.04 .03 .238 −.09
1-month follow-up β SE p Part r F Df p R2
Drive for thinness .12 .38 .168 .11 57.20 2,48 <.001 .69
Bulimic symptoms .13 .26 .029 .13 125.54 2,50 <.001 .83
Body dissatisfaction .01 .37 .934 .01 78.24 2,50 <.001 .75

Note: EMA, ecological momentary assessment.


Bold indicates statistically significant findings at p < .05.
4 VANZHULA ET AL.

behaviors (see Table 2). Higher mean levels of distraction also should assess for habitual nature of ED behaviors (Verplanken, 2003)
predicted higher bulimic symptoms at 1-month follow-up, but not and should control for duration of illness.
drive for thinness or body dissatisfaction. As hypothesized, we found that both emotional avoidance and
distraction during meals predicted higher bulimic symptoms 1 month
later. These results are consistent with prior findings suggesting that
3.3 | Emotion avoidance avoidance coping predicts worsening symptoms in depression and
anxiety disorders (Grant et al., 2013). Interestingly, avoidance coping
Higher emotion avoidance significantly predicted higher subsequent did not predict changes in drive for thinness or body dissatisfaction.
excessive exercise, but not other ED behaviors or meal anxiety (see These findings suggest that ED behaviors, but not cognitions may
Table 2). Higher mean levels of emotion avoidance also predicted increase in response to avoidance coping as a function to relieve anxi-
higher bulimic symptoms at 1-month follow-up, but not drive for thin- ety. These results are consistent with theories that posit that ED
ness or body dissatisfaction. behaviors are a maladaptive coping strategy (Blackburn, Johnston,
Additionally, when we analyzed the data without male participants Blampied, Popp, & Kallen, 2006; Merwin, 2011). For example, emo-
and only with AN participants and age as a covariate, the pattern of results tional avoidance and distraction may prevent individuals with EDs
did not change, with distraction and avoidance continuing to be significant from testing feared predictions (i.e., gaining weight after a meal),
predictors of excessive exercise and bulimic symptoms (ps < .02). which may then strengthen the fear, reinforce reliance on avoidance
coping, and increase the need to rely on ED behaviors (as a form of
avoidance coping) to prevent the feared catastrophe (i.e., future
4 | DISCUSSION weight gain). However, it is entirely possible that over a longer than
1-month time period avoidance coping might also impact cognitions.
This study found initial support for the anxiety-avoidance coping Further research is needed to test this theory.
model in EDs (Krypotos, Effting, Kindt, & Beckers, 2015). Consistent These findings have implications for both meal therapy in treat-
with our hypotheses, distraction avoidance coping during meals ment centers and meal planning outside of treatment. Instead of
predicted lower meal anxiety in the short term and predicted higher encouraging distraction and emotional avoidance during meal therapy,
excessive exercise in the short term and bulimic symptoms in the long clinicians may teach approach strategies during meals, while providing
term. These results are consistent with prior literature on avoidance education on the consequences of avoidance coping. If our results
coping in psychopathology (Beesdo-Baum et al., 2012; Wheaton replicate in meal therapy settings, approach strategies may lead to
et al., 2018) and indicate that avoidance coping may contribute to the increased food intake and reduced ED behaviors in the long term.
maintenance of EDs. Interventions that are focused on approach and tolerating emotions
Distraction, but not emotional avoidance, predicted lower meal during meals (i.e., exposure and response prevention [ERP]; mindful
anxiety several hours later. Despite a reduction in anxiety, avoidance acceptance) may lead to improved long-term outcomes. For example,
coping (both distraction and emotional avoidance) predicted higher ERP has been found to reduce fear of foods and food avoidance
excessive exercise several hours after a meal. Contrary to our expecta- behaviors, is associated with increased caloric intake (Steinglass et al.,
tions, avoidance coping did not predict any other ED behaviors in the 2012), and is feasible to implement during meal therapy in an inpa-
short term. A possible explanation is that most ED behaviors (i.e., body tient setting (Farrell et al., 2019). Acceptance-based strategies may be
checking and purging) are habitual (Walsh, 2013) and may occur after beneficial as they both reduce anxiety in the short term (Helbig-Lang,
meals regardless of avoidance coping because of the habitual nature. Rusch, Rief, & Lincoln, 2015), but, unlike distraction, also improve
Excessive exercise is slightly different than other habitual ED behav- long-term outcomes in ED symptoms (Espel, Goldstein, Manasse, &
iors, such that it is a common compensatory behavior in EDs, but is con- Juarascio, 2016). Future research should continue investigating long-
ceptualized as a compulsion in response to obsessive cognitions about term effects of approach-based meal therapy interventions.
weight or shape, similar to compulsions in obsessive–compulsive disor- This study has several limitations. Our sample size, although large
der (OCD; Davis & Kaptein, 2006; Holland, Brown, & Keel, 2014). In for a clinical EMA sample, is relatively small, and our analyses may be
OCD, avoidance is related to an increased urge to perform a compul- underpowered. The symptoms were assessed using single items
sion, which in our sample may explain why higher excessive exercise designed for this EMA study and the reliability and validity of the mea-
the primary behavior individuals engage in after avoidance coping sures are unknown, though we attempted to adhere to standardized
(Gillan et al., 2014). Additionally, excessive exercise was more common measures. Although surveys were sent at semi-randomized intervals
in our primarily AN sample than other types of compensatory behaviors scheduled around typical mealtimes, we were not always able to assess
such as purging, and therefore we had more variability and power to cognitions immediately following meals. Further, our measures are retro-
detect effects for exercise. Further exercise is a more goal-directed spective and limited by self-report biases. Additionally, we did not assess
action than other types of compensatory behaviors such as restriction, other coping styles. Finally, we did not account for how types of emotion
body checking, and weighing as it requires more time and effort and is regulation strategies may differ across age groups and varied ED diagno-
less likely to occur habitually “on the fly.” Future replication studies ses (Anderson et al., 2018; Danner, Sternheim, & Evers, 2014).
VANZHULA ET AL. 5

To our knowledge this is the first longitudinal investigation of the Garner, D. M., & Garfinkel, P. E. (1982). Body image in anorexia nervosa:
role of avoidance coping in maintaining ED symptoms. This study rep- Measurement, theory and clinical implications. The International Jour-
nal of Psychiatry in Medicine, 11, 263–284.
resents a first step in understanding how avoidance functions during
Garner, D. M., Olmstead, M. P., & Polivy, J. (1983). Development and
meals finding that distraction and emotional avoidance are associated validation of a multidimensional eating disorder inventory for
with increased ED behaviors in the long term. Future research should anorexia nervosa and bulimia. International Journal of Eating Disorders,
seek to replicate these findings in a larger sample and examine a wider 2, 15–34.
Gillan, C. M., Morein-Zamir, S., Urcelay, G. P., Sule, A., Voon, V., Apergis-
range of coping strategies.
Schoute, A. M., … Robbins, T. W. (2014). Enhanced avoidance habits in
obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 75, 631–638.
DATA AVAI LAB ILITY S TATEMENT Gianini, L., Liu, Y., Wang, Y., Attia, E., Walsh, B. T., & Steinglass, J. (2015).
The data that support the findings of this study are available on Abnormal eating behavior in video-recorded meals in anorexia
nervosa. Eating Behaviors, 19, 28–32.
request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly
Grant, D. M., Wingate, L. R., Rasmussen, K. A., Davidson, C. L., Slish, M. L.,
available due to privacy or ethical restrictions. Rhoades-Kerswill, S., … Judah, M. R. (2013). An examination of the
reciprocal relationship between avoidance coping and symptoms of
ORCID anxiety and depression. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 32,
878–896.
Irina A. Vanzhula https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8323-1290
Hamaker, E. L., Kuiper, R. M., & Grasman, R. P. (2015). A critique of the
Margarita Sala https://orcid.org/0000-0002-6775-9607
cross-lagged panel model. Psychological Methods, 20, 102–116.
Rowan A. Hunt https://orcid.org/0000-0002-7626-7049 Holland, L. A., Brown, T. A., & Keel, P. K. (2014). Defining features of
Cheri A. Levinson https://orcid.org/0000-0002-8098-6943 unhealthy exercise associated with disordered eating and eating disor-
der diagnoses. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 15, 116–123.
Helbig-Lang, S., Rusch, S., Rief, W., & Lincoln, T. M. (2015). The strategy
RE FE R ENC E S
does not matter: Effects of acceptance, reappraisal, and distraction on
Anderson, L. K., Claudat, K., Cusack, A., Brown, T. A., Trim, J., Rockwell, R., the course of anticipatory anxiety in social anxiety disorder. Psychology
… Kaye, W. H. (2018). Differences in emotion regulation difficulties and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 88, 366–377.
among adults and adolescents across eating disorder diagnoses. Jour- Kleifield, E. I., Wagner, S., & Halmi, K. A. (1996). Cognitive-behavioral
nal of Clinical Psychology, 74(10), 1867–1873. treatment of anorexia nervosa. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 19,
Berman, N. C., Wheaton, M. G., McGrath, P., & Abramowitz, J. S. (2010). 715–737.
Predicting anxiety: The role of experiential avoidance and anxiety sen- Koo, T. K., & Li, M. Y. (2016). A guideline of selecting and reporting
sitivity. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 24, 109–113. Intraclass correlation coefficients for reliability research. Journal of Chi-
Beesdo-Baum, K., Jenjahn, E., Höfler, M., Lueken, U., Becker, E. S., & ropractic Medicine, 15(2), 155–163.
Hoyer, J. (2012). Avoidance, safety behavior, and reassurance Krypotos, A. M., Effting, M., Kindt, M., & Beckers, T. (2015). Avoidance
seeking in generalized anxiety disorder. Depression and Anxiety, 29, learning: A review of theoretical models and recent developments.
948–957. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 9, 189.
Blackburn, S., Johnston, L., Blampied, N., Popp, D., & Kallen, R. (2006). An Levinson, C. A., Sala, M., Fewell, L., Brosof, L. C., Fournier, L., & Lenze, E. J.
application of escape theory to binge eating. European Eating Disorders (2018). Meal and snack-time eating disorder cognitions predict eating
Review, 14(1), 23–31. disorder behaviors and vice versa in a treatment seeking sample: A
Christiano, B., & Mizes, J. S. (1997). Appraisal and coping deficits associ- mobile technology based ecological momentary assessment study.
ated with eating disorders: Implications for treatment. Cognitive and Behaviour Research and Therapy, 105, 36–42.
Behavioral Practice, 4, 263–290. Levinson, C. A., & Byrne, M. (2015). The fear of food measure: A novel
Cowdrey, F. A., Stewart, A., Roberts, J., & Park, R. J. (2013). Rumination measure for use in exposure therapy for eating disorders. International
and modes of processing around meal times in women with anorexia Journal of Eating Disorders, 48, 271–283.
nervosa: Qualitative and quantitative results from a pilot study. Long, S., Meyer, C., Leung, N., & Wallis, D. J. (2011). Effects of distrac-
European Eating Disorders Review, 21, 411–419. tion and focused attention on actual and perceived food intake in
Curran, P. J., & Bauer, D. J. (2011). The disaggregation of within-person females with non-clinical eating psychopathology. Appetite, 56,
and between-person effects in longitudinal models of change. Annual 350–356.
Review of Psychology, 62(1), 583–619. MacNeil, L., Esposito-Smythers, C., Mehlenbeck, R., & Weismoore, J.
Danner, U. N., Sternheim, L., & Evers, C. (2014). The importance of dis- (2012). The effects of avoidance coping and coping self-efficacy on
tinguishing between the different eating disorders (sub)types when eating disorder attitudes and behaviors: A stress-diathesis model. Eat-
assessing emotion regulation strategies. Psychiatry Research, 215(3), ing Behaviors, 13, 293–296.
727–732. Merwin, R. M. (2011). Anorexia nervosa as a disorder of emotion regula-
Davis, C., & Kaptein, S. (2006). Anorexia nervosa with excessive exercise: tion: Theory, evidence, and treatment implications. Clinical Psychology:
A phenotype with close links to obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychi- Science and Practice, 18(3), 208–214.
atry Research, 142(2–3), 209–217. Spoor, S. T., Bekker, M. H., Van Strien, T., & van Heck, G. L. (2007). Rela-
Espel, H. M., Goldstein, S. P., Manasse, S. M., & Juarascio, A. S. (2016). tions between negative affect, coping, and emotional eating. Appetite,
Experiential acceptance, motivation for recovery, and treatment out- 48, 368–376.
come in eating disorders. Eating and Weight Disorders-Studies on Steinglass, J. E., Sysko, R., Mayer, L., Berner, L. A., Schebendach, J.,
Anorexia, Bulimia and Obesity, 21, 205–210. Wang, Y., … Walsh, B. T. (2010). Pre-meal anxiety and food intake in
Farrell, N. R., Bowie, O. R., Cimperman, M. M., Smith, B. E., anorexia nervosa. Appetite, 55, 214–218.
Riemann, B. C., & Levinson, C. A. (2019). Exploring the preliminary Steinglass, J., Albano, A. M., Simpson, H. B., Carpenter, K., Schebendach,
effectiveness and acceptability of food-based exposure therapy for J., & Attia, E. (2012). Fear of food as a treatment target: Exposure and
eating disorders: A case series of adult inpatients. Journal of Experimen- response prevention for anorexia nervosa in an open series. Interna-
tal Psychopathology. https://doi.org/10.1177/2043808718824886 tional Journal of Eating Disorders, 45, 615–621.
6 VANZHULA ET AL.

Stice, E., Telch, C. F., & Rizvi, S. L. (2000). Development and validation of Wheaton, M. G., Gershkovich, M., Gallagher, T., Foa, E. B., & Simpson, H. B.
the eating disorder diagnostic scale: A brief self-report measure of (2018). Behavioral avoidance predicts treatment outcome with expo-
anorexia, bulimia, and binge-eating disorder. Psychological Assessment, sure and response prevention for obsessive–compulsive disorder.
12, 123–131. Depression and Anxiety, 35, 256–263.
Treasure, J., Cardi, V., & Kan, C. (2012). Eating in eating disorders.
European Eating Disorders Review, 20, e42–e49. SUPPORTING INF ORMATION
Ulian, M. D., Unsain, R. F., Sato, P. D. M., Pereira, P. D. R.,
Additional supporting information may be found online in the
Stelmo, I. D. C., Sabatini, F., & Scagliusi, F. B. (2013). Current and
previous eating practices among women recovered from anorexia Supporting Information section at the end of this article.
nervosa: A qualitative approach. Jornal Brasileiro de Psiquiatria, 62,
275–284.
Verplanken, P., & Orbell, S. (2003). Reflections on past behavior: A How to cite this article: Vanzhula IA, Sala M, Christian C, et al.
self-report index of habit strength. Journal of Applied Social Psychol- Avoidance coping during mealtimes predicts higher eating
ogy, 33(6), 1313–1330. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2003.
disorder symptoms. Int J Eat Disord. 2020;1–6. https://doi.
tb01951.x
Walsh, B. T. (2013). The enigmatic persistence of anorexia nervosa. Ameri- org/10.1002/eat.23254
can Journal of Psychiatry, 170, 477–484.

You might also like