You are on page 1of 8

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/285244768

Classic tower of Hanoi, planning skills, and the Indian elderly

Article · September 2015

CITATIONS READS

3 612

4 authors:

Rakesh Balachandar Ravikesh Tripathi


Simon Fraser University National Institute of Mental Health and Neuro Sciences
78 PUBLICATIONS   1,089 CITATIONS    38 PUBLICATIONS   240 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Srikala Bharath Keshav Janakiprasad Kumar


National Institute of Mental Health and Neuro Sciences National Institute of Mental Health and Neuro Sciences
98 PUBLICATIONS   716 CITATIONS    104 PUBLICATIONS   1,424 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Integrated Intervention Program for Alcoholism (IIPA) View project

dissertation on social cognition in PD View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Ravikesh Tripathi on 05 February 2016.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


East Asian Arch Psychiatry 2015;25:108-14 Original Article

Classic Tower of Hanoi, Planning Skills, and the


Indian Elderly
R Balachandar, R Tripathi, S Bharath, K Kumar

Abstract
Objective: Elderly populations are vulnerable to age-related cognitive decline. Planning, a frontal lobe
function, is reported to be affected in the elderly population. There is a paucity of studies which assessed
planning skills in the elderly Indian population. The present study aimed to examine the utility of the
classic Tower of Hanoi in the assessment of planning skills of elderly Indian subjects.
Methods: A total of 215 (60 of whom were females, all aged 55-80 years) cognitively normal elders and
24 patients with mild Alzheimer’s disease were recruited. All subjects provided informed consent and their
planning skills were assessed using the classic Tower of Hanoi. Performance at each level was measured
by the total time taken to solve, number of moves to solve, and the number of rule violations. Receiver
operating characteristic curve analysis was exploratively performed to test the utility of the Tower of
Hanoi in differentiating patients with mild Alzheimer’s disease from those who were cognitively normal.
Results: Performance measures of cognitively normal group steeply worsened with increasing complexity.
With receiver operating characteristic curve analysis, patients with mild Alzheimer’s disease were poorly
differentiated from cognitively normal group according to their Tower of Hanoi performance.
Conclusion: The Tower of Hanoi test is of limited value for the assessment of planning skills in the
Indian elderly population. There is a need to modify and develop a suitable neuropsychology tool to
assess the planning skills of elderly Indian subjects and further validate it.

Key words: Aged; Intelligence tests; Mild cognitive impairment

Dr Rakesh Balachandar, PhD, Department of Clinical Neurosciences , National ageing.6 Planning is defined as a process of formulating a
Institute of Mental Health and Neurosciences, Bangalore, India.
Dr Ravikesh Tripathi, MPhil, PhD, Consultant Clinical Psychologist, Narayana
sequence of operations intended to achieve a final goal.7 It
Hrudayalaya, Bangalore, India. involves a 2-stage process: initially to formulate a logical
Prof. Srikala Bharath, MD, FRCPsych, Department of Psychiatry, National strategy to determine the sequence of actions required to
Institute of Mental Health and Neurosciences, Bangalore, India.
Prof. Keshav Kumar, PhD Department of Clinical Psychology, National
achieve the specific goal and later the ability to monitor and
Institute of Mental Health and Neurosciences, Bangalore, India. guide towards its successful completion.3,8 The Tower of
Hanoi (TOH), a neuropsychological tool, is often used to
Address for correspondence: Prof. Srikala Bharath, Department of Psychiatry,
National Institute of Mental Health and Neurosciences, Bangalore 560029,
assess planning skills.9,10
India. Planning along with working memory, verbal and
Tel: (91-80) 2699 5271; Fax: (91-80) 2656 4830; visual organisation, judgement and reasoning are crucial
Email: srikala.bharath@gmail.com
for performing instrumental activities of daily living.
Submitted: 23 December 2014; Accepted: 3 March 2015 They are tested during successful completion of a TOH
task. Due to age-related cognitive decline, planning skills
among the elderly fade compared with young adults and
hence performance of tasks in the TOH also worsens with
advancing age.10
Introduction The importance of planning skills is seen among
patients with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and
Executive functions act at the highest level of cognition Alzheimer’s disease (AD). Executive skills compensate
to enable optimal performance of the activities of daily for the memory impairment in amnestic MCI. Dysfunction
living. Studies of ageing have demonstrated predominant in these cognitive domains has a higher prediction for the
age-related decline in executive functions compared conversion of amnestic MCI to frank AD.11 Further, patients
with other cognitive domains.1-4 Executive functions are with AD develop deficits in planning ability during the
presumably mediated by the frontal and temporal lobe course of their illness.12
neuronal system.1,5 One of the important subcomponents of Previous studies among the elderly Indian
executive functions, planning, is also mediated via neuronal population have documented a lack of cognitive tests that
circuits involving the frontal lobe. Neuroimaging studies include planning skills.13-15 As an important cognitive
have reported predominant frontal lobe atrophy in healthy domain, planning needs to be assessed among the elderly

108 © 2015 Hong Kong College of Psychiatrists


Tower of Hanoi and Planning Skills

population. We observed a need for a validated and reliable the first 4 levels of the TOH task, starting from level 1
neuropsychological test to assess the planning skills of an (2-disk task) to level 4 (5-disk task) with increasing levels
elderly Indian population. of complexity. At each level the disks were placed in the
In the current study, we aimed to explore the primary position (extreme left peg), and shifted to the final
utility of the TOH to assess planning skills in an elderly position (extreme right peg) while complying with the
Indian population, including patients with early AD. We rules. On completing the task, subjects moved to the next
hypothesised that the classic TOH offered a means to higher level. Performance measures were based on 3 main
assess planning skills in our elderly population. We also parameters: number of moves required, total time taken to
hypothesised that patients diagnosed with mild AD would complete the task, and number of rule violations during
perform less well on the TOH than matched cognitively each task (mistakes).
healthy controls.
Statistical Analyses
Methods All statistical analyses were performed using R statistics.
As the data (education and performance measure scores)
This study was conducted at the Geriatric Clinic & Services, followed a normal distribution (as assessed using Shapiro-
National Institute of Mental Health and Neurosciences Wilk test) 2-tailed independent t test was used for group
(NIMHANS), India after institutional ethics committee comparisons. Univariate general linear model analysis was
approval. After obtaining informed consent, all subjects also used to detect age-related changes between the groups,
underwent NIMHANS Neuropsychiatry Battery for adjusting for education as a covariate.
Elderly15 testing along with TOH assessment. The randomly selected CNe were individually
A total of 215 cognitively normal elders (CNe) were matched for age, gender, and education with that of mild
recruited from the community (elderly forums) and elderly AD patients to compare their TOH performance measures
caregivers of patients visiting NIMHANS. They were aged using independent t test. We also explored the sensitivity
between 55 and 80 years, and 60 of whom were females. and specificity of the TOH in discriminating / differentiating
All cognitively healthy volunteers gave informed consent patients with mild AD from CNe matched for age,
and were screened for neuropsychiatric disorders using gender, and education by performing receiver operating
Instruments for Comprehensive Evaluation of the Elderly,16 characteristic (ROC) curve analysis.
and, subsequently, the Hindi Mental State Examination
(HMSE).17 All elderly healthy volunteers were independent Results
and autonomous in their activities of daily living. None
reported any subjective memory complaints (relevant Demographic details of subjects are shown in Table 1.
indicator of cognitive decline)18 or were taking any Comparison of results between CNe subgroups, as well as
medication that could interfere with cognition. The CNe that between patients with mild AD and matched CNe on
were divided into 2 groups (< 60 and ≥ 60 years). Subjects TOH are presented in Tables 2 and 3, respectively..
with a score of ≥ 24 in HMSE were included as CNe.19
Those with a score of < 24 were evaluated for causes of Performance of Cognitively Normal Elders
possible cognitive deficits and appropriately managed. Cognitively normal elders aged from 55 to 80 years with
Another 24 patients with AD (4 of whom were a male predominance (73%). Those in the subgroup aged
females), with a mean (± standard deviation) age of 67.1 ≥ 60 years were significantly highly educated than those
years, were recruited from the geriatric clinic, NIMHANS. < 60 years (p = 0.04). In this sample of CNe a steep increase
These patients were clinically diagnosed according to in the performance scores (time taken and total moves) was
the National Institute on Aging–Alzheimer’s Association observed as task complexity increased. The number of CNe
criteria,20 and with a Clinical Dementia Rating Scale score who could successfully complete the task also reduced as
of < 121 by a senior professor of psychiatry / neurology. the complexity increased. An explorative independent t
test between 2 CNe subgroups (< 60 and ≥ 60 years) did
Classic Tower of Hanoi Task not reveal any statistical difference in their performance
The TOH task involves the shifting of all disks (2-5 disks) measures (Table 2). Univariate general linear model
from their primary position to a final position in a minimum analysis revealed that education did not significantly affect
number of moves without violating certain rules. These the performance measures in the CNe group; further there
rules include: (a) a single disk should be moved at a time; was no significant difference in the performance measures
(b) disks should be shifted only on the pegs; and (c) a larger between the 2 age-groups after controlling for education.
disk may not be placed over a smaller disk.10
Studies that have explored planning skills of the Performance of Patients with Mild Alzheimer’s
elderly have used 3- and 4-disk tasks of the classic TOH.3,9 Disease
In order to observe the feasibility of the test in the Indian Subjects with mild AD experienced severe difficulty in
elderly, we included 1-step, lower (2 disks) and higher (5 completing the TOH tasks. Only about 75% of patients
disks) tasks in our planning assessment. Hence we used with mild AD could complete the level 1 (2-disk) task.

East Asian Arch Psychiatry 2015, Vol 25, No.3 109


R Balachandar, R Tripathi, S Bharath, et al

Table 1. Demographic details of subjects.*


Cognitively normal elders Patients with mild p Value
(n = 215) Alzheimer’s disease (n = 24)
Age (years) 63.8 ± 6.4 67.1 ± 8.4 0.07
Gender (female / male) 58 / 157 4 / 20 0.27
Duration of education (years) 12.7 ± 5 13.0 ± 4.9 0.9
*
Data are shown as mean ± standard deviation, unless otherwise specified.

Table 2. Performance measures of subgroups at various levels of Tower of Hanoi.*

Cognitively normal elders Cognitively normal elders Patients with mild


(< 60 years; n = 102) (≥ 60 years; n = 112) Alzheimer’s disease (n = 24)
Duration of education (years) 11.9 ± 5 13.3 ± 4.9 12.7 ± 5
Gender (female / male) 36 / 67 22 / 90 4 / 24
Two-disk task
Total moves 3.1 ± 0.5 3 ± 0.2 3.7 ± 2.2
Mean extra moves 0.1 0 0.7
Total time (sec) 5.7 ± 1.8 5.8 ± 1.7 12.7 ± 10
Rule violations / mistakes 0.02 ± 0.1 0.01 ± 0.1 0.3 ± 0.7
% of Patients completed 100% 100% 75%
Three-disk task
Total moves 11.8 ± 5.6 11.5 ± 5 10.4 ± 7.2
Mean extra moves 4.8 4.5 5.4
Total time (sec) 69.9 ± 57.2 62 ± 40 70.2 ± 49.6
Rule violations / mistakes 0.6 ± 0.8 0.3 ± 0.7 1.5 ± 1.2
% of Patients completed 83.4% 89.2% 50%
Four-disk task
Total moves 27.9 ± 12.3 27.2 ± 11.5 30 ± 26
Mean extra moves 12.9 12.2 15
Total time (sec) 145.5 ± 84.4 138.4 ± 73.5 158.4 ± 87.4
Rule violations / mistakes 0.4 ± 0.7 0.5 ± 0.8 2.8 ± 4.4
% of Patients completed 72.8% 79.4% 33.3%
Five-disk task
Total moves 69.3 ± 20.2 69.6 ± 28.9 -
Average extra moves 38.3 38.6 -
Total time (sec) 278.1 ± 136.4 272 ± 141.1 -
Rule violations / mistakes 0.5 ± 0.7 0.5 ± 0.7 -
% of Patients completed 27.1% 34.8% -
*
Data are shown as mean ± standard deviation, unless otherwise specified.

In subsequent higher levels the number further dropped. Discussion


Patients with mild AD exhibited a trend of reduced
performance compared with CNe (Table 3). A significant The present study aimed to observe the utility of the
difference (p < 0.05) was observed for performance classic TOH to assess planning skills in an elderly Indian
measures of time taken for the 2-disk task and rule population. The TOH was administered to CNe and a sample
violations in 3-disk task. Explorative ROC curve analysis of patients with mild AD. We also attempted to explore
of performance measures revealed poor sensitivity and age-related cognitive decline by stratifying the sample of
specificity in differentiating patients with mild AD from CNe into 2 groups based on their age (< 60 years and ≥ 60
CNe (Fig). years). A steep incline in the performance scores (total time

110 East Asian Arch Psychiatry 2015, Vol 25, No.3


Tower of Hanoi and Planning Skills

Table 3. Comparison of performance measures between patients with mild Alzheimer’s disease and matched cognitively
normal elders.
Cognitively normal elders Patients with mild p Value
(n = 215) Alzheimer’s disease (n = 24)
Two-disk task
Total moves 3.1 ± 0.5 3.7 ± 2.2 0.14
Total time (sec) 6.2 ± 2.1 12.7 ± 10 0.03
Rule violations / mistakes 0.01 ± 0.1 0.3 ± 0.7 0.19
% of Patients completed 100% 75%
Three-disk task
Total moves 9.8 ± 3.3 10.4 ± 7.2 0.74
Total time (sec) 56.9 ± 31.4 70.2 ± 49.6 0.3
Rule violations / mistakes 0.3 ± 0.5 1.5 ± 1.2 0.001
% of Patients completed 84.5% 50%
Four-disk task
Total moves 26.5 ± 10.7 30 ± 26 0.6
Total time (sec) 146.7 ± 70.2 158.4 ± 87.4 0.6
Rule violations / mistakes 0.7 ± 0.9 2.8 ± 4.4 0.06
% of Patients completed 73.6% 33.3%
Five-disk task*
Total moves 70.1 ± 28.5 - -
Total time (sec) 278.1 ± 143 - -
Rule violations / mistakes 1.1 ± 1.5 - -
% of Patients completed 34.5% - -

*
The task could not be performed in patients with mild Alzheimer’s disease, therefore p values could not be calculated.

taken, number of moves) was observed among CNe groups therefore imperative to use ecologically valid and culturally
with increasing task complexity. We would like to stress appropriate tests to make definitive comments about the
that the number of additional steps taken by the elderly to true cognitive status of a particular individual.
solve tasks at each level also increased. This suggests that Previous studies have documented age-related decline
CNe experienced difficulties in planning, completing, and in fluid intelligence such as planning ability.1 Our attempt
proceeding to subsequent higher levels of the task. The to explore the age-related cognitive decline by stratifying
performance scores of our current study were inferior to the the elderly population into 2 groups (< 60 years and ≥ 60
age-matched scores of another study.3 Interestingly, CNe of years) could not demonstrate such changes. We propose
our study exhibited inferior performances in the classical the following observations possibly contributing to this
TOH despite having more years of formal education in finding. First, ageing studies that documented age-related
comparison with the above-mentioned study.3 We can changes have predominantly compared young adults and
confer that the normative data of classic TOH performance elderly subjects.3 The age difference between the 2 groups
for Indian cognitively normal elderly subjects are different. in our study population was very small: group 1 CNe (< 60
School-educated elderly subjects often become years) were aged between 55 and 60 years and group 2 CNe
functionally illiterate due to underutilisation of their reading (≥ 60 years) comprised volunteers aged between 61 and 80
and writing skills, as these skills acquired during schooling years. Age-related decline in fluid intelligence is likely a
may be redundant for their lifestyle or occupation. The gradual process; hence narrow age differences in our study
inferior performance on classical TOH tasks of the Indian failed to demonstrate age-related decline in the planning
elderly in this study, though more educated (13.3 ± 4.9 process. Second, although our CNe were randomly recruited
years) than those in another study (6.3 ± 1.7 years),3 may be and stratified on the basis of age, the older group (≥ 60
explained by functional illiteracy. Alternatively, variations years) were significantly (p = 0.04) more educated than the
in the performance of individuals with a low education younger group (< 60 years).
level could be attributed to one or many other reasons such Education, an important factor in cognitive reserve,
as task familiarity, performance anxiety, attitude towards may contribute to our current finding.22-24 Fewer than 12
the testing situation, and appropriateness of the test for years of formal education indicates a pre-college level
a given population with a low level of education. It is of education, whereas receiving education of ≥ 12 years

East Asian Arch Psychiatry 2015, Vol 25, No.3 111


R Balachandar, R Tripathi, S Bharath, et al

(a)
100

80

60
Sensitivity (%)

40

20 Total time
Total moves
Rule violations
Reference line
0
100 80 60 40 20 0
Specificity (%)

(b)
100

80

60
Sensitivity (%)

40

20 Total time
Total moves
Rule violations
Reference line
0
100 80 60 40 20 0
Specificity (%)

Figure. Receiver operating characteristic curve analyses for the (a) 2-disk task and (b) 3-disk task.

112 East Asian Arch Psychiatry 2015, Vol 25, No.3


Tower of Hanoi and Planning Skills

indicates a college and postgraduate level. Because our CNe CNe groups. Our sample of CNe were all educated, contrary
older group (≥ 60 years) received a mean education of > 12 to our initial intention to recruit predominantly illiterate
years, the majority would have potentially been exposed to elderly.
situations that exercised their cognitive functions including
planning skills, and would thus have performed better than Conclusion
the less educated CNe (< 60 years). Univariate general
linear model analysis, which was used to control the effects Planning skills need to be assessed regularly in cognitively
of education, did not reveal significant differences between healthy elderly subjects, patients with MCI and those with
the 2 age-groups. AD. In this group of elderly we have demonstrated the
need for a culturally appropriate neuropsychological tool to
Performance of Patients with Mild Alzheimer’s assess planning ability. Finally, there is a need to developing
Disease normative data for elderly Indian subjects after validating
Patients with early AD experienced severe difficulty in the planning tool.
solving TOH, as demonstrated by the proportion who could
successfully complete the 2-disk task (level 1) of TOH. Acknowledgement
About 25% of AD patients could not complete this first
level; this percentage further increased to 50%, 66.7% and We thank all participants for their complete cooperation.
100% for the 3-disk, 4-disk, and 5-disk tasks, respectively.
Patients with mild AD performed worse than the Declaration
matched CNe for all performance measures (Table 3). A
significant statistical difference (p < 0.05) was observed The authors declared no conflict of interest in this study.
in rule violations in the 3-disk task and time taken for the
2-disk task. An insignificant difference observed in other References
performance measures could be due to fewer patients
1. Finch CE. The neurobiology of middle-age has arrived. Neurobiol
successfully completing the levels when compared with Aging 2009;30:515-20.
matched CNe (Table 3), hence the statistical analysis could 2. Gazzaley A, Sheridan MA, Cooney JW, D’Esposito M. Age-related
not detect significant differences. Despite the observed deficits in component processes of working memory. Neuropsychology
differences, ROC analysis of the classic TOH performance 2007;21:532-9.
3. Sorel O, Pennequin V. Aging of the planning process: the role of
had poor discriminative ability to differentiate patients with
executive functioning. Brain Cogn 2008;66:196-201.
mild AD and CNe (Fig). Patients with AD predominantly 4. Stricks L, Pittman J, Jacobs DM, Sano M, Stern Y. Normative data
suffer from episodic memory deficit during the early for a brief neuropsychological battery administered to English- and
stages of illness and later develop other cognitive domain Spanish-speaking community-dwelling elders. J Int Neuropsychol Soc
deficits including executive functions.12 In addition, studies 1998;4:311-8.
5. Allain P, Nicoleau S, Pinon K, Etcharry-Bouyx F, Barré J, Berrut G, et
that involve patients with early AD have demonstrated
al. Executive functioning in normal aging: a study of action planning
the importance of executive functions in overcoming using the Zoo Map Test. Brain Cogn 2005;57:4-7.
episodic memory deficits and performing activities of daily 6. Tisserand DJ, Jolles J. On the involvement of prefrontal networks in
living.25,26 Thus we expected that explorative ROC would cognitive ageing. Cortex 2003;39:1107-28.
not discriminate patients with mild AD and healthy controls. 7. Hayes-Roth B, Hayes-Roth F. A cognitive model of planning. In:
Collins AM, Smith EE, editors. Readings in cognitive science: A
Further work with a larger sample and inclusion of another
perspective from psychology and artificial intelligence. San Mateo,
study group with moderate AD would throw more light on CA: Morgan Kaufmann; 1979: 496-513.
this issue. 8. Grafman J. Plans, actions, and mental sets. In: Perecman E, editor.
The following points should be emphasised in this Integrating theory and practice in clinical neuropsychology. Hillsdale,
study. For reasons of long testing hours and the steep NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1989: 93-138.
9. Brennan M, Welsh MC, Fisher CB. Aging and executive function
increase in complexity of tests, our CNe experienced
skills: an examination of a community-dwelling older adult population.
cognitive fatigue.27 There is a need to develop planning Percept Mot Skills 1997;84(3 Pt 2):1187-97.
tasks that start at an easy level and gradually increase in 10. Goel V, Pullara SD, Grafman J. A computational model of frontal lobe
complexity. A task of this kind would accustom individuals dysfunction: working memory and the Tower of Hanoi task. Cogn Sci
to a testing situation and encourage them to perform better 2001;25:287-313.
at subsequent higher levels. Further, we recommend that 11. Marshall GA, Rentz DM, Frey MT, Locascio JJ, Johnson KA, Sperling
RA. Executive function and instrumental activities of daily living in
tasks be designed such that they can be completed in a short mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers
period of time. Dement 2011;7:300-8.
Developing a tool that fulfils the above criteria has the 12. Broks P, Lines C, Atchison L, Challenor J, Traub M, Foster C, et al.
advantage of obtaining a baseline planning performance. Neuropsychological investigation of anterior and posterior cortical
It is advantageous to record baseline planning deficits in function in early-stage probable Alzheimer’s disease. Behav Neurol
1996;9:135-48.
patients with AD, especially to monitor disease progression 13. Das SK, Banerjee TK, Mukherjee CS, Bose P, Biswas A, Hazra A, et
or to evaluate the effects of intervention. A limitation of our al. An urban community-based study of cognitive function among non-
study is the heterogeneity in education level between the 2 demented elderly population in India. Neurol Asia 2006;11:37-48.

East Asian Arch Psychiatry 2015, Vol 25, No.3 113


R Balachandar, R Tripathi, S Bharath, et al

14. Mathuranath PS, Hodges JR, Mathew R, Cherian PJ, George A, Bak Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement 2011;7:263-9.
TH. Adaptation of the ACE for a Malayalam speaking population in 21. Morris JC. The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): current version and
southern India. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2004;19:1188-94. scoring rules. Neurology 1993;43:2412-4.
15. Tripathi R, Kumar JK, Bharath S, Marimuthu P, Varghese M. Clinical 22. Bouazzaoui B, Isingrini M, Fay S, Angel L, Vanneste S, Clarys D, et
validity of NIMHANS neuropsychological battery for elderly: A al. Aging and self-reported internal and external memory strategy uses:
preliminary report. Indian J Psychiatry 2013;55:279-82. the role of executive functioning. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2010;135:59-
16. Sadanand S, Shivakumar P, Girish N, Loganathan S, Bagepally BS, 66.
Kota LN, et al. Identifying elders with neuropsychiatric problems in a 23. Cagney KA, Lauderdale DS. Education, wealth, and cognitive function
clinical setting. J Neurosci Rural Pract 2013;4(Suppl 1):S24-30. in later life. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2002;57:163-72.
17. Ganguli M, Chandra V, Gilby JE, Ratcliff G, Sharma SD, Pandav R, 24. van Hooren SA, Valentijn AM, Bosma H, Ponds RW, van Boxtel MP,
et al. Cognitive test performance in a community-based nondemented Jolles J. Cognitive functioning in healthy older adults aged 64-81: a
elderly sample in rural India: the Indo-U.S. Cross-National Dementia cohort study into the effects of age, sex, and education. Neuropsychol
Epidemiology Study. Int Psychogeriatr 1996;8:507-24. Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 2007;14:40-54.
18. Benito-León J, Mitchell AJ, Vega S, Bermejo-Pareja F. A population- 25. Perry RJ, Watson P, Hodges JR. The nature and staging of attention
based study of cognitive function in older people with subjective dysfunction in early (minimal and mild) Alzheimer’s disease:
memory complaints. J Alzheimers Dis 2010;22:159-70. relationship to episodic and semantic memory impairment.
19. Tiwari SC, Srivastava G, Tripathi RK, Pandey NM, Agarwal GG, Neuropsychologia 2000;38:252-71.
Pandey S, et al. Prevalence of psychiatric morbidity amongst the 26. Baudic S, Barba GD, Thibaudet MC, Smagghe A, Remy P, Traykov
community dwelling rural older adults in northern India. Indian J Med L. Executive function deficits in early Alzheimer’s disease and their
Res 2013;138:504-14. relations with episodic memory. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2006;21:15-
20. McKhann GM, Knopman DS, Chertkow H, Hyman BT, Jack CR 21.
Jr, Kawas CH, et al. The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer’s 27. Ackerman PL, Kanfer R. Test length and cognitive fatigue: an empirical
disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging- examination of effects on performance and test-taker reactions. J Exp
Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Psychol Appl 2009;15:163-81.

114 East Asian Arch Psychiatry 2015, Vol 25, No.3

View publication stats

You might also like